)V1ESI_} RETURNING MATERIALS: P1ace in book drop to LJBRARJES remove this checkout from 1--:y--L your record. FINES will be charged if book #15 returned after the date stamped below. Maw A39 THE CONSTRUCTION OF AN EXPERT SYSTEM TO MAKE MATERIALITY JUDGMENTS BY Paul John Steinbart A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Accounting 1984 '0 ‘1. \j ‘3 \J< /{9 ABSTRACT THE CONSTRUCTION OF AN EXPERT SYSTEM TO MAKE MATERIALITY JUDGMENTS BY Paul John Steinbart Previous research on materiality has focused on the judgments that are made when evaluating audit evidence. How- ever, auditors also make materiality judgments to help plan the nature, timing, and extent of the audit procedures that will be used to provide audit evidenceu These judgments have a direct effect on audit efficiency and effectiveness. This study examines how auditors make these preliminary judgments of materiality. Traditional techniques used to study auditor judgments (eqy, the lens model) only measure the relationship between decision inputs and outputs; they do not explain how those inputs are used to make the judgment. This study constructs an expert system, called AUDITPLANNER, which is capable of actually making materiality judgments. Analysis of AUDIT- PLANNERfis decision rules reveals how various input factors are used to make materiality judgments. Analysis of AUDITPLANNER/s decision rules indicates that the preliminary materiality judgment is influenced by (l) the characteristics of the company being audited, (2) the perceived needs of the users of the financial state- ments, and (3) the degree of risk associated with the audit and the auditor’s own attitudes toward such risk. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to take this opportunity to thank those who helped make this dissertation possible. First, I want to thank God for giving me the talents to pursue this task. My committee chairman, WillianIEL McCarthy, provided insightful criticisnl throughout tflma entire :research. process. My readers, Alvin A”.Arens and George Stockman, both deserve thanks for their helpful and timely advice. Thomas H. Carr provided helpful comments on a preliminary version of the work; Graham Gal joined in frequent and fruitful discussions about the methodology. Bob Byers agreed to permit the use of the computer facilities at Jackson Community College; this research would not have been possible without his assist- ance. The Deloiite, Haskins & Sells Foundation provided the financial support which enabled me to pursue this project. Finally, I want to thank my wife, Linda, and daughter, Stephanie, for their constant support, encourage-ment, and understanding. Their love was fundamental to the success of this undertaking. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 CHAPTER I. PREVIOUS RESEARCH ON MATERIALITY . . . . . . Types of Materiality Judgments . . . . . . Determinants of Materiality Judgments . . Judgments of Accounting Materiality . . Judgments of Auditing Materiality . . . Implications of Prior Research Findings . Types of Cases Used Previously . . . . . Role of Circumstantial Variables . . . . II. THE USE OF EXPERT SYSTEMS TO STUDY DECISION MAKING BEHAVIOR . . . . . . . . . . The Production System Architecture . . . . Production Memory . . . . . . . . . . . Inference Engine . . . . . . . . . . . . Forward-chaining . . . . . . . . . . . Backward-chaining . . . . . . . . . . Working Memories . . . . . . . . . . . . The Information Processing Paradigm . . . Effect of the Task Environment . . . . . Structural Characteristics of the Problem Solver . . . . . . . . . . . . . Heuristic search . . . . . . . . . . . Reasoning by manipulating symbols . . The Problem Solver’s Knowledge . . . . . The nature of expertise: theory . . . The nature of expertise: empirical evidence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Expert Systems as Psychological Theories . The Production System Architecture as a Theory of Cognitive Behavior . . . . . AUDITPLANNER as a Theory of Professional Judgment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii vi. vii. 20 20 21 22 22 23 25 26 28 30 31 33 35 36 38 40 41 41 45 CHAPTER III. RESEARCH METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 Research Tool: EMYCIN . . . . . . . . . . . 46 Reasons for Choosing EMYCIN . . . . . . . 48 Previous successful use of EMYCIN . . . 48 Similarity of the original task for which EMYCIN was designed and materiality judgments . . . . . . . . 49 Formal task analysis of judgments of auditing materialiy . . . . . . . . . . Sl EMYCIN’S satisfaction of the requirements of the formal task analysis . . . . . . 53 Features of EMYCIN . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 Search strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 Representation of knowledge . . . . . . 57 Reasoning under uncertainty . . . . . . 58 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 Construction of Prototype System . . . . . . 62 Choice of Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . 63 Lack of consensus across firms . . . . . 63 Lack of consensus within firms . . . . . 66 Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 Subject selection . . . . . . . . . . . 69 Source of Knowledge for Prototype . . . . 70 Refinement of the System . . . . . . . . . . 7l Explanation of Reasoning During a Consultation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 Explanation After a Consultation is Over . 73 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 Testing AUDITPLANNER . . . . . . . . . . . . 74 Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78 Test Cases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81 Competence of AUDITPLANNER . . . . . . . 82 Reasonableness of model . . . . . . . . 82 Usefulness of AUDITPLANNER . . . . . . . 85 Ease of use and enjoyment . . . . . . . 86 Adequacy of question-answering facilities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 CHAPTER IV. ANALYSIS OF THE SYSTEM . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 The Decision Setting . . . . . . . . . . . . 88 The Judgment Model . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91 Identification of the Materiality Base . . 99 Determination of the type of entity . . 100 Identification of users’ interests . . . 104 Choosing the materiality base . . . . . 106 iv CHAPTER IV. (continued) Choosing a Percentage Rate . . . . . . . Calculation of the Materiality Level . . Tentative level too small . . . . . . Tentative level too large . . . . . . Special Situations . . . . . . . . . . . Audits of nonprofit organizations . . Audits of financial institutions . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . The Nature of the Materiality Judgment Process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . V. CONCLUSION AND SUGGESTED EXTENSIONS . . . . Research Contribution . . . . . . . . Possible Future Extensions to the Work . . Further Development of AUDITPLANNER . . Broadening AUDITPLANNER’s domain of expertise . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Integrating with other audit judgments Comparative Studies . . . . . . . . . . Experimental Studies . . . . . . . . . . Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX I Example of AUDITPLANNER’S explanation capabilities during a consultation . . . APPENDIX II Example of AUDITPLANNER’S explanation capabilities after a consultation . . . APPENDIX III Sample session with AUDITPLANNER . . . APPENDIX IV Evaluation questionnaire . . . . . . . BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . General References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 112 113 115 116 116 117 118 119 126 126 130 130 130 131 131 131 132 133 135 137 139 141 149 TABLE 1. TABLE 2. TABLE 3. LIST OF TABLES Characteristics of test clients . . . . . . . 80 Responses to test questionnaire . . . . . . . 83 Effect of qualitative factors on judgments of auditing materiality . . . . . . . . . . . 120 vi. FIGURE FIGURE FIGURE FIGURE FIGURE FIGURE FIGURE 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. LIST OF FIGURES Theory of Problem Solving Behavior Role of EMYCIN in Construction of an EXPERT SYSTEM . . . . . . . Causal Model of Auditing Materiality . Overview of AUDITPLANNER’S Judgment Model Calculation of Overall Materiality Level Identification of Materiality Base . Calculation of Materiality . vii. 27 47 89 92 93 94 95 INTRODUCTION The concept of materiality is the cornerstone of auditing. The auditorks opinion that an enterprise’s finan- cial statements "present fairly" the results of operations implies that those statements are not materially misstated [AICPA, 1983, para. 3]. In planning the audit, a materiality judgment is made to help determine the extent of testing needed to provide sufficient evidence upon which to base an opinion. Another materiality judgment is made to evaluate the results of those tests. No wonder that Statement on Auditing Standards (SAS)IML 1 states that "the concept of materiality is inherent in the work of the independent auditor" [AICPA, 1979, para. 150.04]. The importance of the concept of materiality is also apparent from its inclusion on the Financial Accounting Standards Boardfs (EASE) original agenda. The FASB issued a Discussion Memorandum [FASB, 1975] on the topic in an effort to develop some guidelines for making materiality judgments. That effort ended, however, with the FASB’sidecision that such guidelines were not feasible. The Board’s present position is that no general standards of materiality could be formulated to take into account all the considerations that enter into an experienced human judgment [FASB, 1980, para. 131]. 2 SAS No. 47 describes the resulting state of affairs and summarizes the reason the reasoning behind the FASB’s decision: The auditorfis consideration of materiality is a matter of professional judgment and is influenced by his perception of the needs of a reasonable person who will rely on the financial statements. [Those needs] . . . are recognized in the discussion of materiality [by the FASB, cited above] . . . that discussion recognizes that materiality judgments are made in light of surrounding circumstances and necessarily involve both quantitative and qualita- tive considerations [AICPA, 1983, para. 6]. Accounting researchers have long been interested in how materiality judgments are made. Holstrum and Messier [1982] review previous empirical research on materiality and conclude that: Definitive comprehensive implications for audit practice or policy formulation are difficult to de- cipher from the materiality research to date [p659]. They point out that previous research has not examined the effect of the type of entity and its industry classification on materiality judgments, nor has there been an attempt to measure the sensitivity of those judgments to the nature of the event or item for which the judgment is being made. Moreover, previous research has concentrated on the materiality judgments that are made when evaluating audit evidence. However, auditors also make materiality judgments in the planning stage of the audit to help determine the extent of audit tests and procedures. The lack of attention paid to this type of materiality judgment reflects the general paucity'of.researchcnuthe auditorfisinitial plan- ning process. Felix and Kinney [1982] state the need for such research: while the amount of research on the auditor’s deci- sion processes is growing, the planning decision processes are not being included in this growth. While the complexities of the setting present con- siderable difficulties, effective planning of the audit is critical to efficient auditing, and research on the planning processes should be encouraged [p.255]. This dissertation seeks to model how materiality judg- ments are made in the planning stage of the audit. The goal is to explain how circumstantial factors, such as the nature of the company being audited, influence those judgments. Techniques from.tflu2 field of artificial intelligence will be used to express the model in the form of a computer program known.as a rule-based expert systenu Such systems are particularly well-suited to modeling tasks with charac- teristics such as those found in judgments of materiality. When intelligent behavior consists (n3 numerous specialized responses to widely varying and largely unpredictable situations, the antecdent - consequent structure of [rule-based expert systems] isolates and represents the appropriate logic of such data- directed behavior in a natural way [Waterman and Hayes-Roth, 1978, p.22]. The expert systems nmmhodology has been successfully applied to the study of a wide range of tasks in accounting and auditing. Such systems have been built to plan for indi- vidual estate taxes [Michaelsen, 1982], to determine the collectibility of delinquent trade accounts receivables [Dungan, 1983; Dungan and Chandler, 1983], to evaluate the causes of fluctuations identified during the process of analytical review [Braun and Chandler, 1982], to analyze EDP controls in an advanced computer system [Hansen and Messier, 4 1983], to evaluate the quality of internal controls [Ga1, 1984], and to decide on the appropriateness of issuing a "subject to" audit opinion [Dillard, Mutchler, and Rama- krishna, 1983]. The construction of an