“‘— L‘ 'm'»: r s I 4H1”:- . 11313.1 «3 _ 3’61. r14 | "t": v" ' _ l" 11"! 11" W"""’"'7F' x’ii r, 40‘", " ... "V‘W 1SP11'l'iankJ *4 ',‘."\'1;. 11.215“ x 31%, “E. M; ’ WA. , _.-.—> t. 13‘ )5. u "111‘; . .111 .~\ imam“ : 1.... <1 '0‘ ‘ul ‘ _‘ ”-1 M Tab 1,; an A, .4 V . ,fis‘H-fifi rvo .J '_ .“ L‘ 7‘. '4 ‘ ”er 'u'M , “-"1-12 . ‘ h ‘ L‘anu L ‘3. .. LF.,.U§:3L:. r- 3'.'}."1‘ mlu4' . "qu‘o .;:51 “fight. f‘f\:.t;' W lflllllllllllflflllllllfllllllflllllfllflfljlll \ This is to certify that the thesis entitled Selected Factors Influencing Perceptions of Dress And Attributions in Occupational Story-Situations presented by Mary Elizabeth Cope has been accepted. towards fulfillment of the requirements for Masters—Jame in Llothingmd Textiles V / Major professor Date W 0-7639 $543 1 ;.;gfi\s 7. . ‘1 "may" J E: (7 (‘13; $9 g3“) 15‘ , 1 MAR 2 71999 @1751? Michigan State niversiry an ~ AR Y ‘i‘ w: 25¢ per day per item RETURNING LIBRARY MATERIALS: Place in book return to remove charge I" fl APR 13 199 from circulation records SELECTED FACTORS INFLUENCING PERCEPTIONS OF DRESS AND ATTRIBUTIONS IN OCCUPATIONAL STORY-SITUATIONS By Mary Elizabeth Cope A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Human Environment and Design 1980 ABSTRACT SELECTED FACTORS INFLUENCING PERCEPTIONS OR DRESS AND ATTRIBUTIONS IN OCCUPATIONAL STORY-SITUATIONS By Mary Elizabeth Cope The object of this study was to examine the effects of selected occupational experiences on an individual's perceptions of dress and attributions in hypothetical story-situations. The sample composed of husband/wife pairs, with at least one school age child residing with them, consisted of 214 men and 222 women. The data were collected in a self-administered questionnaire which included two occupational story-situations revolving around a main character and his/her appearance and clothing. Discriminant function analysis revealed that personal clothing attitudes, occupational appearance requirements, occupational prestige and age were significant factors influencing perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency and attributions to key person and situation. Although, additional analysis did not support relationships be- tween the two variables of employment status and three occupational classifications and perceptions of appear- ance/clothing saliency or attributions in hypothetical story-situations. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author wishes to acknowledge several individuals who provided the guidance and encouragement that is necessary for such an undertaking.,to Dr. Ann Slocum, my advisor, for her continual assistance throughout this entire project., to my committee members Dr. Witter, Dr. Sontag and Dr. Miller for their interest and helpful suggestions., to Jan Vredevoogd for the countless hours she spent as my statistical consultant., to fellow grad- uate students and members of the Quality of Life Research Team for their assistance in data coding., and especially to my mother and father who provided me with the incentive to succeed in my endeavours. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES Chapter 1. Introduction to the Study Statement of the Problem Definition of Terms Hypotheses Assumptions Limitations Review of Literature 2. Conceptual Framework Summary 3. Methodology Description of the Instrument Data Collection Description of the Story-Situations Description of Measures Comparisons of Respondents by Set-Type Hypotheses Testing 4. Results and Discussion Description of the Sample Comparison of Story-Situations Testing of Saliency and Attribution Hypotheses for Men and Women 5. Summary and Conclusions Conclusions by Major Hypotheses Discussion of Results Limitations of the Findings Suggestions for Further Study APPENDICES A. Portion of Quality of Life Questionnaire Used in this Study iii 98 Page B. Interviewer Procedures and Forms Used in the Field 107 C. Coding Procedures for Selected Responses to Story-Situations 112 D. Coding Procedure for Formality of Occupational Dress 115 BIBLIOGRAPHY ll6 iv TABLE 1. 2 3 4. 5 6 7 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. LIST OF TABLES Description of the Questionnaire Sets Age of the Respondents Educational Background of Respondents Employment Status of Respondents Classification of Main Occupation 1977 Income of Respondents Appearance/Clothing Saliency Dimension for the Two Story-Situations Number of Attributions in Each Story- Situation Pearson Product Moment Correlations of Independent Variables for Men Pearson Product Moment Correlations of Independent Variables for Employed Women Discriminant Analysis of Appearance/ Clothing Saliency for Men in Construction Worker Story-Situation Discriminant Analysis of Appearance/ Clothing Saliency for Women in Construction Worker Story-Situation Discriminant Analysis of Appearance/ Clothing Saliency for Men in Typist Story- Situation Discriminant Analysis of Attributions to Situation for Men in the Construction Worker Story-Situation Page 28 44 45 46 47 SO 51 56 S8 61 64 67 71 TABLE Page 15. Discriminant Analysis of Attributions to Situation for Women in Construction Worker Story-Situation 73 16. Discriminant Analysis of Attributions to Typist for Men in Typist Story-Situation 76 17. Discriminant Analysis of Attributions to Situation for Men in Typist Story-Situation 78 18. Discriminant Analysis of Attributions to Situation for Women in Typist Story-Situation 79 19. Spearman Rank Order Correlations of Occupation and Dependent Variables 82 20. Summary of Discriminant Analysis 87 vi CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY For centuries, individuals have understood the importance of acquiring a skill or trade that could be- come their occupation. In our modern world, with today's social mobility, people can aspire to almost any career or professional endeavour. The choice is left open for people to decide for themselves. Career objectives are usually set during the later years of formal schooling or thereafter. Learning an occupation is one of the greatest demands on an individual as s/he becomes an adult. (Clausen, 1968). The experiences of a child cannot prepare her/him for all of her/his adult roles. Brim (1966, p. 18) stated that society demands that the individual meet these changed expectations, and demands that he alter his personality and be- havior to make room in his life for newly significant persons such as his family members, his teachers, his employers and his colleagues at work. Socialization itself involves the acquisition of knowledge, skills and dispositions that prepare indi- viduals to function as members of a society. The pro- cess of occupational socialization begins as a person 2 commences formal job preparation, such as coursework or internships. Occupational socialization involves the passage of an individual from outsider to newcomer to insider within a company. A series of stages appear to exist beginning with recruitment and entry and concluding with acceptance of the newcomer. "One way organizations can match newcomers with the work environment is through socialization, such as changing the person's role expec- tations, skills, or motivation” (Wanous, 1980, p. 190). In the occupational setting, the worker acquires standards that help to define appropriate behavior. As the worker passes through the socialization process, appropriate norms are internalized. Along with other employees s/he begins to share a set of attitudes and beliefs, which when combined form the worker's occupational identity. . Occupations are performed in a social context that is characterized by both general norms, common to the world of work, and more particular rules of conduct applicable to distinct occupa- tions or occupational categories. (Moore, 1969, p. 887). Specific dress expectations constitute one of the norms internalized in the socialization process. Within each occupation, there are written or understood rules concerning the apprOpriate mode of dress. As employees learn their occupational role, they become aware of the appearance requirements expected by members of their occupation. Distinct levels of formality concerning 3 clothing and appearance become apparent to the indi- vidual. Employees realize what modes of dress are acceptable within their occupational environment, whether it is white or blue collar. Throughout this process, people develop attitudes toward occupational dress. Employees connect a mode of dress with role expectations for that position. Statement of the Problem This study focuses on examining the effects of the experience of an occupation on an individual's percep- tions and the attributions s/he would make in an occupa- tional setting. More specifically, it centers on examining whether an individual's perceptions and attri- butions are related to occupational appearance require- ments and formality of occupational dress. It revolves around two occupational story-situations in which the respondent is presented with a character dressed in both normal and deviant work attire. It is felt that the importance of clothing as per- ceived by the respondents and their reactions to its use in the situations is due in part to their own dress expectations. Occupational socialization introduces these formal and informal rules regarding appearance and clothing. This study will contribute to our present knowledge of the perceptions of clothing in the 4 occupational setting. It will increase our comprehension of person perception and attribution theory as it relates to appearance and clothing. It will provide a basis for understanding several of the factors that influence the observer's perceptions and attributions. The objectives of this study were to: 1. Examine the perceptions and attributions of men and women upon exposure to selected occupational story- situations. 2. Examine the selected factors affecting an individual's perceptions of clothing in the story- situations. 3. Examine the selected factors affecting an individual's attributions in the story-situations. Definitions of Terms Attributions - the act of assigning to an individual, group or the environment; an explanation for a person's behavior after exposure to descriptions of that person's actions. In this study, the attributions are classified as those directed toward the key person (self), which are internal, and those directed toward others and the situation. These are external. Attributions often take the form of dispositions, personality traits and abilities. 5 Person Perception - the manner in which an indi- vidual views and judges another individual and his behavior within a situation. Causal - the assignment of responsibility for an act of behavior. Personal Clothing Constructs - an individual's own views and attitudes toward dress eXpectations in the occupational setting. Formality of Occppational Dress — an individual's own attire on a normal work day, measured by the level of formality of the clothing. Occupational Appearance Requirements - the number of appearance requirements expected by the employer. Appearance/Clothing Saliency - this refers to the degree of importance an individual attaches to appearance and clothing in the story-situation. Occupation - the type of employment in which the respondent is currently engaged. Employment Status - this refers to whether the respondent is currently employed in an occupation, in- cluding those on temporary layoffs, strikes or sick leave. Hypotheses and Research Question 1. An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress, and occupational 6 appearance requirements will influence his/her per- ceptions of the saliency of appearance and clothing in the story-situation. 2. An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appear- ance requirements will influence his/her attributions in the story-situations. 3. An individual's occupation will influence his/ her perceptions and attributions in the story-situation. 4. A female individual's employment status will influence her perceptions and attributions in the story- situations. Additionally, the following research question was asked. 1. Will the demographic variables, age, educational level, personal income, occupational prestige and wearing of a uniform influence an individual's perceptions of dress and his/her attributions in the occupational story- situations. Assumptions 1. It is assumed that the respondents took the role of the character when directed to do so in each story- situation. 2. It is assumed that the respondent was freely motivated to express thoughtful accurate responses. 7 Limitations l. The brevity of the responses limited interpre- tations of the data. Additional information might have been obtained through probing if the data had been collected by interview. 2. The views of the respondent may have been in- fluenced by previous mentions of appearance and clothing within the questionnaire. Review of the Literature This portion of the chapter contains a review of relevant research concerningthe influepge of dress in perception and impression formation and also the influence of dress in the occupational setting. Percgption and Dress Research findings by Douty (1963) indicate that clothing can be viewed as an intimate part of a person's perceptual field with a potential for affecting impressions of the person. A person's clothing can give the viewer an immediate impression in an initial encounter or throughout interaction. Hamid (1969) was concerned with the effects of clothing in impression formation. He felt that dress functioned as cues for the classification or categoriza- tion of individuals. It was thought that actions and activities can be attributed to people in different modes 8 of dress. Hamid was attempting to determine whether variances in the perceptions of others were affected by the clothing worn. He found that dress effects depended on sex stereotypes and that dress is an important cue in sex stereotyping. In a later study, Hamid (1972) reports that dress acts as a stimulus cue for role differentiation and personality typing. His study examined the effects of dress manipulations on judgments. Facial makeup and glasses were manipulated to test their effects on obser- vational accuracy, perception and impression formation. Female observers were found to have greater accuracy than men in terms of their perceptions. This was thought to be due to a greater cue dependency exhibited by females. Men, on the other hand, exhibited extreme differences in ratings. One interesting finding was that only a few subjects mentioned glasses, and no one referred to the makeup as influencing factors in their impressions. Ob- servers failed to recognize that these dress cues aided in the formation of their impressions. The power of dress recently received more prominence when John T. Molloy published his books, Dress for Success and The Woman's Dress for Success Book. According to Molloy, the way we dress has remarkable impact on the people we meet professionally or socially and greatly affects how they treat us. (Molloy, 1975). A study by 9 Conner, Peters and Nagasawa (1975) stresses the impor- tance of clothing in the formation of first impressions. They found that clothing has a greater effect on the formation of social impressions that the person does. The influence of clothing style differences on impressions of sociability were studied by Johnson, Nagasawa and Peters (1977). The subjects, male and female college students, were presented with photo- graphs of a female in costumes classified as in-fashion and out-of-fashion. Their findings indicate that clothing style had a significant influence on the impression of sociability. In-fashion styles of clothing created strong impressions of sociability. A study by Buckley and Roach (1974) examined the use of clothing as a communicator of social and political attitudes. They found that the type of clothing worn by an individual can symbolize his social and political attitudes. Subjects reported preferring clothing that they perceived to communicate social and political attitudes much like their own; and they indicated that they would feel good in this type of clothing./ Dress in the Occupational Setting Kelley, Good, and Walter (1974) studied the rela- tionship between adolescent's perceptions of appearance 10 and dress and the role they play in occupations. They found that adolescents were aware of the practical functions of clothing, as