ImamIltylwrwwlnlwrruummwum 6442 IH t-i :. * Michigan S” University ' LI 5-.ARY This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE EFFECT OF A PARENT EDUCATION PROGRAM FOCUSING ON PLAY ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF PRESCHOOL NEUROLOGICALLY IMPAIRED CHILDREN presented by Dawn Marie We lch has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Masters degreeinHealth’ Physical Education and Recreation ZNW ’xé’ V ajor professor Date .August_8,_;t918_ 0-7639 THE EFFECT OF A PARENT EDUCATION PROGRAM FOCUSING ON PLAY ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF PRESCHOOL NEUROLOGICALLY IMPAIRED CHILDREN By Dawn Marie Welch A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Health, Physical Education and Recreation 1978 ABSTRACT THE EFFECT OF A PARENT EDUCATION PROGRAM FOCUSING ON PLAY ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF PRESCHOOL NEUROLOGICALLY IMPAIRED CHILDREN BY Dawn Marie Welch The purpose of this study was to determine the effect of a parent education program on the development of preschool neurologically impaired children. The experimental program focused on parental attitudes toward play, the value of play, the importance of play to physically impaired children, and adaptations of play activities and modifications of play equipment for physically impaired children. The primary design was a quasi-experimental, pretest- posttest model. Eighteen subjects were selected to participate in the eight week program from three physical therapy programs in southern Michigan. They were matched into pairs based on developmental quotients from pretest data collected on a comprehensive developmental exam. All subjects participated in a therapeutic play group; parents of the experimental subjects participated in the parent education program. The Wilcoxon Sign Test for Matched Pairs demonstrated a significant difference between the experimental and control groups in gross motor and social-cognitive development. No significant difference was noted in fine motor, expressive language, and reflexive development. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study was carried out in conjunction with a companion study on therapeutic play. Working with Joanne FitzGerald made this project easier and more enjoyable. I appreciate especially the encouragement and assistance from my major advisor, Dr. Marcia Carter, and from my committee members, Dr. Sharon Menkveld, and Dr. William Heusner. Without Dr. Menkveld's enthusiasm about play oppor- tunities for handicapped children and Dr. Heusner's statistical assistance this thesis could not have been completed. The Michigan Easter Seal Society deserves special thanks for without their financial support this project might never have been completed. I would like also to express my appreciation to the volunteers and to the staff of the facilities who so willingly participated in the play and parent education group. Many thanks also go to the families who so willingly participated in this study and to my many friends who helped me to remember how important play is to all of us. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv LIST OF FfGUREs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v CHAPTER I The Problem. . . . . . . . . . 1 Need for the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Purpose of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 Research Hypothesis. . . . . . . . . . . . 6 Antecedent Problems. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 Research Plan. . . . . . . . . . 6 Rationale for the Research Plan. . . 9 Assumptions Related to the Research Plan . . . 11 Limitations of the Research Plan . . . . . . . 12 Definitions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 CHAPTER II Review of Literature . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 Definitions of Play. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 Theories of Play . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 The value Of Play. 0 O O O O O O O O O O 0 25 The Value of Play to the Physically Handicapped O O C O O O O O O C O O O O O O 27 Parental Influence on Child Development. . . . 29 Parent Education Programs. . . . . . . . . . . 32 Therapeutic Play Programs. . . . . . . . . . . 35 Program Design . . . . . . . . . .'. . . . . . 37 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 CHAPTER III Methods and Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 Description of Population and Sample . . . . . 39 The Independent Variable . . . . . . . . . . 42 The Control and Evaluation of Extraneous Factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 Test Procedures. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 Conduct of Treatments. . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 The Dependent Variables . . . . . . . . . . . 47 Instrumentation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 The Pilot Study. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50 Data Collection. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 Data Analysis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 Chapter IV Page Analysis and Results . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 Test Results for EPCDEC and DDST. . . The Vulpe Play Assessment . . . . . . . . . . 60 Analysis and Results of Anecdotal Data . . . 61 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64 Chapter V Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 Implications for Practice . . . . . . . . . . 68 Implications for Further Research . . . . . . 69 APPENDICES Appendix A - Play Program Goals and Objectives 71 Appendix B - Parent Program-Goals and Objectives . . . . . . . . . . 79 Appendix C - The Denver Developmental Screening Test . . . . . . . . . 81 Appendix C - The El Paso Comprehensive Developmental Evaluation Ct. . . 82 Appendix D - Objective Evaluation Form . . . 87 Appendix D - Pilot Project Evaluation Form. . 88 Appendix E - Demographic Data Form . . . . . 89 Appendix F - Parent Education Anecdotal Data Form. . . . . . . . . . . . 90 Appendix G — Volunteer Training Program . . . 91 BIBLIOGRAPHY 93 iii TABLE LIST OF TABLES Pretest, Posttest, Gain Scores for EPCDEC/ DDST for Five Developmental Categories (in weeks) Wilcoxon Sign Test for Matched Pairs on Five Developmental Categories Play Activities Attempted and Equipment Modified by Parents at Home iv PAGE 55 S7 62 LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE 3-1 Number of Subjects by Location and Group 40 3-2 Correlation Between Test Items 49 CHAPTER I THE PROBLEM The theoretical study of play in children with physically handicapping conditions has been an area devoid of empirical research. Developmental psychologists, early childhood educators, and recreators contend that play is essential to the social, emotional, cognitive, and physical development of children (Piaget, 1954, Murphey, 1972, Ellis, 1973, and Sponseller, 1974). Along with an increasing emphasis on leisure in the world today, there is an increasing emphasis on play as a subset of behaviors which are not separate from other behaviors and therefore are subject to behavioral methods of research (Herron, and Sutton-Smith, 1971). Most of the behavioral research on play has been with normal children as subjects. However, several empirical studies have been devised to study play behavior of mentally retarded children (Ross, 1970, Cleland, Swartz, and Chasey, 1971) and therapeutic intervention has been initiated based on the results of these studies. Michelman (1974) develops a case for the inclusion of play agendas into the programs of institutionalized multi-handicapped children based on observational research. Frequently the medical focus masks the daily living needs of the physically handicapped child. Because of en- forced immobility, sensory motor impairments, and time con- straints of the primary caregiver, and because the child is unable to function independently in play, basic play skills 1 2 often are not acquired (Michelman, 1974). Florey (1971) stresses that the significance of the everyday play of the child requires that professionals who work with children regard it as a respectable area of concern. "When a child cannot play, we should be as troubled as when he cannot eat or sleep" (Florey, 1971). Before play can take place, a child's basic needs for comfort, security, sleep and food have to be met (Maslow, 1970, Braga, 1974). The parents of a child who does not have dev- elopmental problems are required to spend comparatively little time in meeting those basic needs and thus may have time to play with the child if they so desire. On the other hand, the parents of a child with neurological impairment may have to spend a considerable amount of time meeting basic needs. Often spasticity, abnormal reflex activity, and muscle contractures or bony deformities make it difficult to provide constant comfort. Feeding difficulties due to pathological reflexes, poor muscle tone, and coordination make mealtime a traumatic, time consuming event. A large percentage of these children have hyperirritability and hypersenstitivity to auditory and tactile stimuli. These difficulties make bedtime and sleeping difficult for the child, and time consuming for the family. Time and energy for play are likely to be minimized. Factors which are observed to affect the play behaviors of physically impaired children include limited mobility to explore and initiate play activities (Diamond, 1971), de- creased sensory imput which decreases the childs ability to 3 learn about his environment through play (Marx, 1973), and the increased amount of time spent on therapy and caring for the physically handicapped child (Witengier, 1970). Parental feelings of guilt about bearing a child with a problem (Freeman, 1967) and their feelings of inadequacy re- lated to handling and caring for the child frequently prevent meaningful parent—child interaction. Parents often are observ- ed by this author holding their child stiffly without cuddling or rocking. It appears that they are reluctant to toss the child around and tickle him as they would if he did not have a physical problem. If the parents of the neurologically impaired child are not aware of the need for play and/or are apprehensive about engaging in play for fear it will cause physical pain or increased spasticity, the time for play and the quality of play can further be minimized. The family has long been recognized as the dominant factor in the socialization and education of the child (LeMasters, 1970, Brazelton, 1969). Recent evidence suggests that the first five years of life constitute the most critical period for the total development of the child (Leach, 1977). Because of the significance of the early years, parental know- ledge about the physical, social, emotional, and intellectual needs of children is important for the successful integration of these little persons into society. Effective parenting re- quires an understanding of childrens' needs at all stages of their development. The ways in which these needs are met, or are not met, influence a child's overall functioning (Braga, 1974). 