AN ANALYSIS OF PEER ACCEPTANCE AND PERCEIVED PROBLEMS OF GIT-TED JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS Thesis for the Dogma of Ed. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY Donald Wayne Wood 1965 1| mum; mum Lu! (l Q fill 11m will nun 1'4 Ests This is to certify that the thesis entitled AN ANALYSIS OF PEER ACCEPTANCE AND PERCEIVED PROBLEMS OF GIFTED JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS presented by DONALD WAYNE WOOD has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ed.D. degree in Elementary and Special Education 0-169 2L L433. it I‘ 'l :I‘OOea: ABSTRACT AN ANALYSIS OF PEER ACCEPTANCE AND PERCEIVED PROBLEMS OF GIFTED JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS by Donald Wayne Wood The purpose of the first of these interrelated research studies was to evaluate the peer acceptance of the gifted in comparison to the nongifted in the Junior , high school. The purposes of the second study werea (l) to identify those perceived problems of personal and ~social adjustment held in common by all isolates as well as those peculiar to each subgroup, nongifted isolates and ‘ “gifted isolates; and, (2) to identify those perceived ‘ l ”problems held in common by all gifted students as well as l j I. those peculiar to each subgroup, isolate gifted and non- I "isolate gifted. _ Participating in the first study were 2, 733 students I '. n3? V1389 boys and 1344 girls) in grades seven and eight of ,;-; re 118 of these same students (67 boys and 51 girls) These students all resided mighth and ninth graders. fiéonia, Michigan, a large residential suburb of h';«;_ 'ngith higher than average; mean educational s飣§< y :égeconomic level. \ -1 up>£,.tir . berg . c g;' as isalatg 1 1 1 Donald Wayne Wood ' < The California Test of Mental Maturity was admin- ‘ istered, and the 2,733 students were classified by their ‘ {resulting total IQs into six psychometric categories, ,A ranging from highly gifted through educable retarded. A A sociometric device was administered employing three acceptance-oriented questions, each calling for three choices. The students were classifed by the total number of choices received into six sociometric categories '5 ranging from star through isolate. The psychometric and sociometric taxonomies were used to create a grid to compart the total group into thirty-six subgroups for comparison of their relative sizes proportionate to the total group. To facilitate the ' second study, the students were then divided into four , y ’E research categories: gifted isolate, nongifted isolate, {gifted nonisolate, and nongifted nonisolate. ;* In the second study, in order to show the full effect :fdfigacceptance versus nonacceptance, the members of each .' eluded.only high status Students. Regarding the Donald Wayne Wood Three inventories, Vineland Social Maturity Scale, ‘,iiMooney Problem Check List, and Rohde Sentence Completion ' Method, were administered to the four research groups: Agifted isolate, nongifted isolate, gifted nonisolate, and .inongifted nonisolate, to identify those perceived problems N“-Ahe1d in common by all isolates, as well as those peculiar to each isolate subgroup; and, to identify those perceived ‘problems held in common by all gifted, as well as those I ; peculiar to each gifted subgroup. r I The major results indicate that, although the gifted _ 'did not receive greater acceptance as a group than did the il;irapid learners, gifted students of this age group are well :1 accepted by their peers; those students with high socio- ;:§mtric status are more often those of above-average intelli— .I€:: .. {genes gifted did not chocse primarily from within their _ 4. j |_E,‘ group but did choose those with above-average . l agbility as friends; students with above-average ability are velved in both a greater scope and depth of mutual choice i 3'-' AN ANALYSIS OF PEER ACCEPTANCE AND PERCEIVED PROBLEMS OF GIFTED JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS '} , ' t by Donald Wayne Wood A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of if? "I" '1'1~p“'fi‘l FA \ 1 73 1 - iflfi 1 .3 h if 3' [and f a , E s ‘. F}- If I' ’ ‘ A 1-1, .I 1 x I ’7 d. 0 ‘ Y _ I Copyright by ._, V DONALD WAYNE WOOD ‘ 9 yids-‘3 ; e r: A s rh, ‘ ‘ ' "73H€ “A! td1fiit ,'“fl“‘, . ‘r' ' . . : iu.;hu TIPix'hdgk'wJW* 'r‘ ' ,A’e'i.‘ -L." Ly. ‘A 3 s ’ 3." IL L ”71 L 1V": "1 UV» 965"?31W% .- ‘ spy Mss'amz' c. wuwu m We ‘ :Wmnos “at m - - \ ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS To: Dr. John E. Jordan, my committee chairman, and Dr. Gregory A. Miller for their continued interest, encouragement, and guidance throughout my entire doctoral program. To: Dr. William K. Durr for his early assistance in planning the organization of my studies of the gifted 'and for his friendly cooperation and understanding direction throughout the completion of these studies. To: Dr. Charles R. Hoffer, advisor in my minor field, who continued to spend his own valuable time upon becoming professor emeritus, to meet with my committee and me. To: Livonia Public Schools, Livonia, Michigan, for permit- ' ting me to gather the data for these research studies from the five junior high schools; and to Dr. Paul Johnson, Assistant Superintendent, for evaluating the initial prOposal and for his valuable suggestions. To: The principals of the five Junior high schools, Mr. Kenneth Cogswell, Mr. William McMurtrey, Mr. William Warren, Mr. James Casebere, and Dr. Bruce Hudson, for their cooperation in making the necessary scheduling adjustments to provide time and room space for the ad- ministration of the testing necessary for these studies; and to their teachers for the administration of the {L sociometric device. ijbt. Mrs. Mildred Tungate and Mr. Herbert Hutchison, coun- . . £"'i' selors, for their assistance in proctoring the adjust- " éu.ment inventories; and to all Livonia Junior high school TJZJ.counselors for the administration of the psychometric 3? al'ftest. T" .' mar. Stanley Marzolf of Illinois State University for: is: early assurance that the special serviCes area: " . a logical professional choice, and for .hiL 'u" as contribution to my necessary backgrounfl‘ufl“ , 4 ledge in the behavioral sciences.i- e 5%; n$s¢c ‘ h Donald W. Wood Candidate for the degree of Doctor of Education K Thesis: | An Analysis of Peer Acceptance and Perceived Problems of Gifted Junior High School Students 1 Outline of Studies: 1 ‘. Major subject: Special Education Cognate: Guidance and Counseling 1 Minor subject: Sociology L Biographical Items: . Born: August 21, 1926; Putnam, Illinois 4] Undergraduate Studies: Illinois State University, B.S. Ed. " Degree, Divisions of Elementary and Special Education, June, 1949. Graduate Studies: Illinois State University, M.S. Ed. Degree, Divisions of School Administration and Special Education, June, 1954; University of Florida, 1955-56; , Central Michigan University, 1959- 61; Wayne State Univer- -u sity, 1962- 63; Michigan State University, Ed. D. Degree, ‘ June, 1965. Experience: Visiting Counselor, Bloomington, 111., Public Schools, 1947—48; Teacher, sixth and seventh-grade math, science, reading, Metcalf Training School, Normal, 111., 1948-49: Teacher, hospitalized, homebound, Bloomington, 111., Public Schools, 1949-50; Hospital Teacher, physi- cally handicapped, emotionally disturbed, University of Michigan Hospital, Ann Arbor, 1950-51; Teacher, educable mentally handicapped, Fairchild Hall of Special Education, Illinois State University, summer, 1951; Teacher, educable - ! mentally handicapped, Elmhurst, 111., Public Schools, . 1951-53; Educational Director, Hamilton County Council for Retarded Children, Cincinnati, Ohio, 1953-55; Director Special Services, Monroe County Schools, Key West, Fla., 1955-56; Instructor, psychology, special education, guidance, Central Michigan University, 1956—61; Counselor, Livonia, Mich. , Public Schools, 1961--. - ,Professional Memberships: American Association of Univer- ” sity Professors; National Association for Gifted Children; Council of Administrators, Supervisors, and Coordinators of Special Education; Northwest Wayne County Counselors ‘ Association; United Education Profession including DiVisions of Department of Classroom Teachers, Council "for Exceptional Children, Teacher Education, and Associa— tion for the Gifted. are: Fellow, American Association on Mental Deficiency; C Delta Kappa; Honorary Life Member, Ohio State Associa-' “.1” 0 pm of Teachers of Mentally Deficient Youth; and Who's ‘ ,g‘qfi— 11$ American Education. ”1 iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ) ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii VITA . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv . J .LIST OF TABLES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii LIST OF APPENDICES o o o o t n a o o u o o ' x r ‘ Chapter 1 . I I INTRODUCTION 0 o o o o c o o I O O :1' Purposes of the Study . . . . 2 Justification of the Study . . . . .2 . Limitations . . . . . . . . 3 ; _ Definitions of Terms . . . . . . . 3! The Thesis in Perspective. . . . . . . ‘8 -;o II. REVIEW OF RELATED RESEARCH . . . . . . . :9 t 1. {a .’JQ‘ Intelligence and Sociometric Results . . ‘9' 1.7:» Highly Gifted c a o o o u I u a Q ~ 9 h I Gifted o O a I I c a o I I O O 11 Mutual Choices. . . . 15 Personal- Social Adjustment and Sociometric I Results How an Individual is Perceived by Others Peer Evaluation. . . . . . . . Adult Ratings . . How an Individual Perceives Himself. . Self-Report Techniques . ._ . . . Projective Techniques.‘ . . . . . summary I a o o in o- n 6 .5. o n i "D i i o I o o :.RESEIRCH PROCEDURES AND TECHNIQUES OF H... . 13.7.‘Vlbilr; Hath“ to; 12%.! Lsh‘d Pnoc gear 9r“?! ,3 Wimmc fl. mesh " Wfiatuntaflu s, z , L§ ' Chapter Methods of Gathering the Data . California Test of Mental Maturity Sociometric Test . . . . Adjustment Inventories . . . Vineland Social Maturity Scale. . . Mooney Problem Check List . . . Rohde Sentence Completion Test. . . Methods of Collating and Recording the Data. Psychometric Data. . . . } Psychometric Categories Sociometric Data . . L Matrix Tables . Sociometric Categories A Psychometric and Sociometric Categorization of Data . Summary Tables Mutual Choice Tables l Frequency Tables . . ; Adjustment Inventory Data . Data Analysis. . IV. ANALYSIS OF DATA Psychometric Data Sociometric Data . - Psychometric and Sociometric Categorization of Data . . . . . . Decile Distributions. Comparting the Data . Psychometric and Sociometric Means Mutual Choice Data . . . Distribution of Mutual Choices Patterning of Mutual Choices Comparative Involvement in Mutual Choosing . . . . Adjustment Inventory Data. Locating Perceived Problems of Isolates and Nonisolates . . . Vineland Social Maturity Scale. . . . Mooney Problem Check List . . . . Rohde Sentence Completion Method . Locating Perceived Problems of the Gifted and Nongifted . . . Vineland Social Maturity Scale. . . . Mooney Problem Check List . . . . . Rohde Sentence Completion Method . Locating Perceived Problems of the Two Isolate and the Two Gifted Groups . . . Vineland Social Maturity Scale . . . Mooney Problem Check List . . . . Rohde Sentence Completion Method . . . vi 33" V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND APPLICATIONS . . . 115 Summary............. 1.3.5- . . Conclusions . . . . . . 11-? '—'*'N- Implications and Applications. . . . . . “I26 . ‘ Applications . . . . . '123 I1 ' ~ Recommendations for Further Research . . . 128= \‘msnsxcssv....»............ 1.39. urmx.css...............13A. {(-é'g. i .L; :qh'. No-93?) «.0 I i "t 135$5Kfp 4 1 I 373’ ‘l h T: 3" J ' If A. 2.4;”. 'l ‘4! "netrlu _ '.::.. -,.a .l.he actionstti W--0roupa ‘n ru:-;; ;n3.u)ug :- .13” '. _-‘-.‘ . - was Banner: 13613118 9“ a: ’ in; is: $7 . if . . ~ j-‘.L‘E LIST OF TABLES . ‘ I‘. r" Table . Page ‘ 3 7 .‘- -f ..;' 1. Number and Per Cent of Students in Each 4"? ' 9; Psychometric Category . . . . . . . . 49 ‘T;}“w 2. Number and Per Cent of Students in Each AVTEA Sociometric Category . . . . . . . . 50‘ ' ;“3 :L - ,, w i ‘J"_."‘ :;;' I "3. Psychometric Decile Distribution With Mean . 'i “ i-fiv Number of Choices . . . . . . . 51 ‘ _ .W‘gfi 5 ~ , A. Sociometric Decile Distribution With Mean IQs . 52 5. Organization of Psychometric Data Under ~ I Sociometric Categories. . . . . . . . 54 y.. 6. Organization of Sociometric Data Under Psye . " I; chometric Categories . . . . . . . . 55_ "‘ "V7.- Compartmentalization of the Psychometric and ’ . SOCiometriC Data- 0 o a o o o s a 57' M '5% 8. Psychometric Means for Sociometric ' " .FWui categories. c o u 0 s s o o o a o 59 ” Why ; “H“, 9. Sociometric Means Means for Psychometric ‘1::[A5 ” Categories. . . . . . . . . . . . 59 ' 3%" in; Psychometric Distribution of Mutual Choices. . 61 h fi-JW: Sociometric Distribution of Mutual Choices . . 63 -Psychometric Patterning of Mutual Choices . . 65' “g: Sociometric Patterning of Mutual Choices ‘éyffl‘ Comparative Involvement of the Psychometric . Groups in Mutual Choosing ' :fifiemparative Involvement of the Sociometric Groups in Mutual Choosing ' Tferénces Between ISelate and Naniééigte ups on 27 Items.af the VII,;. ‘. ‘ unity Scale 1. ., ' "NW f. 1.1%., * ~. { Page Differences Between Isolate and Nonisolate . Groups on “1 Items of the Mooney Problem ' . g .‘M Check List I I I I I I I I I I I 76‘ A“ y ‘ ' " '18. Cathections of Junior High School Isolates - ' ‘ "if . - and NORiSOlateS I I I I I I I I I ‘ 89 _ . " j ‘ 19. Differences Between Gifted and Nongifted W , ‘ ' 7 ~? ‘ a Groups on 27 Items of the Vineland ' ‘.:3 social Maturity scaleu I 9 I I I I I I 85 ' “ ‘ 20. Differences Between Gifted and Nongifted ‘ . ‘.i Ii Groups on 33 Items of the Mooney Problem , ' . -h Che Ck List I I I I I I I I I I I 89 - I ' . 21. Cathections of Junion High School Gifted ?I:: I . and Nongifted I I I I I I I I I I 92 ' «a! 22. Differences Among the Four Subgroups on- 27 fl ‘ Items of the Vineland Social Maturity ‘ .v“ 1 Scale I I I I D I I I U I I 97:. . ‘r‘. : '1 i i I ' ll. ‘23. Vineland Items Isolates Perceive as Problems. 98 j- 1 2H. Vineland Items the Gifted Perceive aS' 4t‘5 Problems . . . . . . . . . . . 101 =3 Q 25. Differences Among the Four Subgroups on 73 , ftx’I Items of the Mooney Problem Check List . . ’ 103 L ;f - . ~ ,1 ' 26. Mooney Check List Items Isolates Perceive as , nfi’ '-‘ Problems . . . . . . . . . 165 . gwip Mooney Check List Items the Gifted Perceive ED e as Problems . . . . . . . 109 i, ; Cathections Among the Four Subgroups of .1 Junior High School Students. . . . . . 112 i., LIST OF APPENDICES ' i _v {V 1‘ "1." Students by CTMM Appendix A. Sociometric Test Form. . '3”...B' Matrix Tables . ‘. . . 7V I‘C.' Summary Table . . . . P in. Mutual Choice Table . . .-r‘ E. Frequency Distribution of 1‘ " Total Scale IQ Scores . ‘ 5"‘F. Frequency Distribution of Students uIE Number Choices Received 'Poi Sutub berstdI * E [M C! 1" P583 135 137 . 131 1145 15a CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Some gifted individuals do not attain social accep- tance, and an error of Judgment may then be made that I their giftedness will be responsible for a continuing lack of acceptance. When a student is the only gifted member ; of a class, he may be Jealously envied in terms of com- parative success. Thus, some gifted students, indeed, may suffer temporary localized lack of acceptance because of their level of intelligence. Other gifted students, like some of their intellec- tually normal peers, may suffer lack of acceptance 1 because of prejudices or intollerances held by their peer V'group that have nothing to do with the level of intelli- v ‘. rIsence 0 .,,;11m It should be recognized that still other gifted fl indents, along with many of their intellectually normal Purposes of the Study The purposes of this investigation are to evaluate “ t': “e! ‘the peer acceptance of gifted students and to compare the /. perceived problems of isolate and nonisolate gifted and 'MS *nongifted students in the Junior high school. ‘ I ' lustification of the Study 3‘ This study can provide teachers and counselors with _many clues concerning the personal and social adjustments ”b 1 _ ,‘ of those students with either high or low sociometric '7 th’ V «’ m J_ I ~acceptance factors will enable them to aid the high ,7 ‘7' y. ' :6} 'fif: -status student in his bid for leadership and the low status "9 3,? status. A better understanding of acceptance and non-- fi;3’ student in his struggle for social acceptance. The results can contribute to leadership training programs by early identification of gifted students with ‘fleadership potential, and by spot— lighting those who have already begun to emerge as leaders. By pointing out'those dividual students with leadership potential who have not ”33$ emerged as leaders, the study will underscore the fer! social relationships of some students indicate the imper- tance of using sociometric procedures and adjustment inven- tories in evaluating the extent to which the school prc-. Bram is meeting the social—emotional needs of each student. The low sociometric status of school dropfouts, truants, and students who are discipline problems, points out some at the effects of faulty social adjustment upon the parsonality development of the individual. When those individuals with leadership potential who are making a faulty social adjustment are identified by this type of study, and teachers and counselors became canvinced of the resulting ego damaging effects, they may be moved to assist these individuals in their effort to attain acceptance. Upon attaining social acceptance, these students may, thereby, acquire the success I - feeling that will cause them to make the necessary effort -* I, ‘ | I ‘ I ”headed for academic success and eventual recognition by _ I I ‘ i §E§}r_peers as potential leaders. I V l Limitations ‘1.