RETURNING MATERIALS: IV‘ESI_] Place in book drop to IJBRARJBS remove this checkout from .—c-—-. your record. FINES wm be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. p.33 1,2 1399 1 314 of»? "’“' 'OCT 3 0 2006'" NOV :11 2 121192 9:106 m .; ,3 ‘r '5 .1 \- \‘ I . U '5‘-" " . Autrc‘fff 3"" } *b.""‘ i 43.95.1353 i 334 3199: PARENTAL AUTHORITY AS A FACTOR IN DECISION-MAKING OF KOREAN-AMERICAN YOUTH By Heeja Kim Chang A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Administration and Curriculum 1982 ABSTRACT PARENTAL AUTHORITY AS A FACTOR IN DECISION-MAKING OF KOREAN-AMERICAN YOUTH By Heeja Kim Chang The purpose of the study was to examine the importance of parental authority in the moral decision making of Korean-American youth aged l3 and 18. The basic research question was the following: To what extent are the opinions and the feelings of parents taken into account in the decision making of the subjects? The secondary research questions were: (1) Is the extent of influence related to the age of the teen-agers? and (2) Does the extent of influence become greater or lesser in relationship to the time span of the teen-ager's immigrant experience? Based on these questions, two hypotheses were tested: (1) Youth of 13 years tend to demonstrate a higher response to paren- tal authority than the youth of 18 years; and (2) The youth who have been in the United States a shorter period of time are more responsive to parental authority than those of a longer period. A specifically designed instrument (the Authority Scale) was used to examine the effect of the subject's perceptions of parental authority in making moral decisions in belief and action. The instru- ment was administered during an interview of each subject. The Heeja Kim Chang subjects were drawn from Korean immigrant families in metropolitan Minneapolis. The results obtained in the study included the following: (I) there is an age-related difference of responses to parental authority, between l3- and lB-year-olds; (2) there is a length-of- residency-related difference of responses to parental authority, between shorter and longer residency; and (3) there is a tendency for the Korean-American youth to transfer from pgggflt_as authority to myself as authority for making moral decisions. TO MY PARENTS. who have nurtured me in how to love God and how to love people. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer wishes to express her sincere appreciation to her chairman, Dr. Ted Hard, whose guidance, encouragement, and faithful- ness aided this study. Dr. Ward's tireless effort and immediate feed- back provided a professional role model to me. I also wish to thank the other members of my committee for their many helpful suggestions: ' Dr. Benjamin Bohnhorst Dr. Joseph Levine Dr. Lawrence Sarbaugh Appreciation is also expressed to the ministers of Korean immigrant churches in the Minneapolis area who provided help: Rev. Dong Hyung Kim Rev. Kang Ho Lee Rev. Fr. Francis Choe Rev. Yung Jae Kim Rev. Yuh Sang Cho Rev. Byung Chun Yoo The Korean immigrant churches provided various opportunities for interviews of the Korean-American subjects of the study. I am thankful to have been associated with young people who cooperated willingly for the interview. Finally, I wish to express my gratitude to my husband, Yong-Ho, for his personal support and dedicated love, which provided an addi- tional encouragement to complete the study. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ......................... LIST OF FIGURES ....... f .................. INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY AND RESEARCH PROBLEM ..... Background ...................... The Family as the Social World of Koreans ...... Youth in the Korean Family in the United States . . . Importance of the Study ................ Definitions ...................... General Definitions ................. Specific Definitions ................ Assumptions ...................... Research Questions .................. Research Hypotheses .................. Hypothesis Concerning Age of the Subjects ...... Hypothesis Concerning Length of Residency in the United States ................... Summary ........................ PRECEDENTS IN THE LITERATURE .............. Related Developmental Research ............ Piaget's Theory of Logical Development ....... Piaget's View of Morality .............. Kohlberg's View of Moral Thought and Action ..... Belief-and-Action Consistency ............ Related Acculturational Research ........... Acculturating Influences on Youth Value Systems: Family ...................... Acculturating Influences on Youth Value Systems: Parental Authority ................ Acculturation Process of Korean-American Youth . . Summary ........................ iv Page ix Chapter III. PROCEDURES ....................... The Setting ...................... Dimensions of the Study ................ Design of the Study ................. Interviewing the Subjects .............. Hypotheses to Be Tested ............... Instrumentation .................... Computation of the Authority Score ......... Instrumentation Development ............. Pre-Testing ...................... Data Analysis ..................... IV. RESULTS OF THE INVESTIGATION .............. Measures of Parental Authority ............ Belief Dimension .................. Action Dimension .................. Measures of Myself as Authority ............ Belief Dimension .................. Action Dimension .................. Measures of Other Authority .............. Belief Dimension .................. Action Dimension .................. Conditional Analysis of Variance by Age ........ Discussion. Conditional Analysis of Variance by Age . Conditional Analysis of Variance by Residency ..... Discussion: Conditional Analysis of Variance by Residency ..................... Significance Levels of Authority Figures for 13- and lB-Year-Olds Compared by Residency ......... Analysis of Variance: Measures of Authority Figures . Discussion: Measures of Authority Figures by Residency ..................... Belief-and-Action Consistency ............. Pearson's Correlation: Measures of Parent and Myself ...................... Discussion: Belief-and-Action Consistency ...... Summary ........................ Discussion of the Results .............. V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS ........ Summary of the Study ................. Findings ...................... Limitations of the Study .............. Conclusions ...................... Chapter Page Recommendations .................... 79 Implications of the Study .............. 79 Suggestions for Future Research ............ 80 APPENDICES ........................... 82 A. THE AUTHORITY SCALE--PRELIMINARY VERSION ........ 83 B. THE AUTHORITY SCALE--FINAL VERSION ........... 88 C. CORRESPONDENCE ..................... 91 BIBLIOGRAPHY .......................... 94 vi LIST OF TABLES Table Page 3.1 Distribution of Subjects by Age: Initial Sample ..... 32 3.2 Available Subjects by the Years of Residency ....... 33 4.1 REPORTED BELIEF, Parental Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age . . . ...................... 44 4.2 REPORTED BELIEF, Parental Authority: Overall Cell Means by Residency ...... . ................ 45 4.3 REPORTED BELIEF, Parental Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age and Residency .................. 45 4.4 REPORTED BELIEF, Parental Authority: Two—Nay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. 46 4.5 REPORTED ACTION, Parental Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age ......................... 47 4.6 REPORTED ACTION, Parental Authority: Overall Cell Means by Residency ...................... 47 4.7 REPORTED ACTION, Parental Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age and Residency .................. 48 4.8 REPORTED ACTION, Parental Authority: Two-Hay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............... 49 4.9 REPORTED BELIEF, Myself as Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age ...................... 50 4.10 REPORTED BELIEF, Myself as Authority: Overall Cell Means by Residency ................... 