1/7 tJ/de" ABSTRACT ELECTRICAL SKIN CONDUCTANCE AND CARDIAC RESPONSES OF REPRESSERS, MIDLINES, AND SENSITIZERS TO AFFECTIVE STIMULUS FILMS BY Yonkel Goldstein Personality theorists have paid much attention to psychological defensiveness. One conceptualization of defensiveness which was reviewed here is Byrne's notion that defensiveness may be viewed as a behavioral continuum ranging from repressing and ignoring behaviors to sensi- tizing and distorting behaviors. PsychOphysiologists have devoted much attention to the correlates of an organism's accepting and/or rejecting stimulus input. This study was an attempt to investigate the relationship between Byrne's conceptualization of defensiveness and hypothesized psycho- physiological correlates of shutting out or accepting environmental stimulation. Two physioloqical measures--heart rate and electrical skin conductance--were analyzed. Literature was reviewed which suggested that heart rate increases when an organism shuts out stimuli; conversely, when an organism attends to stimuli heart rate decreases. Thereforerone hypothesis of Yonkel Goldstein the study was that when a subject was involved with a filmed vignette, heart rate should decrease. When a subject was not involved with a vignette heart rate should increase. Evidence from the literature was also reviewed suggest- ing that when subjects are defensive electrical skin conduc- tance (ESC) activity maintains its rate or increases. When a subject accepts environmental input ESC activity decreases. Therefore, a second hypothesis of the study was that when a subject was involved with a filmed vignette, ESC should de- crease; when a subject was uninvolved, ESC activity should remain the same or increase. The literature also suggested that repressers show more physiolOgical arousal than sensitizers, both before and dur- ing stimulation. This, then, became a third hypothesis. A fourth hypothesis tested in this study was that re- pressers' physiological patterns when they are involved with vignettes differs from sensitizers' patterns when they are involved and that repressers' physiological patterns when they are uninvolved differs from sensitizers' patterns when they are uninvolved. A fifth hypothesis was that subjects' involvement with the vignettes could be predicted from their personality type as measured by the Byrne Repression-Sensitization Scale, cardiac change scores, and ESC change scores. Forty-five female subjects from Education 484 classes were picked, on the basis of their scores on the Byrne Re- pression-Sensitization Scale, such that fifteen could be Yonkel Goldstein considered repressers, fifteen midlines, and fifteen sen— sitizers. Each subject was shown twelve stimulus vignettes while her heart rate and ESC were being recorded; simultan- eously, she was videotaped. After watching the vignettes and taking a memory test about them, she watched the video- tape of herself viewing the film, stopping the tape to describe her experiences while watching the film. A tape recording of this "recall" was then rated by two judges. From their ratings two vignettes per subject were designa- ted as "involved"--meaning that the subject verbalized an emotional reaction to them--and two are designated as "un- involved"--meaning that the subject failed to verbalize any reaction to them. A multivariate analysis of variance was carried out on these data. The results indicate that repressers are more labile than other groups--ie. they had larger electrical skin conductance standardized standard deviations than sen- sitizers or midlines--during the base rate and while watch- ing the films. The results also indicated that subjects have a lower mean skin conductance level when uninvolved with the vignettes than when they are involved with them. No statis- tically significant interaction was found in this analysis. A multiple regression analysis based on the same six vignettes for all subjects was performed, attempting to predict overall involvement ratings from Repression-Sensiti- zation scores, electrical skin conductance change scores, and cardiac change scores. For all subjects approximately Yonkel Goldstein one-third of the involvement variance was accounted for by Repression-Sensitization and ESC change scores. However, when separate analyses were run on the different personal- ity groups, ESC change scores accounted for approximately 76% of the involvement variance for repressers, 6% of the involvement variance of midlines, and 1% of the involvement variance for sensitizers. The implications of these findings were discussed. These results gave no support to the interpretation of car- diac activity on which these hypotheses were based; however, there were sufficient differences between this study and others so that neither do they damage the basis of those hypotheses. These results confirm the findings reported in the literature that repressers are more physiologically re— active than other personality groups. In spite of the lack of a significant interaction between involvement and person- ality group in the MANOVA, results are interpreted, based primarily on the multiple regression, suggesting that per- sonality groups do differ in their physiological correlates of involvementé-specifically as evidenced by ESC change scores. A clear pattern of a decreasing number of ESC responses were found in vignettes with which repressers became involved; no such clear pattern was found for the other personality groups. ELECTRICAL SKIN CONDUCTANCE AND CARDIAC RESPONSES OF REPRESSERS, MIDLINES, AND SENSITIZERS TO AFFECTIVE STIMULUS FILMS BY c 0“ Yonkel Goldstein A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements . for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1976 To Mom and Dad To paraphrase Sholem Aleichem - if I reach higher than my parents it's only because I'm standing on their shoulders. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Were it not for these people, this dissertation would never have been written. Norm Kagan - who in no small way has made my stay here such a productive one. To him I owe thanks for a million different things: for encouraging me with my first clients, fdr trusting me with his IPR, for provid- ing financial support, for giving me free reign in his lab, and most of all for treating me with such dignity and respect. Maryellen McSweeney - who has such a rare combina- tion of technical competence and dedication to teaching that she has become, for me, a model to be emulated. Tom Adams - who always gave unselfishly of his time and knowledge about personality types and assessment. Bob Wilson - who always allowed himself to be cornered somewhere between his office and mine and whose clear think- ing significantly helped this sometimes-befuddled experimen- ter. Joe Kertesz - for his technical assistance, for giving me access to his students and mostly for his warm support. Peggy Stansberry - who warmed up the machines, sub- stituted for me when I was called to the phone, and most iii of all who kept me company during some very long days. All the Winter Term IPR students - who showed up on time (more or less), let me stick electrodes on them, took my tests, and showed a real interest and excitement for what I was doing. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . CHAPTER I II III IV THE PROBLEM . . . . . . . . Introduction Need Purpose Hypothesis Theory Overview REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . . Heart Rate Electrodermal Activity (EDA) Summary and Conclusions WTHODOLOGY O O O O O O O 0 Physiological Measures Psychological Measures Stimulus Films Sample Procedure Design Experimental Hypotheses Analysis Summary MALYSIS O O O O O O O O 0 Analysis of the Results Summary SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSION AND 'IMPLICATIONS . . . . . . . . Summary Conclusions Discussion Subjective Observations Implications for Future Research Page vii viii . 49 . 90 APPENDIX . O C O O O O O O O O 0 Instructions for Rating Scale B Memory Test C Background Sheet D Vignette Typescript E Procedures for Operating the Polygraph BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . vi Page 105 113 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 3.1 Mean Involvement Ratings and Analysis of Variannce . . . . . . . . . . . 54 4.1 Descriptive Statistics for Physiological Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 4.2 Individual Cell Values . . . . . I . . 81 4.3 Multivariate and Univariate F tests Ho:l . . 82 4.4 Multivariate and Univariate F tests H0:2 . . 83 4.5 Multivariate and Univariate F tests HO:3 . . 84 4.6 Multivariate and Univariate F tests H0:4 . . 85 4.7 Cell Values Used for Testing H :5 . . . . 86 0 4.8 Multiple Regression Analysis . . . . . . 86 4.9 95% Confidence Intervals for Beta Weights . . 86 4.10 Simple Regression of Involvement on ESCC by Group . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 3.1 Design Diagram for MANOVA . . . . . . . . 72 5.1 Records of an Involved and Uninvolved Vignette for a Represser and a Sensitizer . . . . . 101 viii CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Introduction Human behavior, in one form or another, makes up a part of the subject matter for a myriad of academic dis- ciplines ranging from biology to aesthetics. However, traditionally in academia the various sub-specialties have had little to do with each other, as if those who were concerned with the appreciation of beauty had no object of study in common with those who sought to unravel the mys- tery of the optic nerve. Increasingly, as the arbitrary boundaries have become obscured, the need has been felt for inter-disciplinary study, and hybrid fields have devel- oped; psychoPhysiology is an example of such a case. Of course, in the case of psychophysiology, both "parental" fields are themselves quite diversified and, predictably, this "inter-marriage" occurred between the most compatible parts--experimental psychology and that part of physiology which deals with intact homo sapiens. However, once the precedent had been set, it was only a matter of time until the "second cousins," such as personality, clinical and counseling psychology, began to view their particular fields from a physiological perspective. (On the other side of the fence, of course, neurological and endocrine researchers began to look past their dissected Specimens, speculating about complex, living humans.) The advantages of this academic intercourse seem obvious--the broadened perspec- tives can lead to new conceptualizations and fresh strategies in attacking old, persistent problems. The pitfalls, unfor- tunately, are staggering: there is twice as much basic information to be assimilated, there are "translation" problems from one discipline to the other, and special measure- ment problems which come from adding apples and oranges. This study is one that belongs to that second set of "inter-marriages"--specifically between personality psychology, based on clinical assessment,and psychOphysiology; it is a piece of research concerned with translation. Physiologists have for years dealt with "orienting reflexes" and "defensive" reactions; psychophysiologists have conceived of various physiological patterns as being geared towards receiving information from the environment or shutting it out. Person- ality psychologists have classified pe0ple as being introverted, extroverted, defensive or open. Psychotherapists spend a great deal of energy in trying to assess when an individual is being defensive. On an intuitive level it would seem that there should be a fair amount of overlap between many of these concepts. If some equivalence could be established and specific patterns identified, this could both help to clarify a part of the relationship between mind and body and be a helpful indicator to the practising psychotherapist. This study is in the area of interdisciplinary translations. In its broadest sense this study is an examination of the rela- tionship between the psychOphysiologist's environmentally- oriented organism and Specific defense patterns known to personality theorists. Need The general need which this study serves is that of obtaining more data about the relationship of the "mind" to the body. Socrates (Plato, c 390 B.C.) spoke of a body and soul, emphasizing the latter because it is that which gives humans the ability to discern good from evil. Plato was a dualist seeing the soul as an independent entity attached to the body for the extent of the human life. Aristotle (c 340 B.C.), on the other hand, believed that body and psyche were indivisible. Hundreds of years later Descartes (1649) amplified on the concepts of Aristotelian psyche and body, but changed the emphasis so as to separate them. He considered mind and body to be totally separate, picturing mind and body to be entities in two distinct worlds,with the body being subject to natural laws, while the mind was free of such constraints and was alone capable of exercising free will. The immediate problem posed by this dualistic conception of reality was how--if at all--did the two enti- ties interact? Some, such as de Malebranche and Guilincx, denied that there could be any such interaction (Watson, 1968). Hobbes (1651) took an extreme materialist position, ex- plaining everything in terms of molecular interaction. Spinoza (1677), in contrast, took the view that "the order and connection of ideas is the same as the order and con- nection of things." He admitted that mind and body correlated with each other, but denied that events in one sphere could cause things to happen in the other. This view was later expounded upon by Fechner (1966) who viewed mind and body as differing aspects of one unity. He sought to quantify the relation of the two in order to show that increases in mental sensation correspond to proportional changes in physical stimuli. Nietzsche took an even more extreme view saying, "Body am I entirely, and nothing more; and soul is only the name of something in the body" (Edwards and Pap, 1965). The above is not intended to reduce to one paragraph the work of some of the greatest minds in the Western world. It is, however, an attempt to establish the fact that the relation of mind to body has been a matter of major intel- lectual concern for hundreds of years. This study will proceed from the philosophical position that there is a relationship between mind and bOdy; it is an attempt to specify one part of that relationship. There is a second, more practical need which this study bears upon. Psychotherapists spend a great deal of energy trying to be aware of, and making their clients aware of, defenses and resistances. Freud (1924) wrote: A vehement effort must have been exercised to prevent the mental process in question from penetrating into consciousness and as a result it has remained uncon- scious; being unconscious it had the power to construct a symptom. The