7"" +~m ,4 _ V : l: ‘ -~ KY Va WVML”: Shana Ugy’fawsiiy OVERDUE FINES: 25¢ per cm per its RETUMIM; LIBRARY MATERIALS: Place in book return to ream charge from circulation recon #u. ._ :4 fl“\\\\ s (sum fifimww : umnmmmmfl ) ,1 . It ‘ ”fir-388$ (V) A, f, - wln'¥: Fr , " z u (x (1‘ u‘" AA AAA, “ V. 100870i33 t THE IMPACT OF PREVENTIVE PRENATAL EDUCATION ON POSTBIRTH FAMILY ADJUSTMENT By Carol Jean Ducat A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1980 ABSTRACT THE IMPACT OF PREVENTIVE PRENATAL EDUCATION ON POSTBIRTH FAMILY ADJUSTMENT By Carol Jean Ducat Birth of a first child appears to be a time of develop- mental crisis and reorganization for married couples. Prior research suggested that lack of preparation for the degree of disorganization after birth and a subsequent drop in marital satisfaction is common for first-time parents. The present study assessed the ability of prenatal education classes to better prepare couples for the emotional adjust- ment to their first child's arrival. Two 10-week prenatal curricula were constructed. The traditional (T) curriculum focused on medically oriented education for pregnancy, labor, and delivery while the exPerimental (E) curriculum supplemented an abbreviated version of medical and physical content with materials aimed at emotional preparation for parenting. Materials added only to the E classes included communication skills, a decision-making model, information about the impact of a child on the marital dyad, and preparatory planning for postbirth changes. Sixty-six volunteer couples were recruited from applicants for enrollment in local prenatal Carol Jean Ducat education classes. After recruitment, these couples were randomly assigned to type of curriculum and to pretest or no pretest conditions. Mailed questionnaires and semi-structured telephone interviews were completed by one-half the couples before prenatal classes and by all accessible participants at 1 month and 1 year postbirth. Measures included marital satisfaction, communication frequency and satisfaction, decision-making satisfaction, adjustment to life changes, sexual adjustment, degree of discrepancy between the eXpecta- tions and the reality of how parents divided childcare tasks,2nuiparental attitudes toward the baby. These were condensed into five scales: (a) Communication/Decision- Making/Marital Satisfaction, (b) Crisis Adjustment, (0) Sexual Adjustment, (d) Discrepancy Between Actual and EXpected Division of Tasks, (e) Attitude Toward the Baby. Monitered were such potentially-confounding variables as socio-economic status, type of marriage relationship (traditional versus companionate roles), difficulty of the pregnancy, attitudes toward own parents, and attitudes of parents and instructors toward the prenatal classes. Of the 66 couples, 33 completed pretesting, 38 completed the 1-month postbirth observation, and 39 provided data when their children were 1 year-old. Seventeen couples provided Carol Jean Ducat data at all three observations. Compared to E couples, T couples tended to have more traditional role marriages (p 5:.08),to view their marital satisfaction as higher (p .S .06), and to view their own parents more positively (2 5-02). From prebirth to 1 year after birth, E couples tended to increase slightly in marital satisfaction while T couples generally declined or dropped. This change was in the predicted direction, however, a 2 x 2 (education X pretest) multivariate analyses of variance MANOVchWLmonth and 1-year data revealed no statistically significant differences (p 5,.05) between the two groups of couples. A repeated measures MANOVA of the 17 couples with data from all three observations also revealed no significant differences between treatment groups. At 1-month postbirth, however, both T and E couples reported significant disruptions in daily routines (p 5;.05). Post hoc regression analyses revealed that increased postbirth marital satisfaction for both husbands and wives was significantly related to a more traditional view of marriage roles and to a less pleasant pregnancy eXperience. Positive pggtbirth change in marital satisfaction for new fathers was linked with more satisfactory prgbirth decision-making procedures. Lack of significant treatment group differences was attributed to such research difficulties such as: (a) low Carol Jean Ducat receptivity toward the new curriculum due to timing and presentation, (b) near-significant pretreatment differences between groups, (c) the impracticality of a valid, nontreated control group, and (d) inadequate statistical power due to small sample size. Recommendations for future research emphasized the need for evaluation of the role of statistical significance where small samples and intergroup differences are likely. Additional research was suggested to identify: (a) optimal times for intervention, (b) alternative forms of intervention, and (c) methods of enhancing recipients'receptivity. DEDICATION Dedicated to my parents Mary and Arthur Coakes, who instilled and encouraged my desire for education. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This research would have been impossible without the support and cooperation of many people. R My committee members were extremely helpful. John Hurley provided nondirective support in my decision to undertake this study. He further remained available for countless hours of consultation. Ralph Levine freely offered time and assistance with statistical analyses. Lucy Ferguson and Bill Davidson likewise remained available and supportive. Cooperation of the EXpectant Parent Organization was critical in providing access to subjects, developing class materials and presenting the curricula. Special thanks belong to Carol Buzzitta, Babbette Clough, Joann Reisig, LeeAnn Roman, Andrea Schewe and Mary Ann Sesti. The many new parents who shared their reactions and gave of their valuable time are also greatly appreciated. ~Support in funding, materials, computer time and printing were donated by the Ingham County Board of Commissioners and the Michigan State University Cooperative Extension Office and Department of Psychology. Undergraduate research assistants in many instances worked under personal time pressures to complete tasks of iii interviewing and data tabulation. Their enthusiasm for the task was refreshing in times of discouragement. Special thanks go to Beth Dick and Sheryl Goldberg. Finally, my family and friends provided much-needed support both emotionally and physically. Their support ranged from understanding of my discouragement, to timely exortations to continue the work, to provision of relief from the parenting role so that my energies could be focused on completion. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . viii LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix Chapter I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Crisis Theory . . . . . 3 Pregnancy as a Developmental Crisis . . . 7 Parenthood as a Developmental Crisis . . . 1a A Sociological View of the Discrepancies . 28 A Parting Shot . . . . . 31 Education and Preventive Mental Health . . 36 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 II. METHODOLOGY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 Subject Assignment . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 Treatment Conditions . . . . . . . 47 Instructor Selection and Training . . . . 48 Controls for Research Design . . . . . . . 49 Assessment Procedures . . . . . . . . . . 50 Statistical Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . 55 III. RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Scale Construction . . . . . . . 57 Pretreatment Equivalence in Groups . . . . 61 Equivalence in Classroom Treatment . . . . 63 Tests of Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . 64 Post Hoc Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 Chapter Page IV. DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73 Illustrations of Families EXperiencing Great Life Changes . . . . . 75 Implications for Prenatal Education . . . 80 Limitations and Difficulties of This Study . . . . . . . . . . 81 Statistical Limitations . . . . 82 Comparability of Treatment Groups . . 87 Design Difficulties . . . . . . 88 Extent and Timing of Treatment . . . . 89 Presentation of the New Program . . . 92 Discussion of Post Hoc Analyses . . . . . 94 Overview of Research . . . . . . . . . 100 Formulating the Problem . . . . . . . 100 Locating Subjects . . . . . . . . . 101 Designing the EXperiment . . . 103 Identifying Variables for Measurement. 104 Maintaining the Relationship with the Sponsoring Organization . . . . . 106 Locating Funds . . . . . . . . . 108 Conducting the Treatment . . . . . . . 108 Collecting Data . . . . . . . . . . . 109 Analyzing Data . . . . . . . . 111 Suggestions for Future Research . . . . . 113 BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 APPENDIX A Introductory Letter 124 APPENDIX B Research Contract . 125 APPENDIX C Background Information 127 APPENDIX D - Course Content for T and E 128 APPENDIX E - Curriculum Workshop . 134 APPENDIX F Class Topic Checklist 136 APPENDIX C Instructor Evaluation Form 137 APPENDIX H Instructor Reports . . 138 APPENDIX I Questionnaires 139 APPENDIX J Interviewer Training 166 APPENDIX K Outline of Telephone Interviews . . 168 vi APPENDICES (Continued) APPENDIX L APPENDIX M APPENDIX N APPENDIX O APPENDIX P APPENDIX Q APPENDIX R Interscale Correlations for Original Communication, Decision-Making and Marital Adjustment Sub-Scales Before Creating Combined CDMA Interscale Correlations at Each Time of Observation . Item Content for Scales Scale Items' Reliabilities and Correlations with Own and Other Scales . . . . . . . Summary of Alpha Coefficients for Each Scale at Each Observation . . . . . . . . . Correlations Between Husbands' and Wives' Scale Scores at Each Time of Observation . Regression Analysis Summary Tables . . . . . vii Page 172 173 177 181 190 191 192 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Average g-Scores for Couples with Complete Data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 viii Figure LIST OF FIGURES Time Flow of EXperimental Procedures Percentage of T and E Subjects Attending Each Class Period . . . . . . Average Z- Scores on Communication/ Decision-Making/Marital Adjustment (CDMA) Scale for Couples with Complete Data . . . Average Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scores for All Data Collected at Each Observation . . . . . . ix Page 44 65 85 86 CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION "I remember as a student I delivered a child to a 36-year—old woman. It was her first child although she had been married for many years and she expressed no joy in this. She was glad it was over, she wished she could have had an abortion. Would she like to know if the child was all right? No. The next day I asked her how she felt about things. She said she was stuck with it. I remember this. It's been fifteen years, but I still shudder to think of what's happened to that child." (Silverman & Silverman, 1971. p- 24) "From Tampa, Fla.: I am 40, my husband is 45. We have two children under the age of eight. I was an attractive, fulfilled career woman before I had these kids. Now I'm an exhausted, nervous wreck who misses her job and sees very little of her husband. He's got a 'friend,’ I'm sure, and ' I don't blame him. Our children took all the romance out of our marriage. I'm too tired for sex, conversation or anything else. Sign me--Too Late For Tears" (Ann Landers, 1976) "And finally, one day it dawned on me that I actually did feel guilty for having failed completely as a mother. This sense of failure seemed to invade every other aspect of my life. I almost felt unsuccessful as a person. As soon as I realized this, my spirits began to lift. Of course I had failed-~I had set goals for myself that were impossible to achieve...My preparation had not been to eXpand my knowledge, but to plan for a kind of 'fairy tale' existence." (Harrison, 1976) Current literature abounds with reports of disappointed parents. The implications of these unhappy parents for the mental health of their children are sobering. In fact, casual observation of community mental health programs would indicate an increasing number of children and parents seeking psychiatric help. Even more distressing is the apparent shortage of mental health workers who are competent to assist in such problems (Blau, 1969; Miller, Hampe, Barrett & Noble, 1972). What can be done to alleviate this condition? Response to the situation can generally take one of two forms--increase the number of mental health workers or decrease the number of mentally ill through prevention. Extensive training and use of paraprofessional mental health workers is an example of the former reaction. While such responses have been surprisingly effective (Carkhuff, 1968), they have the quality of the actions of the captain of a leaky ship who stocks up on life preservers and inflatable rafts before setting to sea. Just as the ship captain might more effectively take preventive action, the field of mental health might do (well to inspect its theoretical framework for potential mental health "leaks". Gerald Caplan (1961) has been a leader in this area both by introducing the concept of prevention on a wide scale and by investigating some of the principles involved. The family is quickly becoming a central focus of such discussions, both as an incubator for mental distress and as a support system in times of stress. The general focus of this study was the evaluation and prevention of mental stress in young families. The intervention was focused on the birth of the first child, an easily pinpointed event. Crisis Theory In general, crisis has been described as an event in a person's life which disrupts his/her usual equilibrium and confronts him/her with a situation in which previous coping behaviors are no longer adequate. Some of the earliest work done in this area examined a person's reaction to bereavement in disaster settings (Lindemann, 1944). Not only would uneXpected events such as disasters fall under the crisis heading; but according to definition, any event for which the individual's usual coping behaviors are no longer adequate could be termed a crisis. Accord- ingly, some writers have eXplored the concept of development- al crises occurring at significant change points in a person's normal life eXperiences. For example, entry in school for the five-year-old (or nursery school for the three-year-old) is a life eXperience for which previous habitual behaviors may no longer be adequate. Erickson (1968) Spoke of this type of change when he said, "When the human being, because of accidental or developmental shifts, loses an essential wholeness, he restructures himself and the world by taking recourse to what we may call totalism." (p.81) An important dynamic of such adjustments has been illuminated by Parkes (1965) who, while studying 21 psychiatric patients who had lost a spouse within six months of treatment, found that the intensity of grief began to decline in one to six weeks and was minimal after six months. This dynamic becomes crucial in the treatment of people adjusting to crisis as it is during this time of reorganization that a person is more anxious and open to therapeutic intervention (Sifneos, 1972). Crisis theory further asserts that the new coping behaviors developed during this time of stress may result in better or worse psychological adjustment than that which existed before the crisis (Argles & Mackenzie, 1970). Even more important, successful resolution of a crisis has been seen as increasing an individual's coping ability in later stressful situationse-not only has his/her self- esteem been boosted, but his/her general ability to approach a second crisis has been augmented by success (Parad & Caplan, 1960; Caplan, 1961; Sheehy, 1976). Eliot (1955) stated that if the problems created by a crisis remain unsolved or become intensified, they are likely to lead to maladjustment in the form of breakdown, overt conflict or intermittent crisis situations. In his opinion, successful resolution of these problems should include (a) thorough catharsis, (b) acceptance of elements beyond control, (c) relaxation of tensions, (d) reorientation to new situation, (e) ability to use one's eXperiences as fully as before the crisis and (f) re-establishment of stable habits, self—control, etc. These same individual crisis principles have been applied to the family. Hill (1949) noted that within the family structure itself (that is, ignoring other external natural disasters or nationally imposed disasters, such as war) there are three subtypes of crisis: (a) those of accession (e.g., births), (b) those of dismemberment, and (c) those characterized by demoralization (e.g., non- support, infidelity, alcoholism, etc.). Hill (1965) further refined his definition of crisis by Specifying three general characteristics which interact to influence the outcome of a crisis: (a) the stressful event, (b) the resources of the family, and (c) the definition the family gives to the event, i.e, do they define it as a threat. Hill described a crisis as a disorganization in role patterns or a conflict over ownership of roles. In family terms, this is comparable to the earlier definition of crisis as a situation in which old patterns of behavior (in the case of the family, "roles") are no longer effective. Once again, as in the case of individual crisis theory, families can be seen as eXperiencing developmental crises. Scherz (1969) has outlined these developmental points as occurring: (a) with new marriage, (b) at birth of first child, (c) at child's development of mobility (independence), (d) at child's entrance in school, (e) during child's adolescent identity struggle (see also Weiner, 1966), (f) when children have all left home (see also Bruehl, 1971), and (g) at old age. Rappaport (1963) has looked at the tasks of early marriage in great detail. She defined intrapersonal tasks, such as preparing oneself for the new role of husband/wife and accomodating patterns of gratification which have been used premaritally to the new married patterns. She also defined interpersonal activities, such as establishing mutually satisfying patterns of dealing with outsiders and dealing with decision-making. Scherz attempted to define some of the tasks and conflicts as emotional separation versus interdependence, closeness or intimacy vs distance, and self-autonomy vs other responsibility. Additionally, he defined some tasks as specific to a given maturation point. For example, during engagement and early marriage, both partners must develop ways of separating from their original families and finding new ways of remaining connected with the original family without detracting from the new, beginning family. Rappaport & Rappaport (1968) spoke more generally about such transition periods. "...there is set up around each of these major transition points a period of crucial flux both within the individuals themselves and in the inter- personal relationships. Previous patterns of behavior are at least in part inappropriate to the new situation and new patterns are called for. During the period of relative 'unorganization' which is inherent in these transitions, there may emerge new patterns of social behavior which soon become crystalized into relatively stable patterns; in addition, individuals may review and have 'another chance' at reorganizing their individual patterns of motivation, perception, eXpression, defence and behavior in general.” (p. 33) Note the similarity between these conditions and the commonly accepted definition of crisis. With regard to the potential adjustment to such situations, Parkes (1971) made a similar statement. Rejecting the terms "crisis" and "stress", he called such events "psycho-social transitions" and defined them more specifically as major changes which occur over a relatively short period of time, are lasting in effect, and affect the individual's "assumptive world" (that is, a person's perceptions or eXpectations of his world). Parkes further noted that such major transitions can affect all members of the family and necessitate the restructuring of the family unit. Pregnancy as a Developmental Crisis Let us now narrow our focus