-, .zAYfi-‘i. 4 V ACCEPTANCE vERsus REJEcTioN . * ' 0F OTHERS AND sELE iN ' PERSONALITY SELF- REPORTS Dissertation for the Degree of Ph. D, G MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY LARRY M. GERSTENHABER 1974 llilli Ii Mil ‘ 3 1293 0 M“ ”II V W 83 5612 LIBRARY Michigansw Um'vcnity This is to certify that the thesis entitled Hccepfwce Versus ReJecf/on 0/ Offlers Md ; self I." Persona/ify Se/f-IQQPOr-ts ‘_ presented by Larry M. Gerstenhafier has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for __Ehl_degree in M1119 y [/7 1’ . Major professor z, ‘ 5 sons '_ BLUUKBIN MDERY INC. ~~ may ambzn [MAI w ACCEF The research 11 211:; night be adequ izexsions of accept t'ersts rejection of fzzrlation, the pre gazil measures of p :Erszzality Inventorj :‘Efrlist (ICE), Ret‘ Static dlfferentiai :E‘Q’vn ' on 5 Personal ( n. int undergraduate mm cluster analvs 1' 12.5 factor analys IS 37‘1" \uigS communality a E: 3" .T Munt for the do The findings gen .1 “4‘? LETS concerned wi‘ ABSTRACT ACCEPTANCE VERSUS REJECTION OF OTHERS AND SELF IN PERSONALITY SELF-REPORTS By Larry M. Gerstenhaber The research literature which I reviewed suggested that person- ality might be adequately described by two minimally interdependent dimensions of acceptance versus rejection of self (SAR) and acceptance versus rejection of others (ARO). Designed to investigate this formulation, the present study was limited to self-report, paper-and- pencil measures of personality. A battery consisting of Eysenck's Personality Inventory (EPI), LaForge and Suczek's Interpersonal Checklist (ICL), Rotter's Interpersonal Trust Scale, a series of semantic differential scales, and selected scales from the MMPI and Shostrom's Personal Orientation Inventory (POI) was administered to 182 MSU undergraduates enrolled in elementary psychology courses. The BCTRY cluster analysis technique and a non-communality principal axis factor analysis were applied to these data. The BCTRY method employs communality as a measure of the sufficiency of dimensions to account for the domain in question. The findings generally sustained expectations of two large clusters concerned with the acceptance versus rejection of others ' ' "'JI'VL'I' 1‘! 35.3) and self :agsr clusters r E35 an ARO ClUSi aoi acceptance c hostility, or it described by Eve "ezctional" mode 33's social mod assertiveness, w related to £8811: :31: $53 clusters zeestres located :L:s:er,}1,u_’ ap; 'I'iS aCCOunted for 217-3 of SAR'S r8j c."“‘" "“1 SEnerally t Larry M. Gerstenhaber (ARO) and self (SAR), as well as their relative independence. Four major clusters and two or three minor ones were identified. Salient was an ARO cluster characterized by behaviors of warmth, gentleness, and acceptance of others, and by adjectives related to the affection- hostility, or love—hate, Interpersonal Checklist dimension. SAR was described by two major clusters, seemingly representing "social" and "emotional" modes of self-acceptance (SARsoc’ SARemot’ respectively). SAR's social mode was defined by measures related to extraversion and assertiveness, while the emotional mode was defined by measures related to feelings about oneself. The relationship between these two SAR clusters was depicted as a gradient with two general SAR measures located midway between SARSOC and SARem . The fourth major 0t cluster, MAL, apparently dealing with more severe maladjustment than was accounted for by SAR and ARO, seemed at least partly representa- tive of SAR's rejection pole. MMPI scales employed in the present study generally tapped MAL, but an Introversion scale loaded most highly on SARsoc’ and a Suspicion scale loaded inversely nearly as highly on ARO as on MAL. I interpreted these results as suggesting that popular measures of maladjustment are substantially loaded on SAR. In addition to interpreting these clusters, I cautioned that selection of the measures may have partially influenced the outcome of the study. Measures of extraversion-introversion and anxiety (Eysenck's EPI), as well as a social desirability response set scale, tended to link firmly with SAR and MAL, but minimally with ARO. This find- ing suggested that popular theories claiming the sufficiency of :Tizensions of 9"” Personality have t 5;;ears that the ‘ ggasion. Males ercept that 3 min‘ faales but vi th 1 that a set of 58!: 5A3 and ARO adeqt. The main all ccllege students suggested that fu :easures in stud: :larify SAR and A serious error for E2131 concern for regard, Rogers' ‘ a:CEPtance of sel Present findings "Grantee: .Cfm ' ; lc‘ I U31 H‘ Wakely y‘ o {ME-El "‘4 Es F . Larry M. Gerstenhaber dimensions of extraversion-introversion and anxiety to represent personality have overlooked the importance of ARO. In addition, it appears that the tendency to "look good" might be part of the SAR dimension. Males and females were nearly identical on all dimensions, except that a minor Transparency factor was linked with ARO among females but with SAR among males. I also inferred from the results that a set of semantic differential scales could be used to assess SAR and ARO adequately. The main allegations of the present study were supported for college students on self—report, paper-and-pencil measures. I suggested that future researchers expand the number and variety of measures in studies like this one, in order to further validate and clarify SAR and ARO. Additionally, I concluded that it may be a serious error for therapists to focus on enhancing self—esteem without equal concern for the client's orientation toward others. In this regard, Rogers' (1953) belief in an intrinsic linkage between the acceptance of self and others seems sharply inconsistent with the present findings. .\ ." 1 Committee: 4~ 3,- '4 // /’ “if / if. 01’ CL A John R. Hurley, chairperson I '\ I 0 John H. Wakely Martha Karson Charles F. Wrigley u‘ .harA-r in Pa ACCEPTANCE VERSUS REJECTION OF OTHERS AND SELF IN PERSONALITY SELF—REPORTS By a ( Larry M? Cerstenhaber A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1974 i '. l I 'd V | ‘ I all?" '9 C25‘ S C o 5 17' Iwish t0 t :1 teacher, SUPe and support help dissertation can the years of my 35 the data unde tsstion such in :13 reliability. Iver the past se its inception , fitting of this .. Ll‘: ether, th Q I also wish ‘5sz at St . ‘ c minue‘i Supper ling the Past ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to thank Dr. John Hurley, who over the years has been my teacher, supervisor, therapist and friend. His participation and support helped make this project as enjoyable as writing a dissertation can be. Also, Dr. Jack Wakeley, who followed me through the years of my graduate training and who made statistical analysis of the data understandable to the point that I finally began to question such institutionalized psychological concepts as validity and reliability. Dr. Martha Karson was a source of personal esteem over the past several years, and fully supported this project from its inception. Also important was the occasional aid, during the writing of this dissertation, of my friend Dr. George "Bill" Fairweather, who introduced me to BCTRY while I served as his research assistant several years ago. A I also wish to thank the outpatient and child and adoleSCent staffs at St. Lawrence Community Mental Health Center for their continued support. Finally, my heartfelt thanks to Deborah and Lisa Norton, my housemates, who shared many agonized hours with me during the past year. ii REVIEW METHOD. TABLE OF CONTENTS OF THE LITERATURE . O O O O O O O O O O O C 0 Dimensions of Personality. . . . . . . . . . Two Salient Dimensions. . . . . . . . The Self-Other Dichotomy ~ Interpersonal Acts Defined. . . . . . . . . . . . Postulations from Theory. . . . . . . Postulations from Factor Analysis . Postulations from Encounter Group Research. . . Evidence for a Circular Order of Personality Relationship of the Dimensions . . . . . . . Response Set . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cross-Cultural Stability . . . . . . . . . . Summary - Statement of Partial Hypotheses. Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Test Battery . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Self-Report Data vs. Behavior Ratings Instruments . . . . . . . . . . . . . Time of Battery. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Order of Battery . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Statistical Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . The BCTRY Cluster Ana ysis System . . The Communality Problem . . . . . . . Factor Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . Statistical Treatment . . . . . . . . Statement of Hypotheses . . . . . . . RESIJIITS O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I 0 Pilot Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Empirical Cluster Analysis (Total Sample). . Clusters. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cluster 1 (ARO) . . . . . . . . . . . Cluster 2 (MAL) . . . . . . . . . . . Cluster 3 (SAR ). . . . . . . . . . soc iii Variables Page 22 22 22 23 24 24 35 36 36 36 41 42 43 44 45 45 46 46 47 53 54 CIUSter 4 (SARemOt) o o o o o o o Cluster 5 . . . . . . . . . . . . . Cluster 6 (Transparency). . . . . . . Relationship Between Clusters. . . . . . . . . Sufficiency of Clusters to Account for the Domain. Principal Axis Analysis. . . . . . . . . . . Preset Cluster Analysis - BCTRY. . . . . . Introduction to Preset. . . . . Preset Clusters . . . . . . . . . . . Relationship Between Clusters - Preset . Preset Summary Note . . . . . . . . . . Non-Clustering Variables . . . . . . Response Set Measures. . . . . . . . . . Comparison Between Subgroups - Male vs. Female . Comparison Between Subgroups - Non— Specific Groups . . . . . . . . . DISCUSSION. 0 '0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O The Salient Dimensions . . . . . . . . . . . . Cluster 1: Acceptance versus Rejection of Others (ARO) . . . . . . . . . . . Clusters Representing Acceptance versus Rejection of Self (SAR) . . . . . . . The Maladjustment Cluster (MAL) . . . . A Minor Cluster: Transparency (TR) . . Other Groupings . . . . . . . . . . . . Relationship of the Dimensions . . . . . . . . Validity of the Item Sample. . . . . . . . . . Eysenck's "Super Factors". . . . . . . . . . . Response Set Measures. . . . . . . . . . . . . Subsample Comparisons. . . . . . . . . Measurement of ARO and SAR (Clustering of Variables) Implications for Research and Psychotherapy. . Research. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Psychotherapy . . . . . . . . . . . Summary and Conclusions. . . . . . . . . . . . APPmDICES O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O WERENCE S O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 iv Page 57 59 60 61 63 66 67 67 69 79 80 81 81 83 87 93 93 93 95 97 101 101 102 103 105 106 107 108 114 114 118 119 122 148 Table 10 11 12 13 14 15 LIST OF TABLES Page Intercorrelations between cluster scores I, B, S, T (Tryon and Bailey, 1970, p. 164) . . . . . . . . . . 10 Alpha reliabilities of full and shortened TSC scales (Tryon, 1966) O O O O O I I O O O O O O O O O O 26 Description of POI categories. Categories on the POI are presented as a series of opposites . . . . . . 30 Variables for cluster analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 Clusters on empirical V-analysis - total sample cluster structure of expanded clusters . . . . . . . . 49 Oblique factor coefficients - empirical V-analysis . . 51 Correlations between oblique cluster domains . . . . . 61 Number of residuals in .1000-.1999 range (empirical anaIYS1S) O O O O O O C O O O O C O C O O O O O O O O O 65 Clusters on preset V-analysis - total sample cluster structure of expanded clusters . . . . . . . . . . . . 70 Oblique factor coefficients - preset analysis. . . . . 72 Correlations between purported measures of response set. 0 O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O I O O O 82 COMP analysis for males and females. . . . . . . . . . 83 V¥analysis performed on preset clusters as part of COMP analysis - males vs. females. . . . . . . . . . . 85 Empirical clustering of variables - male vs. female. . 88 COMP analysis for non-specific groups - cosines among dimensions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 .a'ole 15 V-analysis 1 COPSD analys 17 Oblique fat tionship of 18 Expected an Table 16 17 18 Page V-analysis performed on preset clusters as part of COMP analysis - random samples, N-llO, N = 72. . . . 90 Oblique factor coefficients relevant to the rela- tionship of SAR and MAL (empirical analysis) . 99 Expected and actual clustering of variables. . . . . . 110 vi Figure LIST OF FIGURES Relationship of the four temperaments to Berne's life positions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of dimensional analyses. . . . . . . . . Lorr and Mc'Nair primary factors . . . . Diagrammatic representation of the SAR gradient. vii Page 3 . 13 . 15 . 