Q 1 . O . . . . 3101.! -. I «.1... 3:415}... 1.5, y? 9:... 9.961... VI. il‘h vilVlg .5; . Tattflqa . w . YE: QVFigt imvlfl_l'"','90uell1$§n u a .D.‘ «PM. it...“ ‘otgvfli5ffz~ v I F , 5 9 .2 , k , 15 I... .1534 a . .117 L; . . . 7. {5. 1.3132“! . u.‘ , . . no . .\ «5‘. \i‘wavlo‘lhnu. V at 2,. 318... 5 . I. . Juli... finfim 13!... .1 . you. I v a. v ' .lnfvai : IlOVveaiiitvflthnltrlrmKN 1411‘” .514.» .J g \5 «E T I V . .. Q11. trivial; if. 1' .I‘ .\ “Eng". % 11 A I. .5!th chug!!! cryitifx. . 515 in :re \ A: . In. .L I x \ t :1 ’3 . . > . :31. 24.1.9”. 3.... gaitisglinzo . . u .- 2. I. .. . . 31 w . 5..- . 1%... , .1.“ 5.1!..sz .whbfiVEII a 7 31:3... h§i§nu «gift I. II... :"FVY by“ . ;§\5.§J :54. 3.55 i 99.0 P > «.701 u. g]: Vm((r 2 3 x . u. . -. i .3622 v... . , 1) ((-3) .N.t s .2; x. Y, . z. . {ytiizntiivthf 0......» fifialfgga .. .94 {£55 m.» .353 a. r v. . . v1.0.1.1: v: .1\ 13.2.... 1.3.5, . .. s5 . i. , . . .. .v1:2§§.uw$wnflmn%vhfitumnhx t3§§5 .1. .1. $9.39... 2. a 1. 3. .H... 1...... :14. I 2.0 lsz E.O!.vItaJ-n§.t.ltv ‘flrllovr’fi.’vflz .1.“ 1 Mali}. . . . 1 II . . I. . .. figwfii . i ’ ' v§ «.1 J. , n ., \.. ‘ x. 1.227, 53.3.... it... «flux. 31:... . ti . 1.x .11 .\ an . 9.3.131. I 3. 1 . a a». . 3.3?!5 .ii .afiuuuu.» \ ,.....$)..fiurh.r%sli$x .. :1 , v 1559... IV! ,Exfihf 6.31 .315 .x m: 1 {$73115ng .“ 5.u&,z5x.5..}h.tfi§ .35.? \yinhu .. .. z :3. I: 1 , It :15... . zfifl‘ififidfiafi.¥. ,.. . .. ‘{snflh{x inaugflfilfiwnrgr Rh}? sag... ,. 3 g .3 aw... . In!" ”5&- =Itl Ir £, "UV 511 {1... . 91.1... W . n 75 14(1),). s.) . . .r 5 , . I. s . z. 2 31.. .fiawvteéin. 5 : 43... u: .5 1 . a . . +3.. I. .1 My}... 3.1; 1 I x r. “6&3! .- IIRIivt .00 0. 7!}. “If.“ . grilg‘zt . WR.’§C. 7.x «in. . , iflxduwi ,\ r. .1.ij . 2. .5! L. b u v u .qt'cgi2n.tlll.v l'lt.'|n’) (I “w I \. II. A. .35... . . hub-3R) . 0a. . 75 , 1 H. I: gzs‘ttlf! . 1.311;... fie. .54.. ~MI .8 NI!YL\~S.§‘11§$ Akkdnw ,11 .Wwwmltlfi \fi‘qYKHMS \Wmmflflnt 550.41.. .) 5 .1 a V y a a. .. , . . a. If... 2:: )2. mum. gun. Luv...) 1 u... .1. a. 5. N. 23).. 91...} .Ifita. “ , . .k. .n !>\ ha . Ru 5 .. y . . 1.1. .efi.....1§x§2.fi0nvti>?2{sw.u«} lungc... £9.11»... tam, unfiflus .130 s. :40}. In. . . J 1 \ .3555 \h. fighxxfiik..5313.i.\3¥\.i 52.55515 w. in“! N. . \ I £110.? . 5 www‘ 15%... X§§§fi 355155.}..whmtn5ikngu, 1 I.» . mgll. ,. . ‘4... . . . 351.)... .3: \ .1 3v . 1 , 2335 \ §\> I} «.2. \(l%.. \«c .5be . fi 9 I 5 15 313%}...xllflt1s‘. Lmfl. 44568.) L. . I 3 , 1. . VI . n \5 . 1§.5\ 53.5.5 I“ hit}! tun-n}... l. VT: 51. . ~ h x t 3.. 3.15%“); w...) r,1§\ 1 Kx;kt§7%fii§n$.§ I}:1., .. \\.1uw.k.u. a; . . 1 «1.. . v 1 [V i s t . 1§.\J . 35‘ .(3. 114.41%: .4321. 123.! a .r v .x\.‘\€%u$ Agass . , . I .. . a . :azctttfivinlics‘,‘ 015: is.) hung”. 2).»..qu I. . 0.94.. 31):} . «.51.! as)..\ 53. u. .. . x. u !.rvaz’§f..t9££§;§‘itli , . . .. . 1 . x a I .9 .1153, 1 £3 i323... 1.111%..N5 , x .. , . ,. \ 3 :09...ka intrusi- Iax‘apl ihttxPrvtllr; I»: \< .413. .uynwfl? «4.3.411... 2......sz I911: . 212%);x Y‘7$§WQ¥ . 5.5115131mnuflhfliur193tauinu{(34315 I u. . .4 a. .1 I 1 , .1 a ..a. . R .-.-.-.v‘fil¢vft!u.€t . , .. .. , . i.., ,. .I. :1)...» .1 , . ., .. . . :hov) MHV!:J.«>J.I~Ivre....hrhi'vv§’igiii.3nXIVIIHMFJIEIITANVO £3.35 . . II ”81.1. €¥$.Y§a) .. «8.9.5... I v1.51! gfifikfllfldih hi.5)11¥n€i\\\fl1txkkflufi} :, >ei>VY|1.-1\ 1 URN-"y 53.3.1 Xvi, )1. 7155 . .. 1-1 “153:“. .135.)ttiitfinll....4Zrflvl‘..ui'£'b.if17€ 01.9051: $11.“. .1115. xi". .55rjw1hlkfleu .\)h¥. 1 ~ .h.1¥ k3.)\,fl~.uu1\w.‘owmfluvi r51. 1.. .1 371117.“..HNN. \JHIWofzfldltnw5TKuumrt31 kWVKSN ‘31:. >945 151.7... 1.3!. . .\\T.1 1|, . 3.11:1...2...» vanadium"! .20.; wig”)... Ce. . 1‘8 3. .. . 35 _ 1 c- .. . )1 . .4. fads»)! . 2-3711)“: 53.1 in “[573... \ 1?}...11 3.5.11, 33:)r911}\l{ih\ \ »£:...\u.l.~v\..15.71 :th. 5.5.1.1.. “$1.51!: , . a1?- . All... 5.35:... nhfi»,.,.vovlz!s.nol.u6ysl In I... 31¢..H«.2<{§..Lx 2.0.. If}. 33.); 35..- .h. {akasfizlfixnsixllégxwzk \, n KS: #30 Z. 3.24.. 3.3%... ... \. .fiu..«afia.43..1...mn4.. V531,} 2. a ‘57.. VIII-“ht Et~AE§Et§§O ii . igfggéx v - .. 55...): #155“ x 55..) 55,... yrs 3‘533515. V)T.fl..»xn.5.15sr>55\\ .57351.$.vs§111\wi}n )f. 5.1:. ‘5 .. «NVEuSWSYWHthN 35.3.1.4. . u 3 N .21.. 62. I‘ll... ‘3. ‘Il’tvilsa I. 911...} 10’ .094: L1 . I :13]... 14.) \551?.724\Q1..1.51L>\131I5 zit... .54., I... y,)..1\\\(..¢.11\...1\{, . 5am- “... 55 .15 . . 551,35 3...? ~51v.a155§1 \\\\$..\I52:l.. 1 5.. ..ci.a‘n...0rv.\c.p5l9.vlvua.ilvl.f;.§’ D Iii! 1.3.: . 51. gm 4.51.».flflvrq‘fllfiuxg3 ii:l>x§.\.§ \igfixévvsr‘ Q11hnnrv5 . Aimfihu'fififlafisi§\?) 43.8%.. . Q“) 7197.“... . .Lmuv‘ I. {v4.1.3.7} ?.n.w“7Y(7!Vru$-§ial.4Nvfiv if. v! I! . . 33. Q)! In; 1 2.41m”... . h. 13.5%) .1xxz.)s..19~vm.%vilx {15.11321manhunujhi§$1.¥h§u§ swwvigafiwxhfln11P§NEzaslh~flhh 325?... Jutziqhnil‘su “9:191:11“! vhf...» . 535.7)...“ tiatxl. .3! 20:30. 3? , n , 33¢. 0.5:)Vz\}.o¢.. ch. .15.... . , a5}.§‘xhi..1.§c.§}.i1 1... 22.5.... .3..{>}(§\Jza\xa>y\s3§(»( 3312...: ..( A... .1 , . . . . 3...... .13.... s 3.1. .J in gut-.28. '61 $.(Orlf'lvfg‘19gii‘gn lingo. V I. 553131» 1575 5‘53,. 554$... 55:. 51u§z$7137\\5\i’)\)h.7!fi 1) {it 5) Si\5‘.\ ‘7’". , i;3§31.5351§\1i. vitlvuuflpi *3...”th IO J n v 1).. \x. 3.4105(1) ‘L.§%\\5 !‘..2.1. 11. at 25555151}. 5“} 1 1 , ~ - 31. L... .51)?! 5.21 .s l , o. gullfiflnetvlu W.‘...~“‘£1I~éi.tl\0zn¥~g IQ It: ii. 2 . .0..i\.flwl..hifimuull;§}flifiaui§i. ”133%.3.kn:15.555§\531u“.5§ 53.3.“... “$3: 1101.. 5 nuns“... 5.6%....«1. .. .. 1 x, , . . 9, +31. db: ”\‘telfl’vzv Otiltizcifz. 11'0" . 3} 321.5%, [11%; 7x ‘4. r: 3W5]: \ .z“i;fi§5dis\l,§r¥5,fi7wfl§qfl 5:5]..“18T1Vul‘. 54.1.2511): I 51%;}311’5’ 5|, 3.51... 2.5.. fix. 4... p. $2.1... tit.._.~.z£:.f:i......£§.c§£: ii..,u.8:u..£.z....£z§h .. .31....i..iz..:....z§¥1§xi:§§§.? .wrifluz.§.u.$. ,.13§§§£n§ ....m2.u.zu;....§.irfihiei....:. 5913.. .. 3.2.4:; 25...: ..zza;§.,.«§.......nv.ai..z...é...ba. . ..... . . 4: .1 :1 d O . .) . . : 1. . . , I I... 1 . ‘ ,.V Lh;tV!...¢. II I l 2‘0... b V; , ‘1 1 . \U.\ - 352.36! $133113?“ 3\3)I...)31351 v ilvaiWV 1h... VfiJflW5¢5R .1 ul\i\ . v III-vi . .Ic ttAAAtu.ro}-bh..vaao\§1.i in 0.21."... . 3 . . )5 )flfll 9311.551 I) 1!..(n “NH“ 3364.!!! ‘55.?{5 51:35L%,w1;\1\£1\55?.\:.§1 $.51. \\3l.$~.a)\..ux .- . 5.5.3.1.. :71... “2515a! "ii, I; 1.5. ;...r. .xc..>59¢ll.vft. £3... £1... igg. . 30...} ....x»:\l£. 1. 3.5V 33.1....ahfixvgfiugsafihfixf?.?12.4finfigadguzcnfi3}.i<24sg¢.swflé§tajs13i€ . .x.31¥.!u1..5~..s . .. {I I. I L .115 . v "VJ . .. . n . . . . , . . \I \. .V. a . ’13,. ‘;,)}h 5‘ ”anlw‘a: 47.)! Z .7155} 1 .5 . I l YiVu r: . i )o 115. ,3gghfilnlgkzxt 2“ In)“ s ‘15 5.0.1113 “3...”! «gnythHsa‘qua‘EM"§3.MHNVJHW5\x 1d. I n ”.013“... ”“5! .3 3.11 . . . . . «a h 5...,r a 5;):luzf‘: 2.751....) (inf? A13 51153155.. . 3Q!- .-. £K¢§§+§ 31.1... $331.; : I 388‘s?! , . 5.. i2. , , (“diva“).......u..<,,§»n.§1w1fifi§saflnfiflhga33%....n £33 339313“... _ s ‘ I , a 2. , x . 3 . . .. a 1.5.4 . Juli: .. .. .. . 12. gig?! . v .. vx)! , If: .5. 1 O u . :lyu , 5.... u p» , . \. \.... 5‘5 ..1IzYst.$5m\Ju.wh-\.uuflhuhuxfli. Yguuulfi- I533 unu%.nn....§t..i?3£.y£;tvfl£ .7 2.1.2631... p 3+3». . . . . .. 2.. .., . .. 1..., . a .. .. 3118..."... 39... thiub.tiri.wlasa33 ..\. , ‘mv.flnfixnufififlfinfl~iifi. . \Brtrlflw , . . . . . . , , y ,s ,v 7 . ”hm. a: tally). .nus. A 1!. I... r fixiivuu Plfci’: .. . . . Ail. If‘xiltlvlkgflhgvtflvlf’.gz¥§ é . . . «iii... 1...- siting-x...» E EEO)... . f/lhvlvsvti:llt‘)lirrlt.£l‘§£ ‘: > . . \ V )1. . £6713)... 8‘55 5..\. .11.... 111 (It? 1:31..) . ’52., 55 I]... . a). . a .1235»): II. is)... 1....1T S 1>I311v11 WA .459 . 1 11775.15} 15 3.5 .f :5: I. If ,vr.vvl.«$t>l.lvf«z{. . t) u y y ? s... . .. .i y III .. a . ,2!\L.>Jn .a.‘ 1.1... . \ v 1!.)31 5'\5\ it 53.} g: . ., . is. x. ..fi.u.325§1n.w2.3.¥2fi€23.21... .54.... u? win. . , ;| 53 \fizqiv. .7! i u , . IV sit I. .5 0013. LE. 1... :1 5 June.“ 3.3.“? .81.... v hfifi. EH. , ‘ I . 5!" $11.31! 3‘ \(. 5V 3! . g .5. I. .eia... .Lx.3..1.h%§fiwfiam . a. 4 34.3.3 3.. v 5.. 5.1??? 2.... on: .. 1 .2139}? . ixisiiflfiuigg Xi... . , t , . glitz; 5.5.4.5! 515).. , :01. .. s . 5 .Uafl.15.. . 5.\,Ilo)flllfiaifi...lou\r...1 J.))VW§.HM¢111?IP§Z§ Mg“. ’25."... 31‘ . (cumin; )1: 1.5.15. \\5 5 “(Ln ,Iltn. . . v. )5 v ztiifis 5‘... 151. a. .n 11 . 11.131. . g'fi‘rg5 v .ggflxfifig5hpziliimmz 1.3.3.21 §7\5 \i‘) ixhéflkiifig. 11.555. I‘ll. . .. - , , .1531; gr 1..., ,I u 5.1“, . $3. 11 III; }\ \iikdi\‘1v)15?\ 3“...” 31): . . 1.75 3‘! 51‘3.‘ 2.401113241»).....3fl{1¥$\n...lw’a‘§i.g kl 1 155 5:5 v... 3:! . l . li’aiz .133.JA\UuN¢-‘(.1;Ntl iiiigéjiiixinihhi a #52 ’5‘ 5 5 {.515 \ figh$1¥5$11ia\ . ii 515.. . , . ¥§§\ )3... )Ezgibmifi 1‘55 31‘ 5“ zWInglv‘. , . 5‘3; figfig figs“... I g}1§l‘§\1§¥hflxfl€dfi.§z f \. ,Nundgi‘uflflnwwnirby .11, figggzi 111: . 3\1..\i~41§. .F.§.\n§:1ai\x.l...\li~wvxu§ - 153555;..11ef. 2 .3...1:21LV$\\(9 Sofia... 3.4:. ”Hr-c.5311! i. .3. x V 11.1.1.3... 11.51.!- < 5.. ~65: x. 11.5.... . _ ‘ , fisfigh;{xitinfiwnsfldhfilav%fi§n¥&.\.fl§ .ta.huu,lf£n3 324.1%“: 1133,3412??? g; . . r . . . .. . A1371?“ 1 , a 1133‘; .1. .«NwVQ aggi‘Iifsigwglii «fly-‘41. 51 7555»).- it . s§.fiiifi\\n.fi\fl‘kw $59915 €‘1ggfi .... x .. . .mzl . ¥i%fi..i\? 2....) v .1... . . . . , s . , \ .‘ . . . ‘kstflelu! 30$. . . I . SM- Wq‘5zwfi‘a . . 1 “ 11 \v .\ . $3151.53...“ 1 . 5% 1c .5“ a .. . . .. .. 2 . . . . 2.... . ._ .7... .wnkkvs...._3. .. 1.7.. .35 s . .1502. I I. infiniq;iu.flifiha)r5k 1 . . an. . .. 7 run“. . . . .13.; S§a\\>i35\anib<$vffl. , .?\?¥§A 1.0. .. . r ’53:)! ?$fli!itl‘)v\u§ulflumi‘iiil ”-31.3155 . . . 35;; 'fi . 5%?5? is: . 191).... His-\xfi‘filits»... gigs 1 . . . , . 3 J'axuzl’...’ . . .5 153.351.3111, has: $557151 "Qz.iuz..3.l¥vf17§‘i \. .. . 11:14:32? 1.333.. atr é : , . . , :7 .1. )um. . fig. lhfifgk\$%flmn. x . . . 1 .s . :5 .220 n.7- . “I. 11.27... .b.NN.: )2} , J75); . Age V1! .; . . . . .. . a - 7 . . , .1 , 3 it: i»)... \ L 5!. v , 15 . a .\ up .\I .1 . I?! ‘ >5 \.... v . L “Lt. .Irv. .. . p .. .. y . igfiuxu I .r? . 373.8”... K . \lu: ,f‘liff .5; . $.11... gugxsszulxiaralgr $33.... .3» g 1?) .2;- 13¥i3§i}.15 . . , \ 131%... 5.. :5 32W: .. I. . g1?111§3§1} . 1%31thfi.11 illfilfi‘g E)...“ u C. . . .nrevléigg. KKK. It ill. 5.511.? (4.. Act with 1271.3 gill. Ito... . . 2 I . 119.. .ofl‘x3arzdaIK§.ow.§.>9xrfvlfllz’uffifliuuulifmlzflttiivrtir {it‘igvlf . . 114.? 2. I . 1.75.97 iii!!! bgéilftl’hflr Riga). .v’ff/ul) \ Iv . .I 5"? .rr &) a. .3 .a. 1 . {gt-£3... as}; $.12er {if ET! A , .5 8§ ,. - _ .\ . .. \ . . . . 9 : Ezravuhtrztvténmuhimfisiflxé . . . 7 t .finfltfitf.szyz .2, gunk. .X . . E EAR-Ir? f4. .4 - . , a it m .mflffixnnrfifi . . . U ‘§ .§ . I r .l . Van . . . l . Ett:§5>{.k . .. . .13.}...129 it.-.» Exeya5t4fiizxituggt 2 .2322. V.i.r.$u..xl.: ..-.:...-.!....z.2é??;¢12?3..§= :13: . . 7!!!! l£.rxl7"s¥f‘v)rlv,1vi . .25.... . . . . . .. .3 .. .3 .,,.3..3.$.<.§vii;. , . ? . 1.: . T . , . r? .......ix.4!..coot+...¥. ,.,.... 4 :r. .. . .i . . . nix-.3 v.33...:.e=r$$.. 1.§urw§?§iw .. :32}- . array... .1‘... gauguhwrwfi... in. «é... (Iii)? sis. thflwnvtasrlsrffin|ibrlotteyang, . . .... llllflillllfllfllllfllllll'lWilli!HIIIIHUHIIWIIIJIUHHI 93 10529 6333 . «nurd't'au'i-é-fi'ufil'j: .5 1 3,... r I I? This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Direct and Indirect Conservation of Fossil Fuel Energy: The Infiuence of Financia] and Phiiosophicai Motivators and Availabie Human Resources, presented by Mari S. Wiiheim ‘ has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PhD degreein Famiiy and Chiid Ecoiogy L// Major professor Date 6; /? 3R MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 RETURNING MATERIALS: bV1ESI.) Place in book drop to remove this checkout from w your record. FINES wil] be charged if book is returned after the date stamped_be10w. .1 A W raga-13837:“ Waugh M2988”? w'\‘ ) 1+0 K093 @618 5“???” PW! DIRECT AND INDIRECT CONSERVATION OF FOSSIL FUEL ENERGY: THE INFLUENCE OF FINANCIAL AND PHILOSOPHICAL MOTIVATORS AND AVAILABLE HUMAN RESOURCES BY Mari Wilhelm A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 1981 6W 7.3 7 / ABSTRACT DIRECT AND INDIRECT CONSERVATION OF FOSSIL FUEL ENERGY: THE INFLUENCE OF FINANCIAL AND PHILOSOPHICAL MOTIVATORS AND AVAILABLE HUMAN RESOURCES BY Mari Wilhelm The develOpment of this research was based upon a management model of motivation and available human resources for making decisions concerning the conservation of fossil fuel energy. The focus of the study was upon a measure of both direct and indirect conservation. The research was conducted as a secondary analysis of data collected during the evaluation of a statewide household energy audit conducted at Michigan State University. Energy consumption data from utility and oil companies served as the measure of direct conservation. Indirect conservation was investigated through analysis of self-reported participation in a variety of behaviors col- lectively defined as voluntary simplicity. The household was the unit of analysis for a total sample of 638 families. Multiple regression analysis served as the primary statis— tical procedure for testing the hypotheses. Mari Wilhelm A 1.8 percentage reduction in direct household energy consumption was found between the years 1977-78 and 1979-80. Nearly three-fourths of the households were found to have practiced at least some voluntary simplicity behaviors. Relative cost of fuel used by the household was the only significant motivator for direct conservation (p=.016). Availability of human resources did not influence direct conservation. Neither did direct conservation contribute to a sense of personal control over energy problems. Both motivation variables and human resources were included in a significant prediction model of voluntary simplicity behaviors. Philosophical perspective contri- buted four percent of the variance (p=.000). Income ade- quacy had a negative relationship with voluntary simplicity participation but contributed only an additional 0.8 per- cent to the variance (p=.022). Reported skills was the strongest predictor accounting for a bivariate contribution of 10.9 percent and a multivariate 12.0 percent of the variance (p=.000). Average household education and percep- tion of the ability to afford a conservation device also had significant contribution to the predictive model. In addition,the practice of voluntary simplicity behaviors explained 3.1 percent of the variance to the measure of personal control (p=.000). © 1982 MARI SUE NILHELM All Rights Reserved ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Many persons deserve a hearty and sincere thanks for their continued support and encouragement during the entirety of my graduate program and especially during the process of my dissertation. As a result of their efforts—- I made it. My doctoral committee provided considerable guidance for which I am especially appreciative. Dr. Arthur Kohrman served on my committee until his sabbatical and later move to Chicago. His encouragement and confi- dence was greatly appreciated. Dr. Margaret Bubloz pro- vided continued support and contributed much to my interest and knowledge of family theory. Dr. Craig Harris was an incredible source of help in learning the research process and in understanding Hal, the computer. For the many hours of his time a very special thank-you. Dr. Beatrice Paolucci was an inspiration through her teaching and amazing ability to challenge students to grow just a bit more. Dr. Joanne Keith served as my major professor and the director of research. Without Dr. Keith's friendship and professional support, I would many times have given in to the pressure of graduate school. Congratulations go to Dr. Keith for surviving!! I would also like to ii acknowledge Dr. Keith's very special family--husband, Bob, daughter, Julie; and son, Robbie. Many times they accepted and treated me as a member of their family. Several friends were made during graduate school. We all shared the same problems which seemed to make life more tolerable. I would like to especially thank Dr. Susan Merkley, Edith Hanlon Katz, Martha Bristor, Bonnie Knutson, Dr. Madeline Trimby and Dr. Carol Darling. In addition, I would like to thank a very special friend, Roger Brooks, for his continued belief in my abilities. A very special group of friends from my teaching years in Dubuque, Iowa were always there when I needed them. To Michael Harvey, many thanks for the letters of support. Beverly Vaninger, Deborah Ioerger, Janet Shelledy Dillavou and Judith Schmidt are truly loved. Many thanks to each of them. A special thank you also goes to my typist Diane Osburn for her patience. Without Diane I'd still be typing. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Vi LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii Chapter I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Background of Problem . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Purpose of Research . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Research Questions . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 Conceptual Framework . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 Definitions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 Energy Alternatives . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 Energy and Lifestyle . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 Reduction in Household Consumption of Fossil Fuel Energy . . . . . . . . . . 27 Voluntary Simplicity Participation . . . . . 35 Human Resource Availability . . . . . . . . 44 III. METHODOLOGY AND HYPOTHBSIZED RELATIONSHIPS . . 55 Research Subsample Selection and Description . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Measurement Procedures . . . . . . . . . . . 60 Measures of Conservation . . . . . . . . . 63 Measures of Motivation . . . . . . . . . . 74 Indicators of Human Resources . . . . . . 81 Statistical Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . 92 Assumptions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 IV. FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . 99 Participation in Direct and Indirect Conservation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100 The Effect of Motivators and Human Direct and Indirect Conservation of Fossil Fuel Energy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100 iv Chapter Percentage Change in Consumption . . . Bivariate Regression Analysis-- Percentage Change in Consumption . . . Multiple Regression Analysis—- Percentage Change in Consumption . . Summary and Discussion of Hypotheses . . Participation in Voluntary Simplicity Summary and Discussion of Hypotheses . . A Comparison of Percentage Reduced Consumption and Voluntary Simplicity Contribution of Conservation Behaviors to Personal Control . . . . . . . . . Testing the Model . . . . . . . . . . . V. OVERVIEW . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of the Findings . . . . . . . . Conclusions and Implications . . . . . . APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Appendix A. Percentage of Households Reporting Agreement to Philosophical Statements Concerning Energy and Conservation, 1979 . . . . . . . . B. Percentage of Households Reporting Agreement to Statements Regarding Personal Control Over Energy PrOblems I 0 O O O O O O O O O O O C. Voluntary Simplicity Score (1980) by Categorical Level of Conservation Actions Taken . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . Page 104 106 108 117 119 130 134 135 138 145 147 152 159 159 160 161 162 Table l. 10. ll. 12. LIST OF TABLES Selected Household Characteristics-Comparison of Research Subsample, 1978, and Michigan Households . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Selected Household. Comparison of Research Subsample, 1978, and Michigan Households . Percentage Change in Household Consumption 1977-78, 1979-80 . . . . . . . . . . . . . Percentage of Households Reporting Participation in Voluntary Simplicity BehaViors O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 0 Participation in Voluntary Simplicity Behaviors: Total Scale and Subscale Mean Scores and Categorical Percentages . Factor Analysis of Voluntary Simplicity Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Household Income Adequacy Levels, 1979 . . Household Scores on a Measure of Philosophical Energy Perspective, 1979 . . . . . . . . . Summary Data of Human Resource Variables . . Personal Control, 1980 . . . . . . . . . . Comparison of Participation in Voluntary Simplicity Behaviors and Reduced Household Energy Consumption . . . . . . . . Bivariate Regression Analysis with Percentage Change in Consumption as the Dependent Variable and Motivators, Human Resources and Demographics as Independent Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi Page 61 62 65 67 71 76 80 91 93 101 107 Table 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. Hierarchial Multiple Regression Analysis with Percentage Change in Consumption as the Dependent Variable . . . . . . . . . . . . . Percentage Reduced Consumption by Philosophical Perspective by Income Adequacy Bivariate Regression Analyses with Total Voluntary Simplicity Scores as the Dependent Variable and Motivators, Human Resources and Demographics as Independent Variables . Hierarchial Multiple Regression Analysis with Total Voluntary Simplicity as the Dependent Variable . . . . . . . . . . . . . Regression Analyses with Direct and Indirect Measures of Conservation as Dependent Variables and Motivators and Human Resources as Independent Variables . . . . . Bivariate Regression Analysis with Personal Control as the Dependent Variable . . . . . Multiple Regression Analysis with Personal Control as the Dependent Variable and Conservation, Motivators, Human Resources, and Contextual Variables as Independent Variables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Percentage of Households Reporting Agreement to Philosophical Statements Concerning Energy and Conservation, 1979 . . . . . Percentage of Households Reporting Agreement to Statements Regarding Personal Control Over Energy Problems . . . . . . . . . . . . Voluntary Simplicity Score, 1980, by Categorical Level of Conservation Actions Taken, 1980 vii Page 110 112 123 126 136 139 140 159 160 161 n 3;... ..- .. LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1-1. Management Model for Conservation Behavior . 15 4-1. Percent Change in Consumption by Knowledge by Income Adequacy . . . . . . . . . . . . 114 4-2. Percent Change in Consumption by Age by Income Adequacy . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115 4-3. Percent Change in Consumption by Rurality by Fuel Type . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 116 4-4. Mean Voluntary Simplicity Score by Skill Level and Income Adequacy . . . . . . . 131 4-5. Mean Voluntary Simplicity Score by Education by Income Adequacy . . . . . . . 132 4-6. Path Analytic Model Showing Direct and Indirect Relationships Between Motivators, Resources, Conservation Behaviors and Demographics . . . . . . . 