to the specific develop- mental crisis of parenthood. Pregnancy, of course, is the first step in this process of becoming a parent. The effect of pregnancy on the mother has been an area of intense focus. In 1951, Caplan reported the outcome of small discussion groups for pregnant women. He noted that eXpectant mothers seem to be more concerned with their emotions and with emotional problems than at other times in their lives. In later work Caplan (1961) observed that pregnant women seemed more susceptible to stress. This observation was made in light of pregnant women's responses to psychological tests. He found that, much like the adolescent test protocol, the normal protocol for a pregnant 'woman was indicative of psychosis, even for women who had no previous history of mental health difficulties. In such tests and the interviews which followed, expectant mothers' dream-like fantasies increased while old conflicts surfaced again. There seemed to be a weakening of external supports to the ego, mood swings, changes in appetites (including sexual), introversion and passivity, and changes in ego-id equilibrium. Caplan hypothesized that these changes might be caused by hormonal changes, role changes, or social-economic worries. He further noted that shortly after pregnancy, the seemingly psychotic material was repressed and the mother not only returned to normal but also forgot that she had eXperienced the stresses described above. In this case, each subject was used as her own control. Along the same lines, Schwartz (1975) examined the reactions of five social caseworkers who were experiencing their first pregnancies. Several of these workers found their pregnancy arousing conflicts and doubts that they had not previously eXperienced. Although some of the conflicts centered around loss of professional identity or the respons— ibility of leaving clients, some of them involved simply what was described as a younger, inexperienced feeling on the part of the caseworker as she functioned in her job. Wenner & Ohaneson (1967) used open-ended interviews to study women's adjustment to their pregnancies. Fifty-two women were seen weekly in a therapy-type setting. These researchers noted that some women evidenced a loss of emotional stability during their pregnancies. Motivation for the pregnancy appeared to play a role in the degree of disturbance eXperienced. Colman & Colman (1973) reported observing a similar change in pregnant women. They found their subjects to be emotionally labile and inclined toward discussing sub- jective eXperiences. The pregnant women further showed a greater accessibility of dream material and a greater need for reassurance. After birth the women were observed to return to a more external orientation and normal behavior in other aSpects. These researchers speculated that it is logical for women to be internally oriented during preg- nancy since they are likely to be acutely aware of inner bodily changes. All the above studies shared a common difficulty in their lack of control groups of nonpregnant women with which to compare the data generated. Perhaps just the psychological focus in a therapeutic setting changed the 10 emotional vulnerability of the women under study. However; the number of studies and commonalities in their findings does lend informal support to the belief that pregnancy, the first step in becoming a parent, is a time of emotional stress for women. While historically very little has been said about the father's reaction to pregnancy, it is now receiving greater attention. Certainly there are logical reasons for an eXpectant father to be reacting to impending parenthood. On a more superficial (and sexist) level, he may be reacting with pride in his accomplishment. In addition, if his wife is behaving in the manner indicated above, the husband is forced to change his behavior to interact with her. A number of studies support the above contention. Col- man & Colman (1973) reported that although men usually do not readily admit to their own emotional upheavals, eXpectant fathers with whom they worked were indeed eXper- iencing such disturbances in response to the pregnancy. In interviewing 20 eXpectant fathers, Obrzut (1976) found 70% initially eXpressing ambivalent feelings toward fatherhood. An indication that at least some expectant fathers adapted dysfunctionally is reported by Hartman and Nicolay (1966) who found that within court cases examined at a psychiatric clinic, eXpectant fathers were disproportionately represented in the population of those who had committed sexual offenses, particularly offenses of a regressive or acting—out nature. 11 EXpectant fathers find themselves faced with the possi- bility of increased financial strain. Since our culture defines this function of providing for the family as the husband's role, such a strain could influence the father's self-confidence. This may particularly be the case when a working wife terminates her employment leaving the family dependent on one income. One study of young, upwardly mobile, eXpectant fathers by Bernstein & Cyr (1957) concluded that only two-fifths of the 31 fathers interviewed expressed open delight at the coming birth. The remainder felt only resigned to the birth or somewhat unhappy about it. The dissatisfaction was particularly related to timing and financial strain. In our present society, one wonders if it is indeed possible to be financially prepared for raising a child. In 1971 the average middle-class family could eXpect to spend $25-30,000 (excluding college education) in raising each of its children (Silverman & Silverman, 1971). Current projections are undoubtedly higher. A study of 60 primiparae and their husbands was undertaken by Liebenberg (1967). His subjects were a relatively homogeneous group of middle class, college- educated parents. He noted that his normal fathers displayed some degree of personality disturbances such as exagger- ated dependency, acting-out, reactivation of unresolved parental problems, somatic symptons, and sexual disturbances. That researcher was impressed with the husbands' ability to 12 use a counselor for support and insight during the preg- nancy period. He also noted that the husbands' participation in counseling almost uniformly facilitated communication and closeness between husband and wife. Certainly any father who is already eXperiencing psychological instability is more likely to be sensitive to the potential, perhaps unconscious, conflicts his child's birth may involve. Curtis (1955) verified such a reaction in three groups of military men where each group had experienced a different degree of psychological adjustment prior to pregnancy. In those cases, the added stress of the change in life may have increased the father's current psychological problems. LaCoursiere (1972) focused discussion on the dependency needs of the husband. As the wife withdraws her attention and focuses on herself, her fetus, and finally the baby, the husband is hypothesized to lose her attention. Thus, the husband, particularly if his own dependency needs were met inadequately in childhood and he has sought their fulfillment in the marital relationship, may eXperience frustration and anger toward his wife and/or child. Freeman (1961) has observed this dimension of hostility in eXpectant fathers. If this reaction causes guilt in the father, depression may ensue. Whether such feelings are repressed or eXpressed, they would create difficulties for the new father. 13 Further support of the increased stress on eXpectant fathers can be found in the incidence of violence during pregnancy. Gelles (1974 & 1975) reported an exploratory study in which, of 80 families interviewed, 55% reported at least one incident of the husband's acting violently toward his wife. Nearly one-quarter of these violent episodes occurred while the wife was pregnant. Interviews with the women who eXperienced beating during pregnancy implicated the husband's sexual frustration, the stress of family transition and changes in activities, biochemical changes in the wife predisposing her to be more critical or depressed, and the defenselessness of the wife as possible triggers for the violence. In a lighter vein, husbands' indentification with the pregnant wife is obvidus in some of the so-called "primitive" customs of couvade. In its simplest form, couvade consists of the father reacting to his child's delivery by either experiencing the labor symptoms or by secluding himself for a short time after his child's birth and restricting his diet almost as if he were giving birth. It is possible that modern man has not given up couvade but'has merely become more sophisticated in his expression of it. In fact, incidence of this phenomenon has been found by some researchers (quoted in Panter & Linde, 1975) to be as high as 20%. Several authors (Hartman & Nicolay, 1966; La- Coursiere, 1972; Trethowan & Conlan, 1965; and Trethowan, 14 1968) have observed the occurrence of physical symptoms in eXpectant fathers which resemble those of the pregnant woman. They noted loss of appetite, nausea, gastrointes— tinal symptoms, and vomiting. Toothaches have also been noted to be unusually prevalent in eXpectant fathers. More unusual and quite uncontrolled observations of an eye specialist (Inman, 1941) and a psychotherapist (Abenheimer, 1946) related specific eye disorders, particularly styes and tarsal cysts, to what was termed an "unusual preoccupa- tion with birth". Trethowan, using a sample of 548 expectant and noneXpectant fathers, observed that symptoms peak during the third and ninth month of pregnancy. He further described a correlation between these symptoms and anxiety about the wife's pregnancy. Hott (1976) partially supported her view of the sequence of problems eXperienced by expectant fathers by turning to popular music. She noted a succession of Paul Anka's recent hits, beginning with "You're Having My Baby", proceeding to "You're a One-Man Woman and I'm a Two-Timing Man", and ending in "I Don't Like to Sleep Alone". Our poor eXpectant fathers are thus seen as the battleground for untold conflicts which they nobly bear in silence--and maybe even in ignorance. Parenthood as a Developmental Crisis But how about parenthood itself? Can it be legitimately called a crisis for married couples? This is a subject of 15 many conflicting views. First this report will examine the literature supporting the crisis viewpoint and then the objections to it. In 1957, LeMasters published some of the first empirical data exploring the effect of a child's birth on parents. Forty—six primiparous parents with children under five years-old were interviewed using a rather unstructured approach. The parents were restricted to middle-class couples between the ages of 25 and 35. No control group was employed. Interview datawere collected to indicate degree of crisis as none, slight, moderate, extensive, or severe. (The rating was arrived at by agreement between the parent and the interviewer.) Crisis was defined as any "sharp or decisive change for which old patterns are inadequate." Eighty-three percent of LeMasters' new parents reported 5 extensive or severe crisis in adjusting to the first child. They observed that extensive or severe crisis eXperience was the norm for the sample. They further noted that these couples did not have a history of maladjustment. All 38 couples reporting a large degree of crisis did appear to have romanticized views of parenthood and felt that they had little, if any, preparation for parental roles. Some of the mothers interviewed were actually bitter about their lack of training. LeMasters eXplained his results by pointing out several possible factors: (a) parenthood may be the real "romantic complex" in our society; (b) adolescents are not prepared 16 in any formal way for parenthood; (c) there is increased complexity in a triad as opposed to a dyad; and (d) parenthood may mark the final transition to adulthood. LeMasters concluded that even when the final adjustment to parenthood is healthy, as in nearly all the cases he studied, parents are confronted with a crisis upon the birth of their first child. A major limitation of the LeMasters' study was its lack of a control group. Perhaps childless couples between 25 and 35 Years of age also experience a sense of re—organi- zation and adjustment but are forced to find something other than a child to blame it on. Recent observations by Sheehy (1976) have indicated that developmental crises occur in general during this age group. Next, only a general restriction was placed on the age of the first child at the time of the parental interview. The child could have been as old as 5 years allowing many intervening variables other than childbirth to have affected the parents' responses. Furthermore, the sample was obtained by informally identifying new parents rather than by attempting to obtain a representative cross section. Perhaps those parents who most readily agreed to be interviewed were those who experienced more problems, or perhaps those parents most readily identified informally were those having trouble with their young child. 17 Finally, in the joint assessment of degree of crisis,' parents could have been cued by interviewers‘ eXpectations that childbirth caused crisis. In 1963, Dyer published results of a second study which further supported LeMasters' positon of crisis at .parenthood. Again Dyer limited his sample to middle-class, unbroken marriages. His couples were contacted within two years of the birth of their child. Thirty-two couples were polled with separate questionnaires administered to both husband and wife. A Likert—type scale was devised to measure the extent to which a child's arrival constituted a crisis in the following areas of family life: (a) husband- wife division of labor, (b) husband-wife division of authority, (c) husband-wife companionship patterns, (d) family income and finances, (e) home-making and house-work, (f) social life and recreational patterns, (g) husband and wife mobility and freedom of action, (h) child-care and rearing, (1) health of husband, wife, and child, and (j) extra-family interests and activities. Dyer reported the following distribution of crisis scores for the couples studied: No crisis--none; Slight crisis--9%; Moderate crisis--38%; Extensive crisis-—28%; and Severe crisis--25%. While this distribution was more moderate than the one LeMasters found, it essentially did support the belief that couples eXperience crisis upon the birth of a child. Dyer's research can be criticized along similar lines to those applying to leMasters‘ findings. Improvements were 18 made in limiting the age of the oldest child. Also by presenting a more Specific focus on ten areas of family life, the couples' responses would have been less easily influenced by a "halo" effect or by interviewer bias and expectation. Again, the method of obtaining a sample was questionable since community residents were simply asked to supply the names of first-time parents. On the other hand, while the samples in the two studies above may not have been representative, they did indicate the existence in typical communities of couples eXperiencing stress with the advent of a child. It is the extent of these feelings in the general population which can most easily be questioned. Unpublished work by Feldman (1965) compared couples with children to couples without children. Feldman found childbearing couples: (a) displaying significantly less verbal communication with each other, (b) talking about children more than about themselves or their relationship, (0) feeling less close to each other as a result of conver- sations, (d) having lower marital satisfaction, (e) having a higher value of marriage, (f) having less marital inter- actions, and (g) being more responsive to conflict. Later, Feldman (1971) examined middle-and upper- middle class couples in both a cross-sectional and a short-term longitudinal study. In both approaches, repre- sentative samples of urban couples were used. First, in a 19 cross-sectional approach, Feldman studied the changes in marital satisfaction and other variables over the life cycle. He found a curvilinear relationship with high happiness or satisfaction occurring in the early years of marriage and in later years after childrearing and with low points being when the last child began school and when the family had teenagers. In the same cross-sectional study, compari- sons were made between those couples who had a child in the home and those who never had children. Those with children had a significantly lower level of marital satisfaction than those without, even though there was no statistical difference in the length of marriage for the two groups. This surprise finding led the author to undertake a more detailed longitudinal study of the effects of becoming parents on a marriage. Feldman compared three groups of married couples over anine-month period. The main study group was composed of primiparous couples. A control group was composed of couples withoutchildren and a third group, to test for the effects of replication of parenthood, included couples having their second child. Measurements occurred at 5 months prepartum and 5 weeks and 5 months postpartum. The following findings arose from that study: (a) While the majority of couples decreased in satisfaction upon the birth of their child, some couples, particularly those with a differenti- ated marital relationship rather than a companionate one, 20 did increase their marital satisfaction. (b) In those marriages with improved satisfaction as a result of childbearing, the mother's attitude emerged as important in that high maternalism and/or high eXpectation on the mother's part that her husband would take part in many of the child-rearing tasks were positively related to increase in satisfaction. (0) A negative attitude toward pregnancy was related to a positive shift in satisfaction after the birth of the child. (d) Both primipara and multipara eXperienced the crisis of parenthood with multipara demonstrating an even greater negative effect. (e) The changes related to children werelowered marital satisfaction, perceived negative personality change in both partners, less satisfaction with home, more instrumental conversation, more child-centered concern, more warmth towards the child, and lower sexual satisfaction. (f) Consensus between husband and wife on child-rearing attitudes was signifi- cantly related to marital satisfaction (as compared with consensus on wife's career orientation). All the above findings are supportive of the viewpoint that becoming a parent puts strain on the marital relationship and may be viewed as a crisis. Finally, Rollins and Feldman (1970) reported their review of the research involving marital satisfaction over the family life cycle. Although their study examined variables over the entire family life, several interesting findings emerged. In general, while husbands appeared to 21 be less affected by stage of life in their subjective evaluation of marital satisfaction, wives reported a sub- stantial decrease in general marital satisfaction and a high level of negative feelings related to marital inter- action during the child-rearing and childbearing stages of the family. Thus, for whatever reason, wives became signi- ficantly dissatisfied beginning in the childbearing portion of their lives. Whether this dissatisfaction could be directly attributed to the birth of children or not, the dissatisfaction did coincide with the presence of children and was likely to be subjectively attributed to the children by their mothers. The group of studies by Feldman corrected some of the deficiences discussed in earlier research. First, control groups entered the picture allowing more conclusive data to be obtained. Next, additional variables, such as style of marriage and maternal attitudes were pinpointed as affecting the couples' adjustment. Some of these variables were even associated with increases in satisfaction. And finally, results were confirmed in both cross—sectional and short-term longitudinal studies. Several studies of the relationship between marital satisfaction and number of children have been done. Hurley and Palonen (1967) were the first'u:study this relationship. They defined a factor called child density as the number of living children divided by the number of years married. 