expert system involves the detailed study of the decision making behavior of an expert in order to identify the basis for that behavior. It is, therefore, inherently descriptive research. In fact, one of the principal motivations for using this methodology is to obtaixla better unnderstanding of current decision making practices. The aim here [in building an expert system] is thus not simply to build a program that exhibits a cer- tain specified behavior, but £9 use the program con- struction process itself as a w_y of explicating knowledge in the field, and_ to use the program text as a medium —of expression of many forms of knowledge about the task and its solution [Davis and Lenat, 1980, p.471]. Descriptive research on current methods of decision making is important. The ultimate goalcflfhuman information processing research in accounting is to improve decision mak- ing. Before decision making can be improved, how- ever, it is useful to evaluate the current quality of decision making, and before decision quality can be evaluated, decision making must be understood [Ashton, 1982, p.vii]. The remainder of this dissertation is organized as follows. Chapter I reviews previous research on materiality. Chapter II discusses the expert system methodology for studying decision making behavior. Chapter III describes the procedure to be used in building the expert system. Chapter IV analyzes the resulting system, examining the rules 5 contained therein in order to demonstrate how various circumstantial factors affect materiality judgments. Chapter V summarizes the findings and explores possible directions for future research. CHAPTER I PREVIOUS RESEARCH ON MATERIALITY This chapter reviews the results of previous empirical research on materiality. Most of that research has examined materiality in relation to the evaluation of audit evidence. However, materiality considerations also affect the planning of the audit. Consequently, the first part of this chapter discusses the relationship between these two judgments. The second part then examines the research findings on each type of judgment. The final part of the chapter discusses the implications of those results for the current research. Types pf Materiality Judgments Statement on Auditing Standards (SAS) No. 47 describes two different times during the course of the audit when the auditor needs to consider materiality. The auditor should consider . . . materiality both in (a) planning the audit and designing audit- ing procedures and (b) evaluating whether the finan- cial statements taken as a whole are presented fairly in conformity with generally accepted accounting principles [AICPA, 1983, para. 8]. The purpose of the initial consideration is to ensure that the auditor plans the extent of audit tests so as to provide adequate empirical evidence upon which to base an opinion on the fairness of presentation of the financial statements. In 6 7 the second instance, the auditor considers materiality in order to determine the effect on that fairness of presenta- tion of any errors that may have been uncovered by those audit tests. Theoretical discussions of materiality distinguish between these two judgments, referring to the former as involving "auditing materiality" and the latter as involving "accounting materiality" [CICA, 1965; Leslie, 1977; Thomas and Krogstad, 1978]. These terms will be adopted here. How- ever, it is important to recognize that they do not refer to two different concepts, but only to two different instances of that concept. In fact, judgments of auditing materiality may be thought of as pro-forma judgments of accounting materiality. Both are concerned with the point at which the amount of errors destroys fairness of presentation: the former makes this determination prior to performing any audit tests, the latter does so after testing is completed. This common con- cern makes it likely that there are many similarities between the two judgments, so that research on one may pro- vide useful insights into the nature of the other. However, because the two judgments are made atand Siebel [1974], and Pattillo [1975, 1976] used questionnaire cases in which both the characteristics of the firm and the nature of the item upon which the materiality judgment was to be based were varied. The series of studies by Pattillo involved over 700 subjects, and included auditors, bankers, financial analysts, and fincancial execu- tives. Participants were asked to determine the materiality of an error in terms of its effect on the fairness of presentation of the financial statenmnnxh Pattillo [1976] summarized the findings of those studies: The ’rule of thundf of 5% to 10% of net income is presently widely used in practice as an overall materiality criterflmm The participants demonstrated this criterion to be the primary basis for their initial determination of an item.s materiality. How- ever, the criterion was freguently supplemented by other guantitative criteria and was modified when called for by the circumstances existing in the judgment situations [Pattillo, 1976, gull, emphasis added]. The absolute size of the item and its affect on the earnings trend were two of the other quantitative factors that were consistently ranked high in importance by all participants. However, the nature of the itenl(eng., whether it was a contingency, an extraordinary item, etc.) was consistently ranked by all participants across all cases as being the single most important factor influencing their judgments of materiality. Boatsman and Robertson [1974], Firth [1979], Hofstedt 10 and Hughes [1977], Messier [1979], and Moriarity and Barron [1976] conducted experimental studies in which several fin- ancial characteristics of the firm were systematically varied in order to determine the effects of those character- istics on materiality judgments. These studies all found that the single most important determinant of materiality was the size of the item in relation to net income. The size of the firm [Firth, 1979; Moriarity and Barron, 1976] and the effect of the item on the trend in earnings [Messier, 1982] were quantitative factors of secondary importance. Most of these experimental studies, however, did not vary the nature of the judgment item. The one exception was the study by Boatsman and Robertson. That study used three types of items: (1) a gain or loss on the sale of noncurrent assets, (2) a change in accounting principle, and (3) a future uncertainty. The nature of the item was found to be significantly related to judgments of its materiality. In fact, inclusion of this variable in the judgment model markedly improved the model’s predictive accuracy. Boatsman.andeobertson reported that a simple model based solely on the size of the item expressed as a percentage of income could only accurately predict 65 percent of the subjects’ judgments and erred on the side of underdisclo- sure. However, when two additional variables were added to the model, one to represent the nature of the item, and the other to represent the degree of risk in the audit, the resulting model had a predictive accuracy of 84 percent. 11 Statistical analysis indicated that most of the improvement was due to the variable representing the nature of the item. An archival study by Frishkoff [1970] reported similar results about the effect of the nature of the item on judg- ments of materiality. Frishkoff examined annual reports in an effort to find which variables were useful predictors of a qualified audit opinion. He reported that a discriminant function based solely on the item’s effect on net income only classified nine percent of the casescnia.better than chance basis. The inclusion of two additional variables, one to represent the size of the client and the other to repre- sent the nature of the item, resulted in a classification accuracy of 91 percent. Besides the nature of the item, Boatsman and Robertson also found that the level of risk perceived in the audit was significantly related to materiality judgments. Ward [1976] examined auditors’ perceptions of the consequences of fail- ing to find an error that affected the amount of net income. There was a large degree of diversity across auditors, with considerable disagreement for those cases in which the effect of the error was to reduce net income. Newton [1977] examined auditors’ attitudes towards risk and how the degree of uncertainty about an item’s resolution affected judgments of materiality. 55 percent of the auditors were risk-averse, while 34 percent were classified as being risk-seeking. In addition, the degree of uncertainty associated with the item appeared to influence judgments of its materiality. 12 In summary, previous research indicates that although the size of an item in relation to net income is probably the most important quantitative determinant of accounting materiality, other qualitative factors representing the circumstances in which the judgment is made are also impor- tant. In particular, both the nature of the item and the auditor’s perception of the personal risk associated with the audit (should material errors go undetected) appear to influence judgments of accounting materiality. Judgments pf Auditing Materiality Only two studies examined the materiality judgments that are made when planning the extent of audit tests and procedures. Cushing, Searfoss, and Randall [1979] asked auditors to estimate the overall level of materiality for an audit and then tested a model designed to allocate that estimate among various financial statement accounts. The study did not specifically address the issue of how auditors make such estimates, but it did find that auditors expressed a high degree of confidence in the accuracw'of those judg- ments. Moriarity and Barron [1979] conducted an experiment which did attempt to determine how judgments of auditing materiality are made. Partners of a major public accounting firm were given summarized financial statements for thirty hypothetical companies and asked to establish the overall materiality level that should be used to plan audit tests and procedures. Five financial variables were manipulated: 13 net income, total assets, the debt-to-equity ratio, the number of shares outstanding, and the trend in earnings. (Total assets was varied by using financial statements that were multiples of one another). The results indicated that net income was the most important factor affecting the judgments of four of the five partners, with total assets being most important for the fifth. Total assets was second in importance for two part- ners, while the earnings trend was second in importance for the other three. In addition, there was evidence of a break- even effect: the importance of net income declined as it approached the breakeven level. Moriarity and Barron did not provide the participants with any background qualitative information about the firms for which the materiality judgments were to be made. In post-experimental debriefings, the participants complained about the lack of such information. Most participants indicated that they would be more familiar with the operations of their clients, the type of management, and management objectives“ Thus, some of the participants said they would like to have known what industry we were dealing with, to whom the audit report would be distributed, and what kinds of audit problems had been experienced in the past [Moriarity and Barron, 1979, pp.129-130]. Discussions in iflua authoritative literature indicate that such information is important. an amount that is material to the financial state- ments of one entity may not be material to the fin- ancial statements of another entity of a different size or nature. Also, what is material to the finan- cial statements of a particular entity might change from one period to another [AICPA, 1983, para. 5]. 