AN INVESTIGATION OF THE POSSIBILWIES OF PAROLE PREDICTION THROUGH THE USE OF FIVE PERSONALITY INVENTORIES Thai: for flu Dogs of Ed D. MICHIGAN STATE COLLEGE Dana“ Reid Thurston I954 g , llllllllll‘llilllHIlllllllmlllllilllllllllIUIHUlIIleIlI 3 1293 10371940 This is to certify that the thesis entitled An Investigation of the Possibilities of Parole Prediction Through the Use of Five Personality Inventories presented by Donald Reid Thurston has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Edng degree mm of Emcation MXM Major professor Date Fabmy 15: 1955 0-169 MSU LIBRARIES .—:,—. RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. as. it" €133 AN INVESTIGATION OF THE POSSIBILITIES OF PAROLE PREDICTION THROUGH THE USE OF FIVE PERSONALITY INVENTORIES BY DONALD REID THURSTON A THESIS Submitted to the School of Graduate Studies of Michigan State College of Agriculture and Applied Science in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOC TOR OF EDUCATION \ Department of Foundations of Education V n 4" 1954 THFSIS fi _..._t.*_ v m A A,_ put isl‘t‘ar , r . ‘ J U I ‘n I s‘. try”, I "Er-P a», ‘J A, Dr L_ Idc EE: ‘ I91“? d ‘1‘: W: U .I‘u r- l '1‘- h w 5 «EU “ L‘; h ABSTRACT A review of the literature pertaining to parole prediction re- vealed that almost all factors previously investigated have been pre- incarceration factors. It seemed that a prediction method should also consider any differences which might exist in the thinking of the pro- Spective parolees at the time they are being considered for parole. Consequently, the purpose of this study was to test one means of dis- covering whether or not there are differences in the thinking of suc- cessful and nonsuccessful parolees, and if such differences were dis- covered, whether or not the nature and extent of such differences would have value as predictors of parole outcome. To discover whether or not there are such differences, five personality inventories were administered to a group of inmates going on parole. The inventories administered were the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN, the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR, the California Mental Health Analysis, the Johnson Temperament Anal- ysis, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. A total of 471 inmates from three Michigan penal institutions were tested in this manner. After all the inmates had been on parole at least one year they were identified as successful if they were under active-supervi- SiOn or had been discharged, or as violators. The successful parol- ees were divided into two categories, the "best" and the "doubtful." The significance of differences between mean scores of the three groups on the subtests of the inventories was determined by using the critical ratio of the difference between two means. It Was found that both the "best" and the "doubtful" groups are more sséieo masculine in attitudes and interests than the violators; the "best" are more confident than the violators; the "best" are less nervous than the violators; both the "best" and the ”doubtful" are more sympathetic than the violators; the "best" are more satisfied with work and recreation than either the "doubtful" or the violators; and both the "best" and the "doubtful" indicate less psychopathic devia- tion than the violators. The differences on the remainder of the forty-nine traits were not significant. The responses of one hundred of the "best" parolees, se- lected at random, were compared with the responses of one hundred of the violators, also selected at random, for each of the items on each of the inventories, by use of the critical ratio of differences between two proportions. There were 132 items that met the criterion of sig- nificance adopted for this study, a level of significance of .93 (7%) or higher. All the complete sets of answer sheets for the sample popula- tion were scored using those items. The mean total score of the successful parolees exceeded the total scores of 82 percent of the violators. The value of various cuteoff scores was demonstrated. For example, 73.3 percent of those who were successful on parole had scores of 40 or more, and 72.4 percent of those with scores of 40 or more were successful, while 60.4 percent of the violators had Scores of less than 40. The biserial coefficient of correlation between the scores of the successful and nonsuccessful parolees and parole outcome was '638 with a standard error of .044. The Kuder and Richardson re- 1iability coefficient was .872 and the reliability coefficient, according to a formula developed by Froelich, was .895. The results of this study establish, within the limitations im- pOSEd by the techniques used in the study, that there are differences iii in the thinking of prDSpective successful and nonsuccessful parolees at the time they are being considered for parole that can be mea- sured objectively, and that these differences do have relationship to parole outcome. iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer is sincerely grateful to Dr. Victor H. Noll for his constant encouragement and advice in the planning, organization, and writing of this report. Dr. Noll, the writer's major professor, has freely given suggestions and constructive criticisms which have made possible the completion of this study. The writer would also like to thank Dr. Harry Sundwall and Dr. James S. Karslake for their help— ful suggestions in regard to the procedure of the study and the sta- tistical analysis of the data. He is also indebted to Dr. Walter F. Johnson for his assistance in the formulation of the study and his gui- dance during the early stages of the collection of data. The writer also thanks Dr. Cecil V. Millard for his continued interest, his en- couragement, and his helpful suggestions. Thanks are further extended to Earnest C. Brooks, formerly the Commissioner of the Department of Corrections, and to Gus Har- rison, present Director of the Department, who gave permission for the study of the inmates under the jurisdiction of the Department and for the use of the facilities of that Department. The writer is also grateful to Warden William H. Barman, Warden Dr. Garrett Heyns, and Superintendent C. D. Miles, for permitting the use of the facili- ties of their respective institutions and making available for study the inmates under their jurisdiction. George Bacon, Deputy Warden in Chal‘ge of the Trusty Division at the State Prison of Southern Mich- igan, was also very gracious in permitting the inmates under his jurisdiction to take part in the study. Mr. James Silby, Institutional Parole Officer at the Jackson institution, contributed greatly to the Study by aiding in the administrative routine during the early stages V of the collection of data. Two of Mr. Silby's inmate clerks contrib- uted much of their time and initiative in the practical administration during the early stages of the study. The writer cannot express too greatly his appreciation to these clerks for their able assistance. All of the parole officers in the state of Michigan contributed :- to the study by rating the parolees included in the study who were under their supervision. The parole officers were very prompt and sincere in making these ratings, and many of them have expressed continued interest and encouragement in the study. ,4; it Finally, the writer wishes to express his appreciation to his wife, Marge Thurston, for her encouragement, patience, sacrifice, and assistance. To all of these and to the many others who have encouraged or added a little to the study, the writer wishes to extend his most since re g ratitude . vi TABLE OF CONTENT 5 CHAPTER I. II. INTRODUCTION ......................... Purpose of Study ...................... Outline of Procedure .................... Justification of Study .................... Definitions of Terms Used ................ Preview of Organization ................. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ............. Previous Parole Prediction Research ........ Early studies ............ ' ........... A Wisconsin study ................... A New Jersey study ............ ‘ ...... An Illinois study ..................... The first Glueck study ................ A Minnesota study ................... The second Illinois study ............... A study of federal prisoners ............ Two more Glueck studies .............. The Attorney General's report ........... An Alabama study ................... Another Illinois study ................. Another Wisconsin study ............... The most recent study ................ A different approach .................. Comparison of factors ................. Summary ............................ vii QNO‘UJN ll 11 I3 15 18 23 29 32 38 40 44 48 53 55 58 61 63 68 CHAPTER Page 111. PROCEDURE AND SAMPLE POPULATION ...... 71 Procedure ........................... 71 Sample Population ...................... 89 Summary ............................ 106 1"" IV. ANALYSIS OF STANDARDIZED INVENTORY I FACTORS ............................. 108 I Summary ............................ 137 I V. THE ITEMS AND THEIR VALIDITY ........... 139 I The Item Analysis ..................... 139 L Validity ............................. 156 Reliability ........................... 162 Summary ............................ 164 VI. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS ..................... l 65 Summary ............................ 165 The procedure ...................... 165 The findings ........................ 170 Conclusions .......................... 181 Recommendations for Application ........... 185 Recommendations for Further Study ......... 187 BIB LIOG RAPHY ............................... 190 APPENDIXES ................................. 195 Appendix A. The Certificate of Parole of the State of Michigan's Department of Correc- tions Including the Conditions of Parole . . . 196 Appendix B. The Inventories Used in the Present Investigation ...................... 197 viii Appendix C . Page Critical Ratios of the Difference Between the Percentage of Re3ponses by Successful Parolees and the Per- centage of Responses by Violators on the Items Included in the Guilford- Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN, an Inventory of Factors STDCR, the Johnson Temperament Analysis, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory ........................ 196 ix TABLE I. II. III. IV. VI. VII . VIII . IX. XI. XIII . XIV xv, LIST OF TABLES A Summary of the Findings of Several Investigators in Regard to the Relationship of Various Factors to Parole Outcome ......... Parole Service Prior to Violation for 529 Violators During the First Nine Months of 1950 ............................... Population Figures for Michigan's Penal Institutions on October 3, 1952 ............... Release Through Parole from Michigan‘s Penal Institutions during 1951 ............... Classification of Parolees into Three Categories According to Parole Adjustment or Type of Violation ...................... Total Number of Inventories Completed for Each of the Three Categories of Parolees ....... A Comparison of Sample Population According to Offense for Which Serving at Time of Parole ....................... The Significance of the Differences in the Success Rates of Various Groups of Offenders ............................ Sample Population by Institutions ............. Sample Population by Race ................. Sample Population by Age at Time of Parole ..... Sample Population by Intelligence ............. Sample Population According to Time Served ..... Sample Population According to Previous Record . . Comparison of Sample Population Scores with Norms on the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN ......................... Page 80 84 85 90 91 92 95 97 98 99 101 102 104 109 r.- ..._a-.v_ . .5-.." .-fi_-..__-. -fi TABLE XVI. XVII. XVIII. XIX . XX. XXI . XXII . XXIII. XXIV. XXV, XXVI. Comparison of Sample Population Scores with Norms on the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR .......................... . Comparison of Sample Population Scores with Norms on the California Mental Health Analysis ......................... Comparison of Sample Population Scores with Norms on the Johnson Temperament Analysis Comparison of Sample Population Scores with Norms on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory ................ Comparisons of the Means and Standard Deviations of the Three Parole Categories for the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN .................... The Differences Between the Means of the Three Parole Categories for the GAMIN with the Resultant Critical Ratios and Levels of Significance ............... s ....... Comparisons of the Means and Standard Deviations of the Three Parole Categories for the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR The Differences Between the Means of the Three Parole Categories for the STDCR with the Resultant, Critical Ratios and Levels of Significance ................ Comparisons of the Means and Standard Deviations of the Three Parole Categories for the Johnson Temperament Analysis . . . . The Differences Between the Means of the Three Parole Categories for the JTA with the Resultant Critical Ratios and Levels of Significance ....................... Comparisons of the Means and Standard Deviations of the Three Parole Categories for the California Mental Health Analysis . . xi 00000 00000 111 112 116 117 120 121 124 125 128 m w TABLE Page XXVII. The Differences Between the Means of the Three Parole Categories for the CMHA with the Resultant Critical Ratios and Levels of Significance ..................... 129 XXVIII. Comparisons of the Means and Standard Deviations of the Three Parole Categories for the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory .............................. 132 XXIX. The Differences Between the Means of the Three Parole Categories for the MMPI with the Resultant Critical Ratios and Levels of Significance ..................... 133 XXX. The Number and Percent of Items with Significant Differences from Each Inventory ............................ , . . 144 XXXI. The Weights Assigned to Items According to the Critical Ratio of Differences in Percentage of Responses ................... 158 XXXII. The Weighted Scores Made by 384 Parolees, Comparing Successful and Nonsuccessful, on 132 Items with Significant Differences .......... 160 XXXIII. The Means and Standard Deviations of Five Parole Categories According to Age at Time of Parole ......................... 173 XXXIV. The Mean Intelligence Quotients and Standard Deviations of Five Parole Categories .......... 173 XXXV. The Means, Standard Deviations, and Medians of Five Parole Categories According to Time Served .................. 175 xii Agrd. .Mr -q‘ _ A ._.u -“ u.—_-_ - A CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Purpose of Study . , t-.. "WI It has frequently been reported that 95 percent of the individ- M“. o a“. A i- uals who enter penal institutions are eventually released. Parole has long been recognized as the best available means for effecting this release. It is inherent in the philosophy of parole that the individual should be placed on parole when he is most ready to take his place as a worth-while member of the community. To this end the empha- sis is being placed on the reformation or rehabilitation of the indi- vidual, rather than on the punitive aSpects of incarceration. The basic assumption in modern penology is that incarceration should prepare the individual for his eventual return to society, should help him and teach him to be a better citizen, a worth-while member of a com- munity. The effectiveness of the correctional process should result in differences in the thinking of those individuals who are ready for Parole and those who- are not ready. Many of the authorities in pe- nOlogy feel there are differences in the inmate's attitudinal make-up which would also indicate readiness for parole. Authors of books on criminology state that the individual's attitudes toward authority and toWard society, his way of reacting to life's problem, his reactions to Stress, and his fundamental temperament, are all basic considera- tions for parolability. They speak of the emotionally mature indi- Vidual. the stable person, as being ready for parole. The sum effect of these factors should result in differences of thinking in the individuals being considered for parole which would indicate the possibility of parole success or failure. A technique for measuring such differences in their thinking and relating these differ- ences to parole outcome would aid considerably in determining when, or if, the individual becomes ready for parole, in terms of his men- tal readiness. It is the writer's purpose, in this study, to test one means of discovering whether or not there are differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees, and if such differences are discovered, whether or not the extent of such differences will have value as a predictor of parole success or failure. During the past twenty-five years there have been deve10ped and standardized many instruments whose purpose is the measurement of various aspects of personality and ‘adjustment. It is the writer's intention to use some of these standardized inventories to discover differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees. Therefore, the hypotheses to be tested by this study are: 1. There are differences in the thinking of successful and non- successful parolees which can be measured objectively by available standardized invento rie s . 2. The nature and extent of such differences will have some reliability and validity as predictors of parole success or failure. Outline of Procedure Although the procedure of the study is discussed in detail in Chapter III, a brief outline at this time is necessary for the intro- duction of the study. Five personality and adjustment inventories were ”189th for administration to a group of inmates about to go on parole, so that measurements of their thinking at time of parole could be obtained. The five inventories selected were the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN, Guilford's Inventory of Factors STDCR, the Johnson Temperament Analysis, the California Mental Health Analysis, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. In selecting these inventories an attempt was made to get valid and reliable instruments that would measure a wide range of factors or traits in the most economical manner, both monetarily and chronolog- ically. . After an adequate waiting period the successful parolees and the parole violators were identified. A more exacting classification resulted in three parole categories: the ”best" of the successful parolees; the "doubtful" group of the successful parolees; and the violators. An indication of the differences in the thinking of these three parole categories was obtained by applying the Critical Ratio test of significance to the differences in the responses, as indicated by subtest mean scores, of the three groups. A further indication of differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees was obtained by an item analysis. The item analysis indicated sig- nificant differences between the percentage of reSponses of successful and nonsuccessful parolees on some of the items. The relationship of these differences to parole outcome was determined by comparing the scores of the successful and nonsuccessful parolees for the items that had significant differences. Justification of Study The possibility of predicting parole success or failure has appealed to many persons. There have been several studies in this field. most of which resulted in the preparation of experience tables, much like the actuarial tables of insurance companies. The majority of the factors included in these tables was obtained from the parole files of the subjects and is largely in the nature of preincarceration factors. If the aim of the corrective process is the rehabilitation of the inmate, then a prediction technique should give considerable weight to the results of that process. The predictive techniques so far de- If vised include only one or two factors directly related to the effects of the individual's incarceration. Authors of textbooks on criminology ' and persons faced with the problem of determining parolability have : come to believe that predictive techniques must include some deter- '5' mination of the accumulative effects of institutionalization which have taken place within the individual. The experience tables have given releasing authorities a great deal of information regarding group characteristics of successful and nonsuccessful parolees and have made parole selection much more meaningful than a mere understanding of human nature. However, the information so obtained is based on group experience and can have little meaning for the individual, per se. The parole board or other releasing authority must still relate the information from the experi- ence tables to the prospective parolee and then base the final deci- sion, in part at least, on a judgment of the individual's thinking as it expresses his attitudes, his perception of his role in society, his Philosophy of life, his temperament, and other aspects of his psycho- logical make-up. There is a very urgent need for a supplemental aid Such as an objective means of determining the inmate's mental readiness for parole. The writer would like to point out, at this time, that the hoped- for reSults of this study would serve as a supplement to all other in- formélliion obtained about the prospective parolee, including the infor- matiOn from the experience tables. The factors included in the ‘1 '.Ili"l II .n. . I. J {E}: “A. 1...... , issuan experience tables will be discussed in detail in the review of the lit- erature, as background information pertinent to this study. However, it is not the purpose of this study to validate those factors or dis- cover additional factors of that nature. Rather, it is hoped that this study will result in a technique that will measure the extent of the inmate's present thinking as it reflects itself in a readiness for parole, at the time he is being considered for such release. Knowledge of this type would be exceptionally meaningful in identifying those cases which might be released prior to the minimum sentence. The writer does not feel that a justification for early pa- role is necessary to this study. One who views this problem unemo— tionally and objectively will realize the logic of releasing as early as possible the individual who has "learned his lesson" and who can be considered as being no longer dangerous to society. This type of knowledge would also aid in identifying the parole violators and re- peated offenders who have been returned to prison for some time and for whom there may no longer be a need for continued incarceration. Although previous incarcerations did not prove benefiCial in those cases, it might well be that the present sentence has made the dif— ference. An objective means of determining this fact would be a valuable aid in deciding whether it is necessary to pass or continue beyond the minimum of the indeterminate sentence, which is used almost exclusively in Michigan. Burgess and Sellin expressed the need of continued study in this area and the possibility of the use of standardized tests in the determination of parolability. In their foreword to Ohlin's recent work, they stated: Everyone recognizes that the prisoner's attitude is impor- taJlt. Especially significant are clinical studies to find out the Prisoner's motivations and any personality problems which may intfirfere with his reformation. Intensive study is needed to probe into his subjective life. Such study is most rewarding when the man first enters prison and during the period preceding his be- coming a member of the prison community. A favorable situa- tion is the diagnostic depot where he is detained for study in or— der to determine the most suitable placement for him in the insti- tution. At present also there are a growing number of personality tests applicable to the study of criminals. Statistical predictive instruments in their present form do not take account of the data obtainable by such interviews and by personality tests. This ma- terial should be used in conjunction with the findings of statistical prediction.1 A few pages later they ask: What is the predictive value of personality tests of the available standardized forms? May not personality tests espe- cially suited to the study of the criminal and of his rehabilitation be devised?2 Definition of Terms Used ”Parolability." Experience and research have taught that there are a great many factors which are likely to determine the probability of the inmate's making a successful adjustment on pa- role. The weight or importance of each of. these factors must be determined on an individual basis. Parolability is the term commonly used to denote this complex of factors. "Technical violation." Before an inmate is paroled, he signs the Certificate of Parole, a copy of which he retains as his .own. This document contains a list of parole conditions which have been read to him and discussed with him. If he violates any of these conditions, Ernest W. Burgess and Thorsten Sellin, Introduction, Lloyd E. Ohlin, Selection for Parole (New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1951), p. 15. 2 . Ibid., p. 17. he is in technical violation of his parole. A duplication of this part of the parole certificate is reproduced in Appendix A. "Recidivist." A repeating offender, one who has been com- mitted to a penal institution for the second or more times is labeled a recidivist, in penological literature. In general usage this term refers to anyone who relapses into a former state or condition. "Offense." The term "offense" is usually used to denote the crime or criminal act with which an individual has been charged. In parole prediction the term is sometimes used to denote the category which includes a comparison of the crimes or charges for which indi- viduals have been committed to penal institutions. ”Alienist." One skilled in the study or treatment of insanity and mental disorders was termed an alienist in earlier times. The use of the term has been largely replaced by the term "psychiatrist" in present usage. Preview of Organization Chapter 11 contains a review of the literature pertinent to this study and a further justification for the study. The procedure and techniques involved in the study are discussed in Chapter III with pertinent information regarding the sample population. Five standard- ized tests were used in this study. The subtest scores of success- ful and nonsuccessful parolees were analyzed by use of the critical ratio. The results of this analysis are discussed in Chapter-IV. The items of the five inventories were subjected to an item analy- sis. The item analysis, the items for which significant differences were found, their application, and their validation are discussed in Chapter V. The summary, conclusions, and recommendations are presented in Chapter VI. . ‘ I I. . CHAPTER 11 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE A little over 100 years ago the concept of parole for adult offenders was first being introduced to the United States. Parole originated in Europe, where it was known as conditional liberation, and was used as early as 1835 in Spain. However, the concept of parole was not generally accepted in the United States for some time, and it was not until just before the turn of the century that legislav tures began passing parole laws. Parole, used in a manner compar- able to present-day parole administration, had its beginning in this country in 1876 when the Elmira Reformatory in the state of New York was first opened. The statute establishing the Elmira Reforma- tory also provided for release by parole. By 1900 twenty states had accepted parole, and by 1910 thirty—two states and the federal govern— ment had adopted a parole system.1 Release figures for 1953 show that there were paroles during that year in every state.2 During 1953, 54.8 percent of all releases from penal institutions were by parole. 1 United States Department of Justice, The Attorney Gen- eral's Survey of Release Procedures, Vol. 4, ”Parole" (Washington: Government Printing-Office, 1939), pp. 1-21. United States Department of Justice, National Prisoner Statistics, No. 11 (Washington: Government Printing Office, July, 1954), p. 4. 3 Ibid., p. 4. 9 In Michigan, parole accounted for 81.9 percent of the total releases during the same year. ‘ The reader may wonder why Michigan's parole rate is so much above the national average. Actually, Michigan was seventh highest in the nation in percentage of total releases by parole in 1953. Washington was highest with 99.5 percent, followed by New Hampshire, Colorado, Ohio, Utah, and California, in that order. At the other extreme are South Carolina with 5.1 percent and Okla- home with 7.5 percent. There were only two states in the entire South that were at or above the national average.5 A major reason for such large differences in type of release from penal institutions is found in the type of sentence employed by the variOus states. In Michigan, for example, all prison sentences are of an indeterminate nature, with a minimum and a maximum, except life sentences and contempt of court sentences. Consequently, most of the releases are by parole. On the other hand, most of the southern states, and some of the others, make quite extensive use of the definite sentence, a rather short sentence much like-a jail term. In many of these states an individual convicted of an offense for which he would go to prison in Michigan is sentenced to a county work camp for a number of months or years. It is not the purpose of this paper to discuss the pros and cons of parole. The fact remains that 95 percent of the inmates are going to be released under one plan or the other, and the question is 4 The writer is indebted to Ayres Raymond, Director of Re- search for the Department of Corrections, and to Harold Kachelski, Assistant Director of Research, for these and all other figures re- lated to Michigan's parolees. United States Department of Justice, National Prisoner Statistics, op. cit., pp. 3—4. 10 whether or not it is better to release them outright or to have them under supervision for a period and to help them in their adjustment to society. The salient fact for this study is that parole is the most common form of release in the United States today, and as such there is need for studies that will aid paroling authorities in their decisions regarding the parolability of an individual. Since the concept of parole is so young in this country, it is not surprising, then, that the research in the prediction of parole out- come has all been within the past thirty years. This chapter will review the parole prediction research reported in the literature. The review will discuss all the studies which have appeared in that liter- ature, so far as the writer has been able to locate them, reporting parole prediction investigations or investigations in regard to factors ~re1ated to parole success or failure. In most of the studies that follow, a given factor is divided into subgroups or subclasses so that varying degrees of relationship to parole success or failure might be determined. In the factor of "misconduct during incarceration," for example, there might be just two subgroups; i.e., none and misconduct. On the other hand, a fac- tor such as "age at time of parole" might have several subgroups such as under 18, 18 to 20, 21 to 22, 23 to 25, 26 to 30, 31 to 35, 36 to 40, and over 40. The subgroupings are decided upon by the individual investigators and sometimes an author reports trying sev— eral groupings in an attempt to obtain the most significant results. In some cases, however, the author reports his findings without giving detailed information regarding his subgroups. 11 Previous Parole Prediction Research 6 Early studies. In 1923 Warner published the results of a study of the records on parole of ex-inmates of the Massachusetts Reformatory. He examined the records of 300 parolees who had been successful parolees, 300 who had violated parole, and 86 inmates who were not granted parole but were required to serve their maximum terms within the reformatory. Warner stated that an inmate was declared a success if he did not violate the conditions of his parole. Warner investigated the factors considered by the Board of Paroles in reaching its decisions, and the potential value of other items avail- able to the board but not utilized. There was a total of sixty-four factors investigated. His conclusions were that only recividism and offense are true criteria, and that the only item not then used which might have prognostic value was the alienist's report. Warner's study appears to be the first of its kind published and it created a great deal of interest. A few months after the ap- pearance of Warner's study, Hart7 published a criticism of the tech- niques used by Warner. Hart pointed out that no tests of significance had been applied in Warner's study. Hart then used Warner's data and tested the significance of relationship by computing the critical ratio of the difference between percentages, using the percentage for the factor and the average success rate. This was a technique which he admitted had not yet come into general use except among Sam B. Warner, "Factors Determining Parole From the M‘l‘ssatzhusetts Reformatory," Journal of Crimila Law and Criminol‘ 23X: 14:172-207. August, 1923. C _ Hornell Hart, "Predicting Parole Success," Journal of 3% Law and Criminolpfl,l4:405-4l4, November, 1923. —-H-h‘ do” . ‘7”“ 12 professional statisticians. Hart next made a table of factors signifi- cant at the 1 percent level, arranged in order of decreasing success rate. The average rate of success was 49.75 percent. The factors found by Hart to be significant at the 1 percent level, with the suc- cess rate of the individuals with that factor in their histories, follow: 1. Partly support unnamed persons ...... 88% 2. Convicted of assault and battery ...... 77% 3. Occupation "none" ................ 75% 4. No previous criminal record ......... 73% 5. Accidental offender ............... 72% 6. Steadily employed ................ 61% 7. "Responsible" and "normal" offender . . 58% 8. Men using cigarettes .............. 44% 9. Men convicted of fraud ...... , ..... 43% 10. "Bad" associates .......... ’ ...... 4 3% ' 11. Men convicted of larceny ........... 43% 12. Six or more misconducts ........... 41% 13. Served one or more jail terms ....... 40% 14. Men convicted of breaking and entering . 39% 15. Parents own property .............. 139% 16. Reported regular church attendance . . . . 38% 17. Reform school records ............. 36% 18. Uses drugs ..................... 22% 19. Mother drank ................... 20% 20. Father served jail sentence ......... 6% 21. Mother arrested or jailed ........... 0% Hart Suggested that: In order to profit by past experience as summarized in table I and as reinforced by other available data, so as in the future to parole as large a fraction as possible of the men who Will succeed and as small a fraction as possible of the men who ____ ___ __._ -L, .1 13 will violate their paroles, all of the information under the ques- tions which have been proved to be significant should be combined into a prognostic score for each man coming up for parole.8 Hart's suggestion of a prognostic score and his procedure of deter- mining the significance of the difference between the success rate of various factors and the average success rate, or in some cases the violation rate or failure rate, has been the basis for the procedure in many of the more recent investigations. A Wisconsin study. Apparently the articles by Warner and Hart served as impetus to additional studies. Although the first of the additional studies was not reported until four years after Hart's report, there were four investigations from four different localities reported within a few years of each other. The first of these was a report by Witmer9 dealing with Wisconsin subjects. Her sample included parolees from the Wisconsin State Prison and the Wisconsin State Reformatory. The prison sample included 214 successful parolees and 116 violators who were paroled between June of 1918 and February of 1921. The reformatory sample con- sisted of 229 successful parolees and 48 violators paroled between June of 1921 and January of 1925. The criterion of success, as de— scribed by Witmer, was abiding by the rules laid down by the parole board for conduct on parole, as indicated by not being returned to penal institutions during the parole period. Witmer found that 88 percent of the violations were within six months after parole. She also reported that half of those who remained on parole longer than 8 Ibid., p. 411. Helen Leland Witmer, "Some Factors in Success or Failure on Parole," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 182384-403, November, 1927. 14 six months before violating did so by committing new offenses. The majority of the violations within the six-month period were technical violations. All of Witmer's data were obtained from the parole files. No tests of significance were cited and, apparently, her conclusions of I "little," "slight," and ”greater" differences were the results of a comparison of the percentages of the violation rate of the subgroups Studied in each factor. Those factors that Witmer concluded showed little difference in rate of violation were: 1. Previous occupation. 2. Lengths of sentence. 3. Marks received in reformatory (demerit marks). The factors resulting in slight differences were: 1. Age at time of parole. The younger parolees had a higher rate of violation. . 2. Marital condition. The single parolees had a higher rate of violation. 3. Use of alcohol. Witmer found a slightly higher violation rate for those who were reported to use alcohol. I 4. Previous record. Those with no previous record had the highest parole success rate. 5. Offense causing commitment. Those individuals convicted 0f Offenses against property had the highest violation rate. Fifty percent of the failures had been convicted for burglary, forgery, and larceny. 6. Grades in school (reformatory only). Those with higher grades made slightly better parole records. 7. Type of community to which paroled (reformatory only). Farm placements resulted in high rate of violation, usually ab- scondance. Paroles to Milwaukee also resulted in a higher 15 rate of violation. Parolees in small cities made the best rec- ords. Greatest differences in violation rate were found in three factors, which were. 1. Superintendent's recommendation. A favorable recommenda- tion by the superintendent showed a ”high correlation" with parole success. 2. Occupation on parole (reformatory only). Those who had worked on farms previously and were returned to farms had a high Success rate. There were more successes for those who found em- ployment that was simpler than the employment previous to incarcer- ation. 3. Monthly earnings. The violation rate decreased as the monthly earnings increased. Witmer concluded her report with the finding that only 3 per- cent of the total number of parolees violated by committing new of- fenses and that very few of these were of a serious nature. Witmer was not attempting to establish a prediction technique and made no conclusions in that regard. A New Jersey study. A year later Bordenlo reported at study that he said was inspired by the articles of Warner and Hart. His subjects were 263 consecutive parolees, aged 17 to 35, from a re- formatory for young men. At the time of publication, Borden was Dire ctor of Statistics of the Department of Institutions and Agencies of New Jersey. However, the name of the institution is not given. Howard G. Borden, "Factors for Predicting Parole Suc- cess," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 19:328-336, No- Vember, 1928. 16 These subjects were paroled between July 1, 1923, and June 30, 1924. The data were collected in August of 1925. Borden describes the suc- cessful as those who were still succeeding on parole over a year after release or who had completed parole. The failures are de- scribed as those who were returned to an institution for either a mis- demeanor or a felony. Absconders are not included in either group and Borden makes no mention of technical violators. Borden's findings are reported in a table which shows "the coefficient of correlation with parole success using no correction for coarseness of distribution." He listed each factor and followed it with an item in parentheses, as listed below. However, he makes no further elaboration of the meaning of the items in the parentheses; consequently, it is assumed that the coefficients of correlation indi- cated for the factors listed in the direction of the items indicated by the parentheses. Support for this assumption is found in Borden's discussion of intelligence, which is quoted below. However, because Borden does not make his meaning clear, there would be serious question regarding any of the writer's conclusions based on Borden's study. Therefore, Borden's findings are reported, but no conclusions will be drawn from his study in the remainder of this chapter. Age at parole (Older) ....................... ..021 Nationality (Native white) ..................... .047 Mental age (Lower M.A.) ..................... .131 Intelligence (Lower) ........................ .164 Time lost for offenses in the institution (More) ..... .027 Conduct in institution (Better) ............... 1. . .030 Industrial rating (Lower) ..................... .138 1e by the commission of new crime as through technical violations .',._.__:__, ------‘---“rr-s—n 0‘ 34 5. Social type. The farm boy had the best rate of success with the criminal-by-accident quite closely second. The hobo and the ne'er-do-well had the highest violation rates. 6. Place of residence. The table showed almost no difference in the violation rate between those sent from Chicago and from small cities. However, the parolees from the open country and small towns had the best parole records. 7. Mobility in relation to record on parole. The transients have a high rate of violation. Furthermore, the rate of technical vio- lations is almost twice as high for this group as the average. 8. Type of neighborhood in which lived at time of offense. The high violation rate neighborhoods were the underworld, Hobo- hernia, and the rooming house. The lowest violation rate was for those who lived in a residential neighborhood. 9. Parole community. The rooming house placement resulted in extremely high violation while the farm and residential communities had the lowest rate of violation. 10. Statement of the prosecuting attorney. The recommenda- tion for leniency was most indicative of successful parole outcome While other recommendations resulted in little differences. 11. Acceptance of a lesser plea. In this sample those who we re allowed to plead guilty to a lesser offense had a much lower Viola-tion rate than those who were not granted that opportunity. 12. Type of sentence. In general the heavier sentences, three to twenty years and ten year to life and two or more consecu- tive Or concurrent sentences, exhibit lower rate of failure, while the Very Short sentences like the one to five years showed the highest me Of all. 13. Length of time served. In this study, mo re than a third of those individuals who had served three years or longer violated at. Sui... ‘l‘ 35 their paroles while the violation rate for those who served eleven months or less was only 13.7 percent. 14. Previous criminal record. Over half of the sample had no previous criminal record and the violation rate among the mem- bers of this group was considerably lower than the average for the total group. The highest violation rate was among those who had previous reformatory experience . 15. Working when arrested. Very little difference in viola- tion rates. The violation rate for those who The rate 16. Previous work record. worked regularly was exceptionally low, only 5.6 percent. of violation increased, as the regularity of employment decreased, up to 738.4 percent for those who had no work history. 17. Last assignment in institution. There were more than fifty subgroups, but in general the barbers had the lowest violation rate , with the office clerks, drivers, library clerks, and receiving and discharge boys next lowest. The higher than average rates were found among those unfit for regular assignment, with the furniture factory, the tailor shop, and the print sh0p next. 18. Punishment record in the institution. Those with no Puni8hment record were 5 percent below the average violation rate While those who had been punished, no matter how often, remained at approximately 8 percent above the violation rate. 19. Age when paroled. In this study it was the younger in- mates who succeeded on parole while the highest violation rate was, for 1:l'lose of 25 years or over. 20. Mental rating. In this study the inferior and very inferior had the highest rate of parole violation. The method of determining the 1Tl’ltelligence rating was not reported, and the ratings were listed 36 as A through E. Those with the superior rating had a considerably lower rate of parole violation. 21. Personality rating. There were seven classifications used in this study: normal; feeble-minded; ego; inadequate; emotional; sexual; neuropathic; and psychotic. It was interesting to note that only 14 of the 3,000 were listed as normal. As in the Burgess study, it was the emotionally unstable group that had the best parole rec- ord. They were 8 percent below the average while the feeble-minded, the ego, the sexual, the neuropathic, and the psychotic were well above the average rate of violation. 22. Psychiatric prognosis. Those with a favorable prognosis had a violation rate of 16.9 percent; those with a doubtful prognosis had a violation rate of 25.6 percent; and those for whom the prognosis was unfavorable violated at a rate of 33.4 percent. 23. Marital status. Seemed to have little bearing, in this study, upon outcome of parole. 24. Use of alcohol. The above comment is also apropos for this factor. Witmer had reported that the younger parolees violated at the highest rate, while Burgess found the youngest and oldest, at the ex- tremes, to be more successful. Tibbitts found that it was the younger inmates who succeeded on parole, while those of 25 years or more had the highest violation rate. The finding by Tibbitts of little relationship between marital status and parole outcome is in agreement with Vold, as far as single and married parolees are concerned. It will be remembered that Burgess and Witmer had found married parolees to be more successful than single parolees. Tibbitts' finding that those with inferior and very inferior intelligence had the highest rate of parole violation while those with 37 superior intelligence were the most successful is the first finding in this direction. It will be remembered that Burgess, the Gluecks, and Vold had all reported little or no relationship between intelligence and parole outcome. This is the first report where it was found that the Negro group showed a greater tendency to violate than any other group. The only other report mentioning race was that of Burgess. Although he reported that the Negro group was the second-largest group, next to native-born whites, he found that the success rate of the Negro group was almost average. Tibbitts' finding that those who had served longer had higher violation rates is support for Burgess' finding in that respect. How- ever, Vold and the Gluecks had found little or no relationship between time served and parole outcome. Tibbitts also devised an expectancy table for violation and non- violation on the basis of the results of the behavior of' his 3,000 sam- ple. However, he modified the method by scoring only those sub— groups where the violation rate was more than 5 percent above or 5 percent below the average violation rate. Those subgroups that were more than 5 percent below the average violation rate were given a "favorable" score, while those that were more than 5 per- cent higher than the average violation rate were scored "unfavorable." The final score consisted of the total favorable and unfavorable points, at a rate of one point for each factor. The table indicated the ex- pected percentage of successes or violators, according to past expe- rience, by score classes. All of the 3,000 parolees of the sample were scored by this method and an expectancy table prepared on that basis. All of the cases with 12 or more favorable points and no un- favorable points would be expected to succeed on parole, while 95.1 percent of those receiving between 10 and 12 favorable points with 38 no unfavorable points should be successful. On the other hand, only 45.4 percent of those receiving 9 unfavorable points and no favorable points would be expected to be successful, while none of those with 10 or more unfavorable points and no favorable points could be ex- pected to succeed. A study of federal prisoners. Sanders reports an investiga- tion to determine the validity of parole predictions.19 He studied the records of 5,912 federal inmates whose sentences were terminated between July 1, 1933, and June 30, 1934. He reported that 89.7 percent had been discharged as successful; that is, no warrants had been is sued against them. Sanders analyzed several factors by de- termining the significance of the difference in the success rate of subgroups and the average success rate. He reported his findings in terms of ”favorable" factors, "unfavorable" factors, and those for which no significant differences were found. The factors where the success rate was significantly greater than the average, or the "fa- vorable" factors were: 1. Age 45 or older at time of parole. 2. School through the fourth grade or less. 3. Convictions of the national prohibition act or other liquor laws; bankruptcy, national bank, and Federal Reserve acts. .4. Married. The factors found to be ”unfavorable" were: 1. Conviction of the narcotic and drug act. 2. Conviction for tranSporting a stolen car over state lines. \ l 1 Barker S. Sanders, "Testing Parole Prediction," Proceed- Mhe Sixty-Fifth Annual Congress of the American Prison As- W (New York: American Prison Association, 1935), pp. 222- 39 3. Postal burglary, larceny, and robbery. 4. Having served two years or longer. 5. Single or divorced. Sanders reported that there was no significant relationship be- tween the following and parole outcome: 1. Mental age. 2. Educational age. 3. English age. 4. Arithmetic age. Sanders joins Burgess, Vold, and the Gluecks, with his finding that intelligence, or mental age, as he reports it, has little or no re- lationship to parole outcome. His findings also support Burgess and Tibbitts in their conclusions that the longer an individual serves the more likely he is to violate parole. Vold and the Gluecks had reported a lack of relationship in their findings. Although Sanders' offenders are federal offenders, and consequently the nature of the offenses for . which they were convicted would be of a different nature than those of state inmates, the general tendency for high violation rates for those convicted of property offenses seems supported by Sanders' findings. The ”favorable" rating for married parolees, as scored by Sanders, is also in agreement with the findings of Witmer and Bur- 8858, who found that married parolees enjoy a high rate of success. Sanders prepared both the Burgess—type expectancy table and the Glueck-type expectancy table from his data. He scored a second sample, a sample of 2,833 parolees released between July 1, 1934, and December 31, 1934. He reported that statistical tests showed that the differences between expected violation and actual violations for the score classes, according to the Burgess system, could be attributed to chance. 