144 viii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background of Problem Carver (1924) noted that the basic problem of living living was, in fact, a problem of conserving energy. The. focus of his statement was the economy of human energy as a rationalization for increased use of fossil fuels. Carver also pointed out that when coal and oil were ex- hausted; wood, alcohol, waterwheels, windmills and solar engines would provide Viable energy alternatives. Nearly sixty years later, evidence supporting the limitations of fossil fuel reserves has brought the energy alternatives mentioned by Carver, as well as others, under serious con- sideration as substitutes for finite fossil fuels. The use of fossil fuel resources in the United States has increased steadily over the years in an effort to improve the level of living. Considerable utility has been derived from their use. Labor saving devices, powered by fossil fuels, made leisure time a reality. Travel became much less difficult and considerably faster. Services were provided at a high enough level outside the home that the household became a center for consumption of goods and services. Consumption would become a symbol of a household's quality of life. Awareness of the limitations of fossil fuels to meet the energy demand has been a major concern of the last decade. The focus of this concern was frequently based on the dependency of the American lifestyle on fossil fuels for abundant and inexpensive energy resources. The in- creasing demand for energy coupled with the rising prices of imported fuels and the extensive cost of extracting less easily accessible fuel sources would eventually result in less energy available for consumption (LeGrand and Robinson, 1980). Concern over loss of the benefits of fossil fuel energy has led to speculations about future energy life- style alternatives (Ford Foundation, 1974; O'Toole, 1976; Lovins, 1977; National Research Council, 1979; Stobaugh and Yergin, 1979; Toeffler, 1980; Schurr, et a1., 1980). In general, the various lifestyle alternatives range from con- tinued growth through use of nuclear power to extensive cutbacks in consumption of energy. The Ford Foundation (1974), for example, identified three future energy scenarios. The Historical Growth Scenario is based on the assumption that consumption of energy resources would increase at about 3.4 percent annually. This growth would require aggressive efforts to balance supplies of energy with increasing demand through development of all energy resources including nuclear power. The second scenario, Technical Fix, included an in- crease in the energy growth rate of approximately 1.9 percent annually to the year 2000. Through a conscious effort towards more efficient use of energy resources this scenario would consume one-third fewer British thermal units (Btus) than the Historical Growth Scenario. Applica- tion of energy conserving technologies would be expected to improve both direct energy savings and indirect savings from energy processing. The third scenario, Zero Energy Growth, involved social and economic systems which are based on a harmony -with the natural environment. Economic growth would be expected to continue through production of durable goods and a larger proportion of services. Conservation would be accomplished through technical fixes and fewer purchases of material goods. As the population became aware of a potential energy shortage, technology frequently was turned to as the answer. Technology may be answering the questions con- cerned with increasing energy supplies, however, the more appropriate question might be the extent to which energy is required and beneficial to society. Amory Lovins (1977), an advocate of soft technology, questioned whether the Historical Growth Scenario, or as he called it, the Hard Technology Path, was truly the answer to societal goals. He pointed out that continued use of oil and coal at the present level and increased development of nuclear power would continue present inefficient use of energy resources. In addition, he noted the steep invest- ment of capital necessary for such developments. Lovins proposed that where energy was concerned, we ask questions concerning what energy source would be best for which par- ticular purpose. Lovins claimed that premium fuels have been used inefficiently in places where other energy resources could accomplish designated goals equally well without being wasteful. In addition to efficient use of fossil fuels through technical fixes, Lovins supported what he called soft energy technologies. Technical fixes included, among others, insulation, heat pumps, and proper ventilation. Soft energy technology included the use of renewable energy resources such as the sun, wind, and vegetation. Decisions being made at present determine our per— sonal as well as our economic future. Lovins as well as other critics of our present pattern of economic progress suggested that while making economic decisions regarding allocation of scarce resources it is necessary to incor- porate social decisions into the process. It is their position that society can maintain a level of living com— parable to present standards while enhancing a quality of living based on personal and human values. This would, however, require an assessment of our current consumption and production patterns (economic decisions) in relation to societal values and goals (social decisions). Theorists supporting Zero Growth or Soft Path Technology Scenarios claimed that their ideas for solving the energy dilemma have fewer long term liabilities and greater societal benefits. Such proposed scenarios vary in scope and intensity. Some suggest a reorganization of the production-consumption system; others pr0pose a back-to- the-land lifestyle. Morrison and Lodwick (1981) suggested that the claims made by soft energy advocates should be treated as hypotheses to be tested. Information gained related to the claimed impacts would be beneficial to policy. Nelson and Honnold (1976) also suggested that the lifestyles proposed by conservationists may be contrary to what is known through sociology and psychology about behavior; primarily that consumption fulfills a status need. Ashby (1977) sug- gested that we first need to begin to find out what people are actually doing in terms of alternative lifestyle changes. It appears that during the decade of the eighties, the following questions concerning lifestyles and energy may grow increasingly important: Which persons are actually behaving in ways positive to conservation of fossil fuels? What factors or characteristics contribute to this behavior? What are the impacts of this behavior? Approximately two—thirds of the direct and indirect consumption of energy can be attributed to the choices and patterns of living in the American households (Hannon, 1975). Direct consumption of energy represents slightly less than one-half of household consumption and is used primarily for transportation and space and water heating. Indirect consumption of energy occurs when households make purchases of goods or services which require energy for production, storage, and transportation to market. This consumption exceeds slightly the amount of energy consumed directly by households. Due to the large amount of energy consumed, house- holds are recognized as primary decision makers involved in both direct and indirect consumption. Purchase decisions made within the household have both short and long range implications for energy use. For example, a large portion of the energy used within the home has been determined by previous lifestyle decisions such as where to live and what kind of house to own. Previous purchases of various house- hold appliances are other contributors to lifestyle demands on energy resources (Morrison and Gladhart, 1976). Making lifestyle decisions based on the fulfillment of goals or wants requires the accessibility of resources. During the last several decades fossil fuels have been readily available as an energy resource. As the limits of fossil fuel supplies have become evident, allocation of energy by households for direct and indirect consumption — — ‘X a. 35.1 _’.."‘$.\ ~ g."- .: .. _ - has come under considerable scrutiny. The household has been viewed as a possible source of direct conservation and potential source for production of goods and services for future less energy intensive lifestyles. Little is known, however, concerning the motivation involved in adoption of less fossil fuel intensive lifestyles. Purpose of the Research The primary purpose of this research was to deter- mine if there were differences in the adoption rate of direct and indirect conservation behaviors among households of various income adequacy and philosophical energy perspec- tives. Direct conservation was investigated through analy— sis of annual residential energy consumption over a period of three years. Indirect conservation was investigated through analysis of self-reported participation in a variety of behaviors reflecting self-sufficiency, recycling, and contributions to ecological organizations. These behaviors have been collectively defined as voluntary simplicity. A second purpose of the research included testing the influences of available human resources within the household on the motivation to practice direct and indirect conservation. Thirdly, the research explored the impact of participation in conservation behaviors on the development of a sense of personal control over energy issues. Personal control was measured through analysis of self-reported answers to five locus of control questions modified for energy use. Research Questions In an effort to contribute to the knowledge con— cerning actions adopted by households which relate to present issues of energy and lifestyle and the factors which contribute to these actions the following research questions were posed: 1. With what frequency are households reducing direct consumption of fossil fuel energy and with what frequency are they participating in behaviors related to voluntary simplicity? To what extent are indicators of income and philosophical perspective towards the energy issue related to a household's reduction in direct energy consumption and/or participa- tion in voluntary simplicity behaviors? To what extent does the availability of human resources influence the relationship between indicators of income and philosophical perspec- tive and a household's reduction in direct energy consumption and/or participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors? To what extent does participation in voluntary simplicity and/or reduction in household energy consumption contribute to a family's feeling of control over energy related stressors? Conceptual Framework Management within the household is directed toward the allocation of resources for the fulfillment of goals. It implies that there can be control over the process and the outcome of an event. A managerial perspective includes four basic con- cepts: values, resources, decision making, and goals. Values have been defined as personal preferences and beliefs about what is desirable (Hungerford, 1978). The expression of values can occur in a variety of ways. Values may be expressed culturally through norms and role expectations. In an economic system the value of a re- source or an end product is also expressed through its media of comparison or exchange value which is more com— monly referred to as price or cost. According to Deising, It is the value system of a culture which deter— mines the extent to which ends can be alternative, which makes some means normative and others neutral, and which allows media of value compar- ison to develop (Diesing, 1972, p. 46). Ashby (1977) maintained that the value of a re- source to the household can be assessed from four defini- tional components. They include cost of the resource within the marketplace, usefulness derived from its con- sumption, symbolic value attributed to benefits derived, and beauty. Within the context of managerial theory values are said to influence the development of goals. Values may thus be expressed in terms of goals or wants. Values are 10 also said to motivate the use of resources while attempting to satisfy goals (Nickell, Rice, and Tucker, 1976; Paolucci, 1977). Goals are frequently described as wants. In most, if not all cases, an individual or group has more than one goal. Resources provide the means whereby goals are suc- cessfully attained. Resources may be tangible commodities such as tools or raw materials. Resources may also be intangible such as time, skills, or knowledge. In addition, resources can be said to be human or nonhuman. Money is a nonhuman resource whereas self-esteem or motivation would be considered psychological human resources (Rice, 1969). When resources are plentiful they can be more easily used to satisfy a variety of alternative goals. When resources are scarce they must be allocated to specific goals. This allocation requires a more conscious process of decision making for goal satisfaction (Gross, Crandall, and Knoll, 1973). It is the decision making process that is the controlling factor in the choice of outcome and effective use of resources. When the outcome is not determined through a conscious choice among alternatives, less effi- cient use of resources may occur. Diesing (1972) identi- fied five types of rationality, two of which are useful for discussion of the decision process relevant to this research. Economic rationality was described as continuous measurement, comparison, ordering and exchange of ll commodities. The objective of economic rationality is the transformation of resources into maximum value. Social rationality was described as concerned with joint action based on shared meaning, trust, and mutual support. The objective of social rationality is the attainment of goals through use of resources. Values have been linked to social decisions and the allocation of resources to economic decisions (Paolucci, 1966). Through the conscious allocation of resources, households make economic decisions among alter- native ends. Through interactions among individual mem- bers, households make social decisions. Control exer- cised over the choice between alternative ends and the allocation of resources are said to be motivated by values. An ecological-systems approach was introduced to facilitate study of the management process within the family (Steidl, 1969; Hook and Paolucci, 1970). This per- spective allows for the conceptualization of the iterative process of management. In other words, rather than being involved in cause and effect relationships, management con- cepts are interdependent. For example, values are said to influence the choice among alternative goals and to moti- vate the allocation of resources towards the achievement of those goals. According to a family ecological model, however, a shift in the availability of resources can create changes in the choices made between various goals. 12 In addition, availability of resources is identified as an important component to the process of value development. The family ecological approach facilitates a focus on the interrelationship between the household and the con- text in which managerial control is being exercised. This context has also been referred to as the environment. Three subenvironments have been defined and include the natural environment, the human—made environment, and the social-emotional environment (Morrison, 1974; Bubolz, et a1., 1979). Resources are available to families from each of the various environments. When a change occurs in one subenvironment, changes will also occur in the other sub- environments. In the case of energy, limits of fossil fuel resources available from the natural environment will follow with changes in each subenvironment including the natural environment. Therefore, decisions regarding the use and development of resources from each area are critical. A shift in the availability of resources can create change through a reallocation of that particular resource along with reallocation and substitution of other available resources. Rice (1969) proposed an economic framework for assessing managerial behavior in which she stated: Change occurs in the family (individual or household) when its values are expressed in terms of wants (or goals) strongly enough 13 to cause decisions which direct behavior in the use of resources to increase or decrease production and consumption (Rice, 1969, p. 6). A discrepancy between what is and what one expects or wants may create change by setting into motion a process of decision making. Change in reaction to the energy crisis was discussed by Perlman and Warren (1977) as a pro- cessing of information whereby the system first notes an incongruency between what is actually occurring and the standard for what should occur. Until recently the comparison value of fossil fuels in respect to other energy resources has been relatively low. Since 1973 sources of natural gas and fuel oil have increased rapidly in price and supplies have been inter- rupted. This has meant that households are paying increasingly larger percentages of their income for home heating, electricity, and transportation. The symbolic value of fossil fuels as a resource could be described in terms of the interrelation between the shared meanings of a society, social interaction, and the individual (Schmitt and Grupp, 1976, p. 325). From this perspective fossil fuel energy could mean a warm house, ability to travel to visit family or friend, a sports car, stylish clothing, or a way to get what is desired out of 1ife.~ For others, it might mean pollution, inequitable distribution of wealth, or an impersonal market economy. 14 Conservationists have been quick to identify and focus on the less positive of the symbolic meanings of fossil fuel energy. They have suggested that society has focused on meanings of energy which are directed towards short-term benefits rather than long-term lifestyle values. In the process, values for the social good may be forfeited. The purpose of this research was to explore the impact of income adequacy and philosophical motivators ahd the availability of specific human resources on the prac- tice of direct and indirect conservation. Figure 1 depicts the underlying theoretical model. Income adequacy, philosophical perspective, and resources interact within the decision process. The imme- diate outcome of the decision process is the allocation of resources to a variety of behaviors. In this research the behaviors were specifically defined as structural and be- havioral changes related to direct conservation and volun- tary simplicity behaviors. Both aspects of behavior in the model were expected to lead to either direct or in- direct conservation of fossil fuel energy. A predicted outcome of participation in conservation was the produc- tion of motivation and resources upon which to draw at a later time. As with all models, only certain variables can be observed within one research tOpic. Variables studied in this project are defined below. 15 532:5. 6.32350 3— .26.2 EoEomucaE ... 0.39“. 9.3228 DEE—Em O\ b.3515 AcetaOEomcoo. mhcgagom 335.8.— / V 82:83. \ 5.5... ///< 0562.98.— A A1111.11....3....§..£ ....— 63.83 32.33 2:8... 16 Definitions Income Adequacy The assessment of the adequacy of the total money income of the household unit. This indicator was based on expenditure and need studies and was then adjusted for household composition and geographic residence. This was an objective indicator of the household's financial ability to purchase resources which have an exchange value within the marketplace. Philosophical Perspective An assessment of the symbolic value of fossil fuel energy to the respondent. This measure was based on the respondent's subjective orientation to issues or problems of energy consumption. Knowledge of Conservation Actions The ability of respondents to identify efficient methods of direct conservation of fossil fuels. Average Household Education The educational attainment of the principal adult(s) within the household. Reported Home Repair Skills The extent to which the respondent perceived household members capable of performing various household repairs. l7 Perceived Money Availability The respondent's personal assessment of the house- hold's ability to make purchases of equipment or devices which would facilitate fossil fuel conservation. Perceived Time Availability The respondent's personal assessment of the amount of time that household members could contribute to the installation of a conservation device. Human Labor The total number of household members who could potentially replace fossil fuel energy with human energy. Number of Employed Household Members Participation of household members within the labor market outside the household. Personal Control Subjective assessment of the respondent's compe- tence, achievement and individual efficacy in managing problems related to energy consumption. Voluntary Simplicity The respondent's judgment of the extent to which households participate in behaviors which have been theoretically defined as a less energy intensive life- style. The behaviors are considered indirect l8 conservation based on reduced purchase of material goods and substitution of human energy for fossil fuel energy. Percentage Change in Consumption An objective measure of direct consumption of fossil fuel energy within the household. This measure was based on actual meter readings obtained from utility companies for the years 1977-1978 and 1979-1980. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Energy is a vital resource for living. Looking at energy from a historical perspective O'Toole (1978) pointed out that the importance of energy is relative to the abil- ity of man/woman to perceive and harness the energy for use. O'Toole further maintained that history has shown that people can and do make choices among the use of alter- native technologies; that changes in use of various energy forms needs not create disaster yet they often result in social change. Energy Alternatives The increasing awareness of the finite character- istics of present fossil fuel energy sources has created' considerable concern regarding our energy future. Imme- diate reactions tended to display a sense of crisis (Morrison and Gladhart, 1976; Boulding, 1974; Perlman and Warren, 1977). More recently, however, the limita- tions of fossil fuels to meet future energy demands have resulted in the recognition of numerous alternative energy sources. LeGrand and Robinson (1980) noted that two objectives in the allocation of resources to 19 20 alternative ends were the achievement of social efficiency and social equity. The allocation of resources to energy alternatives requires consideration of these objectives through time as well as in the present. At present, natural gas and fuel oil provide approximately 75 percent of the energy used within our nation. These energy sources are nonrenewable and the domestic supply and production of fuels from fossil fuel sources is said to have begun an irreversible decline (National Research Council, 1979). Responses to a decline in an available resource traditionally have involved attempts to increase supply. Alternative energy sources such as coal, nuclear power, biomass, geothermal, wind, and solar are possible substitutions for natural gas and fuel oil to increase supplies of energy. Human labor is another potential energy resource. Control over demand for energy resources, i.e., conservation, has also been given considerable attention (Wolf, 1979). One aspect of conservation has been directed towards limiting the extraction of fuels from the ground. Another perspective of conservation has been described as efficient use of energy resources. Natural gas and fuel oil have provided an efficient inexpensive source of energy. Increased use has been associated with economic growth and, therefore, our present level of living. Reduction or change in the use of energy 21 may frequently meet with resistance due to concern over a potential lowered level of living. As a result, a variety of future energy scenarios have been identified and described (Ford Foundation, 1976; National Research Council, 1979; Williams, Kruvant, and Newman, 1979; and Amory Lovins, 1977). In general they each predict two basic future alternatives: increased supply or decreased demand. (The scenarios of the Ford Foundation were discussed in Chapter One.) Williams, Kruvant, and Newman studied the energy consumption of a national sample of household consumers. Based on actual energy consumption in 1972 and 1974 and an analysis of conservation attempts the authors sought to identify impacts of alternative energy futures. Four futures were identified: Business as Usual, Conservation Incentives and Mandates, Acute Shortage, and High Elec- tricity. Each future was discussed from analysis of the potential impacts on metrOpolitan trends of housing, land use, employment and transportation. The population of households was broken down according to five variables to facilitate analysis. These variables included: income (adjusted for family size and geographical location), location, race, age, and family size. The authors predicted few metropolitan changes in the Business as Usual and Acute Shortage Scenarios. A period of acute shortage would be likely to result in decreased housing availability and a shift to public 22 transportation. Households with adequate income resources would be least affected. On the other hand, low income, black, and older populations would experience the most negative impacts. The High Electricity scenario would mean abundant energy from development of coal and nuclear technologies. Growth would be expected to continue at present rates. The Conservation Incentive and Mandate Program would be expected to introduce greater change. The addi- tional input of capital to promote conservation would be expected to result in increased prices for housing and land. A movement towards back to the city and public transportation was also predicted. Once again higher in- come households would be expected to be least affected. Cost of housing and energy efficient transportation would impact primarily on low-income families. Larger families residing in the suburbs would also be expected to experience a squeeze on the pocketbook. Amory Lovins (1977) discussed energy futures in terms of hard and soft paths. The hard energy path would be similar to the Historical Growth scenario of the Ford Foundation and the High Electric scenario described by Williams, Kruvant, and Newman. Lovins described the soft energy path as involving aspects of both supply and demand. Supply of energy would result through the use of renewable energy sources and the harnessing of less environmentally harmful energy sources 23 such as solar. Since these sources of energy would not provide the one overall inexpensive and efficient energy resource, the soft path also incorporates restraints on the demand for energy. Wolfe (1979) viewed the soft path from two perspec— tives: alternative technology and conservation. Conserva- tion was perceived to occur through technical efficiency within the household and patterns of use based on lifestyle and value change. Changes related to lifestyle could occur through voluntary or mandated behavior. All too frequently decisions related to the choice among energy alternatives are concerned merely with the immediate availability of fuel to meet Wants and needs. The basic objectives of social efficiency and social equity if included in the decision process may receive short-term con- sideration rather than the long-term, throughout time consideration suggested by LeGrand and Robinson (1980). Edney (1980) noted that shortages and equality in the common social unit cannot exist together in a society where self-sufficiency requires the scarce resources for functioning. Resolution of scarcity problems in a demo- cratic community, according to Edney, should be focused on alternatives which would preserve individual choice; should not cause long-term problems; should be based on current technological capabilities; and should not create drastic lifestyle changes. 24 Edney further suggested that the dilemma of choice was not one of individual versus group rationality but rather a conflict of human values. Values of identity, competition, survival, freedom of choice, and social power may conflict with those focused on the common good. Stern and Gardner (1980) suggested that energy conservation from a commons framework is a problem of motivating persons to act in ways that promote lifestyles based on long-term social interests. Energy and Lifestyle Nader and Beckerman (1978) referred to lifestyle as the expression of value preferences through consuming behaviors. Value preferences or lifestyles are dynamic rather than static. Lifestyles will change as energy sources change throughout time. Nader and Beckerman noted differences between life- style changes and mere behavior change. Specifically they stated that lowering thermostats or carpooling would not be considered lifestyle changes. Rather they suggested that lifestyle refer to broader changes in social values or ideologies. They identified four: investment and reinvestment, hoarding, consumption, and work. A conflict of values appeared to be the common denominator in the relationship of lifestyle and energy. This is similar to the identification of a "Commons dilemma" discussed by Edney (1980). 