I. . ~ .’ - .. «'n-M'HK‘ ‘ ' *I "*‘ 2 f t «c 2”? 1' "' “4 "I i 55., J: I .‘ ‘N K 5 , . .3. I' '90 h z a» 7; “- aacmwa-mmw nun-r it This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Responsibility of Fifth Graders Related to Authoritarian/Authoritative Parents presented by Richard W. Hill has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D . degree in Family Ecology Major professor MM Date WM 3 /7})/ MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 MSU LIBRARIES .—,_. RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in bodk drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. '91:" F sit ~_/‘ ,- \tga 5 -- 599:4 RESPONSIBILITY OF FIFTH GRADERS RELATED TO AUTHORITARIAN/AUTHORITATIVE PARENTS By Richard warren Hill A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 1981 ABSTRACT RESPONSIBILITY 0F FIFTH GRADERS RELATED TO AUTHORITARIAN/AUTHORITATIVE PARENTS By Richard warren Hill Responsible citizens seem to be a prerequisite for a democratic social system. Low levels of responsible behavior by adults and youth have often been the focus of media attention. This study sought to identify the differing effects of authoritarian and authoritative parents on a child's locus of control and the effect of locus of control on a child's level of responsibility. Forty middle income urban and rural two-parent families, each with a fifth grade child, comprised the sample population. Teachers helped evaluate responsi- bility levels of the children. An interviewer visited each home to explain the study and the instruments. The parents and the child responded individually to their respective instruments in the presence of the interviewer. The teachers completed and returned their instruments at their convenience. Parent style was determined by parent responses to 12 scale items on a seven-point range from authoritarian to authoritative. Agreement or disagreement with scale items on other instruments by the child, the parents, and the teacher determined responsibility level scores of the children. The children also completed a forced-choice instrument to identify their locus of control perceptions. Richard warren Hill Bivariate and multivariate regressions were computed to analyze relationships between parent style and locus of control and between child locus of control and responsibility levels. Limitations of sample size and data collected prohibited controlling for the many genetic and social interaction variables involved in human development. Locus of control accounted for 8.6 percent of the responsibility variance. Children with internal locus of control perceptions were judged more responsible than were children with external perceptions. Mother authoritativeness was positively related to internal locus of control in children, explaining 10.9 percent of the variance. Father authoritativeness explained another eight percent of the variance, but was negatively related. There were few, if any authoritarian parents in the sample. Nearly all scored within the authoritative range of responses. Dedicated to Mildred, my wife, for her love and patience as I worked to understand human behavior these past 41 years. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The completion of this graduate degree was made possible by the encouragement, support, and assistance of many people. Only a few of them can be acknowledged here and these statements are inadequate to express the true appreciation felt for the contributions of each person named. I received academic support throughout the graduate training from many faculty members and fellow students. My committee members were especially helpful in developing my educational program and research plan. The members included Dr. Eileen M. Earhart, committee chairperson, Department of Family and Child Ecology; Dr. Beatrice Paolucci, Department of Family and Child Ecology; Dr. Margaret M. Bulbolz, Department of Family and Child Ecology; and Dr. William A. Faunce, Sociology Department. The help of Dr. Linda Nelson and Dr. Joanne Keith, Department of Family and Child Ecology was crucial and greatly appreciated, during the final stages of writing. Periodic discussions with Dr. Ann Soderman, Project Director for the Employed Mother-Child Responsibility Pilot Study, helped to clarify distinctions between democratic and authoritative behavior of parents. Special thanks are due Mari Wilhelm, fellow student, for her time, patience, and assistance with computer procedures while busy finishing her own graduate program. Many Cooperative Extension and 4-H co-workers contributed to my graduate studies with enthusaistic interest and encouragement. Admission to the doctoral program was recommended by Dr. Norman Brown, iii 4-H Program Director, Michigan; Dr. Gordon Beckstrand, 4-H Program Director, Colorado; and Dr. William Caldwell, 4-H Program Director, Nebraska. Confidence, encouragement, and idea-sharing was always available as needed from Ray Gillespie, Associate Director of Extension, Michigan; Leah Hoopfer and Bonnie Neff, Michigan State 4-H Staff; and from Joanne Thurber-Schultink, Department of Human Environment and Design, Michigan State University. An essential contribution to the completion of this degree was the support and encouragement of our sons, our daughter, and their families, Tony; Rita; Jim; Linda; Kenneth; Scott; Ronald; Todd; Duane; Richard; and Donald. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . LIST OF FIGURES. . . . . . . . . . . . . . I: INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Need . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Conceptual Framework . . . . . . . . . Definitions . . . . . . . . . Research Questions . . . . . . . . . . . Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II: REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . Responsibility . . . . . . . . . Locus of Control . . . . . . . . . . . . Authoritarian vs. Authoritative Style Literature Summary . . . . . . . . . . Research Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . Hypothesis 1A . . . . . . . . . Hypothesis lB . . . . . . . . . Hypothesis 2 . . . . . . . . . . . Hypothesis 3A . . . . . . . . . . . Hypothesis 3B . . . . . . . . . . III: METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . Data Collection Procedures . . . . . . . Measurement Procedures . . . . . . . . . Responsibility Measure. . . . . . . Child's Self Evaluation. . . . Parent Evaluation. . . . . . Teacher Evaluation . . . . . Locus of Control Measure. . . . . Authoritarian-Authoritative Measure . Statistical Analysis . . . . . . . . . Page vii viii 0000me H \D 11 12 16 16 16 l6 17 17 17 18 20 22 22 23 23 24 29 30 34 36 45 Page IV: FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 Parent Characteristics and Child Locus of Control . . . . . . 46 Hypotheses lA.and 1B . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 Findings. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 Child Locus of Control and Responsible Behavior . . . . . . . 53 Hypothesis 2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 ’Findings. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 Authoritarian-Authoritative Parent Style and Child Responsibility . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Hypotheses 3A and 3B . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 Findings. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 Summmary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 V: SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS . . . . . . . . . . . 63 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 Conclusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 Influences on Locus of Control . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 Influences on Responsibility Development . . . . . . . . 66 Implications. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 Implications for Parents . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 Implications for 4-H and Other Youth Serving Organizations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 Implications for Further Research. . . . . . . . . . . . 73 BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 APPENDICES A. Authorization for Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80 B. Source Instruments. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 C. Instruments for this Study. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 vi Table 10 ll 12 13 LIST OF TABLES Page Child Self Evaluation of Responsibility: Frequency of Affiramative Answers to Item Statements . . . . . . . . . . 27 Parent Evaluation of Child Responsibility: Percentage of Fathers and Mothers Who Agreed with the Statements. . . . . 31 Teacher Evaluation of Child's Responsibility: Percentage of Children Judged Responsible by Teachers for Each Scale Item 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O 33 Locus of Control: Percentage of Children Who Agreed with Each of the Internally Focused Statements. . . . . . . . . . 36 Authoritativeness: Percentage of Mother and Father Disagreement with TFI Scale Items. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 Distribution of the Responses to the Scales Used in this Study 0 O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O 44 Authoritarian-Authoritative Parent Style and Child's Locus of Control: Bivariate and Multivariate Analysis. . . . . . 48 Cross Tabulation: Internal and External Locus of Control by Mbther's and Father's Authoritative Level, Combined Mother-Father Authoritative Level, Sex of Child, and Rurality of Sample. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50 The Effect of Sex of Child and Parent Style on Child's Locus of Control: Stepwise Foward Regression. . . . . . . . 52 Locus of Control and Responsibility: Bivariate Analysis. . 55 Cross Tabulation: Responsibility by Sex of Child, Place of Residence, and Locus of Control. . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Authoritarian-Authoritative Parent Style and Child Responsibility: Bivariate and Multivariate Analysis. . . . 59 The Effect of Child's Locus of Control, Parent Style, and Sex of Child on Child's Level of Responsibility: Hierarchical Multivariate Regression. . . . . . . . . . . . 62 vii LIST OF FIGURES Table Page 1 The Research Hypotheses. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l7 viii I. INTRODUCTION This study concerned the importance of responsible citizens to a democratic society. Responsible people were defined as those who are self confident, self accepting, and self directing. They participate in their social settings in individual and unique ways, but with caring concern for the well-being of others. The definition applies to social settings which include the family, the community, the school, the job, the playground, and various levels of government. Sources in the literature which support such a definition of responsibility are presented in chapter two. Responsibility and responsible behavior were considered to be synonymous terms. lies-2.22 Low levels of responsibility often draw the attention of the media. There appears to be a growing concern about inappropriate disposal of chemical wastes by industrial corporations, through ignorance or indif- ference. Examples of waste chemicals polluting ground water have been reported in many states. Newspapers often report fewer than 25 percent of the eligible voters have cast ballots on some local issue. The examples just presented suggest that adults are often less responsible than would be desirable in a democratic social system. But low levels of responsibility are not limited to adults. Media reports, during August 1980 in one rural Michigan community, noted the actions of 5,000 young people at a rock concert. Three persons were killed and six seriously injured in a highway mishap directly related to the concert. Liquor sales to minors were alleged. Neighboring residents were subjected to excessive noise and to language which they found to be obnoxious. Trespassers entered their property. In another town in that county, city council minutes in the local newspaper reported complaints about teenagers "hanging out" on the main street late at night. The result was damage to store fronts and excessive trash to be cleared away each morning at opening time. Families complained that the local park was no longer a pleasant place for them because of groups of young peOple listening to loud music. Similar examples of adult and youth behavior could likely be identified in other locations. The United States is organized so that citizens at all levels may have a voice in the decisions that are made. When people do not exercise that right, the system does not work as well as intended. The best solu- tion to a pressing problem may not have been found because the person with the most apprOpriate idea for solving the problem did not participate and present that idea. If persons with valid and convincing objections to a contemplated action do not go forward with those objections, a damaging result may follow. Responsible citizenship is required if the United States is to solve its economic, environmental, energy, and resource problems in ways that benefit the most and inconvenience the fewest numbers of people. Responsible national leaders, supported by the citizenry, are needed to help solve those same problems on a world scale. Why do some children become responsible persons while others do not? Several scholars have written about the development of responsible behavior. Fingarette (1967) said children eventually become responsible by being treated more and more as though they are