50 4.11 REPORTED BELIEF, Myself as Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age and Residency ............... 51 4.12 REPORTED BELIEF, Myself as Authority: Two-Nay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency . . . .......... I 52 4.13 REPORTED ACTION, Myself as Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age ...................... 52 vii Table 4.14 REPORTED ACTION, Myself as Authority: Overall Cell Means by Residency ...................... REPORTED ACTION, Myself as Authority: Overall Cell Means by Age and Residency .................. REPORTED ACTION, Myself as Authority: Two-Nay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. REPORTED BELIEF, Friend as Authority: Two-Nay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. REPORTED BELIEF, Pastor as Authority: Two-Nay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. REPORTED BELIEF, Teacher as Authority: Two-Hay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. REPORTED ACTION, Friend as Authority: Two-Hay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. REPORTED ACTION, Pastor as Authority: Two-Hay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. REPORTED ACTION, Teacher as Authority: Two-Hay Analysis of Variance--Age by Residency ............. F-Values and Significance Levels for Authority Figures: One-Nay and Conditional Analysis of Variance by Age F-Values and Significance Levels for Authority Figures: One-Hay and Conditional Analysis of Variance by Residency ....................... F-Values and Significance Levels for Authority Figures: One-Hay Analysis of Variance by Residency ....... T-Values and Significance Levels for Authority Figures . . Correlation Coefficients Between Belief and Action of Parental Authority and Myself as Authority ....... Chi-Square Values and Significance Levels of the Direction of Belief and Action ............. viii Page 53 53 54 55 56 56 57 57 58 59 60 62 64 65 68 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1.1 The Southern-Italian Psycho-Moral Familistic System . . . 4 3.1 Distribution by Residency (l3-Year-Olds) ......... 33 3.2 Distribution by Residency (lB-Year-Olds) ......... 33 3.3 Selected Samples for Analyses .............. 34 4.1 Graphic Representation of Correlation Coefficients Between Belief and Action of Parental Authority and Myself as Authorit : (a) 13-Year—Olds of Short and Long Residency, (b lB-Year-Olds of Short and Long Residency, (c) All lB-Year-Olds and All lB-Year-Olds ‘ ...................... 66 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY AND RESEARCH PROBLEM The primary purpose of this study was to determine how impor- tant parental authority is to Korean-American youth in their 13th and 18th years in making moral decisions. A specifically designed instrument was used to examine the effect of the subjects' percep- tions of that authority when making decisions in belief and action. In this regard, the study dealt with the following sets of subjects: I. The l3-year-old of short residency (less than five years) in the United States, The 13-year-old of long residency (more than nine years) in the United States, The lB-year-old of short residency (less than five years) in the United States, The l8-year-old of long residency (more than seven years) in the United States. The design of the study involved the application of various treat- ments to four sets of ten subjects. Background For a better understanding of Korean-American culture, espe- cially focused on the parent-youth relationship, two areas are first discussed. Those are: the family as the social world of Koreans .and the youth in the Korean family in the United States. The Family as the Social World of Koreans The concept of the Korean family subsumes two institutions. The larger and the less important of the two refers to the family as a social group that includes all blood and in-law relatives up to the eighth degree on the father's side and the fourth degree on the mother's side, as well as the parents of the wife (Kim, n.d.). The more important of the two units, the nuclear family, has reference to the family of procreation (father, mother, unmarried children). An expanded household includes members of the family of procreation and some unmarried relatives, while an extended household would refer to two or more related families of procreation. The essential foundation of the Korean family system is the nuclear family. The nuclear family is tightly knit and headed by the oldest surviving male, generally the father. The nuclear family is "father-dominated but mother-oriented." The father is the head of the family. No one, not even the eldest son, to whom many privileges are given, undertakes an enterprise without first obtaining the father's blessing as an indication 4.08, (6) F = 1.989, (7) accept ”0- Age variable in belief dimension did not show an effect among means of age related to parental authority. As the same steps above, F value of length of residency (.106) did not reject Ho. Length of residency effect in belief dimension did not show an effect among means of residency variable related to parental authority. Null hypothesis (Ho) was not rejected. This hypothesis was not supported by an analysis of the data (Tables 4.2 and 4.4). 47 ' Based on an analysis of the data, no two-way interactions were significant at the .10 or .05 levels (Table 4.4). Action Dimension Descriptive data: ACTION, parental authority bypage.--Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram ONEWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.5. Table 4.5.--REPORTED ACTION, parental authority: overall cell means by age. Total Population Age 13 Years 18 Years 1': 9,17 Y'= 11.00 K'= 7.35 (N = 40) (N = 20) (N = 20) Descriptive data: ACTION, parental authority by residenc .-- Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram ONEWAY (SPSS, 1976) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.6. Table 4.6.--REPORTED ACTION, parental authority: overall cell means by residency. (Total N = 40) Residency Short Long X = 10.50 X = 7.85 (N = 20) (N 20) 48 Descriptive data: ACTION, parental authority 91 age and residency.--Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram TWOWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.7. Table 4.7.--REPORTED ACTION, parental authority: overall cell means ' by age and residency. (Total N = 40) Residency Age Short Long 13 years X = 13.00 X = 9.00 (N = 10) . (N = 10) 18 years X = 8.00 X = 6.70 (N = 10) (N = 10) L Analysis of variance: ACTION, parental authority.--With these descriptive data available, a two-way ANOVA procedure (SPSS, 1975) was used to determine whether any significant differences in means existed and whether any significant interactions were present. The results of this statistical test are illustrated in Table 4.8. Discussion: ACTION, measures of parental authority.--The first hypothesis stated that a significant difference would be found between the two age groups, i.e., 13- and lBsyear-olds. This hypothe- sis was supported. Two-way ANOVA showed the age effect significant at the .02 level (Tables 4.5 and 4.8). The second hypothesis stated that a significant difference would be found between the two residency groups, i.e., short and long. This hypothesis was supported. TWo-way ANOVA showed the residency effect significant at the .09 level (Tables 4.6 and 4.8). 49 Table 4.8.--REPORTED ACTION, parental authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. Source of Variation 5“” °f df ”ea" F-Ratio F-Prob. Squares Square Age 133.225 1 133.225 5.750 .022 Length of residency 70.225 1 70.225 3.031 .090 Age x length of residency 18.225 1 18.225 .787 .381 Residual 834.100 36 23.169 Total 1055.775 ‘ 39 27.071 Null hypothesis (Ho) test using Festatistics, about age and length of residency effect in action dimension was the following: F value of age effect was 4.588, and F value of length of residency effect was 2.708 was an effect among means of age related to parental authority. Therefore, the null hypothesis (Ho) was rejected. In length of residency effect in action dimension, there was an effect among means of length of residency related to parental authority. The null hypothesis (Ho) was rejected. Based on an analysis of the data, no two-way interactions were significant at the .10 level (Table 4.8). (Since the sample size was small [40 subjects], .10 was used as a.) Measures of Myself as Authority, Through the Authority Scale instrument (Appendix B), "myself" authority scores were measured to show to what extent the "my opin- ions" were a factor in making moral decisions. This was for the sup- plementary data as an antithesis of parental authority. 