ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF SOME OF THE DYNAMICS OF THE INTERPERSONRL RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PAIRS OF MULTIPLE THERAPISTS By Jerry A. Treppa The purpose of the present investigation was to examine how therapists who have chosen to work together in multiple therapy fulfill each other in terms of their needs. The first part of the study compared the interaction of needs among pairs of multiple therapists with the inter- action of them among randomly paired therapists. The second part of this investigation was concerned with the relation- ship between a therapist's satisfaction in the multiple therapy relationship and the needs of his co-therapist. Two groups of subjects were selected for the study. The multiple therapy group consisted of 12 teams of multiple therapistso The control group consisted of 12 teams of indivi— duals who were paired SO that no individual, who did multiple therapy, Would be paired with his co-therapist from multiple therapy. The two groups are comparable with re5pect to age and the amount of time engaged in individual therapy. Two instruments were used to examine the variables of this study. The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) was used Jerry A. Treppa to study the needs of the therapists. Eleven cards of the TAT were administered to each subject, and the strength of each need was obtained. The subjects' stories were scored in terms of the following needs: abasement, achievement, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, deference, dependence. dominance, heterosexuality, nurturance, and passivity. Two raters were used; the interrater reliability for the scoring of the TAT needs was of sufficient magnitude to warrant the use of this scoring system. The Interpersonal Satisfaction Scale (183) was developed in the context of the present research and is an index of a therapist's satisfaction with his co-therapist in multiple therapy. Each individual in the multiple therapy group was asked to rate on a one to seven scale the degree of satisfaction he felt in inter— acting with his co-therapist. Based on a two—week interval separating the two administrations. the stability coefficient for the 188 is .99. The results of this study can be summarized as follows: I. The needs of pairs of multiple therapists do not complement each other more on a dominance-receptive dimension than those of the randomly paired thera- pists. II. Tentative support was given to the proposition that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more along a dominance-passivity dimension than randomly paired therapists. Jerry A. Treppa III. Some support was found for the idea that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more on a dependence—nurturance dimension than randomly paired therapists. IV. Pairs of multiple therapists are not more similar in their needs for abasement, achievement, aggression, dependence, and heterosexuality than randomly paired therapists. V. Pairs of multiple therapists were found to be more similar in their needs for affiliation, autonomy, and nurturance than randomly paired therapists. VI. The amount of satisfaction derived from interacting with one's co-therapist is not related to his co— therapist's needs for abasement, achievement. affiliation, and heterosexuality. VII. Satisfaction with one's partner in multiple therapy is positively related to his co—therapist's needs for autonomy, dependence, and nurturance. It was concluded that certain needs in an individual partially determine the selection of his co—therapist and that satisfaction in the multiple therapy relationship is related to Some of his co—therapists' needs. A general conclusion of this investigation is that the mutual relation— ship that exists between pairs of multiple therapists is affected by the needs of both therapists. The present paper raised questions about multiple therapists in general and about the interaction between pairs of multiple therapists and gave suggestions which should prove helpful to further investi- cations- AN INVESTIGATION OF SOME OF THE DYNAMICS OF THE INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PAIRS OF MULTIPLE THERAPISTS BY nu” P Jerry A: Treppa A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology Winter, 1969 fi—VMH, —‘ . To my wife Ann and to Bill and Jbsephine, my multiple therapists, who have cared ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS To Bill Kell, the Chairman of my Committee, who nurtured my ideas and fantasies and who supported my attempts to change and learn, I express my sincerest warmth and caring. To Bertram Karon, whose help and creative ideas have benefitted me throughout my graduate training, I offer my thanks. To William Mueller, whose skills as a person, thera— pist, supervisor, and researcher have benefitted me in more ways than I can clearly say, I present my admiration and gratitude. To Dozier Thornton, who chose to be a member of my committee without really knowing me and who supported my endeavors, I am thankful. To my co-therapists, who have experienced much with me and who have helped me to change, I acknowledge my appreci- ation and respect. To the therapists who readily participated in this study, I extend my thanks. To Robert Kurtz and Richard Morrill, who rated the tapes in this study, I am grateful. TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . History of Multiple Therapy The Application of Multiple Therapy in Various Therapeutic Situations Individual Therapy Group Therapy Family Therapy Definition of Multiple Therapy: Multiple Therapy vs. Co-therapy The Importance of the Relationship between Two Therapists in Multiple Therapy Professional vs. Dynamic Attraction in the Selection of a Co-therapist Definitions of the Needs Employed in This Study Complementary Need Patterns between Pairs of Multiple Therapists The Dominance—Receptive Dimension The Dependent-NUrturant Dimension Similar Need Patterns between Pairs of Multiple Therapists Need for Achievement and Autonomy Need for Abasement and Aggression Need for Affiliation and Heterosexuality The Relationship between a Therapist's Satisfaction in a Given Multiple Therapeutic Relationship and the Needs of His Co-therapist HYPOTIIESES a o o a o o o o o u o o o a o o o a o o I Page 21 28 30 30 32 34 34 34 36 39 METHOD . . . . . . Subjects a o - o o o o o o o o o o o o I The Multiple Therapists' Sample The Control Sample Measuring Instruments The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) Interpersonal Satisfaction Scale (158) Procedures The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) Interpersonal Satisfaction Scale (188) Procedures for Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis Hypothesis RESULTS . . . . . Hypothesis 1: Statistical Analysis of the Data 1 2 3 4 o o o o o o n n o o o o a o o o Complementary Needs Along A Dominance—Receptive Dimension Hypothesis 2: Complementary Needs Along A Dependence—Nurturance Dimension Hypothesis 3: Hypothesis 4: Multiple Therapy Related to His Co-therapist's Needs .DISCUSSION . . . . Similarity of Needs Therapist's Satisfaction in o o o a n o o o o c o o o o o 0 Suggestions for Further Research SUMMARY . . . . . REFERENCES . . . . APPENDICES . . . . Page 41 41 41 41 42 42 42 44 44 47 48 48 50 51 52 52 54 56 57 60 74 77 80 87 Table LIST OF TABLES Pairs of needs that are seen as satisfying each other along a dominance-receptive dimension . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Interrater reliability of the TAT needs . . . Conceptual form for data in which no. of pairs of therapists who are similar or bipolar on a given need are presented . . Bipolarity of needs: proportion and between group Z values (both corrected for dis- continuity) for the (N = 12) pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist and control samples who have a bipolar relation- ship with respect to their needs derived from the TAT for deference, dominance and passivity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Complementary needs: Pearson product moment correlations and between group Z values between the multiple therapist and control samples on the dominance-passivity and dominance—deference complementary dimensions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Pearson product moment correlations and between group Z values between the multiple therapist and control sample on the dependence-nurturance dimension . . . . . Similarity of needs: proportions and between group Z values (both corrected for dis- continuity) for the (N = 12) pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist and control samples who are similar in their needs derived from the TAT for abasement, achievement, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, dependence, heterosexuality, and nurturance . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi Page 31 47 49 53 54 55. 57 4 k1 Table Page 8. Pearson product moment correlations between the satisfaction score of each indivi- dual in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) on the ISS and his co- therapist's needs derived from the TAT . 58 vii .fi‘ *- -w r" A,” L: -_ Table A. No. LIST OF APPENDIX TABLES of pairs of therapists in the , multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for deference . of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = I2) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for deference . . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the , multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for passivity . . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the' multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for abasement . . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the- multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for achievement . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for affiliation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for aggression . . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii Page 88 88 89 91 91 92 92 of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for autonomy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for dependence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for heterosexuality . . . . . . . . . . . . of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for nurturance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix 93 93 94 94 INTRODUCTION Some therapists prefer to work alone, while a minority prefer, at times, to engage in the psychothera- peutic venture as a team of two therapists. As utilized in this paper, the use of two therapists who are involved spontaneously in the treatment of an individual, a couple, a family, or a group is defined as multiple therapy. The therapeutic value of multiple therapy is an important subject which has gained increasing attention in the last twenty years. However, it is neither the purpose of this paper to evaluate the merits of multiple therapy as compared to indi— vidual therapy nor to defend the former's usage. The present paper will attempt only to examine how therapists who have chosen to work together in multiple therapy fulfill the needs of each other. History of Multiple Therapy As pointed out by Dreikurs (1950), and Dreikurs, Schulman, and Mosak (1952a), the earliest use of the principles of multiple therapy occurred in the early 1920's by Adler and his associates at the Vienna Child Guidance Clinic. When emotional blocking and resistance occurred, the parents and the child were seen by two counselors. They discovered that frank discussion of the child's diffi- culties in the presence of him accelerated the therapeutic process--that the child responded more readily in such a situation. Hewever, it was not until 1939 that anyone else reported using such an approach. At this time, Reeve (1939) reported that he was stimulated by a discussion at the National Conference of Social Workers in 1933 on the value of having a social worker and a psychiatrist work together with a patient for the purpose of training the social worker to think further about the implications of such an interview. After some consideration of this matter, he came to the con- clusion that such an interview, which he called the ”joint interview”, carried ”. . . values beyond that of teaching social workers how to conduct an interview" (Reeve, 1939, p. 743). and he began to experiment with this method of treatment. He reports that this experiment produced thera- peutic as well as educational benefits. In addition to being better able to gain a clearer understanding of the facts in the patient's situation than in individual psychotherapy, this treatment method provided. . . a Wider opportunity for identification, and thera- peutically, a parental situation is set up insofar . as the social worker and psychiatrist may be indenti- fied as such. While at first it may seem to repre- sent a greater complexity, as a matter of fact, it. presents greater flexibility to the patient by giVing him an opportunity to react to one or the othefi. Theoretically, it affords an Opportunity for t et patient to love and hate without haVing to direch. this toward any one person. It further afforgs im an opportunity to react with a minimum fear 0 re taliation (Reeve, 1939. p. 747). Hadden (1947) too was primarily concerned with adequately teaching students about psychotherapeutic practices and included a medical student in his therapy groups for this reason. Hadden was pleased with the results he obtained. He found that the inclusion of the students in therapy groups made their training shorter and more effective, gave them a better understanding of the power of psycho- therapy, gave them experience under supervision, and helped them to recognize their own interpersonal difficulties. While the trainees, for the most part, did not direct the group sessions, Hadden believed that the effectiveness of the sessions was increased by the student's presence. Whitaker, Warkentin, and Johnson (1949) reported the first intensive use of multiple therapy as a means of treating patients. At the time they wrote their first article they had been using multiple therapy experimentally for three years. Whitaker et a1. state that they had two major objectives for experimenting with multiple therapy. Their primary objective was to develop a means of treatment which would enable the therapists to share with one another the emotional impact of the therapeutic interview and to enhance the capacity of the therapist. The second and more ". . . long-range objective was to develop concepts that would facilitate the teaching of psychotherapy" (Whitaker et a1., 1949, p. 439). They report that initially the second thera- pist was a critical observer who did not participate very actively in the therapeutic process. Because the second therpaist became unhappy with this role, they came to the conclusion that it would be better to allow him to partici- pate more actively. They found that having two therapists working together did not disturb the therapeutic process but facilitated it--that the participation of the two thera- pists held greater significance for the patient than using only one therapist. They discovered that multiple therapy enabled them to see more of their own emotional functioning in the interview and that this recognition helped them to grow professionally. This and subsequent articles by Whitaker and his associates did much to bring multiple therapy to the attention of the professional community. A year after Whitaker et a1. published their original article, Dreikurs (1950) wrote a paper, which is often cited, on the techniques and dynamics of multiple therapy. Dreikurs initially introduced multiple therapy into his practice as a result of certain emergency situations. His experiences with this form of treatment lead him to conclude that multiple therapy was useful for teaching therapists and that it benefited the patients as well as the therapists. Dreikurs proposed that multiple therapy had four advantages which usually characterize dynamic psychotherapy. He believed that multiple therapy: (1) created the proper transference relationship which was conducive to the patient's growth; (2) helped the patient to gain a better understanding of himself: (3) helped the investigation of the patient's problems by providing two people who were sincerely trying to evaluate the facts for the patient's benefit; and (4) stimulated the patient to change his attitudes, goals, outlook and approaches in his life (Dreikurs, 1950, p. 791). Haigh and Kell (1950), who were influenced by Warkentin, focused on multiple therapy primarily as a method for training and research in psycho- therapy. While they felt that multiple therapy raised a number of questions that were worthy of