w-vwvvvmwuw—n“‘I‘I‘Q‘l‘fi',’1‘wmfl"3‘9II'Yii'Yfi“ ' ‘ ‘ ."‘.—V .n3u".'.‘. ‘7',‘ .4. ' REINFORCEMENT RELAY Roms EFREcIs ON BERARQRAL CONTRAST " """ ' The 13 1.051113 Degree Rf Fh D ARCLAGAR STATE LARRERSLTY """ RALRR WILLAM RICHARDS x, _, I ' ’ 1971 A . ' * 7'; 131i; LIB E’ARY l\\\\3\\\\\\1\\2\\1§\\\\1\\ All ALMA! “531$?“ This is to certify that the thesis entitled REINFORCEMENT DELAY: SOME EFFECTS ON BEHAVIGIAL CONTRAST presented by Ralph William Richards has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for __Eh.D.._degree in Elf-1191931 AW 0-7639 ’W? 7 ABSTRACT REINFORCEMENT DELAY: SOME EFFECTS ON BEHAVIORAL CONTRAST BY Ralph William Richards Interactions among component schedules of a multiple schedule of reinforcement were examined. Spe- cifically. the present study sought to determine if delaying reinforcement during one component of a mul- tiple schedule would increase responding during the other component, i.e.. produce behavioral contrast. Also examined was the relationship between the duration of reinforcement delay and the amount of behavioral con- trast. The 35 White Carneaux pigeons were conditioned in one of two standard operant conditioning chambers. Subjects received 20 sessions of nondelayed reinforce- ment on a two-component multiple schedule that had iden- tical variable interval 1-minute schedules as the compo- nents. The training stimuli were a 555 nm. light and a l Ralph William Richards white vertical line superimposed on a 555 nm. surround. For the remaining 15 sessions. subjects were assigned to one of 5 groups. with 7 subjects per group. Four of these groups received reinforcement according to the same multiple schedule as before. but with reinforcement during one component delayed for either 2.5. 5. 10. or 120 seconds. The fifth group was switched to a multiple variable interval 1-minute extinction schedule of rein- forcement. Results showed that the delaying of reinforce- ment during one component of a multiple schedule does produce behavioral contrast. Groups that received the various durations of reinforcement delay or even extinc— tion during the altered component did not. however. show a statistically significant difference in the amount of behavioral contrast. In terms of theoretical implica- tions. it was suggested that neither a reduction in rein— forcement frequency or response rate during the altered component is necessary to the production or maintenance of behavioral contrast. Approved: flL I. é (’( ' 1" Date: ‘ j ,_ (I I I REINFORCEMENT DELAY: SOME EFFECTS ON BEHAVIORAL CONTRAST BY Ralph William Richards A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1971 To Nancy ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my sincere thanks to Dr. Mark Rilling whose unrestrictive and yet instructive gamma-ha gar-r— supervision and guidance in the laboratory provided the environment from which this thesis emerged. Dr. Rilling also served as an excellent advisor and chairman of the dissertation committee. for which I am also grateful. Thanks are also due to Drs. M. Ray Denny. Hiram E. Fitzgerald. and Gordon Wood for their advice and service as members of the guidance and dissertation committee. I would also like to thank Jay Wright and Dr. Glenn I. Hatton for their instruction and assistance on the use of certain photographic equipment and mate- rials. Without their help. clear visual presentation of the results would not have been possible. iii LIST OF TABLES . . LIST OF FIGURES. . INTRODUCTION . . . METHOD . . . . . . RESULTS. . . . . . DISCUSSION . . . . LIST OF REFERENCES TABLE OF CONTENTS iv Page vi 33 4O LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. RESPONSES PER MINUTE DURING Sl OVER THE SIX 3-DAY BLOCKS (SESSIONS 23-40) FOR THE GROUPS WHICH RECEIVED EITHER NO REINFORCE- MENT OR REINFORCEMENT THAT WAS DELAYED FOR 2.5. 5. 10 OR lZO-SEC. DURING $2 . . . . . . 25 2. SUMMARY TABLE OF THE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE ON THE 81 RESPONSE RATES OVER THE SIX 3-DAY BLOCKS (SESSIONS 23-40) FOR THE GROUPS .WHICH RECEIVED EITHER NO REINFORCEMENT OR REINFORCEMENT THAT WAS DELAYED FOR 2.5. 5. 10. OR lZO-SEC. DURING 52. . . . . . . . . . 26 3. PEARSON PRODUCT-MOMENT CORRELATIONS BETWEEN THE MAGNITUDE OF THE INCREASE IN 31 RESPOND- ING AND THE MAGNITUDE OF THE DECREASE IN 52 RESPONDING. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 LIST OF FIGURES (Cont.) Figure Page 4. Response rates to S1 (x's) and $2 (circles) over sessions. During the 10 sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line. the schedule of reinforcement was a multiple VI l—min. VI l-min. with no reinforcement delay. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line. sub- jects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. schedule. but with reinforcement during S2 delayed for lZO-sec.. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 5. ReSponse rates to S1 (x's) and $2 (circles) over sessions. During the 10 sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line. the schedule of reinforcement was a multiple VI l-min. VI l—min. with no reinforcement delay. