MESSAGE ORGANIZATION AND INFORMATION: A STUDY IN THE MEASUREMENT OF - HUMAN INFORMATION PROCESSING ~ Thesis for the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSI’ T" Y JOHN SANFORD MICKELSON 1971 III/III!!! III/IIIIII/III/IIII/I/IIII/IIIII/I I 3 1293 10383 4366 This is to certify that the thesis entitled MESSAGE ORGANIZATION AND INFORMATION: A STUDY IN THE MEASUREMENT OF HUMAN INFORMATION PROCESSING presented by John S. Mickelson has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Jh-D- demeinflmunigfljon I 5x ’ 7 $21 217%? A? XXI/7.7K Mnjor professor 0-7639 \ I MESSAGE ORGANIZATION AND INFORMATION: A STUDY IN THE MEASUREMENT OF HUMAN INFORMATION PROCESSING By John Sanford Mickelson This study was designed to examine relationships between message organization, message uncertainty, and verbal learning. Message organ- ization was manipulated by varying the distance between repeated sub- ject—verb-object triples in a sequence of sentences. Message uncer— tainty was defined as the uncertainty (variability) of word selections for subjects responding to messages where certain words were deleted from the text. Verbal learning was measured by looking at the uncer- tainty of word selections after subjects had read the intact texts. The measure, both for uncertainty and verbal learning, was Shannon's measure for information (H). Two messages were designed for this study. Message Two was con— structed with a larger number of lexical choices than Message One. Message Two was intended to increase the uncertainty of subjects' responses. Also, for both messages, an introductory paragraph was con- structed. The introductory paragraph was intended to reduce response uncertainty. One hundred and eighty (180) subjects within a 2 x 6 design responded to one of six variations for each of the two messages. The results indicated that when messages were presented in a dis- organized form, uncertainty increased significantly. Organized messages scored lower on uncertainty whether one looked at total uncertainty. John Sanford Mickelson scores or individual sentence scores. No significant differences were found for messages with and with- out introductory paragraphs. In some cases, the uncertainty scores were increased while in others, they decreased. No significant differences were found between Message One and Message Two when looking at all message versions. The uncertainty scores for Message Two were significantly greater than those for Message One when looking only at organized versions. The results did indicate a relationship between learning and processes cf uncertainty reduction. The uncertainty scores obtained for messages and sentences did provide an index of learning difficulty. MESSAGE ORGANIZATION AND INFORMATION: A STUDY IN THE MEASUREMENT OF HUMAN INFORMATION PROCESSING By John Sanford Mickelson A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR.OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication 1971 Ir—\~ Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Communication, College of Communication Arts, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree. Director offlThesis Guidance Committee: Chairman ACKNOWLEDGMENTS To my wife, Stephanie . . . who loved, encouraged and helped me. To my Chairman and Director, Dr. Gerald R. Miller . . . who taught, guided and helped more than he knows. To my committee: Dr. Erwin Bettinghaus, Dr. Larry Sarbaugh, Dr. John Gullahorn, and Dr. Eugene Jacobson . . . whose encourage- ment and criticism was indispensable. To Dr. James Campbell . . . who was always available when I needed him. To Tom Gordon and Ed Wotring . . . because having friends makes it all possible. TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I Chapter I RATIONALE AND HYPOTHESES . . . . . . . II III IV Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . Information, Uncertainty, and Message Organization . Information, Uncertainty, and Meaning . . . Information, Uncertainty, and Language Learning . METHOD I I I I I I I I I I I I Operationalizing the Variables . . . . . Study Design . . . . . . . . . . Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . Calculation of Information Scores . . . . . RESULTS I I I I I I I I I I I I I I Raw Scores . . . . . . . . Overall Treatment Effects . . . Effects of Message Organization (Hypothesis 1) . Effects of Introductory Paragraphs (Hypothesis 2) Effects of Semantic Variability (Hypothesis 3) Relationships Between Uncertainty and Learning (Hypothesis H) . . . . . . . DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Information Theories and Language Processing . Message Organization . . . . . . . . . Paragraph Interrelationships . . . . . . Semantic Variability . . . . . . . . . . Uncertainty and Learning . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv iii vi vii 16 22 23 23 2A 25 25 25 25 28 28 29 3“ 31+ 35 37 39 A1 44 Table of Contents (con'd. BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . APPENDIX I . . . . . APPENDIX II . . . . APPENDIX III . . . . Page H6 50 51 65 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 RAW SCORES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 2 FRIEDMAN TWO-WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE (TREATMENTS x SENTENCES) . . . . . . . . . . 27 3 KRUSKAL-WALLIS ONE-WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF DIS- ORGANIZED MESSAGES COMPARED WITH LEARNING TESTS . . 29 u KENDALL RANK CORRELATIONS: MESSAGE ONE TREATMENTS x SENTENCES . . . . . . . . . 30 5 KENDALL RANK CORRELATIONS: MESSAGE Two TREATMENTS x SENTENCES . . . . . . . . . 31 6 KENDALL RANK CORRELATION: MESSAGE ONE TREATMENTS x MESSAGE TWO TREATMENTS . . . . . . 32 vi LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page I ORGANIZATIONAL SCHEMES: . . . . . . . . . . . l7 (a) MAXIMALLY ORGANIZED (b) MAXIMALLY DISORGANIZED 2 DELETION SCHEMES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 vii CHAPTER 1 RATIONALE AND HYPOTHESES Introduction Many reasons have been given for ngt_applying concepts from mathematical communication theory to human language behavior. The most frequent is that the Shannon measure of information does not deal with meaning. What Shannon (1948) says, however, is that semantic ques- tions are not relevant to the engineering problems. Note Shannon does not say that semantic questions are not relevant nor does he say that information measures are not relevant for semantic problems. As Rapoport (1953) points out, ". . . because the semantic problems are not relevant to the technical ones, is not to say the techniques used to solve the technical problems are not useful in solving the semantic problems." (p. 160) According to Rapoport, then, there is some justi- fication for exploring how some of Shannon's mathematical concepts might relate to human language behavior. Before preceding further, it may be useful to clear up one semantic problem in the terminology of mathematical communication theory. Mathe- matical communication theory (Shannon, 1948; Shannon and Weaver, 1957) views communication as a probabilistic process at least at the engineer— ing level. At this level, information is defined as the statistical improbability of a given message with respect to all other messages which are possible in the language of interest. When considering application of mathematical communication concepts to human behavior, it is useful to modify some of the theoretic terms. This need results from an inconsistency between our own intuitions about what informa— tion is and does and its mathematical definition as improbability. What one usually means when labeling a message informative is that knowledge about a subject or event reduces uncertainty. The use of the word "uncertainty" in this context equates with the mathematical communication theorist's use of the word "information." When it is necessary to refer to the statistical improbability of an observation or a message, the label uncertainty will be used. When reference is made to what an observer gains from a message, the label information will be employed. Having clarified how the words information and uncertainty_will be used, one can consider more specifically how these concepts can be applied to human language processes. The present study is specifi- cally concerned with how information and uncertainty are related to: (l) the organization of messages, and (2) learning from messages. Information, Uncertainty, and Message Organization To understand how people process information in their communi- cation transactions, it is necessary to adopt a molar view. People do not communicate phonemes, words, sentences, or grammars in the nor- mal course of events. Most communication transactions go beyond sim— ple, single sentence messages. Usually, communication transactions take the form.of organized sequences of sentences, such as paragraphs or dialogues. The fact of organized sequences implies some guiding purpose. People organize messages to make them less uncertain for the receiver, thereby increasing the