- 5“ ’e'IL" 0". It“ u'mr The second phase is limited ta 118 SI; I» HAD: ’ ’ ‘ $178 as éisfith and ninth 'graaem, ,. ‘“W‘£. .31. LUGE‘ I}. ‘sq9¢4w§p 4 2. This study is also limited to the extent that Livonia, Michigan is a well—to—do, residential, suburban I community within the periphery of the Detroit Metropolitan II. area and does not contain the diversity of socio-economic yfir groups of a typical city of 87,000 population. .5 3. The study employed a group test of mental abil- 'MI ity, rather than an individual test, to determine which stu— I dents were placed in each psychometric category. 4. Negative criteria were not used in the socio- metric test for the sake of not evoking or promulgating \Q' an idea that a given student was rejected by one or more of his classmates. This limits the study in that the reader cannot differentiate between a true rejectee and an isolate. The latter may become relatively well accepted by classmates after more opportunity for interaction. Definitions of Terms I“fiflf:§pciometric Terms v-7cm g] The following terms were coined by J. L. Moreno 'Egl93#), the founder of sociometry, and are unique to “fuiétiometric measurement. Ifigcigmetric test.—-This is a method of evaluating :feelings of the group members toward each other with afict'to a common criterion. It requires individuals fiéf‘r ‘,,e given number of associates for some group 1 .I‘; .f Sociometric status.--This term, sometimes spoken of as group status, refers to the number of choices that each individual receives on a sociometric test. §tar.--The term star refers to an individual who receives a large number of choices on a sociometric test. In his original use of the sociometric test, Moreno reported that some of the pupils "attracted so many choices that they captured the center of the stage like stars." Neglectee.--This term is used to identify the indi— vidual who receives relatively few choices on the socio- metric test. Although he receives some choices, he tends to be neglected by the majority of the group members. Neglectees are also referred to as "fringers," since they are located on the fringe of the group. The term neg— lectee is preferred since it is more definitive. Isolate.--The isolate is an individual who receives no choices, either positive or negative, on a sociometric test. On occasions, he may be referred to as an "outsider" or a "social island,” although these designations are not as common as the term isolate. Most of the individuals receiving no choices are truly isolates; others might be rejectees if the sociometric test contained the possi- bility of negative choices; still other individuals may have had little or no opportunity for interaction with a group, and thus they go virtually unnoticed. Given the q a- Arr— iw 7v opportunity for such interaction, these individuals may ' receive a measure of acceptance. Rejectee.-—The rejectee is an individual who re— ceives negative choices on a sociometric test. Thus, he attracts attention from some group members, but the atten— tion is of a rejective nature. Mutual choice.——This term indicates that two indi- viduals have chosen each other on the same sociometric cri- terion. This is also called a reciprocated choice or a pair. The important aspect of the definition is that the choice must be reciprocated on the same criterion, thus in- dicating a mutual desire to associate with one another in the same group activity. These sociometric terms employed in the study are operationally defined in Chapter III, page 43. Psychometric Terms The terms used to describe students with high intelli- gence have been defined by J. M. Dunlap (1958, p. 149). Superior.——The term superior or rapid learner refers to children who are markedly above average in intelli- gence and have the potential ability to complete college and as adults to assume substantial positions in their com- munities. Gifted.--The term gifted is applied to the top frac- "; ItIOn of the superior group who have good intelligence and .- Mnaw potential promise of making contributions of a high order to their generation. Hi 1 ifted.—-The term highly gifted or extremely giifted is used in reference to a small fraction of the gif‘ted group who have an exceedingly high level of ability andl whose potential powers should enable them to make origi- nal. and significant contributions to the welfare of their owrl and succeeding generations. The terms used to describe students with low intel- ligerme have been defined by S. A. Kirk (1962, p. 85). Slow learner.--The sl w learner is capable of achieving a moderate degree of academic success though at a slower rate than the average child. He is educated in the regular classes without special provisions except an adaptation of the regular class program to fit his slower learning ability. At the adult level he is usually self- Supporting, independent, and socially adjusted. Educable retarded.——The educable mentally retarded child is one who, because of slow mental development, is unable to profit to any great degree from the programs of the regular schools,_but who has these potentialities for deVelopment: minimum educability in reading, writing, sFelling, arithmetic, and so forth; capacity for social adJustment to a point where he can get along independently _win the community; and minimum occupational adequacy such that he can later support himself partially or totally at }”¥: a Marginal,level. Self—concept.—-This term is defined as a person's I view of himself—~what he perceives himself to be and ‘ what he conceives that others consider him to be, con- trasted with what he would like to be. These psychometric terms employed in the study are operationally defined in Chapter III, page 40. The Thesis in Perspective This dissertation is divided into five chapters as ‘follows: l'purposes of the study, justification of the study, limita- tions, and definitions of terms. Chapter II is a review of related research. I Chapter III explains the research procedures and 'techniques of analysis used in this study. This includes Lthe hypotheses tested, population and samples used, methods 1%ef gathering the data, and methods of collating and jifiecording the data. § Chapter IV comprises the analysis of the data. This Chapter I has included the nature of the problem, I I I I | I I I I I I . .. I ‘1 «es the sociometric and psychometric categorization I ”the‘data, distribution and patterning of mutual choices, hdings or the adjustment inventories. iflfifiapter V summarizes the entire studyi'reedréé7fifiethe‘ b the ¥he writer has draws from thé¢d£téhr*iril CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED RESEARCH This chapter presents summaries of related research studies found in the professional literature of psychology, sociology, and education of the past twenty years.‘ The‘ studies included here have contributed either in whole or in part to the development of the measurement of peer accep— tance as it relates to intellectual ability and personal and social adjustment. Intelligence and Sociometric Besults Few studies have been concerned specifically with the relationship between intelligence and sociometric results of peer acceptance. These studies, for the most I;art, took place in the elementary schools, and only one giggudy summarized herein relates to junior and senior high yool students. We a» '[jj 10 M "I was found that 80 per cent of these highly gifted children had above average sociometric status, and the remaining 20 per cent had below average sociometric status. Whereas 53 per cent of these highly gifted pupils fell in the top 7“ 'guartile of their classroom groups in peer acceptance, only 7 per cent of them were in the bottom quartile. Gallagher (1958a, pp. 465-470) investigated the degree of peer acceptance of 54 highly gifted elementary pupils with I Stanford Binet IQ's of 150 or higher. He found these CI ‘Ih .I'ehildren to be significantly more often accepted as "best ‘friends" in regular classroom situations. This significance ‘P‘ I; .was not appreciably affected by sex or grade level. iConversely, the highly gifted group were found to have se- .P%%:lected their friends from the entire intellectual range. ‘h‘ Examining the relationship of peer acceptance to ‘such variables as intelligence, social perceptiveness, and ‘ I“ race level, Gallagher (1958b, pp. 225- 231) asked 54 highly ' 'IIQ' p,‘ I . 1ifs-fitsd elementary pupils to select five friends on a ' H‘IP=I {t q cicmetric questionnaire. The results indicated that _1arity was positively related to intellectual status; 'Jl‘: ' . ' ihighly gifted children were frequently recognized as‘I g," ¥:; and that propinquity was a faste¥.&5 ,3: I": new " auxin _ =3; cially among the elder- - ~AZfiz' 7 11 She studied 25 children with IQs from 160 to 202 in ismades two through seven in New York City and found these 'highly gifted pupils to be well adjusted and accepted by their peers. Gifted A study by Mann (1957, pp. 199- 201) illustrates the use of the sociometric test in the evaluation of the ieffect of part-time special class placement on the peer acceptance of gifted pupils. This study involved 281 fourth, fifth, and sixth- -grade children of which 67 were found to have a Stanford Binet IQ of 130 or higher. The 67 gifted pupils spent half of their school day in special classes. for the gifted and the other half day in their regular Iclasses. It was assumed that such a school program” :might enable the gifted pupils to maintain normal peer relations while obtaining the benefits of special classes. “J’z On the sociometric test, both the gifted and the Hnical pupils gave the majority of their sociometric 1ces to the members of their own group. This ailzt. ;~ --und mu fiber the eppertunity for peer rei- ' «6:: .xU‘L 82m“ . ts be reund in tfigfigua -12 Parents reported that members of the gifted group associated with each other outside school rather than with '1 intellectually average peers. Mann concluded that the sch001 setting helps to produce and reinforce friendships 'F‘ of gifted pupils in and out of school but does not I measurably increase substantial relationships between gifted and typical-children who are together only part- }. time in class. ' ‘ Wood (1961) made a study of 90 pupils in the fourth, fifth, and sixth—grades of a university laboratory school. Sixteen children were considered gifted with California Test of Mental Maturity IQs of 130 through 164. Socio- metric test scores produced the following results: the ~ ;gifted pupils were chosen by classmates of all levels of .::\;ability; most gifted children showed a preference for L Kfimdividuals with ability comparable to their own; those V.., ‘éifiith higher intellectual ability received greater accep- '1 higher Cunningham mean-accetance scoresftmafledfii ‘ ‘3.d$f£erefices were not signifii l3 acceptance of gifted students was as high as, or higher than, that of their nongifted classmates.’ Grace and Booth (1958, pp. 195—196) studied 294 heterogeneously grouped pupils in grades one through six. They found that 8 of the most popular children were also the most gifted; pupils do not become social isolates within the elementary school, as the most gifted children were still among the best liked in the sixth-grade as they had been in the first grade; and the gifted child was not a social isolate within the first six grades of that urban school. A study to determine whether significant differences exist between mentally superior, typical, and retarded pupils in regular upper elementary classes with regard to peer acceptance was done by Miller (1956, pp. 114-119). His study included 120 pupils, 20 in each IQ group of superior, | . typical, and retarded, in the fourth and sixth-grades. j He found that the superior pupils were most wanted as friends by their classmates while the retarded were least wanted, and no group was rejected as a whole; the superior pupils were significantly more accurate than the typical or .the retarded in predicting their own popularity and that of ,wbupils as friends more frequently than they chose the l4 rMiller concluded that up to certain limits on the intellectual continuum, sociometric status increases with intelligence; high peer acceptance is conferred upon the superior, not only because of their intelligence, but I because of socially desirable traits of personality which they seem to acquire with greater ease than do other people; and the ability to judge one‘s own and others' socio- metric status is largely an intellectual task. Grossman and Wrighter (1948, pp. 346-355) studied the relationship between intelligence and sociometric status among sixth-grade pupils and reported that intelli— genes and sociometric status were related, but that high intelligence did not always assure high sociometric status. . Bonney and Powell (1953, pp. 481-495) compared the IQB sf first-grade pupils with high and low sociometric status. Eh: high status pupils had a median IQ of 113 and the ,;lnn status pupils had a median IQ of 97. Another study I Benney (1955, pp. 481— —495) produced essentially the ‘ {fhesults among second— —grade pupils. In this study,= rem with high sociometric status had a.range tin Iaurmem 15 Mutual Choices Intelligence also enters into mutual relationships among school children. Those children who choose each other on a sociometric test tend to be more alike in intelli- gence than the children who do not choose each other. Bonney, Potashin, and Wood (1946, pp. 21-47; 1946, pp. 48-70; 1961) all reported this tendency, based on I studies of mutual choices among elementary school pupils. I These studies show that the extent to which intelligence influences sociometric choices depends upon the level of intelligence of the chooser as well as that of the chosen. This is brought out clearly in a study by Barbe (1954, pp. 60—62) who analyzed the choice process of 244 elementary 1 school children with IQs ranging from 65 to 140. His results indicated that, although there was a general tendency to choose children of higher intelligence as friends, the slow- I { learning children tended to choose pupils of below average ‘ intelligence, whereas the "bright” pupils tended to choose pupils of above average intelligence. 'Approximately 62 per cent of the slow-learning children chose mutual friends from the below average group. In contrast, 80 per cent ‘4' (of the "bright” children chose mutual friends with above -” raverage intelligence. h‘-1 Personal-Social Adjustment and Sociometric Besults The sociometric test is used as a direct measure of geial adjustment. An individual who is highly chosen at; 16 on a sociometric test is considered to be well accepted by his peers and, therefore, to have good social adjustment. In contrast, an individual who receives few or no choices on a sociometric test is considered to have low acceptance among his peers and, therefore, to have poor social adjust— ment. Since individuals with high sociometric status are generally better adjusted socially than those with low sociometric status, they also might logically be expected to have better personal adjustment. Their high status should provide more opportunity for satisfying their psychological needs for security, social approval, and self—respect, resulting in greater personal satisfaction and freedom from tension. The extent to which the sociometric test provides a more general indication of peer acceptance or nonacceptance, can be evaluated in terms of the relationship between socio- metric results and other measures of social and personal adjustment. These measures can be divided into two groups: (1) those showing how an individual is perceived by others; and (2) those showing how an individual perceives himself. How An Individual is Perceived by Others Peer evaluation.