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line. sub— jects were reinforced on a multiple VI l-min. extinction schedule. . . . . . . . . 24 6. Reinforcement rates during Sl (x's) and S (circles) over blocks of sessions (3 sessions per block). During the 3 blocks of sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line. the schedule of reinforce— ment was a multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. with no reinforcement delay. During the 5 blocks of sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line. subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. schedule. but with rein- forcement during 82 delayed for 2.5-sec.. . 28 2 7. Reinforcement rates during S (x's) and S (circles) over blocks of sessions (3 sessions per block). During the 3 blocks of sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line. the schedule of 2 vii INTRODUCTION A multiple schedule requires successive presenta- tion of two or more independent schedules of reinforcement. each in the presence of a different exteroceptive stimulus. Much of the recent research on multiple schedules (e.g.. Reynolds. 1961a; 1961b; 1961c; Reynolds & Limpo. 1968; Terrace. 1968; Weisman. 1969; 1970) has examined the fre- quent interactions that occur between the component sched— ules. An interaction occurs when an organism's behavior during one component in which reinforcement contingencies are held constant is in some way affected by a change in the reinforcement contingencies associated with the other component. Behavioral contrast has been the most widely studied interaction and has been reviewed extensively by Terrace (1966a; 1971a) and Dunham (1968). Behavioral con— trast occurs when the response rate during the unaltered component increases and moves in a direction away from the response rate during the altered component. For example. 1 the changing of a multiple variable interval l-min. var- iable interval l-min. (multiple VI l-min. VI l-min.) schedule to a multiple variable interval l-min. extinction schedule has consistently produced behavioral contrast. i.e.. the response rate during the first component is higher when it is alternated with an extinction conponent than when it is alternated with another variable interval l-min. component. Behavioral contrast is not. however. limited to the special multiple schedule that has extinc- tion as one of its components. as it has been demonstrated in many other multiple schedules. such as multiple vari- able interval l-min. variable interval 5-min. (Guttman. 1959; Terrace. 1968; Weisman. 1969). multiple fixed in— terval l—min. fixed interval 3—min. (Staddon. 1969). multiple variable interval fixed ratio (Reynolds. 1961c; Thompson. 1965: Bloomfield. 1967). multiple variable in- terval differential reinforcement of low response rates (Bloomfield. 1967; Terrace. 1968; Weisman. 1969). and multiple variable interval differential reinforcement of other behavior (Weisman. 1970). It has also been observed when responses during one of the multiple schedule's com— ponents are punished with electric shock (Brethower & Reynolds. 1962; Terrace. 1968). Recent research has required the rejection or modification of several theories of behavioral contrast (e.g.. Reynolds. 1961a; Terrace. 1966a; 1966b; 1968). For example. the demonstration that behavioral contrast can occur without a reduction in reinforcement frequency (Brethower & Reynolds. 1962; Reynolds & Limpo. 1968; Terrace. 1968; Weisman. 1969; 1970) has led to the re— jection of Reynolds' relative frequency of reinforcement hypothesis. Similarly. Terrace (1971a) has modified his earlier position following Wilkie's (1970) demonstration that the delivery of reinforcement independently of the organism's behavior during one component does not produce behavioral contrast and following the suggestion that satiation during one component would not produce behav- ioral contrast. given that different reinforcers were used in the two components (cf. Premack. 1969). While Terrace still. apparently. views behavioral contrast as a by-product of frustration or some other emotional re— Sponses. these emotional responses are now seen to be aroused by a reduction in response rate during one com- ponent that is produced by an inhibition of responding. In each of the previous studies. reinforcement was delivered immediately upon completion of the schedule's requirements. Following the suggestion of several inves— tigators (Brown & Farber. 1951; Holder. Marx. Holder. & Collier. 1957; Amsel. 1958; Renner. 