14 For example, in an enterprise with few, but large, accounts receivable, the accounts individually are more important and the possibility of material error is greater than in another enterprise that has a great number of small accounts aggregating the same total. In industrial and merchandinsing enterprises, inventories are usually of great importance to both financial position and results of Operations and accordingly may require relatively more attention by the auditor than would inventories of a public util- ity company [AICPA, 1979, para. 150.04]. A study by Gibbins and Wolf [1982] provides some empi- rical support for the importance of such factors in making judgments of auditing materiality. Auditors from six public accounting firms were askedtxarank:various environmental factors in terms of their importance in affecting the con- duct of an audit at various stages in the audit process. Although the study addressed the conduct of the audit as a whole, and not materiality, the relationship between plan- ning and materiality noted in SAS No. 47 suggests that the findings for the planning stage of the audit may apply to auditing materiality as well. the nature, timing, and extent of planning -- and thus of the considerations of audit risk and materiality -- vary with the size and complexity of the entity, the auditor’s experience with the entity, and his knowledge of the entity’s business [AICPA, 1983, para. 11]. Gibbins and Wolf found that the following qualitative factors were important in the planning stage of the audit: (1) the service needs of the client, (2) information from prior years/(audit programs and file notes, (3) plans for the sale or major financing of the client, and (4) the nature of the client’s business. In summary, the results of the three studies reviewed 15 in this section tend to indicate that there are similarities between the two types of materiality judgments. The primary financial variable used in both judgments appears to be the amount of net income. That factor is supplemented, however, by other qualitative factors. In the case of accounting materiality, the most important qualitative factor is infor- mation about the nature of the judgment item. In the case of auditing materiality, because those judgments are estimates of what size errors would be material it is likely that information about the nature of the company should be of primary importance. Implications pf Prior Research Findings Two aspects of previous research have important impli- cations for this dissertation:(l) the types of cases used to study materiality judgments, and (2) the role of circum- stantial variables in those judgments. Types pf Cases Used Previously A major problem with previous experimental research on materiality judgments is the abstract nature of the cases that were used. Many of the qualitative factors mentioned in both the results of questionnaire studies and in discussions in the authoritative literature as being important determin- ants of materiality flag. the nature of the item, the nature of the companyis business) were either omitted entirely or were presented in a summary manner. The quantitative factors used in the cases were also typically highly summarized. The 16 following example, taken from the Hofstedt and Hughes study is typical. The case deals with a loss from the writedown of a subsidiary, and participants were asked to state the prob- ability that they would disclose it as an extraordinary item because it was material. Case A: The loss is 5% of operating income, 5% of parent investments, and 10% of subsidiary book value [Hofstedt and Hughes, 1977, p.388, format modified]. The use of such predigested and highly aggregated cues may produce behavior different from what would be observed outside the laboratory [Ebbesen and Konecni, 1980]. Indeed, in gmmtrexperimental debriefings, participants in many of the studies complained about the abstract nature of the cases and indicated that they would have liked to have had additional information to use in making their judgments. Newton [1977] described the nature of this information: All participants claimed that they needed more information because of the many factors which merit consideration in materiality decisions. Typically, questions were asked concerning the firm’s balance sheet, environment (industry and economic condi- tions), history, management, accounting policies, previous materiality decisions, etc. [p.106]. Clearly, future research needs to find ways to use more realistic cases. One advantage of the use of expert systems is that their refinement entails using them to solve real examples of the problem being studied. Thus, all the infor- mation normally available is included in the research study. 17 Role pf Circumstantial Variables The results of previous research indicate that two circumstantial factors, the nature of the item and the level of perceived risk, both influence judgments of accounting materiality. The results of the Gibbins and Wolf study and the discussions in the authoritative literature lead one to suspect that qualitative information about the company being audited plays a similarly important role in judgments of auditing materialityu.After all, if the nature of a known error affects judgments about the materiality of that error, then information about the nature of the company and the users of its financial statements should affect judgments about the level of errors that, if found, would be material. However, although there is some evidence concerning which factors influence materiality judgments, little is known about ghy those factors are importantq and hgg they enter the judgment process. As Carroll [1980] explains, to acquire such knowledge requires more than merely relating decision variables to the final decisions; it requires exam- ining the processes leading up to the decision: The decision analyst is misdirected by the impor- tance of the moment when the decision maker identi- fies a selection. We are seduced by language and common sense into believing that the choice ig the decision. Yet . . . the choice is the end product of the decision, the moment when we see the pigeon in the magician’s hand. The decision is the process of arriving at a choice, the process by which the pigeon got into the magician’s hand [p.69] Researchers in the fields of both artificial intelli- gence and cognitive psychology argue that traditional l8 statistical modeling methods are inappropriate for building models which really explain how judgments are made. The problem, of course, is that statistical methods are not good models of the actual reasoning process, nor were they designed to be. . . . [they are] for the most part, ’shallow’, one-step techniques which capture little of the ongoing process actually used by expert problem solvers in the domain [Davis, Buchanan, and Shortliffe, 1977, p.39]. the output of a quantitative mechanism, be it numer- ical, statistical, analog, or physical (nonsym- bolic), is too structureless and uninformative to permit further analysis. Number-like magnitudes can form the basis of decisions for immediate action, . . . but each is a ’dead end’ so far as further un- derstanding and planning is concerned, for each is an evaluation and not a summary. A number cannot reflect the considerations that formed Lt. This does not mean that people do not, or even that they should not, use such methods. But because of the block they present to further contemplation, we can predict that they will tend to be focused in what we might call terminal activities. In large measure, these activities may be just the activities most easily seen behavioristically and this might account in part for the traditional attraction of such models to workers in the behavioristic tradi- tion. The danger is that theories based upon them -- response probabilities, subjective probabilities, reinforcement schedule parameters -- are not likely to be able to account for sophisticated cognitive activities. As psychological theories they are very likely to be wrong [Minsky, 1975, p.275]. In place of traditional statistical models, the use of models based on the production system architecture is urged in those situations where the goal of the research is to understand hp! the decision was made. A final advantage of [production systems] is their ability to represent the role of the environment in governing the [subject/s] behavior in a way that a more conventional process model cannot. For a [pro- duction system] presents the set of possible actions that the subject can take together with the basis on which he decides between them, whereas a flow- chart or algorithm states only the outcome of that decision [Young, 1978, p. 397, emphasis added]. 19 The expert system to be built in this dissertation is based on the production system architecture. The next chap- ter describes the nature of that architecture and discusses its use as a means of studying decision making behavior. CHAPTER II THE USE OF EXPERT SYSTEMS TO STUDY DECISION MAKING BEHAVIOR The production system architecture has been used to build rule-based expert systems which perform a wide variety of tasks. Examples inlcude systems which diagnose infectious diseases [Shortliffe, 1976], analyze the molecular structure of chemical compounds [Buchanan, Sutherland, and Feigenbaum, 1969, 1970], prospect for mineral ores [Gaschnig, 1982], and diagnose faults in computer systems [Hartley, 1979]. This chapter begins by describing the essential fea- tures of the production system architecture underlying such systems. Then the theoretical paradigm underlying the use of that architecture to study decision making behavior is pre- sented. The final section of the chapter discusses the status of expert systems as psychological theories of exper- tise in the task domain being modeled. The Production System Architecture A production system consists of three components: (1) a production memory or knowledge base, (2) an executive or inference engine, and (3) one or more working memories [Feigenbaum, 1979; Hayes-Roth, Waterman, and Lenat, 1978; Newell, 1973, 19803]. The following sections discuss these 20 21 three components in more detail. Production Memory Production systems get their name because they repre- sent knowledge about relationships among domain variables in th form of conditional rules known as productions. Each rule is of the form: situation => action.iflmaleft-hand side of the rule consists of a set of clauses which describe the situation in which that rule applies; the right-hand side describes the actions or inferences that occur when the rule is executed or "fired".'Three of the rules found in AUDIT- PLANNER (the name given.to the expert system built in this dissertation) are presented below: Rule 1 IF: 1) the client is a public entity, and 2) there is no significant concern about the liquidity or solvency of the client THEN: it is assumed that the principal external users of the financial state- ments are primarily interested in information about the results of current operations. Rule 2 IF: 1) the principal external users of the financial statements are primarily interested in the results of current operations, and 2) net income is above the break-even point THEN: the materiality judgment should be 22 based on net income. Rule 3 IF: 1) the basis for making the materiality judgment is known, and 2) the percentage rate for making the materiality judgment is known THEN: the materiality level equals the product of the percentage rate times the base used to make the materiality judgment. In the next section, these rules will be used to illustrate the various control strategies for production systems. Inference Engine The inference engine is the control strategy for guid- ing the selection and execution of particular rules.‘This control strategy can be described as a recognize-act cycle. This strategy can be implemented as either a forward- or a backward-chaining reasoning process. Forward-chaining. In a forward-chaining or data- directed strategy each recognize-act cycle begins by compar- ing the facts that.describe the current state of the world to the situation part of each production rule. Each rule which has its conditions for firing satisfied is placed into a conflict resolution set. After the entire set of pro- duction rules has been so examined, one of those in that set is selected and fired. The method of selection is called the conflict resolution rule. The firing of the rule constitutes 23 the action part of the cycle. An inference is made, and the statezof the world is changed. If that change has resulted in the goal being attained, the system stops and reports success. If not, the entire cycle begins again with every rule being compared to the new set of facts describing the world. Lfru>rules have their conditions for firing satis- fied on a particular cycle, the system stops and reports failure. The following example, using the three rules presented earlier, illustrates how this forward-chaining process leads to a particular decision. When information is obtained that the client is a public entity and that there is no concern about liquidity or solvency,1nflral would fire.Its firing would add to working memory the assertion that the users of the financial statements are primarily interested in the results of current operations. This would satisfy the first premise clause of rule 2. Upon receipt of information that net income is above the break-even point, rule 2 would fire. Its firing would result in the assertion that the materi- ality judgment should be based on net income. At this point, clause 1 of rule 3 would be satisfied; as soon clause 2 is satisfied, rule 3 would fire and the materiality judgment would be made. Backward-chaining. Sula backward-chainingcn:goal- directed reasoning strategy each recognize-act cycle begins by examining the action part of the production rules in the knowledge base.