. Eva? . J . ‘ . .-v.xo|...._ul..h..u! . . . fifln...‘ m -. .. :whuflvflul v. I vl 11 D. h dl!‘§< ‘0‘ g. . I Wail #- thnlfihwwwrzdh. I 01 t .. {:hYI‘OI . In . v... a o. .1“. |r§551t€¢233§11 ‘t.’ ‘ uni tD. l7]. . T. .wv-L.‘....l§..Z.¢.vu\? . h :‘l.".- .HII‘HJI, a! t.» nv‘. . q .in v u' 1" 9 I 5.]. .uvll ... . a - . - .~ . 1.: u . a I II I «v I Q I C I o 1:0 I~ A. 1‘ . . ul .0 t a . I 1 .0. A y I u U v a u. I v u '4': I 1: 0w L.\o.\ o ‘ w . N vac I 7..-. l I l in! u'1 I r A v 0.11.“... VI(-‘ V1 I '- o‘llv 5‘1 ua¢I\l-I . 0". . ‘ I. c .u .M . u v .y s .III v. to. . .v1.Jn§.v .Hol lat. .v. V! A t. J~IAI .' 1. ..l . l' 0' ‘ .II I -‘btilOo | ' . . a o o. 3 .6 . It ‘ y... L. »’ . u .. o» nu. u o I 7' n .. h. ~ I... f . u... tun .WD 3 .. u \ . . . t. \ a I \4IAN..VI - b D. c Own. d .I.' Cd‘ .oO' I o 1 . v-v 1 a v 0.1.. .a . J. .0 V.|,l . , . 1 v \V-.. n IL-.. 1. 1.. . 3‘1... . ..I‘ ‘In AO‘L‘ ll 3! . . ..u..Wl..oJr......... . .\ Mo.tvi0.t "an“ I } "(ll .‘ u n I. I - . .. f) 3.11:4. v..1lt.oh1..l|"fouhwlco 7“an ”A" . gimm!‘ Iv..b.ln-nfi¢q.0l||u|0\. vs .0 . .a tau! 11v. $§I.I.Iltn.lt1li v .v. a .V :91- . fixhfiu Ezawuf: . . t. - ‘ l -. . c - urn. (ll 1. vbl‘ “.‘olllf'o I'D}. o on $1 v-v .‘n . 1 03‘1‘ v ‘5 . » lug-I... I n . . V.- ..Mz g f- whamxurzunuy.-..ul ...... 4.- z: I. .I PI. . ll N o . u 0‘ . . I. .41 .I. 1“! O? .11 a .0. '“c. I ‘bl. 55.1‘ ' . ca. qtixl‘l-‘l.uhnufl.h.rir.ls $.38 -. if ll}... .04 . u... ..n .01.}. fibgy. . . .l :Vnummmil .... -. lu..........:fi. virrJar. b... - “NWT...- \ 'tn' Hat‘s... I.VV .‘I‘fi.u .c IbI-l 1‘ 0%.... b 1.0:..."- “1%! ‘uuwu‘luhi: Yb- w“. l . v'un ‘06..qu . 19!...»- A‘.ol\'z\l. nl'I‘roA I «II-v... l I . - .... aunt.)...h.u...li¢.lhl.ht..vt¥xt1s!i .inl . ... n u .l- ‘ t‘ubth‘OnCuY‘fllt.‘0II¢.il0§-Su a .l I. ‘II ll.‘. Ian-div...“ 'vifld ‘Q'ID U U. u I '“‘\.R v a x I)... t‘l‘ ‘- 0. O'vlli“-..'~| IV?! ‘1' I ‘. . 111...? _ fivtnflufl'um01‘15H‘I‘1 .Ns ... l. ... 1b.?) .111: a -.\.}|l|to.t I 9 Q sol b .IOQ‘A lv‘llw. lw.-‘..nl .. . .1; - “8% nan.-. “ - ’ o . .- § .\ ‘ ) It . SIN: ‘o‘llllnallvllll‘ I. o. Q It. 1. . a .LllbbL Onnlpnvulmt. I I ‘n‘. I‘lvn I.'“..I).- .3.“ 0 AA u .u- . '' .Inv s CPIllvlll‘ olc .o ‘u. tuft: .‘O- I tn 0 2o "thifl~ 1!" , A V I000.’l"10\.h.h0.d.7|..1|t9vl‘ll‘t1l"‘ ‘ .. . “lag. I. . IIh l.‘ ‘. .d‘lll....l.\|lb\luu_. I a . 1. I! «I a 1.0mm . . .110.‘ OI‘IO“! .lil. “‘0 U‘lll.‘o‘.ll-.1. q. I. I c nl| . 0 I t r p u 0 I. f. .1; n . .2. - ..c I- .Mw... - a I II 0' *9 '16! 4 .00 I . . - . a. 1... . xvi. Ln. K?l.uduktflfiicltrumn ... fill! . : '{Il . “J. 1....H......namm.\.mhflrtnfc.. enunmm Inloh‘tulW - "I‘bIbI-Icflhtmfllbflflnu.fl. in . . itltuuflmq law-Ii ummunr ..a1|\u.-n.:la.vrvll dull. 0.. J '. o.l|v.l.|u.m.. .‘ I“. II c 1‘. l 1: a .I i .P. I H. v \ .10.th O flout act . L ”Lona?!” I (hu%‘ . u .. .nnaarfinb}. firnu. .. .. .l .k 4-. I. {.3. fluidlk.“ . v < . n y'. . I. . Ike-V’s. I .- . ...«. N. - F ’L)» .Fr. l1||ll||lllll||llllllllllllllllllllllllllllzllllllllsllsllllll 3 1293 104762 THESIS J ‘"\ mam” ' M24: “2?“: 2: mate 3 University _______ ._._,. ,_ . ”W“ This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Self Reported Symptomatology in Major Depressive Illness presented by Gregory Alan Holmes has been accepted towards fulfillment ofthe requirements for Ph.D. degree in_C_Q_u_n_5_e]_]'_n_g_,_Ed. Psych. & Special Education [lg/L flCd/w’t 2/26?” 11404 M a ior prole ssor MSU t5 un Affirmamv Action' Equal Opportunity Institution 0.12771 MSU RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to LIBRARIES remove this checkout from _—c_- your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. ‘ ”fifi“fi9‘85&23 .m ‘3‘ "'7; a:. I : ‘fiifir j (/7 ~‘b-Q 5‘ -_ _._ i i.” "'1' 1". . I H ’9 '7' i ‘i-fi’t .1 . " ._.~ i r ' ~- .. . ”Max.- .L. , . . . g'1 5 ‘7. ' 2". . R U 4- ' '0 ‘ . i .‘ ‘. ‘1 i’: A i o q \ /4//- 37/3 SELF REPORTED SYMPTOMATOLOGY IN MAJOR DEPRESSIVE ILLNESS By Gregory Alan Holmes A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Special Education 1983 0.. ABSTRACT SELF REPORTED SYMPTOMATOLOGY IN MAJOR DEPRESSIVE ILLNESS by Gregory Alan Holmes The purpose of the study was to determine whether or not a self rated instrument could differentiate between endogenous and non-endogenous depression. A 163 item test named the Differential Diagnostic Depression Scale (DDDS) was developed to measure 29 symptom categories associated with depression. LNA- The DDDS was administered to a sample of 100 patients with a chief complaint of depression at a consortium of inpatient and outpatient psychiatric centers. The patients were diagnosed as endogenous or non-endogenous depression based on a combination of values from the Dexamethasone Suppression Test (DST) and Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC). logy Two approaches were used to construct scales from the 0005 items. In the a priori approach items were grouped lingo 25 scales based on content similarity. The internal consistency oiithe scales ranged from .57 to .88. A principal component solution was used in the second method of scale constructiontdi; Fifteen factors were extracted with T‘ internal consistency values that ranged from .32 to .92. A. discriminant analysis was used to construct a function that would best separate endogenous and non-endogenous depression for each approachi‘ Thirtynfive~subject5wfrom“eachwgroup“were*randomlymselected for~themanalysisiphaje.fivThevdlicrjfllqant :unction constructed by the a priori approachxécodrectTyl classified 87% 56f theqisgndomlyuflselected geeups. The discriminant function constructed by the empirical approach correctly classified 79% of the randomly selected groups. Cross validation was attempted by using the discriminant functions to classify the 30 remaining individuals in the sample. The classifica- tion accuracy was 57% with the a priori discriminant function, and 43% with the empirical discriminant function. A second discriminant analysis was conducted, using a select number of scales from both the a priori and empirical approach. The second discriminant function based on the a priori approach correctly classified 73% of the original seventy subjects, and 60% of the cross validation groups. The second discriminant function based on 'the empirical approach correctly classified 73% of the original seventy subjects and 57% of the cross validation groups. DEDICATION To Peggy Nay Anderson and in memory of Nash Anderson 11 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS In order to successfully complete a project of this magnitude, the help and support of many individuals and organizations was critical. I would like to thank the following individuals: Dr. Hilliam Farquhar, for his inspiration and guidance throughout the project; Dr. Robert Bielski, for his direction and supervision in the area of affective disorders; Dr. Gerald Osborn, for his perceptive comments on the research project and manuscript; Dr. Andrew Porter, for his statistical consultations throughout the project; Ms. Susan Irwin for her help with data coding, preparation, and statistical analyses. Several other individuals played key roles in the completion of this research. Dr. Christine Shafer was the chief psychiatric resident during the project and made outstanding contributions in many formal and informal areas. Chip Nest, Jay Terbush, and Shelley Smithson functioned as the chief interviewer/raters for the project and it was a delight to work with them. Karen Brainard, Dorothy Stengel, and Randy Scheidt were instru- mental in the day-to-day operation of the study. I would also like to thank Dr. Ashok Kaul and his staff at Ingham Medical, particularly Betsy McPhereson, for their tremendous assistance during the project. Millicent Lane's concise columns in The State Journal were extremely helpful in disseminating information about the study to the Lansing area population. Not to be forgotten are the 100 iii patients who volunteered their time in a very personal way to share aspects of their depression with our research team. Finally, deep appreciation to my fellow members of the Dream Institute for their support during this project. iv CHAPTER 1 THE PROBLEM The classification of major depressive disorders has been one of the most controversial and important areas of psychiatric research over the past six decades. Beginning with the early clinical observations of Freud and Kraepelin, researchers have sought to establish a classification system of depression that would aid clinicians in their understanding of the behavior, etiology, and treatment of the depressed patient. The impetus behind these research efforts is the urgent nature of the problem, which can be drawn from ‘two observations. First, the risk of a given individual developing a major depressive disorder requiring professional intervention is alarmingly high. Recent research suggests that approximately 23% of all females and 10% of all males suffer from major depressive illness requiring professional intervention at least once during their lifetime (Keller, et. al., 1982). Secondly, if not properly' diagnosed and treated, depression is often a debilitating and at times life threatening illness. Several major advances in the neurosciences in recent years have significantly affected the diagnosis and treatment of depression-and serve as a foundation for further research. A recent review of research attempts to establish valid and reliable criteria for a diagnostic. system suggests that depressive disorders can be dicho- tomized into two groups based on the presence or absence of a l constellation of behavioral symptoms, biological markers, and treatment response (Nelson and Charney, 1981). The first group, generally referred to as endogenous depression or major depression with melancholia, is relatively autonomous to environmental change. Furthermore, it is more likely to have neurophysiological correlates, and is more responsive to biological intervention than non-endogenous depression. The second group, non-endogenous depression, is typically defined by the absence of the features mentioned for the endogenous group. Two recent developments are of particular importance in attempts to distinguish endogenous from non-endogenous depression. First has been the discovery that in 50% of endogenous depressives the admini- stration of dexamethasone failed to produce normal serum cortisol suppression. From this finding, the dexamethasone suppression test (DST) was developed. The dexamethasone suppression test has proven to be helpful in corroborating the diagnosis of endogenous depression when it is abnormal (positive), having a specificity of 95% (Carroll, et. al., 1981). The second development was the introduction of Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) for twenty-five major psychiatric disorders, including endogenous depression. Spitzer and his colleagues demonstrated that interviewers using the RDC were able to reliably distinguish endogenous from non-endogenous depression, with coefficients for agreement above .80 for both categories (Spitzer, et. al ., 1978). Need for the Study A need exists for an additional method of differentiating endogenous and non-endogenous depression. The majority of research attempts to classify depressive disorders as either endogenous or non-endogenous used observer ratings of symptoms, rather than patient self ratings. Although self rating scales for depression exist, previous studies have shown that these scales are useful as indexes of severity of illness and do not differentiate between subtypes of depression (Rehm, 1976). A self rated scale that could reliably differentiate subtypes of depression would be of significant value both as a research instrument, and most importantly, as a relatively inexpensive technique for the diagnosis of depression. Both the DST and RDC have limitations which interfere with their utility as diagnostic procedures for the routine clinical evaluation of depression. Although the DST has been found to be a relatively specific laboratory test, 33-50% of endogenous depressives have normal cortisol suppression after dexamethasone administration and are not detected by the test (Carroll, et. al., 1981). Additional drawbacks include the cost of the test and the necessity of having trained personnel to administer and analyze test results. The RDC are designed to be used in conjunction with an in-depth clinical interview which is both expensive and time consuming. Purpose of the Study The purpose of the research reported in the present study was to determine whether a patient self reported instrument could differen- tiate endogenous and non-endogenous depression. Research Hypotheses Gerald Klerman, in his review on research in depression, argues that the concept of endogenous depression embodies four component propositions, or hypotheses, that are testable through empirical investigation (Klerman, 1972). Klerman's four propositions are as follows: 1. Endogenous depression consists of a pattern of behavioral signs or symptoms that occur together at one point in time. 2. The short term episode of endogenous depression, as contrasted to non-endogenous depression, occurs without a recent precipitating event. 3. The premorbid personality pattern of the endogenous depression will be relatively stable and non-neurotic. 