ABSTRACT HARMONY OF SELF-CONCEPT AS A FACTOR INFLUENCING THE VOCATIONAL DEVELOPMENT OF UPPER-CLASS AND GRADUATE MALE COLLEGE STUDENTS BY Richard A. Morril Implicitly, vocational counseling has Operated on the assumption that choice of a vocation is a cognitive function utilizing, both cognitive and affective data about' one's self and the environment. These cognitive processes are used most efficiently when all parts of the person are functioning with a minimum degree of conflict with each other. It has thus been rather widely accepted that per- sons enduring intense periods of internal conflict are unable to make realistic vocational choices. From the position of a self-theorist, a major factor influencing vocational development is the degree of harmony of an individual's self-concept--his internal consistency of his self evaluation. Super (1963) suggested that harmony of self-concept is considered to be related to adjustment, to lack of conflict, and to personality integration. It may, therefore, also be related to ability to formulate a Richard A. Morril vocational choice, that is, to see oneself in an occu- pational role. The present study was designed to investigate the possible relationship between the degree of harmony of self-concept and vocational interest maturity. The study attempted to demonstrate that the variable of harmony of self-concept affects intensity of vocational interests, diversity of vocational interests, and realism of vocational interests, as reflected in psychometric data. The purpose of this study was threefold. It first sought to determine whether there was a relationship between the degree of consistency of self-concept for a group of male college students and their level of vocational interest maturity. Secondly, it sought to determine whether there was a relationship between the degree of ego integration for a group of male college students and their level of vocational interest maturity. Thirdly, it sought to detect differences in the degree of harmony of self-concept and vocational interest maturity between three groups of males with vary- ing degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Participants who volunteered for the study were sixty-nine male juniors, seniors, and doctoral level gradu- ate students at Michigan State University. Twenty were juniors or seniors who came to the M.S.U. Counseling Center requesting vocational counseling and stating that they were having difficulty making a vocational choice. Twenty-seven were juniors or seniors enrolled in In ,4 rt Richard A. Morril upper-division majors in the university and, at the time the study was conducted, indicated no dissatisfaction with their choice of major. Twenty-two were third and fourth year doctoral level graduate students. Subjects completed the Strong Vocational Interest Blank, the Tennessee Self Concept Scale, and the Edwards Personal Preference Scale. The Strong profiles were pat- tern analyzed according to the objective method outlined by Stephenson (1961). The intensity of interests were determined on the basis of the distribution of primary, secondary, and reject patterns. The DIV scale on the Strong was used to assess diversity of vocational interests. A measure of realism was derived as a discrepancy score be— tween the Occupational Level (OL) scale and the Academic Achievement (AA) scale on the Strong. A measure of ego integration was derived from the Trehub (1959) method of scoring of the Edwards. Finally, three scales on the Tennessee were independently used to measure consistency of self-concept. Super's theory of vocational development served as the basis for the generation of the following twelve hypotheses. l. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to the in- tensity of their vocational interests. 2. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to the diversity of their vocational interests. Richard A. Morril 3. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to the degree of realism of their vocational interests. 4. Differences exist in the degree of consistency of self-concept among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vo- cational choice. 5. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to their de- gree of ego integration. 6. The degree of ego integration for male college students is related to the intensity of their vocational interests. 7. The degree of ego integration for male college students is related to the diversity of their vocational interests. 8. The degree of ego integration for male college students is related to the realism of their vocational interests. 9. Differences exist in the degree of ego inte- gration among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. 10. Differences exist in the diversity of vocational interests among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. 11. Differences exist in the realism of vocational interests among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. 12. Differences exist in the intensity of vocational interests among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. The null form of hypotheses one through three and five through eight were tested by Pearson Product-Moment correlations, while the null form of hypotheses four, nine, te VE Richard A. Morril ten, eleven, and twelve were tested with the analysis of variance statistic. The findings of this study indicated that an in- crease in the degree of ego integration accompanies an increase in diversity of vocational interests. There was also a positive, statistically significant, relationship between one of the measures of consistency of self-concept on the Tennessee and diversity of vocational interests. Significant differences among the three groups were found on diversity of vocational interests (combined junior/senior groups were found to be more diverse than the doctoral students), degree of ego integration (combined junior/senior groups were found to be less integrated than the doctoral student group), and consistency of self- concept as measured by the Tennessee V score (combined junior/senior groups were found to be less consistent than the doctoral students). References Stephenson, R. A. A New Pattern Analysis Technique for the SVIB, Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1961, g, 109-134. Super, Donald E. Career Development: Self-concept Theory, ' New York: College Entrance Examination Board, 1963, p. 30. Trehub, A. Ego Disjunction and Psychopathology: Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, §§, I9 - 194. HARMONY OF SELF-CONCEPT AS A FACTOR INFLUENCING THE VOCATIONAL DEVELOPMENT OF UPPER-CLASS AND GRADUATE MALE COLLEGE STUDENTS BY Richard A. Morril A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology 1970 For my wife, Marianne P0 P0 re Ila CO. ii:- Y0: ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Five years ago a rather scared and anxious recent graduate of the University of Massachusetts left familiar surroundings and relationships to come to Michigan State University for graduate study. He arrived on campus know- ing no one and knowing very little about the program into which he had been accepted. This writer leaves M.S.U. having grown as a person and as a professional. Outward symbols of this change and growth are a Masters and Doctoral degree. These degrees represent the completion of course requirements, exami- nations, and theses. They do not, however, accurately convey the richness of my experience here. For this reason it seems appropriate to mention some of the people who have made my years here so meaningful. To these people I would like to offer my sincere thanks and appreciation-~knowing You has made the difference. To the M.S.U. Counseling Center staff, for pro- viding the dynamic base for my training as a psychologist and growth as a person. To my fellow Interns, for sharing the joy and the pain of growth. To Bill Kell, for helping me to experience impact, and change. iii To Maryellen McSweeney, for being a master teacher, and for helping me to come to appreciate and some- what understand quantitative method. To Sam Plyler, for his support and encouragement early in my program, when I was questioning. To Cecil Williams, for being an excellent teacher and advisor, and now more importantly, a deeply cared for associate and friend. To Sue Jennings, for making supervision the rich and meaningful experience that it has been, by being willing to share her creativity and to trust mine. To Richard Does, for being willing to share the intensity of himself with someone struggling to find his own intensity and depth, and for his anger that penetrated to the core of the problem. To the late Buford Stefflre, for having given a great deal in the short time I was able to know him. Finally, to my clients, for giving as much as they were given. iv Ck TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter Page I. NATURE OF THE PROBLEM . . . . . . . . 1 Statement of Purpose. . . . . . . . 4 Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 Research Hypotheses . . . . . . . . 8 Definition of Terms . . . . . . . . 10 Limitations of the Study . . . . . . 13 Organization of the Study . . . . . . 14 II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE. . . . . . 15 The Self-Concept in Vocational Develop- ment: A Brief Historical Overview . . . l6 Self-Concept Implementation Through Vocational Choice. . . . . . . . . l8 Ego Functioning as Related to Vocational Development. . . . . . . . . . . 27 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 III. METHODOLOGY AND ANALYSIS . . . . . . . 35 Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Instruments. . . . . . . . . 38 Statistical Hypotheses, Preparation of the Data, Analysis Procedures . . . . . . 52 Preparation of the Data. . . . . . . 6l AnalYSis. O O O O O O O O O O O 63 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 IV. RESULTS. 0 o o o o o o o o o o o 64 Tests of Hypotheses . . . . . . . . 64 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Chapter V. S7 1:! REFERENCE APPENDICE Appendix A- Le i: B. T) Chapter Page V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, LIMITATIONS AND IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH . . . . 85 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . 88 Limitations . . . . . . . . . . 92 Implications for Future Research . . . 93 REFERENCES. I O O O O O O O O O O O O 95 APPENDICES Appendix A. Letter Requesting Participation of Counsel- ing Subjects for the Study . . . . . . 