110 035 IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII \|\\fi\\\\l\\\\\\\\fl\\\\\|\\\\\\\\\\W\\\\l\\\\\\|\\\\\\\\| - 1. m R A x Y 3 1293 10585 6318 Michigan State Univcmty' v "‘ :1", RESPONSE OF ADULI WILD BROWN TROUT(Salmo trutta) TO LIGHT AND VELOCITY UNDER.SIMULATED BANK COVER IN STREAM CHANNELS BY James Curtis Gruber II A THESIS Suhmitted to Michigan State University 'in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Fisheries and Wildlife 1978 ABSTRACT RESPONSE OF ADULT WILD BROWN TROUT(Salmo trutta) TO LIGHT AND VELOCITY UNDER.SIMULATED BANK COVER IN STREAM CHANNELS By James Curtis Gruber II Experiments were conducted in controlled-flow stream channels to measure daytime response of adult wild brown trout(Salmo trutta) to light and current velocity stimuli associated with overhead coverts attached to vertical channel walls. Groups of 25-30cm trout were offered sets of cover types. Occupation of a specific cover type represented a choice of known stimuli. Regardless of the light in- cident to the channel, the trout most often(p 0.025) occupied the coverts offering greatest darkness within the range of 0.0100-5.0000ft-c° Water velocity at coverts did not significantly affect cover use from 0-149mm/sec. Cover use was greatest(p 0.005) at lowest relative velocity within the range of 150-199mm/sec. This suggested that at 0-149mm/sec, the photic stimulus and response governed cover choice and that at lSO-l99mm/sec, the rheostatic stimulus predominated. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to extend my appreciation to Dr. Ray J. White, committee chairman, for the Opportunity to undertake this study and for his thorough review of the manuscript. Thanks, too, are extended to committee members, Drs. Howard E. Johnson, Eugene W. Roelofs, and John A. King for their review of the text. I am also indebted to the Michigan Department of Natural Resources, particularly the Grayling staff, for the use of their labor, materials, and facilities, and to Dr. Jack R. Hoffert of the Michigan State University Physiology Department and Earl Gillen of the Physics Department, who loaned me light recording equipment. The research and my assistantship were financed by funds from the Michigan State University Agricultural Experiment Station Project 1169, assigned to Dr. White. Finally, a special note of appreciation to my family( Ella & Jim, Scott, Eric, and Dixie) for their moral support throughout the study and for their physical assistance in the field. 11 TABLE LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . METHODS AND.FACILITIES . . . . Experiment 1 . . . . Experiment 2 . . . . Data Organization . . Statistics . . . . . RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . Light Measurements . OF CONTENTS Cover-preference, Light . . . . . Cover-use o o o o o o Cover-preference, Velocity . . . Cover-preference, Light-Velocity Interaction DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS . . A Suggestion for Further Research LITERATURE CITED . . . . . . . APPENDIX . . . . . . . . . . 0 iii Page iv 11 11 11 13 13 15 15 15 20 22 25 27 29 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Numbers of trout occupying coverts, the coverts ranked by relative light intensity within the prevailing range of SUb‘CovertintenSitYQQQQoooooooocooocoo 16 2 Numbers of trout occupying coverts, the coverts ranked by relative light intensity within the prevailing range of sub-covert illumination difference . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 3 Cover use and non-use by trout at each light range . . . . 18 4 Numbers of trout occupying coverts, the coverts ranked by relative current velocity within the prevailing range of V€10C1ties O O O O O O I O O O 0 O O O O O O O O I O O O O 19 5 Numbers of trout occupying the lowest, intermediate, and highest light intensity coverts within each current- veIOCity range 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O O O O 21 A1 Daily maximum and minimum.water temperatures for the East Branch of the Au Sable River at the DNR Grayling Field Office during the smer Of 1977 o o o o o o o o o o o o o 29 iv LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 Raceway showing experimental setup. Not drawn to scale . . 