ABSTRACT THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES AND MANAGEMENT THEORY AND PRACTICE: AN EXPLORATORY ANALYSIS by Gary R. Gemmill The investigation was an attempt to discover what is involved in determining if knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences is useful to managers in solving their problems. The investigation consisted of four major parts. The first part of the investigation consisted of establishing a criterion of usefulness for assessing the use- fulness of knowledge in the behavioral sciences to management theory and practice. The criterion selected stated that knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences in order to be useful to managers must enable them to resolve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively. It was further premised that only control knowledge can be used to solve such problems. The second part of the investigation consisted of establishing a framework for assessing behavioral science literature in terms of control knowledge. The framework con- sisted of a construct of a behavioral change model and an operationalizing process. Gary R. Gemmill 2 The third part of the investigation consisted of applying the framework to three areas of the behavioral sciences to determine if they constituted control knowledge. The three areas selected were psychotherapy, group dynamics, and need— satisfaction psychology. The specific areas examined in psy— chotherapy included Freud's psychoanalytic model, Ellis' rational-emotive model, and Wolpe's conditioning therapy model. The specific areas examined in group dynamics included Lewin's group decision model, Likert's supervisory model, and Roethlishberger's and Dickson's counselor model. The specific areas examined in need—satisfaction psychology included McClelland's need for achievement model and Argyris' inter- personal competence model. Each of these models was described in terms of the constructd a behavioral change model and appraised in terms of whether they constituted control knowledge. It was concluded that presently none of the models could be said to be useful since they did not constitute control knowledge. The fourth part of the investigation consisted of a study of behavioral change models used by ten managers in an industrial firm to handle inadequate performance. Open-ended interviews were used to gather the data. An analysis of the data gathered indicated that the ten managers utilized three types of change models in coping with inadequate performance. The models they used were extensions of their over-all administrative control Gary R. Gemmill 3 patterns. The three control patterns were labeled freedom- centered, authority-centered and development—centered. The patterns were differentiated in terms of the predominant inter- personal tool used by the manager to control and change behavior, the manager's conception of his role, the extent of subordinate role structuring by the manager, the extent of impersonality between the manager and subordinate, and the handling of inadequate performance. The managers did not attempt to sys- tematically measure variables they manipulated to improve per- formance or verify propositions on which manipulation was based. They did not experiment with different alternatives to determine if the control pattern they used was the most efficient and effective alternative for improving performance. The control patterns they used were not directly acquired from the behav- ioral sciences. They were acquired from their experience as subordinates to other managers. They tended to retain features of control patterns they found satisfying and to change features they found frustrating. A conceptual comparison revealed that the models used by the managers were different from the models examined in part three of the investigation. On the basis of the data gathered it was not possible to determine if the managers could increase the performance level of subordinates by using the model analyzed in part three rather than the models they used. It was concluded that the models in part three and the models used by the managers have to be made operational before a comparative experiment can be conducted to ascertain which are more efficient and effective in solving the behavioral problems of managers. I. ‘45 THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES AND MANAGEMENT THEORY AND PRACTICE: AN EXPLORATORY INVESTIGATION BY Gary Robert Gemmill A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Management 1966 “uh, an.- 'n 1'“. if ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to acknowledge indebtedness for the many stimulating ideas provided by the members of my thesis guidance committee: Dr. Eugene E. Jennings, Chairman, Dr. Dalton McFarland, and Dr. Darab Unwalla. A special note of thanks is due the chairman, Dr. Eugene E. Jennings. In a few words it would be impossible to describe the significant influence he has had on my intellectual growth and development during the past six years. I hope that some day I will be able to match the model of excellence he portrays in exploring the psychological problems faced by managers. ii CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS LIST OF LIST OF LIST OF Chapter I. II. TABLES . ILLUSTRATIONS APPENDICES INTRODUCTION Purpose . . Premises and Propositions Research Design . . Organization of the Thesis A CRITERION OF USEFULNESS FOR EVALUATING THE CONTRIBUTIONS OF THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES TO MANAGEMENT THEORY AND PRACTICE The Need for a Criterion for Assessing the Contribution of the Behavioral Sciences to Management Theory and Practice . The Importance of Goals for the Behavioral Sciences The Control Position. . The Non-Control Position . . The Relationship Between Prediction and Control . . . . . . . The Motives of Social Scientists Empirical Determination Versus Control The Applied Social Scientist and the Problem of Values . . . The Applied Areas of the Behavioral Sciences Summary . iii Page ii vii viii ix F‘G‘k1P‘ H 23 23 25 3O 37 4O 42 45 49 52 54 Chapter Page III. A CONVERSION FRAMEWORK FOR TRANSLATING BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE KNOWLEDGE INTO ADMINISTRATIVE TOOLS . . . . . . . . . . . . 55 The Need for a Conversion Framework from a Managerial Point of View . . . . . 55 The Need for a Conversion Framework from an Applied Science Point of View . . 61 A Conversion Framework: The Construct of a Behavioral Change Model . . . . . . 69 The Operationalizing Process . . . . . . 82 The Simple Human Relations Model: An Illustration of the Operationalizing Process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 Analysis of Propositions B.3 and B.4 . . 89 IV. PSYCHOTHERAPEUTIC MODELS FOR CHANGING BEHAVIOR: AN ANALYSIS OF PROPOSITION C:1A . 92 Analysis of Proposition C:1A . . . . . . 92 The Freudian Psychoanalytic Model . . . . 93 The Rational—Emotive Model... . . . . . . 114 The Conditioning Model . . . . . . . . . 137 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 152 V. ENVIRONMENTALISTIC MODELS FOR CHANGING BEHAVIOR: AN ANALYSIS OF PROPOSITION C:1B . 155 The Group Decision Model . . . . . . . . 156 The Supervisory Model . . . . . . . . . . 165 The Counselor Model . . . . . . . . . . . 175 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 183 VI. NEED-SATISFACTION MODELS FOR CHANGING BEHAVIOR: AN ANALYSIS OF PROPOSITION C:lC . 186 Need—Satisfaction Models . . . . . . . . 186 The Need for Achievement Model . . . . . 187 The Interpersonal Competence Model . . . 202 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 244 VII. THE FRAMEWORK UTILIZED TO ANALYZE THE IMPLICIT BEHAVIORAL CHANGE MODELS OF THE TEN MANAGERS: AN ANALYSIS OF THE D PROPOSITIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 246 The Need for an Empirical Study . . . . . 246 iv r. a. u p. z. . . we 0".- J"..~ Q"- — a .4 ‘u. I, 9.. T. C v... s. a... o. P. Chapter Page Implicit Behavioral Change Models: An Analysis of Proposition D.2 . . . . . 247 Control Patterns: The Unit of Analysis . 248 Limitations of the Control Pattern as a Unit of Analysis . . . . . . . . . . 249 Control Pattern Variables . . . . . . . 250 A Summary Comparison of the Three Control Patterns . . . . . . . . . . . . 255 VIII. ANALYSIS OF THE FREEDOM-CENTERED CONTROL PATTERN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 257 The Manager's Rationale for the Pattern . 257 The Manager's Conception of His Role . . 258 Extent of Role Structuring . . . . . . . 259 The Experiencing of a Low Degree of Role Structuring by Subordinate Managers. 260 Impersonality . . . . . . . . 261 The Experiencing of Impersonality by Subordinate Managers . . . . . 261 The Handling of Inadequate Performance . 263 The Experiencing of Ambiguity in Evaluation by the Subordinate Managers . 265 The Experiencing of Pressure by the Subordinate Managers . . . . . . . . 267 The Problem of Self- Motivation . . . . 269 The Ideationally Obsolete: A Special Case of Inadequate Performance . . . . . 270 Modeling as a Mechanism for Self-Motivating . . . . . . . . . . . 273 The Psychodynamics of the Freedom- Centered Control Pattern from the Point of View of the Manager . . . . . . 274 The Psychodynamics of the Freedom- Centered Control Pattern from the Point of View of Subordinates . . . . 278 The Relationship of the Freedom—Centered Control Pattern to Existing Theory and Research . . . . . . . . . 281 Summary and Analysis of D Propositions . 291 Chapter Page IX. ANALYSIS OF THE AUTHORITY-CENTERED CONTROL PATTERN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 294 The Manager's Conception of His Role . . 294 Extent of Role Structuring . . . . . . . 296 Impersonality . . . . . . . . . . 298 The Handling of Inadequate Performance . 300 The Changing of Behavior Through Authority: An Illustration . . . . . . . 304 Money as an Incentive . . . . . . . . 307 Changing the Behavior of Peers . . . . . 309 The Psychodynamics of the Authority— Centered Control Pattern . . . . . . 310 The Relationship of the Authority- Centered Control Pattern to Existing Theory and Research . . . . . . . . . . 312 Summary and Analysis of D Propositions . 319 x. AN ANALYSIS OF THE DEVELOPMENT-CENTERED CONTROL PATTERN . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 321 The Manager's Conception of His Role . . 321 Extent of Role Structuring . . . . . . . 322 Impersonality . . . . . . . . . . . 324 The Handling of Inadequate Performance . 325 Changing Performance Criteria as a Mechanism for Changing Behavior . . . . . 332 Changing the Behavior of Peers . . . . . 333 Recognition as an Incentive . . . . . . . 334 Psychodynamics of the Development- Centered Pattern . . . . . . 336 The Relationship of the Development- Centered Pattern to Existing Theory and Research . . . . . . . . . . . 339 Summary and Analysis of D Propositions . 341 XI. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . 343 A Criterion of Usefulness . . . . . . . . 343 A Conversion Framework . . . . . . . 344 Examination of Existing Knowledge in the Behavioral Sciences . . . . . . 349 Empirical Determination of Control Knowledge Possessed by Managers . . . . . 356 Experimental Comparison . . . . . . . . . 369 BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 382 vi LIST OF TABLES Table Page 5.1 Changes in Consumption Behavior of New Mothers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 160 6.1 Suggested Solution to Emotional Outburst . . 231 6.2 Perceived Importance of Interpersonal Abilities by the Eighteen Executives . . . . 237 11.1 A Comparison of Possible Determinants of Inadequate Performance . . . . . . . . . . . 362 vii LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS The Didactic Technique Comparison of the Two Experimental Techniques for Changing Behavior Subordinates' Perception of the Supervisor's Behavior Scoring Mechanism for the Need for Achievement . The Interpersonal Competence Model-- Behavior Pattern U. The Interpersonal Competence Model—— Behavior Pattern D. . . A Comparison of the Three Control Patterns viii Page 129 157 167 189 218 219 256 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix Page A. INTRODUCTION TO THE INTERVIEW . . . . . . . . 372 B. THE INTERVIEW INSTRUMENT . . . . . . . . . . 375 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Purpose The purpose of this dissertation is to answer the question: can knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences help managers solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively? The thesis is that this question can only be answered through: 1. developing a criterion of usefulness for knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences. 2. developing a framework for converting such knowledge into a form which managers can use . 3. examining the existing knowledge in the behavioral sciences using the aforementioned criterion and frame— work. 4. determining the range of behavioral problems managers encounter and how they presently cope with them. 5. experimentally comparing items 3 and 4 in terms of item 1. This thesis is defended through establishing a set of premises and prOpositions about the relationship between the behavioral sciences and the management of an organ- ization. The premises are statements assumed to be true for the sake of studying the consequences that follow from them. The propositions are statements that can be said to be true or false. Premises and Propositions The premises and propositions to be analyzed are grouped under the following five areas. Criterion Qf Usefulness: The A Premises Premise A.1: To determine if knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences is useful to managers in handling their behavioral problems, a criterion of usefulness must be constructed. Premise A.2: Knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences is useful to managers if it helps them solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively than they could solve them without using such knowledge. Premise A.3: Knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences can only help managers solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively if it is control knowledge that is superior to the knowledge the managers already possess. Conversion Framework: The B Propositions Proposition B.1: Management theory and practice and the behavioral sciences have not identified the problems managers face in converting knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences into a tool they can use for changing behavior. Proposition 3.2: Knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences cannot be used by managers or by applied social scientists without it being converted into a different form. Proposition B.3: The construct of a behavioral change model is capable of converting behavioral science knowledge into a form in which managers can use it for solving their behavioral problems. Proposition B.4: For a behavioral change model to be useful it must meet the following necessary and sufficient conditions: a) it must contain variables that a change agent can control. b) it must possess an objective system for measuring the values of the variables. c) it must state valid relationships between the variables. [Examination.g§ Existing Behavioral Science Knowledge: The‘gugropositions Proposition C.1: Attempts to change behavior in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences do not presently meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. Proposition C.2: Attempts to change behavior in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences consist of the manipulation of the values of individual variables, environmental variables, or both. Empirical Determination of Managers' Approaches £9 Behavioral Change: The Q Propositions Proposition D.1: To determine the usefulness of knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences to managers, the behavioral problems managers face and how they cope with them must be determined. Proposition D.2: Managers implicitly use behavioral change models in handling inadequate performance on the part of their subordinates. Proposition D.3: The implicit behavioral change models managers use in handling inadequate performance: a) are not based on knowledge they acquire from the behavioral sciences. b) lack dbjective measurement systems. Experimental Comparison: The E Proposition Proposition B.1: If the behavioral change models managers use are different from those present in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences, an experiment must be conducted to determine which ones are most efficient and effective in helping them resolve their behavioral change problems. Research Design The research design for analyzing these premises and propositions is as follows. Criterion of Usefulness Premises Since the A premises are premises, they are not to be analyzed. Chapter II, however, provides a rationale for them and demonstrates the consequences that follows from them. Conversion Framework Propositions Propositions 8.1 and 8.2 are analyzed in Chapter III through applying the A premises to selected literature dealing with the application of behavioral science knowledge to management theory and practice. The analysis attempts to answer the following questions. Can managers directly apply knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences? What is involved in applying such knowledge? Propositions B.3 and B.4 are analyzed through the use of deductive logic. They represent a conversion framework for assessing the behavioral sciences in terms of control knowledge. Examination-Q: Existing Knowledge Propositions The C propositions are analyzed by applying the A premises and the B prOpositions to selected literature in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences. The literature selected represents three applied areas: psychotherapeutics, group dynamics, and need-satisfaction psychology. The general rationale for the selection of these three areas is that on an .a priori basis they seem to represent attempts to change behavior through changing individual variables, environmental variables, and both. The specific literature selected and the rationale for its selection is as follows. Psychotherapeutic Models The three psychotherapeutic models analyzed are Freud's psychoanalytic model, Albert Ellis' rational- emotive model, and I ‘Wolpe's conditioning model. There were two major reasons for selecting these three forms of therapy for analysis of the C propositions. First, on an.g priori basis it seemed that they would deal exclusively with changing the behavior of an individual by changing the values of individual variables. Second, they represent verbal models in which words are used to change behavior. The specific proposition analyzed in Chapter IV is: Proposition C:1A In terms of the A premisesand proposition B.4, these three psychotherapeutic models are not presently useful to managers. The proposition is analyzed by describing each model and then appraising it in terms of the A premisesand proposition B.4. Environmentalistic Models The three environmentalistic models analyzed are Kurt Lewin's group decision model, Rensis Likert's supervisory model, and Roethlishberger's and Dickson's counselor model. The reason for selecting these three models for the analysis of the C proposition is that on anlg priori basis they seemed to deal exclusively with changing the behavior of an individual by changing the values of environmental variables. The specific proposition analyzed in Chapter Proposition C:1B: In terms of the A premises and prOposition B.4, these three environmentalistic models are not presently useful to managers. The proposition is analyzed by describing each model and then appraising it in terms of the A premises and proposition B.4. Need-Satisfaction Models The two need-satisfaction models analyzed are David McClelland's need for achievement model and Chris Argyris' interpersonal competence model. The reason 10 for selecting these two models for the analysis of the C propositions is that on an §_priori basis they seemed to deal with changing the behavior of an individual by changing the values of both individual and environmental variables. The specific proposition analyzed in Chapter VI is: Proposition C:1C: In terms oftheiA.premises and proposition B.4, these two need-satisfaction models are not presently useful to managers. The proposition is analyzed by describing each model and then appraising it in terms of the A premises and proposition B.4. ‘Empirical Determination 9; Managers' Approaches £9 Behavioral Change Propositions The D propositions are analyzed by means of an exploratory study of ten managers from one company. The purpose of the study was threefold. First, to determine the general types of problems the managers perceive themselves as having with other participants in their organizations which require them to somehow 11 change their behavior. Second, to determine how they go about making changes in the behavior of the participants ‘they perceive as causing the problems. Third, to deter- mine how they go about handling inadequate performance on the part of subordinates. In short, the study was an attempt to discover and describe the variables the ten managers use in defining behavioral problems and developing techniques to cope with them. The Research Instrument The research instrument used in the study was an open-ended interview format. The choice of this format as an information gathering instrument was based on two factors. First, the study was designed to identify some of the variables managers used to conceptualize their behavioral problems and coping techniques. Because of this it was felt that an open-ended interview format would be the most appropriate research instrument since it allows respondents to reveal their natural frame of reference and hence give more true-to—life replies. Secondly, the open-ended interview format is the most appropriate research instrument for exploring areas 12 where there is little basis for knowing either what questions to ask or how to formulate them.1 Once deciding on the use of the open-ended interview format, the next step was to decide on what kinds of questions to ask the managers. A direct approach to questioning the managers was ruled out on an.§ priori basis. If managers were asked: ”What types of problems do you have which involve behavioral change?", chances are they would not asSign a specific meaning to the question. It is outside their frame of reference and too technical to elicit the information sought. Consequently it was decided to use indirect questions which seemed on ania priori basis to be capable of revealing the managers' notions about behavioral problems and change techniques. The specific questions used and the information each question was designed to provide are reported in Appendix B. The general research strategy underlying their choice is all that will be mentioned here. First, it was decided that the questions used should serve as relatively ambiguous stimuli to get the managers to 1R. L. Kahn and C. F. Cannel, The Dynamics 9f Interviewing: Theory, Techniques, and Cases (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1957). 1 n.1h5i1fl1lllg —wI-l!...-. # l3 discuss certain areas that were thought on an.§ priori basis to involve behavioral problems and change techniques. Therefore, "yes" and "no" types of questions were minimized. When they were used, additional probes were designed to discover the reasons behind the "yes" or "no." Secondly, it was decided that the questions should be arranged in sets based on the funneling process. In the funneling process sets of questions dealing with a certain area are arranged in an order which starts from very general questions and proceeds to very specific questions. An example of the funneling process used is as follows: We recently conducted a study in which we found that some managers felt that one of the most important aspects of their job was to search for areas which need changing, as well as to formulate new objectives to be achieved. In connection with this, they felt that one of the most difficult problems they faced was trying to get other people in their organization to see the need for the change and to go along with them in making the needed changes. They thought that one of the key abilities a manager must have is the ability to get other people to change their behavior. 1The funneling process is discussed in F. N. Kerlinger, Foundations pf Behavioral Research (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1964), pp. 471-73. 1911... h “I... i;- .Hl GIL’. I. 14 In terms of your experience, what do you think about this? Why do you think some people resist change? How do you think managers can go about getting people to change their behavior or ways of doing things? Can you recall anytime in the past when you were faced with a problem like this? What did you do about it? In the sequence of questions following the initial state- ments the questions move from very general ones to very _specific ones. The rationale for using this funneling process is twofold. First, because the general questions are ambiguous in scope they permit the manager to structure his response in the way he thinks is appropriate or meaningful to him. Hence, he can reveal his natural frame of reference. Secondly, the specific questions which follow the general questions provide a measure of the possible gap between how the manager says he behaves and how he actually behaves. For example, if he were asked how he generally handles mistakes that his subordinates make, he might state how he ideally thinks they should be handled rather than how he actually handles them. By moving from this general question area to a more specific one some measure of how 15 he actually handles mistakes is possible. For example, after asking him how he generally handles mistakes, he might be asked for a recent example involving his handling of a mistake. This would provide information on how he actually handles mistakes. The Interview Procedure Each manager at the beginning of the interview was to be informed about the general purpose of the study and the reason he was selected to participate in it. A Verbatim transcript of the introduction to the interviews is reported in appendix A. The introduction was designed to put the manager at ease and to c0pe with any uncertainty he might have about what was going to be done with the information he provided. He was also to be told how the interview would proceed and that he could answer all the questions on the basis of his experience. The interview was to begin with those questions that the researcher assumed to be the least threatening to the manager (see appendix B). In general, the inter- view was to proceed from questions about his subordinates to, questions about the managers he had l6 worked under to, questions about the nature of his role as manager. However, it was decided not to always maintain the exact order of questioning reported in appendix B. Whenever possible the manager was to be allowed to lead the interview. If he mentioned other areas in the interview format while responding to a question, it was decided that the researcher should step out of the question order of the format in order to take advantage of the manager's natural frame of reference. The researcher in conducting the interview was to refrain from making any type of evaluative comment to avoid putting the manager on the defensive, as well as, to avoid putting words into his mouth. The researcher was to minimize all his comments to prevent leading the manager's thoughts. It was decided to record the interview with a tape recorder. The reason for this decision was that it was thought that if notes were taken, or the researcher attempted to memorize the interview content, his perception would be selective resulting in the loss of information that might later on prove to have been l7 meaningful. A survey of the research literature failed to uncover experiments done on the relative effective— ness of notetaking, memorization, or recording in producing accurate information. One researcher suggested that recorders were effective with high level executives but not with supervisors since they tend to freeze in the presence of a recorder.1 Because of inconclusive evidence that recorders are not as effective as note-taking or memorization it was decided to record the interviews. Each manager was to be given a choice as to whether he wanted the interview recorded (see appendix B). To reduce any possible feelings of discomfort a manager might experience in having his conversation recorded he was to be told why the researcher wanted to use the recorder. The recorder was to be a small transistorized unit that could be placed outside his line of vision. It was assumed that these conditions would decrease any discomfort he might experience and that in his conversation he would not hesitate to express his real opinion. C. Argyris, Understanding Qgganizational Behavior (Homewood, 111.: Dorsey Press, 1960), p. 55. 18 Selection 9; the Managers 1:2 _IE Studied To carry out the study it was decided to inter— view ten managers from one company. The reasons for selecting this type of sample were threefold. First, intensive interviews were to be conducted, so in the interest of economy the sample had to be small. Second, the study was to be exploratory, hence the size of the sample and its representativeness was not of immediate importance. Third, the study was designed to find out if the types of behavioral problems and techniques used by managers varied within one company. Thelgample.§elected The company selected for study was engaged in the manufacturer of agricultural and industrial equip— ment and was a division of a larger national corporation. In 1965, the gross sales of the division ran into the millions and it employed 750 people in its plant and administrative offices. Division profits had been declining over the past two years and in an effort to improve performance many technological and organizational 19 changes were taking place. For example, a computer system Was being installed and value analysis teams were being instituted. Access to the company was gained through an acquaintance who was an assistant to the general manager of the division. He secured permission to conduct the study. After permission was granted, ten managers out of a possible fifteen from the top two authority levels of the division were selected to be interviewed. The criterion used to select the ten managers was that in total they represent as many diverse functional areas within the division as possible. The reason for using this criterion was to find out if the types of behavioral problems and techniques used by the managers were unique to their functional areas. The ten managers selected included the general manager of the division and nine other managers who reported directly to him. The functional areas represented by these nine subordinate managers and the number selected from each area is as follows: 20 Number of Functional Areas Managers Accounting 1 Personnel 1 Research & Development 1 Production Control 1 Manufacturing 1 Product Management 4 Total Number of Subordinate Managers 9 The ten managers ranged in age from thirty-nine to sixty— two with the average age being fifty. In terms of the number of years spent working for the corporation, the range was from ten years to thirty-five years with the average being twenty years. Five of the managers had college degrees and five of them had only high school diplomas. Once the ten managers to be interviewed were selected they were contacted and informed about the general nature of the study. They were asked whether they would be willing to participate. All ten of them were willing to participate and were asked to select a one or two hour block of time for the interview. 21 Experimental Comparison Proposition The E proposition is analyzed in Chapter XI through comparing the analysis of the C propositions with the analysis of the D propositions. Organization 9f the Thesis Chapter II consists of an analysis of the A premises and the rationale on which they are based. Chapter III consists of an analysis of the B pro— positions. The analysis is based on an appraisal of selected literature dealing with the relationship between management theory and practice and the behavioral sciences. Chapter IV consists of an analysis of proposition C:lA. The analysis is accomplished through describing the psychotherapeutic models of Freud, Ellis and Wolpe and evaluating them in terms of the A premises and proposition B.4. Chapter V consists of an analysis of proposition C:lB. The analysis is accomplished through describing the environmentalistic models of Lewin, Likert, Roethlishberger and Dickson and evaluating them in terms of the A premises and prOposition B.4. Chapter VI consists of an analysis 22 of proposition CtLB. The analysis is accomplished through describing the need-satisfaction models of McClelland and Argyris and evaluating them in terms of the A premises and proposition B.4. Chapter VII consists of an analysis of the D propositions. The analysis is accomplished through evaluating the information provided by the exploratory study of the ten managers. Chapters VIII, IX and X consist of a description and evaluation of the techniques the ten managers used to change the behavior of other participants in their organization. Chapter XI consists of a summary and comparison of the analysis of the C propositions with the analysis of the D propositions. The E proposition is analyzed and areas for future research are delineated. CHAPTER II A CRITERION OF USEFULNESS FOR EVALUATING THE CONTRIBUTIONS OF THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES TO MANAGEMENT THEORY AND PRACTICE The Need for §_Criterion for Assessing the Contribution 9f_the Behavioral Sciences £9_Management Theory and Practice To determine if the applied areas of the behavioral SCiences have made, or are capable of making, a contribu- ticmn to management theory and practice a criterion of what CCulstitutes a contribution must be developed. Obviously, without such a criterion it would be impossible to ascertain if the behavioral sciences make a contribution t£> management theory and practice. For this reason Premise A.1 is necessary. Premises A.2 and A.3 represent a twofold criterion for assessing the contribution of the behavioral sciences to management theory and practice. Premise A.2 delineates a pragmatic criterion. It states that knowl— edge contained in the behavioral sciences is useful to 23 24 managers if it helps them solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively than they could solve them without using such knowledge. The implication of this premise for the ensuing analysis is that it leads to a fourfold examination of the relationship between behavioral knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences and management theory and practice. First, it leads to an analysis of the existing knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences. Second, it: requires an analysis and description of the behavioral Problems that confront managers. Third, it leads to an analysis and description of the behavioral knowledge managers possess. And lastly, if the behavioral know- ledge possessed by managers is different from that ZPIKesent in the behavioral sciences an experiment must .bEE conducted to determine which knowledge is the most efficient and effective in resolving the managers }3ehaviora1 problems. Hence, premise A.2 leads to an analysis of the existing behavioral science literature, the behavioral problems managers confront, the behavioral knowledge they possess, and a comparison of behavioral knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences with 25 that which managers possess. In accordance with premise A.2, if the knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences is more efficient and effective in solving the behavioral problems of managers it is useful to them. Premise A.3 states that knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences can only help managers solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively if it is control knowledge which is superior to the knowledge the managers already possess. The questions of importance in providing a rationale for this premise are: What is control knowledge? Why is it only useful to managers? The remainder of this chapter will attempt to answer these questions. The Importance of Goals for the Behavioral Sciences Among social scientists there exists two pre- dominant positions about the ultimate goal of the behavioral sciences. One position contends that the ultimate goal is to control human behavior. This will be referred to as the control position. The other position contends that the ultimate goal is to predict human behavior. This will be referred to as the 26 non—control position. An examination of each of these positions is necessary in order to determine if they make any difference for develOping a criterion for assessing the usefulness of the behavioral sciences to management theory and practice. The ensuing discussion of the control and non- control positions regarding the goals of the behavioral sciences may in some respects appear anthropomorphic. It may give the impression that "behavioral sciences" are independent objects which have the human attribute of being goal seeking mechanisms. Of course nothing could be further from the truth. The goals of the behavioral sciences, or more generally science,are set by men. Therefore, when one speaks of the goals of the behavioral sciences one is referring to how certain men have formulated them. It is only possible to examine how different men articulate the goals since there is no way of "proving" that science has goals, or what its goals should be. The crucial issue is if different definitions of goals result in different behavior on the part of social scientists. In discussing the non—control and control 27 positions it will be suggested that each position will result in different behavior on the part of social scientists. For example, a social scientist who articulates goals for the behavioral sciences of description, prediction and control of some social phenomenon focuses his attention and efforts on trying to achieve these goals. To the degree he can describe, predict and control the phenomenon he investigates, he has achieved the goals. Hence goals serve as a reference point for measuring how well he is achieving the goals, and provide a way of rechanneling his efforts if the goals are not being achieved. It is possible, of course, to take issue with the specific goals postulated by different social scientists. This will be done in discussing the control and non—control position. It is not possible, however, to dispense with the statement that there must be some type of goal toward which social scientists expend their efforts. A social scientist, for example, by postu- lating goals of description, prediction and control channels his behavior into achieving these goals. 28 If they failed to direct his behavior, they would be empty goals and an examination of the goals he actually uses to channel his efforts would have to be made. People within and without a given discipline appraise its progress by the goals they attribute to it. They may, for example, assess the achievement of the behavioral sciences in terms of the amount of control knowledge it accumulates. Laymen and some social scientist who have used this goals for appraisal have been disillusioned with the achievements of the behavioral sciences. They look around them and see all kinds of indicators which suggest that the behavioral sciences contain no useful knowledge. Crime, war, hatred, poverty and mental illness abounds. People still continue to smoke cigarettes even though they are allegedly pathogenic for them. Surely, they reason, if there were any substantive knowledge in the behavioral sciences it would be possible to ameliorate these social "sicknesses." Although this assessment may be "true" it certainly does not refute the goals or mean that they can never be realized. It simply suggests that the 29 proper means for achieving the control goals have not yet been discovered. Perhaps, the "causes" of these sicknesses have not yet been found. In sum, these people seem to be dealing primarily with what the state of the behavioral sciences is now, not what it can be in the future. And as Skinner has so aptly stated: Certainly no one is prepared to say now what a science of behavior can or cannot accomplish eventually. Advance estimates of the limits of soience have generally proved inaccurate. The issue is in the long run pragmatic: we cannot tell until we have tried. Moreover, these people are appraising the behavioral sciences in terms of an important value. Namely, the behavioral sciences should produce know- ledge which is "useful" for solving contemporary social problems. As Bierstedt puts it: Indeed, I should assert with some enthusiasm that the ultimate test of all knowledge is its social use and consequences.2 1B. F. Skinner, Science and Human Behavior (New YOrk: Macmillan, 1953), p. 20. R. Bierstedt, "Social Science and Public Service," in Applied Sociology: Opportunities and Problems, ed. A. W. Gouldner and S. M. Milier (New York: The Free Press, 1965), p. 419. 30 Hence, these people judge the behavioral sciences as not useful because they do not provide control know- ledge which would help solve contemporary social problems. The Control Position B. F. Skinner is a representative of the control position. He articulates the goals of science in the following manner: Science is more than the mere description of events as they occur. It is an attempt to discover order, to show that certain events stand in lawful relations to other events. No practical technology can be based upon science until such relations have been discovered. But order is not only a possible end product, it is a working assumption which must be accepted at the very start. We cannot apply the methods of science to a subject matter which is assumed to move about capriciously. Science not only describes, it predicts. It deals not only with the past but also with the future. Nor is prediction the last work: To the extent that relevant conditions can be altered, or otherwise controlled, the future can be controlled. If we are to use the methods of science in the field of human affairs, we must assume that behavior is lawful and determined. We must expect to discover that what a man does is the result of specifiable conditions and that once these conditions have been discovered, we can anticipate and to some extent determine his actions. 1Skinner,2p. cit., p. 6. 31 The three goals postulated by Skinner are sequential in nature. Science moves from description to prediction to control. Descriptive Knowledge The description goal that Skinner assigns to the behavioral sciences is concerned with the accumulation of descriptive knowledge pertaining to human behavior. Descriptive knowledge defines a variable and describes its frequency in the real world. A social scientist, for example, might be concerned with acquiring descriptive knowledge of the amount of job satisfaction present in a particular occupational group. To acquire this knowe ledge, he must define job satisfaction in measurable terms and then apply the Operational definition to the occupational group to determine its actual incident. He might define job satisfaction as statements made by members of the occupational group that indicate they like their job. To measure satisfaction, he would, perhaps, ask the members: "Do you like your job?" If they answer yes, by definition they would be classified as being satisfied with their jobs. Descriptive knowl— edge of job satisfaction would state, in accordance 32 with the measurement device used, the frequency with which members of the occupational group say they are satisfied with their jobs. Predictive Knowledge The predictive goal is concerned with the accumulation of predictive knowledge. Predictive knowl- edge. constitutes a statement of how two variables are related in time so that given the value of one variable, the value of the other variable can be determined. A social scientist, for example, might want to acquire predictive knowledge about the relationship between job satisfaction and employee turnover. Predictive knowlr _edge: of the relationship would tell him.how changes in job satisfaction affect employee turnover. When investigating the relationship between rate of employee turnover and degree of job satisfaction, the social scientist would be interested in both the direction of the relationship and the magnitudes of each variable. Direction deals with whether they are negatively, or positively, or neutrally related to each other. If turn- over was positively related to job satisfaction, it would be possible to state that a high turnover rate 33 is associated with a high degree of job satisfaction. If the relationship was negative, a high turnover rate would be associated with a low degree of job satis- faction. If the relationship was neutral, there would be no discernible association between the two variables. To make predictive statements, a time dimension is added to the association so that the statement of association changes form and the possibility of a curvalinear relationship transpires. If the relation- ship is positive the predictive statement is: if the degree of job satisfaction increases over time, then the rate of turnover will increase. If the relationship is negative the statement is: if the degree of job satis— faction increases over time, then the rate of turnover will decrease. If the relationship is curvalinear the statement is: if the degree of job satisfaction increases over time, then the rate of turnover will first decrease and then increase. These predictive statements represent the possible relationships between two variables in terms of the direction in which they move in regards to each other. 34 It is often possible to predict the relationship of variables without predicting their relative magnitudes. Magnitude deals with how the amount, or degree, of each variable is related to other variables. A social scientist is not only interested in predicting that when the value of one variable increases, the value of the other variable will decrease. He is also interested in how much each will increase or decrease. If magnitude Could be predicted the predictive statement might read: If job satisfaction increases from a value of one to a Value of two, then the rate of turnover will decrease from a value of ten to a value of five. Very few Predictive statements in the behavioral sciences are of this form. Most of them are simply predictive statements of the direction in which the variables move in relation- ship to each other. ‘Control Knowledge Some social scientists feel that when the pre— dicztion goal is achieved their role is over. MacIver, fOr example, states that: 1.: ‘4‘1 } I Inn I.“ 35 All our investigating is a search for relationships and all our science is the knowledge of systems of relations. The more we know, the more widely and more fully do we apprehend things as related to others and things as themselves systems of relations. IBut other social scientists such as Skinner contend tihat this is only an intermediary step in achieving tihe ultimate goal: control over the relationships being investigated. Brown and Ghiselli define the control goal in the following manner: As an aim of science, control refers to the manipulation of the conditions determining a phenomenon in order to achieve some desired end. Hence, a control goal for the behavioral sciences deals withacquiring knowledge that indicates how relationships 1Pe-tween variables can be altered to achieve some desired State of behavior. To state that a social scientist 1Has acquired control knowledge is to imply that he has ‘ 1R. M. MacIver, Social Causation (New York: Ginn and Company, 1942), pp. 97-98. 2c. w. Brown and E. E. Ghiselli, Scientific .Methodiig Psychology (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1955), p. 41. 36 diascovered the determinants of some behavioral phenomenon .arufl that they are amenable to his control. There is an important difference between prediction and control in the behavioral sciences. Prediction does not always lead to the possibility of cxantrol. For example, a predictive statement such as a. high degree of job satisfaction leads to low employee turnover is simply a relating of the description of the 'tvwo variables. By itself the statement is of little use to someone interested in developing a high degree cxf job satisfaction. It does not indicate, as a CHDritrol statement would, how a high degree of job Satisfaction could be developed. A control statement not only indicates the r‘elationship between variables but also its determinants Mflnich are subject to control by man. It describes a Irreferred pattern of behavior and how the pattern can 13e achieved. For example, if low employee turnover was a preferred pattern of behavior and was known to be a function of job satisfaction, the control statement might read: 37 To decrease employee turnover, increase job satisfaction. To increase job satis- faction the following conditions must be established. . . . These conditions can be established through. . . . TWTUS, control statements are different from predictive stuatements. They specify an end to which predictive knowledge is to be applied and how it is to be applied. The Non—Control Position There is disagreement among social scientists over whether the goal of the behavioral sciences should .EEEJ or is in fact, control of human behavior. An example CDf’ a social scientist who adheres to a non-control Position is Chinoy. He claims that: The goal of science is the building of theory, a body of logically interrelated propositions that assert determinate relations amont the phenomena being studied.1 bhowhere in the statement is there an implication that "Theory" is to be used for controlling human behavior. ‘Also it does not indicate if there is any purpose behind "the building of theory." Stated differently, Chinoy's statement does not answer the question of why the social scientist should g 1E. Chinoy, Society: 'AQ Introduction 59 Sociology (New York: Random House, 1961), p. 17. 1h. "‘4 . 38 build a theory. Is it only because there is some intrinsic satisfaction accuring to him for doing it? Or is it intended to be used by someone to solve some problems? Or is it for both of these reasons? In short, of what utility is the theory once it is constructed? Premises A.2 and A.3 state that a theory only has utility to the extent that it can be applied to the real world and allow mankind to achieve improvements. Improvement is the meaning and significance of postu- lating a control goal for the behavioral sciences. Kerlinger has a non—control View which is similar to Chinoy's. He states that: If we said that the aim of science is the betterment of mankind, most readers would quickly read the words and accept them. But the basic aim of science is not the better- ment of mankind. It is theory. A theory is a set of interrelated constructs (concepts), definitions, and propositions that present a systematic View of phenomena by specifying relations among variables, with the purpose of explaining and predicting the phenomena. He goes on to state that theory implies prediction and that prediction implies control. 1F. N. Kerlinger, Foundations 9; Behavioral Research (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1964), p. 11. 39 A number of questions are raised by Kerlinger's point of view. First, is the goal of control to better mankind as Skinner claims and Kerlinger denies? Second,can, as Kerlinger claims, theory be a goal in itself with no other purpose? Third, why cannot the betterment of mankind be the basic aim of the behavioral sciences? What difference would it make if the better— ment of mankind was or was not the ultimate goal of the behavioral sciences? Kerlinger seems to forget that the aim, or goal, of the behavioral sciences is what one defines it to be. Readers who quickly read the words “the aim of science is the betterment of mankind" and accepts them, have defined this as the aim. Subsequently for them it is the aim while for Kerlinger it is not. The important question is whether it makes any difference if it is or is not the ultimate aim. If it does not, then attempts to define the aim are meaningless. Kerlinger does not, however, mention why the basic aim of science can not be the betterment of mankind, and hence sheds no light on what difference it makes as to how the aim is viewed. 40 The Relationship Between Prediction and Control Kerlinger's assertion that control is implied by prediction is somewhat erroneous simply because prediction does not always imply control. A doctor, for example, may be able to predict that a patient with a certain magnitude of illness will die in six months, and yet this knowledge may not allow him to prevent (control) the occurrence. Prevention, or control, only comes after he has isolated the determinants of the illness and can exert control over their magnitude and direction. Thus prediction does not necessarily imply control. Prediction simply means that if one variable has such and such a value, the other variable will have such and such a value. It does not mean that someone can exert control over the values of either variable, nor does it necessarily explain why they have their respective values. So in this sense, postulating a control goal for the behavioral sciences can make a difference in the behavior of a social scientist. If he adheres to a non-control goal, his task may terminate when he describes an association between variables. He may not go on to investigate how the values of the 41 variables can be manipulated to attain some preferred end, or how the predictive knowledge can be utilized. The crucial issue, then, is whether it is possible to have a theory in the behavioral sciences which is not capable of leading to human betterment. Certainly, applied areas of the behavioral sciences like psychiatry seek to better mankind through contructing theories of diseases that will permit its practitioners to control their magnitude and direction. What, then, would be an example of a theory which does not lead to the betterment of mankind? An example would be a theory which imparts no new knowledge of relationships, or one which contains variables that are not controllable by mankind. An example of the latter is the illustration dealing with illness. It simply predicted the outcome of an illness without providing control knowledge. But even this predictive knowledge may potentially lead to the betterment of mankind. Having isolated the disease the medical scientist would be in a position to search for its determinants. If he discovered the determinants and could somehow control their magnitude and direction, :k.