a “I ' . av ;‘ ‘mml? l ‘1... . A . ff“ '- ."/\'.' . ' \4 4 gem.“ 'L‘ .1 "I“ .. a r \7 .TI ‘ m " v ‘. 1 ”3234115": F : murmmrim RETUMUB LIBRARY MTERMLS: Place in book return to raw: charge from circulation record: 0“! 1ul‘l‘|ll I III" I'lr.| .,.. ht! Q ‘5‘... a .0 .IIOIIIIQI ‘4 «hlrui‘ I'L1VII. 0.41‘l10 . AN EVALUATION OF SELECTED OHIO PUBLIC SCHOOLS DRIVER EDUCATION PROGRAMS By Joe Steven Shrader A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum 1979 ABSTRACT AN EVALUATION OF SELECTED OHIO PUBLIC SCHOOLS DRIVER EDUCATION PROGRAMS By Joe Steven Shrader Statement of the Problem The funding of driver education in the high schools is being questioned as budgets become tighter and school systems search for ways to reduce operating costs. If the funding of driver education is to continue, evaluation of high school driver education programs is a necessity. The state of Ohio has not conducted an evalua- tion of its driver education programs in recent years, and the State Department of Education, Driver Education Section, recognized the importance of such an evaluation. The purpose of this study was to measure cogni- tive knowledge of those students who had completed or were just completing a recent driver education course in , randomly selected high schools in Ohio. Specifically, this study had a three-fold purpose. The first was to evaluate cognitive knowledge of driver education stu- dents. The second was to attempt to determine if one Joe Steven Shrader of six types of driver education programs was providing more cognitive knowledge under present conditions than the others. The third purpose was to attempt to identify those variables which may have had an influence on stu- dent success in acquiring cognitive knowledge. Methods of Procedure The Program Research in Driver Education (PRIDE) fifty question multiple-choice test was selected as the measure of cognitive knowledge for this study. A random sample of twelve schools in Ohio was made. These twelve schools fit into six different pro- grams. One class per school was then selected for the evaluation. Students were randomly selected from each class in order to achieve equal cell size for the three analyses of variance (ANOVA) tests for significance which were conducted. The Major Findings 1. Students tested in Ohio were significantly deficient on an 80 percent success criterion. Sixty-three percent of the 326 students achieved 80 percent or more correct. This deficiency was significant at the .95 level. 2. In none of the three programs were 80 percent of the students achieving 80 percent success. Program one (two-phase), program two (three-phase simulation), and program three (four-phase) had 56 percent, 66 percent, and 70 percent respectively. All three were significantly deficient at the .95 level of confidence. 10. ll. 12. Joe Steven Shrader Student scores were significantly deficient on questions their instructors indicated had been covered in their classes. Seventy-one percent of the students achieved 80 percent or more correct. This was significantly deficient at the .95 level of confidence. Program three (four-phase) was significantly better than program one (two-phase) or program two (three- phase simulation). Significance was measured at the .95 level of confidence. No significant difference was found between programs with multimedia and those programs without multimedia. Significance was measured at the .95 level of confi- dence. None of the three programs was significantly better with high, middle, or low GPA students. Significance was measured at the .95 level of confidence. Student score was moderately correlated with cost of the program. A correlation coefficient (r) of .1737 was found. An ”r" of .40 was needed to determine a significant relationship. Student score was not significantly correlated with hours of instruction in the program. An "r" of -.0978 was found, when an "r" of .40 was necessary for signif- icance. Student score was not significantly correlated with class size. An "r” of .0340 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed for significance. Student score was not significantly correlated with the instructors' education in driver education. An "r" of -.0729 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed for significance. Student score was not significantly correlated with the instructors' age. An "r" of .0103 was found, when an "r" of .40 was necessary in order to be significant. Student score was not significantly correlated with the number of years their instructor had taught driver education. An "r" of .0043 was found, when an "r" of .40 was necessary for significance. The author would like to dedicate this study to his parents. His father's dedication to the field of traffic safety has been an inspiration to all who have come in contact with him. To follow in his footsteps is a rewarding challenge. To his mother, a special thanks for typing, editing and moral support, without which, this study may never have been completed. Thank you for being parents who taught your children to set their sights high and to realize that "all things are possible." ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author wishes to thank Dr. Robert E. Gustafson for serving as his advisor during the graduate program. His insight made the author's total graduate program as complete as possible. The author would further like to thank him for serving as his committee chairman. His help and encouragement insured the com- pletion of this dissertation. The author would like to also thank Dr. Robert O. Nolan, Dr. Norman T. Bell and Dr. Joseph Dzenowagis for their time and assistance while serving on the guidance committee of this study. A special note of thanks goes to Mr. Larry Cathell and his staff at the Ohio State Department of Education for their assistance before and during this study. Without their help this study would not have been attempted in the state of Ohio. And thanks to you Jane, for your support during this study and graduate program. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES LIST OF APPENDICES Chapter I. THE PROBLEM The Problem . . . Statement of the Problem . Purpose of the Study . Importance of the Study The Research . . Research Questions Research Hypotheses Methods and Procedures Evaluation Instrument . Scope of the Study . Generalizability . . Limitations of the Study . Definition of Terms Class "C" Test Items Cognitive Knowledge GPA . . . Multimedia Program One Program Two Program Three Range Simulation Special Test Set Organization of Remaining Chapters II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE . Driver Education Evaluation Measures Educational Evaluations . Driver Education Evaluation Studies iv Page vi vii viii Chapter Driver Education Phases Summary III. DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY . Selection of Ohio Selection of Schools Data Collection Contact Letters Telephone Contacts Testing Procedure . Methods of Analysis Organization of Data Summary IV. ANALYSIS OF RESULTS The Sample Data Total Student Achievement Individual Program Analysis Teacher Item Analysis Comparison of Programs GPA/Program Effect Variable Correlations Summary V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, RECOMMENDATIONS APPENDICES Summary of the Study . Research Design Findings Conclusions Recommendations Recommendations for Further Research Discussion BIBLIOGRAPHY . Page Table c~ 4> .b p~ ¢~ O‘U'IDUON .10 LIST OF TABLES Student Distribution for t Analysis Analysis of Program One Analysis of Program Two Analysis of Program Three Analysis of Teacher Covered Material Contrast One: Program Two Contrasted with Program Three Contrast Two: Program One Contrasted with Program Two Two-way ANOVA Table Two-way ANOVA Table Correlations Between Student Scores and Related Variables vi Page 54 61 62 63 64 65 66 68 70 71 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 3.1 ANOVA Design . . . . . . . . . . 54 3.2 Two-way ANOVA Design . . . . . . . 56 3.3 Two-way ANOVA Design . . . . . . . 56 vii Appendix A. B. LIST OF APPENDICES Sample Letter to Superintendents Sample Letter to Principals of Selected Schools . . . . . . . . Sample Letter to Principals of Alternate Schools Sample Form of Telephone Questions Sample Driver Education Knowledge Test and Answer Sheet Sample Record of Test Scores Sample of Items Taught Sample of Teacher Information viii Page 85 87 89 91 93 104 107 117 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM Formal driver education programs have existed in the United States for more than forty-five years, and some form of education for drivers has been utilized since soon after the invention of the automobile. Per- haps one of the earliest forms of education for drivers occurred when the first car salesman taught their pros- pective customers to drive the automobile, frequently a prerequisite of purchase. The popularity, and the availability, of the "horseless carriage" increased the number of vehicles more rapidly than road conditions and education of drivers could accommodate. By the 1930s, schools were beginning to respond to the need to educate the auto- mobile driver, and the first formal driver education courses were inaugurated. In 1930, Amos Neyhart, the "father of driver education," began teaching students behind the wheel on a voluntary basis. In 1934, Neyhart taught at Pennsylvania State College the first complete classroom and laboratory course in driver education. Another early name in driver education was Herbert Stack who offered the first classroom driver education course (81, p. 25). The 19303 further saw the development of the simulator, the range, the combination of classroom, simulation, and behind-the-wheel instruction, as well as the publication of textbooks for driver education (85, pp. A3, 60-62). World War II brought many of these programs to a halt except as training could be used for wartime driving. Car production gave way to airplanes and war materiel. Gasoline was rationed, and replacement parts for existing automobiles were limited to necessary emergencies (85, pp. 75-79). The war had barely ended when the first Presi- dent's Conference for Highway Safety was held in 1945. Four years later the National Conference on High School Driver Education was conducted by the National Commis- sion on Safety Education (81, p. 26). By the early 19505, post World War II production of automobiles was rapidly accelerating to meet the demands of the people who were intent on loosening the wartime restrictions on travel and the use of the auto- mobile (57, p. 25). MOney was more accessible, new highways and super highways were becoming a reality, and mobility became a way of American life. Cars became larger, more powerful, and capable of greater speeds. Vehicle accidents, and the resulting deaths, injuries, and property damage soared, as did the number of drivers and vehicles on the roads (59, p. 59). More and more high schools began offering driver education. The Allstate Insurance Company substantiated its belief in the worth of high school driver education by being the first insurance company to provide insurance reductions for students completing high school driver education. Colleges became more involved in safety pro- grams, and in 1956, The American Driver and Traffic Safety Education Association was organized (81, p. 26). The Driver Education Evaluation Program (DEEP) Study Report to Congress of July 1975 stated that: Basically, the idea of training persons to operate motor vehicles stems from the assumption that trained or experienced persons will perform better in most traffic situations than untrained or inexperienced persons. The beginning of driver and safety education was based primarily on this assumption, and most programs were imple- mented on the basis of their validity for acci- dent prevention. In addition to the common sense emphasis placed on the skills required for driv- ing, a similar emphasis was placed on the develop- ment of assumed safe-driving attitudes, with the belief that such attitudes would result in fewer crashes and that such attitudes could be manipu- lated or developed. The report continued: Unfortunately, it was not until very recently that an attempt has been made to determine scien- tifically which behavioral variables (including attitudes and skills) have a causal relationship with crashes. Recent efforts have also sought to determine whether such variables can be manipu- lated or developed by means of effective training. Measurement of the extent to which a curriculum meets such instructional objectives and various performance requirements has also been emphasized recently. Furthermore, serious attempts are now being made to assess the degree to which such pro- grams are successful in meeting their ultimate goal of crash prevention (81, p. 25). The DEEP Study called the 19608 "the critical period" in high school driver education. It stated that, "Beginning with studies by independent researchers in the early 60's and continuing with the Moynihan and McGuire and Kersh reports in 1968, high school driver education came under severe attack with regard to its claimed effectiveness in crash prevention" (81, p. 28). The study further quoted R. Kaywood as suggesting for the most part that "these voices went unheeded, for during this period HSDE was expanded from providing training for approximately 1 million students to pro- viding training for more than 2 million students" (81, p. 28). If the 603 were the critical period in high school driver education, then the 19703 might be called "the hopeful period." The DEEP Study explained that after the Fifth National Conference for Traffic Safety Educa- tion in December of 1973: It was apparent that four primary thrusts were underway in the traffic safety education area, as follows: Qualitative improvement was being emphasized abbve the need for quantitative expansion. HSDE was becoming an integrated component of a much larger traffic safety education pro- gram for various driver groups. Increasing emphasis was being placed on cost- effective safety education with built-in evaluation based on measurable objectives. Instructional management was moving more toward criterion-referenced courses for students as opposed to time-based instruc- tion (81, p. 28). The state of Ohio has long been active in traffic safety programs. (The first driving simulator, and the first range were developed at Ohio State University in 1930) (80, p. 26). Since 1953, driver education pro- grams have been available in the public schools in the state of Ohio. In 1969, the state recognized the impor— tance of the driver education program in the school curriculum and began subsidizing a part of the cost of the program to the local school districts. In 1969, thirty dollars per pupil was made available to local school districts for the instruction of driver education. This money was derived from driver license fees. By 1972, costs of the programs had risen sharply and the state raised the subsidy to the local school districts to fifty dollars per pupil enrolled in a school-sponsored driver education program. This now amounts to over eight million dollars per year for state support of driver education. In addition, each district may assess a maximum ten dollar fee to each student for program improvement. The death rate per 100 million vehicle miles was enviably lower in Ohio (2.68) than the national average (3.38) in 1977. A supposition might be drawn that there is a correlation between the pertinent interest in driver education programs in the state of Ohio and in the lower number of traffic deaths in the state. There is a trend across the nation to challenge the funding of driver education, and consideration is being given to the possibility of eliminating funding for local high school programs. Partly, this has been brought about because of tightening budgets, but perhaps to an even greater degree, it has been the failure of schools to adequately evaluate their programs to substantiate that students are gaining what they are supposed to gain from driver education classes. The Problem Statement of the Problem To offset this trend of challenge to the funding of driver education, program evaluation will be a neces- sity in each state if legislatures are to continue fund- ing programs. The state of Ohio had not conducted such an evaluation of its driver education programs in recent years, and recognized the need for an updated evaluation. This study attempted to isolate and evaluate several var- iables which have an effect on driver education students in Ohio. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study was to measure the level of cognitive knowledge of those students who had completed or were just completing a recent driver educa- tion course. Specifically, this study had a threefold purpose. The first was to evaluate cognitive knowledge of driver education students. The second was to attempt to deter- mine if one of three types of driver education program was providing more cognitive knowledge under present conditions than the others. The third purpose was to attempt to identify those variables which may have had an influence on student success in acquiring the cogni- tive knowledge. Importance of the Study The former tradition of believing that driver education must be good has been challenged. High school driver education has come under question from several sources in recent years. The most critical of these has been the Insurance Institute for Highway Safety's study on driver education which concluded that driver education causes 2,000 teenage deaths each year (66). Although conclusions of this study have been highly criticized by competent highway safety research personnel, and have since been revised by the IIHS, these negative research conclusions have been damaging to traffic safety programs. Presently, knowledge of driver education effectiveness was best described in a statement in the DEEP Study. ". . No such study has succeeded in producing unequiv- ocal results concerning high school driver education effectiveness (or the lack of it)" (81, p. 84). State and federal agencies which helped fund driver education in the high school have questioned the worth of these programs in terms of the money being spent. Many state legislatures and/or departments of education have requested studies to determine the effectiveness of current driver education programs in reducing accidents and the resulting deaths and injuries on the nation's highways. The Driver Education Section of the Ohio Depart- ment of Education has been interested in conducting such an evaluation of high school programs in that state; hence, the conducting of this study. It was believed that this study could make the following contributions to the driver education programs in the state of Ohio and to the field of traffic safety: Promote better understanding of current driver education programs in Ohio. Give input to the Ohio Department of Educa- tion on their future assistance to driver education programs. Supply information to persons in the traffic safety field in regards to driver education evaluation, both in terms of the results of this study, and its recommendations for similar types of evaluation studies. The Research Research Questions The following research questions were considered in the investigation of selected high school driver edu- cation programs in Ohio: 1. Is driver education in Ohio effective in teaching students cognitive knowledge? Is any of three program types effective in teaching students cognitive knowledge? Is driver education in Ohio effective in teaching what teachers believe they are teaching? Is one of three program types more effective in teaching cognitive knowledge? 10. 11. 12. 13. 10 Is one program.more effective than another with higher, middle, or lower GPA students in teaching cognitive knowledge? Is multimedia in the classroom a major factor in terms of students' success? Is multimedia in the classroom more effective in teaching cognitive knowledge to high, middle, or low GPA students than those class- rooms without multimedia? Is the cost of a program a factor in terms of students' success? Is the number of years of teaching experience in driver education a factor in terms of students' success? Are the total hours of the program related to students' success? Is class size related to students' success? Is teacher age a major factor in terms of students' success? Are teacher preparation hours a major factor in terms of students' success? Research Hypotheses The following research hypotheses were analyzed and tested in this study: 10. 11. 11 Driver education in Ohio is effective in teaching students cognitive knowledge. One of the three programs is effective in teaching cognitive knowledge. Driver education in Ohio is effective in teaching what teachers believe they are teaching. One of the three programs is more effective in teaching cognitive knowledge. One program is more effective than another with higher, middle, or lower GPA students in teaching cognitive knowledge. Multimedia in the classroom is a major factor in terms of students' success. Multimedia in the classroom is more effective in teaching cognitive knowledge to high, middle, or low GPA students than those class- rooms without multimedia. Cost of a program is a factor in terms of students' success. The number of years teaching driver education is a factor in terms of students' success. The total hours of the program are related to students' success. Class size is related to students' success. 12 12. Teacher age is a major factor in terms of students' success. 13. Teacher preparation hours are a major factor in terms of students' success. Methods and Procedures Three teacher variables, one pupil variable, and four program variables were selected to be evaluated. The teacher variables were teacher age, their hours of education in driver education, and their years teaching driver education. The pupil variable was the student GPA. The four program variables were program type, the cost per pupil, total hours of instruction, and number of students in the class. With these program types and variables selected, twelve schools in Ohio were randomly selected and an evaluation was conducted. Evaluation Instrument The evaluation instrument used in the evaluation was a 50 item knowledge test used by the Program Research in Driver Education (PRIDE) project in Iowa. It was developed from class "C" questions developed by Pollack and McDole. These questions were selected from those which covered the critical and most critical subject areas as defined by the HumRRO report (62; 75, p. 131). 13 The test seemed to be the shortest test available which was considered valid and reliable. The test limited reading level as a major factor for student success by using only those questions which had a correlation of .25 or less with reading ability, and further used only those questions which had a reliability coefficient of between .50 and .95 (75, p. 132). Scope of the Study There were several factors which determined the scope of this study. The students were only from the state of Ohio and most of them were selected from the southwest quadrant of the state when programs were avail- able in that area. Not all programs were evaluated, just three types: two-phase, three-phase simulation, and four-phase. In each of the three programs, the programs with multimedia and two programs without multimedia within each of the three programs were selected. Only cognitive knowledge was evaluated. Accident and violation records, driving ability or attitude mea- sures were not used. These evaluation criteria may or may not be better evaluation tools. The results of this study, therefore, are limited to these three programs in Ohio and further, only with regards to levels of cognitive knowledge. l4 Generalizability Since the pupils involved in the study were from within normal classrooms in the state of Ohio and since there was no reason to believe that there was any differ- ence between the southwest quadrant, from.which most of the schools were selected, and the rest of the state, it can be assumed that the findings of this study will hold for all similar driver education programs in the state. There was also no reason to believe that the school selected was greatly different from other schools with similar programs. Therefore, the results of this study can be generalized to those similar programs. Limitations of the Study There are several means to evaluate driver per- formance. This study, however, did not attempt to eval- uate or to predict driver performance, but only to look at one aspect of the driver--his/her cognitive knowledge as measured by a 50 question written exam. Cognitive knowledge cannot predict safe and efficient operation of an automobile.. Due to the cost of the Driver Performance Measure- ment on-road evaluation of driver performance developed at Michigan State University, it or a similar measure was ruled out as a viable alternative for use in this study (30). The years involved in the study of accident and 15 violation records made the evaluation of such records undesirable. Further, some questions have also been raised in regard to accident and violation records because of the inconsistency of both reporting and con- victions. The main objection to this type of evaluation is the extremely small percentage of the driving popula- tion involved each year. The use of only 50 questions on the written exam might be considered too few to measure adequately the students' knowledge of critical and most critical driving knowledge as defined in the Human Resources Research Organization (HumRRO) report. However, the 246 questions (Special Test Set) suggested by Pollack and McDole in the University of Michigan Study, if used, would have increased the number of schools needed as well as make reading level a major concern (64, p. 6). The 50 questions were selected from Pollack and ‘McDole's class "C" item bank. Although the 50 questions ‘were not all directly taken from the 246 queStions in the Special Test Set, they were similar in content area covered. This study was not an experimental study, but rather an evaluation of cognitive knowledge of existing programs. Thus, the results of the study are limited to what was determined to be the best program under present 16 conditions. Recommendations on which program should be used in the future could not be made. Although objectivity is stressed throughout this study, it was impossible to eliminate the author's bias towards driver education--not so much in the conducting of the study, but in terms of what was studied and what questions were asked. These reflected the author's ideas and thoughts. Definition of Terms Class "C" Test Items Class "C" referred to test items developed by Pollack and McDole to measure cognitive knowledge needed for operation of an automobile (64). Cognitive Knowledge "Cognitive knowledge involves the recall of speci- fics and universals, the recall of methods and processes, or the recall of a pattern, structure, or setting. For measurement purposes, the recall situation involves little more than bringing to mind the appropriate material" (9, p. 201). GPA Grade point average was the average of all grades received at the high school level. Low GPA as used in 17 this study was 0.0 to 1.5, middle GPA was 1.6 to 2.5, and high GPA was 2.6 to 4.0. Multimedia Multimedia integrates audio and visual materials into a teaching/learning unit to produce a more complete teaching system. It makes use of instructional materials incorporating immediate feedback to and from the student. Program One A basic driver education course consisting of at least 36 hours of classroom instruction and six hours of in-car behind-the-wheel instruction. Program Two A three-phase driver education program consisting of at least 36 hours of classroom instruction, six to twelve hours of simulation instruction, and four to six hours of in-car behind-the-wheel instruction. Program Three A four-phase driver education program consisting of at least 36 hours of classroom instruction, six to twelve hours of range (multiple-car, off-street driving facility) instruction, six to twelve hours of simulation, and two to six hours of in-car behind-the-wheel instruc- tion. 18 Range ”An off-street area designed and built to incor- porate a variety of realistic traffic situations where a number of cars are used simultaneously, under the super- vision of one of more teachers" (2, p. 247). Simulation "An electromechanical device designed to repre- sent the driver's compartment of the automobile, includ- ing typical controls and gauges. A programmed, group instructional system which employs student interaction with filmed driving environments" (2, p. 152). Special Test Set Two hundred and forty-six test questiOns chosen by Pollack and McDole as ideal questions for measurement of critical automobile driving knowledge test items, as defined by the HumRRO report (64, 46). Organization of Remaining Chapters The succeeding chapters are organized in the following manner: Chapter II is a review of related literature. Chapter III deals with the procedure and ‘methodology used in this study. Chapter IV presents the analysis of data, and Chapter V summarizes the study and discusses the conclusions and recommendations of this study. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE This chapter presents a review of the literature related to this evaluation of driver education programs. The review of the literature was divided into four areas which dealt with evaluation measures in driver education and training, educational evaluation in other subject areas which used similar formats, driver education evalu- ation studies, and evaluation measures used in the evalu- ation of driver education effectiveness. Driver Education Evaluation Measures The use of a written test for evaluation and pre- diction of driver performance has been discussed since evaluation of driver education began. The concept of driver education has been to pro- vide knowledge, skills, and proper attitudes to those participants, which.would in turn make them safer drivers. The major problem has been the evaluation of these three areas. There were varied ideas about which form of evaluation should be utilized. The federal government has ‘mandated that state driver education programs be evaluated 19 20 on the bases of reduction of accidents, and the resulting deaths and injuries of those drivers completing the pro- gram. Although reduction of accidents is one of the rationales for driver education, evaluation on these criteria alone cannot be valid. William Lybrand stated: Prior studies using accident data as criteria have been conducted ex post facto, and in that situation there are no completely valid statisti— cal techniques for separating the influence of driver education from the independent influence of the personal characteristics of the drivers studied. With few exceptions, official accident records were used as criteria in prior evaluative studies and these are known to be incomplete, but that further, there are serious questions about the adequacy of accident data as comprehensive, reli- able, and valid measures of driving performance proficiency, as distinguished from their obvious validity as one measure of highway transportation system effectiveness (41, p. vi). Although accident records should not be used as a sole measure of driver education effectiveness, they can be a useful tool in the overall evaluation. Accord- ing to R. L. Tack: An important measure of the effectiveness of driver education is a student's record of acci- ‘dents and violations. Although driving records may be the most significant measure of driver education's effectiveness, they are only one index of a student's driving behavior. A sub- stantial portion of a student's driving behavior is not measured if only the variables obtained from driving records are considered. Such vari- ables as knowledge gain, the student's perception of the course, classroom and behind—the-wheel activities and total hours of instruction received, as well as various pre-driver education, driver 21 education and post-driver education factors should be considered when examining the effec- tiveness of driver education (75, p. 3). LeRoy Dunn felt that reduction of crashes must be a goal of driver education. He stated that: In order to survive as a viable highway safety countermeasure, rather than just a training service for society, traffic safety education programs must meet the challenge of reducing crashes or their negative consequences (27, p. 18). William Mann felt that "knowledge of both manmade and natural laws are important to a driver, but, the most important area of concentration and evaluation is on the driver's attitude" (42). Richard Bishop reiterated the "we drive as we are" concept. His emphasis was also on developing the attitudes of the young drivers to make them safer vehicle operators (6). Bishop further stated that knowledge alone would not insure safe driving: Any perceptive driver education teacher real- izes the distinction between knowing and behaving. For example, a student may learn the traffic laws and score 100 percent on a written test covering this topic, and then violate many of the same laws following completion of the course. This does not mean that learning facts is not useful in develop- ing proper attitudes and sound judgment. Exposing students to factual information is an excellent beginning when attempting to modify behavior. How- ever, facts alone will not change behavior, so our challenge lies in using facts to influence the understandings, perceptions, emotions, attitudes, values, and motivations which determine behavior (7). 