—-This can be determined by a "guess Vflao" test which requires individuals to identify those group members who best fit each of a series of behavior 17 descriptions. The number of mentions an individual receives on each of the behavior descriptions serves as a measure of ‘17 ‘his acceptance among his peers. Variations in specific ‘w f. characteristics make it impossible to equate the results L--_ from one grade level to another, but some similarity in ‘ descriptive characteristics will be noted in the following i l' ‘. I: studies. ‘|‘ 1 4‘ . Bonney (1943, pp. 449-472) reported significant dif- ir ferences between fourth—grade pupils with high and low I ‘4 ' sociometric status on a number of behavior characteristics. Pupils with high sociometric status were found to be signif- icantly superior on both personal and social behavior de- " scriptions. They were characterized most frequently by . atheir peers as being tidy, good—looking, happy, friendly, ‘infiand cheerful. In their social relations they were de— ‘ ' I“lfi geribed as being enthuSiastic daring, active in recitatiens, w '-5's embibiting leadership in groups. Thus, the pupils who l?undentribute to effective social interactibn,x3ggg sihnfi .1 Mien and ice (1943,.pp- 321,319) qqmmqu;fi ‘_ ":toimnney'saat the eigenvmm may I 18 characterized more frequently as being good-looking, popular, happy, friendly, cheerful, and enthusiastic. In addition, they were noted to enjoy jokes and to initiate games and other activities more frequently than pupils with low sociometric status. Using twenty-one class- rooms at the sixth and seventh-grade levels, Laughlin ‘ I (1954) correlated sociometric results with the behavior descriptions of peers and found the same behavior charac- teristics related to high sociometric status. Gronlund and Anderson (1957, pp. 329-338) compared the characteristics of socially accepted, socially re- jected, and socially neglected pupils in a junior high school population. There were 20 pupils in each category, out of a total population of 158. When these three groups were compared, on the basis of responses to a ”guess who" form, important differences were noted. The accepted pupils were generally characterized as possessing socially de- ‘sirable behavior characteristics similar to those reported in the above studies. Specific characteristics such as good looks, tidiness, friendliness, likeableness, enthusi- asm, cheerfulness, initiative, and sense of humor stood high on the list. In contrast, the socially rejected pupils 4;. were not only overlooked on these positive characteristics, '"hut they were also frequently described as possessing the aEPOsite attributes. They were characterized by their peers being not good—looking, untidy, not likeable, restless, l9 and talkative. The socially neglected pupils tended to be overlooked, on the ”guess who" form, receiving rel- atively few mentions on either positive or negative characteristics. The few mentions they did receive in— dicated that they were quiet and ngt talkative. Appar— ently they were truly socially neglected by their peers. Adult ratings.---Studies using ratings of social ad- justment by adults are difficult to equate, since different procedures were used in different studies and the aspects of social adjustment rated were not uniform from one study to another. In general, however, the studies are in sub- stantial agreement concerning the relationship between the sociometric status of individuals‘ and adults' ratings of their peer acceptance. Olson (1949) compared students from 10 elementary school classrooms by their sociometric standings and de- scriptions of their behavior written by their teachers. Those children receiving the largest number of choices were described most frequently as being dependable, well ad- justed, friendly, quiet, and good natured; while those re- ceiving the fewest choices were described as being shy, bossy, sulky, conduct problems, ill, or new to class. AAlthough there was some overlapping in the descriptions of pupils in the two groups, the characterizations of pupils with high and low sociometricstatus clearly indicated a difference in social adjustment. 20 I In an intensive study of five pupils with high socio- . metric status and five pupils with low sociometric status, I at the elementary school level, Bonney (1947) obtained re- sults somewhat similar to those reported by Olson. In ) general, the highly chosen pupils were characterized by greater conformity and group identification, greater ) emotional stability and control, more social aggressiveness greater dependability, and more frequent behavior indi— cating attitudes of friendliness, cooperativeness, and good I 4 will toward others. 4 Northway (1944, pp. 10—25) made an intensive clinical 1 study of the behavior of 20 fifth and sixth—grade children who were least often chosen on a sociometric test. On the I‘ basis of their behavior patterns, she classified them into three distinct groups. One group was described as being . -listless, with no inner drive or interest in their environ- ment. They appeared to merely exist and exerted little or no effort toward social adjustment. Another group was por— trayed as being quiet and retiring. They had individual interests but showed little or no interest in social inter- action. The third group was depicted as being noisy, boast- ; ;,ful, arrogant, rebellious, and delinquent in classroom ac- {g-tivities. They appeared to be aggressively striving for ’E. .i‘l ‘; 4 ¢§¢ertance by peers, but they used socially ineffective 21 ggw An Individual Perceives Himself .“w Self-report techniques.--Evidence concerning per- sonal and social adjustment has frequently been obtained from the pupils themselves, through the use of adjustment questionnaires, problem check lists, and self—ratings. :1 These self—report techniques reflect how the pupil feels ‘1 about himself and the problems of adjustment he faces. The relationship between sociometric results and the re- .5 sults of self—report techniques provides an indication of how a pupil's feelings toward himself compare with the feelings of others toward him. n : Grossman and Wrighter (1948, pp. 346-355) reported .that sixth-grade pupils who were highly chosen on a socio- 'metric test had significantly higher adjustment scores en the California Test of Personality than thoSe pupils who ‘-“3re rejected on the sociometric test. Using the same adjustment questionnaire, Scandrette In a more detailed 's with low sociometric status. 22 Two rather extensive investigations compared the mental health characteristics of pupils with high and low ‘ sociometric status. In both studies the mental health characteristics were determined by the pupils' responses to Thorpe, Clark, and Tiegs' adjustment questionnaire, en-V ‘ titled Mental Health Analysis. Bedoian (1953, pp. 366-371) reported that pupils with high sociometric status had significantly higher mental health scores than pupils with ' low sociometric status, in 21 of the 22 sixth-grade c1ass~ rooms included in the study. Similar findings were re- ported by Baron (1949, pp. 306-310; 1951, pp. 32-42), for pupils in eleven fifth and sixth-grade classrooms. He noted that the high status pupils tended to feel more self—confident, more physically adequate, more secure in their school relationships, and gave indications of greater emotional stability than low status pupils. r": I Kuhlen and Bretsch (1947, pp. 122-132) compared the nE‘gfirsonal problems felt by pupils with high and low socio- They requested approximately 700 ninth- 9' trio status. f: pupils to check the items of the Mooney Problem. ~;hList which bothered them never, sometimes, orkbdten.p¥ :' 23 low status pupils revealed concern with social skills, unhappiness, lack of status, family problems, and dislike of school. In a study of 696 ninth—grade pupils, Bretsch (1952, pp. 449-504) compared the self—ratings of pupils with high and low sociometric status on eight different social skills. High status pupils rated themselves higher on the social skills than did the low status pupils. This finding seems to indicate that high status pupils are more confident of their ability to perform social skills. Projective techniques.--Projective techniques have been commonly used to measure adjustment. The unique fea- ture of projective techniques is that they present unstruc- tured and ambiguous situations to the individual, and he is permitted to respond in terms of his own perception of the situations. Since the situations are ambiguous, he 5 . ejects his own feelings and interpretations in his re- onses. There are relatively few studies comparing socio- 5t” H , ic results with these measures of adjustment. oNorthway and Wigdor (1947, pp. 186— —l99) compared '. ~i F~. ‘I'Up stat The low status 9 M? '1; at 9.22118 Mk1. ‘nw swimmer te control their;, 24 151-167) reported similar results among college students. A comparison of the sociometrically high and sociometrically low students, on the basis of their responses to the Rorschach and the Thematic Apperception Test, indicated that students with low status showed trends toward greater anxiety and deviate patterns of adjustment. Summary These studies show that intelligence is an impor- tant factor in sociometric choosing. The prestige factor of high intelligence seems to attract attention of peers and thus places the gifted individual in a favorable posi- tion to be chosen on a sociometric test. The results indicate that, as a group, gifted students are distinctly superior in terms of social acceptance by their peers; high intelligence is closely related to high sociometric status, ’ although some gifted pupils are not well accepted by their peers; and students tend to choose as mutual companions see who are similar to themselves in intelligence. Sociometric results have shown substantial agreement ”Lather measures of social and personal adjustment. .peer evaluations and adults‘ ratings indicate merel. story adjustment on the part of pupils Mith§hi ;»i 5'e status. Clinical and follaw~gg»ef‘ law that pupils w1th.1ew seeiemetri " ”J. , efi r ‘ , ;“q:f‘, -53.- ._ L. 'vi—‘w. 25 less satisfactory adjustments to their peers and to their school environment than do pupils with high sociometric status. High and low status pupils tend to view themselves quite differently on projective and nonprojective tech- niques. Pupils in the low status group tend to feel inse- cure, discriminated against, inadequate physically and socially, and show signs of emotional instability. They also compare themselves unfavorably with others. In con- trast, the responses of high status pupils generally reflect feelings of security, self-confidence, and other evidences of good personal and social adjustment. CHAPTER III RESEARCH PROCEDURES AND TECHNIQUES OF ANALYSIS This study was designed and directed toward securing information as to peer acceptance of gifted students and their perceived problems of personal and social adjustment at the junior high school level. , After analyzing the problems to be studied and re- viewing the related research, attention was directed as to what research procedures should be used. It was necessary to formulate the hypotheses to be tested for such a study at the junior high SleOl level, secure information about the parent population involved, select the sample population to be used, determine the type of instrumentation for gathering the data, and decide upon the particular methodology and procedures to be used in collating and recording the data. \ —:d I Hypotheses Test ( The first hypothesis of this study concerns the relationship of the intellectual ability continuum to the social acceptance continuum. There is evidence that the prestige factor of intelligence seems to attract attention of peers and thus places the gifted individual in a favor— able position to be chosen on a sociometric test. 2/ The greater the junior high school stu- dent's intellectual capacity, the more likely he is to be socially accepted by his peer group. A second hypothesis concerns itself with the verti- cal direction of students' social preferences. There is evidence that intelligence enters into mutual relationships among school children. Those students who choose each other on a sociometric test tend to be more alike in intelli- gence than the students who do not choose each other. Those junior high school students involved in mutual choices, show greatest social prefer- ence for individuals with mental ability equal to or higher than their own. In formulating the third hypothesis, it was realized that the sociometric status scores of individuals can be interpreted most accurately when both the social and the personal factors perceived to be in Operation by the individuals involved are identified. There is a set of problems commonly perceived by both gifted and nongifted junior high school social isolates. P'\(~.11-1’j+ . my". ‘_ L‘ .. -~. ul .‘_ .1 Parent POpulation The parent population in this study consisted of all 2,733 seventh and eighth—grade students of the five junior high schools of Livonia, Michigan during the 1962—63 school year. This total of 2,733 students included l,&la seventh— graders (70M boys and 710 girls) in M7 classes and 1,319 28 eighth graders {685 boys and 63“r girls) in 45 classes at the five schools. The classes averaged 30 students. A class list was obtained from each homeroom teacher for each of the 92 classes involved. All 2,733 students were given the California Test of Mental Maturity and the sociometric test. The Livonia public school system has no policy con- cerning grouping by ability at the Junior high school level. Grouping procedures differ from one building to the next, and thus gifted children are scattered erratically throughout the parent pOpulation. Sample POpulation The 2,733 students participating in the first phase of this study were divided into four groups by the scores they received on the sociometric and psychometric tests. According to selective criteria used in this study, 14 students were categorized as gifted isolates, 216 as non— gifted isolates, 177 as gifted nonisolates, and 2,326 as nongifted nonisolates. Less than a week before the testing for the second phase of the study was to take place, it was decided by Livonia school's central office that permission should be obtained in writing from the parents of each child who was to participate. Therefore, the parents were asked if their children might participate in a research study evaluating 29 social adjustment of junior high school students. Dates had already been scheduled by the principal of each building for the administration of the adjustment inventories. In preparing for the testing involved, it was found that one of the 14 gifted isolates located had moved out of district since the first phase of the study was com- pleted. Since the gifted isolate group now numbered only 13 students, the parents of each student were phoned as a follow—up to permission slips being sent home, saying only that their child was one of only 13 students of one of the subgroups of the study, and that it would be greatly appre- ciated if 100 per cent of the students in this group could participate in order to not distort the results of the study. One hundred per cent approval was received for this group, thus insuring a complete cross section of the gifted isolates' perceived problems. In selecting both nonisolate groups, it seemed im- perative that, in order to show the full effect of accep— tance versus nonacceptance, the comparison be between iso- late or low status and high status students. ”Shifts in sociometric status are relatively rare at the extreme sociometric status positions. This would tend to indi— cate that the high and low sociometric status positions are more stable than those in the average sociometric categories and thus can be used with greater confidence” (Gronlund, 1959, p. 131). 