ABSTRACT AN ANALYSIS OF CRITICAL INCIDENTS FOR RECENTLY EMPLOYED MICHIGAN COOPERATIVE EXTENSION AGENTS WITH IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAINING by Fred J. Peabody This study is part of a continuing search for instru— ments and techniques by which to accurately assess training needs of Cooperative Extension Service personnel. It draws heavily upon previous research and proceeds from a base of theory in three areas: basic human needs; role requirements expressed as training needs; and techniques for identifying and analyzing training needs. This research was designed to employ the critical incident technique to analyze the jobs of COOperative Exten- sion Agents. The purposes were to describe the job require- ments perceived as critical by a population of Michigan extension agents, to identify the training needs, and to determine possible differences in training needs and job. requirements according to the employment position and the tenure of incumbent agents. Critical incidents comprised the basic data for this study. A critical incident report form was developed which contained scaled response items of both the importance and difficulty of self-reported incidents which agents perceived as critical for job success. Four hundred and forty-four Fred J. Peabody critical incidents were collected from 74 subjects in 23 small group meetings. The incidents consisted of written descript- ions of effective and ineffective job performance. Effective incidents were inferred as representing competencies already possessed by the reporting agent; ineffective incidents were inferred as representing training needs; and the combination of both were inferred to represent critical job requirements. Incidents were classified among critical performance categories according to a classification system based upon the work of earlier extension researchers. These performance categories consisted of six functional areas of agent job performance. The data were processed by the Computer Laboratory at Michigan State University. The descriptive statistics em- ployed included percentage and frequency distribution, mean scores of importance and of difficulty, and rank-order. The data are reported in 46 contingency tables which show the frequency, importance, and difficulty of effective and inef- fective critical incidents by the assigned critical perform- ance categories. These data are also presented according to the position and tenure of the reporting agents. Over 98.5 percent of the incidents reported by agents were classified within the critical performance categories of the classification system. A hierarchy of frequency with which critical job requirements occurred was found for agents in general. That hierarchy was: Teaching and Communicating, Fred J. Peabody Organizing, Conducting Programs, Administering, Program Plan- ning, and Evaluating. Teaching and Communicating was the most frequent critical performance category for Home Econo— mists and Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents, but 4-H - Youth Agents most frequently reported Organizing incidents. Agents of different tenure reported indidents with similar frequency in all categories except Evaluating. Inexperi- enced agents very infrequently reported incidents classified in the Evaluating category. Similar frequency rank-orders resulted for competen- cies (effective incidents), for training needs (ineffective incidents), and for critical job requirements (combined effective and ineffective incidents). Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents and Home Economists most frequently reported Teaching and Communicating competencies, while 4—H - Youth Agents most frequently reported Organizing competencies. Regardless of position, agents most frequently reported train- ing needs in the Teaching and Communicating category. In general, agents' ratings of the importance of inci- dents resulted in a rank-order hierarchy which differed from that for frequency. The importance hierarchy was: Organizing, Conducting Programs, Program Planning, Evaluating, Teaching and Communicating, and Administering. While ratings by Home Economists resulted in ranking Organizing as the most impor- tant category of critical job requirements, ratings by the 4-H - Youth Agents resulted in ranking Conducting Programs Fred J. Peabody first. Ratings by Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents resulted in ranking the Evaluating category first in importance. Experienced agents reported Program Planning as most important. Inexperienced agents gave top importance to Organizing. Agents generally attached much greater importance to the competencies they possessed than to their needs for train- ing. Differential ranks resulted from agents' ratings of the importance of both competencies and training needs according to their position and tenure. For agents in general, the rank-order hierarchy of difficulty was different from that for either frequency or importance of performing job requirements. The difficulty hierarchy for all agents was: Evaluating, Organizing, Admi- nistering, Program Planning, Teaching and Communicating, and Conducting Programs. Agents generally reported less difficulty performing incidents in which they possessed competencies. Perception of incident difficulty varied by agent tenure and position. Findings are limited to describing the perception of incumbent agents only. Lack of high agreement of incident classification by researcher and judges limits the extent to which the findings ought to be generalized. The use of scaled items in describing critical incidents appears to be a pro- mising refinement to indicate the degree of criticalness of incidents. Several hypotheses related to employment role perception are suggested. AN ANALYSIS OF CRITICAL INCIDENTS FOR RECENTLY EMPLOYED MICHIGAN COOPERATIVE EXTENSION AGENTS WITH IMPLICATIONS FOR TRAINING by Fred J. Peabody A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY College of Education 1968 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author is indebted to Professor Russell J. Kleis who served as chairman of the doctoral guidance committee. His painstaking efforts in directing this thesis and his persistent employment of high scholarly standards are hereby recognized. Acknowledgment is also due the members of the doc- toral guidance committee. Appreciation is expressed to doctors Sheldon G. Lowry, Mason E. Miller, Max R. Raines, and Troy L. Stearns for their direction, assistance, and advice. A special tribute is due Dr. Mason E. Miller who listened patiently and counselled soundly. The Institute for Extension Personnel Development was a valuable source of information. The author is grate- ful for the freedom to draw from several disciplines for this investigation. Gratitude is extended the Michigan CooPerative Extension Service-for study leave policies which permitted the author to pursue a doctoral program. The cooperation of Director George S. McIntyre and of the Extension Program Directors in securing the data for this research is greatly appreciated. This investigation would have been impossible ii without the active c00peration of the agent-subjects who responded in such an excellent fashion to the rather arduous task of writing longhand descriptions of critical incidents. Special thanks are extended to Miss Anne E. Field, Dr. Howard L. Miller, and Mr. Joe T. Waterson who served as judges in the time consuming task of classifying selected critical incidents. This work is dedicated to my wife June and our children, Carl, Jeff, and Anne, who sacrificed most to make it possible. They have shared both the hardships and the satisfactions of accomplishment. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 LIST OF TABLES O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O 0 LIST OF FIGURES I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 LIST OF APPENDICES O O O O O O O O O I O O O I O 0 Chapter I 0 INTRODUCTION 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O I 0 Background . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Problem. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Definition of Terms. . . . . . . . . . . Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II. THEORETICAL AND OPERATIONAL BASIS OF THE STUDY 0 C O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O 0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Basic Human Needs. . . . . . . . . . . . Role Requirements Expressed as Training Needs. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Problems in Id ntifying Training Needs . Training Needs of COOperative Extension Workers. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of Implications of Studies of Training Needs . . . . . . . . . . . . . Techniques for Identifying and Analyzing Training Needs . . . . . . . . . . . . . Applications of the Critical Incident Technique. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Attributes of the Critical Incident Technique. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I I I O METHODOLOGY 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The subjects 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 iv Page ii vi xi [—J \DmWH 11 11 ll 19 19 23 27 28 29 39 39 42 42 42 Chapter Instrumentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . A Classification System for Critical Incidents . . . . . . . . . Pretesting Procedure. . . . Data Collection Procedure Data Analysis . The Hypotheses. Some Questions. Summary . . . . IV. FINDINGS O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O 0 Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Frequency Distribution of Classified Incidents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Relationship of Agent Position and Tenure to CPC's . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hierarchies of CPC's for Incident Frequency, Importance, and Difficulty . . Hierarchy of CPC's for Incident Importance. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Hierarchy of CPC' s for Incident Difficulty. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Agent Tenure and Incident Difficulty. . Agent Tenure and Incident Importance. . Agent Tenure and Incident Effectiveness Agent Tenure, Incident Difficulty, and Effectiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Agent Position, Incident Difficulty, and Effectiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Agent Tenure, Incident Importance, and Effectiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Agent Position, Incident Importance, and Effectiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX. The Problem . . . . . . . . . Respondents . . . . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . . Findings and Conclusions. . . . Conclusions about the Critical Inc' a 1 . Technique . .-. . . . . . . Limitations . . . . . . . . Summary of Conclusions. . . Implications and Recommend t'ons cecal-loco. 0.: 0.000.... :1 oooorfoooo BIBLIOGMPHY O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Page 50 53 54 56 58 61 66 66 68 68 72 82 89 91 93 96 99 103 104 110 118 124 130 134 134 134 136 137 145 150 151 154 158 186 Table 1. 10. 11. 12. 13. LIST OF TABLES Page Distribution of Respondents by Sex and Employment Position. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 Distribution of ReSpondents by Age and Employment POSition O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O 4 5 Distribution of Respondents by Tenure and POSition I O O l C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 46 Respondent Previous Work Experience and Employment Position. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 Respondent Level of Academic Attainment by Employment Position. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Researcher Agreement in Classifying Critical Incidents by Category and Subcategory. . . . . . 69 Agreement of Judges' and Researcher's Classi- fication of a Sample of 44 Critical Incidents. . 7o Researcher Agreement in Classifying 403 Critical Incidents by Category and Subcategory . 71 Distribution among Critical Performance Categories of 438 Critical Incidents reported by Extension Agents. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 Frequency and Rank—order of Critical Job Require- ments for the Critical Performance Category of Teaching and Communicating . . . . . . . . . . . 75 Frequency and Rank-order of Critical Job Require- ments for the Critical Performance Category of organiZing O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O 76 Frequency and Rank-order of Critical Job Require- ments for the Critical Performance Category of Conducting Programs. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Frequency and Rank-order of Critical Job Require- ments for the Critical Performance Category of Administering. O O O O O O O C C C O O C O O O I 79 vi Table 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. Frequency and Rank-order of Critical Job Requirements for the Critical Performance Category of Program Planning . . . . . . . . . Frequency and Rank-order of Critical Job Requirements for the Critical Performance Category of Evaluating . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Critical Incidents among 6 Critical Performance Categories by Position of Reporting Agent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Critical Incidents among 6 Critical Performance Categories by‘Tenure of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution among Six Critical Performance Categories of Agent Reported Effective and Ineffective Critical Incidents . . . . . . . . Frequency and Mean Scores of Importance for Agent Reported Critical Incidents Distributed among Critical Performance Categories. . . . . Frequency and Mean Scores of Difficulty for Agent Reported Critical Incidents Distributed among Critical Performance Categories. . . . . Summary of the Rank-order of Critical Perform— ance Categories by the Frequency, Importance, and Difficulty of Executing Critical Incidents Distribution of Mean Scores of Difficulty for Critical Incidents Classified among Critical Performance Categories by Tenure of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Importance for Critical Incidents Classified among Critical Performance Categories by Tenure of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Frequency Distribution among Critical Perform- ance Categories of Effective Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . vii Page 81 82 85 88 90 93 94 96 98 101 105 Table 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. Frequency Distribution among Critical Perform- ance Categories of Ineffective Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Difficulty among Critical Performance Categories for Effective Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Difficulty among Critical Performance Categories for Ineffective Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Difficulty among Critical Performance Categories for all Critical Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Difficulty among Critical Performance Categories for Effective Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Difficulty among Critical Performance Categories for Ineffective Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Difficulty among Critical Performance Categories for all Critical Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Importance among Critical Performance Categories for Effective Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Importance among Critical Performance Categories for Ineffective Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Importance among Page 105 107 109 111 112 114 117 119 121 Critical Performance Categories for all Critical Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Importance among Critical Performance Categories for Effective Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent . . . Distribution of Mean Scores of Importance among Critical Performance Categories for Ineffective Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent . . . viii 123 125 127. Table 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. Page Distribution of Mean Scores of Importance among Critical Performance Categories for all Critical Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent . . . . 129 Summary of the Acceptance or Rejection of Hypo- theses Based upon an Analysis of Study Data. . . 131 Summary of Mean Scores of Difficulty and Import- ance by Tenure and Position of Reporting Agent as Analyzed in Response to Study Questions . . . 132 Summary of the Frequency Rank-order of Critical Performance Categories and Component Critical Job Requirements for Michigan Extension Agents . 139 Summary of the Rank-order of Critical Perform- ance Categories for Frequency, Difficulty, and Importance of Effective Critical Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . . 146 Summary of the Rank-order of Critical Perform- ance Categories for Frequency, Difficulty, and Importance of Ineffective Critical Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . . . . . . . 146 Summary of the Rank-order of Critical Perform- ance Categories for Frequency, Difficulty, and Importance of all Critical Incidents by Tenure of Reporting Agent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147 Summary of the Rank-order of Critical Performance Categories for Frequency, Difficulty, and Importance of Effective Critical Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . 147 Summary of the Rank-order of Critical Performance Categories for Frequency, Difficulty, and Importance of Ineffective Critical Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . 148 Summary of the Rank-order of Critical Performance Categories for Frequency, Difficulty, and Import- ance of all Critical Incidents by Position of Reporting Agent. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Employee Maintenance and Motivational Needs. . 15 2. A Theoretical Model for the Taking of Organizational Roles . . . . . . . . . . . . l7 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix A ”It! D on: Questionnaire for Extension Agents Reporting Critical Incidents . . . . . . . . Classification System for Critical Incidents Administrator Letter to Respondents. . . . . Researcher Letter to Respondents . . . . . . Confirmation Letter to Respondents . . . . . Examples of Critical Incident Classification xi Page 158 166 176 178 179 181 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background In modern industrial society a highly refined divi- sion of labor has resulted in increased rolel specialization and complex bureaucratic organizations. Hence, the neOphyte incumbent to a professional position within an organization likely faces a complex process of role socialization.2 Roles in an organization are created in order to ful- fill the goals of that organization.3 Roles thus conceived represent organizational needs. Therefore, the organization holds a vital concern for the behaviors which are essential to successful role performance. The new employee might be viewed as a threat to the organization until he is suffi- ciently socialized to permit him to successfully perform his role. 1A role consists of a set of eXpected behaviors for a member of a social group. 2Role socialization is the postemployment process by which a person acquires the skills, knowledge, and atti- tudes associated with successful performance of a professional role. 3Daniel Katz and Robert L. Kahn, The Social Psychology of Organizations (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 19667. Organizations generally have formal role sociali- zationl procedures for new employees. These include induction—orientation2 programs designed to facilitate satisfactory role performance by the new incumbent. The informal socialization seems not to abruptly halt at the end of a few months of employment, but rather extends well into the employment years.3 The input of institu— tional resources for the training of personnel is an investment for maintaining and increasing productivity. The COOperative Extension Service is one of a num- ber of organizations to which the above generalizations may apply. The COOperative Extension Service was created by the Federal Congress in 1914 when it passed the Smith— . ~M .: --«-‘...‘ -. lFormal or planned role socialization are terms used synonymously with training and consist of purpose— fully organized learning experiences sponsored by the employing organization. 2Induction-orientation programs are planned sociali- zation experiences which are provided prior to, or immedi— ately following, assignment to a role. 3Orville G. Brim, Jr. & Stanton Wheeler, Socializa- tion After Childhood: Two Essays (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1966), p. 18. 4Sally J. Olean, Changing Patterns in Continuing Education for Business (Brookline, Mass.: Center for the Study of Liberal Education for Adults, 1967), pp. 8-12. Lever Act. Its purpose is: ...to aid in diffusing among the peOple of the United States useful and practical information on subjects related to agriculture and home economics and to encourage the application of the same... There were 13,766 field and state extension personnel working in the United States on August 31, 1966. During the 12 months previous to this date, 1,480 new staff mem- bers were hired. Thus, 10.8 percent of all personnel were newly hired.2 There were 384 field and state personnel working in Michigan on July 1, 1966. Of that number, 252 were field agents. During the calendar year of 1966 there were 30 agent separations reported.3 Therefore, the rate of sepa- ration or turnover among field agents was 11.9 percent. The Problem Extension administrators are responsible for allocat- ing scarce resources for agent training. To the extent that such training is essential to work role fulfillment, 1U. S., Statutes at Large, Vol. XXXVIII (March, 1913 to March, 1915), p. 372. 