50 Belief Dimension Descriptive data: BELIEF, myself as authority by age.--Ce11 means were generated using the SPSS subprogram ONEWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.9. Table 4.9.--REPORTED BELIEF, myself as authority: overall cell means by age. Age Total Po lati n pu o 13 Years 18 Years 2': 10.07 Y'= 9.20 7'= 10.95 (N = 40) (N = 20) (N = 20) Descriptive data: BELIEF, myself as authority by residency.-- Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram ONEWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.10. Table 4.10.--REPORTED BELIEF, myself as authority: overall cell means by residency. (Total N = 40) Residency Short Long X'= 8.15 X= 12.00 (N = 20) (N = 20) Descriptive data: BELIEF, myself as authority by age and resi- dency.--Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram TWOWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.11. 51 Table 4.11.--REPORTED BELIEF, myself as authority: overall cell means by age and residency. (Total N = 40) Age Residency Short Long 13 years X = 5.60 X = 12.80 (N = 10) (N = 10) 18 years X = 10.70 X = 11.20 (N = 10) (N = 10) Analysis of variance: BELIEF, myself as authority,--Given these descriptive data, a two-way ANOVA procedure (SPSS, 1975) was used to determine whether any significant differences in means existed and whether any significant interactions were present. The results of this statistical test are illustrated in Table 4.12. These data are drawn from the question about belief_about best authority to follow. Discussion: BELIEF, myself as authority.--The first hypothe- sis stated that a significant difference would be found between the two age groups, i.e., 13- and lB-year-olds. This hypothesis was not supported by an analysis of the data (Tables 4.9 and 4.12). The second hypothesis stated that a significant difference would be found between the two residency groups, i.e., short and long. This hypothesis was supported. Two-way ANOVA showed the residency effect significant at the .02 level (Tables 4.10 and 4.12). 52 Based on an analysis of the data, two-way interactions were significant at the .04 level (Table 4.12). Table 4.12.--REPORTED BELIEF, myself as authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. Sum of df Mean Source of Variation F-Ratio F-Prob. Squares Square Age 30.625 1 30.625 1.142 .292 Length of residency 148.225 1 148.225 5.526 .024 Age x length of residency 112.225 1 112.225 4.184 .048 Residual 965.700 36 26.825 Total 1256.775 39 32.225 Action Dimension Descriptive data: ACTION, myself as authority by age.--Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram ONEWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.13. Table 4.13.--REPORTED ACTION, myself as authority: overall cell means by age. Total Population Age 13 Years 18 Years X= 10.75 Y= 9.05 X= 12.45 (N = 40) (N = 20) (N = 20) 53 Descriptive data: ACTION, myself as authority by_residenc .-- Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram ONEWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.14. Table 4.14.—-REPORTED ACTION, myself as authority: overall cell means by residency. (Total N = 40) Residency Short Long X'= 8.00 X'= 13.50 (N = 20) (N = 20) Descriptive data: ACTION, myself as authority by pge andr ’residency.--Cell means were generated using the SPSS subprogram TWOWAY (SPSS, 1975) and are illustrated by condition in Table 4.15. Table 4.15.--REPORTED ACTION, myself as authority: overall cell means by age and residency. (Total N = 40) Age Res1dency Short ‘ Long 13 years X = 5.30 X = 12.80 (N = 10) (N = 10) 18 years X = 10.70 X = 14.20 (N = 10) (N = 10) 54 Analysis of variance: ACTION, myself as authority.--Given these descriptive data, a two-way ANOVA procedure (SPSS, 1975) was used to determine whether any significant differences in means existed and whether any significant interactions were present. The results of this statistical test are illustrated in Table 4.16. Table 4.16.--REPORTED ACTION, myself as authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. Sum of df Mean Source of Variation F-Ratio F-Prob. Squares Square Age 115.600 1 115.600 4.776 .035 Length of residency ' 302.500 1 302.500 12.497 .001 Age x length of residency 40.000 1 40.000 1.653 .207 Residual 871.400 36 24.206 Total 1329.500 39 34.090 Discussion: ACTION, myself as authority.--The first hypothesis stated that a significant difference would be found between the two age groups, i.e., 13- and lB-year-olds. This hypothesis was sup- ported. Two-way ANOVA showed the age effect was significant at the .03 level (Tables 4.13 and 4.16). The second hypothesis stated that a significant difference would be found between the two residency groups, i.e., short and long. This hypothesis was supported. Two-way ANOVA showed the residency effect was significant at the .001 level (Tables 4.14 and 4.16). 55 Based on an analysis of the data, no two-way interactions were significant at the .10 or .05 levels (Table 4.16). (Measures of Other Authority Through the Authority Scale instrument (Appendix B), other authority scores (friend, pastor, and teacher) were measured to show to what extent the opinions of others were important for making moral decisions. This was for the supplementary data. Belief Dimension Analysis of variance: BELIEF, other authority.--A two-way ANOVA procedure (SPSS, 1975) was used to determine whether any sig- nificant differences in means existed and whether any significant interactions were present. The results of this statistical test are illustrated in Tables 4.17, 4.18, and 4.19. Table 4.17.--REPORTED BELIEF, friend as authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. . . Sum of Mean Source of Var1at10n Squares df Square F-Ratio F-Prob. Age 6.400 1 6.400 1.167 .287 Length of residency 12.100 1 12.100 2.207 .146 Age x length of residency 1.600 1 1.600 .292 .592 Residual 197.400 36 5.483 Tota1 217.500 39 5.577 56 Table 4.18.--REPORTED BELIEF, pastor as authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. Source of Variation 333,325 df Sgfigfle F-Ratio F-Prob. Age .225 1 .225 .031 y .861 Length of residency 18.225 1 18.225 2.509 .122 Age x length of residency .025 l .025 .003 .954 Residual 261.500 36 7.264 Total 279.975 39 7.179 Table 4.19.-~REPORTED BELIEF, teacher as authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. Source of Variation Sgflaggs df Sufigge F-Ratio F-Prob. Age 3.600 1 3.600 1.600 .214 Length of residency .100 1 .100 .044 .834 Age x length of residency .900 1 .900 .400 .531 Residual 81.000 36 2.250 Total 85.600 39 2.195 Discussion: BELIEF, other authority.--The first and second hypotheses--concerning age and residency--were not supported here by analyses of the data (Tables 4.17, 4.18, and 4.19). Based on analyses of the data, no two-way interactions were significant at the .10 or .05 levels (Tables 4.17, 4.18, and 4.19). 57 Action Dimension Analysis of variance: ACTION, other authority.--A two-way ANOVA procedure (SPSS, 1975) was used to determine whether any sig- nificant differences in means existed and whether any significant interactions were present. The results of this statistical test are illustrated in Tables 4.20, 4.21, and 4.22. Table 4.20.--REPORTED ACTION, friend as authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. Source of Variation ggflafizs df $25326 F-Ratio F-Prob. Age 8.100 1 8.100 2.253 .142 Length of residency 3.600 1 3.600 1.002 .324 Age x length of residency .000 1 .000 .000’ .999 Residual 129.400 36 3.594 Total 141.100 39 3.618 Table 4.21.