40 Comparison of the mean success score, as determined by the Glueck method, for the successful parolees and that of the violators did result in a significant difference, but Sanders reported that the distribution of scores indicated that the method would be of little use for prediction. Two more Glueck studies. In 1934 the Gluecks published two more studies. The first of these was a study of 500 women released from the Massachusetts Reformatory for Women;0 Again, they con- ducted extensive follow-up investigations, after the women had been released at least five years, to establish the criterion of success or failure. Again, the Gluecks classified their subjects into three groups. There were 15.2 percent who were nondelinquent after release on parole and after expiration of sentence. The second group consisted of those who were delinquent on parole and who reverted to delinquency after expiration of sentence, if never on parole. There were 65.4 per- cent in the group. The third group behaved well under parole super- vision but reverted to unlawful conduct after parole. There were 19.4 percent in this group. Consequently, 84.8 percent had again become delinquent sometime within the five years of their release from the reformatory. This time, rather than using the coefficient of mean square Contingency, the Gluecks determined the degree of relationship of their factors to subsequent adjustment by the amount of deviation fl‘0‘!“ the average nonviolation rate. They studied 153 factors and discovered 15 that they concluded had a high relationship to future Conduct_ They found that the greatest proportion of those who were \ 20 Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck, Five Hundred Delinquent % (New York: Alfred A. Knopf. 1934), 537 PP' 41 later successful in their adjustment had been nondelinquent previous to their incarceration; made constructive use of leisure and had no bad habits; had made successful industrial adjustment; had a high scholarship rating previous to their incarceration; were more stead-— ily employed; had no mental disturbances; showed competence as homemakers; had been exposed to good neighborhood influences within a year previous to their commitment; lived in homes that were in good physical condition within a year previous to their commitment; showed no retardation in school; did not attend church; were living with relatives or foster-parents rather than their own parents pre- vious to commitment; had good work habits; met their economic re- sponsibilities; and had good neighborhood influences during their child- hood. In keeping with their reasoning that it is best to select a few of the most highly related factors for predictive purposes, the Gluecks selected six factors to aid parole authorities in reaching a decision regarding parole. The Gluecks pointed out that the six factors se— lected from the fifteen that showed a high relationship were chosen on the basis of the availability of data in the records and because the intercorrelations of the factors indicated that the six would be the most representative of several closely related factors. The factors the Gluecks selected as indicative of successful parole adjustment were: 1. Retardation in school. Of those who were not retarded in school, 42.5 percent did not recidivate, while only 20.3 percent of those who were retarded one or more years did not recidivate. Of those with no schooling, the success rate was only 13.3 percent. 2. Neighborhood influences within a year of commitment. Of those who lived in neighborhoods where the influences were described as good, 45.5 percent were successful, as compared with only 27.4 42 percent where the influences were described as fair and 21.3 per- cent where they were poor. 3. Steadiness of employment. Those who were regularly em- ployed prior to incarceration were more successful than those who were fairly regularly or irregularly employed and those who had never worked. 4. Economic re3ponsibility. Those who met their responsi- bilities were much more successful than those who did not. 5. Mental abnormality. The success rate of those with no abnormality was highest at 29.2 percent. It was 22.8 percent for the psychopathic, psychoneurotic, and neurasth-enic; 19.2 percent for the‘epileptic, congenital syphilitic, drug addict; 9.0 percent for the alcoholic deteriorate; and 0 percent for the psychotic. 6. Kind of worker in reformatory. Those rated as good had a success rate of 32.7 percent as compared to 18.0 percent for those rated fair and 15.4 percent for those rated poor. The Gluecks indicated that the first five of these factors could be used as a prognostic instrument by the courts with the addition of the sixth factor when the instrument was to be used by the parole aAlthorities. Again, as in their previous study, the factors were weighted by adding the percentages of the subgroup success rates f°r each of the subgroups in which the individual fell on each factor t° Obtain a score, only this time it was a "success score." This score was interpreted in terms of the expected percentage of non- demncluent behavior, delinquent behavior during parole period, anal delinQuent behavior after parole period but not during that period. For example, 100 percent-nondelinquency would be expected for those individuals with total success scores of over 200 while 100 percent delincluency during parole would be expected of those scoring under 100 Q 43 The other Glueck study to appear in print during 1934 was a study of one thousand juvenile delinquents. These delinquents had been referred from the Boston Juvenile Court to the Judge Baker Foundation, a child-guidance clinic. The mean age was 13 years, 5 months, and all had been interviewed and studied in the clinic prior to 1922. Again, a field investigation follow-up was conducted after five years to determine the real degree of success of failure in adjustment. The judgment of recividism was based on arrests on i convictions for serious offenses and minor offenses, and on knowl— Also included was deser- ‘ | edge of offenses not known to the police. tion or dishonorable discharge from the armed forces. The violation rate was 88.2 percent. This time the relationship of the factors to subsequent adjust- ment was determined by the coefficients of mean square contingency. Those factors that were found by the Gluecks to have the highest re- lationship to subsequent adjustment and, consequently, were included in a table for the prediction of delinquent behavior were the following: 1. Discipline by father. Discipline was described as sound, fair, or unsound. The lowest recividism rate was for the sound Subgroup, while the unsound had the highest rate of recidivism. 2. Lack of school retardation. The normal and advanced had the lowest recividism rate. For those retarded one or two years, the rate was higher, while those retarded three or more years had the highest rate of failure. 3. Conduct in school. There were two subgroups. Those for whom no misconduct in school had been reported had a recidivist rate of 72.4 percent, while those reported for truancy or other mis- Conduct had a rate of 91.3 percent. 2 1' l Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck, One Thousand Juvenile De- W (Cambridge: Harvard University,Press, 1934), 341 pp. 44 4. Age at first misbehavior. The rate of recividism went progressively higher as the age decreased. 5. Length of time between onset of delinquency and clinical examination. The rate of recividism was progressively better as length of time became smaller. The Gluecks prepared tables giving the percentages of subse- quent delinquency found among their 1,000 delinquents for each of the subgroups for the predictive factors. The subgroup scores were given in terms of the recidivism rate and the sum of an individual's subgroup scores, in this case, would be a "recidivism score." The exPectancy table resulting from scoring the 1,000 juvenile delinquents indicated that an individual scoring in the best score class would be exPected to be delinquent in 50 percent of the cases while an indi- vidual in the lowest score class would be expected to be delinquent in 92.6 percent of the cases. These two Glueck studies have been included in this review because they indicate the extensiveness of the investigations conducted by the Gluecks and because they further illustrate the Glueck method 01' prediction. Even though the sample populations for these two stud- ies are all women or juveniles, at least one of the findings can be coITI'leared with the other studies reviewed thus far. That is the find- ing that retardation in school is indicative of recividism in delin- quency. If school retardation can be assumed to indicate inferior intelligence, then it would appear that these two studies are, some- What at least, in support of Tibbitts' finding that the intellectually infe rior individuals had a higher violation rate than the other clas- sifications of intelligence. The Attorney General's report. For several years the United S tates Attorney General's office gathered information regarding parole 45 outcome from seventy-five penal institutions located in forty-two states and the District of Columbia, which included the case history records of approximately 95,000 parolees. The cases included all persons of the reporting institutions whose parole began and terminated within the period of January 1, 1928, to December 31, 1935. This study was part of a survey of release procedures, which resulted in a five-volume report published in 1939.22 It was the purpose of this part of the study to determine from an analysis of parole outcome figures which set of characteristics appear to be favorably associated with parole success or failure. A case was considered a failure on parole whenever the official records disclosed that the parolee had violated the terms of release. He was considered a violator whether or not his parole had been revoked. The term "success" means reports of violations were not found in the parolee's record. The Chi—square test of significance was used to determine whether or not a relationship was significant. This sig- nificance was determined for the nation as a whole, and also was de- terulined for each institution, individually, and reported in tables as ”fa-Vorable," "unfavorable," or "neutral." Conclusions were based on the combined figures for the country as a whole. The tables were presented so comparisons for various parts of the country, and other comparisons could be made, as needed. Although the Attorney General's survey is not a predictive Study and does not include an attempt, to develop a prediction tech- nique, it is being included with this review of literature related to Parole prediction so that the Attorney General's parole outcome \— 22 Department of Justice, United States Attorney General's My of Release Procedures, Volume IV, "Parole" (Washington: o"'ernment Printing Office, 1939). 46 analyses can be compared with the analyses reported in the predic- tion studies. The characteristics studied and a brief statement of the conclusions drawn in the study follow: 1. Race. These statistics on parole violations indicate that ‘as a race Negroes have not made as good a record as whites. The data on the other races were so limited that violation statistics based on them would have been of little value. I 2. Marital status. The findings indicate that in the country as a whole, single persons were worse risks for parole than mar- ried persons. 3. Number of dependents. The findings indicate that parolees with dependents make a better record on parole than those without dependents. 4. Recividism. Only 18 percent of the first offenders violated the terms of parole, while 37 percent of the recidivists violated pa- role. Twenty‘four percent of the recidivists committed new offenses while on parole as contrasted with only 10 percent of the first of- fenders. 5. Age at‘ time of first arrest. Criminals who first came into conflict with the law before they had reached the age of 18 years are as a rule poor risks for parole. On the other hand, criminals who were first arrested after having passed 22 years of age make comparatively good records on parole. 6. Nature of offense committed. The results of the analysis present some evidence in support of the view that offenders convicted of robbery, burglary, larceny, forgery, and counterfeiting make poorer records on parole than those convicted of criminal homicide, assault, sex offenses, and liquor law violations. 47 7. Number of associates participating in crime committed. The combined statistics show that the factor of number of associates is not significantly associated with parole outcome. 8. Type and length of sentence imposed. Short-sentence pris- oners made better records on parole than those who had been given long sentences. 9. Employment. Twenty-three percent of the parolees who were employed at the time of their arrest violated parole as con- trasted to 36 percent of the parolees who were not employed. 10. Size of community into which parolee was released. While 37 percent of the parolees residing in cities of more than 100,000 population violated their paroles, only 17 percent of those re- leased into communities with less than 2,500 population were recorded as violators. Of the former, 25 percent committed new offenses as contrasted with only 10 percent of the latter. 11. Nativity. Native-born persons made poorer records on parole than did foreign-born parolees. 12. Age at time of sentence. Offenders who started serving their sentences before they were 25 years old had more unsatisfactory parole records than offenders who began serving their sentences after they had reached the age of 25. On the ‘other hand, offenders who were over 35 at the time they were sentenced seemed to have been better risks for parole than persons who. were under 35. 13. Conduct in prison. The findings showed that parolees Who had behaved well in prison made better records on parole than ParOIees who had breached prison rules. Admission or denial of guilt at time of arrest. Not a 14. Significant factor as far as subsequent parole outcome is concerned. 15. Parole supervisor's prognosis of parolee. Supervisor's proghoses formed on the basis of contacts with the parolee early in 48 the period of supervision were largely borne out by the parolee's final record on parole. 16. Occupation on parole compared with occupation prior to crime. The findings indicate that there was no significant relation- ship to parole outcome. 17. Home conditions of parolee. The parolee's adjustment to community life is generally assisted if he has a wife and family to turn to when released from prison. On the other hand, the prob- lem of social adjustment often becomes very difficult if the parolee must live in a rooming or a boarding house upon release. The Attorney General's finding that as a race the Negroes have not made as good a record as the whites is in keeping with Tibbitts' findings in this regard, while Burgess had indicated no relationship between race and parole outcome. The fact that married parolees have proved to be good parole risks, according to this study, supports'the findings of Witmer: Burgess, and Sanders in this regard. It will be remembered that Burgess and Tibbitts had reported finding that the offender who was without associates had the highest violation rate, while Vold and the Gluecks had reported that the num- her 01’ associates showed very little relationship to parole outcome. The Attorney General's report also states finding that the number 0f associates is not significantly associated with parole outcome. An Alabama study. Another study with the purpose of analyz- mg Various factors related to parole success or failure was a study 2 of Alabama parolees by Graham. 3 Although Graham does not attempt \ 23 b Mary Ruth Graham, These Came Back (University, Ala- ama: University of Alabama, 1946), 134 pp. - .' ~e, 4...... 49 to devise a predictive technique, she does point out that her findings would assist the parole board in its task of defining and crystallizing criteria to be used in the selection of prisoners for release on pa- role. This study covers all parolees released on parole in Alabama from September, 1939, to August, 1944. All the information was ob- tained from the individual parole files and a total of 4,612 parolees were included. The reported violation rate was 10.9 percent. A parolee was described as successful if discharged from parole, dis- Charged from supervision, or under active supervision. The violators had been convicted of new offenses or were returned to prison for 1ihe violation of some technical phase of parole. As in other studies, the various factors were studied in terms of subgroups. Graham ob- tained what she calls an "index of significance" which appears to be the critical ratio of the differences between two percentages. She computed the ratio for ..the differences between the subgroup violation rate and the average violation rate. She stated that an index of less than 1.00 indicates lack of significance, from 1.00 to 2.00, possible significance, from 2.00 to 3.00, probable significance, and over 3.00, undoubted significance. A list of the factors analyzed and'a brief discussion of Graham's conclusions for each follows: 1. Race. This analysis revealed that Negro parolees have proved slightly better performers on parole than white Parolees. Howeve r, neither difference was significant. 2. Sex. The rate of success for women was significantly above the average while that for men was not significantly different from the average. 3. Race by sex. Negro women had the only significant devi- ation from the average success rate. They were significantly more successful. 50 4. Age at time of crime. Parolees who were under 22 years old at the time of arrest had significantly high violation rates while those who were over 31 had significantly high rates of success. 5. Age at time of parole. Parolees of age 21 and less at time of parole had significantly high violation rates while those over 35 were significantly successful. 6. Education at time of crime. The amount of education was not of marked consequence in terms of adjustment on parole. 7. Mental ability (white population only). The results indicated no particular significance relative to intelligence. 8._ Physical condition at time of parole. Again, no significant differences were found. 9. Venereal disease status at time of parole. The findings indicated that venereal disease is not a criterion by which to deny or grant parole. 10. Urban or rural residence at time of crime. Rural resi- dence showed an undoubted relationship to success and urban showed an equally clear relationship to failure. 11. Marital status. Married parolees succeeded at a very high rate on parole. By contrast, single and divorced parolees were much more likely to violate parole. 12. Number of children at time of parole. Parolees with two or more children were very good risks, the rate of success increas- ing .with the number of children. 13. Status of parents at time of parole. The only significant finding was the high rate of success for the category "Father unknown, mother living." 14. Number of siblings. None of the conclusions were signifi- cant . 51 15. Occupation of father at time of parde. The parolees whose fathers were farmers proved to be the best risks. The pro- fessional background portends average success on parole. All other occupations of fathers disclosed an association to failure on parole. The highest index of significance for failure was for those whose fathers were recorded as skilled laborers. 16. Occupation of parolee at time of crime. Farmers and farm workers prior to incarceration and domestic servant and house- keepers showed a definite tendency toward success, while skilled laborers and textile workers showed a like tendency toward failure. 17. Length of tenure on last job at time of crime. The only significant finding was. a. marked tendency toward failure for those who had been employed six months or less. 18. Real property ownership at time of parole. A very sig- nificant rate of success was found for those who own real prOperty. 19. Personal property ownership at time of parole. An ex- tremely high rate of success was found for those owning personal prOperty. 20. Reform school record. Parolees who had attended reform school violated parole at a rate three times that of other parolees, a marked significance. 21. Previous misdemeanors. Parolees with no previous mis- demeanor convictions are more likely to succeed than the average parolee. 22. Previous felonies. Parolees convicted of one previous felony showed a higher failure rate than did the group as a whole. However, parolees convicted of two previous felonies showed a ten- dency to succeed, a favorable index of 1.55. Parolees with three or , more previous felony convictions failed at a very high rate. 52 23. Instant offense. Violators against the public (mainly liquor law violators) and those convicted of manslaughter proved themselves likely to succeed on parole, while offenders guilty of crimes against property were very prone to resume their criminal activities upon release. 24. Associates in crime. In this study the factor of asso- ciates in crime had no significance in terms of parole outcome. 25. Single or plural instant sentences. Those with more than one sentence had a significantly higher rate of violation. 26. Length of sentence. Those serving sentences of two years or less and those serving very long sentences had pr0portionately greater success than those serving from two years through fifty years. 27. Length of time served. The success rate of the parolees who had served two years or less was significantly higher than the average success rate, while that of those who had served over two years was significantly below the average success rate. 28. Prison behavior. Parolees with no demotions violated parole much less frequently than the average. On the other hand, one or more demotions portend a higher rate of failure than the average. _ 29. Number of escapes. Parolees with escape records are quite likely to fail on parole. Graham's finding that the Negro parolees had a better parole record than white parolees is in contradiction to the findings of Tib- bitts and the Attorney General's report. However, Graham points out that neither difference was significant, which was the conclusion reached by Burgess in his study. Two studies, Vold and the Gluecks, had found that age at the time of crime was not related to parole outcome. However, the Attorney General's report had indicated that younger offenders made 53 poor parole records. Graham's finding in this regard supports the Attorney General's findings. There has been disagreement regarding the relationship of age at time of parole to subsequent parole outcome. Tibbitts had re- ported finding that the younger parolees were more successful. Bur- gess had reported the youngest and oldest more successful than all others. Graham's findings are in support of Witmer, in that it was the parolees 21 and less who had high rates of violation while the older parolees were more successful. Another Illinois study. In 1948 Michael Hakeem released a report of a follow-up study of 1,108 parolees for whom parole success or failure had been predicted.24 Hakeem's 1,108 subjects included all of the individuals paroled from an Illinois institution (not named) during the two-year period including 1939 and 1940. The parole files were studied six years later, allowing at least three years of parole for each subject. Success was determined by discharge from parole. The rate of violation was found to be 27.7 percent. Burgess-type predictions had been made for the group at the time of parole. Hakeem stated that the prediction. table used was a modification of the Burgess table. The prediction table had been revised after a refinement study involving 9,729 cases paroled between 1925 and 1935. This refinement was not otherwise reported in the literature. Hakeem did not give the particulars of the refinement study, but stated that the analysis of the 9,729 cases had indicated that three of the original Burgess factors should be excluded from the expectancy 4 Michael Hakeem, ”The Validity of the Burgess Method of Parole Prediction," American Journal of Sociology, 532374-386, March, 1948. 54 table while eight new factors should be added. The three factors excluded were: 1. County from which committed. 2. Statement of judge or prosecutor. 3. Acceptance of a lesser plea. The eight factors that were added were: 1. Extent of contact with relatives or friends. Employment at time of offense. Job in prison at time of parole hearing. Indulgence in drink. Venereal infection. Size of parole community. Type of neighborhood to which paroled. mflO‘U‘ltBUDN Type of job on release. No further elaboration of the factors was reported and sub- group variations were not indicated. Hakeem divided his subjects into two groups, one composed of all the individuals paroled during 1939 and the other composed of all the individuals paroled during 1940. He then compared the exPected rate of violation with the ac- tual rate of violation for the various score-classes in the expectancy table. He reasoned that the total violation rates of the group on WhiCh the predictions were based and the group on Which the pre— dictions were made would have to be made comparable before the comparisons would be meaningful. Consequently, he divided the ex- PeCted total violation rate by the actual total violation rate and multiplied the quotient by the uncorrected percentage of violators in each score-class to obtain what would have been the expected rate baSed on the actual rate of violation. Although no tests of signifi- cance were reported, Hakeem concluded the corrected differences inchcated "remarkable accuracy.” In comparing the predictions and 55 outcomes for the two groups, Hakeem said, "A comparison of the accuracy of the prediction for cases paroled in 1939 with that for cases paroled in 1940 shows no really significant differences." Another Wisconsin study. An additional study of Wisconsin parolees was reported in 1949. This was a study by Alfred C. Schnur of 1,762 inmates paroled from the Wisconsin State Prison 25 between January 1, 1936, and December 31, 1941. Schnur found that 82.5 percent had successfully completed their paroles. He stated that a parolee was considered a success if he was not arrested and convicted for an offense committed with two years after release, which resulted in a sentence of at least six months on probation or commit- ment to an institution. His study was an analysis of some of the factors related to parole success or failure for the purpose of devis- ing a predictive instrument. Schnur stated that the differences re- ported were analyzed by the conventional tests of significance and were significant at the 5 percent level or better. The factors stud— ied and a summary of the findings follow: 1. Previous record. Ninety-five percent of those who had no previous criminal history were successful. There was a decreasing rate of success down to 60 percent for those with three or more previous convictions. 2. Amount of time spent under legal supervision from time of birth up to present sentence. There was a decline from 84 per- cent for those who had been under previous supervision for one year to 41 percent for those of ten years or more. 2 5 Alfred C. Schnur, "Predicting Parole Outcome," Focus, 28:70-75, May, 1949. ' 56 3. Type of offense. Adulterers and bigamists had a success rate of 96 percent. Murderers not also engaged in a property crime had a 94 percent rate compared with 89 percent for those so engaged. Ninety-three percent of the embezzlers were successful, as were 92 percent of the sex offenders. Forgers and confidence game offenders had the lowest rate of success. 4. Age at first arrest. Of those who were over age 35 when first arrested, 92 percent were successful compared with 70 percent for those who were under age 14. Schnur gave no further data on this point. 5. Misconduct citations. Eighty-six percent of the successful parolees had never been cited for misconduct. The success rate decreased to 65 percent for those who had been cited fifteen or more times. 6. Age at leaving school. The older the individual was when he left school, the more chance there was that he would commit a new crime. This factor and the conclusion reached by Schnur did not seem meaningful to the writer, nor did further study of Schnur's discussion of this point. As a consequence, the writer elected to quote Schnur‘s entire discussion of this point. The quotation follows: When we turn to success and failure after release, the next factor in significance is age at leaving school. The older a man is when he leaves school the greater are his chances of committing a new crime. This surprising statement may be . explained this way. A school is operated at the prison. It is observed that the less education a man has when he comes to prison, the more likely he is to go to school in prison. Men who make use of the prison school have better than average chances of leading lawful lives. 7. Age at time of offense. The older the individual was when he came to prison, the more chance there was that he would succeed on parole. 57 8. Age at parole. The same conclusion as for age at time of offense. 9. Marital status. The widowed, separated, and married pa- rolees succeeded at a rate above the average, while single parolees were less than the average. 10. Race. Negro and foreign-born succeeded at a better than average rate. 11. Venereal infection. The venereally diseased had less chance of being successful than the average. 12. Number of accomplices. The parolee who had accom- plices had a better chance of being successful than those with no accomplices. Schnur reported some factors that did not result in signifi- cant differences. Those were: parole period; prior education; in- telligence; use of alcohol; and whether prior residence was rural or urban. Schnur further complicates the picture in regard to race by reporting that the Negro succeeded at a better than average rate. Graham and Burgess had reported finding no significant differences in rate of success, while Tibbitts and the Attorney General had re- ported the opposite finding; namely, that Negroes had a higher vio- lation rate. Schnur's findings in regard to a high success rate for mar- ried parolees is in keeping with the general trend in this regard, but the fact that separated parolees also have a high success rate is not in keeping with previous findings. Although the study was undertaken to provide a basis for a predictive instrument, Schnur concluded by saying that construction of a predictive instrument would not be attempted until a special analysis of each of the factors for which significant differences had 58 Further perusal of the literature been found could be completed. failed to disclose additional reports in this regard. The most recent study. The latest report in the field of pa- role prediction is a book by Lloyd E. Ohlin, a research sociologist in the Illinois Division of Correction. Ohlin's work is a further refinement of the Burgess method of parole prediction. He analyzed twenty-seven factors by using the cases of 1,000 parolees from the Joliet-Stateville and Menard Divisions of the Illinois State Peniten- tiary System. The thousand cases had been paroled at least five years prior to the study, and success was determined by discharge from parole. The twenty-seven factors were divided into subgroups and the violation rate for each subgroup determined by dividing the number of violators by the total number in the group. Four statistical tests were then used to determine which of the subgroups should be se- lected as predictive items. These were tests of the reliability of the data; a critical ratio test of significance; a Q coefficient for the measurement of the association of attributes; and a measure of the predictive efficiency. , Fifteen of the twenty-seven factors did not have subgroups that adequately met the statistical tests imposed, according to Ohlin, and were not retained for use in the prediction table. The subgroups of the remaining twelve factors were marked with l, 0, or X, to indicate whether the subgroup was rated as The factors are favorable, neutral, or unfavorable predictive item. listed below with the subgroup ratings of each: 26 Lloyd E. Ohlin, Selection for Parole (New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1951) . 59 1. Type of offense. Favorable: homicide and assault, and sex offenses. Neutral: robbery, larceny and stolen property, for- gery and fraud, and miscellaneous. Unfavorable: burglary. 2. Sentence. Favorable: all definite sentences. All other sentences are neutral. 3. Type of offender. Favorable: first offender. Neutral: technical first, occasional, and juvenile recidivist. Unfavorable: recidivist and habitual. 4. Home status. Favorable: superior. All others were neutral. 5. Family interest. Favorable: very active. Neutral: ac- tive, sustained, and passive. Unfavorable: no family interest. 6. Social type. Favorable: erring citizen, marginally delin- quent, ”farmer," socially inadequate. Neutral: ne'er-do-well, and sex deviant. Unfavorable: floater, socially maladjusted, drunkard, and drug addict. 7. Prior work record. Favorable: regular. All others were neutral. 8. Community from which committed. Favorable: none. Neutral: urban and rural. Unfavorable: transient. 9. Parole job. Favorable: none. Neutral: adequate and no job. Unfavorable: inadequate. 10. Number of associates. Favorable: three or more. Neutral: none, and one or two. Unfavorable: there was no subgroup marked unfavorable. 11. Personality. Favorable: normal (no gross defects). Neutral: inadequate, unstable, egocentric, gross personality defects, no record. There were no subgroups marked unfavorable. 6O 12. Psychiatric prognosis. Favorable: favorable. Neutral: problematic, doubtful, guarded, unfavorable, and no record. There were no subgroups marked unfavorable. As stated previously, Ohlin excluded fifteen factors after anal- ysis because the subclasses lacked a sufficient degree of association with outcome, or statistical significance, or reliability, or stability. Ohlin then added that he also dropped out factors which were found to be highly correlated with other factors which better met the statis- tical tests of inclusion. Unfortunately, Ohlin does not indicate which factors did not have subgroups that adequately met the statistical tests imposed and which were dropped because of high correlation with other factors which were included in the prediction tables. Ohlin states that each factor was tested several times with various combinations of subgroups before being finally excluded. The follow- ing list gives the factors that were excluded: 1. Time served. Age. Nationality and racial origin. Criminal record. Punishment record. Marital status. Working at time of offense. Last institutional as signment . $004001th Criminal mobility. ,— 0 Neighborhood at offense. 11. Use of alcohol. 12. Venereal infection. 13. Parole community. 14. Parole neighborhood. 15. Mental rating. 61 Each parolee was given one favorable point for every favor- able subgroup in which he fell, one unfavorable point for every un- favorable subgroup, and zero for every neutral subgroup. The final score is the number of unfavorable points subtracted from the fa- vorable points. Ohlin states that the exPerience table is constructed by listing the violation rates for the score groups. The violation rate is computed by dividing the number of violators by the total number of persons in the group. These violation rates give the per- centage of persons who have violated parole within each score group and serve to indicate the violation rate that can be expected for similar groups in the future. A different approach. A different approach to the problem of parole prediction was presented by Ferris Laune in 1936.27 Remind- ing the reader that penal officials had often remarked, in one way or another, that an intelligent inmate's "hunch" about the probable parole success or failure of other inmates was usually a pretty ac- curate guide, Laune attempted to utilize inmate "hunches" in'the development of a predictive instrument. Laune obtained the coopera- tion of two long-term inmates who had college degrees and who pos- sessed high intelligence. Each listed one hundred inmates for whom he believed he could hazard an intelligent guess as to the probability of success on parole and with whom he believed his collaborator was also acquainted. Each of the inmates‘made a guess, on a lOO-point scale, of the probability of success for the 150 mutually known sub- jects. Then a period of from one hour to two hours daily for nearly two months was Spent while the two inmates attempted to justify 27 Ferris F. Laune, PredictinLCriminality (Evanston: North- western University Press, 1936). 62 their "hunches" about each of the subjects. A careful stenographic record was kept of these discussions so that the factors underlying the "hunches" could be isolated and identified. A. total of forty-two "hunch" factors were identified in this manner. The factors were identified by Laune but are not defined here because they were not empirical findings. The inmates' "hunches" of the factors that would aid in predicting parole success or failure, in addition to including statements regarding the usual reference to pre- vious record, steadiness of employment, and others found by the em- pirical studies, also suggested that such factors as an excessive in- terest in clothes, a craving for gay life, shrewdness, and conceit, among others, would identify the prospective successful or nonsuc— cessful parolees. Laune then went on to develOp a 1,701-item questionnaire based on the "hunches" mentioned above and designed to objectively mea- sure the presence or absence of the factors in an individual. A final questionnaire of 161 items was developed which exhibited a correla- tion of .62 with Burgess-type scores. Laune's report is only con- cerned with the development of the scale and does not contain a report of actual administration of the scale to a group of parolees. Because this was the only study reported in the literature that was of somewhat the same nature as the present study, the writer was particularly interested in learning more of any use that had been made of Laune's scale. However, a search of the literature failed to disclose reports of such application. Correspondence with the author disclosed that he had left the correctional field shortly after writing his book and that he had not made further application of his scale. He further stated that he did not think his scale had been of- ficially used in any institution or parole system. 63 Comfirison of factors. Table I has been prepared as a means of summarizing the most important findings of the various investiga- tions of parole outcome. There were several factors on which the findings were in agreement. Ten of the studies investigated the re- lationship of previous record to parole outcome. All of them reported that the group of parolees who had no previous criminal history have a high rate of success on parole. These same investigators reported, also, that the group of parolees who had been sentenced previously had a low rate of success on parole. Another factor on which there was complete agreement was that those parolees who had been steadily employed prior to incar- ceration also had a high rate of parole success. The absence of institutional misconduct also seems to be re- lated to parole success, although two investigators, Ohlin and Witmer, reported finding no relationship. Most of the investigations reported that younger parolees were more likely to violate parole than were older parolees, if age at first arrest, age at crime, and age at parole can be grouped together in this consideration. However, two of the investigators reported find- ings of no relationship, and Burgess found that the younger and older Parolees tend to be more successful than those in between. i In general, the married parolee is regarded as a good parole risk, according to six of the investigations. However, Vold, Tibbitts, and Ohlin found little or no relationship between marital status and pa-I‘OIe outcome._ Those who were convicted of property offenses such as for- gery, fraud, breaking and entering, burglary, and larceny were gen- erally found to have higher violation rates than offenders for other types of crime. However, Ohlin found this true of burglary only, while the relationship of the others to parole outcome was not 64 TABLE I A. SUMMARY OF THE FINDINGS OF SEVERAL INVESTIGATORS 1N REGARD TO THE RELATIONSHIP OF VARIOUS FACTORS TO PAROLE OUTCOME Investigators Aspect of Factor for Which t' ' ' t d Th Rela 10nSh1p IS Repor e Hart Witmer Burgess e Gluecks 1. Length of sentence 1 2 (shorter) ........... O - 2. Misconduct (absence of) + 0 + + 3. Age at first arrest (younger) ........... - 4. Age at crime (younger). . 0 4 5. Age at parole (younger) - ? 6. Marital status (married). + + 7. Previous record (none). . + + + + 8. Previous record (repeated) ........... - - - - 9. Type of offense (property) ........... - - - 0 10. Social type (farmer) + + 11. Intelligence (inferior) . . . O 0 12.. Psychiatric (favorable) . . + + + 13. Associates (none) ..... - 0 14. Prior work record (regular) ........... + + + 15. Time served (longer) . . . - 0 16. Race (Negro) ........ O 0 indicates a finding of little or no relationship. - indicates a high relationship to parole violation. + indicates a high relationship to parole success. TABLE 1 (Continued) 65 Investigators Atto rney G raham Vold Tibbitts Sanders Schnur Ohlin General 3 4 o - + '2 o + + + + + 0 o - - - + .. .. o o + + + + o + + + + + + -5 o + + + + 0 - o o o + + 0 - o o - o + + + + + 0 _ - - o - - o + \ 7 both extremes were favorable. 6 Excluding burglary. Burglary only. 66 significant, and Vold found burglary was the offense, of that group, whose perpetrators would most likely not violate more than the av- erage, while the usual findings resulted for the other offenses in that group. In those investigations where a farm background was reported, all findings agreed in that the individual with a farm background had a better than average success rate. In those investigations that reported on the availability of psy- chiatric reports, there was agreement in that those individuals for whom a favorable report was made had a high rate of parole success. Almost all of the investigators reporting findings in regard to the relationship of intelligence to parole outcome reported little or no relationship. However, Tibbitts found that the intellectually infe- rior parolees had a low rate of parole success. Eight investigators reported findirgs in regard to the number of associates. Five of them found that there was no relationship be— tween number of associates and parole outcome. Schnur, Tibbitts, and Burgess found a high rate of violation for the offender who was alone. None of the investigators found that having associates was related to parole failure. On the remaining factors, for which findings were reported by several investigators, there is a lack of agreement in regard to relationship to parole outcome. One of these concerns the findings in regard to the Negro parolee. The Attorney General and Tibbitts had found that Negroes violated at a rate greater than the average. However, Schnur found that they had a better than average rate of success. On the other hand, Ohlin, Graham, and Burgess found there was no significant difference between the success rate of Negroes and the average success rate. 67 The relationship of length of time served to parole outcome has also been reported by several investigators. Three of the inves- tigators found that there was no relationship, while four others found that the longer an individual served the more likely he was to violate parole. Seven reporters investigated the relationship of length of sen- tence to parole outcome. Three of them found there was no rela- tionship, while two, Burgess and Tibbitts, reported higher violation rates for those with shorter sentences. The Attorney General found, on the other hand, that those with shorter sentences were more suc- cessful on parole. Graham reported that those with short sentences and those with very long sentences were more successful than the average parolee. As far as the writer knows, there is and has been only one state that includes with the material available to the parole board a predictidn of parole success or failure based on expectancy resulting from past experience, and that is Illinois. One of the reasons for this maybe that while a considerable amount of work has been done to develop prediction techniques, there has been a noticeable lack of validation of the techniques, in terms of actual outcome on parole. While investigators have been eager to devise methods and study fac- tors, little has been done to see if predictions coincide with actual behavior. Three studies of this nature have already been discussed, those of Vold, Sanders, and Hakeem. Their findings would indicate that the expected results agreed quite closely with the actual results. These findings suggest that the prediction methods investigated were reasonably accurate. However, these are the only studies that have been reported of investigations made to determine how well predic- tions coincide with actual outcome, a situation which led Monachesi to state: 68 The paucity of data for the empirical verification of pre- dictions based upon the relational patterns of personal and social factors of offenders and conduct has tended to impede the appli- cation of prediction techniques to the practical and everyday work of judicial and correctional authorities. 2 In the most recent book by the Gluecks, 9 they reveal that they are in the process of testing all of their prediction tables. They are also engaged in research in which well over two hundred factors in the background and make-up of offenders are being considered. Summary The literature pertinent to parole prediction and parole out- come reveals that numerous factors have been analyzed by various investigators to determine their relationship to parole outcome, and some authors have devised prediction ‘methods based on such analysis. This review was intended to inform the reader of the factors which have been analyzed and that have served as a basis for predictive techniques thus far devised. A great many factors have been analyzed to determine the re- lationship to parole outcome for several different populations. Study of the individual factors reveals several for which there was com- plete or almost complete agreement. A brief summary of the find- ings in regard to the factors most frequently investigated follows: 2 8 Elio D. Monachesi, "American Studies with Prediction of Recividism," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 41:268-289, September , 1950 . 2 9 Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck, Criminal Careers in Retro- spect (New York: The Commonwealth Fund, 1943), p. 218. 69 1. All investigations found a high rate of success for the parolee with no previous record. Conversely, they also found that the repeating offender was more likely to violate parole than those with less criminal history. 2. All investigators found a high rate of success for those who had been steadily employed prior to conviction. 3. Institutional misconduct was included in ten investigations. Eight of the findings were alike in that absence of institutional mis- conduct was highly related to parole success. However, two investi- gators reported no relationship between institutional misconduct and parole outcome. 4. The married man was found to be a good parole risk by six investigators, although three others reported finding no relation- ship between marital status and parole outcome. 5. Conviction for property offenses was consistently found to be related to a high rate of parole violation except for minor varia- tions. 6. Six investigators included farm background as a subgroup of a factor. All found a high rate of success for the parolee with a farm background, as compared with those with other types of envi- ronment. 7. Five investigators reported consistent findings that indi- viduals who received favorable psychiatric prognoses had a high rate of success. 8. One investigator found inferior intelligence related to pa- role failure, while six others found little or no relationship between intelligence and parole outcome. This apparent lack of agreement of findings was also found for number of associates, length of time served, race, age at time of parole, age at time of crime, type of sentence, and other factors. 70 Although there have been only a few studies aimed at validat- irg parole prediction with actual parole outcome based on validation with a second, nonrelated group, the findings of those studies were that the predictions were remarkably accurate. One investigator has suggested a different approach to parole prediction. He devised a questionnaire based on an analysis of in- mate "hunches" regarding the probability of the parole success or failure of other inmates. This method has yet to be tried empirically. It can readily be seen that most of the factors which have been used to predict parole success or failure are preincarceration factors. The present study has been undertaken to fill a need for an additional factor, a factor that takes into account the mental readiness of the proSpective parolee at the time he is considered for parole. CHAPTER III PROCEDURE AND SAMPLE POPULATION This chapter is devoted to a discussion of the procedures and techniques involved in the study, as well as a discussion of the per- tinent information concerning the sample population. The discussion will point out how the materials and the sample were selected, how the data were obtained, and related information obtained in the course of the study. Procedure In order to determine if there are measurable differences be- tween the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees by the we of standardized inventories, it was first necessary to administer such inventories to a group of prospective parolees. Because the lifferences sought are differences in thinking while the individuals .re still incarcerated and before they are paroled, it was not pos- ible to administer inventories to a group of successful parolees nd at the same time administer the same inventories to those who ad failed. It was necessary to test a group of inmates going on irole and then later identify the successful and nonsuccessful. A review of the standardized tests in the field of personal ljustment indicated that there were several which might lend them- :lves to a study of this nature. A group of studies compiled in a 72 1 book by Hathaway and Monachesi pointed out that the Minnesota Mul- tiphasic Personality Inventory had successfully differentiated between delinquents and nondelinquents. From this it seemed logical that this scale would have promise in the present investigation with parolees. The first report in their book, a report by Capwell, indicated that the Washburn Social Adjustment Inventory and the Vineland Social Ma- turity Scale did not seem to hold a great deal of promise in this re- These scales did not show any real difference between the 2 gard. delinquents and nondelinquents, according to Capwell's conclusions. 3 Perusal of the Mental Measurement Yearbook indicated that many of the other available adjustment inventories should not be included, inasmuch as the reports of their validity and reliability were not Of those remaining, the following were selected because satisfactory. a study of the traits or factors included in them indicated that the composite range of factors or traits measured by these five inven- tories seemed inclusive enough to best measure those factors that appeared to contribute to the possible differences in the thinking of the prospective parolees, in a battery that seemed to be most eco- nomical, both monetarily and in point of time. They were: the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN; Guilford's Inventory of Factors STDCR; the Johnson Temperament Analysis; the California Starke R. Hathaway and Elio D. Monachesi, Analyzing and Uni- _?