‘bv1531-J RETURNING MATERIALS: P1ace in book drop to LJBRAfiJES remove this checkout from w your record. FINES win be charged if book is returned after the date .stamped below. I. .7 J x 7%: “a; ' 3" I. . ' 8'" . . /{)4?5‘ ,, 9’ ~ ‘ «1 fi ' v if 7.9M a .“J -- I' ‘2‘ ‘7‘“! TASK PERFORMANCE AND OBJECTIVE SELF-AWARENESS IN COGNITIVELY ANXIOUS STUDENTS By Ann M. Isenberg A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partiaT fquiTTment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of PsychoTogy 1985 ‘32;EQ/175°e3 ABSTRACT TASK PERFORMANCE AND OBJECTIVE SELF-AWARENESS IN COGNITIVELY ANXIOUS STUDENTS By Ann M. Isenberg Theory suggests that an individual who experiences a large number of cognitive intrusions when anxious will also evidence a high level of distraction during task performance when made highly self-aware (by use of a mirror). This study investigated hypothesized performance decrement resulting from these conditions. One hundred female undergraduates completed measures of trait and state-cognitive anxiety, worry, and cognitive interference. As hypothesized, subjects high in trait-cognitive anxiety scored higher in these areas than those low in such anxiety. Subjects also copied Swedish prose for five minutes. The prediction that those high in trait cognitive anxiety, performing the task in a condition of high objective self-awareness, would evidence the greatest performance decrement of all groups was not supported. Lack of observed performance decrement may be related to the level of absolute anxiety evidenced in the sample. Further considerations and implications for future research was outlined. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank the members of my committee, Dr. Norman Abeles, Chairperson, Dr. Dozier Thornton, and Dr. Elaine Donelson, for their consideration, guidance, and valuable contributions to this study. My thanks, as well, go to Dr. John E. Hunter for his time and much appreciated suggestions concerning the study design and statistical analysis of my data. I also wish to express my deepest appreciation to my family, especially to my mother, Edna Isenberg, friends, and colleagues who have provided ideas, support, encouragement, and at times, a much needed listening ear. They have been and will continue to be a greatly valued part of my life. This study necessitated the use of the following undergraduates who put in long hours to learn the experimental procedure, to run subjects for eight weeks, and to score the copying task - my thanks to Deb Norton, Sherri Szuch, Rhonda McDaniel, and Karen Albosta. Finally, special thanks go to the typists: Jeanne Gray, University Park, Pennsylvania, for battling the flu and the ending of term-break to finish the final thesis draft (pre-orals) and Suzy Pavick, East Lansing, Michigan, who has spent many hours over the past two years helping to prepare proposal drafts, thesis drafts, and - at last - the final copy. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ....................... iv LIST OF FIGURES ...................... v INTRODUCTION ....................... l Cognitive Anxiety ................... 3 Worry ......................... 9 Objective Self-Awareness ............... l5 Structure of the Study ................ Zl Hypotheses ...................... 25 METHOD ........................... 28 Subjects ....................... 28 Materials ....................... 29 Instruments ...................... 29 Procedure ....................... 32 RESULTS .......................... 39 Reliability of Measures ................ 4T Hypotheses ...................... 4l Post Hoc Analysis ................... 50 DISCUSSION ......................... 53 APPENDIX A ......................... 66 APPENDIX B ......................... 71 APPENDIX C ......................... 82 APPENDIX D ......................... 84 APPENDIX E ......................... 87 APPENDIX F ......................... 89 APPENDIX G ......................... 92 REFERENCES ......................... 95 iii LIST OF TABLES Table Page l Analysis of Variance Results for Dependent Measures ....................... 4O 2 Means and Standard Deviations of Pre-Task State-Cognitive Anxiety Scores ............ 42 3 Means and Standard Deviations of Pre-Task Worry Scores ........................ 43 4 Means and Standard Deviations of Copying Task (Performance) Scores ................. 44 5 Means and Standard Deviations of Post-Task State- Cognitive Anxiety Scores ............... 45 6 Means and Standard Deviations of Post-Task Worry Scores ........................ 45 7 Means and Standard Deviations of Cognitive Interference Scores . ................ 47 8 Means and Standard Deviations of Mirror/Plywood Presence Scores ................... 48 9 Means and Standard Deviations of Mirror/Plywood Distraction Scores .................. 48 lo Means and Standard Deviations of Copying Task (Performance) Scores Using Post Hoc Sample Distribution ..................... Sl iv LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page I. Procedural diagram of study, subject selection through experimental phase .............. 35 INTRODUCTION A multitude of variables exist which may be included within the term "individual differences." This study will investigate one specific way in which an individual difference may be manifested: the predominant mode of experiencing anxiety. The crucial question to be examined is this: do individuals react differently under given circumstances based on differences in the modality of the experience of anxiety? Cognitive anxiety can be described as the presence of cognitive intrusions, thoughts or images, which distract an individual from desired focus of attention. When anxious, varying amounts of attention are those divided between the internal concerns brought about by the cognitive intrusions and what is taking place in the environment. "Worry" may also be described as a cognitive activity, one which is generally concentrated on anticipated negative consequences of an activity: failing, not doing as well as others, future ramifications of doing poorly, etc. Objective self-awareness is theoretically present whenever an individual becomes increasingly aware of himself or herself as an object in the world. Attention is focused within on any intraself discrepancy perceived to be salient at that moment. 2 Thus, an individual who experiences a large number of cognitive intrusions when anxious and who tends to be concerned with the prospect of negative consequences of a personal activity should become even more internally focused and aware of perceived intraself discrepancies when placed in circumstances which increase self-awareness. A person who has difficulty concentrating on a task and experiences self-doubt regarding performance should then become increasingly distracted by these concerns within the objective self-awareness paradigm and should experience more performance decrement as a result. Although applicable to an infinite variety of circumstances, the problem just described is one which may have great significance in a clinical setting. Individuals will become increasingly self-aware whenever they are reminded of themselves as objects in the world. This may include situations where they feel the object of any sort of personal attention, especially those in which there may be elements of perceived evaluation or judgment. In many ways, then, the therapy process, with its focus on introspection and the process of change, will increase an individual's sense of objective self-awareness. Clients bring into therapy a host of individual differences which can affect the change process. Individuals prone to rumination and disruptions in concentration when anxious will at times react to the therapeutic process differently than those persons whose concentration and attention are not disrupted or distracted. Because the experience of anxiety may be considered 3 nearly universal within the therapeutic setting at some point, it is important that clinicians be aware of the differential aspects of anxiety experience. Cognitive Anxiety Traditionally, most anxiety questionnaires have reflected the assumption that internal states may be considered as global and undifferentiated (Davidson, l978). More recently, converging lines of evidence from both the clinical-personality sphere and from peripheral psychophysiology have suggested that anxiety should more properly be viewed as multidimensional in nature. Investigation of the nature and reduction of fear has found that intercorrelations among measures of fear employed in desensitization are typically low. It was suggested that specific behavioral systems may thus be separately manipulated (Lang, 1969). Psychophysiological research conducted on fear reduction has also revealed a multidimensionality in anxiety. Hodgson and Rachman (l974) observed desynchrony in different physiological measures, all having great face validity. Further research suggested that it might be possible for different subsystems involved in dysfunctional states to be separately manipulated. Lazarus (1973) called for therapeutic regimes to be individually constructed to reduce distressing symptoms in specific modes and systems evidencing difficulties. The existence of a multidimensional anxiety has been established psychometrically through factor analytic studies of traditional anxiety questionnaires. Barrett (in Schwartz, Davidson, & Coleman, 4 l978, p. 322) conducted an item analysis of anxiety items from a large number of commonly used scales. Results "'indicated two major subsets: (a) awareness of somatic changes...(and) (b) conscious awareness of unpleasant feelings about self or external stimuli.'" In factor analytic studies of self-rating, Hamilton and Buss (in Schwartz, Davidson, & Goleman, p. 322) found that two factors, which were labeled psychic and somatic anxiety, could account for the majority of the variance in test performance. Borkovec (T976) has used the multiple measurement of three separate but interacting response components (cognitive, overt behavior, and physiological) to operationally define anxiety. He also suggested that each system might be separately influenced by different environmental conditions and individual differences. Psychophysiological studies have investigated the specific patterning of physiological processes that are associated with particular behavioral states. Schwartz, Davidson, and Maer (1975) studied hemispheric activation using lateral eye movements as an index. It was found that the complex cognitive tasks employed could be meaningfully differentiated on the basis of underlying hemispheric substrates. Data suggested that the manner in which the verbal-spatial dimension interacts with cerebral asymmetry is different from that of the affective-nonaffective dimension. A study by Davidson and Schwartz (l977, in Davidson, l978, p. 432) asked subjects to generate images in different modalities (visual, kinesthetic). Data from EEG measurement of sensorimotor activation reflected a pattern of activation between the two 5 unimodel imagery conditions. Findings suggest that attention to or the imaging of a stimulus in a particular modality is associated with specific activation in the cortical region specialized to process information in that modality. Davidson and Schwartz (l976) developed a psychobiological model of subcomponents involved in anxiety and its reduction. The initial assumption was that there exist two relatively independent types of anxiety: cognitive and somatic. It was proposed that different procedures utilized in the reduction of anxiety would differ in the degree to which they affected the cognitive as Opposed to the somatic system. Mode-specific manifestations of anxiety were suggested as also appearing across individuals or within the same person at different times (Davidson & Schwartz, l976, p. 402). In addition, the presence of subsystems within each mode would tend to determine the particular type of cognitive or somatic symptom. The authors pr0posed that the direction of the differentiated response to relaxation procedures would depend on asymmetrical activation of either the right or left hemisphere in the given technique employed (p. 402). It was further hypothesized that a process occurring in a given mode would not only strongly affect the same mode, but would also influence other modes to a lesser degree (p. 420). Following principles of psychobiological specificity, Davidson and Schwartz (l976) conceptualized the systems underlying each mode as having a "fixed" channel capacity. It might be assumed that anxiety in a particular mode will place limits on further processing in that mode more than in others (p. 424). 6 Schwartz, Davidson, and Goleman (1978) reported a study utilizing adult volunteers, in which it was predicted that physical exercise would lead specifically to reduction primarily in somatic activity with less effect upon cognitive activity. It was also hypothesized that a technique requiring the self-generation of cognitive versus somatic activity would specifically attenuate anxiety in the cognitive system. Data supporting these hypotheses suggested that specific subcomponents of anxiety may be differentially associated with relaxation techniques engaging primarily cognitive as opposed to somatic subsystems (p. 324). A l4-item self-report measure of cognitive and somatic trait anxiety, the Cognitive Somatic Anxiety Questionnaire (CSAQ), was developed in conjunction with the just-mentioned study. Items were selected from well-known questionnaires and subjected to the agreement of three independent judges. Subjects were asked to rate the degree to which they generally or typically experience the given symptom when feeling anxious (Schwartz, Davidson, & Goleman, 1978, p. 325). Davidson and Schwartz (1976) described a condition of low cognitive anxiety as representing a lack of unwanted cognitive intrusions. Because of the absence of such intrusions, attention is not distracted by activity in the cognitive sphere. A high degree of cognitive anxiety would represent a distraction by activity in the cognitive sphere, such as images or thoughts of failing at a task or about an upcoming event irrelevant to the moment in question. 