22 In a sample of 40 university student couples, they found a- significant inverse relationship between child density and quality of the marital relationship. Thus, the higher the ratio of children to years married, the lower the marital satisfaction. These findings were incidentally replicated by Tinker (1972) who found a -.35 correlation between child density and marital satisfaction. It should be noted that these studies were confined to university couples who had been married an average of less than six years. Figley (1973) attempted to replicate the above results on a sample which included couples at all stages of family life cycle. Using two measures of marital satisfaction, he concluded that there was no significant relationship between child density and satisfaction. However, Figley did note: (a) a dramatic decrease in marital adjustment and communi- cation occurring during the child-rearing period and (b) a low point reached just before launching children. Finally, Miller (1975), intrigued by these conflicting results, broke child density into its component parts of total number of children, number of children in the home and years married in an attempt to see if any of these factors, plus child density and child spacing, could be seen to relate to satisfaction. Miller used both a Likert-type marital satisfaction scale and a conventionalization or social desirability scale. He again included couples at all stages of the life cycle. While Miller found no relationship 23 between child density and marital satisfaction, he did find a significant negative correlation between child density and social desirability. In his words, "perceived perfection in mates and marriages decreases as child density increases" (p. 347). One might say that marital "romanticims" had been affected by increased numbers of children. Ryder (1973), using a longitudinal approach toward a sample of newly-married couples, examined the differences between childless and childbearing couples after one to three years of marriage. He found that wives with children, as compared those without, were significantly more likely to report that their husbands did not pay enough attention to them. After correcting his data for skew, Ryder found that child-rearing husbands were also significantly more likely to make this complaint and that childbearing wives also felt significantly more marital dissatisfaction than those without children. While the above studies all contribute some partial support for the belief that the marriage relationship can be adversely affected by the presence of children, there are in addition a number of somewhat contradictory findings which are reported below. Hobbs (1965) was the first to raise questions about LeMasters' and Dyer's research. His research had several stated objectives: (a) to discover if LeMasters' findings would generalize to a probability sample of first-time 24 parents, (b) to search for variables which were predictive- of difficult adjustment to birth of a child and (c) to perform some preliminary work on the measurement of crisis. Hobbs used questionnaires to investigate a 50% random sample of white, urban, first-time parents located through public health records in Greensboro, North Carolina. Usable questionnaires were obtained from 65% of this sample. Babies' ages ranged from 3 to 18 weeks. Crisis was measured by a checklist of 23 items to which aparent could respond "none, somewhat, or very much bothered by" this item. Crisis was defined as the total extent of ”bother" reported. Mothers' crisis scores were significantly higher than fathers'; however, there was no significant correlation between husbands' and wives' scores. All in all, Hobbs reported 86.8% eXperiencing "moderate" crisis. (All 13 persons with college degrees in this study fell in the slight crisis classification.) Using some obscure manipulation of his data, i.e., somehow classifying his parents into a slight-moderate and an extensive-severe group, Hobbs identified four variables which appeared to be related significantly to the degree of crisis eXperienced. First, as family income increased, there were fewer fathers in the extensive-severe category. Second, mothers' crisis scores were related curvilinearly to income with low and high income mothers eXperiencing significantly more crisis related to the birth. Next, when babies were ill, there 25 were more fathers in the extensive-severe group. Finally,- when mothers had extra help in the home (other than self and husband), there were five times as many in the extensive- severe category as in the slight-moderate. Hobbs concluded that a clearer definition of crisis must be developed. He also suggested a semi-longitudial follow-up of couples after the birth along with clearer assessment of the character- istics of the couple prior to the birth. In a later study using both interviews and a checklist, Hobbs (1968) found essentially the same extent of crisis as with the checklist approach in his previous study. The interview approach, however, resulted in a more even distribution of subjects across the none, slight and moderate categories with 3.7% of the fathers and 18.5% of the mothers reporting a severe crisis. As a result of this study, Hobbs concluded that the techniques of data collection used may be a critical factor in research findings. He furthermore concluded that "On the basis of the present investigation, it would seem more accurate to view the addition of a first child to the marriage as a period of transition which is somewhat stressful than to conceptualize beginning parenthood as a crisis for the majority of new parents" (p.417). Thus, Hobbs did not disagree that there is stress in becoming a parent but rather was doubtful as to the magnitude of the stress. In 1974, Russell attempted to clarify some of the above contradictions through further research. Sampling 26 20% of all Minneapolis city residents experiencing legiti-- mate births during a one year period, she succeeded in obtaining a response rate of 57.9% of the mothers and 53.4% of the fathers. A questionnaire consisting of Hobbs' checklist of degree of "bother" plus an additional checklist ,aimed at pinpointing gratifications of parenthood was used. Russell focused much of her work on these gratifications. She found education and occupation prestige to be inversely related to gratification scores of parents. Placing mother and father roles high on a list of identities was positively related to gratification. (This result could be eXplained by the possibility that fathers who took the time to prepare for parenthood had higher eXpectations for the gratifications of fatherhood and therefore worked harder at gaining jpositive emotional eXperiences with their children.) She also found that only 7.5% of the women and 5.5% of the men felt their marital relationship had deteriorated since the baby's birth, although, of the items checked for gratifica- tion, the source of the pleasure was more likely to be a personal one rather than benefits to the husband-wife relationship. She noted that interview techniques generally seemed to result in a higher degree of reported crisis, her eXplanation being that responses to a questionnaire may underrepresent those eXperiencing more stress who tend not to return their questionnaires. Russell speculated that 27 effective communication partially underlies the negative relationship found between crisis and the variables of marital adjustment, planned pregnancy and conception after marriage. Meyerowitz and Feldman (1966) used a short-term longitudinal approach to investigate the reactions of 400 primiparous couples. Couples were interviewed during the fifth month of pregnancy, at five weeks after delivery and at five months after delivery. Results were reported descriptively. During pregnancy, a general decline in marital satisfaction, more pronounced for the wife, was noted. One month after delivery, couples described their relationship during pregnancy as a low point not to be matched again. When the child was 5 months old, couples 'reported their marital satisfaction to be higher than pre—pregnancy levels, but they anticipated the beginning of a steady decline in satisfaction. In contrast to this verbally expressed satisfaction, the mean percentage of time the couples reported "things are going well" dropped V steadily over the three interviews from 85% to 65%. Further- more, with the arrival of the child, disagreement over child-rearing decreased while there was an increase in complaints related to the marital relationship, i.e., sexual incompatibility, inability to express feelings to the spouse, unshared leisure time and inability to discuss the husband's work. Thus, when asked directly how the baby 28 had affected them, respondents generally agreed that the baby improved the marital relationship, but the responses to more Specific questions did not seem to support this belief. These findings, of course, raised the question of how honest couples feel they can be when asked a straight- forward question about how childbearing affects a marriage. Implicit societal norms would certainly dictate that having children should enhance the marital relationship although they don't say how. Therefore, the couples above perhaps dutifully stated that children had enhanced their marriages while their responses to more specific questions indicated a decline in satisfaction. A Sociological View of the Discrgpancies The discrepancies in research could be attributed to a number of variables. The researchers themselves (Hobbs, 1965; and Russell, 1974) have pointed to factors such as the difference in samples studied, age of child at time of study and research techniques. Further culprits were hypothesized to be the definition of crisis, the lack of control over history and the gratifications of parenthood which may outweigh the stresses reported. This writer favors a more sociological eXplanation, i.e., that the eXperience of parenthood is greatly affected by social class. As early as 1952, Winch stated that the American middle-class suffers the greatest penalties for parenthood. 29 The economic drain and time constraints of children may limit participation by parents in social and work activities which sometimes facilitate upward mobility. Middle-class parents are likely to believe that some advantages are necessary for their children, e.g., perfectly straight teeth or eXpensive educational toys and to worry unduly when these are not available. Parents today are surrounded by messages that they are totally responsible for their children's mental health and growth. Yet, the beliefs about effective child-rearing have changed rapidly, leaving parents with a confusing array of possibilities, most of which conflict with the style in which their own parents raised them. One general observation which can be made is that those studies reporting higher crisis scores and greater declines in satisfaction (Dyer, 1963; Feldman, 1971; and LeMasters, 1971) have included upper-middle and middle-class couples, while researchers who were careful to examine representative cross sections in urban areas (Hobbs, 1965; and Russell, 1974) found less crisis or conflicting information. Jacoby (1969) examined the differences between parenting in a middle-class as opposed to a working—class setting. First, middle-class standards were described as higher, causing parents to see themselves with more responsi- bilities for their child's mental and physical well-being. This same predicament was also noted by LeMasters (1970). 30 Second, Jacoby hypothesized that working-class mothers placed greater value on having children as their major source of self-validation. Middle-class women were seen as more likely to receive primary gratifications from their jobs. This could be true for both parents in light of Russell's (1974) findings that education and occupational prestige were inversely related to gratification in parent- hood. Related to this hypothesis, the professional careers of middle-class parents were more likely to be disrupted by the arrival of a child (even if that only meant less sleep and poorer ability to function), thereby disrupting the usual source of self-esteem for these parents. Next, the middle-class husband-wife relationship was hypothesized to be more strongly established as affectively positive at time of birth. This was attributed to different eXpectations of what a marriage should be and also to the fact that middle-class couples tend to bear children later in life, thus allowing their relationship more time to establish itself as a dyad prior to birth of a child. An additional possibility was that working-class mothers have had more eXposure to the care of young children (as they came from larger families) and are more comfortable and more realistic about parenthood. And, finally, Jacoby asserted that the subjects may not have been totally honest in their responses. Possibly the working-class parents felt they would be admitting to 31 weakness if they shared concerns about adjustment; possibly they were more committed to socially desirable responses. Or, perhaps they did not understand the goals of the researchers as clearly as their middle-class counterparts who might have been more prone to cooperate with the researchers' eXpectations. In any case, the sociological differences noted above appear to this writer to be the most comprehensive and logical explanation for conflicting research results. A Parting Shot The question still remains even after the many investigations within the last few decades, is parenthood a crisis in the family life cycle? Even with the conflicts in research, a very strong case for the incidence of added stress during pregnancy and early parenthood can be made. A number of authors have noted that the birth of a child can sometimes trigger mental illness for the father (Freeman, 1961; Ginath, 1974; Jarvis, 1962; and Wainwright, 1966). Stress was hypothesized to relate to a number of possible factors. Ginath noted the possibility of the occurrence of rivalry with the child, fear of incest, replays of infantile conflicts, or a narcissistic eXpectancy for the child to fulfill the father's frustrated dreams. He further pointed out the significance of parenthood as a maturational factor. Wainwright (1966) included some of 32 the above factors and added preoccupation with financial responsibility and homosexual fears and fantasies. In women, evidence has also been found for increased stress with childbearing. Pugh, gt. gl., (1963) studied the rate of admission to a state mental hospital for women in relation to their reproductive status. They found the rate of first admissions for women who (a) were pregnant, (b) had delivered a child within 279 days, or (c) were both pregnant and recently delivered to be higher than expected. Results bordered on significance. When only first admissions for psychotic reactions were examined, the difference in admission rates for women eXperiencing pregnancy or recent motherhood became significantly greater than eXpected. The researchers noted that the highest risk appeared to be during the first three postpartum months. I Zemlick and Watson (1953) reported on mothers' attitudes during pregnancy, delivery and six weeks after. A general conclusion they drew was that mothers who reported an extremely accepting attitude during pregnancy may have been repressing true feelings. Their implication was that normal feelings for a mother or eXpectant mother may be viewed as socially unacceptable. To the extent that those feelings had been repressed somatic disturbances during pregnancy may have been exacerbated. After birth, these feelings may have expressed themselves in over-protection and anxiety. 0n the other hand, Zemlick and Watson saw 33 mothers who expressed attitudes of rejection toward pregnancy and motherhood as being more likely to meet clinical criteria of good prenatal and parturient adjustment. Of course, one does not know what criteria were used Clin- ically to determine adjustment, and one would also assume that the rejecting attitudes of these mothers were of a slight or moderate degree and not extreme. If marital disagreements or conflicts are viewed as stressful, then findings that college student parents ranked care and discipline of children next to sex among those problems on which they had failed to reach satisfactory adjustments (Landis & Landis, 1948) are deserving of attention. For couples married younger than those mentioned above, similar findings were noted. The most frequent conflict involved one parent seeing the other as giving in too easily while the latter judged the former as too strict. Also noted was the countermanding of one spouse's order by the other. The above observations would lend support to the premise that parenthood is indeed a crisis-like period for beginners. Added to those observations are a number of informally gathered reports. In contacts with mental health workers, this writer has elicited case examples of increased psychological difficul- ties coinciding with pregnancy and birth. Informal polling of colleagues led one clinician to conclude'Uuit, of the 34 cases seen for marital counseling in the small community health clinic where she worked, a large proportion of couples pinpointed the emergence of their marital diffi- culties near the time of a child's birth. In informal contacts with this writer, nurse educators in the Lansing area expressed doubts that their training really provided a parent with any better tools for adjusting after the baby arrived. Although the classes may have attempted to inform the parents of possible stressors, the information was usually presented in lecture format with a grin-and—bear-it attitude that would have prevented internalization and effective planning. Few, if any, programs have aimed at helping parents internalize more emotional material or at teaching skills or techniques for dealing with the problems that may arise. For example, one commonly used film While You're Waiting (The Mott Foundation, 1967) depicted a couple moving through their pregnancy. The film did not idealize pregnancy and realistically presented the couple misperceiving each other's intentions and feelings. A usualclassroom reaction was for couples to chuckle at the miscommunication and for instructors to admonish the couples to "talk to each other". However, there was no classroom discussion of how to communicate effectively. In the face of this void, more progressive and clinically oriented educators have begun recognizing new parents' emotional needs and searching for ways to meet them. 35 Expectant parents themselves have left prenatal parent education classes wondering if they were really prepared for parenthood. They have often felt doubtful that any class could prepare them. New parents, when approached in a supportive, empathic way, were quite aware of the added stress and responsibility they were facing and of the fact that their lives were disrupted by the new family member. Frequently a period of loss of contact and communication between the parents was described. Even for parents who appeared to be making a satisfactory adjustment, the general attitude was often one of "We had no idea it would be like this!" Popular literature has reflected this emerging concern. There has been an increase in articles dealing with the effect of children on family relationships. Two recent articles which have occurred are "Now That You've Had Your Baby: How to handle relationships with your husband, parents, and in-laws" (Panter & Linde, 1975) and "How Much Should a First Baby Change a Marriage?” (Spock, 1976). Informal reports, such as the ones above, again go no further to "prove" the existence of a parental crisis. However, without debating that issue further, it is easy to find acceptance of the necessity of role changes associated with beginning parenthood. The new roles are highly unlikely to have been eXperienced in the recent past by the first—time parent except in cases where caring for younger 36 brothers and sisters has occurred. Simply by virtue of the fact that the role is new and unpracticed, parents would be eXpected to experience initial difficulty with it. The added possibility that the actual nature of the role is uneXpected and may even directly contradict romanticized eXpectations of parenting compounds the situation. Role changes are frequently accompanied by anxiety about per- formance of the role. The inherent stress, whether to a lesser or greater extent, is obvious in a casual examination of the situation. The situation was nicely summed up by Bernstein and Cyr (1957): "Although many of these problems can no doubt be considered 'normal' and eventually self-adjusting, and are frequently mitigated by deep feelings of satisfaction in parenthood, the arrival of a baby can nevertheless set tensions in motion in the mother and father, separately or in relation to each other, which can have serious consequences for the new family." (p. 479) Education and Preventive Mental Health Up to this point, information supporting the existence of increased stress in families associated with the birth of a child has been presented. The focus of this study, however, was an investigation of prenatal parent education as a potential tool for reducing this family stress. Let us turn to a review of the effectiveness of education in reducing the difficulties associated with becoming a parent. 