well as its use as an instru- ment to be manipulated in impressions. Even as adoles- cents, they were aware of the general types of clothing suitable for each occupation. Wood (1977) formulated several theories about uni- forms and role relationships. ”Uniforms thus relates the persons to each other and defines appropriate ways of behaving: it also differentiates the wearer from others" (Wood, 1977, p. 143). In day-to-day interaction, one finds that if a uniform is worn, persons act towards him on the basis of the actions they think appropriate to the role signified by the uniform, rather than on the basis of personal characteristics. Riemer (1977) studied the occupational socialization of journeymen electricians. He examined the manner in which newcomers tended to adopt the use of tools, costumes and electrical jargon. Basi- cally, newcomers tended to carry new tools, and usually more tools than were necessary on the job. As they gained experience they only carried the tools necessary for each job. The relevance of clothing in an occupational setting was originally studied by Form and Stone (1955). They assert that 11 even in occupations where clothing is thought to have only a functional role, the way one dresses may have a far reaching effect on his future even though he is totally unaware of these consequences (Form and Stone, 1955, p. 7). Members of different professions were found to have diverse attitudes toward the use of occupational dress. Form and Stone state that manual workers and office personnel view clothing as having different purposes. Manual workers were thought to use clothing for protection, while office personnel perceived clothing as a symbol to be manipulated to convey impressions. Workers in different occupational groups also tended to display conformity to different dress expectations. If norms governing work dress are violated, white collar workers are more concerned with responses of audiences which are large, impersonal and loosely organized; while manual workers are most concerned with the response of their immediate work group (Form and Stone, 1955, p. 35). It would appear that white collar workers use occupa- tional clothing to project images to the general public and clientele, etc. They also found that those workers who attach little importance to their clothing will expect others to attach the same importance to clothing. (Form and Stone, 1955). The Form and Stone study (1955) was the first to utilize the story-situation of the typist to examine 12 individuals reactions to occupational dress. They asked the respondents to analyze the actions of Elsie, the typist, her work associates and the office manager. They found that those who place low importance on clothing are more inclined to blame the office manager for the condition, while those who feel clothes are important blame the other workers for not dressing better (Form and Stone, 1955, p. 45). In a study of the influence of clothing on hiring agents' judgments of an applicant, Godfrey (1965) found that male and female hiring agents respond differently to the influence of clothing in judging an applicant. An individual's external personal qualities, which in- cluded appearance and clothing, were the second most important job hiring criterion. In another study by Jones (1972) hiring agents' perceptions of appearance norms were examined. She looked at the effects of these perceptions in the interview and the daily work situation. Jones concluded that hiring agents consider skills, job experience, personality and appearance in their evaluation of applicants during the interview. She also found that some individual respondents used appearance as an indi- cator of general ability to handle the job. A later study by Nelson (1975) investigated differences in the executive secretaries' perceptions of the importance 13 their occupational clothing plays in their job per- formances and the appearance norms which they believe to be appropriate for their occupational roles. Nelson found that the majority of executive secretaries be- lieved that adherence to appearance norms was a factor in job retention and promotion. A recent study by Ketch (1979) examined men's per- ceptions of sex roles and dress in a hypothetical occupa- tional setting. It also revolved around a character dressed out-of-role. This study revealed that as clothing became more salient for an individual, the approval of the character in out-of—role dress in the situation de- creased. An increase in clothing saliency was associated with increasing negative sanctions toward the character dressed out-of-role. Ketch concluded that further analysis of appearance and clothing in the occupational setting could be done utilizing experiential variables such as attitudinal clothing variables. Summar; Clofhing appears to function as a perceptual cue thereby affecting impressions. It can signal sociability, or social and political attitudes. Clothing affects the way people treat us when it acts as a stimulus for the categorization of an individual. Observers are not l4 always aware of clothing as a perceptual cue, although perhaps women are more aware of it than men. Occupational clothing is used as tool to differen- tiate workers, socialize individuals and define roles. Members of different occupations use clothing in differ- ent manners due to the dress expectations of their position. Several studies emphasized the importance of clothing in the interview situation. Appearance and clothing seem to be considered important in terms of job hiring criteria. Clothing was perceived by some to be a factor in job retention and promotion. Therefore, it can be concluded that appearancetand clothing affect perceptions of an individual and the actions of others toward that individual. CHAPTER 2 CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK In order to develop the conceptual framework it becomes necessary to xplain how an individual makes sense of the actions of others. How do people make judgments concerning the behaviors of the characters in the story-situations? What factors within observers influence their understanding of the situation? Social perception and attribution theory were chosen as the background for explaining these phenomena. Early work in attribution theory and person per- ception can be traced back to Fritz Heider (1958). Heider believed that direct impressions of another person lead to the assignment of dispositional characteristics. To Heider, dispositional characteristics are stable be- haviors that make the world more predictable and control- lable. According to Secord and Backman (1964) even though a perceiver does not actually witness an individual's actions, s/he can frequently infer an underlying dis- position to the individual from knowledge to the effects of the act. Perceivers may seek a single salient 15 l6 explanation for an act they have observed. Part of the explanation may include assigning traits or characteristics to an individual with regard to the observed act. In this case, the perceiver reads into the cues drawing his own inferences about the person and the situation as presented. In addition, it is clear that people can arrive at some evaluation of another person from almost any data, and that they do so with a high degree of consensus. (Taguiri, 1969). When faced with such minimal information as a few descriptive words, a photographed appearance, or expressive gesture and instructed to come up with an impression, the perceiver is likely to use reason and imagination in order to satisfy the xperimenter. (Secord and Backman, 1964). Different responses from perceivers are the result of their variety of backgrounds and experiences. In day-to-day interaction, one must assess peOple and respond to them. The judgment process of assessment involves perception of the individual and his behavior. In brief meetings, the perceiver doesn't have sufficient time to encode all the elements of the stimulus person. S/he tends to group a few traits and places the individual in a category. In some instances, the perceiver is presented with the categorical information of the person's occupation. Since the other information is minimal, such l7 knowledge will strongly affect her/his perceptions. (Secord and Backman, 1964). Perceivers are likely to attempt to stereotype persons by assigning them member- ship in categories. Individuals hold expectations re- garding peoples attitudes and behavior depending on their position and role. Secord and Backman note that the role assumed will affect the categorization and the traits attributed. We expect the individual to possess and exhibit all the attributes belonging to that category. In early experiences, an individual learns what to associate with each category. A perceiver can rely on his own experiences in judging another's states and intentions. (Taguiri, 1969). Strong prior attitudes guide an individual as s/he attempts to provide standards or frames of reference for understanding the world. Therefore, attitudes directly affect an observer's per- ceptions and attributions toward an individual. Attitudes are derived from various factors. Per- sonal experience appears to have some influence on their developments. Hollander (1971) contends that attitudes are acquired through socialization and reflect cultural and societal influences. Attitudes are learned and remain with an individual as a direct result of previous social interactions. Hollander states that attitudes "help to account for individual differences in reactions 18 to similar circumstances" (Hollander, 1971, p. 148). According to Fishbein (1973) we learn to associate many different characteristics, qualities and attributes with a given object and each of these beliefs affect our over- all feeling about an object. It is thought that only a few salient beliefs serve as primary determinants of an attitude. It is supposed that the individual develops attitudes toward occupational clothing at the stage when s/he inter- nalizes job norms in the socialization process. This process appears to begin formally when an individual starts his first job training. An individual's own occupational dress and that of his co-workers provide a basis that is utilized for understanding the appropri- ateness of certain modes of dress. The occupational dress of an employee as guided by dress codes or normative expectations, constitutes por- tions of that individual's experiences with clothing. These experiences contribute to the development of atti- tudes that a person holds toward appearance and clothing in the occupational setting. Attitudes act as motivating forces to exert control on a person's actions and behavior. According to McGuire (1969), attitudinal selectivity is imposed on perceptions, determining the manner in which a stimulus situation is l9 labelled by an individual. He divides an attitude into the following three components; the perceptual, affective and action component. The perceptual element refers to the stereotype a person holds. The importance or saliency of a stimulus cue will alter the manner in which it is perceived. The affective component deals with liking or disliking the object that is being perceived. The action or behavioral portion refers to the individual's behavior toward the object. (McGuire, 1969). Therefore, an attitude influences the observer's perceptions and liking of a stimulus. It also predisposes the individual to act in a certain manner. Specific attitudes toward dress could influence the observer's perceptions and liking of another person. In the occupa- tional setting, it could influence the behavior of the observer and ultimately his interaction with the stimulus person. Appropriate modes of dress and conformity with role expectations would induce liking and positive interaction. Unfavourable dress could induce dislike and negative interaction. Perceptions of the cause of an action appear to depend on factors within the person and factors within the environment. (Heider, 1958). ‘When the perceiver places blame directly on the person acting, s/he is making an attribution to that person. This is an internal 20 attribution. When blame is placed on other individuals in direct contact with the actor (individual), the perceiver attributes the results to the other. In other cases, the situation/environment is seen as the cause. The latter two cases are labelled as external attributions. According to Newcomb (1965), properties of other persons are not perceived in a vacuum but rather in a context that includes the environmental situation. When a person's behavior conforms with social expec- tations, we tend to regard disruptions as externally caused. When a behavior departs from normative expecta- tions, the cause is attributed to motivational forces within that person. (Jones, Davis, Gergen, 1971). When an observer views a departure from a role, he judges that sample of behavior against a background of role specifications. The perceiver makes attempts to understand why the person deviated from the expected role. According to Newcomb (1958), the perceptual inter- action process between perceivers includes the judging by the observer of the attitudes of the observed. We weight other's attitudes in terms of our own attitudes. In the story-situation, the respondent is presented with characters in normal and out-of—role dress. S/he perceives the character's own attitudes toward clothing. 21 Then, s/he judges that character based on the character's dress, and the situation. The basis for the judgment could be supplied by the respondent's own attitudes or expectations toward appearance and clothing in the story- situation. The respondent probably has some idea of how a construction worker, or typist should be dressed. When the character in the story-situation violates these expectations, attributions are made towards that character. When the actions of the character are not perceived as violations, attributions may be made to others or to the situation. Summary It is clear that perceivers make attempts to pro- vide explanations for the actions of others. These actions or behaviors can be viewed in a variety of situations such as in a photograph, or a normal everyday situation. Information about a behavior can even be provided by a few descriptive words. From this infor- mation perceivers form impressions of another individual. There appears to be a tendency to group the traits of an individual and place that individual into a category. The categorical information could be an occupation, nationality, religion, etc. 22 People as perceivers hold attitudes and expecta- tions toward that categorical individual and his role. These attitudes provide standards for judgment of an individual's behavior. They appear to influence the perceiver's categorization or stereotyping, likes or dislikes and behavior toward an individual. An additional factor affecting the situation of perception and attribution appears to be the locus of causation. If the person acting is perceived as the cause of the behavior, traits or dispositions are attributed to him. Significant others and the situa- tion may also be viewed as factors causing the behavior. CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY This study is part of an ongoing research project on Quality of Life, carried out by faculty and students in the College of Human Ecology at Michigan State Uni- versity. The project is a research effort of the Depart- ment of Human Environment and Design and the Department of Family and Child Science. It is directed by Dr. Ann Slocum and Dr. Margaret Bubolz. The project was funded by the Michigan State University Agricultural Experiment Station and the University of Minnesota.1 This research project was designed to examine an individual's per- ceived quality of life with emphasis on clothing and family living. Description of the Instrument A self-administered questionnaire was utilized as the data collection instrument. Five occupational story- situations formed the basis for the portion of the study reported here. Each story revolved around an actor 1Michigan Agriculture Experiment Station Project #1249, "Clothing Use and Quality of Life in Rural and Urban Communities”, Project #3151, "Families in Evolving Rural Communities", MSU ORD Grant #21347. 