98 FYJ (s LIST OF SPAN DIAGRAMS Diagram Page 1 Empirical analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 2 Empirical analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 3 Empirical analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 4 Preset analysis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 5 Preset analysis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 6 COMP analysis for males and females (higher order ClusterS) . O C O O C O C O . O O C O O O O O O O O O O 86 7 COMP analysis for non-specific groups (higher order ClusterS) O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O 92 viii Appendix A LIST OF APPENDICES AN ARBITRARY CLASSIFICATION OF MMPI ITEMS BY CONTWT O O O C O O O O C O O O O O O O MMPI ITEMS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . EPI ITEMS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ICL ITEMS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . POI ITEMS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ROTTER INTERPERSONAL TRUST SCALE . . . . . TWO-DIMENSIONAL INTERPERSONAL INVENTORY. . SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL SCALES . . . . . . . RAW CORRELATION MATRIX OF ALL VARIABLES. . CORRELATIONS BETWEEN DEFINING VARIABLES OF IN THE EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS. . . . . . . . . PRINC IPAL AXIS MATRIX O O O O O O O O O O O VARIMAX ROTATION ANALYSIS - ROTATED FACTOR CORRELATIONS BETWEEN DEFINING VARIABLES OF IN THE PRESET ANALYSIS . . . . . . . . . . ix CLUSTERS LOADINGS. CLUSTERS Page 122 123 128 130 132 136 138 139 140 142 143 145 147 q uuvfi _. » an! IE} U l‘! H a . c I a. «.1. MI firiJ u‘» .i. ~ .Q ‘ ‘ N‘ 9‘ v; REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Dimensions of Personality For ages man has attempted to classify himself and his environ- ment. The classical view that all nature is composed of four basic elements, air, earth, fire and water, was expanded, first by Hippocrates (c. 400 3.0.) and later by Galen (c. 150 A.D.), to describe men in relation to others. Four temperaments were described, roughly corresponding to the four elements. Thus the sanguine temperament corresponded to air, the melancholic to earth, the choleric to fire, and the phlegmatic to water. Allport (1937) described feelings that correspond to the temperaments. The sanguine person is hopeful, the melancholic sad, the choleric prone to anger, and the phlegmatic apathetic and sluggish. Two Salient Dimensions This ancient system and its behavioral correlates become even more interesting when viewed with the recently accumulating evidence for two prepotent dimensions in personality theory. This evidence, reviewed by Adams (1964), strongly suggests that these dimensions are best defined by "the degree of acceptance or rejection of self" and "the degree of acceptance or rejection of others." This chapter is ~e 1'}: CCU; » A a; 2 concerned with reviewing evidence in the literature relevant to the appearance of these dimensions. The Self-Other Dichotomy - Interpersonal Acts Defined If one employs Adams' (1964) definers of the predominant dimensions of personality, it becomes apparent that any description of personality, and therefore interpersonal behavior, must take into account both Self-Acceptance-Rejection (SAR) and Other—Acceptance- Rejection (ARO). Foa (1961) described an interpersonal act as "an attempt to establish the emotional relationship of the actor toward himself and toward the other, as well as to establish the social relationship of the self and the other with respect to a larger reference group." The division in viewing behavior as meaningful to both self and other is referred to here as the self~other dichotomy. Postulations from Theory The late Eric Berne developed a system, one of the main tenets of which is based on the self—other dichotomy. Berne (1966) believed that an individual's fundamental life position could be defined by two polar dimensions: I'M 0K - I'M NOT OK, YOU'RE OK - YOU'RE NOT OK. According to Berne, the individual who has made the life decision I'M OK.- YOU'RE OK is the individual most likely to be described as healthy. A further discussion of the evolvement of the four possible life positions is presented in Thomas Harris', I’M’OK - YOU’RE OK: A Guide to Transactional Analysis. It is easy to see how Berne's dimensions correspond to the earlier systems described above. A description of this relationship, including current psychological terminology, is presented in Figure l. .mdoaufimoa owHH m.ocuom on mucoamuomamu snow onu mo magmGOfiumHom .H ouswam Mo uo: ou.=ow I Mo uoc a.H mwouwnom haumm< owumawoanm Mo uo: ou.=ow I mo a.H vaoqmumm huwaanwummuH ownoaono MO mu.now I Mo was E.H o>awmmuamn mmmacmm UHHonoamHmz wdfinwamauum Mo mu.:ow I Mo a.H Imamm .hnuammm mmmaaamomom onwawnmm Amcuomv >woaoafiahmu oumaouuoo Aaoaou coauwmoa omwg Hmofiwoaonohmm uamuuao uow>mnom .mmumuoommamv unmauuomaua Li 1"" Iran—r- a-n Ya .. , but . I .. v.1 V..S .‘: r! a. '< I, . , .it a r. ‘10 [J 4 The work of Freedman, Leary, Ossorio and Coffey (1951) also generated a two-dimensional model. Seeking a comprehensive schema for personality description, Freedman et al. developed a system of variables based on four criteria: systematic interrelatedness of variables, interpersonal reference, encompassing of normal as well as abnormal functioning, and ability to transform into an operational statement. They believed that personality exists at three levels - public, conscious, and private (unconscious). The public level can be defined as interpersonal mechanism and is best described by verbs. The conscious and private levels represent interpersonal traits and are described by adjectives. Freedman et a1. separated their list of variables into self and other categories and considered them to be arranged in a circular order of sixteen divisions. Seeking to measure the conscious level, Suczek and LaForge (1955) developed the Interpersonal Checklist (ICL). This measure is scored on the six- teen possible modes of behavior. The checklist itself can be reduced further to two major dimensions: Affection-Hostility and Dominance-Submission. LaForge (1973) reported factor analytic results in accordance with this reduction to two dimensions. A lengthy summary of the earlier work is found in Leary (1957). These two dimensions ally closely with Adams' (1964) suggestion that Self-Acceptance-Rejection and Other-Acceptance—Rejection play a major role. According to Adams, "The dominance-submission axis defines the degree of acceptance or rejection of self, while the affection- hostility axis defines the degree of acceptance or rejection of other." .... 1 l .ub .ua -\ al.- -.__ :1 n-‘ .,_ l,— R! r . ’5 5 Hurley (in press, a), in agreement with this position, observed that positive self—references on the ICL ("self—respecting", "self— confident", "self-reliant", "assertive") align "exclusively" with the Dominance pole, while negative self-referent terms ("always ashamed of self", "self—punishing", "lacks self-confidence") align with the Submissive pole. Foa (1961) also has redefined Leary's dimensions to fit the self-other dichotomy. Foa began by dividing the dimensions into four discrete facets: Dominance, Submission, Love and Hostility. Believing that actions are meaningful both toward other and self, he defined eight profiles: hostility to self, submission to self, dominance of other, submission to other, etc. He also included another variable which he described as mode of action. This can be social or emotional. For example, he suggested that Leary's managerial—autocratic behavior is a blend of social and emotional rejection of other and of social and emotional acceptance of self. Restated in Berne's system, this is the I'M OK - YOU'RE NOT OK position. This can be done for each of Leary's types. In addition, Foa too has placed these interpersonal variables in a circular order. The concept of circular order is discussed in a later section. Postulations from Factor Analysis Behavior Studies: In addition to evidence compiled from a theoretical base, the development of factor analytic methods has made the reduction of large numbers of variables to a small number of dimensions a relatively 18510! I. . "yup!- ILQALAA carter '3-3.’ .rtul IS 20! | 'h‘. tu’h}. ’ic-b.“ '\ “~U“. .r) .; H5 (I! In ~| I. I t an r) A": '5‘,“ u 6 simple process. Foa (1961) reported on a variety of studies. Carter [(1954) (Foa, 1961)] in a variety of situations emphasized three factors: Individual prominence and achievement, Aiding attainment by the group, and Sociability. Borgatta, Cottrell and Mann (1958) reported that two of Carter's three factors obtained in a study in which graduate students were rated by peers on variables based on Carter's three factors. The investigators labelled the factors Individual Assertiveness and Sociability. Individual assertiveness is comparable to the SAR dimension, while Sociability is closer to ARO. Schaefer (1959, 1961), in studies of maternal and child behavior, found evidence for two factors which he labelled love vs. hostility and autonomy vs. control. The closeness of these dimensions to Leary's, and therefore to SAR and ARO, is apparent from the names. Questionnaire Studies: Perhaps the best known researchers in the area of personality questionnaire research are H. J. Eysenck and Raymond Cattell. Eysenck (1953), factoring a large number of traits measured by a variety of instruments, found evidence for three main dimensions: Neuroticism, Extraversion-Introversion, and Psychoticism. Neuroti- cism and Extraversion became the basis for development of the Maudsley and Eysenck personality inventories. Cattell (1964 and earlier) described fifteen primary personality factors which have been embodied in the l6-PF questionnaire. General intelligence was added as the sixteenth factor. Cattell's own factoring of these 16 first order factors suggested four higher order factors: (1) Dynamic integration vs. anxiety, (2) Extraversion— istrovers Crbaroicen analyses only two these Adj .AI“"T 7 Introversion, (3) Cyclothyme vs. Schizothyme constitution, and (4) Unbroken success vs. frustration, of which factors 2 and 4 are highly significantly positively correlated. Peterson (1965) compared analyses of Cattell data for adults and children and found that only two higher order dimensions could be defended. He labelled these Adjustment and Extraversion—Introversion. Wetzel (Peterson, 1965) administered the l6-PF Questionnaire to undergraduate psychology students and obtained peer and parent ratings on traits purportedly measured by the questionnaire. He found two second order factors, which be labelled Dynamic integration (self-perception of adjustment) and Introversion-Extraversion. Although Cattell held that primary factors are most important, Eysenck argued that it is the larger dimensions that are more meaning- ful psychologically (Eysenck, 1972). Using Cattell's correlations for the l6—PF instrument, Eysenck corrected for attenuation and concluded: "As far as we can see, practically all the information contained unreliably in the primaries is contained reliably in the second-order factors." Wiggins (1968) struck a responsive chord to this argument. In a vast review of the personality-structure research he reported: "If consensus exists within the realm of temperament structure, it does so with respect to the importance of the large, ubiquitous, and almost unavoidable dimensions of extroversion and anxiety (neuroticism)." While it is apparent that Eysenck, Cattell and others reported in this section are dealing with similar dimensions, and that these dimensions are somewhat comparable to SAR and ARO, there may be some controversy over which dimensions align with which others. Hurley 8 (personal communication) identified introversion with the submission pole and extraversion with the dominance pole of Leary. In this formulation extraversion~introversion relates closely to SAR. Eysenck's account, however, in a description to prospective buyers of the Eysenck Personality Inventory, is somewhat different (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1968). Neuroticism here seemed more related to SAR, and extraversion-introversion to ARO. Suggestions from Wiggins (1968) and Kassebaum et a1. (1959) also appeared indirectly to tie extraversion—introversion more closely to ARO. The question remains an empirical one which can be viewed within the framework of a cluster analysis of the data of this study. MMPI Studies: Factor analysis of another widely used measure, the MMPI, shmilarly led to the interpretation of a small number of factors. Welsh (1956) claimed that the MMPI, which purports to measure a large variety of factors, can be adequately described in terms of two. Welsh identifies these as A and R. A may align more closely with SAR, while R may align more closely with ARO (Adams, 1964). An alternative interpretation suggests some overlap, with high A scorers falling below the mean on both dimensions (Adams, 1964). Some question may also be raised about the validity of A and R given the lack of computer sophistication at the time of Welsh's work (Charles Wrigley, personal communication). Kulik and Kulik (1972), however, using a revised cluster analysis technique (VCLUST), identified two major clusters whose similarity to A and R was commented on. 9 Kassebaum, Couch and Slater (1959) factor analyzed 32 MMPI scales and identified two major factors as ego-strength-ego-weakness (I) and introversion—extraversion (II). Welsh's A loaded highly on the first while R boasted the highest loading on the second. In addition, "R" was virtually the only variable independent of "I". The R factor has also been interpreted as a measure of "acquiescence" (Abbott, Fry, Abbott, 1972). The effect of response sets will be discussed in a later section. Both studies mentioned incidentally used already constructed scales of items in their analyses. A later, more comprehensive effort by Robert Tryon, Kenneth Stein and Chen-Lin Chu involved a cluster analysis of the total 550 item MMPI on a sample of patients and normals (Tryon, 1966; Stein, 1968; Tryon and Bailey, 1970). The analysis employed the BIGNV component of the BCTRY cluster analysis system, which eliminates the small N limitation of other analyses. Initially the program identi- fied seven item-clusters: Introversion (I), Body Symptoms (B), Suspicion (8), Depression (D), Resentment (R), Autism (A), and Tension (T). Four of the dimensions, Introversion, Body Symptoms, Suspicion, and Tension, accounted for most of the total communality of the item pool. The remaining three were considered dependent clusters. Eliminating nearly two-thirds of the total number of items based on low communalities, the clusters could then be shown to form scales (the TSC Scales). The full-length scales and shortened versions were reported by Tryon (1966). Intercorrelations between cluster scores are reported in Table l. 10 TABLE 1. Intercorrelations between cluster scores I, B, S, T (Tryon and Bailey, 1970, p. 164) I B S T Introversion .47 .27 .68 Body Symptoms .32 .75 Suspicion .48 Tension It is important to note that Introversion and Suspicion shared the lowest intercorrelation of the clusters. Their low dependence on each other is suggested by an}:2 of only .07. In addition, these two clusters appear to have some of the same defining characteristics as SAR and ARO. Suspicion probably relates to ARO, while Introversion relates to SAR. Further study by Tryon (Tryon and Bailey, p. 200), consisting of a comparison of the patient and normal subgroups of his sample, revealed that for the normal group only the dimensions of Suspicion and Introversion held up. Thus, once again two major dimensions, similar to SAR and ARO, adequately described a domain. Postulations from Encounter Grouijesearch Hurley and Force (1971), in an extensive