8C tr an ing ting ABSTRACT INFORMAL COMMUNICATION AND PROPINQUITY AMONG VOLUNTEER CLINICAL FACULTY IN A MEDICAL SCHOOL By David M. Price Purpose of the Study. Medical school administrators seek cost-effective means of improving the flow of information to and among volunteer clinical faculty (preceptors). To a greater or lesser extent, role-related information is carried in informal, unofficial channels as preceptors talk with each other and with other colleagues. If it could be shown that certain formal characteristics of part-time clinical fac- ulty were related to higher rates of informal communication about the College and their role in it, and if it could be further established that such increased communication were positively associated with the incidence of desired perceptions or attitudes, then administrators could be guided accordingly in the selection of clinical teachers and in the conduct of training and support efforts. One set of sudh formal characteristics define propinquity (nearness in time or place) and include solo vs. group practice, relative activity in professional associations, size of community, etc. Accord- ingly, this study sought to answer two questions: 1. Are propinquity factors with respect to practice sete ting and institutional affiliations associated with differences in self- l . ll ‘)1 t: Pr Pr. David M. Price reported communication behavior? 2. Are characteristics of self-reported communication be- havior associated with differences in perceptions of role-related infor- mation and information exchange, and with differences in the relative salience of the preceptor role? Conduct of the study. The population utilized was comprised of part-time, volunteer clinical faculty appointed in the Department of Family Medicine of the College of Osteopathic Medicine at Michigan State University. All subjects were engaged in private, comprehensive family practice and were on the staffs of accredited hospitals. Primary data were gathered by mailed questionnaire. The questionnaire was developed through a lengthy series of steps in an effort to assure the inclusion of essential variables, an appropriate operational definition of vari- ables, and general clarity of language and form. Analysis of the survey data involved the generation by compu- ter of a large number of tables, each involving the direct comparison of two variables. The majority of variable pairings involved either a propinquity measure and an informal communication measure or an informal communication measure and a perception measure, as entailed by the cen- tral purposes of the study. Some additional comparisons were made be- tween propinquity variables and "control" variables. No measures of actual knowledge or of performance were included as outcome criteria against which to compare informal communication behavior. Conclusions. In general, associations were found between key propinquity factors and both the frequency and the duration with which preceptors talked with other physicians about the College's preceptorship le th pr. 19: [5101‘ at David M. Price program. Further, frequency and duration of such informal communication were found to be associated with more favorable perceptions of information about the program and with perceived clarity of the College's expecta- tions for preceptors. Prominent among the study's sixteen specific conclusions were these: --Preceptors in group practice talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program more frequently than do preceptors in solo prac- tice. --Preceptors in urban-suburban communities talk with other phy- sicians about the preceptorship program more frequently than do preceptors in rural-small town communities. --Preceptors who spend considerable time in their hospitals visiting patients talk both more frequently and for longer periods with other physicians about the preceptorship program than do those who spend less time in the hospital visiting patients. --Preceptors active in board and/or staff committee work in their hospitals have more frequent and longer conversations about the preceptorship program during hospital meetings than do those who are less active in hospital affairs. --Preceptors more active in professional associations have more frequent and longer conversations about the preceptorship program at association meetings than do those who are less active. --Preceptors who more frequently (or for longer durations) talk informally with other physicians about the preceptorship program more often have positive perceptions of the information available from all sources about the preceptorship program and more often perceive that David M. Price being a preceptor enhances their professional competence and status, relative to other professional roles. --Preceptors who generally have longer conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program more often perceive that the College has made clear its expectations and more often respond to unclari- ty by working out "what should be done" in collaboration with other physicians. --Preceptors whose conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program are generally about "finding better ways to do our job" (innovation function) more often perceive available role-related information favorably than do those whose conversations are generally about "clarifying what the College expects" (production function). On the basis of these findings, several recommendations are offered to those responsible for the conduct of community preceptorship programs. Criteria for selection of preceptors might include participa- tion in group practice, considerable time visiting hospitalized patients, an active role in the hospital staff and activity in professional associ- ations. Formal effort to enhance communication might take advantage of and further stimulate already existant informal interchange by such means as holding meetings on an area basis at local hospitals. It is recommended that communication research concepts and tools be further exploited for organizational analysis in medical education. Subsequent research on informal communication and propinquity among clinical faculty can probably involve the formulation and testing of hypotheses. INFORMAL COMMUNICATION AND PROPINQUITY AMONG VOLUNTEER.CLINICAL FACULTY IN A MEDICAL SCHOOL By David Mngrice A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Administration and Higher Education 1974 Copyright DAVID McCLELLAN PRICE 1974 DEDICATION to my father ALVIN E. PRICE, M.D. Clinical Assistant Professor Emeritus Wayne State University He has provided me an early and continuing example of a really good physician. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my sincere appreciation to my committee chair- man, Vandel C. Johnson, Ph.D., for wise counsel and gentle encouragement. The director for this dissertion, Richard Vincent Farace, Ph.D., of the Department of Communication, provided step-by-step counsel that was both skilled and unstinting. Other committee members gave much thoughtful consideration to my task and the means of accomplishing it. They were William A. Herzog, Ph.D., of the Department of Communication; James H. Nelson, Ph.D., of the Department of Administration and Higher Education; and Ronald W. Richards, Ph.D., of the Office of Medical Education Research and Development (OMERAD). To Myron S. Magen, D.O., Dean of the College of Osteopathic Medicine and Gerald A. Faverman, MMA., Director of Research and Planning for the College, I owe the freedom and encouragement to pursue this study while employed as a half-time graduate assistant in their offices. For enthusiastic interest and practical support, Robert C. Ward, D.O., Chair- man of the Department of Family Medicine, and members of his staff are much appreciated. Among other persons who have significantly contributed to my understanding of the outstanding issues and promising develOpments in medical education are David S. Greenbaum, M.D., Richard B. Baldwin, D.O., Donald Neston, M.D., Margaret Z. Jones, M.D., Jack L. Maatsch, Ph.D., Ann C. Olmsted, Ph.D., Russell G. Camber, D.O., Arthur S. Elstein, Ph.D., iii and Donald M. Gragg, MQD., Ph.D. The list could well include other per- sons; my sense of grateful good fortune certainly does. I am thankful for the generous and open responses of the community physicians who hold clinical appointments in the Department of Family Medicine, especially to those who welcomed me to their offices for face-to-face interviews. Patricia Grauer did an excellent job of preparing the manu- script. Linda K. Glendening and Joseph Wisenbaker, of the School for Advanced Graduate Studies, gave wise counsel and material assistance in the preparation of appropriate computer programs. Robert A. Kern and Gary.Anthony offered much help in the preparation of data for computer analysis. Use of the Michigan State University computing facilities was made possible though support, in part, from the National Science Foundation. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES viii LIST OF FIGURES xi Chapter I. I. THE PROBLEM. . . . . . . . . . l Practitioners Who Teach. . . . . . . .. 1 External Preceptorships. . . . . . . . 1 Significance for Administrators. . . . . . 4 Propinquity. . . . . . . . . . 5 Purpose of the Study . . . . . . . 6 An Organizational Communication Model . . . . 6 Assumptions. . . . . . . . . . . 8 Limitations . . . . . . . 8 Definitions of Key Terms . . . . . . . 10 Conduct of Current Study . . . . . . . 13 Overview . . . . . . . . . . . 13 II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE. . . . . . . . 15 A Basic Theoretical Approach . . . . . . 16 Basic Communication Concepts . . . . . . 19 Pressures to Communicate . . . . . . . 27 Propinquity. . . . . . . . . 33 .Attitudes and Perceptions . . . . . . . 41 III. METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 Population . . . . . . . . . . . 44 Questionnaire Contents . . . . . . . . 46 Questionnaire Administration . . . . . . 47 Design of the Questionnaire . . . . . . 47 Procedure for Analysis . . . . . . . . 49 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . 50 IV. ANALYSIS: PROPINQUITY AND INFORMAL COMMUNICATION. . . 52 Profile on Propinquity and "Control" Variables . . 52 Practice Setting. . . . . . . . . 52 Activity in Hospitals . . . . . 53 Activity in Professional Associations . . . 56 "Control" Variables. . . . . . . . 57 Chapter Comparison of Propinquity and "Control" Factors. Solo and Group Practice. . . . . . . Frequency . . . .. . . . . . Function . . . . . . . . . Size of Group Practice . . . . . . . Number of Other Preceptors . . . . . . Size of Community . . . . . . . Proximity to Campus. . . . . . . . Activity in Hospitals . . . . . . Time Spent in Visiting Patients . . . . Time in Hospital Meetings . . . . Activity in Professional Associations . . . Summary. . . . . . . . . . V. ANALYSIS: INFORMAL COMMUNICATION AND PERCEPTIONS. . Profile on Perception Variables. . . . . Perception of Information . . . . . Ideas for Improvement . . . . . . Salience of the Preceptor Role . . . . Frequency of Communication . . . . . Frequency and Perception of Information . . Frequency and Perception of Sources . . . Frequency and Salience of Preceptor Role. Frequency with Full-time Faculty and Perception of Information . . . . Frequency with Full-time Faculty and Perception of Interest . . . . Duration of Communication . . . . . . Initiation of Communication. . . . . . Function of Communication . . . . . . Summary. . . . . . . . . . . VI. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS. . . . . . . Summary. . . . . . . . . Background of the Study . . . . . . Specific Purposes . . . . . Theoretical Underpinnings and Prior Research. Conduct of the Study. . . . . . . Limitations of the Study. . . . . . Main Conclusions . . . . . . . Propinquity and Informal Communication . . Informal Communication and Perceptions . . Implications of Findings about "Control" Variables . . . . . . . Implications for Administrators. . . . . Implications for Further Research . . . . vi Page 59 67 67 67 74 76 78 80 83 83 85 89 95 99 100 100 106 110 111 111 114 116 118 . 121 123 125 127 131 135 135 135 136 137 137 138 138 139 140 141 . 143 145 APPENDIX. . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY. . . Sources Cited . General References vii 147 155 155 160 Table 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 4.8 4.9 4.10 4.11 LIST OF TABLES Profile of Preceptor Population: Practice Setting Variables Profile of Preceptor Population: Activity in Hospitals Hours in Hospital Meetings by Category of Self-Judged. ActiVity . O I O O O O I O O O I Profile of Preceptor Population: Activity in Associations. Comparison of Activity in Professional Associations . Profile of Preceptor Population: "Control" Variables . "Control" Variables by Propinquity Variables . . . Comparisons of "Control" Variables . . . . Overall Frequency by "Control" Factors . . . Solo and Group Practice According to Frequency Solo and Group Practice By Location of Most Frequent Conversations. . . . . . . . . . . Solo and Group Practice By Function of Office Communication Office Frequency By Office Function . . . . . Size of Group By Frequency of Office Communication . Group Size By Function of Office Communication . . Number of Other Preceptors By Frequency of Office Communication . . . . . . . . . . Number of Other Preceptors By Function of Office Communication . . . . . . . . . Urban and Rural Preceptors By Overall Frequency . Urban and Rural Preceptors By Location of Most Frequent Conversations . . . . . . viii Page .54 .63 .63 .65 .68 .71 .73 .74 .74 .75 .76 .77 .78 .79 Table Page 4.20 Overall Frequency According to Proximity to Campus . 81 4.21 Proximity to Campus By Frequency of Communication with Full-time Faculty. . . . . . . . 82 4.22 Hours in Hospital to See Patients By Frequency. . . 83 4.23 Hours in Hospital to See Patients By Duration . . 84 4.24 Hours in Hospital to See Patients By Function . . . 84 4.25 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings By Frequency . . . . . . . . . . . 86 4.26 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings By Duration 0 O I O O O I C O 86 4.27 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings By Initiation . . . . . . . . . . 87 4.28 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings By Function . . . . . . . . . . . 88 4.29 Activity in Professional Associations by Frequency. . 90 4.30 Activity in Professional Associations by Duration . . 92 4.31 Activity in Professional Associations by Initiation . 93 4.32 Activity in Professional Associations by Function . . 94 5.1 Profile of Preceptor Population:Perception of Information Sources and Interest . . . . . . 101 5.2 Profile of Preceptor Population: Ideas for Improvement. 107 5.3 Profile of Preceptor Population: Salience of Role . . 109 5.4 Overall Frequency of Communication By Perceptions of Information . . . . . . . . . . . 112 5.5 Overall Frequency By Perceptions of Information Sources 115 5.6 Overall Frequency By Perception of College Interest . 116 5.7 Overall Frequency By Salience of Preceptor Role . . 117 5.8 Frequency with Full-Time Faculty By Perceptions of Information . . . . . . . . . . . 119 ix Table Page 5.9 Frequency with Full-time Faculty By Overall Frequency . 121 5.10 Frequency with Full-time Faculty By Perception of College Interest . . . . . . . . . . 122 5.11 Perceptions of Information By Duration of Hospital Conversations . . . . . . . . . . . 124 5.12 Perceptions of Information By Measures of Conversation Initiation . . . . . . . . . . . 126 5.13 Perceptions of Information By Communication Function in Two Locations . . . . . . . . . . 128 5.14 Suggestions for Improvement and Preferred Source By Function of In—Office Communication . . . . . 130 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1.1 Organizational Communication Model. . . . . . 5 1.2 A Model for the Study of Propinquity and Informal Communication . . . . . . . . . . . 7 xi Chapter I THE PROBLEM Welsh Medical schools typically make extensive use of practicing physicians as teachers of patient care skills and role models for medi- cal practice. Such clinical faculty are ordinarily engaged in full-time private practice and teach only a relatively few hours each week. Though officially appointed to their teaching roles, they do not attend regular, departmental faculty meetings, serve on college committees or have of- fices in the medical school facilities. Clinical faculty are, first and foremost, practitioners; only secondarily, even peripherally, are they teachers. Thus, part-time clinical faculty do not ordinarily participate in the principal communication networks through which "regular" faculty members are inducted, informed and guided, and through which they offer their contributions to policy formulation and curricular design. Be- cause the relationship of part-time clinical faculty to the college and its instructional programs is thus attenuated by time, distance and professional identity, they tend to be poorly integrated into the infor- mation systems of the College.1 The extent to which clinical faculty 1Sinclair, David C., Basic Medical Education, London: Oxford University Press, 1972, p. 46; Merton, Robert K., George G. Reader and Patricia L. Kendall, Th Student Ph sician, Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957, pp.259-60; Weinerman, E. Richard, "Yale Studies in Ambula- tory Medical Care." WWJNNW'S‘h 1 2 understand the particular objectives of the College's program, the shape of its curriculum, the characteristics of its students and its expectations for their roles is a matter of concern among those who administer programs of clinical instruction.2 Although various efforts to inform and involve adjunct personnel are typically mounted by depart- ment leaders, virtually no research on communication with and among clinical faculty is available for the guidance of medical school admin- istrators. External Preceptorships In many medical schools, the "clinical exposure" of students is carried out almost exclusively in hosPitals and clinics operated by or administratively affiliated with the College itself. In such settings, communication with and among clinical faculty is facilitated by the fact that they are physically drawn into the medical education complex to meet with students. Indeed, Reader cites this factor as a prime consid- eration in arguing for teaching medicine in out-patient departments on or near the campus.3 By contrast, in programs which rely upon community facilities outside the medical school complex, clinical faculty are more 2Bowers, John 2., and Robert C. Parkin, "The Wisconsin Precep- tor Program --.A Thirty Year Experiment in Medical Education," Journal of Mgdigal Education, 32:610-612; Slaughter, Donald, "Clinical Clerk- ships for Sophomore Medical Students," Journal of Medical Educ tion, 32:193-199; Herzberg, Frederick, Scott Inkley and William R. Adams, "Some Effects on the Clinical Faculty of a Critical Incident Study of the Performance of Students," Journal of Medic l Educ tion, 35:666-674,p.671. 3Reader, George 3., "Some of the Problems and Satisfactions of Teaching Comprehensive'Medicine," Journ l of Medical Educ tion, 31:544-54. disparate and have fewer opportunities for interchange with regular faculty, departmental administrators and each other. The proportion of clinical instruction carried out in such external settings seems to be growing. Spurred by public demands for more accessible and comprehensive health care, many medical schools are hastening to produce more graduates prepared for and committed to what is variously called general practice, family practice or comprehensive, primary care.4 In turn, this effort is prompting greater emphasis on giving students extensive experience with ambulatory patients.5 Fur- ther, some medical schools are committed to placing students for a major share of their clinical exposure in the offices of private physi- cians or "preceptors." It is argued that such office-based practice most nearly reflects "real world" experience for most would-be doctors and affords them the role models, the kind of patient population and the community setting which they need if they are to understand (and 4Anlyon, William G., "Chairman's Address," Journal of Mgdical Education, 46:917-26; White, Kerr L., "Family Medicine, Academic Medi- cine, and University Responsibility," Journal of the American Medical Agsgclgtlgn, 185:192-6; Silver, George A., "Family Practice: Resusita- tion or Reform?" Journal of the Amerlcgg Medical Association, 185:189-91. 5For arguments in favor of ambulatory teaching, see Fleming, William L., "Teaching of the Family Physician's Approach by a Department of Preventive Medicine," Journgl of the American Mgdical Association, 161:711-3; Smith, Hugo D., "Essays in Medical Education," American Journal of the Disegses of Children, 110:185-8. For a comprehensive review of ambulatory teaching programs, see Gragg, Donald M., "The Teach- ing of Adult Ambulatory Patient Care in U.S. Medical Schools: Character- istics of'Programs," Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1973. 4 perhaps choose) family practice as a career.6 If participation of clinical faculty in information exchange with each other and with College leadership is an endemic problem in medical education, it may be assumed to be particularly acute among clinical faculty of colleges with programs of highly decentralized, "Community-based" clinical education. Signlfigance for Administrators Medical school administrators seek cost-effective means of improving the flow of role-related information to and among volunteer clinical faculty. Formal or official methods (memoranda, workshops, site visits by administrators, etc.) are part of this effort to improve communication. However, such institutionalized efforts comprise only part of the information exchange available to preceptors. Volunteer clinical faculty are also involved -- to a greater or lesser extent -- in informal and more-or-less spontaneous interchange with each other 6Reed, David E., "Twelve Years' Experience with a Comprehen- sive Ambulatory Care Program," Journal of Medical Education, 45:1041-6; Beloff, Jerome S., Meiko Korper, E. Richard Weinerman, "Medical Student Response to a Program for Teaching Comprehensive Care," Journal of Mgdlcgl Educgtlgn, 40:625-57; Cheplove, Max, "The Role of the Family Practitioner in Medical Education," New York State Jggrnal 9f Mgdicing, 68:1128-31; Trowbridge, Mason, "Extramural Preceptorships -- A Return to the Pre-Flexner Era of Medical Education?" New England Jogrngl of Mgdiclng, 258:691-5; Dorn, Robert M., "Preceptors and Preceptorships: The Teaching and Learning of Patient-oriented Care," Journal of the Kansas Medicgl Sogiety, 68:428-31; YOung, L.E., "Personal Physicians," Jggrngl of the Amerlcgn'Medical Society, 187:928-33; "The Teaching of Comprehensive Patient Care," Editorial, American Journal of Public Health, March 1970, pp. 429-32. 5 and with non-faculty professional colleagues about their college and its preceptorship program. However, it is not known whether such infor- mal communication tends to satisfy the information needs of preceptors. Moreover, it is not known what objective characteristics of preceptors are related to high levels of informal communications. If it can be shown that the amount of informal communication among preceptors about role-related matters varies with certain formal characteristics, then selection of preceptors according to these characteristics is a cost- effective method of improving communication among them. Propinquity Propinquity, defined as nearness in place or time, is a char- acteristic of individuals relative to other individuals, groups or insti- tutions. Propinquity factors of particular interest in distinguishing volunteer clinical faculty are: a. practice partners --solo practice --group practice --another preceptor in group --no other preceptor in group b. size of community (urban-suburban vs. rural-small town) c. physical proximity to campus d. activity (time spent) in hospital and professional associations All but the last of the above propinquity variables relate to the "setting" within which preceptors (and other physicians) practice. The term "practice setting" will be used to refer commonly to these characteristics. Purpose of the Sgudy The purpose of the study is to describe the relationships of these propinquity variables to informal communication behavior, and in turn, the relationships of informal communication behavior to certain perceptions or attitudes among one group of part-time, volunteer clini- cal faculty. The study seeks to answer the following broad questions: 1. Are characteristics of practice setting and insti- tutional affiliation (among one set of part-time clinical faculty) associated with differences in self-reported communication behavior? 2. Are characteristics of communication behavior (of one set of part-time clinical faculty) associated with differences in perceptions of the adequacy of role-related information and opportunities for information exchange, and to differences in the relative salience of the preceptor role? An Organizational Communication Model Communications research, as applied to organizations, has attempted to demonstrate that overt and measurable communication beha- vior of individuals is correlated, on the one hand, with manipuable, organizational variables (e.g., decentralization, means of coordination) and, on the other hand, with organization output criteria (e.g., pro- ductivity, employee satisfaction). To the extent that these correlations can be demonstrated, predictive inferences may be suggested which en- tail the operation of communication behaviors as intervening activities between organizational arrangements and various criterion measures on 7 the basis of which institutional leaders make decisions. Thus, communi- cations sdholars have developed theoretical models of the following pattern7: organizational I communication, . criterion variable behavior measure Figure 1.1 Organizational Communication Model In this model, some administrative strategy or action is pre- sumed to have an effect upon the communication behavior of system mem- bers, and, in turn, upon system outputs. For example, it may be hy- pothesized that more delegation of operational decisions (organization- al variable) leads to increased communication about production matters among lower level employees and to lower absenteeism rates (a criterion measure). An "organizational variable" is one subject to direct mani- pulation by organizational managers and a "criterion measure" is an outcome of interest to organizational managers. Note that "communica- tion behavior" is neither, but rather an intervening activity associated with both. It is proposed that this abstract organizational communica- tion model be utilized as a framework for the present study. Propin- quity factors fit the criterion for organizational variables in that, if used as criteria for the recruitment or retention of clinical facul- ty, they become formal features of the organized system susceptible of 7Cf. Nix, Harold L. and Frederick L. Bates, "Occupational Role Stresses," Rural Soclolggy, 27:7-17, p. 7. 8 direct control by institutional leaders. By mapping onto this abstract framework the particular dimen- sions of the proposed investigation, a graphic representation of the present study may be rendered as shown in Figure 1.2. Assaaptions The essential assumptions which underlie this study and the foregoing model are: a. Actual and overt communication activity of indi- C. viduals can be measured and compared on the basis of self-reported behavior. . Certain perceptions on the part of volunteer clinical faculty are valued by the administrators of preceptorship programs and that these include perceptions of available role-related information as adequate, perceptions of role expectations as clear, and perceptions of the role itself as sa- lient or relatively significant. Medical college administrators are able to adapt as criteria for the identification and appointment of preceptors certain objective factors, and that these factors include the "practice setting" and "activity in hospital or association" variables as defined in this study. Limltatlons No measures of actual knowledge of College policies, proce- dures or prescribed role definitions are included in the study. The cofiumoacsaaoo HmauowaH mam muwovcfimoum wo ~.H magmas oHou uoumoooua mo mocmfiamm muoumoa omoHHoo mo umoumucfi coaumuooexo mo huwumao mmooomlmmoacomo mouoom manusm pmuuomoua mouaom manmoam> umoa Cowumauomcfi mo >uHHmsv GOHUNEOMGH w 0 UCDOEN mZOHHmmUmmm AcOfiumoHasaaoo mo uxmucoo Hmofim>£av cowumooa moamaouafimall coaum>occfill coauosvoumll coauocomlucmucoo coaumwufica aofiumusw zocoowoum ZOHHufi>fiuom Hmufimmoz a“ mufi>wuom mseaoo ou hufiafixoua xuwcsaaoo mo mNHm mucueoooua nonuo moouw mo unamll macaw .m> oaomll mumouuma oowuomua wHHDOZHmOMm 10 outcome variables or criterion measures with which communication behavior is compared are wholly limited to preceptors' perceptlgns of available information, of information systems and of the role itself. Moreover, it must be acknowledged that the present study does not extend to measures of association between informal communication and any direct data about preceptor effectiveness (performance criteria). It should be noted that the model, as adopted for present pur- poses (Figure 1.2), does not presume prediction or the demonstration of causal relationships. This is an exploratory and descriptive, rather than experimental, study. Lastly, data was collected from preceptors in a single de- partment of one medical school, all of Whom are engaged in a single type of practice, Definition of Key Terms Clinical faculty is sometimes used to refer to all faculty in clinical departments or all faculty engaged in the teaching of the clinical skills involved in diagnosis and patient care. Throughout these pages, however, the term is used to designate faculty members who are practicing physicians engaged to teach part-time in clinical settings. Preceptor refers to a sub-class of adjunct clinical faculty, namely those practitioners who teach students in their own offices or private clinics. Pracaptorship pggggaa is a generic term covering all aspects of the organized effort required of College personnel in order that students may be afforded clinical experiences in the offices of family physicians. The tenn is meant to embrace the broadest range of such 11 activities: goal formulation, orientation of preceptors, teaching, student participation and evaluation. Propinquity is nearness in time or place. It is used here as the general designation for the set of variables differentiating the papulation of preceptors according to certain characteristics of prac- tice setting and relative activity in their hospitals and professional associations. Qroua pragtice refers to the practice setting circumstance of some preceptors in which they share their offices with one or more other physicians. The term does not distinguish any particular economic arrangement (public corporation, partnership, etc.). Solo practlce is the practice setting circumstance of some preceptors in which they do aa£,share their offices with any other physician. Informal communication refers to those interactions of pre- ceptors which are not a part of organized activities or deliberate mech- anisms of the College itself. Such informal communications, though they may be anticipated by College leaders, are not directly elicited or facilitated by persons acting for the College. Examples of informal interactions are personal conversations between two individuals in the course of a common activity, a telephone call to another preceptor or to a College official to seek clarification, and casual greetings-in- passing. The term is aa; intended to include the submissions of reports, discussions in the course of meetings called by the College authorities, or ccnmversations between preceptors and administrators during site visits by administrators . 