23 24 in an occupational setting. His/her appearance and clothing were assumed to deviate from the appropriate mode of dress. The respondents read each story and answered a series of short open-ended questions. (Appendix A). A pretest was carried out in October, 1977, in the Michigan Counties of Ingham and Oakland. Areas within these counties were designated as the pretest sites and streets were selected randomly in these areas. Eligible participants were husband and wife couples living together with at least one child age 5 to 18 in the household. Graduate students on the research team acted as interviewers. They contacted households and determined whether the couples met the criteria for eligibility. Signed consent forms were required as an indicator of the willing participation of each individual. (Appendix B). Each husband and wife received separate questionnaires. Twenty sets of questionnaires were placed in eligible households. Of those twenty, eighteen sets were completed and retrieved. Each couple was then paid ten dollars for their efforts. Included in the pretest were three open ended story- situations. Three additional story-situations were 25 pretested separately by a member of the research team. Eleven people read only three story-situations and responded to the appropriate questions. In the final instrument, one story-situation was dropped bringing the total to five. After the pretest, it was decided that respondents should be allowed to seal responses after completion. Therefore, large manilla envelopes were included with each questionnaire to insure privacy. Data Collection The site of data collection was Oakland County, Michigan. This county consists of relatively rural and metropolitan areas. The sample included minority groups. A relatively high income level was required to insure that the respondents had the ability to complete the questionnaire. A nationally known agency was employed to obtain the sample and distribute the question- naire in eligible households. Before data collection began, the research agency and the directors of the pro- ject explained the questionnaire to the interviewer and specified the procedures s/he was to follow. The sample for the research project was drawn from three census tracts frames in Oakland County. The frames represented areas that were rural, suburban and 26 urban, the latter with a concentration of a minority group. Pooling of respondents from these three census tract frames limited generalizations to this sample. Those census tracts that met the requirements of a $12,000 median income in 1970 were listed within the three areas and were ordered by number of occupied dwelling units. To insure enough participants, the income requirement was lowered below $12,000 in one area. Systematically from the lists of occupied house- holds, seventy-five sampling points were chosen. Addi- tional blocks were included to insure a large enough area to meet the requirements of four households per cluster. The original households were randomly chosen as designated beginning points for the researcher. Every fifth one from the original was to be contacted. If the occupant was not eligible, was not at home or would not participate, substitutions were made to the left and right. A systematic walk pattern was used. The eligibility requirements for the study were the same as for the pretest. The households had to be composed of a husband and wife with one or more school age (5-18) children residing with them in the home. Eligibility was determined by the interviewers before placement of the questionnaire. A signature 27 on the consent form was required by one of the pair when the questionnaire was delivered, and both were to sign by pick up time. All responses remained confi- dential. Questionnaires were left to be completed for pick up by the interviewer in several days. Envelopes were provided to seal responses for privacy. Each couple who completed their questionnaires received ten dollars. Data collection began November 17, 1977, and was halted in March, 1978, due to limited time and other constraints. Within a cluster area two types of sets were placed alternately. All sets included story-situations in- volving a construction worker, lawyer, typist, foreman, and school teacher. The sex of the character in the latter four story-situations was varied to produce the two set-types as shown in Table l. Husband and wife pairs always received the same set. Table l--Description of the Questionnaire Sets 28 Main Character Occupation Dress Set Carol Construction skirt and a, Worker blouse Mr. Drake Lawyer faded sport a shirt, unpressed pants Ms. Drake Lawyer faded shirt, b unpressed slacks Ann Typist well dressed a Bob Typist well dressed b John Division work clothes a Head Sue Division work clothes b Head Nancy High School revealing a Teacher braless tops Paul High School shirt open to b Teacher waist, tight slacks For this study, responses were examined from the two story-situations involving Carol the construction worker and Bob/Ann the typist. The first story- situation dealt with a female applying for employment on a construction crew. She was the only female to apply, and was also described as wearing a skirt and blouse. No mention was made of the clothing worn by 29 the other male applicants. The following paragraph represents the story-situation as presented to all respondents. Carol read that a local company was hiring workers for their construction crews. Since she had several years experience, she felt confident that she would get a job. After making an appoint- ment for an interview, she arrived at the personnel office wearing a skirt and blouse and was surprised to see that she was the only female in the roomful of applicants. Carol felt that her interview with the personnel director had gone well and was cer- tain that she would be hired. The following day she received a phone call and was told that all the positions on the construction crews had been filled. After the respondents had read the story they answered these questions. ”How would have felt if you were Carol?” ”Why do you think that she was not hired?" "Other comments.” The other story-situation involved Bob/Ann, the typist. This story had several differences from the first. In this case, half of the husband/wife pairs received Set A with Ann as the key person. The other half of the respondents received Set B with Bob as the key person. In the typist situation, the office manager and co-workers are introduced as significant others. Appearance/clothing is stressed several times. It is mentioned that "Bob/Ann liked to wear new good clothes, spent most of his/her money on clothes, and was the best dressed person in the office." The paragraph below was presented to the respondents. 30 Bob/Ann got a job working as a typist in an office. At first s/he got along well with the other people. S/he liked to wear good, new clothes to the office. As a result, s/he spent most of her/his salary on clothes and was the best dressed person in the office. After a short time, Ann was promoted to the job of receptionist, a job that some of the older people wanted. They complained to the office manager. He told them that Bob/Ann was given the job because s/he was always so well dressed, and that it was important to have someone at that job who would make a good impression on:the public. After the respondents had read the typist story- situation they answered these questions. "What do you think of the office manager who promoted Ann because of her appearance?" "me? H "Whatdo you think of Ann's use of clothing to get ahead on the job?" I'Why? H "How do you feel about her co-workers and their response to her promotion?" "Other comments." Data Coding The codes for the open-ended story-situations were initially developed by Dr. Gloria Williams of the University of Minnesota. Ninety individual cases were examined to evaluate emerging patterns within the range of responses. In the coding of each situation, the entire response was read before the data were coded. Any 31 responses outside the realm of the codes were recorded on cards. These cards were grouped into categories and additional codes were developed when appropriate. A collusion variable was developed to attempt to insure the independence of responses between the husband and wife pairs. The coders examined the responses for similarities in content and handwriting. The responses were assigned a rating of no evidence of collusion, possible, or probable collusion. For the purposes of this study, all cases of possible and probable collusion were dropped from the sample. Data for story-situation one involving Carol, the construction worker, were coded in two parts. The husband's responses were coded by three graduate students, two from Michigan State University and one from the University of Minnesota. Initially, the coders were trained by Dr. Slocum and Dr. Williams. The first ten questionnaires were coded together by the group. Every fifth case that followed was check coded for reliability for the first thirty cases and problems were check coded as they appeared. The wives' responses were coded by a graduate student and staff member from Michigan State University. Every fifth case and additional problem case were check coded. 32 The other story-stiuation to be analyzed involved Bob/Ann, the typist. The husbands' and wives' respones were coded by two graduate students, one from Michigan State University and one from the University of Minnesota. Check coding was completed by a professional assisting with the project. Approximately 10 percent of the cases were checked including the problem cases. The occupational clothing variables of formality and appearance requirements were coded by a graduate student from Michigan State University. Approximately one-fifth of the cases were check coded by the project director. The personal clothing constructs and demographics were coded by a group of graduate and undergraduate students working with the project. Again, every fifth case was check coded. Description of Measures Appearance/Clothing Saliency Dimension A five point measure was utilized to categorize the importance of appearance and clothing as based on their responses to such questions as "Why do you think that she was not hired?," and "What do you think of Ann's use of clothing to get ahead on the job?” If clothing appeared to be a factor in their responses, it was coded as being salient to the individual. If their 33 response was followed by additional comments and reasons stressing the importance, it was considered salient qualified. Those responses which stated that clothing was not important were coded not salient. Again, additional support for the lack of importance of clothing resulted in a code of not salient qualified. Responses which included reports of importance and lack of importance were considered mixed or ambivalent. If clothing was not reported by the respondent, it was coded as not mentioned. This measure was collapsed into two categories for the analysis. The category of appearance/clothing is salient was combined with appearance/clothing is salient qualified. The appearance/clothing is not salient cate- gory was combined with appearance/clothing is not salient qualified and appearance/clothing not mentioned. This resulted in the two appearance/clothing saliency cate- gories of l) salient and 2) not salient. (Appendix C). Due to the differences between each story-situation, scales were developed that pertained only to that situa- tion. Therefore, the following measures are related to each story. 34 Attribution Variable - Carol - Construction Worker From the question "Why do you think she was not hired?," the attribution variable was developed. It consisted of the following three categories; 1) Attri- butions to key person as causal, 2) Attributions to other's descriptive qualities as causal and 3) Attribu- tions to other factors in the situation/environment as causal. Any mention of Carol as having some effect on the situation was coded with the key person as causal agent. References to the actions of co-workers or the interviewer were coded as attributions to others as causal. Responses which referred to the job situation, company policies, etc., were coded with the environment as a causal agent. Under each of the three dimensions attributions were both positive and negative. (Appen- dix C). Attribution Variable - Bob/Ann - Typist With the typist situation, an attribution variable was developed that encompasses all three questions. Those questions are as follows: What do you think of the office manager who promoted Bob/Ann because of her appearance?, What do you think of Bob/Ann's use of clothing to get ahead on the job?, How do you feel about his/her co-workers and their responses to his/her pro— motion? From all of the questions the following three 35 dimensions evolved, l) Attributions to key person as causal, 2) Attributions to other's descriptive qualities as causal and 3) Attributions to other factors in the situation/environment. Any mention of Bob/Ann as causal agent resulted in assigning the code of an attribution to the key person. References to the actions of the office manager and co-workers were coded as attributions to other's as causal. Those responses which were made in reference to the job situation, company image, etc., were coded with the environment as the causal agent. (Appen- dix C). Formality of Occupational Clothipg This variable was developed in response to the following open-ended questions. The first question, ”Please describe the uniform, what garments, styles or colors, or what equipment do you wear?, refers to uni- form wearers only. The second question, ”Describe what you usually wear for work, what garments, styles or colors or what equipment do you wear?" applied to all others. This variable indicates the level of formality exhibited in the work attire of the respondents as measured by a six point scale for men and an eight point scale for women. The scale ranged from sweat- shirt and jeans at the most informal level to a business suit for a man or a woman's dress at the most formal level. 36 For women respondents the original eight cate- gories were collapsed into four to achieve agreement between uniform and non-uniform wearers. To create the variable the categories of formality for uniform and non-uniform wearers were combined to create one variable for each sex. The final variable included a six dimension scale for men and a four dimension scale for women. (Appendix D). Number of Appearance Requirements This variable consisted of the number of appearance requirements listed by the respondent. It was developed in response to the statement "please describe any other appearance requirements." This statement pertained to uniform wearers. Non-uniform wearers responded to the following: "Please list the dress requirements, what garments, styles, or colors or aspects of appearance are specified or understood?" The actual number of appearance requirements listed was coded. The appearance requirements measure represented an actual number of dress specifications. A uniform was considered as an additional appearance requirement. Therefore, consistency was achieved between uniform and non-uniform wearers by weighting the uniform by the extent of the garments required. For example, if the required uniform consisted of one garment this was 37 counted as one appearance requirement. If it consisted of a total outfit including footwear, four additional specifications were added to the appearance require- ments . Uniform This dichotomous variable, "Do you wear a uniform, yes or no?" was included as a dummy variable in the statistical analysis. Personal Clothing Constructs A series of five attitudinal statements were summed to indicate the respondent's strength of agreement or disagreement with the importance of clothing in the occupational setting._ Each response to a statement was given a score and the sum of these values is regarded as an index of the respondent's attitude. When the five attitudinal state- ments were summed, a range of responses from five to twenty—five resulted. For those individuals who did not respond to a statement, an average of the four remaining responses was substituted for the missing value. Occppation A three category variable was developed to represent the occupational level of the respondents. The first category, upper level white collar workers, was composed 38 of professionals, technical and kindred, managerial and administrative workers (except farm management). The second category, lower level white collar workers, included sales and clerical workers. Blue collar occupations comprised the third category. Those included were craftsmen and kindred, operators, labourers and household workers. Occupational Prestige This variable represented the occupational prestige of the main occupation of the respondents as determined by the Bureau of the Census occupational classifications. Age Age, a demographic variable left in interval form, ranged in value from 25 to 61 years. Employment Status The measure consisted of two categories. The one category was composed of all individuals who are employed, including those on sick leave or layoffs, etc. The other category was composed of all individuals currently unemployed. Education The number of years of education, a demographic variable, was also utilized as a possible discriminator 39 in the analysis. The range of values were from 6 years of education up to 22 years. Income The respondent's 1977 income,in categorical form, ranged from under $3,000 to over $75,000 for the year. Comparison of Regpondents py Set-Type Since the purpose of the investigation was to examine the effects of the personal experience variables on responses to the story-situations, it became necessary to determine if differences existed between responses to Set A and Set B, with regard to those variables. Set- types were also examined to determine differences that might exist across the appearance/clothing saliency and attribution variables. Use of t-test analysis revealed that significant differences did not exist between Set A and Set B for the personal clothing constructs, number of appearance requirements, and formality of occupational dress. Chi square analysis was performed on the attri- bution variables, appearance/clothing saliency and the categories of appearance requirements. The results were so similar that the null hypothesis could not be rejected. Some differences were presented in the means but these differences were not significant. Therefore, responses for Set A and B were combined for analysis. 