6 2 Top view of cover device, showing locations (A,B,C) of photocell placement during light measurements beneath it. Center of cell surface was 3.6cm away from wall and Sam above 3 treambed O O O O 0 O O O 0 O O O C O O O O O O I O O 8 3 Covert, showing point V where velocity was measured . . . . 10 4 T0p view of channel showing the shaded area at each observation period. Not drawn to scale. Channel widths and covert Sizes greater than aCtual o o o o o o o o o o o o 14 INTRODUCTION A better understanding of the daytime response of wild brown trout(Salmo trutta) to light intensities and current velocities as- sociated with instream bank cover should enable closer adaptation of stream management to the needs of trout. Cover for stream-dwelling salmonids is defined as a position affording protection or security by concealment from enemies. Position choice is a reactive1 response to phototactic, rheotactic, and thigmotactic stimuli which enhances survival. The behavior initiating a cover position choice is regulated and modified by the specific requirements of the fish and conditions in the stream environment (Lindroth 1955, Hartman 1963, Chapman 1966). Preference increases as more environmental stimuli are associated with a position, suggesting the response to cover is governed by a summation of stimuli (Hartman 1963, Haines and Butler 1969). The social environ- ment and food supply also affect position occupation. A review of the literature on cover use reveals that negative phototaxis and response to optimal velocities, relatively low vel- ocities, are universal characteristics of cover choice for adult stream-dwelling salmonids, regardless of other conditions(environment type, seasonal differences, and causative factors of reaponse, hiding, resting, and feeding). The response to overhead cover is primarily a 1 Lindroth (1955) distinguishes between reactive and spontaneous responses. visual (Bassett 1978) response to the relative light intensity beneath the cover (Stewart 1970). Stewart (1970) presented evidence that response by fish to experimental fright-cover devices was strongly related to light intensity under the structures. Use of overhead cover by wild rainbow trout increased with increasing structure size, decreas- ing structure height, and decreasing percentage of holes in the over- head covert. DeVore (1975) demonstrated that wild adult brown trout preferred opaque overhead cover placed 10cm above the stream bed rather than 15 or 20cm above the bed. Butler and Hawthorne (1969) showed that dominant brown, brook, and rainbow trout preferred the shaded areas of a 91.5 x 91.5cm overhead covert versus the shaded area provided by a 61 x 61cm cover. There was very little use of shade provided by a 30.5 x 30.5cm overhead covert. Kwain and MacCrimmon (1969) reported that hatchery rainbow trout at ages 10 and 24 months exhibited a significant preference for the darkest chamber when given a choice of five light intensities(10.000, 0.500, 0.010, 0.002, and 0.000ft-c). Haines and Butler (1969) showed that juvenile smallmouth bass preferred black coverts over clear coverts. Further evidence linking occupation of cover to the relative illumination under the cover is provided by the reaponse of fish to reflected light. Bassett (1978) demonstrated that wild brown trout preferred overhead cover with dark rather than light bed beneath it. Immature brown and rainbow trout showed an attraction to dark back- grounds, selecting black rather than white (MacCrimmon and Kwain 1966, Kwain and MacCrimmon 1967, Ritter and MacCrimmon 1973). However, the selection preference for dark background gradually decreased with habituation. Trout appear to reapond tactually to features of submerged objects. DeVore (1975) demonstrated that brown trout preferred overhead coverts with clear plastic streamers fastened under them versus cover without such a tactile feature. In comparison with overhead cover possessing the primary feature of incident light diminution, overhead cover attached or adjacent to the channel bank also provides a lateral surface which could serve as a tactile stimulus. Brown trout are the most overhead-cover-oriented among the trouts (Butler and Hawthorne 1968). Ritter and MacCrimmon (1973) reported that preference for dark background was higher for brown trout than for rainbow trout. Overhead bank cover has also been shown as the most important factor for determining