1964) that delayed reinforcement elicits frustration. the present experiment examined the effects of delaying reinforcement during one component of a multiple VI l-min. VI l—min. schedule. Specifically. the present experiment attempted to deter— mine if delaying reinforcement during one component would produce behavioral contrast. Also examined was the rela- tionship between the duration of reinforcement delay and the amount of behavioral contrast. METHOD Subjects The thirty-five experimentally naive. adult. female White Carneaux pigeons were maintained at approximately 80% of their free—feeding weight. Apparatus An Industrial Electronics Engineers in-line display cell (model lO-3723—757-L). mounted behind the right key. illuminated each of the two Operant conditioning chambers (Lehigh Valley Electronics. model 1519). A minimum force of approximately 15-g. was required to operate this key; the other keys were covered with masking tape. No house- 1ight was used. but a white pilot light was attached to the rear wall of both chambers. During reinforcement the key light was extinguished and a light within the food aperture illuminated. Rein- forcement was 2.75-sec. access to mixed grain. The timing of this interval began when the subject placed its head 5 runs”- . through the food aperture. thus. interrupting a light beam focused on a photocell. Standard programming and recording equipment was housed in an adjacent room. Procedure Throughout training the key was illuminated by a 555 nm. light (81) or by a white vertical line superimposed on a 555 nm. surround (82). These stimuli were not ad- justed for equal intensity. The duration of each S and l 52 presentation was 30—sec.. excluding reinforcement time and reinforcement delay time. The stimuli were presented in random order with the restriction that neither stimulus occur more than 3 times in succession. A timeout. during which the chamber was dark and responding nonfunctional. separated all stimulus presentations. During session 1 the timeout duration was 1-sec.. while during all subsequent sessions it was 5—sec. Reinforcements set up but not col- lected during one component did not carry over to the next. The pigeons were conditioned to key peck by the method of successive approximation. The shaping session terminated following 30 consecutive key pecks. Sessions 2 q A '- 0 .r-nn-rdrnn .. and 3 also terminated after 30 continuous reinforcements. During sessions 4 and 5. 3O reinforcements were delivered according to fixed ratio 10 and 20 schedules. respectively. All subjects. then. received 20 sessions (60 stimulus pre— sentations per session) of reinforcement according to a multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. schedule. During all of the above sessions. there was no delay of reinforcement. Each of the subjects was. then. assigned to one of 5 groups. with 7 subjects per group. Four of these groups received 15 additional sessions of multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. reinforcement; but with reinforcement during 82 de- layed for either 2.5. 5. 10. or lZO-sec. During the delay period. the key light was extinguished and the pilot light on the Chamber's back wall was illuminated. At the end of the delay period the pilot light was extinguished and the food magazine raised. The fifth group received 15 sessions of multiple VI 1-min. extinction training; re- sponding to S extinguished the key light for lZO-sec. 2 (according to the same VI l-min. schedule). but no rein- forcement was delivered. All subjects were reinforced for responding to S1 as before. VI l-min. with no reinforcement delay. Four of the subjects in each group were run in one of the conditioning chambers and the remaining subjects were run in the other chamber. ,H b __ -_. RESULTS Figures 1-5 show the reSponse rates to S (denoted 1 by the X's) and S (denoted by the closed circles) during 2 the last 25 sessions of training. Each figure represents one experimental group and contains the data for the 7 in- dividual subjects as well as the group mean. To the left of the dashed vertical line are the final 10 sessions under the multiple VI l—min. VI l-min. schedule with no delay of reinforcement. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line the contingencies for S2 reSponding were altered as indicated by the caption above each figure. That is, the subjects represented in Figures 1—4 continued to be reinforced on the same multiple sched— ule as before, but with reinforcement during S delayed 2 for either 2.5, 5, 10, or lZO-sec. The subjects in Figure 5 were not reinforced for reSponding to S during 2 these last 15 sessions. As shown in Figure l delaying reinforcement for 2.5-sec. during S produced different effects on the S 2 2 9 lO reSponse rates of the individual subjects. The S2 response rates were either temporarily increased and then permanently reduced (bird 38), permanently reduced (birds 609 and 30), temporarily reduced (birds 199 and 206), or not affected (birds 2751 and 4304). The group mean evidences a tempor- ary reduction in S response rate. While the 2.5-sec. 2 delay during 82 was inconsistent in its effect on the S2 . .