100 B. Tables 13 to 15. . . . . . . . . . lOl vi Tab ‘5 lo. Table l. 10. LIST OF TABLES Correlations Among the TSC Scores, the Ego Integration Scores, and the SVIB Scores . Test of Hypothesis 10: Analysis of Variance for Differences Among Groups on the TSC V score I O O O O O O O O O O 0 Significant Scheffe Post Hoc Comparisons for Hypothesis 10: Differences Among Groups on Total V Score on TSC . . . . . . Group Means on Experimental Variables. . . Test of Hypothesis 11: Analysis of Variance for Differences Among Groups on the TSC D score 0 O O O O O I O O O I I Test of Hypothesis 12: Analysis of Variance for Differences Among Groups on the TSC C score 0 O C O I I O O O O O 0 Test of Hypothesis 19: Analysis of Variance for Differences Among Groups on the Ego Integration Score . . . . . . . . Significant Scheffe Post Hoc Comparisons for Hypothesis l9: Differences Among Groups on Ego Integration Scores. . . . . . Test of Hypothesis 20: Analysis of Variance for Differences Among Groups on Intensity of Vocational Interests . . . . . . Test of Hypothesis 21: Analysis of Variance for Differences Among Groups on Diversity of Vocational Interests . . . . . . vii Page 64 7O 70 72 73 74 78 78 80 81 ll 12 13 15 Table 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. Significant Scheffe Post Hoc Comparisons for Hypothesis 21: Differences Among Groups on Diversity Scores . . . . . . Test of Hypothesis 22: Analysis of Variance for Differences Among Groups on Realism of Vocational Interests. . . . . Total V, Total D, and Total C Scores on the Tennessee Self Concept Scale for Sixty- Nine Subjects . . . . . . . . Ego Integration Scores for Sixty-Nine Subjects. . . . . . . . . . Number of Primary, Secondary, Reject, and Unclassified Patterns, DIV Scores, and OL-AA Scores for Sixty—Nine Subjects viii Page 81 83 101 102 103 V: r, CHAPTER I NATURE OF THE PROBLEM Implicitly, vocational counseling has operated on the assumption that choice of vocation is a cognitive function utilizing both cognitive and affective data about one's self and the environment. These cognitive processes are used most efficiently when all parts of the person are functioning with a minimum degree of conflict with each other. It has thus been rather widely accepted that per- sons enduring intense periods of internal conflict are unable to make realistic vocational choices. The early literature of vocational counseling clearly separated vocational counseling from personal counseling, believing at that time that there was little if any overlap between the two. Frequently, once a person had been "cured" of his poor adjustment to internal "conflict," he was re- ferred to a vocational counselor for the next step in his grOWth--vocationa1 choice. Counseling psychology has progressed from that dickkotomous position but has maintained the assumption that the better adjusted (that is, less conflict) the individual, the most likely he is to make a realistic and mature choice 1 rea. V00. for sis VOC is. 5114 alSi Che Poi: 50:. Of I wfio acti agai EQE of career. This belief has never been challenged very realistically. Recently, however, some advances in vocational theory and measurement have provided the avenues for such an investigation. From the position of a self-theorist the propo- sition stated above could be that a major factor influenc- ing vocational development is the degree of harmony of his self-concept--his internal consistency of self-evaluation. Harmony of self-concept, defined as the degree to which an individual has a congruent or internally con- sistent evaluation of himself, may be a factor influencing vocational development. Super (1963) suggested that harmony of self-concept is considered to be related to adjustment, to lack of con- flict, and to personality integration. It may, therefore, also be related to ability to formulate a vocational choice, that is, to see oneself in an occupational role. The choice of an occupation requires an individual to state rather explicitly his concept of himself. At some POint in time he must say, "I am this or that kind of per- son." Throughout his life the individual plays a variety 0f roles which provide him with an opportunity to discover who he is and what he wants to be. In play and work aCtivities he tests his abilities and evaluates them agaiJlst his accomplishments and the reactions of others. He finds that he does some things well and gains a sense of satisfaction from them (Crites, 1959): Ch Si Super (1951) stated that those successes tend to develop in the individual a series of little specific pic- tures of the self, which, eventually, add up to a larger picture of the self. As the individual matures he attempts to integrate the various pictures he has of himself into a consistent self-concept. One of the most meaningful ways for an individual to preserve and enhance his image of himself is through his occupational activities. He attempts to select an occupation which will be compatible with his self-concept and which will allow him to make it a reality by permitting him to play the role he wants to play. In light of the foregoing discussion, it would seem logical that translating an internally consistent and harmonious system of self-concepts into an occupational preference would be easier than finding an appropriate role for a conflicted self-concept system. Implementing a harmonious self-concept system in the occupational world should be easier than implementing unintegrated ideas of the self. The present study was designed to investigate the Possible relationship between the degree of harmony of self-concept and vocational interest maturity. More Specifically, the study attempted to demonstrate empiri- call); that the variable of harmony of self-concept affects inteIlsity of vocational interests, diversity of vocational interests, and realism of vocational interests, as re- fle(filed in psychometric data. see] the male groc inte in t inte ing Statement of Purpose The purpose of this study is threefold. It first seeks to determine whether there is a relationship between the degree of consistency of self-concept for a group of male college students and their level of vocational interest maturity. Secondly, it seeks to determine whether there is a relationship between the degree of ego integration for a group of male college students and their level of vocational interest maturity. Thirdly, it seeks to detect differences in the degree of harmony of self-concept and vocational interest maturity between three groups of males with vary- ing degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Theory Donald E. Super has been the major advocate of the position that self-concept formation and implementation are important factors influencing vocational development. There are two major influences apparent in Super's theory (Osipow, 1968). The first influence, self-concept theory, suggests that behavior is a reflection of an individual's attempt to implement his self-descriptive and self- eValuative thought. Along the same line of thinking, but With particular reference to vocations, Bordin (1943) Proposed that responses to vocational interest inven- tories represent an individual's projection of his self- c“meept in terms of stereotypes he holds about occu- Pations. An individual selects or rejects an occupation tn In '1) £1 because of his belief that the field is or is not con- sistent with his view of himself. Charlotte Buehler's (1933) writings in develop- mental psychology constitute the second major influence on Super's work. She wrote that life can be viewed as con- sisting of distinct stages, each stage requiring mastery of different life tasks. Super's conception of career development is based on the framework of these life stages based upon the assumption that vocational tasks reflect larger life tasks. In his career pattern concept, Super suggested that the life cycle imposes different vocational tasks on people at various times of their lives. To fully under- stand an individual's vocational life the entire cycle must be observed. Super (1953) generated the following ten propositions which should underlie a vocational develop- ment theory. 1. People differ in their abilities, interests, and personalities. 2. They are qualified, by virtue of these charac- teristics, each for a number of occupations. 3. Each of these occupations requires a characteristic pattern of abilities, interests, and personality traits, with tolerances wide enough, however, to allow both some variety of occupations for each individual and some variety of individuals in each occupation. 4. Vocational preferences and competencies, the situ- ations in which people live and work, and hence their self-concepts, change with time and experi- ence (although self-concepts are generally fairly stable from late adolescence until late maturity), making choice and adjustment a continuous process. 5. This process may be summed up in a series of life stages characterized as those of growth, explor- ation, establishment, maintenance, and decline, and these stages may in turn be subdivided into (a) the fantasy, tentative, and realistic phases of the exploratory stage, and the (b) the trial and stable phases of the establishment stage. 6. The nature of the career pattern (that is, the occupational level attained and the sequence, frequency, and duration of trial and stable jobs) is determined by the individual's parental socio- economic level, mental ability, and personality characteristics, and by the opportunities to which he is exposed. 7. Development through life stages can be guided, partly by facilitating the process of maturation of abilities and interests, and partly by aiding in reality testing and in the development of the self-concept. 8. The process of vocational development is essen- tially that of developing and implementing a self- concept: it is a compromise process in which the self-concept is a product of the interaction of inherited aptitudes, neural and endocrine makeup, opportunity to play various roles, and evaluations of the extent to which the results of role playing meet with the approval of superiors and fellows. 9. The process of compromise between individual and social factors, between self-concept and reality, is one of role playing, whether the role is played in fantasy, in the counseling interview, or in real life activities such as school classes, clubs, part-time work, and entry jobs. 10. Work satisfactions and life satisfactions depend upon the extent to which the individual finds adequate outlets for his abilities, interests, personality traits, and values; they depend upon his establishment in a type of work, a role which his growth and exploratory experiences have led him to consider congenial and appropriate. In a later revision of his theory, Super (1963) added that self-concept formation requires a person to recognize himself as a distinctive individual, yet at the Same time to be aware of the similarities between himself and others. The self-concept of a well integrated indi- vidual is continually developing, shifting somewhat through life as experiences indicate that changes are necessary to reflect reality. Presumably, the vocational part of an . beha tas} natL self-concept develops in a similar way. As an individual matures, he tests himself in many ways, most of which have implications for educational and vocational decisions. In a further extension of his theory, Super elaborated on the concept of vocational maturity. Vocational maturity allows an observer to assess the rate and level of an individual's career development. Vocationally mature behavior is dependent upon successfully dealing with the tasks involved with each life stage. In view of the fluid nature of vocational maturity, Super defined the concept, "normality," in terms of the congruence between an indi- vidual's vocational behavior and the expected vocational behavior at that age. The closer the two correspond, the greater the individual's vocational maturity. The major theoretical basis for the present study grew out of Donald E. Super's theory of vocational develop- ment. Specifically, the idea for this study was generated from Super's thesis that the degree of harmony of self- concept is a factor relating to vocational interest maturity. It is reasoned that a male's degree of harmony in his self-concept will reflect itself in the intensity, diversity, and realism of his vocational interests as measured on structured interest inventories such as the strong Vocational Interest Blank (SVIB). Males whose self-concepts are highly harmonious Should have more intense vocational interests than males who: whos more some conc more whos it c mat; Prob COHC: aVile: havir theC: the I this whose self-concepts are less highly harmonious. Males whose self-concepts are highly harmonious should also have more diverse vocational interests than males whose self- concepts are less highly harmonious. Males whose self- concepts are highly harmonious should, in addition, be more realistic in their vocational expectations than males whose self-concepts are less highly harmonious. Finally, it could be expected that three groups of males with vary- ing commitments to a vocational choice will show differ- ences in self-concept harmony and vocational interest maturity. Males admitting to having vocational development problems could be expected to have less harmonious self- concepts and less mature vocational interests than either males having made a tentative vocational choice, or males having made a deep commitment to a vocational area. Research Hypotheses The foregoing discussion of vocational self-concept theory and vocational development served as the basis for the research hypotheses that were developed and tested in this study. The twelve hypotheses were: 1. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to the inten- sity of their vocational interests. 2. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to the diversity of their vocational interests. 10. 11. 12. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to the degree of realism of their vocational interests. Differences exist in the degree of consistency of self-concept among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students is related to their degree of ego integration. The degree of ego integration for male college students is related to the intensity of their vocational interests. The degree of ego integration for male college students is related to the diversity of their vocational interests. The degree of ego integration for male college students is related to the realism of their vocational interests. Differences exist in the degree of ego inte- gration among three groups of males with vary- ing degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Differences exist in the diversity of vocational interests among three groups of males with vary- ing degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Differences exist in the intensity of vocational interests among three groups of males with vary- ing degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Differences exist in the realism of vocational interests among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. of an: 5'" J 10 Definition of Terms The following section contains the operational definition of the terms used in the present investigation. Self-concept is defined as an individual's evalu- ation of himself. Harmony of self-concept, refers to: (l) a measure of the degree of consistency of self-concept for a subject, and (2) a measure of the degree of ego integration for a subject. Consistency of self-concept is the degree to which an individual has an internally consistent evaluation of himself. In the present study this concept was measured by the Total C, Total D, and Total V scale scores of the Tennessee Self Concept Scale (TSC). Ego integration is the degree to which an indi- vidual has been able to balance his psychological needs so that there are a minimum of psychological needs with mutually incompatible objectives and high levels of joint strength. Measurement of this concept was accomplished by the Trehub method of scoring of the Edwards Personal Bgeference Schedule (EPPS). The concept of Vocational interests of an indi- vidual refers to the constellation of scores obtained by hJ'Jmonthe empirically defined and objectively scored occupational scales of the SVIB. 11 Intensity of Interest connotes the extent to which a subject's scale scores on the SVIB profile sheet deviate from the "men in general" zones on these same scales. In- tensity of interest is inferred from the relative number of objectively defined primary, secondary, and reject patterns according to Stephenson's (1961) method of pattern analysis resulting from these deviations. Thus subjects with a greater number of such patterns are said to have more in- tense interests than those subjects with fewer patterns. No attempt has been made to give differential weights to the various types of patterns. Tyler (1955) has warned that scale scores indicate the direction of interests but not the degree of interest. Thus primary, secondary, and reject patterns were all weighted equally in measuring intensity of interests. Unclassified Pattern of Interest refers to any one of the eleven occupational groupings on the SVIB profile sheet of a subject which was not analyzed as a primary, secondary, or reject pattern according to Stephenson's (1961) technique of objective pattern analysis. Diversity of Interests refers to the scale of the SVIB measuring "breadth of interests." The scale has been normed by setting the average score of a group of adults to 50, hence high scores of 58 or above indicate a broader than average range of interests. Realism is defined in the relative sense that those subjects with a smaller difference score between the 00: la: the Cat who the VOCE 950: EOE: 12 Occupational Level (OL) scale and the Academic Achievement (AA) scale, on the SVIB profile will be assumed to have a larger component of realism in their occupational interests than subjects with a larger difference score between OL and AA. Vocationally Undecided is the term used to classify male subjects in the study, juniors and seniors, who came to the Counseling Center seeking vocational testing and counseling. Vocationally Decided: Tentative is the classifi- cation employed for male subjects also juniors and seniors, who were enrolled in upper-division academic majors within the university and did not indicate any special need for vocational testing and counseling. Vocationallnyecided: Established refers to the group of male subjects in the study, who were third and fourth year doctoral level graduate students. Vocational Maturity refers to intensity of ‘Iocational interests (as measured by the Stephenson (ibjective pattern analysis of the SVIB occupational scales), diversity of vocational interests (as measured buy the DIV scale of the SVIB), and realism of vocational iJIterests (as measured by the discrepancy score between tale CL and AA scales of the SVIB). aca of all COL; Est. 13 Commitment refers to the differing levels of academic achievement and commitment to a vocational choice of the three groups of male subjects: Group 1 = Vocation- ally Undecided (junior/senior counseling sample), Group 2 = Vocationally Decided: Tentative (junior-senior non- counseling sample), Group 3 = Vocationally Decided: Established (doctoral non-counseling sample). Limitations of the Study Since this study dealt with only the relationship between variables as they existed at a single point in time, no cause and effect relationship can be infered. This study dealt with a single psychological variable (harmony of self-concept) which has been judged a priori to have a possible relationship to vocational development and maturity. The choice of this single variable is in no way intending to suggest that it is either the only determinant or even necessarily the prime determinant of vocational maturity. This study is limited by its sample. The partici- pants are all male university students. No information was available on females or non-university males. No generali- zation can be made concerning the population at large based on the sample in this investigation. 14 Organization of the Study The following chapter will include a review of the literature related to this study. Chapter III will contain a restatement of the hypotheses in testable form, a de- scription of the sample, and a report of the methodology employed. The results will be presented in Chapter IV. Chapter V will contain a summary of the study, a discussion of the results, conclusions, and implications for further research. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE As was stated in Chapter I, this investigation was designed to study the relationship between the degree of harmony of self-concept and vocational interest maturity. This chapter is divided into three main sections. The first section, a brief historical overview of the self- concept in vocational development, is presented as back- ground of the development of research that bridged the gap between personality theory and vocational psychology. The next section, the findings of research studies on the re- lationship between self-concept and vocational development, is presented as a review of previous investigations of self-concept implementation through vocational choice. {The third and final section reviews studies concerning the Irelationship between ego functioning and vocational (development. The purpose of this final section is to Igresent evidence for the influence of ego functioning on ‘wocational development and its relationship to the self- concept. 15 16 The Self-Concept in Vocational Development: A Brief Historical Overview Super has stated that first references to self- concept and vocational development appeared in the writings of Carter (1940). These formulations were imprecise and derived from his research into the interests of adolescents. Shortly after Carter's seminal writing, Bordin, who had worked extensively with structured interest inventories began considering vocational interests as reflections of the self-concept and of occupational stereotypes. Inde- pendently, a general interest in self-theory had been re- vived around Lecky's Self-Consistency teachings and A11- port's interest in Ego psychology. Beginning in 1951 the conceptualization of occu- pational choice as the process of implementing the self- concept began to bridge the gap between personality theory and vocational psychology. In that year Super (1951) wrote a paper linking self-concept to vocational develop- ment theory. Leona Tyler (1951) published a paper on the relationships of aptitudes and interests in young children in which she made use of self-concept theory to explain her findings. In a follow-up study Tyler (1955) followed the same children as they grew older, building her theory of vocational development around the concept of identity. Super (1953) identified self-concept development and vocational self-actualization as essential to a theory of Vocational development. Torrance (1954) described the use 17 of self-concept data in the educational counseling of college students. Tyler and Tiedeman summarized their thinking on self-concept and identity development in work- ing papers drawn on by the scientific Careers Project (Super and Bachrach, 1957), and use was made of the self- concept as the organizing element in The Psychology of Careers (Super, 1957). Anne Roe (1956) also dealt with the importance of the self-concept for occupational choice. In discussing a theoretical society in terms of Utopia she stated that in this imaginary, ideal, society, before an individual would make a vocational choice, he would know himself, and that the process of growing up would include, a full, conscious awareness of the self. He would develop a self- concept in accord with reality and this self-concept would be both known and acceptable to him. Ginzberg, in his early work, alluded to the im- portance of a clear self-concept for making a vocational choice (Ginzberg, 1951). He saw the adolescent as being tempted to consider his choice tentative for a longer time than is justified in terms of his opportunity for training. The adolescent feels that he does not know enough about himself or about the world, or about the role that he would like to play in the future, to make a definitive commitment. The period of indecision could only come to an end, Ginzberg felt, when the individual reaches the conclusion that he finally has a clear insight into himself. 