22 For evaluation of programs and students' learn- ing, written tests can be useful. According to Bishop: Pencil and paper tests, although possessing many weaknesses as an evaluation instrument can (1) stimulate learning; (2) furnish useful infor- mation for counseling individual pupils; and (3) evaluate teaching and learning. Test responses and erroneous thinking that may involve the learner in an accident if not corrected (5). Francis Kenel further stated regarding knowledge evaluation: The attitude approach must be replaced by acceptance of the fact that the primary objec- tive of driver education is to assist students in acquiring those abilities needed to travel efficiently and effectively from one point to another with minimal risk of collision, within the highway transportation system. The accomplishment of this objective involves providing students with the required tools and knowledge to perform the driving task. What are we talking about when we say driver error--poor attitudes, social disabilities? Not really. Research indicates that of those cases in which the driver contributed the primary cause, 72% resulted from inappropriate decisions. Drivers did not know what to do. They did not know relevant factors to consider when processing information. Perceptual failure contributed to over 66 percent of the cases. Either the driver did not want to see, or did not know what or how to look for information in the first place. In an additional 33 percent of the cases, drivers took inappropriate evasive action. Inappropriate evasive action includes everything from locking the brakes to absolutely no response at all. It is generally agreed that the prevention of such failure is possible through education or training. If one believes that accident reduction is a realistic goal of driver education, then it is essential that we assist drivers to develop those abilities needed to drive in an efficient-effective manner. It is critical that they develop the 23 ability to make decisions necessary to adequate management of time and space within a stream of moving traffic. This necessitates that they be taught how and what to look for and how to clas- sify information into meaningful data. This must be based on knowledge and anticipation of what to expect (39, pp. 7, 8). Does knowledge produce safer drivers? There was a consensus that it did. W. Worick said: Knowledge is the foundation for understanding and the springboard for the development of desir- able attitudes toward safe behavior. Ideally every individual should learn and appreciate safety rules; unfortunately this is not always the case. The dramatic reduction in industrial accidents in recent years is the direct result of safety pro- grams designed to educate the worker and, in turn, develop good habits and attitudes. We are realiz- ing similar results with driver education programs. Every technological advance brings new hazards with which man must cope; thus education and the develop- ment of knowledge are a never ending process. Ade- quate knowledge enables the individual to recognize and evaluate dangerous situations (89, p. 24). Richard Kaywood commented that: Some young drivers place too much emphasis on the physical part of the driving task. They think skillful car-handling is good driving. These basic skills must be learned well enough to become almost automatic. But it is the mental part of the driv- ing task that gets most drivers into trouble. To make decisions, drivers use their stored knowledge and the facts gathered from the present traffic situation. . . . Knowledge and skill are essential in the safe operation of motor vehicles. The prediction is based on this information and also on the driver's stored knowledge (38, pp. 7, 8). Kenel again observed: The driving process would be impossible if every time we drive were the first time. What saves us is our ability to store information. we develop a "memory bank" of driving facts which help us predict correctly (39, p. 9). 24 James Aaron gave further credence to the consensus that knowledge produces safer drivers: Experience and information stored in one's memory can have a profound influence on what is perceived, how it is interpreted and therefore what decision is made. In the operation of motor vehicles there are two classes of stored or retained information. These are the long-term and short- term.retention of information. To estimate or predict relationships to other highway users, stored memory banks of traffic laws, rules and regulations, vehicle dynamics, and driver behavior must be called upon. Concepts and strate- gies are recalled for the more complex decision- making problem—solving situation. Further, the performance of life-saving evasive actions under emergency conditions is based on stored informa- tion (2, p. 37). Aaron further stated: Essentially the question must be asked, what does a safe driver really need to know? Further, how can young drivers be taught so that behavioral patterns can be influenced to the point they will become reliable and competent drivers? A systems- analysis approach may be used to define the scope and breadth that driver education must take in order to meet the challenge it faces. Using such an approach, the content of the driver education curriculum should be selected on the basis of those perceptions, judgments, decisions, and actions required to perform the driving task safely and efficiently (2, p. 43). One study conducted in California by Dell Dreyer asserted: Only the written test appears to have some predictive validity. . . . Better performance on the written test tended to be associated with fewer accidents and convictions. The relationship between written test score and subsequent driving record for first time applicants showed that as the number of errors increases, the frequency of accidents and con- victions increase moderately. Those with seven 25 or more errors had more than twice as many acci- dents as those with no errors (25, p. 6). These findings seemed to be consistent with other research. In another study, conducted by Carpenter in California, similar results were found: For all DMV test forms combined, correla- tions of total test scores with driving record variables were all significant, indicating renewal applicants with better prior driving records obtained higher test scores (14, p. 2). Although written knowledge tests are only one means of evaluating driver performance, it is an important and useful tool for evaluating the success of driver edu- cation. Educational Evaluations In reviewing evaluations of educational programs, Michigan's Educational Assessment Program seemed to be similar, if not better than other states' evaluations. In the introduction to the program it was stated that, ”For the purpose of the Michigan Assessment Program, assessment will be defined as the gathering of informa- tion about student achievement which is useful for educa- tional decision making" (55, p. 1). Obtaining copies of evaluation or assessment reports from Ohio, as well as from other states, however, was a problem. Michigan's evaluation reports which were studied were more in terms of results of the evaluations, or the rationale for them, than in the methodology of the 26 evaluation; therefore, not pertinent to this design (48, 49, 51, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55). All the programs evaluated were as a total program. They were not divided into dif- ferent teaching methods to determine if one method was better than another. Although this idea was suggested for evaluation of science programs (55, p. 25) the eval- uation was not conducted in this manner. Several means were employed in Michigan's Educa- tional Assessment Program to evaluate these programs. Written tests were used in vocabulary, reading compre- hension, English usage, and arithmetic. In each case, standardized, norm-referenced tests were used. In addi- tion, an attitude survey and a questionnaire of the child's home environment were used (55, p. 3). The method used in these evaluations was to select or develop objectives for each subject and grade level, and to evaluate accomplishment of those objectives by students (55, p. 11). Although the goal was to eval- uate all students, where tests had to be individually administered, samples of approximately 500 students were used (55, p. 23). Individual student success rate ranged from 48 percent to 87 percent accomplishment in all subject areas (52). The success rate on successful completion 27 of the objectives ranged from 60 percent to 80 percent (51, 52, 54). Driver Education Evaluation Studies Several methods have been employed over the years to evaluate driver education. Usually, these eval- uations were ex post facto type studies, where data gathered for a different purpose was later used to eval- uate the effectiveness of the driver education program. In some instances, these evaluations took place many years later. Until recently, experimental research in driver education had not been attempted. The reason for this was that it was too serious a matter to have a true con- trol group. If the hypothesis of the experimental research was to validate driver education as a viable means of accident reduction, then conversely, the hypoth- esis would postulate that a control was being subjected to greater risks of being involved in a vehicle accident with resultant death, injury, or property damage. The DeKalb County, Georgia, program fell short of being a true experimental study, but it was an attempt to meet the requirements of an experiment (85). The following driver education evaluation studies were reviewed on two factors, format and criterion (or 28 criteria) used to measure success. Results applicable to this study were noted. The Program Research in Driver Education (PRIDE) was conducted in Iowa. The sc0pe of the study was state- wide. The purpose was to evaluate programs in Iowa uti- lizing different phases. The study looked at three different program types: two-phase, three-phase simulation, and four-phase. Two other factors included were (1) when the program was offered (summer, fall, or spring) and (2) whether or not the school was urban or rural. The study used a 50 question pre-post test to evaluate the cognitive knowledge of students. (The same test used in this Ohio evaluation study.) The students were also tested on the Driving Attitude Survey developed by Schuster and Guilford, success on the driver license exam, and accident and violation records for the two- year period following completion of driver education (75). Some conclusions and recommendations in the PRIDE study relative to this evaluation study were (1) regular school programs showed significant gains in knowledge over summer programs, (2) GPA was highly correlated with knowledge test success, and (3) two-phase programs seemed to be superior to the three- and four-phase programs in 29 reduction of accidents. Overall, as in most studies of a behavioral science, no clearcut conclusions were made other than a need for increased parental involvement (75). The California Driver Training Evaluation Study by Margaret Hubbard Jones looked at two programs: two- phase and three—phase simulation taught by both commer- cial and the public schools. Evaluation of programs was conducted by using three criteria: teacher evaluation, license exam records, and accident and violation records. No significant dif- ference was found on any of the three variables. The three-phase simulation program cost an average of eighteen dollars more per pupil than the two-phase with no signifi- cant differences over the two-phase programs (12, p. 11). Several criticisms of this study were noted: (1) All simulation programs were conducted during the regular school day, whereas the two-phase programs were conducted after school, and (2) of the population of 158 schools selected, 34 refused to participate and 13 were finally used (12, p. 11). Further, neither pupils nor instructors were randomly selected (12, p. 9). Michigan's Driver Education Evaluation Project, conducted under the direction of Kara Schmitt, was also 30 an evaluation of existing programs in the state of Michigan. The study examined five different programs: two- phase, two-phase range, three-phase range, three-phase simulation, and four-phase. All students in the state who took driver education during the 1976-77 school year were tested. Evaluation was conducted by use of 75 question written tests developed from Michigan's driver education performance objectives. Some schools were randomly selected for administration of a pre-test as well as the post-test (30). Findings indicated that the after-school and summer programs were significantly better in producing cognitive knowledge than were the regular school-day programs. Teacher age was correlated with test scores. The older the teacher, the lower the scores. Part-time teachers had better results than full-time teachers, and three-phase simulation and three-phase range programs ‘were the best of the five. A driver education evaluation study directed by William Cole at Texas A&M University used an experimental approach to his evaluation. He developed a five-phase program using multimedia as the first phase. The com- parison was based on several variables of the students 31 participating. A written test and an on-road evaluation (The McGlade Road Test) were used (45). The study concluded that there were no signifi- cant correlations between any of the variables such as length of programs or IQ test score. The only variable which significantly correlated was entry-level skills of the students in both cognitive and psychomotor learning. The recommendation was to assure better assessment of [entry level skills and to build upon them. In a study conducted by Charles E. McDaniel, four selected programs were evaluated. They were two- phase, three-phase simulation, three-phase range, and four-phase. The study also examined program length as well as sex of drivers. Three criterion measures were employed: driving knowledge, driving performance (45), and a traffic analy- sis written test. Significant gains were noted in knowl- edge test scores for all programs. Robert E. Gustafson conducted a comparison between a three-phase range program and a four-phase. Three mea- sures of success were used: knowledge, attitude,and skill. Although all groups showed significant improve- ment in knowledge, there was no significant difference between groups on the measure. No significant improve- ment was shown for either group on the Mann Personal 32 Attitude Survey (43), although improvement was shown on the Siebrecht Attitude Scale (70, pp. 2-4). There was a significant correlation between IQ and success on the knowledge test and on the Siebrecht Attitude Scale. The DeKalb County, Georgia, project under the direction of Jack Weaver was the first real effort at an experimental study using a control group in driver education. The groups were randomly assigned from 18,000 students (volunteers) into three different programs. One was the control group with no formal training. The second group received the Safe Performance Curriculum (86, p. 18) of 72 clock hours, and the third group, the Pre-Driver Licensing curriculum of 20 clock hours of instruction. This entire project was designed to provide ideal conditions for the instruction of the Safe Per- formance Curriculum. The evaluation has not yet been completed, but will be conducted by using accident and violation records, as well as intermediate criteria of knowledge and attitudes. Driver Education Phases This section deals with some of the phases which are used in different driver education programs. It is sometimes difficult to separate these methods of teaching 33 into distinctively separate phases. Any teacher, in any given subject area, uses more than one method of instruc- tion for his/her pupils. Driver education methods of instruction have been so divided because the laws govern- ing driver education have set the format of instruction by hours for each of the methods of phases. Further, evaluation of total programs was often used to infer the worth of a particular phase (or phases) used in that pro- gram. Therefore, this section is somewhat of a continu- ation of the preceding section which dealt with driver education evaluation studies as a whole. This section will briefly look at three methods of instruction in the context of the driver education program--simulation, range, and the use of the multi- media response system. The use of driving simulators in high school driver education programs in Ohio was increasing despite the high initial cost to purchase a unit. One obvious reason was the rising cost of vehicles, insurance, gaso- line, and instructors. A simulator can save a school (or schools) money if scheduled properly. Even though simulation can reduce the cost of driver education, one must still consider whether they were better for instruction of students than another method or methods. 