4 The literature reveals that programs have been developed for parents of preschool mentally retarded children (Ross, 1970, Gross and Gross, 1977) and for parents of preschool disadvantaged children (Collard, 1972, Gross and Gross, 1977). Most of these programs have had as their focus improvement of the childs' intellectual functioning and preparation for school. Play as program content has not been researched extensively for neither parent nor play programs. Play is vital to the social, emotional, physical, and cognitive development of the preschool child (Norbeck, 1971, Sutton-Smith and Sutton-Smith, 1974, and Buckland, 1971). Because parental intervention can enhance the play process, this writer contends that by providing an educational pro- gram focusing on play for parents of neurologically impaired children, the overall development of these children can be enhanced. NEED FOR THE STUDY The value of play in the development of the preschool neurologically impaired child is considered by this investiga- tor to be significant. There existsa need to ascertain the extent to which a parent education program focusing on play alters the developmental progression of preschool children with neurological impairments. In reviewing the physical therapy literature, little empirical or experiential research was located related to play or to parent education. Physical therapy curriculum plans routinely omit any discussion of play or a playful philosophy, 5 and the focus in pediatric therapy is on techniques of orthopedic and neurodevelopmental treatment. Physical thera- pists, as a consequence of this lack in their curriculums and literature, rarely incorporate play in their treatment programs. This writer has observed that parents of physically impaired children do not play spontaneously and developmental- ly with their children. Parents often depend on their child's physical therapist for directions concerning how to manage their child at home. Since most physical therapists are not oriented to play or a playful approach, play is not regarded as important. For these reasons, a parent education program which focuses on the value of play to the neurologically impaired child, play skills, and adaptations of play activities and equipment appears warranted. Literature on play in early childhood education, recreation, and psychology (Sponseller, 1974, Diamond, 1971) tends to be based on observation and not on research data. This investigation attempted to document the effect of an educational program, taught with play methods, about childs' play on the developmental level of preschool neurologically impaired children. Only through studies such as this, even though there were significant limitations, can further research be stimulated and the body of knowledge expanded. PURPOSE OF THE sryprg The purpose of this investigation was to determine the effect of a parent education program on the developmental progression of preschool, neurologically impaired children. 6 The parent education program focused on attitudes toward play, knowledge about children's play, the significance of play to development, and adaptations and modifications of play activi- ties and equipment for neurologically impaired young children. RESEARCH HYPOTHESIS The review of the literature has led the investigator to the following research hypothesis: The physical, social, and cognitive develop- ment of preschool neurologically impaired children whose parents participate in a play training program will be significantly greater than the physical, social, and cog- nitive development of the children whose parents do not participate in a play training ANTECEDENT PROBLEMS The following problems were recognized and dealt with before the study was initiated: 1. A scale was selected which yields an overall dev- elopmental age in weeks as well as scores in each of the pertinent areas (physical, social, and cognitive). 2. A specific play program which is appropriate for pre- school neurologically impaired children was developed and pilot tested. This task was undertaken in con- junction with another investigator who was conducting a companion study. RESEARCR‘PLAN This study utilized a quasieexperimental, pretest posttest research design with an available sample with subjects 7 assigned to control and experimental treatments. The inves- tigation was conducted in conjunction with a companion study whose purpose was to determine the effects of incorporating therapeutic play into the physical therapy treatment program of neurologically impaired preschool children. Twenty-seven children were selected from three physical therapy programs in Southern Michigan to be involved in the combined study. Three comparison groups were establised at all three centers. Group A: The control group for the companion study was composed of nine (9) subjects; 3 from each location. These subjects continued with their regularly scheduled physi— cal therapy treatments twice a week for 1 hour. Group B: This group was composed of nine (9) subjects; 3 from each location. They continued with their physical therapy once a week for one hour and participated in the experimental therapeutic play group once a week for one hour. This is the experimental group for the companion study and the control group for this study. Group C: The control group for this study was com- posed of the remaining, nine (9) subjects; three from each location. These subjects received physical therapy for one hour once a week, participated in the therapeutic play group for one hour a week, and in addition their parents were in- volved in a parent education program dealing with play and its importance for the neurologically impaired child. All subjects were pretested using the El Paso Com- prehensive Developmental Evaulation Chart (EPCDEC) or the 8 Denver Developmental Screening Test, and the Vulpe Play Assess- ment. Subjects were matched into pairs on the basis of their overall developmental quotients from pretest scores. The treatments were administered for eight weeks. The subjects for this study were selected from Roosevelt-McGrath School, Wayne, Michigan, Durant Tuuri Mott School, Flint, Michigan and Ingham Medical Center, Lansing, Michigan. All children were involved in physical therapy programs for neurologically impaired children at the time of the study. The Therapeutic Play and Parent Education programs were implemented within these facilities and the pretesting and posttesting were done during regularly scheduled physical therapy sessions at the respective treatment sites. Volunteers were selected from the Therapeutic Recreation and Social Work programs at Michigan State Univer- sity, East Lansing, Michigan to assist the principal investi- gators with the therapeutic play and parent education programs. They participated in a pretraining program for four hours prior to beginning the program. Employing a modified systems approach to program planning, goals and objectives were determined related to the value of play, the significance of play for the development of normal and impaired children, parental attitudes about play, and adaptations and modifications of play activities and equipment for neurologically impaired children. Eight learning sessions were designed utilizing an informal, playful approach, to meet these objectives. 9 Following the eight week play and parent education program, all children were posttested using the same comprehen— sive developmental scale(s). A pilot study was implemented to determine the appropriateness of the goals and objectives, and the relation- ship of the activities to the goals and objectives. Preschool children and their parents from an existing physical therapy program were used for the pilot study. Experts in therapeutic recreation and pediatrics were enlisted to observe and evaluate two play and two parent sessions. These experts also evaluated the objectives and activities for all sessions. Evaluation forms used are found in Appendix D. Goals, objectives, and activities for the therapeutic play program can be found in Appendix A, and for the parent education program in Appendix B. RATIONALE FOR THE RESEARCH PLAN A review of the literature on play shows that most research on the topic is observational. A study of develop- mental levels of impaired children following a play and parent education program is adaptable to an experimental-control group research design even though there are numerous limitations. This investigation may stimulate further research in the area using controlled methods of research. The population for this study was chosen because children with neurological impairments comprise a large segment of the preschool age group that is commonly treated by physical thera- pists. By limiting the subjects to those with neurological impairments rather than including children with orthOpedic 10 and peripheral nerve problems, the physical therapy treatment approach is somewhat circumscribed but will not be specifically defined for each child and each therapist. The eight week period for implementation of the play and parent education programs was chosen because it seemed to be a realistic period of time to maintain parental cooperation. A longer program, and therefore more time between the pretest and the posttest, might yield more definitive results. The three locations selected were all within a reasonable traveling distance for the principal investigators to administer the pre-and post-tests and to implement the programs. The physical therapists and administrators of these facilities were willing to have their clientele participate in the study and were willing to identify children who fulfilled the sample require- ments. The research design facilitated a comprehensive evalua- tion of the child. The El Paso Comprehensive Developmental Evaluation Chart (EPCDEC) was chosen as the primary scale to measure motor (gross and manipulative), reflexive, social, and cognitive development for subjects from one to three years. The Denver Developmental Screening Test (DDST) was chosen and items correlated within the same categories for subjects from three to five years. In addition, the Vulpe Play Assess- ment was administered to all children as it provided a more detailed description of the social-cognitive areas using de- velopmental play items. The data from the Vulpe Play Assessment was used to supplement the developmental examinations and was 11 therefore not analyzed statistically. These combined scales were found to give a comprehensive evalution of the childrens development. These scales were chosen because they can be given in a reasonable amount of time, are comprehensive, and can be administered by a physical therapist. A11 evaluation forms used for statistical analysis are found in Appendix C. ASSUMPTIONS RELATED TO THE RESEARCH PLAN The following assumptions were made in conjunction with the implementation of the study: 1. Physical therapy treatment has a positive effect on the development of preschool children with neurological impairments. 2. The developmental scale selected accurately measured the changes in motor, social, and cognitive development of neurologically impaired children. 3. A representative group of neurologically impaired children in the preschool age range will be available to the researcher because of the early identification clause of the Mandatory Special Education Act (Public Law 198, 1971). 4. Activities designed for the play and parent education treatments were assumed to be appropriate to meet the goals and objectives of the program. 5. The program activities in the therapeutic play and the parent education program were presented in a "fun" atmosphere, incorporating a playful approach by the volunteers and the group leaders. 