- '-.:_.x a ”a. ,, 5 rate of response, such was not the case for its effect on the S1 rate of response. The delaying of reinforcement produced an increase in the S re- for 2.5-sec. during S 1 2 Sponse rate for all subjects and, hence, the group mean. As with the 2.5-sec. delay, Figure 2 shows that the 5—sec. delay had not consistent effect on the S re— 2 Sponse rates. The $2 reSponse rates either showed no change (bird (2655), a permanent increase (bird 622), a temporary decrease (birds 435 and 389), or a permanent decrease (birds 32, 2955, and 2549). In terms of S1 re— sponding, five subjects (birds 32, 435, 2655, 389, and 622) showed an increase in reSponse rate. While two of the subjects (birds 2955 and 2549) showed no apparent in— crease in S1 response rate, it should be noted that neither of these subjects reSponded at a stable rate prior to the introduction of the reinforcement delay, i.e., to the left 11 of the dashed vertical line. The mean curve shows a tem— porary decrease in the S response rate and a permanent 2 increase in the S1 reSponse rate. Figure 3 shows that the lO-sec. delay of rein— forcement produced either a temporary (birds 466, 40, 2032, 1702, and 1794) or a permanent (birds 263 and 990) reduc- tion in 82 reSponding. Six subjects (birds 466, 2631, 2032, 1702, 990, and 1794) showed a clear increase in S1 reSponse rate; the remaining subject's (bird 40) S1 re- Sponse rate curve is only suggestive of an increase in the last 3 sessions (sessions 38-40). The mean S1 and 82 re- sponse rate curves for the lO-sec. delay group are similar to those of the 2.5 and 5—sec. delay groups as the former shows a permanent increase and the latter a temporary de- crease. From Figures 4 and 5 it is apparent that delaying reinforcement for 120-sec. is similar to nonreinforcement in that both procedures reduce S reSponding to a near 2 zero level. In terms of S1 reSponse rates, six subjects (birds 1314, 238, 35, 2518, 889, and 2748) in the 120—sec. delay group showed an increase, and all subjects in the extinction group showed an increase. Mean curves show a 12 reduction in 52 rate to near zero and an increase in S1 rate. In order to determine if the 5 groups differed in the amount of increase in S1 response rates, the final 10 sessions were collapsed into six 3-day blocks (Table 1). Thus, the first block is the mean Sl response rate during the last 3 sessions of the multiple VI VI without rein— forcement delay. The remaining five blocks represent mean 81 rates during the sessions where reinforcement during S2 was either delayed or discontinued. The results of a two—way (trials by groups) analysis of variance with re— peated measures on one of the factors (trials) are pre— sented in Table 2. From this table it is quite clear that the only significant effect was the increase in S response 1 rate over sessions due to the changes in the $2 contin- gencies. The groups did not differ significantly in the magnitude of the S1 response rate increase. Several Pearson product-moment correlations were computed to determine if the magnitude of the increase in S1 responding could be predicted by the magnitude of the decrease in $2 responding. The magnitude of the S1 re— Sponse rate increase was taken as: (the mean S response 1 rate calculated from sessions 23-25)—-(the highest daily 13 S1 reSponse rate during sessions 26-40). The magnitude of the 82 response rate decrease was taken as: (the mean 82 response rate calculated from sessions 23-25)--(the lowest daily S response rate during sessions 26—40). The 2 obtained correlations for each group, as well as the over- all correlation, are presented in Table 3. These corre- rflur-m:r:r-T— . ._.E lations are small and show no consistency in even their sign. Figures 6-8 show the rate of reinforcement during Sl (denoted by the X's) ands2 @enoted by the closed circles) for the subjects in the 2.5, 5, and lO—sec. rein— forcement delay groups. The rate of reinforcement was computed by dividing the number of reinforcements obtained during S (or 82) by the total time that the key was illum— 1 inated by S (or $2). In these figures 3 sessions were 1 combined to form each block. The 3 blocks of sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line were computed from the last 9 sessions of the multiple VI VI with no rein- forcement delay (sessions 17—25). The 5 blocks to the right of the dashed vertical line were computed from the 15 sessions of the multiple VI VI where reinforcemnt during S was delayed (sessions 26-40). As can be seen in these 2 figures, most subjects showed little change in $2 14 reinforcement rate as a result of either the 2.5, 5, or lO-sec. delay contingency. Any reductions, such as shown by birds 4304, 2955, 389, 2631, and 990, were of small magnitude. The reinforcement rates for the 120—sec. delay group are not presented since all of these subjects, ex- cept for bird 1129, showed a reduction in S reinforcement 2 frequency to almost zero. 15 Fig. l.--Response rates to S1 (x's) and $2 (circles) over sessions. During the 10 sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line, the schedule of reinforce- ment was a multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. with no rein— forcement delay. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line, subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI 1-min. VI l-min. schedule, but with reinforcement during 82 delayed for 2.5—sec. RESPONSES PER MINUTE l6 2.5-SEC. DELAY IIO '- I?! Fig. l J f_ {@3121 17 Fig. 2.