2Federal Extension Service, Report of Programs in Extension Education for ProfessionaITExtension Workegg TWashington, BTCC.: U. S. Dept. of Agriculture, ER&T-48, March, 1967), p. i. 3Federal Extension Service, Turnover of Cooperative Extension A ents (Washington, D. C.: U.S. Dept. of Agricul- ture' PDM- ' April, 1967) I p. 10 it is vital to the extension organization. Given the im— portance of training, the planner of extension training must understand the job requirements which are critical for successful performance by extension agents. A basic problem of the planner of extension training is to identify training needs,1 to do it with maximum precision, and to express the needs in behavioral terms.2 The specification of training needs is a task which training officers readily acknowledge, but which few have fully achieved. Korb says: Like the weather, most training pe0ple talk about training programs built upon needs, but the truth of the matter is that very few training programs ...have been built on any thorough going investi- gatiog of needs evolved and revealed at the work site. In this study it is assumed: (I) that training needs for extension agents can and should be "evolved and revealed at the work site"; (2) that training needs will differ ac- cording to the tenure of extension agents and according to the nature of the work agents perform; (3) that it is lTyler considers educational need to be a gap between the present condition of the learner and the standard or norm to which he is being compared. See: Ralph W. Tyler, Basic Principles of Curriculum and Instruction (Chicago, 111.: UniverSIty of Chicago Press, 1950), pp. 4-10. 2Robert F. Mager, Preparin Instructional Objectives (Palo Alto: Fearon PubliShers, 1 62), pp. 13-44. 3David L. Korb ,"How to Determine Supervisory Train- ing Needs," Personnel, Vol. 32 (Jan., 1956), p. 338. necessary and possible to distinguish needs of neophyte agents from those of experienced agents, and needs of agents in one major area of responsibility from those in another. The study investigates a technique for identifying and ana- lyzing extension agent training needs in these terms. Previous investigators have employed a variety of means for identifying agent training needs. The first, and most direct, is simply to ask agents to specify their own needs. This approach was used by Clark1 and Coffindaffer.2 Such procedure has the great merit of involving the agent directly, but it depends upon the dubious assumption that a worker may fully know what he does not know. A second approach involves seeking the judgments of supervisors or "relevant others" concerning the training needs of individuals or groups of agents. This approach is rather commonly used in practice and has been investi- gated with extension supervisors in Ohio by McCormick.3 1Harry E. Clark, "An Analysis of the Training Needs of Wisconsin County Extension Service Personnel" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1960). 2Billy L. Coffindaffer, "Experiences of Beginning CooPerative Extension Agents and their Implications for an Induction Training Program" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1961). 3Robert W. McCormick, "An Analysis of Training Needs of County Extension Agents in Ohio" (unpublished Ph.D. dis- sertation, University of Wisconsin, 1959). If the judgment of others is the sole source for identi- fying training needs, it has the disadvantage of ignoring the agent's own perception of his situation and his need. A third approach, the critical incident techniquel develOped and tested in other vocational settings, appears to take account of the shortcomings of the first two men- tioned approaches. It involves the role incumbent in the analysis of his work role and permits him to specify sig- nificant incidents in role performance. Investigations by Fivars and Gosnell,2 Jensen,3 and Glickman and Vallance4 have been based upon such analyses. These investigations were in such divergent fields as nursing, education, and military services. The critical incident technique was utilized to analyze work performance of incumbents to 1The critical incident technique is a behavioral research method which consists of a set of procedures for collecting and analyzing effective and ineffective behaviors related to the actual performance of a job or activity. 2Grace Fivars and Doris Gosnell, Nursinngvaluation: The Problem and the Process: The Critical Incident Tech- nique (New York: The Macmillan Co., 1966). 3Alfred C. Jensen, "Determining Critical Require- ments for Teachers," Journal of Experimental Education, Vol. 20 (1951), pp. 7g135. 4Albert S. Glickman and T. R. Vallance, "Curriculum Assessment with Critical Incidents," Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 42 (1958), pp. 329-335. identify the critical requirements1 of the job being studied and as a basis for making inferences about training needs of incumbents. The central purpose of this study is to employ the critical incident technique as a means of describing the critical job requirements of extension agents, of identi- fying training needs of agents, and of specifying those needs by tenure and by major responsibility of the agents. Specifically the study is designed to answer seven basic questions: 1. Will job behaviors reported as critical inci- dents2 by extension agents reveal training needs similar to those identified by other research methods? 2. Do training needs differ by agent position and if so, what is the character of the differences? 3. Do training needs differ by agent tenure and if so, what is the character of the differences? lRequirements which are critical in the sense that they are associated with definitely effective or ineffect- ive performance of the job or task under study. 2A critical incident is an episode which occurs in the performance of a professional role, the consequences of which are judged by the incumbent to be definitely effective or ineffective. 4. What are the requirements agents perceive as critical for effective job performance? 5. Do certain critical job requirements occur more frequently than others? 6. How important do agents view the identified job requirements to be? 7. To what extent do agents experience difficulty in performing the critical requirements of their jobs? Definition of Terms Field Extension Agent is a person who occupies a position as an employee of the C00perative Extension Service and who works on a county, multi-county, area, or district basis. Synonymous terms include Extension Agent, or Agent. Field Extension Agents are often identified according to the following subject responsibilities: Agriculture and/or Natural Resources 4-H - Youth Programs Family Living Education or Home Economics An Experienced Agent is considered to be one who has held a field extension position for 18 months or more. None in this study had been employed more than 6 years. An Inexperienced Agent is considered to be one who has held a field extension position for less than 18 months. None in this study had been employed less than 3 months. The Critical Incident Technique is a behavioral research method which consists of a set of pro- cedures for collecting and analyzing effective and ineffective behaviors related to the actual performance of a job or activity. ( A Critical Incident is an episode which occurs in the performance of a professional role, the con- sequences of which are judged by the incumbent to be definitely effective or ineffective. An Effective Incident is a critical incident in which professional role behaviors were perceived as successful role fulfillment by the reporting agent. An Ineffective Incident is a critical incident in which professional role behaviors were perceived as unsuccessful role fulfillment by the reporting agent. Critical Job Requirements are requirements which are critical in the sense that they are associated with definitely effective or ineffective perform- ance of the job or task under study. Critical Performance Categories are major groups of related critical job requirements. A Training Need is a gap between the present con- dition of the learner and the standard or norm to which he is being compared. In this study, a training need is inferred from the report of ineffective incidents. Incident Importance is the extent to which an agent perceiVes an incident as likely to influence his success as an agent. Incident Difficulty is the extent to which an agent felt taxed in executing the tasks involved in a critical incident. Overview A frame of reference for the entire study is develOped in Chapter I. A description of the background for the study is presented along with a statement of the research problem. The:specific questions with which this research deals are stated and important terms are defined. -10- The theoretical and Operational basis for the study are presented in Chapter II. The discussion proceeds from a base of theory in three areas: basic human needs; role requirements expressed as training needs; and techniques for identifying and analyzing training needs. The study design and procedures are described in Chapter III. Information is presented about the study subjects, the instruments employed and the pretesting procedure followed. Data collection and analysis procedures are described and the research hypotheses are presented. Chapter IV contains an analysis of the data with descriptions of the findings pertaining to each hypothesis. A summary of the study, the conclusions, and the implications for further research are presented in Chapter V. .a - m. IF '0‘ 0‘. (II A.» 9' IA (1.1 l (I) Mo 5 pl CHAPTER II THEORETICAL AND OPERATIONAL BASES OF THE STUDY Introduction This study is part of a continuing search for instru- ments and techniques with which to accurately assess training needs of Cooperative Extension Service personnel. It draws heavily upon previous research and proceeds from a base of theory in three areas: basic human needs; role requirements expressed as training needs; and techniques for identifying and analyzing training needs. Basic Human Needs In the absence of a single perfect theory of human behavior, it is necessary to draw from several theories to explain human behavior in the work situation. One of these theories is based upon the concept of human motivation toward need fulfillment. Abraham Maslow1 contends that certain human needs exist which are nearly culture-free or universal. However, the means for ful- filling those needs may be quite dissimilar among cultures. lAbraham Maslow, Motivation and Personality (New York: Harper Brothers, 1954). -11- ,- o¢\ PLL -12- Maslow views the human condition as such that man is never fully satisfied. Man is seeking what is lacked. His conception of the hierarchical arrangement of human needs is as follows: Self-actualization Self-esteem and the esteem of others Belongingness and love Safety Physiological needs If all needs are unsatisfied, the physicflogical needs dominate and the others are relegated to the back- ground. Once physiological needs are fulfilled, other, higher needs become dominant. An individual's need state is not static, but is considered to fluctuate with other factors. In this view, when a need is satisfied, it is no longer a motivating force. Another theory of work behavior relates job satis- faction to a hierarchy of human need fulfillment. Herzberg, Mausner, and Synderman1 postulated that man possesses two sets of needs. The first are "avoidance needs" to evade pain from the environment. The second set includes the ”approach needs" for psychological growth. These reflect lSee Frederick Herzberg, Bernard Mausner, and Barbara Synderman, The Motivation to Work (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1959), pp.*13-19; and Frederick Herzberg, Work and the Nature of Man (Cleveland: The World Publish- ing Co., I966).pp. 71-91. -13- man's need to know, to understand, and to become self- actualized. Empirical testing of this theory revealed differen- tiated factors associated with job satisfaction and dis- satisfaction. It was concluded that satisfying and dissatisfying factors are arranged on a unipolar rather than a bipolar scale. The satisfying group contains the intrinsic motivators inherent in the content of the task or job. The dissatisfying group describes man's relation— ship to the context or environment in which he does his job. These latter, extrinsic motivators are called hy- giene factors by Herzberg, but others1 have labeled them maintenance factors. Studies2 which have tested this "motivational-- maintenance" theory of job satisfaction have offered the tentative conclusion that performance of a new work task tends to generate greater reaction to the context or en- vironment in which that task is performed. In highly stressful situations, maintenance needs emerge as most important. lSee Scott M. Myers, "Who are your Motivated Workers?," Harvard Business Review, Vol. 42 (Jan.-Feb., 1964), pp. 73-88; and Denzil O. Clegg, "Work as a Moti- vator," Journal of Cogperative Extension, Vol. 1 (Fall, 1963). PP. 141-148. 2A number of studies are reviewed by Herzberg, OE. Cite, pp. 93-167. -14- This seems consistent with Maslow's concept of a hierarchy of human needs whereby physiological and security needs are considered basic to other higher needs. Also, one would expect new workers to be concerned about estab- lishing a sense of belonging in the new environment. Contrarily, experienced workers might already have estab- lished such a sense and thus turn their attention toward other tasks at hand. In the motivational sense, they are attracted to fulfill needs for human growth and self- actualization. Myers1 tested the "maintenance--motivationa1" theory with employees of an industrial company. Study conclusions essentially supported the theory since the work satisfying factors seemed to be distinct from work dissatisfying factors. Hence, the unipolar character- istic of the theory held when tested under industrial conditions. An adaptation of Myers' model of this theory is shown in Figure l. lMyerS, OE. Cite, Pp. 73-88. -15- H H wusmam mommz choflwm>flpoz pcm mocmcmucfimz mmwoamEm .em .a ..eanHH momma Hmwoom uxmucoo HMOHmwnm 90w OHEocoom .mcflmm wuflhsomm ImHDMmmHQ coHumucmHHo mommz wee msumum muz¢zmezHHeoz non coeuflsmoomm .mcflmm £u3ouo nmflumm ucmEm>cho< was V\\\ tan .4 n... i . tr. on. 9.... 'Ov "H ‘V‘ A i ‘QV C.) 0" D (I) Q“ -16... The above theories are oriented to needs based upon the psychological nature of the individual. Other theories deal with impinging forces which influence the individual in the work situation. Evidence1 indicates that one's personality is profoundly influenced by environmental interaction. Therefore, the study of sociological rela- tionships and structure in human organizations is stressed by authors like Merton.2 Between the extremes of psychological and sociolo- gical theories of human behavior resides the middle ground of social psychological theories. A theory so classified is offered by Katz and Kahn3 who have suggested a model of socialization into the work role as part of their Open- system theory of human organization. 1See Robert L. Kahn, Donald M. Wolfe, Robert P. Quinn, and Diedrick J. Snoek, OrganizationalStress, Studies in Role Conflict and Ambiguity (New York: John Wiley 8 Sons, 1964), pp. 35-70; and Katz and Kahn, op. cit., pp. 182-187. 2Robert K. Merton, Social Theory and Social Struc- ture (Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1957). 3Katz and Kahn, Op. cit., p. 28. -17- Figure 2 A Theoretical Model for the Taking of Organizational Roles Personal ' Attributes Y: I x | s I I l Jé \ Organi- Role Senders ; Focal Person zational ,_ Role Sent Reggized Befigiior factors Expectation Role 7K ,’ I ‘\ Inter- i'.' ~ Personal Factors Solid lines denote causal relationships; broken lines show influencing factors. ‘ The model in Figure 2 depicts a recent incumbent, or "focal person," as the recipient of information from relevant others as to his proper role. The relevant others, or role senders, have their eXpectations tempered by such organizational factors as institutional goals, needs, policies, etc. Such personal attributes of the focal person as his abilities, values and needs are con- sidered too. Finally, on the basis of these factors, and tempered through interaction, the role senders assess and evaluate the role performance of the incumbent in the focal 1Ibid., pp. 182 and 187. Pan-- 'U‘Vn- .1. (II (I) . .,.. Ui— NI“— Ev. Vp' fiv‘. ill ban ‘F ‘4. ‘ v- ~:. -l8- position. Subsequently, the sender's expectations are communicated to the incumbent. The incumbent in turn, weighs, evaluates, and assesses these "sent messages" on the basis of his own perceptions which are founded upon his personal attri- butes, his interactions with relevant others, and his conception of organizational goals, policies, and the like. From this theoretical View it is apparent that role socialization is likely accomplished by far more than formal induction-orientation and other training experience. The entire system likely plays a part in an intricate process of socialization of the recent incumbent. Two important assumptions are made in relation to the Open-system theory. One assumption is that the orga- nization goals, needs, policies, and structure are not static, but rather occur in what Katz and Kahnl have called a "dynamic equilibrium." The individual incum- bent similarly does not exist in a static condition, but also grows in a process of dynamic equilibrium. This latter assumption appears to be consistent with Herzberg's lIbid., p. 456. 2Herzberg, op. cit., pp. 71-91, 2 -19- l concept of motivating factors and with Maslow's concept of man's prOpensity for self-actualization. Role Requirements Expressed as Training Needs A number of studies have been conducted which deal with some aspect of post-employment role learning by pro- fessional employees. The emphasis has ranged from evalu- ation studies of actual induction-orientation programs, to studies of general training needs. Some have followed a psychological approach to analyze the fulfillment of personality needs in the work situation. Those studies most relevant are found in research conducted with edu- cators, workers in business and industry, and cooperative extension employees. Problems in Identifyinngraining Needs Studies of problems and concerns of new faculty- members in colleges and universities were conducted by McCaul2 and Tracy.3 The former found faculty members 1Maslow, op. cit. 2Harlan R. McCall, "Problems of New Faculty Members in Colleges and Universities," North Central Association Quarterly, Vol. 36 (Fall, 1961), pp. 222-234. 3Norbert J. Tracy, "Orienting New Faculty Members in Colleges and Universities," North Central Association Quarterly, Vol. 36 (Fall, 1961), pp. 2144221. .D‘ nu in M- O 1 -\~ H u u‘U ”V A. V- .5! '4 - i .U ‘(U -20- had experienced such problems as locating secretarial help and understanding administrative policies. Tracy identi- fied concerns for learning curriculum objectives, content to be taught, and similar areas. Allen and Sutherland1 studied personal adjustment of faculty members and isolated such influencing variables as value consistency between graduating and hiring institutions; communication effective- ness in hiring interviews; and contract specification. These studies illustrate a problem common to much behavioral research. The theoretical background which alternately stressed problems, concerns, and adjustments, subsequently delimited the possible findings. The framing of the question conditioned response possibilities. Studies of elementary and secondary teachers illus- trate two possible differences in research approach for determining the content of induction-orientation training. While most authors agree that training should be based upon need, there is not consistent agreement on whose per- ception of need should be measured. Fishburn2 emphasized the self-perceived competencies needed by teachers and their lLucile A. Allen and Robert L. Sutherland, Role Con- flicts and Congruences (Austin, Tex.: Hogg Foundation, THe University of Texas, 1963). 2C. E. Fishburn, "Learning the Role of the Teacher," iggurnal of Teacher Education, Vol. 17 (Fall, 1966), pp. 329- .31, .FH la a: N -21- relative change in a longitudinal study. McCrearyl, on the other hand, developed a check list of administrators' perception of new elementary teachers' training needs. Programs and methods for orienting and inducting new teachers have been described by Archer2 who emphasizes the means of training with limited regard for the ends. It appears to be one thing to show that video tape play- back is an effective method or means of training; it is quite another to show which types of behavior are most important to learn. Several studies in business and industry have attempted to identify a foundation for training programs on the basis of skills, abilities, and understandings necessary for effective job performance. Some of these have focused upon job analysis. Other studies, like those 1Anne P. McCreary, "Determining Individual Needs of Elementary Teachers as a Basis for an Orientation Program, Journal of Educational Research, Vol. 54 (Sept., 1960), pp. 20-24. 