12 The program is of sufficient duration that there was no pretest effect on the posttest. The items in the develop— mental examinations are nonspecific and can be evaluated in different ways through observation. Parents developed skills and knowledge about playing with their child from the training program. Parents transfered their skills and knowledge about playing to the act of playing with their child thereby increasing the quality and quantity of play time. LIMITATIONS OF THE RESEARCH PLAN the The following have been identified as limitations of proposed research plan: There was no way to prevent absence of children and/or parents, due to illness, changes in parents' work schedules, and other obligations. Bias of the researchers, volunteer aides, and associated facility personnel may have affected the results. The developmental scales were as objective as possible to decrease this bias. The inability to regulate the physical therapy program which was administered to the experimental and control groups may have affected the results. There was no way to control the amount and type of play within the physical therapy treatment and within the home environment. The amount and type of play in the home, the playfulness of the parents and other family members, and 13 the playfulness of the physical therapist involved in the child's treatment program are recognized as important factors but were not considered in the data collection or analysis. 5. Test reliability and validity of the developmental exam to be used has not been completely researched though work in this area has begun. 6. The relatively short duration of the play and parent education programs may have affected the results. 7. The sample size was kept small in order that the principal investigator and the principal investigator in a companion study could evaluate all children in the pretest and post- test and also implement the play and parent training programs. This decreases tester bias, but severely limits the generalizability of the study. The results are applicable only to children in southern Michigan as this is the area from which the available sample is drawn. 8. Cooperation of families, volunteers, and facility person- nel might not have been consistent. 9. Socio-economic factors, which are known to affect play behavior, cannot be controlled. DEFINITIONS Neurologically Impaired. Any central nervous system defect manifesting a motor disability can be classified as a neurological impairment. Examples of specific conditions which were included in this study under this definition are: 14 Cerebral Palsy, post encephalitis, post meningitis, hydro- cephalus, post traumatic head injuries and cerebral tumors. Conditions not dealt with in this study were: Downs Syndrome,peripheral nerve injuries, myelodysplasia, and benign congenital hypotonias. Preschool. Children between the ages of one year zero months and four years eleven months were designated as preschool for purposes of this study. Therapeutic Play. Play activities for neurologically impaired children, emphasizing self-initiated, pleasurable (fun) experiences which are oriented toward therapeutic goals; i.e. optimal positioning, use of involved extremities, normalization of muscle tone, and motor patterns, and facilitation of mobility constituted therapeutic play. Therapeutic Play Program. An eight week planned program of group play activities, which was designed to meet previously determined therapeutic goals. Play (for the neurologically impaired child). Behavioral characteristics which are self initiated, pleasurable and intrinsically motivated experiences, and which are receptive and/or expressive, constituted the entity of play. Playfulness. Lieberman defined playfulness as:"apersonal- ity trait characterized by quantity and quality of physical, social, and cognitive spontaneity, manifest joy, and sense of humor (Lieberman, 1966)". Physical Therapy (for the neurologically impaired child). An individualized, goal-oriented session was conducted twice a 15 week emphasizing inhibition of abnormal reflexes and muscle tone, facilitation of normal muscle tone, motor patterns, and developmental sequence, maintenance of range of motion, increase of strength and endurance, improvement of balance and coordination, and improvement of ambulation and gait pattern. Parent Education Program. An eight week program for parents focused on attitudes about play, knowledge about the value of play for neurologically impaired children, and adaptations and modifications of play activities and equipment. Physical Therapy for the Neurologically Impaired Child. An individualized, goal-oriented session was conducted twice a week, emphasizing inhibition of abnormal reflexes and muscle tone, facilitation of normal muscle tone, motor patterns and developmental sequence, maintenance of range of motion, increase of strength and endurance, improvement of balance and coordination, and improvement of ambulation and gait pattern. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Two major areas of literature are considered relevant for this review. Research on play and development is reviewed to examine the appropriateness of the content in planning an intervention program for young neurologically impaired children and their families. Literature on definitions, theories, and variables which constitute play, the value of play and how play affects the social, emotional, cognitive, and physical development of handicapped and non- handicapped children will be reviewed. Parent education literature is reviewed for effective methods of instruction and for applicability to the topic of play. Less extensively reviewed here but still of significance to the study are parental influence on child development, developmental problems associated with neurological impair- ments and their influence on play abilities of children, play in physical therapy, and literature that will support the intended method of study and the instruments selected. DEFINITIONS OF PLAY In the past decade there has been an increasing concern for play and leisure and a definite shift in attitudes away from the puritan work ethic (Ellis, 1972). Researchers are beginning to attack the basic questions of what play is, why people play, of what value is play and playfulness, and what constitutes the group of behaviors we call play. 16 17 Many psychologists, educators and, child development Specialists have spent much time and effort trying to define and delineate the exact nature of play. Huizinga (1949) dealt with the tendency to split categories into exclusive apposite sets by initially defining play as not-work. After extensive study he concluded a more important complementary pair would be play-earnest or play-seriousness; though he felt that seriousness seeks to exclude play, whereas play can very well include seriousness (Huizinga, 1949). Some authors have defined play behavior in terms of its cause or motive. Sapora and Mitchell (1961) defined play as "the aimless expenditure of exuberant energy." Late in the 19th century Groos defined play as "instructive practice, without serious intent, of activities that will later be essential to life" (Groos, 1898). In searching for a satisfactory definition within the literature, the question of whether play is intrinsically motivated or learned behavior inevitably arises (White, 1959, Florey, 1971). Both sides of the issue appear to be equally well documented. Play is thought to be innate behavior for all mammals, though man is seen as the supreme player (Ellis, 1972). Man exhibits a great variety of play behavior as well as very complex play behaviors. Furthermore, only man plays extensively beyond the juvenile period (Norbeck, 1971, Dolhinow, 1971, Morris, 1969, Hunt, 1965). On the opposite end of the continuum, play is viewed as a learned behavior. Play can be explained as learned behavior, 18 made in response to stimuli in the environment, that is not demonstrably critical for survival (Kimble, 1961, Ellis, 1973). Play includes an emotional element of pleasure. Play is spontaneous, is customarily regarded as nonutilitarian and nonproductive, and is in its outward form species Specific (Jackson and Angelino, 1974). Play allows for op- timum exploration of the unknown and is unpredictable in that each player is free to respond in his own way. Play implies freedom from imposed constraints and encourages creativity (Gunn, 1977). Sutton-Smith describes play as a very special type of "knowing", unique in character and having the characteristics of self-control, euphoric-tension, scale reduction, and novelty (1972). He defines stages similar to those explained by Piaget: sensory motor (infancy), symbolic (toddlerhood), and imaginative (childhood). These stages all deal with life in terms of models. Attempts at defining play are numerous with the tendency being to define play according to the particular purpose of the research (Takata, 1969). Frequently researchers incor- porate many definitions and theories or segments of theories to derive an operational definition which will meet their research needs. Several authors have attempted to define and delimit play by reviewing the literature only to conclude that play is undefinable (Millar, 1968, Ellis, 1973). "The gallant attempts to provide direct, comprehensive theories of play are inamnuate partly because they attempt to define and treat play as an activity with a common core and with characteris- 19 tics that distinguish it from all others" (Millar, 1968). Millar proposed that play be used as an adverb to describe how and under what conditions an action is performed. Lieberman (1966) attempted to differentiate between play and playfulness. Quantity and quality of physical, social, and cognitive spontaneity, manifest joy and sense of humor were rated, and Lieberman predicted that these were in fact the expression of one personality trait-- playfulness. Play is a universal human behavior. It is therefore presumably vital to human existance. Societies regard and handle play differently. Some provide it a place of honor and put it to social use. Other societies, notably our own in recent centuries, have held play in dishonor, a course of action that has borne positive results in monumental economic achievements, but at the same time has presented us with a chain of social problems." (Norbeck, 1971). The preceding statement is only one of many examples found in the literature on the history and philosophy of play which indicate that our society has minimized the value of play. Play is presented as trivial, worthless, and something to be done only when all work is done. This idealogy of play, described as "Scroogian" by Brian and Shirley Sutton-Smith (1974), is explained by stating that the industrial revolution which needed many workers had to force a predominantly rural European culture from its festive and seasonal way of life. The idea that work was supreme and play was evil helped to provide the industrial revolution with the human machinery it needed. The creati- vity and individuality which is felt to be necessary in our 20 modern society (Buckland, 1971) were not needed and in fact were discouraged during earlier eras. Several authors have described the official suppression of play in western cultures, particularly in Protestant countries followingthe Reformation (Illick, 1974, Aries, 1962). In contrast non- western societies, view play as "an outstanding, socially approved feature of life which holds a position of honor in religious observances (Norbeck, 1971). Evidence of the puritan work ethic still is obvious in our society today and tends to be transferred from one generation to the next (Gunn, 1977). In spite of this, however, people and animals continue to play from very early in their lives (Smilansky, 1968, Sutton-Smith, 1972). Play is first expressed in the mouth play of infants which is apparent after needs of feeding are met. Unlike exploratory activity, this play does not appear to be controlled by immediate stimulus events (Sutton-Smith, 1972). Neumann, in "The Elements of Play" (1971), analyzed the literature on play and concluded that all activities can be placed on a continuum from "work" to "play". Three criteria are defined which distinguish play: control must be internal, the player must suspend reality (to act " as if"), and the activity must be internally motivated (Sponseller, 1974). 