SYSTEM VARIABLES AND AGRICULTURAL INNOVATIVENESS III EASTERN NIGERIA Thesis for the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY BURL EDWARD DAVIS - 1968 LIBRARY Michigan State University IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIQIIIIIIIIIIIIIISIIIQIIIIIIIIIIIIIII This is to certifg that the thesis entitled SYST I3". VI‘IRIA BL ES \ND .‘IGR I CULT URN INEIICVAT IV EN EL) 5 III EA ST 13115 N 13 ERIA presented bg BURL SJCJI‘IRJ L)?‘."JI';} has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for 1 I1 . 3 degree in Communication 0-169 ABSTRACT The present study was designed to explore the relations between individual modernizing characteristics of Nigerian farmers, the cor— responding system characteristics, (the mean level of the individual characteristics in each of the villages), and the individuals' innovao tiveness. Of primary concern was the interaction between the two .levels of modernizing characteristics in explaining greater amounts of variance in innovativeness. A portion of the data gathered in the three-nation Diffusion Project conducted by the Department of Communication, Michigan State University, and Sponsored by the United States Agency for International Development, was analyzed. Eighteen villages were chosen as the basis for the study. The sample was comprised of 1,1u2 respondents, who were in each case a male, the head of his household, at least 20 years old, and farming some amount of land. Fourteen agricultural innovations were selected for the study including: fertilizer, NS-l maize, Aldrin dust, poultry, oil palm rehabilitation, community plantations, citrus, rice, improved cassava, vegetable seeds, cocoa planting schemes, rubber planting scheme, cashew, and PAID agricultural credit program. Burl Edward Davis Seventeen characteristics of individuals were utilized as inde- pendent variables, including cosmopoliteness, education, familiem, educational aspiration, empathy, knowledge of agricultural workers, economic aspiration, literacy, newspaper exposure, radio exposure, film exposure, agricultural media exposure, credit orientation, level of living, correct agricultural knowledge, formal social participation, and achievement motivation. Each of these variables was aggregated through a measure of central tendency (mean) and utililzed as an inde- pendent variable at the system level. Three guiding hypotheses were selected as the focus of the analysis: (1) that the individual level of innovativeness for respon- dents is positively related to the individual variables, (2) that the individual level of innovativeness is positively related to the system variables, and (3) that the individual level of innovativeness is positively related to the joint influence of the individual and system variables. Three statistical methods were utilized in the analysis of the data: (1) zero-order correlation, (2) first-order and higher-order partial correlations, and (3) multiple correlation. First, zero-order correlations were computed between innova- tiveness and each independent variable. Then, higher-order partial correlations between innovativeness and each of the independent variables were computed, holding constant all other independent variables. To determine the contribution of the system variables, partial correlations were computed for each of the system variables, with the individual level variables held constant. Burl Edward Davis After submitting the independent variables to both zero-order and higher-order partial correlation analysis, the best predictors of innovativeness (variables most highly correlated with innovativeness) from the system variables and the individual variables were included in the multiple correlation analysis, using the multiple regression least squares delete computer program. When zero-order correlations were computed between the inde- pendent variables and innovativeness, 15 of the 17 individual level variables were significantly correlated with innovativeness: cosmo- politeness, education, familism, educational aspiration, literacy, newspaper exposure, level of living, radio exposure, film exposure, agricultural media exposure, correct agricultural knowledge, and formal social participation. System level variables significantly correlated with innovativeness were education, familism, educational aspiration, achievement motivation, empathy, knowledge of agricultural workers, economic aspiration, literacy, newspaper exposure, radio exposure, film exposure, agricultural media exposure, level of living, correct agricultural knowledge and formal social participation. In general, individual level variables were more highly correlated with innova- tiveness than system level variables. First-order partial correlations were computed separately for individual level and system level variables with innovativeness. First, individual variables were correlated with innovativeness with system variables held constant. All but three of the individual level variables were significantly correlated with innovativeness. Next, system level variables were correlated with innovativeness with Burl Edward Davis individual level variables held constant. "Only 7 of_the 17 system variables reached statistical significance in this analysis. Highest-order partial correlations were then computed, to select variables from both individual and system levels most highly correlated with innovativeness, to be included in a multiple cor- relation analysis. Of the 34 variables submitted to the least squares delete computer program, ll were deleted when they did not reach the minimum statistical significance criterion. Thus, a total of 23 variables were retained in the analysis as statistically signi- ficant after the highest-order partial correlation analysis, and these were combined in a multiple correlation analysis. The multiple correlation coefficient for these 23 variables was .68, with a coefficient of determination of .H7. When the individual level variables only were submitted to highest order partial correlation analysis, only six were retained as statistically significant. When entered into a multiple correla- tion analysis, these six individual level variables produced a multiple correlation coefficient of .62, and a coefficient of deter— mination of .38. The difference between the multiple correlation coefficient for individual variables only, and the multiple cor- relation coefficient including both individual and system variables was statistically significant ( p<:.05 ). Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Communication, College of Communication Arts, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree. Guidance Committee: ,Zflfldm, K /%(/c//é€ 27 at. 3 Item-- xii/Me— SYSTEM VARIABLES AND AGRICULTURAL INNOVATIVENESS IN EASTERN NIGERIA by Burl Edward Davis A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication 1968 Kym? (fizz/<07 PREFACE The research on which this dissertation was based was conducted in Eastern Nigeria during 1966-67. The Regional Government of Eastern Nigeria seceeded from the Federation of Nigeria in May, 196T and became the Republic of Biafra. As this dissertation goes to press, civil strife is still raging in the area and neither the present nor future status of the territory is not clear. Thus, the name "Eastern Nigeria" is used throughout this report. A tragic aftermath of the civil strife in this nation is that a large proportion of the res— pondents included in the present study are now no longer living, in all probability. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author wishes to express his sincere appreciation to the chairman of his guidance committee, Dr. Everett M. Rogers, for the assistance and counsel tendered, not only during the writing of the author's dissertation, but during his entire graduate program. Gratitude is also extended to the other membbrs of the author‘s committee, Dr. Hideya Kumata, Dr. Mason Miller, and Dr. Bradley S. Greenberg. The author is also indebted to the United States Agency for International Development and the Department of Communication, Michigan State University, for permission to make use of data collected in Eastern Nigeria for the project, "Diffusion of Innova- tions in Rural Societies." Thanks are also expressed to the numerous co-workers and fellow graduate students in the Department of Communication for the insights and theoretical assistance gained through many hours of discussion and consultation. Special thanks are due to Anant Saxena, who was of great assistance during the analysis of data for this report, and Niels Roling, Graham Kerr, and Joe Ascroft, who furnished invaluable background knowledge and helpful suggestions based on intimate knowledge of the conduct of the project in Nigeria. The author expresses his sincere appreciation to Miss Judy MacGregor, for her many hours of work spent in typing and collating the manuscript. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter I. INTRODUCTION AND DESCRIPTION OF THE PROBLEM Diffusion Research . . . Growth of Cross-Cultural Research Focus of the Present Study The Role of Communication Value of the Present Study Setting for the Study . Objectives of the Present Study General Outline of Following Chapters II. THEORETIC RATIONALE e . . . . Review of Literature . . System Analysis . . . . Theoretic Rationale . . How to Determine System Effects Some Possible Fallacies Dependent Variable . . . Description of Innovations Independent Variables . III. RESEARCH METHODOLOGY . . . . The Diffusion Project . Conduct of the Study . . Phase One . . . . . Phase Two . . . . . Characteristics of the Sample Knowledge of Agricultural Innovations Adoption of Agricultural Innovations Analysis of the Data . . Operationalization of the Variables O O Dependent Variable . . . . Independent Variables Construction of Variables IV. RESEARCH FINDINGS . . . . . . Correlates of Innovativeness . Individual Characteristics System Characteristics . . Comparison of Zero—Order and Variables . . . 0 O 0 Partial Confirmation of Hypotheses . . . . . Analysis for System Effects iv 0 0 Correlations Joint Influence of Individual and System O O Page F'F‘ ooooooucwI-o |-‘ M 12 18 22 27 30 33 3W 37 II? 47 49 49 51 53 55 59 62 63 63 65 69 71 71 71 76 79 84 89 94 Chapter Implications for Change Agencies Implications for Future Research .V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS I Summary I: e e b e Conclusions .. .. Limitations of Present Study BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . e Page 98 98 103 106 109 110 116 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. List of Innovations Studied in Phases I and II in Eastern Nigeria . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 2. Zero-Order Intercorrelations of Newspaper Exposure, Radio Exposure, and Film Exposure . . . . . . . . . 43 3. List of Independent Variables and Their Hypothesized Relationship to Innovativeness . . . . . . . . . . . 46 u. Village Populations, Sample Sizes, and Sampling Rates forNigeria,PhaseII..........e....51+ 5. Personal Characteristics of the Respondents . . . . . . . 56 6. Number of Innovations Respondents Knew Correctly . . . . 57 7. Respondents' Correct Information About 1n Agricultural Innovations O O O O ‘ O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O 0 58 8. Percentage of Respondents Adopting Agricultural Innovations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 9. Percentage of Respondents Adopting Each of the la Agricultural Innovations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 10. Zero-Order Correlations of Individual Level Variables and System Level Variables with Innovativeness . . . 73 ll. First—Order Partial Correlations of Individual Level Variables, System Level Variables with Innova- tiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75 I2. Sixteenth-Order Partial Correlations of Six Individual Level Variables with Innovativeness, After Deletion of ll Variables Not Reaching Minimum Significance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 13. Thirty-Third Order Partial Correlations of 3M Inde- pendent Variables with Innovativeness . . . . . . . 80 la. Comparison of Zero-Order and First-Order Partial Cor- relations of Both Individual and System Variables with Innovativeness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 vi Table Page 15. First-Order Multiple Correlations of Each Variable with Innovativeness, Including Botn Individual andSYStemLCVQISeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee86 16. Twenty-Second Order Partial Correlations of 23 Inde- pendent Variables with Innovativeness, After Deletion of 11 Variables Not Reaching Minimum Significance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90 17. Multiple Correlation Coefficients and Coefficients of Determination of Individual Level Variables, System Level Variables, and Individual and System Level Variables Considered Simultaneously . . . . o 91 18. Comparison of Zero-Order Correlations of 3% Individual and System Level Variables with Three Indices of Innovativeness ( N = 1,1u2 ) . . . . . . . . . . . 126 19. Intercorrelations Between lu-Item, 6-Item, and 3-Item Indices of Innovativeness ( N = 1,1u2 ) . . . . . . 128 20. Comparison of Zero-Order Product-Moment Correlation (r) and Coefficient of Nonlinear Relationship (eta) for an Individual and System Level Inde- pendent Variables and Innovativeness (N = 1,1u2) . 131 21. Mean Values and Standard Deviations for 17 Individual Level, 17 System Level Variables (N = 1,142) . . . 133 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Comparison of Conventional Direct Effects Model, and Hypothesized Direct-Plus—Mediational Effects Model of Innovativeness . . . . . . . . . . . 