4O Hypothesis Testing Stepwise Discriminant analysis was selected as one of the statistical methods to test the first two hypoth- eses for this study because of its ability to classify or group persons into mutually exclusive nominal cate- gories by using a set of independent variables. Its objective is to find functions of the independent vari- ables that maximize the discrimination among groups. (Thorndike, 1978). It is a procedure for estimating an individual's position on a line that best separates groups. (Cooley, Lohnes, 1962). The mathematical objective of discriminant analysis is to weight and linearly combine the discriminating variables in some fashion so that the groups are forced to be as statistically distinct as possible. (Klecka, 1975, p. 435). These weights are the discriminant function coefficients. The weights or coefficients are directly analogous to beta weights in multiple regression. (Thorndike, 1978, Klecka, 1975). With two or more inde- pendent variables, standardized discriminant function coefficients allow the researcher to compare the relative contribution of each of the independent variables to that function. The statistical theory of discrimi- nant analysis assumes that the discrimi- nating variables have multivariate normal distributions. The discriminating variables 41 also have equal variance - covariance matrices with each group as tested by Box's M. In practice, the technique is very robust and strong adherence to these assumptions is unnecessary. (Klecka, 1975, p. 435). In the statistical program used, missing values could only be deleted in a list-wise fashion, meaning that a missing value for any variable resulted in deletion of that case from all analyses. This resulted in a sub- stantial loss of cases numbering from 10 to 32 depending on the analysis involved. In order to derive the discriminant function, variables were selected through a stepwise process. The criterion used to control the stepwise selection was the largest increase in Rao's V, a generalized dis- tance measure. The method selects those variables which contribute to the greatest overall separation of the groups. (Klecka, 1975). The first variable chosen has the highest explanatory power according to the selection criteria, and is considered the most powerful discrimi- nator. Then the next variable is selected such that these two variables in conjunction produce the best dis- criminant function at that criterion level. Basically the process continues until all variables that can add to the improvement of the function are selected. This process often yields a reduced set of variables by 42 ignoring those that are not very useful in discrimi- nating among groups. To determine the success of the discriminant function it is possible to examine several measures. The canonical correlation tells how closely the function and the group variable are related. Likewise, the cano- nical correlation squared explains the proportion of variance in the discriminant function explained by the groups. A second possibility to determine the success of the discriminant function is through the use of Wilk's Lambda. Lambda measures the differences between all group centroids and the homogeneity within the groups. The Wilk's Lambda is considered an inverse measure of the discriminating power in the variables which has not been removed by the discriminant functions. The larger the value of Lambda, the less information remaining to be removed by additional variables. "In discriminant analysis, the percentage correctly classified is analogous to R2 of regression in that it tells how well the function classified the individual” (Morrison, 1969, p. 158). Additionally, Spearman rank order correlations and Chi Square analysis were utilized to test the last two hypotheses. For hypotheses testing, the significance level was set at .05 or better. CHAPTER 4 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION This chapter begins with a description of the sample; followed by an analysis of responses to the story-situations. The hypotheses deal with the respondent's perceptions and attributions in each story-situation. Description of the Sample The original sample gathered by the research team was composed of 237 husband and wife pairs who met the criteria for eligibility. For the story-situations and personal clothing constructs, the data were examined for evidence of collusion. The investigator looked for similarity of the response, in terms of ideas and hand- writing between husbands and wives. 'Evidence of possible and probable collusion as determined by the investi- gator resulted in eliminating 15 pairs of respondents from the sample. This brought the total number of hus- band and wife pairs in the sample down to 222. An addi- tional eight males were eliminated from the sample because they were unemployed. 43 44 The following paragraphs provide a brief descrip- tion of how the sample varied on demographic data. Age_ The male respondents' ages ranged from 25 to 61 years. The largest group of respondents fell in the range of 31 to 40 years, as shown in the table. The female. respondents' ages ranged from 25 to 59 years. Again, the largest group was in the age range of 31 to 40 years. Table 2--Age of Resppndents Working Non-Working Men Women Women Age N Z N Z N % 25 - 30 27 12.6 12 12.7 26 20.5 31 - 40 94 43.8 50 52.8 60 47.2 41 - 50 69 32.2 28 29.6 28 22.0 51 - 6O 21 9.8 5 5.4 10 7.9 Over 60 2 .9 Missing 1 .5 3 2.4 Data ___ TOTAL 214 100.0a 95 100.08 127 100.0 8May not equal 100 percent due to rounding. Education Education was coded in an interval manner. The table below indicates the years of education held by 45 the respondents. For the men, 51.8 percent of the sample fell in the post-secondary range of 13 to 18 years. An additional 39.2 percent reported reaching or completing secondary school. For the total sample of women, the largest group, 61.2 percent had reached and/ or completed secondary school. Approximately 35.6 per- cent of the sample had reached a post-secondary educa- tional level. Table 3--Educational Background of Respondents Working Non-Working Years of Men Women Women Education N Z N Z N Z 6 - 8 10 4.7 3 3.2 3 2.3 9 - 12 84 39.2 55 57.9 81 63.8 13 — 18 111 51.8 36 37.9 43 33.9 19 - 22 8 3.7 l 1.1 Missing 1 .5 Data ___ ___. TOTAL 214 100.0a 95 100.08 127 100.0 8May not equal 100 percent due to rounding. Employment Status Almost all of the men in the sample were working for pay with only eight unemployed at the time of data collection. Only 42.8 percent of the women were employed. 46 Table 4--Emplgyment Status of Respondents Men Women N Z N Z Employed for Pay 214 96.4 95 42.8 Unemployed __8 3.6 121_ 57.2 TOTAL 222 100.0 222 100.0 Classification of Main Occupation The classification of occupations was broken into three categories. For the men, 53.7 percent were employed in white collar positions, and of these 44 percent were in upper level professional, technical or managerial positions. Blue collar positions accounted for 44.9 per- cent of the sample. For the women, there was a fairly even distribution of people in each occupational classification. Blue collar comprised the largest group with 34.7 percent of the sample of women employed in this classification. 47 Table 5--Classification of Main Occupation of Respondents Employed Men Women Occupation N Z N Z Upper level white 95 44.4 30 31.6 collar, professional tech, managerial & administrative Lower level white 20 9.3 31 32.6 collar, sales & clerical Blue collar 96 44.9 33 34.7 Missing data 3 1.4 l 1.1 TOTAL 214 100.0 95 100.0 Personal Income The sample ranged in personal income from under $3,000 to over $50,000. For the men, the largest group was in the $20,000 to $29,000 income bracket. This group comprised 38.8 percent of the sample. The largest income group for the women was in the category under $5,000. 48 Table 6--1977 Income of Respondents Working Non-Working Men 0 Women 0 Women 0 Income N A N A N 0 Under 5,000 4 1.9 36 37.9 16 12.6 5,000 to 9,999 7 3.3 29 30.5 2 1.6 10,000 to 14,999 24 11.2 17 17.9 15,000 to 19,999 49 22.9 7 7.4 20,000 to 29,999 83 38.8 3 3.2 l .8 Above 30,000’ 43 20.1 1 1.1 Missing data 4 1.9 2 2.2 108 85.0 or unemployed ___ TOTAL 214 100.0a 95 100.0a 127 100.0 aMay not equal 100 percent due to rounding. Comparison of Story-Situations The objectives of this research project were to study the perceptions and attributions of the men and women after their exposure to these story-situations and to determine how selected factors affect their perceptions and attributions. Comparisons of these two story- situations as shown in Tables 7 and 8 reveal that dif- ferences exist across the two cases when the attributions variable and the appearance/clothing saliency were examined. 49 For the construction worker situation, clearly appearance/clothing is not salient for most men and women responding. An increased number of the sample responded that clothing was salient in the typist story- situation. While appearance/clothing was not salient for 73.4 percent of the men in the construction worker case, only 60.3 percent responded that it was not salient for the typist. For over half of the women, appearance/ clothing was salient in the typist story—situations. The respondents may have become more attuned to the clothing cues in the typist situation or the questions about appearance/clothing might be responsible for the increases in saliency for both men and women. For the construction worker story-situation, most of the respondents made internal attributions to the key person. Only 49 of the men and a total of 39 women did not make attributions to the key person. In compar- ison, 158 women did not make attributions to others and 157 women did not attributeanything to the situation. It would appear that the respondents viewed Carol as the motivating force within the construction worker story- situation, thereby designating her as causal agent. This could be due in part to the fact that the position was unusual for a woman. 50 .wcfincoou ou opp ucoowoa 0.00H Hmnvo uoc moons x0.00 mm. 0.00H mm 0.00. «am A. All N.s «In Alain mm; m.mq 00 m.mq m0 m.00 0m. 0.0m 00 m.0m 00 0.00 NA k0.00 NNH 0.00H m0 0.00H qHN mllallmlmulmllmdulolfil 0.0m m0 m.m0 N0 «.05 Ana «.0N Hm 0.Hm 0m ~.0H .0 INT. 2 erlllllm wrllllllz coEoz ooEoz no: wcwaozucoz wcHxHoB A ucmncommccH mo mcoHuonuuoo ucoEoz uosmopm comumom-:0 oHan 57 muosuumcou HomHuzv Hquuzv AmHanv ANHNuzv w HHo. Nzo. m.z m.z .oo. m.z zoo. Hmmemww «00m. oqu. «com. meow. among Amauuzv Homzuzv Amounzv AmaHuzv Aaouuzv -maswwmoww H00. H00. m.z H00. H00. H00. xuaHmEpom comm. onm. mcmm. c00m. qmcm. . mmoun wuoauuwcou HmcoHu mucoEoquvom wcHsuoHo -maaooo mo mocmumoam< owHummum HmCOmpom zuHHwEuom choHumaaooo EAOHHCD om< coHumoapm meoocH HmcoHumasooo Av.u:000 a muses 58 mquwE TouHavoz oocm Awmmzv numoaa< nm¢n.- .m.z .m.z m.z 0.2 00 ponEnz Asouzv . 000. . .. Euo as m z monm. m z m 2 H. 0 Asmuzv . mmo. . mm m z womH. m z < Asouzv . H00. cod mo: 0 2 00mm. .u 0m m.z oEoocH owHum0pm HmcoHumaaooo wmoun muoauumcou HmcoHuma mucoEouHscox mchuoHo -masooo Ho oocmpwoaa< owHumoum HmCOmuom quHmEuom HmcoHumasooo EpoMHc: om< coHumospm oEoocH choHuma:ooo coEoz Mczxuos you moHanum> acupcomomcH mo mcoHumHouuoo EcoEoz uospoum cempmomiucH mHomE 59 muospumcoo Hmouzv Hmonzv wcHLuoHu . mHo. mH0. .. . . . mmomuo m z «mmm. qum.u v z m z m z m 2 H m amouo mcoHu Ammuzv Ammuzv -maswoo do ... .. . . . . 0H0. NHO. A H mayo 0 2 v z m z m z mmmm. 00cm. u.H m mmouo muosuumcou choHume mucoaouHscom wchuoHu Twasooo mo oocmumoaa< owHumoum HwCOmuom >UHHmEuom HmcoHummzooo EuOHHcs 00¢ coHumoapm oeoocH HmcoHumasooo AU.ucoov 0H oHan 60 H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her per- ceptions of the saliency of appearance and clothing in the construction worker story-situation. Stepwise discriminant analysis was performed on data for the sample of men (N = 178) and revealed that only one variable, the personal clothing constructs entered the equation. The function using this variable was statistically significant overall, (p = .005) as a predictor of appearance/clothing saliency for the construction worker situation. Thus, appearance/clothing was perceived as being more salient for those respondents with a higher score on the personal clothing construct index. As shown in Table 11, this variable had a discriminant function coefficient equal to 1.000. If only one variable enters the equation it will always have a coefficient of 1.000. As this was the only resulting predictor, it cannot be compared to any others within the function. For this function, Box's M equal to 4.843 was statistically significant (p = .028). Therefore, an unequal variance - covariance matrix existed within the groups but it is assumed that it has not signficantly altered the results obtained "since discriminant analysis is robust with respect to this assumption (Klecka, 1975, p. 435)." 6l mHH u mHmszc< OocH vapoucm mommu mo Monasz Hmuoh mmH u z ucoHHmm uoc mH wcHnuoHo\oocmumoaa< Hq u z ucoHHmm mH wcHnuoHu\oocwumomm< aaouu comm CH mommu mo nonsszm m0.nn u vonHmmmHo zHuoounoo ommocoouom Ammo. u z w.xom mo mocMOHchme .m¢m.q n : m.xom m000. n coHuoczm ucmcHEHuomHa ocu mo mosmonchHm HHmuo>o 000m. u :oHuocam ucmcHEHpomHo onu mo coHumHouuoo HmOHcocmu muosuumcou oooo. ooH.No oooo. ook.