25 Lifestyle is frequently used synonymously (although incorrectly), with quality of life. Quality of life is sometimes measured in terms of goods and services available for use. This measure is nearly always related to the Gross National Product and thus with production and consumption. Nader and Beckerman (1978) noted that several of the theorists they reviewedl had agreed that social change and social action were constrained by the amount of energy available. They noted, however, that within the last three decades energy theorists had not identified changes as necessarily higher, better, or more desirable. This is the view taken by Lovins and other conservationists in support of soft path energy technologies and the incor- poration of social as well as economic considerations into energy decision—making. Households are responsible for two-thirds of the direct and indirect consumption of energy (Hannon, 1975). Morrison and Gladhart (1976) noted that a great deal of consumption occurs as a result of "non-decisions" on the part of families. What they in essence were saying is that families or households form patterns of behavior which result in the consumption of energy. Such family tasks as preparing food, maintaining the household, and providing recreation require direct consumption of energy. lTheorists reviewed included Odum (1971), Cottrell (1955), Adams (1975), and White (1959). 26 Gladhart (1977) studied what he defined as life- style decisions and their relationship to energy consump- tion. Data were collected in May of 1974 on a sample of 216 urban and rural households in Michigan. Using regres- sion analysis to control for the effects of other variables, the specific contribution of various lifestyle features to energy conservation were determined. Findings indicated that multiunit and mobile dwellings used considerably less energy than single family dwellings. Houses with more insulation and houses with fewer doors, windows, and heated rooms required less energy to heat. Households without children and house- holds with older families were also found to use less energy. There was no important difference identified between urban and rural families residential energy use. Rural families, however, used 42 percent more gasoline. This difference was noted for travel to work, shopping for food, and transporting children to activities. It should be noted that the conceptualization of lifestyle used by Gladhart would have been discussed by Nader and Beckerman as patterns of behavior indicative of lifestyle or value preferences rather than the life— style itself. None-the—less, recognition of these be- haviors is important. Gladhart maintained that changes made in behavior patterns which may contribute to con- servation in one sense might result in consumption in another. Change in one behavior alone will not conserve 27 fossil fuels if not considered in the context of all life- style behaviors. Since conservation may result in dollar savings, the reallocation of that money needs to be for goods and services which are also less energy intensive. Reduction in Household Consumption of Fossil Fuel Energy The present study was directed towards understanding the motivations and household characteristics which facil- itate conservation of fossil fuel energy. It is, therefore, necessary to briefly discuss research related to household energy consumption. Based on an analysis of two studies, Grier and Grier (1978) identified three structural factors and a variety of population characteristics which influence consumption. Lowered population density of residential communities, smaller households, more adults per household, more workers per household, more elderly people and greater affluence all contribute to higher overall consumption of energy. Several of these population trends also have poten- tial for conservation. Affluence may afford households the ability to equip homes with energy saving technology therefore providing a potential area of conservation. In addition, higher income households due to initial higher levels of consumption may have more flexibility to conserve. A second trend noted by Grier and Grier was a back- to-the-city movement thus increasing the population density of residential areas. As these homeowners implement reno- vation of older homes, energy efficient technology can be 28 incorporated. In addition, energy costs for transportation may be evidenced. These benefits may be lost, however, if affluent households returning to the city bring with them energy intensive lifestyles. Perlman and Warren (1977) reported that the impact of the 1973-74 energy crisis was limited in the majority of 1440 households interviewed in their study. The main impact was primarily a change in the material resources available to families. They specifically identified money and gasoline as scarce resources. A few households suf- fered reductions of income through job loss; others noted a change in the availability of various goods and services. Changes in behavior related to noted impacts were assessed through analysis of self-reported data on house- hold uses of energy for transportation, heating, lighting, cooking, and air-conditioning. Estimates were made on the amount of energy actually consumed before and during the oil embargo. All figures were converted into British thermal units. A comparison was made based on the prOpor- tion of precrisis energy that was saved as a result of reported behavior changes. Overall, reports indicated a 12 percent reduction during the year following the oil embargo. Self—reported conservation estimates were validated by obtaining actual copies of the utility bills from a sub- sample of the total 1440 households in the study. A 29 comparison of actual consumption and estimated conservation showed that while households somewhat overestimated their attempts at conservation, the amount of energy actually saved was only slightly lower than the estimated figure. Btus saved on heating were estimated at 15.9 million British thermal units (MBTUs) actual consumption records showed a savings of 10.3 MBTUs. Conservation of energy used for other household purposes was estimated at 4.8 MBTUs, actual savings however, were not observed. Perlman and Warren reported that income differences in families resulted in differences in conservation beha— viors. A direct linear relationship was reported between income and absolute reduction in direct household energy consumption. Proportion of energy saved showed a slightly curvilinear pattern. Households with income under $5,000 and greater than $15,000 reported a similar reduction of approximately twenty-five percent. A slightly lower re— duction was reported by other household income groups. William, Kruvant, and Newman (1979) reported on actual consumption data of a national sample of households during 1972 and 1974. Their findings showed a 1.8 percent overall decrease in energy consumption. The reduction was noted primarily in apartment dwellings (-3.3% change) as com- pared to singleTfamily dwellings (0.2% increase); the older, over sixty—five pOpulation (—7.l% change) as compared to 30 middle-aged persons (1.4% increase); and central city resi- dents (—4.9% change) as compared to suburban (-2.4% change) and rural (1.0% increase) residents. A large portion of energy research directed to the study of conservation has resulted from the work of the Family Energy Project at Michigan State University. Their work included data based on self-reported conservation actions as practiced by households, but also included actual measures of conservation as reported through changes in direct consumption over time. Consumption data were obtained from appropriate utility companies once permission was given by the household. Hogan (1976) hypothesized that household energy conservation practices would differ among husbands and wives with varying commitment and congruency to selected values. Four values were studied separately: self-esteem, familism, social responsiveness, and ecoconsciousness. Using one-way analysis of variance, she found that commitment to ecocon- sciousness was the only value which contributed to differ— ences in adOption of conservation practices. Those families with high commitment to ecoconsciousness adopted 72 percent of the practices compared to 46 percent by families with low commitment. Hungerford (1978) examined the relationship of value commitments to residential energy use. Initial analy- sis showed that households reduced gas consumption by nine percent and fuel oil by 15.6 percent. Electricity was 31 reduced but not significantly. Stepwise multiple regression was used to investigate the relationship of value commit- ment and residential energy consumption. Results indicated that an increase in husbands' ecoconsciousness value was related to an increase in consumption whereas an increase in the wife's ecoconsciousness value was associated with a decrease in consumption. Further analysis looked at the relationship between congruency of a husband's and a wife's ecoconsciousness value and change in consumptiOn (1974-76). Ecoconsciousness value scores of husbands and wives were categorized as congruent and incongruent. Congruency was not found as a significant aspect of change in consumption. An analysis of the dif- ferences in ecoconsciousness congruency patterns found the greatest reduction in consumption in households where the wife had high commitment and the husband had a medium com- mitment to values of ecoconsciousness. Keith (1977) investigated household microdecisions related to energy consumption and their relationship to changes in consumption levels from 1973-74 to 1975-76. Data concerning family microdecisions were collected from self— administered questionnaires completed by both husbands and wives (or single parents) of one hundred and thirty house— holds. In addition, a scale was developed to determine a composite measure of conservation practices in which responses were weighted according to intensity of practice and number of adults. 32 Keith found an overall reduction in Btu consumption of 6.3 percent (p=.000). When controlling for fuel type, electricity showed a slight nonsignificant increase in con- sumption. Fuel oil users showed a greater decrease (11.1 percent) when compared with natural gas users (6.6 percent). A significant predictive model of conservation included three variables: addition of a new furnace, in- creased intensity of conservation behaviors, and addition of ceiling insulation. When electricity was weighted to account for conversion and transmission the stepwise regression model included change in the number of people. Morrison, Keith, and Roosa (1978) studied charac- teristics of conserver and nonconserver households by com- paring their scores on eighteen variables. Discriminant analysis was used in order to identify variables which would measure expected differences between the two groups. Morrison et. al., reported that households which actually reduced their direct consumption of energy (conservers) tended to be significantly (p=.013) more aware of their conserving behaviors. The conserving households were also significantly (p=.025) less likely to report acceptability of a scale of fifteen energy policies than were nonconserving households. The authors suggested that these results supported the case for voluntary conservation on the part of households. They further maintained that incentives rather than restrictions might promote greater conservation in the future. It was also reported that conserver groups generally were of a higher income bracket 33 (p=.039). Higher incomes were those households reporting at least a $15,000 annual gross income. Conservers also reported a higher level of education (p=.006). The dif- ference, however, was slight with conservers reporting slightly more than 13.4 years and nonconservers reporting slightly less than 13 years. Morrison, Keith, and Zuiches (1979) identified price of energy supplies as the most apparent impact of recent changes in fossil fuel availability. They reported that households from lower income, lower educational, rural, and older populations experienced greater stress from in- creased cost of fuels. Of various familial characteristics, Morrison et al., found that a reduction in direct household energy consump- tion was significantly related only to educational level. Specifically, households where the male head had some college showed a 8.5 percent reduction whereas households in which the female head had less than a high school educa- tion reduced 10.6 percent. The authors attributed the re— duction to choice and economic need, respectively. Other familial characteristics reported by Morrison et al., showed directional yet nonsignificant differences in actual reduced consumption. The greatest reduction occurred in the middle income groups; households where the head was within the over-45 age group; white collar; and urban households. In addition, households with one to two persons and households with more than five persons showed 34 greater conservation than did medium sized households. Similar reductions were found based on number of rooms in the house. The authors also reported that cost of fuel used for heating was an important factor in the reduction of energy consumption. It was reported that the greater rate of increase in the cost of the fuel used by the households, the greater the reduction in use. Fuel oil showed a 126 percent increase in cost over the period studied; natural gas an 81 percent increase. Fuel oil users reduced con- sumption by 11.1 percent compared to 6.6 percent by natural gas users. The cost of electricity increased by 50 percent and there was a slight increase in use as well. The authors explained this difference by noting that most household. uses of electricity are for lighting and appliances rather than space heating. Thus, the impact may have been of a smaller proportion. Morrison, Keith, and Zuiches reported that belief in the energy problem showed an unexpected influence on conservation. Households where both husband and wife did not believe there was a problem had, in fact, reduced their consumption more than households where both husbands and wives reported belief. The authors maintained that these results supported the idea that conservation tended to be based on economic reasons rather than moral perceptions of an energy problem. 35 Merkley (1980) conducted a secondary analysis of data collected during the evaluation of Pilot Project Conserve (see Harris, et a1., 1980). When testing for the influence of age related factors on conservation, Merkley found that the level of past experience with deprivation and shortages was a significant indicator of a proportional change in energy consumption. Thus households having mem- bers with higher levels of past experience with deprivation (the depression years, world wars) were conserving more than households with less experience. Merkley concluded that two factors might account for this finding. First, she suggested that persons previously having experience with a deprivation have the knowledge and skills to cut back. Secondly she suggested that these persons may be conserving in order to forstall a crisis. Voluntarnyimplicity Participation Voluntary simplicity has been identified as a lifestyle alternative which would involve the sub- stitution of human energy for fossil fuel energy. Based on five values this lifestyle integrates both social and economic decision making into the definition of goals and the use of resources. The term voluntary simplicity was originally coined in 1936 by Richard Gregg to describe a lifestyle involving a singleness of purpose; inward sincerity and honesty; and avoidance of excessive material possessions. Its background 36 is in religious and spiritual philosophies (Gregg, 1977). According to Gregg, voluntary simplicity requires the management of resources from a quality of life based on material goods to one based on a philosophy of life: It means an ordering and guiding of our energy and our desires, a partial restraint in some directions in order to secure greater abundance of life in other directions. It involves a deliberate organization of life for a purpose (Gregg, 1977 p. 20). Gregg identified some basic reasons for a life of simplicity. His first was improvement of the economic system. Gregg maintained that control over individual consumption would act as a deterrent to the greedy and com- petitive nature of our present system of production and distribution. Production of material luxuries requires the use of labor, capital, and raw materials which could be used toward greater social ends. Gregg noted that such production involved the exploitation of humans through price increases, lowered real wages, and excessive labor on the part of the poor to make up for economic losses. He also maintained that participation in the production of luxury items led to frequent unemployment in times of consumer fluctuations and economic depression. In addition, Gregg suggested that material simpli- city was a factor in the political greatness of men such as Lenin, Gandhi, and Buddah. The simplicity of their lives, he maintained, identified the leader with others of small material wealth. This contributed to the self-respect of 37 persons within the society through sharing in the leader's greatness. Self-respect is the basis for a morality achieved through expression of basic values. Elgin and Mitchell (1977) drew from Gregg to iden- tify five basic values on which voluntary simplicity as a lifestyle is said to be based. These values include mater- ial simplicity, human scale, self-determination, ecological awareness, and personal growth. Simplification of material life is based on consumption of goods through examination of the contribution of the good in relation to the four other values. For example, when deciding on the purchase of a new item, its value would be assessed based on its contribution to self-reliance and common social good; and cooperation with rather than control of the natural environ- ment. It is important to note that material simplicity does not mean poverty (Gregg, 1977; Elgin and Mitchell, 1977). On the contrary, the concept of voluntary simpli- city requires that basic needs be met; otherwise an environ- ment for personal growth is missing. Elgin and Mitchell (1977) described material sim— plicity from the perspective that possessions must contri- bute to personal growth. Human scale and self— determination values lead to the goal that living and working environments become human rather than institution controlled. The self-determined individual may seek material sufficiency by producing his/her own goods for 38 consumption. A greater connection can then be identified between work and the contribution of that work to society as a whole. Mitchell and Elgin defined ecological awareness as an acknowledgement of the "interconnectedness and inter- dependence of people and resources." Also involved is the awareness of the finiteness of physical resources. Equally important, however, is the awareness and concern for those less fortunate resulting in a sense of social responsibility. When characterizing those persons living a lifestyle of voluntary simplicity Elgin and Mitchell first noted that there are degrees of participation. Full voluntary simpli- Icity is followed by only about three percent of the popula- tion. Partial participation involves about twice as many persons. A third group is made up of individuals who sym— pathize with the goals of voluntary simplicity but for one or more reasons do not participate. Finally, there is the group that is indifferent, unaware or opposed to the life— style. Specific characteristics of the participants as identified by Elgin and Mitchell included well educated, white males and females from middle or upper class back— grounds. They are most likely in their twenties or thirties and predominantly single. Incomes tended to be bimodal with students making up the greatest portion of the group under $5,000. 39 Leonard-Barton and Rogers (1980) designed an 18- item measure of voluntary simplicity behaviors. They selected items which were most directly related to conser- vation and therefore intended to also relate to three of the five values significant to voluntary simplicity: material simplicity, self-determdnation, and ecological awareness. Several of the questions were derived from behaviors identified previously by Elgin and Mitchell. The scale progressed through three stages to reach its most recent 18-item format. Data provided by Leonard- Barton and Rogers concerning use of the scale were based on the responses of 812 California homeowners to these 18 questions. The authors subjected the scale to factor analysis and regression analysis in order to simplify its contents for future needs. Six factors emerged from factor analysis and were characterized by Leonard-Barton and Rogers as biking, self- suffiCiency in services, recycling of resources (metals, glass), self-sufficiency through making goods, recycling ‘ of durable goods (clothing, furniture), and closeness with nature. The scale items were also regressed on the total voluntary simplicity scores for each respondent. Once again data were based on the responses of the 812 California home- owners. Ninety percent of the variance was accounted for by nine of the scale items. 40 Three studies were conducted using the question- naire at various stages of its deveIOpment. In the spring of 1977, a nine question measure was administered to 215 Palo Alto, California homeowners. A three county study was conducted during the spring of 1979 using an expanded l9-item scale. Half of this sample included households with recently installed solar equipment. During the fall of 1979, the 18-item scale previously described was admin- istered to the 812 homeowners throughout California. Leonard-Barton and Rogers reported that the tendency toward participation in voluntary simplicity accounted for five percent of the variance in predicting conservation practices such as adding insulation or weather- stripping. The complete regression model based on the 1977 data accounted for 22 percent of the variance, thus volun- tary simplicity behaviors actually contributed to one— fourth of the total variance accounted for in the model. Analysis of data collected during the statewide study resulted in low but significant correlation with two variables: turning off the furnace pilot light during the summer and weatherstripping. The authors maintained that these findings indirectly related participation in voluntary simplicity to reduced energy consumption. While no direct significant relationship was determined between voluntary simplicity behaviors and natural gas usage, the connection was made 41 based on the relationship of voluntary simplicity to energy conserving behaviors which in turn were considered predic- tive of gas usage. Leonard-Barton and Rogers also investigated the relationship between voluntary simplicity behaviors and various demographic variables. They found the relationship between voluntary simplicity and income to be only slightly curvilinear. The income variable was trichotomized: High ($26,000+), Medium ($16-25,000) and Low ($15,000 or less). Mean scores of the middle income group on voluntary simpli- city was only slightly higher, 38.2, as compared to 35.9 and 35.8 for low and high income families, respectively. Education was found to have a low yet significant correlation (r=.16; p=.001) to voluntary simplicity scores. Specific items related to education included biking, recycling paper and glass, taking classes to increase self- reliance and contribution to ecological organizations. Education and income were correlated, yet education was related to voluntary simplicity while income was not, thus the authors maintained that the data were supportive of the premise that voluntary simplicity behaviors were indeed voluntary. A skill used to produce a good or make a repair can frequently be used as a substitute for paid services. The majority of voluntary simplicity behaviors require possession of some basic skills. Leonard-Barton and Rogers 42 found significant relationships between voluntary simpli— city and mechanical ability. With an underlying assumption that a large portion of the migrants to rural northern California areas would be persons participating in voluntary simplicity behaviors, Hackett and Schwartz (1980) studied 39 such individuals. Their sample was selected from five areas: three communes, two semi-isolated settlements, and one small village. Sub- jects in larger communities were chosen randomly. In less populated communities respondents were found through referral of other respondents. Hackett and Schwartz reported that the alternative lifestyle participants in their sample were primarily in their late twenties or early thirties. Four were familiar with rural life; most tended to come from predominantly middle-class urban backgrounds. Overall interviews de- tected general themes of concern about simplicity of material goods and importance of personal relationships. Self-reliance was important but the emphasis seemed to be geared towards a cooperative self-reliance. Income for these persons was a result primarily of part—time wage earning jobs. Some of the persons inter- viewed were receiving assistance from other family members or from government agencies in the form of unemployment insurance, social security disability, food stamps, or subsidized medical services. 43 Home production was an economically important aspect of the lifestyle. Self—sufficiency in building their own home was more important to this sample than growing food. Only ten percent of the sample produced over 60 percent of their food needs for summer. Considerable emphasis was placed on recycling. Frequently homes were built from goods salvaged. Seventy-five percent of the homes cost under $1,000 to build. Few of these homes were insulated; most were without plumbing. Service by utilities was infrequent due to both the economic cost of installation and the phil— osophical premise that utility companies are supportive of nuclear power. Within the communes, the sharing of appliances was common. This resulted in a considerably-lower portion of sampled households reporting ownership of appliances when compared to ownership by low income households reported in other studies. The researchers attempted to obtain data on house- hold use of energy. The four major available sources of energy included prOpane, kerosene, gasoline, and wood. Data were based on self reports. The average yearly use of these fuels totaled 16.4 million British thermal units. 