density of brown trout in stream pools (Lewis 1969). Preference for current velocity has been studied with regard to the resting microhabitat and focal point concepts, thus separating the response to current velocity from the response to cover. Despite this dissociation, all resting microhabitat and focal points showed a high spatial correlation with cover (Wickham.1967, Baldes and Vincent 1969). The objective of this study was to examine daytime responses of adult wild brown trout(Salmo trutta) to light and to current velocity stimuli associated with overhead bank cover in controlled-flow stream channels during July and August. Specific hypotheses concerning cover-use behavior were: (1) Trout prefer the covert offering the lowest light intensity relative to that beneath other simultaneously available coverts. (2) Trout prefer the covert with the lowest water velocity relative to that beneath other simultaneously available coverts. METHODS AND FACILITIES The experimental procedure was to place 2 trout of approx- imately equal length and weight in a stream channel which contained bankside coverts of different widths. The positioning of the trout was then recorded 4 times daily for 5 days following a 5-day accli- mation. Raceways of the abandoned fish hatchery at the Grayling Field Office of the Michigan Department of Natural Resources in Grayling, Michigan served as controlled-flow stream channels. The water supply for the channels came from the East Branch of the Au Sable River. Daily maximum water temperatures, measured by a Taylor disk-chart continuous-recording thermometer, ranged from 16.1 to 24.40 during the study period, July-August, 1977. On the same days, the minimum water temperatures were 15.0 and 20.00, reapectively. The minima for the period ranged from 11.7 to 16.1C (Appendix I). The brown trout used for the study ranged from 25 to 39cm in body length(TL). All fish were collected from a lOO-m section of the East Branch of the Au Sable River within the hatchery grounds by electrofishing with a 12-volt D.C., variable amp, backpack shocker. Three raceways, 3.7m wide and 45.7m long, with gravel sub- strate were used. Each raceway was partitioned with screens into 3 dimensionally identica1 12.9 x 3.7m sections (Figure 1), designated A, B, and C, which served as the experimental enclosures. Each .mfimom on 55.3 uoz .msuom Hmucoawummxm M5395 mmaoomm .H shaman section contained 6 coverts, 3 per channel side(a set of coverts). Coverts were made from l/8-inch masonite sheeting and attached to the raceway wall at 3.225m intervals 10cm above and parallel to the channel bed. Each covert was 50cm long, but covers were of various selected widths. Data on light intensity and current velocity were recorded under each covert. Incident light intensity was measured in the atmoSphere about 2m above the water surface of the channels with a selenium photovoltaic cell(Weston, Model 856RB) held horizontally and housed in a waterproof plexiglas casing. The illumination-current output of the cell was recorded on a microvolt ammeter(Keithley, Model 150B) and converted to foot candles. Light intensities under the coverts were measured with the same photocell also held horizontally 5cm above the substrate and with the center of the cell 3.6cm from the channel wall. Three measurements were recorded at intervals of 12.5cm along the length of each cover (Figure 2). The average of the 3 measurements was used as the light intensity datum under that covert. Prior to each experiment, light intensity was recorded beneath each covert at 0900-1000, 1100-1200, 1500-1600, and l700-1800h (EDT). The correlation of sub-covert light intensity with incident light intensity above the channels was recorded daily at l300h (EDT). A correction factor, determined from comparison of various sub-covert light intensities with their atmospheric equivalents, was used to com- pensate for varing atmospheric light conditions encountered throughout the day. The appearance of cloud cover on an otherwise sunny day 3.6cm T (—12.5—®—12.5—®—125 @—lzmu—> A B 0 20cm AL 15 50cm . :1. Figure 2. Top view of cover device, showing locations (A,B,C) of photocell placement during light measurements beneath it. Center ~ of cell surface was 3.6cm away from wall and 5cm above streambed. reduced illumination under coverts by an average of 