37 The nursing profession has usually assumed the role of educating eXpectant parents. In general, the rationale behind the approaches used in many prenatal classes has been to provide information aimed at reducing the usual fear of the unknown found in a new situation. Such informa- tion has aimed at a re-definition of labor and delivery and a concurrent change to a more positive, less anxiety-ridden attitude on the part of the couple. It has also involved Specific training in techniques or tools to be used during the birth process. The Miracle 9: Birth (Brigham Young University, 1974), another parent education film, summed the approach up by enumerating the importance of: (a) realistic eXpectations, (b) positive attitudes and (c) supportive techniques. Until recently, prenatal education has focused on medical information and training for labor and delivery. Three typical approaches have been used as tools or supportive techniques: (a) hypnotic pain control, (b) the Read method which involves relaxation and (c) the psycho- prophylactic method (commonly referred to as LaMaze) which involves focusing attention by maintaining an active role in the birth process. A representative research finding of Ithe efficacy of the approaches has been presented by Huttel (1972), who compared 31 primiparous mothers who had received training in a modified LaMaze approach with 41 control mothers who had received no training. Prepared 38 women required significantly less medication and were judged by hospital staff to demonstrate more self-control during labor and delivery. Chertok (1967), after an extensive review of the literature on the subject, concluded, "At the present time, the advantages of prepared confinement are no longer a matter for doubt. These advantages are of two kinds: (a) those concerned with the mental health of the expectant mother and (b) those concerned with supressing pain in childbirth" (p. 705). Recently the mental health aspect has been receiving increasing support. Wueger (1976) noted the importance of pregnancy as a starting point for dis- cussing eXpectations, desires and feelings about parenthood. Hott (1976) also supported the belief that eXpectant parent classes should begin to include more about feelings and parenting. Education has been seen as a helpful tool in other family settings. For example, Thomas, Chess, and Birch (1968) reported a high degree of success in informing parents (and when possible the child himself) about specific temperamental characteristics of the child and methods of dealing effectively with those characteristics. Such information helped parents modify their behavior so that it did not contribute to an escalation of problems. Along similar lines, Shapiro (1956) reported success in modifying parental attitudes toward children by exposing 39 parents to a series of discussion-type meetings. The more- sessions parents attended, the greater was their attitude change as reflected in questionnaire scores. Of course, those parents who were motivated enough to attend may have been examining their attitudes already and may have changed whether eXposed to the class or not. In the past, our culture has tended to place a high value on education. Prominent presidents have been portrayed diligently studying their lessons by firelight. Education may be losing its favored place, though. The current trend toward teacher accountability may be one indication of a disillusionment in the heretofore unquestionned influence of education. Furthermore, the successful approaches described in the brief review of education above were surely matched, in actual practice.by unpublished failures. Therefore, the above review probably overstated education's efficacy in precipitating change. However, education still may be a potential vehicle for change with which many people might be positively impacted at a low cost and in a relatively short period of time. Therefore, it was considered deserving of continuing investigation in the present research. This study then proposed to modify expectant parent education in an attempt to help parents better deal with the effects of a new baby on family members and relationships. Through the course, greater emphasis was placed on the 40 emotional impact of pregnancy for each of the parents as well as its impact on their relationship. Information about general effects of a baby on the marital relationship (previously unknown to couples or perhaps even misrepre- sented) was discussed. Further input included suggestions for dealing with the predicted changes. Specific communi- cation and decisionamaking skills were presented within the context of pregnancy. Also included was sexuality, a subject which, according to Clark (1974), has remained a major concern for expectant and new parents. The question became whether knowledge plus specific tools presented in the brief format of eXpectant parent training could be effectively utilized by parents during the critical adjustment period after birth. The hope of this research was summed up by Parkes (1971) in his discussion of psycho-social transitions: "...there is the possibility that adequate advance planning, preparation and training can transform what is potentially a major change...into quite a minor transition." (p. 113) Hypotheses Hypothesis One: Psychological, relationship-oriented, prenatal education (E) was predicted to facilitate better postbirth marital adjustment than more traditional, medi- cally oriented prenatal education (T). Adjustment was measured through the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale and self-report questionnaire items. 41 Hypothesis Two: Psychological, relationship—oriented, prenatal education (E) was eXpected to affect couples' postbirth decision-making process more positively than a more traditional, medically oriented education (T). Positive decision-making was measured through self-report of the occurrence of significantly more mutual decisions, as opposed to decisions made by only one partner, and of significantly higher satisfaction with family decisions. Hypothesis Three: Psychological, family—oriented, prenatal education (E) was predicted to more positively enhance the communication patterns of new parents than traditional, medically oriented prenatal education (T). Communication patterns were measured through self-report on questionnaires tapping the frequency of discussions between spouses, the satisfaction of the Spouses with that communication, and evidence of specific times set aside for discussion. Hypothesis Four: Psychological, relationship-oriented, prenatal education (E), as opposed to more traditional prenatal education (T), was eXpected to positively affect beginning parents' perceptions of the extent of crisis and of their adjustment to it. These data were collected through questionnaire items which tapped the responents' perceptions of their confidence and general satisfaction after birth as compared to before the birth. 42 Hypothesis Five: Psychological relationship-oriented, prenatal education (E), as compared to traditional, more medically oriented education (T), was expected to reduce the degree of discrepancy between how couples thought their new family life should be and how it actually was. Such discrepancies in attitudes toward children have been found to be related to the child's later psychological adjustment (Broussard & Hartner, 1970; and Broussard, 1975). The research questionnaire tapped such discrepancies in parents’ perceptions of the disruptiveness of the child and also in the Spouses' perceived participation in child care. Hypothesis Six: Psychological, relationship-oriented, prenatal education (E) was eXpected to enhance couples' postbirth sexual adjustment more than traditional, medically oriented prenatal education. E education provided information about potential sexual difficulties following birth along with suggestions for coping with them. Questionnaire items tapped subjects' own satisfaction with sexual relations and their perceptions of the spouses' satisfaction. CHAPTER II METHODOLOGY Overview Two forms of education were presented to groups of eXpectant parents who had sought training. One form (T) encompassed the physical aspects of pregnancy and birth which have been traditionally covered in such classes; the other form (E) augmented the traditional information with additional information and skills aimed at facili— tating the psychological adjustment of new parents. Subjects were couples who independently completed questionnaires and were also interviewed by telephone on two or three occasions. One-half of the couples eXperienc— ing each form of education were pretested (with an interview and questionnaire) prior to attending eXpectant parent classes. All subjects were asseSsed 4-8 weeks postpartum and again 1 year after the birth (see Figure 1 for time flow). Thus, the design was a Solomon Four-Group Design, as described by Campbell and Stanley (1963), in which the "control" group received a treatment composed of traditional eXpectant parent education as opposed to the revised educa- tion received by the eXperimental group. The advantage of 43 44 FIGURE I Time Flow of EXperimental Procedures September 1976 October 1976 November 1976 December 1976 January 1977 March 1977 April 1977 May 1977 September 1977 October 1977 November 1977 April 1978 April 1978 Initial contacts with October class parti- cipants and random assignment of volunteer Ss to treatment conditions Training of instructors Pretesting of half of October class parti— cipants Treatment began for October class partici- pants (Instructors 1 and 4 teaching T, instructors 2 and 3 teaching E) Initial contact with January class partici- pants and random assignment of volunteer Ss to treatment conditions Pretesting of half of January class parti- cipants October class participants ended treatment January class participants began treatment (Instructors 1 and 4 teaching E, instructors 2 and 3 teaching T) Began first posttesting of October class par- ticipants (4-8 weeks after birth of child) January class participants ended treatment Began first posttesting of January class par- ticipants (4-8 weeks after birth of child) Continued first posttesting to completion Began second posttesting for October class participants Continued second posttesting Began posttesting for January class parti- cipants Completed collection of all posttest data Began processing of data and writing results 45 the design for this study lay in its ability to check for effects of pretesting and of interactions between pretesting and treatment. General areas tapped by the assessment included marital satisfaction, satisfaction with the child, degree of roman- tic eXpectations of parenthood, marital communication patterns, decision making and crisis adjustment. Subjects Subjects were 66 volunteer couples selected from those couples who had enrolled for a 10-week prenatal education class offered by Expectant Parents' Organization located in Lansing, Michigan. All parents were primiparous. Of the 66, 33 were pretested, 38 completed the 1-month follow-up and 39 Provided data when their children were 1 year-old. All three observations were obtained from 17 couples. One couple was eliminated due to their child's severe health problems which involved prolonged hospitalization. Background information was obtained on all 66 original couples. Their ages ranged from 18 to 46 years with an average of 25.5 years. Average educational attainment was 2 years past high school; however, individuals' education ranged from ninth grade through eightyears beyond high school. Income for the couples varied from $4,000 to $38,000 with an average annual income of $18,6000. Couples had been married an average of 3.2 years. One couple was unmarried at the 46 time of pretesting while the upper limit was 9 years of marriage. Ten percent of spouses had eXperienced divorce prior to their current marriage. On the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale, the average prebirth score for the 33 pretested couples was 118. Seventy-four percent of these subjects had dyadic adjustment scores within one standard deviation of Spanier's (1976) average for married persons (i = 14.8, a‘= 17.8). While such a subject pool was not representative of the total population of parents eXpecting their first child, it was probably representative of caucasion middle-class couples who seek special training during their pregnancies. Subject Assignment After submitting their enrollment request forms, those couples who would normally have been assigned to classes beginning in October 1976 (due dates between January 20 and February 20) or classes beginning in January 1977 (due dates between April 20 and May 20) were asked to. participate in this research. A letter was mailed requesting their participation in a study designed to assess and improve the training in eXpectant parent programs (see Appendix A, p. 124, for a copy of the letter). One to two weeks later, couples were contactedby telephone to answer any questions they had and to determine their willingness to participate. Those couples who agreed to participate were randomly assigned to either T or E educational conditions. The 47 couples were then allowed a preference (when possible) of class night within the condition assigned. Couples who were unable to attend the nights of their assigned treatment condition were eliminated from the survey and enrolled in the class night of their choice. Once assigned to a class night, random assignment to pretesting or no pretesting occurred. Random assignment was done in a manner which stratified couples with respect to the time of application for education. A contract (see Appendix B, p. 125) stating the conditions of the evaluation and signed by the researcher was mailed for the couples' signatures. Background demo- graphic information was requested with the return of the contract (see Appendix C, p. 127). Treatment Conditions The first group of couples began classes during October with four instructors who had been trained in both educational approaches. Two instructors employed the traditional programs and two employed the new program. In January, the second group of parents, selected as described above, began classes with the same instructors, each instructor employing the opposite educational approach from that used in the October groups. In the past, the focus of most expectant parent train- ing performed by the EXpectant Parents' Organization had been physiological aspects of pregnancy and delivery. 48 Participants had traditionally been instructed in the use of relaxation techniques for use during birth, maternal and child health, and labor and delivery room procedures. In addition, past training sessions had been taught by several different instructors, each presenting information in her areas of eXpertise. Pregnancy, labor and delivery, and child-care had been the focus for a Six-week class. Then an optional two-week class in breastfeeding and an optional four-week class in breathing and relaxation for childbirth were offered. The present study involved two educational approaches. T left the traditionally presented information intact, but reduced the program to ten weeks taught by the same instruc- tor. In the same ten-week period, E included an abbreviated form of the majority of information traditionally presented plus additional information areas designed to better prepare couples emotionally for pregnancy and beginning parenthood. Additional areas covered included effects of pregnancy and parenting on the marital relationship, decision-making skills, communication tools, identification of and adjustment to family stress, and realistic expectations for the new family unit (see Appendix D, p. 128, for brief content summaries of T and E). Parent manuals were used to supple— ment class materials. 49 Instructor Selection and Training All instructors were trained in both educational approaches before the study began. In the past, instructors had received updating and new program information at monthly meetings which they were required to attend. As a result, all instructors had received a review of the traditional eduCational materials in their monthly meetings. In addi- tion, instructors who had not taught the subjects of breast feeding or breathing and relaxation for birth were required to observe an eXperienced instructor teaching those classes. Training for the new program (E), which merely added to the basic program (T) described above, occurred in a single weekend workshop consisting of 12 hours of training (see Appendix E, p. 134, for agenda). Volunteer research instructors were selected after the training workshop. Volunteers were requested to make a commitment to teaching each educational approach at least once. Instructors were limited to four registered nurses who had attended the weekend workshop and were willing to follow the research design along with its limitation on course content and to teach the subjects outlined in each of the two instructors' guides. Controls for Research Design The teaching manuals for both approaches were used by instructors to guide their classroom approach and 50 structure. Furthermore, the researcher observed selected weeks of classes to determine if course content was pre- sented without contaminating T classes with E information. As a further assessment of class materials, couples were asked to complete a checklist of class topics covered throughout the course on the final night of class (see Appendix F, p. 136, for a copy of the topic checklist). In an attempt to control, or at least to observe, instructor differences in presenting the different programs, brief evaluation forms for each class period were filled out independently by the husband and wife. These evaluations requested subjects to use a four-point scale to rate the instructor's knowledge, enthusiasm, comfort and abilities when presenting topics. They also provided attendance records for the couples (see Appendix G, p. 137, for a copy of the instructor evaluation form). On a personal level, every attempt was made to remind instructors that there was no clear evidence of the superiority of one program over the other. Instructors were asked their personal opinions about the two programs so that the effect of those opinions could be monitored (see Appendix H, p. 138, for monthly instructor report). Instructors' opinions were solicited on five occasions: (a) before beginning, (b) half-way through the October classes, (0) after completing October classes and before starting the January class, (d) half-way through January classes and (e) after completing January classes. 