responsible. Bronfenbrenner (1979) supported that view stating that development is facilitated when the developing person can experience progressively more complex interactions with significant others who encourage a gradual shift of power toward the developing person. Matteson (1975) explained that there is need for young people to work beside adults who encourage them to accept responsibility for tasks and for making decisions. The conditions described as conducive to the development of responsi- bility were more prevalent for young people of previous generations than they are today in the United States. The labor of children has become less necessary to society and the number of hours in school have been extended for them. Coleman (1974) observed that, though schools have expanded to fill much of the time that other activities once occupied for children, they do not substitute for those activities. Those earlier activities of children included more chances for responsibile action. There were situations that gave young people authority over matters which affected other pe0ple. They experienced nearly immediate consequences of their actions and were strengthened by facing them. Coleman stressed the importance of an appropriate balance between protection and opportunity for young people. Some of those laws protect from potential harm, but at the same time make it difficult for many young people to have some of the experiences which would be most bene- ficial to their growth and development. Some laws isolate young people from adult-like responsibilities. For example, the state of Michigan administers a law preventing children under sixteen years of age from performing numerous farm jobs, away from home, which were commonly done by that age group just a few years ago. Delivery truck drivers who once took their sons or daughters with them during the summer to deliver goods are now prevented from doing so by insurance regulations. In Coleman's opinion, the rights of young people to protection have been implemented to the extent that their rights to opportunity have been sacrificed. Throughout history, the continuation of society has depended upon the mixture of persons of all ages. Society is decreasing the experiences for youth to learn responsibility by almost eliminating their role rela- tions with younger children. General adult-youth segregation further reduces the chance to learn responsibility from adult models. Lipsitz (1977) suggested that, given such changes in the developmental experiences available to youth, considerable research should have focused on family and early adolescent relationships. The quantity of such research seems small, however. According to Lipsitz, the Social Research Group has estimated that close to 10 percent of all federally funded adolescent research in fiscal 1973 and 1974 involved the family, but the portion of that funding which dealt specifically with the family and the child in late childhood or early adolescence was minimal. One would expect that both biological and cognitive changes in late childhood and early adolescence should make possible deeper and broader roles as worker, friend to adult, citizen, and member of community organi- zations. Lipsitz found such roles for maturing young people have been little studied. Even more serious is the lack of opportunity for young people to practice such roles in order to develop their new physical and cognitive capacities. Crandall, Katkovsky, and Crandall (1965) found that self reSponsibility was well established by third grade, based on data from 923 third through twelfth graders. Stephens (1979) expressed concern that after children pass age ten, when they are most eager to imitate and help adults, it becomes increasingly difficult to help them develop responsible, caring attitudes toward others. Whiting and Whiting (1975) also considered care and concern for others as an aspect of responsible behavior. They suggested that unless the child learns to care for others during childhood the child may never develop such nurturant feelings. Crandall et a1., Stephens, and Whiting and Whiting support the assumption of this study that fifth graders will have already become responsible or less responsible persons. Conceptual Framework The major purpose of this pilot study was to examine the extent to which certain experiences of children and their acceptance of responsi- bility are related. Ten was chosen as the focal age for the child popula- tion because it is mid-point in the age span of eight to 12 years suggested in the literature as a significant period when children have become responsible or less responsible persons. Schools were considered an appropriate contact point for children and their families and the fifth grade a logical group. Most fifth graders are ten years old and most ten year olds are in the fifth grade. A narrow age span was thought desirable to reduce the effect of age variability. Responsible persons were considered to be self confident, self accepting, and self directing. They participate in their social settings in individual ways with care and concern for others. Since school age children spend considerable time in school, teachers were asked for evaluations to help determine responsibility levels of the children, along with parent evaluations and the child's self evaluation. The major childhood experience which was investigated was the relationship between the responsibility level of children and the tendency of their parents to be either authoritarian or authoritative persons. There is evidence (Mussen, 1960; Tallman, 1970; White and Lippitt, 1960) that authoritative family relationships are more likely to develop responsibility appropriate to a democratic society than are authoritarian relationships. A dictionary definition (American Heritage, 1976) states that an authoritarian advocates absolute obedience to authority. Baumrind (1968) described authoritative parenting in these words: The authoritative parent is supportive but not suffocating, is not afraid of making standards known, values disciplined behavior that is monitored by self control, and engenders such behavior by reasoning and explaining in verbal give-and-take with the child. In contrast, the authoritarian parent is more obedience centered and dogmatic, offers orders rather than engaging in verbal give-and-take, and is less liekly to reason with or explain to the child. (p. 255) Garbarino (1976) supports the importance of parenting authoritatively to serve as a model of behaving responsibly in a democratic social system. Teaching children to live responsibly in a democratic society is not to teach them to expect to live without authority but rather to help them learn to relate effectively with authority, whether possessing it or subject to it. Self direction or independence was a part of the definition used for responsibile behavior. Pepper (1973) agreed with that point of view by stating that if not encouraged to become an independent creative person the child may become an unthinking conformist. The authoritative parent was considered more likely than the authoritarian parent to model and encourage independence. It was considered unlikely that children with a low sense of internal control would make many attempts at independent decision making or creative thinking. Such children would not believe in their own ability to affect outcomes. Crandall et a1. (1965) stated that punishment, discipline, or rewards have little power to change behavior of a child with a strong sense of being externally controlled. An internal locus of control per- ception was considered to be a prerequisite to responsible behavior as defined for this study. Authoritative parents rather than authoritarian parents were considered more likely to permit and encourage children to make important decisions and provide honest feed-back upon which the child could build a sense of internal locus of control. Definitions A summarization of the definitions of important terms used in this study follows: Responsible behavior--Active participation in self confident, self accepting, self directing, unique ways with care and concern for others. Authoritarian parent--A parent who demands unquestioning obedience from children and tends to give orders without explanation or reasoning. Authoritativegparent--A parent who sets standards of expectations, makes them known to the child and engenders selfdmonitored disciplined behavior by reasoning and explaining in verbal give-and-take with the child. Locus of control-~One's sense of feeling able to influence outcomes (internal control) or of feeling unable to influence outcomes (external control). Research Questions The purpose of this study was to provide empirical evidence for answers to these questions: 1. Do children of more authoritative parents have a greater sense of internal locus of control than do children of more authoritarian parents? 2. Do children who have a stronger sense of internal locus of con- trol exhibit higher levels of responsibility acceptance than do children with a stronger sense of external locus of control? 3. Is there a direct relationship between the authoritative charac- teristics of parents and the level of responsibility observed in children? Overview The plan of this investigation was to identify relationships which may exist between father and mother authoritativeness, a child's sense of locus of control, and the level of responsibility acceptance by the child. In chapter two, a review of relevant literature and the research hypotheses are presented. Research methodology is presented in chapter three, covering data collection, the sample population, measurement and analysis procedures, assumptions, and limitations. II: REVIEW OF LITERATURE A definition of responsibility was presented in the opening paragraph of chapter one. Literature supporting that definition is presented first. Next, literature relevant to locus of control is examined, followed by references with a bearing on the contrast between authoritarian and authoritative parenting. The chapter is concluded with a presentation of the research hypotheses. Responsibility According to Fingarette (1967), responsibility emerges when the individual, as a matter of personal concern, accepts something society presents as worthy of concern. If the individual does not accept, does not care, and is not concerned about life as defined by the social group, that individual becomes an outsider even though skilled and intelligent. Such a person may be called amoral, perverse, shiftless, or irresponsible by others in the group. Of course, to accept the pattern of behavior offered by the group requires knowledge of what kind of behavior is expected by those with whom one interacts. Certain approved proactices must be known, accepted, and practiced to be considered responsible by one's own society. In considering the matter of care and concern, Fingarette recognized that it may not always matter who wins at bridge or tennis, but it is necessary to care about certain other outcomes. There are times when one must be held responsible, whether caring or not. A rational person must be held responsible for doing mental or physical harm to another. 10 Fingarette's reference to acceptance of life as suggested by one's social group placed responsible behavior in a cultural context. He inferred that a responsible person is one who behaves generally in ways consistent with the norms of society. His reference to being held responsible for certain outcomes added a moral dimension to responsibility. A responsible person behaves with care and concern for the well-being of others as well as self, as defined by a specific social group. There is a problem with letting Fingarette's proposition stand as a total definition of responsible behavior in a democracy. That fails to recognize need for creativity or self direction, traits of importance in such a society. A person who was a total follower might be considered a responsible person in an autocratic society, but more than following or conforming is required of citizens in a democracy. White and Lippitt (1960) reported on experiments related to democracy and autocracy. They suggested several psychological conditions that foster the development and maintenance of a democratic social system. One of those conditions was self confidence and self acceptance in initiating one's own contributions and in expressing one's own needs. Another study (Stephens, 1979) found that most responsible teens started as family helpers. They had been in responsible adult-like roles. In the Stephens' population, traditional apprentice-type training was available. Even in some of the modern homes, there were responsible jobs for children when families did their own fixing, building and repairing. Responsible jobs were also experienced by young people where the mother was very busy due to a large family, outside employment, or disability, and their help was really needed. Taken together, several 11 references (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Coleman, 1974; Fingarette, 1967; Matteson, 1975; Stephens, 1979; Whiting and Whiting, 1975) indicate that a child is stimulated to become a responsible person when that child's contributions are needed, when there is opportunity for the child to make and help make important decisions, and when there is ample time for the child to be with, imitate, and help responsible adults. Locus of Control Locus of control seemed important to this study because of the need, in a democratic society, for responsible