(kflqrthose rules whose firing will attain 24 the current goal are checked to see if their situation part is satisfied. If any such rules are found, they are fired and the system stops and reports success. If the rules that will attain the current goal do not have their situation part satisfied, then a new set of sub-goals for satisfying those conditions is established. Each rule in the production memory is then checked to see if its firing would alter the current state of the world and attain the current sub-goals. Those rules are then checked again to see if they can fire. This backward-chaining continues until either some rule is finally found which can fire or no such rule is found. In the former case, firing that rule leads to a chain of firings that eventually attains the goal; in the latter case, the system stops and reports failure. The set of three rules presented earlier can be used to illustrate this backward-chaining reasoning process. The system begins with the goal of making a judgment of materi- ality. Scanning the action parts of the three rules reveals that firing rule 3 would satisfy this goal. Consequently, the premises of rule 3 would be matched against the contents of working memory to ascertain whether they are satisfied by the facts known about the current situation. Neither premise is likely to be satisfied at the start of a consultation, so the verification of each of rule 3’s premise clauses would become the new subgoals. The system would then scan the action parts of the remaining rules to see if the firing of any of them would satisfy the current subgoals. This would 25 reveal that firing rule 2 would establish the appropriate base to be used for making the materiality judgment. Both of rule 2’s premises would then be checked to see if they were satisfied by the facts and assertions stored in working memory. The process would eventually lead the system to try to establish the premises for rule 1 so that the first premise of rule 2 could be satisfied. It should be clear that with either type of control strategy the production system architecture exhibits behav- ior that is responsive to the facts of the case at hand. Although the set of production rules is fixed, the particu- lar set of rules that fires on any session, and the order in which they fire, depends upon the facts that describe the current consultation. Working Memories The working memories function as a scratchpad to keep track of the goals being sought and the current state of progress toward their attainment. The set of facts that describes the world and which is compared to the situation part of each rule is contained in these working memories, as are any sub-goals that have been created. Now that the basic features of the production system architecture have been described, it is time to discuss the theoretical paradigm underlying the use of that architecture as a means of studying decision making behavior. 26 The Information Processing Paradigm The information processing paradigm focuses attention on the internal cognitive processes that underly observable behavior. Young [1978] contrasts this approach to the func- tional or behaviorist paradigm: Unlike a functional approach which tends to ask questions about the effect of various controllable factors on certain gross measures of the subject’s performance. .. the information processing approach prefers to ask questions directly about p93 the [subject] is carrying out the task, seeking an answer in terms of the psychological processes and representations that underlie his behavior [pu360]. Newell and Sinmnfs [1972] study of one basic cognitive activity -- problem solving -- led to the development of many of the essential ideas in the information processing paradigm. Their theory of human problem solving behavior is illustrated in Figure l. The figure shows that the problem solver forms an internal representation of the task that reflects how he or she perceives the task. It is this subjective interpretation of the task, rather than the objective statement of the problem, that governs all subsequent cognitive activity. The problem solver possesses a store of methods for solving var- ious tasks and also a store of factual knowledge. The basic cognitive activity consists of a search through these stores for knowledge that can be applied to solve the problem. Thus, problem solving behavior is basically a function of three variables: (1) the task environment, (2) some basic structural characteristics of the problem solver, and (3) the specific knowledge possessed by the problem solver. The 27 TASK ENVIRONMENT Problem SIJlg‘IllCnK Affect cmimnmcm P11081131 SOLVER Internal Representation r—+—% Change rcprcsc HI :I l i( m Sch't‘l mvllmd [mt-mu! gencldl [;!1«_)\A'|1'(]g1‘ Mrllmd store Nulc: lhc cyc 4 indium that input rcpxcwnlatnui |\ not umlcx umlml Hf inputting pmu‘ss Figure 14 Theory of Problem Solving Behavior Sourcez' Newell and Simon (1972. p.89) 28 role of each of these elements is discussed in more detail below. Effect pg the Task Environment The effect of the task environment on decision making behavior has long been recognized [Brunswik, 1955a,l955b]. The task environment includes both the structural properties of the task and the specific content of the task [Einhorn and Hogarth, 1981; Payne, 1982]. Normative theories get their generality by focusing on the former and ignoring the latter. But Newell and Simon’s theory of problem solving indicates that it is the problem solver’s subjective percep- tion of the task that governs behavior. A great deal of evi- dence suggests that the content of the task plays an impor- tant role in shaping this subjective representation. Thus, task content may have an even greater effect on behavior than do the formal properties of the task. For example, Einhorn and Hogarth [1982] found that changes in the composition of the set of alternative hypo- theses that did not alter the objective probability of that set did produce a change in its subjective probability. In one experiment, they asked subjects to decide what foreign language was spoken by a group of robbers. Subjects in one condition were told that four eyewitnesses said that they had heard the thieves speaking in German and four others had said that it was Italianu Subjects in the other condition were told that four eyewitnesses said it was German, two said it was French, one said Spanish, and one Italian. Note 29 that in both conditions one-half of the eyewitnesses said that they had heard the robbers speaking in German. Yet, the subjects in the second condition were were much more certain than were the subjects in the first condition that the lan- guage was German. Changes in task content can directly affect the level of performance. Adelman [1981] found that realistic task content facilitated subjects’ ability to learn from outcome feedback. He had subjects predict grade point averages. When given only abstract cues (e.g., Cue 1, Cue 2) outcome feed- back was not very helpful. But when the cues were correctly labeled (e.g., expectations for academic achievement) sub- jects were able to use outcome feedback to markedly improve their performance. Similarly, subjects"performance on a task involving syllogistic reasoning markedly improved when the syllogisms included realistic causal relationships with which the subjects were familiar [Cox and Griggs, 1982; Hoch and Tschirgi, 1983]. Task content can also hide formal task properties, so that subjects cannot apply previously learned strategies. The ease with which problems that have identical formal properties can be solved varies with the particular content of the task [Newell and Simon, 1972; Hayes and Simon, 1977]. Changes in task content induced by altering the way in which the problem is worded can.even leadiuba.reversal in preferences [Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tversky and Kahneman, 1981]. Apparently, small changes in wording 30 dramatically alter the meaning of the choice. Tversky and Kahneman [1981] provide a graphic illustration. They asked subjects to choose one of two strategies for dealing with a flu epidemic that was expected to kill 600 people if nothing was done. One wording of the choice was as follows: Option one will save 200 peOple, while option two has a one-third chance of saving all 600 but a two-thirds chance of saving no one.‘The other wording was: Option one will result in the deaths of 400 people, while option two has a one-third chance of no deaths but a two-thirds chance of 600 deaths. Many subjects reversed their choices when.the‘wording was changed. In summary, the content of the task appears to signif- icantly affect the way in which the task is perceived, and thus the ultimate behavior of decision makers. Since subtle changes in the way in which a task is described can produce major changes in behavior, it is important that the setting of a task be as realistic as possible. A major advantage of using expert systems to study decision making behavior is that subjects are permitted to act in naturally occuring decision settings. The means by which this is achieved is explained 1J1 Chapter III’s discussion of tflua research methodology. Structural Characteristics 9; the Problem Solver The structural characteristics of the problem solver include: the size and nature of memory, the speed with which memory can be accessed to either retrieve old knowledge or 31 to store new knowledge, and the nature of the basic cogni- tive processes underlying behavior. Two facets of the latter are of particular importance:(l) heuristic search as the fundamental cognitive activity in problem solving, and (2) the use of symbols to represent knowledge and the manipula- tion of those symbols as the basic means of reasoning. Heuristic search. Simon [1980] stated that heuris- tic search is the principal mechanism underlying intelligent problem solving behavior in both humans and computers“ To say that search is heuristic means that it is guided by knowledge about the particular task under consideration and uses that knowledge to quickly focus in on the heart of the problem. This is in contrast to a "blind", exhaustive search wherein each possible action that can be taken is tried until either one works or none do. One reason for heuristic search is that for many problems there does not exist any formal algorithm that is guaranteed to produce a solution. Often there may not even be a clearly defined criterion. Such problems are called ill-structured or ill-defined problems. Judgments of materi- ality are an example of an ill-structured problem. In con- trast, inventory control is a well-structured problem with a known algorithm (the EOQ formula) for its solution. The problem solver is not left totally in the dark when attempting to solve an ill-structured problenn Often there is a good idea of what the important subgoals are, but no fixed method for achieving them. For example, an auditor 32 may know that the determination of materiality involves applying some percentage to a base. There just does not exist any general formula that can be applied to all clients. Instead, the auditor relies upon experience gained from prior audits to determine what the appropriate base and percentage is for this particular client. The production system architecture underlying expert systems is well-suited to capturing this type of conditional, pattern-directed behavior. Even when some general algorithm does exist, time con- straints may preclude its use and make an heuristic search necessaryujRaphael [1976] gives a good example of this in the context of solving cryptarithmetic problems such as the following: BEST : ELDE MASER The goal is to assign a unique digit to each letter so that the equation is true. A blind, exhaustive search of all possible combinations is certain to produce the answer, but such a search would require considering 1,814,400 possible assignments of digits to letters. This approach is not only inefficient, it is clearly not very intelligent either. Basic knowledge of arithmetic can be used to dramatically reduce the size of the search space. For example, it is obvious that the letter M must be assigned the digit one becauseethat.is the largest carry that can be generated by adding two single digits. That fact alone reduces the search 33 space by a factor of ten. (The answer to the problem is: M=1, A=0, B=9, D=8, S=6, T=7, R=2, and E=5). In summary, problem solving involves searching for a way to solve the problem. To be effective, the search must utilize knowledge about the domain acquired previously. The use of that knowledge makes the search heuristic. The production system architecture is well-suited for exhibiting this type of behavior. Reasoning by manipulating symbols. A basic tenet of the information processing paradigm is that concepts are represented by symbols and that reasoning involves the mani- pulation of those symbols [Lachman, Lachman, and Butter- field, 1979]. Symbols are not merely tokens that label some concept; rather, they contain the essence of the concept and permit that essential meaning to be.accessed whenever the symbol is processed [Newell, 1980b]. For example, "Cash" is a symbol, and whenever the problem solver processes that symbol its meaning (e4L, that it is a monetary current asset, is easily misappropriated, eth is available and guides the processing. Task context influences behavior by indicating which of these meanings is most relevant for the given situation. Thus, when doing foreign currency transla- tion the fact that Cash is a monetary current asset is most salient and is retrieved first from memory. On the other hand, when evaluating the quality of internal controls, the susceptibility of cash to theft becomes more relevant. Symbols can be grouped into symbol structures in order 34 to represent more complex ideas. For example, the following set of symbols represents the current ratio: (Quotient Current-Assets Current-Liabilities). New symbols can be created, in turn, to represent these structures (e.g., Current-Ratio). In this way, the problem solver can react to new situations and "learn". Human behavior is characterized by its interruptibil- ity and temporal correlations. That is, work on a task can be temporarily suspended and resumed later without having to start over from the beginning (within limits, of course). Most behavior is also purposive; actions are taken in order to accomplish something definite. Such behavior is said to be goal-directed.flfimzexistence of goals accounts for the temporal correlations of behavior: a sequence of actions has a.establish default values of certain parameters, (3) to control the ordering of subtasks, and (4) to describe the contextual setting in which some goal is appropriate. EMYCIN uses its backward-chaining search strategy to implement functions 1, 3, and 4 described above. The rules involved are called consequent rules, because their premises are checked only if the actions accompanying their being fired help to satisfy the current goal being pursued. The second function, establishing default values, is implemented by the use of antecedent rules. These rules fire in a forward-chaining manner whenever data is obtained which satisfies their premises. They are used to make definitional conclusions and to avoid asking redundant questions. For example, AUDITPLANNER contains an antecedent rule which states that if the client’s main line of business falls into 58 one of several categories hag., manufacturing or retail sales), then the client is definitely not a nonprofit organ- ization.