7 Some Specific examples of cognitive anxiety, as reflected by statements on the CSAQ are as follows. For example, when about to speak before an audience and feeling anxious, an individual might experience: 1) uncontrollable thoughts, such as: 2) 3) 4) 5) 6) 7) What if they think my remarks are stupid? worries over irrelevant matters, such as: I wonder if Mary ever found someone to sit for her kids? trouble keeping_anxietyprovokingpictures out of mind, such as: Picturing the way in which you are standing as awkward and tense. experiencingunimportant thoughts causinggbother, such as: Thinking about the upcoming workday and having trouble concentrating on the speech. trouble keepihg anxiety provoking thoughts out of mind, such as: What if my ending doesn't go over as I've planned? terrifying scenes, such as: Imagining that suddenly you have forgotten what you are about to say and you stand in front of everyone in total silence. experiencing indecision, such as: Should I tell this joke or leave it out? Individuals exhibiting a high level of cognitive anxiety will tend to have attention focused inward, to their own thoughts and 8 images. This will divert attention from external objects and events, including any sort of performance task. It has been noted that generally anxious individuals tend to manifest both high cognitive and high somatic anxiety (pp. 426-427). There is, of course, the question of the accuracy of self-report measures in assessing what is actually being experienced by the subject. In addition, subjects are certainly aware that they are being assessed and respond accordingly. Distortion on the part of subjects may be due to such processes as denial, defensiveness, social desirability, and the subjects' own perceptions (i.e., what personal meaning "anxiety" has for the subject - for example, if anxiety is perceived as a "negative" state, a subject may intentionally reduce the amount of anxiety self-reported). Also, and of great interest in this study, Kazdin (1980) has commented that depending upon the assessment device and response format, individuals have shown a tendency to check extreme values on rating scales, give cautious answers, and to be inconsistent across items. Self-report measures, including the CSAQ, will be utilized exclusively in this study. Despite their limitations, the instruments which will attempt to measure the constructs of interest are felt to be the most appropriate existing at this time. As measured by the CSAQ, the subdividing of anxiety into cognitive and somatic components can be considered an over-simplification. However, authors Schwartz, Davidson, and Goleman (1978) feel that it is a useful starting point for stimulating theory and research (p. 326). 9 A relatively small amount of literature has reported work with adults and children utilizing the cognitive-somatic anxiety distinction. In addition to the Schwartz, Davidson, and Goleman (1978) study previously mentioned, Kirkland and Hollandsworth (1980) used the CSAQ in a treatment study involving college students. Measures of frequency and intensity (interpreted by the authors as measures of "trait“ and "state" anxiety, respectively) of cognitive and somatic anxiety were used in another treatment study for college students by Thompson, Griebstein, and Kuhlenschmidt (1980). A state version of the CSAQ was developed by Heide (1981) in his investigation of relaxation-induced anxiety involving adult volunteers (college students, faculty, and community members). Recently, Fox and Houston (1983) reported the development of self-report measures of cognitive and somatic state and trait anxiety fOr children. Results of this study indicated a relationship between cognitive trait anxiety and task performance. High cognitive state and/or trait anxiety was also found to be associated with more preoccupation and perfbrmance designation. Overall, the authors indicated that results supported the construct validity of the measures developed. m The multidimensional nature of anxiety has also been reported in the considerable literature concerning test (and evaluation) anxiety. In 1952, Mandler and S. Sarason (in Holroyd & Appel, 1980, pp. 129-130) first attempted to differentiate test anxiety from general anxiety theory. They hypothesized that test anxiety 10 consisted of both cognitive (feelings of inadequacy and helplessness, anticipation of punishment or loss of self-esteem) and physiological (autonomic arousal) components. This theory was one of a "learned anxiety drive." Evaluative exam situations elicit higher levels of anxiety drive from test-anxious individuals than from the non-test-anxious. As a result, individuals experience higher levels of autonomic arousal and worry. Mandler and Sarason hypothesized that poor test performance resulted because cognitive and autonomic responses elicited by the anxiety drive were incompatible with responses necessary for effective test performance. Other researchers noted that highly test-anxious individuals tend "to be generally negatively self-preoccupied" and "describ(e) themselves in self-devaluing terms on other paper and pencil measures" (I. Sarason, 1960, in Wine, 1980, p. 355). Sarason suggested that under evaluative stress, such persons respond to heightened autonomic arousal with personalized, self-oriented responses. Attention is directed away from the task and performance suffers (Deffenbacher, 1980, pp. 355-356). Wine (1980) suggested that test-anxious individuals are likely to interpret a wide range of situations as evaluative and to be tuned to cues from other persons which are interpreted as reflecting evaluatively upon them, typically in a negative way. Self-statements often involve a preoccupation with failures in the past or future negative consequences (p. 376). Test anxiety is generally viewed by more recent theory as a primarily cognitive response (I. Sarason, 1975, in 1.6. Sarason, 11 1980, p. 131), a typically self-critical response (Mandler, 1975, in I.G. Sarason, 1980, p. 131) which is to be emphasized over individual differences in autonomic arousal when distinguishing the test-anxious from the nontest-anxious. In Mandler's view, all individuals are aroused physiologically by examination situations, but only the test-anxious are preoccupied with self-critical ruminations. Wine's direction of attention hypothesis (1971; 1982) has also emphasized the significance of the cognitive element in test and evaluation anxiety. She observed that during task performance and under evaluative testing conditions, the attention of highly test-anxious persons is divided between task-relevant variables and internally focused, self-depreciating thinking and perception of autonomic reactivity. Wine (1982) has also suggested that "evaluation anxiety“ cuts across state/trait distinctions. Individuals scoring high on self-report measures of trait anxiety, "whether general or situationally specific, are prone to becoming worried and tense" (p. 208). Thus they will tend to report high levels of state anxiety given appropriate situational circumstances. Although Mandler and S. Sarason (1952) had suggested the dual component nature of test anxiety, it was not until 1967 that research of Liebert and Morris began to oppose the existing trend toward a lack of operationalism and the treatment of state anxiety as a uniform condition. Components of Mandler and Sarason's Test Anxiety Questionnaire (TAQ) were examined by factor analytic techniques. Two classes of factors appeared to emerge: cognitive ("worry") or lack of confidence, and factors which referred to 12 various indices of autonomic arousal ("emotionality"). The Liebert and Morris lO-item worry-emotionality questionnaire was then utilized in a study of university undergraduates which suggested the inverse relationship of worry to performance expectancy (Liebert & Morris, 1967). The scale reflected momentary state anxiety and was expected to fluctuate with conditions related to any given situation. "Worry" has been described as primarily a cognitive concern regarding the consequences of failing, the ability of others as opposed to one's own ability, etc. Examples of "worry" might include: 1) consequence of failing, such as: If I fail this exam, I know I'll fail the entire course and never make it in this major. 2) ability of others, such as: I feel so stupid. I know that everyone else is getting these problems so easily. Wine (1971; 1982) suggested that it was this cognitive (1982) or "worry" (1971) component of evaluation or test anxiety which interferes most directly with cognitive performance (1982, p. 209). It also appears closely related to the attentional interpretation previously described (1971, p. 99). .Wine also believed this cognitive component to be the more stable, serving as a "trigger" for heightened physiological activity (1982, p. 209). "Worry" appears to be the anxiety component most consistently and most strongly related (inversely) to academic performance. There appears to be a weak or no overall relationship between 13 performance and the "emotionality" component (Deffenbacher, 1980; Doctor & Altman, 1969; Morris & Liebert, 1970). In all studies involving evaluative stress, "worry" has been found at higher levels than "emotionality." Highly trait-anxious individuals have reported significantly more "worry" than "emotionality." Thus, evaluative stress appears to elicit a tendency for the highly test-anxious to become preoccupied with worrisome cognitions and only secondarily with self-perceived affective-autonomic arousal (Deffenbacher, 1980, p. 123). Physiological arousal may at some point begin to interfere with performance, but in the sense that an individual's attention may be directed toward the arousal and away from the task (p. 123). The discovery that highly test-anxious persons report significantly more "worry" than "emotionality" thus suggests that previously mentioned characteristics of the test-anxious will also apply to those scoring in the higher range of the "worry" dimension of this measure. "Worry" and "emotionality" appear to be different response classes elicited by different conditions. Not only does the presence or absence of evaluative stress seem associated with varying levels of worry, but a temporal relationship has also been suggested. "Emotionality" is elicited primarily by cues associated with the beginning of evaluation and peaks at its initiation. "Worry" appears elicited by cues which make evaluation and possible failure salient and covaries with input or feedback related to possible failure (Deffenbacher, in I. Sarason, 1981, pp. 122-123). 14 Therefore, in addition to efficiacy performance during exams or other tasks, "worry" should be significantly elevated prior to such situations, making it more likely to interfere with preparation (including learning and rehearsal Ep. 121]). In summary, "worry" appears to be a stable disposition, with scores which are fairly constant across time, that interferes directly with cognitive performance, triggers autonomic activity, and plays a major role in the maintenance of test and evaluation anxiety (Wine, 1982, p. 212). It is possible that worry, as a particular kind of cognitive intrusion, may constitute a subtype of cognitive anxiety. The "worry-emotionality" measure to be used in this study was developed by Morris, Davis, and Hutchings (1981) as a combination- revision of 40-items from previous scales (Liebert & Morris, 1967; Spielberger et al., 1970; Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970; Osterhouse, 1972). "Worry" items chosen reflected specific negative expectations as well as preoccupation with performance, self- evaluation, and a concern about the perceptions of others. "Emotionality" items involved specific physiological and somatic reactions as well as the use of terms which suggested overarousal, nervousness, and general affective discomfort. All of the original (1967) "emotionality" items from the Liebert and Morris questionnaire were retained, while only two of the original "worry" items were judged as "satisfactory" (Morris, Davis, & Hutchings, 1981, p. 549). This study, as in the 1967 Liebert and Morris work and nearly all other mentioned studies, involved university 15 undergraduates. It was conducted for the purpose of producing the revised scale and exploring further theoretical considerations. In addition to the studies mentioned above, the use of a dual-component theory of test or evaluation anxiety, involving cognitive (worry) and emotionality distinctions has been reported in investigations of feedback and test perfbrmance (Morris & Fulmer, 1976), sources of interference while performing under evaluative stress (Deffenbacher, 1978), effects of timed and untimed testing conditions on high-worry subjects (Morris & Liebert, 1969), and in treatment studies, such as those of Kirkland and Hollandsworth (1980) and Finger and Galassi (1977). Objective Self-Awareness The same cognitive processes labeled as anxious self-preoccupation or self-focusing in test anxiety theory (Wine, 1971) "(appear) to be labelled objective self-awareness or self-focused attention" in objective self-awareness theory (Holroyd, Westbrook, Wolf, & Badhorn, 1978, p. 443). In 1972, Duval and Wicklund defined the state of objective self-awareness as one in which persons take themselves to be objects (Holroyd et al., 1978, p. 233) in the world. In this state of "self-focused attention," it was postulated that as a result of events which focus attention inward, an individual would concentrate specifically on an intraself discrepancy. Theory assumed that negative affect, with self-criticism appearing to be the most likely (Hull & Levy, 1974, p. 245), will result because the individual will presumably find shortcomings within him/herself. 16 These shortcomings will be the consequence of perceived discrepancies between aspiration and attainment. Originally, Duval and Wicklund thus believed that objective self-awareness would be aversive condition. However, conscious attention focused on self or on external events (subjective self-awareness) was not seen as a dichotomous situation. It was allowed that attention could oscillate between the self and the non-self. Therefore, it is more accurate to speak of proportionate "increased" or "decreased" objective self-awareness (Hull & Levy, 1974, p. 234). In their original statement of self-awareness theory, Duval and Wicklund (1972) suggested that following the development of negative affect, individuals would first attempt to avoid the self-focus stimulus by finding distractions. If such avoidance were impossible, they would then engage in some sort of discrepancy reduction. Typically, this would involve "efforts to bring a trait or behavior in line with a (personal) standard or aspiration" (Hull & Levy, 1974, p. 236). If no behavioral standard is salient at that moment, self-attentive persons may simply become more conscious of their own present attribution or feelings. They will tend to be more aware of dominant affect and more responsive to that affect (Carver & Scheier, 1978, p. 324). Later expansion of objective self-awareness theory suggested that it is possible fer either negative or positive affect to result depending upon whether attention is directed toward a negative or positive discrepancy. Although evidence from the area of achievement motivation has suggested that virtually all l7 naturally occurring discrepancies are negative, they may be rendered positive by “regent? success experiences (Hull & Levy, 1979). Such self-focused attention may also be thought of as a type of self-confrontation. Sackheim and Gur (1978) have stated that feedback of self will lead to autonomic arousal, negative self-evaluations, defensive reactions, and constriction of ideational content. Subjects' phenomenological reports of their experience during self-confrontation generally describe negative affective states of increased anxiety. Self-confrontation may thus be described as aversive (p. 159). Any stimulus which reminds individuals of their object status will increase objective self-awareness. In theory, "any symbol or reflection of a person will cause a shift of (his/her) focus inward" (Wicklund, 1975, p. 234). The mirror has become the "clear favorite" among researchers as a means of heightening self-focus because of its simplicity and ease of manipulation (Carver & Scheier, 1978, p. 325). Numerous studies utilizing a mirror to produce objective self-awareness have been conducted with university undergraduates (Davis & Brocke, 1975; Duval & Wicklund, 1973; Holroyd, Westbrook, Wolf, & Badhorn, 1978; Hull & Levy, 1979; Ickes, Wicklund, & Ferris, 1973; Paulus, Annis, & Risner, l978). Contrary to original predictions, Wicklund and Duval (1971) observed that subjects' performance on a prose-capying task was facilitated in the presence of a mirror. The same experimental l8 task was repeated by Liebling and Shaver (1973), varying levels of evaluation. The authors suggested that there appeared to be an inverse relationship between level of evaluation and performance facilitation. When subjects were placed in a highly evaluative situation, objective self-awareness was related to a decrement in performance. Questions of mirror validity have generally not been dealt with in the studies mentioned. Wicklund (1975) explained this lack of examination of manipulation checks and validity issues by stating that attempted manipulation checks are “probably worthless in objective self-awareness paradigms" (p. 267). Wicklund's reasoning was as follows. Ideally, a manipulation check should investigate whether a subject's attention has moved to some salient dimension within the self. After the primary dependent measure is completed, an individual might be asked to rate the degree of self-consciousness with respect to that dimension. However, objective self-awareness theory would also predict that any direct question of this kind would, in itself, bring the individual's attention directly to bear on the dimension of interest. As a result, there will be "a washing-out“ of the prior manipulation. In describing the extent of self-consciousness, it would be highly probable that a subject might be using the experimental manipulation as a cue for an appropriate answer (p. 268). One attempt at validation of the mirror was conducted by Carver and Scheier (1978). The Self-Consciousness Scale (SCS) (Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975) was used to measure chronic 19 dispositions to be self-attentive. Results indicated that the "mirror does manipulate, and the private self-consciousness subscale of the 505 does measure, self-attention" (p. 324). A person high in private self-consciousness would be very cognizant of his or her own thoughts, feelings, and motives. Duval and Wicklund (1972) have argued that when individuals are in a state of objective self-awareness, they will evaluate perceptions of self and their behavior in terms of individual standards of correctness (in Sachkeim & Gur, 1978). However, the Duval and Wicklund theory as a whole places little emphasis on the role of individual differences in predicting reactions to self-focus or self-confrontation. Sackheim and Our (1978) have contended that if individuals differ to the degree to which they hold discrepant cognitions regarding the self or in the way in which they assess the evaluative nature of situations, “one would expect that they would also differ in their reactions to self-confrontation" (p. 164). Evidence has suggested that when self-confronted, individuals perceive aspects of themselves they do not like. The perception of such discrepancies is suggested to be aversive and reflected in heightened anxiety. This position would be congruent with the findings of Sackheim (in Sackheim & Gur, 1978, p. 167) which have indicated that individual differences in cognitive discrepancy, a measure of the degree of internal conflict, "predicted individual differences in the magnitude of psychophysiological reactivity to self-confrontation and the type of self-evaluations subjects made after such a confrontation" (p. 167). 20 In a similar conclusion to the Liebling and Shaver findings, Sackheim and Our have suggested that the anxiety engendered by self-confrontation at first facilitates task performance, but at "high levels" produces decrements (p. 169). An individual's tendency to exhibit trait and state-cognitive anxiety and worry to varying degrees certainly constitutes an example of individual differences which may prove to be important variables affecting individual reactions to a state of objective self-awareness. Cognitive anxiety usually involves worrisome, preoccupying thoughts or unpleasant feelings about self or external stimuli. "Worry," as described in the test anxiety literature, generally involves cognitive concern about the consequences of performance (Fox a Houston, 1983, p. 862). An individual possessing a high amount of cognitive anxiety and worry should, as a result of this predisposition toward self-concern, react differently from those lower in such tendencies when made self-aware. This study investigated the effects of the state of objective self-awareness or self-focus on the task perfbrmance of subjects high in cognitive anxiety. Because the cognitively anxious tend to experience more preoccupying thoughts or unpleasant feelings about self or external stimuli, it was hypothesized that the state of high objective self-awareness will interact with and exacerbate this tendency, lead to an increasing amount of distraction away from the task, and thus result in a larger decrement in performance when compared to the low cognitively anxious. As previously mentioned, objective self-awareness, especially for individuals prone to cognitive anxiety and worry, may also be 21 considered a self-confrontation situation. The clinical importance of individual differences in reaction to such a state rests with the fact that objective self—awareness/self-confrontation may be produced by a variety of situations. In addition to mirrors, cameras, videotape equipment, or a tape recorder (the last two often used in certain clinical settings) may have the same effects. As mentioned, a situation in which individuals feel that they are being significantly evaluated will also lead to a state of objective self-awareness. The therapy experience, in general, may be viewed in this way by certain clients. Certain techniques within the therapy process, such as role-play, or merely the disclosure of difficult and painful material, may also produce an intensely heightened state of self-focus. Predominant mode of experiencing anxiety is but one of countless individual differences which may affect an individual's reactions to objective self-awareness/self-confrontation. Once again it is vital that clinicians be aware of these possible differences in situation perception and situational changes in attitudes toward self and others' perceptions of self as a result of therapeutic techniques or the clinical setting as a whole. Structure of the Study The diagram on page 35 describes the administration of the measures in this study. Measures of state-cognitive somatic anxiety and worry- emotionality were completed prior to the introduction of the copying task. It was predicted that those subjects high in trait- 22 cognitive anxiety should not only score more highly in such state anxiety, but also reflect more situational worry, basically a cognitive concern. While it was possible that subjects in the extreme high range of both cognitive anxiety and worry would also evidence high scores in somatic anxiety and emotionality, it was felt that any decrement in performance would result chiefly from cognitively-based anxiety. This interpretation has been covered particularly well in the theoretical aspects of the worry-emotionality and direction of attention hypothesis literature (refer to those sections of the Introduction). Cognitive-somatic and worry-emotionality measures have occasionally been used within the same study (Kirkland & Hollandsworth, 1980), with both cognitive anxiety and worry scales utilized as "cognitive attentional measures." However, it must again be stressed that cognitive anxiety, as measured by the CSAQ, and "worry," as described in the Revised Questionnaire," while similar in their cognitive orientation, are not to be considered the "same" measure. Another purpose of this study was the statistical investigation of the relationship between these two measures. It initially appeared that cognitive anxiety was a more general concept involving a wide variety of different cognitive intrusions, while "worry" appeared to be a more specific concern regarding the anticipated negative consequences of an evaluative or perceived evaluative situation. A second administration of the State-CSAQ and Worry- Emotionality Questionnaire followed the (post-task) Cognitive Interference Questionnaire. As reflections of situational anxiety, 23 it was predicted that for subjects high in trait-cognitive anxiety, scores of state-cagnitive anxiety and worry would increase by a greater amount pre- to post-task than scores of those low in such trait anxiety. As noted on page 14, it has been demonstrated that worry appears to be a more constant, stable tendency throughout the period of evaluation/performance, while emotionality peaks at the beginning of this period, then generally declines. Therefore, those subjects evidencing a high worry score (those subjects also predicted to score "high" in trait, then in state-cognitive anxiety prior to the introduction of the task) should have retained this cognitive state through to the administration of the final (Post-Task) measure. Investigation of any similar pattern for state-cognitive anxiety became an additional hypothesis. Because the use of direct questions, themselves, would tend to produce a state of objective self-awareness, inquiries concerning subject awareness of distraction during the task (specifically the notice of the mirror and perception as a distraction) could not be used as validation of the effectiveness of this manipulation. Yet it was felt that such information on an informal basis would add to further understanding of subject experience. It was predicted that those high in trait and state-cognitive anxiety would report more environmental distraction while performing the task. Specifically, those subjects high in trait-cognitive anxiety and those in the mirror condition would more often acknowledge notice of the mirror and perceive it as a distraction during the c0pying task. 24 A final consideration in structuring this study involved the choice of rationale and the relationship between experimenter and subject during the experimental phase of the study (copying task). It was noted that evaluation apprehension by itself would increase objective self-awareness. An individual concerned about whether his or her performance would be judged favorably would also be aware of possible discrepancies between his or her present state and the goal desired (i.e., performing well). Therefore, the rationale given to subjects stressed the cooperative nature of the study and no indications were given in the instructions that subject performance was to be evaluated (refer to Rationales and Instructions in the Appendices). In addition, the subject-experimenter relationship generally involves experimenter control of the subject, behavior implying that the subject is an object in the world (Duval & Wicklund, 1973). This would provide a strong stimulus for heightened objective self-awareness. Experimenter presence or communication that the subject will be otherwise monitored may also be an additional such stimulus (p. 30). It was therefore necessary to attempt to diminish nonmanipulated sources of objective self-awareness, leaving the mirror as much as possible its major source. For this reason, the experimenter remained outside of the room and with the exception of the copying task, itself, the subjects were able to work at their own speed. Finally, no direct mention was made to the subjects of the mirror or its plywood back when turned to face away from the 25 subject. In this way, it was hoped that subjects' attention would not be drawn to the mirror by direction of the experimenter. Instead, the subject would notice her reflection (or the plywood back) in a natural fashion as she sat down at the desk. Hypotheses In summary form, the following are the hypotheses investigated in this study. To aid in the understanding of the hypotheses, the diagrams of the measures used and of the four groups of subjects are presented here. Cognitive Anxiety (trait-CSAQ) High Low . "high" = upper ;::}:::1X$ Yes high/ low/ half of subject the mirror mirror mirror distribution (i.e., high or high/no low/no " u = low objective No reflective reflective hag¥'of lfigjgct self-awareness mirror mirror distribution produced) Copying Screening phase Pre-task task Post-task Measures: CSAQ-trait —————+CSAQ-state-———+(performance ——+cognitive worry- measure) interference emotionality questionnaire, mood check CSAQ-state (emotion wheel) worry- emotionality, Post-task questionnaire Note. Data was analyzed in terms of mean scores for each group. 26 Hypotheses: 1. Those "high" in trait-cognitive anxiety (scoring in the upper half of the trait-CSAQ distribution)* will also score higher in (a) statgycognitive anxiety and higher in (b) "worry” than those "low" (lower half of distribution) in trait-cognitive anxiety. 2. Those "high" in trait-cognitive anxiety will evidence greater performance decrement than those "low" (scoring in the lower half) in cognitive anxiety. Note: performance on the copying task will be analyzed by taking the mean number of letters c0pied correctly for each group. 3. Those “high" in trait-cognitive anxiety, performing the task in a condition of high objective self-awareness (i.e., before the reflective surface of the mirror) will experience the greatest performance decrement of the four groups. Using the results of the post-task measures: 4. Those "high" in trait-cognitive anxiety will evidence higher (a) state-cognitive anxiety and higher (b) worry scores in both pre- and post-task measurements than those ”low" in trait- cognitive anxiety. 5. Those in the high trait-cognitive anxiety/"high“ objective self-awareness group will evidence the highest (a) state-CSAQ and (b) worry scores of all four groups after completing the c0pying task and (c) state-CSAQ and worry scores will increase pre- to post-task by a greater amount than other groups in terms of mean increase. *Mean trait-cognitive anxiety scores and trait-CSAQ ranges for "high" and "low" groups appear in the Appendix for the 2 x 2 design. 27 6. Those "high" in trait-cognitive anxiety (and state-cognitive anxiety and worry) will evidence a higher amount of cognitive interference during the experiment than those "low" in trait- and state-cognitive anxiety, and 7. Those in the "high" cognitive anxiety/"high" objective self-awareness group will evidence the highest cognitive interference scores of all groups. 