probability that the correct or intended meanings will be elicited. This organization is first seen at the syntactic level. Studies of relations between syntax and information are limited. Grammarians argue that the syntax of a language provides an organizing function for the semantic information in messages. Language syntax has the effect of constraining or limiting the semantic information by pro- viding a pre—determined format for presenting semantic relations. This notion of predetermination suggests that the function words of language might be "overlearned." Thus, if one should delete a syntactic element from a message, the presented information should increase, but not so much as if a semantic element were deleted. The basis for this argument stems from work by Chomsky (1957, 1965) dealing with differences between the surface and deep structure of language. The surface level of lan- guage deals with relations between words in sequences. On the deep structure level (semantic level), words are organized by relational concepts. Studies by Miller, Newman and Friedman (1958); Born, Rubenstein and Sterling (1959); Fillenbaum, Jones and Rapoport (1963); Weinstein, Feldstein and Jaffe (1965); Tannenbaum, Williams and Clark (1969); and Hakes and Foss (1970) bear on this point. The results of each of these studies indicate that for a sample of mutilated text, function words (articles, auxillary verbs, prepositions, conjunctions) are more easily replaced than are semantic words (nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs). Most of these studies are based simply on the accuracy of of an observer's responses to a mutilated text. The Tannenbaum, gt_313 study is more interesting. The researchers were interested in determining whether providing an observer with a form class designator for each deletion would improve his performance. The results were affirmative. For deletions from semantic form classes, observers more frequently chose words from that form class. Verbatim replacements in the semantic.form class remained the same. With function words, observers performed better both at choosing a word from the correct form class and also with making verbatim replacements. The study by Hakes and Foss was constructed to test the utility of relative pronouns (function words) as cues to the underlying deep structure for self embedded sentences. Observers heard sentences, half of which had the relative pronouns deleted; e.g., "The car the man the dog bit drove crashed." These were mixed with sentences which retained the relative pronoun; e.g., "The car that the man whom the dog bit drove crashed." According to two measures of sentence compre- hension — phoneme monitoring and sentence paraphrasing - observers pro- cessed sentences retaining the relative pronoun more effectively. These and other studies (Miller and Friedman, 1957; Ruhanstein and Pollack, 1963; Pollack, 1963; Gross, 1966; Butler and Merikle, 1970; Postman, 1970; Swanson and Wickens, 1970) indicate that language syntax performs an organizing function on language which serves to reduce the presented uncertainty of messages. It is not unreasonable to suppose that the sequencing of sentences in paragraph form serves a similar function. Neither grammarians nor communication scientists have considered at length the interrelationships between sentences. Consider the par- agraph sequence you are now reading. Sentence 1 provides a set for the reader to think about what will follow. Under one form of organiza- tion, each succeeding sentence should operate to reduce the reader's uncertainty about what is meant by that first sentence. To the extent that the organization of this paragraph is effective, the reader's uncertainty about what is meant by the relations between sentences in sequence and how they operate to reduce uncertainty should be removed by the end of the concluding sentence. Although the question of sentence sequencing for paragraphs has not been studied in any depth, one can speculate about the effect on an observer of destroying paragraph structure. Suppose one takes a sequence of paragraphs and reprints them without indenting. If the original paragraphs (sentence sequences) had the effect of reducing uncertainty, it is reasonable to expect that observers could locate and recognize paragraph boundaries. A study by Koen, Becker and Young (1969) obtained results in line with this prediction. Two experiments were conducted to determine: (1) whether observers could recognize paragraph boundaries in unindented prose, and (2) whether the cues to paragraph boundaries were formal (grammatical) or semantic. For the second study, prose passages were replaced by nonsense paralogs. Observers were able to recognize paragraph boundaries in both prose and nonsense versions with reliabilities .86 and .75 respectively. Another study by Darnell (1963) examined the relation between sentence order and comprehension. A message of fifteen sentences was constructed so as to have a deductive order. The original order of these sentences was manipulated to obtain six messages each of which presented an increasing degree of disorder. Certain items were deleted from each message and 83 were asked to replace the deleted items. The mean cloze scores for the seven message forms were found to be signi- ficantly different. Also the degree of disorganization was positively correlated to the difficulty of replacing deleted items. Both of these experiments attribute importance to the organized structure of sentences. The Darnell study indicates this organization might have something to do with the statistical pr0perties of messages. Clearly, the organization of messages does affect the capacity of peo- ple to deal with those messages. The less uncertain the organization of a message, the more probable it is that a receiver will gain the information intended by the producer of the message. The arguments presented in this section give rise to the first hypothesis for this study: H1: The uncertainty presented by an organized sequence of sentences will be significantly less than the uncertainty of the same sentences in a disorganized sequence. The first hypothesis raises questions only about the organization of sentences within a paragraph. One can also ask what is the relation of one paragraph to another. Hypothesis 2 is: H2: There will be a significant decrease in the uncertainty of a message when it is preceded by a message intended to reduce its uncertainty. Information, Uncertainty and Meaning_ To deal with problems of semantic information, it is necessary to think of what a human source does - functionally - when he sends a message. Certainty the source's primary goal is not to have the receiver accurately reproduce the words of the message. More probably the source selects and transmits words that have some probability of eliciting s e- gifig_meanings from the receiver. Thus one would argue after MacKay (1969) that measurement of semantic information is a selective function defined not on words but on meanings. The measurement of semantic information should be based not on the relative frequency of words, but on the relative frequencies of word meanings. The difficulties of assessing information at the semantic level are compounded by problems unsolved at other levels of language. It becomes necessary to deal not only with the informational structure of the language itself but also with the whole semantic space of persons using language to communicate. One must deal with both the denotative and the connotative aspects of meanings of words. One must deal not only with contextual relations between words, but also with the context within which the words are spoken or written. At the semantic level, the statistical structure of language is less dependent upon how it is represented in random samples of literature than upon an individual's meanings for words alone or in sequence. Studies of language acquisition illustrate the conceptual rele— vance of information theory at semantic levels. Luria (1969, 1970) reports research showing that when a child begins to use a word, he applies the word to anything having a central attribute of the word's referent. The child might apply the word "car" to any transportation mode. At first the central feature of car is that it moves or carries. Thus the word "car" might apply to horses, trucks, trains or airplanes. As the child acquires knowledge of "car," he becomes more discriminat- ing in his use of the word. The word begins to regulate his behavior. The child begins to use the word ”car" to discriminate cars from horses, trains and airplanes even though he may yet apply the word to trucks. But, in general, it seems evident that with knowledge acquisition, the variability (uncertainty) between word use and referent is decreased. While there is reason to suspect that the uncertainty presented by messages is a function of semantic variability, few attempts have been made to assess the amount of semantic information in messages. Rosenberg (1969) approaches the problem of semantic information in sen- tences by distinguishing between semantically well integrated (SWI) and semantically poorly integrated (SPI) sentences. He argues that where sentences are semantically well integrated, observers will be better able to recall the accompanying verbal material. The two types of sen— tences are distinguishable by sentence pairs like: (1) The doctor who fired the janitor cured the patient (SWI), and (2) The doctor who fired the janitor shoots the author (SPI). Again, no attempt was made to construct a measure of semantic integration. Nevertheless, on princi— ple, one possible measure is obvious. One can simply count the number of poor integrations and compare this with the number of good integra— tions. The chief problem or disadvantage of this measure is that it is based on a definition of semantic integration that would be unlikely to apply in normal use of language. The SPI sentence above simply has a low probability of occurrence. A more satisfying approach to the semantic complexity of sen— tences is taken by Perfetti (1969). Perfetti employed two measures of lexical density for sentences. One measure was based on the absolute number of semantic words as compared with the number of function words for sentences. The second measure was based upon the number of items of Specific information offered by a sentence. The sentence "The fire- men hurried to the factory that was reported burning" contains three items of information. They are "firemen hurried to factory," "factory was burning," and "someone reported it." This sentence would be con- sidered to have a lower lexical density than the sentence "The museum is providing free public lectures on modern art." Five separate items of information are presented by this statement. The study by Perfetti was designed to determine whether the se- mantic or the syntactic complexity of sentences best predicts diffi- culties people have learning sentences. His findings indicate that increasing either syntactic or semantic complexity increases learning difficulties. The measures of semantic difficulties, however, provided better predictions of learning difficulty. If Luria's report is taken in conjunction with Perfetti's research, there is some basis for arguing that the critical aspect for the sta- tistical structure of language is related to the meanings people have for words. Thus one would expect that the organization of messages Operates somehow on the semantic structure to increase the probability that specific meanings are selected by the receiver. Further, one would expect that to the extent message organization does not increase the probability for specific meanings, learning difficulties will be increased. The preceding arguments give rise to the third hypothesis for this study. H3: If the lexical variability of one message is greater than that of another, uncertainty will also be significantly greater. 10 Information, Uncertainty and Language Learning To understand how people learn from messages, it is necessary to consider the process by which information is gained from messages. Behavioral scientists (Landahl, 1941; Ammons, 1956; Rapoport, 1956; Lenneberg, 1957; Kochen and Galanter, 1958; RapoPort, 1961; Attneave, 1967; Moss and Neidt, 1969; Malic, 1970) have argued that learning can be viewed as a process of uncertainty (entropy) reduction. Ammons suggests that learning outcomes be thought of as uncertainty reduction since information is what removes uncertainty. Malic contrasts physical and intellectual systems. The physical environment constrains Open sys- tems such that they tend to reach a stationary state with a minimum of entropy production. Intellectual systems, in contrast, impose constraints on the environment. Intellectual systems consume entropy (uncertainty). By consuming entropy the intellectual being acts against the environ- ment, changing relations in the environment and heightening levels of organization. These views imply that learning is a process of consum- ing or removing presented uncertainty. In general, research supports this view. A series of studies by Landahl (1941a, 1941b) and Rapoport (1956, 1960, 1961) serve to illustrate the efficacy of learning defined as uncertainty reduction. The Landahl studies (though pre-mathematical communication theory) were designed to test a theory of error elimina— tion based on known properties of neural mechanisms. Landahl predicted both error rate and number of errors made in a learning task would depend upon the number of elements to be learned. One hundred and twenty pairs of four letter words were prepared in groups of A, 8 and 12. Subjects 11 were told that for each stimulus word, there was one response word with which the stimulus word should be associated and that they were to determine the correct word by trial and error. This procedure was carried out to 16 trials, of which the last three were errorless except in two cases. The four word pairs were perfectly learned after six trials, eight word pairs after 10 and 12 word pairs after 14. Both error rate and number of errors increased with number of items to be leaned. Landahl's findings led Rapoport to inquire whether learning was a simple function of number of elements or of the statistical structure of the language; and, if so, could the relation be expressed in infor- mation theoretic terms? Rapoport (1960) taught several groups of sub- jects four artificial languages. Subjects associated irregular geo— metric figures with their respective names in the language. The refer- ents were the same in all four language, five different geometric figures and five different colors. Of 25 possible combinations, 20 were used as referents. Referents were presented in random order and subjects required to make a correct verbal response depending on the language being learned. Languages were structured as follows (p. 88): L1: With each of the 20 figures a single syllable was associated. L2:, With each of the 20 figures a pair of distinct syllables was associated, no syllable being associated with more than one figure. L : With each of the five shapes and five colors a single syllable was associated. The shape always came first. L : The vocabulary of this language is the same as L except there is no systematic correspondence between syllables and aspects (color and shape) of the referents. 12 The differential structure of these languages gives rise to the follow- ing uncertainty calculations: H(Ll) = u2.3 nits H(L2) = 84.6 nits H(L3) = 10.3 nits H .05). Thus, according to this analysis, the third hypothesis is not confirmed. Relationship Between Uncertainty and Learning (Hypothesis 4) Several analyses were performed on the data in Table l to illum- inate relations between language uncertainty and learning. Test scores for the intact messages were first compared with uncertainty scores over the disorganized message versions. Because test questions were cast in the same order as sentences of the disorganized messages, it TABLE 3: KRUSKAL-WALLIS ONE—WAY ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE OF DISORGANIZED MESSAGES COMPARED WITH LEARNING TESTS Treatments Rank Sums D L H DI LI H Message One 147 63 10.08** 127 83 2.7(N.S.) Message Two 134 76 4.8* 138 72 6.2* :'::': P < .01 3" p < .05 30 was felt that if subjects learned nothing by reading the intact mes- sages, there would be no difference between these sets of scores. Table 3 summarizes the results of a Kruskall—Wallis two—way anal- ysis of variance between each disorganized message and the test over its intact counterpart. For three of the four pairs, this test indi- cates that the scores are drawn from significantly different popula- tions. The rank totals (on which this test statistic is based) show that all differences are in the predicted direction. The sign test was also applied to these pairs to determine whether sentence to sentence differences were significant. In all but one case (TB—T11), this test reveals a significant difference between uncertainty scores obtained for a disorganized message version and the test over its intact counterpart. Again all differences are in the predicted direction. TABLE 4: KENDALL RANK CORRELATIONS: MESSAGE ONE TREATMENTS x SENTENCES T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 T1: Organized .80 .96 .71 .69 .78 T2: Disorganized .69 .51 .69 T3: Organized (Intro) .60 .69 T4: Disorganized (Intro) .42* .64 T5: Learning Test .64 T6: Learning Test (Intro) *N.S. (all other correlations are significant, p <:.05) 31 With respect to learning, it was also argued that the uncertainty scores obtained for the mutilated messages should predict learning difficulty for items in the intact text. If this were true, one should find a significant correlation between uncertainty scores for mutilated messages and corresponding test items. Table 4 and Table 5 show Ken— dall rank correlations between sentence uncertainty scores for all treatments on Message One and Message Two respectively (parametric correlations are found in Appendix III). TABLE 5: KENDALL RANK CORRELATIONS: MESSAGE ONE TREATMENTS x SENTENCES T8 T9 T10 T11 T12 T7: Organized .00 .60* .38 .56* .51* T8: Disorganized .11 .07 -.20 -.42 T9: Organized (Intro) .42 .20 .20 T10: Disorganized (Intro) .11 .38 T11: Learning Test .51* T12: Learning Test (Intro) * p <:.05 The fourth hypothesis predicts a significant positive correla— tion between the uncertainty presented by items of information in a message and the learning of those items. For Message One, the only correlation which is non—significant is between the sentences of a dis- organized message version and test scores over the intact message. 