MARRIAGE ADJUSTMENT 0F COUPLES: A FEE-MARITAL ASSESSMENT, AND FOLLOW-UP 1N MARRIAGE A Disscrfohon for the Degree of D11. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNiVERSETY David John Rolfe 1975 LIBR A RY Midi-fin 3:2. tc University J] This is to certify that the thesis entitled MARRIAGE ADJUSTMENT OF COUPLES: A PRES-MARITAL ASSESSMENT, AND FOLLOW-UP IN MARRIAGE. presented by DAVID JOHN ROLFE has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in Family Ecology 122772102111 r/éZCKCl/l/ Major professor Date April 25th. 1975 0-7639 w"-.....-g.4p .an. vv-ao - u .o _ I.-.” ..... .-‘ a" —_ - -. .1.— V‘:'."_ n h....... \ n ‘l ‘ ~ I. -. ' A ~ A...: :‘u’VQV . ~5“' ' .A .. ‘."A. Vb. '. Hy Us: . I. u u “s v r 3.....‘9 h h\ 1;, AN ABSTRACT MARRIAGE ADJUSTMENT OF COUPLES: A PRE-MARITAL ASSESSMENT, AND FOLLOW-UP IN MARRIAGE BY David John Rolfe This study was designed with a twofold purpose. First to construct a practical marriage readiness inventory which would be simple to score, yet provide an estimation of the couple's preparedness for marriage. Second, to test out the inventory on a group of engaged couples. The couples were to be followed up and tested after one year of marriage to ascertain the relationship between pre-marriage and early marriage adjustment. A pilot sample of 144 engaged Catholic couples attend- ing a marriage preparation program in Lansing, Michigan were tested with Hurvitz's Marital Roles Inventory. The main samples consisted of: 166 couples attending a subsequent program in Lansing; 101 mostly Protestant volunteer couples in Christchurch, New Zealand. Pre-marriage adjustment was measured by an adapted version of the Marital Roles Inventory. Consensus on financial matters was measured by Rolfe's Financial =~---"-‘.s Imam masoooov 3.... “In ‘ ‘,. n.:'.a.ue CL 5‘ ~31 ‘..'1 then: David John Rolfe Priorities Inventory. The Luscher Color Test, a projective test, gauged personality stress, and the Color Prediction Test measured empathy in the relationship. The follow-up test battery, administered by mail, consisted of the Marital Roles Inventory, Locke-Wallace Adjustment Test (used here to measure subjective satisfac- tion), and Edmonds Marital Conventionalization Test. Forty-nine of the Lansing area couples were personally tested in their homes. They were also retested with the LCT and CPT. I’On each test, a couple's\index score was calculated, being the product of each person's score, plus the dif- ference between them. Pearson Product Moment Correlations ‘were calculated between independent demographic variables, and couple's pre-marriage index scores. These index scores were correlated with follow-up index scores. Fifty-eight hypotheses were tested. For the American couples, pre-marriage adjustment was related to age, education and length of engagement, and the approval of the parents. Likewise, the pre-marital adjust- ment of the New Zealand couples was associated with parental approval, increased education and absence of religious difference. Consensus for the American couples on financial priorities was related to increased age, education, the n‘ a: has; u- 3 (D t) ‘f T . I ~ Q v0llfl~:.-‘ par =,vvodv:U at . DAV '9 anti. .3 "-= 598' Zea '1'“ A: ~. "" . ‘ . v. N‘e 1:: r1 ‘1 The fOll! "‘2‘ i =‘Q‘ ' t . a ‘ , '.""‘¢Ca1‘ 4 . David John Rolfe woman being of higher socioeconomic status than the man, and subjects having had happily married parents. For the New Zealanders, having a happy childhood was the only associated variable. There was no relationship between the independent vari- ables and LCT index scores for the Americans, but for the New Zealanders, stress was related to lengthy acquaintanceships. Empathy for the Americans was stronger in short engage- 'ments among the less educated who lacked parental approval. In the New Zealand sample, empathy was not associated with any of the independent variables. The follow-up showed that pre-marriage adjustment was significantly related to early marriage adjustment (p > .026 for Americans; p > .001 for New Zealanders). Ad- justment was lower if couples had a child within the first year of marriage. For those American couples visited in the follow-up, the LCT'index correlated (p > .001) with the pre-marriage scores, showing stress levels to remain relatively constant over time. The modal color choice pattern moved closer to Luscher's Ideal norm (Spearman Rho Coefficient: pretest +.810: follow-up +.952). Pre-marriage empathy was unrelated to empathy measured on the follow-up. No relationship was found between pre-marriage consen- sus on financial priorities and later adjustment or satisfaction for either group. tum- 115:5'lm3t1 in Iealand co 52:21: corral .5545 for the grip > .03 Effeztzanaliz 5m: can be .13! :cn'v-‘entio {13:33.5 abc; Other le 33:1 the pr 35 fSllOV-up, Clix-up by m he fatter of ‘39 mural“ e David John Rolfe ‘ Subjective satisfaction in early marriage was related to adjustment, low role strain, (American couples, p > .046; New Zealand couples, p > .001). However, subjective satis- faction correlated at +.4598 for the American couples, and +.6845 for the New Zealand couples (both significant beyond p > .001), with scores on the Edmonds Marital Conventionalization Test. Thus much of the recorded satis- faction can be seen as an outcome of persons' tendencies to .give conventionally polite rather than frank answers to questions about their satisfaction in marriage. Other lesser findings were that there was no difference between the pre-marriage adjustment of couples who completed the follow-up, and those who dropped out after the pretest. Fellow-up by mail was no less valid than in-person testing. The factor of who decided who would be tested first was found to~be unrelated to early marriage adjustment, satisfaction, strain or empathy. Pre-marriage empathy was found to be more balanced in the New Zealand couples, despite the higher level of stress experienced by the men. American couples were found to have difficulty in the reciprocal communication of the man's feelings. New Zealand couples were found to be less traditionally oriented than the American couples in terms of the rigid assignment of 'man's work' and ’woman's work.'v he role c: i any f r: David John Rolfe ’ The role priorities of American couples with children turned away from a pre-marriage companionate orientation, toward the instrumental role orientation found by Hurvitz to be typical of established middle class American couples in the late 19508. r4. \ \J.‘ a-.- MARRIAGE ADJUSTMENT OF COUPLES: A PRE-MARITAL ASSESSMENT, AND FOLLOW-UP IN MARRIAGE BY David John Rolfe A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family Ecology 1975 \ ® Copyright by ~ DAVID JOHN ROLFE 1975 First and and assis: F332” Her' i :39 9er Ct The New 2 23;?! ”Bella: “3335158 her ””1“th in 5 “ant t filCe and thO‘. lame! C035; 2.53:, and DI. . g. I’. . n » :a '31.: ‘ions in- More the <15 cf this a“ “d 1ives. AC WOWLEDGMEN TS First and foremost, I greatly appreciate the encourage- ment and assistance of my wife Peggy in all phases of this project. Her involvement has greatly added to the meaning of the project for me. The New Zealand data was collected by my cousin, 'Shirley Freeman of Christchurch, New Zealand. I much appreciate her cooperation which enabled this study to be bicultural in scope. ‘ I want to thank Dr. Donald Melcer for his continuing advice and thoughtful critiques of the project as it evolved. The other committee members, Dr. Gordon Aldridge, Dr. Jane Oyer, and Dr. Carol Shaffer, have also made many helpful suggestions in the structuring of the project. More than five hundred couples were involved in various parts of this project. I thank them for allowing me into their lives. I also appreciate the efficient data processing managed by the staff of the Computer Institute for Social Science Research at Michigan State University. The satisfaction I have gained from this project has been greatly increased by the enthusiasm of those associated with me in its inception and completion. ii O L) (1) '1 I" I. u.) ‘ i no I” (D r9 O (D v .. m :I 2* v:- rg L) E. {u in I .j c" b ‘LKI If) l‘;] f—‘WW \n 'v: n1 0 ' Q '1 s £01.:- FAR t, “V“ Def:ni* Cjus Dire: Empa? Enga: Pam;i Hap; Ind:' 6!: Pre: ’2 Satil 1 TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER I. THE PROBLEM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Statement of the Problem . . Objectives of the Study . . . Organization of the Thesis . Instruments Used . . . . . Engaged Couples Group One Engaged Couples Group Two, Three andF Follow-up Group One (by mail) . . . . Follow-up Group Two and Three . . . . Control Groups Five, Six, and Seven . Definitions . . . Adjustment . . . Direct Test . . . Empathy . . . . . Engaged Couple . Family . . . . . Happiness in Marri iag @000... Indirect Test . . Married Couple . Predictive Ability Role . . . . . . . Role Expectations Role Priorities . Role Strain . . . . Satisfaction . . . Stability‘. . . . . Status Position . . . Success (in marriage) . Operational Definitions Adjustment . . . . . Adjustment Index . . Color Prediction Index Conventionalization Inde Empathy . . . . . . . . Marital Roles Inventory Index Pre-Marital Adjustment Index Role Expectations . . . . . . Role Priorities . . . . . . . Role Strain . . . . . . . . . X iii 0.0000000000000000 0 o o o 0 0 o o o o O o o o c o o O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O . g . O O O O Page \omxoxomkooooooooomooqqqqqmmmmmmbe HE‘F‘ COO HHHHH HHHOO HHHHH NNHHH [7 r) O m (1) L11 8’ U) () '1 L; U) () 0—4 (O '11 m :12 ’6 III DI DI '1 'l 7 '(J 01, ff (1) ’l J (11 *1 " iv Satisfaction Index . Soc io-Economic Status Success . . . . . . . The Variables . . . . . Theory . . . . . . . . II. REVIEW or LITERATURE . . Class A Predictors . . Age at Marriage . . . Length of Aquaintances Engagement . . . . . Premarital Pregnancy Religion . . . . . . Socioeconomic Factors Class B.Predictors . . Education . . . . . . Parental Divorce . . Happiness of Parent's Happiness in Childhood Parental Attitude . . Birth Order . . . . . Family Size . . . . 3 Mental Health . . . . Sexual Experience . . Summary of Hypotheses in III. MARITAL ASSESSMENT . . . IV. MARITAL ROLES . . . . . . V. COLOR PSYCHOLOGY . . . 2 Index hip Ma 2. ‘1! 9.00.00.90.00 arr I Cha '0 0 VI. EMPATHY AND PREDICTIVE ABILITY VII. FINANCIAL PRIORITIES . . VIII. DECISION MAKING . . . . . IX. CULTURAL FACTORS . . . . t (be... X. METHODOLOGY I: PROBLEMS AND DATA XI. METHODOLOGY II: PROCEDURE (D II GATHERING Selection of Samples and Data Collection United States Samples New Zealand Sample . Description of Instruments Used Group 1 I O O O O O 0 Groups 2 and 4 . . . Group 3 . . . . . . . 12 12 13 13 14 28 33 33 35 36 36 38 44 48 48 50 SO 52 52 53 57 58 61 62 65 79 86 92 98 107 114 116 128 128 128 132 133 133 133 133 XII. XIII. XIV. Groups 5, 6, and 7 . . . . . . . . Follow-up on Groups I, 2, and 3 . . Analysis of Data . . . . . . . . . . Score Calculations on Instruments . Engagement Success Inventory . . Marital Roles Inventory . . . . . Luscher Color Test . . . . . . . Financial Priorities Inventory . Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Test Problem Check List . . . . . . . . . Marital Conventionalization (Retitled Satisfaction in Marriage Test for this study.) . . . . . . . . . . . . . Primary Communication Inventory . . . . . Socioeconomic Status . . . . . . . . . THE POPULATION . . . . . .'. . . . . . . . . . Discussion of Data Gathered in the Pre- Marriage Part of this Study . . . . . . . Discussion of Data Gathered in the Follow- up Part of this Study . . . . . . . . . Data Gathered But Not Used in This Study Group I . . . . . . . . Groups I, II, III . Group IV . . . . . Groups v, VI, VII . h PRESENTATION AND DISCUSSION OF FINDINGS . . . SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION . . . . . . . . . . . Limitations Of the Present Study . . . . . . Suggestions fOr Further Research . . . . . . smacm BIBLIOGMPHY O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O \ APPENDICES O O O I I C I O O O O O O O O I O O O O O A. B. C. D. E. P. G. 'B. I. J. COVER PAGES AND LETTERS . . . DEMOGRAPHIC DATA SHEETS . . . ENGAGEMENT SUCCESS INVENTORY LUSCHER.COLOR TEST . . . . . MARITAL ROLES INVENTORY . . . FINANCIAL PRIORITIES INVENTORY PRIMARY COMMUNICATION INVENTORY LOCKE-WALLACE MARITAL ADJUSTMENT TEST; AND PROBLEMCHECKLIST . . . . . . . . . . . .. EDMOND'S SCALE OF MARITAL CONVENTIONALIZATION (RETITLED FOR THIS STUDY: SATISFACTION IN MARRIAGE TEST) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . DESCRIPTION OF VARIABLES USED IN DATA ANALYSIS. , 134 134 134 135 135 135 136 136 137 137 137 137 138 140 140 169 185 185 185 186 186 187 235 242 243 245 276 276 282 287 290 292 301 305 308 311 313 . - ' “Axma "' Age 0f 9 st- N.‘ H I ' Lstlmte 7 ‘ ~6n~th r and p; ‘ ‘FVI may» . Pelaticn and st. Relatxcn 590359: Acczpar. Experi: tter: . Classifiz decisr 'Age Of e testin tEstin couple PIE-IE ’ time , Table 1. 10O 11. 12. 13. LIST OF TABLES A comparison between probability of divorce and probability of success in marriage, by religious affiliation . . . . . . . . . . . Relationship between education achievement and stability of marriage . . . . . . . . Relationship between age difference of spouses and marital stability . . A comparison of Schaie and Heiss' experimentally established color preference pattern with Luscher's theoretical norm . Classification of family by percentage of decisions made by each parent . . . . . . . Age of engaged men at time of initial test ing O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Age of engaged women at time of initial testing O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O Estimated length of time (acquaintanceship) couples had known each other at time of pre-marriage testing . . . . . . . . . . . Length of engagement as reported by men at time of pre-marriage testing . . . . . . . Length of engagement as reported by women at time of pre-marriage testing . . . . . Years of education completed by engaged men . Years of education completed by engaged "one n O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Socioeconomic status of respondent's fathers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi Page . 42 . 49 53 90 112 . 141 . 142 143 . 144 . 145 147 . 148 . 149 1‘ 9 I 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26O 27. 28. vii Socioeconomic Status Indexes of couples, based on the Socioeconomic Status of respondent's fathers . . . . . . . . . Religious preferences of engaged subjects . . Frequency of item selection on the Financial Priorities Inventory by men and women . . . Financial Priorities Inventory Index scores of couples tested before marriage . . . . . . Modal ranking of roles on 'Husband's Roles,’ as anticipated by engaged men . . . . . ... Modal ranking of roles on 'Wife's Roles,’ as anticipated by engaged women . . . . . . . Modal ranking of roles on 'Husband's Expectations of His Wife's Roles,’ as ranked by engaged men . . . . . . . . . . Modal ranking of roles on 'Wife's Expectations of Her Husbandfis Roles,‘ as ranked by engaged women . . . . . . . . Marital Roles Inventory Index scores of couples tested before marriage . . . Modal response pattern on the Luscher Color Test compared with Luscher's Ideal Norm. Couples tested before marriage . . . . . . Luscher Color Test Index scores of couples tested before marriage . . . . . . . . . . . Color Prediction Index scores of couples tested before marriage . . . . . . . . . . . Modal ranking of roles on 'Husband's Roles,‘ by married men responding to the follow—up questionnaire . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Modal ranking of roles on 'Wife's Roles,' by married women responding to the follow-up questionnaire . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Modal ranking of roles on 'Husband's Preference for His Wife's Roles,‘ by married men responding to the follow-up questionnaire . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151 153 155 157 160 161 162 163 164 166 167 168 172 173 174 -L\ al' :21 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36O 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. viii Modal ranking of roles on 'Wife's Preference for Her Husband's Roles,’ by married women responding to the follow-up questionnaire . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Marital Roles Inventory Index scores of couples without children tested one year after their weddings . . . . . . . . . . Marital Roles-Inventory Index scores of couples with children tested one year after their weddings . . . . . . . . . . Modal response pattern on the Luscher Color Test by Group II couples tested one year after their weddings, responses compared with Luscher's Ideal Norm . . . . . . . . Luscher Color Test Index scores of couples in Group II tested one year after their weddi ngs O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O Color Prediction Index Scores of couples in Group II tested one year after their weddings 0 o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o Locke-Wallace Index scores of couples responding to the follow-up questionnaire Satisfaction in Marriage Test Index scores of couples responding to the follow-up questionnaire . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Group I Correlation Matrix for independent variables, Pre-marital Index and Follow-up Index scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Group II Correlation Matrix for independent variables and Pre-marital Index scores . Group III Correlation Matrix for independent variables and Pre-marital Index scores . Group II Correlation Matrix for Follow-up Index scores, and their relationship to selected Pre-marital Index scores and independent variables . . . . . . . . . . Group III Correlation Matrix for Follow-up Index scores, and their relationship to selected Pre-marital Index scores and independent variables . . . . . . . . . . 175 176 177 179 180 181 183 184 188 189 190 191 [E O" 0 .1 d" 53-11 4"": agar ~O\v- ‘ be. The sa - b .'9' Olc: () ‘) a F‘ ( ' I} w ) On 1 Oh (I) 9‘ 9 ha m :‘f In 91 '1 E32 by o 3 42. 43. 44O 45. 46. 47. 48O 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. ix Summary of findings contrasting the adjustment of couples with children against the adjustment of couples without children . . . . . . . . . . . The satisfaction of couples with their marriages. A comparison between couples with a child, and childless couples . . . . Color choice scores of subjects as differentiated by sex of subject . . . . . Summary of findings contrasting the personality stress of couples, as measured by the Luscher Color Test, with the presence of a child in the first year of marriage . Empathy scores on the Luscher Color Test as differentiated by sex of subject . . . . . . The follow-up Luscher Color Test Indexes of couples compared on the basis of which partner volunteered toxbe first tested . . . The follow-up Color Prediction Indexes of couples compared on the basis of which partner volunteered to be first tested . . . The follow-up Marital Roles Inventory Indexes of couples compared on the basis of which partner volunteered to be first tested . . . The follow-up Locke-Wallace Test Indexes of couples compared on the basis of which partner volunteered to be first tested . . A comparison of the pre-marriage adjustment of couples who responded to the follow-up, with the pre-marriage adjustment of the couples who dropped out of the study after the initial testing . . . . . . . . . . . . . A comparison of the Follow-up Marital Roles Inventory Index scores of couples responding by mail, with couples tested:h1person . . . . A comparison of the Locke-Wallace Test Indexes of couples responding by mail, with couples tested in person . . . . . . . . . . 195 196 209 212 216 225 226 227 228 231 232 234 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM The good marriage is somewhat like a trapeze act. To begin with, it requires a certain aptitude, a basic wish to work with one other person in some death-defying venture. Then it takes a good lot of practice before the real soaring begins. Success requires each partner to be independent, to be strong in a critical way, to be responsible for the unit. Through trust and timing and a certain tension, each can help the other reach new heights. Or just enjoy the flying. ‘Shirley Streshinsky 1974 \. Marriage has been a tOpic of no small interest throughout recorded history. Elaborate religious ceremonies have announced the inception of a marriage, together with the multitude of laws and customs prescribing its nature and direction. At the same time that someone more or less objectively evaluates the potential of each union; someone else has a subjective response to the union itself. In many cultures, such as the Indian and Chinese, it was the heads of the families who chose Spouses for their children. The prime questions were objective; is this a financially sound match for the (extended) family? Is this socially to our family's credit? Any subjective element that entered into the decision was between the family heads. The couple might never meet prior to the wedding. I: D851 A . £3529 1:83 ~— ‘5. P; o'- -5. ‘ ...: O .65 In :55: ..".:‘:‘ at an: a p Spouse, .3 ‘ 3.1. not b . . ~ 32:1; .3. OA‘E :ra 1.3—” 9-“. "" hue n! S . "mes mar] In medieval EurOpe, marriages were also used to seal a peace treaty, join together political entities, and legitimize claims to power. In a sense marriage was for the sOcial system which operated on primogeniture, legitimacy and complicated rules of precedence. Marriage was an integral part of the societal network of a society, ruled and directed on the basis of inherited customs and chattels. In most societies . . . the young were taught that at best they could count on respect and a proper discharge of duties from their spouse, but they could not expect happiness and naturally could not divorce if they failed to achieve happiness [Goode, 1964; 93]. The gradual shift away from an agrarian society has changed the nature of marriage. In the 18th and 19th centuries marriage was an institution at least partially designed for the comfort and well being of its participants. Men at least could choose their brides, paying heed to parental pressures to choose from the "right” social strata. Women were still legally the property of their fathers or husbands, although they could refuse to marry. By about 1900, it was the parties to the marriage who had the chance to subjectively choose a mate. However the subjective, or companionship aspect still had to take into account the objective factor. Social and economic consi- derations were still important in the objective sense. If a couple did not have the credentials for the institution of marriage; proof of employment or house keeping skills, zine subjec 251m. today we afrtar in ma: Thefc: , pe 2::5. Fee a '11:}?e' d'xi : "\n uovl.s. ec: in. mineral. , 2 then the subjective factors of relationship were called into question. Today we are in a new era. Inherited wealth is seldom a factor in marriage. Within much broader boundaries than ever before, persons choose mates on the basis of subjective factors. Few are the persons who mingle only with their “own type” during the main marrying years of 15-35. Thus it is much harder for a person to locate a potential mate who can appreciate and accommodate the former's concept of marriage. Objective factors are, for the first time in history, subordinated to subjective factors. This is in part due to a loss of function. The family has certain functions: economic, status giving, educational, religious, recreational, protective, and affectional. All of these except the affectional function have become markedly less prominent in recent years [Ogburn, 1962]. Hence there is a huge resultant stress on the affectional function. If this fails, then the main fiber of modern marriage is shattered. Unfortunately this one sided emphasis is com- pounded because of a cultural myth. In western societies the young person is given a rather romantic View of marriage and love, and is disappointed to find that marriage is at best contented and dull, and at worst a perceptual ache [Goode, 1964: 93]. This observation is being echoed by pundits, both popular, academic and ecclesiastical, with increasing aggitation in recent years. Marriage is still very popular, Ili— ... 