2Vern B. Archer, Roy A. Edelfelt, and Hite Herbert, "Point Points the Way: Project for the Orientation and Induction of New Teachers," NEA Journal, Vol. 54 (Oct., 1965), pp. 29-30. -22- of Herzbergl, Schafferz, and Walsh3, have stressed the relationship of certain personality needs to job satis— faction. A typical approach has been to analyze satis- faction in work according to adaptations of Murray's4 or Maslow's5 lists of human needs. While such studies help test psychological theories and have implications for personnel management, they have been criticized on a number of counts. One might question the extent to which the individual determines the perception of the job, or the job determines the perception of the individual per- forming it. Another concern begs the question, "Is the satisfied or happy worker the productive worker?" 1See Herzberg, et al., The Motivation to Work, pp. cit., pp. 3-20; and HerzBerg, Work and the Nature of Man, OEe Cite, ppe 71-9le 2R. H. Schaffer, "Job Satisfaction as Related to Need Satisfaction in Work," Psychological Monographs, Vol. 67, No. 14, Whole No. 364, (1953i. 3Richard P. Walsh, "The Effect of Needs on Responses to Job Duties," Journal of Counselling Psychology, Vol. 6 (1959). PP. 194-198. 4Henry A. Murray, Exploration in Personality (New York: Oxford University Press, 1938). 5Maslow, op. cit. -23... It would appear that studies of role socialization which measure satisfaction without consideration of perform- ance level, or which consider an individual's perception as a static entity, are not likely to be useful in identi— fying training needs. TrainingNeeds of Cooperative Extension Workers Related research conducted among c00perative exten- sion workers has been designed to identify perception of role for single positions, or to identify training needed by incumbent agents. Coffindafferl develOped a question- naire to measure difficulty experienced and training needed by 134 low tenured field extension agents in the New England states. His questionnaire employed component tasks drawn from earlier extension studies and from agent narrative reports of job activities. Training needs of Ohio field agents in 3 positions were investigated by McCormick2 using a questionnaire. This instrument was designed around nine areas of com- petency needs identified by an earlier National Extension Task Force for Extension Training (1957). The component items reflected the understanding of a number of leaders as to the role of agents, the organizational goals and ob- jectives, and the potential contribution of several human behavior disciplines. 1Coffindaffer, 0p. cit. 2McCormick, op. cit. -24- Similarly, Clarkl developed a questionnaire to meas- ure the importance of, and the training needs for, the nine major extension objectives outlined in the Scope Report2 as perceived by Wisconsin extension field agents. The 55 component items structured around these nine areas were considered by the author to be representative of the knowledge, understandings, and competencies needed by extension personnel in order to meet organizational objectives. The above authors have provided valuable informa- tion for planning agent training experiences. However, in instances where data were obtained by questionnaire, an underlying assumption is that the items are fully rep- resentative of the important content under consideration. The content validity of the research instrument determines the limits of what can be measured by that instrument. The possibility exists that important content items have not been included. lClark, op. cit. 2This is a popular abbreviation for a publication developed under the chairmanship of Paul A. Miller for the 1957 Extension Committee on Organization and Policy (ECOP) entitled: The Cooperative Extension Service Today; A State- ment of Scope and Responsibilipy (no publisher crediEEdT—_— April, 1958). A Subsequent publication, also known as the 'Scope Report,‘ was designed to amplify the 1958 edition. This latter publication is: The Agricultural Extension Service, A Guide to Extension Programs for the Future: The Scope and Responsibilities of the Coopsrative Extension Service. (Raleigh: North Carolina State College, 1959). -25- In addition to the above extension researchers, other authors in the field have specified factors which they consider to be important for the performance of agents' roles. Kelsey and Hearnel have stressed their perception of important duties performed by agents. Problem areas for agents were identified according to Duncan's2 view of extension philoSOphy and the content of training programs offered agents. A National Task Force3 specified competency needs of agents as implied by the purposes of the Cooperative Extension Service. The divergent approaches employed by various authors in describing the terms and the sources for identifying training needs are itemized in Figure 3. lLincoln D. Kelsey and Cannon C. Hearne, Coo era- tive Extension Work (Ithaca: Comstock Publishing, I963), pp. 65-79. 2James A. Duncan, Training COOperative Extension Workers: The Coordinated Approach (Madison: Cooperative Extension Service, University of Wisconsin, April, 1957). 3National Task Force on Cooperative Extension In- service Training, An Inservice Training Program for Coopera- tive Extension Personnel (Topeka: H. M. Ives & Sons, 1960), pp. 12-15. -25- Figure 3 Terms and Sources of Extension Agent Training Needs Author Term Kelsey & Hearne1 Duties Duncan2 Problem areas National Task Force3 Competency needs McCormick4 Competency needs Clark5 Program Emphasis Coffindaffer6 Work areas Source Author's percep- tion of agent functions. Author's percep- tion of philoso- phy and content of training programs Task Force per- ception of common needs as implied by purposes of extension. Perception of agent competency needs as tested with supervisors and agents. Measures of agent perception of competency needs. Measures of agent perception of task difficulty. 1Kelsey & Hearne, Op. cit., pp. 65—79. 2 . Duncan, op. Cit. 3National Task Force on Cooperative Extension In- Service Training, 0p. cit., pp. 12-15. 4McCormick, op. cit. 5 6Coffindaffer, op. cit. Clark, op. cit. -27- Summary of Implications of Studies of Traifiing Needs Upon examination of the writings of the extension authors cited in Figure 3, there were persistent categor- ies of job behaviors for extension workers. Such behavi- oral categories appeared to be descriptions of functions performed by extension agents. While terminology varied by author, reference to one or more of the following categories was found: 1. 6. Planning and developing extension programs Implementing programs through social systems Communicating and using the educational process Conducting programs for developing human, economic, and other natural resources Evaluating programs, conducting research, and solving problems Administering extension work Furthermore, the following is a summary of impli— cations of researcher decisions which appear to be especially relevant for research related to training needs: 1. The theoretical approach and attendant questions asked by the researcher limits the responses and subsequently the application of results. There is an issue as to whose perception ought to be considered in determining training needs. -23- 3. The identification of the need for and the con- tent of early role socialization on the job appears to be basic to the consideration of methods for accomplishing such socialization. 4. Perception is probably dynamic rather than static. So consideration should be given to impinging factors like environment, group goals and role expectations, and changing psy- chological attributes. 5. The content validity of a behavioral research instrument largely determines what that device can measure. Techniques for Identifying and AnalyzingiTraininngeeds In seeking a method for identifying training needs, the critical incident technique (CIT) was uncovered. The CIT consists of a set of procedures for collecting and analyzing effective and ineffective behaviors related to the actual performance of a job or activity.1 This tech- nique was develOped by Dr. John Flanagan as a method to 1Robert M. Kessel, "The Critical Requirements for Secondary School Business Teachers Based upon an Analysis of Critical Incidents" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1957). -29- obtain a representative sample of actual incidents of air crew behavior for the purpose of identifying critical re- quirements for Air Force Officers in World War II. Since then it has frequently been employed to analyze a particu- lar job by identifying its critical requirements. A critical requirement is: A requirement which is crucial in the sense that it has been reSponsible for outstandingly effect- ive or definitely unsatisfactory performance of an important part of a job or activity in question. Adaptations have been made in the methodology by several investigators since its inception. In recent correspondence, Dr. Flanagan provided a 1963 bibliography of reports of over 200 research applications of the technique.2 Applicatipns of the Critical Incident Technique Flanagan3 reported nine generalized research appli- cations of the method: criteria measures of performance; proficiency measures; training requirements and evaluation; 1John C. Flanagan, "Critical Requirements: A New Approach to Employee Evaluation," Personnel Psychology, Vol. 2 (1949), p. 420. 2Personal correspondence with the author. June 5, 1967. 3Flanagan, "The Critical Incident Technique," Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 51 (1954), pp. 327-358. -30- Many of the early applications of the technique were related to armed services personnel. However, the technique has been used for such divergent purposes as identifying ethical standards of psychologists; determining the critical requirements for private secretaries;1 developing projective tests for dentists; and testing a motivational theory of worker satisfaction on the job.2 Numerous researchers have utilized the critical incident technique in studies related to the educational enterprise. Jensen3 identified some critical requirements for teachers and recommended these requirements as a basis for teacher evaluation and as a guide for in-service growth. Priorities of training requirements for a group Of naval officers were studied by Glickman.4 He also conducted an evaluation of the naval officer candidate curriculum in relation to critical officer behaviors. 1Eugene J. Kosy, "Critical Requirements for Private Secretaries Based upon an Analysis of Critical Incidents" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1959). 2For instance, see: Clegg, Op, cit., pp. 141- 148; Herzberg, Op, cit., pp. 71-79; and Herzberg, et al., Op. cit., pp. 107-120. 3Jensen, Op. cit. 4Glickman & Vallance, Op. cit. -31- Training needs for the staff of a mental institution were identified in a study by Fleming.l Steiner2 used the CIT as a method for teaching and for evaluating the performance of attendants in an institutional setting. Fivars and Gosnell3 develOped objectives for pre-service nursing train- ing programs on the basis of critical incidents collected in the environments where nurses are employed. In an effort to determine what constitutes "good teaching," some authors4 have identified the components of effective and ineffective teaching behavior. Similarly, the components of effective and ineffective administrative personnel selection and classification; job design; Opera- tional procedures; equipment design; motivational and leadership studies; and counseling. 1Jack W. Fleming, "The Critical Incident Technique as an Aid to Inservice Training," American Journal of Mental Deficiency, Vol. 67 (1962), pp. 41-52. 2Kelley E. Steiner and Irene L. Cochran, "Simulated Critical Incident Technique as an Evaluative and Teaching Device," American Journal of Mental Deficiency, Vol. 70 (1966), pp. 835-839). 3Fivars and Gosnell, Op. cit., p. 59. See: Lane B. Blank, "Critical Incidents in the Behavior of Secondary School Physical Education Instructors," The Research Quarterly, Vol. 29 (1958). pp. 1-6; Melvin Goldin, 1rRehaviors Related to Effective Teaching," (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1957); and Kessel, op. cit. -32- behavior by school principals were identified by Cooper.1 The CIT was employed by Hedlund2 in developing a predict- ive device for estimating teaching success of secondary education undergraduates in New York. A standard of performance for nurses was develOped through this tech- nique.3 In the massive study of teacher characteristics by Ryans4, this technique was used to establish the criteria of relevant teacher behavior. Two important criticisms are leveled at the critical incident technique. The first is that each reporter of incidents is basing his report upon his own value judgment.5 1Bernice Cooper, "Analysis of the Quality of the Behaviors of Principals as Observed and Reported in Six Critical Incident Studies," Journal of Educational Research, Vol. 56 (1963), pp. 410-414. 2Paul A. Hedlund, "COOperative Study to Predict Effectiveness in Secondary School Teaching," Journal of Teacher Education, Vol. 4, (1953). pp. 230-234. 3Fivars and Gosnell,op. cit. 4David G. Ryans, Characteristics of Teachers (Wash- ington, D. C.: American Council on Education,il960). 5Hobert W. Burns, "Success Criteria and the Critical Incident Technique," Phi Delta Kappan, Vol. 38 (1956), pp. 73-75. -33- The second relates to the subjective nature of the induc— tive classification of incidents on an a posterori basis,l Dr. Flanagan speaks to both of these criticisms and reflects his appraisal of the potential for the method: ...the critical incident technique, rather than collecting opinions, hunches, and estimates, Obtains a record of specific behaviors from those in the best position to make the necessary observations and evaluations. The collection and tabulations Of these observations make it possible to formulate the critical requirements of an activity. A list of critical behaviors provides a sound basis for making inferences as to requirements in terms of aptitudes, train- ing, and other characteristics. It is believed that progress has been made in the develOpment of procedures for determining activity 5equire- ments with Objectivity and precision... Several assumptions are made when using the CIT. Among the major ones are: l. Extremities of effective and ineffective be- havior can be more accurately identified than those which fall between such extremities.3 1For instance see: John E. Corbally, Jr., "The Critical Incident Technique and Educational Research," Educational Research Bulletin, Vol. 35 (1956), pp. 57-62; "Reply to Hobert W.§urns," Phi Delta Kappan, Vol. 38 (1957), pp. 141-142; and "A Study of the Critical Elements of School Board-Community Relations" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, 1955). 2Flanagan, "The Critical Incident Technique," pp. Cit.' p. 355. 3Corbally, "The Critical Incident Technique and Educational Research," Op. cit., p. 57-60. -34- One must not assume that the middle behaviors . . l are inconsequential. Respondents can report incidents in which results are very clearly recognizable in terms of the goals of the activity. There is a common core of behavior in the performance of a given job or task.2 The CIT is not a set of rigid rules, but instead consists of a flexible set of principles for collecting behavioral data. 3 Therefore, other investigators have used slightly differing adaptations of the method. The steps which follow represent an effort to eclectically incorporate the essentials of the method from reports of several investigators. 1. The specification of the job or activipy to 5e studied. Persons reporting incidents must be clearly ‘instructed as to the job, task, or activity being studied. Time limitations must also be indicated. 1 2 Ibid., p. 58. This point is discussed by both Hedlund, op. cit. and Ryans, Op. cit. 3Flanagan, "The Critical Incident Technique," pp. cit. -35- 2. Determination of the observer group. Flanagan's original concept of this technique called primarily for Observers who recorded the critical behaviors of the subjects under study with only occasional self-reported in- cidents by the subjects.1 Since the mid- fifties, a number of researchers have relied exclusively upon self-reported incidents.2 3. The specification of criteria for observer use in reporting critical incidents. These include the following: a. A description of the situation regarding: (1) Time of occurrence (2) Persons involved (3) Circumstances leading to the incident b. A description of the behavior which relates exactly what was done. c. A statement of why the results of the inci- dent were effective or ineffective. lIbid. 2For example, each of the following has successfully employed self-reported incidents: Frank D. Alexander, "Pre- test of the Critical Incident Technique," unpublished paper, Cooperative Extension Service, Cornell University, circa, 1965; Clegg, Op. cit.; Loren F. Goyen, "Critical Components Of the Work Environment of County Extension Youth Agents" (unpublished Ph.D. study in progress, University of Wiscon- sin); Herzberg, et al., 0 . cit.; Herzberg, Op. cit.; and Fred E. Kohl, "Critical Requirements for Idaho Extension Agricultural Agents" (unpublished Ph.D. dissertation in progress, University of Wisconsin). -36- 4. Selection of a method for collecting represent- ative incidents. Three methods of securing critical incidents have generally been used: individual interviews, group interviews, and mailed questionnaires. Incidents are usually oral or written descrip- tions of behavior in narrative form. The reputed communication advantage of the indivi- dual interview is offset in part by the expense and time incurred in securing the data which results in a decline in the number of subjects that can practically be contacted.1 Some re- searchers have resorted to mailed questionnaires as a means for economically increasing sample size. However, there have been instances of very low rates of instrument returns.2 Some incidents have been discarded due to misunderstood instruc- tions to subjects. Wagner has reported distinct time savings for the group method without loss lAlexander, Op. cit. 2Low returns were reported by Bengt-E Andersson and Stig-G Nilsson, "Studies in the Reliability and Validity of the Critical Incident Technique," Journal of Applied Ps cholo , Vol. 48 (1964), pp. 398-403; Corballfi,"A Study of the Critical Elements of School Board-Community Relations,‘ op. cit.; and Goldin, Op. cit. -37- of quality of reported incidents.1 When inci- dents are collected in small groups with the researcher present, questions and answers help reduce the likelihood of reporting unusable incidents. 5. Establishing the minimum number of incidents required. A study in Sweden showed the first 320 critical incidents for retail store managers provided 100 percent of the categories, and 95.3 per- cent of the subcategories identified from a total of 1,847 incidents.2 Jensen reported that the first 400 incidents for U. S. school teachers revealed nearly all groups of behaviors found in 1,500 incidents.3 It therefore appears that a minimum of 320 incidents is required for a representative sample of job behaviors for professional workers. lRalph Wagner, "A Group Situation Compared with In- dividual Interviews for Securing Personnel Information," Personnel, Vol. 1 (Spring, 1948), pp. 93-107. 2Andersson and Nilsson, Op. cit. 3Jensen, op. cit. -38- The acceptance or rejection of incidents on the basis of established criteria. Actual behavior must be reported and the cri- teria specified in Item 3, above, must be met. Thepperformance of a content analysis upon accepted’incidents. a. Selecting a frame of reference: The intended use of the data determines the criteria for this step. For use in personnel selection, the criteria may be psychological traits. Where training uses are Of prime concern, criteria may relate to training courses or goals. b. Formulation of categories of accepted incidents: This has apparently been universally accom- plished by an inductive process whereby definitions for each category are develOped from the collected data. A panel of judges is commonly used for maintaining objectivity and as a check for reliability. Interpretation of data and reporting results. Data are generally interpreted by describing the categories and/or subcategories of incidents. The frequency of incident occurrence and relationships among variables are often reported. -39- Attributes of the Critical Incident Technique Several reputed attributes of this technique tend to support the conclusion that it is an appropriate method for the identification of behaviors needed for effective role performance.1 It will permit the reporting of be- haviors by the subjects actually engaged in the task. Their perceptions of both the job and the work situation are considered. It will afford insight into both the ineffective and effective behaviors executed in perform- ing elements of workers' roles. This method makes possible the identification of job behaviors which respondents per- ceive as critical. Summary The determinants of agent training needs may reside in a number of sources: A hierarchy of basic human needs suggested by Maslow A maintenance-Motivational theory of work proposed by Herzberg A theory of role taking in organizations offered by Katz and Kahn Such theoretical considerations provide a framework for generating hypotheses about agent training needs. 