_redicting Juvenile Delinquency with the MMPI (Minneapolis: 'ersity of Minnesota Press, 1953), et passim. Dora. F. Capwell, "Personality Patterns of Adolescent Girls: elinquents and Nondelinquents," ibid., pp. 29-36. Oscar Krisen Buros, editor, The Third Mental Measurement Rutgers University Press, 1949), pp. 23- :arbook (New Brunswick: 4. 73 Mental Health Analysis; and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. The various factors and their descriptions, as reported in the manuals, follow: 4 The Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN. C General pressure for overt activity. A tendency for liking and engaging in overt action. A Ascendancy in social situations as opposed to submis- siveness; leadership qualities. M Masculinity of attitudes and interests as opposed to femininity. I Lack of inferiority feelings; self-confidence. N Lack of nervous tenseness and irritability. 5 An Inventory of Factors STDCR. S Social introversion-extraversion. Shyness, seclusiveness, tendency to withdraw from social contacts, versus socia- bility, tendency to seek social contacts and to enjoy the company of others. T Thinking introversion-extraversion. An inclination to meditative or reflective thinking, philosophizing, analysis of one's self and others, versus an extravertive orienta- tion of thinking. D Depression. Habitually gloomy, pessimistic mood, with feelings of guilt and unworthiness, versus cheerfulness and optimism. ‘ C Cycloid disposition. Strong emotional fluctuations, ten- dencies toward flightiness and emotional instability, ver- sus uniformity and stability of moods, evenness of dis- position. 4 J. P. Guilford and H. G. Martin, The Guilford-Martin Inven— Igry of Factors GAMIN, Manual (Beverly Hills: Sheridan Supply Com- ?anY. 1943), p. l. 5 J. P. Guilford, An Inventory of Factors STDCR, Manual :Beverly Hills: Sheridan Supply Company, 1940), p. l. 74 Rhathymia. A happy-go-lucky, carefree disPosition, liveliness, impulsiveness, versus an inhibited, over- controlled, conscientious, serious-minded diSposition. The Johnson Temperament Analysis.6 A Nervous. Restlessness, fidgeting, tenseness, sleepless- ness, tendency to worry, and faulty muscular control are typical symptoms. Depressive. The best known of all the traits and prop- erly included in most temperament tests and classifica- tions. Active. The trait that is shown in the dynamic, lively, hustling, life-of-the-party, "peppy" persons. Cordial. Expre s sive warm-heartedne ss . Sympathetic. A trait that undoubtedly arose as a bio- logical necessity to insure the adequate care of children. Subjective. The trait of being highly self-centered. It may go so far that the individual interprets many things as related to himself, although there may be no real relationship. Aggressive. The trait which causes pe0ple to be push- ful, ruthless, ambitious, conceited, persistent, and de— termined. Critical. This trait is named very naturally. Self-mastery. The tendency to make plans and carry them through relatively undeflected by impulse and Caprice. It is nearly the opposite of impulsive and capricious. It involves a capacity to inhibit but also involves an ability to decide when and how much to inhibit and act accordingly. Roswell H. Johnson, Johnson Temperament Analysis, Manual (Los Angeles: California Test Bureau, 1944), pp. 2-3. 75 The California Mental Health Analysis.7 A Close Personal Relationships. The individual who pos- sesses this asset to mental health counts among his ac- quaintances some in whom he can confide, who show gen- uine resPect for him as a person, and who welcome close friendship of a warm and substantial nature. Such an individual enjoys a sense of security and well-being be- cause of having status with those who mean something to his welfare. B Inter-Personal Skills. The socially skillful individual gets along well with other people. He understands their motives and is solicitous of their welfare. C Social Participation. The socially adjusted individual participates in a number of group activities in which cooperation and mutuality are in evidence. D Satisfying Work and Recreation. The well-adjusted indi- vidual experiences success and satisfaction in his work. He also participates in a variety of hobbies and recrea- tional activities which provide release from tension. He will have chosen tasks that challenge him and that satisfy his need for approval and a sense of achievement. E Outlook and Goals. The mentally healthy individual has a satisfying philosophy of life that guides his behavior in harmony with socially acceptable, ethical, and moral principles. He also understands his environment and the forces and cause and effect relationships which shape his destiny as a member of a social group. He estab- lishes approved personal goals and makes reasonable progress toward their attainment. L Behavioral Immaturity. The behaviorally immature indi- vidual reacts on the basis of childhood (infantile) ideas and desires. He has failed to develop emotional con— trol and thinks primarily in terms of himself and his own comfort. 7 Louis P. Thorpe, Willis W. Clark, and Ernest W. Tiegs, Mental Health Analysis, Manual (Los Angeles: California Test Bu- reau, 1946), p. 3. 76 M Emotional Instability. The individual who is emotionally unstable is characteristically sensitive, tense, and given to excessive self-concern. He may substitute the joys of a phantasy world for actual successes in real life. He is quick to make excuses for failure and to take ad- vantage of those who will serve him. N Feelings of Inadequacy. The inadequate individual feels inferior and incompetent. This feeling may be related not only to particular skills or abilities but may be general in nature. O Physical Defects. The individual who possesses one or more physical defects is likely to respond with feelings of inferiority because of unfavorable comparisons or of handicaps in competition with other persons. It is usually not the physical defect per se that brings unhappiness but the restrictions and social disapprovals which come in its wake. Thus the extremely short, the homely, or the crippled individual may feel that his handicap is in- surmountable . P Nervous Manifestations. The individual who is suffering from nervous symptoms manifests one or more of a variety of what appear to be physical disorders such as eye strain, loss of appetite, inability. to sleep, chronic weariness, or dizzy Spells. ' The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. L Lie Score. A measure of the degree to which the sub- ject may be attempting to falsify his scores by always choosing the response that places him in the most ac- ceptable light socially. F Validity Score. Not a personality scale but a check on the validity of the record. Usually indicates that the subject was careless or unable to comprehend the items. Occasionally indicates a highly individual and independent person or persons who are rather badly neurotic or psychotic. 8 Starke R. Hathaway and J. Charnley McKinley, The Minne- sota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, Manual (New York: The Psy- chological Corporation, 1943), pp. 4-6; and Supplementagy Manual for the Minnesota Multighasic Personality Inventory (New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1946), p. 1. 77 K Correction Score. Also not a personality scale. Essen- tially a correction factor which has been found to be of value in sharpening the discriminatory power of the clinical variables now measured by the inventory.9 The amount of abnormal con- Hs The Hypochondriasis Scale. It is characteristic of the cern about bodily functions. hypochondriac that he is immature in his approach to adult problems, tending to fail to respond with adequate insight. D The Depression Scale. Measures the depth of the clin- ically recognized symptom or symptom complex, depres- sion. A high score suggests a characteristic personality background in that the person who reacts to stress with depression is characterized by lack of self-confidence, tendency to worry, narrowness of interests, and intro- version. Hy The Hysteria Scale. Measures the degree to which sub- ject is like patients who have developed conversion-type hysteria symptoms. The Psychopathic Deviate Scale. Measures the simi- larity of the subject to a group of persons whose main difficulty lies in their absence of deep emotional re- sponse, their inability to profit from experience, and Although sometimes Pd their disregard of social mores. dangerous to themselves or others, these persons are commonly likable and intelligent. Except by the use of an objective instrument of this sort, their trend toward the abnormal is frequently not detected until they are in serious trouble. They may often go on behaving like perfectly normal people for several years between one outbreak and another. Their most frequent digressions from the social mores are lying, stealing, alcohol or drug addiction, and sexual immorality. They may have short periods of true psychopathic excitement or depres- sion following the discovery of a series of their asocial or antisocial deeds. They differ from some criminal types in their inability to profit from experience and in that they seem to commit asocial acts with little thought of possible gain to themselves or of avoiding discovery. 9 Hathaway and Monachesi interpret K as a measure of de- ness and a lack of candor. Op. cit., p. 18. 78 Mf The Interest Scale. Measures the tendency toward mas- culinity or femininity of interest pattern. Two scoring keys were provided, for males and for females. The papers of this study were scored by both scales although. the meaning of the female scale, for male subjects, is not known. Pa The Paranoia Scale. Persons characterized by suspicious- ness, oversensitivity, and delusions of persecution, with or without expansive egotism. Pt The Psychasthenia Scale. Persons who are troubled by phobias or compulsive behavior. The compulsive behav- ior may be either explicit, as expressed by excessive hand washing, vacillation, or other ineffectual activity, or implicit, as in the inability to escape useless think- ing or obsessive ideas. The phobias include all types of unreasonable fear of things or situations as well as overreaction to more reasonable stimuli. Sc The Schizophrenia Scale. Those persons who are char- acterized by bizarre and unusual thoughts or behavior. There is a splitting of the subjective life of the schizo- phrenic person from reality so that the observer cannot follow rationally the shifts in mood or behavior. Ma The Hypomania Scale. The personality factor character- istic of persons with marked overproductivity in thought and action. The word ”hypomania” refers to a lesser state of mania. Although the real manic patient is the lay person's prototype for the ”insane," the hypomanic person seems just slightly ‘off normal. The hypomanic patient has usually gotten into trouble because of under- taking too many things. He is active and enthusiastic. Contrary to common expectations he may also be some- what depressed at times. His activities may interfere with other people through his attempts to reform_ social practice, his enthusiastic stirring up of projects in which he then may lose interest, or his disregard of social conventions. In the latter connection he may get into trouble with the law. A fair percentage of patients diag- nosed psychopathic personality are better called hypo- manic. There were other inventories that might have been included in the place of some of the above. However, the selection of this 79 battery was made because it included more factors or traits in an equal amount of administering time. Of the inventories selected, only the Johnson Temperament Analysis had inadequate reviews in the Mental Measurement Yearbook, mainly because at the time the review was written there had not been any reports based on its use. The writer felt that some of the traits included--specifically, the subjective, the aggressive, the critical, and the self—mastery traits-- would be of particular value in identifying differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees. The material presented by Johnson in the manual concerning research that had been carried out with the scale and of research underway, convinced the writer that the use of the scale would add to the value of the present study. Many of the studies which have been completed on parole vio- lation figures indicate that the majority of the violations were within the first year of parole. Witme‘rlo found that 88 percent of the vio- lations in her study occurred within the first six months. The latest figures available for this state were for the first nine months of 1950. Table II shows the months served on parole prior to violation. This table shows that 75.8 percent of those who were going to violate their paroles did so before the end of one year. These figures indicated that a waiting period of one year would be adequate to identify the majority of the unsuccessful parolees. Thus, by administering the five inventories to a group of parolees and then by waiting a year to determine which of the group were successful, the writer would have obtained the responses of a group of successful parolees which could be compared to the responses of a group of nonsuccessful parolees. 10 Witmer, op. cit., p. 384. TABLE II 80 PAROLE SERVICE PRIOR TO VIOLATION FOR 529 VIOLATORS DURING THE FIRST NINE MONTHS OF 1950 Time Served No. Pct. 1 day to 1 month ......................... 70 13.2 Over 1 month to 3 months ................... 76 14.4 Over 3 months to 6 months .................. 125 23.6 Over 6 months to 9 months .................. 82 15.5 Over 9 months to 1 year .................... 48 9.1 Over 1 year to 1-1/2 years .................. 63 11.9 Over 1-1/2 years to 2 years ................. 39 7.4 Over 2 years to 2-1/2 years ................. 16 3.0 Over 2-1/2 years to 3 years ................. 7 1.3 Over 3 years to 3-1/2 years ................. l 0.2 Over 3-1/2 years to 4 years ................. l 0.2 Over 4 years to 4-1/2 years ................. l 0.2 81 The use of such inventories permitted scores which could be translated into means and standard deviations for the successful and nonsuccessful groups of parolees. The determination of whether or not any obtained differences were real differences would involve some means of testing the significance of the differences. A test of signifi- cance which readily lent itself to this type of data was the "critical ratio." The difference between two statistics, such as two means, is called reliable or significant when the probability is high that the dif- ference cannot be explained away as temporary or accidental. The ”critical ratio" is a means of testing this significance. It employs a ratio of the obtained difference between two statistics and the stand- ard error of that difference. Confidence in the significance of the difference increases as the probability of error decreases.11 The answer to the question of when a difference is to be taken as statistically significant depends on the probability of the given dif- ference arising ”by chance." Usually a difference will be marked as "significant" when the gap between the two statistics points to or signifies a true difference in the parameters in the population from which the samples were drawn. It would seem to be fairly obvious, then, that before a judgment of significance or nonsignifi- cance can be made, some point or points must be found along a prob- ability scale which will serve to separate these two judgment cate- gories. At the same time, it must be recognized that judgments of significance are never all-or-none, but range over a wide scale of probabilities. Experimenters have for convenience chosen several arbitrary standards--called levels of significance or confidence-«of 11 Henry E. Garrett, Statistics in Psychology and Education (New York: Longmans, Green and Co., 1953), pp. 212-215. 82 12 which the .95 and the .99 levels are the most often used. The confidence with which one accepts a difference as significant or non- significant will depend on the level of significance reached. In the normal curve of distribution, £1.96 standard deviations mark off the points to the left and to the right of which lie 5 percent of the cases (2% at each end). When a critical ratio (CR) is 1.96 or more, then it includes at least 95 percent of the cases. Therefore, a level of significance of .95 is considered significant on the grounds that in no more than once in twenty trials would the difference arise "by chance." The .99 level of significance is more exacting than is the .95 level. Again, in the normal curve of distribution, $2.58 standard deviations mark off points to the left and right of which lie 1 percent of the cases. Thus a CR of 2.58 would include 99 percent of the cases. If the CR is 2.58 or more, therefore, it is extremely signifi- cant on the grounds that not more than once in a hundred trials would such a difference be due to chance. It should be emphasized, again, that those standards which have been established as indicating signifi- cance are arbitrary standards usually chosen along a probability scale. 12 In common use the levels of significance are usually re- ferred to as the 5 percent and the 1 percent levels of confidence, respectively. There will be a great deal of discussion in this study referring to the various levels of significance. The writer has hoped to avoid confusion in this regard by consistently using the positive end of the scale of probability when referring to a significance level. Thus, through this study the closer a level of significance approaches 1.00, the greater will be the confidence in the significance of the dif- ference. For example, later in the report are tables which report the various levels of significance obtained from several comparisons of scores on the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN. These levels of significance range from .008 to .989. The .008 level of sig- nificance is almost zero and for this study indicates that very little confidence could be placed in the significance of the difference. On the other hand, the .989 level of significance is very close to 1.00 and indicates that a great deal of confidence could be placed in the significance of that difference. 83 Population figures for Michigan's institutions as of October, , are reported in Table III. The number of releases through le in the year 1951 for these institutions are reported in Table The latter indicated that a majority of the subjects for the study tld come from the State Prison of Southern Michigan located at (son, Michigan, inasmuch as the majority of the state's paroles re from that institution. The House of Correction and Branch Lson located at Marquette, Michigan, was eliminated from the study cause of the travel distance involved, and the Detroit House of irrection located at Plymouth, Michigan, was eliminated because it as not a state-owned institution, although the state houses some of :s inmates there. The Michigan Reformatory is at Ionia, Michigan, Lnd the Cassidy Lake Technical School is at Chelsea, Michigan. In Michigan, the parole board interviews prOSpective parolees to determine if parole should be granted. If parole is granted, in- vestigations of home, community, and job placement are ordered. These investigations, together with the clerical work involved in the issuance of the parole certificate, take approximately one month. This one-month waiting period lent itself to the study at hand. The inventories to be used in this study could be administered to a group of inmates, who had been granted parole and who were waiting to leave the institutions. Then, after the waiting period, the successful and nonsuccessful parolees could be identified and their reaponses to the inventories could be compared to determine if there were differences in their thinking at the time they were granted parole. In this manner the five inventories that had been selected for the study-«i.e., the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN, Guilford's Inventory of Factors STDCR, the California Mental Health Analysis, the Johnson Temperament Analysis, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Inventory--were administered to those inmates of the 84 TABLE III POPULATION FIGURES FOR MICHIGAN'S PENAL INSTITUTIONS ON OCTOBER 3, 1952 Institution Population State Prison of Southern Michigan .............. 5,990 Michigan Reformatory ....................... 1,275 Michigan Branch Prison at Marquette ............ 1,054 Cassidy Lake Technical School ................. 165 Detroit House of Correction: Men ................................. 131 Women ............................... 319 Total ................................... 8,934 State Prison of Southern Michigan, the Michigan Reformatory, and the Cassidy Lake Technical School who had been granted paroles and who were waiting to be released. The inmates being tested had already obtained their paroles and were further assured that the results of the tests to be taken would have nothing to do with their cases thereafter. The fact that they had already obtained their pa- roles tended to reduce any anxiety and apprehension regarding the testing itself. The initial administration of the inventories took place at the State Prison of Southern Michigan. The inventories were administered in the evenings at the state prison at Jackson, Michigan, but at the other two institutions they were administered during the day. It was 85 TABLE IV RELEASE THROUGH PAROLE FROM MICHIGAN'S PENAL INSTITUTIONS DURING 1951 Institution Paroles State Prison of Southern Michigan ............... 1,482 Michigan Reformatory ........................ 575 Michigan Branch Prison at Marquette ............. 127 Cassidy Lake Technical School .................. 166 Detroit House of Correction .................... 156 Total .................................... 2,506 oon discovered that the time required to complete the inventories as from five to eight hours, for those who were able to complete em. The testing period at the state prison was broken up into hour sessions. Many of the inmates were able to finish the entories in two sittings, but a third sitting was required for the ver readers. The order of administering the inventories was as ws: The Mental Health Analysis, the GAMIN, the STDCR, the son Temperament Analysis, and the MMPI. The Mental Health 'sis was printed with larger print than the others, was in easily stood terminology, and printed on what the publishers called ~eze' ' paper; consequently, the writer decided it would aid in g the inmates in the proper frame of mind for the remainder testing. The MMPI was so long (566 items) that it was given 86 last. There was no reason for the order of administering the re- maining three tests other than the writer's decision to keep the order consistent. Each man was told that he could begin a new inventory as soon as he had finished the one on which he was work- ing. Cigarettes were placed on the tables and the men were told they could smoke as they worked. As a means of indoctrination for each new group, the writer discussed the objectives of the study in the light of the inmates' own experiences. He pointed out to them the various ways that the results might serve to benefit the inmate population. He suggested that if a valid scale of parolability could be discovered, it would aid in the discovery of those cases that might be paroled before their minimum sentence'had expired, and that it would aid in the determination of how long a parole iviolator would have to be detained after his return to prison. The writer also pointed out that such a scale might aid the parole board in determining whether or not an individual should be continued beyond his minimum term. This indoctrination seemed desirable to obtain the full cooperation of the inmates. If they had simply been told they were to devote approximately eight hours of their time to a very laborious task of answering 1,309 questions vithout understanding the reason for doing so, the inmates would ndoubtedly have reacted with little interest or care. The writer )Uld have been fearful that the answer sheets would have been irked the easiest way possible, without much thought, or possibly bout the inmate's even bothering to read the questions. In decid- this point the writer could anticipate a great deal of lost time and rt unless he obtained the interest and cooperation of the parolees. as surprising to note the interest engendered in the inmates for study, and almost all of them c00perated wholeheartedly. Some refused to take part, and a further few indicated passive 87 resistance by spoiling their answer sheets in some manner. These papers were not scorable and were not included in the study proper. At the end of five months of testing, 471 men had been tested and the writer's records indicated 400 complete sets of answers to the five inventorie 3. During the year interim the writer corrected the answer sheets by the use of the standardized answer scales available. He also went through the parole files to obtain the following information for each individual included in the sample: 1. Institution from which paroled. 2. Race. Date of parole. Time served prior to parole. Offense for which sentenced. Age at time of parole. 3 4 5 6. Parole officer or place of parole. 7 8. Intelligence quotient. 9. Previous criminal history. The review of the literature had indicated that the usual means of determining failure on parole was the practical criterion of the issuance of a parole violation warrant. This does not necessarily mean that those parolees for whom a warrant has not been issued are therefore making a success of their parole adjustment, since warrants are requested and issued usually only as a last resort. Thus, many arolees are able to stumble through their parole period, or perhaps re led through by the parole officer, without any real capacity for adequate community adjustment. However, in a study of this na— re it would not be possible to determine the actual degree of ad- stment for each and every parolee from a study of his reactions the community, the home, his employment, and his use of leisure 88 time. Therefore, the issuance of a parole violation warrant has been found to be the most practical means of differentiating between the successful and nonsuccessful parolee. This measure of success or failure is a clear, objective measure which can be obtained from the parole files. At the end of one year, a review of the parole files revealed that parole violation warrants had been issued for 36.5 percent of the sample population. The writer had recorded the names and numbers of the parolees under the supervision of the various parole officers. Lists were sent to each parole officer with a request that he rate his parolees as to whether their adjustment was excellent, above av- erage, below average, or borderline. A space was also provided for the parole officer to check if the parolee had violated, this informa- tion serving as a means for double-checking the writer's figures. The review of the parole files had revealed that thirty parolees . had been discharged from their paroles and that forty-seven others had been paroled out‘ of the state. The violators were in three groups: technical violators; violators with new sentences; and absconders. Kelley,13 Forlano and Pintner,l4 and others have shown that it is advisable to use extreme grOups for statistical studies of this nature. Inasmuch as the three categories of parole violators would be included in the lower extreme grouping, the writer decided that the excellent and high average groups should be included in a comparable 13 T. L. Kelley, "The Selection of Upper and Lower Groups for the Validation of Test Items," Journal of Educational Psycholpgy, 30:17-24, January, 1939. 14 G. Forlano and R. Pintne'r, "Selection of Upper and Lower Groups for Item Validation," Journal of Educational Psy_chol%_y, 32: 544-49, October, 1941. 89 high-extreme group. This left the low-average and the borderline groups at the center of the sample population. Because the thirty dischargees had completed their paroles with satisfactory adjustment they were included in the high-extreme group. The writer studied the parole reports of the forty-seven out-of-state parolees and con- cluded that ten of them should also be included in the "best" group. This conclusion was based on the completeness of the parole officers‘ reports and the writer's knowledge of the parole systems of the various states. It was felt that if there was any question regarding the parolee's satisfactory adjustment, then that parolee should be placed in the "doubtful" category. The adjustment of those other than the ten mentioned above was questionable and the "doubtful" category seemed best suited for them. This system of classification resulted in the establishment of three categories: the violators at one extreme; the "doubtful" group in the middle; and the "best" group at the other extreme. This classification has been maintained for 'the first half of the study. Table V indicates the number and type of parolees that made up each category of the classifications. For a variety of reasons it was not possible for all of the individuals to complete each of the tests. Table VI indicates the number of answer sheets available for each inventory and for each of the classifications. There were at least 147 answer sheets avail- able for the two extreme groups with a total of at least 417 answer sheets for the entire group in each test. Sample Population Dividing the parolees into three categories provided a means of comparison of the three types to obtain a more complete picture of the sample population used in this study. The material presented TABLE V 90 CLASSIFICATION OF PAROLEES INTO THREE CATEGORIES ACCORDING TO PAROLE ADJUSTMENT OR TYPE OF VIOLATION Parole Category Total NO Pct. of Péi‘t: Major Adjustment Total Classification or Violation gory Violators 172 36.52 Technical 78 16.5 45.4 New Sentence 62 13.2 36.0 Absconders 32 6.8 18.6 "Best" 163 34.61 - Excellent 32 6.8 19.6 High Average 91 19.3 55.8 Discharged 30 6.4 18.4 Out- of- state Acceptable 10 2.1 6.2 "Doubtful" 136 28.87 Low Average 69 14.6 50.8 Borderline 30 6.4 22.0 Out-of-state Questionable 37 7.9 27.2 471 TABLE VI 91 TOTAL NUMBER OF INVENTORIES COMPLETED FOR EACH OF THE THREE CATEGORIES OF PAROLEES Number Inventory Vio- Doubt- lators Best ful Total Mental Health Analysis ....... 167 162 131 460 Multiphasic Inventory ........ 147 147 123 417 Guilford-Martin GAMIN ....... 163 159 126 448 Guilford STDCR ............ 165 155 123 443 Johnson Temperament Analysis. . 163 153 126 442 in the remainder of this chapter is presented from the point of view of interest only. As stated previously, this study is not meant as a validation of the experience table factors for Michigan parolees nor to discover new factors of that type. However, the information pre- sented hereafter was available for this population and the writer felt it should be’included because of the interest it will have for many readers. Table VII contains a comparison of the sample papulation ac- cording to the offense for which each was serving at the time of parole. The federal system of uniform crime reporting divides of- fenses into two major categories: offenses against prOperty and offenses against persons. By using the critical ratio of the difference between percents as a test of significance, it is noted that the success TABLE VII 92 A COMPARISON OF SAMPLE POPULATION ACCORDING TO OFFENSE FOR WHICH SERVING AT TIME OF PAROLE , Non- Offense Total V10- vio- Best Doubt— lators ful lators Breaking and entering, night ............. 96 32 64 29 35 Breaking and entering, day .............. 11 6 5 2 3 Larceny from dwelling 7 4 3 1 2 Larceny from/in store 17 10 7 2 5 Larceny from/in building . 11 4 7 5 2 Entering ............ 4 3 1 0 1 Larceny from/in factory I 0 1 0 l Entering without breaking. 1 0 1 0 1 Unlawfully driving away auto ............. 29 10 19 13 6 Breaking and entering, ‘auto ............. 10 5 5 2 3 Larceny from auto . . . . . 13 5 8 6 2 Entering auto ......... 3 2 I 0 1 Larceny by trick ...... 6 4 2 l 1 Larceny by conversion . 4 1 3 1 2 Larceny from person 9 3 6 5 1 Grand larceny ........ 16 ' 7 9 4 5 Embezzling .......... 4 1 3 2 1 Receiving stolen property . 2 l 1 0 1 False pretenses ....... 2 0 2 l 1 Possession of burglary tools ............. l l 0 0 0 Uttering and publishing 24 10 I4 9 5 Forgery ............. 9 6 3 1 2 Violation of check law . . . 10 2 8 6 2 Malicious destruction of property .......... Z l 1 1 0 Violation of drug law 19 5 14 6 8 Carrying concealed 8 2 6 3 3 weapons ........... 93 TABLE V11 (C ontinue d) Vio NW” D bt Offense Total vio- Best ou - lators ful lators Escape ............. Disorderly, 3rd ....... Drunk driving, 2nd ..... l 0 1 0 1 Leaving scene of accident ........... 1 1 1 1 0 Felonious driving ...... l 1 0 0 0 Violation gaming law . . . . 2 0 2 2 0 U1 NU) nAN CO rbN U1 Robbery, armed ....... 28 13 15 10 Robbery, not armed . . . . 27 Conspiracy to rob, armed ............ Manslaughte r ......... Assault less than murder . Assault to murder ..... Felonious assault ...... l Negligible homicide ..... Statutory rape ........ — l Indecent liberties ...... 1 Gross indecency ....... Assault to rape ....... Incest .............. Sodomy ............. Attempted sodomy ...... Bigamy ............. Nonsupport ........... Abandonment ......... U1 N N \O y—n LA) HwHHNNNAmO‘NANv—IU‘IN Or—Oo—oNOh—prt—mOt-IOO t—NHOONo‘NAwr—v—NOmN ONHOONwOQNI-‘NINOAN HOOOO‘OwNLflr—OAOOt—O 94 rate of those individuals who committed crimes against property is not significantly different from the success rate for all offenders. The reader will remember that the violation rate for the total group was 36.5 percent, which would mean that the success rate is 63.5 percent. The success rate for those convicted for crimes against property is 60.4 percent, and difference of 2.9 percent. The CR of this difference is .83, with a level of significance of .59. However, a comparison of the success rate of those individuals who were con- victed of offenses against persons, which is 72.8 percent, as compared to the total success rate reveals a significant difference. The differ- ence in this case is 9.3 percent with a CR of 2.06 and a level of significance of .96. This indicates that a significantly greater num- ber of individuals who commit offenses against persons, when com- pared to‘the average rate of success, are likely to be successful in their adjustment to the community after incarceration. The comparison of the success rate of those individuals con- victed of crimes against property, which is 60.6 percent, with the 72.8 percent success rate of individuals convicted of crimes against persons, resulted in a difference of 12.2 percent, with a CR of 2.56 and a level of significance of .989, which is a marked significance. These figures indicate that a significantly greater number of individ- uals who commit offenses against persons are likely to be successful in their parole adjustments than will be individuals convicted of of— fenses against property. These comparisons are presented in table form in Table VIII. The writer also computed the CR for those individuals who had been sentenced for sex offenses as compared with the success rate for the total group. The success rate of those convicted of sex of- fenses was 74.0 percent, a difference from the average success rate of 10.5 percent. This CR was 1.56 with a level of significance of .88. TABLE VIII 95 THE SIGNIFICANCE OF THE DIFFERENCES IN THE SUCCESS RATES OF VARIOUS GROUPS OF OFFENDERS Parole Level Classification of Differ- Crit- of Success Com- . . Groups of Rate arisons ences 1cal Sig- Offenders p (pct.) Ratios nifi- (pct.) cance 1. Total success 1 rate ........ 63.5 1 vs. 2 2.9 0.83 0.590 2. Crimes against property ..... 60.0 1 vs. 3 9.3 2.06 0.960 3. Crimes against persons ...... 72.8 2 vs. 3 12.2 2.56 0.989 4. Sex crimes . . . 74.0 1 vs. 4 10.5 1.56 0.880 l The numerals refer to those in the first column. The difference here is approaching significance and would imply a tendency for more of the sex offenders to make a success on parole greater than the average offender. Reported in terms of percents, 74 percent of those individuals who were sentenced for sex offenses were making a success of their paroles at the end of one calendar year. The reader will remember that the average success rate, for the total group, was 63.5 percent. Of the two sex crimes that included the most individuals in this study (statutory rape and indecent liber- ties), 79.4 percent were making satisfactory adjustements. A total of 72.8 percent of those individuals who were convicted of crimes 96 against persons were making a success of their paroles as opposed to 60.6 percent of those who were convicted of crimes against prop- erty. The critical reader will note that the success rate for the offenders against persons and for [sex offenders is approximately the same but that the CR is much different. The reason for this is found in the number of cases included in the two categories. There were 188 cases included in the offenders against persons group while there were only 50 in the sex offender group. The smaller the num- ber of cases involved, other things being equal, the greater will be the standard error; and the resulting critical ratios, although the ob- tained differences might be the same, will be much different, as in this case. If the trend indicated by this study, that of 74 percent success rate for this group, were to be found in a study involving several hundred sex cases, the results could probably be accepted with confidence. There should be a study of this nature for Mich— igan parolees, to determine whether or not there is a significant difference in this direction. Table IX shows the sample population by institutions. As state previously, the violation rate for the total group was 36.5 per- cent. Taken by, institutions we find that the violation rates for the three institutions included in this study were as follows: State Prison of Southern Michigan ........ 38.0% Michigan Reformatory ................. 32.8% Cassidy Lake Technical School ........... 28.2% It is noted that the violation rates are in proportion to the total pop- ulations of the institutions. This may suggest that the amount of individual attention that the inmate receives at the reSpective insti- tutions is reflected in the parole violation rate of these institutions. 97 TABLE IX SAMPLE POPULATION BY INSTITUTIONS Institution Parole . - , , , State Prison , , Casmdy Lake Totals Classfiication Michigan , of Southern Reformator Techmcal a: Michigan Y School 3 Total sample . 377 55 39 471 i i Violators . . . 143 18 11 172 2 i Nonviolators . 234 37 28 299 -‘ Best ...... 135 16 12 163 Doubtful . . . . 99 21 16 136 The sample population by race is reported in Table X. Sixty- ne and two-tenths percent of the parolees were white, while Negroes ade up 37.4 percent of the total sample. However, only 33.3 per- nt of the white parolees violated parole, while 40.9 percent of the groes were parole violators. The difference between these per- ntages is 7.6 percent. The CR of this difference is 1.60 with a el of significance of .89. This is not a significant difference, rough it is approaching significance. The sample population by age at the time of parole is reported Table XI. The range was from 16 years to 71 years. The follow- CR's were obtained from the differences between the mean age me of parole for the various categories: 98 TABLE X SAMPLE POPULATION BY RACE Race Parole . . - ., , White Negro Indian Mex1can Totals ClaSSlflcatlon No. Pct. No. Pct. No. Pct. No. Pct. :— 1 Total sample. 288 61.2 176 37.4 3 0.6 4 0.8 471 fi Violators ... 96 55.8 72 41.9 2 1.15 2 1.15 172 3' ‘1 Nonviolators. 192 64.3 104 34.8 1 0.3 2 0.6 299 31’ Best ...... 107 65.7 54 33.05 0 0 2 1.25 163 Doubtful . . . . 85 62.5 50 36.8 1 0.7 O 0 136 Best versus Doubtful ................... 3.63 Best versus Violators .................. 3.23 Nonviolators versus Violators ............ 1.95 Violators versus Doubtful ............... .56 each case the direction of the CR was in favor of the first-men- .ned group. These CR's indicate that the difference in age between :- best and the violators, between the best and the doubtful, and be- zen the nonviolators and the violators is significant. This is another y of indicating what many other reporters have found--that the er parolee tends to be more successful in his adjustment. The record folder of each parolee contains an intelligence tient that was obtained during the quarantine period of the individ- s incarceration. Unfortunately, the same test had not been used TABLE XI 99 SAMPLE POPULATION BY AGE AT TIME OF PAROLE Parole Classification Age in Non- Years Total violators Violators Best Doubtful 15-19 27 19 8 9 10 20-24 122 74 48 32 42 25-29 114 63 51 33 30 30-34 77 54 23 30 24 35-39 55 32 23 17 15 40-44 29 21 8 14 7 45-49 ‘ 23 17 ' 6 l3 4 50-54 14 10 4 7 3 5-59 6 6 0 5 1 )-64 2 2 0 2 0 -69 l l 0 l 0 74 l 0 1 0 0 ns 30.475 31.10 29.385 32.98 28.84 9.60 10.10 8.55 11.45 8.40 100 in all cases. The majority of the 10's were obtained from an inter- pretation of the Army Alpha scores. However, the Wechsler-Bellevue had been given to those individuals who could not read and as a vali— dation of those who had extremely low Army Alpha scores. Table XII reports the IQ as noted in the record folders of the sample pop- ulation. The range of 10's was from a low 42 to a high 134. The difference in the means of the "best" as opposed to the "doubtful" was the greatest difference. The CR of this difference was 1.22 with a level of significance of .78. This is not a significant differ- ence. The interpretation of the above information is that for this win-I“ 0L.“ 2: ~ tutu-r. --4m-:I.E sample the IQ, as measured and reported here, does not differentiate between the successful and the nonsuccessful parolee. Table XIII reports the time served by the various groups of the sample population. It should be pointed out that the time served, Tor this study, includes the total number of months served consecu- ively prior to the time of release. This does not necessarily mean he time served on the sentence from which paroled. Foi- example, n individual who has been returned to prison as a parole violator rith a new sentence has served an indefinite period on his. old or revious sentence before starting the present one. In the same tanner, an escapee continued serving on the sentence for which he as serving at the time of escape until it was terminated or annulled :fore he started serving the escape sentence. As a result, it is possible that an inmate might have served veral years on a previous sentence before he started serving on 2 sentence from which he was paroled at the time of this study. e time served as indicated by Table XIII reports the total number months that the individual had served since he last entered prison ore the present parole. Because one individual was released at end of 20 years, it was necessary to compute both the means and SAMPLE POPULATION BY INTELLIGENCE TABLE XII 101 Parole Classification IQ Non- Totals violators Violators Best Doubtful 40-44 1 1 0 0 1 45-49 0 0 0 0 0 50~54 1 1 0 0 1 55-59 2 0 2 0 0 60-64 3 l 2 l 0 65-69 14 8 6 6 2 70-74 34 24 10 11 13 75-79 60 38 22 18 20 80-84 60 38 22 20 18 85—89 47 31 16 20 11 90-94 51 32 19 18 14 95-99 59 42 17 21 21 100-104 40 23 17 10 13 105—109 33 19 14 10 9 110-114 22 16 6 1'2 4 I 15— 119 21 10 11 7 3 120- 124 ll 7 4 5 2 125- 129 7 5 2 2 3 I 30— 134 5 3 2 2 1 Means 91.82 91.65 92.115 92.61 90.495 S -D . 15.60 15.10 16.50 14.60 15.05 102 TABLE XIII SAMPLE POPULATION ACCORDING TO TIME SERVED I: I: Time Parole Classification Served in Non- T . vlonths otal violators Violators Best Doubtful E- 1-6 22 13 9 8 5 1 7-12 119 74 45 38 36 1 13-18 107 71 36 39 32 1 19-24 68 39 29 16 23 1; 25-30 39 22 17 8 14 hi 31-36 33 23 10 12 11 ' 37-42 16 11 5 9 2 43-48 24 16 8 10 6 49-54 7 4 3 4 0 55-60 7 5 2 4 1 61-66 7 3 4 0 3 67-72 7 5 2 4 1 73-78 5 5 0 4 1 79-84 4 3 1 2 1 85-90 1 1 0 1 91-96 0 0 0 0 97-102 0 0 0 0 103-108 1 1 0 1 109-114 2 1 1 1 115-120 0 0 121-126 1 1 1 235-240 1 1 1 Mean 24.2 26.8 22.44 28.3 21.5 5.1). 20.70 22.74 16.62 27.06 14.76 Median 17.99 17.78 17.83 17.96 17.56 I. n D It! , ... .o 103 the medians for the various groupings of the population. A compar- ison of the medians, which do not give such great weight to the ex- treme case, indicates that the differences were very slight. Critical ratios were not computed for the mean difference because the writer felt that the extreme case would tend to invalidate the CR's. Table XIV compares the previous records of the sample pop- ulation. The term "previous record" denotes the police record of It include 3 pre vious '""”"—‘“'T the individual prior to the present conviction. arrests, juvenile institutionalizations, probations, convictions, and any other recorded information that constitutes [the individual's known antisocial history. There are several interesting bits of information to be gained from this table. First of all, it is noted that 80.2 per- cent of those who had no previous criminal history of any kind were making a success of their paroles. The difference between this suc- cess rate and the average success rate, which 'was 63.5 percent, avas 16.7 percent. The CR of this difference was 3.64 with a level 1f significance of .9996, an extremely significant difference. The success rate for what are normally regarded as first ffenders, which includes those with no previous records, was 68.3 In penology, the term "first offender" includes all those They may have ercent. arsons who are serving their first prison sentence. :en in various juvenile institutions, in jail many times, or even on 'obation as a result of a felony conviction, but they are still re- "first offenders" because they are serving a sentence in rded as This would automatically include those prison for the first time. Without the 0 have had no previous criminal record of any kind. o previous record" group, the success rate for the so-called rst offenders," for the sample in this study, drops to 61.6 per- 1;, TABLE XIV 104 SAMPLE POPULATION ACCORDING TO PREVIOUS RECORD Previous Record Parole Clas sification Non- Total V10- vio- Best Doubt- lators ful lators None (no previous history of any kind) ........ 101 20 81 50 31 Juvenile institutions 16 7 9 3 6 Probation ............ 61 18 43 27 16 Juvenile institutions and probation .......... 4 3 1 l 0 Many arrests ......... 19 5 14 5 9 Jail ................ 36 18 18 6 12 Probation and jail ...... 22 9 13 7 6 Juvenile institutions, probation, and jail 6 3 3 2 1 One parole violation 19 9 10 4 6 Two parole violations 8 3 5 0 5 Three parole violations . . 1 0 l 1 0 Juvenile, probation, and parole violation ..... 4 3 1 l 0 One previous sentence . . . 49 20 29 17 12 One previous and juvenile institution 9 0 9 5 4 One previous and jail 25 14 ll 4 7 One previous and parole violation .......... 9 4 5 4 1 One previous and P.V. with new sentence . . . . 20 8 12 6 6 Two previous sentences . . 30 15 15 10 5 Two previous and many arrests ........... 1 0 0 0 1 Two previous and juvenile institutions . . 3 0 3 l 2 Two previous and parole violation ..... 2 1 2 0 1 Two previous and P.V. with new sentence . . . . 7 4 3 2 l 105 TABLE XIV (Continued) Parole Classification Previous Record . Non- Vlo- , Doubt- Total Vlo- Best lators ful lators 3e previous ........ 10 2 8 4 4 [- ee previous and ! larole violation ..... l 1 0 0 0 :ee previous and P.V. ‘ with new sentence . . . . 1 l 0 0 0 ur previous ........ 2 2 0 1} ve previous ......... 1 1 0 0 0 '5 The reader will remember that the average success rate for all parolees was 63.5 percent. Comparing the average success rate to the success rate for "first offenders," excluding those with no previous history, results in a difference of 1.9 percent, with a CR of .39 and a level of significance of .303. There is a good possibility that a difference of this size wOuld result from chance factors and very little confidence can be placed in its significance. The reader will note that a heavy line has been drawn above the classification of three previous sentences. It will be noted that sixteen individuals who were a part of this study had served three previous sentences or more at the time of their present incarceration. It is interesting to note that 62.5 percent of these individuals were making a success of their paroles. As a matter of fact, the CR of the difference between the success rate of the so-called "first of- ienders," which was 68.3 percent when compared with the success rate of those individuals with three previous sentences or more, a 106 rence of 5.8 percent, is .49 with a level of significance of .376. difference in the success rates of the "first offenders," exclud- he group with no previous arrests, and the offenders with three lore previous sentences is .9 percent. The CR of this differ- is .07, with a level of significance of .056. Both differences (1 very likely be due to chance alone. However, these figures can :lly be regarded as meaningful because of the small number of es in the group with three or more previous sentences. It should be pointed out that because there were only sixteen ses in one of the groups for the figures just cited, there would good reason to question the reliability of the figures. However, .e implication of these figures, that the success rate for individuals 'ith no previous record of any kind is comparatively high, that more han half of the individuals with three previous sentences or more are successful parolees, and that the success rate of the "first of- fenders," ex‘c1uding those with no previous record, is not significantly different from repeated offenders, should be eXplored more com- pletely in regard to the Michigan parole population. Summary The California Mental Health Analysis, the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN, the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR, the Johnson Temperament Analysis, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory were administered to a total of 471 inmates who were about to begin their paroles. Because 75 percent of the individuals who are going to violate their paroles have violated at the end of the first year, the determination of parole success or failure, using the issuance of a violation warrant as the criterion, was established at the end of that period. Those who were still - 31' “re 1....- mu“ 107 making a success of their paroles were rated by their parole officers and the total sample population was divided into three classifications: the violators; the "best"; and a "doubtful" group. Tables were pre- pared which permitted a comparison of these three groups to parole outcome. They were presented as points of interest for a Michigan population and not as a means of validating previous studies or of discovering new historical factors that could be used in prediction. A comparison of the three parole groups, on the basis of the offenses for which sentenced, indicated thatithose individuals who were sentenced for offenses against persons were significantly more successful on parole than the average rate of success for all offenses. Comparison of the violation rates of the various institutions included in the study indicated that the violations were in proportion to the total population of the institution, implying that the amount of indi- vidual attention may have a definite relationship to parole adjustment. The comparison. by race indicated no significant relationship. The :omparison for age pointed out that the older inmates tended to be he more successful parolees. The IQ did not prove to be signifi- ant. The comparison of successful and nonsuccessful parolees by 'evious records brought out the fact that a Michigan population is lch like others which have been reported, in that the best index parolability, in this regard, was "no previous record." On the er hand, the success rate of the SOs-called "first offender," ex- :ling those with no previous records, was not significantly differ- from the average success rate or the success rate of those of- ers with three or more previous commitments. T‘— ..- CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF STANDARDIZED INVENTORY FACTORS The five adjustment inventories used in this study provided a total of forty-nine characteristics for which there were scoring scales supplied by the authors. This chapter will deal with an analysis of those characteristics through the comparison of the three groups of the sample population: the violators; the "best" of the successful parolees; and the "doubtful" group of the successful parolees. The next five tables (Tables XV through XIX) have been in- cluded to permit a comparison of the sample population averages with those of the normative populations. The manuals of the inven- tories did not contain sufficient information in regard to the norma- tive populations so that tests of the significance of the differences )etween the normative pOpulation and the sample population could be Ltilized. As a matter of fact, the mean scores for the normative Opulations were interpolated from the tables of norms provided in 1e manuals although the significance of the differences cannot be etermined, the tables have been included because there will be any readers interested in this information. The meaning of the difference between the normative average ore and the sample average score was noted for each factor. If 2 sample population score was the more desirable score, the dif- 'ence was noted as "better." However, if the normative score icated was more desirable, the difference was noted "worse." 5 was done to indicate whether the difference was favorable or :-—»- z— —.r-—— -. . . a; . m Isll ‘Q-fi 109 TABLE XV COMPARISON OF SAMPLE POPULATION SCORES WITH NORMS ON THE GUILFORD-MARTIN INVENTORY OF FACTORS GAMIN 2 Meaning S 1 Norm1 amp 6 Differ- of GAMIN Factors 3 , Mean ence Differ— Mean SUD 4 ence C. General pressure for overt activity. . 13.0 10.03 4.52 -2.97 worse A. Ascendency versus submissiveness . . . 20.5 20.02 5.70 -0.48 worse M. Masculinity versus femininity ....... 19.0 19.24 5.41 +0.24 better 1. Lack of inferiority feelings ........ 34.0 31.41 9.28 -2.59 worse N. Lack of nervous tenseness ....... 25.5 25.38 9.38 -0.12 worse 1 160 university students. 448 parolees. exceed the no rm . 3 + indicates that the mean of the sample is below the norm. - indicates that the mean of the sample sample population score is more desirable. 4 Better = normative population score is more desirable. Worse = - .2:.---_- -—-“-—--'—-?——’q TABLE XVI 110 COMPARISON OF SAMPLE POPULATION SCORES WITH; NORMS ON THE GUILFORD INVENTORY OF FACTORS STDCR 2 Meaning Sample , Norm Differ- of STDCR Factors 3 . Mean ence Differ- Mean S.D. 4 ence 5. Social introver- sion—extraversion. . 16.5 17.8 8.3 +1.3 worse T. Thinking introver— sion—extraversion. . 35.5 29.6 10.0 -5.9 better D. Depression ...... 20.5 17.8 10.7 -2.7 better C. Cycloid disposition. 28.0 21.5 12.6 -6.5 better R. Rhathymia ...... 41.5 36.5 11.2 -5.0 worse 1 388 university students. 443 parolees. + indicates that the mean of the sample exceeds the norm. - indicates that the mean of the sample is below the norm. Bette r Worse sample population score is more desirable. normative population score is more desirable. - F~'-—’O-‘ par-all I “ti-'11:." Amy- 111 TABLE XVII COMPARISON OF SAMPLE POPULATION SCORES WITH NORMS ON THE CALIFORNIA MENTAL HEALTH ANALYSIS 2 Meaning 1 S 1 Norm amp e Differ— of Mental Health Factors 3 , Mean ence Differ- Mean SUD 4 ence A. Close personal relationships ..... 14 15.97 3.29 +1.97 better B. Interpersonal skills ......... . 13 16.42 2.88 +3.42 better C. Social participation. 15 12.54 3.95 -2.46 worse D. Satisfying work and recreation 17 14.29 3.24 -2.71 worse E. Outlook and goals 15 16.86 3.76 +1.86 better 1. Mental health assets ......... 73.5 76.05 12.30 +2.55 better L. Behavioral immaturity ...... 17 12.19 3.17 -4.81. worse I. Emotional instability ....... 16 11.69 3.89 -4.31 worse Feelings of inadequacy ...... 15 13.41 3.79 -l.59 worse Physical defects 14 18.36 2.54 +4.36 better Nervous manifestations . . . . 16 15.68 3.40 -0.32 worse Mental health iabilities . . . . 76.5 71.38 13.27 -5.12 worse l 1200 adults in eight communities. 460 parolees. + indicates that the mean of the sample exceeds the norm. — indicates that the mean of the sample is below the norm. Better = sample population score is more desirable. Worse = normative population score is more desirable. r- :n :4.” Liver 112 TABLE XVIII COMPARISON OF SAMPLE POPULATION SCORES WITH NORMS ON THE JOHNSON TEMPERAMENT ANALYSIS 2 Meaning Temperament Factors Norml sample Differ3- ,Of Mean Mean S.D. ence Differ;— ence A. Nervous .. . . . . . . 5.0 5.11 3.23 +0.11 worse B. Depressive ...... 4.0 6.66 2.70 +2.66 worse C. Active ......... 9.0 9.13 2.60 +0.13 better D. Cordial . . . ..... 12.5 11.92 3.33 -0.58 worse E. Sympathetic ..... 12.3 12.49 3.07 +0.19 better F. Subjective ....... 6.5 8.42 3.09 +1.92 worse G. Aggressive . . . .‘i. . 7.8 7.97 ,2.45 +0.17 worse H. Critical ........ 4.2 6.59 3.58 +2.39 worse I. Self-mastery ..... 14.4 12.68 4.11 -1.72 worse Unweighted norms from 100 men from business college, ght school of high school level, and a few university students. 442 parolees . + indicates that the mean of the sample exceeds the norm. - indicates that the mean of the sample is below the norm. Better = sample population score is more desirable. Worse == normative population score is more desirable. ' ‘0 ‘9...‘.I;.Hh . ‘3‘ 113 TABLE XIX COMPARISON OF SAMPLE POPULATION SCORES WITH NORMS ON THE MINNESOTA MULTIPHASIC PERSONALITY INVENTORY T. 2 Meaning 1 Sample , Multiphasic Scales Norm D1ffer3— ,Of Mean ence Differ- Mean S.D. 4 ence .. Lie .......... 4.0 5.37 2.70 +1.37 worse 1‘. Validity ....... 3.0 7.36 7.27 +4.36 worse S. Correction ..... 13.0 15.54 5.13 +2.54 worse L-IS(C) .Hypochondriasis (with K) ....... 11.3 14.96 5.17 +3.66 worse Hs. Hypochondriasis 4.5 6.83 4.78 +2.33 worse D. Depression ..... 16.6 19.47 4.50 +2.87 worse I-Iy. Hysteria ...... 16.5 19.70 5.35 +3.20 worse Pd(c) .PsvchOpath (with K) 19.0 27.50 3.50 +8.50 worse Pd. Psychopath ..... 14.0 21.50 3.70 +7.50 worse Mfm. Interest (male) . . 20.5 23.84 4.61 +3.34 worse Mff. Interest (female) . 37.5 25.33 4.40 -12.17 (5) Pa. Paranoia ...... 8.0 10.53 3.97 +2.53 worse Pt(c).Psychasthenia . ‘ (with K) ....... 23.0 26.45 5.59 +3.45 -worse Pt. Psychasthenia . . . 10.0 11.11 7.42 +1.11 worse ch) . Schizophrenia . (with K) ....... 22.0 29.86 9.13 +7.86 worse Sc. Schizophrenia . . . 9.5 14.45 10.73 +4.95 worse Ma(c).Hypomania (with K) ....... 17.0 21.86 4.60 +4.86 worse Ma. Hypomania ..... 14.0 19.00 4.95 +5.00 worse l 700 visitors to the University Hospital. 417 parolees. + indicates that the mean of the sample exceeds the norm. - indicates that the mean of the sample is below the norm. 4 Bette r sample population score is more desirable. Worse = normative population score is more desirable. Score could not be interpreted for a male papulation. .-—'-.==‘ "17— . - . 114 unfavorable for the sample population with the realization that the differences could not be regarded as having significant meaning. There were forty-nine factors for which sample population scores were obtained. On one factor a decision regarding desira- bility of score could not be made. The writer scored the sample population answer sheets with the Female scoring key for the Inter- est scale on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory because he wanted to make as many comparisons between successful and non- However, there is no interpretation of that successful as he could. 1F" score for a male population. The normative population score was the more desirable score for the majority of the factors, in fact, for thirty- six of the forty- nine factors. It is recognized that the statistical significance of those dif- ferences is not known and that many of them may have little or no meaning, but the fact that the sample population had less desirable scores on 75 percent of the factors should be pointed out. It is of similar interest to inspect the individual tests in this On the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN, the 'egard. ample populatiOn had less desirable scores on four of the five fac- nrs. On the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR and on the Cal- 1rnia Mental Health Analysis there are an approximately equal nber of factors for which the sample population had the most rirable scores and for which the normative population had the st desirable scores. However, on the Johnson Temperament Analysis and on the aesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory there is, again, a marked rence. On the former the sample population had less desirable as for seven of the nine factors. On the latter the sample 115 population was below the average on all of the factors. Again it is noted that these differences may not be, and many of them do not seem to be, statistically significant, but the direction of the differ- ences seemed worthy of note. Table XX reports the means and the standard deviations for the three parole categories as obtained on the Guilford-Martin Inven- tory of Factors GAMIN. The differences between these means and the critical ratios based on those differences with the resulting levels L's-Ir ...-fl of significance are reported in Table XXI. The critical ratio, as ‘7 reported throughout this chapter, was computed with the following .Jlm: formula: 3'” D Critical Ratio = —, “D h 2 where (TD = FMI + 01312 In these formulas D represents the difference between the means; OM is the standard error of mean 1; and 0'. is the standard error 1 2 of mean 2. There were no significant differences for the Guilford-Martin Factor of General Pressure for Overt Activity or for the Guilford- Martin Factor of Ascendancy in Social Situations. However, both the "best" and the "doubtful" groups were significantly more masculine in their attitudes than the violators, according to the way this factor is scored by the Guilford-Martin inventory. The reader will remem- ber that the description of this factor merely stated "masculinity of attitudes and interests as opposed to femininity." The writer cannot explain why this characteristic should differentiate between success- ful and nonsuccessful parolees. There seems to be decided meaning for all successful parolees because both the "best" and the "doubtful" 116 TABLE XX OMPARISON OF THE MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE GUILFORD- MARTIN INVENTORY OF FACTORS GAMIN Parole Classification actors Best Doubtful Violators Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. [ O 9.99 4.42 9.90 4.25 10.18 4.78 A 19.62 6.01 19.76 5.21 19.83 5.87 M 19.85 5.44 19.86 5.36 18.65 5.37 l 32.43 9.04 31.62 9.47 30.41 9.58 N 27.63 8.66 26.71 8.93 25.17 8.51 groups are significantly more masculine in their attitudes and inter- ests than are the violators. In the factor described by Guilford-Martin as indicating a lack of inferiority feelings or, conversely, self-confidence, it is noted that the "best" group was significantly more free from inferiority feel- ings than the violators. The "doubtful" group was between the other two, but not significantly different in either case. The authors, in this regard, speak of self—confidence as one pole of this character- istic, with feelings of inferiority at the opposite extreme. There is a distinction in the way the ”best" of the successful parolees think of themselves and the way the violators think of themselves, in this regard. The "best" of the successful parolees seems to feel more TABLE XXI THE DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE MEANS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE GAMIN WITH THE RESULTANT CRITICAL RATIOS AND LEVELS OF SIGNIFICANCE Parole Classification L ‘ Best versus Violators 9, Factors 2 ‘— 2 Dif- Score Level I fer- Fa- CR 0f 1‘! 1 S ence vors 18 ' G ..................... -o.19 v 0.33 0.296 A. ..................... -0.21 V 0.31 0.243 M ..................... +1.20 B 1.99 0-953 1 . .— ................... +2.01 B 2.11 0965 . N ..................... +2.46 B 2.57 0-989 / 1 + indicates difference is algebraically greater for the first-mentioned category. 2 - B = score most desirable for Best; V = score most de sirable for Violators; D = score most desirable for doubtful. __—Al 118 TAB LE XXI (Continued) Parole Classification I?" Best versus Doubtful Doubtful versus Violators .7 Dif- Score Level Dif- Score , Level fer- Fa- CR of fer- Fa- CR of ence vors Sig. ence vors Sig. +0.09 B 0.18 0.134 -0.28 V 0.54 0.410 -—0.13 D 0.21 0.164 -0.07 V 0.11 0.084 -0.01 D 0.01 0.008 +1.21 D 1.89 0.938 +0.81 B 0.74 0.538 +1.21 D 1.22 0.774 +0.92 B 0.87 0.614 +1.54 D 1.49 0.860 119 sure of himself, more secure and adequate. The violator apparently is not so sure of himself, does not have the same sense of security or adequacy. Some writers have suggested that this feeling of in- feriority is related to thoughts of feeling "not worthy," usually of someone's love and affection or friendship. This is quite an inter- esting finding because it suggests two possibilities. One is that the 7 return to crime is a form of compensation for the violator's feeling F of inferiority. By doing something against the law and by outwitting F the guardians of the law he proves his worth and also buys his at- _ tention and esteem from others of his kind. The other possibility that suggests itself is that the feelings of inferiority are so hampering the violator that he feels he cannot compete on the open market, either in gainful employment or in personal relationships. On the one hand he steals to get the things he cannot earn and on the other hand he forces the attentions he cannot win (sex crimes). The "doubtful" group was between the other two groups but not significantly different from‘either. The Guilford-Martin factor N is reported as indicating a lack of nervous tenseness or irritability. In this regard, the "best" group was very significantly less irritable or bothered by nervous tenseness than were the violators. Again, the ”doubtful" group was between the other two, but not differing to a significant degree from either of the others. Nervous tenseness and irritability are manifestations of a condition that could be the result of a great many causes. Such conditions are frequently manifestations of emotional conflicts. The writer hesitates to suggest an interpretation of the difference in the thinking of these two groups in this regard. It might be, however, that the fact that the ”best" of the parolees feels less nervous tense- ness and irritability goes hand in glove with his feelings of confidence. He is sure of himself and at peace with himself. He has either 120 solved his conflicts or has deve10ped a more satisfactory emotional balance. This is only speculation. The only interpretation that can safely be given, according to the available evidence, is that the vio- lators feel more nervous tenseness and irritability than do the “best" of the successful parolees, as measured by this inventory. Tables XXII and XXIII report the same type of information for the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR. A study of Table XXIII in- dicates no significant differences. Apparently such factors as Social Introversion-extraversion, Thinking Introversion—extraversion, De- pression, Cycloid disposition, and Rhathymia, as measured by this inventory, do not differentiate between the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolee 5. TABLE XXII COMPARISONS OF THE MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE GUILFORD INVENTORY OF FACTORS STDCR Parole Classification ‘actors Best Doubtful Violators Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. S 17.55 8.49 17.32 7.58 18.36 8.54 T 29.26 9.45 28.39 8.63 29.18 10.18 D 17.39 11.09 17.22 9.74 18.04 10.58 C 20.75 12.52 21.72 12.29 22.06 12.98 R 37.17 10.76 37.12 11.35 36.97 11.26 121 TABLE XXIII THE DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE MEANS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE STDCR WITH THE RE SULTANT CRITICAL RATIOS AND LEVELS OF SIGNIFICANCE Parole Classification Be st versus Violators Factors Dif - Sco re 2 Level fer- Fa- CR of ence1 vors Sig. S ..................... -0.81 B 0.85 0.604 T ..................... +0.08 v 0.07 0.056 D ..................... -0.65 B 0.53 0.406 c -1.31 B 0.92 0.642 R ..................... +0.20 B 0.17 N“ / + indicates difference is algebraically greater for the first-mentioned category. 2 - B = score most desirable for Best; V : score most de sirable for Violators; D = score most desirable for Doubtful. TABLE XXIII (C ontinue d) 122 Parole Classification Best versus Doubtful Doubtful versus Violators )if — Score Level Dif- Score Level .er— Fa— CR of fer- Fa- CR of nce vors Sig. ence vors Sig. 0.23 D 0.24 0.186 -1.04 D 1.09 0.724 +0.87 D 0.79 0.573 -0.79 D 0.71 0.522 +0.17 D 0.14 0.263 +0.82 V 0.68 0.501 -0.97 B 0.65 0.484 -0.34 D 0.23 0.182 +0.05 B 0.04 0.032 +0.15 D 0.11 0.088 123 The statistics for the Johnson Temperament Analysis are re- ported in Tables XXIV and XXV. The most important fact appearing from these tables is the very significant difference in favor of the "best" group, with an also significant difference in favor of the "doubtful" group, as compared with the violators, in the factor Sympathetic. The author describes this characteristic as a trait that undoubtedly arose as a biological necessity to insure the ade- quate care of children. However, a study of the individual items included in this scale would seem rather to indicate a general feel- ing of well-being toward or regard for one's fellow men. The "best" group of the successful parolees is extremely more sympathetic than the violators, but the "doubtful" group of the successful parolees is also significantly more sympathetic than the violators. Johnson does not adequately define the trait, Sympathetic, in the manual for his inventory. The items used to measure the trait are based on the manifestations of the trait that permit an estimation of the degree of the sympathetic trait in the individual but they do not indicate the dynamics of the trait“. The writer does not feel that he has enough information upon which to attempt an interpretation. The other significant difference is in the trait described as Nervous. The violator is much more nervous than the "best" while the "doubtful" tends to be more like the "best" although this difference is not significant. This finding is much like the finding for the Guilford-Martin factor N which indicates a lack of nervous tenseness or irritability. The reader will remember that there was a significant difference in favor of the "best" of the parolees, as in the Nervous scale for this inventory. afi—rr-AFT»»—..—+—. . .. . ' 1:... ‘ TABLE XXIV 124 COMPARISONS OF THE MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF ANALYSIS THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE JOHNSON TEMPERAMENT Parole Classification Factors Doubtful Violators Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. A 4.85 3.18 4.95 3.30 5.58 3.18 B 6.52 2.76 6.63 2.64 6.80 2.66 C 8.89 2.70 9.20 2.51 9.23 2.60 D 11.75 3.34 11.94 3.32 11.80 3.32 E 12.89 3.07 12.69 3.28 11.88 3.01 F 7.92 3.09 8.20 3.22 8.42 3.09 G 7.56 2.43 7.76 2.56 7.96 2.54 H 6.45 3.74 6.44 3.28 6.87 3.63 I 13.50 3.73 13.42 3.62 12.85 3.67 —_“”‘_‘17 "(z w- I; b *- -. . ._ .. 125 TABLE XXV THE DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE MEANS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE JTA WITH THE RESULTANT CRITICAL RATIOS AND LEVELS OF SIGNIFICANCE Parole Classification Best versus Violators Factors 2 Dif- Score Level fer- Fa- CR of ence1 vors Sig. A ..................... -0.73 B 2.04 0.958 B ..................... -0.28 B 0.90 0.681 c ..................... -0.34 v 1.16 0.752 D ............... .‘ ..... -0.05 V 0.14 0.101 E ..................... +1.01 B 2.95 0.996 F ..................... -0.50 B 1.43 0.846 G ..................... -o.4o B 1.33 10.816 H ..................... -o.42 B 1.03 0.696 I ..................... +0.65 B 1.58 0.884 4% + indicates difference is algebraically greater for the first-mentioned group. 2 . t de- B = score most desuable for Best; V a score mos sirable for ViOIatOI'S; D = score most desirable for Doubtf‘fl- 126 TABLE XXV (Continued) Parole Classification Best versus Doubtful Doubtful versus Violators 1.” if- Score Level Dif- Score 1 Level er— Fa- CR of fer- Fa- CR of nce vors Sig. ence vors Sig. 3.10 B 0.26 0.204 -0.63 D 1.63 0.894 0.11 B 0.35 0.268 -0.17 D 0.52 0.396 -0.31 D 0.99 0.678 -0.03 V 0.12 0.091 -0.19 D 0.37 0.288 +0.14 D 0.48 0.368 +0.20 B 0.51 0.392 +0.81 D 2.16 0.960 -0.28 B 0.73 0.532 -0.22 D 0.59 0.441 -0 .20 B 0.68 0 .502 -0.20 D 0.67 0.496 +0.01 D 0.02 0.016 -0.43 D 1.08 0.718 +0.08 B 0.19 0.150 +0.57 D 1.33 0.815 'M‘. S sus-mus “.-.—l W “a." ‘.: 127 The differences between the successful and the nonsuccessful lees on the remaining traits measured by the Johnson Tempera- 1: Analysis did not prove to be significant. Therefore, such traits Depressive, Active, Cordial, Subjective, Aggressive, Critical, and E—mastery, as measured by this inventory, did not differentiate ween those who succeed on parole and those who fail. The same type of statistics are reported for the California ental Health Analysis in Tables XXVI and XXVII. The one factor 1 which there are significant differences is that identified by the uthors as Satisfying Work and Recreation. There is very little lifference in this regard between the ”doubtful" and the violator groups, but both are significantly less satisfied in their work and recreation than the ”best." The description of this trait in the manual of the Mental Health Analysis outlines the manifestations by which a sense of satisfaction with work and recreation are revealed but does not aid in an understanding of the ‘dynamics of the char- acteristic. The writer's interpretation is a subjective one, without conclusive evidence upon which it can be based. However, it seems to the writer that satisfaction in one's work and recreation reflects an attitude that stems, again, from the individual's feelings of self- confidence, security, and adequacy. On the other hand, it might be that the feeling of satisfaction in work and recreation results in a feeling of confidence, security, and adequacy. One of the interesting facts in regard to this trait is that it is the first trait on which the "doubtful" group is significantly different from the ”best" group. In this regard the "doubtful" group and the violators are both signifi- cantly less satisfied with their work and recreation. Although none of the other subtests on the Mental Health Analysis resulted in significant differences, the trend indicated by one subtest should be discussed because of' its relationship to subtests TABLE XXVI 128 COMPARISONS OF THE MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE CALIFORNIA MENTAL HEALTH ANALYSIS Parole Classification Factors Best Doubtful Violators Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. A 16.30 3.12 15.67 3.13 15.88 3.68 B 16.77 2.51 16.15 2.94 16.31 3.14 C 12.72 3.95 12.37 3.96 12.51 3.93 D 14.61 2.82 13.86 3.42 13.88 3.45 E 17.10 2.42 16.52 2.91 16.90 2.83 Assets 77.12 11.63 75.06 12.17 75.20 13.57 L 12.19 3.23 12.40 3.14 12.05 3.12 M 12.02 3.99 11.77 3.96 11.30 3.94 N 13.74 3.71 13.13 3.76 13.02 3.90 0 18.32 2.35 18.15 3.05 18.49 2.16 P 15.56 3.23 15.69 3.50 16.11 3.48 .iabilities 71.66 13.11 71.39 13.92 71.08 13.32 " ‘3 it». —" 42336; . 0‘,‘ 3J- I?» 33*. TABLE XXVII E DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE MEANS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE CMHA WITH THE RESULTANT CRITICAL RATIOS AND LEVELS OF SIGNIFICANCE Parole Classification Best versus Violators Factors Dif- Score2 Level fer- Fa- CR of ence1 vors Sig. ..................... +0.42 B 1.13 0.742 3 ..................... +0.46 B 1.47 0.858 C ..................... +0.21 B 0.48 0.368 D ..................... +0.73 B 1.95 0.949 E ..................... +0.20 3‘ 1.05 0.706 Assets .................. +1.92 B 1.38 0.832 1.. ................ \ ..... +0.14 B 0.38 0.296 M ..................... +0.72 B 1.66 0.903 N +0.72 B 1.72 0.914 0 -0.17 V 0.69 0.444 P -0.55 V 1.49 0.864 Liabilities .......... . . . . . +0.58 B . 0.40 0.310 l + indicates difference is algebraically greater for the first-mentioned category. B = score most desirable for Best; V = score most de- sirable for Violators; D = score most desirable for Doubtful. 130 TAB LE XXVII (C ontinue d) Parole Classification Best versus Doubtful Doubtful versus Violators Dif- Score Level Dif- Score Level ier- Fa- CR of fer- Fa- CR of ence vors Sig. ence vors Sig. +0.63 B 1.72 0.915 -0.21 V 0.53 0.404 +0.62 B 1.89 0.940 -0.16 V 0.43 0.334 +0.35 B 0.74 0.538 -0.14 V 0.29 0.212 +0.75 B 1.99 0.953 -0.02 V 0.03 0.024 +0.58 B 1.87 0.938 -0.38 V 1.13 0.741 +2.06 B 1.47 0.857 -0.14 V 0.10 0.074 +0.21 D 0.57 0.424 +0.35 D 0.94 0.652 +0.25 B 0.55 0.410 +0.47 D 1.02 0.792 +0.61 B 1.45 0.852 +0.11 D 0.25 0.196 +0.17 B 0.54 0.412 -0.34 V 1.10 0.728 -0.13 D 0.35 0.272 -0.42 V 1.01 0.688 +0.27 B 0.17 0.134 +0.31 D 0.19 0.150 ‘ —A 131 .her inventories. The results of the scale labeled Nervous festations are almost in complete disagreement to those found 8 Guilford-Martin GAMIN and the Johnson Temperament Anal- In this case it is the violators who tend to be most free from 00.3 manifestations while the "best" group are more sympto- ic in this regard. While the difference is not significant, it is reaching significance, and is large enough to cause concern over :1- apparent contradiction with the findings of the other two inven- a ries. The reader will remember that the Guilford-Martin GAMIN r‘ 1dicated a significant difference in that the ”best" were much less .; ~ rritable or hampered by nervous tenseness than the violators. The E1 iohnson Temperament Analysis also resulted in a significant differ- ence in that the violators were more nervous than the "best." Both differences were significant and in agreement. The finding on the Mental Health Analysis, although not significant, is not in agreement with the former two. In view of the fact that the other two are sig- nificant and are in agreement, it would seem to raise serious doubt about the validity of the Nervous scale of the Mental Health Analysis. If all three inventories are measuring the same factor, and they should be inasmuch as they are labeled alike, then the results for all three should be alike in relationship to parole outcome. The CR‘s for the remainder of the traits included in the Men- tal Health Analysis do not indicate significant differences This means that such traits as Behavioral Immaturity, Emotional Insta- bility, Feelings of Inadequacy. Physical Defects, Outlook and Goals, Close Personal Relationships, Inter-Personal Skills, and Social Par- ticipation, as measured by this inventory, do not differentiate between successful and nonsuccessful parolees. Tables XXVIII and XXIX report the statistics for the Minne- sota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. The only significant 132 TABLE XXVIII IPARISONS OF THE MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF 'I-IE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE MINNESOTA MULTIPHASIC PERSONALITY INVENTORY Parole Classification es Best Doubtful Violators Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D. 19.29 4.94 19.76 4.47 19.43 4.49 7.29 6.86 7.81 8.10 7.31 7.12 15.70 4.94 15.56 5.34 15.54 5.13 5.59 2.69 5.46 2.81 5.09 2.59 1mm 23.52 3.87 23.90 4.50 24.15 5.24 Pt 10.69 6.98 11.29 7.78 11.59 7.57 Pt“) 26.21 5.64 26.88 5.23 26.33 5.51 Sc 14.20 10.16 14.61 11.31 14.55 10.78 Sc“) 29.88 8.94 30.05 9.70 29.67 8.81 Mif 26.36 3.68 26.51 3.85 26.41 4.82 Pd 21.06 4.02 21.15 3.48 22.05 3.76 Pd“) 27.15 4.35 27.31 3.63 28.01 4.23 Hs 7.16 5.31 6.47 4.62 6.81 4.54 115“) 15.41 5.40 14.66 5.24 14.70 4.78 Ma 18.61 4.85 19.20 5.03 19.10 5.11 Mam 21.71 4.50 22.28 4.42 22.19 4.71 Hy 20.57 5.40 20.39 4.75 19.90 5.54 Pa 10.82 3.95 10.29 4.28 10.76 3.75 u ..4 ..n-g-u anmm- w.m‘T .- 'E"UIA 133 TABLE XXIX THE DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE MEANS OF THE THREE PAROLE CATEGORIES FOR THE MMPI WITH THE RESULTANT CRITICAL RATIOS AND LEVELS OF SIGNIFICANCE Parole Clas sification Best versus Violators Scale 5 Dif- Score2 Level fer- Fa- CR of ence1 vors Sig. D ..................... -0.14 B 0.26 0.202 F ..................... -0.02 B 0.03 0.024 K ..................... +0.16 v 0.60 0.451 L ..................... +0.50 v 1.62 0.895 Mfm ................... -o.63 B .17 0.758 Pt ..................... -0.90 B 1.06 0.711 2 mm ................... -0.12 B 0.18 0.14 Sc ..................... -0.35 B 0.28 0.220 ch) ................... +0.21 v 0.20 0.158 Mff .................... -0.05 0.10 0.078 Pd .................... -0.99 B 2.17 0.970 Pd(c) ................... -0.86 B 1.72 0.914 Hs .................... +0.35 v 0.60 0.231 H .................. +0.71 V 1.18 0.763 8(C) . 9 Ma .................... -0.49 B 0.84 0.59 630 Ma(c) .................. -0.48 B 0.90 0. 06 Hy .................... +0.67 v 1.05 0.7 Pa .................... +0.06 v- 0.14 0,104 _ / + indicates difference is algebraically greater for the first-mentioned category. 2 J B = score most desirable for Best; V a score most de sirable for Violator; D z score most desirable for Doubtful. 4-1 TABLE XXIX (C ontinued) 134 Parole Clas sification Best versus Doubtful Doubtful versus Violators Dif-s Score Level Dif- Score Level fer- Fa- CR of fer- Fa- CR of ence vors Sig. ence vors Sig. -0.47 B 0.57 0.432 +0.23 V 0.55 0.416 +0.52 B 0.56 0.428 +0.50 V 0.53 0.402 +0.14 D 0.22 0.174 +0.02 V 0.03 0.025 +0.13 D 0.39 0.296 +0.37 V 1.11 0.732 —0.38 B 0.75 0.540 -0.25 D 0.41 0.318 -0.60 B 0.66 0.490 -0.30 D 0.32 0.250 -0.67 B 1.02 0.694 +0.55 V 0.84 0.600 «0.41 B 0.30 0.238. +0.06 V 0.04 0.034 --0.17 B 0.15 0.118 +0.38 V 0.33 0.258 -0.15 0.45 0.346 +0.10 0.19 0.153 -0.09 B 0.19 0.142 -0.90 D 2.04 0.958 -0.16 B 0.33 0.258 -0.70 D 1.46 0.854 +0.69 D 0.56 0.422 -0.34 D 0.60 0.231 +0.75 D 1.19 0.764 -0.04 D 0.11 0.086 -0.59 B 0.96 0.664 +0.10 V 0.15 0.118 -0.57 B 1.08 0.718 -0.42 D 0.17 0.134 +0.18 D 0.29 0.228 +0.49 V 0.78 0.564 +0.53 D 1.07 0.714 -0.47 D 0.96 0.664 ‘C-t11 a: r-W‘j ‘ “QR-7 135 difference for this inventory was obtained where it might have been expected, on the Psychopathic Deviate (Pd) scale. However, it is important to note that the difference is significant for both the Accord- "best" and the "doubtful" as compared with the violators. ing to the manual of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, the Pd scale measures the similarity of the subject to a group of persons whose main difficulty lies in their absence of deep emotional 4 response, their inability to profit from experience, and their disre- gard of social mores. The manual goes on to say: Although sometimes dangerous to themselves or others, Except by gr, these persons are commonly likable and intelligent. the use of an objective instrument of this sort, their trend toward the abnormal is frequently not detected until they are in serious trouble. They may often go on behaving like perfectly normal people for several years between one outbreak and another. The most frequent digressions from the social mores are lying, steal- They may ing, alcohol or drug addiction, and sexual immorality. have short periods of true psychopathic excitement or depression following the discovery of a series of their asocial or antisocial deeds. They differ from the criminal types in their inability to profit from experience and in that they seem to commit asocial acts with little thought of possible gain to themselves or of avoid- ing discovery. From the evidence resulting from this study, it can be said that the violators admit to significantly more manifestations of the Psycho- of the athic Deviate than do either the "best" or the "doubtful" 1ccessful parolees. Some writers have suggested that there are two distinct types individuals in the penal institution. Various names have been given group such as the "situational offender," the "accidental offender," The other group is identified as the ' ' oc casional offender." the Suggestions have been made that there nopathic personality. Ld be different institutions and methods of treating the two types 136 of inmates. Much more study of the psychopathic deviate in our penal institutions is indicated. There were no other significant differences for the various scales of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Such sub- tests as the Lie Score, the Validity Score, the Correction Score, the Hypochandriasis scale, the Depression scale, the Hysteria scale, the Interest scales, the Paranoia scale, the Psychasthenia scale, the Schizophrenia scale, and the Hypomania scale, as represented in ' _ _~‘«_.-F :1, 1 O this inventory, did not differentiate between successful and unsuccess- ful parolees. However, the findings on one scale, the Interest scale, should 1‘ be discussed more fully because of their relationship to an Interest scale on one of the other inventories. On the Male component of the MMPI Interest scale, the "best" group had lower mean scores than the violators, which on this scale indicates a more masculine interest pattern. This indicates a tendency toward agreement with the signifi- cant finding on the GAMIN, that the "best" and the "doubtful" were more masculine in attitudes and interests than the violators. It is interesting to note that when the correction (K) score is applied to the Pd scale, there is a trend toward a significant iifference in the same direction as reported for the uncorrected cale although it is not as significant as on the uncorrected scale. m the other hand, when the correction is applied to the Hypochon- riasis (Hs) scale, there is a much stronger trend toward signifi- nce. In this experiment the use of K sometimes intensifies the {nificance of the differences, but on three of the five scales where is applied it tended to reduce what significance there was before re ction. 137 Summary This chapter has dealt with an analysis of the forty-nine author-identified factors of adjustment included in the five inventories used in this study. The critical-ratio test of significance was ob- tained for all factors on the basis of three comparisons: the "best" 7 of all the successful parolees versus the violators; the "best" ver- sus the ”doubtful" group of the successful parolees; and the "doubt- ful" group versus the violators. There were few significant differ- ences obtained, but those that were obtained indicate definite differ- " m1:- Lame... ~:- mm A. ...-hem E ‘1 1 ences in certain characteristics for the three groups. The differentiating characteristics of the "best" group are that they are more masculine in attitudes and interests than the vio- lators; that they are more confident than the violators; that they are less nervous than the violators; that they are more sympathetic than the violators; that they are more satisfied with work and recreation than either the violators or the "doubtful" group; and that they indi- cate less psych0pathic deviation than the violators. The distinguishing characteristics of the "doubtful" group are that they are more like the "best" in that they are more masculine in attitudes and interests than the violators; and also in that they are nore sympathetic than the violators. Like the "best" they also indi- :ate less psychoPathic deviation than the violators. However, on one ignificant trait they are more like the violators in that they are also ignificantly less satisfied in work and recreation than the "best." The violator is distinguished by less masculine attitudes than ther the "best" or "doubtful" groups; by more feelings of inferior- ' than the "best" group; by more nervousness than the "best"; by b appearing as sympathetic as‘either the "best" or the "doubtful" )ups; by not being as satisfied with work and recreation as the 138 “best"; and by indicating more psychopathic deviation than all of the successful parolees, both the "best" and the "doubtful" groups. n sue-x: y- u—T-uj—g'r , -... ....- a -'~".'. . It... .. lu‘fo a}... . . i . l i y D. CHAPTER V THE ITEMS AND THEIR VALIDITY A total of 1,309 items were included in the five adjustment inventories used in this study. That there are differences in the thinking of successful and unsuccessful parolees was indicated by The writer felt that an item tCTYTn‘ "Ru-'1. ' n; ‘1. 5.1-17 A .- ‘ the findings of the previous chapter. analysis would result in a more reliable and exacting measurement of these differences than could be obtained by using the subtest re- The item analysis did reveal a wide range of significance sults. among items, and the complete sets of answer sheets for the total sample were scored for the most significant items to determine whether or not these items differentiated between the parole outcome for suc- cessful and nonsuccessful parolees. The item analysis, the most sig- nificant items, and their validation are discussed in this chapter. The Item Analysis One hundred papers from each of the extreme groups were 156d in the item analysis. The reader will remember that in Table 1 'I the number of answer sheets for each of the three classifications »r the five adjustment inventories were reported. The 100 answer leets were selected from each of the five inventories for the "best" the successful parolees and for the violators by a method of Supra, p. 91. 140 random sampling. The method used was to take every other paper, and then divide the number needed to complete the 100 papers into the number of remaining papers to learn if every third, fourth, or fifth paper would be taken. For example, for the "best" group on the GAMIN there were 159 answer sheets. The writer first shuffled the answer sheets so that they were not in any specific order. He r then took every other answer sheet for the item analysis, starting F with the second paper. Going through the 159 answer sheets in this i manner yielded 79 answer sheets for the item analysis and left 80. : There were still 21 answer sheets needed to make the total of 100 i by Dividing 80 by 21 indicated that to be used in the item analysis. Tak- every fourth paper would come closest to the number desired. ing every fourth paper and adding it to the 79 previously selected The one hundredth paper was taken from the made a total of 99. This center of the 60 unselected papers remaining in this group. process was repeated for the "best" and the violators for each of the five inventories. The total number of answer sheets that had been obtained during the original administration of the inventories included 2,210. One thousand of these were used in the item analysis. For a number of reasons every inmate taking part in the study did not complete Lll of the inventories. It will also be remembered that a few unco-s perative inmates spoiled some of the answer sheets, either by leav- 1g out whole blocks of items, by answering all items one way or iother alike in one block, or by alternating responses to the extent at these answer sheets could not be retained as part of the study. pnsequently, there were not complete sets of five answer sheets for ery inmate included in the study. Altogether, there were 384 com- te sets of answer sheets, which included 1,920 of the 2,210 papers. - hundred and thirty of the 1,000 papers used in the item analysis 141 were in the incomplete group. It was the writer's intention to re- score the complete sets of answer sheets using those items that sig- nificantly differentiated between successful and nonsuccessful parolees and to relate the total scores to parole outcome. By this method of random sampling only 870 of the 1,920 answer sheets which comprised the complete set that were later rescored were also used in the item by analysis, leaving 1,050, or 54.7 percent, that were not included in the a item analysis. :5 An ideal validation study would be to score a completely inde- E pendent set of papers by using the items found in this study to sig- :3 i" nificantly differentiate between successful and nonsuccessful parolees and relate that score to the independent group's parole outcome. This would be true cross-walidation. Because such cross-+validation could not be carried out in the present study, an attempt was made to make the validation as nearly ideal as possible. This would mean select- ing papers for the item analysis in such a manner as to leave as many papers as possible that were independent of the item analysis. The use of all the answer sheets available, rather than just the com— plete sets, added 290 answer sheets to the sample from which the papers for the item analysis were drawn. One hundred and thirty of these were included in the item analysis, composing 10.3 percent 31' the total papers included, thus making a more ideal cross-walida‘ ion than would have resulted if just the complete sets were used. The critical ratio of the difference between the percentage of uccessful parolees answering "Yes" or "No" to the item and the arcentage of violators answering "Yes" or "No" to the item, depend- g on which difference was the greater, was the method used in the rm analysis. The difference was not always the same for the alter- te responses because some of the parolees did not always answer item. On three of the inventories a "'2" or "Undecided" choice 142 was permitted and on the other two inventories with just the two Con- choice alternates, some of the parolees just did not respond. sequently, the differences between the responses of the two groups for both the alternate answers were recorded and the alternate answer with the larger difference was the one used for computing the critical ratio obtained from the differences ratio. This means that the critical in the percentage of reSponses to an item will sometimes be for the ”Yes” or "True" alternate of the item and sometimes for the "No" The response for which the critical ratio was or "False" alternate. computed will be indicated for those items with significant differences in a discussion of those items below. The formula used for the critical ratio of the difference of percentages follows: D 2 CR 3 % GD? a up + 0': (7qu o 1 2 is the stand- where D% is the difference between the percents; 0' 1 is the standard error of ard error of the first percentage; and up 2 the second percentage. Fortunately, Edgerton and Patterson had deve10ped a table of standard errors for percentages, which proved to be extremely time- saving. The CR's for all 1,309 items are presented in Appendix C. There were 102 items significant beyond the .95 level of significance Moreover, there were several other items with CR's that resulted in The item levels of significance just a little below the .95 level. Harold A. Edgerton and Donald G. Patterson, "Table of Standard Errors and Probable Errors of Percentages for Numbers of Cases," reprinted from The Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. X, No. 3, September, 1926, pp. 378-391. 143 analysis had resulted in such few items for which there was a sig- nificant difference that the writer felt those items that were near the minimum CR acceptable might add some weight to a composite score for differentiating between successful and nonsuccessful parolees. The writer realizes that the .95 (.05) and .99 (.01) levels of signifi- He further cance are those usually held to as tests of significance. realizes that these levels are arbitrary and that accepting a lower level means lowering one's standards of exactness. However, when the writer began to think of using weighted scores, lower his standards a little to the .93 (.07) level of significance, 3 he decided to which also permitted the inclusion of those items that had been just This below the previously acceptable minimum level of significance. was done with the realization that judgments of significance are not This would all-or-none, but range over a wide scale of probability. mean that a difference which resulted in a CR of 1.82, with the resulting level of significance of .93, would be due to "chance" not more than once in fifteen trials. By the acceptance of the .93 level of significance as the mini- Table XXX mum, the total number of items was increased to 132. reports the number of items from each inventory and the percentage of each inventory's items that met the criterion of significance adOpted Although this study was not designed as a validation for this study. for the various inventories on a population of this nature, Table XXX is included because it will be of interest to some readers. The writer cannot explain the reason why there should be such a variation in the percentage of items for the various inventories reflecting significant differences, except in relationship to the number 3 Infra, p. 157. 144 TABLE XXX THE NUMBER AND PERCENT OF ITEMS WITH SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES FROM EACH INVENTORY Significant Items Inventory Number Percent GAMIN ......................... 21 11.3 STDCR ......................... 10 5.6 JTA ........................... 38 20.9 CMHA .......................... 21 10.5 MMPI .......................... 42 7.4 of factors in each inventory on which there were significant differ- ences, and then the relationship is not consistent. There were no significant differences on the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR and the percentage of items on which there were significant differ- ences is the lowest of the five inventories, as one would expect. There was only one factor with significant differences on both the California Mental Health Analysis and on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, and the percentage of items with significant differences reflects this fact. The fact that there were two factors with significant differences on the Johnson Temperament Analysis is also reflected in the proportion of items with significant differ- ences. On this basis, then, the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN should have a very high percentage of items with significant 145 differences because there were significant differences for three of its five factors. However, such was not the case, indicating that a large proportion of the discriminatory ability of those factors, for this population, rested in a comparably small number of items. The items for which there were significant differences in All of the items for a specific inventory reSponses are listed below. The item number, as it appeared in the in- are listed as a group. The letter or group of letters ventory, is given before each item. following the item number indicates the scale or scales by which the The authors of the inventories use that item in their inventories. In some instances coding of the scales is that used in Chapter 111. there are no code letters, indicating that the item was not used in Hathaway and McKinley point the scoring of a scale in the inventory. out that the last two hundred items (items 367 through 566) of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory are experimental items and are not included in the published scales. There was no indica- tion in the manual of the Johnson Temperament Analysis why item 2 was not scored on any of Johnson's scales. The notation after the item is the response for which the The reader will note that the answer critical ratio was computed. This is to indicate whether the differ‘ is preceded by a + or a - ence is in the direction of the successful parolees (+) or in the di- It also indicates the way the item is rection of the violators (-). It was necessary to use positive and negative scoring to be scored. because some of the items had different CR's for the alternate 4 Supra, pp. 73-78. 5 Hathaway and McKinley, op. cit., p. 5. 146 responses, as previously discussed Take, for example, item 137 of the Johnson Temperament Analysis, which was answered as fol- lows: Yes Np Successful ................... 10 83 Violators .................... £4 13 Difference ................... 14- 10+ Critical ratio ................. 2.66 1.70 The "No" answer is not significant and cannot be used in scoring the item for parolability. The "Yes" answer is significant, but only if it is scored negatively. Because there were several items of this nature, it became necessary to include some negative-scored items and some positive-scored items. In those cases where the CR was the same for both reaponses the positive scoring was indicated. It means that the CR In other words, this notation has two meanings: was computed for the answer indicated, because the difference in percentage of responses by the successful and the violators was greater for that answer, and the difference was in the direction of the successful, if +, or the violator, if -; it also means that in future The All scoring the response indicated is to be scored either + or - last notation listed with each item is the CR for that item. critical ratios shown are 1.82 or greater corre3ponding to a level of significance of .93 or better. There were twenty-one items included in the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN for which significant differences between the percentages of responses by the successful parolees and by the violators were found. They follow:6 6 The code letters below refer to the scale for which the item is scored by the inventory. Factor M is Masculinity versus 19. 32. 44. 47. 49. 76. 80. 92. 108. 116. 117. 124. Do you have one or more abilities in which you believe you are superior to most other people? ................ Are there ever times when you feel so jumpy you could throw things at people if you did not control yourself? ....... Do you feel bored much of the time? In a group activity do you often find yourself compelled to play an unimpor- tant part? ...................... Do you feel that you are lacking in self-control? .................... Are you ever afraid that you cannot live up to the standards your parents set for you? ....................... Do you frequently feel self-conscious in the presence of important people? Do your interests tend to change quickly? Do you often find it difficult to sleep at night? ....................... Do you ever wish you could have been born at a different time or place or in a different family than you were? Are you frequently in a state of inner excitement or turmoil? ............. Can you (or could you) walk past a graveyard alone at night without feeling uneasy? 000000000000000000 Would you rate yourself as an impulsive individual? -Yes -Yes +No +No +No +No -Yes -Yes -Yes +No +Yes +No 147 1.98 1.98 1.92 2.45 1.91 1.87 2.86 2.08 2.34 2.98 3.39 2.02 1.85 Femininit-y; N is Lack of Nervous Tenseness; I is Lack of Inferior- ity Feelings; G is General Pressure for Overt Activity; and A is Ascendancy versus Submissiveness. 128. A. 141. N 146. A 155. I 169. N 179. I 182. I 183. N Do you ever take the initiative to enliven a dull party? OOOOOOOOOOOOOO Can you stick to a tiresome task for a long time without being prodded or encouraged? ..................... When in a restaurant you are served stale or inferior food, do you usually make a vigorous protest about it? Do you sometimes wish that you were more attractive than you are? ........ Are there times when you feel as if your nerves were raw or "on edge"? Do you often find that you cannot make up your mind until the time for action is past? ................ Do you often show yourself up to your own disadvantage? OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Do you become upset rather easily? . . . . +No +Yes -Yes +No +No -Yes +No -Yes 148 2.14 2.14 1.85 1.85 2.18 2.30 2.04 Significant differences were found for ten of the items, of the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR. They are the following:7 12. DC Do you daydream frequently? ......... -No 2.58 14. DC Are you inclined to worry over possible misfortunes ? ..................... +No 2 .94 57. RC Would you like a position in which you changed from one kind of task to another frequently during the day? ........... +Yes 2.52 80. DS Are you troubled with feelings of inferiority? ..................... +No 2.46 82. STDR Are you inclined to take life too seriously? ....... _ ............... +No 2.99 7 S is Social lntroversion-Extraversion; T is Thinking Introversion-Extraversion; D is Depression; C is Cycloid dispo- sition; and R is Rhathymia. 