26 viii APPENDIX A: APPENDIX B: APPENDIX C: APPENDIX D: LIST OF APPENDICES Correction for Inflated Multiple Correlation . . . Alternative Indices of Innovativeness. . . . . . . AnaIYSis Of cuPVilinearity e e e e e e e e e e e e Mean Values and Standard Deviations of Independent Variables . ix 0 O O Page 123 125 129 133 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION AND DESCRIPTION OF PROBLEM The introduction of technological innovations in traditional agricul- ture is one of the basic elements in rural development programs in less developed countries. The study described in the present thesis represents an attempt to understand one such development program in Eastern Nigeria. Consideration is given to social, psychological, cultural, and overt behav- ioral factors in attempting to describe and explain the processes of diffusion and adoption of fourteen agricultural innovations by individuals in eighteen Nigerian villages. Primary attention is given to the influence of system variables in increasing the amount of variance in innovativeness explained. Diffusion Research For a number of years, research workers in the field of communication and other social sciences have been interested in the spread and acceptance of new ideas. The diffusion of innovations in social systems and among individuals involves the communication of information about the practices from their sources of origin, and their acceptance by the end-users or adop- ters. Generally, the analysis of the diffusion of innovations involves four crucial elements: (1) the innovation, (2) its communication from one individual to another, (3) in a social system, (A) over time (Rogers, 1962, p. 12). Since its initiation in the 1920's, diffusion research has produced an imposing body of research findings, both in the United States and in 1 other nations. Typically, the findings have pointed out the individual, group, and situational elements which influence the innovation-decision process. The elements involved in the investigation of diffusion pro- cesses are manifold; however, a few have received major emphases, for various reasons. For instance, diffusion researchers generally agree that adop- ters can be categorized according to their individual tendencies to be relatively early or late to adopt a new practice. Operationally, receivers are divided into categories according to their relative innova- tiveness. For any given innovation, the period of time since each ad0pted the innovation is recorded and standardized across the unit of analysis (e.g., the village, region, or state). Assuming a normal dis- tribution, and utilizing standard deviations, the earliest 2.5 percent are classified as innovators; the next 13.5 percent, early adopters; the next 3M percent, early majority; another 3n percent, late majority; and the final 16 percent as laggards (Rogers, 1962, pp. 160-171). Another major area of interest in the diffusion research tradition centers around the explication of the innovation decision-making process. The North Central Regional Rural Sociology Subcommittee for the Study of Diffusion of Farm Practices (1961) divided the adoption process into five stages: awareness, interest, evaluation, trial, and adoption. Dissatis- faction with a five-stage conceptualization led others to propose other classifications. Rogers with Shoemaker (in press) speak of four "functions" (rather than "stages"): knowledge, persuasion, decision, and confirmation. Other major generalizations from the diffusion research tradition deal with the S-shaped curve of adoption, the role of the various communication channels at the different stages of the adoption process, and the characteristics of the innovation itself that enhance (or retard) its rate of adoption. Growth of Cross-Cultural Research One striking development in the field of diffusion research is the marked increase in the number of diffusion studies being conducted in countries outside the United States, especially since about 1960. The value of this trend is illustrated in the testing of the cross-cultural applicability of communication principles and generalizations that emerged originally from intra-national research. To achieve maximum utility, researchers need to find hypotheses about the diffusion of innovations that are generally true, regardless of the geographic and cultural locality of the study. This trend toward internationalization of the field of inquiry has been prompted both by the overseas migration of North American researchers, and by a growing number of non-0.8. scientists conducting diffusion re- search. A recent analysis of the publications in the Diffusion Documents Center at Michigan State University revealed that less than 10 percent of the studies listed had been conducted outside the United States in 1960, but by 1965 this percentage had risen to 26.8 percent, or a net increase from 38 to 190 studies conducted in the developing countries over a five-year period (Rogers and Stanfield, 1968, p. 10). A cursory examination of a recent issue of the Bibliography on the Diffusion of Innovations (Rogers, 1967) indicates that well over one-half of the studies listed were authored or co-authored by nationals from developing countries. The study described in the present dissertation represents a further contribution to the growing number of investigations of the diffusion of innovations in less developed countries. Focus of-the Present Study When one considers the various units that may form the bases of analysis in the diffusion of innovations, several possibilities appear. The following simplified typological classification illustrates this point: Independent Variable Individual System Individual A I .B Dependent +__ Variable-, System C J D In many of the diffusion studies conducted to date, much emphasis has been placed upon the individual characteristics of the receiver, and their effect upon his individual decision to adopt or not adopt the innovation (category A in the paradigm). For instance, the relatively earlier adopters in a social system in the United States have generally been found to be younger in age, have higher social status, a more favorable financial position, more specialized operations, and a different type of mental ability than later adopters (Rogers, 1962, pp. 171-178). Socio—economic,demographic, and socio-psychological characteristics of individuals were utilized widely as independent variables in explaining innovativeness in various individuals. However, individuals engaged in the innovation decisionrmaking process clearly do not exist in an isolated state. They are surrounded‘fl by a myriad of influences, ranging from prevailing social system norms _$ and the level of education, wealth, and age of others in their system, to contact with cosmopolite friends, distance from urban centers of population, and the availability of communications media.’ Thus, the present study focuses on the additional amount of variance in innova- tiveness explained by taking into account the "system variables" in the social system under consideration, as well as the "individual variables" (categories A and B in the paradigm). System variables are defined as those structural, compositional, or social-psychological characteristics whose distribution in the system influences the behavior of individuals in that system.1 The central problem considered in the present study is represented by the following questions: (1) Do the properties of a social system influence the behavior of its members? If so, what is the nature and the direction of such influence? (2) How much additional variance in the behavior of the indivi- duals in a social system can be explained by the simultaneous considera- tion of both individual and system variables? 1A more detailed definition is given in Chapter II. The Role of Communication Members of a number of research traditions have been interested in answers to these two questions, especially sociologists. However, the present study is designed to focus on the communication aspects of the problem, from three viewpoints: (l) the introduction of innovations to villagers in a less developed country is essentially a communication process. When viewed from the standpoint of the source or instigator of the change, the introduction of innovations may be classified as either immanent change, which occurs when members of a social system with little or no external influence, create and develop a new idea, or contact change, which occurs when the source of the new ideas is outside the social system (Rogers with Shoemaker, forthcoming, p. 17). Contact change is a change phenom- enon occurring between systems. The present study is an example of centact change, inasmuch as the agricultural innovations included in the study were introduced into each local social system by an outside source (agents from the Eastern Nigerian Ministry of Agriculture), through their communication with the villagers in the social systems. Obviously, other types of consideration are inevitably involved in the process of diffusion of innovations (such as economic, political, cultural, etc.), but communication from the change agency to receivers in the receiving social system is basic to the process of contact change. (2) Once introduced into a social system, the innovation is diffused within the social system by a process of communication. Both interpersonal and mass media channels are utilized in the within-system diffusion process. If communication is viewed as the process by which messages are transferred from a source to a receiver, it may be illus- trated by the SMCR model of communication (Berlo, 1960, p. 72), in which a source (8) sends a message (M) via certain’channels (C) to the receivers (R). Communication factors are vitally involved in the various aspects of the diffusion process within the social system, and are an important element pervading the social change process. A number of social, psychological, and cultural attributes of individuals are relevant as antecedent correlates of diffusion and a number of social, psychological, and cultural attributes of individuals will be changed as a result of the diffusion process, but the central area of concern integral to the entire realm of activities is communication, from one member of the receiving social system to another member of the receiving social system. The diffusion process is conceptualized, therefore, as a multi-faceted communication process involving the spread of new ideas from a source to an audience of receivers via a series of sequential transmissions, as well as by direct contact with the change agency itself. Thus, communication is vital to within-system diffusion of innovations. (3) A number of specific communication variables are considered as independent variables in our analysis of innovativeness within the social system. These include exposure to agricultural communications media, exposure to newspapers, exposure to radio, and exposure to films (see Table 3). In addition, a number of modernization characteristics such as empathy, education, achievement motivation, cosmOpoliteness, etc., which are expected to exert influence upon the receivers' perception and reception of the modernization messages, are included. Value of the Present Study The investigation of innovativeness described in this dissertation may be justified by the following considerations: (1) It is a further contribution to the evaluation and testing of theories of diffusion through studies conducted in crossocultural situations, specifically in less developed countries where studies are few. Many of the generalizations derived from diffusion research have been based on investigations conducted in the United States. The locale of the present study is one of the less developed countries of the world (Nigeria), so that application of existing principles of diffusion in this situation represents an extreme test of their validity and reliability. (2) The present study represents an extension of the analysis of innovativeness to include elements of the village system which affect the rate of adoption, as well as personal characteristics of each individual in the village. The analysis of both levels of variables is widely discussed and utilized in other social sciences, but very few studies use this type of analysis in diffusion research. (3) The inclusion of both system variables and individual variables in the analysis helps prevent the commission of the "ecological fallacy" (Robinson, 1950), the "psychologistic fallacy" and the "sociologistic fallacy" (Riley, 1963, pp. 703-709).1 (A) The present study represents a merging or convergence of the individual level analysis typically employed by psychologists, social psychologists, sociologists, etc., and the aggregative, molar type of analysis typically employed by economists, ecologists, geographers, etc. 1Definitions of these fallacies are discussed in Chapter III. The Setting for the Study The present study is based on an analysis of data representing one portion of a much larger, more comprehensive Diffusion Project conducted by the Department of Communication, Michigan State University. This study was sponsored by the United States Agency for International Development, and was conducted concurrently in three nations in three widely separated and differing areas of the world, Brazil, India and Nigeria. The present analysis derives from the research conducted in Nigeria, where the project was affiliated with the Economic Develop- ment Institute, University of Nigeria, Enugu, and the Nigerian Ministry of Agriculture. The Nigerian diffusion project was designed to investigate the diffusion of health and agricultural information and the acceptance of health and agricultural innovations. Data were gathered on numerous social, psychological, cultural, economic and geographical factors to describe and explain the diffusion and adoption processes. More com- plete information regarding the collection of data, sampling procedures, etc., are given in Chapter IV. The Nigerian study was conducted in three phases. In Phase One, the unit of analysis was the village; the purpose was to analyze villages in terms of their success in programs of planned change. In Phase Two, the unit of analysis was the individual head of a farm household, and the purpose was to determine how the individual's social characteristics, family structure, farm business, communication behavior and various social psychological characteristics were related to his innovativeness, attitude toward change, opinion leadership, etc. 10 The present study utilized data from both Phase One and Phase Two.1 Phase One data were used to derive measures of the social system's characteristics, and form the basis for the determination of which villages to include or exclude in Phase Two; Phase Two data were used to derive correlates of innovativeness for the individuals. Objectives of the Present Study Our objectives for the present study were threefold: (1) To determine whether various attitudinal, cognitive and overt behavioral characteristics found related to innovativeness in other studies were relevant in diffusion research in a less developed nation. (2) To determine whether the distribution of modernizing characteristics within a social system influence individuals toward greater innovativeness, in addition to the individual's own personal characteristics. (3) To derive generalizations and form conclusions which may be helpful in the formulation of a strategy for change by various change agencies. General Outline of Following Chapters A brief summary and introduction to the discussion outline to be followed in the following chapters may be helpful at this point. In Chapter II, a theoretic foundation for the analysis of system effects is presented, including a review of relevant literature, a definitional discussion of system analysis, a description of the meth- odology utilized to determine system effects, some theoretic and 1Phase Three of the Diffusion Project is described in Chapter III. "' . 