SOCEAL GRGANEZAUON 0F WEE WESTERN FOX SQUIRREL Thesis for the Degree of M. S. MECHMN STATE UNWERSEY ROBERT .EOHN BERNARD 1&79 VI in u IIJ3II mnmwmflwmIllgmuyl ,2; PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this chockout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before duo duo. DATE DUE RATE DUE DATE DUE 39.29199: 1” ‘ V“: ‘/l ‘..r i 1‘5— /L’" :nAZZT mm 5941 BP/RiiOésStZGIB 12m. . , , ' APR18200E / - D74 06 05 Tsunmm u — -~ ABSTRACT SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF THE WESTERN FOX SQUIRREL By Robert John Bernard During a two-year study of a discrete wild population of fox squirrels, the existence of an intricate and stable hierarchal form of social structure was documented. Animals were ranked by both the percentile and tabular method of hierarchy delineation, based on interactions obtained at an artificial feeder during consecutive winters. The hierarchy was a straight line mixed sex arrangement with males generally more dominant than females. Hierarchal status advanced with increased age. Extent of minimum home range did not increase with social rank. It was concluded that where food and shelter are adequate, spatial requirements would not limit population numbers. Inventories of two discrete populations of fox squirrels were made by trapping at a single central feeder during the time of winter food scarcity. Population estimates based on a seven day trapping period at the feeder compared favorably with the actual woodlot squirrel populations. SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF THE WESTERN FOX SQUIRREL By Robert John Bernard A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Fisheries and Wildlife T972 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to express special thanks to Dr. Leslie w. Gysel, who while providing guidance and support allowed freedom to pursue those aspects of the study which seemed most significant. For critical review of the manuscript I am grateful to Dr. John A. King and Dr. Rollin H. Baker. Particular thanks is accorded wayne Schmidt and James Kessel for the many cold hours they spent assisting with behavioral observations. My deepest appreciation goes to my wife, Christine, for without her enthusiastic moral and physical support, completion of this study could not have become a reality. This research was financially supported by the Agricultural Experiment Station of Michigan State University. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page PART I SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF THE WESTERN FOX SQUIRREL INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 MATERIALS AND METHODS . . . . . . . . . . . ........ 3 RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ........ 7 DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ...... 20 LITERATURE CITED. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 PART II APPLICATION OF'A SIMPLE METHOD FOR CENSUSING FOX SQUIRRELS INTRODUCTION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 MATERIALS AND METHODS . . . . . . . . . . . . ...... . 3l RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ......... 36 LITERATURE CITED. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 111 TABLE LIST OF TABLES PART I Social hierarchy of lO fox squirrels ranked by the linear method, Hudson Hoodlot. Michigan State University, 197l. . . . . . . . . . . . ...... Social hierarchy of lO fox squirrels ranked by the percentile formula. Hudson Noodlot. Michigan State University, 197l. . . . . . . . . . . . ...... Social hierarchy of 13 fox squirrels ranked by the linear method, Hudson Noodlot, Michigan State University, l972. . . . . . . . . . . ....... Social hierarchy of 13 fox squirrels ranked by the percentile formula, Hudson Noodlot, Michigan State University, l972. . . . . . . . . . . ....... Average social rank for categories of fox squirrels. iv Page 12 T3 T4 15 18 FIGURE LIST OF FIGURES PART I A basket feeder filled with ear corn was used to draw squirrels for both trapping purposes and behavioral observations. . . . . . . ....... Bob (tap) initiates an aggressive threat and displaces the subdominant Oscar from his position on the feeder . . . . . . . . ...... Dominants were intolerant of other squirrels on the feeder tree; however. on the ground they usually fed together peaceably . . . . . . . . . . Effect of age on social rank for the l972 Hudson Noodlot population of fox squirrels, (PHQZH >m mmamoeua> ao >uzmsomma “Xv Sumxaaom SHQZH _ .FNoF .zuwmgo>r:: ouaam camrcurx .uopuooz comes: .uozuoe Lumcpp one an umxcac apoccrscm xom op we xgugucmv; paruom .p mpnop 13 Table 2. Social hierarchy of 10 fox squirrels ranked by the percentile formula, aHudson Noodlot, Michigan State University, 1971. Individual Squirrel (X) No. of No. of Squirrels Squirrels Victori- Social X Victori- ous Rank Name