146. D 148. TRS 169. TC Are you inclined to keep your opinion to yourself during group discussions (not class discussions)? Are you much concerned over the morals of others? Do you feel tired most of the time? Is it easy for you to act naturally at a party? OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Do you like to indulge in a reverie (daydreaming) ? ................... +Yes +No +Yes -No 149 2.35 1.98 2.36 1.97 2.64 The item analysis of the Johnson Temperament Analysis re- sulted in thirty-eight items for which there were significant differ- ences. 19. 21. 36. 45. 8 They are: Is one motive for S to go places so that he can talk about having been there? ......................... Does S think the government is spending too much on relief and pensions? Does s talk slowly (making due allowance for age)? Would S buy an article at the cheaper price if he noticed that the clerk has asked less than the price tag indicates, apparently having misread it? Does S maintain uniformly, courteous behavior to other members of his family ? ........................ Does 5 eat slowly (making due allowance for age)? 000000000000000 +No +No +Yes +No +Yes +Yes 1.98 2.19 1.84 2.05 2.19 2.13 B-Depressive; These factors were identified as follows: C-Active; D-Cordial; E-Sympathetic; F-Subjective; G-Aggressive; H-Critical; and I-Self-mastery. A-Nervous; 51. 52. 57. 59. 61. 70. 84. 90. 95. 99. 105. 110. 122. 127. 129. Is S so sympathetic with those he sees in pain as to want to do something about it ? ....................... Is 5 likely to give way to the wishes of others rather than to seek to have his own way? .................... Does S make a practice of offering help to motorists who need help, but do not ask for it? .............. Does S accept defeat easily without any evidence of his disappointed feeling? ........................ Does S get into scrapes occasionally? Can S relax easily when sitting or lying down? ..................... Does S stand by and avoid protecting an animal from needless suffering? In an automobile accident in which S is involved does he really try‘ to see that any damage he did is made good? Is S usually able to steady a difficult situation where "others lose their heads " ? ....................... Is S independent in making a judgment uninfluenced by whether he likes or ' dislikes the leading supporter of the proposal in question ? .............. Is S considered cherry by some people ? ........................ Is S relatively unaffected in listening to emotional music? ............... Is S almost free from being suspicious of the actions of others? ............ Do death, sickness, pain, and sorrow enter largely into S's dreams? ....... Does S think as well of those with whom he has a disagreement, as before? ..... +Yes +Yes +Yes +Yes -Yes +Yes +No +Yes +Yes +No +Yes +No 150 2.38 2.86 2.21 1.85 1.84 1.98 2.06 1.97 1.91 2.00 2.04 2.13 2.80 2.24 2.33 132. 133. 135. 136. 137. 138. 142. 151. 1 55. 156. 157. 158. 159. 160. 161. 167. Is S hard to please? ............... Does S carry out assignments promptly and systematically? ......... Is S rather optimistic about oppor- tunities for young people? ........... Is 8 "touchy" on several things about himself ? ................... Is 5 bothered at times with the idea that nobody cares for him? .......... Does S look ahead and fail to smile and show interest when passing a beautiful child? Does S find that a minor failure or poor showing of his can be quickly forgotten? Do companions like to be with S? Does S when on a picnic find himself sometimes unable to share the good spirits of the others? .............. Does 5 think well of most people, as to only rarely speak slightingly of them? Does 5 show a cordial attitude only to close friends if at all? ............. Does S think someone does not like him and speaks critically about S to others ? ........................ Does S smile or laugh a good deal? . . . Can S see things as others see them, when he wishes to? Does 8, when he has a grievance straightened out, continue disgruntled for a while ? Does S find it annoying to have any criticism made of himself even though justified and from which he could profit ? OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO +No +Yes +No +No -Yes -Yes +Yes +No +Yes +No -Yes +Yes +Yes +No +No 151 1.90 2.17 2.73 2.14 2.66 2.05 1.88 1.89 2.04 1.82 2.26 2.33 1.94 2.25 2.54 2.83 152 174. D Is 5 appealed to strongly by young lovers who are hampered by opposition? ..................... -Yes 2.26 The California Mental Health Analysis included twenty-one items for which the difference in the percentage of responses by successful parolees and the percentage of reSponses by the violators was significant. These twenty-one items follow: 9. B Can you keep people from feeling too embarrassed when they make a mis— take? ......................... +Yes 2.17 16. D Do you find that the type of work you are doing is sufficiently interesting? . . . +Yes 1.92 22. E Do you believe that you should always be honest in your dealings with people? . +Yes 2.46 28. A Do you have one or more close friends of your own sex? ................. +Yes 2.32 29. A Do you feel that some of your neighbors deserve your friendship? ............ +Yes 2.19 31. M Do you often feel as though you are held back from doing things that you would like to do? ................. +No 2.00 51. L Have you frequently been able to get even with people you dislike by ignoring them? ......................... -No 3.18 56. B Do you make a practice of showing people that you recognize their abilities?. -No 1.87 64. C Do you sometimes travel or go camping with people of your own age? ........ +Yes 2.56 9 The scales of this inventory are identified as follows: A - Close Personal Relationship; B - Interpersonal Skills; C - Social Participation; D - Satisfying Work and Recreation; E «- Outlook and Goals; L - Behavioral Immaturity; M - Emotional Instability; N - Feelings of Inadequacy; O - Physical Defects; P - Nervous Manifestations. 65. 73. 82. 100. 127. 135. 147. 152. 153. 176. 177. 190. C Does it seem that you are left out of things you would like very much to be in? ........................... Do you believe that people who do what is right will eventually be rewarded? Are you frequently dissatisfied because your plans do not work out satisfactor- ily ? Are you frequently bothered by eye- strain? OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Do the members of your family seem to believe in your honesty and truth- fulness ? Have you often felt that you have more than your share of bad luck? ......... Do you seem to catch cold easily? ..... Are many people so unpleasant that you are justified in treating them with in- difference ? ..................... Do you have any brothers, sisters, or other close relatives who are as friendly to you' as they should be? ........... Do members of your family seem to like your friends? Is there someone to whom you can go if you are in trouble? .............. Do you usually like to be where there is much activity? ................. OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO +No +Yes -Yes +Yes -Yes +Yes ~Yes +Yes +Yes -No +Yes 153 3.43 2.35 2.14 2.36 2.80 2.00 2.12 2.30 2.64 2.09 2.60 2.38 There were forty‘two items for which significant differences were found as a result of the item analysis of the Minnesota Multi- They are the following:10 phasic Personality Inventory. 10 The following are the meanings for the code letters of this inventory: F - Validity; K - Correction; L -— Lie; Hs - Hypo- chondriasis; D - Depression; Hy - Hysteria; Pd - Psychopath; Mm - Interest (Male); Mf - Interest (Female); Pa - Paranoia; Pt - Psychasthenia; Sc - Schizophrenia; and Ma - Hypomania. 14. F Ihave diarrhea once a month or more . OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 45. L Ido not always tell the truth. 1am in just as good physical health as most of my friends. 51. 011st 1 am liked by most people who know me .................... 54. F 1am almost never bothered by pains over the heart or in my chest. 55. 14st OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO As a youngster I was suspended from school one or more times for cutting up . 56. F OOOOOOOOOOOOOOO I do many things which I regret afterwards (I regret things more or more often than others seem to) ........................ 94. PdPt 97. MaSc At times I have a strong urge to do something harmful or shocking. . 115. FMme I believe in a life hereafter. 117. MmePa Most people are honest chiefly through fear of being caught. ..... 118. Pd In school I was sometimes sent to the principal for cutting up ..... 126. Mme I like dramatics. I commonly wonder what hidden 136. Hy reason another person may have for doing something nice for me. . . When I was a child, I belonged to a crowd or gang that tried to stick together through thick and thin. 143. Ma I would rather win than lose in a game. 150. L I have never felt better in my life than I do now. 1 60. KDHy My people treat me more like a child than a grown-up. 212- MaSc +True - True +True +True +T rue +False -False +False -False +False +False +False -Fa1 se +False +False +False +False 154 2.90 2.08 1.91 2.38 2.18 1.84 1.84 2.22 1.82 2.02 2.56 2.74 2.45 1.86 1.93 3.05 2.04 1 155 1 224. Pd My parents have often objected to the kind of people I went around with .................. —True 2.84 237. Pd My relatives are nearly all in sympathy with me. ............ -False 1.98 247. F Ihave reason for feeling jealous of one or more members of my family. .................... - True 1 .9 5 252. F No one cares much what happens to you. .................... +True 2.04 268. PdMa Something exciting will almost always pull me out of it when I am feeling low. .............. +True 1.98 272. FDK At times I am all full of energy. . . -False 3.06 303. Sc I am so touchy on some subjects that I can't talk about them. ..... -True 2.30 311. Sc During one period when I was a youngster I engaged in petty thievery. ................... +False 3.03 317. Pa I am more sensitive than most other people. ................ -False 1.99 329. Pt I almost never dream. ......... +True 2.04 362. Pt I am more sensitive than most -False 2.21 other people ................. I tend to be interested in several 372. different hobbies rather than to stick to one of them for a long time . ...................... +True 1. 84 392. A windstorm terrifies me. ...... -True 2.32 Often, even though everything is 396. going fine for me, I feel that I don't care about anything. ....... +False 1.86 It bothers me to have someone 16. watch me at work even though I know I can do it well ........... -True 2.05 2.0 . I have had some very unusual -False 2.82 religious experiences . .......... 156 y 437. It is all right to get around the ( law if you don't actuall break it. . . +False 1.87 f 454. Icould be happy living all alone if in a cabin in the woods or yr mountains . .................. - Fals e 2 . 36 / 457. Ibelieve that a person should / never taste an alcoholic drink ..... +True 2.28 481. Ican remember "playing sickH to get out of something. ........ +False 2.14 485. When a man is with a woman he is usually thinking about things related to her sex. ............ -True 1.90 492. I dread the thought of an earthquake . ................. -False 1 .85 516. Some of my family have quick tempers. ...... . ............ -True 2.08 547. ‘ I like parties and socials. ....... -True 2.40 565. ' I feel like jumping off when I am on a high place. .............. -True 1.86 Validity In Chapter IV it was shown that there were some adjustment characteristics that significantly differentiated between successful and nonsuccessful parolees. The reader will remember that there were significant differences in masculinity of attitude and interests, In feelings of inferiority, in nervous tenseness and manifestations, In a sympathetic feeling, in satisfaction with work and recreation, and in degree of psychopathic deviation. The item analysis described above resulted in 132 items that also significantly differentiated be- ween these two groups. However, none of the differences were so :lear cut that they could be pointed out as including all of the suc- ressful or nonsuccessful parolees. Because no one characteristic or 157 group of items could be used in such manner, the only logical means of utilizing the information was to combine the items into a scale, the composite score of which would indicate the degree of the dif- ference in the thinking of the successful and nonsuccessful parolee and the value of this difference in terms of parolability. Some of the significant differences resulting from the item analysis indicated a need for negative answers while others per- It soon became apparent that there was mitted a positive reply. also a difference in the degree of significance. The difference on some items was extremely significant while on others it barely ex- There were so few items ceeded the minimum level of significance. with extremely significant differences that the writer felt they must have more meaning, for differentiating between successful and non- successful parolees, than those with barely significant differences. It seemed to follow that weighted scores would serve as a means of compensating for- the wide variances in the item validities. The decision to use differential weighting in the scoring was based on the idea that such system might result in making the final scores more meaningful, while it would not change the actual outcome of the scoring. Thus, it might add value to the score while it would Inasmuch as the only disadvantage would not invalidate the score. arise from the more complicated scoring, the writer decided the possibility of a more meaningful score would justify the additional The most logical means of differentiating between Table work in scoring. the weights to be assigned was by the level of significance. XXXI indicates the various levels of significance and the correspond- ing CR's which describe the limits of the assigned weights. As a result, the items were scored *1, 1:2, or a 3:3. The reader will remember that there were 384 complete sets of test answer sheets obtained from the 471 parolees who were 158 TABLE XXXI THE WEIGHTS ASSIGNED TO ITEMS ACCORDING TO THE CRITICAL RATIO OF DIFFERENCES IN PERCENTAGE OF RESPONSES Limits Weights Critical Ratio Level of Significance 1 ................ 1.82 to 2.33 0.930 to 0.980 2 ................ 2.34 to 2.81 0.981 to 0.995 3 ................ 2.82+ over 0.995 originally tested in this study. The validity of the composite score of the items with significant differences for predicting parole out- come could be tested by obtaining scores for the 384 parolees who had complete sets of answer sheets and for whom the parole out- come could be determined. By this time, eighteen months of parole period had gone by and the writer again checked the parole office files so that a com- posite score for the items with significant differences could be ob- tained for the successful and nonsuccessful parolees as of the end of eighteen months instead of one year, thereby gaining advantage of the longer waiting period which permitted a more reliable differ- entiation between success and failure. It was discovered that 41.4 percent of the 384 parolees included in this part of the study had violated their paroles within the first year and a half. This was an increase of 4.9 percent for the additional six months. 159 It should be pointed out, incidentally, in connection with the parole violation rate, that there are approximately 2,500 to 3,000 inmates paroled in Michigan every year. During 1953 the Michigan Parole Board paroled 2,670 inmates of a total of 5,871 who had parole interviews. There are many considerations, in addition to the question of whether or not the prospective parolee is a good risk, that enter into a parole decision. One of the major consider- ations is in keeping with one theory of parole, that inasmuch as 95 percent of the inmates will eventually be released, it is better to release the inmate under supervision and guidance than to turn him loose with no controls. As a result, many cases, even where there is reasonable question of successful adjustment, result in parole, if the parole does not endanger the public safety, especially in those cases where the placement might serve as an aid to the parolee's adjustment. An elaboration of this point is not necessary to the study at hand. However, the interested reader might con- sult the works of Ohlin,ll Dressler,12 and Laune.l3 Table XXXII reports the scores of the items with significant differences for the violators and for the nonviolators, the latter in- cluding those individuals who had been in the "doubtful" category during the first part of the study. The mean score for the entire group was 43.50 with a standard deviation of 16.20. The mean score for the nonviolators was 49.31 with a standard deviation of 15.05. The violators' mean score was 34.52 with a standard deviation 11 Ohlin, op. cit., pp. 19-40. 2 David Dressler, Probation and Parole (New York: Colum- bia University Press, 1951), pp. 16-66. 13 Laune, op. cit., pp. 1-9. 160 TABLE XXXII THE WEIGHTED SCORES MADE BY 384 PAROLEES, COMPARING SUCCESSFUL AND NONSUCCESSFUL, ON 132 ITEMS WITH SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES Parolees Scores '~— Violators Nonviolators —10 to -6 ...................... 2 0 -5 to +1 ...................... 1 0 0 to 4 ........................ 2 0 5 to 9 ........................ 5 1 10 to 14 ...................... 6 4 15 to 19 ...................... 12 3 20 to 24 ...................... 9 7 25 to 29 ...................... 16 7 30 to 34 ...................... 20 21 35 to 39 ...................... 23 17 40 to 44 ...................... 18 14 45 to 49 ...................... 16 28 50 to 54 ...................... 12 32 55 to 59 ...................... 7 29 60 to 64 ...................... 5 27 65 to 69 ...................... 3 21 70 to 74 ...................... l 9 75 to 79 ...................... 1 3 80 to 84 ...................... 0 2 161 of 16.20. The CR of the difference in the means of the violators and the nonviolators was 9.01, indicating an extremely significant difference. The mean score for the nonviolators exceeded the scores of 82 percent of the violators. The writer applied various "cut-off" scores and then compared the scores for the two groups to the parole outcomes to determine what the results might have been if the scale and the cut-off score had been applied to this group be- fore parole. For the cut-off score of 35, the scale would have pre- dicted the outcome of approximately 81 percent of those who were successful but would have missed the outcome of 54 percent of the violators. A cut-off score of 40 would have predicted 73 percent of those who were going to stay out and 60 percent of the violators. On the other hand, a cut-off score of 45 would have correctly iden- tified 67 percent of those who became successful and 72 percent of those who violated their paroles. If only those inmates were paroled who obtained a score higher than the various cut-off scores, the resulting parole viola- tion rates would have proved very interesting. A cut-off score of 35 would have reduced the parole violation rate from 41.4 percent Placing the cut-off score at 40 would have resulted to 32 pe rcent . in a parole violation rate of 27.6 percent, while a cut-off score of It is at 45 would have reduced the violation rate to 22.9 percent. once obvious that the higher the cut-off rate, the better, that is, the lower, would be the parole violation rate. If the score were set high enough, the success rate could be phenomenal, but there would be a correSpondingly high number of individuals not paroled who might also have been successful. In this study, for example, sixty of the individuals who are now making a success of their 162 paroles would not have been paroled if a cut-off score of 40 had been used. As a measure of the degree of relationship between the scores on the scale of items with significant differences and parole success the biserial coefficient of correlation was computed by the following formula: P ‘1 , L9 Z bis 0’ is the mean of the group in the first category and Mq r is where M1) the mean of the group in the second category; 0 is the standard de- viation of the entire group; p is the proportion of the whole group in category one; q is the proportion of the whole group in category two; and z is the height of the ordinate in the normal curveidividing The biserial coefficient of correlation for this scale p from q. computed for this population was .638, with a standarderror of .044. A biserial coefficient of correlation of this magnitude and the demonstrations with the use of various cut-off scores indicate that the composite score of the significant items does differentiate satisfactorily between successful and nonsuccessful parolees, to the extent that the results have predictive value. Reliability A test- retest method of determining reliability was not pos- sible, and a split-half, or odd-even, technique did not seem advis- The split-half method has been criticized because the result- able. ing estimate of reliability varies depending upon the way the scale is split into halves or upon the accidental position of particular items in the original scale. Each of the various ways that a scale is Split gives its own estimate of reliability and there are fairly large 163 fluctuations in the values obtained. Because there are so many ways of splitting a scale, the split-half coefficient is not a unique value. In view of the importance that the halves be as much equivalent as possible and what seemed like a strong possibility that they would not be, the writer decided some other method of estimating relia- bility would be more exacting. Ciarrett10 discusses a method of estimating reliability devel- oped by Froelich,ll which’he states would result in a minimum esti- mate of reliability as compared to the other methods. The formula is: 2 not - M(n-M) r = 11 Z 0't (n-l) where n is the number of items; M is the mean test score for the total group; and o: is the standard deviation of the test scores for the total group. The application of this formula to the present data resulted in a reliability coefficient of .895. c . The Kuder and Richardson formulas for the estimation of 12 reliability were consulted. The data were best suited to the 9 Dorothy Adkins et a1., Construction and Analysis of Achievement Tests (Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1947), pp. 148-160. Henry E. Garrett, Statistics in Psycholgy and Education (New York: Longmans, Green and Co., 1953), p. 336. 11 G. J. Froelich, "A Simple Index of Test Reliability," Journal of Educational Psychology, 1941, 32:381-385. 12 G. F. Kuder and M. W. Richardson, "The Theory of the Estimation of Test Reliability," Psychometrika, 2:251-260, Septem- ber. 1937. 37 '19 .n“.".:*‘hlh: ...-aw : - w“: 164 Case IV formula, which resulted in a reliability coefficient of .872. This formula is .— 6:1.00-5. z - .. at - Npq Mt N I "O l where N is the number of items; Mt is the mean test score for the total group; and at is the standard deviation of the test scores for the total group. Kuder and Richardson pointed out that this formula would never overestimate reliability. -."'."'a5.. ri-r'n‘b :- 4“".47'. '5 Although not in the .90 range, lRV-‘nfiv v the fact that these estimations are minimal values indicates good reliability. Summary An individual item anlysis of the items included in the five inventories used in this study, using the method of obtaining a criti- cal ratio derived from the difference in the percentage of reSponses of one hundred of the "best" of the successful parolees, selected at random, and one hundred of the violators, resulted in finding 132 items with significant differences. Rescoring the complete sets of answer sheets of the sample, using weighted scores, resulted in an extremely significant difference between the mean scores of the successful parolees and the violators. Demonstrations with various cut-off scores indicated the possible use of these items for predict- ing parole success or failure. The biserial coefficient of correla- tion for total score on these items as related to parole outcome indicated, further, that such use of the items would have some val- idity. Two estimates of reliability indicated that the results would be fairly consistent. CHAPTER VI SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary The procedure. A review of the literature pertaining to pa- role prediction techniques and studies revealed that almost all of the factors that have been investigated were preincarceration fac- tors. It seemed to the writer that a prediction method should also take into account any differences which might exist in the thinking of the prospective parolees at the time of consideration for parole. Consequently, it was the purpose of this study to test one means of discovering whether or not there are differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees, and if such differences were discovered, whether or not the nature and extent of such differences would have value as predictors of parole success or failure. The hypotheses tested, therefore, were: I. There are differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees which can be measured objectively by avail- able standardized inventories. 2. The nature and extent of such differences will have some reliability and validity as predictors of parole success or failure. To discover whether or not there are differences in the think- ing of successful and nonsuccessful parolees, five inventories were administered to a group of inmates going on parole. These inven- tories were the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN, the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR, the Johnson Temperament 166 Analysis, the California Mental Health Analysis, and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. A total of 471 inmates from three Michigan penal institutions were tested in this manner. The parolees were identified as successful or as violators after all had been on parole at least one year. A parolee was de— clared successful if he had been discharged from supervision or if he was still under active supervision. The violators were identified as those who had been returned to prison for technical violations, those who had been sentenced as the result of conviction for new of- fenses, and those who had been declared fugitives by reason of ab- The in-state successful parolees were rated by their scondence. excellent parole ad- parole officers on a four-place rating scale: justment; high average parole adjustment, low average parole adjust- ment, and borderline parole adjustment. The parole reports of out- _ of-state parolees were studied by the investigator so a similar rating could be made on the basis of those reports. The out-of- state parolees were adjudged as "acceptable," meaning they were to be classified with the high average and excellent, or in question- able cases as ”unacceptable," based on the nature of their monthly reports and the investigator's knowledge of the parole system of the state in which they were serving their paroles. Three parole cate- the "best" of the successful parolees; a "doubtful" gories resulted: Those parol- group from the successful parolees; and the violators. ees who had been discharged with improvement, those that had been rated as excellent in their adjustment, those that had been rated as high average in their adjustment, and those out‘of-state parolees who had been rated as acceptable were included in the "best" group. All of the other successful parolees who were not included in the "best" category were placed in the "doubtful" category. 167 The following information was obtained from the parole files for each individual included in the sample: 1. Z. 9. Institution from which paroled. Race. Time served prior to parole. Offense for which sentenced. Age at time of parole. Intelligence quotient. Previous c riminal hi sto ry. Date of parole . Parole officer or place of parole. A five-place table was prepared for the various offenses for which individuals were serving, showing the total number who had been convicted for each offense, the number of violators, the num- ber of nonviolators, the number of ”best," and the number of "doubtful" (see Table VII, Chapter III). the critical ratio of the difference in proportions, were made as follows: Four comparisons , using the success rate of those convicted of crimes against property with the total success rate; the success rate of those con- victed of crimes against persons with total success rate; the suc- cess rate of those convicted of sex crimes with total success rate; and the success rate of those convicted of crimes against property with the success rate of those convicted of crimes against persons. The violation rate for each of the participating institutions was determined (Table IX, Chapter III). The sample population was compared according to race (Table X, Chapter III). The critical ratio of the difference in proportions between the percent of white parolees who violated parole and the percent of Negro parolees who violated parole was computed. 168 A five-place comparison of the sample population by age was made (see Table XI, Chapter III), and the mean and standard devia- tion obtained for each of the parole categories. The significance of the differences between the means, determined by obtaining the critical ratio of the difference between two means, was used for "best" with ‘T four comparisons: the ”best" with the "doubtful," the the violators; the violators with the "doubtful”; and the violators with the nonviolators. A five-place comparison of the sample population according The ‘1‘.“ an sage!“ to intelligence (see Table XII, Chapter III) was also made. means and standard deviations of each of the parole categories was determined. The critical ratio of the two groups with the greatest difference in means was computed. These groups were the “best" as opposed to the ”doubtful." 'The sample population according to time served was also compared in a five-place table (Table XIII, Chapter III). The means, standard deviations, and medians for each of the parole categories were computed. The sample population was compared on the basis of their Each individual was placed in a classification previous rec ords . A five- that indicated the extent of his previous criminal history. category comparison was made for each type of offender (Table XIV, Chapter III). In addition, three comparisons of the success rate of various groups were made by use of the critical ratio of the differ- ence in proportions: the comparison of the success rate of those with no previous arrests to the average success rate; the success rate of first offenders, excluding those with no previous criminal history, with the average success rate; and the success rate of first offenders with the success rate of those who had three previous sentences or more . 169 The mean score for the total sample population was deter- mined for each of the factors included in the five inventories. The mean score of the sample population was compared with the mean score of the normative population, on each factor, in terms of which of the groups had the most desirable score. However, statistical tests of the differences were not possible. The significance of the difference between the means of the . threeparole categories, the "best," the "doubtful," and the violat- ors, was determined by computing the critical ratios of the differ- ences between the means on each of the factors of the five inventor- ies. The following comparisons were made: the ”best" with the L; violators; the “best" with the "doubtful"; and the "doubtful" with the violators. There were a total of forty-nine factors for which the three sets of comparisons were made. 'There were included in the five inventories used in this study a total of 1,309 items. The validity of the individual items was de- termined by computing the critical ratio of the difference of the per- centage of responses of one hundred of the successful parolees, selected at random, compared with the percentage of the responses )f one hundred of the violators, also selected at random. All of the complete sets of answer sheets for the sample .pulation were scored using those items which had been found to fe rentiate significantly between the successful and nonsuccessful olees . The items used in this process were weighted according heir discriminating ratios. The sample population was divided two groups so that a comparison could be made of the total ghted scores from the items with significant differences. These LpS were the violators, and the nonviolators, including those who previously been in the "doubtful" category. However, it was .ible to classify the subjects as violator of nonviolator on the 170 basis of parole status at the end of 18 months of parole service instead of the one-year period previously used. Means and standard deviations of the two groups were computed and the critical ratio of the difference of the means determined. The biserial coefficient of correlation was also determined, as were two estimates of re- liability. The findings. There were a total of 471 inmates in the sam- ple population. Of this number 377 were from the State Prison of Southern Michigan, 55 were from the Michigan Reformatory, and 39 were from the Cassidy Lake Technical School. 1. Of the inmates who had been on parole for at least one year, it was found that 172 had violated parole. Of this number, 62 had been convicted of committing new offenses, 78 were returned to prison as technical violators, and 32 were declared fugitives from justice because of abscondence. This is a violation rate of 36.5 percent. A total of 163, or 34.6 percent, were rated as the "best" of the successful parolees, while 136, or 28.9 percent, were rated as "doubtful." 2. The sample population had been sentenced for a total of forty-nine offenses. These offenses were divided into two basic :ategories: offenses against property and offenses against persons. total of 335, or 71.1 percent, of the sample population had been invicted of offenses against property. The number convicted of enses against persons was 136, or 28.9 percent of the total sam- Of this number, 50, or 10.6 percent of the total sample, had n convicted of sex offenses. The success rate for the total sample was 63.5 percent. The cess rate for those convicted of crimes against property was 60.4 cent, the success rate of those convicted for crimes against 171 persons was 72.8 percent and the success rate of those convicted of sex offenses was 74.0 percent. The CR of the difference between A the success rate of the total sample and the success rate of those convicted for crimes against property was .83 with a level of sig- nificance of .590. The CR of the difference between the average success rate and the success rate of those convicted of crimes ...... against persons was 2.06 with a level of significance of .960. The Q CR of the difference between the success rate of those convicted 2 of crimes against property and of the success rate of those con- ; victed of Crimes against persons was 2.56 with a level of signifi- J cance of .989. The CR of the difference between the total success rate and the success rate for those who had been convicted of sex crimes was 1.56 with a level of significance of .880. These figures indicated that persons convicted of crimes against property violated at approximately a normal rate of violation. Individuals convicted of offenses against persons were significantly more successful than individuals convicted of offenses against property. It would then follow, and does follow, that individuals convicted of offenses against persons were likewise significantly more successful than the average parolees. The success rate of sex offenders was not significantly greater than the average, but the difference is approaching signifi- cance. 3. The rate of violation for the State Prison of Southern Michigan was 38.0 percent, while the rate of violation for the Mich- igan Reformatory was 32.8 percent, and that for the Cassidy Lake Technical School was 28.2 percent. It is noted that the violation rates are in proportion to the total populations of the institutions, a fact which may suggest that the amount of individual attention re- ceived by the inmates at the respective institutions is reflected in the parole violation rate of these institutions. 172 4. There were a total of 288, or 61.2 percent, white parol- ees in the sample population, while 176, or 37.4 percent, were Negro. Six-tenths of one percent were Indian and 0.8 percent were Mexican. The difference between the percentage of white parolees who were violators, 33.3 percent, and the percentage of Negro parolees who were violators, 40.9 percent, was 7.6 percent. The CR of this dif- I" ference was 1.60 with a level of significance of .89. This differ- E ence is not significant although it is approaching significance. 5 5. The range in age at the time of parole was from 16 years 5 to 71 years old. Table XXXIII shows the means and standard devia- ;! tions of five parole categories. There were four comparisons made. In each of these the CR was in favor of the first-mentioned group. The CR of the difference in the means was computed for the dif- ference between the "best" and the "doubtful" categories, and it was 3.63 with a level of significance of .9996. The CR for the dif- ference between the means of the "best" and the violators was 3.23 with a level of significance of .9986. For the difference be- tween the means of the nonviolators and the violators the CR was 1.95 with a significance level of .949, while that for the violators and the "doubtful" category was .56 with a level of significance of .424. These findings indicate that the older parolee tends to be the more successful. 6. The range in intelligence quotients for the sample popula- tion was from a low 42 to a high 134. The mean 10's and standard deviations for the five parole categories are shown in Table XXXIV. The greatest difference between any two means was between the "best" as compared with the "doubtful." The CR means of the of this difference was 1.22, with a level of significance of .78. This is not a significant difference and would indicate that the IQ, as T AB LE XXXIII 173 THE MEANS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF FIVE PAROLE CATEGORIES ACCORDING TO AGE AT TIME OF PAROLE Parole Category Mean 3:521:22: Total sample ...................... 30.47 9.60 Violators ......................... 29.38 8.55 Nonviolators ....................... 31.10 10.10 Best .......................... 32.98 11.45 Doubtful ........................ 28.84 8.40 TABLE xxx1 v, THE MEAN INTELLIGENCE QUOTIENTS AND STANDARD DEVIATIONS OF FIVE PAROLE CATEGORIES !4_ Parole Category Mean 3:311:22: Total sample ...................... 91.82 15.60 Violators . . ....................... 92.115 16.50 Nonviolators ....................... 91.65 15.10 Best . . . . ...................... 92.61 14.60 Doubtful . . ...................... 90.495 15.05 I74 measured and reported in this study, does not differentiate between the successful and nonsuccessful parolee. 7. The amount of time served was measured by the total consecutive months that the individual had served since last enter- ing the institution. The range in time served was from 4% months to 20 years. The parolee who had served 20 years was the only inmate who had served beyond ten years and this extreme case resulted in a considerable variation in the means computed. For that reason the medians were also computed. The mean, standard deviation, and median, in that order, for each of the parole categories are shown in Table XXXV. CR's were not computed for the mean differences because the writer felt that the extreme case would tend to invalidate the CR's. A comparison of the medians, which do not give such great weight to the extreme case, indicates that the dif- ferences were very slight. 8. The previous criminal record was divided into twenty- seven classifications, ranging from those with no previous history of any kind to those with five previous prison sentences. The indi- vidual was placed in a classification that indicated the extent of his previous criminal history. The most significant finding was that 80.2 percent of those who had no previous criminal history of any kind were making a success of their paroles. The difference be- tween this success rate and the average success rate, which was 63.5 percent. was 16.7 percent. The CR of this difference was 3.64 with a level of_significance of .9996, an extremely significant dif- ference. The success rate for those that are normally regarded as first offenders, which includes those with no previous record, was 68.3 percent. Without the "no previous record" group the success rate for the so-called “first offenders" drops to 61.6 percent. 175 TABLE XXXV THE MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS, AND MEDIANS OF FIVE PAROLE CATEGORIES ACCORDING TO TIME SERVED Parole Catego rie 3 Means If::il:t:::s Medians Total sample ............. 24.2 20 .70 17.79 Violators ................ 22.4 16.62 17.83 Nonviolators .............. 26.8 22.74 17.78 Best ................. 28.3 27.06 17.96 Doubtful ............... 21.5 14.76 17.56 Comparing the average success rate to the success rate for "first offenders," excluding those with no previous history, results in a difference of 1.9 percent with a CR of .39 and a level of signifi- cance of .303. This difference is not significant and implies that the so-called ”first offender" group, when the individuals with no previous history are not included, succeed on parole at a rate com- parable to that of the average parolee. There were sixteen individuals who had served three previous ' sentences or more at the time of their present incarceration. Sixty- two and five-tenths percent of these individuals were making a suc- cess of their paroles. The CR of the difference between the success rate of the first offenders, which was 68.3 percent, and the success rate of those individuals with three previous sentences or more is .49 with a level of significance of .376. The difference in the I76 success rate of the "first offenders," excluding those with no pre- vious record, and the offenders with three previous sentences or more, is 0.9 percent. The CR of this difference is .07, with a level of significance of .056. However, there were only sixteen cases in the group with three previous sentences or more, which means that conclusions based on these figures are questionable. 9. There were a total of forty-nine factors included in the five inventories administered. The sample means and the norma- tive means were compared in terms of which score was the most desirable. However, no test of significance was used in this com- parison. It was discovered that the score of the normative popula- tions was the most desirable in thirty-six of the forty-nine factors. In other words, the sample population score was less desirable than the normative p0pulation score on a little less than 75 percent of the factors. 10. .Both the "best" and the "doubtful" groups of the suc- cessful parolees were significantly more masculine in their attitudes and interests than the violators, according to the way this factor is scored by the Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN. 11. In the factor described by Guilford-Martin as indicating a lack of inferiority feelings or, conversely, the feeling of self- confidence, the "best" group was significantly more free from inferiority feelings than the violators. 12. The Guilford-Martin factor N is reported as indicating a lack of nervous tenseness or irritability. In this regard, the ”best" group was significantly less irritable or bothered by ner- vous tenseness than were the violators. 13. There were no significant differences for the Guilford- Martin factor of General Pressure for Overt Activity or for the Guilford-Martin factor of Ascendancy in Social Situations. I77 14. There were no significant differences for any of the Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR. Apparently, such factors as Social Introversion-Extraversion, Thinking Introversion-Extraversion, Depression, Cycloid disposition, and Rhathymia, as measured by this inventory, do not differentiate between the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolee s. 15. Both the "best" and the "doubtful" groups were signif- icantly more Sympathetic than the violators, according to the Johnson Temperament Analysis. 16. The violator was significantly more nervous than the “best," according to the way this factor is measured by the Johnson Temperament Analysis. This is in agreement with the finding on the Guilford-Martin inventory for this factor. 17. The differences between the successful and the nonsuc- cessful parolees on the remaining traits measured by the Johnson Temperament Analysis did not prove to be significant. Therefore, such traits as Depressive, Active, Cordial, Subjective, Aggressive, Critical, and Self-Mastery, as measured by this inventory, did not differentiate between those who succeeded on parole and those who failed. 18. Significant differences were found for one of the factors of the California Mental Health Analysis. The "best" of the parolees was significantly more satisfied in work and recreation than either the ”doubtful" or the violator groups. 19. None of the differences for the other traits of the Cal- ifornia Mental Health Analysis was significant. This means that such traits as Behavioral Immaturity, Emotional Immaturity, Insta- bility, Feelings of Inadequacy, Physical Defects, Outlook and Goals, Close Personal Relationships, Interpersonal Skills, and Social 178 Participation, as measured by this inventory, did not differentiate between successful and nonsuccessful parolees. 20. The average score for the violators on the Psychopathic Deviate scale, as measured by the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, was significantly higher than the score for either the "best" or the ”doubtful." There were no other significant differences on the traits 21. Such measured by the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. subtests as the Lie score, the Validity score, and the Correction score, the Hypochandriasis scale, the Depression scale, the Hysteria scale, the Interest scales, the Paranoia scale, the Psychasthenia scale, the Schizophrenia scale, and the Hypomania scale, as repre- sented in this inventory, did not differentiate between successful and unsuccessful parolees. The differentiating characteristics of the three parole 22. groups, according to the traits measured by these inventories, indi- cate that the "best" group are more masculine in attitudes and in- terests than the violators; that they are more confident than the violators; that they are less nervous than the violators; that they are more sympathetic than the violators; that they are more satis- fied with work and recreation than either the violators or the "doubtful" group; and that they indicate less psychopathic deviation . than the violators. The distinguishing characteristics of the "doubtful" group are that they are more like the "best" in that they are more mas- culine in attitudes and interests; and also in that they are more Like the "best" they also indicate sympathetic than the violators. less psychopathic deviation than the violators. However, on one trait they are more like the violators in that they are also signifi- cantly less satisfied with work and recreation than are the "best." 1“.“5. LN Inn-In. "am 1" u r L L . 179 The violator is distinguished by less masculine attitudes than either the ”best” or the ”doubtful" groups; by more feelings of inferiority than the "best" group; by more nervousness than the "best"; by not appearing as sympathetic as either the "best" or the "doubtful" groups; by not being as satisfied with work and rec- reation as the "best"; and by indicating more psychopathic devia- tion than all of the successful parolees, both the "best" and the "doubtful" groups. 23. As a result of the item analysis, it was found that 102 items significantly differentiated between the successful and non- successful parolees at the .95 (5 percent) level of significance, or better. There were 132. items that significantly differentiated be- tween successful and nonsuccessful parolees at the .93 (7 percent) level of significance, or better. The examiner adopted the latter level of significance as the criterion for this study. 24. The successful and nonsuccessful parolees had been identified from the parole office records after each individual in the sample population had served at least one year of parole. The violation rate at the end of one year was found to be 36.5 percent. The parole office files were again examined at the end of eighteen months so that a new identification of successful and nonsuccessful parolees could be made. At this time it was found that the parole violation rate had increased 4.9 percent in the additional six months, making the new violation rate 41.4 percent. 25. It was found that the number of reSponses for each of the alternate responses of an item were not always consistent for Consequently, the significant difference, in some cases, each item. This meant was for one or the other of the responses, but not both. that for scoring purposes, the specific reSponse had to be the one scored and the method of scoring depended on whether the difference 180 was more related to parole failure or parole success. As a result, those reaponses where the significant difference was related to suc- cess were designated in the scoring key as positive (+) items and those that were more related to parole failure as negative (-) items The items on‘ which there were found significant differences between the responses of successful and nonsuccessful parolees were assigned weights according to the size of the CR of the differences. Each item, as a result of these two scoring methods, was scored a :L-l, 1:2, or 1:3. 26 There were 384 complete sets of answer sheets. These complete sets of answer sheets were scored for the items with sig- nificant differences. The mean score for the entire group was 43.50 with a standard deviation of 16.20. The mean score of the violators, as identified at the end of_eighteen months of parole period, was The mean score for the 34.52 with a standard deviation of 16.20. The CR nonviolators was 49.31 with a standard deviation of 15.05. of the difference in the means of the violators and the nonviolators was 9.01, indicating an extremely significant difference. 27. The mean score for the nonviolators exceeded the scores .of 82 percent of the violators. off" scores and then compared the scores for the two groups to the The investigator applied various "cut- parole outcomes to determine what the results might have been if the scale and the cut-off score had been applied to this group before parole. For a cut-off score of 35, the scale would have predicted the outcome of approximately 81 percent of those who were success- ful but would have missed the outcome of 54 percent of the violators. A cut-off score of 40 would have predicted 73 percent of those who were going to stay out and 60 percent of the violators. On the other hand, a cut-off score of 45 would have correctly identified 67 percent 181 of those who became successful and 72 percent of those who violated their paroles. If only those inmates were paroled who obtained a score higher than the various cut-off scores, the resulting parole violation rates would have proved very interesting. A cut-off score of 35 would have reduced the parole violation rate from 41.4 percent to 32 per- cent. Placing the cut-off score at 40 would have resulted in a pa- role violation rate of 27.6 percent, while a cut-off score of 45 would have reduced the violation rate to 22.9 percent. 