--REPORTED ACTION, pastor as authority: two-way analysis of variance-~age by residency. Mean . Square F-Ratio F-Prob. . . Sum of Source of Var1at10n Squares df Age 4.225 1 Length of residency‘ 11.025 1 Age x length of residency .025 1 Residual 201.500 36 4.225 .755 .391 11.025 1.970 .169 .025 .004 .947 5.597 Tota1 216.775 39 5.558 58 Table 4.22.--REPORTED ACTION, teacher as authority: two-way analysis of variance--age by residency. Sum of df Mean Source of Variation F-Ratio F-Prob. Squares Square Age 2.025 t 1 2.025 .786 .381 Length of residency .025 l .025 .010 .922 Age x length of residency 2.025 1 2.025 .786 .381 Residual 92.700 36 2.575 Total 96.775 39 2.481 . Discussion: ACTION, other authority,--The first hypothesis stated that a significant difference would be found between the two age groups, i.e., 13- and 18ump mucmcvmwm mac; so mupoueom>uwp .m> zucmupmmm one; so napalsem>ump .m> zucoowmom ugosm yo muPOTLam>um— mmezmpu xpweogpz< mnaocw mereeaeou .mmgamvw xuvcozuaa cow mpm>mp maneuvevcmrm use mmzpe>uhuu.m~.¢ mpnep 65 mu_o-eemxin FP< u FP< mp mupoieemaum— _~< u —P< mp sacmupmoe mcop mo mopoieemaimp u Amp sucocwmme peogm so mvpoueemzump u map xocmuvmoc ago, ea mcpoieemzimF a 4m. aocmumec “seem so mcpoiemmzimp u mmF "mug meow. ems“. ommm. once. ovum. scum. spam»: _mpo. mmum. mmmm. ammo. memo. anew. pcwems __< m. _.< m_ . 4m. . mm. em, mm, mmczmpm supeogp=< 8.52 23 32mm 5853 5:29:09 .xupeocaam mm :3? use 3.523% 23:28 .3 .538 can 323 5853 3:33:08 cotepmtouiémé ~32. 66 mzop new peocm eo mupoieemanm. Amy .mupouemmximp p—m use mupoieeoximp Ppe Auv .xucmuvmmc use, use ugosm mo mupouemmaimp Any .Aucmuwmmg "supsogpzm me wpmmxs use zuwsoguam Pangaea; mo copuoe u:a__*mwpmn :mmzpmn mucmpowwemou copumpmceou yo covamucommeame ovgaeeuiu._.¢ mgamwm spam»: 1-1-1-1---- aucwupmmc peocm mo mupoueemximp peace; xucmcwmoe acop we mupoiemmxump xocmupmmg ugogm we mupoigemaump PF< mp Fp< m— 4mp mmp 4m_ mmp _ F _ _ F mupousemximp FF< mupoieemzu aucmowmme acop so mupoicmmxi mp PF< wp ‘— _ _ E Any _ - pp< mp pp< mp —.o N.o m.o ¢.o m.o 0.0 ~.o w.o m.o Amp mm— Amp mmp max 67 Pearson's Correlation: Measures of Parent and Myself Correlational analysis (Borg & Gall, 1971) was used to deter- mine whether any significant differences were present as variations in scores. To supplement the correlational data--that is, to see the authority transfer (direction) between belief and action in making moral decisions, especially from parent to myself--chi-square was examined (Table 4.28). The observations following do lend some gen- eral support to the hypotheses of the study by providing data to complement the major test. Discussion: Belief-and- Action Consistency Figure 4.1 shows that belief-and-action consistency (correla- tion) decreases as the age and residency increase. Within each age group, the same tendency happens (Figure 4.1 [a] and [b]). Spe- cifically, Figure 4.1 (c) shows that as the age increases, the belief- and-action consistency of all 18-year-olds decreases. Within each age group, as the residency increases, the belief-and-action consis- tency decreases. The slope for l3-year—olds (the difference in belief-and-action consistency between 13-year-olds of long residency and short residency) is higher than that for lB-year-olds (Figure 4.1 [a] and [b]). The same tendency happens in parental authority and myself as authority regarding correlation coefficients of belief-and-action consistency. 68 .moo. v arse .mo. v are .e_. v a ea eeao_epeeema AoN u zv xeeeepmee meep me me—eueeeaump eee imp seem u use; Aom u zv seceewmeg «seem me mepeugemzrmp nee imp spam u agegm om n zw wepeieeezump pp< u mp om u z mepeueeexrmp pp< u m. “op u zv seeeewmee mee— me mepeieeeaimp u 4mg Aop u zv zeeeepmee usegm we mepeueeeaump a map “op n zv seemepmee meep ee wepeieeeximp n An— Aop u zv aeeeewmee geese me mepeueeeznmp a map ”xex eme.m mm. p~.m Ne. . om. Ne.m om. co. epemxz em. «eepm.w we. om. mm.~ ree~.e etm~.m rem.m eeeeee ace; mp 4m. Amp map Amp Amp mm— .m> peegm .m> mp .m> map .m> mm_ .m> em— .m> mmF .m> Amp .m> mm_ mezeew mewseeseu mecemvu zupeeeu=< .eewpee eee $e__ee we eewuuecpe as» ye mpe>ep eeeeepepemvm eee meepe> eseecmuwguun.m~.e epeeh 69 Summary In considering the operational definition of parental authority, this chapter discussed the results of the investigation in terms of the two definitional components and in terms of the two hypotheses developed for this study. One instrument was used in gathering data: the Authority Scale. Interviews were administered through the instru- ment. This interviewing session lasted approximately one hour. Discussion of the Results Hypothesis 1: Age received support based on the analysis of data. Measures of parental authority_and myself as authority both yielded significant results in this regard, in both the belief and action dimensions. Hypothesis 2: Residency was supported by the results of Authority Scale scores on the data. Measures ofpparental authority and myself as authority both yielded significant results in this regard, in both the belief and action dimensions. The analysis of variance conducted on the dependent variable, parental authority, in this study yielded statistically significant levels at .05 and .10. In order to compare the belief-and-action consistency, Pearson's correlation was used and yielded high corre- lations (higher than .52 and lower than .86). To supplement the correlational data--in other words, to see the authority transfer between belief and action in making moral decisionse-chi-square was examined. 70 This chapter provided a detailed look at the instrument employed and the analysis of data gathered in this investigation. Data-based observations were made regarding the two hypotheses of the study used in the operational definition of parental authority. Chapter V provides an overview of the study and discusses conclusions, implications, and limitations of the study. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS The study grew out of a general interest in multicultural education, focused on moral education and a perceived need for a better definition of its essential components. Little descriptive research has been conducted in reference to the cultural factors in moral education. Rather, most efforts to date have emphasized the theoretical aspects of the moral and conceptual development pro- cesses. Several writings have centered on the concepts required'hithe development and acculturation of youth and parents in order to achieve a multicultural perspective (see Burgess, 1926; Hoffman & Saltzstein, 1967; Kohlberg, 1958; Piaget, 1965; Solomon, 1960; Spiegel, 1957; Szapocznik et al., 1975; Walters & Stinnett, 1971). What emerges is a set of common expectations and ideologies that form a core around which the field of multicultural studies is constructed. At the center of this core is the concept of social interaction with authority./‘ xiCuriously, although the concept is consistently mentioned, little time is spent defining social interaction beyond the generali- zation of mutual interaction. The purpose of the research thus became to define social interaction in a more specific way, i.e., 71 72 parentand youth interaction focused on matters of authority. Further- more, this definition was to be specific to the acculturational studies, thereby requiring the two-dimensional emphasis of a techni- cal definition of parent-youth interaction coupled with a more ideo- logical definition. This dual focus allowed for an explicit, factual understanding of parent-youth interaction focused on issues of authority. Thus the researCh attempted to account for specific value-biased aspects of human interaction. The definition that resulted was based on an authority- receiver's perspective. This, in the writer's judgment, makes the concept measurable and ultimately applicable in multicultural educa- tion. The operational definition of authority is therefore reported here: 1. To what extent are the opinions of the following persons important in making moral decisions in belief: my parent, my friend, my pastor, my teacher, and myself? (Whose advice is likely be§t_about . . . ?) 