Tag No. Sex Age ous Over over X 1 Bob 11 M A 9 0 2 Gene 9 M A 7 1 3 Oscar 989 M A 4 2 4 Irene 7 F J 4 3 5 Niles 996 M J 3 3 6 George 8 M A 2 3 7 Nancy 5 F J 2 3 8 Tom 993 M J l 5 9 Kathy 995 F J 1 7 10 Sonya 991 F J O 6 aPercentile social rank of individual calculated by formula: x - (A + B)/2 when A . percentage of squirrels encountered that X dominates B - 100 percent minus the percentage of squirrels dominating X. l4 o m.F m mom azuax mF F F m. u so chcma NF N N nun m. m wN Fggmsm FF o F F a m. z mN usz oF m is: F m m m. z mN cgoa m m in: an: F e e o.F u mF mcosFm m n is: F F F F m m.F u NN coma 5 mm F m 5 OF F e NF o.F z m¢ usazo: m mm F m m w a m m m o.N 2 Ne coo m Nm F a m m m N m m e o.N z m— vFogn: c mN F F N ¢ m F m m N a o.N z mam sch m RN F F F N m w m N m N m m.N : mma Laumo N w nun F in: in: us: run n.- F in: in- F F m.N : m mama F .02 3:3- AKHMM mEcmb rsbwsm “PM: csmn. OCOEFM cow—a ULGIOI camp “:95: EOF Laumo mam” wm< xmm GE mez FmFUOm zoz mzoFFoFozF >m mmFNOFQF> mo >uzmzomxu va ANNNFaom 4<=oF>FozF .NNmF .qumLm>F:= macaw camFgqu .quuooz comes: .vozums LaocFF mg» »n cognac mFocLF:om on mF mo xgueucoF; FmFuom .N anoF 15 Table 4. Social hierarchy of 13 fox squirrels ranked by the percentile formula, aHudson Uoodlot, Michigan State University, 1972. Individual Squirrel (X) No. of No. of Squirrels Squirrels Victori- Social Minimum Age X Victori- ous Rank Name Tag No. Sex (years) ous Over over X 1 Gene 9 M 2.5 4 O 2 Oscar 989 M 2.5 11 1 3 Tom 993 M 2.0 10 2 4 Harold 16 M 2.0 9 2 5 Don 42 M 2.0 8 3 6 Howard 43 M 1.0 7 5 7 Jean 22 F 1.5 5 5 8 Simone 15 F 1.0 3 6 9 John 26 M .5 3 7 10 Halt 29 M .5 3 8 11 Sherri 28 F .5 l 8 12 Jennie 47 F .5 1 9 13 Kathy 995 F 1.5 O 7 aPercentile social rank of individual calculated by formula: X - (A + B)/2 when A 8 percentage of squirrels encountered that X dominates B - 100 percent minus the percentage of squirrels dominating X. 16 period in 1971, a parallel to Sharp's observations evolved. With one exception, the sequence of capture followed hierarchal rank fairly well, with the subdominants being caught first and the dominants later in the trapping period. Therefore, results obtained by trapping around a feeder would probably not reflect the true age or sex structure of the population being sampled early in the trapping period. A solution is to allow the feeder to become depleted after a few days of trapping, thus forcing even the most dominant animals to enter the traps for food. Hierarchal Structure: Hierarchal rank was markedly affected by both age and sex of the members. Table 5 lists the average rank for four categories for 1971 and 1972. In both years adults ranked significantly higher than Juveniles, and males ranked higher than females. Since more accurate data regarding age structure was available for the 1972 population, a correlation of age with social rank was made (Figure 4) and found to be highly significant at the 1 percent level. Home Range: Allen (1943) concluded that any "cut and dried" approach to home range determination is more likely to be misleading than helpful due in part to seasonal modifications and food availability. The efficacy of home range determination by trapping is also questioned by Hayne (1949a) and Flyger (1960) who note that home range increases with the number of captures. Few squirrels in the 1971 hierarchy were trapped or seen frequently enough for home range determinations; however, in 1972 trap- ping and observation data from November, 1971 to May, 1972 were used to delineate home ranges for 11 animals. A correlation of home range with social rank was made (Figure 5) and found to be nonsignificant. 17 Table 5. Average social rank for categories of fox squirrels. Average Social Rank 1971a 1972b Linear Percentile Linear Percentile Adultsc 2.75 3.12 5.0 5.0 Juveniles 7.33 7.08 9.3 9.3 Males 4.50 4.25 5.0 5.0 Females 7.00 7.22 10.2 10.2 aBased on 10 squirrels bBased on 13 squirrels cAge 1.5 or older 18 l3m- 1| 12_- 1. IIP- <0 rs=.787 th- To ca» 59" i. _1 LL] E; T 8- 10 D {532% l’—- 0» t5;::: §§6- - as: .J 3:: 5*" 1| 58 §°4~ - 3- To 2.. T. l- I. l j l J J .5 II) L5 2K) 215 AGEIN YEARS Figure 4. Effect of age on social rank for the 1972 Hudson Woodlot Population of fox squirrels. (P<0.01). SOCIAL RANK OF SQUIRRELS 19 ll - e rs =.286 DECREASING RANK --- l~ o l 1 1 1 1 l 1' 1.0 2.0 3.0 4.0 5.0 6.0 7.0 HOME RANGE IN ACRES Figure 5. Effect of size of minimum home range on social rank for the 1972 Hudson Noodlot population of fox squirrels.-(n.s.). 