‘ w.- ‘n‘l GSA 42 he would ultimately provide medical practitioners with control knowledge which they could use to betteriman— kind, provided the absence of illness is defined as bettering mankind. Thus regardless of the inner motives of the medical scientist, be they the betterment of mankind or simply curiosity, the ultimate effect of his work may be to provide knowledge that can be used to control the disease. The Motives pf Social Scientists Perhaps, what Kerlinger means to suggest is that a given social scientist exploring social phenomena does not have in mind an immediate knowledge of how his research may lead to the betterment of mankind. In a sense he can not know either of these things until after he conducts his investigation. However, prior to an investigation of some social phenomena a social scientist often sets forth a social or behavioral problem he would like to study. For example, why do some people become dOpe addicts while other do not? Why do some workers in a factory restrict their output while others(do not? Why do some people react to frustration with aggression while others do not? 43 In these instances, the social scientist may obtain his problems to study from those which are thought to exist in his society. He may believe that if he can uncover the determinants of these problems and if they are amenable to control, he can provide knowledge which is capable of bettering mankind, provided he defines betterment as amelioration of these problems. Of course he does not always obtain problems to study in such a manner. Often he simply postulates questions to which he would like to find answers. For example, what is it that holds a society or organization together? A social scientist raising this type of question will look for variables which affect an organization or society "holding together." In his search he will construct some scheme of variables, some method of relating them, and some method for measuring their values. The search process he uses is the same one he would have used had he taken his problem from an array of existing social problems. The important difference is that in this case he has not set out to find a way of lessening existing social problems. But 44 if he should discover the determinants of "holding together" and they are subject to control by man, he would nevertheless provide knowledge which is potentially useful. It would become actually useful when someone defines an organization as "splitting up" and, assuming it to be undesirable, uses the know— ledge to pull it back "together." Hence, it is conceivable that even a social scientist who attempts to answer theoretical questions may ultimately produce control knowledge. It is only when a social scientist has know- ledge of the relationship between variables and can change their magnitudes that he can achieve a better— ment goal he has established. Knowledge henceforth only becomes actually useful when it helps him resolve, or lessen, a problem he confronts better than he could do so without it. Knowledge, on the other hand, is potentially useful if it holds the possibility of a solution and the occurrence of the problem is probable without some intervention on his part. These statements tie together the rationale behind premises A.2 and A.3. They state that knowledge is useful if it can l‘wIIIEuL,rtufl.-u. .bl.‘ firui—Jlalli I . 45 help solve problems (premise A.2) and that it can only be useful if it is control knowledge (premise A.3). Problems after all are essentially betterment variables. They are statements depicting values of variables someone would like to increase, or decrease; for example, low productivity, low return on investment, low job satisfaction, or a high degree of role conflict. Solutions to these perceived problems consist of changing the values (high or low) to a desired level through finding variables that produce these values and manipulating their values. Empirical Determination Versus Control The non—control position which contends that the social scientist's job stops with the construction of theory is frequently based on a view that its application involves value judgments that are non- . . . . l . . SC1ent1f1c 1n scope. It is asserted that the soc1al 1Examples of authors who express an Opinion that "values" have no place in science are: P. M. Hauser, "Social Science and Social Engineering," Philosophy 9; Science, XVI (1949), 209-18: G. A. Lundberg, Social Research (New York: Longman and Green, 1942) and, H. A. Simon, Administrative Behavio; (New York: Macmillan, 1957). 46 scientist is no more competent to make value judgments than anyone else and, therefore, should not determine how theory is to or could be used. Social scientists who adhere to such a viewpoint sometimes uphold a point of view called empirical determinism. Empirical determinism occurs when a social scientist succeeds in describing and predicting some phenomenon in the real world and accepts the relationships as immutable. He, in effect, states that this is the way the world is, not the way it can be or should be. An example of empirical determinism is provided by an article written by Robert McMurry entitled, ”The Case for Benevolent Autocracy." He summarizes the article by saying: Since so many members of lower, middle and even top management in the typical large business enterprises of today are dependent, insecure, and ineffective—-productive only because they are bossed by one or two hard- driving strong autocrats——the outlook for the wide spread introduction of a genuine humanistic, democratic-participative philosophy of leadership in the near future locks dim indeed. But benevolent autocracy, while it is neither idealistic nor inspiring, is practical. It accepts people as they are and recognizes particularly that most pe0p1e prefer to be led. It also faces the fact that there is a dearth of leaders in industry 47 now and in the foreseeable future. It is, the final analysis, simply a technique for making the best of the worst! What benevolent autocracy offers is not a beautiful vision of a world to come. Instead, it simply accepts reality with all of its limitations. While hardly a noble philosophy of management, it does have one invaluable attribute: where i; has been trieg, .i__t; works. in The "accepts things as they are" theme that runs through these statements is what is meant by empirical determinism. The possibility that man could be different, as well as the search for "causes" as to why he is the way he is, are absent. The possibility of changing the behavior of man (the real meaning of the control goal eSpoused by Skinner) is not entertained. When McMurry states that most people are "dependent, insecure, and ineffective," he accepts each Of these characteristics as being immutable. Conse- ql-Iently, he is not concerned with why or how they acquired the se characteristics and why the one or two "strong antocrats " are different . It is interesting to note, in this vein, that Chris Argyris suggests that the reason for people in lower levels of an organization being “dependent, 1R. N. McMurry, "The Case for Benevolent Autocracy," Harvard Business Review, XXXVI (January-February, 1958). 48 insecure and ineffective" is because of the one or two "strong autocrats" controlling the organization. TArgyris, thus, entertains the possibility that although scam-me people may be this way now in the future they can be different if certain conditions are changed. More Specifically, if the one or two "strong autocrats" are Somehow replaced, people in the lower levels of an Organization might become "independent, secure and e ffective. " In terms of values, McMurry places a high priority on the way people are while Argyris places a high priority on the way people can be and, perhaps, Should be if an organization desires to maximize its I. . e ffectiveness. " It is evident from this illustration that a SOcial scientist who adheres to an empirical deterministic POsition is mistaken if he believes he does not exert a value judgment. Certainly to state that things should be the way they are is a form of value judgment. But Ihost importantly, a social scientist who adheres to a Position of empirical determinism will not pursue a control goal. He will not be concerned with changing -C. Argyris, Personality and Organization (New York: Harper & Bros., 1957). _....__,_,, in“? 1. !- _ 49 the nature of the world through the application of control knowledge. The Applied Social Scientist and the Problem 2; Values Social scientists in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences use a general form of premises A.2 and A.3. By virtue of being applied social scientists, they believe that control knowledge can be developed and that it should be used to better mankind. Thus, they are not empirical determinists. Applied social scientists can not escape making value judgments since they are engaged in changing behavior.1 As change agents, they must make a judgment that one pattern of behavior is more desirable than another. To clarify the nature of value-judgments exercised by applied social scientists, it is necessary 1For a discussion of the role of values in the behavioral sciences see: K. D. Benne and G. E. Swanson, "The Problem of Values and the Social Scientist," Journal 9; Social Issues, VI (October, 1950), 2—7; L. Gross, "Values and Theory of Social Problems," in A. W. Gouldner and Miller, gp. cit., pp. 383-97; R. S. Lynd, Knowledge for What? (Princeton: Princeton Univers1t Press, 1945); R. Ross, "Moral Obligations of the Scien ist," in A. W. Gouldner and Miller, 9p. cit., p. 429-38, and C. R. Rogers and B. F. Skinner, "Some ssues Concerning the Control of Human Behavior: A Symposium," Science, CXXIV (1958), 1057-66. 50 to define the terms "value" and "value-judgment." A value is a statement of a desired pattern of behavior. The statement describes the behavior pattern and asserts that it is desirable. One example of a value would be a statement that people should be mentally healthy. Mental health may be defined as the presence of behaviors X and Y.1 By itself the description of the pattern "mental health" is value—free. It is only a description of behaviors associated with the term "mentally healthy." By adding to it the assertion, "people should be, " it becomes a value. A value-judgment is the application of a value to a given situation. A psychotherapist, for example, would exercise a value-judgment if he applied the value "mehtally healthy" to a particular patient and classified him as "mentally unhealthy." The value-judgment would be his decision that the patient should be made "mentally healthy" through the application of control knowledge. \ 1For an interesting discussion of the value Problems inherent in the concept of mental health see: M' Jahoda, Current Concepts g: Positive Mental Health (New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1958). 51 The control knowledge a psychotherapist might apply to make the patient "mentally healthy" would constitute a descriptive statement of how the values of certain variables could be manipulated to produce behaviors X and Y. It might state, for example, that: To achieve behaviors X and Y, decrease the value of A. The value of A can be decreased through performing operations 1, 2, and 3. Thus, control knowledge itself constitutes a value—free descriptive statement. It does not specify an end for its use. The end is determined by the applied social scientist. The applied social scientist often converts the value he accepts as a change goal into a premise. By converting it into a premise, he is able to concentrate on developing knowledge which will permit him to change the behavior of pe0ple that deviates from the value. A psychotherapist, for example, may accept as a premise the value, "mentally healthy." Since the value describes the desired behavior pattern, he can focus his attention on acquiring knowledge that will permit him to change the behavior patterns of patients that do not conform to the desired pattern. “"1 I.» 52 The Applied Areas_gf the Behavioral Sciences For purposes of the analysis that follows the applied areas of the behavior sciences are considered as those areas that share in common the desire to change the behavior of some object, be it a person, group, organization, or society, through instrumenting knowledge contained in the basic disciplines of the behavioral sciences. Examples of applied areas are psychotherapy, group dynamics, social work and educational psychology. These have all emerged from the basic disciplines of the behavioral sciences in order to c0pe with specific behavioral problems. They are all based on the contention that it is possible to change the behavior of people who have problems. A more precise definition of the applied areas is provided by Greenwood. He defines them as areas within the basic disciplines that are: . . . designed to achieve controlled changes in natural relationships by means of procedures which are scientifically based. E. Greenwood, "The Practice of Science and the Science of Practice," in The Planning 9f Change: Readings jg the Applied Behavioral Sciences, ed. W. G. Bennis g; al. (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston), p. 74. 53 He also states that theory in the basic disciplines "consist of laws describing and explaining nature" while theory in the applied areas “consists of principles prescribing ways of controlling nature."l Thus, the applied areas are characterized by theiry concentration on developing techniques for applying knowledge accumu— lated by the basic disciplines to achieve changes in the behavior of people. The psychotherapist, for example, attempts to apply knowledge contained in the discipline of psychology to cure his patients' mental afflictions. The universe of the applied areas of the behav— ioral sciences in the analysis that follows will be considered to be: psychotherapy, group dynamics, and need-satisfaction psychology. Management theory and practice will not be considered an applied area of the behavioral sciences because its relationship to the behavioral sciences is to be analyzed. Expressed in terms of set theory, the intersection of management theory and practice and the applied areas of the behavioral sciences equals an empty set. The intersection 1Ibid. 54 of the two areas is considered problematic and, hence, is to be analyzed in the ensuing chapters. Serum In this chapter the rationale behind the A premises was presented. Without premise A.1 it would be impossible to determine if knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences is useful to management theory and practice. Subsequently some form of criterion of usefulness must be established. Premises A.2 and A.3 provide a possible criterion. Taken together they state that knowledge in the behavioral sciences is useful to management theory and practice if it helps managers solve their problems more efficiently and effectively. It can only do this if it is control knowledge since only with control knowledge could managers manipulate the values of variables to attain a desired end. CHAPTER III A CONVERSION FRAMEWORK FOR TRANSLATING BEHAVIORAL SCIENCE KNOWLEDGE INTO ADMINISTRATIVE TOOLS In this chapter the need for a conversion frame- work is analyzed from both a managerial and an applied science point of View. After the analysis, a conversion framework is constructed. The Need for 2 Conversion Framework from g Managerial Point 9; View Recently there has been much attention given to the relationship of the behavioral sciences to the operation of industrial firms. Much of the literature bearing on this subject reveals the opinion that know- ledge derived from the behavioral sciences can increase the efficiency and effectiveness of industrial firms. Attempts to support the opinion usually results in the listing of concepts and subject matter contained in the 55 56 behavioral sciences in terms of managerial decision 1 . areas. Somet1mes such endeavors are centered on developing either general propositions about human behavior or general frameworks for analyzing the human problems of industrial firms.2 Rarely, however, does 1Examples of such articles are: C. Y. Glock 35 al., Case Studies 1g Bringing Behavioral Science into UseHTStanford, Calif.: Institute for Communication Research, Stanford University, 1961); W. Lazer and E. J. Kelley, "Interdisciplinary Horizons in Marketing," Journal 9; Marketing (October, 1960); F. J. Roethlisberger, "Contr1butions of the Behavioral Sciences to A General Theory of Management," Readings ig Managerial Psychology, ed. H. J. Leavitt and R. J. Pondy (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1964), pp. 518—41; and L. X. Tarpey, "Marketing Research and the Behavioral Science," Bus1ness TOpics, XIII (Winter, 1965), 61-67. Some examples of such articles are: D. W. Belcher, "Toward A BehaV1oral Sc1ence Theory of Wages," Journal 9f the Aggggmy 9; Management, V (August, 196 ), 1 - . In tH1s art1c e Belcher adVances propositions about the relationship between certain concepts in the behavioral sciences and industrial wage practices. For example, he hypothesizes that: "The internal wage structure (of a f1rm) will be more redictable and stable: (1) The more the f1rm is a close , isolated bureaucracy. . . (5) The more social norms and tradition correlate with organizational values, (6) The weaker the pressures of groups within the organization" (p. 116). It seems clear that such pro- positions do not provide managers with administrative tools they can use in designing wage systems for their organization. Pro ably after reading t is article the would wonder, among other things, how they could go about f1nding out what "social norms" and "organizational values" are prevalent in their firms. Belcher's article, however, does not attempt to answer these types of questions; and M. H. Mescon, "Sociodrama and Sociometry: Tools for a Modern Approach to Leadershi ," Journal 9; the Academy 9; Manage- ment, II (April, 1959 , 21—28. Of interest in this article 18 Mescon's statement on page 27 that: "Industry might advantageously investigate more fully the potentialities of these interesting tools of modern leadership." This conclusion is fairly typical of articles which purport to relate the behav1ora sciences to management. The questions that remain unanswered in such articles is: How does one measure the "potentialities" of "interesting” concepts develo ed in the behavioral sciences for the theory and prac ice of management? 57 anyone attempt to specifically indicate how the concepts, propositions, or frameworks can be applied by managers in their day-to—day Operations. Neither, for that matter, do they mention how managers can determine whether the use of such concepts, propositions, or frameworks will improve the performance Of their firms more than not using them. Even less frequent in the literature are examples of behavioral concepts which have been applied by managers, or efforts aimed at converting such concepts into an Operational framework which managers can incorporate in their everyday Operations. In light of these deficiencies, it is no wonder that some of them are bewildered by the literature and question whether it is really possible to apply the behavioral sciences to the process of running a going concern. Lawrence Ferguson, manager Of General Electric's Behavioral Research Service, for example, claims that: . . . several years ago (prior to 1959) when we sought to apply available social science findings to solve specific personnel problems, we consistently found that the social science information available then was of little help to us.1 1 C. Ferguson, "Social Scientists in th L. e Plant," Harvard Bus1ness Review, XLIII (Mabeune, 1964), 135. 58 It is not surprising then that today, as Leeds and Smith phrase it: . . . the businessman wants tO know precisely what social science can offer him, and how its contribution can best be applied. Moreover, he wants to know with a fair amount of precision what difference the application of social science knowledge will make in the operation Of his company. But authors typically do not discuss the potential for application. They leave managers to work out their own problem of direct application and conversion. Developing a formal conversion framework is a formidable task that managers have not attempted to carry out. Just how formidable this task can be is revealed in the following illustration. A manager call him Smith, is reading an article on the relationship of learning theory to management development. The author of the article expresses the opinion that all developmental programs should rest on "known laws Of learning." His purpose in writing the article is to make these laws known to managers so that they can use them in building their own programs. One 1R. Leeds and T. Smith, Using Social Sgience Knowledge lg Business and Industry (Homewood, Ill.: Irwin, 1963), p. 2. 59 of the laws described is the so-called law Of effect. The author presents the law in terms Of a quote from McGeogh, namely: Acts followed by a state of affairs which the individual does not avoid, and which he often tries to preserve or attain, are selected and fixated, while acts followed by states Of affairs which the individual avoids or attempts to change are eliminated. . . Acts (resulting in) satisfying . . . motivating conditions are selected and strengthened, while those leading to consequences which do not satisfy a motivating condition are eliminated. Upon reading this rather precise statement, Smith scratches his head in bewilderment. "How in the world can I go about applying this law to my company's develop- ment program?" he asks. The author of the article, however, has anti- cipated that some readers would find the passage difficult to comprehend so he advances what he feels is a less complex statement of the law: A satisfying outcome strengthens, and an annoying outcome weakens, a modifiable connection between a stimulus and a response.2 1J. A. McGeogh, The Psychology 9; Human Learning (New York: Longmans and Green, 1942), p. 574. 2P. L. Harriman, Handbook 9: Psychological Terms (Paterson, N.Y.: Littlefield, Adams and Company, 1961), p. 89. 60 After reading this passage, Smith's bewilderment is somewhat lessened.. However to him it seems that the law simply means people will do things they find gratifying and not do things they find non-gratifying. Smith's interpretation is, in a sense, correct, because the law deals with the effect of reinforcement on behavior. It holds that behavioral responses which yield satisfaction will be repeated again while those which yield dissatisfaction will not. If a person receives a reward for acting in one fashion and is punished for acting in another fashion, he will repeat the reWarding acts but not the punished acts. According to this hedonistically based law, people supposedly do not repeat behavioral responses to stimuli found, by experience to be non-rewarding. But now that Smith has a vague idea of what the law means, how is he to apply it to his firm's development program? To be able to apply it he would need to operationalize the law by stating in measurable terms what stimuli, subject to his control, are rewarding and non—rewarding to the proposed participants of the program. He would also need to know in measurable terms what types 61 of behavioral responses, which:are presently absent, he wants the program to produce. Only if he had this knowledge could he manipulate the stimuli to get the response he desires. Furthermore, only if he has this knowledge would he possess control knowledge that would be useful in helping him establish a development program. A search of selected management literature Offers support for proposition B. I. Management theory and practice has not identified the problems managers face in converting behavioral change knowledge contained in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences into a tool they can use for changing behavior. The Need for g Conversion Framework from an Applied Science Point pf View Some social scientists, particularly those in the non-applied areas, assume there is no conversion problem in using knowledge provided by the basis disciplines of the behavioral sciences. According to Gouldner: In the standard View of the relation- ship between applied and pure social science there is the tacit assumption that the development of the applied social 4 '1J‘L' 62 sciences requires no special planning and theoretical analysis. It is assumed they possess no distinctive problems and what will be required is to transfer their development. . . . The thought is scarcely entertained, however, that the applied and pure discipline may have differences in their basic interests and thus in their very conceptual roots. Ford and Urban exemplify those who profess a non-conversion assumption when they state: It is quite clear that the practice of psychotherapy is an applied field. A therapist primary purpose is not to establish principles Of behavior, but rather to apply established principles to achieve behavioral changes. His primary purposes is to "cure" patients rather than to build theory.2 They assume that an applied social scientist is one who takes knowledge from a basic discipline and directly applies it to a given situation. A therapist supposedly takes principles of behavior contained in the basic discipline of psychology and applies them to a given case in order to make changes in his patient's behavior. 1 A. W. Gouldner, "Theoretical Requirements of the Applied Social Science," in Marketing and the Behavioral Scippces, ed. P. Bliss (Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1963), p. 7. 2D. H. Ford and H. B. Urban, Systems g: Psycho- therapy: .A Comparative Stugy (New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1963), p. 9. 63 While the non-conversion assumption appears plausible many social scientists in the applied areas take issue with it. In terms of psychotherapy, for example, they believe that at the present time there are no "established principles to achieve behavioral changes." Berelson and Steiner, for example, contend that: There is no conclusive evidence that psychotherapy is more effective than general medical counseling or advice in treating neurosis or psychosis. Strictly speaking, it cannot even be considered that psychotherapy. on the average, improves a patient's chances of recovery beyond what they would be without any formal therapy whatsoever.l If this statement is valid, Obviously the psychotherapist has no "established principles to achieve behavioral changes." He cannot take knowledge from the basic discipline of psychology and directly apply it. An illustration of the conversion problem con- fronted by applied social scientists is provided by examining Berelson's and Steiner's recent effort to B. Berelson and G. A. Steiner, Human Behavior: .Ap Inventory pf Scientific Findings (New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1964), p. 287. 64 compile an inventory Of scientific findings on human behavior. Only a cursory examination of their books reveals that many of the findings are not directly applicable, or even useful, in the sense of solving problems or bettering mankind. One finding, for example, states: "The larger the organization becomes, the more ranks of personnel there will tend to be within it."1 It seems Obvious that this type Of proposition is not particularly useful for someone in an applied area. In terms of premise A.2, it does not specify a problem which it might help solve. In terms of premise A.3, it is not a control statement because it does not specify what applied social scientists could do to change the value Of the independent variable (organization size) nor indicate the determinants of the relationship. Without knowledge of the determinants the value of the independent variable is uncontrollable. The finding also does not specify the context of "larger"; whether it means larger in terms of assets, sales, or number of personnel. Similarly it does not Ibid., p. 368. 'e 4 E- 3.“. thbufii‘ ‘I‘ S V. a - 06 ‘5 a nv-V‘H; . . A—v-~ ~' .e g . a. t-n Vs u... ...855“ V O ~. v. “d. H.» 1: I I- , C C .7. 3 C . I L.» -1 S .C C .7. C L.. ~.\fllllllil C .n t C. E T. a C r e T l a. P~ s a.“ ‘5 \£ 6 \I.‘ Ft *5 e r. 6 .J I. ~._ 6 .7). L“ C. a. e e Aw L. nu —“ s s we. Ce C. :4 :1 9 Lay MOO. r NW C l L e c... ‘ .t .l C a... u. a 11 r S n1 . e r “i: 6 NW .. my. 2. a. r. .C s ~i. .v r- c A L; a: . s a we. .« 1 4M ‘1‘ -.||(;-| 3 1‘1 I - J F .f 7 65 j_rudicate how "rank" is to be measured. Without a technique for measuring these variables the validity of the relationship can not be confirmed and the finding can not be applied, even if it were useful. Another of the findings states that: The efficiency of a large formal organization is sizably enhanced when its own chain of command or decision or communication is tied into the informal network of groups within the organization, so that the network can be used to support the organization's goals. In terms of the A premises this finding is not useful by itself. It does not, in accordance with premise A.2, specify a particular behavioral change problem to which it might be applied. It alludes to the possibility of an "efficiency" problem but it does not specify the parameters of the problem. Every time an organization is labeled inefficient, is the inefficiency due to improper communication? The finding, in accordance with premise A.3, does not constitute a control statement. It does not indicate how, "the network can be used to support the Ibid., p. 370. Md”; mk _. 66 organization's goals." Hence, it does not tell the social scientist or manager how they can make the network support the organization's goals. Moreover, the statement does not refer to Operational definitions or measurement devices for the key variables. How, for example, are "efficiency," "sizably enhanced," "chain of command," "informal network of groups," and "support the organization's goals” to be measured? Without a system for measuring the values Of these variables the finding cannot be applied and the empirical validity Of the proposition can not be determined. Another illustration of the difficulty of applying the findings is the finding that: "The leader's style Of leadership tends to be influenced by the style in which he himself is led."1 Once again, in terms of the A premises this finding is not useful. It does not indicate the significance Of the finding for solving problems. Furthermore, it does not specify the determinants Of "leadership style" or how it can be measured. If a social scientist or manager Ibid., p. 376. 67 is to use this finding to discover if changes in leader- ship styles in an organization are necessary, he needs to have a way Of measuring them. Given that some change is necessary, he also needs to know the determinants of the styles and how their values can be altered in order to implement the changes. The finding, of course, does not furnish this information. The intention has not been to deprecate Berelson's and Steiner's efforts to classify findings since they may be only reporting the existing state of the behavioral sciences. The purpose of analyzing some of their findings has been to evaluate proposition 8.2, which states that knowledge contained in the basic disciplines can not always be directly applied by a social scientist who is trying to better mankind. Some form of conversion framework is necessary. It is not surprising that some social scientists in the applied areas Of the behavioral sciences perceive the need for a conversion framework. They believe, on the basis of their experience, that knowledge existing in the basic disciplines can not be directly applied to specific problem situations. 68 Goulder, for example, states that: It seems evident that the needs of an applied social science, which must above all cope with social change, are not met by all models of present-day pure theory. An applied social science cannot, there— fore, be regarded as entailing the simple transfer of either the established pro- positions or the concepts Of pure science to practical purposes. Elaborating on this point he hypothesizes that: . . .(a) Applied social scientist are more likely to use the concepts than the generalized propositions of their basic disciplines. (b) Not all concepts or theoretical models of pure social science are equally useful to applied social scientists. (c) Applied social scientists will more likely borrow from their basic disciplines those concepts and theoretical models which aid them in understanding or producing changes. (d) When the basic discipline does not provide theoretical systems or concepts aiding the applied social scientist to deal with change, the latter will develOp these himself. (e) These new concepts will, in turn, exert pressures to produce modifications in the theories of the basic disciplines. Apparently Gouldner believes that social scientists in the basic disciplines have not been lGouldner,.9_p. cit., p. 7. 2Ibid., p. 9. 69 interested in developing an applied theory of behavioral knowledge. Traditionally, they have been interested in the description and prediction of human behavior rather than control. Subsequently, the applied social scientist has been left to develop an applied theory of his own making. Implicitly or explicitly he has had to develop concepts and theories he could utilize in carry- ing out his role as a change agent. His efforts, how- ever, have not yet resulted in the construction of a formal conversion framework that could be utilized by all persons who act as change agents. .5 Conversion Framework: The Construct .pf a Behavioral Change Model A conversion framework to be useful to managers and applied social scientists must enable them to trans- form knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences into a set of Operational directives. A conversion framework can only do this if it meets the following conditions: 1. Provides a technique for describing different forms of desirable and undesirable behavior. This is necessary for determining if change 70 occurs and the direction of change. 2. Specifies the variables that produce changes in behavior and provides a technique for measuring them. This is necessary for establishing the validity of relationships. 3. Specifies how the values of change producing variables can be manipulated by a change agent. This is necessary for producing change. When these conditions are met a manager will possess control knowledge. A desired end will be specified, he will have knowledge Of its determinants, and know how to manipulate them. The proposed conversion framework is twofold. First, it consists of a construct of a behavioral change model. As a construct it is deliberately designed to facilitate the scientific investigation of behavioral change. It does this by delineating a set of conceptual variables that can be used to analyze specific attempts to change behavior. Secondly, the framework consists Of an operational process that permits specific behavioral change attempts to be analyzed and evaluated 71 in terms of premises A.2 and A.3. It demonstrates that managers have useful knowledge when they in fact can change behavior. The construct of a behavioral change model consists of: l. A change agent's statement of an undesirable behavior pattern possessed by an Object of change. This statement represents a behavior pattern that the change agent attempts to move the Object of change away from. For purposes of analysis it is labelled, behavior pattern U. 2. A change agent's statement of a desired behavior pattern for an object of change. This statement represents a behavior pattern that the change agent attempts to move the Object of change toward. For purposes of analysis it is labelled, behavior pattern D. 3. A change agent's description of a mechanism for moving the object of change from the undesired behavior pattern (U) to the desired behavior pattern (D). 72 A simple illustration will clarify the meaning of the construct. First, suppose a manager believes one of his subordinates is restricting his output. He will define the problem as one of getting the subordinate to change from behavior pattern U, where he is restricting output, to behavior pattern D, where he is producing at a level which the manager considers acceptable. The mechanism the manager prescribes for the transition between the two patterns may take many forms. Each form reflects his assumptions about what will cause his subordinate to change his behavior. He may, for example, hypothesize that if he increases the subordinate's wages, the subordinate will reciprocate by increasing his output. He may generalize this hypothesis and state that output will always increase with increases in pay. As long as this relationship appears valid, he assumes that it is effective for producing changes in the output behavior of his sub- ordinates. It is possible that the manager may implement different mechanisms such as persuasion, coercion, or 73 a promise of promotion, or some combination Of them, if money does not appear to be a sufficient condition for achieving the changes he desires in his subordinate's behavior. However, these are also subject to the test of whether they lead to the desired response of increased productivity. Definitions p§_Behaviora1, Change, and Model Before proceeding it is necessary to examine the meaning assigned to the key terms in the construct: "behavioral," "change," and "model." The term "behavioral" refers to actions and motives Of the Object of change. Actions are those portions of behavior, verbal and physical gestures, which are capable of being directly observed by others. Motives, on the other hand, are not capable Of being directly observed by others. They can only be assessed by questioning and probing the Object Of change or by inferring the nature of these motives and making predictions about how these will affect his actions. In the latter instance, the accuracy Of the inference is partially determined by the ability to predict actions. But in either case it is motives 74 which provide the explanation for actions. Motives form the definition of situations from which actions ensue. The significance of the term "behavioral" in the construct is that it implies that the change models have consequences in the real world. What the change models are trying to change is actions and motives which are summed together under the term behavioral. Change models have consequences for the real world because they are concerned with moving the behavior pattern of the Object of change from behavior pattern U, to behavior pattern D. The term "change" means to move an Object of change from behavior pattern U, to behavior pattern D. When D is reached the Object of change has by definition been changed. The term "model" refers to a set Of propositions that relate variables involved in making a transition from behavior pattern U to behavior pattern D. The set Of propositions constitutes a transition-producing mechanism that a change agent utilizes to change the behavior pattern of an object Of change. “Hi. 5.6 .w .1 |MII| ‘3'1— 75 The Change Agent and the Object pf Change A change agent is a person whose purpose is the changing of behavior. This definition is universal in scope since it can be applied to all persons involved in changing behavior, be they social scientist or manager. The construct describes the activities a change agent performs in carrying out his purpose. He identifies behavior patterns U and D, and constructs a mechanism for making a transition between them. An Object Of change constitutes the unit of behavior the change agent is interested in changing. It can be a person, group, organization, or society. It also constitutes a universal definition that can be applied to all social units that are to be changed by a change agent. Behavior Patterns U and 2 Behavior patterns U and D are statements of variables and their values. They can be classified on the basis of common variables with differences in the values of variables differentiating each pattern. 76 Behavior pattern U, for example, may be described in terms of behavioral labels such as: high alienation, high mental depression, high anxiety, low productivity, low morale, and low return on investment. Each of these behavioral labels are variables with values (high or low). Behavior pattern D may be described in terms of these variables taking on different values. For example, if U is described as low productivity, D may be described as high productivity. These behavioral labels simply identify desirable and undesirable behavior patterns. By them— selves, they neither explain how the pattern became undesirable nor what can be done to make it desirable. They are only indicative of a behavior pattern being something other than what the change agent considers desirable. They, so to speak, describe an incidence Of a negatively valued social phenomena without suggesting methods Of treatment or prevention. When a change agent uses the descriptions of the two patterns as a change goal for an Object Of change he exercises a value judgment. He states that behavior pattern D is preferable tO behavior pattern U. 77 The descriptions of the two patterns makes his value criterion explicit by specifying the specific behaviors he values. By making behavior pattern D a premise, he is able to construct a transition-producing mechanism for attaining what he has defined as a more preferable behavior pattern. Every behavioral change model contains a diagnostic scheme for identifying behavior pattern U. The scheme describes the variables to be Observed and provides a means for measuring the degree of undesir- ability present in a given situation. Diagnostic schemes in the medical sciences are based on highly elaborate classification schemes Of symptoms of diseases along with very precise diagnostic tools for assessing the incidence and magnitude of disease. The diagnostic tools available to medical practitioners include such things as thermometers, x-ray machines, blood pressure gauges, and stethoscopes. Such tools are used to uncover diseases and their level of severity. Within the behavioral sciences diagnostic tools are not as precise. Those that are available include 78 such things as personality scales, attitude surveys, interviews, and return of investment. These tools are designed to uncover the presence Of certain undesirable patterns of behavior and their magnitude in given situations. They are not designed to change the patterns or explain why they occur. The Transitioneppoducipg Mechanism The process of developing a transition-producing mechanism for moving from behavior pattern U to behavior D is called "theranosis." It is distinct from diagnosis in that diagnosis establishes the presence of an undesirable behavior pattern while theranosis prescribes a mechanism for changing it into a desired behavior pattern. Examples of transition-producing mechanisms include: hallucinatory drugs, hypnosis, operant conditioning, reciprocal inhibition, conceptual insights, acting out, incentive payment plans, participation in decision making, and decentralization. There are two major categories of variables on which transition-producing mechanism may be constructed: environmental variables and change Object variables. 79 Environment variables are those which lie outside the change Object and, hence, are not a property of the change Object itself. Examples of environmental variables are: the behavior of other organization participants, leadership styles, incentive plans, drugs, and group norms. Often times, a change agent attempts to alter or modify the values Of these kinds of variables in order to produce the change he desires in the object of change. Change Object variables are those which lie within the change Object itself. These variables are usually of two types: perceptual variables and response variables. Perceptual variables are those through which the change Object assigns meaning to different environ— mental variables. Sometimes it is assumed that modification of this meaning will lead to the desired changes in behavior. For example, a change agent might be concerned with changing the behavior Of someone who he belfeves is restricting output. He wants to increase this person's output. In searching for the reason output is restricted he notes that the person perceives IJIlOIl .1»! its F! p. I . 1}.» v 80 his boss as a tyrant. Taking note of this the change agent assumes this is the reason the person is restrict- ing output. He hypothesizes that if he can change the person's perception of his boss as a tyrant, he will succeed in changing his output response. If the person no longer perceives his boss as a tyrant, his productivity will supposedly increase. Once the change agent establishes the basic hypothesis he focuses attention on finding a way of changing the person's perception Of his boss. He constructs a mechanism for moving from behavior pattern U, in which the person's productivity is considered low and he sees his boss as a tyrant, to behavior pattern D, in which his productivity is greater and he no longer seahis boss as a tyrant. Response variables consist. of those which make up an individual's behavioral pattern for relating to environmental variables. They deal with how he responds to environmental variables ratherthan how he perceives them. Sometimes change agents assume that a person's behavior can be changed by directly altering or modifying the way he characteristically responds to 81 environmental variables. For example, a change agent may be confronted with a person who experiences an inhibitory anxiety when he is in the presence of his boss. His goal may be to eliminate or reduce this anxiety. He may observe that a person cannot simultaneously be relaxed :and , anxious in responding to some element in his environment. He may, therefore, advance a hypothesis that getting the person to respond with relaxation in the presence Of his boss will inhibit his responding in terms Of anxiety. His problem then becomes one of finding a mechanism for moving from behavior pattern U, in which the person responds with anxiety when in the presence of his boss, to behavior pattern D, in which the person responds with relaxation. He does not, however, directly attempt to change perceptual variables in constructing a mechanism. He is not concerned with why the person sees the situation in such a way that he responds with anxiety. Instead, he attempts to directly alter and modify the person's response to the situation. A change in the percepticui of the person may occur as a result Of altering his responses but it occurs after 82 the change and, hence, is not partioned into the mechanism. In sum a transition-producing mechanism consists of a set of propositions that state how an undesirable behavior pattern can be transformed into a more desirable pattern. Its utilization by a change agent represents the application Of control knowledge to achieve a desired change in the behavior of an object of change. The Operationalizing Process The process of operationalizing behavioral change models consists of converting them from a conceptual form to a form in which they can be applied in a given case to change the behavior of some object. It is a fourfold process. First, behavior patterns and transition-producing variables contained in the model must be identified and defined. The number of variables constitutes the parameter of the model. Second, propositions relating behavior pattern variables to transition-producing variables must be developed. Without such propositions there would by definition, be no model but only a set of variables. Only if a change agent has such propositions can he 83 determine what variables to manipulate in order to reach behavior pattern D. Third, an objective system for measuring the values of the behavior patterns and transition-producing variables must be developed. Without such a system, a change agent could not determine the validity Of the propositions, the magnitude of change, or the effectiveness of the change model he uses. Fourth, statements indicating how the values of transition-producing variables can be changed must be developed. Without these statements a change agent would not know how to change the values of transition- producing variables and, hence, would not be able to apply the model in a given situation. It is only after these processes have been completed that a model can be said to be Operational from an application point of view. The Simple Human Relations Model: .Ap Illustration 2; the Operationalizing Process The simple human relations model is drawn from Dale Carnegie's book entitled, How pp Win Friends and 1For an excellent discussion.of problems involved in operationalizing knowledge from the behavioral sciences see: H. Guetzkow, "Conversion Barriers in Using the Soc1al Sc1ences," Adm1n1strat1ve Sc1ence Quarterly, IV (1959), 68-81. 84 Influence People.1 It is intended to serve as an illustration of the process of Operationalizing behavioral change models.2 The variables comprising the simple human relations model are: individual A, individual B, A's lack of influence over B's behavior pattern, A's desire to influence B's behavior in some manner, a state of nonfriendship between A and B, A's giving of compliments and favors to B, B's acceptance of A as a friend, A's attempt to influence B's behavior pattern, B's acceptance of A's influence attempts, A's threat to terminate his relationship with B, and A's emotional detachment from B. As the title of Carnegie's book suggests, the basic proposition relating these variables is that if A can get B to accept him as a friend, he can influence B's behavior. The model does not specify the specific variables in B's behavior pattern that A desires to 1 . . . D. Carnegie, How pg Win Fr1ends and Ipfluence People (New York: Pocket Books, 1958). 2In constructing the simple Human Relations Model for changing behavior, I have drawn from.H. J. Leavitt, Managerial Psychology (2d ed.; Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1964), pp. 181-88. 1E1. . IE- 1» 9.10 u o t a 85 change through achieving influence. A's general goal is to move from a situation in which he has no influence over B's behavior pattern to a situation in which he has influence over it. The major transition-producing mechanism for achieving influence is A's attempts to secure B's friendship through doing him favors and giving him compliments. The basic proposition is that B will respond to A's compliments and favors by becoming his friend. If B becomes A's friend, A's attempts at influencing his behavior will be successful. He will have moved from a situation of non-influence over B's behavior to a situation of influence over it. This is the basic model for winning friends in order to influence them. There are, however, a few other aspects of it which require explanations. The first aspect is its explanation Of why friend- ship allows one to influence the behavior of others. The rationale appears to be that "friendship" evinces a social Obligation between the parties involved. If A does favors for B and gives him compliments, B owes A something in return. Because of this assumed norm 86 of reciprocity A can ask B, once they are friends, to do something he wants him to do.1 If B has accepted A as a friend, he will do it since non—performance would signify that he did not consider A to be his friend. There is also a mediating factor that increases the probability of B accepting A's influence attempts. If he has accepted A as a friend and values their friendship, he will experience guilt or anxiety in violating the social Obligation of friendship. Hence, it seems that the model assumes there is some type of norm prohibiting violation Of these Obligations and that peOple who do so will experience guilt or anxiety. Another important aspect of the model is the need for A's emotional detachment from B. A has an instrumental purpose for establishing a friendship with B. He values their relationship simply for its contributions in allowing him to influence B's behavior. The rationale for A's needing tO be emotionally detached from B is that if he valued the relationship intrinsi- cally, he would not enter into the relationship with iFog an interesting discussion on the norm of reciprocity see: A. W. Gouldner, "The Norm of Reciprocity: A Preliminary Statement," American Socio— logical Revigy, XXV (1960), 161-78. 