--Response rates to S (x's) and $2 (circles) over sessions. During the 10 sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line, the schedule of reinforce- ment was a multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. with no rein- forcement delay. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line, subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. schedule, but with reinforcement during 82 delayed for 5-sec. RESPONSES PER MINUTE 18 5—SEC. DELAY '°° WA : . :. l 1 L 1': E i?— < f». . :WIAWV “J .. ,. 3:; Fig. 2 19 Fig. 3.--Response rates to S (x's) and S (circles) over sessions. During the 1 sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line, the schedule of reinforce- ment was a multiple VI l-min. VI l—min. with no rein— forcement delay. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line, subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI l-min. VI l—min. schedule, but with reinforcement during 82 delayed for 10—sec. 20 lO-SEC. DELAY «an». «nun». «as». “mm” «a. a j aaaaa J ‘1 ..... j. .V. 4 (J. l... A. ll ‘\ ,YIIIIII'IIIIIIII I'll-I'll. we; 322.2 EA. mmmzoammm / J]! A. SESSIONS 3 Fig. 21 Fig. 4.--ReSponse rates to S (x's) and $2 (circles) over sessions. During the 10 sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line, the schedule of reinforce- ment was a multiple VI 1-min. VI 1-min. with no rein— forcement delay. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line, subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI 1—min. VI l—min. schedule, but with reinforcement during S2 delayed for 120-sec. RESPONSES PER MINUTE IZO-SEC. DELAY an t“ d )- 5? at. M143 Fig. 4 23 Fig. 5.—-Response rates to S (x's) and S (circles) over sessions. During 10 sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line, the schedule of reinforcement was a mul- tiple VI l-min. VI l—min. with no reinforcement delay. During the 15 sessions to the right of the dahsed ver- tical line, subjects were reinforced on a multiple VI l-min. extinction schedule. RESPONSES PER MINUTE 24 EXTINCTION Finn no I20 P d '20 I00 P -4 I- /\ d IOO .0 ’- M'K '1 ‘ I. ‘ \ ‘\{"'I d .0 ’M II \x/ x I” so .- ’H - - / .. to I" 2° F a d h l; d ‘0 to - q .- co ‘ * kw] . I m -I -‘ .0 wk. \ / « - ”IA‘/H\/\/ .. co m - " M 4 40 "“ ‘I r- . - co .. I- -A O 111111111111 V I- " 1:00 .4 I- A V‘/ - so P D“ .0 . 5 4 ISO '1 M0 '4 Ito dDO ~00 ‘00 :40 ‘80 25 TABLE 1 RESPONSES PER MINUTE DURING Sl OVER THE SIX 3-DAY BLOCKS (SESSIONS 23-40) FOR THE GROUPS WHICH RECEIVED EITHER NO REINFORCEMENT OR REINFORCEMENT THAT WAS DELAYED FOR 2.5, 5, 10, OR lZO-SEC. DURING S 2 3-day blocks computed from sessions Bird 23-25 26-28 29-31 32-34 35-37 38-40 199 44.5 33.6 52.3 48.4 55.4 67.7 3‘ 30 37.9 71.7 74.0 87.6 81.9 59.7 '3 2751 46.4 52.2 59.0 59.8 59.9 72.3 R 609 57.3 64.0 70.6 69.0 73.1 72.7 6 206 90.9 87.1 98.2 95.3 117.3 124.6 3 38 42.3 51.6 59.8 73.5 77.6 83.1 A 4304 57.6 57.7 70.1 84.6 97.7 75.4 a; mean 53.8 59.7 69.1 74.0 80.4 79.4 2655 47.4 48.8 57.7 66.3 76.3 84.7 %. 622 65.0 74.3 92.2 91.9 89.6 97.2 .3 32 40.5 43.1 41.8 43.2 49.7 47.3 6 2955 47.8 47.6 71.5 56.9 31.4 61.9 6 2549 95.0 102.8 114.6 97.2 104.1 99.9 g 389 38.1 42.6 46.3 52.6 57.9 56.7 .5 435 58.0 54.1 89.4 103.6 95.7 87.8 mean 56.0 59.0 73.4 73.1 72.1 76.5 990 43.8 47.6 46.2 56.4 58.4 57.7 5* 466 46.0 60.2 89.4 87.2 77.7 71.6 3 2032 36.1 40.4 40.2 54.2 67.7 '67.7 '9 2631 77.8 95.1 103.9 104.2 119.4 108.9 5 1702 25.6 48.3 66.7 78.0 78.3 74.3 g 40 48.2 48.6 39.8 38.6 48.6 61.9 g, 1794 44.2 44.5 57.6 67.2 80.9 66.2 '9 mean 46.0 55.0 63.4 69.4 75.9 72.6 A, 1314 19.6 28.2 35.4 34.6 35.3 39.4 ,3 889 20.5 30.7 35.6 35.9 38.2 38.2 g 2518 32.3 41.3 60.7 71.4 65.0 65.6 . 1129 91.8 83.8 91.4 73.4 82.6 77.9 8 2748 20.5 44.2 65.7 67.6 76.1 67.8 T 35 58.5 92.3 79.1 57.5 66.5 66.2 8 238 81.8 57.4 94.6 87.6 88.0 99.3' H mean 46.4 54.0 66.1 61.1 64.5 64.9 562 18.2 22.6 43.4 58.2 55.4 71.2 28 67.1 66.1 61.0 81.3 80.5 78.3 8 181 32.4 44.1 . 59.2 61.5 57.8 59.1 13 889 26.9 49.7 72.8 70.9 96.2 83.4 3 1258 35.8 51.8 52.1 47.0 46.8 52.4 ‘3 2520 79.0 119.3 96.4 82.2 79.2 87.2 S 4306 80.0 74.9 116.8 150.9 136.3 124.4 mean 48.5 61.2 71.7 78.9 78.9 79.4 26 TABLE 2 SUMMARY TABLE OF THE ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE ON THE 51 RESPONSE RATES OVER THE SIX 3-DAY BLOCKS (SESSIONS 23-40) FOR THE GROUPS WHICH RECEIVED EITHER NO REINFORCEMENT OR REINFORCEMENT THAT WAS DELAYED FOR 2.5. 5. 10. OR lZO—SEC. DURING 52. Source of Variance SS df MS F Between 85 A (groups) 3299.706 4 824.926 <1 gs within groups 78633.622 30 2621.120 Within Ss B (blocks of sessions) 17424.125 5 3484.825 27.64l* AB 1389.556 20 69.477 <1 B x 85 within groups 18911.152 150 126.074 *p < .001 TABLE 3 PEARSON PRODUCT-MOMENT CORRELATIONS BETWEEN THE MAGNITUDE OF THE INCREASE IN 31 RESPONDING AND THE MAGNITUDE OF THE DECREASE IN 82 RESPONDING 2.5-sec. 5-sec. lO-sec. 120-sec. Extinction overall +.37 +.26 -.02 -.11 -.O3 +.O9 27 Fig. 6.