20 Breeding Behavior: Unfortunately, breeding behavior was never observed during the two winter observation periods. Dense sugar maple reproduction significantly reduced visibility around the blind, and this, along with the extreme wariness of the animals, reduced opportunities to observe mating behavior. Pack gt_al, (1967) noted that for gray squirrels, high ranking males are involved in most sexual contacts and probably leave more offspring than subdominant males. It is also quite likely that fox squirrel dominants would, as in feeding, maintain precedence in mating encounters. Heather: Attempts to correlate specific climatic conditions with movements or social rank were unsuccessful due to the multiplicity of factors induced by capricious Michigan winters. Activity on a given day seemed to be inextricably linked to weather on the previous days and days to come. A good example of this occurred on February 5, 1972, when winds were gusting to 43 knots and several different animals of high and low rank appeared at the feeder. This was the first day of a severe storm, and squirrels seemed to be aware of the consequence of waiting it out on an empty stomach. 0n the other hand, on many relatively warm days few squirrels were seen at the feeder. 1 DISCUSSION A well-defined hierarchy very likely exists in fox squirrel populations, due to normal competition, though the feeder used to 21 concentrate squirrels may generate unusual social pressures and cause significant refinement of the structure. Some animals whose home range might infrequently overlap, are drawn to the feeder and exposed to one another for the first time.(:The author witnessed this first meeting on several occasions when two squirrels (of low rank) displayed fear of one another. After a relatively long period of avoidance behavior on the ground, one would rush at the other. Subsequent interactions were accomplished without delay, and reversals were almost never noted::] This implies that strong individual recognition is necessary to maintain a dominance hierarchy, a tenet supported by Guhl and Ortman (1953) in their work on domestic chickens, as well as by Flyger (1955) and Pack 33 21, (1967) in their gray squirrel studies. Overall weather conditions varied a great deal during the two study periods. The winter of 1971 was harsh both in terms of protracted cold temperatures and snowfall. Over a foot of hard crusted snow covered the ground from mid-December to mid-March; a condition that effectively denied squirrels access to their normal food supply. However, during the second winter little snow fell, and frequent warm spells reduced the snow cover to a minimum or to a nonexistent condition, thus allowing animals to exploit their normal food sources. Heather conditions over a long period of time seemed to affect squirrel behavior in hierarchy formation to some extent. The severity of the first winter prompted early formation and led to very consistent behavior patterns. Interactions were always swiftly accomplished and the victor nearly always took his position on the feeder. In 1972, however, observations began earlier and overall weather conditions were far less severe. 22 Another pertinent factor in hierarchy formation was the large component of very young squirrels (6) in the 1972 population, animals with less experience. These young animals were at first unsure of their positions, but soon adjusted quite well into the social structure. Frequently, low ranking animals were observed approaching the feeder, but would leave when they saw dominants were already present. Often low ranking animals were seen as much as 100 yards from cover in the corn field searching for waste corn, and in this position were extremely vulnerable to predation. On several occasions animals were run down by the author on foot, a testimonial to their vulnerability. In all probability, cobs returned to the woodlot from the field by low ranking animals could be usurped by the dominants. This was not actually observed, but circumstantial evidence substantiates that it occurs, and lends credence to the belief that dominance is of strong survival value. Day-to-day weather conditions did not appear to affect squirrel activity, at least in any consistent predictable manner, for both high and low ranking animals would appear at the feeder during a variety of weather conditions. while dominants would feed to repleteness in one hour and then depart, low ranking animals would sometimes remain three or four hours eating discarded fragments and awaiting access to the feeder. Certainly, there is survival value in dominance for exposure to cold and predation are significantly reduced. During the second winter a few animals seemed to prefer eating on the ground rather than on the tree, and careful Judgment was required to discern who was truly the dominant. This tendency decreased as the winter progressed and the "normal” behavior of feeding on the tree 23 became the rule. Much of the abnormal activity centered around Howard, an adult male whose origin was probably associated with urban living. He was the only squirrel during two years of observations who would be aware and tolerant of