51 Furthermore, prior research (particularly that of Feldman, 1971) suggested that subjects should be assessed for: (a) the type of marriage relationship they possess, i.e., companionate versus differentiated and (b) the degree of the wife's comfort or discomfort during pregnancy. Therefore, a four—item scale suggested by Feldman as an indication of marriage style was incorporated in all questionnaires along with an item about pregnancy discomfort which appeared in the pretest and first posttest. Due to the ever—present influence of modeling on human behavior, a person's perceptions of his/her own childhood experiences and his/her own parents' skills in child-rearing may have an effect on that person's perception of self as a parent and ability to adapt to parenting. Therefore, this area was briefly tapped in the questionnaire by asking subjects to evaluate the parenting ability of their own and their spouses' parents. A new baby places an additional financial strain on a family which may be compounded by loss of one parent's income. Given societal role assignments, this strain might particularly influence the father or "provider". Financial strain may be additive with the usual family adjustment sur- rounding birth and was therefore monitored through the questionnaires. Similarly, baby temperament may be a factor which affects parental adjustment. An easy—going, non-irritable 52 baby would certainly cause less disruption than a fretful baby who over-reacts to minor stimulation and change. Therefore, in the final questionnaire, parents were asked to rate their child's temperament along nine dimensions. Motivation for parenthood is another variable which may affect a couple's adjustment to the newborn by influencing the gratifications that parents obtained from their new roles. For example, a parent motivated by the desire to protect and care for a child would be eXpected to eXperience different rewards from early baby care than a parent concerned with living up to socially acceptable goals by bearing a child. Thus, motivation for parenthood was assessed using categories developed by Rhodes (1974). Finally, demographic data were gathered prior to class participation to insure that treatment groups did not differ unduly along those lines. These data included a check on the length of marriage and time spent living together. Assessment Procedures All couples were asked to complete the total evaluation procedure on two or three different occasions. The husband and wife each completed the procedure independently. Evalu- ation occurred for half the participating couples before they began education classes. All couples were then assessed 4 to 8 weeks after the birth of their child and again approximately 1 year after birth. 53 The Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976) is a 32-item scale designed to tap the four areas of dyadic satisfaction, dyadic consensus, dyadic cohesion and affec- tional eXpression. The scale was designed by pooling all items ever used in marital adjustment scales, eliminating duplicate items, judging items for content validity and administering the approximately 200 remaining items to a sample of middle- and working-class married couples and a sample of recently divorced couples. Validity of the Spanier scale was examined in three ways. Content validity was established by three judges who eliminated items not considered to be: (a) relevant measures of dyadic adjustment, (b) consistent with nominal definitions suggested by the author for adjustment and its components, and (c) carefully worded with appropriate fixed choices for answers. Criterion related validity was established by eliminating all items for which the means did not discriminate between the married and divorced sample (95.001). Construct validity was established by comparing the Spanier scale with the widely used Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale. The correlation between these scales was .86 for married and .88 for divorced respondents. Internal reliability consistency was assessed using Cronbach's Coefficient Alpha. Total scale reliability was found to be .96. The Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale was incorporated totally as the first 32 items of each questionnaire. 54 Inquiries about motivation for parenthood were included in the first posttest questionnaire and in phone interviews. Rhodes (1974) established five general positive motivations for parenthood (altruistic, fatalistic, narcissistic, instrumental and conformity responses) and six general negative motivations (responsibility, freedom, physical discomfort, economic and ecological constraints, personal inadequacy and dislike of children). Rhodes' classifica- tions and descriptions were used to score the responses obtained. Additional elements in the self-administered question- naire were designed to examine communication and decision- making patterns, companionate versus differentiated marriage patterns, eXpectations of the baby and the Spouse as a parent, attitude toward own parents, financial changes, sexual adjustment, crisis adjustment, and baby temperament. Husbands' and wives' questionnaires were essentially the same (see Appendix I, p. 139, for copies of the husbands' questionnaires). The telephone interview was conducted by undergraduate psychology students who received course credit for their involvement. Interviewers were trained in the use of open- ended and pursuit questions to elicit information in the desired areas. They were further instructed in the use of empathy and paraphrasing in order to establish rapport with parents and to check the accuracy of the answers they 55 recorded. Answers were written in as much detail as possible by interviewers both during and immediately after the phone interview. Approximately 10 percent of the interviews were done jointly with one interviewer listening on an extension line. These joint interviews were done to maintain con- sistency in interviewer behavior. Appendix J (p. 166) contains an outline of the interviewer training program.' The interview was only semi-structured to facilitate the emergence of uneXpected information and variables. While occasional scaled responses were requested from the subjects during the interview, for the most part only a general topic outline was covered with spontaneous responses being recorded. Interviewers took part in establishing the final format and questions for each interview (see Appendix K, p. 168, for topic outlines of the telephone interviews). Statistical Analysis The hypotheses proposed the investigation of differ- ences between treatment groups along the lines of marital satisfaction, mutual involvement in and satisfaction with family decision-making, communication frequency and satis- faction, perceived adjustment to crisis or change, the discrepancies between "should” and "actual" in a parent's preceptions of his spouse's child-care behavior, attitudes toward the baby, and sexual adjustment. Scores obtained 56 by each couple in the above questionnaire categories were compared at posttest one and at posttest two using a 2x2 multivariate analysis of variance with presence or absence of pretesting and eXperimental condition as factors. In addition, a repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance was performed on the data of those couples who supplied complete information. CHAPTER THREE RESULTS Scale Construction As noted earlier, scales were to be constructed for seven areas of interest using the information collected from individuals' questionnaires and interviews. These included scales of communication, crisis adjustment, marital adjustment, decision-making, discrepancies in views of how tasks should be and actually were divided, attitudes toward the baby and sexual adjustment. A correlation matrix was generated among all items thought to be rationally related to these scales. Final scales were determined by examining items at all three observation periods (pregnancy, 1 month after birth, and 1 year after birth) and selecting those items which correlated highly with their own, as opposed to other, scales and which contributed to their scale's alpha level (see Appendix N, p.177, for a complete description of scale items and Appendix 0, p. 181, for each item's correlation with its own and other scales). Some less reliable items were retained in the final scales due to their rational value in predicting the hypotheses. (E.g., an item asking if regular times were set aside for communi- cation correlated with its own scale .28 although its 57 58 reliability was only .03. This item was retained because much of the class content for the experimental group was geared toward such planning on the couples' part, it did not correlate highly with other scales, and it did not reduce the alpha of its own scale.) As the scales were developed, the three scales of communication, marital adjustment and decision-making were observed to be substantially intercorrelated (minimum 3 = .56, p 5;.05; see Appendix L, p.172), suggesting that these three scales tapped the same characteristics. In addition, items from these scales tended to be inconsistent from one observation period to the next, sometimes correlating more highly with the communication scale, sometimes with decision- making and sometimes with marital adjustment. For example, in an early scale analysis, item 8 of the final combined scale correlated most highly with decision-making for the pretest (g = .50) but at 1 month correlated most highly with communication (3 = .62) and at 1 year with marital adjustment (g = .58). Therefore, these three original scales were combined for analyses (see Appendix M, p.173 for interscale correlation for the final five scales at each observation). Examination of interscale correlations for the three subscales (see Appendix L, p. 172) which were combined to form the Communication/Decision-Making/Marital Adjustment Scale raises questions about the decline in interscale correlations from prebirth to 1-month and 1-year observations. 59 The 5' transformation was applied to test the significance: of differences between those correlations (see Edwards, 1950). From prebirth to both 1 month and 1 year, the decrease in correlation between communication and marital adjustment was found to be significant (p S .05). The correlation between communication and decision-making also dropped significantly from prebirth to 1 year (p S .05). The correlation of marital adjustment and decision-making did not differ significantly between observations. Questionnaire information since it was more available and consistent, was the only source of items for the final scales. The sole exceptionto this was the Attitude Toward Baby Scale which gained most of its information from interviews. Five final scales of Communication/Decision-Making/ Marital Adjustment (CDMA), Crisis Adjustment (CRADJ), Discrepancy Between Actual and EXpected Division of Tasks (DISCR), Attitude Toward Baby (ATTBA) and Sexual Adjustment (SEXADJ) resulted. Appendix N, p. 177, contains a complete description of scale items. Appendix 0, p. 181, enumerates each item's reliability and correlation with its own and other scales. A brief verbal description of each scale follows. CDMA was a 15-item scale. Its average alpha over the three observation periods was .84 (see Appendix P, p. 190 for a summary of alpha coefficients for each scale at each 60 time of observation). Its general content included frequency of and satisfaction with communication, degree of mutuality in and satisfaction with the decision-making process, and the total score for the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale. CRADJ was a seven-item scale with average alpha = .77. It tapped the parent's perception of change and disruption in the family due to the baby. Lower CRADJ scores denoted less disruption and hence were positive. DISCR was a six- item scale (average alpha = .73) consisting entirely of items in which the subject was asked to rate both how tasks of baby care should have been divided in the family and how they actually £333 divided. The absolute difference between these two answers denoted the discrepancy between the responent's ideal image and view of reality. Again lower scores denoted more positive feelings. In almost all cases of differences, participants felt the husband should be sharing more equally in child care. ATTBA was a 15-item scale (alpha = .66) tapping positive and negative reactions toward the baby's presence and the degree to which parenthood was perceived as more or less difficult than eXpected. High scores denoted more positive attitudes. SEXADJ was a four-item scale with an average alpha of .83 which assessed the respondent's perceptions of how feelings of affection and sexuality were eXpressed in the marriage. For most scales, the range of possible answers for individual items differed. In addition, a subject may have omitted one item but answered all other. To equalize item 61 weight, all items in a given scale were set to a common base. To allow utilization of scales with missing items, the average score for all aVailable items in that scale was designated as the scale score. These scaled raw score averages were converted to g-scores for statistical analyses of the hypotheses. Pretreatment Equivalence in Groups It was anticipated that random assignment to treatment groups would guarantee equivalence between groups before treatment. To check this assumption, pre-birth variables were grouped into three general areas of socio-economic variables, perceptions of family and pregnancy, and outcome scale scores; and multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) were performed comparing the two treatment groups. Socio-economic variables included levels of education for husband, wife and their parents, income, length of marriage and whether the couple had lived together. The MANOVA on these variables was not significant (p 5;.39) indicating that the two treatment groups did not differ appreciably in socio-economic status. Perception of family and pregnancy included both spouses' views of their marriage style (companionate versus differ- entiated), the couple's perceptions of their own parents' adequacy in raising children, and the couple's perception of the degree of discomfort eXperienced by the wife during the 62 first two trimesters of pregnancy. A MANOVA indicated that the two groups did not differ significantly along this dimension (p 5 .11). An interesting trend in the univariate F-tests for each of the above variables was that the couples' views of their own parents' adequacy differed significantly (p S .02), with T couples perceiving their parents more positively than E couples. In addition, there was a trend for T couples to perceive their marriages in a more traditional style than did E couples (Xi = 15.56, IE = 14.63, 25. .08). Pretreatment outcome scale scores included CDMA, CRADJ, and SEXADJ. A MANOVA performed on those scale scores indicated that prior to treatment, groups did not differ appreciably on those outcome measures (p,é .11). However, another interesting trend surfaced in the univariate F-tests performed on the variables. T wives had significantly higher CDMA scores (p E .04) than E wives before treatment; and a similar trend (p £;.08) existed for T husbands. A closer look at couple scores on the Spanier Dyadic Adjust- ment Scale alone revealed a similar but sharper difference (p é..01). Furthermore, the variance on both the CDMA and the Spanier scale was significantly greater (p 5;.05) for the couples receiving relationship-oriented education (E). Thus, prior to treatment, the T subjects had marital adjust- ment scores which were both higher and more homogeneous than those of E subjects. 63 In summary, when multivariate analyses were performed' comparing T and E groups prior to treatment, none of the three areas of socio-economic status, perceptions of family. and pregnancy outcome scale scores showed significant differences. Univariate F-tests, however, revealed pretreatment differences with couples receiving physiolo- gically oriented classes showing more positive views of their own parents, better marital adjustment and less variance in adjustment scores. Equivalence in Classroom Treatment In the present study, treatment approaches were eXpected to differ in classroom content while dimensions such as attendance and perceptions of instructors would, hopefully, be comparable. To assess differences in class content, couples were asked in the last class period to complete a checklist of the topics which were covered in the class series (see Appendix F, p. 136, for a copy of the checklist). The topics which were to be covered only in E sessions were analyzed by summing the number of those topics checked by each parent completing the checklist. These total scores were then subjected to a t-test. T subjects were found to score significantly lower (p $;.001) on this total than E subjects. Thus, the content of the treatments differed as eXpected. To check for differences in other classroom variables, a MANOVA was performed comparing T and E subjects on their 64 attendance and on their ratings of instructors' knowledge of, enthusiasm for, comfort with, and ability to present each day's topics. This MANOVA was not significant (p S .07) suggesting that these classroom variables were comparable for both treatment groups. However, T subjects averaged higher class attendance (XT = 7.75 classes, XE = 6.66 classes); and except for the final class, a lower percentage of total E subjects attended each class period than their T counterparts. The difference was particularly notable in the initial three class periods when rationale for the relationship-oriented approach had been heavily emphasized (see Figure 2). Thus, treatment for the two groups differed as eXpected along the lines of the information covered in the classes although it did not significantly differ for such classroom. variables as attendance and rating of instructors. Couples receiving relationship-oriented (E) class content tended toward higher absenteeism than did T couples. Tests of Hypotheses It was hypothesized that relationship—oriented prenatal education (E), as opposed to prenatal education presenting only more traditional physiological information (T), would result in the following postbirth differences in the family: --higher marital adjustment (hypothesis one), --more positive decision-making processes (hypothesis two), 65 FIGURE 2 Percentage of T and E Subjects Attending each Class Period 100 j 80 - Percent of 60 4 SS Attending _ 40 - 20_.———.——--OT - ‘———4———£ E 66 --more frequent and satisfying communication patterns (hypothesis three), ' --better adjustment to the developmental crisis of beginning parenthood (hypothesis four), --more positive attitudes toward the baby and toward the spouse's role in child-care (hypothesis five), and --better sexual adjustment (hypothesis six). Hypotheses one, two and three were combined in the CDMA scale. Hypothesis four was measured by the CRADJ scale. The scales of ATTBA and DISCR both tested hypothesis five; and SEXADJ was employed to measure hypothesis six.= "Subjects" consisted of couples. Statistical analyses were run using both the husband's and wife's scores on all scales as the components of each subject's score. Thus, for example, at the 1-month observation, each subject's score consisted of both the husband's and wife's individual scores for CDMA, CRADJ, DISCR and SEXADJ. Although, as could be eXpected, husbands' and wives' scores were highly correlated (p 5 .05 for every scale except CRADJ at 1 month), the multivariate technique is able to take such relationships between variables into account (see Appendix P, p.190, for correlations between husbands' and wives' scale scores at each observation). All hypotheses were tested simultaneously using 2 x 2 (treatment group by presence or absence of pretesting) multivariate analyses of variance for both the 1-month and 1-year observations. No significant differences were found between treatment groups at either time period. 