87 the specific intention Of changing B's behavior. If he were to become emotionally attached to B, it could seemingly erect a barrier against effecting changes in B's behavior. From an operational point of view, one of the most difficult problems with the modeljis determining when "friendship" is established. How can a person determine when another person becomes his friend? The resolution Of this problem requires the development of a definition of "friendship" in terms of a set of observable characteristics. Only with a definition of this type is it possible to empirically determine the presence or absence of friendship in a given situation. One approach to providing such a definition might consist of a statement that "friendship” has occurred when A asks B to do something he would not otherwise do, and he does it. If A asks B to change his behavior from X to Z and B does it, but would not do it without A's influence, then, by definition, they are friends. B changes to behavior Z because, it is inferred, that he values his relationship with A. 88 For purposes of analysis B's valuation of the friend— ship is labeled F. Using this symbolic set of notations, A wants Z from B and has F to offer in exchange while B wants F from A and has Z to Offer in exchange. If B is hesitant about changing his behavior, A can threaten to terminate F in order to increase the probability that B will succumb to his influence. When A Obtains Z and B obtains F by definition a friendship exists. Using this definition Of friendship, A could not really tell if B was his friend until after he attempted to influence B's behavior. Hence, it is an ad hpg conceptualization in which "friendship" is viewed as simply being a dichotomous variable. A person is either your friend or not your friend. With this definition one only looks at the behavioral result Of friendship and, if it is in line with the foregoing definition, infers that a friendship relation- ship exists between A and B. Simple as this definition is there are innumerable problems in trying to apply it to a specific situation. To apply it a technique for 89 determining if B would have changed his behavior with- out these attempts would also be required since changes in his behavior might occur but not be due to A's attempts. In order to fully Operationalize the model, terms such as "favors," "compliments," "influence," and "emotional detachment" would have to be defined in terms of observable phenomena so that their presence in a given situation could be ascertained; that is, what behavioral actions constitute or signify "favors," "compliments," "influence," and "emotional detachment"? It is only after these terms are defined in observable sets that procedures for measuring their values in a given situation can be developed, and the model to be fully operational requires a system for measuring the values of variables contained within it. Analysis 9: Propositions B.3 and B.4 The construct of a behavioral change model makes it possible to assess behavioral knowledge contained in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences in terms of control knowledge. In accordance with proposition B.4, the construct can only lead 90 to control knowledge when: 1. It contains variables that a change agent can control; 2. It offers an Objective system for measuring the values of the variables; 3. There are valid relationships between the variables. The first condition is necessary because if a change agent cannot exert control over the values of the variables in the model he cannot use it to change behavior. The second condition is necessary because if a change agent cannot measure the values of the variables in the model he cannot determine either the direction of change or its magnitude. The third condition is necessary because if the relationships in the model are not valid the change that occurs may be due to exogenous variables. Hence, the model would not be based on control knowledge. The construct of a behavioral change model coupled with the operationalizing process constitutes a conversion framework. The utility of a specific behavioral change model built through use of the frame- work can only be determined pg hoc. If a change agent 91 can utilize it to change behavior, by definition, it is useful and proposition B.3 would be supported. The construct of a behavioral change model would be capable of converting behavioral science knowledge into a form in which managers could use it for solving their behavioral problems. Jul:- :1 . ?.!’lyr.xe .§H§\A CHAPTER IV PSYCHOTHERAPEUTIC MODELS FOR CHANGING BEHAVIOR: AN ANALYSIS OF PROPOSITION C: 1A Analysis 2; Proposition C:lA In this chapter proposition C:lA will be analyzed through applying the conversion framework to three psychotherapeutic models: Psychoanalysis, Rational- Emotive Therapy and Conditioning Therapy. Each of these models will be described in terms of the construct of a behavioral change model and evaluated in terms of the A premises and prOposition B.4. The three models have two major sources of vari- ables: patient variables and therapist variables. Patient variables are a therapist's statements and observations about the patient's behavior. They constitute a description of behavior patterns U and D when the patient is considered the object of change. Therapist variables, on the other hand, refer to the therapist's behavior and rules for moving the patient from behavior 92 93 pattern U to behavior pattern D. They constitute the therapist's transition-producing mechanism when the therapist is considered a change agent. The Freudian P§ychoanalytic Model Analysis pf Behavior Pattern g In the Freudian psychoanalytic model, behavior pattern U is described in terms of: 1. Symptoms exhibited by P. 2. The determinant of the symptoms. Symptoms are the observable characteristics of the patient's behavior that indicate the presence of a psychological disorder. Since they are only a manifestation of a disorder, treating them will not 1This presentation of Freud's psychoanalytic model is based on the following sources: S. Freud, A General Iptroduction pp_Psychoanalysis, trans. J. Riviere (New York: Permabooks, 1953): S. Freud, Moses and Monotheism (New York: Alfred Knopf, Inc., 1957); S. Freud, N y Iptroductory Lectures pp_Psychoanalysis (New York: W. W. Norton & Co., Inc., 1933); S. Freud, Egg Question p£_pgy.Ana1ysis, trans. J. Strachey (London: The Hogarth Press, 1959); S. Freud, Therapy gpg Technigue, ed. P. Rieff (New York: Collier Books, 1963); P. J. Stern, Egg Abnormal Person and His WOrld: Ag Iptroductigp_pp Psychopathology (Princeton: D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 1963), pp. 196-211; N. walker, A. Short History pp Psychotherapy 1p Theory gpg Practice (New York: The Noonday Press, 1959). j. P 15.. ha 94 eliminate the disorder. The disorder can only be eliminated by changing the determinant of the symptoms. The Relationship Between Symptoms ang their Determinants Freud maintained that at one level of abstraction the cause of the patient's symptoms was a repressed sexual conflict between his id and ego that occurred during his childhood. The conflict alledgedly arose from a situation in which gratification of the id's urges was foregone by the infantile ego because it perceived a danger that would accrue if it permitted gratification. The perceived danger, according to Freud, was "irrational" but the infantile ego was too weak to see it. For example, a child was believed to desire at one time or another, at the urging of his id, intercourse with his mother. But at the same time it was believed that he feared his father would cut off his penis if he attempted to do it. Becoming casterated was alledgedly the danger the child perceived if he sought gratification of the urgings of his id. His 95 fear was held to be "irrational" since "objectively" no such danger existed and logically he should have seen that the desire was incestual and needed to be rechanneled into more socially acceptable mediums. He could not however see these things because his ego was weak. Hence, since he was incapable of handling this "irrational" conflict Freud postulated he would repress it. Repressing it simply meant that he put it out of his consciousness which allowed him to momentarily reduce the "unpleasure" he felt over the conflict. But in repressing it the conflict did not go out of existence, it simply seeped to his unconscious to come back and haunt him later on in the form of symptoms. Symptoms when they appeared were supposed to be an indication of and substitute for the patient's unachieved instinctual gratification. Indeed, the formation of symptoms was called "the return of the repressed." The symptoms that emerged were distorted from the original conflict and appeared on the surface to have no relationship to it. They were in disguised 96 form an "acting out" of the unresolved and unconscious conflict. To eliminate the symptoms, the therapist had to eliminate the repressed conflict since it was held to be the determinant of them. The Relationshipretween the pg and Ego According to Freud, the id and the ego were not rigid and well-defined components of an individual's personality. At infancy, personality was supposedly composed entirely of the id. The id represented unconscious instincts and passions. It was the source of energy for behavior since it pressed for immediate satisfaction of instincts and passions. In pressing for satisfaction, the id operated under what Freud called the "pleasure principle." This principle states that as energy in the id accumulates it is felt as "unpleasure" by the individual and impels him to reduce the feeling by obtaining immediate gratification of the urgings of his id. The source of gratification lies in the environ- ment in which an individual is immersed. Some of the 97 sources in the environment he might utilize to attain gratification could result in the destruction of his self. The id, however, does not discriminate between sources and seeks immediate gratification without regard to self-preservation. The ego emerges to help the individual discrim- inate sources in his environment in his pursuit of gratification of his instincts and passions. Its function is to preserve the self while securing grati- fication. It performs this function by analyzing the degree of danger present in utilizing different sources and channeling the individual's behavior to avoid dangerous sources. The egt> mediates between the demands of the environment and the urgings of the id. It decides how, when, and where the urgings of the id will be gratified in such a way that the self is preserved. The ego may decide to forego immediate gratifi- cation if it would endanger the self and search for substitute sources that are not so dangerous. While the id seeks only pleasure, the ego seeks pleasure and the avoidance of unpleasure. It takes into account 98 objects in the environment which if pursued for their immediate pleasure would result in "self-destruction." The principle under which the ego Operates is what Freud called the "reality-principle." This principle states that individuals tend to seek and utilize Objects in the environment to achieve gratification of urgings of the id without incurring other difficulties which will be felt as unpleasure. In short, the ego delays gratification when the probability of danger is high and seeks substitute sources to avoid unpleasure. It, in effect, substitutes thoughtful behavior for completely impulsive behavior. Since the ego emerged from the id, Freud contended that in a "healthy" person there was no division between them. The person's behavior was under the control of his ego rather than his id. He was aware of his instincts and passions and the dangers which immediate gratification might bring if he followed certain socially unapproved channels. Analysis 9; Sphavior Pattern 2 Behavior pattern D is described in terms of 99 the absence of symptoms and their cause. The therapist removes the patient‘s symptoms by bringing the repressed conflict into his consciousness so that his adult ego can analyze it and see that it is "irrational." As Freud described this goal: "Where id was, there shall ego be." Under behavior pattern D, the patient is to be aware of his repressed conflict. He is to recognize his instinctual desires, the demands of reality, and be able to apply reason to the choice of an effective source of action for gaining gratification. In short, his id is to become subject to the influence of his ego O The T;ansition~—Producing Mechanism The transition-producing question faced in the psychoanalytic model is: How can the patient's repressed conflict be made conscious so that it can be put under the control of his ego rather than his id? Freud constructed four major transition-producing techniques: dream analysis, free association, transference, and the fee-setting process. 100 pream Analysis One of Freud's earliest techniques for bringing the unconscious conflict into the Open was dream analysis. He believed that dreams were a projection of the conflict as well as a dnguised gratification of the repressed desire comprising it. He believed the therapist could discover the nature of the conflict by analyzing the content of the patient's dreams. The therapist was to analyze dream content by means of "interpretation." Interpretation was defined as a process of "discovering a hidden meaning." Freud assumed that a dream was a disguised acting out of the patient's conflict in which his id obtained its desires. The dream was postulated to be a valid representation of his unresolved conflict because in sleep supposedly the repressing and censoring part of the conscious mind relaxed its vigilance. When this occurred, the content of the dream was more directly determined by the content of the unconscious. lFreud, A_General Introduction pg Psychoanalysis, p. 91. 101 Freud thought the therapist could determine the nature of the patient's repressed conflict by interpreting the dream ideas in terms of a sexual conflict between the id and the ego. Dream ideas had to be interpreted because they were assumed to undergo a censorship by the unconscious. The censor- ship process was called dream-work. It was defined as a process in which the meaning of a dream was disguised in order to reduce the unpleasure that might arise if the repressed conflict came to the surface inaidream. Through dream-work the sexual properties of the conflict were translated into symbols which were more acceptable to the individual's consciousness. Consequently, the manifest content of a dream was a distortion of the actual repressed conflict. A dream, so to speak, was not as it appeared to be. Interpretation of the latent meaning of a dream was based on an assumption that almost all dream content was sexual. The assumption was necessary because interpretation consisted of associating non-sexual dream ideas with sexual prOperties of individuals. 102 The interpretative framework for dream analysis consisted of a set of sex symbols for ideas manifested in the dream content. Freud postulated that the number of sex symbols represented in dreams was not great. They consisted of the human body, parents, children, brothers and sisters, birth, death, nakedness, genitals, sexual processes, and intercourse. Using these sexual properties of human beings, Freud attempted to apply them to non-sexual Objects and processes which he believed served as symbols for them. For example, a tree symbolized a penis while climbing represented intercourse. How did Freud establish these symbolic equations? Freud was not clear in stating how one equated a sexual property with a seemingly non—sexual object or process. He in fact stated that: The symbolic relation is a comparison of a quite peculiar kind, the nature of which is as yet not fully clear to us. Perhaps some indication will be found later which will throw some light upon this unknown quantity.l 1Ibid., p. 160. 103 He did, however, indicate how he arrived at some of his associations. He thought that one could derive the association from widely different sources which included fairy tales, myths, songs and the colloquial usage of a language. An example of such a derivation is as follows: We may at first find it surprising that parents appear in our dreams as kings and emperors and their consorts, but we have a parallel to this in fairy tales. Does it not begin to dawn upon us that the many fairy tales which begin with the words 'Once upon a time there were a king and queen' simply means 'Once upon a time there were a father and mother'? In the family life the children are sometimes spoken of jestingly as princes and the eldest son as the crown prince. The king himself is called the father of his people.1 Freud's contention that an individual learns from myths and fairy tales to equate sexual prOperties with non-sexual objects and processes is certainly Open to question. It suggests that human beings unconsciously equate the two. For example, they unconsciously equate trees with penises. Furthermore, it assumes that they unconsciously translate their repressed sexual conflict l;p;g,, p. 168. 104 into non-sexual terms. Without this assumption the therapist would not be able to use Freud's "theory" of the neurosis to "cure" his patient. Why is this so? The primary reason is that the assumption is predicated on another assumption that the “cause" of the patient's symptoms is a repressed sexual conflict. Only by granting the validity of this assumption can the therapist proceed to discover by means of interpre- tation how the patient expresses the sexual conflict in non-sexual terms. Only if the assumption is valid can he take sexual properties and equate them with the patient's non-sexual expression of objects and processes. Sgp equatingthem does not "prove" that the patient's sygptoms are "caused" py.g_represseg_sexual conflict. To understand the meaning of this statement the self- validating bias of the psychoanalytic model must be examined. The patient's equation of sexual properties with non-sexual objects and processes is unconscious as is his sexual conflict. He does not consciously know about either of them. Hence he does not consciously 105 know what his dream really means. As Freud put it: . . . the dreamer really does know the meaning of his dreams: only he does not know that he knows, and therefore thinks that he does not.1 If the dreamer does not know that he knows, how can he know that he knows? Freud's answer is that he can know if the analyst demonstrates to him that the manifest content of his dreams is not what it appears to be. Rather, it represents certain sexual properties and relationships between them which he has repressed. If the patient accepts the symbolic equation and interpretation of the determinant of his symptoms he is pronounced "cured." If he does not, he is resisting and, hence, is still ill. The crucial question is if the patient's acceptance of the interpretation means that his symptoms were actually caused by a repressed sexual conflict. This would not appear to be the case because he has been told what the determinant is by the analyst, who only assumed it. Since the conflict was alledgedly in the patient's unconscious, he can only take the 1Ibid., p. 106. 106 therapist's word for it that such is the case. The therapist does not admit any other determinant and by "teaching" the patient how to "interpret" his illness, he sets in motion a self-validating bias. In short, the patient is taught to accept without evidence the validity of Freud's "theory" of the neurosis. If he does not accept it, he is considered to be ill. Free Association Out of his work on dream-analysis Freud evoked a similar technique which he called "free association.‘ It consisted of a rule that the patient would tell the therapist everything that came into his mind. Freud used to tell his patients: Your talk with me must differ in one respect from an ordinary conversation. Whereas usually you rightly try to keep the threads of your story together and to exclude all intruding associations and side-issues, so as not to wander too far from the point, here you must proceed differently. You will not1ce that as you relate things various ideas will occur to you which you feel inclined to put aside with certain criticisms and Objections. You will be tempted to say to yourself: This or 107 that has no connection here, or it is quite unimportant, or it is nonsensical, so it cannot be necessary to mention it. Never give in to these objections, but mention it even if you feel a disinclination against it, or indeed just because of this. Later on you will perceive and understand the reason for this injunction, which is really the only one you have to follow. So say whatever goes through your mind. Act as if you were sitting at the window of a railway train and describing to someone behind you the changing views you see out- side. Finally, never forget that you have promised absolute honesty, and never leave anything unsaid because for any reason it is unpleasant to say it.1 One reason Freud developed the technique of free association was that he £31: if one could "interpret" the repressed conflict from dreams he could also do it from conscious thoughts based on free association since free association reduced censoring by the conscious mind. Hence, non-sexual ideas which an individual usually censors out of his talks with other people could be subjected to symbolic interpretation. The therapist could equate sexual properties with non-sexual Objects and processes revealed by the patient's free association. lFreud, Therapy and Technigue, p. 147. 108 Transference Transference was another way of finding out what was repressed. Transference, according to Freud was a process in which the patient acteg_out his repressed sexual conflict in the therapy situation. Because the therapist maintained a "neutral" attitude toward the patient any emotional behavior directed by him toward the therapist could only be symbolic of his unconscious conflict. In the analytic situation there was "objectively" no way for the patient to become emotionally involved with the therapist. Consequently emotional behavior by the patient toward the therapist must stem from somewhere else. This somewhere else was the ; patient's repressed conflict which he acted out in the therapy sessions. Freud described the concept of transference in the following manner: The neurotic sets himself to the work because he believes in the analyst, and he believes in him because he begins to entertain certain feelings towards him. The attitude is, in fact--to put it bluntly--a kind of falling in love. The patient repeats, in the form of falling in love with the analyst, 1‘11". nahafi... 109 psychical attitudes which lay ready within him, and which were intimately linked with the inception of his neurosis. . . . What he is showing is the very core of his most private life; pp_;§_pa1pab1y reproducing i3, pp.though ;p_were all happenings pp the present, instead g§_remembering;p, With this the riddle of transference--love is solved. . .1 The technique like the other two is designed to elicit material for the therapist to interpret. It also rests on the symbolic equation of ”interpretation." When the patient accepts the "interpretation” and can apply the symbolic equation by himself he is pronounced "cured." As has been demonstrated, this process of "interpretation" is highly conjectural. The See-Setting Process Freud regarded the therapist's fee as having a direct relationship to the patient's cure. As he put it: It is well known that the value of the treatment is not enhanced in the patient's eyes if a very low fee is asked. Later while discussing the role of the fee in the therapeutic process he states: lFreud, The Qgestion p£_Lay Anglysis, p. 266. 2Freud, Therapy and Technigue, p. 144. 110 Gratuitous treatment enormously increases many neurotic resistances, such as the temptations of the transference-relationship for young women, or the Opposition to the Obligatory gratitude in young men arising from the father-complex, which is one of the most troublesome obstacles to the treatment. The absence of the corrective influence in payment of the professional fee is felt as a serious handicap; the whole relationship recedes into an unreal world; and the patient is deprived of a useful incentive to exert himself to bring the cure to an end.1 According to this passage, the fee was to serve as an incentive for the patient to want to be cured and to do what the therapist told him. Freud, however, did not experiment with the effect of fees on the rate of ”cure." For example, did patients recover faster the greater the relative cost to them of the treatment? Did the increasing total cost of therapy have an influence on the rate at which they accepted Freud's interpretation of the "determinant" of their illness? Did they, in fact, tend to agree with the therapist to stop depleting their financial resources? 1Ibid., p. 145. 111 Freud considered money and the process of setting fees and collecting them a vital part of the therapy process. He felt that money was a sex symbol and that the patient sometimes revealed his repressed conflict when he and the therapist would discuss financial matters. As he put it: The analyst does not dispute that money is to be regarded first and foremost as the -means by which life is supported and power is obtained, but he maintains that, besides this, powerful sexual factors are involved in the value set upon it: he may expect, therefore, that money questions will be treated by cultured people in the same manner as sexual matters, with the same inconsistency, prudishness and hypocrisy. He is therefore determined beforehand not to concur in this attitude, and in his deal- ings.with patients to treat money matters with the same matter-of-course frankness that he wishes to induce in them towards matters relating to sexual life. By volun- tarily introducing the subject of fees and stating the price for which he gives his time, he shows the patient that he himself has cast aside false shame in these matters. Of interest in the passage is the therapist's use of himself as a model. Because Freud thought money represented sexual matters, he felt that the therapist 1Ibid., pp. 143-44. 112 could show the patient how to "rationally" look at them by showing him how "rational" he was in discussing money matters. The therapist, in effect, put himself out as a model that the patient was to emulate. Evaluation 9; the Psychoanalytic Model One problem with the model is that many of the propositions comprising it have not been experimentally validated. Freud's attempt to validate the model was twofold. First, he claimed that if the patients symptoms appeared to be removed, the model must be valid. However, he did not establish through controlled experimentation that the rate of symptom removal from using his model was significantly greater than it would have been had the model not been used. Thus, symptom removal could have been due to variables over which the therapist had no control. The patient could have lost his symptoms without the therapist's influence. Even if Freud could have experimentally determined the symptom removal effectiveness of his model, he would not have known why it was effective. He could have only III... I... no . 113 stated that for some reason the use of the model resulted in a faster rate of symptom removal than not using it. In order to determine why the model worked more effectively, he would have needed to empirically test the propositions comprising the model. For example, all symptoms are caused by a repressed sexual conflict, or transference is an acting out of the repressed conflict. Instead of experimentation, Freud used a self- validating check to test the internal validity of the model. If patients accepted the model's interpretation of the cause of their symptoms, it was valid. To be considered "cured," they had to accept the interpretation. If they did not accept the interpretation they were considered to be resisting.l Hence, the model has a self-validating bias because patients must accept the interpretation to be pronounced cured. Thus, in terms of proposition B.4, the psychoanalytic model does not provide control knowledge. 1A similar criticism is advanced by: C. H. Campbell, Induced Delusions: The Psychopathy 9;, Freudism (Chicago: Regent House, 1957). 114 The empirical validity of the prOpositions comprising the model have not been determined. Moreover, there is no objective measurement system for defining and measuring variables within the model. Therefore, in accordance with premise A.3, the model is not useful. The Rational-Emotive Modell Analysis p§_Behavior Pattern g. The description of behavior pattern U consists of: 1. The patientksdissatisfaction with some aspect of his behavior. For example, he feels depressed, anxiety-ridden, angry, or guilty. These are considered as symptoms. 2. The patient's lack of control over his emotions. This is considered the determinant of his symptoms. IIrithe rational-emotive model, the therapist attempts to Eilter the determinant (2) to remove the symptoms (1). 1This presentation of Ellis's rational-emotive therapy is based on: A. Ellis, Reason and Emotion _i_p ~PSychotherapy (New York: Lyle Stuart, 1963). ,w— 115 The Nature p§_Emotions The patient's emotional control is defined as his ability to influence the experiencing of dissatis— faction. To understand the definition it is necessary to examine Ellis' usage of the term "emotion" and "reason.“ Emotions are defined as both a form of thought and physical sensations. Ellis defines them in terms of the behavior they represent. He states that 'individuals: feel good because (a) they receive pleasant physical sensations (such as good odors, tastes, sounds, sights, and caresses) and (b) they think or believe that some person or thing is delightful or charming. And they feel bad because they encounter unpleasant physical stimuli and they think or believe that some person or thing is horrible, frightful, or terrible.l It is contended that evaluative thoughts are subject to control while physical sensations are not. Emotional control refers to an individual's influence over his own experiencing of discomfort through controlling his evaluative thoughts about 1Ibid., p. 333. 116 himself. His control over them is held to be a function of what is called "reflective appraisal." In behavior pattern U, the therapist maintains that the patient's evaluative thoughts are accepted and internalized without a "reflective" appraisal. The patient thinks, for example, that a given situation is "horrible" and "feels bad" about it because he has been taught to do so. He has not asked himself why it is "terrible" and if "rationally" it is "terrible." He has accepted and internalized evaluative thoughts blindly and without the use of reason. He thinks something is "terrible" because that is what he has been taught to think. Since the patient accepts the evaluative thoughts he has been taught without "rationally" examining them, the therapist contends he has no control over them. His experiencing of discomfort is sustained by his continuous application of unreflectively induced evaluative thoughts. In applying them, he continues to allow his behavior to be directed by the party who taught them to him. In sum, the patient perceives Objects in his environment, evaluates them as "good" or "bad," and 117 responds to them on the basis of his evaluation. However, his evaluations differ in the degree to which they are the product of "reflective appraisals." Responses of discomfort by him are viewed as not caused by external factors in his environment but by his interpretation of them as being "good" or "bad" for him. His inter- pretation of them is colored by his earlier conditioning. He is taught how to appraise objects as being “good" or "bad" without the use of reason. When his appraisal of objects is "unreflective," he lacks control over his evaluative thoughts, and if they are negative, he experiences discomfort. His thinking has been condi- tioned, so to speak, to make him Epipk_he is sick. How and why do the evaluative thoughts he accepts and internalizes make him sick? The Nature 9; Reason The reason for the patient having no control over his emotions, is supposedly because he has accepted and internalized certain "irrational" beliefs deve10ped and taught to him by his parents and society. He has 118 not only accepted these beliefs but uses them, since they are evaluative in nature, to regulate his behavior. And this is held by the rational-emotive therapist to be the heart of his problem. The "irrational" beliefs he accepts and internalizes are norms developed by his society or his parents to control and regulate his behavior. Being norms they are evaluative statements that indicate what is "good" and "bad" for one to do and feel. For example, it is ”good'' for one to seek the approval of others. This norm is designed to channel and direct an individual's attention and behavior in trying to earn the approval of others and avoid their disapproval. But norms if they are to in: control devices must be backed by some form of sanction for non-compliance. There must be a penalty that will increase the probability of compliance. In the case of the foregoing norm, the sanction is that one should feel "worthless" or "bad," if he does not strive to attain the approval of others. If others express disapproval with him, he 119 should feel guilty and ashamed as well as 'worthless' because of his failing. The patient is taught to punish and blame himself for not living up to the norut through saying to himself: "I am worthless because other disapprove of me. This is terrible and I must be punished by my feelings of guilt." When he violates the norm he experiences guilt, because he has been taught that one should be approved by others; and it is "terrible" if he is not. Hence, the evaluative thoughts (emotions) he has accepted without question has resulted in his dissatisfaction. The foregoing suggests that society is partly responsible for the patient's emotional plight since it has implanted the 'irrational' norms by which he evaluates his behavior. It has taught him to blame himself when he violates its norms. The patient, however, is partly responsible because he has accepted these norms without 'reflective appraisal' and keeps reindoctrinating himself with them. He continues to blame himself for not living up to the norm. And blame, Ellis claims, is the essence of all 120 states of emotional dissatisfaction because it is an "irrational" concept. According to Ellis, blame is an “irrational" concept because it is the Opposite of a "rational" concept. The term rational is defined as: . . . showing reason; not foolish or silly; sensible; leading to efficient results for human happiness; producing desired effects with a minimum of expense, waste, unnecessary effort, or unpleasant side effects.1 The concept of blame is irrational because: 1. It is not based on "reason" and, hence, is foolish or silly. 2. It leads to inefficient results for human happiness. According to the definition, an individual's behavior in regards to blame is irrational because: 1. He does not use "reason"in accepting and internalizing it. 2. Accepting and internalizing it results in his unhappiness. Ellis says that the concept of blame is not based on "reason" because it implies an individual is 1Ibid., p. 343. 121 "worthless" when he violates some norm. "WOrthlessness," or “personal worth," is a concept that is strictly definitional. It is not capable of being "scientifically proven" since there is no empirical evidence to which it can be referred. Ellis tells patients: Granting that I cannot prove that you are worthwhile because you exist, by the same token you cannot prove that you are worthless because you do not succeed in life, or do not attain your potential, or cannot win the love of significant others. Because your concept of worthlessness, like my definition of worth, is also a definition. And how can you prove a definition?1 He tells patients that man is "worthwhile" simply because he exists, not because of his extrinsic achievements that others may value. The term "worthwhile" is definitional because by his definition human existence equals personal worth. Because the concept of blame is definitional it is not based on “reason.“ The second reason the concept is "irrational" is that it allegedly results in human unhappiness. Unhappiness occurs because the individual is taught that he should like himself when he succeeds and hate 1Ibid., p. 148. 122 himself when he fails. As Ellis phrased it: . . . he is taught that because others dislike or disapprove of him when he fails to master something, he should accept their evaluation of himself and make it his own. The acceptance of the evaluation of others results in human unhappiness and the patient's current plight. Thus, the concept of blame, by Ellis' definition, is "irrational" and results in "irrational" behavior. Analysis p§_Behavior Pattern Q Behavior pattern D is characterized by the patient's control over his emotions. He is capable of "rationally” analyzing the evaluative thoughts he has unreflectively accepted and internalized. Furthermore, he believes that he does not need the approval of others. Hence, society's control over his emotions decreases while self control over them increases. Ellis describes behavior pattern D as follows: The patient of RT is never considered 'cured' or minimally disturbed until he has learned to truly and consistently challenge his 1Ibid., p. 150. 123 underlying irrational assumptions, to think in a fairly straight manner about himself and his intimate associates as well as about external things and events, and to stand on his own two feet without any dire need for support from the therapist or anyone else.1 Idle therapist attempts to demonstrate to the patient that the norms he has accepted and internalized are the cause of his unhappiness and that they are "irrational." By this he hOpes to give the patient control over his emotions. In the process, he attempts to change the patient's perception and interpretation of external occurrences. The patient is cured when he sees and realizes that the norms he has accepted and internalized are "irrational" and that he can control his own emotions through "rationally" analyzing them. The therapist does not attempt to adjust the patient to societal norms but neither does he attempt the reverse. He helps the patient to rationally analyze them and to remain undisturbed when he is placed in a position where non-compliance in an overt sense with 1Ibid., p. 362. 124 the norms would place him in a disadvantageous position. According to Ellis: Patients . . . normally acquire the philosophy that it is wise to accept unpleasant people and circumstances when (a) it is of practical advantage to do so, or (b) there is really no other choice. 'Thus, they learn to accept unanxiously and unhostilely an unfair supervisor or boss when (a) their job has usual advantages aside from their contact with this overseer, or (b) it is presently impossible for them to get a better position with a less unfair boss. Ellis implies that the patient should not attempt to change his supervisor's treatment of him. Perhaps, the supervisor is unaware that the patient resents the way he treats him and would change his behavior if informed. Ellis however does not indicate why the patient should not seek changes in the supervisor's behavior. He is only interested in the patient not feeling disturbed by the situation. The Transition-Producing Mechanism The transition-producing mechanism consists of teaching the patient how to control his emotions. 1Ibid., p. 362. 125 Ellis describes the therapist's role in a summary form by stating: . . . the effective therapist should continually keep unmasking his patient's past and, especially, his present illogical thinking, or self—defeating verbalizations by (a) bringing them forcefully to his attention or consciousness; (b) showing him how they are causing and maintaining his disturbance and unhappiness; (c) demonstrating exactly what the illogical links in his internalized sentences are, and (d) teaching him how to re-think, challenge, contradict, and re-verbalize these (and other similar sentences) so that his internalized thoughts become more logical and efficient.1 How does the therapist teach the patient "to think straight and act effectively?" There are three aspects to this mechanism: the patient's self-analysis of his emotions; the therapist's demonstration that the emotions are irrational; and the therapist's use of modeling. Selp-analysis pg Emotions The therapist's first step is to ascertain the patient's current emotions. He does this through having the patient do "homework." The Phomework" requires the patient to keep a record of what he tells himself when 1Ibid.. pp. 58-59. 126 he faces situations in which he experiences dissatis- faction. This "homework" provides the raw data for the therapy. HI Demonstration p_ the Irrationality _§_Emotions The therapist demonstrates the irrational basis of the patient's reported emotions. As Ellis puts it: . . . he is shown how he concretely and literally creates most of his self- destructive emotions by consciously or (more usually) unconsciously telling himself certain exclamatory and evaluative sentences. Thus, when he feels hurt by being rejected, he is shown that his feeling is created by (a) the fairly same internalized sentence, "I don't like being rejected," and by (b) the decidedly insane sentence, "It is terrible being rejected, and because I don't like it, I can't stand to be rejected in this fashion.1 The technique used in showing the patient the irrationality of his emotions is called the didactic method. Under the didactic method the patient tells the therapist he (a) feels unhappy because he thinks (c) his boss disapproves of him and thinks he is "worthless." The therapist tells him, in turn, that 11bid., p. 332. 127 he has forgotten about (b) which is his internalized evaluative statement to himself that it is "terrible" not to be approved by his boss, or to be considered "worthy" by him. More precisely, (b) is his statement to himself that: "Because the boss thinks I am 'worthless', therefore, I am 'worthless'. It is 'terrible' to be 'worthless' and not be approved by my boss." The therapist tries to demonstrate to the patient that it is his acceptance and internalization of the norm, that one should seek the approval of his boss and feel "terrible" if he does not attain it, that is making him unhappy. It is not his boss's behavior, or expressed disapproval of him that makes him unhappy. He is making himself unhappy by accepting his bosses evaluations of him as his own and telling himself it is "terrible" that the boss's negative evaluation of him is true. Hence it is not outside events or objects which are causing the patient's unhappiness but rather his internalized sentences about them which he has learned from others. 128 Figure 4.1 presents the patient's emotions chain under behavior patterns U and D. The crucial variable in the transition-producing mechanism is demonstrating to the patient that his internalized beliefs in behavior pattern U are irrational. The patient says to himself: "I am worthless because my boss does not think I am competent." The therapist, in response, tells him: "You are worthless only because you consider yourself worthless. Incompetence only makes you valueless because you think it does. If you continue to believe this you will only continue to make yourself unhappy." Ellis attempts to demonstrate the irrationality of the belief by stating: Let us face it; I tell him. 'Assuming that you do measure your intrinsic and extrinsic value by the same kind of scales, and therefore arrive at the conclusion that you are worthless, you must, once this conclusion is reached, thereafter be prey to everlasting feelings of anxiety, guilt depression, and other kinds of emotional upset. On the other hand, if you do not conclude that you are worthless (because, at bottom, your definition of worth is human, personal perfection), you may live with a minimum of anxiety and hostility. Obviously, 129 BehaviopiPattern‘g (a)< Patient's Chain of Causation >(c) Patient's Statement Patient's Feelings of problem about (a) "Boss thinks I'm 'T'm unhappy." worthless.“ za \\‘ Actual chain ’/’ \\\ of ///'// \ causation $(b)/ Patient's Internalized Belief 1. “One should be considered worthy by his boss." 2. "It is 'terrible' if he isn't." Behavior Pattern Q (C) Patient's Feelings 2‘ about (a) (a) Patient's Statement of problem \\ ”Boss thinks IHn\\ "I'm therefore not worthless.“ Patient's and Actual unhappy." \ Chain of Causation / N .4. Patient's Internali Belief 1. "One is worthy whether others think so or not." 2. "It is 'annoying' to be considered worthless but not 'terrible.'“ Fig. 4.1. The Didactic Technique. 130 then, the only pragmatic course to follow is to assume that you are not valueless.l Ellis claims that with this argument he "proves" to the patient that he is "rationally" only worthless by his own arbitrary definition and if he continues to accept this definition, he will inevitably bring on his feelings of discomfort--particularly anxiety, quilt, and depression--that are not necessary. Modeling The therapist serves as a model of "rationality.' In his role as a model, he teaches the patient to "check assumptions, generalize more accurately, and deduce more logically" by showing him how it is done. He is very active in his relationship with the patient and continuously questions his beliefs and the propositions on which they are based. The process of questioning is meant to show the patient how to think ”clearly" and "scientifically" about himself and his associates. An interesting illustration of the modeling process is what Ellis tells a patient when he fails 1Ibid., p. 155. 131 to do the "homework" (check on what he tells himself when he experiences discomfort). He tells him: 'SO you didn't feel like doing the assignment. Tough! Well you're goddam well going to have to do it if you want to overcome the nonsense you keep telling yourself. And you didn't like me for giving you the assignment. Well, I don't give a shit whether you like me or not. We're here not to have a lovey-dovey relationship--and thereby to gratify you for the moment so that you don't have to work to get better--but to convince you that unless you get off your ass you're going to keep stewing in your own neurotic juices forever. Now when are you going to cut out the crap and gp_something to help yourself?‘1 Through this series of statements Ellis shows the patient a number of behaviors he wants him to emulate. First, that he does not need to be "liked" by other peOple. Second, that he is the only one who can "rationally" control his emotions. Thirdly, because he is the only one who can control his emotions, he is the only one who can "cure" himself. Fourthly, he can only "cure" himself by rationally analyzing his evaluative thoughts. 1.132141- . P- 198- 132 Evaluation p§_the Rational-Emotive Model In terms of prOposition B.4, the model does not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. It lacks an objective measurement system, control statements and empirically validated prOpositions. The Lack p§_pp Objective Measurement System The model does not contain an objective measurement system. For example, it does not specify how "rational behavior can be measured objectively. "Rational" behavior is defined as a set of behavior categories including "showing reason," "not foolish or silly," and "human unhappiness." An objective basis for classifying behavior in terms of these categories is not provided. The technique for classifying beliefs as rational or irrational lacks empirical referents. A belief is held to be irrational if it is not based on "reason” and it results in "unhappiness." There is no objective technique for measuring these variables. The measurement 133 is based on the therapist's subjective estimate of their value. Ellis, for example, contends that the belief, "it is a dire necessity for an adult human being to be loved or approved by virtually every significant other person in his community," is irrational because it is definitional.l One cannot empirically demonstrate that it is a "dire necessity." Why this cannot be accomplished is not indicated. The second reason the belief is irrational is that its acceptances results in human unhappiness. Ellis claims it results in human unhappiness for two reasons; it sets an unattainable goal and it creates a constant anxiety. The reason it sets an unattainable goal is "because even if 99 peOple accept or love you, there will always be the hundredth, the hundred-and- first, and so on, who do not."2 Why someone would always dislike a person is not indicated. Ellis simply contends that since the goal can never be attained, the person must always feel frustrated and unhappy. 1Ibid., p. 61. 2Ibid., p. 61 134 Ellis claims that even if a person could achieve such approval, it would result in constant anxiety over how much he is accepted and approved. Moreover, attempting to maintain the approval of others, would result in the person becoming obsequesious to them. He would become less self-directing. He would do what they wanted regardless of whether or not he liked it. He would,tmerefore, experience "unhappiness" as a result of his self-defined "dire need" for the approval of all significant others. Ellis has not been interested in the empirical validity of this proposition. He, in fact, considers it untestable because no person could ever win such approval. Thus these effects could never occur. Lack p£_Smpirically Validated Propositions Because of the lack of an objective measurement technique, propositions contained in the model have not been empirically validated. For example, the proposition that irrational beliefs are learned from society has not been empirically tested. Moreover, there is no 135 prOposition indicating where society acquires the beliefs. The validity of the model has not been established through a controlled experiment. The possibility exists that changes in the behavior are due to exogenous variables rather than those manipulated in the model. The Self-Validating Bias The model has a self-validating bias. The patient is told by the therapist that to remove his symptoms he must change the nature of his evaluative thoughts. The therapist also tells the patient what his evaluative thoughts should be like. For example, if the patient states he wants the approval and love of all significant others, the therapist indicates how he should change the statement. He should say to himself: "What difference does it really make if I am not approved and loved by all? After all it is what I think of myself which is important. If I say to myself that I don't need their love and approval, 136 when they express disapproval with me I won't feel "bad" because I know it isn't "terrible” that they do so. In short, why should I care whether they approve of me or not? If they disapprove and restrict me from getting something I desire, it makes me annoyed but there is no "reason" why I should feel "terrible about it." It the patient accepts the therapist's state- ments, he is pronounced cured. Acceptance is considered to be a measure of validity for the model. However, it is not an objective or independent measure of validity. The Lack g£_Control Statements The mOdel does not provide control statements for changing the patient's internalized beliefs. For example, it does not specify the behaviors the therapist should exhibit in "forcefully bringing the patient's illogical thinking to his attention." A control statement might be that: If the patient's internalized belief is x, the therapist should use behavior A and statements B. 137 The model, however, does not contain such statements. . . . l The Cond1t1on1ng Model Analypis 2; Behavior Pattern g The description of behavior pattern U is twofold. It comprises: 1. The patient's reported symptoms such as a feeling of discomfort, low productiveness, unsatisfactory sex adjustment, and dissatisfaction with interpersonal relationships. 2. The patient's inappropriate conditioning. This is the determinant of the symptoms. The therapist views the symptoms reported by the patient as arising from his having learned unadaptive responses to situations and objects that are not objectively dangerous. In behavior pattern U, the patient is afraid This presentation of Wolpe's conditioning therapy is based on the following sources: J. Wolpe, ngchotherapy py Reciprocal Inhibition (Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1958); J. Wolpe, A. Salter, and L. Reyna (eds.), TQQIConditionipg Therapies: Egg Challenge pp gsychotherapy (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1965). 138 of something that cannot cause him any actual harm. He has learned to respond to it unadaptively. A response is "unadaptive" if it results in unnecessary, unpleasant and non-beneficial feelings on the part of the patient and it leads to "objectively wasteful" expenditure of energy. Wolpe says "fear," for example, meets this criteria and, hence, is "unadaptive.“ As he put it: The very feeling of fear when no real danger is present (for example, when going up in an elevator) is unpleasant and non-beneficial. The fear may also lead to objectively wasteful expenditure of energy in the avoiding of feared situations (such as climbing stairs in the case of the elevator phobia).1 Wolpe states that there are two major inappropriate responses that are interconnected; anxiety and avoidance. The Anxiety Response Anxiety is the primary inappropriate response. Wolpe claims that: All human neuroses are produced, as animal neuroses are, by situations which evoke high intensities of anxiety.2 lW01pe, pp, git., p.33. 2Ibid., p. 78. 139 He does not explain how or why high anxiety situations produce neuroses. Such explanations are superfluous to him. For therapy purposes he believes one does not need to know why anxiety is evoked in certain situations but only that it does occur. Stated differently, he believes that if one can simply identify the presence of anxiety, he can change it without knowing why it is evoked. According to Wolpe anxiety is: . . . the automatic response pattern or patterns that are characteristically part of the organism's response to noxious stimulation. A noxious stimuli is one that causes tissue disturbance of a kind that tends to lead to avoidance responses.1 The autonomic response pattern is characterized by neurological changes. Examples of neurological changes associated with anxiety include increased muscle tension, tremor, headaches, and decreased mental concentration. The Avoidance Regponse When anxiety occurs it leads to the avoidance of the noxious stimuli. Avoidance reinforces and 1Ibid., p. 34. 140 perpetrates the anxiety response. As long as the noxious stimuli is avoided, the individual will continue to respond with anxiety when confronted with it. But by the same token, he can reduce the probability of anxiety through avoiding as much as possible the noxious stimuli. An executive, for example, who responds with anxiety to the presence of his boss, can reduce the incident of anxiety through avoiding him as much as possible. Identification pp Noxious Stimuli Behavior pattern U is described in terms of the patient's anxiety response to certain noxious stimuli that lead to avoidance behavior. Noxious stimuli are classified into four categories: interpersonal, sexual, non-human object, and free-floating. The therapist tries to identify the specific noxious stimuli to which the patient reSponds unadaptively. If, for example, his symptoms indicate difficulties with inter-personal relationships, the therapist would attempt to identify the Specific 141 anxiety~evoking features of them. The therapist may do this by using the Willoughby Scale of Neurotism. The scale consists of 25 questions pertaining to how an individual feels about certain interpersonal situations. An example of the type of questions on the scale is: Are you self-conscious before superiors? The stimuli here is "superiors.