same vehement effort is again at work during analytic treatment, opposing the attempt to bring the unconscious into consciousness. This we per— ceive in the form of resistances. The pathogenic process which is demonstrated by the resistance we call REPRES- SIGN. Having thus said that resistance to therapy is a mani- festation of a person's defenses (ie. repression), he under- scores the importance of resistance. Every step of the treatment is accompanied by resistance; every single thought, every mental act of the patient's, must pay toll to the resistance and represents a compro- mise between the forces moving towards the cure and those gathered to Oppose it. Neo-Freudians such as Reich (1933) have devoted great impor- tance in their therapeutic technique to dealing with these resistances and defenses. If physiological patterns indica- tive of defensive orientations could be found, it is possible that this pattern could be useful in makingboth clients and therapists aware of the defensive posture, thereby improving or accelerating therapy. Archer 3E El: (1971) wrote: If one of the basic goals of counseling and psychotherapy is to help clients to become aware of the incongruities between what they perceive and feel, on the one hand, and what they are willing or able to actually acknowledge and admit to themselves on the other, then it seemed logical and desirable to provide a client not only an opportunity to study his physical and verbal reactions to a real or simulated interpresonal engagement but to literally per- mit him to see what his internal responses were as well... it seemed reasonable that provision to the client of measures of his physiological responses during the coun- eling session would aid him even more in the process of self-discovery and change. If one accepts this reasoning it seems logical that it could be potentially of even more benefit to the client to have some interpretive help in understanding his physio- logical "read out." Purpose There are two major purposes of this study. The first of these is to test and extend the original psychophysiolog- ical work differentiating heart rate and skin conductance patterns in organisms attending to and shutting out the environment. The original work, done primarily by Lacey and his associates, in the case of heart rate, and Edelberg, in the case of electrodermal activity, primarily used psychophysical stimuli such as photic flashes and cold pres- sor tests; it is unclear to what extent the results can be generalized to vastly more complex interpresonal stimulation; this study will attempt such an extension. The second reason for doing this study is that it will clarify whether, in this area, the personality psychologist, who talks about "styles of interpersonal coping," and the physiological psychologist, who talks about "externally" and "internally-oriented organisms" are simply speaking two diff- erent languages or are really referring to unrelated phenomena. Hypothesis It is expected that there will be a moderate positive correlation between physiological patterns indicative of openness toward the environment and the level of involve- ment in brief scenes in which actors give a variety of messages to the subject. It is hypothesized that there will be a low positive correlation between scores on the Byrne Repression-Sensitization Scale and physiological response patterns. It is hypothesized that there will be a significant interaction effect in immediate involvement between physiological response patterns and sensitizer- represser classification on the Byrne scale, ie. a sensi- tizer's physiological responses when involved will differ from the involved physiological responses of a represser. Theory This study bears directly on two separate theories-- a psychological one concerning defense patterns and a psychophysiological one concerning cardiac and electrical skin conductance response patterns. The theoretical im- plications from personality theory will be considered first. According to psychoanalytic theory a neurotic conflict occurs when there is "a tendency striving for discharge and another tendency that tries to prevent this discharge" (Fenichel, 1945). It is to defend the ego from a realiza- tion of the abhorrent instincts of the id that defense mechanisms come into play. In that sense all defense mech- anisms are restrictive in goal--they seek to keep something from the ego. Fenichel (1945) writes: In fainting, the organism, which has been overwhelmed with too intense stimuli, shuts out the influx of further stimuli. The complicated defense mechanisms of the psychoneuroses may be looked upon as partial faintings. In fainting, all perception is blocked; in repression, some selected perceptions are blocked. He goes on to say: Neurotic characters, instead of reacting to their exper- ience adequately, respond more or less rigidly with the same reaction patterns. They are not only fixated to certain levels of instinctual demands but also to cer- tain mechanisms of defense. Various typolOgies have been employed to characterize typical response patterns. Horney (1937), for example, speaks of four basic means of protection--securing affection, being submissive, obtaining power, and withdrawing. Byrne (1964) sees a continuum of defensive behaviors which at one extreme seek to avoid anxiety-producing stimuli (eg. repression, denial) and at the other seek to control and manipulate the stimulus, thereby avoiding the anxiety (eg. intellectualization, obsessive behaviors, and rumination). Behaviorally, the former involve an avoidance of the stimulus, while the later are characterized by an approach which dis- torts; they are all defense mechanisms in that they seek to reduce the ego's experience of the anxiety. Other theorists, examining different concepts have described a dichotomy similar to the one postulated by Byrne. Hudson (1966), discussing differences between "convergers" and "divergers" says that the former's “defenses lie in the direction of inhibition," while the latter's "defenses lie in a subtle distortion of true openness." Byrne (1964) reviews a host of studies which find differ- ential recognition thresholds or differential accuracy scores for a variety of neutral and conflictual stimuli including words, drawings, Blacky pictures, and tape- recorded sentences. In these studies ease of recognition was varied by using different exposure speeds, illumination intensities, loudness of recordings, or by using successive carbons to decrease legibility or by increasing background White noise. A variety of defense indicators were used in&21.uding readiness to verbalize conflictual material on tilei 'TAT and sentence completion tests, ability to recall failures vs. successes in an experiment, recall of Blacky pictures, California F scale scores, ratings of Rorschach iarldi Machover Figure-Drawings, and psychiatric evaluations beifiied on interviews and case histories. The general find- :irlsys of all these studies were that subjects who perceive t1‘lreatening stimuli as accurately or more accurately than 11$utral stimuli respond in other situations with approach behavior, intellectualization, and sensitization. Those ‘Vrlo perceive conflictual stimuli less well than neutral £3timuli tend to repress and avoid in other situations. It is important to note that according to Byrne's t11'1eory both the repressing and sensitizing ends of the con- ‘tsznuum are considered to be pathological. Byrne (1964) whites: If each end of the repression-sensitization continuum represents an extreme of the respective defense modes, scores on the R-S scale would be expected to have a n r"? r) r1 AA in»- "fight . . IIA’. “div. . '0‘ a Q) (I) n u as...“ '1.- l 1“ v flu“ i n, x T. h...° :‘n 51‘ )5 l’\ ". 'V‘J ! h' “I O h.‘ “T"a 6.5: 10 curvilinear relationship with various indices of psy- chological adjustment. Neither over-intellectualiza- tion of conflicts nor denial of them should result in optimal adjustment . In summary, then, the relevant points from person- ality theory discussed here are: 1) there exists a class <3f Iaehaviors--defense mechanisms--which block the ego's Perception of anxiety-arousing stimuli, 2) peOple often 'adC>E>t typical response postures with rigid reliance on a JPaJthicular set of behaviors being indicative of neurotic adj ustment, and 3) one particular way of conceptualizing these behaviors is along a continuum from always controlling, approaching, and distorting the stimulus to another extreme of always avoiding the stimulus. The second theoretical set of underpinnings this est11:1dy bears upon is the relationship betweeen emotional Eirldi physical events in the human body. The first point in this theory is that there is a relationship. There is ample E3\ridence for such a connection ranging from the subjective feelings such as "butterflies in the stomach," headaches, 61:15 physical tenseness reported by individuals in response i=<> stressful situations, to more formal academic findings EiEMmciating physiological changes with a variety of emotional sgituations. In his review of the field Oken (1967) cites ‘SBIJbstantial evidence showing such diverse biological changes Eiss the secretion of l7-hydroxycorticosteroids by the adrenal 'c=<3rtex, aldosterone secretion, catecholamine (ephinephrine 'E‘IId norepinephrine) secretion, and a variety of autonomic ‘Tfiasponses such as differences in muscle tension, heart rate, 1 Mr- UVH‘I I..' in) O 6 K I) i it. A» I ‘-A 'v ”A WI 'r— (I) “‘5 13‘ ll systolic and diastolic blood pressure, respiration, skin conductance, and finger temperature in response to changes in the psychological environment. Unfortunately, the next step in building this theory-- determining the precise relationship between the physiolog- ical and psychological variables--is not nearly as clear-cut. Indeed, Dykman gt 31. (1968) write: "Psychophysiological research by competent investigators is remarkable for re- Su-‘|-‘t:.s which are inconclusive, contradictory, or paradoxical-- thus attesting to the complexity of the data and the diffi- cality of posing soluble problems." Although philosophers have dealt with this problem at least from the time of Plato, modern psychology only began addressing this problem with the work of William James (1884) who posited that prllzsiological states were evoked directly by affective stim- uli and that what the individual experienced as emotion was due to his own processing of bodily states. Cannon (1927) cllallenged this notion with his demonstration that partially 8evering the central nervous innervation of the viscera did not eliminate emotional behavior. Cannon hypothesized 3- chain of communication going from the viscera to the thal- a~Il‘tus, to the hypothalamus and, from there, to the cerebral <=individual's defensive be- havior in terms of an approach/avoidance continuum. More damaging to the R-S scale is Hoffman's (1970) study, fail- ing to find support for the hypothesis that as stress in- creases repressers will increase avoidance and senstizers will increase vigilance. Lefcourt (1966) made the observa- tion that the strongest evidence supporting the validity of the R-S scale comes from self-report measures lacking in behavioral correlates. In an attempt specifically designed to find a behavioral correlate Thelen (1969) compared the R-S scores of students seeking psychotherapy with those who were not. The findings indicate that "therapy-seekers" were more likely to be sensitizers than repressers. Further- more, males who remained in therapy more than four sessions were more likely to be sensitizers. Thelen concludes, "As evidenced by the behavior of seeking therapy, this study suggests that the R-S scale does have the behavioral re- ferrants, such as avoidance and sensitization, which have been questioned by Lefcourt." Another attack on the validity of the R-S scale comes from studies claiming that the R-S scale measures anxiety because of the high inter-correlation between scores on the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale and the R—S scale. Golin gt gt. 61 (1967) concluded the results of the two tests were prac- tically interchangeable, with scores being determined by two factors--emotionality and defensiveness. There is, however, evidence to suggest that this high correlation is not due to a mis-labelling of the R-S scale. Kaplan (1966), writing about a study which used the Taylor Manifest Anxiety scale in order to classify subjects, says: Despite their low scores on the anxiety scales, the low anxiety subjects in this Study did not appear to be . individuals who are free from the threat of experiencing significant amounts of anxiety (also Truax, 1957). Rather, on the whole, they appear to be individuals who attempt to ward off the threat of anxiety and other distressing affects by heavy reliance on the defenses of repression and/or denial. The high anxiety subjects, on the other hand, appeared to be individuals who deal with anxiety by thinking and talking about it. That is, their style of defense permits conscious awareness of anxiety-arousing ideas (also Spielberger, DeNike, and Stein, 1965). Dublin (1968) has suggested that sensitization may be a "measure of defensive failure resulting in a response characterized by lack of ego adaptiveness which takes the form of anxiety...and which has concomitant observable be- haviors indicating ineptness, hesitancy and indecisiveness" because when put in an ambiguous situation sensitizers, as compared with repressors, were "taken aback." However, this interpretation is clearly not consistent with the data pro- vided by Petzel and Gynther (1968) who found that repressers solved fewer anagrams when they received "ego-oriented" in- structions than when they received task-oriented instructions, while sensitizers exhibited the Opposite pattern. They con- cluded, "Sensitizers...characteristically 'face up to' even 62 small stresses of everyday life and thus may be more likely than repressers to deal effectively with the more severely stressful situation." It will be remembered from the earlier theoretical discussion that Byrne considers there to be a curvilinear relationship between his scale and psychological adjustment. Some of the above validity studies are indicative of that in that depending upon the group or behavior being studied, varying negative characteristics are found at either end of the continuum. Evidence against the curvilinear hypothesis, and in favor of a linear relationship between psychological adjust- ment and repression-sensitization (indicating that repressers are the healthier people) is found in the work of Byrne 2E.El° (1968) (surprisingly!)vnu>found that students who were sen- sitizers had a greater frequency and severity of physical illness as measured by a health survey. In addition, male sensitizers made more frequent visits to the health center than male repressers. Along the same lines, Feder (1967) compared psychiatric patients with medical-surgical patients and found a higher frequency of sensitizers in the maladjusted population and a higher frequency of repressers in the normal population. Tempone and Lambi (1967) confirm this observa- tion in a separate study finding more repressers in their "adjusted" (non-patient) group and more sensitizers in the "maladjuSted" (psychiatric patient) group. In support of the curvilinear relationship Byrne cites 63 the work done by Ullmann, using his Facilitation-Inhibition Scale. (Byrne [1964] considers the Facilitation-Inhibition scale to be virtually "interchangeable" with his R-S scale,' citing correlations of .76 and .88 between the two scales in student populations and a correlation of .94 in a neuro- psychiatric population. Furthermore, he points out that approximately half the items on the F-I scale are included in the revised R-S scale.) Ullmann (1962) compared male neuropsychiatric VA patients' F-I scores with those of male students. Presumably there should be a higher incidence of pathologic defense in the patient population than in the student pOpulation. As expected the patient population had a significantly higher standard deviation than the Students. (Student standard deviation = 6.50 compared with patient standard deviations of 11.22 and 11.44.) Other evidence mustered to support the curvilinear relationship between adjustment and R-S scores can be found in comparing a number of studies. Byrne (1964) discusses Megargee and Mendelsohn's cross-validating an MMPI scale designed to discriminate "extremely assaultive offenders from other criminals and from normals" with the R-S scale. They found that "extreme repressers are seen to fit into the pattern of chronic overcontrol which includes rigidity, conventionality, moralistic views, and (at least in the criminal group) a greater probability of psychosis." On the other side of the continuum Byrne (1964) cites the work of Ullmann and McReynolds, reporting a Significant correlation 64 of -.50 between F-I scores and anxiety ratings in psychi- atric patients, Joy's and Endler's work correlating MMPI scales and R-S scale scores suggesting that in comparison with repressers, sensitizers tend to be depressed, anxious, introverted, discouraged, and tend to respond to the test as would members of the opposite sex, and Byrne, Golightly and Sheffield's correlation between the California Psycho- logical Inventory and the R—S scale which indicated that sensitizers were less well adjusted than repressers. Look- ing over this sample of studies one can see that severe maladjustment is associated with both extremes, with re- pressers having a higher likelihood of acting out aggress- ively and sensitizers being more prone to anxiety, depression, and neuroticism. This argues for psychological adjustment to be found in the middle of the R—S continuum. Merbaum and Badia (1967) found partial support for the interpretation that maladjustment is equally associated with the two extremes of the R-S scale. They exposed male and female subjects to varying intensities of shock and measured their approach or avoidance of the stimulus. They found a curvilinear trend for their female subjects in which those who scored in the middle two quartiles on the R-S distribution withstood higher shock intensities than those at the two extreme quartiles. They concluded, "If, however, one interprets low-shock tolerance within an adjustment frame of reference, Byrne's (1964) prOposition...could be held tenable at least for female subjects." 65 Due to the far greater number of studies which find greater amounts of psychopathology at the sensitizing end of the continuum than at the repressing end, Byrne (1964) reluctantly concludes that greater psychopathology is found at the sensitizing end of the scale. However, the present author does not believe that this necessarily reflects a serious flaw in the theoretical base for the R-S scale. First, there is the possibility that the pathological con- sequences Of repression are most clearly seen in extreme groups such as the aggressive convicts in the Megargee and Mendelsohn study (most other studies used less extreme populations such as groups of students). More important, however, is the fact that the plethora of studies cited finding repressers to be healthier than sensitizers relies on self-report measures to determine psychological adjust- ment. By the very "nature of the beast" one would expect sensitizers to admit with much greater frequency than re- pressers, items which would detract from one's adjustment rating. Therefore, thoughtfluaquantity of research clearly suggests a linear relationship between adjustment and R-S scores, the evidence is not compelling. Stimulus Films The procedure of using films to stimulate physiological reactions is widely used in the past research (eg. Lazarus & Alfert, 1964 and Davison, 1963). Typically these films are pictures taken Of some particular event such as 66 sub-incision rites or motor vehicle accidents which the experimenter believes will be anxiety-producing for the subjects. Kagan and Schauble (1969), however, describe the production of a somewhat different sort of filmed stimulus: In order to create the simulated emotional Situations with which to confront the clients, professional actors were trained to portray the four basic types of affect with varying degrees of intensity, but to avoid use of words which indicate a specific situation or 'story.’ Each actor was instructed to direct the emotion (re- jection, affection, hostility, eth at an imaginary individual directly behind the camera lens SO that the viewer would see the resultant image as if the actor were talking directly to him. Thus, because of their deliberate vagueness and because they treat the viewer as a participant, they require the subject to participate actively in imagining him/herself in the situation. These films have been used as stimuli in one study done by Kagan and Millhouse (1975), a study done by Smith (1975), and in a series of pilot studies done by the present author. Sample Subjects for this study were drawn from the female students taking the Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR) classes (Ed. 484) during the Winter Term, 1976 at Michigan State University. Members of the class ranged in age from 18 to 35 with the modal ages being 20 and 21. Because of the various degree requirements, the class was largely made up of pre-teaching and pre-nursing students and others 67 preparing to enter a variety of "helping professions" such as social work, counseling, and music and recreational therapy. All members of the classes were required to take Byrne's Repression-Sensitization Scale. The distribution for the female class members on the Byrne R-S scale con- formed very closely to the one reported by Byrne gt gt. (1963), having a mean of 46.32 and a standard deviation of 21.47. Keeping within the limits used by Byrne all indivi- duals scoring 69 or above were designated as sensitizers; those scoring between 42 and 52 were considered midlines, and those scoring 22 or below were designated as repressers. Only those persons whose scores fell into one of these three grouping were retained as subjects; all the data from the other students were discarded. One hundred three students from five classes went through the entire experimental procedure. Five were elim- inated as possible experimental subjects because they were taking medication known possibly to affect heart rate. One was eliminated because of poor mechanical recording and one was eliminated because she was pregnant. One was eliminated because she became so emotionally upset watching the first two vignettes that the experimenter decided it was not in her best interests to continue. This left ninety-five potential subjects. Forty-five (fifteen per personality sub-group) were selected because they met the criteria for inclusion in one of Byrne's sub-groups and allowed the groups 68 to be of equal sizes. Procedure During their regularly scheduled IPR classes the pro- cedure of the experiment was explained. It was pointed out that the personality test and physiological feedback might be useful to individuals seeking to know themselves better. All questions concerning the purpose of the experiment were deferred until after all the data had been gathered; a group discussion about the experiment was scheduled for the end of the term. It was also pointed out that though going through the procedure was mandatory to get credit for the class, inclusion of any data in the experimental analysis was Optional. If anyone had any objection to being in- cluded, they were instructed to inform the experimenter after their experimental session and their data would not be included; no one exercised this Option. During the preparatory session a schedule was passed around for class members to sign-up for one two-hour block during the next six weeks. Because progesterone changes occuring during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle are known to decrease electrical skin activity (Mackinnon and Harrison, 1961) the women were asked to sign-up between five and ten days after the end of their menstrual period. For convenience in attaching the electrodes the students were asked to wear a short-sleeve or sleeveless blouse and not to wear panty hose, tights, or stockings. 69 At the end Of the preparatory session all the students were asked to fill out a Byrne R-S questionnaire. The pur- pose of the questionnaire was left ambiguous, but a complete interpretation was promised later in the term. When the subject arrived she was asked to wash her hands thoroughly with Ivory soap and water. While she was washing her hands the experimenter made sure that only two fluorescent lights were on in the experimental room and that the temperature was between 72° and 74° F. When the subject returned to the room she was asked to fill out a background information sheet (Appendix C). Next, two dis- posable silver/silver chloride electrodes (manufactured by American HOSpital Supply, No. 65375-030) were attached to the outer surface of either arm, approximately four inches below the shoulder, after the area was rubbed by a swab of dry cotton. The electrodes were connected to an EKG tach- ograph, Grass Model 7P4D. The electrical signal was ampli- fied by a Grass Model 7DAE DE amplifier and recorded by one pen on a four-channel Grass, Model 7WC8PA strip chart re- corder at a speed of 100 mm per second. After the cardiac electrodes were attached two more disposable silver/silver chloride electrodes were attached, one to the hypothenar eminence of the left hand after it was rubbed lightly with a dry cotton swab, and the other to the volar surface of the left forearm, after it was rubbed vigorously with a dry cotton swab. The electrodes were then connected to a Hagfors bridge (a constant voltage bridge) 70 which in turn was connected to a Grass, Model 7Plb, low level DC pre-amplifier. Finally, the signal was amplified by a Grass DC amplifier, Model 7DAE, and recorded on one channel of a four channel Grass strip chart recorder, Model 7WC8PA, at a paper speed of 100 mm. In addition to these two sets of electrodes one 3/8 inch, silver/silver chloride electrode was attached, using Unibase paste (produced by Parke-Davis) mixed with 50% by volume with Normal saline, to the left leg of the subject after the leg was rubbed with alcohol and connected to a ground. As each electrode was attached to the subject, its purpose was explained. It was explained to the subject that after the recorder was calibrated, a record of her base resting level would be made. Therefore, she was in- structed to relax with her eyes Open for the next several minutes and to acclimate herself to the surroundings. Be- fore the start of the rest period the subject was told over an office intercom system: "You will now have five minutes to relax. After that the films will appear on the screen in front of you. Make an attempt to imagine yourself alone with the person on the screen. If it helps to fantasize a specific situation in which to place that person go ahead and do so. But above all, do your best to react to that per- son as if he or she were Speaking to you personally." The vignettes were then played with variable lengths of blank leader between the filmed segments. (The typescript is included 71 in Appendix D). The start and finish of each vignette and of the rest period was marked on the physiological record by the experimenter. The projector which was used was a 16mm Bell and Howell Autoload. It was located in the in- strumentation room and projected on a translucent screen approximately four feet in front of the subject. The vignettes depicted hostility, manipulation, sexual advance, rejection, and acceptance. As the films were being shown the subject was video-recorded on one-half inch Ampex equipment, so that the recording was a record of the film sound track, any verbalizations the subject made, and the face of the subject. (Exact procedures for running the polygraph may be found in Appendix E.) After the subject viewed all the vignettes the elec- trodes were removed and she was given a chance to clean up. She was then read a questionnaire asking Specific questions about cognitive details of the films. After this the videotape of the subject watching the vignettes was played for her. She was encouraged to stOp the tape and verbalize what she recalled experiencing, accOrding to the recall process as described by Kagan gt gt. (1969). In the final stage of the session, she was shown her printout of her physiological responses and they were discussed with her in view of her reported experience. 72 Design This study is a predictive piece of research in that the essential question being asked is does knowledge of a subject's physiological pattern and the subject's class- ification on the Byrne R-S Scale, enable one to predict involvement or non-involvement with the vignettes? The design, diagrammed in Figure 3.1, is a group by vignette by subject by measure design. Baseline V V l 2’ ’ 12 M1 M2 M3 M1 M2 M3 M4 M5 M6 M7 Group 1 S1 (Repressers) 8 Group 2 15 (Midlines) 516 S Group 3 30 (Sensitizers) S31 Where: Subject Vignette Mean heart rate Mean Electrical skin conductance . Standardized electrical skin conductance standard deviation autonomic lability score cardiac change score electrical skin conductance change score Judges' rating as to involvement s::s:z 3:3::<:m ~Jouncs (ABDP' II II II Figure 3.1 Design Diagram for MANOVA 73 It Should be noted that the judges' involvement rating (M7) is dependent on the subject's response and as such cannot be controlled by the experimenter. There- fore, in order to make an analysis possible with this measure two vignettes were chosen from each category-- involved and uninvolved. The number two was chosen so as to allow the analysis to retain as much data as possible per subject while maximizing the number of subjects re- tained in the analysis. The statistical justification for treating the vignettes as equivalent is that no difference in involvement for any of the vignettes were found; neither was there a vignette by personality group interaction (see Table 3. 