8. Those "high" in trait-cognitive anxiety (and state-cognitive anxiety and worry) will (a) report noticing the presence of the mirror (both reflective surface and nonreflective back) more frequently and (b) more often claim that it was a distraction than those "low“ in trait-cognitive anxiety, ghg_ 9. Those in the "high" trait-cognitive anxiety/"high" objective self-awareness group will report (a) noticing the presence of the mirror and (b) seeing it as a distraction more often than any other group. METHOD Subjects One hundred female undergraduates at a large midwestern university were given the trait version of the Cognitive Somatic Anxiety Questionnaire (Schwartz, Davidson, & Goleman, 1978) and at a later date participated in the experimental phase (copying task) of the study. All subjects were currently enrolled in one of six different courses (introductory psychology, personality psychology, and abnormal psychology). Students in the introductory classes participated in this study for extra credit. Students from personality and abnormal psychology classes were not part of the official subject pool and received no extra credit. The number of subjects from each class was as follows: Introductory Psychology I (Section 1) = N = 56 Introductory Psychology I (Section 2) = N = 3 Introductory Psychology II (Section 1) = N = 4* Introductory Psychology II (Section 2) = N = 1 Personality Psychology = N = 25 Abnormal Psychology = N = 11* Following administration of the trait-CSAQ, all subjects were randomly assigned a number l-lOO. Subjects receiving an odd number (fl_= 50) performed the experimental task before the plywood back of *One case excluded for data analysis. 28 29 the mirror. Subjects receiving an even number (g_= 50) were assigned to the mirror-present condition. Following the completion of data collection, subjects' cognitive anxiety scores from the trait-CSAQ were tabulated, a median split conducted, and subjects thus assigned to "high" and "low“ trait-cognitive anxiety groups. Materials The experimental (copying) task was performed in front of a large mirror (35%" x 47%"). One side was the usual reflecting surface and the other side a nonreflecting, plywood back. The experiment was conducted in a small windowless room (8'6" x 12'). Other objects in the room included a desk on which students worked, a mirror prepped on top of the desk against the wall at an angle of approximately 70°, a straight-back chair, and a filing cabinet. Paper and pencils for the subjects' participation had been placed on the desk top before the subject entered the room. Instruments The Cognitive Somatic Anxiety Questionnaire (Schwartz, Davidson, & Goleman, 1978) was administered in its trait form for the purpose of dividing the subject population into groups of "high" and "low" trait-cognitive anxiety. Both cognitive and somatic scores were recorded for each subject, but only the cognitive score was utilized for subject division and for analyses to address the specified hypotheses. A State version of the Cognitive Somatic Anxiety Questionnaire (Heide, 1981) was administered to all subjects both prior to and following the completion of the capying task. The purpose of this 30 instrument was to assess the level of cognitive and somatic anxiety currently being experienced. Once again, only cognitive anxiety scores were used in the statistical analysis. The State-CSAQ was originally developed as part of a study concerning relaxation-induced anxiety. Therefore, a number of the items were worded to assess the amount of relaxation experienced. For purposes of this study, all such items worded to reflect amount of current relaxation were scored in reverse. For example, if a subject gave a rating of "2" for the statement: my mind feels calm, a "6" was recorded as the amount of anxiety being experienced. In this way, scoring was consistent with the statements designed to measure state anxiety on a 1 (NOT AT ALL) through 7 (VERY MUCH SO) scale. The Worry-Emotionality Questionnaire (Morris, Davis, & Hutchings, 1981) was also administered before and after the experimental task for the purpose of ascertaining the degree of worry currently being experienced. Emotionality scores were recorded, but not used in data analysis. Following the pre-task administration of the State-CSAQ and Worry-Emotionality Questionnaires, subjects were asked to indicate by setting a pointer on an "emotion wheel" what they felt to be their predominant mood at that moment. The instrument used was a modification of the "emotion wheel" developed by Plutchik (1980). For this study, the emotions included on the "wheel" were confined to Plutchik's "eight primary emotions" (a diagram of the "wheel" used appears in the Appendices). In addition to the presence of anxiety affecting performance on a copying task, depression may also produce differences in 31 outcome. The copying task in this study was felt to be similar to the Digit Symbol subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS). Both tasks ask individuals to copy as much material (letters in this study or symbols in the Digit Symbol subtest) as they can in a given amount of time. As in the Digit Symbol test, this copying task primarily measured a subject's capacity to utilize energy in a simple task. A high level of anxiety might cause difficulties in concentration and the ability to focus energy on the task, alone. Poor performance on such a task may also reflect a depressive lack of energy output (Blatt & Allison, 1981). The purpose of using the “emotion wheel" in this study was to provide some exploratory information concerning any possible depressive affect ("sadness" on the Plutchik "wheel") among individual subjects. It was felt that this and other information regarding subjects' affective states might contribute valuable information for the overall analysis and interpretation of study data. In addition, studies by Scheier and Carver (1977) and Scheier, Carver, and Gibbons (1981) both indicate that self-focused attention heightens an individual's awareness of and responsivity to affect. All subjects were asked to copy a sample of Swedish prose for a period of five minutes. Performance scores were based on the number of letters, marks of accent, and punctuation copied correctly during that time period. Following the copying task, subjects were asked to complete the Cognitive Interference Questionnaire (Sarason & Stoops, 1978) 32 to assess the types and frequencies of thoughts which they may have experienced while working on the task. The use of the Cognitive Interference Questionnaire (CIQ) was reported in a study with female university undergraduates. Data indicated that highly anxious persons under stress experience cognitive interference and preoccupation which result in poor performance (Sarason & Stoops, 1978). The CIQ, which results in a single, global score, also requests a self-report of the degree to which subjects' minds "wandered" during the task. The final measure presented to the subjects was a short questionnaire (Post-Task Questionnaire) designed to investigate especially the degree to which the presence of the mirror or plywood backing was recognized and then perceived as a distraction by subjects in their appropriate groups. The "emotion wheel," Cognitive Interference Questionnaire, and Post-Task Questionnaire were presented at the same points during the study for all subjects. The State-CSAQ and the Worry-Emotionality Questionnaire were presented in counterbalanced order. Procedure A procedural diagram of the study appears in Figure 1. The screening and experimental phases of this study were conducted by a total of five persons, this writer and four female undergraduates (three psychology majors, one speech and audiology major) who had undergone a training period of approximately five weeks to become familiar with the instruments and with the experimental procedure. In this way, it was possible to include 33 more subjects in the study, as well as to deal with the issue of experimenter bias. During that time, they had been familiarized with the experimental procedure and had the opportunity to work with pilot subjects. Although these four undergraduates had knowledge of the general topics involved in this study and to which experimental condition each subject had been assigned, they remained unaware of the subject's trait-cognitive anxiety grouping (as did this writer at that time) and of the specific hypotheses to be tested. Volunteers for this study were solicited from the six classes previously Specified. Those who wished to participate for credit (from the four sections of the two introductory classes) signed up for the initially scheduled screening sessions, at which time they were administered the trait-CSAQ. These subjects had been informed that the screening session would be the first part of a two-phase study. They received credit for participation in each phase. After arriving at the screening session, subjects were given the following rationale: We are conducting a pilot study for a project in which subjects will copy written material of graded difficulty. Before we begin the study, we need to know just how similar subjects will be on certain variables. Therefore, we are asking you to take the next few minutes to complete first the consent form, then the questionnaire being distributed. Subjects were told that the times listed on the consent form (refer to Appendix) for each phase of the study might vary, but that they would receive appropriate credit for all participation. 34 It was also mentioned verbally (as well as included on the consent form) that subjects' signatures did not commit them to participation in the second phase of the study, should they be recontacted, although it was hoped that they would choose to continue. Only six of the originally-screened subjects later decided not to participate in the copying task (second) phase. Following completion of the trait-CSAQ, subjects were told that they might possibly be recontacted within the next week to arrange a time to participate in the next phase of the study. Subjects who had signed up for a specified screening time, but missed their session, were rescheduled. Screening times had been scheduled fOr only one week due to time pressures. If rescheduling was not possible during that week, subjects were given a copy of the consent form and trait-CSAQ to complete prior to returning for the experimental phase. By the end of the scheduled week of formal screening, it had become apparent that there was a severe lack of available subjects from the introductory (credit) classes. To achieve the power desired for statistical analysis, a sample of at least 100 subjects was strongly preferred. For this reason, it was necessary to look beyond introductory psychology (subject pool) courses and use volunteers from other psychology classes. This same procedure of having subjects complete the consent form and CSAQ outside of a formal screening session was also followed for subjects from the two classes not participating for credit. In all cases, however, subjects were given the same rationale and basic instructions. 35 .mmmsa Faucwswcmgxm zmaoesu cowuumpmm uumnnam .xuaum eo Eagmmvu ._~ag: co.uoem. .9 ogvoccovumoao Aup—ocovuOEuuxggoz An o.o uuonaam 'F’ .sgou __ou any: ogu so to: a_gu u».u.oc. upaogn osn .xaou - 03.. v.393 m a” .uuuopnsou no: xusum as» cos: uuagumna as» we snag a o>_ouos ago.e Ann .pov «an uuza upon cups m "vogue—Lum.u m:.eo.eaau Lou‘s can .ppomu. .ucoe.soaxo 0:» co scans—ucou ecu an econ «co.uuoac gas a» oucagu oz» co>_o no: ecu a m gu-o ou econ no: oc,~o.gnou cuuu.ez a - Avovgon covuuo—_ou 3.6 L3 .53 05 .0 any ogu a. ou-pn can“ v.39: oc.co.toou .c:. on a» ._.u. may: momuapu «so» use c. an Acne unauuoa yoga v—ou m Nor .eoog ecu vogouco u a goon cove: u.gu none... m .uouu.a5ou oe.ac:o_uuuao umo— coco nos—acco.uuo:o gua».umoa Au oc'occo.»mo:a oucucueeoucu o>.u_coou u mu—paco.uo2u-»geo: Mn o.o oc- .Avooxx—a so Long's. co.u¢vcou .oucaevgoaxo m=.ucoauosgou - .oo.-. Cones: - taco.uma uuonaaa guau .voao—o>ov opauogou co.uuo——ou .u-o H? 0 Oil .oouu-ucouos on on mononaan ppo uu¢—an :~.vou gu.x =o.».—:aoa guanaau union—>.u as. «gauges—o» pp. on: a» you: co.a.uoo N.) m .Axvaum uzu ouuposou o» no on xogu &. ”a_oocu ago: ~ Lo p m>vuuoe azo_e xogu “any v.0“ as. om.ga m.gu so» u.uocu Lao; m _ u~>vuuuc momma—u secuuavoeucv omega c. mucouauuv gmpcoo to .co.o~3coz .gm.oozm gu.x e....e.. pp. use uc.cuoeum "steam yo «was; vacuum cu ao.ga oo.nuao Lo aco.uuon oc.cooau« pasta. c. togu.o o.u nopa’naon no «onus—u —o>o_.goao: ecu Acouuauoeuc. acne no c. 803:3? 22.3 33263. . .mm __o to; Lento voucopoaeoucaou :. vocouu*c_6vo on ——.x og.occo.auo:o xuvpocovuoEu-xggoz uco o .10 nor the comparison of high vs. low groups (1 and 2 vs. 3 and 4) t(94) = .0541, p > .90 were significant. 44 Table 4 Means and Standard Deviations of Copying Task (Performance) Scores Objective Hi9Emit-Cognitive AnxietyLow Self-Awareness M SD M SD Mirror 470.72 74.87 466.36 52.58 N=28 N=22 Plywood Back 430.85 90.15 433.54 79.32 N=20 N=28 3) As evidenced in Table 4, those in the high cognitive anxiety/mirror group (group 1) had the highest mean performance score of all groups. Hypothesis 3 had predicted that this group would show the greatest perfbrmance decrement of the fbur groups. However, comparison of this group vs. the other three groups (1 vs. 2, 3, 4) did not yield significant results for this highest mean score: t(94) = 1.605, p > .10. 4) Hypothesis 4(a), those high in trait-cognitive anxiety, will evidence higher state-cognitive anxiety scores post-task (as well as pre-task) was supported by the data. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 5. The higher mean scores for the high trait-cognitive anxiety group proved significant in the following comparison: t(94) = 3.997, p < .01. Hypothesis 4(b) predicted that subjects high in trait- cognitive anxiety would score higher in worry post-task (as well as pre-task) than those low in trait-cognitive anxiety. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 6. Although mean scores 45 Table 5 Means and Standard Deviations of Post-Task State-Cognitive Anxiety Scores . . Trait-Cognitive Anxiety Objective High Low Self-Awareness M’ SD M SD Mirror 19.36 8.05 14.91 5.37 N=28 N=22 Plywood Back 19.35 6.40 13.25 5.41 N=20 N=28 Table 6 Means and Standard Deviations of Post-Task Worry Scores Trait-Cognitive Anxiety Objective . Htgh *Eow Self-Awareness 11 SD M* SD Mirror 10.04 4.41 7.05 1.46 N=28 N=22 Plywood Back 10.10 4.56 8.86 2.98 N=20 N=28 were higher for the predicted group and data from the comparison of high vs. low groups (groups 1 and 2 vs. 3 and 4) yielded significant results (t(94) = 2.894, p < .01), once again tests for homogeneity of variances did not support the homogeneity of variance assumption. Therefore results for this hypothesis must be considered less conclusive. 46 5) The fifth hypothesis consisted of three parts. The first, that subjects in the high trait-cognitive anxiety/mirror group (group 1) would evidence the highest mean state-cognitive anxiety scores (post-task), was only partially supported by the data. Table 5 indicates that this group mean was the highest. Despite the significance of the comparison result (t(94) = 2.431, p < .05), a one-way analysis of variance of the other three groups (groups 2, 3, 4) also revealed differences among these groups significant at p < .05. Thus it is not possible to make a definitive statement concerning the difference between the first group vs. the other three (1 vs. 2, 3, 4). The second part of Hypothesis 5 predicted that the high trait- cognitive anxiety/mirror group would also evidence the highest worry scores (post-task) of all groups. Both the data presented in Table 6 and the results of a group 1 vs. 2, 3, 4 comparison (t(94) = 1.704, p > .10) do not support this hypothesis. The final part of Hypothesis 5, that the mean change (predicted increase), pre- to post-task, in both state-cognitive anxiety and worry, will be greatest for the high trait-cognitive anxiety/mirror group (group 1) was not supported by the data. 6) It was predicted in Hypothesis 6 that those high in trait- cognitive anxiety will experience a greater amount of cognitive interference during the performance task than those low in such anxiety. Table 7 presents the means and standard deviations for this data. Comparisons of high vs. low groups (groups 1 and 2 vs. 3 and 4) yielded a significant result of t(94) = 3.350, p < .01. 