investigation, they also recognized its therapeutic usefulness. In the same year, Whitaker, Warkentin, and Johnson (1950) published an article in which they discussed the use of multiple therapy for a different purpose. In their ex- perimentation with this method, they found that it was useful in resolving the therapeutic impasse which sometimes arises in individual psychotherapy. They believed that the thera— peutic impasse occurs as a result of a disturbance in the human relationship between the patient and the therapist. Sometimes this disturbance becomes so great that the thera— peutic process is blocked. If the therapist is unable to learn what the difficulty is and act accordingly, to change it, a "stalemate" occurs which frustrates both the patient and the therapist in their quest for growth. Whitaker et al. report that at such times the introduction of a second thera- pist breaks the impasse. The second therapist may be present for one session or for several-—as long as he is needed. If the patient and therapist are able to express their mutual difficulty, the consultant [the second therapist] is more likely to become an effective catalyst to the relationship. In this triangular situation it has seemed logical that the therapist should question his own motives first, so as to provide a better basis for the patient's participation. The presence of the consultant is also valuable because he is able to carry part of the reSponsibility for the patient, so that the thera- pist can express what he had suppressed from fear of his own immaturity (Whitaker et a1., 1950, pp. 644-645). Dyrud and Rioch (1953) and the present writer have also found that using multiple therapy when a therapeutic impasse arises in individual psychotherapy is helpful in getting the patient and the therapist through the impasse. The Application of Multiple Therapy in Various Therapeutic Situations Individual Therapy The benefits of using two therapists have been noted in a variety of therapeutic situations. A number of articles have appeared that discuss the therapeutic advantage of having tWQ therapists when dealing with psychotic patients. Work- ing with psychotics is not an easy task. As Dyrud and Rioch (1953) point out, it is easy for a therapist to become tired, discouraged and angry because of the rigidity and uncommunicativeness that these patients display. Some therapists feel that the expenditure of effort and emotional resources that are required in treating a psychotic patient is too great for one therapist alone (Hayward, Peters, and Taylor, 1952; Whitaker, Malone, Warkentin, 1956; Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964; Fielder, 1967, Warkentin, 1967). One of the major advantages of using multiple therapists with psychotics is that it offers both moral and physical support to the therapists. "With the overtly psychotic and actively suicidal patient, the presence of more than one therapist makes possible a treatment experience which otherwise would be a hazardous undertaking" (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964, p. 170). This mutual support frees the therapists to function in a way that is most helpful to the patient and allows him to react to a wider range of the therapeutic relationship. Whitaker and his associates have found that multiple therapy has been so effective in treating severely disturbed patients that they state that it is preferred treatment with psycho— paths and psychotics. "When a patient is severely ill, so that all the pertinent material is hidden and disguised, the presence of more than one doctor can do much to pick up a greater number of clues as to the conflicts involved" (Hayward et al., 1952, p. 247). Whitaker et al. also believe that multiple therapy allows each therapist to be more professionally and emotionally involved with such patients. Such involvement intensifies the patient's and therapist's emotional experiences with each other and makes the impact of the therapists upon the patient more noticeable and powerful (Whitaker et al., 1956; Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964). When a therapist feels free to be deeply involved with a patient, he can exert a stronger integrative force upon the patient: multiple therapy seems to provide this freedom (Hayward et al., 1952). Therapists who use multiple therapy with severely disturbed patients (psychotics, psychopaths, suicidal patients) believe that multiple therapy is useful in helping these patients, in particular, to clarify their ambivalent feelings. For these patients the experience of intense ambivalent feel— ings is unreasonable and intolerable. "When working with a single therapist, the patient will often show great diffi- culty in expressing any feelings, because of this intolerable mixture of love and hate. When two or more physicians are present, the patient can split his feelings, so that strong affection is expressed for one physician and hostility for another” (Hayward et al., 1952, p. 245). In this way, the patient learns to deal with both kinds of feelings and grows emotionally. Another advantage of using multiple therapists with severely disturbed patients is that it is a method of treat- ment which supports and "gives expression to the patient's regression”, while it provides the control for him and demands that he become responsibly involved with others (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964). One therapist may deal with primary Process material while his co-therapist focuses on the Secondary process content (Frame, 1965). "It appears that one therapist can be selected to represent the reality principle, allowing the patient to give fantasy reactions free play with the other threapist. A stable relationship with one therapist seems to provide a framework, or a safe anchorage, permitting the patient to feel and explore violent reactions which under other circumstances might be restrained through fear of a complete break with reality" (Mintz, 1963a. p. 127). In this way tw° different and conflicting ends can be incorporated together. Group Therapy The use of two therapists in psychotherapy has been applied to other areas of treatment besides individual psycho— therapy. As seen in the literature, interest in multiple . therapy groups has been increasing. Group therapy with two therapists has been used to treat: chronic neurotic patients in a mental hospital (Cameron and Stewart, 1955): eighth grade behavior-problem boys (Daniels, 1958) and girls (Staples, 1959); psychotics (Lundin and Aronov, 19527 Demarest and Teicher, 1954; Orange, 1955); marital couples (Kadis and Markowitz, 1966; Gottlieb and Pattison, 1966); acting-out adolescent boys (Kassoff, 1958; Adler and Berman, 1960); and college students (NUnnelly, 1968). For the most part, articles on the use of multiple therapists in group therapy have been favorable to this method of treat- ment. The use of two therapists in group therapy has im— pressed many therapists with the intensity and variances of 10 transference that can occur in a group (Demarest and Teicher, 1954: Hulse, Lulow, Ruidsberg, and Epstein, 1956; Pine, Todd, and Boenheim, 1963: Mintz, 1963b, 1965). In this regard, such a method of treatment seems to provide a greater dynamic base for interaction than is possible in group therapy with one therapist. As Solomon, Loeffler, and Frank (1954) have pointed out, the interaction which takes place in this setting nurtures both the therapists and the group. The presence of a male and female therapist in the group provides more of an opportunity for patients of both sexes to identify with an adequate person of the same sex and to work out whatever problems they may have with both sexes. While the two therapists provide a positive stimulus for transference reactions, ". . . they [also] stand for masculine and feminine authority; real people against whom family stereotypes of male and female can be tested; and models for an enhanced understanding and acceptance of sexual role" (Mintz, 1965, p. 294). ”The technique of using co- therapists of different sexes also allows for a reversal of the roles that the patient anticipates that figures of either sex must adopt in order to relate to him. . . When the patient is faced with the problem of modifying, in the presence of therapists of both sexes, his expectations of the roles of a man and a woman, he is presented with a situation that actually exists in the family" (Demarest and Teicher, 1954, p. 202). Another advantage of having two therapists in a 11 group is that it helps the patient confront the therapist of "the dreaded sex", and about whom he has anxieties, with the support of the other therapist who belongs to the less frightening sex. As Mintz (1963a, 1965) states, the patient chooses the therapist of the less dreaded sex to support him in his attempt to deal with the therapist of the dreaded sex whom he fears will destroy him. Still another advantage of using this method in group therapy is that it helps the therapist's understanding and appreciation of the individual and group effects (Pine et al., 1963). While it may be true that more complex patterns of resistance and interaction occur in multiple-therapy led groups than in usual group therapy, it also seems true that multiple therapy provides more opportunity to understand this complexity than does regular group therapy (Rabin, 1967a). The two therapists are more able to evaluate the "interplay" of the transference phenomenon in the group than one therapist because the two therapists are able to focus on more patients in a group at the same time, with the same personal contact, than a single therapist can (Lundin and Aronov, 1952). "By having two observers who can view the same phenomenon from different angles, it is possible to more accurately appraise the stimulus field existing in the group at a given time and thus distinguish between reality reactions and transference reactions" (Demarest and Teicher, 1954. p. 201). 12 The use of two therapists in group therapy has proven to be effective with patients who need a lot of authority to control their actions. Kassoff (1958) and Adler and Berman (1960) began using two therapists in a group setting as a matter of necessity with acting-out adolescent boys, whom these authors characterized as having a low tolerance for their feelings of ambivalence. One of the difficulties in treating such a group of individuals is that their intense ambivalent feelings usually block the therapist in his attempt to have them respond to the world in a different way. The therapist's relationship with such an individual usually remains intact only if the therapist is seen in a positive light and does not stimulate negative feelings at the same time. The fulfillment of such a goal is almost impossible and even if this goal could be met, one would certainly question its therapeutic value. Kassoff, and Adler and Berman discovered that using two therapists in a group was effective in dealing with the great ambivalent feelings of the delinquent boys they were treating. ”In the dual leader- ship situation, however, the ambivalent feelings could be handled by expressions of hostility toward one of the group leaders, while the relationship between the group and the other leader would remain relatively intact. This kept channels of communication open . . ." (Adler and Berman, 1960, p. 218). The use of two therapists in group therapy has been criticized by Slavson, a leading psychoanalytic group 13 therapist, on the grounds that it "splits off" the patient's transference feelings between the two therapists (Slavson, 1950, 1960). He argues that the patient's ambivalent feelings cannot be resolved because the presence of the two therapists tend to dilute the transference and may encourage abreaction rather than help the patient. As indicated above, many writers do not agree with Slavson's viewpoint. NUnnelly (1968), who did the first experimental study of the use of multiple therapy in group counseling, obtained results that certainly suggest that Slavson is wrong. Nunnelly found no evidence to indicate that the multiple therapy group did not make as many gains as the group counseling experience led by indivi— dual therapists. In fact, he has some evidence which suggests that group counseling with multiple therapists produces more over-all improvement (in terms of ego strength, social intra— version, perception of mother and father acceptance, and self— acceptance) than those led by single therapists. It is this writer's contention that the use of multiple therapists in a group setting clarifies and dramatizes the transference relationship and actually helps in its resolution. Further, " . . the splitting of transference between a male and female therapist cannot be viewed as an objection inasmuch as every human being develops, biologically and psychologi- cally, in a tWO-parent family" (Mintz, 1965, p. 293). 14 Family Therapy Multiple therapy has become the preferred method of treatment in family therapy (Belmont and Jasnow, 1961: Rubinstein, 1964: Whitaker, Felder, and Warkentin, 1965; Framo, 1965; Sonne and Lincoln, 1966; Rubinstein and Werner, 1967). One of the major advantages of using multiple thera- pists in treating a family is that it prevents the therapist from being engulfed by the family. By working together, the two therapists are better able to deal with the family's maneuvering and problems than one therapist could alone (Rubinstein, 1964). "By virtue of their maintenance of unity in a symbolic 'healthy marriage', the therapists can help each other resolve their experience of dispair, and save each other from being incorporated, against their will, into a family system in psychopathological dyads” (Sonne and Lincoln, 1966, p. 199). If the therapy is not done by multiple therapists, the therapist may become bound by the dependency of the family. A single therapist may often feel helpless in dealing with a family and can be easily seduced in such a situation to support and share various pathological family beliefs. "In breaking the congealed 'people salad' by the addition of another person, the therapist may become free to move in and out of his involvement with the family rather than frozen into it" (Whitaker et al., 1965, p. 339). The presence of two therapists allows one therapist to withdraw when necessary while the other carries on. 15 Another advantage of using multiple therapists in the treatment of a family is that it leads to more accuracy in understanding dynamics and in the selection of thera- peutic approaches. The use of two therapists enables them to intervene more effectively into the family system and to understand what needs to be done to break up the pathological alliances that have been formed to the detriment of all of its members (Rubenstein and Werner, 1967). Still another advantage of this approach is that it ". . . helps allay family anxiety so that the members can more freely explore the depths of their relationships" (Framo, 1965, p. 201). Definition of Multiple Therapy: Multiple Therapy vs Co—therapy At this point, it seems necessary to discuss more thoroughly what multiple therapy means in the context of this paper. As Spitz and Kopp (1963) point out, the use of more than one therapist at one time in a treatment situation has been called by many names: multiple therapy, co—therapy, role-divided therapy, three-cornered therapy, joint inter- view, co-operative psychotherapy and dual-leadership. Hew- ever, it is this writer's belief that it is a mistake to think automatically that multiple therapy is synonymous with the approaches implied by these other terms. While it is true that in all of these approaches two therapists are used, the similarities between multiple therapy, as defined here, and the other approaches are slight. Multiple therapy is 16 more than just having two therapists work together: In this writer's opinion, one of the serious theoretical mistakes that has been made by some writers in discussing multiple therapy is the equation of multiple therapy with c04therapy. ’ Multiple therapy refers to the use of two therapists who are involved intimately, affectively, and spontaneously with each other and their patient(s). The real impact of this approach occurs as a result of the therapists' spontaneous and reciprocal interaction. Multiple therapy is the simultaneous thera— peutic approach by two or more separate and yet related therapists to a single patient, married couple, a family, or patient group. . . . Its therapeutic effectiveness is felt not alone by the patient but also by the therapist. Thus, for the former it makes available a uniquely charged milieu in which there is such openness and spontaneity that growth becomes a necessity. For the latter, supported by a close colleague, there is offered an unlimited possibility to search for and to find new meanings (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964, p. 164). Agreeing with Mullan and Sanguiliano, the present writer views co-therapy as a limited form of multiple therapy. . The relationship between the two therapists in multiple therapy is often existential in nature, while in co—therapy the two therapists rely on distance by role playing. In co—therapy, the two therapists are not oriented toward a phenomenological process but toward fulfilling certain pre- arranged functions. The therapists' roles are deliberately set up by the therapists to solve a specific situation (Mintz, 1963b). The usual situation in co-therapy is for .111! lllfl I I'll!) 17 one therapist to act in a supportive, passive, and good manner while the other therapist plays an aggressive, dominant role. Such an approach: (1) is artificial in nature; (2) limits the therapists' freedom to be themselves and thus grow; (3) depersonalizes the patient by depersonali— zing the therapists; (4) may prove to the patient what he has always thought--that he is inadequate in his attempt to change his environment, since he cannot change his therapists; and (5) encourages our cultural stereotype about how a man and woman should be and feel. As Haigh and Kell (1950) state one of the major characteristics of multiple therapy is that ". . . we have a genuine therapeutic situation where each therapist is ego-involved and in contact with the direct experience of therapy" (Haigh and Kell, 1950, p. 660). The demands of multiple therapy in contrast to co—therapy are much greater in terms of the involvement required of the therapists. The intensity of the therapeutic situation is increased by such involvement which adds to the depth of the encounter. "The relationship between the two therapists in multiple-therapy in contrast to co—therapy calls upon each therapist to be more totally present. . . In co- therapy, this relationship in all of its complicated nuances is more apt to be partly avoided or entirely ignored” (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964, p. 184). The outline which follows that presents the differences between co-therapy and multiple therapy in a concise form is taken from Mullan and Sanguiliano (1964: PP9 184-185). 