,‘h' --J lllllllllllllllllllllllllllllIllllllll'lllll 1 mass 3 1293 10637 1432 ‘ aJ'fi-ZfiARY atemgam fitate . 1 University 3 L . 4 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled INTERCULTURAL EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING PROGRAMS IN SELECTED INSTITUTIONS OF HIGHER EDUCATION IN THE UNITED STATED presented by Bradley Jackson Smith has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Department of Ph . D. degree in Educat ional Administrat ion \J Major professor \ Dr. Ted Ward Date quuj /, [989‘ MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0 12771 MSU RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from LIBRARIES m your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. {33%‘6‘3/ fl ." a‘ r ; 3‘— §;., kJUhl1 1l§3? INTERCULTURAL EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING PROGRAMS IN SELECTED INSTITUTIONS OF HIGHER EDUCATION IN THE UNITED STATES BY Bradley Jackson Smith A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Administration and Curriculum l984 ABSTRACT INTERCULTURAL EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING PROGRAMS IN SELECTED INSTITUTIONS OF HIGHER EDUCATION IN THE UNITED STATES By Bradley Jackson Smith The descriptive study examined the structure of existing full-semester intercultural experiential education programs in selected institutions of higher education in the United States. Attention focused on whether there exists a consensus of educational rationale and a primary cluster of programmatic and logistical factors central to program design and management. The population was 239 CAEL-member (Council for the Advancement of Experiential Learning) institutions. A random sample of 25 institutions was drawn from institutions which offered full-semester, immersion-type, intercultural, experiential learning programs. Three research questions were developed: l. What intercultural program outcomes have been identified in the existing research for contending that intercultural experiential education programs are effective in producing student development? 2. What crucial programmatic and logistical factors essential to program effectiveness can be identified in intercultural experiential learning programs within the scope of the sample? 3. What major barriers have institutions encountered in making their intercultural programs succeed? A Short-Form Survey was designed to collect information on varieties of intercultural experiential learning programs. the direction of program growth, and the existence of printed statements of educational rationale. The Telephone Interview Guide, was designed to focus 25 data-gathering interviews with sample institutions. The findings indicated that 88% of the programs are stable or growing. Students tended to identify the leading benefits in terms of changed perspectives on culture (44%). on life (44%). and on self (28%). A particularly key variable in such programs seems to be the quality of interpersonal relationships. Major problem areas seem to be essentially academic. such as faculty distrust. willingness to accept international assignments, development and assessment of the academic program. and intra-institutional coordination. There is a need for empirical studies on the educational outcomes of intercultural programs which can be consolidated into a clear academic rationale. indicating level of legitimacy. and which can be communicated to the professional academic community. Dedicated to My parents. R00 and RUTH SMITH: who nourished in me a love for learning and whose life-long love and confidence have inspired me to invest my abilities in noble endeavors, and to my wife. JOYCE. whose constant love. sacrifice. and encouragement have made the accomplishment of this project a reality. ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The successful completion of this dissertation is the cooperative effort of many people. To some I want to give special recognition. Dr. Ted Ward. Guidance Committee Chairman and friend. whose scrutiny. suggestions. and encouragement have made this program of study an exciting experience of personal growth. Dr. Norman Bell. Dr. Charles Blackman. and Dr. Eldon Nonnemaker. Guidance Committee Members. who each in his own way was a source of encouragement and assistance. Dr. Joyce Smith, wife. for loving counsel and consistent confidence. Mrs. Barbara Rose. statistician and sister, whose patience. assistance. and suggestions were absolutely invaluable. Dr. Roderick and Ruth Smith. parents. for their many hours of encouragement and help with everything from quiet to computers. Dr. Morris Keeton. for his sincere spirit of cooperation. and commitment to help CAEL be an encourager of research efforts. The many people of Michigan State University. who allowed me the privilege of study and a vision for professional development during very difficult times. My sons. Andrew. Timothy. and Jeremy. for always believing their Dad could do anything. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE LISTOFFIGURESIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII v1. LISTOFTABLESIIIIIIIII IIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII V.“ CHAPTER II INTRODUCTION I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 1 NeEd for the StUdy I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 3 StatementoftheProblem... ..... 4 Purpose Of the StUdy I I I I I I I I I I I IIIIII I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 5 Importance Of the StUdy I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 5 Scope and De1imitations I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 6 Definition Of Terms I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 7 ResearchQuestions........ ....... 7 overvjeWeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee IIIIII IIIIIIIIIII 8 L0 III REVIEVOFTHELITERATUREIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII Theoretical Relationship of Experience and Learning . . . . . . . . 9 NeedforResearch.............................. l0 Growth................ ........ ...............l0 Interactive Learning ..... . ......... . . . ......... . . l2 TheoreticalModels...............................l3 Developmental Matrix of the College Learner.............. l7 CognitiveDevelopment............................18 Cognitive and Ethical Development . ........ . . . . . . . . . . .18 E90 Deve10pment I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I 22 FaithDevelopment............ ...... 25 TheoreticalCorrelates............................26 Appropriateness of Experiential Learning Programs . . . . . . . . . 29 Counterbalancing Traditional Education . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 LearningShifts................................. 33 Contributions to Higher Education . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 Diversity of Experiential Learning Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Intercultural Experiential Learning Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Brief History of Intercultural Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 Value of Intercultural Experiential Learning Programs . . . . . 38 Varieties of Intercultural Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 Factors of Intercultural Effectiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4l Implications for Program Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 iv TABLE OF CONTENTS (cont'd.) Page BarrierstoOvercome...............................55 AssessmentConcerns.............................55 Organizational Barriers to Innovation................. 56 Conditions for Institutionalization................... 56 III. RESEARCH METHODOLOGY . . . . . . . . . ...... . ..... . . . . . . . . 58 ResearchQuestions................................ 58 PopulationandSample.............................. 58 InstrumentDevelopment............................. 60 DataCollectionProcedures................ ...... .....62 Summary........................................64 IVI FINDINGSOFTHESTUDYIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII65 SummaryoftheLiterature......................... 65 Prevalence of Experiential Learning Programs. . . . . . . . . . . . 66 Developmental and Theoretical Validity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 Complementary Educational Approach 67 Student-Reported Interest 68 Analysis of Statements of Rationale 69 Programmatic and Logistical Components 70 Results of the Short-Form Survey 7l Telephone Interview Results 72 Starting Intercultural Programs 76 Components of Current Intercultural Programs 79 Institutional Barriers 90 Summary and Limitations 96 Current Status of the Programs 96 Programmatic Factors 97 Miscellaneous Findings 98 V. CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS 99 Academic Legitimacy 99 Developmental Potential lOO Problem Areas lOl Limitations lDl Program Recommendations l03 Implications for Future Research l04 APPENDIX A Important Components of Experiential Learning Programs. . . . . .l05 APPENDIX 8 Comparison of Goals of University and Overseas Education. . . . .l l2 APPENDIX C Meta-Goals of Traditional vs. Intercultural Instruction . . . . . . ll4 APPENDIXD SampleInstitutions................................llS APPENDIXE Short-FormSurvey................... .......... ....ll6 APPENDIXF TelephoneInterviewGuide...........................ll7 V TABLE OF CONTENTS (cont'd.) Page APPENDIX G Survey Non-Respondents Follow-up Letter . . . . . . . . APPENDIX H Advance Letter to Sample Institutions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .l23 APPENDIX I Suggested Interview Guide . . . . . . . . . ..... . . APPENDIX J Interview Appointment Sheet . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . BIBLIOGRAPHYIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII vi TABLE l0 ll 12 l3 14 l5 l6 l7 l8 l9 LIST OF TABLES Types of Sponsored Experiential Learning Programs in CAEL and NOD‘CAEL IDSITIUITODS e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Loevinger's Theory of Ego Development . . . . . . . . . . . Comparison of Models of Stage-Theory Development . . Student Values and Experiential Learning Outcomes . . W-Shaped Hypothesis of Satisfaction . . . . ..... . . . Comparison of CAEL and Non-CAEL Institutions . . . . . Summary of Short-Form Survey Returns . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Kinds of Intercultural Experiential Programs in CAEL-Member InstitutionSIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII Direction of Growth of Intercultural Programs Over the Last Five Years in CAEL-Member Institutions . . . . . . . . . . Statements of Intercultural Program Rationale . . . . . ProgramExistenceinYears.................. First Intercultural Program at Institution. . . . . . . . . Use of Consultants in Program Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Components of Student Preparation . . . . . . . . . . . . . Mechanisms Used in Screening Students for Intercultural ProgramSIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII Class Levels of Intercultural Program Participants . . . . . . . . . . . Average Group-Size in Intercultural Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page ll 23 27 32 44 59 7l 73 74 75 76 77 78 80 Bl 82 82 Frequency of Student Group-Meetings on Foreign Locations . . . . . . 83 Kinds of Student Documentation of Learning Outcomes . . . . . . . . . vii 84 Table Page 20 Range of Credit in Intercultural Programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 2l Single Greatest Benefit to Intercultural Program Participants . . . 86 22 Methods of Faculty Preparation for Intercultural Assignments . . . 87 23 Characteristics of Effective Faculty Leading Intercultural Groups 88 24 Incentives for Faculty Involvement in Intercultural Programs . . . . 89 25 Personnel Responsible for Arrangements Abroad . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 26 Institutional Problems in Starting Intercultural Programs . . . . . . 93 27 Institutional Problems in Maintaining Intercultural Programs . . . . 95 viii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page l IntersectionofLifeandLearning..........................2 2 Kolb'sLearningCycle................................. l4 3 Kolb'sComplete4MatSystemModel....................... l5 4 Kolb Model: Experiential Learning and Learning Styles . . . . . . . . . 16 5 Kolb's Theory Modified by Argyris and Schon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l7 6 Lickona's Summary of Kolhberg's Developmental Stages OfMoralReasoningIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII19 7 A Description of Perry's Scheme of Cognitive and Ethical Deve1opmentIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 2] 8 Perry's Model of Cognitive and Ethical Development . . . . . . . . . . . 20 9 Contrasted Approaches to Learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 l0 Differentiating Factors in Experiential Education . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 ll W-ShapedHypothesis................................ 43 l2 Comparison of Numbers of Students in First Year and Current YanOfProgramIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII79 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Intercultural experiential education is vying for a place of significance within American higher education. As American society moves rapidly from an industrial to an informational society, the pressures on higher education are increasing. Change is impacting student populations. Between l980 and the year 2000. the l8-24 year-old student population will decrease by 23% (Carnegie Surveys. l975-76). In addition. the student population is becoming steadily older. and reflects changing aptitudes. learning styles. and interests. Such a shift impacts the entire chemistry of higher education. Given declining student populations and revenues. the pressure is on for institutions to offer creative programs responsive to a set of complex and changing student needs. Recruitment and retention are becoming increasingly competitive. Under the growing pressure of competition for survival. educators are concerned about the potential impact upon the quality of education. The drive for self-preservation will either compromise educational standards (soft credits, grade inflation. lower admission standards) or it will revitalize education in new directions. One such potential direction is a new concern for the relationship between learning. experience. and culture. If learning is. as Dewey implied. the interpretation of experience, then should not educators be pressing for a deeper understanding of the dynamics of experience and a more skillful management of the life-learning relationship? If one of the aims of education is to enlarge the 2 intersection of the spheres of life and learning (Figure l). then a cluster of questions arise as to how that task of enlargement can best be facilitated. \ 3:. § Sphere 0f Learning Figure l Intersection of Life and Learning How is experience translated into learning? Can the learning process be compressed at the undergraduate level or expanded into a variety of domains of development through a deliberate sequencing of certain kinds of experience? Is intercultural experiential learning a legitimate approach for new directions in American higher education? The life-learning connection has been important to American society from the very beginning. and traces its roots to its European antecedents. Lord Ashby recounts an old story: . . . that in the medieval University of Paris the professors were disputing about the number of teeth in a horse's mouth. They agreed that the number could not be a multiple of three. for that would be an offense to the Trinity; nor could it be a multiple of seven. for God created the world in six days and rested on the seventh. Neither the records of Aristotle nor the arguments of Saint Thomas enabled them to solve the problem. Then a shocking thing happened. A student who had been listening to the discussion went out. opened a horse's mouth. and counted the teeth. That anonymous student gave us far more than the hardy cliche about the trustworthiness of what comes 'straight from the horse's mouth'; his brash act also signaled the beginning of objective inquiry. the revolt against authority. the empirical attitude. and the linking of academic study with the facts of life. His act introduced research into the university. (Ashby. l963. P.37) Historically. the commitment of American education has been to prepare society‘s leaders and to contribute useful knowledge to the task of building the American society. The spirit of American expansionism in the l9th century broke the bonds of the classical curriculum and co-opted the energy of higher education into the experiential. technological. and industrial revolutions taking place. NEED FOR THE STUDY Education has come to understand its responsiblity for building a better world. World Wars I and II shattered the complacency of American provincialism and awakened the United States to its global responsiblities. The frightening reality that an entire world was at war raised the concurrent awareness that an entire world could be at peace. though not without deliberate effort. Intercultural understanding is recognized as the price of that peace. though it has remained a complex and illusive reality. Such understanding is no longer optional however. given the magnitude of global problems. The days of a justified ethocentricity have passed. The international news media bombards society daily with accounts of religious hostilities. apartheid. nuclear buildups. terrorism. political and economic instability. and life-threatening ecological contaminations. The complexity of these interrelated problems along with the combination of power and antithetical ideologies demands a new response of global concern. cultural understanding. and effective communication. If the American educational enterprise is to continue to serve the critical needs of society. then the deep issues which divide or bind relationships within the 4 global family must be encountered. understood.and resolved. If peace is to come to the global community. history suggests that it will not be by coercion. but by a new level of understanding. Acknowledging the relationship between education and global well-being. the United States government passed the International Education Act in l966: The Congress hereby finds and declares that a knowledge of other countries is of the utmost importance in promoting mutual understanding and cooperation between nations; that strong American educational resources are a necessary base for strengthening our relations with other countries; that this and future generations of Americans should be assured ample opportunity to develop to the fullest extent possible their intellectual capacities in all areas of knowledge pertaining to other countries. peoples and cultures . . . An essential aspect of educational curriculum is its responsiveness to the needs of culture and society. This connection implies that education needs to be preparing individuals who will be effective participants and change agents in their own culture and in light of growing global inter-dependency in other cultures as well. (80 Statutes At Large. Section 2. l066. l966) Can intercultural experiential learning programs provide an effective means to intercultural understanding? The question is difficult to answer. however it is related to the direction of the present research project. STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM By providing an experience in cultural microcosm. some intercultural curriculum strategists claim that the educational outcomes are more holistic and impact more deeply future perspectives. If these claims can be substantiated. they dare not be overlooked. Intercultural programs deliberately place students in cultural environments which challenge one's assumptions on nearly every level as to why attitudes. beliefs and behaviors follow a given pattern. This innovation claims to be an effective approach to: the cultivation of an inquiring mind. cultural sensitivity. values development. and self-understanding. Intercultural experiential programs are viewed by many as experimental. if not moderately high-risk approaches to educational objectives. Consequently. most formal institutions of 5 higher education have not been convinced of the educational legitimacy nor broadened their program options to significantly utilize the potential contributions of intercultural experiential education. Skepticism and indifference continue to characterize their attitudes. Because of these attitudes. a significant gap exists between the potential of intercultural experiential learning programs and the reality of their level of acceptance as a legitimate dimension of American higher education. PURPOSE OF THE STUDY The purpose of such a research study is to examine descriptively the structure of existing full-semester immersion-type intercultural experiential education programs in selected American institutions of higher education. Specifically. the study is conducted in order to see if there exists a consensus of educational rationale and a primary cluster of programmatic and logistical factors central to the design and management of these programs. The objectives will be accomplished by examining three critical areas: l) a review of research evidence for the educational legitimacy of intercultural experiential education programs; 2) an analysis of programmatic and logistical factors essential for program effectiveness; and 3) the identification of major barriers and/or difficulties overcome by educational institutions in order to make intercultural programs succeed. IPPORTANCE OF THE STUDY Given the current pressures for the concerns of quality in higher education. it is important to ask if intercultural experiential education programs can improve the quality of learning. Are there dimensions of understanding important to the aims of American higher education. which can be better learned through intercultural experiential programs? In addition. it may be that such a study holds potential value for evaluation of existing programs. and new program development. Many institutions of higher education are struggling for survival in today's competitive educational market and are looking for distinctive ways to enhance quality and package the educational experience with appeal. The attractiveness of a new array of locations. experiences. delivery systems and activities (characteristics of intercultural experiential education) provide increased potential for program diversity. The development of new programs is a costly process. Studies like the one at hand could possibly save a costly period of trial and error experimentation by providing an understanding of the essential components for successful program construction. Conceivably. such an information base could likewise encourage institutions to take the risk of the intercultural experiment. and in so doing. move a part of the American society a small step in the direction toward intercultural understanding. SCOPE AND DELIMITATIONS Many intercultural experiential programs presently exist within the context of American higher education. A random sample of 25 institutions offering full-term jntgrgultural immersion programs will be drawn from a population of 239 institutions within American higher education which are members of the Council for the Advancement of Experiential Learning (CAEL). Most member-institutions sponsor specific programs of intercultural learning. although the programs represent a broad range of diversity. difficulty and maturity. CAEL institutions are representative of a larger population through their wide diversity. Implications hopefully may be drawn from the study for the applicability of the findings for several different types of educational institutions. DEFINITION OF TERMS Culture: "Culture" is used in a broad sense to describe the activities of the members of a given society in their particular way of living. All aspects of a society's life-patterns are included. from the metaphysical world-view to the artifacts which they produce. Intercultural: "Intercultural" describes a member of one culture encountering and interacting with a member of a different culture. The earlier literature used the term "cross-cultural" in this same way. however. a growing distinction is being made between the two terms. Currently. cross-cultural studies tend to examine common variables across several different cultures. giving the term a meaning of “constant across cultures." Experiential: "Experiential" pertains to those things encountered and learned through direct experience. Such a direct contact-type learning with the realities at hand stands in contrast to vicarious kinds of exposure. Immersion Programs: "Immersion Programs" classify programs which involve a full-time. in-context experience for the learner. RESEARCH QIESTIONS Question 1: What intercultural program outcomes have been identified in the existing research for arguing that intercultural experiential education programs are effective in producing student development? Question 2: What crucial programmatic and logistical factors. essential to program effectiveness. can be identified in intercultural experiential learning programs within the scope of the sample? Question 3: What major barriers have institutions encountered and overcome to make their intercultural programs succeed? OVERVIEW Given the scattered nature of the existing research in areas relating to intercultural experiential education. an examination will be conducted in Chapter II of the relevant studies in the fields of: (l) experiential learning; (2) developmental change relevant to college-age students; and (3) intercultural effectiveness. In Chapter III. the instruments and methodology of the research project will be described. The findings will be described and displayed in Chapter IV. and the implications of the study for application and further research will be presented in Chapter V. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Research on intercultural experiential learning programs involves several related bodies of literature. It is theoretically complex; yet to avoid the difficulty of examining the complex interrelationships involved. is to miss the essential legitimacy in this integrated approach. Attention must be given to the theoretical work done in the area of the connection between experience and learning. At the same time. the specific context is undergraduate higher education which implies an examination of the developmental matrix of the college-age learner. The literature describing the programatic dimensions of experiential education must likewise be investigated. Finally. the distinctives of the intercultural dimension must be examined from a variety of viewpoints. Although the primary focal point of the study at hand is intercultural programs. the larger context of experiential education. learning theory. and developmental change are inseparable considerations. Tl-EORETICAL RELATIONSHIP OF EXPERIENCE AND LEARNING Philosophers and educators have long struggled with a fundamental question of human learning which probes the relationship between experience and learning. Must an individual "discover" that which is worth knowing. or is the individual "taught" knowledge? 10 Need for Research Experiential education has eased its way into higher education through the back door. Faculty observe the enthusiasm and obvious growth which has taken place in students as a result of field experiences. yet have done little research and systematic analysis as to "how" and "why" the development has occurred (Doherty. Mentkowski. and Conrad. l978). Consequently. there has been relatively little empirical research to justify. on educational grounds. using experiential learning options in preference to others (Keeton. l978). Knapp and Davis (l978) have concluded that so few studies have been made of the "genus and species" of experiential learning. no commonly accepted framework or taxonomy yet exists by which all the kinds of programs can be sorted and evaluated. The problems of definition and validity continue to distress curriculum and program developers. Evaluation studies are currently needed so that educators might be able to make decisions based upon which programs are of substantial educational benefit to students and which are not. M The rise of experiential learning programs has been astronomical. Generic to American culture is the appetite for "useful" knowledge. Tough (l977) and Penland (1977) claim that 80% of all American adults engage in self-directed or "do-it-yourself" learning through life-experience. Peterson. Cross. and others 0978) estimate that out of HS million deliberate learners in organizational settings in the U. S. during any one year. 72% of those studying at the post-secondary level are learning from non-academic institutions. Undergraduate institutions of higher education reflect a similar popularity of experiential education programs. The first state-of-the-art survey of experiential learning was conducted in l977 by the Council for the Advancement of Experiential Learning (CAEL). in cooperation with the Educational Testing Service and the Kellogg Foundation. Of the 300 11 CAEL-member institutions. 75% were found to offer one or more sponsored experiential learning programs. Of the 302 Non-CAEL institutions surveyed. 66% offered one or more sponsored experiential learning programs. A wide diversity of kinds of programs were found to have a common experiential dimension (Table l). The sponsored programs identified were; career exploration. career development. cooperative education. pre-professional training. public service internships. service learning internships. personal growth and development. cross-cultural experience. and field research studies. Table l Types of Sponsored Experiential Learning Programs in CAEL and Non-CAEL Institutions CAEL Programs Percentage Non-CAEL Programs Percentage Field Research 87.9 Pre-professional Train.83.3 Pre-professional Train.85.9 Cooperative Education 74.2 Career Exploration 80.l Field Research 72.5 Public Service Intern. 80.l Career Exploration 70.9 Cooperative Education 78.2 Career Development 69.2 Career Development 74.2 Public Service Intern. 62.5 Service-Learning Inter.74.2 Service-Learning Inter.5l.7 Cross-Cultural Exper. 64.5 Personal Growth 50.9 Personal Growth 63.l Cross-Cultural Exper. 48.3 Knapp and Davis (l978. p. l2) The indications of growth among the experiential approach were clearly evident. Growth has continued. Current percentage extrapolations estimate that 1.500.000 students and 50.000 faculty participate in out-of-class experiential learning every year (Little. l983a). The practice has preceded the theoretical justification in most instances. Only recently has experiential learning begun to surface as a coherent idea that encompasses the variety of programs listed above. It is still awaiting full recognition as a legitimate and effective approach within American higher education. In discussing the phenomenon of experiential learning. 12 Little (1983) points out two striking observations: first. the extent and rich variety of experiential learning from an inter-institutional perspective; second. that at a single institution. the dimensions of this movement are almost never appreciated. InteLactive Learning Experiential learning has been gaining momentum throughout this century. Educational philosopher .John Dewey exerted the most influence in promoting the educational legitimacy of experiential learning. Dewey (l938) recognized the high value of "experience" within American culture and sought to provide a philosophical framework for its justification within the formal educational process. If "experience" was essential to the advancement of knowledge and skill in the building of the American society. then why not recognize it as an overt dimension in the formal process of learning? Dewey would not deal with the concept of learning without addressing the issue of personal experience. He postulated one permanent frame of reference for learning--namely the organic connection between education and personal experience. For Dewey. experience was simply the interaction between the individual and his environment. It was continuous. As soon as a certain interaction had taken place and had been reflected upon. it became understood in ways that affected subsequent interactions. Thus. Dewey's conception of the notion of "continuity" came to stand for the process of continuous restructuring. To Dewey. the critical educational issue focused upon the deliberate shaping of learning environments which could best facilitate certain kinds of experience. Selective experiences. in turn. would produce valuable learning. He was not simplisticly advocating "experience" as a magical shortcut to knowledge. but recognized that some experience was consistent with the goals of American education and some experience was miseducative. The task of the educator. from DEWEY's perspective. was to strategically design appropriate learning environments. given the abilities and interests of the learner and the educational objectives. 13 These growth experiences could then be carefully sequenced into a meaningful continuum. Strategizing growth through experience was central to Dewey's conception of progressive education. Learning was inherently related to life. Researchers continue to validate Dewey's theories. The gap that existed between the learner and the curriculum could best be bridged by direct personal involvement (Tumin. 1976). It is not sufficient to merely read. hear. write. and talk about the realities being studied. Experiential learning. by contrast. puts the learner into direct contact with the phenomenon being studied (Keeton. l978). Immediately. the dimension of action is central--testing. improving. applying. At the heart of experiential education is a belief that learning occurs in various and often unexpected ways (Sexton. 1976). that learning goes on throughout life. and that it must be integrated with all other activities of the individual. Theoretical Models The dynamics of experiential learning have become a fertile question in theoretical research. Steinaker and Bell (1979) have investigated the possibility of a hierarchy of experience; positing a sequence of steps through which an individual proceeds or progresses from the initial exposure to an experience. through the dissemination of the experience. Their resulting experiential taxonomy attempts a more holistic view of'human experience than has been provided through the single dimensional views of cognitive. affective. or psychomotor taxonomies. Essentially. this gestalt taxonomy identifies a five-step sequence: 1) Exposure. 2) Participation. 3) Identification. 4) Internalization. and 5) Dissemination. Given the dynamic of experience. the "how" of experiencial learning became the focus for Kolb's theoretical research (1976). Kolb describes his model as a Simple description of the learning cycle in which experience is translated into Concepts which are used as guides in the choice of new experiences ( Figure 2). 14 Immediate / Concrete Experience and followed by is the basis for ‘-‘-.:‘ Testing Implications Observation of Concepts in New and Situations Reflection which are assimilated into the Formation of Abstract Concepts K and Generalizations from which implications for action are deduced which lead to (Doherty. et al.. 1978. p.24) Figure 2 Kolb's Learning Cycle The model also reveals a dialectical interplay between contrasted abilities--concrete/abstract. reflective/active--as predominant learning styles (Figure 3). Through the Learning Style Inventory(1976). Kolb is able to identify which of the learning styles (concrete.reflective. abstract. active) the individual has tended to rely upon and which have been underdeveloped. If one or more of the learning modes is seriously underdeveloped. the learner may be unable to complete the learning cycle successfully since all four occur in sequence (Doherty. Mentkowski.and Conrad. 1978). It is therefore imperative that the learner's dominant style be identified. and their ability to use other learning styles expanded (Figure 4). Traditional educational approaches have encouraged the development of Perceptual and symbolic abilities through the emphasis upon reflective observation and concept formation. The behavioral and affective skills. however. are best developed through active experimenting and concrete experience. 15 Concrete Expeflence "Ne ' 9’3, . F! M ”mwb 95%, 'en the a...“ 9’6 \n ./ 1; I Coo-'0‘ / ’5} ffi'fli d ‘ / a O '. '1; 1/ ; [kWh-flu" :1. (quart. ‘4 fig. W“. [a tletlecttve Acttve \ (n Observatton rlmentetton . 3 '5'" z *m:r.:::'.. w. «we “I ‘i \\ , a ovum mm" \ ,5 d? \\&? tn $4M“ /‘ \ \ Q0 \\ 500‘ on“? C Abstract Conceptunllzetton (CAEL Institute, 1983) Figure 3 Kolb's Complete 4Mat System Model 16 Concrete Experience Practical; action oriented Imaginative; emotional At ease with people People oriented Focus on doing things Broad cultural interests Good at decision making; ”pushy" Good at brainstorming ACCOMMODATOR DIVERGER Business administration Humanities Marketing, sales, training, admin- Counseling, organizational development istration personnel Active Reflective Experimentation Observation Application of ideas Ability to create theoretical models Relatively unemotional Assimilates disparate observations into Less people oriented integrated whole CONVERGER ASSIMILATOR Physical sciences Math; Basic Sciences Physical sciences; engineering Research and planning Abstract Conceptualization (Claxton and Ralston. 1978) Figure 4 Kolb Model: Experiential Learning and Learning Styles Kolb's learning cycle theory has been further modified by Argyris and Schb’n (1974).They have concluded that the object of the field experience is to learn to become more reflective under real-time conditions. so that effective ad hoc theories of action can be created and tested. refinement in one's applied theory against the ideal espoused theory. Mentkowski. (Figure 5). This reflectivity facilitates a constant Doherty. and Conrad (1978) summarized this modification diagrammatically 17 [Concrete Experience . subjected to which leads to Testing Espoused Observation Theories in New and Situations Reflection followed by“\\\\\ e(////// Modified to form a new Espoused Theory mo m.memnpso¥ eo sceesam m.mcoxu_4 m «came; Amm-em .qa .euma .mcoxuwov can. we umem_caa m.uecz =.eoom= m_ Luzon c_ mmozo an cmutaxmc m.umcz mu:o_g waxes acme: "columcammq _ .smcu mcwxaoeo to mmucmacmmcou ecu mm_:t are atone "mmmcmeazq macaw .>u_cocu:e an ucmszm_c:a u_o>u cu mcwmmv .ucmaou oc_uumm do some "Loum>_uoz e_mm ob .uc_oq3w+> ecu memo: czo mac canton some use» a_m; mmucmacmmcou Mumcucou on: mmogu ou »_:o umuam__no mt.:ox "upmm to» “no xoo_ cu m>az "co_ume:mm< poccmuxm toe ctmucouv =.mg:ox cuuatom __.H .xuan xe zuuocum no»: .—ome a axes N m.uw_ nxu_uotqwumc mumtucou xn umccm>om use mcowum_mt case: "mmmcmtmz< moaum mwozoumma =._otaumc m.uegz= on mes-toe-u_-c_-m.ue:3 .ummcmuc_-e_om ”Lobo>_boz .xu_scowcou _m_uom u Lo_>o;mn uoou ”cowumsamm< .Amozm zuooouv xuwuoca_umc _emu_ memos co_uetwaoou n “cacao to mm:__ume ecu meowbcmbcw Lme_mcou on new: “mmmcmtm3< macaw .mcwzuo mo mco_uouomaxm m:_umws .quuomcoum .L_m uoommxom wooo ou a: o:_>w— so pe>ocaam _o_uom to» assume ”coua>_uoz .xu__atoe Co motaom one Amco_unuuqum m_ Levee _o_uom as» to xu_cogu:< u:o_bms:mm< .uw :_gu*3 mpm:u_>_vc_ mo Lo_>o;ma ecu mayo—zoos bump e gamumxmg _o_uom tomca_ o m. mews» ”mmocmcoz< mmoum .Lmugo .awuom mc_um_xm :_au -c_oe use m_oc eoc_ewu >__o_u0m o» a: m>P_ ob co_uam_—no Lo xuau mo mmcmm "Loua>wuoz .omco> wu_> uoc .xu_—ocoe soc» mm>_cmv 3o_ «moonwaammcou .accmuxm Co mmmpotomuc :uzm_t= use mo.a_uc_cn .ecoz "co_ume:mm< _m_uom _occmuxm ac_umme toe ctmucouv muozou .oocogu o» Amm_a_ucpca coco .muc_oaxo_> ocpcoee_u m —ocoe comosu-e_um co co :o_uo___ucouac sauces“ on one »u__ov_w Low :cmocouv vm>_ttu.muuocbcou _o_uom ace m «ups; _e_uom\_ocox ”mmmcmcoz< momoum mwozouemoa .mcomcoq __o co xb_co_u ecu .mc__ .mugmet one to» became; ”=ouco_um -cou: Co mm_n_uc_cq ob bees -umesou passage. ”Laue>_uoz 20 broad generalizability because of this highly select student sample. it is growing in acceptance. Perry interviewed college students from the classes of 1958. 1962. and 1963 at the end of each of their four academic years. Using an open question format in the interviews. Perry gleaned changes in student perceptions over time which presented an interesting pattern of development. The basic movement was from a simplistic dualism toward varying degrees of relativism. The resulting scheme (1968) was somewhat less delineated than the work of the cognitive developmentalists. but very useful for understanding how students interpreted their educational experiences (Figure 7). For students at the lower developmental levels (1-3). findings indicated that they expected educators to have all the answers. When answers were not forthcoming. the resulting ambiguity was highly threatening. Students at the middle developmental levels (4-5). tended not to believe in set answers. They had let go of their confidence in authoritatively correct answers. yet were not able to conceive of post-relativistic commitment. They were consequently. caught in epistemological despair. Students at the higher developmental levels (6-9). discovered that personal commitment was an effective avenue to personal meaning. Even for the unanswered questions. commitment. even tentative. was a helpful way of organizing oneself in the ongoing search (Figure 8). > DEVELOPMENT ) R e Dualism Modified 1 by Multiplicity a Evolving Commitments 4a t / \ Positionl ——-—>2 -—->3 and/or 5 -—->6—-)7 ——-.*8 -—-—)9 —-—> \ /. 4b 1. v i s m Retreat Escape (Perry. 1981. p. 80) Figure8 Perry's Model of Cognitive and Ethical Development Position 1 Transition Position 2 Transition Position 3 Transition Position 4a Transition (and/or) Transition Posttion 4b Transition Position 5 Transition Position 6 Transition Position 7 Transition Position 8 Transition Position 9 21 Authorities know, and if we uoxk hard, read every word, and learn night Ans-e:s, all will be well. But what about thosetJtners I hear «beat? 10ns? And Uncertainties? with each other or don't seem to know. and some give us prob- lems instead of Answers. True Authoritirs mUat be Right, the others axe frauds. We remain Right. Others mu.t be different and kiong. dood Authorities give us problems so we can teeth to tinJ the Right Answer by our own intiegentitnt than “it. But even Good Authorities admit they don't know all the answers yet) Then some uncertainties and different opinions a'v real and ligitimate temporarily, ever rcr Authorities. on them to get to the Truth. But there are so many things they don't know the Answers tot And they won't for a long time. where Authorities don't know the Right Answers, everyOne has a right to his own opinion; no one is wrong! But some of my friends ask me to support my opinions with facts and reasons. Then what right have They to grade us? About what? In certain courses Authorities are not Asking for the Right Answer; They want us to think about things in a certain way, supporting opinion with data. That's what they grade us on. But this “way" seems to work in nest courses, and even out- side them. Then all thinking must be like this, even for Them. Every~ thing is relative but not equally Valid. You have to under- stand how each context HOtfib. Theories are rot Tiuth but metaphors to interpret data with. You haae to think JDOuC your thinking. But if everything is relative, am I relative too? know I'm making the Right Choice? I see I'm going to have to make my own decisions in an uncer- tain world with no one to tell me I'm Right. I'm lost if I don't. When I decxde on my career (or marriage or values) everything will straighten cut. Well, I've made my first Commitment. Why didn't that settle everything? I've made several commitments. I've got to balanCe them- how many, how deep? how certain, how tentatiVe? Things are getting contradictory. I can't make logical sense out of life's dilemmas. This how life will be. I must be wholehearted while ten- tative, fight for my Values yet respect others, believe my deepest values right yet be ready to learn 1 see that I shall be retraCinq this whole JOJLney over and over - Let. X nope, more wisely. Ani differtrt )pin- They ' 1c wurkltlu how can I Figure 7 Some of our own Authorities iisigree 1:13.}: Basic Duality mum-.1 mt... Pt¢°lngitimotc thllylivlty 2.1.:in .1 l r... t ._- Multig‘iieity (sniipslsm) Cot-rd i mitt: Ruletivism suburdinatc Relativism (contextual) generalized Coannitmum foreseen lili 1. 131 Count uncut Orientation i n ("awn tments Evolving Ctaofltments A Description of Perry's Scheme of Cognitive and Ethical Development 22 Perry's key terms are as follows: Dualism: Division of meaning into two realms--Good versus Bad. Right versus Wrong, We versus They. All that is not Success ‘is Failure. Right answers exist somewhere for every problem. and authorities know them. Knowledge is quantitative. Multiplicity: Diversity of opinion and values 'is recognized as legitimate in areas where right answers are not yet known. Opinions remain atomistic without pattern or system. No judgments can be made among them so "everyone has a right to his own opinion; none can be called wrong." Relativism: Diversity of opinion. values. and judgment is derived from coherent sources. evidence. logic. systems. and patterns allowing for analysis and comparison. Some opinions may be found worthless. while there will remain matters about which reasonable people will reasonably disagree. Knowledge is qualitative. dependent on contexts. Commitment: A process of affirmation. choice. or decision (career. values. politics. personal relationship) made in the awareness of Relativism. Agency is experienced as within the individual. Temporizing: Postponement of movement for a year or more. Escape: Alienation. abandonment of responsibility. Exploitation of Multiplicity and Relativism for avoidance of Commitment. Retreat: Avoidance of complexity and ambivalence by regression to Dualism colored by hatred of otherness. Ego Development Loevinger (l976) has developed one of the most inclusive theories of personality development to date (Table 2). Her research involved extensive clinical studies and the use of a projective test. The approach was highly descriptive rather than philosophical or analytical. but is being validated by a growing body of similar >ua>auuonno .oaoun vaoun .>uw=vansa new noduaqusu .mcuouumm xo~aeou .auaxoam uaou Hesumuucou vomoouucu 0500990 can anuOua uo coauucauuao "vn4 veacuouuam no out“ .xuaxoamEou donuauocou xuquwamauasz moSUAau .momxu locuoum .>uaudamsau Haaumoocou sodas“ Icou «anuawucou .uc«m>uoouuum xu‘ucoou unn< uxoucuu Houuon a“ “Mom .ucoa-fluuamuuaom .cOAu umuucou 0H0» .uo«>onon uo nodummauu ~00~U0~0£u>ma .HMUuoodocu>nm can “no oawo~o«m>£m uo cauuuuouuau .mmaaoOu vo>9>cou >«vu>«> nouno Baum yuan manna no “cauoaucououwflv .meuHQOMQ ~1»90m .ucoamvo>oo "vom coqmmoumxo .wUAauu .muccao>o~nua .uuoanuu auaom .u04>ocon uOu uo>uu nos .mvc«aoou vouoaucououuau Aoamo>y muwuacnuuommo .acom Icon .nEoHnoua .ucosunano< u0a>on son .mmcwaovm Macon .>uaaan oouauuua A6400» .oucuumomad «ouuuou .oooucm5uu .muc«nu .munmuz .oflnzouu .c04u00u0»muuawm o>Hmmouuoc van ”on -xmm xannuuvmmo .mvcflaoom nfiavom .mu-v~ .aa .onodw unusa>ooa .~o>e~ maoa>oua can on oc«>~aao acaumauumou cap 0» conuauoa c4 weave gun<: "who: >uu -Hasv4>dvc~ uo weanmauonu ”nu4 oucoucomovuouca .meocOuno uOu uuoauom unv< Baanoum «occuu -050 no no oucuocomoo "604 couuau nucaeeou cud: vacuoocou .Hoauae .oflnumcommou .o>amcouCu ocwmucs .maoum Ou cofluuaou cw «Ann uo chord 004: mmaouuauuomam .OCamco«on and) vvcuoucou o>fiu omuaoamxo .o>«uoasmwcoe .>uo: . o>auou -4oamxo .ucovcvmuv .ma>aouoz odnnc4duunc: uo caduauucacou .nuu»~ucou MOCCa no 0:4aaucouwz ”find ceauoumNOu .wuowc henna ocuuufiaucou nu“) ocunou unv< >ua ndonmu>avca you uuommum "ova maunaq can mamov fiuouaoon .noucoav nomcou new uaaav .EmaUauauu luavm .mnuoocaum vouoaau>01uavm macaw .mEuoc mo conuaaucououuuo mods» mcaxooun new uaaav .ofiacm .moaau Hmcuouxo cu >u«8u0ucou Emacs» uuoamo .oEdan uo noduvnuaoc nuouxo .uzmsnu muaon no noun cow» unaauuUu uo uaou .mmoco>anasmau voumuoouun msoeocound uaumudasva>avcm mucuuco«0mcou ~0u03uuuou0umuuu-m u>NmvaGH oH>um o>fiuucvnu acoHuausuuouum nDOWUmcou ucmsaoFm>mo uabum AnaemuumuoucH ucoemo«o>oo Mouuauanu «cuucou onasaeH omm mo agomzp m.gmmcw>m04 N wFQMH cumum 24 research. Essentially. Loevinger has taken a holistic approach to the study of human development. Such an integrative theory assimilates findings from moral development, cognitive development, and socialization and personality studies. Her thesis has been that there is a core process by which an individual understands the self and the world. Loevinger used the term "ego'I (not in a psychoanalytic sense) to denote her central way of sense-making. Ego development is the mental process by which an individual creates and maintains a consistent frame of reference. Loevinger's theory has also been called social development theory or maturity theory because of the interpersonal and interactive nature of its development. Loevinger has envisioned ego development as the context in which other lines of development have their expression in the personality. Chickering (1983) suggests five implications of Loevinger's findings: l. Development in the cognitive and affective domains is inextricably linked. Full development in one area is difficult to achieve without concomitant development in the other major area. 2. Learning and human development are additive. Learning always occurs in the context of our past history. personal characteristics, and current motives; it is always based on current knowledge and abilities. 3. There is a substantial range of individual differences in developmental stages. Therefore. a variety of approaches to learning is appropriate for individualized education. self-paced instruction. mastery learning, and contract learning. 4. Being better educated. more complex. more competent at a higher stage of development does not automatically lead to greater happiness. better adjustment. or sounder mental health. 