47 Table 7 Means and Standard Deviations of Cognitive Interference Scores Trait-Cognitive Anxiety Objective . _ High Low Self Awareness M SD M SD Mirror 22.61 5.53 19.77 4.26 N=28 N=22 Plywood Back 25.70 7.15 20.68 5.83 N=20 N=28 7) It was further predicted that subjects in the high trait- cognitive anxiety/mirror group (group 1) would experience the greatest amount of cognitive interference. In the 1 vs. 2, 3, 4 group comparison, this hypothesis was not supported. 8) The first part of Hypothesis 8 refers to statement #1 on the Post-Task Questionnaire (for both mirror and plywood conditions). It was predicted that subjects high in trait-cognitive anxiety would report noticing the presence of the mirror/plywood back more frequently than those low in trait-cognitive anxiety. Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 8. A comparison of high vs. low groups (groups 1 and 2 vs. 3 and 4) did not support this prediction: t(94) = .713, p > .40. It was further predicted that those in the high trait-cognitive anxiety group would more often perceive the mirror/plywood back as a distraction during the experiment (this hypothesis refers to statement #2 on the Post-Task Questionnaire). Means and standard deviations are presented in Table 9. Once again comparison of high 48 Table 8 Means and Standard Deviations of Mirror/Plywood Presence Scores Trait-ngfiitive Anxiety Objective . _ Htgh Low Self Awareness M SD M SD Mirror 3.39 .92 2.29 1.19 N=28 N=22 Plywood Back 1.95 1.19 2.10 1.20 N=20 N=28 Table 9 Means and Standard Deviations of Mirror/Plywood Distraction Scores Objective H‘iT’r‘ait-Cognitive Anxietlyow Self-Awareness *M 449—9 SD M SD Mirror 1.36 .68 1.23 .53 N=28 N=22 Plywood Back 1.15 .67 1.14 .45 N=20 N=28 vs. low groups (groups 1 and 2 vs. 3 and 4) did not yield significant results for this prediction: t(94) = .572, p > .50. 49 9) Subjects in the high trait-cognitive anxiety/mirror group (group 1) were expected to report noticing the presence of the mirror more frequently than the other groups (groups 2, 3, 4). Results of t(94) = 4.259, p < .01 were significant for this groups 1 vs. 2, 3, 4 comparison. However, a one-way analysis of variance of groups 2, 3, 4 indicated significant differences between those groups as well: F(2, 67) = 4.085, p < .05. Thus it is difficult to make a more definitive statement concerning the specified hypothesis. In the second part of Hypothesis 9, that this same group (high trait-cognitive anxiety/mirror) would more often perceive the mirror to be a distraction, results were not significant: t(94) = 1.40, p > .10. This hypothesis was therefore not supported by the data. The final statistical analysis specified in the pr0posa1 for this study was a comparison of the Worry-Emotionality Questionnaire and the state version of the CSAQ. In a sense, this was to be an exploratory analysis to investigate the similarity of the constructs involved, both believed to relate to types of state "anxiety." Two items, one "worry" and one "emotionality" (as suggested by the authors of the measure) were added to the Worry—Emotionality Questionnaire to bring the total number of items to 12. The addition of the two items was for the purpose of increasing reliability in the analysis. The two measures (pre- and postatask) were compared using a Pearson correlation (one-tailed) corrected for attenuation with the following formula: SO c xy where: rxy = Pearson correlation rxx J ryy rxx = reliability of x ryy = reliability of y Results were as follows: 1) Worry-Emotionality Questionnaire and state CSAQ (pre-task) = rc = .615. 2) Worry-Emotionality and state CSAQ (post-task) = rc = .647. Only about 35% of the variance is accounted for in both cases, suggesting that there is substantial difference between the two measures. A table of Pearson correlations for measures (with the exception of Cognitive Interference Questionnaire and Post-Task Questionnaire) used in this study are presented in the Appendix. Post Hoc Analysis In addition to the analysis performed to address each stated hypothesis using a 2 x 2 factorial design, a previously unstated exploratory analysis was conducted with a division of the subject sample into three trait-cognitive anxiety groups (thirds of the subject distribution): high, medium, and low. This was an attempt to conduct an analysis of performance task results using more Trait-Cognitive Anxiety High Medium Low Objective Mirror 1 2 3 Self- Awareness Plywood , Back 4 5 6 51 homogeneous groups. Means and standard deviations for trait- cognitive anxiety scores are presented in the Appendix. Table 10 Means and Standard Deviations of Copying Task (Performance) Scores Using Post Hoc Sample Distribution Trait-Cognitive Anxiety Objective High MediUm *Low Self-Awareness M SD *TM 50 M SD Mirror 485.37 80.70 449.91 46.65 477.00 62.79 N=l9 N=21 N=1O Plywood Back 439.07 85.21 416.27 96.54 436.09 77.75 N=14 N=1l N=23 Means and standard deviations for the perfbrmance task scores are presented in Table 10. Although the omnibus F was still not significant (F(5, 92) = 1.766, p > .10), results of a priori contrasts began to reveal some patterns in this data. Significant results were obtained for the following comparisons: 1) mirror vs. plywood groups (1, 2, 3 vs. 4, 5, 6): p < .05. 2) mirror vs. plywood in the high and low anxiety groups (1 and 3 vs. 4 and 6): p < .05. 3) high anxiety/mirror group vs. other groups (1 vs. 2-6): p < .05. When doing similar contrasts using the 2 x 2 design, only the mirror vs. plywood groups comparison resulted in a finding of significance (p < .05). 52 In addition, a non-monotonic U-shaped pattern was noted - higher mean scores in high and low groups, with the lowest score in the middle or medium group. This pattern was evident in bgth_ experimental conditions, a situation considered not as likely to have occurred by chance. In summary, although the presence of a nonsignificant omnibus F suggests that any experimental effects may be weak, the combination of several significant group comparisons, as well as the presence of the U-shaped pattern in both experimental conditions, is worthy of note in this study. Two other exploratory analyses were conducted using the original 2 x 2 design. In the first, an analysis of variance was conducted for pre-task state-cognitive anxiety and worry by class. Because subjects were drawn from both credit and noncredit classes and because there was reason to believe that one instructor's comments may have affected subject perceptions of their participation, it was believed that it would be important to conduct such analysis. The data revealed no significant differences between classes on either variable. A second post hoc analysis was performed to explore the composition of the subject sample for the 2 x 2 design. As noted in Table 1, the omnibus F for trait-cognitive anxiety was significant at p < .01. A comparison of high vs. low groups specifically (groups 1 and 2 vs. 3 and 4) also evidenced significant difference: t(94) = 13.01, p < .01. However, the homogeneity of variance assumption was not met, introducing a note of caution in the interpretation of these results. These same trends were also found in the tests for the post hoc 3 x 2 design. DISCUSSION The focus of this study was the differential performance on a copying task of individuals scoring in a "high" or "low" range of trait-cognitive anxiety. It was further predicted that individuals "high" in trait anxiety would score higher in state-cognitive anxiety and in worry. It was assumed that a high level of cognitive anxiety would distract a subject from concentration on the task. Further, a high level of cognitive anxiety and worry should have interacted with a high level of objective self-awareness (the mirror condition) to produce even greater performance decrement. As seen in Hypotheses 2 and 3, the predicted performance decrement for those high in cognitive anxiety was not supported by the data. In particular, the group of most interest in this study, the high cognitively anxious performing the task in front of the mirror (high objective self-awareness), not only did not suffer the greatest perfbrmance decrement, but actually achieved the highest mean performance source of the four groups. Although a comparison of high vs. low groups did not achieve statistical significance, a comparison of mirror vs. plywood (low objective self-awareness) groups was statistically significant at p <:.05. As shown in Table 4, the mean performance scores of both mirror groups were higher than those of both plywood groups. 53 54 Several issues are involved in the interpretation of these results. It must be noted that the composition of the subject sample did not provide particularly homogeneous groupings nor as much statistical power for investigation of treatment effects as would have an upper-lower quartile selection. In this study, all individuals volunteering for the project were included as subjects and a median split divided high and low groups. The Appendix table showing trait-cognitive anxiety mean scores for the 2 x 2 design notes the range of trait-cognitive anxiety scores for each group. There is no numerical separation between the lowest score of the "high" group and the highest score of the "low" group. Future research attempting to divide a subject population on the basis of trait-CSAQ scores should plan for the screening of as large a group of potential subjects as possible. In this way, a division into upper and lower quartiles of the distribution (and possibly of the CSAQ cognitive scale, as well) would be a more likely occurrence. In addition, and what is probably more important for interpretation of the findings, only four of the 100 subjects scored in the 29-35 range of the trait-CSAQ cognitive scale (this roughly corresponds to the upper quartile of the scale). Only ten subjects scored in the 25-35 point range. The implications of such numbers in terms of the absolute level of anxiety as measured by this scale may be crucial in attempting to explain subject performance. It appears that most subjects simply did not admit to a tendency to experience a great deal of cognitive anxiety. Whether this result reflected a genuine perception of lower anxiety 55 or a desire to appear less anxious to others cannot be interpreted from this data. What level of cognitive anxiety is necessary for certain interactions to occur and performance decrement to be evidenced? This study predicted an interaction in some fashion between high cognitive anxiety (trait and state) and a high level of objective self-awareness to produce conditions leading to a worsening of performance on the task. The tendency to experience and the experiencing of a high number of cognitive intrusions (acting as distraction of attention) should have been exacerbated by a heightening of self-awareness. It is possible that those subjects in the high cognitive anxiety group as defined by this distribution median split were simply "not anxious enough." That is, a higher level of anxiety would have been necessary to lead to the sequence of reactions predicted. The results of this study appear similar to those of Wicklund and Duval (1971), in which the presence of a mirror increased the number of words of German prose copied in a specified period of time. Leibling and Shaver (1973) obtained the same results for the copying of Swedish prose when subjects were performing in a low evaluation situation. In both of these studies, then, the presence of the mirror facilitated performance on simple tasks when evaluation apprehension was low. Leibling and Shaver (1973) suggested a motivational explanation for this improvement in performance. If an attentional decrement (discussed by Wine, 1971) is compensated for by an arousal increment, 56 the result may be a situation in which an individual is simply "'trying harder'" (Wicklund and Duval, 1971, p. 304). In the present study, cognitive anxiety did not seem debilitatingly high; yet for the "high" group, enough of such anxiety may have been present for subjects to use the cues of that anxiety (and possibly of worry, as well) as a motivation to "try harder." Cognitive interference was another variable investigated for contribution to performance decrement. Comparison of high vs. low groups for cognitive interference did reveal a statistically significant difference between these groups. Yet as in the case of trait-and state-cognitive anxiety, the level of interference (of task-irrelevant thoughts) may not have been great enough to result in more performance decrement. Nor did the presence of the mirror seem to appreciably heighten such interference. In addition, the fact that the high anxiety/mirror group reported noticing the mirror but did not perceive it as a distraction from the task lends support to the hypothesis that absolute anxiety was not ”high enough." Although the high trait-cognitive anxiety groups did evidence greater worry scores (both pre- and post-task), the high cognitive anxiety/mirror group (group 1) did not score higher than the other three groups (pre- and post-task). [Post hoc investigation of the pre-task results for worry (group 1 vs. 2, 3, 4) did not yield statistically significant findings, p > .13.] This result suggests that the predicted interaction between high trait-cognitive anxiety and a high level of objective self-awareness leading to higher levels of worry, as well as of performance decrement, was not present. 57 As has been stated, in the present study every attempt was made to reduce evaluation apprehension. Thus, this study came to resemble both that of Wicklund and Duval (1971) and the low-evaluation condition of Leibling and Shaver (1973). Unfortunately this resemblance was unintentional. While efforts were made to keep externally-produced evaluation apprehension at a minimum, it was felt that high levels of cognitive anxiety and worry would introduce an internal sense of apprehension. This should have created concern for achieving a certain level of performance and anxiety regarding a possible negative outcome. This writer believes that individuals who experience high degrees of cognitive anxiety and worry will interpret many different situations as evaluative, including participation in an experiment and many sorts of performance tasks. The concept of worry is a factor which may have an even more direct relationship to evaluation apprehension, per se, As noted in the Results section, the statistically significant data fbr pre- and post-task worry scores must be considered less conclusive. However, they still suggest the possibility that higher levels of trait-and state-cognitive anxiety will also imply higher levels of worry. As a construct, worry, with its emphasis of performance concerns and apprehension about negative consequences, thus seems to tap into the concept of evaluation apprehension. As mentioned in the Results section, worry-emotionality appears to an extent to be a different type of dimension than cognitive-somatic anxiety. Worry may actually be a special type of both evaluation anxiety (Wine, 1982) and in a broader sense, of cognitive anxiety. 