--Reinforcement rates during S1 (x's) and $2 (circles) over blocks of sessions (3 sessions per block). During the 3 blocks of sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line. the schedule of rein- forcement was a multiple VI l-min. VI 1-min. with no reinforcement delay. During the 5 blocks of sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line. subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. schedule. but with reinforcement during S2 delayed for 2.5-sec. 28 2.5-SEC. DELAY 2345678 BLOCKS OF SESSIONS 345678 I 2 Fig. 6 5. o. 5. _ — _ .A l X. l i I o X I X o I ‘.~.‘~‘. x I R I v0 I on I X .. a w m m X o I v. I 0x I 3 X 14 LE L» I I v. I X o I X I X. I X. I In I X0 I v0 _1 ”I .l v. I. Y. .I 'n I. CA I. .A I M I: X o X X o X ‘M‘..... .‘.~.~.f~..~.‘ .‘.-.~ ! .1 CA I OVA X. M X I w R I .an 0X I 6 0X I E - a m M v. I on X. 0X I _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ 5. 0. 6 5. 0. 5. 5. 0. 5. 5. 0. 5. wb323¢ mwa MFZNZNUKOuZEm '1‘ II In! I I 29 Fig. 7.--Reinforcement rates during 51 (x's) and S2 (circles) over blocks of sessions (3 sessions per block). During the 3 blocks of sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line. the schedule of rein- forcement was a multiple VI 1-min. VI 1—min. with no reinforcement delay. During the 5 blocks of sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line. subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. schedule but with reinforcement during S2 delayed for 5—sec. 30 5-SEC. DELAY J l RXXXX ‘ I. l _ 5 LG r—X- x N A m [T w 32 .1 II ‘NNNNNN‘NNNNNN‘N I v. .— 50.5 mhzwzwbmouzfim 18L 5O 5. 0 5. 5. 0. 5. 5. 0. 5. 5. 0. 5. _ _ _ _ _ A _ u. _ Xe. _ I v... .4 x U r X. I X. I v. I X. L CA I. VC .J X C I. V. 1 0X 0 X I o X I X ‘M‘..... -~..‘. .‘..‘.~ .‘.‘~.‘ 5 9 9 2 . % X o 8 X M. X o l 2 X 2 I 3 2 6 2345678 BLOCKS OF SESSIONS .7 Fig. 31 Fig. 8.--Reinforcement rates during Sl (x's) and S2 (circles) over blocks of sessions (3 sessions per block). During the 3 blocks of sessions to the left of the dashed vertical line. the schedule of rein- forcement was a multiple VI l-min. VI 1-min. with no reinforcement delay. During the 5 blocks of sessions to the right of the dashed vertical line. subjects continued to be reinforced on the same multiple VI l-min. VI l-min. schedule. but with reinforcement during 82 delayed for lO-sec. 32 lO-SEC. DELAY 5 O. 5 5 O. 5 5 O. 5 5. O. 5. . X N I V. I VA C I .A I CVA VAC I C VA I VC I X I X J 8 I X L M I C VA I M I VAC I 2 3 Mu. W 4 O I l l 2 v0. W V. 9 0X m I I R . I. A L M I CVA I C VA I C I V. I CA I v6 I VA C I ‘NNNNNNNNNNNNNNNNNNNNNN NNNNNNN CA I VC I v. C X N v. I 6 i I 0 X o I I X. I a 6 4 3 4. 6 M 00 I c X I X o I 2 l I _ _ P F _ _ F _ _ b L! _ _ _ 5. o. 5. 5. o. 5 5. o. 5. 5 o. 5 w k 3 z _ 2 mun 3523461sz 2345678 BLOCKS OF SESSIONS 345678 Fig. 8 DISCUSSION Behavioral contrast is said to occur if. following a change in the S reinforcement contingencies the increased 2 S1 response rate changes in a direction away from that main- tained by S 2. The present experiment was conducted to de- termine whether the delaying of reinforcement during one component of a multiple VI 1-min. VI 1-min. schedule would produce behavioral contrast. The relationship between the duration of reinforcement delay and the amount of behav— ioral contrast produced. if any. was also examined. From Figures 1-4 it is quite clear that the delay- ing of reinforcement during one of the multiple schedule's components did produce behavioral contrast. 0f the 28 sub- jects that received delayed reinforcement during S 24 2. clearly showed behavioral contrast. Three of the four sub— jects that failed to show behavioral contrast (Birds 1129. 2955. and 2549) also failed to show stable response rates prior to the introduction of the reinforcement delay. The responding of the remaining subject (Bird 40) was 33 34 suggestive of behavioral contrast but only during the last 3 sessions of the reinforcement delay contingency. Using a modified discrete—trial paradigm. where both S1 and 82 were associated with different durations of reinforcement delay and where the delay was not associated with a stimulus change. Keller (1970) also demonstrated be- havioral contrast. The conclusion that delayed reinforce- ment produces behavioral contrast. thus. seems to hold across large variations in procedure. Groups that received reinforcement delays of 2.5. 5. 10. or 120—sec. or even extinction during S2 did not show a statistically significant difference in the magni- tude of behavioral contrast. This failure to find magni- tude differences may. however. be due more to the between- subjects design employed than to the absence of a rela- tionship. Most studies concerned with the amount of be— havioral contrast (e.g.. Reynolds. 1961b; 1963; Thompson. 1965; Bloomfield. 1967) have employed a within-subjects design. There were. however. two related reasons for the present study's between-subjects design. Since behavioral contrast follows a temporal course and even disappears after extended training (Terrace. 1966b). a within-subjects design would require counterbalancing the presentation 35 order of the different delay durations. But. the effect of a particular duration of reinforcement delay has also been shown to depend on the manner in which it is introduced (Ferster. 