34 In a study conducted in California, there was no significant difference in student performance between those students who were instructed with simulation and those who were not (12). One significant difference was that the cost of simulation was $18 per pupil. One explanation for this might be that all the simulation programs were in-school programs, whereas the others were all after-school instruction. A leading authority on the use of simulators, Richard Bishop, had several comments on simulation for high school driver education programs: A simulator is a laboratory device that enables the operator to reproduce under test conditions phenomena likely to occur in actual performance. Simulators are helping to meet the quality- quantity cost challenge facing driver education. Research with both aircraft and driving simu- lators used for education purposes strongly sup- ports the premise that a transfer of learning occurs from the simulator environment to the actual task (8). Richard A. Meyerhoff, Chairman, Driver Education Department of Waterloo Public Schools, Waterloo, Iowa, explained that simulation can be effective as a tool in helping the student take in and process data from the driving environment. He also called for updated simula- tion films: 35 I am firmly convinced that a new generation of simulation films can save valuable in-car time and at the same time produce more competent drivers (47, p. 12). He further put the burden on the teacher for making simulation successful: In the simulator setting much teacher inter- vention is needed to make things fly (47, p. 12). Vernon P. Sample pointed out that the idea of simulation was not unique to driver education and that it was very successful in other fields as well: The U.S. Air Force and all commercial airlines have for years capitalized on the simulator as a training aid in preparing their pilots. Submarine and tug boat crews receive simulator training. Atomic power plant personnel, diesel locomotive engineers and, of course, astronauts all have received simulator training, to name but a few, and all with very positive results. Why then has the driver training simulator alone been singled out from all others and subjected to so many chal- lenges as to its value as a training aid and its cost effectiveness? (68, p. 29) Sample also noted that two areas must be closely examined: the instructor and equipment utilization. Sample added: A poorly prepared and motivated instructor teaching in a simulator that has been poorly scheduled leads the most objective observer to the obvious conclusion that this type of train- ing is ineffective and costly, and the simulator takes the bum rap (68, p. 29). Glenn Winningham pointed out the difficulty of trying to separate one phase of a program as the cause of success or failure of that program: 36 Simulation represents only one phase of a multi-phase curriculum and must be coordinated with other instructional phases if optimum bene- fits are to be realized. Therefore, communica- tion between all driver education staff members is vitally important to assure consistency and positive transfer throughout multiple-phase learning activities (88, p. 28). Ming H. Land explained that some of the teaching methods used in simulators are the same as those used in the instruction of industrial arts (or other subject areas) and have been for some time (40). One of these techniques was to have the students "drill" a procedure or skill. Land observed that John Locke once proposed the method of teaching in habit formation which was a matter of repetition. "In other words," Land elaborated, "it was a matter of 'drill' in the time-honored sense of the term." Land went on to indicate the various uses of technological advances: One of the concommitant benefits of the tech- nological developments in the area of educational methodology has been the development of media and techniques to improve the teaching-learning pro- cess. A representative sample of the recent inno- vative practices that incorporate media and teach- ing methodology to achieve specified behavioral goals are reported by Buffer to include simulation, micro-teaching, video tape, programmed learning, cybernetics, and computer assisted instruction (40, p. 27). There appears to be a continually expanding interest in programmed instruction. Programmed texts and lessons presented via a teaching machine are used in a variety of programs and courses. 37 Some schools already have programmed learning centers and some have developed multimedia pro- grams. . . . Programmed instruction which utilizes multimedia instructional materials and diversified methods of teaching will become an important fea- ture in American education . . . (40, p. 79). There were further benefits from the use of simu- lation in a teaching program, according to Land: Simulation and games also may have special values for motivating low achieving students as they add a new dimension of reality to teach- 1ng. . . . The climate today favors educating capable students more than was done a few years ago; also slower learners are provided greater assistance with their learning problems than ever before. Various individualized instruction plans and approaches on all levels of education have been tried . . . (40, p. 54). Rummel and Pine studied the effectiveness of simulation against non-simulation in teaching numerical control concepts. Experience indicated that games have high potential for stimulating interest and for motivating students. Plans for capitalizing on this motivation need to be developed and put into practice (40, p. 68). Land went on to caution that simulation or any other teaching method was not a panacea: Susskind fears that education may prove to be one of the fields in which the province of tech- nology has been oversold. He advises educators to take advantages of technology in every field of human endeavor and, at the same time, to recog- nize the limitations of it (40, p. 2). The application of new media and advance tech- nology to educational method will be developed gradually into a large scale in the future. The process, however, will be an evolution one con- trolled by rational decisions. It is not and will not become a revolution in teaching methods . . (40, p. 78). 38 Another phase of driver education instruction used sparingly in Ohio was the off-road driving facility or range. Similar to simulation, it can reduce cost over a long period of time, but the high initial cost pro- hibited many schools from utilizing this learning tool. Further, teachers who were properly trained in the use of a range were at a premium. Several studies have attempted to determine if the inclusion of the range program was as effective or more effective than another form of teaching. A study was conducted in North Carolina to determine if range programs resulted in a better score on driver license exam than did non-range programs (19). The conclusion was that they did not. In fact, it appeared that the non-range programs did better than the range programs. Although the study was relatively well designed, it did have two problems. Driver license examiners were used to evaluate a driver education program, and in the analysis of variance test, equal variance was assumed but~ggll size was not equal. Further, two— and three- way interactions were also significant. To attempt to draw any conclusions from this form of analysis was not warranted. In a study of range and simulation programs con- ducted by Robert O. Nolan at Michigan State University, 39 no significant differences were found between the two methods of instruction in driving skill, attitude, or knowledge (60). In a study discussed elsewhere in this chapter, Robert E. Gustafson found that a range program was better than a combination range and simulation program on driving skill. No significant differences were found between general driving knowledge, specific driving knowledge, driving attitudes, or on the traffic problems and road problems sections of the final read test (35). In a literature search conducted by Forrest M. Council, et a1. (2), several studies were noted in the use of range facilities for advanced driver education programs: In a small but well controlled evaluation of their program, the GM staff used 60 patrol offi- cers from the Oakland County Michigan Sheriff's Department. One group received the 8 hour train- ing program and one group was the control. The trained group exhibited a 50% reduction in acci- dents and an 80% reduction in cost per accident (20, p. 17). Peevey (1975) suggests that, "The goal of any emergency instructional program should be to give the student cognitive and manipulative information and skills, experience to reduce the trauma of the emergency,skills related to his general driving performance (some may prefer defensive driving), and optimum vehicle maintenance procedures (20, p. 17). Existing literature concerning advanced driver education programs which include training in emer- gency skill indicates that there is (l) a set of consensus maneuvers which are employed, with some 4O modification, almost universally and (2) there is a lack of well controlled evaluation and conclu- sive findings concerning the effectiveness of these programs (20, p. 17). There seems to be no conclusive proof that the range phase of driver education was either beneficial or harmful to driver education students in high school driver education programs (20, p. 17). The last method of instruction to be covered is the use of a multimedia response system. The state of Ohio has been installing more and more of these systems each year. R. Samuel Parker stated that there were many advantages to the use of multimedia in the driver educa- tion classroom (63): Advantages to the teacher: Provides uniformity and flexibility to the classroom curriculum. Provides immediate feedback on individual student and group performance. Captures real life traffic situations and attitudinal role-playing sequences which gener- ate good group discussion opportunities. Brings scientific equipment and experiments via photography to the driver education classroom. Gives the teacher an opportunity to concen- trate on the highest functions of his profession-- to counsel, guide, and tutor according to the identified individual and group needs. Provides a communication system that has almost limitless applications. It can be used to administer tests, take roll, receive and reflect opinions, value judgments and feelings, test levels of comprehension, etc. 41 Advantages for the student: Provides continuous opportunities to interact with the programmed lessons and through peer group discussions. Provides learning experiences in a dynamic way that is challenging, yet holds attention. Provides immediate reinforcement for correct responses. Provides for active participation rather than passive, "told what to do" observations. Provides private response communication with the teacher (63, p. 16). Parker emphasized that multimedia use was only effective when used by a competent instructor. As did Land, Parker noted that multimedia was not a panacea. Gail H. Silver expressed the same basic feelings about the use of multimedia in the classroom. "The system is flexible; the only restriction is the teacher's creativity." Silver also pointed out that multimedia was not skill oriented as was a simulator; it was knowledge oriented: When it comes to developing actual skill, our simu- lators are more effective, and naturally some amount of on-street driving is needed. However, it is hard to evaluate just how effective a multimedia response system is because of simulation (71, p. 11). Further, according to Silver, the biggest advan- tage was the number of students who could be instructed at one time: The biggest advantage, in my estimation, would be the number of students that can be taught effec- tively at one time. We know from our evaluation that it is an effective means of providing large 42 group instruction, and that in this approach, each student is attentive and responsive. He has to be. The system is structured that way (71, p. 11). The only study found which tried to isolate the effect of multimedia instruction on driver education instruction was conducted by Charles E. McDaniel at Michigan State University. He found no significant dif- ference attributable to the AEtna Drivocator (multimedia) instruction (44). Summary This chapter presented a review of the literature related to this evaluation of driver education programs. A major problem in driver education has been the evaluation of its programs. It has been difficult to reach a consensus as to which type of evaluation was more valid and reliable. While the federal government man- dated that state driver education programs be evaluated on the basis of reduction of accidents, and the resulting deaths and injuries of those drivers completing the pro- gram, most experts in the field asserted that evaluation on these criteria alone cannot be valid. There was a general consensus that knowledge pro- duced safer drivers, and that written knowledge tests were an important tool for evaluating the success of driver education. 43 Evaluation measures cannot minimize the impor— tance of attitude, of the "we drive as we are" concept as stated by Bishop. Michigan's Educational Assessment Program used written tests to evaluate students in vocabulary, reading comprehension, English usage, and arithmetic. In each case, standardized, non-referenced tests were used, as well as an attitude survey and a questionnaire on the child's home environment. Until recently, most driver education evaluation studies were ex post facto type studies. More recently, the Program Research in Driver Education PRIDE conducted a statewide study in Iowa. The purpose was to evaluate programs in Iowa utilizing different phases. Some con— clusions and recommendations of the PRIDE study were pertinent to this evaluation study. Other studies and evaluations looked at a variety of program types, attempting to ascertain if one program was more effective in driver education than another or others. Findings and conclusions varied, as did the methods of evaluations. General summarization of these evaluations was not possible. Jones' study found no significant difference in driver education taught by commercial and by public schools. 44 Schmitt found that summer programs were signifi- cantly better in producing cognitive knowledge, that older teachers were less effective than younger ones, that part-time teachers had better results than full- time teachers, and that three-phase simulation and three-phase range programs were best of the five tested. Cole's study recommended better assessment of entry level skills for driver education. McDaniel conducted a study of four programs and concluded that significant gains were noted in knowledge scores for all programs. The study by Gustafson found a significant cor- relation between IQ and success on the knowledge test and on the Siebrecht Attitude Scale. The DeKalb County, Georgia, project has not been completed, but was using accident and violation records to test three groups of students. Simulation, range, and the use of multimedia response systems have been used and evaluated by numerous teachers of driver education. Generally, studies con- cluded that simulation was beneficial when scheduled and used by a competent instructor, and that multimedia had many advantages for students of various ability and learn- ing levels. Conclusions as to the effectiveness of the range in driver education were varied. There seemed to be no conclusive proof that the range phase of driver 45 education was an effective teaching/learning tool--or that it was not. The next chapter will describe the design and methodology used to complete the study. It will describe in detail the selection of Ohio, data collection, contact letters, telephone contacts, testing procedures, methods of data analysis, and organization of data. CHAPTER III DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY This chapter outlines the design and methodology used in the study. Topics described in detail are selec- tion of Ohio as the location for the study, selection of specific schools in Ohio, data collection, contact letters, telephone contacts, testing procedures, methods of data analysis, and organization of data. Selection of Ohio The state of Ohio had not evaluated its driver education programs in recent years and recognized the need for such a procedure. When personnel in the state were contacted concerning an evaluation of the state's driver education program, the cooperation and assistance from the state Department of Education were excellent. Further, the author was from the state of Ohio and was familiar with driver education in that state. The state of Michigan was considered because of the author's immediate proximity, but was eliminated because of recent studies which had been conducted by the Michigan Depart- ment of Education. 46 47 Selection of Schools Seventeen schools were selected for use in this study. Twelve schools were selected for the evaluation, and five schools were selected as alternates. These seventeen schools were randomly selected from a list supplied by Mr. Larry Cathell of the Ohio Department of Education, Driver Education Section. The schools selected had driver education programs which fit into the six cate- gories selected for evaluation in this study. When pos- sible, the schools on the list were in the southwest quadrant of Ohio. Of the schools selected, fourteen were in the southwest quadrant, three were not. Further, of the twelve schools evaluated, ten were from the southwest quadrant and two were not. Use of schools not in the southwest quadrant was necessitated by the insufficient number of suitable programs in the southwest quadrant. Data Collection The collection of data for this study consisted of three phases: initial contact with the school by letter; telephone contact with the principal, driver education supervisor, and/or the driver education teacher; and the school visitation for testing. Contact Letters Three different letters were sent to the schools which participated in this study. The first letter was 48 sent to each superintendent, explaining the study, how the school was selected, and requesting his cooperation in completing the study (see Appendix A for a sample form). The second letter was sent to the building principal, explaining the study and asking his coopera- tion. The letter informed him that he would be contacted by telephone to arrange the details of the visitation (see Appendix B for a sample of the form sent to the principal). The third letter was sent to the building prin- cipals of the alternate schools selected, informing them of their selection, and that they would be contacted if their school was needed for the completion of the study (see Appendix C for a sample form). A letter from.Mr. Larry Cathell of the Ohio Department of Education was to be included in the letter to the school principal. However, this communication was not received, and thus was not used. This omission caused some confusion as to whether the state of Ohio or the author was conducting the study. Contact was made by telephone to each building principal. The author clarified that this study was conducted for a doctoral dissertation with the cooperation of the Ohio Department of Education. There were no further problems in this regard. 