persons to think and act individually as well as to follow orders and do what is expected by society. The definition used here for responsible behavior included a self direction component. It was considered unlikely that persons who generally felt controlled by outside forces and other people would make much effort toward self direction. It seemed inconsistent to expect a person to func- tion responsibly without having a general sense of internal control-a confidence in being able to affect outcomes. Rewards and punishment or discipline, during childhood years may have little power to change behavior if the child senses external locus of control (Crandall et al., 1965). From the view of such children, it would not seem worth it to try to change because they would not believe their changing would make any difference. Locus of control deals with both positive and negative outcomes (Lefcourt, 1966). According to Lefcourt, those who sense internal locus of control feel substantially responsible for both positive and negative results from own actions, but those sensing external locus of control feel little if any responsibility for what happens. If the result was good 12 it was a matter of luck. If the result was undesirable, fate or bad luck caused it. In a study reported by Lefcourt, the externally oriented showed a higher tendency to be conformists than did internally oriented subjects. Chandler (1980) found that mothers of the internally oriented not only allowed children more autonomy and self direction at an earlier age than did mothers of the externally oriented, but they also provide earlier intentional training. Governing parents had quite external children. Parents concerned with issues of power, those governing parents, apparently communicated a sense of powerlessness to their children, in Chandler's opinion. To summarize, Chandler found that the parents of internal locus of control children employed authoritative methods of discipline, were accepting, non-restrictive, and rewarding of independence, and they made suggestions as opposed to just giving orders. Locus of control is consistently related to a variety of personality scales, with internal scorers describing themselves as more active, independent, and effective than external scorers (Hersch and Scheibe, 1967). Crandall et al., Lefcourt, Chandler, and Gurin, Gurin, Lao, and Beattie (1969) predict generally more responsible behavior from children with internal locus of control than could be expected from externals, with the possible exception of persons who are economically disadvantaged or who are the long time victims of prejudice. Authoritarian vs. Authoritative Style One might expect that a childhood spent in a democratic family would be the best preparation for becoming a responsible citizen in a democratic l3 society. This seems, however, not to be the case. The American Heritage Dictionary (1976) defined "democratic" as a belief in social equality. A small child cannot possibly possess the experience, competency, or knowledge to make important decisions as the social equal of the adults in the family. Bashor (1976) stated that the democratic ethic was very strong but complex and that there was ample evidence to justify some exercise of parental authority. Evidence also suggested that authority and democracy were and perhaps must be in a tension relationship with one another. Baumrind (1978) listed four propositions with wide popular and scientific support to explain why childhood was not the time for pure equality with parents: 1. Children are inferior to adults in the competencies required to survive independently and therefore require special protection. 2. Children undergo successive qualitative transformations requiring commensurate changes in social status as they pass from one stage of development to the next. 3. Self-determination in adulthood is a product of maturation and not a gift bestowed by permissive caretakers. 4. Adult authority properly exercised in early years is positively related to later independence. (pp. 179-96) Baumrind suggested that when children can, alone, assume the consequences of their acts they may be trusted with the decisions leading to the acts. Smith (1977) found that acceptance of parental authority by children was strongly associated with the child's perceptions of parental expertise and benefits received from parents. The quality of the personal relation— ship between parent and child was also an important determinant of the child's acceptance of parental authority. It seems likely that these two 14 conditions would be met more often under authoritative than under authoritarian parenting styles. The Dictionary, Bashor, and Baumrind provided evidence that a parenting style which exerts more parental authority than would be possible in a purely democratic family is crucial to the development of responsi- bility in children. Smith contributes the indication that such authority is more likely to be accepted by the children if the parents are competent and on friendly, caring, accepting terms with them. Tallman (1970) studied the variables critical for effective family problem solving and hypothesized that the structure should allow for both open channels of communication and centralization of authority. In addition, an atmosphere should be provided which allows for conflict of ideas while maintaining consensus as to goals. Such a family structure requires a rather subtle distinction between authority and authoritarianism. Tallman contended that families thus structured should be able to achieve their goals, be creative, and generally foster the innovativeness necessary for adapting to a changing society. Tallman's typology was designed to identify the family with effective problem solving behavior. In such a family the children are treated as though they are responsible people but the parents do not abdicate their authority. The family described is neither democratic nor authoritarian, but authoritative. Tallman emphasized the necessity of creativity and innovativeness for a family to adept to a changing society. Creative innovative people-- the good problem solvers--are also needed to bring about necessary changes in society. Children growing up totally dependent upon authoritarian parents for direction and decisions of all kinds are not likely to advance many new ideas when social or individual changes and improvements are needed. 15 Coopersmith (1967) contrasted the authority of parents with authori- tarian parents, relative to the development of self esteem. He found that families of children with high self esteem established the most extensive sets of rules and were zealous in enforcing them. The effect was to estab- lish the authority of the parents, define the environment, and provide standards by which the child could judge competence and progress. According to Coopersmith, parents of high self esteem children had a special way of dealing with limits. Such parents were non-coercive and recognized the rights and opinions of the child. The child's views were sought, opinions were respected, and concessions granted when possible. Children entered discussions as significant participants and gained the benefits of self assertion. Self esteem is certainly important to self acceptance and independent, confident self direction. Hill (1980) discussed self direction, drawing attention to the widesprad impression that independence first becomes an issue at the time of adolescence. He said that middle class families tend to value indepen- dence from the time the young child begins to tie shoes, if not before. According to Hill, studies have shown that the most independent adolescents feel the most respect and affection for their parents. Parental attitudes which permit the child some independent action as a participating member of the group should aid the development of responsible behavior. Such parenting style is more authoritative than authoritarian. The references in this section clearly indicate that the authoritative style of parenting is more likely to develop responsible children than will authoritarian or democratic styles. Baumrind (1978) contended that parents have the obligation to provide children with genuine choices from 16 among a few good options, consistent with each child's stage of development. Literature Summary The references in chapters one and two which contributed to an under- standing of the definition of responsible behavior include Bronfenbrenner (1979), Coleman (1974), Crandall et a1. (1965), Fingarette (1967), Libsitz (1977), Matteson (1975), Stephens (1979), White and Lippitt (1960), and Whiting and Whiting (1975). References relating to the nature and importance of locus of control were Chandler (1980), Crandall et al. (1965), Gurin et a1. (1969), and Hersch and Scheibe (1967). The importance of authoritative parenting as contrasted to authori- tarian or democratic parenting was established by Bashor (1976), Baumrind (1968, 1978), Coopersmith (1967), Garbarino (1976), Hill (1980), Mussen (1960), Pepper (1973), Smith (1977), Tallman (1970), and White and Libbitt (1960). Research Hypotheses From the conceptual framework set forth, from the research questions posed, and from the review of literature, the following hypotheses were developed. The hypotheses are graphically presented in Figure l. Hypothesis 1A The more authoritative the mother, the greater is the child's sense of internal locus of control. Hypothesis 1B The more authoritative the father, the greater is the child's sense of internal locus of control. l7 Hypothesis 2 The greater the child's sense of internal locus of control, the higher is the level of responsibility observed in the child. Hypothesis 3A The more authoritative the mother, the higher is the level of responsibility observed in the child. Hypothesis 3B The more authoritative the father, the higher is the level of responsibility observed in the child. Authoritative 1A Internal 2 A Mother or .___‘L.B._,Locus of Control a) Responsible Father Child Child Authoritative 3A A Mother or 3B 9 Responsible Father Child Figure l The Research Hypotheses III: METHODOLOGY The following methodological steps have been described in this chapter: data collection, sampling procedure, a description of the sample popula- tion, limitations of the study, measurement procedures and analysis strategies. The data were gathered as part of a pilot study, entitled, "The Acceptance of Responsibility in Cildren: A Comparison of Families with Employed and Non-Employed Mothers," funded by an All University Research Initiation grant from Michigan State University.1 The major purpose of the study was to identify differences in the degree of responsible behavior in children of dual-employed and single employed, two-parent families. It was a specification of the Employed Mother-Child Responsi- bility Pilot Study that each family would have at least one fifth grade child and that half the families have both parents working and the other half would have only the father employed. There were to be 20 of each type of family in the sample population. Forty families, meeting the stated specifications were identified for the study. The directors of the Employed Mothers-Cild Responsibility Pilot Study considered it desirable to have a mix of rural and urban families in the sample population. The selection of East Lansing and Charlotte, Michigan, schools met that criterion. The fact that families in those two locations also enjoy similar economic levels was an important consideration. Two graduate students were hired for the Pilot Study to assist with the project and to interview the families. The author of this dissertation was one of those research assistants. 1All University Research Initiation, Family and Child Ecology Department, Grant #681. 18 19 The following time schedule was established and followed for the Employed Mothers-Child Responsibility Pilot Study: Design sampling procedures, select and/or develop instruments July-Sept., 1980 select and train interviewers Conduct interviews Oct.-Jan., 1981 Code and keypunch data Feb.-Mar., 1981 Analyze data Apr.