‘This rule prevents AUDITPLANNER from asking whether General Motors is a nonprofit entity. EMYCIN-based systems store factual knowledge about the subject of a consultation in the form of fact triples such as the following: (CLIENT TYPE-OF-ENTITY PUBLIC). The first element is termed the context; it serves to associate facts about an object in the domain. In this case, it states that the fact represented here pertains to the client being audited.(If the client is a nonprofit organization, some attributes may only apply to certain funds or fund types; in that case, the context variable would be the name of the relevant fund). The second element of the triple is the name of the attribute, in the example, the type of entity that the client is. The third element represents the value of that attribute; in this case, the client is a public entity. These fact triples are formed in response to user answers to questions and to the firing of production rules. Reasoning under uncertainty. The task analysis indi- cated that some of the information upon which judgments of auditing materiality are based is inherently uncertain. For example, the amounts of the items in interim financial statements are used to estimate what the final, annual amounts of those items will be. EMYCIN-based expert systems deal with this type of reasoning by assigning certainty factors.(CFs) to the facts about the consultation and also 59 to the rules used to manipulate those facts. Shortliffe and Buchanan [1975] explain the mathematics underlying the manipulation of CFs. CFs represent subjective degrees of belief and are defined to be the difference between the degree of belief in some assertion, given the available evidence, and the degree of disbelief in that assertion given that same evidence.‘This definition is simi- lar to Einhorn and Hogarth%;[l982, 1984] concept of the net strength of a hypothesis being the difference between the weight of the evidence in favor of and against that hypo- thesis. CFs range in value from -1.0, representing absolute certainty that the assertion is not true, to +1.0, which represents absolute certainty that it is true. As mentioned above, CFs are associated with both fact triples and with the actions of rules. Thus, a CF of .9 attached to the fact triple (CLIENT TYPE-OF-ENTITY PUBLIC) means that there is a high degree of belief (but not absolute certainty) that the client is a public entity. A CF of 1.0 attached to the action of a rule means that whenever that rule’s premises are satisfied the specified inference can be made with absolute certainty. A rule is fired in EMYCIN-based systems only if the absolute value of the CF associated with its situation part is at least equal tc>.2. The calculation of the CF of the situation part of a rule depends upon the relationship among the clauses that make up that situation. For example, 60 AUDITPLANNER contains the following rules: Rule 40 IF: 1) the client has publicly-traded debt or equity securities, or 2) the client has restrictive debt covenants that are measured in terms of results of current operations THEN: it is definite that the client is a public entity. (1.0) Rule 11 IF: 1) the client is a public entity, and 2) there is no concern about liquidity or solvency THEN: it can be assumed that the users of the financial statements are primarily interested in the results of current operations. (1.0) The first rule is a disjunctive rule: that is, it can fire if either of its premises are satisfied. Therefore, the CF attached to the assertion that is made whenlthe rule is fired equals the product of the maximum CF of the premises and the CF of the action. In this case, if the CF of premise l was .7 and the CF of premise 2 was .9, the CF of the resulting assertion would be:.9 (the maximum of the two premises) times 1.0 (the CF of the rule/s action). The second rule, on the other hand, is a conjunctive rule: that is, both of its premises must be satisfied in order to fire the rule. In this case, the value of the resulting assertion equals the value of the minimum CF attached to either premise times the CF of the rule. Thus, if the CF of premise l was .7 and that of premise 2 was .9, then the CF of the assertion would be .7 times lJL This 61 ensures that the degree of belief attached to an assertion cannot exceed the degree of belief in the weakest link supporting that assertion. To ensure that the CF of an assertion never exceeds 1.0 in absolute value, EMYCIN uses the following method for dealing with the situation where several rules fire and make the same conclusion. The first rule to fire attaches a CF with the assertion as described above. Any additional rules which fire, however, serve only to reduce any remaining uncertainty. For example, if the first rule makes an asser- tion with a CF of AL and another rule fires which makes the same assertion with a CF of .7, the combined CF after both rules have fired is .8 + (.7 x (l- .8)) = .94. EMYCIN-based systems will attempt to apply all rules that conclude about a given assertion unless the firing of a rule would lead to absolute certainty (CF = +1.0 or -1.0) in the assertion, in which case only that rule is fired. Thus, EMYCIN-based systems meet the requirement that interpreta- tion systems rigorously examine all possible values of a parameter. Moreover, the mechanics of the CF calculation permit EMYCIN-based systems to deal with incomplete data in an effective manner that results in only a gradual degrada- tion in the quality of performance. If some of the informa- tion discussed in the previous example was missing, so that only one rule could fire, the system would still be able to make a conclusion about the type of entity that the client is (albeit with less certainty than if kxnfli rules had 62 fired). Summary The use of a software tool such as EMYCIN enables the accounting researcher to use the expert system methodology to study decision making behavior without having to devote an inordinate amount of time to programming. EMYCIN has been used successfully to build expert systems in a wide variety of task domains. The task analysis indicated that judgments of auditing materiality require auditors to interpret a large body of uncertain information about a client. EMYCIN contains the procedures necessary for a rule-based expert system to perform tasks of that type. The next part of this chapter describes how the initial prototype version of AUDITPLANNER was built. Construction 9E Prototype System The process of acquiring expertise and encoding it into the knowledge base of an expert system is referred to as "knowledge acquisition" [Buchanan et al., 1983]. EMYCIN permits most of this knowledge acquisition to be done inter- actively with an expert. The expert uses the current version of the system to perform the task, notes the areas where there are gaps or flaws in the system’s logic, and suggests changes or additions to the knowledge base to correct those problems. This interactive process presupposes, however, the existence of a working version of the system. This section describes how the initial prototype version was built. 63 Choice 9E Subjects A major constraint on the choice of subjects for this dissertation is that EMYCIN, as well as other software tools available for building expert systems, cannot accomodate conflicting rules based on different judgment models. There is considerable evidence that there is a lack of consensus among auditors from different accounting firms for a wide range of judgments, including materiality. Moreover, several studies indicate that this lack of consensus also exists among auditors from the same accounting firm. Because of this apparent lack of consensus, and also due to the time required to build and refine an expert system, AUDITPLANNER is an expert system reflecting the rules-of-thumb used by an audit partner of a single accounting firm. The remainder of this section reviews the evidence of a lack of consensus among auditors and also examines the role of individual- specific models in the study of decision making behavior. Lack 9E consensus across firms. Holstrum’s [1981] review of empirical research on consensus in a wide range of audit judgments lead him to conclude that there is a lack of consensus among auditors for many of those judgments. In general, the most crucial aspect oftfluaauditor judgment research to date is the lack of consensus among auditors in typical judgments made in the audit process [Holstrum, 1981, pp.3l-32]. Previous research on materiality indicates that there is a lack of consensus on these judgments. Pattillo [1976] reports that the level of consensus among auditors regarding 64 the materiality of various errors was quite low, and in fact was lower than that for any other group in his study which included bankers, financial analysts, and financial execu- tives. Firth [1979] also measured the consensus of auditors in comparison to bankers, financial analysts, and chief accountants and similarly found the lowest degree of consen- sus among auditors. In fact, he reported that there was no consensus among auditors in about one-third of the cases. Lewis [1980] found significant differences among auditors from four different public accounting firms in terms of the threshhold at which they would disclose a contingent liabil- ity arising from a lawsuit. One possible cause of the lack of consensus in these studies is that they all confounded the decision about the materiality of the item with the decision about the proper method of its disclosureu Indeed, in its Discussion Memoran- dum (n1 Materiality, tflua FASB explicitly distinguishes between these two judgments: The determination of the materiality of an item, transaction, or situation is just one step in the financial accounting and reporting process. For example, in considering the disclosure of litiga- tion, a determination that a suit is material is only part of the process. A judgment must also be made as to the type and extent of disclosure to be made in the circumstances, considering the degree of materiality involved [FASB, 1975, para. 169]. Thus, it is possible that the reported lack of consensus found in the studies cited above may be due to (l) a lack of consensus about the materiality of the item , (2) a lack of consensus over the proper method of disclosing that item, 65 given that it is material, or (3) both of the previous reasons. A study by Ward [1976] provides some support for the second explanation. He asked auditors to rank the importance of twenty factors representing legal and technical aspects of the audit environment and found a statistically signifi- cant level of agreement on those rankings. However, he also found a marked lack of consensus about the impact of an error on the auditor’s legal liability. On the other hand, Messier [1982] did ask auditors to make separate decisions about the materiality of a writedown of inventory and its proper method of disclosure. He found a lack of consensus among auditors from "Big-8" and "non Big- 8" accouting firms. Mayper [1982] asked auditors to evaluate the materiality of of various internal control weaknesses. He reported that the level of consensus across auditing firms was only 45 percent. The evidence for a lack oflconsensus across auditing firms regarding judgments of materiality should not be sur- prising. Einhorn [1974] points out that differences in back- ground and training can lead to a lack of consensus among experts. Each accounting firm has its own audit methodology which is reflected in its audit manuals and in its training programs. The Gibbins and Wolf [1982] survey found that one factor which significantly affected all phases of the audit was the firm’s general audit methodology. Cushing and Loebbecke [1983] read the audit manuals of twelve public 