49 Telephone Contacts Approximately one week after the contact letters were mailed, a telephone call was made to each principal to determine a convenient date and time for the visita- tion and testing procedure. Two of the schools contacted had changed programs, necessitating the use of alternate schools. Dates and times for the administration of the test were determined by the telephone contact (see Appendix D for a sample form). The principals were asked to have ready at the time of the visitation the GPA's for the students to be tested, and the cost per pupil of the driver education program. Two schools requested and received further information before the administration of the test. In a few instances, further contact had to be made with the classroom teacher, driver education supervisor/coordinator, or supervisors of instruction to obtain information. These contacts were made as necessary by telephone. Two of the schools contacted had completed their spring programs, and arrangements were made to administer the test at the completion of the summer term. 50 Testing Procedure The testing procedure was consistent at all schools. Students at each school were informed of the reason for the evaluation. They were asked to do their best on the test, even though the results would not be used for grading purposes. The students were given a test booklet andzn1answer sheet. They were asked to put their name on the answer sheet in order to match the GPA and the test score of each student. The CPA and the test score were the only items of information kept by the author for the purposes of data analysis (see Appendix E for test and answer sheet). The tests were scored and the scores were recorded with GPA's, when available. Two schools did not have a record of student GPA's (see Appendix F for sample form). While the students were taking the test, the teachers were asked to read the test and to indicate the items which had been taught during the current term, as well as those which had not been taught at the time of the testing, but would be covered. They were also requested to indicate those items which they did not plan to teach by the completion of the current term. These data were also tabulated for later analyses (see Appendix G for sample form). Instructor and program data information were requested for use in the data analysis. Instructors 51 completed this information form while the students were taking the test (see sample form in Appendix H). Methods of Analysis Two methods of data analysis were used. One method, t-tests, were hand scored and analyzed based on an 80 percent success criterion. The other analyses were done by computer. These included three analyses of variance and six Pearson cor- relations. All analyses were conducted using a .05 alpha level when applicable. Organization of Data The dependent variable for the study was cogni- tive knowledge as measured by the PRIDE fifty question multiple-choice test. Three analyses were conducted to determine pro- gram effectiveness. In each of the three analyses a t-test was used in which the number of students who achieved 80 percent or more correct was compared to an 80 percent success criterion. In the first of these three analyses, all 326 student scores were used to determine if students tested in Ohio were achieving 80 percent success. The hypoth- eses to be tested were: 52 H0: U 3 80% H . 1. u > 80%. For the second analysis, program one (two-phase) used 102 student scores, program two (three-phase simu- lation) used 114 student scores, and in program three (four-phase) 110 student scores were used. Three separate t-tests were compared to an 80 percent success criterion. Hypotheses tested were: 0: uPl > 80% H1: UP]. < 800/0 HO: uPZ > 80% H1: uPZ < 80% HO: UPB > 800/0 H1: UP3 < 8070. The third analysis compared all 326 adjusted student scores. To determine each student's adjusted score, each test question that the individual instructors indicated not having covered in his class was eliminated from the total possible for his/her students. The per- centage correct for each student was then tabulated, compiled for all 326, and the percentage achieving 80 53 percent was again compared to the 80 percent success criterion. Hypotheses tested were: H uA 3 80% 0: H1: “A < 8070. Three different analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to analyze the data. The first ANOVA was a one—way, planned compari- son, to determine if one of the three programs tested was significantly better than another. Program one (two-phase) was contrasted with program two (three-phase simulation), and program two was contrasted with program three (four-phase). The hypotheses were: II 0 II O HO: $2 + uPl - uP2 For this ANOVA, eighteen students were randomly selected from each of the twelve schools in order to achieve equal cell size (see Figure 3.1). The second ANOVA conducted was to determine the relationship of programs and GPA's. The purpose was to see if one program was better than another for low, middle, or high GPA students. A two-way ANOVA was used with the following hypotheses: P1 M1 M2 C1 C2 C3 C4 54 P2 M1 M2 18 18 M1 M2 18 18 C5 C6 C7 C8 P3 M1 M2 l8 18 With Multimedia Without Multimedia Program Type Classroom Replications per cell 18 18 C9 C10 C11 C12 18 Figure 3.l.--ANOVA Design. l8 18 L 18 55 HO: a1 = O GPA HO: Bj = 0 Program HO: aBij = 0 Interaction. For this ANOVA, seven students in each of the nine cells were selected. The small number was due to the availability of these students (see Figure 3.2). The third ANOVA also used a two-way design to determine the relationship of multimedia and programs. Specifically to see if multimedia worked better in one program than in another. The hypotheses were: HO: ai = 0 Multimedia H0: Bj = 0 Program HO: aBij = 0 Interaction. A total of 174 student scores were used for this analysis, 29 in each cell (see Figure 3.3). A fourth ANOVA which was to be conducted was eliminated because the data did not allow for equal cell size. Several cells would have had no student scores in them, so it was decided not to attempt to analyze this research question. 56 P1 P2 P3 G1 R; R; R; G7- R7= R7= 7= 6 3 R7= R7= R7: P = Program Type G1 = GPA.0.0 - 1.5 CZ = GPA 1.6 - 2.5 G3 = GPA 2.6 - 4.0 R = Replications per cell Figure 3.2.--Two-way ANOVA Design. P1 P2 P3 M1 M2 M1 M2 M1 M2 = = = = R: = 29 29 29 29 29 29 P = Program Type M1 = With Multimedia M2 = Without Multimedia R = Replications per cell Figure 3.3.--Two-way ANOVA Design. 57 Six correlations were studied to determine the relationship of variables to students' success as measured by the written test. Student scores were correlated with cost of the program, education in driver education of the teacher, total hours of instruction in the program, number of students in the class, age of the teacher, and years of teaching experience in driver education. For these cor- relations, all 326 student scores were used. Summary This chapter outlined the design and methodology used in the study. Topics described were the selection of Ohio for the study, selection of schools to be used, data collection, contact letters, telephone contact, test procedures, methods of data analysis, and organization of data. The data and the results found in the study are organized and displayed in the following chapter. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF RESULTS Chapter III described the design and methodology used in the study. This chapter reports the findings of the twelve analyses which were conducted. The Sample Data Total Student Achievement The first analysis conducted was a t-test to determine if 80 percent of the students were achieving 80 percent or more correct on a test of cognitive knowl- ledge. The t-test used was designed for dichotomous data where t = x - u/s % /7T. Table 4.1 displays the data for this analysis. Of 326 students tested, 208 achieved 80 percent or more correct. This was 63 percent of the students who had satisfactory achievement. When this 63 percent was compared to an 80 percent success criterion, it was found to be significantly deficient. The results of the analy- sis indicated a t of 12.6. A.t of 1.658 was needed at the .05 alpha level to reject. Therefore, the hypothesis was rejected. 58 59 TABLE 4.1.--Student Distribution for t Analysis. Percent Correct N % 90-99 76 23 80-89 131 40 70-79 73 22 60-69 34 10 50-59 8 2 40-49 4 1 30-39 0 0 20-29 0 0 10-19 0 0 0- 9 0 0 Total 326 100* H1 p < 80 Critical t 12.6 1.658 @ .05 Tabled t * May not add to 100 percent due to rounding. 60 Individual Program Analysis The second analysis conducted consisted of three separate t-tests. These tests also used a t-test for dichotomous data. Each of the three programs was sepa- rately tested to determine if any one of the three was achieving the goal of 80 percent or more correct. Table 4.2 displays the data for program one (two- phase). This table indicates that 56 of 102 students, or 54 percent, achieved 80 percent or more. When 54 percent was compared to the 80 percent success criterion, it was determined to be significantly deficient. A t of 10.40 was found compared to a tabled t of 1.671 needed to reject at the .05 alpha level. The hypothesis was, therefore, rejected. Program two (three-phase simulation) had 75 of 114 (66 percent) of the students achieve 80 percent or better. When 66 percent was compared to 80 percent, it was found to be significantly deficient. A t of 6.67 was determined when a t of 1.671 was needed to reject at the .05 alpha level. Therefore, the hypothesis was rejected. Table 4.3 displays the data for this analysis. Table 4.4 displays the data for program three (four-phase). This table indicates that in program three (four-phase), 77 of 110 students, or 70 percent, achieved 80 percent or better. When 70 percent was compared to 80 percent, it was also significantly deficient. A t of 61 TABLE 4.2.--Analysis of Program One. Percent Correct N % 90-99 13 13 80-89 43 43 70-79 34 34 60-69 8 8 50-59 3 3 40-49 1 1 30-39 0 0 20-29 0 0 10-19 0 0 0- 9 0 0 Total 102 100* H0: 11 Z 80 H1: 1.] < 80 Critical t = 10.40 Tabled t = 1.671 @ .05 * May not add to 100 percent due to rounding. 62 TABLE 4.3.--Ana1ysis of Program Two. Percent Correct N % 90-99 26 23 80-89 48 42 70-79 25 22 60-69 12 11 50-59 2 2 40-49 1 1 30-39 0 0 20-29 0 0 10-19 0 0 0- 9 0 0 Total 114 100* H11 Ll < 80 6.67 1.671 @ .05 Critical t Tabled t * May not add to 100 percent due to rounding. 63 TABLE 4.4.--Analysis of Program Three. Percent Correct N % 90-99 37 34 80-89 40 36 70-79 14 13 60-69 14 13 50-59 3 3 40-49 2 2 30-39 0 0 20-29 0 0 10-19 0 0 0- 9 0 0 Total 110 100* H0: 11 3 80 H1: 11 < 80 5.0 1.671 @ .05 Critical t Tabled t * May not add to 100 percent due to rounding. 64 5.0 was tabulated when a t of 1.671 was needed to reject at the .05 alpha level. The hypothesis was, therefore, rejected. Teacher Item Analysis The third analysis, which also used a dichotomous data t-test, was to determine if students were success- fully learning what teachers indicated they were covering in the class. Table 4.5 displays the data for this analy- sis. Of 326 students tested, 231 achieved 80 percent or more correct. This figure computed to 71 percent. The 71 percent was compared to 80 percent and was found to be significantly deficient. The results of the analysis indicated a t of 7.89. A t of 1.658 was needed to reject at the .05 alpha level; therefore, the hypothesis was rejected. TABLE 4.5.--Analysis of Teacher Covered Material. N % Achievement 80% or greater 231 71 Achievement less than 80% 95 29 Total 326 100 H0: 11 Z 80 Critical t = 7.89 Tabled t = 1.658 @ .05 65 Comparison of Programs The fourth analysis was to determine if any one of three programs was significantly better than any of the other programs. A one-way, planned comparison, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was utilized. A significant difference was found between program two (three-phase simulation) and program three (four-phase) in favor of program three. A t of -2.504 was found when a t of i 1.980 was needed for significance at the .05 alpha level. Table 4.6 presents the data for contrast one. TABLE 4.6.--Contrast One: Program Two Contrasted with Program Three. Estimated Population Standard t t Value Error value ZDF Probability -1.9444 .7765 -2.504 213 .013 Tabled t = i 1.980 @ .05 * Contrast 1 HO: UPZ - UP3 = 0 H1: UPZ - UP3 75 0 Critical t -2.504 Tabled t = i 1.985 @ .05 H0: rejected * P2 = Program Two; P3 = Program Three. 66 No significant difference was found between pro- gram one (two-phase) and program two (three-phase simula- tion). A t of .0972 was found when a t of i 1.980 was needed for significance at the .05 alpha level. Table 4.7 presents the data for contrast two. TABLE 4.7.--Contrast Two: Program One Contrasted with Program Two. Estimated 213 Population Standard t t Value Error value 'DF Probability .0972 .7765 .125 213 .900 Tabled t = i 1.980 @ .05 * Contrast 2 H uPl - uP2 = O 02 H1: UPI - UPZ # 0 Critical t .125 Tabled t = i 1.980 @ .05 HO: not rejected * P1 = Program One; P2 = Program Two. GPA/Program Effect The second ANOVA was conducted to measure if one one of the three programs was significantly better than another with low, middle, or high GPA students. 69 explained in the first ANOVA conducted which concluded that program three was better than program one or program tWO. Two-way interaction (aBij) was not significant. An F of 1.192 was tabulated when an F of 3.07 was needed for significance at the .05 alpha level. Data for the two-way ANOVA where H0: Bj = 0 was rejected is presented in Table 4.9. Variable Correlations The final six research questions were correla- tions between student score and six variables. The data for these correlations are contained in Table 4.10. Three program variables and three teacher variables were selected to be correlated. The three program variables were the cost per pupil, total hours of instruction, and number of students in the class. The teacher variables were teacher age, their hours of education in driver education, and their years of teaching driver education. Student score was moderately correlated with cost of the program. A correlation coefficient (r) of .1737 was found. An "r" of .40 was needed to determine a significant relationship. Student score was not significantly correlated with hours of instruction in the program. An "r" of 70 TABLE 4.9.--Two-way ANOVA Table. Source of Sum of Mean Significance Variation Squares DF Square F of F Multimedia 42.618 1 42.618 2.057 .153 Program 358.259 2 179.129 8.645 .001 Interaction 49.380 2 24.690 1.192 .306 Error 3501.959 169 20.722 Total 174 22.706 HO: ai = 0 Multimedia H0 not rejected HO: Bj = 0 Program H0 rejected HO: dBij = 0 Interaction HO not rejected Tabled F Interaction Tabled F Programs Tabled F Multimedia 3.92 3.07 3.07 71 TABLE 4.10.--Correlations Between Student Scores and Related Variables. Variable *Value of rxy Cost of Program .1737 Hours of Program -.0978 Class Size .0340 Teacher Education -.0729 Teacher Age .0103 Years Teaching .0043 % 7Significant r = .40. -.0978 was found, when an "r" of .40 was necessary for significance. Student score was not significantly correlated with class size. An "r" of .0340 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed for significance. Student score was not significantly correlated with the instructors' education in driver education. An ”r" of -.0729 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed for significance. Student score was not significantly correlated with the instructors' age. An "r" of .0103 was found, and an r of .40 was necessary in order to be significant. 72 Student score was not significantly correlated with the number of years their instructor had taught driver education. An "r" of .0043 was found, when an "r" of .40 was necessary for significance. Summary 'This chapter reported the results of the data as analyzed. Several significant findings were discovered, and these will be discussed in Chapter V. Chapter V will also include a brief summary of the study, conclusions, recommendations, recommendations for further research, and a discussion. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, RECOMMENDATIONS Chapter IV contained the findings of this study. This chapter contains a brief summary of the study, a brief description of the research design, findings, con- clusions, recommendations, recommendations for further research, and a discussion. Summary of the Study The primary purpose of this study was to measure the level of cognitive knowledge of those students who had completed or were just completing a high school driver education course. Additional information concern- ing program type, use of multimedia, instructors, and pupils that could have a significant effect on student achievement was also gathered. The following research questions were examined: 1. Is driver education in Ohio effective in teaching cognitive knowledge? 2. Is any one of the three programs effective in teaching students cognitive knowledge? 73 74 3. Is driver education in Ohio effective in teaching what teachers believe they are teaching? 4. Is one of three programs more effective in teaching cognitive knowledge? 5. Is one program more effective than another with higher, middle, or lower GPA students in teaching cognitive knowledge? 