1953). Thus. a particular reinforcement delay might have different effects depending on whether it was presented at full duration (not preceded by shorter rein- forcement delays) or gradually increased over sessions (preceded by shorter reinforcement delays). So.“while there were distinct advantages to using a betwee-subjects design. such a design may not have been the most sensitive to differences in the magnitude of be- havioral contrast. It should be noted. however. that a recent between-subjects experiment (Taus & Hearst. 1970) was able to show statistically significant differences in the amount of behavioral contrast. ‘ A second possible reason for the failure to find a relationship between duration of delay and the amount of behavioral contrast concerns the value of the baseline VI schedule. Reynolds (1963) suggests that the reinforcement rate during the unaltered component in part determines the magnitude of behavioral contrast. Perhaps. the use of a VI 3-min. baseline. which Reynolds has shown to be quite sensitive to behavioral contrast. would have been more 36 appropriate. A third possibility is that the function relating delay duration to the amount of behavioral con- trast reaches asymptote at or before the minimum delay duration of the present study (2.5—sec.). Indeed. a more positive conclusion might have been possible if some shorter delays had been included. Results of the present study also have important implications for conditions necessary to the production of behavioral contrast. From Figures 6-8 it is clear that behavioral contrast can be produced without reducing the reinforcement rate during the altered component. This finding is in agreement with several studies that were specifically designed to hold reinforcement rate constant during the altered component (e.g.. Terrace. 1968; Weisman. 1969; 1970). Terrace (1971a) maintains that behavioral contrast is a by-product of some emotional responses that are pro- duced by the subject's reduced and inhibited responding during the altered component. 0f the 21 subjects in the 2.5. 5. and lO-sec. delay groups. four showed no reduction in S2 response rate; however. all of these showed behav- ioral contrast. Nine subjects showed only a temporary re— duction in S2 responding and eight of these showed 37 behavioral contrast. 0f the eight that did permanently reduce $2 responding. six showed behavioral contrast. Clearly. a permanent reduction in responding during the altered components is not necessary to the production or maintenance of behavioral contrast. The present data further suggest that behavioral contrast may occur regard- less of any_reduction in responding during the altered component. While only 4 subjects did show behavioral contrast without a reduction in responding during the altered com- ponent. it does seem that Terrace's position needs some modification. Keeping within the rubric of his emotion— ality theory. it may be that the response rate reduction. like behavioral contrast. was just a frequent by-product of some emotional responses elicited by the reinforcement delay. In support of this interpretation. it should be noted that in many cases the response rate reduction. like behavioral contrast (Terrace. 1966b). disappeared with further training. Extended training (Terrace's subjects received 60 sessions) might have shown the response rate reduction to be temporary in all subjects. If one accepts the emotionality interpretation of behavioral contrast. there still remains the problem of 38 specifying the necessary and sufficient conditions that produce the frustration or other emotional responses. Guttman's suggestion that the weaker of two reinforcement schedules can become "functionally negative." while in- trinsically appealing. is not very useful. There are no specified or readily apparent criteria for classifying one of two schedules of equal reinforcement frequency as the weaker. For example it is unclear why a DRO should be con- sidered weaker than a VI of equal reinforcement frequency. and. yet. it is quite clear that changing a multiple VI VI to a multiple VI DRO does produce behavioral contrast (Weisman. 1970). Premack's (1969) suggestion that Contrast results if and only if there is a change in the aversiveness associated with one of the components in the schedule. [p. 136] may be the most workable hypothesis at present. This hy- pothesis is. of course. quite similar to Bloomfield's (1969) contention that behavioral contrast is produced by "a worsening of conditions" during one of the components. If behavioral contrast occurs the stimulus associated with the altered component should possess all the properties of other aversive stimuli--subjects should learn a new response to escape from the stimulus and the stimulus should serve as an elicitor of aggression and as a 39 punisher of ongoing behavior. Likewise. if no contrast is observed. these properties should not be present. Although a beginning has been made in studying multiple schedules within these different paradigms (e.g.. Rilling. Askew. Ahlskog. & Kramer. 