-June, 1981 Five instruments were used to gather data for this study. One was completed by the mother and the father to assess the degree to which the parent was authoritarian or authoritative. Another, completed by the child, was intended to identify the child's locus of control. The child also responded to a second instrument which established a level of the child's responsibility as viewed by the child. The fourth instrument was completed by the father and the mother to obtain their individual appraisal of their child's responsibility level. The fifth instrument was the teacher's evaluation of the child's responsibility level. All data for this study were checked and recorded on code sheets by the two interviewers. Coding reliability was established by quality checking 33 percent of the data. The error rate in coding was found to be less than 0.1 percent. Frequencies were computed on each item on the five instruments and out-of-range values were found and corrected. Keypunching was done and verified by trained personnel in the computer laboratory at Michigan State University. ‘Missing data were less than 0.4 percent and were not deemed to have a significant effect in biasing the data. 20 The Sample To provide balance between rural and urban families in the study, twenty families were selected in each school district. When the fifth grade classes had been identified the names of the children were arranged in alphabetical order in each school district. A random numbers table was used to select from the lists those families which were invited to participate in the study. The remainder of the selection process differed between the two districts. In the urban district the research assistant followed the initial letter of invitation with a phone call to the family within two days of the time the famdly should have received the letter. Forty families received the letter of invitation in that district and twenty of them accepted. In the rural district 62 randomly selected families were invited by letter to participate in the study. School authorities asked that no additional contact be made until families had responded through the school or by phone to the research assistant. The school's request was honored. The first 20 families to respond became study participants. No information was obtained in either district about why some families chose to participate in the study and others did not. It was considered possible that parents with responsible children might have accepted in greater proportion than did parents of less responsible children. The teachers and the administrators in the rural district agreed to help determine if there was a significant difference between the responsi- bility levels of children in that district who were in the study and the children in families who chose not to participate. The teachers completed responsibility evaluations of the fifth grade children in all the families 21 chosen at random and invited to participate in the study. The teachers did not know which families were study participants and which had chosen not to take part. An analysis of variance found no significant difference in the responsi- bility levels of the 41 children in non-participant families and the responsibility levels of 21 study children, as evaluated by teachers. The analysis of variance produced an F value of .7197 with a probability of .4900. Forty mothers, forty fathers, forty-one fifth graders, and seven teachers comprised the sample population for this study. One family had two children in the fifth grade, though they were not twins. The parents with the two fifth graders provided individual responses for each child. The child population was comprised of 20 boys and 21 girls; 73% were 10 years old, 23.8% were eleven years old and one child was nine at the time of the visit. Two mothers and one father had not graduated from high school. High school graduation was the highest level of formal education attained by 21 percent of the mothers and 12 percent of the fathers. Seventeen percent of the fathers and 12 percent of the mothers were graduates of a four year college, and 40 percent of the fathers and 26 percent of the mothers had attended graduate school. In 1970, 23 percent of the males and 14 percent of the females aged 25 and over, lving in Ingham County, Michigan, had completed four or more years of college. The corresponding figures for Eaton County were 12 percent and 8 percent, respectively (Michigan Family Sourcebook, 1980). The school districts in this study were located in Ingham and Eaton Counties. 22 None of the fathers earned less than $15,000 annually. Twenty-four percent of the fathers earned over $30,000 per year and the remainder had an annual income between $15,000 and $30,000. The annual income of 17 percent of the mothers was between $15,000 and $30,000. The remainder of the working mothers earned less than $15,000 annually. These incomes compare to the 1975 median income of $16,631 for husband- wife families in Michigan, according to the Michigan Family Sourcebook (1980). Limitations The characteristics of the sample population should be kept in mind when evaluating the results of this study. No single-parent families were included in the sample and only fifth grade children were measured for responsibility levels. Generalization of findings are subject to the following additional limitations: 1. The sample population of 41 children was relatively small. 2. The sample was only representative of some fifth grade classes in two non-randomly selected schools within large school districts. 3. The population families had more formal education and a higher level of income than the general population in Mid Michigan. This was partly due to the exclusion of single parent households and slight over representation of dual employed parents. Data Collection Procedures An interviewer visited the home of each participant family to explain the study and the measurement instruments. The father, the mother, and the fifth grade child then completed responses to their respective instru- ments in the presence of the interviewer. Discussion between family 23 members, before the questionnaires were completed, was discouraged, but interviewers answered participant questions relative to understanding the instruments and the study. The home room teacher of each child in the study completed an instrument evluating the child's level of responsible behavior. Those instruments were delivered to the schools and picked up several days later, when completed. Measurement Procedures The data collection procedures planned for the Employed Mother- Child Responsibility Study included three questionnaires for evaluating the child's level of responsibility. One was designed for the child's self evaluation, one was for a teacher evaluation of the child, and the other was used by the mother and father to individually rate their child. Two other instruments were included to measure the authoritarian vs. authori- tative orientation of the parents and to determine locus of control for the child. Responsibility Measure It was concluded from the literature review that persons oriented toward responsible behavior in a democratic social system will possess most of the following characteristics: (a) self confidence, (b) self acceptance, (c) self direction, (d) knowledge, acceptance, and adherence to family and community norms, (e) willingness to work for changes in norms when appropriate for the well-being of the group or its members, and (f) care and concern for others. Item criteria were developed each with a direct relationship to one or more the characteristics of responsible persons, as described in 24 the previous paragraph. The criteria are presented as questions followed by parenthetic letters showing relationship to specific responsibility characteristics above. To be included as a measure of responsible behavior each instrument item, in the opinion of the author, must have shown a clear association with one or more of these criterion questions. Does the focal child: 1. Perform tasks and make decisions with confidence and self direction? (a,c) ' 2. Fulfill expectations of family and community, such as completion, orderliness, promptness, and quality of work? (d.e.f) 3. Express ideas about needed changes in family, school and community norms? (a through f) 4. Participate in family, school, and other group decisions? (a,b,csd) 5. Respond to the needs of others? (a,b,c,e) 6. Persist in assigned or assumed tasks? (a,d) 7. Accept consequences of own actions? (a,b,c,d,f) Child's Self Evaluation. The Social Attitude Scale (SAS) developed by Dale B. Harris, Institute of Child Welfare, University of Minnesota, was used for the child's self evaluation. Harris (1957) said the SAS was intended to discriminate children who have a reputation among their peers for responsibility as contrasted with children who have little reputation for responsibility. According to Harris (1980) the SAS was a carefully developed self evaluation instrument, shown to be the best among several personality test predictors of post high school performance. The scale was substantially correlated with other measures of personal and social adjustment. Each item on the scale was justified as reflecting 25 behavior classified as reliable, accountable, loyal, or as doing an effective job. According to Harris, evaluations of peers and teachers relative to four behavior classifications were used to provide a criterion for the scale. Classes composed of 227 boys and 228 girls and their teachers were asked to nominate the three boys and the three girls who were best described by each of the following statements: (Each child and each teacher made nominations.) 1. This person can be depended upon. When a promise is given to do something, you know it will be done. Work is well done and this person can be trusted. 2. This person is a square shooter and does not take advantage of or cheat others. When something has been done wrong, this person will admit it and not try to blame others. 3. This person thinks for the good of others, not always for self, and is loyal to the group. 4. This person is one who gets things done. On a class project, on a committee, or on a work job, this person gets right to work and can be counted on to do it well and promptly. An 89-item scale was administered to those same school classes a day or two after they made their nominations. Scores on the SAS were related to scores on a tally for each of the four characteristics resulting from the nomination procedure. The number of times a given child was nominated for one of the behavior characteristics was his/her score on that trait. Only a very few children received no nominations but another few received the majority of the votes cast. No one behavior characteristic appeared to be more closely related than another to the sum of the scores of the remaining three, so a total score obtained by summing the four trait scores appeared justified. 26 Criteria for selecting the items most associated with responsibility to be included in the final form of the SAS were determined as described below. Twenty-five boys and 25 girls scoring highest in the sum of the four characteristics were selected from the total population tested. An equal number of boys and girls were selected from the unnominated or low scoring children to constitute a criterion sample of less responsible children. Using these criteria, 50 items were retained for the SAS from the original 89. A copy of the 50 item SAS is in Appendix B. For the Employed Mother-Child Responsibility Pilot Study, some items were re—worded and three items were added to the scale. Of the 53 items, 46 met the criteria for the responsibility measures of this study. The child self evaluation instrument with the items used for this study identified is included in Appendix C. Less than 25 percent of the children selected a low responsibility response to the following scale items; (1) I get in trouble in school, (2) I've had trouble with the law or police, (3) If I received too much change, I'd return it, (4) I can go on working when the teacher leaves the room, (5) I miss school for no good reason, and (6) Cheating is alright if you don't get caught. Over 75 percent of the children gave the responsible answer to these scale items: (1) I have been sent to the principal for being bad, (2) I waste time, (3) I am.concerned when others are treated badly, (4) Honesty is the most important thing, (5) I am on time for meals, (6) If everyone pitches in to help, it can be done, and (7) I like to organize things. Frequencies of the agreement responses to each of the 46 items on the child self evaluation responsibility measure are shown in Table l. 27 TABLE 1 Child Self Evaluation of Responsibility: Frequency of the Affirmative Answers to Item Statements. Scale Items Fgequigizig If everyone pitches in to help, it can be done. 98 (40) Honesty is the most important thing. 93 (38) One should care for parents when they are old. 93 (38) I'd know what to do if home alone during a tornado watch. 90 (37) I do the best I know how. 85 (35) I am on time for meals 83 (34) I can go on working when teacher leaves room. 83 (34) If I received too much change, I'd return it. 83 (34) I'm concerned when others are treated badly. 83 (34) It's important to do necessary things before fun things. 81 (33) I feel bad when I must disappoint others. 78 (32) If lost in strange city, I could find help. 78 (32) I like to organize things. 76 (31) Every person should help his town or city. 73 (30) Good citizens should vote. 73 (30) If a friend were injured I'd know what to do. 68 (28) I have regular jobs around the house. 68 (28) I'm capable of staying home alone for hours. 66 (27) Can find things to do if teacher doesn't. 66 (27) People can rely on me. 66 (27) I volunteer for special projects at school. 63 (26) I get up with alarm clock or when called once. 61 (25) I stick to a job till it's finished. 61 (25) I'm concerned about current events. 59 (24) Parents are satisfied with way I spend money. 54 (22) Policemen are helpful. 51 (21) I work out my own problems without help. 41 (17) 28 TABLE 1 (cont'd.). Scale Items Fgequen§i2:) I finish what I start. 39 (16) *Do something for your neighbor only if he does something for you. 34 (14) *Parents must remind me to put things away. 34 (14) *I must be reminded to keep clean. 31 (13) *I have trouble getting school work done on time. 27 (11) I've been elected leader or captain. 24 (10) *I let others plan when working in a group. 22 ( 9) I've been chosen room helper or for errands. 22 ( 9) *If you don't like to do it, you'll get someone else to do it if you are smart. 12 ( 5) *Our country would be better off if there were no elections and no one had to vote. 12 ( 5) *I would let a friend down. 10 ( 4) *I miss school for no good reason. 7 ( 3) *Teacher complains I don't finish my work. 7 ( 3) *I get in trouble in school. 5 ( 2) *I've had trouble with the law or police. 2 ( l) *I waste time. 2 ( l) *I am often late for school. 