18 MULTIPLE-THERAPY CO—THERAPY Transactional, leading to en— Interactional and self- counter (change in all) actional (change in patient(s)) [Such an activity is existen- [Such a way of behaving is tial in mood. All members of characterized by self—deception the situation are active par— in the therapists and their ticipants who exist only in patient(s). These deceptions relationship to each other] are manifested in their inter— action. The effort seems to be on changing the patient] Non-teleological Teleological Phenomenological Pseudo—Scientific Intimate Less intimate Tendency away from role Tendency to role play playing Status mobility Status fixity Function: Satisfaction of Satisfaction limited to a all participants through change in patient toward a deep modification of exist— set goal ing relationships Overt and continuous use of Covert use of the therapists' the participants' unconscious unconscious and overt use of the patient's some of the time. The Importance of the Relationship between Two Therapists in Multiple Therapy While it has been recognized that the crucial factor in the success of multiple therapy is the relationship between the two therapists, very little has been written about it. The study of the dynamics of this relationship deserves more attention. Each team of multiple therapists must have a mutual relationship, in which both are deeply involved with each other, if they are to be effective. Mullan and Sanguiliano 19 believe that the deep feeling the two therapists have ”. . . for one another causes them to respond to the patient in a meaningful and integrated manner. . . . Moreover, it seems important for the two therapists to be closer to each other than to any one of the patients they are with” (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1960, pp. 561—562). The therapists need this kind of relationship in order to prevent the patient from repeating his life long pattern of separating and conquering his parents and in order to help the therapists realize their growth wishes. By being involved with each other in a healthy manner, the therapists help the patient recognize the self-defeating aspect of separating the multiple therapists. Multiple therapy ceases to be effective at those times when the two therapists are in an emotional conflict. While the patient may be extremely able to increase the dif- ficulties between his therapists, he is not the cause of their emotional conflict. The patient's attempts to separate his therapists could not work unless the relationship between them contained the seeds of conflict before the patient was seen (Kell and Burow,in press). At the times that the conflict occurs in the therapeutic process feelings of pain, betrayal, jealousy, anger, competition, fear, etc. may be felt by one or both of the therapists. Such feelings of hostility, rejection, etc. stemming from the conflict may be transferred. If the two therapists develop a situation which makes the patient a scapegoat for their unresolved conflicts (a situation 20 that "simulates" the original family group), ". . . then the basic purpose of the co-therapist method will have been lost. The patient will have no recourse but to further strengthen sick defenses which were erected during his childhood” (Lundin and Aronov, 1952, p. 79). If the conflict between the therapists is very intense and shows no signs of resolu- tion, the therapists may have to terminate their working together or the patient may leave therapy. However, in most cases, the conflict is worked out to the benefit of the therapists and the patient. The therapists benefit by the resolution of the conflict because it has helped to push their growing edge and to restore their ability to under- stand their patient again. The therapists show the conflict is resolved when they can feel pleased with their inter- action. Anything which furthers the therapists' growth in the interaction helps the patient also. One of the major things a patient may learn from the resolution of the con— flict is that ". . . an adult man and woman can be puzzled, anxious, or even in conflict, and that these same adults can resolve their problem by talking to each other. Such an outcome between adults, particularly a man and a woman, is likely to have been more rare in such a client's past experience” (Kell and Burow, in press, p. 405). 21 Professional vs Dynamic Attraction in the Selection of a Co—therapist The preceding discussion about the effect of the therapists' interaction in multiple therapy raises questions about the factors that cause them to choose each other and that allow them to work together effectively. The multiple therapy relationship cannot be built upon the professional similarities which exist between the two therapists; the relationship demands more than that. “The multiple therapy relationship is not mutual because of similar training or identical experience. Rather, it becomes mutual in our need to exist simultaneously and as a unit in an on—going process" (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1960, p. 562). However, professional similarities do seem to create a basis for the initial pro- fessional attraction between two therapists. Similar experiences, therapeutic approaches, theoretical viewpoints, views about life, etc. seem to function to select for each therapist the sort of colleague with whom he is likely to interact. Borrowing a concept from Winch (1958) it seems that these similarities define for each therapist a "field of eligible co—therapist candidates" within which he is likely to choose his partner for multiple therapy. The next logical question is, what are the factors that cause a therapist to choose his partner rather than another person for multiple therapy? Professional similarities may define the "field of eligibles”, but it is important to know the 22 psychological factors that set the limits of the field and that determine the selection within that field. It is this writer's belief that each therapist's selection of his co- therapist from this "field of eligibles" is based upon dynamic factors. When two people engage in multiple therapy, one can surmise that there is something about the relation- ship which causes each of them to want to be with the other. If two therapists do multiple therapy together, it seems reasonable to believe that each therapist derives some gratification in the interaction with the other. "If it is reasonable to speak of gratification, then it makes sense to postulate some needs in each which the interaction (and hence the behavior of the other) may gratify" (Winch, 1958, p. 16). Of all the needs which could be mentioned only a few seem to be relevant to the present study. Eleven needs were examined in this study. The definitions of the needs used in this study have been modified from those cited by Murray (1938) and Edwards (1953). The needs that were used and the definitions of them are given below. Definitions of the Needs Employed in This Study Abasement (Aha): A person with a need for Abasement values himself little, is highly self-critical, and is depressed by feelings of unworthiness. The goal of this need is to admit inferiority, error, wrong-doing or defeat in order 23 to avoid blame, pain, or punishment and to gain forgiveness and approval. A person with a need for Abasement tends to: suffer an insult or injury without fighting back; feel compel— led to confess, apologize, or promise to improve; adopt a meek or humble attitude; feel guilty when he does something wrong; accept blame when things go wrong; desire punishment for mistakes or errors; feel better for giving in than fighting and complaining about his rights; feels depressed by inability to handle difficult situations; blame, belittle, or humiliate himself; and feel inferior to other people in most respects. Achievement (Ach): A person with a need for Achievement has a desire or a tendency to work at something as rapidly and as well as possible. The aim of such a person is to try to do everything well and to accomplish something that is creditable. An individual with a need for Achievement desires to attain tasks that require skill and effort; wants to overcome any obstacle; likes to work at difficult tasks; enjoys competition; sets difficult goals for himself which he attempts to reach; wants to be recognized as an authority; wants to belong to a profession; continually strives to be successful; displays a determination to win; wants to do things better than other people and wants to write a great play, novel, etc. Affiliation (Aff): A person with a need for Affiliation wants to be with and interact with other people. This is 24 the type of person who seeks to form and maintain intimate relationships with people. The aim of the need for affilia— tion is to form a mutually enjoyable, enduring, and reciprocal relationship with another individual. A person with a need for Affiliation will participate in friendly groups, form new friendships, ask people to do things with him, share things with friends, enjoy cooperating with others more than working alone, enjoy social gatherings, is loyal to friends and institutions. A ression A ): An individual with a need for Aggression has a tendency to push forward his own goals, interests, or beliefs, despite opposition; he attacks rather than avoid a threatening situation. The need for Aggression is accom- panied by anger which gets the individual to forcefully prose- cute his own ends if such action is necessary for a goal to be reached. Such an individual displays self-confidence and boldness in action and in expressing himself. An individual with a need for Aggression will attack contrary points of view; show anger; attack, injure or kill an animal or person; tell other people what he thinks of them; speak in a forceful way; destroy a physical object; express or show hatred; get revenge for insults; injury or moral wrongs; criticize others publicly; like sports with physical contact--e.g., football, boxing, etc.; curse or blame individuals who get in his way; read about violence; enjoy arguing. 25 Autonomy (Aut): An individual with a need for Autonomy wishes to have self-government without outside control. The aim of such a person is neither to direct others nor be led but rather to be left to do things in his own way, without restriction or coercion-~in other words, he wishes to be independent and free to behave in a manner that suits him at any given time. An individual with a need for Autonomy wants to remain unattached, does not ask others for their opinions in making his decision, avoids domineering authorities, defies authority, feels free to do what he wants, avoids reSponsibilities and obligations, goes against convention, and seeks environments with few rules. Deference (Def): The aim of a person with this need is to move towards, salute, and conform to the wishes of someone he sees as superior to him. An individual with a need for Deference follows suggestions, admires great men, accepts suggestions rather than insist on working things out on his own, accepts the leadership of a more experienced or know- ledgeable person, chooses a superior ally, attempts to do something by copying an idolized leader, follows instructions and does what is expected, attempts to find out what others think, tells others they have done a good job, reads about great men, believes in conformity, and cooperates eagerly. Dependence (Dep): A person with a need for Dependence craves affection, understanding, and tenderness and seeks out people on whom he can rely. The goal of this need is to be dependent 26 upon someone who will gratify one's desires or who will help with one's pains. Such a person tends to depend upon others for aid, advice, forgiveness, support and protection. A person with a need for Dependence will ask to have others provide help when in trouble, enjoy receiving sympathy, nourishment, and understanding about personal problems, accept favors cheerfully, seek help when depressed, like others to feel sorry when he is sick, feel lonely in solitude, receive a great deal of affection, will like others to be concerned when he is hurt or suffering from hardships. Dominance (Dom): A person with a need for Dominance desires to influence or control the feelings, ideas, and/or actions of other individuals. The aim of the need for Dominance is to induce individuals to behave in a manner which conforms with one's feelings and desires. A person who displays this need will argue for his cause against another; try to lead; govern, manage people; enjoy organizing or directing the actiVities of a group; take initiative to get people to co-operate; seek to have his ideas prevail; try to get individuals to see him as the leader; try to supervise and direct the behavior of others; work to be the leader of a group and dislike a secondary role; endeavor to be elected to various offices; will make laws and set standards for others, and will stimulate or initiate various programs or ideas. 