30%, whereas sunlit periods on cloudy days increased sub-covert light by an average of 333%. In deriving the sub-covert light values for each observation period, the 1300-h incident light recording was adjusted according to atmospheric light conditions(overcast, haze, clear) which prevailed for the half hour before the observation period. A current velocity under each covert was recorded prior to each experiment and was assumed to be constant throughout the experiment. The current velocity was measured with a pygmy current meter at a point 5cm upstream from the covert, 5cm from the channel wall, and 5cm above the bed (Figure 3). Constant stream discharge through the channel was maintained by regulating the depth of the head pool above the raceways. Water depth in each channel was 38.1cm. Experiment 1 Three coverts which were 50cm long and of 3 widths(l4,l7, and 20cm) were randomly assigned to positions 3.225m apart on each channel side in 9 experimental channel sections. Two fish of ap- proximately equal size and weight were placed in each section. After a 5-day acclimation, the position of each fish was recorded at 0900- 1000, 1100-1200, 1500-1600, and 1700-l800h (EDT) for 5 days. During each observation hour, each covert was searched once. Observation of fish under cover was aided by shining the light from a 12-volt spotlight into a mirror affixed to the end of a 2-m.pole. From a channel bank position, the observer, by placing the mirror underwater beside the covert, could see any fish that might be under the covert. Positioned initially at the downstream end of the lO a“ as“ Figure 3. Covert, showing point V where velocity was measured. 11 covert, the mirror was slowly moved forward until a fish was detected. Trout seldom showed that the light and mirror disturbed them, unless the beam of light was directed at the head region. Use of cover was defined as occupation of a position where any part of the trout's body was under cover (Bassett 1978). Fish which moved into a covert when inadvertently frightened by the observer were excluded from the data, along with subsequent observations regarding that covert through- out the same day. Fish frightened out from a covert were recorded as having the predisturbed position. Experiment 2 Three 50cm-long coverts of another range of widths(20,22, and 24cm) were randomly attached as before, and the previous experi- mental proceedure repeated. Data Organization Light intensity under cover was arbitrarily divided into 3 ranges, 0.0100-0.0999, 0.1000-0.9999, and l.0000-5.0000ft-c. Four ranges of differences in sub-covert illumination between the lowest and highest intensity within a set of coverts were established: 0.0010- 0.0499, 0.0500-0.0999, 0.1000-0.4999, and O.5000-3.0000ft-c. Current velocity data were also partitioned into 3 ranges, 0-99, 100-149, and 150-199mm/sec. Sets of coverts were placed within the appropriate light and velocity ranges. Statistics Analysis to determine cover preference was performed using relative light intensities and current velocities within the various 12 ranges. Cover use was tested for relationship to light intensity and current velocity separately. Frequency data were initially tested for randomness, then analysed using contingency tables employing the Chi-square test statistic(p$ 0.05). A 2-sample t-test was used to test the mean difference in daily cover use between experiments(p.<. 0.05). Cover preference was tested by the Scheffe proceedure for analysing multiple comparisons among means with designed contrasts. Mean cover- use per day for each light intensity, low, medium, and high, was sub- stituted into the apprOpriate contrasts to detect significant preference at the level of p50.025 (Gill, in press). RESULTS ‘Light'Measurements Light intensity under the coverts was a function of the width of the covert and the magnitude and angle of incident light. During any given observation period, the small, medium, and large coverts of any channel side, had the highest, medium, and lowest light intensities, respectively, regardless of absolute light intensity above the channel. Thus, the relative intensities of sub-covert il- luminations within a set of covers was constant.' However, the dif- ference in absolute light intensity between the smallest and largest ‘cover increased as incident light intensity