1). Experimental Hypotheses I. The groups of sensitizers (G1), Repressers (G3) and mid- lines (G2) will differ in cardiac and ESC activity during the base period. A. Mean heart rates for the groups will not be the same. B. Mean ESC levels for the groups will not be the same. C. Mean standardized ESC standard deviations for the groups will not be the same. II. The groups of sensitizers (G1), repressers (G3) and mid— lines (G2) will differ in cardiac and ESC activity while viewing the film. A. Mean heart rates for the three groups will not be 74 the same. B. Mean autonomic lability scores for the three groups will not be the same. C. Mean cardiac change scores for the three groups will not be the same. D. Mean ESC levels for the three groups will not be the same. E. ESC standardized standard deviations for the three groups will not be the same. F. Mean ESC change scores for the three groups will not be the same. III. The physiological variables will differ on those vignettes with which the subject is rated as being involved from those vignettes on which the subject is rated as being uninvolved. A. Mean heart rates for involved and uninvolved vignettes will not be the same. B. Mean autonomic lability scores for involved and uninvolved vignettes will not be the same. C. Mean cardiac change scores for involved and un- involved vignettes will not be the same. D. Mean ESC levels for involved and uninvolved vignettes will not be the same. E. ESC standardized standard deviations for involved and uninvolved vignettes will not be the same. F. Mean ESC change scores for the involved and unin- volved vignettes will not be the same. 75 IV. There will be an interaction between personality types and involvement ratings. A. Repressers will have their highest scores on all physiological variables when they are uninvolved with vignettes. Their lowest physiological scores will be when they are involved with vignettes. Sensitizers will have their highest scores on all physiological variables when they are involved with the vignettes; their lowest physio- logical Scores will be when they are uninvolved with the vignettes. V. Physiological pattern, as defined by cardiac and ESC change scores, and the Byrne Scale will together predict a subject's involvement with the vignettes. A. Subjects with high R-S scores and high cardiac and ESC change scores Should receive higher involvement ratings. Subjects with low R-S scores and low cardiac and ESC change scores will receive lower involvement ratings. Analysis The four moments about the mean were initially com- puted in order to determine the applicability of parametric test statistics. I Hypotheses I, II, III, IV were tested using a multi- variate F test. When significant differences were found univariate and step down F tests were also performed. In addition, when significant differences were found in the groups effect Tukey post-hoc comparisons were carried out. 76 The repeated measure design employed allows for testing a group's effect, an involvement-non-involvement effect, and a groups by involvement interaction. Hypothesis V was tested with a step—wise multiple re- gression analysis using the model: y = B0 + Ble + BZX2 + B3X3 where y - level of involvement X1 = R-S score X2 = ESC change score X3 = cardiac change score It Should be noted that the data on which this last analysis is based are different from the data used in testing the first four hypothesis. Here, instead of using two in- volved and two non-involved vignettes for each subject (which would result in an equal involvement level for every- one) six vignettes for which the physiological records were clear for all subjects were chosen and the involvement and physiological variables were averaged across the six vignettes. Thus, this analysis relies on scores for each subject, as opposed to scores for each vignette. Summar Forty-five female students from Interpersonal Pro- cess Recall classes were chosen as subjects on the basis of their scores on the Byrne Repression-Sensitization (R—S) Scale. They were videotaped and had their heart rates and electrical skin conductance levels recorded as they watched twelve vignettes geared to provoke some emotion- al reaction. The videotape of them watching the 77 films was then played back for them and they were encouraged to describe their recollection of what they were experienc- ing as they watched the tape. This was in turn tape-recorded and the tape was rated by two judges. A multivariate re- peated measures design was used to test for a group effect, during the baseline, a group effect while watching the films, an involvement effect while watching the films and a group by involvement interaction. In addition, a multiple regression was used to predict level of involvement on the basis of the R-S Scale score, change in heart rate, and change in skin conductance. The rationales for the validities of the Byrne Scale and the involvement scale were also discussed. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS In this chapter the results of the study with the appropriate Statistical analyses are presented. The gen- eral outline is to present first the descriptive statistics which were used to summarize the data and to determine whether parametric or non-parametric tests would be most appropriate. Next the results of the repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance are presented, followed where appropriate, by the results of post-hoc Tukey compar- isons. Lastly, a regression analysis is presented in which involvement levels are predicted from personality test scores and electrical skin conductance change scores. Table 4.1 lists the means, the variances, the skewnesses, and the kurtoses of all the physiological variables. Means and variances were calculated according to standard formulas. Skewnesses and kurtoses were calculated such that a normal distribution would have a score of zero. It will be noted that the only variables which seriously depart from a normal distribution are the log transformations of the electrical skin conductance levels. Since the only purpose of carrying out the log transformations was to 78 Table 4.1 Descriptive Statistics for Physiological Variables 79 Variable Mean Variance Skewness Kurtosis Base Mean Heart Rate 86.556 72.071 .420 -.036 Base Mean Skin Con- ductance 20.418 67.651 .027 -.217 Base Log Mean Skin Conductance 1.262 .056 -l.682 3.661 Base ESC Standard Deviation 12.531 60.024 .621 -.262 Involved Mean Heart Rate 88.356 92.109 -.270 .544 Involved Mean Skin Conductance 21.568 54.533 -.294 -.180 Involved Log Mean Skin ‘ Conductance 1.293 .043 -2.086 6.108 Involved ESC Standard Deviation 11.531 57.993 .534 -.566 Involved Heart Rate Lability 51.167 43.943 .116 .002 Involved Heart Rate Change 2.289 15.221 -.736 .313 Involved ESC Change .244 1.393 .051 -.440 Uninvolved Mean Heart Rate 90.822 137.240 1.018 .342 Uninvolved Mean Skin Conductance 19.046 38.432 -.l35 .872 Uninvolved Log Mean Skin Conductance 1.238 .035 -l.764 4.114 Uninvolved ESC Standard Deviation 11.162 41.423 .610 .004 Uninvolved Heart Rate Lability 49.032 65.223 .074 .127 Uninvolved Heart Change 3.333 19.614 -.510 .017 Uninvolved ESC Change -.111 1.385 .344 .016 80 increase the approximation to the normal distribution a decision was made to replace the log transformations with the original skin conductance levels. Making this substi- tution, it is then clear that nowhere is the assumption of normality too badly violated and therefore a parametric statistical analysis is appropriate. A multivariate analysis of variance is carried out next. Four questions are asked: 1) is there a difference between groups on base levels of the physiological variables, 2) is there a groups effect during stimulation on the physi- ological variables, 3) is there an involvement effect during stimulation on the physiological variables, and 4) is there a group by involvement interaction? The individual cell values are presented in Table 4.2. The MANOVA tables are presented in Tables 4.3, 4.4, 4.5 and 4.6. When apprOpriate univariate add Step down F ratios are included, along with Tukey post-hoc comparisons. Because the four hypotheses mentioned abOve are being tested, in order to keep the overall probability of making a Type I error at or below .05 it is necessary to set indi- vidual critical values at .0125. Furthermore, the overall alpha level for a given hypothesis (.0125) must be divided by the number of variables that contribute to the mean vector (three in the first case--mean heart rate, mean skin conduc- tance and standardized standard deviation-- and six in the others--mean heart rate, mean skin conductance, ESC stand- ardized standard deviation, heart rate lability, heart rate 81 Table 4.2 Individual Cell Values Variable Total Repressers Midlines Sensitizers BHR 86.5556 88.333 86.0667 85.2667 BMSC 20.4178 25.0067 17.4133 18.8333 STBES .0004 .8700 -.2513 -.6173 IMHR 88.3556 89.3333 86.7333 89.0000 UMHR 90.8222 92.8333 89.3667 90.2667 IMSC 21.5678 26.1800 20.3600 18.1633 UMSC 19.0456 21.9667 18.4800 16.6900 ISTSC -.0000 .8467 -.3360 -.5107 USTSC .0004 .8313 -.5693 -.2607 IHRL 49.999 56.4826 48.3429 45.1736 UHRL 50.0644 52.879 48.2309 49.0832 IHRC 2.2889 1.2333 2.6000 3.0333 UHRC 3.3333 3.7333 2.4333 3.8333 IESCC .2444 .8333 -.1000 0 UESCC -.1111 -.5667 .1000 .1333 Key: BHR = Base Heart Rate, BMSC Base Mean Skin Conductance, STBES standardized base electrical Skin conductance ard deviation, IMHR = involved mean heart rate, UMHR = unin- volved mean heart rate, IMSC = involved mean skin conductance, UMHR = uninvolved mean skin conductance, ISTSC = involved standardized Skin conductance standard deviation, USTSC = uninvolved standardized skin conductance standard deviation, IHRL = involved heart rate lability, UHRL = uninvolved heart rate lability, IHRC = involved heart rate change, UMHRC = uninvolved heart rate change, IESCC involved electrical skin conductance change, UESCC = uninvolved electrical Skin con- ductance change Stand- 82 lability, heart rate change and ESC change scores) leaving a critical value. of .0041 or .0021. Finally, the reader is cautioned in interpreting step down F ratios, that these ratios provide residual tests of Significance so that when statistical significance is reached in the case of one var- iable one may not validly continue to test successive var— iables. The reader is also cautioned that Tukey post-hoc comparisons were performed at the .05 level of significance, considerably above the overall alpha level described above. They are included here as a data-snOOping technique to sug- gest hypotheses for future research and are not intended as formal tests of these hypotheses. Hypothesis 1: Repressers, midlines, and sensitizers differ on physiological measures mean heart rate, mean skin conductance level, and standardized electrical skin conduc- tance standard deviations during the base period. Table 4.3 Multivariate and Univariate F Tests H0:1 F Ratio for Multivariate Test = 5.3537 D.F. = 6, 80 P less than .0002 . Hypothesis Univariate P less Step P less Variable Mean Square F than Down F than Bas HR 37.9556 .5150 .6013 .5150 .6013 Base ESC 244.4629 4.1272 .0231 3.6380 .0352 Standardized Standard Dev. 9.0088 14.5475 .0001 12.6551 .0001 Tukey (.05 level) Sensitizers Midlines Standardized Standard Deviations -.6173 -.2513 Repressers .8700 83 The results in Table 4.3 indicate that personality groups do differ in their physiological base rates. Both the univariate and step down F ratios indicate that the standardized base ESC is a Specific physiological variable which differentiates between groups. The Tukey comparison suggests that repressers have a higher ESC standardized standard deviation than sensitizers or midlines. Hypothesis 2: Repressers, midlines, and sensitizers differ on physiological measures mean heart rate, mean skin conductance, standardized electrical skin conductance standard deviation, heart rate lability, heart rate change, and electrical skin conductance change during stimulation. Table 4.4 Multivariate and Univariate F Tests H0:2 F Ratio for Multivariate Test = 3.8515 D.F. = 12, 74 P less than .0002 variable giggtgziiie univifiate Ptfiiis Difiipr Ptfiiis Mean HR 138.1056 .3395 .7141 .3395 .7141 Mean ESC 698.0507 4.9063 ' .0122 4.6270 .0155 ESC Stand. 31.7270 12.9838 .0001 11.6389 .0002 Standard Deviation HR Lability 270.7167 2.0047 .1346 1.3181 .2791 HR Change 17.4389 .6509 .5268 4.1761 .0230 ESC Change .2667 .1080 .8979 .5277 .5944 Tukey .05 level Midlines Sensitizers ESC Standardized -.452 -.385 Standard Deviation Repressers 1.6780 5' 84 The multivariate F ratio given here indicates that personality groups did differ while watching the films. Both the univatiate and step down F's indicate that stand- ardized electrical skin conductance is a particular variable which registered this difference. The Tukey intervals suggest that repressers have a higher standardized standard deviation than the other two groups. Hypothesis 3: There are differences on the physiolog- ical variables mean heart rate, mean Skin conductance, stand- ardized electrical skin conductance standard devitaion, heart rate lability, heart rate change, and electrical skin con- ductance change between those vignettes with which the sub- jects were rated as being involved and those vignettes with which the subject was rated as being uninvolved. Table 4.5 Multivariate and Univariate F Tests H0:3 F ratio for Multivariate Test = 3.3683 D.F. = 6, 37 P less than .0095 . Hypothesis Univariate P less Step P less Variable Mean Square 5‘ than Down F than Mean HR 273.8000 4.1333 .0484 4.1333 .0484 Mean ESC 286.2722 16.1707 .0003 11.1645 .0018 Stand. ESC .0000 .0000 .9947 .2607 .6125 Standard Deviation HR Lability 14.4500 .1502 .7003 2.8771 .0979 HR Change 49.0889 1.1140 .2973 .1361 .7143 ESC Change 5.6889 2.0645 .1582 .7421 .3946 85 The F ratio in Table 4.5 indicates that there is a significant difference between involved and uninvolved vignettes. The step down and univariate F's indicate that mean skin conductance is particularly sensitive to this difference. It is unnecessary to perform Tukey post-hoc comparisions because there are only two groups--involved and uninvolved-—being compared here. The actual cell values for the one significant variable--mean Skin conductance, are 21.5678 micromhos and un- involved mean skin conductance involved mean skin conductance 19.0456 micromhos. Hypothesis 4: There is no significant interaction between involvement ratings and personality types. Table 4.6 Multivariate Test H0:4 F Ratio for Multivariate Test = 1.9631 D.F. = 12.74 P less than .0401 (No further statisitcs are given for this hypothesis because the null hypothesis is not rejected.) Judging by the F ratio in Table 4.6 the null hypothesis cannot be rejected; therefore, one cannot conclude that there is a significant interaction between personality groups and involvement . Next, the question "does a knowledge of a subject's R-S score, cardiac change score, and ESC change score allow a predicition to be made regarding the subject's involvement with the vignettes" is asked. Table 4.7 contains the cell values computed from the six vignettes used for the regression analysis. 