6. Is multimedia in the classroom a major factor in terms of students' success? 7. Is the cost of a program a factor in terms of students' success? 8. Is the number of years teaching driver educa- tion a factor in terms of students' success? 9. Are the total hours of the program related to students' success? 10. Is class size related to students' success? 11. Is teacher age a major factor in terms of students' success? 12. Are teacher preparation hours a major factor in terms of students' success? Research Design TWelve schools from Ohio were randomly selected for use in this evaluation study. The names of schools which fit into the six categories selected were obtained 75 from Mr. Larry Cathell, Section Chief, Driver Education Section, Ohio Department of Education. A total of 326 students were tested at the twelve schools, using the 50 question PRIDE driver education cognitive knowledge test. Five separate t-tests were conducted as well as three ANOVA and six correlations to best analyze the data collected. Findings 1. Sixty-three percent of the students in Ohio scored 80 percent or better in cognitive knowledge. This was significantly below the hypothesized 80 percent at the .95 level of confidence. 2. Program one had 56 percent achieving 80 per- cent or more correct. Program two had 66 percent achiev- ing 80 or more correct and program three had 70 percent of the students achieving 80 percent or more correct. All three of these programs were significantly below the hypothesized 80 percent success criterion at the .95 level of confidence. 3. Seventy-one percent of the students achieved 80 percent or more correct in cognitive knowledge on the material which instructors said they had taught. Overall improvement was noted (from 63 percent to 71 percent achieving 80 percent or more correct) when items which had not been covered were eliminated. This was 76 significantly below the 80 percent hypothesized at the .95 level of confidence. 4. Program three (four-phase) was significantly better (.95 level) than program one (two-phase) or pro- gram two (three-phase simulation). There was no signifi- cant difference at the .95 level between programs one and two. 5. None of the three programs was significantly better at the .95 level with any of the three GPA groups. However, the higher the GPA, the higher the student score. This produced a significant GPA main effect in the ANOVA test. 6. Multimedia had no significant effect on stu- dents' cognitive knowledge at the .95 level of confidence. There was a significant main effect for program, again suggesting program three to be more effective than pro- gram one or program two. 7. Cost of the program had a moderate correla- tion (.17) with students' score. This moderate correla- tion was not significant and cannot be considered a major reason for student success. A correlation "r" of .40 was needed for significance. 8. No significant correlation "r" was found between students' score and their instructor's experience teaching driver education. An "r" of -.0978 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed for significance. 77 9. Hours of the program were not significantly correlated with student scores. An "r" of .0340 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed for significance. 10. Class size was not significantly correlated with student scores. An "r" of .0103 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed for significance. 11. There was no significant correlation found 'between student scores and the hours of teacher prepara- tion. An "r" of -.0729 was found when an "r" of .40 was needed to be significant. 12. Instructor years teaching driver education showed no significant correlation with student scores. An "r" of .0043 was found when an "r" of .40 was neces- sary for significance. Conclusions The following conclusions are based upon the findings of the study. 1. Driver education students in the state of Ohio have a deficient level of cognitive knowledge when tested on an 80 percent success criterion. 2. In none of the three programs were students achieving 80 percent mastery. Three separate t-tests concluded a significant deficiency in each program. 78 3. Students were significantly deficient of 80 percent correct on those items which their instructors indicated had been taught. 4. The four-phase program (program three) was significantly better than the three-phase simulation (program two) or the two-phase (program one). 5. No program was significantly better than another with any of the three GPA groupings. Students with higher GPA in each program did score higher as a group than lower GPA groupings, as should be expected on a written cognitive knowledge test. 6. Multimedia had no significant effect on student achievement. 7. The cost of the program had no significant effect on student success. 8. The number of hours in the driver education program did not have a significant effect on student success. 9. Class size had no significant effect on student scores. 10. Teacher age did not have a significant effect on student scores. 11. Instructor hours of teacher preparation had no significant effect on student scores. 79 12. The number of years instructors had been teaching driver education had no significant effect on student scores. Recommendations The following recommendations are based on the observations, findings, and conclusions of the study. 1. Ohio teachers of driver education should have made available to them a set of specific instruc- tional objectives for their driver education students. Testing could then be conducted on accomplishment of those objectives. There were no such set of objectives available in Ohio at the time of this study. 2. Four-phase programs should be utilized when schools have access to a range and simulator, and when these can be made available to students without signifi- cant cost increase. 3. A close examination needs to be made on the use of multimedia in driver education classrooms. The use of multimedia showed no significant gain over those programs without multimedia. Recommendations for Further Research The following are recommendations for further research based on the observations, findings, conclusions, and recommendations of this study. 80 l. A study should be conducted to determine what type of teacher preparation, and how much, can increase student achievement. 2. This study should be replicated after the teachers of Ohio are provided objectives. 3. This study should be replicated using other criteria such as an attitude scale and an in-car perform- ance test . 4. A study should be conducted to determine if students can learn an acceptable level of information in 36 hours of classroom instruction, or if increased hours of instruction can significantly improve student achieve- ment. 5. A study should be conducted to compare three- phase range programs with four-phase programs, three- phase simulation, and two-phase programs. This would help determine if range instruction can improve student achievement when not in combination with simulation, as this study indicated. 6. A study should be conducted to determine the relationship of students' IQ or GPA and achievement on a knowledge test, achievement on an on-road BTW evaluation, and subsequent accident and violation involvement. 7. A study should be conducted to determine the effectiveness of multimedia instruction in the driver education classroom. 81 Discussion The t-tests conducted showed the students tested in Ohio, as a whole, to be deficient on an eighty percent or more correct criterion. Although an eighty percent criterion is the standard which is most used, it may have been too stringent for this evaluation. A large segment of the students were in the 75 to 80 percent correct category. Although computation placed these students in a deficient grouping, most teachers would agree that 75 percent correct would be a passing score. Driver license examiners also set their criteria at 70 to 75 percent to "pass." If the criterion was 75 percent, the number of students making acceptable scores would increase to 75 percent for all the test questions. Further, 83 percent of the students achieved 75 percent or more correct of the questions which teachers indicated they had covered in the driver education class. These figures tend to show that, in general, the students were achieving rela- tively well in learning that material which the teachers were teaching. One might question if the teachers are teaching what needs to be taught. As previously stated, a set of objectives should be developed, adopted, and made avail- able to the driver education teachers in Ohio. This could help to insure more uniform.and complete instruction 82 for the students. Beyond that, evaluation of driver edu- cation would be easier and more accurate. The end result might be better drivers. Multimedia as a method of instruction showed no significant improvement in student scores. This may have been due to mechanical problems or to an overuse of packaged instruction. Students may be tired of watching just films or packaged units which can leave the teacher out of teaching. Clarification of service contracts of state-owned multimdeia units, as well as provisions for replacement of worn-out equipment, needs to be studied. Most of the schools which the author visited had experienced problems ‘with mechanical failure of the multimedia equipment. It was the author's opinion that the most import- ant variable was the teacher and his/her dedication to the students. A well prepared, enthusiastic teacher will teach students adequately regardless of the program type which he/she has available. The added phases make instruction easier, quicker, more complete, and can be less expensive. They can make an effective teacher more effective. The number of questions that instructors indi- cated that they had not covered in class was relatively small. A trend not to teach all of the natural forces affecting driving was noted. Other questions frequently 83 not covered were methods of insuring compliance with the posted speed limits. Once again, it should be noted that if a set of objectives was available and utilized this situation could be improved, providing the objectives were designed to cover these areas. Several programs were taught one phase at a time. Teachers were not informed as to what other teachers in the same school were doing in other phases. This lack of communication between programs can only be a detriment to the programs' effectiveness, as well as to the students within these programs. The state should mandate integration of all phases of any driver education program. This might insure coordination and communication between teachers who are teaching the same students. APPENDICES 84 APPENDIX A SAMPLE LETTER TO SUPERINTENDENTS 85 May 1, 1979 Room 70 Kellogg Center Highway Traffic Safety Center Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824 Mr. C. M. Roush, Superintendent Madison Local Schools 601 Hill Street Middletown, OH 45042 Dear Sir: Madison High School is one of eighteen schools randomly selected for an evaluation of selected driver education programs in Ohio. This study is under the guidance of the Ohio Department of Education, Mr. Larry Cathell, Section Chief. Your cooperation for this evaluation is essential and greatly appreciated. The evaluation will consist of a brief knowledge test which I will administer at your school's convenience. I will make arrangements directly with the high school for the evaluation, but I wanted to inform you of it. If you have any questions, please contact me at the above address or Mr. Larry Cathell, Section Chief, Driver Education Section, Ohio Department of Education, (614) 466-4230. Thank you for your cooperation. Sincerely, Joe Shrader Ph.D. Candidate Graduate Assistant JS:1j1 86 APPENDIX B SAMPLE LETTER TO PRINCIPALS OF SELECTED SCHOOLS 87 April 30, 1979 Room 70 Kellogg Center Highway Traffic Safety Center Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824 (517) 353-1790 Mr. Jack Davis, Principal Madison High School 1368 Middletown-Eaton Road Dear Mr. Davis: Madison High School is one of 18 schools randomly selected for an evaluation of selected driver education programs in Ohio. This study is under the guidance of the Ohio Department of Education headed by Mr. Larry Cathell, Section Chief. Your cooperation for this evaluation is essential and greatly appreciated. The evaluation will consist of a brief knowledge test which I will administer at your school's convenience. The test should not take more than 50 minutes. I will need two items of information from.your school which I will discuss with you when I call next week. They are Grade Point Average for those pupils tested (to remain anonymous) and the approximate cost per pupil for driver education. Thank you very much for your cooperation. Sincerely, Joe Shrader Ph.D. Candidate Graduate Assistant 88 APPENDIX C SAMPLE LETTER TO PRINCIPALS OF ALTERNATE SCHOOLS 89 April 30, 1979 Highway Traffic Safety Center Room 70 Kellogg Center Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48824 Mr. Larry E. Irwin, Principal Lakota High School 5050 Tylersville Road West Chester, OH 45069 Dear Mr. Irwin: Lakota High School was one of 18 schools, in a random selection, to be selected as an alternate for an evalu- ation of selected driver education programs in Ohio. This study is under the guidance of the Ohio Department of Education, headed by Mr. Larry Cathell, Section Chief. Your cooperation is essential and greatly appreciated. Since your school was selected as an alternative, I may not have to contact you, but I wanted to inform you in case your school was needed. Thank you for your cooperation. Sincerely, Joe Shreader Ph.D. Candidate Graduate Assistant 90 APPENDIX D SAMPLE FORM OF TELEPHONE QUESTIONS 91 TELEPHONE QUESTIONS Principal's Name Teacher's Name # of Driver Education Classes Class Period Time of Class Approximate Class Completion Date / / Approximate Number of Students in Class Date for Evaluation / / Type of Class Procedure: Days per week Hours per day Information Needed 1. Approximate cost per pupil 2. GPA of each pupil in class (coded) 92 APPENDIX E SAMPLE DRIVER EDUCATION KNOWLEDGE TEST AND ANSWER SHEET 93 DRIVER EDUCATION KNOWLEDGE TEST Instructions: Read the statement and all possible answers. Select The the best answer and mark the corresponding letter on the answer sheet provided. Please do not write on the test booklet. Put your social security number on the answer sheet only. If you do not have a social security number available, put your name on the answer sheet. most effective devices for protecting passengers when in an accident are: a. b. c. d. You a. b. c. d. It day a. b. c. d. Seat belts Safety door latches Padded instrument panels Deep center steering wheels need not obey a traffic control device when: Other vehicles ignore the device No other traffic is present A police officer directs you to do otherwise If is safe to ignore it is important to drive slower at night than you do during the because: There is more traffic You may get sleepy You cannot see as far ahead Drivers tend to be more careless at night When driving in an area where there are many pedestrians it is most important to: a. b. c. d. Put your headlights on so they can see you better Keep your speed down to 15 mph Watch for an indication that they will enter the road Stop at every intersection and proceed when safe 94 10. 95 If you miss your exit on a freeway you should not: a. Proceed to the next exit where you can reenter the freeway and return to your proper exit b. Back up on the road or on the shoulder c. Continue on the freeway and find an alternate route d. Resume normal speed until you approach next exit If you are about to drive away from the curb, you should: Sound your horn and go ahead Signal and pull into the street Signal, yield right-of—way and pull into the street Signal, wait for the first vehicle to pass and pull into the street Q-DU‘QJ 0 If you are backing up and want the rear of your vehicle to go left turn the: a. Top of the steering wheel to the right b. Bottom of the steering wheel to the left c. Top of the steering wheel to the left d. Left side of the steering wheel to the right When driving you should stay at least: a. 1/2 of a second behind the vehicle in front of you b. 3/4 of a second behind the vehicle in front of you c. 1 second behind the vehicle in front of you d. 2 seconds behind the vehicle in front of you You should drive in the right lane of a 6-lane highway when: a. Driving slower than the traffic in the other lanes b. You are preparing to exit on the left c. When you see traffic entering the highway from the right d. You want to pass other vehicles on the highway The most important reason for passing a truck traveling at 45 miles per hours in a 55 miles per hour zone is: a. To improve your ability to see b. To advance your position in traffic c. To reduce your travel time d. To avoid exhaust fumes ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. l6. 17. 