12 Rola-related information is any information which has as its referent some aspect of the preceptor role or the College's preceptor program or experiences of persons occasioned by the conduct of their function as preceptors. angant-function is one of the subsets of informal communica- tion variables. It refers to the purposes or uses of communication in an interaction between two or more persons mutually concerned with the referent of the message(s). The three classes of content-function to be used here are (a) getting the job done (production), (b) exploring alternative means of getting the job done (innovation), and (c) main- taining the relationships which comprise the common social system (maintenance). Percaptions is the general term comprising the third set of variables in the study. The meaning of the term as it is used here is close to (if not synonymous with) that of "attitudes." Whereas the first set of variables (Pr0pinquity) describes the relative closeness in time and place of preceptors to other physicians, and the second set (Informal Communications) describes certain overt, interpersonal behaviors, this set is meant to describe the internal states or affec- tive orientations of the preceptors toward selected "objects." These objects are (a) the totality of role-related information available to them, (b) the means by which they receive this information, and (c) the salience (relative importance or value) of the preceptor role. Some of these terms, as well as other concepts upon which their meanings rest, receive further definition and elaboration in the following chapter. The precise operational definitions of those terms 13 which are labels for variables will be further specified in the course of reporting and analyzing the data (Chapters IV and V). Conduct of Current Study Primary data for the current study was gathered by mailed questionnaire from preceptors in the Department of Family Medicine of the College of Osteopathic Medicine at Michigan State University. Ex- cluded from the total list of eighty-eight preceptors were three who do limited teaching in campus classrooms and four who practice in the University's Health Service. Of the eighty-one remaining, all are in the private practice of comprehensive, family medicine. A more limited fund of data, introduced here largely for illustrative purposes, was gathered during a preliminary phase of open- ended interviews with ten of the preceptors. Overview Chapter II is devoted to a survey of theoretical and research literature of relevance to the present study. This survey is organized in five parts, three of which correspond with the three major sets of variables as illustrated in Figure 1.2. Chapter III, entitled "Method- ology," further specifies the population surveyed, the steps taken to develop a questionnaire and the means of analysis. Chapters IV and V are devoted to a report and analysis of the findings. In Chapter IV, a profile of the entire papulation on the propinquity and "control" variables is presented, "control" variables are compared with propinquity variables and propinquity variables are compared with informal communication variables. Chapter V contains a l4 profile of the population on the perception variables and analysis of the comparisons of informal communication variables with perception variables. Finally, the investigator's conclusions, reflections and recommendations comprise Chapter VI. Chapter II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE No single theoretical heritage suffices as underpinning for studies in organizational communication. One is obliged to reach into several fields for both conceptual and empirical grounding.1 The reader will notice, however, that the two streams of theory most frequently cited in these pages are general systems theory, especially as elaborated by James G. Miller,2 and field theory, prominently associated with such 3 4 seminal thinkers in social psychology as Kurt Lewin and Leon Festinger. This review of theoretical and empirical literature is organ- ized in five sections, including three which relate directly to the three sets of variables in this study. The first section focuses upon general systems theory as a general conceptual framework. The second surveys literature relative to communication concepts either implicit or ex- plicit in this study. Thayer, Lee, "Communication and Organization Theory" in Dance, Frank E.X. (ed.), Human Communication Theory, N.Y.: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1967. p. 72. 2See especially "Living Systems: Basic Concepts," Behavioral Scienca 10:193-237, 1965. 3See especially Flald Theory in Social Science, N.Y.: Harper,l951. 4See especially Theory ang Eageriment in Social Communication, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1950. A valuable secondary source on the work of Lewin and Festinger is Deutsch, Morton, and Robert M. Krauss, Theories in Social Psychology, N.Y.: Basic Books,1965. pp. 37-76. 15 16 Next, a transitional section briefly reviews literature bearing upon generalizations about pressures to communicate which have been said to devolve upon members of human social systems. The fourth section proceeds to consideration of that particular set of formal characteris- tics which is of particular importance here, namely propinquity factors. Empirical studies which demonstrate associations between distance or affiliation and social interaction comprise most of the citations in this section. The fifth section corresponds with the third set of variables shown in Figure 1.2, i.e., perceptions or attitudes as related to pro- pinquity and communication variables. A Basic Theoretical Fraaework Terms such as "communication" and "information" have been used in many different ways and often interchangably. Such usage is quite adequate to most purposes of general discourse. However, it is essential to the development of coherent theory and scientific precision that certain distinctions be made. Moreover, it is desirable that definitions for such basic terms be stipulated in such a way that application is possible across a wide range of referents and at various levels of organization. A most promising approach to this task -- a central one for any "young" science -- is that of general systems theory. Its most prominent expositor, as applied to the social sciences, is James G. Miller: 17 General systems theory is a set of related definitions, assumptions, and propositions which deal with reality as an integrated hierarchy or organizations of matter and energy. A "system," most simply described, is a set of interdependent elements. The word "interdependent" implies that each element or com- ponent is constrained or conditioned by the state of the other elements. The word "set" suggests common properties. It also suggests that, for something to be called a system, it must have "at least one measure of the sum of its units which is larger than the sum of that measure of its units." This is to say that a system is a configuration, a gestalt, as well as a mere collection of parts. Miller's forceful example is that a man with his head is much more than a man's body plus his separ- ate head.6 General systems theory conceives of the universe as composed of systems nested within systems, in which each system is embedded in a larger system in relation to which it is a subsystem. "Such a con- cept can be applied, for instance, to cells within tissues, to organs within an organism, to companies within an industry, to nations within an alliance.7 5 0p. cit., p. 193. 6 Ibid., p. 201. 7Berrien, Kenneth F., General and Social Systems, New Brunswick, N.Y.: Rutgers University Press, 1968. p. 15. 18 Briefly, the principal advantages of general systems theory are three in number. First, it permits the generation of propositions which may be applied over the wide ranges of levels of organization. That is, the molecule, the cell, the organ, the individual, the group, the society are all systems and, as such, may be analyzed in terms of a common set of concepts. The outcome of value is ease of generaliza- tion across system levels.8 Second, widely disparate ayaaa of systems may be compared and contrasted in reference to dimensions which they share. This is possible because systems may be living or non-living, conceptual or material, and so forth.9 Third, in contrast to many social science theories, general systems theory is non-prescriptive and avoids-reductionism. That is, rather than approaching a system -- let us say, a department in a medical school -- with some preconception of an ideal state for that system or some "key" to the analysis of system function, the investigator working from a general systems orientation can be rather more descriptive and inclusive.10 These advantages are presumed to pay off in terms of traditional scientific values such as objectivity, precision and susceptibility of empirical verification. 81bid., pp. 214-7. 9 Ibid., p. 214. 0Farace, Richard V., and Hamish M. Russell, "Some Communication Implications of Major Organizational Theories," mimeographed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1971. 14-8. 19 Basic Communication Concepts Miller says that even more basic to the understanding of gen- eral systems theory than the concept of "system" are the concepts of "matter," "energy" and "information." Relying on the known relations of matter and energy, including the principle of the conservation of energy, Miller typically uses the joint term.matter-energy to signify the most basic physical "stuff" that may be said to flow between systems or system components, without specifying the form or structure or type of f1ow.11 Information is that matter-energy which exhibits pattern or form.12 By contrast, unpatterned or random matter-energy is mere "noise."13 It is important to note two features of this definition of "information." The notion of information as patterned matter-energy presumes a perceiver of that pattern; only when system members actually detect a form in the matter-energy can it be properly labeled "information." Moreover, "in- formation" in this sense is a much simpler concept than.”information" as we ordinarily use it. "Information" need not have "meaning." "Mean- ing " is the significance of information to a system which possesses it.14 Thus, telegraphic signals are "information" whether or not the person at the receiving point knows Morse code. It is enough that he or she recognizes a pattern in the pulses. 11Op. cit., p. 193. 121pm, p. 193-4. 13 Ibid., p. 199. 14 Ibid., p. 193. 20 Communication, according to Miller, is ”the change of informa- tion from one state to another over space," or information processing. This is the process engaged in when one transmits sounds via electrical impulses over a telephone line or even projects his voice to another across a room. ". . . .informational patterns can be processed over space and the local matter-energy at the receiving point can be organized to conform to, or comply with, this information."16 The concept implied here is one of a referent common to both sender and receiver. Build- ing upon Miller‘s exposition, David K. Berlo succinctly defines "com- munication" as "a process involving the transfer of matter-energy that carries symbolic (i.e., referential) information."17 Thus "communication” is seen to be a sub-set of "information." Matter-energy moving in a system has "information value" only to the extent that system. members perceive patterning or form. In turn, in- formation has "communication value" only as system members have agreed- upon referents for the pattern. Note that these terms are defined in such a way that the per- ceptions of receivers are key criteria for proper classification. On the other hand, it should be clear that no solipsistic implication is intended; it is not denied that "real" information may be in my environ- ment without my attending to it or actively and momentarily perceiving 15 Ibid., p. 198. 16Ibid., p. 199. 17"Human Communication: The Basic Proposition," mimeograph. Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1970. pp. 2-3. 21 it. Indeed, a most useful pair of distinctions have been offered by information theorists which explicate the commonplace observation that there is typically more information in a given system than any of its members may apprehend in a given moment. The first of these distinctions is between absolute and ala- tributed information. "Absolute" information refers to the total amount of information available in a system, whereas "distributed" information refers to the homogeneity of information among members of the system.18 The distinction permits one to probe beyond, let us say, a complaint that "not enough information is available," to whether the problem is a genuine lack on the part of the system as a whole or simply a maldis- tribution of available information resources within the system. The second distinction is between information which is avail- able in the environment and that which is ultimately utilized by an individual or organization.19 The distinction highlights the ubiquitous use of "filters" or "screens" or (when they become explicit or deliber- ate) "decision rules" by which system members accept some information and reject or ignore other information. A relationship between these two distinctions is clear: the distribution of information in a system will depend, in part, upon the decision rules by which various system members screen out that informa- tion deemed "useless" or "irrelevant." 18Brillouin, Leon, Science and Information Theory, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1967. pp. 265-6. 19Reindl, Max H., "Propositions on Information Management of Innovation Processes in Organizations," unpublished doctoral disserta- tion, Michigan State University, 1970, pp. 36-9. 22 Consideration of distribution leads to a still more funda- mental notion in General Systems Theory. It is the concept of entro , the tendency of the matter-energy in a system to disperse chaotically or randomly. Familiar in physics as the Second Law of Thermodynamics, the concept of entropy signifies the disorder, disorganization or lack of patterning which is the most probable outcome of unconstrained movement of elements in a system.20 Recalling the definition of information as ”patterned matter-energy," the formal relationship between entropy and information is clear: information decreases as entropy increases. 'Miller cites evidence from the calculations of physicists and information theo- rists to support his contention that this relationship is more than merely formal or statistical and that it applies equally to large and small 21 systems. Building upon Miller's utilization of "entropy,” Berlo formed the closely related concept of uncertainty and gave it a "central" im- portance in his analysis of communication functions. A system may be said to be characterized by certainty to the extent to which "all alter- native events within the system can be articulated, and some probability attached to each." In other words, "the more organized a system is, the more predictable it is." Uncertainty, then, is a measure of dis- order or lack of structure or unpredictability.22 20 Miller, op. cit., p. 195. 21 Ibid., pp. 196-8. 22 Berlo, David K., "Essays on Communication," mimeographed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1970, p. III - l. 23 Again following Miller, Berlo stipulates a negative relation between information and uncertainty: "the more information that is transmitted, the clearer the pattern of the system becomes . . . and the more predictable it is."23 Thus, more information is required to render predictable an uncertain system. Moreover, it may be said that the function of information is to reduce uncertainty. Communication function or content-function has been considered from several perspectives by various authors. Two of the most fully deve10ped and most promising are reviewed here. The first of these is 24 A Russell Ackoff's content-free, but message-centered approach. message (set of "signs") produces responses in a receiver, thus effect- ing an alteration in his/her "purposive state." On the basis of this theoretical assumption, Ackoff proposes a set of categories into which messages may be indexed according to how they (are supposed to) change the receiver. He offers the following taxonomy: "information" changes the probabilities of choice, "instruction" changes the efficiencies of alternatives for action, and "motivation" changes the values of out- comes and thereby the basis for selecting among them. Berlo would apparently have us believe that Ackoff's analytic frame has nothing whatever to do with the participants in the communica- tion exchange.25 Actually, when taken in the context of his "behavioral 23 Ibid 0 24”Towards a Behavioral Theory of Communication," Management Science, 4:218-34, 1957. 250p. cit., p. III-2. 24 theory” with its emphasis on the "purposive state," Ackoff's analysis is not so simple-mindedly message-bound and participant-free.26 However, to say this is to suggest that, while no "straw-man," neither is Ackoff's categorization as scientifically advantageous as Berlo suggests. Indeed, Farace and McDonald find Ackoff's alternative wanting on just these grounds. Two serious problems are encountered in these message-centered approaches. The first is that where Operationalization has been carried out and studies con- ducted, the amount of variance explained in various dependent measures has been relatively small. Second, the techniques for categorizing or generating messages have not been worked out very effectively. Berlo offers the fullest of several discussions of functional categories based upon human purposes, rather than upon the messages themselves. He characterizes the difference in approach succinctly: Instead of asking what messages do to people, we can look at what peOple do to messages. Instead of asking what effect communication has, we can ask what uses people make of it, what purposes it serves for both participants.28 Berlo's three categories of function are production (getting the job done), innovation (finding new ways to do it), and maintenance 26See Buckley, Walter, Sociology and Modern Systems Theory, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, Inc., 1967. p. 120-1. 27Farace, Richard V. and Donald McDonald, "New Directions in the Study of Organiztion Communication," to be published in Personal Psychology, Spring 1974. 28Berlo, op. cit., p. III-4. 25 (of the system and its members).29 It will be recognized that these are the variable categories introduced, and later defined, in Chapter I. The Egggaaglga function is perhaps the most familiar. Communi- cation is used for production purposes when it is used to give instruc- tions, review performance and set down expectations for what and how much and by what methods therpredetermined end is to be reached. Pro- duction communication, in short, has to do rather directly with "getting the job done" -- it being presumed that "the job" has been defined in advance. The innovation function comes into play as persons begin to search for new ways of doing things or for new things to do. Innovation communication is "exploratory" behavior, "antagonistic to productivity" in the short run, but essential for long-term survival.31 The maintenance function is the use of communication to keep the system (classroom, nation, dyadic relationship) and its components (sub-groups, "power elites," individuals) intact. Maintenance communi- cation is different than either production or innovation communication in that it contributes to system survival or system enhancement, rather than directly to system output. Moreover, maintenance is a somewhat more complex function than theuother two. Berlo distinguishes three sub-categories: maintenace of self-concept, maintenance of interpersonal relations, and maintenance of the social system (including rules or 291bid., pp. III-lO-lZ. 30Ibid., p. III-10. See also Farace and McDonald, 0p.cit., p. 16. l 3 Ibid. 26 nonms governing production, innovation and maintenance communication).32 All three functions are crucial to any system, at least in the long run.33 The scientific utility of these functional discriminations is that systems and their components may be characterized and distin- guished on the basis of the relative frequency or prevalence of each of these three functions at any given time. Moreover, correlations between function and various other measures of organizational outcome may be attempted to gain knowledge about the relative effectiveness and effici- ency of alternative information distribution patterns, message forms and interaction "styles."34 Thus, Farace, and Connelly report a study relating communication behaviors distinguished by Berlo's function .categories to self-reported work satisfaction among employees in a 35 and McDonald used sociometric data to large commercial organization, identify liaison persons in each of three communication networks corre- sponding to these same functional designations.36 32Ibid., p. III-ll. See also Farace and MdDonald, 0p.cit., p. 17. 33 Ibid. 34Farace and McDonald, op. cit., pp. 22-3. 35 Farace, Richard V. and Richard A. Connelly, "Organizational Communication Correlates of Herzberg's Theory of WOrk Satisfaction," mimeographed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1970. 36McDonald, Donald, Communication Roles and Communication Con- tent in a Bureaucratic Setting. Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. 27 Pressures to Communicate An interesting convergence of systems theory and field theory may be seen in the importance afforded in each to the notion of un- predictability. Miller's treatment of "entropy" and Berlo's discussion of the ”central concept of uncertainty" were introduced in the preceding section. In both cases, the function of information was said to be the reduction of unpredictability. Similarly, Festinger asserts that individuals require support for their perceptions in social reality. That is, persons confronted with ambiguity in their environment will seek to "anchor" their judgments by seeking consensus in a reference group. To accomplish this, accord- ing to Festinger, persons "will initiate communications either to ascer- tain what others believe or to influence others' beliefs in the direction of their own."37 Lewin noted that gay social situations are particularly "cogni- tively unstructured," leading to uncertainty among system members. In such unstructured situations, according to Lewin, persons may be very cautious about initiating communications. However, presuming that stay- ing in the new system is a positive goal, individuals will be actively atuned for any information which has the effect of clarifying what they may expect and thus reducing tension-producing uncertainty.38 Thus, for the field theorists, as for those working within a general systems framework, less thoroughly organized or predictable systems will tend to be characterized by greater rates of information 37 Op. cit., p. 5. 38Deutsch and Krauss, op. cit., pp. 45-8. tr in SY en fr re. co f0: thl me: me: pre tr: am in EXE Opf gr: Pr: be 57: 28 transfer. Lewin's concepts of the social-psychological impetuses to information exchanges fit well within the more general pr0position of systems theory that matter-energy "flows" between interdependent compon- ents in response to environmental inputs and internal stresses.39 In addition to the general pressure to Communicate arising from the internal needs of individuals to secure their beliefs in "social reality," Festinger generated a series of propositions about forces to communicate which derive from group membership. Of particular interest for present purposes is his hypothesis that pressure on members to com- municate to others in the group concerning a given item is related to the relevance of that item to the functioning of the group.40 "Cohesive- ness," in this context, means the resultant of all the forces acting on members to remain in the group. Thus, while there may be little or no pressure on a given individual to communicate about an "item X" in a transitory or casual social group, such pressure may be considerable in another group in which consensus has consequences for group locomotion or in which the forces to remain part of the group are stronger.41 For example, a physician may perceive the same magnitude of discrepancy in Opinion among fellow members of a spontaneously formed conversational grouping in the hospital cloakroom and among the members of his group practice. According to Festinger's hypotheses, that same physician will be under greater pressure to communicate in the second than in the first 39Katz, Daniel, and Robert Kahn, The Social Psychology of Organlzations, N.Y.: John Wiley and Sons, 1966. pp. 14-26. 40"Informal Social Communication," Psychological Review, 57:271-282, 1950. pp. 274. Allbid. de tn so va th be C0. 1102 Pa' 29 of the two situations. There is a rather large and fascinating body of literature on socialization in organizations. To what extent may the socialization process be considered a pressure to communicate or a partial "explan- ation" for such informal interchange as may exist among lower members of the social system? The answer is not clear. Parsons defined socialization as "the acquisition of the re- "42 Etzioni quisite orientations for satisfactory functioning in a role. defines it as "the mechanism through which the existing consensus struc- ture and communications practices are transferred to new generations of participants.”43 Merton, in a much quoted formulation, designates socialization as "the processes by which people selectively acquire the values and attitudes, the interests, skills, and knowledge -- in short, the culture -- current in the groups of which they are, or seek to become, a member."44 Etzioni distinguishes between the patterns of socialization in coercive, utilitarian, and normative organizations. Because he identi- fies universities, hospitals and research organizations as "the least normative of the normative organizations,"45 it is not clear what patterns of consensus, communication and socialization to expect among 42Parsons, Talcott, The Social System, Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1951. p. 205. 43Etzioni, Amitai, A Cppparative Analysis of Complex Organiza- tions, Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, 1961. p. 150. 44Merton, et a1, 0p.cit., p. 287. 450p. cit., p. 146. 30 volunteer clinical faculty on the basis of Etzioni's theory. Several papers discussing socialization as explicity applied to medical education yield much interesting insight, but nothing which may be taken as evi- dence supporting expectations of a particular rate or direction of in- formation exchange among practicing physicians or physician-teachers. Indeed, one medical sociologist declares that much expressive socializa- tion is "unorganized" in professional organizations.46 With these caveats duly noted, it seems fair to say that Etzioni (as well as other students of socialization) would identify the Family Medicine preceptorship program at Michigan State University as a pre- dominently normative organization. The salient criteria would seem to be voluntary membership and the fact that the preceptors' function is more to introduce students to the physician's role than to inculcate a specific set of instrumental skills. On this assumption it is relevant to extrapolate several hypotheses about patterns of communication in normative organizations from the theories of Etzioni and others. There will be a fair amount of horizontal communication to complement and support the downward communication of goals. Moreover, communication gaps will be relatively less frequent than in coercive or utilitarian organizations, because status levels are less differentiated, alienation 46Kendall, Patricia, "Medical Education as Social Process," a paper presented to the American Sociological Association, 1960, and cited by Etzioni, op. cit., p. 141. See also Bloom, S.W., "Sociology of Medical Education: Some Comments on the State of the Field," Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, 43:143-83. 