28. The biserial coefficient of correlation between the total scores for those ‘items that had significantly differentiated between the successful and nonsuccessful parolees and parole outcome was .638, with a standard error of .044, which indicates such use of the items would have some validity. 29. The reliability coefficient, according to the Kuder and Richardson Case IV formula, was .872, and the reliability coeffi- cient, according to a formula developed by Froelich, was .895. These estimates of reliability indicate that the results of this scale or set of items would have a satisfactory reliability for this type of instrument . Conclusions The first hypothesis of the study, that there are differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees which can be measured objectively by available standardized inventories has been confirmed. Individuals respond to the items of a questionnaire- type inventory by indicating how they think about themselves, how they think other people regard them, how they feel about their role in society, how they feel about their relationship with other people, 182 and the things they believe about the way other people think. The fact that there were significant differences between the responses to individual items by the successful parolees and the nonsuccessful parolees establishes, within the limitations discussed below, that there are objectively measurable differences between the thinking of successful parolees and the nonsuccessful parolees. The second hypothesis, that the nature and extent of such dif- ferences will have some reliability and validity as predictors of parole success or failure is also confirmed. The biserial coeffi- cient of correlation measuring the relationship of the total weighted scores on the item for which there were significant differences be- tween the responses of successful and nonsuccessful parolees to parole outcome was of sufficient magnitude so that predictions based on these scores could be accepted with confidence. The two esti- mations of reliability are minimal estimations and are high enough to indicate that the results would be fairly consistent. It should be pointed out, at this time, that these conclusions are drawn from group findings and, as a result, are applicabl‘eito group predictions only. The significant differences on which these findings are based are differences between means and percentages, and as such, cannot be applied, with certainty, to individual predic- tions. It should be recognized that there is considerable overlapping in the distribution of figures on which means and percentages are based and as long as there is such overlapping any predictions based on such figures can be utilized only as group predictions. This study did not investigate the possibility of individual prediction as it was the purpose to determine whether there are differences in the thinking of parolees at the time of parole consid- eration, and if there are, what the nature of those differences are and if they would have predictive value. There are differences and 183 the differences are such as to suggest that a scale like the one used here would have some value in the prediction of parole outcome. The fact that the findings are based on group predictions does not invali- date them as an additional kind of information that could be consid- ered by a parole board in determining the parolability of an individ- ual. It has been demonstrated, for example, that the use of various cut-off scores would result in increasingly higher success rates. The parole board would also know that the prospective parolee's score was above or below the cut-off score and that a certain per- centage above that score are successful on parole while a certain percentage of those with scores lower than the cut-off score are failures. Of course, this information would have to be related to the total information available regarding the individual, but it is an additional type of information that has not previously been available. The value of such an instrument would depend, in part at least, on how truthfully the prospective parolee answers the items. Otherwise, his score might indicate how well he can fake his answers. The major criticism of the use of a questionnaire-type instrument in situations of this nature is that it is comparatively easy for the testee to answer the items to his own advantage, especially in situ- ations where so much depends on the outcome. The possibility of such faking would depend on the subtlety of the questions, among other things. The "good" responses on this scale were determined from the results of the item analysis and did not result in any consistent pattern of responses. Consequently, faking would probably be difficult on this scale since the prospective parolee presumably would not know what kinds of responses are ”good." There are other factors that should be taken into consideration in a discussion of the limitations of this study which might possibly have some effect on the results. The first of these was an error in 184 technique. Unfortunately, the writer, in his eagerness to make use of all the available test papers in the random sampling to select the papers used in the item analysis, did not use complete sets of test papers, i.e., sets with five answer sheets for each subject. Conse- quently, almost all of the complete sets of test papers that were later available for a cross validation had one or more papers which had been used in the item analysis. It was pointed out that 870 of the 1,920 answer sheets used in the cross validation had also been used in the item analysis, or 45.3 percent of the total. This is not true cross validation, which actually means trying out the results of the item analysis on a completely independent set of papers. The fact that almost half of the papers used in the validation of the dis- criminatory value of the items were papers which had been used in the selection of the items in the first place undoubtedly tends to give a Spurious effect to the results. Therefore, a true cross validation on a new set of subjects is imperative. A. further limitation lies in the fact that at the time of the analyses included in the study, some of the potential parole violators were identified as successful and regarded as such in the study. The reader will remember that the classifications of successful and non— successful parolees were made after all the subjects had been on parole at least one year. It was pointed out that previous parole statistics for Michigan indicated that approximately 75 percent of those who were going to violate parole would have done so at the end of the first year. At the end of one year 36.5 percent of the original group had violated parole. show that eventually in the neighborhood of 45 percent will violate Recent statistics of Michigan's parolees parole. Since the analysis of the subtest scores and of the individual items was based on classifications as successful or nonsuccessful at the end of one year, the critical reader might question whether or not 185 the differences in the thinking of the two groups would still be sta- tistically significant afterpthose parolees in the successful group who were to become violators had been reclassified. There is a possi- bility that the final classification would change the statistics involved in the conclusions of this study and such possibility should be kept in mind as a limitation of the study when the conclusions are con- sidered. Recommendations for Application A scale could be devised which consisted of the items found by this investigation to differentiate significantly between the thinking The writer of the potentially successful and nonsucessful parolees. would recommend that such a scale be devised and put into use by the Michigan Department of Corrections. For the present, the writer recommends that an individual's score should not be regarded as a negative indication of parolability except in the case of those with extremely low scores. The purpose of obtaining the scores of sev- eral thousand parolees, for example, all those paroled in a given year, would be to obtain data for the preparation of an expectancy table based on the relationship of the scores on the scale of these An several thousand inmates and their ultimate parole outcome. expectancy table based on the experience of several thousand parol- ees would make available to the parole board an indication of the expected probability of success and nonsuccess for those who ob- In other words, an individual's score could tained a certain score. interpreted in terms of the expected rate of success or fail- then be are for a given score-class as determined by the experiences of the sample on which the table was based. f 186 The score would indicate the readiness of the individual for parole as indicated by the degree that his thinking is like that of the successful parolee or like that of the nonsuccessful parolee, as determined by the percentages of successful or nonsuccessful parol- ees with like scores. It is recognized that this is but a limited sampling of the individual's thinking and that it does not take into account such things as the individual's motivation for success or failure, the deterrent effect of the incarceration, the degree of determination to make good, and the reaction that an individual will have toward friends or relatives who will influence him in one direc- tion or the other. Consequently, the parole board would have to de- termine what effect these and other relevant factors might have on the individual, in relation to this thinking at the time of parole. In other words, this information regarding the type of thinking of the individual as interpreted in terms of expected success or failure, based on past experience with others of like scores, would be an additional factor to be considered by the parole board with all other information at their disposal. An indication that an individual's thinking was like that of the successful or the nonsuccessful parolees, eXpressed in terms of the parole outcome of those with like scores, would be of value to a parole board in helping to reach a decision as to whether or not an individual would be a good parole risk. One of the major uses of the proposed scale would be as an aid to the parole board in a de- cision regarding parolability at the initial parole hearing on the expiration of the minimum sentence. A finding that the individual's score indicated a likelihood for failure, based on past experience, might suggest a need for continued treatment. A scale of this nature would also aid in the discovery of those cases that might be considered for parole before the eXpiration of the 187 minimum sentence. The results of the scale might also be one of the deciding factors in the determination of how soon a parole vio- lator is ready to return to society. In other words, a scale that would give a valid interpretation, in terms of eXpected parole out- come based on past experience, of a prospective parolee's thinking at any time he is being considered for release would be a valuable aid to a parole board in reaching a decision regarding the parola- bility of that individual. Recommendations for Further Study The previous discussion has already pointed out the fact that the items found by this investigation to differentiate significantly be- tween successful and nonsuccessful parolees should be validated by comparing the scores of several thousand parolees with their ulti- mate success or failure on parole. This should be done so that there can be continuous refinement of the scale, so those items which no longer continued to differentiate reliably between success- ful and nonsuccessful parolees could be eliminated from it. The fact that this study establishes that differences in the thinking of successful and nonsuccessful parolees can be measured objectively should lead to the development of more scales devised specifically for this purpose. It is recognized that the present form of the items discovered through this study is not the most advan- tageous. However, changing the items in any way so that they would be alike in form might seriously affect the validity of the scale. Consequently, the writer suggests experimentation with other forms of the items to determine whether or not a uniform molding of the item would be equally effective. 188 It is quite possible that an analysis of the items found by this study to distinguish between successful and nonsuccessful parolees would reveal specific areas or traits that would even more sharply forecast parole outcome. A. study of these items might possibly re- veal some of the differences in the basic dynamics of the behavior of the successful and nonsuccessful parolees. Experiments of the nature of the present investigation should be conducted with other scales composed of traits not included in this study. There may be other areas of thinking that could be objectively measured that would also differentiate between parole success and failure. The most discriminating of any additional items discovered might then be combined with the most discriminating of the present items in a further refinement of this method of parole prediction. During the process of the present investigation, there was another area of which the writer became aware that needed further investigation. It was found that the parole success rate of the first offenders, excluding those with no previous criminal history, was very much like the success rate of individuals who had served at least three previous sentences. However, there were only sixteen cases in the latter group, and the results cannot be accepted with confidence. It seems to the writer that this has important impli- cations for the determination of parolability. As a rule, the first offenders, which includes all those who have not previously served a prison sentence, regardless of other criminal history, are regarded as better parole risks than repeating offenders. However, the im- plication of the finding of this study, that it is only the offender with no previous history of any kind who is the better risk, while the "first offender" who has a previous record but not to the extent of a previous prison sentence, has a violation rate equal to that of repeating offenders, certainly has strong implications for parole 189 decisions. A comparison of the success rates of several hundred individuals in these three categories might be enlightening. BIBLIOGRAPHY Adkins, Dorothy, et al. Construction and Analysis of Achievement Tests. Washington: United States Government Printing Office, 1947, 292 pp. Allen, Robert M. "A Review of Parole Prediction Literature," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 32:548-554, Jan- uary, 1942. Allen, Robert M. "Problems of Parole," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminoloiy, 38:7-13, March, 1947. Barnes, Harry E., and Negley K. Teeters. New Horizons in Crim— inology. New York: Prentice Hall, 1943, 1061 pp. Bates, Sanford. Prisons and Bgyond. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1936, 327 pp. Borden, Howard G. ”Factors for Predicting Parole Success," Jour- - nal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 19:328-336, November, 1928. Branham, Vernon C., and Samuel B. Kutash, editors. Encyclopedia of Criminolm. New York: Philosophical Library, 1949, 521 pp. .Burgess, Ernest W. "Factors Determining Success or Failure on Parole," Part IV, in the workings of the Indeterminate Sen- tence Law and the Parole System in Illinois by Andrew A. Bruce and others, Journal of Criminal Law and Crim_ino_lpg_y, 19:214-286, March, 1928. Burgess, Ernest W. "Is Prediction Feasible in Social Work," Social Forces, 7:533-545, June, 1929. Burgess, W. C. "Here's A Challenge!" Rotarian, 71:2-3, July, 1947. 191 Euros, Oscar Krisen, editor. The Third Mental Measurement Year- book. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press, 1949, pp. 23-114. Capwell, Dora F. ”Personality Patterns of Adolescent Girls: De- linquents and Nondelinquents," in Analyzing: and Predicting Juvenile Delinquengr with the MMPI, by Starke R. Hathaway and Elio D. Monachesi. Minneapolis: University of Minne- sota Press, 1953, pp. 29-36. Dressler, David. Probation and Parole. New York: Columbia Uni- versity Press, 1951, 337 pp. Dressler, David. Parole Chief. New York: The Viking Press, 1951, 310 pp. Edgerton, Harold A., and Donald G. Patterson. "Table of Standard Errors and Probable Errors of Percentages for Numbers of Cases," reprinted from The Journal of Applied Psychology, 102378-391, September, 1926. Forlano, G., and R. Pintner. "Selection of Upper and Lower Groups for Item Validation," Journal of Educational chhology, 32: 344-349, October, 1941. Froelich, G. J. "A Simple Index of Test Reliability," Journal of Educational Psychology, 32:381—385, May, 1941. Garrett, Henry E. Statistics in Psychology and Education. New York: Longmans, Green and Company, 1953, 454 pp. Glueck, Sheldon, and Eleanor T. Glueck. Five Hundred Criminal Careers. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1930, 365 pp. Glueck, Sheldon, and Eleanor T. Glueck. Five Hundred Delinquent Women. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1934, 537 pp. Glueck, Sheldon, and Eleanor T. Glueck. One Thousand Juvenile Delinquents. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1934, 341 pp. Glueck, Sheldon, and Eleanor T. Glueck. Later Criminal Careers. New York: The Commonwealth Fund, 1937, 403 pp. 192 Glueck, Sheldon, and Eleanor T. Glueck. Juvenile Delinquents Grown Up. New York: The Commonwealth Fund, 1940, 330 pp. Glueck, Sheldon, and Eleanor T. Glueck. Criminal Careers in Retro- spect. New York: The Commonwealth Fund, 1943, 218 pp. Glueck, Sheldon, and Eleanor T. Glueck. After-conduct of Discharge_d Offenders. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1945, 114 pp. Graham, Mary Ruth. These Came Back. University, Alabama: University of Alabama, 1946, 134 pp. Guilford, J. P. An Inventory of Factors STDCR. Beverly Hills: Sheridan Supply Company, 1940. Guilford, J. P., and H. G. Martin. The Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN. Beverly Hills: Sheridan Supply Com- pany, 1943. vHakeem, Michael. "The Validity of the Burgess Method of Parole Prediction," American Journal of Sociology, 53:374-386, March, 1948. Hart, Hornell. "Predicting Parole Success," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 14:405—414, November, 1923. Hathaway, Starke R., and J. Charnley McKinley. The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. New York: The Psycho- logical Corporation, 194 3. Hathaway, Starke R., and Elio D. Monachesi. Analyzing and Predict- ing Juvenile Delincmency with the MMPI. Minneapolis: Uni- versity of Minnesota Press, 1953, 153 pp. , Huff, Ray L. "Is Parole Prediction a Science," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 37:384-389, January, 1947. Johnson, Roswell H. Johnson Temperament Analysis. Los Angeles: California Test Bureau, 1944. T. L. ”The Selection of Upper and Lower Groups for the Validation of Test Items," Journal of Educational Psychology, 30:17-24, January, 1939. Kelley. 193 Kelso, Robert W. "How to Predict Behavior on Parole," Survey, 61: 373-374, December, 1928. Kuder, G. F., and M. W. Richardson. "Theory of the Estimation of Test Reliability," Pchhometrika, 2:251-260, September, 1937. Lanne, William F. "Parole Prediction as Science," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 26:377-400, September, 1935. La Roe, Wilber. Parole with Honor. Princeton: Princeton Univer— sity Press, 1939, 295 pp. Laune, Ferris F. Predicting Criminality. Evanston: Northwestern University Press, 1936, 163 pp. Laune, Ferris F. "The Scientific Status of Parole Prediction," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 27:214-218. July, 1936. Lindquist, E. F. Statistical Analysis in Educational Research. New York: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1940, 266 pp. Monachesi, Elio D. ”An Evaluation of Recent Major Efforts at Pre- diction," American Sociological Review, 6:478-486, July, 1941. Monachesi, Elio D. "American Studies in the Prediction of Recidi- vism," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 41:268-289, Septembe r , 1950. Ohlin, Lloyd E. Selection for Parole. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1951, 142 pp. Pigeon, Helen D. Probation and Parole in Theory and Practice. New York: National Probation Association, 1942, 420 pp. Ray, Joseph M. "Scientific Parole--A Proposal," Journal of Crim- inal Law and Criminology, 37:384-389, January, 1947. Reckless, Walter c. Criminal Behavior. New York: McGraw, Hill Book Company, 1940, 532 pp. Reckless, Walter C. "The Implications of Prediction in Sociology," American Sociological Review, 6:471-477, August, 1941. 194 Sanders, Barker S. ”Testing Parole Prediction," Proceeding of the Sixty-fifth Annual Congress of the American Prison Association. New York: American Prison Association, pp. 222-233, 1935. Sandiford Peter, and J. A. Long. Validation of Test Items. Toronto: University of Toronto, 1935, 126 pp. ~. Schnur, Alfred C. "Predicting Parole Outcome," Focus, 28:70-75, May. 1949. Sutherland, Edwin H. Principles of Criminology. Third edition. New York: J. B. Lippincott Company, 1939, 634 pp. Thorpe, Louis P., Willis W. Clark, and Ernest W. Tiegs. Mental Health Analysis. Los Angeles: California Test Bureau, 1946. Tibbitts, Clark. “Success or Failure on Parole Can be Predicted," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminolggy, 22:11-50, May, 1932. United States Department of Justice. The Attorney General's Survey of Release Procedures. Vol. 4, "Parole." Washington: Government Printing Office, 1939, 664 pp. United States Department of Justice. National Prisoner Statistics. No. 11. Washington: Government Printing Office, July, 1954, 4 pp. Vold, George B. Prediction Methods and Parole. Hanover, New Hampshire: The Sociological Press, 1931, 135 pp. Vold, George B. ”Do Parole Prediction Tables Work in Practice," Publications of the American Sociological Sociefl, 25:136-138, May, 1931. Vold, George B. "Prediction Methods Applied to Problems of Clas- sification Within Institutions," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 26:202-209, July, 1935. Warner, Sam B. ”Factors Determining Parole From the Massa- chusetts Reformatory," Journal of Criminal Law and Crimi- nology, 14:172-207, August, 1923. Witmer, Helen Leland. ”Some Factors in Success or Failure on Pa- role," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 18:384-403, November, 1927. APPENDIXES 196 APPENDIX A. THE CERTIFICATE OF PAROLE OF THE STATE OF MICHIGAN'S DEPARTMENT OF CORRECTIONS INCLUDING THE CONDITIONS OF PAROLE STATE OF MICHIGAN DEPARTMENT OF CORRECTIONS CERTIFICATE OF PAROLE -- Inmate No ............................. , now confined Whereas -- in is eligible for parole; Therefore, in consideration of the signed promises made by said inmate, We, the Michigan Parole Board, hereby order that the said inmate be paroled after to the confines of ..---__---_______--_.._----_----- County, under the supervision of Parole Officer ........ N we Phone Address for a period of ending providing the conditions of this parole are faithfully carried out. ............................ .---_-__--_--__-_..-_.---------------, County or State We further order that said inmate will proceed to m _- y where he will be employed by Name Address Address Lnd will reside with Name SPECIAL CONDITIONS Given at Lansing under the seal of the Michigan Department of Corrections this of ................. _ MICHIGAN PAROLE BOARD st: . 1 sub-:— -- v — 9190 93999138 among 30 unzouzts 30 aouasald q 'Jooggo along for Kq paiapdo are so 310mg 30 suopmuoo [moods qons 0; do am [1m '11 'KIIHQ'B Km 10 was an 04 £11m; in! 101 award out I ‘01 °s.moq quouosooI dean um I assurance puo ‘s;uauloaom ‘mnoqoosaqm Km JO scum [[813 paumoml .IBOQJO 910mg Km daaq max I 'Joomo 310mg Km mos; isanbas .Io .IsiiaI [no 0; aouo ;o [Idol [EM ‘ Q} 'JOOQJO along Km )0 uoIssruuad sq; ;noq;I.n pious; eoqod .10 1311; undo o fiuIAoq suosaod .10 ‘uonunrsul Iouad o J0 sensual; ‘saolomd qua e;oogunmuloo .Io moposso ;ou [11ml '8 among 310mg Km :0 uoIssqusd oq'; moms ‘;qap u; paAIOAuI omooaq .lo ‘auo J0 asoqomd eq; .10; [snow opIAOId .IO 2313;qu Joiom [no camp .10 fnq ;ou mAI ’ .H CO 'Ioomo 910mg Km Io uofssrumad sq; ;uoq;pn aloud no .9qu Lmlm Ion In“ ' If: 2193an along Km ;0 nolssiumod sq; ;noq;pa paIOJod um I qoiqn 01 snow .10 [moon sq; saoaI ;ou [MI ' w 'sfian BuImJo;-;Iqoq .Io sonoorou Kuo osn .Iou ssaoxo o; ssfiodoaaq Bugsorxom; asn ;ou mil ' 'Inmmn 9113 919159109 9‘1 1811111 1J0d3-I 91111 ms PWPWI 'papIAoad sumo; no mm 0; nodes Iranian o 8}[BIII mm 1 ‘Jaomo 910mg Km fq pannnsut so .10 ‘q;uouI '8 MO ' CV3 . ammonium in! 10199 -.Ia;u; isaq sq; 0; pm: human; mom won: I ;oq; puo;s.19pun 1 3190930 310mg £111 0; amount) qons 110d“ mm 1 ‘910Iod uo Buipoaoons Km mayo Kotu qoiqm qof .Io OOHOPISBJ £111 To saspto Knuomrp [no II 'uoISSIm axed sgq OAIOOOJ puo .Iaowo alosod Km ;oo;uoo may mm 1 qog .Io oouaprsad aSuoqo o; hossmau 1! p119 III '5 'po;on.l;sug so mango 910mg Km ;oo;uoo KIoIoIpouImI mm I uogoupssp [m ;o resins uodfl '1 :;oq; soJBo pus ssIude lqmq op 1 mail 'EIHEIH (IEILNIHJ H'IOHVJ d0 SNOICLIGNOO (IIVS EIHJI DNIGNVLSHEIGND CINV GIN OJ: (William DNIAVH so (1qu {)NIAVH (INV ‘uosl-Id 01 trauma! 3am Km III Imus-I Km etc-rad sun 10 “09??“ sq; :0 no 0; do can 0; amps; ;oq; ofipapnouq no; Bupmq puo ‘OIOJ‘Bd uodn asoalas [In so; uonompgsuofll 197 APPENDIX B THE INVENTORIES USED IN THE PRESENT INVESTIGATION 1. The Mental Health Analysis. 2. The Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN. 3. An Inventory of Factors STDCR. 4. The Johnson Temperament Analysis. 5. The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. MENTAL HEALTH ANALYSIS—Adult Series, Form A W by Louis P. Thorpe and Willis W. Clark Ernest W. Tiegs, Consultant Do not write on this booklet mules: told to do so by the examiner. irections: If you are to use a special answer sheet, the method of answering questions is explained I the answer sheet. If you are to mark youf answers on this booklet, the questions will be rswered by making a cutie around the YES or NO. Do the following examples: Have you ever been to a mov- On some of them you will make a circle around lug picture theatcrl YES NO YES, and on others you will make a circle Are Y?" 1333 than 311“” years around NO. When told to begin you are 0 38¢ YES NO to go right on from one page to another until 0n the following pages are more questions. you have finished them all. ‘ me ......................................................................... O ccupation or Grade .............................................. I0 ......................................... . ............. Age ................ Birthday .................................................. Sex: M—F Month Day Iminer ................................................................... Organization........... .................................................. PERCENTILE 3:; (Chart percentile rank llerel Score lie 1 5 lo 20 fl 4Q 50 00 70 N .0 I. Lib......... IIIIPIIIIIIII A. m. In. . . . . . m___ . . __ I....I..--I I I I ........ I I I I I I I (Freedom from) . o. Em. Ins. ..... (m)___ . . ...... I....I....I I I I I I I. I I I I (Fractionation!) C. Fl. Ina. ..... (n) __ I.......II I I I I ........ I I - I I I I Mutton) o. rt. Def. ..... (o) _ I....I....I..- I ...I I l I II ...I....I..-I (Freedom from) E. Nor. Man. . . . . (p)___ . . __ III I I I I I I I I........II ( mm, 1 5 10 20 N 40 50 CO 70 fl .0 '. A“. - - - - I I I I I I I I I I I I I A. CLPer. Rel. . . . . (a) _ I----I----I I- I I I I I I I I I B. lntp. Sk. . . . . . (bl___ . . _ l.---..--l|---.- I.......I I I I I I I I I :. Soc. Par. ..... (cl _ I....I....I ........ I ........ I ........ I ........ I ........ I ........ I ........ I ........ I....I....I >. Sat. w. and n. . . . m__ . . _ I....I....I ........ I ........ I ........ I I I I ........ I ........ I....I....I ' 0t. and GI: ..... (o) __ I...I...I I I I I I I ..I I......I| lScore...... —-——IIIIIIIIIIIII l 5 10 20 ” 40 50 CO 70 N N I” PERCENTILE fluid“. 19“. by California 'reet Bureau. Wt under International t Union. snowman-m tUnIon. mmgcuuomu “mulewmmmunm. Printedtnu.8.A. "I I‘ _ A a ;'___'f '1. arm. 10. 14. ll. 12. 13. Do several people seem to think that you are making a success of your work? YES NO Have you found that you can . talk freely with one or more of the people with whom you are associated? YES NO I Do you often have to start eat- ing a meal before the others be- cause they make you wait so long? YES NO I Have you found that you can succeed better by getting tough when someone tries to take ad- vantage of you? I Is someone willing to help you in choosing clothes and other personal effects? YES NO I Are conditions affecting you often so bad that you feel as though life is hardly worth living? NO Have you found that your feel- ings, or moods, about life fre- quently change? YES NO Are you often worried about possible dangers that you can- not control? . YES NO Can you keep people from feel- ing too embarrassed when they make a mistake? ‘flS NO Do you usually keep from show- ing that you dislike to lose in games or contests? Do people often expect so much of you that you are forced to make a good many excuses? Would you rather be with a group of people. than find en- tertainment for yourself? ' Do you prefer activities requir- rng cooperation to those in WhICl'l you work alone? Have you found it difficult to get acquainted with the people you would like to have as friends? YES NO. ‘15. 16. 18.. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. the required amount of time YES .Do you usually take part or have definite interest in one or more active sports? YES NO I Do you find that the type of work you are doing rs suflici- ently interesting? Have you often felt less at- tractive than others because of the shape of your legs? YESONO Are you concerned because you are too tall to look right wrth most of your friends? 55 N0 0 Are you sensitive because of skin blemishes that detract from your appearance? Do you like to spend more than on your work? Do you usually refrain from do- ing the things you believe to be wrong? DO you believe that you should always be honest in your deal- YES NO ings with people? ' . Do you have a difficult time YES NC sleeping? 9 DO you believe that people have a right to do what they like so long as they do not m-P terfere with the rights of others. Do you have the habit 0‘ bit. YES NO ing your fingernalls? I I Are you usually quick enouglI to get a good seat at a show YES No other gathering? I it easy ‘0 le in. lines at or Other 15 N0 I “so Have you found get ahead of peop games, theatres, places? ‘ ore Do you have one or m ? YES ”I I close friends of your Own sex. {your Do you feel that some 0 - d- neighbors deserve your men will ship? 5.....- c/ L/ V NoI g :7 30. _. 32. 33. f. 34. 37. ll. 12. Do you have a friend who will talk with you about your problems? Do you often feel as though you are held back from doing things that you would like to - do? - Do you usually make a point of finding out what your friends like to do? Do your friends seem to think that you have a good sense of humor? Do you often become so con- cerned with your own prob- lems that you fail to notice the people around you? Have you found it difficult to settle on definite beliefs con- cerning life? Are you a member of a club or other group where people do Interesting things together? Have you found that you get along best when people are willing to give you a loan from time to time? Do you usually take an active part in things rather than think or read about them? ‘ Are you a working member of the Red Cross or some other organization which assists underprivileged people? Does it seem to you that most pe0ple like to compete with others more than you do? Do you feel unattractive be- cause you have a protruding or receding chm? Do you feel that you have the Opportunity of doing many of the things that make life inter- esting? Do you usually feel good after you have worked hard? C. I! r 44. res NO 45. res NO ‘ w m 46. res NO 47' ' b res NO 48' b 49. res NO to 50. res NO 51- In res, NO 52- res NO 53' res NO 54- . . res N0 55- res NO .. 56. res NO ° 57. resto 58. res NO d _3_ Are you satisfied with the type of work you are doing because it will lead to something better? YES NO Have you been unhappy be- cause you are not considered as 800d lOOking as you would like? YES NO Have you noticed that you hum a great deal of the time? Do you believe that everyone should have equal rights under the law? Do you believe that everyone has the opportunity of making an honest living? Do you sometimes have dizzy speHs? Do you find it hard to sit still for long? Have you frequently been able. to get even with people you dis- like by ignoring them? Have you found that it pays to avoid people who try to pre— vent you from doing what you like? Do you know someone with whom you can talk over your business or other problems? Have you found that someone else will usually get the things you want if you don’t beat them to it? Have you found that it is usually better to stay away from home for awhile when someone has been unfair to you? Do you make a practice of showing people that you recog- nize their abilities? Have you found that it is well to tell people when they show good judgment? Do you often find it necessary to defend your beliefs in the face of strong opposition? b L d a NO res res NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO {1” v— v-.. 59. 61. 62. 63. 65. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. Do you usually find that it is much more pleasant to think about necessary activities than to engage in them? Do you enjoy helping people out of their difficulties? Do you often feel that members of your family do not think as well of you as they should? Do many of your associates seem to think that their ideas are better than yours? Do people often claim that they are more competent in their work than you are? Do you sometimes travel or go camping with people of your own age? Does it seem that you are left out of things you would like very much to be in? Is the kind of work you are do- ing easy enough so that you can do it well? Do you prefer to keep your job or present kind of work to seek- ing employment that pays more money? Are you worried about things that are said about you be- cause you are too thin? Are you concerned about com- ments made because you are overweight? Are you troubled because peo— ple seem to notice that your teeth are uneven or otherwise unattractive? Do you believe that everyone should be loyal to his friends and associates? YES NO 81. res NO 82- 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 80. 83. 84. 85. cu get a great deal of relic for your eyes by squint- ing? YES NO Do you believe that people who do what is right will event- ually be rewarded? Do you believe that people have a right to decide things for themselves so long as they do not harm anyone else? Do you constantly make plans for carrying on a successful career? Are you usually consulted con- cerning things members of your family are planning to do? Have you often found that by stirring up a little trouble you can get what you want with- out delay? Do you have a number of friends among members of the opposite sex? Do you frequently have good times with fellow members of some group or organization? Have you found that it is sur- prisingly easy to hurt other people’s feelings? Do you make a practice of go- ing out of your way to help people? Are you frequently dissatisfied because your plans do not work out satisfactorily? Do you often feel depressed without knowing the reason for your feelings? Have you often become so ab- sorbed in personal thoughts that you failed to notice what was going on around you? Do you make a practice of giv- ing people credit for the things they know? YESNO YESNO YES NO YES NO YES N0 1 YESNO e YESNO mm .337. \Kn‘, Do you take part in the pro- grams of a service club, work- er’s organization, or other such noun? Do you have a group of good friends in addition to the one or two persons you know best? . Are you often disturbed be- cause people act as though they cannot depend on you? Do you go to dances or other socials when you have the chance? Have you found that you can do your best work when you have the help of friends? . Are you troubled because peo- ple notice something wrong With your mouth or lips? Are you worried because there rs something wrong with your feet or legs that it is difficult to conceal? Do you usually look forward wrth pleasure to the duties of the next day? Does it seem to you that you are making satisfactory prog- ress in your work responsibili- tics? Do You have as much time for Play and recreation as you should? 19° You stutter some of the time? Have you found that you are tired much of the time? Do you believ e that eve one should be kind to animals? 90 YOU often find drumming” or talking to yourself With your fingers yourself? 100. YES NO 101. res no 102. YES NO NO YES NO YES NO‘ YES NO YES NO YES NO 4 YES NO YES NO 9 YES NO YES NO e 113. YES NO P _5_ 103. 104. 105. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. Are you frequently bothered by eyestrain? YES Do accidents or injuries seem to hurt you more than they do most people? No Have you found that you usually get what you want . most quickly by demanding it? YES NO Do you have some'close friends among your relatives? YES NO Do you feel that most mem- bers of your family think as well of yOu as they should? Y‘ Do you have so many problems that you are often justified in stretching the truth a little in solving them? YES NO Do you enjoy letting people know when they have done something well? YES Can you usually stop a quarrel between two people without hurting their feelings? YES 3 No Does it usually take you con- siderable time to get over dis- appointments? YES NO Have you often had the feeling that you do not get what is coming to you in life? YES NO Do you find it very difficult to relax and take things easier? YES N0 0 Do you usually prefer working with a group rather than alone? YES NO Q Have you usually found it dif- ficult to ‘get along with persons of the opposite sex? YES NO Have you found that you can get out of unpleasant responsi- bilities by appearing to be be- low par physrcally? YES N0 0 h r: d n 114. 115. 116. 1.17. 118. 119. 120. 121. . 122. 123. 124. 125. 126. 127. 128. gum—_n Do you call on your friends when you have the time? Do , your friends appear to think that you are good at get- ting things done in group ac- tivities? Are you troubled because peo- ple notice that you have scars ' . that show? ~ Do you have the opportunity of seeing your favorite kinds of motion pictures? Do you listen regularly to some of your favorite radio programs? Are you troubled because your hair rs either too thin, straight, or curly to be attractive? Have you been troubled be- cause of things people say about the color of your hair? Do you believe that everyone should receive enough free edu- cation to fit him for demo- cratic living? Do you believe that people of other colors, races, and beliefs are entitled to their rights? Are you often troubled by a buzzing sound in your ears? Are you sometimes troubled with nightmares? Do you believe that you should obey the laws even though you do not agree with them? a Do you usually prefer to do things on the spur of the mo- ment rather than after plan- ning them? Do the members of your fam- ily seem to believe in your honesty and truthfulness? Do you often have good times at social gatherings in your home? P YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO res no YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO 9 YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO 129. 130. 131. 132. 133. 134. 135. 136. 137. 138. 139. MO. 141., _5_. Have you found that it usually pays to tell peOple frankly about your likes and dislikes? YESI N0 Have you found that many people are unreasonable in ex- pecting you to keep your feel- ings to yourself? . Are you often concerned about what the future may have in store for you? Do you often help people have a good time at social affairs? YESDNO Are you usually careful of YES YES what you say, about your YES friends? Do you usually show an inter- est in the things your acquaintf YES ances are doing? Have you often felt that you have more than your share of Y bad luck? Do you engage in one or more hobbies in which some of your friends are interested? Does it seem to you that most of your associates are more at- tractive physically than you are? Does it seem to you that most people think about themselves and tend. to forget others? Do you find it difficult to se- cure as much attention as most Y people do? Do you sometimes go to con- certs, lectures, or entertain- ments with a group of ac- quaintances? Do you like to spend part of your time painting, drawmg, or writing? Are your duties often so inter- esting that you like to work Y hard? 0 h 1: YES YESNO 3N0; YES 9 YES 4 d d__——l-——-" ESNO m N0 N0 es No- my NI I [see I I I J l__ Ill u_____n Are you concerned because of difficulty in controlling muscles of your hands or feet? . YES NO Do you feel discouraged be- " cause poor eyesight keeps you from doing your best work? Do you feel your work is so important that you do it well? Do you occasionally feel that your muscles are trembling? Do you seem to catch cold easily? Are certain of your religious beliefs so fundamental that you would not care to change them? Do you consider good 'charac- ter as important as knowledge or skill? Do you often feel a tension in your chest? If you were in need of financial assistance, do you know of someone who would help you? Are many people so unpleasant that you are justified in treat- mg them with indifference? Do you have any brothers, sis- ters, or other close relatives who are as friendly to you as they should be? Do you know any people who can be trusted to keep your secrets? Have you found that you get along best when you don’t . concern yourself much about other people’s feelings? Are you careful not to talk much about the things you can do? ' '19 YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO res NO res NO I YES NO b 157. 158. 159. 160. 161. 162. 163. 164. 165. 166. 167. 168. 169. 170. _7_ Have you foundthat you often seem both to love and hate some one? ’ Have you found that it is usually inadvisable to tell peo- ple about their faults? Are you often tempted to give up trying to solve your many problems? Have you found ways of get- ting out of many of your un- pleasant duties? YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO Do you sometimes take part in planning or in leading group activities? Do you like to take an active part in civic or political mat- ters? Have you often felt that you need more courage than most people if you are to be success- Do you enjoy carrying on con- YES NO YES NO YES NO versations at group gatherings? YES NO Have you noticed that much good usually comes from your failures? Do you like to spend part of your time workrng in a garden or similar outdoor actrvrty? Do you worry about what peo- ple think because you must wear eyeglasses? Are you concerned because you are too stoop-shouldered to look well? Do you often feel embarrassed because you believe your skin to be unsightly? Do you get'a great deal of plea- sure from raising animals or spending time with pets? 0 I! I: (I YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO 171. 172. 173. 174. 175. 176. 177. 178. 179. 1110. 181. 182. 183. 184. 185. 186. I - l Do you believe in fulfilling hi?" promises even if you dis- ' e very much to do so? Do you believe that there are some acts that are always right and some that are always wrong? Do you frequently wake up with a stiff neck? Do you often have trouble with sneezing spells? Do you often have shooting pains in your head? Do members of your family seem to like your friends? Is there someone to whom you can go if you are in trouble? Do you often talk over per- sonal matters with other peo- ple? Have you found that if you want to be comfortable, you must look out for yourself? Have you found that if you let your work go, someone else will usually help you finish it? Have you found that it is bet- ter not to talk about peeple be- hind their backs? As a rule, do you prefer having people do things for you to do- tng them yourself? Do your friends appear to think that you are fair in your dealings with them? Is it easy for you to be friend- with people even when they isagree with you? Do you make it a point to treat your friends when you have the opportunity of doing so? Do you enjoy trading, buying, or selling things? . YES NO YES NO YES NO YES NO YESNO YESNO YESNO 187. 188. 189. 190. 191. 192. 193. 194. 195. 196. 197. 198. 199. 200. Does it seem to you that most ple can work harder or anger than you can? YES NO Have you found it almost im- possible to be as successful as most of your acquaintances? YES NO Have you found that it is usually someone else’s fault when you are blamed for things? Y3. "0 ( Do you usually like to be where there is much activity? Do you sometimes enjoy your- self by going fishing, camping, or biking? "3"” Do you have good times col- lecting stamps, coins, or other _ objects? ‘I‘o Are you troubled because so many people notice how bow- legged or knock-knead you are? Y5.” Do you often have an enjoy- able time playing a musical in- strument? Are you worried because some- thing is wrong with your hands? "? '0 Do you believe in spending part of your time or money 1n ' assisting worthy causes? 73"” I I Are your arms extremely tense NO I much of the time? YES I ' I rrsNO' Do you frequently have sick headaches? Do you think that family life is essential to the welfare of society? YES NO I Do you feel that there is some I great plan in the universe and that you have a part in it? THE GUILFORD-MARTIN INVENTORY OF FACTORS G A M l N (Abridged Edition) Mr. Mrs. Name Miss Date (Cross out two) Nearest age (encircle): Raw Scores: G A M I 16 20 25 30 35 40' 60 56 60 “7." or “No." C-Scores: -...--" ---- DIRECTIONS: Below you will find some questions which are to be answered by encircling either “Yes" Read each question in turn, think what your opinion or your behavior has usually been, and draw a circle around the answer that best describes your behavior or opinion. Encircle the “i" only when you are unable to decide between the “Yes” and “No." BE SURE TO ANSWER EVERY QUESTION. There is no right answer to any of these questions except the answer that tells how you think or feel about it. 1. 9 9‘???" ”9°.“ 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 26. 26. 27. Do you believe that you know your own characteristics about as well as most peeple know theirs? .................................. . . . . . . . . . . ............................ Yes Does your personality stand out as being quite different from that of other peoplei. .. . Yes When climbing stairs do you often take the steps two at a time? ...................... Yes Do you often feel the need for a rest during the day?................ Yes When you think you recognize someone you see in a public place, do you inquire of him whether you have met him before? ....... . ..... . . . . .............. . . . ........ . . . Yes Do you have one or more abilities in which you believe you are superior to most other people?- . ..... ............. ..................... .......................... . ..... . Yes Do you express such emotions as delight, sorrow, anger, and the like. readilyf. . . . . . . . Yes Do you feel that people almost always treat you right? . . . . . . . . . ......... . . . . . . . . . .. . Yes Do you become very annoyed when you find a window stuck when you want to open it? Yes Do you find it difficult to get rid of a salesman to whom you do not care to listen or give your time? ..... . ............. . ........... . .............. Yes Have you ever kept a personal diary of your own accord?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ...... Yes When going somewhere in an automobile, does it bother you considerably to get caught in slow-moving traffic? .................. Yes Are you afraid of deep water? .................... . ....... . . . . ........................ Yes Are you inclined to be quick in your actions? ..................................... . . Yes Do you frequently feel thwarted because you cannot do as you want to? ......... .. . . . Yes Do you usually hesitate to take a seat in the front of a lecture room or church if to do . so makes you appear conspicuous? ................................................. . . Yes Doyoucryrathereasily?......... .................... Yes Do you believe you have been bossed too much for your own good‘!. . . . . ........ . ..... Yes Are there ever times when you feel so jumpy you could throw things at people if you did not control yourself? ............................ . . . . ....... . . . . . . . ........... . . . . Yes Doyou always knowwhat to do next?. .................... Yes At work or at play. do other people find it hard to keep up with the pace you sett. .. . . . Yes Do you often wish you were stronger so you could “smash" some one who is stronger than you? ........................................... ...... . ................. Yes Do you (or would you) ever haggle over a price with a tradesmen or dealer?. . . . . . . . Yes Do you often find that you can think of smart things to say only after it is too later. . Yes Do you find it easy to start a conversation with a stranger? ..... . . . . . . . . . ..... . . . . . Yes Do you often become irritated over little annoyances?............. Yes If an acquaintance of yours has been spreading untrue and uncomplimentary stories about you,.do you usually “have it out" with the person?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . Yes Copyright I943 by Sheridan Supply Co.. Beverly Hills. Cali]. (Not to be reproduced in rhole or in part billion! nn'iien permission of the copyright em.) Had". ND "00'0"“ H". H HHHH N 0O,“ «swears-e No No No N0 No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No or raccoon @“Q0 10 11 12 13 14 16 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 . . .s _,__- M Page 2 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 58. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. Do you feel deeply sorry for a mistreated horse? ...................................... Yes Do you always feel that you can accomplish the things you want to do? ........ . ...... Yes Are you happiest when you get involved in some project that calls for rapid action‘f. .. Yes Do you sometimes crave something intensely without knowing what it is you want?. .. Yes Do you feel bored much of the time? .......... . ...................................... Yes When a parent. teacher, or boss scolds you, do you ever feel like weeping? ........... Yes Are you rather good at blufling when you find yourself in difficulty?. . .. ............... Yes Are you inclined to be slow and deliberate in movement? ............................ Yes Would you rate yourself as a tense individual?. . . . . ........ - ......................... Yes When you are suddenly upset emotionally, does it take much time to recover your composure? ....................................... . ............................... Yes When a clerk in a store waits on others who should come after you, do you usually call his attention to the fact? ........................................................... . Yes Does it annoy you to hear someone make fun of your clothes? ........................ Yes Have you often felt that you are a rather awkward person? ........................... Yes Do you wake up feeling tired in the morning? ........................................ Yes Do you usually eat more rapidly than the average person, even though there is plenty oft tim e? ............................ . ............. . . . . . ................ . ............. Yes Do you dislike to have people watching you while you are working? .......... . ........ Yes In a group activity do you often find yourself compelled to play an unimportant part‘f. Yes When troubled or upset because things go wrong. are you inclined to suffer from indi- gestion, acid stomach, or other distress? .................. . .......................... Yes Do you like love scenes in a movie or play? ............................................ Yes Do you feel that you are lacking in self-control? ................... . ..... .. . . .......... Yes Have you ever, on your own initiative, organized a club or group of any kind?. . . ..... Yes Are you ever afraid that you cannot live up to the standards your parents set for you? Yes Do you feel strongly against kissing a friend of your own sex and age? ..... . .......... Yes Have you ever been afraid of contracting tuberculosis or some other serious disease?. . Yes Does it sometimes seem to you that in life’s competitions you are usually left behind? Yes Have you ever been hesitant about making application for a job in person? ...... . . . . Yes Do you prefer the study of mathematics and science to that of literature and music?. . Yes Do you get angry very easily?. ... Yes Do you feel confident that you can cope with almost any situation that you will meet in the future?. ...... . ........... . ........... . . .. ........ . ....... . ............ . ..... Yes Does it bother you considerably to have your teacher or your boss call upon you unexpectedly in a group? ............................................................ Yes Can you go into a dark cellar or basement alone without even the slightest trembly feeling? . .......................... ........ .......... Yes When you are walking with others, do they often have difficulty in keeping up with you? ...................................... , ........................................ Yes Does it make you uncomfortable to be “different"?. . . . . ..................... . ....... Yes Is your health generally better than that of most people? . . . ........................ . Yes Do you often become tense or excited either at a movie or when listening to the radio? Yes Would you rather work for a good boss than for yourself? . . . . ........... . . . . . . . . . . . . . Yes Would you rather be a florist than a miner? ............... . . ............ . ......... . . Yes Do you sometimes wish you were in another office (or school or factory) where your companions were more congenial? .................. . ........ . ....................... Yes Do you feel tired out most of the time? ................. ' ......... . .............. . ...... Yes Do you (or would you) like to take on new and important responsibilities such as organising a new business enterprise? .............. . ......... . ........ . .............. Yes Are you very good at making money as compared with others of your own age and sex? ........... .. ................................. ......... Yes Are you afraid of snakes? . . . . . ........ . . ........... . ...... . ........ . ....... . ......... Yes Are you particularly uneasy when waiting for a slow person to finish either saying or doing what he started? ....... . ...... . .............. . ......... . . . ...... . . . . . ....... . . Yes Are you oversensitive to criticism of yourself? ........ . .............. . .............. . Yes Do you feel sorry for a fish that is caught on a hook?. . . . . . . ......................... Yes Are you the kind of person who is “on the go” all the time he is awake?. . . . . . . . . .. . . . Yes Are you easilystartled byunexpected stimuli? ..... Yes Do you find it difilcult to say “No" to a salesman who tries to sell you something you donotreailywant?............. ....... ...... Yes "I «ensue-ensue...“ 'Q “'00..“ HHH-idfl-QQHNH 'iNO-wfl 0'. NH’*HOOH “O 'O-Q '4 HHH‘QO‘ No No No No No No No No 28 29 30 3] 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 0 73 74 75 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 87. 88. 89. 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. 95. 96. 97. 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 108. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 118. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. 121. 122. 123. 124. 12 5. 126. 127. Do you frequently feel self-conscious in the presence of important people? ............ Would you rather be an artist than a political organizer? ............................ Do you usually work faster than the average person of your sex and age? ............. Were you happier when you were younger than you are now? ........................ . Do your interests tend to change quickly? ............................................ When you are attracted to a person of the opposite sex whom you have not met, do you usually make an active attempt to get acquainted even though the circumstances may make this quite diflcult? ............................................................ Does the sight of large bugs and spiders ever give you a “creepy" feeling? ............ Do you suffer keenly from feelings of inferiority? ........................ . ............ Can you relax easily when sitting or lying down9 ...................................... Do you find it difiicult to solicit funds even in a cause in which you are interested?. .. . Do you become nervous and tense when competing in a contest, such as tennis, golf, or debating? ........................................................................ Do younger people have an easier and more enjoyable life than you do?. . . .. . . . . . . . . . When you become emotional do you sometimes come to the point of tears? ........ . . . . Do you usually start to work on a new project with a great deal of enthusiasm? ....... Does it seem to you that you never do things in a way that wins the attention and approval of others? . .. ................. . ............................................ Do you usually speak out in meeting to oppose someone who you feel sure is wrong?. . Do you often find it difficult to sleep at night9 ............................. . ........... Do you ever fear that you are getting lost9 ............................................ Are you inclined to keep quiet when out in a social group? ............................ Would you rather be a building contractor than a nurse? ............................. Do you sometimes want to move to a new town or community because you do not find congenial people where you are? ................................................... . . Do loud noises tend to upset you? .................................................... Are you disgusted at the sound of foul language? ............. . ...................... Can you turn out a large amount of work in a short time? .............. . ............ When promotions in rank, salary, or position are being made, does it seem that you are given less attention than others? ............................. . . .................. Do you usually shrink from meeting a crisis or emergency9 ................... y ........ Do odors of perspiration disgust you? . . . . .......... .. ........................... . . . . . Do you find it dificult to goon with your work if you do not receive enough encour- “sealant? eeaeeeeoeeoeeeeeseeoeeeeeeeeeeeeesea eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee eseeeeooeeeeee While not otherwise occupied, are your hands almost always busy in such acts as drumming on the table, twisting a chain or rubber band, etc. ? ....................... . Do you often find yourself hurrying to get places even when there is plenty of time?” Do you let others “run over you” more than you should for your own good? .......... Would you rather be a private secretary than an explorer of new geographic territory? Do you ever wish you could have been born at a different time or place or in a different family than you were? ...................................................... Can you usually sit still without fidgeting? .......................................... Would you rate yourself as a talkative individual? ................................... Have there been many people with whom you have come in contact who did not care to associate with you? ............................................................ . . . Are you disgusted at the sight of ragged or soiled fingernails? . . . . . . . . . ...... . ..... . . . Do you usually feel restless when listening to a lecture? .............................. In being thrown by chance with a stranger, do you usually take the initiative in intro- ducing yourself? ............ . ......... ...................... Do you ever wish that you were taller or shorter than you are? .................. . . . . . Are you frequently in a state of inner excitement or turmoil? ......... . ............ . . . Can you (or could you) walk past a graveyard alone at night without feeling uneasy? If you hold an opinion that is radically different from that expressed by a lecturer, do you usually tell him about it either during or after the lecture?. ............... Are you frequently absent-minded? ............................................ Do you feel deeply sorry for a bird with a broken wing9 ............................ Do screeching sounds (like a fingernail scratching on the blackboard) send terrific “chills” up and down your back9 ............................................... . . . . . . Can you always think of a good excuse when the situation demands it? ............ . . . . Are you usually confident of your abilities? ............................ . . . . . . . . . .. . . . Would you rate yourself as an impulsive individual9 ..... . ....................... . . . . . Do you feel compelled to change your bodily posture frequently while sitting?. . . .. . . . 11130 y:u feel that the average person has made a better adjustment to life than you ave .......................................... ............ . Do you have nervous habits such as chewing your pencil or biting your fingernails?” Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes ‘ Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 0‘9"...“ OC'QNHN H HHHN'§'~9 "OHN'O “9000‘“ "90'. “I" ‘0“ "9'4”". "INN-IO 0". Hflflflfl " H Page 3 No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 88 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 98 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 118 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 128 124 125 126 127 .— 1-4.; Page 128. 129. 130. 131. 132. 133. 184. 135. 136. 188. 139. 140. 141. 142. 143. 144. 145. 146. 147. 148. 149. 150. 151. 152. 153. 154. 155. 157. 158. 159. 160. 161. 162. 168. 164. 165. 166. 167. 168. 169. 170. 171. 172. 173. 174. 175. 176. 177. 178. 179. 180. 181. 182. 183. 184. 185. 186. 4 Do you ever take the initiative to enliven a dull party9 ................... . . . ...... When you were a child were you usually made the “goat" by your playmates (such as being forced to be on the unpopular side while playing games)? ..... . ..... . . . . . . . . . . Do you like to speak in public?. .. ....................... . ................. . ......... Do you feel physically inferior to your associates? . . . ........... . . . . . ....... . . . . . . . . . . Would you rather be a dress designer than a forest ranger?. . . . . . . . . . . . ........... . . . . Arenyou able to come back to a state of calm readily after an exciting situation is pas ................................................. . ...... . ....... Do you like to bear responsibilities alone? ................................. . . . . . . . .. . Do you (or would you) like to go hunting with a rifle for wild game?. ....... . ...... . When present, with others, at the scene of an accident, do you usually take an active partinhelpingoutifneeded?” ............. ......... :Vhentyou become angry. do you get over it rather quickly when the cause for anger spas ................ ....... . ...................... ....... . ...... .. Do you tend to prefer quiet rather than exciting amusements9 ................... . . . . . . Do you often wish your appearance were different than it is? .................. . . . . . . When you find that a piece of merchandise you have bought is defective, do you find it easy to demand an exchange or refund? .................... Can you stick to a tiresome task for a long time without being prodded or encouraged? Does it make you jittery to handle a loaded gun? ................... .. . . Are you frequently afraid that other people will not like you? ..... . ......... . . . . . . . . . Have you ever been regarded as a daredevil9 ..................... . . . . . . ..... . . . . . . . . . . Do you consider yourself a nervous person? .................. When in a restaurant you are served stale or inferior food, do you usually make a vigorousprotestaboutit? ........................................ Does your mind often wander so badly that you lose track of what you are doing?. . . . Does it annoy you to see a person biting his fingernails? . . ........ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Are you easily discouraged when things become difiicult?. . ........... . . . . ....... . . . . Does a difficult decision or emotional crisis ever leave you so exhausted that you cannot go on with your regular activities? . . ..... . . . . .......................... . ........... . . Are you inclined to rush from one activity to another without pausing for rest?.. . . . . . Do you always stand up for your rights when they are endangered?.. . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . Do your friends seem to have a better time than you do?. ....... .. Does the sight of pus disgust you9 .............. . ..................... . . . . . . . . . ....... Do you sometimes wish that you were more attractive than you are?. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Do you ever experience a feeling of vague uneasiness without knowing the reasons?. . Do you have any nervous habits like twitching your face, neck, or shoulders?. . . . . . .. . Are you disgusted at the sight of an unshaven man? ................................. Are you able to play your best in a game or contest against an opponent who is much superior to you?............. ...... Do long-continued noises “get on your nerves”? ............... . ............... . ...... Do you generally prefer to take the lead in group activities9 ........................... Do you often wish that you were physically stronger than you are? .......... . . . . . . . . . . Areyoufearfulofburglars?. . . . ................... ....... Do you think you use up more energy than the average person in getting things done? Do you like to sell things (that is, to act as a salesman)? .......................... . . . Do you have one or more hobbies or skills at which you are outstanding?. . . . . . . . . . . . . Are you often so much “on the go" that sooner or later you wear yourself out? ....... Does it annoy you to see a person clean his fingernails in public? . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Are there times when you feel as if your nerves were raw or “on edge"?. . . ........... Do peOple usually give you credit for having good judgment? ......................... Do you often feel bubbling over with excess energy? . . . . . ............... . . . . . .. . . . . . Are you easily disturbed by distracting stimuli when doing mental work?. . . . . . . . . . . . Do you often feel reluctant to meet the most important person at a party or reception? Do other people regard you as a lively individual? .................................... Do you have a strong dread of fire9 .............................................. .. . . . Do iyou often feel that few obstacles can stand in the way of your reaching your final gas 39 ............................................................... . .............. As a child did you often naturally fall into positions of leadership? ..... . ............ Do you ever have the feeling that there is nothing to live for? ........................ Do tyou often find that you cannot make up your mind until the time for action is Dan, oooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo O ..... C ....... O OOOOOOOOOOOO .I.....- Do you prefer going to a dance rather than to a prize-fight?. . . . . ............... . . . . . . Do you seek to avoid all troublesome situations9 ...................................... Do you often show yourself up to your own disadvantage9 ......... . ................. . . Do you become upset rather easily9 .................................................. Are you willing to take a chance alone in a situation where the outcome is doubtful?” Do you have any hestitation about calling down a person who does not play fair?“ Do you resent being "kidded" about your peculiarities? ............................. . BE SURE YOU HAVE ANSWERED EVERY QUESTION Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 9‘ 0'0".“ “NW“ 9Q"... 9'. "OH-OH N'OMO-‘NN '0 °QNN9C°ON~9O N~H~N9OH°¢9QGOH9¢-DONON HWQNHNNQ “Mr; No No No No No No No No No No 128 129 130 131 182 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 141 143 144 145 l 146 l 147 143 119 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 g 180 . 181 182 183 184 186 186 l “Yes," ‘,‘?" or "."No a circle around the answer that describes your behavior best. to decide between the “Yes" and the “No." AN INVENTORY OF FACTORS S T D C R Scores: S .............. T .............. D .............. C .............. R .............. implication of right or wrong in any of these questions. 1. Do you express yourself more easily in speech than in writing? ............................................ Yes 2. Are you inclined to limit your acquaintances to a select few? ................................................ Yes 3. Do you generally prefer to take the lead in group activities? ................................................ Yes 4. Are you ordinarily a carefree individual?. -- ................................ . ............................. Yes 5. Do you like work that requires considerable attention to details? ........................................ Yes 6. Are you inclined to be moody? ...................................................... Yes 7. Do you usually have difficulty in, starting conversations with strangers? .......................... Yes 8. Are you inclined to act on the spur of the moment without thinking things over? ............ Yes 9. Do you work much better when you are praised? - Yes 10. Do you like to change from one type of work to another frequently? ................................ Yes 11. Are you self-conscious in the presence of your superiors? .................................................. Yes 12. Do you daydream frequently? Yes 13. Do you subscribe to the philosophy of “Eat, drink, and be merry, for tomorrow we die?" .. ......................... Yes 14. Are you inclined to worry over possible misfortunes? ........................ Yes 15. Are you frequently somewhat absent- minded? ............................ Yes 16. Are you relatively unconcerned about what others think of your actions? ........................ Yes 17. Are you inclined to keep 1n the background on social occasions? .- ............... Yes 18. Are you more interested in athletics than in intellectual things? .. .................... Yes 19. Are you impatient when waiting for a member of your family or for friends? .................. Yes 20. Do you like to speak in public? ........................... Yes 21. Are you inclined to live in the present, leaving the past and the future out of your thoughts? ........................................................ Yes 22. Do you have frequent ups and downs in mood, either with or without apparent cause?. .Yes 23. Are you inclined to be slow cmd deliberate in movement? .................................................... Yes 24. Are your feelings rather easily hurt? .......................................................... Yes 25. Do you enjoy getting acquainted with most people? ............................... Yes 26.: Are you inclined to keep quiet when out in a social group? -_ ................. Yes 27. Do you adapt yourself easily to new conditions, that is, new places, situations, surroundings, etc? ............ Yes. 28. Do you express such emotions as delight, sorrow, anger, and the like, readily? ................ Yes 29. Are you inclined to think about yourself much of the time? .................................................. Yes 30. Are you inclined to analyze the motives of others? ....... g .............................. Yes 31. Do you usually keep in close touch with things going on around you? .............................. Yes 32. Do you often have the “blues"? - ................................................................... Yes 33. Do you "get rattled" easily at critical moments? ................................................. Yes (Publisher: Sheridan Supply Co" Beverly Hills. Calif.) (Copyright 1940, by I. P. Guilford) V'Q-OQQNJ-fifi) “O‘Q'SJQQWJ'QQ'O‘OQ'O QNJQNDWJ-O 'O‘Q'O'QHDQ'Q No No No N o No No N o No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No INSTRUCTIONS: Below you will find some questions which are to be answered by encircling either Read each question in turn, think what your behavior has usually been, and draw Encircle the ‘?" only when you are unable BE SURE TO ANSWER EVERY QUESTION. There is no (Dmflmmtwav-fi 1".— x" 3;” ‘ PAGE 2 34. Is it usually difficult for you to make decisions? ................................ Yes 35. Do you ever feel that the world is distant and unreal to you? ................................................ Yes 36. Is it difficult to “lose yourself" even at a lively party? .. Yes 37. Do you shrink from speaking in public? ........................................................... Yes 38. Do you have difficulty in making new friends? ........................................... Yes 39. Would you rate yourself as an impulsive person? ........................................................................ Yes 40. Were you ever the "life of the party?" .................................... ‘ ........................................................ Yes 41. Are you frequently in low spirit? ................................................................. Yes 42. Does it bother you to have people watch you at your work? .............................................. Yes 43. Do you frequently find yourself in a meditative state? . ........................... Yes 44. Are your daydrearns frequently about things that can never come true? ............................ Yes 45. Are you inclined to be shy in the presence of the opposite sex? .......................................... Yes 46. Are you inclined to be overconscientious? .. ................ Yes 47. Do you often crave excitement? ..................................................................... Yes 48. Do your interests change very quickly? ......................................................... Yes 49. Are you inclined to ponder over your past? .................................................................................. Yes 50. Do you ever feel "just miserable" for no good reason at all? .................................................. Yes 51. Are you often troubled about feelings of guilt? .................................... Yes 52. Do you often experience periods of loneliness? ............................... . ....Yes 53. Are you much depressed when others criticize you? .................................................................. Yes 54. Are you worried about being shy? .--.-.Les 55. Would you rather spend an evening reading at home than to attend a large party?....Yes 56. Do you worry over humiliating experiences longer than the average person? ...................... Yes 57. Would you like a position in which you changed from one kind of task to another frequently during the day? ......... . ........................... Yes 58. Do you often find that you have made up your mind too 1ate?... Yes 59. Would you rate yourself as a tense or ‘high strung" individual? ........................................ Yes 60. Does your mind often wander while you are trying to concentrate? .................................. Yes 61. Do you nearly always have a “ready answer" for remarks directed to you? ...................... Yes 62. Are you inclined to ”jump at conclusions"? ............. Yes 63. Do you usually prefer to let some one else take the lead on social occasions? .................... Yes 64. Do you ever daydream? ................ Yes 65. Do you ever change from happiness to sadness, or vice versa, without good reason? ...... Yes 66. Do you usually derive pleasure from being “in the limelight" on social occasions? ............ Yes 67. Is it difficult to hurt your feelings, even when the joke is on you? .......... Yes 68. Do you often try to find the underlying motives for the actions of other people? .............. Yes 69. Are you inclined to stop and think things over before acting?“ ............................................ Yes 70. Do you generally feel uncomfortable when you are the center of attention on a social occasion? Yes 71. Do you consider yourself less emotional than the average person, that is, less easily upset? Yes 72. After a critical moment is over, do you usually think of something you should have done but failed to do? .............. Yes 73. Would you rate yourself as a lively individual? ......................................................................... Yes 74. Are you philosophically inclined? ............................................ Yes 7 5. Do you often have a feeling of unworthiness? .............................................................................. Yes 76. Can you usually keep cheerful in spite of troubles? ................................................. . .................... Yes 77. Do you like to play pranks upon others? ............................................. Yes 78. Do you often feel that people are observing you on the street? ............................................ Yes 79. Do you feel lonesome even when with other peorie? ................................................................ Yes 80. Are you troubled with feelings of inferiority? ................................................................................ Yes 81. Would you rather be a scientist than a politician? ..... . ................................................................ Yes 82. Are you inclined to take life too seriously? ............................................................................ , ....... Yes to w) '0 no "0 «o '0 "0 '0 -o '0 vs) no "0 '0 No 3’ No 3 No 3 No 3 No 3 No 3 No 4 No 4 N o 4‘ No 4- No 4‘ No 4i No 4! No 4'. No 42 No if No 5f No 51 No 52 No 53 No 54 No 55 No 56 No 57 No 58 No 59 No 60 No 61 No 52 No 63 No 64 No 65 No ‘35 No 7 No 58 No 69 No 70 N o 71 No 72 No 73 No 74 No 75 No 76 No 77 No 78 No 79 No 30 No 31 No 8.7. ...4 83. In social conversations, are you usually a listener rather than a talker? .............................. Yes 84. Do you frequently feel that people around you are talking about you? ................................ Yes 85. Do you like to have time to be alone with your thoughts? ...................................................... Yes 86. Do you find it difficult to go to sleep at night because experiences of the day keep “running through your head"? ..................................................................................... Yes 87. Are you inclined to take your work casually, that is, as a matter of course? ...................... Yes 88 Are you inclined to avoid meeting certain people on the street (bill collectors and 89. 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. 95. 96. 97. 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 103. 1.04. 105 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. 121. 122. 123. 124. 125. 126. 127. the like not included)? ............................................................................................. Yes Do you find it easy, as a rule to make new acquaintances? ................................................ Yes Are you inclined to be quick and sure in your actions? ................................................. . ......... Yes Are you troubled about being self- conscious?” ... ..-Yes Do you often feel restless while listening to a lecture? ............................................................ Yes Do you behave that people often misunderstand what you say? .......................................... Y es Do you limit your friendships mostly to members of your own sex? ...................................... Yes Does your mind wander badly so you lose track of what you are doing? ............................ Yes Are you often in a state of excitement? .. .......... . ............................ Yes Do you dislike to talk about yourself, even to close friends? .................................................. Yes Do you prefer to be conservative in the matter of dress and personal appearance? ........ Yes Do you like to discuss the more serious questions of life with your friends? ........................ Yes Are you inclined to keep your opinions to yourself during group discussions (not class discussions)? ................................................................................................................................ Yes Do you enjoy thinking out complicated problems? ...................................................................... Yes Are you inclined to be introspective, that is, to analyze yourself? ..................... . .................... Yes Are there times when you seek to be alone and you cannot bear the company of anyone? .............................................................................................. Yes Are you much concerned over the morals of others? ................................................................ Yes Do you frequently take time out just to meditate about things in general? ........................ Yes Are you usually unconcerned about the future? ......................................................................... Yes Do you usually become so absorbed in watching an athletic contest that you com- pletely forget yourself? Yes Can you relax yourself easily when sitting down? ..................................................................... Yes Are you usually a ”good mixer"?.. ............................................................ Yes Do you usually prefer a “slapstick" comedy to a serious drama at the movies? ................ Yes Do you frequently find it difficult to go to sleep at night, even though you are tired? ...... Yes Would you rate yourself as a happy-go-lucky individual? .................................................... Yes Do you ever take your work as if it were a matter of life or death?.. .................................... Yes Do you often “have the time of your life" at social affairs? ...................................................... Yes Do you think there is a great deal more happiness in the world than misery? .................. Yes Are you frequently “lost in thought"? .................................................................... Yes Have you often lost sleep over your worries? ................................................................................ Yes Do you like to mix socially with people? ................................ Yes Do you believe that the morals of modern youth are generally superior to those of former generations? ........................................... Yes Are you inclined to think over your failures long after they are past? .................................. Yes Are there times when your mind seems to work very slowly and other times when it works very rapidly? ....................................... r ................................................. Yes Are you inclined to avoid all people whenever possible? ........................................................ Yes Do you enjoy participating in a showing of Rah Rah" enthusiasm? .................................. Yes Do you usually feel disappointments so keenly that you cannot get them out of your mind? ........................................................................................ Yes Do you derive more real satisfaction from social activities than from anything else? ...... Yes When you stop to consider your future, does it usually seem very optimistic? ................ Yes Are you sometimes so "blue" that life seems hardly worth living? ..................................... ...Yes "J '0 N) ’0“) fifl‘fi'x) 'Q‘OQQNJ’O'O'OND'O'QN) ‘0") '0 Q'Q "J'ONJNJ PAGE 3 No No No No No No No No No No No No No No N o No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No N o No No No 83 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 -1. .‘_l".._.L-' ht‘fi‘flj . 1 .— “...; 33.‘ PAGE 4 128. 129. 130. 131. 132. 133. 134. 135. 136. 137. 138. 140. 141. 142. 143. 144. 145. 146. 147. 148. 149. 150. 151. 152. 153. 155. 156. 157. 158. 159. 160. 161. 162. 163. 164. 165. 166. 167. 168. 169. 170. 171. 172. 173. 174. 175. Do you usually take the initiative in making new friends? Yes Do you spend a great deal of time in thinking over past mistakes? Yes Would you be very unhappy if you were prevented from making numerous social contacts? Yes Do you often feel that there are very few things in life worth living for? ............................ Yes Do you often run over in your mind the events of the day before going to sleep at night? Yes Do you often feel that social affairs are a waste of time? Yes Do you frequently feel grouchy? Yes Are you annoyed when a boisterous person attracts attention to himself in public? ........ Yes Are you frequently bored with people? Yes When failing to have your own way, do you often resort to resentful thinking? ................ Yes Do you usually keep in fairly uniform spirits? Yes . Do you usually prefer to take your recreations with companions rather than alone? ...... Yes Are you usually in good spirits? Yes Have you ever been bothered by having a useless thought come into your mind repeatedly? Yes Are you usually well-poised in your social contacts? Yes Does it upset you much to lose in a competitive game? Yes Do you spend much time in thinking over good times you have had in the past? .............. Yes Are you often hesitant about meeting important people? Yes Do you feel tired most of the time? Yes Do you ever have a queer feeling that you are not your old self? Yes Is it easy for you to act naturally at a party? Yes Do you get tired of people rather quickly? Yes Do you like to have many social engagements? Yes Do you ever have to fight against bashfulness? Yes Are you frequently "lost in thought" even when supposed to be taking part in a conversafion? Yes Do people find fault with you more than you deserve? Yes . Do you often feel conspicuous in a group of people? Yes Are you sometimes bubbling over with energy and sometimes very sluggish? .................. Yes Do you often speculate about why people behave as they do? Yes Do you find it almost impossible to take another person fully into your confidence? ........ Yes Have you found books more interesting than people? Yes Have you often felt listless and tired for no good reason? Yes Do you prefer action to planning for action? Yes Do you often philosophize about the purpose of human existence? Yes Do you become angry very quickly and also recover very quickly? - ........ Yes Do you often think or dream of what you will be doing five years from now? .................... Yes When you are bored do you feel like stirring up some excitement? Yes Do you usually feel well and strong? Yes Do you enjoy entertaining people? Yes Is your own mood very easily influenced by people around you, that is, by happy people or sad people? Yes Does it embarrass you a great deal to say or do the wrong thing in a social group? ........ Yes Do you like to indulge in a reverie (daydreaming)? Yes Do you believe that “every cloud has a silver lining"? Yes Do you often feel ill at ease with other people? Yes Can you usually let yourself go and have a hilariously good time at a gay party? .......... Yes Do you dislike to stop and analyze your own thoughts and feelings? Yes Are you inclined to avoid all complicated problems of any sort? Yes Do you think such questionnaires as this one are “silly"? Yes NJ") ‘3“) Q’OMV'O'OM'O“) -o~v«>-o-oov-o-J-ow«ow-o-o«o “O-O'ONDNJWDMND-OQHJ "D‘O'V'Q’O‘QNJNJ'Q No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No No 128 129 130 131 132 133 135 136 137 136 139 MC 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 JOHNSON TEMPERAMENT ANALYSIS Devised by Roswell H. Johnson Directions DO NOT OPEN THIS BOOKLET UNTIL YOU HAVE READ ALL OF THESE DIRECTIONS 1. 10. 11 11. 12. You are not to write or mark on thi: booklet in any way, but you will indicate your answers on the machine-scoring answer sheet as explained below. All. of the questions of this analysis refer to the person about whom they are being answered. his Pereon is called S, or the subject. These questions do not apply to any other person who may be describing this person, S, the subject. The words, “he” “him" or “his", mean the subject, whether a man or woman. Be sure you understand each question; read it twice if necessary. Please answer every question; you can give your opinion if you are uncertain about the answer. Do not think too long about any one question; answer as soon as you have thought it through and She" gO_0n with the next question. Consider this person from the viewpoint of “knowing him as I 0: I think that in the given situation, the answer would be ............ ,” and then mark it- When an answer would be different if one considered the past rather than the present, answer as of the present, unless the question expressly refers to the past. On the answer sheet you are given three columns in which to mark your answer as follows: PLUS (+) means “decidedly yes” or “mostly so". MID- means “undecided”; neither definitely yes nor no. MINUS (——) means “decidedly no” or “mostly not so”. P1,, - {753 do not mark the MID. column, unless the answer really belongs there to describe 3 Person, S, the subject. Your answ lines in th row is the er to each question is indicated by making a pencil mark within the pair of dotted e column which will show your answer. Be sure that the number on the answer Same as the number of the question in the booklet. 5.2:}: 31?“? answer sheet on a smooth, hard surface while marking your answers. Each pencil answer; Ould be a heavy, black line filling the space Within the pair of dotted lines. 'Wher'r the Tequir dare to be machine scored, you will be supplied with a specral electrographw penal winch t: 6 f0? thi: purpose. If you change an answer, erase your first mark completely. Ex 1 - Sheitanbagions or other comments are desirable and may be recorded on the reverse of the answer n0t elsewhere. Now, you a:- e an e to write clearly or print your name and other requested data in the spaces provided sw er sheet ;—-then, you will open the booklet to the first question; find answer row numb . . . er 1 on your answer sheet; and proceed until you have fm1shed the booklet. Copyright. 1941. by Roswell H. Johnson Published by California Test Bureau 5916 Hollywood Boulevard. Hollywood 28. California 1w" '1??? “3.. Questions Mark your answers on the answer sheet. Do not mark on this booklet. Write any comment you care to make on the back of the answer sheet. 1. Now-.4190»: 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. Does S want no more than two children in his family even though his health and income are satisfactory. Is one motive for S to go places so that he can talk about having been there? Is S relatively calm when others are getting rattled? Does S think the government is spending too much on relief and pensions? Does S resent efforts of others to tell him what to do? Does S make a considerable use of the telephone, dictionary, or atlas when not necessary? Does S sometimes have sudden unexpected jerks of some of his muscles even though nothing has hap- pened? Is S very eager to have his own business, or be an independent professional man, or if in an organizav tion to be in a position to give orders rather than to take them? Does S write in for samples, catalogs, solve puzzles, or submit questions to radio programs at times? Is S easy-going in the matter of discipline? Does S tend to say what comes to mind without enough thought as to whether it would be better left unsaid? When hunting or fishing is S free from concern about the pain he inflicts on game, live bait, or fish? Does S usually try to avoid being made a chairman ‘of a committee or an officer of an organization? Does S give in or stop during a controversy to “keep the peace”? Does S have a voice that flows evenly and smoothly? Is 8 inclined to say little except in response? Does S have jerking motions of some muscles when unexpected things happen? Is S considerate in his demands on employees, relatives, or pupils? Does S talk slowly (making due allowance for age)? Does S sometimes surprise his acquaintances by unexpected actions? Would S buy an article at the cheaper price if he noticed that the clerk has asked less than the price tag indicates, apparently having misread it? Does S act deliberately rather than impulsively? Does S often keep his views to himself because they do not seem important enough to tell others? Does S move about a good deal at a social gathering? Can S make a speech or public performance without stage fright? Is S likely to stay on the veranda by preference when some of the others go for tennis or a swim? Does S have phobias, i.e., an unwarranted and disturbing hatred or fear of any object or group of ob- jects or situations? Is S good at “breaking the ice” in a social gathering? Does S have the same religion, politics, or phi1050phy as his parents? Is S constantly careful to protect his health? _3 _ Laps: y-‘rrs- inal-.7 in: I" ..I-9". ”M u a - -v1' 7 p .. ,'_'#.'¥"' .- — ...— 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 41. 42. 43. 45. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. S4. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. Is 8 so sure of himself that it sometimes annoys others? Does S quickly recover his composure after an accident or similar disturbing incident? Is S less attentive than most individuals to things going on around him? Is S rather indifferent to maintaining the dignity and privileges of his job or place in life? Can S work in a room with many others talking and work efficiently without strain? Does S maintain uniformly, courteous behavior to other members of his family? Is S the kind of a person one might call a “self starter”? Can S enjoy a rest when there are distracting noises and movements about? Does S love to travel and when on a trip does he seek new experiences characteristic of the country? Does S chew pencils or bite fingernails? Does S sometimes say things that are dominating so that peoples’ feelings are sometimes hurt? Does S have few interests or activities of his own choosing? Does S “stick-to-it” at the cost of much inconvenience rather than give up? Does S have some thought pressing itself on his attention too much of the time to his annoyance? Does S eat slowly (making due allowance for age)? Is S as much influenced in his behavior by consideration of general welfare as by considerations of his own advantage? Does S sleep well? Does S take responsibility with reluctance, because he is doubtful of his fitness for it? Does S think that modern prisons coddle the prisoner too much so as to interfere with needed pun- ishment? Would you consider S a “go-getter”? Is S so sympathetic with those he sees in pain as to want to do something about it? Is S likely to give way to the wishes of others rather than to seek to have his own way? Would S feel sympathetic with conscientious objectors in time of war, where it is a war of invading other countries by one’s own country? Does S try to convert people to his views in several fields in which he is not an expert? Does S prefer to take a passive role in the clubs to which he belongs? Does S become disturbed by harmless rattles, crickets or the wind? Does S make a practice of offering help to motorists who need help, but do not ask for it? Does S use all reasonable precautions to prevent accidents? Does S accept defeat easily without any evidence of his disappointed feeling? Would S probably resort to corporal punishment in the case of deliberate disobedience by his mm child at age ten? _4_ 61.. 63. 65. 67. 69. 70. 71. 73. 74. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 85 . 86. 87. 88. 89. Does S get into scrapes occasionally? Does S have a habit of blinking eyes or pulling at ears? Does S fail to finish what he sets out to do, often enough to be a bother to him? Does S incline to ride rather than walk when the distance is intermediate? Do S’s “teeth get on edge” when hearing some noises? Does S buy on credit to excess? Is 5 emphatic in voice and manner? Can S get along with children of various ages without becoming irritated by them? Has S made more than one loan out of kindheartedness in which he was “worked” and never repaid? Can S relax easily when sitting or lying down? Does S favor zoning the city to control residence areas for negroes or orientals? Does S become so scared or apprehensive at times so as to feel hot, or shivering or have skin get goose-pimples, (goose-skin, goose-flesh)? Is S impatient with a child’s strong desire for a worthless object? Does S feel strongly convinced of the correctness of his views when in a controversy, excluding those in which he is expert? Does S worry more than the circumstances warrant? Does S sometimes get quite “keyed-up” (exclusive of drinking)? Does S make plans well in advance of the event and carry them out? Does S often get so wakeful as to be disinclined to go to bed at the usual time? Does S tend to put off doing things past the time that would be best? Does S take necessary risks of misfortune without undue worry? In traveling does S watch out to help the aged, infirm, or those with children rather than leave such acts to the officials? Does S talk less than his share when with others? Does S break out in more explosive action or words than would be expected from the cause? Does 5 stand by and avoid protecting an aninal from needless suffering? Does S think less well of his ability than the facts warrant? Is S Opposed to the parole system for criminals? Does S try to “get things going” in the community to which he belongs? Is S almost always truthful to others? Is S lively enough so someone might refer to him as “always-on-the—go”? In an automobile accident in which S is involved does he really try to see that any damage he did is made good? _5_ _ Fr;1:1"i—. W unearth-fl f..— AS. It I ‘ V 91. 93. 94. 96. 97. 98. 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. Is S inclined to have a few select friends rather than a large circle of friends and speaking acquaint- ances? Is it very hard for S to take blame, so that he seeks to avoid it? In social contacts is S thought of as warm-hearted? Do his failures come hard to S? Is S usually able to steady a difficult situation where “others lose their heads”? Does S put into his conversation quite a few “knocks” about others? Is singing or whistling often started by S out of the joys of life? Does S express his emotions readily? Is S independent in making a judgment uninfluenced by whether he likes or dislikes the leading sup porter of the proposal in question? Is S really fond of only a few people? Does S make efforts to get others to laugh and smile? Is S much interested in the affairs of other people? Does S refrain from complaining, when the other is late to an appointment? Is S sometimes thought of as a “wet blanket”? Is S considered cheery by some people? Does S think that someone is definitely unfriendly to him and works against him? Does S nearly always find it easy to take an interest in other peoples’ interests in conversation? When S does criticize, is it always tactful and really meant to be helpful? Does S give judgments only after a weighing of the pros and cons? Is S relatively unaffected in listening to emotional music? -.4 _—..—-_._h __ -..—— Does S think less well of rivals than they deserve? Does S express his satisfaction when he sees beautiful things? Does S give very little time in his conversation to the criticism of people and thing“ . . . . . . I not do Does S sometimes think people are looking at him or talking about him when they are real? ing so? Does S pay his debts and keep his promises when it is possible? Does S get over bad news quickly? Does 8 take criticism easily without resentment? Do various satisfactions keep S’s life so full that life seems very much worth livingP Does S find it easy to be impartial when called on to judge? Does S “put his foot in it” often (make a tactless blunder)? _5_ 121. 122. 123. 124. 125. 126. 127. 128. 129. 130. 131. 132. 133. 134. 135. 136. 137. 138. 139. 140. 141. 142. 143. 144. 145. 146. 147. 148. 149. 150. 151. 152. 153. Is S hearty in greeting people? Is S almost free from being suspicious of the actions of others? Does S sometimes get the experience in hearing speakers of thinking that the speaker is referring to S? Does S have a tendency to do some things beyond what good judgment would indicate? Does S tend to exaggerate his grievances? Does S adapt readily to new difficult conditions and situations? Do death, sickness, pain, and sorrow enter largely into S’s dreams? Does S live an easy-going life with only few enthusiasms to express? . E“ Does S think as well of those with whom he has a disagreement, as before? .. Does S often ponder on the misfortunes of his past? Doe's S show a uniform rather than a varied expression in talking? Is S hard to please? Does S carry out assignments promptly and systematically? law Is S likely to be jealous? Is S rather optimistic about opportunities for young people? Is S “touchy” on several things about himself? Is S bothered at times with the idea that nobody cares for him? Does S look ahead and fail to smile and show interest when passing a beautiful child? Is S well pleased with life and so never considered committing suicide? Is S prejudiced in favor of his own club, college, state, etc.? . Does S smile much? Does S find that a minor failure or poor showing of his can be quickly forgotten? Is S unsuccessful in acting, impersonating or relating incidents effectively? I. s logical and scientific in his thinking? ' Does S comment on many shortcomings in the shows he sees and the books he reads? Does S find that the memories of illness or pain pass out of mind fairly soon? Does S feel abused notibeing able to do something, instead of adapting to it by some substitute activity? Does S prefer to be with adults nearly all the time rather than with children part of the time? Does S only seldom express any grievances? Does S have his opinions influenced by looking at things from the standpoint of his experience, occupation, or training? Do companions like to be with S? Does S often have the blues? After seeing a tragic motion picture or drama, does S quickly return to normal, rather than continue being disturbed for a while? _7_ 154. Are personal interests unable to sway S from sound decisions? 155. Does S when on a picnic find himself sometimes unable to share the good spirits of the others? 156. Does S think well of most people, as to only rarely speak slightingly of them? 157. Does 8 show a cordial attitude only to close friends if at all? 158. Does S think someone does not like him and speaks critically about S to others? 159. Does S smile or laugh a good deal? 160. Can S see things as others see them, when he wishes to? 161. Does S, when he has a grievance straightened out, continue disgruntled for a while? 162. In voting does S study the personalities and issues, sometimes voting for a candidate of the other party, rather than regularly voting the same party ticket straight? 