96 As your speed increases it is most important to: O-OU‘OJ It a. b. c. d. You a. b. c. d. Put on your headlights Look farther ahead Drive with both hands on the wheel Roll up all the windows is unsafe to: Glance at the outside mirror Check your controls Focus on the road just in front of the hood Look along the left and right side of the road should signal a turn: Well in advance of the turn Right before the turn Upon turning Only at controlled intersections If 1 or 2 of your wheels drop off the edge of the pavement: QaOU‘W You a. b. c. d. Hold the steering wheel loosely Ease back onto the road after slowing down Slow down quickly by braking hard Increase your speed and drive back on the road should be most careful when turning or stopping: Just before it starts to rain During the first half hour of rain After it has been raining all day A half hour after it stops raining When driving in heavy fog during the day you should use: a. b. c. d. High beam headlights Low beam headlights Parking lights No lights If the rear of your vehicle is skidding to the left you should: a. b. c. d. Move the steering wheel back and forth in a zig-zag pattern Turn the top of your steering wheel to the left Hold your steering wheel from moving until out of skid Turn the top of your steering wheel to the right 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 97 When there is a strong wind coming from your left or right you should: a. Slow down and steer away from the wind b. Steer into the wind being careful not to oversteer c. Let the steering wheel slip through your hands d. Stay in the right lane as close to the shoulder as possible In order to get out of a skid you should: Keep your foot off the brake Turn the front wheels towards the edge of the road Let the steering wheel slip through your hands Keep a constant pressure on the gas pedal When nearing the top of a hill on a narrow road: a. b. c. d. If Keep far to the right Speed up Avoid blowing your horn Get ready to pass any slow vehicles in front of you a speed limit is not posted before a curve: Continue at the same speed Assume that it is better to take this curve at a higher speed Slow down to 35 to 40 mph Judge how sharp the curve is and change your speed accordingly If you cannot see around a curve you should: Slow down more than you normally would Continue as you would through any curve Speed up to get out of that hazardous area quickly Drive around the curve at 5 to 10 mph Because there is often slow-moving traffic on country highways you should: a. b. c. d. Stay in the left lane and drive at the speed limit Be ready to adjust your speed to the speed of traffic Pass slower vehicles on turns if they are slowing down Generally drive 10 to 15 mph under the speed limit in order to be safe 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 98 When driving around a curve your vehicle will tend to: a. Speed up b. Move to the outside of the curve c. Stay in the center of the lane d. Move to the inside of the curve If you know that you will soon be making a turn you should: a. Look well ahead to locate the turning point b. Blow the horn several hundred feet before the turn c. Flash your bright lights to warn other traffic d. Speed up so as to avoid making other vehicles wait If there is a change in the legal speed limit you should first: Quickly slow down Slow down and proceed cautiously Look in your rearview mirror . Check your speedometer moo‘m To avoid spinning the tires on a slippery surface you should: a. Alternately use the brake and gas b. Increase speed slowly c. Shift from drive to neutral d. Start in second gear with fast but steady power When coming to an intersection it is most important to: a. Stay in the same lane b. Look for and follow traffic controls c. Flash your brake lights d. Open a window and turn down the radio Cross traffic should be checked before entering an intersection: a. When traffic has been heavy b. Only when you have a stop sign c. Where there is a traffic light d. At all times When you come to an intersection where there are no traffic controls you should: a. Speed up and blow your horn b. Slow down so you can stop before the intersection c. Come to a stop before you come to a crosswalk 6. Continue at the same speed and watch for traffic 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 99 When about to pass you should generally: a. b. c. d. Move up very close to the lead vehicle, then change lanes Drop back and change lanes far behind lead vehicle Maintain usual following distance until you change lanes Move up close to lead vehicle and drop back to warn him you are about to pass a 2-lane road, do not: Pass moving traffic on the left Pass vehicles making a left turn from the right lane Look in front of the vehicle you want to pass Change your speed in order to pass After passing a vehicle on a 2-lane road you should generally: a. b. c. d. As Turn sharply back to the right lane Gradually turn back into the right lane Stay in the left lane until you see oncoming traffic Slow down and then move into the right lane you approach a freeway on an entrance you should: Keep checking traffic ahead on the entrance and behind on the freeway Not take your eyes off the vehicle ahead of you on the entrance Begin to pick up speed and pass other vehicles on the entrance if necessary Drive around the vehicle in front of you if it is taking too long to enter the freeway you freeway exit has a deceleration lane you should: Slow down as much as possible on the main road before entering the deceleration lane Drive alongside and pull in front of slower moving traffic in the deceleration lane Move into the deceleration lane as soon as possible Keep your speed constant once you enter the deceleration lane When entering a freeway from an entrance with an acceleration lane, you should: Stop to check for traffic at the end of the entrance Enter the freeway at top speed and slow down to the speed limit afterward Use the acceleration lane to get up to the speed of the freeway traffic Use the shoulder to gain speed before you enter 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 100 If you get sleepy while driving it is best to: Take anti-sleep pills Stop for a cup of coffee Stop and exercise until you wake up Rest or change drivers if possible 040703 If you are taking medicine for a cold you should: a. Know the effects of the medicine before you drive b. Not drive with anyone else in the vehicle c. Not drive at night or just after taking the medicine d. Only drive if it is an emergency When possible pedestrians walking along the road should walk: a. On the left side facing traffic b. On the right side with traffic c. On the edge of the road rather than on the shoulder d. On the side with the least traffic Before you change lanes or turn you must: a. See if it can safely be done and then signal b. Check traffic by using your sideview mirror c. Blow the born before pulling into traffic d. Move into the far left lane Under normal conditions the top speed limit for driving in a business district is: a. 15 mph b. 20 mph c. 25 mph d. 30 mph You must stOp at a railroad crossing when: a. A train is stopped less than 200 feet from the crossing b. The crossing does not have a gate or a signal on it c. There is more than 1 set of tracks at the crossing d. The crossing signal is on or a train is close and approaching If you must leave the road at high speeds, you should: a. Head toward oncoming moving objects b. DrOp to the floor c. Apply the brakes as hard as possible d. Steer away from large, rigid objects 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 101 If an oncoming vehicle crosses the center line and drives into your lane you should: a. Speed up and drive on the shoulder b. Stop as quickly as you can c. Drive into his lane if it is empty d. Slow down and steer to the right If you drive off the road when traveling at a high speed you should: 3. Turn the wheels sharply toward the road and apply the brakes b. Straighten the wheels gradually and pump the brakes c. Step hard on the brake and straighten the wheels d. Speed up and turn the wheels toward the road The message on this sign might be: 0‘0) Stop Ahead, Side Road, Reverse Turn One Way, Keep Right, No U-Turn Pass With Care, Slower Traffic Keep Right, Do Not Pass Trucks Use Right Lane, Yield Right-of-Way, Reduce Speed Ahead When you see this sign at a corner you should first come to a stop: a. Out far enough to see cross traffic b. Before the crosswalk c. Within the crosswalk d. After crossing the crosswalk This sign means: Slow down to 35 mph and prepare to enter curve Exit ahead, exit speed 35 mph Construction area, slow down to 35 mph and use right lane only 35' Vehicles turning right must reduce speed upw' to 35 mph This sign means: a. b. c. d. Barricade ahead, prepare to detour Slow down, pavement ends Right lane ends, prepare to merge Construction ahead, caution, no shoulder 102 50. The message on this sign might be: a. Stop Ahead, Detour 1000 feet, Pedestrian "‘="“‘ Crossing b. Speed Limit 30 mph, Reduce Speed Ahead, No U-Turn c. Soft Shoulder, Bump, Dip d. Divided Highway, Hill Road Narrows Go back and answer any questions you may have skipped over. Check your answer sheet to make sure all your answers are clearly marked and that any erasures are completed. Check your answer sheet to make sure your social security number or name is on it. 103 ANSWER SHEET Social Security Number A B C D A B C D 0000000000000000000000000 0000000000000000000000000 0000000000000000000000000 0000000000000000000000000 6 2 27. 8901 2233 32. 34 33 35 678901 3333/44 42. 3 4 44. 56 8 4].». 4 47. 49. 50. 0000000000000000000000000 0000000000000000000000000 0000000000000000000000000 0000000000000000000000000 O. .0 12345678901 11 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 17. APPENDIX F SAMPLE RECORD OF TEST SCORES 104 Social Security Number Name \OCX)\ICJ\U'|J-‘UJNil-m| 25. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 105 HH Howoouo‘mbwml-i 000.0000 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. Social Security Number 106 High School # 63 PA llllHlHlllilllllllHilllllllltllllllll APPENDIX G SAMPLE OF ITEMS TAUGHT 107 School School # Program Type Instructors: Please read all the test questions and mark all ques- tions in the appropriate column. The evaluation will be on those questions covered in your class. 1 = Subject has been covered 2 = Subject has not been covered but will be before completion of the course 3 = Subject has not and will not be covered in this course The most effective devices for protecting passengers 1 2 in an accident are: a. Seat belts b. Safety door latches c. Padded instrument panels d. Deep center steering wheels You need not obey a traffic control device when: 1 2 3. Other vehicles ignore the device b. No other traffic is present c. A police officer directs you to do otherwise d. It is safe to ignore it It is important to drive slower at night than you 1 2 do during the day because: a. There is more traffic b. You may get sleepy c. You cannot see as far ahead d. Drivers tend to be more careless at night When driving in an area where there are many 1 2 pedestrians it is most important to: a. Put your headlights on so they can see you better b. Keep your speed down to 15 mph c. Watch for an indication that they will enter the road d. Stop at every intersection and proceed when safe ‘108 10. 109 If you miss your exit on a freeway you should not: a. Proceed to the next exit where you can reenter the freeway and return to your proper exit b. Back up on the road or on the shoulder c. Continue on the freeway and find an alternate route d. Resume normal speed until you approach next exit If you are about to drive away from the curb, you should: a. Sound your horn and go ahead b. Signal and pull into the street c. Signal, yield right-of—way and pull into the street d. Signal, wait for the first vehicle to pass and pull into the street If you are backing up and want the rear of your vehicle to go left turn the: Top of the steering wheel to the right Bottom of the steering wheel to the left Top of the steering wheel to the left Left side of the steering wheel to the right When driving you should stay at least: a. b. c. d. 1/2 of a second behind the vehicle in front of you 3/4 of a second behind the vehicle in front of you 1 second behind the vehicle in front of you 2 seconds behind the vehicle in front of you You should drive in the right lane of a 6-1ane highway when: a. b. c. d. Driving slower than the traffic in the other lanes You are preparing to exit on the left When you see traffic entering the highway from the You want to pass other vehicles on the highway The most important reason for passing a truck traveling at 45 miles per hours in a 55 miles per hour zone is: a. b. c. d. To improve your ability to see To advance your position in traffic To reduce your travel time To avoid exhaust fumes ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. It If pav a. 110 your speed increases it is most important to: l 2 3 Put on your headlights Look farther ahead Drive with both hands on the wheel Roll up all the windows is unsafe to: l 2 3 Glance at the outside mirror Check your controls Focus on the road just in front of the hood Look along the left and right side of the road should signal a turn: 1 2 3 Well in advance of the turn Right before the turn Upon turning Only at controlled intersections l or 2 of your wheels drop off the edge of the 1 2 3 ement: Hold the steering wheel loosely b. Ease back onto the road after slowing down c. Slow down quickly by braking hard d. Increase your speed and drive back on the road You should be most careful when turning or stopping: l 2 3 a. Just before it starts to rain b. During the first half hour of rain C. d. Whe sho If 1ef a. b. c. d. After it has been raining all day A half hour after it stops raining n driving in heavy fog during the day you 1 2 3 uld use: High beam headlights Low beam headlights Parking lights No lights the rear of your vehicle is skidding to the 1 2 3 t you should: Move the steering wheel back and forth in a zig-zag pattern Turn the top of your steering wheel to the left Hold your steering wheel from moving until out of skid Turn the top of your steering wheel to the right l8. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 111 When there is a strong wind coming from your left 1 2 3 or In right you should: Slow down and steer away from the wind Steer into the wind being careful not to oversteer Let the steering wheel slip through your hands Stay in the right lane as close to the shoulder as possible order to get out of a skid you should: 1 2 3 Keep your foot off the brake b. Turn the front wheels towards the edge of the road c. Let the steering wheel slip through your hands d. Keep a constant pressure on the gas pedal When nearing the top of a hill on a narrow road: 1 2 3 a. Keep far to the right b. c. d. If If a. b. c. d. Speed up Avoid blowing your horn Get ready to pass any slow vehicles in front of you a speed limit is not posted before a curve: 1 2 3 Continue at the same speed Assume that it is better to take this curve at a higher speed Slow down to 35 to 50 mph Judge how sharp the curve is and change your speed accordingly you cannot see around a curve you should: 1 2 3 Slow down more than you normally would Continue as you would through any curve Speed up to get out of that hazardous area quickly Drive around the curve at 5 to 10 mph Because there is often slow-moving traffic on 1 2 3 country highways you should: Stay in the left lane and drive at the speed limit Be ready to adjust your speed to the speed of traffic Pass slower vehicles on turns if they are slowing down Generally drive 10 to 15 mph under the speed limit in order to be safe 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 112 When driving around a curve your vehicle will tend to: a. Speed up b. Move to the outside of the curve c. Stay in the center of the lane d. Move to the inside of the curve If you know that you will soon be making a turn you should: a. Look well ahead to locate the turning point b. Blow the horn several hundred feet before the turn c. Flash you bright lights to warn other traffic d. Speed up so as to avoid making other vehicles wait If there is a change in the legal speed limit you should first: Quickly slow down . Slow down and proceed cautiously . Look in your rearview mirror . Check your speedometer D-DO‘OJ To avoid spinning the tires on a slippery surface you should: a. Alternately use the brake and gas b. Increase speed slowly c. Shift from drive to neutral d. Start in second gear with fast but steady power When coming to an intersection it is most important to: a. Stay in the same lane b. Look for and follow traffic controls c. Flash your brake lights d. Open a window and turn down the radio Cross traffic should be checked before entering an intersection: a. When traffic has been heavy b. Only when you have a stop sign c. Where there is a traffic light d. At all times 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 113 When you come to an intersection where there are no 1 2 3 traffic controls you should: a. Speed up and blow your horn b. Slow down so you can stop before the intersection c. Come to a stop before you come to a crosswalk d. Continue at the same speed and watch for traffic When about to pass you should generally: 1 2 3 a. Move up very close to the lead vehicle, then change lanes b. Drop back and change lanes far behind lead vehicle d. Maintain usual following distance until you change lanes d. Move up close to lead vehicle and drop back to warn him you are about to pass On a 2-lane road, do not: 1 2 3 a. Pass moving traffic on the left b. Pass vehicles making a left turn from the right lane c. Look in front of the vehicle you want to pass d. Change your speed in order to pass After passing a vehicle on a 2-lane road you should 1 2 3 generally: a. Turn sharply back to the right lane b. Gradually turn back into the right lane c. Stay in the left lane until you see oncoming