30 There were 65 high status students included in the 177 gifted nonisolates located in the first phase of the study. Permission slips were sent to the parents of those high status students still residing in the district. By the testing days scheduled by the principals, 28 of these had been returned, thus establishing the gifted nonisolate group to be tested. There were 688 high status students included in the 2,326 nongifted nonisolates. Sixty-eight high status stu— dents were randomly selected (every tenth student). Per- mission slips were sent to the parents of those high status students still residing in the district. By the testing days scheduled, 31 had been returned, thus estab- lishing the nongifted nonisolate group to be tested. Seventy—two of the 216 nongifted isolates located were randomly selected (every third student), and per— mission slips were sent to the parents of those students still residing in the district. By the testing days scheduled, 46 had been returned, thus establishing the nongifted isolate group to be tested. In summary, the sample population to be tested totaled 118 students: gifted isolate, l3; nongifted isolate, 46; gifted nonisolate, 28; and nongifted non— isolate, 31. All these students were given the three ad- justment inventories. 31 Methods of Gathering the Data The instruments used to gather the data for this study were the California Test of Mental Maturity, Vineland Social Maturity Scale, Mooney Problem Check List, Rohde Sentence Completion Test, and a sociometric test. The first four are standardized tests where validity and relia— bility have been established. The sociometric test used is not standardized but is of the type previously validated in similar situations. California Test of Mental Maturity The California Test of Mental Maturity was selected as the group screening instrument to locate the gifted students for this study because of its rather high corre— lation with accepted individual tests of mental ability. The test was administered by the counseling staff in each of the five junior high schools. The fact that these counselors were already familiar with this test and had ad— ministered it repeatedly, insured the necessary reliability of administration. The answer sheets were collected and sent to the publisher for machine scoring, thus insuring scoring accuracy. The scores were returned to the writer on class section lists for district—wide tabulation. The chief features of this test are ”its analysis into language and nonlanguage abilities and into five factors: memory, spatial relationships, logical reasoning, 32 numerical reasoning, and vocabulary. Reliabilities, in one— grade ranges, vary from .89 to .97 for whole forms. There is a correlation of .88 with the Stanford Binet Test" (Sullivan, Clark, & Tiegs, 1947, p. 156). Altus (1955, pp. 143-144) drew the comparison that the California Test of Mental Maturity has the desirable feature of offering both language and nonlanguage IQ scores similar to the verbal and performance IQ scores on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. In her article comparing the CTMM and WISC, she gives the following evalu- ation: WISC - Full Scale - Mean 84.5, SD 17.4 CTMM - Total - Mean 8M.8, SD 17.u She found ”an intercorrelation of .77 between the WISC full scale IQs and the CTMM total IQs," and concluded that "the WISC and CTMM are markedly comparable as to group assessment and roughly comparable as to individual scores and major breakdown into verbal and nonverbal abilities.” Sociometric Test A sociometric test of the type originated by Moreno (1934) was administered to all seventh and eighth-grade classes by their homeroom teachers. The test consisted of three criteria with three choices each. An allowance of three to five choices is usually sufficient to reveal the relative position of an individual in the group. As 33 summarized by Jennings (1950, p. 19), "the individuals who attract the greater portion of the choices on the basis of a small choice allowance, continue still to profit dispro- portionately under the larger choice allowance, and the num- ber of individuals unchosen under the first condition is not substantially reduced under the second condition.” The writer attempted to select criteria referring to different kinds of social situations occurring in the junior high school which offer opportunity for interaction in groups of various sizes. Care was taken that the choices would be made on a friendship basis rather than a working companion basis so that the popularity of the gifted could be evaluated apart from the possibility of their being cultivated as potential academic helpers. Directions were printed at the head of the socio- metric test and read aloud by the teacher as the students read them silently. The class packets of completed test forms were then returned to the writer for tabulation. A copy of the sociometric test form used in this study may be seen in Appendix A. ”The sociometric test, itself, is not a test in the sense that the term is commonly used, but rather a tech— nique” (Gronlund, 1959, p. l). ”Sociometric nominations have generally proved to be one of the most dependable of rating techniques. When checked against a variety of practical criteria dependent upon interpersonal relations, 34 such ratings have been found to have good predictive validity" (Lindzey & Borgatta, 195A). Gronlund (1959, p. 129) compared the results of sociometric studies made among adolescents and found that sociometric status scores are fairly stable, even over a period of almost two years. ”These findings are understandable when we consider some of the features of sociometry. First, the number of raters is large, including all group members. Second, an individual's peers are often in a particularly favorable position to observe his typical behavior. They may thus be better judges of certain interpersonal traits than teachers, supervisors, and other outside observers. Third, and probably most important, is the fact that the opinions of group members, right or wrong, influence their actions and hence partly determine the nature of the individual‘s subsequent interactions with the group. Other comparable groups may be expected to react toward the individual in a similar fashion. Sociometric ratings may thus be said to have content validity in the same sense as worksamples" (Anastasi, 1961, p. 622). Adjustment Inventories A battery of adjustment inventories was selected to yield the broadest possible picture of an individual's per— ceived life problems. From this battery, the writer has attempted to determine how the members of the gifted and nongifted, isolate and nonisolate groups perceive their environment, their positions in it, and the role they see important people, such as parents and teachers, playing in their lives. In order to draw comparisons and contrasts of the groups being studied, it was necessary for all instru— ments included to yield quantitative results. This battery of adjustment inventories was adminis- tered, by the writer, to 59 members of the isolate group (13 gifted and M6 nongifted students) and to 59 members of the nonisolate group (28 gifted and 31 nongifted students) with the assistance of several counselors as proctors. Adjustment-testing instruments may be grouped into two major categories. One, the inventories and rating scales, presents to the subject, or to an informant, a broad, structured stimulus situation, e.g., questions to be answered, problem check lists, or trait names to be rated, and the responses are quantitative. Ratings on this type of instru— ment can be made by the individual for his own traits, or they may be rated by other people who know him. Two of the inventories used in this battery, the Vineland Social Matur— ity Scale and the Mooney Problem Check List, fall into this first category. In tests in the second category, the subject is pre- sented with an informal, ambiguous, nonstructured stimulus, the responses to which will be influenced, if not entirely 36 controlled, by his personality dynamics. The third inven— tory used in this battery, the Rohde Sentence Completion Test, falls into this second category. The three adjustment inventories used in this study and the procedures for administering and scoring them are described below. Vineland Social Maturity Scale.—-The Vineland Social Maturity Scale, designed by E. A. Doll (19u7, pp. 1—2), ”pro- vides a definite outline of detailed performances in respect to which students show a progressive capacity for looking after themselves and for participating in those activities which lead toward ultimate independence as adults. The items of the Scale are arranged in order of increasing aver— age difficulty, and represent progressive maturation in self—help, self—direction, locomotion, occupation, communi— cation and social relations.” Only items 75 through 101 were used. It seemed im- portant to not include those early items indicating a degree of dependence as to insult the adolescent personality and yet to begin early enough in the sequence of items to insure identification of the most socially immature members of the group. Items beyond 101 pertain to individuals who are fulltime employed. ”Under favorable conditions the Scale may be admin— istered with the subject of the examination acting as his own informant. Results obtained in this way tend to be 37 slightly higher on the average, but are in some cases lower, than those obtained from independent informants. Often the subject is a better informant than someone else” (Doll, 1947, p. 11). In this study each student acted as his own informant. The counselors for each junior high school who assisted as proctors also served as judges of the authenticity of the responses made by students from that building. An individual may be ranked in one of five ways on each item on the Vineland Scale, yielding O, .5, or 1 point of raw score. The categories are as follows: items done regularly with neither artificial incentive nor undue urging (1); items done occasionally, in transitional or emergent state (.5); items not done at all or only rarely or under extreme pressure (0); items which could be done if subject were allowed (1); items the subject has no opportunity to do, (1) if within range of continuous point scores, (O) if within range of continuous zero scores, and (.5) if within intermediate range. Two half-credits were counted as one full point of score. Mooney Problem Check List.-—Mooney (1950, p. 4) develOped the Problem Check Lists to help students express their personal problems in areas known to be important in the adjustment of adolescents. The Problem Check List used in this study was ”the junior high school form containing seven problem areas: health and physical development; school; 38 home and family; money, work, the future; boy and girl re— lations; relations with people in general; and self—centered concerns.’1 ”The procedure of administration is simple. All di- rections needed are on the cover page. Students read through the list and mark the problems which are of concern to them" (Mooney, 1950, p. 3). The problems marked on each list were counted for each problem area and totaled. Rohde Sentence Completion Test.-—In the Sentence Completion Test developed by Rohde (1957, pp. 96-47), only the opening words are provided, the subject being required to write the ending. "Included in this test are 18 objects: mother, father, friends, God, religion, opposite sex, same sex, home, work, teachers, laws, et cetera, around which emotional and social adjustment and adaptation revolve and with which foci of conflict are associated.” A study of ”cathections,” or the acceptance or rejection of these ob- jects, was used in this study. ”In the administration of this test, directions at the top of the page are read aloud. It is suggested to the group that whatever responses the individual cares to make will be entirely acceptable, and the examinees write their responses” (Rohde, 1957, p. 62). The entire 65 item folder was administered since the 18 cathected objects were scattered randomly throughout the 65 items. This helped 39 to secureresponses on all cathected objects and to insure a greater depth of projectivity in the responses on these objects. Those objects which were accepted, or for which an attachment was expressed, were classified and scored under the heading of positive cathection. Objects for which dis— like or revulsion was expressed, were classified and scored under negative cathections. If neither acceptance nor re— jection was indicated, it was classified and scored as neu- tral. Methods of Collating and Recording the Data Several aspects of this research study made it either impossible or impractical to utilize electronic data proc- essing equipment. It therefore became logical to utilize a number of tables in a step by step collation of the data. These tables were not only the most simplified manner of collating the data, but samples of them in the appendices should serve to show the reader the logic involved in the methods of analysis. The particular methodology and pro- cedures used to collate and record the data are described in the following paragraphs. §§ychometric Data The IQs used in this study were all total IQs obtained from results of the California Test of Mental Maturity. The names on each of the 92 class lists were put in alphabetical 40 order (girls, followed by boys), and the CTMM total score was entered beside each name. Psychometric categories.—-The mental ability groups are defined in Chapter I, page 6 and are operationally de- fined here as follows: highly gifted, IQs of 150 and up; gifted, IQs of 130 through 149; rapid learner, IQs of 115 through 129; average mental ability, IQs of 85 through 114; slow learner, IQs of 70 through 84; and educable retarded, IQs of 50 through 69. These psychometric groupings or categories have been created by educators through the years for the purpose of homogeneous grouping by ability in an effort to improve in- Struction through individualization. The IQ limits placed on these categories are arbitrarily set by this writer and draw their antecedents from research related to psychometric instruments and special education programs. The scores of 85 and 115 are the breaking points of validity of the group paper and pencil tests of mental abil- ity. The scores of 50 and 150 are the breaking points of validity of the Stanford Revision of the Binet. The score of 130 was set by educators of gifted children when they suggested that a child have a mental age of 1.3 times his chronological age in order to be considered a gifted child. The score of 70 was set by educators of the mentally re- tarded as the standard below which social maladjustments 41 and emotional disturbances resulting from frustration and lack of acceptance would off-balance the academic gains were the child placed in the regular classroom. Sociometric Data When a sociometric test has been administered to a classroom group, the resulting data will include the list of choices each pupil has made on each sociometric criterion. The 92 class packets of completed sociometric tests were returned to the writer from the homeroom teachers. The tests from each class were put in alphabetical order (girls, followed by boys) and checked with the original class lists to make certain each student in the room returned the socio- metric test. The completed test packets were filed according to their class number and as to which junior high school the students attended. These packets were set aside until the matrix tables were constructed and the data could be record- ed on them. Matrix tables.-—A modified version of the matrix table or graphic plotting, originally constructed by Jennings (1950) in her study of leadership, was used to meet the specific needs of this study. A separate matrix table was constructed for each of the 92 classes on each of the three criterion used in the sociometric test, for a total of 276 tables. The three matrix tables for one class are in Appendix B. 42 Each class packet of the sociometric tests was con— sidered separately, and each criterion was on a separate table, making three tables for each class. The girls' names (first name and last initial) were written down the left of the table in alphabetical order, followed by the boys' names. The students were numbered consecutively from tOp to bottom and across the top margin of the table. The heavy line drawn both vertically and horizontally between the list of girls and the list of boys divided the matrix table into four parts and made it easier to re- cord choices. The diagonal line drawn from the upper left— hand corner to the lower right—hand corner served as a guide in identifying mutual choices. At the left of the names, vertical columns were used for summarizing choices given, and at the bottom of the matrix table the rows were used for summarizing information on choices received. The sociometric test results were recorded by placing the choices made by each individual in the prOper column opposite the chooser's name. The columns were then totaled and the number of choices received by each student was re— corded. The sociometric test results were recorded by plac— ing the choices made by each individual in the prOper column opposite the chooser‘s name. The columns 43 were then totaled and the number of choices received by each student was recorded. The mutual choices were identified by starting at the diagonal line in the upper left—hand corner and going down column one to determine if any of the students whom person number one chose also chose him, et cetera. The mutual choices were circled on each matrix table and totaled for each individual. The complete tabulation of the socio- metric test data was recorded in the matrix tables. Sociometric categories.