163. Does S refrain from giving a kiss, hug, pat on the back or otherwise manifesting pleasure in meeting friends, except as needed for politeness, after an absence of a fortnight or so? 164. When S loses something, is he almost free from the tendency to think that some one else stole or mislaid it? - 165. Does S give too high an importance to his own interests and fields of knowledge in comparison with others? 166. Does S find it hard to get started on a task that needs to be done? 167. Does S find it annoying to have any criticism made of himself even though justified and from which he could profit? 168. Can S “stand-up” under adversity well? 169. Does S often feel sad because of his inferiority in some repects? 170. Does S show a friendly attitude in his voice or expression? 171. Does S prefer not to pass a cemetery, so as not to be reminded of death? 172. Does S spend only very little time or no time grumbling about the condition of his work? 173. Does S succeed in preventing his emotions swaying his judgment much? 174. Is S appealed to strongly by young lovers who are hampered by opposition? 175. In disagreements with associates does S find it hard to understand how the other can possibly differ from him, so very one sided does the matter seem? 176. Does S find that grief over war victims and refugees comes to mind often? 177. Does S spend too freely in view of his income? 178. Does S think he has many warm friends? 179. Does S estimate his friends too favorably, in comparison with others whom he judges more severely? 180. Is S almost free from being disturbed by either his immaturity or aging? 181. Does S have spells of liveliness (lasting at least several days) rather than staying at about the saint level? 182. Does S have spells of being sad and depressed (lasting at least several days) rather than Staying it about the same .level? ' If Now look back over your answer sheet to make sure you have an answer for every question» you do not know, make the best guess you can. - ur Be sure to look to make sure you filled in the blanks at top of answer sheet shomf‘g yo name, etc. _3_ DO NOT OPEN UNTIL TOLD TO DO SO 29 [9an [at :1... MW MULTIPHASIC PERSONALITY INVENTORY STARKE R. HATHAWAY. Ph.D.. and I. CHARNLEY McKINLEY. M.D. This inventory consists of numbered statements. Read each statement and decide whether it is true as {—12 plied to you or false as applied to you. You are to mark your answers on the answer sheet you have. Look at the example of the answer sheet shown at the right. If a statement is TRUE or MOSTLY TRUE, as applied to you. blacken between the lines in the column headed T. (See A at the right.) If a statement is FALSE or NOT USUALLY TRUE, as applied to you, blacken between the lines in the column headed F. (See B at the right.) If a statement does not apply to you or if it is something Section of an- swer sheet cer- rectly marked 'rr aléé niil that you don't know about. make no mark on the answer sheet. Remember to give YOUR OWN opinion of yourself. Do not leave any blank spaces if you can avoid it. i In marking your answers on the answer sheet, be sure that the number of the statement agrees with the number on the answer sheet. Make your marks heavy and black. Erase completely any answer you wish to change. Do not make any marks on this booklet. Remember. try to make some answer to every statement. NOW OPEN THE BOOKLET AND GO AHEAD. Published by THE PSYCHOLOGICAL CORPORATION. New York Copyright 1943 by the University of Minnesota 47-18l TB DO NOT MAKE ANY MARKS ON THIS BOOKLET l. 2. 3. 4. 5. 10. ll. 12. I3. 14. 15. 16. I7. 18. 19. 20. 21. 23. 24. I like mechanics magazines. I have a good appetite. I wake. up fresh and rested most mornings. I think I would like the work of a librarian. I am easily awakened by noise. . I like to read newspaper articles on crime. My hands and feet are usually warm enough. . My daily life is full of things that keep me in- terested. . I am about as able to work as I ever was. There seems to be a lump in my throat much of the time. A person should try to understand his dreams and be guided by or take warning from them. I enjoy detective or mystery stories. I work under a great deal of tension. I have diarrhea once a month or more. Once in a while I think of things too bad to talk about. I am sure I get a raw deal from life. My father was a good man. I am very seldom troubled by constipation. When I take a new job. I like to be tipped off on who should be gotten next to. My sex life is satisfactory. At times I have very much wanted to leave home. At times I have fits of laughing and crying that I cannot control. I am troubled by attacks of nausea and vomiting. N 0 one seems to understand me. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31 . 32. 33. 34. 35. 3'7. 38. 39. 41. 44. 45. I would like to be a singer. I feel that it is certainly best to keep my mouth shut when I'm in trouble. Evil spirits possess me at times. When someone does me a wrong I feel I should pay him back if I can. just for the principle of the thing. I am bothered by acid stomach several times a week. At times I feel like swearing. I have nightmares every few nights. I find it hard to keep my mind on a task or job. I have had very peculiar and strange experi- ences. I have a cough most of the time. If people had not had it in for me I would have been much more successful. . I seldom worry about my health. I have never been in trouble because of my sex behavior. During one period when I was a youngster I engaged in petty thievery. At times I feel like smashing things. . Most any time I would rather sit and daydream than to do anything else. I have had periods of days. weeks, or months when I couldn't take care of things because I couldn't' 'get going." . My family does not like the work I have chosen (or the work I intend to choose for my life work). . My sleep is fitful and disturbed. Much of the time my head seems to hurt all over. I do not always tell the truth. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE ‘ . .; “.7-1' "f '7 ~.—.— - ‘17-?" ‘1 t MEL-e” 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. My judgment is better than it ever was. Once a week or oftener I feel suddenly hot all over, without apparent cause. When I am with people I am bothered by hear- ing very queer things. It would be better if almost all laws were thrown away. My soul sometimes leaves my body. I am in just as good physical health as most of my friends. I prefer to pass by school friends, or people I know but have not seen for a long time. unless they speak to me first. A minister can cure disease by praying and putting his hand on your head. I am liked by most people who know me. I am almost never bothered by pains over the heart or in my chest. As a youngster I was suspended from school one or more times for cutting up. I am a good mixer. Everything is turning out just like the prophets of the Bible said it would. I have often had to take orders from someone who did not know as much as I did. I do not read every editorial in the newspaper every day. I have not lived the right kind of life. Parts of my body often have feelings like burn- ing, tingling, crawling, or like "going to sleep." I have had no difficulty in starting or holding my bowel movement. I sometimes keep on at a thing until others lose their patience with me. I loved my father. I see things or animals or people around me that others do not see. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. 89. I wish I could, be as happy as others seem to be. I hardly ever feel pain in the back of the neck. I am very strongly attracted by members of my own sex. I used to like drop-the-handkerchief. I think a great many people exaggerate their misfortunes in order to gain the sympathy and help of others. I am troubled by discomfort in the pit of my stomach every few days or oftener. I am an important person. I have often wished I were a girl. (Or if you are a girl) I have never been sorry that I am a girl. I get angry sometimes. Most of the time I feel blue. I enjoy reading love stories. I like poetry. My feelings are not easily hurt. I sometimes tease animals. I think I would like the kind of work a forest ranger does. I am easily downed in an argument. Any man who is able and willing to work hard has a good chance of succeeding. These days I find it hard not to give up hope of amounting to something. Sometimes I am strongly attracted by the per- sonal articles of others such as shoes. gloves. etc., so that I want to handle or steal them though I have no use for them. I am certainly lacking in self-confidence. I would like to be a florist. I usually feel that life is worth while. It takes a lot of argument to convince most people of the truth. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. 95. 96. 97. 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. l 09. 110. Once in a while I put off until tomorrow what I ought to do today. I do not mind being made fun of. I would like to be a nurse. I think most people would lie to get ahead. I do many things which I regret afterwards (I regret things more or more often than others seem to). I go to church almost every week. I have very few quarrels with members of my family. . At times I have a strong urge to do something harmful or shocking. . I believe in the second coming of Christ. . I like to go to parties and other affairs where there is lots of loud fun. I have met problems so full of possibilities that I have been unable to make up my mind about them. I believe women ought to have as much sexual freedom as men. My hardest battles are with myself. I have little or no trouble with my muscles twitchm' g or jumping. I don't seem to care what happens to me. Sometimes when I am not feeling well I am cross. Much of the time I feel as if I have done some- thing wrong or evil. I am happy most of the time. There seems to be a fullness in my head or nose most of the time. Some people are so bossy that I feel like doing the opposite of what they request. even though I know they are right. Someone has it in for me. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 120. 121. 122. 123. 124. 125. - 126. 127. 128. 129. 130. 131. I have never done anything dangerous for the thrill of it. I frequently find it necessary to stand up for what I think is right. I believe in law enforcement. Often I feel as if there were a tight band about my head. I believe in a life hereafter. I enjoy a race or game better when I bet on it. Most people are honest chiefly through fear of being caught. In school I was sometimes sent to the principal for cutting up. My speech is the same as always (not faster or slower, or slurring; no hoarseness). My table manners are not quite as good at home as when I am out in company. I believe I am being plotted against. I seem to be about as capable and smart as most others around me. I believe I am being followed. Most people will use somewhat unfair means to gain profit or an advantage rather than to lose it. I have a great deal of stomach trouble. I like dramatics. I know who is responsible for most of my troubles. The sight of blood neither frightens me nor makes me sick. Often I can't understand why I have been so cross and grouchy. I have never vomited blood or coughed up blood. I do not worry about catching diseases. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE ‘. 11“” nurture “v.3_ge tnr '; ‘21“, m... bun-1— 132. I like collecting flowers or growing house 133. 134. 135. I36. 137. 138. 139. l 40. 141. I42. I43. I44. 145. 146. l 47. 148. 149. 150. 151. plants. I have never indulged in any unusual sex practices. At times my thoughts have raced ahead faster than I could speak them. If I could get into a movie without paying and be sure I was not seen I would probably do it. I commonly wonder what hidden reason another person may have for doing something nice for me. I behave that my home life is as pleasant as that of most people I know. Criticism or scolding hurts me terribly. Sometimes I feel as if I must injure either my- self or someone else. I like to cook. My conduct is largely controlled by the customs of those about me. I certainly feel useless at times. When I was a child. I belonged to a crowd or gang that tried to stick together through thick and thin. I would like to be a soldier. At times I feel like picking a fist fight with someone. I have the wanderlust and am never happy un- less I am roaming or traveling about. I have often lost out on things because I couldn't make up my' mind soon enough. It makes me impatient to have people ask my advice or otherwise interrupt me when I am working on something important. I used to keep a diary. I would rather win than lose in a game. Someone has been trying to poison me. 152. 153. 154. 155. 156. 157. 158. 159. 160. 161. 162. 163. 164. 165. 166. 167. 168. 169. I70. 171. 172. 173. Most nights I go to sleep without thoughts or ideas bothering me. During the pat few years I have been well most of the time. I have never had a fit or convulsion. I am neither gaining nor losing weight. I have had periods in which I carried on ac- tivities without knowing later what I had been doing. I feel that I have often been punished without cause. Icry easily. I cannot understand what I read as well as I used to. I have never felt better in my life than I do now. The top of my head sometimes feels tender. I resent having anyone take me in so cleverly that I have had to admit that it was one on me. I do not tire quickly. I like to study and read about things that I am working at. I like to know some important people because it makes me feel important. I am afraid when I look down from a high place. It wouldn't make me nervous if any membm of my family got into trouble with the law. There is something wrong with my mind. I am not afraid to handle money. What others think of me does not bother me. It makes me uncomfortable to put on a stun! at a party even when others are doing the same sort of things. I frequently have to fight against showing that I am bashful. I liked school. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE -__—-— —_g-=‘ -——= u: -— m. 175. 176. 177'. 173. 179. 180. 181. 182. 183. 184. 185. 186. 187. 188. 89. 190. 191. 192. 193. 194. 195. I96. I have never had a fainting spell. I seldom or never have dizzy spells. I do not have a great fear of snakes. My mother was a good woman. My memory seems to be all right. I am worried about sex matters. I find it hard to make talk when I meet new people. When I get bored I like to stir up some excite- ment. I am afraid of losing my mind. I am against giving money to beggars. I commonly hear voices without knowing where they come from. My hearing is apparently as good as that of most people. I frequently notice my hand shakes when I try to do something. My hands have not become clumsy or awk- ward. I can read a long while without tiring my eyes. / I feel weak all ever much of the time. I have very few headaches. Sometimes. when embarrassed. I break out in a sweat which annoys me greatly. I have had no difficulty in keeping my balance in walking. I do not have spells of hay fever or asthma. I have had attacks in which I could not control my movements or speech but in which I knew what was going on around me. I do not like everyone I know. I like to visit places where I have never been before. 197. 198. 199. 200. 201. 202. 203. 204. 205. 206. 207. 208. 209. 210. 211. 212. 213. 214. 215. 216. 217. 218. 219. Someone has been trying to rob me. 1 daydream very little. Children should be taught all the main facts of sex. There are persons who are trying to steal my thoughts and ideas. ' I wish I were not so shy. I believe I am a condemned person. If I were a reporter I would very much like to ' report news of the theater. I would like to be a journalist. At times it has been impouible for me to keep from stealing or shoplifting something. I am very religious (more than most people). I enjoy many different kinds of play and recreation. I like to flirt. I believe my sins are unpardonable. Everyan tastes the same. I can sleep during the day but not at night. My people treat me more like a child than a grown-up. In walking I am very careful to step over side- walk cracks. I have never had any breaking out on my skin that has worried me. I have used alcohol excesdvely. There is very little love and companionship in \ my family as compared to other homes. I frequently find myself worrying about some- thing. It does not bother me particularly to see animals suffer. I think I would like the work of a building contractor. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE ... v— . ”.4" ‘a’b. fun ’1 ’e ‘? .Ll‘el L-J‘JAA 1,; ‘ “s. -i fl...1il 220. 221. 222. 223. 224. 225. 226. 227. 228. 229. 230. 231. 232. 233. 234. 235. 236. 237. 238. 239. 240. 241. I loved my mother. I like science. It is not hard for me to ask help from my friends even though I cannot return the favor. I very much like hunting. My parents have often objected to the kind of people I went around with. I gossip a little at times. Some of my family have habits that bother and annoy me very much. I have been told that I walk during sleep. At times I feel that I can make up my mind with unusually great ease. I should like to belong to several clubs or lodges. I hardly ever notice my heart pounding and I am seldom short of breath. I like to talk about sex. I have been inspired to a program of life based on duty which I have since carefully followed. 1 have at times stood in the way of people who were trying to do something, not because it amounted to much but because of the principle of the thing. I get mad easily and then get over it soon. 1 have been quite independent and free from family rule. I brood a great deal. My relatives are nearly all in sympathy with me. I have periods of such great restlessness that I cannot sit long in a chair. I have been disappointed in love. I never worry about my looks. I dream frequently about things that are best kept to myself. 242. 243. 244. 245. 246. 247. 248. 249. 250. 251. 252. 253. 254. 255. 256. 257. 258. 259. 260. 261. 262. 263. I believe I am no more nervous than most others. I have few or no pains. My way of doing things is apt to be misunder- stood by others. My parents and family find more fault with me than they should. My neck spots with red often. I have reason for feeling jealous of one or more members of my family. Sometimes without any reason or even when things are going wrong I feel excitedly happy. "on top of the world." I believe there is (1 Devil and a Hell in afterlife. I don't blame anyone for trying to grab every- thing he can get in this world. I have had blank spells in which- my activities were interrupted and I did not know what was going on around me. No one cares much what happens to you. I can be friendly with people who do things which I consider wrong. I like to be with a crowd who play jokes on one another. Sometimes at elections I vote for men about whom I know very little. The only interesting part of newspapers is the "funnies." I usually expect to succeed in things I do. I believe there is a God. I have difficulty in starting to do things. I was a slow learner in school. If I were an artist I would like to draw flowers. It does not bother me that I am not better look- ing. I sweat very easily even on cool days. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE ._ -‘ I _.._—.<__——-—— ..T—:. _ 264. 265. 266. 267. 268. 269. 270. 271. 272. 273. 274. 275. 276. 277. 278. 279. 280. 281. 282. 283. 284. 285. I am entirely self-confident. It is safer to trust nobody. Once a week or oftener I become very excited. When in a group of people I have trouble thinking of the right things to talk about. Something exciting will almost always pull me out of it when I am feeling low. I can easily make other people afraid of me. and sometimes do for the fun of it. When I leave home I do not worry about whether the door is locked and the windows closed. I do not blame a person for taking advantage of someone who lays himself open to it. At times I am all full of energy. I have numbness in one or more regions of my My eyesight is as good as it has been for years. Someone has control over my mind. I enjoy children. At times I have been so entertained by the cleverness of a crook that I have hoped he would get by with it. I have often felt that strangers were looking at me critically. I drink an unusually large amount of water every day. Most people make friends because friends are likely to be useful to them. I do not often notice my ears ringing or buzzing. Once in a while I feel hate toward members of my family whom I usually love. If I were a reporter I would very much like to report sporting news. I am sure I am being talked about. Once in a while I laugh at a dirty joke. 286. 287. 288. 289. 290. 291. 292. 293. 294. 295. 296. 297. 298. 299. 300. 301. 302. 303. 304. 305. 306. I am never happier than when alone. I have very few fears compared to my friends. I am troubled by attacks of nausea and vomit- ing. 1 am always disgusted with the law when a criminal is freed through the arguments of a smart lawyer. I work under a great deal of tension. At One or more times in my life I felt that some- one was making me do things by hypnotizing me. I am likely not to speak to people until they speak to me. Someone has been trying to influence my mind. I have never been in trouble with the law. I liked "Alice in Wonderland" by Lewis Carroll. 1 have periods in which I feel unusually cheer- ful without any special reason. I wish I were not bothered by thoughts about sex. If several people find themselves in trouble, the best thing for them to do is to agree upon a story and stick to it. I think that I feel more intensely than most people do. There never was a time in my life when I liked to play with dolls. Life is a strain for me much of the time. I have never been in trouble Ecause of my sex behavior. I am so touchy on some subjects that I can't talk about them. In school I found it very hard to talk before the class. Even when I am with people I feel lonely much of the time. I get all the sympathy I should. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE I ‘ "i-u'.:m .:.In “-41).. m m “l 307. 308. 309. 310. 311. 312. 313. 314. 315. 316. 317. 318. 319. 320. 321. 322. 323. 324. 325. 326. 327. 328 I refuse to play some games because I am not good at them. At times I have very much wanted to leave home. I seem to make friends about as quickly as others do. My sex life is satisfactory. During one period when I was a youngster I engaged in petty thievery. I dislike having people about me. The man who provides temptation by leaving valuable property unprotected is about as much to blame for its theft as the one who steals it. Once in a while I think of things too bad to talk about. I am sure I get a raw deal from life. I think nearly anyone would tell a lie to keep out of trouble. I am more sensitive than most other people. My daily life is full of things that keep me interested. Most people inwardly dislike putting them- selves out to help other people. Many of my dreams are about sex matters. I am easily embarrassed. I wony over money and busineu. I have had very peculiar and strange experi- ences. I have never been in love with anyone. \ The things that some of my family have done have frightened me. At times I have fits of laughing and crying that I cannot control. My mother or father often made me obey even when I thought that it was unreasonable. . I find it hard to keep my mind on a task or job. 329. . 330. 331. 332. 333. 334. 335. 336. 337. 338. 339. 340. 341. 342. 343. 344. 345. 346. 347. 348. 349. 350. 351. I almost never dream. I have never been paralyzed or had any un- usual weakness of any of my muscles. If people had not had it in for me I would have been much more successful. Sometimes my voice leaves me or changes even though I have no cold. No one seems to understand me. Peculiar odors come to me at times. I cannot keep my mind on one thing. I easily become impatient with people. I feel anxiety about something or someone almost all the time. I have certainly had more than my share of things to worry about. Mostofthetimelwishlweredead. Sometimes I become so excited that I find it hard to get to sleep. At times I hear so well it bothers me. I forget right away what people say to me. I usually have to stop and think before I act even in trifling matters. Often I cross the street in order not to meet someone I see. I often feel as if things were not real. I have a habit of counting things that are not important such as bulbs on electric signs. and so forth. I have no enemies who really wish to harm me. I tend to be on my guard with people who are somewhat more friendly than I had expected. I have strange and peculiar thoughts. I hear strange things when I am alone. I get anxious and upset when I have to make a short trip away from home. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE 352. 353. 354. 357. 359. 360. 361. I have been afraid of things or people that I knew could not hurt me. I have no dread of going into a room by myself where other people have already gathered and are talking. I am afraid of using a knife or anything very sharp or pointed. . Sometimes I enjoy hurting persons I love. . I have more trouble concentrating than others seem to have. I have several times given up doing a thing because I thought too little of my ability. . Bad words. often terrible words. come into my mind and I cannot get rid of them. Sometimes some unimportant thought will run through my mind and bother me for days. Almost every day something happens to frighten me. I am inclined to take things hard. 362. I am more sensitive than most other people. 363. 364. 365. 366. At times I have enjoyed being hurt by someone I loved. People say insulting and vulgar things about me. I feel uneasy indoors. Even when I am with people I feel lonely much ~ of the time. / 367. 369. 370. 371 I am not afraid of fire. . I have sometimes stayed away from another person because I feared doing or saying some- thing that I might regret afterwards. Beligion gives me no worry. I- hate to have to rush when worln'ng. I am not unusually self-conscious. 372. I tend to be interested in several different hob- bies rather than to stick to one of them for a long time. 373. 374. I feel sure that there is only one true religion. At periods my mind seems to work more slowly . than usual. 375. When I am feeling very happy and active, ' someone who is blue or low will spoil it all. 376. 377. 378. 379. 380. Policemen are usually honest. At parties I am more likely to sit by myself or with just one other person than to join in with the crowd. I do not like to see women smoke. I very seldom have spells of the blues. When someone says silly or ignorant things . about something I know about, I try to set him 381. 382. 383. 384. 385. 386. 387. 388. 389. 390. 391. 392. 393. 394. right. I am often said to be hotheaded. I wish I could get over worrying about things I have said that may have injured other peo- ple's feelings. People often disappoint me. I feel unable to tell anyone all about myself. Lightning is one of my fears. I like to keep people guessing what I'm going to do next. ' The only miracles I know of are simply tricks that people play on one another. I am afraid to be alone in the dark. My plans have frequently seemed so full of difficulties that I have had to give them up. I have often felt badly over being misunder- stood when trying to keep someone from mak- ing a mistake. I love to go to dances. A windstorm terrifies me. Horses that don't pull should be beaten or kicked. I frequently ask people for advice. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE to?) 395. 396. 397. 398. 399. 400. 401. 402. 404. 405. 406. '407. 408. 409. 410. 411. 412. 413. 414. The future is too uncertain for a person to make serious plans. Often. even though everything is going fine for me. I feel that I don't care about anything. I have sometimes felt that difficulties were pil- ing up so high that I could not overcome them. I often think, "I wish I were a child again." I am not easily angered. If given the chance I could do some things that would be of great benefit to the world. I have no fear of water. I often must sleep over a matter before I decide what to do. .Itisgreattobelivinginthesetimeswhenso much is going on. People have often misunderstood my intentions when I was trying to put them right and be helpful. I have no trouble swallowing. I have often met people who were supposed to be experts who were no better than I. I am usually calm and not easily upset. I am apt to hide my feelings in some things, to the point that people may hurt me without their knowing about it. At times I have worn myself out by undertak- ing too much. ' I would certainly enjoy beating a crook at his own game. It makes me feel like a failure when I hear of the success of someone I know well. I do not dread seeing a doctor about a sickness or injury. I deserve severe punishment for my sins. I am apt to take disappointments so keenly that I can't put them out of my mind. 415. 416. 417. 418. 419. 420. 421. 422. 423. 424. 425. 426. 427. 428. 429. 430. 431. 432. 433. 434. 435. 436. If given the chance I would make a good lead- er of people. It bothers me to have someone watch me at work even though I know I can do it well. I am often so annoyed when someone tries to get ahead of me in a line of people that I speak to him about it. Attimeslthinklamnogoodatall. I played hooky from school quite often as a youngster. I have had some very unusual religious ex- periences. One or more members of my family is very nervous. I have felt embarrassed over the type of work that one or more members of my family have done. I like or have liked fishing very much. I feel hungry almost all the time. I dream frequently. I have at times had to be rough with people who were rude or annoying. I am embarrassed by dirty stories. I like to read newspaper editorials. I like to attend lectures on serious subjects. I am attracted by members of the opposite sex. I worry quite a bit over possible misfortunes. I have strong political opinions. I used to have imaginary companions. I would like to, be an auto racer. Usually I would prefer to work with women. People generally demand more respect for their own rights than they are willing to allow for others. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE 437. 438. 439. 440. 441. 442. 443. 444. 445. 446. 447. 448. 449. 450. 451. 452. 453. 454. 455. 456. It is all right to get around the law if you don‘t actually break it. There are certain people whom I dislike so much that I am inwardly pleased when they are catching it for something they have done. It makes me nervous to have to wait. I try to remember good stories to pass them on to other people. I like tall women. I have had periods in which I lost sleep over worry. I am apt to pass up something I want to do because others feel that I am not going about it in the right way. I do not try to correct people who express an ignorant belief. I was fond of excitement when I was young (or in childhood). I enjoy gambling for small stakes. I am often inclined to go out of my way to win a point with someone who has opposed me. I am bothered by people outside. on streetcars. in stores. etc., watching me. I enjoy social gatherings just to be with people. I enjoy the excitement of a crowd. My worries seem to disappear when I get into a crowd of lively friends. I like to poke fun at people. When I was a child I didn‘t care to be a mem- ber of a crowd or gang. I could be happy living all alone in a cabin in the woods or mountains. I am quite often not in on the gossip and talk of the group I belong to. A person shouldn't be punished for breaking a law that he thinks is unreasonable. 457. 458. 459. 460. 461. 462. 463. 464. 465. 466. 467. 468. 469. 470. 471. 472. 473. 474. 475. 476. 477. I believe that a person should never taste an alcoholic drink. The man who had most to do with me when I was a child (such as my father, stepfather, etc.) was very strict with me. I have one or more bad habits which are so strong that it is no use in fighting against them. I have used alcohol moderately (or not at all). I find it hard to set aside a task that I have undertaken. even for a short time. I have had no difficulty starting or holding my urine. I used to like hopscotch. I have never seen a vision. 1 have several times had a change of heart about my life work. Except by a doctor's orders I never take drugs or sleeping powders. I often memorize numbers that are not im- portant (such as automobile licenses. etc.). Iamoftensorrybecauselamsocrossand grouchy. I have often found people jealous of my good ideas, just because they had not thought of them first. Sexual things disgust me. In school my marks in deportment were quite regularly bad. I am fascinated by fire. Whenever possible I avoid being in a crowd. I have to urinate. no more often than others. When I am cornered I tell that portion of the truth which is not likely to hurt me. I am a special agent of God. If I were in trouble with several friends who were equally to blame. I would rather take the whole blame than to give them away. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE .... .-_‘._ . ,3” " 478. 479. 480. 481. 482. 483. 484. 485. 486. 487. 488. 489. 490. 491. 492. 493. 494. 495. 496. r I I have never been made especially nervous over trouble that any members of my family have gotten into. I do not mind meeting strangers. I am often afraid of the dark. I can remember "playing sick" to get out of something. While in trains. busses, etc., I often talk to strangers. Christ performed miracles such as changing water into wine. I have one' or more faults which are so big that it seems better to accept them and try to control them rather than to try to get rid of them. When a man is with a woman he is usually thinking about things related to her sex. I have never noticed any blood in my urine. I feel like giving up quickly when things go wrong. I pray several times every week. I feel sympathetic towards people who tend to hang on to their griefs and troubles. I read in the Bible several times a week. I have no patience with people who believe there is only one true religion. I dread the thought of an earthquake. I prefer work which requires close attention. to work which allows me to be careless. I am afraid of finding myself in a closet or small closed place. I usually "lay my cards on the table" with peo- ple that I am trying to correct or improve. I have never seen things doubled (that is. an object never looks like two objects to me with- out my being able to make it look like one object). 497 498. 499. 500. 501. 502. 503. 504. 505. 506. 507. 508. 509. 510. 511. 512. 513. 514. 515. 516. . I enjoy stories of adventure. Itisalwaysagoodthingtobefrank. Imustadmitthatlhaveattimesbeenworried beyond reason over something that really did not matter. I readily become one hundred per cent sold on a good idea. - I usually work things out for myself rather than get someone to show me how. I like to let people know where I stand on things. It is unusual for me to express strong approval or disapproval of the actions of others. I do not try to cover up my poor opinion or pity of a person so that he won't know how I feel. I have had periods when I felt so full of pop that sleep did not seem necessary for days at a time. I am a high-strung person. I have frequently worked under people who seem to have things arranged so that they get credit for good work but are able to pass off mistakes onto those under them. I behave my sense of smell is as good other people's. I sometimes find it hard to stick up for my rights because I am so reserved. Dirt frightens or disgusts me. I have a daydream life about which I do not tell other people. I dislike to take a bath. I think Lincoln was greater than Washington. I like mannish women. In my home we have always had the ordinary necessities (such as enough food. clothing. etc.) Some of my family have quick tempers. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE _,_ _. _.-n— -I= _b _— 517. ' 518. 519. 520. 521. 522. 523. 524. 525. 526. 527. 528. 529. 530. 531. 532. 533. 534. trying to do a thing. 535. I cannot do anything well. I have often felt guilty because I have pre- tended to feel more sorry about something than I really was. There is something wrong with my sex organs. I strongly defend my own opinions as a rule. \ In a group of people I would not be embar- rassed to be called upon to start a discussion or give an opinion about something I know well. I have no fear of spiders. I practically never blush. I am not afraid of picking up a disease or game from door knobs. I am made nervous by certain animals. The future seems hopeless to me. The members of my family and my close rela- tives get along quite well. I blush no more often than others. I would like to wear expensive clothes. I am often afraid that I am going to blush. People can pretty easily change me even though I thought that my mind was already ' made up on a subject. I can stand as much pain as others can. 3 I am not bothered by a great deal of belching of gas from my stomach. ' Several times I have been the last‘ to give uxp My mouth feels dry almost all the time. "-5. Ltk" I 536. 537. 538. 539. 540. 541. 542. 543. 544. 545. 546. 547. 548. 549. 550. 551. 552. " 553. It makes me angry to have people hurry me. I would like to hunt lions in Africa. I think I would like the work of a dressmaker. I am not afraid of mice. My face has never been paralyzed. My skin seems to be unusually sensitive to touch. I have never had any black. tarry-looking bowel movements. - - Several times a week I feel as if something dreadful is about to happen. I feel tired a good deal of the time. Sometimes I have the same dream over and ovgr. I like to read about history. 1 m7... partials and socials. I never attend a sexy show if I can avoid it. I shrink from facing a crisis or difficulty. I likh repairing a door latch. Sometimes I am sure that other people can tell what I am thinking. . I like to read about science. 1‘ am afraid of being alone in a wide-open ‘ place. 554. 555. J If Iiwere an artist I would like to draw children. I sometimes feel that I am about to go to pieces. GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE '5' t' I..? AI.‘-M MK -..—_ .y .' A O .— 3 n-a'vv—4— * 556. 557. 558. 559. 560. 561. I am very careful about my manner of dress. I would like to be a privatekwcretary. A large number of people are guilty of bad sexual conduct. I have often been tightened in the middle of the night. . I am greatly bothered by forgetting where I put things. I very much like horseback riding. 562. The one to whom I was most attached and whom I most admired as a child was a woman. (Mother. sister. aunt. or other woman.) 563. I like adventure stories better than romantic stories. 564. I can apt to pass up something I want to do when others feel that it isn't worth doing. 565. I feel like jumping off when I am on a high place. 566. I like movie love scenes. .4 “‘11.... I _ “a... 4. . 198 APPENDIX C CRITICAL RATIOS OF THE DIFFERENCE BETWEEN THE PER- CENTAGE OF RESPONSES BY SUCCESSFUL PAROLEES AND THE PERCENTAGE OF RESPONSES BY VIOLATORS ON THE ITEMS INCLUDED IN THE GUILFORD-MARTIN INVENTORY OF FACTORS GAMIN, AN INVENTORY OF FACTORS STDCR, THE JOHNSON TEMPERA- MENT ANALYSIS, AND THE MINNESOTA MULTIPHASIC PERSONALITY INVENTORY (* denotes items at the .93 [.07] level of significance or better) 199 Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 1 0.54 37 1.10 73 0.57 109 0.77 2 0.30 38 0.91 74 0.85 110 1.42 3 1.12 39 1.28 75 1.24 111 1.56 4 0.42 40 0.92 76 2.86* 112 0.99 5 0.71 41 0.31 77 1.36 113 0.28 6 198* 42 0.64 78 1.28 114 0.71 7 0.74 43 0.44 79 0.72 115 0.19 8 1.74 44 2.45* 80 2.08* 116 339* 9 1.79 45 1.13 81 0.72 117 2.20* 10 0.96 46 1.46 82 1.02 118 1.27 11 0.36 47 191* 83 0.31 119 1.57 12 0.99 48 1.43 84 1.11 120 1.41 13 0.15 49 1.87* 85 0.73 121 0.99 14 0.68 50 1.23 86 0.29 122 0.59 15 1.71 51 1.13 87 0.41 123 1.38 16 1.29 52 1.03 88 0.16 124 1.85* 17 1.65 53 1.18 89 0.47 125 0.43 18 0.75 54 0.42 90 1.55 126 1.15 19 198* 55 1.21 91 0.15 127 1.26 20 1.56 56 1.74 92 2.34* 128 2.14* 21 0.60 57 1.09 93 1.64 129 0.34 22 0.73 58 0.95 94 1.01 130 1.42 23 1.18 59 0.46 95 0.66 131 1.27 24 1.28 60 1.08 96 0.91 132 0.74 25 0.44 61 1.28 97 0.82 133 0.67 26 0.95 62 0.28 98 0.57 134 0.69 27 0.57 63 1.38 99 1.31 135 0.22 28 0.37 64 0.33 100 0.21 136 0.80 29 1.66 65 0.72 101 0.86 137 1.05 30 1.22 66 0.64 102 0.79 138 0.59 31 0.99 67 0.63 103 0.98 139 1.49 32 192* 68 1.34 104 1.15 140 0.90 33 1.18 69 0.28 105 0.43 141 2.38* 34 1.01 70 1.28 106 1.40 142 0.72 35 1.64 71 0.93 107 0.87 143 0.38 36 1.45 72 0.32 108 298* 144 0.75 Guilford-Martin Inventory of Factors GAMIN (Continued) 200 Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 145 0.36 156 0.56 167 0.88 178 0.39 146 2.14* 157 1.21 168 0.61 179 2.18* 147 1.13 158 0.36 169 1.85* 180 0.25 148 0.28 159 1.13 170 0.79 181 0.73 149 0.64 160 1.56 171 0.70 182 2.30* 150 0.68 161 0.78 172 0.86 183 2.04* 151 0.30 162 1.48 173 1.44 184 1.15 152. 1.13 163 0.34 174 1.75 185 0.43 153 0.33 164 0.61 175 1.04 186 0.63 154 1.25 165 0.14 176 0.56 155 1.85* 166 0.59 177 0.42 201 Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 1 0.77 37 0.31 73 1.14 109 0.33 2 1.43 38 0.26 74 1.27 110 1.25 3 1.10 39 1.70 75 0.83 111 0.48 4 0.30 40 0.45 76 0.38 112 1.15 5 1.67 41 0.69 77 0.71 113 1.18 6 0.15 42 0.75 78 0.89 114 0.72 7 0.30 43 0.44 79 0.97 115 1.10 8 0.32 44 0.38 80 2.46* 116 0.46 9 0.90 45 1.42 81 0.14 117 0.95 10 0.87 46 0.73 82 2.99* 118 0.38 11 0.43 47 0.28 83 0.59 119 1.41 12 258* 48 0.73 84 0.17 120 0.99 13 1.16 49 1.31 85 0.36 121 1.02 14 2.94* 50 0.43 86 1.10 122 1.14 15 1.14 51 0.30 87 0.70 123 1.13 16 1.27 52 0.85 88 1.52 124 0.45 17 1.13 53 0.87 89 1.47 125' 0.99 18 1.14 54 0.40 90 0.86 126 1.29 19 0.85 55 0.75 91 1.16 127 0.19 20 1.25 56 1.35 92 0.29 ' 128 1.79 21 1.13 57 252* 93 1.13 129 0.72 22 1.69 58 0.42 94 0.67 130 0.56 23 0.99 59 1.40 95 1.18 131 1.09 24 1.22 60 0.65 96 0.69 132 0.99 25 0.76 61 0.28 97 0.85 133 0.81 26 0.14 62 1.06 98 0.83 134 1.40 27 1.65 63 1.02 99 0.68 135 1.03 28 1.70 64 0.85 100 2.35* 136 0.47 29 1.70 65 0.50 101 0.45 137 1.39 30 0.45 66 1.28 102 1.70 138 0 .63 31 0.97 67 1.71 103 0.72 139 1.32 32 0 .78 68 0 .99 104 1.98* 140 1.27 33 1.22 69 0 .16 105 1.30 141 0 .99 34 0.81 70 1.13 106 0.85 142 0.43 35 1.02 71 0 107 1.31 143 0.71 36 1.46 72 0.31 108 0.75 144 0.28 Guilford Inventory of Factors STDCR (Continued) 202 Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 145 1.07 153 1.59 161 1.73 169 2.64* 146 2.36* 154 0.61 162 0.58 170 1.63 147 0.32 155 0.56 163 1.28 171 1.19 148 197* 156 1.28 164 0.66 172 1.64 149 0.68 157 0.57 165 1.67 173 0.71 150 1.41 158 0.90 166 0.48 174 0.62 151 1.74 159 0.89 167 0.85 175 0.57 152 0.63 160 0.90 168 0.71 203 Johnson Temperament Analysis Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 1 1.18 37 1.28 73 1.39 109 0.71 2 1.98* 38 1.28 74 1.21 110 2.13* 3 0.29 39 0.78 75 1.12 111 1.27 4 2.19* 40 0.63 76 0.88 112 0.25 5 1.19 41 0.56 77 0.96 113 1.06 6 0.79 42 0.99 78 0.45 114 1.65 7 0.29 43 0.91 79 1.14 115 1.17 8 0.47 44 1.18 80 1.20 116 0.76 9 0.99 45 2.13* 81 0.83 117 1.45 10 1.04 46 0.86 82 0.84 118 0.75 11 0.64 47 1.07 83 1.63 119 0.99 12 1.58 48 0.14 84 2.06* 120 1.19 13 0.29 49 1.55 85 0.35 121 0.49 14 0.29 50 0.64 86 1.35 122 2.80* 15 1.34 51 2.38* 87 1.55 123 0.28 16 0.44 52 2.86* 88 0.62 124 0.43 17 0.68 53 1.15 89 0.42 125 1.38 18 1.74 .54 1.19 90 1.97* 126 1.09 19 1.84* 55 1.50 91 0.99 127 2.24* 20 1.42 56 1.29 92 1.18 128 1.30 21 2.05* 57 2.21* 93 0.44 129 233* 22 1.79 58 1.25 94 0.70 130 1.13 23 1.27 59 1.85* 95 1.91* 131 0.76 24 0.70 60 0.57 96 0.83 . 132 1.90* 25 0.58 61 1.84* 97 0.97 133 2.17* 26 0.70 62 0.69 98 1.27 134 1.01 27 1.67 63 0.47 99 2.00* 135 2.73* 28 0.70 64 0.57 100 0.72. - 136 2.14* 29 0.86 65 0.51 101 0.59 137 2.66* 30 1.18 66 1.41 102 0.44 138 2.05* 31 0.16 67 0.71 103 0.30 139 0.98 32 1.09 68 1.06 104 0.57 140 0.46 33 1.36 69 0.79 105 2.04* 141 0.18 34 1.14 70 1.98* 106 0.16 142 1.88* 35 0.45 71 0.30 107 0.58 143 1.58 36 2.19* 72 0.75 108 1.28 144 1.54 Johnson Temperament Analysis (Continued) 204 Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 145 1.03 155 2.04* 165 1.69 175 1.45 146 1.08 156 1.82* 166 1.01 176 0.85 147 0.43 157 2.26* 167 2.83* 177 0.42 148 0.99 158 2.33* 168 0.75 178 1.03 149 1.14 159 1.94* 169 0.74 179 1.02 150 0.55 160 2.25* 170 1.54 180 1.27 151 1.89* 161 2.54* 171 0.87 181 1.13 152 0.62 162 1.02 172 0.77 182 0.76 153 1.26 163 1.69 173 0.82 154 0.29 164 0.42 174 2.01* California Mental Health Analysis 205 Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 1 0.69 37 0 73 2.35* 109 0.30 2 0 38 0.99 74 0.55 110 0.31 3 0.92 39 0.52 75 1.41 111 0.42 4 1.30 40 0.70 76 0.75 112 0.37 5 1.45 41 1.77 77 1.25 113 0.95 6 0.40 42 0.76 78 1.81 114 0.80 7 0.44 43 0.95 79 0.81 115 1.50 8 0.16 44 1.81 80 0.34 116 0.80 9 2.18* 45 1.25 81 1.33 117 0.31 10 0.33 46 0.44 82 2.14* 118 1.67 11 1.52 47 0.92 83 0.29 119 0.93 12 0.71 48 0.71 84 0.56 120 0.64 13 1.57 49 0.49 85 0.58 121 1.25 14 1.63 50 0.28 86 0.58 122 1.53 15 0.75 51 3.18* 87 1.76 123 0.30 16 1.92* 52 1.63 88 1.69 124 0.17 17 0.51 53 1.40 89 1.03 125 0.49 18 0 54 0 90 0.72 126 0 19 1.09 55 1.38 91 0.30 127 2.80* 20 0.56 56 1.87* 92 0.26 128 0.94 21 1.00 57 1.32 93 1.76 129 0.30 22 2.46* 58 0.32 94 0.44 130 0.14 23 1.68 59 0.15 95 1.28 131 0.89 24 0.66 60 0 96 1.33 132 1.03 25 0 61 0.97 97 0.35 133 0.51 26 1.48 62 1.27 98 1.35 134 0.41 27 0.78 63 0.14 99 0.75 135 2.00* 28 2.32* 64 2.56* 100 2.36* 136 0.15 29 2.19* 65 3.43* 101 1.12 137 0.76 30 0.25 66 0.55 102 0.68 138 0.14 31 2.00* 67 0.70 103 1.71 139 0.91 32 1.29 68 0.91 104 0.18 140 0.57 33 1.54 69 1.27 105 0.78 141 1.31 34 0.85 70 0.46 106 0.21 142 0.34 35 0.28 71 0.75 107 0.57 143 0.66 36 1.57 72 0.79 108 1.76 144 0.61 California Mental Health Analysis (Continued) 206 Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 145 0.86 159 0.44 173 1.62 187 0.64 146 1.17 160 1.71 174 0 188 0.30 147 212* 161 0.70 175 0.61 189 1.32 148 1.71 162 0.56 176 2.09* 190 2.38* 149 0.40 163 0.71 177 262* 191 1.43 150 0.73 164 0.58 178 0.14 192 0.57 151 1.40 165 0.99 179 0.86 193 0.28 15.2 2.30* 166 0.34 180 0.99 194 0.85 153 264* 167 1.77 181 1.40 195 0.79 154 0.16 168 1.77 182 0.24 196 1.12 155 1.27 169 1.03 183 1.25 197 1.14 156 0.82 170 0 184 1.43 198 1.27 157 0.99 171 0.47 185 1.42 199 0.45 158 1.58 172 0.75 186 1.07 200 0.76 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory 207 Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 1 0.97 37 1.15 73 0.45 109 1.76 2 1.41 38 1.73 74 1.31 110 0.69 3 0.97 39 0.98 75 1.19 111 0.14 4 0 40 0.58 76 0.42 112 1.26 5 0.57 41 0.50 77 0.14 113 1.55 6 1.70 42 0.97 78 0.61 114 1.02 7 1.40 43 0.93 79 1.71 115 1.82* 8 0.19 44 0.23 80 0.59 116 1.28 9 0.64 45 2.08* 81 1.72 117 2.02* 10 1.59 46 0.81 82 1.59 118 2.56* 11 0 47 0.49 83 0.71 119 0.94 12 1.24 48 1.04 84 1.14 120 1.41 13 0.53 49 0.55 85 0 121 0.68 14 2.90* 50 0.51 86 1.54 122 0.57 15 0.14 51 1.91* 87 0.36 123 1.27 16 0.19 52 1.39 88 1.20 124 0.78 17 0.71 53 0.86 89 0.45 125 0.51 18 0.47 54 2.38* 90 0.98 126 2.74* 19 0.63 55 2..l8* 91 0.25 127 0.31 20 1.76 56 1.84* 92 1.09 128 1.16 21 0 57 0.58 93 0.61 129 1.28 22 1.27 58 1.73 94 1.84* 130 0.29 23 0.81 59 0.30 95 0.74 131 0.56 24 0.20 60 0.58 96 0 132 0.14 25 1.15 61 1.71 97 2.22* 133 1.27 26 0.42 62 0.46 98 1.62 134 0.14 27 1.09 63 0.28 99 0.42 135 1.76 28 1.41 64 0.57 100 0 136 2.45* 29 1.03 65 1.02 101 1.23 137 0.86 30 0 .47 66 0 .20 102 1.49 138 0 .47 31 0.33 67 0.14 103 0.58 139 1.29 32 1.54 68 0.14 104 1.61 140 1.49 33 1.56 69 0.38 105 0.30 141 0.98 34 0.71 70 1.02 106 1.22 142 1.62 35 0.90 71 0.52 107 1.44 143 1.86* 36 0.56 72 1.42 108 1.54 144 0.85 208 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (Continued) Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 145 0.34 181 0.56 217 0.45 253 1.51 146 0.82 182 1.60 218 0.89 254 0.43 147 0.14 183 1.24 219 0.94 255 0.89 148 1.78 184 0.51 220 1.42 256 1.14 149 0.36 185 0.81 221 0.69 257 0.20 150 1.93* 186 0.38 222 0.29 258 0.83 151 0.33 187 0.74 223 1.54 259 0.75 152 0.97 188 0.47 224 2.84* 260 0.14 153 0.59 189 0.55 225 1.06 261 0.43 154 1.18 190 0.48 226 1.69 262 0.30 155 0.29 191 0.63 227 0.49 263 0.29 156 0.83 192 1.50 228 1.22 264 0.57 157 1.09 193 0.29 229 0.44 265 0.29 158 1.29 194 0.84 230 1.14 266 1.25 159 1.14 195 0.44 231 1.03 267 0.32 160 3.05* 196 1.03 232 1.57 268 1.98* 161 0.23 197 1.25 233 0.31 269 1.67 162 1.01 198 0.41 234 0.70 270 0.28 163 1.32 199 1.62 235 0.85 271 1.29 164 0.21 200 1.40 236 0.61 272 3.06* 165 0.57 201 1.19 237 1.98* 273 0.40 166 0.47 202 0.24 238 0.63 274 0.31 167 0 .58 203 0 .42 239 1.55 275 0.49 168 0.59 204 1.28 240 0.99 276 1.03 169 0 .59 205 0 .58 2.41 0 .29 277 1.74 170 0.43 206 1.79 242 0.43 278 0.63 171 1.43 207 1.21 243 1.33 279 0.88 172 0.61 208 0.57 244 0.71 280 0.14 173 1.80 209 1.02 245 0.57 281 0.85 174 0.14 210 1.09 246 0.58 282 1.72 175 1.02 211 0.24 247 1.95* 283 1.32 176 0 212 2.04* 248 0.85 284 1.01 177 1.63 213 0.77 249 1.43 285 0.14 178 0.91 214 1.20 250 0.28 286 0.68 179 0.47 215 1.18 251 0.77 287 1.71 180 0.32 216 1.15 252 2.04* 288 0.71 209 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (Continued) Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 289 0.48 325 0.52 361 0.47 397 0.31 290 1.21 326 0.49 362 2.21* 398 0.29 291 0.59 327 1.19 363 0.50 399 0.85 292 0.44 328 1.47 364 0.93 400 1.28 293 0.22 329 2.04* 365 0.26 401 0.14 294 0.15 330 0.88 366 0.60 402 1.29 295 1.28 331 1.18 367 0.85 403 0.61 296 0.43 332 0.99 368 1.42 404 0.45 297 0.46 333 0.76 369 1.57 405 0.15 298 0.28 334 0.82 370 1.38 406 0.47 299 1.37 335 0.30 371 1.16 407 0.84 300 O .99 336 0 .82 372 1.84* 408 0 .16 301 0.99 337 1.73 373 0.42 409 1.13 302 0.72 338 1.14 374 0.43 410 0.14 303 2.30* 339 1.76 375 0.85 411 1.55 304 0.44 340 0.64 376 0.47 412 0.76 305 0.75 341 1.06 377 0.46 413 0.57 306 1.19 342 1.40 378 1.15 414 0.49 307 0.42 343 1.40 379 0.42 415 0.72 308 0.42 344 0.68 380 1.66 416 2.05* 309 0.99 345 1.02 381 1.60 417 1.13 310 0.83 346 0.69 382 0.14 418 1.34 311 3.03* 347 0.42 383 0.14 419 1.13 312 1.49 348 0.57 384 0.59 420 2.82* 313 0.44 349 1.46 385 1.33 421 0.75 314 0.43 350 1.11 386 1.73 422 0.86 315 0.61 351 0.44 387 0.30 433 0.48 316 1.04 352 0.92 388 1.60 424 0.37 317 1.99* 353 0.42 389 0.45 425 1.34 318. 1.67 354 0.37 390 0.71 426 1.13 319 0.99 355 1.80 391 0.96 427 1.39 320 1.09 356 0.54 392 2.32* 428 0.68 321 0.76 357 1.18 393 0.49 429 0.30 322 0.30 358 1.24 394 1.58 430 0.92 323 0.85 359 0.33 395 1.00 431 0.15 324 0.50 360 0. 33 396 1.86* 432 0.71 210 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (Continued) Item CR Item CR Item CR Item CR 433 1.02 467 0.57 501 0.58 535 0.69 434 1.22 468 1.15 502 0.84 536 1.14 435 1.63 469 1.33 503 0.42 537 0.44 436 1.22 470 1.28 504 0.85 538 0.79 437 1.87* 471 1.02 505 0 539 1.47 438 0.96 472 0.26 506 0.49 540 1.48 439 0.45 473 1.05 507 1.27 541 0.31 440 0.69 474 0.28 508 0.90 542 0.70 441 0.42 475 0.99 509 0.76 543 1.49 442 0 4.76 0.92 510 1.19 544 0.71 443 0 477 1.13 511 0.17 545 1.14 444 1.30 478 1.70 512 0.66 546 1.15 445 0.36 479 0.15 513 0.85 547 2.40* 446 1.69 480 0.42 514 0.18 548 0.89 447 1.71 481 2.14* 515 0.71 549 0.53 448 1.29 482 0.14 516 2.08* 550 , 0.98 449 0.56 483 0.99 517 0.36 551 0.30 450 0.42 484 1.34 518 1.16 552 0.76 451 1.03 485 1.90* 519 0.45 . 553 1.67 452 0 486 1.04 520 1.69 554 1.27 453 1.14 487 0.63 521 0.58 555 1.46 454 2.36* 488 0.56 522 0.29 556 0.55 455 0.43 489 0.99 523 1.28 557 0.70 456 1.64 490 1.22 524 0.71 558 1.03 457 2.28* 491 0.30 525 1.48 559 0.94 458 0.99 492 1.85* 526 0.49 560 0.60 459 0.18 493 0.70 527 1.57 561 0.50 460 0.49 494 0.83 528 0.90 562 1.50 461 0.56 495 0.56 529 1.76 563 0.32 462 0.59 496 1.42 530 0.87 564 1.67 463 0.29 497 0.93 531 1.03 565 1.86* 464 0.85 498 1.16 532 0.97 566 1.29 465 0.99 499 1.74 533 1.01 466 0.43 500 0.57 534 0.92 v' 79179.7}... 1“ " . a L’ ? 7 "f I . I. o rib-i ~. 34 "s t w ' 01 , . v P . Q «L I 1., , “name .- .. %\ ya a -‘ '5. _J ._ ‘- .-\ w“ 1"? 5.1"? . ' - z 'v . ' ’\ _ - .' ' If ’ A. .4- -- '7‘ 1' if“) ’ . ‘m t I .'11. .' 1 I 'r 1' 1" ' ' f ‘,1__ . ' r 1 r —"—— — —'—_—~ "I7'111114111111111111111“