EXAMINING THE INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC DIMENSIONS OF UNGULATE MOVEMENT AND RESOURCE SELECTION By Kyle Martin Redilla A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Fisheries and Wildlife – Master of Science 2017 ABSTRACT EXAMINING THE INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC DIMENSIONS OF UNGULATE MOVEMENT AND RESOURCE SELECTION By Kyle Martin Redilla Large mammalian herbivores are extensively-studied worldwide, often to gain new insights into the relationship between these animals and their environment. Elucidating a mechanistic basis of processes such as movement and resource selection can inform conservation and management practice. Therein, relating these processes to intrinsic (both state- and stagerelated) and extrinsic dimensions (abiotic and biotic characteristics of the environment) is of paramount importance. I center my thesis on the role of these two dimensions in ungulate movement and resource selection. In chapter 1, I focused on the extrinsic dimension by employing both linear and non-linear regression techniques to evaluate the plausibility of “critical temperatures” in movement of moose (Alces alces) in Norway, using a rich dataset of GPS-location data. I found weak evidence for these thresholds in the movement of moose, and I discuss this finding in light of a changing climate. In Chapter 2 I studied the intrinsic dimension via an examination of individual variation in resource selection of elk (Cervus elaphus) in the Ozark Mountains of Missouri. I investigated the consequences of prevailing practice, whereby individual information is pooled to fit an aggregate-level model, by fitting models at the level of each individual elk and making comparisons. I found that important inferences can be missed if resource selection is only considered at the aggregate level. In summary, my research demonstrates the importance of using wild-living individuals and multiple modeling perspectives to develop functional population- or species-level inferences regarding the roles of intrinsic and extrinsic factors in animal-environment relationships of ungulates. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am deeply indebted to all those who made this research possible, and who believed in me along the way. I am grateful to my major advisor, Dr. Robert Montgomery, for seeing potential in me as a directionless undergraduate and granting me the opportunity to conduct research in the RECaP laboratory. I am thankful to my current thesis committee members, Drs. Joshua Millspaugh and Jerry Urquhart, for invaluable guidance in forming useful research questions and interpreting findings in light of established theory, current applied perspectives, and broader global ecological processes. I would also like to thank Dr. Brian Maurer for advice on maximizing my potential as a quantitative ecologist, and consultation on my research as well. I also thank staff members in the department of fisheries and wildlife, who assisted me with the logistics of my degree, particularly Jill Cruth and Marcia Barr, whose willingness to understand and answer any of my questions saved me a great deal of time and energy. I would like to acknowledge my colleagues in RECaP, whose hard work and dedication to their craft provided an invigorating climate for work, especially Steven Gray, Arthur Muneza, and Remington Moll, for being role models that I learned a great deal from as I acquired my footing and adapted to this discipline. I am especially grateful to Rem for his encouragement, practical advice, and contagious passion for discussing any and all topics related to ecology, uncertainty, and learning. I owe a tremendous amount of gratitude to my parents, Martin Redilla and Donna Gee, for always encouraging me to pursue my passion and giving me everything I ever needed to be successful. Finally, I would like to thank my close friends, who made my own self-belief possible and taught me some crucial life lessons and skills: Karl Supal, you taught me how to grind (an iii invaluable skill in the RECaP lab); Seth Rockafellow, you taught me the value of my own intellect; Tyler Trepanier, you taught me that I can handle more than I think; Sara Tanis, you taught me how to treat the ones I love; Benjamin Goheen, you taught me a lot about everything, and you give me hope for humanity; Mitchell Goheen, you taught me how to love everyone, and you give me hope for finding balance in this life; Molly Robinett, you taught me how to love myself, and you give me hope for the future. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................................................................ vii LIST OF FIGURES ..................................................................................................................... viii INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................................... 1 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................ 5 CHAPTER 1 ................................................................................................................................... 9 INVESTIGATING SIGNATURES OF HEAT STRESS IN WILD-LIVING MOOSE ................ 9 Abstract ....................................................................................................................................... 9 1.1. Introduction .................................................................................................................. 11 1.2. Methods ........................................................................................................................ 14 1.2.1. Study area................................................................................................................ 14 1.2.2. GPS-tracking and temperature ................................................................................ 15 1.2.3. Movement Metric Calculation ................................................................................ 15 1.2.4. Mixed-effects modeling .......................................................................................... 17 1.2.5. Probabilistic Movement Metrics ............................................................................. 19 1.3. Results .......................................................................................................................... 20 1.4 Discussion .................................................................................................................... 23 1.5. Conclusions .................................................................................................................. 27 Acknowledgements ................................................................................................................... 29 APPENDIX ............................................................................................................................... 30 REFERENCES .......................................................................................................................... 46 CHAPTER 2 ................................................................................................................................. 53 EVALUATING THE CONSEQUENCES ON ECOLOGICAL INFERENCE OF AGGREGATING WILDLIFE TELEMETRY DATA WHEN ESTIMATING RESOURCE SELECTION FUNCTIONS ......................................................................................................... 53 Abstract ..................................................................................................................................... 53 2.1. Introduction .................................................................................................................. 55 2.2. Methods ........................................................................................................................ 58 2.2.1. Resource selection data ........................................................................................... 58 2.2.2. Environmental variables ......................................................................................... 59 2.2.3. RSF modeling ......................................................................................................... 59 2.2.4. Comparison between aggregate- and individual-level RSFs .................................. 62 2.3. Results .......................................................................................................................... 64 2.3.1. Number and composition of parameters ................................................................. 65 2.3.2. Parameter estimates ................................................................................................ 65 2.3.3. Predicted relative probabilities ............................................................................... 66 2.4. Discussion .................................................................................................................... 67 Acknowledgements ................................................................................................................... 73 APPENDIX ............................................................................................................................... 74 v REFERENCES .......................................................................................................................... 80 CONCLUSION ............................................................................................................................. 86 REFERENCES .......................................................................................................................... 89 vi LIST OF TABLES Table 1.1. Review of articles published between 2010-2015 that have directly referenced the temperature thresholds suggested by Renecker and Hudson (1986) to develop models or discuss climate related impacts on moose. .............................................................. 31 Table 1.2. Counts of temperatures recorded by GPS collars of GPS-tracked moose in central Norway from July through August of each year during 2006-2010. Counts during crepuscular period (20:00 – 06:00 next day) above, counts of temperatures recorded during daytime period (08:00 – 18:00) middle, differences of temperature (ΔT) between crepuscular and daytime periods below. ........................................................ 33 Table 1.3. Comparison of ARMA error structure for linear mixed models of crepuscular (left half) and daytime (right half) movements of moose, sorted by ascending AIC score. 34 Table 1.4. Comparison of best approximating LMMs. The top three linear mixed models of movement rate (Rate) and mean turning angle (Turn) response variables during crepuscular and daytime movement metrics, sorted by increasing AIC score. ........... 35 Table 1.5. Comparison of best approximating AMMs. The top three additive mixed models of movement rate (Rate) and mean turning angle (Turn) response variables during crepuscular and daytime movement metrics, sorted by increasing AIC score. ........... 36 Table 1.6. Best approximating LMMs, daytime hours. Parameter estimates for best approximating linear mixed models of daytime movements, as determined by AIC. Max. daytime collar temperature (Tday) was the temperature covariate used in these models. ......................................................................................................................... 37 Table 1.7. Best approximating LMMs, crepuscular hours. Parameter estimates for best approximating linear mixed models of crepuscular movements, as determined by AIC. The temperature covariate used in each model is indicated in parentheses. Tcrep = max. crepuscular collar temperature, Tday = max. daytime collar temperature, and ΔT = Tday – Tcrep. ........................................................................................................................... 38 Table 2.1. WAIC values for top 10 aggregate-level resource selection function models. + indicates an environmental variable included in the model, spaces left blank if not included. Environmental variable abbreviations as follows: D2t = distance to twotrack road; Dpav = Distance to paved road (m); Slope = slope (degrees); Aspect = aspect (degrees); Rd dens = road density (km/km2); D edge = Distance to nearest wooded edge; % Can = percent forested canopy cover; Rx burn = years since prescribed fire; Habitat = indicator variables for 8 cover types. .................................. 75 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1. Summer locations of GPS-tracked moose in Central Norway during 2006-2010. Norway is shaded light blue, other countries colored olive. ....................................... 39 Figure 1.2. Mean ln-transformed speeds (± SE) of moose movement in Central Norway, during the months of July and August of each year from 2006-2010 (n = 152 moose). Speeds were estimated from continuous-time correlated random walk movement models fit to each summer track using the package crawl in R. ...................................................... 40 Figure 1.3. Boxplots of daytime (a) and crepuscular (b) movement metrics calculated from GPS data on moose tracked during July-August of 2006-2010. Movement metrics are binned by maximum collar temperature recorded during the respective period for which the movement metric was calculated. .............................................................. 41 Figure 1.4. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs (± 95% CI shaded grey) for movement rates of male moose at median elevation during daytime (a) and crepuscular periods (b-d). Daytime movement rates predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular movement rates predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). ....................................................................................... 42 Figure 1.5. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs (± 95% CI shaded grey) for mean turning angles of male moose at median elevation during daytime (a) and crepuscular periods (b-d). Daytime turning angles predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular turn angles predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). ........................................................................................................... 43 Figure 1.6. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs of movement rates calculated from probabilistic movement paths. Predictions for daytime (a) and crepuscular (b-d) movement rates of male moose. Daytime movement rates predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular movement rates predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). ................................................................... 44 Figure 1.7. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs of mean turning angles calculated from probabilistic movement paths. Predictions for daytime (a) and crepuscular (b-d) movement rates of male moose. Daytime movement rates predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular movement rates predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). ................................................................... 45 Figure 2.1. Inclusion of parameters at the individual level in top models (a panel) and top 5% of models (b panel) as ranked by WAIC for elk reintroduced into the Missouri Ozarks viii (2011-2014). Green boxes (panel a) indicate the selection parameters in line with the row was included in the top model as ranked by WAIC. Shade of purple (panel b) is proportional to the number of times the corresponding parameter was included in the top 5% of sub-models, where the darkest shade corresponds to all models and lightest shade indicates that parameter was not included in any of the top models.. .............. 76 Figure 2.2. Aggregate-level random effects distributions (boxes) and individual-level RSF parameter estimates (blue dots) with 95% Bayesian credible intervals (CIs, in translucent blue) for reintroduced elk in the Missouri Ozarks (2011-2014). The middle horizontal bars within the boxes are the point estimates of the mean hyperparameters of the random effects distributions. The light gray horizontal bars within the boxes are point estimates for the standard deviation hyperparmeters, and the ends of the boxes represent 95% Bayesian credible intervals on mean + standard deviation point estimates............................................................................................. 77 Figure 2.3. Principal components analysis (PCA) ordination of the 88 individual elk reintroduced into the Missouri Ozarks (2011-2014) based on individual-level parameter estimates for selection of 9 resource covariates. Grouped by intrinsic factors (panel a) and by GPS-collaring and release year (cohort; panel b). ...................................................... 78 Figure 2.4. Predicted relative probabilities of use of the elk restoration zone based on and aggregate-level RSF (panel a) and individual-level RSFs for 20 randomly selected elk (panel b) reintroduced to the Missouri Ozarks (2011-2014). ..................................... 