31 is lower, and consensus is higher between leaders and lower participants.47 Nevertheless, confident prediction of patterns of communication behavior based upon the nature of the social group is elusive as applied to the population under study here. Bureaucratic theory (including that which takes bureaucratic concepts as touchstones) may be of limited value in the analysis of professional organizations. The presumption persists, even in the analysis of the most normative of normative organ- izations (e.g., ideological political organizations), that policy deci- sions are made at the top. In fact, professional organizations often consist of a number of groupings , each working out policy matters in their own way, each implementing their own professional values, and negotiaping with designated leaders and with each other on points of stress-producing disagreement. Such a perspective suggests that certain pressures to communi- cate may derive from informal social structures "mediating between indi- ['49 viduals (or task groups) and large formal organizations, and also that a relative lack of downward communication from organization leaders 47 Op. cit., p. 140-1. 8Bucher, Rue, and Joan Stelling, "Characteristics of Profess- ional Organizations," ournal of Health and Social Bapavior, 10:3-15. 49 Merton, Robert K., Social Theory and Social Spructure (Rev. Ed.) Glencoe, I11.: The Free Press, p. 106. 32 may leave much potential conflict hidden or unacknowledged, thereby lessening pressures toward communication among system members and sub- groups. On the other hand, Katz and Kahn assert that an assymmetry of communication needs of superiors and subordinates leads to an increase in lateral communication among subordinates. Horizontal exchange can be an escape valve for frustration in communicating upward and downward; and sometimes it can operate to accomplish some of the essential business of the organization.51 Similarly, Burns found informal "lateral communication essen- tial to the proper functioning of the vertical system” in his study of departmental managers in a British factory.52 Thus, "the ground at a lower level was prepared for likely action" (emanating from top levels).53 Still another perspective emerges from Davis' study of execu- tives within the "Jason Company." Formal and informal communications sys- tems seem to be jointly active, or jointly inactive. Where formal communication was inactive. . ., the grapevine did not rush in to fill the void. . .; insteadSAthere simply was lack of communication. 0‘Mouzelis, Nicos P., Organization Bureapgracy: An Analysis of Modern Theories London: Routledge and K. Paul, 1967. p. 161. 51 Op. cit., p. 247. 52Burns, Tom, "The Directions of Activity and Communication in a Departmental Executive Group," Human Relations, 7:73-97, p. 96. 53Ibid. Davis, Keith, "Management Communication and the Grapevine," Harvard Business Raview, 31:43-49, p. 45. 33 The same conclusion has been expressed in pithy fashion by a prominent sociologist: "If communication fails in one direction, it will fail in the others."55 Propinquity Against the background of the foregoing general considerations about various pressures to communicate, somewhat more detailed atten- tion may now be given to the "force to communicate" of particular relevance to this present study. In Chapter I, "propinquity" was defined as proximity in time or space. Physical proximity is apparently universally acknowledged by theorists in the behavioral sciences to be a factor in determining who will talk with whom. Thus, Schein's discussion of the psychological dynamics of informal group formation in organizational settings allows for the operation of "formal factors," including physical location.56 Homans, a sociologist and proponent of Exchange Theory, speaks of "almost inevitable interactions" occasioned by the mere fact of persons being "thrown together by physical geography."57 Another sociologist, however, makes clear that the effect of propinquity is indirect, rather than direct. 55Homans, George C., The Human Gropp, N.Y.: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1950. p. 462. 56Schein, E.H., Organizational Psychology, Englewood Cliffs, IV.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1970 (Second Edition). p. 82a3. 57Op. cit., p. 97. 34 Each social world, then, is a culture area, the boundaries of which are set neither by territory nor by formal group membership, but by the limits of effective communication.58 This seeming contradiction of other commentators is grounded in the point that technology has limited the effect of geographical dise persion.59 Shibutoni does not acknowledge, however, that much of the technology remains limited by distance factors even as it is distance- 1imiting. Thus, the telephone, while permitting a user to free himself from gross limitations of distance factors, is still more likely to be used for local calls than long-distance calls and for low-toll, as opposed to high-toll, "long distance" calls.60 Thus, rather than being obviated by "distance binding" technologies, propinquity factors are "merely" modified, rendered more complex and, where they are a factor (as in the present study), made less powerful as bases for confident prediction. From the point of view of general systems theory, proximity is "a necessary but not sufficient condition for system formation."61 58Shibutani, T., "Reference Groups as Perspectives," American Jgurnal of Sociolggy, 60:562-70, 1955. p. 566. 59Ib1d. 60Zipf, G.K., "Some Determinants of the Circulation of Infor- mation," American Journal of Psychology, 59:401-21, 1946. 61Berrien, op. cit., p. 23. 35 "Boundaries," defined abstractly as that which separates one system from another, have the function of filtering inputs and outputs.62 This means that any transmission of information across a boundary entails resistance and, inevitably, some modification in or transformation of the information itself.63 To the extent, then, that sending a message to another person at some distance entails "breaking through" system boundaries, the sending will be correspondingly more difficult, less effective and, hence, less likely of even being attempted. The homeostatic principle,64 that any force which disrupts a system will be countered by other forces which tend to restore it to a steady state, favors small communication loops. Smaller circuits mean quicker feedback and closure. Size of loop, especially when system boundaries are crossed, is a major theoretical determinent of direction in communication flow and of the character of habitual patterns of interaction.65 J.G. Miller cites many examples of the importance of physical propinquity to system structure and process, and is careful to apply this observation equally to biological and social systems. G.A. Miller applied this generalization to communication in social systems as follows: 621bid., p. 32. 63Ibid., p. 22-3. 64Katz and Kahn, op. cit., p. 23. 65Ibid., p. 235. 660p. cit., p. 207-8. 36 When a large number of people belong to the group, it is reasonable to assume that the likelihood of messages passing from one person to another is inversely proportional to the distance between them. The greater the distance, the lower the traffic density.67 Miller's reasoning is supported by an array of empirical stud- ies conducted on widely varying populations and social settings. In a study of the thirty-seven workers in an office of a large corporation, Gullahorn found that distance was the "most important factor in deter- mining the rate of interaction between any two employees."68 From a study in another kind of organizational center, an academic one, Lund- berg, et al report similar effects of physical proximity.69 A number of studies have demonstrated that personal inter- action among residential neighbors is largely a product of proximity. Merton studied a large housing development in New Jersey and concluded that "quite apart from other factors, sheer propinquity played a major part in determining the patterns of personal interaction."70 Other empirical studies with findings in support of a large role for resi- dential propinquity utilize a variety of methodologies, designs and 67Lan ua e and Communication, N.Y.: McGraw-Hill, 1951. p. 262. 68Gullahorn, J.T., "Distance and Friendship Factors in the Gross Interaction Mix," Sociometgy,15:123-34, 1952. p. 134. 69Lundberg, C., V. Hertzler, and L. Dickson, "Attraction Patterns in a University,"'Sociometry12:158-69, 1949. 7OMerton, Robert K., "The Social Psychology of Housing," in Dennis, W. (ed.), Current Treads in Social Paychology, Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1948. p. 204-5. 37 particular populations.71 Barnlund and Harland reviewed empirical studies that support Miller's theoretical arguments about propinquity factors and concluded that: An inverse relationship has been found be— tween the physical distance separating per- sons and the likelihood of communication between them, with interaction increasing as distance decreases, unless there are physical barriers that intervene . . It must be acknowledged that the last phrase of this other- wise forceful generalization is, on reflection, a serious limitation of generalizability. The situation under consideration by "office land- scapers" or architects is quite different than the one under investi- gation in the present study. Support for the generalizations reported here appears to be quite sufficient to allow confident prediction in the case, let us say, of full-time university faculty whose offices are located in a given wing of an academic building. However, when the population of interest is widely scattered in a variety of types and arrangements of primary work places, an exploratory investigation seems 71Powell, R.M., "Sociometric Analysis of Informal Groups -- Their Structure and Function in Two Contrasting Communities," Sociometry 15:367-00, 1952; Caplow, T., and R. Forman, "Neighborhood-Interaction in a Homeogeneous Community," American'Sociological;Review, 15:357-66, 1950; Festinger, L., S.-Schacter and K. Back, Social PreSSures in Infor- mal Groups: A Study of Human Factors in Housing, Stanford, California: Stanford University Press, 1963; Blake, R., C. Rhead, B. Wedge and J. Morton, “Housing Architecture and Social Interaction,"‘Sociometpy, 19:133-9, 1956. 72Barnlund and Harland, "Propinquity and Prestige as Determin- ants of Communication Networks, " Sociometry, 26: 467-79, 1963, p. 468. 38 more prudent than an experimental design. It is interesting to note that the effects of propinquity upon informal communication are often assumed by medical educators and other physicians in their arguments favoring group practice. Thus, Boan cites as the principal advantage of group practice the "curbstone consulting" which it facilitates.73 Young asserts that group practice leads to "74 It has been observed teamwork and "stimulating professional company. that changes in the nature of medical care, particularly specialization, will increasingly make inter-physician communication common among solo practitioners.75 This observation does not constitute a contradiction of the generalizations about group practice. In any case, as common as such beliefs may be, empirical studies of these assertions apparently have yet to be published. By contrast, the other primary work place of physicians has received a goodly amount of attention. There are many studies of physi- cian behavior in hospitals. Wenrick, ap_al, mapped sociometric inter- actions of staff physicians in four midwestern hospitals on the basis of informal case discussions, formal consultations, and social 3 Boan, J.A., Group Practice, Toronto: Royal Commission on Health Services, 1966. p. 14. 74Op. cit., p. 932. 75Welf, Stewart C., and Ward Darley, Medical Education and Practice, Evanston, 111.: American Association of Medical Colleges, 1965. p. 72-3. 39 interactions.76 The physicians most frequently nominated were then interviewed to determine how they differed as a group from their colleagues. Among the findings were that the "informal educators" tend to spend more time at their hospitals, serve on more hospital committees and belong to more professional associations than their colleagues. Moreover, thirty-two of the thirty-nine physicians so identified were either in- ternists or surgeons, and not one was a general practitioner.77 The authors do not report the percentages of specialists in their total population. Coleman, Katz and Menzel report on a series of studies using similar techniques to trace the flow of innovation information and adoption of new drugs.78 Their principal finding was that interpersonal relations among physicians seemed to be the most important factor in the adOption of innovation. Further, they identified both hospitals79 and shared offices80 as the primary physical contexts in which such informal communication took place. .A 1969 study conducted by telephone interviews among full-time faculty of a medical school sought to discover the relative importance 76‘Wenrick, J.W., F.C. Mann,‘W.C. Morris and A.J. Reilly, "In- formal Educators for Practicing Physicians," J urnal of Medical Education, 46:299-305, 1971. 77Ibid., p. 203. 78Coleman, James S., Elihu Katz and Herbert Menzel, Medipal Innovation: A Diffusion Study, N.Y.: Bobbs-Merrill Co., Inc., 1966. 791b1d., p. 145. 801bid., p. 156. 40 of different sources for information regarding changes in medical educa- tion. Again, face-to-face, personal contact was the most frequently cited means of learning about new developments.81 There is evidence in a study of a research center at a uni- versity that there is little strictly social, as opposed to business, communication among professional colleagues and that it has little effect on the perceptions of each other by participants. The investiga- tor concludes that work-related communication tends to influence social communication, rather than vice-versa.82 The implication seems to be that research designs for the study of inter-member communication in professional groups may omit social relations as a separate variable without much risk of losing significant information. Lastly, from a theoretical, management point of view, March and Simon identify sheer prOpinquity ("exposure to contact") as one of three factors affecting the frequency of interaction in a group.83 Further, in terms that echo Miller's systems theory propositions, they hypothesize that the greater the communication efficiency, the greater will be the communication channel usage. 81Saul, Ezra V., and Suzanne Bryder, "One Faculty's Sources of Information Regarding Changes in Medical Education," qurnal of Medical W, 44:1091-4, 1969. p. 1093. 82Smith, Alfred 6., Communications and Status: The Dypamics of J28 search Center, Eugene, Oregon: University of Oregon Press,l966. p.39-45. 83March, James G., and Herbert Simon, Organizations, N.Y.: John Wiley and Sons, 1958. p. 68-71. 841b1d., p. 167-9. 41 Attitudes and Perceptions Organizations so structured that members can deal realistically and effectively with their tasks will provide powerful sources of social and psychological satisfaction. Physical decentralization in an organization, according to Berelson and Steiner, tends to be associated wit "more different and often more discordant understandings and points of view" among organization members and, also, with greater toleration of such differ- ences of perception.86 This assertion, based on an extensive review of sociological research, is consistent with the medical school studies of Merton and his associates. They speak of a "phenomenon of observability" which operates among physicians practicing in groups. Working under the close scrutiny of their medical peers, physicians in group practice tend to be more "conducive to ready conformity" with whatever professional norms may be current.87 Bloom found that volunteer clinical faculty perceived problems of communication as a cause of serious problems far less often that did full-time faculty.88 Berelson and Steiner have written that members most closely identified with their organization, while more likely to conform to its norms, are also more likely to criticize, though their 85Lawrence, Paul R., and Jay W. Lorsch, Organization and En- vironment, Homewood, 111.: Richard D. Irwin, Inc., 1969. p. 17. Berelson, Bernard, and Gary A. Steiner, Human Behavior: An Inventory of Scientific Findings, N.Y.: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc. 870p. cit., p. 77. 88Bloom, Samuel W., "The Medical School as a Social System," Milbank Memppial Fund Qpartarly, Vol. 49, No. 2, April 1971. p. 130-1. 42 criticism tends to deal with means, rather than ends.89 Katz and Kahn conclude that the information needs of lower level organization members can be at least partially satisfied by hori- zontal (peer) communication in the relative absence of regular and reliable communication from the leadership.90 On the basis of this claim, it seems reasonable to expect reported levels of satisfaction with all work-related information to be almost as high for system.members who enjoy high levels of horizontal (peer) communication as for those who have better "pipelines" to the t0p echelons and, hence, more access to "authoritative" information. This reasoning would be supported by the work of Hovland and Weiss who have demonstrated that the so-called "sleeper effect" in information credibility depends upon the more rapid decay over time of the effects of the source than of the content.91 On the other hand, Cartwright cautions that more research is needed to determine whether the effect of source decays when the source and the receiver maintain a concrete relationship.92 Farace and Connelly studied the attitudes toward available work-related information and perceptions of the information system of employees in a large commercial establishment who had been categorized 89 Op. cit., p. 379. 900p. cit., p. 243-5. 91Hovland, C.I., and W. Weiss, "The Influence of Source Credi- bility on Communication Effectiveness," Public Opinion Qaarperly 15:135-50, 1952. 2 9 Cartwright, Darwin, "Power: A Neglected Variable in Social Psychology" in Bennis, Warren G., K.D. Benne and R. Chin, The Planning of Change, N.Y.: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1966. p. 416. 43 as "work satisfied" or ”work dissatisfied" on the basis of Frederick Herzberg's theory of work satisfaction. "Work satisfied" employees re- ported more timely attention to their work problems by supervisors (pro- duction information), more favorable assessments of information about plans and policies (innovation information), and more attention to per- sonal, as well as work, problems by supervisors (maintenance communica- tion).93 Support for Mouzelis' dictum that "bad communication does not necessarily lead to conflict"94 may be drawn from.Habbe's findings in a questionnaire survey (fifworkers in two industrial plants. In the plant in which fifty-five percent of the workers agreed that the company "does a good job of telling you what's going on and what's being planned" there was a "wish to know more" than in the other plant where only eight- een percent of the workers felt that their company was communicating ade- quately to its employees. Habbe's conclusion is that "supplying more information to employees creates the desire for more information."95 It would seem that a respondent's "need to know" may be both satisfied by the leaders' messages and further stimulated by them. 93Op, cit., p. 11-16. 94 Op. cit., p. 161. 95Habbe, 8., "Communicating with Employees," Studies in Per- sonnel Policy, No. 129. N.Y.: National Industrial Conference Board, 1952, p. 39, cited by Guetzkow, Harold, "Communications in Organizations," in March, James G. (ed.) Handbook of Organizations, Rand McNally and Co., 1965, p. 540. Chapter III METHODOLOGY This chapter outlines the methods used to investigate the prob- lem described in Chapter I. First, the population utilized is described. Next, an overview of the contents of the questionnaire is proVided and the means of administering it is summarized. The process followed in developing the final questionnaire is described next. Lastly, the methods used to analyze the questionnaire data are described. Population The present investigation was conducted by surveying volunteer clinical faculty in the Department of Family Medicine of the College of Osteopathic Medicine at Michigan State University. Members of this par- ticular set of clinical faculty are all appointed by the University's Board of Trustees at the ranks of Clinical Assistant Professor or Clinical Associate Professor. With a few exceptions (all of which were excluded from the study), these preceptors are engaged in the private practice of compre- hensive, primary care to entire families. All have membership on the staff of an accredited general hospital. This p0pu1ation is scattered over much of the southern half of Michigan's lower peninsula, except that few are located in Detroit or 44 45 its immediate suburbs. Preceptors practice in both urban-suburban and rural-small town settings. They are staff members of hospitals which are both large and small, exclusively osteopathic and mixed D.O.-MgD. They practice alone or in offices shared with other physicians. Some have practices almost exclusively office-based and others spend many hours each week treating hospitalized patients. Some do general surgery and obstetrics; others refer patients requiring such care to specialists. While all are members of the statewide professional organization and nearly all are members of constituent district chapters, they vary widely in their relative activity in these associations. Students are assigned singly to spend one afternoon each week in a preceptor's office for a period of five weeks. Initial assignment. is made part way through the first year when students have acquired only the most rudimentary skills in the diagnosis of various pathologies, but are already engaged in intensive classroom study of physical diagnosis, history taking, interviewing and other basic clinical skills. The College's integrated curriculum is founded in part upon the educational principle of early reinforcement of didactic learnings in the actual set- ting in which they will be ultimately utilized. .Additionally, it is argued, early exposure to the setting, patient population and role models of family practice affords beginning students a kind of "reality test" for their professional aspirations and concepts of "what it is like to be a doctor." Thus preceptors are expected to provide an experiential introduction to the person and work of the family physician, as well as some supervised practice in the exercise of newly acquired skills involv- ing physical examination and doctor-patient interaction. 46 There were eighty-eight preceptors at the time of the survey. Of these, four were excluded from the study because they practiced in the University's Olin (Student) Health Center. .Another three were ex- cluded on grounds that, in addition to being preceptors, they had class- room teaching responsibilities on the campus and were, thus, strikingly unlike other volunteer clinical faculty at Michigan State University or elsewhere. Hence, the population actually surveyed numbered eighty-one. Questionnaire Contents The questionnaire used to gather data for this study contains items designed to elicit information corresponding with the three sets of variables outlined in Chapter I (see Figure 1.2). Thus, there are questions about the various propinquity variables (solo or group prac- tice, distance from campus, time spent in hospitals, etc.). Secondly, there are questions about informal communication with other physicians (frequency, duration, initiation and content-function). The third general set of questions was designed to elicit attitudes toward avail- able role-related information, perceptions of the program's information distribution system and the relative saliency or value of "being a preceptor." Another small set of questions was included as "controls." Three variables outside the design of the study have a potential for confounding or distorting the data, particularly as regards perceptions of the adequacy of role-related information and the information distri- bution system. They are: --length of time in the preceptorship program --visit(s) to the‘preceptor's office by an official of the College 47 --attendance of the preceptors at meetings for preceptors on the campus. The investigator believed that these variables are not system- atically related to the basic propinquity variables. Nevertheless, measures were taken on these potentially confounding variables in an attempt to safeguard the validity of the findings. The questionnaire itself is reproduced as an appendix to this dissertation. Qpestionnaire Administration The questionnaire was administered by mail, together with a self-addressed return envelope. In an effort to assure a large percent- age of returns, a cover letter signed by the chairman of the Department of Family Medicine introduced the project and the investigator to the preceptors and appealed for their cooperation. Telephone calls to the offices of the subjects who were slow to respond were made by the investigator two weeks after mailing. Design of the Qaeationnaira Development of the questionnaire proceeded through several stages as follows: Full-time faculty "brain-storming" session. Six members of the full-time faculty of the Department of Family Medicine met with the in- vestigator to "brain-storm" factors which they felt might be associated with high levels of informal communication among preceptors and with positive attitudes toward the program and role-related information. At the time of the session, five of the six faculty participants were newly arrived at the University from the private practice of comprehensive C] "R am ET; 48 family medicine. Full-time faculpy questionnaire. Eight physician members of the Department were then surveyed to produce data from their own recent experience as family practitioners regarding the principal dimensions (variable groups) of the study. This step yielded helpful guidance both for the design of the project (definition and selection of vari- .ab1es) and the shaping of questionnaire items. Open-ended interviews with preceptors. Face-to-face interviews of forty-five minutes to one hour in length were conducted with ten subjects selected from the population of preceptors. Interviewees were selected at random from each of four general localities chosen to correspond with the propinquity factors which could be determined ob- jectively by the investigator (3010 vs. group practice, urban vs. rural-small town and proximate to campus vs. remote from campus). Of particular significance in this stage were the perceptions and verbal formulations of the interviewees which helped operationalize previously indistinct variables (e.g., proximity to campus). Models from similar study. The wording of many specific items was guided by models available in a study of communication practices in a large, complex organization.1 Review of fipal_pppgp_gpaip. The adequacy of the items emerging from the foregoing stages was further assured by submitting them to the critique of four physician-faculty members and an experienced researcher 1Berlo, D.K., RuV. Farace, RmA. Connelly and H.M. Russell, "Relationships Between Supervisor-Subordinate Communication Practices and Employee Turnover, Attendance and Performance Evaluations." 'Mimeo- graphed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1971. 49 in organizational communication. Follow-up interviews. As a final check upon the validity of the questionnaire, follow-up interviews were held with five of the re- spondents. The principal objective in these interviews was to ascertain the degree to which reSpondents in fact interpreted items in the manner intended by the investigator. Procedure for Analysis Data produced by the questionnaire survey were analyzed by means of direct comparison of appropriate pairs of variables. Compari- sons between two variables, both of which had categorical values, pro- duced contingency tables with frequencies and percentages in the several cells and totals in both dimensions. Comparisons between two variables, one of which had categorical values and the other of which had continu- ous values, produced means and standard deviations for each category and for the total p0pu1ation. These procedures were accomplished by utilizing two computer programs. For comparisons of two variables both of which had categori- cal values, the program used was ACT (version 3), on file at the Computer Institute for Social Science Research (CISSR) at Michigan State Univer- sity. The ACT program generates contingency tables with both frequen- cies and percentages. For comparisons of two variables, one of which 'had categorical values and the other or which had continuous values, the program used was the MSU STAT System, Version 3.05B. Both programs are written for Michigan State University's CDC 6500 computer. 