~H mooo. ammo. ooo.. mcoouoHo Hmcomuoo > m.oox munEmH c. mocsoo > m.omm m.xo.2 mononuooomoo MO QUCmU > m.omx MO mucmu NO mUCmU mvnfimd COHUOC—Hh flmuflucm -Aoocoom c. mocmoo -ococwom > m.omm -Aoocwwm m.xHo3 ucmcoeouumwo mmuomwum> coHumsuHmuxu0um noxuoz coHuoouuwcoo ozu CH cw: p00 mxocoHme wchuoHo\oocmumoaa< mo mmeHmc< ucmcHEHuomHQTTHH oHan 62 When this discriminant function (composed of the personal clothing constructs) was used to predict group membership, only 57.07 percent of the individuals were correctly classified into the groups to which they were originally assigned. Thus, although the variable was a significant predictor, it was not very successful at dichotomizing persons on the basis of appearance/ clothing saliency. "When one group is much larger than the other, almost all individuals are classified into the larger group." (Morrison, 1969, p. 161). Without knowledge of prior probabilities it is diffi- cult to interpret the classification table in that it is hard to determine how well the discriminant function has correctly classified individuals into groups. Discriminant analysis performed on the sample of working women (N = 84) resulted in‘a discriminant function with an overall significance of .0335 (Table 12). The dummy variable dealing with whether or not the subjects wore a uniform was the first to enter the equation with a discriminant function coefficient of .7572. The second variable entered, the index of personal clothing constructs, had a discriminant function coefficient of .7926. The final variable to enter the equation was age. It's coefficient of -.4642 makes a negative contribution to the function in that it opposes the direction 63 of the first two variables. Appearance/clothing was perceived as being more salient as the respondents ages decreased and as their personal clothing construct indices increased. Additionally, uniform wearers were more apt to respond that appearance/clothing was salient. Although the overall function was statistically significant, the uniform variable was not significant for Rao's V., indicating that by itself wearing or not wearing a uniform was not significant in separating those for whom clothing was salient from those for whom it was not. However, in combination with the other variables it contributed significantly to the classi- fication. Personal clothing constructs as a predictor pro- vided the best discrimination with the highest coefficient. Age, the variable contributing the least to the function, was approximately half as powerful of a discriminator as was personal clothing constructs. The resulting discriminant function correctly classified 61.54 per- cent of the group. For both men and women, neither formality of occu- pational dress nor occupational appearance requirements were included in the discriminant function. Yet, the presence of personal clothing constructs in the dis- criminant function for both men and women partially 64- cm H mmeHmc< OucH @0u0ucm mommo mo honfisz Hmuoe mm H z ucoHHmm uoc mH wchuoHo\oocmumoam< mm u z ucoHHmm mH mchuoHu\mocmumoaa< anouo comm CH mummu mo poneszm «m.H0 u UOHWHmmmHo xHuoouuou ommucoouom oooo. u : m.xoo .o mocmunochom .ook.o u z m.xoo mmmc. u coHuocsm ucmcHEHuomHa onu mo oocmonchHm HHmuo>o Homm. u coHuoczm ucocHEHHoch Gnu mo coHumHouuoo HmoHcocmu Hood. ~oo.H oooo. Nom.o mono. mkoo. o~oo.- sw< Ho muoauumcoo wowouooo somo. moo.o ammo. okm.a oomo. mano. omok. Hegemuma AN mooH. moo.~ muoz. moo.~ HmoH. mooo. Nam“. ELoLHco A. > m.oam nonsmo CH mocmoo > m.oom m.xHo3 mocwHoHoowoo mo mocmo > m.omm mo oocmo Ho oocoo munEmH coHuocam popoucm -ooucwom a. «momoo -Loocwmm > m.oom -ooocoom m.xH.3 oemcosuuomwo mmuomoum> cmEo3 UmonaEm you coHuoouHmtzuouw poxuoz coHuosuumcoo ocu cH mxocoHHmm wchuoHu\oocmumoaa< mo mHmszc< ucmcHEHuomHanuNH oHan 65 supports the hypothesis. Therefore, the null hypothesis is only partially rejected. Hypothesis 1b H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress, and occupational appearance requirements will not influence his/her per- ceptions of the saliency of appearance and clothing in the typist story-situation. H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her per- ceptions of the saliency of appearance and clothing in the typist story-situation. Discriminant analysis was performed on the sample of men (N = 180) with the appearance/clothing saliency dimen- sion for the typist. Personal clothing constructs, age, occupational prestige and the wearing of a uniform, were significant discriminators. Age, with a discriminant function coefficient of -.6280 and personal clothing constructs with a coefficient of -.6037 rank first and second as the predictors providing the best discrimination. Occupational prestige with a coefficient of -.4422 and uniform with a coefficient of .3119 also contribute to the function as shown in Table 13. The negative co- efficients all oppose the direction of the positive coefficient of the uniform variable. Those individuals who were younger and uniform wearers, with higher occupational prestige and with a higher index on 66 personal clothing constructs were more apt to respond that appearance/clothing was salient. Occupational prestige and the uniform variable do not provide as much discriminating power, nor do they contribute as much separation of those for whom clothing was salient from those for whom it was not, as the first two vari- ables entered (Table 13). Although statistically significant, the discrimi- nant function derived for appearance/clothing saliency correctly classified only 56.6 percent of the individuals into groups. Again, the ability of the function to correctly classify individuals would appear not to be very successful. Analysis of the saliency of appearance/clothing dimension for the typist did not derive a statistically significant discriminant function for employed women. In the analysis of men's responses to appearance/ clothing saliency for the typist, neither the formality of occupational dress or occupational appearance require- ments entered the discriminant function. Personal clothing constructs was the only hypothesized variable found to distinguish among the groups. Thus, although a discriminant function was not derived for women and only one of the three variables entered the equation for men, partial support of the hypothesis exists. There- fore, the null hypothesis must be partially rejected. 67 00H 0 mmeHmc< OucH commucm mommo mo wonEDZ Hmuoe qHH u z ucoHHmm uoc mH wcHnuoHu\oocmuwoao< 00 n z ucoHHmm mH wcHsuoHo\oocmumoaa< aaoho comm CH mommo mo uonEszw nm.0m u ponHmmmHu zHuoouuou owmucoopom moo“. u z m.xoo mo mocmowowowwm .moo.~H u z m.xoo mqoo. u coHuocsm ucmcHEHuomHa ocu mo mocmOHchme HHmuo>o momm. u coHuocam ucmcHEHuomHo onu mo coHumHouuoo HmoHcocmu ammo. Hoo.H Hmoo. koo.mH mooo. okoo. oHHm. spoons: Ho owHumoum oNoH. omH.~ mNoo. ooo.oH Nmoo. ooNo. -oo.- Hmconumooooo Hm ooHo. HHm.m HNoo. omm.~H Amoo. Nmmo. oo~o.- mw< Ho muosuumcoo weanooHo Hooo. MHo.o Hooo. mHo.o oooo. Hmoo. Amoo.- Hmcomuoo AH > m.omo season on «mango > m.omo m.xH.3 nomaonoomoo mo mocmo > m.omx mo oocmo mo oocmo manmH :oHuocam monoucm -ooocwom a. mwcmoo -oofiooom > m.omm -Howcwom m.xHo3 OsmcHEHLomHo mmHomwum> :oHumsuHmuzuoum umHmzH ocu CH so: how mxocoHHmm wcHsuoHo\oocmumoam< mo memec< ucmcHEHuomHauumH oHpmH 68 Testing of the Attribution Hypotheses Hypothesis 2a H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will not influence his/her attributions to Carol in the construction worker story-situation. H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her attri- butions to Carol in the construction worker. Discriminant analysis of men's and women's attribu- tions to Carol revealed that significant functions were not derived for either sex. Therefore, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. Hypothesis 2b H . An individual's personal clothing constructs, formalIty of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will not influence his/her attributions to others in the construction worker story-situations. H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her attri- butions to others in the construction worker story- situations. Discriminant analysis of men's and women's attribu- tions to others in the construction worker story- situation failed to derive a statistically significant function with which to distinguish individuals. There- fore, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. 69 Hypothesis 2c Ho: An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will not influence his/her attributions to the situation in the construction worker story-situation. H1: An indiviudual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her attributions to the situation in the construction worker story-situation. Discriminant analysis performed on the sample of men (N = 185) derived a discriminant function with an overall significance of .0310. Personal clothing con- structs, first to enter the equation with a coefficient of -.7036 was followed by occupational appearance require- ments and age with coefficients of .6150 and -.5248 respectively. The second variable, occupational appearance requirements contributed the greatest overall separation between groups who did and did not make attributions to the situation. This was indicated by the change in Rao's V. (Table 14). Again, personal clothing con- structs and age are providing the function with negative contributions, thereby opposing the direction of the posivite coefficients. Thus, attributions to the situa- tion increased as the personal clothing constructs and age increased and as the number of occupational appearance requirements decreased. Although the discriminant function with the inclusion of personal clothing constructs, occupational appearance 70 requirements and age, was significant overall, it correctly classified only 58.54 percent of the groups. Discriminant analysis performed on the sample of working women (N = 85) derived a significant function (p = .0233) composed of four variables. The first variable to enter was occupational prestige with a discriminant function coefficient of -.8605 (Table 15). The second variable to enter the equation, formality of occupational dress, had a coefficient of .5677. The last two variables to enter, personal clothing constructs and occupational appearance requirements had coefficients of .5175 and -.4241 respectively. Occupational prestige and occupational appearance requirements provide negative contributions to the function, and work in opposition to the positive coefficients. Therefore, increases in attributions to situation were influenced by increases in occupational prestige and occupational appearance requirements and decreases in the personal clothing construct index and formality of occupational dress. The first variable occupational prestige constributes the most to the discrimination between those who made attributions to the situation and those who do not as indicated by the size of the coefficients. Occupational prestige also creates separation among the groups as indicated by the significant change in Rao's V. The last variables all provide some separation among the 71 me n mHmmHmc< OucH monoucm mommo mo ponenz Hmuoe HoH u z cowumouom on meoouaowuuu< oz 0H n z coHumnuHm ou mcoHuanuuu< aaouu comm CH mommo mo noneszm em.wm u pOHmemmHo mHuoouuou owmucooumm oooo. n : m.xoo mo mocmuouwcwwm .oom.o u z m.xoo 0H00. u coHuocsu uamcHEHuomHo ocu mo mocMOHchme HHmuo>o qum. n coHuocam acmcHEHuowHo onu mo coHuoHouuoo HmOHcocmo No... mom.~ HHNo. oko.o oHMo. mNno. oon.- uw< Hm mucoEoqucom oocmumona< omoo. moo.m oomo. koo.o oomo. omoo. onHo. Hmcowumooooo AN muoauumcoo wcwoooHo oHHo. HoN.m oHHo. Ho~.m mmao. omoo. omok.- .mcomumm AH > m.omm manmH on omcmou > m.omm m.xHH3 unmouwoomoo mo wocmo > m.omm MO mocmu MO mucmu wfin—Emd COHHUP—Dh Uwhmufim -Hoocoom :H mwcmoo -Hoocoom > m.omm -Howcwom a.xHA3 ucmcfiefiuomfio moHomHum> :oHumsuHMTHHoum poxuoz coHuosuumcoo mzu :H so: now coHumsuHm ou mcoHuanuuu< mo mmeHmc< ucmcHEHuomHnnqu oHnt m 72 groups, but their discriminating power was reduced in comparison to that of the first variable. The dis- criminant function, although significant, correctly classified only 61.63 percent of the individuals into the groups to which they had been originally assigned. In an examination of the two functions derived for men and women's attributions to situation, it is evident that the three hypothesized predictors, (personal clothing constructs, occupational appearance requirements and formality of occupational dress) enter into one or both of the discriminant functions. This provides partial support of the hypothesis. Therefore, the null hypothesis must be partially rejected. Hypothesis 2d H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will not influence his/her attributions to Bob/Ann in the typist story-situation. H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her attri- butions to Bob/Ann in the typist story-situation. Discriminant analysis of the men's (N = 131) attribu- tions to typist revealed an overall significant function (p = .0191). Occupational appearance requirements, first to enter the equation, had a discriminant function co- efficient of .8052. The second variable to enter, per- sonal clothing constructs, provided a negative contri- bution to the function with a coefficient of - 7101 which 73 00 n mmeHmc< oucH nououcm mommu mo ponenz HmuOH N0 u z coHumSUHm ou mcoHunnHuuu< oz 00 u z coHuwauHm ou wcoHuspHpuu< azouu comm CH mommu 00 nonsnzm 00.H0 n onMHmmmHu xHuoouuoo owmucoouom H000. u z m.xom mo oocmOHchme .H00.0 n : m.xom 0000. u coHuocsm ucmcHEHuomHa onu mo oocmOHMchHm HHmuo>o NH00. n :oHuocsm ucmcHEHuomHa ago 00 coHumHouuoo HMOHcocmo mucoEoqucom oocmumoam< 0HOH. 000.0 cho. ch.mH 0000. 0000. H¢N¢.- HmcoHumaaooo H0 muosuumcou wannOoHo 000H. 000.0 00H0. 000.0H 0000. 0000. mHHm. Hmcomuom H0 mmoun HmcoHumaaooo 0500. 000.0 mch. 00H.0 0H00. 00H0. m~0m. mo ~AuHHmEpom Hm owHumoum 0000. 000.0 0000. 00m.q 0H00. mmq0. 0000.- HmcoHumasooo AH > m.omm awnEmH so mmcmoo > m.ooo m.xHH3 ucmuuwmowoo mo oocmo > m.Omx mo oocmo mo oocmo manmH coHuocnm monoucm -Hoocwom on mwcmoo -Hmocoom > 0.00m -ooocwom m.xHA: Ocmcoefieumwo mmHomHum> coHumzuHmHNMOum woxpoz coHuosuumcou mnu :H :oEo3 u00 :oHumsuHm ou mcoHuanHuuu< mo mmeHmc< ucmcHEHuomHouumH oHnme m 74 signifies that the two variables are moving in opposite directions. Thus, an increase in attributions to typist was associated with an increase in the personal clothing constructs index and a decrease in occupational appear- ance requirements. Both variables help in separating the groups. Each variable has a significant Rao's V and a significant change in Rao's V as shown in Table 16. This discriminant function composed of occupational appearance requirements and personal clothing constructs, was slightly better at classifying individuals than the previous functions. When this discriminant function was used to predict group membership, 67.50 percent of the respondents were correctly classified into those groups to which they had originally been assigned. Discriminant analysis of working women revealed that a function could not be derived for women's attributions to the typist. Even though only one function was derived, there was partial support for the hypothesis due to the presence of occupational appearance requirements and personal clothing constructs in the men's discriminant function. Therefore, the null hypothesis is partially rejected. Hypothesis 2e H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appear- ance requirements will not influence his/her attributions to others in the typist story-situation. 75 H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appear- ance requirements will influence his/her attributions to others in the typist story-situation. A significant discriminant function was not derived for men or women's attributions to others. Therefore, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. Hypothesis 2f H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appear- ance requirements will not influence his/her attribu- tions to the situation in the typist story-situation. H : An individual's personal clothing constructs, formality of occupational dress and occupational appear- ance requirements will influence his/her attributions to others in the typist story-situation. Discriminant analysis performed on the sample of men (N = 182) resulted in the derivation of an overall statis- tically significant function (p = .0346) for attributions to situation. Two predictors entered the discriminant equation. (Table 17). The first, occupational appearance requirements, with a coefficient of .6678, provided the most separation among groups with a significant Rao's V and a significant change in Rao's V. The second variable to enter, occupational prestige, with a coefficient of .6493 contributes to the separation also indicated by the significant Rao's V, but the change in Rao's V was not significant. As occupational prestige and occupational appearance requirements increase, attributions to situa- tion increased. 76 HmH n mmeHmc< oucH vououcm mommo mo nonesz Hmuoe 0H u z umHmah Ou maoHuanuuu< oz 00H u z umHaas cu mcoHunnHuuu< asouo nomm :H momma mo Honfidzm 00.00 u vonHmmmHu zHuomuuoo ammucoouom ooom. u z 0.xoo oo econowoocwfim .omm.o u z m.xoo H0H0. u :oHuocsm ucmcHEHpomHa ecu uo oocmonchHm HHmuo>o 0000. n coHuocsm acmcHEHuomHo ecu mo coHumHouuoo HmOHcocmo muuauumcoo wcwouono mooo. moo.o HHHo. moH.o HoHo. memo. HoHA.