18 From the vague connections made by Carter have come the sophisticated and complex work of Super. The relationship between self-concept and vocational develop- ment is now clearly established (Super, 1953; Brophy, 1959; Stewart, 1959; White, 1959; O'Hara and Tiedeman, 1959; Tageson, 1960; Englander, 1960; Norrell and Grater, 1960). Now the task is to differentiate aspects of the self-concept and various aspects of vocational development. Self-Concept Implementation Through Vocational Choice The central hypothesis for this study grew from that part of Super's theory of vocational development that deals with self-concept implementation through a vocational choice. It is relevant, therefore, to review a number of studies that have been conducted inquiring into the re- lationship between self-concept and career choice. Norrell and Grater (1960) tested the hypothesis that subjects who can accurately predict their interests, defined in terms of SVIB scores, are more aware of them- selves, awareness of self defined in terms of scores on selected scales of the Edwards Personal Preference Sche- dule (EPPS). Those S's who were able to predict accur- ately one-half or more of their interests on the SVIB were (assigned to a "high awareness of self" group. Twelve of ‘the EPPS scales were judged to possess relevance to self- awareness. These needs were: achievement, autonomy, Eiffiliation, intraception, dominance, nurturance, change, 19 and heterosexuality. For low self-awareness the needs identified were deference, order, succorance, and abase- ment. Subjects in the high awareness group were expected to score higher in the appropriate EPPS categories than 8'5 in the low self-awareness group. The results were all in the expected direction, but only two needs, order and succorance, were significant beyond the 0.05 level of confidence. Brown and Pool (1966) replicated the Norrell and Grater study. They again found the only two scales that remained significantly related to self-awareness were order and succorance. Englander (1960, 1961) had female majors in ele- mentary education, another area of education, and another field complete Q-sorts of (1) items about "self, and (2) statements about elementary teachers. He found that the elementary majors had the greatest degree of congruency between the sorts and concluded that individuals select or reject teaching in accordance with their respective perception of it as being compatible or incompatible with the self-concept. Using a somewhat different approach, Morrison (1962) obtained corroborative results. He administered ‘Q-sort tasks to forty-four second semester nursing stu- Cients and forty-three sixth quarter education students, i111 females. The nursing students were to sort on their Self-concept, their concept of a nurse, and their concept 0 53 There will be no relationship between the degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occu- pational scale scores. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be positively related to their intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occupational scale scores. Symbolically: p > 0 There will be no relationship between the degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pat- tern analysis of SVIB Occupational scale scores. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be positively related to their intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occupational scale scores. Symbolically: p > 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occu- pational scale scores. Symbolically: p = 0 54 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, will be positively related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p > 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and their diversity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be positively related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p > 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be positively related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p > 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and their diversity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 55 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, will be positively related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between CL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p > 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and their realism of vo- cational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between OL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be positively related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between CL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p > 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and their realism of vo- cational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be positively related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between CL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p > 0 10 56 There will be no relationship between the con- sistency of self-concept for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between OL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 Differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “1 # p2 # p3 Mean score for vocationally undecided group (juniors/ seniors counseling group) Legend: “1 p2 = Mean score for vocationally decided: tentative group (juniors/seniors noncounsel- ing group) u3 = Mean score for vocationally decided: established group (doctoral students non- counseling group) No differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “l = 11 12 13 57 Differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: pl # p2 # U3 No differences will exist in the degree of self-concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “l “2 u3 Differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “1 ¢ p2 f u3 No differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, will be inversely related to their degree of ego integration, as mea- sured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. Symbolically: p < 0 14 15 16 58 There will be no relationship between con- sistency of self-concept for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be inversely related to their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. Symbolically: p < 0 There will be relationship between consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be inversely related to their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. Symbolically: p < 0 There will be no relationship between con- sistency of self-concept for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of ego integration for male college students, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, will be inversely related to the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of the Occupational scales of SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p < 0 g... AI ,_ S 59 There will be no relationship between the degree of ego integration for male college students, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, and their intensity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of the Occupational scales of SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of ego integration for male college students, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, will be inversely related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p < 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of ego integration for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, and their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 The degree of ego integration for male college students, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, will be inversely related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p < 0 There will be no relationship between the de- gree of ego integration for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, and their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between CL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Symbolically: p = 0 19 20 21 60 Differences will exist in the degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vo- cational choice. Symbolically: “l # p2 # p3 No differences will exist in the degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vo- cational choice. Symbolically: “l = “2 = p3 Differences will exist in the intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of the SVIB, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: pl # p2 # u3 No differences will exist in the intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of the SVIB, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “l = “2 = u3 Differences will exist in the diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on the SVIB profile, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “1 ¢ “2 # p3 61 H No differences will exist in the diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on the SVIB profile, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “l = HZ = u3 H22 Differences will exist in the realism of vo- cational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between the 0L and AA scores on the SVIB profile, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vo- cational choice. ' Symbolically: “l # “2 # u3 H No differences will exist in the realism of vocational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between the 0L and AA scores on the SVIB, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Symbolically: “l = “2 “3 Preparation of the Data Consistency of Self-Concept Scores The initial procedure in the preparation of the data involved the acquisition of each individual's Total V, Total D, and Total C scores on the TSC (see Appendix B, Table 12). 62 Interest Intensity Patterns The next step in the preparation of the data in- volved the pattern analysis of the Strong Vocational Interest Blank profiles for each subject according to the Stephenson objective pattern analysis. The frequencies of primary, secondary, reject, and unclassified patterns were tabulated for each subject. The accuracy of the analysis procedure was verified by a second, independent, scoring (see Appendix B, Table 14). Diversity of Vocational Interests Each individual's diversity of vocational interest score was obtained from his DIV score on his SVIB profile (see Appendix B, Table 14). Realism Measure The score on this measure was computed for each subject by taking the difference between the Occupational Level (OL) and Academic Achievement (AA) scales of the SVIB, subtracting AA from OL (see Appendix B, Table 14). Ego Integration Scores The final procedure in the preparation of the data involved the computation of the ego integration score for each subject using the Trehub method of scoring of the EPPS (see Appendix B, Table 13). 63 Analysis The null form of hypotheses one through nine and thirteen through eighteen were tested for statistical significance by the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient. The level of significance was set at the .05 level for the testing of all correlational hypotheses in the study. The null form of hypotheses ten, eleven, twelve, nineteen, twenty, twenty-one, and twenty-two, were tested by analysis of variance. Scheffe (1959) post hoc com- parisons were computed to further analyze the data in instances where the null hypothesis was rejected. The statistical manipulations involved in this study were accomplished by use of a computer, utilizing programs written by the Michigan State University Computer Laboratory. Summary This chapter presented a description of the samples used in this study. The procedures used in the gathering and preparation of the data were presented. Reliability and validity information of the instruments used in mea- suring the variables pertinent to the study were presented. Finally, the statistical hypotheses for the purpose of testing the research hypotheses outlined in Chapter I were presented. CHAPTER IV RESULTS This chapter presents an analysis of the data and reports findings of the hypotheses tested. Each hypothesis is restated and the findings relevant to each hypothesis are presented. Tests of Hypotheses Table 1 lists the results of the correlational hypotheses. Reference will be made to this table in the TABLE l.