50 In general, the choice of variables for pairing and comparison followed from the principal purposes of the present investigation as set forth in Chapter I. That is, propinquity variables were compared with informal communication variables and informal communication vari- ables were compared with perception-attitude variables. Additionally, "control" variables were compared with the propinquity variables, as mandated by the design of the study. More specific explanations of and rationale for the choice of particular measures for pairing, in instances where several related measures were available, may be found in the text accompanying the presentation and discussion of data in Chapters IV and V. In coding questionnaire responses, all uncompleted items were tabulated separately ("no response") and percentages calculated for them. This procedure was followed on the assumption that a respondent's failure to complete an item may be a significant, though denotatively indistinct, response and should not be "lost" in percentages calculated only on completed items. Also coded as "no response" were the few responses spoiled by respondents' failure to comply with instructions. A number of items listed responsesoptions which read "Other (please specify): ." In those instances in which respondents utilized this option, the investigator coded their responses in the "standard" categories whenever they seemed alearly to fit. Mar The present investigation was conducted by a mailed question- naire survey of 81 volunteer clinical faculty (preceptors) in the De- partment of Family Medicine, College of Osteopathic Medicine, Michigan 51 State University. All subjects are in the private practice of compre- hensive, family medicine, are staff physicians in accredited general hospitals, and are members of osteopathic professional associations. All have similar responsibilities toward similar students. They differ on other formal characteristics of interest in this study, such as the geographical and associative aspects of their private practice settings. The questionnaire was developed in a series of steps utilizing full-time faculty members recently engaged in private practice, models drawn from analogous studies, and open-ended interviews with a subset of the population. Several follow-up interviews were conducted as a partial check upon the questionnaire's validity. Analysis of the survey data involved the generation by compu- ter of a large number of tables, each involving the direct comparison of two variables. The majority of the variable pairings followed direct- ly from the purposes of the study as stipulated in Chapter I, namely the exploration of possible associations between propinquity factors and informal communication factors, and between informal communication fac- tors and perception-attitude factors. Chapter IV ANALYSIS: PROPINQUITY AND INFORMAL COMMUNICATION Questionnaires were returned by 70 (86%) of the 81 preceptors in the population utilized for the study. One respondent returned his partially completed questionnaire with a letter explaining that he was so new to the program that he found it impossible to respond to many items. When inspection revealed that many more than half of the in- formal communications and attitude questions were unanswered that ques- tionnaire was eliminated. Thus, 69 respondents, or 85% of those sur- veyed, were finally included in the analysis. Profile on Prppinqui§y_and "Cgpprol" Variables Practice Setting. Totals for the reSpondents show that 44 of the 69, or 64%,are in group practice. One reSpondent noted that he and the physician with whom he shared an office were never present in the office at the same time. Because the principal purpose of this study is to investigate relationships between physical proximity and informal peer communication, this respondent was recorded as a solo practitioner. Those respondents in group practice were asked to indicate how many of the other physicians with whom they shared an office were also preceptors. Sixteen had no other preceptor practicing in their office, 16 had one other, 8 had two others and four had three or more. Includ- ing the solo practitioners, 59% of the preceptors have no other preceptor 52 53 sharing their office. Of those who have at least one other preceptor "close at hand” in their offices through most of each typical working day, 23% have one other and 18% have two or more others. Forty-two (61%) of the preceptors identified the communities in which their offices are located as "urban-suburban," while 27 (39%) judged their practice settings to be "rural-small town." Eighty-one percent say it takes them more than one-half hour to drive to the University campus. Percentages for all the values of these practice setting vari- ables are displayed in Table 4.1. Activity in Hospitals. Respondents vary widely in the extent to which their practice includes hospital visitation of patients. Asked, "On~the average, how much time do you spend in the hospital each week when you go there for the purpose of seeing patients?" they re- port as few as 2.5 hours and as many as 27.5 hours. (Responses of "2-3 hours per week" were recorded as 2.5 hours, etc.) The mean for all respondents was 12.7 hours per week, with a standard deviation of 5.48. Two measures were taken on another kind of activity in hospi- tals, namely involvement in policy formulation and operational super- vision. First the preceptors were asked, "Relative to other members of your (principal) hospital's staff, would you describe yourself as 'more active than most' in the affairs of the ho3pital?" Forty-three percent answered, "yes -- more active." The same percentage judged themselves to be "about average" and the rest (14%) said they were "less active" than most. The preceptors were then asked to estimate the number of 54 Table 4.1 Profile of Preceptor Population: Practice Setting Variables PRACTICE PARTNERS Solo practice 36% group practice 64% --2-doctor group 38% --3-doctor group 13% --4-doctor group 13% OTHER PRECEPTORS IN PRACTICE no other 59% --solo 36% -—group 23% some other 41% —-one other 23% --two others 12% --three or more 6% SIZE OF COMMUNITY urban - suburban 61% rural — small town 39% PROXIMITY T0 CAMPUS (driving time in good weather) 1/2 hour or less 19% more than 1/2 hour 81% 55 hours per month they spent in hospital board or committee meetings, excluding regular meetings for the entire staff. Answers ranged from none to 18 hours. The mean was 5.4 hours per average month, with a standard deviation of 3.60. Table 4.2 Profile of Preceptor Population: Activity in Hospitals TIME SPENT IN HOSPITAL FOR PURPOSE OF VISITING PATIENTS range 2.5-27.5 hrs./wk. mean 12.7 hrs./wk. standard deviation 5.47 ACTIVITY IN HOSPITAL AFFAIRS (relative to other members of staff) more active than most 43% about average 43% less active than most 13% TIME SPENT IN HOSPITAL BOARD OR COMMITTEE MEETINGS range 0-18 hrs./mo. mean 5.4 hrs./m0. standard deviation 3.61 Because the self-judgments about relative "activity" and the reported hours spent in hospital board and committee meetings were intended as complementary measures on the same dimension, it is interest- ing to note their relationship. The results of this comparison are shown in Table 4.3. 56 Table 4.3 Hours in Hospital Meetings By Category of Self-judged Activity Relative activity Hours Spent in Boardpapd Committee Meetings Mean Stan. Dev. more active than most 7.4 3.82 about average 4.3 2.65 less active than most 2.4 1.89 It will be seen that there is a regular and positive relation- ship between these two measures. This result indicates that, in subse- quent comparisons with communication variables, similar findings may be expected for these two measures on what is essentially one variable (activity in hospital affairs). Activity in Professional Associations. The respondents vary rather widely in their relative activity in the principal professional associations. Measures were taken on both district (local) and state- wide osteopathic associations. The most frequent response for both items was to the option "attend most meetings, but little or no committee wor ." The results are shown in Table 4.4. Table 4.4 Profile of Preceptor Population: Activity in Associations District State don't attend 16% 22% attend, but little or no committee 38% 57% work attend, serve on committees 29% 17% officer or chairman of major 17% 4% committee 57 That respondents tend to be mutually active or inactive at both district and state levels is demonstrated by Table 4.5. Table 4.5 Comparison of Activity in Professional Associations Category of Activity in State Association District Associations don't attend cmte. officer attend no cmte. work chm, don't attend (n=11) 53% 8% 0% 0% attend, no cmte. (n=26) 27% 51% 8% 33% attend, cmte. work (n=20) 20% 28% 50% 0% officer or chairman (n=l3) 0% 13% 42% 67% n=15 n=39 n=12 n=3 ”Control" Variables. Three measures were taken on factors out- side the design of the study (neither propinquity variables, informal communications variables nor perception-attitude variables). These factors were identified as having a potential for confounding or dis- torting the findings, particularly as regards associations between pro- pinquity factors and perceptions of the adequacy of role-related infor- mation and of the information distribution system. Although the in- vestigator did not believe that these variables were systematically related to the basic propinquity variables, measures were taken on each of them as a check on this belief. The three "control" factors are: --length of time in the preceptorship program --visit(s) to the preceptor's office by an official of the college --attendance of the preceptors at meetings for preceptors on the campus. 58 Though the purpose for including these measures prompts their being labelled "control" variables, it will be seen that they describe the population in terms of individuals' formal participation in the preceptorship program. A profile of the population on "control" factors is shown in Table 4.6. In general terms, 71% have been preceptors for more than one year and only 10% for less than six months. One out of every four preceptors has been visited in his office by an official of the college. Slightly more than half of the reSpondents have attended at least one of the meetings for preceptors held at the University campus. Table 4.6 Profile of Preceptor Population: "Control" Variables LENGTH OF TIME IN PRECEPTOR PROGRAM less than 6 months 10% 6 - 12 months 20% more than one year 71% VISITS TO PRECEPTOR'S OFFICE BY COLLEGE OFFICIAL no visits 74% one visit 10% two visits 12% three or more 4% ATTENDANCE AT MEETINGS FOR PRECEPTORS attended no meetings 46% attended one meeting 20% attended two meetings 23% attended three or more 10% Note: In this and many subsequent tables, percentage figures do not always add to 100%. This is due to rounding error. 59 gppparison of Propinquity and "Control" Factors It is possible that the first preceptors in this still relative- ly new program were selected from among physicians in group practice and that, later, the selection process was "biased" in the direction of physicians in solo practice. It is also conceivable that early preceptors were appointed from those closest at hand, with the College obliged to look farther afield as the program grew and more preceptors were needed. To determine whether such relationships exist, a set of contingency tables was generated. Table 4.7 reveals that there apa relationships between length of time in the preceptorship program and the several propinquity vari- ables. The proportion of subjects in group practice is larger for those who have been in the program.for more than one year than it is for those who have been in the program for one year or less. The proportion of subjects whose offices are in urban areas is larger for those who have been in the program for more than one year than it is for those who have been in the program for one year or less. Although the diff- erences are not as large as those in the latter two comparisons, the proportion of preceptors whose offices are more than one-half hour's driving time from the campus is larger for those who have been in the program for more than one year than it is for those who have been in the program for one year or less. In no case are the differences so large as to constitute a reversal of proportions between the respective categories of propinquity factors or a deviation or more than four percent from the means for the entire population. Table 5.6 also shows comparisons of the same set of three propinquity variables with the number of times an official of the College 60 New Nma Nms NNN Now see Nam Nma Nwm NNN wa Nma wa was Non New .HS.MVH mmoH Ho cmzu whoa .un N\H Nuaafixoum Nwm Non Nmm mac Nam Nnm qu Hops“ Nae Nmm New Mme Nam Nmo Nno NNm mafia .aaoo mo onwm New Nmo Nun N00 N50 Nme ch Nun 90H Non Nmm qu Nam Nmm Nun Nmm Nm¢ oaom whoouumm.oofiuomuo moHcmHum> hufiuvafieoum up moanmfium> :Houuooo: m.¢ maan mamuou mwcfluoma once no N wafiuooa moo mmcwumme on mozHHmmZ H< moz ouoa no A mudmfi> on mA poo» H cmfiu ouoE mmoH no pooh H zom umoaHm mmcHuooa onoa no N waHuooa H mmcHnooB on mUZHHMME H< moz onoa no H muHmH> on mH .n% H menu onoa mmoH no .n% H zmn son co£3 mUOHnoo maHnup .owmno>m can c9 “coHnmoso mnOuomm Honucoo >n mononvmnm HHmno>o m.¢ oHan 66 length of time in the program, an observation consistent with the pre- viously noted relationship between these two "control" variables. With reSpect to the comparison between overall frequency and attendance at meetings, Table 4.9 shows a virtually identical percentage distribution between those who have attended no meetings and those who have attended at least one meeting for preceptors at both extremes of the overall frequency dimension. Combined responses for the "once a month" and "once a week" category yield percentages closer to distribu- tions in the total population (41% attending no meetings and 59% attend- ing at least one meeting). In sum, analysis of comparisons between "control" variables and overall frequency reveals that those preceptors who are relatively new to thesprogram and preceptors who have had visits by College officials tend! to communicate more frequently than their fellow preceptors. ‘More- over, it is rural-small town preceptors who have tended to join the program more recently and to have been visited by representatives of the College. These associations, particularly, must be born in mind when assessing the legitimacy of inferences from this study. The remainder of the chapter is devoted to analysis of con- tingency tables generated to compare propinquity variables and informal communication variables. .Again, the propinquity variables are: --Practice partners --solo vs. group practice --number of other physicians in the group --Number of other preceptors in group "Size of community (urban-suburban vs. rural-small town) --Proximity to the campus (1/2 hour or less driving time in good weather vs. more than 1/2 hour driving time) --Activity in hospitals and associations. 67 W Freguency. The principle communication behavior variable to be compared with the solo vs. group and the group size variables is, of course, frequency of work-related conversations in the office or on the telephone. However, it is important to simultaneously consider data about frequency of interchange in the other settings in which physicians come together. In this way, one may investigate the possibility that frequency measures for different conversational contexts merely cancel each other out. To this end, the solo vs. group variable was compared also with communication frequency during patient visitation in the hospital, during hospital meetings and during professional association meetings, as well as with a general frequency variable. The results of. these multiple comparisons are summarized in Table 4.10. Analysis of the comparison of in-office peer communication with the solo-group variable reveals that 45% of preceptors practicing in groups talk with other physicians about the preceptor program at least once a month while in their offices, as opposed to 28% of preceptors in solo practice. The virtually equal percentages for those reporting "more than once a week" conversations must be treated with caution, due to the fact that these percentages represent only two subjects in each column. It will be seen that combination of the last two rows reveals that 12% of solo practitioners and 20% for group practitioners have such conversations in their offices weekly or more often. For the second frequency variable included in Table 4.10, the percentages reveal that, while in the hospital to visit patients, 3010 and group practitioners behave almost exactly the same with respect to peer communication about the preceptor program. 68 Table 4.10 8010 and Group Practice According to Frequency OFFICE. Question: In your office or on your telephone, about how frequently do you usually talk with other physicians about the pre- ceptorship program? Solo Group almost never 72% 55% once a month 16% 25% once a week 4% 11% more than once a week 8% 9% HOSPITAL ROUNDS. Question: In the past month, can you recall having any conversations with other physicians about thegpreceptorship pro- gram while you were in the hospital to see patients? 8010 Group no 44% 45% yes, once or twice 52% 50% yes, three or more times 4% 5% HOSPITAL MEETINGS. Question: At hospital staff or committee meetings, how frequently do you talk with anyone about the,preceptor program? 8010 Group almost never 75% 75% once a month 20% 16% more than once a month 4% 9% ASSOCIATIONS. Question: Before or after osteopathic association meet- ings, how frequently do you talk with anyone about the preceptorship program? Solo Group almost never 64% 66% once every 3-4 meetings 20% 16% at least once every meeting 8% 16% (no response) ( 8%) ( 2%) 69 Table 4.10 (cont'd.) OVERALL. Question: On the average, during periods when you have a student, how frequently do you talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program? Solo Group almost never 36% 25% once a month 20% 14% once a week 32% 41% almost daily 8% 16% (no response) ( 4%) ( 5%) 70 One of every four preceptors in either group reported utiliz- ing hospital staff or committee meetings as opportunity for frequent conversations with other physicians. Of those who did, a higher per- centage of preceptors in group practice report that such conversations occur more than once a month. Similarly, with respect to interchange about the preceptorship program in the course of professional association meetings, the percent- age of those reporting that they "almost never" have such conversations is virtually equal for the two groups. Once again, a higher percentage of the preceptors in group practice record reSponses in the category representing the highest frequency of communication. The last communication frequency variable shown in Table 4.10 is an overall or general variable, not referring to behavior in a par- ticular setting or social context. Moreover, questionnaire items em- phasized that subjects were to respond in terms of communication fre- quency during periods when they actually had a student coming to their office. Thus, the rates are predictably lower for those saying they "almost never" talk with other physicians about some aspect of the program. The distribution of responses over the four categories of this communication frequency variable is consistent with the results of the other frequency measures. The "no" and "aLmost never" reSponses on the foregoing variables were virtually equal in the two groups, except for the in-office frequency variable. A roughly similar percentage spread exists between the in-office frequency and the overall frequency variables in the "almost never" category. Similarly, the larger 71 percentages of group practice preceptors reporting frequent communication on the overall variable is consistent with responses on the other con- text-specific variables. A second kind of question was put to the subjects in an effort to further explore possible associations between propinquity variables and informal communication about their role as preceptors. This question was: Where do you most frequently have conversations with other physicians about the College or the preceptor program? (Please place a "l" by the most frequent location and a "2" by the second most frequent location.) Responses to this question are shown in the following table. Table 4.11 Solo and Group Practice By Location of Most Frequent Conversations most freguent 2nd most frequent solo rou solo group in office or on phone 12% 48% 16% 23% in hospital 52%: 36% 16% 39% at association meetings 16% 2% 24% 18% at College's campus 8% 2% 4% 0% other 4% 2% 8% 2% (no response) ( 8%) ( 9%) (32%) (18%) The table shows clearly a pattern consistent with the data analyzed above. Preceptors who share an office with other physicians report that their office. is the most frequent site of conversations about the College or the preceptorship program to a degree far in excess of that reported by preceptors practicing alone (48%, as opposed to 12%). The dominant first choice of solo practitioners and the clear second choice of group practitioners is the hospital. Most of those who 72 recorded responses in the "other" category wrote in "none" or put a string of zeros next to the response options. One respondent, however, reported social gatherings as his second most frequent place of con- versation. The disproportionately large percentage of solo practition- ers who failed to record a second most frequent location may mean that they, more than those in group practice, could not as readily think of a second location where they had a noteworthy number of conversations about the preceptorship program. Function. Subjects were asked, "When you talk about the preceptorship program with other physicians in your office or on your telephone, what do you most frequently talk about?" Respondents could choose among five response options: —-c1arifying what the College expects --genera1 topics, sharing experiences --finding better ways to do our job --other (please specify): --don't have such conversations The first three of these response options serve as the opera- tional definitions of the three levels of the function variable: Pro- duction,.Maintenance, and Innovation. The same wording was used con- sistently each time function measures were taken throughout the ques- tionnaire. In every case, on this and each other function measure, responses to "other" could be fit into one of the three function categories. 73 Table 4.12 Solo and Group Practice By Function of Office Communication 'solo rou production 12% 14% maintenance 28% 55% innovation 20% 9% no such conversations 32% 23% (no response) ( 8%) ( 6%) According to this data, the in-office conversations of pre- ceptors in group practice serve maintenance purposes (at least as de- fined above) to a larger extent than is true for preceptors in solo practice. Solo practitioners among the preceptors report a larger proportion of innovation communication. It must be borne in mind that these particular data do not say anything about the amount of communica— tion in any of these categories of function. The figures in Table 4.12 reflect only the proportion of respondents in each group who choose a given category as best characterizing the nature of his most typical in-office peer communication, regardless of the amount of that communi- cation. This latter observation raises an interesting question about the relationship of in-office frequency to in-office function, especi- ally given the demonstrably high in-office frequency rates for preceptors in group practice. Unfortunately, the high percentage of preceptors who say they "almost never" talk with other physicians in their offices about the program renders comparative data suspect on the grounds of small cell size. What data there are show maintenance communication as the dominant category for all levels of frequency and a higher pro- portion of innovation communication among those who communicate most frequently in their offices. 74 Table 4.13 Office Frequency by Office Function Production Maintenance Innovation once a month (n=15) 0% 87% 13% once a week (n=5) 20% 80% 0% more than once a week (n=6) 0% 67% 33% Size of Group Practice Closely related conceptually to the distinction between solo and group practice is the question of group size, i.e., the number of other physicians with whom a preceptor shares an office. The following is restricted to those frequency variables on which clear differences emerged in the comparison of preceptors in solo and group practices. Table 4.14 Size of Group By Frequency of Office Communication OFFICE. Question: In your office or on you telephone, about how fre- quently do you usually talk with other physicians about the preceptor- ship program? 2-doctor 3-doctor 4-doctor group group group almost never 58% 56% 44% once a month 23% 33% 22% once a week 15% 0% 11% more than once a week 4% 11% 22% OVERALL. Question: 0n the average, duringpperiods when you have a student, how frequently do you talk with other physicians about some aspect of the preceptorship program? “2—doctor 3—doctor- -4-doctor rou groupV_ group, almost never 35% 22% 0% once a month 4% 44% 11% once a week 42% 22%. 55% almost daily 12% 11% 33% (no response) ( 8%) ( 0%) ( 0%) n=26 n=9 n=9 75 Because of the incidence of very small cell size in this table, no confident conclusions can be made from these data. The data do indicate a positive relationship between group size and the highest rates of communication frequency. Table 4.15 compares group size and the function of in—office communication about the preceptorship program. Again, the function variable does not measure amount of conversation, but only the pre— dominant nature of respondents' communication, irrespective of the frequency or duration of such communication. Table 4.15 Group Size by Function of Office Communication 2—doctor 3-doctor 4-doctor groups groups or larger clarifying what the College expects 12% 11% 22% general topics, sharing experiences 58% 56% 44% finding better ways to do our job 4% 11% 22% no such conversations 23% 22% 0% (no response) ( 4%) ( 0%) (11%) n=26 n=9 n=9 A far higher proportion of respondents in all categories report maintenance communication ("general topics, sharing experiences") as the most frequent in—office communication function. The rather regular, positive relationship of group size and the choice of innova— tion as the most frequent function is rendered most tentative by the fact that there are only one, one and two responses respectively, in the cells of that row. 76 Number of Other Preceptors Within the group practices represented in this population, the number of officially appointed preceptors ranges from one to four. Twenty-eight of the 44 preceptors in group practice have another pre- ceptor as a practice partner; 16 preceptors in group practice are the only members of their groups holding a College appointment. Table 4.16 Number of Other Preceptors By Frequency of Office Communication no other 1 other 2 others 3 others almost never 56% 50% 63% 50% once a month 25% 19% 25% 50% once a week 6% 25% 0% 0% more than once a week 12% 6% 13% 0% n=l6 n=l6 n=8 n=4 The data presented in Table 4.16 show that preceptors who have just one other preceptor in their group talk about theypreceptorship program considerably more frequently than do those with two or three other preceptors in their group. Even those with no other preceptors in their group reported more frequently such in-office communication. It should be borne in mind that these data do not reflect the frequency of preceptors talking with other preceptors, but with physicians gen- erally. The study included no measures of inter-preceptor communication per se. Table 4.17 shows that the greatest proportion of members in each preceptor category chose the operational definition of the main- tenance variable ("general topics, sharing experiences") as the best characterization of their most frequent conversations with other 77 physicians in their offices. None of the preceptors with three other preceptors in their groups reports innovation as the most frequent content—function. Combination of the last two propinquity categories yields a figure of eight percent for innovation communication choice of those preceptors practicing with two or three other preceptors. The resulting picture is one of a negative association between number of preceptors and the choice of innovation as the most frequent communication function. Table 4.17 Number of Other Preceptors By Function of Office Communication no_9ther 1 other 2 others 3 others production* 19% 6% 13% 25% maintenance 38% 69% 50% 75% innovation 13% 6% 13% 0% no such conversations 25% 13% 25% 0% (no response) ( 6%) ( 6%) ( 0%) ( 0%) n=16 n=16 n=8 n=4 *For the exact wording of response options as they appeared in the questionnaire see Table 4.15. These operational defini- tions for levels of the function variables were used consis- tently throughout the questionnaire, each time a separate measure was taken on communication function. When the data for frequency and function are considered together it will be seen that the preceptors with no other or only one other preceptor in their groups both talked more frequently while in their offices and more often talked mostly about how they might do their jobs better. 