79 ix INTRODUCTION The collective ability to measure and map various ecological phenomena has expanded dramatically over the last two decades, as evidenced by a myriad of novel data types and volumes, ushering in a new era of inquiry that has been likened to the bioinformatics movement of the early 2000’s (Jones et al., 2006; Michener and Jones, 2012). Unprecedented rates of data volume and data acquisition have now become commonplace due to advancements in technology and networking, leading to a “data deluge” (Baraniuk, 2011). Novel data collection methods including sensor networks, such as the National Ecological Observatory Network (NEON), automated sensing (including camera trap technology), and both aircraft- and satellite-borne remote sensing platforms are now regularly used to collect environmental observations that have great utility for ecological research (Keller et al., 2008; Pfeifer et al., 2012; Porter et al., 2009, 2012). Not surprisingly, developments that facilitate the quantitative analysis of big ecological data have emerged in tandem, from best practices in data management to statistical methods and software tools (Borer et al., 2009; Purves et al., 2013; Steiniger et al., 2009). These advancements have altered the trajectory of scientific discourse in virtually all types of applied ecological research, but one area of particular growth has been animal movement ecology. This previously marginal sub-discipline only received cursory attention until recent years, when a paradigm shift has been facilitated by decreasing costs and increasing flexibility of tracking methods, most notably those based on Global Positioning System (GPS) technologies (Hussey et al., 2015; Kays et al., 2015; Nathan et al., 2008). A central theme of this new paradigm is a shift in focus from Eulerian (i.e. place- or population-based) approaches to Lagrangian (i.e. individual-based) approaches for studying movement and space use (Nathan et al., 2008; Smouse et al., 2010). When combined with data derived from sensor networks and 1 remote sensing systems, high-resolution tracking data holds promise for revealing mechanistic connections between individual animals and their environment (Cooke et al., 2004; Schick et al., 2008). Recent advancements in Global Positioning System (GPS) telemetry technology provide a venue to study the mechanisms underlying fundamental concepts in animal ecology, such as home range estimation and resource use (Cagnacci et al., 2010). A mechanistic understanding of species movement through human-dominated landscapes is of critical importance to both conservation and management (Fleishman et al., 2011). Indeed, animal-borne telemetry and Geographic Information System (GIS) software have been indispensable tools to wildlife biologists and managers for at least 25 years (Millspaugh and Marzluff, 2001). Barriers to a mechanistic mode of inquiry have traditionally been driven by lack of data however, reflecting the advancements outlined above, animal movement data has recently become voluminous and ubiquitous enough to earn the “big” data title (Kays et al., 2015; Urbano et al., 2010). Databases specific to animal movement data have been created to archive data and facilitate collaboration. For example, as of January 2017, the open-access repository known as Movebank included over 387 million locations on 635 taxa (Wikelski and Kays, 2017). Coupled with the growth across these data collection/management spheres, there has been ongoing development of statistical and software tools necessary to accommodate these increasingly resolute data (Calabrese et al., 2016; Gurarie et al., 2016). Many methods have been proposed for modelling the behavioral mechanisms of animal movement using both Fisherian and Bayesian techniques, but the Bayesian framework offers an intuitive way to accommodate uncertainty of latent states in these types of models (Calabrese et al., 2016; Clark, 2005; Gurarie et al., 2016; Hobbs and Hooten, 2015; Johnson et al., 2008). 2 Studies of ungulate ecology can particularly benefit from sophisticated analyses of telemetry and remotely-sensed data. Movements and behaviors of these animals typically occur over vast spatiotemporal scales, and their relatively large body size permits the attachment of tracking devices that can collect precise data over long periods of time (Kays et al., 2015). Given the ecological and cultural significance of many ungulate species across the globe, there is growing concern around the ways in which these species will respond to continued anthropogenic disturbance and changing climate patterns. Herein, my thesis investigates questions in ungulate ecology arising from the complex interaction between individuals, their movement and resource selection behaviors, and the intrinsic and extrinsic factors affecting these processes. In chapter 1, I explored the relationship between Scandinavian moose movements and a changing climate, specifically rising ambient temperatures. Using five years’ worth of relocation data from over 150 GPS-tracked moose in Norway, I derived movement metrics over daytime and crepuscular periods and employed linear and non-linear regression techniques to test established theory pertaining to the “critical temperatures” beyond which moose behavior is expected to change considerably (Renecker and Hudson, 1986). In discussing this analysis, I address the support for these established thresholds and the importance of using free-roaming animals to delineate threshold responses to extrinsic factors. In chapter 2, I explored the consequences of analyzing resource selection by elk in an aggregate manner, as is conventionally done to achieve statistical inference at the population level. With four years of relocation data on elk introduced into southern Missouri, along with associated GIS layers mapping environmental features in the landscape, I fit discrete-choice models at the aggregate and individual levels and compared inferences between these two broad methodologies. I report on the significance of discounting individual variation in this key ecological process when informing conservation and 3 management. My thesis examines both intrinsic and extrinsic factors on ungulate behavior and in this way, provides a novel computational and analytical framework for assessing movement and resource selection using Bayesian statistical methods. 4 REFERENCES 5 REFERENCES 1) Baraniuk, R. G. 2011. More is less: signal processing and the data deluge. Science, 331 (6018):717 – 719. 2) Borer, E. T., Seabloom, E. W., Jones, M. B., Schildhauer, M. 2009. Some simple guidelines for effective data management. Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America, 90 (2):205 – 214. 3) Cagnacci, F., Boitani, L., Powell, R. A., Boyce, M. S. 2010. Animal ecology meets GPSbased radiotelemetry: a perfect storm of opportunities and challenges. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 365 (1550):2157 – 2162. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2010.0107. 4) Calabrese, J. M., Flemming, C. H., Gurarie, E. 2016. Ctmm: an R package for analyzing animal relocation data as a continuous-time stochastic process. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7 (9):1124 – 1132. DOI:10.1111/2041-210X.12599. 5) Clark, J. 2005. Why ecologists are becoming Bayesians. Ecology Letters, 8 (1):2 – 14. DOI:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00702.x. 6) Cooke, S. J., Hinch, S. G., Wikelski, M., Andrews, R. D., Kuchel, L. J., Wolcott, T. G., Butler, P. J. 2004. Biotelemetry: a mechanistic approach to ecology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19 (6):334 – 343. 7) Fleishman, E., Blockstein, D. E., Hall, J. A., Mascia, M. B., Rudd, M. A., Scott, J. M., Sutherland, W. J., Bartuska, A. M., Brown, A. G., Christen, C. A., Clement, J. P., Dellasala, D., Duke, C. S., Eaton, M., Fiske, S. J., Gosnell, H., Haney, J. C., Hutchins, M., Klein, M. L., Marqusee, J., Noon, B. R., Nordgren, J. R., Orbuch, P. M., Powell, J., Quarles, S. P., Saterson, K. A., Savitt, C. C., Stein, B. A., Webster, M. S., Vedder, A. 2011. Top 40 priorities for science to inform US conservation and management policy. Bioscience, 61 (4):290 – 300. 8) Gurarie, E. 2016. What is the animal doing? Tools for exploring behavioral structure in animal movements. Journal of Animal Ecology, 85 (1):69 – 84. 9) Hobbs, N. T., Hooten, M. B. 2015. Bayesian models: a statistical primer for ecologists. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. ISBN: 9780691159287. 10) Hussey, N. E., Kessel, S. T., Aarestrup, K., Cooke, S. J., Cowley, P. D., Fisk, A. T., Harcourt, R. G., Holland, K. M., Iverson, S. J., Koelk, J. F., Flemming, J. E. M., Whoriskey, F. G. 2015. Aquatic animal telemetry: A panoramic window into the underwater world. Science, 348 (6240):1255642-1 – 1255642-10. DOI:10.1126/science.1255642. 6 11) Johnson, D. S., London, J. M., Lea, M.-A., Durban, J. W. 2008. Continuous-time correlated random walk model for animal telemetry data. Ecology, 89 (5):1208 – 1215. 12) Jones, M. B., Schildhauer, M. P., Reichman, O. J., Bowers, S. 2006. The new bioinformatics: Integrating ecological data from the gene to the biosphere. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 37 (1):519 – 544. 13) Kays, R., Crofoot, M. C., Jetz, W., Wikelski, M. 2015. Terrestrial animal tracking as an eye on life and planet. Science, 348 (6240):aaa2478-1 – aaa2478-9. DOI:10.1126/science.aaa2478. 14) Keller, M., Schinel, D. S., Hargrove, W. W., Hoffman, F. W. 2008. A continental strategy for the National Ecological Observatory Network. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 6 (5):282 – 284. 15) Michener, W. K., Jones, M. B. 2012. Ecoinformatics: supporting ecology as a data-intensive science. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 27 (2):88 – 93. 16) Millspaugh, J. J., Marzluff, J. M. 2001. Radio-tracking and animal populations: past trends and future needs In J. J. Millspaugh and J. M. Marzluff (Eds.), Radio tracking and animal populations (pp. 383 – 393). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. DOI:10.1016/B978012497781-5/50016-5. 17) Nathan, R., Getz, W. M., Revilla, E., Holyoak, M., Kadmon, R., Saltz, D., Smouse, P. E. 2008. A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105 (49):19052 – 19059. DOI:10.1073/pnas.0800375105. 18) Pfeifer, M., Disney, M., Quaife, T., Marchant, R. 2012. Terrestrial ecosystems from space: A review of earth observation products for macroecology applications. Global Ecology and Biogeography, 21 (6):603 – 624. 19) Porter, J. H., Hanson, P. C., Lin, C.-C. 2012. Staying afloat in the sensor data deluge. Trend in Ecology and Evolution, 27 (2):121 – 129. 20) Porter, J. H., Nagy, E., Kratz, T. K., Hanson, P., Collins, S. L., Arzberger, P. 2009. New eyes on the world: Advanced sensors for ecology. Bioscience, 59 (5):385 – 397. 21) Purves, D., Scharlemann, J., Harfoot, M., Newbold, T., Tittensor, D. P., Hutton, J., Emmott, S. 2013. Time to model all life on earth. Nature, 493 (7432):295 – 297. 22) Renecker, L. J., Hudson, R. J. 1986. Sesonal energy expenditures and thermoregulatory responses of moose. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 64 (3):322 – 327. 23) Schick, R. S., Loarie, S. R., Colchero, F., Best, B. D., Boustany, A., Conde, D. A., Halpin, P. N., Joppa, L. N., McClellan, C. M., Clark, J. S. 2008. Understanding movement data and 7 movement processes: current and emerging directions. Ecology Letters, 11 (12):1338 – 1350. DOI:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2008.01249.x. 24) Smouse, P. E., Focardi, S., Moorcroft, P. R., Kie, J. G., Forester, J. D., Morales, J. M. 2010. Stochastic modelling of animal movement. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 365 (!550):2201 – 2211. 25) Steiniger, S., Hay, G. J. 2009. Free and open source geographic information tools for landscape ecology. Ecological Informatics, 4 (4):183 – 195. 26) Urbano, F., Cagnacci, F., Calenge, C., Dettki, H., Cameron, A., Neteler, M. 2010. Wildlife tracking data management: a new vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 365 (1550): 2177 – 2185. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2010.0081. 27) Wikelski, M., Kays, R. 2017. Movebank: archive, analysis and sharing of animal movement data. Hosted by the Max Planck Institute for Ornithology. www.movebank.org, accessed on 1 March 2017. 8 CHAPTER 1 INVESTIGATING SIGNATURES OF HEAT STRESS IN WILD-LIVING MOOSE Abstract Heat stress from rising ambient temperatures associated with global climate change threatens the persistence of numerous species of wildlife as heat-induced behavioral changes can decrease fitness and alter population dynamics. Moose (Alces alces) are purported to be heatsensitive mammals that become stressed when ambient temperatures rise above specific temperature thresholds. However, these temperature thresholds were established in a study which linked physiological changes in thermoregulation to ambient temperatures in a small study group (n = 2) of captive-reared moose. Temperature thresholds for organisms vulnerable to climate change should be rigorously evaluated from wild-living animals whenever possible. Here, I examined the effects of ambient temperature on movements of wild-living moose to investigate potential temperature thresholds beyond which movement is altered. Analyzing animal movement in this manner provides a natural way to relate environmental conditions to behavior. I fit both linear and additive mixed models to daily movement metrics (movement rate and mean turning angle) calculated from GPS locations collected on 152 moose tracked in central Norway during 2006-2010. I modeled these movement metrics as a function of temperature recorded by GPS collars during different periods of the day, as well as sex, mean elevation, and month. I found little evidence of a threshold response in moose movement to temperature during either period, but note responses to high within-day temperature differences. I documented differences between crepuscular and daytime movement responses to temperature, suggesting that moose 9 exhibit a compensatory strategy for mitigating the effects of high temperatures by moving more during the crepuscular and night hours when temperatures are high. These results suggest that onset of problematic heat stress in wild-living moose may not occur at the previously proposed temperature thresholds, or that heat stress mitigation strategies are not well-correlated with the movements considered. Additionally, within-day variability in ambient temperature may be an important factor to consider when determining the onset of heat stress in moose. 10 1.1. Introduction Warming temperatures associated with global climate change have been causally linked to changes in the behavior, reproduction, distribution, and abundance of a variety of species (Harley et al., 2006; Humphries et al., 2004; Parmesan and Yohe, 2003; Root et al., 2003; Thomas et al., 2004). Moose (Alces alces) are temperate-zone obligates that are well-adapted to the cold and can tolerate temperatures as low as -32 °C (Karns, 2007; Renecker and Hudson, 1986), but unseasonably warm temperatures purportedly induce physiological heat stress (Renecker and Hudson, 1986; Schwartz and Renecker, 2007). In response to heat stress, moose are expected to engage in thermoregulatory behaviors such as the selection of habitat providing thermal refuge, and compensatory activity schedules (Broders et al., 2012; Dussault et al., 2004). This heat sensitivity supports the notion that a warming climate is linked with recent (within the last three decades) declines in moose populations at the southern extent of the species’ range globally (Dou et al., 2013; Lenarz et al., 2009; Murray et al., 2006). Other possible drivers implicated in these declines include pathogens and underestimated effects of wolf (Canis lupus) predation (Lankester, 2010; Mech and Fieburg, 2014; Murray et al., 2006). While temperature is likely a factor limiting the southern geographic range of this species, and continued trends in climate change may affect moose survivability in certain populations, the precise effect of warming temperatures on population viability remains poorly understood (Lenarz et al., 2010; van Beest et al., 2012). For example, a viable population of moose with relatively ideal demographic characteristics resides in southern Ontario despite exposure to apparently unfavorable climatic conditions (Murray et al., 2012). Identifying the mechanism(s) by which 11 rising ambient temperature affects individual animal fitness is crucial to assessing the risks that changing climate patterns may pose to populations. Studying animal movement is a useful way to examine climate-related impacts for many species (McKellar et al., 2005; Nathan and Giuggioli, 2013; Patterson et al., 2009; Schick et al., 2008). In the case of ungulates, temperature and precipitation have been linked to movements of white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus; Brinkman et al., 2005) and wild boar (Sus scrofa; Thurfjell et al., 2014). In addition, fine-scale movements of migratory caribou (Rangifer tarandus caribou) in the Arctic can be affected by availability of ice (Leblond et al., 2016), and the interaction of plant phenology and climate can affect large-scale movements of red deer (Cervus elaphus; Pettorelli). Exploring the movement-temperature relationship in moose provides an ideal opportunity to investigate potential fitness impacts of rising temperatures, for two reasons: 1) there are physiological temperature thresholds that have been proposed for moose (McCan et al., 2013; Renecker and Hudson, 1986); 2) there is support for altered behavior in response to ambient temperatures near these thresholds (Melin et al., 2014; Street et al., 2015; van Beest et al., 2012, 2013). It has been suggested that moose are stressed by heat when ambient temperatures rise above 14° C (57° F) in summer and above -5° C (23° F) in winter (Renecker and Hudson, 1986, 1990). These seasonal thresholds however, were derived from a study of 2 captive-reared cow moose kept in a provisioned fenced enclosure (Renecker and Hudson, 1986). Evidence of heat stress in this study was based on physiological responses, namely increased respiratory rate and open-mouth panting. The authors postulated that when moose are stressed by heat they reduce both activity and food intake (Renecker and Hudson, 1986). Despite the narrow scope of this assessment, these thresholds have been referenced scores of times since 1986. For example, > 15 12 papers published between 2010 and 2015 in 11 different journals studying 10 moose populations worldwide have either used these thresholds to develop models or cited them in discussing climate-related impacts on moose (see Table 1.1). However, because the exact mechanism by which rising ambient temperatures could have population-level consequences for moose still remains unclear, the utility of the thresholds presented by Renecker and Hudson (1986) has been questioned (Lowe et al., 2010; McGraw et al., 2012). One possible mechanistic explanation is that when ambient temperature rises above some seasonal thresholds, moose movement would be compromised to the point where foraging effort would be reduced. If these effects were sustained because of heat stress, weight loss would be incurred which, in turn, would deteriorate body condition potentially leading to a reduction in fecundity and fitness. Despite support of this explanation as a conceptual model in the literature, the first connection (altered movement and activity beyond these thresholds) has received only marginal support (DelGuidice et al., 2011; Sand, 1996; Testa and Adams, 1998). For instance, while there is support for temperature-induced changes in moose movement behavior (resource selection and habitat use) at temperatures near the proposed thresholds (Broders et al., 2012; Melin et al., 2014; van Beest et al., 2012, 2013), another study found no difference in such behavior of moose (e.g., habitat use) with respect to these temperature thresholds (Lowe et al., 2010). More specific changes in wild-living moose movement behavior observed during warm periods include reduced activity, decreased distance traveled, and selection for habitat providing thermal refugia (Demarchi and Bunnell, 1995; Dussault et al., 2004; Street et al., 2015). A compensatory effect has also been suggested whereby, during warm periods, moose shift the bulk of their activity to night-time hours when conditions are comparatively cooler (Dussault et al., 2004). That being said, no mechanistic link has connected variation in movement to 13 population-level consequences given the thresholds suggested by Renecker and Hudson (1986). Therefore, while the Renecker and Hudson thresholds have been often applied by the research community as benchmarks for understanding moose response to heat stress (Renecker and Hudson, 1986; McCan et al., 2013; Table 1.1), their adequacy for describing population-level processes in free-living moose is blurred by inconsistencies in behavioral responses to ambient temperature and possible compensatory behaviors during warm periods. Here, I analyzed a large set of movement data from a population of moose in central Norway to answer a fundamental research question: are there thresholds in ambient temperature above which wild-living moose movement changes significantly? Specifically, I searched for changes in daytime and crepuscular movements in relation to temperature during these periods. I also investigated the possibility of compensatory movement schedules by relating both daytime temperature and within-day temperature difference to crepuscular movements. Analyzing animal movement in this manner provides a framework to assess the mechanistic role that climate change phenomena may have for species population trajectories. I discuss the implications of this research for understanding how warming temperatures might affect moose fitness, and I examine the utility of my approach for detecting climate-induced changes in moose behavior from movement paths and associated environmental data. 1.2. Methods 1.2.1. Study area Through a partnership with the Norwegian Institute for Nature Research (NINA), I obtained data on moose that were tracked across central Norway (64°32’ N, 12°15 E), in NordTrøndelag, Sør-Trøndelag, and southern parts of Nordland counties from 2006 to 2010 (Fig. 1.1). The study area ranged from coastal regions to alpine zones containing mountain, boreal forest, 14 and cultivated land biomes (Van Moorter et al., 2013). The majority of the study area was forested, consisting primarily of coniferous stands and to a lesser extent deciduous forests (Bjørneraas et al., 2010). Cultivated land was particularly common at lower elevations, along the fjords on the west coast (Moen, 1999). 