--Corre1ations among the TSC scores, the Ego Integration scores, and the SVIB scores. Ego $32.2 232.2 32.2 Integration Score SVIB INTENSITY Score -.015 .012 .103 -.150 SVIB DIV Score -.098 .207* -.201 -.295* SVIB REALISM Score -.l43 -.060 .005 .129 EGO INTEGRATION Score -.045 -.111 -.021 1.000 *Significant at the .05 level. .9 64 65 discussion of the results of each of the correlational hypotheses. The tables listing the results of each of the analysis of variance hypotheses will be included separately, within the discussion of each of those hypotheses. Test of Hypothesis 1 H1 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, will be positively related to their intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occupational scale scores. H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occupational scale scores. As can be seen in Table 1, there was no significant relationship between degree of consistency of self-concept as measured by the TSC V score and intensity of vocational interests. The null hypotheses was retained. Test of Hypothesis 2 H2 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be positively related to their intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occupational scale scores. H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occu- pational scale scores. 66 As Table 1 indicates, there is no significant relationship between degree of consistency of self-concept as measured by the Total D score on the TSC and intensity «of vocational interests (r = .012). The null hypothesis was retained. {Test of Hypothesis 3 H3 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be positively related to their intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occupational scale scores. There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of SVIB Occu- pational scale scores. A correlation of .103 between degree of consistency 0f self-concept as measured by the Total C score on the TSC and intensity of vocational interests does not approach 'tlleerequired level of significance, thus the null hypothesis Was retained . Ehéjgt of Hypothesis 4 H4 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, will be positively related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and their diversity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. 'woa: ' AL. it 67 As reported in Table 1, there is no significant relationship (r = -.098) between the degree of consistency of self-concept as measured by the Total V score on the TSC and diversity of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis 5 H5 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be positively related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and their diversity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. As Table 1 indicates, there is a significant re- lationship (r = .207) between consistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and diversity of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was rejected and Hypothesis 5 was confirmed. Test of Hypothesis 6 H6 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be positively related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and their diversity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. 68 Table 1 reveals no significant relationship (r = -.201) between degree of consistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and diversity of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis 7 H7 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, will be positively related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between CL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and their realism of vo- cational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. A correlation of -.l43 between degree of self- concept, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and realism of vocational interests does not approach the re- quired level of significance. The null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis 8 H8 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be positively related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and their realism of vo- cational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. _ T'fl‘r 69 As reported in Table 1, there is no significant relationship (r = -.060) between degree of consistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and realism of vocational interests. The data did not support a rejection of the null hypothesis. Test of Hypothesis 9 H 9 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be positively related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. There will be no relationship between the con- sistency of self-concept for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Table 1 reveals no significant relationship (r = .005) between degree of consistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and realism of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis 10 H 10 Differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. 70 H No differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. TABLE 2.--Test of Hypothesis 10: Analysis of variance for differences among groups on the TSC V score. Source of d f Sum of Mean F 5:322:1- Variation Squares Square Value Level Between Groups 2 905.0346 452.5173 3.53 P < .03* Within Groups 66 8464.5000 128.2500 *Significant Table 2 reports a significant difference which was found among the three groups of males on consistency of self-concept as measured by the total V score on the TSC. The null hypothesis was rejected. Since the null hypothesis was rejected, the data were further analyzed with Scheffe post hoc comparisons. The confidence intervals for the comparisons that are significant at the .05 confidence level are summarized in Table 3. TABLE 3.--Significant Scheffe post hoc comparisons for Hypothesis 10: Differences among groups on Total V score on TSC. 2. .31 < “2 - u3 < 16.67 “.5 D.: .._n s. ‘l_ “y 71 The first significant comparison indicates that the two junior/senior groups combined together are significantly different from the group of doctoral level graduate students on degree of self-concept as measured by the Total V score on the TSC. As reported in Table 4 (page 72), the higher mean scores of the combined junior/senior groups suggests that they are more variable, and less consistent in their self-concepts, than the doctoral students with their smaller mean score on the Tennessee V scale. The second significant comparison indicates that the vocationally decided: tenta- tive group is significantly different from the vocationally decided: established group on degree of consistency of self-concept as measured by the Total V score on the TSC. Again, as reported in Table 4, the higher mean score of the junior/senior non-counseling group suggests that they are more variable, and less consistent in their self-concepts, than the doctoral students with their smaller mean score on the Tennessee V scale. Test of Hypothesis 11 H Differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. 11 H No differences will exist in the degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. 72 em.ma ov.nm oo.moa vm.oe n~.oa om.m¢ Hm.m mcwammGSOOIGOZ Hmuouooo om.vm mo.mm m~.moa mo.mv Hm.m mo.om mm.m mcwaomsooolsoz uoficom\noflooo mm.om mm.nm mm.mm on.o¢ mv.m mm.mm mo.m mafiaomcsou uoaoom\uoficso ouoom ouoom O onoom o ouoom > Ema moan moan cowuwuoouoH 0mm Iamom Iuo>Ho IsmusH mamom monouomoum mamom xsmam amouoan Hmcomuom monmsom umoosoo maom oommossoa Hmsoflumoo> maouum .moaomwnm> amusosauomxo so memos osouoll.v mqmsa 73 As can be seen in Table 5, the probability of occurrence did not approach statistical significance among the three groups of males on degree of consistency of self- concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC. The null hypothesis was retained. TABLE 5.--Test of Hypothesis 11: Analysis of variance for differences among groups on the TSC D score. fair-".1 Source of Sum of Mean F Signifi- Variation d'f° Squares Square Value cance Level Between Groups 2 1568.3982 784.1991 1.8362 P < .16* Within Groups 66 28187.0866 427.0771 *Non-significant. Eflsst of Hypothesis 12 H Differences will exist in the degree of con- 12 sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. H There will be no differences in degree of con- sistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Table 6 indicates no significant difference among the three groups of males on degree of consistency of 74 self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC. The null hypothesis was retained. TABLE 6.--Test of Hypothesis 12: Analysis of variance for differences among groups on the TSC C score. Mean F Signifi- Source of Sum of canoe Eh Variation 'f' Squares Square Value Level E5 Between Groups 2 68.4732 34.2366 .7973 P < .45* Groups 66 2834.0796 42.9406 3' *Non-significant Test of Hypothesis l3 H13 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, will be inversely related to their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. H There will be no relationship between consis- tency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC, and their degree of ego integration, as mea- sured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. As Table 1 indicates, there is no significant relationship between degree of consistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total V score on the TSC and degree of ego integration. The null hypothesis was retained. 75 Test of Hypothesis l4 H14 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, will be inversely related to their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. H There will be no relationship between con- sistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and their degree of ego inte- gration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. Table 1 reported no significant relationship (r = -.111) between degree of consistency of self-concept, as measured by the Total D score on the TSC, and degree of ego integration. The null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis 15 H15 The degree of consistency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, will be inversely related to their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. H There will be no relationship between consis- tency of self-concept for male college students, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and their degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. A correlation of -.201 between degree of consis- tency of self-concept, as measured by the Total C score on the TSC, and degree of ego integration does not approach the required level of significance. The null hypothesis was retained. vb! a -.——-r— .. ‘wumm‘x’ ~ . I “ .‘I A -.'I r 76 Test of Hypothesis 16 H The degree of ego integration for male college students, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, will be inversely related to the intensity of their vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of the Occupational scales of SVIB profiles. 16 H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of ego integration for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Trehub scoring of ’A EPPS profiles, and their intensity of vo- 4 cational interests, as measured by the Stephen- son pattern analysis of the Occupational scales of SVIB profiles. Table 1 reported no significant relationship (r = -.150) between the degree of ego integration and E; intensity of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis 1? H The degree of ego integration for male college students, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, will be inversely related to their diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. 