study of pre-lab, lab, and post-lab data for 50 participants in an eight-day laboratory experience, identified two clusters of personality variables. These clusters conformed to their expected dichotomy of self and other orientation. What is noteworthy is that: 11 "Within the Self-Acceptance and Other-Acceptance clusters, 46 of the 48 (96%) correlations are positive and sta- tistically significant. A sharply different pattern is apparent within the Across—Cluster Rectangles where only 15 of the 42 (36%) correlations are significant and positive." In addition, the ICL variables, Love-Hate and Dominance-Submission, correlated highly in their expected clusters. Data on the effects of trainers on their T—groups have also been discussed by Hurley (in press, a). In addition to his own findings which further confirm the saliency of SAR and ARO, Hurley has kept up a correspondence with Bolman (Hurley, 1973; Bolman, l971a,b; Hurley, 1971; Bolman, 1973, in press) (in Hurley, in press, b). During this time Bolman conducted and reported on two independent studies of the effects of trainers on T—groups. Most recently, Hurley (in press, b) found that only one cluster accounted for nearly all of the sta- tistically significant correlations among the eleven most important variables utilized by Bolman (1971, 1973). This cluster's positive pole was marked by a trio of measures (Trainer's Congruence-Empathy, Liking for trainer, and Trainer's affection toward the group), while the negative pole was denoted by another trio (Tension of group, Conditionality of trainer, and Discomfort with trainer). Hurley regarded this cluster as measuring ARO. Interestingly, the most peripheral member of this cluster was the variable Dominance of trainer, which seemed to represent SAR. This latter variable did not correlate significantly with any of the ten other variables in Bolman's 1971 study, and correlated only marginally (-.28) with Other's Learning in Bolman's 1973 study. The independence of 12 Dominance of trainer from the six polar markers in both studies sug- gests that Dominance is unrelated to ARO. Lieberman, Yalom and Miles (1973) also reported on the leaders of encounter-type groups. From data based on ratings by group members and outside observers, four rotated factors of leadership function were isolated: (l) Emotional stimulation, (2) Caring, (3) Meaning- attribution, and (4) Executive function. Only caring and meaning- attribution correlated significantly with outcome. Caring was described as incorporating protecting, loving, supporting behavior. It was clearly a warm/cold, love-not-love dimension, or ARO. Meaning— attribution by contrast is representative of SAR. Meaning-attribution was that function of the leader concerned with the translation of feelings and behaviors into ideas. In so much as the leader gives meaning to the experiences of the participants, he may be seen as dominant. In the schema of this report, this type of dominance aligns with SAR. Given these data, it is plausible to conclude that further research on encounter groups will continue to reveal the prepotency of SAR and ARO. It is also likely that continued factor analytic studies will reveal these dimensions and provide stronger empirical backing for theoretical formulations such as those of Berne and Leary. This study, as its first goal, using Tryon's Cluster Analysis techniques with selected self-report personality measures, was designed to do this. A summary is contained in Figure 2. 13 Accepts others You're OK Love Caring Melancholic Sanguine Depressive Healthy Submission Dominance I'm nut OK I'm 0K Rejects self Accepts self Introversion (Tryon) Meaning attribution Phlegmatic Choleric Schizoid Paranoid Hostility You're not OK Rejects others Suspicion and mistrust (Tryon) Eysenck Extraversion—Introversion} Eysenck Neuroticism-Stability Figure 2. Summary of dimensional analyses. va1 Ci. ¢_L. (" I]; I!) ‘v .“ u.‘ ‘ ‘ h ‘: 0 n on 14 Evidence for a Circular Order of Personality Variables Up to this section the author has been concerned with selecting evidence for a two-dimensional theory of personality. Further examina- tion of Leary's system, however, suggested the possibility of describing interpersonal behavior in a meaningful circular order (statistically known as circumplex). Foa (1961), in redefining Leary's types, discussed ordering them in a circumplex. Statistically such an order may be said to be present when "the correlations between any two variables Xi and are a function of their sequential separation. The correla- tions of variable Xi should decrease monotonically and then increase monotonically as we move around a circular order." (Lorr and Mc'Nair, 1963) Lorr and Mc’Nair (1963) began their study of interpersonal behavior with 13 hypothesized factors of interpersonal behavior. They ended up with nine primary factors which could be arranged in a circumplex order. These are shown in Figure 3. From these primary factors they obtained three higher order factors described as control, dependence, and affiliation vs. detachment. Expanding the circle (Lorr and Mc'Nair, 1965), they confirmed 15 interpersonal behavior categories in a circumplex order. It may be noted from Figure 2 that the axes of the Lorr—Mc'Nair circle are comparable to Leary's dimensions. Relationship of the Dimensions Research in the areas of self-acceptance and other-acceptance in time late 1940's and early 1950's often suggested a strong positive ggx ‘relationship between self and other acceptance (Sheerer, 1949; Stock, 15 Dominant- Controlling Rebr lious Susp cious Affi iative— Trusting Inhibit-d- Nur urant— Supportive Dependent Figure 3. Lorr and Mc'Nair primary factors. 16 1949; Omwake, 1954). Frequently using five-point scale type measures, experimenters reported correlations between self-acceptance and other-acceptance ranging from .37 to .74. All correlations reported in these studies were significant. In contrast, factor analytic studies like those reported earlier suggest the saliency of orthogonal factors. Remember that by defini- tion orthogonal factors have a correlation of .00 with each other. They are drawn by successive factoring from the intercorrelations and residuals of the intercorrelations of the variables involved. Thus, the appearance of dimensions interpretable as self and other acceptance will, by necessity, be independent of each other. In this case it is impossible to study those variables which may offer some clues to the possibly complex relationship between the two. By contrast, employing oblique clusters allows this. Oblique clusters are assumed to be correlated until they demonstrate otherwise. According to Tryon and Bailey (1970), oblique clusters provide a more natural description of the domain. The question of the independence of dimensions of self and other acceptance-rejection, however, is influenced by the distinction between statistical vs. theoretical definitions of independence. Statistically, independent dimensions must be uncorrelated (g - .00). Theoretically, however, it is unlikely that wholly independent constructs exist in the personality questionnaire domain. By hypothe- sizing independence for statistical purposes, the author is merely suggesting a minimal interdependence between SAR and ARO. Cu .. Plyvfl‘ 17 Clinically, Carl Rogers (1951, p. 520) suggested that the raising of self-acceptance will automatically result in the raising of other- acceptance. This is a currently held belief among many clinicians and has led to techniques of psychotherapy which are essentially client-centered and relate little to the client's relationship with the therapist and others. Hurley and Force (1971), however, in their study of these dimensions in the T-group, reported that "only limited overall gains (in interpersonal competence) are possible when increments in other-acceptance are small, no matter how great the gains in self-acceptance are." This, too, is the conclusion reached clinically by this author and several colleagues (personal communication). The controversy over the relationship between SAR and ARC is addressed in this paper. The BCTRY system presents a method for describing the relationship of clusters. The second goal, therefore, of this study is to support the contention that SAR and ARO are functionally independent dimensions. Response Set In any study employing self-report inventories, it is necessary to confront the possibility that one's chosen measures are actually measuring a "response set" of the subject. The two most often dis— cussed response sets are social desirability and acquiescence. Wiggins (1968) attributed the variance involved in personality inventories to four sources: (1) bybproducts of the strategy under which the scale was constructed, (2) item characteristics which A l8 produce method variance, (3) organized response styles which exist in_§s, and (4) manifestations of the particular content domain. Edwards et a2. (1962), in an analysis of 58 MMPI scales and three other personality scales, examined three factors. All these factors, according to Edwards, are based on the response style of the subject. Edwards stated: "We believe that the first factor can be best described as a social desirability factor and the second factor as an acquiescence factor." The third factor suggested the tendency of _§s to lie. Social desirability (SD) is defined here as the tendency to give a "'True' response to an item with a socially desirable scale value (SDSV) or a 'False' response to an item with a socially undesirable scale value." Acquiescence is simply the tendency to respond "True" to personality items. Thus Edwards (1964) concluded: "It may be argued that individuals who obtain high scores on the trait scale are responding to the items in terms of the trait which the scale was designed to measure, but it is equally plausible that they are responding to the items in terms of a trait which the scale was not designed to measure, namely, the tendency to give socially desirable responses." To measure the SD factor, Edwards developed two social desira- bility scales drawn from MMPI items, one of 79 and one of 39 items. Substantial correlations were reported between the SD scale and other MMPI scales (Edwards, 1957). Edwards (1964) also reported that the correlations between 43 MMPI scales and the SD scale were directly related to the proportion of items in the MMPI scales keyed for socially desirable responses. Fox (1966) and Dicken and Wiggins (1964) were unable to substantiate Edwards' claims. '5 19 The relationship of SD and acquiescence to other interpretations has been noted. Adams (1964) interpreted SD as comparable to the Affection—Hostility axis of Leary (and therefore ARO). Wiggins (1968) and Crowne et al. (Hurley, in press, a) reported a higher correlation of SD with SAR. A comparison of Edwards et al. (1962) and Kassebaum et a1. (1959) is also revealing. Welsh's A and R appear to differentiate between factors I and II no matter what they are called. In the Edwards et a1. rotated factor data Welsh's A loaded -.93 on I (SD) and R loaded —.90 on II (Acq). In Kassebaum et al. A loaded +.88 on I (ego strength), while R loaded +.69 on II (extrav-introv). In fact, R "boasts the highest loading on II and is virtually the only variable independent of I" (Kassebaum et al., 1959). That R is measuring an acquiescence factor has also been suggested by Abbott et a1. (1972, 1973). It appears likely, then, that response sets not only account for some of the variance in factor studies, but that they are probably related to important personality clusters themselves. Whole books, notably by Edwards (1970), have been written attempting to clarify the role of response sets, with no satisfactory conclusions presented. The present study does not attempt to deal with this subject compre- hensively, or perhaps even adequately. A hypothesis relevant to the response set question is, however, suggested. Cross-Cultural Stability Foa (1961) described interpersonal actions in terms of a series of components (see earlier review under "Interpersonal Theory"). The components are (1) content: rejection or acceptance, (2) intensity 7’: r!- AP ‘bb g: ’5‘ s» .IJ Q; , . \t. 20 of action, (3) object: self or other, and (4) mode: social or emotional. Foa suggested that "the direction of the third and fourth principal components is culturally determined." Apparently, intensity also plays a major role as Foa defined four cultural types. These are: (l) The emotional mode is more intense than the social mode and the individual comes before the collectivity. (2) The emotional mode is also more intense but the collectivity comes before the individual. (3) The social mode is more intense and the individual comes before the collectivity. (4) The social mode is also more intense but the collectivity comes before the individual. The comparison of cross—cultural profile patterns is clearly beyond the scope of this paper. Summary_- Statement of Partial Hypotheses Evidence has been reviewed relevant to the appearance of two prepotent dimensions of personality which may best be described as Self-Acceptance-Rejection and Other-Acceptance-Rejection. Further, it appears likely that these dimensions are minimally interdependent. The major goal of the present study is to assess this evidence. Thus, two partial hypotheses can be stated. H1: In the administration and subsequent analyses of a series of personality inventories, two salient reliable dimensions will occur. These dimensions can be described in terms of Self- Acceptance—Rejection, and Other-Acceptance-Rejection. 