5. It demands . . . the creation of a human lifelong learning system that involves the collaborative efforts not only of educators and employers. but also of 25 libraries. public and private television. newspapers and magazines. churches. and community organizations (pp. l4-l5). F_aith Development Fowler (l975) has discerned a pattern of faith development in his detailed interviews with over 100 subjects. which has resulted in a six-stage theory of faith development. Fowler's theory closely parallels the cognitive development models. The movement likewise is from simplicity to complexity. from external/literalistic expressions to internal/principled values. Stage One:INTUITIVE/PROJECTIVE FAITH Fowler characterized stage one as a childhood stage covering ages 2-7 when the rise of imagination is in full power. Often images which occur during stage one have life-long effects. either positively or negatively. Fowler described this period in a child's life as a time when the mind is religiously pregnant. Stage Two: MYTHIC/LITERAL FAITH Fowler describes this stage as the stage of stories. The child can now create and retell powerful stories. The symbolism is largely taken at face value and interpreted with a quality of literalness. Stage Three: SYNTHETIC/CONVENTIONAL FAITH Fowler identified stage three as beginning with the teen years but may characterize adult experience as well. Essentially. it is during stage three that identity is formulated. It is a synthetic stage in that it pulls together one's valued images and values, conceptualizes God as an extention of existing relationships. and struggles for a sense of worth and selfhood. Stage Four: INDIVIDUATIVE/PROJECTIVE FAITH Fowler suggests that this stage four may begin in the late teens, but more probably in the early 20s. Some individuals may not encounter it. however. until the late 305. During this stage. the individual steps outside of the circle of interpersonal relationships which have sustained his life previously. Now comes the task of sorting out who he is apart 26 from the group and supporting structures. The concern here is boundaries. where the self ends and others begin. Stage four is very much concerned with a "fit" between an authentic self and a set of ideological commitments. Stage Five: CONJUNCTIVE FAITH What has been made clear in stage four is now made more permeable and porous. Not only is there an awareness of personal identity. but there is much more about personal identity which is now owned but not yet understood. There is a readiness for a relationship with God in all of His mystery. There is likewise a correlative readiness for a relationship with persons and groups very different from ourselves. Stage five is a time in which paradox is alright. and truth lives within a broad context of paradoxical tension. Stage Six: UNIVERSALIZING FAITH In the highest stage. the individual experiences a shift from the self as the center of experience and meaning. The center now becomes a participation in God. at home in a commonwealth of being. For the Christian this is living in the Kingdom of God. These are liberating people. subversively liberating. People like Martin Luther King. Jr.. Thomas Merton. Mother Teresa. Dietrich Bonhoeffer. and Ghandi all became vibrant and powerful selves in negating themselves and affirming self only for the sake of affirming a higher meaning. Fowler is careful not to limit his theory of faith development to just a religious frame of reference. Faith is defined in the broadest of terms. and refers to one's orientation to and conception of the ultimate determining realities in one's life. Faith may apply to religious. philosophical. or political concerns. Theoreticarl Correlates As these several stage theories have been considered. it has become apparent that they appear to be isomorphically parallel. distinct yet similar. Correlational studies are very difficult in developmental theory because of the continual refinement of scoring systems which affect reliability. These models can 27 m mPQMH vouauooucH amfi>wuoaox ea ocaumoe >u3m5ba>a§3 ma§COua< can: ucaum «5.3: 3:95 pal—cu namonuéz uo mcanmwuocu onwuwaoauo>uca owunwaaavfi>wocu no oooum "mun cofiuwnOm o>auoavao macauuaou >Eoc0u=< nuuauucou Hoauom amw>aum~om canwuaoa ~H< nan uuomuom o>auuahcou uncuucuwoncou um mouum mum ceauwnom .n.uoao aqauom occauQUacaaBoo new avawuu wcw>ao>na cuoucou «anus: uncauaaom .~ .oHnaucoanom o>uuuonoum uuouuo was 344: >uuuwamwuaax uncauouomo .u>«ncouCH \o>aua=vw>wvcH ouu3cw unecou«z coucowuo on» oca>~o>cw Huwoauuomam Ha>ouma< oucfluuwooHuum occauuaox .a .UCAQHo: HocOauco>cou :xom coco: >uwufiaaauanz unawuauomo .chOCOAom \owuonucxm umMBHOucou “n ovaum ”n cowuwuom aneuom unooone ououocou aawomu oanwuu0>o¢ .n u>«u«0amxm «oucoasuuacH audauao occauouoao .o>wuaasmwcuz .>uo3 ~uuou«A\0wnuxz o>wuuououm uaom “n ooaum "N newuauom ououucoo accuuaOAuwunoau o>wuwo~mxu Acouuooouou .H .ucuvcvaoo o>auuonoum ucoennwcam\vuo30m Inwauaa "cOwuauomo .UCw>«ouom \o>wu«sucu o>aaaamau ”A coaum "A acauwnom ououucou uncano~o>oo unvamoHo>oo ucoeuo~u>oo Anna: a acounum a ucoenoHo>oo HacOmuomuoucH ucoamoHo>oo nuwnm ucoaaoHo>oo com o>wuwcmou «>«uwcoou o>wuwcoou uoo:«>oeq uuazom noocw>oog ouonanox ahuom uomnwm ucmsqo—w>mo agomspummmum we mpmcoz mo comwcmaeou 28 be said to be related. though not developmentally identical (Table 3). It is observed in the comparison offered that cognitive development is both the threshhold and ceiling for other related forms of development. Thinking must be at least functioning at the concrete operations level for other kinds of development to begin. Developmental research has significant implications for higher education. though general practices expose a very large gap between themselves and the research. Nonetheless. several inferences might be drawn from the comparison of the several stage-theory models presented: l. Most traditional college students age 18-22. can be located at stages 3-5 on most of the schemes. At this level. cognitive development moves into the sphere of formal operations in a normative sense. Self-awareness becomes primary in a psychological and social sense. 2. Stage advancement is almost always made via a sense of disequilibrium. As dissonance is set up. the individual is motivated to resolve it by moving to the more adequate structures of the next higher stage. The college years are filled with disequilibrium. Students leave the home base. They face a new set of social freedoms and responsibility. They are exposed to new value systems and world views. All the potential for stage-theory development across a number of domains is present. If there is not a sufficient level of disequilibrium. students remain content. then become increasingly apathetic and bored. If there is too much disequilibrium present. students may become overwhelmed. discouraged. and regress developmentally. 3. Deliberate learning experiences which challenge the inadequacy of conventional thinking (stage 3-4). are essential to the development of maturity in most models presented. 4. When individuals reach the level of stage 5 on most schemes. decisions are primarily internally guided. 29 5. There seems to be a "curricular fit" for experiential learning programs at the college level. There need to be legitimate experiences which enable a person to: objectively examine one's own cultural context; develop skills of self-reflection; process different points of view without confusing these with personal positions; identify one's personal value system; develop respect for others; and define the boundaries of one's own self and potential limits. APPROPRIATENESS OF EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING PROGRAMS The American commitment to education has traditionally been a commitment to knowledge with a purpose. Traditional approaches to higher education have stressed the classroom as the place of learning. the teacher as the leader. the written word as the transmitter and essence of thought and ideas. and the course as the organizing structure for teaching and learning (Valley. l976). Although this has been a successful approach for some. it has also served to create an unfortunate learner passivity. Children are conditioned into attitudes of docility. compliance. and dependence in the formative years of education. For most students. the classroom is a familiar environment in which all the needed information is provided in lectures and the textbook. or through the reserve shelf in the library. The problems that are assigned in the course usually have predetermined correct answers. and neither the problems nor their solutions impinge directly on the student's lives or others. In coursework. students have little need to develop interpersonal skills or to learn to work through emotionally difficult relationships (Duley. l98l ). Counterbalancing Traditional Edugtion Experiential education has the potential to counterbalance the learner- passivity encouraged in students by the present educational system (Sexton. 1976). Tumin (l976) does not see experiential education as the answer to the present educational woes. but he does insist that many important things are not learned well 30 in traditional schools. and many important things are not learned at all. These basic weaknesses affect virtually all students. Experiential education complements the traditional approaches in its emphasis upon the theory/action connection (Figure 9). INFORMATION ASS IMILATION EXPERIENTIAL Strengths: Highly efficient, Strengths: Highly effective,- rapid transfer through symbolic action comes first, intr1n51c medium, quantity predicted motivation, contact With direct realities, integrative, connects theory to action Weaknesses: Less effective, Weaknesses: Less efficient, students must know symbolic time consuming limitations medium, confusion of meanings, on the predictability and action comes last, little quantity of learning intrinsic motivation, unclear connection between theory and action Figure 9 Contrasted Approaches to Learning It is the central role which experience provides which brings it to the heart of the curriculum that sets experiential learning in a different category. Yet. just "experience" is no guarantor of learning. Wallace (l977) suggests that experiential education must be characterized by four objectives: l) planned; 2) affective; 3) individual; and 4) thoughtfully evaluated. From a program planning perspective. educators must design ways in which the students will be prepared for the experience. activities which will facilitate certain kinds of experience. and ways in which the experience might be meaningfully evaluated. Wilson (l982). however. cautions that learning cannot always be easily explained nor planned. One of the advantages of experiential education is that it expands the range of learning options for students. This becomes particularly significant for 31 students who are less academically inclined. Action has the power to motivate these learners. help them feel more comfortable in the learning process. and tap more deeply their own ailities (Tumin. 1976). The reason it is such a powerful motivator for learning is because it is success-oriented. meaningful. and real. It offers opportunities for real-life problem solving in which feedback is uncontrived and immediate. The results. likewise. are real physical and emotional consequences. Since experiential learning programs tend to involve students in the design. implementation and management of their own learning. they have a vested interest and are more internally motivated. Little (l98l) has identified l2 outcomes of experiential learning relating to student values. These outcomes for students include opportunities to do the following: 1. apply. integrate. and evaluate the body of knowledge and the method of inquiry of a discipline or field via firsthand participation; 2. acquire skills and values specific to a profession. occupation. social institution. or organization; 3. acquire and develop functional skills and attitudes necessary for effective adult life; 4. develop the ability to learn in a self-directed fashion; 5. develop and use an ethical perspective or stance and develop moral reasoning or judgment in ethically complex situations; 6. test careers by exploration or confirmation of career choices and gain documented work experiences; 7. become responsible citizens of the community by identifying issues of social concern and developing skills for citizen participation; 8. have access to knowledge not easily attained through classroom instruction (such as exercise of political power); 9- identify Problems for further study; 32 Table 4 Student Values and Experiential Learning Outcomes Value Saught by Students Came to college ‘to get a better job“ (76 percent of freshmen; Astin. 1981). “Have a specific goal in mind after graduation‘ (85 percent of res ndents; Roizen. fulton. and lrow. 1978. p. l05 . feel it is important “to be well off financially“ (two thirds of respondents; Roizen. fulton. and lrow. l978. p. lOb). Hant autonomy in deciding what is learned i es ecially true for adult learners; Cross. 978 . Hant 'to be an authority in my field“ as a major life)objective (72.9 percent of freshmen; Astin. 198) . Hant ‘to help others in difficulty“ as a major life)objective (62.9 percent of freshmen; Astin. l98l . Hant 'to formulate the values for my life" ( 62 percent; Astin. I974). Uant to finish school in a reasonable amount of time. heed money to pay for educational and living expenses. Hant access to opportunity for status. careers. education. and so forth. especially if this acc- ess has traditionally been denied. Hant flexibility in scheduling tine for education (especially true for adult learners. (Kendall, 1983. PP. 30-31) ".DL-‘gr—JE ‘o’r"cb‘.i}.’r1.'c‘.ft‘c’ of Dies .5 Va l'u’t-"s' wi ii.“ Outcomes and features 0| tagiei‘iciitlal train- ing Programs -w.---a~-.—- »._... .—-.. . u - highly congruent with outt0mes 2. 3. and a. lhe contacts nude thrOugh experiential learning upp- ortunities also support this goal highly congruent with Outcones l. 2. c and a. Congruent with cutcOues 2 and 6. In addition. many argue that graduates with experience have higher earning potential in the first job after graduation. Highly congruent with outcune 4. to outcones l. 3. 7. 8. and 9. Also relevant highly congruent with cutc0ues l. 2. 6. 8. and 9 highly congruent with Outcomes 7 and )0. Man. service learning programs stress the importance of a synergistic relationship between Service outcomes and educatioiial goals. highly congruent with ootcoues 3. 4. 5. and I. Must experiential learning programs do not delay graduation. however. some programs do requifc more time than classroom-based curricula. highly congruent with potential finantial assis- tance from experiential leaining. Partitipatiun in experiential learning can hurt it the work is unpaid and takes large auwunts ui tluw. lhis factor needs careful attention from faculty and Supervisors in order to mate caperlcntldl learn- ing available to low-and middle°intone students. Outcone ll. congruent with all outcomes. Accomplishment of each Outcone increases the probability of access to future opportunities. Can be congruent or incungruent with the fea- tures of eaperiential learning programs. this is an important factor to include in the can- sideration of fair exchange anong the three primary parties. 33 ll]. provide service to the community and contributing to the solution of social problems; ll. earn money to pay for living expenses or to support one's education; 12. have a stimulating and valid change of pace from classroom learning. A number of interesting correlations have been identified between student learning expectations (Little. l983. pp. 28-29) and commonly experienced outcomes of experiential learning programs (Table 4). L__e_arning Shifts The college years are particularly strategic years for shifts in perception. In a seven-year longitutional study (Mentkowski and Strait. l983). evidence suggested students at Alverno College changed in the ways they perceived and understood learning. First. students changed from an understanding of learning as comprehending knowledge and content. to seeing "learning as a process". and developing a variety of ways of learning and ways of thinking. This was evidenced by a gradual upward movement on the Perry positions of intellectual and ethical development. Second. students shifted in learning styles from concrete and reflective/observing to abstract and active/experimenting as measured by Kolb's l=e_arn_irm Style Inventory (l976). Movement was toward a more balanced pattern. Students shifted in seeing learning in terms of "how to perform" which involved the process of thinking about "doing" and about the "context" of performance. At least three patterns of developmental change have been observed by Doherty. Mentkowski. and Conrad (1978) in the experiential curriculum: from particular skills toward integrated effectiveness. from "closed" to "open" learning situations. and from other-directed to self-directed learning. 34 Contributions to Higher Education The impact of experiential learning upon the field of education has opened up a new range of educational options. In a time of a shrinking pool of l8-22 year-old college students (23% l980-2000). new opportunities afforded by the development of new delivery systems have attracted a new. wide market of adult learners as well as provided new flexibility to traditional undergraduate programs. The contribution of experiential learning programs has made educational programs attractive in a number of distinctive ways (Weathersby and Henalt. l976): --They offer relevant. real-life experience which many students seek. --They are concerned with the affective process in learning. --They allow greater freedom and responsibility on the part of the student for his or her own education. --They are more flexible and can accommodate a variety of learning styles. --They provide financial incentive for those students who could not otherwise afford to spend a large amount of time in a traditional institution. --They provide advantages through wages or stipends paid to them as a part of their work placement. --They allow earning while learning increases the attractiveness of these kinds of study programs. --They open opportunities to older people. women. low-income groups. and minorities. giving them access to higher education (pp. 144-147). A by-product of the knowledge explosion has been a proliferation of new SPlatialities of knowledge. Many of these have come about through cross-disciplinary BXPerimentation. Sub-disciplines have combined with other sub-disciplines to address a new need in society. For instance. political science and business combined to form public administration. They then combined with recreation (a sub-disciplne 35 cowuuosum meucmwcmaxm cw mcouomu m:_uo_ucmcmmmwo OH massed 3.33 .0663 new 39.00 oscusw 3552.5 Ac_e> .a .kaa .eoeeoa a xu_=ov mciooz mccow moucoom 0cm m:< .o c9920 c2320 co=< w ucmcui to so :25 390.5 cm 59. 202:5 9.: .3 02023 coon mm: 9.6.. mi... .202 -i.l..I.i I! . 3260.00 cesmunoo 35:3 0005 23.3 BEE 6:30:50 9:009. 390.com 5:32... 2262? no.1 D m _ _ E9090 9.53% o. ouco:oo..u .o 93:955.”. 0 Succeed; 0E9. 5.; 3639939: £3.82 9 39080209.. u coming: :64 m _ 25035 .0 .33 u_Eoumo< .mEaao . _ 59.25:. 30.00.0213 ,uozmcmb .mmucEQi. Suzi u ill! .coEc.o>00 o moocomm 9:90.... o {5305 new $059.6 0 p. _ L L Eutectic“. 690:6 335.30 a M a _ _ fl _ 923 one 2...; «00:85 .309305 .0 8.59.23 0 . I 5339 wczuaocou . , s _ c. >563 ocacmcu o _ _ .2903 ocscum 0 223503 235 u c2599... .350 n "cquQosoo a ceamsfiaoueaw m .coxo 99m: 8066 8539 Pod 3:0; Eoocmmflu ES, .cwtauco? 997.com. wEfS. owiouwd Eco mocc. manEmu.mo oE._.=:u 206nm mczflfiocozé Saginaw on. .0 320200 NCUL‘ 69:32.". .0 9332 m 36 of physical education) to form recreation administration. These new sub-disciplines need internship experiences to help define and interpret them (Little. l98l). Experiential internships have served to bring the issues of education and society closer together in the learning process. DIVERSITY OF EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING PROGRAMS As the American society has become increasingly information-oriented. significant groups of people have found it difficult. if not impossible. to gain access to formal educational systems. The entrenched 19th century model of education was too inflexible. At least four groups (Houle. l976) were largely excluded from the system: 1) people who gained their experience before they acquired their theory (acquired theory inductively); 2) people who preferred to guide their own learning; 3) people who wanted guided but personalized programs of study; and 4) people attempting to build a learning plan of life-experience equivalencies. Experiential learning programs became a viable response to this market. Consequently. programs began to be developed which conformed to the shape of student needs rather than pre-determined structures (Table l). Faculty within experiential education have redefined their roles more as experience facilitators than the traditional roles of information disseminators. Experiential educational programs have continued to expand in innovative ways bringing the worlds of work and education closer together and redefining learning in non-collegiate settings. INTERCULTURAL EXPERIENTIAL LEARNING PROGRAMS The specific additive of the intercultural dimension to experiential learning may become a potent agent of the kinds of student development envisioned in the aims of American higher education. 37 Brief History of Intercultural Studies American students began to sojourn abroad in significant numbers with the development of junior-year-abroad programs between World Wars I and II. These sojourns were viewed in the tradition of the British Continental Tour and seemed to require no additional justification. Following World War II. intercultural understanding assumed new importance. The American awareness of global inter-relatedness spawned an explosion of studies and projects focused upon functional efficiency. Most research in the era of the 1950's was dominated by business and government. In the early 1960's. researchers began to examine the factors which influenced intercultural success or failure. One of the first studies to examine interpersonal effectiveness was the Carnegie Project (Cleveland. Mangone. and Adams. l960). Oberg's descriptive studies (l958) focussed on adjustment dynamics and introduced the term "culture shock." Hall (l959) contributed to the field with a study of non-verbal cross-cultural differences. During the 1960's. a wide variety of non-traditional college programs were developed in response to student discontent with the traditional university model of education. Students responded strongly to the new opportunities for intercultural learning programs and community-based experiential programs. The Peace Corps contributed much of the research during this period. The Peace Corps studies attempted to predict intercultural success by examining personality traits. small group training. background analysis. situational factors. total-person analysis. and a wide variety of variables. The results of the Peace Corps studies were largely inconclusive. but did produce the development of a variety of audio-visual and experiential training resources. As funding diminished for the Peace Corps. other studies were undertaken by the business community and U.S. military (Gudykunst. et al.. l977; Tucker. 1973). The research of the l970's has been primarily concerned with "adaptiveness" and understanding the dynamics of cultural integration. 38 There have been numerous difficulties with the quality of the early research in intercultural or "cross-cultural" studies. Many of the studies are bivariate-relationship studies or anecdotal treatments of the subject rather than comprehensive multivariate empirical examinations. Consequently. much of the early literature is non-analytical. non-rigorous. and lacks a solid conceptual base. In addition. many of these studies are characterized by a macroscopic perspective. dealing with problems at a general level. using the group as the lowest unit of analysis. These seem to overlook the most critical level. the interpersonal level. Finally. much of the research has been conducted in an isolated way. unrelated to the larger framework of intercultural learning and other fields of research. The difficulty of controlling the variables in the early research has somewhat reduced its credibility and created a piecemeal picture of a very complex issue. Currently. the movement in the research seems to be toward a more inclusive view (holistic) of the intercultural experience as a developmental process. V_a;ue of Intercultural Experiential Learning Prom If the concept of culture includes all the dynamic interactions of a people. in addition to just an understanding of language. values. norms. and artifacts. then the concept of intercultural experience will involve what happens to people who represent different cultures when they interact. To program this kind of dynamic is to invite a vast array of processes to take place. The Council on International Educational Exchange (CIEE) and Michigan State University issued a joint statement (l979) describing some of these processes: Cross-cultural experiential learning can be defined as the acquisition of skills. knowledge. and competencies through a learner's contact with and reflection upon the direct realities of a host society . . . . Cross-cultural experiential education is pre-eminently integrative in nature. The student connects with the host culture at all levels of his being. Such programs offer opportunities for the acquisition of factual knowledge. for synthesizing data. for determining patterns of meaning. for developing powers of independent observation. and for the application of knowledge and understanding to the immediate situations at hand. At the same time. the student is provided 39 opportunities for greater self-confidence. awareness. and understanding of his or her own culture and values; for the testing of effective patterns in interacting with people and situations; and a corresponding potential for the development of personal maturity and capacities in the learning process itself (”Statement of the Task Group . . .." 1979). Most formal educational programs have been reluctant to incorporate cross-cultural experiential education into the received curriculum. Of those who have. many deal only with marginal samplings of cross-cultural experiential education or else restrict it to highly defined traditional academic objectives. Many institutions misperceive cross-cultural experiential learning as being either haphazard insights of random experience or simply studying the traditional subjects in a different cultural setting. In contrast to the institutional indifference to experiential learning. college students indicated a different attitude. The Carnegie Surveys (1975-76) of 25.000 faculty. 