17 H There will be no relationship between the de- gree of ego integration for male college stu- dents, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, and their diversity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the DIV score on SVIB profiles. Table 1 reported a significant relationship (r = -.295) between degree of ego integration and diver- sity of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was rejected and hypothesis 17 was confirmed.1 1As noted in Chapter III, the ego integration score is designed such that a high score indicates less 77 Test of Hypothesis.18 H18 The degree of ego integration for male college students, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, will be inversely related to their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between CL and AA scores on SVIB profiles. H There will be no relationship between the degree of ego integration for male.college stu- dents, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, and their realism of vocational interests, as measured by the discrepancy score between 0L and AA scores on SVIB profiles. Table 1 reported no significant relationship (r = .129) between degree of ego integration and realism of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was retained. Test of Hypothesis l9 H19 Differences will exist in the degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vo- cational choice. H No differences will exist in the degree of ego integration, as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vo- cational choice. degree of ego integration than a low score. Therefore, the negative correlation observed for hypothesis l7 indi- cates that individuals with high ego integration scores are less diverse in their vocational interests than indi- viduals with low ego integration scores. In other words, there is a positive relationship between degree of ego integration and diversity of vocational interests; the more integrated an individual is, the more likely he is to be diverse in his vocational interests. 4‘. uh... WET 78 Table 7 reports a significant difference among the three groups of males on degree of ego integration as mea- sured by the Trehub scoring of the EPPS. The null hy- pothesis was rejected. TABLE 7.--Test of Hypothesis 19: Analysis of variance for differences among groups on the Ego Integration score. Source of d f Sum of Mean F 5:322:1- Variation Squares Square Value Level Between Groups 2 1311.1832 665.5916 3.0855 P < .05* Within Groups 66 14237.2494 215.7159 *Significant Since the null hypothesis was rejected, the data were further analyzed with Scheffe post hoc comparisons. The confidence intervals for the comparisons that are significant at the .05 confidence level are summarized in Table 8. TABLE 8.--Significant Scheffe post hoc comparisons for Hypothesis l9: Differences among groups on Ego Inte- gration scores. 1. 15099 < ul + “2 - 2u3 < 21063 20 5010 < uz - < 11.72 ”3 Note: Group 1 = vocationally undecided (junior/ senior counseling); Group 2 = vocationally decided: tenta- tive (junior/senior non-counseling); Group 3 = vocationally decided: established (doctoral non-counseling). 79 The first significant comparison indicates that the two junior/senior groups combined together are signifi- cantly different from the group of doctoral level graduate students on degree of ego integration as measured by the Trehub scoring of EPPS profiles. As reported in Table 4, the higher mean scores of the combined junior/senior groups suggests that they are less integrated than the doctoral students with their smaller mean score on Ego Integration scale. The second significant comparison indicates that the vocationally decided: tentative group is significantly different from the vocationally decided: established group on degree of ego integration. Again, as reported in Table 4, the higher mean score of the junior/senior non-counseling group suggests that they are less integrated than the doctoral students with their smaller mean score on the Ego Integration scale. Test of Hypothesis 20 Differences will exist in the intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of the SVIB, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. H20 H No differences will exist in the intensity of vocational interests, as measured by the Stephenson pattern analysis of the Occupational scale scores of SVIB profiles, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commit- ment to a vocational choice. 80 As can be seen in Table 9, there is no significant difference among the three groups on intensity of vocational interests. The null hypothesis was retained. TABLE 9.--Test of Hypothesis 20: Analysis of variance for differences among groups on intensity of vocational interests. Em . . . : Source of Sum of Mean F Slgnlfl— ;' Variation d'f' S uares S uare Value cance ' q q Level ‘ Between if Groups 2 1.6854 .8427 .5983 P < .55* L4 Within Groups 66 92.9544 1.4084 *Non-significant Test of Hypothesis 21 Differences will exist in the diversity of vo- cational interests, as measured by the DIV score on the SVIB profile, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. H21 H No differences will exist in the diversity of vocational interests, as measured by the DIV score on the SVIB profile, between three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Table 10 reports a significant difference among the three groups of males on diversity of vocational interests as measured by the DIV score on the SVIB. The null hypothesis was rejected. 81 TABLE 10.--Test of Hypothesis 21: Analysis of variance for differences among groups on diversity of vocational interests. Signifi- Source of d f Sum of Mean F cance Variation Squares Square Value Level Between Groups 2 1958.5686 979.2843 11.27 P < .0001* pg Within E' Groups 66 5734.4760 86.8860 E *Significant % Since the null hypothesis was rejected, the data were further analyzed with Scheffe post hoc comparisons. The confidence intervals for the comparisons that are significant at the .05 confidence level are summarized in Table 11. TABLE ll.--Significant Scheffe post hoc comparisons for Hypothesis 21: Differences among groups on Diversity scores. 3 2. 5.47 < “2 - u3 < 18.89 Note: Group 1 = vocationally undecided (junior/ senior counseling); Group 2 = vocationally decided: tenta- tive (junior/senior non-counseling); Group 3 = vocationally decided: established (doctoral non-counseling). The first significant comparison indicates that the two junior/senior groups combined together are signifi- cantly different from the group of doctoral level students 82 on diversity of vocational interests. As reported in Table 4, the higher mean scores of the combined junior/ senior groups suggests that they are more diverse in their vocational interests than the doctoral students with their smaller mean score on the Diversity scale. The second significant comparison indicates that the vocationally decided: tentative group is significantly different from the vocationally decided: established group on diversity of vocational interests. Again, as reported in Table 4, the higher mean score of the junior/senior non-counseling HZ: group suggests that they are more diverse in their vo- cational interests than the doctoral students with their smaller mean score on the Diversity scale. Test of Hypothesis 22 H22 Differences will exist in the realism of vo- cational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between the 0L and AA scores on the SVIB profile, among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. ' H No differences will exist in realism of vo- cational interests, as measured by the dis- crepancy score between the 0L and AA scores on the SVIB, between three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vo- cational choice. As can be seen in Table 12, there is no signifi- cant difference among the three groups of males on realism of vocational interests as measured by the discrepancy 83 score between the CL and AA scores on the SVIB. The null hypothesis was retained. TABLE 12.--Test of Hypothesis 22: Analysis of variance for differences among groups on realism of vocational interests. Source of d f Sum of Mean F 5:332:1- Variation Squares Square Value Level Between Groups 2 9.2500 4.6250 .0418 P < .95* Within Groups 66 7302.6294 111.6459 *Non-significant Summary Five of the twenty-two null hypotheses tested in this study were rejected. The findings of this study indi- cated that an increase in the degree of ego integration accompanies an increase in the diversity of vocational interests. There was also a positive statistically significant relationship between one of the measures of consistency of self-concept on the Tennessee and diversity of vocational interests. Significant differences among the three groups were found on diversity of vocational interests (combined junior/senior groups were found to be more diverse than the doctoral students), degree of ego integration (com- bined junior/senior groups were found to be less integrated 84 than the doctoral student group): and consistency of self- ‘concept as measured by the Tennessee V score (combined junior/senior groups were found to be less consistent than 'the doctoral students). In the next chapter these results will be discussed aand implications for further research presented. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, LIMITATIONS AND IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH From the position of a self-theorist, a major factor influencing vocational development is the degree of harmony of an individual's self-concept--the internal consistency of his self-evaluation. This position has been discussed most thoroughly by Super (1963) who sug- gested that harmony of self-concept is related to appropri- ate adjustment, to lack of conflict, and to personality integration. He has also stated that harmony of self- concept and its concomitant "outcomes" are related to the ability to formulate a vocational choice, that is, to see oneself in an occupational role, and to make choices which will result in behaviors bringing that "role" into reality. In a general sense, the present study attempted to begin an investigation of some of the questions Super (1963) posed in his discussion of harmony of self-concept as a factor influencing vocational development. Two questions asked by Super which have piqued the research 85 86 interest of the present writer are: (l) is translating an internally consistent and harmonious system of self-concepts into an occupational preference easier than finding an appropriate role for a conflicted self-concept system? and (2) is implementing a harmonious self-concept system easier than implementing unintegrated ideas of the self? The present study was designed to investigate the possible relationship between the degree of harmony of self-concept and vocational interest maturity. The study attempted to demonstrate that the variable, "harmony of self-concept" is related to intensity of vocational inter- ests, diversity of vocational interests, and realism of vocational interests. The purpose of this study was threefold. It first sought to determine whether there was a relationship be- tween the degree of consistency of self-concept for a group of male college students and their level of vocational interest maturity. Secondly, it sought to determine whether there was a relationship between the degree of ego integration for a group of male college students and their level of vocational interest maturity. Thirdly, it sought to detect differences in the degree of harmony of self- concept and vocational interest maturity among three groups of males with varying degrees of commitment to a vocational choice. Participants who volunteered for the study were sixty-nine male juniors, seniors, and doctoral level 87 graduate students at Michigan State