21 H2: These dimensions will be independent of each other. Several sub-hypotheses may be stated in partial form. These will become more defined in later sections of this proposal. H3: A small group of items or scales can be drawn from the large pool to adequately measure the dimensions. H4: Eysenck's extraversionrintroversion factor will cluster closely with SAR, while Neuroticism will cluster with ARO. H5: Response set variables will not lessen the meaningfulness of the dimensions. In: METHOD Subjects Subjects for this study were 182 undergraduate psychology students at Michigan State University. A sample size of at least 160 was desired on the basis of an arbitrary acceptable standard error of .08 in a commonly accepted formula for the standard error of measurement. Students were offered extra credit in their classes for participating in research programs. As additional incentive, the students were offered feedback on their performance. A numbered identification system preserved the students' anonymity. Procedure Subjects completed a battery of self-report personality measures lasting approximately one and one-half hours. The shortness of the battery presented a limitation to adequately sampling the domain of personality measures of this type. The length of one and one-half hours was justified by the experimenter on three grounds: (1) past experience; acceptability to college students because of past experience with tests of this length, (2) fatigue; one would expect some fatigue to accumulate and negatively affect a much longer battery, and (3) motivation; one would expect motivation to answer thoughtfully and truthfully to decrease with the battery's length. 22 23 Test Battery Choice of Instruments: The goal of this study was stated as an assessment of evidence for a two-dimensional interpersonal theory of personality based on the dimensions Self-Acceptance-Rejection (SAR) and Other-Acceptance- Rejection (ARO). Faced with a vast domain of available instruments, the experimenter was forced to choose among them. The initial choice in this case was to limit the study to self-report measures. While this confined the study to the conscious level (Freedman et al., 1951) of interpersonal traits, it also simplified and hastened data collection and allowed for a larger sample than otherwise might have been possible. The problems encountered by omitting data from the "public" and "unconscious" sectors in favor of the "conscious" are discussed below. While the experimenter's degree of comfort and familiarity with a given instrument undoubtedly influenced the domain from which he chose, some broad criteria were operative: (1) simplicity-instru- ments were chosen that were made up of yes-no, checklist, and semantic differential type items; (2) clustering-—instruments were chosen from which meaningful clusters were expected to emerge; and (3) self and other dimensions-instruments were chosen which appeared related to SAR and ARO [to better assess prior evidence, the experimenter was obliged, for at least part of the battery, to use instruments and scales employed in earlier studies; this criterion highly influenced the choice of scales to be used when it was necessary to choose only certain scales from a larger instrument], (4) shorter measure- ’3. 24 instruments were chosen which might reveal SAR and ARO in a shorter instrument [this criterion was of import in choosing some shorter, semantic scales of an experimental nature]; and (5) response set- instruments were chosen which would incorporate ways of testing the effects of response sets. The reader can see how these broad criteria operationalized in the choice of each instrument below. Self-Report Data vs. Behavior Ratings Before going on to the instruments themselves, however, perhaps some note should be made of the split between self—reports and reports by others. The present study was limited to self-report data. Unfortunately, the correspondence between this type of questionnaire data and behavior ratings has been uniformly low. While Cattell has argued that there is a "secure linkage" between behavior rating and questionnaire factors, Becker (1960) disagreed. Becker reviewed Cattell's data and arguments, along with his own, and drew opposite conclusions. Peterson (1965) wondered whether higher order dimensions might do better. In reporting a study by Wetzel, Peterson concluded that for second order factors there is some minimally significant correspondence between the methods. This evidence is plainly an insufficient base for concluding that self- report and report by others will yield identical results. The present study, then, is restricted to self-reports of personality. Instruments (1) Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI): The MMPI is so widely used that little description is necessary. According to Dahlstrom and Welsh (1960), the MMPI "was designed to 'q .4 a b~¢ :g,a fl) 25 provide an objective assessment of some of the major personality characteristics that affect personal and social adjustment." The instrument is made up of 566 True—False items. Initially these were divided up into scales based on psychopathology. The major scales are Hypochondriasis (Hs), Depression (D), Hysteria (Hy), Psychopathic deviate (Pd), Masculinity-Femininity (Mf), Paranoia (Pa), Psychastenia (Pt), Schizophrenia (Sc), and Hypomania (Ma). Another scale, Social introversion (Si), was later added and routinely included in the profile. Three validating scales, L (lie), F (validity), and K (correction), were also included. It is common knowledge, however, that more than 200 MMPI scales now exist. Many of these describe "normal" rather than pathological behaviors. According to Dahlstrom and Welsh (1960), the item pool may be arbitrarily classified into a broad spectrum of content areas as shown in Appendix A, page 122 Factor analytic studies of the MMPI were presented earlier (Postula- tions from Factor Analysis). Because of the excessive length of the MMPI, three abbreviated versions were considered: Tryon's four major clusters (Tryon and Bailey, 1970), scales A and R (Welsh, 1956), and the Mini-mult (Kincannon, 1968). Tryon's clusters (I, introversion; B, body symptoms; S, suspicion and mistrust; T, tension and worry) plus a shortened version of Edwards' (1970) Social Desirability scale form the portion of the MMPI used in this study. Tryon's technique of key cluster analysis was used to identify his four clusters (I, B, S, and T). The technique, to be discussed later, eliminates and retains items based on their communality. Thus, Tryon's clusters may be 26 presented as scales which purport to represent the full MMPI. Three additional clusters found by Tryon were discarded because they were extremely dependent on the other four. The instrument used in this study, with items keyed for the appropriate scale, is included as Appendix B, page 123. Internal consistency reliabilities for the full scales (to be used in this study) and their shortened versions are reproduced below (Table 2). TABLE 2. Alpha reliabilities of full and shortened TSC scales (Tryon, 1966) Scale n Alpha (rtt') n Alpha (rtt') I (Introversion) 26 .925 17 .911 B (Body Symptoms) 33 .919 17 .886 S (Suspicion) 25 .854 17 .830 T (Tension) 36 .923 17 .882 A discussion of Edwards' 18 item SD scale is included below (Response Set). Dimensions A and R (represented by corresponding scales) were also considered. It was concluded that because of the lack of sophisti- cation of computer technique at the time of that study.(l956), A and R might be less valid a short form than others. Although worthy of note, the Mini—Mult was also rejected for the present study. It consists of 71 items drawn from the MMPI by a process of clustering and choosing items within clusters that were 27 scored on the largest number of clinical and validity scales (Kincannon, 1968). Research using the Mini-Mult (Kincannon, 1968; Armentrout and Rouser, 1970; Harford et al., in press) does not encourage its use in this study. Armentrout (1970) obtained significant correlations between the raw scores on the MMPI and the Mini-Mult (except males on scale F). The Mini-Mult profiles did not, however, predict full MMPI profiles, and the author discourages use of the shorter instru- ment with a college population. In addition, the Mini-Mult seems to predict the full measure more closely when extracted from it, rather than administered separately (Streiner et aZ., 1973). (2) Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI): The EPI measures two "superfactors" which, according to Eysenck (1953), account for most of the variance in the personality domain. These are extraversion-introversion and neuroticismestability. In a review of the test, Lingoes (Buros, 1970) accepts the orthogonality of the factors for non—psychotic samples. The instrument consists of 57 "Yes-No" items and is available in parallel forms (A 8 B). The availability of American college norms is useful. The test also contains a "lie" scale. Split-half reliabilities are reported between .74 and .91 (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1968). In addition to measuring a small number of dimensions based on a factor analytic model, the EPI provides an opportunity to examine the "fit" of Eysenck's factors with SAR and ARO. A reproduction of the EPI is included as Appendix C, page.128. 28 (3) Interpersonal Checklist (ICL): The ICL is a "l34-item list of words or phrases which may be used to obtain self-descriptions or description by others with respect to an interpersonal domain of personality." (LaForge, 1973) The instrument, an outgrowth of the work of Leary, was not copy- righted by the authors (Suczek and LaForge, 1955). The experimenter is encouraged to use his own judgment and creativity. The ICL represents something of a marker measure in the present study. The summary factors LOVE-HATE (Lov) and DOMINANCE-SUBMISSION (Dom), to which the instrument may be reduced, appear to be suitable definers of the hypothesized SAR and ARO dimensions. In addition to the Lov and Dom summary scores, the ICL provides two additional factors. Ain (Average Intensity) is based upon the assignment of an intensity level to each item in the list. There are four levels and each of the 16 subscore scales contains an equal number of items at each level. Average intensity (Ain) can be used as a measure of social desirability (LaForge, 1973). Number of Items Checked (Nic) is self—defining and may be viewed as a measure of acquiescence (LaForge, 1973). Internal consistency reliability estimates (based on communalities) for the summary scores are all above .90. The ICL is reproduced as Appendix D, page 130. (4) Personal Orientation Inventory (POI): The POI (Shostrom, 1964) purports to provide a comprehensive measurement of values and behavior seen to be of importance in the development of self-actualization. The inventory consists of 150 29 two-choice value and behavior items and is scored for the following: Time Competence (Tc), Other/Inner Orientation (I), Self Actualizing Value (SAV), Existentiality (Ex), Feeling Reactivity (Fr), Spontaneity (S), Self Regard (Sr), Self Acceptance (Sa), Nature of Man (Nc), Synergy (Sy), Acceptance of Aggression (A), Capacity for Intimate Contact (C). A description of these categories is presented in Table 3. In a factor analytic study of the P01, Tosi and Hoffman (1972), using an oblique rotation, identified three factors: (I) Extraversion, (II) Openemindedness, and (III) Existential Non-Conformity. Highest loadings on factor I were obtained by Acceptance of Aggression, Spontaneity and Feeling Reactivity. Highest loadings on factor II were obtained by Nature of Man, Synergy, and Time Competence. Highest loadings on factor III were obtained by Existentiality, Self Accep- tance, and Capacity for Intimate Contact. Silverstein and Fisher (1972) reported somewhat comparable factors. The latter authors, however, set out to measure what they believed to be a "built-in" factor structure in the P01 based on item overlap from scale to scale. The authors concluded that "there is some suggestion that item overlap may be responsible for at least the first two factors in the empirical data, although the possibility remains that the item overlap itself is due to relationships among the latent variables that the POI was designed to measure." The POI has also been compared with the EPI and the MMPI. Knapp (1965) reported high negative correlations between EPI Neuroticism and P01 measures of time competence (Tc) and self regard (Sr), while high positive correlations obtained between EPI Extraversion and POI 30 TABLE 3. Description of POI categories. Categories on the POI are presented as a series of opposites Category Tflme competent (Tc) Lives in the present (Time incompetent) Lives in past or future Inner-Directed (I) Independent, self- supportive (Other-directed) Dependent, seeks sup- port of others' views Self-Actualizing Value (SAV) Holds values of self- actualizing people Rejects values of self-actualizing people Existentiality (Ex) Flexible in applica- tion of values Rigid in application of values Feeling Reactivity (Fr) Sensitive to own needs and feelings Insensitive to own needs and feelings Spontaneity (S) Freely expresses feelings behaviorally Fearful of expressing feelings behaviorally Self-regard (Sr) Has high self-worth Has low self-worth Self-acceptance (Sa) Accepting of self in spite of weaknesses Unable to accept self with weaknesses Nature of man, Constructive (Nc) Sees man as essen- tially good Sees man as essen- tially evil Synergy (Sy) Sees opposites of life as meaningfully related Sees opposites of life as antagonistic Acceptance of aggression (A) Accepts feelings of anger or aggression Denies feelings of anger or aggression Capacity for Intimate Contact (C) Has warm interpersonal relationships Has difficulty with warm interpersonal relationships 31 variables spontaneity (S), self regard (Sr) and acceptance of aggression (A). Shostrom and Knapp (1966) offered evidence suggest- ing that the POI predicted therapeutic growth more completely than the MMPI. In addition, POI Time competence was shown substantially correlated (r = -.65) with the MMPI Psychastenia scale, the highest intercorrelation between the scales of the two instruments. Three POI scales were chosen to represent the instrument in this study. Nature of Man (Nc) was chosen as a purported measure of ARO. SAR was represented by the Self—Regard (Sr) scale. An additional seven item scale was chosen (Idef) (Shostrom, 1964) which purported to be a shortened measure of the Inner vs. Other Directedness scale. The scales are presented in Appendix E, page (5) Interpersonal Trust Scale: Rotter (1967) defined interpersonal trust as "an expectancy held by an individual or a group that the word, promise, verbal or written statement of another individual or group can be relied upon." A 40-item Likert-type scale was developed on the basis of this defini— nition. Inclusion of an item on the scale was based on internal consistency with other trust items, relative independence of social desirability (measured by the Marlowe-Crowne SD scale), and spread of scores on the 5-point scale. Fifteen filler items were included to partially disguise the purpose of the scale. These filler items were eliminated in the present battery. The scale is "additive", meaning that a high score "shows trust for a variety of social objects" (Rotter, 1967). 