1See discussions by: Burns, Op. cit.,; Corbally, "The Critical Incident Technique and Educational Research," 0 . cit.; Flanagan, "The Critical Incident Technique," pp. git.; and Fleming, op. cit. -40- The results of previous studies of extension agent training needs suggest there may be six categories of needs which persist for extension agents: 1. Planning and develOping extension programs 2. Implementing programs through social systems 3. Communicating and using the educational process 4. Conducting programs for developing human, eco- nomic, and other natural resources 5. Evaluating programs, conducting research, and solving problems 6. Administering extension work. Related research suggests that the method employed to identify training needs mlgpp: limit possible subject response; fail to measure perception of the incumbent; focus upon methods of meeting training needs rather than identifying them; treat perception as a static rather than dynamic quality; and fail to identify important needs. It appears the critical incident technique offers a method for surmounting, at least to a high degree, each of these problems. The CIT seems an apprOpriate method for identifying training needs and for specifying job requirements of extension agents as perceived by incumbents. The eight- step procedure generally followed by those employing the CITLis-as follows: -41- Specify the job or activity to be studied Determine the composition of the observer group Specify the criteria for observer use in re— porting critical incidents Select a method for collecting representative incidents Establish the minimum number of incidents required Accept or reject incidents on the basis of established criteria Perform a content analysis of accepted incidents Interpret the data and report the results. CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY Introduction The methods employed to determine critical job requirements and to identify training needs of field extension agents are described in this chapter. Four major sections are developed. The first presents demo- _graphic characteristics of participating agents. The next describes the instruments employed and the pretest- ing procedure followed. Another describes the data collection procedures and finally, data analysis is dis- cussed and research hypotheses are presented. The subjects On the basis of findings of Andersson and Nilsson and of Jenson, it appeared that approximately 70 subjects would be needed to provide the minimum number of critical incidents required for this study. Since it was unneces- sary and economically unfeasible to include all Michigan agents, it was necessary to decide whether to use a ran- dom sample of all agents, or to select a pOpulation of agents with contiguous years of service. The latter de- cision was made since it appeared to minimize the probable age and tenure differences among respondents. -42- -43~ Both experienced and inexperienced agents' percep- tions of job requirements and training needs were desired. However, the first three months of agent work seemed largely devoted to induction experiences and therefore was judged to be prerequisite for enabling agents to report critical incidents. Agents thus were selected as sub- jects in chronological order of their tenure beginning with those employed three months prior to launching the investi- gation and extending back over whole years until more than 70 had been selected. This resulted in 74 subjects who comprised the total population of Michigan COOperative Extension Agents who had experienced not more than six years, nor less than three months of duty and were employed in a field agent position on December 1, 1967. A dichotomy was arbitrarily established according to agent tenure. Agents with less than 18 months of exten- sion employment were considered to be inexperienced since such a period seemed a reasonable requirement for agents to grasp important role expectations. Agents with 18 or more months of extension employment were considered to be experienced since all would have had an Opportunity to plan an extension program and at least begin to execute the plan. Agents were asked to report such personal demogra- phic characteristics as age, sex, employment position, tenure, previous work experience, and educational level -44- since such variables might be differentially related to training needs. About two—thirds of the agents comprising the study pOpulation were males and one-third were females. As indi- cated in Table l, the 25 Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents were males and the 22 Extension Home Economists were females. Among the 27 4-H - Youth Agents, 24 were males and three were females. Approximately 90 percent of the 4-H — Youth Agents were below 36 years of age and 70 percent of them fell within the 26 through 35 year range. Extension Agricul- tural Agents were older, with 80 percent of them 31 years or Older; and 40 percent of them 36 years or older. The distribution of Extension Home Economists' ages formed a dichotomy with 27 percent in the youngest (21-25) age group and 54 percent in the older (over 40) age group. This might reflect a typical pattern of the young home economist employed until the child bearing years, leaving employment to bear children, and then returning to profes- sional employment in later life. Fifty percent of the agents were classified as ex- perienced and fifty percent were classified as inexperi- enced agents as previously explained. Table 3 shows that about three-fourths of the 4-H - Youth Agents were -45- oo.ooa mm oo.oa v oo.¢m m oo.ov OH oo.om m «o o w z mucmmd mousommm amuspwz a HMHOOHOOHHmd oo.ooa mm me.vm m mo.m m mm.v H mm.v a hm.hm w w z mumflfiocoom mfiom coamcmuxm oo.ooa hm HmDOB on.m H ow Hm>0 av.h N ovlmm wo.hm OH mmnam mm.mm m omnmm mm.ma m mNIHm w z mucmmfi "mumww >3 nusow I mav msoum mom ZOHBHmOm BZMZNquzm OZ¢ m0¢ Mm mfizmozommmm m0 ZOHBDmHmBmHQ oo.ooa as sm.mm ea Hm.ma OH mm.mm Hm am.om ma em.va as w 2 dance . 00.00H as ms.mm mm mm.oo as w z sauce oo.ooa mm uuumul ml oo.ooa mm w z mucmmfi mousommm amusumz w HMHDDHDOHHmd N mqm¢9 oo.ooa mm oo.ooa mm o o m z mumfleocoom mfiom coflmcmuxm 00.00H hm Hmuoe HH.HH m mHmEmm mm.mm em mam: m 2 ”Mom mvcmm¢ QUDOM I mlv ZOHBHmOm BZMZNOAQZM QZ¢ xmm Mm mBZmnzommmm m0 ZOHBDmHMBmHQ H mamma -46- .mzucoa m swap mmwa pmmoamfim comb pm: mcoz .mnpcOE ma cmcu mmma How sofluflmom coemcwuxm cw pawn was 0:3 mmozu mumz mucmmm pmocmwummxmcHn .mummhm swap OMOE pmmoamEm some pm: 0:02 .Ouoe Ho mausoe ma How cofluflmom coemcwuxm cm pawn pm: 0:3 mmonu wumz mucmmm UmocmflHmmxmm bbhbbm. mm. mmhmmw mm. mmhmmw. mm bbhbbm. em amsoe oo.om km oo.mm a ms.ms OH no.4k om nemocmaumaxmcH oo.om Am oo.~s ma mm.vm NH mm.m~ A memocmaumaxm m z w z w i z m z Hmuoa ucmm< muwwEocoom mucmmd "muscme OOHOOmOm amusumz mfiom zusow I mlv w amusuHsOflumd GOflmcmpxm ZOHBHmOm Q24 mmDZMB Nm mBZMQZOmmmm m0 ZOHBDmHmBmHQ m mqmdfi .Hms©H>HU:H mac ma pmuuommu Ob mmE mwocmHummxm mOOH>mHm Hmum>mm moch mucmpsommmu mo MOQEOO may swap kummum 85m HHHB mCEOHoum -47- HH.m m oo.mH m mo.m N on.m H msoz Hm.OH m oo.om m mo.m m on.m H nonmmmmm ms.mm em oo.om a ma.ma a ms.os Ha suumsecH one mmmchsm m>.mm mm oo.mv NH we.MH m mm.mm s OOH>umm OHHbOm Hm.mm he oo.ss Ha mm.am ma sm.me ma maaromme m .62 w mz. w mz w mz Hmuoe ucvm< mumHEocoom mosmHummxm OOHDOmmm Hmusumz wEom mucmmm xuoz w HmusuHsOHumd GOHmsOuxm nusow I mlv msoH>mHm ZOHBHmOm Bzmzwoqmzm 02¢ MUZMHmmmx-m MMOK mDOH>mEnH .HZm-QZOnHmm-m v MHm/wm. inexperienced, while over 70 percent of the Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents were experienced. The Home Economists were almost equally divided; about 55 percent were classed as experienced and about 45 percent were inexperienced. Teaching was the most frequently reported previous work experience for agents in this study. A considerably larger proportion of Extension Home Economists and 4-H — Youth Agents had teaching experiences before extension employment than did Extension Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. The second most frequent work eXperience was in business and industry. Public service (other than teaching) ranked a close third and was the most frequently reported experience of Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. Public service was considered to include employment with such agencies as Social Welfare Departments, the U. S. Forest Service, Michigan Department of Conservation, U. S. Soil Conservation Service, Farmers Home Administration, and others. Research experience was reported by about 10 percent of the agents. Extension Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents more frequently reported research experience than either 4-H - Youth Agents or Extension Home Economists. -49- About eight percent of all agents had no previous work ex- perience before extension employment. The educational backgrounds of the agents are re- ported in Table 5. Approximately four-fifths of them had academic credits beyond their Bachelor's degree. About one-half had completed their Master's degree or had worked beyond it. Such accomplishments were more frequently reported by Extension Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents than by either 4-H - Youth Agents or Extension Home Economists. Incumbents to the latter two positions most frequently reported having a Bachelor's degree plus some credits, but not enough for a Master's degree. TABLE 5 RESPONDENT LEVEL OF ACADEMIC ATTAINMENT BY EMPLOYMENT POSITION Agricultural Extension and Natural Academic 4-H - Youth Home Resource Attainment Agent Economist Agent Total % N % N % N % Bachelor's 5 18.52 7 31.82 4 16.00 16 21.62 Bachelor's Plus 15 55.56 8 36.36 5 20.00 28 37.84 Master's 4 14.81 3 13.64 6 24.00 13 17.57 Master's Plus 3 11.11 4 18.18 8 32.00 15 20.27 Doctor's _Q 0 _Q 0 _2 8.00 2 2.70 * Total 27 100.00 22 100.00 25 100.00 74 100.00 -50- Instrumentation Two principal instruments, plus associated instruct- ions, were develOped for this investigation: A Critical Incident Report Form, attached in Appendix A; and a Classi- fication System for Critical Incidents, Appendix B. The Critical Incident Report Form was developed for use of respondents in self-reporting incidents. This form was structured to assure that reported incidents would meet the criteria for acceptance noted in Chapter II. The content of the questions was designed to meet these cri- teria. The order Of questions about each incident was adapted from that followed in oral interviews by Alexander.1 The questions were: . Exactly what did you do? What circumstances led up to the incident? What was your objective? Who was involved? What were the results of your action? When did this incident take place? O‘U'llfiwNi-J e e This research is concerned with the significance of agent training needs and job requirements. One indication of the significance of such needs and requirements is the frequency with which agents report them. The Critical Incident Report Form was designed to collect information about critical incidents so they could be classified into lAlexander, op. cit., pp. 1-5. -51- common groups of agent needs or job requirements. The frequency with which incidents occurred would be one indi- cation of the significance of such needs or requirements. That is, large numbers of incidents in a given group would tend to indicate more significant activities; small num- bers Of incidents would tend to indicate less significant activities. However, frequency alone seemed not to provide a complete picture since it did not fully indicate the degree of criticalness of a need.1 To illustrate, a commercial fisherman might seldom need to swim, but if the need arose but once it would be highly critical. Therefore, an indi- cation of the importance of a training need seemed to be a considerable refinement over the frequency of occurrence alone. Respondents were asked to indicate on a seven-point scale the extent to which they believed each self-reported incident was likely to influence their success as an exten- sion agent. It was also desired to determine if agents differen- tially reported the difficulpy experienced in executing critical incidents. Respondents were asked to indicate on 1Interpretation of incident frequency is discussed by: Corbally, "The Critical Incident Technique and Edu- cational Research," 0 . cit.; Glickman and Vallance, pp. plp.; Flanagan, "Critical Requirements: A New Approach to Employee Evaluation," Personnel Psychology, Vol. 2 (1949), pp. 419-425; and Andersson and Nilsson, Op. cit. -52- a seven-point scale how difficult it was to do each self- reported incident. The questionnaire was constructed so respondents could provide descriptions of three effective and three ineffective incidents, for a total of six incidents. A separate page was used for reporting each incident with the pages alternating between effective and ineffective incidents. Effective incidents were considered to indicate competencies or skills possessed by the reporting agents. Ineffective incidents were considered to indicate training needs of the reporting agents. The combination of all ef- fective and ineffective incidents, when classified, was considered to reflect agent job requirements. The potential for maximizing the number of usable reported incidents seemed greatest with small group meet- ings of respondents. Since the respondent's understanding of the instructions was deemed to be crucial to the validity of the research data, incident descriptions, special effort was made to insure adequate and uniform communication of instructions. A full page of instructions, including ex— amples of incidents from other professions, preceeded the critical incident report form. To provide audio reinforce- ment to the visually presented instruction, this page was tape recorded and the four-minute recording was played in -53- each group meeting. The general information was narrated on the recording by the researcher, but the sample incidents were each narrated by a different voice. During data col- lection meetings, the subjects were encouraged to both read and listen to the instructions. Respondents were asked to provide the following per- sonal demographic information: Name Employment experience Sex Educational level attained Age Extension tenure A classification System for Critical Incidents A classification system was required to meaningfully interpret the critical incidents collected in this research and relate them to the variables under study. Other re- searchers using the CIT have apparently, without exception, classified the collected incidents a posteriori. Typically the incidents were collected and the classification system was then inductively developed from the collected data. In the present study a classification system was develOped a priori. It was noted in the review of extension literature that there appeared to be six functional groups of agent job behaviors which persisted among the writings of several authors in the field. These six groups of agent behaviors served as the major categories of the classification -54- system used in this study. These categories are herein referred to as critical performance categories (CPC's). Hypotheses were generated about the CPC's and about the relationship of other variables to the CPC's. Component subcategories were assigned to each category. The subcategories were generalized statements of agent behaviors. They were primarily derived from over 200 items used by earlier cited extension researchers.l m mcHacmHm Ewumoum mCHHmuchHEp4 mfimumoum msHuOOUOOU OOHNHGmmuO msHOMOHssEEOU was msHsommB whommumu mesmEHOmumm HMOHOHHU moz¢2m0mmmm HdUHBHmU w UZOZfi mBZmQHUZH HflUHBHMU m0 ZOHBDmHMBmHD 0H mHmde -86- success with about equal frequency. That frequency, how— ever, was so uniformly low as to rank performance in this category fourth in a ranked list of six CPC's by incumbents in each position. The distribution of incidents in the Program Planning category was less consistent. Incidents reported by both 4-H Agents and Home Economists were more frequently clas- sified here than were Agricultural and Natural Resource Agent reported incidents. While numbers involved are small, it does suggest that 4-H Agents and Home Economists may more frequently perceive program planning and develOpment beha- viors as critical job requirements. Critical incidents reported by Agricultural and Natu- ral Resource Agents were more frequently classified in the Evaluating category than were incidents from other agents. Apparently these agents more frequently perceive evaluation and research behaviors as critical job requirements. The distribution of critical incidents among CPC's according to the tenure of the reporting agent is shown in Table 17. Experienced agents reported 70 incidents which were classified as Teaching and Communicating behaviors. These represented 32.0 percent of the classified incidents reported by experienced agents. Eighty, or 36.5 percent of the incidents reported by inexperienced agents were classi- fied in the Teaching and Communicating category. This -37- category was therefore ranked first by frequency of inci- dents for both experienced and inexperienced agents. How- ever, it will be noted that a greater prOportion of the incidents reported by inexperienced agents were classified in this category. Both experienced and inexperienced agents were con- sistent in the number and proportion of incidents classified in the categories of Organizing, Conducting Programs, and Program Planning. The difference in prOportions for each Of these categories was less than 1 percent. Only a slight difference occurred in the Administering category. Hypothesis sz is supported from the data in Table 17 for experienced agents, but not supported in all cases for inexperienced agents. Only one category, Evaluating, failed to be affirmed as a CPC for inexperienced agents. This was a CPC for experienced agents. Incidents classified in this category were 1.8 percent and 6.8 percent respectively Of those reported. Apparently inexperienced agents much less frequently perceived evaluation and research behaviors as being critical job requirements. These data supported Hypothesis sz in all except the Evaluating category. -88- 00.00H ems m sm.s an m os.e km s so.HH Hm m so.aa mm m sa.mm moa H mm.sm omH scam w z Hmuoe oo.QOH mHm m mw.H v m em.m MH e m>.mH mm m mH.mH me m e>.mm mm H mm.mm om mmmmI m z pmocmHHmmwmcH oo.OOH mHm m mm.m mH o mm.m «H v om.OH mm m mo.om we m om.vm mm H om.Hm on xcmm w z pmocmHnomwm BZMOd GZHBMOmmm m0 mszmB Mm mMHmowmfido Hmuoe msHumon>m OGHCCMHm Emumoum mcHuOuchHEpfl mfimumoum OCHDOOOCOU mcHNHsmmuo OGHOMOHCOEEOO paw mcHnommB whommpsu mosmfiuomnmm HMOHOHHU mUZHEmOmmmm HHUHBHmU w 0202¢ mBZmQHUZH HdUHBHmU m0 ZOHBDmHmBmHQ NH MHm¢B -89- Hierarchies of CPC's for Incident Frequency, Importance, and Difficulty Hypothesis H3 postulated a hierarchy of critical per- formance categories according to incident frequency, impor- tance and difficulty. Specifically it stated: H3 The distribution among critical performance cate- gories of critical incidents reported by extension agents are differentially arranged in rank-order according to: a. The frequency of reported effective incidents b. The frequency of reported ineffective incidents c. The frequency of all incidents d. The mean score of importance of all incidents e. The mean score of difficulty of all incidents Table 18 shows the frequency distribution among six CPC's of effective and ineffective incidents, and the totals of these incidents. The Teaching and Communicating category contained the greatest number of effective incidents, Of ineffective incidents, and hence of total incidents. The same rank-order occured for the six CPC's regardless of in- cident effectiveness. While percentages varied, no variance was sufficient to alter the rank-order of categories. For instance, nearly 8 percent more ineffective than effective incidents were classified as Teaching and Communi- cating behaviors. Conversely, more effective than ineffective incidents were classified in the categories of Organizing and -9o- o.QOH mmv o m.v mH m N.