human presence without rapid departure. Irene, the highest ranking female in 1971, was extremely antago- nistic toward other subdominants. Oddly enough, Simone, the highest ranking female in 1972, was also a despot and would not allow some subdominants to feed even on the ground under the feeder. These two animals would sometimes chase other squirrels from the vicinity of the feeder, behavior that could be mistakenly interpreted as territorial to the casual observer. Actual conflict between squirrels was rare, and interactions were quickly and smoothly accomplished. Sometimes one animal would chase another, but the subdominant was clearly established and always exhibited avoidance behavior. Of 12 animals captured in January, 1971, only four remained in the January, 1972 population of 20 animals. This attrition rate is certainly severe but quite plausible considering the number of predators present in the woodlot. The extent of emigration was unknown, but considered to be small. Observation during the second winter enabled the gathering of additional data to substantiate fox squirrel hierarchal behavior and changes in the structure over the course of a year. In addition, after one year of trapping, animals could be aged more accurately and the relationship of age to hierarchal rank more easily determined. Upward shifts in rank did seem to occur with age particularly with males. Gene, who was second in 1971, became the most dominant animal 24 in the woodlot. Oscar moved from third to second place, and Tom, who was number 9, moved up to third place the second winter. Kathy, however, who was third from the bottom in 1971, dr0pped to the bottom position in 1972. When trapped, Kathy appeared to be in good physical condition and was known to have carried a summer litter of young animals, so was apparently normal physiologically. The results are inconclusive, but tenure is probably an important factor in achieving high social rank, though aggressiveness is likely linked to genetic factors and increased age and experience does not guarantee advancement. The percentile method of hierarchy determination as pr0posed by Komai 9531. (1959) and utilized by Pack $1391. (1969) and Ozoga (1972) is a useful method of defining hierarchy structure. Although it is easy to use, generally requires fewer observations, and takes into account hierarchal inconsistencies, it does have one shortcoming; it assumes that a given animal will have a heterogeneous exposure to other members of the hierarchy. This situation may not occur because of non-random feeding habits. For example, in the 1971 hierarchy, Tom is ranked above Kathy by the percentile method, but in actuality was dominated by Kathy. The tabular method in this case is correct while the percentile method is in error. These inconsistencies, however, are few and small in magnitude, as evidenced by the close correlations obtained by the two methods. The elaborate and precise hierarchal system developed by fox squirrels allows the conclusion that they are social animals. Fisler's (1969:28) contention that ”the concepts of territory and hierarchy should be considered as opposite extremes of a continuum of mammalian organizational systems," leads to the conclusion that fox squirrels 25 with their intricate hierarchies probably exhibit very little territoriality or suffer intraspecific restrictions on their home ranges. The poor correlation between social rank and home range strongly implies that this conclusion is correct. One general characteristic of territories or home ranges is that more food exists within the occupied area than is utilized (Calhoun 1952). This restriction of the privilege of occupying space irrespective of food abundance imposes limitations to population growth (Davis 1950). Fox squirrels travel long distances during periods of winter food shortage (Allen 1943), which would necessitate crossing territorial boundaries, if they existed, to obtain access to a point source of food such as a corn field. The social hierarchy arrange- ment allows exploitation of an abundant food source by many individuals rather than the restrictive use by only one animal as in a territorial system. In short, a hierarchy system allows a greater number of animals to occupy a given area than does a territorial system. If adequate food and shelter are available, spatial requirements would probably not restrict population numbers of fox squirrels, certainly a very important management implication. 26 LITERATURE CITED Allen, D. H. 1943. Michigan Fox Squirrel Management. Mich. Dept. of Cons., Lansing, Michigan. 404 pp. Bakken, A. A. 1952. Interrelationship of Sciurus carolinensis (Gmelin) and Sciurus niger (Linnaeus) Tn mixed populations. PhD Thesis. Univ. Hisc., Madison, Hisc. 188 pp. Calhoun, J. B. 1952. The social aspects of population dynamics. J. Mammal. 