“ If an individual answers yes to the question, it is deemed an anxiety—evoking or noxious stimuli. The therapist may attempt to identify other anxiety-evoking stimuli by presenting the patient with different interpersonal situations and asking him how he would react to each. For example, what would you do if after leaving a shop you notice that you have been short changed? If the patient says he would not go back, his response is deemed inappropriate because the therapist infers it is an anxiety—evoking situation. Analysis 9; Behavior Pgttern Q Behavior pattern D is described in terms of the absence of symptoms, anxiety responses, and avoidance 142 responses. The patient no longer responds unadaptively to specific non-objectively dangerous stimuli. The Transition-Producing Mechanism The transition-producing mechanism consists of: prescribing inhibitory responses, developing set, and implementing inhibitory responses. Prescribipg Inhibitory Responses The general mechanism for moving from behavior pattern U to behavior pattern D is the principle of rec1procal inhibition. The principle states that: . . . if a response inhibitory to anxiety can be made to occur in the presence of anxiety- evoking stimuli it will weaken the bond between the stimuli and the anxiety.1 The implementation of the principle requires the therapist to provide the patient with stimuli that is incompatible with the anxiety and avoidance responses he currently makes. Two responses are held to be incompatible, or reciprocally inhibit each other, if the patient can be trained to make an adaptive response lWolpe, §p_§;,, pp, cit., p. 10. 143 to noxious stimuli that prevents his making his current unadaptive response. According to Wolpe, an example of a response that will inhibit the anxiety response is a "relaxation“ response. He claims that the two responses are incompatible because one cannot feel anxious and relaxed at the same time. Wolpe has delineated four basic types of inhibitory responses: 1. Assertive - used against anxiety produced by interpersonal situations. 2. Sexual - used against anxiety produced by sexual relationships. 3. Relaxation - used against anxiety produced by non-human objects. used against "free-floating" anxiety where stimuli cannot be uncovered. 4. Respiratory For purposes of analysis emphasis will be placed on the assertive response. It will be assumed that behavior pattern D is where the patient substitutes an assertive response for an anxiety and avoidance response. Wolpe defines the assertive response as follows: It refers not only to more or less aggressive behavior, but also to the outward expression of friendly, nonanxious feelings.l 144 Wolpe claims the assertive response is an inhibitory response because each act of assertion by a patient will, to some degree, reciprocally inhibit his anxiety and avoidance responses. A patient cannot avoid and be assertive simultaneously in anxiety- evoking interpersonal situations. Since avoidance maintains his anxiety, it will be reduced by his being assertive. In short, the assertive response is used to overcome neurotic (non-Objective) anxiety in interpersonal relationships and the avoidance of them when possible. Developing Set The therapist explains the principle of reciprocal inhibition to the patient and shows him how it relates to his symptoms. He, then, creates a suggestibility set in the patient by indicating how his symptoms can be eliminated. Wolpe does this by telling the patient: These fears can be overcome if they are opposed by another, stronger emotion. I shall demonstrate this to you by means of an example or two. 145 Suppose a university student goes to a meet- ing of a society that he has joined because of real interest in its field. A professor has been invited to lecture at this meeting, and afterward, at discussion time, the student wishes to get up and ask a question or make a criticism. Although he has no doubt about the good sense of what he has to say, at the very thought of saying it his heart thumps, his knees tremble, and his hands sweat. This fear is clearly unadaptive as, objec- tiVely, no dire consequences could reasonably be expected to ensue from his speaking. There are two possible outcomes—-he may get up and speak in spite of his fear, or he may remain in his seat. He will in fact speak only if the motivation to speak is stronger than the fear. Just as fear tends to suppress the impulse to speak, this impulse, whenever it can be expressed, suppresses the fear to some extent and, through so doing, slightly weakens the habit of reacting with fear to this particular kind of situation. If the student should repeatedly speak on such occasions, this fear will be progressively weakened and eventually disappear. By con- trast, another fearful student who at the same meeting has been frozen to his seat will not have improved at all.1 Having given the patient a general description of his problems and what must be done to correct them, the therapist shows him in detail how to apply the "lesson“ to his own situation. Wolpe, for example, lWolpe, loc. cit. 146 told one of his patients: The main source of your anxieties (is) other people. You become anxious when they disapprove of you, or even if there is a possibility that they may, in all sorts of circumstances in which their opinions cannot make any real difference to you. Suppose, for example, that while out visiting, you want to expressaapolitical viewpoint but do not do so because Mr. X may think less of you. In reality you could not be harmed by his low Opinion. In failing to express your View, however, you are expressing anxiety, and so keeping up the strength of your anxiety habit. If, on the contrary, you were to express it, the feeling behind it would force down the anxiety to some extent, weakening the anxiety habit.1 Wolpe has communicated a number of things to the patient in this passage. First, he has told him what his ”problem" is by indicating the noxious stimuli (other people) to which he responds with anxiety (fear of being disapproved) and avoidance (suppresses Opinion). Second, he has told him that his response is inappro- priate because the noxious stimuli is not objectively- dangerous to him since their low opinion could not "actually" harm him. Third, he has told him how to resolve his problem (express his opinion). Fourth, lIbid., p. 123. 147 he has told him that following his advice will result in a "cure" (the development of set). Implementing pp Inhibitopy Response The last step the therapist takes in the therapy process is to let the patient try the assertive response and, then, analyze the result- Usually where the patient does not try the new response, the therapist exerts pressure on him to adopt it. A glimpse of this process is revealed by Wolpe when he says: As much pressure as seems necessary is applied to motivate the actual performance of the requisite behavior in appropriate circumstances. NOt only is he told that it will result in his feeling better, but his attention is focused on the enormity of any injustices being perpetrated on him, and in addition he is shown how undignified and unattractive to Others it is for him to behave in his accustomed spineless way. These pressures lead to the desired behavior in most cases, though not always immediately. In subsequent interviews he will be asked to recount his experiences in relevant situations, and his handling of them will be discussed and corrected. The therapist then, pressures the patient into changing his behavior. Only when he tells the therapist he has tried does he become “cured." lIbid., p. 117. 148 Evaluation p£_the Conditioning Model In terms of proposition B.4 and premise A.3, the model is not useful. It lacks an objective measurement technique, empirically validated proposi- tions, and control statements. Lack pj_gp Objective Measurement Techniqge One of the key variables in the model is classifying behavior as adaptive or non-adaptive. Wolpes states that ”behavior is adaptive to the extent that it is 'worthwhile'."1 He states that behavior is worthwhile if it does not result in unnecessary feelings and "objectively wasteful expenditure of energy." However, he does not indicate how the therapist can determine that feelings are unnecessary and that energy is being objectively wasted. In regards to the fear of elevators example, climbing stairs does not necessarily lead to an "objectively wasteful expenditure of energy." It may 1Ibid., p. 32. 149 ”objectively" be beneficial to the person when viewed from the standpoint of his physical health. Another example of the problem of determining the adaptiveness of a response is prescribing an assertive response. Under what conditions 18 an assertive response adaptive? WOlpe states that discretion must be used in prescribing it. The criterion of discretion he suggests is that ”no unpleasant reper— cussions can be reasonably expected from his behavior.” He states, for example, that if the patient "resents (his) boss's surly manner, it would in most cases be foolhardy to give frank expression to this resentment." In such cases, he maintains that anxiety is functional since it prevents the patient from encountering an "objective danger," the boss discharging him. Logically, the possibility exists that by expre551ng his feelings the patient might alleviate the anxiety- provoking situation. The boss may be unaware of his feelings and when informed change his ”surly" manner. 1Ibid., p. 118. 150 Hence, the assumption that the boss would resent the expression and penalize the patient is open to question. Similarly, even if he were discharged he might be better off since the situation would be eliminated and he would have to look elsewhere for a job. There is a chance that he might find a new boss without a "surly" manner. Thus, the possibility exists that the need for assertiveness could be eliminated through changing the anxiety-evoking features of the patient's environment. Changes in the values of symptomatic variables are measured subjectively. The therapist evaluates changes in their values on the basis of what the patient reports to him and his own subjective estimate of changes in the patient's behavior. The possibility exists that the patient could report changes without actually making changes in his behavior. Thus, the model does not contain an objective system for measuring changes in behavior. 151 The Lack pi Empirically Validated Propositions The model has a self-validating bias. The therapist tells the patient that his problem consists of making an unadaptive response to noxious stimuli. He also tells the patient he can only eliminate the response through using an adaptive response in place of the unadaptive response. He is told that if he tries the new response, his unadaptive response will automatically disappear. If he reports that he is using the new response, the therapist concludes the model must be valid. He does not experimentally determine if the patient would have recovered without his intervention, or if some other technique had been used. The Lack p§_Control Statements The model does not provide control statements. For example, it does not specify under what conditions an assertive response would be adaptive other than to say that no undesirable consequences should accrue to the individual. Moreover, it does not specify in "7 152 measurable terms the behaviors that comprise the assertive response. For example, it does not indicate what specific behavior is associated with an "outward expression of friendly, nonanxious feelings." Summary PrOposition B.4 states that a behavioral change model is useful if it contains variables a change agent can control, possesses an objective system for measuring the values of variables and states empirically validated relationships between the variables. The three psychotherapeutic models examined do not presently meet these conditions. Thus in accordance with premise C:lA they presently cannot be considered as useful to managers. fro the extent that they are representative of other psychotherapeutic models, proposition C.l is supported. Attempts to change behavior in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences would not presently meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. 153 In terms of premise A.2 none of the three models can be considered as presently useful to managers in solving their behavioral change problems. It is not yet possible to determine if they could help managers solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively than they could solve them without using them. The reason for this is twofold. First, the behavioral change problems managers confront and the models they use have not been identified. Thus it is not known if the use of the three models would help them solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively. Secondly, the three models do not specify behavioral problems confronted by managers. It is not yet known if the range of behavioral problems dealt with in the models are conceptually similar to the range of behavioral problems confronted by managers. Therefore it is not yet possible to ascertain if the models would be useful to managers. In terms of premise A.3, there are two reasons the models can not be considered as presently helpful to managers in solving their behavioral problems more 154 effeciently and effectively. First, they do not as yet constitute control knowledge. Secondly, the control knowledge possessed by managers has not been identified. Therefore it is not possible to determine if the knowledge provided by the models is superior to that possessed by managers. if CHAPTER V ENVIRONMENTALISTIC MODELS FOR CHANGING BEHAVIOR: AN ANALYSIS OF PROPOSITION C:lB In this chapter proposition Cl:B will be analyzed through applying the conversion framework to three environmentalistic models: the group decision model, the supervisory model, and the counselor model. Each of these models will be described in terms of the construct of a behavioral change model and evaluated in terms of the A premises and proposition B.4. Unlike the psychotherapeutic models that focus on changing the individual himself to change his behavior, the environmentalistic models focus on changing environ- mental elements to change behavior. The central proposition is that a change agent can change behavior through manipulating environmental variables. 155 )i’l I J 156 . . l The Group Dec1s1on Model Behavior Patterns U and~D In Lewin's group decision model, behavior pattern U consisted of certain food habits of housewives. They did not use certain food items such as beef hearts, canned milk, cod liver oil, or orange juice. Behavior pattern D was described as one in which they used these items. The Transition-producing Mechanism Lewin and his colleagues devised a series of experiments to determine the relative effectiveness of two different transition-producing mechanisms. The two mechanisms are portrayed in figure 5.1. Mechanism A consisted of lecturing the housewives on the need lLewin reports on and analyzes these experiments in the following sources: K. Lewin, "Group Decision and Social Change," in G. Swanson, T. Newcomb and E. Hartley (eds.), Readipgp ip_Social Psychology (Rev. Ed.: New YOrk: Henry Holt & Co., 1952); K. Lewin, "Frontiers in Group Dynamics: Concepts, Method and Reality in Social Science; Social Equilibria and Social Change," Human Relations, vo1. 1, #1 (1947). pp. 5-42. L. i 7 5 l .HOa>man mcamcmno Mom nonvedsoou HancoEeuomxo 03» may no QOmHHmmEOU H.m .mHm .omcmso mos» we mom .ousnoma he on meme on poneuomoum A Hafiz xoono A omcmno A onsuooa no A omcmso All. mamspa> uoa>mnmn umnu paop ou soamaoop cowmmsomap How poo: .IHOGH mo ca momcmnu mHmSOfl>HOGH m.amspe>apsH msonw do ousuooa oommmummm msgecnooa mosmmw mo mpoommm mo pads mocmno Mo msgasfluoa Mifimwmmmwwfi ousuooa we confluomonm Alllllllll omsmno A mHMSUH>Hpsw HOa>mson CH momcmno you too: so ousuoog mo oummonmmd magacnooe mo muoommm mmsmzo mo ODOHCEOOB omcmnu mo was: < Emflcmnomz 158 to change. As Lewin described the mechanism in one experiment: In three of the groups attractive lectures were given which linked the problem of nutrition with the war effort, emphasized the vitamin and mineral value of the three meats, giving detailed explanations with the aids of charts. Both the health and economic aspects were stressed. The preparation of these meats were discussed in detail as well as techniques for avoiding these characteristics to which aversions were oriented (odor, texture, appearaaqg, etc.). Mimeographed recipes were distributed. The lecturer was able to arouse the interest of the groups by giving hints of her own methods for preparing these "delicious dishes," and her success with her own family.1 Mechanism B consisted of lecturing the housewives on the need for change, holding a group discussion, asking members to indicate if they would change, and telling them that a check would be made to see if they change. As Lewin described it: Again the problem of nutrition was linked with that of the war effort and general health. After a few minutes, a discussion was started to see whether housewives could be induced to participate in a program of change without attempting any high-pressure salesmanship. The group discussion about 'housewives like lK. Lewin, "Group Decision and Social Change," p. 202. 159 themselves' led to an elaboration of the obstacles which a change in general and particularly change toward sweetbreads, beef hearts, and kidneys would encounter, such as the dislike of the husband, the smell during cooking, etc. The nutrition expert offered the same remedies and recipes for preparation which were presented in the lectures to the other groups. But in these groups preparation techniques were Offered after the groups had become sufficiently involved to be interested in knowing whether certain Obstacles could be removed. In the earlier part of the meeting, the women were asked by a showing of hands who was willing to try one of these meats within the next week.1 The outcome of the experiments was that mechanism B seemed to lead to more changes in behavior than . 2 . . mechan1sm A. HouseW1ves exposed to mechan1sm B had a higher incidence of change. The results of one experiment are presented in Table 5.1. The results indicated that more new mothers exposed to mechanism B incorporated orange juice and cod liver oil in their babies diet than those exposed to mechanism A. lIbid. 2&4... p. 205. 160 TABLE 5.1 CHANGES IN CONSUMPTION BEHAVIOR OF NEW MOTHERS “5 Percentage of Mothers Who Used Orange Juice as Prescribed Two weeks after Four weeks after experiment experiment Mechanism A 36 55 Mechanism B 85 100 Percentage of Mothers Who Used Cod Liver Oil as Prescribed Two weeks after Four weeks after experiment experiment Mechanism A 18 52 Mechanism B 46 88 The experiments did not indicate why mechanism B was more effective in changing behavior. All that could be concluded was that for some reason or other mechanism B tended to result in a greater incidence of change in the food habits of the housewives. Evaluation In terms of proposition B.4, the model does not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavior change model. It lacks empirically L. 3 . n I l ,. '. 1.5 .5 '.'&nS¢~e~ Ant VIAH' p u. in, r) .IPQ" wr- \ Ni» V n the e the e a: (l) U) 161 validated propositions, an Objective measurement system, and control statements. Lewin was uncertain as to whether the greater incidence of change was due to: l) the group discussion, 2) asking individuals to make a decision as to whether they would change, 3) telling the individuals a check would be made to see if they followed through with their decisions, and 4) some combination of these factors. He did not conduct any further experiments to isolate the effects of these factors. Based on the data from the experiment it could not be determined if one factor was more important in producing the change. One of the reasons Lewin advanced for the greater effectiveness of mechanism B was simply its "group setting." He felt that this setting performed three major functions necessary for changing behavior. First, he thought the group discussion "unfroze" the individuals' behavior from their previous pattern of behavior. It supposedly did this by permitting them to express their anxieties about the change as well as by showing them what other people felt. Second, he thought the lecture .— i _ L provided the movement necessary for the change by indicating how it was to take place. Lastly, he thought the decisions made by the individuals in the group "refroze" their changed behavior so that it became persistent. Individuals through making the decision to change committed themselves to the new behavior. But for some reason or other this explanation seemed insufficient to Lewin. He suggested that mechanism B was more effective because of group processes operating on the individuals. He contended that the "group" set norms which it expected its members to carry out. If certain members violated the norms, the other members applied pressure to bring their behavior back under the influence of the norm. Lewin assumed, however, that most people desire to support group norms. He concluded that the reason it is harder to change an individual's behavior under mechanism A is because the individual does not want to depart from group standards. Given this assumption, he postulated that the easiest way to change the 162 2" "Fr 163 individual's behavior is to change the group norms. As he stated the "principle”: . . . experience in leadership training, in changing of food habits, work production, criminality, alcoholism, prejudices, all indicate that it is usually easier to change individuals formed into a group than to change any one of them separately. As long as group standards are unchanged, the individual will resist changes more strongly the further he is to depart from group standards. If the group standard itself is changed, the resistance which is due to the relation between individual and group standard is eliminated.1 In stating this principle Lewin went beyond the data obtained in the Iowa studies. First, in the experiments conducted at Iowa the groups were simply, as far as can be ascertained from the data, aggregates of individuals since it was not known if they shared a common norm. There was in fact no attempt to determine whether they shared a common.xemup which differentiated them from other groups. In one of the experiments the experimental group was made up of Red Cross club members who met regularly. In another experiment the group was made up of new 1Ibid., p. 208. 164 mothers who neither knew each other, nor lived close to each other. Lewin, on noting these differences, claimed that groups of individuals need not be a permanent organization for mechanism B to work more effectively. Yet, he suggests that the reason for their greater incidence of change was due to the changing of a group norm. A group norm that was not identified in the experiments. Even more surprising is that he clearly states that the decision in the experiment did not involve a group goal or norm but was a decision about individual goals in a "group setting." The individuals in the group did not develop a consensus as to whether they should change; it was strictly a personal choice. Since some of the groups were non—permanent it seems dubious that even if a consensus had ensued, that an individual would, if he disagreed, go along with it because of the lack of sanction power. The test of a group is its ability to control deviants through sanction power. Lewin's reasoning that the change was due to the threat of group pressures for deviation seems erroneous 165 on the basis of the data gathered in the experiments. There is no evidence to suggest that his assumption that peOple desire to live up to group norms is valid. One simply cannot, on the basis of the experiment, conclude that the "group" serves as an important determinator of behavior in the process of change. Without knowledge of the reason for the greater incidence of change under mechanism B it is not possible to ascertain the general applicability of the model. Does it work only for changing food habits or can it be generalized to other forms of behavior? In short, what range of behavior could it be used to change? . l The Superv1sory Mpdel Analysis g§_Behavior Pattern g The description of behavior pattern U consists of: 1. An observation that the productivity of subordinates is lower than desired. lThe supervisory model is drawn from R. Likert, New Patterns pg Management (New YOrk: McGraw-Hill, 1961). 166 2. An observation that subordinates have unfavorable attitudes toward their supervisor, organization goals, and each other. 3. A ten statement questionnaire indi— cating portions of the supervisor's behavior that subordinates perceive as undesirable. The subordinates‘perception of the supervisors behavior is ascertained through the use of a ten item questionnaire.1 The ten items are listed in Figure 5.2. Subordinates of a given supervisor are asked to indicate if they strongly agree, agree, disagree, or strongly disagree with the statements. In behavior pattern U subordinates strongly disagree with the statements while in behavior pattern D they strongly agree with them. Behavior Pattern Q The description of behavior pattern D consists of: 1. An observation that the productivity of subordinates is higher than it was in behavior pattern U. lD. Katz, "Human Interrelationships and Organi- zational Behavior," in S. Mailick and E. H. van Ness, Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice—Hall, 1962), p. 183. 167 =~ _ Behav1or Behavior The Ten Statements Pattern U Pattern D 1. He thinks of employees as human beings not just as people to get Strongly Strongly the work done. disagree agree 2. He will go to bat or stand up Strongly Strongly up for me. disagree agree 3. He deals fairly with peOple Strongly Strongly I work with. disagree agree 4. He knows the job well and can Strongly Strongly give right answers. disagree agree 5. He gives help when I really Strongly Strongly need it. disagree agree 6. He likes to get our ideas and Strongly Strongly tries to do something about them. disagree agree 7. He is quick to praise people Strongly Strongly rather than to criticize. disagree agree 8. He keeps me well informed. Strongly Strongly disagree agree 9. He keeps men posted on how Strongly Strongly well they are doing. disagree agree 10. He hears complaints and Strongly Strongly grievances. disagree agree Fig. 5.2. Subordinates' perception of the supervisor's behavior. 168 2. An observation that subordinate have favorable attitudes toward their superior, organization goals, and each other. 3. A ten statement questionnaire indicating portions of the supervisor's behavior that subordinates perceive as desirable. The subordinates perception of the supervisor's behavior in behavior pattern D is shown in figure 5.2. They strongly agree with the ten statements about their supervisor's behavior. The Transition-ppoducing Mechanism The transition-producing mechanism attempts to change the subordinates' perception of their supervisor's behavior to that described in behavior pattern D. If their perception changes, it is hypothesized that their attitudes and productivity will change. The supervisor is to change their perception by changing those portions of behavior specified in the ten statements. For example, he is to try to obtain their ideas and do something about them. If he incorporates this behavior, it is assumed 169 subordinates will perceive the change and change their attitudes and productivity. The supervisory model assumes that subordinates expect their supervisor to exhibit the behavior specified in the ten statements. It is inferred that subordinates agreeing strongly with the ten statements expect these behaviors. Because they perceive them as being present they have more favorable attitudes and are more productive. Subordinates disagreeing strongly with the statements are inferred to have the same exceptations. However, since they do not perceive their supervisor's behaVior as being consistent with the expectations, they have unfavorable attitudes and lower productivity. In short, it is hypothesized that subordinates enter the organization with certain expectations about the behavior of the peOple they work under. When the behavior of their supervisor is not in accord with their expectations, they develop negative attitudes that lead to low productivity. Their experience in the organization is not consistent with their expectations. 170 To change their attitudes and productivity the supervisor must change his behavior toward them. Evaluation In terms of proposition B.4, the model does not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions for a useful behavioral change model. It does not provide adequate control statements and an Objective measurement system for all the variables contained in the model. Lack f pp Objective Measurement System Likert has developed a "principle" for super- visors to use in designing administrative style that will make their subordinates' experience consistent with their expectations. The principle is called the "principle of supportive relationships." It states that: The leadership and other processes of the organization must be such as to ensure a maximum probability that in all interactions and all relationships with the organization each member will, in the light of his back- ground, values, and expectations, view the experience sis supportive and one which builds T; “V 1.. H' . f“ "” "”31. (n (‘7 ) (I) 4* h-o "UOL; The q: H 171 and maintains his sense of personal worth and importance.1 The principle asserts that the supervisor should consider the "expectations" of subordinates and arrange their "experiences" in the organization in such a way as to produce "favorable attitudes." Likert does not indicate how the supervisor is to measure such things as subordinates' "sense of personal worth and importance." He claims that if subordinates perceive their environment as supportive they will maintain their "sense of personal worth." "Supportive relationships" appears to equal "personal worth and importance." This tautology is revealed in the following passage: Experiences, relationships, etc., are considered to be supportive when the individual involved sees the experience (in term of his values, goals, expectations, and aspirations) as contributing to or maintaining his sense of personal worth and importance.2 The question of course is how the supervisor is to determine if his subordinates view their experience 1Ibid., p.103. 21bid. 172 as supportive. Moreover, if they fail to view it as supportive what can he do to change their perception? In regards to the first question, Likert states: To apply this general principle, a superior must take into consideration the experience and expectations of each of his subordinates. In determining what these expectations are, he cannot rely solely on his observations and impressions. It helps the superior to try to put himself in his subordinate's shoes and endeavor to see things as the subordinates see them but this is not enough. Too often, the superior's estimates are wrong. He needs direct evidence if he is to know how the sub- ordinate views things and to estimate the kinds of behavior and interaction which will be seen by the subordinate as supportive. The superior needs accurate information as to how his behavior is actually seen by the subordinate. Does the subordinate, in fact, perceive the superior's behavior as supportive?1 He suggests that such information can be gathered from a survey using the ten statements or a facsimile. He also suggests that the supervisor might get such infor- mation by creating an atmosphere in which his subordinates give candid expression of their perceptions and reactions to him and each other. He does not indicate how the supervisor can create such an environment. As shall lIbid., pp. 103-104. 173 be demonstrated in the next section, the ten statements do not constitute an adequate basis for the supervisor to change his behavior. Lack p£_Adeguate Control Statements The generality of the behaviors specified in the ten statements raises application problems. For example, what specific behaviors on the part of the supervisor indicate to subordinates that he "thinks of them as human beings?" Does he say to them: "Look I don't think of you just as people to get work done but rather as 'human beings.'" If not, what specific behaviors does a supervisor use to indicate to his subordinates that he thinks of them as "human beings?" An answer would necessitate a definition of a "human being." The studies do not provide such a definition. It might be contended that if the supervisor incorporated the behaviors specified in the other nine statements he would be treating subordinates as "human beings." By engaging in these behaviors he would automatically receive a favorable evaluation on the 174 first statement. Granting that this possibility exists, the supervisor may still not know what Specific items to change in his actual behavior toward his subordinates. For example, in terms of the second statement what specific behaviors constitute. "batting" or “standing up" for subordinates? Under what condition and with whom does he "stand up?" For him to receive a favorable evaluation, must subordinates directly observe him "standing up?" Or can he simply tell them he has done so? In short, how does he determine when and how to "bat" and "stand up" for subordinates so that they will endorse his behavior? Another example of the difficulty in using the ten statements as a basis for changing behavior is the fifthxstatement. How can a supervisor determine when subordinates "really need help?" Does he wait for them to ask for it? What if it is not possible for him to "help?" The ten statements do not constitute adequate control statements. They do not indicate how the behavior specified in the statements can be implemented. 175 Likert also does not indicate why the supervisor cannot change the expectations of subordinates so that their experience would be supportive. He seems to assume that expectations are rigid and incapable of change. Hence, the need for the supervisor to change the experience of subordinates. The Counselor Modell Behavior Patterns g_and Q In the counselor model behavior pattern U is described in terms of the presence of felt personal and work problems by a lower participant in an organization. Behavior pattern D is described in terms of the participant no longer experiencing these problems. The Trangition—producing Mechanism The transition—producing mechanism developed by Roethlishberger and Dickson consisted of a counselor lThe counselor model is drawn from F. J. Roethishberger and W. J. Dickson, Management and the WOrker: Sp Account p£_g Research Program Conducted py_the Western Electric Compapy Hawthorne Works, Chicago ’(New YOrk: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Science Edition, 1964). 176 who used a non-directive interviewing to change the behavior of low level participants. The counselor was to be an impartial third party in the relationship between the lower level participants and the higher level participants. Lower level participants were to be able to come to him and discuss their problems without the fear of it adversely affecting their jobs or relationships with higher level participants. The counselor was to play a non-directive role in his discussion with the participants. He was not to advise them on how to solve their problems. He was only to help them clarify their problems. In helping participants clarify their problems, the counselor was to make use of non—evaluative and non-directive questions. For example, if a low level participant told him: “I can't stand this place anymore. My boss is such a bastard and he is always on my back," lthe counselor was to respond to this series of statements by saying: "You say your boss is always riding you?“ This was supposed to be the first step in helping the employee clarify his feelings and the reasons for them. 177 Such questioning was to continue until the employee had found a way out of his problems. In handling personal problems the counselor was to listen and not to advise the employee. He was not to directly attempt to change the employee's behavior. The counselor's role was casted in the following terms: The counselor's sole object is to lead the employee to a clear understanding of her problem such that she herself comes to realize what action to take and then assumes responsibility for taking it.1 This assumed that the employee wanted to solve her problem and could do it herself.if the counselor could show her the way. Evaluation In terms of proposition B.4 the model does not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. It lacks an objective measure— ment system, empirically validated propositions, and control statements. 1Ibid., p. 600. 178 Lack f pp Objective Measurement System It is not possible to determine the efficiency and effectiveness of the model because the range of behavior it attempts to change is not identified in measurable terms. Personal and work problems of participants who came to the counselor are defined on an individual basis. What constitutes a problem and how the counselor is to define it are unclear. Apparently, participants define their problems in behavior pattern U.and reports they no longer have these problems in behavior pattern D. This is strictly a subjectivelneasurement of the effectiveness of the model in changing behavior. Lack p§_Empirically Validated Propositions The proposition on which the model is based is a facsimile of the catharsis hypothesis that emerged from Freud's early work in psychotherapy. The cathersis hypothesis states that the level of hostility felt by an individual is inversely related to the opportunity he has for verbalizing it. The greater the opportunity 179 for him to verbalize his hostility, the lower his felt level of hostility. The hypothesis is based on an emotidnal constipation postulate. If individuals can not discharge their hositility it will accumulate. In the counselor model the problems presented by participants are held to be hostility bearing. By verbalizing their problems to the counselor, their hostility dissipates and they no longer.perceive a problem. The model is, hence, a perceptual model aimed at changing the participants’perception of their environment. The counselor and his use of a non- directive interviewing technique constitute environmental variables for changing the perceptions of participants. There is no evidence in the Hawthorne studies that indicates that the counselor is effective in changing perceptions. Furthermore, the studies do not specify if changing perceptions is sufficient to dissipate the problems felt by participants. The model seem to suggest that there is no objective basis for their problems and that their environment does not have to change but only their perception of it. There is some evidence that suggests that change in perceptions is not adequate.1 Strauss and Sayles claim that: Although counseling might help an individual make a better adjustment to a poor environment (say to an inept supervisor) it didn't improve the environment itself. Employees often began to feel that they were wasting their time talking to a counselor who could do nothing for them, and ended up almost as frustrated as before. Lack 9; Control Statements 180 The model while it is intended to be non—directive is actually directive. The directive function performed by the counselor is clearly revealed in Roethlishberger's and Dickson's description of how the counselor was to handle work problems of participants. A participant named Mary comes to a counselor and expresses dissatis- faction with her present job. The counselor in response: might ask her if she has ever discussed the problem with her supervisor. Mary replies that she has thought about discussing it with her supervisor but has never done so. The counselor asks her why. Mary answers that she really doesn't know why but guesses that he might misunderstand her. Now at this stage of 1See for example R. Likert, New Patterns 9; lfiflfifififlfiflflb 2 G. Strauss and L. R. Sayles, Personnel: The Human Problems p£_panagement (Englewood Cliffs, N. J. Prentice-Hall, 1960), p. 219. 181 the interview the counselor is focusing Mary's attention upon a possible source of remedying her problem. She is directing her attention toward the supervisor. After some discussion, Mary concludes that many of her fears are groundless and decides that she will see her supervisor.1 In this illustration it can be very clearly seen that the counselor directs Mary's attention to her expressed "problem.“ The counselor by asking Mary if she had discussed the problem with her supervisor is directing her attention. He presents her with an alternative course of action that she may not have perceived before as being feasible. About the only sense in which the counselor is non-directive in this situation is not directly telling Mary how to solve her problem. It is a subtle form of masterminding by the counselor. He hOpes that through asking Mary questions he can make her believe she is solving her problems. But the use of questions forces Mary to arrive at the "solution" the counselor wants her to consider. lRoethlishberger and Dickson, pp, cit., p. 600. 182 Further evidence of masterminding is revealed by the description of the counselor's next move after talking to Mary: The next day the counselor is in the department and is called over by the supervisor. He says he wants to talk to her about Mary Jones. Mary, he says, came to him yesterday and said she wanted a transfer to a different kind of work. He is perplexed as to why she is dissatisfied with her present job and what he should do about her request. The counselor at this stage may ask the supervisor a number of questions such as: Did Mary tell you why she is dis- satisfied with her work? Do you see any possibilities for advancement within your own department which would obviate the necessity for a transfer? Do you think that Mary understands these possibilities? Questions of this kind, as will be readily seen, may give the supervisor a number of ideas.1 The questions the counselor asks the supervisor certainly provides direction for the supervisor's behavior. Indeed, it "may give the supervisor a number of ideas." Ideas the counselor wants the supervisor to implement. With the coming of these ideas to the supervisor, he and Mary achieve a state of harmony. As described by Roethlisberger and Dickson: He may very likely say that he has always thought Of Mary as a good worker and had planned to 1Ibid. 183 advance her to other work as rapidly as possible. Following this discussion with the counselor, he then calls Mary up to his desk and gives her a more adequate understanding of the Situation. As a result of this process, she goes back to her work in better spirits. She is restored to her normal effectiveness and her efficiency may rise.1 What would have happened if the supervisor could not h .i have promised Mary a promotion? Would her problem have been dissipated? These are very crucial questions because Mary's adjustment may have resulted from changes in her environment rather than simply changes in her perception of it. The change may have been a result of changes in her supervisor's behavior. He called her in and attempted to find out what was troubling her. He also promised her a promotion. It would seem possible that her adjustment could be due to these factors rather than her interview with the counselor. WY Proposition B.4 states that a behavioral change model is useful if it contains variables a change agent lIbid. 184 can control, possesses an objective system for measuring the values of variables and states empirically validated relationships between the variables. The three environ— mentalistic models examined do not meet these conditions. Thus in accordance with proposition C:lB they presently cannot be considered as useful to managers. To the extent that they are representative of other environ— mentalistic models, proposition C.1 is supported. Attempts to change behavior in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences would not presently meet the neces- sary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. In terms of premise A.2 none of the three models can be considered as presently useful to managers in solving their behavioral problems. It is not yet possible to determine if they could help managers solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively since the models used by managers have not been identi— fied. Thus it is not known if the use of the three models would help them solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively. In terms of premise A.3, 185 there are two reasons lodels cannot be con51dered as presently helpful inagers in solv1ng their behavioral problems more siently and effectively. First, they do :itute control knowledge Secondly, the edge :fore .edge wssed possessed by managers has not been it 18 not p0831b1e to determine if provided by the models 18 superior by managers. not yet control 1dentif1ed. the to that CHAPTER VI BED-SATISFACTION MODELS FOR CHANGING BEHAVIOR AN ANALYSIS OF PROPOSITION C:lC Need-Satisfagpipn,flpdg15 In this chapter proposition C:lC will be analyzed h applying the conversion framework to two types of atisfaction models; the need for achievement model e interpersonal competence model. Each of these will be described in terms of the construct of a oral change model and evaluated in terms of the A as and proposition B.4. The need for achievement model concentrates on ng behavior by changing need structure. It is on the premise that increasing the need for achieve- ill result in increases in actual achievement, if nmental conditions permit satisfaction of the need. terpersonal competence model takes the need for rowth as a given and attempts to manipulate environ- conditions to permit satisfaction of the need. The delineates the type of organization environment that llow satisfaction of the need for "self-growth." 186 «gym-w" 187 The Need for Achievement Modell sis of Behavior r p Behavior pattern U is described in terms of a eed for achievement and low actual achievement. The Need for Achievement Conceptually need for achievement refers to the e to which an individual is concerned with attaining goal. In operational terms, the definition repre- a score on an achievement test. The achievement usually consists of showing an individual a series ur pictures depicting possible achievement situatipns sking him to write a story about it. A description ch a picture follows: 1This presentation of McClelland's need for vement model is drawn from the following sources: own, Social Psychology, New York: The Free Press, Chapter 9; D. McClelland, "Business Drive and nal Achievement," Readings ;p_Managerial Psychology, . J. Leavitt and L. R. Pondy (Chicago: University icago Press, 1964), pp. 122-46; D. C. McClelland, evement Motivation Can Be Developed," Harvard ess Review, XLIII (November-December 1965), 6ifor D. lland.p;,§l., The Achievement Motpyp (New York: ton-Century-Crafts, 1953); D. C. McClelland, The ving Society (Princeton; D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 188 scription of the picture: A man with a white irt and tie sitting at what looks like a draft— ; board. In front of him on the board is a all family photograph which he is gazing at ther pensively.l After the individual is shown the picture he is to write a short story about what is happening in at events have led up to it, what is being thought person in it, and what will happen next. The is then scored for the degree of need for achieve- xpressed in it. The scoring mechanism is presented in Figure 6.1. ghest possible score, or degree to which the achieve- eed is expressed, is 11 points per story. To attain score each of the items listed in the scoring mecha- auld have to be expressed in the story. The story have to contain statements indicating AI, N, I or ?), Ga+, Ga-, Bp, Bw, Nup, G+, G-, and Ach th. f these items are given a value of 1 so that the n score possible per picture is 11. Defining a person as having a high need for ement is somewhat arbitrary since there are many 1McClelland, "Business Drive and National ement." P- h—If-‘nlsh I ‘l '( .- IX _ , , ‘ . '__' _. ., w: _— 189 .ucoEo>mH£om now poo: onu How Emacmnomfi moanoom .H.© .mam .AIOV uommmm O>Hummmc no .A+Ov Hmom Canaan on mcflhuu CH uoommm O>Huamom wouMOHocH SOH£3.>Houm CH ucoEoumpm .Hoa>mnmn pouomufiplamom was Apa3 omennmmfiwm no Hmom was onm3ou EH: mam: mma moo IOEOm moumoapca SOH£3 wuonm CH ucoEoumum u .Ammv adamant cOmnom on» Eoum no .Asmv ucoEGOHH>om Eonm mcHEEoum ucoecamnum Hmom Op moaomumeo mounoach noanz mnoum onu Ge ucoaoumum .ANHV oEoouso samuumocs cm o>m3 Ho .AIHV Hammmooosmss .A+Hv Hummmoo loom Honuao mum SOH£3 Hmom mo ucoacampum um poEHm anoum ca UOQHHOmOO moaua>auo< u .xumov musaamm Ho A+mwv Hmom mo unmeoflmuum moaummHOHucm ma Hospa>apce msflzonm mnoum ca ucoEoumum .Hmom uQOEo>OH£om cm nomon Ou mucmz ocooEOm umnu muoum CH noofioumum n AHOV AQDZV Amm HO 3mv A +H» H v «Hmov sz manommm ucmscamupm snow as pommma mo mumum mmonm ucmusuoz ucoecemuu< Hmow ou moaomumno MnH>Huo¢ HmncoEoHDmGH oEoousO pcoEchuu¢ Hmow ucmEm>mH£O¢ How pmmz 190 3 scores (1 to 11) indicating the degree of nent need revealed by the test. In light of this, be said that a person with a score of 5 has a high r achievement? Yes and no. It is high in compari— h others who score below 5 and lower than those who bout 5. Hence, when one talks about a high need .ievement operationally he must specify the com- 'e base he is using. What the achievement test score represents is the icy with which the individual tends to think spon- sly in achievement terms when that is not clearly 2d of him. It indicates how closely he approxi— :he complete achievement sequence. Did he define a u, want to solve it, think of means of solving it, of difficulties that get in the way of solving it r in one's self or in the environment), think of who might help in solving it, and anticipate the hood of success or failure? If he only presented these items in writing his story, he will receive r score than if he presents more than two items in ,quence. To illustrate how the need for achievement is [two stories that were written in response to the 191 titni of the picture previously presented will be d. Story S_ engineer is at work on Saturday When it is et and.he has taken time to do a little day- :aming; He is the father of the two children ‘the pdcture--the husband of the woman shown 'has a happy home life and is dreaming about 1e pleasant outing they have had. He is also >king forward to a repeat of the incident which now giving him pleasure to think about. He Ins on the following day, Sunday, to use the :ernoon to take his family for a short trip.1 Story'S a man is an engineer at a drafting board. The :ture is of his family. He has a problem and concentrating on it. It is merely an everyday currence--a problem which requires thought. How n he get that bridge to take the stress of pos- ble high winds? He wants to arrive at a good lution of the problem by himself. He will dis- ss the problem with a few other engineers and ke a decision Which will be a correct one--he s the earmarks of competence.2 Without going into the scoring mechanism in , the person who wrote the first story would receive m need for achievement score than would the person romathe second story. According to McClelland, the 1Ibid., pp. 124-25. 2Ibid., p. 125 192 writing story A would receive a score of (-1) ;he story is completely unrelated to achievement. fson writing story B would receive a score of -mately (+6) because there are six ideas in his :hat are scorable as related to the achievement :e (AI, Ach Th, N, Ga+, Bw, and Nup). In his 1e has defined a problem, indicated he wants to Lt, thought of difficulties that get in the way of 3 it, thought of people who might help him solve it, :icipated the likelihood of success. This is, in a, how the variable, need for achievement, is ad. Actual Achievement Actual achievement is more difficult to measure he need for achievement. Having such a measure is ary for finding out if a high need for achievement 3 in high actual achievement. Stated differently, hinking in achievement terms on a fantasy test pre— hat a person will actually achieve things in the orld, or non-test situation? McClelland advances the proposition that: 193 ople who spend a lot of their time thinking out getting ahead, inventing new gadgets, fining problems that need to be solved, con— dering alternative means of solving them, and lling in experts for help should also be people 0 in real life pp_a lot of these things or at e very best are readier to do them when the casion arises.1 t the validity of this proposition a measure of chievement need and actual achievement is necessary. For measures of actual achievement McClelland has used proxy variables such as salaries, grade point [es, and rate of promotion. He has found, for .e, that men in corporations earning less than )0 a year have the lowest average need for achieve- icores while those making between $20,000 to $25,000 2 But men in the bracket :he highest average scores. $25,000 have a somewhat lower average achievement than those in the $20,000 to $25,000 bracket. ding to McClelland, the reason achievement need 5 of managers declines somewhat after the $25,000 y bracket is that managers in higher brackets are g well enough to relax a little."3 I— 1Ibid., p. 126. 2Ibid., pp. 128—29. 3Ibid., p. 129. 194 a is pp Behavior 2r D Behavior pattern D is described in terms of flu need for achievement and a high rate of actual evement. The object of change has moved from a ation in which his achievement need and actual evement were low to a situation in which they relatively higher. Egansition-producing anism Originally it was thought that to increase magnitude of the achievement need and the actual evement rate of people one had to uncover the ial determinates of the need for achievement.1 ed differently, what "causes" some people to have .gh need for achievement and others to have a low [for achievement? At first, McClelland traced the determinates :he achievement need to early independence and :ery training of children by their parents. The ans, in turn, were viewed as deriving these Ad‘— 1McClelland, The Achieving Socipty. 195 ‘inging practices from the Protestant Ethic that ssed hard work and self—reliance. After uncov- .g these determinates, McClelland suggested that .ges in the magnitude of the achievement need of ters of a nation, as well as changes in the mag- .de of their actual achievement, would require .ges in the nation's child rearing practices. One .he most significant variables in child-rearing :tices was believed to be father dominance. Father .nance was hypothesized to result in the child iming dependent on others for direction and to dis- 'age him from trying to gain mastery over his .ronment. Hence it prevented him from becoming spendent and mastery—oriented. The presence of :e two characteristics was postulated to be the .ical determinant of the achievement need. If they 2 absent, the child would not develop a high need for .evement or a high level of actual achievement. McClelland, has recently hit upon a way to rt-circuit this long and involved cultural process increasing the magnitude of the achievement need. 1McClelland, "Achievement Motivation Can Be aloped." 