32 Although the scale offered a number of items important to this study, seemingly in relation to the dimension of Other-Acceptance- Rejection, the present author would like to offer a cautionary note. Viewing a high score on the interpersonal trust measure as necessarily healthy and contributing to interpersonal competence may be naive. Current events suggest that trust in social institutions (government, police, etc.) is often misplaced. A study accentuating this point was reported by Hochreich and Rotter (1970): "Have College Students Become Less Trusting?" Data obtained on the Interpersonal Trust Scale from 1964 to 1969 show a drop in mean scores (male and female combined) from K = 72.41 to'K = 66.64. An examination of individual items, however, clearly demonstrated that the drop in trust was less likely in items where the students had personal contact with the social agent (e.g., government). The items in this scale appear most closely related to other-acceptance variables. The Rotter Inter- personal Trust Inventory is presented as Appendix F, page 136. (6) Semantic Differential Inventory (SEM): This section represents a series of seven-point semantic dif- ferential scales drawn from three sources. (a) Eight scales were drawn from the Two Dimensional Inter- personal Inventory, Experimental Version (Hurley, in press, c) (Appendix G, page 138). The eight were chosen on the basis of inter- correlations which demonstrated their ability to cluster into dimensions of self-acceptance-rejection and other-acceptance-rejection. SAR was comprised of shows-hides feelings, active-passive, strong-weak, likes-dislikes self, and open-guarded. ARO was comprised of 33 gentle-harsh, accepting-rejecting, and warm—cold. Because of the computer answer sheet, the scales were shortened from ten to seven points. (b) OK-ness ratings (Hurley-Force, 1971): Two semantic differen- tial scales were chosen based on how OK the individual views both himself (OKs) and others (OKo). These items were based on the theory of Berne (1966) (described in Postulations from Theory). In the Hurley-Force paper (1971), OKs correlated significantly and positively with LaForge's Dom factor. OKo correlated significantly and positively with LaForge's Lov factor. The two (OKs and OKo) also correlated positively and significantly with each other, leaving some question as to the ability of this simple measure to differentiate between SAR and ARO. Unfortunately, by error, OKo was left off the instrument in this study. (c) A series of five semantic scales was included which appeared to ask more directly about the hypothesized dimensions. These were extraverted-introverted, dominant-submissive, stable-unstable, accepts self-rejects self, accepts others-rejects others. In addition to individual scale scores, two summary scores were used accounting for the two hypothesized dimensions (SAR, ARO). SEM 1 (for SAR) included the SAR scales, from (a) above, I'M OK—I'M NOT OK, Extraverted-Introverted, Dominant—Submissive, and Accepts-Rejects Self, a total of nine scales. SEM 2 (for ARO) included the ARC scales from (a) above, along with Stable-Unstable and Accepts- Rejects Others, a total of five scales. The attempt to gain informa- tion by using both overall scale scores and individual item scores ." .‘Mi"" 34 presented a problem. Because of this mixture of scale and item scores one might have expected the items to correlate highly with their respective summary scores. Clusters built around these groupings may have been somewhat artifactual. The semantic differential inventory is reproduced in Appendix H, page 139. (7) Response Set Measures: "set" to For purposes of this study, measures of a tendency or respond in a manner unrelated to the content of the items were drawn from the larger instruments. Social desirability, defined as the tendency to give a "True" response to an item with a socially desirable scale value (Edwards et aZ., 1962), was measured by Edwards' revised (1970) 18 item scale derived from the MMPI item pool, and separately by the ICL Ain score. Edwards' revised scale represented an attempt to control for acquiescent responding by dropping all but nine of the False-keyed items from the 39 item SD scale. Of the remaining 18 items, half were keyed True and half False. Edwards reported the same mean level of responding on both scales. The Ain score is a measure of the average intensity of respond- ing on the ICL. Items on the ICL were divided into four intensity levels. Intensity and social desirability were assumed to be related (Kogan, in LaForge, 1973). For a set of 126 items, Kogan found a correlation of intensity with judged social desirability of -.74 in a sample of 46 neuropsychiatric patient raters and -.73 in a sample of 94 university student raters. LaForge suggested that this 35 correlation resulted from a tendency to judge extremely intense interpersonal behaviors as inapparopriate in most situations, and hence as socially undesirable. For this reason Ain was employed in this study as an estimate of social desirability. In addition to social desirability, acquiescence (tendency to give a "true" response) and lying (tendency to fake) are response sets worthy of note. The present study could only include one measure of each of these. Acquiescence was measured by the ICL Nic score. This score, number of items checked, in its extremes, was a measure of a tendency to answer true (or in this case yes) to personality items. The EPI Lie scale was used as a measure of the tendency to lie. This tendency was regarded as different from social desirability (Edwards et al., 1962). It was not intended that these scales would comprehensively deal with the question of response sets. That challenge was beyond the scope of this study. Their purpose was to provide some measure of the level of responding due to response set, and an idea of where these response sets cluster with major personality dimensions. Time of Battery Times suggested for the measures were (in minutes): (1) iMMPI scales 25 (5) Interpersonal Trust 10 (2) EPI 15 (6) Semantic differential 10 (3) ICL 15 (4) POI scales 10 Total 85 mins The time above did not include time to read and understand instructions. bl 36 Order of Battery The tests were administered in the following randomly selected order: (1) Interpersonal Checklist (ICL) (2) Personal Orientation Inventory Scales (P01) (3) Interpersonal Trust Scale (4) Semantic Differential Inventory (5) Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) (6) MMPI Scales (MMPI) Statistical Method Variables for Cluster Analysis: An examination of the test battery described above resulted in a tally of 33 possible variables for cluster analysis. Variables are presented in Table 4. Next to each measure is its code name for the study. These appellations were used interchangeably with full names throughout this report. A table identifying the predicted clustering of each variable, and actual clustering in the study, is included as Table 18 (page 110). Statistical Technique: The BCTRY Cluster Analysis System In order to determine a statistical technique appropriate to this study, the author reviewed several techniques of factor and cluster analysis. The system of techniques seemingly most suited to both study and author is the BCTRY, developed by the late Robert ..fl... ntu misfih uwnuns \uauv Uni-u ~ .1- i flu H Q§EMU~ F-.N‘H‘1> av HQ .~ ¢\ (.3 37 mm AHau< m>auuammmm mmomzz Ame aofiuaamsm maszmu amuse-caucus scones Ame maouaasm scam mmmomm swan Hszz aHv :oamum>ouuaH Hmzz nammm msonmnmoeam AZMmV swab N ZMm ouoom mumaaom Qm< lawanMHn H Sam muoom humssbm mmuuxm Hmm H HmDMH madam umauH umaua Houuom 0H myooo< mumeuo muoufiumnmumuuu< ammoo< mamm muumfimmumuamuo< m mmHom wummmm-mamm mamammaanamuuamafiaoa zHHom emuumuauuumacH Hem >ouuaHIvmuum>muuxm voxomno eozH eo no: a.Hueo a.H Ae.uaouV e UHz unamuauumaasz AmmeHa mamm mmxaamfinummxfia Hmauam zH< suamaouaH mwmum>< zm<3 sumznwaoo upmwmfia >ogaoH mum: .m> o>oa zmmo smegmaouamao caucmamm zGQuUH aofimmaa upam .m> mucmqaaon 40H 0.3323 2800 Cam: . mamum 9:5me ouamwmai «woo ABM”; . mason mgmfli moanmfium> .muommmav maanum> moanwaum> .oHSwmuav manmaum> no .02 mo .oz mammamcm umumsau now moanmaum> .e mgm season mm. mm. ow. oo. oo. oo. «H. no. Ho. m>Heo< he. oo. No. oo. on. oo. ma. 9 ememm Hm. oo. oo. Ho. we. Ho. oo. o zuzon «s. oo. oo. oo. em. mm. HH. 9 Hszz co. co. Ho. oo. mm. Ho. co. m > damaog cw. ow. mm. Nam mm. mm. em. so. Ho. No. oo. no. «a. mqezmo em. oo. as. oo. co. co. mm. a ammoo< em. oo. oo. oo. co. co. cm. a msoou< on. as. oo. Ho. oo. oo. mm. a 2M<2 cm. em. as. Ho. co. «on No. No. mm. >oaqu co.H No. oo. oo. Ho. oo. HN.H n N mmm H om< ” u m . a no .35 aawfim so no so no No Ho 2: “333 museum Houmaau .Amfiamu vmvnnmwm N: Hoops (Nufiamaaaaoo Hmfiuumm mandamus muoumsao wovomoxm mo ousuoouum noumsao oaoamm HouOu I mfiwhamaml> Hmoaufimsw do muoumoao .n mam¢a 50 so. suaaanmfiaum ow. sufiufiamp nausea om. so. am. ma. so. oo. co. so. as. zHHom es. «m. oo. oo. no. no. No. a museum Hm. mm. oo. Ho. mo. Ho. ea. c zmmo em. em. oo.H 5H. co. am. an. «a. «a. H 2mm UGO memmmmmw "so am. suaaanmaamm mm. suaufiam> nausea Na. co. es. oo. Ho. aH. no. a on es. oo. mm. oo. oo. Hm. so. a 2H4 m 82.22 " 0 mm. suaafinmaaom as. suauaaa> season mm. ow. mN. oo. oo. mo. AH. no. No. ozomam mm. am. am. Ho. oo. «a. co. mo. mo. mqmOHHUH n n M>HHU< H N om< N sum mHmovamu .oz MHmsvamu .oz .uHeam no moHanum> MHosvaou .oz mHmstmmu .oz .uHmao so mmHannm> "ummmum ”HmoHuHesfl mumonmn "unmoum “HmoHuHQam muoaHmmn AmHmmHmam HmoHuHmaov mwdmu mmoH.|oo0H. :H mHmsvaou mo umnazz .w mHm oHoaog No. No. HN. oo. NH. so. so. oN. N>Hao< Ns. oo. no. NN. co. sH. a HHNHNN mm. oo. NN. NN. No. oo. o szoa NN. oo. mm. mN. No. No. a Hszz so. oo. NH. ss. Ho. no. a > aHmaon mm. mm. Hm. so. No. Ho. NH. no. on so. om. os. NH. NH. NH. Ho. sz om. oo. Ns. Ho. co. Ho. sN. NH. Nmomzz ms. oo. on. No. NN. so. 9 nos: so. oo. NH. Ho. as. no. a Naomzz HN. oo. sN. oo. ss. No. a omzHNN sN. oo. oH. Ho. Nm. no. a mzmmmm. N H42 " o sm. muHHHanHmm No. NUHoHHm> aHmaon no. mm. on. so. so. oo. oo. NN. NHazoo sN. oo. Ho. co. co. Nm. a meooos mm. oo. co. co. co. no. a amooos Hs. oo. oo. co. so. as. a ass: as. Ho. so. oo. oo. Ns. o >0HHoH 8H 8. Ho. 8. 8. 8H m N E H ems " o m.n so .azo oHoaHm no so No No Ho Hoe HmumaHo monouo umumsHo .HMHHUH vmvamdwm N: HmHOH N: HMHuumm mumnHmoa mumumoHo venomous mo musuoouum HoumoHo oHQBMm Houou I mHthmaml> Hammad no muoumoHo .m MHN aHmaoo so. so. mN. oH. so. oo. mo. oo. zHHoN ss. No. No. No. oo. No. o monomm oN. oH. Ho. oo. mH. o zmmo a .mmmmmmmw "no mo. NuHHHomHHmm so. 533.3 338 HA. No. Hm. No. NH. so. oo. oo. NHmsam No. No. on. Ho. NN. Ho. Ho. Ho. ozomam oo. oo. Hm. No. oo. Ho. o zooHoH on. Ho. as. oo. no. oo. o mmHoN so. no. on. Ho. Ho. HH. o Homoos NN. Ho. oo. oo. Ho. oo. o onNHH NN. so. oo. oo. Ho. No. o MozH oo.H NH. sN. oo. Ho. oo. o H zoo local s Nam ” o. m.o so .aoo «Hmch mo so mo No Ho Hoe youmsHo mmuoom umumaHo .anHmu vwncmmxm N: Hmuoa N: HmHuumm mumaHmma Ho.uaooo o moose 72 TABLE 10. Oblique factor coefficients - preset analysis Clusters Variables 1-ARO 2-MAL 3-SAR 4-SAR S-Transparency soc emot POISR .24 -.57 .49 .74 .48 POINC .30 -.12 .13 .04 .18 POIIN .01 -.25 .11 .20 .41 TRUST .22 -.18 .17 .16 .19 EPILIE .02 -.13 -.05 -.07 -.10 EPIEXT .37 -.16 .64 .32 .32 EPINEU -.14 .86 -.30 -.53 -.26 TENS -.17 .89 -.20 -.46 -.25 USP -.41 .50 -.06 —.16 -.24 ODY -.16 .80 -.18 -.39 -.23 INT -.28 .59 -.79 -.65 -.48 D .24 -.78 .49 .59 .31 NIC .23 .37 .05 —.06 .00 AIN -.07 .48 -.23 -.35 -.27 ICLDOM .ll -.46 .64 .71 .44 ICLLOV .65 -.02 .04 —.04 .21 SHOFEE .14 -.22 .31 .33 .65 WGENTLE .54 .04 .09 -.04 -.03 ACTIVE .06 -.25 .44 .36 .26 .ACCEPT .59 -.14 .21 .23 .26 STRONG .08 -.24 .40 .52 .19 OPEN .39 -.21 .36 .42 .70 WARM .63 -.13 .21 .18 .30 LIKSEL .18 -.38 .30 .79 .29 IMOK .26 -.40 .32 .79 .25 EXTRAV .22 -.14 .79 .40 .38 DOMIN -.04 -.12 .67 .44 .24 STABLE .25 -.34 .17 .49 .15 ACCSEL .32 -.52 .43 .77 .30 SEM 1 .31 —.45 .79 .91 .82 SEM 2 1.00 -.27 .31 .40 .31 SEX .28 .17 .10 .08 .04 73 wow MMTENS EP INEU NIC I \ / l \ SEX / / \ \ / \ LIKSEL GENTLE / SEM *ARM \ I OK STRONG ICLLOV ACCEPT \ X SARemot s a 2 AGGOTR\ flLmn C x / POIN TRUST STABLE POISR MMSD SPAN diagram 4 - Preset analysis 74 /l\ ’/” 1LEXTRAV DQQiE. /// I SARsoc ACTIVE POINC "MMINT \ GENTLE l I LLO l C V w RM / \ I DOM / TRUST \ SEM [2:1ng \ STIYNG ‘\\ CCOTH sm\ \ POISR \ / SAR I"”’*CCSEL emot IMOK LIKSEL STABLE SPAN diagram 5 - Preset analysis 75 The Cluster Structure Analysis again added GENTLE to the cluster. Table 9, column 3 (page 70) demonstrated that the addition of GENTLE to the ARO cluster would raise the reliability of the cluster. The author therefore considers GENTLE to be a member of the cluster in further discussion. -§é§emot* SARemot represented the greatest change from empirical to preset cluster analysis. The cluster was defined by IMOK, LIKSEL, ACCSEL, POISR, SEM l and ICLDOM. The addition of the two key dependent variables, ICLDOM and SEM l, greatly strengthened this cluster. The mean intercorrelation between definers rose more than .20 from the empirical analysis, to become .56. Partly because of these additions and partly because of being preset second, the cluster accounted for .35 of the initial communality. The domain validity of the cluster was .96 (rather than .94) and the reliability was .93, up from .88. The total communalities of the definers remained high. The greatest changes were observed in the partial communalities (Table 9, page 70). POISR, which had so much of its variance accounted for by MAL, now had nearly all of it (.49 of a totalh2 of .58) on SARemot' ICLDOM and SEM 1, both of which had been extremely diffuse in the empirical analysis, have most of their communality accounted for by SARem on the preset. Their 0t oblique factor coefficients (Table 10, page 72) were also higher on the preset. POISR had a higher coefficient (.74 from .68), while the other definers had 23's that were slightly lower than on the empirical analysis. The reason for this can be seen by comparing SPAN diagrams l, 2 and 5 (pages 52, 55, 74). NOte in diagram 1 that 76 POISR is the definer of SARemot