1.2.2. GPS-tracking and temperature During February-March or November 2006, and February-March 2007 and 2008, moose were darted from a helicopter to immobilize them, and a sedative administered to facilitate attachment of GPS collars (Bunnefeld et al., 2011). 171 moose (145 adults, 26 calves) were fit with GPS/Global System for Mobile communications (GSM) collars, 7 of which were Tellus GPS collars (Followit AB/Televilt, Lindesburg, Sweden) and the remaining 164 were GPS PLUS/GPS PRO Light collars (VECTRONIC Aerospace GmbH, Berlin, Germany). 50 moose (37 F, 13 M) were collared in 2006, 87 moose (63 F, 24 M) were collared in 2007, and 31 moose (17 F, 14 M) were collared in 2008. Locations were recorded during 2009 and 2010 as well, tracking 67 moose (54 F, 13 M) in 2009 and 25 moose (22 F, 3 M) in 2010. The collars had an expected battery life of 3 years while attempting a locational fix every 2 hours (Bjørneraas et al., 2011; Bunnefeld et al., 2011). These GPS collars recorded ambient temperature from an onboard sensor at each successful location fix. Although these temperatures might be biased by factors such as prevailing conditions or placement on the body, the offset tends to be consistent and collar temperatures provide a reliable index of ambient air temperature (Ericsson et al., 2015). 1.2.3. Movement Metric Calculation Using the data acquired from these efforts, I calculated moose movements from 214,024 GPS locations (107,049 daytime fixes, 106,975 crepuscular fixes) of 152 moose in central 15 Norway recorded during the summer months (July and August) of 2006-2010. I centered my assessment on summer because this is the time of year when behavior-altering heat stress in moose is suggested to be most obvious (Broders et al., 2012; Dussault et al., 2004), when migratory individuals in this region are likely to be occupying summer home ranges, and to minimize effects of parturition and rearing that occur during the early summer (Bjørneraas et al., 2011; Rolandsen et al., 2016; Solberg et al., 2007). After screening the GPS data for erroneous fixes following established procedures, I subdivided the data so as to assess variation in moose movement response to temperature between daytime and crepuscular periods (Bjørneraas et al., 2010). Crepuscular periods of each day were defined as the time from 8 p.m. to 6 a.m. the following day, so as to encapsulate the movement path between both onset of civil twilight at night and offset of civil twilight in the morning during July and August. This approach allowed me to assess the effects of temperature on moose movements when activity/foraging effort is typically at the highest, and it is consistent with daily activity patterns of other large herbivores in temperate regions (Cederlund, 1989; Ensing et al., 2014). I defined the daytime period of each day as the hours between 8 a.m. and 6 p.m., given the remainder of the recorded locations fell within these hours. I calculated two movement metrics during both the daytime and crepuscular periods of each summer day: i) movement rate - calculated as the distance travelled (the sum of the inter-location distances) divided by the length of the period (10 hours), and ii) mean turning angle - where the turning angle of a location is the angle formed between the line extending beyond that location in the direction of travel from the previous point, and the line segment between the current location and the next. I considered angles to be constrained by 180°, such that a clockwise angle of 270° would be equivalent to a counter-clockwise angle of 90° in the opposite direction. 16 1.2.4. Mixed-effects modeling I modeled movement rates and mean turning angles as a function of temperature in a linear mixed model (LMM) framework for movements during each period. In models of daytime movements, I used the maximum collar temperature recorded (Tday) during the respective daytime period. To directly assess crepuscular movements in relation to ambient temperature, I used maximum collar temperature recorded during the respective crepuscular period (Tcrep) as the temperature covariate. I used the maximum recorded temperatures because I was interested in moose response to specific thresholds rather than average conditions; however, a preliminary analysis using mean daily temperatures estimated by local weather stations did not differ qualitatively from the results presented here. Additionally, to test the possibility of compensatory movements during warm periods, I modeled crepuscular movements in two other cases, each utilizing a different temperature covariate: Tday of the prior daytime period, and ΔT, the difference between maximum collar temperatures recorded during the daytime prior and the current period (ΔT = Tday – Tcrep). I included individual moose (id) and year within id as random intercepts. Year was modeled as a random effect within id because these factors were only partially crossed, meaning the variation in movements between years is confounded by the differences in movements between the different groups of moose tracked each year (Pinheiro and Bates, 2000). The remaining fixed effects for each model were sex; elevation, calculated by taking the average of the digital elevation model estimates (resolution 25 m, altitude 1 m) at each location recorded during the corresponding period; and month. Thus, the global LMM was of the form: Yijk = β0 + βtempx1ijk + βelevx2ijk + βmox3ijk + βsexx4ijk + u1j + u2jk + εijk 17 (1) where Yijk is the ith observation of any of the three movement metrics calculated for the jth individual in year k; β-parameters correspond to population mean response and effects of temperature, elevation, month (base July), and sex (base female); u1-2 correspond to normallydistributed random effects of moose id j and year k within id j; and εijk is a temporallyautocorrelated error term. I included both sex and month in the model because of differences observed in preliminary analysis of within-day variation in movement rate (Fig. 1.2), and because males may regularly travel greater distances than females (Bjørneraas et al., 2012; Van Moorter et al., 2013). Elevation was included in the models because altitude is one of the main environmental gradients in this region, and preliminary analyses revealed a large variation across the study population relative to variation within individuals (Bakkestuen et al., 2008). I tested temporal autocorrelation structures, prior to fitting LMMs, by fitting the global linear mixed models for each movement metric with an autoregressive (AR) moving-average (MA) error structure. I separately fit all 8 combinations of orders 0-2 for both the AR and MA components, as well as AR(0) MA(3) and AR(3) MA(0). Models were compared using AIC, ranking models according to ascending AIC value. I conducted these tests separately for both crepuscular and daytime movements, and selected the AR-MA structure having the lowest AIC value for each. To ensure reliable estimates of collar temperature and elevation were available for a given period, I only included crepuscular and daytime periods having at least 5 (out of 6 possible) recorded GPS locations. Although other factors influence fine-scale movements of moose I focused on examining the relationship between moose movements and temperature while minimizing model complexity (Leblond et al., 2010). In addition to the linear mixed models, I also wanted to test for the potential of nonlinear relationships in these data. Specifically, I hypothesized that the relationship between movement 18 metrics and temperature measures could be curvilinear. Therefore, I evaluated non-linear patterns by developing an additive mixed model (AMM) framework. Additive models are especially useful for this analysis because they would help reveal a specific temperature threshold at which movements significantly change, as might be expected if movement is affected at the Renecker and Hudson thresholds (i.e., 14o and 20o C in the summer (Fewster et al., 2000; Large et al., 2013; Renecker and Hudson, 1986). I tested the same fixed effects as above (sex and month), with thin plate regression splines placed on the continuous terms (temperature and elevation; Wood, 2003). The same random effects structure used for the LMMs was employed in the AMMs, as well as the best ARMA structure. These models took the form: Yijk = β0 + ftemp(x1ijk) + felev(x2ijk) + βmox3ijk + βsexx4ijk + u1j + u2jk + εijk (2) where ftemp and felev are unknown smooth functions of temperature and elevation to be estimated from the data, and all other terms are the same as in (1). The best approximating models were selected by the lowest AIC score (Burnham and Anderson, 2003). This resulted in a total of 16 LMMs and 12 AMMs (additive models without the continuous covariates are equivalent to linear models, thus they were not re-fit in the AMM analysis) for each response, giving a total of 28 models under evaluation for the movement metrics in each period. I conducted all statistical analyses in the R statistical computing environment using packages nlme and mgcv (Pinherio et al., 2015; R Core Team, 2015; Wood, 2011). 1.2.5. Probabilistic Movement Metrics I also developed a companion modeling effort using a probabilistic movement estimator to quantify the moose movement metrics. I performed these additional steps to assess sensitivity of results to differences in sample size and periods of time where GPS locations were closer in space, as these factors could influence the bias of movement metric estimates. I predicted these 19 movement paths from continuous-time correlated random walk (CTCRW) models fit separately for each moose in each summer using the R package crawl (Johnson et al., 2008). The CTCRW is a state-space model based on Brownian motion, where the estimated GPS location at time = 𝑡 + 1 is a function of the current movement state. This includes the current velocity, and parameters for velocity autocorrelation and velocity variation, which influence both the predicted location and the associated Brownian error (Johnson et al., 2008). I then used these models to predict GPS locations at the same two hour intervals the GPS fixes were attempted and, using these predicted locations, calculated movement metrics for the same exact periods used before (i.e., those having 5 or 6 locations obtained on the particular moose during the respective time of day). With these metrics in hand, I repeated the analysis outlined above exactly as described and evaluated the similarity in the predictions with tests of correlation. My goal here was thus to evaluate whether probabilistic movement paths that accounted for autocorrelation and variation in velocity revealed different patterns from those developed using the straight-line calculations performed on the raw GPS locations. 1.3. Results After screening the GPS data and omitting periods having less than 5 successful GPS fixes, 17,531 daytime periods and 16,799 crepuscular periods each consisting of 5 or 6 GPS fixes remained for calculating movement metrics, represented by 152 moose. 95% of the maximum daytime (Tday) and maximum crepuscular collar temperatures (Tcrep) recorded were between 17 and 41 °C and 13 and 29 °C, respectively. Temperature differences between daytime and respective crepuscular periods (ΔT = Tday – Tcrep) fell between 0 and 17 °C for 95% of the time. The collars consistently recorded temperatures near and above the proposed thresholds, during both daytime and crepuscular hours (Table 1.2). The movement rates calculated in both periods 20 were positively skewed, which was improved by log-transformation prior to all analyses. Although bounded by 0 and 180°, mean turning angles were approximately normally distributed, with 97.5% of data falling within 55.3 and 151.2°. Variation in both movements during periods of similar temperatures was large, although movement rates were characterized by more outliers than mean turning angles during both daytime and crepuscular periods. The best autocorrelation structure for crepuscular movement rate was ARMA(2,1) as determined by both the lowest AIC values, and all other models scored best with the ARMA(1,1) structure, including all models of daytime movement metrics (Table 1.3). The best approximating AMMs and LMMs for both daytime and crepuscular movement rate as determined by lowest AIC included all four terms, with the exception that month was excluded from the top LMM for daytime movement rate (Tables 1.4 and 1.5). The best approximating AMMs and LMMs of turning angle during the crepuscular period were of the same form, including temperature for each estimate tested (Tday, Tcrep, and ΔT), as well as sex and month. However, temperature was not included in the best approximating AMM for daytime turning angle, although the global model was within ΔAIC = 1 (Table 1.5). I report the mean turning angle predictions of the global model in this case (Fig. 1.5), as the main focus here is on the effect of temperature, and a ΔAIC =1 suggests weak evidence to prefer one over the other (Burnham and Anderson, 2003). The runner-up LMM for each model of mean crepuscular turning angle was the global model, and was within ΔAIC = 2 for each estimate of temperature tested (Table 1.4). The top AMM for crepuscular movement rate revealed a positive relationship with increasing Tcrep and increasing Tday, indicating that moose moved greater distances at night when it is warm during both day and night (Figs. 1.4b and 1.4c). Further, the linear effect of ΔT on 21 crepuscular movements evidenced by the top LMMs was much less than the effects of Tcrep and Tday on crepuscular movement rate (Table 1.7). This corresponded to a decreasing relationship between Tday and daytime movement rates, which was also reflected in the opposite signs of the temperature effects in the LMMs between periods (Tables 1.6 and 1.7). Interestingly, this decreasing relationship between Tday and daytime movement rate leveled off near ~ 30 °C, while the increasing relationship between temperatures (both Tday and Tcrep) and crepuscular movements did not exhibit a tapering off effect (Fig. 1.4a). This suggests rate of movement during the day may reach a limit even as temperatures during this period continue to rise relatively high. These findings support the notion that moose move greater distances at night, even as Tcrep increases. A distinct temperature threshold was not evident in any of the best approximating models, although the best approximating AMM for mean turning angle during the daytime period, as well as that for crepuscular movement rate in relation to ΔT, both indicated a curvilinear relationship with increasing estimates. There is a notable concave downward relationship predicted for movement rate as ΔT approaches the extreme observed values, which suggests the possibility of a threshold in within-day temperature variability on crepuscular movements (Fig. 1.4d). That mean daytime turning angles began to increase beyond Tday = 30 °C, co-occurring with the leveling off of daytime movement rate, indicates that although moose may have stopped moving less with increasing temperature, their movements became less directed (Figs. 1.4a and 1.5a). Much of the variation in each of the movement metrics went unexplained by the best approximating models, as evidenced by the substantial amount of residual variation relative to the variation of the random effects (Tables 1.3 and 1.4). The effect sizes of the temperature terms in the LMMs were small relative to the scale of the response 22 variables, and effects of the other predictors (Tables 1.3 and 1.4). Conducting the same analyses using movement metrics calculated from CTCRW-derived GPS locations did not reveal any differences in either LMMs or AMMs; responses were highly correlated at r = 0.949 and r = 0.993 for mean turning angle and movement rate, respectively. Subsequent predictions from models fit to movement metrics calculated on predicted and raw GPS locations ways were nearly identical, as evidenced by high correlations between predicted values for the two movement modeling approaches (r > 0.995 for Figs. 1.6 and 1.7). 