© SN v m.HH Hm m m.mH mm N o.vm mOH H m.vm omH scam w z Hmnoe c.00H omm o.OOH mHm e m.s OH e H.e m m m.m mH m 4.0 4H s m.mH em a m.s AH m a.mH em m m.mm om m m.mH ms m m.mm mm H m.mm am H m.om we scam w z xcmm w z m>HuommmmcH m>Hnomeem musmpHocH HmoHuHHU Hence mcHumon>m mcHGGme Emnmoum mcHHmumHsHfip< msmumonm mcHuospcou mcHNHsmmHO mcHHMOHsDEEOU pom mcHsommB wuommumo mosmEHOmHmm HmoHuHHU mBzmaHUZH HGUHBHmU W>HfiummmmZH 02¢ m>H90mmmm DMBmOmmm BZMU< mO.mmHmOUMB¢U m02<2fi0mmmm Hm msHocmHm Emumonm msHHmumHsHEp¢ mfimumoum msHuospsou msHNHsmmHO msHHMOHsdEEOU one mstommB anommumo mocmEHOwnmm HsOHuHHU mmHmOOWBHU moz¢2m0mmmm Hm a No.4 mH H mm.s 4H schcmHm summons G mm.s mm m mm.s mm maHnmuchHsea m om.e ms N Hm.s as msmumonm msHuOOUOOU H NH.m mm s Hm.s mm mcHNHcmmuo m ms.s om m ke.s on mcHumoHcsssoo paw mcHnommB umpuo muoom musmp HOUHO mHOom mucmp >uomwumu Ixcmm new: IHOQH Ixcmm cmmz IHOGH mosmfinomumm mosmunomEH mo .02 mocmuuomEH mo .02 HmOHuHHU awesome pmosmHnmmxmcH "musmmfl poocwHHmmxm Bzmwd UZHBmommm m0 mmbzma Mm mMHmOUmedo moz<2m0mmmm HdUHBHMU UZOSd QmHmHmmdqu mBZHQHUZH HdUHBHmU mom mUZHBmOQZH ho mmmOUm de2 m0 ZOHBSmHmBmHQ mm NHmdB -102- The similarity of the mean scores of importance for all incidents can be seen in Table 23. This contrasts sharply with the differences noted in Table 22 for mean difficulty scores. There was more variability among the means Of importance scores for inexperienced than for experi- enced agents. Mean importance scores ranged from 4.47 to 4.93 for experienced agents while the range was from 4.29 to 5.12 for inexperienced agents. This lends support to the postulate that with extension work experience, agents become more homogeneous in their perception of appropriate job behavior. Table 23 shows that importance scores for the Teach- ing and Communicating category resulted in a fifth-place rank of importance for both experienced and inexperienced agents. The category Of Conducting Programs was almost identically _ perceived by both groups. Incidents in the Organizing category were however perceived as more important by inexperienced than by experienced agents. The derived ranks for the Organizing category were first and fourth respectively for these two groups of agents. So few incidents reported by inexperienced agents fell into the Evaluating category that little can be inter- preted from the importance scores. These incidents were rated as the least important by experienced agents. -103- Agent Tenure and Incident Effectiveness It was postulated that agents would differentially report incident effectiveness according to agent tenure. Specifically it was hypothesized: H6 The frequency of report of effective and of inef- festive incidents varies among critical performance categories according to the tenure of the agents reporting: a. Experienced agents more frequently than inexperienced agents report effective in- cidents which are classified in the Program Planning category. b. Experienced agents more frequently than inexperienced agents report ineffective incidents which are classified in the Administering category. The frequency distribution of effective incidents among CPC's by tenure of reporting agents is shown in Table 24. The Program Planning category contained more effective incidents for experienced than for inexperienced agents, although the difference was less than 2 percent. This difference is in the direction postulated, thus supporting Hypothesis H6a. Table 24 reveals an amazing similarity of the frequ- ency of reporting effective incidents by CPC. The greatest difference for experienced and inexperienced agents was found in the Evaluating category, but it did not reach five percent. This suggests that agents of different tenures do not differentially perceive effective incidents. -104- Next, the hypothesis of possible differences of the frequency of reporting ineffective incidents by tenure of reporting agent was tested. Table 25 shows the distribution of ineffective incidents by CPC for experienced and for inexperienced agents. The Administering category contained more ineffective incidents for inexperienced than for ex- perienced agents. This is contrary to the direction postu- lated. These data failed to support Hypothesis H6b. About 9 percent more ineffective incidents were clas- sified in the Teaching and Communicating category for inex- perienced than for experienced agents. The Evaluation category contained 5.45 percent more ineffective incidents for experienced agents. A similar situation existed in the Organizing category which contained 4.55 percent more inef- fective incidents for experienced agents. These results suggest possible differentiation of training needs in these 3 categories based upon agent tenure. A ent Tenure, Incident Difficulty and Effectiveness In addition to testing the above hypotheses, these data were analyzed to seek answers to several questions. The first was: Q1 What is the relationship of agent tenure to the difficulty of executing effective and ineffective incidents? -105- oo.OOH bmw mm.e OH Hm.m mH mv.mH em mm.mH om mm.mH me mH.mm em unmoumm HOQEOZ ”mucmpHOsH usmmfi HHH oo.o0H bHI mm.H m mm.m b hm.hH mH mm.vH mH hm.hH mH m>.me we unmoumm HmbEdz "mucmpHosH ucmmfi pmocmHHmmxmsH oo.OOH bl! hm.h m mv.m o vo.mH mH mH.mH om Nm.HN em vo.mm hm unmoumm Hmnfisz "mpsmpHocH ucmmd pmocmHHmmxm Hmuoe mcH mcHquHm>m chHm Esumoum mcHHmpmHsHEp4 mamumonm msHuODOGOO msHuHcmmHO msHHMOHCOEEOU pew OCHnomme muommumu mocmEHOmumm HMOHHHHU Bzmwd OZHBmOmmm m0 mmbsz Mm mBZMDHUZH m>HButhMZH m0 mmHmOOmBHU m02<2m0mmmm HdUHBHmU 02024 ZOHBDmHmBmHQ wuszOmmm oo.ooH Mil MH.v m Nv.m vH om.n SH mm.mm om ve.mm mm mm.om mo unmoumm Hmnfisz "mucmpHoaH ucmmfi HHd Bzmwd UZHBmOmmm m0 mmDZMB wm mBZmaHUZH mm mqmm scmHm Emumonm osHHmuchHfipm mamnmoum maHuOOpcou OOHNHGMOHO msHHMOHCOEEOU was manommB whomwumu mocmauowumm HmOHuHHO m>HBUmmmm m0 mmHmOUmfiflu MUZHZmommmm HfiUHBHmU UZOZ< ZOHBDmHmBmHQ wuzmbommm vN MHMHB -106- The mean scores of difficulty for effective incidents are reported in Table 26. In general, experienced agents reported higher difficulty scores for effective incidents than inexperienced agents reported. The means of difficulty scores for all effective incidents were 3.43 and 3.15 respectively for the two groups of agents. The range of mean difficulty scores for inexperienced agents was from 2.75 to 4.00; scores for inexperienced agents ranged from 2.33 to 3.58. Evaluating was the most difficult category for ex- perienced agents; for inexperienced agents, Teaching and Communicating was the most difficult. Experienced agents apparently perceived Teaching and Communicating incidents as less difficult since the derived ranking for this cate- gory was fifth place. The Conducting Programs category was nearly a full point, on a seven-point scale, more difficult for experi- enced than for inexperienced agents. Similarly, the mean difficulty score in the Administering category was more than a full point greater for the experienced agents. With the exception Of the Evaluation category, there was little variability in the frequency of reporting effective incidents. These data suggest that incidents do not vary signi- ficantly in the frequency with which they are reported as effective according to agent tenure. However, it appears -107- mH.m mOH mv.m mOH Hmuoa oo.m m oo.e b DGHumsHm>m se.m e mk.m m mchamHm anemone mm.m a om.m m mcHnmumHaHsea om.m em ms.m em mamumonm mCHuOOUCOO mm.m mm mm.m mm mcHNHcmmno mm.m mm mm.m mm mcHumoHasssoo pom mcHsommB Hmpuo muoom mucmp Hmpuo muoom mucmp whommuwu Ixcmm cmmz IHOaH Ixcmm cams IHOOH mosmfiuomumm ansoHeeHa no .02 HstoHeeHo Ho .02 HmoHuHuo "mucmm¢ poocmHummxmsH ”mucmmd pmosmHummxm BZMUH UZHBmOmmm m0 mmDZMB wm mBZmaHUZH M>H80mmhm mom mmHmowmfiflu moz<2mommmm HdUHBHmU UZOZd MEHDUHMMHQ m0 mmmOUm z¢m2 m0 ZOHBDmHmBmHQ mm MHmm «H.w n pH.e w mcHQGMHm Emumomm s mm.m aH H mm.s mH mcHumumHaHsea m mo.m SH m me.m om msmnmoua msHuUspcou m eH.s mH m mH.s am mcHNHammuo m HH.m as e as.m km maHanHcsssoo poo manommB Hmpuo muoom mucmp Hmpuo whoom mucmp wuommpmu Ixcmm com: IHOGH Ixcmm new: IHOOH mocmEHOMHmm suHsoHeeHa mo .02 HstoHeeHo no .02 HmoHuHuo “mucmmd pwocmHummxmcH "mucmm< pmocmHHmmxm BZmO< GZHBmOmmm m0 MMDsz Mm mBzmaHUZH m>HBUmmmmzH mom mMHmOQMBHU muzflzmommmm Hm 00.0 0 00.0 0 0 00.0 0 mchamHm Emumoum 00.0 0 00.0 0 0 0H.0 0 mcHnmuchHsea 0 NH.0 0H 0 0H.m 0H 0 sH.0 0m msmumoum mcHuOSOGOU H 00.0 0H H H0.0 0H 0 00.0 00 mcHuHammno 0 00.0 00 m 00.0 00 m 00.0 0H mcHumqusesoo pas manommB umpuo muoom meson HOUHO muoom mpcmp HOUHO muoom musmp mnemmumu Ixcmm com: IHOGH Ixcmm com: IHOOH Ixsmm com: IHUCH OOGMEHOHHmm suHsoHeeHa mo .02 wuHsoHeeHa mo .02 mesoHemHo 00 .oz HmoHuHuo "musomm monsOmmm Hmusumz pom HMHOHHOOHHOH umpmHEosoom meow "mucmm< apoow I mIe Bzmo< OZHBmommm mo ZOHBHmOm Mm mezmoHqu m>HBUmmmm mom mmHmomemO muz¢zmommmm HdUHBHmO U202< MBHDUHmmHQ mo mmmoum zm 0 00.0 0 m 00.0 0 m 00.0 m mchcmHm Emumoum m 00.0 0 H 00.0 0H 0 00.0 0H mcHnmanaHsea 0 00.0 0H 0 00.0 0 0 sm.0 HH 05000000 mchOSOOOO H 00.0 0H 0 00.0 0 0 00.0 00 ocHNHcmmuo 0 00.0 00 0 00.0 00 0 00.0 00 mcHumoHasssoo paw mcHnommB HmUHO muoom musmp Hmpuo whoom musmp umpuo OHOOm macmp whommumu Ixcmm com: IHOGH :xcmm cmmz IHocH Ixsmm com: IHOCH mocmEHOMHmm HstoHeuHo no .02 HstoHeeHo mo .02 stsOHmeHo no .02 HmoHano "mucmmd OOHSOmmm Hmnsumz pcm HMHOuHOOHHm< "mumHEosoom meow ”museum Susow I mIe Bzmwfi OZHBmommm ho ZOHBHmOm wm mfiszHUZH MDHHUmWMMZH mom mmHmOwMB40 muzazmommmm HHUHBHMU 02024 MBHDUHmmHQ ho mmmOUm z¢m2 MO.ZOHBDmHmBmHQ om mHmfia -115- Economists. Difficulty scores for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents resulted in ranking the Organizing category first in difficulty, while scores for 4-H Agents and Home Economists resulted in ranking it third in difficulty. Derived difficulty rankings in sixth place were found in the Conducting Programs category for both 4-H Agents and Home Economists, but the latter agents reported lower difficulty ratings by .60 points. Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents gave higher difficulty ratings to the Conducting Programs category. The Administering cate- gory received the highest mean difficulty score from the Home Economists. This resulted in a second ranking for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents, but a fourth for 4-H - Youth Agents. The mean scores of difficulty for ineffective inci- dents reported by 4-H — Youth Agents and by Home Economists were nearly identical. Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents however, had a lower mean difficulty score. In general, Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents perceived the ineffective incidents they reported as less difficult to execute than incumbents to the other positions. A com- parison of the mean scores of difficulty in Tables 29 and 30 revealed that all agents reported higher difficulty_ scores for ineffective than for effective incidents. The difference was only slight, however, for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. -116- Difficulty scores for effective and ineffective inci— dents are combined in Table 31. Difficulty scores for Teach- ing and Communicating incidents ranged downward from 3.67 for 4-H - Youth Agents, to 3.44 for Extension Home Economists, and to 3.00 for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. However, the reverse order held for the Organizing category. For both effective and ineffective incidents, the Organizing category was consistently perceived as most difficult for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. The conducting Programs category was perceived as least difficult by Extension Home Economists. Incidents in the Administering category were perceived as about equally difficult for 4-H — Youth and Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents, with respective mean scores of 3.53 and 3.62. It was the most difficult category for Extension Home Economists. Program Planning was rated as considerable more difficult by 4-H Agents than by other in- cumbents. Home Economists perceived Program Planning inci- dents as more difficult than did Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. The numbers in the Evaluation category are small, but it will be noted this category was rated the most difficult by 4-H - Youth Agents. Others rated it about equally difficult. In general, Extension Home Economists and Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents about equally rated the difficulty -ll7- H0.0 00H 00.0 H0H 00.0 00H Hanoe m 00.0 HH 0 00.0 0 H 00.0 0 0aHumsHm>m 0 00.0 0 0 00.0 HH 0 0H.0 0H mchcmHm Emumoum 0 00.0 0H H NH.0 0H 0 00.0 0H mcHnmuchHsea 0 00.0 00 0 00.0 0H 0 Hm.0 00 mamumonm msHuOOOGOU H 00.0 00 m 00.0 00 0 H0.0 H0 mcHuHcmmno 0 00.0 00 0 00.0 00 0 00.0 00 mcHumoHasssoo was mcHsoomB Hmpuo muoom wusmp Hmpuo muoom mucmp Hmpuo mnoow musmp whommpmu Ixcmm use: IHocH Ixcmm com: IHOCH Ixsmm Com: IHocH OOCMEHOmHmm suHsoHeeHa no .02 NwH26HmeHo 00 .oz .HmHsoHemHo no .02 HmoHano "mucmmm mousommm Hmuspmz pom HMHSHHDOHHmd "mumHEOGOOm mEom "mucmmm npsow I mIv Ezmwd UZHBmOmmm m0 ZOHBHmOm Mm mBZmoHUZH HdUHBHmU HHQ mom mMHmowmfidu muz<2m0hmmm HdUHBHMU UZOSd WBHDUHmmHQ m0 mmmoum 24m: m0 ZOHBDmHMBmHQ Hm mHm49 -118- of executing all reported incidents. The means were 3.44 and 3.41 respectively. However, the 4-H - Youth Agents reported greater difficulty. They provided an overall mean difficulty rating of 3.60. Agent Tenure, Incident Importance and Effectiveness Attention was next directed toward possible dif- ferences in importance scores according to the tenure of the reporting agent and the effectiveness of the critical incident. The question was: Q3 What is the relationship of tenure to the importance of executing effective and ineffective incidents? The distribution of mean importance scores for effective incidents among critical performance categories is shown according to the tenure of the reporting agent in Table 32. There was only slight variation in the mean scores among the categories. Scores for experienced agents ranged from a high Of 5.59 to a low of 5.13, giving a range of .46 points. The range for inexperienced agents was also narrow, from a high of 6.00 to a low of 5.38. A range of only .39 occurred among the mean scores for all agents. Inexperienced agents assigned slightly higher impor- tance scores for Teaching and Communicating and for Organi- zing categories. The Conducting Programs category was the only one in which experienced agents gave a higher importance score, but this difference was only .20 points. In general, -119- Hm.m mOH wv.m mOH HMHOB 0 00.0 m 0 0H.0 0 mcHumsHm>m H 00.0 o w mH.m m mcHssmHm Ewumoum 0 00.0 0 0 0m.0 0 mcHumuchHsea 0 00.0 00 m 00.0 00 msmsmonm msHHODUGOU 0 H0.0 00 H 00.0 00 0eHNHcmmno m 0s.0 00 0 00.0 00 mcHumOHcsssoo pom mcHnommB Hmpuo whoom musmp Hmpno muoom mucmp whommumo Ixcmm com: IHosH Ixcsm new: IHOOH OOCMEHOMHOH mocmuuomEH mo .02 moswuHOmEH mo .Oz HMOHHHHO "musmmé poosmHHmmxmcH "mpsmmfi pmostummxm Bszd UZHBmOmmm m0 mmDZMB mm mBZMQHUZH m>H90mmmm mom mmHmomeGU mozHBUmmmMZH mom mmHmOGMB¢U MUZ¢BMOQEH m0 mmmOUm 24m: mm mHQmB Hmuoe mchmsHm>m osHCCMHm Emumoum msHHmuchHEU< mEmHmOHm OCHHOSOQOU mcHNHsmmHO mcHHMOHsOEEOU pcm mcHaommB anommumu mocmEHomHmm HMOHHHHU m0 ZOHEDmHmBmHQ -122- Inexperienced agents rated incidents in Evaluating, Organi- zing, and in Conducting Program categories as more important than other categories. The mean importance score for ineffective incidents by experienced agents was 4.07 and for inexperienced agents it was 3.77. Hence, experienced agents reported higher importance scores for ineffective incidents than did inex- perienced agents. The Opposite condition was reported for effective incidents in Table 32 where inexperienced agents' importance scores exceed those of experienced agents. There was a much lower importance score attached to ineffective incidents than to effective incidents. Mean importance scores for experienced agents ranged from 5.48 for effective incidents (Table 32) to 4.07 for ineffective incidents (Table 33). The range for inexperienced agents was from 5.61 to 3.77. The comparable range for experienced and inexperienced agents' importance scores was from 1.41 to 1.81 respectively. Importance scores for effective and ineffective incidents are combined in Table 34 according to agent tenure. The more important categories for experienced agents were: Program Planning, Conducting Programs, Administering, and Organizing. The more important categories for inexperienced agents were: Organizing; Conducting Programs; Program Planning; and Evaluation. Numbers of incidents in the latter -123- 00.0 0H0 00.0 0H0 Hayes 0 00.0 0 0 00.0 0H mcHumsHm>m 0 00.0 0H H 00.0 0H mcHaamHm gaseous 0 00.0 00 0 00.0 00 meHumuchHsea 0 00.0 as N H0.0 00 msmumoum msHuOOUCOU H NH.0 00 0 H0.0 00 mcHuHcmmuo 0 00.0 00 0 00.0 00 maHnmoHasssoo pew manommB Hmpuo muoom mucmp Hmpuo whoom mucmp. whommumo Ixcmm ewes IHocH Ixsmm new: IHOGH mosmfihomumm mosmuHOMEH MO .02 wosmunomEH mo .02 HsOHuHHU "musmmm UmosmHHmmxmcH "mpcmmd pmosmHnmmxm Bzmwm OZHBmommm ho mszmB Mm mBZMQHUZH HdUHBHmU HH4 mom mmHmomeQU HUZHZmommmm HdUHBHmU UZOZd MUZHBmomSH m0 mmmOUm de2 ho ZOHBDmHMBMHD vm mqmflfi -124- category were very small. The differences noted earlier between importance scores for experienced and inexperienced agents very nearly disappeared when scores were combined for effective and ineffective incidents. Table 34 shows a mean importance score Of 4.77 for all incidents reported by experienced agents. The mean score for inexperienced agents was 4.68. A striking contrast results when difficulty scores reported in Tables 26, 27, and 28 are compared with impor- tance scores in Tables 32, 33, and 34. Whether incidents were effective, ineffective, or a combination of both, agents recorded considerably higher scores for importance than for difficulty. This holds regardless of agent tenure. A ent Position, Incident Importance an Effectiveness Attention was next directed toward possible differ- ences in importance scores according to the position of the reporting agent and the effectiveness of the critical incident. The question was: Q4 What is the relationship of position to the importance of executing effective and ineffective incidents? Table 35 shows the importance scores for effective incidents according to the position of the reporting agent. In general, the mean scores of importance for all effective incidents did not extensively vary by the position of the -125- 00.0 00 00.0 00 00.0 00 Hmnoe 0H.0 0 0 0 00.0 0 mcHumsHm>m 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 mchcmHm Emumoum 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 mcHumuquHssa 0 H0.0 0H H 00.0 0H m 00.0 00 msmnmoum . mCHHODUCOO 0 00.0 0H 0 00.0 0H 0 H0.m 00 mcHNHcmmuo N 00.0 00 m 00.0 00 0 00.0 0H mcHumoHcsssoo pom mcHsommB nacho muoom musmp Hmpuo muoom mpsmp HOOHO whoom musmp whommumu .Ixcmm use: IHOCH Ixcmm new: IHOQH Ixsmm new: IHOsH mesmEHOmHmm mocmuuomEH mo .02 mocmuuomEH ,mo .oz OOCMHHOQEH m0 .Oz HmoHuHHU "mucmmd OOHOOmmm Hmususz w HmHsuHsOHHmd "mumdeocoom mfiom "massed capo» I mIv Bzmwm wzHamommm mo oneHmOm Mm mazmoHqu m>H90mmmm mom mmHmowmeHBUMWWMZH mom mmHmOUWBdU NH mm “mgnmc -HocH mo .02 vo.w oo.m om.m Nm.m mn.v vo.v mm.m -HmUHo whoom Ixcmm cams ma Ha mm mm mpcmw twocH mocmuuomEH mo.Joz "mucmm¢ susow u mr¢ Hmuoe mcfiumnam>m mcHGGMHm Emnmoum mCHHmancHEG4 mEmHmOHm mafluosccou mcfluflcmmuo mcfluMOHCSEEOU cam mcflzomma whomwumu mommEHOHHmm HMUflyflno mozmzmommmmuquHBHmu UZOZfl MUZdBmomEH m0 mmmOUm z¢m2 EOKZOHBDmHmBmHQ mm mqmfifi -128- rated ineffective incidents in the Conducting Programs category as more important than other categories. The categories of Program Planning and Evaluating were both infrequently reported and rated low in importance by 4-H - Youth Agents. Extension Home Economists on the other hand, rated ineffective incidents in the categories of Program Planning and Organizing as holding more importance than those in the Conducting Programs category. The more important categories for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents were Evaluating and Administering; the Teaching and Communicating category was ranked lowest by them. It was noted earlier that importance scores for inef- fective incidents were consistently lower than those for effective incidents, regardless of agent tenure. When com- paring Tables 35 and 36, that same difference was found, regardless of agent position. Mean Scores of importance for ineffective incidents were lower than those for effect- ive incidents by 1.62 points for 4-H - Youth Agents, 1.78 for Extension Home Economists, and 1.45 for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. Importance scores for effective and ineffective inci- dents were combined in Table 37 according to agent position. The mean score of importance for all incidents reported by 4-H - Youth Agents was slightly higher than for agents in -129- am.a FVH so.a HmH am.v mmH Hmuoe H Hm.¢ HH m aw.m m m ov.s m mcHumsHm>m m mm.¢ m m so.s HH m om.v OH mcHacmHm Emumoum m om; mH m $4 3 v :6 mH mcHHmfiEEwm a ma.s mm m mo.v mH H mH.m am mamumoum mcfluospcoo m mm.v mm H NH.m mm m mm.a Hm mcHNHcmmuo m we.