—-The sociometric categories used in this study are defined in Chapter I, page 5 and are operationally defined here as follows: star, 15 choices and up; above average, 12 through 14 choices; average, 7 through 11 choices; below average, 4 through 6 choices; neglectee, 1 through 3 choices; and isolate, O choices received. The method of classifying the students into the sociometric categories given here is based on Bronfenbren— ner‘s (1945) fixed frame of reference. ”This reference indi- cates the critical sociometric status scores for varying numbers of choices and sociometric criteria. The upper and lower limits presented in this reference are actually limits of statistical significance at the .O2 and .03 level. Re— ceiving as few choices as the value indicated in the lower limit, or as many choices as the values indicated in the 44 upper limit, would be expected less than two, or three, times out of a hundred by chance alone. The teacher can be fairly confident that pupils classified as neglectees and isolates (lower limit) and stars (upper limit) have been placed in the proper socio- metric category. The values for the lower and upper limits may be applied to any group which contains no fewer than ten persons and no more than fifty persons. Thus, the frame of reference remains fixed for groups of varying size, and the number of students in dif- ferent groups can be compared directly even though the size of the groups is different. However, the criteria used and the number of choices allotted must remain the same for all classroom groups, and the classroom groups must contain be— tween ten and fifty pupils” (Bronfenbrenner, 1945). Psychometric and Sociometric Categorization of Data Inasmuch as the basic hypothesis of this study was the comparison of the intellectual ability continuum and the social acceptance c6ntinuum, it seemed most logical to establish grids, compartmentalizing the various sets of data into the psychometric and sociometric categories in as many ways as possible. The summary tables, the mutual choice tables, and the frequency tables convert the raw data into collated categories from which the comparative grids have been constructed. 45 Summary tables.--Summary tables were compiled, one for each of the 92 classes. Each table lists the students as they appeared on the matrix tables, their IQs with the resulting psychometric categories, the number of choices received on each sociometric criterion, and total number of choices with the resulting sociometric categories. The summary table for one class is in Appendix C. The compara- tive data on each student as shown on the summary tables made it possible to construct two frequency tables to be described later. Mutual choice tables.--Along the base of each matrix table is a column of totals of mutual choices. Mutual choice tables were constructed, one for each of the 92 classes, and students involved in mutual choices were listed by their matrix table numbers. The mutual choices were counted and recorded on the mutual choice tables along with both students’ matrix table numbers, IQs, psychometric category assignment, total choices received, and sociometric category assignment. A mutual choice table for one class is shown in Appendix D. Frequency tables.--By using the data from the sum- mary tables, it was possible to count the number of students who fell at each IQ score into each particular socio— metric category. A frequency distribution of this data is recorded in Appendix E. 46 It was also possible to count the number of students who fell at each sociometric choice number and into each particular psychometric category. A frequency distribution of this data is recorded in Appendix F. Adjustment Inventory Data The three completed adjustment inventory forms for each of the 118 students were scored. The scored forms were divided in the following three ways for comparative item analyses. For the first consideration the forms were divided into two groups, isolate versus nonisolate. For a com- parison all forms were then separated into two groups, gifted versus nongifted. Finally, all forms were sorted into four groups; gifted isolate, nongifted isolate, gifted nonisolate, and nongifted nonisolate. Since all three inventories yielded quantitative re— sults, the results could be tabulated and recorded for each defined group. The frequency with which individuals responded on various items facilitated the compilation of lists of commonly perceived problems for each of the defined groups. Data Analysis The analysis of data in Chapter IV shall consist of a narrative presentation interspersed with tables to provide as neny views of the data as possible. The narration 47 eritunerates the more descriptive statistics from the tables t:c> add clarity and emphasis. In the analysis of the data from the second phase of 'triea study, under adjustment inventories, the formula for cocxnputing the standard error of the difference between two ssaanle percentages was applied to the comparative percent- eagxes for the subgroups and only those items showing a ggrweater difference than might be expected to occur by :saxnpling error have been mentioned. Op1’p2 = '11—— + "N— The critical ratio, t, was also calculated for these ifitenns, and where there was sufficient difference between tile: respective percentages to be significant at the .01 :Le\/el, the items were listed as showing a significant differ- ernze between the subgroups. p -p t=—l____2_ Cpl—p2 These formulas were applied as recommended by Smith (1958, pp. 61-63). CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF DATA The data gathered in this study have been presented in comparative tables designed to give the reader alternate views of the data, tabulated under both psychometric and sociometric categories. These tables are accompanied by a narrative presentation to focus attention on the more sig— nificant descriptive statistics that are the bases of the conclusions and implications presented in Chapter V. For the broadest possible view of the psychometric and sociometric data yielded by the first phase of this study, it is necessary to refer to Appendices E and F which include the initial frequency distributions under both psychometric and sociometric categories. Psychometric Data The psychometric data compiled in the frequency table in Appendix F have been summarized in Table l to show the number and per cent of the total group which fell in each psychometric category. It may be noted that 56.6 per cent of the total group tested were included in the average IQ range, and that 31.5 per cent scored in the rapid learner group as 48 49 compared to 4.5 per cent in the slow learner group. There were .4 per cent in the educable mentally retarded range as contrasted to 7 per cent in the gifted range. TABLE l.-—Number and per cent of students in each psychometric category. Psychometric CTMM Number of Per Cent of Category IQ Students Students Highly Gifted l5O - Up 1 .O4 Gifted 13o - 149 190 6.95 Rapid Learner 115 - 129 862 31.54 Average 85 — 114 1547 56.61 Slow Learner 7O - 84 122 4.46 Educable 5o — 69 11 .40 Totals 2733 100.00 Sociometric Data The sociometric data compiled in the frequency table in Appendix E have been summarized in Table 2 to show the number and per cent of the total group which fell in each sociometric category. It may be seen here that 29 per cent of the total group are included in the average sociometric range, and that 11 per cent ranked in the above—average category as contrasted to 19 per cent in the below-average category. There were 16 per cent in the neglectee category and 8 per 50 cent in the isolate group as compared to 16 per cent in the star category. TABLE 2.--Number and per cent of students in each sociometric category. Sociometric SEEESZS Number of Per Cent of Category Received Students Students Star 15 — Up 447 16.40 Above Average 12 — 14 306 11.20 Average 7 - 11 796 29.12 Below Average 4 - 6 513 18.77 Neglectee 1 - 3 441 16.12 Isolate o 230 8.39 Totals 2733 100.00 Psychometric and Sociometric Categorization of Data The comparison of the intellectual ability continuum and the social acceptance continuum is enhanced by tables showing alternate views of the data, first under psycho— metric categories, then under sociometric categories. These tables have been summarized in as many ways as possible to lend added emphasis. Decile Distributions In order to give a definitive view of the resulting distribution of cases along the intellectual ability 51 continuum, the rank order of the 2,733 students shown in the frequency table in Appendix E was divided into deciles, and a mean number of choices was computed for each decile. This decile distribution is shown in Table 3. The apparent overlap of the categories within Table 3 and Table 4 resulted from the fixed ten per cent in each decile category. There are, therefore, individuals with the same scores placed in different deciles. TABLE 3.--Psychometric decile distribution with mean number of choices. CTMM Mean Number Decile IQ Range Choices First 127—153 9 Second 122—127 9 Third 118—122 10 Fourth 114—118 9 Fifth 110—114 8 Sixth 107—110 8 Seventh 102-107 9 Eighth 98—102 8 Ninth 91- 98 7 Tenth 60- 91 7 It may be noted that the fifth and six deciles each have a mean number of 8 choices, and that the fourth and seventh deciles each have a mean number of 9 choices. However, the third decile has a mean number of 10 choices as contrasted to a mean number of 8 choices for the eighth decile. The first and second deciles have means of 9 choices contrasted to means of 7 choices for the ninth and tenth deciles. 52 To give a more definitive view of the resulting dis- tribution of cases along the social acceptance continuum, the rank order of the 2,733 students shown in the frequency table in Appendix F was divided into deciles and a mean IQ was computed for each decile. This decile distribution is shown in Table 4. TABLE 4.-—Sociometric decile distribution with mean IQS. Range in Number Decile of Choices Mean IQ First 17-41 . 112 Second 13-17 111 Third 11-13 112 Fourth 9-11 110 Fifth 8- 9 110 Sixth 6— 8 109 Seventh 4— 6 108 Eighth 3- 4 110 Ninth 1— 3 108 Tenth O— 1 105 The mean IQs for all ten deciles fall in the upper half of the average range of mentality with seven IQ points difference between the mean IQ for the first decile and the mean IQ for the tenth decile. Except for the mean IQs of the third and eighth deciles, there is an upward progression in mean IQs from the tenth to the first decile. Comparting the Data To give a more detailed picture of the data, two tables, with comparable compartments, were compiled to more precisely compare the psychometric and sociometric data. Table 5 shows the number and the per cent of students of each psychometric category as they ranked under the socio— metric categories. Of the average IQ group, 15.1 per cent ranked as stars whereas 18.4 per cent of the gifted and 19.5 per cent of the rapid learners ranked as stars. Of the average IQ group 11.0 per cent fell in the above average sociometric category whereas 10.7 per cent of the rapid learners and 15.8 per cent of the gifted group fell into this category. Looking at the below average IQ groups, it will be J noted that 15.6 per cent of the slow learners and 54.5 per cent of the retarded group rank as isolates as compared with 9.2 per cent of the average IQ group, 5.5 per cent of the rapid learners, and 7.4 per cent of the gifted. Table 6 shows the number and the per cent of students of each sociometric category as they ranked under the psy— chometric categories. Of the star group, 7.9 per cent of the stars were gifted; whereas, 5.9 per cent of the average sociometric group and 6.0 per cent of the isolate group were gifted. 54 mmsm omm as: mam oos oom as: nachos oo-om sooH sm.om so ss.o sm.os sa.o ss.o ooosooom as o o a m H a oaooosom asuos soos so.ms so.oa so.sm so.mm ss.oa ss.o possess mma as ma mm om ma os soan sooa sm.o sm.ss sm.os sm.om so.aa sa.ma assumo sass mas oom mom sms oss mmm omssosa omaumaa soos sm.m ss.as so.ss sm.mm ss.os sm.os pontoon moo mo ems oms oom mo mos oaosm sooa so.s so.ss sm.os ss.om so.ms so.os ooa-oms oos as mm am so om mm ooosao oo-oma sooa so so so sooa so so oooeao a o o o a o o sasmam ossooe o m.m.a o.m.o sa.oa oa.ma.ma oo-ma noasowooso ouMHOmH mmpooamoz owmmm>< .m.m.s mwmsm>< scum assume zoamm owmpm>¢ m>on¢ logommm mmfisowoumo oonpoEOfioom .nossommpmo OHmeEosoow woos: sumo oahmeonozmd mo economecmmLO|l.m mqm< Q\ Q\ o 0\ . 0 .WO 0\ o o\ . o\ JHIWH BOOH sow som 3 Km cl. \oH Om pm m \60 ommkosfis mom H mH osH do Om o o>oo< sooH so. am.m sH.mm so.sm 2d.s so o:-mH so: H oH mmm moH mm 0 seem do-om aw-os aHH-mm omH-mHH oaH-omH o:-omH manpoe ooogmpom accused ommgo>< Locsoofl oopmHU powwow moagomopmo oHnsosom son oaosm sHsmam dashes uOHoom moflcomopmo ofispoeosoamm .moosomopmo oesooEozoamd Loos: spec casmeosoom mo coopsmocnwsOuu.m mam< \ . mmH-nHH sen.Hm sus.H smn.a soa.n sam.oH eom.m. soH.o tocsMoa new we :mH omH own no moH oHosm smm.o sHm. sHm.H emH.H ems H eoH.H smm.H maa-omH omH aH mm Hm s: m mm ooocHo db: a. see. so so so sac. so so e.smsw H o o o H o o sHsmHm o m.m.H o.m.a HH.oH aH.mH.mH op-mH mHmpoe opoHomH moooonoz mmmgo>< “mamas mmdsm>< Loom mmspommpoo onom ommgo>¢ o>op< oflgpoe .Jl sogommm mosgowopmo osppoanoow .spdo ostmEosoom ocm oHLpoEozoamo esp so coapmeHmpsmEpLooEoon-.S mqmTota1 Actual Per Cent Psycho- Number Possible Number of metric of Mutual Mutual Involve— Categories Students Choices Choices ment Highly Gifted l 9 2 22 Gifted 190 1,710 659. 39 Rapid Learner 862 7,758 3,207 Ml Average 1,547 13,923 5,377 -‘39 Slow Learner.c 122 1’098 33% 3O Educable} Retarded ll 99 19 19 TOTALS 2,733. '24,597 9,596 39 TABLE 15.-—Comparative involvement of the sociometric cate— gories in mutual choosing. Total Actual Per Cent 50010- Number Possible Number of metric of Mutual Mutual Involve— Categories Students Choices Choices ment Star 447 4,023 2,740 68 Above Average 306 2,754 1,68“ 61 Average 796 7,16“ 3,376 H7 Below Average 513 “3617 1,327 29 Neglectee 441 3,969 A69 12 Isolate 230 2,070 0 0 TOTALS 2,733 2u,597 9,596 39 71 The isolate group, those who received no choices, had no mutual choices and thus might be construed to have little or no interaction with their peers in their classroom groups. Adjustment Inventory Data The second phase of the study consisted of the admin— istration of the three adjustment inventories to a total group of 118 students from the 2,733 students participating in the first phase of the study. Three separate tabulations were made for each of the three adjustment inventories; first, for the isolate versus nonisolate groups; second, for the gifted versus nongifted groups; and finally for four groups: gifted isolates, non— gifted isolates, gifted nonisolates, and nongifted non— isolates. These three separate tabulations were made in an effort to compile a set of problems commonly perceived by these groups. The groups involved in each tabulation were compared by the percentage of involvement of the members of each group on each item of the three adjustment inventories. Locating Perceived Problems of Isolates and Nonisolates Separation of the 118 students into isolate and non— isolate groups yielded the following data: 72 The isolate group consisted of 3M boys and 25 girls with chronological ages from 13.7 through 16.5 and CTMM IQs of 60 through 141. The non isolate group consisted of 33 boys and 26 girls with chronological ages from 13.7 through 15.9 and CTMM le of 92 through 149. Vineland Social Maturity Scale.