11 methodological fallacies that must be avoided, and a listing of both independent and dependent variables used in the present study. 'Chapter III discusses the research methodology utilized in the investigation, including a description of how villagers, respon- dents, and interviewers were selected, how data were collected, and how the research instrument was formulated and revised. .A brief description of methods of correlational analysis utilized is included, along with a description of how both dependent and independent variables are operationalized. In Chapter IV, the findings of the investigation are presented, in both verbal and tabular form, and in Chapter V, the findings are summerized, conclusions are stated, and implications for future research are given. CHAPTER II THEORETIC RATIONALE Review of Literature Although scarcely mentioned by diffusion researchers, the concept of "system effects" is not new. Durkheim (IDS?) showed not only that suicide rates vary considerably among different religions, but also that suicide rates for members of a given religion are much lower when these individuals are a minority in the society. He maintained that research methods dealing with social groups rest wholly on the basic principle that social facts must be studied as "things"; that is, as realities external to the individual. Although Selvin (1958) subsequently accused Durkheim of ignoring the individual, treating the group as an entity, thus com- mitting the error of reification,l Durkheim apparently was one of the first to recognize the influence of system effects in addition to indi- vidual characteristics in personal behavior. Groves and Ogburn (1928) investigated the marriage rates for men and women, and found that they varied inversely with the sex ratios of the communities in which the subjects lived. Paris and Dunham (1939) obtained similar results in studies of rates of psychosis. Stouffer (19u9) investigated attitudes toward promotion among soldiers in a military police unit and an air force unit, and found that the indivi- dual's attitude toward promotion was influenced markedly by the general lReification is defined as the conversion of an abstraction or mental construct into a supposed real thing (Webster's New Collegiate Dictionar , 1958. Springfield, Massachusetts: G. 8 C. Merriam Co.) 12 13 rate of promotion in the unit of which he was a member. Generally, pro- motion was evaluated more highly in units where group promotion rates were low, than in units where group promotion rates were high. Blau (1960) reported that data from an analysis of a public assis- tance agency showed that the prevailing values in a work group had structural (system) effects. In some cases, the group values and the individual‘s orientation had similar, but independent, effects on the con- duct of the individual; in other cases, they had opposite effects; and in still others, the effects of the individual‘s orientation were contingent on the prevalence of this orientation in the group, a pattern which identifies characteristics associated with deviancy. Blau further commented that most social surveys tend to make individuals the focus of the analysis, and ecological studies typically examine social units with- out separating social conditions from the individual‘s own characteristics. Blau thus called for the simultaneous use of indices of social structure fit (system effects) and of individual behavior, such as the present study contemplates. Other sociological studies dealing with system variables include Berelson 3: 2i! (1959),who showed the effects of community composition in terms of party affiliation on voting behavior; Lipset 23 ii“ (1956), who found system effects in their study of a labor union; and Davis g£_§£3 (1961), who encountered system effects in their study of the Great Books reading groups. A number of research investigations examined the influence of system effects in formal organizations. Becker and Stafford (1967) conducted 14 a mail survey of luo savings and loan associations in Illinois, to discover the effect upon organizational efficiency of five independent variables, including the size of the organization (measured in assets), the growth rate of the surrounding community, the adoption of innova- ,K; tions, the size of the administrative component, and the management's leadership style. These five variables explained 40 percent of the Variance in the organizational efficiency of the institutions. Sapolsky (1967) studied nine retail organizations in six depart- ment stores, using personal interviews, and found that three major innovations suggested by store executives were not implemented because of the nature of the stores' organization and reward systems. Similarly, in a study of factors associated with the success or failure of various staff proposals (innovations), Evans and Black (1967) found that the structure of the organizations studied, the attributes of the staff-line relationships that affected the innovation process, and the attributes of the innovative proposal were positively associated with the adoption of innovations. Shepard (1967) classified various organizations as either innova- tion-resisting or innovation-producing, based on an analysis of numerous system variables which influenced the attitude of the members toward innovations. Within the rural sociology diffusion research tradition, a number of studies gave further evidence of the influence of system variables on the adoption of innovations. Marsh and Coleman (1954), for instance, studied 393 farm operators in one Kentucky County, and found that socio- economic characteristics and the neighborhood of residence were both 15 positively related to the adoption of recommended farm practices. Even when socio-economic characteristics were held constant, it was found that the extent to which farm operators adopted farm innovations was, in part, a function of the neighborhood of residence. Flinn (1963) investigated the influence of community norms in ‘fl? predicting agricultural innovativeness. Defining norms as a pattern for behavior in a social system with both objective and subjective qualities which can be inferred from overt actions and verbal responses, he found that community norms relative to innovativeness, as inferred from overt action, account for more variation in farmer innovativeness than any other variable studied. Five variables taken together explained 69.1 percent of the variance in innovativeness, with community norms alone accounting for 20 percent of the variance explained. In a study of 393 farmers in 12 neighborhoods in Kentucky, Young and Coleman (1959) found that farmers in high adoption neighborhoods ascribed scientific farming attitudes to their neighbors, and said they were frequently guided by the influence and opinions of their neighbors. Van den Ban (1960) studied #7 Wisconsin townships to test the hypothesis that the social organization and culture of locality groups influence adoption more than individual socio—economic factors (such as education, farm size, net worth, etc.). After dividing the townships into four groups according to their innovativeness scores, van den Ban found significant differences in innovativeness among the groups, be- cause of differences in social structure. Coughenour'(l96u) analyzed data on the diffusion of five farm innovations in 12 Kentucky localities for speed of diffusion and factors 16 relating to it, and found positive relationships with socio-economic and attitudinal resources of each locality, along with the nature of social relationships with information sources and media contacts. Duncan and Kreitlow (195“) matched and compared 19 pairs of rural neighborhoods on the adoption of 30 school innovations, using an index of 25 farming practices and four elements of organizational participation. The neighborhood was the unit of analysis, and the mean score of 10 respondents in each neighborhood was set as the acceptance value for the entire neighborhood. Heterogeneous neighborhoods were found consistently more favorable toward a majority of the innovations, indicating the influence of both social structure and norms. By including a system variable called "norms on innovativeness," Rogers (1961) was able to improve predictions regarding the innovative- néss of truck farmers, when earlier studies had been characterized by low prediction levels. Qadir's (1966) analysis of "compositional"1 effects and their influence on the adoption of technological innovations by some 600 villagers in 26 Philippine neighborhoods indicated that system variables (e.g., mean media exposure, mean education level, etc.) were effective as predictors of individual innovativeness. He concluded, "The in- vestigation in the rural area of the Philippines confirms that the barrio as a locality group has an effect on the individual‘s behavior toward adoption of technological change." QadirHSusage of the term "compositional effects," is practically synonymous with the usage of the term, "system effects" in this dis- sertation. 17 Regarding this review of literature, several generalizations may be drawn: 1. Although a number of studies mentioned system effects as part of the overall research design, almost none focused on system effects as the central problem of the research. In fact, many of the studies considered system variables only after other variables were unsuccessful in predicting innovativeness; thus, system effects were considered only secondarily and in a residual capacity. 2. Many studies investigated the effect of the personal character- istics of a large number of individuals upon the dependent variables. Typically, little attention was given to the socio-cultural and communi- cation environment surrounding these individuals. These dimensions are essentially system variables. 3. When the influence of the social system was considered in the analysis, often only one system (e.g., one village, one community, etc.) was analyzed for its influence upon the dependent variable. Al- most no earlier studies included a sufficiently large number of com- munities (or other systems) to permit testing of hypotheses about community characteristics and adoption of innovations with any degree of rigor. In the study herein described, we go beyond the analysis of the characteristics of individuals, and include characteristics of the system (i.e., the village) in which the individuals (whose behavior we seek to explain) reside. I." .