32 Because it is argued in this study that organized message estimates of uncertainty will be better predictors of message uncertainty, this finding is in line with the predictions of this study. For Message Two, the correlations (Table 5) are both more and less encouraging. These correlations are more encouraging because the only significant ones are between organized message versions and test items. They are less encouraging because the organized message with an introduction does not correlate with test items. TABLE 6: KENDALL RANK CORRELATION: MESSAGE ONE TREATMENTS x MESSAGE TWO TREATMENTS 11.18.12.291qu Tl: Organized 0.00 T2: Disorganized -.033 T3: Organized (Intro) 0.00 T4: Disorganized (Intro) 0.29 T5: Learning Test —.0.07 T6: Learning Test (Intro) 0.29 All correlations are non—significant. Because the deletion patterns Two are identical, one might suppose for the various message versions are schemes. If this were so, one would the different messages to correlate with one another. for both Message One and Message that the uncertainty scores obtained a function only of the deletion expect the scores obtained under Table 6 shows 33 the Kendall rank correlations between Message One and Message Two counterparts. None of these correlations are significant. The fourth hypothesis for this study suggests that learning and uncertainty are closely related. Three separate analyses of data obtained for this study confirm the hypothesis. CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION Information Theories and Language Processing_ The research conducted in this study bears upon several impor- tant theoretical and methodological problems of human communication and language behavior. Several scientists, notably MacKay (1950, 1951, 1955, 1969) and Bar-Hillel (1964) have constructed models for human language processing and/or learning based upon Shannon's math- ematical theory of communication. Generally, these models suggest that human information processing is a function of organization and the statistical characteristics of either stimuli or messages. Fur- ther, these models imply that both degree of organization and the statistical properties of messages can be tapped by Shannon's lOga- rithmic measure of information (or uncertainty). The findings of this study provide evidence of strong relations between information and uncertainty processes and both message organization and learning. Moreover, they indicate that the Shannon measure is not only useful for research into human information processing, but also sensitive to a variety of message manipulations. Whereas theories of human information processing have been avail— able for some time, little research into these processes has been con-. ducted. This shortcoming has been due largely to a lack of sufficiently powerful methods for analyzing and manipulating the semantic structure of messages. The methods developed to construct messages for this 35 study are powerful, both for constructing messages and for analyzing extant messages. The methods used here for paragraph organization can be considered a first approximation of a grammar for messages which goes beyong the level of sentences. This approach should extend con- siderably the range of message research that can be attempted as well as the precision with which it can be conducted, for it permits inspec- tion of aspects of sentences and messages that are not easily manipu- lated by the methods of traditional and modern sentence grammars. While these claims may seem somewhat far reaching, a fuller dis— cussion and interpretation of the results of this study should provide support for their validity. Message Organization Previous research has shown a relation between sentence order and comprehension. Darnell (1963), for instance, constructed six mes- sages each of which presented an increasing degree of disorder. He found that the degree of disorganization was positively correlated with the difficulty of replacing deleted items. The present study is both a replication and an extention of Darnell's results. The first hypothesis of the present study predicted that uncer- tainty scores obtained for disorganized message versions would be signi- ficantly higher than the scores obtained for organized message versions. This prediction was strongly confirmed. The total uncertainty scores obtained for disorganized message versions are significantly higher than the scores obtained for organized message versions. To this extent, the present study is only a replication of the Darnell study 36 with two exceptions. First, the organized version of Darnell's mes- sage was arrived at by constructing a deductive argument. While both deductive and inductive arguments do appear in texts, they do not always appear in a straightforward manner. The methods devised for this study are not limited to any particular argumentative form. Thus, they are more general. Secondly, the Darnell study employs an n32. word deletion scheme which does not permit one to tap the exclusively semantic dimensions of the message. In going beyong the Darnell study, the present study also shows that the uncertainty scores obtained for individual sentences are sig— nificantly lower for organized message versions. Where several studies have shown that the disruption of normal sentence organization effects sentence comprehension, this study suggests that the interrelations between sentences might also effect sentence comprehension. Studies by Marks and Miller (1954), Herriot (1967), Bever, Lackner and Kirk (1969), Hakes and Foss (1970), and Levelt (1970) show that variations in the grammar of sentences affect ease of processing or learning. The present study shows that message processing ease is more than a func— tion of grammar. The results of this study suggest that one can organize mes- sages so as to significantly reduce the amount of presented uncertainty. By extending the line of research begun here to a greater range of messages and types of organization, it should be possible to determine what kinds of message organization will achieve optimum uncertainty reduction for the recipients of these messages. Research along these lines must, however, impose greater control on the research design. Several questions are left unanswered concerning relations between 37 message organization and uncertainty. Some of these questions are: 1. How do uncertainty scores obtained with organized and disorgan— ized message versions compare with uncertainty scores that could be obtained if the sentences were presented independently of one another? 2. How are the uncertainty scores influenced by the prior knowledge of subjects? 3. How do different individual cognitive styles effect how people respond to mutilated messages? 4. How do factors of personality and intelligence relate to different kinds of organizational schemes? These are only a few of the questions which need to be investigated by future researchers. Paragraph Interrelationships Only one previous study dealing with paragraph interrelationships was found in the literature. Koen, Becker and Young (1969) showed that subjects were able to recognize paragraph boundaries in unin- dented prose. The present study attempted to go one step further and demonstrate the specific nature of this interrelationship. It was argued that if two paragraphs in sequence were related, the first par- agraph in the sequence would have the effect of reducing the uncer- tainty scores for the second paragraph in two ways. First, it was felt the language introduced in the first paragraph would constrain the lexical choices made by subjects as they responded to the second par— agraph. Second, the first paragraph was designed to present the 38 information necessary to accurately fill in the first deleted semantic partial. The results, however, failed to support this hypothesis. The effect of the introductory paragraphs was in some cases to raise uncertainty scores and in others to lower them. No clear trend emerged. One reason why this treatment may have failed to produce signi- ficant results is that only one subject made use of the information in the introductory paragraph to accurately supply the first semantic partial for the second paragraph. Also, there may possibly have been some kind of interaction between the writing styles for the two