4. Endogenous depression will run an autonomous course, i.e., will not be reactive to environmental alteration. In this study an empirical investigation of the first proposition was conducted. It was hypothesized that: 1. A. self report measure designed t0~ assess empirically supported constructs will differentiate between endogenous and non-endogenous depression. - 2. A self report measure will yield interpretable constructs which will contribute to the understanding of the differences between endogenous and non-endogenous depres- sion. Theory Although there has been a wealth of research over the past seventy years on the phenomena of depressive illness, there remains no clear consensus among investigators regarding the exact classification of such disorders (Kendell, 1976; Eysenck, 1970). Several conceptual and methodological problems interfered with attempts at establishing an accurate nosological system. A major problem was that symptoms associated with depression, such as disturbance in mood, anxiety, and decreased concentration occur in a range of psychiatric conditions and are also part of the normal adaptation response (Klennan, 1972). One of the fundamental conceptual issues in depression research is whether depression can be best explained by a unitary or binary model of classification. Proponents of the unitary model of depression argue that all depressive illness has basic underlying similarities, and that all observed clinical differences can be explained by severity of the illness (Klerman, 1972). In the unitary model distinct demarcations of subtypes of depression are seen as impossible. In contrast, the binary, or pluralistic model of depression holds that it is possible to distinguish between at least two subtypes of depression, and that differences cannot be explained by severity alone (Rosenthal and Klerman, 1966). The two most frequent binary models referred to in the literature are the endogenous-reactive dichotomy and the psychotic-neurotic dichotomy. hi the former, depressions can be subdivided based upon their reactivity to environmental change (Gillespie, 1929). The endogenous depression, once established, is thought to run an independent or "autonomous“ course, i.e., be relatively unaffected by external stimuli. As such, these depressions are often thought to be of biological origin. 0n the other hand, reactive depressions are so named because of the relative responsive- ness to external stimuli and are thought to originate as reactions to life stress. In the psychotic-neurotic binary model, emphasis is placed on the presence or absence of psychotic symptoms in making the distinction between depressions (Kendell, 1976). Psychotic depressions are characterized by symptoms such as hallucinations, delusions, and degree of ego regression, whereas neurotic depressions are defined by the absence of these psychotic symptoms. Unfortunately, as MacFadden (1963) noted, the labels and descriptive terms for both dichotomies have been used interchangeably by some researchers, contributing to confusing and conflicting research reports. For example, some researchers equated endogenous with psychotic depression, when in fact the phenomena can occur independent of one (another (Andreasen (and Grove, 1982). Similarly, the terms neurotic and reactive depression have been used interchangeably while research suggests the terms have different implicit and explicit meanings (Akiskal, et. al., 1978). There are two research approaches that have been used to establish the validity of the binary, or pluralistic model of depression. One approach is by demonstrating that subtypes of depression exist, based upon differences in clinical features, or symptomatology. The second approach is to demonstrate that identified subtypes respond differentially to various treatment modalities, such as medication or psychotherapy. In this study, the former approach is used to study the differences in the self report of symptoms, as measured by the 0005. Overview of Remaining Chapters In Chapter Two, the relevant literature is reviewed in the following areas: symptom differences, differential treatment response, and the efficacy of previous patient rated instruments. The research design and procedures are presented in Chapter Three. In Chapter Four, the analyses of the results are presented. Conclusions and recommen- dations for further research are presented in Chapter Five. CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF LITERATURE In the following review, three areas of research are examined. First, a review of attempts at classifying depression based on symptom frequency is presented. Second, there is a brief summary of research on treatment response among subtypes of depression. In the final part of the review, there is a consideration of attempts of using patient rated instruments to measure subtypes of depression. Symptom Frequengnytudies One of the earliest attempts at distinguishing between subtypes of depression was made by Freud (1956) in his monograph "Mourning and Melancholia." Freud felt that mourning was a normal response of an individual to the loss of a loved one, whereas melancholia was a pathological, or abnormal process. He believed that melancholia could be distinguished from the mourning process in that melancholia was associated with strong self reproach, whereas there was no loss of self esteem in mourning. Another early attempt to classify depression was conducted by Kraepelin (1921) who also felt it was possible to distinguish a normal reaction of sadness from clinical depression. Based upon his clinical observations, Kraepelin subdivided clinical depression into two types - psychogenic depression, and manic depressive insanity. The major characteristics of the manic depressive group were that the attacks of illness appeared to have no precipitant, and that the course of illness was “independent," or autonomous, from events that occurred during their hOSpitalization. Psychogenic depressions were believed to be precipitated by external circumstances and influenced by the milieu during the course of treatment. Gillespie (1929) reported his clinical observations of a group of 25 depresed inpatients who were given physical and mental examinations. He found that their depressions could be differentiated according to degree of reactivity to external and internal stimuli. Two major groups were discovered: reactive and autonomous depressions, with the latter having a subgroup which was labeled as "involutional." The reactive depressions were primarily characterized by presence of a precipitant and their responsiveness during the course of treatment in the hospital. Approximately 90% had clearly identifiable events which were thought to precipitate the patients presenting problems. Furthermore, the reactive depression group was judged to be responsive to their physician or other variations in the hospital milieu, such as visits by family members, or staff changes. Gillespie observed that this group was also more responsive to everyday events such as changes in the weather. The autonomous group was characterized by a lack of responsiveness to most of the events mentioned above. However, Gillespie was able to identify a precipitant for the majority of the patients in the autonomous group, and concluded that the distinctive feature of this group was the autonomous nature of the illness once the depression had been activated. Other characteristics of the autonomous group were increased “restless activity,“ feelings of remorse, frequent self accusations, and a family history of psychoses. A small subgroup of the autonomous depressions were labeled as involutional, and exhibited a preponderance of hypochrondriacal concerns and poor insight. Lewis (1938) challenged the early attempts at subdividing depression and claimed that depression could be classified according to severity alone. He stated that the manic depressive illnesses most likely represented an acute, severe depression, whereas the other clinical depressions were “...chronic, mild depressions." Most importantly, he argued that differences in symptoms could not be used in a meaningful way to separate subgroups. There have been several attempts to discriminate endogenous from non-endogenous depressions using factor analysis of symptomatology. Hamilton and White (1959) used factor analysis on 17 clinical features in a group of 64 depressed male inpatients. Four factors were extracted which they interpreted as "retarded depression," “agitated depression," "anxiety reaction," and "psychopathic depression." The first factor was felt to represent an endogenous depression, and the symptoms with the highest loading on the factor were depressed mood, self reproach, psychomotor retardation, suicidal ideation, and loss of interest in activities. Heckowicz, Cropley, and Muir (1971) attempted to replicate Hamilton's findings in a study of 52 depressed males on an inpatient unit; in Canada. Four factors were found in the factor analysis. Comparison of the results with the Hamilton results indicated little similarity between the two sets of factors, and none of the factors resembled the ”retarded depression” found in the earlier study. The authors suggested that the relatively small sample sizes in both 10 studies could be a major reason for the lack of agreement between the studies. Kiloh and Garside (1963) conducted a factor analysis on 35 clinical features noted as present or absent in 143 cases of depressed outpatients. Two factors were extracted, the first of which was interpreted as a general factor; the second, a bipolar factor. The second factor was found to have a high correlation with the clinical diagnosis of endogenous or neurotic depression. The clinical features with the highest correlation with the diagnosis of endogenous depres- sion were early awakening, morning worsening, distinct quality of mood, and psychomotor retardation. Neurotic depression was characterized by reactivity to the environment, presence of a precipitant, self pity, and variability of depressed mood. They argue that their results support the “...traditional dichotomy of depressive cases into neurotic and endogenous varieties..." An attempt to replicate Kiloh and Garside's original study was conducted by Kiloh and his associates (1972) using 145 inpatients in Australia. Principal component analyses on the data fron the original study and the replication produced two factors which were similar in nature. The first factor was a bipolar factor which the authors believed to be descriptive of endogenous depression. Unlike the first study, no general factor was discovered, and was thus considered to be an artifact of the analysis technique used in the first study. The second factor was thought to be descriptive of neurotic depression. Features associated with the endogenous factor were depth of depres- sion, early awakening, psychomotor retardation, and quality of the 11 depression. The neurotic factor was characterized by reactivity, presence of a precipitant, duration, and inadequate personality. The authors speculated that, whereas endogenous depression is likely to have the quality of a categorical illness due to the tendency for the scores to cluster together, neurotic depression was more likely to be a dimensional illness. Individuals with neurotic depression would not be expected to exhibit endogenous features, whereas individuals with endogenous depression may respond to their illness with neurotic behaviors. McConaghy, Joffe, and Murphy (1967) attempted to replicate Kiloh and Garside's 1963 study. One hundred outpatients were evaluated for the presence or absence of 43 clinical features, 35 of which were included in the Kiloh and Garside study. Two factors were extracted, and the authors concluded that the factor loadings for each clinical feature did not indicate that either factor could differentiate neurotic from endogenous depression. Eight features, thought to be most representative of endogenous and neurotic depression were selected for factor analysis, and neither of the two factors that were extracted were associated with either type of depression. The authors attributed the differences between their results and the Kiloh and Garside study to possible interviewer bias in Kiloh's study and different methods of patient selection. The interviewers in the Kiloh study were aware of the clinical diagnosis during the interview. Secondly, Kiloh and Garside performed their analysis on the third of their sample that they believed had “definite" diagnoses, whereas, the HcConaghy study did not exclude patients from the analysis. 