- ._ l... _ u I 7 ..._4 HT 18 System Analysis Because of its emphasis upon the complex functional inter-relations between members of a group, organization, culture, or social system, system analysis seems to be peculiarly appropriate to the study of the introduction of innovations in less developed countries. What are the distinctive characteristics of system analysis? What is the precise meaning of the various terms and concepts utilized in this type of investigation? Schramm (1965) offers the following definitional discussion of a system: When we refer to a system, we mean a boundary-main- taining set of interdependent particles. By inter- dependence, we mean a relationship [among] parts such that anything happening to one component of a system affects, no matter how slightly, the balance and relationship of the whole system. By boundary- maintaining we mean a state in which the components are so related that it is possible to tell where the system ends and its environment begins. From the standpoint of diffusion research, we can define a develop- ment system as a complex of interrelated parts where innovation-receiving units are linked to each other and to a central innovation-introducing unit or change agency through channels of communication. The linkage of the two subsystems-~the change agency (or the source) and the adopter population (or the receivers)--is a basic requisite for innovation dif- fusion and development. For the purposes of the present study, the major components are: (1) the source or the change agent responsible for in- troduction of (2) innovations (3) in a receiver (or adoEter) pOpulation 19 of individuals, groups, or organizations (u) who are linked to each other and to the sources by channels of communication. An individual variable is defined as any of a group of seledted social, psychological, cultural, or communication characteristics of individuals within the social system, which affect their individual innovativeness in adopting a given innovation. System variables are defined as the mean level of any of a group of selected social, psychological, cultural, or communication characteristics within the social system, which affect the innovative- ness of individuals within the social system. A system effect is defined as any observable or measurable change in the attitude or behavior of the individuals within the social system, that can be attributed to the influence of the system variables. In certain research traditions, analyses are sometimes made which explore the complete network of interrelationships between the social, cultural, psychological, and communication variables in a given social system. However, such an investigation (often called a "social system analysis") lies beyond the realm of the present study, for several reasons. First,many attempts to conduct such investigations have been primarily descriptive in nature. The present investigation purposes to find correlates of innovativeness which can be useful in predicting the adoption or rejection of new technologies. Second, few reliable and appropriate research methods have been devised to conduct such a study. Of course, for many purposes, a full 20 analysis of the entire social system with all the complexities and interrelationships of its components is not necessary. Most studies have been somewhat selective in their fecus. The present study also is designed as only a partial analysis. It is not designed to present a comprehensive description of every facet of the social systems selected for study, but rather to explore certain rather general hypotheses regarding various socio-cultural and communication variables, at both the individual and system level, and their relation to a single dependent variable (innovativeness). This study, therefore, deviates from the "system analysis" often utilized in physics, engineering, or the natural sciences. It is a "system analysis" only to the extent that it utilizes selected system variables to explain variance in the dependent variable. The essential feature of system analysis pertinent to this study is that data for selected characteristics are gathered from various levels in a total system (i.e., from the sub-systems compos- ing the total system), and analysis is made of their function, both independently and jointly, within the system under study. Thus, one is concerned in the system approach not only with the major or primary effects of the independent variables, but with their interaction effects as well. Various terms have been used by different authors in describ- ing the various elements involved in systems analysis. Blau (1957) gave the following definition of structural effects: 21 The general principle is that if ego's X affects not only ego's Y but also alter's Y, a structural effect will be observed, which means that the distribution of X in a group is related to Y, even though the in- dividual's X is held constant. Such a finding indi- cates that the network of relations in the group with respect to X influences Y. It isolates the effects of X on Y that are entirely due to or transmitted by the process of social interaction. Contextual analysis has been described by both Sills (1961) and Riley (1963, pp. 700-703) as an approach by which the effects of the properties of groups or collectives on individuals have been studied. Davis Efnéif (1961, pp. 215-225) use the term compositional effects to describe the analysis of the influence of variables formed from the properties of members of the collectivities. Another term used (Valkonen, 1966) is ecological analysis, which may take either of two forms: (1) taking areal units as wholes in their own right, for some- what global analysis, or (2) taking properties obtained by aggregating properties of smaller units (e.g., of individuals). Selvin and Hagstrom (1963) classify the properties of a system into (1) aggregate properties, based on characteristics of smaller units within the system being described, and (2) integral properties, which are not based on smaller units, but on the system as a whole. Lazarsfeld and Menzel (1961) disagree with these terms, and speak instead of analytic and global properties of a system. To illustrate these concepts, an aggregate or analytic property of a system might be the mean level of exposure to the mass media of communication, whereas an integral or global property might be the presence or absence of a mass media facility such as a radio station, in the system under consideration. 22 Cattell posits three categories of system variables (1951): (1) sygtality‘variables, which describe the behavior of the social system acting as a whole, (2) structure variables, which describe internal characteristics, and interaction among members, and (3) 22227 lation variables, which describe the distribution of status, persona- lity, and attitude-interest variables among the members of the system. Undoubtedly these semantic disagreements have been occasioned by efforts to speak more precisely and accurately about the variables being considered by various researchers. In the present study, our primary concern is not to end this debate but to ascertain whether variables of this general description help explain additional amounts of variance in the innovativeness of the respondents in the study. We employ two broad categories of variables in this respect: (1) 229$? vidual variables, which pertain to individual characteristics of the respondents, and (2) systems variables, described elsewhere as con- textual, compositional, Structural, aggregate, analytic, or population variables. Theoretic Rationale In considering the effects of system variables within the Nigerian villages studied, we ask: How do they influence the innovativeness of the individuals in these villages? In what ways do the system variables affect the individual's decision to adopt or not adopt the innovations? Several possibilities exist. l° There is, first of all, the possibility that the differences between the villages included in the study are random. This would mean 23 that the variance in the system variables from village to village simply is an expression of the normal distribution of such variables, and has no systematic relationship to the dependent variable. Thus, one would expect to find differences regarding the system variables as one moves from village to village, but these differences would not be significantly correlated; i.e., they would not covary concomitantly beyond chance expectations with differences in innovativeness in the various villages. Such a situation would be expressed by the null hypothesis: HO = The various elements of the total system in the villages are not related to the innova- tiveness of the residents of these villages. 2. A second possibility is a situation in which the differences in the adoption of innovations by the villages are associated with variance in a single independent variable among the villages. Take, for instance, a situation where innovativeness varies beyond chance expectations from village to village. If the presence of a single variable (e.g., educational level) also varies from village to village, one might hypothesize that different levels of education among the villages explain the different adoption rates. Such a situation may be expressed by the following mathematical equation: Y;j = a + 1313?]. + bzxij In this expression, Yij is the expected value of the dependent variable of individual i living in the social system j and having the value xij of the independent variable; E5 is the aggregated (e.g., the mean) level of the independent variable in the social system j, and a, bl’ and b2 24 are constants. For example, if Y is the innovativeness of individual farmers in Nigerian villages, the individual level variance of it in the whole population would be explained by the education (for example) of the individuals (assuming b2 # 0), and additionally, by the mean level of education inthe social system (assuming bl # 0). If b1 and b2 have the same sign, both individual level education and mean level of education have an effect in the same direction. If b1 and b2 have different signs, the variables on the two levels have opposite effects. Further, if bl = O and h2 = 0, there is no non-random effect at all; if bl = 0 while b2 # 0, there is only an individual level effect; if bl # O and b2 = 0, there is only a systems effect. In the last case, education would affect the behavior of individuals only through the mean level of education in the given social system. In most cases, however, because of the complexity of the total situation confronted by the villages, it is probably not realistic to assume that the variance in the dependent variable (such as innovative- ness) can be explained by one independent variable, such as level of education. We therefore must look further. I 3. A third possibility, which occupies the position of central concern in the present investigation, is one in which a number of in- dividual variables and a number of systems variables operate to explain the variance in the dependent variable. Individual A, living in social system Z, would have some score on the dependent variable Y, produced both by his individual score for independent variable X,and by its interaction with the mean level for independent variable X, 25 throughout his social system. Similarly, individual A would have indi- vidual scores for every independent variable considered (x1, x2, x3...xn), and the social system would have a mean level for every independent variable considered (E1, :5, I,...§£). Thus, the individual variables and the system variables (aggregated individual variables) act independently and jointly to influence the dependent variable for individual A in the social system. The entire system's position on the dependent variable is represented by some measure of central tendency (SUCh as the mean) for every individual included in the sample. In summary, the present study is designed to examine the effect on the individual's innovativeness of individual independent variables plus system independent variables. We hypothesize that the two levels of the independent variables (i.e., individual and system) operate linearly and interactively to explain the variance of the dependent variable within the individuals studied. We propose to examine whether the level of the independent variables within the system operates as an intervening variable, to augment the effect of the level of the independent variable within the individual. Figures 1a and 2a illustrate this point. Many studies have been designed to explicate direct relation- ships Qa in the paradigm, the effect of individual characteristics ”g¥f upon innovativeness). We propose to investigate whether these indivi— dual characteristics are mediated in some way (i.e., either increased or decreased impact) by the characteristics of the system in which the individuals are found. We therefore are proposing to compare 1a, the 26 Individual Characteristics Individual Innovativeness of Nigerian Farmers }.of Nigerian Farmers (Independent Variables) (Dependent Variable) Figure la: Conventional Direct Effects Model of Individual Innovativeness. Individual Characteristics Individual Innovativeness of Nigerian Farmers ) of Nigerian Farmers (Independe t Variables) (Dependent Variable) ystem Characteristics of Nigerian Villages (Independent Variables) Figure lb: Hypothesized Direct—Plus-Mediational Model of Individual Innovativeness Figures la and lb: Comparison of Direct Effects Model and Direct-Plus-Mediational Effects Models of Individual Innovativeness 27 direct effects, with B, the individual or direct effects as mediated by the system effects, and are hypothesizing thatJUDwill be greater. The guiding hypotheses on which this analysis is predicated may be stated formally in the following manner: H1 The individual level of innovativeness for Nigerian villagers is positively related to the individual level of selected moderrization characteristics. H The individual level of innovativeness for Nigerian villagers is positively related to the system level of selected modernization characteristics within each village. H3 The individual level of innovativeness for Nigerian villagers is positively related to the joint in- fluence of the individual level and the system level of selected modernization characteristics within each village. Naturally, if H3 is supported, H1 and H2 must also be supported. How to Determine System Effects As indicated earlier, Durkheim (1938, 1951) was one of the first social scientists to give attention to the study of systems effects. In calling for the recognition of influences external to the individual, ‘fifi Durkheim (1951, pp. 37-38) observed: But if no reality exists outside of individual con- sciousness, it [i.e., sociological method] wholly lacks any material of its own...0n the pretext of giving the science a more solid foundation by estab- lishing it upon the psychological constitution of the individual, it is thus robbed of the only object proper to it. It is not realized that there can be no sociology unless societies exist, and that soci- eties cannot exist if there are only individuals. 28 Thereupon Durkheim described a method for isolating these social influences (roughly comparable to system effects) by finding the relation- ship between the distribution of a given characteristic in-a social system and an effect criterion, while holding this characteristic con- stant for individuals. Since individual differences have been thus controlled, any significant covariation between the variables and the effect criterion is due to the characteristics of the system, or system effects. Blau (1960) also argued for the simultaneous consideration of individual and system characteristics, so as to isolate the effect of the system's characteristics. He pointed out: ...by treating individuals or even subgroups as independent units of analysis that can be classi- fied and re-classified according to any one of their characteristics, this procedure (i.e., the observation of regularities among individual members or subgroups) necessarily ignores the unique constellation of relationships between groups and individuals in the organization-—its Gestalt. Blau therefore proposed a type of analysis similar to that described by Durkheim. In speaking of the influence of values, he stated: "To isolate the external constraints of social values from the influence of the individual's internalized values, it must be demonstrated that the 1T} prevalence of a value in a group is associated with social conduct when this value is held constant for individuals" (Blau, 1960, pp. 178-179). He outlined the following 3-step strategy: (1) An empirical measure Z is obtained on some characteristic of individual members of a system. 