University. Twenty were juniors or seniors who came to the M.S.U. Counseling Center requesting vocational counseling and stating that they were having difficulty making a vocational choice. Twenty-seven were juniors or seniors enrolled in upper- division majors in the university. Twenty-two were third and fourth year doctoral level graduate students. Subjects completed the Strong Vocational Interest Blank for Men, the Tennessee Self Concept Scale, and the Edwards Personal Preference Schedule. The Strong profiles were pattern analyzed according to the objective method outlined by Stephenson (1961). The intensity of interests was determined on the basis of the distribution of primary, secondary, and reject patterns. The DIV scale on the Strong was used to assess diversity of vocational interests. A measure of realism was derived as a discrepancy score between the Occupational Level (OL) scale and the Academic Achievement (AA) scale on the Strong. A measure of ego integration was derived from the Trehub (1959) method of scoring the Edwards. Finally, three scales on the Tennessee were independently used to measure consistency of self- concept. Results of the analysis of the data indicated that an increase in the degree of ego integration accompanies an increase in diversity of vocational interests. There was also a positive, statistically significant, relation- ship between one of the measures of consistency of 88 self-concept on the Tennessee and diversity of vocational interests. Significant differences among the three groups were found on diversity of vocational interests (combined junior/senior groups were found to be more diverse than the doctoral students), degree of ego integration (com- bined junior/senior groups were found to be less inte- grated than the doctoral student group), and consistency of self-concept as measured by the Tennessee V score (combined junior/senior groups were found to be less consistent than the doctoral students). Conclusions A general conclusion emerging from this investi- gation is that a personality factor, harmony of self- concept, does influence vocational development. The fact that there were statistically significant differences be- tween the junior/senior groups and the graduate student group on the variables of consistency of self-concept and ego integration suggests that male college students in the process of making a vocational choice are also, at the same time, struggling to achieve an internally consistent evaluation of themselves and a balance of psychological needs. The differences on the self-concept variable sug- gest that male college students who are highly committed to a vocational choice are less variable, and more inte- grated, in their self-concepts than are male college 89 students with less of a commitment to a vocational choice. 'The differences on the degree of ego integration suggest that male college students who are highly committed to a vocational choice have achieved a greater balance of their psychological needs than have male college students with less of a commitment to a vocational choice. This would seem, in a general way, to support Super's proposition that harmony of self-concept is related to adjustment, to lack of conflict, to personality integration, and to ability to formulate a vocational choice. Some implications for counseling practice may be derived from these findings. In Chapter I the early dichotomy made between "vocational counseling" and "per- sonal counseling" was discussed. The findings of this study suggest the obsolescence of the practice of first "curing" a person of his adjustment problems and then re- ferring him to a vocational counselor for the next step in his growth--vocational choice. The personality development of male college students and the vocational development of male college students would seem to be inter-related, based on the findings of this study. Male college stu- dents who have made a commitment to a vocational choice appear to have a more harmonious self-concept than male college students with less of a commitment to a vocational choice. This would seem to suggest that helping persons, such as counselors, might want to consider assisting individuals who are in the process of making a vocational 90 choice by broadening an approach to vocational counseling. In addition to the typical services of vocational interest testing, occupational information dissemination, and occu- pational exploration, the counselor might want to consider spending time with a client helping him to become clearer in his ability to be aware of his self-concept--the kind of person that he is and how this might affect his vocational choice. Hopefully, as a male college student becomes more aware of himself, and is able to be internally consistent in his evaluation of himself and achieve a balance of his psychological needs, it would be easier for him to see himself in an occupational role and make a vocational choice. A second, more tentative conclusion, drawn from this study is that diversity of vocational interests is related to consistency of self-concept and degree of ego integration. The low magnitude to the correlations be- tween these variables does not make them useful for pre- dictive purposes. In light of the significant differences between the junior/senior groups and the graduate student group on diversity of vocational interests, the variable does seem to merit additional attention for future theory building. However, the direction of the results of the significant differences among the groups on diversity was opposite to the predicted direction. 91 The findings seem to suggest that male college stu- dents who are highly committed to a vocational choice are less diverse in their vocational interests than male col- lege students with less commitment to a vocational choice. The male doctoral candidate has narrowed the breadth of his interests considerably beyond the male undergraduate student. Because of the specificity of jobs related to the Ph.D., it would seem that the self-concepts of doctoral students would lead them to be specific in the way they see themselves. Future research could be useful in attempting to link more clearly the variable of diversity of vocational interests to harmony of self-concept. The present study produced tentative evidence for the existence of a link, although the found direction of the diversity for a sample of male college students was opposite to that hypothesized. This study attempted to relate ego integration to harmony of self-concept and vocational interest maturity. The significant results with the ego integration variable do seem to suggest that the degree to which an individual has been able to balance his psychological needs is of some import in his vocational development. Thus, for the self theorist ego integration, as used by Trehub, seems synonomous with "harmony" of self-concept. If future research could more clearly differentiate psychological needs and the areas of possible imbalance 92 there could be significant implications for counseling practice. For example, in working with college students experiencing difficulty making a vocational choice, if counselors could assess specific areas of need imbalance, these areas could be specifically focused on in counseling to help a client resolve the conflict. Finally, the results of this study seem to warrant the conclusion that future research linking self-concept development and vocational development could prove fruit- E ful. The basic proposition of Donald Super that making a L? vocational choice is a way of implementing a self-concept is worthy of continued research. Limitations The major limitation of the present study is the size of the sample and sampling procedures employed. An increase in the size of the sample might be expected to increase the magnitude of a number of the relationships between variables in the study. The findings in this study can be legitimately applied only to a population of male college students. Replication of the study with a non-college population would increase the generalization of the findings. Inclusion of female subjects would also add a further dimension to the study of self-concept and vocational development. The sampling procedures used to obtain the junior/ senior counseling sample might have limited the significance 93 of the findings. Students use various problems as pre- senting concerns when requesting counseling services. Sometimes these presenting problems do not accurately reflect the source of a student's difficulty. A future study might want to control more rigidly for degree of disturbance of the counseling sample. :1 “iii! SI 3 ‘ Another limitation in this study could have possi- bly arisen from the non-validated conceptions of intensity, realism of vocational interests, and degree of ego inte- gration and the non-validated methods of measuring these was. .mmm: _ . _ ‘ . fl . i constructs. A final limitation of this study is the fact that the data were collected in a cross sectional manner. The assumption was made that the differences among groups were due to maturational factors. This hypothesis cannot be confirmed with the cross sectional data obtained in the present study. Implications for Future Research Future research linking harmony of self-concept and vocational development could be enhanced by removing some of the limitations of the present study. Larger samples and more rigidly controlled sampling techniques might contribute to a greater magnitude of the relation- ship between variables observed in the present study. Replication with a non-college population and with female subjects would enhance generalizability of the study. 94 Future studies might also profitably be devoted to the validation of concepts and measures used in the present study. Validated conceptions and measures of intensity and realism of vocational interests, and ego integration would enhance the precision of research in these areas. The use of multivariate procedures might prove to be valuable in attempting to further link harmony of self- concept to vocational interest maturity. The univariate design of the present study did not take into account the interaction of variables bearing on vocational development. Finally, the use of longitudinal rather than cross sectional data would greatly add to the precision of this type of vocational development research. REFERENCES REFERENCES Allport, G. W. "The Ego in Contemporary Psychology." Psychological Review, 1943, 52, 451-478. Anastasi, Anne. Psychological Testing. New York: MacMillan Company, 1961. Anderson, T. B. and Olson, L. C. "Congruence of Self and Ideal-self and Occupational Choices." Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1965, 44, 171-176. Barron, F. "An Ego Strength Scale Which Predicts Response to Psychotherapy." Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1953, 11, 327-333. Blocher, D. H. and Schutz, R. A. "Relationships Among Self-Descriptions, Occupational Stereotypes, and Vocational Preferences." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1961, 8, 314-317. Bordin, E. S. "A Theory of Interests as Dynamic Phenomena." Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1943, 3, 49-66. Brophy, A. L. "Self, Role, and Satisfaction." Genetic Psychological Monograph, 1959, 29, 263-308. Brown, R. A. and Pool, D. A. "Psychological Needs and Self Awareness." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1966, 13, 85-88. Buehler, Charlotte. Der Menschliche Lebenslau als Psychologiches Problem. Leipzig: Hirzel, 1933. Campbell, D. P. "Stability of Interests Within an Occu- pation Over Thirty Years." Journal of Applied Psychology, 1966, 52, 51-56. Carter, H. D. "The Development of Vocational Attitudes." Journal of Counseling_Psychology, 1940, 4, 185-191. 95 - 1.. J5; . J -.— . _,—. 