21 THEORIES OF PLAY, At the 1970 White House Conference on Children, participants recognized that play behavior cannot occur until the basic needs such as food, health, warmth, and security have been met (Jackson and Angelino, 1974). Maslow formulated the "hierarchy of needs: as a construct through which we can view the growth of human beings toward psychological health and self-actualization (Maslow, 1970). The basic needs can be classified into five groupings: (1) physiological (2) safety (3) belongingness and love (4) esteem and (5) self-actualization. The significance of this formulation is not that it looks at human beings as inherently growth oriented. In the literature on play, nothing was found which attempted to place play into this hierarchy of needs. Elements of what we call play can be seen in all of the groupings beyond physiological and safety needs (Braga and Braga, 1974). This basic need concept has been expressed in terms of conditions inhibiting or facilitating play (Florey, 1971). The conditions which inhibit play (hunger, anxiety, and fear of pain) are manifestations of physiological and safety needs, which are lower on the hierarchy of basic needs. The conditions which Florey delineates as facilitating play (novelty, opportunity for exploration, imitation of competent role models) are higher on the hierarchy of basic needs, to be sought after physiological and safety needs have been met. Gunn suggests that following the crisis period of an illness or disability, after basic needs have been met, 22 the patient will be ready for play activites to meet the need for optimal arousal (1975). In order for the individual to meet his needs for optimal arousal the play activity must meet certain characteristics: self-regulation, increasingly complex skill acquisition, intrinsic rewards, creative and imaginary responses, and free of external constraints (Ellis, 1973). Many theories have been advanced in the past which attempt to explain why people play. The most common are the classical theories of surplus energy, instinct, recapitulation, preparation for life, and relaxation. The surplus energy theory maintains that play is caused by excess energy which must be expended. The instinctual theory assumes genetic inheritance: play behavior of children is natural and instinctive. The recapitulation theory states that through play the player recapitulates the history of the development of his own species. Groos's view of play as preparation is based on behavior as instinctive. The player is perfecting his instinctive skills so that when they become critical they will be effective. The relaxa- tion theory implies rest and recuperation from work (Ellis, 1973). The more recent theories of play are concerned with the actual form of the play behavior and attempt to explain play in terms of cause and effect. The best known of these are generalization (re-enactment of rewarding work experiences), compensation (satisfaction of needs not met through work), catharsis (expression of emotions in a 23 harmless way), psychoanalytic (repetition of unpleasant experiences or tendencies), developmental (play as caused by the growth of the childs intellect), and learning (the normal process which produces learning) theories (Ellis, 1973). Of these the developmental and learning theories seem to be most applicable to the present study and will therefore be discussed in greater detail. Developmentalists view play as having various stages, each based on learned behavior from the previous stage (Erikson, 1950, Piaget, 1962). Piagetian theory sees play as "the expression of one of the phases of progressive differentiation occuring when assimilation is dissociated from accommodation" (Piaget, 1962). Assimilation refers to the process by which the living creature internalizes the information it receives in terms of its previous habits and preferences as it makes this information part of its already existing knowledge. Accommodation refers to the adjustment the organism must make to reality in order to assimilate properly any unique or novel aspects which the environment may provide. Play is the direct result of the "primacy of assimilation" (Piaget, 1962). Piaget sees play as closely tied to the growth of intelligence and as a condition of pure assimilation (Flavell, 1963). Piaget proposed three types of games: practice games, symbolic games, and games with rules. These types of games correspond closely to his three stages of intellectual growth: the sensory-motor level, the preoperational 24 level and the operation level. Piaget also suggests criteria which distinguish play from non—play activities: Play as (1) an end in itself, (2) spontaneous, (3) pleasura- ble, (4) relatively lacking in organization, (5) marked by freedom from conflicts, and (6) as overmotivated (Flavell, 1963, Jackson and Angelino, 1974). Information seeking as knowledge seeking (epistemic) behavior also can have characteristics which are similar to play. Play as a subset of behaviors can be reduced to stimulus- response psychology, and play can be explained as merely learned behavior. Schlosberg (1947) described play in terms of stimulus response concepts of generalization, thresholds, and learning. In the past decade, learning theorists have begun to look at curiosity, arousal, and attention in learn- ing (Smilansky, 1968). Moffitt attempts to show that play is an important mode of sensory—motor, perceptual—motor, language and cognitive development (1972). Much of what the author describes as play would be called exploration or learning by others. In their recent book, The Power of Play, Caplan and Caplan attempt to substantiate their premise that the power of play is all pervasive. They view play as an entity which can aid physical growth, strengthen personality, encourage personal relations, further creativity and the joy of living, and advance learning (Caplan and Caplan, 1974). A recent integration of two existing theories appeared most promising as a guide for the practitioner as well as 25 the researcher. The first theory contends that quiescence is not the natural state of the organism, and for that reason the organism interacts with the environment. It postu- lates that an intermediate level of arousal is optimal. Play seems to be the behavior that comes from a need for stimula- tion and produces knowledge about the constantly changing environment (Ellis, 1973). For more than 100 years scholars have been formulating theories to explain why people play. Basic to all of these theories is the concept that play is valuable to animals and humans and therefore is a legitimate area of concern. THE VALUE OF PLAY In reviewing the literature regarding the value of play the following quotations are considered by this writer to be of significance: Through play that emerges in a normal, natural pattern, children discover who they are, how their bodies and minds work, and how they feel about themselves and others. Play is childrens response to life, if they are to be vital, creative, healthy individuals. If we can be- lieve that play is as necessary to our wholeness as work and that the two can mesh with each other, life will take on additional meaning and joy" (Evans, 1974). Play makes life more interesting. Play makes us enjoy being with each other a lot more. Playful people are more versatile. Versatile people are flexible and creative." (Sutton- Smith and Sutton-Smith, 1974) Childs play is the infantile form of human ability to deal with experience by creating model situations and to master reality by experiment and planning (Erikson, 1955)." 26 The sheer pleasure of playing is extremely important to learning because it encourages the child to explore. Here in play, we have two essentials of intellectual growth-- interest and experimentation" (Hartley, 1971). Play is the most dynamic childhood learning method. The young child plays from early morning until he goes to sleep at night. It is the most natural way for a child to use his capacities, to grow and learn skills" (Caplan and Caplan, 1974). Play habits of the young become a matter of critical importance in their preparation for adulthood. Those who play poorly when young will have inept dreams for the social futures in the societies in which they live" (Erikson, 1973). All of the above statements support the concept that play is of value to man, though each view is supported by a different body of knowledge. Many professionals from various disciplines are showing increased concern for the management of play to achieve goals which they judge to be worthy (Sutton-Smith, 1971). Some authors argue that if the activity is managed or manipulated by others it is no longer play (Gunn, 1977, Neumann, 1974). Most who are concerned with children, how- ever, agree with the following concepts: play is the main mode of learning in children (Hartley, 1971, Garvey, 1977, Arnold, 1955, Bruner, 1975); play can be successfully employed as a technique with emotionally disturbed children (Klein, 1960, Axline, 1947); play is the basis for creativity, versatility, and imagination (Curry, 1975, Lieberman, 1965, and Sutton-Smith, 1974); and play helps a person to perceive the reality of the world (Hartley, 1971). 