increased. Light intensities ranged from 0.0125ft-c under the large covert(24cm) at the east side of the channel during the first obser- vation time(O900-lOOOh) on a heavily overcast day to 4.8147ft-c under the small covert(l4cm) at the east side of the channel during the last observation time(1700-l800h) on a sunny day. Coverts on the east side of the channel were shaded by the channel wall throughout the first 2 observation periods of each day, while covers on the west side were shaded by the wall throughout the final 2 observation periods of each day (Figure 4). During any given observation period, no overlap between ranges of light intensities under sets of coverts on Opposite sides of the channel occured, although overlap of light intensities under different sized coverts between observation periods, days, and 13 l4 Observation 1 Observation 2 0900 - 1000 h 1100 - 1200 h 3 II E I] I] N III I: II I: Observation 3 Observation 4 1700-1800 h S Figure 4. Top view of channel showing the shaded area at each observation period. Not drawn to scale. Channel widths and covert sizes greater than actual. 15 experiments occured frequently. Cover:preference,7Light Regardless of the light incident to the channel, the trout most often(p$0.025) occupied the coverts offering greatest darkness within the set (Table 1). Variation in magnitude of difference between least and greatest sub-covert illumination did not significantly affect positioning by fish (Table 2). There was no significant dif- ference in the cover-preference response between ranges of light in- tensities, ranges of illumination differences, and experiments. Cover-use Cover use was significantly greater at the lowest light. range, 0.0lOO-0.0999ft-c, and least at the highest light range, 1.0000- 5.0000ft-c (Table 3). There was no significant difference in cover- use in the various light ranges between experiments. However, cover- use was significantly greater(p$0.05) in experiment 2(847.) as Opposed to experiment 1(7SZ) (Table 3). ‘Mean cover use for both experiments was 80%. Cover:preferenceL Velocity Water velocity at coverts did not significantly affect cover use within the 2 lowest velocity ranges, 0-99 and lOO-l49mm/sec, although cover use was greatest(p $0.005) at lowest relative velocity within the highest velocity range, lSO-l99mm/sec (Table 4). l6 Table 1. Numbers of trout occupying coverts, the coverts ranked by relative light intensity within the prevailing range of sub-covert intensity. Range of sub-covert light intensity (ft-g) Covert width(cm) Relative Total within light 0.0100 0.1000 1.0000 0.0100 set *** intensity -0.0999 -0.9999 -5.0000 -5.0000 Experiment 1 l4 lightest 10 (24%) 25 (19%) 26 (27%) 61 (23%) 17 medium 16 (39%) 53 (39%) 32 (43%) 101 (37%) 20 darkest 15 (37%) 57 (42%) 37 (39%) 109 (40%) Experiment 2 20 lightest 19 (20%) 42 (25%) 10 (23%) 71 (23%) 22 medium 32 (34%) 49 (29%) 9 (21%) 90 (30%) 24 darkest 42 (45%) 77 (46%) 24 (56%) 143 (47%) Combined Results ** ** * ** lightest 29 (22%) 67 (22%) 36 (26%) 132 (23%) medium. 48 (36%) 102 (34%) 41 (30%) 191 (33%) darkest 57 (42%) 134 (44%) 61 (44%) 252 (44%) *** A set of coverts consists of 3 coverts attached to one side of a channel section. *1: Within these columns occupancy ratios were significantly different from each other at p30.025. * Occupancy ratio differs at p$0.0S. 17 Table 2. Numbers of troug occupying coverts, the coverts ranked by relative light intensity within the prevailing range of sub-covert illumination difference. Within-set difference in light intensity between darkest and lightest sub-covert shadowg(ft-c) Covert width(cm) Relative Total within light 0.0010 0.0500 0.1000 0.5000 0.0010 set ** intensity -0.0499 -0.0999 -0.4999 -3.0000 -3.0000 Experiment 1 l4 lightest 9 (24%) 5 (24%) 25 (19%) 22 (26%) 61 (23%) 17 medium 14 (37%) 5 (24%) 54 (42%) 28 (34%) 101 (37%) 20 darkest 15 (39%) 11 (52%) 50 (39%) 33 (40%) 109 (40%) Experiment 2 20 lightest 20 (18%) 19 (24%) 23 (30%) 9 (25%) 71 (23%) 22 medium 41 (37%) 23 (30%) 18 (23%) 8 (22%) 90 (30%) 24 darkest 51 (45%) 36 (46%) 37 (47%) 19 (53%) 143 (47%) Combined Results * s * * lightest 29 (19%) 24 (24%) 48 (23%) 31 (26%) 132 (23%) medium 55 (37%) 28 (28%) 72 (35%) 36 (30%) 191 (33%) darkest 66 (44%) 47 (48%) 87 (42%) 52 (44%) 252 (44%) ** ch * A set of coverts consists of 3 coverts attached to one side of a annel section. There were no significant differences in occupancy ratios within this column between sub-covert light-difference ranges. 