33(2):139-159. Davis, D. E. 1950. Malthus - a review for game managers. J. Hildl. Mgmt. l4(2):180-183. Davis, D. E. 1964. The physiological analysis of aggressive behavior. Pp. 53-74. In Hilliam Etkin (Editor), Social behavior and organization among vertebrates. Univ. Chicago Press. 307 pp. Fisler, G. F. 1969. Mammalian organizational systems. Los Angeles County Museum of Nat. Hist. Publ. 167. 32 pp. Fitzwater, H. 0. Jr. 1943. Color marking of mammals with special reference to squirrels. J. Hildl. Mgmt. 7(2):l90-192. Flyger, V. F. 1955. Implications of social behavior in gray squirrel management. Trans. N. Am. Hildl. Conf. 20:381-389. Flyger, V. F. 1960. Movements and home range of the gray squirrel Sciurus carolinensis, in two Maryland woodlots. Ecology Guhl, A. M. and L. L. Ortman. 1953. Visual patterns in the recognition of individuals among chickens. Condor 55:287-298. Gysel, L. H. 1971. Beechnut production in Michigan. J. Hildl. Mgmt. 35(3):5l6-519. Hayne, D. H. 1949a. Calculation of size of home range. J. Mammal. 30(1):l-18. Hicks, E. A. 1942. Some major factors affecting the use of two inventory methods applicable to the western fox squirrel, ‘ Sciurus ni er rufiventer (Geoffrey). Iowa State Coll. Jour. SCI. I6: 9 -3 5. 27 Komai, T., J. V. Craig, and S. Hearden. 1959. Heritability and repeatability of social aggressiveness in the domestic chicken. Poultry Sci. 38(2):356-359. Mohr, C. 0. 1947. Table of equivalent populations of North American small mammals. Amer. Midl. Nat. 37(1):223-249. Ozoga, J. J. 1972. Aggressive behavior of white-tailed deer at winter cuttings. J. Hildl. Mgmt. 36(3):861-868. Pack, J. C., H. S. Mosby and P. B. Seigel. 1967. Influence of social hierarchy on squirrel behavior. J. Hildl. Mgmt. 31(4):720-728. Sharp, H. M. 1958. The art and technique of live-trapping gray squirrels. Penn. Coop. Hildl. Res. Unit Report No. 3, Penn. State Univ., University Park, Pa. Seigel. S. 1956. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. McGraw-Hill Book Co., New York. 312 pp. Smith, C. 1968. The adaptive nature of social organization in the genus of tree squirrels, Tamiasciurus. Ecol. Mono. 38:31-63. Stokes, A. H., and D. F. Balph. 1965. The relation of animal behavior to wildlife management. Trans. N. Am. Hildl. and Nat. Resources Conf. 30:401-410. PART II APPLICATION OF A SIMPLE METHOD FOR CENSUSING FOX SQUIRRELS 28 APPLICATION OF A SIMPLE METHOD FOR CENSUSING FOX SQUIRRELS The development of efficient and expedient census methods has been the subject of much concern and distress for wildlife investi- gators. Arboreal mammals such as squirrels are particularly difficult to census, because many of the usual methods relying on vocalization, tracks or habitat disturbance are unworkable. Most commonly, squirrel populations have been measured by direct trapping or some additional modification involving sight records and/or tag returns from hunting, generally time-consuming methods. ‘Friley (1955), using live traps to capture squirrels, found the Lincoln Index (Davis 1963:107) method of calculating preseason hunting populations of fox squirrels (Sgigrg§_ £1993) more reliable for indicating trends in animal numbers than per-unit effort trapping success. Leaf nest counts as an index of squirrel abundance, were used by Goodrum (1937), though he expressed dissatisfaction with the results in some habitats, as did Allen (1943) in all Michigan situations. Allen (1943) used in his studies direct trapping generally applied in a grid pattern to monitor population levels. Flyger (1959) compared three census methods, two using the Schnabel (1938) modification of the Lincoln Index, and the third based upon time-area counts as proposed by Goodrum (1940). The first was based on trapping results, the second on the visually determined ratio of marked to unmarked animals (trap-sight records) obtained during observations. He found the latter gave the most satisfactory results. Nixon 35 21, (1967) also used grid trapping results for their comparison of various mathematical predictions of population numbers. They con- cluded that assumptions must be made which may not be true in regard 29 30 to trap response but that, in lieu of better methods, mathematical functions may be used to produce population estimates. Sharp (1958) took advantage of the social hierarchy described by Flyger (1955) and lack of territoriality in gray squirrels to attract animals with a feeder to a central location for trapping. He emphasized that con- ditioning animals by prebaiting is the key to success in trapping. The intent of this paper is to report the results obtained using the same procedure for censusing fox squirrels, where sight records of previously trapped and marked animals provided a Judgment as to the efficacy of the method. STUDY AREA Two woodlots on the farm property of