66 accounting firms and classified each firm into one of four categories reflecting the degree of structure reflected in their written materials. The author of this study also noticed distinct differences in the discussions of auditing materiality in the audit manuals of ten different accounting firms. Lack of consensus within firms. Few studies have examined the degree of consensus on audit judgments among auditors from the same accounting firm. However, two studies do report a lack of such consensus. The first is a study by Mock and Turner [1979] about auditors’ievaluations of the quality of internal control and their subsequent choice of sample sizes for substantive tests. Although all auditors were members of the same accounting firm, there were consid- erable differences in their choices of sample sizes. More important, the lone previous empirical study of auditing materiality [Moriarity and Barron, 1979] found.a lack of consensus in those judgments among partners from the same accounting firm. Only two partners were in agreement on a majority of the cases. The differences found in the Moriarity and Barron study probably reflect significant differences in the back- ground of the participants. Some of the partners had had experience primarily with profit-oriented firms, while others had dealt mainly with nonprofit organizations. Post- experimental interviews indicated that these two groups had distinct differences in the types of problems they looked 67 for in an audit. Moreover, only two of the partners had had much experience in statistical sampling applications where this type of materiality judgment.is more frequently made and explicitly quantified. In fact, the lack of such experi- ence caused a sixth subject to refuse to perform the task. Implications. It appears that there are significant differences in the audit judgments made by members of different accounting firms. There is also some evidence that there is a similar lack of consensus in the judgments of auditing materiality made by members of the same accounting firm. This evidence should not be surprising. A basic tenet of human information processing research istflmnzthe task environment typically contains many items of information which are redundant in nature; this redundancy, and the adaptive nature of human behavior can produce a variety of equally useful strategies for solving a particular problem. We must expect to find different systems (even of the same species) using quite different strategies to perform the same task.]2an1not aware that any theorems have been proved about the uniqueness of good, or even best, strategies. Thus, we must expect to find strategy differences not only between sys- tems at different skill levels, but even between experts [Simon, 1980, p.42]. The construction of an expert system which reflects one strategy for making judgments of auditing materiality may play an important role in understanding how those judg- ments are made. Simon [1980] argues that progress in under- standing problem solving behavior depends upon developing a taxonomy of the alternative strategies for solving a given 68 task: research on the performance of adaptive systems must take on a taxonomic, and even a sociological aspect. We have a great deal to learn about the variety of strategies, and we should neither disdain nor shirk the painstaking, sometimes pedestrian, tasks of describing that variety. That substrate 2E descrip- tion lfi ii necessary b9 BE BE the taxonomic sub- strate has been b9 modern biology [p.42, emphasis added]. This dissertation represents one step toward develop- ing such a taxonomy for the task of making materiality judg- ments. Moreover, Dukes [1965] points out that studies of an individual subject in which the researcher is focusing on how a particular problem is solved have played a major role in psychological studies of behavior. Problem-centered research on only one subject may, by clarifying questions, defining variables, and indicating approaches, make substantial contribu- tions to the study of behavior. Besides answering a specific question, it may (Ebbinghaus’ work, 1885, being a classic example) provide important ground- work for the theorists [Dukes, 1965, p.78]. The construction of AUDITPLANNER will be based on the materiality judgments of an expert auditor in many different situations. Previous research, as pointed out in Chapter One, either held the situation in which materiality judg— ments were to be made constant, or else only varied them slightly. The use of the expert systems methodology: on the other hand, permits for more complex and realistic decision settings. Such variation of situations is essential if the goal is to explain the role of that factor in making materi- ality judgments. In this regard, this dissertation should add to our knowledge of the materiality judgment process. 69 Subject selection. Eleven public accounting firms with.offices.in Detroit were contacted in order to find a subject for this study. Ten of those firms agreed to provide either their entire audit manual or those portions which described their approach toxnaking materiality judgments. Reading those manuals indicated that there were considerable differences across firms in termssuch objective standard of materiality exists. How, then, can expertise be tested? There are no generally accepted methods for evaluating the expertise of auditors. Although auditors must pass an examination in order to become certified, that test is not used later to evaluate their expertise. This problem is not unique to accounting; it is faced in many fields of professional endeavor. Needless to say, the absence of any simple means of testing and evaluating human expertise makes the evalua- tion of expert systems difficult. Nevertheless, guidelines do exist for evaluating expert systems. Gaschnig et a1. [1983] point out that the appropriate method for evaluating the performance of an expert system depends upon its stage of development. Systems in the early 76 stages should be evaluated in terms of the correctness of their general line of reasoning, while systems that are approaching the stage of commercial release must undergo more extensive testing of the "correctness" (or at least the "acceptability") of their recommendations. Gaschnig et a1. explain why they feel that systems in the early stages of development should not be evaluated only in terms of the "correctness" of their recommendations: One problem with this emphasis on final performance is that it fails to take into account the micro- structure of problem-solving behavior, which can be extremely important in permitting the extrapolation from representative instances of behavior to make judgments concerning overall competence. Evaluators want to be convinced that the system is consistentLy getting the right answers for the—Fight reasons [p.252, emphasis added]. Gaschnig et a1. then describe a method of evaluating expert systems which will provide such assurance: the expert system needs to be exercised within a wide-ranging series of test situations aimed at dis- covering ways to make the system fail. The experts engaged in evaluating system performance must have full access to all aspects of behavior, so that they can push and probe, looking for weaknesses and defi- ciencies. This would seem b9 rule out blinded, com- parative studies ii 32 appropriate framework for expert-system evaluation, 3b least Eb the early bbgggb 12 the development life cycle [p.252, emphasis addedII Consequently, the evaluation of AUDITPLANNER consists of two separate procedures. First, the expert auditor who served as the source of the expertise embedded in the system was asked to determine whether the system is adequately per- forming on the types of clients which have been used to test it during the refinement stage. Although this evaluation is 77 subjective, it is similar to the manner in which the subject evaluates the performance and expertise of junior auditors. Once the expert felt that the system was performing adequately'on the test cases, a more formal evaluation by other experts was then made.‘This evaluation entailed using the systenlto make materiality judgments for clients that differ in a number of ways from those used to develop the system. The purpose of the testing was to discover the limits of the systenfis expertise. The focus is on identify- ing areas where additional rules are needed if the system is ever to be useful as a practical tool. The use of these "outside experts" provides two other benefits. First, it provides a general measure of the degree generality of the problem solving strategy of the system. The question is whether the rules used by one expert to make materiality judgments are considered reasonable by other experts. Second, it permits an assessment of the potential usefulness of the system. In other words, do the experts see any use for AUDITPLANNER? It is important to stress that this evaluation by the "outside experts" is not used to determine whether the system is finished. Rather, it is intended solely to provide additional data for analyzing the rules contained in the knowledge base. It is worthwhile to reiterate at this point the underlying purpose of this research: to increase our understanding of how various environmental factors affect materiality judgments. As Gaschnig et a1. [1983] point out, 78 it is not necessary to have a fully-developed, commercially usable system in order to accomplish such a purpose: Once a system begins generating performance, it becomes an important part of the laboratory appara- tus available to the knowledge engineer and cogni- tive analyst to gain fresh insights into the domain of expertise for which that system was built. The true goal 9__f evaluation should not b_e Lo show how well _a system doaes what i_t was designed Lo Lo b__Lut rather, Lo gain agreater appreciation of the pro- cessL structure, and limits 2L expertise. This sys- tem can later be parlayed into new levels of expert performance in successive system developments [p.252, emphasis added]. The remainder of this section discusses the methods and results of the testing of AUDITPLANNER. Subjects Three managers and three seniors in the same public accounting firm as that of the expert subject participated in the evaluation of AUDITPLANNER. Their cooperation was secured by means of an office memo written by the partner who served as the subject for this research. The managers ranged in experience from six to nine years, with a mean of eight years. The seniors had from three and one-half to four and one-half years experience, with a mean of four years. The seniors had at least one and one-half years experience in making judgments of auditing materiality. The subjects also had fairly diverse backgrounds, with areascfifspecial expertise including insurance companies, savings and loans, and closely-held businesses. 79 Test Cases Each participant was asked to bring the working papers for two clients to the evaluation session. The clients that were used in the evaluation were not a random sample; instead, they were chosen in order to maximize the breadth of situations over which AUDITPLANNER would be tested. This was necessary in order to obtain an accurate assessment of its overall level of competency and also in order to obtain additional information about the generality of the knowledge contained in the system. In addition, some of the test clients had characteristics not present in any of the clients used to develop AUDITPLANNER.flHmareason for this was to provide another test of the system’s competency: would it at least take a logical approach to new situations? The exact charac-teristics of the test clients are listed in Table 1. Procedure Each evaluation session lasted one hour, during which time the participants used AUDITPLANNER to make materiality judgments for their clients. After collecting demographic information about the participant, the author provided instructions on the use of AUDITPLANNER. The participant used AUDITPLANNER to make a materiality judgment for one of the test clients, compared that judgment to the actual materiality level used for that client, and evaluated the quality of AUDITPLANNER’S judgment. This process was then repeated for the other test client. TABLE 1. Characteristics of test clients CLIENT Machine tool manufacturer Machine tool manufacturer Insurance company Restaurant Automobile dealership School district Boy Scout Council Computer manufacturer Retail supermarket Retail supermarket Common carrier - trucking Common carrier - trucking Microcomputer retailer FEATURES involved in a major acquisition a subsidiary of a foreign parent a subsidiary profitable nonprofit organization nonprofit organization suffered a loss for the current year subject of litigation and made some major acquisitions subject of an inquiry by a regulatory agency private entity under- going incorporation 81 Upon completion of the one hour session, each partici- pant was givenLa questionnaire with which to evaluate the overall performance of AUDITPLANNER, as well as several aspects of its usefulness. Participants completed the ques- tionnaire in their own offices and returned it to the author later that day. Appendix IV contains a copy of the question- naire. Results