86 Table 4.7 Cell Values Used for Testing H0:5 Repressers Midlines Sensitizers Involvement Rating .3467 .5780 .6593 Heart Rate Change 2.1000 1.9733 2.0867 ESC Change 1.6400 1.7800 1.6400 Hypothesis 5: The cardiac and ESC change scores and the R-S scores will predict a subject's level of involvement. Table 4.8 Multiple Regression Analysis l . . . - 2 . . F Signifi- Multiple 2 R Simple Variable Ratio cance R R Change R RS 12.630 .001 .476 .227 .227 .476 ESCC 8.185 .007 .594 .353 .126 .380 HRC .865 .358 .605 .366 .013 .173 From this analysis one may set up the following regression equation, using RS scores and ESC change scores to predict involvement level: y = .2408 + .0045 (RS) + .0293 (ESCC) where y = involvement 1evel,RS = Byrne Scale score, ESCC = electrical Skin conductance change score Table 4.9 95% Confidence Intervals for Beta Weights Constant .0964 < 8 < .3852 RS .0020 < 8 < .0070 ESCC .0072 < B < .0513 87 If one treats R2 as an indicator of the percentage of the variance accounted for by a variable, one will note that by itself R-S scores account for over 22% of the involve- ment variance; taken together with ESCC the two variables account for over 35% of the involvement variance. HRC is dropped because it does not appreciably add to the predice tion of involvement. If one examines the individual ESCC values for the different personality groups (Table 4.2) one notes that there is a much bigger difference between the involved and uninvolved scores fOr repressers than for the other two groups. Because of this observation, it was decided to compute the regression for the various sub-groups, although this was not originally planned. Because HRC did not contri- bute significantly to the predicition of involvement and the values in Table 4.2 gave no reason to think that HRC varied across R-S groups, it was dropped from the computation. Table 4.10 Simple Regression of Involvement on ESCC by Group Group F Ratio Significance R R2 Beta Repressers 41.206 .000 .872 .760 .0849 Midlines .927 .353 .258 .066 .0191 Sensitizers .180 .679 .117 .014 .0064 The szalues indicate that for repressers ESCC accOunts for 76% of the involvement variance; for midlines ESCC accounts 88 for over 6% of the variance; for sensitizers ESCC accounts for over 1% of the variance. Summary The results of the tests of five research hypotheses were presented in this chapter. The first hypothesis was that personality groups would differ in physiological base rates. The multivariate test at the .0002 level indicated that this was so. Step down F ratios indicated that the standardized electrical skin conductance standard deviation contributed most of any variable tested to this effect. The second hypothesis tested was that personality groups would differ in phySiological response to the vignettes. A multivariate test at the .0002 level indicated that this was SO. Step down F ratios indicated that standardized electrical skin conductance standard deviations contributed most heavily to this effect. Tukey post-hoc comparisons at the .05 level suggested that repressers have a greater standard deviation than the other two groups. The third hypothesis tested was that there would be a difference in physiological response between vignettes with which the sub— ject was rated as being involved and those with which She was rated as being uninvolved. The multivariate test at the .0095 level indicated that this was so. Step down F ratios showed that mean skin conductance in the involved vignettes was significantly higher than mean skin conductance in the uninvolved vignettes. The fourth hypothesis tested 89 was that there was an interaction between personality groups and involvement with the vignettes. This was not confirmed by the data. The fifth hypothesis tested was that a linear multiple regression equation could be written which would predict involvement from Repression-Sensitization scores, electrical skin conductance (ESC) change scores, and heart rate change scores. An equation was written accounting for approximately one third of the involvement variance using RS scores and ESC change scores. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSION, DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS Summary Personality theorists have paid much attention to psychological defensiveness. One conceptualization Of de- fensiveness which was reviewed in detail here is Byrne's (1961) notion that defensiveness may be viewed as a be- havioral continuum ranging from repressing and ignoring behaviors on the one hand, to sensitizing and distorting behaviors on the other. PsychOphysiologists have devoted a great deal of attention to the correlates of an organism's accepting and/or rejecting stimulus input. This study was an attempt to investigate the relationship between Byrne's conceptualization of defensiveness and hypothesized psycho- physiological correlates of defensiveness. Two specific physiological measures--heart rate and electrical skin con- ductance (ESC)--were analyzed. According to the work of Lacey (1956), heart rate tends to increase when an organism shuts out stimuli; conversely, when the organism attends to stimuli heart rate tends to decrease. Therefore, it was hypothesized that when a subject becomes involved with a filmed vignette She is attending to the vignette, and therefore 90 91 her heart rate should decrease. When she is uninvolved with a vignette she is likely not to be attending to it, and her heart rate Should increase. The work of Edelberg (1970, 1972) provided the back- ground for making predictions about ESC activity. According to this work when subjects are defensive and shut out in- coming stimulation ESC activity maintains or increases its rate. When a subject accepts environmental input ESC acti- vity decreases. Therefore, it was hypothesized that when a subject is involved with stimulus vignettes her ESC activity should decrease; when she is uninvolved, ESC activity should remain the same or increase. The literature (Davison, 1963; Simal & Herr, 1970) suggested that repressers should show more physiological arousal than sensitizers. In an attempt to replicate this finding, differences between personality groups as defined by the Repression-Sensitization (R-S) Scale were hypothesized. Forty-five female subjects from Education 484 classes were picked, on the basis of their scores on the R-S Scale, such that fifteen could be considered repressers, fifteen midlines, and fifteen sensitizers. Each subject was shown twelve stimulus vignettes while her heart rate and ESC were being recorded; simultaneously, she was video recorded. After watching the vignettes and taking a memory test about them, she watched the videotape of herself viewing the film. She was asked to stop the tape and describe her experiences while the tape was being made, according to the recall 92 procedures described by Kagan gt gt. (1969). A tape re- cording of this recall was then rated by two judges. From their ratings, two vignettes per subject were designated as "involved"--meaning that the sdbject had verbalized an emotional reaction to them--and two were designated as ”uninvolved"--meaning that the subject had failed to ver- balize any reaction to the vignettes. A multivariate analysis of variance was carried out on this data. The results indicated that repressers were more variable than other groups--ie. they had larger standardized standard deviations than sensitizers or midlines-~during the base rate and while watching the films. The results also indica- ted that subjects had a lower mean skin conductance level when uninvolved than they did when they were involved. No statistically significant interaction was detected in this analysis. A multiple regression analysis based on the same six vignettes for all subjects was performed attempting to pre- dict overall involvement ratings from R-S scores, ESC change scores and heart rate change scores. For all subjects approximately one-third of the involvement variance could be accounted for by R-S scores and ESC change scores. How- ever, when separate analyses were run on the different per- sonality groups ESC change scores could account for approximately 76% of the involvement variance for repressers, 6% of the involvement variance for midlines, and 1% of the invlovement variance for sensitizers. 93 Conclusions l. A significant difference exists between R-S groups on the physiological measures tested during their base rates. Variability of ESC, as measured by the standardized stand- ard deviations, reflects this difference best. Repressers seem to be more variable than the other groups. 2. A significant difference exists between R-S groups on the physiological measures tested while they were watching the films. Variability of ESC, as measured by the stand- ardized standard deviation, reflects this difference best. Repressers seem to be more variable than other groups. 3. A significant difference exists in physiological response between those vignettes with which the subject was rated as being involved and those vignettes with which the subject was rated as being uninvolved. In particular, mean skin conductance was Significantly lower when the subjects were uninvolved than when they were involved with the vignettes. 4. No significant interaction between involvement and R-S groups was found in the data based on the two involved and the two uninvolved vignettes per subject. 5. R-S scores and ESC change scores account for 35% of the involvement variance. Heart rate change scores do not account for an appreciable percentage of the variance. 6. The data collected from the same six vignettes per sub- ject suggests that ESC change scores are an extremely potent predictor of involvement for repressers--accounting for 76% 94 of the involvement variance--while it is a poor predictor of involvement for midlines and sensitizers--accounting for approxiamtely 6% and 1% respectively. Discussion It seems clear on the basis of the findings reported above that none of the hypotheses concerning heart rate were supported. There can be two explanations for this. The first possibility is that increasing or decreasing heart rate is not a correlate of psychological involvement in this case because of some idiosynchracy of this study. Perhaps this is true because of the relatively low level of stimulation (compared with real life situations or movies of gruesome accidents) used in this research. Perhaps it is true because the subjects were females or because of some other demographic characteristic of the sample (one is reminded that Lacey's most impressive findings were based on male subjects [Lacey gt gt., 1963]). Perhaps this is true because of the varied subject matter of the vignettes. (The reader will recall that in one study Lacey obtained different physiological reSponses depending on the nature of the topic--sexual or aggressive--being dealt with [Lacey gt gt., 1963]). Perhaps this is true because the measurement procedures used here were not sensitive enough to pick up the small .cardiaC‘ changes Lacey does. The second possibility is that the hypothesis that increasing heart rate is associated with rejection of the environment 95 is simply wrong. Perhaps Obrist gt gt.'s (1970) explana- tion of cardiac rate in terms of metabolic requirements or Westcott and Huttelocher's (1961) explanation in terms of respiratory artifact or some as yet unformulated.theory has greater validity. This research however, Sheds no light on the probabilities of these possibilities. All that can be said is that the results cannot be taken to support Lacey's hypothesis; however, there are sufficient differences in the characteristics of the experiments so as not to be damaging to his hypothesis either. Results of this study are considerably more positive with regard to ESC. ESC variability does appear to be greater for repressers than for other members of the pop- ulation. This confirms the results of Davison (1963), and Lazarus and Alfert (1964). This great variability in con- ductance response on the part of repressers in at least suggestive of the Freudian notion that it takes a great deal of psychic energy to defend against something coming into consciousness. Clearly, however, this hypothesis requires further study. Surprisingly, mean ESC level was greater during vignettes when subjects were involved with the material than when they were uninvolved. The original thinking here suspected the Opposite--that when subjects were involved there would be a decreasing ESC level, manifesting itself as a lower ESC mean. In fact, looking at the data shows that that did happen. How- ever, involved vignette scores also reached a higher maximal 96 level at the start of a vignette than did uninvolved vig- nette scores, thus yielding the reported results. This suggests that perhaps ESC slope might have been a superior, more direct measure for the present purposes. This also suggests a possible explanation for some apparent discrep- ancies in the literature. For some groups ESC activity as measured by mean level may be decreasing while ESC activity as measured by responses (bumps) may be increasing. When ESC change scores are examined in the multiple regression analysis Edelberg's (1972) hypothesis receives. its strongest support. Taken alone, ESC change scores account for over 14% of the variance for all subjects. Taken together with RS scores they account for over 35% of the variance. However, if one just looks at repressers, ESC change accounts for a whopping 76% of the variance. This poses some interesting questions. The first of these is why is there such a clear interaction between RS and ESC change scores in the regression analysis, but not in the MANOVA? The reader is reminded that rejection of the null hypothesis was much more difficult in the MANOVA because more hypotheses and more variables were being tested, resulting in lower critical values. Secondly, the reader is reminded that the two analyses were performed on different sets of data. In the case of the MANOVA two involved and 'two uninvolved vignettes were arbitrarily selected for subject. In the case of the regression the same six vignettes were selected for each subject. This means that some 97 vignettes, such as ones with which the subject became only reluctantly involved, were included