2 the authors When comparing this use to a nationwide sample noted that these voluntary simplicity households used approximately half the amount of energy used by poor families. 2Newman and Day (1975:90) 44 Human Resource Availability Changes in an economy through a decline in wage rates, fewer work opportunities, scarcity of goods, or changed expectations of the consumer results in adjustments in consumption and production (Strumpel, 1976; Katona, 1972; Caplovitz, 1979; and Elder, 1974). Money income is one such resource. When a "shortage" of income occurs, house- holds have been found to make attempts to raise income, save less, reduce market consumption, and increase house- hold production. A reduction of expenditures and attempts to generate alternate or supplementary sources of income were responses to loss of income during the depression (Elder, 1974). Possible new income sources included the entry of mother into the labor market, money from relatives or boarders, and public assistance. Reduction of expenditures involved a change to a more labor intensive household and a reassess- ment of consumption norms. Elder maintained that successful adaptation to impacts of the depression was dependent on other household resources including motivation and familial and environmental support systems. Socioeconomic status and intelligence were identi- fied as family characteristics which contributed to moti- vational level. Elder suggested that the socioeconomic status of a family influenced the image of self, problem solving skills, and a sense of competence. Lower class families have more firsthand experience with the lessons of 45 economic hardships, however, Elder maintained that middle class families provided a greater range of problem solving skills and knowledge of a greater range of alternatives. Elder further contended that motivation to act only led to frustration if skills to act were lacking by the individual or family. Some inconsistency can be noted in the motivation of families to adapt during the depression. Income, occu- pation, and educational level were found to contribute to problem solving skills and recognition of alternative avenues of adaptation. These same motivators, however, frequently created greater status quo rather than coopera- tive efforts and reduced consumption. This phenomenon occurred most frequently in middle income families. Caplovitz (1979) explored the coping strategies of families according to their rankings as victims of infla— tion and recession. An objective measure was developed by income class and perceptions concerning the ability of income to keep up with rising prices. Caplovitz reported that over half of the 1,982 families interviewed in 1976 indicated that they were worse off financially than in previous years. Approximately ten percent reported being unemployed. Social characteristics of families most affected included the poor, the semi- skilled and unskilled, the poorly educated, the blacks and Spanish speaking. 46 In assessing the use of coping strategies according to financial impact of inflation, Caplovitz identified five strategies: income raising, reduced expenditures, bargain- hunting, greater self-reliance, and sharing with others. Income raising involved one or more family members spending additional hours employed.outside the home. Various other activities included performing odd jobs (frequently not reported on income tax forms) or making investments. Families curtailed expenses by changing consumption patterns for food, entertainment, clothing, vacations, and transpor- tation. Self-reliance was achieved by making more repairs, saving money, and discovering unknown talents. Sharing included receiving and offering help among friends. The proportion of families participating in each strategy increased as their objective financial status decreased. When controlling for income distribution the proportion of households participating in the various strategies was negatively related to income. Income raising was an exception to this pattern. Caplovitz sug-' gested that the ability to increase income is not a matter of choice but rather one of opportunity. In a review of research, Strumpel (1976) pointed out factors which influenced success or failure in accommo- dating to economic shifts. Strumpel first noted that long term adjustment occurs slowly. Female participation in 47 the labor force can increase the supply of money to the family, thus at least initially eliminating the need to cut back. Young families especially those with children re- quired the greatest amount of consumer goods and services. Along with families in the lower socioeconomic strata they reported the least flexibility to consume fewer goods. In addition, poorer families tended to be less confident of their abilities to counteract economic crises. Perlman and Warren (1977) noted the importance of material and nonmaterial resources in maintaining the family's capacities of integration, adaptability, and cognitive competence. Material resources were listed as housing, money, automobiles, insulation and so on. Included as nonmaterial resources were knowledge, skills, informa— tion, human energy, self-esteem, and kinship supports. Adaptability refers to the ability of families to make resource substitutions. Cognitive competence involves the information processing of decision making. Perlman and Warren maintained that the most impor- tant function of families is to meet the needs of family members by performing a variety of tasks. To perform these tasks families must call upon both their material and nonmaterial resources. It is assumed that families vary in their pool of resources and therefore differ in the con— straints and opportunities within a problem situation. 48 As an example, Perlman and Warren looked at the importance of income as a resource which influenced adapta- tion to pressures of energy shortage. The authors noted that while all income groups reported price as a motivator to reduce consumption of energy, the availability of money was an important factor in the family's flexibility within the problem. Perlman and Warren also noted that shortage of gasoline--another material resource--resulted in consider- able changes in tasks performed inside and outside the home. Numerous families reduced their use of the car and use of household appliances. Twoéthirds of the families reported that if the energy crisis kept them at home it would be a positive outcome. One-third, however, reported that staying at home would increase tension. Case and Harris (1980) studied the influence of material and nonmaterial resources on reported household conservation actions. Their model of analysis assumed that a household's effectiveness in performing various conservation actions was dependent upon the availability of a variety of resources. Analysis of these data found meaningful breakpoints for education and income. They reported that households with high school education or less reported fewer conserva- tion actions than those households having at least some college. Households with incomes below $10,000 reported fewer conservation behaviors than higher income households. 49 They also found that household structures had in— teresting relationships to reported conservation action. Two adult households reported more actions than single or multiple (more than two) adult households. Percentage of children in the home showed a positive significant rela— tionship to conservation behaviors. In terms of household resources, Case and Harris reported that money available for energy conservation devices, home repair skills available, and two adults in the household showed strongest associations with energy conserving behaviors. Knowledge of actions had a negative relationship to actually taking the action. Using path analysis to observe indirect effects the data showed that rural location had a positive relationship with conservation. This relationship was explained in terms of rural households being more dependent on more ex- pensive heating fuels, having higher skills, and less knowledge of conservation actions. Education and income were found to have a contradictory association with inter- vening variables. Education was reported to have a posi- tive relationship to attitudes and fuel type but a nega- tive relationship to skills. Income was positively related to house type, skill level and knowledge but showed a negative relationship with attitudes, fuel type, and number of adults. Thus, it can be seen that house- holds with specific characteristics positive to energy 50 conserving behaviors may in fact, have other characteris— tics which more or less constrain behavior change. As part of the analysis of data for the evaluation of Pilot Project Conserve (1980), the researchers looked at the relationship between resources and resulting energy conservation. Resources included in the analysis were knowledge of conservation actions, accuracy of estimated savings possible through conservation, education, income, ability to afford conservation device, number of occupants, health reasons, expected difficulty in obtaining materials or installation of conservation device. The six best bivariate predictors were included in a multivariate re- gression analysis computed against a composite energy con- servation action scale. Health, number of occupants, accuracy of estimated savings, number of suggestions, effectiveness of suggestions, and education were found to explain eight percent of the variance. A similar analysis was done for motivational factors. Although many of the attitudinal and situational motivators were positively related to taking conservation actions, only four percent of the variance was accounted for. The model included ecoconsciousness, presence of children, belief in the energy problem, rurality, presence of married adults, percentage of household income spent on heating fuel. The final report also included an assessment of the affect of the conservation actions on actual reduced 51 consumption. Addition of wall insulation was the only single measure which was significant at or below the .05 level. Using the five "best" indicators from this re- gression together with participation in the project accounted for four percent of the variance in the per- centage change in energy consumption. Gurin and Gurin (1976) discussed the relevancy of internal-external control to economic attitudes and beha— vior. The authors used the terms personal efficacy and control ideology. Both were defined as bases of expec- tancy referring to individual perceptions over what con- trolled individual goal achievement. Personal efficacy reflects the extent to which individuals perceive them- selves in control. External control reflects as power- less to affect change. The authors maintained that the distinction between personal (internal) control and con- trol ideology (external) was predictive of behavior and attitudes. The authors performed a factor analysis of res- ponses to a variety of questions concerning bases of expectancy, achievement, and interpersonal trust. Six factors resulted, two of which fit the described personal and control ideologies. The authors then compared the factors on several classes of variables: social and economic status; goals and values; and personal and general 52 economic reactions and behaviors. For purposes of this review, I will focus only on findings related to personal control. Multiple classification was used to determine the contribution of demographic characteristics to personal control. Income contributed the most to the variance and had a beta weight of .24. Occupation, education, and race contributed in that order. The authors determined that feelings of control were related to the actual economic resources under one's control. Gurin and Gurin assumed that a sense of personal control would result in more successful income management. They defined income management as the ability to live within one‘s income and manage to save a portion of that income. Findings confirmed this hypothesis. The authors reported that individuals with greater personal efficacy are less concerned about external crises and feel that their economic well-being is in a large part dependent upon their own efforts. It was also reported that those individuals with a sense of personal control were more willing to take risks to get ahead. Belief in personal control was not, however, found to be related to a sense that inflation and recession are controllable. Neither did personal control respondents want government to inter- fere into personal economics. The inability of the data to show strong relation- ships between personal efficacy and an individual's own 53 personal economic situation caused the authors to question whether individuals were maintaining a general belief that they were responsible for success yet had lost a sense of control over their own lives. The authors were especially concerned as to what impact this would have. Looking specifically at the interaction between a sense of personal efficacy and various objective factors Gurin and Gurin reported that the introduction of status indicators reduced the effect of personal efficacy in the research previOusly mentioned. Thus the interaction of environmental and psychological factors is considered quite important in that while objective conditions mediate the impact of psychological factors so do the psychologi- cal factors mediate the impact of objective factors. In summary, two factors emerge from the literature on motivation towards energy conservation: income and values. Household income represents the ability of the household to purchase energy either directly for such uses as heating the home or gasoline for transportation or in- directly in the form of goods and services. If income is scarce, the household's ability to make purchases is lowered. If the price of energy becomes consistently higher and the household income does not increase propor- tionately, the household is less able to purchase energy at the same rate without substituting income which might be used in another way. In either case, conservation of energy may occur. 54 On the other hand, if the household members possess attitudes or values which recognize present patterns of energy use as inequitable or potentially harmful to society, that household may alter its behavior in order to maintain its value base. The ability of a household to act upon its motivation, however, is dependent upon the availability of resources which may be substituted for present forms of energy. This review has focused upon human resources. One such resource is human energy which can be directly substituted for fossil fuels under some circumstances. Other human resources such as knowledge, a sense of per- sonal control, and perceptions of time and money may facil- itate adaptation to a less energy intensive lifestyle. The overall assumption made is that decisions determined by households concerned with management of resources will determine behavior patterns of households. Ideally, behavior patterns are reflective of lifestyle or value preferences. As a result, these decisions will determine the type of energy consumed and the level of consumption. CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY AND HYPOTHESIZED RELATIONSHIPS The purpose of this study was to determine the in- fluence of financial and philosophical motivators on direct and indirect conservation of energy. Direct conservation was determined through an analysis of change in actual consumption patterns. A measure of indirect conservation would necessarily involve considerable monitoring of change in household's purchases and participation in the home pro— duction of goods and services. For purposes of this re- search a measure of voluntary simplicity was obtained and used as an indicator of indirect conservation. In addition, the study was designed to assess the influence of various human resources on the performance of conservation behaviors. Data used for the analysis were collected during the evaluation of "Project Conserve." Project Conserve is a computerized energy audit designed to provide indivi- dualized information to households regarding potential energy conservation. An evaluation of Project Conserve was designed to determine the influence of conservation information 55 56 provided in the form of a computerized printout. The evaluation was conducted by a team of researchers at the Institute for Family and Child Study at Michigan State University. The evaluation was designed to include two tele- phone interviews. The interviews were conducted during the spring of 1979 and the fall of 1980. The telephone interviews were limited to approximately twenty minutes. In order to ask all of the needed questions and to include data on household structural characteristics for groups not returning the audit form, an additional questionnaire was mailed to all households which participated in the first telephone interview. Also during the first tele- phone interview respondents were asked if they would be willing to sign a permission form allowing the release of actual household consumption data from appropriate utility and oil companies. Data were then requested for those households, having signed the permission forms, for the period from June 1977 through June 1980. Both follow-up interviews asked questions related to attitudes, demographic characteristics, and adoption of energy conserving measures. The 1979 telephone interview was contracted out to Neal and Associates in Ann Arbor, Michigan. The 1980 interviews were conducted by Detroit Marketing Service in Detroit, Michigan. Both companies were responsible for 57 providing raw data tapes. The tapes were checked by the research team for correctness. Research Subsample Selection and Description The primary goal of this research was to explore the relationship between various motivators and human resources on a household's pattern of direct and indirect conservation of energy. One adult from each household was considered the unit of observation. The household was selected as the unit of analysis. For a household to be included in the research subsample it was required that sufficient data be available to adequately measure direct and indirect conservation. This resulted in two primary criteria. First, it was necessary for a household member to have completed a series of eleven questions developed to assess participation in voluntary simplicity. As noted previously, participation in such behaviors is said to be related to a less materialistically oriented philosophy and thus less consumption of energy—intensive goods and services. Data were considered adequate if no greater than 20 percent (two) questions were left unanswered. The voluntary simplicity questions were asked of respon- dents in 924 households. Of this group, there were no households which were unable to meet the criterion. The second criterion for selection in the sub— sample was concerned with the completeness of household 58 energy consumption data for July 1977 through June 1978 and July 1979 through June 1980. Permission to obtain this information was requested of the total evaluation sample.3 Permission was given by 52.1 percent. Households from which no permission was granted were eliminated from the subsample. For most cases in which the consumption data were available, it was complete for both years required. In some instances, however, it was necessary to extrapolate consumption for intermittent periods which were missing. Extrapolation procedures resulted in conservative esti- mates of actual consumption. Complete explanation of the extrapolation procedure is given in the Final Report of Pilot Project Conserve (Harris et a1., 1980). The data base contained 638 households meeting both criteria. These households were selected as the re- search subsample for the analysis in this report. Basic demographic characteristics of the subsample are reported in Tables 1 and 2. For purposes of generalizability the same descriptive characteristics are provided for all households within the State of Michigan. Demographics were collected on the research subsample in 1979, data for the State of Michigan are reported for the latest date available. In the research sample, the household member res- ponding to the interview questions was primarily 3Permission was actually requested for a three year period from June 1977 to July 1980. Only the first and last year were included in the analysis for this study. 59 middle-aged; nearly 60 percent fell between the ages of 35 and 64. Less than half the respondents had attended college. Eighty percent, however, had at least a high school education. Slightly more than half were female. When compared to the State of Michigan the research sample consisted of a similar distribution of males and females. Age and educational level appeared somewhat higher in the sample than in the population as a whole. Ninety-seven percent of the respondents owned their homes. Sixty percent of the households were located in small to medium sized cities or towns; approximately 30 percent were located in the country and ten percent in large cities. Households in the research subsample consisted of an average of 3.2 members. Nearly 70 percent (67.7) of the households had two adult members; one-fifth had more than two adults. Eight percent were single adult house- holds. Children were found in 48.1 percent of the house- holds. Just under half of the households had one income earner. Twenty-seven percent were two income families. Nearly one-fifth had no employed members. The median household income was $18,709. When compared to the state population, household characteristics of the research sample were somewhat dif— ferent. Home ownership was more prevalent in the research sample because the evaluation sample of the Statewide 60 Project Conserve overrepresented single family residences. Income level was slightly lower perhaps due to the nonres- ponse of reported income for nearly ten percent of the research sample. Household structure was not greatly different, yet tended to underrepresent single parent households. Seventy percent of the households used natural gas for heating; another 19 percent used fuel oil. Electricity, wood, and propane, in that order, completed the heating fuel types used by households. The larger percentage of households within the research sample which were heated by electricity and wood when compared to heating fuels used by households throughout the State of Michigan was reflec- tive of the shift in household heating fuels which occurred during the 19705. Measurement Procedures Dependent and independent variables were developed from questions asked during the evaluation of Statewide Project Conserve. Specifically those variables used as predictors of conservation were based on data collected in 1979. Dependent measures of voluntary simplicity and personal control were each develOped from questions asked during the 1980 telephone interviews. The measure of con- servation was collected over a three year period from June of 1977 through June of 1980. 61 Table 1. Selected Household Characteristics - Comparison of Research Subsample, 1978, and Michigan Households Michigan Respondent Households Research Characteristics (in thousands) Subsample 100% 100% (N=638) Gender of Respondentl (1976) Female 52.9 51.3 Male 47.1 48.7 Age of Respondent2 (1970) Less than 25 8.1 3.2 25 to 34 21.6 19.6 35 to 44 18.0 17.8 44 to 64 35.2 39.2 65 and older 17.0 20.4 Missing -- 0.2 Education Level3 (1970) Less than High School 47.2 19.7 High School 33.7 41.1 Some College 14.8 30.9 Graduate Work 4.3 8.3 1State Data Source, Andrews, M.P. and Boger, R.P. (Eds.), Michigan Family Sourcebook, University, 1980. Michigan State 2State Data Source, Verway, David I., Michigan Statistical Abstracts, 1979. Table I-l6, p. 62. 3State Data Source, Verway, David I., Michigan Statistical Abstracts, 1979. Table IV-2, p. 150. (Based on adults age 25 or older.) 62 Table 2. Selected Household Characteristics. Comparison of Research Subsample, 1978, and Michigan Households. Michigan Household Characteristics Households Subsample 100% (Year) 100% (Year) Number of Household Occupantsl (1970) One 8.0 Two 33.4 Three or Four 38.1 Five or More 20.5 Average 3.27 3.2 Home Ownership2 (1976) Own 76.0 97.2 Rent 24.0 2.4 Missing -- 0.5 Household Income Level3 (1979) Under $ 5,000 5.7 5.8 $ 5,000 - $ 9,999 12.3 13.5 $10,000 - $14,999 15.0 12.2 $15,000 - $19,999 18.8 18.2 $20,000 - $24,999 19.0 16.9 $25,000 - $29,999 11.9 8.3 Over $30,000 17.3 15.2 Missing -- 9.9 Median $19,500 $18,709 Household Structure4 Two Adults with Children 31.4 35.3 Two Adults - No Children 34.5 32.4 Female with Children 10.9 0.6 Male with Children 2.2 0.8 More than Two Adults with Children -- 11.4 More than Two Adults - No Children -- 11.4 Single Adult 20.9 8.0 Fuel Used for HeatingS (1970) Natural Gas 69.9 71.2 Electricity 2.5 4.1 Propane 3.6 2.2 Fuel Oil 22.0 18.8 Wood 0.2 3.8 Coal, Other 2.2 -- 1State Data Source, Verway, David I. Michigan Statistical Abstract, 1979. Table I-l7, p. 63. 2State Data Source, Verway, David I. Michigan Statistical Abstract, 1979. Table II-l, p. 81. 30.8. Bureau of Census, Current POpulation Reports, 1979. 4State Data Source 5State Data Source, Housing Characteristics for State, Cities, and Counties, Vol. I, Part 24, Michigan 1970. Census of Housing, Bureau of Census, 1972. 