58 Although the results of this study suggest that individuals experiencing higher levels of cognitive anxiety also experienced similar levels of worry, this may not always be the case. Cognitive anxiety deals with a number of types of cognitive intrusion. For some individuals, focus on anxiety in the form of cognitive intrusion may not be specifically related to concern about negative consequences or evaluation issues. Thus if the concept of evaluation apprehension may be closer in intent to worry than to cognitive anxiety, a similar study using worry as a focal construct might be worthwhile. Another possibility for future study would be the deliberate introduction of different levels of evaluation apprehension within the cognitive anxiety-objective self-awareness paradigm. It is possible, as with the Leibling and Shaver (1973) and Paulus, Annis, and Risner (1978) studies, that evaluation apprehension as introduced external to the individual is a crucial mediating variable. A related question is whether all evaluation is seen as aversive. To what extent do individual differences in internal tendencies toward evaluation anxiety and perceptions of external situations as evaluative affect response to manipulation of high-low evaluation conditions in this type of study? Although this study predicted an interaction between cognitive anxiety (and probably worry) and a state of high objective self-awareness to produce performance decrement, it is possible that other variables such as current life situation and mood state, not considered in this work or particularly emphasized, might be vital ingredients in such a response. This study was conducted during a four-week period in which most subjects had at least one set of midterm exams 59 scheduled. Such factors might have a certain influence on sensitivity to performance issues-either a temporary increase in apprehension or a sense of relief concerning the ending of the exam period and a possible increase in self-confidence if the individual did well. The second variable mentioned, mood state, was the focus of an exploratory aspect of the study. Interest was chiefly in terms of a screening of some sort for depression or depression-like symptoms ("sadness" on the Emotion Wheel) which might provide an alternative explanation for performance decrement. Over 90 of the 100 subjects reported their mood prior to the copying task as being either "acceptance" or "anticipation." No further analysis was conducted. No attempt had been made to obtain a rating of affect intensity or post-task mood. A more intensive investigation might be recommended for future research. According to Scheier and Carver (1977), self-focused attention heightens a person's awareness and responsivity to affect, regardless of the nature of the affect (positive or negative). Any effects on performance of mood/affect on this study are difficult to assess due in part to both a lack of intensity ratings and a lack of operational definitions of "acceptance" and "anticipation." Were these mood states positive or aversive and for which subjects? Just what is "acceptance," "anticipation"? Future work may be advised to investigate the use of a different instrument, along with ratings of intensity. Another variable which may affect copying task performance is subjects' differential ability to print letters rapidly. Many subjects 60 used script even though they were instructed to print. All data sets were considered valid as long as each letter was distinguishable for purposes of observing correctness of letter, accent, and punctuation. It is possible that some individuals did attempt to follow the instructions faithfully. If printing were for any reason Tawkward for some subjects or more preferable for others, the speed of copying the prose may have been affected for this reason alone. The problem of subject adherence to these particular instructions is difficult within this paradigm. To reduce self-awareness from sources other than the mirror and to reduce evaluation apprehension, the experimenter was outside the room and could thus not monitor subjects' behavior. And from a practical standpoint, it was simply not possible to exclude all data sets in which a subject had written rather than printed the Swedish prose. Nevertheless, this individual difference variable should be considered in any research involving a c0pying task. The results of the post hoc exploratory analysis of the performance data lend support to the importance of creating more homogeneous groupings. When the subject sample was divided into three groupings of trait-cognitive anxiety level (high, medium, and low), a greater number of the same a priori comparisons evidenced statistically significant results. The results of this exploratory analysis also reveal an interesting pattern which, while again not supporting the hypothesis of performance decrement, is nevertheless worth noting. A non-monotonic, U-shaped pattern was evidenced for both mirror and plywood experimental conditions. High and low groups achieved mean scores 61 in each case higher than the "Medium" group. And those higher scores were more so in the mirror than in the plywood condition. As noted in the Results section, comparisons of all mirror vs. plywood groups and high vs. low groups in the mirror condition proved statistically significant. Interpretation of higher performance scores in mirror as Opposed to plywood conditions has been suggested previously in this section. It is felt that such interpretations apply equally to this 3 x 2 design. Possible explanations for the U-shaped scoring pattern and a statistically significant difference between high and low groups in the mirror condition are more problematic and suggested interpretations not readily available in the self-awareness literature. It is possible that individuals in the low group, not experiencing much distraction, were able to focus well on the task and achieve a higher level of performance. In the high cognitive anxiety group, there may have been a sufficient amount of internal cues of anxiety to provide some additional motivation to "try harder," also resulting in higher performance scores. For this group, the condition of high objective self-awareness may have then interacted with the higher level of cognitive anxiety to provide stronger anxiety cues and an even greater instance of "trying harder." Thus, in the‘mirror condition, the higher cognitive anxiety group evidenced even higher scores than the low anxiety group. In the case of the medium group, there may'have been "just enough" anxiety experienced to not allow these individuals to be in the same internal circumstances as the low group, yet "not enough" anxiety 62 felt to provide that additional motivation. The condition of high objective self-awareness produced a higher performance mean in the mirror as opposed to plywood condition, yet scores in both conditions remained depressed below those of both high and low groups. Although such a non-monotonic pattern is not often seen in both experimental conditions by chance, it must be noted that the omnibus F for performance task for this design did not achieve statistical significance. Treatment effects, if existing, are most probably weak. The importance of this exploratory analysis may rest more in the changes evidenced when groups were made more homogeneous, introducing more statistical power into the analysis. Results of this study lend support to the conclusion that self-attention is not necessary phenomenologically aversive (Carver & Scheier, 1978). This work was an attempt to investigate one possible variable which might lead to an aversive reaction: the interaction of a high level of objective self-awareness with a high level of cognitive anxiety. It was hypothesized that such an interaction might produce an internal state very similar to that of evaluation apprehension. It was further predicted that such an aversive reaction would be evidenced in an increased number and/or intensity of cognitive intrusions, worry, and cognitive interference, leading to increased distraction of attention and performance decrement. In a clinical setting, all of these variables will be involved at various times: perceptions of possible evaluation by the therapist on the part of the client; increased self-awareness as the client is asked to become more introspective and possibly to focus on experiences of anxiety, fear, and other emotion; the use of audio 63 and video tapes, note-taking (the former often the case when the therapist is in training); and certainly high levels of cognitive anxiety and worry for many clients at some points at least. As this anxiety increases, what might happen to the client's ability to focus and concentrate on any point of thought and discussion? The performance task in this study was a simple one, but demanding of an individual's ability to concentrate and focus her energy on such a task for a given period of time. Client anxiety levels within a clinical setting will often be far higher than those of high group in this study. Would the level of anxiety in the clinical setting be the level "high enough? to trigger the mentioned interaction? In addition, the concept of "clinical" as opposed to "normal" anxiety may play a role in the clinical setting. Clients entering therapy with anxiety-related disorders or unusually high levels of anxiety (especially cognitive; often high levels of both somatic and cognitive) may experience such interaction on a continual basis during therapeutic intervention. In conclusion, while this study generally supported the prediction that a higher level of trait-cognitive anxiety would suggest higher levels of state-cognitive anxiety, worry, and cognitive interference during a simple performance task, the data failed to support the focal hypothesis: that these higher levels of anxiety and worry would interact with a high level of objective self-awareness to produce the greatest observable performance decrement among all groups. The most prominent explanation for the failure to observe statistically significant differences in task performance appears to 64 be the lack of a "high enough" level of cognitive anxiety among subjects to produce the predicted interaction. Also, as noted, the concept of evaluation apprehension is an important factor in performance decrement in other objective self-awareness research. It was predicted that the above mentioned interaction would produce an internal state similar to that of evaluation apprehension, providing a mechanism by which performance level should decrease. Yet because cognitive anxiety, consisting of a variety of cognitive intrusions, is a broad concept, individuals experiencing this type of anxiety may or may not experience it in more directly "evaluative" terms. Mood state is a third variable which may have implications for task performance. While an effort was made to assess each subject's pre-task mood, these "moods" (as listed on the Emotion Wheel) were not well-defined, nor was any attempt made to assess intensity of mood or post-task change. Any attempt to replicate this study should include the following changes: first, the screening of a large enough group of potential subjects to allow division of "high" and "low" trait-cognitive anxiety groups by quartiles (preferably including a high level of "absolute" anxiety on the CSAQ scale, itself). As noted in the post hoc exploratory analysis, more homogeneous groupings of subjects will create more statistical power for analysis. And by creating a more genuinely "high" anxiety group, the probability of the predicted interaction leading to performance task differences should increase. Second, a more thorough investigation of subject mood or affect would help to focus on alternative explanations for any observed 65 performance decrement. Clear, operationally-defined terms and the addition of affect intensity ratings and post-task assessment should be included. Third, an additional analysis might be considered, utilizing state-cognitive anxiety and worry scores. This suggestion follows the same reasoning as that of intensity of mood state. A subject's present mood and its intensity may have a more direct bearing upon performance than a more general tendency toward a given experience (although it would still be predicted that a high level of trait- cognitive anxiety would suggest a higher level of state-cognitive anxiety and worry, as well). Finally, a change in design of this study, allowing for inclusion of high and low levels of evaluation apprehension, might also help to more specifically focus on a source of performance difference. Evaluation levels could be produced by alteration of instructions to the subject, suggesting a much greater evaluative component to task performance. APPENDICES APPENDIX A ADDITIONAL TABLES APPENDIX A ADDITIONAL TABLES Reliabilities of Measures N=lOO N=98 Trait-CSAQ .807 .815 State-CSAQ (pre-task) .866 .865 Worry-Emotionality (pre-task) .887 .893 Cognitive Interference .779 .778 Questionnaire* State-CSAQ (post-task) .887 _ .887 Worry-Emotionality (post-task) .870 .873 Post-Task Questionnaire .416 .422 *With item #11 deleted. 66 67 Pearson Correlations of Selected Measures* Pre-task Post-task Trait-CSAQ State-CSAQ WE State-CSAQ WE Trait-CSAQ 1.000 .460 .443 .470 .391 State-CSAQ .459 1.000 .541 .764 .553 (pre-task) - Worry-Emotionality .443 .541 1.000 .339 .751 (pre-task) State-CSAQ .470 .764 .339 1.000 .572 (post-task) Worry-Emotionality .391 .553 .751 .572 1.000 (post-task) *N=98. 68 Means and Standard Deviations of Trait- Cognitive Anxiety Scores for Exploratory Analysis Trait-Cognitive Anxiety Objective . . High Medium Low Mirror 24.32 3.42 17.95 1.40 13.00 2.71 N=l9 N=21 N=lO Plywood Back 25.00 3.76 18.00 1.67 13.48 1.78 N=14 N=11 N=23 Scoring ranges were as follows: High anxiety between 21-35. Medium anxiety between 16-20. Low anxiety between 8-16. 69 Means and Standard Deviations of Trait- Cognitive Anxiety Scores for 2 x 2 Design Objective Hi9wait-Cognitive Anxietyw Self-Awareness M’ SD M SD Mirror 22.71 3.68 15.14 2.71 N=28 N=22 Plywood Back 23.30 4.42 14.00 1.98 N=20 N=28 Note. "High" anxiety scores ranged between 18-35. "Low" anxiety scores ranged between 8-18. 70 Performance Raw Scores by Group* Group 1 High Trait Cognitive Anxiety/Mirror N=28 311 617 400 419 542 550 492 510 458 483 384 510 530 426 483 371 578 427 361 607 432 493 489 415 510 492 392 498 Group 2 High Trait Cognitive Anxiety/Plywood Back N=22** 521 419 377 374 452 710** 238 383 501 431 540 517 366 404 703** 532 566 374 489 243 452 438 Group 3 Low Trait ngnitive Anxiety/Mirror N=22 497 534 489 438 422 470 472 532 335 468 413 601 507 443 522 475 476 391 406 446 454 414 Group 4 Low Trait Cognitive Anxiety/Plywood Back N=28 511 463 281 401 476 454 409 381 374 328 510 315 562 416 334 650 417 492 458 485 454 510 358 374 469 399 431 427 *2 x 2 design. **Includes "outlyers" indicated by**. APPENDIX B INSTRUMENTS APPENDIX B INSTRUMENTS (attached to CSAQ) If you have a reading, speaking, and/or writing knowledge of Swedish, Norwegian, cn~ Danish, do not go on to complete this questionnaire. Please return this copy to the experimenter. 71 72 CSAQ Please rate the degree to which you generally or typically experience the following symptoms when you are feeling anxious. Use the following scale: 1 2 3 4 5 Not at all Very much so ______j. I find it difficult to concentrate because of uncontrollable thoughts. 