27 Heterosexuality (Het): A person with a need for Hetero- sexuality is attracted to and interested in persons of the opposite sex. The goal of such an individual is to form and encourage a relationship with the opposite sex. A person with a need for Heterosexuality will enjoy the company of the opposite sex; be fond of dancing; engage in social activi- ties centered around both sexes; wishes to embrace, kiss, hold hands, and engage in other sexual activities; wishes to be in love; participates in discussions about sex; has erotic dreams and/or fantasies; seeks to be regarded as physically attractive by those of the opposite sex; and reads materials about sex. Nurturance (Nur): A person with a need for Nurturance aids, protects, supports, encourages any individual who can be described as being weak, disabled, tired, inexperienced, defeated, humiliated, lonely, dejected, sidk or threatened by danger. A nurturant person is one who is tolerant toward people who are distressed and/or hurt. Such a person helps when people are in trouble, assists others less fortunate, treats people with kindness and sympathy, forgives others, does small favors, is generous, sympathesizes with individuals who are hurt or sick, and has others confide in him about their problems. Passivity (Pas): An individual with a need for Passivity feels most comfortable in those situations in which his behavior is externally rather than internally initiated and 28 controlled. The goal of such a person is to be the object of action rather than the cause of action; he is a person who is characterized by a lack of spontaneous activity. An individual with a need for Passivity enjoys secondary roles, lets others dominate, avoids exerting himself, and wants to be free of making a decision. An individual with this need will let others take the lead, does not participate readily, will wish to receive benefits without effort or without making his wishes known, may submit without resistance, will wait for external stimulation before acting, will relax or daydream rather than reach out for what he wants, and will make no effort to control events. Complementary and Supplementary Needs A number of writers have indicated how multiple therapists seem to supplement and complement each other's personalities and resources (Demarest and Teicher, 1954; Linden, 1954; Solomon et al., 1954; Gans, 1957; Mintz, 1963b; Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964; Kell and Burow, in press). Solomon et a1. advocate that it is essential that the relationship between the two therapists be complementary for their proper functioning in multiple therapy. "It is logical to assume that one therapist's assets and limitations will not be exactly similar to another's. . ." and that the personality differences between them add greatly to their relationship (Demarest and Teicher, 1954, p. 200; Mullan and 29 Sanguiliano, 1964). Kell and Burow believe that the word "complement" is a key concept in any discussion of multiple therapy. They believe that successful multiple therapy requires that the two therapists respond to each other in complementary and collaborative ways over time. This writer believes that the two therapists in multiple therapy could not respond in such a complementary manner unless they comple- ment each other at a dynamic level. Complementary, as used here, means that as one therapist engages in a certain be— havior, which has been aroused by his needs, his co—thera— pists's needs will be gratified by this action and vice versa. Another way of stating this is that each therapist has certain needs, which when fulfilled, provide satisfaction to the other therapist. From a theoretical standpoint, it is only reason- able to expect that the more Person A wants to give what Person B wants to receive (and the more B wants to give what A wants to receive), the easier it will be for the pair to form and main- tain a viable relationship. The more that such conditions exist, the greater will be the mutual reward of the pair members and the lower the cost of achieving it (Levinger, 1964, p. 154). Complementarity of needs can occur in two ways. The first way two individuals' needs may complement each other occurs when they differ in degree of the same need. For example, when one individual has a high need and the other has a low need for dominance, the difference between them on this need is seen as complementary. The second way two individuals' needs can complement each other occurs when they differ in 30 the kind of need expressed. For instance, when one person has a high need for nurturance and another individual has a high need for succorance, their needs can be seen as comple— mentary (Winch, 1958; Tharp, 1963). Complementary Need Patterns between Pairs of Multiple Therapists The Dominance-Receptive Dimension It is thought that the therapist-therapist inter— action will be optimal when the two therapists' needs com- plement each other on a dominance-receptive dimension. The receptive cluster of needs is represented by the needs for deference and passivity (Winch, 1958). Gans (1963), super- vising teams of psychiatric residents who did group therapy together, found that the two therapists did poorly together when they were in conflict along a dominance-passivity di— mension. He states that such a conflict disrupted their co-operative venture and led to feelings of resentment. It seems reasonable to expect that two therapists would not function well together in multiple therapy if they both were high in the need for dominance or high in the need for passivity. Two therapists who are at the same time dogmatically active may find themselves competing with each other for the attention . . . [of the patients]. Conversely, two passive therapists may similarly fail to fulfill the needs . . . [of their patients]. Furthermore, they will neither support nor complement each other at those times when adequate handling of the problems . . . [of their patients] is impeded by the personality difficulty or "blind spot" of an individual therapist (Solomon et al., 1954, p. 177). 31 Since this writer and other investigators (Linden, 1954; Sonne and Lincoln, 1954; Rabin, 1967a) have made an analogy between a good multiple therapy relationship and a healthy marriage, it seems appropriate to consider the studies investigating the patterns of needs of marital partners to see if these studies can shed any light on the present investigation. One of the major findings that Winch (1958) derived from his exhaustive work on mate selection was that the Spouses needs tended to complement each other in terms of dominance-deference and that this complementarity influenced mate selection. From his work, Levinger (1965) concludes that marital partners function well together when one of them needs to dominate the other needs to accept such domination. Thus the above seems to offer some support for the belief that therapists who have chosen to work together in multiple therapy fulfill each other's needs along a dominance-receptive dimension. Table 1 shows the ways two individuals' needs can satisfy each person in terms of this dimension. Table 1. Pairs of needs that are seen as satisfying each other along a dominance—receptive dimension. Person A Person B 1. High need for dominance 1. Low need for dominance 2. High need for passivity 2. Low need for passivity 3. High need for deference 3. Low need for deference 4. If A is high in need for 4. B is high in need for dominance passivity 5. If A is high in need for 5. B is high in need for dominance deference 32 The Dependent-NUrturant Dimension The second complementary need pattern that seems relevant to the multiple therapeutic relationship is the dependent—nurturant dimension. The multiple therapeutic relationship, in order to be enjoyable and profitable, must contain the seeds that enhance the well—being of the two therapists. In order for the relationship to be truly "mutual", the dependency needs of each therapist must be met. "Our OWn experience as well as that of others who have done multiple therapy tells us that being able to depend on each other is often times vital to the multiple therapeutic venture" (Kell and Burow, in press, p. 408a). Experience with multiple therapy suggests that each co- therapist attaches importance to his partner because he recognizes at some level that he receives something from his partner that is of consequence and that he appreciates what his partner has to offer (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964). Successful multiple therapy seems to occur when each thera— pist feels free to be the therapist or the patient of the co-therapist (Warkentin, Johnson and Whitaker, 1951). As Kell and Burow indicate, the relationship between the two therapists provides an opportunity for both therapists to satisfy their nurturant and dependent needs. Evidence from studies dealing with the patterning of needs in marriage indicate that the dependent—nurturant dimension is an important variable in understanding intimate 33 relationships. Katz, Glucksberg, and Krauss (1960) found that wives' satisfaction with their marriage was positively related to their husband's need for nurturance and that the husband's satisfaction in this relationship was positively related to their wives need for nurturance and dependence. Winch's Work (1958) led him to conclude that complementarity of needs along the dependent—nurturant dimension was very important in the selection of one's spouse. From the above information, it seems that studying the dependent—nurturant dimension for complementarity of needs among teams of multiple therapists may be helpful in gaining greater under— standing the multiple therapeutic relationship. It seems necessary to point out that the present writer does not view nurturance and dependence as polar opposites. It is not necessarily true that a highly nurturant person is automatically low in his need for dependence. It is quite probable that a highly nurturant person would great— ly like to have his dependency needs met. What the above statements imply is that if complementarity of needs exist between two therapists along a dependent-nurturant dimension, it occurs because one therapist's need for dependence is complemented by his co—therapists' need for nurturance and not because they differ in their need for dependence or nurturance. There is no basis for theorizing that 311 of the needs of the teams of multiple therapists should be comple- mentary. In fact it is thought that the therapeutic 34 relationship demands that the two therapists be similar on some of their needs if they are to function well together. Similar Need Patterns between Pairs of Multiple Therapists Need for Achievement and Autonomy It would not be expected that two therapists would function well together in multiple therapy if they differed in degree in their need for achievement or in their need for autonomy. As Levinger (1964) points out, it does not make sense to posit that two individuals function best to— gether when one is high and the other is low in their need for achievement. "For a maximal coordination of their efforts, maximum similarity of achievement motivation might be most advisable" (Levinger, 1964, p. 155). Also, it seems highly unlikely that a therapist with a high need for autonomy would choose to work with a co—therapist with a low need for autonomy. It seems that if they differed greatly on this particular need, the relationship would be frustrating to both of them; one therapist would be striving for independence while the other would be trying to form a dependent relationship. Need for Abasement and Aggression Since our society and many of our theories of per- sonality have spent a considerable amount of energy focusing 35 on the issue of aggression, it seems appropriate to consider what role aggression plays in the relationship between the two therapists who do multiple therapy together. A number of writers (Leary, 1957; Winch, 1958) have reasoned that two individuals will be attracted to each other when one individual has a high need for aggression and the other has a high need for abasement. There exists a large number of individuals who are most comfortable . . . when they are tied to a hostile partner. . . . An intense symbiotic relationship exists between those who are least anxious when hurting or derogating and those who are least anxious when receiving negative actions (Leary, 1957, p. 344). While such a dyadic relationship can be described as recipro— cal or complementary, it does not appear to be conducive to the individuals' emotional growth and well—being. It is believed that the multiple therapeutic relationship is not based upon such a destructive interaction. It is hard to think that pairs of multiple therapists are so disturbed in their interpersonal relationships that they chose to work together in order to satisfy these devastating needs. In a similar way, it is unlikely that an individual would choose to work with someone whose need for aggression dif— fered greatly from his OWn, since working together may pro— duce undue tension and anxiety. As Whitaker (1967) stated, the relationship between the therapists must be such that both therapists feel equally strong. It is believed that the two therapists will view each other in that way when they are similar in their need for aggression. 36 Need for Affiliation and Heterosexuality It is expected that therapists who choose to work together will be similar in their needs for affiliation and heterosexuality. Talking to multiple therapists, the author has observed that one of the major factors that many thera- pists use in selecting a co-therapist is the degree to which they feel an individual wants to associate with people. Some therapists have indicated that they have avoided working with certain people because they feel the latter places too many demands on them to be friendly, close, etc., while other therapists have said that they could not work with an individual because he wanted too much distance from people to suit him. It is thought that feelings of anger, depriva- tion, disapproval, distrust, etc. would arise in the multiple therapy relationship if the two therapists differed in their need to form and maintain intimate relationships. For these reasons, it is believed that two individuals who differed in their need for affiliation would not choose each other as partners in multiple therapy. This writer believes that two therapists could not work together if they operated at different levels of sexual— ity. It is believed that both therapists must enjoy the company of the opposite sex to roughly the same degree in order for each of them to feel appreciated as male and female. "It is my hunch that perceiving your opposite sexed co-therapist as attractive (including sexuality) and feeling comfortable 37 with your feelings of attraction is one factor in a more successful co-therapy relationship . . .“ (Rabin, 1967b). It is also thought that the two therapists could not be comfortable with each other if one therapist had a high need and the other had a low need for heterosexuality. It would seem that their needs in such a situation would conflict and drive them away from each other. The Relationship between a Therapist's Satisfaction in a Given Multiple Therapeutic Relationship and the Needs of His Co-therapist This writer contends that a therapist's satisfaction in doing multiple therapy is a function, in part, of his co- therapist possessing certain needs. The second part of this investigation will examine the relationship between a thera— pist's satisfaction in a given multiple therapeutic relation— ship and the needs of his co-therapist. If one views multiple therapy as a symbolic healthy marriage, then it is logical to expect that each therapist will be satisfied in the relationship to the degree that his co—therapist possesses needs which enhance him. Another way of saying this is that each therapist will be happy in the multiple therapeutic relationship when his co—therapist has needs that make him a more fully functioning person. If this relationship is true, one would think that a therapist's satisfaction in the relationship would be positively related to his co-therapist's needs for achievement, heterosexuality and nurturance and 38 negatively related to his need for abasement. One would also think that a therapist would be satisfied with his co-thera- pist when his co—therapist possessed certain needs which caused him to strive to increase the interpersonal relation- ship between them. If a therapist's satisfaction is related to his co-therapist's needs to form a closer relationship with him, it is expected that a therapist's satisfaction is positively related to his co-therapist's needs for affilia— tion and dependence and negatively related to his need for autonomy. to work in each 1. HYPOTHESES This study proposes that therapists who have chosen together in multiple therapy fulfill certain needs other. It is hypothesized that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more on a dominance—receptive dimension than randomly paired therapists. a. It is hypothesized that the strength of the bi— polar relationship in terms of the needs for deference, dominance, and passivity will be greater in pairs of multiple therapists than randomly paired therapists. It is hypothesized that the strength of the relationship between dominance and passivity will be greater in pairs of multiple therapists than randomly paired therapists. It is hypothesized that the strength of the relationship between dominance and deference will be greater in pairs of multiple therapists than in randomly paired therapists. It is hypothesized that the strength of the relation- ship between dependence and nurturance will be greater for pairs of multiple therapists than randomly paired therapists. 