18 Table 3. Cover use and non-use by trout at each light range. Sub-covert light intensity (ft-c) Total 0.0100 0.1000 1.0000 0.0100 Sets of -0.0999 -0.9999 -5.0000 -5.0000 covert widths(cm) Use Non-use Use Non-use Use Non-use Use Non-use Experiment 1 * (14,17,20). 43 13 136 30 92 46 271 89 (75%). (25%) Experiment 2 * (20,22,24) 91 5 167 41 46 10 304 56 (84%) (16%) Combined Results ** ** ** 134 18 303 71 138 56 575 145 (88%) (12%) (81%) (19%) (71%) (29%) (80%) (20%) ** Differences in occupancy ratios between light-ranges were significant at p50.001. * Differences in occupancy ratios between experiments were significant at p£00050 19 Table 4. Numbers of trout occupying coverts, the coverts ranked by relative current velocity within the prevailing range of velocities. Sub-covert water velocity (mm/sec) Sets of Relative covert water Total widths(cm) velocity 0 - 99 100 - 149 150 - 199 0 - 199 Experiment 1 lowest 34 (47%) 55 (31%) 7 (33%) 96 (35%) (14,17,20) medium 20 (28%) 54 (30%) 8 (38%) 82 (31%) highest 18 (25%) 69 (39%) 6 (29%) 93 (34%) Experiment 2 lowest 60 (30%) 20 (34%) 28 (65%) 108 (36%) (20,22,24) medium 77 (38%) 14 (24%) 5 (12%) 96 (31%) highest 66 (32%) 24 (42%) 10 (23%) 100 (33%) Combined Results ** ** * ** lowest 94 (34%) 75 (32%) 35 (55%) 204 (35%) medium 97 (35%) 68 (29%) 13 (20%) 178 (31%) highest 84 (31%) 93 (39%) 16 (25%) 193 (34%) ** Within these columns occupancy ratios were not significantly different. * Difference in occupancy ratio was significant at p50.005. 20 Cover-preferenceLVLight-Velocity Interaction Preference for the position offering lowest relative current velocity occured at the upper velocity range, 150-199mm/sec. However, preference between the coverts having different light intensity within this velocity range was random (Table 5). Table 5. 21 Numbers of trout occupying the lowest, intermediate, and highest light intensity coverts within each current-velocity range. Water velocity (mm/sec) Covert width(cm) Relative within light Total set *** intensity 0 - 99 100 - 149 150 - 199 0 - 199 Experiment 1 l4 lightest 19 (27%) 34 (19%) 7 (33%) 6O (22%) 17 ‘medium 24 (33%) 64 (36%) 6 (29%) 94 (35%) 20 darkest 29 (40%) 8O (45%) 8 (38%) 117 (43%) Experiment 2 20 lightest 49 (24%) 12 (21%) 13 (30%) 74 (24%) 22 medium 60 (30%) 21 (36%) 14 (33%) 95 (31%) 24 darkest 94 (46%) 25 (43%) 16 (37%) 135 (45%) Combined Results * * ** * lightest 68 (25%) 46 (20%) 18 (28%) 132 (23%) medium 84 (30%) 85 (36%) 22 (34%) 191 (33%) darkest 123 (45%) 105 (44%) 24 (38%) 252 (44%) *** A set of coverts consists channel section. *‘k of 3 coverts attached to one side of a Occupancy ratio was not significantly different. 'k Water velocity range occupancy ratios within these columns for relative cover light intensities were significantly different from each other at p.<_ 0.025. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS This study demonstrates that adult brown trout in a "semi- natural" environment respond preferentially to overhead bank cover offering the lowest light intensity within the range of 0.0100- 5.0000ft-c. Because this response was consistent in degree at all light intensities examined, I conclude that light levels never oc- curred which were lower than the trout's light threshold for cover seeking behavior. Under laboratory conditions, a light preference threshold between 0.0005 and 0.0001ft-c has been established for 2- year-old rainbow trout, based on their choice of 2 chambers with different illuminations (Kwain and MacCrimmon 1969). However, the preference threshold for response to sub-covert light may be dif- ferent than the illumination response threshold in absence of cover. That the response to light was similar for coverts of both experiments suggests that the response to the overhead cover was primarily a visual response to darkness beneath the cover, not a lateral line reSponse to the mass of the overhead cover. That cover use was greatest at the lowest sub-covert light range and least at the highest sub-covert light range is evidence that position choice was a response to photic stimuli (Table 3). Light intensities within the lowest range occured only on cloudy days. Cover-use on