25.000 undergraduates. and 25.000 graduate students indicated that although only 13% of the undergraduates had received credit for experiential learning. 55% of the students were interested in being able to do so. Until recently. student enthusiasm for the value of an intercultural experience was more of an intuitive rationale than an established. empirical fact. It has been assumed to be important for self-development. however. that has been difficult to measure. Although there is a large body of research on cross-cultural interaction. relatively little research has focused on the impact of cross-cultural experience. Hull and Lemke (1975) found that. in a survey designed to indicate whether students had gained a better understanding of their home culture. 84% of the students involved in an intercultural experience responded positively. 80% of the students involved in an experiential placement in their home country responded positively. and only 53% responded positively who had remained in the traditional campus-bound program. In the same study. 87‘]. of the off-campus students indicated that their experience 40 forced them to question their values. A survey of 387 institutions (Baker. 1978) found that 77% of the student teachers were in fact working with culturally different students. In Abrams' (1978) research which looked at long-term results of students in the Antioch Education Abroad program. he noted that students reported that it was largely their contact with the culture outside the classroom that had the most significant effect upon them within their study-abroad experience. Henson's (1979) survey of 301 undergraduates at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign who had cross-cultural educational experiences indicated that 67% rated their out-of-class experiences to have been of greatest value to them. and an additional 33% rated both experiences to be of equal value! Hull (1981) suggests that formal cross-cultural experiential learning programs have extraordinary educational value and at times ought to be ends in themselves within the undergraduate curriculum. He asserts that they complement and strengthen the traditional goals of liberal arts education. They are not to duplicate educational experiences. delivery systems. or learning activities available at the home campus (Harrison and Hopkins. 1967. APPENDIX B). Rather. Hull suggests five goals: (1) personal awareness (a context of distinctly different cultural values); (2) development of personal competence (learning to cope with change); (3) understanding and acceptance of other peoples and other ways (being among them); (4) perceptual knowledge (dealing with stereotypes. new ways of thinking and feeling); and (5) practical understanding (testing theories and concepts for use). A major value of experiential learning is what Schein and Bennis (1965) have called the "meta-goals” of learning. This is the value of "learning how to learn." Aside from specific content. this involves the development of problem-solving processes. Varieties of IntercultiLal Programs Intercultural learning programs have evolved into a number of specialized educational options. The most traditional program is simply a study abroad 41 program. Most international study programs involve consortium arrangements whereby students enroll in a foreign institution and take traditional courses which are then credited at the home campus. This is particularly popular for language study and specialized appreciation courses. Some students choose intercultural contexts in which to do field research. such as historical research. archaeological research. or ethnographic research in anthropology. Advanced students occasionally qualify for honors programs in foreign research centers which are often tied to a specific project. Senior students can choose to do internships in international contexts: teaching. engineering. and medicine have been areas frequently used. Service experience programs. such as the Peace Corps. have involved thousands of young people in community-action projects in developing countries. Cultural immersion programs have used the intercultural context as an environment for personal development often focusing upon attitude change and values clarification and development. By far the most populated program is the travel study approach in which a small group travels together to various locations which relate to a particular topic. Students generally are involved in background reading prior to the travel. listen to lectures on site. and write a major report at the conclusion of the tour. Nearly all the program options can range in length from one month to a year. and carry equivalent levels of academic credit as might be earned should a student be involved in full-time study on the home campus. Factors of Intarcultural Effectiveness A major difficulty in intercultural research has been the problem of definition and control of variables. This has posed the chicken and the egg dilemma: are the outcomes of intercultural experiential learning programs the result of variables which define the individual prior to the experience or variables which are inherent in the experience itself? Some of the prior variables mentioned in the literature are: age and sex. prior intercultural experience. the nature of 42 that experience. skill level. personality structure. language ability. ability to take initiative and deal with ambiguous situations. amount of orientation training. and status within the individual's own culture. Some of the cultural variables which have been identified are; length and nature of the experience. the need for language acquisition in the new culture. the "strangeness" of the new culture. transcultural common interests. the political context. the degree of value difference and world view in the new culture. the presence of cultural mediators. the status of the people most frequently encountered in the new culture. mobility. and the degree of cultural immersion (Pearson. 1981). Several interesting phenomena have come to characterize intercultural adjustment. The first is "culture shock" (Oberg. 1958). This process is described by Oberg as an anxiety which results from losing one's sense of when to do what and how. The progression he describes is: (l) euphoria; (2) hostility; (3) understanding and humor; (4) acceptance; and (5) reverse shock upon reentry. The symptoms (Brislin. 1981) may include: excessive preoccupation with the cleanliness of one's drinking water; food and surroundings; great concern over minor pains; excessive anger over delays and other minor frustrations; a fixed idea that people are taking advantage of or cheating one; reluctance to learn the language of the host country; a feeling of hopelessness; and a strong desire to associate with persons of one's own nationality. Victims of culture shock also experience a decline in inventiveness. spontaneity. and flexibility to the extent that it interferes with their normal behavior. Culture shock does have some positive aspects to it which are sometimes overlooked. Adler (1975) suggests seven ways in which it may facilitate learning: (1) Learning involves change and movement from one cultural frame of reference to another. This provides changes in the cultural landscape. (2) Individuals undergo a highly personal experience in culture shock which is of special significance to themselves. 43 (3) Change becomes provocative. Individuals are forced into some form of introspection and self-examination. (4) Adjustment is extreme in its ups and downs. (5) Confrontation forces personal investigation of relationships. (6) New ideas force behavioral experimentation. This becomes a trial-and-error process until appropriate behavioral responses emerge. (7) Individuals have at their disposal an unending source of diversity with which they can compare and contrast their own previous experiences (pp.13-24). Ruben and Kealey (1979) have found tentative evidence to suggest that. in some cases. the persons who will eventually be the most effective may undergo the most intense culture shock during transition. After studying the adjustment patterns of sojourners over time. Gullahorn and Gullahorn (1963) suggested that the ups and downs of satisfaction followed a predictable pattern. This became a widely recognized phenomena labeled the "W-shaped hypothesis" (Figure 11). HIGH MODE RATE \/ A B C D E F Beginning Middle End of After Return Sojourn Home (Gullahorn 8: Gullahorn. 1963. PP.33-47) Figure 11 W-Shaped Hypothesis 44 The W-Shaped Hypothesis suggested a six-point adjustment pattern (Table 5). Trifonovitch (1977) described point A as the "honeymoon stage". during which the individual is excited about all the new experiences. Point B. however. begins when frustrations arise at not being able to solve problems in familiar ways. This typically results in hostility. At point C. an individual's sense of humor returns and the settling in adjustment is made. At point D. an individual begins to feel at home in the culture and is able to meet the day-to-day challenges successfully. At point E. reverse culture shock sets in. People return to their home culture and feel confused at not being able to fit in. At point F. the readjustment is finally made. Table 5 W-Shaped Hypothesis of Satisfaction A B C D II F “Honeymoon ”Hostility“ “Humor" "At Home“ "Reverse “Readjustmcnt Stage” Culture (Trifonovitch, Shock“ 1977) Enthusiasm Frustration, Beginning Comfortable, Confused and with new inability of adjust- coping with alienated experiences to solve ment problems at reentry problems efficiently to home in familiar culture ways (Trifonovitch. 1977. pp.18-22) In an analysis of long-term experiences of foreign students and sojourning professors. Klineberg and Hull (1979) could find no evidence for a W-shaped curve. The downswing from A to B could not always be found. which of course eliminated the upswing to C. This finding seemed to be at first confusing. There are several possible explanations. however. The Gullahorn and Gullahorn study was conducted in the late 1950's and was widely known. The Klineberg and Hull study was conducted in the late 1970's. It is entirely possible that the earlier study had 45 affected the field of intercultural preparation so significantly that the downswing had been largely prevented. It is also possible that foreign hosts were much more accustomed to sojourners in the 1970's than in the 1950's. Studies have indicated a number of common barriers to intercultural effectiveness. Ethnocentricity is a major barrier. This is the tendency to view people unconsciously by comparing them to our standards instead of their own (Porter. 1972). As a response to this problem. Gudykunst. Hammer. and Wiseman (1977) suggested the development of an intermediary "third-culture" point of view. This point of view would be different from both one's own cultural perspective and from that of the new culture. The "third-culture" perspective is characterized by: (1) open-mindedness toward new ideas and experiences; (2) empathy toward people from other countries; (3) accuracy in perceiving differences and similarities between the host culture and one's own; (4) nonjudgmentalness; (5) astute non-critical observation of one's own and other's behavior; (6) relationship-building skills; and (7) a lack of ethnocentricity. Another major barrier to intercultural effectiveness is stereotyping. Allport (1958) developed the conception of an exaggerated belief associated with a category. This is observed in intercultural encounters in which an individual is lumped into a certain category simply because of certain characteristics. not because they have been considered on a differentiated basis. In stereotyping. the areas of greatest contrast are likely to be given the greatest prominence in the stereotype. Inaccurate and highly simplistic stereotypes obviously provide a rich source of misunderstanding at the interpersonal level. The inverse has also been demonstrated (Pool. 1965): the greater the degree of first-hand experiences gained in an intercultural relationship. the less prevalent becomes the actual stereotyping. 46 A variety of attitudinal and communigtional factors have been examined for their impact upon intercultural effectiveness. A great number of the earlier studies were concerned with identifying personality types which could be classified for predictive purposes in intercultural effectiveness. Many of the Peace Corps studies were of this nature. Current opinion however. is that personality is not very useful in predicting intercultural adjustment (Guthrie. 1975). Instead. clusters of variables have been identified through research in the areas of cross-cultural communication. social psychology. and intercultural education. A study by Cleveland. Mangone. and Adams (1960) suggests that technical skill. high motivation. cultural empathy. political sensitivity. organizational ability. and personality are the central elements of intercultural effectiveness. Guthrie and Zekrick (1967) described the ideal Peace Corps volunteer as sensitive to feelings of others. patient. soft-spoken. service-oriented. intelligent. initiative-taking. wishing to help others. tolerant of inconveniences. extroverted. and having a "live-and-let-live" attitude. Assertiveness. excessive frankness. argumentativeness. and derisiveness were characteristics to be avoided. Arensberg and Niehoff (1971) suggested a number of behaviors: be honest; begin discussions with subjects which will be least threatening to the other party; respect local values. conventions. taboos. and prejudices; maintain confidences; refrain from making judgments about nationals or their culture; avoid nostalgic comparisons between the new culture and one's own; avoid identification with particular social groups or factions; be aware of the potential for discrepancies between what people say and what they do; participate in the local culture; strive to earn some ceremonial social customs; and learn some of the national language. Barna (1972). identified barriers to intercultural effectiveness as; language. nonverbal communication. preconceptions and stereotypes. the tendency to evaluate or prejudge. and anxiety which inhibits expression. 47 Adler (l974) suggested three characteristics of the "multi-cultural man": (l) he is psycho-culturally adaptive; (2) he is ever undergoing personal transitions; and (3) he maintains indefinite boundaries of the self. Ruben (1976) stressed behavior as opposed to attitudes. intentions. beliefs. or values. He identified seven crucial communicative-behavior dimensions: (1) display of respect; (2) interaction posture (judgmentalness); (3) orientation to knowledge; (4) empathy; (5) role behavior; (6) interaction management. and (7) tolerance for ambiguity. Samovar and Porter (l976) identified eight variables relating to intercultural effectiveness: (1) ethnocentrism. world view. absolute values. stereotyping. prejudice; (2) social organization; (3) patterns of thought; (4) and role prescriptions; (5) language; (6) use and organization of space; (7) time conceptualization. and (8) nonverbal expression. Gudykunst. Hammer. and Wiseman (1977) concluded that successful intercultural adjustment depended upon three factors. each of which can be described in terms of behavioral components. The first was the ability to deal with psychological stress. Included were the abilities to deal with frustration. stress. anxiety. different political systems. pressure to conform. social alienation. financial difficulties. and interpersonal conflict. The second factor was the ability to communicate effectively. Included was a meaningful dialogue with other people. dealing with communication misunderstandings. and dealing effectively with different communication styles. The third factor was the ability to establish interpersonal relationships. Included were the abilities to develop satisfying interpersonal relationships. understand the feelings of other people. work effectively with other people. empathize with others. and deal effectively with different social customs. 48 Klemp (1977). identified three inclusive factors for intercultural success; cognitive and communication skills. empathy skills. and motivation. Since aproximately 80% of communication is nonverbal. these skills need to be finely tuned. Likewise fluency in verbal and written areas is important. Klemp cites four cognitive skills essential to success: (1) information-processing skills related to learning. recall. and forgetting; (2) conceptualizing skills which enable individuals to bring order to the informational chaos that constantly surrounds them; (3) the ability to understand many sides of a controversial issue; and (4) the ability to learn from experience . . . to translate observations from work experience into a theory that can be used to generate behavioral alternatives. The second factor of success was empathy. which included: positive regard for others; giving another person assistance; ability to control impulsive feelings of hostility or anger that makes others feel powerless and ineffective. The third factor was motivation. Klemp concluded that a person who takes a proactive stance. who initiates action and works to dissolve blocks to progress. will. with few exceptions. have the advantage over a person who is reactive. who does not seek new opportunities. but sees the world as a series of insurmountable obstacles. Duley (1977) has suggested seven skill areas for intercultural adaptation: (1) information source development; (2) cultural understanding; (3) interpersonal communication; (4) commitment to persons and relationships; (5) decision making; (6) self-understanding; and (7) self-reliance. Benson (1978) identified the major variables influencing intercultural effectiveness as language skills. communication skills. friendliness. attitudes. interactions. reinforcing activities. socially appropriate behaviors. job performance. satisfaction. and mobility. Kohl (l979). identified an extensive cluster of skills for overseas adjustment: sense of humor; ability to fail; low/goal orientation; tolerance for 49 ambiguity; open mindedness; non-judgmentalness; empathy; communicativeness; flexibility; curiosity; warmth in human relationships; motivation; self-reliance; strong sense of self; tolerance for differences; and perceptiveness. Hawes and Kealey (l981) studied technical advisors for the Canadian International Development Agency and found two factors which were among the best predictive measures of intercultural effectiveness: interpersonal skills and self-assertion. Implications for Program Design What are the implications of these research findings for program development? Despite the broad range of isolated factors researched. a gestalt is needed in order to begin the planning process. Pearson (1981) suggests five steps: 1. A clear conceptualization and delineation of objectives is necessary. 2. Some assessment of the individuals and the host culture environment is essential. 3. Appropriate orientation training should be provided. 4. Selected reinforcing activities should be planned. 5. On-site contact people should be located and learning situations planned. The development of orientation programs has an interesting history. It may be organized into three periods. In the early period of intercultural education. essentially the two decades between World Wars I and II. a very traditional orientation approach was employed. Students were generally preparing to study in universities abroad and were given very introductory training. Area studies were conducted to familiarize the students with essential information about a specific country and its role in the world. Language study was encouraged if not required. except for those who were going abroad specifically to study a foreign language. Generally. students were expected to study corresponding areas of content such as history. art. literature. political science. and mathematics as though they were 50 merely continuing their studies at their home university. A few “do‘s and don'ts" were included which addressed stereotypical cultural norms. but little training was provided which developed a genuine sensitization and appreciation for the culture into which one was headed. During the middle period of student orientations. the late 1950's and early 1960's. large numbers of students traveled to overseas destinations by ocean liner. This provided a period of eight to ten days for shipboard orientations consisting of discussions concerning cultural issues. lectures. films. seminars. area studies and extensive interviews with resource persons from that culture. Careful planning between students and their adademic advisors was possible. This was a fairly effective transition time which allowed one to let go of the familiarity of their own culture gradually and prepare for drastic changes. The advent of jet air-travel destined shipboard orientations to become a thing of the past. and ushered in the modern period. Often. the orientation which replaced the shipboard variety was a very brief area study. some directed reading. and in a matter of hours. students were deposited ”cold turkey" into a totally new culture. Some programs continued the orientation on-site; however. many did not. The result was often confusion and disorientation. Presently. a variety of orientation approaches is available for trainers to use. Most approaches can be categorized in terms of one of six basic orientation models: 1. _f_i_e Intellectual Approafl assumes that a cognitive understanding of a culture's people. customs. institutions and values is of major importance if a Person is to function effectively in a foreign culture. Downs (1969) has described this approach as using lectures. readings. films. and other multi-media Presentations to transmit information. The cultural assimilator is an applied training technique based on this approach. It was originally developed by Fiedler. 51 Mitchell. and Triandis (l97l). based on cognitive learning assumptions. to transmit specific information about a foreign culture to members of another culture through programmed learning materials. It has been found to lessen adjustment problems of Americans in the host culture. 2. Ih_e Area Simulation Approagfi attempts to create a specific environment which is as similar as possible to that of the host culture. Thus. programs have taken place in areas that are similar to the host culture in climate. scenery. and configuration. Much of the Peace Corps training utilized this approach. 3. The Self-Awareness Approac_tl is based upon the assumption that a person who has an accurate self-understanding will adjust better to another culture. T-group techniques and sensitivity training are characteristically used in such an approach. The self-awareness approach is generally used in conjunction with another approach rather than in isolation. 4. The Culture Awareness Approafich is designed to provide a "culture-general" orientation. This approach. in contrast to the self-awareness approach. focuses on the characteristics of culture in general. not the individual. The assumption here is that an understanding of the nature of culture will enable a person to interact with a person of another culture with greater understanding and effectiveness. Kraemer (1974) has combined this approach with the self-awareness approach under the label of a "cultural self-awareness". which assumes that if a person is first aware of their own cultural being. then that person will be able to better understand another person in a different cultural system. 5. The Befivioral APPYEQ focuses on teaching specific behaviors used in the host culture (David. 1972). It is assumed that if persons learn the skills necessary to behave appropriately in another culture. they will be able to function more effectively. 52 6. The Interaction Approaglinvolves intense interaction between Americans and host country nationals. By becoming aware of cultural backgrounds. values. and learned behaviors which influence cultural perceptions. it is assumed an individual will interact more knowledgeably once in the host culture. Most orientation programs combine elements from a number of the above approaches. With the mounting body of research indicating the need for interpersonal skills and affective awareness. it seems evident that increased emphasis needs to be placed on processing skills. Harrison and Hopkins (1965) insisted early that the goal of orientation training was not to introduce new content so much as it was to help the individual develop more independence from external sources of decision-making and problem definition. Then motivation must be developed to make choices and commitments to action in situations of stress and uncertainty. One of the best recent resources in the area of intercultural training is the three-volume series by Landis and Brislin (1983). Volume I deals with issues of training theory and design. Volume II discusses training methodology with a particular focus on the context of training. Volume III deals with international education and area-studies. These works are definitely for the advanced trainer and pose such key questions as: Whose reality prevails when action is required?; What exactly does intercultural competence entail?; and Do trainers have a right to change people. especially if the change involves a challenge to their core values? Brislin and Pedersen (1976) suggest seven goals for orientation programs: 1. Improve awareness of customs and interpersonal skills. including techniques for dealing with culture shock phenomena. 