2. To what extent are the opinions of the following persons important in making moral decisions in action: my parent, my friend, my pastor, my teacher, and myself? (Whose advice do I fgllgg_on . . . ?) These two components reflect the intention to create a defi- nition that is measurable and applicable. The first represents a belief dimension and the second an action dimension, which provide a view of how the authority operates. 73 Summary of the Study The primary purpose of the study was to determine how important parental authority was in the making of moral decisions by Korean-American youth aged 13 and 18. A specifically designed instrument was used to examine the effect of the subject's percep- tions of parental authority in belief and action in making moral decisions. The design of the study involved the application of the treatment to the following four sets of subjects: 1. The 13-year-old of short residency in the United States. 2. The 13-year-old of long residency in the United States, 3. The lB-year-old of short residency in the United States, 4. The lB-year-old of long residency in the United States. The subjects were purposively selected for treatment from the entire 13- and lB-year-old population. Interviews were conducted with ten individuals in each of the four sets. The instrument was used to measure the Korean-American young people's perceptions of parental authority in making moral decisions. The first independent variable considered was the age of the subjects, whether 13 or 18 years old. The second independent vari- able was the length of residency in the United States, whether "short residency" or "long residency," l to 5 years and 9 to 18 years, respectively. The dependent variable was the importance of specific roles (persons) as authority in the matter of moral decisions. Five roles (authority persons) were examined: parent, friend, pastor, teacher, and myself. 74 Findings Analysis of variance statistics were examined to determine the significances of the two independent variables (age and resi- dency) in relation to the dependent variable (authority score). One- and two-way ANOVAS were run on the dependent measures. To see conditional relationships or interaction of independent variables, conditional ANOVAS were used (Rosenberg, 1968). Significant differ- ences in main-effects means were found within the dependent variable, especially in the area of perept_as authority and my§e1f_as authority in belief and action dimensions. Follow-up t-tests on cell means found the interactions to be caused by significant differences between groups. There were sig- nificant values (t-values) between l3-year-olds of short residency and lB-year-olds of long residency, especially in the area of peregt_ as authority in the action dimension (p < .05), friegg_as authority in the belief dimension (p < .05), andmygelf_as authority in the belief and action dimensions (p < .05). In order to compare the belief-and-action consistency in relation to age and residency, Pearson's correlation was used. To supplement the correlational data--in other words, to see the authority transfer (direction) between belief and action, in the area of EQESEE and mygelfr-the chi-square was calculated. In general, belief and action correlated statistically significantly from .524 to .868. In general, the consistency of belief and action decreased as age and length of residency increased. There were greater inconsistencies between the belief and action dimensions of the lB-year-olds than 75 between those of the 13eyear-olds, at a chi-square value of 8.21 (p < .005). There was a greater inconsistency between the belief and action dimensions of long residency than of short residehcy in mygelf_as authority, at a chi-square value of 3.42 (p < .10). Hypothesis 1 stated: "Youth of 13 years tend to demonstrate a higher response to parental authority than the youth of 18 years." Results obtained in the study supported this hypothesis. The Author- ity Scale test yielded an F-ratio significant at the .05 level (.02) in peregt_as authority in the action dimension, and at the .05 level (.03) in mygelf_as authority in the action dimension. Hypothesis 2 stated: AThe youth who have been in the United States a shorter period of time are more responsive to parental author- ity than those of a longer_period." This hypothesis was supported by the data obtained in the study. The pereflt_as authority in the action dimension yielded an F-ratio significant at the .10 level (.09). Mygelf_as authority in both the belief and action dimensions yielded F-ratios significant at the .001 level (.001) and .05 level (.02). Limitations of the Study In drawing conclusions relative to the results of this study, the fOllowing limitations must be considered: 1. The study was confined to the Korean-American population of one city and of only two age levels: 13- and 18eyear- olds. 2. The one instrument used to measure authority effects was designed Specifically for use in the study. There is, of 76 course, a limitation in the validity of any instrument that uses self-reported verbal data to indicate value choices, beliefs, and moral decisions. 3. Both independent variables examined in the study were defined in a highly structured manner and should be extrapolated with caution. 4. Subjects were selected for interviewing without regard to social class and other demographic variables. 5. One of the key dependent variables, perept as authority, was not examined in its component parts: paternal author- ity and maternal authority. Conclusions Keeping in mind the limitations of the study, and recalling the two components of the definition of authority, one can draw a number of conclusions from the results. It is clear that within an "American-dominated but Korean- oriented" setting, patterns of norms and values supported parental authority over the other authority sources examined, i.e., friend, pastor, or teacher. Based on the results obtained in testing Hypothesis 1, it can be concluded that there is an age-related difference of responses to parental authority, between 13- and 18-year-olds. It can be hypothe- sized that a reduction of parental influence occurs across time. Further, from the results of testing Hypothesis 2, it can be concluded that there is a 1ength-of-residency-related difference of responses to parental authority, between shorter and longer residency. 77 It can be hypothesized that perceptions of parental authority decreased in proportion to the length of the duration of the adolescents' experience as an immigrant. The data obtained showed as well a dis- tinct tendency for the youth to transfer from peregt_as authority to mygelf as authority. This seems to be unique to the Korean-American social structure. That is, while there are many studies Showing the American adolescent shifting from perept_to peer group as authority influence, this change from a parental to an autonomous source seems unique to the Korean-American adolescents and helps to explain their isolated socialization. The period of adolescence is especially important to Korean- American young people in the acculturation process. Even though they continue in an authority-dependent position, they gain, compared to childhood, both heightened intellectual powers and information that permit them to articulate and explain the demands and expectations now encountered. They are better able to understand the forces shap- ing them and their futures. The development of these cognition skills emphasizes the importance of a parental authority that guides and advises as much as it controls and constraints. Thought (or, as the study preferred to narrow down the term, "belief") was in Piaget's (1965) moral subject formulated in collabo- ration with others from early life onward. This belief is concerned with social interactions and the rules that guide them. The Korean- American youth's belief is socialized in the sense of being formed by parents who make their moral views knowable through interactions. This is not unique in itself, but the manner in which the development 78 is encouraged may be. The moral development within the accultura- tional process appears to be a prerequisite for autonomous belief. As noted, the data showed a strong tendency for