para- graphs. The introductory paragraphs were written in a more normal style than were the mutilated paragraphs. Whereas the mutilated passages were built around the four partials per sentence organizational scheme, the introductory paragraphs were not. No constraints were imposed on the writing of introductory paragraphs except the need to include the first semantic partial. Although this study yielded no clear results concerning para- graph interrelationships, some suggestions for future research can be made. Any future research into paragraph interrelationships should more accurately specify the nature of the paragraph relationships. The method used to construct the messages for this study could and should have been used to analyze the introductory paragraphs to determine whe- ther and to what extent the semantic partials presented by the introduc— tory paragraphs were related to those in the mutilated messages. Also, the test hypothesis should not be directional. If the semantic partials for the introductory paragraph are largely independent of those in the second paragraph, the level of uncertainty may be raised for both messages. Finally, some assessment should be made of the uncertainty 39 presented by the introductory paragraphs before they are coupled with the mutilated passages. Semantic Variability Two previous studies (Rosenberg, 1969; Perfetti, 1969) show that the ability of people to comprehend language is a function of the meanings available to them. Rosenberg provides evidence that when sentences are semantically well integrated, subjects will be better able to recall the accompanying verbal material. Perfetti employs two measures of semantic variability: (l) the number of semantic partials, and (2) the number of different words from semantic form classes. Perfetti presents evidence that both of his semantic mea- sures better index comprehension that do other measures of sentence comprehension (e.g., syntactic complexity), and that of these two mea— sures, the semantic partial is the better index. The two messages constructed for this study presented the same number of semantic partials. The messages varied only as to the number of different lexical choices making up those partials. Message One partials use 14 different words and Message Two partials use 24. For this reason, it was predicted that Message Two uncertainty scores would be greater than Message One scores. The results fail to confirm this prediction. Still, several possible limitations of the present study suggest this hypothesis should not be ignored in future research. One explanation of the failure to support this hypothesis is that the deletion schemes constructed for the two messages were iden- tical. Each deletion for a sentence was the deletion of one semantic 4O partial. The semantic partials for Message Two were designed to be independent of one another in terms of lexical choices. Message One partials were designed so that each pair (AB, BC, . . ., LM) was related by the lexical choices. It was this difference in the con— struction of the partials which was intended to bring about the increased variability of the Message Two set. Now, unless the dele— tion scheme tapped this difference between the messages, one would not eXpect the uncertainty scores to reflect the differential construction. For a deletion scheme to tap this dimension of the messages, sequential pairs of partials must be deleted (e.g.; AB, CD, . . ., LM). The deletion scheme actually employed taps only one pair, AB. The fact that the differential aspects of message construction were not tapped by the deletion scheme gives rise to a second reason why this hypothesis merits further study. The interdependence of the semantic partials for Message One operates throughout the message only in the organized versions. If one looks at the uncertainty score dif— ferences between the messages for organized versions only, the Mes— sage One scores are significantly lower than those for Message Two (Mann—Whitney U = l, p‘< .05). So, when one looks at semantic varia— bility in conjunction with the organizational pattern which gives rise to it, semantic variability does seem to have a significant effect. Future research should also consider the source of semantic variability. While Perfetti argues that semantic variability is a function of number of lexical choices, the presented research has posited that semantic variability is probably a function of the number of different meanings a subject might have for a word. One can argue from this perspective that there is no a_priori reason to expect 14 41 different words to have fewer meanings than 24 different words. Uncertainty and Learning_ Prior research by Landahl (1941a, 1941b), Lenneberg (1957), and Rapoport (1956, 1960, 1961) using artificial languages indicated a possible relation between the uncertainty of a language and the dif— ficulties people had learning the language. Because of the artifi- cial nature of these languages and the many points on which they were different from natural language, the present study was designed to determine whether these same processes operate for natural language. The fourth hypothesis for this study concerned the extent to which processes of learning are related to uncertainty reduction. Whereas one can accurately calculate the presented uncertainty of an artificial language (the number of referents for each symbol is known), the same is not true for natural languages. Some means for estimating the uncertainty of a message has to be constructed. The mutilated, disorganized messages for this study were used to index how much uncertainty was presented by the individual sentences. The organ- ized message versions were used to index the extent to which an organ— ized pattern for these sentences would reduce that uncertainty. Keep- ing these points in mind, it is possible to consider the hypothesis that learning is a function of uncertainty reduction. To test whether learning is a process of uncertainty reduction, subjects were first asked to read the intact message versions and then respond to a test covering those messages. The tests for this study were designed to ask questions calling for subjects to identify semantic 42 partials which had been deleted from the mutilated messages. The questions were presented in the same order as the sentences of the muti- lated message versions. It was argued that if the uncertainty scores obtained for responses to test questions were lower than the scores obtained for disorganized message versions, these lower scores must have resulted from what subjects learned. The data confirmed this prediction, both with respect to total message scores and on a sentence- to-sentence basis. In fact, inspection of uncertainty scores obtained for test questions indicated they were in all cases lower than the scores obtained from organized message versions. A more powerful aspect of the relationships between learning and uncertainty is revealed by the intercorrelations between uncertainty scores for sentences under the different treatments. It was hypothesized that the correlations between uncertainty scores for sentences of organ- ized message versions and test items would be better than those between disorganized versions and test items. For Message One, both organized and disorganized message versions were found to correlate significantly with test items. For Message Two, only organized versions (though not all) correlated significantly with test items. Even though some dis— organized message versions did correlate with test items, it can be shown that organized message versions were significantly better pre— dictors of learning difficulty. Tables 4 and 5, found in Chapter 3, show eight organized/disorganized pairs of correlations. Of these eight pairs, only one shows a higher correlation with a test for a disorgan— ized as compared with its organized counterpart. Under the sign test, the probability of such an event is less than .05. In other words, the uncertainty scores obtained from organized message versions are signifi— 43 cantly better predictors of learning difficulty than those obtained from disorganized messages. A third aspect of relations between uncertainty and learning has to do with the source of uncertainty. One possible interpretation for the findings of this study is that the obtained uncertainty scores are a function of how many times a semantic partial appears in the text, whether mutilated or intact, before its deletion. In other words, the uncertainty score for a semantic partial may be related only to its frequency of appearance in the test. If this were the case, one would expect the scores for sentences in the two messages to be significantly correlated. Because both the organizational schemes and deletion schemes are identical, this is a reasonable exPectation. None of the correlations between a Message One and a Message Two counterpart are significant. In fact, five of these six correlations (Table 6) are near zero or