-—In order to present the relative differences in social competence of isolates versus nonisolates as measured by the 27 Vineland items used, Table 16 was constructed showing item number, item, and per cent of greatest possible total for each of the 27 items. The differences between the percentage ratings of the isolate group and the nonisolate group were not suffi- ciently different on any of the twenty-seven items to be of statistical significance. However, there was a greater dif- ference than might be accounted for by sampling error on seven of the twenty—seven items, These items, therefore, are of value in testing the third hypothesis of this study. These seven items are listed below according to the greatest difference between the percentage ratings of the isolate and nonisolate groups. The greatest difference was shown on item 75,_cares for self at table, which included the final table prepara— tion of various food items for one's own consumption such as baked potato, boiled eggs, cutting meat, etc. The 73 TABLE l6.-- Differences between isolate and nonisolate groups on 27 items of the Vineland social maturity scale. Per cent of Greatest'Possible Total 59 59 Number Item Isolates Nonisolates 75 Cares for self at table 78 61 76 Makes minor purchases 79 79 77 Goes about home town freely 76 74 78 Writes occasional short letters 57 54 79 Makes telephone calls 75 89 80 Does small remunerative . work 79 75 81 Answers ad; purchases by mail 42 35 82 Does simple creative work 64 64 83 Is left to care for self or others 84 84 84 Enjoys books, newspapers, magazines 79 79 85 Plays difficult games 70 82 86 Exercises complete care of dress 86 91 87 Buys own clothing accessories 64 69 88 'Engages in group activities 65 78 89 Performs responsible routine chores 88 81 9O Communicates by letter 35 35 91 Follows current events 61 65 92 Goes to nearby places alone 64 61 93 Goes out unsupervised daytime 68 68 94 Has own spending money 81 86 95 Buys all own clothing 44 58 96 Goes to distant points alone 41 54 97 Looks after own health 82 86 98 Has a job or continues schooling 100 100 99 Goes out nights unrestricted 47 53 100 Controls own major expenditures 59 75 101 Assumes personal responsibility 75 81 74 isolate group exceeded the nonisolate group by 78 to 61 per cent on this item. Controls own major expenditures, item 100, included making major purchases from allowances or earnings with only general advice of what to buy. The nonisolate group surpassed the isolate group by 75 to 59 per cent on this item. Makes telephone calls, item 79, involved looking up phone numbers and making local phone calls for practical purposes. The nonisolate group excelled by 89 to 75 per cent on this item. Buys all own clothing, item 95, involved selection and purchase of all major clothing items, either with money earned, from an allowance, or on credit account. The non— isolate group surpassed the isolate group by 58 to 44 per cent on this item. Engages in group activities, item 88, included participation in the following: athletic teams, clubs, social or literary organizations, dances, parties, trips, and outdoor sports. The nonisolate group excelled by 78 to 65 per cent on this item. Goes to distant points alone, item 96, consisted of planning itinerary, making reservations, and meeting any emergencies that arise. The nonisolate group surpassed the isolate group by 54 to 41 per cent on this item. Plays difficult games, item 85, included playing relatively complex or skilled games and Sports such as baseball, basketball, tennis, pool, or card games, and understanding rules and methods of scoring. The nonisolate group excelled by 82 to 70 per cent on this item. ~The nonisolate group achieved greater social compe- tence on six of the seven items listed above. Of the six, two items, 95 and 100, were from the Self Sufficiency area of social competence; two items, 85 and 88, were from the Social Participation area; one item, 79, was from the Com- munication area; one item, 96, was from the Locomotion area. The only one of the seven items which ranked isolates higher than nonisolates was item 75 from the Self Suffi— ciency area. On the twenty remaining items the differences between the percentage ratings of the isolate and nonisolate groups were so small as to be totally depreciated by the statistical allowance for sampling error. Mooney Problem Check List.——In order to present the differences in perceived problems of the isolate and non— isolate groups as indicated by the Mooney Problem Check List, Table 17 has been constructed showing item number, item name under the seven problem areas, and the percentage of the isolate and nonisolate groups who identified them— selves with each problem. 76 TABZFZ 17.—-lllffexnn.cen befinyren 1:3913t1: and run.is“L< A of tne Mooney problem cheek list. Pe‘“ C"it ildfa(ifléj Fr'iblcwn Item ~ —“”_‘ 'Number Item Under Problem Area Isolate Kshisclate Health, Physical Deve opment 2 Don't get enough sleep 36 49 3 Have trouble with my teeth 27 13 40 Not good-looking 31 19 73 OVerweight 37 27 178 Not being as strong as some other kids 22 12 School 6 Getting low grades in school 29 i5 7 Afraid of tests 34 15 9 Don't like to study 54 39 10 Not interested in books 17 39 41 Afraid of failing in school work 29 14 43 Trouble with Spelling or grammar 25 15 78 Can't keep my mind on my studies 56 44 79 Worried about grades 41 31 115 Teachers not practicing what they preach 19 31 147 Trouble with oral reports 44 20 150 Afraid to speak up in class 37 22 Home and Family 15 Never having any fun with mother or dad 29 14 85 Wanting things my parents won't give me 25 ‘3 Money, Work, the Future 53 Wanting to buy more of my own things 34 3 55 T06 little Spending money 34 34 124 Wanting to know more about college 29 no 192 Not knowing what I really want 9 24 195 Wondering what becomes of people when they die 19 3) Boy, Girl, Relations 92 So often not allowed to go out at night 34 24 196 Learning how to dance 5 12 27 Bashful 29 12 28 Being left out of things 34 2O 29 Never chosen as a leader 37 7 61 Being teased 32 19 63 Feelings too easily hurt 31 17 98 Being picked on 25 7 100 People finding fault with me 31 17 166 Getting into arguments 17 32 Self-Centered Concerns 31 Being nervous 41 31 34 Being afraid of making mistakes 46 29 67 Trying to stop a bad habit 25 41 69 Giving in to temptations 15 31 105 Sometimes wishing I'd never been born 32 20 136 Being careless 12 25 171 Feeling ashamed of something I've done 17 31 172 Being punished for something I didn't do 25 12 77 Of the total 210 items on the check list, only 41 are involved here as the differences between the percentage ratings of these groups on 169 items were so small as to be totally depreciated by the statistical allowance for sampling error. Two items showed a sufficient difference in percent— age between the isolate and nonisolate groups to be of statistical significance. These were items 147, trouble with oral reports, under the problem area School and 29, never chosen as a leader, under the problem area Relations to People in General. Each item was mentioned more fre- quently by the isolates. On thirty—nine items, there was a greater difference between the percentages of the isolate and nonisolate groups than might be accounted for by sampling error. On twenty-eight of these items, more of the isolates identified themselves with the problem; whereas, on eleven of the thirty-nine, more of the nonisolates identified themselves with the problem. Under the first problem area, Health and Physical Development, there was a difference in percentage between the isolate and nonisolate groups on five items. The iso— lates marked four of the five more frequently: have trouble with my teeth, notggood—looking, overweight, and not being as strong as some other kids. The nonisolates marked one more frequently: don't get enough sleep. 78 Within the second problem area, School, there was a difference in percentage between the two groups on ten items. The isolates marked eight of the ten more frequently: getting low grades in school, afraid of tests, don't like to study, afraid of failing in school work, trouble with spelling or grammar, can't keep my mind on my studies, worried about grades, and afraid to speak up in class. The nonisolates marked two of the ten more frequently: got interested in books and teachers not practicing what they preach. In the third problem area, Home and Family, there was a difference in percentage between the two groups on two items. In both cases the isolates marked the item more fre— quently: never having any fun with mother or dad and wanting things my parents won‘t give me. 4. 'Under the fourth problem area, Money, Work and the Future, there was a difference in percentage on five items. The isolates marked two of the five items more frequently: wanting to buy more of my own things and too little spending money. The nonisolates marked: wanting to know more about college, not knowing what I really want, and wondering what becomes of people when they die. Within the fifth problem area, Boy and Girl Relations, there was a difference in percentage between the two groups on two items. In both cases the isolates marked the items more frequently: learning how to dance and so often not allowed to go out atfinight. 79 In the sixth problem area, Relations to People in General, there was a difference in percentage on seven items. The isolates marked six of the items more frequently: bashful, being left out of things, being teased, feelings too easily hurt, being picked on, and peOple finding fault with me. The nonisolates marked one item more frequently: getting into arguments. Under the seventh problem area, Self-Centered Con- cerns, there was a difference in percentage on eight items. .The isolates marked four items more frequently: being ner- vous, being afraid of making mistakes, sometimes wishing I'd never been born, and being punished for something I didn't do. The nonisolates marked four items more fre- quently: trying to stop a bad habit, giving in to temp- tations, being careless, and feeling ashamed of something I've done. Rohde Sentence Completion Method.--In order to pre- I sent the material from both variables evaluated by the Rohde's method for the isolate and nonisolate groups, Table 18 has been constructed listing the 18 objects included, with three columns showing the percentages of each group giving positive, neutral, or negative cathection of each object. The per cent of negative and positive responses for a given group will show, not only whether it is to be con— sidered that members of such a group are likely to have 80 TABLE l8.—-Cathections of junior high school isolates and nonisolates. Per cent Isolate Per cent Nonisolate ObJECt Posi- Neu— Nega- Posi- Neu- Nega- tive tral tive tive tral tive BOYS 39 37 24 46 37 17 Children 51 2O 29 51 24 25 Family 44 44 12 66 31 3 Father 53 j 31 17 61 2O 19 Fighting 36 10 54 32 O 68 Friends 75 9 17 81 9 10 Girls 32 31 37 32 36 32 God 81 19 O 83 14 3 Iiome 32 19 49 42 19 31 Laws 64 7 29 76 l2 12 Money 54 14 32 54 7 3C IWother 63 25 12 63 20 17 ENBOple 35 36 25 53 3it 1“ f{Sligion 7E 19 7 73 19 9 Scflioolwork 44 17 39 61 2 37 iSUicide 19 2+ 58 IO 14 76 “Teachers 64 9 27 73 12 15 Work. 61 7 32 58 O 42 81 problems relative to the object in question, but the dis- parity between the percentages will also indicate the extent of the problem. Of the total 18 objects included there was no suffi- cient difference in the percentage of positive cathections between the isolate and nonisolate groups to be considered statistically significant. However, on five objects, there was a greater difference between the percentage of the posi- tive cathections of the isolate and nonisolate groups than might be accounted for by sampling error. On all of these objects the isolate group showed less acceptance of the object than did the nonisolate group. On the object, Family, the nonisolate group showed 66 pmm‘cent acceptance and 3 per cent rejection, as com- Pared.to 44 per cent of the isolate group showing acceptance and 12 per cent showing rejection. For the object item, figmg, the nonisolate group Qiowed 42 per cent acceptance and 39 per cent rejection, 33 Clompared to 32 per cent of the isolate group showing a'CCthance and 49 per cent rejection. Concerning the object relating to Léflég the non- isolate group showed 76 per cent acceptance and 12 per cent r'e.l°8<:tion, as compared to 64 per cent of the isolate group ShOWing acceptance and 29 per cent rejection. For the object, People, the nonisolate group showed E: 43 Der*cent acceptance and 14 per cent rejection, as com- 82 pared to 39 per cent of the isolate group showing acceptance and 25 per cent rejection. On the object pertaining to Schoolwork, the nonisolate grmmp showed 61 per cent acceptance and 37 per cent re- jection, as compared to 44 per cent of the isolate group showing acceptance and 39 per cent rejection. In addition, there were a total of seven objects for which there was a difference between the percentage of nega- tive cathections for the isolate and nonisolate groups. Three of these, Home, Laws, and People, are listed above in the discussion of positive cathections. The others not given there are Fighting, Suicide, Teachers, and W235. Regarding the object, Fighting, the nonisolate group showed 68 per cent rejection and 32 per cent acceptance, as compared to 54 per cent of the isolate group showing re- Jection and 36 per cent acceptance. For the object relating to Suicide, the nonisolate group showed 76 per cent rejection and 10 per cent accep- tance, as compared to 58 per cent of the isolate group ShCWwing rejection and 19 per cent acceptance. Concerning the object, Teachers, the isolate group ShO‘Wed27 per cent rejection and 64 per cent acceptance, as Chompared to 15 per cent of the nonisolate group showing I’eJEECtion and 73 Per cent acceptance. In reference to the object, prk, the nonisolate EKVNAD showed 42 per cent rejection and 58 per cent 83 acceptance, as compared to 32 per cent of the isolate group showing rejection and 61 per cent acceptance. For five of the objects there was a difference be- tween the percentages of neutral cathections for the isolate and nonisolate groups. On all five objects the isolate .‘ group showed the greatest percentage of neutrality. Included here are the objects: Family, Father, Fighting, Schoolwork, and Suicide. The nonisolate group showed the greatest acceptance of the objects: Family, Home, Laws, People, and Schoolwork. This group showed greatest rejection of the objects: Fighting, Suicide, and prk. The isolate group showed greatest rejection of the objects: Home, Laws, PeOple, and Teachers. ngating Perceived Problems of the Gifted and Nongifted Separation of the 118 students into gifted and non— Sifted groups yielded the following data: The gifted group consisted of 21 boys and 20 girls with chronological ages from 13.7 through 16.0 and CTMM 198 of 130 through 149. The nongifted group consisted of 46 boys and 31 girls with chronological ages from 13.7 through 16.5 and CTWMA IQs of 60 through 128. Vineland Social Maturity Scale.--In order to present 13“? relative differences in social competence of the gifted VerTMis the nongifted as measured by the 27 Vineland items 8M used, Table 19 was constructed showing item number, item, and per cent of greatest possible total for each of the 27 items. The differences between the percentage ratings of the gifted group and the nongifted group were not sufficiently different on any of the 27 items to be of statistical significance. However, there was a greater difference than might be accounted for by sampling error on twelve of the twenty—seven items. These items, therefore, are of value in testing the third hypothesis of this study. These twelve items are listed below according to the greatest difference between the percentage ratings of the gifted and nongifted groups. The greatest difference was shown on item 82, goes figmple creative work, which included making useful articles, doing repair work, cooking, baking, sewing, gardening, writing simple stories or poems, producing Painting or drawings. The gifted group excelled by 7M to 58 per'cent on this item. Goes to distant points alone, item 96, consisted of plarniing itinerary, making reservations and meeting any amergencies which arise. The gifted group surpassed the nongifted group by 57 to #2 per cent on this item. Goes out unsupervised daytime, item 93, involved EQINE away from home without supervision, being responsible for’movements, and revealing discreet behavior. The 85 TABLE l9.--Differences between gifted and nongifted groups on 27 items of the Vineland social maturity scale. Per cent of Greatest Possible Total 41 77 Number Item Gifted Nongifted 75 Cares for self at table 71 69 76 Makes minor purchases 82 75 77 Goes about home town freely 79 73 78 Writes occasional short letters 51 58 79 Makes telephone calls 84 81 80 Does small remunerative work 81 75 81 Answers ads; purchases by mail 45 34 82 Does simple creative work 74 58 83 Is left to care for self or others 85 83 84 Enjoys books, newspapers, magazines 87 76 85 Plays difficult games 76 77 86 Exercises complete care of dress 90 87 87 Buys own clothing accessories 70 65 88 Engages in group activities 76 7O 89 Performs responsible routine chores. 