‘ z“ ‘¢’ 3:13” 3 ’15".._ . 'w: ',.I.”"I 43'|I?\..fqrfi‘1€ '( -- ;- 3.: N ”w b 3 ' ‘ 3.337? 933% We 3’93; .' 31's,”- - . 3:23-1:33" . ' \f" ’V‘\|,. '1 , ‘. ”"35 ‘4. 1' fiJ,’<,J./F¢i'_fiw. . p ' fl . u) kl. 3. .. : '.“"J.3LS"_I.*I"‘A "Q \ . 44'" t 2.. " . .. ., . . ' t? -' ' '— . Mu .. SJ - I: . u .. .3‘ WI I_ .k 5' f . x' ', . ‘,' _ .‘ y S _ #39?“ .}.{I.\‘,‘ 09' 3" “'4’” ' . “no -‘ 3&4 :- .. 25"':'¢2»V.3.v:\r.< .. t‘ ‘Ti&3;.fij3;3s-f¢: ‘ 3-333: 13‘ _ '3 ’ . | " | . '13 . *8 3 .180 3:0 5’ ,3 “ I If.“ Izl: {'11. '.\\“" w,‘ h) w, J l... I)! n '6" It. Id. 0 . 3 [t4 \ ‘ :A',|4_v..:J if? I’LL“. T. 4 I ""333" ‘ 3 ’ "33311;:35*???22-7:"' ’ -T ‘ I i I ‘lll .\ 5 2°: 33353 3.13:3?» . |~ . '0‘ ..‘. .,-.‘. at? 3931‘“: fig”): ‘1», MW ‘33”;1 w :3“: ,3 - '3} “Ext-{riggi} 33‘» 3:22;- ‘1‘}: ’1' & f” I: J W i? ,- ‘, 3 1“" pk ‘l_wi\ In"; ;'3 53‘. ) 9'; -'v, '-- “‘33"? ' I. L i‘I-Q '." ‘I‘I':I.::t' 3" :35 $:g‘;* .H \w 12-9 r\*¥'““**’,“?~‘czw 61'“ .v” If.“ m ",6. 3 J- ”\ ‘3: V" ' 94 '3n’""}°‘\‘:‘f?‘".~‘ .1 If; 3‘}:- u {f} 3.93;. ,1 y. - '33:; 353 .fr ‘9‘" ' ‘3‘: L91- J' '3. ‘W ll[Killlllllllllll LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Shame and Guilt: Activators, Associated Unconscious Dangers, and Defenses presented by Morton Chester Mirman has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. , Psychology degree in Major pro essor I (; Elaine Donels n Gershen Kaufman Date 5/61/ b), "I" MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0 12771 )V1531_} RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to LIBRARlES remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date IEEg g c stamped below. , €51 S _ 3W M... MAY 1 5 N30? 230 SIIAME AND GUILT: ACT NATDRS , ASSOCIATED l/IVCUIISCIUIIS HANGERS, AND DEFENSES BY Horton Cites ter Ni men A DISSfRTATIUIl Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR 0F PIII LOSUFIIY Department of Psychology 1984 ABSTRACT SHAH! Alla BUILT: ACTIVATURS , ASSOCIATED ”NCO/(561003 HANGERS , Aha DHEIISf S BY Morton Chester Mi moon The present research investigated the relationships between proneness to shame or guilt and a number of variables hypothesized to be related to these phenomena. A single shame score and a single guilt score was obtained for each participant by standardizing and than summing the relevant measures from the following group: The Korpi Shame-Guilt Scales. the Cattell Embarrassing Circumstances Test, the Revised Stanford Shyness Survey. and the Hosher Forced Choice Guilt Scales. The relationships between these composite scores and: (a) the scores on the subscales of the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory, and (b) the prevalence of a variety of themes produced in TAT stories, were then examined. The results were mixed, although a number of interesting findings did emerge. First, as predicted, shame-proneness correlated positively with dependency, as well as with the prevalence of TAT "Rejection” themes. These findings supported the notion that the experience of shame is initially associated with an event through an experience of rejection, and that, as a consequence, rejection becomes an unconscious danger associated with a shame-inducing event. Secondly, several findings converged to provide support for the Kaufman/Tomkins model in which shame is a unitary state with a single affect underlying its variations. and guilt refers to a group of affective states-~shame among them--with moral transgression as the activator unifying the category. This was in contrast to the Lewis model in which shame and guilt are seen as different, but parallel, unitary processes, each associated with opposite ends of a continuum of psychological differentiation. A third finding stemmed from an analysis of the items of the Korpi Shame Subscale, and suggested that shame activators can be meaningfully organized into groups of such activators to which individuals are differentially sensitive. Finally, though short of significance, sex diffefences emerged in proneness to shame and to guilt. In contrast to most previous research, males were found to be more shame-prone, and females, more guilt-prone. To Dani: Fan hen he£p when I asked éan it Fan hen ematianai suppanz when I needed it Fan hen patience when I Ahauzdn't have expected it And flan hen £ave aLwaya. ii ACKNDNLMGHIHITS I would like to first thank Professors Gershen Kaufman and Elaine Donelson, who co-chaired my committee. It was largely through Dr. Kaufman that I became interested in studying the experience of shame. He-helped me to understand the profound role it plays in the development