negative. This finding would seem to support MacKay's (1969) argument that message uncertainty is a function of the meanings triggered in people by messages. While it is argued throughout this study that uncertainty is a function of semantic variability, the results by no means conclusively support this argument. Though the results are generally supportive, further research is needed. The present study, where it considers relations between uncer- tainty and learning, has several weaknesses. First, a better index of the uncertainty of the messages could be obtained by having different groups of people respond to the mutilated texts in such a way that an index of uncertainty for each partial could be obtained. Such a pro- cedure would permit one to get a clearer view of how the partials are 44 related one to another. Second, uncertainty indices should be obtained for each sentence as it stands alone. As this study was conducted, one could argue there is no basis for saying that uncertainty was reduced by organizing the sentences. The converse could be true. The disorganized message versions may have produced greater uncertainty because of the disorganization such that scores for sentences of dis— organized versions might be higher than scores obtained for sentences standing alone. These possibilities should be investigated in future research. Summary This study explores four general questions: (1) how is message organization related to uncertainty? (2) how do paragraphs in sequence effect uncertainty? (3) how is semantic variability related to uncer— tainty? and (4) how is message uncertainty related to processes of learning? The evidence provided by this study provides a partial answer to the first question. A decrease in message organization clearly increases the uncertainty presented by messages. With respect to the second question, no clarification is provided by this study. One can only speculate that paragraph interrelationships are not simple. What— ever the interrelationships, they are more complex than anticipated. The third question is also not clearly answered. While the study fails to support the stated hypothesis, the failure is to some extent a failure of the design. The fourth question, concerning relations between learn— ing and uncertainty, is confirmed for this study. Learning does seem to be a process of uncertainty reduction. Also, the uncertainty of 45 messages and/or sentences does provide an index of learning difficulty. Whereas the findings of this study are encouraging, further research is indicated and necessary. Although procedures are designed to obtain information values for natural language messages, the design of the messages employed in this research is somewhat artifi— cial. Rarely, if ever, do our everyday messages take the overbear- ingly redundant form of the messages used here. There is also the question of just how meaningful the messages employed here are. Other, more specific, weaknesses have already been pointed out. The above comments are by no means meant to denigrate the find- ings of this research. It only seems necessary to be careful how the results are interpreted until more research has been conducted. BIBLIOGRAPHY .l. Ill—I. I III III I ’ltlllln I I BIBLIOGRAPHY I Ammons, R. B., "Effects Of Knowledge On Performance: A Survey And ‘ Tentative Formulation,” JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY. Vol. 54, 1956, pp. 279-299. ifAttneave, P., APPLICATIONS OF INFORMATION THEORY TO PSYCHOLOGY. Holt, “ Rinehart and Winston, New York, 1967. Bar-Hillel, Y., LANGUAGE AND INFORMATION. Addison-Wesley, London, 1964. Bever, T. C., Lackner, J. R. and Kirk, R., "The Underlying Structure Of Sentences Are The Primary Units Of Immediate Speech Processing," PER- CEPTION AND PSYCHOPHYSICS. V01. 5, 1969, pp. 225-234. Born, M. A., Rubenstein, H. and Sterling, T. C., "Sources Of Contextual Constraint Upon Words In Sentences," JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHO- PHYSICS. Vol. 57, 1959, pp. 171-180. Brillouin, L., "Mathematics, Physics, And Information," INFORMATION AND CONTROL. v61. 1, 1957, pp. 1-5. Butler, B. E. and Merikle, P. M., ”Uncertainty And Meaningfulness In A Paired-Associate Learning," JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 9, 1970, pp. 634-641. Chomsky, N., SYNTACTIC STRUCTURES. Mouton and Co., The Hague, 1957. Chomsky, N., ASPECTS OF A THEORY OF SYNTAX. M.I.T. Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1965. Darnell, D. K., ”The Relation Between Sentence Order And Comprehension," SPEECH MONOGRAPHS. Vol. 30, 1963, pp. 97-100. Fillenbaum, 8., Jones, L. V. and Rapoport, Amnon, "The Predictability Of Words And Their Grammatical Classes As A Function Of Rate Of Deletion From A Speech Transcript," JOURNAL OF VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 2, 1963, pp. 186-194. Gross, H. S., "The Effect On Word Recognition Of The Frequency Of Word Association," LANGUAGE AND SPEECH. Vol. 9, 1966, pp. 52-62. Hakes, D. T. and Foss, D. J., "Decision Processes During Sentence Com- prehension: Effects Of Surface Structure Reconsidered," PERCEPTION AND PSYCHOPHYSICS. Vol. 8, 1970, pp. 413-416. Herriot, P., "Phrase Units And The Recall Of Grammatically Structured Nonsence," BRITISH HOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY. Vol. 58, 1967, pp. 237—242. 46 Hyman, L. M. and Kaufman, H., "Information And The Memory Span,” PERCEP- TION AND PSYCHOPHYSICS. Vol. 1, 1966, pp. 235-237. Kochen, M. and Galanter, E., ”The Acquisition And Utilization Of Informa- tion In Problem Solving And Thinking," INFORMATION AND CONTROL. Vol. 1, 1958, pp. 267-2880 Koen, F., Becker, A. and Young, R., "The Psychological Reality Of The Paragraph," JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 8, 1969, pp. 49-53. Landahl, H. D., ”Studies In The Mathematical Biophysics Of Discrimination And Conditioning I," BULLETIN OF MATHEMATICAL BIOPHYSICS. Vol. 3, 1941, pp. 13-26. Landahl, H. D., "Studies In The Mathematical Biophysics Of Discrimination And Conditioning II: Special Case: Errors, Trials, And Number of Pos- sible Responses," BULLETIN OF MATHEMATICAL BIOPHYSICS. Vol. 3, 1941, pp. 71-77. Lenneberg, E. H., ”A Probabilistic Approach To Language Learning,” BEHA- VIORAL SCIENCE. Vol. 2, 1957, pp. l—l2. Levelt, W. J. M., "Hierarchical Chunking In Sentence Processing," PER- CEPTION AND PSYCHOPHYSICS. v61. 8, 1970, pp. 99-103. Luria, A. R., ”Speech Development And The Formation Of Mental Processes," A HANDBOOK OF CONTEMPORARY SOVIET PSYCHOLOGY. (M. Cole and I. Maltz— man, eds.), Basic Books, Inc., New York, 1969. Luria, A. R., HIGHER CORTICAL FUNCTIONS IN MAN. Basic Books, Inc., New York, 1970. MacKay, D. M., ”Quantal Aspects Of Scientific Information," PHILOSOPHY MAGAZINE. Vol. 41, 1950, pp. 289-311. MacKay, D. M., "Mindlike Behavior In Artifacts," BRITISH JOURNAL OF THE PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE. Vol. 2, 1951, pp. 105-121. MacKay, D. M., "Complementary Measures Of Scientific Information-Con- tent," METHODOS. V61. 7, 1955, pp. 63-90. MacKay, D. M., INFORMATION, MECHANISM AND MEANING. M.I.T. Press, Cam- bridge, Mass., 1969. Malic, D., ”Entropy And Philosophy," PROGRESS OF CYBERNETICS. Vol. 1, (j. Rose, ed.), Gordon and Breach, London, 1970, pp. 149-154. Marks, L. E. and Miller, G. A., "The Role Of Semantic And Syntactic Con- straints In The Memorization Of English Sentences," JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 3, 1964, pp. 1-5. 47 Mickelson, J., AN APPLICATION OF INFORMATION THEORY TO HUMAN COMMUNI- CATION BEHAVIOR. Unpublished Masters Thesis, Wichita State Uni., 1958. Miller, G. A. and Friedman, E. A., "The Reconstruction Of Mutilated English Texts," INFORMATION AND CONTROL. Vol. 1, 1957, pp. 38-55. Miller, G. A., Newman, E. B. and Friedman, E. A., "Length Frequency Statistics For Written English,” INFORMATION AND CONTROL. Vol. 1, 1958, pp. 370—389. I)“ Moss, D. E. and Neidt, C. 0., ”Applicability Of Information Theory To “ Learning," PSYCHOLOGICAL REPORTS. V01. 24, 1969, pp. 471-478. Perfetti, C. A., "Lexical Density And Phrase Structure Depth As Varia- bles In Sentence Retention," JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 8, 1969, pp. 719-724. Pollack, I., "Message Uncertainty And Message Reception III. Effect Of Restriction Of Verbal Context," JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 1, 1963, pp. 392-395. Postman, Leo, "Effects Of Word Frequency On Acquisition And Retention Under Conditions Of Free Recall Learning," QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPER— IMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY. Vol. 22, 1970, pp. 185-195. Rapoport, A., ”What Is Information," ETC. Vol. 10, 1953, pp. 247—260. Rapoport, A., "On The Application Of The Information Concept To Learn- ing Theory,” BULLETIN OF MATHEMATICAL BIOPHYSICS. Vol. 18, 1956, pp. 317-3210 Rapoport, A., "A Derivation Of A Rote Learning Curve From The Total Uncertainty Of A Task,” BULLETIN OF MATHEMATICAL BIOPHYSICS. Vol. 22, 1960, pp. 85-97. Rapoport, A., "The Perfect Learner," BULLETIN OF MATHEMATICAL BIO— PHYSICS. Vol. 23, 1961, pp. 321-335. Rosenberg, S., "The Recall Of Verbal Material Accompanying Semantically Well Integrated And Semantically Poorly Integrated Sentences,” JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. V01. 