1.. .. 196 txagun to experiment with teaching people with a Ci for achievement how to behave like people with tweed for achievement. He is teaching them the system used for measuring the achievement need training them to write stories that will earn 'high score. As he putSit, he teaches them: r to think, talk, act, and perceive the world ;e a person with a high need for achievement. :ulates that such training will increase their need iievement. He believes that if they learn to attend .evement dimensions for the purpose of writing 5, they will begin to do this when they are not ; such stories. They will begin to think in ament terms in their everyday life so that their f actual achievement will increase. Evaluation p; the Achievement Model In terms of proposition B.4, the model does not he necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful cnal change model. It lacks objective measures for riables contained in the model, empirically vali- pmopositions and control statements. 1Ibid., p. 10. 197 pj_pp Objective Measure gtu§l_Achievement and gigpi Validipy pp asitions At the present time there is little information -able that deals with the effectiveness of the pro- for increasing the magnitude of the achievement need. Ft >usly, the effectiveness of the program can only be .‘qi- fmined by observing whether the incidence of actual “FT-1 2... ‘hip- evement among individuals exposed to the program sig- :antly increases after the program is completed. :ases in the magnitude of the achievement need does :onstitute a measure of the effectiveness of the pro— because the measurement of the need for achievement intaminated by the program. Stated differently, 'iduals exposed to the program are taught how to ase their scores on the achievement test. One would, fore, expect their scores to increase after exposure e program. Determining the effectiveness of the program in ing behavior requires the measurement of the indi- l's rate of actual achievement before and after the ing. At the present time, McClelland has some crude ators of the changes in behavior that follow from 198 : tc> the program. In one training program, for :aixteen executives in one company were taught 3e lrigh achievers while a matched control group I asexi to the company's usual type of development . Two years after the program a follow-up d that executives exposed to the achievement pro- d been promoted faster than those in the control But there appeared to be an unanticipated conse- with the control group. The company's usual train- >gram seemed to have a negative affect on their pro- rate. As McClelland put it: part this appeared to be due to the fact that ase men who had attended the regular company irse had slowed down somewhat; that is, they re advancing somewhat less rapidly after the irse than they had before, whereas our par- :ipants had continued to forge ahead. So far we could tell, the reason the regular group awed down was that the company course had ressed the importance of 'not rocking the boat,‘ representing the company image as it was out- ned for them in the course. Ixmlly possible to tell from the study if the achieve- raining resulted in the executives significantly' simgtheir incidence of actual achievement? 1McClelland, loc. cit. 199 The answer, in large part, is negative. The use :e of promotion for a measure of actual achievement : best, a proxy variable. The study provided no : measure of the executives' actual achievement : before or after their exposure to the program. It ierefore, possible that their higher rate of promo- Ls not attributable to greater actual achievement air part but on other factors which were not ited for in the follow up studyu IPut differently1the :ion mayormay nothave been solely a function Of their 1 achievement. Along similar lines, it is questionable whether y is a good proxy variable for measuring actual vement. Just because a manager receives a higher y than people below him does it necessarily follow his actual achievement is greater? In another training program, fifty-two men from participated in a ten day seminar. Among the fifty- en there were bicycle shop owners, fiber exporting s, lawyers, bankers, and politicians. How effective he program? McClelland says that it is still a e too early to tell exactly how effective it was. er, as he put it: 200 it is too soon to know what the long-range 'esults will be, but between six and ten lonths after training, two-thirds of the men Lad become unusually active in business in :ome readily observable way: e.g., they had :tarted a new business, expanded their old >usiness, greatly increased profits, or taken :ctive steps to investigate a new product line. inly one-third of these men had been unusually ctive in similar ways in the two years prior 0 taking the course. In short, the course rould appear to have doubled the natural rate if unusual entrepreneurial activity in this roup.1 McClelland furnishes the following illustrations ople who appeared "to have doubled the natural rate usual entrepreneurial activity:" . One man who owned a small radio shop decided to start a paint and varnish factory. It has succeeded, and he has opened another radio shop. . One has decided to go to work daily instead of leaving his shop to his assistant. . A wealthy family has started a new sugar mill.2 trations 2 and 3 seem to indicate a decision to try hing different. They do not really indicate if the e actually achieved something in terms of the vement sequence. Similarly, just because "the e would appear to have doubled the natural rate of Ibid., p. 20. Ibid., pp. 20, 22. 201 ual entrepreneurial activity," does it necessarily ow that their actual achievement will increase? t not increases in actual achievement require not a desire, or need to achieve, but also an ability :hieve? On an.p priori basis it would seem that just (ing in achievement terms would not necessarily Lt in a greater incidence of actual achievement. It i seem necessary for an individual to have the ability :ansform his achievement desires into specific behav- that will result in actual achievement. It is ear if McClelland's program actually increases a .cipant‘s ability to achieve. pp Control Knowledge McClelland does not indicate how he arrived at :Onclusion that participants in the India program _pd their rate of entrepreneurial activity. He does Lention how he determined their existing level of 'ity or how he measured changes in it. Similarly, »es explicate the unit of comparison: entrepren- 1 activity. 202 The possibility exists that the changes observed articipants were due to exogenous variables. Thus, 5 not possible to state that the transition-produc- mechanism leads to changes in actual achievement. 1 The Interpersonal Competence Model sis of Behavior ern‘g The description of behavior Pattern U is a high for self-growth coupled with a low interpersonal etence which is a function of certain environmental itions. The Need for Self-Growth According to Argyris, the self is an inter- onal phenomena. As he defines it: The self is the individual's needs, values, abilities, and defenses integrated into an organized whole or entity that has some mean- ing to the individual.2 onsiders the self to be an interpersonal phenomena '— 1This presentation of Argyris' interpersonal etence model is drawn from: C. Argyris, InteppersonaL etence and Organizationap Effectiveness (Homewood, : Richard D. Irwin, 1962). 2Argyris, pp, cit., p. 17. 203 lse the certain needs, values, abilities, and defenses liCh it is composed are acquired and learned from >ns external to the individual. The interpersonal competence model is a need—sat— :tion model since it postulates that an individual I need for "self-growth" that can be satisfied or Jited by his environment. "Self-growth" is held to function of experimentation with new values, ideas, In awareness Of one's impact on others and their :t on him. If an individual's environment is such he can not experiment with new values, or ideas, :annot gain feedback on his impact on others, his for self—growth will be frustrated. In such nnces, his self will become a static entity in that :ehavior will be of a reoccurring nature and hence predictable. Interpersonal Feedback From Argyris' conceptualization of self-growth In be seen that “feedback" from an individual's 'onment plays an important part in his self-growth. lSt receive information about his impact on others, 10W changes in his values and ideas affect their 204 ption of him in order to grow. Argyris believes that only "descriptive valuative" feedback will facilitate the process of growth. Descriptive non-evaluative feedback is rack that describes a relationship without placing ue judgment on it. As he puts it: . . descriptive non-evaluative feedback . . . .ttempts to describe and not evaluate. For example, there is a significant difference >etween saying 'you shouldn't behave in x manner' and 'I experience the following feel- .ngs when you behave in x manner.‘l >es not explicitly indicate ppy there is a "signi- 1t difference" between the two statements he cites 118 example. He simply seems to suggest that the : statement explicitly espouses a value judgment. Ly, one should not behave in x manner. Why should descriptive evaluative feedback be ied if the self is to grow? Argyris answers this pstplating that descriptive evaluative feedback 3 to "defensive behavior" in individuals that inhib- their self-growth. Defensive behavior supposedly bits self-growth because it maintains the existing 1Ibid . p 18 205 It defends the self from change. As Argyris it: 'he individual will tend to accept those experiences consonant with his self; and he rill tend to distort, deny, and reject that >ehavior that is different from, and is not .mmediately integratable with, his self.1 :es not indicate how an individual determines if riences are consonant or dissonant with his self, 1y he should become defensive when he receives :iptive evaluative feedback. He assumes the indi— al will become defensive with such feedback. If interpersonal feedback is to facilitate self :h it must not put an individual on the defense. :15 suggests that such feedback occurs when an Jidual "accepts" his own, or someone elsés behavior : is without evaluating it. As he expresses it: Acceptance may be defined as perceiving one's awn or others' behavior as the self or others intended it to be perceived. Acceptance implies nothing as to whether one likes or agrees with vhat one sees. It simply means that the individ— ial is willing to see it, to accept it as existing in the way the person who manifests it wants it perceived. The Operational criteria for whether an individual A is accepting of his (or someone else's) behavior are as follows: Argyris, loc. cit. 206 a) In the case where x is A's own behavior, does A permit feedback about x from others without becoming defensive or making them defensive for giving such feedback? b) In the case where x is someone else's behav- ior, does A provide feedback to B about x without being defensive or inducing B to be defensive?l rding to the definition, acceptance means that an Iidual gives and receives non-evaluative feedback >ut becoming defensive. When this occurs, the ssary condition for high interpersonal competence and -growth is met. The individual is open to new values ideas and is aware of his impact upon others. use of this openness, ‘he has a potential for E—growth.' Interpersonap Competence Argyris defines "interpersonal competence" as a component of "administrative competence." "Admin- ative competence" is defined as an "organization's .ities to achieve its objectives, maintain itself arnally, and adapt to its external environment." >rding to.Argyris, administrative competence breaks 1Ibid., p. 19. 2Ibid., p. 15. 207 1 into two analytically separable components: 1) tech- il, rational, and intellectual competence which agedly deals with things and ideas necessary for anization effectiveness; and 2) interpersonal com- ance which allegedly deals with interpersonal relation- >s necessary for organization effectiveness. Inter— :onal competence is defined as the degree to which ;icipants of an organization are aware of their lCt upon others and others' impact upon them, and degree to which interpersonal problems are handled uch a way that the problems do not reoccur. Inter- onal competence is considered to be low when these ables assume low values, and vice-versa. As Argyris puts it: Interpersonal competence increases as individuals: a) Are aware of their impact upon others and others' impact upon them. b) Solve problems in such a way that the same (or similar) problems remain resolved.1 Organizational Effectiveness Argyris postulates that increases in interpersonal etence may result in increases in organization L 1Ibid., p. 26. 208 iziveness. "Organization effectiveness" appears to ‘Ehe degree to which an organization achieves its 'tives, maintains itself internally, and adapts to xternal environment. He does not indicate how one » measure the values of these variables. For >1e, how can the degree to which an organization :ains itself internally be measured? The Determinant p§_Low IpterpersonaL Competence The determinant: of low interpersonal competence astulated to be a function of an inappropriate set rganizational values. In behavior pattern U, the nization values "rational" behavior and devalues tional" behavior. It, so to speak, operates on an mption that efficiency and effectiveness decrease as ession of feelings and emotions increase. It assumes "emotional" and "rational" behavior can be separated segmented by participants. Participants are to inter— strictly on the basis of their formal roles. They to be impersonal in their orientation toward each er and interact on a task related basis.1 1For an interesting discussion on the role of arsonality in organizations see: P. Selznick, indations of the Theory of Organization, " American Lological Review, XII (1948), 25-35. (PH—h— i1- .. 209 Argyris asserts that as participants commit elves to the organization's value of rationality, will emphasize the rational, intellectual aspects teractions and suppress the emotional aspects. For 1e, they will say such things as "Let's keep feel- Out of the discussion." When the emotional aspects eir behavior is suppressed, interpersonal difficul- will either be suppressed or disguised and brought rational, technical, intellectual problems. Due e lack of interpersonal feedback, participants will it difficult to develop competence in dealing with ' emotions and feelings in their interpersonal rela- hips. Their interpersonal competence will be low use they are not aware of their impact on others and 's' impact on them, and interpersonal problems will >e resolved in such a way that they do not reoccur. The Effects pg Low Lpterpersona; Competence Argyris goes on to postulate that because the ession of emotions and feelings is not permitted, the Lcipants will establish personal and organizational 1ses to help them suppress their own feelings or ait others in their attempts to express their emotions _A" 210 feelings. As he states it: If feelings are suppressed, the tendency will be for the individual not to permit himself or others to own their feelings. For example, the individual may say about himself, 'No, I didn't nean that,’ or 'Let me start over again. I'm confusing the facts.‘ Equally possible is for one individual to say to another; 'No, you shouldn't feel that way,‘ or 'That's not effective executive behavior,‘ or 'Let's act like mature people and keep feelings out of this.'1 aking statements like, "No, you shouldn't feel that " participants are allegedly transmitting evaluative back that lowers interpersonal competence and hence -growth. Argyris hypothesizes that another way to prevent icipants from violating the organization's value ationality is to block out, or refuse to consider, 3 and values which, if discussed, could reveal sup- sed feelings and emotions. He believes that such a nsive reaction may lead to a-barrier to intellectual s as well as values. Participants will tend to limit selves to those ideas and values that do not violate organization norms or values. This, in effect, fur— decreases interpersonal competence and self-growth. rgyris states it: lArgyris,pp.cit., p. 41. ‘1..- IF. a.» I 1. 211 he iJniividuals in the organization will tend o decrease its capacity to be open to new ideas rui values. As the degree of openness decreases, the capacity to experiment will tend to decrease, rui the fear to take risks will tend to increase. s the fear to take risks increases, the proba- ility of experimentation is decreased and the ange or scope of openness is decreased, which .ecreases risks. We'have a closed circuit that rould be an important cause for the loss of ritality in an organization.1 [dividuals cannot achieve self-growth, the organiza— cannot either. If individuals will not experiment new values and ideas, neither will the organization. iet effect is that the organization's achievement of objectives and adaptability to its environment is than it would be if such conditions were not present. As openness to new values and experimentation eases, interpersonal competence decreases. As rpersonal competence decreases, it has a number of equences for the behavior of the individual and the [vior of the organization. As Argyris describes it: If individuals are in social systems where they are unable to predict accurately their inter- personal impact upon others, and others' impact upon themselves they may begin to feel confused. 'Why are people behaving that way toward me?‘ 'Why do they interpret me incorrectly?‘2 '— lArgyris, loc. cit. 21bid., pp. 142., 1| km.m. 212 rding to the passage, since participants receive no rpersonal feedback about how they "emotionally" ct each other they become suspicious of each others ves. Supposedly, because they do not really know they think of each other, they mistrust each other. Mistrust is postulated as leading to defensive vior on the part of the participants. In this rd, Argyris says: Since such questions (e.g. 'Why are people behav— ing that way toward me?') are not sanctioned in a rationally dominated system, much less answered, the confusion will tend to turn to frustration and feelings of failure regarding interpersonal rela- tions. In an attempt to maintain their sense of esteem, the members may react by questioning the nonesty, and genuineness of the interpersonal oehavior of their fellow workers. Simultaneously, they may place an even greater emphasis upon the rational, technical interactions, in which they are probably experiencing a greater degree of success. The increased emphasis upon rationality will act to suppress the feelings even more, which in turn, will decrease the probability that the questions of confusion and the mistrust (of self and others) will 3e explored.1 passage suggests that when mistrust becomes prevalent 1 organization participants will react by emphasiz— :he "rationality” value. They will stress inter- an on an impersonal and taskfrelated basis. This 1Ibid., p. 44. 17‘ V 213 reaction, in turn, will increase the suppression of emotions and mistrust. There is another consequence for the participant's behavior that supposedly transpires when emotions are suppressed and mistrust emerges. Argyris states the con- sequence in the following manner: As interpersonal mistrust increases, and as the capacity (individual and organizational) to cope with this mistrust decreases, then the members may tend to adapt by 'playing it safe.‘1 The important question is why should participants react to "mistrust" with an increased tendency to "play it safe?" Argyris believes that since they cannot gain interpersonal feedback they will tend to avoid new behavior patterns because the patterns may be negatively evaluated by other participants. To “play it safe" they will simply repeat patterns that they have used in the past without eliciting negative feedback. He equates "playing it safe“ with defensive conformity. Defensive conformity apparently means that participants will adhere to traditional methods and 1Ibid., p. 41. ( 214MW / techniques that reflect the "rationality" value of the organization. It assumes that participants desire to maintain, or improve upon, their present position and that they perceive that this can only be done by "playing it safe." The increasing incidence of mistrust and con— formity supposedly has another consequence for the participants' behavior. As they increase, the partici- pants' commitment to their roles in the organization becomes external in nature. They gain their primary satisfaction from increases in their salaries and status and gain little satisfaction from their role per- formance itself. The reason this occurs is obscure. Apparently, it is attributed to the value the organiza- tion places on “rational" behavior. Since participants comprise the organization and it values "rationality," they will also value it highly. Internal commitment to a role is an "emotional" process and hence is something to be avoided. 216 The organization consequence of mistrust, con- formity, and external commitment is that decision—making by participants becomes rigid. Since they want to take no chances with new ideas, or experiment with them, they simply apply decision rules and techniques they have learned in the past. The net effect on the organization ”XL is that in decision—making there is no self-growth or organizational growth. If the organization's environ— ment is changing and requires new decision rules and techniques, organization effectiveness may be decreased. Another organization consequence of the partici— pants' behavior under low interpersonal competence is that they began to engage in certain non-productive behaviors which further decreases organizational effec— tiveness. Because of the prevalence of mistrust among participants, Argyris postulates that they will engage in non-productive activities whose aim is to provide them with a rationale for their actions in case any one should question their judgment or actions. They also gather data and keep a record of mistakes that other participants make in order to give them some control if these people cite them for mistakes. According to Argyris, they often build "JIC" files, which stand for 217 just in case, to protect themselves from absurd questions their boss might ask them. For example, the boss might ask them how many parking spaces there are in the com— pany's parking lot. A summary of the total description of behavior pattern U is presented in Figure 6.2. Analysis pp Behavior Pattern p Figure 6.3 portrays behavior Pattern D. The pattern primarily differs from behavior U in that the organization's values have been changed. Communication of emotions, "authentic relationships," interpersonal competence, and internal commitment are valued. These new values are associated with reverse changes in the values of other variables used to describe behavior pattern D. Under the organization values in behavior pattern D, the expression of "emotional" aspects of interpersonal relationships is encouraged. The encouragement changes the characteristics of interpersonal relationships. Non-evaluative feedback is transmitted and received by participants and they are open to new values and ideas and willing to experiment with them. With the increase f'f—h-r-‘t. 218 .D snonumm HOH>mSOmIIHOUOE monouomaou HMGOmuomumusH one .m.o .mam 1.1mm... . Io>Huoommo l . amon MN 1: ll amcoaumua .muonuo l. . I. Icmmno BOA .om mo mo>HuOE In .Ho >m mono A: 1 mo unsuumaz Us on on on a non _.mCOHuom _ Induce on Hows muosuo mam: Ho mmca ms xomn on .GOHuomm . 1:00 pan soaouo Iaoom pom woesuauum . :coaumEHomcH J. Imaumm mo . . loo coauomnap . .po>HOm mmum mmope mosam> mseumH OOHSOm up no end opp no nos Op mama HMGOmuom :30 Do: on .om IsEsoom mommouoca I one Hmcom memmzmfio umouw ca uoa>mn mumamm paw Q .mmcH InomuonsH .nm . .uoe>mnon Ion O>Hu :OHuOEOHm Iaomm pom mmona msoa OEo IOSponmsD .Qm so mmumnu .mnonpo .mOpsueuum .mosam> moacoawwoum isooco . so owned a a3 a a m c . .mGOHmHomU I¢.m O as o mwm3msa .m 3wanwwwo om.uowoow . Imam paw Hoa>mn mcaxme .nuzoum m m . p H AN Ion abdoeumn so be muapamam .Mm Imaom 30A .me .po>Hooou pom memmsmao “mono a VI F. .L co>Hm we xomnpoom [I II . r.o>aumsam>o newswonm .MN coflummecmgno Hmspa>wch Mom mucouomfioo mQHEmGOAHMAom mosam> How moocoswomcou mooomswomcoo HmoomnomuoucH aMGOmHomuoucH HMGOeumnasmmnO mo Hmuumumnu .QH Owifl 219 .Q snouumm HOH>m£omllampoz oocmuomeoo HMQOmuomHoucH one .m.o .mHm /\ .om Op . f .mmoco>au . mpaauowsoo mumnuo mason o>Hmcomop pew mmcflaoom loommm Hmcoan .HOH>M£OQ 1:02 .he .I pew mopsu I. IMNHcmmHO nmam .0m .7 . mucosamsw A. Iflpum mmopfl - _ A . on ucoauaa _ L .muonuo mo monam> .msoHuom IEoo Hmsumuce jlump/.308 opp III HMGOmHOQ G30 .ON . as xomn on Ca umsne .o .po>aom ppm monouomeoo coanmEuomsH mcH . v wmum mama .mmcaaoom ppm Hmsomnomuouca lumHsEooom ca .waomufl HQOHQ HMCOm .J .mmmpH .mppsu .l.s.mmanmooauma Hoa>m£on O>Hu no“ Scum luomnonsH .Qm Iauum .mosHm> Ion oausonusm: Iosponms: oz .QTT cOauommmHumm .mnm no so 3o: QDHB mi mo on: one so umoummnm n cOaumncmEeuom mammnmEm umouw mooamfloop mo>anoa .Q nommEH mo Ixo ou ammo . msaxme be r III . nMP. mumsonm3< .mmL am .msoeuoao muaaanaxoam .mm npzonmlmaom use I .po>Hooon mo coHumoacsE Ii paw co>em IEoo paw Hoa>m£ we xomnpomm finon.amcowumn do o>aumsam>o memmnmfio umouw Icon usosgmnm .mN I All coHpmNHsmsuo . HmspH>HOcH How mucouoaeou m anmsoaumaom mosam> Mom mmocosvomsou moososgomcoo HMQOmHomuousH HMGOmHomuounH HMGOeumuecmmnO MO Hmuomumso OMH 220 in non-evaluative feedback and Openness to change, inter- personal competence increases. Participants become aware of their interpersonal impact on each other and attempt to solve interpersonal difficulties in such a way that they do not recur. As interpersonal competence increases; mistrust, external commitment, and defensive conformity decreases. As they decrease, rigidity in decision—making and non-productive behavior decreases. As they decrease, organizational effectiveness increases. The Transition-producing Mechanism The transition—producing mechanism is directed at changing the organization's values from those present in behavior pattern U to those present in behavior pat- tern D. The mechanism consists of communicating the diagnosis, establishing a T-group, non-cognitive inter- action, and modeling. Communicating the Diagnosis The change agent diagnoses the behavior pattern present in the organization using the interpersonal competence model. He shows the participants the values 221 they are operating under and the behaviors to which they lead. However, he does not evaluate the values and behaviors. He describes what pp_pppp is going on and has the participants decide if they would like to change their behavior. If they choose to do so, the change agent's next step is to establish a T—group in which they can analyze their impact on each other and question their current values and feelings. The geGroup A T-group according to Argyris: Is most difficult to describe in a few words. Basically it is a group experience designed to provide maximum possible opportunity for the indi— viduals to expose their behavior, give and receive feedback, experiment with new behavior, and develop everlasting awareness and acceptance of self and others. How does the change agent design a "group experience" that "provide maximum possible opportunity for individuals to expose their behavior?" According to Argyris, the change agent is not to enact a directive role. The only expressed goals he Offers to participants is to study their own interaction as a group. As he states it: Ibid., p. 156. M 222 The T—group experience begins with the educator making explicit that it is designed to help human beings 1) to explore their values and their impact upon others, 2) to determine if they wish to modify the old values and develop new ones, and 3) to develop awareness of how groups can inhibit as well as facilitate human growth and decision-making.1 Exactly how these changes are to take place is not indi~ cated. For example, how the T-group experience is to "develop awareness of how groups can inhibit as well as facilitate human growth and decision-making" is not indicated. The change agent is not to perform a directive function or provide a power structure for the T-group. He is to provide no rules for the interaction of parti- cipants. Moreover, he is not to delineate tasks, topics, or agenda for discussion. As Argyris states: To begin . . . the educator states that he has no specific goals in mind for them. Moreover, he offers no specific agenda, no regulations, rules, and norms by which they should act. This 'vacuum' of leadership, goals, and so on, is created so the members can fill it with their own leadership, goals, rules.2 1Ibid., p. 145. 2Ibid., p. 157. 3 223 Modeling The change agent is to assume the role of a model T-group member. As a model member, he is not to give advice but to provide feedbacks to other participants about how they affect him. He is to encourage them to FR talk about how he affects them. He is to receive feed- back from them without becoming defensive. Argyris describes the initial actions of the change agent as follows: The educator pp the outset would tend to focus on those interpersonal interventions that tend to help the members 1) to become aware of their present (usually) low potential for establishing authentic relationships 2) to become more skill- ful in providing and receiving non—evaluative descriptive feedbacks 3) to minimize their own and others' defensiveness, and 4) to become increasingly able to experience and own their feelings. He is to do these things by serving as a model member. Argyris does not precisely indicate how the change agent is to "focus on interpersonal interventions" that result in the accomplishment of T-group objectives. What he does is indicated in the following passage: Ibid., p. 169. 224 One way that I have found to be helpful during the early stages of a T-group is to tell the group that I understand how they could feel dependent upon me. But I believe that learning can take place more effectively if they first develop an increasing sense of trust of one another and a feeling that they can learn from one another. I freely admit that silence is not typical of me and that I need to talk, to be active, to participate. In fact, I may even feel a mild hostility that I am in a FA situation in which I cannot participate in the way *- that I desire. Thus, anything they (members) can do to help me "unfreeze" by decreasing their 1 dependence upon me would be deeply appreciated. I add that I realize that this is not easy, and that I will do my share to help decrease the dependence 1 upon me.l 5" ”ll- This passage illustrates the change agent's use of himself as a model member of the T—group. He exhibits the behav- iors that he wishes the others participants to exhibit. For example, he discourages "dependence" upon him for direction because he wants to avoid the "traditional educational approach“ to training. In the passage, Argyris, in effect, tells the participants that he will share his problems with them and would like, in turn, for them to share their problems with him and each other. This action supposedly shows the participants that he wants them to provide feedback on his behavior as well as feedback on their behavior toward each other. Ibid., p. 158. 225 Non-cognitive Interaction While the change agent shares his problems and gives feedback to the other participants, he does not attempt to share his "skills" in psychology with them. He does not strive to provide participants with psycho— logical tools for analyzing each others'behavior in "depth." This restriction on the change agent's behav- ior arises from Argyris' contention that T-groups are fundamentally different from therapy groups. A T—group in contrast to a therapy group: . . . emphasizes the constructive, healthy, positive aspects of life. It has to, if for no other reason than that the educator is not there to lend his skilled professional advice to analyze the pathological aspects of their behavior. The group, if it is to succeed, must rely primarily upon its member resources with minimum depth interpretation from the educator. Apparently, the change agent is not to give participants advice or evaluate their behavior in "depth" terms. But if this is so, exactly what is it that the change agent is to do? Why, for that matter, does a T-group require a change agent? How does it emphasize the "constructive, healthy, positive aspects of life?" What are ”the con- structive, healthy, positive aspects of life?" Why, in 11bid., p. 174. 226 short, does not the change agent "lend his skilled pro— fessional advice to analyze the pathological aspects of their behavior?" And what "member resources" must the T-group rely on? Argyris' reason for not providing professional advice or sharing professional skills with participants is: Without conceptual tools to get at pathology or their deeper motives, the members must discover what Bettelheim calls the "good experiences" of life, those that are the heart of sane living. If they are unable to do so (even with the help of the educator) then the group will be left at the mercy of the destructive forces and eventually fail. Here is a crucial reason why T-groups need to have as their goals the increased effectiveness of all members and of the group as a whole. These goals act to coerce (responsible) members to behave constructively. Otherwise, since they lack the clinical concepts to cope with destructive pathology, they will tend to fail. This also illustrates why T-groups need to be composed of relatively healthy individuals, that is, individuals who have avail- able to them and are able to help others express constructive, caring behavior that can overcome the destructive tendencies.l This reason assumes that the behavior the change agent is trying to change is not pathological in nature. The change agent is to help members to discover "what Bettelheim cans the "good experiences" of life, those 1Loc. cit. 227 that are the heart of sane living." However, Argyris does not explicate the "good experiences of life." One might begin to think that interaction in a T—group is the same type of interaction that the parti- cipants engage in every day. Argyris claims that such is not the case. As he puts it: One difference between a T-group and everyday relationships is that eventually everyone receives the feedback (nonevaluative) presum— ably in a climate of support and openness with a minimal distortion or evaluation on the part of the sender. This difference assumes that "a climate of support and openness with minimal distortion or evaluation on the part of the sender" can not occur in everyday relation- ships. Argyris does not indicate why they can not occur in everyday relationships. Changing Values: §p_Illustration Argyris says that one of the problems that emerges in the early stages of a T-group: . . . is the apparent need on the part of members to appoint a leader or a chairman. Typically, this need is rationalized as a group need because 'without an appointed leader a group cannot be effective.‘ For example, one member said, 'Look, I think the first thing we need is to elect a 1Ibid., p. 175. 228 leader. Without a leader we are going to get nowhere fast.‘ Another added, 'Brother, you are right. Without leadership there is chaos. People hate to take responsibility and without a leader, they will goof off.‘1 This "apparent need" for leadership arises because of the ambiguity of the situation and the participants' past educational experience. Somewhere or other they have been taught that without leadership chaos occurs. Argyris handles this "problem" by starting a dis— cussion with the participants in order: . . . to help the group members explore the underlying assumptions expressed by those individuals who wanted to appoint leaders. For example, in the case illustrated above, both men began to realize that they were assuming that people 'need' appointed leadership because, if left alone, they will not tend to accept responsibility. This implies a lack of confidence in and trust of the people around the table. These men are suggesting that without an appointed leader the group will flounder and become chaotic.2 It is not clear why the preposition that without leader— ship, chaos would ensue, is invalid. To simply "psych— ologize" the possible implications of the proposition is not to invalidate it. 1Ibid., p. 170. 2Loc. cit. 229 The empirical validity of the proposition depends both on one's conception of leadership and a statement of the conditions under which such a proposition would be "true." For example, if one defines "leadership" as a goal setting and means arranging function, and defines chaos as the absence of goals and means, the proposition would be true on the basis of definition. Assuming this was the case, it would not necessarily follow that such a proposition implies a "lack of confidence in and trust of people." The expression of the proposition by execu- tives might simply reflect their conditioning. It may be that they have been "taught" that a group situation in which someone does not exercise the leadership func- tion cannot accomplish anything. This certainly would not imply that they "mistrust" each other as Argyris claims. What Argyris seems to be suggesting in the fore- going passage is that the goal of the T-group is to increase the participants' awareness of their impact on each other. They are to accomplish this by providing feedback to each other. If this is the case, Argyris himself performs the leadership function because he set 230 forth the goal of the T—group and articulates the means for accomplishing it. Evaluation of the Interpersonal Competence Model In terms of proposition B.4, the model does not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions for a useful behavioral change model. It lacks an objective measure- ment system, empirically validated proposition, and con— trol statements. The Lack pf §g_0bjective Measurement System Argyris attempted to measure the variables in the model and their relationship through interviewing and observing the behavior of eighteen executives from one company. In the study he set forth the following question and hypothesis: In another question, the executives were asked how they would handle an emotional outburst in the meeting. The hypothesis is that the execu- tives would tend to cope with feelings by reject- ing them or by transforming them into intellectual, rational problems. The data confirms the hypothesis. The reasoning behind the hypothesis was that if the organization valued "rationality" and devalued the .i‘.‘ 1Ibid., pp. 59, 60 231 expression of "emotions," participants should react to "emotional outbursts" by suppressing them, or trans— forming them into "rational" problems. He predicted that when participants were asked about how they would handle "emotional outbursts," they would respond in ways which suggest they would suppress, or transform, the outbursts. The data he claims "confirms the hypothesis" is reproduced in Table 6.1. According to Argyris, the TABLE 6.1 SUGGESTED SOLUTION TO EMOTIONAL OUTBURST (Per Cent) 18 2 II a) If possible, stop the meeting. Calm the people down by saying that they are not prepared to discuss the subject. More groundwork is needed. Call off the meeting . ... .89 b) Let the feelings be expressed. You might learn something about the individuals ... ... . . . 6 Total solutions of 'leaving the fieldf. . . ...95 Solutions of dealing with feelings... . ... ... O "data" demonstrated that the executives did not attempt to cope with "emotional outbursts" by "handling the feel- ings." Supposedly, it, indirectly, meant that the fl—KF— 232 executives adhered to the organization's value of "rationality" and devaluing of "emotions." Because they did not mention that they would directly cope with the outburst, Argyris assumed they had internalized these values. The internalization of these values, in turn, supposedly decreased their interpersonal compet— ence so that they did not have the "ability to handle" the outbursts. The Pdata" would seem to raise some questions that Argyris does not answer. First of all, were the executives who answered his question reacting to their actual experience, or were they expressing "ideal" opinions? Had they ever encountered such situations? If so, what did they do? What consequences did they see as arising from their action? Answers to such questions might enable one to determine if these problems are common and how execu- tives actually cope with them. It is important to have such knowledge since it is possible that if they rarely encounter such problems they lack an opportunity to develop a skill in coping with them. 233 Assuming the executives actually experienced such problems, why did they fail to learn how to handle them? Is their failure to mention the possibility of "dealing with the feelings" primarily due to the organization's value system, or an inability on their part to handle such situations? Stated differently, if they do not Exp cope with emotional outbursts by "dealing with the feel- ings" is it primarily because of the strict "rationality" “I!“ value, or because they do not know how to do so, or is it some combination of these two reasons? To illustrate the importance of these questions, assume that their failure to "deal with such feelings" is a result of the organization's values. Also assume that the executives internalize these values and as a result fail to develop "interpersonal competence." The importance issue becomes one of explaining where the organization obtained its value of "rationality” and the suppression of emotions. Obviously, since the organiza- tion acquires its values from people within it, the values must originally come from certain people within it. The question, then, becomes: where do these certain people acquire these valuesi’ Did they, for example, learn these values while in college? 234 While it cannot be determined from the data Argyris presents, it would seem possible that the organization's value creators, as well as the eighteen executives, learned the values of "rationality" and suppression of emotions outside of the organization. Some of them might have learned them in certain business schools which, perhaps, inculcate these values through stressing the use of rational and impersonal techniques and procedures for running organizations. Perhaps, by teaching so-called traditional "principles of management" they indoctrinate the students in "rationality." Argyris in his works certainly alludes to this possibility. He has claimed that such principles as span of control, and unity of command lead to harmful psychological consequences. One of these consequences is a lowering of interpersonal competence which alleg- edly decreases organization effectiveness. This would suggest that such principles are dysfunctional because they detract from their expressed purpose of increas- ing organizational effectiveness. According to this thesis, the executives and value creators of the organization do not receive 235 training in college, or from their educational experience in general, that develops their "interpersonal abilities." Since they do not receive such training, T—group train— ing is needed to develop these abilities. If this thesis were valid, it might be possible to prevent low inter- personal competence of executives through restructuring their educational exposure in college. The “proper" values could be inculcated so that there would be no need for T-group training later in their careers. Argyris has nothing to say about this possibility. Another set of data Argyris interprets as a measure of a rationality value held by an organization is also open to other interpretations. This particu- lar measure consisted of asking the eighteen executives: 1) what characteristics make one an effective leader; 2) what characteristics influence promotions; and 3) what types of assignments are the most effective for developing executives. The executives responses are described in Table 6.2. Table 6.2, according to Argyris, indicates that the eighteen executives think "interpersonal abilities" are unimportant in determining effective role performance. 236 It should be pointed out that the executives did not explicitly state that "interpersonal abilities" were unimportant. Simply because they failed to mention it at all is the reason it received a zero value in Table 6.2. Once again, one of the pertinent questions the data raises but does not answer is why the executive did not mention interpersonal abilities as an important factor in "effective" leadership, getting ahead, and developing their subordinates? Is it because they were taught in college and by society at large that such abilities are not important? Are they taught that tech- nical competence and hard work should be the important factors rather than a "pleasing personality?" Unfor- tunately we cannot answer these questions from the data Argyris accumulated. Moreover, to provide answers we would need to know exactly what he means by "interper- sonal abilities." Since its meaning is not well articu- lated, it is not surprising that the executives did not mention it. Assuming it refers to transmitting and receiving descriptive feedback, the reason for not men- tioning it may, once again, be attributed to inappropri- ate conditioning. a) b) 237 TABLE 6.2 PERCEIVED IMPORTANCE OF INTERPERSONAL ABILITIES BY THE EIGHTEEN EXECUTIVES + Characteristics of Effective Leadership (Per cent) Ability to articulate one's ideas. . . . . . . . 89 Ability to direct, to be aggressive . . . . . . 72 Ability to think quickly; high intelligence . . 50 Interpersonal abilities . . . . . . . . . . . . 0 Leadership Factors That Influence Promotions (Per cent) N 18 Technical, professional competence . . . . . . . 89 Persuasive, "seller of ideas". . . . . . . . . . 72 High intellectual capacity . . . . . . . . . . . 6l Interpersonal abilities . . . . . . . . . . . . 0 Characteristics of Effective Executive Development (Per cent) N = 18 Give individual a job that challenges his rational, technical competence . . . . . . . . . 72 Give individual a job that provides challenging, interpersonal as well as the intellectual rational experience. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 238 The Lack of Empirically Validated Propositions Argyris has attempted to indirectly validate the model through gathering data on changes that occur in the behavior of T-group participants. The intended purpose of the training was to change the values held by partici- pants in order to achieve high interpersonal competence. However, Argyris has not tried to find out if the train- ing actually changes values held by the participants. In the validation study, he examined certain changes in their behavior and inferred that changes in values had produced them. What behavior of participants changed after T-group training? To answer the question, the tech— nique used to measure changes in behavior must be exam- ined. The measure used was a modified form of Kelly's Role Reportory. The modified form was called the Person Description Instrument. The instrument required execu- tives to describe eleven persons besides themselves within their organization. They were asked to compare the persons in sets of three and asked to give an important way in which one of the three persons differed nu I "a 239 from the other two. They were then asked to state the logical opposite of this characteristic. Examples of characteristics mentioned by the executives are as follows: Differentiating Characteristic Logical Opposite l. Mentally alert 1. Mentally dull 2. warm in personal relation— 2. Cool in rela- ships. tionships with each other. The first characteristic mentioned is postulated to be a description of the person based on his technical abilities The second characteristic, on the other hand, is held to be a description of a person based on his interpersonal abilities. The validation study by Argyris indicated that executives exposed to T-group training increased the use of interpersonal terms (such as characteristic 2) in describing themselves and others. Participants in the T-group when compared with executives in a control group increased the use of interpersonal terms in describing themselves and other people. This might well be expected since the T-group training stresses interpersonal factors. However, such evidence does not "validate" the interper- sonal competence model. 240 Just because participants change their descrip— tions of other people does not mean that they experiment more with new ideas, give more descriptive feedback, become less rigid in making decisions, and engage in Inore productive behavior. Moreover, it does not indicate .if organizational effectiveness actually increases with cflmanges in the participants' descriptions of other , i People. Lastly, it does not indicate if the participants acztually changed their values or behavior toward others. Argyris summed up the "validation study" by stating : These findings are interpreted as further evidence of the effectiveness of the laboratory training in increasing the desire and ability to perceive and respond to interpersonal and emotional aspects of behavior. Evenif it is granted that the interpretation is correct, :it: cannot be determined if "the desire and ability to E3€31Tceive and respond to interpersonal.and emotional asPects of behavior" actually results in changes in the lbeallavdor predicted by the interpersonal competence model. Irt: cannot be, in sum, determined if they have been taught 11c)“, to handle interpersonal problems and how to make t: . . . 1“Gilrown behaVior more effective. \ lIbid., p. 220. 241 The Lack of Control Statements One of the key assumptions on which the model is based is that changes in values will result in changes in behavior. Argyris expresses this assumption in the following manner: Re-education in human relations should focus more on a change in basic values than on acquiring skills. The development of valid values will tend to lead automatically to the development of proper skills. On the other hand, the skills without appropriate changes in values becomes, at best, an alteration Whose lack of depth and manipulative character will become easily evident to others. Skills follow values; values rarely follow skills. Skills can be used to illustrate values. But they ought not to develop human relationships. 'L I __~- The assumption states that changes in values will result in changes in behavior, rather than the reverse. Argyris does not explain how and why the "development of valid values will tend to lead automatically to the development of proper skills." Moreover, he does not explain what he means by "valid values" and "proper skills." Hence there is no way to measure the empirical validity of this assumption. 1Ibid., p. 135. 242 According to Argyris, changing values is a diffi- cult process for two reasons. The first reason is that: . . . changing the values of individuals effectively requires that the educational processes used should not violate the very values that the executives (in this case) are being asked to consider. Most lecture and dis- cussion teaching is typically based upon values similar to those that the executives presently hold (for example, the teacher is in control; he directs; he focuses on the objectives; and he emphasizes rationality). Consequently, there is a need for a significantly different process 2g education.l This reason is actually an assumption that values cannot be changed if the process of change is itself based on the same values. Since he assumes the values on which "most lecture and discussion teaching is typically based upon" are "similar to those that the executives presently hold," he postulates the only way to change their values is to change the form of teaching. The change agent, so to speak, is not to control, direct, focus on objectives, and emphasize rationality. Apparently, Argyris believeSathat the change agent can change his behavior without changing his values. Through refraining from using the traditional education techniques, he can change the values of participants in ___ 1 Ibid., p. 154. "' gin”. . ’ i l VT—f. 243 the T-group. But where did the change agent learn the "appropriate values?" Was his college education, in terms of professional techniques, different from that of most people? If so, how exactly did it differ? Argyris does not provide answers to these questions- The second reason changing values are difficult is that: . . . values are not on the same order as skills to influence people in order 'to keep them happy.‘ Values are the emotionally rooted, intellectually expressed 'personal directives' that individuals hold that serve to compel their behavior in a particular direction. They cannot be ‘taught,' 'issued,' 'sold' or 'plugged into' people (who have an opportunity to choose freely) regardless of the oratorical, persuasive and pedagogical skill of the teacher.1 This reason is questionable. What does it mean to say values are "emotionally rooted?" Does it mean that they are innate rather than acquired? Obviously they cannot be innate from Argyris' viewpoint since he hopes to somehow change them. Argyris is rather unclear about how and where values are acquired. It is only an assumption that values cannot be taught or "plugged into" people. He presents no empirical evidence that would suggest the Loc. cit. 244 assumption is valid. Without uncovering the determinants of values it is doubtful if a transition-producing mech- anism for changing them could be based on control know— ledge. To state that they are a function of "emotions" ad& nothing in the way,.of control knowledge. Summary Proposition B.4 states that a behavioral change i model is useful if it contains variables a change agent can control, possesses an objective system for measur— ing the values of variables and states empirically validated relationships between the variables. The two need—satisfaction models examined do not meet these con- ditions. Thus in accordance with proposition C:lC they presently can not be considered as useful to managers. To the extent that they are representative of other need-satisfaction models, proposition C.1 is supported. Attempts to change behavior in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences would not presently meet the neces— sary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. 