1.3"?“ 0.0- no'V. "' the I —o. 0'" 3 lo. these f. 3.1:; the s ------ att‘ 4:;1‘o0 :---- nav-~a InouO I UOéVUjhs h..’. . 3.. ..:g to ( . -:...V‘p-. ~~*.‘:a ‘ but, divorce is reaching incidences almost as high in some parts of the United States. These facts may be theoretically interesting, but knowing the situation is of little help to the counselor or clergyman attempting to help couples decide if they have a strong enough relationship to build a viable marriage. As will be demonstrated in the body of this dissertation, resorting to currently available "marriage readiness inventories” will be of little help. Statement of the Problem This study will describe the design of a practical marriage readiness inventory. \The inventory will be used with engaged couples. The couples will be followed up and tested one year after marriage to assess the relationship between pre-marriage, and later marital adjustment. Objectives of the Study 1. Design an instrument for indirectly measuring a couples readiness for marriage. To meet the requirements of an indirect measure. ”(a) Examinee Shall be neither self conscious nor aware of the intent of the study and (b) that the form of the attitude being measured shall not be destroyed in the process of describing it" [Frumkin, 1952: 216). 2. The instrument will be easily interpretable by the non-research oriented cOunselor or clergyman. .zoo' F11 ,—...b“ 4. "’ 2:;;:ies, c : Our” JO Act 0... o. ‘ .- 0 IA. .595 u m. .‘O 'f'“ no-.I:' lVé P -- C. 135 q 3:. . “»b 1‘ ‘ é".:‘ O-ua -.‘ 500‘ Tie {‘95 ‘5: 1.18:9 'ui I ‘5. I' D.‘ O; “ A . ““~‘Un CO ‘ "o 1:“- 7“ ~.-; ‘_-‘~ "933 an; ‘0 =Ctfié’ s a: O‘- O .- .‘J (1 ’0 1" 3. The data obtained will be in a form which provides practical information on the couples relationship. 4. The instrument will require minimal equipment, facilities, cost and time to administer. 5. ”The procedure shall be such that the subjects do not feel that they are tricked or purposefully deceived” [Olson and Ryder, 1970: 443]. 6.’ Test the instrument on an experimental population. 7. Retest population one year after marriage to .ascertain the predictive value of the instrument. Organization of the Thesis Q‘ The rest of this study will be organized as follows. First there will be a listing of the instruments used and definition of pertinent terms. Following this will be a discussion of the theoretical aspects of marriage prediction and success. A review of the literature relevant to this study will be used to generate working hypotheses. A section on methodology will discuss current marriage readiness and prediction instruments used by other re- searchers, and the problems encountered in using volunteers. The second section on methodology will discuss the selection of samples and data collection. The particular instruments used will be described, as well as the procedures and techniques of data analysis. This will be followed by a full description of the sample, analysis of findings, and the ways the hypotheses were substantiated or rejected by u. .'\ as i, '22:: sec:: —,I.-TAO ‘1 ma .Ouv V‘ 1:32:25 c . .‘~~~.,. ‘I H l .ui‘n-... c v hv-"b ' ”A... . h V- ,v 4.... .‘ ‘O'55. Fawn. the data secured in this study. A final chapter will present conclusions of the study together with limitations and implications of the findings. Instruments Used Engaged Couples Group One Engagement Success Inventory Marital Roles Inventory Prediction of Partner's Response on Marital Roles Inventory Engaged Couples Group Two, Three and Four Marital Roles Inventory . Luscher Color Test Predicting Partner's Response on Luscher Color Test Financial Priorities Inventory Follow-up Group One (by mail) Marital Roles Inventory Primary Communication Inventory Marital Adjustment Test Problem Check List Follow-up Group Two and Three Marital Roles Inventory Primary Communication Inventory Locke-wallace Marital Adjustment Test Problem Check List ‘ 1 P Pan. . ( Luscher u” x. aA‘A tel a. oboi 3'32 11‘. L16; Viv§._ ”Q‘ e-Cl .ne text 5, Q3"! ‘ “,9?“ b y. .‘\. I baa . ‘ué‘f‘ .‘5' n: Marital Conventionalization Test Luscher Color Test Predicting Partner's Response on Luscher Color Test (color test only administered to 49 couples in group two tested in their homes) Control Groups Five, Six, and Seven Marital Roles Inventory Luscher Color Test Predicting Partner's Response on Luscher Color Test Primary Communication Inventory Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Test \ Problem Check List 5 Marital Conventionalization Test (on group five only) Definitions Adjustment The term which describes the couple's relationship as observed by an outsider and evaluated in terms of his standards, not the couples. Direct Test The type of test that is obvious in its intent, and responses to which can be easily manipulated to express any impressions the subject wishes to convey. Empathy The ability to sense another persons feelings, and put oneself in "their shoes." ' H u rvi -v‘:u' :83. a .9 - a " “-" A? -P 3) "a: U; ' I. "‘ I .‘I 5' I "- v-A0.ern, Engaged Couple A male-female pair who have formally announced their plans to have a legal marriage ceremony. Family A group of persons including a mother and father, legally married, and one or more children. A small family has one or two children; a medium size family has three or four children; and a large family five or more children. Happiness in Marriage A term used by many researchers to imply pleasant feelings about being married. ~A vague term that variously implies adjustment, satisfaction or stability. It will not be used in this study except in reporting other observers' research. Indirect Test A type of test that is disguised in its purpose. The person taking the test is not fully aware of the structure of the test, and is thus less able to manipulate responses. Married Couple A male-female pair who report they are legally husband and wife. Predictive Ability The application by a subject of the skill of empathy (see above) in accurately foretelling his or her partner's responses to a set of items. .— u.) I; ’1) .. 1.435! w: "O IOOO‘ .1 I I De 7“!" u H “1 ~~.4H V's-y eaves as ‘: .. I "F" ‘45“.‘ '- .“q’ , ....“‘N‘ One V ‘ 5d . ”\r f . (h The din 51 i" DA. ‘3 . «uses, a“, H ., II! 1 l V. "A" ‘ ‘ h‘.‘r \ . is. 1“” ‘\ Role A group of feelings, behaviors, expectations, rights and duties, which delineate the character ascribed to a status poSition. Role Expectations The behaviors and feelings which an individual perceives as being an integral part of a status position. Role Priorities An individually or communually established way of defining the relative importance of particular rights and duties incumbent to a particular role. Role Strain The difference between the person's priorities for his roles, and his partner's expectations of the same roles. Satisfaction That condition in marriage where the relationship is pleasing to both couple members, and they see it approxima- ting their internal standards of what marriage ideally should be like. Stability A term used by other researchers to suggest variously the conditions of being well adjusted, satisfied, not in a state of disorganization, and not divorced. The term will only be used in this study in the process of reporting other researcher's findings. Aplace i: zier's social 23:5ch can he firmle. f3. .15. duties. 1 I’m ta :1 fies who a: 2:55 ”"5 a: 'bsui ”£4536 {‘95 Inventc Li have ”Pi- :.:se to Doc“: kl... be £108.: in they 'n‘. 10 Status Position A place in the social order which defines the status holder's social value relative to persons in other statuses. A person Can hold several different positions in society for example, father, policeman, neighbor, each with its own role, duties, and rights which may or may not be consonant. Success (in marriage) This term is used exclusively to describe married couples who are both adjusted and satisfied in the way 'these terms are herein defined. Operational‘Definitions Adjustment Measured by calculating a couple's score on the Marital Roles Inventory. An adjusted couple will rank their roles, and have expectations of their partner's roles, which are close to modal. In addition, the partner's role priorities will be close to the other's role expectations of spouse. Hence they will be overall low in strain. Adjustment Index Calculated by summing each person's role expectation, role priority and role strain scores to form a cumulative individual strain score. The partner's sum scores are added tOgether, and the difference between them added to this to form the Index. I Q Van 9 H" “a 5““ -;a.'2 sxres ! 1 £35.52 1581! ms in 5Ynono the Ear; I “‘53:. Role . “a I‘ W win] I . 11 Color Prediction Index This index is arrived at by summing the couple's empathy scores (see below), and adding in the difference between their scores. Conventionalization Index This index is calculated the same way as the above mentioned indexes, and uses each couple member's score on the Edmond's Marital Conventionalization Scale (retitled for this study as: Satisfaction in Marriage Test) as its 'basis. An individual's rank order difference will choose the color and B's actual choice Marital Synonomous with Empathy empathy score is the product of the between A's prediction of how B plates on the Luscher Color Test, pattern. Roles Inventory Index the Adjustment Index. Pre-Marital Adjustment Index The Adjustment Index derived from a couple's scores culthe Marital Roles Inventory administered prior to marriage . Role Expectations Role expectations will be measured by comparing a Person's ranking of their role expectations of Spouse [.41 O O a: 1‘19 1393: '25.! 9:011; sequent ite feted with is by sane 32:65. vi t}: i235 be ran " Stain, 12 with the modal ranking of these roles by same sex persons in their group. The instrument used for this and the two subsequent items will be the Marital Roles Inventory. Role Priorities The individual's priorities for their roles are compared with group modal role rankings of those same roles by same sex persons. Role Strain The individual's priorities for their roles are compared with the partner's preferences for how the roles should be ranked. The greaterxthe discrepancy, the greater 5 the strain. Satisfaction Index A couple's satisfaction index will be calculated by summing their response scores on the Locke-Wallace Marital .Adjustment Test, and adding in the difference between the partner ' s scores . Socio-Economic Status Index The socio-economic status score of the woman's father is subtracted from the status score of the man's father. A negative value indicates a couple in which the wife is ”marrying down:' a positive score a union in which the husband is ”marrying down.” T. .Lu .u‘ and ,. 3' :- ‘ouoes “hi 13 Success A couple will be designated as successful when both their Satisfaction and Adjustment Indexes are in the tOp quartile for their group. The Variables In this study, there are multiple independent variables which have to be taken into consideration. In Chapter II: Review of Literature, the relevance of all these variables will be discussed. The variables to be considered “are as follows: age age difference birth order .length of time couple have known each other length of engagement education religion socioeconomic status of families of origin size of families of origin birth of child in first year of marriage happiness of subjects' childhoods happiness of parents' marriages parental approval of the couple's marriage The relationship between these variables and scores on the test batteries will be calculated. _.-v e I.“ ”Va—yrs. '7 ;"U (1. (D 'U The ~ '1 55.3.: am. 2:32.233 5 (.‘1 3e expre Ejzses t0 9‘... :2 303.; a :3. ' "‘ ‘O vet tn nee: tne 1 I“ . Q n‘UAG,‘ '.V. WW...“ 'ue: 14 The dependent variables are the pre-marital adjustment, empathic ability, consensus in financial priorities and personality strain of the engaged couples. The variables will be expressed in terms of index scores calculated on responses to the pre-marital questionnaires. These indexes will be compared with scores received on the follow—up tests of marital adjustment and satisfaction administered one year after the couple's wedding. A significant relationship between the independent and dependent variables will be 'recorded when findings are found to be at, or better than, the .05 level of confidence. \ Theory . This study uses the role interactional framework to focus on the relationship between engaged (later married) couples. The framework looks at the family in terms of the interaction of its members. This pattern of interaction is subdivided into roles assumed by, or assigned to, individuals. A key process is that of role-taking, whereby individuals take on the characteristic duties and privileges of a particular role set. Role Interactional theory is most often focused on the dynamic relationship between husbands and wives. Patterns of behavior, aspirations and personality characteristics, are all considered components of roles. Roles are defined in different ways depending on the context of situations and the frame of reference of the observer. Roles are “-315“ grad fro: : :._, As ind: .arzei a la: s-n "PK ‘5 L... may}, l.;,.' O 5 :.h. O ~:-vv .: 5 ~3-3..'3'- LO a agave to (3 "ob ' L..- a Part; 15 learned from important persons in the life of each indivi— dual. As individuals approach marriageable age they have learned a large variety of roles, some situation specific such as “baby sitter," Others of global application such as ”adult." Each role is seen to be in a context usually referred to as status. The status delineates the place relative to other positions in the social framework in which a particular role is lived. Status, like role, is partially a global concept which permeates all arenas of thought and activities, for example, caucasian, and partly .situation specific, for example, father. The more global the frame of reference of status, the more it has to do with the individual's social standing in his community. Personal status is situation specific and involved with more intimate interactions [Nye and Berado, 1966]. An individual builds his identity on the basis of the status he holds, and within this context acts the role he feels is appropriate to his status in both global and particular terms. he learns how to act in situations based on his previous experience in interacting with other mem— bers of his family. A common repertoire of understandings is amassed, and it is through these that a family is able to function. Through the process of role taking, of modeling behaviors, acquiring ideas, motives and feelings, each person learns how to relate to others in the framework of a family group. Part of role taking is acquiring expectations :32. :thers . .: :naf‘flf‘ ,n “UOUVO O no 0 :.ag.‘i go Jessi! (3] ((2H, .. ’j 0’ .9 ran) 1’ (7) 16 of how others in the family will act and feel. Such knowledge is needed for a person to be able to perform his role appropriately [Nye and Berado, 1966]. Exactly how role taking is accomplished, and what role signifies in part depends on the theoretical bias of the investigator. Hurvitz, in his survey on the concept of role in theory found three main uses: (1) (2) (3) From sociological theory comes a concept of role as the behavioral aSpect of the status of husband and wife, the role set which link the individual as an actor to the social structure; From the sociological study of the family comes the concept of role as the expression of an attitude associated with the source and kind of control exercised in the family, indicating whether it is authoritarian and tradi- tional or democratic and companionship; From the psychotherapists comes the concept of roles which have developed as a result of interaction with persons who have played an important part in the early development of the husband and wife, and which now have meaning for them in terms of the symbolic meaning the husband and wife have for each other [Hurvitz, 1958: 55]. Cottrell made a clear statement about roles in terms cw marital adjustment from the psychodynamic viewpoint; First, marriage adjustment may be regarded as a process in which marriage partners attempt to re-enact certain relational systems of situations which obtained in their own earlier family group. Second, the kinds of roles that marriage partners bring to the marriage will deter- mine the nature of their marriage relation- ship and the degree of adjustment that they will achieve. Third. we as a tee: situaticr slugs ca‘. marriage itsttrell Bessari ; ”Ihm-g’; 1» ”nun-é». $8€d c’.:o I ...... “arrowg ”“0”! ‘ o O. I. ‘H t... “V9.33. VS O o-‘zflu ' arr-o:.zes tn; r. 9 'e' 2 -~.e of ti : "n -1. 395$ :‘ .9 Vlev‘oe" ‘7‘ “L ha 5 a. ‘v .,. ‘ a iesea‘ 4 2:5. ‘ H CS fi‘? ”\ ‘1';t \ 2 an ‘le‘fi 17 Third, maladjusted marriages may be regarded as a result of the failure of the marriage situation to provide a system of relation- ships called for by the roles that the marriage partners bring to the marriage [Cottrell, 1933: 107-115]. Bossard and Boll, used a more static approach which circumscribed the types of roles [Bossard and Boll, 1955]. Herbst narrowed the field even further to consider behavioral relationships [Herbst, 1952]. Mead, on the other hand, emphasizes the reciprocal nature of role taking, "taking the role of the other" [1934: 254]. Burgess is credited with suggesting that the family can be viewed as a unity of interacting personalities, to which Dollard added ”each with a history in a given cultural milieu” [Ney and Berado, 1966: 101]. Hill added the concept of considering families to be an "arena of interacting personalities" to allow for a more flexible conceptualizing incorporating change, conflict and uneven growth of constituent parts of the family [Nye and Berado, 1966: 100]. In passing, this writer feels that the role interac- tional framework is not fully adequate to cope with conflict. As will be seen in the section on reveiwing the literature, many researchers, including those prominent in the field such as Burgess, Wallin, Cottrell, Locke, more recently Hurvitz and others, conceptualize no conflict as being healthy and part of a good marital adjustment. he role 2:". .13 :e SCI‘. LIZEIEIES . . . . “ "O‘flfi o O -.....U.: C. ‘Cuolo Jr: “N 40‘ “on: “-v . . ., 5.": 2"‘ v “New... I :‘I:o“. 8.“. I I HCaEgE ”I. P ‘1. :‘AE H v- o Ova-y U 41‘ I ‘v “O 065.3 1.: M. u M -= . 'I-- t . .::£..‘_'; g .,. o u g“ '19:“... "n h‘ .1 “D“‘2: a VI i I“ w “a 18 The role interactional model can be strengthened by borrowing conceptually from the systems theory which posits that an open (live) system like the family or a marriage must have. some tension, a way to respond to new events and differences. In this respect absence of conflict is an indication of incomplete interaction, and a symptom of impending decay in the relationship (Buckley, 1967; von Bertalanffy, 1968]. Not only is conflict necessary but it is inevitable. It is inevitable because all persons ex— perience conflict as they grow up in their family of origin, and learn a way to COpe with it. They learn how to take the role of a person expressing conflict, and of the person responding to conflict. Since this occurs in intimate relationships people learn to expect conflict in intimacy. As Kassorla has noted, intimacy reawakens both the comfor- table and conflicting feelings and expectations from pre- vious close relationships [Kassorla, 1973]. Laing's postulations about family themes carries this concept further. Laing has written that each family has its own themes and prescribed roles. The players change from generation to generation but the theme (script) remain the same [Laing, 1969b]. This is so, irrespective of the externally viewed functional viability of the theme or role. Each role is needed to reinact a theme even if it means that one player has to become psychotic for the theme to continue. The role interactional framework has not clearly understood :e f'Ictional :2“: are part :: rcessary azzzions. Flu: ‘ “3.5 the 19 the functional nature of self or other directed expectations which are partially or fully destructive to the individual, but necessary for a family interaction network in particular situations. This thesis describes an attempt to measure and assess adjustments in marriage and to provide a simple, practical way of replication for persons involved in preparing couples for marriage. To reach this goal it is necessary to borrow conceptually from other frameworks. Newcomb's little known .theory of balance proves helpful in part. Newcomb appears to have borrowed from both systems theory and the role interactional framework. He indicates that individuals have the tendency to maintain a balance in relationship ”between perceived similarity of attitudes and sentiments" [Newcomb, 1961: 12]. Sentiments here signifying the extent of liking or disliking for the other person. Changes in perceived similarity lead to imbalance in the relationship, causing strain or discomfort which in turn lead to efforts to reduce the strain and reachieve balance. Balance is a condition in which the amount of liking of another person is in keeping with the degree of Similarity of the two person's attitudes [Newcomb, 1961]. Taking this concept back into the role interactional framework we suggest that a couple needs to achieve a balance of role taking and role expectations before they have the basis for a workable marriage. In this context we -' m -M.=m| tLiexpect U Ii: which (5 “#13:, gcal 522;: cancer. u 31.-a 'f'v- O‘O" 0 .‘ :.s 12.321339 OI... .- u . ' 1 ' *~.-.: ‘5 ”1“ ‘DCF I n . an. J” ‘ “an I. ".3: 9531C - 1‘: ‘VAI-A ‘P! In :ova $.‘ a 2m: --= A515 .1.” ~ 3., 3 N..~“ar C( 334 1.1101131». ;.. 