8, 1969, pp. 732—736. Rubenstein, H. and Pollack, I., "Word Predictability And Intelligibil- ity," JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 2, 1963, pp. 147-158. Shannon, C. E., "A Mathematical Theory Of Communication," BELL SYSTEM TECHNICAL JOURNAL. v61. 27, 1948, pp. 379-423, pp. 623-656. Shannon, C. E. and Weaver, N., THE MATHEMATICAL THEORY OF COMMUNICATION. University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ill., 1957. Staniland, A. C., PATTERNS OF REDUNDANCY: A PSYCHOLOGICAL STUDY. Cam- bridge University Press, Cambridge, Ill., 1957. 48 Swanson, J. M. and Wickens, D. D., "Preprocessing On The Basis Of Fre- quency Of Occurrence," QUARTERLY JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY. Vol. 19, 1969, pp. 378-383. Tannenbaum, P. H., Williams, F. and Clark, R. H., ”Effects Of Gramma- tical Information On Word Predictability," JOURNAL OF COMMUNICATION. Vol. 19, 1969, pp. 41-48. Taylor, W., "A New Tool For Measuring Readability,” JOURNALISM QUAR- TERLY. Vol. 30, 1953, pp. 415-433. Treisman, A. M., "Verbal Responses And Contextual Constraints In Lan- guage,” JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR. Vol. 4, 1965, pp. 118-128. Weinstein, R. S., Feldstein, S., and Jaffe, J., "The Correlation Of Vocal Context And Lexical Predictability," LANGUAGE AND SPEECH. Vol. 8, 1965, pp. 56-66. 49 APPENDICES APPENDIX I SEMANTIC PARTIALS APPENDIX I SEMANTIC PARTIALS Message One A. TASK R CONCEPT B. CONCEPT R UNCERTAINTY C. UNCERTAINTY R REDUCED D. REDUCED R INFORMATION E. INFORMATION R GAINED F. GAIN R PROCESS G. PROCESS R LEARNING H. LEARNING R PATTERN I. PATTERN R ORGANIZATION OF STIMULI J. ORGANIZATION R CODE SEQUENCES K. CODE SEQUENCES R SENTENCES L. SENTENCES R INFORMATION M. INFORMATION R UNCERTAINTY Messaga_Two A. COMMUNICATION R STUDY B. PEOPLE R SYMBOLS C. CODES R ORGANIZED D. SEQUENCES R SENTENCES E. INFORMATION R TRANSACTIONS F. MESSAGES R UNCERTAINTY G. SEMANTIC R AMBIGUITY H. MESSAGE RECEIVER R RESOLVE I. UNDERSTANDING R OCCURS J. MEANING R ACQUIRED K. MESSAGE CONTENTS R LEARNED L. UNCERTAINTY R REDUCTION M. COMMUNICATION R ACHIEVED 50 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message One: Organized This involves the uncertainty and its reduction by information. The states that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained. We assume that uncertainty is reduced as is according to some mental process. Generally, the takes place as is gained in the process of learning. Information is in the learning as one learns patterns. That which is gained in the process of learning is the of . In the of , patterns organize stimuli into code sequences. The learning of patterns involves the organization of stimuli into like . This patterning or of into like sentences facilitates transmission of information. Thus, the organization of stimuli into code sequences facilitate transmission and reduction. 51 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message One: Disopganized That which is gained in the process of learning is the of . Generally, the takes place as is gained in the process of learning. The learn- ing of patterns involves the organization of stimuli into like . The states that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained. Thus, the organization of stimuli into code sequences facilitates transmission and reduction. This involves the uncertainty and its reduction by information. This patterning or of into like sentences facilitates information transmission. We assume that uncertainty is reduced as is according to some mental process. In the of , patterns organize stimuli into code sequences. Information is in the learning as one learns patterns. 52 INININI APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message One: Organized With Introductory Paragraph The study of human communication draws upon concepts from a variety of different disciplines. Among these are concepts like organ- ization, memory, information, learning and so on. The task in which you are here participating is concerned with generating data to help assess the relationships between several of these concepts and uncertainty. This involves the uncertainty and its reduction by information. The states that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained. We assume that uncertainty is reduced as is_ according to some mental process. Generally, the takes place as is gained in the process of learning. Information is in the learning as one learns patterns. That which is gained in the process of learning is the of In the of , patterns organize stimuli into code sequences. The learning of patterns involves the organization of stimuli into like . This patterning or of into like sentences facilitates transmission of information. Thus, the organization of stimuli into code sequences facilitate transmission and reduction. 53 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message One: Disopganized With Introductory Paragraph The study of human communication draws upon concepts from a variety of different disciplines. Among these are concepts like organization, memory, information, learning and so on. The task in which you are here participating is concerned with generating data to help assess the relationships between several of these concepts and uncertainty. That which is gained in the process of learning is the of . Generally, the takes place as is gained in the process of learning. The learn- ing of patterns involves the organization of stimuli into like . The states that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained. Thus, the organization of stimuli into code sequences facilitates transmission and reduction. This involves the uncertainty and its reduction by information. This patterning or of into like sentences facilitates information transmission. We assume that uncertainty is reduced as is according to some mental process. In the of, , patterns organize stimuli into code sequences. Information is in the learning as one learns patterns. 54 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Messaga One: Intact This task involves the concept uncertainty and its reduction by information. The uncertainty concept states that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained. We assume that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained according to some mental process. Generally, the reduction takes place as information is gained in the process of learning. Information is gained in the learning process as one learns patterns. That which is gained in the process of learn- ing pattern is the organization of stimuli. In the process of learning, patterns organize stimuli into code sequences. The learning of patterns involves the organization of stimuli into code sequences like sentences. This patterning or organizing of stimuli into code sequences like sentences facilitates transmission of information. Thus, the organi— zation of stimuli into code sequences facilitate information trans- mission and uncertainty reduction. 55 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message One: Intact With Introductory Paragraph The study of human communication draws upon concepts from a variety of different disciplines. Among these are concepts like organization, memory, information, learning and so on. The task in which you are here participating is concerned with generating data to help assess the relationships between several of these concepts and uncertainty. This task involves the concept uncertainty and its reduction by information. The uncertainty concept states that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained. We assume that uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained according to some mental process. Generally, the reduction takes place as information is gained in the process of learning. Information is gained in the learning process as one learns patterns. That which is gained in the process of learn- ing pattern is the organization of stimuli. In the process of learn- ing, patterns organize stimuli into code sequences. The learning of patterns involves the organization of stimuli into code sequences like sentences. This patterning or organizing of stimuli into code sequences like sentences facilitates transmission of information. Thus, the organization of stimuli into code sequences facilitate information transmission and uncertainty reduction. 56 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS MessagaaOne: Test The following questions concern the paragraph you have just read: 1. As we learn , we gain the of 2. When is gained, uncertainty is . 3. Stimuli are organized into much like 4. The statement ”uncertainty will be reduced as information is gained" refers to the . 