« am a mm.v mm m om.v ow usumoHasssoo w mcflnomwfi Hmpno muoom mucmp Hmouo mnoom mucmw Hmcuo whoom mucmp muommumu Ixcmm cmwz IHUGH Ixcmm cow: IHUGH :xcmm com: :HUCH modMEHOMHmm mocmuuommw mo .02 mocmuuomEH mo .02 mocmuuomEH mo .02 HMUfluHHU "mucwm< mUPSOmmm amusumz paw Housuasoflnmm "mumHEocoom meow "mucmmd cusow u miv Bzmw< UZHBmommm m0 oneHmom wm mezmaHUzH aduHBHmU add mom mmHmowmaflo muz¢2mommmm Q¢UHBHmU OZOZ¢ WUZ¢BmOAZH MO mmmoom de2 .mo ZOHBDmHMBmHQ hm qudfi -l30- other positions. The Home Economists and Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents had nearly identical mean importance scores for all incidents (4.64 and 4.67, respectively). The 4-H Agents rated the importance of incidents in the Conducting Programs category much higher than did other agents. The 4—H Agents also recorded higher importance values for Administering incidents than did the Home Econo- mists. The Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents reported more Evaluating incidents, and they placed considerably higher importance scores upon those incidents. Summary Agent reported critical incidents were classified among six critical performance categories. ’Hypotheses about the frequency, importance, and difficulty of executing those categories of incidents were tested. In addition, hypothesized relationships of the critical performance categories to agent tenure and employment position were tested. A summary of the acceptance or rejection of the study hypotheses is presented in Table 38. These data were also analyzed to answer four questions about the relationship of agent tenure and agent position to the difficulty and importance of executing effective and ineffective critical incidents. A summary of the results of this analysis is presented in Table 39. -131- TABLE 38 SUMMARY OF THE ACCEPTANCE OR REJECTION OF HYPOTHESES BASED UPON AN ANALYSIS OF STUDY DATA Analysis: Hypothesis: Supported Failed to Support H a x 1b x c x d x e x f x Hza (1-4) x a (5-6) x b (1-5) x b (6) x H a x 3b x c x d x e X H a x 4b x H a x 5b x H a x 6b x -l32- hw.v wm.v vm.v mm.q hb.v HHm m>HuommmmcH m>Huommmm mm.m oh.m vo.v hh.m no.v ov.m wm.m mm.m Ho.m mv.m ”mucmpflocH HMUHuHHU mo mmuoom mocmuuomEH com: av.m vv.m om.m wm.m wo.m HHa m>HuommmmaH m>Huommmm mv.m mn.m mm.m mm.m mm.m mm.m mo.m mm.m mH.m mv.m "mucmoflocH Hmowpfiuu mo mmuoom muHSUHmmwo com: mZOHBmMDO NQDHm OB.mmZOmmmm ZH QmeAdZ< m¢.mBZmU¢ UZHBMOmmm m0 ZOHBHmom 02¢ .um< mOHSOmmm amusumz 6cm amusuazoflum< umflsocoom mEom coflmcmuxm ucmma nose» : mnv "cofluflmom ucmm< poocmflummxmcH omocmflummxm "mudcme ucmmfl mHQMHHm> ucmm< MMDZmB Wm MUZ¢Bm0m2H QZGANBADUHhmHQ ho mmmOUm de2 ho Nmfizzbm mm mqmfifi -l33- Agents generally assigned greater difficulty scores to ineffective than to effective incidents, regardless of the tenure or position of the reporting agent. Inexperienced agents gave lower difficulty scores than experienced agents to both effective and ineffective incidents. The 4-H - Youth Agents assigned lower difficulty scores than other agents to effective incidents. Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents reported less difficulty for ineffective incidents than did other agents. Agents generally assigned greater importance scores to effective than to ineffective incidents, regardless of the tenure or position of reporting agent. Experienced agents gave lower importance scores than inexperienced agents to ef- fective incidents. Inexperienced agents gave lower importance scores to ineffective incidents than did experienced agents. The 4-H — Youth Agents reported higher importance scores than other agents for effective, ineffective and all incidents. The Home Economists and Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents reported nearly identical importance scores for all incidents. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The Problem This research was designed to employ the critical incident technique to analyze the jobs of COOperative Extension agents. The purposes were to describe the cri- tical job requirements as perceived by a population of extension agents, to identify the training needs as in- ferred from self-reported incidents of ineffective job performance, and further, to determine possible differences in training needs according to the position and the tenure of incumbent agents. Respondents All respondents in this study were employees of the Michigan COOperative Extension Service who had eXperienced not more than 6 years, nor less than 3 months of duty and were employed in a field agent position on December 1, 1967. A summary of findings about the respondents is given below: 1. Of the 74 reSpondents in this study, 25 were Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents; 22 were Extensicn Home Economists, and 27 were 4-H - Youth Agents. About two-thirds of the -134- -l35- agents were male and one-third were female. 2. Over one-half of the 4-H - Youth Agents were between 21 and 30 years of age while over one- half of the Extension Home Economists were over 40 years. Nearly two-thirds of the Agri- cultural and Natural Resource Agents were between 31 and 40 years old. 3. The 4-H — Youth Agents were more often inex- perienced in extension work while the Agricul- tural and Natural Resource Agents were more often experienced. The Extension Home Economists were more equally divided between the experience groups; about 55 percent were experienced and 45 percent were inexperienced. 4. Teaching was the most frequently reported pre- vious work experience for 4-H - Youth Agents and for Extension Home Economists. Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents more frequently reported public service1 as their previous work experience. 5. Over two-thirds of the Extension Home Economists and the 4-H - Youth Agents held a Bachelor's degree or had earned credits beyond it, but not enough for a Master's degree. Nearly two-thirds of the Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents 1Public service primarily consisted of employment with governmental agencies like the U. 8. Soil Conservation Service. -l36- held a Master's degree or higher. Forty percent had earned credits beyond a Master's Degree. Procedures Critical incidents were collected in small group meet- ings from incumbent field extension agents. The incidents were classified by the researcher according to a classifi- cation system develOped a priori and based upon the work of earlier extension investigators. Three judges classified 3 random sample of incidents. As outcomes of the research procedure: 1. The 74 respondents reported 444 critical inci- dents in 23 meetings. Attendance at those meetings ranged from 2 to 11 agents each. Mean attendance per meeting was 3.2 agents. Each agent, as requested, reported 6 incidents, of which 3 were perceived as effective and 3 in— effective. Mean respondent time per incident was about 17 minutes. 2. Four hundred thirty-eight, or 98.6 percent of a total of 444 critical incidents, were classified among the categories of the classification system which had been develOped a priori. 3. The researcher's agreement of third independent classification with the second was 89.6 percent -l37- for categories and 77.2 percent for subcategories. 4. One or more judges agreed with the researcher's classification for 75 percent of the incidents by categories and 56.8 percent of the incidents by subcategories. Two or more judges agreed with the researcher's classification of 47.7 percent of the incidents by categories and with his classification of 29.5 percent by subcategories. The descriptive statistics employed to analyze cri- tical incidents included percentage and frequency distribu- tions, mean scores of both importance and difficulty, and derived rank-order. The data were processed by the Computer Laboratory at Michigan State University. The findings were presented in detail in Chapter IV. The balance of this chapter is devoted to a restatement of the principal findings and to the conclusions based upon those findings. Findings and Conclusions The critical performance categories outlined in Table 40 are generalized groups of critical incidents which reflect agents' perceptions of critical requirements for successfully performing their jobs. The subcategories are more specific job requirements perceived as critical by extension agents. There is a hierarchy of frequency with which the job require— ments occur. More than one-half of all incidents were classi- fied in two critical performance categories: (1) Teaching -l38- and Communicating, and (2) Organizing. It is therefore con- cluded that Michigan Extension Agents most frequently perceive such component behaviors as teaching, sending and receiving messages, organizing groups, and implementing programs through social systems as critical for successful job per- formance. The critical performance categories of Evaluating, Administering, and Program Planning are less frequently per- ceived as critical job requirements. That is, agents do not as frequently View as critical such behaviors as conducting applied research or solving problems; managing resources and administering extension work; or planning and developing extension programs. Agent perception of job requirements differ somewhat by agent position. The 4-H - Youth Agents more frequently perceive Organizing behaviors as critical job requirements, while Extension Home Economists and Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents more frequently view their jobs as requiring Teaching and Communicating behaviors. Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents, more often than others, perceive tasks related to Evaluating as critical job requirements. Agricultural Agents do not very often view Program Planning behaviors as critical job requirements, while extension youth workers and home economists do. -139- TABLE 40 SUMMARY OF THE FREQUENCY RANK-ORDER OF CRITICAL PERFORMANCE CATEGORIES AND COMPONENT CRITICAL JOB REQUIREMENTS FOR MICHIGAN EXTENSION AGENTS Critical Performance Categories Critical Job Requirements TEACHING AND COMMUNICATING 1. Using appropriate method to communicate or teach. 2. Analyzing communication pat- terns and/or organizing messages. 3. Adapting communication and teaching to the individual. 4. Motivating the clientele. ORGANIZING 1. Using existing groups and/or relating parts of the social system. 2. Organizing groups. 3. Identifying and/or using early adopters or leaders. 4. Developing rapport with clientele. 5. Considering values, attitudes, needs, etc. of self and others. 6. Analyzing the power structure. CONDUCTING PROGRAMS 1. Providing competent technical information. 2. DevelOping leadership abili- ties of self or others. 3. DevelOping insight into and/ or exciting pe0p1e about develOpment potential. 4. Interpreting the impact of change and trends. 5. Learning technical information. Critical Performance Categories ADMINISTERING PROGRAM PLANNING EVALUATINGa -l40- Critical Job Requirements Managing work consistent with resources. Coordinating work with co- workers. Making decisions consistent with extension policies. Using knowledge of the struc- ture, function, policies, and programs of extension. Obtaining support for exten- sion programs. Using human resources in program develOpment. Analyzing social, political, or economic situation. Determining the availability of resources for programs. DevelOping a program plan. Thinking creatively or reasoning logically. Conducting applied research. Using the problem solving approach. Conducting Opinion surveys. aAffirmed as a critical performance category only for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents and for inex- perienced agents. -l4l- Agent perception of critical job requirements differs only slightly according to agent tenure. They appear to agree about a great majority of job requirements. However, inexperienced agents much less frequently perceive Evaluating and other research behaviors as critical for job success. In general, Extension Agents appear to perceive the critical requirements of their jobs as occurring in similar frequency, regardless of their own competency in fulfilling those requirements. The competencies possessed by agents, as inferred from effective incidents, and the training needs of agents, as inferred from ineffective incidents, form similar rank-order hierarchies. It is concluded that in general, agent training needs are distributed in about the same manner as agent competencies. The rank-order of importance varied considerably from that of frequency. In general, agents appear to rate as most important for job success behaviors like: (1) working with social systems, and (2) conducting programs. These results suggest that the measure of importance provided an indication of the "degree of criticalness" of job requirements beyond that revealed by an analysis of incident frequency. The rank-order of difficulty differed from that of either incident frequency or importance. Agents appear to generally perceive Evaluating behaviors as much more difficult than behaviors related to Conducting Programs. -l42- Agents reported higher values on a seven-point scale for incident importance than for incident difficulty. Per— haps this reflects the agents' need to achieve success or possibly their need to avoid failure. Exprienced and inexperienced agents differ in their perception of the difficulty of performing incidents. Ex— perienced agents reported higher difficulty scores than inexperienced agents. Perhaps those with longer tenure have a greater sense of belonging to the organization and are less fearful to admit difficulty. Or, perhaps agent perception of appropriate role behavior expands with sociali- zation, resulting in greater complexity and hence, greater difficulty of execution. Agents differ in their perception of the importance of critical job requirements according to their tenure. Experienced agents attach most importance to Program Planning behaviors, while inexperienced agents view Organizing beha- viors as most important. In this study, there was less variance in importance scores for experienced than for in- experienced agents. This lends support to the postulate that with extension employment experience, agents become more homogeneous in their perception of apprOpriate job behavior. There was little difference between the two tenure groups in the frequency of reported effective incidents. This may have resulted from the failure of the classification -l43- system to discriminate subtle differences in perceptions of effectiveness. Or, perhaps agents' perceptions of their own effectiveness develOp at the early induction period; they might be develOped previous to employment. Training needs appear to differ according to agent tenure. Such needs, as inferred from ineffective incidents, were present in each critical performance category. Differ- ential training needs are suggested according to agent tenure for the critical performance categories of Teaching and Communicating, Organizing, and Evaluating. It is more difficult for experienced agents to per- form critical incidents whether the incidents are effective or ineffective. In addition, agents seem to have more dif- ficulty in performing ineffective than effective incidents. Lack of training appears to contribute to agent difficulty in performing incidents perceived as critical for job performance. Agents differ in their perception of the difficulty of executing effective and ineffective incidents acCording to the position of reporting agents. The 4-H - Youth Agents expressed more difficulty with ineffective incidents than other agents. While it was more extreme for 4-H Agents, agents in general appeared more likely to experience dif- ficulty with ineffective incidents where agents lacked train- ing, than with effective incidents where the agent possessed needed competencies. -l44- Agents differentially perceive the importance of both effective and ineffective incidents according to agent tenure. Experienced agents rated ineffective incidents as more important and effective incidents as less important than did inexperienced agents. However, both experienced and inexperienced agents seem to attach more importance to incidents in which they possess competencies than to inci- dents in which they lack competencies-—inexperienced agents appeared eSpecially likely to do so. It is a conjecture that the greater importance attached to effective incidents might reflect agent needs to attain self-esteem and the esteem of others. Agents of different positions appear not to agree upon the relative importance of either effective or inef- fective incidents. Youth agents, for instance, accord greater importance to their competency of Administering their work, while Home Economists view their competencies of Conducting Programs as more important. Differences by position recur in the hierarchy of agent training needs too. The 4-H Agents rate training needs related to Conducting Extension Programs as most important; Home Economists attach more importance to Program Planning training needs; while Evaluation or research training needs appear more im- portant for Agricultural and Natural Resource Agents. -l45- A summary of the rank-order of Critical Performance Categories by the frequency, difficulty, and importance of critical incidents according to the tenure of the reporting agent is presented in Table 41 for effective incidents; in Table 42 for ineffective incidents; and in Table 43 for all incidents. A summary of the rank-order of Critical Performance Categories by the frequency, difficulty, and importance of critical incidents according to the position of the reporting agent is presented in Table 44 for effective incidents; in Table 45 for ineffective incidents; and in Table 46 for all incidents. Conclusioggabout the Critical Incident Technique It is concluded that the critical incident technique lends itself to the collection of incidents in small groups of 2 to 11 respondents. It is possible for persons supply- ing data to write 3 effective and 3 ineffective critical incidents in less than 2 hours. A central purpose of this study was to employ the critical incident technique as a means for identifying the job requirements and agent training needs. It was not known whether this technique would identify needed job behaviors which had not been identified by other methods. The incident G H G H H G G G G maHumsHm>m G N G G N G H N G mchcmHm ewumoum N N G G G N N H G mcHumuchHsee H G G G G G N G G meaumoum mcfluosooou G G N N N N G G N mcHNHcmmHo G G H G G H G G H mcHumoHasasoo pom mcflnomme mocmu wpaoo wocwsw mocmu muaso wocmov oocmu weaso mesons muommumu uHOQEH Iflmmflo Iwum IHOQEH Iflmwflo Imum IHOQEH IHMMHQ umHm mocmEuomem mucmmfi Ham mucmmm poocmflnmmchH mucmmm Umocmflmwmxm HMOHDHHU Bzmw< OZHBmommm mo mmDZMB Mm mBzm0HUZH AfloHBHmU m>H9000002H mo 002m m G G H G G G G G mchamHm emumoua G G G G G G G N G maHumuchHsGN G G G G G G N G G msmumoum mQGDOSUCOU N N N G N H H N N maHNHcmmHo H G H N H H G G H mcHumoHcsseoo pom mcflnomme mocmu muaoo wocmsv momma muaso wocmso mommy wuaso mocmov whommumo IHOQEH AHMMHQ Imum IHOQEH IGMMGQ Imum IHOQEH Iflmwflo noun mocmeuomuwm mpcmwm Hafi mucmmm poocwflummxmcH mucumfl poocmflummxm HMOHDGHU BZMU< UZHBMOmmm mO mmDZMB mm mBZWOHUZH AdUHBHmU m>HBUmmmm mo m02m H G G G G G N G G mchamHm smumoum G G G G G G H G G maHumpchHsGG G G N H G G G G N msmumoum mofluosocoo m a m m H N m m a mnfluwcomuo N G H N N H G N G mmHumoHcsssoo can msflcowme mocmu moaoo hocosv mocmu huaoo woomsv mocmu hpaso hocmsv wuommumu IHOQEH IGMMHQ loom IHOQEH IHMMHQ .rmnh IHOQEH IGMMGQ mum mocoEHomumm mucumm .m.z G mm wpmGEoooom meow mucmmm nusow I mue HGUGDGHU 92m0¢ UZHBmommm 00.20H9Hm00 Mm mBZMQHUZH A¢UHBHmU m>HBUmmmm mo mUZ¢BmOmSH 02¢ .WBADUHhhH0 .NUZWDOMMM mom mmHmOUMB¢U m02¢zmommmm A¢UHBHmU m0 mm0m0IMZ¢m mma mo Mm¢220m we mqm¢B . u. G H G N H G G N G mcHumsHm>m A G G G G G G H G G GVcHEBHm emumoum G G G G G G G H G mcHuwuchHsG< N G G G G G N G G msmumoum mofluosoooo H N N H N N G G N GEHNHcGaGHo G G H G G H G G H mcHumoHasssoo pom mcflnomma wocmv muaso mocmsw mocmu >paoo woomsw mocmu huaoo hocmsv whommumu IHOQEH IGMMHQ umnm IHomEH IGMMGQ Imum IHOQEH IGMMGQ nmnm mocmEHomuwm mucmm¢ Ham mpcmm< poocmGHmQWocH mucmmd omocmfiummxm HMOGDHHU Bzmw¢ UZHBMOmmm mOfmmDZMB 20 mBZMOHUZH A¢UHBHmU 04¢ 00 MUZ¢BmOmSH 02¢ .MBADUHmmH0 .Nozmoommh mom mmHmOUMB¢U MUZ¢SmOmmmm A¢UHBHmU mo 200MOIMZ¢M mm& 00 Nm¢220m mv MAM¢B -148- H N G G G G G H G mcHumsHm>m N G G G G G G N G mcHaamHm gaseous G G G G H G G G G maHumumHaHEGG G G N N G G H G G msmumoum mofiuooocou G H G H N N N G H mcHNHcmmHo G G H G G H G G N mcHumoHasssoo pom msflcomoa mocmu >u050 wocmsw mocmu muHSo hocmso mocmu mu050 mocmov muommumu IHomEH IGmwGQ Imum IHOQEH IGMMGQ Imnm IHOQEH IGMWGQ Imum mocmfiuowumm mucomd .m.z G .m¢ mpmHEocoom mEom mpcmmm cusow I mIG HMUGHGHU Bzmw¢ UZHBmOmmm mo ZOHBHmom wm m8200H02H 0¢UHBHmU 00¢ m0 mUZ¢BmomzH 02¢ .28000HmmH0 .wozmbommm mom mmHmOUmB¢U MU2¢Smommmm 0¢UHBHmU mo 000MOIMZ¢M mmB mo Nm¢220m GG mqmde H G G G G G G H G mcHumsHm>m G G G H N G G N G mchcmHm smnmoum N N G G H N G G G mcHumuchHEGG G G N G G G H G G msmumoum mcfluoowcou G H G N G G N G N mcHNHcmmHo G G H G G H G G H mcHumoHcsssoo com mcflnomme oocmu GDHSO mocmsv mocmu GDHDQ mocmsv mocmu Guano wocmov whommumo IHOQEH Immmwo Imum IHOQEH IGMMGQ Imum IHOQEH IHmMGQ Imum mocmfiuomumm mucmmw .m.z G .m¢ mumGEomuom 050m mucom¢ canow I mIG HMUGDGHU BZWD¢ UZHBmommm m0 ZOHBHmom Mm mBZMOHU2H 0¢UHBHMU m>HfiummmMZH mo m02¢8m002H 02¢ .WBODUHO0H0 .MUZMDommm mom mmHmowMB¢U m02¢£mommmm 0¢UHBHmU mo mm0MOI22¢m 028 m0 wm¢ZSDm mv m0m¢8 -l49- classification system employed in this study was largely developed from items contained in questionnaires used by other extension researchers. Therefore, if the critical incident technique were to identify important job behaviors not contained in previous research instruments, one would expect such behaviors to fall outside the classification system. Since it was possible to classify over 98.5 percent of the incidents within this classification system, it was concluded that the critical incident technique failed to reveal significant numbers of important job behaviors be- yond those shown by other research methods. However, this result suggests that the critical incident technique revealed needed job behaviors which are similar to those revealed by other research methods. In addition, it made possible the ranking of needed behavior according to frequency, impor- tance, and difficulty, thus providing evidence of the degree of criticalness of identified needs. The classification system employed in this study was sufficiently comprehensive for the researcher to classify nearly all the reported incidents with about 90 percent cate— gory consistency. However, judges' agreement was considerably less, thereby lending credence to claims of subjectivity for the system employed in this study and for the critical inci- dent technique in general. Therefore, it appears that this -150- classification system requires refinement and further test- ing to enhance both its comprehensive nature and its reli- ability as a research instrument. It does seem reasonable to conclude that this system was sufficiently comprehensive for the researcher to classify the critical incidents re- ported by the agents in this study. The critical incident technique as employed in this study permitted a description of job requirements as per- ceived by a population of Michigan extension agents; it permitted inferences of training needs for those agents; and it revealed differences in inferred job requirements and training needs by position and tenure of participating agents. Limitations l. The application of the critical incident technique in this study involved only incumbent agents, thereby resulting in a reflection of the percep- tion of this single group. 