1. . "’PT' 133:: .. , -n , i. :‘H‘ awo‘.y C:- 3010: C91m): 20 would expect to find a balanced workable marriage to be one in which there is a high and equal concensus about role behavior, goals and aspirations. A relationship where there is high concensus about only one partner's role would be in imbalance. This concept of a couple's potential for a work- able marriage being assessed in terms of high and equal concensus will permeate the research design of this thesis. The concept of balance is also used with the theory of color psychology. The psychology of color is based on the proposition that individuals choose or reject colors on the basis of their personality makeup. In addition, particular colors have gained definite psychological values, some through cultural processes others through more deeply ingrained forces. Luscher, the foremost world authority on color has written: Color is an inate, spontaneous language common not only to all mankind but also to all living creatures with color Vision. It is the language spoken thousands of times a day in the life of every human being, but only on rare occasions does it cross the frontiers of the unconscious, where it serves as a mother tongue. The only time that individual colors take on a conscious significance are when they are used as signals or signs (red traffic lights, for example, yellow for danger) and in these contexts they lose almost all their content, tending as they do to become lifeless symbols [Luscher, n.d.]. Colors are also used to describe moods. A man sees red, feels blue, is green with envy or purple with rage. He talks of yellow cowards, white hope, black despair, pink tea parties, and brown tastes. In his flags and .. .., n. . O .. ...E,___a_ is that t 35 395 c 21 emblems, religious rituals, customs, superstitions, he uses color as a reflection of his feelings, for these colors vividly portray the emotions within him [Birren, 1962: 9]. Colors also have a significance in more mundane terms. The sugar manufacturer knows "that he must package it (sugar) in a blue container or at least have blue prominently on the package somewhere, that he must avoid green at all costs. .." [Luscher, 1970: 10]. Colors are often deliberately used to create a mood or accentuate a desired feeling state. In religious 'Observances color has specific meaning. Red is used on the feast of martyrs, typifying that they shed their blood for the testimony of Jesus; also on the feast of the Holy Cross-- that cross which was anointed with the Blood of the Lamb: and at Whitsuntide, when the Holy Ghost descended in the likeness of tongues of fire [Walker, 1916: 46]. In short, colors are symbols with deep meaning, meaning that transcends most factors of race, sex, or social origin. Preference for one color and dislike of another is a reflection Of a person's current state of mind, glandular balance, or both [LUscher, 1970]. The significance of color, however, is frequently . relegated to the level of "quaint." Because few persons recognize the real significance of color, a test of color preference becomes an almost ideal assessment of personality. Persons do not feel defensive taking a quick test which they believe will tell the examiner absolutely nothing! —--;-fl v ' I II Oll‘ Hi '4‘. 522:: cf 5: w.’--‘ fifl‘Fal :‘HVOJ-‘Ov )I ' .« onn )- : “use tines: 213.1 .ltV S ‘ A .:".. .,. 1 ;_, Ibo: :teAKAH .2 ..”. V - :;ei;er2t of At this L: the of : 22?.Qrk thf iicore “3,: ';.':N1 .MI.Dr Of 1 3 mater .‘E'ué. «Owns ’ v- hi: H (he- {“p I Its acti" \. 22 In this respect, the researcher quickly obtains an estimate of significant aspects of each person's areas of psychological stress. Using our modification of Newcomb's balance theory, we would look for couples having similar personality scores on the color preference test. Theoreti- cally speaking, such couples would have a good fit in terms of personality, and we would expect them to have one ingredient of a successful marriage. At this juncture we return to Mead’s concept of "taking the role of the other” [92, 213.]. In the interactional ’framework the ability to take the role of other people is a core ingredient. A perSon cannot respond to the role behavior of others without a particular ability: empathy. The greater the skill in empathy, the more closely two persons can interact and coordinate the direction of their relationship. Empathy implies the ability to anticipate the behavior of the other, to exercise predictive ability. A person in empathy with his partner can predict the partner's choice pattern whether the choices are over personality inventory responses, or predicting the partner's role expectations or color choices from finite lists. This ability will be tested in the thesis. The color test has the practical advantage of being quick to administer, suitable even for non-literate subjects, and sufficiently indirect to minimize a subject's effort at manipulation. Theoretical problems, although not part of the interactional framework, impinge on the context of this full" I: . v .4» ”A" ..I. bo-V. a image v.1: ' Q “up... “I. .‘ -‘:~-:~‘3uue.. t ' In, "-I- q "*:--o $395 5 2.3.): an: 23 study. Increasingly there are pressures external to marriage which work against success in marriage. Fromm- Reichman feels that western culture is producing the kinds of life experience which worsens the human condition, and make marriage'more stressful. She cites the increasing overdependent behavior of adults, the lessening of personal friendliness and its open expression, and the magical thinking and ideas of grandeur which western cultures actively promote [Fromm-Reichman, 1950]. Parsons and Bales [1955] note that the family has transferred some of its functions to other units of society and has become more specialized. Foote [1954] sees women as having increasingly less economic dependence on their families, plus more financial responsibility, shared authority, and more com- panionship with their husbands. The suddenness and breadth of change leaves many people confused about the precise nature of marital roles. They probably saw their parents living a parallel pattern in marriage. For example, If a man is a good provider, not excessive in his sexual demands, sober most of the time, and good to the children, this is about all a woman can reasonably ask. Similarly if a woman is a good housekeeper and cook, not too nagging, a willing sex partner, and a good mother, this is all a man can really expect [Bernard, 1964: 687]. Yet, even as they learn the apprOpriate roles, they feel the pressure toward an interactional pattern in marriage. The contrast is multidimensional. They are not prepared for the scope of change. The interactional pattern Miner 011.5» M. '. ~. . Q. Lia re 3 V5;n\. . u‘V‘ - persona caticns are ta}: :2 A aie. t A ‘- 3.35.1: i. ,2 inc: TIME .:ve, z O~n~n G s i 5.40 w, u‘ 5.5“ | , c “‘D11 5'55. ‘Q . to ‘ 'L i. .‘vi‘ 1“ N‘ .‘y 3f : § \ 3.“. 0“: 24 . . . demands a great deal more involvement in a relationship on the part of the participants . . . Emphasis is placed on personality interaction. The role qualifi- cations specified in the paralled pattern are taken for granted; they may even be added to. But whatever they are, they constitute only a minimum; far more is demanded. Companionship, expression of love, recognition of personality (as dis- tinguished from mere role performance) are among the other characterizing specifications of this pattern [Bernard, 1964: 688]. As Burgess and Wallin clearly saw it, "the central shift in the nature of marriage is from a conception of marriage as a status to that of marriage as a companionship relation" [Burgess and Wallin, 1953: 25]. This clash of previous experience with current expec- tations leads to stress and doubt. ‘bne way to assuage doubt and escape from stress is to fantacize. The romantic myth of marriage is as harmful to successful marriages as the stresses which the myths seek to neutralize. Some of these myths include the idea that relationships spontaneously improve over time, the more time a couple spend together the better because their partner is capable of satisfying all needs: negative feelings are best never expressed; marriage is easy but it is hard to find the right partner [Olson, 1972]. Ryder has also suggested ". . . the ideal of compatibility or better, the general value orientation which seems to include compatibility as an ideal, may paradoxically go along with the avoidance of the non-rational and essen- tially affective aspects of human relationships"‘ [Ryder, 1967: 812]. “F" ' “:1 5 Lu: 5,- -m‘] .3 u.» 0-" 6‘ . p »~---o‘ 0‘ .u...3n L: .. '.‘V- t-‘Fp; .fifOQ3dayu "I‘I A... ” 7 -.. . he. :'I-.- ‘ u " v. f' i . l‘ en ' i..l a; ~.:‘;'-Cn fi. :35 a hea ‘ndl. . . tn ma.ba 5‘ “s K .:--'I NOE a], a” \vu J“ 4 a 5 z». _ l ‘ I“ F‘ \n l t .Ia.‘ 25 The combined forces of change in marital role expecta- tions, cultural strain inimicable to success, and the romantic myth, make it harder for couples to cope with the transition from being unattached individuals to engaged couples, and finally marriage partners. Harrower [1956] feels that this transition can be accomplished better if both partners know their own and each others goals in life. As she aptly puts it "to be forewarned is to be forearmed" [p. 190]. Making the initial adaptation to marriage in an open non-traditional structure, places a heavy burden on the effectiveness of interpersonal communication. However an Open structure is better able to handle change and utilize a wider scope of experiences [Rausch, Goodrich and Campbell, 1963]. Nonetheless, the engagement period and the first few months of marriage are critical periods. Between single status and marriage is the status of engagement. Rapoport [1963] outlines the interpersonal tasks of this status as follows: make oneself ready to take over the role of husband or wife; disengage oneself from very close relationships that would compete or inter- fere with commitment to the marriage; readjust gratification patterns to fit the newly formed marital relationship. If a couple is able to accomplish these tasks, they are more ready for marriage. However, to accomplish these transi- tions, each person will need skills of role taking, empathy, and the ability to specify role expectations. l A K.“ ' "3&1" I'm-ran- '- 26 The final theoretical problem is the definition of terms. At first glance it would appear that marital adjustment, success, satisfaction, and happiness are inter- changeable terms. There are many overlapping definitions of marriage success, adjustment and satisfaction even though these terms are not really synonomous. An understanding of Vickers' term ”appreciating" will help clarify the difference. Vickers noted that ”. . . the observation of the 'actual' and its comparison with the 'norm'--are indisoluably im- portant in their own right. This combination process I call appreciation" [Vickers, 1968: 149]. the categories by which we discriminate, the standards by which we value the repertory of responses from which we select, and our rules for selection are all mental artifacts, evolved, learned, and taught by the cultural process and more or less peculiar to the culture which produces them. This process is a circular process, in which all these settings of the appreciative system are constantly being modified by their own exercise [Vickers, 1968: 178-179]. Appreciation is a personalized set of culturally influenced normative yard sticks. Using this framework, marital satisfaction is the subjective appreciation of the marriage by the person in the marriage. In contrast, marital adjustment is the state observed by an onlooker. The onlooker applies his own appreciative system to someone's marriage. The norms of one subculture are used to evaluate the life style of another subculture. 3:):er :5 the 51:3 22:15 of .- 3 .F""1 C.‘ u ..v arse r I: pn-uu‘ b no. 35.-5U. we ‘1. ...-'A n ..g: vdod‘ eC ;-:: :.ass .' 72:33. I h . :EIS'le “-539 5‘43: 5“ I- V . 4‘ No ‘A I . »e In" It." — III C- l E<~Y :.e 5d a ‘5. 27 The onlooker describes the marriage as 'well adjusted' when the subject's marriage satisfies the normative standards of the farmer's appreciative system. In this way a couple can be well satisfied with their marriage while simultaneously judged by others as maladjusted. This could easily be the predicament, for example, of an upper class Jewish couple living in a lower class Protestant community. Persuing this further, we arrive at the concept of marriage success. No matter how satisfied a couple is with .their relationship or how adjusted others may view them, the net result needs to be such that the couple can function adequately in their community, in the social and economic spheres, and prepare their children for satisfying and functional lives, also in the context of their community. Success then is a melding of satisfaction and adjustment to produce a functioning community integrated life style. To summarize, we have discussed the centrality Of role taking and role expectations in preparing for marriage, and have shown that roles are defined in many different ways. The part played by empathy in this process has been discussed. The utility of procedures taken from the psychology of color has been recommended. Balance theory has been augmented to suggest a basis fer measuring marital relations. The theo- retical considerations of the process of role change from single adulthood to marriage status have been explored. Some . of the methodological problems in defining terms have been clarified. 0C 1! '9‘ On :32 t: EXLSt. ‘ 1 ‘ 4 6 Gnu “.1, L'f‘ Q uwfien have I CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE In fifty years, unless there is some change, the tribal custom of marriage will no longer exist. . . . The mystery and beauty of marriage and the rearing of children has pretty well broken down. John Watson 1927 Only ten years ago, all the prevailing criticisms were to the effect that the family was in decline: that it was diminished and weakened as a social institution, performing fewer social functions, a prey to moral deterioration; in short - on its way out. This kind of position — for which there is not a shred of evidence - was attacked. The family - by a few of us who took issue with the question then - was, on the contrary, upheld as a much improved social institution. . . . Only ten years after, however, a new bombard- ment of criticisms has arisen which is almost exactly the reverse . .. these new critics see it (the family) as the greatest and most tenacious obstacle to human improvement. . . . I cannot believe that the family has changed so radically in so short a time: from a decadent weakling to a tower of strength. Indeed, the family in society sometimes seems to me to have the continuing stability and firmness of a rock, when compared with the critics who dash themselves into a frenzied foam of ever-changing consternation about it. And sure enough, when each tide changes, the rock remains. Ronald Fletcher 1972 28 ~ 1 any I 1": .e 1 izazkgrcm 3 ' .An in £‘yoioa' t‘lh— "sass: ha "‘Aah- a. .J. -5595 3’} . O a Q.‘ a n. .. I.._e:‘5 “c ?: a. - «Be-spur "o..] 9 I.- Lbefise: T..e \ J37.7.5 with ‘ . -i:'='\ ~~~-.s, I 3“- ! . .«4 3:39: I_’ u: 5‘ 29 There is a vast body of publications on the impact of background factors in later marital success. In addition, the areas of marital roles, empathy, and financial management have received attention. For clarity, each topic will be presented individually, followed by this author's hypotheses. A summary of all hypotheses will conclude the chapter. Unnumbered hoards of writers have built on a combination of research findings and home-spun wisdom. Groves and Groves, for example, include a section on ”Courtship" in their 1947 volume .entitled The Contemporary American Family. The section begins with a survey of animal.behavior, the behavior of primitive tribesmen, and finally gets to U.S. courtship patterns. In reading this, one can only assume that Ernest and Gladys Groves had an arranged marriage. Other writers, such Robert 0. Blood [1955] provide a factual, narrative of steps which lead up to wedding planning. He mentions the need for training in the skills of relationship, and evaluating the relationship objectively. The area of skill training has advanced greatly since Blood's suggestion. Other writers have approached the area of planning and support for marriage from a different angle. Although they have not called it such their thinking shows apprecia- tion of network theory, and the interrelatedness of com- ponents of married living. Christensen, basing his discussion on the works of Burgess, Wallin and Cottrell, I ' “LA ~15 c S;::;re 5 Ex) 1‘ h‘ r a 3“ ‘I -.‘ g r‘ 6': 3.- e ‘0‘” re“: ‘ HA “fun“ Jr‘n L I ‘. § ~ 30 summarizes factors affecting the outcome of marriage, and identifies four areas for consideration. His four areas are: a compatible society; emotional maturity; pair unity; and marital adaptability [1958]. de Lissovoy, found that a kin network of economic and psychological support, coupled with involvement in church activities helped sustain the marriages of teenagers [1973]. Rapoport and Rapoport noted that mate selection is based on meshing the family and work related styles of both pro- spective spouses. ”The bread winner's pattern of relations 'in both regions (work and family) is likely to have much the same form because in both cases his behavior will depend upon his beliefs and expectations about his 'self' and others” [1965: 238]. Murstein, arrived at a similar conclusion in finding that ". . . what is important is the compatibility of roles with goals, not whether the roles are homogamous or complimentary" [1970: 470]. In general researchers have been less interested in how to make a good marriage. Instead they have been fascinated with measuring aspects of relationship. Two main foci have emerged from their work: the couple's adjustment, and background data. Strenuous efforts have been made to link the two, with varied success. Many writers have cataloged these attempts. Albert quoted a number of peOple on the importance of disseminating know- ledge about marriage prediction and marriage success in general [A1bert,l967]. It is amusing that Albert rises to :2 :L'allenge ’_r_1 31 the challenge by publishing the information in an almost unobtainable provincial journal. Stephens also cataloged the predictive information, and ranked factors in order of strength of predictive value. (1) those in which couples are ranked by a researcher observing them, (2) those in which the criteria of marital success is measured by comparing divorced with not-divorced couples, (3) questionnaires which ask, in a number of ways, “how do you get along?" Stephens feels that all of these methods allow certain cautious conclusions to be made. In his strongest Class A predictors he includes: 1. age at marriage ' . 2. length of acquaintanceship~ 3. premarital pregnancy 4. religiosity 5. similarity of faith 6. social class rank of couple 7. social class difference The second group, Class B predictors: 1. education 2. previous divorce 3. divorced parents (of man) 4. happiness of parents' marriage 5. where to live (not in the city) 6 . parental approval 7. social activity L con; 5. new Stephen 2% st der 1 ‘l “Sharing BC“'ev. 12v Ix. ‘Q ": ‘ t 3. -\":L‘ I “k“ ‘ Aa‘ E u_‘§‘ I 32 Third and.weakest group, Class C: 1. large age difference 2. sibling status (may be only child) 3. .poor relationship with parents 4. couple's (poor) relationship before marriage 5. mental health of individuals [Stephens, 1970]. Stephen's approach, however, is contested. Udry is the most derisive of this approach stating that "no one has produced systematic, convincing evidence that personality :matching in courtship is important in any way for marital ‘success' [Udry, 1966: 291]. He concludes that of 26 pre- marital factors associated with later marital adjustment, only two, equal mental ability and parental approval of mate, have anything to do with the relationship in question. All the other 24 background factors can be fully explained by considering them aspects of social homogamy [Udry, 1966]. BOwever, Bollingshead [1950] concluded that race,, color, religion, ethnic origin, class position and education were all found to be stratifying factors determining the type of person an individual would marry. He found that people tend to marry persons who are culturally very similar thus lending support to the theory of homogamy of choice. Burchinal surveyed the trends in marriage patterns and concluded that marriage was a poor risk for those from low status backgrounds with limited education, premarital pregnancy, or requiring the continued financial support of parents. These factors all correlate with age [Burchinal, 1965]. Eificrt. 35 . " :5 0’0. 0 . :atzl’. fol -"3. 99-h“ “use 5.9;; a-.- . V A ”"r‘ vdvn ' . . " '--A.. - .' 1 .7 ’°‘:OU"S h- Mh‘onal . ‘4":- 33 In short, marriage is a poor investment for very young, couples. Contrary to some researchers, Terman [1938] found that the following background factors had little or no correlation with marital happiness scores: family income, occupation, presence or absence of children, amount of religious training, birth order, number of opposite sex siblings, spouses of different age, or having different levels of education. Oaklander [1971] tested 29 engaged college couples and found that similarity in background and similarity in self esteem, both correlate with the presence of dis- functional communication. His findihg appears to question the generally held theory that homogamy of choice is functional. Oaklander's findings need careful replication, expansion and follow-up over time, before they can be taken as a serious challenge to the general body of research. Class A Predictors ‘ Age at Marriage In a pioneering study by Hart and Shields [1926] it was found that the best age to marry (and to stay married) was 26 for men and 24 for women. The risk for divorce was ten to one-hundred times as great for couples where one or both of them were less than 19 years old. Glick also reported that marriages of younger persons were less stable [1967, 1962]. Burchinal [1965] found that the younger ._ HA' '—-"‘ a t.- D Q , ’- n "’ 4 .-.‘- u ‘ I653». . . :.‘ar §.--. . 1" < kil Q ' EQ‘V up;- ‘a..‘:e. “Id. 1 D “§ba. s :I,‘ A}. ‘* J V .- 34 the bride the lower the status of the groom. Duval examined some of the reasons why teenage marriages have a high failure rate, and concluded that "few teenagers get as adequate preparation for marriage as they do for the other careers they enter, and so are unprepared for their jobs as husbands and wives" [Duval, 1960: 77]. Herrmann [1965] noted that teenage married couples suffer from: meager resources, rapid onset of parental responsibilities, and overly optimistic expectations about the nature of 'marriage. They quickly get into debt, and unexpected expenses precipitate a crisis. With a low level of marital skill such couples are‘candidates for marital dissolution. Burchinal and Chancellhr [1963] compiled the then available knowledge on young marriages and found that only 10 percent of high school brides marry high school grooms. One-third to one-half of the brides are pregnant, and three-quarters of the grooms are involved in premarital pregnancies. Younger marriages involve more crossing of religious lines than is found with older couples. Landis and Landis [1963] reported on 1,425 high school age marriages. Within three years, 20 percent of the couples had separated, divorced or annulled the marriage. (This was almost three times the rate of breakdown over the same length of relationship as found for the general population.) Similar findings have been published by Moss and Gingles [1965]. If. S‘- - V up i;- ,:_l:'.. ..a . '_§.