of the self, as well as the importance of working with shame in therapy. Dr. Kaufman's involvement in this research far exceeded that expected of a committee member. Throughout the course of this project he exhibited the rare capacity to be very excited by and involved in the work, and at the same time, respectful of the fact that, at bottom, it was really my project. This, as well as his generosity and warmth as both friend and teacher. are deeply appreciated. I feel very fortunate to have been able to work with Dr. Donelson. Her knowledge and skill as a researcher was invaluable. The important questions she raised. as well as her considerable editorial input were important in shaping the final product. Perhaps even more important, however, was her encouragement of my autonomy and the respect she showed for my ideas. For this, along with her unwaivering support and the considerable amount of time and energy she gave so freely, I am very grateful. Professors Bert Karon and John Hurley also made important contributions in their roles as committee members. I thank Dr. Karon for his incisive suggestions regarding my use of the TAT, for sharing his considerable statistical expertise. and for his often needed sense of humor. I would like to thank Dr. Hurley for his editorial input, his attention to detail, and for his willingness to spend a good deal of time discussing the research. particularly toward the end, when I most needed it. I spent many delightful hours with Hes Novak discussing my ideas about shame and guilt. This was not only enjoyable, it was also instrumental in helping to clarify my thinking. I also wish to thank Richard Genirberg for his help with the RNF word processor and the small army of research assistants who helped me with tasks ranging from coding the data to making.eopies of questionnaires to discussing conceptual issues. And finally, I would like to thank my wife Dori: She willingly made sacrifices throughout the course of this project, not the least of which was living with an obessional, preoccupied. and generally unavailable. husband. Dori also spent endless hours discussing many aspects of the research, making suggestions. and toning down my often over-intellectualized notions with the profound, grounded-in-reality insightfulness that l treasure in her. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page [IST OF TABLES .................................................... vii [[37 OF FIGURES .................................................... ix lIST 0F ABBREVIATIONS ............................................... x INTRUDNCTIUN ...... .................................................. I Shame and Guilt: A Comparison .................................. 3 Shame and the Instincts ........................................ 7 Shame. Ego Functioning and the Self ........................... ll Shame and Affect Theory ....................................... 20 Shame: Discussion and Synthesis ............................... 23 GUiIt 0.0.0.0.... OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 26 Previous Research .. .................. . ...... . ...... ........... 3l Hypotheses .................................................... 36 METHOD ooooooooooo oooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo ‘6‘} Participants .................................................. Ah Procedure .... ........ . ........................................ hh Testing Instruments ........................................... AS Korpi Shame-Guilt Test ..................................... h5 Susceptibility to Embarrassment Test ....................... A6 Mosher Forced Choice Guilt Scales .......................... #7 Revised Stanford Shyness Survey .......................... .. #9 Interpersonal Dependency Inventory ......................... 50 Thematic Apperception Test ................................. Sl TAT Ratings ...... .......... .. ........................... 53 Predictions ..... .............................................. 56 RESULTS ooooooooooooooooooooo ooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo 59 Reliability of TAT Ratings .................................... 59 Descriptive Data .............................................. 60 Shame and Guilt Scores ..................................... 62 Omnibus fi-Test ....... ......................................... 65 Sex Differences ............................................ 65 Shame Group Differences .................................... 67 Guilt Group Differences .................................... 