the subjects here to transfer authority from paregt_to mygelf_rather than to a peer group. It also seems clear from the study that a two-level opera- tional definition of authority (e.g., developmental, acculturational) is requisite for an effective multicultural studies definition. It can be concluded that the combination of age and length of residency allows both content and values to offer direction for improved cur- ricula that will support cultural diversity and the individual uniqueness of the youth. Based on the results obtained, it can be stated that the operational definition itself, as developed, was an effective, viable construct upon which to base the study. The resulting instru- ment, which permitted the personal interviews to be carried out in an efficient manner, produced data that processed effectively in measures of the place of authority in making moral decisions. The instrument, the Authority Scale, worked well foraIsecondégeneration test as an indicator of attainment of the concept of authority. It appeared to be able to pick up the adolescent's perceptions of authority and to what extent they influence moral decision making by Korean-American youth. Finally, based on data obtained from the study, it can be concluded that both 13- and lB-year-old Korean-Americans are capable of understanding and applying an operational definition of authority. 79 Recommendations Implications of the Stugy_ 3 Given the result obtained, the acknowledged limitations, and the conclusions stated above, some important implications for educa- tion emerged from the study. It is first to be noted that the results supported earlier research in development (such as Kohlberg, 1958, and Piaget, 1965) and more acculturational research in the area of parent-youth interac- tions (Hoffman & Saltzstein, 1967; Solomon, 1960; Szapocznik et al., 1975; Walters & Stinnett, 1971). Those studies established an approp- riate age range for learning about such multicultural moral education topics as parental authority. The implication is that, in terms of readiness and receptivity, parent and adolescent represent an excel- lent curricular focus for multicultural moral studies. The study also supported earlier developmental and accultu- rational research that stated that moral or multicultural education can make differences in the positive way in which individuals adjust to life in an adopted country. Improving the curricula can have a combined outcome. First, the importance of biological maturation (age) can be understood within a particular cultural context, i.e., the family system of the immigrants' homeland. Second, the time span for accul- turation can be understood and accounted for within a developmental framework. Specifically for Korean-American youth, it is important to understand the place of parental authority and how that may vary according to the age and length of residency in the United States of 80 each adolescent. The implication is that teachers must be taught (in such cases where they are not already aware of the situation) to pay attention to both the acculturational and developmental aspects of the learning environment. ' A final observation is that this study provides a model for concept development and for the acculturational concept on both the level of belief and action. The implication for multicultural moral studies is that concepts can be made specific and measured. Further, these concepts can be stated as operational definitions that ultimately will provide adolescents with learning that they can apply. l/Sflggestions for Future Research / The preSent study has just begun to establish the viability of certain multicultural educational strategies such as curricular approaches. The writer believes this study ought to be replicated along several dimensions. First, the number of the subjects for this study was quite small. A similar study should be attempted for larger groups of subjects. The study should also be replicated using different age levels, in an effort to determine a structural developmental maturation for the perception of the importance of authority. Moreover, replication should occur with different socio- economic and ethnic groups so that the receptivity of various adoles- cent populations can be compared. 81 The definition of authority as used in this study should be reworked and refined so the core concept of authority can become an established standard by which to measure multicultural perspectives. In similar fashion, the instrument of the Authority Scale should be subjected to item analysis and systematic development so that future researchers have a stronger instrument at hand to measure the factor. Other tests and combinations of tests should be tried in an effort to achieve maximum correlation of measures and to further refine data collection. The gender variable should likewise be explored in future research to determine what combinations of variables other than age and length of residency relate to the development of multicultural moral perspectives. Finally, the value systems of the adolescent that emerge in response to internal conditions and external demands are the construc~ tions of an active agent; they are fundamental to our understanding of human belief and action. They merit much further investigation. This study has attempted to answer many questions. Still more remain to be explored. The need for answers is certain as we move into an age of increasingly complex and critical interdependencies. It is hoped that this study has provided some clues and shed some light. Moreover, it is hoped that this work has contributed to an understanding of the Korean-American youth and their relationships with each other, their parents, and with their world. APPENDICES 82 APPENDIX A THE AUTHORITY SCALE--PRELIMINARY VERSION 83 84 .wv-OEm cmu H nguer .N .mm_x eao H eaeeoez .e .euee ea mpeeee ee meepx pee: .m .ucmsmgwums Leena seuee mpcmeee >5 ee meme exee e» e>ez H segues: . .eeeeam x5 cw e>eg on new we eerx pee: . .gewee as see emeece e» :epm_pms sews: . NMQ’ .peozem :* zeeem ee mueeneam peg: .— Nlddd N11UVIA £39.49 oahwa 59.51....“ 1.53.0...“ 331.33 SJOnA [U.OUU I PJ 1:3 J 0.1. wIfiiI J SS dwaew 0d Ud J0 1.0 1...“ J 0.1. 1. UP. D. lu U 1. 1. :e zeppoe H on eew>ee omen: peeueeesp 3e: wmmaeum weave: ecu cw ee>Pp so» m>eg mee— 3e: om< ceeeemee Lee x_:e eee Fepueeeveeee sge> m? mpsp meH~mozhn< mzh .eewpeseeeee Lee» eeMIMxeezh xem .memeeeee .:e_pme=e :eee ee Emu? eee esp xe_e emeepe 85 N I. d .3 d .A a 9 J B S e S L. J .e .3 we a a I... U: 0 u u .3 .e J nr 4+ J S S co zopwmm H on eeH>ee ewes: Sueaaodml 10M afieuanv queaaodml nuequodmI $531 aueauodwl [Jan uceuLeaeH 3e: .mcweeee we; meueem ecu cw ewwp segues: .mH .eHeHm one use new ueeee meewemeee e>en H can: .mH .eu em on emeHHeu goeez .NH .emeZeu He 5 genes 3 meme peg: .eH .eHcmeez Lem eeeuee ee necege news: .mH ”Heemepeees wees mmHH as axes H cee 3e: .eH .mueeeee HE ea xeee eezmee :ee H segues: .mH .meeewee me emeeee ee eHeeme news: .m— .xem Heeweeeece m>ez :ee H segues: .HH .Lee as» ea on see H segues: .oH .ese; we xeeem ea emeemceH sews: .m .eEHe eceem HE em ee e» meHHH>Heee He meewx pee: .m 86 N I. d n... .d K a D. J D. S 9 S I... la a 3 .4. a 3 IL U: 0 u u I... a J p 1.. J S S :e 3eHHem H ee eew>ee ewes: aueaaodmI 10" afieaanv gueauodml queaaodwl ssa1 aueauodwl KuaA ueeaeeaEH 3e: .eae; mepmeee e eu mueesee ecem :ee H segues: Hmveeeweeizee ee Hmveeeeeeupewm :eeeees< e>ee eeu H eeseeez .Heeucee epgwe peeee seex eu eee: H segues: .eHeEem :ee H segues: .egeuHee :eecex Hecepuweeeu gee :Heu:_e2 eHeegm e3 segues: .xenee H see eH>eE ee meeHx pee: .eeeee :ee H eeseegz .xem eupmeeee ecu Heeee meewumeee e>ez H Hes: .eeeaee esp =_ nges ea cemeee sore: .eeHLeee Se H eeuee meeeeee HE new: e>HH ea e>ez H segues: .mm .mm .mm .mm .mm .eN .mm .NN .HN .om 87 ”w .1 .d .3 .a K 8 D. J D. S e S lo M a 3 1.. a I. U. 0 U U I. a J D. .4 J S S :0 ZOFFO$ H on eeH>ee ewes: aueaaode 40M abeaanv aueaaodml quezaodml $531 nueaaodml KJaA peeueeeEH 3e: .eHeeee geesex see: eNHHeHeem eHzesm H sesaeez .eeoeo ae HeeHe HHe 8.8H eaeeoez .eeweeeee :e e>eg :eu H segues: .mueeeeum no: eee es; meeewee e>eg eee H Leases: .eHe ee xewm ege xezu see: meceeee gem HHHHHeHm -ceemes egeem H ece .meeemwm .msezeeee HE gees 3e: .emHH :H mmeeeem we meweees Heeee meewpmeee e>eg H neg: .memwxe eeu segues: .ceHHeeeee :H e=He> we eeegp Leeaesz .eeee ae axaoe eoeez .eHQHe eee e_ asaHeoee m.e»HH ea msezmce esp eepm :ee H segues: .ezaea.HHe we eeHH page eaeeaz H ewe: .oe .mm .mm .mm .mm .mm .em .mm .Hm .om APPENDIX B THE AUTHORITY SCALE--FINAL VERSION 88 whose, aAWCfl» 15 89 AUTHORITY SCALE (ILLUSTRATION) b.4091, 31501115 ' ‘ 130052- aAVIw 63° 75— 'SHEEEEEQ can. .. (Belief Dimension) \ . ' Jfiyr 1W5 skflfitll which versus“ *0 choose A (Action Dimension) (24 Moral-Issue Questions) (Five Authority Figures) a 3 i T Friendsw [Pusher ] [Teach a. 5. Myself 9O .eseeem es» s? :geee ea semsee sews: .eHsHm esp use eee usese msewpmeee e>es H ses: . .ee em on emeHHee sees: .Hewmsweeee egos ewHH :5 eses H see 3e: .masesee :5 e» sees sezmse see H sesues: .xem Heemeesese e>es see H sesues: .ses es» on em see H sespes: .eses we seeem op emeemsep sews: eseEm see H sespes: .mmws see H sesues: .suwee :5 see emeese ea seHmHHes sews: meeee weeee SDHSI...J SDHSI..J 331.33 331.33 ILUuOUU [U.OUU I:.3J_p1. 