-A- n . _‘ .Iu...~v- d”. baa&s \ ‘ ‘ ~C' .."” ‘s- . ."" bl. d , £;.. ‘5-“ "' “Um-v. . a . . . .. ‘. o h “*.':-e tn: . :‘g Q“ ’0‘ I UV~.‘~ :.‘9 lf‘aq ...., :3, I] 35 In summary, there is a strong inverse relationship between marital instability and age. Hypothesis Hypothesis 1. There will be a positive relationship between age and pre-marital adjustment. Length of Aquaintanceship This topic is variously arrived at by asking couples how long they have known each other or how long they have '”kept company.” Favorable responses to this question indicate that a long acquaintanceship is preferable [Burgess and Cottrell, 1939; Locke, 1951; Terman, 1938; Terman and Oden, 1947]. " Burgess and Wallin [1953] reported in their study of 1,000 couples that short acquaintanceship and high score on their Engagement Success Inventory were related. However, the scores went down when couples cameto»know each other better and infatuation was replaced by information. Thomas [1967] found that a‘six month acquaintanceship was vital. Couples who knew each other less than six months prior to marriage had a twenty percent chance of break up in the first year of marriage. Once the six month period was past, Thomas found that more time made very little difference. Bayer studied 73,000 12th grade students in 1960, and followed-up 39,000 of them in 1965. He concluded that although early dating was related to early marriage, it was the length of dating (in months) that was of importance. 553:: that 36 He found that persons of lower socioeconomic status started dating later which was followed by early marriage, whereas those of higher socioeconomic status started dating earlier in life yet delayed their marriages [Bayer, 1968]. Hypothesis Hypothesis 2. Length of acquaintanceship before marriage will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Engagement There is a general agreement that longer engagements are good omens for a positive relationship. Terman [1938] found that six month engagement or longer was ideal for men, and women needed at least three months. Burgess and Cottrell recommended at least nine months for both sexes; Locke recommends a year or more [Burgess and Cottrell, 1939; Locke, 1951]. Only Terman and Oden [1947] found no relation- ship between length of engagement and subsequent marital adjustment. Hypothesis Hypothesis 3. Length of engagement will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Premarital Pregnancy The literature is full of studies on this aspect of marriage. Premarital pregnancy is often seen as the best predictor of marital collapse. However, the author has I 'H‘“ a 2::‘ in ch: 1:: after. t". 5‘35. Rat :2;- :.cy is £219.! to 'l 2:32 :5 5C .he i i:;‘:ted a 234333 .335 \‘Q ~ “.‘3! is; ‘ \V‘ “&*SS: \ H 37 found in clinical practice that premarital pregnancy is more often than not associated with a constellation of factors. Rather than initiating a crisis, premarital pregnancy is merely the signal light which alerts the observer to the crossroads of crisis. The crossroads were evident long before the signal was in evidence. The author's findings in this area will be documented in a subsequent report. Mazer completed a careful 5 year epidemiological study .on Martha's Vineyard, Massachusetts. He found that multi- problem households were characterized by psychiatric, educational, marital and socioelegal difficulties. Pre- maritally pregnant teenage girls were but one evidence of family predicament. Such families were not over represented on the welfare roles, but were evenly distributed across the range of social classes [Mazer, 1972]. The incidence of premarital pregnancy is variously estimated as occurring in 25-83 percent of high school age marriages [Anderson\and Latts, 1965; Branham, 1965; Burchinal, 1959, 1960; Christenson and Rubenstein, 1956; Dooghe, 1968; Inselberg, 1962; Reiner and Edwards, 1973]. Christenson and Meissner [1953] followed-up 137 Indiana couples involved in premarital pregnancy between 1919 and 1952. They con- cluded that premarital pregnancy was a significant factor 'in divorce. The relationship was even stronger for couples who delayed their marriage until just before the expected birth. In addition, they found that couples who conceived f" ‘CISE t‘ :3, k .. .¢.SL k "TA“ wn‘ S.L'.‘:J ' o. - Q n 4.: . n . I I ‘~¢~QU.4 n.‘ I a mint. 1 u D e ”we . S C» : c Pt . NV. ‘3‘ M “‘1“ .ul\ I 4‘ SM. ‘1 \ c t 'l E t 1 1 at .. .1. . S 4 . I a t a m «M 3 a. .s A .5 . 3‘ 3 a 4‘ \~ s (a v a a .. u u is Pa .5 h. u N: ‘R 1. .Ausfi o» n no. \ \h 5:“ uh. K x \ h.“\ Q! ‘i‘ h‘ 1‘ .‘. O ‘C" l \ i c . u .. 38 their first child soon after the wedding have a higher rate of divorse than those who wait for several months or years to start their families. Quite obviously there is a strong relationship around the relative timing of conception in marriage, and subsequent divorce. Hypotheses Hypothesis 4. There will be an inverse relationship between having a child in the first year of marriage, and adjustment in marriage. Hypothesis 5. There will be an inverse relationship between having a child in the first year of marriage, and satisfaction in marriage. Religion It is evident that religion functions as a culture. Lenski [1961] quotes Weber to the effect that ". . . every major religious group develops its own distinctive religious orientation toward all aspects of life . . ." [Lenski, 1961: 7]. Bossard and Boll [ ] detail the aspects of life permeated by the culture of a religion, and conclude that interfaith marriages are unions of two different cultures. The Catholic religion shapes the lives of its followers. For example, in general Catholics, more so than Protestants, stress obedience over intellectual autonomy [Lenski, 1961] are less prone to anomie [Dean, 1962] but also are more likely to be in lower level jobs than educated non*Catholics [Rapoport and Rapoport, 1965]. Social mobility is even fasted b; . it a; re: act ha'. 3::estant c :rbzlity. Lass; also a: 232-Cat? - I . ". effected by church involvement. As Lenski has concluded '. . . it appears that involvement in the Catholic church does not have the same consequences as in the white Protestant churches. At best, it seems to be irrelevant to mobility, and at worst, something of a hindrance" [p. urn. Lenski also found that the differences between Catholics and non-Catholics are still apparent even after adjustments are made for social class position [Lenski, 1961]. The relationship between religion and social culture is not a one way street. Burchinal and Chancellor [1963] found a correlation of plus .24 between urbanism and mixed marriage suggesting that urbanism may be more influential than religion in some situations. The reason why mixed marriages occur is a topic for speculation. A common explanation is that such marriages occur according to the ratio of Catholics to non-Catholics in an area, with additional consideration given to ethnic and socioeconomic status factors [Bouma, 1963; Burchinal and Chancellor, 1962; Thomas, 1956; Vincent, 1959]. Heiss [1960] studied 1,167 persons in New York, ranging in age from 20 through 59. His findings were especially relevant in explaining Catholic intermarriage, with less application to Protestants and Jews. His conclu- sions suggest that there is a particular family pattern permeating beyond religious boundaries which predisposes persons to intermarry. Religiously intermarried persons are characterized by: 4O 1. Non-religious parents 2. Greater dissatisfaction with parents during the early years 3.. More strife in the early years of the family 4. Less integration in the early years of the family 5. A greater degree of emancipation from the parents at the time marriage occurred Thomas [1951] and Vincent [1971] both suggest that mixed marriages are on the increase. Vincent suggests .that 33% of Catholics are involved in interfaith marriages. Thomas also quotes the Catholic Directory for 1950 which indicates that 26.2% of marriages were interfaith marriages. He feels that the rate is closer to 30%, and at least 50% if all the marriages not sanctioned by the church are also - included. Heiss [1961] reports that the mid-town Manhattan study of 1,660 persons found 35.2% of those married were in an interfaith marriage. Figures are often inaccurate because of conversion. Crockett, Babchuk and Ballweg [1969] samples 233 mid-western families. Of those couples where one was raised in a different faith 83% changed to a common affiliation. Of these, 68% changed close to the time of the wedding. The change is in the direction of the spouse with the most education. Besancency [1970] digested the findings of a number of studies on interfaith marriage researched on 0.5. and Canadian populations between 1943 and 1962. He concluded ". . . there is no important trend in intermarriage for ‘2' ‘1ch 1n :2.-5: betas-t“ 25:: is no J 7e than it. , . . ' .,. . 'F‘Ru‘ '5‘ 3“ “|fi\uan "no 0: a a‘. '-==- 69:5 5: - n . ‘ “3311's; '2:;‘ . alcv‘l . ae“ 9.- u ‘5 litters Y § 0‘... teaSOH‘ 41 Catholics in this country . . ." [p. 66]. The ratio remains steady between 26.1 and 29%. Besancency felt also that there is no logical reason to suppose that more Catholic women than men marry non-Catholics. However the inaccurate way in which data is collected, and the habit of confusing prewedding converts with life-long Catholics, leads to the possible conclusion that more Catholic women than men actually marry non-Catholics [Besancency, 1970]. The other aspect of interfaith marriages of interest in this study is the relative rate of divorce. Several observers have linked interfaith marriage, premarital pregnancy and low socioeconomic status with divorce [Burchinal and Chancellor, 1963; Christensen and Rubenstein, 1956]. Hence it appears to be a cluster of facts rather than the interfaith combination pg£_§g which leads to divorce. Landis [1949] reported on over 4,000 cases. He found where divorce occurred involving a Catholic and a Protestant, it was the combination of a Catholic husband and a Protestant wife that was more likely to end in divorce or separation. He reasoned the difference was due to the fact that 75% of divorce are initiated by wives. Catholic women would find it hard to do this against their church's ruling. Also the Catholic church's requirement that children must be raised Catholic puts a heavier burden on a Protestant wife since she would be faced with raising children in a faith to which she could not subscribe. Burchinal and o 1‘ scene: [ we... . 5‘. ‘ -.sen..a- 7 Vernon 2.7339 rate C" ' ‘ ~53 & 0 0’1)» DO. 9‘ x \— 5559102.: ex;liatic \ :32 Cathol; 42 Chanceller [1963] could not replicate Landis' finding of differential rates. Vernon felt that there is much distortion in reporting divorce rates [1960]. Table l. A comparison between probability of divorce and probability of success in marriage, by religious affiliation. Religious Percentage Percentage affiliation divorcing enduring Both Catholic 4.4 95.6 'Both Jewish 5.2 94.8 Both Protestant 6.0 94 .0 Mixed Catholic- Protestant 14.1 85.9 Both no faith 17.9 82.1 Source: 'Vernon, 1970: 295-298. As Vernon indicated, in the first column the probability of divorce increases 200-300 percent for mixed marriages. In the second table the difference in success is only about 10 percent. The final area to be considered is that of frequency of attendance of religious services. Burgess and Cottrell found in the 1930s that a high level of religious activity was positively associated with good marital adjustment [Burgess and Cottrell, 1939]. Dyer and Luckey [1961] did not find this association. Gordon :5: Catholic sale rate interfait :5: 35 both 'trgs may i 311:;er 3 l axial- dist; ] I .253 sea-cut '45 is not‘ 43 Gordon [1971] found that of persons whose parents are both Catholic 92 percent have some type of faith. The com- parable rate for Protestants is 68% and for those born into an interfaith union 34%. It sounds like Catholics have the best of both worlds. LeMasters writes, however, that “things may seem simple for the Roman Catholics, but this is largely an illusion. The fact is that considerable social distance separates a really devout Catholic from a less devout member of that faith. . . . Theoretically, .this is not a mixed religious marriage, but in actuality it is” [LeMasters, 1957: 328-329]. In summary, religious belonging is a complex and significant factor affecting the outcome of marriages. Hypotheses Hypothesis 6. Entering an interfaith marriage will be negatively related to pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 7. Interfaith marriages will not be different in level of adjustment from same faith marriages. Hypothesis 8.‘ Interfaith marriages will not be different in level of satisfaction from same faith marriages. Hypothesis 9. Frequent church attendance by both partners will be positively related to marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 10. Frequent church attendance by both partners will be positively related to marital satisfaction. ’ b .n .5. :: miaoe ..::., ,. . 4.5.93.8 .8313 ~lo‘. . k :4": 13 u 9 ."Q ......3t;cn V ‘f I; 3““), . ‘.“'OU.. . I A. ‘ ’:‘~ :‘ n . .1. “1’ a ‘- “v Mab.e“. 2"" n ‘ H" ‘ "5118.1 44 Socioeconomic Factors In this section, Stephen's final to "Class A" predictor of marriage success, socioeconomic rank and socioeconomic difference will be considered together. Terman [1938] did not exactly measure socioeconomic status in his 1930 study, but he did find little or no correlation between family income and occupation, and happiness in marriage. Moss [1964] and Mazer [1972] found no relationship between socioeconomic status and the type of family system which produced unstable or acting out ‘children. Other observers, working individually, found a positive relationship between low socioeconomic status, early marriage and premarital pregnancy [Burchne11, 1958; Burchnell and Chancellor, 1963; Dooghe, 1968; Moss and Gingles, 1959]. Blood commented on the theme of homogamy and socioeconomic status. "The social pressures and personal preference that cause homogamy in the first place help to cement homogomous marriages" [Blood, 1969: 71]. Winch has observed that most people do not experience great shifts in status, although there is more mobility in distinctively urban occupations [Winch, 1963]. If a woman marries up socially it is more acceptable socially, and leads to less marital strife than if she marries down [Besancency, 1970; Brown, 1951]. Glass, studying an English group of 4,858 marriages, found that the majority were homogomous, with only 315 where the woman married up, and 134 where she married down [Glass, 1954]. (- V & 1 er} t, L. l of ..11 ‘on to 1 no". ‘ . “-0 p d . . r" = Ad M . -: :' no. I lesion. . :“ ' “I e. 2. a. 0 De ‘I . “n “U o .a . . s. ~nu I u a s. R. -nm .6... ..u. .5. .rs .. ._. a n. a vact‘ ‘ta w s an‘ vs 1 'n .:H s ‘0. . on. : - ‘w “.n s I“... ‘- .5 '32" .‘Q. q... 4S Roth and Peck assessed the marital adjustment of couples in relation to their social class status. They found that 53 apercent of homogonous marriages had good adjustment. However if the man married down this dropped to 35 percent, and still lower to 28 percent when the woman married down. Roth and Peck also suggest that downwardly mobile people in general produce unstable marriages. Disruption occurs if only one spouse is upwardly mobile [Roch and Peck, 1951]. This was confirmed by Westley and Epstein [1969] in their intensive longitudinal study of Canadian families. LeMasters reports that much of the information about the impact of social class on marital adjustment had not been recognized at the time Burgess and Cottrell, Locke, Burgess and Wallin, and Terman completed their major studies on marital adjustment. This may explain why Terman found no relationship between occupation, level of income, and marital adjustment. He had not fully allowed for a major variable [LeMasters, 1957]. In short the major researchers missed the fact that social class is a cultural designation in the same way as is religion. Hence it is pertinent to review the more outstanding differences between the working class and middle class cultures. For all Americans rationality is an active element in the norms of action, but we recog- nize a lower class cultural complex by the fact that rationality is subordinated to the dominant value of security, and its traditiona— listic, particularistic, concomitants. We thus expect to find, in the lower class as in the upper class, rationality harnessed to the services of traditionalistic, particularistic 355 a: epposi culari servic applic are ma each C With t :ne er relat: eases :52hng In a | ?' . ‘5‘! t1- ., ‘.‘ 4 .;: I '35 {1“ v a». ' ‘4‘n 6. L s | I EVE 46 ends and means. In the middle class the Opposite obtains, with traditionalistic, parti- cularistic elements being harnessed in the service of rationality, permitting its free application to limited areas while other areass are maintained in a given position. But in each case one set of values is held primary, with the other in a secondary position; the one encompasses the other, and it is this relationship that yields the striking differ- ences in cultural orientation among classes [Schneider and Smith, 1973: 26]. In a similar way, authority is conceived of in different conceptual terms. Kohn suggests that in middle class families, authority is used to teach the acceptable- 'motives and feelings. By contrast, Kohn indicates that working class authority is aimed at controlling overt acts \ [Kohn, 1959]. ~ Probably a degree of cultural fusion has taken place due to the spread of urbanization, itself having a specific cultural form, although like class, urbanization also reflects the impact of wider, national cultural influences [Nottridge, 1972]. Rainwater and Handel [1964], suggest that the tradi- tional working class patterns of role segregation in marriage are breaking down. Working class marriages are showing signs of becoming more like middle class nuclear families. However this view is not everywhere evident. Klein, writing about English cultural themes in the mid 1950s notes that working class patterns are enforced from within to the extent that ". . . if a wife continues to look even reasonably attractive after marriage, she is V 27359.): cc £55: 139]. 235 that 1 tin; cla: :fteselv :2 21a: th égaeral p skenzie [ F" 1* earlv 21;: .t 60“; u" i ' sea: Her. I .n: 47 adversely commented upon, by women as well as men" [Klein, 1965: 109]. Hoggart [1959] writing on the same theme, noted that publications, sold for, and preferred by the working class, reinforce and continue their cultural view of themselves and others. Bossard and Boll [1950] point out that the milieu of life shapes the ritual of living. A general pervasive theme runs through each cultural group. Mackenzie [1973] studied skilled craftsmen in America in the early 19708 and again confirmed the self perpetuating ‘nature of patterns and relationships. "Overwhelmingly blue collar workers choose other blue collar employees as friends, while clerks and managers remained similarly isolated" [p. 153]. In addition to this, companions are chosen better than two to one only from the same group of skilled workers. It is not extreme to conclude that cross-class marriages are subject to strains not unlike those experienced in cross (national) cultural marriages. Kingsley Davis described the predicament in terms of the difficulties experienced by children in such marriages, but the same is true for the spouses. Since marriage is an institutional mechanism for procreating and rearing children, the requirements of the status ascription in a caste order practically require the marriage of equals. The wife reared in a social stratum widely different from her husband's is apt to inculcate ideas and behavior incompatible with the position the children will inherit from their father, thus creating hiatus between their status and their role [Davis, 1949: 378]. E‘gpoti r47. be yes Frtpo '- C‘a“ "bH. a 42:35 4 at 5“ C... I U “.5 a] 5..» s m: ‘§ I .’A_a‘ V 'r:.v'a‘ C ‘é C“ ‘&:85 e .. I ‘1‘“ S‘a: ‘ ‘. 53:4 -: 4- 1“e¥ ‘ Q“ \im. ~‘e‘ ‘ I" . “UR 48 Hypotheses Hypothesis ll. Homogamy of socioeconomic background will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 12. Homogamy of socioeconomic background will be positively associated with marital adjustment. Hypothesis 13. Homogamy of socioeconomic background will be positively associated with marital satisfaction. Class B Predictors Stephens suggested five class B factors having 'intermediate value in predicting marital success. These factors are: education, divorce of parents, happiness of parent's marriage, happiness of childhood, and parent's approval of the marriage. Education Education is noteworthy for three reasons: its acts to level social class and religious differences, in less than average amounts it is associated with low socioeconomic status and premarital pregnancy, and in liberal amounts associated with marital adjustment. Koos [1963] sums up the first point "college friends, for example, become marriage partners without the strict regard for family characteristic of the upper class or for ethnic qualities characteristic of the lower class” [p. 53]. The relationship between low status, pregnancy and interrupted education has already been noted in this thesis. Emotional instability 1.-.: y”, I:)J:‘at : 0. - ~£ .3233” ‘D-. 4:. R- u.' I r‘ ' ‘ 0V '- _ . ‘- n. x,‘ . . I. . I“.‘ ‘7- V..~u !.' u "“5 2 O L \ SK ‘\ I u 2 d 5., . , I it) A} p, ' 49 is associated, in women, with dropping out of high school, which is often coupled with pregnancy [Bauman, 1967]. Cervantes [1965] did a very careful study of 25 matched pairs of lower class students in each of six cities. He found that school dropouts reported their homes as signifi- cantly less happy (P less than .001) than did the graduates. Bernard noticed the ”Glick Effect" in her research, i.e., income, education, and occupation are three closely associated status variables. As the variables increase, .the proportion of stable marriages also increases [Bernard, 1966]. The table below summarizes the findings on the relationship between stable marriages and education. 5 Table 2. Relationship between education achievement and stability of marriage. Educational level associated Researcher with stability Burgess and Cottrell [1939] H: BA or BA+ W: BA or BA+ Davis [1929] . W: Beyond HS Hamilton [1926] equal level: preferably some college beyond HS King [1951] H: BA or BA+ Locke [1951] w: HS or HS+ Terman [1938] B: HS+ Terman and Oden [1947] W: more than H if difference not large H-husband, Wswife, B=both, HS=high school, BA=college degree Weses 3290:! 