67 Further repeated measure MANOVAS were run using attendance and the three prebirth scale scores (CDMA, CRADJ and SEXADJ) as covariates in an attempt to control for the trends in differences between treatment groups at pretesting. Once again, no significant treatment effects were found. In a second series of statistical tests, a repeated measures design was used to examine the scores of the 17 couples for whom there were data at all three observations. Once again, no significant differences were found between treatment groups. However, g1; couples changed signifi— cantly in scores from pretesting to the 1-month posttest and from the 1-month posttest to the 1—year follow-up. The major variable accounting for the change was the CRADJ scale, which showed more disruption in the couples' family lives immediately after birth than during pregnancy or at 1 year postbirth. (see Table 1, p. 68, for average g-scores of these couples at each observation.) The null hypotheses could not be rejected at the .05 level of significance. Post Hoc Analyses An attempt was made, using multiple regression analyses, to determine which pretest variables were predictive of more positive postbirth marital adjustment. To accomplish this, data from all couples, regardless of treatment condition, 68 TABLE 1 Average g-Scores for Couples with Complete Dataa Scale Time of Prebirth l-month 1-year Observation 0" e d7 8 o” 9% CDMA T .270 .176 .007 .037 -.147 .127 E —.280 -.406 .084 -.175 -.090 -.225 CRADJ T - 128 .139 .198 .161 .207 .147 E .083 .163 .362 .137 .197 .066 SEXADJ T .166 .066 .376 .092 “.065 .107 E -.522 -.394 -.491 -.360 -.568 -.730 DISCRb T -.204 -.209 .078 .114 E -.206 .149 .254 .386 ATTBAb’C T -.227 -.o77 E .188 -.080 a y = 17 (9 Ts and 8 Es) Scale not administered prebirth. 0‘ C Scale not administered at 1-month postbirth. 69 were utilized. Since the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale has been established as internally reliable and externally valid, it was used to calculate four change scores, one for each individual husband and wife at 1 month and at 1 year after birth. (Note the shift from considering the couples as units to considering husbands' and wives' scores separately.) The regression analyses were performed to identify predictors for these change scores. A total of 30 couples had complete questionnaire data for both the pretest and the 1 month follow-up; and 28 couples had complete data for the pretest and 1 year follow up, with 17 couples common to both groups. Demographic data and all pretest questionnaire items for both husband and wife were considered as potential predictors of change in marital adjustment. In addition, change scores for both husband and wife on each of the three outcome measures common to all observations (CDMA, CRADJ and SEXADJ) were considered. Before attempting the regression analysis, items to be considered as predictors had to be reduced to a manageable number. Accordingly, correlations were computed between each potential predictor item and the Spanier change scores obtained for husband and wife. Only those items which were significantly correlated (p S, .05) with the Spanier change score were included in a given regression analysis. Finally, those items selected for analysis were examined with regard 70 to their relationships to each other. Where high correla—- tions existed between potential predictor items, they were combined to create a single composite variable. For example, five questionnaire items, two requiring both husband and wife to indicate the percentage of time s/he had felt satisfied with the marriage relationship, two requiring both husband and wife to indicate the percent- age of family decisions with which s/he had felt satisfied, and one requiring the wife to indicate the percentage of time things had been going well, were all significantly correlated with both husbands' and wives' Spanier change scores at 1 month and at 1 year. Therefore, these items were selected for further analyses in predicting each of the four change scores examined. In addition, each of these five items correlated highly with each other. Therefore, they were averaged as a composite variable for purposes of the regression analyses. The final variables used for each regression analysis along with summary tables of the analyses can be found in Appendix R (p. 192). Cut-off points are indicated in the summary tables by solid lines for the point at which the significance of the prediction begins to increase as new variables are added and by dotted lines for the point at which the combined predictors are no longer significant at p g;.05. The following are observations of these data and trends: 1. Positive postbirth marital adjustment for the wife at 71 1 month, husband at 1 month, and husband at 1 year corre- lated negatively with what might be termed a general expres- sion of family well-being during pregnancy. This expression of well-being was composed of the variable described above as a combination of percentage of time things have been going well, percentage of decisions agreed with, and percentage of time satisfied with the marriage relationship. 2. Postbirth marital adjustment at 1 month for both husband and wife was positively related to a number of factors that suggested traditional attitudes toward family roles. These included the wife's belief that she should predominantly arrange for baby care when she must be away, the wife's eXpectation that the addition of a child would cause great changes in her lifestyle, the husband's report of less time spent daily in conversations, the wife's report of less consulting with her husband when decisions were made and the husband's belief that his wife should feed the baby more often than he. 3. The husband's postbirth marital adjustment at both 1 month and 1 year related positively to satisfactory prebirth decision-making. This decision-making included both husband's and wife's reports of higher satisfaction with their own and their spouse's participation in decision-making, husband's reports of a higher degree of joint decision- making and wife's reports of a greater degree of satisfaction with decisions about parenting. 72 4. No pretest variable satisfactorily predicted the wife's change score for 1 year at p 5_.05. The best predictor was her eXpectation of a great deal of change in her rela- tionship with her spouse (R S .068). 5. The following three relationships appeared unrelated to the trends identified above or to each other: (a) An inverse relationship existed between how well the husband thought his wife's parents did raising her and the husband's positive marital adjustment at 1 month. (b) A positive correlation existed between the husband's adjustment at 1 month and how much he expected the new baby to change his sleeping and eating habits. (c) The wife's degree of satisfaction with her job situation during pregnancy was negatively related to her husband's positive marital adjustment at 1 year. In summary, the regression analyses would indicate that postbirth marital adjustment for first-time mothers and fathers, related positively to a marriage with more traditional roles, but related inversely to more positive views of the pregnancy eXperience. Also new fathers eXperienced improved marital adjustment after birth if a satisfactory decision-making procedure existed before birth. Significant predictors of the mother's marital adjustment change when her child was 1 year old were not found. CHAPTER FOUR DISCUSSION All couples in this study, regardless of treatment, changed significantly in scores from pretesting to 1 month postbirth and from 1 month postbirthfimal year postbirth. Furthermore, the changes were such that at 1 month postbirth, couples' scores were significantly different from their scores at prebirth and at 1 year postbirth. The major measure contributing to that change was the Crisis Adjustment Scale (CRADJ) which was highest at the 1-month observation. The CRADJ scale asked the couple to describe how much change was anticipated (pre- birth) or actually had occurred (postbirth) in a number of household routines such as eating and sleeping, relationship with spouse, sexual relationship, leisure time, etc. This result then revealed, not uneXpectedly, that after 1 month a new baby had impacted family routines signifiCantly more than the parents eXpected and that 11 months later the disruptions had been significantly reduced. The foregoing information would support use of a crisis model to describe the family process of integrating a child. According to repeated measures analyses, the severity of the event was uneXpected by the couples, and they adapted to the changes, 73 74 over time returning to a more normal, predicatable routine. Supporting the proposition that the family changes occurring at birth may have a long-term effect on the marital relationship were the serendipitous findings of significant declines in correlations between elements of the marital relationship. Within the CDMA scale, the subscale correlations of communication with decision-making and marital adjustment decreased significantly from pre- birth to 1 year (see Appendix L, p. 172). A similar, but nonsignificant, decline occurred in correlations between husband's and wife's scores for CDMA and CRADJ (see Appendix Q. p. 191). These findings suggested that the birth of a child may have contributed to overall disorganization and loss of congruence in several aspects of marital adjustment and in spouses' level of agreement about their marital satisfaction. There were indications that these correlations might be slowly increasing again both for spouses' degree of agreement and for the relationship between communication and marital satisfaction although the data were insufficient to confirm that trend. In light of the above findings, the perspective researcher would be advised to extend follow-up measurements well past the initial postbirth changes if long-range conjectures about family changes are desired. The parent-to- be might use this information for anticipatory planning 75 and for encouragement while attempting to survive the immediate postbirth confusion and the potentially longer- lasting marital disruption. Illustrations of Families Experiencing Great Life Changes A look at the characteristics of those research parents who eXperienced the greatest life changes was thought to be potentially enlightening. Accordingly, four couples were identified who had 1 month posttest crisis scores in the top 10 percent as determined by individual scores and the couples' composite scores. Couple A had been eXperiencing difficulties before the prebirth interview. They had been married only 1 month before discovering the pregnancy. At the prebirth inter- view, the couple was seeing a marriage counselor and had apparently separated. The husband stated that marriage and a pregnancy were "too much at one time." Marital problems became compounded with physical ones as the baby's birth was late and attempts to induce labor failed. Finally, labor began spontaneously but complications led to a Caesarian section after which the infant was kept in intensive care. Mother and infant did not see each other for two days. Further financial burden was added by the mother's medical eXpenses which were not covered by insurance and required the father to work at two jobs. At 1 month postpartum, both husband and wife eXpressed dissatisfaction 76 about the time they had to talk. The baby's crying was identified as a stimulus for marital conflict. This couple, then, eXperienced a series of difficulties concurrent with, and related to, their child's birth. These diffi- culties added to the degree of crisis and change they eXperienced. Couple B had been married 2 years before this planned conception. Both husband and wife worked with children and had many detailed eXpectations about parenthood. The mother, who worked with handicapped children, expressed intense prebirth fears that her child might be defective. After a difficult 23-hour labor, the baby was taken by Caesarian section. The husband described labor and delivery as "crude" and "a difficult job for hospital staff." Once the baby was home, both parents were startled by the difference between the child and their eXpectations. The mother was surprised at the extent of her infant's social responses but disappointed at the amount of time required and the restrictions placed on her by child-care. The father felt that his wife was eXperiencing fulfillment in her maternal role, but he was disappointed and repeatedly eXpressed a longing for the infant to grow up so he could enjoy it more. He felt his wife was overly tired and vented her anger on him for ”not doing his share." He was also surprised by his frustration and anger at hearing the baby cry. Neither parent felt household routine had returned to 77 normal by 2 months after birth. This couple, then, in addition to a difficult labor and delivery, eXperienced violations of many specific eXpectations of their new baby and their shared parental role. Couple C had been married 5 years and conceived after 3 years of attempting to have children. It was necessary for them to consult a fertility specialist during that time. A miscarriage had occurred before the successful pregnancy. Both husband and wife had Specific eXpectations of the rewards and costs of the coming baby. The husband had prepared himself by doing volunteer work with infants. Delivery involved a Caesarian section that "panicked" the mother. (Did she consider it another failure after her miscarriage and the need for a fertility specialist?) She had expected a boy rather than a girl. When first seeing her infant after birth, she didn't like it. The infant was described as crying a great deal. Both parents stated that they had eXpected changes with the baby but the demands and the extent of the change were greater than anticipated. They repeatedly stressed the necessity of placing the baby's needs first. So these parents also found themselves facing a crying baby who violated their eXpectations. Couple D was surprised to learn of their pregnancy since the mother had been told that she was not ovulating. Their discussions during pregnancy were psychologically and developmentally oriented and included thoughts about the 78 baby, each partner individually and their relationship. They appeared to value marital communication and to rely heavily on it during the first trying months of parenthood. The father had eXpected and wanted a boy rather than a girl. Their child required much more attention and time than they had anticipated as she was born with heart problems and without a thyroid gland. (In fact, this couple was elimin- ated from statistical analysis because of the severity of health problems which necessitated cross-country trips to Special clinics.) The mother stated she had eXpected "more enjoyment and less worry." In each of these cases the new baby violated parental eXpectations and hopes. The uneXpected situations included health problems, excessive crying, wrong sex, and more demands for time and care than was eXpected. Three out of four eXperienced Caesarian births, while the incidence of C-sections in the total subject pool was only 9%. The Caesarian birth is a deviation from the normal vaginal delivery for which the parents have specially prepared themselves through prenatal classes. C-Sections are often based on potential health hazards for the baby and/or mother. Anesthetics and surgical procedures usually result in separation of the mother and infant for a period of time after birth. Klaus and Kennell (1976) have empha- sized the importance of early contact in facilitating attachment. Lack of early contact, while not precluding 79 attachment, may limit the contributions of biological mechanisms, such as hormonal changes, and may contribute to initial reactions such as Mother C's statement that she didn't like her baby when she first saw it. Many hypotheses could be made about the cause of the violations of these parents' eXpectations. Was it lack of opportunity for initial attachment? the constitutional nature of the children? lack of adequate educational preparation? inability of the parents to incorporate negative experiences? excessive emphasis on being the "good" parent? fear of inadequacy as the child did not develop as eXpected? In any case, it is encouraging that 1 year after birth, these parents did not, on the average, describe their children's temperaments much differently than did the total sample. Thus, those early perceptions did not necessarily carry on through the children's lives. In fact, probably over time the physical problems began to correct and parents learned to cope, allowing them to adjust their self-concepts. Mother D eXpressed tre— mendous relief Simply from diagnosis of her baby's ailment. Mother A illustrated the impact of a difficult infant on her parental self-concept when she indicated that she began to feel much better about herself once the baby was doing well. 8O Implications for Prenatal Education The above findings have implications for later parenting classes. First, it appeared that parents gener- ally had inaccurate expectations of the disruptiveness of a new baby. They may have also had false expectations about early rewards in parenthood (certainly several of the couples scoring high on CRADJ at 1 month did). Prenatal classes could strive to correct these misconceptions and to provide anticipatory guidance for the realities of new parenthood. Parents could be informed of the time-limited nature of the disruptiveness surrounding beginning parenthood. Parents could likewise be warned of the severity of the disruption in an effort to promote better anticipatory planning. Research results indicated the importance of better preparation for the possibility of a Caesarian birth. The local prenatal groups and health department currently provide some information and special films for couples who have planned Caesarian births. However, in the present study, parents with uneXpected C-Sections who had attended the usual prenatal classes complained that they were unprepared. Part of the lack of preparation undoubtedly stemmed from the attitude that "it won't happen to me”. With the growing incidence of Caesarian births, parents may need information about the likelihood of this event, the details of what will happen, and the manner in which 81 they can help each other create a positive experience during and after the surgery. Classes may need to emphasize the brevity of labor and delivery in comparison with the many subsequent years of parenting. EXpectant parents often anticipate that the birth eXperience will be a peak emotional eXperience. Prenatal classes usually increase this eXpectation, rather than tempering it with more realistic views. Certainly birth can be a beautiful experience for parents, but it can just as easily prove to be exhausting and frightening. 