12 Rosenthal and Gudeman (1967) conducted a factor analysis of the clinical features of 100 female outpatients and inpatients and extracted a bipolar factor which they interpreted as endogenous. Symptoms with the highest loading on the factor were lack of reactivity, concentration difficulties, distinct quality of mood, psychomotor retardation or agitation, and midnight awakening. Symptoms with a negative loading on the factor were self pity and irritability. Kay, Garside, Beamish, and Roy (1969) confirmed the existence of a bipolar factor in their study of 104 of inpatients selected retro- spectively fron hospital records for a five year follow-up study. Thirty-five features were rated as absent or present, based on the hospital records. Symptoms with negative signs on the factor were guilt, psychomotor retardation, severity of depression, hopelessness, suicidal behavior and nihilistic ideas. The authors concluded that this cluster was similar to the endogenous syndrome described by Hamilton and White (1959), Kiloh and Garside (1963), and Rosenthal and Gudeman (1967). However, it should be noted that the 104 patients represented 29% of the cases eligible for analysis, with 71% rejected due to diagnosis, incomplete data, or inability to contact the subject. Garside, Kay, Hilson, Deaton, and Roth (1969) rated 269 depressed inpatients on the Depressive Category Type Scale, (DCTS), which was composed of 15 items believed to distinguish endogenous from neurotic depression. A principal component analysis was conducted and the distribution of the first component scores was calculated. Both the distribution of component scores and the sum of unweighted raw scores had a bimodal distribution, leading the authors to argue that the patients could be divided into an endogenous group and a second group 13 characterized by the absence of endogenous features. The features with the greatest weight on the first component were autonomous course, psychomotor retardation, distinct quality, good premorbid personality, and morning worsening. Klein (1974) observed that most factor analytic studies have shown a bipolar factor that cannot be reduced to a severity factor alone. He suggests that research, in attempting to demonstrate a binary view of depression, has been handicapped by researchers using different definitions of endogenous depression. Klein notes some researchers believe endogenous depression refers primarily to a lack of a precipitant, whereas others use endogenous depression to refer to the lack of reactivity of the depression to environmental changes. He observes, as have others, that many so called endogenous depressions have been found to have a precipitant whereas others have not. He suggests using the term "endogenonorphic depression” to describe the symptom pattern traditionally associated with endogenous depression, as the term endogenomorphic does not necessarily imply a biological etiology. Furthermore, he believes that endogenomorphic depressions can then be subdivided into two types: endogenous depressions, and precipitated depressions with endogenous features. In making the distinction, Klein implies that the endogenous-reactive conceptuali- zation of being polar opposites is incorrect, and that the two concepts are independent from one another. Lewinsohn, Zeiss, Zeiss, and Haller (1977) conducted a factor analytic study that supported Klein's hypothesis that endogeneity and reactivity are independent dimensions. Three groups of depressed outpatients were assessed on 35 symptoms, and a separate factor for 14 endogeneity and reactivity was identified in all three samples. No bipolar factor was extracted, lending support to Klein's two dimen- sional model. Symptoms with the highest loading on the endogeneity factor were helplessness, self reproach, psychomotor retardation, lack of reactivity, loss of interest, and distinct quality of mood. Daly and Cochran (1970) investigated the occurrence of clinical features in 241 white females admitted to the inpatient unit of a North Carolina Hospital. Individuals with a diagnosis of involutional melancholia were compared with all other diagnoses, based on the presence or absence of 31 clinical features. Eight of the 31 comparisons proved significant by chi square. Those individuals in the involutional group were more religious, compulsive, had fewer difficulties in their adult lives, were more likely to receive ECT, antidepressants, and supportive psychotherapy, and exhibited either psychomotor agitation or retardation. A second analysis compared subjects classified as endogenous depressives with those diagnosed as reactive depressives, and 18 comparisons were reported as significant. The endogenous group was described as more religious, less likely to abuse alcohol, and have fewer passive aggressive features or childhood and adult difficulties. Hatussek, Soldner, and Nagel (1981) attempted to confirm the existence of an endogenous syndrome by analyzing symptom frequency and using cluster analysis on the symptoms of 198 subjects who had been previously hospitalized for depression. The subjects were diagnosed as either endogenous or neurotic by Research Diagnostic Criteria. In comparing the symptom frequency between the two groups, the endogenous group was significantly related to eight symptons: morning worsening, 15 lack of reactivity, short duration, distinct quality of mood, psycho- motor retardation, indecisiveness, sudden onset, and delusions. The symptoms associated with neurotic depression were sadness and neuroticism. The cluster analysis determined eight symptoms as being characte- ristic of the endogenous syndrome: distinct quality of mood, lack of reactivity, withdrawal from social contact, impulse inhibition, disturbance of the circadian rhythm, physiological disturbance (appetite or sleep), sudden onset, and absence of a precipitant. They conclude that it is possible to detect an endogenous syndrome by cluster analysis, and believe that the syndrome cannot be defined by the presence or absence of a single item. They suggest the presence of an "endogenous component" with varying levels of strength in all depressive illness. Andreasen and Grove (1982) used cluster analysis to classify a sample of 275 depressed inpatients. One hundred and six symptoms were rated based on the results of a semi-structured interview. Information on course of illness, family history, and treatment variables were used to validate the clusters. Four groups were found as follows: a severely depressed group with endogenous features, a moderately depressed group, a bipolar group, and a depressed group with psychotic features. The authors concluded that the results indicate that subtypes of depression exist and differ in terms of both severity of illness and profile of symptoms. They stated that depressive disorders are ”...probably not a unitary and homogenous phenomena...", and that the 16 results were independent validation of the subtypes described by Research Diagnostic Criteria. Feinberg and Carroll (1982) have recently reported a successful attempt at separating unipolar endogenous depression from non-endogenous depression using discriminant analysis. One hundred and sixty-five inpatients and outpatients were diagnosed as endogenous or nonendogenous based on material from the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia, clinical interviews, and response to treatment. Each patient was rated on several clinical and historical features, as well as the Hamilton Rating Scale. The ratings for all patients with an HRS score over 10 were used in a discriminant analysis in an attempt to validate clinical diagnosis. A discriminant function that separated the two depressed groups contained eight items: decreased appetite, guilt, presence of a precipitant, agitation, delusions, work and interests, retardation, and loss of pleasure. Eighty-four percent of the patients were classified correctly using the discriminant function. Matussek and Luks (1981) found significant differences in the thematic content of endogenous and non-endogenous depressives. Fifty- three women with past episodes of depression were classified as having had endogenous or non-endogenous depression according to Research Diagnostic Criteria. Interviews were conducted with each subject to determine the themes, or concerns of the depression, and to determine whether or not the subject could see a connection between their illness and the contents mentioned. Three themes were reported more frequently in the non-endogenous group: concerns over separation from significant others, arguments 5mg/dl) were classified as endogenous depressions. Those with negative or normal (55mg/dl) were classified according to their RDC diagnosis. The Dexamethasone Suppression Test has been demonstrated to be a highly specific biological marker for endogenous depression, with a specificity rate of 96% (Carroll, et. al., 1981). Individuals with a research diagnosis of "Major Depressive Disorder, Probable or Definite Endogenous" subtype were classified as endogenous depressions. .All other research diagnoses for depression were classified as non-endogenous depressions. 42 Figure 3-2 Diagnostic Classification of Patients Doee the enhjeet neet not for other depreeainni Upon intake oi aeiaeion. doee the client patient oeeeent ieeiinee and/or eyeotoea ei deeeeeaieni yea no i ~Adnigia§lggggns i. Ieeeaeeh Questionnaire Inc (or eejoe lapeeeeive lieotder? IUDCYFF lndoaenoua? 2. Dean-e t haeone Suppression feet (081) 3. sane/Inc yea yea yea eooi oi Endogenous- Ielanehoiie eebjeeta pool oi Moe—ledoaeeoee eehjecte the 0005 were formed by two methods of item selection: an “a priori" method and an "empirical" method. The design for the study was essentially correlational. 43 m constructed by grouping items with similar content together. were then eliminated from each scale if they decreased the internal consistency of the scale. grouped into scales based on their highest loading on factors extracted by principal components analysis, which is correlational in nature. Both methods of item selection were compared by computation of a discriminant function, which is also correlational in nature. 3'3e level to ensure that the endorsement rate for each item was within an acceptable range. .95 were to be withheld from further analyses. Research Hypotheses The main hypotheses for the study were as follows: 1. 2. A flow chart of the analytic procedures is presented as Figure The first activity consisted of examining the item endorsement A self report instrument formed by the a priori method will differentiate between endogenous and non-endogenous depression. A self report instrument constructed by the empirical method will differentiate between endogenous and non-endogenous depression. Differences between endogenous and non-endogenous depression found through hypotheses #1 and #2 will lead to interpretable constructs about the nature of endoge- nous and non-endogenous depression. Analysis Procedures Scales on In the a priori method, scales were In the empirical method, items were first Items with an endorsement level below .05 or above The rationale for Ahcve .15 44 Figure 3-3 Analytic Procedures For DDDS.) Exp-inc Responses 1222£££ (ltee Frequencies) ltee Frequency 8.95. (.05 ggnsigcr is: F scale A Priori Approach 22222 1:... er r-——‘L-—l A priori Classifi- Discard cetion into Scales £5355 Internal Consistency values of Scales “ Internal 3.1.9.3.! ” l_ V" Consistency highs iron original 6? Scale [7 r ital delete ) E Chi-Square Analysis humus Significance ..l°' '15 level Place item in E Scale lapiricsl Approach Correlation Matrix Place its-s into ‘ ® pivgde ss-ple into Endogenous and Mon- __.® Endogenous groups 45 Figure 3-3 (cont ' d) 23 Principal letsin Factors I Cn-ponent with vith eigen values 2%D::::::' one (I) in Diagonal greater than one Lover ii itec included ns ett Internal Consistency of Factors E Discrisinant Analysis Higher if its. included or r 3.39 131255; function [or interpretation and separation 46 withholding these items was that they would have little variance and would not contribute to the discrimination of types of depression. After screening all items for endorsement level, two alternate strategies were followed for selecting items to be used in the construction of scales. In the first approach items were grouped into scales based on an a priori examination of item content. After this grouping, the internal consistency of each of the scales was calculated by the use of coefficient alpha. Each item in a given scale was examined to determine whether the internal consistency would increase if the item was deleted. Items that lowered the internal consistency of the scale were removed from that scale. A three by four chi square was used to analyze each item. Respondents were separated into three diagnostic groups based on their RDC diagnosis of probable, definite, or nonendogenous depression. Items that lowered the internal consis- tency of a given scale but were significant at the .15 level or below in the chi square analysis were removed from the original scale and placed in a general scale labeled as endogenous, or E Scale. In the second approach to scale construction, all items were placed in a correlation matrix and a principal factor solution was done without comnunalities in the diagonals. The principal factor solution was used to examine the relationship among items and to find out how the item responses related to one another. A varimax rotation was used following the principal factor solution. This procedure maximizes the within factor loading for each item. Only those factors with a sum of squares (eigenvalue) in excess of one were rotated. Items that did not load higher than .30 on any of the rotated factors were withheld from further analyses. The internal consistency of each factor was computed 47 with coefficient alpha, and items that lowered the reliability of a given factor and did not load higher than .39 were withheld from further analyses. However, some of these items were retained as part of the scale in an attempt to create an equal number of items as the a priori approach. After the research scales were constructed by the two approaches, the scales were used in a discriminant analysis in an attempt to find a combination of scales that would best discriminate between the two groups. Thirty-five subjects from each diagnostic group were randomly selected for the discriminant analysis and fifteen subjects from each group were withheld for puroses of cross validation. The discriminant analysis was used to compare actual versus predicted group membership for each of the two diagnostic categories. The percentage of correct classification for known groups was compared for the a priori and the empirical method. The discriminant function derived from the analysis phase was used for both methods to predict the group membership for those subjects withheld for cross validation. A second discriminant function was constructed for each approach using those scales identified in the literature as the best predictors of endogenous depression. The discriminant analysis was conducted on the seventy subjects identified above, and cross validated on the group of thirty individuals withheld for that purpose. Finally, the individual items that discriminated between the diagnostic groups placed in a separate scale. The group means on the scale were compared for the seventy randomly selected subjects. A second comparison of group means was conducted for the two cross validation groups. 