00 (00) *I have been sent to the principal for being bad. 00 (00) *Cheating is alright if you don't get caught. 00 (00) 1"Always" and "Often" were combined as the affirmative response to compute the frequencies of unstarred items. *These items were reverse scored for the responsibility scale. "Sometimes" and "Never" were considered the responsible answers for these items. 29 The original value range of 4 to 184 for this scale was reduced to a range of 0 to 20 by simple prOportion. The transformation was necessary in order to give this scale equal weighting with the three other scales which comprise the responsibility variable for this study. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for this scale was found to be .833. The mean was 13.99 and the standard deviation was 1.79. Parent Evaluation. The parent questionnaire for evaluating the child's responsibility was developed for the Employed Mother-Child Responsibility Pilot Study. The instrument contained 44 items designed to elicit parents' expectations and attitudes about task allocation to and completion by the focal child. The father and the mother were to respond to the scale individually by agreeing or disagreeing with each of the 44 statements. Parents were instructed by the interviewers to make their responses with only the focal child in mind, not another child or all of their children if there were others in the family. Twenty of the scale items on this parent questionnaire were considered to fulfill the criteria established for measuring the child's level of responsibility. the instrument for parent use in evaluating the child's responsibility level, with the appropriate 20 items identified, is included in Appendix C. Over 95 percent of the mothers and fathers said their child does not find excuses to stay home from school, is a caring person, and if home alone in a tornado would know what to do. About 70 percent of the mothers and fathers said they do not have to remind their child to complete household tasks. There were only four items on which there was a difference of ten percent or more between aggregate percent of father 30 and mother responses. The items included: (1) I must be continually after my child to finish things, (2) My child is more mature than other children the same age, (3) It is hard for my child to find something to do when adults do not suggest, and (4) If lost in a strange city my child could find help. A complete summary of the parent responses to the 20 items on the parent evaluation scale is presented in Table 2. The value range for this instrument was 0 to 20. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for this scale was .562 for father responses and .640 for mother responses. The means were 15.88 for fathers and 16.56 for mothers. Standard deviations were 3.11 for fathers and 3.55 for mothers. Teacher Evaluation. The third scale used to measure child responsi- bility was the Minnesota Personality Profile II, developed by the Univer- sity of Minnesota Institute of Child Development and Welfare. The measure is a teacher rating scale consisting of a ten item Likert type design. It has a five-point designation of behavior for each item in general adjustment, realism, persistence, responsibility, attentiveness, dependency, flexibility, calmness, sensitivity, and compliance. A copy of the instru- ment is included in Appendix B. Johnson and Bommarito (1976) cite the wide use of the instrument for a diversity of predictive purposes with various sample populations. According to Harris (1980) the score weights at the five positions in each scale item were arrived at by consensus of six judges as representing desirability from a general functional mental health viewpoint. The scoring weights represent an approximation to a simple ordinal scale of 31 TABLE 2 Parent Evaluation of Child Responsibility: Percentage of Fathers and Mothers Who Agreed with the Statements. Scale Items Fathers Mothers Z (N-41) Z (N=41) Child is a caring person. 95 (39) 98 (40) If home alone during tornado watch, child would know what to do. 100 (41) 98 (40) Child can prepare own meal. 90 (37) 95 (39) If lost in strange city, child could find help. 88 (36) 95 (39) Child is effective in solving own problems. 88 (36) 90 (37) If friend were injured, child would know what to do. 90 (37) 90 (37) Child usually finishes what starts. 88 (36) 88 (36) Child can stay alone for several hours. 88 (36) 85 (35) Child regularly participates in extra- curricular activities. 83 (34) 85 (35) Child is more mature than others same age. 54 (22) 68 (28) *I must remind my child to finish household tasks. 68 (29) 64 (25) Child gets up without being called. 51 (21) 51 (21) *I must be continually after my child to finish things. 34 (14) 51 (21) *Child wastes a lot of time. 21 ( 9) 24 (10) *Child is occasionally in trouble at school. 19 ( 8) l7 ( 7) *Child forgets to give me phone notes. 22 ( 9) 15 ( 6) *It's hard for child to find something to do if adults don't suggest. 2 ( l) 15 ( 6) *Getting to meals on time is hard for my child. 7 ( 3) 10 ( 4) *I must usually call my child several times to get up for school. 14 ( 6) 10 ( 4) *My child often finds excuses to stay home from school. 2 ( l) 2 ( l). *These items were reverse scored for the responsibility scale. Disagree- ment was the response indicating the more responsible behavior. 32 psychological desirability. Though no normative data were established, the ratings did correlate substantially with post-high school performance adjustment, according to Harris. Five was the desirable answer for each item. For the Employed Mother-Child Responsibility Pilot Study, five scale items were added to this instrument. Of the final 15 items on the scale, 11 were considered as meeting the criteria established for measuring the child's level of responsibility for this dissertation research. The teachers' evaluation instrument, with the 11 selected items identified, may be found in Appendix C. The teachers indicated that over 75 percent of the children in the sample are sensitive individuals, they will go out of their way to please, and are compliant enough to agree to sensible requests. Teachers rated less than 45 percent of the children as high in leadership or extra-curricular activities. From 60 to 70 percent of the children were rated as responsible on the remaining scale items. Teacher responses are summarized in Table 3. The original value range for the responses to this scale was 11 to 55. The range was transformed by simple proportion to 0 to 20 to provide equal weighting when combined with the other three child respon- sibility scales. The teacher responses were found to have a mean of 13.78 and a standard deviation of 4.09. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for the teacher scale was .900. It was determined that a combination of the four measures of the child's responsibility level would be taken as the responsibility variable of the child for statistical procedures. The rationale for that decision 33 TABLE 3 Teacher Evaluations of Child's Responsibility: Percentage of Children Judged Responsible by Teachers for each Scale Item. Scale Items Fgeque§EZE§ Compliant 83 (34) Sensitive/insensitive 78 (32) Accepts responsibility for own actions 68 (28) Considerate of classmates 68 (28) Dependent 68 (28) Persistent 68 (28) Reliability 68 (28) Attentive at school 64 (26) Frequency of task completion 61 (25) Extra-curricular activities 41 (17) Leadership 24 (10) 1Teacher responses 4 and 5 on the five point scale were combined to compute the above frequencies. was that each evaluator (child, father, mother and teacher) sees the child from different perspectives and that all observations together reflect a more composite view of the child than any of the separate viewpoints. The combined child responsibility scale, the result of adding the evaluations from the child, the father, the mother, and the teacher, was found to have a Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient of .884. The value range was 0 to 80 with a mean of 60.10 and a standard deviation of 9.37. 34 Locus of Control Measure The review of literature suggested that in order to behave responsibly in a democratic social system one would need some sense of being able to influence outcomes. It seemed reasonable to expect that children would need to gain a sense of internal locus of control before they would be willing to make many decisions, present new ideas, or accept the conse- quences of their actions which are all important aspects of responsibility. Rotter's I-E scale was chosen to measure the child's sense of locus of control. Rotter's 29-item, forced choice I-E instrument (Appendix B) was produced after several revisions based on item analysis, social desir- ability controls, and studies of discriminant validity (Robinson and Shaver, 1973). The item-total correlations were based on data from 200 male and 200 female elementary psychology students. A Kuder- Richardson internal consistency analysis yielded r-.70 for both males and females. A sub-group of that population produced a test-retest reliability coefficient after one month of r-.72 (N-60). Another sub-group yielded a coefficienc of 48.55 after two months (N-ll7), according to Robinson and Shaver. Hersch and Scheibe (1967) found Rotter's I-E scale consis- tently related to a variety of personality scales. During the pre-test of the instrument, fifth grade children seemed to understand most of the scale items even though it was not designed for their age group. However, it seemed too long for them and they tired of it well before they had finished. The scale was therefore reduced to 13 items (Appendix C). The reduction was based on the item correlations shown in Robinson and Shaver. Items with 35 highest correlations were used. Anastasi (1968) explained that rejecting items with low correlations increases the homogeniety of a scale but narrows the criterion coverage. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for this thirteen item scale and this sample population was .318. The reliability analysis revealed three items with extremely low or negative item-total correlations. Those three items were removed from the locus of control scale. The remaining ten-item scale was found to have a reliability coefficient of .395. Although a reliability coefficient of .40 is often considered a minimum acceptable statistical level, the ten-item scale was retained in this pilot study on conceptual grounds. The range of values for the locus of control scale was 0 to 10. The mean response was 6.85 and the standard deviation was 2.14. Over 75 percent of the children in this sample agreed with the following scale items, indicating an internal perception of locus of control: (1) Misfortune results from one's own mistakes, (2) People get the respect they deserve, (3) Success is a matter of hard work, not luck, (4) Getting others to do something depends on ability, not luck, and (5) The number of friends you have depends on how nice a person you are. Less than 50 percent of the sample agreed with: (1) When I make plans, I'm certain they will work and (2) I don't believe luck or chance has a large role in my life. A summary of all the agreement responses to items indicating an internal perception of locus of control is shown in Table 4. 36 TABLE 4 Locus of Control: Percentage of Children Who Agreed with Each of the Internally Focused Statements. Frequencies Internally Focused Scale Items Z (N841) Getting others to do something depends on ability, not luck. 93 (38) The number of friends you have depends on how nice you are. 83 (34) Misfortune results from one's own mistakes. 78 (32) Success is a matter of hard work--not luck. 78 (32) People get the respect they deserve. 76 (31) Getting what I want is not a matter of luck. 73 (30) When capable persons don't become leaders, it is because they missed opportunities. 59 (24) By being active in political affairs, citizens can control world events. 54 (22) When I make plans I'm certain they'll work. 42 (17) I don't believe luck or chance has a large role in my life. 29 (12) Authoritarian-Authoritative Measure The literature review supported the concept of identifying the attitude of parents along a continuum from authoritarian to authoritative as a variable related to a child's locus of control and responsibility level. There was evidence to indicate that one should expect authoritative parents to relate to responsible children through influence on the child toward an internal sense of locus of control. Based on the literature review it seemed apparent that authoritative parents would be expected to exhibit the following characteristics: 37 (a) confidence in and open-mindedness toward influence from others, (b) fairness about equal rights, opportunities, and responsibilities, (c) friendliness and goodwill in attitude and action toward others, (d) openness toward individual autonomy, (e) acceptance of group norms as necessary to the maintenance of any group, and (f) respect for rules, regulations, and authority figures established within the framework of the five preceding characteristics. Item criteria were established, each of which, related directly to one or more of the characteristics of authoritative parents as described in the previous paragraph. Letters in parentheses following each criterion indicate one or more of the above characteristics with which it relates. Instrument items were sought which would indicate the degree to which: 1. Each family member is encouraged and expected to have a voice in family decisions, solving family problems, and in performing family tasks. (a,b,c,d) 2. The children in the family are helped to understand the reason behind family rules, regulations, and values. (b,c,d,e) 3. Famdly norms encourage flexibility and self direction among family members. (a,b,d) 4. There is opportunity for consensus among family members about role allocation within the family. (a,b,d,f) The scale chosen for measuring parents' authoritarian-authoritative orientation was the Traditional Family Ideology (TFI) Scale by Levinson and Huffman (1955). The scale was designed to assess differences in family ideology along an autocratic-democratic continuum. It appears from the literature review and an examination of the TFI scale that the term "authoritarian-authoritative" can be substituted for "autocratic- democratic" with reference to the scale, without negative consequences. 