245 In terms of premise A.2 neither of the models can be considered as presently useful to managers in solving their behavioral problems. It is not yet pos— sible to determine if they could help managers solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effect- ively since the models used by managers have not been identified. Thus it is not known if the use of the two models would help them solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively. In terms of premise A.3, there are two reasons the models can not be considered as presently helpful to managers in solving their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively. First, they do not yet constitute control knowledge. Secondly, the control knowledge possessed by managers has not been identified. Therefore it is not possible to determine if the know- ledge provided by the models is superior to that posses- sed by managers. CHAPTER VII THE FRAMEWORK UTILIZED TO ANALYZE THE IMPLICIT BEHAVIORAL CHANGE MODELS OF THE TEN MANAGERS: AN ANALYSIS OF THE D PROPOSITIONS The Need for an Empirical Study Premise A.2 states that knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences is useful to managers if it helps them solve their behavioral problems more efficiently and effectively than they could solve them without using such knowledge. This premise leads to proposition D.l. To determine the usefulness of know- ledge contained in the behavioral sciences to managers, the behavioral problems managers face and how they cope with them must be determined. Without this analysis the usefulness of knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences can not be ascertained. Stated differently, without knowing the types of behavioral problems managers face and how they c0pe with them, it is not possible to determine what types 246 247 of behavioral change models will be useful to them in solving these problems. Moreover, it is possible that the implicit model they presently use is as effective or even more effective than the ones available to them in the behavioral sciences. The models analyzed in Chapters IV, V, and VI may or may not be as effective in changing behavior as the models managers typically use. This is an empirical issue that can only be resolved through experimentation. One of the first steps in resolving the issue is determining the types of change models managers use and if they differ from those in the behavioral sciences. This was the scope of the present study. The study did not go beyond this stage and experimentally compare the models analyzed in Chapters IV, V and VI with those managers were discovered to use. Implicit Behavioral Change Models: ‘Ag Analysis 9; Proposition D.2 The study was designed to provide data on which to analyze proposition D.2. This proposition states that managers implicitly use behavioral change models in handling inadequate performance on the part of their 248 subordinates. On an g_priori basis it was reasoned that inadequate performance represents a U type behavior pattern. It was assumed that managers implicitly formulate propositions about the determinants of inade- quate performance and how changes in their values will lead to adequate performance. These propositions con- stitute a transition-producing mechanism. The study was designed to reveal the particular mechanism the ten managers used to c0pe with inadequate performance. Control Patterns: The Unit 9; Analysis The first step in analyzing the data was to search for variables managers mentioned in discussing how they coped with inadequate performance. As variables were extracted from the interviews, patterns were discovered. Certain managers would mention and describe similar ways of coping with inadequate Performance. It became apparent that to understand the implicit behavioral change models the managers used, the control environment they established had to be examined. The control environment dictated the type of 249 implicit change model utilized. In total, three basic control patterns were dis- covered. The patterns were labeled freedom-centered, authority-centered, and development—centered. The freedom-centered pattern was constructed from the inter- view with the general manager. His control pattern was different from the patterns used by the nine managers who worked under him. The authority-centered pattern was constructed from interviews with four managers engaged in roles that dealt with highly repetitive activi— ties such as accounting and manufacturing. The develop- ment—centered pattern was constructed from interviews with five managers who were engaged in roles that dealt with highly non-repetitive activities such as research and development and product management. Limitatigns g; the Control Pattern as a Unit _g_f_ Analysis The managers did not explicitly articulate the three patterns. The patterns were constructed through interpreting what the managers articulated and how their statements fit together to form a pattern. Thus, because 250 of the exploratory nature of the study, the patterns are heuristic devices. As such, they represent an interpre- tative framework that was impossed on the data. They provide in terms of a pattern a descriptive insight into the implicit behavioral change models the ten managers utilized. Because of the small sample on which the study was based, it is not possible to generalize on the prevalence of the three patterns. Moreover, it is not possible to say that these patterns are exhaustive. No attempt was made to determine which pattern was the most efficient and effective in controlling and changing the behavior of subordinates. Control Pattern Variables The variables extracted from the interviews that were used to differentiate the patterns are as follows. Conception 2f the Managerial Role The three control patterns delineated stemed in large part from the managers'conception of their role. Differences in role conception resulted in differences in control patterns and, therefore, implicit change models. 251 Extent 9; Role §tructuring Extent of role structuring by managers was two- fold. First, there were differences in the extent to which they defined the role content of their subordinates. Second, there were differences in the extent to which they defined role performance techniques for their subordinates. The values assigned to these variables in the three patterns were a function of the managers' role conception. The extent of role structuring was estimated from statements made by the managers. On the basis of an inter-pattern comparison, it was assigned a low, medium, or high value. An assignment of a high value meant that in comparison with the other two patterns the extent of role structuring was greater. Extent.9§.;mpersonality Extent of impersonality in the relationship between the manager and subordinate was twofold. First, there were differences in the extent to which managers took individual differences into account in designing a control environment. A manager who did not take 252 individual differences into account used the same control pattern for all his subordinates. He implicitly assumed that they would all respond to the same control pattern with similar responses He also attributed homogeneous work motives to his subordinates. Hence, impersonality deals with the universalism of a given control pattern. Second, there were differences in the extent to which managers considered the expression of emotions and feelings by their subordinates as relevant to their relationship. A manager who considered them non-relevant emphasized rational and role—oriented behavior. He viewed his subordinates personal life as being isolated from his role in the organization. Extent of impersonality was considered a present or absent variable. A relationship was either classified as impersonal or not impersonal. The Handling g; Inadequate Performance The ten managers differed in the mechanisms they used to cope with inadequate performance. There were six variables that differentiated the mechanism used in 253 the three control patterns; role performance criteria, amount of feedback on role performance, extent of collaboration, extent of participation in correcting inadequate performance, and motives attributed to subordinates. Role Performance Criteria The criteria the managers used for evaluating the role performance of subordinates differed. They differed in the emphasis they placed on technical and non-technical criteria. Technical criteria refers to such items as percentage increase in sales. Non-technical criteria refers to such items as attitude toward work. The criteria they used was their basis for establishing the need for change. Deviations from them are descriptions of behavior pattern U while the criteria itself describes behavior D. Amount of Feedback 9g Role Performance The amount of role performance feedback the managers gave their subordinates differed. They differed in how often they discussed role performance and when they initiated a discussion. 254 The division had no formal procedure for appraising role performance. Thus, each manager was able to develop his own procedure. Extent pf Collaboration The managers differed in the extent to which they asked subordinates for their views on the reason for inadequate performance. Extent of Participation in Correcting Inadequate Performance The managers differed in the extent to which they participated in helping subordinates develop a program for improving their performance. Since it was not possible to determine the degree of help given in patterns where it was found present, it was considered a present or absent variable. The extent of participation was a function of the managers' view on the determinants of inadequate performance. The Psychodynamics of the Patterns An attempt was made to discover where the managers acquired the control patterns they used. This analysis 255 focused on proposition D.3. Namely, the implicit change model managers use are not directly acquired from the behavioral sciences. .5 Summary Comparison of the Three Control Patterns Figure 7.1 compares in summary form the three control patterns. 256 .msumuumm Houucoo woman may no cowaummfioo m .H.h .mHm 5.30.8 m mm. wow wow Hogacsomulsoc pew HMOflsflome mow mOW ucmuxm Sofipme m 09 mmpmazosx mo Homoamtrma samm coaum>fluofi Hmaocmcflm Imamm mm» 02 02 02 wow oz Hmoacnome Hogacnoma 02 oz 02 oz usouxm pcmuxm Amara m 08 30H m 09 u H>m u o.mo£mn EOWWm m umaaouusoo Hm©H>Oum mmumsflpuonsm ou pmcmammm m>HuoE xuo3 ucmcHEOUmHm mosmEHomumm mumswmpmsa pomuuou mopmsfipuonsm msamamn aw Uwummflofluumm mocmfiuomumm mumsgmpmsa How mGOmmmH so mmumcapuonsm Suaz pmumuoanaoo mopmsflpuonsm ou xomnpmmm musmfiuomuwm oamaummm popa>oum moumsflouonsm mo mocmfiuomumm mpmsam>m 0p Hmmmcme an poms maumufluu ucmcHEoomnm .M "mmumsfipnonsm an mosmEuomumm muwsgmpmsfl-mo mafiaosmm GHMEauHmmH mm mmsflammm paw mGOHwoem mo mmcmnoxm pmumnamcoo cumuumm Houucoo mcacmwmmp :H mmocwnmmmap amspw>apca pmnmoamcoo .n .m "HmsomHmQEH mm3 mmumsapuonsm paw Hmmmcma som3umn mflnmcoaumHmH mag nuanz ou usmuxm mmumcfipuonsm mo mmswacnomu mocmfiuomumm ppm usmusou maou 03¢ pmusuosuum Hmmmsmz mmumsapuonsm on mean IGOflumamH CH maou man mo coflpmmosoo m.nmmmcmz mmumsapuonsm mo Hoa>mnmn mmsmso cam Houusoo ou poms Hoou AMGOmHmmHmDGH usmsafioomum sommmm huwuonusm mufismflnem pmumucwu pmumusmo poucuswu lucmfimon>mn Impwuonusm IEOpmmHm cumuumm Houucoo no game moanmwum> cuwuuwm CHAPTER VIII ANALYSIS OF THE FREEDOMrCENTERED CONTROL PATTERN The Manager's Rationale for the Pattern The general manager's perception of a competitive threat constituted the rationale for the control pattern he developed. He believed the threat could be handled by improved performance of his division. He described the threat in the following manner: We must produce things that are new and better. A man can say that we've improved our product as much as we can improve them but maybe a competitor comes along and brings in a brand new idea. This happens repeatedly in our business. We'll be on top for awhile, have the best product, the furtherest advanced one. We'll build this product for awhile, and be very well satisfied with it and then somebody will come along and make something a little bit better. Well up until the time the competitor makes something a little bit better, the engineerng department doesn't really have much incentive to try and improve this thing. It might be that the engineers will say this is the best I can do, so what can I do about it? But then he sees somebody else outsmart him and, then, he realizes he wasn't so smart. 257 258 This happens to us just over and over and over again in every single line we produce. There is somebody just a little smarter than we are. Sometimes we get ahead of them and then they get ahead of us. According to the manager, the division could not afford to wait for competitors to spur its people to innovate. He felt that he had to get in front of his competition rather than merely meet it. His answer was the development of a freedom- centered control pattern. The pattern was intended to establish an environment capable of generating "things that are new and better." The Manager's Conception 9§_His Role The manager considered his role to consist of creating a control environment for generating new ideas and improvements. He believed that the appropriate environment was related to the freedom of his subordinates to define their roles and role techniques. His function after providing them with freedom was to evaluate their results. The results he expected were new ideas and improvements. 259 Extent 2; Role Structuring The freedom the manager provided subordinates consisted of an Opportunity to define their roles and to develOp role performance techniques. He did not tell them of what their role consisted, on what activities to work, or how to go about carrying out their roles. The role situation confronting his subordinates was ambiguous. They were left to define and structure their roles. The low extent of role structuring was reflected in the manager's statement that: The objective here is to have each department head run his own department as much as he can within the bounds of the corporation objectives which I am here to direct. Now I think in some areas we will have people who are much more self-motivated than others. In cases where they lack self-motivation it is, of course, my job to try and check and recheck, and to encourage them either by continuously questioning, or by praise, or by proding to do the best possible job they can and to keep on improving. Later on he stated: Now my department managers will come to me and say: 'what do you think we should work on. You tell us what to work on.‘ Well, I tell them, I'm paying them to run their departments. They should find the things to work on. 260 The Experiencing pf.a Low Degree pf Role Structuring by Subordinate Managers The nine subordinate managers experiencing of the low degree of role structuring by the general manager provided a measure of validity for the description of his control pattern. All of them indicated that he rarely told them what to do, what problems to work on, and what techniques to use in performing their role assignments. One of the managers experienced the ambiguity in the following manner: I have been very unclear about my assignment. I am hesitant, however, about having it clarified probably because the reaction generally is that it's clear enough and you should take it from there. It has particuiarly galled me over the years that the direction has been so broad that it leaves me without any program. A typical example is that he tells me we need something new. Well you can't Operate just on the basis of telling the boys to think up something new. And yet he pounds the table and tells us we have to bring something new to the sales department. In what area, what problems need to be solved, where is the sales potential, where shall we direct our efforts, he doesn't indicate. I get nowhere except he needs something new, and you've got to get these boys to produce more. 261 Impersonalipy Because the manager granted freedom to all subordinates, his control pattern was universalistic. He did not account for individual differences in establishing his control pattern. Moreover, his notion of self-motivation precluded personalized inter- actions with subordinates. A self—motivated subordinate was defined as one who did not need to ask the manager what to do and how to do it. The manager did not consider expression of emotions and feeling by subordinates as appr0priate role behavior. His interaction with subordinates was very infrequent and always task oriented. He expected them to independently carry out their role assignments. The Experiencing pf Impersonality .py Subordinate Managers It was thought that in response to the ambiguity in role structuring and evaluation that the subordinate managers might band together in order to cope with it. Such, however, was not the case. They did not interact with each other socially and only rarely within the organization. When they did interact with each other it 262 was strictly on the basis of their formal roles. Seldom, if ever, did they discuss their problems with each other, or indicate that they desired this form of interaction. Two of the nine, however, seemed to desire it. As one of them phrased it: I often thought that it might be well if our top manager had his department heads meet at the country club for a breakfast meeting. On completion of the breakfast spend about 4 hours completely relaxed in a conversation about anything you wanted to talk about without any pressure. Discuss your problems and what you think is needed to do a better over-all job for the division. Then, perhaps, have lunch and in the afternoon play golf, swim or anything you want to, but get acquainted. The statements indicated a plea for a more personalized form of interaction within the division. Apparently, this manager felt that he should be able to discuss his problems with the general manager and other subordinate managers and be personally acquainted with them. Another manager reacted to the impersonality of the freedom-centered pattern by stating: I would like to have an operation on a more personal basis. I am perhaps, somewhat at fault in this regard. I am not a particularly extroverted person. But I still enjoy having someone indicate to me that he would like to talk to me on a personal basis. I perhaps 263 don't take the initiative and am not aggressive enough in establishing that manner of contact, but I still enjoy it. I like to feel that I can go into my supervisor and talk to him about my problems in the department and have him curtiously listen if nothing more, rather than get off that subject and squelch immediately my discussion. My experience has been that tendency. The statements indicated that this manager would have liked a more personalized form of interaction with the general manager but was confused as to whom should initiate it. He blamed himself in part for the lack of it rather than the general manager. However, he believed that the general manager's duty was to talk to him about his problem and disliked his not doing so. This type of reaction to the freedomrcentered control pattern might be expected because the general manager is simply refusing to help this manager structure his role. Hence, it is consistent with his conception of his role. It is the subordinate's duty to structure his role and performance techniques. The Handling pf Inadequate Performance The manager used technical criteria to appraise the performance of subordinates. Engineering managers, 264 for example, were evaluated on the number of product improvements they achieved. Product managers were evaluated on the basis of increased profits. When the manager evaluated a subordinate's per- formance as being inadequate, he attributed it to a lack of self-motivation. To correct the deficiency, he pressured the subordinate to do something different or to come up with something new. However, he never indicated the nature of this "something." It was the task of the subordinate to decide what specific changes to make in his behavior in order to improve performance. Hence, the manager did not participate in helping subordinates improve their performance. The manager described his apporach in the following manner: I think in the majority of cases I try to impress upon them the need for improvement. We will see a sales trend where the product line may be dropping and our competitor is coming in and getting the buSiness. I point this out to the people and try to indicate to them how serious it is for them (they're payed on bonuses) and the company to allow this condition to continue. (The external threat once again. Note how he uses it to justify the need for improvement.) That sometimes works. And if improvements did not occur, he stated: 265 As a last resort I tell them they either got to do something or it means their jobs. In some cases, it's as serious as that. Usually we have never resorted to firing people because we couldn't get them to react. Maybe that time will come. The manager never told subordinates how well they were doing in a positive sense. He believed that their performance could always be improved. He also felt that if he were to compliment them they would become content with their current performance. The Experiencing pf Ambiguity ip Evaluation p1 the Subordinate Managers All nine subordinate managers experienced ambiguity in the evaluation of their role performance. They reported that the general manager never told them how satisfied he was with their performance. The only evaluative cue they received was his increased pressure to get improved performance. One manager described his experiencing of the evaluative ambiguity in the following manner: I don't know if the present manager and the one which preceeded him feel that I, or as far as I know, anyone else who is a department head isn't doing the job that is expected of a manager. Just how well he regards what I am doing, I 266 don't know. I am sure it must be passible or I wouldn't be here. I have got some increases in salary and that is also a means of recognition. We have had very few personal discussions about my work. The statements indicated that the subordinate experienced ambiguity in evaluation. They also offered support for the validity of the general manager's statements about how he evaluated the role performance of subordinates. Because of the ambiguity in evaluation, some subordinates established their own indicators. The indicators they established allowed them to determine when to make changes in their behavior. One manager, previously mentioned, said that the fact he still had his job and had received salary increases indicated to him that the general manager must have considered his performance "passable." Another indicator that he thought important was stated as follows: I handle all the patent matters for the department and I know he respects my ability in this regard. He doesn't review patent matters which are quite important, he depends on my judgements about them. So indirectly he does respect that ability. Later in the interview this subordinate mentioned that he thought it was his knowledge of patent matters that accounted for the rise to his present position. 267 This suggested that subordinates in the presence of ambiguity in evaluation may develop self—indicators on the basis of what they considered their greatest ability or interest. This subordinate, for example, had defined his key function as the handling of patents and used the manager's lack of interference in this area as an indicator of successful role performance. This indicator conflicted with the general manager's desire to generate new ideas. He thought the subordinates most important function was to generate new ideas. The Experiencing pf Pressure py the Subordinate Managepg The only direction the subordinate managers experienced was generalized pressure to constantly improve. One manager described his experience in the following manner: He expects too much from everybody in too little time. It creates an atmosphere of frustration where you just know damn well you can't please him regardless of how hard you try so what the hell do you want to bat your head against the wall for. So you go about todays work and get todays work done and that's about it. Your not pleasing the boss, you know that. He's never satisfied. 268 The statements indicated that this manager experienced ambiguity in the general manager's evaluation of his role performance. He, also, experienced the generalized pressure to constantly improve and found it somewhat annoying. Another manager described his experience in the following manner: I don't feel that he's aware of the amount we are producing because he has no way of knowing the amount of work that is required to turn out a particular piece of final development work. But he still has the feeling that with the over-all direction we've got to produce more. We got to get our subordinates to work more. Now that is a little too broad. There is no way I can help. I know that any department can be improved, so what good does it do to have him tell me that we have got to do better. Particularly, when I know he's telling all the department the same thing. The statements indicated that the subordinate found both the ambiguity in evaluation and the generalized pressure frustrating. What was particularly interesting was his state- ments about how he responded to them. If he actually decreased, or minimized, his performance because he felt the general manager was never satisfied, he would be 269 engaging in a form of self-defeating behavior. The more he minimized his role performance, the more the general manager might be dissatisfied with his performance, and the greater might be the pressure he would exert on the subordinate to improve. If such were the case, the subordinate's frustration might increase over time. The possibility exists that the subordinate might build up a resistance to the pressure so that he would not find increased pressure as frustrating. This seemed to be the case with this subordinate since he expected the pressure. The Problem.p£ Self-Motivation The manager saw the lack of self-motivation as a perplexing problem. As he phrased it: We have different types of individuals in the various department heads. Some of them are very much inclined to push things as fast as they can to go ahead, while we'll have another maybe that drags along. With the different department heads we've got various degrees of self-motivations. I think I've got more trouble getting department heads to carry their responsi- bilities farther and to motivate themselves, than I do with department heads I have to hold back. Even though, if I had my preference, I would much rather prefer to hold back a subordinate than I would to try to motivate him. 270 This problem of motivating people is a problem with me. I don't know whether it is with other people or not. These statements indicated his concern with self— motivation. Also he was bewildered about the lack of self-motivation. Apparently, he assumed that his subordinates would be eager to accept and use the freedom he provided them. Furthermore, he assumed that they had the ability to be self—motivated and generate new ideas. Without self-motivation, he believe they could not productively use the freedom he provided. The ldeationally Obsolete: ,A Special Case 9; Inadequate Performance When subordinates did not respond to increased pressure for improvement, the manager thought that, per- haps, they had run out of ideas. His strategy in such cases was to provide them with sources of new ideas. In this connection, he said: We have and are sending people to seminars. We have and are sending people to other plants. We hope that they will see what other peOple are doing and, then, come back and say this is what I should do because someone else has been successful doing it. 271 I've got a real problem with one department head who we are sending to school for a month trying to jar him out of a complacent feeling and a desire to stay status quo. I don't know if this fellow is so complacent but I think he's fearful of making a move that could be wrong, that might bring criticism. Hopefully he will be put in contact with other people who will indicate what they've been doing and their problems will probably be similar. They're being thrown together with a group of people who are having these same kinds of problems. Maybe some of them will be doing something and if they're not then, hopefully, the course presented by the instructor will be such that these fellows will have a broader outlook and a more, say, futuristic viewpoint of the things that go on around the factory. This is a problem that is not easy. The manager sent the subordinate to an external environment where it was hoped that through exposure to other people‘s problems and approaches he would come back with new ideas. But he hOped for more than this. He hOped that whatever it is that makes people self- motivated would be initiated by the training program. If the training did not accomplish this, he would have to send the subordinate back every time he depleted the ideas he acquired from associates in the seminar. It seemed odd that the general manager did not feel that anyone in the division, including himself, 272 could help the department head generate new ideas. A partial answer was that he believed there was no one else in the division that had experience in the department head's functional area, or role. He believed the department head had to be sent out to interact with other people in similar functional areas and roles. The general manager was unclear about what the department head needed to generate new ideas and improvements. However, he knew there had been few changes in the department's operations over the past five years. Also costs had been increasing. Since his use of increased pressure failed to improve the department's performance, he assumed that the depart- ment head had run out of ideas. He thought the way to correct the impoverishment was to expose the department head to people in similar roles who, hope- fully, would have new ideas. Deficiency of new ideas was viewed by the manager as a consequence of a lack of self—motivation. He thought that if the department head had been self- motivated he would not have run out of ideas. The 273 manager hoped that by exposing the department head to new ideas he would become self—motivated. Modeling as a Mechanism for Self—Motivating About six months prior to the study, the manager hired a young Ph.D. candidate in the field of communications. Previous to his employment the young Ph.D. candidate had contacted the general manager to find out if he could use the division as a source of data for his dissertation. The manager consented with a provision that he would reveal some of the problem areas he found to the manager. The Ph.D. candidate agreed and within a month he provided the manager with a twenty page list of problem areas he had uncovered. The manager was so impressed with the report that he asked him if he would be willing to work on these problems and join the company as his assistant. The young Ph.D. candidate accepted the position. The manager felt that his new assistant was a prime example of a self-motivated person. He did not need to be told what to do, or how to do it. He was a man with new ideas that he was not afraid to try out. 274 The manager gave him authority to make decisions in many of the functional areas of the division and left him to define his own role. He hOped that the assistant would serve as a model to his other sub- ordinates Of a self-motivated person so that they would become like him. He felt that the assistant would keep them from being content with thestatus quo since he was constantly suggesting changes in their Operations. Thus the manager tried to self—motivate sub- ordinates by providing them with a model of a self- motivated person. In doing so, he assumed that sub- ordinateswho are not self—motivated can become so by Observing a person who is and emulating him. The model also exerted pressure on them to try and match up with the model. The Psychodynamics pf the Freedom— centered Control PattErn fme~ the Point pf View pf the Manager Insight into why the general manager used a freedomrcentered control pattern was gained through exploring his experience with people he had worked under in the past. In the past, he thought he had 275 been exposed to a freedom—centered control pattern in which he was not told what to do or how to do it. As he described it: I was never told really how to go about performing my job. There was no such thing as job descriptions when I started. You might say a lot of it was self- motivating. I don't believe my superior, at the time I was a product manager had any idea of what was the best thing to work on. The statements indicated that he perceived himself as being given an ambiguous role assignment. However, he felt that it had to be that way because the superior did not have the capacity to structure his role. He believed that the superior had properly performed the managerial role and did not find the experience frustrating. Thus, it is not surprising that he has incorporated the pattern for controlling his own subordinates. He felt that his subordinates expected him to tell them what to do and how to do it. This, of course, was in conflict with his conception of the manager as a provider of freedom. Because of their demands for role structuring, he assumed they lacked self-motivation. If they were self—motivated, they would not ask him what 276 and how to work. After all, he did not make such demands on his superior. As he described it: Now there is a change in philiosopy right or wrong. Now my department manager will come to me and say what do you think we should work on? You tell us what to work on. Well, I tell them I am paying them to run their department. They should find the things to work on. The statements indicated that he expected subordinates to structure their roles. Insight into the meaning the general manager assigned to self-motivation was, also, gained from examining his past experience. He felt that a self- motivated subordinate was one who could define his own role content and, appraise his own performance. He does not have to be told what to do or how he is doing. The manager considered himself to be a self— motivated person. This was revealed in a discussion of his early work experience. As a salesman he said: I was a salesman and I knew what I was doing. A sales job is very gratifying because you know what you've done when you get the order. You know what you've done at the end of the year. SO you don't need somebody to tell you how you're doing. As an engineer he stated: 277 I knew I was bringing products to them, brand new ideas, patents that were very valuable to them. I need not need to be told how I was doing. Later as a product manager he stated: As a product manager of a section here our group in the division was making a higher percentage of profits. So once again no one had to tell me how I was doing. This series of statements indicated that he felt it was appropriate that none of his superiors structured his role. It is not difficult to see why he did not inform his subordinates on the adequacy of their performance. The manager considered a self-motivated subor- dinate to be one who was capable of generating new ideas. In terms of his own experience, he felt that the generation of new ideas had been responsible for his rise to general manager. This was revealed in his statements that: I've always been able to do something different. As a salesman, I designed so to speak. If I saw something I thoughtneeded a better way of doing, I would send suggestions to the company. Few of the salesman I was with ever did that. They sold. They did better selling and I was never the best salesman but I was down 3rd or 4th or 5th. But I was making the company more money because I was contributing something beyond sales. 278 The statements indicated the importance he assigned to his ability to generate new ideas as an important factor of his success. This was the "something beyond sales" that he provided the company. Apparently, the general manager believed the freedomrcentered control pattern under which he operated and his self-motivation were responsible for the new ideas he generated. He realized, of course, that it was necessary for the company to value these ideas. In this connection, he said: I think these ideas brought my name to the attention of everybody up the line because they would say, here is an idea Worthington sent us. I know this is true because I had to get patents on them. Applying for patents indicated to him that his performance was adequate. The Psychodynamics pf the Freedom-Centered Control Pattern From the Point 9: View pf Subordinates None of the nine subordinates of the general manager copied his control pattern in managing their subordinates. They developed two different control patterns, the authority-centered and the development? centered. 279 The general manager seemed to have set in motion with his control pattern a set of dysfunctions if he believed the pattern to be the best one for generating new ideas. Subordinates did not copy his pattern in managing their own subordinates. The lack of emulation suggested that the manager through granting freedom to his subordinates did not guarantee that they, in turn, would grant it to their subordinates. Due to his refraining from participation in structuring their roles he presented them with role Options. They were free to select the type of control pattern they would use with their subordinates. It was presenting them with role options which constituted the freedom of the general manager's control pattern. But it was an anxiety—ridden freedom for subordinates. Somehow or other they had to structure their roles and develop control patterns of their own. What accounted for the control patterns the subordinates developed in this ambiguous situation? The subordinates did not COpy the control pattern the general manager used even though it was an alternative they might have chosen. There appeared to be two major 280 factors that accounted for the patterns they used with their own subordinates. The first factor was the control pattern they had worked under in the past. If they had found the pattern satisfying, they tend to emulate it. If they found it dissatisfying, they developed a control pattern of their own in which they attempted to eliminate the elements they found dis- satisfying. “é The second factor that may have accounted for the control patterns the subordinates used was personality attributes. They were free to design their roles and develop a pattern that meshed with their personality. This opportunity for personalization may have lowered their experienced frustration in the ‘ ambiguous environment under which they were expected to perform. The general manager had given his subordinateS‘ the impression that they were sources of ideas and improvements while he was the receiver of them. He had told them they were the boss in their role area. Under the authority—centered pattern subordinates reversed the manager-subordinate relationship used by the general 281 manager. They saw themselves as sources of ideas and improvements and their subordinates as receivers of them. They told their subordinates, so to speak, "I am boss and a source of ideas and improvements, you are here to carry them out." Under the development—centered pattern, subor- dinates reacted by defining themselves and their subor— dinates as both receivers and sources of new ideas and improvements. They told their subordinates, "I am a source of ideas and improvements but so are you. We are here to help each other develop." The Relationship pf the Freedom— Centered Control Pattern pp Existing Theory and Research The Prevalence ‘gf Ambiguity According to recent studies, ambiguity appears to be a very prevalent phenomena in contemporary organizations.1 The studies indicate that many people I in organizations are not clear about their role For an excellent study on ambiguity see: R. L. Kahn, D. M. Wolfe, R. P. Quinn, and J. D. Snoek, Qggapizational Stress: Studies ip_Role Copglict and Ambiguity (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1964). 282 assignments, how other participants evaluate their performance, and the scope of their authority. A study by Dill suggested that managers often Operate in environments where they are given role assignments without any indication of their content or how to perform them.1 The environment he described was similar to the general manager's freedom-centered control pattern. The Function p§_Ambiguipy Dill's study suggested a dominant reason managers who make ambiguous role assignments and evaluations can not always make them explicit. The roles they assign might be innovative in nature. When such is the case a manager may only know that the purpose of the role is to come up woth something new. He may not be able to define what this something new is to be, or how it can be accomplished. Obviously, if he could do this on an ex ante basis the role, by definition, would not be an innovative one. 1 W. R. Dill, T. L. Hilton and W. R. Reitman, The Ngw Managers: Patterns pf Behavig; and Development (Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1962). 283 Because of an inability to specify actual output requirements of innovative roles and the techniques for performing them, it is difficult to establish specific performance standards for evaluating the role efficiency and effectiveness of incumbents. About the only performance standard a manager can use is an ad hoc one. Did the incumbent come up with something new and was it useful? At best, this would only be a relative measure of performance since the manager cannot know for certain if output would have been greater if another subordinate had been placed in the role, or if the present subordinate had been more efficient and effective in his behavior. The difficulties of determining what is to be done, how it is to be done, and how performance can be appraised reflects the difficulty the manager faces in trying to change the behavior of subordinates assigned innovative roles. The use of ambiguity as a control technique is one alternative he may resort to in per- forming his duties as a manager. Since he cannot build "do-lOOps" for the roles Of his subordinates, he may use a ratchet principle by which he continuously pressures them to change. His only directive to them may be that 284 they must do better without specifying the meaning of "better," or what techniques subordinates should use. Another condition that makes it difficult for a manager to structure the roles of subordinates is where he lacks knowledge in their specialities. Thus, objectively he may not be able to tell some of his subordinates what to do and what to work on. If he lacks knowledge of the different role areas under his jurisdiction, he may only be able to appraise performance on the results achieved. His lack of knowledge may lead him to conclude that he cannot structure their role. Through pressuring them with vagueness and telling them to improve, he may focus their attention on improving and coming up with something new. Hence, ambiguity in structuring their roles may result in changes in their behavior. The,§ffectsupf Ambiguity The study by Dill sheds additional light on how managers cope with ambiguous environment. When faced with ambiguous environments, they seem to develop a system of "cues" that indicates how well they are doing 285 and when they smuld make changes in their behavior. One of the "new managers," Olin Larson, for example, used the number of changes his boss had to make in the reports he submitted to him to determine how "successful" his actions were. The fewer the number of changes the boss made, the more successful Larson believed his actions were, and the less the need for him to make changes in his behavior. Hence, the number of changes made in the reports was an indicator of whether he should or should not attempt to make changes in his behavior. Another of the "new managers," Tony Rodwell, received an unanticipated but very definite cue that other peOple in his organization expected him to change his behavior. He was unexpectedly shifted from a high power potential position in his organization to a lower power potential position without an explanation as to why he was being shifted. When Rodwell started working for the company he was made assistant to the factory manager who thought Rodwell had a great potential as a manager. The factory manager in order to develop Rodwell's potential as a manager asked him to become an assistant to the chief 286 scheduler of the company. The position of chief scheduler was an important position within the company. The current incumbent of the chief scheduler position was about 45 years old and had received no formal college education. He had worked his way up to the position in about 20 years with the company. The factory manager respected him for his experience and technical competence but thought that he was somewhat deficient as a manager. When discussing this new assignment with Rodwell, the factory manager suggested that Rodwell might move into the position of chief scheduler within a few months, if all went well. If this happened, he planned to give the chief scheduler Rodwell's current job as his personal assistant. After six months in the position of assistant to the chief scheduler Rodwell was rather abruptly shifted to a position of lesser importance by the factory manager who did not explain to Rodwell why he was being trans- ferred. One of the crucial factors responsible for the shift seemed to be that the factory manager perceived Rodwell as violating certain organization norms. 287 One of the norms the factory manager thought Rodwell violated dealt with not discussing your immediate boss's deficiencies with people at the same or a higher organizational level than your boss. The factory manager alluded to this norm and Rodwell's violations of it when he said: One night, Tony dropped in after work. He sat down right where you are sitting and began to castigate his boss. That was real bad taste . . . . There is only one person you have to satisfy, and that is your immedi- ate boss. There was, however, a condition under which complaining was perceived by the factory manager to be legitimate. As he put it: You've got to accept what he says until you've proved by analysis and evaluation that he's wrong. Now if you've proved that something is wrong and your boss won't listen to you, then it's Okay to come and complain to me. But with Tony, it was all opinions and speculation. I saw right away it was very wrong.2 It seems that the factory manager thinks it is legitimate to complain about the behavior of one's boss if he can "prove by analysis and evaluation that he's wrong." He. of course, felt that Rodwell did not do this. It was 1 Ibid., p. 75. 21bid., p. 76. 288 shortly after this encounter with Rodwell that the manager decided to transfer him to a lower ranking position. His expressed reason for transferring Rodwell was to place him in a test situation to find out if Rodwell could change his "inappropriate" behavior. He did not give Rodwell an explanation for the transfer. Another factor which seems to be responsible for Rodwell's transfer was his orientation to the role assignment the factory manager gave him. He thought the factory manager expected him to be aggressive and innovative in carrying out the assignment. Consequently, when he started working for the chief scheduler he was very aggressive in trying to get him to make many changes in the operations of the department. The chief scheduler thought that many of the changes Rodwell suggested were based on inadequate analysis and he refused to implement them. Rodwell, in response, became frustrated and eventually, as we have seen, poured out his frustration to the factory manager. As previously indicated, the factory manager never told Rodwell the reason he was being transferred. At 289 first Rodwell attributed his transfer to organizational "politics." He thought that some important friends of the chief scheduler were worried that the chief scheduler was being "squeezed" out and intervened by making the factory manager transfer Rodwell. But later Rodwell began to discount this reason and to analyze his own behavior in his role as assistant to the chief scheduler. He thought on the basis of his analysis that he had been too aggressive toward the chief scheduler and realized he should not have poured out his frustrations to the factory manager. As he put it: I just didn't pay too much attention to him and started reorganizing the department. It wasn't good human relations.1 He went on to say: I probably didn't make any conscious effort to gain the confidence and liking of the chief scheduler . . . but I was just so damn sure that I wouldn't have to live with him too long that I guess I just sort of fumbled into the situation. The important point of this discussion about Rodwell is that it clearly illustrates the managerial use of ambiguity to change behavior. Rodwell was never 1Ibid., p. 71. 290 told by the factory manager why he was being transferred. The factory manager expected Rodwell to diagnose his own behavior and to make appropriate changes in it. Rodwell, as we have seen, did this rather than asking the manager why he had been transferred. Similarly, Rodwell's role asSignment as an assistant to the chief scheduler was ambiguous from the start. The factory manager did not tell Rodwell what he was to do and how he was to do it. He did not tell him that he was not to try and make many changes, or that he should not complain about the chief scheduler's behavior. If the factory manager had told Rodwell these things, Rodwell might not have made the mistakes he did. One might say that such action on the part of the manager would have been "better." Such a judgment would imply that the factory manager did not intentionally create an ambiguous situation for Rodwell. He had no rationale for not clearing up the ambiguities Rodwell confronted. But if the ambiguity was an oversight, why did he not tell Rodwell the reason for the transfer. Unfortunately, on the basis of the data gathered in Dill's study, the question cannot be answered. On an 291 intutitive basis the possibility exists that the manager was using ambiguity as a tool for developing Rodwell's "managerial talents." He intended the transfer to be a "test" for Rodwell. But a "test" of what? Apparently, he wanted to see how Rodwell could diagnose his own behavior and make appropriate changes. Perhaps, he also wanted to see if Rodwell could become "self-reliant" in carrying out role assignments. If this was what he intended, his use of ambiguity was effective. Rodwell did diagnose his own behavior and make changes in it. Summary and Analysis pimp Proppgitions Under the freedomrcentered control pattern, behavioral change on the part of subordinates was a built in expectation of the manager. In accordance with the ratchet principle, the manager expected subordinates to continuously make changes in their behavior so that their performance constantly improved. The manager did not explicitly state what behaviors the subordinates were to change to meet this expectation. Regardless, of their level of performance, he told them they had to improve 292 without specifying what should be improved or how they should attempt to improve it. He only examined global response variables, such as sales or cost trends, and pressured them to change their behavior through instru- menting a generalized ambiguity. In short, he did not discuss reasons for inadequate performance and did not help establish programs for improving their performance. The locus of change under the freedom—centered pattern was the subordinates. They were expected to define the appropriate behavior that would result in improvement and to make changes in their behavior that they considered necessary for improvement. Support was offered for proposition D.2 since the manager used an implicit behavioral change model for improving the performance Of subordinates. Support was Offered for proposition D.3 since the manager's implicit behavioral change model was not directly based on knowledge he acquired from the behavioral sciences. It was based on knowledge he gained from experience as a subordinate to other managers. Furthermore, it lacked an objective measurement system and was not derived 293 from a controlled experiment. NO attempt was made to empirically test the proposition that subordinates will generate new ideas if they are left alone. Similarly, no attempt was made to experiment with alternative control patterns. CHAPTER IX ANALYSIS OF THE AUTHORITY- CENTERED CONTROL PATTERN The Manager's Conceptlpn f His Role Under the authority—centered control pattern, the manager conceived his role to be one of determining and controlling the work behavior of subordinates. He determined both their role content and performance techniques and communicated it to them. He, then, observed their performance to make sure they performed properly. He assumed that if he told subordinates what to do and how to do it he would have few problems with them. The manager used authority as a mechanism for changing behavior. He believed that because he was in a higher position in the division than his subordinates, ht had the right and duty to order their behavior. If they failed to perform in terms of his expectations, he believed he simply needed to order them to change their behavior. 