48 Sunlna ry The literature on endogenous and non-endogenous depression was examined for symptom categories of each type of depression. A research team generated 141 items that were believed to match the content of those categories. The items were conbined with twenty items from the Modified Crowne-Marlowe Scale, and the resultant instrument was named the Differential Diagnostic-Depression Scale (ODDS). A sample of 100 depressed individuals was obtained through a consortium of inpatient and outpatient psychiatric centers. The ODDS was administered to each subject as well as the Dexamethasone Suppression Test (DST) and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS). Individuals were diagnosed as endogenous or non-endogenous depression based on a combination of Dexamethasone Suppression Test values and Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC). In the data analysis, two approaches were used to construct scales from individual items. In the a priori approach, items were grouped in scales prior to analysis based upon similarity in content. The internal consistency for each scale was calculated, and items were deleted if they lowered the reliability Of the scale. A chi square analysis was done on all of the 0005 items. Items that discriminated at the .05 level or below were retained in a general scale. In the second approach to the construction of scales, a principal conponent solution was used on all items. Factors were eitracted until the sum of squares (eigenvalue) was less than 1.0 or the factor was not interpretable. The internal consistency of each\9f the factors was determined, and items were deleted that lowered the reliability and had 49 factor loadings of less than .39. Some of the a priori scales were omitted to ensure an equal number of items in each approach. After the two methods were used to construct two separate sets of scales, a discriminant analysis was used to find a discriminant function which would accurately classify the depressed groups. The accuracy of each method was calculated to compare the efficiency of the two procedures. A second discriminant analysis was conducted using a select number of scales for each approach. Finally, individual items that discriminated between endogenous and nonendogenous items were placed in a scale and a comparison of group means was conducted. CHAPTER FOUR Results of the Data Analysis In Chapter Four, the results of the tests of each of the criteria for item selection as well as the major hypotheses are presented. The results of the tests for item selection for both the a priori and empirical method are presented in the first section, with emphasis placed on the items that were retained or withheld from further analysis. The results from the discriminant analyses for both approaches are presented in the second section. Item Endorsement Level The distribution of responses for each item was examined to determine the item endorsement level. The rationale behind this examination was that items that were endorsed in one direction by a large proportion of the sample would have little variance. None of the responses to the individual items had an endorsement level that exceeded .95. All items, with the exception of the social desirability items, were retained for further analysis. The social desirability items were held for future research efforts. A Priori Scale Construction In the a priori method of item selection, items were grouped in scales according to content similarity (See Table 3-1). The internal consistency of each scale was computed using Cronbach's alpha SO 51 coefficient. If the internal consistency increased when an item was eliminated it was removed from the scale. All items were also screened by chi-square analysis. Items that lowered a scale internal consistency but were significant at the .15 level or below were removed from the original scale and placed in a heterogenous scale labeled endogenous or E Scale. Fifteen items decreased the internal consistency of the original scales and were not signficant at the .15 level or below. These items are listed in Appendix C with the respective alpha coefficients of their original scale. Ten additional items were found to lower the internal consistency of their original scale if included and were significant at the .15 level or below. These items were removed from their original scale and placed in the E Scale and are listed in Appendix D. . Following the examination of the internal consistency and chi-square tests, 12 additional items were withheld from further analysis in order to match the number of items generated by the empirical approach to item selection. This was done to ensure an equal number of items for both approaches for the discriminant analysis. The 12 items were nested in four scales and are listed in Appendix E. The criteria for the elimination of these items was that: 1) none of the items were significant at the .15 level or below; and, 2) none of the four scales (sadness, fear, indecision, and social withdrawal) had prior research support as good discriminators of subtypes of depression. After the 27 items were deleted, 114 items remained for further analysis. The retained items are listed by their respective scale in 52 Table 4-1. Alpha levels are included for 21 of the 25 a priori scales. The reliabilities ranged from .57 to .88. The alpha level was not computed for four of the scales due to the SPSS program limitation that each scale have at least three items. A correlation matrix for the 25 scales is presented as Appendix F. Empirical Scale Construction In the empirical approach of scale construction, all 0005 items except for the social desirability items were placed in a correlation matrix. A principal factor solution was done without communalities in the diagonals. A varimax rotation was executed for factors with an eigenvalue greater than one. Fifteen interpretable factors were extracted from the above analysis as follows: Factor 1: General; Factor 2: Self Reproach; Factor 3: Suicidal Ideation and Behavior; Factor 4: Agitation; Factor 5: Anorexia and Height Loss; Factor 6: Dependency; Factor 7: Interpersonal Dissatisfaction; Factor 8: Memory Impairment; Factor 9: Crying; Factor 10: Body Integrity; Factor 11: Delusions and Hallucinations; Factor 12: Pessimism; Factor 13: Loss of Energy; Factor 14: History; Factor 15: Diurnal Variation and Somatic Anxiety. Eighteen of the items had low loadings on the 15 factors (below .30) and were withheld from further analysis. The internal consistency for the retained items on each factor was computed using the Cronbach alpha coefficient. Nine items were found to lower the internal consistency of a factor and had factor loadings less than .39. These nine items were withheld from further analysis and can be found in Appendix G. The internal consistency of the factors ranged between .31 to .92. 53 Table 4-1 Scales Determined by the A Priori Approach SCALE 1 Item # 86 100 153 Scale 2 Item # 31 51 89 109 Scale 3 Item # 37 39 131 Scale 4 Item # 19 52 103 138 156 161 Scale 5 Item i 60 Scale 6 Item i 11 35 43 75 Scale 7 Item # 15 20 43 ANXIETY; Reliability ' .75 As the days pass, I seem to get more nervous. Sometimes I get so nervous, I panic. I feel tense. CRYING; Reliability = .82 I continue to cry over the event that caused my depression. I seem to cry for no reason at all. I'm really tired of crying. I can't cry even when I feel like it. CONCENTRATION; Reliability 3 .83 I can't seem to keep my mind on one thing. I can't concentrate. I can hardly think at all. DELUSIONS/HALLUCINATIONS; Reliability 8 .71 I hear things that other people don't hear. I feel as if things are not real. I have felt that people have been making me do things by controlling my mind. I think of strange things that are too bad to talk about. I see things that other people don't see. I hear strange things when I'm alone. EATING DISTURBANCE OR HEIGHT LOSS; Reliability = .77 Food doesn't taste good to me anymore. I've been losing weight recently, even though I'm not trying to. IMPULSE INHIBITION; Reliability 8 .78 I hold myself back from doing what I want. I can't let go and have fun anymore I feel so bad I don't feel anything anymore. I can't remember the last time I had a good laugh. I don't allow myself to get involved. REACTIVITY; Reliability - .66 I feel my condition is hopeless. Nothing seems to make me feel better. I feel so bad I don't feel anything anymore. Scale 8 em # 7 58 99 102 119 162 Scale 9 Item 5 Scale 10 Item # 5 68 137 150 Scale 11 Scale 13 Item # 30 101 54 Table 4-1 (continued) LOSS OF INTEREST; Reliability I .76 I have little interest in being with friends. I've lost interest in doing my work. I've lost my interest in sex. I have little desire to eat. I eat less now than usual. Most of the time, I'm not interested in sex, but once in awhile I am. MEMORY; Reliability . .88 I can't count on my memory. I have a hard time remembering things. I don't remember things as well as other pe0ple do. OIURNAL VARIATION; Reliability 8 .71 I wake up refreshed. I feel worse in the morning than any other time in.the day. I wake up depressed and feel like things are going to get worse. I feel tired and jittery when I wake up. ONSET; Reliability 8 .51 My depression came on me quickly. My depression seemed to come out of nowhere. HOPELESSNESS; Reliability 8 .85 I have no hope for myself. Things aren't going to get any better for me. No one can help me now. HISTORY; Reliability 8 .74 Other people in my family have had problems with depression. I mm the only one in my family that I know of that has had problems with depression. Someone in my family has been hospitalized for depression. PSYCHOMOTOR; Reliability I .79 I feel like my heart is racing. I don't have any energy. I'm so tired I don't care about anything anymore. I feel nervous and edgy. Everything bout me is slowed down. I'm tired most of the time, but sometimes I have a burst of energy. I feel like my mind is racing. Item 4 87 124 I45 'Spale 15 Item # 77 82 93 94 95 110 Scale 16 Scale 17 Item 4 26 27 28 47 62 73 76 Scale 18 tem 21 81 85 108 134 Scale I9 ‘_—'36 78 114 Scale 20 Item # 10 I4 17 18 59 66 92 55 Table 4-1 (continued I'm ashamed of the fact that I feel so tired. I feel like my body is speeded up. I've come to live with the fact that I'm always tired. RESPONSIBILITY; Reliability c .73 My ability to manage my life changes alot. I have lost my sense of responsibility. People can't count on me anymore. I could not survive unless someone took care of things for me. Although I feel bad, I can do things if I have to. I cannot manage my own life. OBSESSIVENESS; Reliability 3 .57 I have the same thoughts over and over again. I can't seem to get rid of thoughts that bother me. SELF REPROACH; Reliability - .85 feel worthless and no good. can't stand myself anymore. don't like myself. feel empty and hollow. don't see how anyone can stand being around me. feel powerless. feel guilty. HHHHHHH SEVERITY; Reliability = .75 My depression is so overwhelming, I find it hard to go on. My depression is so severe I drive people away. I'm very depressed. I am more depressed than anyone I know. My depression varies from mild to severe. INSOMNIA; Reliability . .81 I wake up early in the morning and can't back to sleep. I have problems going to sleep. I wake up alot during the night. SOMATIC; Reliability e .82 My chest feels tight. I have problems with constipation. People tell me I sigh alot. I have problems with diarrhea. My body feels like it is rotting inside. I have problems with headaches. I have chest pains frequently. Item i 107 129 132 133 135 143 149 Scale 21 Item # 33 42 98 146 Scale 22 em # 57 Scale 23 Item # 225 Scale 24 Item # 115 111 Scale 25 Item # I48 71 56 Table 4-1 (continued) Although people don't believe it, I know there is something wrong with my body. I have a lot of hard to place aches and pains. I have difficulties breathing. My stomach seems to be upset alot. My body is going to pieces. My breathing seems different now. I find myself sighing alot. SUICIDE; Reliability - .84 I've made a serious attempt to harm myself. I tried to comit suicide, but I knew it wasn't going to I95: tried to commit suicide. I think about suicide alot, but I know I won't do it. DISTINCT 0UALITY* I'm more depressed than usual. LENGTH* My depression started less than a year ago. IRRITATION* I get upset easily. The smallest things seem to upset me. PRECIPITANT* I know what caused my depression. I get depressed over nothing. *Reliability not calculated as the original scale had less than 3 items 57 After the 27 items were deleted in the empirical approach, 114 items remained for further