in the regression anal- ysis although they were excluded from the previous analysis. This difference concerning the interaction suggests some considerations which will be discussed under "Implications for Future Research." However, because the regression anal- ysis here provides the more powerful test and because the direction of differences between the groups on ESC change scores is the same in both sets of data, it is assumed that an interaction does in fact exist. The second question posed by the large difference in percentage of involvement variance accounted for by ESC change scores within the separate groups is why this is so. One hypothesis is that the answer lies in the differential meaning of involvement for repressers and sensitizers. A represser who indicated she was involved was not likely to have been defensive while watching the vignette. (Repressive defenses would tend towards denying involvement.) A sensi- tizer who indicated she was involved may or may not have been defensive. (Sensitizing defenses might manifest them- selves as over-involvement--perhaps not truly felt by the subject, but nonetheless, rated as true involvement on the rating scale employed.) The involvement scale does not allow one to differentiate between true involvement and de- fensive involvement. Therefore, when a represser is rated as being involved, one can equate the work "involved" with ”non-defensive"; when a sensitizer is rated as being involved, 98 one cannot make such an inference. If the data is seen in this light, it strongly sup- ports Edelberg's (1972) contention that "enhanced electro- dermal activity with retarded recovery may signal a defensive reaction." When there is a clear indication of psychological defense, ESC change scores behave as Edelberg would predict--1arger ESC change scores for involved than uninvolved vignettes. To test fully this hypothesis one would want to Show that when sensitizers and midlines are defensive their ESC change scores behave similarly. The problem in this study is that for those two groups there is no clear measure Of when they are being defensive. Again, the reader is reminded that this interpretation is not supported in that there was no significant interaction in the MANOVA. Therefore even the extremely low critical alpha levels, the relatively small number of subjects, and the fact that the results are in the same direction as in the regression analysis all suggest that this is a valid result, caution is advised. If this finding is correct, it also suggests a possible explanation for discrepant results in the literature. Most studies do not report the characteristics of their samples in regard to Openness to stimulation. It is possible that sub- jects in studies coming to differing conclusions differ in psychological defensiveness. 99 Subjective Observations The subjective observations of the experimenter fall into three broad categories: remarks about the subjects, comments about the procedure, and impressions of the data. Sensitizers tended to be almost hysterical, somewhat flighty, and highly enthusiastic. More often than not they seemed to be nervous about the procedure and Often asked questions to confirm that they really were not going to be given "Shots" or to ascertain whether or not the movies were "gross." Repressers, on the other hand, rarely verbalized any of these concerns. Interestingly, however,of the five people who missed their original appointments and needed to be re-scheduled, four were repressers. The fifth, although not a represser, as defined by the strict criteria in this study, tended toward the repressing end of the R-S score class distribution. There are some important costs and benefits associated with using the procedure employed in this study. Its biggest drawback is its length and cumbersomeness. It requires approximately two hours of time per subject and a substantial initial financial outlay for the equipment. Furthermore, it lacks highly discrete stimuli and relies on the Skill of a clinical "inquirer" (the person asking questions of the subject) which is difficult to keep constant across subjects. However, it is precisely some of these "drawbacks" which are simultaneously the greatest assets of the method. For example, 100 the lack of highly discrete stimuli is a function of its relatively high approximation to the real world (as compared with a photic flash). Its reliance on a Skilled inquirer allows the subject to deal with what was on her mind. The importance of this can be illustrated by the following anecdote: one vignette was of a black male threatening, with obscene language, the viewer. Most viewers saw this as an aggressive theme and, if they reacted, reacted with fear, anger, or some defensive posture. Two viewers focused on the fact--unti1 then completely unnoticed by the exper- imenter--that the man in the film had a gap between his two front teeth. As both these subjects had similar character- istics their reactions were ones of sympathy for the "poor, ostracized" individual. In all likelihood more clean-cut, precise experimental procedures would have missed these sorts of nuances. Finally, the records themselves deserve some comment. As suggested by the findings already presented, clear visual differences in ESC are found between the involved and unin- volved represser vignettes. Visual inspection also differ- entiates repressers from sensitizers. Examples, taken from actual records are given in Figure 5.1. These examples suggest that useful data may be obtained by using two addi- tional measures of ESC activity: slope and a measure which would differentiate between large and small responses (the reader is reminded that in this study all "bumps" of k micromho or greater were lumped together. 101 350m 35pm Uninvolved Represser Involved-Represser 20pm .20um Uninvolved Sensitizer Involved Sensitizer Records of An Involved and Uninvolved Vignette for a Represser and a Sensitizer Figure 5.1 102 Visual inspection of the data also suggests ESC mean levels differentiate between personality groups; they also suggest that ESC mean level is sensitive to an interaction between involvement and personality groups. The figures in Table 4.2 tend to support this, although this is not sup- ported by the statistical tests reported in the previous chapter. One reason for this may be a large error variance with respect to ESC mean level. This big error variance suggests that perhaps the personality categories used in this study can be further refined so as to reduce the amount of individual variation within the groups. Implications for Future Research One need for further research is the need to confirm or reject the specualtion that the large interaction suggested in the multiple regression between ESC change scores and R-S Scale scores is valid. This study should be replicated using fewer physiological variables—-perhaps only ESC change scores and ESC standardized standard deviations. A theoretical question is raised about whether ESC change scores correlate with psychological defense generally or only with repression. Specifically, some measure of sensitizing defense--which differentiates between true in- volvement and defensive involvement--needs to be taken and then correlated with ESC change scores. It would be ideal if the rating of "non-involvement" could differentiate between a non-defensive lack of involvement, due perhaps to disinterest 103 and a defensive non-involvement. A second theoretical question about whether the greater variability of represser's ESC levels correlates with Freud's notion of the expenditure of psychological energy to main- tain psychological defense deserves consideration. Measur- ing ESC levels in a variety of situations in which one would expect to find differential psychological energy expenditures would be helpful. Another question which is raised by this study is whether further refinement of the personality categories will yield stronger correlations between physiological measures and psychological constructs. In addition, just as psycho- logical categories can be further refined, so can the physiological measures. Discriminating amount of ESC re- sponse and looking at slope of ESC appear to be two such possible refinements. A variety of questions concerning heart rate and en- vironmental screening remain. Would positive results have been obtained with male subjects? Would dividing vignettes according to theme yield different results? IS some other translation of Lacey's "acceptance and rejection of envir- onmental stimuli" such as "approach and "avoidance" of stimuli, rather than involvement or non-involvement with the stimuli, more accurate? Finally, it would be interesting to learn if the ESC change score patterns associated with represser's Openess and defensiveness have any practical use. Using a represser 104 as a client, a future study might provide on-going feedback about ESC activity to the therapist, the client, and/or both, seeking to see if such feedback affects the outcome or rate of therapy. APPENDICES APPENDIX A Instructions for Rating Scale Rate as involved if the person says he felt a par- ticular feeling or wanted to do a particular behavior. The only exception to this is if the only behavior the person can identify is laughter or if the only feeling is that it (the vignette) was humorous. Rate as "hesitantly involved" any vignette where the person labels a reaction cautiously--ie. I would have felt scared--I kind of wanted to help him etc. Rate as unin- volved any vignette where the person says "I was bored," I didn't get into it, etc. or where the only reaction is laughter or humor. Also rate as uninvolved any vignette the person can't recall or labels no reaction. Any vignette not rateable in one of the above three categories rate as "can't be rated." 105 APPENDIX B Memory_Test RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A WOMAN ASKS WHETHER ANYONE HAS EVER SAID ANYTHING ABOUT YOUR SHAPE. What is the length of her hair? What is the color of her blouse? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A WOMAN CRIES, SAYING SHE THOUGHT YOU LIKED HER. What is her approximate age? Describe her hair style. RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A MAN APOLOGIZES AND SAYS HE FEELS BAD AND CRIESr Describe any facial hair that he had. What was he wearing? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A MAN SAYS THAT HE'S NOT SCARED OF YOU. What was the color of his top (word that person used in question before). Was he wearing any jewelry? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A COUPLE WAS LAUGHING. Who is sitting on your right, the man or the woman? Which one, if any, are wearing glasses? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A WOMAN SAYS YOU DON'T WANT ME AROUND HERE ANY MORE. What was the color of her blouse? 106 107 Describe any jewelry she may have been wearing. RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A WOMAN SAYS YOU HAVE A GREAT DEAL OF POTENTIAL. How did her blouse close? eg. zippered? How much of her could you see? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A MAN SAYS HE REALLY LIKES BEING WITH YOU. Describe any facial hair he had. What color was his shirt? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A MAN SAYS HE WANTS TO FEEL YOU. How long is his hair? How did his shirt close? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A MAN SAYS HE THOUGHT YOU REALLY CARED. Is he wearing a ring? During part of the vignette he looks at you and during part of the vignette he looks down. Which comes first? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A WOMAN SAYS THAT NO ONE HAS EVER BEEN SO NICE TO HER. How long were her sleeves?- What was the color of her dress? RECALL THE VIGNETTE WHERE A MAN SAYS HE'S GOING TO KICK YOUR.ASS. Describe any facial hair he may have had. Describe the pattern on his shirt. APPENDIX C Background Sheet Name Age Marital Status Are you taking any medication If so, what? When was the last time you ate When was the last time you had coffee or tea _ When was the last time you smoked a cigarette 108 APPENDIX D Vignette Typescript Female (middle-aged): Anyone ever said anything to you about your shape? Well, its different. Not that that's not fine, but it is different. Female (young): I thought you liked me. I really thought you liked me. I guess I know what you're thinking. I ah I thought that you really liked me and I uh oh wow... Male (young, red sweater): I feel pretty bad. I can't. I mean uh. I tried. I tried and I just uh can't do it. I mean I'm sorry. I mean I'm sorry I just uh my stomach is getting all uh. I'm, all I'm just getting all up tight and I couldn't do it. I'm sorry. I'm sorry. Male (young, red sweater): So what are you looking at. I'm not scared of you. You can't do anything to me. You piece of shit. Black male and female: Laughing at you. Female (old, green blouse): I know you don't want me around here. I know you just don't want me here anymore. You don't need me. Alright. You don't want me here, I'll leave. Female (young): You did pretty well, uh. I think you have a great deal of potential. Um. I think if you keep working at it. And keep trying. Um. You'll get much better. Male (young, with moustache): You know what. I really like being with you. You really make the day special you know. I just really think you're neat. You really are. Male (young, with moustache): I want you. I want to feel you. I want to lie next to you. I want to touch you. And I want you to touch me. Male (young, with moustache): I thought we had something. I thought that you really cared. I thought you felt the way I did. Why did you let me make a fool of myself? 109 110 Female (young, low cut blouse): This is really hard for me to say. You've been very nice to me. No one ever has been that nice to me. I just wanted to say I appreciate it. Male (black): I tell you man I can't take much more. Get the fuck off my back. I'm going to have to kick you ass. I'm going to have to kick your fucking ass. APPENDIX E Procedures for Operating the Polygraph Turn main power switch on at least one-half hour before machine will be used. Turn to "stand-by" the power switches of the two ampli- fier units to be used at least one half-hour before machine will be used. After leads have been attached to the subject turn power switch on cardiac amplifier to "on." Adjust driver sensitivity and base position if necessary. Base line should be at the mid-line and sensitivity should be such that -100 mv will move the.needle 2 cm. Turn polarity switch to "use" (up). Tachograph scale should be set at 60-180. Turn tackograph to calibrate - center, +2 cm., -2 cm, and mark on the record. Turn tackograph to AC low. Adjust threshold so that the record is clear. If this cannot be done turn tackograph to AC Fast. Re-adjust threshold. Turn amplifier on. Adjust driver sensitivity and base position if necessary. Base line should be at the mid-line and sensitivity should be such that -100 mv will move the needle 2 cm. Turn polarity switch to "use" (down). Turn sensitivity to .01 mv/cm. Bring needle back to midline using "zero adjust" on the skin conductance bridge. Turn sensitivity down to l mv/cm. 111 10. 