1.4 Discussion I found little evidence of summer temperature thresholds beyond which moose movement significantly changed. This result comes after I carried out a rigorous assessment of the movement of 152 wild-living moose, evaluated across vast spatio-temporal extents and over two different periods (daytime and crepuscular), using two separate modeling frameworks (linear and additive) for each of two techniques for quantifying movement (raw GPS locations and predicted locations based on CTCRW models). I did not find any concordance with the temperature thresholds proposed by Renecker and Hudson (1986), despite repeated exposure to higher temperatures in both periods. Rather, the effect of maximum daytime and crepuscular temperature (Tday and Tcrep) on crepuscular movement metrics as estimated in the AMMs indicated linear relationships between movements and temperature, and of the same direction for each metric (Figs. 1.4b and 1.4c; 1.5b and 1.5c). These results do not dismiss the possibility that a temperature threshold in moose movement exists. Rather, they suggest that at a 12-hour scale, such thresholds do not scale-up to the population-level movement metrics I employed, or are not applicable to this population of wild-living moose during the summer months. Indeed, this study is limited by a restriction to the summer months. Additional research should quantify this relationship during times where altered movement could have more obvious fitness implications 23 (e.g., mating season). The broad-scale nature of this approach also limits the ability to document behavioral responses to temperature at fine scales (e.g., within-hour movements in a small habitat patch) and does not necessarily rule out behavioral responses at such scales. Yet the lack of any threshold response resulting from exposure to previously proposed critical temperatures is a key finding in light of the evidence that moose alter their behavior (habitat selection and use) near these same temperatures (Broders et al., 2012; McCan et al., 2013; Melin et al., 2014; Renecker and Hudson, 1986; Street et al., 2015; van Beest et al., 2012, 2013). The predicted relationship of crepuscular movement rate with ΔT is also interesting, as it provides evidence that within-day fluctuations in ambient temperature could be a potential indicator for threshold responses in movements at the daily scale. Thus, my study supports the notion that ‘thresholds’ may be better referred to as a general range in which ambient temperature could start to trigger summer heat stress in moose (McGraw et al., 2012; Renecker and Hudson, 1990). Furthermore, considering variability in temperature in addition to ambient temperature levels may allow for more precise prediction of the onset of this heat stress. The stronger positive effects of Tday and Tcrep on crepuscular movement rates that I documented relative to the effect of ΔT provides evidence that moose move more at night during warm periods in general (Figs. 1.4b and 1.4c; Table 1.4), which is consistent with previous findings of Dussault et al., (2004). In that assessment, the authors identified a positive effect of increasing temperature on nighttime activity, where activity was estimated from motion sensors affixed to the GPS collars. While they did not find a negative effect of ambient temperature on daytime moose activity, these results suggest that this may be the case if there exists a positive relationship between moose activity and movement rates. This is a reasonable assumption, as a recent study found decreased daytime activity levels of moose during warm periods (Street et al., 24 2015). These results suggest that moose may exhibit compensatory movement or activity schedules, moving more at night and less during the day as temperatures increases. That being said, the size of this effect was much less than the effects of either temperature estimate on crepuscular movement rate (Tables 1.6 and 1.7). This is likely due to the leveling-off phenomenon displayed in predictions of daytime movement rates as Tday approached 30 °C (Fig. 1.4a), where the movement rate remained steady at about 48.9 m/hr (SD ± 57.6 m/hr) thereafter. Coupled with the concave upward trend of Tday with daytime turning angle near this temperature range (Fig. 1.5a), these results suggest that moose may start engaging in different behavior at this point, possibly avoiding harassment by insects or searching for thermal refuge (Renecker and Hudson, 1990). The differences revealed between the AMMs of mean turning angle for each period paint a similar picture. While a negative linear relationship was found between crepuscular turning angles and Tday, Tcrep, and ΔT (Table 1.6), the nonlinear relationship during the day indicates moose may not exhibit the same behavioral responses to warm temperatures during the day as they do during the crepuscular and night hours (Figs. 1.4a-1.4d). The challenge of achieving a mechanistic understanding for the demographic consequences of high ambient temperatures is further complicated by how intrinsic factors such as body mass and maturity may affect the movement-temperature relationship. Because I assessed coarse population-level relationships between movements and temperature, I only included sex as a fixed effect. This assumes calves do not move any differently than adults, and that sex captured some of the variation in movement caused by body mass. However, the large random effect variation suggests that including these other factors in analyses could strengthen the model fits. Regional variability in other interacting factors, such as disease prevalence, predation, and changes in both forage quality and selection are also important for informing a 25 mechanistic understanding. If the range of moose is limited by operative temperatures in summer, assessing the influence of these other biotic factors would be necessary. That mature conifer stands may be selected during warm periods, but are typically lacking in abundance of high-quality forage compared to younger forests, suggests a possible trade-off between thermoregulation and forage availability (Bjørneraas et al., 2011; van Beest et al., 2012). But careful evaluation of the implications of this general concept for individual and population-level moose fitness is necessary and currently lacking. In North America, parasites such as the winter tick (Dermacentor albipictus) might affect fitness, and increased transmission of the meningeal worm (Parelaphostrongylus tenuis) to moose has been implicated as an overlooked cause of population declines (Lankester, 2010; Murray et al., 2006). Elevated infection rates have been attributed to a warming climate, especially during winter, as this has been associated with greater abundance of the primary mammalian host (white-tailed deer; Odocoileus virginianus) in moose range (DelGuidice et al., 2002; Whitlaw and Lankester, 1994). Additional examination of the multifaceted ways in which external biotic factors and ambient temperature regimes can interact to affect moose populations is necessary if underlying mechanisms do exist. This study highlights the complex nature of the relationship between abiotic stimuli and animal movement at broad scales. Studying the effects of abiotic conditions on physiological stress of captive animals (e.g. Renecker and Hudson, 1986; McCan et al., 2013) provides a helpful, but perhaps limited, reference point from which to understand the behavior of wildliving animals. Analysis of animal movement data can be illustrative of the physiological and behavioral states of free-ranging individuals as they interact with their environment (Gurarie et al., 2016). For instance, increasingly high resolution data can facilitate models that account for different “modes” or “states” of movement, which depict the interaction of an individual’s 26 internal state and the environment (Morales et al., 2004; Nathan et al., 2008; Patterson et al., 2009; Schick et al., 2008). Employing flexible frameworks to account for the effects of environment on these different characterizations of movement might grant an enhanced understanding of the relationship between physiology and abiotic conditions in wild-living moose. However, the ability to precisely characterize the abiotic conditions experienced in the wild will continue to be challenging, particularly where compared to the access that captive studies afford. For instance, the reliability of collar temperature measurements may diminish as habitat selection varies with temperature (e.g. selection of open water and marshes; Street et al., 2015). Such behavioral influence on collar estimates of ambient temperature could possibly buffer the effects of a threshold response at the temperature extremes. Therefore, there are pluses and minuses to studies that occur along the captive-to-wild continuum. This point emphasizes the more philosophical position that choosing the most appropriate modeling techniques from such a wide, often esoteric range of possible methods can prove to be restrictive for many ecologists (Gurarie et al., 2016; Schick et al., 2008). 1.5. Conclusions Declines in moose population abundance have been attributed to a variety of factors beyond rising ambient temperatures, but the southern edge of the moose range is still predicted to shift northward as the climate continues to warm and heat stress becomes more likely (Lenarz et al., 2010; Murray et al., 2012). However, the thermal conditions under which heat stress in moose might negatively affect fecundity after accounting for these factors remains unknown, and the physiological temperature thresholds for moose as inferred from a small sample of captive animals (Renecker and Hudson, 1986; Table 1.1) should be applied with caution. Determining temperature thresholds for wild-living moose movement would enhance our ability to predict the 27 effects of abiotic conditions on body condition, and thus survival. My study took an important step towards that end by identifying a relatively smooth and continuous relationship between temperature and moose movements. Additional work could build on this study by exploring this relationship at different scales, in other seasons, or in the context of other potentially important biotic factors (e.g., habitat heterogeneity). 28 Acknowledgements I am thankful for a relationship between the RECaP lab and NINA, without which I would not have been able to conduct this analysis. I acknowledge the effort put forth by personnel at NINA to conduct a large scale tracking operation that resulted in the high-quality GPS data used herein, and I am grateful to the Norwegian Environment Agency and the Research Council of Norway for funding this aspect of the research. 29 APPENDIX 30 APPENDIX Table 1.1. Review of articles published between 2010-2015 that have directly referenced the temperature thresholds suggested by Renecker and Hudson (1986) to develop models or discuss climate related impacts on moose. Citing article Journal Context Lenarz et al., 2010 [16] Journal of Wildlife Management Lowe et al., 2010 [32] Canadian Journal of Zoology Bjørneraas et al., 2011 [42] Wildlife Biology Rolandsen et al., 2011 [67] Ecosphere Rempel 2011 [68] Ecological Modeling Broders et al., 2012 [10] Alces Murray et al., 2012 [18] Canadian Journal of Zoology van Beest et al., 2012 [17] Animal Behaviour Dou et al., 2013 [13] Ecological Research McCan et al., 2013 [27] Canadian Journal of Zoology van Beest et al., 2013 [28] PLoS ONE Melin et al., 2014 [29] Global Change Biology “These upper critical temperatures,” p. 1013 “Thus, when temperatures exceeded critical thresholds,” p.1033 “…thresholds that are regularly exceeded,” p. 51 “…respiratory rate, and metabolic rate when ambient temperatures rise above -5°C in winter and 14°C in summer,” p. 9 “In summer, thermal stress begins at about 14 °C and is fairly severe at 20 °C,” p. 3360 “…relatively low, upper critical temperature limit,” p. 54 “…upper critical temperature limits can cause,” p. 431 “Upper critical temperature thresholds for moose under captive conditions,” p. 724 “…critical temperature of the moose,” p. 630 “…indicate heat-stress thresholds for moose at 14 and 20 °C,” p. 893 “…locations were classified by temperature in relation to seasonal thermoregulation thresholds,” p. 3 “The thresholds for thermal stress in moose, as suggested by,” p. 1116 31 Table 1.1 (cont’d). Citing article Journal Olson et al., 2014 [69] Alces Street et al., 2015 [30] Journal of Wildlife Management Monteith et al., 2015 [70] Oecologia 32 Context “…estimated upper critical temperatures (Tuc) of moose as,” p 105 “…particularly at temperatures exceeding the upper thresholds,” p. 3 “…and have the lowest upper critical temperature of any northern ungulate,” p. 1145 Table 1.2. Counts of temperatures recorded by GPS collars of GPS-tracked moose in central Norway from July through August of each year during 2006-2010. Counts during crepuscular period (20:00 – 06:00 next day) above, counts of temperatures recorded during daytime period (08:00 – 18:00) middle, differences of temperature (ΔT) between crepuscular and daytime periods below. Year 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 Year 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 < 15 64 458 473 197 60 Tcrep (°C) [15:17) [17 - 19) [19 - 21) [21 - 23) [23 - 25) [25 - 27) [27 - 29) ≥ 29 123 306 514 546 537 301 99 27 808 1112 1312 875 653 307 164 464 980 1190 979 546 432 271 243 196 332 556 649 365 248 59 35 67 86 141 146 91 57 12 11 47 Tday (°C) < 21 [21 - 23) [23 - 25) [25 - 27) [27 - 29) [29 - 31) [31 - 33) [33 - 35) ≥ 35 94 94 94 94 94 94 94 94 94 591 591 591 591 591 591 591 591 591 681 681 681 681 681 681 681 681 681 290 290 290 290 290 290 290 290 290 63 63 63 63 63 63 63 63 63 Year 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 <1 86 259 206 105 30 [1 - 3) 283 768 614 352 76 [3 - 5) 103 311 336 156 36 [5 - 7) 65 185 207 84 20 ΔT (°C) [7 - 9) [9 - 11) [11 - 13) [13 - 15) ≥ 15 97 506 489 342 213 227 1385 1422 945 564 383 1148 1155 789 550 95 651 544 351 221 31 136 127 91 55 33 Table 1.3. Comparison of ARMA error structure for linear mixed models of crepuscular (left half) and daytime (right half) movements of moose, sorted by ascending AIC score. Model df AIC Response = ln(Movement Rate) ARMA(2,1) 11 33803.96 ARMA(2,2) 12 33805.50 ARMA(1,1) 10 33881.73 ARMA(3,0) 11 33956.68 ARMA(1,2) 11 33992.39 ARMA(0,3) 11 33998.61 ARMA(2,0) 10 33999.84 ARMA(1,0) 9 34040.00 ARMA(0,2) 10 34032.73 ARMA(0,1) 9 34086.27 ARMA(0,0)* 8 34491.44 Model df AIC ARMA(1,1) ARMA(2,1) ARMA(2,2) ARMA(3,0) ARMA(0,3) ARMA(2,0) ARMA(1,2) ARMA(0,2) ARMA(1,0) ARMA(0,1) ARMA(0,0)* 10 11 12 11 11 10 11 10 9 9 8 34916.56 34917.42 34920.34 34931.68 34937.96 34944.88 34948.26 34953.78 34989.14 34996.7 35065.97 Response = Straightness Index ARMA(1,1) 10 -529.156 ARMA(2,1) 11 -528.701 ARMA(2,2) 12 -526.248 ARMA(3,0) 11 -507.374 ARMA(0,3) 11 -505.425 ARMA(2,0) 10 -494.310 ARMA(0,2) 10 -492.866 ARMA(1,2) 11 -491.816 ARMA(1,0) 9 -485.026 ARMA(0,1) 9 -484.701 ARMA(0,0)* 8 -480.498 ARMA(1,1) ARMA(2,0) ARMA(0,2) ARMA(2,1) ARMA(3,0) ARMA(0,3) ARMA(1,0) ARMA(0,1) ARMA(1,2) ARMA(2,2) ARMA(0,0)* 10 10 10 11 11 11 9 9 11 12 8 -2063.72 -2062.65 -2062.47 -2061.73 -2061.5 -2061.46 -2061.22 -2060.92 -2060.65 -2059.92 -2052.76 Response = Mean turning angle ARMA(1,1) 10 -20749.8 ARMA(2,1) 11 -20748.1 ARMA(2,2) 12 -20746.6 ARMA(3,0) 11 -20690.2 ARMA(0,3) 11 -20687.5 ARMA(2,0) 10 -20686.0 ARMA(0,2) 10 -20684.2 ARMA(1,2) 11 -20683.4 ARMA(1,0) 9 -20673.8 ARMA(0,1) 9 -20672.7 ARMA(0,0)* 8 -20655.9 ARMA(1,1) ARMA(2,1) ARMA(2,2) ARMA(3,0) ARMA(0,3) ARMA(1,0) ARMA(0,1) ARMA(2,0) ARMA(0,2) ARMA(1,2) ARMA(0,0)* 10 11 12 11 11 9 9 10 10 11 8 -23009.4 -23007.4 -23006.3 -22987.4 -22986.6 -22982.2 -22982.1 -22981.5 -22981.2 -22979.5 -22976.1 * denotes global model with no autocorrelation structure 34 Table 1.4. Comparison of best approximating LMMs. The top three linear mixed models of movement rate (Rate) and mean turning angle (Turn) response variables during crepuscular and daytime movement metrics, sorted by increasing AIC score. Response Model df AIC Daytime movements + Elevation + Sex + Elevation + Sex + Month + Sex + Elevation + Sex + Month Elevation + Sex + Month Elevation + Sex 11 12 10 10 9 8 36261.37 36270.40 36270.55 167026.1 167027.1 167043.6 Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn Tday Tday Tday Tday Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn Tcrep Tcrep Tcrep Tcrep Tcrep Tcrep Crepuscular movements + Elevation + Sex + Month + Sex + Month + Elevation + Sex + Sex + Month + Elevation + Sex + Month + Sex 11 10 10 10 11 9 36727.61 36729.52 36767.34 158862.0 158863.3 158868.4 Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn Tday Tday Tday Tday Tday Tday + Elevation + Sex + Sex + Elevation + Sex + Sex + Elevation + Sex + Sex + Month + Month 11 10 10 10 11 9 35914.53 35924.94 35942.18 154935.6 154937.6 154948.4 Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn ΔT ΔT ΔT ΔT ΔT + Elevation + Sex + Sex + Elevation + Sex + Sex + Elevation + Sex Sex + Month + Month 11 10 10 10 11 9 36204.18 36208.70 36209.52 154990.4 154992.3 154993.7 + Month + Month + Month + Month + Month 35 Table 1.5. Comparison of best approximating AMMs. The top three additive mixed models of movement rate (Rate) and mean turning angle (Turn) response variables during crepuscular and daytime movement metrics, sorted by increasing AIC score. Response Model df AIC Daytime movements + s(Elevation) + Sex + Month + s(Elevation) + Sex + Sex s(Elevation) + Sex + Month + s(Elevation) + Sex + Month s(Elevation) + Sex 14 13 11 10 12 9 36206.89 36220.19 36241.85 167029.1 167030.1 167045.1 Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn Crepuscular movements s(Tcrep) + s(Elevation) + Sex + Month s(Tcrep) + Sex + Month s(Tcrep) + s(Elevation) + Sex s(Tcrep) + Sex + Month s(Tcrep) + s(Elevation) + Sex + Month s(Tcrep) + Sex 13 11 12 11 13 10 36693.45 36731.52 36731.62 158864 158867.3 158870.4 Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn s(Tday) s(Tday) s(Tday) s(Tday) s(Tday) s(Tday) + s(Elevation) + s(Elevation) 13 12 11 11 13 10 35872.33 35898.85 35926.94 154937.6 154941.6 154950.4 Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn s(ΔT) s(ΔT) s(ΔT) s(ΔT) s(ΔT) + s(Elevation) + s(Elevation) 13 12 11 11 13 11 36164.86 36170.36 36206.41 154992.4 154996.3 154997.7 Rate Rate Rate Turn Turn Turn s(Tday) s(Tday) s(Tday) s(Tday) + s(Elevation) + s(Elevation) s(Elevation) + Sex + Sex + Sex + Sex + Sex + Sex + Month + Sex + Sex + Sex + Sex + Sex + Sex + Month + Month + Month + Month + Month + Month + Month + Month 36 Table 1.6. Best approximating LMMs, daytime hours. Parameter estimates for best approximating linear mixed models of daytime movements, as determined by AIC. Max. daytime collar temperature (Tday) was the temperature covariate used in these models. Fixed effect Estimate SE Random effect Std. Dev. Movement Rate Intercept 3.714 Temp (Tday) -9.3E-3 Elevation 2.4E-4 Sex 0.424 0.041 Moose ID 9.6E-4 Year: Moose ID 2.4E-4 0.049 Intercept Intercept Residual 0.222 5.2E-4 0.696 Mean Turning Angle Intercept 92.58 Temp (Tday) -0.020 Elevation -8.8E-3 Sex -6.903 1.236 Moose ID 0.038 Year: Moose ID 1.8E-3 0.886 Intercept Intercept Residual 3.163 0.035 28.36 37 Table 1.7. Best approximating LMMs, crepuscular hours. Parameter estimates for best approximating linear mixed models of crepuscular movements, as determined by AIC. The temperature covariate used in each model is indicated in parentheses. Tcrep = max. crepuscular collar temperature, Tday = max. daytime collar temperature, and ΔT = Tday – Tcrep. Fixed effect Movement Rate Intercept Temp (Tcrep) Elevation Sex Month Estimate SE Random effect Std. Dev. 3.880 0.033 -1.6E-4 0.577 0.119 0.056 Moose ID 1.7E-3 Year: Moose ID 7.9E-5 0.064 0.018 Intercept Intercept Residual 0.261 7.0E-4 0.760 Mean Turning Angle Intercept 111.61 Temp (Tcrep) -0.485 Sex -5.774 Month 1.249 1.254 Moose ID 0.053 Year: Moose ID 0.693 0.451 Intercept Intercept Residual 1.828 1.299 24.36 0.050 Moose ID 1.0E-3 Year: Moose ID 7.9E-5 0.063 0.017 Intercept Intercept Residual 0.259 6.5E-4 0.759 1.073 Moose ID 0.035 Year: Moose ID 0.680 0.442 Intercept Intercept Residual 1.707 1.261 24.39 0.041 Moose ID 1.3E-3 Year: Moose ID 8.0E-5 0.061 0.018 Intercept Intercept Residual 0.244 9.1E-4 0.765 0.489 Moose ID 0.045 Year: Moose ID 0.668 0.433 Intercept Intercept Residual 1.562 1.491 24.42 Movement Rate Intercept Temp (Tday) Elevation Sex Month 4.069 0.020 -3.0E-4 0.570 0.098 Mean Turning Angle Intercept 108.4 Temp (Tday) -0.269 Sex -5.620 Month 1.740 Movement Rate Intercept 4.550 Temp (ΔT) 0.011 Elevation -2.1E-4 Sex 0.567 Month 0.049 Mean Turning Angle Intercept Temp (ΔT) Sex Month 101.3 -0.105 -5.606 2.584 38 Figure 1.1. Summer locations of GPS-tracked moose in Central Norway during 2006-2010. Norway is shaded light blue, other countries colored olive. 