78 Size of Community For this propinquity variable, the informal communication variables of interest are overall Or general frequency and the locations of most frequent communication. Table 4.18 Urban and Rural Preceptors By Overall Frequency Question: On the average, during periods whenfiyou have a student, how frequently do you talk with other physicians about some aspect of the preceptorship program? urban rural- suburban small town almost never 26% 33% once a month 17% 15% once a week 45% 26% almost daily 12% 15% (no response) ( 0%) ( 3%) According to the data in Table 4.18, preceptors in urban- suburban practice settings have somewhat more frequent conversations with other physicians during periods when they have students than do preceptors in rural and small town settings. The proportions of the two groups reporting monthly and daily rates is quite similar; the difference between the two groups is in the relative proportions re- porting that they have such conversations about once a week. Asked "ypapa_do you most frequently have conversations with other physicians about the College or the preceptorship program?" the preceptors responded as indicated in Table 4.19. 79 Table 4.19 Urban and Rural Preceptors By Location of Most Frequent Conversations most frequeng' 2nd most frequent combined urban rural urban rufal urban rural in office 29% 44% 14% 30% 43% 74% in hospital 45% 37% 31% 30% 76% 67% at assn. mtgs. 7% 7% 24% 15% 31% 22% at campus 5% 4% 2% 0% -- -- other 5% 0% '5% 4% —- -- (no response) (10%) ( 7%) (24%) (22%) -- —- This table reveals that substantially more often than urban- suburban preceptors, rural-small town preceptors find their offices the most frequent location of conversations with other physicians about the College or the preceptor program. Three out of every four rural pre- ceptors identified their offices as one of the first two most frequent locations for such conversations. Fewer than half of the urban-suburban preceptors did so. It would be wrong to suppose that the slightly higher percentage of urban preceptors who identify their hospitals as the most frequent location reflects a heavier use of hospitals by urban preceptors. Other comparisons in the study show that rural preceptors spend an average of 2.8 hours per week more than urban preceptors in the hOSpital for the principal purpose of patient care and that they spend more time talking with other physicians while there (3.4 hours per week, as opposed to 2.7 hours per week). While urban preceptors report more time spent in hospital board or committee meetings, the differences on this measure are less striking (5.6 per month for urban, 5.1 per month for rural). 80 The row in Table 4.19 reporting percentages for association meetings shows one of every four urban-suburban preceptors saying that such meetings are the second most frequent location for conversations about the program, as opposed to a smaller percentage for rural-small town preceptors. This finding is consistent with the different patterns of activity in the associations for the two groups. Ninety percent of the urban preceptors, as opposed to 74% of the rural pre- ceptors, attend district association meetings. Twenty-four percent of the urban preceptors, as opposed to 7% of the rural preceptors, serve on committees of the state-wide association. Proximity to Campus The proximity to campus variable was operationalized as "1/2 hour or less driving time" vs. "more than 1/2 hour." For the Lansing- East Lansing area, in which the University is located, a radius of 1/2 hour driving time would include all of the metropolitan area (urban, suburban, and "satellite".sma11 towns). Moreover, for the vast majority of persons living within 1/2 hour's driving time radius, a telephone call to the campus would be a local (non-toll) call. For this propinquity variable, the informal communication variables of particular interest are most frequent locations of con- versations with other physicians, frequency of communication with full- time faculty, and overall or general frequency. It should be borne in mind, while considering the following figures, that preceding compari- sons revealed no disproportions between proximate and more remote pre— ceptors in terms of their formal participation in the preceptor program 81 (length of time in the program, visits from College officials, attend- ance at on-campus meetings.) The interest here is to discover whether those closer to the campus more often communicate about the preceptorship program and, particularly, whether their proximity is associated with more frequent use of the College facilities for such conversations and whether they report more recent conversations with full-time faculty members. Table 4.20 shows the responses by proximity to the question "On the average, during periods when you have a student, how frequently do you talk with other physicians about some aspect of the preceptorship program?" Table 4.20 Overall Frequency According to Proximity to Campus 1/2 hour or less more than 1/2 hour almost never 62% 21% once a month 8% 18% once a week 31% 39% almost daily 0% 16% (no response) ( 0%) ( 5%) n=13 n=56 Unfortunately, there were relatively few respondents from within the 1/2 hour driving time radius (n=13) due to the exclusion of some nearby preceptors from the study. The figures must therefore be interpreted with caution. According to Table 4.20, those preceptors from farther away more frequently talked with other physicians about the College and the preceptorship program. 82 The preceptors were also asked, ”When was the last time you talked with a full-time faculty member in the Department of Family Medicine about the preceptorship program?" A comparison of these responses and the measure on proximity to campus reveals no large diff- erences between proximate and more remote preceptors, as shown in the next table. Table 4.21 Proximity to Campus by Frequency of Communication with Full-Time Faculty 112 hour or less more than 112 hour never 31% 36% 2 months or more ago 38% 39% less than 2 months ago 31% 25% n=13 n=56 Comparison of responses to questions about where conver- sations most frequently take place and the proximity to campus variable are of particular interest only in respect to those who identified the College's campus as the most frequent location. Nine percent of the preceptors more remote from the campus mentioned the campus as the most frequent location for conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program. None of those from the more proximate areas did so. Again, those farther away identify the campus as the second most frequent location more often than do those closer to the campus 83 (21% and 15%, respectively.) Activity in thngospital Two separate aspects of the preceptors' relative activity in their hospitals were included in the study. One focused upon the time preceptors spend in their hospitals visiting patients. The other focused upon the preceptors' involvement in hospital board and committee work. Measures of informal peer communication were taken relative to each aspect of hospital activity. The following two sections report on these findings. Time Spent Visiting Patients. Preceptors were asked to estimate how much time they spend at the hospital each week when they go there "for the purpose of visiting patients." The responses were recorded in hours per week. Means were calculated for each level of three informal communication variables: frequency, duration and function. The results of these comparisons are presented in the following series of three tables. Table 4.22 Hours in Hospital to See Patients By Frequency Question: In the past month, can you recall having any conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program while in the hospital to see patients? hrs./wk. in hospital to seeypatients no (n=3l) 11.9 yes, once or twice (n=35) 13.3 yes, three or more times (n= 3) 13.3 total (n=69) 12.7 84 Table 4.22.reveals that those who can recall having at least one conversation about the preceptorship program within the last month spent, on the average, almost 1 1/2 hours per week more in the hospital visiting patients. No difference resulted between those who had one or two conversations and those who had three or more. Table 4.23 Hours in Hospital To See.Patients By Duration Question: Did at least one of these conversations (in the hospital dur— ing the past month) last more than five minutes? hrs./wk. in hospital to see patients no (n=10) 12.4 yes (n=23) 13.8 can't remember (n= 4) 14.3 (had no such conversations) (n=32) 11.8 total (n=69) 12.7 These data show that those who recall having conversations of at least five minute's duration spend almost 1 1/2 hours per week more in the hospital seeing patients than those who say they had no conver- sations of that length. Table 4.24 Hours in Hospital To See Patients By Function Question (for those who report conversations in the hospital within the past month): What did you talk about? hrs./wk. in hospital to see patients clarifying what the College expects (n=9) 12.3 general topics, sharing experiences (n=24) 13.3 finding better ways to do our job (n=5) 15.5 (no such conversations) (n=31) total (n=69) 12.67 85 According to the data in Table 4.24, those identifying the innovation function as the best characterization of their recent conver- sations in the hospital spent more time in the hospital visiting patients than did those in either other category. Also, those reporting main- tenance as the function of their conversations spent more time than those reporting production. Time in Hospital Meetings. Two propinquity variables were included by which to measure relative activity in the affairs of the hospital. Preceptors were asked, "Relative to other members of your (principal) hospital's staff, would you describe yourself as 'more active than most' in the affairs of the hospital?” They were also asked "Excluding regular meetings for the entire staff, about how much time do you presently spend in hospital board or committee meetings in an average 2333p?" A comparison of these two propinquity variables was analyzed in the opening pages of this chapter and a positive relation- ship was found between them. Whether these two measures On what is essentially the same variable will yield similar results in comparisons with communications variables may be seen in the following series of tables. The communications variables are frequency, duration, initiation and function. According to Table 4.25, there is a regular and positive re- lationship between the preceptors' description of themselves as more or less active in hospital affairs and the frequency of their communication about the preceptorship program in hospital meetings. All preceptors describing themselves as "less active" than most say that they "almost never" talk with others in hospital meetings about the preceptorship 86 Table 4.25 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings by Frequency Question: At hospital staff or committee meetings, how frequently do you talk with anyone about the preceptorship program? more active about less hrs./mo. than most average active in meetings* almost never 60% 83% 100% 5.0 once a month 30% 10% 0% 6.7 more than once a month 10% 7% 0% n=30 n=30 n=9 *These figures, it should be remembered, are for time spent in hospital board and committee meetings, excluding regular meetings for the entire staff. Table 4.26 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings By Duration Question: When you talk about the preceptorship program with physicians at hospital staff and committee meetings, how long do these conversations generally last? more active about less hrs./mo. thag most avgrage 'aCtive in meetingg don't talk 37% 27% 33% 4.9 less than 5 min. 30% 47% 56% 5.1 more than 5 min. 27% 17% 11% 6.9 (no response) ( 7%) (10%) ( 0%) 4.9 n=30 n=30 n=9, 87 program. Those who report occasional conversations of this kind spend more time (1.7 hours per month more) in hospital board or committee meetings, than those who "almost never" have such conversations. How- ever preceptors who talk about the program approximately once a month are not distinguished from those who talk about it "more than once a month." These data do not show the same negative relationship between "activity" and those who "don't talk" as was so evident in Table 4.25. This is curious, inasmuch as the duration item immediately followed the frequency item in the questionnaire. However, there.i§_a regular and negative association between activity and the shorter conversations, and a regular and positive association between activity and the longer conversations. Also, those who report conversations of longer duration (n=l4) also report 35% more time per month spent in hospital board and committee meetings than do those who report shorter conversations (n=28). Table 4.27 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings By Initiation Question: Who generally starts these conversations (at hospital meetings)? more active about less hrs./mo. _£han most average active ’in'meetings don't talk 37% 27% 33% 4.9 less than 5 min. 30% 47% 56% 5.1 more than 5 min. 27% 17% 11% 6.9 (no response) ( 7%) (10%) ( 0%) 4.9 n=30 n=30 n=9 88 Only six respondents said other persons generally started the conversations about the preceptorship program which arose before or after hospital/board committee meetings. The largest proportion of those in each activity category said that initiation was about equal. According to these data, no clear pattern of relationship exists between initiation and self-judged activity. However, there is a clear differ- ence on the sister variable, hours in hospital meetings. Those saying that "others start" spend 16% more time in hospital board and committee meetings than do those who say "about equal" and 29% more than those who say "I start." Table 4.28 Hospital Activity and Hours in Hospital Meetings By Function Question: What do you talk about in these conversations (before or after hospital board or committee meetings)? more active about less hrs./mo. than most average active in meetings production* (n=9) 7% 20% 11% 3.0 maintenance (n=24) 43% 23% 44% 6.7 innovation (n=12) 13% 23% 11% 5.9 no such conversations (n=11) 20% 13% 11% 6.0 (no response) (n=l3) 17% 20% 22% 3.7 n=30 n=30 n=9 *Production, maintenance and innovation functions were represented in the questionnaire by the following phrases, respectively: "clarifying what the College expects," "general topics, sharing experiences," and "finding better ways to do our job." 89 Those preceptors who judge themselves to be of "about average" activity in hospital affairs, report a rather balanced distribution over the three function levels. A larger proportion of them characterized their communication as having to do with production and innovation than is true for either the more active or the less active. The largest mean number of hours spent in hospital board and committee meetings (6.7 hrs./mo.) is associated with those respondents who characterize their conversations in hospital meetings as "general topics, sharing experiences" (maintenance communication). Those who say they talk about "clarifying what the College expects" (production communication) report the lowest mean number of hours in meetings (3.0 hrs./mo.), far below the mean for the entire population (5.4 hrs./mo.). Activity in Professional Associations The last of the propinquity variables in this study is rela- tive activity (and, hence, proximity in time and space.‘with other physicians) in the principal professional organizations. In Michigan, the general professional society open to all osteopathic physicians is the Michigan Association of Osteopathic Physicians and Surgeons (MAOP&S). The state association has constituent local affiliates organized on a county basis, some covering several counties. In the first section of this chapter, a table was presented showing that respondents tend to be mutually active or inactive in both of their associations. The informal communication variables with which the activity in associations variables were compared are frequency, duration, initi- ation and function. 90 Table 4.29 Activity in Professional Associations By Frequency Question: Before or after osteopathic association meetings, how fre- quently do you talk with anyone about the,preceptorship program? almost never (n=45) don't attend 82% once every 3-4 mtgs. (n=12) 0% at least once every mtg. (n=9) (no response) almost never once every 3-4 mtgs. at least once every mtg. (no response) 0% (18%) n=ll don't attend 87% 7% 0% ( 7%) n=15 District AssociatiOns attend, no attend, do officer or cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. 81% 60% 25% 15% 20% 33% 4% 15% 42% ( 0%) ( 5%) ( 0%) n=26 n=20 n=12 State Associations attend, no attend, do officer or cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. 72% 33% 67% 15% 25% 33% 8% 42% 0% < 5%) ( oz) ( oz) n=39 n=12 n=3 91 Table 4.29 shows a rather regular and positive relationship between activity in professional associations and frequency of communi- cation about the preceptorship program, except for the officer and committee chairman (most active) category. In Table 4.30, the proportion of those respondents reporting conversations of more than five minutes' duration rises with each category of activity, from least to most. This regular and positive relationship is true for activity in both district and state associa— tions. No clear relationship is discernible in Table 4.31 between activity in professional associations and initiation. Those who do attend association meetings much more often report that initiation is "about equal" than they report that it is generally started by themselves or by their colleagues. Table 4.30 shows that, in most categories of relative activity, respondents chose "general topics, sharing experiences" (maintenance) as the best characterization of the content—function of their conversa- tions at association meetings. In general, for the levels of activity, the proportion of those electing the maintenance response equals or exceeds the proportion electing production and innovation combined. No shifting of function is discernable as the relative activity of those who attend the associations increases from those who are just members to those active in the committee structure and again to those who are in leadership positions. 92 Table 4.30 Activity in Professional Associations By Duration Question: When you talk about the preceptorship program with other physicians before or after an osteopathic association meeting, how long do these conversations generally last? District Association don't attend, no attend, do officer or attend cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. less than 5 min. (n=l9) 9% 31% 25% 42% more than 5 min. (n=15) 0% 12% 25% 58% don't talk (n=l9) 36% 31% 35% 0% (no response) (n=16) (55%) (27%) (15%) ( 0%) n=11 n=26 n=20 n=12 State Association don't attend, no attend, do officer or attend‘ cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. less than 5 min. 13% 28% 33% 67% more than 5 min. 0% 21% 50% 33% don't talk 33% 33% 8% 0% (no response) (53%) (18%) ( 8%) ( 0%) n=15 n=39 n=12 =3 93 Table 4.31 Activity in Professional Associations By Initiation Question: Who starts these conversations (before or after an association District Association attend, no attend, do officer or cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. 15% 0% 8% 0% 15% 17% 35% 35% 75% 31% 20% 0% (19%) (30%) ( 0%) n=26 n=20 n=12 StatefiAssociation .attend, no attend, do officer or cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. meeting)? don't attend others start (n=5) 0% I start (n=6) 9% about equal (n=25) 0% don't have such conversations 18% (n=14) (no response) (n=l9) (73%) n=11 don't attend others start 0% I start 7% about equal 7% don't have such conversations 40% (no response) (47%) n=15 10% 8% 0% 8% 8% 33% 36% 67% 67% 18% 8% 0% (28%) ( 8%) ( 0%) n=39 n=12 n=3 94 Table 4.32 Activity in Professional Associations By Function Question: What do you talk about in these conversations (before or after association meetings)? production* (n=8) maintenance (n=l9) innovation (n=7) don't have (n=17) (no response) (n=18) production maintenance innovation don't have (no response) District Association don't attend, no attend, do officer or attend cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. 0% 0% 25% 25% 9% 27% 20% 58% 0% 15% 5% 17% 27% 35% 25% 0% (64%) (23%) (25%) ( 0%) State Association don't attend, no attend, do officer or attend cmte. work cmte. work cmte. chm. 0% 10% 25% 33% 7% 26% 50% 67% 7% 13% 8% 0% 40% 20% 8% 0% (47%) (26%) ( 8%) ( 0%) n=15 n=39 n=12 n=3 *For the exact wording of response items as they appeared in the questionnaire, See Table 4.24. 95 Summary A profile of the population on the propinquity and "control" variables was presented in a series of tables (Table 4.1 through Table 4.6). It was pointed out that the so-called "control" factors may also be viewed as describing the population in terms of its members' participation in formal aspects of the preceptorship program. Propinquity factors were then compared with "control" factors. Size of community (rural vs. urban) was found to be related to each of the "control" variables. Length of time in the program was found to be related to each of the propinquity variables. As a result of these findings, the "control" variables were compared with overall frequency of informal communication. Length of time in the preceptorship program and number of visits by College officials were both found to be posi- tively related to overall frequency. Preceptors in solo practice and preceptors in group practice are virtually identical in the proportions in which they report that they talk informally about the preceptorship program with other phy- sicians encountered during hospital rounds, in hospital staff meetings and in professional association meetings. However, those in group practice report in significantly higher proportions that they have such conversations in their offices -- and, correspondingly, report higher levels of communication frequency on an overall frequency measure. One in every two preceptors in group practice identified their offices as the.most frequent location for conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program, as opposed to one in eight of the preceptors in solo practice. 96 Relative to their counterparts in solo practice, preceptors in group practice more often said that in-office discussion of the preceptorship program was mainly about maintenance matters ("general topics, sharing experiences"). A smaller percentage of group prac- titioners than solo practitioners characterized such in—office con— versations as having mostly to do with innovation ("finding better ways to do our job"). For the second propinquity variable, size of group practice, a positive relationship is apparent between the number of physicians in the group and the highest frequency ("almost daily" or "more than once a week"). Size of group practice is also associated with the frequency with which the subjects characterize their in-office communi- cations as "finding better ways to do our jobs." Confidence about both of these findings is conditioned by the rather small numbers of preceptors in the larger group. The third propinquity variable, number of other preceptors in the group, is closely related to group size in concept. However, an exactly opposite finding emerges from analysis of these data. Pre- ceptors with no other or only one other preceptor in their group both talked more frequently in their offices about the program and more often talked mostly about how they might do their jobs better, as compared with those in groups with two or three other preceptors. When preceptors from urban and suburban areas were compared with preceptors from rural and small towns, the urban preceptors re- port somewhat more frequent communication during periods when they have a student in their offices, less frequently identify their offices as 97 the location of most conversations and more often say that professional associations are the second-most frequent site of their discussions about the preceptorship program with other physicians. 'This latter finding is consistent with the urban preceptors' more frequent attend- ance at meetings of district (local) professional associations. With respect to proximity to campus, respondents living within a 1/2 hour driving time radius report less overall frequency of communication with other physicians about the preceptorship program, no large difference with respect to frequency of interaction with full-time faculty and no instances of the campus being identified as the second most frequent location of conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program (as oppoSed to 9% for those farther away than 1/2 hour driving time). Propinquity with respect to the hospital as a work-site for preceptors was measured by asking how much time they spend in the hospital when they go there for the purpose of seeing patients. There is a positive association between this variable and communication frequency, and between this variable and duration of conversations. Those identifying the nature of their conversations as innovation report more time devoted to visiting hospitalized patients than those choosing the maintenance option, who, in turn, devote more time to hospital rounds than those saying that their conversations were about clarifying College expectations (production). Those who judge themselves to be "more active in the affairs of the hospital" more often report frequent conversations and longer conversations about the preceptorship program in the context of hospi- tal meetings. However, the "more active" and "less active" subjects 98 were not much different with respect to the initiation of these con- versations or their content function. Responses of those saying that their level of activity in hospital affairs is "about average" report the lowest percentage of self-initiated conversations and the most "balanced" distribution on the function variable. A second measure of relative activity in hospital affairs was hours per month spent in hospital board or committee meetings. On this measure, positive relationship was discovered for frequency, duration, conversations started by others and maintenance communication. The final propinquity variable examined was activity in the principal professional associations. Relative activity in the associ- ations (attendance and committee work) was found to be positively associated with frequency and duration, but not associated in any way with initiation or function. Analysis and reporting of findings continues in Chapter V. The main focus of that chapter is on comparison of informal communi- cation variables with perceptions and attitudes. Chapter V ANALYSIS: INFORMAL COMMUNICATION AND PERCEPTION Most of the last chapter was devoted to comparisons of pro- pinquity factors and informal communication factors. Correspondingly, the present chapter is principally devoted to comparisons of those same informal communications factors with measures on the study's attitudinal variables. The informal communications variables and the discrete mea- sures taken on each are: frequency (variously operationalized in different measures) --in the office (including office telephone) --in the hospital while there to see patients --at hospital staff or committee meetings --at professional association meetings --overall (not specific as to location or social context) ——with full-time faculty duration --in the hospital while there to see patients --at hospital staff or committee meetings --at professional association meetings initiation --at hospital staff or committee meetings --at professional association meetings function —-in the office (including telephone) --in the hospital while there to see patients --at hospital staff or committee meetings --at professional association meetings This chapter is organized according to these informal communi- cations variables. In each instance, comparisons between selected 99 100 measures on the informal communication variables and appropriate per- ception-attitude variables are analyzed. Before moving to the first of these comparisons, however, data for the entire population on the perception-attitude variables are presented in a series of three tables. The accompanying text in- cludes illustrative comments of the respondents gleaned both from the questionnaires and from face-to-face interviews. Profile on Perception Variables Perception of Information. The data in Table 5.1 provide a profile on the entire population according to their responses on six questionnaire items. The questions were designed to elicit views on the information available to preceptors about the preceptorship pro- gram and on the means by which they received, or would prefer to re- ceive, such information. With respect to the amount and timing of information, approxi- mately one—fourth said they "get adequate information" and another one- fourth said they "don't get information.' Nearly one half said they get information, but that the information they get is "too little and too late." One rural preceptor in group practice said he "would like to know a lot more." Among the specific things he would like to know about are the satisfaction of students with the program and the effect of preceptors' evaluations in the overall evaluations of students by the College. However, another preceptor in rural, group practice said he was thoroughly satisfied with the amount and timing of information available to him, explaining, "I just get on the phone. All I have to do is make my needs known." 