39 40 It is hypothesized that pairs of multiple therapists are more similar in terms of their needs for abase- ment, achievement, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, heterosexuality, and nurturance than randomly paired therapists. It is hypothesized that a therapist's satisfaction with his partner in multiple therapy is related to his co-therapist's needs. a. It is hypothesized that a therapist's satis- faction with his co-therapist is positively related to his co-therapist's needs for achieve- ment, affiliation, dependence, heterosexuality, and nurturance. b. It is hypothesized that a therapist's satisfaction with his co-therapist is negatively related to his co-therapist's needs for abasement and autonomy. METHOD Subjects The Multiple Therapists Sample In the present study, 12 pairs of multiple therapists who volunteered to be in this investigation at Michigan State University were included in this sample. The mean age of these therapists is 33 and the average amount of experience that each therapist has had in individual therapy is 3.8 years. Each one of the therapists chose his co—therapist because he thought that their working together would be pleasing to him. These pairs of therapists vary in the amount of time they have worked together from a few months to 3 years. The Control Sample The control sample is composed of another set of 12 male therapists and 12 female therapists from Michigan State University. The mean age of this group of therapists is 29 and the average amount of experience that each therapist has had in individual therapy is 3.6 years. Some of these therapists have done multiple therapy while others have not and do not wish to do so. From this sample of 24 therapists, each individual was paired with a therapist of the opposite 41 42 sex in such a way that no individuals would be paired with their co—therapist from multiple therapy. Measuring Instruments The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) The selection of a measuring instrument to examine the variables in this study was based primarily on three criteria. The first criterion was required for this study was that the instrument should provide for the measurement of the needs of the subjects, since inferences were to be made at this level. Secondly, because the subjects in this study were psychologically sophisticated, it was thought that the instrument should be ambiguous enough to prevent them from unwillingly influencing the results. Finally, it was thought that the testing for each subject should take no longer than one hour since the people who volunteered for this investigation had many professional responsibilities to meet. Based upon the above criteria, it seemed logical to utilize the TAT. Interpersonal §§tisfaction Scale (185) The ISS is an index of a therapist's satisfaction with his co-therapist in multiple therapy. This scale re— quires that each multiple therapist rate himself as to the degree of satisfaction he derives from the relationship he has with his co-therapist. 43 The degree of satisfaction a therapist feels toward his co-therapist was rated on a one to seven scale, seven indicating the greatest amount of satisfaction. The directions and the 188 are: On the following scale, you are asked to rate the amount of satisfaction you derive from inter- acting with your co—therapist in multiple therapy. Satisfaction in this context relates to how happy you are with what your co-therapist is generally able to offer you as a person. Circle the number (no "in between" ratings please) which best des- cribes the amount of satisfaction you derive from working with your co-therapist. A "1” indicates that you wish to continue with your co—therapist because you believe he has the potential to fulfill your basic needs. While you may be anxious about how the relationship will develop, you feel satis- fied enough with your co-therapist that you are ready to explore some of your feelings and attitudes with him. If you were to circle this number, you would be indicating that you respect and like your co-therapist but that you do not know each other as well as you would like to. A "4” indicates that your relationship with your co-therapist has been so satisfying to you that you have complete trust in him. Such a score indicates that your co- therapist is able to satisfy your basic needs-- that he is able to reSpond in a helpful way to the things that are very important to you. A "7” indi— cates that an almost ideal relationship. If you were to check this number, it would indicate generally you could not be happier with what your co-therapist supplies you. Such a relationship would yield maximum satisfaction. Try to fill out this scale as honestly as possible. How satisfied you are with your co-therapist 44 Procedure The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) Eleven cards of the TAT were administered to each subject. All subjects Were given cards 1, 2, 38M, 4, 10, 12M, 13MF, l6, and 20. In addition, the males were given cards 6BM and 7BM, While the females were given 6GF and 7GF. The instructions to the subjects were as follows: I am going to show you some pictures, one at a time; your task will be to make up as dramatic a story as you can for each. Tell me what has led to the event shown in the picture, describe what is happening at the moment, what the characters are feeling and thinking; and then give the outcome. Do you understand? Since you have sixty minutes for eleven pictures, you can devote approximately five minutes to each story. Here is the first picture (Modified form from Murray, 1943, p. 3). The stories were electrically recorded, and these recordings were used to analyze the stories in terms of the needs of each subject. Two raters were used to score the content of the stories. Prior to the training sessions, the raters Were given the definitions of each need and Were asked to memorize these definitions so that they would have a clear understanding of the meaning of each need. During the first training session, the raters were instructed in how to score the content in terms of Murray's scoring system. This system analyzes the needs of "the hero" in each story to determine the strength of the various needs of an individual. The strength of each of the eleven needs with which the study is concerned was rated on a zero to five scale. A score of five is the highest possible 45 score that a need can be given in a story while the score of zero indicates that the need is not expressed. The strength of a need is determined by the intensity, frequency, duration, and importance with which it appears in a story. "The slightest suggestion of a variable (e.g., flash of irritability) is given a mark of 1, whereas an intense form (e.g., violent anger) or the continued or repeated occurrence of a milder form (e.g., constant quarreling) is scored 5. Marks of 2, 3, and 4 are given for intermediate intensities of expression" (Murray, 1943, p. 8). In order to help the raters rate the strength of a need when it was determined by the frequency with which it appeared in the story, the following rules were established: 1. One vague statement of a need: score 1 2. One clear statement of a need: score 2 3. Two clear statements of a need: score 3 4. Three clear statements of a need: score 4 5. Four or more clear statements of a need: score 5 The raters were also instructed that if they were undecided between two ratings on a need (e.g., 2 or 3) they should choose the lower of the two. Once these instructions were explained to the raters, they listened to three practice protocols. In these training sessions, they were asked to identify the needs expressed by "the hero" in each story without scoring them for their strength. Once they identified the needs, which they did 46 extremely well, the author explained to them his reasons for rating the needs as he did. Following this procedure, the raters listened together to four protocols from both samples and were asked to rate independently the strength of the needs expressed in each story. At the end of each story, the raters discussed the discrepancies between their ratings while the author helped them to resolve their differences. After proceeding through the four protocols in this manner, the training sessions ended, and the raters scored the remaining protocols. Since each need can be rated on a zero to five scale for each of the eleven cards, the total score can range from 0-55. Murray's correction factors for equating for the average length of the subject's stories were then applied to the total scores (Murray, 1943). If these correction factors were not applied, an individual's relative position on a need would depend more on the length than the content of his stories. The two corrected total scores were averaged and this latter value was used as a basis for analyzing the results. Based on the two judges' independent scoring of the needs of all the individuals in both samples, an estimate of the interrater reliability was obtained. Table 2 summarizes the interrater reliability for the scoring of the TAT needs as defined in this study. 47 Table 2. Interrater reliability of the TAT needs. r TAT Need (n = 48) Abasement .94 Achievement .77 Affiliation .87 Aggression .90 Autonomy .73 Deference .85 Dependence .83 Dominance .70 Heterosexuality .71 Nurturance .81 Passivity .75 The above reliability coefficients indicate a rela— tively high degree of association between the two sets of ratings and Suggest that the scoring system for the needs studied in this paper is quite reliable. Inter ersonal Satisfaction Scale 188 The ISS was not given to the control sample since the sCale was designed specifically for the multiple thera— pist sample. The latter group of subjects (N - 24) took the ISS twice with a two—week interval separating the two ad- ministrations. TWo administrations were required in order to obtain the stability coefficient for this instrument. The first administration of this scale occurred immediately after the subject had completed the TAT. The stability coefficient for the ISS is .99 which indicates that the 188 is a highly reliable instrument for measuring a therapist's satisfaction with his co-therapist in multiple therapy. 48 Procedures for Statistical Analysis of the Data Hypothesis 1 In sub-hypothesis la, the multiple therapy sample and the control sample were treated as two separate distri- butions. To test sub-hypothesis la, the median score for each sample on each need was obtained (i.e., the median of all scales in each distribution). Then the scores for each sample were dichotomized at the median, and each pair of therapists were split into two mutually exclusive categories: the one category represented the number of pairs of thera— pists who are similar on a given need, and the other category represented the number of pairs of therapists who were bi— polar on a need. A pair of therapists were seen as similar on a need if pppp their scores were above or below the median of their sample. A pair of therapists were seen as being bipolar on a need if one member of the pair had a score above the median while the other individual's score was below the median. Data dichotomized in such a manner for both samples would resemble those given in Table 3. To test sub-hypothesis la that pairs of multiple therapists have a more bipolar relationship in terms of their needs for dominance, deference, and passivity than randomly paired therapists, the proportion of pairs who were bipolar on a given need was determined for each sample. The data were then analyzed using the significance test for the difference between two proportions. 49 'Table 3. Conceptual form for data in which no. of pairs of therapists who are Similar or bipolar on a given need are presented. Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample No. of pairs of thera— pists who are similar A C on a given need No. of pairs of thera- pists who are bi- B D polar on a given need To test sub-hypothesis lb and 1c that pairs of multiple therapists have certain needs which are more complementary to one's partner than randomly paired therapists, inter— therapist correlations for the multiple therapist and control samples were obtained for these two sub-hypotheses. In order to obtain these correlations, each pair of thera- pists was examined on one of the two needs appropriate to each sub-hypothesis. The score of the member of each pair that was higher on this need was paired with his partner's score on the other need relevant to the complementary sub- hypothesis. A hypothetical example, using sub-hypothesis lb as a base, may help clarify the operation. Suppose John and Laura are one pair of therapists from a given sample and Jehn has a score of 20 on need for dominance while Laura's score is 15 on this need. Since thn's score on dominance is higher than Laura's, his score on dominance would be 50 paired with Laura's score on passivity. Such a procedure was performed for each pair of therapists in a given sample, and the product-moment correlation between the two variables relevant to the sub—hypothesis (in the above example, the correlation between dominance and passivity) was determined. Then the multiple therapist and control samples were com— pared on the sub-hypothesis by testing the significance of the differences between the two correlation coefficients obtained from these samples. Hypothesis 2 To test the hypothesis that pairs of multiple thera- pists complement each other more on a dependencevnurturance dimension than randomly paired therapists, a product-moment correlation based on an individual's score on dependence and his partner's score on nurturance was obtained for each group. The hypothesis was statistically tested by using the significance of the differences between the two correlation coefficients obtained from the samples. Hypothesis 3 To test the hypothesis that pairs of multiple thera- pists are more similar in terms of their needs for abase— ment, achievement, aggression, autonomy, dependence, hetero- sexuality, and nurturance then randomly paired therapists, the proportion of pairs who are similar on a given need was 51 obtained for each sample. The data were analyzed using the significance test for the difference between two proportions. Hypothesis 4 To test the hypothesis that a therapist's satisfaction with his partner in multiple therapy is related to his co— therapist possessing certain needs, the ISS scores of each individual in the multiple therapist sample was correlated with his co-therapist's needs. A t-test of the hypothesis of zero correlation was computed for each need in each sub- hypothesis. RESULTS Hypothesis 1: Complementary Needs Along a Dominance-Receptive Dimension In Hypothesis 1 it was proposed that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more on a dominance—receptive dimension than randomly paired therapists. This hypothesis was divided into three parts. The first part predicted that pairs of therapists have a more bipolar relationship in terms of their needs for deference, dominance, and passivity than randomly paired therapists. The tables in Appendix A show the raw data of the number of pairs of therapists in each sample Who are similar or bipolar in terms of their needs for deference, dominance, and passivity. The data reported in Table 4 Show the results of the significance tests for the difference between the proportion of pairs of therapists in the multiple and control samples Who have a bipolar relationship with respect to their needs for deference, dominance, and passivity. There were no significant differences between the groups in the proportion of pairs of therapists who have a bipolar relationship in their needs for deference, dominance, and passivity. Sub-hypothesis la, that states that pairs of multiple therapists have a more bipolar relationship in 52 53 regards to these needs than randomly paired therapists, was not confirmed. Table 4. Bipolarity of needs: proportion and between—group Z values (both corrected for discontinuity) for the (N = 12) pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist and control samples who have a bipolar relationship with respect to their needs derived from the TAT for deference, dominance and passivity. Proportion of Proportion of Pairs of Multiple Pairs of Randomly TAT Need Therapists who Paired Therapists Betweeg-group Have a Bipolar Who Have A Bipolar Relationship Relationship Deference .458 .458 .00 Dominance .458 .375 .41 Passivity .500 .500 .00 The second part of hypothesis 1 (lb) predicted that the strength of the relationship between dominance and passivity will be greater in pairs of multiple therapists than randomly paired therapists. The third part of this hypothesis (1c) proposes that the strength of the relation- ship between dominance and deference will be greater in pairs of multiple therapists than randomly paired therapists. The data reported in Table 5 Show the results of the signifi- cance tests for the differences between the Pearson product moment correlation coefficients of the two groups on the dominance-passivity and dominance-deference dimensions. 