1969; Rilling. Kramer. & Richards. 1971; Terrace. 1971b; Weisman & Premack. 1966). more research will be required before a definitive conclusion can be made. LIST OF REFERENCES LIS T OF REFERENCES Amsel. A. The role of frustrative non-reward in noncon- tinuous reward situations. Psychological Bulletin. l958._§5. 102-119. Bloomfield. T. M. Behavioral contrast and relative rein— forcement frequency in two multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1967..lg. 151-158. Bloomfield. T. M. Behavioral contrast and the peak shift. In R. M. Gilbert and N. S. Sutherland (Eds.) Animal discrimination learning. London: Academic Press. Pp. 215-241. Brethower. D. M. and Reynolds. G. S. A facilitative effect of punishment on unpunished behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1962. 5. 191-199. Brown. J. S. and Farber. I. E. Emotions conceptualized as intervening variables-~with suggestions toward a theory of frustration. Psychological Bulletin. 1951. 48, 465-495. Dunham. P. J. Contrasted conditions of reinforcement: a selective critique. Psychological Bulletin. 1968. 62, 295-315. Ferster. C. B. Sustained behavior under delayed reinforce- ment. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1953. 45, 218-224. Guttman. N. Generalization gradients around stimuli asso- ciated with different reinforcement schedules. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1959. 58, 335- 340. 40 41 Holder. W.. Marx. M.. Holder. Elaine. and Collier. G. Response strength as a function of delay of reward in a runway. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1957. 2;. 316-323. Keller. J. V. Behavioral contrast under multiple delays of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science. 1970. 39_(5). 257-258. Premack. D. On some boundary conditions of contrast. In J. Tapp (Ed.). Reinforcement and behavior. New York: Academic Press. 1969. Pp. 120-145. Renner. K. E. Delay of reinforcement: A historical re— view. Psychological Bulletin. 1964. §;, 341-361. Reynolds. G. S. Behavioral contrast. Journal of the Ex- perimental Analysis of Behavior. 1961a. 4, 57-71. Reynolds. G. S. Relativity of response rate and reinforce- ment frequency in a multiple schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1961b. 4, 179-184. Reynolds. G. S. Contrast. generalization. and the process of discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1961c. 4, 289-294. Reynolds. G. S. Some limitations on behavioral contrast and induction during successive discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1963. g. 131-139. Reynolds. G. S. and Limpo. A. J. On some causes of behav— ioral contrast. Journal of the Experimental Anal- ysis of Behavior. 1968. 44, 543-547. Rilling. M.. Askew. H. R.. Ahlskog. J. E.. and Kramer. T. J. Aversive properties of the negative stimulus in a successive discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1969. 44, 917- 932. 42 Rilling. M.. Kramer. T. J.. and Richards. R. W. Aversive properties of the negative stimulus during learning with and without errors. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological Association meeting. New York. 1971. Staddon. J. E. R. Multiple fixed-interval schedules: Transient contrast and temporal inhibition. Jour— nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1969. 4;, 583-590. Taus. Sharron E. and Hearst. E. Effects of intertrial (blackout) duration on response rate to a positive stimulus. Psychonomic Science. 1970. 12_(5). 265-266. Terrace. H. S. Stimulus control. In W. K. Honig (Ed.). Operant behavior: Areas of research and applica- tion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. 1966a. Pp. 271-344. Terrace. H. S. Behavioral contrast and the peak shift. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1966b..2. 613-617. Terrace. H. S. Discrimination learning. the peak-shift. and behavioral contrast. Journal of the Experi- mental Analysis of Behavior. 1968. 44, 727-741. Terrace. H. S. By-products of discrimination learning. In Bower and Spence (Eds.). Learning and motiva- tion. 1971a. y, in press. Terrace. H. S. Escape form S-. Learning and motivation. 1971b. in press. Thompson. D. M. Punishment by SD associated with fixed ratio reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1965. g, 189—194. Weisman. R. G. Some determinants of inhibitory stimulus control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1969. 43, 443-450. 43 Weisman. R. G. Factors influencing inhibitory stimulus control: differential reinforcement of other be- havior during discrimination training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 1970. 44, 87—91. Weisman. R. G. and Premack. D. Positive and negative generalization gradients depending on the relative rates of reinforcement in two components of a schedule. Paper read at Psychonomic Science meet- ing. St. Louis. 1966. Wilkie. D. M. On some determinants of behavioral contrast. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Manitoba. 1970. I‘IIC AIIIIIIIIIIIAIIIIII“