3 9 wk , go- dcao Lei: '12ka . ..” Ass.‘ 0. . I (a I "”35: .1 8" " ‘I‘A I O “. '- 4.1: 2:64... . astute r :1 n: SO Hypotheses Hypothesis 14. Where the averaged intra-couple educa- tional level is more than 12 years this level of achievement will be positively associated with pre—marriage adjustment. Parental Divorce This factor was not included in all of the major studies, and is also not included in this thesis. The only major researcher to look into this area was Locke [1951] and he found no relationship between the marital status of 'parents and the marital stability of their children's marriages. The death of a parent has a similar effect on children as does divorce, yet the factor is not even mentioned by the major researchers. Happiness of Parent's Marriage In a sense, this is the positive compliment of the previous section. The literature overwhelmingly supports the proposition that if a person's parents are happily married, then he has a high probability of following in their footsteps [Cottrell, 1933; Locke, 1951; Schroeder, 1939; Terman, 1938; Terman and Oden, 1947]. Other obser- vers, notably Burgess and Wallin [1953], Burgess and Cottre11[l939], Curtis and Mahan [1956] and Wallin [1954], found that it is especially important for the man to have had parents who were happily married. Westley and Epstein [1969] suggest the reason for this relationship between ‘ 2152131 Ed! 2.3511. If his tionsf the labor the c healt A ha; Hazy chi .. :.1~ " qu'd. :.236 a: s t i v E . ' w {113 ‘;c- l \\‘S‘\ 1 D :1 _ — 51 parental marriage and subsequent marital stability of children. If his parents have a positive status rela- tionship (the father being Of higher status than the mother), a balanced division of labor, and a father-lead pattern of authority, the child is almost certain to be emotionally healthy [p. 166]. A happy, stable marriage not only produces emotionally healthy children, but also gives the children a role model to follow. They ”know" what marital happiness can be like. Since most persons use their parent's marriage as a norm ’ 'against which to evaluate their own marriages, it is not surprising that they would try to emulate a good thing [Mayer, 1967]. People whose parents did not have a good marriage will be at a disadvantage: they will have to model against an example, not knowing at a deep level specifically how a happy marriage Operates. Ryder found two interesting points in this respect. If a husband is able to fit in well with the cultural stereotype of his role, he is happier. So is his wife if he is able to demonstrate his interest, understanding and support for her. On the other hand ”. . . wives who report negative things about the families in which they grew up are found in marriages where both spouses complain about the marriage" [Ryder, 1970: 62]. Hypothesis Hypothesis 15. Reported happiness of parents' marriages will be positively related to the pre-marriage adjustment of the couple. at \l I w ..o. 2:;2ess 232:3;85 1.. on“, " ”‘U";V1 f '3: a"-‘\ 52 Happiness in Childhood Again findings substantiate the suggestion that happiness in childhood predisposes people to have happy marriages [Burgess and Wallin, 1953; Locke, 1951; Terman, 1938; Terman and Oden, 1947]. This general finding is also substantiated in intensive small sample studies by Goodrich, Ryder and Rausch [1968] and also by Curtis and Mahan [1956]. Hypothesis Hypothesis 16. Reporting happiness in childhood will be positively related to prefimarriage adjustment. \ Parental Attitude All of the studies mentioned previously (see above) which assessed this item found that happiness in marriage was associated with parent's approval Of the marriage. Udry decided that this was one Of the two premarital factors associated with marital happinesss which could not be fully explained by social homogamy [Udry, 1966]. Hypothesis Hypothesis 1?. Parental approval prior to marriage will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 18. Reporting difficulty with the in-laws 'will be positively related to parental disapproval of the :marriage prior to the marriage. Hypothesis 19. Reporting difficulty with the in-laws will be inversely related to marital adjustment. . I v “‘ .21. :e 1“. M ‘— Re ‘nfi .l'llvfl‘ 7‘ host.“ 1|. I-—.... "-:'-'35 a: “a 'O“: an r). h'. . I. if!” ‘\ A :- ‘vn: . '- 53 Hypothesis 20. Reporting difficulty with the inflaws will be inversely related to marital satisfaction. Table 3. ~Relationship between age difference of spouses and marital stability. Findings favoring Researcher marital stability" Bernard [1934] H: 0-10 years older w: 0-5 years younger Burgess and Cottrell [1939] H: 1-3 years older, or same age as wife Locke [1951] About same age King [1951] ' sH: 4-7 years Older Haw: same age Terman and Oden [1947] All differences favorable, except when husband is 1-3 years younger H = husband W = wife Burr Suggested that no difference in age is the most satisfactory, and that age difference has a curvi-linear relationship with marital stability [Burr, 1973]. Hypothesis Hypothesis 21. There will be a curvilinear relation- ship between age difference and pre-marriage adjustment. Birth Order The value of this factor is hotly contested. There are basically four responses to it: Thurstone and Thurstone [1930], and Stroup and Hunter [1965] find no relationship Gav. k. u '3“; o ~u~ . a . . ‘ ‘ \ ‘I A N --:“c‘1d' :‘o::.““" In. '09.. 4 ”“v-: - ‘m.=b a u}... ~ h‘... Q -~ 5:30 :.“0'73 ’ «.." l 54 between personality characteristics and ordinal position of birth. The second category of response is that of Koch [1960], Forer [1969] and others. Their view is that ordinal position is a factor in determining personality. The bulk Of research supports them. First born children are more ready to participate as volunteers [Capra and Dittes, 1962; Suedfeld, 1964]; react with more anxiety to experimental situations and drop out more readily [Schachter, 1959]; ‘endorse an earlier assumption of responsibility by children [Harris and Howard, 1968]; are more Often more dependent than second children (Sears, 1950]; are more likely to enter college [Smelser and Steward, 1968]; are disprOportionally represented among lists of eminent peOple [Galton, 1874]; and are seen as being more jealous and selfish [Davis and Havighurst, 1947]. Oldest males tend to marry to an earlier age than later born males [Murdoch, 1966; MacDonald, 1967]. However age of marriage for females is not signi- ficantly associated with their birth order [MacDonald, 1967], but Oldest females have a harder time in marriage than oldest males [Hall, 1965]. Other studies suggest that a child's sex and ordinal position in the family do influence, not only the way parents and siblings treat the child, but also shape career choices, social relationships and educational attainment [Altus, 1959; Forer, 1969; Koch, 1960; Sutton-Smith, 1964]. '..- ‘ "IL. 4.33 .Ceu . '. 1. _TE.‘V’ :e‘a --'--- nu... I. .53: ab“ "‘ .0 :9}! v '-::..u“‘.) a A. u: I, .73» .7 A 4 ‘Vnfi O I 'A “‘11: r“ ‘ u. ' v I |‘- in \" z ‘ SH I \c H“ w 1- 'v ’~ . t l 33. - L . t :"‘ x‘ . 55 Heiss found intermarriage across religious lines is posi- tively related to being a youngest or only child, negatively related to being the oldest child [Heiss, 1960]. A group of researchers found that couples who had a premarital pregnancy as one of their problems in marriage, also had a greater than chance probability of one or both of them having come from mono-sex sibling constilations [Dame, 22.2l-r 1966]. Forer [1969] describes in general terms the different traits of persons inthe four birth positions. The oldest tends to expect other people to be relatively less capable. The middle child has less specific expectations about the capabilities of other people. The young- est may see others as more adequate while the only child tends to think, 'I am most secure when there are parents around to take care of me, but when they are not there, I have no one to turn to for help, so I'd better learn to take care of myself as much as possible [p. 6]. The third approach is based on Adler's theories and championed strongly by Walter Toman. For Toman, "birth order rules all" [Toman, 1970]. It is the factor which explains much of marriage. Toman has a complicated formula for assessing a person's birth status which takes into account details about the individual and his parents. Toman has found that disturbed couples have less rank compli- mentarity. Divorced couples evidenced fewer opposite sex siblings and more early losses of family members. At the same time, happily married couples had more Opposite sex siblings, and complimentary rank relationship. Fu- Ln‘t "I v 3‘. . I do.‘ -. . ‘ .l' "l?- '005. 5.: 25211011 .. 1H». Of.“ 1 .hh a“ RV t 1H! r c e w . . and b vs .1 0 Lin «r1: o 3. J 5 m1 3 £1 at u. .7. C m #5» +5 .. .. .5 q. a. .5 . o a 3.“ n\.- v ..“- F‘. O c. a." ..c. ..... We. "4; 81a "Vt 56 The key concepts in TOman's work are rank or ordinal position, and sex. For example, considering a man who is the oldest child in a two sibling family with one younger sister. He will find that his best marriage match is a woman who is also from a two child family and who has one older brother. Both Of them had experience with Opposite sex siblings, and experiences in ordinal positions that compliment the other. It is beyond the scope of this thesis to catalog all the fine points of Toman's position, but his basic prOposition will be tested. The fourth position on birth order is that Of KOnig who states that from early infancy we are modelled by being either male or female. We are equally modelled and destined by our rank at birth. . . for everyone of us the rank of birth was chosen according to the plan of life assigned to him by destiny. . . . the inner nature of the individual is wisely prepared to conform to these assignments of destiny [K6nig, 1963: 55]. KOnig elaborates further that each person in their particu- lar rank order of birth has a special relationship with and to the world. He does step back from this whole approach a little, and indicates ". . . only the social habits and no others are determined by the place the child holds in his family circle” [K6nig, 1963: 55]. Unfortunately, KOnig did not see fit to research destiny, and supports his assertions with personal anecdotes, biblical parables and the sayings of historic personages. « v . ."73 ’2'" :' outd‘ 95‘ 4. " s 5121 2". C I h: ‘ a, in. \' n ‘fitr- ‘t in i ‘38 L ‘41 av. ‘6; ; 0. S7 Hypotheses Hypothesis 22. Complimentary rank order and sex position will be positively related to pre—marriage adjustment. Family Size Much has been said in folk lore about the glories of a large family. Today, Zero Population Growth fans laud the small family. Both types of families have their strengths and their weaknesses. The major studies mentioned earlier 'in this thesis do not address themselves to the relative size of families of origin of the subjects. Rosen [1964] found a curvi-linear relationship between value transmission and family size. Thayer [1971] found that persons coming from large families are less accurate than persons coming from small families, in reporting certain private emotional ex- periences. Thayer's and Rosen's studies raise the possibi- lity of family size Operating as a facet of micro-culture. .Although Bossard and Boll did not empirically support their findings they report distinctly different family patterns in large as opposed to small families. Some of these qualities are as follows. In large families parenthood is extensive, cooperation with the leader, discipline, and specialization of role and function are stressed. The group is emphasized over the individual. Large families are vulnerable to major crises such as the death of a parent. Small families are characterized by an 1 :a:a.l tL‘. 35!? 15 a L’EILC'CC he ‘ “343:9. ror . O N Q- ‘c 4% \‘.‘ I “Q a f: ‘ ‘\ . ‘5‘, \\ 5'". 9: K i \ a‘o: . s, a “‘ 58 overall theme of planning. Parenthood is intensive, and there is an interactional pattern which tends to be demo- cratic-cooperative. Activity is individualized, and there is strong pressure to succeed [Bossard, 1963; Bossard and Boll, 1966]. The summaries of these authors appear to favor the large family, however, their studies do indicate the difference in modus Operendi between the two types of family systems. The nature of this difference makes it reasonable to view a marriage between a "small family" per- son and a "large family” person as a type of cross-cultural marriage. For convenience, we are using Rosen's categorizations with regard to family size: \ Small family: one to two children Medium family: three to four children Large family: five or more children [Rosen, 1964]. Hypotheses Hypothesis 23. There will be a positive relationship between similarity in size of family of origin, and a couple's pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 24. There will be no difference in pre- marriage adjustment between couples in which both are from small families, and couples where both are from large families. Mental Health This is one of Stephen's final Class C indicators. The evidence is conflicting. If we start with Opler's I'm‘: "".’ 1 ”:_‘, ;A.A;l a, ‘ ..“..‘j ‘1 S9 findings from a 1956 Midtown Manhattan Community Mental Health Research Study, prospects are grim indeed. Opler and his colleagues found over 80 percent of people studied, had some evidence of emotional disability. 36.3 percent had mild symptom formation, 21.8 percent moderate symptom fOrmation, and 23.4 percent had marked, severe or incapaci- tating impairments [Opler, 1967]. However, these findings were not used in reference to predicting marital stability. Generally speaking, psychodynamic observers and researchers suggest that people marry mates with compatible ineuroses. As Kassorla has written, "we all look for the person who, like ourselves, has the same happiness limits, as well as the same style for dealing with guilt, anxiety, fear and other emotional pain" [Kassorla, 1973: 14]. Other writers focus more on the pathology of this process, seeing it in part as a reliving of earlier experiences. "In many cases it will be found that lying behind the actual problem is the conflict concerned with the relationship to the parents, and that this has been, or is still being, projected onto the partner as resentment and hatred" [Griffith, 1957: 124]. The research in this area of early marriage adjustment suggests several possibilities. Murstein [1970] followed two groups of 99 and 98 couples respectively through their courtship and early marriage. He found that people chose partners with similar levels of self acceptance, and that progress in courtship is aided by this quality, plus .. :jxrazge :ges v1“. 22:12: at 2;:2: he "= " "es '0. 'b JV t 2:31:01 :3... neg *' Dbl. ‘ \ "M. i 4...?“ f“ :h"‘ P‘ V g -"r". b. . I“ ' ha ..““. P U 9 r ..‘ .*.1~ . . 60 comparable degrees of neutroticism. He also found that couples who have deeper levels of self disclosure are better at predicting each others responses. In addition, the higher the individual's self esteem, the more selective is his process of mate selection. Progress in courtship was found to be significantly associated [E’ less that .01] with mental health of the man [Murstein, 1967]. Barry also found that the mental health of the husband is crucial in assuring marital happiness. If a husband is able to be emotionally ’supportive, the possibility of severe, destructive conflict is greatly reduced (Barry, 1970]. Murstein, with another group of 60 volunteer couples married just over one year, found that the wife's self acceptance, plus her ability to accurately predict her husband, were positively related to marital happiness [Murstein, 1972]. This does sound like a situation of women accommodating the quirks of their husbands! Eshleman [1965] surveying 82 young couples married six months, found that personality adjustment is related to marital integra- tion. Barry [1970] and Trost [1967] both suggest that homogamy is helpful in social background characteristics. Trost suggests that it is perceived rather than actual homogamy in personality characteristics which is important and operative (Trost, 1967]. Rapoport and Rapoport [1965] found that students who married while in college were academically better, and had faer re. . - “hoe... D - :‘aa. C. ‘O-t‘. . 4 : cg .Vk “l0: :s- .. 61 fewer marital problems than students who married after. graduation. 0n the other hand, Martinson [1955] studied well matched control and experimental samples of 59 high school girls, and concluded that ". . . persons who marry (in high school or soon after) demonstrate greater feelings of ego deficiency than do persons who remain single" [p. 162]. Udry [1967] has remained unimpressed by all this research. He hypothesized that personality match would not predict who would marry who. He followed 150 volunteer engaged couples and conlcuded that there was no evidence that would indicate ". . . that those engaged couples who break their engagements have different personality match from those who marry" [p. 723]. He concluded that mate perception was a more important factor. Since this thesis is not intended as a study of personalities, no attempt will be made to measure individuals in this way. However, mate perception is an important factor, and will be fully covered in the section dealing with prediction and empathy. Sexual Experience Most researchers have asked in retrospect "did you have sexual relations prior to marriage?" Since folk lore says that no one has sex before marriage--except prostitutes and men of questionable character--the results are a fore- gone conclusion. All of the major studies mentioned earlier 35:21: pe 2:: ll, 2:qu vi fitment zzis who may. vi: 23:31 a fallacy 0 2:3, Re 21:51 p Thi Ifiiiness XL: w fill a ‘6' -. ‘- ' h .."~ii n nu ‘AI 0 \a a 62 by such people as Hamilton, Davis, Turman, Burgess and. Cottrell, Burgess and Wallin, and Locke, find that pre- marital virginity in wives is associated with good marital adjustment. Curiously, Terman [1938] also found that hus- bands who refrained from having premarital intercourse, except with their "virgin" wives-to-be, also had good marital adjustment. This contradiction points to the fallacy of asking people to admit to socially unacceptable acts. Reference can also be made to the estimate of pre- marital pregnancy presented earlier in this thesis. This writer felt that using this question in a marital readiness test would only provoke resistance and dissimula- tion among couples, and make the Counselor's work harder. Summary of Hypotheses in Chapter II Hypothesis 1. There will be a positive relationship between age and pre-marital adjustment. Hypothesis 2. Length of acquaintanceship before marriage will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 3. Length of engagement will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 4. There will be an inverse relationship between having a child in the first year of marriage, and adjustment in marriage. Hypothesis 5. There will be an inverse relationship between having a child in the first year of marriage, and satisfaction in marriage. lh.:’ !" p w'ivo' o n - u . -.'..5 D C Hoe”. '. H Al . ,, I- to. In"; ....v. v n P 'u I. 9- AA 1 ..‘.. A Q. _' o~-_.l 9‘ M as; "O U ‘. h" I ‘Q ‘ .\‘~ ~.‘ , ‘. _. \ «5.; “4‘: . ‘i . h 4. 5: '§ 63 Hypothesis 6. Entering an interfaith marriage will be negatively related to pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 7. Interfaith marriages will not be dif- ferent in level of adjustment from same faith marriages. Hypothesis 8. Interfaith marriages will not be different in level of satisfaction from same faith marriages. Hypothesis 9. Frequent church attendance by both partners will be positively related to marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 10. Frequent church attendance by both partners will be positively related to marital satisfaction. Hypothesis ll. Homogamy of socioeconomic background will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 12. Homogamy of socioeconomic background will be positively associated with marital adjustment. Hypothesis l3. Homogamy of socioeconomic background will be positively associated with marital satisfaction. Hypothesis 14. Where the averaged intra-couple educa— tional level is more than twelve years this level of achievement will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 15. Reported happiness of parent's marriages will be positively related to pre—marriage adjust- ment of the subjects. Hypothesis 16. Reporting happiness in childhood will be positively related to pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 17. Parental approval prior to marriage will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. 64 Hypothesis 18. Reporting difficulty with the inflaws will be positively related to parental disapproval of the marriage prior to the marriage. , Hypothesis 19. Reporting difficulty with the in-laws will be inversely related to marital adjustment. Hypothesis 20. Reporting difficulty with the in-laws will be inversely related to marital adjustment. Hypothesis 21. There will be a curvilinear relation- ship between age difference and pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 22. Complimentary rank order and sex 'position will be positively related to pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 23. There will be‘a positive relationship between similarity in size of family or origin, and a couple's pre-marriage adjustment. Hypothesis 24. There will be no difference in pre- marriage adjustment between couples in which both are from small families, and couples where both are from large families. CHAPTER I II MARITAL ASSESSMENT If you see a gentleman that is courteous, obliging, and good-natured to everybody, except a certain female that lives under the same roof with him, to whom he is unreason— ably cross and ill-natured, it is his wife. If you see a male and female continually jarring, checking, and thwarting each other, yet under the kindest terms and appelations immaginable, as my dear, etc., they are man and wife. The present state of Matrimony in South Britain: \ Wives eloped from husbands 2,361 Husbands ran away from wives 1,362 Married pairs in a state if sepa- ration from each other 4,120 Married pairs living in a state of open war, under the same roof 191,023 Married pairs living in a state of inward hatred for each other though concealed from the world 162,320 Married pairs living in a state of coldness and indifference for each other\ 510,123 Married reputed happily in the esteem of the world 1,102 Married pairs comparatively happy 135 Married pairs absolutely and entirely happy 9 Married pairs in South Britain 872,564 Anonymous 1785 65 l a? ‘I .2 ...e'.'