farthest from the dimension definer Y. Then in diagram 2 observe that ICLDOM and POISR are midway between the clusters. The result of adding ICLDOM and SEM l to the cluster can be viewed in diagram 5. POISR is now closer to being the center of the cluster, while the semantic differential items are slightly farther off. The cluster structure analysis component again added STRONG and STABLE to the cluster. Table 9 (page 70) demonstrated that neither variable raises the reliability of the cluster. In addition, they had low (.30) communalities. The author does not consider them part of the cluster. SAR was preset third, after SARem , because of the SAR . -—-soc- soc addition of ICLDOM and SEM l to the latter. The cluster was defined 0t by EXTRAV, DOMIN, MMINT (reflected) and EPIEXT, as before. The decision to add ICLDOM and SEM l to SARem , and then to preset ot SARSOC third, in some ways mathematically weakened this cluster. The mean correlation between the defining variables lessened from .41 to .30, reflecting a spreading out of the cluster. Again, this is pulled down by low, inaccurate communality estimates. The cluster, in this position, accounted for .10 of the communality. In other ways there was little change in the cluster. The reliability remained virtually the same at .84, and its domain validity coefficient also remained virtually equal (.92) to its ‘value on the empirical analysis. Interestingly, the oblique factor (:oefficients for the defining variables of the cluster also remained 77 the same, reflecting their stability in relation to the other clusters. A somewhat different note was sounded in viewing the communali- ties, both total and partial (Tables 5 and 9, pages 49, 70). While the total h? remained the same, partial hz's on SARSOC were greatly lessened for MMINT, EXTRAV and DOMIN. For MMINT there was a definite shift from sharing .28 and .11 of its variance on the empirical analysis with MAL and ARO, respectively, to sharing .23 of h? with SARemot’ and virtually none with ARO and MAL. The diffuse nature of this cluster is also described in SPAN diagram 5 (page 74). In the diagram it is difficult to suggest a center for the cluster. It can be seen that MMINT, like ICLDOM and SEM l, is more closely related to SAR than on the empirical analysis. emot .55E- The maladjustment cluster was selected fourth in the preset analysis in order to allow the ARO and SAR clusters to pick up a larger part of the correlation matrix. This, of course, led to a lessening of the total communality accounted for by the cluster. Its proportion of the communality was .15, as opposed to .28 in the empirical analysis. The cluster definers remained the same; MMSUSP was not added as a definer of MAL. Its relatively lowh2 (compared with the cluster definers), relatively low oblique_£g, and position on the SPAN diagram did not suggest any gain from adding it as a definer. In addition, adding it would not have increased the cluster reliability coefficient (Table 5, page 49). Clearly, then, MAL with its original definers remained a salient cluster. Its reliability and domain validity remained about equal to their values on the empirical analysis (.91, .95, respectively, 78 Table 9, page 70). Interestingly, the interrelatedness of this cluster also suffered in the preset. The mean correlation among the definers equalled .45, a reduction of about .20. This is reflected in SPAN diagram 4 (page 73). The cluster appears at the top of the diagram, grouped loosely around the dimension definer Y. The defining variables MMBODY and MMTENS clustered to the left and closer to MMSUSP than previously. EPINEU spread the cluster to the right. MMSD appears in the lower right of the diagram, reflecting its polarity with the other MMPI scales. Had it been reflected and appeared at the top of the diagram, it would have been off the page. Total communalities of the definers, while remaining high, were generally lower than on the empirical analysis (Table 9, page 70). The range of_112 on this cluster was from .65 to .74. Note also that the partial communalities of the definers were more diffuse (Tables 5 and 9, pages 49 and 70) than on the empirical analysis. EPINEU, for example, had .30 of its total_l'_1_2 of .71 accounted for by SAR . MMSD, as a more surprising example, had approximately equal emot portions of variance on MAL and SARemot° This was apparently related to the addition of ICLDOM and SEM 1 to SARemot and was a result of the sensitive relationship between the MAL and SAR clusters. The oblique factor coefficients for all variables on MAL remained about the same as they were on the empirical analysis (Tables 6 and 10, pages 51 and 72). Three variables were added in Cluster Structure Analysis: MMSUSP, AIN, and NIC. MMSUSP was once again most highly related to MAL 79 (f9 = .50) but maintained its relationship with ARO (£3 = -.41) Once again its addition would not have raised the reliability of the cluster (Table 9, page 70). It is, therefore, not considered a true definer of the cluster. AIN and NIC, having been eliminated as a cluster pair, correlated most highly with MAL. Their oblique _§g's were .48 and .37, respectively. Without their own cluster they have very low total_h2's in the domain (.40, .31, respectively). They were not added as cluster definers then, for purposes of this analysis. Transparency (TR). Because of its possible uniqueness as a cluster, Transparency, while only defined by two variables, was retained for the preset analysis. With SEM 1 assigned to SARemot’ there was no chance of raising the reliability of the cluster. In fact, all the mathematical relationships of this pair of definers remained virtually identical to the results of the empirical analysis and therefore will not be repeated in this section. Cluster structure analysis once again added POIIN to this cluster. The reader may compare Table 9, columns 3 and 4 (page 70) to see that its addition as a definer of this cluster would not have raised the reliability of the cluster. This cluster then remained defined by a doublet. Relationship Between Clusters - Preset The oblique factor correlations between clusters for the preset analysis are presented immediately below those for the empirical analysis for easy comparison by the reader (Table 7, page 61). 80 Mbst notable amongst these intercorrelations was the reduction in the relationship between SAR and ARO. In the empirical analysis, EARO vs. SAR = .34 and EARO vs. SAR = .39, while the preset soc emot reduced this to_r__ARO vs. SAR = .29 and-EARO vs. SAR = .30, soc emot more closely approaching independence. The relationship of ARO with MAL was also lessened (£_= -.25 to.£ = -.21), but MAL's rela- tionship with both SAR clusters remained essentially the same. The relationship between the two SAR clusters was also higher on the preset (E = .49 to_£ = .63). Finally, the relationship between Transparency and SARemot changed from .42 to .55. One may also observe the changes of the SAR clusters by comparing their partial communalities (Tables 5 and 9, pages 49 and 70). The definers of SARSOC empirically had most of their communality accounted for on that dimension. On the preset they shared more of their communality with SARemot' The definers of SARemot which empirically shared a sizable portion of their communality with MAL, on the preset had most of it accounted for on SAR . emot Preset Summary NOte The preset key cluster analysis accounted for .92 of the total communality of the domain. The decrease was primarily due to the elimination of the NIC-AIN cluster. Total communalities of individual variables within the domain were generally bettered, with the notable exception of NIC and AIN (see Table 9). Again with these exceptions, and some larger residuals for SEX, the number of residuals in the .1000—.l999 range was not greatly increased. Tryon refers to this as 81 a criterion for accepting the statistical soundness of the preset analysis. Non-Clustering_Variables The BCTRY system eliminates variables as non-clustering if their common variance with the domain is less than .20. In the empirical analysis this category included three variables: the Nature of Man (POINC), Rotter's Trust Scale (TRUST), and the EPI Lie scale. SEX was added by the preset. Four other variables had communalities of below .30 on the preset: Inner-directedness (POIIN), ACTIVE, AIN, and NIC. Note once again that POINC and TRUST, which lie outside this domain, appear to share some unique factor as suggested by the principal axis analysis. Variables that do not cluster may be either unreliable, or a source of unique variance. Rotter (1967) reported a split-half reliability of .76 for a sample of 547 students, suggesting that the TRUST variable may be unique. No appropriate measure of internal consistency was reported for POINC. Response Set Measures Four supposed measures of response set were included in the variables of the study: MMSD, EPILIE, AIN, NIC. AIN on the ICL and the SD scale of the MMPI purported to measure social desirability. The EPI lie scale was purported to measure a tendency to lie. NIC on the ICL was supposed to be a measure (albeit a poor one) of acquiescence. 82 EPILIE had a communality below .20 in the domain and therefore did not cluster. A review of its raw correlations (Appendix I, page 140) showed it to be virtually unrelated to other variables of the study. It does appear in the principal axis analysis as a definer of factor 5 (rotated). The MMPI Social Desirability scale (reflected) was a definer of the MAL cluster. MMSD was also highly related to the two SAR clusters (fg's = .49, .59, respectively). AIN and NIC clustered together on the empirical analysis. When this cluster was eliminated, AIN and NIC clustered loosely with MAL (fg's = .48, .37, respectively). The raw correlations between the purported measures of response set are presented below in Table 11. AIN correlated .67 with NIC but TABLE 11. Correlations between purported measures of response set Variable AIN NIC MMSD EPILIE AIN .67 -.37 .06 NIC -.23 -.12 MMSD .01 insignificantly with the others. This is puzzling in that AIN and MMSD were both supposed to measure social desirability. NIC also correlated very low with MMSD and EPILIE. MMSD and EPILIE did not correlate at all. 83 Comparison Between Subgroups - Male vs. Female In order to perform the analysis, the data were divided into males and females. There were 97 females, 76 males, and 11 §s who did not identify sex. The COMP analysis program of BCTRY demands that clusters for each group be preset on the same definers. The definers from the preset analysis were used. The program computes an index of collinearity between the clusters for each group which is geometrically equal to the cosine of the angle between them (cosine 6). The cosine 6 values may be read like correlation coef- ficients. The program also performs an empirical V-analysis using the original clusters as variables. The table of cosines between dimensions for males and females is presented below (Table 12). The numbers in the diagonal repre- sent the similarity between groups for each individual cluster. TABLE 12. COMP analysis for males and females Cosines among dimensions Females ARO SAR SAR MAL TR emot soc Males ARO .67 .30 .26 -.28 .37 .38 .82 .62 -.57 .53 emot SAR .37 .60 .81 -.44 .54 soc MAL -.38 -.61 -.45 .79 -.44 TR .36 .58 .62 -.41 .62 84 Thus, the relationship between ARO (male) and ARO (female) was repre- sented by a cosine of .67, a moderately high value. The cosine between SARemot (male) and SARemot (female) was .82. Between SARSOC (male) and SARSOC (female) the cosine was .81. Between MAL (male) and MAL (female) the cosine was .79. The relationship of Trans- parency for males and females is .62. The higher order V-analysis generated three clusters. Their definers, oblique factor coefficients, reliabilities and validities are reported in Table 13. In this case, without referring to the specific statistics, note that the male and female clusters for each dimension, ARO, SAR (both clusters), MAL, and Transparency, did cluster with their counterparts, with one exception. In cluster 1, the two SAR dimensions cluster together for both males and females. Cluster 2 represents ARO, again for both males and females, and also reflects the relationship of the Transparency cluster for females to ARO. Cluster 3 represented the relationship of at least part of the SAR dimension to MAL. The reader is also referred to SPAN diagram 6, which is some- what more difficult to read than the previous diagrams. Note that the author has drawn in double arrows between clusters that represent the same dimension for different groups. For example, ARO is repre- sented in the lower right corner of the triangle. Its configuration between males and females appears extremely close and is pictured by the small double arrow. What is most important in comparing the hypothesized dimensions over the two groups is that the reader note the length of these arrows. Transparency is abbreviated as (TR) in the SPAN diagrams. 85 TABLE 13. V-analysis performed on preset clusters as part of COMP analysis - males vs. females Oblique is on Total Cluster Clusters Definers cluster communality scores C1 SAR - MALE D .83 .75 emot SAR - MALE D .82 .71 soc SAR - FEMALE D .82 .74 emot SAR - FEMALE D .81 .69 soc TR.