1:3Jp1. J SS J SS so sorrow usese ewes H on eeH>ee ewes: mH euH>ee ewes: emeeeum eeewss esp s_ ee>HH so: e>es mseH 3e: em< mshHaozh=< uzh .PP P 04 «1 <3 15 46 r~ a: as c: O F xem 91 w l d J... d K 3 D. In D. S D. S I? In 3 3 a... 3 3 IL U: 0 u u I: 3 J p 1.. In S 5 so :oHHoH H oe eoH>ee emos: N l. .d n: .d K 3 D. J D.— S P S lo J 3 3 a... 3 3 IL u. o u u I: 3 J p 1.. J S S noose ewes mH eoH>ee ewes: .oeoeo a» Hesse HHe m.os segues: .museeaum no: ese es: meeewss e>es see H sesees: .eHo so :on ese Hes» ees: musesee so: HHHHHsHm (seemes esesm H ese .mseumHe .mseseose :5 sons 3e: .esHH sH mmeooam so msHeeee noose mseHemeee e>es H ses: .soHueoeee sH ezHe> mp esesp sesues: .uees o» mxoos sews: .oHeHe ese es meoseosa m.es_H o» msezmse esp esHm see H sesues: .eeose HHe me esHH Hes: sense: H ses: .eses msHmsas e on easesee esem see H sesues: .HmveseHssxos so HoveseHssHsHm seoHsee< e>es see H sesees: .Hosuceo sesHs geese :e:: o» eee: H sesues: .eseuHeo geese: HesoHuHeese see sHeusHes eHoosm e: sesees: .Honse H see eH>oe so mesHs Hes: .em .mm .mm .FN .om .mH .m— .N— .oH .mH .ep .mH .NH APPENDIX C CORRESPONDENCE 92 93 Heeja K. Chang 2053 Knapp Ave., #E-l St. Paul, MN 55108 October, 1981 Dear , This is Mrs. Heeja Chang, who is currently doing research on "Parental Authority as a Factor in Decision Making of Korean-American Youth," in part to satisfy requirements for the Ph.D. degree which I am completing at Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan. This study needs l3-year-olds and 18-year-olds for interview- ing. The interviewing will last 1/2 to 1 hour. I was given your name from your minister of your church. However, your name will not be used in any report of the study. I will call you to arrange time and place for the interview. Thanks for your cooperation. Please call me if you have any ques- tions. My telephone number is 644-4383. Thanks. Sincerely yours, Heeja K. Chang BIBLIOGRAPHY 94 BIBLIOGRAPHY Bandura, A., and MCDonald, F. "Influences of Social Reinforcement and the Behavior of Models in Shaping Children's Moral Judgments." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 67 (1963): 274-81. Baumrind, Diana. "Authoritarian vs. Authoritative Parental Control." Adolescence 3 (1968): 255-72. . "Current Patterns of Parental Authority." Developmental Psychology Monograph, Part 2; 4,1 (1971). Becker, James M., ed. Schooling‘for a Global Ag_, New York: McGraw- Hill Book Company, 1979. Bengtson, V. L. "The Generation Gap: A Review and Typology of Social- Psychological Perspectives." Youth and Society 2 (1970): 7-32. . "Generation and Family Effects in Value Socialization.“ American Sociological Review 40 (1975): 358-71. Bengtson, V. L., and Starr, J. M. "Contrast and Consensus: A Genera- tional Analysis of Youth in the 19705." In Youth (74th Yearbook, National Society for the Study of Education). Edited by R. J. Havighurst and P. H. Dreyer. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1975. Berger, Henry G. Ethno-pedagogy: A Manual in Cultural Sensitivity, With Techniques for Improving Cross-cultural Teaching by Fitting, Ethnic Patterns. Albuquerque, N.M.: Southwestern Cooperative Educational Laboratory, Inc., 1968. Block, J. H. "Generational Continuity and Discontinuity in the Under- standing of Societal Rejection." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 22 (1972): 333-45. Boehm, L., and Nass, M. L. "Social Class Difference in Conscience Development." Child Development 33 (1967): 565-74. Borg, Walter R., and Gall, Meredith 0. Education Research. New York: Longman, Inc., 1979. Brittain,C. V. "Adolescent Choices and Parent-Peer Cross-Pressures." American Sociological Review 28 (1963): 385-91. 95 96 "Age and Sex of Siblings and Conformity Toward Parents Versus Peers in Adolescence." Child Development 37 (1966): 709-14. Broderick, Carlfred B. “Beyond the Five Conceptual Frameworks: A Decade of Development in Family Theory." Journal of Marriage and the Family 33 (1971): 139-59. Burgess, E. W. "The Family as a Unity of Interacting Personalities." Family 7 (1926): 3-9. Campbell, Ernest Q. "Adolescent Socialization." In Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research. Edited by David A. GoSlin. Chicago: Rank’McNally, 1969. Carey, Philip, and Mariam, Alemayehu G. "Socialization and the Pro- cess of Migration: The Case of the International Student in the U.S." In Sourcebook on the New Immigration. Edited by Roy Simon Bryce-Laporte. New Jersey: Transaction Books, 1980. Chang, Won Ho. "Communication and Acculturation: A Case Study of Koreans in Los Angeles." Ph.D. dissertation, University of Iowa, 1972. Clausen, John A. “Family Structure, Socialization, and Personality." In Review of Child Develo ment Research. Edited by Lois W. Hoffman and'Martin L. Ho man. New.York: Russell Sage Founda- tion, 1966. Colby, A. "Logical Operational Limitations on the Development of Moral Judgment." Dissertation Abstracts International. Ann Arbor: University Microfilms, 1973. Cowan, P. A. et a1. "Social Learning and Piaget's Cognitive Theory of Moral Development." Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology 2 (1969): 261-74. Crowley, P. M. “Effects of Training Upon Objectivity of Moral Judg- ment in Grade School Children." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 8 (1968): 228-32. Davis, A., and Havighurst, R. J. "Social Class and Color Differences in Child Rearing." American Sociological Review 2 (1966): 698-710. Donovan, Frank R. Raising Your Children: What Behavioral Scientists Have Discovered. New York: Thomas Y2 Crowell Company, 19682 Duvall, Evelyn M. "Conceptions of Parenthood.“ American Journal of Sociology 52 (1946): 193-203. 97 Emmerich, Helen J. "The Influence of Parents and Peers on Choices Maggaby Adolescents." Journal of Youth and Adolescence 7,2 Ericson, Martha C. "Social Status and Child Rearing Practices." In Readings in Social Psychology. Edited by R. M. Newcomb and Eugene L. Hartley. New York: Henry Holt and Company. Feather, N. T. Values in Education and Society. New York: Free Press, 1975. . "Family Resemblances in Conservatism: Are Daughters More Similar to Parents Than Sons Are?" Journal of Personality 46 (1978): 260-78. . "Values in Adolescence." In Handbook of Adolescent Psychology.. Edited by Joseph Adelson. New York: JOhn'Wiley and Sons, 1980. Feuer, L. The Conflict of Generations. New York: Basic Books, 1969. Friere, Paulo. Pedagogy of the Qppressed. Translated by M. 8. Ramos. New York: Harder and’Harder, 1970. Habermas, J. Knowledge and Human Interests. Translated by J. J. Shapiro. Boston: Beacon Press, 1971. Han, Sang-en. "A Study of Social Religious Participation in Relation- ship to Occupational Mobility and Self-Esteem Among Korean Immi- grants in Chicago." Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University, 1973. Hayes, William L. Statistics for the Social Sciences. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1973. * Hess, R. 0., and Easton, 0. “The Child's Chan ing Image of the President." Public Qpinion Quarterly 24 1960): 632-44. Hill, R. Family Development in Three Generations. Cambridge, Mass.: Schenkman, 1970. Hoffman, M. L., and Saltzstein, H. "Parent Discipline and the Child's Moral Development." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 5 (1967): 45-67. Hyun, Paul S. The Understanding of Korean Churches and the Chicagg, Presbyteryfs Strategies for Ethnic Churches. Chicago: Asian PreSbyterian Churches and Program Cabinet, 1978. Issac, Stephen, and Michael, William B. Handbook in Research and Evaluation. San Diego, Calif.: Robert R. Knapp PubliSher, 1971. 98 Jennings, M. K., and Niemi, R. G. "The Transmission of Political Values From Parents to Child." American Political Science Review 62 (1968): 169-84. The Political Character of Adolescence. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1974. Johnson, Arthur. Dialogue on Youth: An Interdisciplinary View. Seminar Series No. 1. Center for Youth Development and’Research, University of Minnesota, August 1970. Kang, Shin-Pyo. "The East Asian Culture and Its Transformation in the West: A Cognitive Approach to Changing World View Among East Asian Americans in Hawaii." Ph.D. dissertation, University of Hawaii, 1973. Kendel, D., and Lesser, G. Youth in Two Worlds. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1972. Kim, Bok Lim. "Koreans in the U.S.A." Unpublished paper presented at the Cultural Seminar of Social Rehabilitation Service in Washington, D.C., n.d. Kim, Doo Hun. Cultural Value Reflected in Korean Family System. Kun-kuk University, n.d. (In KoreanT) Kim, Young Yun. "Inter-Ethnic and Intra-Ethnic Communication: A Study of Korean Immigrants in Chicago." Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Western Speech Communication Association, Phoenix, Arizona, November 1977. King, Karl. "Adolescent Perceptions of Power Structure in the Negro Family." Journal of Marriage and the Family 31 (1969): 751-55. King, 0. E.; Scopetta, M. A.; and Szapocznik, J. "Issues in Develop- ing a Clinical/Research Model for Latins." Paper presented at the 20th Anniversary Symposium of Social Work Skills, Hollywood, Florida, October 1975. Kohlberg, Lawrence. "The Development of Modes of Moral Thinking and Choice in the Years 10 to 16." Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, 1958. . "Stage and Sequence: The Cognitive-Developmental Approach to Socialization." In Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research. Edited by David Goslin. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1969. . "From Is to Ought: How to Commit the Naturalistic Fallacy and Get Away With It in the Study of Moral Development." In Cognitive Development and Epistemology. Edited by Michel. New York: Academic Press, 1971. (a) 99 . "Stage of Moral Development as a Basis for Moral Education." In Moral Education: Interdisciplinary Approaches. Edited by Beck, Crittendgn, and Su11ivan. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, 9 . Korean Directory. Minnesota: Minnesota Korean Association, 1981. Kraut, R. E., and Lewis, S. H. "Alternate Models of Family Influence on Student Political Ideology." Journal of Personalitygand Social Psychology 31 (1975): 791-800. Kwon, Peter H. "Report on the Needs of the Korean Community and Churches in the U.S." Unpublished Paper, Asian American Presby- terian Caucus, July 27, 1972. Lee, Dong Chang. "Korean Family Organization in the United States: Role and Value Conflicts in a Foreign Culture." Paper presented at the 73rd Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Asso- ciation, Mexico City, November 1974. McRae, D. ‘"A Test of Piaget's Theories of Moral Development." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 59 (1954): 14-18. Mosher, Ralph, and Sullivan, Paul. A Curriculum in Moral Education for Adolescents. Boston: Boston University, 1974. Nie, Norman H. et a1. SPSS: Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1975. Oh, Hyun-Shik. "Church Work With Oriental Youth." Ph.D. disserta- tion, California Graduate School of Theology, 1971. Park,-Hankyu. Religious Affiliation and Cultural Adjgstment of Korean People in the Chicago Area. Evanston: NorthWestern, 1971. Piaget, Jean. The Origins of Intelligence in the Child. New York: Norton, 1963. . The Moral Judgment of the Child. New York: Free Press, 1965. Original publication inCEnglish in 1932 by Routledge and Kegan Paul, London. Six Psychological Studies. New York: Random House, 1967. . The Mechanisms of Perception. New York: Basic Books, 1969. Rosenberg, Morris. The Logic of Survey Analysis. New York: Basic Books, Inc., Publishers, 1968. ' 100 Scopetta, M. A., and Alegre, C. "Clinical Issues in Psychotherapy Research With Latins." In Proceedings: Second National Confer- ence on Drug Abuse. New York: National Association for the Prevention of Addiction to Narcotics, n.d. Smith, Barbara. "Adolescent and Parent: Interaction Between Develop- mental Stages." Center for Youth Development and Research Quarterly Focus, University of Minnesota, Fall 1976. Smith, L. M., and Kleine, P. F. "The Adolescent and His Society." Review of Educational Research 36 (1969): 424-36. Solomon, Daniel. "Influence on the Decision of Adolescents." Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, 1960. . "Adolescents' Decisions: A Comparison of Influence From Parents With That From Other Sources.’ Marriage and Family Living 23 (1961): 393-95. ' Spiegel, J. P. "The Resolution of Role Conflict Within the Family." Psychiatry 20 (1957): 1-16. Stewart, John. Value Development Education. .Part II. Final Report: An Evaluative Study offithe High-School-Use Films Program of Youth Films, Incorporated. July 1973. Stuart, R. B. "Decentration in the Development of Children's Concepts of Moral and Causal Judgment." Journal of Genetic Psychology 3 (1967): 59-68. Sullivan, Edmund. "A Study of Kohlberg's Structural Theory of Moral Development: A Critique of Liberal Social Science Ideology." Human Development 20 (1977): 352-76. Sunoo, Harold Hak-Won. "Roots of Social Resistance in Asia and Its Impact on Asian Americans." Opening Statement--Earl Lectures and Pastoral Conference. Berkeley: The Asian Center for Theology and Strategies, n.d. Szapocznick, Jose et a1. Acculturation: Theory, Measurement, and Clinical Implications. '1975. Tapp, June L., and Kohlberg, Lawrence. "Developing Senses of Law and Legal Justice." Journal of Social Issues 27,2 (1971): 65-91. Thomas, Darwin L. "Parental Control and Support in Socialization and Adolescent Conflict: A Cross-National Study." Ph.D. disserta- tion, University of Minnesota, 1968. Thomas, L. E. "Political Attitude Congruence Between Politically Active Parents and College-Age Children." Journal of Marriage and the Family 33 (1975): 791-800. 101 Thornburg, Hershel. Contemporary Adolescence: Readings. Belmont, Calif.: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co., 1971. Tomasi, Lydio F. The Italian-American Family: The Southern-Italian Family Process 6f Adjustment to an Urban America. New York: Center for Migration Studies of New York, Inc., 1972. Transaction: The Interplay Between Individual, Family, and Society. New York: Science House, 1972. Troll, L.; Neugarten, B. L.; and Kraines, R. J. "Similarities in Values and Other Personality Characteristics in College Students and Their Parents." Merrill-Palmer Quarter1y_15 (1969): 323-36. Utech, 0. A., and Hoving, K. L. "Parents and Peers as Influences in the Decisions of Children of Differing Ages." The Journal of Social Psychology 78 (1969): 267-74. Wagley, Charles, and Harris, Marie. Minorities in the World. New York: Columbia University Press, 1964. Walters, James, and Stinnett, Nick. "Parent-Child Relationships: A Decade Review of Research." Journal of Marriage and the Family, 33 (1971): 70-111. White, Martha S. "Social Class, Child Rearing Practices, and Child Behavior." American Sociological Review 22 (1957): 704-12. Whiteman, P., and Kosier, K. "Development of Children's Moralistic 'Judgment." Child Development 35 (1964): 843-50. Yoo, Jai-Kun. "Personality Adjustment of Korean Children in the U.S." Korea Journal 13,12 (1973): 4-12. ARIES HI 1 “mmmm