.: 33323280 . :12 "'“at ‘1: g ‘ n 66 The previous chapter dealt with background factors and. reported how they have been assessed in the literature. This chapter focuses on the assessment of current function- ing in marriage, with emphasis on the relationship rather than what went before. Assessing adjustment is a precarious business both socially and professionally. The above quotation written in 1785 aptly sums up the typical feelings about adjustment, but does very little to help us understand what adjustment is. This author has already defined how the term will be used in this thesis, but, everyone else has their own idea. Burgess and Cottrell wrote in 1939 that A well adjusted marriage. . . may be defined as a marriage in which the attitudes and acts of each of the partners produce an environment which is favorable to the functioning of the personality of each, particularly in the sphere of primary relationships [p. 10]. By 1953 Burgess had teamed up with Locke and defined a well adjusted marriage as . . . a union where the attitudes and the acts of husband and wife are in agreement on the chief issues of marriage such as handling finances and dealing with inlaws, where they have come to an adjustment upon interests, objectives and values: where they are in harmony on demonstrations of affec- tion and the sharing of confidences; and where they have few or no complaints about their marriage [p. 383]. In the 14 year time span, the definition has shifted emphasis from individual needs to couple needs. Locke wrote in 1951 that: sf at" 'b 67 . adjustment (is) associated with adaptability, as measured by 'giving in' in arguments, not being domineering, slow- ness in getting angry, and quickness in getting over anger [p. 205]. This shows a keener understanding that conflict is part of marriage. Burgess and Wallin used the following criteria to measure marital success or adjustment: 1. 2. 3. permanence of union marital happiness measured by self report satisfaction in the marriage measure by statements about each other and about the union The type of consensus which is measured by an absence of disagreements and the presence of agreement Love and affection ‘ Sexual satisfaction . companionship, confiding in each other and having common interests compatibility of personality and temperament [Burgess and Wallin, 1953]. Many subsequent researchers used their work as a norm, and many replications and variations on their work have followed. Hicks and Platt [1970] did an extensive review of the literature on marital happiness and stability and arrived at the following broad conclusions: Q 0 03 £1105 (A) N H 0 marriage and divorce decisions are influenced by the macro-social system stability is a function of many factors only one of which is marital happiness the instrumental role of the husband is crucial to marital happiness children detract from marital happiness low happiness may be associated with marital stability researcher should not rely on self report to test marital happiness. "' :‘Fec‘ no .5. 1 23.367.57.15 | ,. . Q :2: ..s{ n‘ I“ " e: l t :N‘V:A k“. "wuss. u 7‘:V:w : ,".vc _. . IAA"! k.‘".v “ ‘ .‘I:~ . 5‘ IVH.“V‘ 1h: n I h tk “I i-fi; . N“. vv“e:e a ". p J: 6“, u" 'r ~ {M C 68 Kieren and Tallman, in assessing marital competence suggested that "consensus in regard to role is critical for effective problem solving" [1972: 248]. To achieve this consensus requires resolving interpersonal problems. In turn this pivots on acknowledging problems in the first place. Traditionally, however, acknowledging problems is scored by researchers as proof positive of poor adjustment [Kieren and Tallman, 1972]. This of course is due to the underlying value, not questioned by early researchers, that conflict and discensus are automatically bad, inimicable to adjustment. Fortunately this attitude is changing as researchers realize that conflict is normal and even necessary in a relationship. ‘ Landis suggests that adjusting early on in marriage is important and can capitalize on a couple's eagerness, and lack of repertoire of engrained disfunctional patterns. Landis also feels that marriagewhile both partners are in college is a good way to begin the relationship [Landis, 1955]. Marchand and Langford basically agree with him in their study of married students [1952]. A considerable portion of research has focused on the communication pattern in marriage. Koller, for example, documented in 1951 that wives discuss more topics of impor- tance before entering marriage than did their grand- mothers [19Sl]. Presumably this signals a better start in using communication in marriage. Bienvenu found that good communication, as might be expected from his middle la. III In t; 1 :9: and ya " “If a. o :.i.u..lj( l:"t~‘ Anou‘l A 'I u u .. ' ~€zq|§3-‘i’ ‘ ' ‘1‘3‘Ofly‘ ““v-...J‘ v ‘ 69 class subjects, was described as consisting of: handling anger and differences: good voice tone and good under- standing; listening; and self disclosure [Bienvenue, 1970]. Many researchers have used either Locke and Wallace's Marital Adjustment Scale or Locke's Marital Relationship Inventory in conjunction with the Primary Communications Inventory. These are all self report inventories. As might be expected results on the two tests correlate significantly to suggest that satisfied people communicate ‘better. Kahn [1970] in addition found that non-verbal communications in particular, are correlated with marital satisfaction. Navran [1967] found that the stronger relationship was between marital satisfaction and verbal communication rather than non-verbal communication. Locke, Sabagh, and Thomas [1956] also tested for the relationship of communication and satisfaction with empathy. Their results were inconsistent. The other main approach is to identify areas in the marriage, measure consensus over these areas and then measure adjustment. Unfortunately lack of agreement among researchers about what constitutes the areas, and how adjustment should be measured, makes studies narrow in their generalizability. Bowerman suggests nine areas over which to measure adjustment: family expenditure; recreation; relationships with in-laws: relationships with friends; religious beliefs and practices; sexual relationship; homemaking tzzes an I.|I.l‘ H" II“IE.O‘E 3:95 11'. 1' :s m u an ..v‘e area 1:15 bej ‘I ." "‘ I 1 Jan-..": [ -. "‘“r‘yo a: ..‘HU‘ . ,. -. J-l‘ ' >1! Ia‘.‘y ' 4 'Mi 6'3““ 3C a .' :-‘ 4".“ Q 70 duties and responsibilities; philosophy of life; and bringing up children (Bowerman, 1957]. Keeley [1955] also listed nine areas which included companionship; Burr [1970] _lists six items: Murstein and Beck [1972] are satisfied with five areas. Bowerman, for example, found multiple correla- tions between his nine areas and marital adjustment [Bowerman, 1957]. The conclusion is that consensus itself is important irrespective of how the researcher wants the quality to be focused. Some items are less important than others. Locke found no significant difference between happily married and divorced couples on such'factors as religious differ- ences, and the presence or absence cf children [Locke, 1951]. Wallin [1957] found that the absence or presence of religious activity made no difference:h1the level of satis- faction--provided the marriage was sexually gratifying. Again the findings suggest that consensus itself is the vital ingredient. Apparently time is an irrelevant factor. Burr [1970] found that satisfaction increased with time; Luckey [1966] found the Opposite; and Kirkpatrick and Hobart [1954] found no rate relationship between time in a status and ability to take roles or empathize. Evidentally a couple either can or cannot reach pervasive consensus. Researchers have a lot to tell us about the difference between well adjusted and badly adjusted marriages. Spiegel points out that the early months of marriage are marked by an e . Us. 1 n , ”IOC‘U . "'05 ~ 6 will .. . I;h:9v: A rs ku'iv‘. u ..“ " .333: II. t‘ . . ‘P.‘D A - ‘u-...., g“ " 9.. V‘ , 'h.‘.e \ "*7 4... NJ.“ ‘.= y - ,- l b J. I 3% nug‘. ar; 0. a ‘h. S“: r‘ i“ 'h ‘A u‘é‘: \ H I ‘:"‘ F 4: 1 8‘: '\3 K. ‘. Q— g . ‘1: \‘S? ‘ . :I I‘- ‘1‘1‘ U‘ _ 71 conflict since one or both partners are involved in a role system without sufficient knowledge of the requisite role behaviors [Spiegel, 1957]. Dominion takes us a step further and suggests that if couples are unable to establish the necessary minimum physical and emotional relationship, at least by the fifth year of marriage, then they are in the first stage of marital breakdown [Dominian, 1968]. Renne adds that persons who are discriminated against by the culture suffer financially, physically and emotionally from discrimination, and hence their marriages fragment ‘more easily [Renne, 1970]. Once people get into marriage the areas of disagree- ment are largely over decision making, about friends, money, time spent together, goals in life, in laws and companionate interaction. As Landis indicated, husbands and wives basi- cally list the same areas of contention, in the same order, commenting that some couples live with areas unresolved even after 20 years of marriage [Landis, 1958]. It is customary in some circles to argue over whether or not wives who work outside the home lower marital adjustment. Cover [1968] finds that the working class wife is better adjusted in her marriage if she doesn't work. Blood and Hey [1963] find the Opposite for working class wives, unless they have a higher status job than their husbands. Axelson [1963] found that husbands or working wives were in favor of their wives working, more so than husbands who had ' w-p ! tied. up ‘ A . . . :u v. ,- a I:- “. ya 'i“;!‘ h '- AIu-«g‘ b. ”a,“ ,“ u “A“. ‘o u. l ' v ..a ”I \ I\. ‘ .3. 5’ ‘ I». . h A 5V a ulv III (In ll! 72 non-working wives. However, the marital adjustment of, couples in which the wife worked, was lower. Gianopulus and Mitchell [1957] concluded that it is the individuals rather than the situation which determine whether or not there will be conflict over the issue. This all suggests that consensus is the basic ingredient. Either a couple has it, or dissatisfaction spreads to most areas of marriage rather like a progressive disease. Couples in the throes of unadjustment are really not experiencing different events, but, they perceive those (events differently than would adjusted couples. ". . . people with happy marriages argue about the same things as people with unhappy marriages. The difference lies in how the couples handle their disagreements" [Ferguson, 1974: 14]. Clements, to his surprise, found that spouses in stable marriages were no better than those in unstable marriages in having awareness of how their be— havior affected their spouses [C1ements, 1967]. Frumkin, however, found that] unadjusted spouses exaggerate the mutua- lity of all interests irrespective of their significance to marital adjustment [Frumkin, 1953]. Kotlar found that spouses in unadjusted marriages see their spouses as being less affectionate and less dominant. Adjusted spouses saw themselves and each other as being affectionate and equal in dominance [Kotlar, 1965]. Whitehurst [1968] found that pe0p1e who are more peer oriented than family oriented have lower marital adjustment. "I may. . 3‘“ A.‘ '“Vfi:v , u v 0“ 73 Preston, g£_gl. [1968] tested 116 married and 55, engaged couples. Their conclusions supports the theme of other studies. People on opposite sides of a conflict take note of differences in their partner, where persons with much affection see their partners as similar to themselves. Luckey also found that marital satisfaction is related to a closeness between A's self concept, and the spouse's conception of A; and to a closeness between A's ideal self concept and conception of his spouse _[Luckey, 1960]. Marital adjustment is also affected by the persona- lity of the couple members. 'Udry suggested that the search for personality "matches" is futile [Udry,l967]. On the other hand, studies by Gill [1955]; Jacobson [1952]; and Pickford, 35 al. [1966a and 1966b] suggests that satisfied married couples have similar traits or attitudes. Katz 2E.2£- [1963] suggests ". . . the degree to which persona- lity needs are satisfied in marriage is reflected in ones evaluation of, and ability to interact effectively with, a spouse” [p. 213]. In two studies by Murstein and Glaudin, they used both the interpersonal check list and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. With the former test they found little connection between personality and marital adjustment. Using the later test, they found that marital adjustment for women was correlated with the absence of high "lie” or "masculine" scores. For men, a Sign“ £25231 j: 355 and Mr. 1. ’ .iv .v.s a 55:92:31] 231:]? ..-. 1‘ l u, I Ry 74 lack of psychiatric character disorder and lack of insensi- tivity-rigidity pattern was positive. In short, marital adjustment is evident when couples have similar personali- ties and an absense of psychiatric disorder. The final consideration concerns inter-generational factors affecting marital adjustment. The birth of children, especially the birth of the first child, is a crisis parti— cularly if the pregnancy is unplanned [Dyer, 1963; LeMasters, 1957; Ryder, 1973]. Imig studied 181 college-age couples, and found that the birth of a child activates the husband- 'wife marital role expectations. Previously satisfied equalitarian couples experience serious strain when children enter the family. Children can be detrimental to marital satisfaction [Imig, 1971]. However, Udry, after reviewing a welter of studies, concludes that there is no reliable relationship between the presence or absence of children, and marital adjustment [Udry, 1966]. Lang in his 1932 study of 22,000 couples concluded that the timing of children is important: in the first two years of marriage couples without children are happier [Lang, 1932]. Christensen and Philbrick [1972] suggest that the key factor is the value that parents place on having children. The situation is a little different with parents and in-laws. They exist before the marriage and relationships with them do not disapper. Griffith notes that marriage requires a reorientation of responsibilities. The first ii 3 Em w :2 9:13.14 alize {‘ 21'- bad f I {aim 21:521' 1 Extial. l the 51 fiprgb \ I"!q ~«3ESS i'ea of 71! 75 duty of a child is now no longer to his parents, but to its partner which both parents and the couple need to realize [Griffith, 1957]. This task is seldom completed and bad feelings continue on for many years. The success or failure in completing the necessary reorientation, is closely linked to the emotional health of the couple and especially of their children [Westley and Epstein, 1969]. In the studies by Landis and Landis [1958], women said in- law problems were the second worst,men said third worst. Burgess and wallin found in-law problems to be the greatest ’area of difficulty for both men and women [1953]. This is not to suggest that contact with parents and in-laws is destructive. Irving completed two studies of working class families in Toronto, and found that contact with parents occurred more often than some observers realized and these contacts are "the most emotion-laden segment of the kinship system,"'and help sustain couples in their new marriages [Irving, 1972: 6]. Relating with parents, and the event of children, are the two areas which need approaching with skill, a skill a couple brings to all relationships, or have not yet developed. In reviewing this whole area of adjustment, the writer is aware of a particular ability--consensus ability-ewhich begins to form and grow during courtship. If, as Murstein says, a couple makes good courtship progress, this consensus ability builds to where a couple are in tune with themselves and each other, Values and goals are shared, 1“ 1 EC 1J2" » 9| a .Lu]| I“. k‘;n a u ,1... ‘s i -\ o\U .5. “b Ire 3. "A; ,4: in. A 'n‘ ~~és 2 2 q 0' VVHH- ..nn .3 wit. '§ Mur‘ Fl . 1536 I" 76 and the patterns of behavior are functionable. By the time the wedding occurs, a foundation for marriage is mapped out. The couple has established a system in which they view themselves as being "two of a kind,“ and are able to live this way. Diversity augments rather than threatens their bond. If a couple marry before the engagement status work is complete, they may not have reached the Optimum, or necessary level of consensus ability to function effectively as a couple in marriage. If this is the case either they separate, or settle into a stable unsatisfactory pattern. This may eventually get worse as the system functions in- creasingly painfully, and leadxto divorce and/or serious mental and physical impairment of one or both partners. The gradual break-down is sign-posted by couples pointing to more and more areas of dissatisfaction, by them stressing their differences rather than their similar or coordinating qualities, and through increasing emotional disturbance in the children. Breakdown is hastened by the overall cultural flow which stresses excitement, the badness of aging, and gratuitous consumption. The depersonalization of sex-- sex seen as an encounter of the moment rather than as a relationship through time—-abets marital decay. The role of the man in family and society depicts maleness as a conglomorate of strong-silent-hunting-shooting-fishing- loves-football-holds-his-booze-never-shows-feelings- because-that-would-be-effeminate. As many studies show, --:‘ :0: tinL‘E “.55.: ' I:al.odats W use... cw" \ n ‘2 1......“ .233 :0 ‘ ,. ‘R- .4": ‘Ja 'VA' to vu.‘~." ‘ t v:. . '|‘~ t “a ..Z .. ‘l I ~,'- g ' s. . I i“ H: . . .. :I- ‘ N: :k '2 I t‘ ‘ v“5 A,‘ o . ‘ \ A \‘f - 77 the inflexible, non—feeling person is not only a good candidate for the psychiatrist's couch but also a poor risk in marriage. Somehow, a couple who avoid these cultural traps, who are able to build consensus ability, must continue to build their relationship to avoid stagnation or decay. Maintaining the relationship requires a quality of adaptive- ness to events while maintaining a deeper shared conviction about values and goals. A couple who are in empathy with each other, who continue to share goals and role expecta- 'tions while external factors are in flux, have thus the capacity to maintain a workable consensus transferring and transposing a basic reportoirexof skills from situation to event to situation. It is this kind of couple who will score in the satisfied-adjusted range of the test battery used in this study. Others, oblivious to, or basically repressing signs of system breakdown, will score in the satisfied-unadjusted category. Those couples, aware of escalating breakdown but doing nothing to remedy it, will fall into the dissatisfied-unadjusted category. Then there will also be a few who are dissatisfied-adjusted, who have a basic consensus but are reaching for the stars and are aware of not reaching them. The operational descrip- tions of these categories are outlined in Chapter XI: MethodolOgy II: Procedure. 912-41255 J'—.— I... 78 Hypotheses Hypothesis 25. There will be a positive relationship between Engagement Success Inventory scores, and satisfaction scores as measured by the Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Test in the follow-up study. Hypothesis 26. There will be a positive relationship between adjustment scores gained on the pre-marriage testing, and adjustment scores gained on the follow-up. .-O ' l ’4.._ . ‘Vui‘y 5. 7 .I:" C u..‘. 2 . - : ”1‘” A. c.4'.‘ ‘ F. ‘ ~- - ‘ x a“... o C“. %FI§A‘ it. w” 1‘a~ ‘ "'«3‘ CHAPTER IV' MARITAL ROLES In reviewing this particular area, the writer soon found that everyone knows what a role is, everyone has their own ideas about the roles which are really significant. Blood defines roles as follows: "A role is a collection of 'rights and duties expected of an incumbent of a particular position in a system of relationships" [Blood, 1969: 43]. Merton adds to Blood's statement the~fact that ". . . each social status involved not a single associated role, but an aray of roles" [Merton, 1957: 110]. This aray often gets out of hand as Good [1960] and Rose [1951] have separately noted; it is impossible for a person to meet all their role obligations. This quite naturally leads to the necessity of ordering priorities, and conflict over what cannot be humanly accomplished. Roles are learned from birth onward, and the sanctions and rights which accompany each role are deeply imbedded in the unconscious mind. Parsons and Bales suggest that the role divisions which lead a wife to take over the internal-expressive role in the family, and the husband to handle the external- instumental role are so deeply rooted in the structure of society as to be unchangeable [Parsons and Bales, 1955]. 79 -::’~ '1 «.1. II' F ‘ ~qu84 2.215 2:: 's it {‘1‘ . I" On... N sunk: SEX 13v E 80 Dahlstrom cites Brun-Gulbrandsen to the effect that the basic ”traditional“ pattern of sex roles are taught children very early in life despite professed divergent beliefs of the parents [Dalhlstrom, 1967]. Opler concludes that "studies of hermaphrodites have indicated that social and familial expectations can shape the outcome toward male or female gender roles, regardless of the actual genetic sex involved" [Opler, 1967: 257-258]. Evidentally changes in role behaviors and expecta- tions are difficult to accomplish. When two persons marry, each with their own set of ”truths” about sex roles, con-' flict is ensured. Blood suggests that: Role conflicts are difficult to COpe with because they are laden with value judgments. Role expectations are 'evaluative standards,’ so any husband who violates his wife's ex- pectations is shirking his duty or infringing on her rights. . . the unconscious acquisi- tion of role expectations in childhood lends them an appearance of absolutism [Blood, 1969: 205]. For a couple to handle this significant area of marriage there needs to be a clear idea of what the roles are and what is expected in the roles: the ability to perform expected behaviors: and some flexibility in ex- pectations and performance. This all adds up to a functionable consensus. The literature is not too helpful in delineating roles in marriage. Hurvitz divides roles into three main areas: functionsl, control, and symbolic, and subdivides the first two categories [Hurvitz, 1958]. Dunn ’[1960] “.~uha mmnma ovuma mocha hH.~ mm.~ em.m cOHumw>oo ousosmuw ho.- Hm.- mo.- coo: mm.mm mm.mm em.ma Hod ooa owe Adaoa mm. om.~ mm.a H m m on Ho>o o~.H No. N. H On I aw pa.~ Ho.n ma.m . m o o on u as mm.o mm.ma v~.o h mN ca mu 0 mm an.m~ mv.c~ ~o.o~ an en mm on I m~ vo.mm Hm.o~ oo.ov on we on «N 1 AN hh.- um.m~ mh.m~ mu aw mm oN I ma mm.a Ho.m ma.n N m n ma I ha ha Moos: HHH an H HHH HH. H msowc osono moose ozone ozone msouu anomoumu mod omsucoouom :Oausawuunwo .mcwunou stuwsw MO dawn us :06 commons Mo 0mm .9 manna 142 hNIhH HmuoH mmnoH modem Ho.~ Hm.~ vo.m coHuMH>oo ousocmum ~m.o~ om.o~ mm.om sows mm.mm mm.mm om.ma HoH mmH ooH Hosea om. v~.H H m on Hobo co. mm. H H on u mm mm. om.H v~.H H m N am u hm om.m Ho.m no. v .o H um I mm Hm.m oO.mH hm.HH m mm mH vN I mm mo.Hm OH.v~ Om.hm mm co co Nu I HN om.mv oH.am NH.mm we mo Ho cm I mH mm.OH mw.MH MH.m HH MN MH mH : hH O~.H «o. N H hH noose HHH HH H HHH HH H @5090 osono osouo osouo moose macho muomoumo 00¢ omnucoouom sOHuanuunHO .mswumou HsHuHsH mo 02H» um sues: caboose mo mod .5 oHnt 143 «mum osmum oamuv carom mm.mH mm.nm mo.m~ coHumH>oo oumocmum mm.Hm Ho.om ov.om sow: om.00H om.mm om.mm HOH omH omH Asses oo.mH 0H.eH ow.HH om he mm em no>o om.H o~.H om.H m v e um I ma ov.m om.o oo.o mH mm Hm we I no om.m OH.N om.m . h h pH me I mm om.aH 0H.HH oo.m mm Hm mm on I Hm oo.m om.m ov.m oH HH on on I mm om.mH oo.