101 Table 5.1 Profile of Population: Perception of Information, Sources and Interest AMOUNT, TIMING. Question: How do you feel about the total amount of information available to you from all sources about the preceptorship program -- and when you get it? don't get information 26% get it, but too little and too late 46% get adequate information 23% (no response) ( 4%) QUALITY. Question: How do you feel about the accuracy and usefulness of the information which you get from all sources about the preceptor- ship program? it's accurate and useful 43% it's accurate, but often not useful in 16% my situation it's useful, but I often doubt its accuracy 9% it is neither useful nor accurate 3% (no response) (29%) CLARITY. Question: How clear is it to you what the College expects you to do as a preceptor? it's really not very clear 16% not clear, but I decided myself what 33% is needed not clear, but other physicians and I together 13% have worked out what should be done the College has made it sufficiently clear 35% (no response) ( 3%) SOURCE VALUE. Question: Which ONE of the following sources of informa- tion has been most valuable in your role as a preceptor? journals and formal presentations at 10% conventions mailings from the College 23% full-time faculty members and/or Director 12% of Education at your hospital other physician colleagues and students 38% other 10% (no response) ( 7%) 102 Table 5.1 (cont.) PREFERRED SOURCE. Question: Where and how would you most prefer to get your information about the preceptorship program and the College's expectations for your role? the present arrangement is sufficient 22% more, better presentations by officials 12% at general meetings small discussion and problemesolving sessions 19% visit to my office by College representative 33% other (please specify) 1% (no response) (13%) COLLEGE INTEREST. Question: How do you feel about the interest of the College leaders in your experiences and perspectives as a preceptor? they have shown no sign of interest 17% I assume they are interested, but they 43% haven't done much to allow me to make my input they're definitely interested and have 28% welcomed receiving my suggestions (no response) (12%) A preceptor in an urban, group practice for whom being a preceptor is very important declared that the amount of information is "adequate because I don't need much," adding "I have very little, but I don't much care." It is interesting to note, however, that this same pre- ceptor is very active in his hospital and reported a moderate fre— quency of conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program. Those commenting particularly about timing said that the major problem was not knowing in advance what the students have covered in their on-campus classes. They complain that, whereas they can find this out from the students, too much time is lost doing so after the students arrive in their offices. 103 Forty-three percent of the respondents judged available in- formation from all sources to be "both accurate and useful." Sixteen percent said "it's accurate, but often not useful in my situation." Several preceptors said that they had been sent "lots of information" early in the program, but that it had not been "germaine" or was "too limiting." One preceptor could recall receiving something rather re- cently in the mail, but couldn't recall what it had been. Only two preceptors said that available information was "neither useful nor accurate." Nine percent found it useful, but said they often doubt its accuracy. With respect to accuracy, one preceptor commented in an interview that "The bigwigs tell us what they want us to hear." A rather large proportion of the total (29%) declined to answer this particular question. One wrote an explanatory comment in the margin: "receive so little information as to be unable to give a good answer." Another preceptor, one who marked the "accurate and useful" option, had responded in an earlier interview with a shrug and a diffident "I have no reason to fault it." In interviews, several preceptors stressed their preference for "official and direct" communication from an "authorized individual." The "clarity" variable is complex. One response option was simply "it's really not very clear" what the College expects of its preceptors. Sixteen percent chose this option. The next two response options also say it is not clear what the College expects, but go on to present alternative ways of responding to that lack of clarity. Thirty- three percent said it is "not clear, but I decided myself what is needed." Thirteen percent said it is "not clear, but other physicians 104 and 1 together have worked out what should be done." Thirty-five percent said "the College has made it sufficiently clear." One preceptor who chose the "not very clear" option had said in a preliminary interview, "I Lhiflk I know what to do, but it's not too clear. It would be helpful to have things spelled out." Another who responded similarly on the questionnaire had said in a preliminary interview: I'm not sure what the College expects of me. For example, am I supposed to go so far as to allow students to "glove up" for minor (office) surgery? The students seem satisfied (with what I do), but is the College? Interestingly, this comment came from a man whose office is close to the campus and who reports that an important source of information about the program is full-time faculty encountered in the hospital and at district professional association meetings. Asked which source of information has been most valuable, the largest proportion (38%) said "other physicians and students." Twelve percent said full-time faculty and/or the Director of Education at their hospital (who, depending on the hospital and the individual, may represent a strong link between the College and local communities). Of those identifying the more formal sources as most valuable, 10% said journals and presentations at conventions, while 23% said mailings from the College. One of those who said that journals and formal presenta- tions at conventions had been most valuable explained in a marginal note that he received "very little communication from the College." His answer fits with the fact that he reports that he "almost never" talks with other physicians at the hospital or at association meetings and, in spite of the fact that he has another preceptor practicing 105 with him, reports "no such conversations" in his office within the previous month. All of the ten percent who indicated that their most valuable source was "other" than any of the options presented explained that their own personal experience was the source most helpful to them. With respect to where and how they would prefer to get their information about the preceptorship program and the College's expecta- tions for their role, 22% said that "the present arrangement is suffi- cient." Twelve percent want "more, better presentations by College officials at general meetings." One-third of all respondents would prefer a visit to their offices by a representative of the College. One in five would most like to have small disucssions and problem— solving sessions. One preceptor who indicated on his questionnaire that "the present arrangement is sufficient" would prefer that the College "print something up and send it out. One can't go to meetings all the time." This response is from a solo practitioner with very little hospital practice (1 hr./wk.) who is "less active" than most in the affairs of his hospital and does not attend district association meetings. By contrast, several of those favoring small discussion and problem-solv- ing sessions stress the importance of "give and take" among colleagues (having, in other contexts, attested to its importance on a wholly voluntary and informal basis in their own practices). One of these said that he thought such periodic sessions ought to be mandatory for preceptors, adding that he "can't see how (the program) can carry on without it." Another preceptor wants feedback from students as guidance for his own growth. He suggests "evaluation of physicians by students 106 and then discussion with physicians about the student recommendations and criticisms."' The question about preferred source of information prompted the following on the questionnaire of a preceptor from a rural-small town, group practice some distance from the University: I would like a general education pro- gram for preceptors, giving a condensed review of what the students receive at the College. (I want this) for two reasons: (1) it updates my knowledge, and (2) it would give me better insight into the students' thinking and know- ledge. Asked about the interest of College leaders in their experi- ence and perspectives as preceptors, the largest proportion (43%) responded that "I assume they are interested, but they haven't done much to allow me to make my input." Seventeen percent said "they have shown no sign of interest" and 28% said "they're definitely interested and have welcomed receiving my suggestions." In an interview, one said that "professionals in education don't want to listen to men in the field." Another offered that it is "hard to say . . . I think so, I don't know how much value the preceptor's contribution makes (to the whole educational program). I don't know how they know what I'm doing." One of those who said "they're definitely interested" had told in an interview (in response to the same question) about the time he came unannounced to the College facilities. "The minute I identified myself to the secretary, I was ushered in and talked with the dean over coffee." Ideas for Improvement. Four items in the questionnaire had to do with ideas for the improvement of the preceptorship program. Table 5.2 presents the resulting data. 107 Table 5.2 Profile of Preceptor POpulation: Ideas for Improvement Have you ever had any specific ideas about how the preceptorship program could be im- proved? yes no, not at this time (no response) If yes: 8. Have you shared your idea(s) with other physicians? no only briefly considerable discussion (no response) . Have you ever communicated your idea(s) to any full-time faculty of the Depart- ment of Family Medicine? yes no (no response) Briefly outline your suggestions in the space below. (written suggestions cate- gorized by general nature of those sub- mitted by each respondent). administration of preceptorship program conception or "philosophy" of program selection of preceptors criteria for student admissions (no response) percent tota1+ 64% 32% ( 4%) 10% 35% 19% (36%) 19% 44% (36%) 26% 25% 3% 3% (44%) percent ideas 16% 54% 30% 30% 70% 46% 44% 5% 5% 108 Sixty-four percent of the respondents said they did have specific ideas or suggestions. Qf phage, 16% said they had not shared them with other physicians, 54% said they had shared them "only briefly" and 30% said they had had "considerable discussion" with other physi- cians about their suggestions. Asked whether they had ever communicated their suggestions to any full-time faculty member of the Department of Family Medicine, 30% said they had and 70% said they had not. These latter two items were included in the questionnaire as a gauge of system openness. On this basis, it is apparent that a very high percentage (84%) of those with specific suggestions have offered them for discussion, at least briefly, with other physicians, whereas a much smaller proportion (30%) have in any way communicated those ideas to the persons with responsibility for shaping or managing the program. Respondents were asked to write their suggestions on the questionnaire. Each reapondent's ideas were then categorized aa_a yhpla, yielding a single value for each respondent irrespective of the number of suggestions listed. Most of the respondents offered mostly or most emphatically suggestions about either the administration (oper- ation, management) of the program (46%) or the conception or "philoso- phy" of the program (44%). Five percent (two preceptors) wrote suggestions deemed by the investigator as mostly concerned with the selection of preceptors and another 5% had ideas mostly about criteria for student admissions, a concern which has bearing upon the preceptor- ship program but goes well beyond it also. 109 Table 5.3 Profile of Preceptor Population: Salience of Role Which of the following affiliations contributes more to your growth in clinical competence as a family -practitioner? being a preceptor participation in professional associations neither contributes much (no response) .Again, which of the following affiliations contributes Inore to your growth in clinical competence as a family 'practitioner? being a preceptor consultation and educational programs at my hospital neither contributes much (no response) Which of the following affiliations do you think contributes more to your patients' regard for you? being a preceptor my position(s) in the hospital staff neither contributes much (no response) 457. 25% 297. ( 1%) 23°. 67% 7% ( 3%) 22°. 17% 557. ( 67o) 110 Salianga pi Ehé Pippappor Role. Table 5.3 presents the last three items on the questionnaire. Each compares participation in the preceptorship program with some other role and asks which role contri- butes most in a given respect. Asked which affiliation contributes more to growth in clinical competence as a family practitioner, 45% said "being a preceptor" and 25% said participation in professional associations, while 29% said neither did. Asked the same question about a comparison with consulta- tion and educational programs at their hospitals, 23% thought being a preceptor contributed more, as opposed to 67% for the hospital affilia- tion, while 7% thought neither did. Asked which affiliation contributes more to their patients' regard for them, 22% said "being a preceptor," 17% said "my position(s) in the hospital staff" and 55% said that neither contributes much. One preceptor, highly placed in his hospital leadership, chose "being a preceptor" over the hospital affiliation as contributing more to his growth. In a preliminary interview, he had said, "Some of us enjoy teaching and the College allows us to do it. We're not just tolerated, as GP's often are in other places." A preceptor who had judged the preceptor role as "contributing more" in all respects but hospital consultation and education said in an interview that being a preceptor "keeps me sharp" and "I know that the students report to each other about which preceptors are good." A rural preceptor said about his patients, "I think they think, 'you must be pretty good or they wouldn't send students out here.‘ " On the other hand, another wrote on his questionnaire an explanation of his choice of the "neither contributes muc " Option to the last question: 111 (My patients) don't know, nor do they care, about what committees I'm on, etc. The patients' regard for the doctor is based primarily upon rapport, interest, time and the results of person-to-person contact -- in short, the "doctor-patient relationship." Freguency of Communication Measures of communication frequency are compared with various per- ception-attitude variables in the next series of five tables. Only two measures of frequency are utilized for this purpose, as opposed to the multiple frequency measures utilized in Chapter IV. In the preceding chapter, the principle purpose was to systematically compare each pro- pinquity variable with communication measures most closely related to it. Thus, for example, relative activity in the affairs of the hospital was compared with communication behavior in hospital staff and committee meetings. In the present context, however, it suffices to utilize communication measures which are not situation- or context-specific, when they are available, since the perception-attitude variables also measure preceptors' judgments about their overall experience. Frequency and Perception of Information. Table 5.4 presents data resulting from comparisons of the variable named "overall frequency" with three perception of information variables. With respect to the question about the amount and timing of information, it will be seen that 35% of those who say that they "almost never" talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program also say that they "don't get" information about the program. This figure represents a larger prOportion of those saying they "don't get it" than is true of any of 112 Table 5.4 Overall Frequency of Communication* By Perception of Information AMOUNT, TIMING. Question: How do you feel about the total amount of information available to you from all sources about the preceptorship program -- and when you get it? almost once once almost never a mo. a wk. daily don't get it (n=18) 35% 27% 19% 22% get it, but too little and too late 45% 55% 54% 33% (n=32) get adequate information (n=16) 20% 18% 23% 44% (no response) (n=3) ( 0%) ( 0%) ( 4%) ( 0%) QUALITY. Question: How do you feel about the accuracy and usefulness of the information which you get from all sources about the preceptor- ship program? almost once once almost never a mo. a wk. daily accurate and useful (n=30) 45% 45% 38% 66% accurate, but often not useful (n=11) 15% 18% 23% 0% useful, but I often doubt its 5% 9% 12% 11% accuracy (n=6) neither accurate nor useful (n=2) 5% 0% 4% 0% (no response) (30%) (27%) (23%) (22%) CLARITY. Question: How clear is it to you what the College expects you to do as a preceptor? almost once once almost never a mo, a wk. daily it's really not very clear (n=ll) 35% 0% 8% 11% not clear, but I decided myself what 25% 73% 27% 22% is needed (n=23) not clear, but other physicians and I 5% 9% 19% 22% together have worked out what should be done (n=9) College has made it sufficiently 30% 18% 42% 44% clear (n=24) (no response) ( 5%) ( 0%) ( 4%) ( 0%) n=20 n=1l n=26 n= 9 113 the other categories. Similarly, of those saying that they communicate "almost daily" with other physicians, the percentage of those saying that they "get adequate information" is higher than for any other group. On the whole, the picture is one of progressively higher evaluations of available information by each category of communication frequency, from least frequent to most frequent. Again, in the case of the comparison of overall frequency with perceptions of the accuracy and usefulness (quality) of available in- formation, the most favorable evaluation is rather definitely associated with the category representing those who talk informally with other physicians "almost daily" during periods when they have students. Sixty- six percent of these preceptors say they find the information available to them both useful and accurate. However, in this case, the relation- ship is not regular and progressive, as it was in the previous compari- son with the amount and timing variable. Those who report informal communication "once a month" and "once a week" are not less critical of the adequacy of available information than those who say they "almost never" talk with other physicians. It is interesting to note that the most frequent communicators were more inclined to doubt the accuracy of information (perhaps reflecting their greater exposure to informal and, therefore, "unofficial" information). On the clarity variable, the preceptors in the highest frequency categories report that they perceive the College as having made "suffi- ciently clear" what is expected in percentages substantially higher than those who communicate informally less often. Those who "almost never" talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program have, 114 by far, the largest proportion who say "it's really not very clear." Interestingly, those reporting that they talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program "once a month" also report that they have decided themselves' what needs doing in a proportion three times that of any other group. There is a positive relationship between fre- quency and the choice of the response option "not clear, but other phy- sicians and I together have worked out what should be done." Frequency and Perception of Sources. Table 5.5 presents the results of comparisons between the overafl.frequency variables and two measures of perceptions of information sources. In the first instance, preceptors were asked to identify the source (or combination of two similar sources) which had proved "most valuable in (their) role as a preceptor." No pattern of association between these two variables emerges from this comparison. Asked where and how they would prefer to get their information, the preceptors' responses are, again, not clearly associated with their relative frequency of informal communication with other physicians about matters related to their role as preceptors. Eyeguency gnd Perception of Interest. The comparison presented in Table 5.6 is between overall frequency and perception of the interest of College leaders in the preceptors' experiences and perspectives. No clear associations can be inferred from these data. The preceptors in the "almost daily" category have the lowest proportion of those saying that College leaders have shown no interest, but they have not more often said that College leaders are "definitely" interested. Moreover, the most critical group are those who talk with other physicians 801 t i c jor. ful phy 0th (no PRE} get EXP! pre: DOD Smai ViS: 0th‘ (110 115 Table 5.5 Overall Frequency* By Perceptions of Information Sources SOURCE VALUE. Question: Which one of the following sources of informa- tion has been most valuable in your role as preceptor? almost once once almost ngvgr a mo. a wk, daily journals, formal presentations (n=7) 15% 9% 8% 11% mailings from the College (n=16) 25% 45% 15% 22% full-time faculty, hosp. dir. of educ. 10% 9% 12% 22% (n=8) physician colleagues and students 30% 36% 50% 33% (n=26) other (n=7) 15% 0% 8% 11% (no response) (n=5) ( 5%) ( 0%) ( 8%) ( 0%) PREFERRED SOURCE. Question: Where and how would you most prefer to get your information about the preceptorship program and the College's expectations for your role? almost once once almost never a mo. a wk. daily present arrangement is sufficient 20% 27% 23% 22% (n=15) _ more, better presentations by officials 10% 18% 8% 22% (n=8) small problem-solving sessions (n=13) 25% 18% 15% 22% visit to my office by College rep. 30% 18% 35% 33% (n=23) other 0% 0% 4% 0% (no response) (15%) (18%) (15%) ( 0%) n=20 n=ll n=26 n= 9 *for operational definition, see Table 5.4. 116 Table 5.6 Overall Frequency* By Perception of College Interest INTEREST OF COLLEGE. Question: How do you feel about the interest of College leaders in your experiences and perspectives as a preceptor? almost once once almost pever a mo, a wk. daily they have shown no sign of interest 15% 18% 23% 11% (n=12) I assume they are interested, but they 40% 36% 46% 56% haven't done much to allow me to make my input (n=30) they're definitely interested and have 30% 36% 19% 33% welcomed my suggestions (n=l9) (no response) (15%) ( 9%) (12%) ( 0%) =20 n=ll n=26 n= 9 *for operational definition, see Table 5.4. as frequently as "once a week." Freqpency;pnd Salience of Preceptor Role. Table 5.7 compares overall frequency with three measures of the relative salience of the preceptor role. In the first of these salience items, preceptors were asked whether being a preceptor contributed more than participation in professional associations to their growth in clinical competence. There is a clear relationship between frequency of informal communication and the choice of "being a preceptor," except that those who "almost never" talk with other physicians seem not to fit the pattern. An explanation might lie in a comparison between overall frequency and the measures of activity in the association. Unfortunately, no such comparison was made. It is noteworthy that the "almost never" category has the largest percentage of those saying "neither contributes much." In any case, 117 Table 5.7 Overall Frequency* By Salience of Preceptor Role Question: Which of the following affiliations contributes more to your growth in clinical competence as a family practitioner? almost once once almost never a mo. a wk. daily being a preceptor (n=31) 45% 18% 38% 78% participation in prof. ass'ns. (n=17) 15% 55% 27% 11% neither contributes much (n=20) 40% 18% 35% 11% (no response) (n-l) ( 0%) ( 9%) ( 0%) ( 0%) Question: Which of the following affiliations contributes more to your growth in clinical competence as a family practitioner? almost once once almost never p mo. a wk. daily being a preceptor (n=18) 15% 9% 19% 44% consultation and educational program 65% 82% 73% 56% at my hospital (n=46) neither contributes much (n=5) 15% 0% 8% 0% (no response) ( 5%) ( 9%) ( 0%) ( 0%) Question: Which of the following affiliations do you think contributes more to your patients' regard for you? almost once once almost ngygr a mo, a wk. daily being a preceptor (n=15) 35% 18% 8% 44% my position(s) in hospital staff (n=12) 10% 27% 19% 22% neither contributes much (n=38) 55% 36% 65% 33% (no response) ( 0%) (18%) ( 8%) ( 0%) n=20 n=ll n=26 n=9 *for operational definition, see Table 5.4. 118 the percentages of those for whom being a preceptor is more important is 18% for those in the "once a month" category, 38% for those in the "once a week" category and 78% for those in the "almost daily" category. A similar finding emerges from the comparison of overall fre- quency and the second salience variable, this one focusing on "being a preceptor" versus "consultation and educational programs at my hospital." The percentages saying that being a preceptor contributes more to growth in clinical competence are only half as large as in the previous case. Yet the same pattern is apparent. Again, those in the "almost never" category have the largest percentage saying that neither contributes much. Except for those preceptors in the "almost never" category, the percentage of those identifying the preceptor role as most important rises steeply with increased overall frequency of communication. On the third salience variable, the regular, progressive increases in percentages identifying the preceptor role as most important is not present. However, the data still show that the largest percentage choosing the "being a preceptor" option is in the "almost daily" cate- gory. Again, despite the relatively high percentage of the "almost never" category choosing the preceptor role as most important, a very high percentage of this group also says that "neither contributes much." Frequency witthull-time Faculty and Perception of Information. Table 5.8 compares a measure of frequency of communication with full- time faculty and three perception of information variables. The group saying they have "never" talked with a full-time faculty member" have, by far, the highest percentage of those saying they "never get" informa- tion (42%, as against 6% and 26%). Overall the group most favorable 119 Table 5.8 Frequency with Full-Time Faculty* By Perceptions of Information AMOUNT, TIMING. Question: How do you feel about the total amount of information available to you from all sources about the preceptorship program -— and when you get it? 2 mos. or less than never more ago 2 mos. agp don't get it (n=18) 42% 6% 26% get it, but too little and too late (n=32) 50% 39% 48% get adequate information (n=16) 4% 44% 26% (no response) (n=3) ( 4%) (11%) ( 0%) QUALITY. Question: How do you feel about the accuracy and helpfulness of the information which you get from all sources about the preceptor- ship program? 2 mos. or less than never more ago 2 mos. ago accurate and useful (n=30) 17% 61% 56% accurate, but often not useful (n=ll) 21% 17% 11% useful, but I often doubt its accuracy 13% 6% 7% (n=6) neither accurate nor useful (n=2) 4% 0% 4% (no response) (n=20) (46%) (17%) (22%) CLARITY. Question: How clear is it to you what the College expects you to do as a preceptor? 2 mos. or less than never more ago 2 mos. ago it's really not very clear (n=ll) 21% 6% 19% not clear, but I decided myself what 50% 17% 30% is needed (n=23) not clear, but other physicians and I 17% 22% 4% together have worked out what should be done (n=9) the College has made it sufficiently 8% 50% 48% clear (n=24) (no response) (n=2) ( 4%) ( 6%) ( 0%) n=24 n=l8 n=27 *Question: When was the last time you talked with a full-time faculty member in the Department of Family Medicine about the preceptorship program? 120 in its evaluation of the amount and timing of information is the group saying they talked with a full—time faculty member some time ago. Only one individual (4%) of the "never" group rated the amount of information as adequate. Again, with respect to "quality" of available information, it is the middle group that gives the highest overall evaluation. Almost half of those who say they "never" have talked with a full-time faculty member declined to answer this question (perhaps because 42% of them had just finished saying that they "don't get" information). However, even with the smaller proportion spread over the response options, this "never" group still registered higher proportions than the other groups in the "not useful" and "doubtful accuracy" categories. 