analysis. These items are listed along with their respective factor loadings in Table 4-2. .A correlation matrix for the 15 factors is presented as Appendix H. The correlation matrix was computed for the retained items on each factor. Discriminant Analysis Following the construction of the research scales by the a priori and empirical approach, a discriminant analysis was exetuted to find a combination of scales that would best descriminate between the two depressed groups. Thirty-five subjects from each diagnostic group (endogenous and non-endogenous) were randomly selected for the analysis, and the remaining subjects were held for cross validation. One discriminant function was constructed for each approach. The standardized function coefficients for the variables used in both approaches is presented in Table 4-3. Each coefficient indicates the relative contribution of the variable to the discriminant function. In the a priori approach, the scales anxiety and precipitant made the largest relative contribution to the discriminant function, whereas the scales impulse inhibition and length made the smallest contribution. In the empirical approach, the scales pessimism and general made the largest contribution, whereas suicidal ideation and crying contributed the smallest amount to the discriminant function. The discriminant functions were then used to classify the seventy cases that were randomly selected from the two depressed groups. The classification results are presented in Tables 4-4 and 4-5. Eighty seven percent of the seventy cases were classified correctly with the a priori approach versus 77% correct classification with the empirical 58 Table 4-2 Scales Selected by the Empirical Approach ._____________________________________________________________________________ Factor 1: GENERAL; Reliability = .92 Item # Factor Loading 44. Everything about me is slowed down. .78 43. I feel so bad I don't feel anything anymore. .73 158. All the joy is gone out of my life. .71 47. I feel empty and hollow. .69 54. I can't remember the last time I had a good laugh. .67 57. I'm more depressed than usual. .63 45. I don't take chances anymore. .58 35. I can't let go and have fun anymore. .58 75. I don't allow myself to get involved. .56 85. I'm very depressed. .55 131. I can hardly think at all. .52 97. I feel sad. .51 20. Nothing seems to make me feel better. .49 150. I feel tired and jittery when I wake up. .47 73. I feel powerless. .45 139. I can't seem to think about anything but how bad I feel. .45 2. I don't have any energy. .44 130. I want to be alone most of the time. .42 59. My body feels like its rotting inside. .41 147. I usually get along with people but lately my seem to have been falling apart. .39 58. I've lost interest in doing my work. .39 59 Table 4-2 (continued) Item # Factor Loading 142. I'm afraid about what will happen in the future. .38 69. I have pulled into myself. .37 21. My depression is so overwhelming, I find it hard to go on. .35 93. People can't count on my anymore. .33 29. I have been slowly getting more and more depressed over the past few weeks. -.47 Factor 2: SELF REPROACH/ANXIETY, Reliability 8 .9O 26. I feel worthless and no good. .71 28. I don't like myself. .67 27. I can't stand myself anymore. .61 12. I watch what I am doing with other people. .58 11. I hold myself back from doing what I want. .58 6. I have many different fears about the present and the future. .54 76. I feel guilty. .52 41. I don't seem to have any control over my life. .47 36. I feel nervous and edgy. .45 86. As the days pass, I seem to get more nervous. .44 74. I have the same thoughts over and over again. .36 100. Sometimes I get so nervous, I panic. .32 Factor 3: SUICIDAL IDEATION AND BEHAVIOR, Reliability - .86 98. I've tried to commit suicide. .82 33. I've made a serious attempt to harm myself. .78 42. I tried to commit suicide, but I knew it wasn't going to work. .77 Item # 63. 146. 53. 117. 121. Factor 4: 123. 124. 1. IO. 37. 78. Factor 5: 102. 119. 60. 9. 157. 38. 60 Table 4-2 (continued) I have been hospitalized for depression. I think about suicide a lot, but I know I won't do it. There have been times in my life when I had no idea what I did. I've done things too terrible to think about. If I weren't so afraid of dying, I'd commit suicide. AGITATION, Reliability = .82 I feel like my mind is racing. I feel like my body is speeded up. I feel like my heart is racing. My chest feels tight. I can't seem to keep my mind on one thing. I have problems going to sleep. ANOREXIA/HEIGHT LOSS, Reliability = .86 I have little desire to eat. I eat less now than usual. I've been losing weight recently, even though I'm not trying to. Food doesn't taste good to me anymore. I eat more when I get depressed. I wake up early in the morning and can't get back to sleep. Loading .64 .54 Item i Factor 6: 94. 110. 95. 81. 82. 127. 39. 99. Factor 7: 126 O 111. 62. 65. 105. 83. 115. 162. Factor 8: 91. 67. 61 Table 4-2 (continued) Factor DEPENDENCY, Reliability 8 .82 I could not survive unless someone took care of things for me. I cannot manage my own life. Although I feel bad, I can do things if I have to. I have little interest in being with friends. My depression is so severe I drive people away. I have lost my sense of responsibility. I find it impossible to make a decision. I can't concentrate. I've lost my interest in sex. INTERPERSONAL DISSATISFACTION, Reliability 8 .83 I have had difficulties getting close to people for a long time. The smallest things seem to upset me. I don't see how anybody can stand being around me. I pull away from people. I seem to drive people away. I get irritated a lot. I get upset easily. Most of the time I'm not interested in sex, but once in a while I am. MEMORY IMPAIRMENT, Reliability 8 .62 I have a hard time remembering things. I can't count on my memory. Loading .81 .78 Item # 118. 84. Factor 9: 51. 109. 89. 66. 31. Factor 10: 135. 133. 92. 107. 129. 159. Factor 11: 19. 161. 103. 138. 62 Table 4-2 (continued) Factor Loading I don't remember things as well as other people seem to. .71 I can't do anymore than I'm doing now. .34 CRYING, Reliability 8 .80 I seem to cry for no reason at all. .72 I can't cry even when I feel like it. .63 I'm really tired of crying. .61 I have problems with headaches. .58 I continue to cry over the event that caused . my depression. .52 BODY INTEGRITY, Reliability 8 .58 My body is going to pieces. .57 My stomach seems to be upset a lot. .53 I have chest pains frequently. .47 Although people don't believe it, I know there is something wrong with my body. .41 I have a lot of hard to place aches and pains. .40 My body seems to be working all right. -.69 DELUSIONS/HALLUCINATIONS, Reliability 8 .67 I hear things that other people don't hear. .74 I hear strange things when I'm alone. .74 I have felt that people have been making me do things by controlling my mind. .48 I think of strange things that are too bad to talk about. .42 63 Table 4-2 (continued) Item # Factor Loading Factor 12: PESSIMISM/HOPELESSNESS, Reliability 8 .31 49. Things aren't going to get any better for me. .54 149. I find myself sighing a lot. .50 156. I see things that other people don't see. .48 34. I have no hope for myself. .39 148. I know what caused my depression. .38 155. I can't seem to get rid of some of the thoughts that bother me. -.60 113. When things are going well I feel like I have more energy. . -.68 Factor 13: FATIGUE, Reliability 8 .72 50. I'm tired most of the time, but sometimes I have a burst of energy. .65 145. I've come to live with the fact that I'm always tired. .61 87. I'm ashamed of the fact I feel so tired. .61 Factor 14: HISTORY, Reliability 8 .72 101. I am the only one in my family that I know of who has problems with depression. .80 30. Other people in my family have had problems with depression. .78 122. Someone in my family has been hospitalized for depression. .69 64 Table 4-2 (continued) Item i Factor Loading Factor 15: OIURNAL VARIATION/SOMATIC ANXIETY, Reliability 8 .52 68. I feel worse in the morning than any other time in the day. .61 17. People tell me I sigh alot. .60 154. I feel miserable in the morning. .48 132. I have difficulties breathing. .36 55. I'm afraid of being alone. .35 I4. I have problems with constipation. .34 65 Table 4-3 Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficients for the A Priori and Empirical Approaches A PRIORI EMEIBlCAL Variable Coefficient Variable Coefficient Anxiety -.901 General -.601 Crying -.182 Self Reproach -.333 Concentration -.616 Suicidal Ideation Delusions/Hallucinations .204 and Behavior -.O38 Eating .130 Agitation .142 Impulse Inhibition -.020 Anorexia/Height Loss .5237 Reactivity -.O91 Dependency .296 Loss of Interest .379 Interpersonal Dissatisfaction .337 Memory .317 Memory Impairment .359 Diurnal Variation .602 Crying -.078 Onset -.108 Body Integrity .230 Pessimism .343 Delusions/Hallucinations .101 History .112 Pessimism .641 Psychomotor Change .324 Fatigue .050 Responsibility .078 History .043 Obsessiveness .129 Diurnal Variation] Reproach -.102 Somatic Anxiety .373 Severity .286 A Sleep .493 Somatic .249 E Scale .314 Distinct Quality -.356 Length -.028 Irritation .299 Precipitant .618 Suicide -.461 66 Table 4-4 Classification Results of Discriminant Function with A Priori Scale Construction Number Predicted Group Membership Actual Group of Cases Endogenous Non-Endogenous Endogenous 35 29 6 Non-Endogenous 35 4 31 2 - X " 39e4, p.05). The following factors were used in the empirical approach: General, self reproach, agitation, anorexia/weight loss, body integrity, diurnal variation/somatic anxiety. 68 Table 4-6 Cross Validation Results of A Priori Discriminant Function Number Predicted Group Actual Group of Cases Endogenous Non-Endogenous _ Endogenous 15 8 7 Non-Endogenous 15 6 9 Table 4-7 Cross Validation Results of Empirical Discriminant Function Number Predicted Group Actual Group of Cases Endogenous Non-Endogenous W Endogenous 15 6 9 Non-Endogenous 15 8 7 69 The discriminant function accurately classified 73% of the original seventy subjects and 57% of the cross validation group, which was not significantly different than chance (p>.05). Following the discriminant analysis, the eighteen items that discriminated between probable, definite, and non-endogenous depression via by chi-square analysis were placed in a scale. The group means were compared for the endogenous and non-endogenous groups for the seventy randomly selected subjects. There was a significant difference between the two group means (p<.01). However, there was no significant differences in the group means for the cross validation groups (p>.05). Research Hypotheses In Chapter Three, the following research hypotheses were stated: 1. A self report instrument formed by the a priori method will differentiate between endogenous and non-endogenous depression. 2. A self report instrument constructed by the empirical method will differentiate between endogenous and non-endogenous depression. 3. Differences between endogenous and non-endogenous depression found through hypotheses 1 and 2 will lead to interpretable constructs about the nature of endogenous and non-endogenous depression. Based on the results of the original discriminant function with the study sample, the first hypothesis was rejected. The classifica- tion accuracy based on the a priori approach dropped from .87 to .57 in cross validation, which is not significantly different than chance classification. These results were confirmed with the second discrimi- nant function using selected scales. 