~H OH.~H mm Ne mm «N I mH om.mH om.mH ov.vH mm no we mH I mH oo.OH ov.m~ om.o~ cm as mm «H I h o.v o.» om.v m OH «H mmoH no u HHH HH H HHH HH H @5090 osouo osouo moose msouu moonu mHnmmosoucHssom HO mouse: mommucoouom :OHusoHuumHO .mcHummu omsHuHoBIonm mo oEHu um nonuo zoom szocx on: mOHmsoo HoHcmoocmuchsoosv 08H» mo :MoCOH omuoEHumm .w OHQMB 144 mAIHv «NIH m¢I~ momma mm.¢ vm.¢ mm.m cowumw>m© numccmum hm.m mm.m ow.h cum: mm.mm mm.mm mm.mm Hoa mma nma Adaoe Hm.a m vm uw>o Hm.a mm.~ n v em I mm mm. on. vo. a a a AN I ma mm.v om. wv.v m H h mu I ma mm.m om.a w~.H c m m ma I ma mm.ma vo.m m~.ma «A ma «m «H I as mm.ma vm.oa ao.¢a ma ma mm o I h no.~m mm.am mm.vm mm mm vm w I v mp.m~ sm.mq mv.m~ - «p oq ¢ can» mama HHH HH H HHH HH H macaw macaw macho msouu macho macaw unmfimmmmcm mouuccouwm cowusnfluumwo mo manna: .mcaumwu «mowuumEIwum mo 08w» um :05 an wmuhommu mm unmEQmmmam mo camcoq .m magma 145 mHIHv «NIH mqum mmcmm mo.v mp.q mo.m cofluMfl>mo nnmccmum Hm.m mH.m HH.h cam: mm.mm mm.mm h¢.mm HOH moa omH A¢BOE vN.H N vm HQ>O Ho.m om.N I m v vm I «N mm. mN.H H N am I ma mm.H NH.m I N m ma I mm om.m Ho.m mv.~ - v m v ma I ma «m.mH mo.m om.NH ma mH om NH I OH mw.oH ¢¢.HH om.NH ha ma ON a I h mm.mm mw.HN mm.ov cm mm mm m I v mh.m~ Hm.am ¢>.m~ hm mm I am e can” mmma HHH HH H HHH HH H macho macho macho mzouw msouu QDOHU unmawmmmcm mmmucmoumm Godusnauumwo mo mango: .mcwummu mmmwuHmEImum mo wEwu um :mEo3 xn wwuuomou mm ucwEmmmmcm mo numcmq .OH magma 146 waiting for the final end to freedom makes waiting for the wedding seem the more interminable. In the area of educational achievement, the American groups of men had completed one third of a year more than their future wives. Table 11 and 12 show that both men and women in Group I have a little more education than subjects in Groups II and III. In Group III the educational achieve- ment of men and women is essentially the same. The socioeconomic status of respondents was assessed by assigning to them the socioeconomic status index of their fathers, using the classification system of Reiss, 35 El. [1961]. '. Data for this part of the study is less extensive than elsewhere due to difficulties in data collection. The item on father's occupation was accidentally omitted from the demographic data sheet of Group I. Thus the 55 couples reported on are those who responded to the follow-up ques- tionnaire (which also asked for a description of the father's occupation). Group II respondents were asked to list their father's occupation on the pre-marriage questionnaire. However, the data was of too poor a quality to use. Many persons reponded to the question by writing that their father, for example, worked for Oldsmobile, or was a "salesman." These replies were too vague to code. Once again, the data on Table 13 is taken from the follow-up, questionnaires of the 94 couples who responded. 147 omIm omIoa mHIoa omcmm mm.m mo.~ om.~ cOwumw>oc pumonmum av.ma m>.ma mm.¢a coo: mm.mm oo.ooa mm.mm Hoa med oma Agaoa mm.aa -.n ma.m . NH NH ma ma can» once mm.oa ev.aa ma.m~ Ha em ma r Ammumme .uoamnommv ma H5.a~ ma.vm aa.qm -. am mm mm ma I ma Hm.ma ha.~v oo.m~ mg on ov Acmum may ma v>.m~ om.a vn.m on N w AH I ca um.m v oH cusp mama HHH HH H HHH HH H msouu . macho msouu macaw moouu macaw cowumospw no nude» wmmucmoumm GOwusnwuumaa .cwE pmmmmoo an woumamfioo cowumoopm mo whom» .HH manna 148 mHIm HNIHH mHIm mmcmm mm.m Hm.H mm.H GOHumw>wp pumpsmum mm.MH mm.mH Hm.MH new: mm.mm Ho.ooH mm.mm HOH mmH omH Ageoa mm.m He.m oc.m . a a a we can» muos Ra ma .3 8.3 a mm mm W 33mg . .uonnommv wH hm.~m mn.om om.hm mm Hm om mH I mH mh.o~ ov.mv oo.om Hm mm we Apmum may NH on.m~ Hv.~ hm.v on v 5 HA I oH mm. mm. H H oH can» mmmH HHH HH H HHH HH H muons daouo muons dsouo macho macho coaumoscm mmousmouom coHuonHHumHo «0 mumma .cmEOS pmmmmco an pmumHQEoo coHumooUm Ho whom» .NH oHnma 0,- ~ I. I‘..- 149 .HthImoN "meHH .mm MM .mmHmm anm cwxau maoHuMUHHHmmMHU maumam UHanoomoHoom Ahoy cmEmammaw “HNnv 0mm: maoum unmanammmw “Ammv aoxcmn “HNNV amnommu “Hva amaoammnm “Hmmv amm3MH “vav aouoaaumaw muamam>aaa "mpaHoaH mcoHummaooo mum anm “mow aoumaumacaewm madam “Homv ammeHoou «HHmv adammHmm mHMmmHon3 «Away amasmmo "Home ammMGME coHuoaaumcoo pmaamHmm "mwaHoca maoaammaooo mmw 0H2 AMHV amaonoH mcHHaHnEmmmm oHoHnm> aouoE pmHHmeca “HmHV am>aaw xoaau “HNmV aofiham>Hwa “HmNV oaamnome umwaHoaH maoHummaooo mum 30A .maHmccoHummam maIonHOM on» 0» popcomma 0&3 mmHmaoo mmonu :0 cho comma mmaamamg oo.ooH oo.ooa oo.ooH . aoa «a mm gases mn.HN mm.mH mH.wH NN mH oH mum cmH: mthm mm.hv mm.Ne eo.m¢ . av ov vN mum was moIvm Nm.>H Nb.mN mv.mN - mH NN «H mum 30H mmIm mm.0H vm.oH Hm.oH - HH 0H m amEamm mm.H . . mH.N Nm.H N N H a3oaxca IIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIzmzozlIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII oo.oOH oo.oOH oo.ooH HoH vm mm H Hm.OH wH. N o aoeama ma.H Nm.H N H azoaxaa I'll-I"lu'III-I.'Il.l.I--""'Illu"lllvlvlulu"-'-lu'-zg'Il'lv'"""ll'll"-|llu"'".l HHH HH H HHH «HH «H maoaw macaw macaw macaw maoaw macaw maumum ammuaooaonu GOHuammmumHnl. Udaoaooooaoom .uaonumm n.ucopcommma mo maumum oaaoaooooaoom .MH oHnda 150 Group III couples responded with sufficient clarity that the data gathered on the pre-marriage questionnaire was usable. For all three groups, the category of farmer was kept separate. There was no way to distinguish the tennant of a ten acre plot from the gentleman farmer, owner of several thousand acres. . Table 14 shows the distribution of Socioeconomic Status Indexes (SES Indexes) for couples in the three groups. Groups I and III appear to be skewed toward women coming .from higher status backgrounds, whereas in Group II, the trend is the opposite. Because the data is not more complete, it is not reasonable to analyze the meaning of these findings. It is only in the area of religious preference that we find a noticable difference between the United States and New Zealand groups. As mentioned earlier, the New Zealand Catholic Marriage Guidance Council was unwilling to allow their couples to participate in this study.. Con- versely the author's main access to couples in the United States was through the Catholic Pre-Cana programs. 151 .Haaacamxcmn maumam acan: a scam mgoc cm: can umfi meEH gHuHmom d 658963 maampm amamaa .amfixfi mama ficakfiacmafimflw cafiEQHXHQwafiucchwuwgfifl”mzhaosmfivaktacmflfimxwuafiadgkficawmfififimHfiamrfi_nmfifiacdfivxflBnr M_EHG_QQRH.:NE¥VQRH.mdfificimannHfiaamfiammfifladcaumfinngéa&W#_¢ 3+ 8 3.. 2+ 3 3- me... 8 Han mmcmm mm.mm om.mm v0.00H HoH mmH omH gases ... oe.vv mo.mw .. an moH mama ca HN.mN om.NH mH.m mN HN MH amsamm ac czoaxca mm copaoc Ica sacs ac mac ccac3 cH monacw mm.m om.m om.N v o v o¢+ am>o om.m ow. mm. v H H o¢+ cu Hm+ mm.m co.m mm. v m H om+ cu HN+ mm.m oo.m mm.H N mH m 0N+ cu HH+ mm.0H oo.w om.N HH oH v OH+ cu H+ mm.v N.H oc.m m N m o mm.m oo.m mH.m v 0H m HI cu OHI mm.oH ov.m om.N HH m v HHI ca ONI om.m oN.H mm.H oH N m HNI cu QNI om.m oo.m mN.H v m N HNI cu owI Nm.h om.m hm.v m w h HVI ac>o HHH HH H HHH HH H III macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw maHm> xmwaH cmmucmcaom :cHuanHaumHo a.macnumu m.ucopccmmca mo maamum cHEoccomchom mcu cc comma .mchacc mo mmxcccH maumum cHEococcchom .vH mHQMB 152 Reviewing table 15 indicates that the women are a little little more likely than the men to state a religious pre- ference. It is also of interest that the United States data, gathered from participants at a Catholic-sponsored program shows a strong tendency for persons to state a religious "preference.” This is deceptive. Persons attending other Catholic-sponsored programs and inter- viewed informally by the author turned out to be Catholic in name only. Likewise, a number of those describing them- selves as "Protestant" were in fact of no faith. Several "Protestants” did not know the meaning of the word Protestant. '\ The profile of religious preference in the New Zealand sample is closer to probable religious behavior, showing about one fifth to have no religious affiliation. The actual prOportion of unaffiliated, non-practicing church members, and generally agnostic persons is probably even higher than these figures indicate. The Financial Priorities Inventory was administered to Groups II and III as part of the pre-marriage test battery. Some translation was needed before the test could be used with the New Zealand sample. "Gasoline" became "Petrol." "Utilities" posed a problem momentarily. To an Englishman or New Zealander the word sounds like an oblique reference to the bathroom. The category ended up as ”Gas, electricity, coal for heating, light, cooking, etc." 153 mm.mm oo.ocH oo.QOH HOH moH cmH mnaaoa om.mH Hw.m om.N oN o q cacz mN.mN mw.HN om.NN NN mm mm uamamcucam mm.n oN.vN oo.mN v «NH cNH OHchucU IIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIZMZO3IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII mm.ma mm.mm mm.ma HOH mmH ooH mqsaoa NN.- mv.a mN.m -. «N «a e unoz NN.oN Hm.mN Nm.Hm HN Nv Hm usmumcuoam na.w mN.mm Nm.vm N moH MQH UHHoauaU IIIIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIII I IIIIIIII zszIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIII II unaHacu 302 ch2 mo HOHHm m: ccdHowfl 302 GHQ: ma UOHHm mo HHH macaw HH macaw H macaw HHH macaw HH macaw H macaw ummoucocacm aoHuanHaumHa .nu00nnau woodmao Ho mmoccacuoam wacamHHum .mH «Hana 154 Looking across the rows in Table 16, there is a commonality between the men and the women of both groups. It is interesting that in both groups it was the men who placed a higher emphasis on "Supplies and etc. for anticipated baby." New Zealand men placed comparatively the highest premium on "Vacation." The only group to place a low priority on "Clothes-hers" was the American women. This appears contradictory, since women usually feel committed to decent self care. Does the rating on this item reflect a. .low priority on appearance in the mostly Catholic American sample? Or is the need for good clothing taken for granted? The data provides no obvious answer. The New Zealanders, living in a country which has both adequate public transportation, and an absense of trans- continental distances, placed a low emphasis on "Car pay- ments.” “Church contributions" are also low on the list when compared with the American sample. In summary, it appears that generally, financial priorities are not sex determined, but culturally oriented. The FPI Indexes were calculated as explained pre- viously. As is evident from reviewing Table 17, the FPI 'Index scores of the two groups are similarly distributed, with Group III couples scoring, on the average, slightly higher. 155 mass 3 2958.3an N and you 823a an .9."ch 6.56 R . 3. gram a 89.8 a8 a «$36 383m an .98 H8389?“ 8 . .2 was??? ”308 am madam 36: Na mfiuafiam mm 8893 gm He .3 3188 ”.968 ma 33 now 839% «a aficfiao as mm mfiaommv ma .2 mfiaommo an 9033s, a .98 $53 an 89.3 a8 8 .3 gamma 28m mm 3?ng “.908 no. .flficoo 636 8 mama I 8:88 am .3 mums I 8508 mm £9182 umfioa R 33 bummed 3 8323 mm .2 $93 and am .mfi Havana: 2 $95: 58 3 38 a8 mango 3 .3 m2 I 9238 on man I 3508 me I in fig .2 mafia. 3588 am .3 833 3 Eu: I 8508 8 «.388 z. .3 spammed an .m 85a. raucous 3 mafiafisa 3 I 6.5 mad we mg .30 N» .m .mfi H38: 3 8.39 Ended 9 8333: 3a .3 a8 8 .N 6.5 83 8 .uaa 83 em 3% 56 8H «933a 8 .m .maa gm HN .3 3% Ne 355m ad in and mm .m Sagan E gum a... .3 a8 Ra _ 3:38 8a .v «0333: 3 833.39 8 .3 Hg a2 .3 Huoaauz an .n and am sum 8 . sum 3H 88 SH .N 88 8 BB 3 88 9.4 have a: .H ago: fix 59. 8: “.88 33 .8: an awed x8498 8: a mad xfim .5803 can 58 3 Wang .mQHuHaoaam 39.3qu «5 so 830ng EH Ho gum .3 0.33. 156 .amumo amoE ccmcnc mamumlfiaz magmas 6ng mo gvoam mo ampac ca .cmumHH mm swam Em .oaHsEoHammza EmHmmu 52 may so 3:29 an 8 e2 823 .mfiHOmmc. mm méfi 6% a8 .ficmHgHsE amHHmfi fiHuHam Em .mfiaa mo mass: HHsu How savanna 8mm . .aofism «5 mo Baas 3 H326 ms. 8989a 38333 353a? Emma 62 nomad 8 ma $2... mm @336 mm: ummfi Nana—Eco "scam 8020 8 ”EH 888a m an: HHH 989:. Bauman: o Ham: H .mm .... . . ”.85 NuaEsEB a page fiHfiaebo N .9. madame o 8% Hmaoammmaoaa N Ham: H 8328 a .3 Ham: H Ham: N 80388 N flBaasr m .8 @5520 be N 3:83: m mSe Habammwmoua m 8% HSoammmmoE a .2 8% gauged m 90338 m magma . 968 a mamaaa .mxoom N . Hm flaw a and a mama I manHo N macaw HH .8 maxim 6.8m a 9288 as a mafia a mamaIccg “.368 mH .8 .qu baHafimHa m .mfi 32388 a . I mafia: umxoom oH 88amHHmumHz mH .aN 35.5mm 58 HH 895 a man I 3308 S gamma 853a? mH .R 80388 HH mug a8 2 madam 8828a HH mHaINmSa c.1968 GH .8 88538.42 «H madam .38m 3 838s, 3 833$ HN .mN .nHHEoo sound 3 whammy 333% S 3;..."ch ”.968 «H gamma 3:8 a ..a whammy wBMaHmaa «H 88cmHHw8Hz HH .3 FHHHammHo 8 805 mm .mm 8mg 835 mH :05 a8 HH ugflamufi mm mfiammHo N8 NN .8 838g 3 .flaucoo 585 NH 809333: GN ....EH Nuadhmmao ma .HN 33 a8 838% NH 8288 3 madam 38: ma gags H». .8 .EH 58 NH $820 wBEHa 3 88a E was I 8:38 an .mH 5.53 cm: 5803 so: aocao {@998 Ho: HHH macaw @398 mac HH 980 scum GLEBV .GH 3nt 157 «NINH «NImH magma «o.mH «m.NH coHUMH>mc camccmum m«.m« NN.H« com: mm.mm om.mm HOH me Adsoe am.H cm. H H mama muoneouaa mm.H ow. N H mN I 0N mm.m o«.m N «H mm I om mN.mN oo.mH «N on am I om mo.«m o«.mN I mm «« m« I o« mN.0N om.mN HN m« an I on Nm.N ON.mH m NN mN I 0N mm.H om.m N w mH I oH .. .. .. .. a I H HHH macaw HH macaw HHH macaw HH macaw cmcma caocm mommaaccaom acquanaaumHa .omcHaaaE caomoa acumen anmaoo Ho mmaccm xcwcH macnccbaH mcHuHacHam Hcaoaccam .NH oHnma 158 In examining the tables (18 - 22) related to this in- ventory, at the pre-marriage level, we will observe Hurvitz's [1961] suggestion that a rank order difference of three or more on an item has significance, and indicates strain in that particular area. Groups I and II are so similar as to be, for all practical purposes, identical. In comparing this United States data with the Group III data from New Zealand few differences are observed. The New Zealand men plan on being less involved inreligicn, than their US counterparts. The New Zealand men are also more willing to assist their wives with household chores. In this latter respect the New Zealand men are less traditionalistic in the way they differentiate between men and women's work. American and New Zealand wive's expectations for themselves are essentially the same. This suggests that ethnicity and religious affiliation are not differentiating factors in this area. In the area of 'Husband's Expectations of His Wife's Roles' the only difference between the American and New Zealand men is that the latter expect their wives to share their relative disinterest in religion. The above-mentioned themes continue in Table 21. Again, the New Zealanders, this time the women, de-emphasize the need for their men to be religious, and accentuate their expectation that the men will help them with household chores. 159 In short the differing religious emphasis could easily be a reflection of Catholics stressing religious activity more than Protestants. On the other hand, that New Zealand men plan, and New Zealand women expect men to be more involved in domestic chores is probably a cultural difference. Turning to Table 22, the reader will notice that the MRI Index scores are distributed in a comparable fashion in each of the groups. 160 .Nwaum was» cH coma mm maoucm>aH mmHom HMUHamz can cu mcHumHoa mcHnmu acmavmmnam was many so mmHoa Ho uamamuoum oumHmEoc aom xHoammma mmm«« .Noaum may no ccHaocm mHnu ca ooamonaoc cac3 maouco>cH mwnu aao ocHHHm mHuccaaco mamauamm anon £UHS3 CH mmHmaoc >Ha04 HOH me ««H «macaw ca acnaaz N m N mmoacaHu woman: a HH 0H o>Hucaacmcamca NuHaaEEow 0H 0H . HH acxma conHcmo m N m amaoHan aom ace Ho Hmoo: m m I m cmaa cu amauamm Hoaxcm HH m w QOHmHHma thBmm ccHuomam m m m moacnc am: saws 0HH3.chm N N N mcH>HH can :amm « « « 30am aoawHan mHmm H H H mmaz cu cowammaow m m m mach chnomac: mam QGUHQON 3&2 awn! mD UOHHm mD HHH macaw HH macaw H macaw acmcha Ho mucoacamum .coa commune an ocuachHucc mm ..mchm n.6amnmax. ac mmHoa mo mcaxaca Howe: .mH OHAMB 161 .mvaum man» ca 00m: mm hacucm>cH mmacm Hmuaamz can cu unaumama moanma uccswchSm can maau cc mcha mc ucchumum mamamecc acm chcmmmm mmmua . .mcsam can no coauccm was» cw cmamcamccc cacz Nacucm>ca was» ucc cmaaam advocaacc maccuamm anon scan: Ca mwamscc cho« HOH HA m v 0H I‘MHMNQ w mmH AH m m CH WFVHMNG va AH m m 0H IOV'HMNQ N amscam cw amnEbz acxME :camacoo mcccmcwu woman: 30am cmacaasc mama m>aamuccmcamca hua::EEcw acacaanc acu c0503 mo Hmccz caucus: ca accuamm Hmcxom acxmececm ccmnmcg ca :cwcmmacw coacaazc acm mmamw :cwmaama mawaam mcwacmam hoses :ano mmaom ocuaucn 302 HHH macaw can: ma HH macaw uoaam m: a usage ««mcaca Mc muccamumum .cmac3 ccmnmco an vcummacaucm mm ..mcacm n.0mwz. cc mmaca mo mcaxcma chcz .ma manna 162 HOH mOH va macaw :H amnEaz HH HH HH amxme achHcco O O OH mmcawaam mcmmcmz m m m 30am amaUHHSO chm OH OH O m>Humuammmamma maaaaasco N N x m acacHan acw amecs mo Hmccz N m N camnman cu amaaamm Hmaxmm v N a m acxmemacm H H H camnmaa ca acaammecw N v v amacHan acm mcamo a O N achHHca NHHEMM ocHucmam O O O hmaca :amc mecm ”Hafiz Ea flaw g 35:: . .ccfi ccmmmac Mn cwxama mm ..mchm m.muH3 mHm no macaumucmmxm m.camnmam. ac mcha mo mawxama Have: .ON mHnt 163 «3.4 w 01I~ 0““ m mmH HH «1 QHVN va HH m QHQN macaw aH acnaaz mmcamawm wwmamz m>Humuacmwamoa NuHaaEEcU amxmfi achHcca acacHan acm cog uc Hmacz acauamm Hmaxcm acHwHHma NHHEMM «caucwam mcacnc ac: :qu cmaz chm waH>HH may aamm 30aw acacHan chm «NH: cu acHaumEcU anon uaonmmaon mam canHch 3cz HHH macaw can: ma Ha macaw acHHm ma H macaw mcha mo pamaouaum ..mchm m.c:anuam acm mc macHunuccmxm n.0uH3 .acacb cmwawac an ccxama mm . ac moHca «c waaxama Hmccz .HN mHQMB 164 ONNIOO OONION OvNIOO mwamm O¢.Om OO.mm ON.vm acHuma>ma aamaamum m~.mma aa.m¢a m~.vma ammz. O0.00 m0.00H Om.OO HOH OOH OOH AOHQB .. NN.H .. .. N .. ONN I OON .. NN.H Om.H .. N N OON I OON OO. OO.H Om.H V H m N OmN I ONN OO.H OO.m ON.N N m w OHN I OON O0.0 O0.0H Ov.m O OH O OOH I OOH O0.0H N0.0H vO.HH HH NH OH ONH I OOH ON.HN OO.HN OO.NH NN Om ON OOH I OOH ON.ON OH.ON H0.0N vN On On OOH I ONH mN.¢N OH.ON OO.NN ON mm Nm OHH I OOH v0.0 OH.O O0.0 O OH OH OO I OO OO.m HO. ON.N v H e ON I OO HHH Ha H HHH HH H macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw cwawa maccm mcwmuaccacm acHuanHaumHo .mwuHaamE macmmn amamcu mchaco ac «cacao xcaaa Nacuac>aa mchm Hwaaamz .NN cHnma 165 The rank ordering of colors by group and by sex are outlined in Table 23. For a full discussion of the significance of the particular combinations of colors, the reader is referred to Luscher [1970]. From the writer's clinical experience, when green appears toward the end of the rank, the individual is working hard to avoid conflict or confrontations. Brown near the beginning of the rank suggests a need for care and attention. Both groups of men appear to be trying to avoid conflict, and those in Group 1111 have a strong need for personal care. It is the added stress in the men which causes Group III to be significantly different from the Luscher Ideal norm. Nonetheless, the Index scores of the two groups are comparably distributed, suggesting that, despite the greater stress experienced by Group III men, couples are balanced in their intra-couple level of stress. The same distribution of scores is noted on the Color Prediction Index (See Table 25). Thus, it appears that even though the sex factor affects the outcome of color ranking, the couples' scores are evenly distributed, and couple members in general have an equal, or compatible level of stress.~ II 3mm 68: amacflm can. mmmawt SH 68: 389. 166 mo. OE. 58: HHH macaw I cmsfia 98o 8. SO. 5:03 Ha macaw I cg aa 955 mo. NON. 58: HHH macaw I Hosanna ......c OS. 5.2 HHH macaw I 6:83 NO. MOO. 5on HH macaw I amfimaq .8. m8. . 52 Ha macaw I amfimaa Tmé 3. EN. HHH macaw I 85mg mo. OHO. . HH macaw I aoncmaq mucmudéma 833880 dam «Iflfiaoflaweo Saunamaaoo aqua whgnmwmm mmaw #83 2305 83 50am uoHcH> 30.2mm cma 8.53 I HHH macaw woman OBO 58m 33 30:0» :55 awaofi. Ba 55 I HHH 986 x83 awe Egan ago? :83 83 Ba 303% g I aa 965 Omaw VBMHQ 555 cmoaw uoHcH> 553m» coa 33 am: I HH macaw ago ocean scan 83 58am ”.03.; 83% Ba .8380 HHH 996 x83 I afim ann 89.5 33.9 Ba .63...» 83 8328 a 98m Vega 553 aflm ago? 32? 58m 33 B... .983 93:83 lam“ fig geisha Gama summon 3E. Sic-Wow pan commune 8338 530 E . .mwaaaao: madman Bag monaoo Eabz Huoca Facaomaq £33 359.00 um? acHoo aofimfi m5 :0 :amflam omaommwa H394 .ON oHan. 167 OOION OOION cwccm O0.0 O0.0 aoHacH>mc caccacaw ON.Nm ON.OO ammz O0.00 O0.00 HOH OOH AOHOB OO.H ON.H N N vO I OO NO.N ON.H m N OO I OO NO.N ON.v O N am I OO 3.... om; ..q ma 3 I 3 HN.ON ON.ON ON NO we I Ow OO.¢H O0.0H OH HO On I Om ON.HN O0.0H NN Hm an I on O0.0 ON.OH OH NH ON I ON NO.N OO.NH O HN ON I ON aaa cacao aa cacao HHH cacao Ha cacao cwaaa cacom mcwauaccacm aoHaanHaamHo .conaamE cacmcn ccumcu mmHmaco uc mcaccm xccaH umca acHoo acncmaq .vN cHnMB 168 «OIOH «OINH cmcmm N0.0H O¢.OH acHaMH>mc camcamam HN.Ov O0.0m sac: O0.00 O0.00 HOH OOH 94909 O0.0 OO.m O O «O I OO OO.H OO.m N O OO I OO OO.HH O0.0 NH OH «O I OO OO.vH O0.0 .OH OH me I ma NN.NN O0.0N ON mv aw I ov H0.0 O¢.vH . O ON ON I On O0.0 ON.OH OH NN vm I On H0.0 ON.OH O NH ON I ON NO.N OO.N O OH ON I ON OO.H ON.H N N OH I OH .. OO. .. H OH I OH HHH macaw HH macaw HHH macaw HH macaw cwama macaw ncwouacoacm ccHuanHaumHo .oonaaME cacucn acumcu mchaco mo mcacon xooaH acHuUHamam achU .ON mHQMB 169 Discussion of Data Gathered in the Follow-up Part of this Study To more fully appreciate the modal responses on the Marital Roles Inventory of couples tested one year after their weddings, Hurvitz's original study data is included on the tables. Hurvitz based his findings on a group of 104 couples, residents of California, tested in 1957-1958. The mean age of the men was approximately 40, of the women approximately 35. The modal couple had been married 8 to 14 years, and had two children. Fifty percent were Jewish, 'thirty-five percent Protestant, ten percent Catholic and five percent of no religion.. Forty percent of the men and thirty percent of the women were.ccllege graduates. The majority of the men were in business or the professions [Hurvitz, 1961]. Such a group could hardly be more differ- ent from the samples surveyed in this study. When Hurvitz's findings are compared with those obtained from the couples with children in this study, the rankings are remarkable mainly for their similarity. The area of 'Husband's Roles' indicates only one difference: Hurvitz's men saw themselves as having a greater say in decision making. There is a difference between Groups I and II in the emphasis on the husband as the manager of money, but the divergence from Hurvitz is not substantial. 