63 Measures of Conservation The primary objective of this research was to ex- plore direct and indirect conservation of fossil fuel energy within households. Direct conservation of energy was measured by percentage reduction in consumption. In— direct conservation was measured based on self—reported participation in a set of behaviors defined as voluntary simplicity. The first research question was specifically con- cerned with the percentage of households participating in conservation behaviors: 1. With what frequency are households reducing direct consumption of fossil fuel energy and with what frequency are they participating in behaviors related to voluntary simplicity? An explanation of the development of each conser- vation measure is provided in this section. In addition, discussion of the analysis based on the first research question is also provided here. Analysis of the first research question was intended to be exploratory and therefore no hypotheses were tested. Frequencies, per— centages, and mean scores are reported for percentage change in direct household consumption of fossil fuel energy (Table 3) and for participation in voluntary sim- plicity behaviors (Tables 4 and 5). 64 Percentage Change in Consumption. Actual consumption of energy within the household was determined by obtaining utility company records for the years 1977-78 and 1979-80. Consumption data were initially collected and coded by month. Extrapolations were computed based on heating degree days if a month was required to complete the data. The monthly consumption of each fuel was summed for each year to provide an annual consumption figure. Measures of natural gas, electricity, prOpane, and fuel oil were converted to British thermal units (Btus) to facilitate computations and analysis. The following conversion factors as reported by Newman and Day (1975) were used: Natural Gas - 1,031 Btus per cubic foot Electricity — 3,412.8 Btus per kilowatt hour Fuel Oil - 138,800 Btus per gallon Propane - 21,000 Btus per pound The number of Btus of each fuel used within the household was added together to determine the total Btu consumption. This provided a precise measurement of the household's annual consumption. Yearly fluctuations in temperature may result in varying fuel requirements used for space heating. Space heating has been identified as the largest single end use of energy within the home; therefore, it is necessary to control for changing requirements. The number of annual heating degree days was used to standardize yearly con— sumption figures. 65 The resulting figures were used to compute per- centage change in annual consumption. This was accom- plished by dividing the difference in consumption between 1977-78 and 1979-80 by consumption during 1977-78. This measure provided an estimate of conservation relative to previous consumption. Summary data of the percentage change in consumption are presented in Table 3. Table 3. Percentage Change in Household Consumption 1977-78, 1979-80 Level of Change (N) Percentage Greater than 4% 232 36.4 Reduction 4% Reduction to 4% Increase 232 36.4 Greater than 4% Increase 174 27.2 638 100.0 Mean - .018 Standard Deviation .156 Standard Error .006 Findings One—third of the research subsample (36.4 percent) had reduced their household consumption of energy more than four percent. An increase in consumption of greater than four percent was observed in 27.2 percent of the households. The remainder of households were within i four percent of their first year's consumption. The mean 66 change of -.018 indicated that the average household was realizing slightly less than a two percent reduction in direct energy consumption. Voluntary Simplicity Participation. Structured questions developed by Leonard-Barton and Rogers (1980) to assess the extent to which persons were practicing behaviors related to a philosophy of voluntary simplicity were used in this study as a dependent variable measuring household voluntary simplicity participation. The questions were based on a survey of households claiming voluntary simpli- city participation which was implemented by Elgin and Mitchell. Leonard—Barton and Rogers provided an 18-item Likert measure which they subjected to factor analysis and regression analysis in order to provide researchers with a streamlined measure. For purposes of this research 11 of the 18 questions were used. Nine questions were chosen which were shown through regression analysis to account for 90 percent of the total voluntary simplicity score variance. The remaining two variables were selected based on the results of the factor analysis conducted by Leonard—Barton and Rogers. Two factors of interest to this research were self-sufficiency in goods and self- sufficiency in services. The final two questions com- pleted the inclusion of all variables which loaded on these factors at .3 or greater. These questions, resulting frequencies and percentages can be found in Table 4. 67 Bocx u.:oa m>m= um>oz 30: uo: usn own adamconmooo xaco 0:0 who: no 039 mcoHumuflcmmuo HmoamoHoom 09 ousnfiuucoo m.o mcflmmflz m.mm HQ>OZ m.v~ 36m m.e~ meow ~.ea see: H.e umoz va mwsuwcnsm no aceruoac one: m.H schema: m.vm um>mz n.mm >Hamcoflmmooo ~.a~ haucmsvmuh ~.m sneeze: m.m msezaa means one: meooo an Noceaoeuusmuuamm ~.o mafimmfiz m.em um>mz >.oa nucoe a 00:0 m.ow um>mz N.m xooz m 0020 ~.~ mono m>m= o.am x003 Hum w.m muses Hmum>om moEwB Hmuw>om m.m moses xcmz m.vm wmpxuw>m m.vm manfimmom wm>ocw£3 va :kmv wmfloumxm MOM mHQNon mmofi>umm a mpooo mmctcoxm m.o m.o mcflmmflz H.wm «.mm uu>oz N.~H e.ma sane:0nmeooo m.a >.e Sancwsvoua m.m e.e seamed: e.~e e.me m>e3e< Am. Awe mmauuomlmumh mmmau mummmmmBCZ mCHHo>oom ~.e acumen: v . OM 0202 m . mm 050% H.m wcmz m.m> oz >.ma who: ~.o~ mo» «.ma HH< Amv mafia umomEoo o>mm Ann I: moanmuomm> paw muflswm 30pm UOOE :H wocofluflmmsmlmawm m.hv H®>®z v.ma mamscmz I xoom o.ma mpcmflwm Eoum >HHMCOMmmooO m.m~ upcmflum Eoww hauccsmmum H.v mmcao mco o.c~ mmmao mco cps» muoz va maaflxm CH noduosuumcH v o mafimmflz v mm wo>mz H.w xfiamcoflmmooo o w >Hucoskum m m seamed: o.em msezaa Awe ucu >HHEmm :H HA0 omcmcu meM>u®m :H Nocmflofimwsmlwaom Ammmuzv .moamomnsm >2 pomsouo .muofi>mnom xufloaamawm humucsao> CM cofiummwowuuwm mcfiuHOQQm mpaocomso: mo ommucoouom .4 dance 68 Responses on individual items were recoded in order that a response which indicated no participation received a score of zero; a response indicating the highest level of participation received a four. The over- all voluntary simplicity index was computed by summing the recoded raw scores on individual items and dividing by the total number of questions answered. The resulting index had a range from zero to four. This final score was cate- gorized to indicate the distribution of the sample among levels of participation. Summary data are presented in Table 5 which indicates a moderate level of participation. Reliability of the voluntary simplicity scale was tested. Chronbach's alpha of .59 indicated a moderate level of reliability. Deletion of any item did not im- prove the reliability. In addition to having a moderate level of relia- bility, the items chosen for inclusion in the total index were considered conceptually meaningful. For further refinement, however, the 11 questions on voluntary simpli— city were subjected to factor analysis. Factor analysis is a statistical technique used to examine interrelation— ships among variables with the objective of representing them in a smaller number of hypothetical variables (Kim and Mueller, 1978). 69 The measure of voluntary simplicity used in this research was determined through factor analysis4 to in- clude behaviors related to self—sufficiency in services, self-sufficiency in goods, self-sufficiency in food, recycling, and contributions to ecological organizations. Factor loadings greater than .300 resulted in the inclu- sion of item within the factor (Table 6). Measures for each factor were constructed by sum- ming the scores of each variable relevant to the specific factor. Prior to summing, the raw scores were multiplied by the regression coefficient (factor loadings) in order to appropriately weight the influence of the variable to the factor score. As with the total index, the factor scores were categorized into levels of participation in order to facilitate discussion. Frequencies, percentages, and mean scores are reported for the total voluntary sim— plicity scale and for each of the five subscales in Table 5. Findings. The mean score for the voluntary sim— plicity scale was 1.509, based on a possible range of zero to four. No respondent received the maximum score; 3.455 was the highest total voluntary simplicity score achieved. Approximately one-fifth (21.5 percent) of the respondents indicated frequent participation in voluntary 4Factor loadings from varimax rotation were used in computing the subscales. Oblique rotation resulted in delineation of similar factors. 7O simplicity behaviors by household members. The majority, 54.1 percent, reported occasional participation, indicating that a very large percentage of the households were parti- cipating in at least some voluntary simplicity behaviors-- some of the time. Another 23.7 percent reported infre- quent participation while less than one percent of the respondents reported that no one in their household had performed any voluntary simplicity behaviors. An analysis of the subscales provided insight into Specifically what types of behaviors were most frequently practiced. Slightly more than half of the sample reported frequent recycling. Two questions determined the scores for this category. The first asked the extent to which households recycled newspapers; the second asked the extent to which glass jars and bottles were recycled. Frequencies for both questions indicated that approxi— mately half of the sample usually or always recycled news- papers and glass jars or bottles. Approximately 13.5 per- cent indicated that they never recycled. Within the self—sufficiency in services subscale, results indicated that greater than one-third of the respondents indicated high levels of participation in producing services within the home. Less than ten per- cent reported that these behaviors were never performed. Over half (56 percent) of the respondents indicated that a household member always changed the oil in their car. Twenty-eight percent reported never doing this. 71_ I: m.e N.e e.H m.e .: seamen: m.>m m.ma 5.5m m.mH m.m w.o uo>oz e.H e.mn m.mm m.em m.am e.mm sabedsdmumaH e.e m.o~ «.ma e.m~ o.Hm H.em saaeeoemeooo e.e m.mm e.Hm s.mn N.mm m.am massed Ho waamSmD ucoonom CH COHUMQAOHuMmm mo Ho>oq Ammmuzv Ammmuzv Ammmnzv Ammmuzv Ammmuzv Ammonzv mcoflumeccmwo mafiaomoom poom mpooo mooa>uom muHOHHmEHm ou wumucsHo> coflusnfluusou :H xocofloammsmlwamm acuoe manomnsm can mamom acuoa mmamomnsm NDHOHHmEflm NHMDGSHO> momcpcoouom HMOHuommuno pan mmuoom cum: umH0fi>c£om huHOHHQEHm humucsao> ca coflummHOHuumm .m magma 72 Nmm. who. mvm. vmm. mom. man. mNm. Gav. ahm. vmm. mmmHm oaoxomm mummmmmzoc maomoom enhanced are: mumam one: mafia medEou moanmwomo> mam mpflsum SOHO oaomOHm opflm moofl>umm Ho mpoom omcmnoxm Haaxm CH cofluosuumcfl copuoo umo SHflEmw CH Hflo wmcmao moonsommm machoom mpoow soaoaoeuuemuuamm coon socmnoammsmamamm mmoa>uom suedeoeuusmamamm COHumoso muofl>msmm huHOHHmEHm Sumucsao> mo mammamc< Honocm .o wanes 73 Similarly, over half the respondents reported that they or some family member had gotten some type of instruction in skills either from classes, friends or instruction manuals. Slightly less than half (45.5 percent) reported riding a bicycle at least several times per week. One-fourth of the respondents indicated frequent exchange of goods and services while 60 percent reported that they had never exchanged goods or services. Self—sufficiency in foods was reported at a high level by only 31 percent of the sample respondents; 27.7 percent reported no participation. Self-sufficient be- havior in this category involved having a compost pile and growing fruits and vegetables for household consumption. Of the research sample 26.2 percent had a compost pile and 35.5 percent grew some fruits and vegetables. Another 33.9 percent grew some fruits and vegetables; 30.5 percent grew none of the fruits and vegetables eaten by household members. Self-sufficiency in goods was based on having made gifts, clothes, or furniture rather than having purchased them. Observation of individual questions showed that few respondents (13.5 percent) reported that their household always or usually made their own gifts. An additional 21.4 percent reported frequently making gifts while the majority of respondents (40.2 percent) reported occasional performance of this action. One—fourth of the respondents indicated that no gifts were ever made. A larger 74 proportion of respondents, 38.7 percent, reported that their household never made their own clothing or furniture. Forty-five percent indicated that they made some or at least a few small items. Another 16.3 percent reported having made many or most of their clothing or furniture. Observation of the total subscale indicated that the majority of households produced few of their own goods. A very small percentage of respondents indicated contribution to ecological organizations. Less than seven percent were actually contributing at the time surveyed. Another 6.1 percent gave occassionally or had previously done so. The majority, 87.3 percent, had never contri— buted to an ecological organization. Measures of Motivation Income Adequacy is conceptually defined in this research as the comparative ability of a household to make direct and indirect purchases of energy. The proportion of income required for lower income households to make energy purchases is greater than for families with higher incomes. As the market value of energy increases it would follow that lower income households would sense a greater stressor and possibly greater motivation to conserve. The use of income levels when explaining consump- tion change is valuable, yet, does not take into consider- ation the income needs of the household as determined by 75 family size, family life cycle stage, or geographical cost of living differences. The income measure used in this research took into account the previously mentioned needs to compute an objective adequacy ratio following the examples of Ackerman (1977) and Moen (1980). The questions concerned with income adequacy were asked during the first telephone interview conducted in June of 1979. The income adequacy variable was computed following the example of Ackerman (1977). The following information reported here is from Ackerman, pp. 51-53. 1. Each respondent was assigned a Bureau of Labor Statistics equivalency number which adjusted for a moderate level of living for different family composition. Variables used include: age of oldest child, number of children in household, number of adults in household, and age of primary income earner. 2. Each respondent was assigned a BLS number which adjusts the BLS standard budget for a moderate level of living for different geographic loca- tion. 3. The family income variable was recoded to dollar amounts. 4. The objective measure was computed via the following measure: 76 (Ifiecume Toufl.Famfly'Lxrme Adamrwy ==SumflaniBumfim. EamiurcamxsiERXiX<3xgnmxuc1xnadmm Rank) Mmknatelxwel Equhmdemaerdxm Enuhndemaermnm 100 100 Summary data of the income adequacy variable are presented in Table 7. Data indicated that the sample was relatively well distributed among adequacy levels. Household income level has been positively related to levels of direct and indirect consumption of energy. This relationship has been explained through the discussion of lifestyle decisions (Morrison and Gladhart, 1976; Gladhart, 1977). Therefore, higher income households, due to initially higher levels of consumption, could have the greatest flexibility for potential conservation. Lower Table 7. Household Income Adequacy Levels, 1979 Level of Adequacy (N) Percentage Very Low 92 14.4 Low 118 18.5 Medium 261 40.9 High 90 14.1 (Missing) _11 12.1 638 100.0 Mean 1.320 Standard Deviation .718 Standard Error .030 Actual Minimum — Maximum .122 to 3.895 77 income households would experience the greater stress from the increased prices of fossil fuel energy sources. It was hypothesized that they would be forced economically to reduce consumption in order to reduce their fuel bill. Based on research concerned with the relationship between income level and conservation and the research concerned with how families handle limited economic resources (Caplovitz, 1972; Elder, 1974) the following hypotheses were established: Hypothesis 1A Level of income adequacy will have a curvilinear relationship with the percentage change in direct household consumption of fossil fuels. Specifically households with low and high levels of income adequacy will reduce consumption more than house- holds in middle levels of income adequacy. Hypothesis 1B Level of income adequacy will be negatively related to participation in behaviors related to voluntary simplicity. Fuel Cost. Two additional variables were included in the regression analysis of percentage change in consump- tion. Fuels used for heating vary according to their abso- lute cost and according to the individual increase in the cost of each fuel type. It was determined that the cost of various fuel types and the relative increase in cost should be viewed as additional determinants of a household's income adequacy. Case and Harris (1980) regressed a conservation action score onto fuel type ordered by cost and found a 78 significant bivariate relationship. Using path analysis they determined that rural households, using more expensive fuels, tended to perform more conservation actions than urban counterparts. Fuel cost increases were used to determine the relative shift in prices between fuel types. Based on data provided by Keith (1981) fuel oil was given the highest rating, natural gas and propane second with electricity experiencing the least increase. These variables were included only in the regres— sion analysis of percent change in conservation. Both variables were hypothesized to have a positive relation- ship with reduced consumption. Hypothesis 2A Relative fuel cost and relative change in fuel cost will each be positively related to reduction in direct household consumption of fossil fuels. Philosophical Perspective. Philosophical perspec- tive represents aspects of the symbolic value of energy within society. Questions designed to elicit the respon- dent's philosophy towards energy were asked during the November 1980 telephone interview. Six questions were selected from among a variety of attitudinal questions pre- viously used in surveys conducted by the Family Energy Project at Michigan State University. The selected ques— tions have been previously defined as representative of ECOCOHSCIOUSHGSS values . 79 Each question was answered on a scale of strongly agree to strongly disagree (Appendix A). Answers were coded so that the responses receiving the highest code were those indicating the strongest ecoconsciousness value. An index was formed by summing the raw score for the responses to all questions for each respondent. This number was then divided by the total number of questions answered. A scale score was computed for all households which responded to at least four of the six questions. A total of 15 households (2.4 percent) did not meet this criterion. Chronbach's alpha reliability was computed for the scale. All six questions resulted in a reliability coefi- cient of .74349. Deletion of any item did not improve the alpha. The continuous score was categorized into levels which indicated agreement or disagreement with a strong ecoconsciousness value. Mean score and category frequen- cies are reported in Table 8. 80 Table 8. Household Scores on a Measure of Philosophical Energy Perspective, 1979 / Household Level (N) Percentage Strong Agreement 289 45.3 Agreement 324 50.8 Disagreement 9 1.4 Strong Disagreement l i 0.2 Missing ‘ 15 2.4 Mean 2.08 Standard Deviation .467 Standard Error .019 An ecoconsciousness value has been reported to have a positive relationship with taking actions related to technical and behavioral conservation (Hogan, 1976) and also with direct reduction of energy consumption within the household (Hungerford, 1978). The theoretical concep— tualization of a lifestyle of voluntary material simplicity is also based on a value system which supports a positive interaction between humans and environmental awareness (Leonard-Barton and Rogers, 1980; Gregg, 1977; Elgin and Mitchell, 1977). The following hypotheses were established: Hypothesis 3A Level of philosophical perspective will be positively related to a reduction in household energy consumption. Hypothesis 3B Level of philosophical perspective will be positively related to participation in behaviors related to voluntary simplicity. 81 Indicators of Human Resources Individuals and households have a variety of re- sources which are used to achieve goals. These resources may be tangible or intangible; human or nonhuman. One purpose of this research was to assess the influence of human resources on behaviors related to the conservation of fossil fuels. Nine human resources were identified from the list of research variables; eight are entered into the analysis as independent variables, one as a dependent variable. Explanation of the development of each human resource variable is included in this section. A summary of hypotheses regarding expected relationships of individual variables to percent change in household energy consumption and participation in voluntary simpli- city behaviors is also included. Mean scores and standard deviations for human resource measures which served as independent variables are provided in Table 10. Summary data for the human resource, personal control, are presented in Table 11. Knowledge of Conservation Actions was used as a measure of the extent to which respondents could identify efficient methods of direct conservation of fossil fuel energy. Respondents were asked the following questions in the June 1979 interview: 82 Let's talk about one area in which living costs can be reduced--ELECTRICITY AND HOME HEATING-- and let's think about ways these costs can be reduced without giving up basic comforts. Think of a friend who might ask you for advice on how to save on electricity and heating fuel bills. What are the most important things you would advise your friend to do? Respondents could mention up to six suggestions. A know- ledge score was created. The respondent was given two points for suggesting attic insulation, wall insulation, basement or crawl space insulation, storm windows or doors, thermopane windows, caulking or weatherstripping, flue restrictor, clock thermostat, lowering thermostat, closing off rooms, or solar heating. One point was received for each other suggestion including hot water heater insula- tion, covering windows with plastic, installing a fire- place cover, servicing heating system, and turning off pilot light during summer. The knowledge scale was created by dividing the total score by the number of responses; if no suggestions were offered a score of zero ' was assigned. Thus the conservation knowledge scale has a minimum of zero and a maximum of two. Promotional campaigns have frequently been based on providing information to consumers regarding conserva- tion practices and their benefits. Data have suggested that information on how to save energy is relatively in- effective if given alone (Gordon, 1980). Knowledge of conservation methods was not found predictive of actually 83 taking conservation actions (Case and Harris, 1980). Knowledge was not found predictive of conservation as re- ported in the final report of Pilot Project Conserve (Harris, et a1., 1980). In the last two studies neither bivariate nor multivariate analysis proved significant. For purposes of this research it was hypothesized that if motivation were accounted for, knowledge would be positively related to reduced household consumption of energy and participation involuntary simplicity behaviors. Average Household Education was used as a measure of the educational attainment of the principal adult(s) within the household. When there were two adults in the house- hold the average level of schooling was computed. If only one adult was present in the household, the level of educa- tion for that adult was used for the educational measure. Higher levels of education are frequently associated with higher levels of income adequacy. Case and Harris (1980) determined that education had a positive relation— ship with ecoconsciousness and therefore with a greater likelihood of performing conservation actions. The nega- tive relationship of education with skills, however, resulted in a contradictory relationship. Multivariate analysis of resources included education in a predictive model which accounted for eight percent of the variance in reduced consumption (Harris, et a1., 1980). 84 Elgin and Mitchell (1977) determined that parti- cipants of voluntary simplicity behaviors were well educa- ted. Leonard-Barton and Rogers found a low yet significant correlation of education with voluntary simplicity scores. For purposes of this research it was hypothesized that when motivation was accounted for, education would have a posi- tive relationship with reduced household consumption of energy and participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors. Reported Home Repair Skills was created to measure the extent to which household members appear capable to perform a variety of household repairs. The following questions were asked in the June 1979 telephone interview: Now I would like you to suppose that you needed to have some repair or remodeling work done on your present home. Which of the following tasks would you or a member of your household feel confident to do? ASSUME THAT YOU HAVE THE TIME AND THE MONEY TO DO THESE THINGS. I. How about putting together the frame of a wall with 2 x 45? 2. Put in or replace sewer and water pipes? 3. Replace a toilet or sink for an existing plumbing system? 4. Put up or replace dry wall on a frame of 2 x 48? 5. Install or replace an electric light fixture? 6. Install a new storm door? 7. Put insulation in the attic? 8. Do you think that you or a member of your household could cut open a wall to repair the plumbing, electrical work, or heating ducts, and return the wall back to its original condition? 