70 The second hypothesis was also rejected. The classification accuracy based on the empirical approach dropped from .77 to .43, which is not significantly different than chance. This was supported by the second discriminant function using selected factors. Due to the instability of the accuracy of classification for both approaches, the third hypothesis was rejected. No clear, consistent differences emerged that led to interpretable constructs. Summary The item endorsement level of all items in the 0005 was examined to identify items that had an endorsement level greater than .95. None of the items had an endorsement level greater than .95. All items, with the exception of the social desirability items, were retained for further analysis. In the a priori scale construction items were grouped in twenty nine scales according to content similarity. The internal consistency of each scale was computed by coefficient alpha. The frequency distribution of responses was analyzed by chi-square. Fifteen items decreased the internal consistency of the scale and were not signifi- cant at the .15 level or below. These items were withheld from further analysis. Ten additional items lowered the internal consistency of the original scale and were significant at the .15 level. These items were placed in a general scale. One hundred and fourteen items remained for further analysis. Twelve additional items were omitted to ensure equal numbers of items in both approaches to scale construction. In the empirical approach to scale construction, fifteen factors were extracted by a principal component solution. The factors were rotated by a varimax rotation. Eighteen items failed to load higher 71 than .29 on any of the factors and were withheld from further analysis. The internal consistency of each scale was conputed by coefficient alpha. Nine items were found to lower the reliability of a scale when included and had factor loadings less than .39. These items were withheld from further analysis. A discriminant analysis was used to construct discriminant functions for both sets of scales. In the a priori approach, the discriminant function correctly classified 86% of a randomly selected subgroup of a sample of depressed subjects and 57% of a group selected for cross validation. The discriminant function constructed by the empirical approach correctly classified 77% of the randomly selected subgroup and 43% of the group selected by cross validation. A second discriminant analysis was used for both approaches using scales selected on the basis of the literature review. In the a priori approach, the discriminant function accurately classified 73% of the randomly selected groups and 60% of the cross validation groups. In the empirical approach, the discriminant function accurately classified 73% of the randomly selected groups, and 60% of the cross validation groups. The eighteen items that discriminated between the diagnostic groups were aggregated. A comparison between group means for the seventy subjects selected at random was significant at the .01 level. However, there» was no significant difference (p<.05) between group means for the cross validation groups. CHAPTER 5 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS In Chapter 5, four sections are presented. The first section contains a summary of the research project. The conclusions are drawn in the second section, and a discussion of the findings is presented in the third section. The fourth section of the chapter contains impli- cations for future research. em The purpose of the study was to determine whether or not a patient self rated instrument could differentiate endogenous and non-endogenous depression. Two major conclusions were drawn from a review of the literature. Three of the 22 studies failed to support the conclusion that depression was not a homogenous entity. Symptoms that most frequently distinguished endogenous from non-endogenous depression were psychomotor disturbance, autonomous course, self reproach, distinct quality of mood, and middle or terminal insomnia. The second conclusion was that although there had been several self rated measures of depression in the literature, none were designed to differentiate subtypes of depression. After the review of the literature was complete, 29 symptom categories of depression were identified. A research team generated 141 items that were designed to represent these categories. The resultant instrument was named the Differential Diagnostic Depression 72 73 Scale (ODDS). A sample of 100 individuals with the chief complaint of depression was obtained through a consortium of inpatient and out- patient psychiatric centers. The DDDS was administered to each subject as well as the Dexamethasone Suppression Test (DST) and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (SADS). Individuals were diagnosed as endogenous or non-endogenous based on a combination of DST values and Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC). In the data analysis, two approaches were used to combine items into research scales. In the a priori approach, items were grouped into scales based on similarity in content. The frequency of responses per diagnostic category was compared by chi-square, and the internal consistency of each scale was computed. Fifteen items decreased the internal consistency of their original scale and were not significant at the .15 level or below. These items were deleted from further analysis. Ten items decreased the internal consistency and were significant at the .15 level. These items were placed in a general category labeled endogenous, or E Scale. Twelve additional items were deleted to ensure an equal number of items as the empirical approach. A principal component solution was used in the empirical approach and 15 factors were extracted. A varimax rotation was used. Eighteen items failed to load on any of the extracted factors and were withheld from further analyses. The internal consistency of the factors was computed and the nine items that lowered the internal consistency and did not load higher than .39 on their respective factors were deleted. A discriminant analysis was used to construct a function that would best separate endogenous and non-endogenous depression for each approach. Thirty five subjects from each group were randomly selected 74 for the analysis phase. The discriminant function constructed by the a priori approach correctly classified 87% of the randomly selected groups, and was significant at the .05 level. The discriminant function constructed by the empirical approach correctly classified 77% of the randomly selected groups, and was significant at the .005 level. Cross validation was attempted by using the discriminant functions to classify the 30 remaining individuals in the sample. The classifi- cation accuracy was 57% with the a priori discriminant function, and 44% with the empirical discriminant function. A second discriminant function was constructed for both approaches using a reduced number of scales. In the a priori approach, the second function correctly classified 73% of the original seventy subjects, and 60% of the cross validation groups. In the empirical approach, the second function correctly classified 73% of the cross validation groups. Finally, eighteen items that discriminated between non-endogenous, probable, and definite endogenous depression were aggregated. There was a significant difference (p<.01) between the endogenous and non-endogenous group means for the original seventy subjects. However, there was no significant difference (p>.05) between the group means for the cross validation groups. Conclusions The conclusions reached from the view of the literature were as follows: 1. Previous studies on symptom differences between endogenous and non-endogenous depression had considerable variation in sample composition, diagnostic criterion, and range of symptoms included for analysis. The 1. 5. 75 Nineteen of the 22 studies reviewed supported the conclusion that depression can best be explained using a binary, or pluralistic paradigm. The most frequently cited symptom differences in the litera- ture between endogenous and non-endogenous depression were psychomotor disturbance, autonomous course, self reproach, distinct quality of mood, and middle or terminal insomnia. conclusions from the study are as follows: There was a significantly higher percentage of females in the endogenous sample than in the non-endogenous sample (x2, p<.05). There were no other significant differences in the demographics for the two groups. None of the 141 DDDS items had an endorsement level greater than .95. One hundred and fourteen items survived the tests for internal consistency in the a priori approach. Twenty five scales were constructed ranging in reliability from .51 to .88. Fifteen factors emerged from a principal component solution in the empirical approach. One hundred and fourteen items had factor loadings above .39 and did not decrease the internal consistency of the factors. The reliability of the factors ranged from .31 to .92. The discriminant function constructed by the a priori approach correctly classified 87% of a random sample of 70 of the 100 original subjects. 0n cross validation, the correct classifi- cation shrank to .57. 76 6. The discriminant function constructed by the empirical approach correctly classified 77% of a random sample of 70 of the 100 original subjects. On cross validation, the correct classification shrank to .44. Discussion The Differential Diagnostic Depression Scale (ODDS) is a self rated questionnaire. developed as an inexpensive alternative for the differentiation of subtypes of depression. In the pilot study of the instrument, two approaches were used to combine items into scales: a priori classification and factor analysis. The two discriminant functions constructed from the different approaches classified the random sample of 70 subjects with impressive accuracy (87%. and 77%, respectively). However, the accuracy rate proved to be highly unstable, dropping to 57% for the a priori function and 44% for the empirical function with the cross validation groups. The instability can be explained by three possible sources. The first source is error variance in the scales, or the variables used in the discriminant analyses to construct the functions. The second potential source of error was variance in the diagnostic classifications. In addition, there may have been systematic differences between the two groups used in the classification and cross validation phases of the study. There was considerable variability in the reliability of the scales used for both approaches. Although attempts were made in both approaches to maximize the internal consistency of the scales, considerable inter-scale differences existed in scale reliability. The alpha coefficients ranged from .51 to .88 with the a priori approach, 77 and .32 to .92 with the empirical approach. The scales in both approaches with low reliability may have contributed to the instability of the classification accuracy. One reason for the differences in the reliability of the scales can probably be attributed to differences in scale lengths. Reliability of scales increases with longer scales. Furthermore, some of the content areas that have been associated with endogenous depression may have been under-represented by items. For. example, although the review of the literature in Chapter 2 showed that "distinct quality of mood" was one of the most consistent symptoms of endogenous depression, it was represented by only one item on the ODDS. The reason for having only one item was that certain symptom categories did not readily lend themselves to item generation. The research team made every reasonable effort to generate items for each category. A second reason for the variability in the instrument may have been that the items differed in the reading level required for their comprehension. No tests were performed on the items to ensure that items were not above an eighth grade level of reading comprehension, however, efforts were made to keep the vocabulary of each item as simple as possible. Criterion variance may also have contributed to the shrinkage in classification accuracy between the two sample groups. The endogenous group was formed by collapsing two diagnostic subtypes: those indivi- duals with a 'probable' endogenous depression and those with a 'definite' endogenous depression. Some researchers have advocated eliminating the probable endogenous group from classification studies because there are false positives in the group (Feinberg, et. al., 1982; Spitzer, 1978). The inclusion of the probable endogenous group 78 in the study may have had the effect of minimizing the differences between the two diagnostic groups. Secondly, although the reliability of the RDC has been established (Spitzer, 1978; Gibbon, 1981; and Holmes, et. al., 1983) questions remain about the validity of the diagnostic categories. Carroll has argued that the RDC is not as accurate in classifying endogenous depression as his clinical diagnostic method (Carroll, et. al., 1981). Nelson and Charney (1981) have stated in their review that other diagnostic criteria, such as the DSM-III, may be more sensitive to endogenous depression. The phenomena of endogenous depression may be unstable in itself. Carroll (1982) has argued that the DST may suffer a loss of sensitivity and specificity if it is not administered in the middle of the episode. Unfortunately, the lack of research in measuring the episodic nature of endogenous depression did not permit an accurate classification of the sample on this variable. Implications and Recommendations for Further Research The pilot study of the DDDS was an initial attempt to develop a self rated instrument that. would differentiate endogenous and non-endogenous depression. Considering the difficulties discussed above, further research is strongly recommended as follows: 1. Construction of new discriminant functions with the probable endogenous group eliminated fron the analysis and classifi- cation phases. This step would likely increase the separation of the two groups by removal of the false positives. Further research is required into the nature of the probable endo- genous group before they can be included in classification studies. 2. 