overcast days was greater(84.5%) than on sunny days (75.8%). This may be indicative of a characteristic of cover-seeking ‘22 23 behavior by trout. On clear days, slanting shadows from stream banks may, by virtue of their great contrast with adjacent sunlit stream bed, afford protective concealment from viewing by predators above the channel. On overcast(but still bright) days when lighting is diffuse and reaches more equally into most parts of the stream, viewing by predators may not be hampered or distracted by contrasting intensities, and the trout may have to seek smaller, darker niches, if they are to have concealment. The greater cover-use in experiment 2 was an arti- fact of the disprOportionate number of observations in the lowest light range. The interaction of light and velocity occurred only within the highest velocity range, 150-199mm/sec. Thus, I conclude that the rheostatic stimulus and the response to it afforded the trout a behavior for minimum energy expenditure and predominated as the major factor in cover choice at current velocities of 150-199mm/sec(0.49- 0.65ft/sec). Haines and Butler (1969) reported that areas of low current velocity are important in shelter use by juvenile smallmouth bass only as long as an area of darkness and visual reference are present. The fish positioned themselves beneath black overhead plates which were enclosed on 2 sides in a mean channel water velocity of 28cm/sec in strong preference to clear overhead plates enclosed on 3 sides and in mean velocity of lZcm/sec. This suggested that at these vel- ocities, the reaponse to photic stimuli predominated over the response to rheostatic stimuli. Use of higher water velocities might have elicited a different reSponse. Furthermore, at 12cm/sec the fish 24 significantly preferred a black overhead plate with 3 sides rather than a slanted black overhead plate with relative higher light inten- sity and lower current velocity beneath. When the current was increased to 28cm/sec, the difference in occupancy became nonsignificant. These findings are in agreement with the present study and indicate that at low water velocity, the photic stimulus and response govern overhead cover choice and that at high water velocity the rheostatic stimulus predominates. One might suspect that the velocity interaction with light stimuli may have confounded the results. However, since only 11% of the observations occurred within the light-velocity interaction range(150-199mm/sec), the results of this study represent a true response to light level beneath cover. Cover use, although random in the velocity range of 100-149 mm/sec, was greatest for the cover possessing the highest current velocity (Table 4). Thus, there appears to be a preferred range of sub-covert velocities somewhere between 125 and l75mm/sec. This study supports the findings of Baldes and Vincent (1969) who reported that wild brown trout of about 20cm preferred resting microhabitat water velocities within a range of 122 to 213mm/sec where no overhead cover was involved. The 2 trout in a section usually occupied opposite sides of the channel. When both fish were using cover, those covers were on Opposite sides of the channel 75% of the time, and only thrice in 224 observations were the 2 fish under the same cover. These observations support the findings of Bassett (1978), who demonstrated that 25 interactive behavior(social interaction) influenced cover choice by subordinate avoidance of dominant trout. Despite this avoidance re- action, if one considers the observations in which both trout occupied cover on the same side of the channel, they did so on the darker side of the channel 68% of the time. This preference for the darker side was significant(p$ 0.01). There was no significant deviation from this preference on cloudy days. Thus, the disadvantage of having 2 trout per section was that the variability and degree of social interaction with its associated influence on cover reaponse were unknown. To examine the possible influence of body size on reaponse to cover(light and velocity preference and cover-use), cover occupancy was analysed for 25-29cm trout and 30-39cm trout. Cover occupancy for 25-29cm fish accounted for 65% of the observations. There were no differences between the 2 size ranges, thus I conclude that body size had no influence