27 THE VALUE OF PLAY TO THE PHYSICALLY HANDICAPPED CHILD The value of play to the physically handicapped child is less well documented than the value of play to man in general. Finnie devotes one chapter in her book Handling the Young Cerebral Palsied Child at Home (1975) to play. In it she states: Play is equally important for the Cerebral Palsy child. He, too, must become aware of himself, explore, and get to know his hands, feet, face, and so forth; learn about himself in relation to others, and understand how the world around him works. . . . Because of his difficulties in moving and balancing, in eye- hand coordination--and often with the addition- al handicap of defects in seeing and hearing-- he needs lots of help. . . . His handicap prevents him from learning through play in a natural way, so unless he has help and encour— agement, he will not be able to learn as he plays, or to reach his potential" (Finnie, 1975). In an article entitled "Play is Valid" (1968), Frank says of play and learning: With his sensory capacities, the child learns not only to look but to see, not only to hear but to listen, not only to touch but to feel and grasp what he handles. He tastes whatever he can get into his mouth. He begins to smell what he encounters. He can and will--if not handicapped, impaired, or blocked--master these many experiences through continual play. . . the most intensive and fruitful learning activity in his whole life cycle" (Frank, 1968). The deficits caused by play deprivation in children are clearly recognized (Bruner, 1966, Schults, 1965, White and Watts, 1973). Play deprivation has been studied in laboratory animals (Suomi and Harlow, 1976) with results showing that monkeys without playmates, to provide sensory and motor stimulation, develop socially disturbed 28 characteristics and never learn to play as adults. Play deprivation in handicapped persons has been described in relationship to the blind (Frampton, Kerney, and Schattner, 1969, Morris, 1974), to the mentally retarded (Ross, 1970, Cleland, Swartz, and Chasey, 1971, Horne and Philleo, 1976), and to the physically handicapped (Cliff, Gray, and Nymann, 1977, Witengier, 1970, Gralewicz, 1973, and Michelman, 1974). Through interviews with parents of multihandicapped children, Takata found evidence that the handicapped child lacks opportunity for successful play experiences (Takata, 1971). Lack of appropriate models for play is a major factor in play deprivation. Since the physically impaired child may be unable to function in play by himself, he fails to acquire skills even at the lowest level of his ability (Michelman, 1974). The Education For All Handicapped Children Act of 1975 (Public Law 94-142) mandates that educational agencies shall identify all unserved handicapped children and provide a free appropriate public education, integrating the handi- capped with the non-handicapped to the appropriate extent. Related services which are required to help a child benefit from special education are also mandated. Play, Michelman proposes, is a neglected, overlooked factor which has an important influence on learning and should be part of the special treatment the child receives (Michelman, 1974). Gralewicz completed a comparative study of multihandi- capped and non-handicapped preschool children based on the 29 hypotheses that the multihandicapped child plays less, spends more time playing alone, and has fewer play companions than the non-handicapped child (Gralewicz, 1973). The results supported all hypotheses except that the multihandicapped child spends more time playing alone. The time spent playing alone was found to be similar in both groups, but the multi- handicapped child had less playtime with others because of attendance in therapeutic programs and more time spent in his physical care. The author contended that the develop— mental gains from an effective therapy program may compensate for decreased playtime and that compensatory play programs can be developed once specific areas of deprivation are identified. PARENTAL INFLUENCE ON CHILD DEVELOPMENT Information sources which relate to the attempt to understand the significant influence parents have upon their children have varied widely. Supported by a heavy accumu- lation of evidence suggesting that the first years of life are instrumental in terms of cognitive development, Caldwell (1967) calls for supplementing the family environment with priming resources. Caldwell's discussion of priming resources is primarily in the cognitive domain and directed toward preparation of the child for formal education. Gordon in "The Beginnings of the Self: The Problem of the Nurturing Environment", stresses the importance of children's early years on the establishment of a sense of self that then serves as a filter for all future life 30 experiences (Braga and Braga, editors, 1974). The major premise of this article is that feelings about the self are influenced by the childs active experiences with the environ— ment and by the attitudes and models of significant others. Bayley (1964) indicated that the self-picture is fairly well integrated by the third year of life. The results of her study suggested that the mothers affectional‘ behavior toward her child in the first three years of life was related to the childs friendship, c00peration, and attentiveness at the time of school age. Concerning motor development, Seefeldt discusses the importance of "critical periods" in relationship to sensory stimulation. The author utilizes experiments on animals and humans to support the theory that the reduction of essential sensory stimulation results in delayed or inappropriate motor responses. Seefeldt supports provision of enriched environments by proposing that early movements, if repeated often enough, are incorporated into cell and phase assemblies within the central nervous system. This process facilitates improved future learning of more complex movements (Seefeldt, 1975). Baumrind (1967) studied child rearing practices associated with competence in the young child. A basic assumption from which this and other similar studies proceed is that physical, cognitive, and social development of young children is largely a function of child rearing practices. The child's energy level, his willingness to explore and 31 will to master his environment, and his self control, sociability, and buoyancy are set not only by genetic structure but by the stimulation and interaction provided by his parents (Baumrind, 1967). In developing Maternal Behavior Rating Scales, play and social interaction were used as criteria to indicate maternal awareness and quality of maternal interaction (Ainsworth, 1971). The mother who responds apprOpriately to her child does not overstimulate him by interacting in too intense, too vigorous, too prolonged, or too exciting a manner. She is also unlikely to understimulate because she picks up and responds to the signals of boredom which the baby gives. The quality of parent-child interaction was emphasized and the author contends that children of highly sensitive mothers follow more normal patterns of development (Ainsworth, 1971, 1973). Saunders and Keister (1973) found that very young children placed in day care settings actually lost ground in their physical and mental abilities. Children who had previously walked or talked were no longer able to walk or talk. This was related to the lack of stimulation provided by the environment. Brophy (1970) assertsthat it is not the amount of stimulation but the way the stimulation is organized in the home that is important. The amount of stimulation provided and therefore the childs achievement can be influenced by the mothers impression of herself (Strom and Greathouse, 1974) . 32 The three original Parent Child Development Centers funded by the Administration for Children, Youth, and Families (ACYF) have many unique features, but share the basic premise that in order to help parents learn and change, the stresses on the lives of parents must be alleviated (Gross and Gross, 1977). In these centers, information on growth and develop- ment is offered to the parents in conjunction with supportive and counseling services in a group setting. Father participation in playing and learning is emphasized by McDiarmid, Peterson, and Sutherland in their book on parent participation in child development (1975). They also stress making every learning situation enjoyable for the parent and the child and offer suggestions on how to do so. Parent participation in the early education of handicapped children is considered of such importance that the Bureau for the Education of the Handicapped, United StatesOffice of Education, requires that parent participation be built into proposals for many of its federal grants (Cohen, 1977). It is apparent from the literature search that parents have significant influence on the development of their children, both normal and handicapped. PARENT EDUCATION PROGRAMS The literature reveals that until recently educators involved in preschool programming have virtually ignored the critical influence of the home on child behavior and learning (O'Connell, 1975, Gordon, 1975). O'Connell suggests that a 33 systematic procedure for relaying developmental information to parents with children of all different ages and handicaps is needed. Programs have been initiated which provide developmental information to parents of children with develop- mental disabilities (Diamond, 1971, Flint and Deloach, 1975, and Morris, 1973) and disadvantaged families (Gross and Gross, 1977, Collard, 1972). Parent information and support groups have been reported to have varying degrees of success with parents of disabled children. In a study which investigated the effect of play pro- grams on preschool children with learning disabilities and multihandicapping conditions, it was concluded that early sensory and movement experiences are necessary to prepare children for learning. The results of the study led to the conclusion that parent/child sensory motor activities should be initiated shortly after birth and should be done on a continuing basis at home (Martin and Evans, 1972). Once the need for assistance to parents is determined, it is necessary to identify the most effective way that this assistance can be provided. Parent education programs have existed in the past with little research to objectively evaluate the effectiveness of methods (Pickerts and Fargo, 1971). Program goals were rarely specified and programs were continued if participants liked them and recommended their continuation. Dinkmeyer described the "C" parent group; the participants gollaborate, gonsult, glarify, gonfront, show goncern, maintain gonfidentiality, and are gommitted to ghange (Dinkmeyer, 1973). 34 The best known approaches to parent education are the mental health and learning focused approaches. The former, known also as the affective self—examination approach to parent education was evaluated by Brim (1965). The focus was on improving the mental health of parents and therefore improving the mental health and learning abilities of children. The results were inconclusive. The learning focused approach was suggested by Pickerts and Fargo (1971) as being adaptable to evaluation and successful because it has clearly defined goals which can be translated into specific behaviors or tasks that will indicate whether or not the parent understands his role as parent and its significance to the development of the child. In research on methods of parent-education groups, the question of discussion vs. lecture always arises. Hereford (1963) compared semi-structured discussion groups, with lecture control groups. The parents who attended the discussion groups did show significantly (.05) greater changes in their attitudes and behaviors than did the parents in the lecture groups. A comprehensive review of parent-education programs for low income families is offered by Chilman (1973). Parent education groups have not proven effective in making signifi- cant changes in knowledge and attitudes of parents in the low income populations. However, in studies of educational intervention with mothers of disadvantaged infants it was found that children made significant I.Q. gains in comparison 35 to a group whose mothers did not receive an education program. The cost of the programs and the extent of intervention (i.e. home training to allow for practice of skills, toy lending, etc.) were found to be significant factors in the provision of such programs. THERAPEUTIC PLAY PROGRAMS Results of a survey of therapeutic play services in childrens general hospitals in the United States showed that less than half of the hospitals surveyed provided any play services to hospitalized children. The study concluded that more emphasis is needed in the use of play to help children work through anxieties associated with surgery and in play sessions to promote physical therapy (Williams, 1970). "Just as early infant movement comes as the natural response to frequent physical handling by the mother and