170 In reviewing 'Wife's Roles' the groups are similar in all respects, except that Group II women are comparatively less interested in being the decision maker. What husbands expect of their wives is reviewed in Table 28. Hurvitz's men emphasized their wives' role as decision maker, but downrated her role as model of women for the children. The two American Groups in this study basically reversed the emphasis of Hurvitz's men on these two items. The final area, that of the wives' expectations of their husbands, continues the picture of similarity. In only one area, management of finances, do Hurvitz's group place a differing priority than that seen on the modes realized in this study. It is interesting that role performance and preference is so uniform between disparate groups over a fifteen year time period. In all groups the men see themselves as primarily the bread winners, and their wives as custo» dians of the children. The wives see themselves as compa- nions first and custodians second. The women in this study still want their husbands to be companions first, before being bread winners. Hurvitz's women had resigned them— selves to second place at the time they were studied. 'For the two American groups studies in this thesis, the above pattern represents a dramatic upending of the husband's roles from their preferences prior to marriage. 171 At that time they too valued companionship above all else. This reversal is possibly the source of heightened tension in those couples with children. The couples without children in Groups I, II and III all place companionship, the husband-wife relationship, first in priority. These wives, unlike those with children, place the sexual relationship second for themselves and their husbands. With children on the scene, sex, as it were, gets side-tracked. A perusal of Table 30 shows the New Zealand couples 'tc be the most comfortably adjusted of those couples without children. A plausible explanation of this is the religious composition of the particular groups: The mostly Catholic couples in the American groups may feel they "should be" having children, and "should not be" using birth control methods such as contraceptive pills. They may also be openly bombarded by relatives and clergy to start their families right away. The New Zealand couples are not as likely to suffer this type of harrassment, hence ex- perience less marital strain, and higher levels of adjustment. 172 wasp comma .HmI ON acHoo» CH oouacmma on aoa HHH3 Naowcumo .maIchHcH Ho cEHa an acaoHan can HHH macaw aH mHmaco mac NHaOOO HHOOHH NuH>aam Ecamm .mcaum mHsa :H coma mm macaao>aH moHcm HmuHaoz can cu waHucHoa moHomu aacawmmnam can mHnu ac mcha «0 muccacuoum cuonEco acm xHoacmmd comaa .Noaum on» Ho acHucom mHnu aH ccaccHn Iacc cac3 Nacuam>aH mHaa vac onHHm mHuocaacc macauamm anon acHn3 aH monaco mHao« an «N as «ea ma aa «cacao ca accesz O O O O O O mccacaHH owmamz N O O HH OH OH o>Hamuaomcamoa muHcaEEcw O O O O HH HH aoxafi achHooo .. .. .. O O O acaoHan acm ace Ho Homo: N m m e m m mHHB cu aoauaom Hoaxom O N N O N .O acHwHHoa NHHEMH coHuomam a O m ca- m a mcaoac am; spa: caaz cams m m N a a a aca>aa can came .. .. .. m v w 30aw aoaoHHac mHom H H H N N N oHHa cu acHammEcw m w v N O N mncn oHcsomaoa mHm HHH HH H _ HH H macaw macaw macaw *NHQEBO macaw macaw mo . wwcmavHHno «a HOH m0 mufimfiwumum coacHan aacnuH3 monaco :uHs mchacw cu waHoacmmoa ace ooHaacE an ..moHom .cchcaoHumoaw maIsoHHoO can m.ccaomam. so moHca Ho Occhaa Hacoz .ON oHnma 173 HHOOHH NUH>aa= scam“ .Ncaam mHnu sH coma mm >aouao>aH mchm HouHamz on» cu waHuMHca moHnmu uaoawomnam can mHnu ac mcha «0 uacacaoum cuchEco acm xHoammmm coma; .Noaum can no acHuocm mHsu aH ooaochacc mac: Nacuao>aH mHsa uac ccHHHw NHuooaaco maoauamm nucn acHaz aH monaco NHaoa vv N Vin OOH N OH HH tn O cIIH q-rn N O H ,_| .... MNHMV'I‘O‘DQQ I-l .... «macaw :H aanaz amass achHcco mocamaHu cwmamz 30aw acacHan mecm m>Humaacmoamoa muHaaEEcw acacHHao aou aoficz mo Homo: panama: cu acaaacm Hmaxcm acxmsmeom camnmaa ca acHaomEcw acaoHHao acm mcacw .ccHwHHoa NHHEsH chHacoam mecca came mchm HHH macaw HH macaw H macaw acaoHHsc.uac£uH3 mchacU HH macaw H macaw auua>asm acacHan :uH3 nchaco «amcHoa mo muaoacaoum .oaHaaacHumoaw maIonHou on» on waHoacmmoa ach3 ooHaaaE No ..moHom m.oHH3. ac mcha mo waHxasa Homo: .NN cHnoa '174 HMN O v HO!” O v ur-IMN a VI!) OOH Hmr-IMNLDOQQ‘O‘I‘ H H HO‘ HH OWHNMV‘I‘O‘IIDCD H Fir-l HH OhHNthfiflv¢ H macaw aH acneaz awxmfi achHooo mooacaHm owmamz Bcaw acaoHHsc chm c>Humuacmcamoa NuHaaEEow acaOHHso acu aoEOB mo Homo: accnmaz cu aoaaamm Hmaxcm amxmaoacm panama: cu acHaomEco soacHan acm mcamw acHwHHca NHHEMH coHuomam . NccoE came mchm HHH macaw HH macaw H macaw coacHHao uacnuHs monaow NuH>aam HH macaw H macaw acaoHHnu nqu mchacU mcha Ho muncfioumum ..mcHom n.cmH3 mHm acm ooacacwoam n.6amnmam. so mcHoa mo waHxaoa Homo: .caHaaccHumoaw maI3cHHom on» cu wcHoacmmca cmE wcHaaME an .ON oHnma 175 Om ON as «OH OH HH cacao ca accesz O O O s O N mcccmcaa caucus O O O O OH OH o>Huoaacmoamma NuHaaEEcw O O O OH HH HH acxcfi achHooo .. .. .. O O O amaoHHno aou cos Ho Homo: N m N O m v cHHz ca acauamm Hoaxcm N v v N O O acHwHHoa NHHEMH cOHaomam v N N aa a m mmaosc aha: caaz mama m N m H . N N waH>HH can aamm .. .. .. m v m 3caw acaOHan chm H H H N H H cHH3 cu acHaomEcw O O O O N O mncn ochcmacn mHm HHH HH H HH H macaw macaw macaw NUH>aam macaw macaw soaoHan uacnaHs mchacw acaoHan mcha Ho muacecamam :uH3 mchacw ..mchm m.caaomam acm acH coacacucam n.0HH3. ac moHca Ho waHxaoa Hoocz .ochaaoHamoaw manchch on» on wcHoacmmoa ac503 ccHaaaE an .ON cHnma 176 OOHIOO ONHIOO ONHIvv cwamm .u N0.0H NH.HN O0.0H .>co camcamum O0.00 O0.00 H0.00 sac: O0.00 ON.OO v0.00 OO ON vv Hmaoa. OO.N OO.H NN.N O H H cHnmaccm ac: .. .. .. .. .. .. OOH I OOH .. OO.H .. .. H .. ONH I ONH I. O. 00 a 00 0. DO mwfl|omfl .. OO.H .. .. H .. OOH I OOH .. OO.N .. .. N .. OvH I OOH .. OO.v .. .. O .. OOH I OOH .. OO.H OO.¢ .. H N. ONH I ONH .. O0.0H OO.v .. OH N OHH I OHH OH.O O0.0N OO.v N ON N OOH I OOH vN.OH O0.0H ON.NN O OH OH OO I OO ON.OH O0.0H OO.HH O NH O OO I OO V0.0N N0.0 OO.HH O O O ON I ON vN.OH OO.v H0.0N a O OH OO I OO NO.NH OO.H N0.0 O H O OO I OO Ov.ON .. NN.N O .. H Ov I Ow HHH HH H HHH HH H macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw cwama caccm mcwouacoacm acHaanHaumHo .mwanoca aHoau acaum amok mac acumen aoaoHHno uacnuHa nchacc Ho moaccm xccaH >aouaw>aH meom HmuHamz .OO cHnsB 177 .canu mHaa ac.accmmc aca coco HHH macaw can» «manc3cHHcH cac3 Nona cEHu can as OHan c waH>cn ccuacmca acHg3 HHH macaw aH chacc mac NHac mc3 causes vONIvO OOHIOOH owacm NN.OO OO.HN acHucH>oO camcscam Nv.va H0.0NH ace: H0.00H O0.00 OH HH HOHOB ON.O .. H .. OHN I OON ON.OH .. .O .. OOH I OOH ON.OH .. . O .. ONH I OOH OO.HN O¢.Ov O O OOH I OOH O0.0H O0.0 N H OOH I ONH N0.0N Ov.Ov O O OHH I OOH ON.O .. H .. OO I OO HH macaw H macaw HH macaw H macaw cwaca oacom mowcuacoacm «acHuanHaamHo coo acumen acaoHHno aqu mchaco mo moacom xocaH Naouac>aH OcHom HOuHaOz .mwaHooc3 chaa acuwo ace» . HO «Home 178 The use of the Luscher Color Test and Color Prediction Test on the follow-up was limited to the 49 couples in Group II who were visited in their homes by the experimenter. An examination of Table 32 shows clearly that there Ins been a slight shift in color preference during the year sflnce the pre-test. The shift is toward Luscher's Ideal hbrm. The mean Index score and standard deviation are very cflcse to those of the pre-test statistics (see Table 24). The Color Prediction test statistics show a similar imend. The parallels between the pre-test and follow-up suggest a consistency of this quality (predictive ability) over the time interval. \ 8mm 58: acacflm Em macaw: .3 .23. £89. 179 8.x mom. Ha macaw I Saw S no 3.x mom. 583 Ha cacao I am: Ha cc 8.x mom. I g H cacao I HERE 8. HOO. as H cacao I 3695 mo. OHO. AcanO H 98o I acscmaa 8.x NOO. Ha cacao I .3283 @98ch Hame GOHamHmaaB chm tmaccacamacco moflcacaaoc cams Pamficcam xcmHn Nmaw GBOan achH> aooaw maHn BOHHoN cca amEcS HH maDHw VHUOHQ Nmaw aoHcH> S55 . accaw scHHoN coa caHn cm: HH macaw x83 scam €ch 88m 83? c8 333a 83 SN manna. scam "omeaamZImamv 83:8 aa 968 £83 Nmaw a30an uchH> accaw 30HHoN oaHn Hoca monaB HH macaw song 553 Omam $33 soda» :85 83 Ba «H83 m . 358a 583 5:98 5an far. 558 Baa 28cm swam 833nm amccc acme .Eacz HcccH w.§H5H3cwam9§ ammccmmma quwocmsaHmfi aouHm aces mac powwow monacc HH 98w 3 puma. acHoo amncmaq 05 ac aaoaumm omacmuoa Hg .NO 3an 180 NmImH mmcmm_ O0.0 acHHMHme oamwamum O0.00 ammz Om.mm we Hmc caccamam O0.00 ammz O0.00 Ov Hayes OO.¢ N OO I OO ON.OH .O OO I OO NN.OO OH Ov I Ov O0.00 . OH OO I OO ON.NH O ON I ON NH.O O . OH I OH mmwmuamoamm acHuanHaumHo . mwama macom . manage: aacfi amums ammm mac omumma HH macaw aH mmHmacc Ho nmaocn xmcaH acHaoHpmam acHoo .vO mHnms 182 The Locke-Wallace Test was administered to all groups on the follow-up. It is very much a subjective assessment of how the marriage is functioning. It was remarkable just how many persons indicated they were "perfectly happy" on item 1, of the test. Groups II and III are very close in the distribution of scores as outlined on Table 35. Group I evidently contained a number of supremely optimistic couples which pushed up the group mean. For some reason, a number of 'pecple evidently felt that they had to express having reached perfection. This is partly due to a fear of expressing negatives, which some persons view as loveless attacks upon the spouse. No doubt others had the wish to keep their marriage completely private from outsiders. The Satisfaction in Marriage Test was added to the follow-up battery for Groups II and III when it was observed that the follow-up questionnaires received from Group I couples were overly glowing: they were just too good to be true! , The Satisfaction in Marriage Test is actually Edmonds' Marital Conventionalization Test under a disguised name [Edmonds, gt al., 1967]. The distribution of scores depicted on Table 36 show that Group III couples tended to give even more conventional responses than did couples in Group II. The banality of some of the test items elicited grafiti from several subjects. 183 NHOIOOH OHOIOOH OHOINNH mwamm O0.00 O0.0N OH.Ov acHamH>mo oamoamam OO.NON OO.vON O0.00N ammz O0.00H N0.00H O0.00 OO «O OO Hmaoa OO.N OH.O NO.HN O O NH OHO I OOO OO.NH OO.vH H0.0H N «H O OON I OON O0.0N O0.0N O0.0H O «N HH ONN I OON OO.NN O0.00 O0.0N IHH ON OH OON I OON O0.0H OO.vH O0.0H O OH O OON I ONN O0.0H O0.0 ON.N . v O a OHN I OON .. OO.H NO.H .. H H OOH I OOH OO.N OH.N NO.H H N H ONH I OOH .. OO.H .. .. H .. OOH I OOH .. .. NO.H .. .. H OOH I ONH HHH HH H HHH HH H macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw macaw mwcma maoom mmwmuamcamm acHaanHaamHo .maHmascHummaw maIchHcm ms» ca waHoacmmma mmHmaco Ho mmacom xmoaH acme mcaHHa3ImxocH .mm mHnma 184 «OHIOH ONHIOH mwamm O0.00 NO.NO soHumH>mc camoamam O¢.OHH OO.NHH ammz O0.00H N0.00H OO OO H4908 OH.O .. N .. OOH I OOH OO.NH O0.0 N O ONH I OOH H0.0N HH.OO IO ON OOH I OOH NO.NH HN.NH O OH OOH I ONH ON.OH ON.OH O OH OHH I OOH OO.N O0.0H O OH OO I OO OO.N Oe.O O O ON I OO ON.OH O0.0 O O OO I ov OH.O O0.0 N O OO I ON OO.N OO.H H H OH I O HH macaw H macaw HH macaw H macaw mmaocm mmwcuamcamm acHuanHaamHo .maHmaacHuwmaw maIsoHHcH map cu waHoaommma mmHmacc mo mmaoom xmosH umma mwsHaamz aH acHuomOmHamm .OO mHnma 185 Data Gathered But Not Used in This Study Group I Individuals were asked to predict how their finace would fill out both parts of the Marital Roles Inventory. When couples were tested it was found that many people did not know what to do, or if they did, found the test very provoking. Some persons were observed to be c0pying their own responses on the Marital Roles Inventory, and using their scores as a verbatum estimate of their partner's. These factors suggest that the test results would be of guesticnable value, hence, they will not be reported on in this thesis. . Data gathered on the Engagement Success Inventory was also not used for different reasons. The ESI has been used by many researchers, and its value well documented. In light of the need to curtail the cost of this project, the body of data was set aside for future study. Groups I, II, III The responses on the Primary Communication Inventory used in the follow-up section of this project were set aside for later use. Although the PCI appeared to be a useful measure of certain aspects of communication, the data was set aside to help in cost economy. The-testing of several of the hypotheses has also been reserved for later study for the same reason. 186 Group IV Analysis of the underage, clinical group was also set aside for reasons of project expense. It is hoped that the data already gathered will be usable as the basis for a future longitudinal study of couples in this age category. Groups V, VI, VII In examining the findings from Groups I, II, and III, it was decided that they were sufficient for the purposes of this project. Thus the control group data was also set 'aside for use at a later date. The control group data could have provided an interesting comparison with the ex- perimental groups. However, the control groups had not been tested prior to marriage so the comparison could not be complete. Using control group data would have markedly increased the complexity and expense of the research, thus practical considerations also suggested cutthugback the scope of this project. CHAPTER XIII PRESENTATION AND DISCUSSION OF FINDINGS The hypotheses will be presented in numbered order, each followed, where appropriate, by interpretative comments. The reader is directed to the correlation matrixes (Tables 37 - 41) for summaries of the statistical findings. Table '37 covers both the pre-marriage and follow-up data for Group I Tables 38 and 40 refer to Group II; and Tables 38 and ‘0 4leresent the data for Group III, the New Zealand sample. Readers can bear in mind that the research project was designed to measure the marriage adjustment of couples not individuals, so for example, when discussing the rela- tionship of the age factor to pre-marriage adjustment, it will be the couple's averaged age that will be the factor under observation. The scope of this project was again limited by the cost factor. Thus some hypotheses have been set aside for later examination. These are listed in order, and desig- nated as being reserved for later analysis. The results of the New Zealand sample are restricted in the follow-up phase by a time factor. It was only possible to follow-up the first 45 couples originally tested. There was however, a very high rate of return. 187 Table 37. 188 Index scores. Group I Correlation Matrix for independent variables, Pre-marital Index and Follow-up Pre-marital Follow-up Locke- Marital Roles Marital Roles Wallace Inventory Inventory Test Index Index Index Average age -.1327* .056 Age difference -.l442 .042 Length of -.0115 -engagement ns Length of +.0084 acquantanceship ns . Religious —.0344 -.1228 -.3770 difference ns ns .002 Average -.l7l4 education .020 Happiness of +.1542 parent's marriage .033 Happiness of +.ll40 childhodd ns Parental approval -.l405 .047 Socioeconomic ~.0522 -.ll77 +.0905 Status Index ns ns ns Pre-marital MRI -.0074 ns Follow-up MRI -.0074 +.0993 ns ns *In Tables 37 - 41, the correlation value between two items is listed, with the level of significance listed below. Where the level of significance is below .05, ns indicates that the correlation is not significant. Table 38. 189 Group II Correlation Matrix for independent variables and Pre-uarital Index scores. DB Marital Pirancial LIBcher (blor Ibles Priorities Color - Pre— Inventory Inventory Test diction Index Index Index Index Average age -.l485 +.1622 -.0096 +.0605 .028 .019 ns ns Age difference -.0906 -.0321 +.060l +.0889 ns ns ns ns Length of -.l436 -.0104 -.0699 -.1638 engagalent .033 ns us .017 Length of --.0144 -.O934 -.0063 -.1057 acquaintanceship ns ns ns ns mligious +.1137 \ +.l3‘75 +.0590 -.0632 difference ns .039 ns ns Average -.1279 +.l988 -.0832 -.1210 edmation . 051 . 005 ns . 060 Happiness of +.0702 -.2088 -.0118 -.0432 parent's narriage ns .004 ns ns Ihppiness of -.O447 -.1054 +. 1120 +.1138 childhood ns ns ns ns Parental — . 0816 -. 0877 - . 1200 - . 1477 approval ns ns .062 .029 Socioeconomic -.0001 -.1562 . -. Status Index ns .031 Erica]. Roles . -.1166 -.0495 +.085‘7 Inventcry Index ns ns ns Financial Priorities -.1166 . -.1054 -.1008 Inventory Index ns ns ns Test Index ns ns .001 Color Prediction +.0857 —.1008 +. 3683 . . Index ns .001 190 We 39. Group III Correlation Matrix for independent variablesand Pre-marital Index scores . Marital Financial Luscher Color Rules Priorities Color Pre- Inventory Inventory Test diction Index Index Index Index Average age +.0348 -.0096 -.1435 -.0133 ns ns ns ns Age difference -.0244 -.0892 -.1070 +.0648 ns ns ns ns Length of -.0243 -.1659 +.0224 +.0863 engagement ns .050 ns ns Length of -.0001 +.0645 +.1708 -.0627 acquaintanceship ns ns .044 ns Religious +. 1727 +. 0458 +. 1215 -. 0897 difference .042 ns \ ns ns Average -.1915 +.1181 +.0851 +.0239 education .028 ns ns ns Happiness of +.0954 +.0095 -.0937 -.0277 parent's narriage ns ns ns ns Happiness of -.0921 +. 2556 -.1698 +.0473 chilcmood ns .005 .045 ns Parental -.l675 +.0263 +.0205 -.1482 approval .047 ns ns ns Socioeoommlc -.1380 -.0587 . Statm Index ns ns MRI Index . -.2598 +.0565 +.1859 .005 ns .031 FPI Index -.2598 . +.0480 +.1684 .005 ns .048 121‘ Index +.0565 +.0480 +.1417 ns ns ns CP Index +.1859 +.1684 +.l417 . .048 ns .031 Table 40. 191 Group II Correlation Matrix for Follow-up Index scores, and their relationship to selected Premarital Index scares and independent variables. Follow-up Testing Indexes Ltscher Color Marital locke- Satisfaction Color Pre- Ibles Wallace in Marriage Test diction Inventory Test Test Index - Index Index Index Index Pre-marriage Indexes 101' Index +.4453 . . +.2047 -.1072 -.1316 .001 .024 ns ns CP Index . . +.1639 +.2107 -.2288 +.0177 ns .021 .013 ns 'F'PI' Index . . . . -.1616 +.1052 -.1256 ns ns ns MRI Index . . . . .+.2015 . . . .026~ Independent variables meioeconomic . . . . +.0559 -.0757 +.0948 Stains Index ns ns ns mligious . . . +.O468 -.0133 -.2565 difference ns ns .007 Follow-up Indexes F—MRI Index +.1816 +.2060 . . -.l758 -.0064 ns ‘ us .046 ns L-W Index -.3415 -.2330 -.l758 . . +.4598 .008 .054 .046 >.001 SIM Index -.2743 -.0685 -.0064 +.4598 .030 ns ns >.001 121‘ Index . . +.3909 +.1816 -.3415 -.2743 .003 ns .008 .030 GP Index +.3909 . . +.2060 +.2060 -.0685 .003 ns ns ns Table 41. Group III Correlation Matrix for Follow-up Index scores, and their relationship to selected Pre- marital Index scores and independent variables. Follow-up Testing Indexes Marital Locke- Satisfaction Roles Wallace in Marriage Inventory Test Test Index Index Index Pre-marriage Indexes LCT Index +.0254 -.1132 -.1236 ns ns ns CP Index -.0032 +.0217 +.2068 - ns ns ns Financial Priorities -.2398 +.1645 -.0470 Inventory Index ns \ns ns MRI Index +.5112 .001 Independent variables Socioeconomic -.2483 +.2058 -.0022 Status Index ns ns ns ZReligious +.1974 -.1027 -.1667 difference ns ns ns Follow-up Indexes F-MRI Index . . -.4952 -.3810 .001 .012 ZL-W Index -.4952 . +.6845 .001 >.001 SIM Index -.3810 +.6845 . . .012 >.001 193 Findings on the whole group follow-up will be reported in a forth-coming paper. Despite the reduced group size, the findings are still significant, and make a substantial contribution to this thesis. The Hypotheses Hypothesis 1 There will be a positive relationship between age and pre-marital adjustment. This hypothesis was tested by correlating the couple's 'mean age (simple average of the man's and the woman's ages) with their Marital Roles Inventory Index (MRI Index). The findings are contradictory. For the‘United States groups it appears that chronological maturity is an asset in pre- marriage adjustment. The New Zealand group shows the reverse, although the results are below the level of significance. Hypothesis 2 Length of acquaintanceship before marriage will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. The couple's length of acquaintanceship was determined by averaging the time estimate given by the two partners. This figure was in turn correlated with their MRI Index. In all three groups this factor was found to be non-signi- ficant in pre-marriage adjustment. A suggestion that time in a relationship has little bearing on relationship growth. 194 This is in keeping with Kirkpatrick and Hobart's earlier findings [1954]. Hypothesis 3 Length of engagement will be positively associated with pre-marriage adjustment. The averaged length of engagement was correlated with the couple's MRI Index. Only in Group II was the length of engagement found to be a significant element in pre- marriage adjustment, and then only at the .033 level of 'significance. This adds continuing emphasis to the validity of Kirkpatrick and Hobart's research mentioned above. \ Hypothesis 4 There will be an inverse relationship between having a child in the first year of marriage, and adjustment in marriage. This hypothesis was only examined for Group II as ‘this group had a high level of return, and the greatest number of couples with children. The Follow-up MRI Index (F-MRI Index) scores of couples with children were compared vtith the Indexes of childless couples. A one-way ANOVA was ‘calculated and a significant difference found between the groups. The presence of children in early marriage is associated with lower adjustment. This is in line with Bernard's [1973] conclusions from surveying research :findings of other investigators. LeMasters [1957] and .001) to the role behavior in marriage of these childless couples. Satisfaction is closely related to agreement on role behavior, and to the tendency to give conventional responses. Pre-marriage stress and empathy did not show up as being important in terms of adjustment or satisfaction at the time couples were follow- up. The findings on those couples in Group II tested per- 'sonally, were contrary to expectations. Which couple member volunteered to be tested first was not an indicator of any particular degree of adjustment, satisfaction, stress or empathy. \ Similarly, mailed responses were not significantly different from responses gathered by a personal visit to the couple's home. Those who dropped out of the study before the follow-up, had not scored differently in terms of pre-marriage adjustment than those who stayed in the complete study. In comparing the findings of the American groups with those of the New Zealand group, some general comments are in order. Socio-cultural factors do influence Financial Priorities. Midwest American Catholicism is a factor, which by comparison, militates for traditionalism in roles, the division of tasks into 'man's work' and 'woman's work,‘ and lop-sided husband-wife empathy. New Zealand men, more so than American men, exhibit high stress on entering 242 marriage. The women in both national groups show less. stress. However, the pre-marital anxiety of the men in Group II abated somewhat by the time they were followed-up. The major finding of the study is that pre-marriage adjustment and role priorities are indicators of role adjust— ment in early marriage. Although the Financial Priorities Inventory does not correlate significantly with early marriage satisfaction or adjustment, at the pre-marriage stage, it is sensitive to some of the couple's differences in background which disturb the processyouautiyoursnxuee awonicemuin1mxfiecu; in