2. I worry too much over something that doesn't really matter. 3. My heart beats faster. 4. I feel jittery in my body. 5. I imagine terrifying scenes. 6. I feel like I am losing out on things because I can't make up my mind soon enough. 7. I can't keep anxiety provoking pictures out of my mind. 8. I get diarrhea. 9. I become immobilized. _____JO. Some unimportant thought runs through my mind and bothers me. _____11. I feel tense in my stomach. _____l2. 1 can't keep anxiety provoking thoughts out of my mind. _____l3. I nervously pace. 14. I perspire. 73 (attached to the first pre-task questionnaire the subject received) As we have mentioned, in this pilot study, we need to know to the extent possible just how similar subjects are on several dimensions. Therefore, in addition to the copying tasks, we will be asking you to complete a series of self-report measures. Please complete the questionnaire attached to this sheet. When you have finished, slide it under the door and wait for the next measure to be passed in to you. Follow this same procedure for all measures. 74 CSAQ-STATE Please circle one number to the left of each statement to indicate how you feel right now, that is, at this moment. 00 not spend too much time on any one statement but give the answer which seems to describe your present feelings best. Not Very at much all Somewhat so 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1 My mind feels calm. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 2. My muscles are tense. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 3. I am jittery. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 4. My mind is at peace. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 5. I feel distracted by thoughts. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 6. My heartbeat is calm and regular. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 7. I am worried. l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8. My body feels relaxed. l 2 3 4 5 6 7 9. My breathing is calm and regular. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 10. I am having pleasant thoughts and images. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 11. My body feels restless. l 2 3 4 5 6 7 12. I am presently worrying over possible misfortunes. 75 N-E Directions: To the left of each of the following statements, indicate your feelings, attitudes, or thoughts as they are right now in relation to this study. Use the following numerical scale. 1. The statement does not describe my present condition. The condition is barely noticeable. The condition is moderate. The condition is strong. 0'1th The condition is very strong; the statement describes my present condition very well. I feel my heart beating fast. I feel regretful. I am so tense that my stomach is upset. I am concerned about the possibility of doing poorly. I have an uneasy, upset feeling. I feel that others will be disappointed in me. I am nervous. I feel I may not do as well in this study as I could. I feel panicky. I do not feel very confident about my performance in this study. I feel high strung. I am afraid that other subjects will be able to do a better job in this study. 76 (attached to the first post-task questionnaire given) Now we need some additional information concerning the similarities of the subjects participating in this study. Once again, please complete the attached questionnaire. When you have finished, slide it under the door and wait for the next measure to be passed in to you. Follow this same procedure for all post-task instruments. 77 C10 I. We are interested in learning about the kinds of thoughts that go through people's heads while they are working on a task. The following is a list of thoughts some of which you might have had while doing the task on which you have just worked. Please indicate approximately how often each thought occurred to you while working on it placing the appropriate number in the blank provided to the left of each question. Example: 1 = never 2 = once 3 = a few times 4 = often 5 = very often 1. I thought about how poorly I was doing. 2. I wondered what the experimenter would think of me. 3. I thought about how I should work more carefully. 4. I thought about how much time I had left. 5. I thought about how others have done on this task. 6. I thought about the difficulty of the task. 7. I thought about my level of ability. 8. I thought about the purpose of the experiment. 9. I thought about how I would feel if I were told how I performed. 10. I thought about how often I got confused. 11. I thought about things completely unrelated to the experiment. 11. Please circle the number of the following scale which best represents the degree to which you felt your mind wandered during the taskgyou have just completed. Not at all 1 : 2 : 3 : 4 : 5 : 6 : 7 Very much 78 Post-Task Questionnaire-M (Given to the subjects in the "mirror" condition) 79 Post-Task Questionnaire-M Please answer the following questions concerning your experience during this experiment. Use the following numerical scale. l. #00“) This statement does not describe my experience. This statement describes my experience only slightly. This statement describes my experience somewhat. This statement describes my experience very well. I was aware of the presence of the mirror. I found that the presence of the mirror distracted me from concentrating on the tasks. I was aware of the presence of the experimenter outside the room. I found that the passing of materials under the door distracted me from concentrating on the tasks. Please answer "yes" or "no" to the following questions. Yes Yes No If currently enrolled in PSY 160, you have also taken PSY 170. No If currently enrolled in PSY 170, you have also taken PSY 160. 80 Post-Task Questionnaire-P (given to the subjects in the "no mirror" (i.e., plywood back facing subject) condition) 81 Post-Task Questionnaire-P Please answer the following questions concerning your experience during this experiment. Use the following numerical scale. 1. 2 3. 4 This statement does not describe my experience. This statement describes my experience only slightly. This statement describes my experience somewhat. This statement describes my experience very well. I was aware of the presence of the plywood board. I found that the presence of the plywood board distracted me from concentrating on the tasks. I was aware of the presence of the experimenter outside the room. I found that the passing of materials under the door distracted me from concentrating on the tasks. Please answer "yes" or "no" to the following questions. Yes Yes No If currently enrolled in PSY 160, you have also taken PSY 170. No If currently enrolled in PSY 170, you have also taken PSY 160. APPENDIX C SUBJECT DEBRIEFING APPENDIX C SUBJECT DEBRIEFING Written debriefing mailed to all subjects. The experiment in which you participated was actually an investigation of performance differences between subjects "high" in cognitive anxiety vs. those subjects "low" in such anxiety under conditions of "high" objective self-awareness. Cognitive anxiety was initially measured during the screening phase of the study. Half of the questions on this first questionnaire determined each individual's score in this type of anxiety. Cognitive anxiety refers to the tendency of an individual, when anxious, to experience various kinds of cognitive intrusions - thoughts and images - which often distract the attention of the individual away from the task at hand. For instance: you are giving a speech in front of a group and suddenly experience the thought: what if I can't remember what I'm going to say next? This is an example of cognitive anxiety. Individuals experiencing a great deal of cognitive anxiety tend to be more self-focused in these thoughts and images. Somatic anxiety (the physical manifestations of the anxiety experience) scores were recorded but not used in the experiment itself. Objective self-awareness refers to the tendency of an individual to see him or herself as an object in the world. The mirror used in this experiment was a way of manipulating this experience, increasing a subject's objective self-awareness, making it much more likely that she would become more self-focused. Often when individuals become highly self-aware, they also focus on any intra-self discrepancies existing at the moment, something (5) that they are dissatisfied with about themselves. Those subjects who performed the experiment in front of the mirror's plywood backing were in a state of 19w objective self-awareness, not being reminded of themselves as objects in the world and thus not becoming more self-focused. Any cognitive anxiety and/or tendency at that moment to be highly self-focused would not be heightened by this condition. Many situations will produce an increase in objective self- awareness, including the experience of being a subject in an experiment and any concern the subject may have that he or she is being (or is about to be) evaluated. For this reason, in this experiment we attempted to reduce all other sources of objective self-awareness aside from the mirror. This would make it possible to more accurately isolate the mirror as the chief source of such self-awareness in this study. Thus we set up the experiment as if it were a pilot study in which subjects would be helping future research efforts in an area which 82 83 was not personally threatening to any subject (i.e., overall, reducing evaluation apprehension). Also, the experimenter was not in the room with the subject, once again reducing the possibility of evaluation apprehension as well as reducing distraction for the subject. The mirror was not mentioned until the Post-Task Questionnaire-M to avoid directing the subjects' attention to it in a more artificial way. For those subjects who scored in the "high" range of cognitive anxiety on the initial screening measure, it was hypothesized that because of the increased tendency to be cognitively distracted and most probably self-focused, such tendencies would be reatly heightened in a state of "high" objective self-awareness (mirror?. The chief hypothesis of this study is that subjects in this group (high cognitive anxiety/high objective self-awareness) will do less well on the c0pying task than subjects in any of the other three groups. In addition to performance scores on the Swedish cap ing task (# of correct letters and marks of punctuation and accent , other post-task measures investigated the degree to which cognitive interference and various other types of distraction were experienced during the experiment. In addition, two measures assessing the experience of cognitive anxiety and worry at that moment (state measures) were administered both before and after the copying task to investigate possible changes in the level of anxiety and worry during that period. Somatic anxiety and emotionality (similar to somatic anxiety) scores were once again recorded but not used in the study for analysis. The recording of the subjects' predominant mood involved an exploratory aspect of the study. Because such a mood as a "depressed" feeling or sadness may result in a decrease of energy output, such a mood might be an alternative explanation for a poorer perfbrmance on the copying task, and such infbrmation is thus important for an overall discussion of the study results. This study should be completed over the course of the next two terms. If you would like an abstract of the study results, please either give me an SASE this term and I will mail you a copy as soon as the study is completed or contact me c/o the Department of Psychology. 135 Snyder Hall next Fall. If you have any additional questions concerning the study and/or your participation, please contact me at 353-5193, 19 Snyder, or by mail c/o 135 Snyder. Ann M. Isenberg APPENDIX D RATIONALES AND INSTRUCTIONS TO SUBJECTS APPENDIX D RATIONALES AND INSTRUCTIONS TO SUBJECTS Rationale given to subjects prior to administration of the trait-CSAQ. We are conducting a pilot study for a project in which subjects will copy written material of graded difficulty. Before we begin the study, we need to know just how similar subjects will be on certain variables. Therefore, we are asking you to take the next few minutes to complete first the consent form, then the questionnaire being distributed. 84 85 Rationale given to subjects upon arrival for the second (copying task) phase of the study. As we mentioned before, this is a pilot study for a project in which subjects will copy written materials of graded difficulty. Before we can perform any study on the effects of varying levels of difficulty, we must establish a scale to measure the difficulty of written material. This is what you will be helping us do in this pilot study. By measuring how much of a given written material a random sample of people can c0py accurately, we can not only rank order different materials by difficulty, but we can see how much harder one passage is to copy than another. You will be copying material of one level of difficulty and other subjects will be copying material of other levels. Because this is a study to establish a scale of measurement, we need to find out just how similar participating individuals are in certain ways. Therefore, we will also be asking you to complete some questionnaires before and after the copying task, itself. APPENDIX E CONSENT FORM ' 86 Instructions for experimental procedure verbally given to each subject before the start of the experiment. To prevent distraction, I will be outside the room throughout the experiment. I will be giving you the first measure as you enter the room. Please sit at the desk to your left. You will find pencils and paper for the copying task on the desk. When you finish the first questionnaire, slip it under the door and I will slide the next measure in to you. (Experimenter demonstrates). Follow this procedure for the first three measures. All instructions for completing these measures also appear on the instruments, themselves. After the third measure, I will slide the copying task under the door. Please read the instructions carefully. When you are finished and are ready to begin, knock twice on the door (Experimenter demonstrates). When you have two knocks in return, you may begin copying the Swedish prose, by printing, on the paper you will find on the desk. When you hear a second double-knock (Experimenter demonstrates), stop copying immediately and slide all materials under the door. I will then slide the next measures in to you. Following the c0pying task, there will be four short questionnaires for you to complete. Please follow the same procedure of sliding the completed measures under the door. After the fourth questionnaire is completed, I will come into the room and you will be able to ask any questions you might have. APPENDIX E CONSENT FORM This study involves two stages. You have volunteered for this first phase in which we will ask you to complete a questionnaire after you have read and signed this form. If you have reading, writing, or speaking knowledge of Norwegian, Swedish, or Danish, you will not be eligible to participate. In such a case, please return this form to the experimenter immediately. In this study, it is necessary for us to know how similar