- Hmcomumm H0 mucoBoHHSUOm oocmumoaa< oNoo. oHH.o omso. oHH.o Hooo. okko. mmow. Hmcooumoaooo AH > m.omm moose; cw «mango > 0.0mm 0.xHoz ucmfluoommoo mo oocmo > m.omx mo mocmo mo oocmo monEMH coHuocam voyeucm -oo.chm :. mmcmoo -Hmocwflm > 0.0mm -Howcwom 0.x.oz somewe.uumoo mmHBmHum> coHumSuHmuzuoum ummee onu cH so: you mumHaze Cu mcoHuzaHuuu< mo meszc< quGHEHuomHnuu0H oHan 77 The discriminant function with the inclusion of occupational appearance requirements and occupational prestige, although significant, correctly classifies only 63.5 percent of the individuals into the groups. An analysis performed on the sample of working women (N = 85) derived a discriminant function with an overall significance (p = .0134). Four predictors entered the equation. (Table 18). Occupational prestige and age, the first two predictors to enter the equation, with coefficients of -.5785 and -.5316 respectively, contribute negatively to the function. Both provide significant group separation as indicated by Rao's V and the change in Rao's V. The last predictors, personal clothing constructs and income entered the equation with coefficients of .4710 and - 4647 respectively. Again, income contributed negatively to the function. The last two variables significantly contribute to separation of those who made attributions to the situation and those who did not. A decrease in discriminating power should be noted. Therefore, an increase in attributions to situation was influenced by increases in age, occupa- tional prestige and income and decreases in the personal clothing construct index. The discriminant function derived with the four predictors (occupational prestige, age, personal clothing 78 NQH N mwmmeC< OUCM Umhmucm MQmmU MO HOQESZ HQUOH 00H u z :oHomaon Oo «coHoscHHuo4 oz 00 n z :oHumnuHm ou mcoHuacHHuu< nacho comm :H momwu mo neceszm 00.00 H vonHmmmHo xHuoouuou owmucoouom ANNA. n : «.xom «0 «ocmoHoHcmHm .Ho~.~ u z «.xom 0000. u coHuocsm ucmcHEHuomHo ocu mo mocmoHMchHm HHmuo>O 000H. n coHoocsm ucmcHEHuomHa ocu mo coHumHopuoU HmoHcocmo owHumoum Hmoo. mmm.~ ono. Hom.o memo. Hmoo. mooo. H«ooHo«o:ooo Am mucoEouHSUOM oucmumoam< Homo. «no.0 Homo. omo.o omoo. oon. ono. HmcoHo«o=««o AH > m.om0 mmcemq :H «mcmoo > m.o«m «.xHHz oc«H«Hom«oo mo mocmo > w.0m¢ mo mocmo «0 ”Demo QfiAEmA floquGSh fiflhflucm -HoHomHm :H «mc«;o -HoHcmHm > m.o«m -HoHcmHm m.xHH3 uc«:HsHuo«Ho ««HE«HA«> coHuwsuHmLNHoum umHmNH ocu CH am: now mcoHumsuHm oo mcoHuscHuuu< mo mHthmc< ucmcHEHuomHouumH oHcmH 79 00 n mHm0Hmc< oucH monoucm mommo mo Hocadz Hmuob 00 u z coHuwsuHm ou mcoHuscHuuu< oz 00 u z coHuwauHm 0u mcoHuscHuuu< asouu comm CH mommo mo HocEszm 00.00 u monHmmmHo 0Huoouuoo owmucoouom ommm. u z «.xom Ho «ogmoHngmHm .Hoo.mH u : m.xom 00H0. n coHuocsm ucmcHEHHomHa ocu mo mocmOHchme Hkuo>o 0000. u coHuocsm ucmcHEHHomHa ocu mo coHumHouHoo HmoHcocmo mmoo. omm.~ voo. on.mH onHo. ommm. voo.- «soocH Ao muosuumcoo mgngoHo oomH. ogm.m oHHo. AMH.HH moHo. 0Hmm. oHAo. H«go«g«m Am Aooo. ~mm.m AAHo. Hom.m omHo. «moo. onm.- «m< AN owHumoum memo. omo.m memo. amo.m «Amo. mmoo. mm0n.- HogoHomooooo AH > m.o«m «ogEmH gH «mgmgo > m.o«m m.gHH3 «og«Honm«oo mo oocmo > m.omm mo mocmo 00 mocmo mchmH coHuoczm mououcm -HngmHm gH «mg«go -HngmHm > m.o«m -HngmHm «.gHoz oomgHngomHo m«Hg«HH«> coHumsuHmnzHOum umHmzH ocu :H :oEoz How :oHumsuHm 0u mcoHuscHuuu< wo mmeHmc< ucmcHEHuomHnuu0H oHcmH m 80 constructs and income) had the highest percentage correctly classified of all the functions. This func- tion correctly classified 72.73 percent of the individ- uals into the groups to which they had been assigned originally. Even though personal clothing constructs was the only hypothesized predictor to enter the discriminant function, partial support of the hypothesis exists. Therefore, the null hypothesis must be partially rejected. Testing of the Hypotheses Involving Occupation Hypothesis 3 H0: An individual's occupation will not influence his/her perceptions and attributions in the story- situations. H : An indiviudal's occupation will influence his/her perceptions and attributions in the story- situations. Spearman rank order correlations were utilized to determine the relationships between the respondent's main occupation and his/her perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency and attributions in the two story-situations. For the men, appearance/clothing saliency and the classi- fication of main occupation had a low negative correla- tion although statistically significant (p = .022). The negative correlation signifies that as the level of occupation decreases, clothing became more salient. Men's attributions to situation for the typist and the 81 classification of main occupation were positively correlated at a significance level of .0124. Therefore, as the respondent's occupational level increased, attri- butions to situation for the typist increased. Spearman rank order correlations revealed only those two significant relations as shown in Table 19. The lack of high correlations for men and the absence of any correlations for women suggest that relationships between the respondent's occupation as represented in these data, and their perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency and attributions to the story-situations do not exist. Therefore, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. Testing of the Hypotheses InvolvingyEmployment Status Hypothesis 4 HO: The employment status of the female respondents will not influence their perceptions and attributions in the story-situations. H1: The employment status of the female respondents will influence their perceptions and attributions in the story-situations. Chi Square analysis was performed to examine the relationship between the employment status of the respon- dents and their perceptions of the saliency of appearance/ clothing and attributions in the story-situations. For the construction worker and typist story situations, no significant relationships were found between employment status and perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency 82 Table l9--Spearman Rank Order Correlations of Occupation and Dependent Variables Dependent Variables Men Women Construction Worker Story Appearance/Clothing Saliency N.S. N.S. Attributions to Carol N.S. N.S. Attributions to Others N.S. N.S. Attributions to Situation N.S. N.S. Typist Story-Situation Appearance/Clothing Saliency -.l430 N.S. (0220201) Attributions to Typist N.S. N.S. Attributions to Others N.S. N.S. Attributions to Situation .5321 N.S. (A = 201) 83 or attributions to key person, others or situation. Therefore, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. CHAPTER 5 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The main purpose of this research study was to examine how an individual's selected work-related experiences and attitudes will affect his/her per- ceptions of clothing and attributions in a hypothetical occupational setting. The study consisted of a sample group of husband and wife pairs residing together in Oakland County, Michigan. Each husband/wife pair had at least one school age child (5-18 years) residing with them. Separate but similar analyses were per- formed on the data obtained from the husbands and wives. There were 214 men and 222 women included in the analysis. The data for this research study were collected in a self-administered questionnaire between November 1977 and March 1978. The data collection instrument included five occupational story-situations, two of which were utilized for this study. Each story-situa- tion was composed of one main character and several additional minor characters who were referred to or mentioned specifically. The appearance and clothing 84 85 of the main character were clearly stated in all stories. Each respondent answered a series of short open-ended questions after reading a brief paragraph describing the occupational story-situations. Dependent variables of appearance/clothing saliency and attribution were developed from the respondents' answers to the open-ended questions. Additionally from the questionnaire, predictor variables were developed from portions of the occupational clothing section and basic demographics. The objectives of this study were to: 1. Examine the perceptions and attributions of men and women upon exposure to selected story-situations. 2. Examine the selected factors affecting an individual's perceptions and attributions in the story- situations. 3. Examine the selected factors affecting the respondents' perceptions of clothing in occupational situations. Conclusions by Major Hypotheses Major Hypotheses One An individual's personal clothing constructs, for- mality of occupational dress, and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her perceptions of the saliency of appearance/clothing in the story-situations. For both men and women, personal clothing constructs, was the only hypothesized predictor found to have dis- criminating power for appearance/clothing saliency in the 86 two story-situations. (Table 20). The dummy variable, wearing a uniform to work, and two demographic variables, age and occupational prestige entered the discriminant function as additional predictors. Appearance and clothing was perceived as being salient as values in- creased for the personal clothing constructs indices, and occupational prestige and as the age of the respondent decreased. The absence of the two variables, occupational appear- ance requirements, and formality of occupational dress from the discriminant function, indicates that these two predictors as measured do not influence the perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency. Also the presence of the dummy variable wearing a uniform to work suggests that additional clothing related experiences besides those hypothesized may be useful in explaining the importance of appearance/clothing cues. It is evident that a combination of personal clothing constructs and demographics distinguishes individuals into salient and not salient clothing categories. Although, the accuracy with which group membership could be pre- dicted with these variables was little better than if the individuals had been classified into the groups at random. 87 coHumSuHm oo mcoHu oHAq. onm.- o0o0.- mo0n.- -ogHAoo< omHer :oHuQDUHm ou mcoHoscHuuu< 00H0. 0000. H000 - 0000.- Hoxuoz coHuoauumcoo 0000. 0000.- uoxuo3 coHuoauumcoo zocoHHmm wchuoHo \oocmumoaa< Co803 coHumnon 0000. 0000. cu mcoHuacHuuu< :0mumm 0oz ou HOH0.- 0000. 98833.33. “.3000. :oHumaun ou mcoHuscHuuu< 0000.- 00H0. 0000.- uoxuoz coHuosuumcou Amoo.- oHHm. om~o.- NH00.- omHer 000.H umxuos coHuoauumcou xocoHHmm wchuoHo \oocmumo00< mmmuo cm: mooauumcoo HmcoHu mocoEouHacom 0chuoHu -m0:ooo mo oocmumoaa< owHoonm Hmcomuom quHmEpok HocoHomazooD Euochs 00< :oHumosmmlloEoocH HmcoHummaooo mucoHonwooo coHuocsm ucmcHEHuomHa moNchmvcmum mcoHumsuHm-xwoum 030 ocu How mmeHmc< ucmcHEHHowHo mo 0p:EE:m-00 oHccH 88 Major Hypothesis Two An indiviudal's personal clothing constructs, for- mality of occupational dress and occupational appearance requirements will influence his/her attributions in the story-Situations Discriminant analysis resulted in the derivation of significant discriminant functions for attributions to key person(typist) for men and attributions to situation (construction worker and typist) for both men and women. (Table 20). Analyses could have derived a total of twelve functions for men and women, but only five were actually derived as mentioned above. The three hypothesized variables, personal clothing constructs, occupational appearance requirements and formality of occupational dress, appear in one or more of the functions as pre- dictors of attribution to situation. Increases in attri- butions to the key person and situation were generally associated with increases in occupational prestige, age and personal clothing construct index. Combinations of the hypothesized predictors and demographics do in- fluence attributions to situation, but again the accuracy with which individuals can be correctly classified into groups was not very successful. Since only one function resulted for attributions to key person(typist) it would appear that the eight predictors do not discriminate well among these groups. In fact, the hypothesized predictors, along with the dummy variable of wearing a uniform to work and the 89 demographic variables, do not distinguish among groups at all for attributions to others. Therefore, addi- tional variables would be needed to discriminate between those individuals who make attributions to key person and others and those who do not. Major Hypothesis Three An individual's occupation will influence his/her perceptions and attributions in the story-situations. The investigator believed that an individual's life experiences would influence his/her perceptions and attributions in certain situations. Specifically it was thought that the respondent's own experiences in an occupation, would influence his/her perceptions of the importance of appearance/clothing in an occupational setting. Also, the researcher hypothesized that the life experiences of an occupation would influence the way in which the respondents attributed causality in an occupa- tional situation. As indicated by Spearman rank order correlations, one can conclude that the main occupation of a respondent when represented as one of three levels (upper level white collar, lower level white collar and blue collar) does not influence the manner in which s/he perceives the importance of appearance and clothing, nor does it affect his/her attributions in the hypothetical story-situations. 90 Major Hypothesis Four A female individual's employment status will in- fluence her perceptions in the story-situations. The employment status of the respondents did not influence their perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency or their attributions in the two story-situa- tions. Although chi square analysis did not show signifi- cant differences between the perceptions and attributions between employed and unemployed women, several factors may have influenced the results. If respondents were employed for pay, laid off or on sick leave, they were considered employed. All others were considered unemployed. The nonworking group in the sample may have been employed in various occupations prior to the study. If the currently employed women had worked previously, they might have been socialized in the same manner as the employed group. These nonworking women would have already been exposed to similar occupational experiences in terms of dress expectations, as those who were considered employed at the time of the study. This could account for the lack of differences between the two groups. Discussion of Results The researcher theorized that the experiences and expectations of an occupation along with its socialization process would be influencing factors on an individual's 91 perceptions of events in an occupational situation and also her/his attributions of causality in that situation. Therefore, it was hypothesized that an individual's clothing attitudes developed during the occupational socialization (personal clothing constructs) and dress expectations (as reflected in occupational appearance re- quirements and formality of occupational dress), along with the respondent's occupation itself would influence that individual's perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency and his/her attributions in an occupational story-situation. Thus, it was believed that an unemployed individual would be without these experiences of an occupation and dress expectations, and would, therefore, respond differently to the occupational story-situations. As shown in the conclusions of the major hypotheses, portions of that theory were partially supported. From an examination of the discriminant analysis, performed a total of sixteen times for men and women utilizing the same eight predictors, it is possible to conclude the following results. Formality of occupational dress appeared in a discriminant function only once. Additionally, the demographic variable of education did not appear in an equation at all and income entered only one time. Although formality of occupational dress and income contribute to the functions in which they appear, they did not enter as consistently as other predictors. 