- MALE D .77 .62 TR - FEMALE D .70 .54 Domain validity .92 Cluster reliability .96 C2 ARO - FEMALE .92 .85 ARO - MALE .69 .50 TR - FEMALE .64 .54 Domain validity .91 Cluster reliability .82 C3 SAR - FEMALE D -.84 .74 emot SAR - MALE D -.84 .75 emot MAL - FEMALE D .80 .68 Domain validity .95 Cluster reliability .91 86 / TR \ l ’ \ Cl / C2 ARO \’ \ SAR soc SAR soc C3 -MAL SPAN diagram 6 - COMP analysis for males and females (higher order clusters) 87 One more result to aid in identifying differences between groups is presented. The author performed empirical V-analyses on each group to see if any differences in clustering occur. The variables in each cluster are presented side by side in Table 14. The positions of individual variables will be discussed in the discussion section. Comparison Between Subgropps - Non-Specific Groups In order to perform the analysis, the data deck was shuffled and divided into subgroups of_N = 110 subjects and N = 72 subjects. The definers of the preset cluster analysis were used to prepare the groups for comparison. Cosines between clusters for the two groups are presented in Table 15. Once again, it is the numbers in the diagonal that are of greatest interest. The cosine value between random groups for ARO was .68, again moderately high. For SARemot the cosine was .80. For SARSOC it was .77. The highest cosine between random groups is attained by MAL (cosine = .83). Trans— parency attained a cosine of .62 between groups. The V-analysis on the clusters of both groups resulted in three clusters (Table 16). Cluster 1 appeared to include both SAR.dimen- sions and MAL, which was highly negatively loaded on the cluster. The reliability of the cluster is .92 and its domain validity is .96. Transparency appeared to span clusters 1 and 2, representing perhaps a bridging of these major dimensions. Cluster 2 was the ARC dimension with ARO for both groups loading highly on the cluster. The cluster's reliability was .82 and its domain validity .91. Cluster 3 repre- sented a relationship between MAL for both groups and SARSOC for both 88 TABLE 14. Empirical clustering of variables - male vs. female Variables Cluster Male Female SEM 2 (D) SEM 2 (D) ICLLOV (D) ACCOTH (D) WARM (D) GENTLE ARO ACCEPT (D) ACCEPT (D) ACCOTH ICLLOV GENTLE WARM (D) OPEN SEM 1 (D) LIKSEL (D) ICLDOM (D) ACCSEL (D) POISR (D) IMOK (D) IMOK (D) POISR MMINT (D) STABLE (D) SARemot ACCSEL (D) MMSD (D) LIKSEL (D) STRONG STABLE ACTIVE EXTRAV (D) SEM l EPIEXT (D) MMINT (D) DOMIN (D) ICLDOM (D) SAR LIKSEL (D) EXTRAV (D) 8°C DOMIN (D) ACTIVE EPIEXT STRONG AIN MMTENS (D) EPINEU (D) EPINEU (D) MMTENS (D) MMBODY (D) MAL MMBODY (D) MMSD (D) MMSD (D) MMSUSP NIC MMSUSP Other OPEN (D) AIN (D) Clusters SHOFEE (D) NIC (D) POIIN (D) POIIN (D) 89 TABLE 15. COMP analysis for non-specific groups - cosines among dimensions Group 1, N = 110 ARO SAR SAR MAL TR emot soc ARO .68 .34 .36 -.31 .37 Group 2 SAR .34 .80 .60 -.55 .57 N = 72 emot SAR .29 .61 .77 -.43 .58 soc MAL .27 -.57 -.42 .83 -.44 TR .45 .50 .55 -.32 .62 90 TABLE 16. V-analysis performed on preset clusters as part of COMP analysis — random samples, N-llO, N = 72 Cluster Oblique £2 Total scores 'Clusters Definers on cluster commun. (h ) on D's C1 SAR - Group 1 D .84 .72 emot SAR - Group 2 D .81 .66 emot MAL - Group 1 D -.80 .81 MAL - Group 2 D -.77 .75 SAR - Group 1 .76 .80 soc SAR - Group 2 .71 .67 soc TR - Group 1 .70 .57 Cluster reliability .92 Domain validity .96 C2 ARO - Group 2 .88 .79 ARO - Group 1 .74 .57 TR - Group 2 .66 .53 Cluster reliability .82 Domain validity .91 C3 MAL - Group 1 D .46 .81 MAL — Group 2 D .44 .75 SAR - Group 1 D .43 .80 soc SAR - Group 2 D .35 .67 soc Cluster reliability .74 Domain validity .86 91 groups. The loadings on this cluster were comparatively low, as was its reliability of .74. Its domain validity was .86. SPAN diagram 7 presents these relationships geometrically. This diagram is confusing because of the loose relationships in cluster 3. Once again, the reader should note the close relationships for all but the Transparency cluster between comparable dimensions on the two groups (notated by ++). 92 \ \ / / TR1 TR2 \ \ / \ \ SAR 1 I ,SAR 2 C2 \ x emOt emot \ ARC 1/ C \ 1 \ / ARO 2 -MAL 1 -MAL 2 SPAN diagram 7 - COMP analysis for nonrspecific groups (higher order clusters) DISCUSSION The Salient Dimensions The data generally supported the contention for two salient and reliable dimensions representing self—acceptance-rejecrion (SAR) and other-accepLance-rejection (ARO). More specifically, data analyses revealed four major clusterings of variables, repre- senting ARO, maladjustment (MAL), and two SAR clusters, representing its social and emotional modes. MAL, while highly linked to both SAR clusters, also appeared to represent a unique variant related to more severe maladjustment. The findings suggested that SAR and ARO were nearly sufficient to account for the personality question— naire domain as represented in this investigation. The clustering of individual variables is described in Table 18 (page 110). 'Clgsis£_l: _A:ceptange versus Rejection of Others (ARO) ‘“-HAI, - ._ «.a_. A reliable and valid cluster apparently representing the ARO dimenSJOn was salient. The dimension contained constructs like warmth, gentleness, being accepting, and accepting others. It was also described by Interpersonal Checklist (ICL) statements like: "can be frank and honest", "helpful", "considerate", etc. Further— more, it was moderately negatively linked with MMPI items (MMSUSP) related to a construct of suspicion (oblique_££ = —.4l). This ARO 93 94 cluster accounted for about 25% of the common domain variance. The domain variance figures reported in the discussion represent the largest portion of variance attained by the clusters in the different analyses. Mathematically and geometrically the cluster was fairly clear. Its saliency was underscored by the fact that most of the communality of each variable included in the cluster was accounted for by the cluster (Tables 5 and 9, pages 49 and 70). SPAN diagrams l, 4, and 5 (pages 52, 73, and 74) also depicted this cluster as fairly tightly knit. It appeared to occupy a distinct portion of the domain as represented on a sphere. The cluster was also theoretically sound in the sense that it was saturated by measures which were clearly relevant to acceptance and rejection of others. Employing the individual scales of the semantic differential instrument along with its SEM 2 summary score may have artifactually strengthened the ARO cluster in the analyses. Of ARO's six defining variables, four were individual scales already predictably related to the summary score SEM 2. Closer examination, however, suggests that this is a minor point. Not only are the correlations between the individual scales and SEM 2 not so high (.59 to .69) as to clearly indicate an artifact, but the ICLLOV and SEM 2 scores, which form the core of this cluster, contribute most importantly to its validity. Thus, it appears that the cluster would not be greatly altered by methodological refinements. Finally, the scales and items contributing to this ARO cluster may not have adequately represented the ARC construct, perhaps due 95 to a restrictive selection of ARO related measures. The ICLLOV items and its low communality (22 = .49) suggested some uniqueness in this ARO cluster, as did its modest relationships with both POI Nature of MAN (NC) and Rotter's Trust Scale (TRUST). While the semantic differential items also had low communalities (h2 .41, -—WARM = h2 - 35 h2 = 34 h2 = 38 preset) the relia- ° ’ -ACCOTH ' ’ ° ’ ’ -— ACCEPT ’ —- GENTLE bility of these one-item scales could not be estimated. Thus, the unique variance of these items cannot be determined either. In terms of scale scores, then, ARO was defined only by SEM 2, ICLLOV, and a limited relationship with MMSUSP, with both of the latter having low domain communalities. A more satisfactory delineation of ARO requires further research. Clusters Representing Acceptance versus Rejection of Self (SAR) Two distinct, but conceptually sound, SAR clusters were found which jointly accounted for up to 45% of the communality. Their saliency in the domain seems undisputed. These clusters were labelled SAR (SARSOC) and SAR (SAR ), conforming social emotional ' emot with Foa's (1961) description of facets. Thus, Foa suggested that within a facet structure one must take into account that there are both social and emotional modes of acceptance and rejection. The SAR clusters identified in this study appeared to embody this distinction. SARsoc contained the Extraverted vs. Introverted, and Dominant vs. Submissive semantic differential items, the reflected MMPI Introversion scale, and the EPI Extraversion scale. This cluster was interpreted as representing the individual's perception or 96 definition of his own action or behavior. SARemot’ on the other hand, consisted of measures more related to the individual's feelings about and evaluation of himself. These measures, POI Self-Regard, Accepts self vs. Rejects self, I'm OK vs. I'm not OK, and Likes self vs. Dislikes self, were conceptually divergent from the elements of SARSOC. The SAR clusters, while highly valid and reliable, were some- what diffuse. This was reflected by lower intercorrelations of their definers as compared with the ARO and MAL clusters. Also the MMINT and EPIEXT measures shared significant communality with other clusters (MMINT with MAL, and EPIEXT with ARO). Additionally, two important SAR variables, the Interpersonal Checklist Dominance factor (ICLDOM) and the SAR semantic differential summary score (SEM 1) were about equally dependent on each SAR cluster. ICLDOM had empiri- cal oblique fg's of .64 and .66 on SARSOC and SARem , respectively. 0t The comparable SEM 1 values were .79 and .82. SEM 1's content seemed slightly biased toward SARem (five items) and less toward SARSOC 0t (three items). ICL items appeared about equally divided, but were difficult to apportion between clusters because of that instrument's complex scoring system. The nearly equal associations of ICLDOM and SEM l with both SAR clusters presented a problem in interpretation. The preset cluster analysis demonstrated that the addition of these two measures to SARemot strengthened that cluster but weakened SARsoc’ and also enhanced the intercluster linkage. A comparison of SPAN diagrams 2 and 5 (pages 55 and 74) revealed that the SAR clusters moved closer 97 together on the preset. While this solution also lessened relation— ships of ARO with the SAR clusters, a solution which strengthened one SAR cluster at the expense of another seemed arbitrary. The decision to add ICLDOM and SEM l to SARSOC would likely have produced the opposite result. For this reason an interpretation which allowed ICLDOM and SEM 1 to be partially subsumed under each SAR cluster and which also provided a psychological basis for the relationship of the two SAR clusters was chosen and is presented in Figure 4. The two clusters are depicted as representing ends of a self-acceptance- rejection (SAR) gradient with ICLDOM and SEM l centrally located between SAR and SAR . soc emot The Maladjustment Cluster (MAL) The MAL cluster was an unexpected finding. It was defined by the MMPI scales for Tension, Body Symptoms, reflected Social Desira- bility, and EPI Neuroticism. MMPI Suspicion and Introversion were also highly related (:9 = .50 and .59, respectively, on the empirical analysis). MAL plainly contained a heavy loading of measures usually used to assess pathology or maladjustment. The cluster accounted for up to 28% of the communality in the analyses. In retrospect, this cluster's appearance seems less surprising. Since dimensionality is determined not only by the similarity of relationships with some referents, but by a minimal interdependence on others, the sheer similarity of the MMPI and EPI pathology items was strongly suggestive of a common body of referents. Two of these MMPI scales (MMINT and MMSUSP) appeared to importantly contribute to the relationships of MAL with SARSOC and ARO. MAL and ARO were only 98 PAISR | STABLE AgSSEL I K VbKSEL STABLE emotional Figure 4. Diagrammatic representation of the SAR gradient. 99 slightly linked (E = -.25, empirical, -.21, preset). MMSUSP, which loaded —.41 on ARO, and shared equal, but modest, communality with ARO (.17) and MAL (.17), largely accounted for their linkage. MMINT partly accounted for the MAL and SARSOC (E = .35, empirical) linkage, sharing nearly equal communality with each cluster (.28, .34, respectively). The oblique factor coefficients of MMINT on MAL and the SAR clusters suggested a strong negative relationship between SAR and MAL. Table 17, below, abstracted from Table 6, provides a review of the relevant factor coefficients. TABLE 17. Oblique factor coefficients relevant to the relationship of SAR and MAL (empirical analysis) MAL SARsocial SARemotional POISR -.57 .49 .68 EPINEU .86 -.3O -.52 MMTENS .89 -.20 -.48 MMINT .59 -.79 -.S6 MMSD -.78 .49 .57 ICLDOM -.46 .64 .65 ACCSEL -.52 .43 .79 SEM 1 -045 .79 .82 polar opposite of some portion of the SAR gradient. Examination of these relationships suggests that MAL may be a It was earlier 100 noted that MAL's interdomain correlations with SAR and SAR soc emot were —.35 and -.59, and were largely unchanged by the preset analysis. Furthermore, on the empirical analysis the negative relationship between SARemot and MAL (g = -.59) was somewhat stronger than the linkage of SARSOC and SARem (5 = .49) (Table 7, page 61). SAR ot emot’ then, appeared strongly negatively linked to MAL. In addition, scru- tiny of Table 17 disclosed that negative MAL coefficients consistently accompanied positive coefficients on at least one of the SAR clusters, and vice versa. SPAN diagram 2 (page 55) facilitates visualization of the evidence for polar opposition. In this diagram, if both SAR clusters were grouped closer to ICLDOM and SEM l, and if these variables were moved to the right (180° from MAL), the polarity case would be strengthened. Although it would have facilitated confirmation of the two salient dimensions hypothesized, the evidence did not fully warrant viewing MAL as a polar opposite of SAR. Too much variance was left "unanchored" to support that interpretation. Furthermore, MAL seemed more highly linked to the emotional mode of SAR than to the social mode. What can be stated more confidently is that, within this domain of tests, measures of pathology appear highly biased in the direction of measuring a rejection of self. The author concluded, then, that an MAL cluster did exist; it was highly negatively dependent on the SAR dimension, but in addition it did suggest a real dimension of maladjustment, not satisfactorily accounted for by the present SAR and ARO dimensions. 