subjects are on a number of variables. During this first phase, you will be providing us with some of that information. We would like you to answer the statements on the questionnaire as truthfully as possible. All information provided during this study will be coded to protect subject confidentiality. This first phase of the project will last between 15 and 30 minutes. You may ask the experimenter any questions you like and you are free to terminate your involvement in the study at any point. We will be selecting a number of you to return to participate in the second stage of this project, the copying of written materials. At this point, we do not know how many subjects this will involve. Therefore, once again we emphasize that we would like as truthful a sample as possible when completing this first questionnaire. Those subjects selected to participate in the second phase of the study will be recontacted by the experimenter and asked to return to a specified location at a mutually arranged time. The second phase of the study will involve the copying task, itself, as well as a series of 87 88 self-report questionnaires before and after the task which will provide additional information concerning similarity of the subject sample. The second stage of the project will last from 30-45 minutes. A written explanation of the details of the study will be provided to all subjects as soon as project data collection is completed. At that time, subjects may also arrange to meet with an experimenter to ask for any further information desired. Once again, all information you provide will be coded to maintain confidentiality. You may also ask the experimenter any questions you might have and you are free to terminate your involvement with the project at any time during this second phase. You will receive credit for all participation in this project if outlined by your course instructor. Please read this information carefully and feel free to ask the experimenter any questions you might have. At this point, we are asking you to provide your written consent to participate in the first phase of the study (completing the initial questionnaire) and willingness to participate in the second phase (copying task and additional questionnaires) should be recontacted. Once you have read this form carefully, please indicate your consent to participate by signing and dating this form on the lines immediately below. volunteeris signature Date Experimenter's signature Date APPENDIX F EMOTION WHEEL APPENDIX F EMOTION WHEEL Note on the construction of the "Emotion Wheel" The "Emotion Wheel" was drawn on a square of posterboard measuring 10" x 11". A movable pointer of a contrasting color was then attached to the "Wheel's" center (refer to the artist's illustration of the instrument). 89 90 (attached to the "Emotion Wheel") Please indicate by means of the pointer what you feel your predominant mood to be AT THIS MOMENT. When you have set the pointer, please pass the instrument under the door and wait for the next task. 91 Emotion Wheel . a co .5? ‘3 I: Q to .9 9. 55” ”a .2» 9 'c 8 a” ”4'9 APPENDIX G COPYING TASK APPENDIX G COPYING TASK (instructions for copying task) Now we are going to ask you to copy a sample of some written material. Read the instructions carefully. When you have finished, KNOCK TWICE on the door, then return to your seat. When you hear TWO KNOCKS in return, begin copying the material. Please PRINT all letters as they appear and INCLUDE punctuation and ALL marks of accent (i.e., ' 0, etc., above letters). When you hear TWO KNOCKS once again, STOP and slip the copying task and your c0py under the door. Return to your seat and wait for the next questionnaire to be passed in to you. 92 93 Brddsaden pa landet mellan Gateborg och Stockholm och enligt vad man sade mig nastan 5vera11t i Sverige 3r rag, havre, arter, b6nor och litet korn. Det finns litet vete i Skane och even i trakten av Uppsala. Denna plats var Odens residens och den forna huvudstaden i Skandinavien, och i dess n'a'rhet finns det manga spar av odling och rester av gangen glans. Jordbrukets tillstand i Sverige 5r mycket underlagset de flesta andra lfinders. Farorna blir varken valtade eller sladdade eller regelbundet harvade. Jorden pldjs upp h6gst avsevart at mitten av tegen, medan bagge sidorna beeras mer och mer av den, 85 att kanterna blir nastan nakna berget. Deras svangda form 1 bagge andar hindrar vattnet att passera, medan dréneringen i ett sadant klimat borde vara ett av de fornamsta ankemalen. Den obrukade mark som skall tagas i ansprak far odling rensas frfin skog medelst eld, som ibland sprider sig alltfar langt och kring den odlade biten lamnar en bred och fdga inbjudande Edemark, som 3r ogastvanlig met all vegetation. Fdrstdringen av skogen 5r otvivelaktigt orsaken till att vissa omraden inte langre producerar samma sadesslag eller samma kvantiteter som.tidigare. Spar efter akrar, som nu 5r overvuxna med ljung eller mossa patraffas overallt p5 hedar och bergssluttningar i Skottland, Wales och norra England och likasa i Sverige, Norge och p3 Island. Det f5ref611 anmarkningsvart, att 94 de vildaste stallen vi passerade p5 vagen Eran Halsingborg till Gateborg och darifran till huvudstaden inte var bevuxna med ljung utan med mossa eller ett slags grovt gras. Den sad och det ha som finns i Sverige brukar hassjas pa trara- mar, som fir resta flera fot Ever marken. Sadeskarvar som sattes upp far att torka pa akrarna 5r inte stallda i tvé parallella rader, som lutar sig mot och mater varandra i toppen med tv§ karvar som hatt eller huva, utan 1 en rundel och tackta med en hattkarve, som breds ut far att skydda mot regn. Herdarna i Sverige liksom p5 Island har lurar av bjark. Tva urholkade bitar av bjdrktra laggs tatt tillsammans och bindes om med naver av samma tradslag, sa att det bildas ett runt tar. Det ljud man astadkommer med denna lur fir gallt och vilt men inte obehagligt. Far och kor samlas p5 vissa tider och stallen med hjalp av dessa lockrop. Det a: pa samma satt korna kallas samman av herdarna i Alperna. Vad jag sag av sodra Sverige var ingenting sarskilt lockande far 5gat, men eftersom.1andskapet 5vera11t fir angenamt omvaxlande och ur andra synpunkter erbjuder mycket nytt f6r iakttagaren, 5r det intressant for en resande. Hela Sveriges paradis 3r Skane, dar manga av adeln och de f6rm5gna slar sig ned om sommaren. Man mater dar vackra alléer och andra f3rsk5nande inslag. Men eftersom det inte 5r min avsikt att beskriva de sBdra REFERENCES REFERENCES Blatt, S.J., & Allison, J. (1981). The intelli ence test in personality assessment. In A.I. Rabin (Ed.), Assessment with yprojective techniques: A concise introduction. Neinork: Spring Publishing Co. Borkovec, T.D. (1976). Physiological and cognitive processes in the regulation of anxiety. In G.E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research (Vol. 1, pp. 261-312). New York: Plenum Press. Buss, D.M., & Scheier, M.F. (l976). Self-consciousness, self- awareness, and self-attribution. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 463-468. Carver, C.S., & Scheier, M.F. (l978). Self-focusing effects of dispositional self-consciousness, mirror presence, and audience presence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 324-332. Carver, C.S. (1979). A cybernetic model of self-attention processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 1251-1281. Davidson, R.J. (l978). Specificity and patterning in biobehavioral systems: Implications for behavior change. American Psycholggy 33, 430-436. Davidson, R.J., & Schwartz, G.E. (1976). The psychobiology of relaxation and related states: A multiprocess theory. In 0.1. Mostofsky (Ed.), Behavior control and modification of physiology activity (pp. 399-442). Englewood Clifs, NJ.: PFEfitice-Halll Davis, J.E., & Brock, T.C. (1975). Use of the first person pronouns as a function of increased objective self-awareness and performance feedback. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 381-388. Deffenbacher, J.L. (l978). Worry, emotionality, and task-generated interference in test anxiety: An empirical test of attentional theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 29, 248-254. 95 96 Deffenbacher, J.L. (1980). Worry and emotionality in test anxiety. In I.G. Sarason (Ed.), Test anxiety: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 111-128). Hillside, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum ASsociates, Publishers. Doctor, R.M., & Altman, F. (1969). Worry and emotionality as components of test anxiety: Replication and further data. Psychological Reports, 24, 563-568. Duval, S., & Wicklund, R.A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press. Duval, S., & Wicklund, R.A. (1973). Effects of objective self- awareness on attribution of causality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9, 17-31. Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M.F., & Buss, A.H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting_and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522-527. Finger, R., & Galassi, J.P. (1977). Effects of modifying cognitive versus emotionality responses in the treatment of test anxiety. Journal of Consultingiand Clinical Psychology, 35, 280-287. Fox, J.E., & Houston, B.K. (1983). Distinguishing between cognitive and somatic trait and state anxiety in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 862-870. Heide, F.J. (1981). Relaxation-induced anxiety: A psychophysiological investigation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University. Hodgson, R., & Rachman, S. (1974). Desynchrony in measures of fear. Behavior Research and Therapy, 1g, 319-326. Holroyd, K.A., Westbrook, R., Wolf, M., & Badhorn, E. (1978). Performance, cognition, and physiological responding in test anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psycholggy, 81, 442-451. Holroyd, K.A., & Appel, M.A. (1980). Test anxiety and physiological responding. In I.G. Sarason (Ed.), Test anxiet : Theor , research, and applications (pp. 129-151). Hillside, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum ASsociates, Publishers. Hull, J.G., & Levy, A.S. (1979). The organizational functions of the self: An alternative to the Duval and Wicklund model of self-awareness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37. 756-768. Ickes, W.J., Wicklund, R.A., & Ferris, C.B. (1973). Objective self-awareness and self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 202-219. 97 Kazdin, A.E. (1980). Research design in clinical psychology. New York: Harper and Row. Kirkland, K., & Hollandsworth, J.G. (1980). Effective test-taking: Skills-acquisition versus anxiety-reduction techniques. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 431-439. Lang, P. (1969). The mechanics of desensitization and the laboratory study of human fear. In C.M. Franks (Ed.), Behavior therapy: Appraisal and status. New York: McGraw-Hill. Lazarus, A.A. (1973). Multimodel behavior therapy: Treating the "basic id.“ Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 156, 404-411. Liebert, R.M., & Morris, L.W. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction and some initial data. Psychological Reports, 29, 975-978. Liebling, B.A., & Shaver, P. (1973). Evaluation, self-awareness, and task performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 297-306. Morris, L.W., & Liebert, R.M. (1969). Effects of anxiety on timed and untimed intelligence tests: Another look.. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3;, 240-244. Morris, L.W., & Liebert, R.M. (1970). Relationship of cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety to physiological arousal and academic performance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, gs, 332-337. Morris, L.W., & Fulman, R.S. (1976). Test anxiety (worry and emotionality) changes during academic testing as a function of feedback and test importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 817-824. - Morris, L.W., Davis, M.A., & Hutchings, C.H. (1981). Cognitive and emotional components of anxiety: Literature review and a revised worry-emotionality scale. Journal of Educational Psychology, 13, 544-555. . Osterhouse, R.A. (1972). Desensitization and study-skills training as treatment for two types of test anxious students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 12, 301-307. Osterhouse, R.A. (1976). Group systematic desensitization of test anxiety. In J.D. Krumboltz & C.E. Thoresen (Eds.), Counseling methods. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Paulus, P.B., Annis, A.B., & Risner, H.T. (1978). An analysis of the mirror-induced objective self-awareness effect. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 12, 8-10. 98 Plutchik, R. (1980). A language for the emotions. Psychology Today, 13, 68-78. Sackheim, H.A., & Gur, R.C. (l978). Self-deception, self-confrontation, and consciousness. In G.E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self- e ulation: Advanced in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 139-197). Neinork; Plenum Press. Sarason, I.G., & Stoops, R. (1978). Test anxiety and the passage of time. Journal of Consultinggand Clinical Psychology, 4g, 102-109. Scheier, M.F., & Carver, C.S. (1977). Self-focused attention and the experience of emotion: Attraction, repulsion, elation, and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 625-636. Scheier, M.F., Carver, C.S., & Gibbons, F.X. (1981). Self-focused attention and reactions to fear. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 1-15. Schumacher, A.S., Wright, J.M., & Wiesan, A.E. (1968). The self as a source of anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32. 30-34. Schwartz, G.E., Davidson, R.J., & Maer, F. (1975). Right hemisphere lateralization for emotion in the human brain: Interactions with cognition. Science, 190, 286-288. Schwartz, G.E., Davidson, R.J., & Goleman, D.J. (1978). Patterning of cognitive and somatic processes in the self-regulation of anxiety: Effects of mediation versus exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 321-328. Spielberger, C.D., Gonzales, H.P., Taylor, C.J., Algaze, B., & Anton, W.D. (1978). Examination stress and test anxiety. In C.D. Spielberger & I.G. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 5, pp. 167-191). Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere Publishing Company. Thompson, J.G., Griebstein, M.G., & Kublenschmidt, S.L. (1980). Effects of EMG biofeedback and relaxation training in the prevention of academic underachievement. Journal of Counselihg Psychology, 21, 97-106. Wicklund, R.A. (1975). Objective self-awareness. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 233-275). New York: Academic Press. Wicklund, R.A., & Duval, S. (1971). Opinion change and performance facilitation as a result of objective self-awareness. JOUrnal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 319-342. 99 Wine, J.D. (1971). Test anxiety and the direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin, zg, 92-104. Wine, J.D. (1980). Cognitive-attentional theory of test anxiety. In I.G. Sarason (Ed.), Test anxiety: Theory, research, and gpplications (pp. 349-385). Hillside, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Wine, J.D. (1982). Evaluation anxiety. In H.W. Krohne & C. Laux (Eds.), Achievement, stress, and anxiety (pp. 207-219). New York: Hemisphere Publishing Company.