29 (2) These individual scores are aggregated into one index for each system, ng. (3) Determine the relationship between the system attribute (ng) and some dependent variable (W), while holding the individual variable (Z) constant. Thus, the effect of ng on W will be system effect.l Tannenbaum and Bachman (196%) criticize this strategy of Blau’s on the grounds that Blau dichotomizes continuous variables, both for individual (rows) and system (column) characteristics. Two serious weak- nesses are pointed out: (1) the problem of contaminating individual differences with system effects, and (2) the problem of contaminating system effects with individual effects. Tannenbaum and Bachman say these weaknesses may be remedied in several ways. One method is to utilize more precise matching of the individual variable (Z). The larger the number of categories of the individual variable used in matching, the greater the accuracy. Instead of a 2 X 2 table, this would produce a N X 2 table, N being the number of categories used in matching on the individual variables. This method could also be utilized to expand the range of group variables (ng). Another method described by Tannenbaum and Bachman utilizes cor- relational analysis. By the use of partial correlation, a system effect can be isolated by correlating between ng and W with Z partialled out. An individual level effect can be determined by correlating Z and W, with ng partialled out. 1 ._ Blau's ng corresponds to blxj, z to b2xi., and W to Yi' as used in the present dissertation. ] 3 30 'If linearity between the variables can be assumed, the individual and group variables may be used as independent variables for multiple regression analysis. In the present study, we utilize correlational analysis, includ- ing Pearson product-moment zero-order correlation, partial correlation, and multiple correlation techniques. For further details of the plan of analysis, see Chapter III. Some Possible Fallacies In analyzing the data gathered in a study of individual and system variables, and in subsequent generalizations from the analysis, several fallacies are possible. Riley (1963) describes two general sets of fallacies that may be committed: (l):fallacies arising because analytical methods fail to fit the model, and (2) fallacies arising because methods fail to fit the facts. If the analyst's model refers to individuals in roles, but the analysis is based on systems (large or small collectivities or aggregates), there is a possible aggregative fallacy. On the other hand, if his model refers to the system, but his analysis is based on individuals, there is a possible atomistic fallayy. Perhaps the best known error commonly committed in the form of an "aggregative fallacy" was pointed out by Robinson (1950): the ecological fallacy. This is the error of assuming that relationships found between characteristics of groups of people, taken as a group, hold true for the individuals within those groups. In Robinson's example, census data for groups of individuals indicated a negative 31 correlation between foreign birth and illiteracy. However, it cannot be assumed, on the basis of this data, that a foreign-born individual will necessarily be literate. Thus, a group analysis is inappropriate if the hypothesis refers to the individual. Conversely, a conclusion about groups of individuals in census areas based only upon individual data is subject to a possible atomistic fallacy. If the hypothesis refers to the group, then the analysis must be based on groups. The second set of fallacies described by Riley, in which the method fails to discover the relevant facts, also subsumes two easily committed errors. A psychologistic fallacy is committed if the researcher. when explaining individual behavior, looks only at the characteristics of each individual, disregarding or overlooking the significance of - n ML! 5 factors such as the character of the village or community. Rogers (forthcoming) remarks that, in much previous diffusion research, "Because the data were gathered from individuals as the units of response, our focus has been upon individuals' intra-personal variables, largely to the exclusion of social structural and interpersonal variables." By including an analysis of the characteristics of the social system, in addition to individual intra-personal variables, the \f present study avoids this error. The second error, the sociologistic fallacy, is committed when the individual level processes are disregarded when interpreting rela- tionships between variables describing collectivities. For example, 32 a group analysis might show a positive correlation between transiency rates and suicide rates. If suicide rates are explained in terms of a social process, and individual level variables are omitted, a sociologistic fallacy may have been committed.1 Selvin (1958) reanalyzed the methodological reasoning in Durkheim's Suicide (1951) and concluded that Durkheim paid too little attention to the individual. He accused the early sociologist of treating the group as an entity, and committing the error of reification.2 Kendall and Lazarsfeld (1950) contend that the principle of ana- lyzing group composition can lead to positive gains if the data allow for simultaneous group and individual level analysis. There is no reason why unit data cannot be used to characterize individuals in the unit. A man who does not have malaria in a unit where the incidence of malaria is very low probably feels differently about his state of health than does the man who has no malaria but serves in a unit with high incidence.... In terms of actual analysis, the matter can be restated in the following terms: Just as we can classify people by demographic variables or by their atti- tudes, we can also classify them by the kind of en- vironment in which they live. The appropriate vari- ables for such a classification are likely to be unit data. A survey analysis would then cover both per- sonal and unit data simultaneously. In the investigation described herein, we have attempted to avoid the psychologistic and sociologistic fallacies by including both system lRiley's psychologistic and sociologistic fallacies are similar td Scheuch's (1965) description of the individualistic fallacy, which is the negation of the usefulness of an explanation that treats the collectivity as collectivity, and the group fallacy, which is the con- verse of the above. Reification is defined in a footnote on page .12. 33 and individual variables in our analysis, and avoid the aggregative and atomistic fallacies by basing the\analysis on the appropriate level (i.e., system or individual). The partial correlation technique also aids in preventing confounding system variables with individual verifibles, and ‘—a—- vice versa. Dependent Variable The dependent variable utilized throughout thelresent study is innovativeness, defined as "the degree to which an individual is relatively earlier in adopting new ideas than other members of his social system" (Rogers, 1962, p. 20). Over the ten years preceding this study, the Eastern Nigerian Ministry of Agriculture introduced and actively promoted a number of agricultural innovations, some 1n of which were chosen for inclusion in the second phase of the Nigerian study (see Table 1). Although these 14 innovations vary widely in type, in amount of skill required for successful cultivation, in adaptability to various climatic and soil con- ditions, initial investment required, etc., they are available and economically feasible for a large number of Eastern Nigerian farmers. Many of these innovations are utilized simultaneously, and others are complementary. It is therefore held to be theoretically logical to use adoption or non-adoption of these 1n innovations as a criterion of inno- vativeness.l 1See Appendix Bfor analyses using two additional indices of innovativeness. 34 Description of Innovations Aldrin dust is an insecticide manufactured by the Shell Oil Company. Although it kills all kinds of insects, it is especially used against the yam beetles-~a pest that flies in from its breeding grounds near a river at or near planting time, sustains itself tem- porarily on the yam seed tuber, and later attacks young tubers. Ap- plying Aldrin dust to the seed tuber before planting kills the yam beetle. The insecticide is distributed by extension agents. GCH-7 Cassava is a new cassava variety that was introduced be- cause of its high-yielding characteristics. From the standpoint of quantities grown and consumed, cassava is the most important food crop in Eastern Nigeria. Stem cuttings, about one foot long, are inter- planted on the sides of the yam heaps. Three innovations—~oil palm rehabilitation, rubberplanting, and cocoa plantingf-are highly similar. These are major tree crops, and are an important part of the six-year development plan of the Eastern Region. In order to adopt one of these innovations, a farmer is required to furnish a relatively large amount of land, a minimum of five acres. He signs a formal contract with the Ministry of Agriculture, which obligates him to clear, space, mulch, and maintain his land ac- cording to specific procedures. The Ministry supplies seedlings, fertilizer, and instructions without charge for a period of five years. Fertilizer isea chemical compound designed to supplement the natural fertility of the soil. An aggressive promotion program has been conducted to encourage its purchase and use. This program has a 3S specially trained staff, its own sales campaigns, and distributors, and special demonstrations are conducted to show farmers how to apply fertilizers on cassava, yam, and maize. The Fund for Agricultural and Industrial Development (FAID) is an organization established to encourage the use of government agri- cultural credit. At the time of the study, however, the program was suffering from administrative difficulties, and loans were difficult to obtain. Community plantations are organized by representatives of the Ministry of Rural Development to encourage cooperative use of village land for agricultural development. Generally, the plantation is de- voted to one of the major tree crOps being promoted by the Ministry of Agriculture. The rural development officers organize village cOOpera- tives, formally register the association with the government, survey and measure land, instruct villagers as to the availability and requirements of Ministry of Agriculture programs and other development projects, and assist them in getting SUpplies and services from the Ministry of Agriculture. Improved livestock is an innovation designed to promote the breeding and raising of higher quality farm animals. Generally, the adoption of this innovation required economic means for purchasing the necessary foodstuffs, veterinary services, and pens for the animals. Only a few farmers have adopted the program, and most of the beef and pork consumed was imported from other parts of the country. 36 NS-l Maize is a hybrid corn variety that has high-yielding qualities. The new variety was developed Specifically to supply food fer the poultry program, but many people use it for food also. The Ministry of Agriculture supplies the seed without charge and it sup- posed to purchase the farmer's harvest later. However, the latter feature of the program was suffering from administrative difficulties at the time of the study. Improved poultry breeding consisted mainly of teaching farmers proper management, supplying them with chicks and foodstuffs, providing them with disease preventives, and making markets available for the eggs. Problems such as high cost of feed, high incidence of disease and death among chicks, and low price of eggs were besetting the pro- gram at the time of the study. Improved rice is an innovation that requires a whole complex of new technologies, and is characterized by considerable expense. Rice improvement programs include improved seed, new cultural practices, seed and nursery preparation, weeding, use to fertilizers, and of dams, irrigation, and drainage. Because of its complexity and expense rice improvement is sometimes incorporated into village plantations. The Stork oil press is a hydraulic hand press for obtaining oil from palm nuts. Although it is a considerable improvement over the conventional screw presses and other traditional methods, the new press costs a great deal of money (about $612.00) in addition to relatively high labor costs. Detailed, technical operating instructions are also required. These disadvantages probably prevented wide scale purchase and utilization of the innovation. 37 Eggetable seeds are sold in small packets at minimal cost, and include onions, tomatoes, and other European vegetables, as well as local varieties. At the time of the study, vegetable planting was receiving increasing attention, with some 80,000 packets being sold in 1966. From the standpoint of their use and characteristics, these lu innovations can be classified according to the following pattern: 1. Food Crops-—Cassava, maize, rice, vdgetables, and cocoa. 2. Land=Commitmen€L InnovationsL-Oil palm, cocoa, rubber, community plantations, and cassava. 3. Divisible InnovationsL-Aldrin dust, fertilizers, NS-l maize, and citrus crops. 