't ' n 'f This eXperiment was not successful in obtaining statistically Significant differences between treatment .groups. The most conservative conclusion to be drawn from the lack of Significance was that the implementation of this particular psychologically oriented educational experience was not powerful enough to affect a family's adjustment to a first child in a Significantly different manner from a more typical, medically oriented prenatal education. In light of this a major issue to be examined is whether statistical significance should be given such a powerful role in determining the effectiveness of human services. Even moderate trends toward differences in preven- tive research may be sufficient reason to continue and refine a program. A small trend identified in a short-term. 82 longitudinal study may amplify over succeeding years into a major difference. Furthermore, the benefits of even small psychological advantages are difficult to price. This experiment was beset with many difficulties in execution and in statistical analysis. Without some of those difficulties, Slight trends in data might have assumed statistically significant proportions. Such dificulties should be taken into account in evaluating this human service program. Enumeration of those difficulties follows. Statistical Limitations First, the number of subjects for whom data could be collected was small. At 1 month postbirth, 38 subjects were available (19 couples in both T and E), while the 1-year follow-up examined data for 39 couples (20 in T and 19 in E). For the repeated measures analyses, only 17 couples (9 TS and 8 ES) had complete data. Such small samples required substantial differences in outcome measures to achieve statistical significance. For the above cell sizes, differences between treatment groups would have had to be large (.4 and .5 standard deviation) for a reasonable chance of finding existing Significant differences (Cohen, 1969). Next, pretreatment MANOVAS did not reveal statistically significant differences between treatment groups although 83 the two groups were not clearly comparable when pretest and classroom variables were examined. AS noted in the results section, the group that received psychologically oriented education (E) tended to have lower marital ad- justment as measured by the CDMA scale. When only the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale was examined, this difference was significant. Further difficulty in establishing statistically Significant differences between the two treatment groups evolved from their wide range in scores. The psychologically oriented group (E) had a significantly greater variance in CDMA prebirth scores than did their physiologically educated counterparts (T). Hence, the standard deviations for E couples (.27 for husbands' scores and .58 for wives') were virtually so great (eSpecially in comparison with the standard deviations of the T subjects which were .14 and .16, respectively) as to preclude the finding of Significant differences even though the mean CDMA score was .25 points lower for T than for E. It is interesting to note that this Significant 'difference in CDMA score variance did not recur in the posttest observations. Perhaps the psychologically oriented education truncated extreme positive and negative scores. (The effect was not caused by attrition of couples who scored at the extremes.) Perhaps couples with very positive marital satisfaction Scores were negatively 84 influenced by the educational approach and/or the eXperience of beginning parenthood while couples with very low marital satisfaction eXperienced the opposite effect. Or perhaps those couples at the extremes merely regressed toward the mean. There was no clear evidence that either of these conjectures was more probable than the other. Figure 3 shows marital adjustment by treatment group and sex for those 17 couples with data from all three observations. For the combined Communication/Decision- Making/Marital Adjustment (CDMA) scale, E husbands and wives increased from prebirth to 1 month while T husbands and wives decreased. The same changes occurred to a lesser degree from prebirth to 1 year. A similar trend emerged when Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale scores were averaged at each observation time for all subjects with data at the time of observation (see Figure 4). Once again, from prebirth to 1 year, T husbands and wives decreased Slightly in marital satisfaction while the E subjects increased. In addition, from prebirth to 1 month, T decreases and E increases were even more marked for both sets of data. Thus, data trends, while not significant, did support the first hypothesis that psychologically oriented prenatal education can promote more positive postbirth marital adjustment than physiologically oriented education. 85 FIGURE 3 Average Z-Scores on Communication/Decision-Making/Marital Adjustment (CDMA) Scale for Couples with Complete Data* .30 .20 .10 .OO g-scores -.10 -.20 -.30 —.40 Prebirth 1 month 1 year mam Noose. 4L Time of Observation * for T, N = 9; for E, N = 8 86 FIGURE 4 Average Spanier Dyadic Admustment Scores for all Data Collected at each 0bservation* 125 ‘T 120 __ F T Q Spanier L T 6‘ Dyadic Adjustment '— E 9 Score 115 ._ r E 0‘ 110 1 #1 41 I Pre-birth 1-month 1-year Time of Observation * N's, ranging from 16 to 27, waried with time of measurement, treatment group and sex. 87 Comparability of Treatment Groupg In spite of random assignment to treatment, several statistically nonsignificant trends were noted in the pretreatment measures. These included tendencies for couples receiving physiologically oriented education (T) to view their own parents more positively. to view their marriages as having more traditional roles, and to display higher levels of marital adjustment than did E couples. All of these trends likely worked against confirming the stated hypotheses. First, as parents tend to raise their own children in the way in which they themselves were parented, one would eXpect those parents with more posi- tive views of their own parents to have felt more positively toward themselves in a parenting role. Hence, this attitude toward their own parents may have promoted a more positive parenting self-concept for T parents. This positive self- concept could have generalized to outcome measures of perceptions of children and spouses. Second, marriages having traditional roles, as compared to those with a companionate style, were found by Feldman (1971) to be more likely to increase in marital adjustment following birth. The addition of children was postulated to create an opportunity for more role fulfillment for the wife. Note that T wives, with their more traditional view of marital role, were the only group that increased in Spanier Dyadic Adjustment scores from 1 month to 1 year postbirth (review 88 Figures 3 and 4). Thus, again, the tendency toward a traditional role in T couples would have worked against the hypothesis that couples receiving psychologically oriented education would eXperience better postbirth marital adjustment. And finally, higher pretreatment marital satisfaction scores for T Subjects decreased the possibil- ity of E couples surpassing the T high scores at post- birth. Thus, all tendencies toward differences in treat- ment groups prior to classes may have hindered establishment of significant posttreatment differences. Design Difficulties A major drawback in the present research design was the absence of a true control group. Both treatment groups requested and received prenatal education classes. The couples, by seeking prenatal education, behaviorally demonstrated a belief that information could assist them in coping and a value of preparing for future events. Participation in any prenatal education program (or merely the intent to do so) may have been enough to stimulate forethought and planning for coping effectively with a life change. According to the literature review, middle-class couples would be eXpected to decline in postbirth marital satisfaction as compared with prebirth measures; however, the present research did not find a significant decline in marital 89 satisfaction for couples receiving prenatal education of either a medically oriented or psychologically oriented nature. Review of Figures 3 and 4 shows, except for T wives, a decline in marital satisfaction from 1 month to 1 year. The fact that these trends were inconsistent and statistically nonsignificant may indicate that parents who chose to attend prenatal classes, compared to the general population of those not seeking prenatal education, were somewhat better prepared to adapt after birth regardless of the nature of their classes. Extent and Timing of Treatment Many factors may have weakened the impact of the innovative psychologically oriented training program. Ten 2-hour classes (or more accurately, the six to eight class periods attended by the typical couple) may have been insufficient time to cause significant behavioral or attitudinal changes. Furthermore, the maximum class time originally planned to be devoted to psychological aspects of pregnancy and parenthood was 9 hours (less than 50 percent). In practice, unfamiliar psychological materials were often condensed, reduced or even eliminated by instructors when faced with time pressures. The timing of an educational program so that its audience is maximally receptive to materials may be crucial. At the time of eXpectant parent classes, couples' concerns, 90 as elicited through agenda-gathering during the initial class period, were focused mainly on the whys and hows of labor and delivery and the physical well-being of infant and mother. In fact, of the 102 mothers and fathers interviewed immediately after birth, 67 indicated that preparation for labor and delivery was the most helpful part of their classes and/or that they would have liked to spend more class time on that area. Only two parents would have preferred less time on the topic. Furthermore, 20 of the 49 E parents made negative remarks about decision-making or relationship-oriented class content, suggesting less of that content and/or stating a dislike for it. Only nine parents requested more family-oriented material or viewed it as a significantly helpful part of the class. Negative comments ranged from a belief that couples should have worked-out personal problems before becoming pregnant to comments that the instructor did not have the background to teach those skills or was uncomfortable and presented this material unclearly. One father felt that the emphasis on the change in the marriage made couples more apprehensive-- he would have preferred more positive classes. After birth, in the midst of many life-style changes, couples may have been more receptive to information on emotional adjustment to beginning parenthood. For example, one mother laughed when asked what she would leave out of the classes and then commented that while attending classes, 91 she felt family relationships should not be included. She then added that after birth she had recognized the value of the topic. In a 3-month follow-up of 29 primi- parous and 19 multiparous couples who had attended prenatal classes, Tiedge (1978) found 70 percent reporting that relations with their spouse were strained, different or not going well. The overall tendency for most subjects to decline in measures of marital satisfaction from 1 month to 1 year after birth (review Figures 3 and 4) would also indicate that research subjects for the present study experienced increased postnatal stress. Further support for higher postnatal receptivity to psychological issues can be found in the final interview when parents were asked if they would have been interested in attending a discussion group for new parents after birth. Of 78 parents interviewed, there were 39 “yes", 16 "maybe", and only 17 "no" responses (6 parents were not asked). Parents were asked when such a group would optimally be offered, and a wide range in child's age (from newtxuv1 to 3 years) was suggested. Responses clustered at birth to 3 months, 6 to 7 months, 1 year, and 2 1/2 years. While "discussing parenthood" is certainly a less emotion-laden topic than discussing family relation- ships, the two areas could be expected to interact with each other. In any case, the vast majority of research parents identified their needs and difficulties with regard to parenting as being subsequent to the first 6 months of the child's life. 92 Thus, with regard to timing of material, these eXpectant parents appeared most receptive to learning about labor and delivery-~an unknown and imminent event. After birth, as their child matured, parents became increas- ingly preoccupied with the parenting role (and probably with more psychological, family-oriented issues). There- fore, a prenatal class may have been an inappropriate time to eXpect couples to be receptive to psychologically oriented materials. Presentation of the New Program Another major variable concerned the discomfort of instructors in presenting the new program. A11 instructors eXpressed or displayed discomfort when presenting information about declining marital satisfaction after the birth of a child or the negative aspects of parenting. One wrote, "I felt very uncomfortable presenting such negative facts to the couples re: parenting and the marital relationship. I think class #10 ended on a positive note...but I felt strongly the anxiety from the couples when the negative aSpectS were discussed." The cause of her discomfort was verbalized by another instructor who said, "I don't like to tell them bad news. This is supposed to be a happy time in their lives." This discomfort persisted despite the instructors' eXpressed commitment to the rationale behind such information. Of 18 comparisons of the two approaches which were collected 93 from the five instructors at various times during their participation, 14 placed the psychologically oriented E approach as somewhat better or far superior to T; three were neutral; and one classed the traditional approach (T) as somewhat better. (This last instructor later rated the E program higher.) The conflict felt by instructors in their attempts to present new, sometimes anxiety-provoking, material to which they had eXpressed a commitment is also clear in the following series of quotes from one instructor's evaluation reports. (The first quotes are as she taught the psycho- logically oriented curriculum.) "The whole presentation has me up-tight and I realize the tremendous amount of studying and rehearsing that will be necessary..." "The decision- making process has me stumped and I am apprehensive with regards to presenting something I don't 'own' yet. Wish we could modify it." (The next quotes are after finishing the new curriculum and while teaching traditional and familiar content.) "The modified program was difficult to incorporate into my own words. It definitely put a lot of pressure on me as an instructor. I understood the basic concepts but the presentation of the materials did not flow easily and I felt the couples perceived it also." "The idea of communicating with each other as a couple is an important issue... I feel bad I can't let them know about the stresses currently in their life and the ways we feel one can cope with them." 94 Given both the instructors' discomfort with and the parents' negative reactions to the more psychologically oriented material, an interaction may be hypothesized with instructors presenting material with low self—confidence, parents reacting negatively to the material, instructors then feeling even less comfortable, etc. The spiral effect created by such an interaction would certainly have obstructed a positive, effective exposure to E class content. Discussion of Post Hoc Analyses Two major trends identified in the post hoc analyses provided further substantiation and extension of Feldman's (1971) short-term, longitudinal findings. He found positive marital adjustment at 5 weeks and 5 months post- partum to be related to a traditional marriage style and to a negative perception of pregnancy. The finding of the present research further substantiated Feldman's evidence by correlating positive change on a well-validated, multi-item marital adjustment scale with husbands' and wives' prebirth variables. At 1 month both husband and wife were likely to eXperience more positive marital adjustment if they had viewed their relationship in a traditional role style. At 1 month for both husband and wife, and at 1 year for the husband, marital adjustment related inverser to perceptions of general well-being during pregnancy. Thus, the present data extended Feldman's finding to the father as 95 well as the mother. A discussion of these two relationships and the remaining findings of the regression analyses follows. Human perceptions of well-being are usually relative to the conditions surrounding them. The degree of general life satisfaction perceived by a person at any given time may be directly dependent on the level of satisfaction existing just prior to that time. Hence, an inverse relationship between positive pregnancy attitudes and post- birth attitudes is understandable. The eXpectant parent who prized pregnancy may be generally more disappointed by the stresses of beginning parenthood than the parent who had a less pleasant pregnancy eXperience. This phenomenon may be especially applicable to the first-time mother who may have received deferential treatment and Special nurtur- ance from her family and social group during her pregnancy. The woman who particularly enjoyed that Special care, for whatever reason, is faced with the opposite circumstances when she must nurture her own child and when her family and- husband turn their attention to the newborn. Still missing is information concerning couples' level of marital adjustment and general well-being before pregnancy. Do couples become pregnant in an attempt to correct an already unsatisfactory situation? If so, their very positive reaction to pregnancy may be a result of renewed hope which would likely turn to disillusionment 96 under the stresses of beginning parenthood. On the other hand, high pregnancy satisfaction could be simply a continuation of pre-pregnancy dyadic adjustment which is disrupted by the intrusion of a baby. The present research found a positive relationship between a traditional marriage style and postbirth marital adjustment. A traditional marital relationship is one in which the role eXpectations establish the wife as primarily responsible for home management and child~care while the husband provides financial and physical resources for the family. This contrasts with a companionate marriage style in which the husband and wife tend to share the same tasks and roles and to value this shared orientation over well- delineated, separate roles. In the case of a traditional- role marriage, the wife's role is incomplete until she has children to care for. Hence, both husband and wife might be eXpected to find their marriage more congruent with their role eXpectations, and therefore more satisfying, with the birth of their first child. Additionally, the regression analyses revealed a tendency for father's poStbirth marriage adjustment to be related to prebirth decision-making adjustment. If fathers are eXpected to articulate with the larger society for their families, decision-making may be an important aspect of that role. A smoothly functioning prebirth decision-making procedure should be an asset which carries over to the 97 postbirth period, thus enhancing the husband's satisfac- tion with his family life. Conversely, unsatisfactory decision-making prior to birth may deteriorate further under the added stresses of the new postbirth roles. It is surprising that no pretest item significantly predicted change in the wife's marital adjustment at 1 year after birth. One might postulate that a new mother eXper- iences much more change in her family role than does her spouseu Since she usually is the parent assuming most responsibility for infant care. (This postulate is supported by the research variable which came close to significance, p g .068, as a predictor of more positive marital adjustment, i.e., the wife's expectation of great change in her emotional and sexual relationship with her husband.) Most likely the marital relationship does indeed change, and the wife's anticipation of that change can contribute to her adjustment. But that change may be based on so many individual characteristics that its direction and magnitude are unpredictable. For example, the new mother's family and life goals would be eXpected to contribute to her adjustment. Does she value the parent- ing role as a life goal? Her own developmental stage would affect adjustment. Has the new mother established her identity separate from that of a mother or wife? Her relationship with her own mother (the parental model) may come sharply to the foreground. Do old childhood conflicts 98 return to haunt her as her baby grows? The list seems endless, making the levels of adjustment varied and unpredictable. The additional relationships identified by the regression analyses rest on the slender thread of single questionnaire items. Thus, they offer a weaker base for Speculations and, in some cases, are difficult to rational- ize. It is understandable that the husband's marital satis- faction at 1 month related positively to the degree to which he eXpected the baby to change his sleeping and eating habits. Even in a marriage where the wife provides the. majority of child-care, a new baby would be likely to disturb the Sleeping habits of the rest of the household, and to the extent that the infant's needs disrupt the mother's usual routines, she might be unable to keep meals on schedule. Getting meals may be symbolic of having needs met for the husband. In any case, his anticipation of such changes would help him better adjust without blaming his wife and eXperiencing subsequent loss in marital satisfaction. The remaining two relationships are not so obvious. Why would the husband's marital change have been negatively related to how well he thought his wife's parents did raising her and to how satisfied his wife was prenatally with her job situation? Regarding the latter relationship, 99 if the wife's prenatal employment satisfaction is indi- cative of her over-all job satisfaction, then the birth of a child for the satisfactorily employed woman may interfere with her job situation and diminish her related sense of self-esteem after birth. For the woman who dislikes her job, the baby provides relief from the job plus an alter- native source of self-esteem (motherhood). In this study, then, the husband's marital adjustment at 1 year may have been a reflection of his wife's general satisfaction with her role at that time. If this were true, a similar relationship would have been unlikely at 1 month simply because most mothers did not eXpect to be employed that soon after delivery. A violation of the husband's expectations may eXplain the relationship identified between his perceptions of his wife's parents and his marital satisfaction at 1 month. Perhaps the husband who sees his wife's parents very positively eXpects similar excellence from her and is disappointed by her ineXperience and inability to live up to his expectation, particularly during the disruptive period immediately following birth. Similarly, the‘ husband eXpecting poor performance from his wife may be pleasantly surprised by her unexpected adequacy. Dis- appointment or pleasure at a spouse's abilities would likely be reflected in the husband's marital satisfaction. In summary, post hoc analyses generally supported earlier studies which showed a positive view of pregnancy 100 and a companionate marriage style to be related to lower marital satisfaction in the initial postbirth year. New evidence showed the father's postbirth adjustment to be related to satisfaction with decision-making procedures-- another potential support for the importance of traditional family role eXpectations--and to his eXpectation of disrup- tion in Sleeping and eating patterns. The new mother's 1 year postbirth adjustment was less predictable. Overview of Research At this point, a brief chronicle of the events involved in this research may give the reader some perspective on the complexity of longitudinal, eXploratory research as well as some issues and problems specific to this study. Formulatingpthe Problem I became interested in the area studied for both professional and personal reasons. Professionally, an interest in preventive mental health was the end result of several years of clinical training. The necessity of producing a research-oriented dissertation for completing a doctoral program was a further pressure which could not be ignored. Personally, I had reached a decisive stage in my own life cycle with regard to childbearing and parenting. This led to informal observations of other couples in the process of integrating children with their families and lifestyles. 