79 Refining the scales used in the a priori and empirical approaches. There are several methods that can be used in this approach. First, some of the shorter, less reliable scales should be lengthened and the internal consistency recalculated. Second, a subset of the scales generated in the study could be selected for the construction of a new discriminant function. The selection of the scales should be based on the following criteria: the scale should be reliable, have a low correlation with another scale, and discriminate between the two depressed groups by univariate analysis. Use the DST and medication response to decrease criterion variance. Another approach to the diagnostic classification of endogenous depression is the use the DST and/or medication response to form the known groups for the discriminant analysis and classification. APPENDICES APPENDIX A 80 Appendix A Differential Diagnostic Depression Scale (ODDS) ~ All items are to be marked: OT 8 Definitely Irue of me T 8 True of me NT 8 Not True of me ONT 8 Eefi'n’itely Not lrue of me (DT) (T) (NT) (ONT) 1. I feel like my heart is racing. () () () () 2. I don't have any energy. () () () () 3. I'm so tired I don't care about ' anything anymore. () (l () (l 4. People can read my mind. () () () () 5. I wake up refreshed. () () () () 6. I have many different fears about the present and future. () () () () 7. I have little interest in being with friends. () i) (i (l 8. I am careful about my manner of dress. () () () () 9. Food doesn't taste good to me anymore. () () () () 10. My chest feels tight. () () () () 11. I hold myself back from doing what I want. (1 (l (l (l 12. I watch what I am doing with other people. () (l (l (l 13. My depression is mild but always there. () () () () 14. I have problems with constipation. () () () () 15. I feel my condition is hopeless. () () () () 16. I can remember “playing sick” to get out of sonething. () () () () 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 81 Appendix A (continued) People tell me I sigh a lot. I have problems with diarrhea. I hear things that other people don't hear. Nothing seems to make me feel better. My depression is so overwhelming, I find it hard to go on. My depression came on me quickly. I have a hard time making a decision on small things but I do alright on the important things. I just can't be courteous to people who are disagreeable. My depression started less than a year ago. I feel worthless and no good. I can't stand myself anymore. I don't like myself. I have been slowly getting more and more depressed over the past few weeks. Other people in my family have had problems with depression. I continue to cry over the event that caused my depression. Hhen I don't know something I try to cover it up. I've made a serious attempt to harm myself. I have no hope for myself. I can't let go and have fun anymore. I feel nervous and edgy. (DI) () (l () () (l () () (l (l () () (l () (l (l (l (l (l () (i (T) () (l () (l (l () (l (l () () () i) () (l () (l () (l () () (NT) () (l () () (l () () (l () () (l () () () () (l (l (l (I () (DNT) () () (l (l () () () () (l (l (l () () (l (l () () I) () I) 82 Appendix A (continued) (DT) (T) (NT) (ONT) 37. I can't seem to keep my mind on one thing. () () (l (l 38. I wake up early in the morning and can't get back to sleep. () () () () 39. I can't concentrate. () () () () 40. I never like to gossip. () () () () 41. I don't seem to have any control over my life. () () () (l 42. I tried to commit suicide, but I knew it wasn't going to work. () (l (l (l 43. I feel so bad I don't feel anything anymore. () () () () 44. Everything about me is slowed down. () () () () 45. I don't take chances anymore. () () () () 46. Nothing makes me feel better. () () () () 47. I feel empty and hollow. () () () () 48. I do not hesitate to go out of my way to help someone in trouble. () () () () 49. Things aren't going to get any better for me. () (l (l (l 50. I'm tired most of the time, but sometimes I have a burst of energy. () () () () 51. I seem to cry for no reason at all. () () () () 52. I feel as if things are not real. () () () () 53. There have been times in my life when I had no idea what 1 did. () () () () 54. I can't remember the last time I had a good laugh. () (I (l (l 55. I'm afraid of being alone. () () () () 56. I rarely check the safety of my car no matter how far I am traveling. () () () () 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. 83 Appendix A (continued) I'm more depressed than usual. I've lost interest in doing my work. My body feels like its rotting inside. I've been losing weight recently, even though I'm not trying to. I feel like my mind is racing. I don't see how anybody can stand being around me. I have been hOSpitalized for depression. Sometimes I deliberately hurt someone's feelings. I pull away from people. I have problems with headaches. I can't count on my memory. I feel worse in the morning than any other time in the day. I have pulled into myself. Hhen I'm not thinking about what depresses me, I can think clearly. I get depressed over nothing. I am sloppy about my manner of dress. I feel powerless. I have the same thoughts over and over again. I don't allow myself to get involved. I feel guilty. My ability to manage my life changes a lot. I have problems going to sleep. IDT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) (T) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) INT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) (our) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. 89. 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. 95. 96. 97. 98. 99. 84 Appendix A (continued) No one can help me now. It is sometimes hard for me to go on with my work if I am not encouraged. My depression is so severe I drive pe0ple away. I have lost my sense of responsibility. I get irritated a lot. I can't do anymore than I'm doing now. I'm very depressed. As the days pass, I seem to get more nervous. I'm ashamed of the fact that I feel so tired. I sometimes try to get even rather than to forgive and forget. I'm really tired of crying. Things I like to do don't seem as much fun as they used to be. I have a hard time remembering things. I have chest pains frequently. People can't count on me anymore. I could not survive unless someone took care of things for me. Although I feel bad, I can do things if I have to. No matter who I'm talking to, I'm a good listener. I feel sad. I've tried to commit suicide. I've lost my interest in sex. IDT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) IT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) INT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) (out) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. 107. 108. 109. 110. 111. 112. 113. 114. 115. 116. 117. 118. 119. 85 Appendix A (continued) Sometimes I get so nervous I panic. I am the only one in my family that I know of who has problems with depression. I have little desire to eat. I have felt that people have been making me do things by controlling my mind. I do not intensely dislike anyone. I seem to drive people away. My depression seemed to come out of nowhere. Although people don't believe it, I know there is something wrong with me. I am more depressed than anyone I know. I can't cry even when I feel like it. I cannot manage my own life. The smallest things seem to upset me. There have been occasions when I took advantage of someone. Hhen things are going well I feel like I have more energy. I wake up a lot during the night. I get upset easily. People are there to help me now that I need them. I've done things too terrible to think about. I don't remember things as well as other people seem to. I eat less now than usual. (PT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) IT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) INT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) (ONT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) 120. 121. 122. 123. 124. 125. 126. 127. 128. 129. 130. 131. 132. 133. 134. 135. 136. 137. 138. 86 Appendix A (continued) I have not deliberately said something to hurt someone's feelings. If I weren't so afraid of dying, I'd commit suicide. Someone in my family has been hospitalized for depression. I feel like my mind is racing. I feel like my body is speeded up. I wake up often, but finally fall into a deep sleep. I have had difficulties getting close to people for a long time. I find it impossible to make a decision. Before voting, I thoroughly investigate the qualifications of all the candidates. I have a lot of hard-to-place aches and pains. I want to be alone most of the time. I can hardly think at all. I have difficulties breathing. My stomach seems to be upset a lot. My depression varies from mild to severe. My body is going to pieces. The urge to tell someone off is not part of my make-up. I wake up depressed and feel like things are going to get worse. I think of strange things that are too bad to talk about. IDT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) IT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) INT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) (ONT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) 139. 140. 141. 142. 143. 144. 145. 146. 147. 148. 149. 150. 151. 152. 153. 154. 155. 156. 157. 87 Appendix A (continued) I can't seem to think about anything but how bad I feel. If I could cry, I would feel better. I need people more now than before. I'm afraid about what will happen in the future. My breathing seems different now. If I could get into a movie without paying and be sure I was not seen, I would probably do it. I've come to live with the fact that I'm always tired. I think about suicide a lot, but I know I won't do it. I usually get along with people, but lately my relationships seem to have been falling apart. I know what caused my depression. I find myself sighing a lot. I feel tired and jittery when I wake up. I need to be by myself. There have been times when I feel like rebelling against people in authority even though I knew they were right. I feel tense. I feel miserable in the morning, but have high hopes for the day. I can't seem to get rid of some of the thoughts that bother me. I see things that other people don't see. I eat more when I get depressed. IDT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) IT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) INT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) (DNT) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) I) 158. 159. 160. 161. 162. 163. 88 Appendix A (continued) All the joy is gone out of my life. My body seems to be working all right. I admit my mistakes. I hear strange things when I'm alone. Most of the time I'm not interested in sex, but once in a while I am. My feelings about myself change a lot. (PT) I) I) I) I) I) I) (T) I) I) I) I) I) I) INT) I) I) I) I) I) I) (ONT) I) I) I) I) I) I) APPENDIX B 89 Appendix B Consent Form Study: Self Reported Symptomatology *In Major Depressive Illness Investigator: Gregory Alan Holmes, M.A. Doctoral Candidate, Counseling Psychology Robert J. Bielski, M.D. Department of Psychiatry I, , have had the above named study explained to my satisfaction and I freely consent to participate in the study. I have been informed that my decision to participate in this study will in no way alter the treatment I will receive for my depression. I understand that I have been asked to complete three procedures during this study. These three procedures are as follows: 1. I will be interviewed by members of the research team. The interview will last for approximately 1-1/2 hours, during which time I will be asked questions about my depression. 2. I will be given the Dexamethasone Suppression Test, a blood test for plasma cortisol concentration. I will be given 1 mg of Dexamethasone in a tablet form and be asked to take the tablet at 11:00 p.m. I understand that there is minimal risk in taking this medication. I will then have a small sample of my blood drawn the following day at 4:00 p.m. 3. I will be given the Differential Diagnostic Depression Scale (ODDS), a 163 item questionnaire. I agree to participate in the procedures described above. I understand that the amount of risk and discomfort involved in this study is very small; being no greater than that usually involved in drawing a small blood sample. I understand that the benefits to me 90 Appendix B (continued) from participating in this study will be a special evaluation of my depression. I further understand that I may speak to and/or meet with one of the study psychiatrists at this time. I further understand that I may ask questions before signing this consent form, or any time thereafter, that my participation in this study is voluntary and that I am free to withdraw at any point without penalty. I further understand that the results from these procedures are confidential and can only be released to others with my written permission. Signed: (Subject) (Date) (Hitness) (Date) xc: Subject File APPENDIX C 91 Appendix C Items Deleted from ODDS During A Priori Approach M Item Original Alpha IF: Number Scale Included/Deleted Significance 140 Crying .68 .82 .75 4 Delusions/Hallucinations .70 .71 .88 157 Eating .70 .77 .34 12 Impulse Inhibition .777 .779 .78 113 Reactivity .50 .66 .74 90 Loss of Interest .762 .763 .24 154 Diurnal Variation .681 .712 .34 41 Hopelessness .813 .855 .63 84 Responsibility .731 .769 .35 155 Obsessiveness .332 .566 .56 83 Sadness .737 .758 .67 125 Insomnia .757 .813 .93 159 Somatic .786 .82 .41 116 Social Hithdrawal .764 .788 .82 141 Social Withdrawal .764 .789 .65 APPENDIX D 92 Appendix D E Scale Items ———_—_———___—_———————-——————-——_————'——-—_-—. Item Original Alpha IF: Number Scale Included/Deleted Significance 7O Concentration .772 .826 .06 53 Delusions/Hallucinations .704 .704 .15 55 Fear .541 .724 .O4 29 Onset .114 .517 .02 63 History .563 .744 .01 163 Reproach .848 .867 .07 13 Severity .696 .748 .14 121 Suicide .831 .841 .008 147 Social Withdrawal N/A N/A .07 117 Self Reproach N/A N/A .004 APPENDIX E 93 Appendix E Scales Deleted by ‘9" "1°" A_'_'_"_3CI‘_____ Item Original Scale Significance 23 Decision Making .26 127 Decision Making .58 6 Fear .28 142 Fear .16 97 Sadness .40 158 Sadness .16 151 Social Withdrawal .53 130 Social Withdrawal .68 126 Social Withdrawal .50 105 Social Withdrawal .52 69 Social Hithdrawal .30 65 Social Withdrawal .80 APPENDIX F 94 APPENDIX F. CORRELATION MATRIX FOR A PRIORI SCALES. Anxiety Crying Concen D a H Eating Impulse Reactive Anxiety 1.00 .22 .57 .29 :38 .49 .38 Crying .21 1.00 .12 .14 .27 .01 .14 