Michigan State University in Ingham County comprised the study area. These woodlots were nearly equal in both size and species composition. Toumey Hoodlot, the first, is a 20-acre old-growth beech-maple stand surrounded by pasture land, while Hudson Hoodlot located R-mile away is an l8-acre all-aged beech- maple forest. Field corn is usually planted on the north and west boundaries of Hudson Hoodlot, while pasturage is the main use for the areas adjacent to the south and east boundaries. Both woodlots contain wild, relatively discrete, unhunted populations of fox squirrels. Their wildness could be a result of predator pressure and infrequent human activity in the area. The woodlots contain few mast trees other than the sporadically producing American beech (Fagus grandifolia) (Gysel 1971) and consequently do not normally carry high populations of squirrels. In Michigan wintering squirrels often rely on auxiliary food supplies such as field corn (Fouch 1961). 31 METHODS Trapping activities began in July, 1970 in Toumey Hoodlot and continued until March of 1972, for a duration of 21 months. Hudson Hoodlot trapping was initiated in January, 1971 and continued 17 months until May of 1972. Trapping was done on a year-round basis during nearly every month of the study periods and was accomplished by two basic methods. The first was an application of the feeder system advocated by Sharp (1958) for gray squirrels to attract the animals to the traps at a given location. The second was an evenly distributed grid system wherein each trap serviced an area of .90 acres. The grid provided an opportunity to test the distance squirrels would travel to some central location such as a feeder for trapping. The feeders were simply basket arrangements made of chicken wire and wood which could hold a bushel of ear corn (Sharp 1958). When hung on a tree, they were readily accessible and visible to squirrels within the woodlot and could provide food without service for long periods of time. Six traps were placed in a semi-circle below the feeder and also baited with ear corn. The traps were of a collapsible metal (wire) variety which measured 9 x 9 x 24 inches. As Sharp (1958) noted, the behavioral mannerisms of squirrels at the feeders make their capture possible. Fox squirrels have a hierarchal form of social arrangement and do not appear to suffer intraspecific restrictions on their home ranges (Bernard 1973). This makes feasible trapping at a central location without fear that territorial boundaries might restrict animal movements to a central location. Five feeding stations were maintained for much of the time in each woodlot, and at selected III I l I 32 intervals four were emptied to test whether animals would transfer their activity to one central feeder. Allen (1943) observed that fox squirrels usually travel over a ten-acre area on a seasonal basis. Hhen five feeders were operating, each serviced approximately four acres, coverage thought to be adequate to attract most squirrels in the woodlots. Hhen only one central feeder was operating, the maxi- mum distance a squirrel in the woodlot would have to travel became 800 feet. In addition to the grid and feeder methods of trapping, another popu1ation monitor was maintained. Both woodlot populations were being observed as part of a behavioral study, and the identity of nearly all animals was established. All animals captured were ear tagged and most were dyed with Nyanzol A (Fitzwater 1943) in a characteristic pattern that allowed visual identification of individuals. Much time was spent in the woodlots, particularly from January through March, making behavioral observations, so that sight ratios of marked to unmarked animals could be maintained. Hhen no unmarked animals were seen or trapped, the population was considered to be inventoried. Therefore, both trap and sight records were used to determine what was considered to be an efficacious estimate of the actual number of squirrels in the respective populations (Figs. 1 and 2). The minimum number of squirrels known to be alive in a given month was determined by adding the number seen or trapped that month to those marked during an earlier trapping period but not seen or recaptured until some subsequent date (Figs. 1 and 2). It is felt that the population estimates, especially during January through March, very nearly represented the actual number of animals present since trapping was frequent and observations nearly continuous. 33 Number of Squirrels on I 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 4 1 L 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 J A S 0 N D J F M A M J J A S 0 N D J F M 1970 l97l 1972 —— Actual population based on grid trapping, feeder trapping,and visual observation. Population assessment based on seven days trapping at a single central feeder. ' Figure l. Fluctuation of the Toumey Hoodlot fox squirrel population showing estimate based on feeder trapping. 