to multisensory stimulation, so the young child uses his body most vigorously in play with other children and in response to toys and activities that he enjoys (Marx, 1973)". This statement is the basis for a program implemented at the United Cerebral Palsy Center of Manhattan which integrates physical therapy into a preschool play/education program. Parent instruction in physical therapy/play activities is an integral part of this program. The approach utilized is group activities, with learning experiences planned for each individual based on objectives prepared for each child during the multidisciplinary staff conferences. Positioning children and stimulating movement are found to be effective 36 methods of incorporating physical therapy into the classroom. Self-care and perceptual training are integrated into the pro- gram and activities are carried out by the teachers. Compar- ison studies of this method have not been reported in the literature though the importance of such studies was stressed by the author (Marx, 1973). In England, play groups have become increasingly popular in conjunction with physical therapy and speech therapy (Leyland, 1976). The neurodevelopmental approach to treatment of young cerebral palsy children (Bobath and Bobath, 1969) originated in London, and is commonly used by therapists throughout England. Because it is a dynamic method which is incorporated into handling techniques with children, it is very adaptable to a play setting providing the ratio of therapists to children is high (Leyland, 1976). To date research has not been reported of comparison of play groups and individual treatment programs. A play center for developmentally handicapped infants and young children (three months to two and one half years) was founded early in the 1970's in southern California. The goals of ELic (Edward Levy Infant Center) were to provide a play center with stimulation in a loving and playful atmos- phere, and to provide emotional support for the parents, so often denied them by their extended families and the community (Diamond, 1971). Though this center serves as a model for other programs which are being developed, no controlled research methods are being used to determine its effectiveness. 37 It is difficult to determine from reviewing the literature whether there is a scarcity of preschool programs for handicapped with a play approach, or only a scarcity of authors who have chosen to report their findings. Nowhere in the literature was there a model which one might follow in the establishment of a play/physical therapy program. PROGRAM DESIGN Peterson (1971) seems to offer the most workable program planning procedures using the systems approach. In this model, the purpose of the program is defined and goals and objectives are specified prior to implementation of the program. These program goals are used as guidelines to develop behavioral-learning objectives, which are descriptions of the behavior expected after instruction. Procedures for reaching the objectives are then designed. An ongoing, formative evaluation plan is simultaneously developed (Peterson, 1974). Initially stating the objective, provides a workable rationale for selecting program activities to meet the objective and furnishes direction when selecting appropriate leadership techniques for conducting the activi- ties (Witt and Witt, 1975). Several developmental tests were reviewed during the literature search. Those reviewed extensively were: The Denver Developmental Screening Test, The Bayley Scales of Infant Development, The Cattell Infant Intelligence Scales, The Milani-Comparette Test for Motor and Reflex Development, The Preschool Attainment Record, The El Paso Comprehensive 38 Developmental Evaluation Chart, The Vulpe Assessment Battery, and the Wolanski Assessment (Frankenburg and Dodds, 1973, Krajicik, 1977, Milani-Comparetti and Gideoni, 1967, Cliff, et a1, 1975, Wolanski, 1973). Several play assessment scales were also reviewed to determine feasibility of inclusions in the study (Knox in Reilly, Ed., 1974, Sutton—Smith and Sutton- Smith, 1974, and Vulpe, 1977). SUMMARY Literature on play and deve10pment, the value of play to handicapped and non—handicapped children, and the importance of play programs was reviewed. Though the material evidence is minimal, an attempt was made to document the appropriateness of play as content in planning an intervention program for parents of neurologically impaired children. Parental influence on child development is well supported by studies in various disciplines; medicine, psychology and psychiatry, and education. Parent education groups as a means to provide information was explored to determine its effectiveness. CHAPTER III METHODS AND PROCEDURES The purpose of this investigation was to determine the effect of a parent education program on the developmental progression of preschool neurologically impaired children. The parent education program focused on attitudes toward play, knowledge about child's play, the significance of play to development, and adaptations and modifications of play activities, toys, and equipment for physically impaired young children. The program was administered to parents of children participating in a physical therapy playgroup program as a part of a companion study. DESCRIPTION OF POPULATION AND SAMPLE The study utilized a quasi-experimental, pretest-post- test control-group design with an-available sample. Twenty- seven preschool children, male and female, who were enrolled in physical therapy programs for neurologically impaired child- ren were pretested using a comprehensive developmental scale. All children had chronological ages between one year zero months and four years eleven months at the onset of the study. They were living at home with their parents or foster parents and were transported to a physical therapy department twice a week for individual treatments. Eighteen (18) children were participating in a companion study and were grouped as follows: Nine (9) children received physical therapy twice a week for one hour for the eight week duration of the study. Nine (9) 39 40 children received physical therapy once a week for one hour and participated in a therapeutic play group once a week for the eight weeks. Eighteen (18) children were participating in this study and were grouped as follows: The control group consisted of the nine (9) children receiving physical therapy and the therapeutic play group once a week for the eight weeks. The experimental group consisted of nine children who received physical therapy once a week, the therapeutic play group once a week, and in addition, their parents attended a parent education program on play for the eight week period. Three (3) children for each of the three groups were selected from each of three (3) locations within southern Michigan. Location Group Group Group A B C (Physical Therapy) (Physical Therapy (Physical Thera- And Play) py, Play And Parent Education) 1. 3 3 3 2. 3 3 3 3. 3 3 3 \fontrol experimentaL/ W Therapeutic \control experimentaL/ Play *’ 4‘ Parent Education Figure gfl.fo Hancoaao~o>ov ouofimaoo now u xavcuan< com cowma>uoqnm ouoHoo «\n .00» m uOOu H saw: nuomoua nouacwoooa + mofinoo no nooamaon mm {OH—“fl Adam ou>uo «was: weasuoau oaoauuoav human: .vouqu comedy: xuoan compass xuoan .wOu uouw umuhuuuu Hana uaocx no case .vouu m3o< .vaoo .naoo m ouuuuueH m unusuaau .vsmuo coo «Hovom «noaow opus: nuuao cacamm m>wuoouuuaa .uouwusn amazon“ bow «mash waumumam macaw abode wnoa new: odouaan non: ao nuance «o aownou a a: smash vmoun m manual nu manna muons excau mafia .nwuo: a mafia cu canoe: maosumaoa «soon cw «ousuuqa .ugu .Hmuwuuu> Hauwuua> c.uw q «vaunuo>o :ocwa: osocx :« madam oouauoua aoaoz a ooaoz nouuuwan acuouuam Hana nausea aaan spouse vocumuu even: ocean commune who: acouuoouav «amass «noose occuaau .uun H uOOu H uo~a0u on a moans: N heme moo: n\~ uaoaaom m we assay a no uoaow no exams so avocados mu sauunml manuaa «moan: anon oaaov mafiauunn so» upon Smog mauuon acuu evumsxoup «vanaxuan muawuucovu .nooeu new: .03 .H now: A 0» munwom anon: manna :«nuuu cease exam: exams anonuunn weanuoau who; N sumac: manna o «damn use“: «vuubuou avcwo accuse: no case 30m a non: N mocunaoo «via cannons: cm mo punch .wcavcoum .Haoa axons N Huaoom Hmaoom odosmcmq . 0>wumuv< acuot nauoz nuns» to>uuchou Iamcoouum o>unmwuaxm owwawcaa o>uuoasnwnox neuoz «cum nacho ououu an omnomm swan omnomm amen omaomm amen ounomm swan med swan oz< umaumm "mXMHH Emmy zmmzamm onHwxmamom P m>NuNsmouuamfioom mwmswama m>wmmmuexm uouoz scam Nouoz mmouu Hmna\omnomm Mom mmmoom zHmn H>Hm mom .Hmmeemom .emwemmm H.¢ mgm¢H 56 The raw data found in Table 4.1 was subsequently subjected to analysis using the Wilcoxon Sign Test for Matched Pairs. Following the collection of all data, gain scores for all subjects were calculated. Differences in gain scores were computed and ranked without respect to sign, the smallest difference being ranked as one. Signs were then reattached from the original values. The sum of the ranks of positive differences (T+) and the sum of the ranks of negative dif- ferences (T-) were then computed. Referring to the Distribu- tion of the Sign-Ranked Statistic T for alphaat .l determina- tion of significance was made. Table 4.2 shows the applica- tion of the Wilcoxon Sign Test for Matched Pairs to the data. N. enaNe .eamoNeNeNNm ANNmoNumNueum es I + N I I Am m e o m Hummus huowoumo m>Nmemom + you N I IN I We m.s u we NN n we m.N u +e Nuns o u H «end I H m.NH I H w n H eem.ma n I9 I I o «I e «NI o+ o w¢+ m+ m ¢+ 0| 0 ONI o N+ N e+ mI m NmI m.MI m.m NHI 0+ 0 om+ n.NI m.N NI m I I o m+ m oH+ m.m+ m.m NH+ ¢+ e 0+ n.N+ m.N N+ c 7 I I 0 cl 0 qu ml m «HI ml m OMI HI N HI m 5 I I o NI N NHI m.NI m.H oI m.NI m.H NI mI m mHI N N+ N N+ H+ H N+ m.HI m.H cl m.HI m.H NI «I e ml H e e eII e (Ire C C C C C n n n n n e e e e e d r d r d e. d r d r e e e e e e e e e e n k :1 n k f n k f n k :1 n .k f 8 n :1 8 n f 8 n .t 8 n #1 8 n f .1 a i i a .1 i a I. i a i i a i as NR D S R D co R D S NR D S R nu ma: xm>wmemmm m>NuNcwm0INmNoom mwmswwmq m>NmmNprm acuo: wmfim Houoz mmouw swam Ac w +9 .NN I mmHmn m>Hh zo N.¢ mqmo goal a n III! .. I I I JL 1 r (1290.... I.. 35.5.0.1. 3.23 3 .. I..I I I I II .I .III II. I.. . .I III I 53%|. II J '1'- II II. 1 I II II. I I gufioh my: o~<3muqum¢I I... .2005 zOI 01.90: 82.3w M; thv 05 . _ 3 I 23).: I I 300% _ I. I I I I. :0...me . 2. a: W . 80230.. 25:0 I59 . zo quz205st 85.010. I . I I I I. III I . 1 .I I II I . I .III. I. . 3.00221 E830. . I .mimaom W I . IIIIIwMDUEI _ mmznz . .II II _ 9 II III. I III. I . IIIIIIII I I. ~$nhxoé wwIN.2mIVI0I0m~ . III II I. 0:9me I. 2.03: . m :5. 50a . (5.2 :0 <91... .. . .. 9 1e 23038.5... ._ omz m: IszDIr . IImouoz .zwmwta n Immzmnon _ 28> awouxhxuzan an»: I . .300 I I- ._ u...0m..m.IzOZ EEEIB I <010 g 23: «$5 8:0? n I51: ..0 <93 . . . . . _ . . _ . . . . _ . r . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . n .0 n I Emzosuoufionioa. p.95. 22:0. 92: o» ozMaowz0... 29.3.. m 26.33%... 5.3 55.1 3?.» I 95 ~02. I an . V 2358 I .520: 1. ”mm: . $25; 3535; 99.015 m not. H . I. .II I. gmzizogw . w 821: 3:6 1 229;) 52.250 5.; :3 21: $53 I.. l I I I I I I I V .I: mzotan _ MEI: 9.3.5.. 200% mum: . wa1tl1 Jul I L I III 1| 1| IL I I |nv I I IIIIII III I I IIII I I .I I I I mafiasI 02.1»05 IO 95L :I: 503330: wwhIw Cash—Z. u .I III C I II: L l. 1| II I I — I r — — _ - — — - .