101 A Minor Cluster: Transparency (TR) Because it accounted for only 6% of the communality on both the empirical and preset analyses, this doublet cluster of Open vs. Guarded (OPEN) and Shows feelings vs. Hides feelings (SHOFEE) appeared peripheral to the search for major dimensions. The cluster also con- tained POIIN, a measure of independence and self-supportiveness, tangential to the domain. The cluster was retained as much for its possible importance as a behavioral construct as for mathematical reasons. Openness and the showing of one's feelings were viewed as highly important behaviors in the development of interpersonal competence. While both variables were expected to cluster with SAR (Table 18, page 110), they showed ties to both SAR and ARO. The relation— ship between Transparency and both SAR clusters, however, was stronger than its correlation with ARO (Table 7, page 61). A review of partial communalities and oblique factor coefficients (Tables 5 and 6, pages 49 and 51) also suggested that Transparency was related to both dimensions. OPEN largely accounted for Transparency's link with ARO, sharing a sizable portion of its communality (.15) with that cluster. Openness and showing feelings as a Transparency cluster, then, were viewed as related to both SAR and ARO, accounting for some of the latter's interdependency. Other Groupings Two other groupings of variables reported from the principal axis analysis deserve note: POINC and TRUST, and ACTIVE, WARM and STRONG. The factors which loaded on either POINC and TRUST, namely, 102 factors 10 (unrotated, Appendix K), and factor 9 (rotated, Appendix L), strongly supported confirmation of the originally expected relationship between these variables and ARO. Factor 9 demonstrated the shared loadings of these variables with ARO variables. ‘Factor 9 also loaded -.32 on MMSUSP, the one MAL variable related to ARO. SPAN diagram 1 (page 52) illustrated this relationship. POINC and TRUST lie off the sphere but in the general area of ARO. In terms of content, the POINC and TRUST items appeared to deal with a kind of social-political orientation toward others. The temptation to view this factor as an important adjunct of ARO was counteracted by the low correlation between POINC and TRUST (E = .14). Another grouping of variables that appeared to overlap SAR and ARO showed up as factor 7 (rotated, Appendix L). This factor loaded heavily on ACTIVE, WARM, and STRONG, all semantic differential items. On the BCTRY, ACTIVE was generally part of SARSOC while STRONG fell toward the center of the SAR gradient. WARM was a definer of ARO. However, these variables all had low communalities: 2 2 2 h 20, h .25, pWARM = ._ ACTIVE = - __ STRONG = .36. Since it is impossible to predict the unique variance represented by these one-item scales, it is also impossible to determine whether these variables account for a dimension linking SAR and ARO. In addition, because this factor accounted for less than 5% of the variance, it was clearly not a salient dimension. Relationship of the Dimensions SARSoc and SARem were only mdldly associated with ARO, thereby 0t confirming a second hypothesis (page 44). The most direct estimates 103 of this relationship were the correlations between oblique cluster domains (Table 7, page 61). The correlations between ARO and SARSOC were .34 (empirical) and .29 (preset). Thus, not more than 11.5% of the cluster's variances were shared between them. Likewise, for ARO and SARem , quite comparable correlations, r = .39 (empirical) 0t and .30 (preset) accounted for not more than 15% of their covariances. In addition, generally for all the analyses, those variables that loaded highly on ARO loaded very minimally on the SAR clusters, and vice versa (Tables 6 and 10 and Appendices K and L). SPAN diagram 5 (page 74) further supports this view. If each cluster were right at the ends of the dimension defining axes, then all clusters would be completely independent. As the clusters move away from these points they become related. It can be seen that the ARC cluster was centered on the dimension definer Z. The emotional SAR cluster centered to the left of the dimension definer X, along the are from Z to X. The distance between the clusters represents their relation- ship. The same view can be used to see that SARsoc and ARO were not precisely orthogonal, and therefore slightly interdependent. This minimal interdependence of SAR and ARO conflicts with the theoretical position of Rogers, cited earlier (page 17) and has serious implica- tions for personality theory and psychotherapy (to be discussed later). Validity of the Item Sample The validity issue concerns whether the fallible, real scores on the measures are comparable to "true" scores derived from an infinitely large battery of personality measures. The domain validity coefficient, 104 separately determined for each cluster (Tables 5 and 9, pages 49 and 70), estimates this relationship from knowledge of the intercorrela- tions of the clusters. Domain validity coefficients for the four major clusters ranged from .91 to .95 (empirical), very close to the preset findings (.92 to .96). These high coefficients provided ample evidence for validity of the test sample to represent the domain, and thus for accepting the third hypothesis that the item sample in this study has a high validity coefficient with respect to the per- sonality questionnaire domain. Additional validational evidence was provided by the non- specific groups cross-validation study (page 87). That study deter- mined the relationship (cosine 6) between groups for each cluster. The cosine values for SARSOC (.77), SARemot (.80), and MAL (.83) were quite high. For ARO the cosine value was slightly lower (.68), but still adequate. These values demonstrate substantial similarity of clusters across groups which further supports the view that the clusters represented the relevant domain adequately. A serious concern.was whether the experimenter's choice of instruments unduly influenced selection of clusters which confirmed the hypotheses. Tryon and Bailey (1970) noted that the clusterings sometimes result from "forces in the thinking of the investigator that cause him to measure some attributes and not others, or social and biological forces currently at work generating Special correlations between some attributes...in a particular sample." The next step, then, must be an expansion of the number and variety of measures in studies similar to this one to further test the saliency of SAR and ARO (see Implications, page 114). 105 Eysenck's "Super Factors" Controversy over whether Eysenck's Extraversion-Introversion factor (EPIEXT) as measured by the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) would cluster more closely with SAR or ARO was described earlier (page 8). Eysenck's description of extraversion clearly contained the suggestion of its relation to ARO, while conceptually it seemed more sensible to put it with SAR. In the clustering process EPIEXT had a domain communality of only .47, making its clear interpretation difficult. Its communality was unequally divided between SARSOC (.30) and ARO (.15), while its oblique factor coefficient (empirical) was .64 with SARSOC and .38 with ARO. Thus, EPIEXT clustered mainly with SARsoc’ but also maintained some tie to ARO. It seemed reasonable to conclude that EPIEXT was primarily a measure of SAR, permitting acceptance of the initial part of hypothesis 4 (page 44). The second part of the hypothesis, that Eysenck's neuroticismr stability (EPINEU) will cluster with ARO, was rejected, as EPINEU had practically no relationship to ARO (£9 = -.l6)° It was, however, a major definer of MAL (£9 = .86) and highly negatively related to SARSOC (£3 = -.52). Thus, it appeared that the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) was heavily biased toward measuring both maladjust- ment and normality related to acceptance and rejection of self, and had only minimal linkage with the acceptance and rejection of others. 106 Response Set Measures Two measures of response set, AIN (the average intensity level of the items checked) and NIC (number of items checked), both taken from the ICL, clustered together and were minimally dependent on other clusters and variables. Although highly intercorrelated (r = 067), they were also largely independent of ICLDOM and ICLLOV (5's of —.25, .Ol, .07, and .17), as suggested by LaForge (1973). If NIC and AIN were actually measuring response sets, then ICLLOV and ICLDOM appeared largely free of these. In addition, for this measure at least, the two response sets, social desirability and acquiescence, were highly related. Were this conclusion generalizable, it would contradict the generally accepted implication that these are relatively independent response sets (Kassebaum at al., 1959). However, there is strong evidence that this conclusion may not generalize to other data, as AIN correlated only -.37 with the MMPI social desirability measure. Apparently these separate measures of "social desirability" were not assessing the same thing. The "social desirability" response set needs further exploration. When the NIC-AIN cluster was dropped from the preset analyses, the two variables linked with maladjustment, as did MMSD, as one of its reflected definers. In addition, MMSD loaded highly on both SAR clusters but low on ARO (£3 = .28, empirical, .23, preset). These data support the generally accepted contention that, as scores on maladjustment measures go up, scores on social desirability measures go down, and vice versa. In addition, the data were confirmatory 107 to Wiggins (1968) and Crowne et al. (Hurley, 1972) in that social desirability was interpreted as a facet of SAR. The fourth response set measure (EPILIE), measuring a tendency to lie, fell far outside the domain. It also appeared isolated in the factor analysis. This made any interpretation relative to its effect difficult, aside from noting that it neither occupied pivotal positions nor was its variance explained by the clusters. Thus, neither AIN, NIC, nor EPILIE occupied pivotal positions in the major clusters or had their variance explained by these clusters, confirming hypothesis 5 (page 44) for these measures of response set. However, this did not hold for "social desirability" as measured on the MMPI. For MMPI items a tendency to "look good" moderately influenced scores on the MAL and SAR clusters. While this overlap could be interpreted as detracting from the earlier interpretation of SAR, it is equally plausible to view this tendency as a legitimate characteristic of highly self-accepting individuals. In this sense it did not seem surprising that a social desirability measure related to SAR. Subsample Comparisons The two SAR clusters and MAL appeared essentially identical across sexes (Table 12, page 83). This was confirmed in SPAN diagram 6, and also in Table 14 (page 88). ARO, while reasonably similar for men and women (cosine = .68), showed more instability than the other major clusters. The ARC sex differences were strongly linked to differences in Transparency (TR). SPAN diagram 6 (page 86) 108 shows that TR clusters for males and females were widely separate, with TR for females being linked to both ARO and SAR clusters. The only significant difference here was the relationship of the variable OPEN to the ARC and SAR clusters. For women, Openness appeared more indicative of ARO, while for men it was closer to SAR. A more minor sex difference appeared in the clustering of the response set variables AIN and NIC. AIN (ICL social desirability) and NIC (ICL acquiescence) were more highly linked with maladjustment for men than for women. For the two non-specific groups, the four major clusters were essentially identical across groups. Additional evidence was provided by the higher order V-analysis reported in Table 16 (page 90). Cluster 1 contained both SAR clusters for both groups, MAL for both groups, and TR for Group 1. SARSOC for Group 1 was the only non- definer included in the cluster. This clustering accentuated the SAR—MAL linkage. Cluster 2 contained the ARC clusters for both groups and TR for Group 2. The two ARO clusters were strongly linked (SPAN diagram 7, page 92). The TR clusters were again spread between the major clusters in much the same way as in the male-female comparison. Thus, these data essentially confirmed the results of the male- female analysis, and hypothesis 6, that subsamples of the total sample reveal identical clusters in the domain. Measurement of ARO and SAR (Clusteringgof Variables) The adequacy of an item, scale or test to measure a construct is based on its ability to discriminate between constructs. This was 109 operationalized by choosing variables which had high empirical factor loadings on one cluster and low loadings on all others (Table 6, page 51). Highest factor loadings (clustering) for the variables on each of three analyses, in addition to predicted a priori clustering, are given in Table 18. The semantic differential scales were apparently adequate measures of SAR and ARO. They distinctly differentiated between SAR and ARO, and between SARSOC and SARemot better than other measures. Those scales that loaded highly on ARO, GENTLE (vs. harsh), WARM (vs. cold), ACCEPT (accepting vs. rejecting), and ACCOTH (accepts others vs. rejects others) all discriminated well. SEM 2, the ARO summary score, discriminated less adequately between these dimensions. All of these variables had been predicted to measure ARO. STABLE, also a predicted measure of ARO, clustered with SARemot' EXTRAV (extraverted vs. introverted) and DOMIN (dominant vs. submissive) both discriminated SARSOC from other dimensions. Dominant was especially unlinked to ARO, and neither R component related highly to SARem The best SARe measures were LIKSEL (likes ot' mot self vs. dislikes self), IMOK (vs. I'm not OK), STABLE (vs. unstable) and ACCSEL (accepts self vs. rejects self). All except STABLE were predicted measurescfl SAR. SEM l, the SAR summary score, was only a fair discriminator of SAR from ARO and contained elements of both SAR clusters. The remaining predicted semantic differential SAR indi— cators, STRONG (vs. weak) and ACTIVE (vs. passive), related to SAR but not to a differentiating degree. Contrary to expectation, SHOFEE (shows vs. hides feelings) and OPEN (vs. guarded) formed a separate llO mm. A42 am. Aav he: mm. Ans has new mamas: on. he: on. max on. as: om< mmsmzz ow. a4: om. Ans qougoH u 08 uoam mm. mam Haw Ans m mo wcfiumumsau Hmduom van umuummxm .mH mqmHso< o~.- om< em. om< as. om< om< mqszmo ma. me no. Aav ma me. Any my m Awaaumumaau Hmsuo< As.uao0v ma mamas wwsmaas you can: mpmnasz 112 wmuowvmum mus ua umnu momma :umumaao oz: .mHOHUMM .mU .oanwaum> mo wcfiuwumSHolcoc mucmmmuamu woman xGMHm .mcabaou MMHUm Ga NoumSHo mo nmcfimmv you vaMHm Aavn .HMumaao uoa vases mHAmHum> «no umnu .wafiumumSHo mo :oauuuvoum on uammmuaou mmomam xamamw ss.- om< am. Anv om< mm. Anv om<. oma msooo< uosm uoam uoam ea. mam an. Aav .m