Concen .57 .12 1.00 .18 .26 .43 .51 Schizo .29 .14 .18 1.00 .19 .45 .33 Rating .38 .27 .26 .19 1.00 .33 .34 Impulse .49 .Ol .43 .45 .33 1.00 .56 Reactive .38 .14 .51 .33 .34 .56 1.00 Interest .35 .21 .38 .30 .67 .47 .50 Memory .32 .23 .44 .15 .05 .16 .26 Diurnal .52 .13 .55 .16 .30 .38 .42 Onset .12 -.13 .20 .19 .32 .16 .30 Hopelessness .24 .23 .28 .42 .26 .42 .54 History .12 -.05 .10 .20 -.O7 .17 .05 Psymotor .62 .19 .59 .42 .37 .55 .53 Response .30 .15 .51 .43 .23 .46 .38 Obsess .56 .30 .52 .31 .39 .49 .35 Reproach .61 .29 .45 .43 .31 .58 .55 Severity .44 .21 .55 .33 .31 .46 .69 Insomnia .41 .35 .33 .26 .43 .18 L19 Somatic .41 .42 .41 .44 .40 .37 .42 E Scale .11 .37 .15 .44 .34 .18 .29 Distinct .32 .31 .32 .23 .45 .41 .48 Length .07 .02 -.Ol .18 .31 .23 .IO Irrate .42 .16 .36 .14 .09 .32 .34 Precip -.06 -.O4 -.16 -.13 .13 -.O6 -.26 Suicide .09 .21 .03 .35 .24 .09 .21 95 APPENDIX F. CORRELATION MATRIX FOR A_PRIORI SCALES (CON'T.). Interest Memory Diurnal Onset Hopelessness History Anxiety .35 .32 .52 .12 .24 .12 Crying .21 .23 .13 -.03 .23 -.05 Concen .38 .44 .55 .20 .28 .IO Schizo .29 .15 .16 .19 .42 .20 Eating .67 .OS .30 .32 .26 -.O7 Impulse .47 .18 .38 .16 .42 .17 Reactive .50 .26 .42 .30 .54 .05 Interest 1.00 .11 .35 .24 .46 .05 Memory .11 1.00 .23 -.01 .22 .16 Diurnal .35 .23 1.00 .07 .20 .13 Onset .24 -.01 .07 1.00 .13 -.O9 Hopelessness .46 .22 .20 .13 1.00 .04 History .05 .16 .13 -.09 .04 1.00 Psymotor .56 .26 .51 .14 .36 .06 Response .49 .21 .31 .09 .50 .12 Obsess .40 .34 .43 .21 .32 .16 Reproach .43 .35 .4O .09 .58 .07 Severity .42 .29 .37 .27 .47 .05 Insomnia .45 .04 .32 .11 .21 -.O4 Somatic .58 .17 .40 .19 .45 .05 E Scale .43 .22 .13 .12 .37 .17 Distinct .51 .04 .28 .13 .33 .01 Length .32 -.23 .Ol .34 .14 —.20 Irrate .30 .33 .31 .10 .20 .21 Precip -.04 —.18 -.O7 .14 -.17 -.05 Suicide .26 .16 .08 -.O7 .37 -.03 96 APPENDIX F. CORRELATION MATRIX FOR A_PRIORI SCALES (CON'T.). Psymotor Response Obsess Reproach Severity Insomnia Anxiety .62 .30 .56 .61 .44 .41 Crying .19 .15 .30 .29 .21 .35 Concen .59 .51 .52 .45 .55 .33 Schizo .42 .43 .31 .43 .33 .26 Eating .37 .23 .39 .31 .31 .43 Impulse .55 .46 .49 .58 .46 .18 Reactive .53 .38 .35 .55 .69 .19 Interest .56 .49 .40 .43 .42 .45 MEmory .26 .21 .34 .35 .29 .04 Diurnal .51 .31 .43 .41 .37 .32 Onset .14 .09 .21 .09 .27 .11 Hopelessness .36 .50 .32 .59 .47 .21 History .06 .12 .16 .07 .05 -.O4 Psymotor 1.00 .44 .46 .60 .48 .37 Response .44 1.00 .40 .49 .56 .33 Obsess .46 .40 1.00 .52 .56 .48 Reproach .59 .49 .52 1.00 .60 .28 Severity .48 .56 -.56 .60 1.00 .36 Insomnia .37 .33 .48 .28 .36 1.00 Somatic .55 .47 .44 .50 .50 .47 E Scale .32 .38 .26 .38 .33 .37 Distinct .43 .23 .40 .31 .38 .36 Length .13 .09 .05 .01 .05 .21 Irrate .35 .32 .37 .42 .42 .18 Precip -.11 -.31 -.O3 -.18 -.28 -.01 Suicide .15 .20 .15 .26 .23 .27 97 APPENDIX F. CORRELATION MATRIX FOR A PRIORI SCALES (CON'T.) Somatic E Scale Distinct Length Irrate Precip Suicide Anxiety .41 .11 .32 .07 .42 -.06 .09 Crying .42 .37 .31 .02 .16 -.O4 .21 Concen .41 .15 .32 -.Ol .36 -.16 .03 Schizo .44 .44 .23 .18 .14 -.13 .35 Eating .40 .34 .45 .31 .09 .13 .24 Impulse .38 .18 .40 .23 .32 -.O6 .09 Reactive .42 .29 .48 .06 .34 -.26 .21 Interest .58 .44 .51 .32 .30 -.O4 .26 Manory .17 .22 .04 -.23 .33 -.18 .12 Diurnal .40 .13 .28 .Ol .31 -.O7 .08 Onset .19 .02 .13 .34 .10 -.14 -.O7 Hopelessness .45 .37 .33 .14 .20 -.17 .37 History .05 .17 .Ol -.20 .21 -.05 -.O3 Psymotor .55 .32 .43 .13 .38 -.11 .15 Response .47 .38 .23 .09 .32 -.31 .20 Obsess .44 .26 .40 .05 .37 -.O3 .15 Reproach .50 .37 .31 .Ol .42 -.18 .26 Severity .50 .33 .38 .05 .42 -.28 .23 Insomnia .47 .37 .36 .21 .18 .Ol .27 Somatic 1.00 .47 .40 .25 .29 -.15 .26 E Scale .47 1.00 .32 .08 .14 -.O7 .59 Distinct .40 .32 1.00 .33 .19 .02 .16 Length .25 .08 .33 1.00 -.03 .13 -.01 Irrate .29 .14 .19 -.03 1.00 -.31 .07 Precip —.15 8.07 .02 .13 -.31 1.00 .07 Suicide .26 .59 .16 -.01 .07 .07 1.00 APPENDIX G 98 Appendix G Factor Items Deleted Item Number 79 Suicide Ideation/Attempts .861 .857 153 Agitation .803 .802 125 Agitation .822 .801 114 Dependency .819 .817 77 Interpersonal Dissatisfaction .832 .831 25 Memory Impairment .601 .425 71 Memory Impairment .616 .425 151 Loss of Energy .722 .70 5 History .729 .693 APPENDIX H 99 APPENDIX E. CORRELATION MATRIX FOR.EMPIRICAL FACTORS. Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 6 Factor 1 1.00 .67 .29 .52 .34 .53 Factor 2 .67 1.00 .22 .47 .26 .55 Factor 3 .29 .22 1.00 .17 .29 .26 Factor 4 .52 .47 .16 1.00 .42 .46 Factor 5 .34 .26 .29 .42 1.00 .25- ictor 6 .63 .55 .26 .46 .25 1.00 Factor 7 .58 .56 .13 .32 .11 .55 Factor 8 .37 .38 .11 .21 -.O4 .25 Factor 9 .25 .31 .29 .32 .25 .23 Factot 10 .41 .46 .27 .46 £38 .51 Factor 11 .36 .37 .39 .25 .14 .31 Factor 12 .34 .12 .10 .16 .26 .07 Factor 13 .48 .42 .15 .16 .07 .23 Factor 14 .14 .16 .06 .06 -.05 .11 Factor 15 .31 .16 .15 .28 .15 .34 100 APPENDIX B. CORRELATION MATRIX FOR EMPIRICAL FACTORS (cont.) Factor 7 Factor 8 Factor 9 Factor 10 Factor 11 Factor 12 Factor 1 .58 .36 .25 .41 .36 .34 Factor 2 .53 .37 .31 .46 .37 .12 Factor 3 .13 .11 .29 .27 .39 .33 Factor 4 .32 .21 .32 .46 .25 .10 Factor 5 .11 -.O4 .25 .38 .14 .16 Factor 6 .55 .25 .23 .51 .31 .26 Factor 7 1.00 .29 .14 .38 .31 .07 Factor 8 .29 1.00 .27 .16 .13 .02 Factor 9 .14 .27 1.00 .37 .22 .05 Factor 10 .38 .16 .37 1.00 .32 .34 Factor 11 .31 .13 .22 .32 1.00 .23 Factor 12 .07 .02 .04 .34 .23 1.00 Factor 13 .36 .25 .05 .29 .27 .19 Factor 14 .24 . 23 .06 .04 . 25 .10 Factor 15 .12 .03 .29 .21 .29 .17 101 APPENDIX B. CORRELATION MATRIX FOR EMPIRICAL FACTORS (cont.) Factor 13 Factor 14 Factor 15 Factor 1 .48 .14 .14 Factor 2 .42 .16 .16 Factor 3 .15 .06 .15 Factor 4 .16 .06 .28 Factor 5 .07 .05 .15 Factor 6 .23 .11 .34 Factor 7 .36 .24 .12 Factor 8 .25 .23 .03 Factor 9 .05 .06 .29 Factor 10 .29 .04 .21 Factor 11 .27 .25 .29 Factor 12 .19 .10 .17 Factor 13 1.00 -.01 .14 Factor 14 -.01 1.00 .03 Factor 15 .14 .03 1.00 BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Akiskal, H.S., Bitar, A.H., Puzantin, V.R., et. al. The nosological status of neurotic depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1978, 35, 756-766. Andreasen, N.C., and Grove, W.M. The classification of depression: traditional versus mathematical approaches. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1982, 139, 45-52. Bielski, R.J., and Friedel, R.O. Prediction of tricyclic antidepres- sant response. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1976, 33, 1479-1489. Carroll, B.J. The dexamethasone supression test for melancholia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1982, 140, 292-304. Carroll, B.J., Feinberg, M., and Greden, J.R., et. al. A specific laboratory test for the diagnosis of melancholia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1981, 38, 15-22. Crowne, D.P., and Marlowe, D. The approval motive: studies in evaluative dependence. New York: John Wiley, 1964. Daly, R.J., and Cochran, D.M. Affective disorder taxonomies in middle-aged females. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1970, 117, 257-266. Endicott, J., and spitzer, R. A diagnostic interview: the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1978, 35, 837-844. Eysenck, H.J. The classification of depressive illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1970, 117, 241-250. Feinberg, M., and Carroll, B.J. Separation of unipolar endogenous depression from non-endogenous depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1982, 384-391. Feinberg, M., Carroll, B.J., Smouse, P.E., and Rawson, S.G. The Carroll rating scale for depression: III. Comparison with other instruments. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1981, 138, 205-209. Freud, S. Mourning and melancholia. In Collected Papers. London: Hogarth Press, 1956. 102 103 Garney, M.P., Roth, M., and Garside, R.F. Diagnosis of depressive syndromes and the prediction of ECT response. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1965, 111, 659-674. Garside, R.F., Kay, D.K., Hilson, I.C., Deraton, I.D., and Roth, M. Depressive syndromes and the classification of patients. Psychological Medicine, 1971, 1, 333-338. Gibbon, M., McDonald-Scott, P., and Endicott, JJ. Mastering the art of research interviewing. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1981, 38, 1259-1262. Gillespie, R.D. The clinical differentiation of types of depression. prs Hospital Reports, 1929, 79, 306-344. Gurney, C., Roth, M., Kerr, T.A., and Schapira, K. The bearing of treatment on the classification of affective disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1970, 117, 251-255. Hamilton M., and Whie, J.M. Clinical syndrome in depression states. Journal of Mental Science, 1959, 105, 985-997. Holmes, G., Smithson, S., Shafer, C., and Bielski, 8. Teaching students to diagnose depression: a pilot study of research diagnostic criteria. Professional Psychology: Theory and Practice, 1983, in press. , 1, Hughes, J.R., O'Hara, M.W., and Rehm, C.P. Measurement of depression in clinical trials: an overview. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 1982, 43, 85-88. Kay, D.K., Garside, R.F., Beamish, P., and Roy, J.R. endogenous and neurotic syndromes of depression: a factor analytic study of 104 cases. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1969, 115, 377-388. Keller, M.B., Klerman, G.L., Lavori, P.W., Fawcett, J.A., Coryell, W., and Endicott, J. Treatment received by depressed patients. Journal of the American Medical Association, 1982, 248, 2848-2855. Kendell, R.F. The classification of depression: a review of contemporary confusion. British Journal of psychiatry, 1976, 129, 15-28. Kendell, R.F. The classification of depressive illness. London, Hogarth Press, 1968. Kendell, R.E., and Gourlay, J. The clinical distinction between psychotic and neurotic depressions. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1970, 117, 257-266. Kiloh, L.G., Andrews, G., Wilson, M., and Biandin, G.N. The relationship of the syndromes called endogenous and neurotic depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1972, 121, 183-196. 104 Kiloh, L.G., and Garside, R.F. The independence of neurotic depression and endogenous depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1963, 109, 415-463. Klein, D.F. Endogenomorphic depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1974, 31, 447-454. Klenman, G.L. Clinical research 'hi depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1972, 24, 305-319. Klenman, G.L., Endicott, J., Spitzer, R., Hirschfield, R.A. Neurotic depressions: a systematic analysis of multiple criteria and meanings. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1979, 136, 57-61. Kraepelin, E. Manic Depressive Insanity and Paranoia. Edinburgh: E and S Livingston LTD, 1921. Lewinsohn, P.M., Zeiss, A.H., Zeiss, R.A., and Haller, R. Endogeneity and reactivity as orthogonal dimensions in depression. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 1977, 164, 327-332. Lewis, A.J. States of depression: their clinical and aetiological differentiation. British Medical Journal, 1938, 2, 875-878. MacFaden, H.W. The classification of depressive disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1963, 109, 415-463. Matussek, P., and Luks, 0. Themes of endogenous and non-endogenous depressions. Psychiatry Research, 1981, S, 235-242. Matussek, P., Soldner, M., and Nagel, 0. Identification of the endogenous depressive syndrome based. on the symptoms and characteristics of the course. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1981, 138, 361-372. McConaghy, N., Joffe, A.D., and Murphy, 8. The independence of neurotic and endogenous depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1967, 113, 749-784. Nelson, J.C., and Charney, 0.8. Primary affective disorder criteria and the endogenous-reactive distinction. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1980, 37, 787-793. Nelson, J.C., and Charney, 0.5. The symptoms of major depressive illness. American Journal of Psychiatry,.l978, 35, 773-782. Overall, J.E., Hollister, L.E., Johnson, M., et. Ial. Nosology of depression and differential response to drugs. Journal of the American Medical Association, 1966, 195, 946-948. Prusoff, B.A., Klerman, G.L., and Paykel, E.S. Concordance between clinical assessments and patients' self report in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1972, 26, 546-552. 105 Raskin, A., Schulterbrandt, J.G., Reatig, N., and McKeon, J.J. Differential response to chlorpromazine, imiprimine, and placebo. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1970, 23, 164-173. '2 .1 Rhem, L.P. Assessment of Depression. In M. Hersen and A. Bellack (Eds.), Behavioral assessment: a practical handbook. New York: PergamonFPress, 1976. Roth, M. Depressive states and their borderlands: classification, diagnosis, and treatment. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 1960, 1, 135-155. Rosenthal, S.H., and Gudeman, J.E. The endogenous depressive pattern. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1967, 16, 241-249. Rosenthal, S.H., and Klerman, G.L. Content and consistency in the endogenous depressive patterns. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1966, 112, 471-484. Spitzer, R., Endicott, J. and Robins, E. Research diagnostic criteria: rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1978, 35, 773-782. Weckowicz, T.E., Cropley, A.J., and Muir, W. An attempt to replicate the results of a factor analytic study in depressed patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1971,27, 30-31. I J. «Ilfllnl . Win”...