92 This suggests that those three variables in their present form do not aid in the discrimination of appearance/ clothing saliency and attributions in the story- situations. The dummy variable regarding whether or not the respondent wore a uniform to work entered twice in functions derived to distinguish between appearance/clothing saliency. This suggests that the dummy uniform variable, an addi- tional measure of the respondents appearance requirements in itself, further supports what had theorized. Personal clothing constructs, occupational appearance requirements, occupational prestige and age were found to be the most powerful discriminators. Each appeared four or more times in functions. The presence of the first two variables make it possible to conclude that attitudes toward occupational clothing, and occupational appearance requirements are significant in influencing perceptions and attributions in occupational settings. Additionally, it should be noted that as an occupational level increases, occupational prestige also increases. Therefore, if one considers that the variables of occupation and occupa- tional prestige are somewhat similar in what they measure, it is possible to suggest that occupational prestige also represent the life experience of an occupation. Thus, even though Spearman rank order correlations did 93 not relate occupational level to perceptions of appearance/clothing saliency and attributions to the story-situation, one can see through occupational prestige that this condition may exist. From an examination of the analysis, it is possible to conclude that a combination of the hypothesized vari- ables and demographics do result in the derivation of significant discriminant functions. Basically, certain combinations of the predictor variables did distinguish between the saliency of appearance/clothing and attri- butions in the story-situations. When one examines the success of these discriminant functions, it is realized that although they are significant, they are not very useful in dichotomizing individuals. At best, the percentage correctly classified was 72.7. Thus, even the derivation of a significant function does not insure that it will correctly classify a large percentage of the individuals. As mentioned previously, the unequal group sizes hinder the interpretation of the classification table, thus making it difficult to determine the success of a discriminant function. Several factors should be considered in order to de- rive an increased number of significant functions with large percentages correctly classified. First of all, the absence of formality of occupational clothing as a predictor would seem to suggest that this variable might 94 not have been accurately represented or it may just not be important. Perhaps a level of formality scale should have been created and tested for reliability, independent of the research study. The same is true for personal clothing constructs and occupational appearance requirements. Also, employment status as a variable needs to take into account whether an individual had ever worked, the number of occupations s/he had held, the length of time in each and length of time since s/he became unemployed. The presence of the dummy variable of wearing a uniform to work indicates that more precise variables might aid in the power of discrimination. Additionally, as in regression, variables should not overlap in terms of the information they contribute to the function. If occupational prestige were not entered into analysis as a predictor, income and education might have appeared as more powerful discriminators. The researcher can conclude that occupational experience as reflected in personal clothing constructs and appearance requirements are influencing factors on an individual's perceptions of a characters use of appearance/clothing in a hypothetical story-situation. These two variables also influence a person's attributions to key person and to situation. To derive a more 95 successful discriminant function, it would appear that perfection of these predictors is necessary, and there- fore, additional research is required to identify and measure these variables. Limitations of the Findings The SPSS discriminant analysis program used did not have the capacity to handle missing data, therefore, if cases had any variables with missing values they were excluded from the analysis. This resulted in the exclusion of a substantial number of cases from the analysis. Even though each story-situation varied, the clothing of each character was specifically mentioned. As the respondents were exposed to additional story-situations, it is possible that they were more attuned to the clothing cues. If the increased exposure stimulated attention, it might have encouraged the respondents to mention appearance and clothing more frequently in their answers. Also, the presence of previous questions within the questionnaire may have had some influence on the respondents' answers to the open-ended questions. If the researcher had been present during data collection, the respondents' questions and uncertainties about the story-situations could have been clarified. Additionally, if data had been collected in an interview, 96 the researcher could have probed the respondent for further information and insured that the respondents' intentions were clearly recorded. This would have allowed the researcher to make sure that interpretations of the respondents' answers were complete. Suggestions for Further Study If a researcher were to pursue a similar study with these two or other story-situations and discriminant analysis, better results might be obtained with a slightly different combination of demographic and experiental variables. The researcher could consider age of the respondents, number of years on the job, types of previous occupations, etc. It is likely that a grouping of demographics and experiential variables might prove to be a more power discriminator. Addi- tionally, with discriminant analysis, the researcher should consider the effects of group size on the percentage correctly classified. It is likely that groups closer in size would be more successful in dichotomzing individuals. Further analysis of these story—situations might include the derivation of a variable that would span all five of the story-situations. For example, a researcher might examine the group of who continually made attributions to the key person in all story-situations. 97 Similar comparisons could be made between those who always made attributions to others, to the situation or those who responded that clothing was salient or not salient in all stories. Another study could involve an attempt to quantify the degrees of deviancy exhibited by each character's costume. A relationship could exist between the degree of deviancy of the costume, the perception of saliency and the attributions made by the respondents in that story-situation. This would entail developing the variable independent of the situation and testing it for reliability. Additionally, the circumstances within the situation would need to be studied. APPENDICES APPENDIX A PORTION OF QUALITY OF LIFE QUESTIONNAIRE USED IN THIS STUDY 98 INTERPERSONAL SITUATIONS The following five situations may occur. Please respond to the questions following each one and add any comments you would like to make. 10.] Carol read that a local company was hiring workers for their construction crews. Since she had several years experience, she felt confident that she would get a job. After making an appointment for an interview, she arrived at the personnel office wearing a skirt and blouse and was surprised to see that she was the only female in the roomful of applicants. Carol felt that her interview with the personnel director had gone well and was certain that she would be hired. The following day she received a phone call and was told that all the positions on the construction crews had been filled. lO.la How would you have felt if you were Carol? lO.lb th do you think that she was not hired? lO.lc Other comments 99 10.3 Bob got a job working as a typist in an office. At first, he got along well with the other people. He liked to wear good, new clothes to the office. As a result- he spent most of his salary on clothes and was the best dressed person in the office. After a short time. Bob was promoted to the job of receptionist, a job that some of the Older people wanted. They complained to the office manager. He told them that Bob was given the job because he was always so well-dressed, and that it was important to have someone at that job who would make a good impression on the public. l0.3a what do you think of the office manager who promoted Bob because of his appearance? why? lO.3b What do you think of Bob's use of clothing to get ahead on the Job? Why? l0.3c How do you feel about his co-workers and their response to his promotion? lO.3d Other conments 100 10.3 Ann got a job working as a typist in an office. At first. she got along well with the other peOple. She liked to wear good. new clothes to the office. As a result, she spent most of her salary on clothes and was the best dressed person in the office. After a short time, Ann was promoted to the Job of receptionist, a job that some of the older people wanted. They complained to the office manager. He told than that Ann was given the job because she was always so well-dressed, and that it was important to have someone at that Job who would make a good impression on the public. lO.3a what do you think of the office manager who promoted Ann because of her appearance? Why? lO.3b Hhat do you think of Ann's use of clothing to get ahead on the job? Why? l0.3c How do you feel about her co-workers and their response to her promotion? l0.3d Other comments 101 GENERAL CLOTHING INTERESTS This section contains statements on clothing interests which some people have. For each statement, please indicate how much you disagree or agree with the state- ment as a description of YOU. Read each statement, and CIRCLE THE NUMBER that best describes YOUR feelings. For example, circle "1" if you strongly disagree with a statement, circle "3" if your feelings are in between (that is, you equally agree and disagree), and circle "5" if you strongly agree with it. Please be sure to answer every question. 5.4 The way people dress for l 2 3 4 5 a job interview makes a difference in whether or nor they are hired. 5.11 It is important to wear 1 2 3 4 5 clothing that is appro- priate for the occasion. 5.16 People judge your work 1 2 3 4 5 performance by the way that you are dressed. 5.21 The way people dress on 1 2 3 4 5 the job can make a difference in their opportunities for advancement. 5.26 Employers or super- 1 2 3 4 5 visors notice how workers dress on the job. 102 OCCUPATIONAL CLOTHING For many people a large number of hours each day are spent working. In various parts of the questionnaire we ask about your work, and in this part we focus on your occupa- tional clothing. If you work at two jobs, please answer the following questions with respect to your main 'ob, that is, the one in which you spend the most time. If you spend an equal amount of time on two jobs, it is the one which provides the most income. 12.1a Do you wear a uniform for your job? ( ) No > GO TO QUESTION 12.2a ON NEXT PAGE. ( ) Yes 1- 12.lb Please describe the uniform. What garments, styles or colors, or what equipment do you wear? 12.lc Why do you wear a uniform? CHECK AS MANY AS APPLY. Required by employer A v Personal preference Safety Health Custom; generally expected Practical Provided by employer Provides identification Other AAA/\AAAA VVVVVVVV (please specify) 103 12 1e Besides the uniform, are there any appearance requirements for your job? ( ) Yes————) 12.1f Please describe any ()No 12.1g other appearance requirements. Are these requirements specified in writing by the employer? ( ) Yes ( )No ( ) DOES NOT APPLY GO TO QUESTION 12.5a ON THE NEXT PAGE. 12.2a Are people in your position expected by your employer to dress in a particular manner or present a partic- ular appearance for work? ( ) Yes > 12.2b ( )No ( ) DOES NOT APPLY 12.2c If YES, are the require- ments specified in writing? ( ) Yes ()No Please list the dress requirements. What garments, styles or colors, or aspects of appearance are specified or under- stood? 104 12.3 Describe what you usually wear for work. What garments, styles or co ors, or what equipment do you wear? 11.5 13.2a 13.9b 13.7a 105 Are you presently self-employed, employed for pay, either full- or part-time, or are you receiving some pay while temporarily laid off, on strike or on sick leave? ( ) N0 )= GO TO QUESTION 13.1 ON PAGE 33. ( ) Yes > CONTINUE ON TO QUESTION 12.1a ON THE NEXT PAGE. How old were you on your last birthday? Age at last birthday If you are working now OR are temporarily laid off OR on strike OR on sick leave, what kind of work do you do? What is your main occupation called? (If you have two jobs, your main occupation is the job on which you spend the most time. If you spend an equal amount of time on two jobs, it is the one which provides the most income.) Main occupation What is the hi best level of formal schooling that you have completed? CHECK ONE. ( ) Less than 8 grades of elementary school ( ) 8 grades of elementary school ( ) 1-3 years of high school ( ) Completed high school and received diploma or passed high school equivalency exam College graduate, bachelor's degree Post bachelor's course work Master's degree Post master's course work Ph D., Ed.D. Other professional degree (such as MD, DO, JD, DDS): AAA/\AA VVVVVV (pléase specify) 106 13.11b About how much of this total family yearly income do you estimate that YOU will earn in 1977? ESTIMATED PORTION OF TOTAL FAMILY INCOME, 1977, EARNED BY YOURSELF ( ) Does not apply, not employed in 1977 ( ) Under $3,000 ( ) $ 3,000 - $ 3,999 ( ) $12,000 - $14,999 ( ) $ 4,000 - $ 4,999 ( ) $15,000 - $19,999 ( ) $ 5,000 - $ 5,999 ( ) $20,000 - $24,999 ( ) $ 6,000 - $ 6,999 ( ) $25,000 - $29,999 ( ) $ 7,000 - $ 7,999 ( ) $30,000 - $34,999 ( ) $ 8,000 - $ 8,999 ( ) $35,000 - $49,999 ( ) $ 9,000 - $ 9,999 ( ) $50,000 - $74,999 ( ) $10,000 - $11,999 ( ) $80,000 and over APPENDIX B INTERVIEWER PROCEDURES AND FORMS USED IN THE FIELD 107 November, 1977 OAKLAND COUNTY LIFESTYLE Interviewer Instructions TYPE OF INTERVIEHING TECHNIQUE For this study you will not be doing any actual interviewing with a respondent. You will. however, screen households within each area to determine eligibility for placement of questionnaires. and you will be required to return to those households to pick up and verify completion of those questionnaires. ELIGIBLE RESPONDENT/HOUSEHOLD In order for a household to be eligible for placement of questionnaires, the following criteria must be met: 1.; The household must be occupied by a married couple. 