85 Each variable was treated as dichotomous; any response other than "yes" was treated as "no." The number of responses "yes" were summed and made into a nine-point interval scale with zero indicating no skills. Possession of skills was found an important pre- dictor of taking conservation actions (Case and Harris, 1980). Home production of goods and services also requires a variety of skills. Elder (1972) found that households with a variety of skills were better able to c0pe with effects of the depression. For purposes of this research it was hypothesized that number of skills would be posi- tively related to both direct and indirect conservation. Perceived Money Availability was determined a sub- jective indicator of income adequacy. In other words, this variable was used as an indicator of how the house— hold perceived their money resources. The scale ranged from less than fifty dollars to over $2000 and was created from the following series of questions. Let's talk about some things that you might do in the future. As you know, most of the things that can be done to conserve energy--and to reduce your monthly bills—-require some kind of invest- ment at first. It takes a while before you can get your money back, and some peOple can afford these investments and others cannot. If an energy conservation device were devel- oped which would help you save a lot of energy, would you be able to buy such a device if its total cost were: 86 1. $ 50 2. $ 200 3. $ 500 4. $1000 5. $2000 Again each variable was treated as dichotomous; any response other than "yes" was treated as "no." The number of posi- tive responses was summed and treated as a six point inter- val scale with zero indicating no available financial resources. Perception of the amount of available money was hypothesized to have a positive relationship with direct conservation and a negative relationship with indirect conservation. Perceived Time Availability was considered an indi- cator of the amount of time respondents felt they had to install an energy conservation device. The scale ranged from less than five hours to fifty hours and was created from the following series of questions: If such a device were easy to install and not expensive, would you or someone in your household have the time to install such a device if it took: 1. 5 hours to install 2. 10 hours? 3. 20 hours? 4. 50 hours? Each item was treated as dichotomous; any response other than "yes" was treated as "no." The number of positive responses was summed and treated as a five point interval scale with zero indicating no available time resource. It was hypothesized that a perception of greater time 87 availability would be positively related to participation in direct and indirect conservation of fossil fuel energy. Number of Household Occupants was viewed as a measure of the availability of human energy resources with— in the household. The measure was actually constructed from the perspective of the number of adults and number of children separately. Case and Harris (1980) found that households with two adults tended to take more conservation measures than did households with one, or more than two adults. Number of children was found to have a significant positive bi- variate relationship with taking conservation actions. When other variables were accounted for, however, number of children was dropped from the prediction model. The availability of human energy within the house- hold was hypothesized to increase the practice of voluntary simplicity behaviors. The greater the number of adults the greater the amount of human energy available. When children are present within a household it was determined that more time would be spent in providing goods and ser- vices within the home. Based on the concept of resources, the number of adults within the household was also viewed as the avail- ability of labor to perform conservation actions related to direct consumption, therefore, number of adults was hypothesized to have a positive relationship with direct 88 conservation of energy. In a study of energy consumed during life cycle stages, Fritzsche (1981) indicated that households in which children were present tended to require more energy and thus had less flexibility for conservation. Number of children within the household was hypothesized to have a negative relationship with percentage change in con- sumption. Number of Employed Adults was a measure of human resource indicating the ability of a household to increase the availability of income. The measure was created by counting the number of household income earners among the two primary adults within the household. Eichenberger (1975) found that dual employed house- holds tended to use their household appliances less. Having fewer people within the household during the daytime tended to result in less direct consumption of energy within the household. Case and Harris (1980) did not find a signifi- cant relationship between number employed and taking con-A servation actions. Dual employed households would be expected to have higher incomes and thus greater ability to purchase goods and services outside the household. In addition, they would he expected to perceive having less time to perform production within the home. Dual employed households were hypothesized to have a positive relationship with direct conservation of fossil 89 fuel energy. They were also hypothesized to have a negative relationship with participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors. Summary of Human Resource Hypotheses 4A. When all motivators, human resources, and contextual variables are accounted for, a positive relationship will exist between percentage reduction in consumption and the following human resources! 1) Knowledge 2) Average Household Education 3) Reported Skills 4) Perceived Money Availability 5) Perceived Time Availability 6) Number of Adults 7) Number of Employed Adults and a negative relationship will exist between percentage reduction in consumption and the following human resource: 8) Number of Children 4B. When all motivators, human resources, and contextual variables are accounted for, a positive relationship will exist between voluntary simplicity scores and the follow- ing human resources: 1) Knowledge 2) Average Household Education 3) Reported Skills 4) Perceived Time Availability 5) Number of Adults 6) Number of Children; and a negative relationship will exist between voluntary simplicity scores and the following human resources: 7) Perceived Money Availability 8) Number of Employed Adults 90 5A. When all motivators and human resources are accounted for the following contextual var- iables: age, home ownership, and rurality should provide no further contribution to the variance in percentage change in consump- tion. 5B. When all motivators and human resources are ‘accounted for the following contextual var- ‘iables: age, home ownership, and rurality should provide no further contribution to the variance in voluntary simplicity partici- pation. Personal Control was computed from a series of questions asked in November of 1980. Due to the difference in time of data collection for the measure of personal con- trol and the other human resources, this indicator was used in the analysis as a dependent variable to measure the impact of participation in direct and indirect conservation on the development of human resources. A sense of personal control has been related to competence and achievement especially in education and career. Gurin and Gurin (1976) have also discussed the relevancy of perceived personal control and economic behavior. For the research reported here, personal control was defined as a psychological human resource. Five Likert-type statements were taken from the internal control scale and adapted to specify energy problems as the emphasis of the control. 91 Table 9. Summary Data of Human Resource Variables. Standard Human Resource Mean Score Deviation (N) Knowledge 1.536 .332 607 Number of Adults 2.221 .758 638 Number of Children 0.980 1.211 638 Reported Skills 5.340 2.138 638 Average Household Education 12.970 2.066 638 Perceived Money Number of Availability Perceived Time Employed Adults (%) Hours (%) (%) LT s 50 10.3 LT 5 13.6 None 10.3 $ 50 27.4 5 12.1 One 47.3 $ 200 22.9 10 17.2 Two or More 27.4 $ 500 15.7 20 i 16.3 Missing 6.0 $1000 11.4 50 40.8 $2000 12.2 92 Each question was answered strongly disagree, disagree, agree or strongly agree. Answers were coded so that those indicating highest control were given a score of three; those indicating the lowest control were given a score of zero. A total score was computed by summing the raw score of each variable and dividing by the total number of statements answered. This continuous variable was cate- gorized according to agreement levels. Summary data are provided in Table 10. Personal control was used as a dependent variable. It was hypothesized that participation in direct and in- direct conservation would increase the sense of personal control. Statistical Analysis One goal of this research was to explore the extent to which households had been participating in direct and indirect conservation of energy. In addition it was pro- posed to assess the relationship of motivation and avail—g ability of human resources to active conservation behaviors. Direct conservation was measured through a com- parison of actual consumption of energy within the house- hold as determined from records provided by utility companies. Indirect conservation was measured by 93 Table 10. Personal Control, 1980. Percent _N_ Strongly Agree 29.8 190 Agree 69.4 443 Disagree 0.5 5 Strongly Disagree 0.0 0 Missing 0.3 2 Mean: 2.02 Standard Deviation: .3425 Minimum - Maximum: 0 to 3 develOping an index from responses to eleven questions con- cerned with the extent to which households practiced beha- viors related to a lifestyle defined as voluntary simpli- city. To address the first question, measures of direct and indirect conservation were categorized according to levels of participation. Mean scores, absolute frequencies, and relative frequencies were provided for comparison. In addition, the total voluntary simplicity score was factor analyzed in order to delineate underlying dimensions. Des- criptive statistics were also provided for each voluntary simplicity subscale. Multiple regression was selected for analysis of relationships between direct and indirect conservation and motivation and human resource availability. Multiple regression provided examination of two issues relevant to the research problem. 94 First, the procedure provided a predictive model developed from the collective contribution of motivation and human resources to the variance in both direct and indirect conServation. Use of regression with hierarchial inclusion of independent variables in the equation facil- itated the development of the predictive model based on a previously defined conceptual model. Secondly, multiple regression analysis allowed examination of the interrelationship of variables. Speci— fically, this procedure facilitated an examination of the impact of human resource availability on the motivation variables through ovservation of the standardized beta coefficients. Finally, path analysis was selected as a final test of the analytical model. This procedure facilitated exam- ination of differing relationships between motivation and human resources and each conservation mode. The final model allowed for a greater grasp of the whole picture. Assumptions 1. Survey research is an appropriate means for gaining information regarding social and behavioral dimen- sions of conservation. 2. Self-report data is a reliable measure of actual behavior. 3. A lifestyle of voluntary simplicity is, in fact, a less energy (fossil-fuel) intensive lifestyle. 95 Limitations This study was a secondary analysis of data collec- ted for an evaluation of a household energy-audit project. Data, therefore, were not collected solely for the goals and objectives of this research. However, exclusion or omission of variables potentially related to motivation and human resources were reviewed and were not considered a serious problem in testing the theoretical model. The generalizability of findings is limited based on any biases of the sampled community. The goals of the research team in the evaluation of the energy audit resulted in overrepresentation of households living in single family dwellings and owning their own homes. Households living in multiple unit dwellings and renting were underrepresented. A major limitation of this study was the selection of only one household member to be interviewed, the under- lying assumption being that the responses of one member would be representative of the entire household. Previous research, however, has indicated that agreement of husbands and wives on various demographics and attitudinal survey questions cannot be relied upon (Ballweg, 1969; Byrne and Blaylock, 1963; Safilios-Rothschild, 1968; Van Es and Shingi, 1972). Ballweg (1969) distinguished between nonevaluative, or hard data, and evaluative or soft data. Specifically, he studied the responses of 179 couples to two questions-- one considered hard data and one soft data. As expected, 96 considerable differences were found between couple consensus. The question dealing with family income was viewed as hard data and was found to have husband and wife consensus greater than 60 percent of the time. The soft data question was concerned with which parent had the final say in child discipline. Only 23 percent of the couples were in agree- ment in their responses to this question. Fairly high levels of husband and wife consensus on income was observed by Terber (1955) and also by Haberman and Elinson (1967). Byrne and Blaylock (1963) concluded from a study of 36 married couples that similarity in political attitudes and in more general attitudes was similar among couples. They also found, however, that couples assumed greater consensus than actually existed. The 36 couples included in this study were college students or professionals, thus the findings may be restricted to a relatively well educated population. Van Es and Shingi (1972) studied the consensus of 324 husbands and wives on twenty—five attitude questions.- They concluded that where attitudes are concerned it should not be assumed that either the husband or the wife can represent the whole family. They noted, however, that when_the attitude is somewhat culturally determined more consensus occurs among husbands and wives. Safilios-Rothschild studied 160 families from Detroit and 250 Athenian couples. The Detroit sample consisted of data from both the husband and wife, whereas 97 the Greek data included responses from 133 wives and 117 husbands. A comparison of Detroit husbands'and wives'res- ponses about decision making showed that less than half of the couples agreed or slightly disagreed on their responses. This meant that actual serious disagreement was found in 55.1 percent of the cases. The Greek data did not permit matching. Results, however, indicated that significant differences did result even between the aggregate samples of husbands and wives. Van Es and Shingi (1974) looked at aggregate res- ponses of husbands and wives. Significant differences between the mean scores of husbands and wives were found on eight of the twenty-five attitudinal questions. Subsam— ples based on husband's occupation, husband's education and wife's education were unable to provide evidence that more homogenous groups improved the consensus. Thus it was concluded that distribution of scores for males and females could not be considered similar. Congruence of husbands' and wives' energy attitudes was studied by Gladhart (1977) on four energy attitude scales: ecosystem awareness, human responsibility, life- style flexibility, and ease of cutting back. Correlations were found in the moderate range for each scale. Hungerford (1978) looked at the relationship of a husband's and a wife's ecoconsciousness value and actual change in consump- tion from 1974 to 1976. Congruency was not found as a significant aspect of change. Rather the greatest of soft data as a result of self-report and are, therefore, subject to the perceptions of the respondent. Voluntary simplicity behaviors, since they involved actual activity were viewed as somewhat more generalizable than philosophi— cal perspectives or sense of control. The reader is advised to View the findings with these limitations. CHAPTER IV FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION This research study was designed to explore the extent to which households had participated in direct and indirect conservation of fossil fuel energy and to further analyze the motivations and human resources which influence conservation behaviors. Several behaviors cumulatively defined as voluntary simplicity were chosen as the measure of indirect conservation. Percentage change in actual household consumption of energy was chosen as the measure of direct conservation. Findings related to the first research question were reported in detail in Chapter III and will be sum- marized in this section. Findings related to the remaining three research questions are reported in this chapter. Each research question is addressed with the analysis of direct conservation discussed first followed by the analysis of voluntary simplicity behaviors. The primary statistical procedure employed to test the hypotheses was multiple regression analysis. 99 100 Participation in Direct and Indirect Conservation The first research question was concerned with the percentage of households participating in conservation be- haviors. Approximately one-third of the sampled households had reduced their consumption of fossil fuel energy, used primarily for space heating, by more than four percent during the 1979-80 heating season as compared to the 1977— 1978 heating season. Sampled households answered a series of 11 ques- tions designed to measure the extent to which households have participated in lifestyle behaviors focused on self- sufficiency, recycling, and contribution to ecological organizations. Approximately three—fourths of the house- holds had at least occasionally practiced some of the behaviors. Approximately one-fifth of the households reported active participation. As indicated by Table 11 households which reported active participation in volun- tary simplicity are not necessarily the same households which reduced their direct consumption of fossil fuels. The Effect of Motivators and Human Resources on Direct and Indirect Conservation of Fossil Fuel Energy The second and third research questions were speci- fically concerned with the impact of motivators and speci- fic human resources on change in consumption and partici- pation in voluntary simplicity behaviors. 101 mmmnz Aanv Amvnzv Ammnzv Amvuzv mm.H who: N.o o.m m.mH m.w no unwound usom SQ GOHDQESmcou hmuocm mcflmmouocH mpaosomsom lauzc immuzc Ammanzc Assnzv me.a neendsnmnoo senonm an N.o m.m o.om m.o moccao pccOHMAcmHm oz BDHB mpaonomso: Amnzv Aomuzv Ammauzv Aamnzv mm.a who: no usoouom m.o m.> H.om o.m usom >2 monocm mcfl>uomcou mpaozomsom Ho>mz accofimmooo usoswowm m>Huo< S 8.4.W mao>oq coflum>uomcou pcooumm unmouom unmouom unmouom % 00.70. ems s w w. a muccmflofluumm mHmEmm humpcsao> .e?.9 so so AW.4 coaumfismcou hmuocm paozomsom poospom paw wHoH>msmm thOAHQEHm hucucsHo> :H :oHDcQAOHuHcm mo COmHHmQEOO .HH mange 102 Regression analysis was the primary statistical procedure employed to test the hypotheses related to these research questions. Bivariate regression analysis provided an indication of the total effect of each motivator and human resource on direct and indirect conservation of energy. Multiple regression analysis provided an indica- tion of the effect of the predictors when other character- istics were taken into account. Regression analysis provided various criteria used to assess the value of motivators and human resources. Among those used for discussion in this analysis were the standardized regression coefficient, the R square, the partial correlation, the F value and the probability. An explanation of these statistics is provided here. The standardized regression coefficient, more com— monly referred to as beta or beta weight, was used to indi- cate the relative importance of independent variables in explaining the dependent variable. The beta is often des- cribed as the direct effect of a predictor on a dependent variable. In a bivariate analysis the beta would be con- sidered the total effect of one variable on another. Since no other predictors are accounted for, bivariate analysis provided betas which were zero order relationships. Multiple regression analysis provided beta weights which were based on the direct effect of the independent variable when the effects of one or more select variables were taken into account. 103 The R-square provided an assessment of the good— ness of fit of the regression model. It indicated the proportion of variation in the dependent variable which was explained by the total of independent variables. Two sta- tistics related to R2 which were also used in the analysis included R2 change and adjusted R2. R2 change is a mea- sure of the additional amount of variance accounted for by inclusion of another predictor variable. Adjusted R2 does not increase with the addition of each new predictor variable as does R2 but is sensitive to the effects of a larger number of predictors. When the adjusted R2 shows a decrease it is an indication that the predictor model would lose its goodness of fit if applied to a new set of data (Hull and Nie, 1981). The partial correlation is the correlation of two sets of residuals determined by removing the effect of other predictors first from the dependent variable and then from the predictor itself. When squared, the partial cor- relation can be used to approximate contribution to vari-' ance in the dependent variable which was not previously accounted for by predictor variables already in the regres- sion equation. By observation of the partial correlation it was possible to determine relationships among the pre- dictors. The F-Value provided a score which indicated the ratio of explained variance to unexplained variance. The greater the explained variance the obvious reduction in 104 unexplained variance resulting in a ratio with a larger numerator and a smaller denominator thus a larger F-value. This value is associated with the probability that the pre- dicted difference will not be a chance occurrence. For this study the significance level was set at .10 indicating a willingness to be incorrect in predictions only ten percent of the time. Percentage Change in Consumption In this section two research questions are addressed and the following hypothesized relationships were examined: Research Question 2. To what extent are indicators of income adequacy and philosophical perspective towards the energy issue related to a family's household reduction in direct energy consumption? Hypothesis 1A There will be curvilinear relationship between income adequacy and percentage change in direct household consumption of fossil fuels. Lower and higher levels of income adequacy will result in greater reduction of consumption. Hypothesis 2A Relative fuel cost and relative change in fuel price each have a positive linear relationship _ with a reduction in direct household consumption of fossil fuels. Hypothesis 3A Philosophical perspective is positively related to a reduction in household energy consumption. 105 Research Question 3. To what extent is the availability of human resources related to a household's reduction in direct energy consumption? Hypothesisk4A A positive linear relationship exists between percentage reduction in consumption and the following human resources: 1) Knowledge 2) Average Household Education 3) Reported Skills 4) Perceived Money Availability 5) Perceived Time Availability 6) Number of Adults 7) Number of Employed Adults and a negative linear relationship exists between percentage reduction in consumption and the following human resource: 8) Number of Children Hypothesis 5A When all motivators and human resources are accounted for, the following contextual variables-— age, home ownership, and rurality—-should provide no further contribution to the variance in per- centage change in consumption. Percentage change in energy consumption was regres- sed separately on each motivator, each human resource and each contextual measure in order to determine bivariate relationships. This procedure facilitated clarification of the individual relationships between predictors and percentage change in consumption and, thus, provided a base for higher order analysis. Summary data of the bi- variate relationships are presented in Table 12. 106 Multiple regression analysis was used to test the hypotheses concerned with the interrelationships of the motivators, the eight human resources, and the three con- textual variables in explaining variation in change in consumption. Summary data of the multiple regression analysis are presented in Table 13. Bivariate Regression Analysis- Percentage Change in Consumption A reduction in consumption was indicated by a negative percentage change while a positive score indicated an increase in consumption. Based on the negative scoring of the dependent variable a negative regression coefficient indicated a positive relationship of the independent variable with percentage reduction in consumption. Findings of the Bivariate Analysis. Results of the bivariate regression analysis for percentage change in household consumption scores are presented in Table 12. None of the human resources was significantly related to a percentage reduction in consumption. One motivation and three contextual variables, however, were significant contributors. Motivators. Relative fuel cost had a significant relationship with a reduction in consumption indicating that higher cost fuels resulted in some conservation of 107 ooo. ooo.m moo. soo.n snflamnsm ooo. omH.m moo. Hoo.u mom omo. mmo.m ooo. oso.u oaomnmcao msom mmHQMHHm> acszmacoo ooo. moo. ooo. moo. omsoflaem umnsoz ooo. ooo. ooo. oHo.u cmuoaflao no umnsoz ooo. ooH. ooo. mHo.: coflnmosom oaosmmoom monum>< mom. mom. ooo. Hmo. maaflxm owuHOQmm mom. mom. ooo. mmo.u mofl>mo oncomg on mnflaflna mmm. Hmm. Hoo. ooo.: monaflnmaflm>a mane oam. ooo. Hoo. omo.: muasoa no umnaoz oom. ooH.H moo. ooo. moomazocm mGOMZOmmm cmEsm ooo. ooo. ooo. ooo.: womsqm©< wsoocH mom. ooo. Hoo. ooo.: m>fluumdmumm Hmoflbd0moHflnm ooa. mo.a moo. mmo.n unflnm moflum Hmom oHo. mo.m ooo. ooo.