54 Table 5. Complementary needs: Pearson product moment cor- relations and between group Z values between the multiple therapist and control samples on the dominance—passivity and dominance-deference complementary dimensions. Correlation on Correlation on Between- the two vari- the two vari- group Z Complementary Need ables for ables for Pattern (N=12) pairs of (N=12) pairs of therapists in therapists in the multiple the control therapist sample sample Dominance-Passivity .320 -.286 1.33 Dominance-Deference .307 .199 .246 As Table 5 indicates, neither of the two comparisons made resulted in significant differences between the groups. Thus, one can conclude that sub-hypothesis 1b and 1c were not confirmed. However, inspection of this table shows that the result in the dominance—passivity dimension (related to sub-hypothesis lc) was in the predicted direction (2 = 1.33, P g .092). Based on the results of sub-hypotheses la, 1b, and 1c, Hypothesis 1, which proposed that multiple therapists complement each other more on a dominance—receptive dimension than randomly paired therapists, was not supported. Hypothesis 2: Complementagy Needs Along A Depgndence- Nurturance Dimension Hypothesis 2 proposed that pairs of multiple therapists complemented each other more on a dependence-nurturance 55 dimension than randomly paired therapists. This hypothesis predicted that the strength of the relationship between dependence and nurturance will be greater in pairs of multiple therapists than in randomly paired therapists. The date reported in Table 6 presents the findings of the significance tests for the differences between the Pearson product- moment correlation co-efficients of the two groups in the dependence-nurturance dimension. Table 6. Pearson product moment correlations and between group Z values between the multiple therapist and control sample on the dependence—nurturance dimension. Correlation between Correlation between Between-group dependence-nurturance dependence-nurturance Z for pairs of therapists for pairs of thera- in the multiple thera- pists in the control 'pist sample (N = 24) sample (N = 24) .385 .110 1.28 There was no significant difference between the cor- relations of the groups on the dependence-nurturance dimension. While Hypothesis 2, which states that multiple therapists complement each other more on a dependence-nurturance di- mension than randomly paired therapists, was not confirmed, the results were in the predicted direction (Z = 1.28, P §_.100). 56 Hypothesis 3: Similarity of Needs Hypothesis 3 predicted that pairs of multiple thera— pists are more similar in terms of their needs for abasenent, achievement, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, dependence, heterosexuality, and nurturance than randomly paired thera- pists. The tables in Appendix B show the raw data of the number of pairs of therapists in each sample who are similar or bipolar with respect to these needs. The data reported in Table 7 show the results of the significance tests for the difference between the proportion of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist and control groups who are similar in their needs for abasement, achieve— ment, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, dependence, hetero— sexuality, and nurturance. No significant differences between the groups were found in the proportion of pairs of therapists who are simi— lar in their needs for abasement, achievement, aggression, dependence, and heterosexuality. Three significant dif- ferences between the groups were found in their needs for affiliation (z = 1.642, p g .05), autonomy (z = 1.69, P g .05) and nurturance (Z = 2.07, P g .05). Pairs of therapists in the multiple therapy group were more similar in their needs for affiliation, autonomy, and nurturance than those in the control groups. Hypothesis 3 which states that pairs of multiple therapists are more similar with respect to their 57 needs for abasement, achievement, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, dependence, heterosexuality and nurturance was partially confirmed. Table 7. Similarity of needs: proportions and between- group Z values (both corrected for discontinuity) for the (N = 12) pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist and control samples who are similar in their needs derived from the TAT for abasement, achievement, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, dependence, heterosexuality, and nurturance. Proportion of Proportion of (N=12) Pairs (N=12) Pairs of Multiple Randomly Between— TAT Need Therapists Paired Thera- Group Z Who Are pists Who Are Similar Similar Abasement .625 .458 .82 Achievement .542 .542 .00 Affiliation .625 .292 1.64* Aggression .500 .500 .00 Autonomy .542 .208 1.69* Dependence .666 .666 .00 Heterosexuality .375 .542 -.82 Nurturance .792 .375 2.07* * P §_.05, one-tailed Hypothesis 4: Therapist's Satisfaction in Multiple Therapy Related to His Co—therapist's Needs Hypothesis 4 predicts that a therapist's satisfaction with his partner in multiple therapy is positively related to his co-therapist's needs for achievement, affiliation, dependence, heterosexuality, and nurturance (sub—hypothesis 4a) and negatively related to his co—therapist's needs for abasement and autonomy (sub—hypothesis 4b). The data 58 reported in Table 8 Show the Pearson product moment cor— relations between the satisfaction score of each individual in the multiple therapist sample on the ISS and his co— therapist's needs. Table 8. Pearson product moment correlations between the satisfaction score of each individual in the multiple therapist sample (N = 24) on the ISS and his co—therapist's needs derived from the TAT. Correlation between A Therapist's TAT Need Satisfaction in Multiple Therapy and His Co—therapist's Need Abasement (a) .065 Achievement .074 Affiliation .036 Autonomy (a) .545* Dependence .526* Heterosexuality .143 Nurturance .345** (a) Predicted negative correlations. * P .005, one tailed ** P .05, one tailed Sub-hypothesis 4a, which proposes that a therapist's satisfaction in multiple therapy, is positively associated with his co-therapist's needs for achievement, affiliation, dependence, heterosexuality, and nurturance, was partially confirmed. While no significant correlations appeared between an individual's satisfaction in multiple therapy and his co-therapist's needs for achievement, affiliation, or heterosexuality, the correlations associated with needs for dependence (r = .526, P g .005) and nurturance (r = .345, P g .05) were significant. It would appear that the amount of satisfaction derived from interacting with one's 59 co-therapist is definitely related to the latter's needs for dependence and nurturance. Sub—hypothesis 4b, which states that a therapist's satisfaction with his partner is nega- tively related to his partner's needs for abasement and autonomy, was not confirmed. The predicted relationship between satisfaction and need for abasement did not material- ize. It can be seen that a significant correlation between satisfaction and need for autonomy (r = .545, P g .005) was in a positive, rather than in the predicted negative, direction. This finding suggests that satisfaction with one's co-therapist is positively related to the latter's need for autonomy. The above findings offer partial support for Hypothesis 4 which proposes that a therapist's satisfaction with his partner in multiple therapy is related to his co-therapist possessing certain needs. DISCUSSION The present study was based on the proposition that therapists who have chosen to Work together in multiple therapy fulfill certain needs in each other. This proposition was divided into two central parts. It was suggested first that there will be more patterning of needs among pairs of multiple therapists than randomly paired therapists. Second, it was proposed that a therapist's satisfaction in the multiple therapy relationship will be related to some of the needs of his co—therapist. In order to test the validity of the first part of this premise, three hypotheses, established on the theoretical orientation discussed earlier, were developed. In Hypothesis 1 it was postulated that therapists who have chosen to Work together in multiple therapy will complement each other's needs along a dominance-receptive dimension more than randomly paired therapists. No support was found for Hypothesis 1. Pairs of multiple therapists do not differ from randomly paired therapists along the dominance-receptive dimension. Some multiple therapists have chosen co-therapists who are similar to them in their needs for deference, dominance, or passivity, while other therapists appear to have sought persons who were higher or lower than themselves in terms 60 61 of these needs. It was also found that pairs of multiple therapists do not complement each other more than randomly paired therapists in terms of their needs for dominance and deference. Thus, based on these findings, it can be said that complementarity along a dominance-receptive dimension is not basic to the multiple therapeutic relationship and does not influence the selection of one's co-therapist in multiple therapy. waever, the results in terms of the interaction between dominance and passivity for pairs of therapists were in the predicted direction (P g .09). The present evidence offers tentative support to the proposition that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more along a dominance-passivity dimension than randomly paired thera- pists. It is thought that this complementarity influences the selection of one's co—therapist and has a bearing on how they function together in multiple therapy. The present data indicate that a therapist with a high need for dominance chooses a co-therapist who has a high need for passivity and vice versa. The author believes that the therapist- therapist interaction is optimal when the therapists' needs for dominance and passivity complement each other. If the needs of a pair of therapists do not complement each other along a dominance-passivity dimension, a conflict may arise between them and make their relationship competitive rather than co-operative. This conclusion is congruent with 62 Solomon et al.'s (1954) and Gans' (1962) idea that therapist- therapist conflicts are minimal when the hierarchy of domis nance over passiVity is reconciled. The importance of keeping the therapist-therapist conflicts to a minimum was discussed earlier. At that time, it was pointed out that multiple therapy becomes impotent when the two therapists are in an emotional conflict. While resolution of a conflict, if it is possible, is beneficial to the therapists and the patient, lack of conflict between the therapists is generally more productive to the thera— peutic interaction. Perhaps therapists unconsciously seek to decrease the probability of a conflict in terms of the hierarchy of dominance over passivity by dynamically select- ing a co-therapist who will complement their needs in this area. This evidence indicates that the dominance-passivity dimension as it relates to multiple therapy needs to be examined further. An interesting fact about the therapists included in this investigation emerged when Hypothesis 1 was examined. Most of the therapists (28 out of 48) had a score of 0 on their need for deference and the average score for all thera- pists on this need was 1.08. To the degree that one can generalize from the present sample of therapists to thera- pists in general, the above findings suggest that therapists are people who prefer to work things out in a manner that suits them instead of doing what is expected of them. Since 63 therapists are engaged in an activity that generally encourages individuals to conform to their own standards rather than to conform to someone else, it is not surprising that they Should behave in such a manner and that their need for deference should reflect this pattern--that their need for deference is almost non—existent. In Hypothesis 2 it was postulated that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more on a dependence— nurturance dimension than randomly paired therapists. While this hypothesis was not confirmed, the results indicate a trend in the predicted direction (P g .10) and provide some support to the idea that the dependence—nurturance dimension is an important variable in understanding the dynamics of the interpersonal relationship between pairs of multiple therapists. The results indicate that teams of multiple therapists are paired in such a manner that a therapist's need for dependence is positively related to his co-thera- pist's need for nurturance. Thus, it seems that the higher a therapist is in his need for dependence the greater the probability is that he will choose a co-therapist who is highly nurturant--a person whose need for nurturance is comparable in strength to his need for dependence. Based on this complementary need pattern, both therapists in a team appreciate the dependent and nurturant needs of their co- therapist becauseeach therapist is enhanced by them. 64 The mutuality that exists in multiple therapy is clearly evident in the interaction of the therapists' needs for dependence and nurturance. The relationship is mutual only when the two therapists add to each other by accepting what the other therapist has to offer at a given time-- whether the manifestation arises from dependent or nurturant needs. Mutuality in multiple therapy exists when one person feels free to call on his co-therapist and vice versa (Mullan and Sanguiliano, 1964). Unless each individual can draw on his co—therapist's resources to the degree he needs to and believes that his co—therapist generally wants what he is emotionally able to give, their multiple therapy venture will be unsuccessful. Multiple therapy ". . . will be helpful to the patient only when they move during the sessions in the direction of increased mutuality (Mullan, 1967, p. 131). It is believed that such mutuality cannot develop if the therapists' needs for dependence and nurturance are not com- plementary. In order for each therapist to be as strong and whole as possible in multiple therapy, the therapists must be comfortable with the dependency needs of each other. When therapy is emotionally exhausting and trying, it is nice to know that you can rely on your co-therapist for nourishment. Such a relationship epitomizes the meaning of intimacy in the deepest sense because the two individuals' responses to each other are complementary and collaborative over time. 65 Many therapists would argue that such emotional experiences as regression, abreaction, and primary process must occur in order for effective therapy to take place. Agreeing with Kell and Burow (in press), the author believes that multiple therapy provides a way of promoting such ex- periences so that they facilitate the patient's growth. Experience in multiple therapy indicates that such experiences occur faster, are more intense, and produce more beneficial results than in individual therapy. It is thought these experiences are not as complete in individual therapy because the single therapist is faced with an almost impossible task of being in contact with reality (so that he can pull the patient out of the unconscious material) and of regressing with the patient at the same time. As all therapists know, it is indeed difficult to be two things at once. In con- trast, in multiple therapy one therapist is more able to tolerate and enter into the regression with the patient than in individual therapy because he does not have to defend as much against losing himself in the patient's unconscious, since he can depend upon his co—therapist to be integrated with reality and to know when the regression, etc. have served their purpose. Thus, it can be seen that the inter— action of the therapists needs for dependence and nurturance is important in multiple therapy, since ". . . the freedom to depend is the basis on which a therapist may enter into a regressive feeling experience with a client in which both 66 permit themselves to lose contact with emotional reality" (Kell and Burow, in press, p. 409). In Hypothesis 3, it was postulated that pairs of multiple therapists are mona similar in terms of their needs for abasement, achievement, affiliation, aggression, autonomy, dependence, heterosexuality, and nurturance than randomly paired therapists. This hypothesis was partially confirmed. It was found that pairs of multiple therapists are not more similar in their needs for abasement, achievement, aggression, dependence and heterosexuality than randomly paired therapists. Thus, the evidence suggests that similarity between pairs of therapists in terms of these needs is not a determining factor in the selection of a multiple therapy partner. Pairs of multiple therapists were