11. 12. 112 Switch subject in on skin conductance bridge. Bring needle to approximately midline position by bal- ancing micromhos on the skin conductance bridge. Record the number of micromhos needed to balance on the record. Turn timer on. Run the machine at speed of 100 mm/minute. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLI OGRAPHY Altrochi, J., Parsons, O.A. & Dickoff, H. Changes in self- ideal discrepancy in repressers and sensitizers. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 61, 67-720 Archer, J., Fiester, T., Kagan, N., Rate, L., Spieling, T. & Van Noord, R. New method for education, treatment, and research in human interaction. Journal of Counsel- ing Psychology, 1972, 19, 275-281. Aristotle, On the Soul (Translated by W.D. Ross) In R.M. Hutchins (ed.) Great Books of the Western World (vol. 8) Chicago: Encyclopaedia Britannica, 1952 (c. 340- 322 B.C.). Ax, A.F. The physiological difference between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1953, 15, 433-442. Bahrick, H.P. Incidental learning under two incentive conditions, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1954, 11, 170-172. Benjamin, L.S. Statistical treatment of the law of ini- tial values (LIV) in autonomic research: A review and recommendation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1963, g;, 556- 566. Berlyne, D.F. Conflict and the orientation reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1961, 63, 476-783. Berlyne, D.E. Curiosity and exploration. Science, 1966, 153, 25-33. Berlyne, D.F., Craw, M.A., Salapatek, P.H. & Lewis, J.L. Novelty, complexity, incongruity, extrinsic motivation, and the GSR. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 66, 560-567. . Bernstein, A.S. Phasic electrodermal orienting response in chronic schizophrenia: II. Response to auditory sig- nals of varying intensity. Journal of Abnormal Psycho- logy, 1970, 15, 146-156. 113 114 Bittker, T.E., Buchsbaum, M.S., Williams, R.B. & Wynne, L.C. Cardiovascular and neurOphysiologic correlates of sen- sory intake and rejection II. Interview behavior. Psychophysiology, 1975, 13, 434-438. Block, J. Ego control scales in the MMPI. In W. Dahlstrom & G. Welsh (eds) An MMPI handbook: A guide to use in clinical practice and research. Minneapolis: Univer- sity of Minnesota Press, 1960. Bull, K. & Lang, P.J. Intensity judgments and physiological response amplitude. Psychophysiology, 1972, 9, 428-436. Buss, A.H. Psychopathology. New York: Wiley, 1966. Byrne, D. The repression-sensitization scale-rationale, reliability, and validity. Journal of Personality, Byrne, D. Repression-sensitization as a dimension of person- ality. In B.A. Maher (ed.) Progress in Experimental Personality Research, vol. 1, Academic Press: New York, 1964, 170-220. Byrne, D., Barry, J. & Nelson, D. Relation of the revised repression-sensitization scale to measures of self- description. Psychological Reports, 1963, 1;, 323-334. Byrne, D., Golightly, C. & Sheffield, J. The repression- sensitization scale as a measure of adjustment: rela- tionship with CPI. Journal of Counsulting Psychology, 1965, g2, 586-589. Byrne, D. & Sheffield, J. Responses to sexually arousing stimuli as a function of repressing and sensitizing defenses. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1965, 19, 114-118. Byrne, D., Steinberg, M.A. & Schwartz, M.S. Relationship between repression-sensitization and physical illness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1968, 1;, 154-155. Callaway, N.E. & Dembo, D. Narrowed attention. A psycholog- ical phenomenon that accompanies a certain physiological change. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 1958, 79, 74-90. Campos, J.J. & Johnson, H.J. The effects of verbalization instructions and visual attention on heart rate and skin conductance. Psychophysiology, 1966, 3, 305-310. 115 Cannon, W.B. The James-Lange theory of emotion: A critical examination and an alternative theory. American Jour- nal of Psychology, 1927, 99, 106-124. Carrier, N.A. & Orton, K.D. Skin conductance trends during learning by bright, normal, and retarded children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1964, 99, 315-317. Chase, M. Stress: The role of defensive predisposition (repression-sensitization) in GSR reactivity, defen- sive behavior and stress reduction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, 1967. Crider, A. & Augenbraun, C.B. Auditory vigilance correlates of electrodermal response habituation Speed. Psycho- Danish, S.J. & Kagan, N. Emotional stimulation in counseling and psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theogy, Research and Practice, 1969, 9, 261-263. Darrow, C.W. Electrical and circulatory responses to brief sensory and ideational stimuli. Journal of Experimen- tal Psychology, 1929, 99, 267-300. Darrow, C.W. The rationale for treating the change in gal- vanic skin response as a change in conductance. PsychophysiolOgy, 1964, 9, 31-38. Davis, R.C. Response patterns. Transactions of the New York Academy of Science, 1957, 19, 731-739. Davison, L.A. Adaptation of a threatening stimulus. Unpub- lished doctoral dissertation. University of California, Berkeley, California, 1963. Deane, G.E. Human heart-rate responses during experimentally induced anxiety: A follow-up with controlled reSpira- tion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 91, 193-195. Deane, G.E. Human reart rate responses during experimentally induced anxiety: effects of instructions on acquisi- tion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1966, 1;, 772-73. Depue, R.A. & Fowles, D.C. Electrodermal activity as an index of arousal in schizophrenia. Psycholggical Bulletin, 1973, 19, 233-238. 116 Descartes, R. Meditations. In L. Bair (transJ Essential Works of Descartes, New York: Bantam, 1961 (1649). Docter, R.F. & Freidman, L.F. Thirty-day stability of Spon- taneous galvanic skin responses in man. Psychophysi- ology, 1966, 9, 311-315. Dublin, J.E. Perception of and reaction to ambiguity by repressers and sensitizers: A construct-validity study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1968, 99, 198-205. Dykman, R.A., Reese, W.G., Galbrecht, C.R., Ackerman, P.T. & Sunderman, R.S. Autonomic responses in psychiatric patients. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1968, 147, 237-303. Easterbrook, J.A. The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological_ Review, 1959, 99, 183-201. Edelberg, R. Electrical properties of the skin. In C. Brown (ed.) Methods in Psychopysiology. Baltimore: Williams & Williams, 1967. Edelberg, R. The information content of the recovery limb of the electrodermal response. Psychgphysiology, 1970, 9, 527-539. Edelberg, R. Electrical activity of the skin. In N.S. Greenfield, & R.A. Sternbach (eds.) Handbook of Ps cho h siolo , Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc.: New York, 1972, pp. 367-418. Edelberg, R. Electrodermal recovery rate, goal-orientation, and aversion. Psychophysiology, 1972, 9, 512-520. Edelberg, R. Mechanisms of electrodermal adaptations for locomotion, manipulation or defense. In E. Stellar & J.M. Sprague, J.M. (eds.) Progress in Physiological Ps cholo , vol. 5, Academic Press, New YorE, 19737 155-2I0. Edelberg, R. 5 Wright, D.J. Two galvanic skin response effector organs and their stimulus specificity. Psychophysiology, 1964, 9, 39-47. Edwards, P. & Pap, A. (eds.) A Modern Introduction to Philosophy, The Free Press: New York, 1965. Elliott, R. The significance of heart rate for behavior: A critique of Lacey's hypothesis. Journal of Personalipy and Social Psychology, 1972, 99, 398-409. 117 Elliott, R., Bankout, B. & Light, T. Differences in the motivational significance of heart rate and palmar conductance: Two tests of a hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 99, 166- 172. Epstein, S., Bourdreau, L. & Kling, S. Magnitude of the heart rate and electrodermal response as a function of stimulus input, motor output, and their inter- action. PsychophysiolOgy, 1975, 99, 15-24. Fechner, G.T. Elements of Psychgphysics (vol. 1) (Transla- ted by H. Aller: ed. by E. Boring and D. Howes) New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1966. Feder C.Z. Relationship of repression-sensitization to adjustment status, social desirability, and acquies- cence response set. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1967, 99, 401-406. Feder, C.Z. Relationship between self-acceptance and adjust- ment, repression-sensitization, and social competence. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1968, 19, 317-322. Fenichel, O. The Psychoanalytic Theory of Neurosis. W.W. Norton & Co., Inc.: New York, 1945. Fenz, W.D. & Epstein, S. Manifest anxiety: unifactorial or multifactorial composition? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1965, 99, 773-780. Féré, C. Note sur les modifications de la tension électrique dans 1e corps humain. Comptes rendus des Séances de la Société de Biologie, 1888, 99, 23533. Flanagan, J. Galvanic skin response: emotion or attention? Proceedings of the 75th Annual Convention of the Amer- ican Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., 1967, 7-9. Freud, S. The Dynamics of Transference In P. Rieff (ed.) Therapy and Technique, New York: Collier Books, 1963, (1912), 105-116. Freud, S. A General Introduction to Psychoanalysis (Trans- lated by Joan Riviere) Simon & Schuster: New York, 1971 (1924). Gang, M.J. & Teft, L. Individual differences in heart rate responses to affective sound. Psychophysiolqu: 1975, 99, 423-426. 118 Gellhorn, E. Central nervous system tuning and its impli- cations for neuropsychiatry. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1968, 147, 148-162. Gellhorn, E. Further studies on the physiology and path- ophysiology of the tuning of the central nervous system. Psychosomatics, 1969, 99, 94-104. Gellhorn, E. The emotions and the ergotropic and tropho- tropic systems. Psychologische Forschung, 1970, 99, 48-94. Gendlin, E.T. & Berlin, J.I. Galvanic skin response correl- ates of different modes of experiencing. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1961, 91, 73-77. Germana, J. & Klein, 8.8. The cardiac component of the orienting response. Psychophysiology, 1968, 9, 324- 328. Golin, S., Herron, E.W., Lakota, R. & Reineck, L. Factor analytic study of the manifest anxiety, extraversion, and repression-sensitization scales. Journal of Con- sulting Psychology, 1967, 99, 564-569. Goodenough, D.R., Witkin, B.A., Koulack, D. & Choen, H. The effects of stress films on dream affect and on reSpiration and eye-movement activity during rapid- eye-movement sleep. Psychpphysiology, 1970, 99, 313-320. . Gordon, A. & Glass, D.C. Choice ambiguity, dissonance and defensiveness. Journal of Personality, 1970, 99, 264-272. Gordon, J.E., Martin, B. & Lundy, R.M. GSRs during repression, suppression, and verbalization. Journal of Consultisg Psychology, 1959, 99, 243-251. Graham, F.K. & Clifton, R.K. Heart rate change as a com- ponent of the orienting response. Psychological Bulletin, 1966, 99, 305-320. Gray, A.L. Autonomic correlates of chronic schizophrenia: A reaction time paradigm. Journal of Abnormal Psy- chology, 1975, 99, 189-196. Gruzelier, J.H. & venables, P.H. Evidence of high and low levels of physiological arousal in schizophrenics. Psychophysiology, 1975, 99, 66-73. 119 Haggard, E.A. On the application of analysis of variance to GSR data: I. The selection of an appropriate measure. Journal of Expsrimental Psychology, 1949, 99, 378-392. Hare, R.D. Denial of threat and emotional response to im- pending painful stimulation. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1966, 99, 359-361. Hare, R.D. Orienting and defensive reSponses to visual stimuli. PsychOphysiology, 1973, 99, 453-464. Hare, R.D. & Blevings, G. Conditioned orienting and de- fensive responses. Psychophysiology, 1975, 99, 289- 297. Hebb, D.O. Drives and the CNS (Conceptual Nervous System). Psychological Review, 1955, 99, 243-254. Hobbes, T. Leviathan. In R.M. Hutchins (ed.) Great Books of the Western World, vol. 23, Chicago: Encyclopaedia Bfitannica, 1953 (I651). Hodges, W.F. & Speilberger, C.D. Effect of threat of shock on heart rate for subjects who differ in manifest anxiety and fear of shock. Psychgphysiologyy 1966, 9, 287-294. Hoffman, H. Use of avoidance and vigilance by repressers and sensitizers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical PSYChOlQHXI 1970, 99, 91-96? Hord, D.J., Johnson, L.C. & Lubin, A. Differential effect of the law of initial values (LIV) on autonomic var- iables. Psychophysiology, 1964, 9, 79-87. Horney, K. The Neurotic Personality of Our Time. W.W. Norton & Co.: New York, 1937. Howe, E.S. GSR conditioning in anxiety states, normals and chronic functional schizophrenic subjects. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1958, 99, 183-189. Hudson, L. Contrary imagination: aspsychological study of the young student. .Schocken Books: New York, 1966. Israel, N.R. Leveling-sharpening and anticipatory cardiac response. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1969, 99, 499-509. James, W. What is an emotion? Mind, 1884, 9, 188-205. Johnson, L.C. Some attributes of spontaneous autonomic acti- vity. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychol- an, 1963' _5_6-’ 415-D20 120 Johnson, L.C. & Lubin, A. Spontaneous electrodermal acti- vity during sleep and waking. Psychophysiology, 1966, ‘9, 8-17. Jones, H.E. The study of patterns of emotional expression. In M.L. Reymert (ed.) Feelings and emotions: The Mossehart symposium. McGraw-Hill: New York, 1950. Kagan, N. & Millhouse, J. Physiological responses of IPR- trained and non-IPR trained teachers. Unpublished manuscript. Michigan State University, 1975. Kagan, N. & Schauble, P. Affect simulation in interper- sonal process recall. Journal of Counsleipg_Psycho- logy, 1969, 99, 303-313. Kagan, N., Schauble, P., Resnikoff, A., Danish, 8., Krathwohl, D. Interpersonal Process Recall. The Journal of Ner- vous and Mental Disease, 1969, 148, 365-374. Kaplan, F. Effects of anxiety and defense in a therapy- like situation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology; 1966, 19, 449-458. Kaplan, H.E. Social interaction and GSR activity during group psychotherapy. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1963, 99, 449-458. Kaplan, M.F. Interview interaction of repressers and sensi- tizers. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1967, 99, 513-516. Katkin, E.S. & McCubbin, R.J. Habituation of the orienting response as a function of individual differences in anxiety and autonomic lability. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 99, 54-60. Kintsch, W. Habituation of the GSR component of the orient- ing reflex during paired-associate learning before and after learning has taken place. Journal of Mathemat- ical Psychology, 1965, 9, 330-341. Kleinsmith, L.J. & Kaplan, S. Paired-associate learning as a formation of arousal and interpolated interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1963, 65, 190-193. Klorman, R., Wiensenfeld, A.R., & Austin, M. Autonomic reSponses to affective visual stimuli. Psychophysiol- Koepke, J.E. & Pribram, K.H. Habituation of GSR as a function of stimulus duration and spontaneous activity. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1966. 99, 442-448. 121 Lacey, B.C. & Lacey, J.I. Studies of heart rate and other bodily processes in sensorimotor behavior. In P.A. Obrist, A.H. Black,.J.Brener, & L.V. DiCara (eds.) Cardiovascular PsychoPhysiology, Chicago: Aldine- Athenton, 1974, 538-564. Lacey, J.I. The evaluation of autonomic responses: Toward a general solution. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1956, 91, 123-164. Lacey, J.I. PsychOphysiological Approaches to the evalua- tion of psychotherapeutic process and outcome. Re- search in psychotherapy, vol. 1. Proceedings of the American Psychological Association Conference, I958, Washington, D.C. 1959. Lacey, J.I. Somatic response patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In M.H. Appley & R. Trumbull (eds.) Psychological Stress: Issues in Re- search, Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, 1967, 14-42. Lacey, J.I., Kagan, J., Lacey, B.C., & Moss, H.A. The vis- ceral level: Situational determinants and behavioral correlates of autonomic response patterns. In P.H. Knapp (ed.) Expressions of the Emotions of Man, Inter- national Universities Press: New YorE, 1968, 161-196. Lacey, J.I. & Lacey, B.C. Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships. In P. Black (ed.) Physi- ological Correlates of Emotion. Academic Press: New York, 1970, 205-227. Lacey, O.L. An analysis of the appropriate unit for use in the measurement of level of galvanic skin resistance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1947, 91, 449-457. Lacey, O.L. & Siegel, P.S. An analysis of the unit of measure- ment of the galvanic skin response. Journal of Exper- imental Psychology, 1949, 99, 122-127. Lader, M.H. Palmar skin conductance measures in anxiety and phobic states. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 1967, 99, 271-281. Lang, P.J. The application of psychophysiological methods to the study of psychotherapy and behavior modification. In A.E. Bergin and S.L. Garfield (eds.) Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Changs, Wiley & Sons: New - York, 1971, 75-125. Lang, P.J. & Buss, A.H. Psychological deficit in schizo- phrenia: II. Interference an activation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 19, 77-106. 122 Lazarus, R.S. & Alfert, E. The short circuiting of threat by experimentally altering cognitive appraisal. Jour- nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 99, 195- 205. Lazarus, R.S., Speisman, J.C., Mordkoff, A.M. & Davison, L.A. A laboratory study of psychological stress produced by a motion picture film. Psychological Monographs, 1962, 19, 1-35. Lazarus, R.S., Speisman, J.C. & Mordkoff, A.M. The relation- ship between autonomic indicators of psychological stress: heart rate and skin conductance. Psychoso- matic Medicine, 1963, 99, 19-30. Learmonth, G.J., Ackerly, W., & Kaplan, M. Relationship be- tween palmar skin potential during stress and person- ality variables. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1959, 99, 150-157. Lefcourt, R.M. Repression-sensitization: A measure of the evaluation of emotional expression. Journal of Con- sulting Psychology, 1966, 99, 444-449. Lindsley, D.B. Emotions. In 8.8. Stevens (ed.) Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley: New York, 1951. Lomont, J.F. The repression-sensitization dimension in rela- tion to anxiety responses. ggurnal of Consultigg Psychology, 1965, 99, 84-86. Lomont, J.F. Repressers and sensitizers as described by themselves and their pairs. Journal of Personality, 1966, 99, 224-240. Lovallo, W. The cold pressor test and autonomic function: A review and integration. Psychophysiology, 1975, 99, 268-282. MacKinnon, P.C. & Harrison, J. The influence of hormone associated with the pituitary-adrenal and sexual cycle activity on palmar sweating. Journal of Endo- crinology, 1961, 99, 217-225.. McCurdy, H.G. Consciousness and the galvanometer. Psycho- lqgical Review, 1950, 91, 322-327. McReynolds, P., Acker, M. & Brackbill, G. On the assessment of anxiety IV. By measures of basal conductance and palmar sweat. Psychological Rsports, 1966, 99, 347- 356. 123 Malmo, R.B. & Davis, J.F. Physiological gradients as in- dicants of "arousal" in mirror tracing. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1956, 99, 231-238. Malmo, R.B., Shagass, C. & Smith, A.A. Responsiveness in chronic schiZOphrenia. Journal of Personality, 1951, 99, 359-375. Martin, R.D. & Edelberg, R. The relationship of skin resis- tance changes to receptivity. Journal of Psychoso- matic Research, 1963, 1, 173-179. Merbaum, M. & Badia, P. Tolerance of repressers and sensi- tizers to noxious stimulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1967, 19, 349-353. Merbaum, M. & Kazaoka, K. Reports of emotional experience by sensitizers and repressers during an interview transaction. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psycho- logy, 1967, 19, 1013105. Obrist, P.A. Some autonomic correlates of serial learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and verbal Behavior, 1962, 9, 100-104. Obrist, P.A. Cardiovascular differentiation of sensory stimuli. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1963, 99, 450-459. Obrist, P.A., Black, A.H., Brener, J. & DiCara, L. (Eds.) Cardiovascular psychOphysiology: Current issues in response mechanisms, biofeedback and methodoIEgy, Chicago: Aldine-Atherton, 1974. Obrist, P.A., Webb, R.A. & Sutterer, J.R. Heart rate and somatic changes during aversive conditioning and a simple reaction time task. Psychpphysiology, 1969, 9, 696-723. Obrist, P.A., Webb, R.A. & Stutter, J.R. Cardiac deceler- ation and reaction time: An evaluation of two hypoth- eses. Psychopsysiology, 1970, 9, 695-706. Obrist, P.A., Wood, D.M. & Perez-Reyes, M. Heart rate during conditioning in humans: Effects of UCS intensity, vagal blockage, and adrenergic block of vasomotor activity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1965, 70, 32-40. Oken, D. The psychOphysiology and psychoendocrinology of stress and emotion. In M.H. Appley & R. Trombull (eds.) Psycholo ical Stress, Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, 19 7. 124 Oken, D., Grinker, R.R., Heath, H.A., Herz, M., Korchin, S.J., Sabshin, M. & Schwartz, N.B. Relation of phy- siological reSponse to affect expression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1962, 9, 336-351. Orne-Johnson, D.W. Autonomic stability and transcendental meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1973, 99, 341- 349. Panek, D.M. & Martin, B. The relationship between GSR and Speech disturbances in psychotherapy. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 99, 402-405. Petzel, T.P. & Gynther, M.D. Task performance of repress- ers and sensitizers under ego-oriented versus task- oriented instructions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1968, 99, 486-487. Plato. Dialogues (Translated by B. Jowett) In R.M. Hutch- ings (ed.) Great Books of the Western World, vol. 7. Chicago: EncycIBpaedia Britannica, 1952 (c390-348B.C.). Porges, S.W. & Raskin, D.C. Respiratory and heart rate components of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 99, 497-503. Raskin, D.C. Attention and arousal. In W.F. Prokasy, & D.C. Raskin (eds.) Electrodermal Activity in Psygho- lo ical Research, New York: Academic Press, 1973, 12 -156. Raskin, D.C., Kotses, H. & Bever, J. Autonomic indicators of orienting and defensive reflexes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 99, 423-939. Reich, W. Character Analysis (translated by V. Carfagno) New York: Farrar, Straus & Gironx, 1972 (1933). Roessler, R., Alexander, A.A. & Greenfield, N.S. Ego strength and physiological responsivity: I. The relationship of the barron ES scale to skin resistance, finger blood volume, heart rate and muscle potential responses to sound. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1963, 9, 142- 154. Roessler, R., Burch, N.R. & Childers, H.E. Personality and arousal correlates of specific galvanic skin responses. Psychophysiology, 1966, 9, 115-130. Schacter, S. The interaction of cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional state. In L. Berkowitz (ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 1, New York: Academic Press, 1964. 125 Schacter, S. & Singer, J.E. Cognitive social, and physio- logical determinants of emotional state. Psycholog- ical Review, 1962, 99, 121-128. Schacter, S. & Wheeler, L. Epinephrine, chlorpromazine, and amusement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Schill, T., Emanuel, G., Pedersen, V., Schneider, L. & Wachowiak, D. Sexual responsivity of defensive and non-defensive sensitizers and repressers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1970, 99, 34-47. Simal, P.J. & Herr, V.V. Autonomic responses to threatening stimuli in relation to the repression-sensitization dimension. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1970, 19, 106-109. Smith, Jonathan, C. Meditation as Psyghotherapy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MiEhiéan State University, 1975. Sokolov, E.N. Neuronal models and the orienting reflex. In M.A.B. Brazier (ed.) The Central Nervous System and Behavior. New York: Josiah Macy, Jr. Foundation, 1960. Sokolov, E.N. Higher nervous functions: the orienting reflex. Annual Review of Physiology, 1963,.99, 545- 580. Sokolov, E.N. Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Macmillan, 1963. Stennett, R.G. The relationship of performance level to level of arousal. Journal of Experimental Psycholggy, 1957, 99, 54-61. 49' Speisman, J.C., Lazarus, R.S., Mordkoff, A. & Davison, L. Experimental reduction of stress based on ego defense theory. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964, 99, 367-386} Speisman, J.C., Osborn, J. & Laxarus, R.S. Cluster analysis of skin resistance and heart rate at rest and under stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1961, 99, 323-343. Spinoza, B. Ethics. (Translated by W.H. White, Revised by A.H. Striling) In, R.M. Hutchins (ed.) Great Books of the Western World, vol. 31, Chicago: Encyc10paedia Britannica, 19977 (1677). 126 Spohn, H.C., Thetford, P.E. & Cancro, R. The effects of phenothiazine medication on skin conductance and heart rates in schiZOphrenic patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1971, 999, 129-199. Stern, J.A. Stability-lability of physiological response systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1966, 134, 1019-1027. Stern, J.A. & McDonald, D.G. Physiological correlates of mental disease. Annual Review of Psychology, 1965, -]I._6_’ 225-2650 Sternbach, R.A. & Tursky, B. Ethnic differences among housewives in psychOphysical and skin potential responses to electric shock. Psychophysiology, 1965, 9, 241-246. Syz, H.C. & Kinder, E.F. Electrical skin resistance in normal and psychotic subjects. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 1928, 99, 1026-1035. Tan, B.K. Physiological correlates of anxiety: a prelimin- ary investigation of the orienting reflex. Journal of the Canadian Psychiatric Association, 1964, 9, 63-71. Tempone, V.J. Extension of the repression-sensitization hypothesis to success and failure experience. Psy- chological Reports, 1964, 99, 39-45. Tempone, V.J. Some clinical correlates of repression- sensitization. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1964, 99, 440-442. Tempone, V.J. & Lambi, W. Repression-sensitization and its relation to measures of adjustment and conflict. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1967, a, 131-136. Thayer, J., & Silber, D.E. Relationship between levels of arousal and responsiveness among schizophrenic and normal subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1971, 11, 162-173. Thelen, M.H. Repression-Sensitization: Its relation to adjustment and seeking psychotherapy among college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- ggy, 1969, 99, 161-165. Thetford, P.E., Spohn, H.E., Everds, J.P. Psychophysiological patterns of response in schizophrenia. Journal of Ner- vous and‘Mental Disease, 1974, 158, 104-111} 127 Ullmann, L.P. An empirically derived MMPI scale which mea- sures facilitation-inhibition of recognition of threat- ening stimuli. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1962, ‘99, 127-132. Uno, T. & Grings, W.W. Autonomic components of orienting behavior. Psychophysiology, 1965, 9, 311-321. Van Der Valk, J.M & Green, J. Electrical resistance of the skin during induced emotional stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1950, 99, 303-314. Venables, P.H. Psychophysiological aspects of schiZOphrenia, British Journal of Medical Psychology, 1966, 39, 289- 295. — Venables, P.H. & Christie, M.J. Mechanisms, instrumentation, recording techniques, and quantification of responses in W.F. Prokasy and D.C. Raskin Electrodermal Activity in Psychological Research, New York?' Academic Press, 1973, 2-124. Vigouroux, R. Sur 1e r61e de la resistance électrique des tissus dans l'électrodiagnostic. 99mptes rendus des Séance de la Societé de Biologie, 1879, 99, 336-339. Walker, E. Action decrement and its relation to learning. Psychological Review, 1958, 99, 129-150. Watson, R.I. The Great Psychologists. Philadelphia: J.B. Lippincott Co., 1968. . Wenger, M.A., Clemens, T.L., Coleman, D.R., Cullen, T.D. & Engel, E.T. Autonomic reSponse Specificity. Psycho- somatic Medicine, 1961, 99, 185-193. Westcott, M.R. & Huttenlocher, J. Cardiac conditioning: the effects and implications of controlled and uncon- trolled respiration. Journal of Experimental Psychol- ggy, 1961, 99, 353-359. Wiener, M., Carpenter, B. & Carperter, J.T. Determination of defense mechanisms for conflict areas from verbal laterial. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1956, 99, 215-219. Wilder, J. Stimulus response the law of initial value. Bristol: John Waight, 1967. Wood, D.M. & Obrist, P.A. Effects of controlled and uncon- trolled reSpiration on the conditioned heart rate response in humans. ggurnal of Experimental Psychology, 1964, 99, 221-229. 128 Zahn, T.P., Rosenthal, D. & Lawlor, W.G. Electrodermal and heart rate orienting reactions in chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 1968, 9, 117-134. HICHIGR 1 111 129 T ATE UNIV. LIBRQRIES lllllllllflllllWlllllVlllllllllllIIIWHI 3103215053