— LI h —I I P n I r - h - h > IF I I I I I I— . n I h I o «3 n :v w Sn n 1; z n... «a a 8 o. 2 D o. 2 2 n. a. : 2 o u s o a V n ma» DATE TUUHS DIRECTIONS BIRTHDATE IEOSP. NO. Try to get child to smile by smiling, talking or waving to him. Do not touch him. When child is playing with toy, pull it away from him. Pass if he resists. Child does not have to be able to tie shoes or button in the back. . Move yarn slowly in an are from one side to the other, about 6" above child's face. Pass if eyes follow 90° to midline. (Past midline; 180°) 5. Pass if child grasps rattle when it is touched to the backs or tips of fingers. 6. Pass if child continues to look where yarn disappeared or tries to see where it went. Yarn should be dropped quickly from sight from tester’s hand without arm movement. 7. Pass if child picks up raisin with any part of thumb and a finger. 8. Pass if child picks up raisin with the ends of thumb and index finger using an over hand FUJNH O approach. 9. Pass any en- 10. Which line is longer? 11. Pass any 12. Have child copy closed form. (Not bigger.) Turn crossing first. If failed, Fail continuous paper upside down and lines. demonstrate round motions. repeat. (3/3 or 5/6) When giving items 9, 11 and 12, do not name the forms. Do not demonstrate 9 and 11. 13. When scoring, each pair (2 arms, 2 legs, etc.) counts as one part. 1h. Point to picture and have child name it. (No credit is given for sounds only.) 15. Tell child to: Give block to Mommie; put block on table; put block on floor. Pass 2 of 3. (Do not help child by pointing, moving head or eyes.) 16. Ask child: What do you do when you are cold? ..hungry? ..tired? Pass 2 of 3. 17. Tell child to: Put block on table; under table; in front of chair, behind chair. Pass 3 of A. (Do not help—Ehild by pointing, movihg head or eyes.) 18. Ask child: If fire is hot, ice is ?; Mother is a woman, Dad is a ?; a horse is big, a mouse is 7. Pass 2 of 3. 19. Ask child: What is a ball? ..1ake? ..desk? ..house? ..banana? ..curtain? ..ceiling? ..hedge? ..pavement? Pass if defined in terms of use, shape, what it is made of or general category (such as banana is fruit, not just yellow). Pass 6 of 9. 20. Ask child: What is a spoon made of? ..a shoe made of? ..a door made of? (No other objects may be substituted.) Pass 3 of 3. 21. When placed on stomach, child lifts chest off table with support of forearms and/or hands. 22. When child is on back, grasp his hands and pull him to sitting. Pass if head does not hang 23. Child may use wall or rail only, not person. May not crawl. 2h. Child must throw ball overhand 3 feet to within arm's reach of tester. 25. Child must perform standing broad jump over width of test sheet. (8-1/2 inches) 26. Tell child to walk forward, macaw-«y heel within 1 inch of toe. Tester may demonstrate. Child must walk A consecutive steps, 2 out of 3 trials. 27. Bounce ball to child who should stand 3 feet away from tester. Child must catch ball with hands, not arms, 2 out of 3 trials. 28. Tell child to walk backward, «(DcQCOm toe within 1 inch of heel. Tester may demonstrate. Child must walk A consecutive steps, 2 out of 3 trials. DATE AND BEHAVIORAL OBSERVATIONS (how child feels at time of test, relation to tester, attention span, verbal behavior, self-confidence, etc,): 157. 10-70 Distributed as a service by Mead Johnson Laboratories back. APPENDIX 87 EVALUATION Please identify any objective(s) which are not stated clearly. Comments Please identify any objective(s) you feel does not relate to the goal statement. Comments Please identify any additional objective(s) which you feel would be necessary to achieve the purpose of the study. Please identify any sub-objective(s) which are not stated clearly. Comments Please identify any sub-objectflmis) you feel does not relate to its respective objective. Comments Please identify any additional sub-objective(s) which you feel would be necessary to meet its objective. Comments Please identify any activity(ies) you feel does not relate to its respective objective(s). Comments Please identify any additional activity(ies) which you feel would be necessary to meet its respective objective(s). Comments Are the sub-objectives and activities appropriate for preschool Cerebral Palsy children? 88 PILOT PROJECT EVALUATION THERAPEUTIC PLAY PROGRAM AND PARENT EDUCATION PROGRAM 0N PLAY As you observe the play/parent education pilot session, please comment on the following areas: 1. Organization (time structure, order of presentation, etc.) 2. Preparation 3. General Atmosphere 4. Leadership and Direction 5. Participant Interaction 6. Activities (therapeutic, fun, motivating, etc.) 7. Additional Comments APPENDIX E 89 Child's Name _fi Birthdate Siblings' Name Siblings' Ages Address Mother's Name Father's Name Mother's Birthdate Father's Birthdate Educational Level: Mother K - 8th grade K 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 High School 1 2 3 4 College 1 2 3 4 Post-grad Diagnosis of Child K - 8th grade High School College Post-grad Phone Treatment Services for Child (where and number of times per week) Additional programs your child participates in? Additional programs you are involved in related to your child Describe your child APPENDIX 90 ANECDOTAL DATA FORM PARENT EDUCATION ON PLAY NAME SESSION # DATE EVALUATOR Answer the following questions with brief descriptions of inter- actions observed during the parent group sessions. Use direct quotations whenever appropriate. Did parent(s) comment on any play activities tried or equip- ment or activities modified during the past week? Did they comment on anything learned in the past session(s) and/or their response to any of the information presented? Do parents appear comfortable (i.e. interact easily and Spontaneously) with each other? Describe situation. Do parents appear comfortable (i.e. interact easily and spontaneously) with their child (children)? Describe situation. Describe childs' initial response when parents re—enter the room after the play sessions. Describe parents response upon returning to their child. Comment on the playfulness of the mother Comment on the playfulness of the father Comment on the playfulness of the child in the play group, in the presence of parents Were there observable changes in the parents responses during the session? Describe ‘W—ti‘rvw .fi‘fi fi‘vv—fWfiVyfiv W‘V ..... . . v v v—w—v ‘Yfiiii‘fii““‘f‘ijr‘1iA‘j“I‘Yr—V—v-‘V ‘ ‘Vfi YVV'V' General Comments Vw‘Vw v APPENDIX C 91 VOLUNTEER TRAINING PROGRAM First Session I. II. III. Fingerpainting with Pudding Half of the volunteers will each be given the description of a child of preschool age, who has been diagnosed with a neurological impairment. The description will include the child's age, disability, personality characteristics, mental status, etc. They will then be requested to role play their assigned child. After a brief description of the Therapeutic Play session the rest of the volunteers will act as volunteers in a modified therapeutic play session. Roles will be reversed after fifteen minutes. Following the play session there will be a group discussion dealing with the participants' feelings during the session, the activity itself (pudding painting), and ways in which the activities can be therapeutic yet fun. Time: One hour Videotape on the therapeutic play program. Time: thirty minutes Policies and Procedures A. Time, place and transportation discussion B. Expectations of the volunteers 1. Versatility between parent and play group. 2. Assist group leaders in the preparation of the area and equipment. 3. Assist with the dressing of the child. 4. Each volunteer will be assigned to specific children. 5. Participate in free play with the children as they arrive. 6. Observe children and parents' reactions for recording after each session. 7. Encourage children to participate in the group. 8. Encourage children to interact, but do not allow them to hurt each other. 9. Assist in cleaning program area. Time: thirty minutes Second Session I. Roleplay Parents of Handicapped Children After a brief description of the parent education program on play, half of the volunteers will be given the description of a preschool neurologically impaired child, including the same characteristics as those given for II. III. 92 the play program. They will then be requested to role play the results of these children in a modified parent educa- tion session on play. The rest of the volunteers will be asked to act as volunteers in the session. Roles will be reversed after fifteen minutes. A group discussion will follow dealing with the participants' feelings and reactions, handicaps such as neurological impairments and parental attitudes towards these and play. Time: One hour Observation and Recording A segment of the Therapeutic Play videotape will be shown and the volunteers will be asked to record their observations. Discussion will follow concerning observa- tion and recording with emphasis on the problem of interpretation. Another segment of the video will then be shown and they will be asked to record their observa- tions again. Comparisons will then be made. Time: Forty-five minutes Handouts and Further Discussion and Questions A. An opportunity for the volunteers to ask any further questions or stimulate further discussion will be provided. B. Handouts 1. Schedule of both programs . Chapter on "Play" by Finnie . Development of Play . "How To Play With Your Baby" (Asconi) Time: Fifteen minutes 2 3 4 BIBLIOGRAPHY 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 93 BIBLIOGRAPHY Ainsworth, M. "Four Scales for Rating Maternal Behavior", Child Development, 42, 1971, pp 1057-1069. Ainsworth, M. "The Development of Infant-Mother Attach- ment:, in B. M. Caldwell and H. N. Ricciuti (Eds.), Review of Child Development Research, Vol. III, Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1973, pp. 76-84. Aries, P. Centuries of Childhood, Social History of Family Life, New York, KNOPFF, 1962. Arnold, A. Your Child's Play, New York, Essandess, 1955. Auerbach, A. Parents Learn Through Discussion, Principles and Practices of Parent Group Education, John Wiley and Sons, 1968. Axline, V. M. Play Therapy,_The Inner Dynamics of Childhood, Boston, Mass., Houghton—Mifflin Co., 1947. Baumrind, D. "Child Care Practices Anteceding Three Patterns of Preschool Behavior,“ Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75, 1967, pp. 43-88. Bayley, N. "Consistency of Maternal and Child Behaviors in the Kerkeley Growth Study:, VITA HUMANA, 1964, pp. 73-95. ‘ Bishop, D., and Chace, C. "Parental Conceptual Systems, home play environment and potential creativity in children," Journal of Exceptional Child Psychology, 12, 1971, pp. 318-338. Bobath, B. and Bobath, K. The Motor Deficit in Patients with Cerebral Palgy, Clinics in Developmental Medicine, #23, London, Heinimann Medical Books, Ltd., 1969. Braga, J. and Braga, L. Growing with Children, Englewood Cliffs, N. J., Prentice Hall, Inc., 1974. Brazelton, T. B., Infants and Mothers, New York, Dell Pub. Co., 1969. Brim, 0. Education for Child Rearing (2nd. edition), Philadelphia, Russell Sage Foundation, 1965. Brophy, J. E. "Mothers as Teachers of their own preschool children; The influence of socioeconomic status and task structure on teaching specificity, Child Developmegg, 41, 1970, pp. 79-94. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 94 Bruner, J. S. "The Cognitive Consequences of Early Sensory Deprivation, in The Disadvantaged Child; Boston, Mass., Houghton Mifflin, 1966, pp. 137-144. Buckland, C. Toward a Theory of Parent Education: A Comparative, Qualitative Analysis of Learning Models, (Doctoral Dissertation, University of California, 1971), Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 32/07-A, University Microfilms #72-02781. Caldwell, B. "What Is The Optimal Learning Environment For The Young Child?", American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 37, 1967, pp. 8-21. Callahan, S. Parentipg Principles and_Politics of Parenthood, Penguin Books, 1974. Caplan, F. and Caplan, T. The Power of Play, Garden City, N. Y., Anchor Press/Doubleday, 1974. Chilman, C. "Programs for Disadvantaged Parents" in Review of Child Development Researpp (Vol. III) Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1973. Clelland, C.C., Swartz, J.D. and Chasey, W.C. "The Role of Play Games and Toys in Recreational Programming for the Moderately and Profoundly Retarded", Therppeutic Recreation Journal, Fourth Quarter, 1971, pp—152-155 and 188. Cliff, 8., Gray, J. and Nymann, C. Mothers Can Help, The El Paso Rehabilitation Center, El Paso, Texas, Guynes Printing Co., 1974. Cohen, S. "Teacher Receptivity to the Concept of Parent Participation in the Education of Handicapped Children", Rehabilitation Literature, 38.5, May 1977. Collard, R. R. "Exploration and Play in Human Infants? Leisure Today, AAHPER, June 1972. Courtney, J. Davis, J. M. and Solomon, P. "Sensory Deprivation: The Role of Movement", Perceptual Motor Skills, 13, 1961, pp. 191-199. Curry, N. E. "Dramatic Play as a Curricular TooTL in Play as a Learning Medium, Sponseller, D. (ed., National Association for the Education of Young Children, Washington, D. C., 1974. Qprry, N. E., Arnaud, S. Play; The_Child Strives Toward \§21£:Realization, National Association for the Education ' bung i dren, Washington, D. C., 1975. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 95 Diamond, F. "A Play Center for Developmentally Handi- capped Infants", Children, Sept./Oct. 1971, pp. 174n178. Dinkmeyer, D. "The Parent "C" Group, Perspnnel apd Epidance Journal, 53,4, 1973,pp. 252—256. ' ‘fo Dolhinow, P. "At Play in the Fields, Natural Histogy ‘Magazine, Dec. 1971, pp. 66—71. Ellis, M. J. "Play: Practice and Research in the 1970's, AAHPER, Part I, June 1972. Ellis, M. J. Why People Play, Englewood Cliffs, M. J., Prentice Hall, 1973. Erikson, E. H. Childhood and Society, New York, Norton,1950. Erikson, E. H. In Identity: Youth and Crisis, AAHPER, June, 1972. Evans, M. W. "Play is Life Itself", Theory Into Practice, 13, 4, 1971, pp. 267—272. Finnie, N. ngdli g thefigounggperebral Palsigd Childfiat Home, N. Y. Duttoh—SunEise, Inc., 1975. Flavell, J. H. The Play Psychology of J. Piaget, Princeton, N. J., Van Nostraun-Rheinhold, 1963. Flint, W. and Deloach, C. "A Parent Involvement Program Model for Handicapped Children and Their Parents", Exceptional Children, 41, 8, May 1975, pp. 556-559. Florey, L. "An Approach to Play and Play Development", American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 25,6, 1971, pp. 275-286. Frampton, M., Kerney, E. and Schattner, R. Fopgotten Epildren, Boston, Porter Sargent, 1969. Frank, L. "Play is Valid", Childhood Education 44,7, 1968, pp. 433—440. Frankenburg, W. and Dodds, J. Dgnger Developmental Spreeping Test Mgpual, University of Colorado Medical Center, c 1969, 1970. Freeman, R. D. "Emotional Rhactions of Handicapped Children", Rehab. Litegatpre , 28,9, Sept. 1967,pp. 274-282. Garvey, c.\é1§z, Cambridge, Mass., Harvard Univ. Press, 1977. Gordon, I. "The Beginnings of the Self: the Problem of the Nurturing Environment", in GrowingfiWithTChilgrng Braga, J. and Braga, L., Englewood Cliffs, Prentice Hall, Inc.,1974. 46. 47. 48. 49 O 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 57. 58. 59. 60. 96 Gordon, I. "Stimulation Via Parent Education", Children, 16,2, 1969, pp. 57-59. Gralewicz, A. "Play Deprivation in Multihandicapped Children", AJOT, 27,2, 1973, pp. 70-72. Griswold, P. Play Together, Parents and Babies, U.C.P. of Central Indiana, Indianapolis, Indiana, March, 1972. Groos, K. The Play of Man (Translated, E. L. Baldwin), New York, Appleton, 1898. Gross, B. and Gross, R. "Parent-Child Development Centers, Creating Models for Parent Education", Children ngay, 6, Nov/Dec 1977, pp. 18-22. Gunn, S. L. "Play and the Fully Functioning Person", Rehabilitation Literature, 38,8, Aug. 1977, pp. 257—259. Gunn, S. L. "Play as Occupation: Implications for the Handicapped", AJOT, 29,4, April, 1975, pp. 222-225. Hartley, R. E. "Play is the Essential Ingredient", Childhood Education, 48,2, Nov. 1971, pp. 80-84. Hereford, C. Changinngarental Attitudes Through Group Discussion, Foundations for Mental Health, Austin, Texas, 1963. Herron, R. E. and Sutton-Smith, B. Child's Play, New York, John Wiley and Sons, Inc. 1971. Huizinga, J. Homo Ludens: A Study of the Play Element in Culture, New York, Routeledge and Kegan, 1949. Hunt, J. McV, "Intrinsic Motivation and its Role in Psychological Development in Playfi (Levine, ed.), Nebraska Symposium in Motivation, Lincoln, Nebraska, University of Nebraska Press, 1965. Illick, J. "Child-Rearing in Seventeenth Century England and America", in DeMause, L. (Ed.), The Histogy of Childhood, New York, Harper and Row, 1974, pp. 303-350. Jackson, D. W. and Angelina, H. R. "Play as Learning", Theory Into Practice, 13,4, pp. 317-322. Kimble, G. A., Hilgard and Marquis, Conditioning and Learning, 12th edition, New York, Appleton, Century- Crofts, 1961. 61. 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. .750 97 Klein, M. "The Psychoanalytic Play Technique", American Journal ongrthopsychiatry, 25, 1955, pp. 223-237. Knox, S. "A Play Scale" in Play AstxploratogyLearning, Reilly, ed. Beverly Hills, Sage Publications, 1974. Krajicek, M. and Tearney, A. eds. Detection of Develpp- mental Problems in Children, Baltimore: University Park Press, c 1977. Leach, P. Babyhood, Alfred A. Knopf, Inc. New York, 1977. LeMasters, E. E. Parents in Modern America, Homewood, Ill. the Dorsay Press, 1970. Leyland, S. J. "Special Play Groups in an Assessment Center, Nursing:Times". 72,2 Nov. 1976, pp. 1815-1817. Lieberman, J. N. "Playfulness: An Attempt to Concep- tualize a Quality of Play and of the Player", Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, New York, April, 1966. Lieberman, J. N. "Playfulness and Divergent Thinking. An Investigation of their Relationship at the Kindergarten Level", Journal of Genetic Psychology, 107, 1965, pp. 219-224. Madaras, L. Child's Play, Peace Press, Culver City, California, 1977. Martin, Sister M. M., Ovans, P. M. "Learning Games are Pathways to Cognizance for Young Handicapped Children in Therapeutic Recreation", Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 6, 4, Fourth Quarter, 1972, pp. 153-157, 171. Marx, M. "Integrating Physical Therapy Into a Cerebral Palsy Early Education Program", Physical Therapy, 53,5, May 1973, pp. 512-514. Maslow, A. Motivation and Personality, New York, Harper and Row, 1970. McDiarmid, N., Peterson, M. and Sutherland, J. Loving and Learning, New York, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1975. Michelman, S. 8., "Play and the Deficit Child", in Play as Explpratory Learpipg, Reilly, ed. Beverly Hills, Sage Publications, 1974. Milani-Comparetti, A. Gidoni, E. A. "Routine Developmen- ta1.Examination in Normal and Retarded Children",-Develo - mental Medicine and Child Nepgplogy, 9, 1967, pp. 631-638. fvf .1W_r.<fifisvv“—11 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. 89. 90. 98 Moffit, M. W. "Play as a Medium for Learning", Leisure Today, AAHPER, 1972. ’ Morgenstern, F. G. "Psychological Handicaps in the Play of Handiapped Children", Dgyelopmental Medicine and Child Neprology, 10, 1968, 115. ' Morris, A. G. "Parent Education, for Child Education Being Carried Out in a Pediatric Clinic Playroom", Clinical Pediatrics, April, 1973, pp. 235-239. Morris, D. The Human Zoo, New York, McGraw Hill, 1969. Morris, R. H. "Evaluation of a Play Environment for Blind Children" Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 8,4, fourth quarter, 1974, pp. 151-155. Murphey, L. B. "Infants Play and Cognitive Development", 13.3}31 and Development, Piers, M. ed., New York, Norton, 1972. Neuman, E. A. "The Elements of Play, Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Illinois, 1971, in Play as a Learning Medium, Sponseller, (Ed.), National Association for Education of Young Children, 1974. Norbeck, E. "Man at Play', National History Magazine, Dec. 1971, pp. 48-53. O'Connell, C- "The Challenge of Parent Education", Excgptional Children, 41,8, May 1975, pp. 554-556. Peterson, C. "Applications of Systems Analysis Procedures to Program Planning in Therapeutic Recreation Service", in Avedon, E. Therapeutic Recreation Service, Englewood Cliffs, Prentice Hall, Inc., 1974. Piaget, J. Play, Dreamsi and Imitation in Childhood, (translated by C. Gattengro, and F. Hodgson), New York, Norton, 1962. Pickarts, E. and Fargo, J. Parent Education, New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1971. Piers, M. Ed. Play and Development, New York, Norton, 1972. Ross, D. "Incidental Learning of Number Concepts in Small Groups", American Jpprpal ofoental Deficiency, 74, 1970, pp. 715-725. ’ ”' Sapora,.A. Y., and Mitchell, E. D. Thefiflfieory_prlay and Respeation, (3rd ed.), New York, Ronald Press, 1961. 91. 92. 93. 94. 95. 96. 97. 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 103. 1040 105. 99 Saunders, M. and Keister, M. "Family Day Care: Some Observations" Bethesda, Md., Leasco Information Products, Inc., Document, Ed. 072 856. Schlosberg, H. "The Concept of Play", Psychology Review, 54, 1947, pp 229—237. Schultz, D. D. Sensopy Restriction: Effects on Behavior, New York, Academic Press, 1965. Seefeldt, V. "Critical Learning Periods and Programs of Early Intervention", paper presented at the National Convention of the Alliance for Health, Physical Education and Recreation, Atlantic City, New Jersey, March 15, 1975. Smilansky, S. The Effects of Sociodramatic Play on Disadvantaged Childrgp, New York, John Wiley and Sons, 1968. Sponseller, D. Play as a Learning Medium, National Association for the Education of Young Children, Washington, D. C., 1974. Strom, R. E. and Greathouse, B. "Play and Maternal Self- Concept", Theory Into Practice, 13,4, 1974, 297-301. Suomi, S. J. and Harlow, H. F. "Monkeys Without Play" in Play, Bruner, Jolly and Sylvia, Editors, Basic Books, Inc., N. Y., 1976. Sutton-Smith, B. "Children at Play", Natural History Magazine, Dec. 1971, pp. 54-57. Sutton-Smith, B. and Sutton-Smith, S. How to Play With Your Children (and When Not To), New York, Hawthorne Books, Inc., 1974. Sutton-Smith, B. "Play as a Transformational Set" AAHPER, June 1972. Takata, N. "The Play History, AJOT, 23,4, July/Aug. 1969, pp. 314-318. Vulpe, S. G. The Vulpe Assessment Battepy for the ~Atypical Child, National Institute on Mental Retardation. v rt v t Y Toronto, Ontario, Canada, 1977, pp. 296- 306. White, B. and Watts, J. Experience and Environment: Major Influences on the Deve10pment of the Young Child (Vol I), Englewood Cliffs, Prentice Hall, 1973. White, R. W. "Motivation Reconsidered: The Concept of Competence", Psychology Review, 66,5, 1959, pp. 297-333. 106. 107. 108. 100 Williams, Y. B., "Therapeutic Play Services in Childrens General Hospitals in the United States", Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 4,2, second quarter, 1970, pp. 17-21. Witengier, M. "An Adaptive Playground for Physically Handicapped Children", Physical Therapy, 50,6, June 1970, pp. 821-826. Wolanski, N. "A New Method for the Evaluation of Motor Development in Infants, Polish Psychological Bulletin, 4,1, 1973.