0n the clarity variable, those who say they have "never" talked with a full-time faculty member have, by a wide margin, the low- est proportion of those saying that the College has made its expectations clear (8%, as opposed to 50% and 48% for the other two groups). It has the highest percentage of the three groups for preceptors saying that they have responded to the lack of clarity by figuring things out by themselves (50%, compared to 17% and 30%). Again, preceptors who talked with a full-time faculty member two months or more ago have the least unfavorable view of available information. It is interesting to note that the third category of the clarity variable ("not clear, but other physicians and I together have worked out what should be done") has only one preceptor who has talked with a full-time faculty member within two months, as opposed to four each for the other two groups. This is a clue to the fact that those 121 who communicate most frequently with physicians about the preceptorship program are not necessarily those who also communicate most frequently with full-time faculty. The following table demonstrates this fact. Table 5.9 Frequency with Full-Time Faculty By Overall Frequency overall, with Othergphysicians frequencyiwith full-time faculty 2 mos. or less than never more ago 2 mos. ago almost never 29% 22% 33% once a month 17% 17% 15% once a week 46% 28% 37% almost daily 4% 22% 15% (no response) ( 4%) (11%) ( 0%) No general pattern of relationship can be seen in Table 5.9 between these two variables, except for the ones which could have been anticipated from the foregoing comparisons of the two frequency measures with the same set of perception of information measures. The preceptors who have communicated two months or more ago with full—time faculty are those who register the largest percentage of most frequent overall communication with other physicians, a finding consistent with their generally more favorable perceptions of information. The very low incidence of "almost daily" communication with other physicians by those who have never talked with any full-time faculty member is consistent with their generally heavier representation in the less favorable cate- gories on the perception of information variables. Frequency with Full-Time Faculty and Perceptionfiof Interest. Table 5.10 compares frequency with full-time faculty and perceptions of the interest of College leaders in the experiences and perspectives of 122 preceptors. Once again, a larger percentage of those who have talked with full-time faculty two months or more ago have the most favorable perception of available information. Those who have never talked with a full-time faculty member register the smallest percentage in the category representing the most favorable perception, but not the largest percentage with the most unfavorable perception. Table 5.10 Frequency with Full-Time Faculty* By Perception of College Interest. PERCEPTION 0F COLLEGE INTEREST. Question: How do you feel about the interest of College leaders in your experiences and perspectives as a preceptor? 2 mos. or less than never more ago 2 mos. ago they have shown no sign of interest 17% 11% 22% (n=12) I assume they are interested, but 46% 39% 44% they haven't done much to allow me to make my input (n=30) they're definitely interested and 17% 50% 22% have welcomed receiving my suggestions (n=l9) (no response) (n=8) (21%) ( 0%) (11%) n=24 n=18 n=27 *for operational definition, see Table 5.8. The group with the largest percentage of most favorable per- ceptions is the group which reports talking with full-time faculty two months or more ago. It is interesting to recall in this connection that no clear association was apparent between overall frequency and percep- tion of College interest (see Table 5.6 and accompanying text). 123 DuratiOn of Communication Three separate measures of duration were included in the study, each focusing specifically upon communication behavior in a specified situation: during hospital rounds, at hospital staff and committee meet- ings, and at meetings of professional associations. For purposes of comparing the duration variable with perception of information variables, and in the interests of such parsimony of analysis as seems possible in a study of this kind, only one of these duration measures is utilized. This procedure seems justified on the basis that clear relationships were found in comparisons of all three duration measures and the appro- priate propinquity variables with which they were compared (see Tables 4.23, 4.26, and 4.30). The choice of which duration variable to use for present purposes was based upon the number of preceptors answering each question. The duration measure with the largest number of responses was the one associated with informal communication of preceptors while in the hospital to see patients. According to the data presented in Table 5.11, preceptors who recall conversations of more than five minutes' duration more often re- ported that they "get adequate information" and somewhat less frequently criticize available information as "too little and too late.‘ The pro- portions of those who talked for more than five minutes are not differ- ent for those who characterized available information as "accurate and useful" or "neither accurate or useful." However, those who report having longer conversations substantially less often expressed doubts about its accuracy than those who report shorter conversations (9% and 20%, respectively). Also, 22% of those who talked for more than five 124 Table 5.11 Perceptions of Information By Duration of Hospital Conversations AMOUNT, TIMING. Question: How do you feel about the total amount of information available to you from all sources about the preceptorship program -- and when you get it? less than more than 5 minutes 5 minutes don't get it 20% 17% get it, but too little and too late 60% 52% get adequate information 10% 22% (no response) (10%) ( 9%) QUALITY. Question: How do you feel about the accuracy and helpfulness of the information which you get from all sources about the preceptor- ship program? less than more than 5 minutes 5 minutes accurate and useful 40% 43% accurate, but often not useful 0% 22% useful, but I often doubt its accuracy 20% 9% neither accurate nor useful 0% 0% (no response) (40%) (26%) CLARITY. Question: How clear is it to you what the College expects you to do as a preceptor? less than more than 5 minutes 5 minutes it's really not very clear 30% 17% not clear, but I decided myself what is needed 40% 22% not clear, but other physicians and I together 10% 26% have worked out what should be done the College has made it sufficiently clear 20% 30% (no response) ( 0%) ( 4%) n=10 n=23 125 minutes report that available information is, while accurate, "often not useful," whereas none of the preceptors reporting shorter conversa- tions did so. With respect to clarity, differences between those who report shorter and longer conversations emerge in every category. A substanti- ally larger proportion of those who recall conversations of more than five minutes' duration report that "the College has made (its expecta- tions) sufficiently clear" and a substantially smaller proportion say "it's hot really very clear" what the College expects. A far higher proportion of those who had longer conversations (26% as opposed to 10% for those having shorter conversations) say that, while expectations are not clear, they have worked out what should be done in collaboration with other physicians. Initiation of Communication Two measures on the initiation variables were included in the study. For initiation of conversations in the context of hospital meet- ings, a relationship seemed present on one measure of activity in hos- pital affairs, but not on the other. For initiation of conversations taking place at professional association meetings, no relationship was found. Accordingly, both initiation measures are utilized for purposes of exploring possible relationships between initiation and perception of information variables. Table 5.12 presents the resulting data. With respect to the perceptions of amount and timing of information available from all sources, a favorable perception is reported by a larger proportion of those saying that others generally start the conversations than of 126 Table 5.12 Perceptions of Information By Measures of Conversation Initiation AMOUNT, TIMING. Question: How do you feel about the total amount of information available to you from all sources about the preceptorship program -- and when you get it? hospital meetings association meetings I others about I others about start start egual start start egual don't get it 17% 15% 15% 11% 7% 32% get it, but too little, 677. 547. 487. 637. 53% 58% too late get adequate info. 17% 31% 33% 26% 33% 11% (no response) ( 0%) ( 0%) ( 4%) ( 0%) ( 7%) ( 0%) CLARITY. Question: How clear is it to you what the College expects you to do as a preceptor? hpspital meetings association:geeting§_ I others about I others about start start egual start start egual it's really not very 17% 15% 15% 11% 7% 16% clear not clear, but I decided 0% 31% 33% 47% 7% 42% myself what is needed not clear, but other phy- 50% 0% 11% 11% 27% 16% sicians and I togeth- er worked it out the College has made it 33% 46% 41% 32% 53% 21% sufficiently clear (no response) ( 0%) ( 8%) ( 0%) ( 0%) ( 7%) ( 5%) n=6 n=13 n=27 n=19 n=15 n=19 127 those who say "I start." .Those saying that they typically initiate conversations are more heavily represented in the category of those who say they get information, but get it "too little and too late." These relationships hold for both measures of initiation. For the clarity variable, once again a favorable perception of available information was more often registered by those who say that others generally initiate conversations. No other clear relationships are apparent on both initiation measures. It must be acknowledged that the very small number of preceptors reporting that they initiate con- versations before or after hospital meetings (column 1 in Table 5.12) renders even this cautious analysis a rather tentative undertaking. Function of Communication 0f the four measures taken on the function variable, two were selected for comparison with perception of information variables. They are the ones associated with communication behavior in the hospital while there to see patients and in preceptors' offices. The selection was made on the grounds that the largest number of preceptors responded to these items and the concurrent factor that these two locations were clearly identified as the most frequent sites of conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program. In the analysis which follows, associations will be deemed to exist only in those instances in which similar differences are observed on.pp£h_measures of communication function. According to the data in Table 5.13, those preceptors char- acterizing their conversations as primarily serving to clarify the College's expectations (production) more often than preceptors in other 128 .wcoauficwmop Hmcowumuomo now HH.m maan moms mu: mus smug am": an: one ANoNV ANo V ANo V ANm V ANo V ANo V Ammaosmmu ocV ummHo Nos qu Nam NNe Nmm NNN sauaofioaumsm us some aonHoo meow on pasonm umn3 use pmxuos m>m£ Hosuowou H can No NNN NmN NON Nag NHH mamsuammee guano use .uuoao no: women: ma umss New Nae Nam Nmm Nee New maomws twosome H use .ummau uoc Now NNN Nmu Ne NHH NHH sumac Num> soc NHHmmu m.uN waHmago ANoeV ANo V ANmNV ANeNV ANNNV ANHHV Ammaoamou 05 No NHH No Rm No No Hummus no: mumpsoom Honuwm: moan No No Nm NON NNN NNN .suom peace amumo H use .Hsuoms No NHH NNH NeH NHH NHH Human: uoc cmumo use .mumuauom Noe Nwm Nos ch Nee Nom Humans was muwusuoc soascmq< ANoNV ANo V ANm V ANe V NNo V ANo V Ammcoammu oeV Now New NmN New NHH NHH coaumauouca «assumes um» Noe qu Nuq Nee NmN NwN ouaH oou .mHuuNH oou use .uN new New No NmN NaH NHH NHH as now u.coe ezHEH £ng HMUHmmOS QUHHMO kufimmOS OUHMHO HMUHmmOa— QUHHHG oON ow ou wwws .uommo mcHHmSm Illllmdmmmmmmmmmmllll umuuon weapaam .moaaou kuocmm m.oonHoo wcwmmwumao mcowumooq 039 CH coHuocsm cowumowcsaaou %m kcowumauomcH mo mcowumooumm ma.m oHan 129 categories say (1) that they get information, but too little and too late, (2) that they often doubt its accuracy and (3) that they have found the College's expectations unclear and have responded by deciding on their own "what is needed." On the other hand, those who character- ized their conversations as "finding better ways to do our job" (innova- tion) have a generally more positive view of available information and information distribution than do those in the other two categories. Specifically, none of them express doubts about the accuracy of avail— able information and the highest proportion of them say the College has made its expectations sufficiently clear. In no categories of any of the three perception of information variables do those in the main— tenance category register notably higher or lower proportions than those in both of the other function categories. Table 5.14 compares suggestions for improvement of the pre- ceptorship program and also preferences for sources of information about the program with categories of content—function. There are no notable differences among the preceptors in each of the function cate- gories with respect to whether or not they have specific ideas for improving the program. However, those in the production category much more frequently than the others offered ideas judged by the investiga- tor to be mostly about how the program's administration (operation, management) might be improved, whereas those in the maintenance category more often than the others listed suggestions having mostly to do with the basic conception or "philosophy" of the program. Interestingly, though all three categories of preceptors said they had suggestions, those in the production category actually bothered to write them out more often than those in the maintenance category and far more often 130 @"C Hmuc @"d— ANHHV Axmav ARC v Amocommmu ocv No NM No assoc Nmm Rafi N¢¢ o>Nuwucomoumou monHoo mo mowmmo he on uHmH> NHH New NNN msoammom wca>aomuaoaoowa Hanan Nmm NmH NHH mamaofimmo hp encapsucomoua nouuoo .ouoa NHH Nmm gum ucowowmmsm a“ uaoawwcmuum uammmum ago Mason usoh you macaumuuomxm m.oonHoo ago was adumoum manmuoummooua mnu unoow Gown—850mg.“ Mach uom cu along so.» canoes so; was mamas "coaummso .mompom nmmmmmmmm Axomv Axmmv ANNNV Apouommo mcowummwmsm ocv NC No No scammwavm ucmpsum you mfiuouauo No No No muouamomum mo coauomaom NNN Nmm NNN Emuwoum mozznm0moawsm: Ho cwfimop .aoaumoocoo NNN New wmm awuwoua masmuouaoooua mo cowumuumwaaawm "msoaaom no can“ -uomoumo macaumowwsm cmuufius .mmwa mmnH ANo V ANo V ANHHV Ammcoemou oaV Nam NmN NNN menu man“ as nos .0: N3 NE N3 was. wpo>oumaa on pasoo amuwoum manm uuouaoomum any so: usonm mood“ camaooam ass was uo>o sch o>mm "cowummso .m4mnH mwa Houumn wcaumnm mcoamduoomxo mswpcam Hmuocow mcwhmwumfio coauoowcsaaoo ooammo-cH mo coauocsm mm mousom consummum was ucm8m>0MmaH now macaumowwSm «H.m manna 131 than those in the innovation category. With respect to where and how they would prefer to get their information, preceptors in the innovation category less often than the others perceived the "present arrangement (as) sufficient" or small problem-solving sessions as a preferable means of getting information. Rather, they said they preferred more and better presentations by College officials in general meetings. "A visit to my office by a College representative" was deemed most preferable by a large proportion Frame-“1‘1 (44%) of those in the production category, by a lesser proportion (33%) of those in the innovation category and by only 11% of those in the maintenance category. Summary A profile of the entire preceptor population on the perception- attitude variables was presented in a series of three tables. The accompanying text included many illustrative comments of respondents, gleaned both from the questionnaires and from ‘notes taken in face-to- face interviews. The balance of Chapter V was wholly devoted to comparisons between informal communication variables and the various perception variables. This part of the chapter was organized by headings corres- ponding to the major informal communication variables, on some of which multiple measures had been taken: frequency, duration, initiation and content-function. The two most general frequency measures, overall frequency with other physicians and frequency with full-time faculty, were utilized for present purposes, on grounds that the perception variables 132 also measure preceptors' judgments about their overall experience. Nine perception variables were compared with.the overall frequency variable. In general terms, positive associations were apparent between overall frequency of communication with other physicians and favorable perceptions of (l) the amount and timing of available in- formation, (2) the quality of available information and (3) the clarity of the College's expectations of preceptors. Clear associations were also found between overall frequency (on the part of those who reported at least ppm; interchange with colleagues) and the proportions of those who said that being a preceptor "contributed more" in personal terms than did other professional roles. No relationships were clear in comparisons of overall frequency with (1) choices of the most valuable source of role-related information, (2) preferred source or (3) per- ceived interest of College leaders in preceptors' perspectives and experiences. With respect to frequency of communication with full-time faculty, those who have never talked with a full-time faculty member most often reported the least favorable responses on each of four perception variables. However, those in the category representing the highest frequency do not register the pppp favorable perceptions in proportions as high as those in the category representing a middle range of frequency. This finding must be interpreted in the light of a comparison between overall frequency with other physicians and fre- quency with full-time faculty. The latter comparison shows that those in the middle range category of frequency with full-time faculty report the highest rate of overall frequency with other phySicians. The 133 overall variable had already proven to be strongly associated with the perception of information variables (though not with the perception of interest variable). To gauge possible relationships between duration of communica— tion and perceptions of information, the most frequently answered of the three duration measures was selected for comparison. Those whose con- versations generally last more than five minutes, compared with those who report shorter conversations, (l) more often reported that they get an adequate amount of information, (2) more often criticize the useful- ness of the information they get, (3) lggg often say that they doubt the accuracy of the information, (4) more often say that the College has made its expectations clear and (5) more often say they have collabor- ated with other physicians in working out "what should be done" as preceptors. Both measures of initiation of communication included in the study were compared with the perception of amount and timing variables and with the perception of clarity variable. Those saying that their conversations about the preceptorship program are generally started by others more often report favorable responses on both perception vari- ables. Two of the four function measures in the study were selected for comparison with perception variables. In general terms, those characterizing their conversations as having to do mostly with innova- tion report favorable views of available role-related information in larger proportions than either those in the maintenance or those in the production categories. On the other hand, those who report that most 134 of their conversations are concerned with trying to figure out what is expected of them (production) more often report less favorable views of information than either of the other two groups. No differences are apparent between the three function cate- gories with regard to whether or not preceptors report having specific ideas about how the preceptorship program might be improved. However, those in the production category much more often than the others offered ideas having to do with how the program should be administered, whereas those in the maintenance category more often made suggestions about the basic design or "philosophy" of the program. Receiving information via a visit to their offices was preferred by a large proportion of those in the production category, by a lesser proportion of those in the innovation category and by a considerably smaller proportion of those in the maintenance category. Those in the innovation category less often than the others perceived "the present arrangement (as) sufficient" and more often favored presentations by College officials in general meetings. The next and final chapter reports the investigator's conclus- ions, recommendations and observations. Chapter VI CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS This concluding chapter consists of four main sections: (1) background considerations and questions which gave rise to the present study, and the Specific problems, limitations and conduct of the in- vestigation; (2) the main conclusions of the study; (3) some implica- tions of the conclusions for medical school administrators; and (4) some recommendations for subsequent research. Summagy Background of the stud . Part-time clinical faculty tend to be poorly integrated into the information systems of their medical schools. They are, first and foremost, community practitioners; only secondarily, even peripherally, are they teachers. Physically removed from the medi- cal school environs, they do not ordinarily particpate in the formal and informal communication networks through whith "regular" faculty are inducted, informed and guided, and through which faculty contributions to policy formulation and curricular design are made. ‘Medical school administrators seek cost-effective means of improving the flow of information to and among volunteer clinical faculty. Formal or official methods (memoranda, worksh0ps, site visits by admini- strators, etc.) are part of this effort to improve communication. Vol- unteer clinical faculty are also involved -- to a greater or lesser ex- tent -- in informal or unofficial interchange with each other and with 135 136 non-faculty professional colleagues about their College and its program of off-campus clinical education. If it could be shown that certain formal characteristics of part-time clinical faculty were related to higher rates of informal communication about the College and their role in it, and if it could be further established that such increased communication were positively associated with the incidence of desired perceptions or attitudes, then administrators could be guided accordingly in the selection of clinical teachers and in the conduct of training and support efforts. Formal and readily determined characteristics of physicians who are clinical faculty members, or who might be considered for appoint- ment, prominently include certain features of their practice setting and patterns of professional activity. .Are they in solo or group practice? If they are in group practice, how many other physicians are in the group? Is the practice located in an urban area or a rural area? How far is the practice from the medical school campus? To what extent does the nature of the practice include care of hospitalized patients? How much time does the clinical faculty member, or potential clinical faculty member, devote to hospital board and committee work? How active is he or she in the principal professional organizations? .Answers to these questions yield a measure of the relative propinquity, or nearness in place and time, of physicians with other physician colleagues. Moreover, answers to questions of this sort are easily determined and involve relatively little subjective judgment. Specifip pupposes. Accordingly, this present study sought to answer two broad questions with reSpect to one set of volunteer, part- 137 time clinical faculty in a college of medicine: 1. Are propinquity factors with respect to practice setting and institutional affiliations associated with differences in self-re- ported communication behavior? 2. Are characteristics of self-reported communication behavior associated with differences in perceptions of role-related information and information exchange, and with differences in the relative salience of the preceptor role? Theoretical undeppinnings and prior research. General systems theory, especially as developed by James G. Miller, was adopted as a basic theoretical framework within which to elaborate the basic communi- cation concepts explicit or implicit in this study. The centrality of the notion of unpredictability in both general systems theory and field theory was noted, and the contribution of field theorists to an under- standing of social-psychological pressures to communicate were reviewed. Both theoretical literature and empirical studies about propinquity as a factor in social interaction were reviewed, drawing upon commentary from a variety of disciplines and research in a variety of social settings. Lastly, those few studies which have compared communication behavior with perceptions of information and other attitudinal outcomes of social interaction were reviewed. Conduct of the study. The population utilized was comprised of part-time volunteer clinical faculty, or preceptors, appointed in the Department of Family Medicine of the College of Osteopathic Medicine at Michigan State University. Primary data were gathered by mailed questionnaire. The 138 questionnaire was developed through a lengthy series of steps in an effort to assure the inclusion of essential variables, an appropriate operation- al definition of variables and general clarity of language and form. .Analysis of the survey data involved the generation by com- puter of a large number of tables, each involving the direct comparison of two variables. The majority of variable pairings involved either a propinquity measure and an informal communication measure or an informal communication measure and a perception.measure, as entailed by the cen- tral purposes of the study. Some additional comparisons were made in an effort to explore possible associations between propinquity variables and several "control” variables. Limitations of the study. No measures of actual knowledge or of performance were included as outcome criteria against which to com- pare informal communication behavior. Data were gathered from volunteer clinical faculty in a single department of one medical school, all of whom are engaged in a single type of practice. The demonstration of causal relationship is beyond the sc0pe of the study. It is exploratory and descriptive only. in Conclusions Conclusions emerging from the analysis of questionnaire data are sumarized in the following sixteen statements. They are grouped under two sub-headings, correSponding to the two broad purposes of fldssUMy. 139 Ppopinguity and Infprmal Communication 1. Preceptors in group practice talk with other physicians about the,preceptorship program more frequently than do preceptors in solo practice. 2. This difference of solo and group practitioners in overall frequency of communication may be accounted for by the differences in frequency of communication which takes place in the preceptors' offices. 3. Preceptors in larger (urban-suburban) communities talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program somewhat more fre- quently than do preceptors in smaller (rural-small town) communities. 4. Preceptors whose offices are located within l/2 hour's drive from the University campus talk with full-time faculty about the preceptorship program only slightly more often than do those farther away and have pp; more often used the campus as an important locus for conversations about the program. 5. Preceptors who spend considerable time in the hospital visiting patients talk both more frequently and for longer periods with other physicians about the preceptorship program than do those who spend less time in the hospital visiting patients. 6. Preceptors active in board and/or staff committee work in their hospitals have more frequent and longer conversations about the preceptorship program during hOSpital meetings than do those who are less active in hOSpital affairs. 7. Preceptors more active in professional associations have more frequent and longer conversations about the preceptorship program at association meetings than do those who are less active. 140 Informal CommuniCationrand'Perceptions. 8. Preceptors who more frequently talk informally with other physicians about the preceptorship program more often have positive perceptions of the information available to them from all sources about the preceptorship program. 9. Preceptors who more frequently talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program more often perceive that being a preceptor enhances their professional competence and status, relative to other professional roles. 10. Preceptors who more frequently talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program do pp£.more often have favorable per- ceptions of the College's interest in their experiences and perspectives as a preceptor. ll. Preceptors who more frequently talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program do .ppp meaningfully differrfrom preceptors who communicate less frequently with respect to identifica- tion of those sources deemed most valuable or with respect to those sources deemed preferable. 12. Preceptors who have never talked with a full-time faculty member in their department more often have unfavorable perceptions of available information and more often have unfavorable perceptions of the College's interest in their experiences and perspectives as a preceptor. l3. Preceptors who generally have longer conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program more often have favor- able perceptions of available information. 141 14. Preceptors who generally have longer conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program more often perceive that the College has made clear its expectations and more often respond to unclarity by working out "what should be done" in collaboration with other physicians. 15. Preceptors whose conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program are generally about "finding better ways to do our job" (innovation) more often perceive available role-related in- formation favorably than do those whose conversations are generally about "clarifying what the College expects" (production). 