67 Interactive Effects ........................................ 70 Hypotheses ...... ..... ..... . ....... ..... ............... . ....... 70 Dimension One Hypotheses-~Precipitating Events ............. 73 Dimension Two Hypotheses-~Associated Unconscious Dangers ... 7h Dimension Three Hypotheses--Escape Defenses ........ . ..... .. 75 Dimension Four Hypotheses--Avoidance Defenses .............. 76 Additional Results ... ....... ... ........................... .78 ozswsszon ........ _ .................................... 86 Methodological Issues ................. ............... .. ....... 87 Validity of Shame and Guilt Measures ............... ........ 87 TAT Measures .................. ............................. 89 Findings ............. ..... ... ................................. 9I Shame and Dependency .. ....... . ................. ............ 9l Shame and the TAT Measures ........... ... ........ . ..... ..... 9h Sources of Shame and the Division of the Korpi Shame Scale . 95 Sex Differences ... ............... . ............ . ......... ... 97 Shame and Guilt: A Comparison of Models .. ................. IOI Summary ..... ........ . ........................................ l03 Directions for Future Research ............................... IOA ARPENDICES .. ...................................................... I07 APPENDIX A - Tables .......................................... I08 APPENDIX B - Figures . ..... .. ...... ... .............. . ........ . l36 APPENDIX C - Instruments ............. ................... ..... Iho APPENDIX D - Training Manuals .............. ........ .......... I65 APPENDIX E - Research Consent Form ........ ........ ........... I77 APPENDIX F - Debriefing Form ........ . . ............ . ....... .. I79 APPENDIX G - Post Hoc Shame- -Activator Subscale Questionnaire . I8I REFERENCES ... ..................................................... I8h vi IIST OF TABLES Table Page Tables in Body I. Summary of Hypotheses ......................................... AZ 2. Inter-Scale Correlations ....... . ........ ... ................... 63 3. Number of Males, Females and Males and Females Combined in Each of Nine Shame X Guilt Groups ...................... 66 A. Mean Scores Grouped by Level of Shame-Proneness ............... 68 5. Mean Number of TAT Themes Grouped by Level of Shame-Proneness . 69 6. Mean Scores Grouped by Level of Guilt-Proneness ..... .......... 7l 7. Mean Number of TAT Themes Grouped by Level of Guilt-Proneness . 72 8. Mean Scores Grouped by Gender .......... ....................... 8l 9. Mean Number of TAT Themes Grouped by Gender ................... 82 IO. Elementary Linkage Analysis of TAT Variables .................. 83 ll. TAT Intercorrelations for Both Males and Females .............. 85 Mean Scores Mean Number Tables in Appendix A for Males Grouped by Level of Shame-Proneness .... 109 of TAT Themes for Males Grouped by Level Of Shame-Proneness .....OQOOO.........OOOOOOOOOOOOOO ‘10 Mean Scores Mean Number for Females Grouped by Level of Shame-Proneness .. III of TAT Themes fon Females Grouped by Level of Shame-Proneness ..................... ..... . ..... . llZ Mean Scores for Males Grouped by Level of Guilt-Proneness .... ll3 Mean Number of TAT Themes for Males Grouped by Level of Guilt-Proneness ................. ...... ... ....... Ilh vii ll. 12. ‘30 IA. IS. I6. 17. I8. IS. 20. 2l. 22. 23. 2h. 25. 26. 27. Mean Scores for Females Grouped by Level of Guilt-Proneness .. ll5 Mean Number of TAT Themes for Females Grouped by Level of Guilt-Proneness ................................. ll6 Intercorrelations Among Korpi Shame Subscales ................ 117 Mean Scores on TAT Card I Grouped by Gender ............ ...... ll8 Mean Scores on TAT Card 2 Grouped by Gender ......... . ........ ll9 Mean Scores on TAT Card 3BM Grouped by Gender ...... .......... l20 Mean Scores on TAT Card 3GF Grouped by Gender ......... ....... I21 Mean Scores on TAT Card 6BM Grouped by Gender ................ 122 Mean Scores on TAT Card 7BM Grouped by Gender ................ l23 Mean Scores on TAT Card 88M Grouped by Gender ................ 12h Mean Scores on TAT Card I3MF Grouped by Gender ............... l25 Mean Scores on TAT Card lh Grouped by Gender ... .............. l26 Mean Scores on TAT Card l8GF Grouped by Gender ...... . ........ l27 Correlations Among Males Between Shame and Guilt Measures and Objective Dependent Variables .. ...... ......... ...... l28 Correlations Among Females Between Shame and Guilt Measures and Objective Dependent Variables ....... ........... ..... l29 Correlations Between Shame and Guilt Measures and Objective Dependent Variables ....................... I30 Correlations Among Males Between Shame and Guilt Measures and TAT Variables . ........ .......... ....... ............. I3I Correlations Among Females Between Shame and Guilt Measures and TAT variab'es ....OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOI000...... 000000 I32 Correlations Between Shame and Guilt Measures and TAT Variables ... .................................... I33 Correlations Between Korpi Shame Subscales and Objective Dependent Variables ... ....... . ......... ... l3h Correlations Between Korpi Shame Subscales and TAT Themes ...... .... ................................ I35 viii figure ................. I. 2. IO. Number of Stories as a Function Number of Stories as a Function Number of Stories LIST OF FIGURES ....... .................................... Page Containing Moral Transgression Themes of Shame-Proneness and Gender ............ I37 Containing Punishment-Atonement Themes of Shame-Proneness and Gender ..... . ...... -l37 Containing Punishment Atonement or Compensation-Atonement Themes as a Function of Shame-Proneness and Gender ............................. I37 Number of Stories as a Function Number of Stories as a Function Number of Stories as a Function Number of Stories Containing Moral Goodness Themes of Shame-Proneness and Gender ............ I37 Containing Inadequacy Themes of Shame-Proneness .......... ............. I38 Containing Rejection Themes of Shame-Proneness ........ . .............. I38 Containing Exposure Reduction Themes as a Function of Shame-Proneness ................ I38 Number of Stories as a Function Number of Stories Containing Independence Themes of Shame-Proneness ....................... I38 Containing Positive Evaluation Themes as a Function of Shame-Proneness ................ I38 TAT Variables Grouped by Type ...... . ...... . ..... . ......... I39 ix [157 DE ABBREVIATIONS Guilt ................................. ....... Composite Guilt Measure Shame . ...... . .............. . ................. Composite Shame Measure KG ......... ..................................... Korpi Guilt Subscale KS ........................... ........ ........... Korpi Shame Subscale Task Competence (TC) ................. Shame Subscale--Task Competence Social lncongruity (SI) .......... Shame Subscale-~Social Incongruence Body (B) ..... ..... . .............. . .............. . Shame Subscale--Body Relationship (R) ....................... Shame Subscale-~Relationships Feelings (F) ... .......... ................... Shame Subscale--Feelings EMB .. .......................... Susceptibility to Embarrassment Scale SHY . ................................. Revised Stanford Shyness Survey MSG ... .................. . .................. ‘ Mosher Sex-Guilt Subscale MHG ............. ........ ............. Mosher Hostility-Guilt Subscale MMCG ....... ................. Mosher Morality-Conscience-Guilt Subscale IEMREL ....... ......... .. ....... Emotional Reliance on Others Subscale ILSOCO ........... ...... ...... Lack of Social Self-Confidence Subscale IAUT .................................. Assertion of Autonomy Subscale TAT ............. .......................... Thematic Apperception Test Exposure of Inadequacy (TATla) ................. ........ .. TAT theme involving exposure of inadequacy Moral Transgression (TAle) ...................... ........ TAT theme involving moral transgression Rejection/Abandonment (TATZa) ......................... TAT theme involving rejection or abandonment Punishment (TATZb) ................................. ...... TAT theme involving punishment Exposure Reduction (TAT3a) ............................ TAT theme involving reduction of exposure Punishment;Atonement (TAT3bI) ........................ TAT theme involving punishment for wrongdoing Compensation-Atonement (TAT3b2) ...................... TAT theme involving ammends made for wrongdoing Total Atonement (TAT3bI + TAT3b2) ........ TAT theme involving punishment or ammends made for wrongdoing Independence (TAThaI) '......... ...... . TAT theme involving independence, power or competence Positive Evaluation (TAThaZ) ............ TAT theme involving positive evaluation or attractiveness Total Nonrejection (TAThaI + TAThaZ) .............. TAT theme involving independence or positive evaluation Evil-Projection (TATth) .................. TAT theme involving nonhero-originated hurtful acts Moral Goodness (TATth) ............. .......... TAT theme involving morally good deeds by hero Total Moral Purity (TATth + TATth) ........... ..... TAT theme involving Evil-Projection or Moral Goodness xi Introduction Francis Broucek recently wrote that "shame is to self psychology what anxiety is to ego psychology--the keystone affect" (Broucek, I982. p. 369). Nevertheless. shame. described by Rycroft (I968) as the "Cinderella of the unpleasant emotions" (p. I52), has received relatively little attention in the literature and what has been written has failed to yield a consensus as to its nature. The sociologist Helen Merrell Lynd (I958) attributed the neglect of shame to the lack of Clarity about the meaning of the word, which she claimed is often used interchangeably with, or subsumed under the heading of, "guilt." According to Lynd. guilt is about a deed or a behavior whereas shame is about the self. Guilt is a response to moral transgression, the commission of a wrongful act; shame entails the exposure of a basic inadequacy of one's self. As a result of the (neglect of this distinction in conjunction with the pervasive focus on guilt proper by writers since Freud, shame has been largely ignored. Helen Block Lewis (l97l) has written that the concern with guilt--to the.neglect of shame--is a consequence of the disparate emphases placed on two of.the mechanisms of identification, aggressive and anaclitic. She cited three reasons for this (Lewis, I979). The first deals with sex differences in superego development. Freud des- cribed superego development in terms of a male model. It is formed out of identification with the father, stemming from the internalized castration threat. Through this process, the parental threat becomes I attached to certain impulses which results in the experiencing of these impulses as wrong (i.e.. guilt). However, Freud also described anaclitic identifications which involve the threat of loss of parental love. The loss of parental love becomes loss of ”self-love” via loss of esteem in the eyes of the parents. According to Lewis, both males and females develop anaclitic identifications out of which shame arises, but: because the anaclitic identifications begin early in relation to mother and because males must renounce some of their feminine identifications (while females need not). androcentric thinking automatically assigned shame an inferior place to guilt in a hierarchy of controls. (Lewis, I979, p- 379-380) Lewis claimed that the second reason for the differential emphases on shame and guilt concerns the dominance of an individualistic view of human nature. She stated that: Hartman's concept of an autonomous ego deepened psychology's commitment to an individualistic concept of human nature. The superego . . . is understood to function in the service of the developing autonomy of the ego, with guilt its prevailing mode. (p. 376) The third reason concerns the distinction between the self and the ego. The ego has been described as an individual's on-going ”motiva- tional-cognitive structure built up around the self” (Chein, I9hh, p. 3Ih), while the self has been described as a content but not a constituent of the mental apparatus (Kohut, I982). That is, the self is the mental representation that an individual has of himself or herself at a particular moment in time. The study of the self has taken a back seat to that of the ego and its negotiations with the instincts. Thus shame, the affective response to the experience of one's self as inadequate, has been neglected in favor of guilt, the response to one's giving in to “bad impulses." Beyond these explanations, however, lies the question of motivation: What are the dynamics behind the great concern with guilt and the virtual neglect of shame? Guntrip (I969) wrote that we prefer to see people as "bad but strong" rather than "weak and afraid": Historically in ideology and psychologically in the individual, the area of bad impulses, control, guilt and depression lay right across the path of psychodynamic investigation and blocked the way to deeper insight, as it was intended to do. (p. I38) It is less disturbing to think of our pain as stemming from the possession of mighty animal instincts than from the fact that ”deep within our makeup we retain a weak, fear-ridden infantile ego that we never completely outgrow" (p. 125). This notion bears directly on the question of why shame has been so neglected. We prefer to focus on guilt-producing conflicts stemming from the powerful forces within us, rather than on the shame-producing conflicts that stem from a fragile, needy self plagued with self-doubt and the fear of rejection and