4. Elite innovationss-PAID agricultural credit, livestock, rice planting, and the Stork oil press. Independent Variables Three criteria were utilized in the determination of the variables to be included in the present analysis: lLand commitment: innovations are innovations which require the setting aside of a considerable portion of land in order to adopt. 2Divisible innovations are innovations which may be tried on a small scale prior to complete adoption. - 3Elite innovations are innovations which, because of high capital requirements, and agricultural skill demanded, can be adopted 6nly by farmers in an advantageous economic situation. 38 Table 1: List of Innovations Studied in Phases I and II in Eastern Nigeria Name of Innovation Percent of Farmers Adopting:___ Phase I Phase II Fertilizer 22 39 Oil Palm (rehabilitation scheme) 20 17 NS-l Maize (hybrid variety) 16 32 Aldrin dust (insecticide) / 12 33 Poultry (improved breeds) 6 19 Rice production 5 8 Rubber production 5 u Vegetable production 5 7 Cocoa production a 5 Community plantations u 13 Cassava (improved variety) 2 7 Livestock l NSc Cashew l 3 Pineapple -b NS Citrus crops - 10 PAID - 2 Farm settlements - NS Stork oil press - NS Adopted no innovations 53 37 aPercentages are based on 947 respondents in Phase I and 1,3u7 respondents in Phase II. bDashes indicate that the percentage adOpting is less than one percent. CNS means this innovation was not studied in Phase II. 39 (1) Previous research, both in the United States and in cross- cultural settings, with special attention to research in less-developed nations. (2) Earlier analyses of similar data from the Nigerian Diffusion Project by coworkers on the staff. (3) Intuitive and theoretical reasoning. Very little research has been conducted earlier in diffusion studies which deal with the influence of system variables on innovativeness, so that background and previous investigations afford limited empirical assistance in the final choice of variables to be included. 0n the basis of the above criteria, the following independent variables were selected and included in the data analysis: (1) Cosmopoliteness. Cosmopoliteness is "the degree to which an individual's orientation is external to a particular social system" (Rogers, 1962, p. 183). It is hypothesized that earlier adopters are more cosmopolite than later adopters. In the United States, Ryan and Gross (1993) found that hybrid corn innovators traveled more often to urban centers such as Des Moines than did average farmers. Menzel and Katz (1955) found that innovative medical doctors made more trips to out-of—town professional meetings than non-innovators. Goldsen and Ralis (1957, pp. 25-28) found that Thailand farm innovators were more likely to visit Bangkok. Out of 73 publications in the Diffusion Documents Center at Michigan State University dealing with the relation- ship between cosmopoliteness and innovativeness, almost 81 percent reported a positive relationship (Rogers and Stanfield, 1966, p. 26). #0 (2) Education. As a social characteristic that can enable potential adopters to perceive the relative advantages of innovations more readily, and can assist in implementing the break with traditionalism, education is expected to be positively related to innovativeness, at both the indi- vidual and system levels. Rahim (1961) found education positively correlated with innovativeness in Pakistan, and Rogers and Stanfield (1966, p. 22) reported that 79.6 percent of 193 publications in the Michigan State University Diffusion Documents Center dealing with educa- tion's relation to innovativeness indicated a positive relationship. (3) Familism. To the extent that an individual farmer's orien— tation toward his own primary family group is superordinate to his orientation toward other groups within the social system, he will be disinterested or resistant to change, especially if change is per- ceived as threatening maintenance of the family. Familism is there- fore expected to be negatively related to innovativeness at both the individual and system level. (9) Educational Aspiration. Educational aspirations are defined as the level of education individuals would desire if they could be young again. Since a high level of educational aspiration is believed to indicate a more modern outlook, this variable is expected to be positively related to innovativeness at both the individual and systems level. (5) Achievement Motivation. A precise and consensually accep- table definition of achievement motivation is yet lacking in diffusion literature. McClelland defined achievement motivation as "the desire .+1 to do well, not so much for the sake of social recognition or prestige, but to attain an inner feeling of personal accomplishment" (1961, p. 76). Neill and-Rogers stated that achievement motivation is "that value, instilled in the individual through the socialization process, in which the individual feels a need or desire to excel in reaching certain goals only for the satisfaCtion of reaching the goals and not for the rewards of the goals or ends involved" (1963, p. 2). Generally, achievement motivation is taken to indicate a desire for excellence. McClelland reported that this variable was positively related to levels of entrepreneurial activity, economic growth, and rate of national economic growth. Neill and Rogers (1963, p. 12) found occupa- tional achievement motivation among Ohio farmers positively related to productive man work units, man days of labor on the farm, size of farm and innovativeness. There is, therefore, considerable theoretical reasoning to indicate that villagers with high levels of achievement motivation are more innovative in adOpting new ideas and practices. (6) Empathy. Empathy was described by Lerner (1958) in various ways: "the capacity for identification with new aspects of [the res- pondent's] environment" (p. 99), "the capacity to see oneself in the other fellow's situation" (p. 50), "the capacity for rearranging the self-system on short notice" (p. 51), "mobile sensibility" (p. 99), and "psychic mobility" (p. 51). Throughout all these descriptive phrases, the central theme is "the individual's ability to put himself in another's role." Although findings regarding the role of empathy in the modernization process have been mixed, it is generally believed E2 that empathy is positively related to innovativeness, at both the indi- vidual and system level. ' (7) Knowledge of Agricultural Change Agents. Contact with change agencies has been found to be positively related to innovativeness in a great many studies reported in diffusion research literature. Rogers and Stanfield (1966, p. 26) report that, out of 136 studies dealing with the relationship between change agency contact and innovativeness, 91.9 percent reported a positive relationship. In Nigeria, agricultural workers from the Ministry of Agriculture were the change agents seeking to intro- duce change into the rural areas. It is hypothesized that acquaintance with these agricultural workers and familiarity with their work is positively related to innovativeness, at both the individual and systems level. (8) Economic Aspiration. As measured in the Nigerian study, economic aspiration is more oriented toward a desire for economic develoP- ment of the entire village, than toward a desire for personal aggrandize- ment. Thus, a high degree of economic aspiration_should indicate a high level of desire for modernization, economic advancement, and improvement of the village conditions. It is therefore hypothesized that economic aspiration is positively related to innovativeness, at both the individual and system level. (9) Literacy. In many research investigations conducted, literacy (along with education) seems to emerge as one of the general correlates of innovativeness. The innovative person is likely to be literate, as well as have more education than others in his social system. The 43 weight of evidence seems to overwhelmingly indicate a positive relationship between these variables and innovativeness. Rogers and Stanfield (1966, p. 22) found that 19 out of 27 publications dealing with the relationship between innovativeness and literacy reported a positive relationship. 1 (10) Newspaper, Radio and Film Exposure. Taken together, these three variables form a mass media exposure index. They are somewhat interrelated, as Table 2 indicates. However, because there are marked differences in the mean exposure to these three communications media, they will be included in this analysis as separate variables. When ranked according to mean amount of exposure to these three media, the order is: (1) radio, (2) newspaper, and (3) film (see Appendix D ). Table 2: Intercorrelations Among Newspaper Exposure, Radio Exposure, and Film Exposure (N = 1,1H2). Variable ‘Variable 2 3 1. Newspaper Exposure .47 .28 2. Radio Exposure -—- .40 3. Film Exposure _-- Exposure to the mass media of communication is probably a crucial factor in promoting innovativeness and economic development in less- developed nations. Lerner (1956), Deutschmann and Fals Borda (1962), an and Frey (1966) all found a strong positive relationship between mass media exposure and innovativeness. Rogers (1966) found that mass media exposure was highly correlated with innovativeness in Bolivia, explain- ing 67 percent of the variance in innovativeness in one city, and 10 percent of the variance in another. Newspaper, film, and radio expo- sure in Nigeria is therefore expected to be positively correlated with innovativeness at both the individual and the systems level. (11) Agricultural Media Exposure. From one viewpoint, this variable overlaps the previous three variables considerably, and is in fact highly correlated with newspaper exposure, radio exposure, and film exposure (r = .33, .43, .42, respectively). Its specific function is to measure the exposure to agricultural messages being disseminated by the mass media. It has the additional function, however, of being an indirect measure of contact with extension agents or agricultural workers. Agricultural media exposure is therefore expected to be positively re- lated to agricultural innovativeness at both the individual and the systems level. (12) Credit Orientation. A favorable attitude toward the use of agricultural credit to finance further investment in agricultural pro— duction is a characteristic generally associated with more innovative farmers. In a traditional or subsistence system, decisions for agri- cultural alternatives are based not on monetary gains but rather on the protection of one's livelihood. A subsistence-level farmer cannot jeopardize his supply of food and his income without some knowledge of us what is involved in such a venture. Thus, innovativeness is expected‘ to be positively related to credit orientation, at,both the individual and systems level. (13) Level of Living. As an indicant of socio-economic status, level of living could be viewed as either a product or an antecedent of innovativeness. Since many of the innovations included in this study require some degree of economic prosperity, it is hypothesized that level of living is positively related with innovativeness. Further, since certain of the 14 innovations are oriented toward community action, level of living at the system level should also be positively related to innovativeness. (l4) Correct Knowledge of Agricultural Innovations. A necessary prerequisite to the successful adoption of agricultural programs, cor- rect knowledge of the innovations is expected to be positively related to innovativeness. As a cognitive correlate of innovativeness, Rogers and Stanfield (1966, p. 24) found that knowledgeability was re- ported to have a positive relationship in 53 out of 66 research studies dealing with the variable. (15) Formal Social Participation. Individuals who more actively participate in the activities of their social system are more likely to be innovative. As one index of social participation, group member- ship is expected to be positively related to innovativeness. Rogers and Stanfield (1966, p.26) found that 123 out of 156 research studies dealing with group participation reported a positive relationship to innovativeness. 46 Table 3 lists the independent variables utilized in this analysis, with the hypothesized relationship to innovativeness. ' Table 3: List of Independent Variables with their Hypothesized Relationship to Innovativeness. Variable‘ Relationshipa Individual System 1. Cosmopoliteness + + 2. Education + + 3. Familism' - - 4. Educational Aspiration + + 5. Achievement Motivation + + 6. Empathy + + 7. Knowledge of Agricultural Workers + + 8. Ebonomic Aspiration + + 9. Literacy + + 10. Newspaper Exposure + + 11. Radio Exposure + + 12. Film Exposure + + 13. Agricultural Media Exposure + + 14. Credit Orientation + + 15. Level of Living + + 16. Correct Knowledge of Innovations + + 17. Formal Social Participation + + aSince the relationship of these variables is expected to be interactive for both the individual level and system level, the same direction of relationship is gypggbggiggdéforoeagh_yariable at both the individual and system level. CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY The Diffusion Project The present study is based on an analysis of a portion of the data gathered in the three-nation Diffusion Project conducted by the Department of Communication, Michigan State University, and sponsored by the United States Agency for International Development. This project was initiated in December, 1964, and terminated in December, 1968. Broadly speaking, the study concentrated on pro- blems associated with the introduction of modern technology among peasants in the nations of Brazil, India, and Nigeria. Only the data gathered from the Nigerian portion of the study were used in the present investigation. Among the objectives of the Diffusion Project was the specific goal of gathering information useful to change agents in their efforts to introduce innovations. This information included the identification of village innovators and opinion leaders, and their distinctive social and economic characteristics, communication behavior, attitudes, and values; the role and influence of various communication channels such as the mass media, opinion leaders, interpersonal communication, and demonstrations; the response to various communication and economic 47 48 incentives which were introduced into the country;.gpd the influence of price incentives, land tenure, credit, marketing practices, the shift from Subsistence to commercial agricultural production, and other economic factors. The Diffusion Project was designed to consist of three data- gathering phases in each of the three nations. The first phase was an analysis of the relative success or failure of various programs of change in agricultural production in some 80 villages in each country. The unit of analysis was the village, and the data were secured from secondary sources and through interviews with village leaders and change agents. The second phase was an analysis of data obtained mainly through structured personal interviews with villagers living in some 20 vil- lages in each of the three countries. The unit of analysis was the farm family, and respondents were male household heads. The purpose was to trace the diffusion of farm innovations within the villages, and especially to study the role of innovators and opinion leaders in the diffusion process. In the third phase, certain communication techniques and incen- tives for the ad0ption of innovations were introduced into each of the nations. The effectiveness of these incentives and techniques were evaluated through observation and follow-up interviews as part of a controlled field experiment to last over several years. However, political disturbances and civil strife in Nigeria forced curtailment of the project in this nation} so that Phase See Preface. 