16. Preceptors whose informal communication with other physi- cians about the preceptorship program was characterized as having to do mostly with "clarifying what the College expects" (production) more often prefer receiving information via a visit to their offices by a College representative than do those in the innovation category and much more often than those in the maintenance ("general topics, sharing experiences") category. Implications of Findings about "Control" Variables Throughout this dissertation, the term "control" has consist- ently appeared in quotation marks. This practice was adopted to signify that, although this term is conventionally associated with experimental designs, it is used here in the context of a descriptive study. This caveat notwithstanding, the rationale for including "control" factors in the study was to provide some check upon implications drawn from associations between propinquity factors and informal communication, in the event that "control" factors should turn out to be associated 143 with propinquity factors. Some such associations between "control" factors and propin- quity factors have emerged from the analysis. Length of time in the preceptorship program is related to each of the several propinquity variables with which it was compared. Thus, preceptors in group prac- tice, preceptors practicing in urban areas and preceptors whose offices are located more than one-half hour's drive from the campus have been in the program longer, on the average. Moreover, because length of time in the program seems to be associated with overall frequency of communication, conclusions about relations between such overall fre- quency of communication and the several propinquity variables must be assessed accordingly. Because of the finding that the highest frequency of communi- cation is disproportionately associated with preceptors who have been in the program for one year or lggg, conclusions about the higher fre- quency of communication among preceptors practicing in groups would seem to be further strengthened. Moreover, the conclusion that urban preceptors talk more frequently about the preceptorship program emerged despite any supposed "advantage" accruing to rural preceptors as a "result" of being new to the program and receiving more visits. Communication effects which may be speculatively attributed to the "control" or formal participation variables should be viewed within the perspective of data on the overall proportions of the "con- trol" factors. Only twenty-six percent of the preceptors in the entire population have had a visit from a College representative and only 30% have been in the program for one year or less. Given the assumption 143 of causal relationship for the associations between "control" variables and overall frequency of communication, a much larger proportion of those newer to the program and receiving visits from College officials might conceivably have overturned some of this study's conclusions. Implications for Administrators Administrators responsible for the design and conduct of commun- ity preceptorship programs in general medicine would be well advised to stimulate informal communication among volunteer clinical faculty. The frequency and duration of such informal interchange is clearly associated with favorable perceptions of available information, with the perceived clarity of the College's expectations and with the per- sonal salience of the preceptor role. Furthermore, the informal communication of community physicians who volunteer as part—time clinical faculty may be seen as a kind of contribution to the College, just as is their time actually spent with students. The data in this study indicate that, in private offices, in hospital corridors, in meeting rooms and in convention halls, pre- ceptors are -- to greater or lesser degrees -- taking time to talk with one another and with non-preceptor colleagues about the College's program and their own part in it. The phrase "taking time" is signi- ficant. When these physicians talk about the preceptorship program, they are taking time from some other activity or pursuit and freely "giving time" to the College and its interests. When such communication takes place in preceptors' offices or the corridors of their hospitals, as most of it does, it very clearly represents a giving of time which could be directly invested in the 144 production of additional income. Such "opportunity costs" represented by the informal communication of volunteer clinical faculty deserve to be assessed as genuinely valuable. Moreover, as a kind of contribution to the College and to the medical education enterprise, such an in- vestment should be protected, nurtured and responded to in kind by College leaders. These data provide some significant clues to ways in which informal communication among clinical faculty may be furthered by medical school leaders. First, to the extent consistent with other criteria, more preceptors might be selected from among physicians in group practice. Second, a criterion for selection might be a relatively large amount of time spent in the hospital for purposes of visiting patients. Third, a criterion for selection might be a relatively active role in the affairs of the hospital. Fourth, a criterion for selection might be a relatively active role in the principal professional associations. Fifth, already appointed clinical faculty members who are relatively active in their hospitals, with respect both to patient care and institutional leadership, might be identified for particular attention in efforts to disseminate information. Sixth, formal programs of orientation, in~service education and problem-solving could be mounted on an area basis in community hospitals to exploit and further stimulate the informal communication already existant in these settings. 145 Seventh, visitation by preceptorship program administrators or other full-time faculty members to the offices of clinical faculty might be concentrated particularly among those preceptors who are known to be less active in their hospitals and/or in professional associations. Implications for Further Research The present study should afford encouragement to students of medical education and professional organizations generally. First, a high percentage of returns was returned by a popula- tion of private physicians, an outcome not often achieved. Success in this instance was probably due to the relatively small size of the population, the identification of the subjects with the institution and the access afforded by the College leaders. Second, the finding of generally clear associations in a descriptive study suggests that subsequent investigators may more con- .fidently risk the formulation and testing of hypotheses by inferential statistics, e.g., the chi—square or similar tests. Third, these data indicate that hospital affiliation is a highly salient factor for this population of physicians in general, faunily practice. This crucial dimension might have been altogether missed by this investigator, had he not conducted preliminary surveys and :interviews before constructing his instrumentation. Early assump- tions; and much advice had suggested that the patient care responsi- bilitzies and, by inference, the communication ambit of these family Praetflltioners would be much more limited to office practice. 146 Fourth, the concepts.and other tools of communication research have an applicability to research in medical education.which is fore- shadowed in the present study. Descriptive investigations and case studies of this kind will further explore the operation of communica- tion behavior as intervening activity between organizational arrange- ments subject to manipulation or influence by institutional leaders and the various criterion measures on the basis of which those same institutional leaders make decisions among alternative policies or practices. Beyond such relatively unsophisticated methodologies as represented here are more powerful research tools, such as network analysis, now being developed and tested by communications researchers. If the present study, in any substantial way, encourages medical educa- tors to exploit communication research approaches to organizational analysis, it will have served no mean purpose. APPENDIX l. 2. 3. QUESTIONNAIRE FOR PRECEPTORS IN FAMILY MEDICINE What is your name? my name How long have you been in the preceptorship program? less than 6 months _____6 - 12 months _____more than one year How many times has an official of the College visited you in your office? No. of visits How many meetings for preceptors have you attended at the campus? No. of meetings Do you share an office with another physician? no yes If Yes: — a. How many others? No. of others b. How many of the others are preceptors? No. of other preceptors Please check the response which best describes the location of your office. urban-suburban rural-small town 147 7. 10. 11. 12. 148 How long does it take you to drive to the MSU campus in good weather? 1/2 hour or less more than l/2 hour 'In your office or on your telephone, about how frequently do you usually talk with other physicians about the preceptorship program? almost never once a month once a week more than once a week When you talk about the preceptorship program with other physicians in your office or on your telephone, what do you most frequently talk about? clarifying what the College expects general topics, sharing experiences finding better ways to do our job other (please specify): don't have such conversations On the average, how much time do you spend in the hospital each week when you go there for the purpose of seeing patients? hrs./wk. When you are in the hospital to see patients, about how much time do you usually spend talking with other physicians each week about any- thing whatever? hrs./wk. In the past month, can you recall having any conversations with other physicians about the preceptorship program while you were in the hospital to see patients? no yes, once or tw1ce yes, three or more times 13. 14. 15. 149 If Yes: a. What did you talk about? _____clarifying what the College expects general topics, sharing experiences _____finding better ways to do our job other (please specify): b. Did at least one of these conversations last more than five minutes? no yes can't remember Relative to other members of your (principal) hospital's staff, would you describe yourself as "more active than most" in the affairs of the hospital? yes - more active about average no - less active Excluding regular meetings for the entire staff, about how much time do you presently spend in hospital board or committee meetings in an average month? hrs./month At hospital staff or committee meetings, how frequently do you talk with anyone about the preceptor program? almost never once a month more than once a month 150 16. When you talk about the preceptorship program with physicians at hospital staff or committee meetings, how long do these conversations generally last? don't talk less than 5 min. more than 5 min. 17. Who generally starts these conversations? others start I start about equal 18. What do you talk about in these conversations? clarifying what the College expects general topics, sharing experiences finding better ways to do our job other (please specify): don't have such conversations 19. How active are you in your district osteopathic medical association at this time? _____don't attend attend some meetings, but no or little committee work attend most meetings, serve on committees _____officer or chairman of major committee(s) 20. How active are you in the state-wide association (MAOP&S)? _____don't attend attend some meetings, but no or little committee work attend most meetings, serve on committees officer or chairman of major committee(s) 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 151 Before or after osteopathic association meetings, how frequently do you talk with anyone about the preceptorship program? almost never _____pnce every 3 - 4 meetings at least once every meeting When you talk about the preceptorship program with other physicians before or after an osteopathic association meeting, how long do these conversations generally last? ,_____less than 5 min. .____Jmore than 5 min. ._____don't talk Who generally starts these conversations? others start I start about equal don't have such conversations What do you talk about in these conversations? clarifying what the College expects general topics, sharing experiences finding better ways to do our job other (please specify): don't have such conversations Where do you most frequently have conversations with other physicians about the College or the preceptorship program? (Please putua "l" by the most frequent location and a "2" by the second most frequent loca- tion.) in my office or on my telephone in the hospital at osteopathic association meetings at the College's facilities on campus other (please specify): 152 26. Which ONE of the following sources of information has been most valu- able in your role as a preceptor? journals and formal presentations at conventions mailings from the College full-time faculty members and/or the Director of Education at your hospital other physician colleagues and students other (please specify): 27. How clear is it to you what the College expects you to do as a pre- ceptor? (Choose the one answer which best fits your perceptions.) it's really not very clear not clear, but I decided myself what is needed not clear, but other physicians and I together have worked out what should be done the College has made it sufficiently clear other (please specify): 28. On the average, during periods when you have a student, how frequently do you talk with other physicians about some aspect of the preceptor- ship program? _____almost never once a month once a week _____almost daily 29. When was the last pime you talked with a full-time faculty member in Department of Family Medicine about the preceptorship program? never less than 2 months ago 2 months or more ago 30. 31. 32. 153 How do you feel about the total amount of information available to you from all sources about the preceptorship program -- and when you get it? don't get information get it, but too little and too late get adequate information How do you feel about the accuracy and usefulness of the information which you get from all sources about the preceptorship program? it's accurate and useful it's accurate, but often not useful in my situation it's useful, but I often doubt its accuracy it is neither useful nor accurate Have you ever had any specific ideas about how the preceptorship program could be improved? yes _____no, not at this time If Yes: a. Have you shared your idea(s) with other physicians? no only briefly considerable discussion b. Have you ever communicated your idea(s) to any full-time faculty member of the Department of Family Medicine? yes no c. Briefly outline your suggestions in the space below. 154 33. How do you feel about the interest of College leaders in your experi- ences and perspectives as a preceptor? they have shown no sign of interest I assume they are interested, but they haven't done much to allow me to make my input they're definitely interested and have welcomed receiving my suggestions 34. flpgre and how would you most prefer to get your information about the preceptorship program and the College's expectations for your role? (Check one only.) the present arrangement is sufficient _____more, better presentations by officials at general meetings _____sma11 discussion and problem-solving sessions _____a visit to my office by College representative other (please specify): 35. Which of the following affiliations contributes more to your growth 36. 37. in clinical competence as a family practitioner? being a preceptor consultation and educational programs at my hospital neither contributes much Again, which of the following affiliations contributes more to your growth in clinical competence as a family practitioner? being a preceptor participation in professional associations neither contributes much Which of the following affiliations do you think contributes more to your patients' regard for you? being a preceptor my position(s) in the hospital staff neither contributes much BIBLIOGRAPHY 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. BIBLIOGRAPHY Sources Cited . Ackoff, Russell, "Towards a Behavioral Theory of Communication," Management Science, 4:218-34, 1957. Anlyon, William G., "Chairman's Address," Journal of Medical Educa- tion, 46:917-26. Barnlund, D.C., and Harland, C., "Propinquity and Prestige as Deter- minants of Communication Networks,” Sociometpy, 26:467-79, 1963. Beloff, Jerome S., Korper, E., R. Weinerman, "Medical Student Response to a Program for Teaching Comprehensive Care," Journal of Medical Edppapion, 45:1047-59, . Berlo, David K., "Essays on Communication," mimeographed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1970. , "Human Communication: The Basic Proposition," mimeograph, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1970. , R.v. Farace, RWA. Connelly and H.M. Russell, "Relation- ships Between Supervisor-Subordinate Communication Practices and Employee Turnover, Attendance and Performance Evaluations." Mimeo- graphed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1971. Berelson, Bernard, and Gary A. Steiner, Epman_fiehayigxgyép_lpygppp;y of Scientific Findings, N.Y.: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1964. Berrien, Kenneth F., General and Social Systems, New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press, 1968. Blake, R., C. Rhead, B. Wedge and J. Morton, "Housing Architecture and Social Interaction," Sociometr , 19:133-9, 1956. Bloom, Samuel W., "Sociology of Medical Education: Some Comments on the State of the Field," Milbank Memorial Fund Qparterly, 43:143-83. , "The Medical School as a Social System," Milbank Memorial Fpnd Qparterly, Vol. 49, No. 2, April 1971. Boan, J.A., Group Practice, Toronto: Royal Commission on Health Services, 1966. 155 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 156 Bowers, John 2., and Robert C. Parkin, "The Wisconsin Preceptor Pro- gram -- A Thirty Year Experiment in Medical Education," Journal of Medical Education, 32:610-12. Brillouin Leon, Science_pnd Information Theory, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1967. Bucher, Rue, and Joan Stelling, "Characteristics of Professional Organizations," Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 10:3-15. See Buckley, Walter, Sociology and Mgdern Systems Theory, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1967. Burns, Tom, "The Directions of Activity and Communication in a Departmental Executive Group,” Hpmap_§gla§122§, 7:73-97. Caplow, T., and R. Forman, "Neighborhood Interaction in a Homogeneous Community," American Sppiplogical Review, 15:357-66, 1950. Cartwright, Darwin, "Power" A.Neglected Variable in Social Psychology" in Bennis, Warren G., K.D. Benne and R. Chin, The Plannin of Chan e, N.Y.: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1966. Cheplove, Max, "The Role of the Family Practitioner in Medical Educa- tion," New York State Journal of Medicine, 68:1128-31. Coleman, James S., E. Katz and H. Menzel, Medical Innovation: A Diffusion Study, N.Y.: Hobbs-Merrill Co., Inc., 1966. Davis, Keith, "Management Communication and the Grapevine," Harvard Business Review, 31:43-49. Deutsch, Morton, and Robert M. Krauss, Theories in Social Psychology, N.Y.: Basic Books, 1965. Dorn, Robert M., "Preceptors and Preceptorships: The Teaching and Learning of Patient-Oriented Care," Journal of the Kansas Medical Society, 68:428-31. Etzioni, Amitai, A Comparative Analysis of Cppplex Organizations, Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1961. Farace, Richard V., and Richard A. Connelly, "Organizational Communi- cation Correlates of Herzberg's Theory of Work Satisfaction," mimeo- graphed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1970. , and Donald McDonald, "New Directions in the Study of Organization Communication," to be published in Personal Psychology Spring 1974. , and Hamish M. Russell, "Some Communication Implications of Major Organizational Theories," mimeographed, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1971. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 157 Festinger, Leon, Theopy and Experiment in Social Communication, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1950. , "Informal Social Communication," Psychological Review, 57:271-282. ,.S. Schacter and K. Back, Social Pressures in Informal Groups: A Study of Humgp Faptprs in Housing, Stanford, California: Stanford University Press, 1963. Fleming, William L., "Teaching of the Family Physician's Approach by a Department of Preventive Medicine," Jpprnal pf the American Medical Assopiation, 161:711-3. Gragg, Donald M., "The Teaching of Adult Ambulatory Patient Care in U.S. Medical Schools: Characteristics of Programs," Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1973. Guetzkow, Harold, "Communications in Organizations," in March, James G. (ed.), Handbook of Organizations, Rand MtNally and Co., 1965. Gullahorn, J.T., "Distance and Friendship Factors in the Gross Inter- action Mix,” Sociometpy 15:123-34. Habbe, S., "Communicating with Employees," Sgpdigg in Personngl Polic , No. 129. N.Y.: National Industrial Conference Board, 1952. Herzberg, Frederick, Scott Inkley and William R. Adams, "Some Effects on the Clinical Faculty of a Critical Incident Study of the Perform- ance of Students," gougna; of Medical Education 35:666-674. Homans, George C., Thg pran Qrogp, N.Y.: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1950. Hovland, C.I., and W. Weiss, "The Influence of Source Credibility on Communication Effectiveness," Public Opinion Qparterly, 15:135-50, 1952. Katz, Daniel, and Robert Kahn, The Social Psychology of Organizations, N.Y.: John Wiley and Sons, 1966. Kendall, Patricia, "Medical Education as Social Process," paper pre- sented to the American Sociological Association, 1960. Lawrence, Paul R., and Jay W. Lorsch, Organization and Environment, Homewood, 111.: Richard D. Irwin, Inc., 1969. Lewin, Kurt, Field Theory in Social Science, N.Y.: Harper, 1951. Lundberg, G., B. Hertzler, and L. Dickson, "Attraction Patterns in a University," Sociometr , 12:158-69. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 158 March, James G., and Herbert Simon, Organizations, N.Y.: John Wiley and Sons, 1958. McDonald, Donald, nggunication Roles and Communication Content in .A Bureaucratic Settin , Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Merton, Robert K., Social Theory and Social Structure (Rev. Ed.), Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press. , "The Social Psychology of Housing," in Dennis, W. (ed.) Cppggnn ngnds in Spgial Psychology, Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1948. , G.G. Reader and P.L. Kendall, The Student Physician, Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1957. Miller, James C., "Living Systems: Basic Concepts," Behavioral Sgience 10:193-237, 1965. Miller, G.A...Langpage and Communication, N.Y.: McGraw-Hill, 1951. Mouzelis, Nicos P., Organization Bureaucracy; AnzAnalysis of Modern Theories, London: Routledge and K. Paul, 1967. Cf. Nix, Harold L. and Frederick L. Bates, "Occupational Role Stresses," Rural Sociology, 27:7-17. Parsons, Talcott, Thg Social System, Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press, 1951. Powell, R.M., "Sociometric Analysis of Informal Groups -- Their Structure and Function in Two Contrasting Communities," Sociometgy 15:367-99, 1952. Reader, George 3., "Some of the Problems and Satisfactions of Teach- ing Comprehensive Medicine," Journal of Medical Education, 31:544-54. Reed, David E., "Twelve Years' Experience with a Comprehensive Ambula- tory Care Program," Jnurnal of Medical Education, 45:1041-6. Reindl, Max H., "Propositions on Information Management of Innovation Processes in Organizations," unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Saul, Ezra V., and Suzanne Bryder, "One Faculty's Sources of Informa- tion Reagrding Changes in Medical Education," Journal of Medical Education, 44:1091-4, 1969. Schein, E.H., Orgapizational Psychology, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1970 (Second Edition). 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 159 Shibutani, T., "Reference Groups as Perspectives," American Journal of Sociology, 60:562-70, 1955. Silver, George A., "Family Practice: Resusitation or Reform?" Jgurnal of the American Medical Association, 185:189-91. Sinclair, David C., Basic Medical Education, London: Oxford Univer- sity Press, 1972. Slaughter, Donald, "Clinical Clerkships for Sophomore Medical Stu- dents," Journal of Medical Education, 24:193-199. Smith, Alfred G., Cpmmunicatlons and Status: The Dypamics of a Research Center, Eugene, Oregon: University of Oregon Press, 1966. Smith, Hugo D., "Essays in Medical Education," Americap Journal of the Diseases of Children, 110:185-8. Snoke, Parnie S., and E.’ Heinerman, "Comprehensive Care Programs," Journal of Mgdical Education, 40:625-57. Thayer, Lee, "Communication and Organization Theory" in Dance, Frank E.X. (ed.), Hyman ngmpnlpatlon Theory, N.Y.: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1967. Trowbridge, Mason, "Extramural Preceptorships -- A Return to the Pre-Flexner Era of Medical Education?" New England Journal of Medicine, 258:691-5. Weinerman, E. Richard, "Yale Studies in Ambulatory Medical Care," New England Journal of Medicine, 272:947-54. Wenrick, J.W., F.C. Mann, W.C. Morris and A.J. Reilly, "Informal Educators for Practicing Physicians," Journal of Medicgl Education, 46:299-305, 1971. White, Kerr L., "Family Medicine, Academic Medicine, and University Responsibility," Journal of the American Medical Association, 185:192-6. Wolf, Stewart G., and Ward Darley, Medical Education and Practice, Evanston, I11,: American Association of Medical Colleges, 1965. Young, L.E., "Personal Physicians," Journal of the American Medical Society, 187:928-33. Zipf, G.K., "Some Determinants of the Circulation of Information," American Journal of P9 cholo , 59:401-21, 1946. "The Teaching of Comprehensive Patient Care," (editorial) American Journal of Ppblic Health, March 1970. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 160 General References . Ad Hoc Committee on Education for Family Practice, Meeting the Chal- lenge of Family Practice, Chicago: American Medical Association, 1966. . Becker, Howard S. and Blanche Geer, "The Fate of Idealism in Medical Schools," American Sociological Review, February 1958, pp. 50-56. , B. Geer, F.C. Hughes and A.L. Strauss, B0 8 in White: Student Culture in Medical Sahool, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1961. . Berlo, David K., The Process of Communication, N.Y.: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1960. Collins, Barry E. and Harold Guetzkow, A Social Psychology of Group Processes for Decision-Making, N.Y.: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1964. . Committee on Medical Economics, Organization and Management of Family Practice, Kansas City: American Academy of General Practice, 1968. Deutsch, Morton, "Field Theory in Social Psychology" in Lindsey, Gardner (ed.), Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. I, Cambridge, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 1954, p. 181-222. . Eaton, Joseph W., "Social Processes of Professional Teamwork," American Sociological Review, 16:707-713, 1951. Evans, John R., "Organizational Patterns for New Responsibilities," Journal of Medical Education, 45:988-99, 1970. Faulkner, James M., "Medical Education and the Physician: The Shattuck Lecture,” New England Journal of Medicine, 250:929-932, 1954. Georgopoulos, Basil S., Or anizational Research on Health Institutions, Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, 1972. , and Floyd C. Mann, The Communit General Hos ital, N.Y.: Macmillan Co., 1962. Geyman, John P., "Conversion of the General Practice Residency to Family Practice," ournal pf the Amerlcan Medical Association, 215: 1802-1807, 1971. Gouldner, A.W., "The Norm of Reciprocity: A Preliminary Statement," American Sociological Review, 25:161-179, 1960. Haas, J. Eugene and Thomas E. Drabek, Complex Organizations: A Sociological Perspective, N.Y.: Macmillan, 1973. Haggerty, : PIOblem of Teaching Comprehensive Community Care," American Journal of the Diseasesof Children, 116:509, 1968. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 161 Meyer, Roger J., "Medical Education and Medical Practice Demonstra- tions," Journal of Medical Education, 38:596-602, 1963. Miller, George E., Teaching and Learning in Medical Schools, Cam- bridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1961. Price, James W., Organizational Effectiveness: An Inventory of PrOpo- sitions, Homewood, 111.: Richard D. Irwin, Inc., 1968. Reader, George G. and Mary E.W. Goss (eds.), Comprehensive Medical Care and Teaching (a'report on the N.Y. Hospital - Cornell Medical Center Program), Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Press, 1967. Rittelmeyer, Louis F., Jr., ”Teaching the Family Physician's Approach, as Built Around General Practitioners," Journal of the American Medical Association, 161:705-7, 1956. Simon, Herbert H., Administrative Behavior, second edition, N.Y.: Macmillan, 1965. Wescoe, W. Clark,"Praceptors as General Educators,” Journal of Medical Education, 31:598-604, 1956. Wolf, George A., Jr., "The Preceptorship System at U.V.M.," Journal of Medical Education, 32:199-203, 1957. HICHIGRN STRTE UNIV. LIBRRRIES IIHI WI HI llll ”I Will UH || IN |l1| ”III! II Ml” H IWII 31293103834358