abandonment. This research was originally intended to be a study of the experience of shame. It soon became clear, however, that any discussion of shame would necessitate a consideration of guilt as well. The project thus evolved into an investigation of both shame and guilt, with its primary focus being a side-by-side comparison of these two phenomena along a number of dimensions to be discussed below. El [21,” . A number of writers have compared shame and guilt (Alexander, I938: Lewis, l97l; Piers 8 Singer, I953; Wallace, I963). Franz Alexander (I938) pointed out the difference between guilt and what he referred to as "inferiority feelings"--which will be seen shortly to correspond quite closely to later descriptions of shame. He argued that while both inferiority feelings and guilt involve a tension between the ego and the ,- —.. .. v""““—" .._-- - “J“; ego ideal, they are "fundamentally different psychological phenomenon." The sense of guilt is always felt as an unpleasant tension stemming from the expectation of a deserved punishment. It is the "intrapsychic reduplication of the fear of an external danger, the fear of retaliation on the part of those persons whom one has attacked. is attacking or wants to attack" (p. AZ), but who one believes does not deserve to be attacked. Thus a sense of justice must be present in order for there to be feelings of guilt. The feeling of inferiority, on the other hand, requires no sense of justice. According to Alexander: ‘5 4‘) /6the feeling is not so much not being good in a moral sense but , being weak. inefficient, unable to accomplish something. In I inferiority feelings it is not implied, as it is in guilt . feelIngs. that the self-condemnatIon Is the result of wrongdoing. (p. AA) The deepest source of inferiority feelings is the early conflict between the wish to grow up and the regressive pull to passive dependency. The ego. which identifies itself with the independent attitude. reacts to intrusions of the wish to regress with feelings of inferiority. Thus for Alexander. "inferiority feelings are pre-social phenomena, whereas guilt feelings are results of social adjustment" (p. A7). In a classic monograph on shame and guilt, Piers and Singer (I953) elaborated on the difference between these experiences. Their major distinctions can be restated in the following way. First of all. shame stems from tension between the ego and the ego ideal and so is a I , I/ response to a shortcoming--the failure to reach an ego ideal goal. Guilt is generated by tension between the ego and the superego; it occurs when a superego boundary is violated. .Thus "guilt accompanies transgression: shame. failure" (p. ll). Secondly, the unconscious threat involved in shame is abandonment--the fear of expulsion or ostracism and its consequences for a dependent self. ,Behind the fear of shame is the fear of parental contempt and the consequent abandonment and death by emotional starvation. The unconscious threat involved in guilt, on the other hand. is mutilation or castration--the fear of active punishment by a superior. Piers and Singer also described what they called the "shame-guilt cycle.” This refers to the situation where shame and guilt are alternately activated. creating a spiral of increasingly intense shame and guilt states. =//Leon Wallace (l963) described both shame and guilt as defenses against the feeling of annihilation. However, whereas shame protects the individual from the loss of external narcissistic supplies (as discussed below). guilt protects against the loss of supplies from the superego. Shame, according to Wallace, precedes the development of the superego and thus also guilt. It is a more primitive mechanism whose activation decreases as superego introjections become more complete so that the source of narcissistic supplies is shifted inward. In Shame and Guilt in Negrosis, Helen Block Lewis (I97I) discussed the relationship between shame, guilt, identification. and the superego. She expanded on Freud's hypothesis that the development of the superego involves identification as a response to the oedipal conflict. According to Freud, by identifying with one's parents the child is able to resolve the conflict between the love for them and the fear of castration by them in reprisal for sexual and aggressive wishes. Lewis described two mechanisms by which identification takes place. The first involves the "incorporation of the castration threat, so that the person experiences it 'from within'" (p. 20). This process is an attempt by the child to escape from the role