2. Help the student develop basic communication skills by teaching key phrases in the foreign language and teaching techniques for speaking through interpreters. 3. Impart knowledge about the foreign culture. complete with practical skills and know-how needed to get along satisfactorily in the culture. 53 4. Attempt to impart sensitivity to others. reducing prejudice and inducing respect. even toward those foreign cultural values and practices that make little sense to the student. 5. Attempt to induce enthusiasm for the job. 6. Provide opportunities to maintain skills during periods of absence from other cultures. 7. Above all. emphasize honesty in relations with others (pp. 2-5). Intercultural education research has indicated some broad directional findings which can be quite helpful in the preliminary stages of program development. Heisler (1973). and Hursh and Brozak (1979). in independent studies have concluded that there are no significant differences between students who are interested in intercultural programs and those who are not. The findings of Hull and Lemke (1975) suggest that domestic intercultural programs are rated highly (96%) by students in terms of their willingness to recommend the program to others. Compared with the already high ratings of students in overseas programs (89%). it is even more impressive. There seems to be significant evidence to indicate that a variety of intercultural options might be provided students with good results. The area of the host country may be a factor which needs careful consideration. Of students who participated in programs in Europe. Africa. the Middle East. Central and South America. and the Far East. the students involved in non-European locations responded more positively on experimental tests. Studies on the length of stay have also provided interesting implications for program design. In a study of attitude outcomes. the most positive student-reported outcomes came from those who had studied off-campus 27-52 weeks. and the second most positive response Came from students who had studied off-campus 10 weeks or less. These responses may suggest that one semester may be too brief a time to permit an optimistic upswing. 54 Hull and Lemke (1977) have made a number of recommendations for program development: 1. Do not try to offer "all things to all people"; focus on one thing and do it well. 2. Include your program within general education requirements. 3. Establish an office responsible for off-campus and intercultural programs. Such an office would likely facilitate: coordination between various non-residence programs; educational development and assessment of program effectiveness; development of orientation and post-sojourn experiences. programs and seminars to assist in the educational and transitional adjustments of participants and to increase the meaningfulness of the programs; selection and overseeing of personnel; and recruitment and counseling of student applicants to place students in programs best suited and relevant to their own educational needs. 4. Have a regular course during the sojourn. 5. Orientation training should be open to affective concerns and dynamics. 6. Select the sojourn directors very carefully. Qualities to look for might include: experience with students. ability to understand their developmental needs. and a positive feeling toward them; experience with off-campus programs and experiential learning; personality which is open and sensitive. willing to encourage close relationships between himself and each student without serving "in loco parentis" or as an armchair psychiatrist; enough academic background to gain a fair hearing from faculties. remembering that he/she is primarily an educator. In a study done by Gray (1977). 320 institutions were surveyed to identify program elements which contributed most to their ongoing intercultural programs. Gray concluded that there were four central factors to ongoing success: 1. a formally specified public commitment; 2. strong administrative support (especially from the academic sectorN 3. interculturally qualified faculty. experienced in fieldwork and research; 4. interdisciplinary and inter-institutional links (p. 230). 55 BARRIERS TO OVERCOME Change is one of the more threatening prospects to entrenched systems. Educational systems are no exception. Tradition maintains a firm grip on the status-quo of a system that has served the American enterprise so successfully. In spite of the attractive potential of intercultural experiential learning programs. they represent an approach which is not necessarily well served by the current educational infrastructures. The difficulties need to be carefully identified and responded to in appropriate ways and at an accommodating rate if genuine acceptance is to come. Assessment Concerns Within the field of experiential learning. one of the current frontiers of development is the refinement of assessment mechanisms. Defining the variables. developing new instruments of measurement. and relating those appropriately to a broad diversity of individual experience are tasks which must be effectively addressed before experiential education will receive wide-scale acceptance. Duley (NSIEE. #6). has summarized the assessment process as: (1) identifying the learning; (2) articulating it with the rest of the learner's program; (3) documenting the learning; (4) measuring; (5) evaluating; and (6) recording the outcomes. ..lenks and Murphy (1981) report a survey of 85 faculty members in California. Utah. and Nevada which revealed a variety of monitoring and assessing activities currently in use: written journals. logs. and progress reports; concurrent seminars and field experiences; supervisor evaluations; individual conferences with students; occasional site visits; and frequent telephone contacts with field supervisors. Research efforts presently underway are tending to interpret the assessment process as an all-encompassing dimension of experiential learning--from pre-planning to final evaluation. 56 OrganizationaLBarriers to Innovafition Hefferlin (1969) identifies a number of obstacles to change in educational institutions which apply to the predictable resistance on the part of institutions to ready acceptance of intercultural experiential education programs. Hefferlin observes that: (l) organizations are inherently passive; (2) volunteer organizations. such as higher education. tend to attract members who agree with their activities. thus. innovation is unlikely; (3) organizations tend toward institutionalism and ritualism by which means are transformed into ends; (4) organizations with paid personnel come to embody the vested interests of its employees; (5) the purposes and support of higher education are basically conservative; (6) academic professionals are socialized (18-22) to agree with the practices and values of academia; (7) because of academic freedom. any change must come by consensus not fiat; and (8) collegial decision-making is an extended process and may come too long after it was needed. Conditions for Institutionalization Three factors have been identified by Little (1983). which must be addressed if institutional change is to occur. The first factor is overcoming the organizational barriers. As has been mentioned above. inertia comes in a wide variety of forms within institutions. A commitment must be gained from the public. trustees. faculty. and administrators if an intercultural experiential program is to become a major educational program. It must be able to demonstrate to the satisfaction of its various publics that it is of benefit. If advantage can be shown in the areas of recruitment. retention. motivation. and productivity. then it is likely commitment will follow. It is likewise essential to approach the introduction of a new program in an evolutionary manner. taking care to highlight the factor of compatibility with existing practices and institutional objectives. yet without forfeiting its innovative character. 57 The second factor for institutional change is leadership. Faculty and administrative support are necessary but not sufficient conditions in themselves to produce change. There must be someone to lead the fight. A new cause must be championed. and that leadership must be exercised with skill. influence. and understanding. The third factor involves the conditions for change. Good planning. which demonstrates the incremental achievability of the program. is essential for program adoption. Decision-makers need to see the stage-by-stage master plan of the proposed change or the fear of the unknown will likely erode their support. Once the plan has support. the distinctives of the new program must be clearly communicated to all the publics who will be affected. Perhaps the most difficult aspect of new program establishment concerns financial resources. Hefferlin (1969) has suggested that a new program will be tolerated if it costs no money or if it brings its own support. It will be resisted if the funds it requires could be used for the expansion of existing programs. Trouble certainly will arise if a new program is forced upon some part of the institution which requires existing resources to be divided to include it. CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY The purpose of the study has been to examine selected full-semester immersion intercultural experiential education programs in order to identify the consensus of educational rationale and to understand the primary cluster of programmatic and logistical factors central to new program development. RESEARCH QUESTIONS Question 1: What intercultural program outcomes have been identified in the existing research for arguing that intercultural experiential education programs are effective in producing student development? Question 2: What crucial programmatic and logistical factors essential to program effectiveness can be identified in intercultural experiential learning programs within the scope of the sample? ngstion 3: What major barriers have institutions encountered and overcome to make their intercultural programs succeed? POPLLATION AND SAPPLE The population of the study was composed of 239 member-institutions of the Council for the Advancement of Experiential Learning (CAEL). CAEL began in March l974 as a project of the Educational Testing Service and has grown into a significant organization interested in reviewing. trying out and validating assessment procedures. and furthering the use and crediting of experiential learning. CAEL member institutions are typical of colleges and universities in many 58 59 Rafi .oH .aa .mkmx .ma>ma a aamcgv .gaex:u S. on c. m n _ on S 3.330.380 32502280 can 8 Ne a c. N N 3 8. 5390 .3335 9553:— mcmEauJ 333m sauce—t eotu< 3. 5N n N _ N Nm me .3220; ..o _~_oom 3.7.53:— nn «N c N N N am 2. 33.3w of?» 9:52... 2 S n a I I so 8 acefiaara cocauavw 8 E e. a _ a N... E 2:22.80 39an.260 .2533:qu S 3 a o v m mm 3 co 38«0 cocauoixm N m: m N_ N h we no =2e0 40.0.5.2 4m V0 4.4.5.62 44.3 $430.62 4030 4039.82 4.434 n: =8 2.3M». e 5 3.8.33 ..Sau.~t=uou 2558.5 e2 uueMuw 3583.33.— 3&4. b.8443 48.836 .8 to .E 3.» .8 P32 35%? .o: 49.838 343%. Ewen aunt 62 SEE 3:26 :2 ..eoém £320 .634» 3580 m.mm m.: vmuS—Cmq zucanu m.o~ ~.m_ ucmncmaavcH 9: 06m. 355 wdmuqmnuuwm a 33cc... .6 max» $3-52 .55 meowosuaomcs smmna= 0.04 mm 0.04 04 0.40 mm on 0 mm 04 :npcoermcos m t w a w a w a w t maze enumoum 004 u z 404 u z Ammuzv Ammuzv 04m2nmlcoz m4mamm mQONupU4umcH m204ppp4umc4 mcowupu4umcH =onmopm= 4muoe :memoumlcoz: meowuzpwumcH LmQEmz-40<0 cw mamcmocm pmwpcmwcmaxm Feczp—ausmch mo muc_¥ m mpnmh 74 mmmucooumm n 0 wucopwoph u * "meoz 004 mm4 mm on mm m4muoa 0.0m 00 «.0v pm m.m m m.m 4 0 N posmnfi 02 0.40 N0 N.0N hm v.0v mm 0.00 «4 vv 44 0mmmmuoo4 4.00 00 0.vN mm 0.me vN m.mv m4 ev 44 wemm 5.x m4 m.m N4 m.m m n.0 N e 4 mmemuumo w w m u w e w a w 0 Susouo mo :04uomu4o 004 n z 404 n z Ammuzv “Omuzv Ammuzv 4mu09 m4mEdmicoz m4mamm m:04u5u4umc4 mcoapsuHumcH mc04usu4umCH :Emu00p0= 4muoe =emu00umucoz= mcospsssomcH meemz-smae own; age cm>0 mamcmocm ~mc=u_:ucmuc4 mo zuzocw we :owuumcwo 0 mpnmh w0muawouwm hocmpwwuh u 0 11 O 0.0m 00 0.0m 00 0.04 0 0.0 N 00 0 pwzmcm oz N.00 044 0.00 00 0.00 mm 0.00 04 N0 04 oz 0.04 00 0.0 04 0.00 04 0.00 04 00 0 wm> m t w t w x w t w # 040:04umm mo mucwemumum 004 u z 404 n z Ammuzv 400uzv Ammnzv 4muOH m4memmlcoz m4mEmm m:04usu4ump4 mc04usu4umcH m:04upu4umCH =Emu00pm= 4muop =emnmoumncoz= mpacowpmm Emsmocm 40L34430mec4 0o mangmumum 04 mpnmh "@802 76 interculural programs were started. The second cluster involves components essential to maintaining the programs once they have been established. _t_arting Intarcultural Program; Age of Program. The majority of institutions in the study had intercultural programs five years old or less. Such may indicate either the timely rise of such programs. or perhaps the difficulty of maintaining such programs beyond the first few years. Table 11 Program Existence in Years Years in Existence Frequency Percentage 1-5 13 52 6-10 8 32 Above 10 4 l6 n=25 First Program. When asked how intercultural programs started in their institution. respondents identified a variety of first programs. The open-ended responses indicated the consensus of different program types (Table 12). "Short-Term Programs" are generally exposure approaches limited to just a few days. perhaps even a weekend. In most cases these programs involve visiting another cultural context; however. some institutions have attempted to import a new cultural context to the campus for several days. "Travel-Study Programs" generally depend on a "tour" approach to area or topical studies. These programs often consist of a three to five week excursion accompanied by a faculty member. Students are exposed to the actual objects of study. usually with the help of local experts. "Exchange Programs" offer opportunities for students and faculty to take 77 the place of another student or faculty member in an institution in another culture. These arrangements are usually made on an individual basis. "Intercultural Internship Placements" provide experiential opportunities for students to apply the expertise of their academic major in a work setting in another culture. "Independent Studies" are individualized programs usually developed in response to student initiative. They frequently involve working in a foreign research center or a specific on-going project in another cultural setting. "Service-Learning Placements" provide opportunities for students to participate in service-oriented projects in another culture. perhaps best typified by the Peace Corps. Table 12 First Intercultural Program at Institution Type of Program Frequency Percentage Short-Term Program 4 16 Travel-Study Program 3 12 Intercultural Exchange Program 3 12 Intercultural Internship Placement 3 12 Independent Studies 1 4 Service-Learning Placements l 4 Some respondents indicated the type of institutional mechanism by which intercultural programs started. Consortium participation was identified by 24% of the respondents. and 16% identified departmental programs instead of a specific type of program. Although respondents confused the accuracy of specific frequencies of program types by mixing categories. it did serve to both provide a profile of program types and common institutional mechanisms. 78 Role of Initiator. Findings indicated that the individual responsible for starting intercultural programs at an institution was most typically an interested faculty member (60%). Administrators were responsible for initiation in 36% of the institutions. but provided a supportive role in a much larger percentage of the institutions. Consultants. The role played by consultants in the early stages of program development was mixed. Only 20% of the institutions used professional consultants in designing their programs. No professional consultants were used in 48% of the institutions. however. colleagial advice was sought both internally and externally from those with intercultural experience. Table 13 Use of Consultants in Program Design Category Frequency Percentage Consultants 5 20 No Consultants 12 48 NA 8 32 First Ye_ar Costs. Respondents were hesitant to give approximate costs for the first year of their program. The complexity of cost variables due to the diversity of programs. hidden costs of development. and cooperative efforts between institutions made cost estimates nearly impossible. The consensus was that the first year was more costly than additional years once the program was established. Locations. Findings indicate that the locations used for intercultural programs varied widely due to the networks available to institutions. Literally. 79 programs have placed students all over the world. Cluster patterns did emerge however. indicating that the predominant areas were the following: (1) Western Europe. (2) Mexico. and (3) the Far East. Components of Currant Intarcultufil Proggaaia Student Numbers. Student participation in intercultural programs has increased as programs have stabilized and earned a place in the curriculum. Two clusters have emerged indicating the number of students participating in intercultural programs (Figure 12). The first cluster involves 8-15 participants. The second cluster involves 26-35 participants. These numbers may indicate either program capacities or degrees of student interest. Finances. Findings indicated that 68% of the programs were self-supporting. and 24% still depend on subsidy finances. No data was given on 8%. Percentage Number of Institutions 40 10 36 9 32 3 28 7 24 6 20 5 16 4 12 3 8 2 4 1 “(other of Students 1 - I D- - First year of program - Current year of program Note n=25 Figure 12 Comparison of Numbers of Students in First Year and Current Year of Program 80 Student Preparation. A broad range of items were reported on the kinds of things done to prepare students for intercultural experiences. The responses were organized into eight related clusters (Table 14). The dominant pattern of preparation consisted of one to four information meetings (44%). and assigned readings (36%). Table 14 Components of Student Preparation Type of Requirement Frequency Percentage 1-4 Information Meetings 11 44 Special Readings 9 36 On-Site Orientation 7 28 Language Training 6 24 Advisor Sessions 5 20 Full-Semester Course 4 16 2-3 Week On-Campus Orientation 3 12 Area-Study Term Paper 1 4 No Preparation 1 4 Note: Institutions frequently employed more than one requirement. Voluntary flarticipation. All the institutions in the sample depended upon voluntary student participation with one exception. One institution had made an intercultural experience an upper division general education requirement. 81 Screening Mechanisms. Students are involved in some kind of screening process in 76% of the intercultural programs in the study. Seven clusters of mechanisms were identified (Table 15). Table 15 Mechanisms Used in Screening Students for Intercultural Programs Screening Mechanism Frequency Percentage Written Application 12 48 Personal Interview 11 44 Letters of Reference 9 36 Submitted Academic Transcript 6 24 Statement of Personal Objectives 5 20 Informal Assessment 4 16 Language Proficiency 2 8 No Screening Process 6 24 Note: Institutions frequently use multiple mechanisms. glass Level of Participants. Respondents generally indicated that freshmen lacked the necessary learning skills and maturity to be able to derive maximum value out of an intercultural experiential program (Table 16). Often seniors. unless the experience was incorporated into requirements of the major. found it difficult to commit an entire semester away from campus. The prime years seem to be the sephomore and junior years. both from a developmental and academic perspective. 82 Table 16 Class Levels of Intercultural Program Participants Class Level Frequency Percentage Freshman 4 l6 Sophomore 12 48 Junior 19 76 Senior 14 56 NA 3 12 Note: Institutions frequently allow a range of class levels. Program Costs. Findings indicated that institutions are nearly divided on the item of additional fees beyond the normal on-campus costs to students. Additional fees are required by 44% of the institutions. and not required by 48%. No data were given by 8%. Transportation costs were cited as the single most significant variable. Studant Groups. One way to keep intercultural programs as cost-effective as possible is by organizing students into groups for preparation. transportation. housing. and instruction. It was found that the most common group size consisted of 10-20 individuals (Table 17). Table 17 Average Group-Size in Intercultural Programs Group-Size Frequency Percentage Individualized Program 4 16 10-20 5 20 Above 20 2 8 NA 14 56 83 Is there a consensus on how often students should meet together once they are situated in an intercultural context to process what they are experiencing? Respondents tended to favor daily (16%) and weekly (32%) meetings (Table 18); however. several kinds of non-group programs impact these findings. Table 18 Frequency of Student Group Meetings in Foreign Locations Frequency of Meetings Response Frequency Percentage Individual Basis l 4 Daily 4 16 Weekly 8 32 2-3 Times 2 8 NA 10 40 l=e_arning Objectives_and Outcomes. Respondents indicated that 40% of the programs require students to to write learning objectives. The majority (52%) however. do not require students to participate in the writing of learning objectives. By contrast. students in 68% of the programs are required to document their learning outcomes. Only 24% of the programs do not require documentation. and 8% were unsure of the policy concerning either learning outcomes or documentation. Most documentation was made via traditional means. although a variety of means was represented (Table 19). 84 Table 19 Kinds of Student Documentation of Learning Outcomes Kinds of Documentation Frequency Percentage Journal 11 44 Written Coursework 7 28 Final Paper 6 24 Learning Contract Specifications 2 8 Written Self-Evaluation 2 8 Research Project 2 8 Committee Interview 2 8 No Documentation 6 24 NA 2 8 Note: Institutions frequently require multiple documentation. Asseésmants and Credits. One of the key concerns of those contemplating the addition of intercultural programs to the curriculum involves the issue of assessment and credits. Findings indicated that assessment by the professor was the most common (48%). Only half as frequently was the assessment done by the Program Director (24%). Other methods of assessment identified were major reports or projects (20%). final examinations (8%). and seminar dialogue (4%). The amount of credit to be attached to certain kinds of experience is a difficult decision. The study indicated that two categories of individuals generally made the determination. On-campus faculty were the determiners in 64% of the programs and on-site instructors were the determiners in 32% of the programs. A cooperative determination was the policy in 4% of the institutions. Intercultural Program credits were transcripted as on-campus equivalents in 88% of the institutions. The range of credit possible in intercultural programs essentially paralleled the range 85 available in traditional on-campus programs. The predominant range was 11-15 semester hours (Table 20). Table 20 Range of Credit in Intercultural Programs Amount of Credit in Semester Hours Frequency Percentage 3-10 8 32 11-15 10 40 Above 15 6 24 NA 1 4 Findings indicated that: 28% of the programs earned lower division credit only; 36% earned upper division credit only; and 32% of the programs offered both levels. Although few studies have been done (16%) on the impact of intercultural experiential learning on subsequent academic performance. institutions are committed to a broader range of educational objectives than mere grades. The 84% of the institutions which did not carry out empirical studies on academic performance seem to be satisfied with the variety of outcomes observed by faculty and reported by students. Students have indicated to program leaders that the single greatest personal benefits have been changes in perspectives on culture (44%). life (44%). and self (28%). These outcomes provide a striking parallel to many of the statements of educational objectives for intercultural programs cited in Chapter II and traditional Liberal Arts goals. 86 Table 21 Single Greatest Benefit to Participants in Intercultural Programs Benefit Frequency Percentage Exposure to a New Culture 11 44 Expanded Perspectives 11 44 Personal Growth (Self-Confidence) 7 28 Employment Advantage 4 l6 Socialization/Good Feelings 4 16 New Perspective on U.S. Culture 3 12 Language Learning 2 8 Different Styles of Teaching 2 8 Improved Writing Skills 1 4 Adaptation Skills 1 4 Living with Host Family 1 4 Note: Some institutions reported more than one benefit. Faculty Prepa_ration. As crucial as faculty are to solid intercultural experiential programs. a troubling pattern of "no preparation" emerged in response to inquiries into how faculty are prepared for intercultural assignments (Table 22). Most institutions (84%) did not provide any formal orientation for faculty. Two approaches depended upon faculty initiative: faculty request for information; and international exchange Opportunities for faculty. Four approaches were provided for faculty: information meetings about recruiting students. communicating benefits. and program expectations; briefings by former program directors; joint three-week orientation meetings with students; and organized faculty development activities. 