39 Figure 1.2. Mean ln-transformed speeds (± SE) of moose movement in Central Norway, during the months of July and August of each year from 2006-2010 (n = 152 moose). Speeds were estimated from continuous-time correlated random walk movement models fit to each summer track using the package crawl in R. 40 Figure 1.3. Boxplots of daytime (a) and crepuscular (b) movement metrics calculated from GPS data on moose tracked during July-August of 2006-2010. Movement metrics are binned by maximum collar temperature recorded during the respective period for which the movement metric was calculated. 41 Figure 1.4. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs (± 95% CI shaded grey) for movement rates of male moose at median elevation during daytime (a) and crepuscular periods (b-d). Daytime movement rates predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular movement rates predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). 42 Figure 1.5. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs (± 95% CI shaded grey) for mean turning angles of male moose at median elevation during daytime (a) and crepuscular periods (b-d). Daytime turning angles predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular turn angles predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). 43 Figure 1.6. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs of movement rates calculated from probabilistic movement paths. Predictions for daytime (a) and crepuscular (b-d) movement rates of male moose. Daytime movement rates predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular movement rates predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). 44 Figure 1.7. Predictions based on best approximating AMMs of mean turning angles calculated from probabilistic movement paths. Predictions for daytime (a) and crepuscular (b-d) movement rates of male moose. Daytime movement rates predicted as a function of daytime temperature, and crepuscular movement rates predicted as a function of crepuscular temperature (b), daytime temperature (c), and the difference between daytime and crepuscular temperature (d). 45 REFERENCES 46 REFERENCES 1) Bakkestuen, V., Erikstad, L., Halvorsen, R. 2008. Step-less models for regional environmental variation in Norway. Journal of Biogeography, 35:1906 – 1922. 2) Bjørneraas, K., Herfindal, I., Solberg, E. J., Sæther, B.-E., van Moorter, B., Rolandsen, C. M. Habitat quality influences population distribution, individual space use, and functional responses in habitat selection by a large herbivore. Oecologia, 168:231 – 243. 3) Bjørneraas, K., Solberg, E. J., Herfindal, I., Van Moorter, B., Rolandsen, C. M., Tremblay, J.-P., Skarpe, C., Sæther, B.-E., Eriksen, R., Astrup, R. 2011. Moose Alces alces habitat use at multiple temporal scales in a human-altered landscape. Wildlife Biology, 17:44 – 54. 4) Bjørneraas, K., van Moorter, B., Rolandsen, C. M., Herfindal, I. 2010. Screening global positioning system location data for errors using animal movement characteristics. Journal of Wildlife Management, 74:1361 – 1366. 5) Broders, H. G., Coombs, A. B., McCarron, J R. 2012. Ectothermic responses of moose (Alces alces) to thermoregulatory stress on mainland Nova Scotia. Alces, 48:53 – 61. 6) Brinkman, T. J., Deperno, C. S., Jenks, J. A., Haroldson, B. S., Osborn, R. G. 2005. Movement of female white-tailed deer: effects of climate and intensive row-crop agriculture. Journal of Wildlife Management, 69:1099 – 1111. 7) Bunnefeld, N., Börger, L., van Moorter, B., Rolandsen, C. M., Dettki, H., Solberg, E. J., Ericsson, G. 2011. A model-driven approach to quantify migration patterns: individual, regional and yearly differences. Journal of Animal Ecology, 80:466 – 476. 8) Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R. 2003. Model selection and multimodal inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. 9) Cederlund, G. 1989. Activity patterns in moose and roe deer in a north boreal forest. Holarctic Ecology, 12:39 – 45. 10) DelGiudice, G. D., Riggs, M. R., Joly, P., Pan, W. 2002. Winter severity, survival, and cause-specific mortality of female white-tailed deer in north-central Minnesota. Journal of Wildlife Management, 66:698 - 717. 11) DelGiudice, G. D., Sampson, B. A., Lenarz, M. S., Schrage, M. W., Edwards, A. J. 2011. Winter body condition of moose (Alces alces) in a declining population in northeastern Minnesota. Journal of Wildlife Diseases, 47:30 – 40. 12) Demarchi, M. W., Bunnell, F. L. 1995. Forest cover selection and activity of cow moose in summer. Acta Theriologica, 40:23 – 36. 47 13) Dou, H., Jiang, G., Stott. P., Piao, R. 2013. Climate change impacts population dynamics and distribution shift of moose (Alces alces) in Heilongjiang Province of China. Ecological Research, 28:625 – 632. 14) Dussault, C., Ouellet, J.-P., Courtois, R., Huot, J., Breton, L., Larochelle, J. 2004. Behavioural responses of moose to thermal conditions in the boreal forest. Ecoscience, 11:321 – 328. 15) Ensing, E. P., Ciuti, S., de Wijs, F. A. L. M., Lentferink, D., ten Hoedt, A., Boyce, M. S., Hut, R. A. 2014. GPS based daily activity patterns in European red deer and North American elk (Cervus elaphus): indication for a weak circadian clock in ungulates. PLoS ONE, 9, e1D6997. 16) Ericsson, G., Dettki, H., Neumann, W., Arnemo, J. M., Singh, N. J. 2015. Offset between GPS collar-recorded temperature in moose and ambient weather station data. European Journal of Wildlife Research, 61:919 – 922. 17) Fewster, R. M., Buckland, S. T., Siriwardena, G. M., Baillie, S. R., Wilson, J. D. 2000. Analysis of population trends for farmland birds using generalized additive models. Ecology, 81:1970 – 1984. 18) Gurarie, E. 2016. What is the animal doing? Tools for exploring behavioral structure in animal movements. Journal of Animal Ecology, 85 (1):69 – 84. 19) Gurarie, E., Bracis, C., Delgado, M., Meckley, T. D., Kojola, I., Wagner, C. M. 2016. What is the animal doing? Tools for exploring behavioral structure in animal movements. Journal of Animal Ecology, 85:69 – 84. 20) Harley, C. D. G., Hughes, A. R., Hultgren, K. M., Miner, B. J., Sorte, C. J. B., Thornber, C. S., Rodriguez, L. F., Tomanek, L., Williams, S. L. 2006. The impacts of climate change in coastal marine systems. Ecology Letters, 9:228 – 241. 21) Humphries, M. M., Umbanhowar, J., McCann, K. S. 2004. Bioenergetic prediction of climate change impacts on northern mammals. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 44:152 – 162. 22) Johnson, D. S., London, J. M., Lea, M.-A., Durban, J. W. 2008. Continuous-time correlated random walk model for animal telemetry data. Ecology, 89:1208 – 1215. 23) Karns, P. D. 2007. Population distribution, density and trends In: A. W. Franzmann and C. C. Schwartz (Eds.), Ecology and Management of the North American Moose (Second edition; pp. 125 – 139). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution. 24) Lankester, M. W. 2010. Understanding the impact of meningeal worm, Parelaphostrongylus tenuis, on moose populations. Alces, 46:53 – 70. 48 25) Large, S. I., Fay, G., Friedland, K. D., Link, J. S. 2013. Defining trends and thresholds in responses of ecological indicators to fishing and environmental pressures. ICES Journal of Marine Science, 70:755 – 767. 26) Leblond, M., Dussault, C., Ouellet, J.-P. 2010. What drives fine-scale movements of large herbivores? A case study using moose. Ecography, 33:1102 – 1112. 27) Leblond, M., St-Laurent, M. H., Côté, S. D. 2016. Caribou, water, and ice – fine-scale movements of a migratory arctic ungulate in the context of climate change. Movement Ecology, 4:14. DOI:10.1186/s40462-016-0079-4. 28) Lenarz, M. S., Fieberg, J., Schrage, M. W., Edwards, A. J. 2010. Living on the edge: viability of moose in Northeastern Minnesota. Journal of Wildlife Management, 74:1013 – 1023. 29) Lenarz, M. S., Nelson, M. E., Schrage, M. W., Edwards, A. J. 2009. Temperature mediated moose survival in Northeastern Minnesota. Journal of Wildlife Management, 73:503 – 510. 30) Lowe, S. J., Patterson, B. R., Schaefer, J. A. 2010. Lack of behavioral responses of moose (Alces alces) to high ambient temperatures near the southern periphery of their range. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 88:1032 – 1041. 31) McCann, N. P., Moen, R. A., Harris, T. R.2013. Warm-season heat stress in moose (Alces alces). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 91:893 – 898. 32) McKellar, A. E., Reudink, M. W., Marra, P. P., Ratcliffe, L. M., Wilson, S. 2005. Climate and density influence annual survival and movement in a migratory songbird. Ecology and Evolution, 5:5892 – 5904. 33) Melin, M., Matala, J., Mehätalo, L., Tiilikainen, R., Tikkanen, O.-P., Maltamo, M., Pusenius, J., Packalen, P. 2014. Moose (Alces alces) reacts to high summer temperatures by utilizing thermal shelters in boreal forests – an analysis based on airborne laser scanning of the canopy structure at moose locations. Global Change Biology, 20:1115 – 1125. 34) Mech, D. L., Fieburg, J. 2014. Re-evaluating the northeastern Minnesota moose decline and the role of wolves. Journal of Wildlife Management. 78:1143 – 1150. 35) McGraw, A. M., Moen, R. A., Overland, L. 2012. Effective temperature differences among cover types in northeast Minnesota. Alces, 48:45 – 52. 36) Moen A. 1999. National Atlas of Norway: Vegetation. Hønefoss, Norway: Norwegian Mapping Authority. 37) Monteith, K., Klaver, R. W., Hersey, K. R., Holland, A. A., Thomas, T. P., Kauffman, M. J. 2015. Effects of climate and plant phenology on recruitment of moose at the southern extent of their range. Oecologia, 178:1137 – 1148. 49 38) Morales, J. M., Haydon, D. T., Frair, J., Holsinger, K. E., Fryxell, J. M. 2004. Extracting more out of relocation data: building movement models as mixtures of random walks. Ecology, 85:2436 – 2445. 39) Murray, D. L., Cox, E. W., Ballard, W. B., Whitlaw, H. A., Lenarz, M. S., Custer, T. W., Barnett, T., Fuller, T. K. 2006. Pathogens, nutritional deficiency, and climate influences on a declining moose population. Wildlife Monographs, 166:1 – 30. 40) Murray, D. L., Hussey, K. F., Finnegan, L. A., Lowe, S. J., Price, G. N., Benson, J., Loveless, K. M., Middel, K. R., Mills, K., Potter, D., Silver, A., Fortin, M.-J., Patterson, B. R., Wilson, P. J. 2012. Assessment of the status and viability of a population of moose (Alces alces) at its southern range limit in Ontario. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 90:422 – 434. 41) Nathan, R., Getz, W. M., Revilla, E., Holyoak, M., Kadmon, R., Saltz, D., Smouse, P. 2008. A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105:19052 – 19059. 42) Nathan, R., Giuggioli, L. 2013. A milestone for movement ecology research. Movement Ecology, 1:1. DOI:10.1186/2051-3933-1-1. 43) Olson, B., Windels, S. K., Fulton, M., Moen, R. 2014. Fine-scale temperature patterns in the southern boreal forest: implications for the cold-adapted moose. Alces, 50:105 – 120. 44) Parmesan, C., Yohe, G. 2003. A globally coherent fingerprint of climate change impacts across natural systems. Nature, 421:37 – 42. 45) Patterson, T. A., Basson, M., Bravington, M. V., Gunn, J. S. 2009. Classifying movement behavior in relation to environmental conditions using hidden Markov models. Journal of Animal Ecology, 78:1113 – 1123. 46) Pettorelli, N., Mysterud, A., Yoccoz, N. G., Langvatn, R., Stenseth, N. C. 2005. Importance of climatological downscaling and plant phenology for red deer in heterogeneous landscapes. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 272:2357 – 2364. 47) Renecker, L. A., Hudson, R. J. 1986. Seasonal energy expenditures and thermoregulatory responses of moose. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 64:332 – 327. 48) Renecker, L. A., Hudson, R. J. 1990. Behavioral and thermoregulatory responses of moose to high ambient temperatures and insect harassment in aspen-dominated forests. Alces, 26:66 – 72. 49) Pinheiro, J. C., Bates, D. M. 2000. Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-PLUS: Statistics and Computing. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. 50) Pinheiro, J. C., Bates, D. M., Debroy, S., Sarkar, D., R Core Team. 2015. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-121. 50 51) R Core Team. 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/. 52) Rempel, R. 2011. Effects of climate change on moose populations: Exploring the response horizon through biometric and systems models. Ecological Modelling, 222:3355 – 3365. 53) Rolandsen, C. M., Solberg, E. J., Herfindal, I., Van Moorter, B., Sæther, B.-E. 2011. Largescale spatiotemporal variation in road mortality of moose: Is it all about population density? Ecosphere, DOI:10.1890/ES11-00196.1. 54) Rolandsen, C. M., Solberg, E. J., Sæther, B.-E., Van Moorter, B., Herfindal, I., Bjørneraas, K. 2016. On fitness and partial migration in a large herbivore – migratory moose have higher reproductive performance than residents. Oikos, DOI:10.1111/oik.02996. 55) Root, T. L., Price, J. T., Hall, K. R., Schneider, S. H., Rosensweig, C., Pounds, J. A. 2003. Fingerprints of global warming on wild animals and plants. Nature, 421:57 – 60. 56) Sand, H. 1996. Life history patterns in female moose (Alces alces): the relationship between age, body size, fecundity and environmental conditions. Oecologia, 106:212 – 220. 57) Schick, R. S., Loarie, S. R., Colchero, F., Best, B. D., Boustany, A., Conde, D. A., Halpin. P. N., Joppa, L. N., McClellan, C. M., Clark, J. S. 2008. Understanding movement data and movement processes: current and emerging directions. Ecology Letters, 11:1338 – 1350. 58) Schwartz, C. C., Renecker, L. A. 2007. Nutrition and energetics In: A. W. Franzmann and C. C. Schwartz (Eds.), Ecology and Management of the North American Moose (2nd edition; pp. 441 – 478). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution. 59) Solberg, E. J., Heim, M., Grøtan, V., Sæther, B.-E., Garel, M. 2007. Annual variation in maternal age and calving date generate cohort effects in moose (Alces alces) body mass. Oecologia, 154:259 – 271. 60) Street, G. M., Rodgers, A. R., Fryxell, J. M. 2015. Mid-day temperature variation influences seasonal habitat selection by moose. Journal of Wildlife Management, 79:505 – 512. 61) Testa, J. W., Adams, G. P. 1998. Body condition and adjustments to reproductive effort in female moose (Alces alces). Journal of Mammalogy, 79:1345 – 1354. 62) Thomas, C. D., Cameron., A., Green, R. E., Bakkenes, M., Beaumont, L. J., Collingam, Y. C., Erasmus, B. F. N., de Siqueira, M. F., Grainger, A., Hannah, L., Hughes, L., Huntley, B., van Jaarsveld, A. S., Midgely, G. F., Miles, L., Ortega-Huerta, M. A., Peterson, A. T., Philips, O. L., Williams, S. E. 2004. Extinction risk from climate change. Nature, 427:145 – 148. 51 63) Thurfjell, H., Spong, G., Ericsson, G. 2014. Effects of weather, season, and daylight on female wild boar movement. Acta Theriologica, 59:467 – 472. 64) van Beest, F. M., Milner, J. M. 2013. Behavioral responses to thermal conditions affect seasonal mass change in a heat-sensitive northern ungulate. PLoS ONE, DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0065972. 65) van Beest, F. M., Van Moorter, B., Milner, J. M. 2012. Temperature-mediated habitat use and selection by a heat-sensitive northern ungulate. Animal Behavior, 84:723 – 735. 66) Van Moorter, B., Bunnefeld, N., Panzacchi, M., Rolandsen, C. M., Solberg, E. J., Sæther, B.E. 2013. Understanding scales of movement: animals ride waves and ripples of environmental change. Journal of Animal Ecology, 4:770 – 780. 67) Van Moorter, B., Visscher, D. R., Jerde, C. L., Frair, J. L., Merrill, E. H. 2010. Identifying movement states from location data using cluster analysis. Journal of Wildlife Management, 74:588 – 594. 68) Whitlaw, H. A., Lankester, M. W. 1994. The co-occurrence of moose, white-tailed deer and Parelaphostrongylus tenuis in Ontario. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 72:819 – 825. 69) Wood, S. N. 2003. Thin plate regression splines. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society (B), 65:95 – 114. 70) Wood, S. N. 2011. Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society (B), 73:3 – 36. 