294 295 One manager described the process of correcting subordinates deviations from his definition of their role in the following manner: Of course in some cases they don't do things the way you tell them to. So you simply go back and check it and find out what's going on. When you find out you simply tell them to shape up. You can get them started on things and they'll work on it for a month or two and everything will be going fine. But unless you watch it, it will drift back into a problem. This is normal in industry. He elaborated by stating: Most of these things happen if you lose touch with your people and given them too free a hand. In other words, you lose control and they wander Off and if you don't bring them back to conform to standard procedures, then you've got real problems on your hands. These statements indicated that the manager felt it necessary to observe the behavior of subordinates closely to see that they carried out his definition Of their roles. Moreover, the manager seemed to think that changing subordinates was not a problem. He was the boss and, by virtue of his position, had the right to order their behavior. If subordinates balked at his 296 orders, he simply had to demand compliance. As one manager put it: As a general rule, when you talk to a subordinate you request them to do a job. You usually don't have a problem. Now and then they don't agree with the method you want done a d sometimes they get a little ornery agout it. But as a general rule, when you say this is the way I want it done, then they say well your the boss. Extent pf Role Structuring There was a high extent of role structuring in the authority-centered pattern since the manager believed it was an important component of his role. One manager alluded to the importance of role structuring when he stated: You issue instructions to them on some of the things that really have to be done, covering all the fine points. Then you expect that they carry it out. Another manager expressed the importancelhe attached to role structuring this way: Ultimately I'm telling them how I want them to function in the department. If this conflicts with their basic interest they really have no other alternative but to be wrong. You have to find out how the man you work for wants you to work. If your not in agreement or have a basic conflict in this area, the best thing you can do is find a man you can work for. 297 These statements were taken as being indicative of the manager's concept of his role in terms of being a role structurer. He felt it was his duty to define subor- dinates' role content and techniques. It was their duty to accept the role structure the managers designed. Initially the manager had to spend a great deal of time with subordinates telling them what to do and how to do it. After initial role structuring, the manager believed there was less need to interact with subordinates. He felt he needed to occasionaliy observe their performance to insure that it was consistent with his role definition. If it was not, he thought that he had only to order them to change. One manager put it this way: If a man is performing real well, I very seldom give him a work assignment. He knows what he has to do and he does not require a dictorial situation at all. The more he works for you, the more he knows how far he can and should go. If he moves a little too far you have to bring him back. This statement indicated that once the roles of subor— dinates were structured the manager felt interaction with them could be at a minimum. Subordinates knew what activities they were expected to perform and how 298 they were expected to perform them. All he needed was an observational system to spot deviations. Besides conserving time, the authority-centered control pattern had the appearance of precision. As one manager said: Sure their human and make errors. Sometimes you have to tell them something twice and next time you speak a little louder. But I think this is normal. I've had to almost say this is an order to supervisors but I only had to do it once to correct the situation. Impersonality The manager was impersonal in his relationship with subordinates. His control pattern did not account for individual differences, or the feelings and emotions of subordinates. The stress placed on suppressing feelings and emotions was revealed by one manager who said: I recall a case where a young foreman would openly show anger in front of his work force. He would slamebang things around. This cannot be done. You can't display this type of thing. And I had a hell of a time over a period of 2 to 4 years with him. After I initially talked to him you could see he was striving to improve but this habit perhaps was hereditaryh-who knows? I had to bite him in the tail once in awhile when he'd break away but eventually he came around. 299 The manager felt that expression of the emotion Of anger by the foreman was not role relevant. His approach to correcting it was to assert his right of authority and order the foreman to stop it. The fore- man eventually learned to suppress his anger and, hence, changed his behavior in front of his men. It is doubtful, however, if it stopped him from becoming angry. It seems likely that he only learned not to show his anger to other people. The manager emphasized rationality and authority in his relationship with subordinates. One manager displayed this orientation when he said: I think the most important thing is that a subordinate respects his boss. I've worked for many people I didn't like but I did respect their judgement. From a social or personal standpoint we were not compatible. But as long as I can respect a man, I can work for him and do an efficient job. The same thing holds for my people. They don't have to like me but they have to respect me. The statements indicated that the manager thought that "respect" belonged to the world of rationality while "liking" belonged to the world of emotions. A manager, he believed, did not have to be.liked because it is an emotional form Of behavior. Respect, on the other hand, 300 was thought to be a rational form of behavior. According to the manager, a superior attains the "respect" of his subordinate through the exercise of authority and through displaying his intelligence. Indeed, he believed that he had demonstrated intelligence by advancing to a superior position. Since he had greater intelligence, he felt it was only right that he told subordinates what to do. He felt that a manager should not ask subordinates for suggestions because they might feel he does not know anything. He would appear unintelligent and lose their respect. The Handling pf Inadequate Performance The manager believed that correcting inadequate performance required him to point out deficiencies in the behavior of subordinates and threaten them directly, or indirectly, with the exercise Of authority. One manager described the process of handling inadequate performance in the following manner: In most cases you have to sit right down with the individual and point out exactly what is happening and what you think is wrong with him. Sometimes you get down 301 to the very fact that he has good knowledge and does a good job but he doesn't have the push or drive which is needed for that particular job. So you talk to him about it. You tell him he has got to shape up. Generally, they'll improve quite a bit in that area. These statements indicated that the manager used authority as a technique for correcting inadequate performance. Through "talking" to subordinates, he explicated his performance expectations. He, then, threatened to exercise his right to sanction their behavior if compliance did not ensue. In short, he simply pointed out what he considered inadequate performance and ordered subordinates to change. Another manager exemplified this approach when he stated: I almost always in discussions with my people point out their weaknesses rather than their strong points. You may cover them lightly but the main thing you cover is his weaknesses. It's the improvement in the weaknesses which is going to bring him along. It's my responsibility to point them out. The statements were taken as evidence that the manager felt it was part of his role to order the behavior of subordinates. He believed that he had to point out their "weaknesses" so that they would function in terms of his definition of their roles. 302 The manager seemed to believe that it was unnecessary to point out the "strong points" of subordinates because it was their duty to perform to the best of their ability in the role structure he designed. Complimenting them on their "strong points" would have meant that they were not expected to perform with maximum exertion in the role structure provided. It would have suggested that they had done him a personal favor. One manager described the manner in which he handled discussions about inadequate performance with his subordinates in the following terms: This is almost by now a canned speech to them. You've got some weaknesses and if you improve them it's going to help you a great deal. The question then is whether they're willing to discipline themselves to attempt to make this progress or improvement. Sometimes they do and sometimes they don't. Most of the people this approach works on are the eager ones, ambitious, the good producers. While they don't care to be criticized, even objectively, they do accept it for what it's worth and in general they go out and make a sincere effort. You may have to, if it's a glaring weakness, repeat this to them a number of times providing you see progress is.being made. 303 The statements revealed the manager's machine—like approach to control. The use of a "canned speech" levels out individual differences. Furthermore, by stating that his criticisms are "objective," he implies that only "rational" behavior is relevant in their relationship. These two factors emphasize, once again, the impersonal nature of the control pattern. The manager believed his approach was effective with what he called "eager" subordinates. He expanded his notion of an "eager" subordinate by stating: I'm not really interested in anyone who isn't eager. If he desires, he will let you know. He's the one coming in about money, whether he should do this or that, what will help him. The statements suggested that an "eager" subordinate was one whom acknowledged the manager's role as a designer of his activities. By asking the manager what he should do and how Ineshould do it, the subordinate validates the manager's conception of his role. If an "eager" subordinate has promotion aspirations, it would be expected that he would be willing to comply with the manager's definition of his role and suggestions for improving performance. 304 The Changing pf Behavior Through Authority: ‘Ap Illustration One manager described a problem he was having ‘with one of his subordinates in the following manner: We've had a problem with one of our older people not a particularly good one. He was somewhat of a renegade because he'd improvise his own systems and procedures and nobody could work with him because of his personality traits. One of the cardinal sins was he was not keeping his programs up—to—date so engineering changes were not being taken into account. This was costing us money. The worst single thing was you couldn't get anybody to work with him because he wouldn't accept any outside help. This was his area and he wanted nobody else in there. It was primarily a personality problem. He'd been around here a long time and couldn't get along with any of the foreman. He was really a problem child and yet he's been in our organization 20 years. The statements indicated that he believed the problem occurred because the subordinate refused to operate under the role structure provided. The subordinate improvised his own systems and procedures for performing his role. Hence, the manager branded him a "renegade." The manager believed that part of the subordinate's difficulties resulted from his relationship to the previous manager. As he described it: 305 He had been given a lot of freedom, perhaps a little favoritism in a manner of speaking in that he could come in at night when he wanted and felt like it. SO it was not a tight ship he was operating under. When I came in so did law and order. He was fighting law and order and that's why we were having a problem. According to the manager, the previous manager of the subordinate did not prOperly perform the managerial role. He did not properly use his position of authority to structure the subordinate's role. Instead, he provided the subordinate with "a lot of freedom" to structure his role. In short, the previous manager did not run a "tight ship." He allowed emotions to enter the relation- ships in the form of favoritism and failed to structure the subordinate's role. The manager's approach to correcting the performance of the subordinate was to assert his right to order the behavior Of the subordinate. As he destribed it: So we had a couple of meetings which were very straight forward, it was an either/or situation. We went in and spent most of my time on his defects and that these defects had to be corrected or I would have to look for another man for his job. 306 The choice was left to him. 'It's up to you, you make the decision. I told you what our department requires and if you want to conform to it alright.‘ It was an either/or situation. He either had to conform or he was going to be removed. He decided to conform. The statements revealed his use of authority as a threat to attain changes in the behavior of the subordinate. In effect, he told the subordinate to either change his behavior, or leave the division. If the subordinate decided to stay, the manager made it very clear that he was to structure his role and order his behavior. When questioned about how effective the change had been, the manager replied: Some of these things are almost miraculous. The transition of these people happen almost overnight. By 10 O'clock the next morning people came up to his superior and said: "What happened to him?" The change was this obvious and apparent. Certainly, the change was not "miraculous." The subor- dinate had no other choice if he wanted to keep his job. The subordinate's reported new "willingness to let others help him" and to "follow law and order" was a change imposed upon him by the manager. The manager did not consider the emotional reactions of the subordinate to the change he proposed because he 307 considered it irrelevant. Moreover, he did not collaborate with the subordinate to find out his reasons for not performing in the fashion the manager thought he should perform. The manager did not consider the possibility that the subordinate might disguise his behavior and minimize his productive behavior. The subordinate may have decided to "change" his behavior for the moment in order to search for a new job. Another possibility could have been that he had so much time invested in the division that he did not want to lose senority rights and pension benefits. In either case, his compliance with the order to change his behavior would only be apparent at the overt level. The manager did not consider the effects of his directive on the subordinates covert behavior. He was not concerned with whether the subordinate resented the directive. Money 5 n Incentive Money was viewed as the chief work-motive of subordinates. The manager believed that when subordinates contracted to work for the division they exchanged the right of ordering their own behavior for wages. The 308 organization assigned the right to managers in various positions. The manager could use the right to structure the roles of their subordinates. One manager described the nature of this per- ceived contract by stating: When you go to work for a company you get paid for letting them tell you what to do. You agree to let them tell you what to do so long as its moral. One manager stressed the importance of money in the following manner: There are many things important in a job. It's nice to have pleasant surroundings, washrooms, a title, and an office and so forth but the paycheck, this may go against many human relations, is the thing that motivates people more. It will unless situations are completely intolerable and unbearable overcome many sins and most of the people who work for me respond best to this. The statements indicate the importance the manager assigned to money as an incentive. According to this manager the productivity of subordinates varied directly with the rate of their compensation. As he put it: I still think basically man is lazy and very few of us want to do anymore than we have to. I feel that an incentive system of whatever type your talking about is 309 the greatest thing there is. In unions, of course, this is taboo in general. But still your profit motive, I'm talking about take home pay, is what they say made America great and I think this is what inspires and motivates people. Sure they want to advance positionewise and so forth but if they advance position and don't derive some monetary benefits it doesn't amount to much. I realize that your textbooks bring out so many factors and that wages may be the 3rd or 4th or 6th item mentioned. This is a lot of poppy cock. He seemed to believe that subordinates could only be forced out of their "inherent laziness" through compensating them. Even though he was aware of other possible motives he discounted their importance. He believed that people work to earn a living and will only surrender the right to order their own behavior (to be lazy) for compensation of some form. It was not surprising to find that he used salary increases to reward those subordinate who met his expectations. Changing the Behavior pf Pgers Due to the manager's emphasis on the use Of authority as a tool for changing behavior, he felt frustrated in dealing with people in the division over whom he had no formal authority. Since he had no formal 310 authority over them, he felt he could not effectively change their behavior. The only way he thought he could influence their behavior was to appeal to a higher authority. As one manager described this process: I can walk into a meeting with other department heads. They have no control over me and I've none over them so your completely dependent on yourself. If they're not interested in buying any- thing at the moment you don't get the job done. The alternative, if you feel it is a critical matter, is to discuss it with the general manager. He either backs you or does not, but we do have problems in this area. Thus, his solution to the problem of influencing peers was to appeal to a higher authority. The Psychodynamics p§_the Authority- Centered gontrol Pattern None of the managers classified as Operating under the authority-centered pattern perceived themselves as having worked under a manager who used a similar pattern. One of them described his experience in this way: I've never been watched or supervised closely. I prefer this method. I don't want someone to lay out in the morning when I walk in that this is the job 311 you'll do today, or this is what your going to do tomorrow. As far as I can remember no one I've worked for has done this to me. Maybe once in awhile they would call you in and say this has to be done at such and such a date. But that was about it. They never really told me how to do my job. Why, then, did the manager use an authority—centered administrative pattern if he disliked its use with him? One likely reason seemed to be that he believed that he was different in an important way from most people. He felt that he had greater ability and know- ledge and that it was, therefore, only right for him to order their behavior. While he believed most people were inherently lazy, he felt he was an exception. These characteristics were revealed when he said: I think number one you have to be endowed with a certain degree of intelligence. You have to have things given to you over which you have no controlw Once it's been decided whether you've been given this gift then you havthO be willing to use it. There's blood, sweat and tears. You have to be willing to give of yourself. From the statements it can be seen that he thinks "intelli- gence“ is not equally distributed among people. Conse- quently, only a few have the knowledge to direct the behavior of others. 312 This manager also felt that people who have a high degree of intelligence should be willing to use their superior knowledge to direct the behavior of people who have a lower degree of intelligence. Since he assumed "intelligence" was innate, it seemed only natural that he did not concern himself with developing those who have a low degree of intelligence. It is not at all surprising that he did not consider develop- ment of subordinates to be a vital component Of the managerial role. Instead, he felt the most important component was "giving" subordinates his greater knows ledge through structuring their roles for them. The Relationship pf The Authority-Centered Control Pattern pp Existing Theory and Research Allegedly, the authority model has been present in the Americanrculture for sometime. Its survival seems to rest on it simplicity as a technique for controlling and changing behavior and its apparent effectiveness in certain situations. If a manager can specify the exact changes he 'wants in the overt behavior Of his subordinates, and they are willing to accept and implement his orders, the 313 model would hawaalugh probability of being effective. The two possible conditions that would impair the effectiveness are: l. The manager cannot specify the behavior changes he wants in concrete terms. 2. The subordinates of the manager are not willing to accept and implement his orders. The Problem pf Specifying the Behavior £2 be Changed The effectiveness of the authority model depends in part on the manager's ability to specify in concrete terms the behavior he wants subordinates to exhibit. To clarify the meaning of specifying behavior in concrete terms, consider the following illustration. A manager tells a subordinate that he wants him to become more of a "leader" without specifying what he means by the term. He expects the subordinate to move from behavior pattern U in which he is not a "leader" to behavior pattern D in which he is a "leader." Obviously, this is an ambiguous change goal since the manager does not specify the overt behavior he is referring to when he uses the term "leader." Consequently, the subordinate does not know what aspects of his behavior he should change in order to be labelled a "leader." 314 If the manager were to define the term "leader" as giving orders to subordinates rather than asking them to do things, the change goal would become more concrete. He would be telling the subordinate that in behavior pattern U, he is not giving his subordinates direct orders but is asking them to do things. In behavior pattern D, he would expect the subordinate to do the reverse. The subordinate would now have a more explicit basis for making changes in his behavior. Moreover, the manager would have defined the behavior to be changed in observable terms. He could easily see if the subordinate made the appropriate changes. If the subordinate did not, he could evoke his right to sanction the behavior of the subordinate. In sum, the manager who uses the authority model must be careful to specify the behavior he wants changed in observable sets. The managers classified in the study as operating under an authority-centered pattern did this. One manager, for example, said he had a problem with a subordinate who exhibited anger in front of his own subordinates through "slamrbanging things around and cursing." The manager, because this was an 315 emotional outburst, told the subordinate to stop "slams banging things arOund and cursing" in front of his men. The manager assumed this would stop him from "showing anger." A manager's ability to specify the desired changes in behavior in concrete terms is, in part, a function of the nature of the situation in which his subordinates operate. If his subordinates operate under situations that are highly repetitive and where output is easily measured, the ability to specify in concrete terms the behavior desired would be greater than if the reverse were true. In such situations he can determine a priorily what is to be done, and the behavior that will accomplish it. He can, so to speak, determine the "best" behavioral alternative for accomplishing the task since he deals with a "deter— ministic" situation. The managers in the study who were classified as using an authority—centered control pattern were in functional areas where roles were of a highly repetitive nature. When a manager's subordinates hold innovative. roles in which the situation they confront are unique 316 and their output, on both an a_priori and ad hes basis, is difficult to measure it is difficult to specify overt behaviors for subordinates. Under such conditions the situations are not "deterministic" and the manager cannot apply "do-loop" logic to delineate the roles of subordinates. In such situations ambiguity might be used as a control technique. The Problem pf Subordinates' Acceptance p§_Orders The authority model has been attacked by social scientists on two grounds. The first attack is based on a proposition that people in general resent being given orders by incumbents of higher authority positions. Supposedly, this resentment is a function of an equali- tarian norm Operating in the American culture. The norm states that everyone is "equal" and, hence, prohibits the existent of authority differential. Because authority differentials are present in many organizations, it is postulated that participants in low level authority positions will resent the issuing For examples of such attacks see: C. Argyris, Personalipy and Organization (New York: Harper and Bros., 1957); D. McGregor, The Human Side pf Enterprise (New York: McGraw—Hill Co., Inc., 1960), Chapter 2. 317 of orders to change their behavior by participants in 'higher positions. The resentment is hypothesized to result in the lower level participants exerting a Ininimum of effort in carrying out the orders they receive. This line of attack has not resulted in direct evidence that would support the resentment proposition. There have been few attempts to measure "resentment," "performance at a minimum level," and the incidence of an "equalitarian norm." The second attack on the authority model focuses on a generalized "need for participation" by individuals in decisions that affect them.1 When the need is frustrated by the issuing of orders without an opportunity for participating in formulating them, it is hypothesized that the person will resent it. The resentment causes him to exert as little effort as possible in carrying For examples of this attack: R. Likert, N23 Patterns pf Management (New York: McGraweHill, 1961), pp. 1-4: J. G. March and H. A. Simon, Organizations (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1958), p. 95; R. Tannenbaum and F. Massarik, "Participation by Subordinates in the Managerial Decision-Making Process," Canadian Journal pf_Economics and Political Science, XVI (1950), 408-418. 318 out the order. Sometimes the resentment is referred to as "resistance" to change. While there have been empirical studies on participation, the results are inconclusive. Some studies indicate that the lack of participation does not necessarily lead to lower productivity. The conditions under which it does are not clear. It was not within the scope of the present study to determine the effects of the authority-centered control pattern on the behavior of subordinates. Hence, it is not known how the productivity and job satisfaction of subordinates was affected by the pattern. An alternative interpretation Of why subordinates might resent the exercise of authority is that they consider the manager's orders unreasonable, or view him as being incompetent. If they feel the order is unreasonable and they have no respect for the manager issuing it, they may resent the order and do as little as they have to in carrying it out. This assumes that the individual desires to stay in the organization and thinks this would be impossible if he questioned the order itself, or the 319 competence of the manager issuing it. Without this assumption the possibility would exist that he could question such things and, perhaps, overcome his resentment. Why this possibility is not mentioned by those who attack the authority model is unclear. They simply seem to discount such a possibility by assuming the individual will incur some sort of penalty if he does it. Summary and Analysis pf Q Pgoppsitions The manager used authority to change the behavior of subordinates. When trey deviated from his expectations, he specified the behavior they were to change. Support was offered for proposition D.2 since the manager used an implicit behavioral change model for improving the performance of subordinates. Support was Offered for proposition D.3 since the manager's implicit behavioral change model was not directly based on knowledge he acquired from the behavioral sciences. It was based on knowledge he gained from experience 320 as a subordinate to other managers. Furthermore, the change model lacked an Objective measurement system and was not derived from a controlled experiment. CHAPTER X AN ANALYSISCW‘THE DEVELOPMENT- CENTERED CONTROL PATTERN The Manager's Conception pf His Role The manager conceived his role to be one of develOping subordinates. He believed that interaction with them was a key element in their development. As one manager stated: My relationship with my subordinates has been to give them as much freedom as I can by helping them outline a work program, having overall general supervision, and being avail— able as a consultant whenever they wanted to talk. Even in areas where I'm satisfied they know more about the subject than I do, I still like them to come to me for discussion. I think they can clarify their thinking by talking about it. He believed that to carry out his role he had to serve as a source of development by acting as a consultant. Another manager revealed the importance he assigned to development in the following passage: Some managers surround themselves with peOple who make them look good. The thought being if this guy down here is any good he might end up getting my job so to avoid that I'm going to hire some safe people. 321 322 This is a ridiculous thing. If a man's ambitious, as most of us are, and surrounds himself with sub-par people, the company can't afford to let him leave that job because they have nobody to put in it, except sub-par people. They probably won't do this so they'll let him set. If he surrounds himself with strong peOple, just their strength make him stronger and his performance better. And when he moves up, they'll move up with him. To this manager the measure of role success was how well he developed subordinates. Promotion was a function of developing subordinates. Extent pf Role Structuring Similar to the manager in the freedom-centered pattern, the managers stressed freedom as an important component of his control pattern. However, he utilized a bounded freedom. Freedom was bounded in the sense that he participated in helping subordinates structure their role content and performance techniques. In contrast to the manager in the authority—centered pattern, he encouraged his subordinates to participate in the role structuring process. He did not define their roles himself. 323 One manager described the role structuring process in the following manner: I don't like to give a man a job and run right out and tell him how to do it. Telling him, 'this is step one and when you get through with it this is step two. I give our peOple a lot of freedom in doing the task they're assigned. I don't box them in so they have a chance to express themselves in their job. If I have a particular task I want a fellow to do, I'll have him come in and explain what I want him to do and when I want him to do it. I always ask for his suggestions how we ought to go about it. These statements were taken as an indicator of a medium extent of role structuring. The extent of role structur— ing by the manager was greater than it was under the freedomrcentered pattern but less than it was under the authority-centered pattern. Another example of a medium extent of role structuring was furnished by another manager who stated: The type of people I deal with must be individuals and must be independent, originating types of thinkers or they're of no value. We need creative peOple. Creative people are bound to be independent, they want to do things the way they think they should be done. Frequently its a balance between having them follow specific directions and not liking it, or going along with his 324 way of doing it and perhaps following up on it if it doesn't work with what I think should be done. Sometimes we do it both ways simultaneously. Here's an example. (Points to a drawing for a new part) I thought it might well be done one way, and the product engineer thought another. He drew it up both ways and we are going to try both of them. The statements indicated that the manager attempted to provide subordinates with some structuring but allowed them role options. Impersonality Impersonality was low in the sense fln¢.expression of feelings and emotions by subordinates was viewed as a legitimate form of interchange. As one manager put it: I think it is important in a relation— ship between a manager and his people that they feel free to come in and discuss their particular problems and feelings about other departments, relationships, salaries and even home affairs. The rationale he offered for his View was that these factors influenced the performance of subordinates. Therefore, they had to be dealt with in developing them. Although the pattern was universal in application, individual differences were taken into account because 325 development was considered an individual process. Sub- ordinates were considered to be unique in their deficiencies. Consequently the manager believed he had to hold individual discussions with them. The Handling 9: Inadequate Performance The manager appraised the performance of sub— ordinates on the basis of both technical and non-technical criteria. An example of non-technical criteria was provided by a manager who stated: I think more of a feeling of the attitude of the individual gives me a clue as to whether he is producing to the best of his ability. If the fellow is interested in his job I find that he will come to me for discussions, will attempt to outline his own programs ahead of what is scheduled for him to do, and will bring to my attention some of the competitOrs'products which have features he thinks are better than ours. He will tend to put in some extra time, evenings or Saturdays. You get a feeling of just how much he's producing within his capacity limitations, by his attitude toward his work more than anything else. This manager seemed to use predominately non-technical criteria for determining the adequacy of the performance of subordinates. One of the criteria he used was their expressed desire to gain new knowledge through going 326 to the manager for consultation on their problems. Such action signified their acceptance of his role as being a developmental one. Similarly, if they finished their role assignments early and sought out the chance to learn something new, they demonstrated interest in being developed. The subordinate was expected to reciprocate in kind for knowledge gained from the manager. One example was the subordinate providing the manager with knowledge about competitor's products. The sharing of knowledge expectation followed from the manager's View that development was a two-way process in which he and subordinates exchanged knowledge and, in doing so, grew together. He believed that the exchange of knowledge gave subordinates experience in playing the role of develOper. Thus, when they moved into a managerial role they supposedly would be able to play it properly. It was not surprising that this manager stressed developing a better "attitude" toward work in attempting to help subordinates improve performance. As he put it: 327 Of course there's a time for a discussion as to what's expected from him and what he's actually doing. I simply try to create a better attitude toward his work. I try to find out if there is anything about his particular assignment he doesn't like, whether he'd prefer work in another area, or work with someone else. We just,have to find out what the basic problem is and then, maybe together we can find a solution to it. The statements indicated that the manager collaborated with subordinates on the reasons for inadequate performance. He saw improving performance as a two—way process in which he and subordinates searched together for the problems and solutions to it. The manager viewed inadequate performance as a result of deficiencies in knowledge or dissatisfaction with the work role. If he believed the determinant was dissatisfaction with the work role, he attempted to change it. If he believed the determinant was inadequate knowledge, he attempted to provide the necessary knows ledge. He assumed that given new knowledge the subordinates would use it so that their performance would improve. One manager described the process of providing new knowledge in the following manner: 328 You might run into a problem where a man isn't doing what you would like him to do with a specialty range of products. A man has a hesitancy to try to sell a segment of the line he doesn't understand very well. The statement clearly indicated the critical role the manager assigned to a deficiency in knowledge as the cause of inadequate performance. ‘O$.lh,w.nl'._l : *0 - I.» To improve performance the manager stressed increasing the subordinate's knowledge. As he put it: It takes a little doing sometimes to get him receptive to the point where you can train him in this area. The only way I know to go about this is to sell him on the idea that this is something that can be beneficial to him as well as the company, as though he were a customer. a ". . It's awfully difficult to order a man to learn something. You have to stimulate or motivate him to do this. This manager felt subordinates had to be sold on using the knowledge he provided them. Selling, in the sense he used the word, was providing subordinates with know- ledge and showing them why they should use it. He assumed that subordinates had to understand what they could gain from its use before they would change their behavior in the desired direction. 329 The manager elaborated on his sales approach by saying: You've got to remember I'm sales oriented. I figure the job of a salesman is to get. other peOple to do what it is you want them to do, whether its buy a pnOduct or pay for it. As far as I'm concerned this is no different than selling a product. Your selling an idea, its an intangible instead of a piece of machinery but I think if you approach almost anyone properly and explain why things should be done you can do it. You explain why it needs to be done for the betterment of the company and why it needs to be done for the betterment of the particular individual. If what your selling is a sound product, I think you can get the job done. He went on to say: This is the approach you have to use with people. Call it selling or public relations or whatever you want to but its trying to get someone to seriously consider what your talking about and, then, present it in such a way that when he considers it, he thinks it's a good idea. It's got be not just from thexcompany's standpoint but in some measure from his standpoint. 'The company will prosper, get bigger, he'll be recognized as an integral part of the accomplishment.‘ The statements clearly revealed the emphasis the manager gave to increasing subordinates' knowledge through Selling them on what its implementation will do for ‘them. His concern for their motives in changing their 330 behavior was very apparent. The manager actively participated in helping his subordinates improve their inadequate performance. He helped them develop specific ways of changing their behavior rather than ordering them to change, or pressuring them with ambiguity into changing it. One manager described this process of participation in the following manner: I think everybody from year to year have one or two salesman who might be turning in unsatisfactory performance. The thing we do when this occurs, one of us in the office either myself or Mr. X (in the past I've handled it but Mr. X has progressed to the point where I think he can handle it as well as I can) plans two or three selling trips with this man. We go with him when he calls on his customers, knocks on doors, and cold canvassing to his actual meetings to observe him in his operations to see what he's doing which might adversely affect his sales performance. The manager attempted to diagnose the role performance difficulties of subordinates by observing them while they were actually engaged in role behavior. Apparently, he felt that each situation was unique and, therefore, each subordinate's behavior had to be observed individually ‘to find the reason for inadequate performance. 331 After the diagnosis, the manager attempted to aid subordinates in developing new techniques for performing their roles. As this manager described it: Then, when we do find it, we try to point out to him what's happening and sit down with him and work out with him, perhaps, a new and more interesting presentation. Thus, he actively participated in helping subordinates improve performance. Such action on his part was congruent with the view that development was a two—way process and that inadequate performance occurred because of deficiencies in knowledge. Coupled with participation in increasing the knowledge of subordinates was the offering Of emotional support when they were uncertain that they could apply their knowledge effectively. One manager described the process of emotional support as follows: When my people get a little bit shakey and call me and want to know if there's any particular method they should use, I generally try to get them calmed down by saying; 'Don't forget you've been in this business for six years. Now even if you. were at it one day, you'd know a little bit more about it than these people do. They don't know anything about it so it's up to you to tell them about it. Don't get shakey about it because everything you 332 say is information they need and want and will have to use in their evaluation Of the requirements.‘ This generally gets them calmed down. It is interesting to note that the manager casted the role of salesman as imparting knowledge to customers. A salesman, so to speak, was to develop customers. Changing Performance Criteria é§.A Mechanism for Changing Behavior The manager sometime used changes in performance criteria as a basis for changing the role behavior of subordinates. An illustration of this was provided by a manager who stated: He (a previous boss) had only one criticism and this I thought extremely unusual because I have lots more criticism of my people. He didn't think I got out enough and contacted people who were active in our sales area. In other words, our dealers, customers, research people in universities, and that type of thing. Well, as a result of his criticism, I joined several organizations and became an Officer in many of them. I make some very close contacts with a number of research people. I felt it helped a lot. I enjoyed it andwanted to do it Before but didn't think I should spend that much time. When the supervisor criticized his role behavior, the subordinate responded by changing his role behavior. 333 He thought the superior was trying to help him develop. Later the subordinate said: Well, it wasn't long after that, that I was, indirectly at least, criticized for spending too much time. I was questioned about whether these activities were paying Off; whether we got enough out Of the meetings to warrant attending them. SO I curtailed it. The superior changed the criteria and the subordinate corresponding changed his behavior. He simply curtailed the activities that the superior now evaluated negatively. Changing the Behavior 9; Peers When the manager was faced with a problem that required changing the behavior of peers, he stressed "selling" them on the need for change. Like the manager in the authority-centered pattern, he saw the alternative of having a mutual superior intervene. As one manager described it: Now if your dealing with someone whose on the same level of responsibility and authority and you can't sell him, then you've got some real problems. Then you have three alternatives: (1) forget the idea, (2) continue to try to sell him on the idea, and (3) ultimately discuss it with people who are the superior of both of us and let them decide whether its worth doing. 334 His first reaction to changing the behavior of peers is to try and "sell" them on his idea. He is, so to speak, to help them increase their knowledge. Only as a last resort does he believe the issue should be resolved by a person in a higher position. Recognition §§.§fl Incentive The manager assumed subordinates had an intrinsic desire to develop and were motivated to do so by recogni— tion. As one manager put it: The biggest thing is recognition of the work they've done which is a thing which is too frequently overlooked. We're inclined to expect them to produce and when they do we take it as a matter of course without any compliments in their direction, or a pat on the back, or public recognition of any kind. I think this causes them to fall into more routine activities then if they're rewarded, not necessarily through salary increases although that is one way. I think, however, recognition on the personal basis is just as important as salary increases, if not more so. It encourages them to do more and better work. Recognition was not the important motivating factor. Its function was to provide subordinates with feedback on how well they were developing. The desire for development seemed to be the most important motivating factor. 1' lat l 335 Money was not considered a prime incentive of subordinates. As one manager said: I suppose most peOple would tell you money is the reason why people work hard. I don't feel that's true. Money is a necessity, of course, you've got to live but I don't think you can motivate people as such with money. I think most people just enjoy doing a good job even when someone else doesn't know about it. SO I think you've got something going for you right there. This manager discounted the importance of money as an incentive and stressed an "inner motive" for wanting to do a good job. He elaborated on this "inner motive” by stating: Almost everybody enjoys doing a good job if he's recognized for it. I think recognition does a pretty good job of motivating people. Ambition is certainly a strong motivating force. If he does a good job and is recognized for it, you give him a little more to do, a little more responsibility and give him the right to do it. Hence, he favored recognition, Opportunities for develop- ment and special assignment as incentives. The manager attempted to establish an environment in which subordinates could gain a personal sense of accomplishment through controlling the results of their role performance. As one manager described it: It :i- ‘ 336 One of the ways we have been motivating our people is to truly make our fellows managers of their respective territories. We don't make any decisions regarding their territories without their recommendations and advice. We'll even let them make a mistake if we think they're genuine and sincere in their efforts. We don't like them to make any bad ones but sometimes a man can learn more rapidly by doing what he thinks he has to do and stubing his toe can wake him up into doing it right. They fee1~ they are a part of a decision making team and not just having a bunch of edits handed down to them. This manager attempted to motivate subordinates through providing them with an opportunity for development. He felt that they could often acquire new knowledge through making mistakes. Psychodynamics pf the Development- Centered Pattern Some of the managers who were classified as using the development—centered control pattern had previously worked under what they called "autocratic" managers. Without exception they found working under such managers a dissatisfying experience. They felt that the managers did not allow them to develOp. The "autocratic" managers,so to speak, over—determined their roles and they found it dissatisfying. 337 The development-centered managers were sometimes aware that they were modelling their pattern after control patterns they found satisfying in the past. In discussing how he would try to get a subordinate to do a task he did not like, one manager said: The only way anyone can do a job that's “R. disagreeable to them is through full under— standing of why it needs to be done. As I think back this approach was always used with me and is probably why I use it with my subordinates. If a man understands the need for some- thing being done, you usually don't have to push him to get it done. It's when he either doesn't understand the need, or doesn't believe it that he drags his feet. Most of the managers had, also worked for managers who used a development—centered control pattern. They found such a control pattern satisfying. One manager described his experience with it in the follow- ing manner: The second manager I worked under Operated more in the manner I personally like to operate in. He was nowhere near the authori- tarian the first man was. He had just as much authority and everybody recognized it but he didn't wear it on his sleeve all the time. This man got you to do the things he wanted you to do through discussion, guidance, and suggestions. He'd say: 'would you like to handle this for me?‘ And maybe he'd give a 338 suggestion on how he'd think you might want to handle it. This was a reverse of the first one. It was a very pleasant experience. It is not difficult to see why this manager simulated the development—centered pattern. He found it, by experience to be satisfying. Another manager described his experience along similar lines. He said: The fact he generally allowed me to make my own decisions was one reason I liked working for him. When he questioned why I had done something, I'd explain in detail why I'd done it that way, and I usually got his support. At that time he'd advise you on experiences he'd run across and you hadn't yet experienced which indicated he was trying to develop you more. He continuously gave me more responsibility than I had previously. No matter how much background you've got there's always an awful lot you can learn otherwise. Often times he would take time out to explain in detail some of the things which certainly ungraded your knowledge of things and of the procedures he desired. Thus, the experience he found most satisfying was one in which the manager concentrated on expanding the know— ledge Of subordinates. 339 The Relationship pf the Development- Centered Pattern pg Existing Theory and Research The development-centered control pattern appears to be consistent with performance appraisal theories directed at developing subordinates. Maier, for example, uses a problem-solving appraisal interview as a prototype of excellence.1 In the problem-solving interview, the manager views his role as one of stimu- lating growth and development of subordinates. He assumes that discussing work problems will lead to improved performance and that increased responsibility will serve as an incentive to grow. Moreover, he believes that both the subordinates and manager can learn and develop from sharing their experience and views. The process of handling inadequate performance in the development-centered control pattern is similar to using what Leavitt calls a "collaborative model."2 The collaborative model consists of the following interchanges between the manager (M) and subordinate (S): 1N. R. F. Maier, "Three Types of Appraisal Inter- view," Personnel (March-April, 1958), pp. 27—40 and The Apggaisal Interview (New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1958). 2H. J. Leavitt, Managerial Psychology (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1964), second edition, pp. 189-205. 340 1. Both (M) and (S) diagnose the inadequate performance of (S). 2. Both (M) and (S) suggest alternative ways (S) can improve his performance. 3. Both (M) and (S) communicate the implication of alternatives to each other. 4. (S) selects alternative (M) can accept. 5. (S) attempts to change and (M) supports. Leavitt claims that in this model (S) controls the changes in his behavior. When the collaborative model is generalized to the develOpment—centered pattern it rests on an implicit authority threat. The manager maintains his right to order the behavior of subordinates. TO help subordinates develop, he may refrain from completely exercising the right, but the right to order their behavior is always present. Thus, it is possible that Changes in the behavior of subordinates are due to an implicit authority threat rather than his development-centered control pattern. ~ ‘l.r-.!l. I}. 341 Summary and Analysis gfug Propositions The manager attempted to correct the inadequate performance of subordinates by uncovering deficiencies in knowledge, offering a rationale for change, giving an opportunity to counter the rationale, and helping develop new behaviors. His first step in determining the need for change was to establish performance standards, such as increases or decreases in sales volume. When a need for change was indicated the manager looked for reasons actual performance was below expected performance. He examined the subordinate's behavior pattern to find out what he was doing "wrong." When he uncovered what he thought was the "cause," he delineated waysfor correcting the behavior. The manager's next step was to give the subordinate a reason for the change and his suggestions for the change. In the process, he allowed the subordinate to question the need for change and the proposed changes. After they arrived at the types of changes the subordinate was to make, the manager helped the subordinate to develop and implement the new behaviors. When the subordinate attempted changes, the manager encouraged him and the for was 342 and gave him support when he encountered difficulties recognition when he succeeded. Support was Offered for proposition D.2 since manager used an implicit behavioral change model improving the performance of subordinates. Support offered for proposition D.3 since the manager's implicit behavioral change model was not directly based on knowledge he acquired from the behavioral sciences. It was based on knowledge he gained from experience as a subordinate to other managers. Furthermore, the model lacked an Objective measurement system and was not derived from a controlled experiment. CHAPTER XI SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS A Criterion g: Usefulness One cannot rule out on an g_priori basis any of the areas of the behavioral sciences as potentially capable of contributing to management theory and practice. The usefulness criterion must be applied to all areas to determine their contribution. The A premises established a criterion for determining if knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences is useful to managers. Premise A.2 stated that such knowledge is useful if it enables managers to solve their behavioral problems. Premise A.3 stated that such knowledge can lead to more efficient solutions if it constitutes control knowledge. Premise A.2 led to an examination of the behavioral problems faced by managers and the behavioral problems dealt with in the behavioral sciences. Premise A.3 led to an examination of the control knowledge possessed by managers and control knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences. 