87 Table 22 Methods of Faculty Preparation for Intercultural Assignment Method of Preparation Frequency Percentage No Preparation 11 44 Contract Foreign Personnel 4 16 Teaching in Related Field 3 12 Informal Briefing 2 8 Send to Training Institute 1 4 Serve on Selection Committee 1 4 Faculty Exchange 1 4 Orientation Program 1 4 NA 2 8 Note: Institutions may use more than one method. l;_e_a__der Cha_r_acteristics. Findings indicated that the predominant characteristic essential to an effective intercultural program leader. is interpersonal skill (Table 23). Those faculty that have proven to be effective leaders are effective with students (32%). people in general (16%). and the host population (12%). They also tend to be open. flexible. wise. knowledgeable. and hardy. Cited also are the desirable qualities of initiative. good judgment. and effective communication skills. These same qualities are in demand for leadership needs on the home campus. which makes long-term use of veteran leaders in intercultural programs somewhat unlikely. 88 Table 23 Characteristics of Effective Faculty Leading Intercultural Groups Characteristics Frequency Percentage Good with Students 8 32 Qualified in Their Field 5 20 Flexible. People-oriented. Good 4 16 Judgment. Administrative Ability. Outstanding Teacher Effective with Host Population. 3 12 Initiative. Previous Experience. Spirit of Adventure Openness. Stamina. Expert 2 8 Language Facility Tolerance. Spends Time with Students. 1 4 Able to Function away from Family NA 10 40 _aculty Incentives. Given the characteristics of effective faculty members for intercultural program leadership. it is understandable that respondents indicated the difficulty of recruiting good faculty personnel. Findings indicate that institutional incentives for such program assignments seem to be quite minimal (Table 24). Other than a few institutions whose commitment is reflected in attractive benefits. little tangible benefit is to be gained through such an assignment at most institutions. 89 Table 24 Incentives for Faculty Involvement in Intercultural Programs Incentives Frequency Percentage Paid Transportation 7 28 Paid Salary 5 20 Family Included 3 12 Paid Housing 3 12 Expense Allowance 2 8 Teaching Vacation 2 8 Professional Development 1 4 Personal Satisfaction 1 4 NA 9 36 Administrative Coordination. Respondents indicated that in 88% of the institutions. there was a central coordinator for all intercultural programs. In only 12% of the institutions were arrangements structured differently. A variety of assigned positions were delegated with regard to the detailed arrangements overseas (Table 25). Table 25 Personnel Responsflnle for Arrangements Abroad Personnel Frequency Percentage On-Campus Program Director 12 48 Foreign Director 7 28 Foreign Contact Person 4 16 Consortium Staff 3 12 Travel Agents 1 4 Placement Consultant 1 4 NA 3 12 Note: Institutions may combine administrative approaches. 90 INSTITUTIONAL BARRIERS Research Question 3: What mflr barriers have institutions encountered in making their intercultuLaUrogLams succeed? Four clusters of factors have been identified in the study which respondents indicated were crucial issues challenging the development of intercultural experiential programs at their institution. The open-ended responses were not exhaustive in detailing the problem areas. but provided a profile of problem dimensions. The problem areas cited were: academic; financial; personnel; and administrative. The academic dimension posed the greatest number of difficulties in starting intercultural programs for a variety of reasons. The academic area tends to be quite conservative. Intercultural experiential programs represent a threat to the status quo in that they generally defy the traditional structure of set classes. written tests. timed lecture periods. and a geographically close community of resources. Intercultural programs are typically less-contained and more remote. They also demand a new category of systems from the academic establishment. which is different and inconvenient at times. Orientation programs present a specialized challenge and require costly amounts of human effort and detailed curriculum. Institutions may not have personnel properly trained to conduct orientations. or individuals who are knowledgeable about or interested in what should comprise the curricular sequence. Registration problems commonly occur because students are off-campus in intercultural programs when preregistration plans need to be made. Students may find it difficult to register for the courses they need to take the next semester or term. particularly since students often rearrange their schedules several times during the registration period until a plan is negotiated to best fit their time needs. The academic community tends to be fearful of taking academic risks which may raise questions in the minds of 91 accrediting examiners and state education officials. These fears. together with the above mentioned factors. must be addressed and resolved to gain the needed support from the academic community. A second problem area identified by the respondents was finances. The exploratory stages of identifying what kind of program would provide the best fit for both the students and the institution. where could programs be conducted. and how could budgets protect the institution and still be within reach of students. all require costly investigation. Program costs must either be subsidized or covered by student fees. How these issues are decided will have an impact on the kinds of programs which can be offered. and their appeal to students. Complications with student aid requirements. what to pay faculty for extended assignments. flucuations in the international monetary markets. and high costs involved in moving people and materials all contribute to a complex cluster of issues which must be resolved. The personnel questions become even more significant as a program becomes more removed from the home campus and less dependent on the infrastructures of the institution. In many respects. the faculty leader. in an international situation. becomes the institution incarnate. Consequently. the choice of the Program Director and the faculty group leader are very significant (Table 23). The use of foreign directors may potentially produce an even greater loss of control. and create a sense of distance between the intercultural programs and on-campus programs. Respondents indicated anxiety over expectations administrators seemed to have about how "underused" (generally incompetent) faculty or staff might be used in filling new positions in a fledgling intercultural program. The issue of student recruitment is a significant one. New programs must communicate an image which will attract student participants. If such recruitment undermines other 92 institutional programs. a politically destructive climate could result. Recruitment then. must involve an internal and an external thrust. The fourth problem area involves administrative concerns. New projects present the insecurity of unforeseen crises. International policies often come into the decision-making process when planning for intercultural programs. Language barriers complicate communication. Foreign contracts. public relations materials. an the total logistics of arranging for living and learning in another culture. can become an overwhelming task. Specific responses are presented in Table 26. 93 Table 26 Institutional Problems in Starting Intercultural Programs Factors Frequency Percentage Academic Faculty distrust of new programs 5 20 Planning the academic program 3 12 Lack of coordination in programs 2 8 Registration problems 2 8 Developing orientation program 2 8 Complexity of assessment 1 4 Impact on accreditation status 1 4 State recognition of program 1_ fl_ 1.7. £0 Financial What to charge students 6 24 Start-up costs 4 16 Compensation to faculty 2 8 Student aid complexities l 4 Financially victimized internationally l 4 Cost of transporting materials 1_ _4_ 1.5. £9. Personnel Recruiting students 5 20 Selection of Program Director 3 12 Faculty departmental replacements 3 12 Finding qualified foreign staff 2 8 Using existing institutional personnel _1_ _4_ 1.9. 56. Administrative Publicity/Public Relations 4 l6 Ignorance of international mechanisms 1 4 Contracts with location resources 1 4 Language barrier l 4 Investigating on-site locations 1 4 Finding good housing 1 4 Last minutes decisions _1_ 4_ m 40 Note: Institutions frequently identified multiple factors. 94 The four same cluster areas of problems were identified by respondents when they addressed difficulties encountered in continuing intercultural programs (Table 27). The order of frequency of these clusters was altered in the on-going programs. The area of financial difficulties moved from the second most mentioned problem to the fourth. Administrative concerns moved from the fourth position to the second position (Table 27). Fewer problems were mentioned for on-going programs than for starting programs: Academic (68%)48%); Financial (60%)24%); Personnel (56%)28%); and Administrative (40%)32%). Once intercultural programs were underway. faculty distrust of such programs gradually ceased to be a major barrier. Instead. but perhaps related. the leading academic difficulty was recruiting faculty to accept intercultural teaching assignments. The reticence was perhaps due to two other factors mentioned by respondents: a lack of experiential knowledge of intercultural programs. and resistance on the part of departments to releasing faculty for assignments outside the on-campus teaching loads. Another factor related to several items mentioned by respondents was the concern for the coordination of intercultural programs with: departmental planning; on-campus and field personnel; and term synchronization on campus. Administrative concerns related primarily to communication needs internally and externally. Personnel issues centered on student recruitment. and behavior once in the program. The important task of finding a replacement program director was mentioned only 20% as many times as student issues. Financial difficulties essentially involved budget planning. Fluctuating international exchange rates provided a constant problem for set budgets. Program costs could soar without warning. Some programs found it difficult to provide the advance funding required to reserve housing and program space abroad. This 95 proved particularly difficult when the lending period crossed the fiscal year end. In some cases. student financial aid packages were complicated by the kind and location of the program they had chosen. Table 27 Institutional Problems in Maintaining Intercultural Programs Factors Frequency Percentage Academic Recruiting faculty 2 8 Lack of experiential knowledge by professors 1 Departmental resistance to release faculty Need to internationalize faculty Professors late turning in grades Interdepartmental coordination Assessment of student experience Appropriate training Linkage between faculty 8: field staff Quality control of program Complexity of term synchronization J’s ..4 INI—Jd—J—J—Jd—J—Jc—l b mlbbbhbhbbb Administrative Better communication of program to students 4 l6 Continued public relations Continued support from top administrators (.0 —l N 100 |-‘ I53 u: Personnel Student recruitment Monitoring student behavior Finding a new program director Student illness and emergencies bud—Aron) .J oops-boom N Financial Exchange rates affecting costs Yearly advance money for reservations Financial aid for students ..a 0‘: nail-04> N blurs Note: Institutions frequently identified multiple factors. 96 SUMMARY AND LIMITATIONS The research study which has been undertaken attempted to examine descriptively the structure of existing full-semester immersion-type intercultural experiential education programs in selected American colleges and universities. The study has surveyed and interviewed practioners in the programming field to identify what practices are actually in use. Attention has focused on the question of whether there exists a consensus of educational rationale and a primary cluster of programmatic and logistical factors central to the design and management of such programs. Current Status of the Program; Although intercultural programs have involved large numbers of students and seem to be providing legitimate learning experiences. the theoretical distinctives of such educational approaches appear to be only recently concerns within the educational dialogue. Developmental Psychology. Intercultural Communication. Learning Styles. Cognitive and Ethical Development. Sociology. and Curriculum Development are fields which have been brought together in the design of effective intercultural experiential learning programs. A wide variety of programs have emerged and continue to evolve as opportunities and needs change. The practice in this field has largely preceded a unified theory. Just recently has the vocabulary of intercultural experiential education even approached consensus. Even so. there continues a lack of clarity in definitions about the kinds of programs now in use. Nomenclature not only remains unclear to many. but is changing. The use of such key terms as "cross-cultural" and ”international studies" has shifted in meaning over the past two decades. Although institutions are very ready to posit a claim for the value of their intercultural programs. few have done empirical studies to firmly establish the kinds of learning outcomes which have 97 resulted from program participation. Although 88% of the sample report that their programs are stable or growing (Table 9). only 24% have printed statements of the educational value of their programs (Table 10). There appears to be a correspondence between the core values cited in the statements of educational rationale and the student reports of program benefits (Table 21). Programmatic Factors The study indicated that most of the programs were started from a small. low-commitment base. Consortium participation was the most frequent entree (24%). followed closely by departmental (16%) and short-term (16%) programs. Most of the programs are young. 52% being five years old or less. As such. it was possible for most individuals interviewed to recall the person who had initiated intercultural programs at the institution. Most were either faculty (60%) or administrators (36%). and developed programs without the help of professional consultants (48%). Frequently. the faculty initiator was co-opted into directing the program as it grew. therefore. changing categories. Most intercultural programs are self-supporting (68%). and are nearly evenly divided over whether program fees beyond a typical semester's costs are required. Students must apply for acceptance in 76% of the programs studied. though a variety of seven different kinds of screening mechanisms were identified by respondents (Table 15). Intercultural programs tend to favor upper-division students: Juniors (76%); Seniors (56%); Sophomores (48%); and Freshmen (16%). The prime years from a developmental and academic perspective seem to be the sophomore and junior years. Most intercultural programs involve student groups of 10-20 individuals and meet together at least weekly during the program. Findings indicated that only 40% of the programs required students to write learning objectives for their experiences. but 68% required students to document their learning outcomes. 98 Student work was assessed primarily by the instructing professor (48%). but on-campus faculty were the primary determiners (64%) of the amount of credit to be given for a student's experience. Findings indicated that: 28% of the programs offered lower division credit only; 36% offered upper division credit only; and 32% of the programs offered both levels. Students report the greatest benefit to them in participating has been in the areas of changed perspectives and personal growth (Table 21). These are areas difficult to measure accurately and conceivably contribute to the lack of empirical studies on student outcomes. Miscellaneous Findings Respondents indicated that students who had established a positive interpersonal relationship with their group leader. program director. or host family reported a more positive assessment of their intercultural experience. The interpersonal skills quality emerged also as a leading characteristic of those who functioned effectively as intercultural group leaders. The consensus reported was that the more distant the relationship between the students and those leading the program. the less powerful the program impact. The implications of such a relationship should be carefully explored in future research. CHAPTER V CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS Intercultural experiential learning programs appear to represent a powerful potential within American higher education. ACADEMIC LEGITIMACY Educational research is continuing to press the case for both cognitive and affective development. Research indicates an interrelationship of both domains if full development is to occur in either. Intercultural experiential learning appears to be able to motivate affective development more effectively than the highly controlled environment of traditional higher education. Both approaches. used in a complementary strategy. contribute advantages which make the traditional aims of American education more reachable. Intellectual development and character development. historically characteristic of the commitment of American higher education. may be approached through such a strategy. The education of the whole person should include the ability to not only provide active leadership locally. but to think and choose globally. Intercultural experiential learning programs broaden the range of learning opportunities which is increasingly important as the age-range of students continue to expand. Such programs tend to provide responsive opportunities for a wide variety of student interests. as indicated by the highly positive attitudes toward the programs by most student participants. Findings indicate that the optimum time for most students to participate in intercultural programs are either the sophomore or junior year. 99 100 Students are particularly receptive to new perspectives and will have time to bring a more mature influence to the college campus in their final semesters of study. Institutions of higher education are slowly accepting intercultural experiential learning programs. As an emerging movement. the quality of programs is uneven from institution to institution. The time seems to be appropriate for institutions to give serious attention to quality preparation of faculty who are interested and willing to contribute leadership to the area. It appears that a significant bridge between a program and a student's development. is the dimension of significant interpersonal relationships with program leaders and other program participants. This interpersonal core ought not to be taken for granted by program administrators. The careful selection of program personnel is perhaps as important as the program activities. DEVELOPlifiNTAL POTENTIAL Research suggests that the mid-college years (20-21) are particularly prime times for development. Students are maturing cognitively. socially. emotionally. and ethically. Structural developmentalists indicate that most growth proceeds by stages. induced by a sense of discontent or frustration. Such disequilibrium can be strategically introduced. Students placed in very different cultural environments seem to be particularly open to new kinds of understanding. The whole person is engaged. If too much disequilibrium is experienced. the individual despairs and is not open for new growth. The lack of sufficient disequilibrium will not challenge an individual beyond his present state of adequacy. The question for educators it seems. asks how optimum challenge can be strategically managed in the learning process? If intercultral experiential learning can regulate the level of challenge. it seems reasonable to assume that individual development can be accelarated and to a certain extent directed. The foundational evidence for such potential growth has emerged in the findings as students reported the resultant changed perspectives 101 about culture (44%). life (44%). and self (28%) (Table 21). If the relativistic thinking of the reported expanded perspectives (44%) is combined with the reported cultural introspection (12%). an even broader change in objective perspective (56%) is evidenced. PROBLEM AREAS Findings suggest that the critical problem area in both the initiation and maintenance phases of intercultural programming centers on the institution's faculty. Academic barriers were cited by 68% of the institutions as representing factors which must be overcome to start a new program. Faculty distrust of new programs was the single most cited barrier (20%) in the Academic category (Table 27). Not only was there faculty inertia. but 44% of the institutions who had faculty interested in an intercultural assignment had no method of preparing them (Table 23). Further. few incentives (Table 25) are offered by institutions to motivate faculty toward overseas work. and 84% did not provide an orientation for faculty who were accepting intercultural assignments. It would appear that for intercultural experiential education programs to win a place of academic respectability in the United States. the faculty dilemma needs to be addressed. Another observation about problems identified in the study has to do with the relative shifts between problem areas in the start-up phases. and those encountered in on-going programs. In the starting phase of a program. financial problems were rated the second most prevalent (60%). In the maintenance phase. financial problems had moved to fourth most prevalent (24%). Administrative problems followed the inverse pattern (Table 28). LIMITATIONS The study was subject to several limitations which may have had a bearing on the outcomes. The population selected was the CAEL institutional membership in the United States. There were several outstanding programs which could not be 102 examined within the scope of the study because their institutions were outside the CAEL population. In addition. CAEL-member institutions. by their affiliation. would likely represent a higher interest in experientially-oriented programs than institutions at large. Generalization should not be extended unduly. given the size of the sample. Another limitation was the methodology of telephone interviewing. Although it has many advantages. a telephone interview lacks the visual non-verbal cues present in face-to-face interviews. The use of open-ended questions in the interview make this even more important. Subtle cues can often take an interview right to the critical issues. Telephone interviews are also at a disadvantage in that they do not provide one with a sense of the atmosphere of a program headquarters. which can either attract or discourage potential program participants. A third cluster of limiting variables involves the wide variety of kinds of programs within intercultural experiential education. Given the problems of definitions within the field. interview responses may have varying shades of meaning. Individuals being interviewed chose among different programs at their institution and focused on only one for purposes of responding with specific data. The choice was often arbitrary. attempting to come as close as possible to the full-semester immersion-type intercultural experiential education program announced in the advance letter. A different program choice might have yielded different results. Institutions which relied primarily on consortium networks. were not able to provide logistical data in several areas of the study. Consequently. numerous items were not applicable (NA) to their program at the institutional level. These institutions essentially contracted those functions to the organizational staff of the consortium. 103 The orientation of the study was deliberately skewed toward program implementation . The primary focus of the study revolved around "how" programs were functioning. not the theoretical underpinnings which may affect their legitimacy or appropriateness. Time was also a limitation. The study necessarily brought together a number of interesting disciplines: social-psychology; intercultural communication; curriculum development; program administration; and educational research. Each vied for attention in its own direction. which had to be carefully rationed. if the study were to be kept in balance and workable. PROGRAM RECOMMENDATIONS To the institution interested in possibly starting intercultural experiential learning programs. three suggestions are offered: 1. Bagin on a small sca__le_._ Perhaps the most problem-free approach is to participate in a consortium of programs. In the consortium. the individual institution has the fewest direct responsibilities. but also the least amount of direct control over program content. emphasis. and outcomes. Another approach is to encourage departments within the institution to sponsor specific intercultural experiential activities related to their subject area. These often evolve into very fine additions to the departmental program and offer students intercultural opportunities. Such small-scale. low-commitment approaches allow an institution to experiment with programs until the most appropriate kinds of programs have been identified. 2. Cultivate leadership. Encourage faculty to participate in opportunities related to their field of interest. yet offering an intercultural dimension. Since resistance to the development of most new programs begins in the faculty. champions of new programs must be cultivated. 