traffic d. Slow down and then move into the right lane As you approach a freeway on an entrance you should: 1 2 3 3. Keep checking traffic ahead on the entrance and behind on the freeway b. Not take your eyes off the vehicle ahead of you on the entrance c. Begin to pick up speed and pass other vehicles on the entrance if necessary d. Drive around the vehicle in front of you if it is taking too long to enter the freeway If your freeway exit has a deceleration lane you 1 2 3 should: a. Slow down as much as possible on the main road before entering the deceleration lane b. Drive alongside and pull in front of slower moving traffic in the decleration lane c. Move into the deceleration lane as soon as possible d. Keep your speed constant once you enter the deceleration lane 114 When entering a freeway from an entrance with an 1 acceleration lane, you should: a. Stop to check for traffic at the end of the entrance b. Enter the freeway at top speed and slow down to the speed limit afterward c. Use the acceleration lane to get up to the speed of the freeway traffic d. Use the shoulder to gain speed before you enter If you get sleepy while driving it is best to: l a. Take anti-sleep pills b. Stop for a cup of coffee c. Stop and exercise until you wake up d. Rest or change drivers if possible If you are taking medicine for a cold you should: 1 a. Know the effects of the medicine before you drive b. Not drive with anyone else in the vehicle c. Not drive at night or just after taking the medicine d. Only drive if it is an emergency When possible pedestrians walking along the road 1 should walk: a. On the left side facing traffic b. On the right side with traffic c. On the edge of the road rather than on the shoulder d. On the side with the least traffic Before you change lanes or turn you must: 1 a. See if it can safely be done and then signal b. Check traffic by using your sideview mirror c. Blow the horn before pulling into traffic d. Move into the far left lane Under normal conditions the top speed limit for 1 driving in a business district is: a. 15 mph b. 20 mph c. 25 mph d. 30 mph 115 You must stop at a railroad crossing when: l 2 3 a. A train is stopped less than 200 feet from the crossing b. The crossing does not have a gate or a signal on it c. There is more than 1 set of tracks at the crossing d. The crossing signal is on or a train is close and approaching If you must leave the road at high speeds, you should: 1 2 3 a. Head toward oncoming moving objects b. Drop to the floor c. Apply the brakes as hard as possible d. Steer away from large, rigid objects If an oncoming vehicle crosses the center line and l 2 3 drives into your lane you should: 3. Speed up and drive on the shoulder b. Stop as quickly as you can c. Drive into his lane if it is empty d. Slow down and steer to the right If you drive off the road when traveling at a high 1 2 3 speed you should: a. Turn the wheels sharply toward the road and apply the brakes b. Straighten the wheels gradually and pump the brakes c. Step hard on the brake and straighten the wheels d. Speed up and turn the wheels toward the road The message on this sign might be: 1 2 3 a. Step Ahead, Side Road, Reverse Turn b. One Way, Keep Right, No U-Turn c. Pass With Care, Slower Traffic Keep Right, Do Not Pass d. Trucks Use Right Lane, Yield Right-of-Way, Reduce Speed Ahead When you see this sign at a corner you should first 1 2 3 come to a step: a. Out far enough to see cross traffic b. Before the crosswalk c. Within the crosswalk d. After crossing the crosswalk 116 48. This sign means: 1 2 3 a. Slow down to 35 mph and prepare to enter curve b. Exit ahead, exit speed 35 mph c. Construction area, slow down to 35 mph and use right lane only I d. Vehicles turning right must reduce l§'§j speed to 35 mph 49. This sign means: 1 2 3 a. Barricade ahead, prepare to detour b. Slow down, pavement ends c. Right lane ends, prepare to merge d. Construction ahead, caution, no shoulder 50. The message on this sign might be: 1 2 3 a. Stop Ahead, Detour 1000 feet, Pedestrian Crossing b. Speed Limit 30 mph, Reduce Speed Ahead, No U-Turn c. Soft Shoulder, Bump, Dip d. Divided Highway, Hill, Road Narrows Go back and answer any questions you may have skipped over. Check your answer sheet to make sure all your answers are clearly marked and that any erasures are completed. Check your answer sheet to make sure your social security number or name is on it. APPENDIX H SAMPLE OF TEACHER INFORMATION 117 School # Name Class # # of Students Program 1 2 3 4 Multi—media Yes Teacher Information 1. Age 2. Years teaching driver education 3. # of quarter hours driver education teacher preparation Program Information 1. Total hours of the program 2. Cost per pupil Student Information Student # Test Score Student # Test Score 1. 16. 2. 17. 3. l8. 4. l9. 5. 20. 6. 21. 7. 22. 8. 23. 9. 24. 10. 25. 11. 26. 12. 27. 13. 28. 14. 29. 15. 30. 118 No GPA lllllllllllilll BIBLIOGRAPHY 119 10. 11. BIBLIOGRAPHY Aaron, James E., and Marland K. Strasser. Driver and Traffic Safety Education. 2nd ed. New York: Macmillan, 1977. Driving Task Instruction. New York: r—TI—dacml {1,1974. Allgaier, E., and S. Williams. "Effectiveness of a Driving Simulator." Traffic Safety Research Review, 1959. Bachmann. "Dropping Out Not So Bad." The Educa- tion Digest, December, 1971. Bishop, Richard. "A System of Grading for Driver Education." Report of a committee appointed by the State Department of Education. Tallahassee. . "A Theory of Driving Behavior." East Lansing: Highway Traffic Safety Center, Michigan State University. . "Learning and Crading--Do They Mix?" The Florida Institute for Continuing University Studies, Tallahassee. . "Questions and Answers About Driving Simu- Iators." East Lansing: Highway Traffic Safety Center, Michigan State University. Bloom, Benjamin S. Taxonomy of Educational Objec- tives. New York: David MCKay, 1956. Borg, Walter R. and Meredith D. Gall. Educational Research, An Introduction. 2nd ed. New York: *David McKa , 1971. Bromley, David G., Mary Lynn Crow, and Martha S. Gibson. "Grade Inflation, Trends, Causes, and Impli- cations." Phi Delta Kappan, June, 1978. 120 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. l8. 19. 20. 21. 22. 121 California Committee for Traffic Safety Education. "The California Driver Training Evaluation Study: A Comparative Analysis." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, April, 1975. Campbell, Donald T., and Julian C. Stanley. Ex eri- mental andeuasi-Experimental Designs for Research. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1970. Carpenter, David W. ”An Evaluation of the California Knowledge Test and the University of Michigan Item Pool." U.S. Department of Transportation, Federal Highway Administration, 1976. Chall, Jeanne S. "Minimum Competency in Reading: An Informal Survey of the States." Phi Delta Kappan, January, 1979. Chansky, Norman M. "Resolving the Grading Problem." Phi Delta Kappan, January, 1973. Cole, William M. "The Texas A & M University Compe- tency Based Five-Phase Driver Education Program." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, October, 1976. Coppin, Ronald, and Margaret Hubbard Jones. "The California Driver Training Evaluation Study: A Reply to W. G. Patterson." Journal of Traffic Safety Edu- cation, July, 1975. Council, Forrest M., et al. "Effect of Range Train- ing: Comparison of Road Test Scores for Driver Edu- cation Students." Highway Safety Research Center, University of North Carolina. . "Emergency Skills Resources for Range ReIated Driver Education." Highway Safety Research Center, University of North Carolina, September, 1975. Council, Forrest, Rita Roper, and Michael C. Sadof. Evaluation of North Carolina's Multi-vehicle Range Program in Driver Education: A Comparison of Driving Histories of Range and Non-range Students. Highway Traffic Research Center, University of North Carolina, August, 1975. Cox, D. R. Planningof Experiments. New York: John Wiley and Sons{’l958. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 122 Dayton, C. Mitchell, and Clayton L. Stunkard. Sta- tistics for Problem Solving. New York: McCraw-Hill, 1971. ' Delahanty, B. David. "Myths About Older Teachers." Phi Delta Kappan, December, 1977. Dreyer, Dell R. "An Evaluation of California's Drivers Licensing Examination." Research Report No. 51, March, 1976. Driver Education Committee, Transportation Research Board. "Current Activities in Driver Education Research." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, October, 1975. Dunn, LeRoy. "Driver Education Evaluation." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, January, 1977. Edwards, Dorthy S. Evaluation of Laboratornyethods for the Study of Driver Behavior: FinaI Rgport. American Institute for Research, Silver Springs, Maryland, May, 1969. Eno Foundation for Highway Traffic Control. "Personal Characteristics of Traffic Accident Repeaters." Saugutuck, Connecticut, 1948. Forbes, T. W., et a1. Research on Driver Performance Measures: Task 1. East Lansing: CHighwayTraffic Safety Center, Michigan State University, 1971. Freeberg, Norman E. Development of Driver Performance Measures for Use in the N.CT”Licensing Program: A Study Plan. Highway Safety Research Center, Uni- versity of North Carolina, 1970. Glass, Gene V., and Julian C. Stanley. Statistical Methods in Education and Psychology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1970. Goldstein, Leon G. "The California Driver Training Evaluation Study: Rejoinder to Peck and Jones' Reply." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, October, 1975. Griffin, Lindsay I., Brian Powers, and Catherine Mullen. Impediments to the Evaluation of Highway Safety Programs. Chapel Hill: Highway Safety Research center, University of North Carolina, June, 1975. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 123 Gustafson, Robert Earl. "A Study to Compare the Effectiveness of Instruction in the Allstate Good Driver Trainer and on the Multiple Car Off-Street Driving Range with the Multiple Car Off-Street Driving Range." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965. Evaluation of Driver Education. Paper presented to driver education class, Michigan State University. Hagen, Roger E. "Sex Differences in Driving Per- formances." Human Factors, April, 1975. Kaywood, Richard, et a1. Drive Ri ht. Clenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman, I977. Kenel, Francis. "Quality in Driver Education." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, April, 1977. Land, Ming H. 29th Yearbook of ACIATE. Oxford, Ohio: Miami University, 1979. Lybrand, William. "A Study on Evaluation of Driver Education." Clearinghouse, Springfield, Virginia, July 31, 1968. Mann, William A. "Difficulties in Changing Human Behavior." Paper presented to the National Safety Congress, 1968. "Personal Attitude Survey." East Lansing: Highway Traffic Safety Center, Michigan State Uni- versity. McDaniel, Charles E. "A Study to Compare the Rela- tive Effectiveness of Selected Laboratory and Class- room Programs in Driver and Traffic Safety Education.‘ Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. McGlade, Francis S. "A New Test for Use in Driver Licensing, Education, and Employment." New York: Center for Safety Education, New York University, 1962. McKnight, A. James. Driver Education Task Analysis: Task Descriptions. 4 vols. CSpringfiéld, Virginia: National Technical Information Service, 1970. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 124 Meyerhoff, Richard A. "Simulation Now and the 80's." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, January, 1979. Michigan Department of Education. An Educational Health Check: The Michigan Educational Assessment Program. 41978. Michigan Department of Education. Interpretive Manual 1977-78 Michigan Educational Assessment Program. 1978. Michigan Department of Education. Needs Assessment: A Step Toward Quality Education. February, 1975. Michigan Department of Education. State Summary of Results, 1975-76 Michigan Educational Assessment Program, 3rd’Report. 1976. Michigan Department of Education. The Status of Basic Skills Attainment in Michigan Public Schools. 1978. Michigan Department of Education. Technical Report, Michigan Educational Assessment Program. June, 1977. Michigan Department of Education. Technical Report, 1975-76 Michigan Educational AssessmentIProgram, 4th Report. 1976. Michigan Department of Education. The Long Range Plan of the Michigan Educational Assessment Program. July, 1977. " Miller, Thomas L. "An Inventory of Critical Driver Knowledge for Motor Vehicle Operators Age 65 and Older in Ingham County, Michigan." Ph.D. disserta- tion, Michigan State University, 1978. Motor Vehicle Manufacturers Association. MVMA Motor Vehicle Facts and Figures. Detroit, 1979. National Commission Safety Education. Summary of Results of Studies Evaluatiog Driver Education. Washifigton,CD.C.: National Commission onCSafety Education, 1961. National Safety Council. Accident Facts. Chicago, 1978. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 125 Nolan, Robert O. "A Comparative Study of the Teach- ing Effectiveness of the Multiple Car Off-Street Driving Range and the Aetna Drivotrainer." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965. Ohio Department of Education. ”Standards for Driver Education Programs." 1977. Ohio Department of Education. The Development and Validation of Attitude, Knowledge, and Performance Tests for Evaluation Driver Education Curricula. Systems Research Group, The OhioCStatePUniversity, 1974. Parker, R. Samuel. "Using Multimedia in the Class- room." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, April, 1977. Pollock, W. T., and T. L. McDole. Development of a National Item Bank for Tests of Driving Knowledge: Final Technical Report. Ann Afbor: Highway safety Research Institute, 1973. Prothero, Jon C. "Improving the Driver Education Curriculum." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, April, 1975. Robertson, Leon S., and Paul L. Zabor. "Driver Education and Fatal Crash Involvement of Teenaged Drivers.” Insurance Institute for Highway Safety. Washington, 1979. (Mimeographed.) Robinson, Allen. "A Post-Test for Driver Education Based On: A Resource Curriculum in Driver and Traffic Safety Education." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1972. Sample, Vernon P. "A Case for Driving Simulators." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, October, 1976. Schmitt, Kara. Michigan Driver Education Evaluation Project. Lansing: Department of Education, 1978. Siebrecht, Elmer B. Manual of Directions: Siebrecht Attitude Scale. New York: New York UniVersity center fOrPSafety Education. Silver, Gail H. "Driver Education and the Multimedia Response System." Journal of Traffic Safety Educa- tion, October, 1971. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 126 Silvernale, Leslie R. and Malcom D. Whale. "Does Driver Education Go Far Enough?" Traffic Safety, September, 1969. Smith, Donald L. "Critique of the Michigan Driver Education Evaluation Study." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, January, 1979. Swensen, Raymond O. "The Development and Testing of a Simulated Dynamic Driving Hazard Perception Test." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1976. Tack, R. L. Iowa Driver Education Evaluation Study: Final Report, Program Research in Driver Education, PRIDE. Des Moines: Iowa Department of Public Instruction, 1975. U.S. Department of Commerce. A Study on Evaluation of Driver Education. [by William A. Lybrand]. Washington, D.C.: The American University, July 31, 1968. U.S. Department of Transportation. Driver Education and Training: Final Report. Darien, Cdnnecticut: Dunlap and Associates, 1968. U.S. Department of Commerce. Driver Education and TrainingProgect for the National Highway Safety Bureau. E1 egundo, GaIifOrnia: *Institute for Educational Development, June 30, 1968. U.S. Department of Commerce. Driver Education and Training:-Plans for Evaluating the Effdctiveness of Programs. New York: New YorkCUniversity, May, 1968. U.S. Department of Commerce. Evaluation of Driver Education and Training Programs. [by Harry H. Harman]. ‘Washington,*D.C}: ’Highway Research Board, March, 1969. U.S. Department of Transportation. The Driver Educa- tion Evaluation Program (DEEP) Study. waShington, D.CL: ICovernment Printing Office, 1975. Vernon, R. J., and M. B. Phillips. A Study of Public School Driver Education in Texas. Texas Transporta- tion Institute, Texas A & M University, 1972. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. 89. 127 Waller, Patricia F. "Have the Schools Failed?” Paper presented to the Annual American Medical Association--American School Health Association Session on School Health, New York, 1973. . "How Effective Are Driver and Traffic Safety Programs?" Traffic Safety, July 1976. Warner, W. L. "The Educational Evolution of Instruc- tional Programs of Driver Education." Ph.D. disser- tation, University of Wisconsin, 1969. Weaver, Jack K. "Does Quality Driver Education Pro- duce Safer Drivers?" Journal of Traffic Safety Education, June 1978. Wenzinger, Carl J., Jr. ”Trends in Driver Educa- tion." Journal of Traffic Safety Education, October, 1976. Winningham, Glenn. "New to Simulation? How to Get Help." South Central Regional Educational Repre- sentative Aetna Life & Casualty. Journal of Traffic Safetnyducation, October, 1978. WOrick, W. Wayne. Safety Education: Man, His Machines, and His Envifonment. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1975.