AUDITPLANNER was used tornakelnateriality judgments for 13 clients. (The last session lasted two hours and used three test clients). Eight of those recommendations were judged as being acceptable. In general, AUDITPLANNER usually was more conservative than the auditors; that is, the materiality levels it recommended were lower than those recommended by the participants. The possible causes for this conservatism are discussed in the next chapter’s analy- sis of the system’s materiality judgment model. The test sessions were useful as another form of system refinement, as additional rules were suggested for several new types of clients. Discussions with the principal subject indicated that these rules should be added to the system because they reflected expertise in areas outside of the subject’s prior experiences. Overall, the six managers and seniors said that they felt that AUDITPLANNER approached the materiality judgment in a reasonable and logical manner. Moreover, all six indi- cated that they would like to be able to use such a tool if 82 it ever became available. Table 22 presents a frequency distribution of the responses to the questionnaire evaluation of AUDITPLANNER. The remainder of this section discusses those results. Competence _o_f AUDITPLANNER. Questions 7 and 22 addressed.the issue of AUDITPLANNERksgeneral competence. Ffivtaof’the six participants disagreed with the statement that AUDITPLANNER was not competent, and four of the six agreed with the statement that it was competent. The one subject who felt that AUDITPLANNER was not competent was also the one subject who indicated that he would approach the materiality judgment in a different manner than did AUDITPLANNER. Discussions during the sessions indicated that the subject had a consistently higher materiality threshhold than did AUDITPLANNER. Three of the subjects indicated that they would accept AUDITPLANNER’S recommendations about the materiality level to be used in planning the audit (question 8). Taken together, the responses to these three questions indicate that the evaluators felt that AUDITPLANNER showed evidence of a basic level of confidence in making judgments of audit- ing materiality, but that they were not yet ready to accept its recommendations in every situation. Reasonableness of model. Questions 24, 3, 15, and 19 addressed the issue of the generality and reasonableness of AUDITPLANNERTS approach to making materiality judgments. The 83 TABLE 2. Responses to test questionnaire QUESTION TOPIC s5 5 g 2 s9 7. AUDITPLANNER not competent O l 0 3 2 22. AUDITPLANNER is competent 0 4 2 0 O 8. Would accept AUDITPLANNER’S recommendations 0 3 1 2 O 24. Would approach judgment in different manner than did AUDITPLANNER 0 1 0 5 0 3. AUDITPLANNER asked irrelevant questions 0 0 0 3 3 15. AUDITPLANNER’S logic hard to follow 0 0 0 3 3 l9. AUDITPLANNER’S logic easy to follow 2 4 0 0 0 4. AUDITPLANNER would be useful as a training device 2 3 l 0 0 18. Would not want subordinates to use AUDITPLANNER as a training device 0 0 1 4 1 5. AUDITPLANNER would be useful as a decision aid (see note 1) l 4 0 0 O 6. Would want to use AUDITPLANNER as a decision aid 0 6 0 O O 14. Would permit subordinates to use AUDITPLANNER as a decision aid 0 5 0 O 1 ll. AUDITPLANNER would be more useful as a training device than as a decision aid 0 0 4 2 O 12. No conceivable use for AUDITPLANNER O O 0 4 2 21. Flow of dialogue easy to follow 2 4 0 0 O 84 TABLE 2 (continued) §Aé§2§2 1. Easy to use 2 4 O 0 O 25. AUDITPLANNER jumped around from topic to topic 0 0 0 5 1 13. Hard to use 0 O O 4 2 9. Did not enjoy using AUDITPLANNER 0 0 0 4 2 20. Enjoyed using AUDITPLANNER 1 4 l 0 0 10. AUDITPLANNER was too slow 0 O O 3 3 2. poor help facilities 0 0 2 3 l 17. inadequate question-answering capabilities 0 l l 4 0 23. HOW and WHY features useful 2 l 3 O 0 16. Used question-answering features extensively O 2 3 0 1 Key: SA = Strongly Agree A = Agree N = Don’t know or neutral D = Disagree SD = Strongly Disagree note 1 - one person did not answer question 5. His responses to the other questions about usefulness were: as a training device - agree want to use as decision aid - agree more useful for training than as decision aid - neutral no use at all - disagree permit subordinates to use as a decision aid - agree would not let subordinates for training - disagree 85 responses indicate that the evaluators agreed with the logic of the model. Five of the six evaluators indicated that they approached judgments of auditing materiality in the same way that AUDITPLANNER did. All Six indicated that AUDITPLANNER did not ask irrelevant questions; and all six agreed that it was easy to follow the system’s line-of-reasoning. Usefulness of AUDITPLANNER. An important question in building an expert system is whether there is any potential use for it other than as a method for studying decision making behavior; Questions 4, 5,Eh,ll, 12, 14, and 18 all addressed this issue of usefulness. The responses to those questions support the comments made during the sessions that the evaluators would like to use a tool like AUDITPLANNER. All six evaluators agreed with the statement that they would like to use AUDITPLANNER as a decision aid. All agreed that it would be useful as a decision aid (question 5) and five of the six indicated that they would permit their subordin- ates to use it as a decision aid. Five of the evaluators indicated that they thought that AUDITPLANNER would be useful as a training device. They also indicated that they would not mind having their subord- inates use it as a training device. The responses to question 11 (whether AUDITPLANNER would be more useful as a training device than as a decision aid) were inconclusisve, although there was a slight bias in favor of its use as a decision aid. 86 Ease of use and enjoyment. Several questions also addressed the evaluators’ general attitude towards the sys- tem in terms of ease of use and enjoyment (questions 1, 9, 10, 13, 20, 21, and 25). The responses were quite favorable. Of particular interest is the uniformly favorable reaction to the system’s sequencing of questions and flow of dialogue (questions 21 and 25% All six evaluators agreed that the system asked questions in a natural order that was easy to follow and understand. This reaction is in agreement with the findings by Aiello [1983] that users found the backward- chaining control strategy easy to understand. Adequacy of question-answering facilities. Responses to questions 17 and 23 indicate that the evaluators felt that AUDITPLANNER/s question-answering capabilities were quite helpful. However, during the sessions most evaluators made only limited use of those facilities, preferring to just answer the systenVs questions and then examining its recommendations. This subjective impression is supported by the responses to question 16, which indicate that four of the six evaluators felt that they did not make extensive use of those facilities. Apparently, however, the little use that was made generated a favorable response. Summary. The six auditors who used AUDITPLANNER to make materiality judgments for their own clients indicated that they found the system easy and enjoyable to use. The flow of dialogue was natural and easy to follow. 87 The evaluators indicated that they felt that AUDIT- PLANNER exhibited a basic level of competence in making judgments of auditing materialityu However, they were not willing to always accept its recommendations. The major source of disagreement centered on the proper materiality threshhold to be used. Subsequent discussions indicated that disagreements on this point are defensible differences of opinion. Indeed, the evaluators did agree that the logic of the model employed by AUDITPLANNER was reasonable and easy to follow. Finally, the evaluators were unanimous in their enthu- siasm for the system and their belief that it would be useful both as a training device and as a decision aid. Several rules were suggested for dealing with clients out- side the principal subjectfs primary areas of expertise and a willingness to help in further development of the system was expressed. In conclusion, AUDITPLANNER was able to competently and successfully make materiality judgments for clients with which the partner who served as the subject of this research had previous experience. The results of having six other auditors from the same accounting firm use AUDITPLANNER to make materiality judgments for their clients indicates that the logic underlying AUDITPLANNER’s approach toward making those judgments reflects the general approach used by that accounting firm. The next chapter describes the nature of that judgment process. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF THE SYSTEM This chapter examines the model of the materiality judgment process that is represented in the production rules of AUDITPLANNER. First, the setting in which those judgments are made is described. This description explains the role played by materiality in the audit planning process of one public accounting firm. Then the method by which those judg- ments are made is discussed. English translations of the system’s rules are presented in order to illustrate exactly how various qualitative factors enter into the materiality judgment process. The Decision Setting Figure 3 illustrates the role played by judgments of auditing materiality in the audit planning process of the firm studied in this dissertation. The figure shows that those judgments are used to set the precision level of the audit tests. However, materiality is not the only factor that affects the precision level. The clientfs service needs and certain aspects of audit risk also influence the choice of a precision level. Figure 3 shows business risk as a factor that affects materiality. Business risk represents risks to tjualauditor 88 89 Business Risk ;> Materiality Client Expectations Audit Risk A) Precision Level / \V Sample Sizes Figure 3. Causal Model of Auditing Materiality 9O hag., litigation) arising from the expression of an opinion on the client’s financial statements. In response to such risks the auditor may choose to reduce the materiality level used to plan the extent of audit tests. Boatsman and Robert- son [1974] found that business risk was a significant pre- dictor of their subjects’ materiality judgments. Figure 3 shows that the precision level is then used to help determine sample sizes for audit tests. Audit risk also affects the choice of sample sizes. Thus, Figure 3 illustrates the relationship between audit risk and materi- ality that is discussed in SAS No. 47: Audit risk and materiality, among other matters, need to be considered together in determining the nature, timing, and extent of auditing procedures [AICPA, 1983, para. 1]. Audit risk is the risk that the auditor will conclude that the financial statements are not materially misstated when, in fact, material errors do exist. SAS No. 47 breaks audit risk down into three components:(1) inherent risk, which is the risk that material errors would occur in the absence of a system of internal controls, (2) control risk, which is the risk that any errors which do occur will not be detected or prevented by the system of internal controls, and (3) detection risk, which is the risk that the auditorks procedures will fail to find any material errors that may exist. Figure 3 indicates that audit risk affects both the precision level and the choice of sample sizes. The inherent risk component of audit risk affects the precision level by 91 reducing that level for the amount of uncorrected errors the auditor expects to find, based on previous audits of the client. The control and detection risk components of audit risk both directly influence the choice of sample sizes. The level of control risk is derived from the assessment of the quality of internal controls. That assessment is then used to determine the level of detection risk that can be accep- ted and still achieve the desired overall level of audit risk. The actual sample sizes that will be used in audit testing are then determined by means of a formula that uses both the precision level and the level of detection risk. In summary, judgments of auditing materiality are but one factor used to establish the precision level of audit tests. Those judgments of materiality are influenced by the auditor’s assessment of business risk. Consequently, AUDIT- PLANNER contains rules to deal with both materiality and the evaluation of business risk. The remainder of this chapter describes the logic of that judgment model. The Judgment Model Appendix III contains a sample consultation with AUDITPLANNER. That transcript shows that AUDITPLANNER asks the user for basic facts about the client being audited and then uses those facts to recommend an overall materiality to be used in planning audit tests. Overview Figures 4-7 illustrate AUDITPLANNERHS judgment model. These figures are based on the data flow diagrams that are Prior Year's Materiality Levels Financial Characteristics of the Client Nonfinancial Characteristics of the Client Future Plans of the Client#__.._————"':? Nature of the Audit Engagement ‘,,r””””;7 Intended Uses of the Client's inancial Statements Calculate Overall Materiality Level Kitterlaiew Burirpnv Figure 4. Overview of AUDITPLANNER'S Judgment Model 93 Financial Characteristics of the Client ‘\$$ Identify. Nonfinancial Characteristics Materiality of the Client >: Base Future Plans of the Clientl,;? Intended Use of the Client's Financial Statements \_ Select /' Percentage Percentage Rate Rate Nature of the Audityf/yyy,;7 2.0 Engagement \/ \/ Prior Year‘s Materiality Levels Calculation of Materiality 3.0 \/ Financial Characteristics of the Client \/ \ KQIIEIlenew finintpnv Figure 5. Calculation of Overall Materiality Level 94 Client's Industry Form of Ownership““~“‘f> Client's Capital Structure\ I Nature of Debt Covenant549 Determination of the Type of Entity 1.1 Future Plans of Client4,r€7 Alllua 30 edfil Identification Financial Measures of of Liquidity and Solvency \; User's Interests /' 1.2 sisaiaiul s‘iesn Income from Continuing Operation yPaid-In-Capital ENE‘E‘7A Prior Year's Inddm2--“7> Choosing the Long Term Debt 9“~‘j% Materiality Materiality Base Base 3 I Current Assets _.