34 20" ........... .3. .......... ........ ...... ......... Number of Squirrels ................ -.-.;.:.:.:. .... !-:-.-.-.-.-. 1 I J J J J_ J_ J J L l J J x J F M A M J J A S O N D J F M A‘ M 1971 [972 -———- Actual population based on grid trapping, feeder trapping ,ond visual observation. Papulalion assessment based on six days trapping at a single central feeder. Figure 2. Fluctuations of the Hudson Hoodlot fox squirrel population, showing estimate based on feeder trapping. 35 The question to be answered was whether animals could be induced to come to a single point source of food and thereby become trapped with a minimum of effort, with the attendant conviction that a representative proportion of the population had been captured. A11 grid stations and feeders except the central one in each woodlot were depleted at least two weeks prior to trapping at the single central feeder. This allowed squirrels time to transfer their feeding forays to the single remaining feeder. Here, trapping periods varied in length from four to seven days during the difficult winter period for squirrels (Figs. 1 and 2). During the second half of these periods, the central feeder was also emptied of corn to ensure that the only source of corn was contained in the traps. The trapping periods were not continuous but were in part chosen in response to weather conditions. Trapping was always preceded by a conditioning period where traps were propped open and animals allowed free access to bait (corn) within the traps. RESULTS A short trapping period of 7 days or less during periods of winter food scarcity proved efficient for capturing nearly all animals present in the woodlots (Figs. 1 and 2). The Hudson Hoodlot papulation was inventoried during 1971 and 1972, and the Toumey animals only in 1972. In Toumey, trapping at the single feeder for seven days yielded seven animals (88%), when the actual population was considered to be eight. All the animals were taken during the first four days of trapping. The January, 1971 Hudson Hoodlot population was actually twelve animals, and the estimate based on six trap days was eleven animals or 92% of 36 the population. In March of 1972 the Hudson population was considered to be 17 animals, and the feeder estimate was 14 animals, or 82% of the actual population. This estimate was based on six trap—days, though all squirrels were caught by the fifth day. Animals which had previously been taken at one or more of the other four feeders readily transferred their feeding forays to the central feeder when it alone held corn. Grid trapping periods following the inventory based on the single feeder resulted in the capture of only one new animal, a strong indication that the feeder could draw squirrels from the entire woodlot. Intentions were to attempt the method during other seasons, but overall trapping success was inconsistent and a reliable estimate could not be made. DISCUSSION As in any approach to censusing animals, there are advantages and shortcomings to be considered. One major drawback of this method is the effect food shortage or abundance may have on trapping success. As Barkalow $5.31, (1970) noted, in an exceptionally good mast year squirrels may be overwhelmed by food and thus be literally untrappable. The application of direct trapping as a census method, therefore. requires a food shortage to function efficiently. This condition occurs with some certainty in Michigan nearly every year, for even though autumn food may be abundant, crusted snow and ice usually make it unavailable to foraging squirrels during the January through March winter period. Examination of field conditions will aid the investi- gator in deciding when and if these or other conditions of food shortage exist. Application of the method during two winters of markedly varied 37 snow conditions showed quite favorable results. In 1971 deep crusted snow made access to buried food supplies difficult if not impossible. In 1972 the snow cover was far less permanent, and at frequent intervals squirrels had free access to their usual food sources. During the periods between thaws, however, animals came well to the feeder and were quite easily trapped. By necessity the season for censusing, using feeders and live traps, becomes the winter period of food scarcity, not always the most desirable time to obtain population data. However, early winter numbers of animals, though somewhat less, may approximate fall numbers. The data in Figures 2 and 3 reflect higher numbers of animals in January than in November, most certainly a result of increasing effectiveness of trapping as food became more difficult to obtain. Hinter estimates are useful since they reflect popu1ation numbers