- nmoo Hose m>fipmamm mnouc>fluoz mocmoflwflcmflm Hmucm mumswm mucmHOmemoo moanoflum> on m coflmmmummm ucwccmmmccH m omnaonmocmum monommmz pompcmmmwcH mm moanmmumoama paw mGOMDOmmm :mEDm .muoum>fluoz can manoeum> unmocmmmo man no GOHHQESchU CH mmccao mmmucmoumm cuHB mflmhaccd coammmummm mpMHHm>Hm .NH manna 108 energy. The amount of variance accounted for, however, was less than one percent. Differences in the price increases between fuel types did not contribute signifi- cantly to conservation. Neither income adequacy nor philosophical perspective had a significant effect on direct fossil fuel conservation. Human Resources. Only three of the human resources contributed to the variance in percentage change in con- sumption. Their contribution, however, was negligible. None of the resources was significantly related to direct conservation. Contextual Variables. Three contextual variables were included in the analysis. Home ownership, age, and rurality each had significant relationships with percentage change in consumption. Each accounted for less than one percent of the variance. Multiple Regression Analysis-Percentage Change in Consump- tion Multiple regression analysis was the statistical» tool used to test the hypotheses. The format of the regres— sion model involved the assignment of specific inclusion levels to each independent variable. Motivators were given the highest priority, human resources were second and contextual variables were given the lowest priority for inclusion. This design facilitated explanation of the additional contribution of human resources to a reduction 109 in household energy consumption. In addition, the inclu- sion of contextual variables at the lowest priority showed the extent to which the motivators and human resources included for analysis were unable to explain away contri- bution or rurality, age, and home ownership. Summary data are reported in Table 13. Findings. With all variables accounted for only relative fuel cost and age of respondent maintained their significance. Age actually increased the amount of variance accounted for in the zero order relationship from less than one percent to 1.1 percent in the multivariate model. The total regression model accounted for only 3.3 percent of the variance in percentage change in energy consumption. The equation had a probability of .150 and therefore did not meet significance requirements. The R square is used to indicate the incremental increase in the amount of variance accounted for by the regression equation. According to Hull and Nie (1981) a decrease in the adjusted R square is an indication of when inclusion of additional variables into the model results in a less generalizable model. The more generalizable model can then be determined through observation of any decrease in the adjusted R2. This decrease occurred on the third step of the regression analysis. The resulting model is identified in Table 13. llO .mmbmmmm> mmnu ocomocH UmeS mocos m>muomcmmm ouommEoo 4 .m0m>m:on commm>mmmcoo uoommp momuompmmm Cm mocmumomEm mum mcmumomcam manommm> mom an» MC COHUmpcm on» sumz ommmmmocm mGSmm> m cmumsmcm m .mmcoa GOmuomwmmm ammono map ou ucm>mmmm momummumum ou ccommmmmoo monocucommm cm momsmmmm mooo. V oom. "mocoomomsomm Ammo. o mmo. "mumsoo m mmmoo.mv mom.m "m mmomm>o omo. omo. moo. mmo.- mommmuom oom. moo.m moo. ooo.: dmsmumozo msom ooo. mom.m mmo. mom.u mom omo. mmo. ooo. mooo.: mummmnmmmo>< mono: oo>mooumm ooo. mmo. ooo. mmo.: ommOmdsm mononmmocmm omo. ooo. ooo. mmo.: commoosom omonmmsom mommm>¢ ooo. mom. moo. ooo.: ammommoo oo umnaoz omo. moo. moo. mmo.- mommmnmmmm>m meme om>mmonmm ooo. mom. moo. mmo. mmmmxo omnmommm mmo. mmo. moo. mmo.: mumoom mo monsoz ooo. moo. moo. mmo. moommsoom ooo. mmo. ooo. ooo.: momoomom msoocm mmo. mmo. moo. . Ammo.no omo.u m>muomdmumm mmomodomOmmgm oom. ooo.m moo. mmoo.uo mmo.: ammoo momma mmsm omo. ooo.m ooo. mmmoo.nv ooo.: mmoo mono m>muommm mocmommmcmmm mmmcm mmcmso mucmmommmmoo mGHQMHmm> om mmosqm acmmmmmmmm ucmpcmmowcm .m m UQNHUHMUCMuvm mmnmmmo> ucmpcmmmm msu mm COmuQEszou cm mmcmno mmmucmommm £um3 mmmhmmcm cemmmmmmmm ommmumsz mmmnommummm .mm omnoe 111 Three of the four motivators were included in this model which contributed to 1.3 percent of the variance and was significant at a probability of .045. Relative cost of fuel, price shift, and philosophical perspective had beta weights of -0.76, -.052, and -0.26 respectively. Negative coefficients were expected due to the negative value of the reduction measure. Income adequacy did not contribute significantly to the model. An attempt was made to discover whether the non- significant contribution of income was caused by a curvi- linear relationship between the variables. One way analysis of variance was computed for percentage change in consump- tion by income adequacy categories. No significant dif- ference was found between groups. However, the relationship between income adequacy and conservation was curvilinear in the expected direction. To determine if there were possible interaction effects, "breakdowns" were computed for a selected set of independent variables. The first interaction observed was that between percentage change in consumption and income adequacy and philosophical perspective. As can be seen in Table 14 the greatest conservation was found in households with high income adequacy where the respondent indicated a low energy philosophical perspective. Mean scores indicated that low and medium income groups conserved an average of 2.7 per- cent whereas very 1ow and high income groups conserved under one percent (Table 14). 112 Table 14. Percentage Reduced Consumption by Philosophical Perspective by Income Adequacy Income Adequacy Philosophical Perspective Low Medium High Mean N Very Low -- -O.9 0.9 _ 0.02 92 Low -- -2.7 -2.9 2.63 . 118 Medium —o,7 -1,3 -3.7 2.76 261 High -6.0 -1.3 —0.5 0.88 90 Mean Households with medium scores on philosophical per- spective conserved an average of 2.7 percent if their in- come adequacy was low and conserved approximately 1.3 percent if they fell within the medium income adequacy groups. Households which indicated high philosophical perspective scores tended to have greater reduction in con- sumption due to mean reduction scores of 2.9 percent in the low income adequacy group and 3.7 percent in the medium income adequacy group. Households which reported high scores on philosophical perspective and fell into the extreme categories of income adequacy showed only small overall percentages of reduced consumption (Table 14). A slight shift from the bivariate contribution was noted in the effect of income adequacy with the addition of knowledge. Since knowledge was negatively related conservation, not in the hypothesized direction, a ll3 breakdown was computed for percentage change by income adequacy and knowledge scores. Results indicated that higher level of respondent knowledge was related to the greatest amount of conservation in medium income groups. Knowledge was least beneficial in very low income groups (Figure 4.1). Age maintained its significant relationship to percent change in consumption. Breakdowns of percent change in consumption indicated that households with older members tended to conserve at approximately the same per- centage despite income level. Within both younger and middle-aged populations a curvilinear relationship was observed. Younger households had increased rather than reduced consumption; households within low and medium income adequacy groups had the least increase. With the exception of the very low income adequacy group, middle-aged households showed reduced consumption. Low and medium income adequacy households had reduced their consumption by the greatest percentage (Figure 4.2). Overall rural households tended to conserve more than did urban dwellers. This difference was especially evident in homes where fuel oil was used (Figure 4.3). Rurality was not a factor contributing to conservation differences in homes heated with electricity. Natural gas users living in cities, however, reported greater conser- vation than did rural users of natural gas. 114 3 l 1 know d 0/ --. . ° medium ugh CHANGE -1 CONSUMPTION v -3 -5 -7 INCOME ADEQUACY LEVELS vl-very low I- low m—medium h-high Flgure 4-l. Percent Change in Consumption by Knowledge by Income Adequacy. llS SP 3 1 °/o F. CHANGE -1 CONSUMPTION -3 ‘5 INCOME ADEQUACY LEVE LS VI very low I-low m-medium h-high Figure 4—2. Percent Change in Consumption by Age by Income Adequacy. ll6 3 f 1 °/. /—:2--—- urban rural CHANGE -1 CONSUMPTION " -3 -5 —7 o —9 ~11 FUEL TYPE rug-natural gas fo-fuel oil e-electricity Figure 4.3. Percent Change in Consumption by Rurality by Fuel Type. ll7 Summary and Discussion of Hypotheses Findings indicated that 36.4 percent of the house- holds in the research subsample had reduced their consump- tion of fossil fuel energy within the household by more than four percent. The change in consumption among all subsample households showed a 1.8 percent overall decrease. This was consistent with the findings reported by William, Kruvant, and Newman (1979) based on a national sample of households during 1972 and 1974. Percentage change in household consumption was regressed onto specified motivators, human resources, and contextual variables to assess their influence on conser- vation. Findings indicated that one motivator, relative fuel cost, and one contextual variable, age, were signifi- cantly related to a change in consumption. The relationship of fuel cost to percent change in consumption was negative indicating that as the rela- tive cost of the type of fuel used to heat the home in- creased, households were more likely to decrease their consumption of that fuel. Thus, that part of Hypotheses 2A concerned with relative cost of fuel was retained. Breakdowns of fuel type according to rural and urban residence indicated that rural households using fuel oil and electricity reduced their consumption significantly more than did their urban counterpart. Income adequacy was determined to have a slight curvilinear relationship with households in the low and ll8 middle adequacy ranges having the greatest conservation. A test for curvilinearity, however, did not prove signi- ficant. Households with low and medium adequacy levels were found to have the greatest percentage of reduced consumption. This was especially true for those house- holds in which the respondent indicated a higher level of knowledge concerning conservation actions, reflected a socially responsible philosophy towards energy issues, and indicated an age range between 36-54. Since percentage change in consumption was an objective measure obtained from actual utility company records and not from self report, it was determined that the indication of change was in fact a reliable finding. While there are some questions as to the consensus between household members on their responses to survey questions, consensus on measures of income has been found to be acceptable by various researchers, and was used in this study as a measure of the household's ability to purchase goods and services. Philosophical perspective and knowledge, however, are not necessarily representative of the entire household and therefore represent a limita- tion to the generalizability of their relationship with change in consumption to the household as a whole. More accurately the findings can be interpreted as households in which at least one person expresses a certain ll9 philosophical position or possesses a certain level of knowledge. Participation in Voluntarnyimplicity In this section two research questions are addressed and the following hypothesized relationships are examined. Research Question 2. To what extent are indicators of income adequacy and philosophical perspective towards the energy issue related to a family's participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors? Hypothesis 1B Income adequacy is negatively related to parti- cipation in behaviors related to voluntary simplicity. Hypothesis 3B Philosophical perspective is positively related to participation in behaviors related to volun- tary simplicity. Research Question 3. To what extent is the availability of human resources related to a household's participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors? Hypothesis 4B A positive relationship exists between voluntary SimpliCity scores and the following human resources. 120 1) Knowledge 2) Average Household Education 3) Reported Skills 4) Perceived Time Availability 5) Number of Adults 6) Number of Children; and a negative relationship exists between volun- tary simplicity scores and the following human resources: 7) Perceived Money Availability 8) Number of Employed Adults Hypothesis SB When all motivators and human resources are accounted for the contextual variables of age, home ownership, and rurality should provide no further contribution to participants in voluntary simplicity behaviors. Similar to percentage change in consumption the total voluntary simplicity score was regressed separately on each motivator, each human resource and each contextual measure in order to ascertain bivariate relationships. Summary data of the bivariate-relationships of the total voluntary simplicity scale and predictor variables are presented in Table 15. Multiple regression analysis was the statistical tool used to test the hypotheses concerned with the inter- relationships of the two motivators--income adequacy and philosophical perspective; the eight human resources; and the three contextual variables in explaining variation in reported voluntary simplicity participation. Summary data of the multiple regression analysis performed with Total 121 Voluntary Simplicity as the dependent measure are presented in Table 16. Findings of the Bivariate Analysis. The results of the bivariate regression analysis for the total voluntary simplicity score are presented in Table 16. All but two of the predictors had significant relationships with voluntary simplicity. Motivators. Income adequacy and philosophical perspective both had significant bivariate relationships with voluntary simplicity behaviors in the hypothesized directions. The measure of philOSOphical perspective appeared to be the stronger of the two with a direct effect of .200 and significantly contributed to four percent of the variance in voluntary simplicity scores. Income adequacy had a negative relationship to participation in voluntary simplicity and accounted for less than one percent of the variance. The negative relationship was ex- pected and indicated that as the adequacy of income lowered, participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors increased. Human Resources. All but one of the human resources had significant positive relationships with voluntary sim- plicity scores. Three of the bivariate relationships were not in the expected direction. Ability to afford a con- servation device and number of employed adults were 122 hypothesized to have negative relationships; knowledge was expected to be positively related to voluntary simplicity participation. Reported skills accounted for the greatest indivi- dual contribution to the variance, 10.9 percent, in volun- tary simplicity scores. The beta weight of .330 was signi- ficant with a probability of .000. Time availability was the second highest contributor among the resources and accounted for nearly five percent of the variance with a beta of .214. Next in order of importance was the number of household occupants. Number of children entered the re- gression model with a beta weight of .189 and contributed to 3.6 percent of the variance. Number of adults was entered into the bivariate model with a beta weight of .140 and accounted for approximately two percent of the variance. Ability to afford a conservation device, average household education, and number of employed adults were the remaining human resources investigated. Individual contri- bution to the variance of the dependent variable was less than two percent for each measure and their beta weights were .132, .121, and .070 respectively. A measure of know— ledge was negatively related to the dependent measure with a nonsignificant beta of —.038. Contextual Variables. Three contextual variables were included in the regression model. Two of the measures, 123 mmo. mmo. ooo. ooo. ooo. mom.mm omo. mom. ooo. mov.om ooo. mmm.n omm. mmo. moo. mmo.: ooo. vmo.m moo. ooo. moo. omm.m mmo. mmm. moo. mom.mm mmo. mmm. ooo. vmo.mm omo. ova. ooo. mom.m~ mmo. mom. ooo. vom.om ooo. vmm. ooo. mam.oo mom. omm. mmo. Nmm.m ooo. mmo.: ooo. mvm.om ooo. oom. cocoommmcmmm mopcm ommsqm masomOmmmooo on m commmmmmmm m pommpmmccmum mm momzmmmmoamo can moOHDOmom cosom can no momoom mumommmsmm mmoucdmo> Hobos .mmouc>muoz cam numz mwmxmmcm mmcmmmczo mEo: mummomom mom momncmmm> mmopxwmcoo wmpwazocm ComoHQEm mmnfisz EOHumosom pmocmmsom ommmw>< wom>oo ouomom on mummmnm mumsom mo meEDZ comommco mo meEDZ mummmnammm>< mama mmmmmm ownHOQmm IIIIIIIIIIIIIII moomzommm cmEsm momsqw©< mEoocH m>muoommmmm mwom£m0m0mm£m mmoum>mmoz mmmnmmmm> ucoocommpcm mammmo> acoccmmmccH omnmmmm> ucmpcmmmo COHmmwmmmm mummmm>mm .mm ombce 124 age and rurality, have been described in previous literature as influencing voluntary simplicity behavior. Since the primary purpose of the research was to explore voluntary simplicity in terms of motivation and human resources the contextual variables were included in the analysis to deter- mine the extent to which the resource and motivation varia- bles would account for variance previously attributed to demographics. Age was negatively related to voluntary simplicity behaviors indicating that as the age of the respondent increased the likelihood of household participation in voluntary simplicity would decrease. Age accounted for 4.6 percent of the variance in total voluntary simplicity scores. Rurality had a significant positive relationship with voluntary simplicity behaviors with a beta weight of 0.173 and accounted for three percent of the variance. Home ownership was not significantly related to voluntary sim- plicity participation. Multiple Regression Analysis--Voluntary Simplicity Participation Multiple regression analysis was used to test the hypothesis based on the additional impact of human resources on voluntary simplicity participation. The format of the regression model involved assigning variables specific inclusion levels. Motivators were given the highest priority, human resources were second and contextual variables were given the lowest priority for inclusion. 125 Two objectives were achieved through use of multi- ple regression analysis. First, it was possible to deter- mine a predictive model of voluntary simplicity based on selected motivators and human resources. Secondly, the hierarchial format facilitated an exploration of the con- ceptual model. Findings. The regression model with all variables entered accounted for 21.7 percent of the variance in voluntary simplicity participation. The equation had a probability of .000. Human resources accounted for an additional 14.8 percent of the variance beyond the 4.8 percent accounted for by the motivators. Two of the three contextual variables contributed to an additional 2.1 percent of the variance. Motivators. Philosophical perspective was the first motivator to enter the regression model. The posi- tive direction of the standardized regression coefficient (.188) indicated that those households who reported higher scores on the philosophical perspective scale were some- what more likely to participate in voluntary simplicity behaviors. Philosophical perspective accounted for four percent of the variance as in the bivariate analysis. Income adequacy maintained its significant negative contribution to participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors. The relative effect of income adequacy increased 126 .mOUOE o>mu0mcomm m CH OOOMOMOQEH mmonu mCmOMOHUCm mmmnmmmm> mod cam hummmmsm mo cOmumpwm mam nmmz commomOCH m tommsnom ¥ N .HOUOE m>mpom©omm ammono map on mam>mmmm momummmmum Ou Ocommommoo mummsmcommm Cm mmmommm m mooo. v ooo. "mocmomomoomo moom. o omm. “mumoom m mmooo.omo omm.mm no mmoum>o mmo. oom.o ooo. mmo. omomnmoao msom omo. ooo.m moo. momm.- moo moo. omo.om omo. momm. mummmuom moo. mom.o ooo. mmo.- moommzoos mmo. omm.o ooo. ooo.: ommOmdsm nonsoz omm. oom.m moo. mooo. o moo. mumoom mo nonsoz mmm. omo.m moo. Ammo. o moo. mummmnommo>m mama omm. oom.m moo. looo. o mmo. omuommno mo nonsoz moo. omo.m ooo. .mmoo. o mmo. mom>mo ouomom on mummmno ooo. omo.mm omo. mmmm. o omm. acmuoooom omoommsom mooum>m ooo. moo.oo omm. momm. o omm. mmmmxm ommuoomm mmo. mmm.o ooo. loom.uo omm.- momoomom msoocm ooo. mom.om ooo. mimom. o mom. m>muoodmumm moomndomOmmsm mocmumwmcmmm moucm wwcmno mucmmOmwwOOU mOmQMmmm> Om mmmsqm commmommmm ucopcomoccH o m ommmoumocouo mmnmmmm> ucopcomoo mam mo wumommmEmm ammmcsmo> mouoe num3 mmmwmcd< commmmmmom mmmmumsz momsommmmm: .om Omnme 127 from the bivariate analysis as the beta weight rose from -.091 to -.154. Income adequacy accounted for only an additional 0.8 percent of the variance. Together, philo- sophical perspective and income accounted for 4.8 percent of the variance in total voluntary simplicity scores. Human Resources. Human resources significantly accounted for an additional 14.8 percent of the variance in voluntary simplicity scores. Of the seven original significant resources only three maintained their signifi- cance when other resources and motivators were accounted for. Reported skills was, by far, the most influential human resource. The amount of variance accounted for by skills increased from the bivariate 10.9 percent to 12 percent with a direct effect of .238. Average household education was the second human resource to enter the regression model. Education accoun- ted for 1.6 percent of the variance with a positive beta weight of .114 significant at a probability of .000. This relative effect lowered only slightly from the zero order effect of .121. Ability to afford a conservation device was strong enough to enter the regression model on the fifth step with a significant effect. Addition of this variable, however, only increased the variance accounted for by 0.5 percent. This was considerably less than in the bivariate 128 model where this perception had a significant effect on voluntary simplicity scores with a beta of .132 and contri- buted to 1.7 percent of the variance. As other variables entered the model their significance to this measure was reduced. When changes in the partial coefficients were observed, the greatest reduction noted in the influence of the ability to afford a conservation device occurred when skill was accounted for in the equation. The remaining human resources did not enter the regression equation at a \ probability level less than .10. Contextual Variables. Contextual variables cum— mulatively accounted for an additional 2.1 percent of the variance. Rurality was the greatest contextual contributor accounting for an additional 1.4 percent of the variance and indicated that the motivation and human resource indi- cators included in the analysis were unable to totally account for the contribution of rurality to voluntary simplicity behavior. Age accounts for an additional 0.7 percent of the variance, reduced from a bivariate contri- bution of 4.6 percent. It appeared that motivation and resources were able to account for the majority of the influence of age. A decrease in the adjusted R2 was used as the criterion for selection of the most appropriate predictive model (Table 16). This decrease occurred at the eighth step in the regression analysis. The resulting model 129 accounts for 19.6 percent of the variance and is signifi- cant with a probability of .000. A breakdown of the effect of income adequacy on voluntary simplicity scores in the presence of skills is presented in Figure 4.4. The presence of skills within the household is especially influential as the adequacy of income decreases. Low skill availability resulted in lower participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors across income adequacy levels; very low and high levels of adequacy appear the most affected. Average household education (Figure 4.5) appeared to have increasing influence on voluntary simplicity participation as adequacy of income decreased. This is especially noticeable in the very low income group. The influence of a subjective sense of money availability for a conservation device was less evident. The higher extremes of participation occurred when low income households per- ceived more money availability. The lowest extreme in participation was evidenced in households with higher income adequacy and when the respondent indicated low perception of money availability. It was noted that the addition of children to the regression equation lowered the direct effect of income adequacy on voluntary simplicity participation. One child in a household increased participation within each income adequacy level. Two children somewhat reduced participa- tion. Addition of children to a household had the greatest 130 impact on voluntary simplicity participation in very low income adequacy households. Summary and Discussion of Hypotheses The two research questions dealt with in this sec- tion were concerned with the influence of income adequacy, an ecological perspective and a variety of human resources on participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors. Overall it was determined that a fairly large percentage of house- holds (75.6 percent) participated in at least some activi- ties which had an underlying dimension of a less energy intensive lifestyle. It was determined that such participa- tion was indeed motivated by the adequacy of one's income and a symbolic value system which suggested a responsibility for one's environment. The first two hypotheses were therefore accepted. Income adequacy significantly contributed to less than one percent of the variance. The direction of the relationship was negative, as indicated by the beta coef- ficient of -.154. This finding was supportive of the hypothesis that as the adequacy of one's ability to pur— chase goods and services within the market place decreased there would be an increase in their production within the home. Philosophical perspective was a significant contri- butor to the variance in voluntary simplicity scores accounting for four percent of the variance. The 131 2.0a I 1.5 MEAN VOLUNTARY SIMPLIC I TY SCORE v 1.0i 0.5 low medium SKI LL LEVEL INCOME ADEQUACY LEVELS I-very low l-low m-medium h-high Figure 4.4. Mean Voluntary Simplicity Score by Skill Level and Income Adequacy. ' 132 2.51 MEAN 2.0‘ VI/ VOLUNTARY SIMPLICI TY SCORE 1.5 1.0 INCOME ADEQUACY LEVELS vl-very low l-low m—medium h- high 1o 12 14 15 EDUCATION (W8) FIGURE 4.5. Mean Voluntary Simplicity Score by Education by Income Adequacy. 