3. Explorathe full range of program development. New programs need to have a wholeness to their development. Intercultural experiential learning programs are no 104 exception. Exciting brochures are not sufficient to produce a high quality educational product. All phases of program development. operation. and evaluation should be examined before the commitment is made to offer a new program (see Appendix A). For those institutions already committed to intercultural experiential education. several suggestions are offered: 1. Focus on growth. The temptation to recruit students to learn in exotic and exciting places is perhaps to miss the most valuable potential. A change in cultural context may be an end in itself. but the impact on life perspectives seems to be the most powerful outcome. 2. Develop wholisticassessment. A wide variety of change is possible through intercultural experiential learning programs. Assessment processes need to attempt to identify the kind of change which has occured. Such change. as indicated by student reports. often is not adequately reflected in academic performance alone. 3. Create a desire to participatag Faculty incentives for involvement need to be seriously reevaluated. Quality benefits need to become associated with intercultural programs and promoted so that new leadership is continually emerging. IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH 1. Need for empirical studies. Empirical studies need to be done consistently on the educational outcomes of intercultural experiential learning programs. They are needed to better understand the kind of changes which can be planned for. and to contribute to the growing rationale of educational acceptance. 2. factors impacting change. A second area which needs research attention is the effects of intercultural experiential programs on individual change over time. Factors responsible for certain kinds of change need to be isolated so that program designers can more effectively develop programs to encourage change in specific directions. APPENDICES 105 APPENDIX A Important Components of Experiential Learning Programs 1. Developing a Program Rationale A. Phi1050phical-educational justification l. 2. 3. Equivalence to actual offered coursework, transfer credit. etc. Recognition of learning that is only possible via expe- riential learning (as in on-the job training). Competency equivalencies. B. Institution-oriented Justification 1. 0‘ ab “3 N e 0'5 e Attracting new types of students to the institution. Expanding the size of the student body. Using the institution's resources more efficiently. Responding to faculty interest. Moving into a new (Substantive) field or area. Improving. strengthening, or making more relevant a partic- ular existing major or cluster of majors. C. Student-oriented Justification Improving accessibility to job market or to graduate or professional school. Decreasing the time required for undergraduate education. Improving breadth of experience and practical relevance of a student's education. Increasing goal-oriented motivation of students. Enhancing self-directed change on the part of the student. Responding to special needs of special groups of students. Enhancing student participation in their assessment. 11. Defining Prggram Characteristics 1. 2. Administration of the program. Coordination of the program with other institutional affairs. III. A. 106 Size of the program (number of individuals involved). Duration of the program. Location of the program, facilities required. General educational objectives of the program. Intended learning outcomes of the program. General character of the processes involved in preparing. educating. and assessing the students in the program. Personnel in the program. Developing Faculty Resources Staffing 1. 2. Additional hiring for the program. Released time for existing personnel to the program. Faculty and staff training in assessment techniques. 1. 2. 3. 4. Extent and character of training needed. Number of staff needing such training. Faculty resistance. Clarification of faculty responsibilities. Relationship between non-traditional and traditional academic programs and faculties l. 2. 3. Joint versus exclusive assignment to experimental and traditional programs. Involvement in policy formation by faculty not participat- ing in the program. Methods for defining released time in experimental programs. IV. Defining Criterion Standards A. Basis for standards 1. 2. Institutional requirements. Student goals.. 3. 107 Employer requirements. 8. Nays standards can be defined Minimum competence versus levels of competence. Descriptions versus examples. Checklist of skills versus overall level of functioning. Expert judgment versus typical performance of a group of competent individuals. Competence demands in the field versus competence of students in a generally comparable course. C. People involved in setting standards 1. 2. 3. 4. Faculty in the program. Other faculty. Students. Field supervisors. 0. Properties of good standards 1. 2. 3. Fairness. Realism. Consistency. V. AdmittinggStudents 1. Determination of appropriate qualities of prospective students in relation to program objectives and characteristics. Dissemination of information concerning the program. Recruitment of appropriate students. Admission. a. Comparison Of students against appropriate qualities specified. b. Selection or advising students regarding appropriateness. Administration of financial aid to students. Orientation of students to the program. 108 VI. Financing the Program A. Expenditures 1. New personnel. a. Experts needed. b. Consultants versus full-time. c. Advisors from other academic institutions. 2. Released time to current personnel. 3. Overhead direct and indirect costs. 4. Cost of alternate procedures. 5. Equating resource allocations to credit-hours or other standard budget units. 8. Income 1. Special student fees. a. Assessment fee. b. Fee associated with credit granted. c. Special program fees. 2. Other income. a. Formula allocation of tuition, state funds. or endowment. b. Special programmatic allocation. VII. Preparing Students for Assessmegg A. Sponsored learning 1. 2. 3. 4. Orienting students to off-campus learning possibilities. Clarifying off-campus learning objectives. Developing learning contracts for field experience. Familiarizing student with assessment to expect. 8. Non-sponsored learning 1. Seminars on portfolio construction. 109 2. Formative advice on portfolio development and how it will be evaluated. 3. Identification of prior learning. 4. Articulation of learning to degree plan. 1. Measuring nature and extent of competence. 2. Determining whether competence meets requirements or corresponds adequately to learning plan. 1. Determining whether credit is to be awarded. 1. Faculty agreement regarding interpretation of standards and matching learning with requirements. 2. Faculty consistency (through tine across departments) in VIII. Appraising Learning Outcomes A. Defining requirements for appraisal 1. Documentation requirements. 2. Types of evidence of learning required. 8. Appraisal C. Evaluation 2. Determining amount of credit. D. Quality control of appraisal appraising competence. 3. Educative benefit of assessment. IX. IntegratinggLearning With Future Plans 1. Reporting assessment decisions to the student. 2. Discussing reasons for assessment decisions. 3. Reviewing original objectives with respect to the learning process and its evaluated outcomes. 4. Relating assessed competencies with respect to original objectives. degree requirements. and vocational interests. 5. Discussing in a formative. future-oriented sense the instructional and situational conditions which best fostered learning. Reviewing any which did not. 6. Initiating plans for future learning. 110 X. Coordinating Off-Campus Learning Sites 1. Locating positions “in the field“ that satisfy the program rationale and program characteristics. Placing students in off-campus positions consistent with individual student objectives and desires. Coordinating field living and working arrangements as necessary. Maintaining contact with students and supervisors as necessary throughout the off-campus work. Conducting formative evaluation. a. Progress toward learning objectives. b. Satisfaction with position. c. Recommendations for remainder of off-campus study. d. Changes in learning contract. XI. Advising Students A. Exploration 1. Values clarification. 2. Delineation of interests. 3. Career exploration. 8. Planning 1 Setting of individual educational goals. 2 Development of a plan for meeting goals. 3. Development of competence statements. 4 On—going review of the plan in a formative. critical fashion. C. Coping 1. Relating individual goals to learning possibilities that exist in the institution or community. 2. Helping student understand the value and relevance of assess- ment and learning outcomes. 111 XII. Defininngredit Policies 1. Delineating the domains of learning in which credit may be awarded. 2. Setting limits on amount and type of credit permitted via experiential learning. 3. Differentiating between records of experience and records of learning outcomes as acceptable evidence for credit. 4. Establishing credit equivalencies for particular competence requirements. 5. Determining procedures whereby amount of credit for unusual learning is estimated. 6. Establishing review and appeal procedures. XIII. Recording_Learning Outcomes A. Objectives 1. Translating learning outcomes into appropriate academic units. 2. Providing an account of the substance or nature of learning. 3. Characterizing the level or depth of learning to third parties. Monitoring progress toward degree. Serving student needs for credentials. Maintaining archives. meh . Protecting privacy. B. Recording methods Recording credit hours only. Designating credits for degree, area requirements, etc. Crediting equivalent course. bWND-d Recording credit and providing narrative description. (Willingham & Geisinger, 1976. pp. 5-9) 112 APPENDIX B Comparison of Goals of University and Overseas Education Some Major Goals of University Education Communication: To communicate flu- ently via the written word and, to a lesser extent, to speak well. To master the languages of abstraction and generalization, e.g., mathema- tics and science. To understand readily the reasoning, the ideas, and the knowledge of other persons through verbal exchange. Decision Making: To develop criti- cal judgment: the ability to test assertions, assumptions, and opin- ions against the hard facts and the criteria of logic. To reduce sus- ceptibility to specious argument and to be skeptical of intuition and emotion. To search for the best, most rational, most economi- cal, and elegant solution. Commitment: Commitment is to the truth. It requires an ability to stand back from ongoing events in order to understand and analyze them and to maintain objectivity in the face of emotionally involv- ing situations.. Difficult situa- tions are handled by explanations, theories,‘reports. Ideals: To value the great prin- ciples and ideals of Western soci- ety: social justice, economic progress, scientific truth. To value the sacrifice of present rewards and satisfactions for Some Divergent Goals of Overseas Education Communication: To understand and communicate directly and often non-verbally through movement, facial expression, person-to- person actions. To listen with sensitivity to the hidden con- cerns, values, motives of the other. To be at home in the exchange' of feelings, attitudes, desires, fears. To have a sympa- thetic, empathic understanding of the feelings of the other. Decision Making: To deve10p abil- ity to come to conclusions and take action on inadequate, unreliable, and conflicting information. To be able to trust feelings, atti- tudes, and beliefs as well as facts. To search for the possible course, the viable alternative, the durable though inelegant solu- tion. Commitment: Commitment is to peeple and to relationships. It requires an ability to become involved: to be able to give and inspire trust and confidence, to care and to take action in accord- ance with one's concern. Difficult situations are dealt with by stay- ing in emotional contact with them and by trying to take constructive action. Ideals: To value causes and objec- tives embedded in the here-and-now and embodied in the groups and per- sons in the immediate social envi- ronment. To find satisfaction, enjoyment, and self-esteem from 113 future advancement of these ideals and to find self-esteem and satis- faction from one's contribution toward distant social goals. Problem Solving: A problem is solved when the true, correct, reasonable answer has been dis- covered and verified. Problem solving is a search for knowl- edge and truth. It is a largely rational process, involving intel- ligence, creativity, insight, and a respect for facts. (Harrison & Hopkins, 1967, pp. 435-436) the impact one has directly on the lives of others who live mostly in the present and to work with them toward the limited, concrete goals which are important to them. Problem Solving: A problem is solved when decisions are made and carried out which effectively apply people's energies to overcoming some barrier to a common goal. Problem solving is a social pro- cess involving communication, interpersonal influence, con- sensus, and commitment. 114 APPENDIX C Meta-Goals of Traditional Vs. Intercultural Instruction Meta-Goals of Traditional College and University Classrooms Source of Information: Infor- mation comes from experts and authoritative sources through the media of books, lectures, audio-visual presentations. "If you have a question, look it up." Learning Settingg: Learning takes place in settings designated for the purpose, e.g., classrooms and libraries. Problem-Solving Approaches: Prob- lems are defined and posed to the learner by experts and authorities. The correct problem-solving methods are specified, and the student's work is checked for application of the prOper method and for accuracy, or at least for reasonableness of results. The emphasis is on solu- tions to known problems. Role of Emotions and Values: Prob- lems are largely dealt with at an ideational level. Questions of reason and of fact are paramount. Feelings and values may be dis- cussed but are rarely acted upon. Criteria of Successful Learning: Favorable evaluation by experts and authorities of the quality of the individual's intellectual pro- ductions, primarily written work. ApprOpriate Meta-Goals for Cross-Cultural Training Source of Information: Informa- tion sources must be developed by the learner from the social envi- ronment. Information-gathering methods include observation and questioning of associates, other learners, and chance acquaintances. Learning Settings: The entire social environment is the setting for learning. Every human encoun- ter provides relevant information. Problem-Solving Approaches: The learner is on his own to define problems, generate hypotheses, and collect information from the social environment. The emphasis is on discovering problems and developing problem-solving approaches on the Spot. Role of Emotions and Values: Prob- lems are usually value- and emotion- laden. Facts are often less relevant than the perceptions and attitudes which pe0p1e hold. Values and feel- ings have action consequences, and action must be taken. ' Criteria of Successful Learning: The establishment and maintenance of effective and satisfying rela- tionships with others in the work setting. This includes the ability to communicate with and influence others. Often there are no cri- teria available other than the attituudes of the parties involved (Harrison 8. Hopkins, 1967, pp. 437-438) 1“ the relatimship- 115 APPENDIX D Sample Institutions Adelphi University: Garden City. New York American University: Washington. D. C. Bloomsburg University: Bloomsburg. Pennsylvania Broward Community College: Pompano Beach. Florida College of St. Scholastica: Duluth. Minnesota Evergreen State College: Olympia. Washington Kent State University: Kent. Ohio Linfield College: McMinville. Oregon National College of Education: Evanston. Illinois National University: San Diego. California North Shore Community College: Beverly. Massachusetts Northland Institute: Midland. Michigan Ohio State University: Columbus. Ohio Rockland Community College: Suffern, New York Southern Illinois University: Carbondale, Illinois Stephens College: Columbia. Missouri Towson State University: Towson. Maryland University of Connecticut: Storrs. Connecticut University of Kentucky: Lexington. Kentucky University of North Carolina: Charlotte. North Carolina University of Northern Iowa: Cedar Falls. Iowa University of Oklahoma: Norman. Oklahoma University of South Alabama: Mobile, Alabama University of Wisconsin: Platteville. Wisconsin Webster University: St. Louis, Missouri+ 116 APPENDIX E Short-Form Survey MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY COULGE OF EDUCATION EAST [ANSING - NItLlllhAN o «cu-um DEPAITIENT Of ADMINISTIATION AND CUIUIICUIUM EMCILSON MALL April 12, 1984 A research study is currently being conducted through Michigan State University to identify and better understand what makes intercultural experiential education programs succeed. Likewise, we hope to identify some of the critical difficulties in developing and operating such programs and spell out implications for program improvement. We are particularly interested in obtaining your response because of the CAEL affiliation of your institution. Would you take about two minutes right now and respond to the following items and return them in thE_envelope provided? Thank you for your assistance during this very busy time of the year. 1. What kinds of intercultural experiential learning programs do you presently offer. ___Dne-month programs located in another culture __ Foreign travel-study .__ Study abroad options in foreign institutions ‘__ Full-semester (term) immersion experience in another culture ___Other: 2. Over the past five years have these programs in general: ___decreased ___remained the same ___increased 3. Do you have a printed statement of educational rationale about intercultural experiential education and its place in your programs? ___Yes __.No (If so, would you please enclose a copy of that statement in the return envelope with this survey. or send under separate cover?) 4. Please complete the following information concerning the person in your institution most closely related to the above programs who would have a detailed understanding of program operations? Name: Title: Address: Phone: Thank you for your kind assistance. Dr. Ted Hard 1 ‘ Prof. of Curriculum MSUI) an AI/Imalnv Arum/(final ()ppornuul v [muffin-u 117 APPENDIX F Telephone Interview Guide Introduction: From the information I have received from y0ur institution, I am aware you have several kinds of intercultural programs, but the purpose of this call is to focus upon only your full-semester interCuItELaLiflglierSlple‘Qgflllfl. This is the kind of program that typically places a student for a semester full-time in a culture different from the student's home culture, allowing a “hands-on“ experience in intercultural learning. I am asking that all your responses focus specifically on the full semester intercultural experiential program. 0k? I. Tell me how this intercultural semester program got started at you institution: 2. Hhen? 3. ' Who was the original instigator in getting the IP started at your instit? 4. What was their role at the time? 5. Did you use professional concultants to design y0ur program? 6. Where have you conducted IPs? . Is the program self-supporting? m N 0 Approximate start-up costs/first year? U) . Number of students first year? 10. Number of students on the average per year now? 118 MAJOR OBSTACLES 11. Tell me about some of the specific difficulties you encountered in developing your program 12. Briefly, what kind of difficulties do you continue to face in y0ur institution concerning intercultural programs? 119 13. Tell an about what kinds of things you do to prepare students for intercultural experiences. Prerequisite courses: Orientation workshop: 14. Voluntary participation? 15. Screening process? 16. Class level? 17. Additional fees? CURRICULUM 18. Do students write their own learning objectives? 19. Average size of a student group? 20. How often does a student group meet together on location? 120 21. Do students document their learning outcomes? How? 22. What do you think a student would say is the single preatest benefit of an IP? 23. How is a student's experience assessed? 24. Who determines the credit? 25. Possible range of credit? 26. Level of credit? 27 Course equivalences? 28. Has any research studies been done on academic performance of program participants? Results: Send copy? FACULTY PREPARATION 29. How are faculty prepared and involved in the IPS? 121 30. Essential characteristics of faculty group leader? 31. Special Faculty orientation training? General Areas: 32. Specific incentives for faculty involvement? 33. Central coordinator of all IPs? 34. Who is responsible for arrangements abroad? living? working? travel? Dr. . you have been very helpful and I greatly appreciate ‘9‘— your investment of tine. Have a good day. 122 APPENDIX G Survey Non-Respondents Follow-up Letter _§_§R§,WORRIED! You have probably already returned the short two-minute survey we mailed to you about two weeks ago; just in case yours was lost along the way, another copy is enclosed. Your response will be greatly appreciated since other phases of this research cannot be carried out until we receive this necessary data. If your response has crossed in the mail with this reminder, please disregard this reminder; if however your response has not been sent in yet, we would greatly appreciate your taking about two minutes right now to complete the attached page. Thank you for your assistance, xi _ l Research Project Director This item was printed on yellow paper to gain attention. 123 APPENDIX H Advance Letter to Sample Institutions MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY COUIGE OI EDUCATION EAST lANSING 0 MICHIGAN 0 ““440.“ warm OF ADHINISTIAHON AND CURRICULUM MN HALL This project involves interviewing selected individuals across the country who are knowledgeable about intercultural and experiential learning programs in general and full-semester—ihtercultural experiential education ro rams in specific. Herhope to better understand what the key programmatic and log- istical factors are which make such programs successful and effective. as well as identify some of the critical difficulties. I am writing in advance of our telephone call because I know how valuable your time is, particularly at this time of year. Altogether the interview should only take about fifteen minutes. If by chance I should call at an inconvenient time. please indicate that. and we can set a specific time for a later call. Your help and that of the select others being asked to participate in this effort to better understand what makes intercultural experiential learning programs effective, will both strengthen the evaluation end of existing pro- grams and give quality guidance to new program development. I will greatly appreciate your assistance in this effort. If you have any questions. please do not hesitate to contact me by phone at (517) 524-6833 or (517) 750-1200 ext. 292. Cordially. Bradley J. Smith Research Project Director MSU is a all/imam Anion/Equal Opportunity hummu- 124 APPENDIX I Suggested Interview Guide SUGGESTED INTERVIEW GUIDE During the telephone interview. we would like to touch upon the following sug- gested content areas. There may be some information which would be helpful for you to have at hand. He will attempt to follow the three-part sequence in the interview. Please give brief thought to each of the areas below. and I look forward to talking with you soon by telephone. 1m 1. How did intercultural programs get started at your institution? 2. What major obstacles were encountered in developing your programs? 3. How are students prepared for intercultural experiences? 4. How are faculty prepared for intercultural and experiential facilitation? 5. How are learning events structured and processed in your intercultural experiential programs? FIXED RESPONSES This will involve YES. NO, and short-answer responses to what you have done and currently do in your programs. HARD DATA 1. How many students are in your programs? 2. what are the major cost factors? 3. what has been the impact upon specific majors? 4. Hhat data has been gathered on learning outcomes? 5. What impact have your programs had on academic performance? 125 APPENDIX J Interview Appointment Sheet Telephone Interviewgfiuide NAME: INSTITUTION: ADDRESS:___ _— ..— .- PHONE: Goodmorning Dr. This is Bradley Smith and I wrote to you recently indicating I would be calling as part of a research project underway at Michigan State University, under the direction of Dr. Ted Hard who is an international figure in non-formal education. Dr. has recommended you as the most knowledgeable person about intercultural experiential education programs at , so I am interested in your response to some questions about what you have learned in making your programs work. I know your time is valuable and my questions should take only a few minutes. Is this a good time for you to talk? Yes No Wu"— When w0uld be a good time for me to call back? Thank you. I will look forward to talking with you then. If you have any questions about this research study, I would be happy to answer them now or later. Date Time Result Suggested Time To Callback Secretary's Name r—-.—m_- .a-‘v..- - -----—.-.—--s—- --. -.-_-—--—-H--—.—.---—-----—o--1 NA= No Answer NR= Will Return Call C= Completed Ref- Refused IC= Incomplete Interview BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Abrams. I. The impact of Antioch education through experience abroad. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Studies Association. Washington. D.C.. February 1978. Adler. P. S. Beyond cultural identity: Reflections upon cultural and multicultural man. Topic; in Culture ngrning. l974. 2_. 79. . The transitional experience: An alternative view of culture shock. JournaLlof Humanistic Psychology. 1975. l_§(4). l3-23. Allport. G. U. The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday Anchor. l958. Arensberg. C. M.. & Niehoff. A. H. Introducing social change: A manual for community development (2nd ed.). Chicago: Aldine-Atherton. l97l . Argyris. C.. 8: Schon. D. A. Theory in practice: Increasing professional effetiveness. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1974. Ashby. E. The university ideal. The Center Magazine. l963. 5(1). 37-41 . Baker. 6. C. Multicultural education: the state of the art. ERIC. l978. Barna. L. Stumbling blocks in interpersonal intercultural communication. Readings in Intercultural Communications. 1972. I. Benson. P. Measuring cross-cultural adjustment: the problem of criteria. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. l978. g0 ). 2l-37. Brislin. R. w. & Pederson. P. Cross-cultural orientation programs. New York: Gardner. l976. CAEL Institute: "Learning and Personal Development: The Synthesis of Knowledge and Experience". Georgetown University. August. l983. Carnegie Council on Policy Studies in Higher Education. A classification of institutions of higher education: rgvised gdition. Berkeley. Calif.: l978. Chickering. A. W. Education. work. and human development. In T. Little (Ed.). Making Sponsored Experiential ngrning Standard ngticg. New Directions For Experiential Learning. San Francisco: .Jossey-Bass. l983. . The modern american college. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. l981. 126 127 Claxton. C. 5.. 8: Ralston. Y. Learning styles: Their impact on teaching and administrjtion. Washington. D. C.: American Association for Higher Education. Report no. l0. l978. Cleveland. H.. Mangone. G. J. & Adams. T. C. The overseas americans. New York: McGraw Hill. 1960. David. K. Intercultural adjustment and applications of reinforcement theory to problems of culture shock. Trends. l972. 1(3). Dewey. J. Experience and edgation. New York: Macmillan. 1938. Doherty. A.. Mentkowski. M. & Conrad. K. Toward a theory of undergraduate experiential learning. New Directions for Experiential Learning. 1978. 1. 23-35. Downs. J. F. Fables. fancies and failures in cross-cultural training. Trends. 1969. _2_. 3. Duley. J. S. Learning outcomes: the measurement and evaluation of experiential learning. NSIEE Panel Resource Paper. #6. . Nurturing service-learners. Synergist. l98l. 9(3). l2-l5. Duley. J.. & Gordon. S. College sponsored expgfiential learning--A CAEL handbook. Columbia. Md.: CAEL. l977. Fiedler. F.. Mitchell. T.. & Triandis. H. The cultural assimilators: an approach to cross-cultural training. Journal of Applied Psychology, l97l. §_5_. 95-102. Fowler. J. U. Eaith: tha structural developmental perspectiva. (Report of the Research Project on Faith and Moral Development.) Cambridge. Mass.: Harvard University Divinity School. l975. Gray. A. w. International/Intarcultural education in selected stat_a collegaa and universitias: an overview and five cases. washington. D. C.: American Association of State Colleges and Universities. l977. Gudykunst. v. 8.. Hammer. M. R. 8: Wiseman. R. L. An analysis of an integrated approach to cross-cultural training. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. l977. 99-l 10. Gullahorn. J. & Gullahorn. J. An extension of the u-curve hypothesis. Journal of Sofil Issues. 1963. l_9_(3l. 33-47. Guthrie. G. M. A behavioral analysis of culture learning. In Richard w. Brislin. Stephen Bochner. & Walter J. Lonner (Eds.). Cross-cultural. perspectives on learning. New York: Sage and Uiley/Halstead. l975. Guthrie. G. M.. 8: Zektick. I. N. Predicting performance in the Peace Corps. Journal of Social Psychology. 1967. u. ll-l2. Hall. E. T. The Silent Language. New York: Doubleday. 1959. 128 Harrison. R. 8: Hopkins. R. L. The design of cross-cultural t_r_aining. with examples from the peace corps. Washington. D. C.. 1965. . The design of cross-cultural training: an alternative to the university model. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science. 1967. 3(4). 431-460. Hawes. F. 8: Kealey. D. J. An empirical study of Canadian technical assistance: adaptation and effectiveness on overseas assignment. International Journal of Intgrcultural Relations. 1981. §(3). 239:258. Hefferlin. J. B. L. Dynamics of Academic Reform. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1969. Heisler. M. K. Factors which differentiate united states students who wish to study in cross-cultural educational programs. Corvallis: Oregon State University. 1973. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation) Henson. H. Evaluating students' perceptions of study abroad programs. NAFSA NewslettJey. 1979. 311(6). l44ff. Houle. C. 0. Deep traditions of experiential learning. In M. T. Keeton (Ed.) Experiential Learning. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1976. Hull. U. F. Cross-cultural experiential programming. International Review of Education. 1981. .2_l(1). 64-75. Hull. W. F.. & Lemke. U. H. The assessment of off-campus higher education. International Review of Edgation. 1975. 2_1_. 195-206. Hull. v. F.. Lemke. W. H.. 8: Houang. R. T. The american undergraduate. off-campus and overseas; A study of the educational validity of such programs. New York: Council on International Educational Exchange. 1977. Hursh. B. A.. & Borzak. L. Toward cognitive development through field studies. Journal of Higllar Education. 1979. §(_J_(1). 63-77. International Education Act. In United States. 80 Statutes At Large, Section 2. 1066. 1966. Keeton. M. T.. 8: Tate. P. J. Learning by experience--what. why. how. New Directions for Experiential Learning. 1978. 1_. 1-8. Kendall. J. C. Values as the Core of Institutional Commitment: Finding a Common Ground. In T. C. Little (Ed.) Making Sponsored Expe______rientia1 Learning Standard practice. New Directions for Experiential Learning. 2_D_. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. June 1983. Klemp. G. 0. Three factors of success. In D. 1.1. Vermilye (Ed.) Relating work and aducation: Currant issues in higher education. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1977. Klineberg. 0.. & Hull. F. At a foraign university. New York: Praeger. 1979. 129 Knapp. J.. 8: Davis. L. Scope and varieties of experiential learning. New Directions for Experiential Learning. 1978. 1. 9-21. Kohlberg. L. Moral development and identification. In H. Stevenson (Ed.). Child Psychology. 62nd Yaarboolg National Society for tha study of education. Universiy of Chicago. 1963. 277-332. Kohls. L. R. Survival kit for overseas living. Chicago: Intercultural Network. 1979. Kolb. D. A. Laarning style inventory: self-scoring test and intarpre_tation booklet. boston: McBer and Company. 1976. Kraemer. A. J. Workshop in intercultural communigation. Alexandria: George Washington University Human Resource Research Organization. June 1974. Landis. 0.. 8: Brislin. R. W. (Eds.) l-_-l_andbook of intercultural traininLVolumes I. II. III. New York: Pergamon Press. 1983. Levinson. D. The seasons of a man's life. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. 1978. Lickona. T. (Ed.). Kohlberg. L.. Rest. J.. G: Selman. R. Moral development and beh_javior: Theory. research. and social issuaa. New York: Holt. Rinehardt and Winston. 1976. Little. T. C. HistorLand Rationala for Experiential Learning. PANEL Resource Paper No. 1. Raleigh. N. C.: National Society for Internships and Experiential Education. 1981 . . Taking an inventory of institutional experiential learning. In T. C. Little (Ed.) Making Sponsorad Experiential LejarninLStandard Practice. New Diragions for Experiential Learning. ga. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. June 1983. . The institutional context for experiential learning. Making Sponsored Experiential Learning Standard Practice. New Diractions for Ex eriential Learnin . 231. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. June 1983b. Loevinger. J. Ego develgpmant: concaptions and theories. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. l976. Mentkowski. M.. 8: Strait. M. A longitudinalfistudy of student change in gagnitiva devalopmentfand generic abilitiaLinjn outcome-centared liberal arts crriculum. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association. Montreal. April 1983. Milwaukee. WI: Alverno Productions. 1983. Oberg. K. Cultura shock and tha problem of adjustment to naw cultural eivironmants. Washington. D. C.: Department of State. Foreign Service Institute. 1958. 130 Pearson. R. P. Measuring adjustment and attitude change. New Directions for Experiential Learnirfi. fl! 1981. 21-30. Penland. P. R. Individual self-planned learning in Ameri_ca. Pittsburgh: Graduate School of Library and Information Sciences. University of Pittsburgh. 1977. Perry. W.. Jr. Forms of intellectual and ethicalgevelopmant in the college years: A scheme. New York: Holt. Rinehart & Winston. 1968. . Cognitive and ethical growth: the making of meaning. In A. Chickering (Ed.) The Modern American Colle e. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.1981. Peterson. R. E.. & others. Toward lifelong learning in America: A sourcebook for planners. Berkeley: Educational Testing Service. 1978. Piaget. J. Ih_e origins of intellifiance in childrag. New York: International Universities Press. 1952. Pool. 1. de 8. Effects of cross-national contact on national and internationalimages. In Herbert C. Kelman (Ed.). InternatioLal Behavior: A Socialfisychological Analysis. New York: Holt. Rinehart and Winston. 1965. Porter. R. E. An overview of intercultural communication. In Larry A. Samovar & Richard E. Porter (Eds.). Intercultural Communication: A Reader. Belmont. Calif.: Wadsworth. 1972. Rest. J. Development in judging moral issues. Minneapolis. MN: University of Minnesota Press. 1979. Ruben. B. Assessing communication competency for intercultural adaptation. Group Ed Organization Studies. 1976. 1_(3). 334-354. Ruben. B.. & Kealey. D. Behavioral assessment of communication competency and the prediction of cross-cultural adaptation. InternaLtional Journal of Intercultural Relations. 1978. 3(1). 15-47. Samovar. L. A.. 8: Porter. R. E. (Eds.). Intercultural communication: A reader (2nd ed.). Belmont. CA: Wadsworth. 1976. Schein. E.. 8: Bennis. W. Personal and organizational growth through the laboratory mathod. New York: Wiley. 1965. Sexton. R. F.. 8: Ungerer. R. A. Rtati0files for eiperiential education. ERIC/Higher Education Research Report. a. 1975. Statement of the task group on the role of experiential learning in cross-cultural programs. Michigan State University. mimeograph summary. March. 1979. Steinaker. W.. 8: Bell. M. R. The experientLal taxonomy. New York: Academic Press. 1979. 131 Tough. A. Major laminfifforts: Recent research and future decisions. Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies in Education. 1977. Trifonovitch. G. Culture learning/culture teaching. Educational Perspectives. 1977. l_6_(4). 18-22. Tucker. M.. Raik. H.. Rossikr. D.. & Uhes. M. Improving cross-cultural training and measurement for cross-cultural learning; Denver. CO: Center for Research an Education. 1973. Tumin. M. Valid and invalid rationales. In M. T. Keeton (Ed.). Experiential learning: San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1976. Valley. J. R. Experiential education and the nontraditional student. In Robert F. Sexton Dimensions of Expariential Education. National Center for Public Service Internship Programs. 1976. 9-16. Wallace. J. A. The educational values of experiential education. In D. Batchelder 8: E. Warner (Eds.). Beyond experience: TheLexperiential approach to cross-cultural education. Brattleboro. VT: The Experiment Press.1977. Weathersby. G.. & Henalt. A. J. Cost-effectiveness of programs. In M. T. Keeton (Ed.). Experiential Learning. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1976. Willingham. W. W. & Geisinger. K. F. Developing an operational model for assessing experiential learning. In W. W. Willingham & H. S. Nesbitt (Eds.).Implementinga Program for AssessinLExperiential Learning(CAEL Project Report). Columbia. Md.. 1976. Wilson. A. H. Cross-cultural experiential learning for teachers. Theory Into Practice. 1982. Q3). 184-192. GENERAL REFERENCE BIBLIOGRAPHY Abrams. I. The impact of antioch education through experience abroad. Alternative Higher Education. 1979. a. 176-187. Abrams. I Study abroad as cultural exploration. New Directions for Ex eriential Learnin . 181. 1_1_. 59-73. Astin. A.. et al. '_[l_1_a Amerigm Frashngn: National Norms for Fafl974. Los Angeles: Cooperative Institutional Research Program. American Council on Education. Laboratory for Research in Higher Education. University of California. 1974. Astin. A.. et al. lh_e American Freshman: Natiorgl Norms for Fall 1981. Los Angeles: Cooperative Institutional Research Program. American Council on Education. Laboratory for Research in Higher Education. University of California. 1981. 132 Backman. E. L. Approaches to international education. New York: Collier Macmillan Pub. 1984. Barrett. G. V.. 8: Bass. B. M. Cross-cultural issues in industrial and organizationl psychology. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.). Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally College Publishing Company. 1976. Batchelder. D. Training U. 8. students going abroad. Education and Traliing. 1978. _2_. 45-63. Batchelder. D.. 8: Warner. E. Beyond experience. The Experiment in International Living. Brattleboro. VT: The Experiment Press. 1977. Battsek. M. A practical analysis of some aspects of study abroad. Journal of Gengal Education. 1962. 1_3. 225-242. Bloom. B. S. )Ed.) Iaxonomy of educational objectives. handbook 1: Cognitive domain. New York: David McKay Co.. 1956. Brislin. R. W. Cross-cultural encountaegsi face-to-face interaction. New York: Pergaman Press. 1981. Cerroni-Long. E. L. Experiential learning and cultural anthropology. Experienifil Learning_and Cultural Models (NSIEE Panel Resource Paper 11a). 193. 6-11. Chickering. A. W. Developmental change as a major outcome: In M. T. Keeton (Ed.). Ex eriential Learnin . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1976. . Education and identity. San Francisco: Joseey-Bass. 1969. . Exparience__and laarning: An introduction to experiential learning. Change Magazine Press. 1977. Coelho. G. V. Personal growth and educational development through working and studying abroad. Journal of Social Issues. 1962. l_8_. 55-67. Colby. A. The relation between moral and cognitive development. In 0. Bush and S. Feldman (Eds.). Cognitive Dex/_alopment and Social Development: Relatioships_and Implications. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1975. Coleman. J. S. Differences between experiential and classroom learning. In Morris T. Keeton and Associates (Eds.). Experiential Learning. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1976. 49-61. Cross. K. P. Accent on laarnigg. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1976. Cross. K. P. The adult learner. Currant Iss_uas in Higher Education 1978. Washington. D. C.: American Association for Higher Education. 1978. Damen. W.. & Hart. D. The development of self-understanding from infancy through adolescence. Child Levelopment. 1982. Q. 841-864. 133 D'Anglejan. A.. & Tucker. G. R. Communicating across cultures: An expirical investigation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1973. 1(1). 121-130. DeCoster. D. A.. & Mable. P. (Eds.) Understandingjoday's Studants. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1981. Druian. G.. Owens. T.. & Owen. S. Experiential education: a search for common roots. Journal of Expariential Education. Fall. 1980. Dustin. D. L. Classroom and experiential education. Improving college and univarsity teaching. 1981. 24(4). 166-168. Erdynast. A. Field experience education and stage theories of development NSIEE Occasional Pa ers. 1981. Erikson. E. H. Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues Monograph. 1(1). New York: International Universities Press. 1959. Fayerweather. J. The internationalization of business. Illa Annala of tha American Academy of Political__and Social Scienca. 1972. 493. l-ll. Festinger. L. A theory of cognitiva_dissonance. Stanford. Calif.: Stanford University Press. 1957. Flack. M. J. Results and effects of study abroad. Annals of thafAmericaa A_(cademy of Political and Social Sciencfie. 1976. 4_2_4. 107-117. Fowler. J. W. Toward a developmental perspective on faith. Religious Education. 1974. 6_9_. 207-219. Freed. M. N. In quest of better questionnaires. Personnel and Guidance Journal. 1964. Oct.. 187-188. Gordon. S. C. Experiential education for the community college student. In Robert F. Sexton (Ed.) Dimansions of Experiential Education. National Center for Public Service Internship Programs. 1976. Goldstein. M. B. Legal issues in experiential education. (NSIEE Panel Resource Paper#3) . Policy issues in experiential education. (NSIEE Occasional Papers #3). 1982. Hall. E. T. _Tfia Hidden Dimension: Maa's Use of Space in Public and Privata. London: Bodley Head. 1966. Harris. J. 6.. Jr. Identification of cross-cultural talent: The empirical analysis f the peace corps. In R. Brislin (Ed.). Culture learning; Concapts applicamns and research. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii. 1977. 134 Haugsby. T. . Reflections on a typology for experiential learning. ExperientiaiEducation. 1980. 5(3). 3. Heath. D. Growing up in collage. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1968. Hefferlin. JB. L. Dynamics of academic reform. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. l969. Hemsley. T. R.. 8: Sell. 0. K. A study-abroad program: An examination of impact on student attitudes. Teaching Political Science. 1979. 6. 387-412. Hildebrandt. H. W. Communication barriers between German subsidiaries and parent American companies. Michigan Business Review. 1973. 25(4). 6-14. Holmes. H.. 8: Guild. S. A manual of teachinLtechniques for intarcultuv;_l education. Amherst. MA: UNESCO. 1971. Hoopes. D. S.. 8: Ingram. L. A team approach to expanding cultural awareness of faculty and staff. In D. S. Hoopes (Ed.). Raadings in intercultural communication. Vol. V: Intapcultural Programming. Pittsburgh. 1976. Jenks. C. L.. 8. Murphy. C. J. A Sampler of E>-:pgientiaLCoursea. San Francisco: Far West Laboratory for Educational Research and Development. 1981 . Kafka. E. P. Effects of Overseas Study On Worldmindedness:and Other Selected Variables of Liberal Arts Studenta. (Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation) Michigan State University. 1968. Kagitcibasi. C. Cross-national encounters: Turkish students in the United States. Interlational Journal of Intercultural Relations. 1978. _2_(2). 141-160. Kealey. D. J.. 8: Hawes. F. An empirical study of canadian technical assistance: Adaptation and effectiveness on overseas assignment. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 1981. 5. 239-258. Keeton. M. T. Experiential learning: Rationale. chapacteristics. and assessment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1976. Knowles. M. S. The modern practice of adult education: Andragogy versus pedagogy. New York: Association Press. 1970. Kohlberg. L. Stage and sequence: The cognitive developmental approach to socialization. In D. Goslin (Ed.). Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research. Chicago: Rand McNally. 1969. . The cognitive developmental approach to moral education. Phi Delta Kappa. 1975. 10. 670-677. Kohlberg. L.. 8. Mayer. R. Development as the aim of education. Harvard Educational Review. 1972. 3;. 426-434. 135 Kolb. D. Experiential learning: experiencus the source of Learning and development. Englewood Cliffs. N. J.: Prentice-Hall. 1984. . Stage theories and disciplinary learning environments: diverse pathways for growth. In. A. Chickering (Ed.). Baa modern american colle e. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. l979. Krathwohl. D. R.. Bloom. B. S.. 8: Masia. B. B. Iaxonomy of educational objectives. Handbook II: Affective domail'l. New York: David McKay Co.. 1964. Lambert. H. Comparison of cognitive development theories of ego and moral development. Proceedings of tha 80th Annuaij Convention of the American Phychological Association. 1972. 1. 115-116. Landers. T. Cross-cultural experience: A vital component of inservice education. NASSP Bulletin. Ap. 1973. 92-94. Lasker. H.. Moore. J.. 8. Simpson. E. L. Adult developmentand appiaaches to learning. (National Institute of Education). Washington. D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office. 1980. Leonard. E. W. Attitude change in a college program of foreign study and travel. Educational Record. 1964. 15. 1973-181. Levine. S.. 8: Gordon. G. Maximizing returns on mail questionnaires. Public Opinion Quarterlap. 1958. @2358). 568-575. Loevinger. J.. 8. Wessler. R. Measuring ego dcfllopmant I: construction and gag of agentence completion test. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 1970. Mayes. N. H. Teacher training for cultural awareness. Education and Trainin .1978. 2. 35-44. McGonigal. R. A. A Model for the Cross Cultural Intayaction Training of Adults. (Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation). Michigan State University. 1971. McHugo. G.. 8: Jernstedt. G. C. The affective impact of field experience education on college students. Alternative Higher Education. 1979. 3(3). Moore. D. T. Discovering the pedagogy of experience. Harvard Educational Review. 1981. _5_l_(2). 286-300. Murphy. .1. M.. 8. Gilligan. C. F. Moral development in late adolescence and adulthood: a critique and reconstruction of Kohlberg's theory. Human Develo ment. l980. ga. 77-104. Neff. C. B. Introduction: cross-cultural encounters. New Directions for Experiential Learning. l_1_. 1981. Nixon. J. E. The mechanics of questionnaire construction. Journal of EducflnflResaarch. 1954. 51(7). 481-487. 136 Oberg. K. Culture shock: Adjustment to new cultural environments. Practical anthropology. 1960. 1. 177-182. Ogbu. J. U. Origins of human competence: a cultural-ecological perspective. Child Development. 1981. §_2_. 413-429. Permaul. J. S. Monitoring and supporting experiential learning. NSIEE Panel Resource Paper #5. 1981. anister. A. 0. Impact of study abroad on the American college undergraduate. Denver: Colorado: University of Denver. 1972. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 063882). Pinar. W. (Ed.) Curriculum theorizing. Berkeley: McCutchan Pub. Corp.. 1975. Publication manual of the aflerican psychological association. 2nd edition. Baltimore. Md.: Garamond/Pridemark Press. Inc.. 1974. Robin. S. S. A procedure for securing returns to mail questionnaires. Sociology and Social Research. 24-35. Roizen. J.. Fulton. 0.. 8. Trow. M. Technical Report: 1975 Carnegie Council National Surveys of Highar Education. Berkeley: Center for Studies in Higher Education. University of California. 1978. Ruben. B. D.. Astcling. L. R.. 8. Kealey. D. J. Cross-cultural efectiveness. Ind. Hoopes. P. D. Pederson. 8. G. Renwick (Eds.). Overview of Intercultural Education. Training._and Research (Vol. 1: Theory). Washington. D. C.: Georgeton University. SIE TAR. 977. 92-105. Sell. D. K. Research on attitude change in U. S. students who participate in foreign study experiences: past findings and suggestions for future research. International Journal of Intercultural Rel_ations. 1983. _7_. 131-147. Sexton. R. F. Dimensions of experiential education. (National Center for Public Service Internship Programs). 1976. Sikkema. M.. 8. Niyekawa-Howard. A. M. Cross-cultural learning and self growth. New York: International Assoc. of Schools of Social Work. (Grant No. 5T21 MH 12582). Soth. N. Experiential education from a cultural viewpoint: the peer evaluation meeting as a ritual of enculturation. Alternative Higher Education. 1981. 6(2). 89-95. Spaulding. 8.. 8: Flack. M. J. The world's students in the United States: A review and evaluation of research on foreign students. New York: Praeger.1976. Stening. B. W. Problems in cross-cultural contact: a literature review. International Journal of IntméultLal Relations. 3. 269-313. 137 Stephenson. J. B.. 8. Sexton. R. F. Experiential education and the revitalization of the liberal arts. In. S. Hook. P. Kurtz. 8. M. Todorvich (Eds.) The philosophy of the curriculum: the need foganeraliducafition. Buffalo: Prometheus Books. 1975. Tillman. M. J. The lisle fellowship: A case study. New Directions for Experienfial Learning. 1981. ll_. 45-57. Triandis. H. C. Theoretical framework for evaluation of cross-cultural training effectiveness. International Journal of Intarcultural Relations. 1977. 1_(4). 19-45. Wallace. J. A. Characteristics of programs for study abroad. Journal of General Education. 1962. 1_3. 251-261. Walsh. W.B. Theories of person-environment interaction: Implications for the collage student. Iowa City. Iowa: The American College Testing Program. 1973. Whitaker. U. G. Equipping faculty for their emerging role as experiential educators. New Directions for Experiential Learning. 1978.1_. 61-71. Wilkins. M. The maturing of multinational entepprise. Cambridge. Mass.: Harvard University Press. 1974. Willingham. W. W. 8. Geisinger. K. F. Developing an operational model for assessing experiential learning. In W. W. Willingham 8. H. S. Nesbitt (Eds.) Implementing a program for assessing experiential learning. CAEL Project Report. 1976.