at a time when normally only mortality is operating, as opposed to fall when mortality, migration, and even natality may be acting. Another pertinent factor to consider is the effect weather conditions may have on trapping success. For example, severe weather in very early winter may lead to reduced or complete cessation of squirrel activity (Allen 1943); however, by mid-January most squirrels in the woodlot became quite active. Severe cold spells or stormy weather sometimes restricted movements on a day-to-day basis, but the investigator can judge these conditions and trap during more "normal" weather conditions. Consideration should also be given to the possibility of catching a predator such as the raccoon (Procygn_lotor) at the trapping site. If this situation should occur, the location may be rendered ineffective 38 until the offender is removed. During winter in Michigan, this is seldom a problem, though it could become a serious drawback in more southern states where raccoons and other omnivores remain active through the winter months. Hith shortcomings considered, however, the method has worked well for Michigan fox squirrels in winter, giving good results when compared to known populations. The centrally located feeder appeared to attract animals over an 18- to ZO-acre area, and perhaps could be expected to be effective over an even larger area. Sharp (1958) found that one feeder for 28 acres was adequate to attract most gray squirrels for trapping, and fox squirrels have been known to travel more than a mile during severe winters (Allen 1943). It is entirely possible that one feeder for 30 acres might suffice, though the efficiency of the method might decline. Another advantage is the small number of traps needed and the length of time needed to service them. An hour was required to service 20 traps on a grid system, while only 15 minutes was needed to check the six traps at a feeder station. The social behavior, particularly the dominance hierarchy, plays an important part in determining success of this method. Hhen two or more animals appear at the feeder, the subdominants are forced from the feeder to the ground and the traps, where their capture is virtually assured. A trapped animal does not frighten others nearby, but rather seems to reinforce them that all is well. On one occasion, all six traps held animals. In woodlots with large populations, more traps should be utilized to ensure that a trap is available for each approaching squirrel. 39 Present evidence shows that feeder trapping is an efficacious method of capturing squirrels, requiring relatively limited time and equipment. Further investigation of the method is in order on a larger scale, especially in different geographical situations where effects of winter are not so predictable. 4O LITERATURE CITED Allen, D. H. 1943. Michigan Fox Squirrel Management. Mich. Dept. of Cons., Lansing, Mich. 404 pp. Barkalow, F. 5., Jr., R. 8. Hamilton, and R. F. Soots, Jr. 1970. The vital statistics of an unexploited gray squirrel population. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 34(3):489-500. Bernard, R. J. 1972. Social organization of the western fox squirrel. M.S. Thesis. Michigan State Univ., East Lansing. 41 PP. Davis, D. E. 1963. Estimating the numbers of game populations. Pp. 89-119. In H. S. Mosby (Editor), Wildlife investigational techniques. 2nd edition. The Wildlife Society, Washington, D. C. 419 pp. Fitzwater, H. 0., Jr. 1943. Color marking of mammals with special reference to squirrels. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 7(2):l90-192. Flyger, V. F. 1955. Implications of social behavior in gray squirrel management. Trans. N. Am. Hildl. Conf. 20:381-389. Flyger, V. F. 1959. A comparison of methods for estimating squirrel populations. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 23(2):220-223. Fouch, H. R. 1961. Mast crops and fox squirrel populations at the Rose Lake Wildlife Experiment Station. Mich. Dept. Cons. Game Div. Report No. 2332. Friley, C. E. 1955. Criteria for investigating fall fox squirrel populations. J. Wildl. Mgmt. 19(1):89-93. Goodrum, P. D. 1937. Notes on the gray and fox squirrels of eastern Texas. Trans. N. Am. Wildl. Conf. 2:499-504. Goodrum, P. D. 1940. A population study of the gray squirrel in eastern Texas. Texas Agric. Exp. Sta., Bull. 591. 43 pp. Gysel, L. H. 1971. Beechnut production in Michigan. J. Wildl. Nixon, C. M., W. R. Edwards, and L. Eberhardt. 1967. Estimating squirrel abundance from live-trapping data. J. Hildl. Mgmt. 31(1):96-10l. 41 Sharp, W. M. 1958. The art and technique of live-trapping gray squirrels. Penn. Coop. Wildl. Res. Unit Report, No. 3, Penn. State Univ., University Park, Pa. Schnabel, Zoe Emily. 1938. The estimation of the total fish populations of a lake. Am. Math. Monthly 45(6):345-352. ‘tittiiititiiit