2. The results of this study are subject to the limitations of cross-sectional research. Changes over time can only be assumed to be represented in the variables under study. 3. This research describes agent job behaviors as they currently exist. It should not be interpreted Summaryfiof -151- as describing them as they ought to be or as they may be at some future date. Similarly, the objec— tives in the work situation were those actually being pursued and not necessarily those that ought to be pursued. The relatively low level of agreement of category classification by researcher and judges limits the extent to which these findings ought to be generalized. Confidence in the findings of this research is limited at points due to the small number of subjects involved. This was particularly evident when analyzing variables by agent position. Conclusions There is a hierarchy of frequency with which agent job requirements are perceived to occur among cri- tical performance categories. Agent perceptions of job requirements differ some- what by agent position. Agent perceptions of job requirements differ only slightly by agent tenure. In general, agent training needs are distributed among critical performance categories in about the same frequency as agent competencies. 10. ll. ~152- Agents appear to differentiate between the fre- quency with which job requirements occur and the importance attached to those requirements. Agents differentiate among frequency, importance, and difficulty of performing critical incidents thereby suggesting that the degree of criticalness varies with the measure employed. Agents generally seem to place higher ratings upon the importance than upon the difficulty of performing the incidents they reported. Agents apparently differ in their perceptions of the difficulty of performing incidents according to their tenure since experienced agents reported higher difficulty scores than did inexperienced agents. Agents appear to differ in their perceptions of the importance of job requirements according to their tenure. The frequency with which self-reported competencies occur among critical performance categories does not seem to differ by agent tenure. The frequency with which training needs occur among critical performance categories differs according to agent tenure. 12. l3. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. -153- Lack of training evidently contributes to agent difficulty in performing incidents perceived as critical for job performance. Competencies and training needs seem to differ according to agent position. Perceptions of the importance of competencies and training needs appear to differ according to agent tenure. Agents of different positions apparently do not agree on the importance of competencies and training needs. The critical incident technique lends itself to collection of incidents in small groups of 2 to 11 respondents. The critical incident technique failed to reveal significant numbers of job behaviors beyond those shown by other research methods. The critical incident technique makes possible the ranking of needed job behaviors according to their frequency, importance, and difficulty. The classification system develOped for this study was sufficiently comprehensive to classify agent reported critical incidents, but both re- finement and further testing are needed. -154- Implications and Recommendations The results of this study offer several practical applications to the Cooperative Extension Service. The cri- tical performance categories and subcategories provide a description of extension agents' views of the critical requirement of their job. The results of this job analysis should provide understanding of the things agents consider critical for successful job performance. Future research with such "relevant others" as admi- nistrators, Specialists, and clientele would reveal additional dimensions of agent job requirements as perceived by others. Agent training needs were identified as they occurred within the reality of the situation in which agents work. Hierarchies were specified for the frequency with which train- ing needs occurred; for the difficulty associated with those needs; and for the importance attached to the training needs. These hierarchies provide a potential criterion for establish- ing a priority of agent training. Future research in this area, if it is to analyze the variables by agent position, should involve greater numbers of agents. This study revealed that the 3 categories of job requirements most frequently reported contrasted sharply with those least frequently reported. Such evidence could be use- ful for extension administrators as they make training and other personnel management decisions. Ii .4, -155- It appears that the initial 3 month induction period may be very important in develOping agent perception of the critical requirements of their jobs. After 3 months, the agents in this study held similar perceptions of the fre- quency with which job requirements occurred. This is not to imply that agents do not change their ideas about some aspects of their jobs. Their perception of difficulty and importance do seem to change with time. Indeed, agents appeared to report greater difficulty as they gained job experience. The need for agent training does not seem to decline with increasing tenure, but rather changes in emphasis. The agents reported more difficulty in performing incidents in which they lacked competencies. This suggests that special efforts might be necessary if training were to be provided in such areas as conducting applied research, planning extension programs, and administrative behaviors. It seems useful to know that agents do not generally view the importance of critical job requirements in the same way they report the frequency of their occurrence. It would probably require considerably less motivation to alter com- petencies of agents in those areas they already believe to be of greater importance. The results of this study should provide useful evi- dence for making judgments about differentiating training -156- on the basis of agent position and/or tenure depending upon the training needs to be met. It appears that the use of scaled items in conjunction with the critical incident technique offers Opportunities for researchers to refine the interpretation of the results. Further application and refinement of such scaling efforts appear potentially rewarding. The classification system employed in this study re- quires considerable refinement. Since it does seem to hold promise for analyzing agent job performance, other researchers may make adaptations in the system. Studies in changes of perception of appropriate role behaviors would likely be promising terrain for future re- search. The results of this study suggest the hypothesis that with socialization, agent perception of apprOpriate role behavior expands, resulting in greater complexity and hence greater difficulty of performance. It is recommended that a longitudinal research project for studying changes in agent job perception over time be assigned to a division of the Michigan Cooperative Extension Service. An hypothesis which grows from this study is that agents become more homogeneous in their perceptions of appro- priate role behavior as length of extension work experience increases. The question of changes in agent perception of role behavior during the initial 3 months of induction seems -157- promising for future research. Also, to what extent do agents differ in employment role perception at the point of entry in the extension organization? APPENDIX A A QUESTIONNAIRE FOR EXTENSION AGENTS REPORTING CRITICAL INCIDENTS Please complete the information requested below. No personal information will be revealed for individuals. You are asked to give your name only in case more information is needed. You are assured that individual information will remain confidential. Background Information 1. Name: 2. Today‘s Date: 3 . Sex: B Male [:1 Female h“ Present Age: Present Extension Position 5. On what date did you join the Michigan Cooperative Extension Service? 6. Have you held other extension positions? [3 Yes C] No If yes, please give the following information: Type of Position How Long Held? 8. What type(s) of work did you do before joining the Cooperative Extension Service: Dates of Service Nature of Responsibility Employer 9. What is the highest academic degree you have completed? (Please check): Major area of completed degree(s): C] Bachelor's C] Master's E] Specialist's E] Doctor's 11). How many term hours of graduate work have you completed beyond your highest degree? -158- -159- The Information Below Will Help You Complete This Questionnaire On the following pages you are asked to provide examples of things you have done in connec- tion with your job as an extension agent. Some things you may feel were particularly effective, others you might now judge were very ineffective things for an Extenstion agent to do. This research method is called the critical incident technique. The purpose of this study is to identify the specific actions which are required for the performance of an extension agent's role. Since you are in the best position to know what you do in your job, your frank answers are extremely important. This method has been widely used in other professions. Examples of both effective and in- effective incidents are given below. These are provided just to give you an idea of the kind of information needed. You will notice that the examples contain answers to each of the following questions about the incident: Exactly what did you do? What circumstances led up to the incident? What was your objective? Who was involved? What were the results of your action? When did this incident take place? OWFWNH An Effective Incident for a Teacher I was teaching a unit of tumbling in a seventh grade gym class. I asked one boy to practice some stunts outside the class and then demonstrate them to the rest of the class. This boy was self-conscious due to his size and age. His neuro-muscular skills were poor and the other kids made fun of him. I wanted him to develop self-confidence by learning how to do some stunts well. When the rest of the class saw him demonstrate the stunts, they were very surprised. This was an effective incident because the class began to respect this boy. He became more confident and was treated as "one of the gang." This happened in January of 1965. ' An Ineffective Incident for a Nurse When on duty in the hospital emergency room I became irritated with the number of forms that had to be filled out before a patient could be treated. I was short-tempered with my supervisor and I told her this was a needless waste of time. I felt that carbon impregnated forms could reduce the time and effort and permit quicker treatment. She was angry for my lack of tact and refused to listen to my idea. My encounter with the supervisor happened when a child was in terrific pain, but I wasn't permitted to treat him until the parents had com- pleted 3 different forms. I wanted to improve the speed of patient care. However, this was an ineffective incident because it resulted in severed communication with my supervisor and the idea I suggested was never used. This was in October of 1966 An Effective Incident for a School Superintendent I brought some data before the school board about the drop-out rate in our school. I pro- posed that groups of teachers, local citizens, and consultants be contacted to study this matter and suggest methods for combating the drop-out situation. I became concerned when I found that our drop-out rate was at its highest level in 25 years. I wanted to prevent this loss of human potential. This proved to be an effective incident since the board endorsed the idea in April of 1966 and as a result, a new "drop-in" prOgram is now being initiated. An Ineffective Incident for a Minister '- I I contacted ten fellow clergymen to promote the idea of a supervised "Teen-Center" in our community. When we met, the others seemed favorable o the idea, but I could never get their active support. I was promoting the idea because th e had been a rash of juvenile delinquency. I thought a Teen-Center would help prevent it. This as an ineffective incident because the other clergymen failed to act on the idea or to sugge t other alternatives. This happened just before Christmas in 196A. -l60- CRITICAL INCIDENT REPORT FORM Think back over your experience as an extension agent in Michigan. You probably have done Inany things in your job which you would now judge as being very effective - things that would cflaaracterize you as an effective agent. Think of a single such incident in which you feel you vnere particularly effective. Please describe what happened: 1. The circumstances: What I did: My objective: Person(s) involved: Results: When did this incident take place? Month Year To what extent do you believe this incident is likely to influence your success as an extension agent? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. 1+. 5. 6. 7. Little Influence Some Influence Great Influence How difficult was it for you to do the incident you described above? Please circle the most appropriate number: I 1. 2. 3. A. S. 6. 7. Not Difficult Somewhat Difficult Very Difficult —161- CRITICAL INCIDENT REPORT FORM Once again, think back over your experience as an extension agent in Michigan. You probably did some things which you would now judge as being very ineffective. Of course, these things may not be typical of your normal performance, but rather are things you did on the job which you now judge as being particularly ineffective. Please describe what happened: 1. The circumstances: 2. What I did: 3. My objective: 1+. Person(s) involved: 5. Results: 6. When did this incident take place? Month Year '7. To what extent do you believe this incident is likely to influence your success as an extension agent? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. u. 5. 6. 7. Little Influence Some Influence Great Influence 8. wa difficult was it for you to do the incident you described above? Please circle the most appropriate number: 10 2. 30 LL. 5. 60 7' Not Difficult Somewhat Difficult Very Difficult -162- CRITICAL INCIDENT REPORT FORM Think back over your experience as an extension agent in Michigan. You probably have done many things in your job which you would now judge as being very effective - things that would characterize you as an effective agent. Think of a single such incident in which you feel you were particularly effective. Please describe what happened: I. The circumstances: What I did: My objective: Person(s) involved: Results: When did this incident take place? Month Year To what extent do you believe this incident is likely to influence your success as an extension agent? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. LI. 5. 6. 7. Little Influence Some Influence Great Influence How difficult was it for you to do the incident you described above? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. 1+. 5. 6. 7. Not Difficult Somewhat Difficult Very Difficult -l63- CRITICAL INCIDENT REPORT FORM Once again, think back over your experience as an extension agent in Michigan. You probably did.some things which you would now judge as being very ineffective. Of course, these things they not be typical of your normal performance, but rather are things you did on the job which yTNl now judge as being particularly ineffective. Please describe what happened: I. The circumstances: What I did: My objective: Person(s) involved: Results: When did this incident take place? Month Year To what extent do you believe this incident is likely to influence your success as an extension agent? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. II. 5. 6. 7. Little Influence Some Influence Great Influence How difficult was it for you to do the incident you described above? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. II. 5. 6. 7. Not Difficult Somewhat Difficult Very Difficult -164- CRITICAL INCIDENT REPORT FORM Think back over your experience as an extension agent in Michigan. You probably have done Inany things in your job which you would now judge as being very effective - things that would characterize you as an effective agent. Think of a single such incident in which you feel you ‘were particularly effective. Please describe what happened: 1. The circumstances: What I did: My objective: Person(s) involved: Results: When did this incident take place? Month Year To what extent do you believe this incident is likely to influence your success as an extension agent? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. 1+. 5. 6. 7. Little Influence Some Influence Great Influence HOw difficult was it for you to do the incident you described above? Please circle the most appropriate number: 1. 2. 3. 1+. 5. 6. 7. Not Difficult Somewhat Difficult Very Difficult -165- CRITICAL INCIDENT REPORT FORM Once again, think back over your experience as an extension agent in Michigan. You probably (iid.some things which you would now judge as being very ineffective. Of course, these things they not be typical of your normal performance, but rather are things you did on the job which y_\‘A\_-’/’ I I - ‘:\\’_1 r I .3»..- '. - ‘7’ - L'.‘\’ I" - ‘ \ _:\' - ‘3’ 11 ‘7“... \\-)/ '5 ‘ J \\-' J" 2 "‘3 I._ f". \. , .-. ‘ ~ i ‘60.; \\ " 7' \\ '. .I ”J.omm.2"K‘?.MJ1L:’..‘£‘L-’ELVJ EA” nut-..Im: 3'“ 7’ 1'1'31 42“ I" J)“ 51 55'64'111’ ' -~. ——-._ _—_..___ -__.—-———- g ,5 7 ’/ J?” I «#49153813oT/7mmzon. 4) ;;nén;; -7.....__ — _.__. mafia-“‘1'“ L :\.1/"! f! i, £5,757“ 4' ~ / .- [J ML 4s-~flw.1 m-“ f”. 4.111.451” c»? 'I'II’D 05mm .1-i/55.’..’.’/I’l:£ 9056013. 065M417 05cm“: (LT/PE REM?" I" "fife-.mo IAICC‘STA menu MIDLAND ' . WEXFJRJ ’V‘ ‘ . LL.‘1)1|i‘t‘1r]'O\/LLT\J '-'-..... c -‘w‘. .. crud. oo‘ 1-." O“? van-o. . pry) .. .- ‘ o‘ .‘G, .‘ . f. 'Y ‘W' .p..¢ . I : H ' a. ' \ I g . I, \ I J‘UL.A‘—, .J-L\_’ I .‘.J-_I'_'i.) '('.-“UI.I)J. IJ . . -» M—q-g-wo Q ~-..~-. . -.~—.- ‘..- ~-—_u. «OI-“Q .---— «*M I. \. ,.___..4 \‘ ‘ , - 1 ,I m I) ,J ', .11" 'r H 'I .- -r"-j- ' :7 I": -. -‘ j',/"_ MONTCALM GRAIIOT 5,10!wa 1" .L‘. ..“..11. .'..‘.JI..' .1114 J... II..I.-L-:‘.1"../ri ‘ I M 0 c O Nk.- e-‘-~-o»--_-o--..-.-.o-Gn—..-—--.. ...-.-.-.I ‘S q fif/I’ . . 10m $717.70” $h'lAWA. 1' AlLCK-Wf.‘ £3.4A’fif 7‘70” 1N5" 1,“ lell,"’6'P.’—. III! M. 1 -_ ‘KALA/JA'CALHJUh Mun/v .‘msr‘mw. VIN/15..“ 9). 0‘ '10 coo/“1 .— A—J - ...- _J..'J 51.2135". Yaw... 1311501151541 E! 114.5130: ...————-'__- _.—.-— --—.___._—.‘..._.-..—.-. —- EROJ , "1' ...«w— “.-.I o.- -5- /__T. 4 . ‘_~#1H# J ”mt-arm's»: u... .a'Ls. '15: 42.0- 1 .1— ‘ A //-- " I. ‘ fr, i (I ,- 0 C I . C :/ I ' I. ..—.. . a. v” {7‘ 1;; ' -ru..‘ ‘ 1 I Tint. 23AM: ran. .axa ..m: :1) | I I” I l .. . I ’ I ' i {I 1.. ‘ '3 m . -.~. “Pony --~-onsa --m..—~~- 7- ”1‘ . . . Q‘ -. ~‘-r-‘ - _ L- — -I—b- _ - - ..."H-I ‘ . -.J- -— -- --- _- . --_ __-___.__'_.__‘_.-__._._‘ ”J... _-_ - __ m-_ -"w I»... ‘r..— —’-_”-— - ~ _ ‘ _____ —-- ‘ - ‘-~‘ I 2 m, ”a :5. 5" e-:‘ 65' 34' 55° ‘11 L" "*- iAEIIhmzz-Tsssasqi3.". 4.4:. aux-:5: . -:GZ;.:..‘:. ...2'31’. 1:,;-'—;;. $3.1! Let-"4.: :;-.'.;.'.';::.im“ 73.37;: . ; 55.1: ;..'..'.. .-..‘:T. .- .1712”; ;‘-..;T.':‘-' f. f1 0-121’.