found to be more similar in their needs for affiliation, autonomy, and nur— turance than randomly paired therapists. It would appear that an individual selects a partner for multiple therapy Who is similar to him in his needs for affiliation, autonomy and nurturance. Earlier an analogy was made between the mul- tiple therapy relationship and a healthy marriage. In this regard, it is interesting to note the similarity of Katz et al.'s (1960) findings with married couples to the present study. In their study, Katz et a1. discovered that married couples were similar to each other in terms of their needs for abasement, affiliation, autonomy, and nurturance. It was earlier theorized that two individuals who differed greatly in their need for affiliation would not 67 select each other for multiple therapy. The results tend to support thta belief. Multiple therapy is a co—operative effort; it cannot be effective unless both therapists feel comfortable with their co-therapist's needs for intimacy. If two therapists differed in their need for forming inti- mate and reciprocal relationships and chose to work together, the relationship would not endure because each therapist would distrust and disapprove of his co-therapist. If a therapist with a low need for affiliation were coupled with a therapist Who had a high need for affiliation, he would probably feel that his co-therapist demanded too much involvement from him and would struggle to keep from being engulfed. On the other hand, the therapist who has high needs for affiliation may feel deprived and angry in a re- lationship in which his co-therapist does not want to be as intimately involved as he does. In this situation, one therapist would be striving for greater closeness and the other for more distance. Such a relationship would breed competition rather than co-operation and companionship which are needed for successful multiple therapy. The results of this investigation indicate that an individual chooses a co-therapist who is similar to him in his need for autonomy. Unless the two therapists were similar to each other in their need for autonomy, they would have difficulty in reSpecting and appreciating each other and would be in conflict when it came to dealing with the 68 autonomous strivings of their patients. The individual with a high need for autonomy would be unwilling to submerge his individuality. "A person seeking an expression for his self-expansive tendencies through any form of behavior will resent and re- sist any intrusion into his activities . . . [and will] in general resist any encroachment of his sphere of freedom and mastery" (Angyal, 1965, p. 10). Such an individual would probably encourage a patient to express his or her drives for eXpansion and mastery; his partner, if low in need for autonomy, would have contradictory needs to the first and this basic conflict between them would manifest itself in the patient's lack of growth. An individual with a low need for autonomy would be more Willing to limit the eXpres- sion of his needs so that they fit into the environment in which he finds himself. If extremely low in his need for autonomy, the individual may invite exploitation to the cha- grin of his co-therapist who has a high need for autonomy. It is thought that two therapists could not work together effectively when their needs are so different-—when they approach their environments so differently. It is predicted that the more similarity between the therapists in their need for autonomy, the greater the mutual reward they will experience, the lower the cost of achieving it, and the more they will help the patient develop competence and self- reliance, without guilt. 69 As predicted, the findings indicate that pairs of multiple therapists were more similar in their need for nurturance than randomly paired therapists. This result suggests that Similarity between individuals in terms of their need for nurturance is one of the determining factors in selecting a partner for multiple therapy. One of the ways a therapist's need for nurturance is expressed in therapy is in terms of the individual's general Willingness to become involved in a patient's problems; the Willingness to become involved in another individual has a definite bearing on how a therapist interacts With a patient. One would surmize that the greater the difference between a pair of therapists the more they would differ in their goals in therapy and in their approach to the patient's concerns. Similarity between the therapists in their need for nurturance would add to their feelings of appreciation of each other in terms of each recognizing the potential of what the other has to offer. Most therapists want to be needed and have chosen a profession that rewards this desire. In multiple therapy, the desire to be needed can be greatly rewarded or frustrated since the therapist has two people available to react to him--the patient and his co-therapist. In a multiple therapy relationship in which the therapists differ in their need for nurturance, the individual with the low need for nurturance may feel "un-needed“ at the level of involvement 70 in which he is operating. Suffering from the feeling that he is not really needed may engender feelings of inadequacy, jealousy, anger, etc. in this individual. He may respond to such feelings by unconsciously withdrawing from the relationship in some way or by striving to give more to the patient than he wants to (or more than the patient needs at a given time). On the other hand, the therapist with a high need for nurturance may feel disappointed with his co-therapist because the latter is not as involved with the patient as he is. In such a situation, the therapist with the high need for nurturance may make the following mistakes which will compound the problem: (1) he may view his co-therapist as lacking "something" and may try to dis- cover what it is so that he can change him; (2) he may hold himself responsible for his co-therapist's behavior and feel inappropriate guilt; and (3) he may decrease what he gives to the patient with the hope that his co-therapist will feel better-—all of which makes the two therapists and the patient feel inadequate. It is thought that the probability of avoid- ing such a damaging conflict is increased when the therapists are similar in their need for nurturance. The second major part of this investigation was con- cerned with the relationship between a therapist's satis— faction in the multiple therapy relationship and the needs of his co-therapist. In Hypothesis 4, it was proposed that satisfaction with one's co—therapist is positively related 71 to his co-therapist's needs for achievement, affiliation, dependence, heterosexuality and nurturance and negatively related to his.needs for abasement and autonomy. There is no evidence to indicate that satisfaction with one's co- therapist is related to one's co-therapist's need for abase- ment, achievement, affiliation, or heterosexuality. Pre- viously it was shown that pairs of multiple therapists are more similar in their need for affiliation than randomly paired therapists. Based on this finding, it was theorized that this need influences the selection of one's co-therapist and is important to the multiple therapy relationship. If this is true, one wonders why satisfaction with one's co- therapist is not related to his partner's need for affilia- tion. Perhaps the need for affiliation is a necessary but not a sufficient factor in the multiple therapy relationship. The results indicate that the amount of satisfaction derived from interacting with one's co-therapist is positively associated with his co—therapist's needs for autonomy, de— pendence, and nurturance. It was predicted that satisfaction . a I with one's co-therapist is pOSitively related to the latter 3 need for dependence and negatively related to his need for autonomy because the direction of these needs are different: the direction of the need for autonomy is toward more self— assertiveness, while the direction of the need for dependence is toward decreasing the separation from another person. While the direction of these two needs may seem conflictual, in reality they are not. These two needs can be complementary in an individual. 72 Both in the homonomous and autonomous trends the person aims at overcoming his separation from the "object," but the object is not the same in the two kinds of reunion, even when it is "physi- cally” identical. The object of mastery is nothing pgp_an object. It is foreign and dissimilar to the person--meaningful only insofar as it can be made to serve him and thus be assimilated into his sphere. The object of homonomous strivings is conceived of as having a "selfhood," a meaningful experience of its own--either that of another human being or some other intrinsically valuable entity with which it is possible to form a community. . . As one strives to master and govern the environment, one discovers that one cannot do this effectively by direct appli- cation of force, by sheer violence. One must under- stand and respect the laws of the environment, go along with them, so to say, which means assuming a homo- nomous attitude. On the other hand, bringing one's best to a loving relationship requires not only a capacity for self-surrender but also a degree of proficient mastery of one's world, of resourcefulness and self-reliance. Without these qualities one does not have much to offer the other, and the relationship may deteriorate into dependency and exploitation (Angyal, 1965, Pp. 28-29). In order for multiple therapy to be effective, each therapist must possess needs which cause him to want to be a separate individual and yet motivate him to become a part of a larger unit. In a good multiple therapy team, each therapist wants his co-therapist to be a separate individual Who also has a desire to belong to a relationship that is more than himself. ”Thus, though there is separateness as individuals, there is a human relationship between them, which is not either one of them separately. The realness of the relation- Ship and its uniqueness is highly related to their separate- ness as individuals, and is also highly related to the meaning of their relationship to the therapeutic endeavor" (Kell and 73 Burow, in press, p. 408). In this light, it is understandable that a therapist's satisfaction with his co-therapist is positively related to both the autonomous and dependency needs of his co-therapist. If the autonomous and dependency needs of a thera— pist are not well integrated (do not balance each other), the interaction between the two therapists will not be too satisfying. If the need for autonomy predominates, the individual will have difficulty in accepting the help of his partner. Such an individual will be separate when it is inappropriate and unconsciously reject his partner for seek- ing to become a unit with him. If this need is excessive in relationship to the need for dependence, the mutuality of the relationship will be destroyed and will impair the patient's capacity to depend upon others. On the other hand, if the need for dependence is excessive in relationship to the need for autonomy, the individual will resent his co- therapist's separateness as an individual and will make un- realistic demands upon him. Such a relationship, if actualiz- ed, would be symbiotic in nature and would prevent separa- tion and individuation that are needed for growth—-both the therapists' and the patient's. Based on the preceding discussion, it is not sur- prising that the results Show that the amount of satisfaction a therapist derives from interacting with his co-therapist is positively related to his co-therapist's needs for de- pendence and nurturance. If effective multiple therapy 74 requires that each therapist must be able to give and re- ceive something from his partner, it is logical that an individual will be satisfied in this relationship to-the degree that his partner has needs which allow the former to give and take something from him. A general conclusion of this investigation is that there are dynamic factors operating in the interpersonal relationship in teams of multiple therapists. The present evidence indicates that certain needs in the individual influence the selection of his co-therapist and that satis— faction in the multiple therapy relationship is partially determined by some of the needs of his co-therapist. Suggestions for Further Research A logical question which arises when one thinks about multiple therapy is whether or not multiple therapists have a greater need for dependence than therapists who do not engage in multiple therapy. Kell (1968) and the present writer do not believe this is true. We believe that multiple therapists may be more conscious of their needs for dependency than other therapists and that one of the ways the former seek to have their dependency needs met is by doing multiple therapy. FUrther research should focus on this question and on the broader issue of whether or not multiple therapists differ psychologically from therapists who do not engage in multiple therapy. MW". J71“ .IJ—I: ‘ _ ‘A.v-. 75 It was previously stressed that multiple therapy becomes ineffective when the two therapists are in conflict. The writer believes that the degree of congruence between a therapist's perception of himself and his co-therapist's view of him and vice versa has a bearing on how they inter- act with each other and their patient(s). This conceptualiza- tion raises the following questions: Will a team of therapists have difficulty in helping a patient resolve a conflict in a particular area if a therapist's view of self and his co- therapist's view of him are incongruent in this area? Does the discrepancy between these perceptions change with the length of time a team of multiple therapists have worked together? What factors determine whether or not these dis- crepancies become conscious and what effect do they have? A fruitful approach to further investigations would be to seek answers to these questions. While he has no statistical evidence to offer to support this observation, the writer has observed from the data in this study that the females were generally more satisfied with their co-therapist than the males were. Further research is needed to determine if this observation is a correct one. If it is, many interesting questions arise as to why such a discrepancy exists. This study has not examined the relationship of male pairs of multiple therapists. Perhaps the factors that operate in two males choosing to work together, besides not having a female co-therapist to work with, are different than those 76 found for the heterosexual pairs of therapists used in this study. The writer feels this is an interesting topic and should be explored. Repeating this investigation with more subjects could increase precision and add to our knowledge. One major advantage of having more subjects is that it would allow us to use multiple regression theory to investigate the degree to which the needs considered together account for the variance of an individual's satisfaction with his co—therapist. SUMMARY The purpose of the present investigation was to examine how therapists who have chosen to work together in multiple therapy fulfill each other in terms of their needs. The first part of the study compared the interaction of needs among pairs of multiple therapists with the interaction of them among randomly paired therapists. The second part of this investigation was concerned with the relationship be— tween a therapist's satisfaction in the multiple therapy relationship and the needs of his co-therapist. Two groups of subjects were selected for the study. The multiple therapy group consisted of 12 teams of multiple therapists. The control group consisted of 12 teams of indi- viduals who were paired so that no individual, who did multiple therapy, would be paired with his co-therapist from multiple therapy. The two groups are comparable with respect to age and the amount of time engaged in individual therapy. Two instruments were used to examine the variables of this study. The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) was used to study the needs of the therapists. Eleven cards of the TAT were administered to each subject, and the strength of each need was obtained. The subjects' stories were scored in terms of the following needs: abasement, achievement, 77 78 affiliation, aggression, autonomy, deference, dependence, dominance, heterosexuality, nurturance, and passivity. Two raters were used; the interrater reliability for the scoring of the TAT needs was of sufficient magnitude to warrant the use of this scoring system. The Interpersonal Satisfaction Scale (ISS) was developed in the context of the present research and is an index of a therapist's satisfaction with his co-therapist in multiple therapy. Each individual in the multiple therapy group was asked to rate on a one to seven scale the degree of satisfaction he felt in interacting with his co-therapist. Based on a two—week interval separat- ing the two administrations, the stability coefficient for the ISS is .99. The results of this study can be summarized as follows: I. The needs of pairs of multiple therapists do not complement each other more on a dominance—receptive dimension than those of the randomly paired therapists. II. Tentative support was given to the proposition that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more along a dominance-passivity dimension than ran- domly paired therapists. III. Some support was found for the idea that pairs of multiple therapists complement each other more on a dependence-nurturance dimension than randomly paired therapists. IV. Pairs of multiple therapists are not more similar in their needs for abasement, achievement, aggression, 79 dependence, and heterosexuality than randomly paired therapists. V. Pairs of multiple therapists were found to be more similar in their needs for affiliation, autdhomy, and nurturance than randomly paired therapists. VI. The amount of satisfaction derived from interacting with one's co-therapistis not related to his co- therapist's needs for abasement, achievement, affiliation, and heterosexuality. VII. Satisfaction with one's partner in multiple therapy is positively related to his co-therapist's needs for autonomy, dependence, and nurturance. It was concluded that certain needs in an individual partially determines the selection of his co-therapist and that satisfaction in the multiple therapy relationship is related to some of his co-therapists' needs. A general con— clusion of this investigation is that the mutual relation- ship that exists between pairs of multiple therapists is affected by the needs of both therapists. The present paper raised questions about multiple therapists in general and about the interaction between pairs of multipletherapists and gave suggestions which should prove helpful to further investigations. REFERENCES Adler, J. & Berman, I. R. Multiple leadership in group treatment of delinquent adolescents. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1960, 10, 213-225. Alexander, L. & Moore, M. Multiple Therapy in private psychiatric practice. Amer. J. Psychiat., 1957, 113, Bellak, L. The Thematic Apperception Test and the Children's Apperception Test in Clinical Use. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1954. Belmont, L. P. & Jasnow, A.. The utilization of co—therapists and of group therapy techniques in a family oriented approach to a disturbed child. Int. J. Group Psychother 1961, 11, 319-328. Bowerman, C. E. & Day, Barbara R. A test of the theory of complementary needs as applied to couples during courtship. Am. Sociol. Rev., 1956, 21, 602-605. Brooks, W. W. Satisfaction of the manifest dominance and deference needs in competitive situations. Dissert. Abstr., 1964, pp, (5), 3121. Cameron, J. L. & Steward, R. A. Y. ‘Observations on group psychotherapy with chronic psychoneurotic patients in mental hospitals. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1955, i. 346-360. Carmen, P. M. The relationship of individual and husband- wife patterns of personality characteristics to marital stability. Dissert. Abstr., 1955, 1;, Part 1, 1113. Daniels, M. The influence of the sex of the therapist and of the co-therapist technique in group psychotherapy with boys: an investigation of the effectiveness of group psychotherapy with eighth grade, behavior- problem boys, comparing results achieved by a male therapist, by a female therapist, and by the two therapists in combination. Dissert. Abstr., 1958, .;§, 1849, Part I. 80 81 Demarest, E., 8. Tercher, A. Transference in group therapy; its use by co-therapists of opposite sexes. Psychiat., 1954, 11, 187—202. Dreikurs, R. Techniques and dynamics of multiple psycho- therapy. Psychiat. Quart., 1950, 14, 788—799. Dreikurs, R., Schulman, B. H., & Mosak, H. Patient-therapist relationship: I. Its advantages to the therapist. Psychiat. Quart., 1952, gp, 219-227. (a) Dreikurs, R., Schulman, B. H. & Mosak, H. Patient-therapist relationship in multiple psychotherapy. II. Its advantages for the patient. Psychiat. Quart., 1952, pp, 590-596. (b) Dyrud, J. E. & Rioch, M. Multiple therapy in the treatment program of a mental hospital. Psychiat., 1953, 1Q, 21_26 0 Edwards, A. L. Manual for the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1953. Framo, J. L. Rational and techniques of intensive family therapy. In I. BQSZOrmenyi—Nagy and J. L. Framo (Eds.). Intensive family therapy. New York: Harper and Rowe, 1965, pp. 143-212. Demarest, E. & Teicher, A. Transference in group therapy: its use by co—therapists of opposite sexes. Psychiat., 1954, 11, 187-202. Gans, R. W. The use of group co—therapists in the teaching of psychotherapy. Am. J. Psychother., 1957, 11, 618-625. Gans, R. W. Group cotherapists in the therapeutic situation: a critical evaluation. Int. J. group Psychothgp., 1962, 11, 82—88. Gottlieb, A. & Pattison, E. M. Married couples group psycho- therapy. Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 1966, 14, 143-152. Greenback, R. K. Psychotherapy using two therapists. Amer. J. Psychother., 1964, 18, 488—499. Hadden, S. B. The utilization of a therapy group in teaching psychotherapy. Amer. J. Psychiat., 1947, 103, 644—648. Haigh, G. & Kell, B. L. Multiple therapy as a method for training and research in psychotherapy. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol., 1950, 4;, 659-666. 82 1"Ia‘i’Ward, M. L., Peters, J. J., & Taylor, J. E. Some values of the use of multiple therapists in the treatment of psychoses. Psychiat. Quart., 1952, pp, 244-249. Heiss, J. S. & Gordon, M. Need patterns and the mutual satisfaction of dating and engaged couples. J. Marr. Fam., 1964, pg (3), 337-338. Hulse, W. C., Lulow, W. V., Rindsberg, B. K., & Epstein, N. B. Transference reactions in a group of female patients to male and female co—leaders. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1956, p, 430-435. Issacs, G. Team conflict--a recapituation of conflict in the home: a descriptive clinical case study in nursing therapy. Dissert. Abstr., 1965, g; (4), 6545. Kadis, A. L. & Markowitz, M. The therapeutic impact of co-therapist interaction in a couples group. In J. L. Mareno (Ed.), International handbook of group psychotherapy. New York: Philosophical Library, 1966, 446-455. Kassoff, A. Advantages of multiple-therapists in a group of severely acting-out adolescent boys. Int. J. Katz, 1., Glucksberg, S., and Krauss, R. Need Satisfaction and Edwards PPS scores in married couples. J. Consult. Psychol., 1960, 14, 205-208. Kell, B. L. & Burow, Josephine Morse. Human Developmental Therapy. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, in press. Kell, B. L. Personal Communication. 1968. Kerchoff, A. C. & Davis, K. E. Value consensus and need complementarityin mate selection. Amer. Sociol. Rev., 1962, 11, 295—303. Kritzer, H. & Phillips, C. A. Observing group psycho- therapy-—an affective learning experience. Am. . Leary, T. Interpersonal diagnosis of personality. New York: TheRonald Press Co., 1957. Levinger, G. the on need complementarity in marriage. Psychol. Bull., 1964, 91 (2), 153-157. 83 Llnden, M. E. The significance of dual leadership in gerontolic group psychotherapy: studies on gerOntOIic human rational, III, Int. J. Group Psychother., 1954, 3, 262-273. Loeffler, F. & Weinstein, H. M. The cotherapist method: special problems and advantages. Group Psychother., 1954, p. 189-192. Lundin, W. H. & Aronov, M. The use of cotherapists in group psychotherapy. J. Consult. Psychol., 1952, 1Q, 76-80. MacLennan, B. W. Co-therapy. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1965, 1;. 154-166. Mintz, Elizabeth A. Special values of co—therapists in group psychotherapy. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1963' J-_31 127-132 (a). Mintz, Elizabeth A. Transference in co-therapy groups. J. Consult. Psychol., 1963, 11, 34—49 (b). Mintz, Elizabeth A. Male-female co—therapists: some values and some problems. Amer. J. Psychother., 1965, 12, 293-301. Mullan, H. & Sanguiliano, Iris. Multiple psychotherapeutic practice: preliminary report. Amer. J. Psychol., 1960, 14, 550—565. Mullan, H. & Sanguiliano, Iris. The therapist's contribution to the treatment process. Springfield, 111.: Charles C. Thomas, 1964. Mullan, H. Discussion Voices: The Art and science of psychotherapy, 1967, 1 (2), 12-13. Murray, H. A. Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938. Murray, H. A. Thematic Apperception Test Manual. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University, 1943. NUnnelly, K. G. The use of multiple therapy in group counseling and psychotherapy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1968. Orange, A. J. A note on brief group psychotherapy with psychotic patients. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1955, p, 80-83. 84 Palonen, Donna J. Interpersonal perceptions and marital adjustment. Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1966. Pine, 1., Todd, W., & Boenheim, C. Special problems of resistance in co—therapy groups. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1963, 1Q, 334-362. Rabin, H. M. How does co—therapy compare with regular group psychotherapy. Am. J. Psychother., 1967, £1 (2), 244—255 (a). Rabin, H. M. Personal communication. 1967 (b). Reeve, G. H. Trends in therapy: V. A method of coordinated treatment. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat., 189, 2, 743—747. Rosow, I. Issues in the concept of need—complementarity. Sociometry, 1957, 29, 216-233. Rubinstein, D. Family therapy. Int. Psychiat. Cliniqg, 1964, 1 (2), 431—442. Rubinstein, D. & Weiner, O. R. Co-therapy teamwork relation— ships. In G. H. Zuk and I. Boszormenyi-Nagy (Eds.). Family therapy and disturbed families. Palo Alto: Science and Behavioral Books, 1967, pp. 206—220. Rychlak, J. F. The similarity, compatibility, or incom- patibility of needs in interpersonal selection. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 1956, g (3), 334—340. Sabath, G. Intertransference: transference relationship between members of the psychotherapy team. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1962, 1; (H), 492-495. Slavson, S. R. Analytic grouppppychotherapy with children, adolesggnts and adults. New York: Columbia University Press, 1950. Slavson. S. R. Discussion. Int. J. Group Psychothg£., 1960, 1Q, 225h226. Solomon, A., Loeffler, F. J., & Frank, G. H. An analysis of co-therapists interaction in group psychotherapy. Int. J. Group Psychother., 1954, 1, 171—180. Sonne, J. C. & Lincoln, Geraldine. The importance of heterosexual co—therapy relationship in the con— struction of a family image. Psychiatric Res. Rep., 1966, 19, 196-205. 85 313121;, H. H. & Kopp, S.B. Multiple psychotherapy. In M. Rosenbaum and M. Berger (Eds.). Group Psychotherapy and group functions. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1963: pp. 391-406. Staples, E. J. The influence of the sex of the therapist and of the co-therapist technique in group psycho— therapy With girls: an investigation of the effectiveness of group psychotherapy with eighth grade, behavior problem girls, comparing results achieved by a male therapist, a female therapist, and by two therapists in combination. Dissert. Abstr., 1959, 12, 2154. Stein, M. H. The unconscious meaning of the marital bond. In V. W. Eisenstein (Ed.). Neurotic interaction in marriage. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1956, pp. 65-80. Tharp, R. G. Psychological patterning in marriage. Psychol. Bull., 1963, §Q_(2), 97-117. Tharp, R. G. Reply to Levinger's note. Psychol. Bull., 1964, Q1 (2), 158-160. Warkentin, J., Johnson, Nan,L., & Whitaker, C. A. A comparison of individual and multiple psychotherapy. Psychiat., 1951, 14, 415-418. Warkentin, J. Partners in psychotherapy. Voices: The art and science of Psychotherapy, 1967, 1 (2), 7—12. Whitaker, C. A., Warkentin, J. & Johnson, Nan L. A philosoph- ical basis for brief psychotherapy. Psychiat. Quart., 1949, 11, 439—443. Whitaker, C. A., Warkentin, J., & Johnson, Nan L. The psychotherpeutic impasse. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat., 1950, 19, 641-647. Whitaker, C. A., Malone, T. P., & Warkentin, J. Multiple therapy and psychotherapy. In F. Fromm—Reichmann and J. L. Moreno (Eds.). Progress in ppychotherapy. Vol. 1, New York: Grune and Stratton, 1956, pp. 210-216. Whitaker, C. A., Felder, R. E., and Warkentin, J. Counter— transference in the family treatment of schizophrenia. In I. Boszormenyi-Nagy and J. L. Framo (Eds.). Intensive family therapy. New York: Harper and Row, 1965, pp. 323-341. 86 Vflaitaker, C. A. Personal communication. 1967. ‘Winch, R, F. and Mere, D. A. Does TAT add information to 'interviews?_ Statistical analysis of the increment. J. Clin. Psychol., 1956, 12, 316-321. Winch, R. F. Mate selection: a study of comp1ementary needs. New York: Harper & Brothers, 1958. , APPENDIX A The Raw Data of the No. of Pairs of Therapists in each sample Who Are Similar or Bipolar in Terms of Their Needs for Deference, Dominance, and Passivity. Table A. APPENDIX A Number of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for deference. Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for deference 6 7 No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for deference 6 5 Table B. NUmber of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for deference. Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for deference 6 8 No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for deference 6 4 as 89 Table C. NUmber of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for passivity. Multiple Therapy Control Sample Sample No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for passivity 6 6 No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for passivity 6 6 APPENDIX B Fl-Eflli— Vs...— The Raw Data of the No. of Pairs of Therapists in Each Sample who are Similar or Bipolar in Terms of their Needs for Abasement, Achievement, Affiliation, Aggression, Autonomy, Dependence, Heterosexuality, and NUrturance APPENDIX B Table D. Number of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for abasement. Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for abasement 8 5 No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for abasement 4 7 Table E. Number of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for achievement. Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for achievement 7 6 Nb. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for achievement 5 6 91 Table F. 92 No. of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for affiliation. Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample Nb. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for affiliation 8 3 No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for affiliation 4 9 Table G. No. of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for aggression. Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for aggression 6 6 No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for aggression 6 6 Table H. No. of pairs of therapists inwthe multiple 93 therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipélar in terms of their need for autonomy. No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for autonomy 'No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for autonomy 4* Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample 7 2 5 10 Table I. No. of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for dependence. No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for dependence No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for dependence Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample 8 8 4 4 94 'Dable J. ‘No. of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and in the control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for heterosexuality. No. of pairs of therapists who are similar in their need for heterosexuality No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for heterosexuality Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample 4 7 8 5 Table K. No. of pairs of therapists in the multiple therapist sample (N = 12) and intzhe control sample (N = 12) who are similar or bipolar in terms of their need for nurturance. No. of pairs of therapists who are Similar in their need for nurturance No. of pairs of therapists who are bipolar in their need for nurturance Multiple Therapist Control Sample Sample 10 4 l'l ICHIGRN STHT E UNIV. LIBRARIES HI I" WWW\IHIIIWIHW 03126357 312931