38 In fact, the former term is considered more precise and is used in the remainder of this study when referring to the TFI scale. The scale is based on five personality factors: conventionalism, authoritarian submission, exaggerated masculinity and femininity, extreme emphasis on discipline, and a moralistic rejection of impulse life. The authoritarian extreme of the continuum is characterized by hierarchical conceptions of family relationships, discipline in child-rearing, and sharp dichotomization of sex roles. Characteristics of the authoritative extreme of the continuum include decentralization of authority, greater equality in husband-wife and parent-child relationships, and increased individual self-determination. The TFI scale is essentially an application of ideas gained from clinical interviews dealing with childhood recollections and family relationships. Each scale item was kept simple, casual, and chosen to represent as many as possible of the five aspects of personality identified earlier (Levinson and Huffman). The scale was composed of 40 items. The (corrected) split-half reliability for the TFI scale was .84 for a sample of 109 adults. The sample was composed of 67 men and 42 women, aged 20 to 40, enrolled in psychology classes at Cleveland College. On four projective questions concerning various family roles and practices, the responses of the lowest and the highest scoring quarters on the TFI were compared. The results indicated that the categories which differ- entiate high and low scorers reflect the variables on which the TFI was originally constructed. An abbreviated twelve-item form of the TFI scale was presented to five groups (total of 507 subjects) in Boston. The groups tested 39 included Harvard Summer Session, Boston University sophomores and freshmen, registered nurses, and student nurses. The Harvard summer group had a split-half reliability on the initial test of .92 and a sixdweek test- retest reliability of .93. The Levinson and Huffman hypothesis that individuals are relatively consistent in their tendency to take an authoritative or an authoritarian stand in various ideological spheres is supported by significant correlation with other scales including Religious Conventionalism. The twelve items were selected from the 40 item scale according to discriminatory power and simplicity of each item, and considering the broadness of over all item content. The abbreviated version yielded about the same results as the 40-item original instrument, according to Levenson and Huffman. After a careful examination of the twelve-item TFI, it was concluded that each of the twelve items met one or more of the criteria established in this study for discriminating authoritarian-authoritative parents. It was accepted as the total measure of that variable, and is included in Appendix C. Each parent was asked to check the scale individually without conferring with the other. Their responses were on a seven-point range from "very strongly agree" to "very strongly disagree." As scale items were stated, agreement was an authoritarian position and disagreement was a more authoritative answer. "Very strongly agree" was coded l and "very strongly disagree" was coded 7, making the higher score the more authoritative position. On only one item the majority of the parents agreed with the more authoritarian position: "The Family is a divinely ordained sacred 40 institution." On all other items at least half of the parents supported an authoritative position. On the following four scale items more than 75 percent of the mothers and fathers evidenced an authoritative position: if children are told too much about sex they will experiment; a man should not be expected to have respect for a woman if they have had pre-marital sexual relations; it is unnatural for women to have authority over men, and if a child is unusual in any way parents should try to make the child become more like other children. The mother and father responses to the authoritarian-authoritative scale are summarized in Table 5. Overall, in aggregate percentages, the fathers scored more towards the authoritarian end of the continuum and the mothers more toward the authoritative end. However, there were no items in which the percentages exceeded a difference of more than ten percent between mothers and fathers. This would suggest that most of the parents in the sample population tended to be more authoritative than authoritarian. This is supported when examining the means and standard deviations as described below. The value range for this scale was 12 to 84. The reliability coefficient for the scale was .856 for fathers and .787 for mothers. The mean score for fathers was 62.22 with a standard deviation of 14.22. For mothers the mean score was 63.65 with a standard deviation of 12.47. A score of 48 would be the theoretical neutral point between the two ends of the scale continuum. All scores within a range of one standard deviation on either side of the mean scores for both fathers and mothers were on the authoritative side of the mid-point of the scale. The frequency distribution for this and all the other scales used in this study are summarized in Table 6. PLEASE NOTE: Pages 41 and 42 lacking in number only. Text follows. Filmed as received. UNIVERSITY MICROFILMS. 43 TABLE 5 Authoritativeness: Percentage of Mother and Father Disagreement with TFI Scale Items. Frequencies Scale Items Fathers Mothers 7. (NI-41) Z (Na41) If child is unusual, parents should try to make the child more like others. 93 (38) 95 (39) If children are told too much about sex they will experiment. 78 (32) 85 (35) Man should not be expected to have respect for a woman if they've had pre-marital sexual relations. 78 (32) 83 (34) It's unnatural for women to have authority over men. 76 (31) 78 (32) Determination and drive are the most important male qualities. 68 (28) 76 (31) Mother has failed if children are messy or rowdy. 66 (27) 76 (31) Husbands should have main say-so. 56 (23) 66 (27) Don't let child talk back to parents, or they'll lose respect for parents. 66 (27) 61 (25) Women don't understand wife role if they object to "obey" in marriage ceremony. 56 (23) 61 (25) There's nothing lower than one who does not feel love, gratitude, and respect for parents. 63 (26) 54 (22) Sex and crime facts show need to crack down on children to save moral standards. 56 (23) 51 (21) Family is a divinely ordained sacred institution. 24 (10) 15 ( 6) 1 II I! I "Very strongly disagree, strongly disagree,‘ and "disagree" were combined to compute the above frequencies. 44 m¢.~H mo.mo mniow quNH mom. Magoo: ~N.¢H ~N.~o Hmimu «mima cam. nonumm «mahum o>wuouauosus< .omwumuauozu:< ¢H.N mm.o CHIN oHIo mmm. Houucou mo mdooq mm.m oa.oo mnimm omlo «mm. confinaou mo.c m~.ma main omlo Goa. Hosanna mm.m om.oH omioa ouio oeo. nonuoz Ha.m mm.mH ONIw omlo New. nonumm mn.H mm.ma wHINH culo mmw. vHHAU "hufiafinwmsoamom coauofi>on and: owamm omomm unoaoammmoo meadow wumvcmum Hmauo< ofinfimmom hufiafinowaom .hcsum menu aw pom: meadow dam on momaoamom onu mo :ofiuanauuman o mqmdh 45 Statistical Analysis Analysis was done on the CDC 6500 Computer at Michigan State University, using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. Bivariate regression was selected for analysis of the relationship between child responsibility and locus of control, to determine if the children with a strong sense of internal control exhibited higher levels of responsibility acceptance than did the children with a more external sense of control. Bivariate regression was also used to analyze the relationship between locus of control and style of parent, to learn if children of more authoritative parents felt a greater sense of internal locus of control than did the children of the more authoritarian parents in the sample. Step-wise multiple regression was used to compare the influence upon child responsibility from the child's sense of locus of control with influence from parent style. The purpose here was to learn if there was any direct relationship between the authoritarian-authoritative style of parents and the level of responsibility observed in their children. IV. FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION Bivariate and multivariate regression analyses were employed to test the hypotheses. The findings from these procedures have been reported in this chapter in three major sections; parent characteristics and the child's sense of locus of control, child sense of locus of control and child responsibility, and parent characteristics and child responsi- bility. The final section is a composite regression analysis examining, simultaneously, the relationships found to be significant in the three hypotheses. Parent Characteristics and Child Locus of Control Of particular interest to this study was the child's sense of locus of control. Internal locus of control was considered basic to responsible action in a democratic social system. It was considered likely that the nature of the parent child interactions was an important influence on the development of a sense of locus of control within the child.‘ The specific interest in this research was to examine if a child's locus of control might be affected by the authoritarian or authoritative interaction of the parents. Hypotheses 1A and 18 1A - The more authoritative the mother the greater is the child's sense of internal locus of control. 1B - The more authoritative the father the greater is the child's sense of internal locus of control. 46 47 Findings. Hypothesis 1A was supported by bivariate regression analysis. Authoritativeness of the mother was regressed on locus of control and was found to be significant at the probability level of .037. Hypothesis 13 was not supported. The fathers' scores, when regressed on locus of control, were not significant with a probability of .692. The zero order correlation was -.064. Less than one percent of the variance was explained by this relationship. Multivariate analysis was carried out to determine the combined effect of mother's and father's level of authoritativeness. A stepwise forward regression procedure was used. Mother and father scores were regressed on locus of control. Mother scores entered the equation on step one with a beta weight of .507 and an overall significance of .037, explaining 10.9 percent of the variance. Father scores entered the equation on step two with a -.333 beta weight and an overall significance of .021, explaining an additional eight percent of the variance. Both variables together explained 18.9 percent of the variance. The multir variate analysis supports the findings of the authoritative mother as being positively related to the development of locus of control in children this age. In addition this analysis demonstrated that the father's level of authoritativeness, after taking into account the mother's score, contributed significantly to the child's locus of control but not in the direction hypothesized. These findings are summarized in Table 7. Discussion. Further examination of the data was carried out to clarify understanding of these relationships. Fifty-one percent of all children in the study scored above the mean for locus of control. 