1+9 Three had to be cancelled in Nigeria after its initiation. Thus, only the first two phases of the Nigerian Diffusion Project were utilized in the present study. Conduct of the Study Phase One Selection of Villages: A random sample of 30 countries was selected from among the 76 counties in the Ibo- and Ibibio-speaking areas of the Eastern Region of Nigeria. Within each county, at least two villages were chosen for the study. The villages were chosen in pairs: half of them were "success" villages, and half of them were "failure" villages, in terms of their acceptance of Ministry of Agriculture extension programs. Several criteria were used in selecting the villages. The change agent must have worked in the village for at least nine months. The selection was also based on ratings by the agricultural super- visors and the county supervisors regarding the "most successful" and the "least successful" villages in the counties, as related to innovations sponsored by the Ministry of Agriculture, and on examina- tion of extension records concerning the number of innovations in operation in a given village, the number of farmers participating, and the amount of supplies distributed during the previous years. After some additions and substitutions necessitated by defi- ciency in pairings, the list of villages included 34 "success" vil- lages, and 37 "failure" villages. Of the 71 villages, 52 were Ibo- speaking and 19 were Ibibio-speaking. 50 Two weaknesses in village selection procedures should be mentioned. Extension agents and county supervisors were generally reluctant to designate a village as a "failure" village. Thus the reliability of this designation may be questionable. In addition, the 30 counties chosen in the first stampling step were not ranked on relative success, so that a "success" village in one county may well be a "failure" village in another. Selection of Respondents: Data were obtained in Phase One from village leaders, change agents, and innovators. Leaders were chosen through sociometric nominations. Six typologies of leadership were utilized: (1) civic, (2) religious- traditional, (3) religious-modern, (4) administrative-traditional, (5) administrative-modern, and (6) educational. In addition, two informal leadership typologies were utilized: (1) village affairs opinion leadership, and (2) village farming opinion leadership. Leadership nominations were taken first from the village school head- master and village chiefs, then from those nominated by the headmaster and chiefs, then throughout the village until some ten informants had been interviewed. From 13 to 16 leaders and innovators were selected and interviewed in each village. Instrument Construction: The interview schedules were translated into Ibo and Ibibio, and reviewed and revised by the research staff. These revised schedules were then back-translated into English, to check for ambiguities. A two-week pretest revealed further weaknesses in the instrument, leading to deletion or revision of certain words or questions. 51 a Interviewer Selection and Training: Criteria used in selecting interviewers included education, cultural origin, age, and experience with rural people. All interviewers were school teachers (or the equivalent) holding the Grade II teaching certificate. All interviewers were at least 25 years old, and spoke the dialect of the village where they worked. Interviewer training included one week's orientation at research headquarters, Enugu, two week's pretest in the field, and another week at headquarters in revising procedures and discussing problems that were encountered. Data Collection: Phase One interviewing was conducted between May 15 and August 27, 1966. Altogether, a staff of 11 Nigerian inter- viewers contacted 947 rural people in 71 villages throughout the Eastern Region. Interviewers averaged three completed interviews per day, with each interview consuming an average of one to one and one-half hours. Each interviewer worked in each village for two weeks, and was visited at least once each week by a supervisor who corrected mistakes and re- viewed the interviewer‘s conduct in the village. ~Phase Two Village Selection: From the total of 71 villages studied in Phase One, the 34 "success" villages were chosen as the basis for the sample population in Phase Two. "Failure" villages were dropped from consideration, since it was desired to study change where it had occurred and "failure" villages were characterized by very low levels of adoption. 52 After ranking on access to the outside world and institutional development, 18 villages were chosen as the basis for the Phase Two study, with 9 villages used in the pre4test. Respondent Selection: In Phase Two, the unit of analysis was the individual. The respondents were in each case a male, the head of his household, at least 20 years old, and farming some amount of land. As indicated in Table 4, the general sample was comprised of 1,142 re3pondents, with the modal seleCtion from each village ap- proximating 65, and the unweighted sampling rate approximating 57 percent of the respondents in each village. To insure an adequate number of innovators and early adopters in each village for incorporation into the field experimental activi- ties in Phase Three, some 205 innovators and ex-servicemen were added purposively to the sample. In certain descriptive statistics, these 1,142 general sample respondents and 205 innovators and ex-servicemen are considered together, but for all correlational analyses conducted in the present investigation, the general sample of 1,142 respondents was utilized. The population on which the correlational analyses are based are indicated in each table of findings in Chapter IV. Instrument Construction: The initial interview schedule was constructed, based upon two pilot studies, one in Western Nigeria and one in the Midwest Region. A five-week pretest was conducted in nine villages, and appropriate revisions and deletions were made in the interview schedule. The schedule included structured response items, opennended response items, and interviewer ratings. 53 Interviewer Selection and Training: From among the Phase One interviewers, nine "senior" interviewers were selected fer Phase Two. In addition, nine "junior" interviewers (with less education and age) were selected. Each interviewing team was comprised of one senior and one junior interviewer. A total of seven weeks was spent both in the field and at headquarters, learning interviewing techniques, rapport-building,field behavior, etc. Data Collection: Phase Two interviewing was conducted between November 2, 1966 and February 15, 1967. Altogether, a staff of 18 Nigerian interviewers talked with 1,347 farmers in 18 villages in the Eastern Region. Each team of two interviewers worked in each village for 6 to 7 weeks, and was visited at least once per week by a super- visor. In addition to personal interviews, interviewers made personal observations of the village and respondents' property. Each interview averaged one and one-half hours in length, and two or three interviews were completed each day. Characteristics of the Sample All respondents in the study were males. Nearly four-fifths (78 percent) said that farming was their major occupation; i.e., the job at which they spent the most time and/or earned the most money. Of the remaining 22 percent of the sample, all but three respondents said farming was a secondary occupation. Slightly over half (52 percent) of the respondents were between 30 and 49 years of age; well over half (56 percent) had never attended 54 Table 4: Village Populations, Sample Sizes, and Sampling Rates for Nigeria, Phase Two. General Innovators, Total Village Sample or Bx-Service- Total Eligible Sampling Census men Sample Resp's. Rate Omasi-Agu 65 14 79 162 49% Obolo-Eke 61 l 62 62 100 Eka Uruk Eshiet 65 16 81 287 28 Nung Ikot-Ikot 63 16 79 250 32 Udo Offiong Utit Uruan 52 l 53 61 85 Nsukara 66 10 76 152 50 Itu Ezinihite/ 66 16 82 156 53 Amaova Oduma/Amankanu 65 16 81 300 27 Umuduru 80 O 80 83 96 Umuoke/Uga 65 9 74 116 64 Okwudo/Umuoseke 64 16 80 147 54 Ulli/Umuaku 64 16 80 ---a ---a Obigbo 67 15 82 250 33 Umubiakwe 69 12 81 209 39 Uwana 64 16 80 197 41 Owutu 65 17 82 149 55 Ihiala/Umuezeawala 66 14 80 162 49 Obgoji 36 o 36 36 100 Totals 1142 205 13a? b b aThe population totals for this village were not available, so no sampling rate can be computed. bSince only incomplete data are available, accurate totals cannot be computed. However, of the villages where data are given, the mean population is approximately 164 people, and the unweighted sampling rate is approximately 55 school, and another third had not completed their primary education. Thus, the typical respondent in the study was a male farmer, the head of his household, who was between 30 and 49 years of age, and who had little or no formal education (Table 5). Knowledge of Agricultural Innovations Respondents were read a list of the 14 agricultural innovations studied in Phase Two of the project, and asked what information they had concerning each innovation. The interviewers then scored respondents as to whether the information was correct. All interviewers were care— fully trained and tested in the technical aspects of each innovation, and specific points of information about each innovation were printed on the interview schedules to assist the interviewer in his rating. Only 3 percent of the respondents had no correct information about any of the 14 innovations. At the other extreme, 9 percent of the res— pondents had correct information about 11 to 14 of the innovations. Over one-third (35 percent) knew something about four innovations, and another third (33 percent) knew about seven innovations. Table 6 shows the number of innovations about which respondents knew at least one correct item of information. From 70 to 80 percent of the respondents had correct information about the four best—known agricultural innovations: poultry (79 percent), fertilizer (79 percent), oil palm rehabilitation (74 percent), and NS—l maize (70 percent). However, between these four best-known innovations and the fifth best-known innovation, a sharp decrease in information appeared. Only 40 percent of the respondents had correct information 56 Table 5: Personal Characteristics of the Respondents (N = 1,347). Characteristics Percentagesa Occupation: Farmer 78 Non-farmer 22 Age Group: 20 to 29 years 15 30 to 39 years 24 40 to 49 years 28 50 to 59 years 18 60 to 69 years 10 70 and older 5 Formal Education: Never attended school 56 Primary, incomplete 33 Primary, complete 8 Secondary, incomplete 1 Secondary, complete 2 University, incomplete 0 University, complete -- aIn order to include every respondent interviewed in Phase Two, these percentages are based on N = 1,347, which includes 205 innovators and ex-servicemen purposively selected for the sample. bLess than 1 percent. 57 Table 6: Number of Innovations Respondents Knew Correctly. Innovations Percentagea None 3 One 7 Two 6 Three 10 Four ' 12 Five 12 Six 11 Seven 10 Eight 7 Nine 6 Ten 7 Eleven to fourteen 9 100 aThese percentages are based on an N of 1,347 (see footnote, Table 5). 58 Table 7: Respondents' Correct Information About Agricultural Programs. Innovations Known Correctly Percentagea Poultry 79 Fertilizer 79 Oil Palm Rehabilitation 74 NS-l Maize 7O Citrus 4O Aldrin Dust 38 i Improved Cassava 35 Rubber Planting Scheme 33 Community Plantations 33 Cocoa Planting Scheme 32 Rice DevelOpment Scheme 25 Vegetable Seeds 1? FAID Credit Program 12 Cashew ll aPercentages, which are based on N = 1,347, sum to more than 100 percent because many respondents knew about more than one innova- tion (see footnote, Table 5). 