85 84 9O Communicates by letter 34 35 91 Follows current events 72 59 92 Goes to nearby places alone 71 58 93 Goes out unsupervised daytime 77 63 94 Has own spending money 89 8O 95 Buys all own clothing 57 47 96 Goes to distant points alone 57 42 97 Looks after own health 93 79 98 Has a job or continues schooling 100 100 99 Goes out nights unrestricted. 54 49 100 Controls own major expendi- tures 73 53 101 Assumes personal responsi- bility 87 73 gifted group surpassed the nongifted group by 77 to 63 per cent on this item. Looks after own health, item 97, consisted of safe- guarding health with regard to rules of hygiene, contagious or infectious diseases, illnesses and accidents. The gifted group exceeded the nongifted by 93 to 79 per cent on this item. Assumes personal responsibility, item 101, involved directing own social affairs, being considerate of the wel- fare of others, exercising discretion in personal activities. The gifted group excelled by 87 to 73 per cent on this item. Follows current events, item 91, included being able to discuss general news and sports events and following these with some continuity. The gifted group surpassed the nongifted by 72 to 59 per cent on this item. Goes to nearby places alone, item 92, involved going outside the limits of the home town into areas that are rel— atively unfamiliar and being personally responsible for own arrangements. The gifted exceeded the nongifted by 71 to 58 per cent on this item. Answers ads; purchases by mail, item 81, involved responding to magazine, radio, television advertising by mailing coupons, requesting samples, sending for literature, and ordering from catalogs. The gifted group exceeded the nongifted group by 45 to 34 per cent on this item. 87 Enjoys books, newspapers, magazines, item 84, con— sisted of reading for practical information or personal en- joyment. The gifted group excelled by 87 to 76 per cent on this item. Buys all own clothing, item 95, involved selection and purchase of all major clothing items, either with money earned, from an allowance, or on a credit account. The gifted group surpassed the nongifted group by 57 to 47 per cent on this item. Controls own major expenditures, item 100, included exercising discretion in providing for major expenses from allowances or earnings with only general advice from others. The gifted group surpassed the nongifted group by 73 to 63 per cent on this item. Has own spending money, item 94, consisted of using allowance or earnings with reasonable discretion for personal needs. The gifted group excelled by 89 to 80 per cent on this item. The gifted ranked higher than the nongifted on all twelve of these items. Six items, 93, 94, 95, 97, 100, and 101, were from the Self—Direction area; two items, 92 and 96, were from the Locomotion area; three items, 81, 84, and 91, were from the Communication area; and one item, 82, was from the Occupational Activities area. 88 On the fifteen remaining items the differences between the percentage ratings of the gifted and nongifted groups were so small as to be totally depreciated by the statistical allowance for sampling error. Mooney Problem Check List.-—In order to present the differences in perceived problems of the gifted and non- gifted groups as indicated by the Mooney Problem Check List, Table 20 has been constructed showing item number, item name under the seven problem areas, and the percentage of the gifted and nongifted groups who identified themselves with each problem. Of the total 210 items on the check list, only 33 are involved here as the differences between the percentage ratings of these groups on 177 items were so small as to be totally depreciated by the statistical allowance for sampling error. Three items showed a sufficient difference in per— D . \D ntage betwecn the gifted and nongifted groups to be of C . ( statistical significance. These items were afraid of tests, trouble with spelling or grammar, and trouble with writing. These items are all under the problem area School and men— tioned more frequently by the nongifted. On thirty items, there was a greater difference be— tween the percentages of the gifted and nongifted groups than might be accounted for by sampling error. On ten of these items, more of the gifted identified themselves with 89 TABLE 20.--Differences between the gifted and nongifted groups on 33 items of the Mooney problem check list. _-a———_.. " ' - ‘ ' " E"'_'_"‘ Per Cent Marking Problem Item Number Item Under Problem Area Gifted Nongifted Health, Physical Development 2 Don't get enough sleep 51 38 3 Have trouble with my teeth 10 _ 2 40 Not good-looking 32 21 73 Overweight 2O 39 School 6 Getting low grades in school 12 , 2 7 Afraid of tests 7 3“ 10 Not interested in books 12 2 42 Trouble with arithmetic 22 4Q 43 Trouble with spelling or grammar 2 30 45 Trouble with writing 7 89 78 Can't keep my mind on my studies 42 55 79 Worried about grades 24 42 147 Trouble with oral reports 22 38 150 Afraid to speak up in class 22 33 184 Not interested in certain subjects 37 “9 Home and Family 15 Never having any fun with mother or dad 15 25 50 Parents not understanding me 24 34 118 Parents not trusting me 34 21 119 Parents old~fashioned in their ideas 42 ' 2b 153 Not telling parents everything 22 34 Money, Work, the Future 18 Having no regular allowance 10 87 192 Nof knowing what I really want 10 2C 195 Wondering what becomes of peOple when they die 17 27 Boy, Girl, Relations 21 Not allowed to use the family car 15 29 92 So often not allowed to go out at night 22 33 People in General 27 Bashful 12 25 3O Wishing people liked me better 44 33 96 Wanting a more pleasing personality 51 39 134 Missing someone very much 22 34 Self-Centered Concerns 69 Giving in to temptations 34 17 136 Being careless _ 27 14 138 Forgetting things 44 29 139- Being lazy 34 20 90 the problem; whereas, on twenty of the thirty, more of the nongifted identified themselves with the problem. Under the first problem area, Health and Physical Development, there was a difference in percentage between the gifted and nongifted groups on four items. The gifted marked two of the four more frequently: don't get enough sleep and not good-looking. The nongifted marked two more frequently: have trouble with my teeth and overweight. Within the second problem area, School, there was a difference in percentage between the two groups on eight items. The nongifted marked all eight items more frequently: getting low grades in school, not interested in books, trouble with arithmetic, can‘t keep mygmind on my studies, worried about grades, trouble with oral reports, afraid to speak up in class, and not interested in certain subjects. 1n the third problem area, Home and Family, there was a difference in percentage between the two groups on five items. The gifted marked two items more frequently: parents not trusting me and parents old-fashioned in their ideas. The nongifted marked three items more frequently: never having anypfun with mother or dad, parents not under- standing me, and not telling parents everything. Under the fourth problem area, Money, Work and the Future, there was a difference in percentage on three items. The nongifted marked all three more frequently: having no 91 regular allowance, not knowing what I really want, and won— dering what becomes of people when they die. Within the fifth problem area, Boy and Girl Relations, there was a difference in percentage between the two groups on two items. The nongifted marked each more frequently: not allowed to use the family car and so often not allowed to go out at night. In the sixth problem area, Relations to People in General, there was a difference in percentage on four items. The gifted marked two items more frequently: wishing people liked me better and wanting a more pleasing personality. The nongifted marked two items more frequently: bashful and missing someone very much. Under the seventh problem area, Self—Centered Con- cerns, there was a difference in percentage on four items. The gifted marked all four items more frequently: giving in to temptations, being careless, forgetting things, and being lazy. Rohde Sentence Completion Method.--In order to pre- sent the material from both variables evaluated by the Rohde's method for the gifted and nongifted groups, Table 21 was constructed listing the 18 objects included, with three columns showing the percentages of each group giving positive, neutral, or negative cathection of each object. 92 TABLE 2l.--Cathections of junior high school gifted and nongifted. Per cent Gifted Per cent Nongifted ObJGCt Posi- Neu— Nega- Posi- Neu- Nega- tive tral tive tive tral tive Boys 42 39 2O 43 36 21 Children 46 29 24 53 18 29 Family 71 24 5 47 44 9 Father 56 29 15 57 23 2O Fighting 22 10 68 4O 3 57 Friends 71 15 15 82 5 13 Girls 32 34 34 33 33 35 God 83 15 2 82 17 1 Home 42 27 32 5 14 51 Laws 71 10 2O 7O 9 21 Money 56 12 32 53 9 38 Mother 56 24 2O 66 22 12 People 49 34 ' 17 44 35 21 Religion 76 17 7 73 2o 8 Schoolwork 56 5 39 51 12 38 Suicide 10 15 76 17 21 62 Teachers 61 17 22 73 ‘7 21 Work 54 2 44 62 4 34 93 Of the total 18 objects included, there was only one, Family, where there was a sufficient difference in the per— centage of positive cathections between the gifted and now— gifted groups to be considered statistically significant. Of the gifted group, 71 per cent showed acceptance for the object, Family, with 24 per cent neutral, and 5 per cent showing rejection, as compared to 47 per cent of the non- gifted group showing acceptance, 44 per cent neutral, and 9 per cent showing rejection. However, on four objects, there was a greater dif- ference between the percentage of the positive cathections of the gifted and nongifted groups than might be accounted for by sampling error. On all of these objects the non- gifted group showed greater acceptance of the object than did the gifted group. On the object relative to Fighting, the nongifted group showed 40 per cent acceptance and 57 per cent rejec- tion, as compared to 22 per cent of the gifted group showing acceptance and 68 per cent rejection. For the object, Friends, the nongifted group showed 82 per cent acceptance and 13 per cent rejection, as com- pared to 71 per cent of the gifted group showing acceptance and 15 per cent rejection. Concerning the object, Mother, the nongifted group showed 66 per cent acceptance and 12 per cent rejection, as compared to 56 per cent of the gifted group showing acceptance and 20 per cent rejection. 94 Pertaining to the object, Teachers, the nongifted group showed 73 per cent acceptance and 21 per cent re- jection, as compared to 61 per cent of the gifted group showing acceptance and 22 per cent rejection. In addition, there were a total of four objects for which there was a difference between the percentage of negative cathections for the gifted and nongifted groups. One of these, Fighting, is listed above in the discussion of positive cathections. The others not given there are Home, Suicide, and Work. Regarding the object item, Hpme, the nongifted group showed 51 per cent rejection and 35 per cent acceptance, as compared to 32 per cent of the gifted group showing re- jection and 42 per cent acceptance. For the object, Suicide, the gifted group showed 76 per cent rejection and 10 per cent acceptance, as com- pared to 62 per cent of the nongifted group showing re- jection and 17 per cent acceptance. In regard to the object, prk, the gifted group showed 44 per cent rejection and 54 per cent acceptance, as compared to 34 per cent of the nongifted group showing rejection and 62 per cent acceptance. For five of the objects, there was a difference be- tween the percentages of neutral cathections for the gifted anulriongifted groups. On one of these the nongifted group showed a greater percentage of neutrality. This was the object, Family. The gifted group showed a greater per- centage of neutrality on the objects: Children, Friends, Home, and Teachers. The nongifted group showed the greatest acceptance of the objects: Fighting, Friends, Mother, and Teachers. This group showed the greatest rejection of the object: Home. The gifted group showed the greatest acceptance of the object: Family. This group showed the greatest rejec- tion of the objects: Fighting, Suicide, and Work. Locating Perceived Problems of the Two Isolate Groups and the Two Gifted Groups Separation of the 118 students into gifted isolates, nongifted isolates, gifted nonisolates, and nongifted non— isolates yielded the following data: The gifted isolate group consisted of 5 girls and 8 boys with chronological ages from 13.7 through 16.0 with a mean CA of 14.3 and CTMM IQs of 130 through 141 with a mean IQ of 134. The nongifted isolate group consisted of 20 girls and 26 boys with chronological ages from 13.7 through 16.5 with a mean CA of 14.4 and CTMM IQ's of 60 through 128 with a mean IQ of 107. The gifted nonisolate group consisted of 15 girls and 13 boys with chronological ages from 13.8 through 15.4 with a mean CA of 14.4 and CTMM IQs of 130 through 149 with a mean IQ of 136. 96 The nongifted nonisolate group consisted of 11 girls and 20 boys with chronological ages from 13.7 through 15.9 with a mean CA of 14.7 and CTMM IQs of 92 through 127 with a mean IQ of 112. Vineland Social Maturity Scale.--In order to present the relative differences in social competence among the four basic subgroups as measured by the 27 Vineland items used, Table 22 was constructed showing item number, item, and per cent of greatest possible total for each of the four basic subgroups on all 27 items. Tables 16 and 22 were used to compare the sets of perceived problems for the total isolate group and the two isolate subgroups, gifted isolates and nongifted isolates. Table 23 has been constructed to show those problems per- ceived by the total isolate group, those perceived by the gifted isolates, and those perceived by the nongifted iso- lates. Comparing gifted isolates and nongifted isolates to all isolates, there are problems that both subgroups have in common, problems that other isolates have which gifted isolates do not have, and problems which nongifted isolates that other isolates do not have. The differences between the percentage ratings of the Siftuad isolate group and the nongifted isolate group were “0t sufficiently different on any of the 27 items to be of statistical significance. However, there was a greater 97 mm mm m» mm apHHHnHmCOQmmu Hmcomsmd mdfidmm¢ Hoa an om ow mm mohapaucmdxm LOnmS :30 machucoo QOH mm mm s: om umpoaspmmpc: magmas pzo meow mm OOH ooa ooa ooa wsHHoonom moscflpeoo so now a new mm 5» mm om . mm spasm: :30 sound mxooa so am as me an ocean mpcaoa assumes op mooc mm we we a: mm msfiepoao ago Has mssm mm >5 mm mm up accoe wcfivcodm cso mm: am mm mu mm mm maduamn vmma>pmasmcs use meow mm mm as mw mm macaw mmomHa manned on meow mm om ms mm mm mucm>m ucmnpso mzoaaom am am mm om Hm soupoa so mopmowcseEoo om ms mm hm mm monoso meauson mandmcoamms mapomnom mm as mm , mm mm _ mmfiua>duom macaw CH mommwcm mm mm ms mm mm moanommmoom mcfinuoao :30 mmsm em .mm mm mm mm mmmnu mo memo mpmadeoo mmmfiopmxm mm am am me me moeow pasofiucflo mamas mm mu mm we mm mmcaumwws .mnmdmamzmc .mxoon whencm aw mm mm 2m mm mnmnuo no mamm new menu on puma mH mm mm mu am up xso: m>Hummmo mHQSHm moon mm mm m: mm om . Hams,mn mommnonSQ mmuw msmzmc¢ Hm mm cm as aw xsoz m>HpmnmCSEmn Hanan moon om mm mm as .mo mHHmo osoeooaop moss: as mm mm Hm m: mumppma unonm anneammooo moves: we mm ow up up . mammnm :30» neon psonm meow we mm mm as aw mommnonsa.pocfle mmxmz we mm mm up Hm mans» um mach pom mmsmo mu mumaomflcoz mumaomacoz muwHOmH mumHomH EmpH nonezz oopsamcoz cognac oopmawcoz cognac sopH fiance manflmmom pmopmmpw no name 9mm .madom hufisspws Hmfioom vcmHmcH> on» no named am so mdzopwnsm snow one wcosm mmocmmcmufinrr.mm mqmm mpcmsma wCHHHmp poz .mmH mm Hm mm Hm 650: pm mEoHCOCC CHMCCmo mmsomHu 0p oHnmCs omH mm mm mm :m mmmUH CHmCu CH umCOHmewucHo mpCmam mHH mm mm NH am we wCHpmspp uOC mpCmme wHH mH mH mH Hm mpCoCmd as an CoNHOHpHAo wCHmm oHH 0H N am Hm we m>Hw p.Coz mquCmq he meHCu wCHCsz mm mH HH mm Hm 65 mo Code 00» wCHuomaxm muCoLCm 2m mm mm mm mm vs wCHprumCoCCC uoc mqusmm om mm 3 mm am can no CmCuoE Csz Csm 5C8 wCH>mC nm>mz mH >HHEmm CCC med: mm mm m: .Hm mpoonnsm CHmquo CH umumopopCH uoz :wH mm om w: mm . mommmHo HHCQ HmH em mH am Hm mmoHo :H o: xooom oo oHoCC< omH 0H 5 NH Hm zCoEmE Loom 02H mm mm mm mH muCOdmC sto Csz chsoue ezH mm mm mm m Lamond mos» pans wCHoHpomLa no: owCommB mHH om mm om o: Cocommp m Csz wcon wCprom uoz aHH m: om m: mm mommeo CH mmmHumos Home cmpmo om MHH m: :H mm o: mmpmsw psonm pmHsCoz on m: mm mm on mmHvzpw as CO UCHE as ammx u.Cmo we m: o: m: we zczpm CH oEHu cmCOCm wCHpcmdm poz me mm 5 mm m wCHpHC3 Cqu mHCCOCB m: mm 0 cm m CmEEme Co wCHHHQO Csz oHnsoee m: mm Hm H: mm OHCmECpHCm Cqu manope m: mH a mm mm xsozHoozom CH wCHHHmm Co pHmCC< H: mm mH mm o mxoon CH pmumoCmpCH uoz OH mm mm om me mesum on oxHH u_Coo m mm s H: a mono» Co onsCa s 0H :H mm m Hoocom CH wmpmsw 30H wCHuuoo w HOOCum OH :H om Hm mUHx Congo meow mm wcoppm mm wCHmp uoz we. mm HH HH om homo as Cqu mHCCOCB mzH mm Hm m: mH quH63Co>o ms mH HH mm m Capo» me Cqu mHnsosu m>m: m m: cm Om am ammHm cwsocm new u.Com m acoEQoHo>oQ Haunhcm .CuHmo: mpmHOchoz oumHomHCoz mumHomH muwHOmH mmp< EmHnosm CmpC: EmpH Canasz oooCchoz socho oopechoz sounds sopH EmHCOCm wCHwaE QCOCo mo pcmo Com . pwwa xuifio 53H.,_..