48 Hmo. Nm.c and. mmq. «no. no.m mmm.I umsumm .N amum mmo. mc.q moa. Hmm. nmo. no.c mom. ensue: .H mmum Houuaou mo mauoq do vommouwom moamum ucmumm "oumwum>auaaz Houuaoo mo moooa Nmo. mma. I «co. «co. Nae. mma. I «oo.l so vmmmmuwom mahum nosumm Houuaoo mo maooa mmo. no.q moa. Hmm. mmo. mo.q Hmm. so vommouwom mahum nosuoz "mumwum>am m Ham m m>oaom .mHm Inm>o Nu .fiuaaz .me Iumuom mumm mwmhama< cu m .mfimhama< oumHHm>HuH=z can mumwuo>am "Houucoo mo maooq m.cHHno can mahum uaoumm o>fiumufiu05u5HuMuauonus< m.uonumm .m swam mac. muoum m>fiumufiuonus< m. “ma—HOS .N amum mas. xmm m.uaaao .H scum “Houuaoo «0 msoog w.cafinu so vommouwom mamum uaoumm mom vaano mo xom .mam m Han Iuo>o u .uHDS o>oaom Iumuom cu m wuom mfimhamn< "Houuooo «0 @5004 .GOfimmouwmm vumshom omfisnmum m.paasu no oahum unnumm m mqm<9 mam vawno to sow mo “queen was 53 that the authoritativeness of fathers has a significant influence on the child's locus of control but toward an external perception. Authoritative- ness of the parents explained 18.9 percent of the child's locus of control variance when sex of the child was not controlled. In considering these findings, it is necessary to remember that only seven of the 80 parents in the sample scored in the authoritarian half of the value range on the authoritarianlauthoritative scale, and most of those were near mid-range. Nearly all parents in the sample exhibited some level of authoritativeness. The number of authoritarian parents was too small to make conclusions about strongly authoritarian parents. Child Locus of Control and Responsible Behavior It is difficult to conceptualize a person who behaves responsibly in a democratic social system, according to the definition for responsi- bility, without a substantial sense of internal locus of control. The objective here was to look for evidence that responsible behavior is significantly related to internal locus of control in fifth grade children. The literature suggested that orientation toward responsible behavior was well established by age eight or nine (Gurin et al., 1969). Hypothesis 2 The greater the child's sense of internal locus of control, the higher is the level of responsible behavior observed in the child. Findings. Hypothesis 2 was supported by bivariate regression analysis. Locus of control regressed on the child's responsibility level was found to be significant at a probability level of .067. The value 54 used for the child's responsibility level included evaluations from the child, the teacher, the mother, and the father. Hypothesis 2 was also supported by bivariate analysis when locus of control we regressed on the responsibility evaluations from the child and from the teacher. Significance at the probability level of .016 was found for the child's self evaluation and at a probability of .038 for the teacher's evaluation. Hypothesis 2 was not supported by bivariate analysis when locus of control was regressed on the father or the mother evaluations of the child's responsibility. Significance was at a probability level of .298 for the father score and at .691 for the mother score. The zero order correlations were .166 and .064 respectively. These analyses are summarized in Table 10. Discussion. Empirical evidence supports the concept of a relation- ship between locus of control and responsibility, when the responsibility measure includes evaluations from the child, the teacher, the father, and the mother. According to the evidence, children with a greater sense of internal locus of control were evaluated as being more responsible than the children with a more external locus of control perception. When subscales of the child responsibility measure were used in the analysis differing results were obtained. It was found that children evidencing internal locus of control were more responsible than those evidencing external locus of control, according to the child and the teacher evaluations. Neither parent appeared to evaluate the internally controlled child as being significantly more responsible than the externally controlled child. 55 Hmo. cod. coo. «no. Hmo. oea. coo. ouoom ensue: mam. HHH.H mac. nee. mam. HHH.H sea. muoum assume mmo. qmo.e ooH. own. mmo. emo.e own. muoom nonomma oao. emm.o oea. «hm. oHo. mmm.e cum. ouoom vafieu moo. ecm.m one. man. moo. eon.m mam. ouoom Hence I suaaapameoammm cease so vommouwmm Houuooo no moqu “ouo«Hm>Hm m Ham m meosom .wam sua>o u .uaaz .mam nuance moan memaaaa< N on a .mamhfima< oumfiuo>wm "huaawnwmaoamom can Houuooo mo mnoog o~ mdmwuaaz “we. mas. moo. moo. “we. mas. maa.u “cause new. oqo. .Hoo. Nmo. mew. oeo. Nmo.- tango: huaawnfiwaoamom so commouwom maaum uamuom "mum«Hm>Hm m Has a m>oamm .wam Ium>o Nu .uH:z .wfim Iuousm moon manuamo< cu m .mwmhaoo< museum>aufioz can oumaHm>Hm "huqafinfimooomom vaano van mahum unmumm m>Humuwuonusosmm .wfim Ium>o Nu .uanz .me Iuouam doom mwmhaoo< cu m .:0flmmmuwom ouoaum>fiuanz Hmoa£oumumwm ”huwawnwmoonmom mo Ho>oq m.na«£u do cameo no now new uaaum bemoan .Houuaou mo «soon m.eaaeo mo Hummus one ma manHuoouum o>Huomuum m>Huomuum o>HuoouumaH onu we once huo> mHHmomD hHoumuoooz on ou momma m>Huo0uumoH No: mH o>Huomuum so: .m H N m c m moon? mnu pH mH xoon moon on umna on owns muonuo may moon mHnHHm momsuxo you muHHHnHm moamHn mHouHoHumn ou moHuH moaHuoaom moxma.aooHom uncanny momma Nmooo on amps How huHHHnHmoOQQou oxmu o: moon HHms 30: .c m n e N H oaom a: m>Hw mownuhom ImHnoouu hhm> EH: oxma oHnoouu oHuuHH mo manm pH mH uH can: no“ ou an m o no: on hHHmmo as o>Hw u.aos o co monum ouHso moxma saga a: mo>Hu huo> do mm>Ho Nxmmu m on umHmumd on moon HHos 30: .m H N m c m NHomaHn pH on owns moons ouoom NHomaHn uponm muoHoa poow no dynamo: gnu 30:: cu oHumHHmou oHumHHmou pom muHamm hHmuoHnaoo Boom u.omoon amazoaom hHuHmm :30 mHn macaw No: mH UHumHHmou to: .N n e m N H uooHHooxm poou ommuo>< “Hum uoom umH uooaumofiom Hmuoomw m.pHH:o mHnu .oOHono 55 GH .H .pHHao man no onumu “30% co noncommunoo umnu manage no puos osu «0 Hanson onu muHuB .amuH some mo ummH on» up ooHH «so so um=0HuusuumoH muommoafiz mo huHmuo>Hoa mummHmz aHaao no musuaumaH HH mHHwoum huHHmnomuom 88 m mmanHOQ imHadm .ommoHa ou msOonm 008 H own: hHHmmm huo> .hnosoa momma «HnamemeH .eawam H suHsonmaa ammuanHm on den oxomm a mmmmHa ou hma mH: mo uoo @000 m uH oxmu u.:mu mmaHu Imaom .umom moo>umz mmmvH so: ou unmom cu BOHm N m mummoomu oHnHmawm ou moouw< m amnu mHouuoou use monHomm mm: s unmaouHuxo umuwm oaoo moHuuom m mumSuo muomm Ion mGOHummm Imam oHn IHmomm moxma a nHm: mxomm omnu uooooomoo .umuam HHme :30 mom: H m>HumHmou .mumuu loco omumo N oHOu mH on mesa mo muHmonno moon No: mH uanHaaoo 30: N nus: ou ppm: .m>HuHmommoH e mocowwo moHnuoa .nwooa N:uH oxmu: on one HHoa so: m VONmHmm N m>Hmmmm aw; Mfl QmNU um £035 30: q =oenu was suns shape: cu momma H oooooanoH Ho ooH mHHmmm hum> aeHHnu when an oHonmHm 3o: m unopemdopaH m :30 mHS GO £038 mum> mamH locum monomH No: mH unopoommo 30: .OH 89 Rotter's I-E Scale (Correlations are those of each item with total score, excluding that item.) 1. a. Children get into trouble because their parents punish them too much. b. The trouble with most children nowadays is that their parents are too easy on them. Filler 2. a. Many of the unhappy things in people's lives are partly due to bad luck. .26 b. People's misfortunes result from mistakes they make. 3. a. One of the major reasons why we have wars is because people don't take enough interest in politics. 2, There will always be wars, no matter how hard people try to prevent them. .18 4. a. In the long run people get the respect they deserve in this world. 2, Unfortunately, an individual's worth often passes unrecog- nized no matter how hard he tries. .29 5. a. The idea that teachers are unfair to students is nonsense. 2, Most students don't realize the extent to which their grades are influenced by accidental happenings. .18 6. a. Without the right breaks one cannot be an effective leader. .32 b. Capable people who fail to become leaders have not taken advantage of their opportunities. 7. a, No matter how hard you try some people just don't like you. .23 b. People who can't get others to like them don't understand how to get along with others. 8. a. Heredity plays the major role in determining one's personality. b. It is one's experiences in life which determine what they're like. Filler 9. a. I have often found that what is going to happen will happen. .16 b. Trusting to fate has never turned out as well for me as making a decision to take a definite course of action. 10. ll. 12. l3. 14. 15. l6. l7. 18. a. lo‘ a. 90 In case of the well prepared student there is rarely if ever such a thing as an unfair test. Many times exam questions tend to be so unrelated to course work that studying is really useless. Becoming a success is a matter of hard work, luck has little or nothing to do with it. Getting a good job depends mainly on being in the right place at the right time. The average citizen can have an influence in government decisions. This world is run by the few people in power, and there is not much the little guy can do about it. When I make plans, I am almost certain that I can make them work. It is not always to plan too far ahead because many things turn out to be a matter of good or bad fortune anyhow. There are certain people who are just no good. There is some good in everybody. . In my case getting what I want has little or nothing to do with luck. Many times we might as well decide what to do by flipping a coin. Who gets to be the boss often depends on who was lucky enough to be in the right place first. Getting people to do the right thing depends upon ability, luck has little or nothing to do with it. As far as world affairs are concerned, most of us are the victims of forces we can neither understand, nor control. By taking an active part in political and social affairs the people can control world events. Most people don't realize the extent to which their lives are controlled by accidental happenings. There is really no such thing as "luck." .24 .30 .27 .27 Filler .29 .31 .36 91 19. a. One should always be willing to admit mistakes. b. It is usually best to cover up one's mistakes. Filler 20. a. It is hard to know whether or not a person likes you. .27 b. How many friends you have depends on how nice a person you are. 21. a, In the long run the bad things that happen to us are balanced by the good ones. .15 b. Most misfortunes are the result of lack of ability, ignorance, laziness, or all three. 22. a. With enough effort we can wipe out political corruption. 2, It is difficult for people to have much control over things politicians do in office. .23 23. 2, Sometimes I don't understand how teachers arrive at the grades they give. .26 b. There is a direct connection between how hard I study and the grades I get. 24. a. A good leader expects people to decide for themselves what they should do. b. A good leader makes it clear to everybody what their jobs are. Filler 25. 2, Many times I feel that I have little influence over the things that happen to me. .48 b. It is impossible for me to believe that chance or luck plays an important role in my life. 26. a. People are lonely because they don't try to be friendly. 2, There's not much in trying too hard to please people, if they like you, they like you. .20 27. a. There is too much emphasis on athletics in high school. b. Team sports are an excellent way to build character. Filler 28. a. What happens to me is my own doing. 2, Sometimes I feel I don't have enough control over the direction my life takes. .24 29. a, Most of the time I can't understand why politicians behave the way they do. .11 b. In the long run the people are responsible for bad government on a national as well as on a local level. Note: Score is the total number of underlined choices (i.e., external items endorsed). APPENDIX C INSTRUMENTS FOR THIS STUDY mhm3H< amumo uo>mz mNmBH< uo>oz um>oz moEHumaom mmaHuoaom omuwo owuwo ohm3H< uo>mz mhmaH< uo>oz Hm>mz mmafiumaom mmafiuwaom fimuwo :muwo mkm3H< um>mz m%m3H< um>mz uo>mz moEHumaom moaHumaom nmumo moaHuoaom ooumo mmaHumaom comma moaHuoEom .oaHu :0 upon xuos Hoonom he onuuom mHnsouu o>mn H .HHM .muo> pHsosm conned m .coNHuHu ooow m on 0H .OH coumo m>m3H< .uH eunuch oHno3 H .ondaonm oHHns mwomno none oOu xumn ma o>mw uoHSmmu m «H .m mhm3H< uo>oz .mUHHoe no 3mH one nuHs mHnsouu pH coon o>m£ H .mm mmafiuwfibm .me05 HOW 03H“ GO 3“ H .N .mHnoouu ouoH ma muow u0H>mnmn he .Hoonum oH .ou souwo mam3H< .mmHuuooou nonuo oH oHdoma cu wooden: owns moons omouooooo an H .m mhm3H< uumz .mHHmmmm oHHnse no muoo>m uomuuoo uoonm ooououooo an H .e mmEHumaom .HawaHms mm omaoUHHom noonoou H .m« .oo ou nwsooo m: o>Hm u.omooo wonummu onu can: as ou mmch .53 on mass 3 on .Hoosom 3 .N amumo mhm3H< .oopoHamu onon uoonuHa uumum H owns anon H .H pom umuHH unoh ham dem uoo muoosoumum osu wonuoom Hoow mumnuo .oo mH umSmom ecu NH Ho .oouu uo>oo mH nuance «no HH mmmmm_oHouHo .mHHn3m oH mono mono mH uomaoumum mnu NH mmmmmmmmm_mHouHo .oaHu mnu Ho umoa mono mH uaosoumum on» HH.mmmmm.oHuuHo .uaHu mnu mo HHm mayo mH oomsoumum onu HH.MNmmmM uHouHo .uH uoonm onHooH umooon o>Hm umshlluomaoumum sumo usonm NGOH OOu onnu u.:on .hHoHdmu xuos .mauuH on .monnu mmmnu uoonm Hoom so» so: m3o£m noHna uo3mom may mHouHu mono pom comm .muwamom moons no uanu on one muonu .mmuwm uoo on mHnood moon .mma one has moo mem mHnomn oaom noHsa no muomaoumum each was when "mnOHuuoan ZCHHN mHmm m.QHHmo 92 93 m>m3H< no>oz moEHuoEom omumo no>oz mmaHuoaom soumo mhm3H< moaHuoaom dunno ohm3H< no>oz :wumo mhmaH< nm>oz mmaHumaom m>m3H< nm>mz moEHumaom omuuo nm>oz mmaHuoaom ooumo mmm3H< mmaHumaom amumo m>m3H< nm>oz omumo mmm3H< nm>oz mmaHuoaom ohmBH< no>mz moaHuoaom omuwo no>mz moaHuoaom souwo ohm3H< moaHuoaom nmuwo mhm3H< no>oz ooumo mmm3H< no>mz moaHuoaom whoaHs nm>mz mmaHumaom omumo no>oz mmaHuoaom oouwo mmm3H< nm>mz mmaHuoaom omuwo mhm3H< moEHumEom couwo mhm3H< nm>mz .mHaoma zuH3 umooos on on mH onnu nomunoasH once one .ammu m mo onuamo no doonw m mo nuommH vmuomHm coon m>mn H .30: 30oz H noon >no> one on H .mpomnno can on no nmnHon Boon m we ammono noon m>ms H .aoon ecu no use oH nonuoou onu sons onxnoa no om coo 0:3 moonu Ho moo an H .Hoonum oH .na oxmB H Gena no .moco hHoo poHHmu cons no HUOHo aanm am nuHa «Hooks an a: now H .08 so hHmn emu mHnoom .hoaoa women H hm3 man soHa poHHmHumm onm muoonmm h: .Hoonom now mumH am H .onoomHa osu mo once on mHeomm nozuo umH H .asonm m oH xnoa H cons .maHu momma H .omaoa mnu mesons on On one“ answmn o>m£ H .NHnHmHos monounu monn oHdooa nonuo mom H saga ooonmuooo am H .pmn onon now HdeooHnn man on noon coon o>m£ H .Hoonum oH .mnoon Hmnm>om no« moon mac: waHmmum Ho «Hammad an H .oommmn ooow m m>m£ u.coo H coca Hoonom aonm moan hmum H .NN .oN .mN .eN .mN .NN .HN .ON .