133 The relationship with voluntary simplicity was positive supporting the hypothesis that participation in voluntary simplicity behaviors was related to higher scores on a measure of philosophical perspective. The findings concerned with the relationship of voluntary simplicity behavior and human resources resulted in the acceptance of only two hypotheses. Reported skills and average household education did contribute signifi- cantly in the hypothesized direction. All other hypotheses were rejected. The addition of human resources to the regression equation accounted for an additional 14.8 percent of the variance. Only three resources; reported skills, average household education, and perception of the ability to afford a conservation device had a significant contribution. Reported skills was by far the more influential resource having a positive relationship with voluntary simplicity and accounting for an additional 12 percent of the variance. Average household education also had a positive relation- ship with voluntary simplicity. Perception of the ability to afford a conservation device had a positive relationship with voluntary simplicity indicating that as respondents perceived money as available for conservation they were also more likely to report participation in voluntary sim- plicity behaviors. This was not in the hypothesized direc— tion. A cross tabulation indicated that households who had actually taken actions concerned with direct 134 conservation of energy were also those households who re— ported higher levels of voluntary simplicity participation (Appendix C-l). This finding may indicate that households performing voluntary simplicity behaviors were indeed attempting to reduce direct consumption through technical and behavioral changes. Availability of resources was also found to in— fluence the direct effect of income adequacy on voluntary simplicity participation. As the three significant resources entered the regression model the relative effect of income adequacy increased. This finding indicated that in the presence of skills, education, and perceived money availability, households with less adequate incomes were 'more likely to participate in less energy intensive life- style behaviors of voluntary simplicity. This appears to be a form of resource substitution--the substitution of human labor for money income. Availability of human resources does not appear to influence the effect of philOv sophical perspective. A Comparison of Percentage Reduced Consumption and Voluntary Simplicity To judge which was the more effective in determining conservation, the individual motivators and an index of human resources were each tested as predictors for both direct and indirect conservation. The results of each analysis are reported in Table 17. 135 Percentage Change in Consumption. Relative fuel cost was the most influential motivator in contributing to reduced consumption explaining 1.1 percent of the variance. Philosophical perspective was the only other motivator to contribute. Human resources were not strong enough predic- tors to enter the equation. The total model was not signi— ficant. Voluntary Simplicity Participation. Availability of human resources was the strongest contributor to parti- cipation in voluntary simplicity behaviors. Both motiva- tors, philosophical perspective and income adequacy contri- buted significantly to the variance. The increase in standardized regression coefficients from bivariate to multivariate models reinforced the evidence of their inter- relatedness. Contribution of Conservation Behaviors to Personal Control Lovins (1977) and other conservationists have suggested that participation in less energy intensive life- styles would result in a lessening of our dependency of fossil fuels and an increase in our sense of personal control over energy needs. The management/decision-making framework of this research also suggested that as one parti- cipated in experiences or behaviors s/he would most likely be better prepared for similar situations in the future. Perlman and Warren described this as residuals. Within 136 o "mocmommmcmmm o.vm "mmosvm m Hmm.mm "m Hammm>o ooo. mmo. mom.l mmo.: moasomom meooom ooo. mmo. mom. oom. m>muomdmumo HMOHSQOmOmmcm o ooo. omm. omm. mmou90mom sass: >umomHQEmm hmomcsmo> mmm. "mocmomwmcmmm mmo. "mmmsdm m mo.H um Hacmm>o ooo. ooo. moo. moo.» mocmoo momma mono mmo. ooo. mmo. ooo.i momoqmpm OEOOCH omm. moo. mmo.: omo.i e>muomdmumm mmomzmomOmmzm mmo. mmo. oom.i ooo.: umoo moon :OHOQESmCOU Om omcmcv mmmucmommm mocmommmcmmm mmcmco mucOmOmwmmoo mucmmommwmoo mmmnmmmm> mumsqm COmmmmmmmm :ommmmmmmm Ocmpcomoocm m cmmmpmccccum mummmm>mm . momnmmmm> ucmpcmmmpcm mm mmomsommm spasm can mmoum>mpoz can mmmnmmmm> mcmpcommo mm :Omum>momcoo HO momsmmmz uommmpcm pom poommo cumz mommmuca OOHmmomowm .om Omnme 137 the management model used for this research, conservation behaviors were hypothesized to contribute to the development of human resources, specifically, personal control. The following research question was asked. Research Question 4. To what extent does participation in voluntary simplicity and/or reduction in household energy consumption contribute to a family's feeling of control over energy related stressors? Hypothesis 6A Reduction in the direct consumption of fossil fuels within the household will be positively related to a sense of personal control. Hypothesis 6B Participation in voluntary simplicity behavior will be positively related to a sense of personal control. Findings. The hypothesis was tested using regres- sion analysis. Observation of the bivariate coefficients showed that between direct and indirect conservation the indirect method of voluntary simplicity was the only signi- ficant contributor to a sense of personal control (Table 18). Participation in behaviors related to voluntary simplicity accounted for 3.1 percent of the variance. Reduced con- sumption was actually negatively related to a sense of control as indicated by the positive beta when a negative beta was expected. 138 When motivators, human resources, and contextual variables were accounted for the direct effect of voluntary simplicity participation decreased from .175 to .105. Hypothesis 6B was accepted and Hypothesis 6A was rejected. Both motivators, five of the human resources, and age had significant total effects on a sense of personal control. The motivators and resources were all positively related whereas age was negatively related. When account- ing for the effects of all other variables both motivators maintained a significant contribution. Resources, average household education, perception of available money and number of children enter the model with a significant con- tribution. Number of children became negatively related to personal control when age entered the equation. Age had a significant yet negative contribution. Testing the Model According to the model used to direct the analysis, it was hypothesized that motivations and human resources would result in decisions to behave in ways which would have potential for fossil fuel energy conservation. It was also hypothesized that the motivations and resources could be attributed, at least to some extent, to age and geographic location. The final aspect of the conceptual model was that participation in conservation behaviors would result in an increased or reinforced sense of control over energy problems. 139 ooo. omm. ooo. mmo.- summouom ooo. moo.om ooo- omm.: mom HGDOXOOCOU omo. . mom. ooo. omo. moommzoom omo. ooo. moo. mmo. mumsoa mo Monasz mom. moo.m ooo. ooo. mummmnommo>¢ mama om>mmoumm ooo. omo.m mmo. mmm. canommno mo monasz moo. omm.o omo. mmm. mmmmxo ounuogmm moo. moo.o mmo. mmm. omAOmQEo mmoolmmoomm moo. omo.mm omo. omm. mmmmeMmmm>< mono: om>mmonmm ooo. mom.mm omo. omm. oomumosom omonmmoom momuo>4 mmomsommm cmfism ooo. ooo.mm mmo. mom. m>muomdmumm mmomaoomOmmam moo. mmm.o omo. omm. momsomom mEooom mmomm>muoz mmm. mmm. ooo. omo. commdsomooo omosomm momouom ooo. ooo.mm mmo. omm. mumommoamm mumuaomo> COHHM>H®mCOU COSMOHMflCOHm HOHZHM OHMDHVW mUCQHOHMMOOU mmHQmmHHm> Om m GOmmmmmmmm ucmocmmmpcm m Ummmpmoocmum Omnmmmm> OCOUCOQOQ ecu mm momucoo mmcommom cam: mmmmmmc¢ COHmmomoom OOMmmc>mm .mH OHQMB 140 ooo. "mocmomwmcmmm oom. "muooom m mom.o no mmoum>o omo. omo. moo. mmo.: mummmusm moo. mmm.om omo. mmm.: mom mmo. mmo. ooo. ooo. moommzoom moo. omo. ooo. omo. mumsom mo.umns:z ooo. omo. moo. omo.- mummmnommo>m mama om>mmoumm ooo. moo. moo. omo.- omAOmosm mmsoimmocmo mmm. ooo. moo. omo. mmmmxm ommuoomm omo. oom.o ooo. omo.- omuommno oo nunssz omo. oom.m ooo. ooo. momoomom msooam mmo. mmm.o ooo. ooo. mummmnommm>o mono: om>mmoumm ooo. moo.m mmo. omm. m>muomomumm moomsdomOmmcm ooo. moo.mm mmo. moo. acmmoooom omoomosom momum>< mmm. mmm. ooo. ooo. composomooo omosomm ucmonmm ooo. ooo.mm mmo. oom. mumommosmo muwuoomo> moccomwmcmmm Hmucm mmcmzo mucwmommwmou mOHOMHmm> Om mmwsqm commmmmmmm mcoocommocm o m ommmonoocmmo mmmnmmmo> OCOOCOQOOCH mm mwmnmmmm> Hmsuxmucou can .moomsommm spasm .mmoum>muoz .comum>momcou paw mmnmmmm> acmpcmmmo may no momucoo deOmmmm sums mmmwmmsm COHmmmmmmm ommmpmsz .om omnce 141 Based on previous regression analyses a path analysis was constructed to test the overall model. A human resources score was developed from the scores of six resources included in the final regression model for volun- tary simplicity: reported skills, average household educa- tion, perception of money availability, perception of time availability, number of adults and number of children. The index was computed by first computing a proportion score for each selected resource. This was done by dividing the actual score by the possible score. A total score was computed by summing the proportion score for each of the resources. Personal control was regressed on each conservation measure, the motivators, the human resource score, and the contextual variables. In addition voluntary simplicity was regressed on income adequacy, philosophical perspective, human resources and the contextual variables. Direct con- servation was regressed on fuel cost, age and rurality. The motivators and human resource score were each regressed on the contextual variables. Age. Age was determined to have a direct negative relationship with a sense of personal control. It was concluded, however, that older persons may not have par- ticipated in voluntary simplicity because they did not feel they had the necessary resources. Leonard-Barton and Rogers (1980) arrived at a similar conclusion, noting that older persons were frequently not physically 142 capable of many of the self-sufficient behaviors required in voluntary simplicity. Older persons, however, did tend to reduce their direct consumption of fossil fuels more than younger households. This relationship, however, contributed to the negative relationship of age to a sense of personal control over energy problems. Rurality. The direct relationship of rurality to a sense of personal control was nonsignificant. Rural households tended to have lower incomes and a stronger sense of available resources both of which contributed to participation in voluntary simplicity. Participation was a significant contributor to a sense of control. Income Adequacy had a positive direct relationship with personal control as well as an indirect negative rela- tionship through participation in voluntary simplicity. It was, therefore, concluded that households who were able to substitute human energy for a less adequate income were more likely to sense personal control over problems. Philosophical Perspective had both direct and in- direct positive relationships with personal control. The positive relationship was to be expected since some of the questions related to philosophical perspective were directed towards personal responsibility for ecological problems. The attitude of responsibility not only personal control. When resources facilitated participation in voluntary simplicity, however, an indirect positive relationship was noted. Conservation. Of the conservation behaviors, only ____________ to a sense of personal control over energy problems. As noted previously, however, participation in voluntary sim— plicity was facilitated through possession of a variety of human resources and both income and philosophical motivation. 144 .moEoSmOEoo Oco mco_>uzom cozaiomcoo .39. some: 6.29522 503.00 35962201 325:. Ocm 39.5 9:295 .opos. 0:395 5nd one mmaofi \mmomoommm \ $615.25 ..\\ . V E5239 z..- 34 6 \ Mill \\ \\ a. ‘ webmammma \ \ I o\ \I oo.\ \ moo or . ._.mm N.om m.vm o.om m.vm m.m o.m o.m Umpwmz mm womwcm Umowma coo oz :02: mm mm: OB mtoow mo>mmmcz OB pmmumucm mm< mommmumo .m.D tomumucm mm mmmEmm >2 m.v m.v o.m o.m m.m m.o m.m m. v.mm m.om m.m m.m m.hv o.>m m.mm o.oo m.mm N.mm o.mo o.oe mam>mm mo mm>mm Ommoz one usocmsomce wmsmsm #5094 mocomommma somm Umscmmcoo moom msu m®>mmmma mcm>mm cosmoocoo mm < moxmz mo mm>om ucwmmmm one pmsosm m c0mud>mwmcoo mommmmz wommmmmo mmmcomum owmmommo owmm< momma mmmcompm mcmmmmz mmmmmwmo hmmcompm mommmmmo oommd mwmm< xmmcompm .onom .comum>mmmcoo pom mmmocm mamcmmocoo musmfiwumum HmOHSQOmOHmam Op Ocmfiommmd mammmommm momocwmoo: mo commemommm .mi< omnde 159 APPENDIX B m.o o.o mammmmz m.o Ii 30cm p.coa m.o m.m oommmmmo wmmcompm m.v m.vm mommcmmo H.oo m.mh mmmm¢ o.vm o.o momo< mmmcomum OODOOM coO mmmfimm .Omcccmm MH mem Eomm mam: H umnz #00 C00 H.m m.o v.m mammmmz m.o ii m.o BOOM m.con m.mm m.m~ v.mm mwmmmmmm mmmcomum m.mo o.mo o.oo mmuommmo m.mm o.mm o.vm mmmm< m.H o.m o.m womm< mmmcomum xosm mo Om: mmmmcm Emmnomm Hanna: 8 mm mmmm um>o monocoo mmuumm ammmcm map psond mcm£u>c< OD u.:mo mammnomm mmmocm mo>o momucoo mmcommmm mcmcmmmom mucmfimmmmm on ucmEmmmmm mamumommm mpmonmmsom mo mmoucmomom .mim Omnoe 160 APPENDIX C Table C-l. Voluntary Simplicity Score (1980) by Catagorical Level of Conservation Actions Taken, 1980. (N=638) Conservation Voluntary Action Simplicity Mean Level Score (N) Low 1.30 254 Medium 1.60 294 High 1.80 90 161 BIBLIOGRAPHY 5.... n J v . 3..., . r. . c.1435. BIBLIOGRAPHY Ackerman, Norleen Marion. "The Relationship of Objective and Subjective Family Income Adequacy to Selected Measures of Perceived Life Quality." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1977. Adams, R. N. Energy and Structure. Austin: University of Texas Press, 1975. Ashby, Eric. Reconciling Man With the Environment. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press, 1977. Ballweg, John A. "Husband-Wife Response Similarities on Evaluative and Non-evaluative Survey Questions," Public Opinion Quarterly 33, Summer, 1969. Boulding, Kenneth E. "The Social System and the Energy Crisis," Science 184, April 19, 1974. Bubolz, Margaret M.; Eicher, Joanne B.; and Sontag, M. Suzanne. "The Human Ecosystem: A Model," Journal of Home Economics, Spring, 1979. Byrne, Donn and Blaylock, Barbara. "Similarity and Assumed Similarity of Attitudes Between Husbands and Wives," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 67, 1963. Caplovitz, David. Making Ends Meet: How Families Cope With Inflation and Recession. Sage Library of Social Research, 1979. Carver, Thomas Nixon. The Economy of Human Energy. New York: The MacMillan Company, 1924. Case, Duncan F. and Harris, Craig K. "A Multivariate Analysis of the Effect of Household Characteristics on Reported Energy Conservation Actions in the Home." Unpublished paper, Michigan State University, East Lansing, 1980a. Cottrell, T. Energy and Society. New York: McGraw-Hill, 9 162 163 Diesing, Paul. Reason in Society: Five Types of Decisions and Their Social Conditions. Westport, Connecticut: Greenwood Press, 1962. Edney, Julian J. "The Commons Problem Alternative Perspectives," American Psychologist 35 (2), 1980. Eichenberger, MaryAnn. "A Comparison of Ownership of Selected Household Appliances and Residential Energy Use by Employed and Nonemployed Homemakers in the Lansing, Michigan, Area." M.A. thesis, Michigan State University, 1975. Elder, Glen H. Children of the Great Depression: Social Change in Life Experiences. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1974. Elgin, Duane S. and Mitchell, Arnold. "Voluntary Simpli- city: Life-style of the Future?" The Futurist 11:4, 1977. Ferber, Robert. "On the Reliability of Responses Secured in Sample Surveys," Journal of the American Statistical Association750, September, 1955. Fritzsche, David J. "An Analysis of Energy Consumption Patterns by Stage of Family Life Cycle," Journal of Marketing Research 28, 1981. Gladhart, Peter M. "Energy Conservation and Lifestyles: An Integrative Approach to Family Decision-Making," Journal of Consumer Science and Home Economics 1:4, 1977. Gladhart, Peter M. "Determinants of the Demand for Residen- tial Energy; A Family Level Analysis," revised, Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station, Journal Article # 8317, Michigan State University, 1977a. Gregg, Richard. "Voluntary Simplicity," The Co-Evolution Quarterly, Summer, 1977. Grier, George and Grier, Eunice. "Demographic Patterns and Imperatives Affecting Energy Conservation," Foresight-Volume II: Energy Conservation in Cities, prepared by the Congressional Research Service, Library of Congress, December, 1978. Gross, Irma H.; Crandall, Elizabeth W.; and Knoll, Marjorie M. Management For Modern Families. 3rd ed. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1973. 164 Gurin, Gerald and Gurin, Patricia. "Personal Efficacy and the Ideology of Individual Responsibility," in Strumpel, Burkard (Eds.), Economic Means For Human Needs: Social Indicators of Well-Being and Discontent. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, 1976. Haberman, P. and Elinson, J. "Family Income Reported in Surveys: Husbands Versus Wives," Journal of Marketing Research 4, May, 1967. Hackett, Bruce and Schwartz, Seymour. "Energy Conservation and Rural Alternative Life—Styles," Social Problems 28 (2), December, 1980. Hannon, B. "Energy Conservation and the Consumer," Science 189: 95-102, 1975. Harris, Craig K.; Keith, Joanne G.; Tortorici, Joseph Fr.; Wilhelm, Mari S. Final Report on the Evaluation of Statewide Project Conserve in Michigan. Unpublished report submitted to the Michigan Energy Administra- tion, Michigan Department of Commerce, Lansing: Michigan State University, 1981. Harris, Craig K.; Keith, Joanne G.; and Wilhelm, Mari S. Final Report on the Evaluation of Pilot Project Conserve in Michigan. Unpublished report submitted to the Michigan Energy Administration, Michigan Department of Commerce, Lansing: Michigan State University, 1980. Hogan, Mary Janice. "Energy Conservation: Family Values, Household Practices, and Contextual Variables." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1976. Hook, Nancy C. and Paolucci, Beatrice. "The Family as an Ecosystem," Journal of Home Economics 62:5, 1970. Hull, C. Hadlai and Nie, Norman H. (Eds.) SPSS Update 7—9. New York: McGraw—Hill Book Company, 1981. Hungerford, Nancy. "Relationship of Husband/Wife Ecocon— sciousness Value to Direct Household Energy Consumption." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1978. Katona, George. "The Human Factor in Economic Affairs," in Campbell, Angus and Converse, Philip E. (Eds.) The Human Meaning of Social Change. New York: Russell Sage FoundatiOn, 1972. 165 Keith, Joanne Goodman. "Direct Household Energy Consump- tion 1973-74, 1975-76: The Impact of Family Microdecisions Upon Levels of Consumption." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1977. Kim, Jae-On and Mueller, Charles W. Introduction to Factor Analysis. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1978. LeGrand, Julian and Robinson, Ray. The Economics of Social Problems. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc., 1980. Leonard-Barton, Dorothy and Rogers, Evertt M. "Voluntary Simplicity°in California: Precursos or Fad?" Paper presented at the American Association for the Advancement of Science, San Francisco, 1980. Lovins, Amory B. Soft Energy Paths: Toward a Durable Peace. New York: Harper and Row, 1977. Merkley, Susan. "The Effect of Past Experience on Current Energy Consumption and Conservation Patterns: The Interaction of Historical Time, Social Time and and Life Time." Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing, 1980. Moen, Phyllis. "Developing Family Indicators," Journal of Family Issues, March, 1980. Morrison, Bonnie Maas. "The Importance of a Balanced Perspective: The Environments of Man," Man- Environment Systems 4: 171-178, 1974. Morrison, Bonnie Maas and Gladhart, Peter Michael. "Energy and Families: The Crisis and the Response," Journal of Home Economics 68: 15-18, 1976. Morrison, Bonnie Maas; Keith, Joanne G.; and Roosa, Mark. "Household Energy Conservers and Nonconservers: A Discriminant Analysis Profile." Paper presented at the Michigan Home Economics Association Conference, Research Reporting Session, Mackinac Island, August 11, 1978. Morrison, Bonnie Maas; Keith, Joanne Goodman; and Zuiches James J. "Impacts on Household Energy Consumption: An Empirical Study of Michigan Families," in Sills, David; Wolfe, Charles and Unself, Charles (Eds.), Sociopolitical Impacts of Energy Use and Policy Contents. Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Science, National Research Councils Committee on Nuclear and Alternative Energy Systems, 1980. 166 Morrison, Denton E. and Lodwick, Dora G. "Toward Research on the Social Impacts of Soft and Hard Energy Systems: The Lovins' Claims as a Social Science Challenge." Paper revised for the Annual Review of Energy, March, 1981. Nader, Laura and Beckerman, Stephen. "Energy as it Relates to the Quality and Style of Life," Annual Review of Energy 3: 1 - 28, 1978. National Research Council. Energy in Transition 1985-2010. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company, 1979. Nelson, L. D. and Honnold, J. A. "Planning for Resource Scarcity: A Critique of Prevalent Proposals," Social Science Quarterly, 1976. Newman, Dorothy K. and Day, Dawn. "Energy in the Home," The American Energy Consumer. Cambridge: Ballinger Publishing Company, 1975. Nickell, Paulina; Rice, Ann Smith; and Tucker, SuZanne P. "Managing Environmental Resources," Management in Family Living, 5th ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1976. Odum, H. T. Environment, Powerjand Society. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1971. O'Toole, James. Energy and Social Change. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, 1978. Paolucci, Beatrice. "Contributions of a Framework of Home Management to the Teaching of Family Relation— ships," Journal of Marriage and the Family 28, 1966. Paolucci, Beatrice; Hall, Oivre A.; and Axinn, Nancy. Family Decision Making: An Ecosystem Approach. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1977. Pearlin, L. I. and Schooler, C. "The Structure of Coping," Journal of Health and Social Behavior 19:2, 1978. Perlman, Robert and Warren, Roland L. Families in the Energy Crisis: Impacts and Implications for Theory and Policy. Cambridge: Ballinger Publishing Company, 1977. Rice, Ann Smith. "An Emerging Economic Framework for Analyzing Family Managerial Behavior," The Family" Focus on Management. American Home Economics Association, 1969. 167 Safilios-Rothschild, Constantina. "Family Sociology or Wives' Family Sociology." Report Number 4, The Center for Developmental Studies in Cognition, The Merrill-Palmer Institute, 1968. Schmitt, Raymond L. and Guipp: Stanley E. "Resource as a Symbol," Social Science Quarterly, 1976. Schurr, S.; Darmstadter, J.; Perry, H.; Ramsay, W.; and Russell, M. Energy in America's Future: The Choices Before Us. Baltimore: The John Hopkins University Press, 1979. Steidl, Rose E. "An Ecological Approach to the Study of Family Managerial Behavior," The Family: Focus on Management, American Home Economics Association, lg69 O Stern, Paul and Gardner, Gerald T. "A Review and Critique of Energy Research in Psychology," Social Science Energy Review 3:1, 1980. Stobaugh, Robert and Yergin, Daniel. Energy Future. New York: Random House, 1979. Strumpel, Burkhard. "Responses to Economic Adversity: An Agenda for Research in a Changed Environment," in Strumpel, Burkhard (Ed.), Economic Means for Human Needs. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, 1976. A Time to Choose: America's Energy Future. Final Report by the Energy Policy Project of the Ford Founda- tion. Cambridge: Ballinger Publishing Company, 1974. Toffler, Alvin. The Third Wave. New York: William Morrow and Company, Inc., 1980. Van Es, J. C. and Shingi, P. M. "Response Consistency of Husband and Wife for Selected Attitudinal Items," Journal of Marriage and the Family, November, 1972. White, L. The Evolution of Culture. New York: McGraw- Hill, 1959. Williams, J. S.; Kruvant, W.; and Newman, D. "Metropolitan Impacts of Alternative Energy Futures," in Unseld, Charles T.; Morrison, Denton B.; Sills, David; and Wolfe, Charles P. (Eds.), Sociopolitical Effects of Energy Use and Policy. Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Sciences, 1979. 168 Wolfe, Charles P. "Recommendations for Future Research on the Sociopolitical Impacts of Energy," in Unseld, Charles T.; Morrison, Denton E.; Sills, David; and Wolfe, Charles P. (Eds.), Sociopolitical Effects of Energy Use and Policy. Washington, D.C.: National Academy of Sciences, 1979. , H “3...;— 'V‘ "IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 3