96 Congdon, C. 8. Self Theory and Chlorpromazine Treatment. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Vanderbilt University, 1958. Crites, J. O. "Ego-strength in Relation to Vocational Interest Development." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1960, 1, 137-143. Crites, J. 0. Vocational Psychology. New York: McGraw- Hill Company, 1969. Cronbach, L. J. Essentials of Psychological Testing. New York: Harper and Row, 1960. Edwards, A. L. Manual: Edwards Personal Preference Schedule. New York: The Psychological Corpor- * ation, 1959. Englander, M. E. "A Psychological Analysis of Vocational Choice: Teaching." Journal of Counseling Englander, M. E. "Q Sort: A Means to Explore Vocational Choice." Educational and Psychological Measure- ment, 1961, 21, 597-605. Fitts, W. H. Manual: Tennessee Self Concept Scale. Nashville, Tennessee: Tennessee Department of Mental Health, 1965. Ginzberg, E., Ginsburg, S. W., Axelrad, S., and Herma, J. L. Occupational Choice. New York: Columbia University Press, 1951. Havener, P. H. Distortions in the Perception of Self and Others bnyersons Using Paranoid Defenses. Unpub- lished Doctoral Dissertation, Vanderbilt, Uni- versity, 1961. James, F. "Occupational Choice and Attitude Change." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1965, 13, 311- 315. Kibrick, A. Tiedeman, D. V. "Conception of Self and Per- ception of Role in Schools of Nursing." Journal of Counseling Psyghology, 1961, 8, 62-69. Korman, A. K. "Self-esteem Variable in Vocational Choice." Journal of Applied Psychology, 1966, 52, 479-486. Lecky, P. Self-Consistency. New York: Island Press, 1945. “‘- m. 5 v . u' \‘- ‘ b, r I .I 97 Morrison, R. L. "Self-concept Implementation in Occu- pational Choices." Journal of Counseliqg Murray, H. A. Explorations in Personality. New York: Oxford University Press, 1938. Norrell, G. and Grater, H. "Interest Awareness as an Aspect of Self-awareness." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1960, 1, 289-292. Oppenheimer, E. A. "The Relationship Between Certain Self Construct and Occupational Preferences." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1966, 13, 191-197. Osipow, S. H. Theories of Career Development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1968. Piety, K. R. The Role of Defense in Reporting on the Self Concepy. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Vanderbilt University, 1958. Powers, M. K. "Permanence of Measured Vocational Inter- ests of Adult Males." Journal of Applied Psychology, 1956, 42, 69-72. Roe, A. The Psychology of Occupations. New York: Wiley, 1956. Scheffe, H. The Analysis of Variance. New York: Wiley, 1959. Schletzer, V. M. A Study of the Predictive Effectiveness of the Stronngocational Interest Blank for Job Satisfaction. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1963. Schuh, A. J. "Use of the Semantic Differential in a Test of Super's Vocational Adjustment Theory." Journal of Applied Psychology, 1966, 52, 516-522. Segal, S. J. "A Psychoanalytic Analysis of Personality Factors in Vocational Choice." Journal of Counsel- ing Psychology, 1961, 8, 202-210. Small, L. Personality Determinants of Vogational Choice. Psychological Monographs, 1953, 67, No. 351. Stephenson, R. R. "A New Pattern Analysis Technique for the SVIB." Journal of Counseling Psychology, 98 Stephenson, R. R. "Occupational Choice as a Crystallized Self Concept." Journal of Counseling_Psychology, 1961, 8, 211-216. Stewart, L. H. "Mother-Son Identification and Vocational Interest." Genetic Psychological Monograpp, 1959, pg, 31-63. Strong, E. K. Vocational Interests of Men and Women. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1943. Strong, E. K. Vocational Interests Eighteen Years After College. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1955. Strong, E. K. (rev. by David P. Campbell). Manual: StrongyVocational Interest Blanks. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1969. Super, D. E. "Vocational Adjustment: Implementing a Self Concept." Occupations, 1951, 3g, 88-92. Super, D. E. "A Theory of Vocational Development." American Psychologist, 1953, 8, 185-190. Super, D. E. The Psychologypf Careers. New York: Harper and Bros., 1957. Super, D. E. Career Development: Self-Concept Theory. New York: Coliege Entrance Examination Board, 1963. Super, D. E. and Bachrach, P. B. Scientific Careers and Vocational Development Theory. New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia Uni- versity, 1957. Super, D. E. and Crites, J. O. Appraising Vocational Fitness. (Rev. ed.) New York: Harper and Row, 1962. Super, D. E. and Crites, J. 0. Vocational Development: A Framework for Research. New York: Teachers College Bureau of Publications, 1959. Tageson, C. E. The Relationship of Self Perceptions to Realism of Vocational Preference. Washington, D.C.: Catholic University of America Press, 1960. Torrance, E. P. "Some Practical Uses of Knowledge of the Self Concept in Counseling and Guidance." Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1954, .lér 120-127. 99 Trehub, A. "Ego Disjunction and Psychopathology." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1959, 88, 191-194. Tyler, L. E. "The Relationship of Interests to Abilities and Reputation Among First Grade Children." Edu- cational and Psycholggical Measurement, 1951, ii, 255-264. Tyler, L. E. "The Development of Vocational Interests: The Organization of Likes and Dislikes in Ten-Year- Old Children." Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1961, 88, 33-44. Warren, J. R. "Self-Concept, Occupational Role Expectation, and Change in College Major." Journal of Counsel- ing Psychology, 1961, 8, 164-169. Wayne, S. R. The Relation of Self-Esteem to Indices of Perceived and Behavioral Hostility. UnpubliShed Doctoral Dissertation, Vanderbilt University, 1963. umm' SEAT. 1 I I I APPENDICES ‘é A'Q‘... I'J'I \g' APPENDIX A LETTER REQUESTING PARTICIPATION OF COUNSELING SUBJECTS FOR THE STUDY 100 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY East Lansing - Michigan 48823 Counseling Center - Student Services Building Dear Student: :3 "rt-r i We at the Counseling Center believe that one of the ways we have of becoming increasingly helpful to students like yourself is through careful study of your problems and of our effectiveness in helping you with them. We ask that you help us in our study of student problems by partici- , pating in a research project that we are currently under- ‘PM taking at the Counseling Center. i ‘ uni—.1; -. If you agree to participate, your commitment would consist of completing three tests before seeing your counselor for the first time. Two of the tests are often used in vocational counseling and, if appropriate for your situation, could be interpreted to you by your counselor. The third test is a research instrument which takes about thirteen minutes to complete. It will not be interpreted. In summary, your participation consists of taking three tests. Two of the tests are ones often used in vocational counseling. We want to emphasize that your willingness to participate in no way affects your seeing a counselor at the Center. Our most important consideration is to assist you with the resolution of the problems that stimulated you to seek our help. We do want you to know, however, that through the participation of students like yourself in such research projects as this we have an opportunity of studying in a more objective way some of the kinds of problems students encounter and the ways in which we can help the student to a resolution of his difficulties. Sincerely, Cecil L. Williams Associate Professor Richard A. Morril Intern APPENDIX B TABLES 13 TO 15 TABLE l3.--Tota1 V, Total D, and Total C scores on the Tennessee Self Concept Scale for sixty-nine subjects. 101 Subject Group Tosal Togal Togal Subject Group Tosal Togal Togal t i “0' No. Score Score Score NO‘ NO' Score Score Score 1 1 67 104 37 36 2 52 102 34 2 1 70 109 24 37 2 48 77 37 3 1 26 127 32 38 2 44 121 27 4 l 30 61 18 39 2 54 122 28 5 l 51 118 28 4O 2 29 131 28 6 1 41 80 27 41 2 61 102 31 7 1 36 78 19 42 2 79 125 21 8 1 50 78 28 43 2 63 107 27 9 l 41 88 18 44 2 57 123 33 10 1 24 58 26 4S 2 52 95 21 ll 1 44 121 26 46 2 54 106 29 12 l 31 120 30 47 2 60 118 35 13 1 46 92 22 48 3 34 138 29 14 1 53 77 28 49 3 42 99 35 15 1 70 105 34 50 3 31 123 21 16 l 57 108 45 51 3 53 107 32 17 l 53 92 36 52 3 45 114 27 18 l 39 162 33 53 3 39 112 25 19 l 45 82 22 54 3 33 99 22 20 1 59 105 24 55 3 41 111 30 21 2 42 124 28 56 3 46 93 26 22 2 42 120 22 57 3 47 132 27 23 2 47 117 45 58 3 43 130 22 24 2 69 136 49 59 3 56 129 26 25 2 54 115 29 60 3 37 101 24 26 2 45 111 36 61 3 36 111 27 27 2 39 84 25 62 3 40 130 33 28 2 25 123 21 63 3 29 78 27 29 2 52 130 27 64 3 48 84 30 30 2 46 74 22 65 3 44 65 19 31 2 45 84 25 66 3 29 68 31 32 2 41 100 27 67 3 34 127 39 33 2 45 110 41 68 3 29 120 23 34 2 36 94 19 69 3 56 132 28 35 2 43 99 33 *Group 1 s vocationally undecided (junior/senior counseling), males. Group 2 - vocationally decided (junior/senior non-counseling): tenta- tive, males. Group 3 - vocationally decided (doctoral non-counseling): established, males. 102 TABLE 14.--Ego integration scores for sixty-nine subjects. Subject Group Ego Integration Subject Group Ego Integration No. No.‘ Score No. No.*; Score 1 1 9 36 2 12 2 1 51 37 2 41 3 l 8 38 2 42 4 1 38 39 2 34 S 1 33 4O 2 28 6 l 54 41 2 15 7 1 28 42 2 32 8 1 13 43 2 0 9 1 19 44 2 28 10 1 53 45 2 0 11 l 34 46 2 23 12 l 13 47 2. 5 13 1 14 48 3 27 14 l 17 49 3 4 15 l 30 50 3 3 16 1 15 51 3 21 17 l 36 52 3 14 18 1 9 + 53 3 52 19 1 23 54 3 10 20 1 42 55 3 9 21 2 13 S6 3 18 22 2 13 S7 3 36 23 2 5 58 3 10 24 2 15 59 3 6 25 2 52 60 3 16 26 2 27 61 3 ll 27 2 25 62 3 22 28 2 23 63 3 18 29 2 44 64 3 ll 30 2 33 65 3 22 31 2 23 66 3 14 32 2 30 67 3 36 33 2 64 68 3 2 34 2 9 69 3 2 35 2 38 *Group 1 - vocationally undecided (junior/senior counseling), males. Group 2 - vocationally decided (junior/senior non-counseling): tentative, males. Group 3 I vocationally decided (doctoral non-counseling): established, males. .1()3 TABLE lS.--Number of primary, secondary, reject, and unclassified patterns, DIV scores, and OL-AA scores for sixty-nine subjects. Subject 6:093 prinary Secondary 13.566: Unclassified DIV OL-AA No. No. 1 1 2 2 6 1 57 -11 2 1 o 1 7 3 so 12 3 1 o 2 7 2 66 - 4 4 1 2 1 7 1 55 6 5 1 2 1 7 1 57 19 6 1 2 1 7 1 26 21 7 1 o o 10 1 55 26 8 1 2 1 7 1 44 6 9 1 2 2 6 1 64 16 10 1 1 o 7 3 43 25 11 1 2 1 7 1 57 10 12 1 2 o 8 1 57 17 13 1 3 1 7 o 52 6 14 1 o 1 7 3 57 - 2 15 1 o o 11 o 57 4 16 1 o o 8 3 61 22 e 17 1 2 1 6 2 62 -12 r 18 1 2 1 8 o 66 4 a 19 1 1 4 4 2 so 8 r 20 1 2 1 8 o 43 16 . 21 2 2 2 6 1 62 35 « 22 2 1 1 7 2 66 1 'f 23 2 o 4 6 1 55 23 e 24 2 o 2 6 3 48 33 25 2 o 2 5 4 57 4 . 26 2 3 o 6 2 44 1o 27 2 1 o 5 5 41 7 28 2 o 2 6 3 61 7 29 2 o 2 9 o 66 9 3o 2 o 2 8 1 75 2 31 2 o 2 7 2 s2 10 32 2 4 2 5 o 48 22 33 2 2 2 6 1 57 o 34 2 1 1 8 1 55 5 35 2 o 1 8 2 55 9 36 2 1 o 6 4 62 8 37 2 1 1 7 2 37 17 38 2 1 2 7 1 64 15 39 2 4 1 6 o 61 3 4o 2 3 1 4 3 61 10 41 2 5 o 6 0 so 23 42 2 1 1 7 2 44 7 43 2 2 o 7 2 53 -1o 44 2 o 2 8 1 55 2 45 2 2 o 8 1 77 6 46 2 o 3 7 1 61 - 4 47 2 2 1 7 1 46 3 48 3 2 o 8 1 50 21 49 3 5 o 6 o 46 2 so 3 o 2 8 1 46 2 51 3 o 2 6 3 39 - 4~ 52 3 4 o 6 1 35 2 53 3 o 1 7 3 41 16 54 3 1 2 6 2 43 22 55 3 1 o 7 3 48 - 6 56 3 2 1 7 1 39 - 1 57 3 o o 7 4 41 9 58 3 3 1 7 o 48 17 59 3 1 1 5 4 52 9 60 3 2 o 7 2 61 7 61 3 3 1 7 0 so 17 62 3 2 1 6 1 48 11 63 3 o 2 8 1 48 20 64 3 4 o 6 1 46 1 .65 3 1 2 7 1 so 28 66 .3 4 o 5 2 28 19 67 3 3 2 4 2 19 7 68 3 o 3 7 1 48 0 69 3 o 4 5 2 39 27 *Group 1 - vocationally undecided (junior/senior counseling), males. Group 2 - vocationally decided (junior/senior non-counseling): tentative, males. Group 3 - vocationally decided (doctoral non-counseling): established, sales. ov‘tv HICHIGRN STRTE UNIV. LIBRARIES IIHIWINlllH‘lWIWIIWIWHIHllllHIllHHMHl 31293103573048