———"' 3 Current Liabilities ,av" I Figure 6. Identification of Materiality Base 1.3 ’ 95 Calculate Materiality Base :> Tentative Materiality Percentage Rate \; Level 7 4.1 {sAaq Airterxsiew enrieiual \/ Materiality Base Qgrrent Assets Test Adequacy Stockholder's Equity ‘ o Materiality' Level 4.2 Percentage Rate \V vi, (,4, Last Year's Materiality Level ‘\ Kitterxaiew Buritpnv Figure 7. Calculation of Materiality 96 used in the structured analysis of information systems [DeMarco, 1979]. The bubbles represent the processes or decisions that are made during a consultation. The labeled arcs represent the information that is used to make those decisions. Figures 4-7 represent the judgment model in increasing levels of detail. Figure 4 provides a highly summarized pic- ture of the entire process. It indicates that the following information is used to calculate the overall materiality level: 1. The materiality levels used in prior audits of the client. 2. Various financial characteristics of the client. 3. Various nonfinancial characteristics of the client. 4. Future plans of the client. 5. The nature of the audit engagement. 6. The intended uses of the client’s financial statements. The financial characteristitmsof the client that are used to calculate an overall materiality level include the following: 1. Current assets 2. Current liabilities 3. Long-term debt 4. Paid-in capital 5. Retained earnings 6. Income from continuing operations 7. Prior years’ income 97 The nonfinancial characteristics include: 1. The industry of the client 2. The client’s capital structure 3. The nature of the client’s debt covenants 4. The nature of the client (is it a subsidiary) The future plans of the client refer to any client plans for the issuance of additional debt or equity securi- ties. The nature of the audit engagement refers to whether or not this is the initial audit of the client. The intended uses of the client/s financial statements represent certain types of business risks which affect the materiality calcu- lation. Figure 5 illustrates the major steps involved in this calculation. Various financial and nonfinancial characteris- tics of the client, together with information about the client’s future plans, are used to select the appropriate base for the materiality calculation (bubble 14H. Informa- tion about the intended use of the financial statements and the nature of the audit engagement is used to select the percentage raterto use in calculating materiality (bubble 2.0). The percentage rate, the materiality base, prior years’ materiality levels, and various financial character- istics of the client are all used to make the actual materi- ality calculation (bubble 3.0). AUDITPLANNER uses the following production rule, known as a goal rule, to ensure that each of these three major activities is performed during the course of a consultation: 98 Rule _Q_(note: the numbers on rules are for reference purposes only) IF: l)Ii;is definite that the client isrunza non- profit organization, and 2) Information has been gathered about the appropriate base for the materiality calcu- lation and whether there are any special business risks which need to be considered when planning the audit, and 3) An attempt has been made to deduce the percentage rate used to determine the materiality level, and 4) An attempt has been made to deduce the over- all materiality level that will be used to plan the extent of audit procedures and tests THEN: It is definite that a judgment of auditing materiality which will be used to plan the extent of audit procedures has been made. This rule is called a goal rule because its action part satisfies the goal of the consultation: making a judg- ment about the overall materiality level to be used in plan- ning audit tests. (The reason for clause 1 is that the procedure for dealing with nonprofit organizations differs somewhat.fron1the procedure for profit-oriented entities. Each fund or fund type of a nonprofit organization has its own materiality level). The attempt to satisfy the premises of rule 80 results in the creation of a goal tree which orders the major sub- tasks that have to be performed. First, AUDITPLANNER deter- mines whether or not the client is a nonprofit organization. If it is, then a different goal rule is tried. If not, AUDITPLANNER then gathers background information about the client in order to identify the appropriate base for the 99 materiality calculation.Next, information about any special business risks is gathered and evaluated. That information is used to select a threshold rate. The materiality base is then multiplied by the threshold rate to derive an overall materiality level. The reader is reminded that this ordering of the materiality judgment process was designed to produce a consultation whose logic was easy to follow. No claim is made that the subject necessarily follows the steps in the sequence outlined above. For example, an auditor may assess the risks associated with the audit prior to determining the basis for the materiality calculation. Nevertheless, these steps are all part of the process of making the materiality judgment, and the rules used by AUDITPLANNER to perform those steps do accurately reflect the way that the expert subject makes those decisions. The remainder of this section describes how each of these three major decisions (represented by bubbles 1.0, 2.0, and 3.0 in Figure 5) is made. Identification gf the Materiality Base Figure 6 illustrates the steps involved in identifying the appropriate base for the materiality calculation (bubble 1.0 in Figure 5). Three steps are involved. First, nonfinan- cial client characteristics and the client’s future plans are used to determine what type of entity the client is. Then that decision and various financial measures of liquid- ity and solvency are used to infer the primary interests of 100 the users of the client/s financial statements. Finally, the inferred users’ interests and other financial characterist- ics of the client are used to select the materiality base. Determination f the type _f entity. Figure 6 shows that the following information is used to determine what type of entity the client is: 1. The client’s industry 2. Whether or not the client is a subsidiary (form of ownership) 3. The client’s capital structure 4. The nature of any debt covenants 5. Future plans of the client These factors are all mentioned in SAS No. 22 as being items that should be considered when planning an audit. Subjects in previous empirical studies also mentioned that they would like to have such information upon which to base their materiality judgments. However, neither the authoritative literature nor previous empirical research has explained how these factors influence materiality judgments. Figure 6 shows that they are used to help determine what type of entity the client is, and this information in turn is used to eventually select the appropriate base for calculating the overall materiality level. Moreover, examination of the rules used to determine the type of entity the client is shows exactly Egg these qualitative factors influence materiality. AUDITPLANNER has seven rules which it uses to deter- lOl mine what type of entity the client is (bubble 1.1). Rule 7 IF: 1) It is likely that the client may be a private entity, and 2) The client is not filing with a regulatory agency in preparation for the sale of its securities in a public market, and 3) The client does not intend to "go public" in the next two or three years THEN: The client is a private entity. ule 2 so H F: 1) It is likely that the client may be a private entity, and 2) A: The client is filing with a regulatory agency in preparation for the sale of its securities in a public market, or B: The client intends to go public in the next two or three years THEN: The client is a public entity. Rule 4_ IF: 1) The client has publicly traded debt or equity securities, or 2) The client has restrictive debt covenants that are measured by or depend on periodic financial statement amounts or ratios that involve results of operations THEN: The client is a public entity. Rule _3 IF: The client’s main line of business or industry classification is insurance THEN: The client is a public entity. Rule 5 IF: 1) The client does not have any publicly traded debt or equity securities, and THEN: Rul (D 2) 3) l) 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) 3) 102 The client does not have any restrictive debt covenants that are measured by or depend on periodic financial statement amounts or ratios that involve results of operations, and The client is not controlled by a public entity It is likely that the client may be a private entity. The client does not have any publicly traded debt or equity securities, and The client does not have any restrictive debt covenants that are measured by or depend upon periodic financial statement amounts or ratios that involve results of operations, and The client is controlled by a public entity, and The client is a wholly-owned subsidiary, and The principal external users of the client’s financial statements are creditors or others who are primarily interested in the client’s financial position rather than in results of Operations, and The client’s parent considers it to be a private subsidiary It is likely that the client may be a private entity. The client does not have any publicly traded debt or equity securities, and The client does not have any debt covenants which are measured by or depend on periodic financial statement amounts or ratios that involve results of operations, and The client is controlled by a public entity, and 103 4) A: The client is not a wholly-owned subsidiary, or B: The principal users of the client’s financial statements are not creditors or others who are more interested in the client’s financial position than in results of operations, or C: The client’s parent does not consider it to be a private subsidiary THEN: The client is a public entity. These rules represent the firnfs definitions of public and private entities. Rules 40 and 56 define the clear-cut situations. Rules 94 and 9S deal with the situation where theeclient;is a subsidiary'of a public entity, and outline the conditions that must be met in order for it to be con- sidered a private entity. The transcript in Appendix III indicates that the user is asked to supply the information needed to establish the validity of the premises in these seven rules. Rule 53 is particularly worth noting, because it is an example of the importance of information about the clientis industry classification. The justification for this rule is that the regulatory agencies which are among the major users of insurance companies’ financial statements are interested in their results of‘operations, regardless of the form of ownership. This rule illustrates one of the advantages of using the expert systems methodology to study the materiality judgment process. Rule 53 represents one way in which infor- mation about the client’s industry can influence those 104 judgments. However, because there are many other variables which intervene between this factor and the ultimate materi- ality level, it is not likely to be a statistically signifi- cant predictor of those judgments. Gibbins and Wolf [1983] reported that plans for future sales or financing by the client were an important factor influencing the conduct of the audit. Rules 7 and 26 show one way in which that factor affects materiality judgments. If the auditor knows that a privately-held client is going to become publicly-held in the near future, it is considered to be a public entity. The rationale is that current owners are likely to become shareholders and will have the same interests as do the owners of publicly-held entities. With these seven rules AUDITPLANNER is able to deter- mine what type of entity the client is. Figure 6 shows that this decision is then used as one of the factors that helps to identify the primary interests of the users of the finan- cial statements (bubble 1.2). The next section explains how these interests are inferred. Identification gf users’ interests. SAS No. 47 states that " the auditor’s consideration of materiality . . . is influenced by his perception of the needs of a reasonable person who will rely