52 CHAPTER 2 EVALUATING THE CONSEQUENCES ON ECOLOGICAL INFERENCE OF AGGREGATING WILDLIFE TELEMETRY DATA WHEN ESTIMATING RESOURCE SELECTION FUNCTIONS Abstract Telemetry data are commonly used to assess animal-habitat relationships via the quantification resource selection functions (RSFs). As the output of these models are often designed to inform conservation or management practice, inference is typically desired at the population-level. Thereby, it has become common practice to aggregate data from all telemetered animals prior to fitting an RSF. To account for individual variation in selection among telemetered animals, these models typically include a random effect by animal id. These approaches are valuable for quantifying broad scale selection, but when the focal population may be comprised of various intrinsic categories (e.g. sex or age class) or clustered spatially (e.g. two sub-populations occupying different areas of the landscape), information relating to individual animal decision-making may be obscured by the act of aggregating data. Here I investigated individual variation in resource selection among a population of reintroduced elk (Cervus elaphus) in the Missouri Ozarks. I modeled elk location data, collected from Global Positioning System (GPS) collars, using a Bayesian discrete choice RSF model. I fit an aggregate-level model, according to prevailing practice, and then batch-processed models at the level of each individual elk. I compared the outputs of the top aggregate- and individual-level models via examination of three metrics; 1) the number and composition of parameters in top models, 2) the estimates of parameters in global models, and 3) the predicted relative probabilities of use. Via 53 this comparison I discovered substantial variation in all three metrics. Furthermore, I could not detect any conformity at the individual level by age, sex, or year of reintroduction and telemetrycollaring. My work demonstrates that important ecological variation is lost when resource selection analyses are aggregated at the population level. I discuss the implications of this analysis for management and conservation practice and present some guiding principles for developing RSFs at the individual level. 54 2.1. Introduction Research on animal-habitat relationships is a cornerstone of ecological inquiry providing insights across both theoretical and applied dimensions (Johnson 1980; Morris 2003). Evaluating animal decision-making in relation to habitat characteristics has implications for optimal foraging, predator-prey interactions, survivorship, reproduction, life history, and, correspondingly, population-level processes (MacArthur and Pianka 1966; Charnov 1976; Rosenzweig 1981, 1991; Morris 2003). Applied assessments of animal-habitat relationships are predicated on the ability to track animal movement over time. While a variety of data collection methods inform these efforts, much of habitat selection research rely upon telemetry data (Craighead and Craighead 1971; Craighead et al., 1971; Kenward 2001; Thomas and Taylor 2006; Montgomery and Roloff 2013). Telemetry technology has expanded dramatically in the last 50 years with subsequent growth in the methods necessary to model these data. Sophisticated and complex models have been developed to rigorously quantify animal movement, selection, habitat suitability, as well as population abundance and performance (Hirzel and Lay 2008; Gaillard et al., 2010; Thurfjell et al., 2014; Avgar et al., 2016; Boyce et al., 2016). Examples of the formative techniques for quantifying habitat selection include compositional analysis (Aebischer et al., 1993), Chi-square goodness-of-fit tests (Neu et al., 1974; Byers et al., 1984), ranking methods (Arthur et al., 1996), and logistic regression (Thomasma et al., 1991). Many of these models operate in a similar fashion in that used habitat units (e.g., at locations detected via telemetry) are compared to habitat units which is deemed to be either unused or available (i.e., locations not detected via telemetry). The statistical comparison of used to unused/available was unified under the broader resource selection function (RSF) framework (Boyce and McDonald 1999; Manly et al., 2002). Herein, the RSF 55 models the use versus unused/available data as a function of environmental variables, or resources, where the output is a value proportional to the probability that location is used given the resources present (Manly et al., 2002). In other words, an RSF is always proportional to an RSPF (resource probability selection function) that quantifies the actual probability of using landscape units. The ability to accurately estimate an RSPF, which can be desirable in a number of scenarios, is determined by the extent to which non-use habitat units can be reliably quantified (Manly et al., 2002). Given that estimation of availability is typically much more feasible than determination of non-use in wildlife studies, the use/available framework tends to be most common (Johnson 1980; Thomas and Taylor 1990, 2006). In this way, RSFs can provide insight into the combinations of resources that are necessary to sustain wildlife populations (Manly et al., 2002).While there are important sampling elements to consider including the ways in which available or unused habitat units are measured, the RSF framework has become a widely-used approach among wildlife ecologists to model animal-habitat relationships (Erickson et al., 2001; Keating and Cherry, 2004; Arts et al., 2008; Fieburg et al., 2010; Montgomery and Roloff, 2013). Given that management and conservation efforts are typically developed at the population-level, researchers typically deploy telemetry collars/tags on a number of animal subjects and then aggregate the resultant telemetry data at the population-level to fit one RSF (Thomas and Taylor 2006). Importantly, aggregating telemetry data across animal subjects obscures any individual variation that may exist in the ways in which these animals respond to the environment. This can bias inference, particularly when relocation data are unbalanced across individual animals, as is often the case (Gillies et al., 2006; Aarts et al., 2008). To account for individual variation, researchers may fit models where animal id is included as a random effect. 56 This random effects component (fit as either a random intercept or random slope) relaxes the assumption of independence among data points, potentially mediating issues with unbalanced sampling efforts by allowing parameters in an RSF to vary according to an aggregate-level probability distribution (also called population-level or top-level; Mysterud and Ims, 1998; Gillies et al., 2006; Thomas, Johnson and Griffith 2006; Aarts et al., 2008; Hebblewhite and Merrill 2008; Duchesne et al., 2010; Hooten et al., 2016). However, adjustments to the slope or intercept of a model may be insufficient to account for the true variation inherent to the input data. At risk here is the potential to misidentify resource selection which can confound inference and problematize prevailing management or conservation philosophy. As Marzluff et al. (2004) note, individuality in wildlife resource selection can be extreme, warranting an examination of the consequences of that individuality. Here I investigated the consequences of aggregating animals, where variation is assumed to be constrained statistically, in RSF analyses. Modeling elk (Cervus elaphus) resource selection in a reintroduced population in southern Missouri, I compared aggregate-level to individual animal RSFs. Using a hierarchical discrete choice model to evaluate these RSFs, I treated individuals as random effects at the aggregate level according to conventional practice. I then fit the same model at the level of each individual elk to compare variation in selection tendencies using multivariate techniques. I based this comparison on three areas of inference common to resource selection analyses: 1) the number and composition of parameters in top models, 2) the estimated RSF parameters of global models, and 3) the predicted relative probabilities of use. I discuss the ramifications of variation across these three metrics for management and conversation practice. Finally, I provide guidance on the framing of future analyses of RSFs and habitat selection research more broadly. 57 2.2. Methods 2.2.1. Resource selection data Elk (n = 88) were captured in Kentucky for relocation to the Missouri Ozarks in June of 2011 (14 males, 19 females), 2012 (8 males, 26 females), and 2013 (3 males, 36 F). After a period of acclimation in a fenced enclosure, these elk were introduced onto Peck Ranch Conservation Area (Fremont, MO), a 9,327 ha plot of land managed by the Missouri Department of Conservation. Both of these areas are contained within the broader elk restoration zone, an 896 km2 study area in southeast Missouri delimited by the Missouri Department of Conservation because of the region’s high density of abundant public lands and high predicted habitat suitability for elk (MDC 2010). Prior to release, all elk ≥ 1 year old were fitted with a GPS collar (RASSL custom 3D cell collar, North Star Science and Technology, LLC, King George, VA, or G2110E Iridium/GPS series model, Advanced Telemetry Systems, Insanti, MN). These collars were set to a 2.5 hour fix attempt schedule, storing locations internally at this frequency and uploading information every 5 hours (aside from two collars, set at 2 and 4 hours, respectively). Capture and handling protocols were approved by the University of Missouri Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol 6909). Environmental variables were measured at the used locations (as determined by the GPS telemetry system) and available locations, which were defined using the radius of available habitat method (Durner et al., 2009). This involved delimiting a circle around each used location having radius equal to 𝑐(𝑎 + 2𝑏), where a, b, and c represent the mean hourly movement rate, the standard deviation of the movement rate, and the number of hours between locations, respectively. Locations were then randomly sampled from within these circles to determine the corresponding available locations for the subsequent used locations, hereafter referred to as a “choice set”. 58 2.2.2. Environmental variables I used a geographic database of environmental variables developed by Smith (2015) to describe the study area. These included 11 different variables each depicted as rasters at a resolution of 30 m. This database included percent tree canopy cover (2011 US Forest Service National Land Cover Database (www. mrlc.gov/nlcd11_data.php, accessed 8 Jan 2015), number of years since prescribed burn, aspect (degrees), slope (percent), distance to wooded edge (m), the interspersion and juxtaposition index (IJI; Griffith et al., 2000), road density (km paved or gravel road/km2 within 95 km2 circle), distance to paved road (m), distance to closed two-track roads (m), and distance to public gravel roads (m; for more information see Smith (2015)). Finally, I also considered habitat type, the only categorical variable considered, which had eight categories: warm-season grassland, cool-season grassland, shrubland, woodland, savannah, forest, glade, and forage opening. 2.2.3. RSF modeling I fit discrete choice RSFs because they can accommodate availability data that is defined separately for each location, which can provide a more realistic approximation of animal choice (Cooper and Millspaugh, 1999, 2001; McCracken et al., 1998). I selected a Bayesian framework given the difficulties of likelihood-based approaches and the flexibility of Bayesian methods when fitting random-effects logistic regression models (Browne and Draper 2005; Gelman et al., 2014a). For every used location I developed 5 available locations to define each discrete choice set. I developed the RSFs at the aggregate- and individual-levels. The individual-level model is defined as follows, where the probability of an individual elk choosing alternative 𝑙 from a set of 𝐶 feasible habitat units at unit 𝑖 is given by 59 𝑃𝑖𝑙 = ∑𝐶 𝑒 𝜷𝑿𝑖𝑙 𝑐=1 𝑒 (1) 𝜷𝑿𝑖𝑐 where 𝜷𝑿𝑖𝑙 = 𝛽1 𝑋𝑖𝑙1 + 𝛽2 𝑋𝑖𝑙2 + ⋯ + 𝛽𝑘 𝑋𝑖𝑙𝑘 (2) is the utility of unit 𝑙 to the individual being considered, consisting of k slope parameters measured on each used and available unit. The concept of utility derives from economic theory and is analogous to satisfaction; eqn. (1) is valid under the assumption that the individual in question always chooses the resource unit having the greatest utility (Cooper and Millspaugh 1999). I slightly extended eqns. (1) and (2) to develop the aggregate-level model, where I make explicit the probability of individual 𝑗 choosing alternative 𝑙 from a set of 𝐶 feasible alternatives to unit 𝑖, defined as 𝑃𝑖𝑗𝑙 = 𝜷 𝑿 𝑒 𝒋 𝑖𝑗𝑙 (3) 𝜷𝒋 𝑿𝑖𝑗𝑐 ∑𝐶 𝑐=1 𝑒 with utility function now defined as 𝜷𝑗 𝑿𝑖𝑗𝑙 = 𝛽𝑗1 𝑋𝑖𝑗𝑙1 + 𝛽𝑗2 𝑋𝑖𝑙𝑗2 + ⋯ + 𝛽𝑗𝑘 𝑋𝑖𝑗𝑙𝑘 (4) and 𝛽𝑗𝑘 ~𝑁𝑜𝑟𝑚𝑎𝑙(𝜇𝑘 , 𝜎𝑘2 ) where the individual parameters for selection of environmental variable 𝑘 (the 𝛽𝑗𝑘 for all 𝑗 = 1, 2, … , 88 individuals), are assumed to be normally-distributed random effects following some population distribution. The parameters of the population distributions for each environmental variable 𝑘 (𝜇𝑘 and 𝜎𝑘2 ) are referred to as “hyperparameters”(Hobbs and Hooten 2015). Thus, the aggregate-level model was a hierarchical random slopes model, where inference can be made on 60 the central tendency of selection for each environmental variable 𝑘, or the “mean hyperparameter” 𝜇𝑘 , as well as variation among individuals, or the “standard deviation hyperparameter” 𝜎𝑘 . I conducted aggregate-level model estimation using the following uninformative priors for all hyperparameters: 𝜇𝑘 ~𝑁𝑜𝑟𝑚𝑎𝑙(0, 10) (5) 𝜎𝑘 ~𝑈𝑛𝑖𝑓𝑜𝑟𝑚(0, 10) Prior to model fitting, I examined evident collinearity among the environmental variables on a per-individual basis, and excluded redundant environmental variables until all pairwise correlations were |r| ≤ 0.6. I then constructed a global model at the aggregate and individual levels consisting of all remaining environmental variables such that this condition was satisfied. I fit these models in the Bayesian package Stan (Stan Development Team 2016a) using R and RStan as an interface (R Core Team 2016; Stan Development Team 2016b). For each individual model, I used four chains of 1000 draws each, with a burn-in period of 200. This was likely more simulation than necessary, given the simple structure of the models (i.e. nonhierarchical) and in most cases 1000 iterations is more than enough to reach convergence to the posterior distribution using Stan (Vehtari et al., 2016). However, I used this approach to ensure that any individual differences that may arise in the comparisons (next section) would not be attributable to Monte Carlo error. Additionally, to fit all models, I used the high-performance computing cluster developed and maintained by the Institute for Cyber-Enabled Research at Michigan State University. With the large number of processors and excess RAM, I was able to fit the models in parallel and remotely. I assessed convergence of all models by ensuring that for all parameters the potential scale reduction factor, 𝑅̂ , was below 1.1 and the effective sample 61 size, 𝑛̂𝑒𝑓𝑓 , was greater than 100 (Gelman et al., 2013). I assessed goodness of model fits using posterior predictive checks, whereby I computed the probability that a test statistic, 𝑇, calculated on new data simulated from the model, 𝒚∗ , is more extreme than 𝑇 calculated on observed data, y (Gelman et al., 2013; Hobbs and Hooten 2015). I used the chi-square test statistic to conduct these checks for the aggregate-level global model and all individual-level global models as follows Pr(𝑇(𝒚∗ , 𝜽) ≥ 𝑇(𝒚, 𝜽)|𝒚) 𝑇(𝒚, 𝜽) = ∑𝑖 (6) (𝑦𝑖 −𝑝𝑖 )2 𝑝𝑖 where 𝜽 represents the parameters for the fitted model, and 𝑝𝑖 is the probability associated with the 𝑖 th choice. The first expression in (6) returns a Bayesian “𝑝-value”, 𝑃𝐵 , which I used to diagnose lack of model fit (Gelman et al., 2013; Hobbs and Hooten 2015). 2.2.4. Comparison between aggregate- and individual-level RSFs I assessed patterns of individual variation in resource selection among the individual and aggregate levels by comparing: 1) the parameters included in top models, 2) parameter estimates of all global models, and 3) predictions of the relative probability of use expressed across the study area. To make comparisons based on the model selection approach, I fit all subsets of the global model for each individual following examination for collinearity, carrying out estimation as described above to perform what was essentially a “dredge” (Wiens et al., 2008; Cade 2015). I ranked models using WAIC, a fully Bayesian estimate of out-of-sample predictive ability, determining the top model for each individual to be that with the lowest WAIC score (Watanabe 2010; Vehtari et al., 2015; Gelman et al., 2014b). I evaluated the inclusion of parameters in the 62 top and runner-up models by calculating “inclusion rates” in the top 5% of models for each individual. To make this calculation, I ordered all models by increasing WAIC score and, using only the models contained in the top 5% of this ordering, divided the number of models in which a parameter was included by the total number of models in this list. I used WAIC to rank models because it is both fully Bayesian and computationally efficient (Vehtari et al., 2015). I also performed this selection procedure for the aggregate-level model, where sub-models retained the same hierarchical format used in the global model, but varied with respect to the RSF parameters and associated hyperparameters. I compared individual variation in parameter estimates to the estimated aggregate-level distribution on a per-parameter basis using a graphical approach. As an aggregate-level estimate of the individual variation in parameter estimates, I added the lower and upper limits of the mean hyperparameter CIs to their corresponding limits for the standard deviation hyperparameters for (𝑙) (𝑙) (𝑢) (𝑢) each environmental variable, giving the following interval [𝜇̂ 𝑘 − 𝜎̂𝑘 , 𝜇̂ 𝑘 + 𝜎̂𝑘 ] for all k parameters, where l and u represent the lower and upper limits for the 95% CIs, respectively. This calculates conservative intervals within which one standard deviation of the individual random effects (~ 66% of theoretical individuals’ parameters) is expected to be contained under the aggregate-level model assumptions. I refer to this metric as the 95% “random-effect CI”, as it characterizes the random-effect distributions with uncertainty around the hyperparameter estimates incorporated. Additionally, I investigated patterns of estimated selection tendencies (i.e., the composites of individual parameter estimates) among individuals using principal components analysis on the RSF parameter point-estimates. Ordination in parameter space has been used previously to investigate variation in selection and movement patterns among individuals (Hanks et al., 2011; Pape and Löffler 2015). I only used the individual-level 63 estimates for which uncertainty was comparable among individuals, and I compare among intrinsic factors and year of GPS-collaring and release, hereafter referred to as “cohort”. For the final comparison, I produced predictive maps of the relative probabilities of use for each top model across the entire study area. I evaluated the logistic of the estimated RSFs, exp(𝑅𝑆𝐹) /(1 + exp(𝑅𝑆𝐹), at each 30m resolution cell within the study area, returning the predicted relative probability of use of that unit under the estimated model(s). 