¢H_ .mHm .NH. .oH .mH .QHM .mH .NHM 94 omuNo ohm3H< no>oz moEHumaom ohmst no>mz moEHuoEom omuNo nm>oz mmaHumEom couNo mam3H< moaHuoaom oouNo mmm3H< no>mz omuNo mhm3H< nm>mz moaHumaom mxm3H< nm>mz moaHumaom amuNo no>oz mmeHuoaom omuNo mNm3H< mmEHumaom couNo ohmBH< nm>oz couNo mmm3H< no>oz mmaHuoaom mNm3H< no>mz moEHumEow amuNo nouNo m>m3H< no>oz muaHuoaom mmm3H< nm>mz moaHumaom couNo nm>mz moaHuoEom couNo mhm3H< moEHumaom souNo ohm3H< no>oz souNo mmm3H< nm>oz mmaHumEom .osN onm umnu mwcHnu onoo mnonn monnu hnmmmmuoo on On nonunOQEH m.uH .50h nON onnuoEom mmov on NH mHao noncwHoo naom noN meHsumaom on On oHon poow m mH uH .ocoo on one uH ooh m on on oH monouHa ooozno>o NH .huHo no oaou mHs onaHon oaHu oaom woman vHaonm conned mno>m .monnu ouHommno Ou oxHH H .ma No onsuosom muuoexo on cons maov oaoHnN m uoH oHooa H .NHomha noN xnoa ou mH uH cmnu ammo onu nON xnos ou oomunonaH onoa m.uH .omso: man masons mm: H mmcHnu NE Noam use On 08 ooHamn on m>mn hHHmoms muomnmm A: .ouo> cu m>mn u.opHp mHnomn pom mGOHuoon m>m£ u.ooHo m3 NH NNo nmuumn on pHsos hnunooo nso .moomHnN ha uoHomnmmHo cu m>m£ H sons can HmmN H .umw mesa um£3 m>nummv «Hmoom .uao anon mwanu mos ago do mononoNNHo m moxma ouo> m.oomnod moo .omoHo one now: NHosz emmx on pmvoHaon on on m>m£ H .anma aenn pHooo H .NUHo owomnum m pH umOH mambo: H NH .xnoa Na :mHoHN u.:ov H monsoon monHdaou nonomou NZ .Nq .Hc. .oe .am .mm .Nmm .om« .mm. .qmm .mm .Nm« .HNk .omm .mN .mNm 95 mhm3H< no>oz moaHuoaom :ouNo nm>mz mmaHumaom :muNo m>m3H< moaHumaom :wuNo mmm3H< no>mz :ouNo m%m3H< nm>mz moaHuoEom mNm3H< nm>oz mmEHumaom :muNo no>oz mmaHuoaom :ouNo who3H< moaHuoaom :uuNo ohm3H< no>oz :oUNo mhm3H< no>mz moaHuoaom m>m3H< no>mz moaHuoEOm :muNo nm>oz moaHuoaom :muNo mhm3H< moaHumaom :muNo mmm3H< no>mz .vonoom mmnm>mn onm3 mamuH omens. .onom chu noN onoom Hmuou can :H o003H0:H no: onm3 mamuH omoshe .mmmon H mm mo:m30HHm he 0:0:m :mu H .00 on um£3 30:3 HHH3 H .ponofi:H ono3 0:0HnN : NH .noum3 ovmcnou m mH onosu 0:: 0:0Ho 080: a: H NH 00 cu ums3 30:3 H .moo:onoN:oo nonomohlu:mnmm o:muum mu:mnm: h: .w:0H: mace nouumn 00 cu oxHH H mw:H£u NH :m3m uH on onum H .00 on ooh : :m>Hw :onz .uH o>nomop u.:00 hosu :mg3 omsmH::e new :onoHHsu .Hoonom um muumfiona HmHooam n0N noou:0Ho> H .30: we 30:: mnmnuo m>ms :mzu nmzumn NHmmha n0N uao mw:Hnu Mn03 H .pHo on: Nona :un3 mu:onma n50» No mnmu oxmu oHsosm so» .m30:x m:o 0: NH uann HHm NH mumou :0 w:Hu:o£o .unwam on: 50% NH uH 0v 0» omHo 0:0oaom now On Nnu HHH3 so» .00 cu monounsm on: 90% w:H:uoaom oxHH u.:0o 50% :mnz .mms .Nm .Hm .owe .me .me« .Ne .oe .nw .qu .mqm 96 monw< mmnmmmHn .Hoonom noN a: now HHH3 onmxmn onONon moaHu Hmnm>om oHHno ha HHmo ou m>mn NHHmsm: H .mH«~ monwmmHn mmnw< .:0HuomNmHumm me On mm:0a mowm:ma oHHnu A: .NH monwmmHn monm: .mmoon mnu 0::0n: manoNno: m:m\o£ mxmmu on» nuHB wou00::0o on no: 0H30£m mo:m30HHm n.0Hch < .HH monwmmHa mmnw< .owm mnooN Hmnm>mm mnm3 mono :mnu oHnHm:onmon mmoH on: :manHno mHoa3 man :0 .OH oonw< monwmmHa .w:H0w on: keno mnmn3 0:: w:Hoo on: :onoHHSU um53 N0 xomnu 000w coax 0u u:muno:aH m.uH .m monwmmHn mmnw< .n0H m gmH:HN on no£\aH: now on oHHso he nmuNm hHHmo:Hu:ou on On o>mn H 0:HN H .m«. oonw< omnwmmHn .ou:m30HH: answon : meow oHHno A: .N oonm: omnwmmHn .u:munomaH hHoamnuxu mH mo:mo:muum Hoonom .N oonwmmHn ounw: .munmum on umn3 moan:HN hHHmom: 0HH£0 A: .n« monwmmHa ounw: .xmmu oHNHoodm m mudeaoo on w:HHHoN noN oHHao m m:HH:HomHv\£mH::d ou u:munoeaH mH uH .q oonw< monmmmHn .:manH:u m:HH:HomH0 \SmHoam on hm3 poow m mH mason onu 0:30nm xn03 mnuxo w:H:mem< .m omnmmmHn monw: .omson man o:=0nm w:H:Hm£ :mnu :onoHHno n0N u:mun0:aH ends on: .on35 no moHumHnum m: £03m .mmHuH3Huom anooHnnoolmnuxu .N omnwmmHn omnw< .omoon man 0::onm on On mach anowmn :m>Hm on: :manHno a: .H .nmnuo some w:HuHam:00 usozuH3 .u:uaou:um 30mm upon: w:HHomN nHmnu ommmmnmwo umon 30Hn3 om:0:mon man oHunHu on poxmm ono3 mu:onmm .:on0HH:o nHmnu No HH: no: 0HH£0 nosuo:m no: .mu:uaoumum man on w:Ho:0:mon :mn3 pHHnu Hm00N osu NH:0 0:HB :H o>m£ ou m:0Huo:num:H Hmnno> :o>Hw mnm3 mu:mnmm "muoz oHHno N0 :0HumsHm>m u:onmm 97 .H00:0m EonN :80: w:H%mum noN momsoxm mo:HN :ouNo pHH:0 a: .Hoo:um u: mH::0nu :H nw:\aH: mumm mHHm:0Hmm000 n0H>m:m: n.0HH:0 N: .m:0NHuH0 0H:Hm:o:mmn 06000: On :0n0HH:0 w:H:0mmu :H m>HuomNNo mH H00:um nso .mummnmuaH :30 nHm:u moons: cu ounN on: :mnoHH:o :m:3 maHu m o: oH:0:m 000:0HH:0 .0HH:0 Na noN pnm: mama: 03H: :0 0Hnmu m:u ou w:Huuou .HHmu onoo: :m:3 mmwmmmma 0:0:nuHou ma 03H» 0n muownoN pHH:0 a: .NHHEmN 0:: :H nm:uoa\oNH3 m:u N0 NuHHH:Hm:0:mmn 0:» o: :05: known: 0H00:m :oHumnmnon: Home no 0:: xn3oompom .aHm: can: on «new an unseen o:m\m: u:mpHN:oo a.H .huHo ow:mnum m :H umOH 08:00: 0HH:0 NE NH .huH>Huo: umowmom no: 00 muHsom :0:3 0: on w:H:umaom w:Ho:HN maHu vnm: m o>m: :mnuHH:0 N: .00HH00 m:Ho: u:0:uH3 H00:0m noN a: snow pHH:0 z: .:on0HH:0 p00:n0::mHo: nm:u0 :uH3 HHo3 m:0Hm mumw 0HH:0 z: .mnmmmmom: NH mHmoa :30 mH: unmeun: 0n oH:m mH oHH:u A: .moHuH>Huom anoannso mnuxo :H moquHoHunm: oHH:u z: .080: 0:: ovauso w:H:n03 h: hm:0a mnuxm m:nmo hHHm:0Hmmuuo 0HH:0 a: .0m: memo o:u :mnoHH:0 nm:u0 :m:u mnouma mnoa memo: nmu:wsmp\:om m: %:m 0H:03 H .mmso: o:u meson: om:memm mxmmu muoH:300 0n VHH:0 he 0:Hamn 0a 03:: H .oaHu m u: mnao: Hono>0m noN NHmmaH: h: zoom on u:mum:aou mH 0HH:0 he HuoN H .om*. .mN*_ .wN .NN .oNgm .mNa. .QN .mNs .NNs_ .HNe .ON .NH: .NH: .NH .NHs .NHem .qHa 98 00nw:0HQ 00nw< 00nw< 00nw:0Hn 00nw< 00nw:0Hn 00nw00Ha 00nm< 00nw< 00nw:0HQ 00nw:mHn 00nw< 00nw< 00nw00Hn 00nw00Ha 00nw< 00nw< 00nw00Hn 00nw< 00nw00Hn 00nw00Hn 00nw< 00nm< 00nw:mHn 00nw< 00nw:0Hn 00nw:0Hn 00nw< .00n00: 0mn030n 0n03 080uH 000:9. .0H000 0H:u noN 0n000 : 0u::800 0o 00H:u0u 080uH 0m0:u 3H:o« .08n0Nn0: 0:0\0: 0:0:u ou 00u:H0n 0H 00::30HH: 0.0HH:0 NZ .00n:H:H 0n03 0:0HnN : NH 00 on 0::3 30:: 0H:03 0HH:0 N: .0:0H: 080: NH :ou:3 00::n0u : No 00:0 :H 00 0a u::3 30:: 0Hoo3 0HH:0 A: .0n:0 NH00 0:: .008Hu00: .mH:08 noN 0Hn00:0: n:H:w0n : :0 :0n0HH:o :00: 00 mu:0n:: noN u::un0:8H m.uH .080H:0n: :30 n0:\0H: w:H3H00 u: 03Huo0NN0 huu0n: 0H 0HH:0 >8 H00N H .xn03 0n:m n0 00:0: :oaa noN :0 00HH0n 0: 0n w::0> uH: : HHHuo 0n: 00:nw :uNHN 0:u :H :0n0HH:o .0n:0 0:: 0n:> 0:u 0» w:H:H:un0: :n03 00 0N0:\:08 0:: :n030000: 00 :0803\0HnHw 03:: :0:8 huu0n: HHHum 03 .080: n30 :H .8H: 0:HH:H00H0\:0H::: HHH3 NHH::u:030 up: 0HH:3 : noN 0:00:: 003:0H 0HH:0 >8 0m:H:u m:Hno:wH N0 uH::: : 03:: H .8n0Nn0: 0Hoo:0 mn0:808 hHH80N H::0H3H0:H 0:0“ u::3 u:0:: 00:H:u 03:: 03 .hHH8:N : 0< .00::30HH: n:H:w0n : w:H3Hw :::u n0:u:n uH 0000: 0:0\0: 0: 0HH:0 n00 on >0:08 0:0 03H» 0: n0N0n: 08 .:0mn0: w:Hn:0 0 0: 00:Hn0000 0: 0H000 0HH:0 >8 .0NHH n0:\0H: :H u:H0: 0H:u u< .:n038:0u :00Hu00n: 0:: :H :030HH0: AHH80N n00 .m00:0n0N:00 H00:00 on on H :0:3 >00 00 0:: n0:u:0u 0.0HH:0 m8 0::3 w:Hn:0: 0n 0n:3noN :00H H .08Hu N0 uoH : :0u0:3 0HH:0 N: .00 .me« .Nqs .H0 .00« .mm .mm .Nm .om .mm .cms .mm .Nm .Hme: 99 m c m N H 08H0 03H0:000: 03H0:000: 03H0:000: 03H0:000::H 0:0 N0 0008 8n0> 3HH::0= 8H000n00oz 0: 00 00:08 03H0:000::H NH00:00 :H 0HH:0 0H:0 0H 03H0:000: 30: .m H N m 0 m w:0n3 0:0 0H :H :0:3 0n0:00 :00: 00:: 0H:HH: 000:0:0 n0N 80HHH:H0 0080H: 8H00H:HN0Q 00 00Hn8 008H0080m 00::8 800H0m I:0:00n 00::8 N0000 0:0 n0 0: 00:3 noN huHHH:H0:0:00n 0::0 0:\0 0000 HH03 30m .0 m n 0 N H 08000H::0n0 8n03 a: 03Hw 0::8 0H::0n0 0H00HH m:H:08:: N0 00H:0 0H 0H :0:3 :0H 00 0H: : : 0:: 0:\0 8HH000 :H m: 03H» 0.:03 : 00 0:0H0m 00Hsv 00:08 :0:3 :0 003Hu 8n03 :0 003HU N:0:0 : 0: 00H0n0: 0:\0 0000 HH03 30: .m H N m e m 0:HH 0H 0:\0 NH00 080:: 00:H0: 000w 00:3 N0 0n:3: 0n000 0:0 30:: 0H00HH:0n NH00 000:: 0H0 0:: 00H::N 1:: 8H000H8800 00 8000 0.:0009 0::3080m 10HH00n thH08 :30 030:: N0:\0 0H 0H00HH00n 30: .N« n 0 m N H 0:0HH00xm 000w 0w:n03< nH:m noom “0H 0:08000n0: H:n0:0w 0.0HH:0 0H:0 .:0H:H:0 88 :H .H« .800H 0::0 noN 0HH:0 0:0 N0 w:H0:n nsoh 00 00:0:00nn00 0::0 00:n:: n0 0n03 0:0 N0 n0:8:: 0:0 00Hn3 .800H :00: N0 0N0H 0:0 00 0:HH 0:0 :0 «0:0H000n00:H ennao no nonsaana>0 nmnuamn 100 m 00:0HH0: 10Hna: .0000H: 00 0:0Hx:: 008 H 0n0: 8HH000 8n03 .8:0:08 00:08 N mnnnxmnnan .0000: H sunaunnnna 0000:wHH0 00 :H0: 0:00m 0 0000H: 00 803 N0 0:0 0000 m H0n0:00 0000H 008H0 I080: .0Nom 0:03n0z N 0:00H 30: 00 0:00: 00 30Hm 0:00:0009 m 0000::0n 0H:H0:00 00 000nw< m 80:0 0Hon0:00 0:: 0w:HH00N 0:: H N 03H00H00n 0H00 .8n0n0 00:3 N0 00H: I:00 :00No 10::0 000: N0:\0 0H 0::HH:800 30: N 0 0n:: 00 0n0: .03H0H0:00:H 00:0NN0 w:H:00: . :w008 N000n0: 0H:0 0H 03H0H0:00:H\03H0H0:00 30: 0 0:08 i00Hox0 n00N: :300 00H000m m 0n0:0o 0000n0n 100ww00 0H: IH0:00 00:08 a snag 0:000 :0:0 00:00 nnnxm :30 000: m N 00x0H0: N0HH:0 0H:0 0H 0000 00 :0:8.30m 03H000m 0 H :00H0 0:0 000:00HN:H :0H3 0NHn0: n0 00H 00 00:08 8HH000 3n0> nannau 0:80 an mnanxmnn 3o: m m :30 :0 :008 8n03 080H: 0:00:0800:H Ion: 00H:008 N:00n0: 0H:0 0H 0:00:0000 30: .OH .m« .N« 101 0000Hm800 800H00 :n03 H :0 000::00 0: 00 0H:0 002 0n0:00 H0n0:00 00 0000: .8000: H 00H0H3H000 :H 0n:: 00:00 n030z H m:Hn:0:: .03H0 1H0:00:H .000n00 1H0:00:H G03 .0H000 0H:0 n0N 0n000 0 00::800 00 00H0000 00: 0n03 0800H 000:8« N m 000n0000:H 0000H0800 008H00800 :0:3 0:00 0:: 0000HQ800 8HH::0H0 :n03 8HH000: :n03 0 OUQHOUUQ 0:: 0000H:800 8HH000: :n03 m 0000H:800 0803H0 MHH0N 0:0:0H3 N0000H8800 00:08:mH00: 00H0000 0n: :00N0 30: N m :0 000::00 0: 0H:00::0000 00 0H:0 800H0m NHH000: N m n0000H 0HHH:0 :0H0300 0 w:H0: 00 0000: :H 000n00:H 0:: w:H000H 0: M03H0008 :H 00000n00:H 0 0H:00::0000 08:3H0 0008H< m anpmnnmn snawnm 800080 0:80 an annannon so: 0 0HHH:0 :H:0n0000H w:H:0H0300 0000n00:080n m :00N0 :H:0 In0000H 03H0 IHmoa 000H30nm NQH:0n0000H 0.:00n0: 0H:0 000n :08 0H:03_3om N m n0000H :::0 n030HH0N :00N0 0n08 0:: 003H03:H UU>HO>§fi EOUHQm 0 n0000H 0: :0 I00:0 008H00800 003H03:H 8HnH0m m n0000H 0 00 :000:0 :00N0 .03H000 8n0> N00H0H3H000 n0H00Hnn0010n0x0 :H 0:\0 0H 003H03:H 30: N m 0000: 0n0:00 03H0H0:00:H 00 03H0H0:00:H :00No 008H0080m e 0000: 0n0:00 00 03H0 IH0:00 >HH000= n Humans: .000n00H0 3:00 8n0> N00008000H0 N0 0HH:0 0H:0 0H 000n00H0:00 30: .NH .0H .NH .NH .HH 102 Locus of Control Scale Directions: For each number there are two statements. Please circle 'a' 2; 'b' to show which of the statements in each pair you most agree with. *5. o°|0 IU‘ Many unhappy things in people's lives are partly due to bad luck. . People's misfortunes result from the mistakes they make. In the long run people get the respect they deserve in this world. . Unfortunately, an individual's worth often passes unrecognized no matter how hard he tries. Without the right breaks one cannot be an effective leader. Capable people who fail to become leaders have not taken advantage of their opportunities. Becoming a success is a matter of hard work, luck has little or nothing to do with it. Getting a good job depends mainly on being in the right place at the right time. The average citizen can have an influence in government decisions. This world is run by the few people in power, and there is not much the little guy can do. . When I make plans, I am almost certain that I can make them work. It is not always wise to plan too far ahead because many things turn out to be a matter of good or bad fortune anyhow; In my case getting what I want has little or nothing to do with luck. . Many times we might just as well decide what to do by flipping a coin. Who gets to be the boss often depends on who was lucky enough to be in the right place first. Getting people to do the right thing depends upon ability, luck has little or nothing to do with it. As far as world affairs are concerned, most of us are victims of forces we can neither understand, nor control. By taking an active part in political and social affairs the people can control world events. *10. 11. *12. 13. 103 Most people don't realize the extent to which their lives are controlled by accidental happenings. . There is really no such thing as "luck." It is hard to know whether or not a person really likes you. How many friends you have depends on how nice a person you are. Sometimes I can't understand how teachers arrive at the grades they give. There is a direct connection between how hard I study and the grades I get. Many times I feel that I have little influence over the things that happen to me. . It is impossible for me to believe that chance or luck plays an important role in my life. *These items were deleted from the scoring after factor analysis. _The underlined letter indicates the statement considered to represent the external locus of control position. 104 .00:H00n0 8H0:H3H0 .:0H000H00:H 00n000 0 0H 8HH80N 0:8 .0 m N H .:08 n03o 80Hno:0:0 No 0:0H0H00: :H :0803 000H: 00 H0n:0:::: 30:0800 0H 0H .8 m N H .00Hnn08 0n03 80:0 0n0N0: 0:0H00H0n H0ox00 0:: 03:: 80:0 NH :0803 0 noN 000:00n 03:: 00 00000880 0: 00: 0H:0:0 :08 < .o m N H .80:0 noN 000:00n 000H HHH3 0: 00H0 no .0000n0: 0H: 00 :00: :H00 00 003oHH0 0: n0>00 0Hao:0 0HH:0 0 .m m N H .:0H0H:80 w:H3Hn0 0 0:0 :0H00:H8n0000 0n: :08 H00n 0 N0 00H0HH000 0::0n088H 0008 0:8 .0 m N H .0NH3 0 0: 00 0:008 0H 00:3 0:000n00:: 0.:00 00H3n00 0N0Hnn08 0:0 80nN 80:0 0n03 0:0 03080n 00 0:03 0:3 :0803 .m m N H .0H :0H3 w:H0:08Hn0:x0 :H n0N 000 0w 00 8H0:HH 0n: 80:0 .800 000:: :008 000 0H00 0n: :0n0HH:0 NH .N m N H .0n00008 8HH80N :H 00:8:0 :H08 0:0 03:: 00 0:»:0 0:0:00: 0:0 0wn0H 0:: 8: 0:: .w:H:0 000w 0 0H 0N0Hnn08 :H 80HH0000 080m .H m N H nm> Om G o H N m o H N m o H N m o H N m o H N m o H N m o H N m o H N m z < .000H000:0 no H0n0a0z u z .00nw00Ho + a .00nw: u < .00nm00H0 8Hm:0n0m I am .00nw: 8Hm:0n0m I :m .00nw00H0 8Hw:on00 8n0> u :03 .00nw0 8Hw:0n00 8n0> n <03 .30H0: 00:H:H:x0 0: .800H 00:0 00 00:0:00n n008 0000H0:H 00 800H :000 N0 0:0nN :H n0:8:: 0:0 0H0nHo 0H00m 03H000Hn0:0:<\::Hn00Hn0:0:< "0:0H008n00:H 105 .00n00:000 H0n08 n00 0300 00 w:H0w 0n: 03 NH 0H:00: w:008 :0 n00n0: :300 :00n0 00 030: HHH3 03 00:0 30:0 80HH0n088H H0nx00 0:: 08Hn0 :0 0000N 0:8 .00:0n0: 0H: noN 000:00n 0:0 0000H00nw .030H 000nm 0 H00N 00: 0000 0:3 :00n0: 0 :::0 n030H w:H:08:: 8H0n0: 0H 0n0:8 .:0n0HH:0 n0:0o 0:HH 0n08 0: 00 8H: 00w 0H:0:0 00:0n0: 0H: .803 8:: :H H000::0 0H 0HH:0 : NH .n0:008 0 00 00H000 n0: :H 00HH0N 00: 8030n no 80008 HH0 0: 0n: :0n0HH:0 000:3 :0803 0 .NH .HH .0H .m Qm> Gm 4m