on response to cover within the range of 25- 39cm. I also examined the possible influence of time of day on response to cover, light and velocity preference and cover-use. There were no differences between observation times, thus I concluded that time of day had no influence on response to cover between 0900-1800h EDT. Due to the constantly changing light under the covers, the problem of Operant reinforcement or conditioning for a particular site was not a factor determining response to cover. A Suggestion for Further Research There is a definite management need for cover-habitat quantification for stream-dwelling salmonids. This could be achieved 26 by the develOpment of a trout cover rating system based on light and velocity stimuli and their associated reSponse to cover. To develOpe such a rating system, further knowledge about the interaction of light and velocity stimuli as they relate to cover occupation, must be.acquired. One might, by playing light and velocity stimuli against one another, determine a cover index function. The application of this cover index could be used to predict trout pOpula- tions(biological variables), and thus to manage accordingly. LITERATURE CITED LITERATURE CITED Baldes, R.J. and R.E. Vincent 1969. Physical parameters of macro- habitats occupied by brown trout in an experimental flume. Trans. Am. Fish. Soc. 98:230-238. Bassett, C.E. 1978. Effect of cover and social structure on position choice by brown trout(Salmo trutta) in a stream. M.S. Thesis. Michigan State Univ., E. Lansing. 181 pp. Butler, R.L. and V.M. Hawthorne 1968. The reactions of dominant trout to changes in overhead artificial cover. Trans. Am. Fish. Soc. 97:37-41. Chapman, D.W. 1966. Food and Space as regulators of salmonid pOp- ulations in streams. Am. Naturalist 100:345-357. DeVore, P.W. 1975. Daytime behavioral responses of adult brown trout(Salmo trutta) in stream channels. 'M.S. Thesis. Michigan State Univ., E. Lansing. 38 pp. Gill, J.L. Design and analysis of experiments in the animal sciences. Volume 1. Iowa State Univ. Press, Ames, Iowa. (In press). Haines, T.A. and R.L. Butler 1969. Responses of yearling small- mouth bass(Micropterus dolomieui) to artificial shelter in a stream aquarium. J. Fish. Res. Bd. Can. 26(1):21-31. Hartman, G.F. 1963. Observations on behavior of juvenile brown trout in a stream aquarium during winter and spring. J. Fish. Res. Bd. Can. 20:769-787. Kwain, W. and H.Rw MacCrimmon 1967. The behavior and bottom color selection of the rainbow trout, Salmo ggirdneri, Richardson, exposed to different light intensities. Anim. Behav. 15: 75-78 0 . 1969. Further Observations on the response of rainbow trout, Salmo ggirdneri, to overhead light. J. Fish. Res. Bd. Can. 26(12):3233-3236. Lewis, S.L. 1969. Physical factors influencing fish pOpulations in pools of a trout stream. Trans. Am. Fish. Soc. 98:14-19. 27 28 Lindroth, A. 1955. Distribution, territorial behavior, and movements of sea trout fry in the river Indalsalven. Rept. Inst. Freshwater Res. Drottingholm, 36:104-119. MacCrimmon, H.R. and W. Kwain 1966. Use of overhead cover by rain- bow trout exposed to series of light intensities. J. Fish. Ritter, J.A. and H.R. MacCrimmon 1973. Effects of illumination on behavior of wild brown trout(Salmo trutta) and rainbow trout (Salmo ggirdneri) exposed to black and white backgrounds. J. Fish. Res. Bd. Can. 30:1875-1880. Stewart, P.A. 1970. Physical factors influencing trout density in a small stream. Ph.D. Dissertation. Colorado State Univ., Fort Collins. 88 pp. Wickham, M.G. 1967. Physical microhabitat of trout. M.S. Thesis. Colorado State Univ., Fort Collins. 42 pp. APPENDIX 29 Table A1. Daily maximum and minimum water temperatures for the East Branch of the Au Sable River at the DNR Grayling Field Office during the summer of 1977. Temperature C Temperature C Date High Low Date High Low 7/11 20.6 14.4 7/28 16.7 13.9 12 20.6 16.7 ' 29 17.2 15.0 13 21.2 16.1 30 19.4 15.0 14 19.4 14.4 31 20.0 15.6 15 22.2 17.2 8/ 1 19.4 15.0 16 21.7 16.7 2 17.2 15.0 17 23.3 18.3 3 16.7 12.2 18 20.6 18.3 4 18.3 14.4 19 23.3 16.7 5 18.9 15.6 20 24.4 20.0 6 19.4 16.1 21 22.8 19.4 7 17.8 16.1 22 21.1 15.6 8 18.9 15.0 23 20.6 16.1 9 16.7 13.3 24 18.3 15.6 10 17.8 14.4 25 18.9 15.0 11 16.1 15.0 26 18.3 12.8 12 17.8 11.7 27 17.8 12.2 13 16.1 13.9 14 16.7 12.8 "Iiiii‘iiiii‘iiiiiii