2.3. Results The 88 elk in this study were GPS-tracked between June 1, 2011 and September 15, 2014 (35 elk released in 2011 (22 female, 13 male), 24 elk released in 2012 (17 female, 7 male), and 29 elk released in 2013 (26 female, 3 male)). At the aggregate level, I fit the global discrete choice RSF model and sub-models using 141,197 choice sets consisting of 1 used location and 5 available locations. I detected high collinearity (0.61 ≤ |𝑟| ≤ 0.84.) between the distance to gravel road and road density variables for all individuals, and I excluded the former variable from consideration given that two other variables (distance to paved road and distance to two-track road) quantified proximity to roads. Thus, an identical set of sub-models was fit to all individuals. Individual-level models were estimated based on 95 to 4865 choice sets depending on the elk, and more than 50% of the models were fit with between 783 and 2071 choice sets. I achieved convergence of all models fit (𝑅̂ < 1.1 for all parameters at the individual level and hyperparameters at the aggregate level). The Bayesian p-value test for the global aggregate-level model indicated no lack of fit, i.e. could have reasonably generated the observed data (𝑃𝐵 = 0.35, Hobbs and Hooten 2015). Bayesian p-values for all individual-level global models fell between 0.16 and 0.58, with an average value of 0.33. 64 2.3.1. Number and composition of parameters The global model at the aggregate level had the highest predictive ability of all submodels considered (see Table 2.1), while the top individual-level models had an average of 14.4 parameters with a range of 7 to 17 (Fig. 2.1a). The order of importance of parameters for predictive ability was also mixed, although habitat and slope were in all but two and five of top models, respectively. Conversely, aspect was only included as a predictor in the top model for 44% of the elk (39 of the 88 top models). The exclusion of any one parameter from top models did not appear to be linked with patterns of exclusion for other parameters. The remaining parameters were included in top models for between 62% and 86% of the elk (between 54 and 76 of the 88 top models, respectively). Habitat and slope also had the highest inclusion rates, having a value of 1.0 for the majority of elk (i.e., included in the top model and all of the 5% runner-up models; Fig. 2.1b) I observed low inclusion rates (≤ 0.2) of the distance to two-track roads parameter for four elk and the distance to paved roads parameter for three elk, as well as IJI for one elk (Fig. 2.1b). 2.3.2. Parameter estimates At the aggregate level, thirteen of the seventeen 𝜇𝑘 estimates under the global model had 95% credible intervals (CIs) which did not overlap zero. These included estimates for all 𝜇𝑘 for continuous environmental variables as well as all categorical habitat types except forest, shrubland, glade, and warm-season grassland. However, the estimated 95% random-effect CIs contained zero in only two cases: slope and forage opening. Aggregate-level point estimates of individual variation (the 𝜎𝑘 estimates) varied from a minimum of 0.04 for aspect, to 1.71 for distance to two-track. The latter was large relative to estimates of 𝜎𝑘 for the remaining environmental variables, as all other estimates fell below 0.67 (Fig. 2). There was a high degree 65 of variation in the individual-level estimates of selection, and point estimates of the habitat type parameters were exceptionally variable, extending far beyond random-effect CIs (Fig. 2). However, uncertainty around these point estimates was high for most individuals, as evidenced by the large CI’s around these estimates (Fig. 2.2). Conversely, variability among individuals in selection for distance to two-track roads appeared much smaller than was estimated at the aggregate-level, as virtually all point estimates fell within the random-effect CIs (gray boxes, Fig. 2.2). Given the relatively large uncertainty around individual-level habitat type parameter estimates, I based the principal components analysis (PCA) on parameters for only the continuous environmental variables. Ordination by PCA revealed that some individuals were much more different from one another in terms of combined selection estimates of the nine resource covariates, yet none of the factors examined accounted for the observed extreme selection tendencies (Fig. 2.3a). A small degree of clustering was evident based on cohort (Fig. 2.3b). 2.3.3. Predicted relative probabilities Predictive mapping of the relative probability of use throughout the elk restoration zone under the global models was vastly different among individuals, and when compared to the aggregate-level predictions (Fig. 2.4a). Of the randomly-selected individuals I used to inform these plots, less than half displayed patterns of selection similar to the estimated population-level predictions. The remaining individuals varied with respect to both general areas of relative use probability and uniformity of this use. For example, elk, at an aggregate level, were predicted to select habitat units found in the southeastern portion of the elk restoration zone where the predicted probability of use was very low for many of the individual elk (Fig. 2.4b). Yet even 66 among these individuals, areas of high predicted relative use varied along a gradient of uniform patterns to being much more clustered, varying in scales (Fig. 2.4b). 2.4. Discussion Under the current paradigm for evaluating wildlife resource selection, telemetry data of collared/tagged individuals are typically aggregated prior to model-fitting (Thomas and Taylor, 2006). Aggregating data among individuals is done in the interest of making inferences about animal populations that can provide, among other things, information that is relevant to management and conservation. My study revealed substantial differences between models fit at the aggregate and individual animal levels across three metrics including the number and composition of parameters in top models, the estimated parameters among global models, and predicted relative probabilities of use. This analysis demonstrates that variation in resource selection among individual animals may not be well accounted for by simply including a random effect in aggregate models. While results speak to variation among one population of elk in this ecosystem in Missouri, I find this variation to particularly interesting given that my study animal is highly gregarious and often expected to respond similarly to the environment. These results support existing research which demonstrates the importance of considering individual animal differences in habitat selection, even when the species of interest exhibits sociality (Gillingham and Parker, 2008; Putman and Flueck, 2011; Pape and Löffler, 2015). Further, I could not detect any real conformity in elk-habitat relationships by age or sex class (Figs. 2.3a and 2.3b). At the aggregate-level, the global model received the most support based on ranking by WAIC, yet the inclusion of parameters among individual top models was highly variable. The assumption that observations (i.e. choices between available habitat units) follow some population process with parametric form may have influenced which variables are considered 67 important to selection by all individuals. The fact that the global aggregate-level model achieved the best ranking by WAIC could also be affected by the large sample size, given the tendency for information criteria-based ranking methods to favor more complex models with large sample sizes (Piironen and Vehtari, 2016). The individual variation revealed by our model-ranking analysis suggests that a particular resource may influence decision-making for certain elk more than others. Differential selection may arise in cases where there is a functional response in resource selection, or differences in resource selection/avoidance among individuals (Mysterud and Ims 1998; Haydon et al., 2008). It is difficult to distinguish the effect of overall available habitat from the effect of individual differences in selection, but analysis at the individual level affords the opportunity to explore why individual animals may apparently select habitat units differentially based on available resources. For example, a comparison of RSF models for individual moose made by Gillingham and Parker (2008) revealed that inclusion of a particular selection parameter in the top model varied with the availability in an individual’s home range. Investigating why individuals may differ in this way can provide information that would be obscured if telemetry data were aggregated and only one model was fit. Variation in parameter estimates at the individual level was not commensurate with the variation identified by the σk estimates (representing random effect of animal ID) of the aggregate-level model. For example, with respect to distance to two-track roads, variation among individual-level parameter estimates was much lower than what would be suggested by the aggregate-level model (Fig. 2.2). This observation reflects a potential issue with standardizing variables when evaluating RSFs at the aggregate level. It is common practice to standardize predictor variables to facilitate comparison of effects when evaluating RSFs (e.g. Thomas et al., 2006). In the present study, the aggregate-level hyperparameter estimates were all based on the 68 standardized environmental data at used and available habitat units for 88 elk, while our individual-level parameter estimates were based on data standardized for each individual separately. This should provide a more accurate depiction of the comparative effects between environmental variables on an individual basis; in the case of distance to two-track roads, it suggests that elk respond much more consistently to this environmental variable than would be depicted by the aggregate-level model, where it has a small, negative effect on selection of a resource unit, as opposed to an unknown effect that could vary in both magnitude and direction (Fig. 2.2). Differential use of habitat units based on habitat type may, in part, account for the relatively large uncertainty of corresponding individual-level parameter estimates (Fig. 2.2). If avoidance of specific habitat types by individuals occurs, low use or non-used of these habitat types can bias parameter estimates, which is a known issue with categorical covariates in such models (problem of separation; Menard 2002). Modelling at the individual level allows further investigation into relationships between sampling and RSF parameter estimates (Gillingham and Parker, 2008). Spatial variation in the predicted relative probability of use indicated broad-scale differences in selection strategies between aggregate- and individual-level models (Figs. 2.4a and 2.4b). Because these predictive maps do not reflect parameter uncertainty, it is difficult to quantify the extent that this variation may be attributed to error as opposed to real differences in habitat selection strategies among individual elk. It may be that individual animals are selecting for specific environmental features, and the configuration of those features should be expected to be heterogeneously distributed (Aarts et al., 2008; Paton and Matthiopolous, 2015). Additionally, given that predictive mapping with RSFs is a very useful tool for management of ungulate species, assessing individual-based predictions could offer unique advantages (Johnson et al., 69 2004). For example, predicting relative probability of landscape use (or the absolute probabilities of use when using an RSPF) for individuals could help inform predictions of space use as populations increase following a reintroduction. Predictive mapping based on individual RSF models may present greater potential opportunities for conservation and management of solitary animals, or animals of conservation concern whereby efforts may be focused on managing habitat to meet the needs specific individuals. With recent developments in technologies that can produce highly resolute movement and remotely-sensed data, we have an unprecedented ability to model habitat use by individual animals that can even reveal insights into specific behavioral and ecological mechanisms (Kays et al., 2015). The idea that consistent individual differences among animals (i.e., personality) have important evolutionary and ecological consequences has been amassing a large amount of support for many processes, including habitat selection (Réale et al., 2010; Leclerc et al., 2016; Merrick et al., 2017; Spiegel et al., 2017). Indeed, the potential to derive fitness implications for populations based on differences in movement among individuals has been demonstrated for elk (Haydon et al., 2008; Morales et al. 2010). In this study, aggregate models missed important inferences for conservation and management, including: the importance of certain resource covariates in predicting selection, such as aspect; the inflated effects of certain resource covariates on selection, such as distance to paved roads; that more extreme individuality in combined selection habits could not be attributed to intrinsic factors; and that selection of resource units across the greater landscape may not be as consistent. These results are consistent with an emerging body of research demonstrating potentially substantial variation in habitat selection strategies within populations of highly gregarious species (e.g., elk and reindeer Rangifer tarandus; Sawyer et al., 2007; Pape and Löffler, 2015) 70 My study demonstrates that modelling resource selection at the individual level can reveal important variation in selection strategies within populations of animals that could have been missed by aggregate-level models. That being said, I did identify some consistencies between the aggregate and individual levels. For example, the consistent inclusion of slope and habitat type in top individual models compliments the result that slope and forage openings had the greatest effects on selection at the aggregate level. However, apparent inter-individual differences in selection as determined by RSF modelling can only be investigated by fitting individual-level models, because the selection tendencies of individuals is a prerequisite that is not estimable when fitting typical aggregate (i.e. hierarchical) models. Other benefits to modelling resource selection at the individual level were beyond the scope of our study but provide opportunities for future study. Quantifying variation in the movement process at the individual-animal level could be particularly relevant. There have been a number of individualbased movement models developed in recent years, and methods that simultaneously estimate resource selection and movement have been introduced (Thurfjell et al., 2014; Avgar et al., 2016). Employing models of animal movement can be useful for defining more biologically feasible unused or available habitat units. In conclusion, via this assessment we do not suggest that researchers should abandon aggregate-level RSF modelling with random-effects to quantify individual variation. In this study, relatively strong effects of slope and forage opening on selection of a habitat unit were found at both the aggregate and individual levels. However, I caution against discounting individual variation in habitat selection. Recent developments in this area highlight the common consideration of individual differences in studies of behavioral or population ecology, a viewpoint which should be adopted in applied wildlife conservation research (Merrick and 71 Koprowski, 2017). Thus, researchers and managers should fit models at the individual level so that they can appreciate the consequences of that variation on management practice. When using RSFs to quantify resource selection, I reiterate the importance of multi-scale efforts that have been advocated by others, and assert that both individual- and aggregate-level models can provide valuable inference for management (Gillingham and Parker, 2008; Pape and Löffler, 2015). The methods that we used here provide a framework that other ecologists and managers can use to assess the role of individual variation in their study system. These approaches can be useful for evaluating the utility of population inference from RSFs which will vary among systems and research objectives (Hanks et al., 2011). Computational improvements to hierarchical methods for estimating movement and resource selection are also being developed (Hooten et al., 2016). For researchers and managers limited by computational resources or statistical expertise, fitting individual models provides a better alternative, or at least a supplement to, aggregating across individuals for population-level inference in resource selection analyses. In studies of resource selection, approaches that do not consider individual-level selection are liable to giving superficial weight to certain resources and/or habitats, while underestimating the importance of others and potentially missing interesting ecological relationships. I therefore recommend that whenever feasible, practitioners model habitat selection at the individual level if aggregate approaches will be used to inform management decisions. 72 Acknowledgements I express my deepest gratitude to T. Smith, B. Keller, and J. J. Millspaugh for preparing this dataset and granting me access to conduct the analysis contained herein. I thank R. A. Montgomery and J. J. Millspaugh for invaluable guidance during this study. I humbly thank the Missouri Department of Conservation and the United States Fish and Wildlife Service for permission to use this data as well, as it was collected as part of a project funded by the MDC and a USFWS Wildlife Restoration grant. 