343 344 The Conversion Framework In Chapter III it was demonstrated that both applied social scientists and managers experience a conversion problem in trying to apply existing knowledge from the basic disciplines of the behavioral sciences. The problem exists because not all existing knowledge in the basic disciplines has been accumulated to solve behavioral problems. It has not been intended to pro— vide control knowledge. Moreover, in accordance with proposition B.l, neither management theory and practice nor the behavioral sciences hawaidentified the problems faced by managers in converting knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences into a tool they can use for changing behavior. An analysis of Berelson's and Steiner's findings on human behavior revealed that, in accordance with proposition 3.2, not all knowledge in the behavioral sciences can be used in its present form to solve specific behavioral problems. To solve specific behavioral problems it must be control knowledge. Control knowledge permits a change agent to manipulate conditions determining a phenomenon in order to achieve a desired end. 345 The finding pertaining to leadership styles was cited as an example of knowledge that could not be used to solve a specific behavioral problem. It did not specify a desired end or the determinants of the phenomena "leadership styles." Hence, by definition 2 it did not constitute control knowledge. {11‘ The construct of a behavioral change model I coupled with the operationalizing process wasznopoax1as a conversion framework. Proposition B.3 stated that the iii framework permits knowledge in the behavioral sciences to be appraised in terms of control knowledge. Behavior pattern U identifies a behavioral problem and behavior pattern D describesea"desired end." The transition- producing mechanism describes how the "conditions determining a phenomenon" can be manipulated to achieve the "desired end." Proposition B.4 stated that a behavioral change model built through use Of the conversion framework constitutes control knowledge if it contains variables that can be manipulated by a change agent, possesses an objective system for measuring the values of variables and states empirically valid relationships between the variables. 346 The analysis Of the relationship between the behavioral sciences and management theory and practice presented in Chapters II and III suggested that a gap currently exists between those who develop behavioral knowledge and those who try to make use of it for solving specific problems. Many social scientists in the basic disciplines of the behavioral sciences have not been concerned with the practical use Of their research by applied social scientists and managers. Commenting on the gap, Leeds and Smith suggest that: Some social scientists feel that they are not responsible for determining the practical uses of their research. Other social scientists feel that they do not know how to implement their findings; they lack the skills needed to translate research findings into action programs. Very little training in social engineering is given to social scientists, and only a few have had practical experience in ;implementing their knowledge.1 Apparently, some social scientists, particularly those in the basic disciplines, have not been interested in the application of their research to change behavior. .Moreover, their training and role conception have not lR. Leeds and T. Smith, Using Social Science Knowledge ip Business and Industry (Homewood, Ill.: Irwin, 1963), p. 122. In ’JIL“ ‘ . _ I 1 347 prepared them to translate research findings into action programs. While they have been trained to describe and predict behavior they have not been trained in how to control it. They have been conditioned to be descriptive rather than prescriptive. Contrariwise managers and applied social scientists have been conditioned to be prescriptive as well as descriptive and operate in a world where they serve as agents of change. They are expected to change behavior and control its direction. Research findings from the basic disciplines of the behavioral sciences are considered useful if they help them perform their change agent role more efficiently and effectively. The analysis presented in chapter III suggested that they experience a conversion problem in trying to use such findings because the researcher, in many cases, does not explore how the findings can be used to achieve behavioral change. Stated differ- ently, the researcher does not attempt to translate the findings into action programs for changing behavior. Thus change agents who want to make use of 348 the findings for changing behavior must convert them into action programs. According to Leeds and Smith, the chasm between the developers and users of behavioral knowledge has resulted in a proposal for a new Speciality that does applied research. As they state it: Some social scientists argue that the person who is talented in analyzing situations and conducting research may not have the talent to implement findings or translate them into an action program. One solution which has been suggested is to have a Specially trained person, a third party, who takes the research findings of the social scientist and ascertains how they can be utilized by the manager in the organization.1 The necessity for a middleman between the developers and users of behavioral knowledge is a moot issue. The important factor is that a conversion function must be performed. The conversion framework presented in chapter III was designed to facilitate research on the conversion function and to serve as a model of the conversion process. lLoc. cit. 349 Examination 9; Existipg_Know1edge ig the Behavioral Sciences ‘ The conversion framework developed in chapter III was applied to a selected body of knowledge in the applied areas of the behavioral sciences: psycho- therapy, group dynamics and need-satisfaction psychology. In terms of proposition B.4, presently none of the models analyzed in chapters IV, V, and VI constituted control knowledge. They lacked either objective measurement systems, empirically validated relationships between variables, statements indicating how to manipulate the value of transition-producing variables, or some combination of these factors. Thus in terms of prOposition C.1 the models did not presently meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. In terms of proposition C.2, the models attempted to change behavior by changing the values of individual variables, environmental variables, or both. While presently the behavioral change models examined in chaptersIV, V and VI do not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral 350 change model it is possible that in the future they could be transformed into useful models. The analysis of the models demonstrated that in terms of proposition B.4 they did not yet meet the necessary and sufficient conditions. However, it is possible that in the future they could be made to conform with these conditions. Hence it7can not be concluded that the models are not actually useful. It can only be concluded that at the present time they do not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. In the future,attempts must be made to determine if models derived from the behavioral sciences can be translated into control knowledge if, in their present form, they do not constitute control knowledge. Stated differently, if in their present form they do not meet the necessary and sufficient conditions for a useful behavioral change model can they be made to conform to these conditions in the future? Can objective measurement systems be established for variables used to describe behavior patternsU and D as well as the transition-producing mechanism? Can the relationships 351 between the behavior pattern variables and the transition— producing variables be empirically validated? Can statements be developed that indicate how a change agent can manipulate the values of transition-producing variables? In accordance with proposition B.4, if the answer to the questions is affirmative the models would be useful since they would constitute control knowledge. Acquiring answers to the questions is an experimental issue that must be resolved in the future. Another area for future research consists of applying the conversion framework to other literature of the behavioral sciences. The universe of literature examined in the present study represented only a fragment of behavioral science knowledge. It is possible that presently there exists behavioral knowledge that meets the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. Only through applying the conversion framework to other literature in the behavioral sciences can this possibility be explored. The general applicability of the models examined in Chapters IV, V and VI, as well as other models, to 352 behavioral problems of managers represents another area for research. In short, can behavioral change models that are not explicitly designed for use by managers be made useful to them through use of the conversion framework? To answer this question a systematic study of the range of behavioral problems dealt with in the behavioral science literature as well as those dealt with by managers would have to be conducted. Such a study would permit a conceptual comparison of behavioral problems dealt with in the behavioral sciences with those confronted by managers. It would make it possible to determine if they share in common a particular range of behavioral problems. A twofold problem exists in trying to compare behavioral problems dealt with in the behavioral sciences with those managers experience in carrying out their organizational role. First, some of the behavioral problems described in the behavioral science literature have Specific reference to industrial settings (e.g..Likert's Supervisory Model), while others do not (e.g.,Ellis' Rational-Emotive Therapy). 353 While seemingly irrelevant, the range of behavioral problems described in a non—industrial setting cannot be discounted on an g_priori basis. Conceptually they may be similar to some of the behavioral problems confronted by managers. If some of the problems are conceptually similar, models built by social scientists to resolve~the shared range of problems may prove on further analysis to be useful to managers. For example, if a set of irrational beliefs influences the adequacy of a man's performance, use of the rational-emotive model, assuming it was made Operational, might improve performance. Thus while the rational-emotive model was not develOped in an industrial setting and was not designed to help managers with their behavioral problems it may prove to be useful to managers in coping with some of their behavioral problems. The second problem in conceptually comparing ranges of behavioral problems, centers on develOping a definition of a behavioral problem. In terms of the conversion framework, behavior pattern U is a behavioral problem in the sense that the change agent wants to move the object of change away from it. However, the 354 description of the pattern does not indicate why the change agent considers the behavior pattern a problem or undesirable. It Simply describes the behavior pattern he considers a problem and attempts to change. Sometimes in the behavioral science literature a range of behavioral problems thought to exist in an industrial setting are simply described. No prescriptive mechanism is proposed to resolve the problems. The social scientist describes the incidence of problems (e.g. high alienation) without indicating how to ameliorate them. In such cases, further research is necessary to determine how the values of the variables describing the problems can be changed (e.g. from high alienation to low alienation). In effect, transition- producing mechanisms for resolving the problems have to be constructed and tested. Sometimes the description of a behavioral problem by a social scientist represents an invention on his part and is not perceived as a problem by managers. For example, in one of the Hawthorne experiments the researchers labelled a range of employee behavior: 355 restriction of output.1 Restriction of output meant that the employees worked at a lower output level than they were actually capable of achieving. At the time of the study the management of the company, considered the employees output to be adequate. The issue, then, is did restriction of output constitute a behavioral EHIAI; problem for managers? If it is assumed the managers were informed i on restriction and came to consider it a problem, the E? problem of how to change the behavior emerges. The Hawthonxaexperiments, however, did not provide a transition-producing mechanism for dealing with restriction of output. Experiments on how to change this behavior were not conducted. Thus, in this instance, even though managers may have been made aware of a problem the social scientists did not provide a mechanism for resolving it. It is possible then that the behavioral sciences may share a range of behavioral problems with management theory and practice but not possess mechanisms for resolving the Problems. 1F. J. Roethlisberger and W. J. Dickson, Management and the Worker (Science Editions; New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1964), pp. 531-537. 356 Empirical Determination 9; Control Knowledge Possessed pthanagers PrOposition D.l stated that to determine the useful of knowledge contained in the behavioral sciences to managers the behavioral problems they face and how they cope with them must be determined. An exploratory study was designed to identify and describe behavioral change models used by ten managers to handle inadequate performance. In terms of the conversion framework, inadequate performance and improved performance constituted descriptions of behavior patterns U and D. The managers' attempt to correct inadequate performance constituted the transition- producing mechanism. In accordance with proposition D.2, the study revealed that the ten managers implicitly used behavioral change models in handling inadequate performance. They used three types of transition-producing mechanisms in correcting inadequate performance of subordinates. The transition-producing mechanisms they used stemmed from the control patterns they implemented and represented attempts to correct deviations from them. '1 357 In accordance with proposition B.4, the transition-producing mechanisms used by the ten managers did not presently meet the necessary and sufficient conditions of a useful behavioral change model. The managers made no attempt to systematically measure F‘ variables they manipulated to improve performance or ‘1 verify propositions on which manipulation was based. I3. I All“ The general manager, for example, did not empirically IIP‘ " . y e .2 test the prOposition that an ambiguous directive will result in improvements. He did not attempt to measure its psychological effects on subordinates or discover the conditions under which it might be effective. The range of alternatives the managers considered in devising mechanisms for handling inadequate perform- ance-was small. They did not experiment with different alternatives to determine if the particular model they used was the most efficient and effective alternative for improving inadequate performance. From the data gathered it was not possible to state if they could increaSe the efficiency and effectiveness of improving inadequate performance by using knowledge from the behavioral sciences. 358 In accordance with proposition D.3, the control patterns used by the ten managers were not based on knowledge directly acquired from the behavior sciences. The control patterns were acquired from their experience as subordinates to other managers. The major reason they did not use knowledge from the behavioral yfitq sciences was that they were not exposed to it. However, theqppssibility exists that even though they did not directly acquire their control patterns from the behavioral sciences the patterns could be identical by coincidence. In terms of the behavioral change models examined in chapters IV, V and VI, the managers' models appeared to be conceptually different. There are a number of areas for future research on behavioral change models used by managers. One area is determining if there are other types of control patterns. It is likely that the present study did not exhaust all of them. In connection with such an extended analysis an effort could be made to determine under what conditions a homogeneous pattern prevails in an organi- zation. For example, if the general manager had used an 359 authority-centered pattern would his subordinates have used other patterns? Another area of research is assessing the impact of different control patterns on subordinates. Are the psychological effects of the pattern different? What is the effect of each pattern on productivity? Are there conditions under which one pattern is more effective than others and vice-versa? A systematic study of the range of behavioral problems faced by managers is another area for research. What types of behavioral problems do they have with subordinates, superiors, peers and themselves?‘ The present study was restricted to problems of inadequate performance. Finding managers who have been exposed to some of the findings of the behavioral sciences and deter- mining how the exposure has affected their role performance is another area for research. Have they tried to use the findings? If so, how have they tried to use them? What changes in their practices have followed from the exposure? Do they perceive a conversion problem? If so, how have they reacted tO the problem? 360 A Conceptual Comparsion gf_the Bphavioral Change Models Useg_py_the Ten Managers with Those Examined ip_Chapters .131. Yasdfl The models examined in chaptersIV, V, and VI were not explicitly designed to help managers handle problems of inadequate performance. However, it is possible to conceptually compare them with the models used by the ten managers. In short, were the models used by the ten managers for handling problems of inadequate performance conceptually similar to the psychotherapeutic, environmentalistic and need- satisfaction models examined? - w-“ Psychological Determinants g§_ Inadequate Performance The managers classified as using freedom- centered or authority-centered control patterns appeared to rule out psychological determinants of inadequate performance by usingimpersonality as a basis for relating to subordinates. Impersonality resulted in their not taking individuals differences into account and considering "emotional behavior" to be administratively irrelevant. Hence on an g_priori 361 basis they excluded psychological problems of subordinates as potential determinants of inadequate performance. For example, they did not consider the possibility that inadequate performance may be influenced by a negative emotional state based on a set or irrational beliefs, or by the frustration of certain needs. ) Environmental Determinants of : Inadequate Performance : The managers in the study appeared to rule out get the possibility that inadequate performance may be influenced by environmental factors. For example, they did not consider the possibility that their own behavior may influence the performance of their subordinates. In fact, they did not attempt to discover systematically or precisely what behavior their subordinates expected from them. A Summary Comparison gf_Determinants g§_Inadeguate Perfopgance Generalizing the models analyzed in chapters IV, V and VI to the problem of inadequate performance there are many possible determinants of inadequate performance. Figure 11.1 indicates the range of possible FIGURE 11.1 362 A COMPARISON OF POSSIBLE DETERMINANTS OF INADEQUATE PERFORMANCE Determinants from the Behavioral Sciences 1. Repressed sexual conflict 2. Irrational beliefs 3. InapprOprate conditioning 4. Low need for achievement 5. Low interpersonal competence 6. Lack of group decision 7. InapprOpriate supervisory behavior 8. Lack of Opportunity for discussing problems Determinants used by the Ten Managers 1. Laziness 2. Low intelligence 3. Low wages 4. Lack of self- motivation 5. Inadequate knowledge 6. Dissatisfaction with role activities 363 determinants. The figure also indicates the determinants attributed to inadequate performance by the ten managers. From the standpoint of improving actual role performance it is important to uncover the actual determinants of inadequate performance. The determinants inferred by a manager dictates the type Of transition- producing mechanism he uses for improving performance. For example, in the authority-centered pattern the manager attributed inadequate performance to either laziness, or low intelligence, or low wages. If he believed the determinant was laziness, he ordered the subordinate to change and threaten him to ensure compliance. If he believed the determinant was low intelligence, he made his ordering of the subordinate's behavior more explicit. If he believed the determinant was low compensation, he raised the compensation. In the development-centered pattern the manager attributed inadequate performance to deficiencies in knowledge or dissatisfaction with work activities. If he believed the determinant was deficiencies in knowledge, he attempted to improve performance through correcting the deficiencies. If he believed the determinant was 364 dissatisfaction with work activities, he tried to make them more satisfying. In the freedom-centered control pattern the manager attributed inadequate performance to the lack of self-motivation. He attempted to improve performance through ambiguously pressuring subordinates to change their behavior. If the managers had used the models examined in chapters IV, V and VI, they would have broadened the sc0pe of determinants they assigned to inadequate performance and the techniques they used to improve it. For example, if a set of irrational beliefs contributed to a subordinate's inadequate performance, they may have used the rational-emotive model to improve it. The important question is if the determinants of behavior pattern U described in the models can be generalized to inadequate performance. If they can be generalized, under what conditions would the use of the various models by the managers increase the performance of subordinates? These two questions reveal an area for additional research. _' I’JM . .h" 365 Determining the Conceptual Similarities g; Behavioral Change Models Determining the conceptual similarities of behavioral change models is an area for future research. Is it possible for behavioral change models to be conceptually similar even though the variables contained in them are labelled differently and the range of behavioral problems they are designed to deal with are different? If some of the models are conceptually similar it would be possible to reduce the number of models. The process of conceptually comparing can be illustrated by examining the psychotherapeutic models analyzed in chapter IV. The three models analyzed shared in common a general range of behavioral problems; a patient's reported unhappiness and anxiety. However, each model assigned different determinants to these problems which led to the use of different transition- producing mechanisms for resolving them. All three model builders claimed their model was effective in dissipating the shared problems. Given the apparent differences can they all be right in claiming effectiveness for their model? 366 At a broad conceptual level the models appear similar. In each model the patient is taught the therapist's theory of why he is "sick" and how he can be cured. When he accepts the theory and implements it he is considered "cure." Conceptually, the transition-producing mechanism seems to consist of providing a patient with an authoritative interpretation of his symptoms and convincing him that he will be cured if he accepts and implements the interpretation. In this reSpect the three models seem to be conceptually similar. Determining the validity of this conceptual ccamparison is ultimately an experimental issue. For example, if a controlled experiment was conducted on the three psychotherapeutic models and it was found that they were equally effective in changing the patient's behavior, it might suggest that they were conceptually Similar. Eclecticism and Model Building An important issue raised by the foregoing analysis is whether the contributions of the behavioral sciences to management theory and practice will be .1 367 eclectic in nature. The models examined in chapters IV, V and VI tended to emphasize different determinants of similar problems and prescribed different mechanisms for coping with them. For example, the rational— emotive model attributed a patient's symptoms to a set of irrational beliefs. The prescriptive mechanism CIA; entailed substituting rational beliefs for irrational beliefs. On the other hand, the psychoanalytic model , attributed a patients symptoms to a repressed sexual iwf' conflict. The prescriptive mechanism entailed bringing the repressed conflict to the surface. From an eclectic point of view the important question is if, in terms of application, behavioral change models are mutually exclusive. If they are not mutually exclusive would combining them result in a "better" model? If changes in both individual and environmental variables can produce changes in behavior, under what conditions would a model that combined both sets of variables be more effective? Pure individual models and pure environmental models limit the transition-producing mechanism. The pure individual model holdsthe environment constant 368 and attempts to adjust the individual to it. It assumes the problem is with the individual who should be made to fit into his existing environment. On the other hand, the pure environmental model holds the individual constant and attempts to adjust the environment to him. It assumes the environment should be adjusted to the individual. It would seem that if changes in both individual and environmental variables can produce changes in behavior a model that combines both might be more effective under certain conditions. For example, increasing the need for achievement which is an individual variable may not always result in changes in actual achievement. The environment in which the individual Operates may not present an opportunity for achievement. Thus to instrument changes in the actual achievement level of an individual both individual and environmental variables may have to be manipulated. Ultimately the possibility of building eclectic behavioral change models is an experimental issue. The gxbssibility of combining models can only be determined 369 by attempting to combine them and testing their relative effectiveness. Experimental Comparison In accordance with proposition E.l, since the behavioral change models examined in Chapters IV, V and “A VI were different from those used by the ten managers an experiment must be conducted to determine which ones are most efficient and effective in helping them handle inadequate performance. An experiment is necessary because it is possible that the change mechanisms used by the managers may be more efficient and effective in improving inadequate performance than the change mechanism analyzed in Chapters IV, V and VI. Experi- mentation would involve exposing managers to the behavioral change models contained in the behavioral sciences. If the models were made Operational and the managers implemented them, their relative efficiency and effectiveness in improving inadequate performance could be compared with the mechanism the managers customarily used. The models that were most efficient and effective vuould be the most useful. 370 The general sequence of events in ascertaining the relationship between the behavioral sciences and management must begin with the empirical identification of the range of behavioral problems confronted by managers. After identifying the range of problems a! and how the managers cope with them, the behavioral sciences may be examined to see if the knowledge contained within them might be useful to managers in I!” u 2 1 Solving their problems. If the knowledge contained in the behavioral science is different from that possessed by managers, or was not designed to solve their behavioral problems, it must be converted and experimentally compared with the existing practices of the managers. If experimentally it is demonstrated that the knowledge is superior to that held by the managers, it would be useful to the managers. A PPENDICES APPENDIX A INTRODUCTION TO THE INTERVIEW 373 This interview is a part of a study I am doing for my dissertation at the Graduate School of Business at Michigan State University. The purpose of the study is essentially threefold First: To find out the kinds of problems successful managers have with people they work with and' how they deal with these sorts of problems. Second: TO find out how they go about getting the people they work with to do things for them. Third: To find out how they go about making changes in their job and their organization in general. The reason I am interviewing you is that your position indicates to me that you are a successful manager. Because of this I am very much interested in your Opinions and experience in regards to the things we will be discussing today. What I will do in our discussion is ask you some questions bearing on the three areas I have just mentioned. Before we begin, I want to stress to you that any information or Opinions you give me will be treated as privileged communication. NO one besides us will ever know what your views and Opinions are. SO I hope you won't be afraid to tell me what you really think and feel about the things we will be discussing. I am sure you realize how important it is for me to get accurate information about how you really think and feel about the matters to be discussed. This (tape recorder) is my traveling secretary. Because it may be rather distracting for me to take written notes and, more importantly, because I want to get down what you have to say as accurately as possible, I would very much like to record our conversation. I aSsure you that no one besides me will ever hear this tape. Do you have any objections to using it? 374 Before we go on, do you have any questions about what has been said so far? If you should have any questions as we proceed, please feel free to ask them. APPENDIX B THE INTERVIEW INSTRUMENT 376 I would like to begin by asking you a few questions about your relationship with the people who work under you. 1. What type of work climate have you tried to maintain with your subordinates? Information sought: The types of variables managers considered important in their relation- ship with their subordinates. In general, what type of treatment do you think subordinates expect from their boss? Information sought: The types of variables managers consider important in their relationship with their subordinates. What is the most frequent problem, if any, that you have with your subordinates? (If they answer they do not have any problems with their subordin- ates, they are to be asked why). Information sought: The variables managers used to describe problems with their subordinates. Do you recall any time in the recent past when you wanted a subordinate to do something and. you had difficulty getting him to do it? (If the answer is affirmative, they are to be asked to describe the situation and what they did about it). Information sought: The variables managers use in describing difficulties with their subordinates and how they handle such difficulties. Suppose you wanted a subordinate to do something and he refused to do it. What might you do about it? What could you do about it? Have you ever experienced a situation in the past like this? (If the answer is affirmative, they are to be asked about what they did). 377 Information sought: The managers' approach to a (situation that requires them to change the behavior of the subordinate and how much they stress the use of authority in such Situations. Have any of your subordinates ever disagreed with you about what to do in a given situation? (If the answer is affirmative, they are to be asked for examples of disagreements and what they did about them). Information sought: The managers' approach to dissagreements with their subordinates. How do you go about determining whether your subordin- ates' performance is satisfactory? That is, whether they are doing their job as well as they can or whether they are just doing enough to get by? Information sought: The variables they use for assessing their subordinates' role performance. Assuming you found a subordinate whose performance was not satisfactory, what would you do about it? Can you remember a recent example? (If the answer is affirmative, they are to be asked to describe the example and indicate what they did about it). Information sought: The variables they use to change their subordinates' role performance. In general, what do you think motivates peOple to perform up to their full capacity rather than to just do enough to get by? NOw I would like to change direction and ask you some questions about your relationship with peOple you have ‘worked under. 1. In general, how would you describe the type of relationship you have had with bosses you have worked for in the past? 378 Information sought: The variables they stress in their relationship with people in higher authority positions. How free were you to carry out your assignments? Information sought: Their perceived degree of freedom in their relationship with their superiors. If you made mistakes were you sometimes afraid to tell your boss about them? Why, or why not? Information sought: If they attempted to conceal mistakes from their superiors and their rationale for doing so. If you were unclear about what your boss wanted you to do, were you sometimes hesitant to ask him to clarify it? Why, or why not? Information sought: Their preception of role ambiguity and how they experienced it. Have you usually felt that you knew how satisfied your boss' has. been with your performance? Information sought: Their perception of ambiguity in evaluation of their role performance and how they experienced it. What thing indicated to you that your boss was either satisfied or dissatisfied with your work? Information sought: The variables they use to determine how well they are performing their role and whether they should make changes in their behavior. 10. 379 We have found that at some time or other many people have had some type of problems with their bosses and we are interested in the particular kinds you might have had. WOuld you mind telling us about a couple of them? How did you handle them? Information sought: The problems they encountered that they though were important and how they dealt with them. Did you ever disagree with your bosses? Could you give me an example? How was the disagreement handled? Information sought: How they handled disagreements with superiors and if, and how, they attempted to change their behavior. Did your bosses ever try to get you to do things differently than you usually did them? Could you give me some examples? What did they usually do to get you to do these things? Information sought: Their perception of whether and how their superiors attempted to change their behavior. We have found that many peOple would like to see their boss change their behavior in some way. Is there anything you would like your boss to change? Are you doing anything to facilitate these changes? If yes, what? If no, why not? Information sought: The types of change they would like to see their superiors make and whether, and how, they have attempted to facilitate these changes. Now I would like to change direction again and ask you some questions about your job. 1. Would you briefly tell me what your job consists of as you see it? 380 Information sought: Their conception of their role. If you could redesign your job so that it would be most satisfying to you, what would you like to have changed about it? Have you attempted these changes? Why or why not? Information sought: Their perceived dissatisfaction with their role and how they have attempted to change it. Since you have been in your current job have you made many changes? Which one do you think was the most significant? Why? Information sought: Their conception of important change variables and how they attempted to make changes. Let's assume now that you were made president of this company tomorrow, would there by any changes you would like to make? If yes, what are some of them? Do you foresee any problems in getting them accomplished? Have you ever suggested these changes to anyone? If no, why not? If yes, to whom? What was their reactions? Information sought: Their conception of change, dissatisfaction with the organization, and the problems they perceive in making changes. We recently conducted a study in which we found that some managers felt that one of the most important aspects of their job was to search for areas which need changing, as well as to formulate new objectives to be achieved. 381 In connection with this, they felt that one of the most difficult problems they faced was trying to get other people in their organization to see the need for the change and to go along with them in making the needed changes. They thought that one of the key abilities a manager must have is the ability to get other people to change their behavior. In terms of your experience, what do you think r1. about this? 4 Why do you think some people resist change? How do you think managers can go about getting people to change the ways of doing things? V1 Can you recall any time in the past when you were faced with a problem like this? What did you do about it? Information sought: How important they think change is in their roles, why they think people resist it, and how they attempt to COpe with it. BIBLIOGRAPHY 383 BIBLIOGRAPHY Adelson, J. "The Teacher as a ModeL" ,Amerigan.$ghglar, XXX NO. 3 (1961), 383-406. Allport, G. W. "Catharsis and the Reduction of Prejudice," Journal g; Social Issues, I, No. 3 (1945), 3-10. . The Use 9: Personal Documents lg Psychological Science. New York: Social Science Research Council, 1942. Argyris, C. "Understanding Human Behavior in Organizations: One View Point," Edited by Mason Haire. Modern Organization Theory. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1959, pp. 115-54. . Personality and Organization. New York: Harper & Bros., 1957. . Interpersonal Competence and Organizational Effectiveness. Homewood, Ill.: Irwin, 1962. . Integrating the Individual and the Organization. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1964. Bakan, D. "A Perspective on Psychoanalytic Theory," Social Science XL, No. 4 (1965), 18-22. Baritz, L. The Servants gf_Power: A History gf the Use pf Social Science ip_American Industry. Science edition. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1965. Bass, B. M. Leadership, Psychology, and Organizational Behavior. New York: Harper & Bros., 1960. Bavelas, "Leadership: Man and Function," Administrative Science Qparterly, V, No. 4 (1960), 491-98. Benne, K. D. "Democratic Ethics in Social Engineering," Progressive Education, XXVI, NO. 7 (1949), 26. 384 . "Operational Research in Health Education,"' Health Education Monographs, No. 5 (1959), 20-8. Benne, K. D., and Swanson, G. E. "The Problem of Values and the Social Scientist," Journal p§_Social Issues, VI, No. 4 (1950), 2-7. Bennis, W. G. "A New Role for the Behavioral Sciences: Effecting Organizational Change," Administrative Science Quarteply, VIII (1963), 125-65. “5 . Changing Organizations. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1966. If GI? ‘iD-I' Bennis, W. G., Benne, K. D., and Chin, R. (eds.). Thg Planning gfi Change: Readings lg the Applied Behavioral Sciences. New York: Holt, Rinehart w¢ and Winston, 1961. 0.-. ' Berelson, G. and Steiner, G. A. Human Behavior: Ag Inventopy 2; Scientific Findings. New York: Harcourt, Brace and WOrld, Inc., 1964. Bierstedt, R. "Social Science and Public Service," Applied Sociology: Opportunities and Problems. Edited by A. W. Gouldner and S. M. Miller. New York: The Free Press, 1965. Blake, R. R. and Mouton, J. S. The Managerial Grid: Key Orientations for Achieving Production Through People. Houston, Texas: Gulf Publishing Company, 1964. Bradford, L. P., Gibb, V. R., and Benne, K. D. T-group Theory and Laboratory Method. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1964. Brehm, J. W. and Cohen, A. R. Explorations ip Cognitive Dissonance. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1962 Brown, C. W. and Ghiselli, E. E. Scientific Method ip Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1965. 385 Brown, R. Social Psychology, New York: The Free Press, 1965, Chapter 9. Campbell, C. H. Induced Delusions: The Psychopathy g; Freudism. Chicago: Regent House, 1957. Cartwright, D. "Basic and Applied Social Psychology," Philosophy_p£_Science, XVI, NO. 3 (1949), 198-208. Cattell, R. B. "The Scientific Ethics of 'Beyond,'" Journal g; Social Issues, VI, No. 4 (1950), 22. Chapple, E. D. "Applied Anthropology in Industry," Anthropology Today. Edited by A. L. Kroeber pp g1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1953. Chapple, E. D. and Sayles, L. R. The Measure pf_Management: Designing Organizations for Human Effectiveness. New York: The Macmillan Co., 1961. Chein, I., Cook, S. W., and Harding, J. "The Use of Research in Social Therapy," Human Relations, I (1948), 512-32. Chin, R. "Problems and Prospects of Applied Research," The Planning 9: Change: Readings ip Eh; Applied Behavioral Sciences. Edited by W. G. Bennis, K. D. Benne and R. Chin. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1961, pp. 667-76. . "The Utility of System Models and Developmental Models for Practioners," The Planning gf_Change: Readings 1g the Applied Behavioral Sciences. Edited by W. G. Bennis, K. D. Benne and R. Chin. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1961, pp. 201-14. Chinoy, E. Society: '52 Introduction E2 Sociology. New York: Random House, 1961. Coch, L. & French, L. "Overcoming Resistance to Change,“ Human Relations, I, (1948), 512—33. 386 Cohen, A., Stotland, E. and WOlfe, D. "An Experimental Investigation of Need for Cognition," Journal pf Abnormal Social Psychology, LI (1955), 291-94. «Dill, W. R., Hilton, T. L. and Reitman, W. R. The New Managers: Patterns 2; Behavior and Development. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1962. Ellis, A. Reason and Emotion ;p_Psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart, 1963. Feigl, H. "The Difference Between Knowledge and Valuation," Journal pg Social Issues, VI, No. 4 (1950), 39-44. Ford, D. H. and Urban, H. B. Systems g; Psychotherapy: A Comparative Study. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1963. Foster, G. M. "Relationship Between Theoretical and Applied Anthropology," Human Organization, XI (Fall, 1962), 5-16. Frank, J. D. "The Dynamics of the Psychotherapeutic Relationship: Determinants and Effects Of the Therapist's Influence," Psychiatry, XXII (1959), 25-38. Fran, L. K. "The Interdisciplinary Frontiers in Human Relations Studies," Journal g£_Human Relations (Fall, 1954), 9-23. Frenkel-Brunswik, E. "Intolerance of Ambiguity as an Emotional and Perceptual Personality Variable," Journal 2; Personality, XVIII (1949), 108-43. Freud, S. A General Introduction :2 Psychoanalysis. Translated by J. Riviere. New York: Permabooks, 1953. . Moses and Monotheism. New York: Alfred Knopf, Inc., 1948. . New Introductory Lectures pp Psychoanalysis. New YOrk: W. W. Norton & Co., Inc., 1933. 387 . The Question pf_Lay Analysis, 1926. Translated by J. Strachey, London: The Hogarth Press, 1959. . Therapy and Technique. Edited by P. Rieff. New York: Collier Books, 1963. Geiger, G. "Values and Social Science," Journal 2; Social Issues, VI, No. 4 (1950), 8-16. Gibb, C. "Leadership," Handbook pg Social Psychology, II, Edited by G. Lindzey. Cambridge: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co., 1954. Glock, C. Y. 33 pl. Case Studies 33 Bringing Behavioral Science into Use. Stanford, Calif.: Institute for Communication Research, Stanford University, 1961. Gouldner, A. W. "Explorations in Applied Social Science," Social Problems, III, NO. 3 (1956), 173-81. . "Theoretical Requiremenusof the Applied Social Sciences," American Sociological Review, XXII, No. 1 (1957), 92-102. . "The Norm of Reciprocity: A Preliminary Statement," American Sociological Review. XXV (1960), 161-78. Greenwood, E. "The Practice of Science and the Science of Practice," The Planning 2; Change: Readings ip_the Applied Behavioral Sciences. Edited by W. G. Bennis, K. D. Benne and R. Chin. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1961, pp. 73-82. Gross, L. "Values and Theory of Social Problems," Applied Sociology: Opportunities and Problems. Edited by A. W. Gouldner and S. M. Miller. New York: The Free Press, 1965, pp. 383-97. Guest, R. H. Organization Change: The Effect pf Successful Leadership. Homewood, 111.: Richard D. Irwin, Inc., 1962. 388 Guetzkow, H. A"Conversion Barriers in Using the Social Sciences,“ Administrative Science Quargerly, IV, No. 1 (1959), 68—81. Hauser, P. “Social Science and Social Engineering," Philosophy pf Science, XVI (1949), 209-18. Hobbs, N. "Sources of Gain in Psychotherapy," American PsycholoqiéfiJ XVII, No. 11 (1962), 741-47. Jahoda, M. Current Concepts g; Positive Mental Health. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1958. Jaxnms,lE. "Social Therapy: Technocracy or Collaboration?" Journal pf_Social Issues, III, No. 2 (1947), 59-66. Jennings, E. E. 5p Anatomy pf Leadership, New York: Harper & Bros., 1960. Kahn, R. L. "Human Relations on the Shop Floor," Human Relatigns gpg_Modern Management. Edited by E. M. Hugh Jones. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Co., 1958. ' Kahn, R. L. and Cannel, C. F. The Dypamics g§_ Interviewing: Theory, Technique, and Cases. New York: John Wiley & Xons, Inc., 1957. Kahn, R. L. g; el- Organizational Stress: Studies ip_ Role Conflict and Ambiguity. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1964. Katz, D. "Human Interrelationships and Organizational Behavior," Concepts gpg_IS§ues ip_Administrative Behavior. Edited by S. Mailick and E. H. van Ness. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1962, pp. 166-86. ' Katz, D., and Kahn, R. L. The Social Psychology g; Organizations. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1966. finish": '1' - V's—‘2 Ir 389 Katz, D., and Sarnoff, I. "Motivational Base of Attitude XLIX (1954), 115-24. Kerlinger, F. N. Foundations pf_ Behaviore;_Research. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1964. Knight, F. "Evaluation of the Results of Psychoanalytic Therapy,‘ American Journal gf_Psychiatry, XCVII (1941), 434—46. *4 Landsberger, H. A. Hawthorne Revisited. Ithaca, N. Y.: Cornell University Press, 1958. I Leavitt, H. Managerial Psychology. Chicago: University h { of Chicago Press, 1964, Chap. 10-14. i Lee, A. M. "The Clinical Study Of Society," American Sociological Review, XX (December, 1955), 648-53. Leighton, A. H. Human Relations in e_Changing World: Observatiomson the Use of the Social Science. New York: E. P. Dutton and Co., 1949. Lewin, K. Field Theory ip_Socia1 Science: Selected Theoretical Papers. New York: Harper & Bros., 1951, especially pages 188—237. "Frontiers in Group Dynamics: Concepts, Method and Reality in Social Science: Social Equilibria and Social Change,“ Human Relations, I, No. l (1947), 5—42. I'Group Decision and Social Change," geadings lg Social Psychology. Edited by G. Swanson, T. Newcomb and E. Hartley. Rev. ed. New York: Henry Holt & Co., 1952. Lewin, K., Lippit, R., and White, R. "Patterns of Aggressive Behavior in Experimentally Created 'Social C1imates,'" Journal gf_Social Psychology, X (1939), 271—99. 390 Likert, R. "A Technique for the Measurement of Attitudes," Archives g: Psychology, CXL (1932). New Patterns_pf_Management. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1960. Lippitt, R. "Value-Judgment Problems of the Social Scientist Participating in Action—Research,” gpe Planning g£_ghange: Readings ;p_phe_App1ied Behavioral ficiences. Edited by W. G. Bennis et al. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1961. pp. 689-94. Lundberg, G. A. Social Research. New York: Longmans Green & Co., 1942. Lynd, R. S. Knowledge for What? Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1945. Maccoby, E., and Maccoby, N. "The Interview: A Tool of Social Science,“ Handbook pf Social geychology. Vol. I. Edited by G..Lindzey. Cambridge: Addison—Wesley Publishing Co., Inc., 1954, pp. 449- 87. MacIver, R. M. Social Causation. New York: Ginn and Co., 1942. Mann, F. C. "Human Relations Skills in Social Research," Human Reletions, IV (1951), 341-54. "Studying and Creating Change: A Means to Understanding Social Organization," Research ip, Industrial Human Relations: A_Critical Appraisal. Edited by C. M. Arensberg e; e;, New York: Harper & Bros., 1957, p. 150. 1/Mann, F. C., and Neff, F. W. (eds.) Managing Major Change ;p_OrganizetionS: Ap_Undeveloped Are; g; Administration epg_Socia1 Research. Ann Arbor, Mich.: The Foundation for Research on Human Behavior, 1962. "391 March, J. G. and Simon, H. A. Organizations. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1958. McClelland, D. ep g1. The Achievement Motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1953. McClelland, D. C. The Achieving Society. Princeton: D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 1961. . "Business Drive and National Achievement," L4. Readings gp Managerial Psychology. Edited by H. J. I Leavitt and L. R. Pondy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1964, pp. 122-46. "Achievement Motivation Can Be Developed," Harvard Business Review, XLIII, No. 6 (1965), 6. McGregor, D. The Human Side pf Enterprise. New York: McGraw-Hill Co., Inc., 1960, Chap. 2. McMurry, R. N. "The Case for Benevolent Autocracy," Harvard Business Review, XXXVI, No. 1 (1958), 16. Merton, R. K. "The Role of Applied Social Science in the Formation of Policy," Philosophy pf Science, XVI (1949), 161-81. Morrow, A. J., and French, J. R. P. '"Changing a Stereotype in Industry," Journal g: Social Issues, II, NO. l (1945), 33-7. Newman, J. W. "Working With Behavioral Scientists," Harvard Business Review, XXXVI, No. 4 (1958). Olmsted, D. W. Social Groups, Roles, and Leadership: ,Ag Introduction pg the Concepts. East Lansing, Mich.: Institute for Community Development, Michigan State University, 1961. Olmsted, M. S. The Small Group. New York: Random House, 1959. Pages, M. "The Sociotherapy of the Enterprise," Human Relations, XII, NO. 4 (1959), 317-34. 392 Ridgeway, V. F. "Dysfunctional Consequences of Performance Measurements," Administrative Science Quarterly, I, NO. 2 (1956). Roethlisberger, F. J. "Contributions Of the Behavioral Sciences to a General Theory of Management,“ Readings lg Managerial §§ychology. Edited by H. Leavitt and L. R. Pondy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1964. pp. 518-41. Roethlishberger, F. J., and Dickson, W. J. Mepagement epg_ the WOrker: Ap_Account of e_Research Progggg. Conducted py_the Western Electric Company, Hawthorne Works, Chicago. Science edition. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1964. Ross, R. "Moral Obligations of the Scientist,“ Applied Sociology: Opportunities and Problems. Edited by A. W. Gouldner and s. M. Miller. New York: The Free Free Press, 1965. pp. 429—38. Schein, E. "Brainwashing," Interpersonel Dynamics. Edited by W. G. Bennis fl 1. Homewood, 111.: Dorsey Press, 1964. "Management Development as a Process of Influence,“ Industrial Management Review, II, No. 2 (1961). Schein, E. H., and Bennis, W. G. Personal and Orgenizetione; Change Through Group Methods. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1965. Selznick, P. "Foundations of the Theory of Organization," American §pciological Review, XIII (1948), 25-35. Shepard, H. A., and Bennis, W. G. "A Theory of Training by Group Methods," Human Relations, IX (1956). 393 Shepard, H. and Blake, R. "Changing Behavior Through Cognitive Change," Human Organization, XXI (1962), 88-96. Shils, E. A. "Social Science and Social Policy," Philosophy 2; Science, XVI (1949), 222-23. Simon, H. A: Administrative Behavior. 2d ed. 1957. New York:, Macmillan, 1945. Skinner, B. F. Science and Human Behavior. New York: #4- Macmillan, 1953. t Soffer, C. The Organization from Within: A_Comparative Study 2; Social Institutions gased.gp.g H j Sociotherapeutic Approach. London: Tavistock Institute, 1961. Stern, P. J. The Abnormal Person and His WOrld: ,53 Introduction pg Psychopathology. Princeton: D. Van Nostrand Co., Inc., 1964. Strauss, G. and Sayles, L. R. Personnel: The Human Problems gj_Management. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1960. Tannenbaum, R. and Massarik, F. "Participation by Subordinates in the Managerial Decision-Making Process," Canadian Journal 9; Economics and Political Science, XVI (1950), 408-18. Tannenbaum, R., Weschler, I. R. and Massarik, F. Leadership and Organization: §_Behavioral Science Approach. New York: .McGraw-Hill, 1961. Tax, S. "Anthropology and Administration," America Indigena, V (1945), 21-33. Thibaut, J. and Coules, J. "The Role of Communication in the Reduction of Interpersonal Hostility," Journal 'gfi Abnormal and Social Psychology, XLVII (1952), 770-77. 394 Vroom, V. H. Some Personalitnyeterminants pf the Effects 9; Participation. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1960. Walker, N. A Short History 9: Psychotherapy ;p_Theory and Practice. New York: The Noonday Press, 1959. White, R. K., and Lippitt, R. AutoCracy and Democragy: ,Ap Experimental Inquiry. New York: Harper & Bros., 1960. Wilensky, J. L. and Wilensky, H. L. "Personnel Counseling: The Hawthorne Case," American Journal pg Sociology, LVII (1951/52). Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy py Reciprocal Iphibition. .Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1958. Wolpe, J., Salter, A., and Reyna, L. (eds.). The Conditioning Therapies: The Challenge gp Psychotherapy. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc., 1965. Wolpe, J. ”The Comparative Clinical Status of Conditioning Therapies and Psychoanalysis," The Conditioning Therapies: The Challenge ;p_Psychotherapy. Edited by J. Wolpe, A. Salter, and L. Reyna. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc., 1965, pp. 5—16. . Ila-1'. . I" MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES 1“WI111111111111I!WllH1111111111111 31293106161809