of passive victim by ”becoming" the aggressor. (It should be noted that "castration anxiety", while originally referring to the male fear of literally losing his penis, is today more generally interpreted metaphorically. That is, it is seen as referring to the fear of being harmed and thus losing one's power-- originally thought of as a masculine trait. Despite its original meaning, as well as its sexist implications, it is now generally seen as a concept relevant to both men abd women.) In this way. the threat from the parent attaches itself to particular impulses and these impulses are then labeled as "wrong." This is the experience of guilt. The second mechanism of identification is the "emulation of an admired or beloved parental figure" (p. 2I). It involves the "incorporation of an ego ideal: an admired imago who serves as a positive model for the child's activities" (p. 21). This identification process is an attempt by the child to live up to this internalized, admired imago and so avoid the loss of parental love.‘ Loss of parental love becomes loss of self-love via loss of esteem in the eyes of the parents. This is the experience of shame. Thus shame and guilt are seen by Lewis as different but equally advanced superego functions developed along different routes of identification. Both shame and guilt are superego states which, though they differ in their phenomenology, have a common source in internalized aggression. Lewis explored further the relationship between shame and guilt by looking at individuals differing in "psychological differentiation" (i.e.. "field-dependence" versus ”field-independence“). The differenti- ation construct, a construct that encompasses a wide range of differences in personal style, successfully predicted that proneness to shame or guilt would be related to other personality characteristics. Field-dependent persons have a perceptual and cognitive style that can be described as less differentiated than field independent-persons. They tend, for example, to find it more difficult to perceive an item as separate from an imbedding context, are more likely to produce human figure drawings that lack articulation and differentiation of body parts. and are more influenced in their judgments by the opinions of others (Lewis. I97I: Witkin. 1950; Witkin. Lewis 5 Well, 1968). tewis found that the experience of shame occurs within a self-organization A pattern of field-dependence and porous self-boundaries suggesting a self that is inadequately differentiated from the environment. The self-organization associated with guilt. on the other hand, involves field-independence and solid self-boundaries (Lewis, l97l; Witkin. Lewis 8 Weil, I968). These findings appear to be consistent with Wallace's notion that individuals experiencing shame attribute selfjesteem regu- lating capacities to the environment while those experiencing guilt attribute it to themselves (superego). Shame_and_the_1astiaets .‘ J. A number of psychoanalytic writers have described’shame_a§_afljggge that serves to counter the instinctual drives (Fenichel, I937: Freud, I905/l962. I9lh/l963, l92h/l963: Jacobson, l96h: Levin, I967: Mayman, I967: Wurmser, l98l). Freud (I905/l962), in discussing the sexual instincts. wrote that there is an aptitude for polymorphously perverse sexuality in all of us. However, this sexual instinct has to "struggle against certain mental forces which act as resistances" (p. 28). These, KW r..——‘r fl he wrote. are shame, disgust. and morality. Freud argued further that while the inhibition of instinctual drives is largely learned, the learning process is built on certain constitutional inhibitory forces that develop as the child matures. ln I9Ih Freud elaborated on the function of shame, writing that it serves to insure narcissistic satis- faction by seeing to it that the ego measures up to the ego ideal. In I92h Freud wrote that shame acts as a signal to redirect and limit the f\// expression of sexual energy to avoid rejection. .hifi. Fenichel (l9h5) described shame as arising from early punishment / experiences in response to loss of or lack of bladder control. Shame l//I thus becomes the ”specific force directed against urethral-erotic temp- // I )\\\\\ v/ tations” (p. 69)--the two-fold pleasures of phallic-sadistic intrusion \‘ and the passive foregoing of control. Fenichel also described shame as a motive for defense directed primarily against exhibitionism and scop- .. ...-ov- w ophilia. He quoted Freud in stating that: "I feel ashamed" means "I do not want to be seen." Therefore \