2. The couple must have one or more children from five years of age through 18 years of age. 3.) The husband and wife must both consent to filling out a questionnaire. In order for a household to be considered complete. BOTH questionnaires are to be completely filled out and must be accompanied by a signed consent form. RESPONDENT INCENTTVE In order to show their appreciation for respondent's co-operation. Michigan State University will issue a $10.00 check to each family who participates in this study. These checks will be mailed directly to the household approximately four to six weeks after they have completed the questionnaires. Additionally. a summary report of the findings of this research project will be mailed to the- participating households upon completion (this will be a couple of months after receipt of the check.) QUOTA Each area has a quota of four completed households. This means that four husband/wife sets and consent forms will be completed for a total of eight questionnaires per area. SAMPLING PROCEDURE Standard sampling procedure is to be used for this study. Proceed to the corner indicated by a red x on your area mapsheet. Begin at the household indicated in the bottom right-hand corner of your mapsheet. this becomes your first designated household and should be written in on your first call record. If you are unable 108 Oakland County Lifestyle Interviewer Instructions to place the questionnaires at the designated household, you will substitute by going to the residence to the right, then to the left. then by skipping fbur households from your designated one, and continuing this pattern until you have glaced them with an eligible household. Please look at the following examp e 19. Deals .. Omaggogpgmagama This is the pattern that you will fbllow in covering your blocks to determine eligibility for placement. CALLBACKS There are three callbacks required on the first household attempted for each set of questionnaires to be completed. Let's examine some possible field situations. Since you can only place your questionnaires in households meeting certain criteria it would be futile to make three callbacks on a household containing a widow over 65. Hhen you begin. work in an area and run into a no answer at one of your designated households. check with the residence to the right, explain the purpose of your visit and ask if their neighbor meets the eligibility requirements. If they do, you should continue to call on that household; if not, ask the person you are speaking to if they meet the requirements and attempt placement. In other words, screen your neighborhood efficiently for eligible households before attempting callbacks and you will minimize the number of trips made to an area considerably. INTERVIENING HINTS ' Make sure that at least one (either husband or wife) has signed the consent form and is certain that the other spouse will do so befbre leaving the questionnaires. * Stress confidentiality. * Remind respondents that the 510. GO and the summary report will only be sent to households who successfully complete both questionnaires and sign the consent fonn. ' State a specific date and time for pick-up of questionnaires and arrange for both spouses to be present if possible. ' Call your respondents before you return to your area to pick-up the questionnaires. 109 MI" 0' LIFE "00!“. fkl. l!" nip-ico- STAY! uni-mm mama-tn no: out ' “ll-So" M A ta t W= A l‘eflinou inue-asoo ' 3m ‘ 31': contact "a "M ”a" Caplets call rotor-eI gala: call record um self (tale 7. 8. Q or lOll code amn, eaalain purposes of study all incentive pay-ent mm: 1- "Inlet? MIKE—,1eninaso satanic-r! . . 1. have a cane age 3 3353-21: {3103 all record . mtly living in anemia? O . ’0 tum and wife emu, living together in household? I'm. we!“ \ l [_is spouse also none? 1 Siva further mlanatian to soon Give W explanation to one : spous- spouse Obtain written consent from both Quin ‘m‘ h“ one sense? 1.4—)1 rams-sac. mun-aw ___,- spouses? . Careless call recon ; (code a) ft! '3 b 39; leave nest'o laminate naires with sparse int-anion . tanoiasa can F gm.. ”a . 7 Paper clip consent ran- as insioal . record (coca s 21:33 res M. a.“ of front cover of questionnaire . , , or spouse who is res nova. "ace 2.; ”an“ ; Con-m front mm 3) am but - mm s' - Ir envelope. Do m t . g g . “a ‘ on : :chtLrec-sre. a an o ts up data seal Siva spouse who is present his! I“. "9"“ cm '°"" '1‘” 1“ her o-ostiomsira one envelope. 1 ans osoor clip to can record Calm front {coon 1) and back Iv——)l "one to tater-inn if ' 3' of to" m: olan sick as a spouse consentse . M to detonmo if too- are n time “I 1 minced and ready for aica as m , flan another 31‘“ m pics up data ’ hone later to deter-cine 1 and tire "'——"" loo-n to pics to onstionnaires ‘ if ootn are car-elated and ‘ , Open 9' a i:‘ snveloces and edit '0“! '°' pick up l"“.‘." '"f‘""‘“ questionnaires flan ”a” ‘ous. oIIna res : ' l a YB If one soovse nos can- follow recast-seed .rocedures s c up date plated ouostioeeaire. if one or note boostionnairas an and tin ‘ ‘ h . partially or totally inoculate. -——1—-I l::,‘:;,:..u " "- Transfer ouestiornaires to "1‘ a ”"1 V ' ' . . l t e ll recon 3w:id’::z:n:h::‘: a: ' Rat-are to slot as suestionnaires , :r;,:, :h, 2: con. large «(n-.3. Open 9‘ s T? envelopes ans aeie i sent for- " sis-as questionnaires In: use auction-aim 1 Cnecl for both signatures on '. again if no nant :- casent fer- ,» insice W. 1' m , m tut: follo- reco-v-enond mm' if I a m‘:.g;: fl" one 3' 5°“ "UQ'J'CI-flllfl'. 0'. partially or totally metro-eta ‘ “"'" h" m “ ' i j record 1 plate-s Call rec-mi tau-22'." mi tra-w'ar W10,- sNI vu- ngzn «- ; wires to M" a ll‘ -:-w-l'.»- . l "1.0a?" Us alias-Ta: . .r 1‘; aarrl'ifl'. It!" i i I l ' i‘aoer :lip consent '1m and ten. 1 i 1'" .-...... "ace Catamarans?! Victim "I! (val-“urn 'nfilr‘j'sasfl‘. rut-nu fr. “ll-'3. .' 1'“ l. I 110 MIC HIGAN STATE UNXVE R SITY (0|.th 0' WW moor mum-muons November 15. l977 This is to introduce an interviewer from (name of market research agency). interviewer is asking your participation in a study of the quality of life of families in Oakland County. Michigan. The research project and questionnaire have been developed by the Departments of Family and Child Sciences and Human Environment and Design. College of Human Ecology at Michigan State University. ' The project has been funded by the Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station. You and your spouse's cooperation in granting a short interview and in completing self-administered questionnaires will be sincerely appreciated. and your names will in no way be linked to your responses. Sincerely, m. £4! Margar I M. Bubolz. Professor Family and Child Sciences Madame Ann C. Slocum, Assistant Professor Human Environment and Design lll MIC HIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY COlLEG! 0' HOW WY EAST LANSING ' MICHIGAN . 0.1! Fall 1977 CONSENT FORM He, the undersigned. willingly consent to participate in a study about the quality of life of Michigan families. He do so with the understanding that our responses will contribute to the goals of the research project being conducted by the College of Human Ecology at Michigan State University and the Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station. The purposes of the study have been explained to us, and they are repeated in the letter attached to the questionnaire. Thus. we have knowledge of the aspects of the study. He agree to complete the questionnaires as accurately and completely as we are able. He further understand that our names will in no way be linked to the answers we have given. and we reserve the right to withdraw from the study at any time. We desire to participate in this research and consent and agree. PLEASE SIGN YOUR FIRST AND LAST NAMES. Wife‘s Signature Date Husband's Signature Date Street Address City/Town. State Zip Code He. the undersigned. guarantee complete anonymity to the persons whose signatures are above. Their names will in no way be linked to the responses given. we further agree to pay the abovesigned family an amount of $10.00 upon receipt of the two conpleted questionnaires. He will be happy to answer any questions they might have about completing the questionnaires. Please call 5l7-353-5389 or 517-355-1895. ”flag-mm £45.15/ 4“ g, paw 7 o Dr. Margaret M. Bubolz. Professor Dr. Ann C. Slocum. Assistant Professor Family and Child Sciences Human Environment and Design APPENDIX C CODING PROCEDURES FOR SELECTED RESPONSES TO STORY—SITUATIONS 112 CONSTRUCTION WORKER STORY-SITUATION Attributions to Key Person as Causal Agent "Because she was a female." ”She did not project the image of a construction worker.” ”Maybe not physically strong enough for jobs available." "Would like to believe she was not hired because her experience did not equate to those who were." Attributions to Others as Causal Agents "Better qualified applicants.” "The other applicants had more experience." "Someone had better experience than her.” ”Evidently, the interviewer felt that Carol was not the best person for the job offered." Attributions to the Situation as Causal Agent "The obvious is that the employer was biased in his assessment of Carol as her appearance was femimine." At the time they (company) didn't want to hire any females." Appearance/Clothing Saliency Dimension l. Appearance/clothing is salient ”Because her appearance did not reflect that of a construction worker.” "Her appearance may have given her the look of not being able to do the job." 113 2. Appearance/clothing is salient qualified. ”Perhaps she looked too femimine and they thought her not capable of outdoor work." 3. Appearance/clothing is not salient. ”I don't think the way she dressed cost her the job.” 4. Appearance/clothing is not salient qualified. ”You are trying to imply that Carol was not hired because she wore a skirt and blouse. I don't feel that is the case. There might have been something else about her person that was wrong." BOB/ANN TYPIST STORY-SITUATION Attributions to Key Person as Causal Agent "If the receptionist job was her goal she used her dress very appropriate." "I think Ann did the right thing, if that's what it took to get ahead." Attributions to Others as Causal Agents "They (co-workers) had put good years into the company and hard work, but that with family and bills they had, how they dress was what they could afford." "Manager should never admitted promotion based on clothes." "They (co-workers) should have dressed like Bob." ”I think he (office manager) promoted Bob because he got along well with people." 114 ”I think he was a good manager who was concerned about the company." Attributions to the Situation as Causal Agent "Having time and experience should entitle you to a better job before others trying to buy their way up." "Seniority is of value." Appearance/Clothing Saliency Dimension l. Appearance/clothing is salient ”That job should be filled by someone who cared about his appearance." ”Good appearance is important.” 2. Appearance/clothing is salient qualified ”I think a person should look neat, but he also should have the qualifications.” "A person should make a good employee first and how they dress should be second.” "Bob likes clothes and the job requires it." 3. Appearance/clothing is not salient qualified ”Clothes alone don't get the job done." APPENDIX D CODING PROCEDURE FOR FORMALITY OF OCCUPATIONAL DRESS 115 FORMALITY OF OCCUPATIONAL DRESS Men Sweatshirt, tee-shirt or work shirt and/or work pants, jeans or Levis or coveralls or special sport clothing. 2 Shirt or sport shirt and slacks with no mention of tie or sports coat (shirt or sport shirt worn with jean, Levis or work pants coded "1"). 3 Shirt, slacks and special jacket or sports coat with no mention of tie or shirt, tie and slacks with no mention of sports coat. 4 Shirt, slacks, jacket and tie (no mention of suit). 5 Choice of suit or sports coat with tie (tie assumed if not specified). 6 Suit and tie, business suit, 3-piece suit, or vested suit. Women 1 Jeans, Levis, special sport clothing. 2 Slacks with blouse, top or sweater (uniforms called "pantsuits" were assumed to be slacks and tops). 3 Choice of pantsuit (i.e., slacks and top or dress). 4 Shirt and top or dress (no mention of slacks or pantsuit). BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Brim, Orville G. Jr., "Socialization Through the Life Cycle." in Socialization After Childhood: Two Essays, pp. 3-49. Editedby Orville G. Brim Jr. and Stanton Wheeler. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc. 1966. Buckley, Hilda Mayer, and Roach, Mary Ellen. "Clothing as a Nonverbal Communicator of Social and Political Attitudes." Home Economics Research Journal 3 (December l974):94-102. Clausen, John A. (ed.) Socialization and Society. Boston: Little, Brown and Company, I968. Conner, Barbara Hunt, Peters, Kathleen, and Nagasawa, Richard H. "Person and Costume: Effects on the Formation of First Impressions.” Home Economics Research Journal 4 (September I975): 32-41. Cooley, William W., and Lohnes, Paul R. Multivariate Procedures for the Behavioural Sciences. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., I962. Douty, Helen 1. "Influence of Clothing on Perception of Persons." Journal of Home Economics 55 (l963):l97-202. Fishbein, Martin. "Attitude, Attitude Change and Behaviour A Theoretical Overview." in Attitude Research Bridges the Atlantic, pp. 3-I6. Edited by Philip Levine. American Marketing Association, 1975. Form, W. H. and Stone, G. P. "The Social Significance of Clothing in Occupational Life.” Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station Technical Bulletin, 247, East Lansing, Michigan, 1955. 116 117 Godfrey, Lina Rowe. "The Importance of Clothing and Appearance as Influential Factors in an Interviewing Situation for a Secretarial Position." Unpublished Master's Problem, Michigan State University, 1965. Hamid, Paul N. ”Changes in Person Perception as a Function of Dress." Perceptual and Motor Skills 29 (l969):l91-194. Hamid, Paul N. "Some Effects of Dress Cues on Observa- tional Accuracy, A Perceptual Estimate and Impression Formation." Journal of Social ngchology 86 (1972):279-289. Heider, Fritz. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: Wiley and Sons, I958: Heider, Fritz. "Perceiving the Other Person." in Person Perception and Interpersonal Behaviour, pp. 22-36. Editedlby Renato Taguiri. Stanford: Stanford University, Press, 1958. Hollander, Edwin F. Principles and Methods of Social Ps cholo . New York: oxford University Press, l571. Johnson, Barbara Hunt, Nagasawa, Richard H. and Peters, Kathleen. "Clothing Style Differences: Their Effect on the ImpreSsion of Socialability.‘ Home Economics Research Journal 6 (1977): 58-63. Jones, Edward E., David, Keith E., and Gergen, Kenneth J. ”Role Playing Variations and Their Informational Value for Person Perception.” in Readin s in Experimental Social Psychology, pp. I-l5. Edited by Anthony N. Doob and Dennis T. Regan. New York: Meredith Corporation, 1971. Jones, Susan. "Hiring Agents' Perceptions of Appearance Norms Used by Their Organizations to Judge Female Employees in White Collar Positions During the Interview and Daily Work Situations.” Unpublished Master's Thesis, Auburn University, 1972. Kelley, Eleanor, Good, Evelyn, and Walter, Sarah. "Working Class Adolescents' Perceptions of the Role of Clothing in Occupational Life." Adolescence 9 (l974):185-199. 118 Ketch, Karen Walsh. "Men's Perceptions of Sex Role and Dress in Projective Occupational Story- Situations." Unpublished Master's Thesis, Michigan State University, 1979. Klecka, William R. "Discriminant Analysis.” in Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. pp. 434-462. Edited by norman Nie et al. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc. 1975. McGuire, William G. "The Nature of Attitudes and Attitude Change." in Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 3. Edited by Gardener Lindzey and Eliot Aronson. Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 1969. Molloy, John T. Dress for Success. New York: Warner Communications Company, 1975. Molloy, John T. The Women's Dress for Success Book. New York: Warner Communications Company, 1977. Moore, Wilbert. "Occupational Socialization." in Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research. Edited by David A. Goslin. Chicago: Rand McNally and Company, 1969. Morrison, Donald G. "On the Interpretation of Dis- criminant Analysis.” Journal of Marketing Research 6 (l969):156-163T Nelson, Rogene Siert. "Clothing Preferences and Appearance Perceptions of Female Executive Secretaries in Relation to Secretarial Roles.’ Unpublished Master's Thesis, University of Nebraska, 1975. Newcomb, Theodore M. "The Cognition of Persons as Cognizers." in Person Perception and Inter- Personal Behaviour. Edited'by Renato Tagiuri and Luigi Petrullo. Stanford: Stanford University, 1958. Newcomb, T. M., Turner, R. H., and Conserve, P. E. Social Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Riemer, J. W. "Becoming a Journeyman Electrician." Sociology of Work and Occupations 4 (1977lz87198. 119 Secord, Paul F. and Backman, Carl W. Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1964li Shaver, Kelly C. An Introduction to Attributions Processes. Massachusetts: Winthrop Publishers, Inc., 1975. Taguiri, Renato. ”Person Perception." in Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 3. Edited by Gardner Lindzey and Eliot Aronson. Massachusetts: Addison—Wesley Publishing Company, 1969. Thorndike, Robert M. Correlational Procedures for Research. New York: Gafdher Press, Inc., 1978. U.S. Bureau of the Census. Statistical Abstract of the United States 1977. 98th ed. Washington, D.C.: 1977. Wanous, John P. Organizational Entry. Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 1980. Wood, S. M. "Uniform - It's Significance as a Factor in Role Relationships." Sociological Review 14 (l966):l39-151. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES 1111111111111111111111111111111111111111111111111111111111111111 31293102473737