5. transmission and reduction are facili— tated by the organization of stimuli into code sequences. 6. The paragraph describes a involving a certain 7. Information transmission is facilitated by into . 8. There is some mental process involved when uncertainty is reduced and is . 9. Patterns organize stimuli into code sequences in the of . 10. In the learning , information is as one learns pattern. 57 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message Two: Organized involves the of how people use symbols or codes organized into sequences like sentences. use , codes and organized sequences or sentences in informa- tion transactions. When codes are organized in sequences like sen- tences for , the resulting messages produce uncertainty. The or used in information transactions produce uncertainty or semantic ambiguity. Thus, infor— mation transactions involve which produce and create semantic ambiguity which message receivers must resolve. Message uncertainty or semantic ambiguity must be resolved by the message receiver before can . As is resolved by the message receiver, understanding occurs and meaning is acquired. Through message receiver resolution, under- standing occurs, meaning is acquired and are . With the occurrence of understanding and by learning of message contents, uncertainty is reduced. As meaning is acquired through learning of message contents and reduction of uncertainty, is . 58 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message Two: Disorganized Message uncertainty or semantic ambiguity must be resolved by the message receiver before can . The or used in information transactions produce uncertainty or semantic ambiguity. Through message receiver resolu- tion, understanding occurs, meaning is acquired and are . use , codes of or organized sequences, or sentences in information transactions. As. meaning is acquired through learning of message contents and reduction of uncertainty, is . involves the of how people use symbols or codes organized into. sequences like sentences. With the occurrence of understanding and by learning of message contents, uncertainty is reduced. When codes are organized in sequences like sentences for , the resulting messages produce uncertainty. As is resolved by the message receiver, understanding occurs, meaning is acquired and message contents are learned. Thus, information transactions involve which produce and create semantic ambiguity which message receivers must resolve. 59 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message Two: Organized With IntroductoryaParagpaph The task in which you are here participating is concerned with generating data to help assess the relationships between several com- munication concepts and uncertainty. Among these are concepts like "code," information, learning and so on. The study of communication draws upon many of these concepts. involves the of how people use symbols or codes organized into sequences like sentences. use , codes and organized sequences or sentences in informa- tion transactions. When codes are organized in sequences like sen- tences for , the resulting messages produce uncertainty. The or used in information transactions produce uncertainty or semantic ambiguity. Thus, informa— tion transactions involve which produce and create semantic ambiguity which message receivers must resolve. Message uncertainty or semantic ambiguity must be resolved by the message receiver before can. . As is resolved by the message receiver, understanding occurs and meaning is acquired. Through message receiver resolution, under- standing occurs, meaning is acquired and are . With the occurrence of understanding and by learning of message contents, uncertainty is reduced. As meaning is acquired through learning of message contents and reduction of uncertainty, is . 60 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Messaga Two: Disorganized With Introductory_Paragpaph The task in which you are here participating is concerned with generating data to help assess the relationships between several com- munication concepts and uncertainty. Among these are concepts like ”code,” information, learning and so on. The study of communication draws upon many of these concepts. Message uncertainty or semantic ambiguity must be resolved by the message receiver before can . The or used in information transactions produce_ uncertainty or semantic ambiguity. Through message receiver resolution, understanding occurs, meaning is acquired and are . use , codes or organized a sequences, or sentences in information transactions. As meaning is acquired through learning of message contents and reduction of uncer- tainty, is . involves the of how people use symbols or codes organized into sequences like sentences. With the occurrence of understanding and by learning of message contents, uncertainty is reduced. When codes are organized in sequences like sentences for , the resulting messages produce uncertainty. As is resolved by the message receiver, understanding occurs, meaning is acquired and message contents are learned. Thus, informa- tion transactions involve which produce and create semantic ambiguity which message receivers must resolve. APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message Two: Intact With Introductory Paragraph The task in which you are here participating is.concerned with generating data to help assess the relationships between several com— munication concepts and uncertainty. Among these are concepts like "code," information, learning and so on. The study of communication draws upon many of these concepts. Communication involves the study of how people use symbols or codes organized into sequences like sentences. People use symbols, codes and organized sequences or sentences in information transactions. When codes are organized in sequences like sentences for information transactions, the resulting messages produce uncertainty. The sequences or sentences used in information transactions produce uncertainty or semantic ambiguity. Thus, information transactions involve messages which produce uncertainty and create semantic ambiguity which message receivers must resolve. Message uncertainty or semantic ambiguity must be resolved by the message receiver before understanding can occur. As semantic ambiguity is resolved by the message receiver, understand- ing occurs and meaning is acquired. Through message receiver resolu- tion, understanding occurs, meaning is acquired and message contents are learned. With the occurrence of understanding and meaning acquisi- tion by learning of message contents, uncertainty is reduced. As meaning is acquired through learning of message contents and reduction of uncertainty, communication is achieved. 63 APPENDIX II INSTRUMENTS Message Two: Test 1. can only after semantic ambiguity is resolved. 2. Uncertainty is produced by the or used 10. in information transactions. As are meaning is acquired, understanding occurs through message receiver resolution. Codes, organized sequences or sentences and are used by in information transactions. is as meaning is acquired through learn- ing or the reduction of uncertainty. The uses people make of symbols or codes organized into sequences like sentences are involved in the of . Uncertainty is reduced along with the occurrence of understanding and by learning of message contents. Messages produce uncertainty when codes are organized in sequences like sentences for . Meaning is acquired and message contents learned as is resolved by the message receiver. which produce and create semantic ambi— guity which must be resolved by message receivers are involved in information transactions. 64 IIIIIII‘I APPENDIX III PARAMETRIC ANALYSES APPENDIX III PARAMETRIC ANALYSES ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE (MODEL II): TREATMENTS BY MESSAGES Source 83 df MS F Columns (Treatments) 76.7632 5 15.3526 (5.9 )* Rows (Messages) 5.6606 1 5.6606 (2.04) Interaction 12.7612 5 2.5523 Error 347.7001 168 2.0696 TOTALS 442.8855 179 * (p <:1001) Error and interaction terms are pooled to arrive at F. PEARSON PRODUCT MOMENT CORRELATION: MESSAGE ONE TREATMENTS X SENTENCES Tl: Organized .81 .85 .76 .84 .90 T2: Disorganized .89 .94 .66 .73 T3: Organized (Intro) .90 .72 .75 T4: Disorganized (Intro) .60 .74- T5: Learning Test .86 T6: Learning Test (Intro) 65 APPENDIX III PARAMETRIC ANALYSES PEARSON PRODUCT MOMENT CORRELATION: MESSAGE TWO TREATMENTS x SENTENCES T8 T9 T10 T11 T12 T7: Organized -.03 .79 .61 .61 .60 T8: Disorganized .32 .15 -.34 -.52 T9: Organized (Intro) .83 .22 .34 T10: Disorganized (Intro) .24 .64 T11: Learning Test .62 T12: Learning Test (Intro) PEARSON PRODUCE MOMENT CORRELATION: MESSAGE ONE TREATMENTS x MESSAGE TWO TREATMENTS T7 T8 T9 T10 T11 T12 Tl: Organized -.04 T2: Disorganized -.39 T3: Organized (Intro) .01 T4: Disorganized (Intro) .44 T5: Learning Test .07 T6: Learning Test (Intro) .32 66 MICHIGAN STQTE U III“ :31 IB NIV. L RnRIES AllIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 0 66 38343 IIII 2931