\,.Lu ....... .. .._ 1:- F, . .m wk :7. uJJQLwDSm LSOM m3.» wCOEm mwocmkmhhfifllv.mm Mdmxfiw lC)4 MH HH mm mm on u.coHo H wcanoaom sou omnoHcsa mcHom msH om mm pH mH ocoo o>.H mcanoaon Co coeonms mcHHoom HHH OH mm mm mm zuaH wCHum OMH Om am pm mw wCHadunumaa smH OH mm HH mH unoHoAao MCHom mmH mm mH mm Hm econ coon so>oc u.H mcHnon noeroaom mOH OH mm mm O oOCouHMCoouuHon wCquaH 30H mH mm om m mCHaLAoz mOH OH m: mH mH mCoHpmquop 0» CH wCH>HO mm mm me am Hm anos can a noun o» wcHsus em mm mm m: am mmxwumHE wCmeE no CHaAhm wCHom am mm mm m: Hm mCo>CvC wCHum Hm mChwocoo voumucoonuHom mm mm mm mH Chonnsum wCHmm OOH mm mm em mm Congo» as mchoa moH mm mm om O mqusswhm OuCH wCHpqu OOH mm OH Om Hm Cons zCo> oC0~E0m wCHmmHz :MH mH 3H em we me nqu pHsmC wcHocHC oHaoom 00H OH : 2m Hm . Co meoHC wCHom Om mm mm H: mm zuHHmCOmuoq wCHmmoHQ whoa m wCHquz mm MH Hm om Hm puss AHHmmm oo» meHHoom mo OH Hm mm mm uaonm ommep wCHmm mm mm HH om am comma» wCHmm Ho om am am am Coupon 65 ooxHH «Haooq wcHanz om OH 3 mm O: CovmmH a ma Comono Lm>mz mm mH Hm mm mm owchu so ago pcoH wcHom mm MH HH mm mH HCOCnCm um Hmsocmo CH oHaomm mm HH AH Hm goo oanoooo on» pzooo nose oou mcchHCe maH MH HH mm mm mocmc 0» so: wCHCCwmq OOH om HH HH Hm somouo cm 0» Connor: mcHoHooo meH mm Hm em mm uanC um pso ow 0» ooonHm so: coueo om mm s wH AH Hm moHusoq on om ou.oocono «HupHH 069 mm ON OH om O Cmo mHHemu on» on: on umonHm uoz Hm mcoHuoHom HCHo .som mm mm :m O 6H8 zone can: oHaomd no acacoon chz wCHCoCCoz mOH mm mm mm Om uoHLLwE pow hw>o HH.H uH wCHhmncoz now mm. Hm em mm ace: sHHmmC H was: wCHzocx no: N: om mm ms oonHoo “some ones 30:: o“ mcfiucmz mmH mm. mm s . a. Haas $3:on 0H :2 OH a , a uuHH cos mm mm. HH mm NH mace ”noon umHCCoz aHHemm 0H s. x mH 41" ‘1‘! mocmzoaam awaswmh o: mCH>m= .ot tip .xLoz .Hmcoz 105 TABLE 26.--Mooney check list items isolates perceive as problems Per cent of Group Marking Problem Area Number All Gifted Nongi fted Non- Isolates Isolates Isolates Isolates Health, Physical Development 3 27 33 12 73 37 43 27 143 39 20 178 22 31 2O 12 Home, Family 15 29 39 29 1“ 5O 32 5 29 116 31 19 118 39 29 119 54 31 153 29 33 14 186 46 20 School 6 29 35 15 7 34 41 15 9 54 69 5O 39 41 29 39 26 14 42 41 31 43 25 3O 15 76 62 47 78 56 59 44 114 46 27 147 44 52 20 149 31 12 Money, Work, The Future 18 25 3O 17 19 31 12 192 37 24 Boy, Girl Relations 23 31 12 92 34 37 24 165 31 19 196 25 39 22 12 198 31 25 People in Genered. 27 29 33 12 29 37 46 35 7 30 54 32 61 32 54 19 62 39 20 100 31 46 17 Self Centered Concerns 31 41 83 31 34 46 54 43 29 139 39 24 172 25 39 22 12 isolates do not, and p otlems which nongifted isolates pg: ceive that other isolates do not. There are eight items of the Mooney which assist us in locating perceived problems of isolates. Those items are listed below. Health and Physical Development Not being as strong as other kids School Don't like to study Afraid of failing in school work Home and Family Never having any fun with mother or dad ikp'PVTilEitl Relati'wns Learning how to dance Relations to People in General Never chosen as a leader Self-Centered Concerns Being afraid of making mistakes Being punished for something I didn‘t do Twenty-three items of the Mooney help to locate the Perceived problems of nongifted isolates. Health and Physical Development Have trouble with my teeth Overweight Not being as strong as some other kids School Getting low grades in school .Afraid of tests 107 Don‘t like to study Afraid of failing in school work Trouble with arithmetic Trouble with spelling or grammar Can't keep my mind on my studies Trouble with oral reports Home and Family Never having any fun with mother or dad Parents not understanding me Not telling parents everything Money, Work, the Future Having no regular allowance Not knowing what I really want Boy and Girl Relations So often not allowed to go out at night Learning how to dance Relations to People in General Bashful Never chosen as a leader Self-Centered Concerns Being nervous Being afraid of making mistakes Being punished for something I didn't do Twenty-five items help to locate perceived problems of gifted isolates. Health and Physical DevelOpment Trouble with my eyes Not being as strong as some other kids School Don't like to study Afraid of failing in school work Not spending enough time in study Not getting along with a teacher Poor memory 108 Home and Family Never having any fun with mother or dad Being criticized by my parents Parents not trusting me Parents old-fashioned in their ideas Clash of opinions between me and my parents Money43Work, the Future Family worried about money Boy and Girl Relations Too little chance to go to parties Deciding whether to go steady Learning how to dance Thinking too much about the opposite sex Relations to PeOple in General Never chosen as a leader Wishing people liked me better Being teased Being talked about People finding fault with me Self-Centered Concerns Being afraid of making mistakes Being lazy Being punished for something I didn't do Tables 19 and 25 were used to locate and compare the sets of perceived problems for the total gifted group and the two gifted subgroups, isolate gifted and nonisolate gifted. Table 27 has been constructed to show thoserprob- lems perceived by the gifted group, those perceived by the isolate gifted, and those perceived by the nonisolate gifted. Comparing isolate gifted and nonisolate gifted to all gifted, there are problems that both groups have in 109 TABLE 27.--Mooney check list items the gifted perceive as problems. Per cent of Group Marking Problem Area Number All Isolate Nonisolate Non- Gifted Gifted Gifted Gifted Health, Physical Development 143 39 18 178 31 16 Home, Family 15 39 25 85 31 18 116 31 17 119 42 54 26 186 46 21 School 9 69 46 41 39 23 76 62 48 114 46 29 115 39 22 149 31 17 Money, Work, The Future 19 31 14 124 50 35 Boy, Girl Relations 23 31 13 165 31 21 196 39 18 People in General 28 39 27 29 46 25 3o 44 54 33 61 54 26 62 39 23 96 51 57 39 98 31 18 100 46 23 166 39 22 Self-Centered Concerns 34 54 35 67 43 3O 69 34 43 17 104 36 22 136 27 32 14 172 39 18 110 common, problems that other gifted students perceive which isolate gifted do not, and problems which nonisolate gifted perceive that other gifted do not. Below are five items of the Mooney which appear to assist us in locating perceived problems of all gifted students. However, it will be noted that these same items appear in either the isolate gifted or the nonisolate gifted listing but not in both. Home and Family Parents old-fashioned in their ideas Relations to People in General Wishing people liked me better Wanting a more pleasing personality Self-Centered Concerns Being careless Giving in to temptations Eight items help to locate perceived problems of nonisolate gifted. Teachers not practicing what they preach Money, Work, the Future Wanting to know more about college Relations to People in General Wanting a more pleasing personality Getting into arguments ill Self—Centered Concerns Being careless Giving in to temptations 2+ Eight items help o locate perceived problems of rlonisolate gifted. Sphool Teachers not practicing what they preach Money, Work, the Future Wanting to know more about college Relations to People in General Wanting a more pleasing personality Getting into arguments Self-Centered Concerns Trying to stop a bad habit Giving in to temptations Lacking self-confidence Being careless Twerdgy—fiama itenns help: to jhdcaime pens eivwal prmflylems Cl? -isolaie gifted. Health and Physical Development Trouble wi n my eyes t Not being as strong as some other kids Rohde Sentence Completion Method.-—Ih order to pre- serit; the material from both variables evaluated by the :ROijéi's method, Table 28 was constructed listing the 18 Otheflts included, with three columns showing the percentages Of eachof the four subgroups, giving positive, neutral, or n , . . . eggtive cathection of each object. TABLE 28.—-Cathections 112 students. among the four subgroups of junior his? Object Positive Neutral Negative Positive Neutral Negative Per Cent Gifted Isolate Per Cent Nongifted isolate BOYS 39 31 31 39 39 33 Children 5U 8 39 50 EU ?0 Family 5U 31 15 U1 U8 11 Father U6 31 23 5U 3O 15 Fighting 15 31 5U U1 U 5U Friends 62 15 23 78 7 15 Girls 39 15 U6 30 J) 35 God 138 O O 76 SU 0 Home 5U 31 15 86 15 53 Laws 62 O 39 65 9 is Money 5U 15 31 5U 13 33 Mother 62 23 15 6;; 26 11 PeOple 39 39 23 39 35 35 Religion 85 15 0 72 20 9 Schoolwork 5U 8 39 U1 9O 59 Suicide 8 8 85 22 28 50 Teachers 69 8 23 63 9 28 Work 69 8 23 59 7 35 Per Cent Gifted Nonisolate Der Cen longifted Nonisolate Boys U3 U3 1U U8 2 19 Children 143 39 18 53 10 33 Family 79 21 0 Sr 39 7 f’ather 61 29 11 61 3 so 1*ighting 25 O 75 39 O 61 E9riends 75 1U 11 87 3 10 Girls 29 U3 29 36 33 36 ()0d 75 21 U 90 7 3 rhyme 36 25 39 U8 13 39 [Jaws 75 1U 11 77 10 13 5'1 Oney 57 11 3:? 52 3 :42. MOther 5a 25 21 71 16 13 Peeople 5U 32 1U 52 36 13 5o 9§N\ U\r+ F‘C p10 ;¢~\ Q;F{ FiH mtu c>o OO :fWN GIN "\N C>O "\N ~O¢fi “IN "\N a mooaono HmSuaz x oo>amomm mooaono >4 at N X E E x >1 B: X M Y X X >/ E > >‘B y I A I I K >1 k a d! .> Hfiwm .w Adana eaowwm .w mmHhmno .w Hawwmzm .m mQEwH .m hnommhc .m Hawmmsm .x :maa .m H< .x naaoacam .m mwSoae Amway nmflovcwm .m ooflhm .m hccmc .m co>¢am Aachv nonpp< om 0d: €18 HNMJMONCC O‘OHNM-ITWIOF—COO‘OHNMJLAO be O‘CHNMJU‘ N >iE 8 a a M FiF‘F‘H'4 HrdrflrdrinJfllQ:QlfilfllfliQJQIQJ"\WHWfofi .aN cczaauww .9 hedge .m sHku .w cacaoo .o popmmpaz .7 mauOuOH> .2 wCHmnom: .h ammocaa .: mw>amw .m gamma .m sauna Jm QCHAoSQwo .o memahwnu .a commSm .< oocmpmcoo C.{.C.$.C.C.7u?v2u79?.7UPVPVPV7VI_I T.T. CAWIC» .LHUIOAUILnVrDhWCL7uTLnuAVCv}.OI TIII..LTL .D+W,t_d.L.U/0 QTL.9£§.H.C§6.L camz 13¢“fnffifflfflffifflffifl\fl\fl\fl\fl\fl\fl‘F\W\F\N\N\F\”\V\W\W\N\V\W\W\W\W\M mooacno C m m o S u m a a a d a m coauotw N 66593 Hoonow Exam Leigh cmsufiflg. 1439 OJH «\H hwu o<3 nJc OJH 9pm «3N r40 oc: «\N r40 'r‘H "\H cc: cc? -b«v IAN HO IAN NO NW NW Pit-i 4i“! Hun HH CH3 xocn In ;¢«\ "ha a mooaono gasps: x oo>dooom mooaono x >4><>< >< ><>< >’. NNNNNCUNMM MM \anl nw:: .> HSmm .w Aflcmv uaopm; .m mwapwno .m Hawmwsm .m moEwh .m hpommgc .m Haouwsm .3 cwad .m H< .m naaoocmm .m masons .m Amway naaoucmm .m oosum .m ancma .m smpwum .a “coma aszawa t“. E >1 E: V E a a x x an E5! HNM:U\\CF\Q/ cm: H mmjmor-cocro Hm MdmONw (3‘0 HN mam rzpnrdrirlrapardnlnsm Hr-‘I .u Gamaapms .9 thLB .m adamm .m mascou .0 umpmmLm: .7 mfigcp0H> .2 mcwmpooo .h mmwucwu .m mfipasw .r maawo .m mspmx an mcfipmgp¢o .U odoapmno .m Copmnm moCmumcoo -v~«\«\«\«\w\«1«\«\«\«\«\w\«\w\"\fl\fl\"\"\".F\Fun f'" r‘“\ ( MMrV\ r~fi:w a“: (LC... (c [HFWICC CtCt(.8 pochva.d d_d 8 6 I.L.L.L .C.Lhu,ouH.anc»n1:c.c.L 0,6nunanv I I_L.L.L l Enrtat .).,. |I V ”4.111. 95hr?» 8T... asap noowono .FM c o m o n o m H H a s n m ccfiuomw w compo Hoogom swam neacsb cmEuHS; 140 MO (“0-4 mm 00 Int-1 NH Sch gira r¢c> PIC) r1r¢ “If! (v.4 o o c>c> c>c> uan \OM HO (EH NO Pic-i .d-M 00 NH NH NW 00 1‘“! NH 0‘0] :0 a noowono Haausfi x uo>38m $398 >1 77>." XX K K N MKS I ‘ »:> I \ 81> >1 11a swan .> Hamm gamma oHoaam .m moapano .m Hammmsm .m mosmh .m maowmpo .m Hammmsm .& cmH< .x H¢ .m naaoucam .m maSoce Amouv naaoucmm .m oosum .m acumc .m so>ouw AGOHV pdzupc .m .m .< >1 I ‘ M x E: x><> r— >1 x a x x V HNMJmOD-CDOOHCUMJWOFQO‘OHNMJMOFfiO‘O HNMJ‘UW r—IHHHHHHHHHNNNNNNNNNNMMMMMM .N cn>aaunall .9 scape .m saamm .m mascou .c pmgmwpmx .: mflpcpofiw .2 «Campomc .h mmmvcfia .a mH>Hmm .m mapmo .m Assam mewptnpmo u mamapmsu .m cathw oucmuwCoo 0C) .¢ "HfirfiWN"\W\"\N\W\W\F\N\Nvfir1(flWN”\W\F\“\Fufltfiffiffifl\fl\fl\fifl"M“ {Cert Shlrl €€£a7uaaaaaa_datttt 7»T.O/OIU.LnVrDfiWrtLCILAUIDHUILAV .LT.T.I I I rfihuCCZ I 070.3.Ln7rDhWCCPVT. Oadz nooaono .HiJu rr : o m o z o m H H o z a m :ofipomm a .s..a Hoocom nmfim noficsw caspfic; APPENDIX C SUMMARY TABLE 141 142 SUMMARY TABLE Jr. High Scho 01 Grade 7 Section. 5 Matrix CTMM Sociometric Total Table Name Total P* Criteria Choices S** Number IQ C I II III Received C 1 Constance A. 102 A 3 3 4 10 A 2 Sharon B. 104 A 2 4 9 15 S 3 Charlene c. 106 A 6 5 2 13 AA 4 Catherine D. 111 A 3 6 7 16 S 5 Kathy E. 88 A O O O O I 6 Carla H. 80 SL 3 1 2 6 BA 7 Sylvia H. 108 A 2 5 2 9 A 8 Lindsay J. 127 RL 4 4 2 10 A 9 Georgina M. 94 A O l 0 1 N 10 Victoria N. 115 RL 4 2 4 10 A 11 Margaret 0. 97 A 2 2 1 5 BA 12 Connie S. 113 A 2 2 2 6 BA 13 Sally S. 132 G 6 5 8 19 S 14 Trudy T. 112 A l l 1 3 N 15 Marilynn Z. 115 RL 7 5 6 18 S 16 Arthur A. 112 A 7 7 5 19 s 17 Steven B. 123 RL 0 O O O I 18 Daniel E. 115 RL 0 O O O I 19 Bruce E. 100 A 2 3 O 5 BA 20 Randolph H. 117 RL 1 1 2 4 BA 21 Thomas H. 107 A 2 1 2 5 BA 22 Randolph K. 109 A 4 3 1 8 A 23 Al K. 113 A 1 O l 2 N 24 Alan K. 118 RL 1 l l 3 N 25 Russell P. 100 A 5 6 u 15 s 26 Gregory P. 122 RL 12 l2 12 36 S 27 James R. 100 A O 2 2 4 BA 28 Russell 8. 98 A 4 2 5 11 A 29 Charles S. 109 A O O O O I 30 Harold S. 115 RL 7 7 5 19 S 31 Paul V. 114 A 2 3 3 8 A 32 Hugh w. 104 A 3 2 3 8 A * Psychometric Category ** Sociometric Category HG Highly Gifted S Star G Gifted AA Above Average RL Rapid Learner A Average A Average BA Below Average SL Slow Learner N Neglectee E Educable I Isolate APPENDIX D MUTUAL CHOICE TABLE 143 ..v~.~v./\.T u.~...&~.v-r~ .~<...~..:\< 1AA .HH.H < - < w -w a - < moa-:ofi mm-mm HHH.HH am - a a -w a - a ooH-eHH sm-am HHH.HH.H m - m mm-ma am - am mma-mHH mm-om .H m - m malma a - am ooH-mHH mm-om HHH.HH.H m - m ma-ma a - gm maalmaa ma-om .HH.H am - a m -HH < u a soa-mm Hm-mm .HH.H an - m m -mm < - am ooH-mmH ma-mm HHH.HH.H m - m ma-mm a - am mHH-mmH ma-mm HHH.HH m - m mfl-ma a - < HHH-ooH :-mm .HH m l m mH-mH < - a :oH-QOH m-mm HHH.HH.H am - < a -m am - a sHH-moa om-mm .H flmomm mmofloco moflpowmpmo mGH manESZ Sofipmpflho OHmeEOHoOW .HmQESZ HmpOB owflpwEOSOKAmm HmpOB mHQmE 2296 xapemz m searemm s 66666 Hoocem swam mqmde MOHomo Q< mmmtm>< mmmpo>< 666m zoaom o>op< mmflpomopmo QHmeEoHoom 0H mamom Hmeoe mmmoom 0H md<©m Q< mwmpo>< owmnm>< mem OH popesz onmm o>on< mHmom Hmuoe Hmpoe mmHLommumO OHLHOEoHoom UOSCHHCOOIIM XHQZMmm< 148 OH O m a O O O OO OH O O O a H O OO Om H O O O O a O OO O O a O O O OO mm a O O O O OO mm O O O O H O OO :O O O 3 O O O HO :O O a O O O O OO 3 O O OH i O O OO O: H OH OH OH O O HO O: O O O OH O O OO OO O O O O a O OO O: O O OH OH 3 3 NO HO O O HH OH O O OO OO 2 OH OH OH O OH OO OO O HH OH OH O OH OOH OO O O OH OH O O HOH HO O OH O OH O OH mOH :O O HH HH mm a HH OOH ON H OH OH HO OH HH OOH OO 2 O O :H O HH OOH OO O OH OH mm O O OOH OO O OH OH OO O OH OOH ON OH O OO OO O NH OOH O O H Hm OO O O. OOH mpcmcspm OOOHOOH OOOOmHOOz OOOLO>< OOOLO>< OOOLO>< LOHO OH LOQEOZ onOm O>on< OHOOm Hmpoe HOOOB OOHLQOOHOO OHmeEoHOom OOOCHHCOOIIm XHQZWOO< 149 OOOO CUP—IN mow—4 KOCJJ-fi'r—l \OCU-ZI'J‘r—i OH OH 3H OH 3m OOO mm r-{r—i (\lr—i (UH—T H3O OHO OOO MHHN ,_{ .II‘ HJF—if—i MMQ'J'CU O\L(\-:I"—:TL(\ SCH-3010} OOO as: mHeOOH OO OO H HO OO H OO (\Jr—i rHrAH (I) N OCH—{H mmH O CO OOCOOOOO LOOEOZ Hmpoe OOOHoOH OOOOOHOOZ OOOLO>< OOOLO>< onOm OOOLO>< LOHO OH O>on< OHOOO OOHLQOOOOO OHLOOEoHooO HOHOB OOSCHHCOOulm XHQZMOO< APPENDIX F FREQUENCY DISTRIBUTION OF STUDENTS BY TOTAL NUMBER OF CHOICES RECEIVED 150 151 Hoar—{HMO} HLflfflq U\L(\O\CD\O OH OH OH OH OH Om Hm mm Om am Om Om Om Om Om OO HO mO H OO 2O OO H OO NO OO OO O: H H: H H(flr4 .: Hrnc3:- madam:: :J\e4 U\Lr\ HAOJmcm cu11\e4.—+Cu:1\cu:: cu #— ,._.| .H odwuoradno:rU\OwH:rdi HrHr4 HlH HHHPemHqummmmmm.q-L\ r-{n—i v—l macmOaOO LOQEOZ Hares OOOLOOOO LOCLOOH OOOLO>< LOCLOOH OOHOHO OOHHHO OOOHoQO memoscm SOHO OHQOO OHSOHO OHLHOE :oHOom mmHhoOOHOO OHLHOEozomOO LOOEOZ OO>HOOOO OOOHOOO mo mmmzaz H< LOCLOOH OOOOHO OOHOHO OOOHQOO LOQEOZ OHQOOOOm onO OHOOO OHanm OHLHOE HOOOE :oHOoO OOHLoOOHOO OHLOOEogohOO prejz lll’.‘II|I OOOCHHCOOIum XHQZMOO< "llllllllllllllll“