59 regarding the citrus innovation. At the lowest level of correct infor- w mation, 12 percent of the respondents had correct information about the PAID credit program, and 11 percent knew about the cashew innovation. Adoption of Agricultural Innovations Respondents were asked if they had ever (planted, joined, used, or bought) each of the 14 innovations studied. Somewhat less than two- fifths (37 percent) of the respondents had never used any of the inno- vations, less than one-fifth (19 percent) had tried one, and smaller percentages had at some time used two (14 percent) or three (10 percent) of the innovations. Only 1 percent had used nine or more of the innova~ tions. Eighty percent had used three or less (including none) of the innovations (see Table 8). Considerable differences were found in the percentage of respon- dents adopting each of the innovations. As indicated in Table 9, almost two-fifths of the respondents had adopted fertilizer (39 percent), almost a third had adopted NS—l maize (32 percent), almost a fourth had adopted Aldrin dust (23 percent), and almost a fifth had adopted poultry (19 percent). Below these first four innovations, the percentage of adoption declined rapidly. Less than 10 percent of the respondents had adopted either rice (8 percent), improved cassava (7 percent), vegetable seeds (7 percent), cocoa (5 percent), rubber (4 percent), or cashew (3 percent). Only two percent of the respondents had adopted the use of FAID credit. Thus, the adoption rate was quite low for most of the innovations, with certain innovations being adopted by hardly anyone. In general, 60 the highly divisible innovationsl (fertilizer, NS-l maize, Aldrin dust, and citrus) had the highest rate of adoption, with these innovations requiring a substantial commitment of land (such as cocoa, cassava, oil palm, rubber, community plantations) being adopted by fewer res— , pondents. Almost two-fifths (37 percent) of all respondents had adopted no innovations at all (see Table 8). As indicated in Table 1, similar percentages of respondents adopting the innovations were found in both Phase One and Phase Two of the study, with one or two notable ex— ceptions. Table 8: Percentage of Respondents Adopting Agricultural Innovations. a Number of Innovations Adopted Percentage of Respondents 0 37 l 19 2 l4 3 10 4 7 5 6 7 8 9 l-‘MMQU’I or more 100 aPercentages based on N = 1,347 (see footnote, Table 5). 1See Chapter II for the definition of divisible innovations. 61 Table 9: Percentage of Respondents Adopting Each of the 14 Agricultural Innovations (Phase Two). Innovation Percentage Adoptinga Fertilizer 39 NS-l Maize 32 Aldrin Dust 23 Poultry 19 Oil Palm Rehabilitation 17 Community Plantations l3 Citrus 10 Rice 8 Improved Cassava 7 Vegetable Seeds 7 Cocoa Planting Scheme 5 Rubber Planting Scheme 4 Cashew 3 FAID Credit Program 2 aPercentages, which are based on N = 1,347, sum to more than 100 percent, since many respondents adopted more than one innovation (see footnote, Table 5)° 62 Analysis of Data Three statistical methods were utilized in the analysis of data in this study: (1) Zero-order correlation. (2) Higher-order partial correlation. (3) Multiple correlation. First, zero-order correlations were computed between‘innovative- ness and all other independent variables (both individual and system). Then, higher-order partial correlations between innovativeness and each of the independent variables were computed, holding constant all other independent variables. To specifically determine the contribution of the system variables, partial correlations were computed for the system level independent variables, with the individual level effects held constant. After submitting the system variables outlined previously to both zero-order and higher-order partial correlational analysis, we selected the best predictors of innovativeness for inclusion in a multiple cor- relation analysis. Similarly, the best predictors from the zero-order and partial correlation analysis of the individual level variables were included in this further analysis. Thus, the best predictors from the system variables analysis, and the best predictors from the individual level analysis, were submitted to multiple correlation analysis, using the multiple regression least squares delete computer program, to for— mulate a paradigm of variables that predict innovativeness and possess significant explanatory power. 63 Operationalization of Variables Dependent Variables In recent years, attempts have been made to devise indices of innovativeness which will accurately measure respondents' behavior concerning the adoption of recommended innovations.l In operationalizing the index of innovativeness utilized in the present study, a measure was chosen which indicated the total number of innovationsnly a small proportion of the sample population, in each analysis. Formula for correcting an inflated multiple R: An obtained multiple correlation coefficient can be corrected or "shrunken" to give a better measure of the population R by the use of the following formula: 123 124 2- -2M Rc-lk (N—m) where: N = size of the sample m = number of variables in the problem (N - m) = degrees of freedom 2=(l-R2) In the findings of the present investigation, R = .68, in the analysis computed for 23 significant individual and system level variables, N = 1,142, and m = 23 (not including 11 variables not reach- ing the minimum significance criterion of .05); k2 = (l — .682) or .53. Substituting these values into the formula, we have: 3 (1,141) 2 R = 1-.5 c (1,115? which yields: R2 = .46 and R = .678 C C and the correction is negligible. APPENDIX B Alternative Indices of Innovativeness During the course of the present investigation, we were concerned with questions regarding the reliability and validity of the index of innovativeness utilized in this study. These questions focused on the "non-unidimensionality" of the index, as indicated by (l) the seeming competitiveness of various innovations in the index with other innovations, (2) various rationales apparently utilized by Nigerian farmers for adopting or not adopting each of the innovations, and (3) the wide disparity between innovations regarding agricultural skill required, amount of investment required, and relative advantage for the farmer. At least two other indices of innovativeness have recently been utilized in conducting similar analyses of these data: (1) a three-item index, utilizing the most widely adopted innovations in the list: fertilizer, Aldrin dust, and NS—l maize (Keith, 1968); and (2) a six- item index, including cocoa, rice, cassava, vegetable seeds, poultry, and NS-l maize, constructed by factor analysis of all fourteen innova- tions, and selecting the "cleanest" factor which explained the largest amount of variance in innovativeness (Salcedo, 1968). These two additional indices of innovativeness were included in the present analysis, for information purposes, with the results shown in Table 18. 125 126 Table 18: Comparison of Zero-Order Correlations of 34 Individual and System Level Variables with Three Indices of Innovativeness (N = 1,142) II. Variable I. Individual Cosmopoliteness Education Familism Educational Aspiration Empathy Knowledge of Agricultural Workers Economic Aspiration Literacy Newspaper Exposure Radio Exposure Film Exposure Agricultural Media Exposure Credit Orientation Correct Agricultural Knowledge Formal Social Participation Achievement Motivation System Cosmopoliteness Education Familism Educational Aspiration Achievement Motivation Empathy Knowledge of Agricultural Workers Economic Aspiration Literacy Newspaper Exposure Radio Exposure Film Exposure Agricultural Media Exposure Fourteen Item Indexa .53 .35 .03 .03 .14 .09 .06 .16 .11 .11 .13 .11 .08 .17 .22 Six Three Item Item Index Index .24 .25 .32 .33 -.03 -.09 .18 .26 .25 .35 .29 .39 .10 .12 .30 .30 .32 .24 .27 .35 .22 .22 .43 .55 -.02 .04 .42 .57 .23 .29 -.03 .06 -.008 -.004 .04 .07 —.ll -.27 .08 .19 .04 .06 .09 .16 .l4 .16 .12 -.O2 .06 .02 .04 .02 .006 .09 .15 .06 .16 .21 127 Table l8--Continued. _H, Fourteen Six Three Variable' Item Item Item Indexa Index Index Credit Orientation -.05 -.06 -.05 Level of Living ' .13 .02 .05 Correct Agricultural .28 .22 .33 Knowledge ‘ ‘ Formal Social Participation .18 .08 .12 aThe fourteen-item index of innovativeness is the index utilized in the present analysis. Several explanatory comments are in order regarding these alter- native indices of innovativeness: (1) As indicated by Table 18, the fourteen-item index of innova- tiveness and the three-item index of innovativeness yielded very similar zero-order correlations, and the six-item index of innovativeness yielded lower correlation coefficients in almost every instance. (2) The six—item index of innovativeness was constructed from a factor analysis of all 14 innovations, and was attended by some degree of arbitrariness regarding the number of rotations to be utilized, minimum level of loading to be taken into account in deter— mining communalities, and amount of intercorrelation with items in other factors to be tolerated. In view of our desire to utilize as realistic an index of innovativeness as possible, our decision was to utilize the l4—item 128 index of innovativeness, since its wider range of innovations offered several obvious advantages. In our considered opinion, the use of either of the other two indices here described would not have appre- ciably improved the reliability of the findings, nor significantly improved the coefficient of determination. Although less than per- fect, the fourteen-item index is believed to have been the best choice among the three alternatives, from a theoretical, statistical, and practical point of view. Table 19 shows the intercorrelations between the three alter- native indices of innovativeness. Table 19: Intercorrelations Between l4—Item, 6-Item, and 3-Item Indices of Innovativeness (N = 1,142). 6-Item Index 3-Item Index l4-Item Index .78 .69 6-Item Index ——— .48 3-Item Index -—- _-- APPENDIX C Analysis of Curvilinearity As outlined in Chapter II, one of the assumptions on which the present analyses rest is linearity of the data. When the means of the arrays of the successive columns and rows in a correlation table follow straight lines (at least approximately), the regression is said to be linear or straight-line. Regression lines which "best fit" the means of the successive columns and rows in the table can be calculated, and used as the basis for prediction of the dependent variable. When the drift or trend of the means of the arrays (rows or columns) cannot be well described by a straight line, but can be represented by a curve of some kind, the regression is said to be curvilinear, or in general, nonlinear. When the regression is non- linear, a curve joining the means of successive arrays will fit these mean values more exactly than will a straight line. Hence, should a truly curvilinear relationship be described by a straight line, the scatter or spread of the paired values about the regression line will be greater than the scatter about the better-fitting regres- sion curve. The smaller the spread of the paired scores about the regression line or the regression curve, the higher the relationship between the two variables. For this reason, a zero-order product- moment correlation calculated from a correlation table in which the 129 130 relationship is curvilinear will always be less than the true rela- tionship. If the regression is significantly nonlinear, it makes considerable difference whether the correlation ratio (eta) or the product-moment correlation (r) is the measure of relationship. But if the correlation is low and the regression is not significantly curvilinear, theproduct—moment correlation (r) will give as adequate a measure of relationship as the correlation ratio (eta). Examining Table 20, we see that considerable differences exist for every independent variable between the product-moment correlation (r) and the correlation ratio (eta), indicating that the relationship of many of these independent variables to innovativeness is highly curvilinear. Since the relationships of these independent variables to innovativeness is curvilinear, the correlations computed on the assumption that the relationships are linear are not a satisfactory basis for predicting innovativeness. The curvilinear relationships also explain the unexpectedly low correlation coefficients we obtained in our analyses, since the zero-order correlations were based on the assumption of linearity. 131 Table 20: Comparison of Zero-Order Product-Moment Correlation (r) and Coefficient of Non- linear Relationship (n) for 34 Indivi- dual and System Level Independent Variables and Innovativeness (N = 1,142). Zero—Order Correlation,of Product-Moment Nonlinear Correlation Relationship (r) (eta) Variable Individual System Individual System Level Level Level Level Cosmopoliteness .26 .03 .36 .18 Education .35 .14 .46 .26 Familism -006 -e 18 .21 .3” Educational Aspiration .23 .09 .30 .26 Achievement Motivation .03 .06 .21 .24 Empathy .29 .16 .38 .21 Knowledge of Agricul- .34 .11 .43 .28 tural Workers Economic Aspiration .12 .11 .24 .23 Literacy .34 .13 .43 .23 Newspaper Exposure .32 .11 .51 .22 Radio Exposure .31 .08 .41 .21 Film Exposure .24 .17 .38 .26 Agricultural Media .52 .22 .59 .33 Exposure Credit Orientation -.O2 -.05 .22 .19 Level of Living .38 .13 .45 .20 132 Table 20--Continued. Zero-Order Correlation of Product-Moment Nonlinear Correlation Relationship (r) (eta) Variable Individual System Individual System Level Level Level Level Correct Agricultural .53 .28 .63 .42 Knowledge Formal Social .35 .18 .41 .26 Participation APPENDIX D Table 21: Mean Values and Standard Deviations for 17 Individual Level, 17 System Level Variables (N = 1,142).a Variable Cosmopoliteness Education Familism Educational Aspiration Empathy Knowledge of Agricultural Workers Economic Aspiration Literacy Newspaper Exposure Radio Exposure Film Exposure Agricultural Media Exposure Credit Orientation Level of Living Correct Agricultural Knowledge Formal Social Participation Achievement Motivation Individual System Mean Standard Mean Standard Value Deviation Value Deviation 3.07 3.57 3.07 1.19 0.55 0.77 0.55 0.24 0.63 0.96 0.63 0.37 2.33 0.81 2.33 0.24 3.98 2.51 3.98 0.77 0.31 0.46 0.31 0.20 2.43 1.48 2.43 0.21 6.66 13.01 6.66 2.98 1.28 4.46 1.28 0.93 2.66 4.53 2.66 0.92 0.76 1.49 0.76 0.40 3.04 3.36 3.04 1.09 3.20 2.17 3.20 0.67 2.58 2.75 2.58 0.90 11.32 6.39 11.32 3.30 2.65 1.81 2.65 1.05 5.09 1.44 5.09 0.20 aSee Chapter III for operationalizations of these variables. 133 "11111111111111111115