73 APPENDIX 74 APPENDIX Table 2.1. WAIC values for top 10 aggregate-level resource selection function models. + indicates an environmental variable included in the model, spaces left blank if not included. Environmental variable abbreviations as follows: D2t = distance to twotrack road; Dpav = Distance to paved road (m); Slope = slope (degrees); Aspect = aspect (degrees); Rd dens = road density (km/km2); D edge = Distance to nearest wooded edge; % Can = percent forested canopy cover; Rx burn = years since prescribed fire; Habitat = indicator variables for 8 cover types. D2t + + + + + Dpav + + + + + + + + + Slope + + + + + + + + Aspect Rd dens + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + D edge + + + + + + + + % Can + + + + + + + + 75 Rx burn + + + + + + + + + Habitat + + + + + + + + + WAIC 321786.1 323171.8 323894.1 324310.7 324410.7 324608.6 324759.4 324865.3 326220.7 Figure 2.1. Inclusion of parameters at the individual level in top models (a panel) and top 5% of models (b panel) as ranked by WAIC for elk reintroduced into the Missouri Ozarks (2011-2014). Green boxes (panel a) indicate the selection parameters in line with the row was included in the top model as ranked by WAIC. Shade of purple (panel b) is proportional to the number of times the corresponding parameter was included in the top 5% of sub-models, where the darkest shade corresponds to all models and lightest shade indicates that parameter was not included in any of the top models. a b 76 Figure 2.2. Aggregate-level random effects distributions (boxes) and individual-level RSF parameter estimates (blue dots) with 95% Bayesian credible intervals (CIs, in translucent blue) for reintroduced elk in the Missouri Ozarks (2011-2014). The middle horizontal bars within the boxes are the point estimates of the mean hyperparameters of the random effects distributions. The light gray horizontal bars within the boxes are point estimates for the standard deviation hyperparmeters, and the ends of the boxes represent 95% Bayesian credible intervals on mean + standard deviation point estimates. 77 Figure 2.3. Principal components analysis (PCA) ordination of the 88 individual elk reintroduced into the Missouri Ozarks (2011-2014) based on individual-level parameter estimates for selection of 9 resource covariates. Grouped by intrinsic factors (panel a) and by GPS-collaring and release year (cohort; panel b). 78 Figure 2.4. Predicted relative probabilities of use of the elk restoration zone based on and aggregate-level RSF (panel a) and individual-level RSFs for 20 randomly selected elk (panel b) reintroduced to the Missouri Ozarks (2011-2014). a b 79 REFERENCES 80 REFERENCES 1) Aarts, G., MacKenzie, M., McConnell, B., Fedak, M., Matthiopolous, J. 2008. Estimating space-use and habitat preference from wildlife telemetry data. Ecography, 31(1):140 – 160. 2) Aebischer, N. J., Robertson, P. A., Kenward, R. E. 1993. Compositional analysis of habitat use from animal radio-tracking data. Ecology, 74(5):1313 – 1325. 3) Arthur, S. M., Manly, B. F. J., McDonald, L. L., Garner, G. W. 1996. Assessing habitat selection when availability changes. Ecology, 77(1):215 – 227. 4) Avgar, T., Potts, J. R., Lewis, M. A., Boyce, M. S. 2016. Integrated step selection analysis: bridging the gap between resource selection and animal movement. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7:619 – 630. 5) Boyce, M. S., Johnson, C. J., Merrill, E. H., Nielsen, S. H., Solberg, E. J., van Moorter, B. 2016. Can habitat selection predict abundance? Journal of Animal Ecology, 85(1):11 – 20. 6) Boyce, M. S., McDonald, L. L. 1999. Relating populations to habitats using resource selection functions. Trend in Ecology and Evolution, 14(7):268 – 272. 7) Boyce, M. S., Vernier, P R., Nielsen, S. E., Schmiegelow, F. K. A. 2002. Evaluating resource selection functions. Ecological Modelling, 157:281 – 300. 8) Browne, W. J., Draper, D. 2006. A comparison of Bayesian and likelihood-based methods for fitting multilevel models. Bayesian Analysis, 1(3):473 – 514. 9) Buskirk, S. W., Millspaugh, J. J. 2006. Metrics for studies of resource selection. Journal of Wildlife Management, 70(2):358 – 366. 10) Byers, C. R., Steinhorst, R. K., Krausman, P. R. 1984. Clarification of a technique for analysis of utilization-availability data. Journal of Wildlife Management, 48(3):1050 – 1053. 11) Cade, B. 2015. Model averaging and muddled multimodal inference. Ecology, 96(9):2370 – 2382. 12) Charnov, E. L. 1976. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9:129 – 136. 13) Cooper, A. B., Millspaugh, J. J. 1999. The application of discrete choice models to wildlife resource selection studies. Ecology, 80(2):566 – 575. 14) Cooper, A. B., Millspaugh, J. J. 2001. Accounting for variation in resource availability and animal behavior in resource selection studies In J. J. Millspaugh and J. M. Marzluff (Eds.), Radio Tracking and Animal Populations (pp. 4 – 15). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 81 15) Craighead, J. J., Craighead, F., C. 1971. Grizzly bear – man relationships in Yellowstone National Park. Bioscience, 21(16):845 – 857. 16) Craighead, J. J., Craighead, F. C., Varney, J. R., Cote, C. E. 1971. Satellite monitoring of black bear. Bioscience, 21(24):1206 – 1212. 17) Duchesnse, T., Fortin, D., Courbin, N. 2010. Mixed conditional logistic regression for habitat selection studies. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79(3):548 – 555. 18) Fieburg, J., Matthiopolous, J., Hebblewhite, M., Boyce, M. S., Frair, J. L. 2010. Correlation and studies of habitat selection: problem, red herring or opportunity? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 365(1550):2233 – 2244. 19) Gaillard, J.-M., Hebblewhite, M., Loison, A., Fuller, M., Powell, R., Basille, M., van Moorter, B. 2010. Habitat-performance relationships: finding the right metric at a given spatial scale. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Science, 365(1550), 2255-2265. 20) Gelman, A., Carlin, J. B., Stern, H. S., Dunson, D. B., Vehtari, A., Rubin, D. B. 2014a. Bayesian Data Analysis (Third Edition). Boca Raton, Florida: CRC Press. 21) Gelman, A., Hwang, J., Vehtari, A. 2014b. Understanding predictive information criteria for Bayesian models. Statistical Computing, 24:997 – 1016. 22) Gillies, C. S., Hebblewhite, M., Nielsen, S. E., Krawchuk, M. A., Aldridge, C. L., Frair, J. L., Saher, D. J., Stevens, C. E., and Jerde, C. L. 2006. Application of random effects to the study of resource selection by animals. Journal of Animal Ecology, 75(4):887 – 898. 23) Gillingham, M. P., Parker, K. L. 2008. The importance of individual variation in defining habitat selection by moose in northern British Columbia. Alces, 44:7 – 20. 24) Griffith, J. A., Martinko, E. A., Price, K. P. 2000. Landscape structure analysis of Kansas at three scales. Landscape and Urban Planning, 52(1):45 – 61. 25) Hanks, E. M., Hooten, M. B., Johnson, D. S., Sterling, J. T. 2011. Velocity-based movement modeling for individual and population level inference. PLoS ONE, 6(8):e22795. 26) Hebblewhite, M., and Merrill, E. 2008. Modelling wildlife-human relationships for social species with mixed-effects resource selection models. Journal of Applied Ecology, 45(3):834 – 844. 27) Hobbs, N. T., Hooten, M. B. 2015. Bayesian models: a statistical primer for ecologists. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 82 28) Hooten, M. B., Buderman, F. E., Brost, B. M., Hanks, E. M., Ivan, J. S. 2016. Hierarchical animal movement models for population-level inference. Environmetrics, 27(6):322 – 333. 29) Hirzel, A. H., Le Lay, G. 2008. Habitat suitability modelling and niche theory. Journal of Applied Ecology, 45(5):1372 – 1381. 30) Johnson, D. H. 1980. The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology, 61(1):65 – 71. 31) Johnson, D. J., Thomas, D. L., Ver Hoef, J. M., Christ, A. 2008. A general framework for the analysis of animal resource selection from telemetry data. Biometrics, 64(3):986 – 976. 32) Kays, R. Crofoot, M. C., Jetz, W., Wikelski, M. 2015. Terrestrial animal tracking as eye on life and planet. Science, 348(6240):1222 – 1232. 33) Keating, K. A., Cherry, S. 2004. Use and interpretation of logistic regression in habitatselection studies. Journal of Wildlife Management, 68(4):774 – 789. 34) Kenward, R. E. 2001. Historical and Practical Perspectives In J. J. Millspaugh and J. M. Marzluff (Eds.), Radio Tracking and Animal Populations (pp. 243 – 273). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 35) Merrick, M. J., Koprowski, J. L. 2017. Should we consider individual behavior differences in applied wildlife conservation studies? Biological Conservation, 209:34 – 44. 36) Leclerc, M., Vander Wal, E., Zedrosser, A., Swenson, J. E., Kindberg, J., Pelltier, F. 2015. Quantifying consistent individual differences in habitat selection. Oecologia, 180:697 – 705. 37) MacArthur, R. H., Pianka, E. R. 1966. On optimal use of a patchy environment. The American Naturalist, 100(916):603 – 609. 38) Manly, B. F., McDonald, L. L., Thomas, D. L., McDonald, T. L., Erickson, W. P. 2002. Resource Selection by Animals: Statistical Design and Analysis for Field Studies (Second Edition). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. 39) Marzluff, J. M., Millspaugh, J. J., Hurvitz, P., Handcock, M. S. 2004. Relating resources to a probabilistic measure of space use: forest fragments and Stellar’s Jays. Ecology, 85(5):1411 – 1427. 40) McCracken, M. L., Manly, B. F. J., and Vander Heyden, M. 1998. The use of discrete-choice models for evaluating resource selection. The Journal of Agricultural, Biological, and Environmental Statistics, 3(3):268 – 279. 41) Missouri Department of Conservation (MDC). 2010. Elk restoration in Missouri. Missouri Department of Conservation, Jefferson City, USA. 83 42) Montgomery, R. A., and Roloff, G. J. 2013. Habitat Selection In S. A. Levin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Biodiversity (Second edition, Volume 4; pp. 59 – 69). Waltham, MA: Academic Press. 43) Morris, D. W. 2003. Toward an ecological synthesis: a case for habitat selection. Oecologia, 136:1 – 13. 44) Mysterud, A., Ims, R. A. 1998. Functional responses in habitat use: availability influences relative use in trade-off situations. Ecology 79(4):1435 – 1441. 45) Neu, C. W., Byers, C. R., Peek, J. M. 1974. A technique for analysis of utilizationavailability data. Journal of Wildlife Management, 38(3):541 – 545. 46) Paton, R. S., Matthiopolous, J. 2015. Defining the scale of habitat availability for models of habitat selection. Ecology, 97(5):1113 – 1122. 47) Pape, R., Löffler, J. 2015. Ecological dynamics in habitat selection of reindeer: an interplay of spatial scale, time, and individual animal’s choice. Polar Biology, 38:1891 – 1903. 48) Putman, R., Flueck, W. T. 2011. Intraspecific variation in biology and ecology of deer: magnitude and causation. Animal Production Science, 51(4):277 – 291. 49) R Core Team. 2016. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/. 50) Ramsey, F. L., Usner, D. 2003. Persistence and heterogeneity in habitat selection studies using radio telemetry. Biometrics, 3(2):332 – 340. 51) Réale, D., Dingemanse, N. J., Kazem, A. J. N., Wright, J. 2010. Evolutionary and ecological approaches to the study of personality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B., 365:3937 – 3946. 52) Rosensweig, M. L. 1981. A theory of habitat selection. Ecology, 62(2):327 – 335. 53) Rosenzweig, M. L. 1991. Habitat selection and population interactions: the search for mechanism. The American Naturalist, 137(Supplement: Habitat Selection), S5 – S28. 54) Thomas, D. L., Johnson, D., Griffith, B. 2006. A Bayesian random effects discrete-choice model for resource selection: population-level selection inference. Journal of Wildlife Management, 70(2):404 – 412. 55) Sawyer, H., Nielsen, R. M., Lindzey, F., Keith, L., Powell, J. H., Abraham, A. A. 2007. Habitat selection of rocky mountain elk in a nonforested environment. Journal of Wildlife Management, 71(3):868 – 874. 84 56) Sawyer, H., Nielsen, R. M., Lindzey, F., McDonald, L. L. 2006. Winter habitat selection of mule deer before and during development of a natural gas field. Journal of Wildlife Management, 70(2):396 – 403. 57) Smith, T. N. 2015. Broad-scale resource selection and food habits of a recently reintroduced elk population in Missouri (Master’s thesis). Retrieved from https://mospace.umsystem.edu/xmlui/bitstream/handle/10355/49138/research.pdf?sequence= 2andisAllowed=y 58) Spiegel, O., Leu, S. T., Bull, C. M., Sih, A. 2017. What’s your move? Movement as a link between personality and spatial dynamics in animal populations. Ecology Letters, 20:3 – 18. 59) Stan Development Team 2016a. The Stan C++ Library, Version 2.14.0. http://mc-stan.org. 60) Stan Development Team 2016b. Rstan: the R interface to Stan, R package version 2.14.1. http://mc-stan.org. 61) Thomas, D. L., Taylor, E. J. 1990. Study designs and tests for comparing resource use and availability. Journal of Wildlife Management, 54(2):322 – 330. 62) Thomas, D. L., Taylor, E. J. 2006. Study designs ad tests for comparing resource use and availability II. Journal of Wildlife Management, 70(2):324 – 336. 63) Thomasma, L. E., Drummer, T. D., Peterson, R. O. 1991. Testing the habitat suitability index model for the fisher. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 19(3):291 – 297. 64) Thurfjell, H., Ciuti, S., Boyce, M. S. 2014. Applications of step-selection functions in ecology and conservation. Movement Ecology, 2(1):4. 65) Vehtari, A., Gelman, A., Gabry, J. 2016. Practical Bayesian model evaluation using leaveone-out cross-validation and WAIC. Journal of Statistics and Computing, DOI:10.1007/s11222-016-9696-4. 66) Watanabe, S. 2010. Asymptotic equivalence of Bayes cross validation and widely applicable information criterion in singular learning theory. Journal of Machine Learning Research, 11:3571 – 3594. 67) Wiens, T. S., Dale, B. C., Boyce, M. S., and Kershaw, G. P. 2008. Three way k-fold crossvalidation of resource selection functions. Ecological Modelling, 212(3-4):244 – 255. 85 CONCLUSION Conservation and management of ungulate species will require a mechanistic understanding of the ways in which extrinsic and intrinsic factors change movement and habitat selection. The ability to obtain location data on these animals at relatively fine temporal resolutions for long periods of time suggests that the movement ecology paradigm is well-suited for discovering these mechanistic links. When coupled with on-board data-loggers (e.g., temperature) and data from remote sensing platforms or autonomous sensor networks, there is opportunity to study the influence of both extrinsic characteristics of an individual’s surroundings, and of intrinsic properties. My study took important steps towards identifying the underlying mechanisms while developing quantitative techniques to model these data with increased accuracy. I envision the contributions of my thesis to be two-fold. First, in chapter 1 non-linear modeling revealed little evidence of heat stress thresholds in movement of moose in central Norway. Using a population-level model, I was able to demonstrate that physiological thresholds of moose established about 30 years ago may not affect moose movement as much as had previously been thought. The results further verified the importance of studying free-living animals whenever possible. Second, in chapter 2 I evaluated the pitfalls of the practice of analyzing the process of resource selection at the aggregate level. Even when accounting for individual variation by incorporating mixed-effects models (i.e., random effect by individual animal) to fit resource selection functions, this work demonstrated that potential inferences could still be missed. This finding has important implications for wildlife management and conservation practice. 86 The main strength of this study is to simultaneously treatment of both the aggregate- and individual-level perspectives, while remaining in the Lagrangian framework of treating the individual as the sample unit, in accordance with prevailing discourse in movement ecology. Specifically, I used movement data derived from individual tracks and associated temperature readings from collar-borne loggers to evaluate my research question in an aggregate perspective. Additionally, I employed a Bayesian discrete choice modelling framework to estimate resource selection, which is predicated on the notion of individual choice and was therefore highly appropriate for estimating choice at the individual level. In this way, it makes intuitive sense to think about individual heterogeneity in the process of resource selection that may not be effectively captured by aggregate methods. Admittedly, a salient weakness of my study is that I did not account for both processes (habitat selection and movement) in either of the focal analyses. These processes are intricately linked; habitat likely plays a major role in deciding how and when ungulates move, and vice versa (Thurfjell et al., 2014). Thus, quantifying both simultaneously could better inform the mechanistic roles of important intrinsic and extrinsic dimensions. Clearly, efforts to conserve ungulate species will be greatly benefitted by the quantitative analysis of movement and habitat selection, in addition to other processes (Nathan et al., 2013). However, scaling up inference to the population level is paramount for making management decisions. Development of holistic methods to do this is an area of active research, and computationally efficient hierarchical modeling approaches show much promise (Hooten et al., 2016; Jonsen, 2017). However, besides the fact that many recent open-source software packages have been developed with analysis of individual movement tracks in mind (e.g., Calabrese et al., 2016), the notion that consistent differences among individuals (i.e., personality) could have 87 implications for management is gaining attention, which raises questions about the scenarios in which such aggregate methods make biological or ecological sense, or are even practical (Merrick and Koprowski, 2017; Speigel et al., 2017). Nevertheless, it remains critical to combine the power of open-source analytical tools and mechanistic models of movement and habitat selection with data collected from distributed sensor networks, remote sensing platforms, and telemetered individuals, as well as intrinsic biological information. This thesis is a testament to the need for multiple analytical perspectives when nurturing a mechanistic understanding of ungulate space use and fitness at multiple scales. 88 REFERENCES 89 REFERENCES 1) Hooten, M. B., Buderman, F. E., Brost, B. M., Hanks, E. M., Ivan, J. S. 2016. Hierarchical animal movement models for population-level inference. Environmetrics, 27:322 – 333. DOI:10.1002/env.2402. 2) Jonsen, I. 2017. Joint estimation over multiple individuals improves behavioural state inference from animal movement data. Nature, 6:20625. DOI: 10.1038/srep20625. 3) Merrick, M. J., Koprowski, J. L. 2017. Should I consider individual differences in applied wildlife conservation studies? Biological Conservation, 209:34 – 44. 4) Nathan, R., Giuggioli, L. A milestone for movement ecology research. Movement Ecology, 1 (1):1. DOI:10.1186/2051-3933-1-1. 5) Speigel, O., Leu, S. T., Bull, C. M., Sih, A. What’s your move? Movement as a link between personality and spatial dynamics in animal populations. Ecology Letters, 20:3 – 18. DOI:10.1111/ele.12708. 6) Thurfjell, H., Ciuti, S., Boyce, M. S. 2014. Applications of step-selection functions in ecology and conservation. Movement Ecology, 2:4. 90