‘ APPENDIX D COOPERATIVE EXTENSION SE RVICE HICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ° EAST LANSING ' MICHIGAN 48823 Office of the Direcnor AND U.8. DEPARTMENT OF AGRICULTURE COOPERATING December 4, 1967 Dear Co-Worker: You recently received a letter from your Program Director telling you about some research I want to do with Extension agents. You were selected because you are in a unique position to contribute to this study. The purpose of this research is to try to find what specific actions are necessary for performing the role of an Extension agent. You will be asked to write brief descriptions of things you have done in your job as an Extension agent. Examples are needed of things you feel were particularly effective. In ad- dition, examples are needed of things you have done that you might now judge as ineffective things for an extension worker to do. This is an effort to analyze the job of an agent, not to evaluate you as a person or the work you are doing. I wait to assure you that all responses will be treated confidentially. By meeting in small groups it will be possible to clarify instructions and answer your questions. I will call you in the near future to see if you are willing to participate, and, if so, to schedule a two-hour meeting. I sincerely hope you will be willing to help in this effort. Your participation is vitally needed. Very truly yours, Fred J. Peabody FJP:mas -l78- APPENDIX E COOPERATIVE EXTENSION SERVICE MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ° EAST LANSING ' MICHIGAN 48823 CEO: of the Direcnor AND U.S. DEPARTMENT OF AGRICULTURE COOPERATING TO: I want to confirm our meeting for collecting Extension re- search data. The meeting is planned for the following: TIME DATE LOCATION I look forward to seeing you at that time. Some agents have asked for more details about the kind of information needed in this study. You will remember that we are requesting examples of both effective and ineffective things you have done in your job as an Extension agent. ’This research method is called the critical incident technique. It has been used in several professions to analyze jobs in those fields. I am enclosing a couple of examples of critical incidents from other professions. These are provided just to give you an idea of the kind of information needed. You will be asked to write three effective and three ineffective incidents from your own experiences as an Extension agent. This will probably take about two hours. I really appreciate your willingness to COOperate in this study. I hope the results will provide some answers to questions about what it takes to be a successful Extension agent. Sincerely, Fred J. Peabody -l79- -180- EXAMPLE OF AN EFFECTIVE INCIDENT FOR A SOCIAL WORKER Attendance by mothers receiving Aid to Dependent Children at a series of "Money Management" meetings had been very poor. A teacher, a home economist and I co- Operatively planned the meetings with the mothers ,but the mothers would not attend. We asked several mothers why they had not participated, but their answers were evasive. In June of 1967 we asked a social scientist to help us construct a questionnaire so the women could anonymously indicate why they hadn't attended. We found that many were afraid their ADC payments would be out if they managed their money better. This was an effective incident because we were able to assure the mothers that funds would not be reduced if they participated. As a result about fifty percent of the eligible mothers atten- ded the series when it was repeated. EXAMPLE OF AN INEFFECTIVE INCIDENT FOR A HIGH SCHOOL PRINCIPAL The curriculum in our school was geared for college preparatory students, but less than 50% of our students went on to college. Many of our high school graduates were not able to get jobs because they lacked vocational training. I worked with the teachers, the superintendent, the board of education, and some concerned citizens to orga- nize curriculum study groups. I hoped to initiate a new vocational program, but the cost for implementing it was over a million dollars. This was an ineffective incident because we were not able to get the public enthused about it and the voters rejected the issue in two elections. We still have only the college preparatory curriculum. EXAMPLE OF AN EFFECTIVE INCIDENT FOR A COLLEGE PROFESSOR I was asked to act as a consultant to the board of directors of a small marketing firm. This firm was in serious financial difficulty. I carefully studied the situation and recommended several changes in the organi- zation; these included marketing procedures, staff training and corporate structure. My objective was to save the firm from financial ruin. On July 1, 1964 the board reorganized the firm according to the guidlines I had suggested. This was an effective incident because the firm has now developed a sound financial base. . 'Jffl . APPENDIX F EXAMPLE NUMBER 1 OF CRITICAL INCIDENT CLASSIFICATION The effective critical incident which follows was re- ported by an Agricultural Extension Agent. Three judges agreed with the category and subcategory classification of this incident by the researcher. One of the farmers with whom I had been working had about 30 acres of muck... and another 30 acres that could be cleared. He was considering planting potatoes on this, however it would have required...a system to pump water out and then later to irrigate the potatoes. The circumstances: What I did: I collected information on the kinds and costs of the equipment needed for potatoes (and) how many acres would be necessary to recover costs. Provided information (about) how this enterprise and his present Operation would compli- ment one another. My objective: To provide him with economic data re— garding the enterprise for him to make a good decision. Person(s) involved: Farmer and myself. Results: He decided not to enter the potato enter- prise and purchased another farm that would compliment his existing operation. ‘1 Classification of Example Incident Number 1 Classifier: Category: Subcategory: Researcher Conducting Programs Providing Technical Information Judge 1 Conducting Programs Providing Technical Information Judge 2 Conducting Programs Providing Technical Information Judge 3 Conducting Programs Providing Technical Information -181- -182- EXAMPLE NUMBER 2 OF CRITICAL INCIDENT CLASSIFICATION The effective critical incident which follows was reported by an Extension Home Economist. Two judges agreed with the category classification of this incident by the researcher, but none agreed with the subcategory classi- fication. The circumstances: On surplus food pick-up day, in coopera- tion with the Department of Social Services, I gave an informal demonstration of the use of surplus commodities in making pizza. What I did: Besides making the pizza, there were samples available, recipes, and a sign-up bulletin board for sewing classes. Instead of nar- rating the demonstration, I simply answered questions. My objective: ...to meet some low income families, to make known the resource material available here, and to get them to meet as a group. Person(s) involved: About 90 peOple of income low enough to , qualify for surplus commodities. Results: Not only did they clean out my supply of pizza recipes, they shared with me and each other some ideas they had found successful in making better use of surplus food..... Classification of Example Incident Number 2 Classifier: Category: Subcategory: Researcher Teaching and Using apprOpriate method to Communicating communicate or teach Judge 1 Conducting Providing technical information Programs Judge 2 Teaching and Analyzing communication patterns Communicating and/or organizing messages Judge 3 Teaching and Motivating the clientele Communicating -183- EXAMPLE NUMBER 3 OF CRITICAL INCIDENT CLASSIFICATION The effective critical incident which follows was reported by a Natural Resource Agent. One judge agreed with the category and subcategory classification of this incident by the researcher. The circumstances: What I did: My objective: Person(s) involved: One way of using idle lands in our county is to use them for cow-calf beef farming operations. Many beef producers already existed in the county, but more emphasis was needed to encourage this kind of an enter- prise and to improve management on existing farms. I organized a beef producers associ- ation to promote beef farming in our county. A meeting was held to deter- mine if there was enough interest and assistance was provided to form the organization. To utilize idle lands and to improve management of existing beef farms. Beef Producers Results: This was an effective incident as several demonstrations on management were held and there was a 100% increase in market- ing of cattle through (the) COOperative feeder sale. Classification of Example Incident Number 3 Classifier: Category: - Subcategory: Researcher Organizing Organizing groups to achieve program objectives Judge 1 Program Analyzing social, political, Planning or economic situation Judge 2 Organizing Organizing groups to achieve program objectives Judge 3 Teaching and Motivating the clientele Communicating -184- EXAMPLE NUMBER 4 OF CRITICAL INCIDENT CLASSIFICATION The ineffective critical incident which follows was reported by a 4-H - Youth Agent. No judge agreed with either the category or subcategory classification of this incident by the researcher. The circumstances: What I did: My objective: Person(s) involved: Results: A real need existed for jobs for young teens in my area. I made contact with berry growers and the farm labor service to use teens if I could get enough in- terested... I talked it over with the Ag Agent and {got his blessing to put a notice in the paper of the Opportunity. I gave our Extension phone number for youth to call if interested. I did not tell our secretaries much about it other than typing the notice. To find youth interested in working in the berry fields and to provide trans- portation for them. Myself, a fine office staff, and a total of 182 youth. Our office was swamped with calls for about four days. Our secretaries were not prepared by having a list ready, etc. As a result, it disrupted our office and caused rough feelings until I got it straightened out. Classification of Example Incident Number.4 Classifier: Category: Subcategory: Researcher Administering .Managing work consistent with resources Judge 1 Program Analyzing social, political, Planning or economic situation Judge 2 Conducting DevelOping insight into and/or Programs exciting people about develop- ment potential Judge 3 Teaching and Motivating the clientele Communicating BIBLIOGRAPHY ~186- BIBLIOGRAPHY Books Biddle, Bruce J., Rosecranz, Howard A., and Rankin, Earl F. Studies in-the Roleiof-theNPublic School Teacher. Columbia: Social Psychology Laboratory, The Uni- versity of Missouri, 1963. Blau, Peter, M. Bureaucracy in Modern Society. New York: Random House, 1956. Borg, Walter R. Educational Research:, An Introduction. New York: David McKay Co., 1963. Brim, Orville G., and Wheeler, Stanton. Socialization after Childhood: Two Essays. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1966. Festinger, Leon, and Katz, Daniel (eds.).. Research Methods in the Behavioral Sciences. New York: The Dryden Press, 1953. Fivars, Grace, and Gosnell,.Doris. Nursing Evaluation: The Problem and the Process: sThe.Critical Incident Technique. -New.York: The MacmilIan Co., 1966: Flanagan, John C. The AviationnPsychology,Program“in.the. Army Air Forces-..Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1948. Gross, Neal, Mason, Ward S. and McEachern, Alexander W. Explorations in.Role Analysis: .Studies-ofnthe School Superintendency. New York:' John Wiley and Sons, 1958. Herzberg, Frederick, Mausner, Bernard,.and Synderman, Barbara. The Motivation to Work, New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1959. Herzberg, Frederick. Work and the Nature of Man. Cleveland: The World Publishing Co., 1966. Kahn, Robert L., Wolfe, Donald M.,-Quinn, Robert P., and Snoek, Diedrick J. Organizational Stress, Studies in Role Ambiguity. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1964. -187- Katz, Daniel and Kahn, Robert L. The Social Psycholo of Organizations. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1966. Kelsey, Lincoln D., and Hearne, Cannon C. Cooperative Extension Work. Ithaca: .Comstock Publishing Co., 1963. Lasswell, Harold D., Leites, Nathan, and Associates. Language of Politics-. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, 1947 and 1965. Lindvall, C. M. (ed.) Defining Educational Objectives (3). Pittsburgh:~.University of Pittsburgh Press, 1964. Mager, Robert F. Preparing Instructional Objectives. Palo Alto: Fearon Publishers,‘I962. Maslow, Abraham.. Motivation and Personality. .New York: Harper Brothers, 1954. McGregor, Douglas. {The Human Side.of Enterprise.- New York: McGraw-Hili, I960. . . i McNemar, Quinn. Ps cholo ical Statistics-. New York: John Wiley and Sons, I932. , Merton, Robert K. Social Theory and Social Structure. Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1957. Murray, Henry A. Exploration in.Persenali§y. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938. Ryans, David G. Characteristics of-Teachers- .Washington: American Council on Education, 1960- Travers, Robert M. W. An Introduction to.Educational. Research. »New York: The Macmillan Co., 1958. Tyler, Ralph W. Basic Principles of Curriculum and Instruc- tion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1950. Vroom, Victor H. Work and.Motivation.. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1964.. *— Yoder, Dale. Personnel Management and Industrial Relations. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-HalI, 1962. -l88- Articles and.Periodicals Andersson, Bengt-E., and Nilsson, Stig-G. "Studies in the Reliability and Validity of the Critical Incident Technique," Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 48 (1964), pp. 398-403. Archer, Vern B., Edelfelt, Roy A., and Herbert, Hite. "Point Points the Way: Project for the Orientation and Induction of New Teachers," NEA Journal, Vol. 54 (Oct., 1965), pp. 29-30. Blank, Lane B. "Critical Incidents in the Behavior of Secondary School Physical Education Instructors," The Research Quarterly, Vol. 29 (1958), pp. 1-6. Burns, Hobert W. "Success Criteria and the Critical Inci- dent Technique," Phi Delta Kappan, Vol. 38 (1956), pp. 73-75. Clegg, Denzil 0. "Work as a Motivator," Journal of Coopera- tive Extension, Vols 1 (Fall, 1963), pp.*141-148. Cooper, Bernice. ”Analysis of the Quality of the Behaviors of Principals as Observed and Reported in Six Critical Incident Studies," Journal of Educational Research, Vol. 56 (1963), pp. 410-414. Corbally, John E., Jr. "The Critical Incident Technique and Educational Research," Educational-Research . Bulletin, Vol. 35 (1956), pp. 57462. . "Reply to Hobert Burns," Phi Delta Kappan,.Vol. 38 (1957), PP. 141—142. Fishburn, C. E. "Learning the Role of the Teacher," Journal of Teacher Education, Vol. 17 (Fall, 1966), pp. 329- 331. Flanagan, John C. "Critical Requirements: A New Approach to Employee Evaluation," Personnel Psychology, Vol. 2 (1949): PP. 419-425. . "Critical Requirements Approach to Educational Objectives," School and Sociepy, Vol. 71 (1950), . "The Critical Incident Technique," Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 51 (1954), pp. 327-358. -189- Flanagan, John C. "Personnel Research and the Better Use of Human Resources," Personnel, Vol. 35 (1958), pp. 50-59. Fleming, Jack W. "The Critical Incident Technique as an Aid to Inservice Training," American Journal of Mental Deficiency, Vol. 67 (1962), pp. 41-52. Glickman, Albert S., and Vallance, T. R. "Curriculum Assessment with Critical Incidents," Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 42 (1958), pp. 329-335. Hedlund, Paul A. "Cooperative Study to Predict Effective- ness in Secondary School Teaching," Journal of Teacher Education, Vol. 4 (1953), pp. 230-234. Hughes, E. C. "The Making of.a Physician," Human Organi— zation, Vol. 14 (Winter, 1956), pp. 21-25. Jensen, Alfred C. "Determining Critical Requirements for Teachers," Journal of.Experimental Education, Vbl- Korb, David L. "How to Determine.Supervisory Training Needs," Personne1,-Vol- 32 (Jan., 1956), pp. 335- 352r McCall, Harlan R. "Problems of New Faculty Members in Colleges and Universities," North Central Association Quarterly, Vol. 36 (Fall, 1961), pp. 222—234. McCreary, Anne P. "Determining Individual Needs of Elementary Teachers as a Basis for an Orientation Program," Journal of Educational Research,.Vol- 54 (Sept., 1960), pp. 20-24. Myers, Scott, M. "Who Are Your Motivated.Workers?," Harvard Business Review,.Vol. 42-(Jan.-Feb., 1964), pp. 73-58. Schaffer, R. H. "Job Satisfaction as Related to Need Satis- faction in Work," Psychological Monographs, Vol. 67,. No. 14, Whole No. 364 (1953), pp. 1-2 . Steiner, Kelley E., and Cochran, Irene L. "Simulated Critical Incident Technique as an Evaluative and Teaching Device, American Journal of Mental Deficiency, Vol. 70 (1966), pp. 835-839. ‘ -190- Tracy, Norbert J., "Orienting New Faculty Members in Colleges and Universities," North Central Association Quarterly, Vol. 36 (Fall, 1961), pp. 214-221. Wagner, Ralph. "A Group Sitaution Compared with Individual Interviews for Securing Personnel Information," Personnel, Vol. 1 (Spring, 1948), pp. 93-107. Walsh, Richard P. "The Effect of Needs on Responses to Job Duties," Journal of Counselling_Psychology, Vol. 6 (1959), pp. 194-198. Reports Agricultural Extension Service. A Guide to Extension Programs for the Future: VThe Scope and Responsibilities of the Cogperative.Extension Service. Raleigh: North Caro- Iina State College, 1959. Allen, Lucile A., and Sutherland, Robert L.. Role Conflicts and Congruences. Austin: Hogg Foundation, The Uni- versity of Texas, 1963. Duncan, James A. Training Cooperative.Extension Workers: The Coordinated Approach.m.Madison: CoOperative Extension Service, University of Wisconsin, April, 1957. Federal Extension Service. Report of Programs in Extension Education for Professional Extension Workers. Washing- ton: U. S. Department of Agricuiture, ER&T-48, March, 1967. . Turnover of COOperative Extension Agents. Washing- ton: U. S. Department of AgricuIture, PDM-4, April, 1967. Hill, Maurice L. and COpeland, Harlan. Guidelines for Induction Training. Washington: Federal Extension Service, U. S. Department of Agriculture, ER&T-186, Sept., 1963. Johnson, Alton C., and Cassell, Roy D.. Appraising Personnel in the Coqperative Extension ServiCe-- Madison: National Agriculturai Extension Center for Advanced Study, University of Wisconsin, 1962. -191- King, William B. Certain Critical Requirements for.the Job of the.Vocationa1 Rehabilitation Counselor in Georgia. _Atlanta: State Department of Education, October, 1960. Lavery, William E., Heckel, Maynard C., and Johnson, Alton C. Developing Job and Position Descriptions in the Cooperative Extension Service. Madison; .National Agricultural Extension Center for Advanced Study,. University of Wisconsin, 1965. Michigan COOperative Extension Service.4 Your Appointment. East Lansing: Michigan State University, May, 1965. National Task Force on COOperative Extension Inservice Training. An Inservice Training program.for Coopera- . tive Extension Personnelu .Topeka: H. M. Ives & Sons, 1960. . Induction Training for County Extension Agents- TOpeka:. H. M. Ives & Sons, 1960. Olean, Sally J. Changing Patterns in Continuing Education for Business. vBrookline, Mass.: Center for the Study of LiEeral Education for Adults, 1967. The Cooperative Extension Service Today: AIStatemopppof.Scope- and Responsibility.t.Published for the 1957 Extension Committee on Organization and Policy (ECOP), by a subcommittee under the chairmanship of Paul A. Miller. UnpublishedlMaterial Alexander, Frank D. "Pretest of the Critical Incident Tech- nique." Unpublished paper, COOperative Extension Service, Cornell University, circa, 1965. Clark, Harry E. "An Analysis of the Training Needs of Wisconsin County Extension Service Personnel." Un- published Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1960. Coffindaffer, Billy L. "Experiences of Beginning Cooperative Extension Agents and their Implications for.an Induc- tion Training Program." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, UniverSity of Wisconsin, 1961. -l92- Corbally, John E., Jr. "A Study of the Critical Elements of School Board-Community Relations." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, 1955. Goldin, Melvin. "Behaviors Related to Effective Teaching." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wis- consin, 1957. Goyen, Loren F. "Critical Components of the Work Environ- ment of County Extension Youth Agents." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, in progress. Kessel, Robert M. "The Critical Requirements for Secondary School Business Teachers Based upon an Analysis of Critical Incidents." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1957. Kohl, Fred E. "Critical Requirements for Idaho Extension Agricultural Agents." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, in progress. Kosy, Eugene J. "Critical Requirements for Private Secre- taries Based upon an Analysis of Critical Incidents." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1959. McCormick, Robert W. "An Analysis of Training Needs of County Extension Agents in Ohio." Unpublished Ph.D. disser- tation, University of Wisconsin, 1959. Ranta, Raymond R. "The Professional Status of the Michigan Cooperative Extension Service." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1960. Other Sources Flanagan, John C. Personal correspondence, June 5, 1967. Turabian, Kate L. A Manual for Writers of Term Papers, Theses, and Dissertations.“ Chicago: Phoenix Books, The Uni- versity of Chicago Press, 1955. U. S. Statutes at Large. Vol. XXXVIII, March, 1913 — March, 1915. Webster's Seventh New Collegiate Dictionary. Springfield, Mass.: G. & C. Merriam Co., 1965. "IT'l'i‘llljllllllflllllflT