101 My further interest in beginning parenthood was greatly stimulated by beginning a literature review which increasingly indicated potential major difficulties for the couple adjusting to the birth of a child. Contrary to popular social myths, instead of adding joy to the marital relationship, the advent of children appeared to decrease marital satisfaction! The question which began to form, then, was whether some type of preventive action might lessen the effects of beginning parenthood on marital satisfaction. The feasibility of investigating the question immediately rested on a long-term research commitment and on accessibility of a subject pool. Locating_Subjects A ready-made, easily accessed population was thought to be available through prenatal education groups. Accordingly, the two local groups, The Association for Shared Childbirth (ASC) and the EXpectant Parent Organiza- tion (EXPO) were contacted. An ASC representative was discouraging. She gave no indication of support for . integration of preventive materials with the prenatal classes themselves. She further pointed to the poor attendance of their postbirth presentations. The ExPO president, on the other hand, was enthusiastic about using the prenatal classes as a preventive treatment. She had been informally discussing similar changes with a professor 102 who had recently become a father. They had been consider-- ing such classroom strategies as presenting more realistic, balanced pictures of parenthood and providing decision- making and stress reduction techniques to eXpectant parents. Thus, serendipitous timing provided a potential source of subjects and the impetus to progress to considerations of measurement and design. Use of a control group was a major design difficulty in this research. A "true" control group in the present study would have consisted of parents who asked for prenatal education, were unable to obtain it, and were willing to participate in the follow-up. This might well have produced a source of error since highly motivated couples would have undoubtedly sought education elsewhere (through other prenatal groups and reading). They then would have become prenatally educated "false" controls or would have had to be dropped from the control group. Either option would have altered the composition of the control group making it no longer comparable to the treatment group. In addition, the type of couple who would have volunteered for research follow-up after being refused education would have differed from the volunteer couple that knew it would receive education. Even if the preceding were not the case, differential drop-out rates would have been likely, with couples who were refused education being less motivated to complete the follow-up. And finally, given ethical and 103 financial concerns, the EXpectant Parent Organization, out: of its own commitment to prenatal education, would not have cooperated with research proposing to turn away a portion of its applicants. If the difficulty in recruiting a "true" control group was fraught with problems when the initial subject pool was to be gathered from the same source (i.e., couples requesting prenatal education), the difficulties in recruiting a control group comparable to the treatment group from another source appeared insurmountable. Accord- ingly, the present research was conducted as a comparison of two different types of educational treatment, one which covered the more usual prenatal topics of identifying and coping with physiological changes during pregnancy and birth and one which replaced some physiological content with methods for identifying and coping with emotional changes. The fact that all subjects received some treatment likely lessened the overall differences between groups on outcome measures, thus making statistically Significant results less likely. Designing the Experiment The details of the eXperimental design have been presented in the methodology section. A rigorous eXperi- mental design requiring random subject assignment, assessing possible effects of exposure to testing procedures, and alternating instructors' assignment to treatment groups was selected to insure the strength of any findings. 104 Identifying Variables for Measurement Next identification of specific hypotheses and methods of measuring outcome were considered. Marital satisfaction, along with change or the degree of crisis eXperienced, had already been clearly identified in the literature review. Other outcome measures were chosen to examine the impact of specific content areas in the revised curriculum. These included decision-making processes, extent of communication, attitudes toward baby, eXpectations of the spouse's role and sexual adjustment. Only in the area of marital satisfaction did a well-validated measure already exist-- the other scales required construction. It should be noted that the additional outcome measures were often selected based on the interests and needs of the cooperating persons in EXPO. In research requiring such extensive commitment and cooperation, involvement of personnel in planning was considered crucial. This became even more obvious as the research progressed. Methods of collecting the data and timing of the observations were the next concern. A forced-choice question- naire was selected as it Could be sent and returned by mail. It also facilitated the administration of the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale as part of its contents. As less structured information would be valuable in an eXploratory study, semi-structured interviews were also chosen. They allowed the flexibility required to gather spontaneous responses and uneXpected information while still "guiding” 105 the interview content so that similar areas were addressed- by all couples. Undergraduate psychology students provided a potential source of interviewers. While face-to-face contacts may have provided more nonverbal information, the feasibility of transporting interviewers or motivating parents to transport themselves to such interviews was low. Consequently, telephone interviews were selected as an acceptable medium. Such interviews avoided transportation difficulties and also provided parents with a somewhat anonymous situation for self-disclosure. Times for measure- ment were selected such that the first postbirth observation might gather data on reactions to the crisis while it was still occurring or fresh in the parents' minds. One year was thought to be a time by which full adjustment to the role change would.have occurred and parents might have a perspective to Share about their adjustment. Of course, the issue of control of extraneous variables raised its head. Several sources of uncontrolled effects which might influence outcome measures included: (a) person- ality and history of the subjects, (b) human resistance to change and reaction to unmet expectations as a result of the new curriculum, (c) classroom variables including instructors' attitudes, (d) differences in interviewers, and (e) characteristics of the new baby. As these variables could not be directly controlled, except perhaps by matching, which causes other difficulties, they were monitered; and random assignment of subjects was retained 106 as the tool for initial equalization of treatment groups. The effects of unpredictable human variability were a constant concern, and as already noted, those effects on this research could not be adequately controlled. Maintainingpthe Relationship with the Sppnsoripg Organization Cooperation of ExPO instructors was critical to the success of this study. Good management techniques dictated that involvement of instructors in planning would have increased their commitment to presenting new class content and evaluating its impact. While participation and input were solicited from instructors, most did not actively participate. Reasons for nonparticipation were varied. Instructors were paid by the hour for classroom time. They were not reimbursed for nonclassroom activities such as preparing materials or attending curriculum meetings. Furthermore, instructors had already been becoming resentful of the extra demands EXPO was placing on their time even before introduction of the research. Consequently, in Spite of efforts to solicit instructor input, only three highly committed nurses were actively involved in designing the new curriculum. Meanwhile, intensive ongoing efforts focused on the development of two parents' manuals and curriculum guides for standardization of educational treatments. The three interested instructors (including the organization president), the professor mentioned earlier, and myself 107 were involved in those efforts. I had to rely on the three instructors to design teaching materials and lesson plans for the traditional physiological information. As this material was familiar to all EXPO nurses, the three instructors relied on brief outlines and explanations obtained from old instructors' guides to construct their materials. To make psychological content less threatening, I produced a detailed teaching guide for those class sessions. This limited instructor involvement in developing unfamiliar psychological curriculum contributed to major problems during the instructor training session outlined in Appendix E (p. 134). Extensive, detailed guidelines for presenting psychological information dwarfed the terse, informational outlines provided for physiological content. Additionally, training content for the instructors' workshop focused mainly on unfamiliar topics--of course, the new emotional- relational content. Consequently, instructors attending the workshop found themselves faced with what appeared to be an overwhelming amount of new content and very little familiar material. Resistance to the new material was heightened by this discrepancy.- Furthermore, some instructors were highly committed to particular content areas, e.g. breast-feeding, labor and delivery exercises, etc., which they felt were slighted by the new approach. Hence, although the original plan had been to try to convert all instructors to the new content and new parent manuals, the end result of the 108 workshop was four instructors who volunteered to teach the new content and old content one time each for the research. Locating Funds Funding supports were necessary to defray typing and printing costs of the parent manuals, to provide instructors with pay incentives for participation in training, and to pay postage and supply eXpenses for the questionnaire and interview follow-up. Support of the Michigan State Univer- sity Cooperative Extension Service was gained for typing and printing of parent manuals. The Ingham County Board of Commissioners provided grant funds for postage, instructor re-imbursement, and printing supplies. The Michigan State University Department of Psychology provided envelopes, stencils and paper for the questionnaires and interview forms. All of these contacts required time and paperwork. Given multiple funding sources, it appeared to be advan- tageous to be able to demonstrate cooperation from other funding groups to any potentially resistant source. Conducting the Treatment Classes began with high levels of anxiety on the part of instructors. The degree of tension can best be described by the behavior of one instructor 15 minutes before her new class was to begin. She was nearly frantic about whether all the materials were there, whether her class would respond, and whether She could adequately provide leadership 109 for group discussion. The surprising aSpect was that this- teacher was the only one who had always led her class in a discussion style in the past and who, for scheduling reasons, was pg: involved in the research. Her reaction was typical of research instructors and may have been an outlet for more extreme tensions which they were unable to express directly. Samples of class sessions were observed from behind a one-way mirror. Classroom atmosphere, unexpected class- room events, and deviations from curriculum were recorded in a log book. This provided opportunity to at least moniter some of the many uncontrollable human factors already mentioned above. Collecting Data As the beginning of data collection approached, interviewers were recruited and trained. Interviewers proved themselves to be conscientious and enthusiastic for the most part; however, they required monitering to see that they completed their assigned interviews and that their interviews covered the designated areas. For the latter, it was sufficient to assign occasional joint interviews in which one interviewer listened to another's call and provided feedback. To insure interview completion, it was necessary to maintain regular contact with each interviewer regardless of how reliable that interviewer seemed. One interviewer, after 8 months of consistent work stopped 110 interviewing and did not inform me of her difficulties until it was too late to collect the remaining data. Finally, I was available by phone at any time the interviewers initiated contact. These phone contacts proved helpful in allowing interviewers to review their own skills and approach and also in providing support after a difficult interview. Difficult interviews ranged from the minor case of a resistant interviewee to more extreme cases of spontaneous abortions of the current pregnancy or revelations of rather severe emotional difficulties by the interviewee. The task of data collection was arduous. First, the birth of the child had to be identified. In addition to relying on parents to send postcards as notification of the birth, newspaper birth announcements were read and phone contacts initiated if the due date was several weeks past. Once the birthdate was known, questionnaire mailings and interviews were attempted. Suddenly parents found they had little time to complete the observations. Parents complained about the repetitiveness of questionnaires. They also claimed insufficient time. Delinquent question- naires were followed by one or two postcard reminders and in some cases by a phone call. Interviews were more readily completed as many of the parents enjoyed talking about their eXperiences in great detail. This was less true at the 1—year follow-up; however, the majority still loved to talk! This is not to imply that scheduling and completing the phone interviews was easy. 111 Often parents were very busy, were difficult to reach at home, and had trouble scheduling a convenient interview time. In addition, the interviewers themselves were influenced by time pressures as well as by personal reactions to some parents. Accordingly, interviewers were aked to write a brief description of their reactions to each interview and interviewee. Analyzinngata Analysis of the more structured questionnaire data was relatively straightforward as answers were transferred directly to computer forms. Classification of the open- ended interview data was a complex task. Initially, an attempt was made to create general categories of responses to each open—ended question. Responses were so varied, however, that the number of categories often reached 20 to 30 per question, making this process too unwieldy. Eventu- ally, interview data were classified primarily on 2- or 3—point scales--either yes-no options or classifications such as none-some-great. For example, a parent's answers were classified as indicating "a great deal of", "some" or "no" concern about finances. Even though inter-rater reliability was possible under these circumstances, the valuable variability of the unstructured responses was drastically reduced. In the final analysis, use of inter- view data was avoided. That wealth of information remains untapped. 112 Finally, a full 3 1/2 years after the initial steps were taken in this research, as the data analysis and writing draw to a close, the time-consuming nature of the study becomes inescapable. The would-be researcher may adequately assess his/her commitment at the beginning of a project, but it is unlikely that accurate judgements of time and effort can be made. In a society changing as rapidly as ours, it may even be difficult to predict that- the researcher will wish to continue at the same job or residence for the duration of a longitudinal study. Unpredictable life changes may occur for the researcher, just as they influence the subjects of the study. (I had first-hand eXperience of beginning parenthood while still analyzing data.) Even an intense interest in the topic is likely to become satiated and change to other areas over a long period of time. However, the work can be challenging and the findings fascinating even without the blessings of statistical significance. A sense of humor can help, and it is in this vein that several of Murphy's Laws (Dickson, 1979, p. 123) are offered for the reader's enjoyment: "If anything can go wrong, it will; "Nothing is ever as simple as it seems; "Everything takes longer than you eXpect: "If you see that there are four possible ways in which a procedure can go wrong, and circumvent these, then a fifth one, unprepared for, will promptly develop; and 113 "Nature always sides with the hidden flaw." 17‘ «L Suggestions for uture Research It is difficult to document the prevention of poor mental health. Without even beginning to address such thorny issues as the components of positive mental health and their measurement, how does one prove that something missing would have been present had it not been for a brief experimental manipulation? Yet, this is the task for preventive mental health efforts; and if we are not prepared to abandon preventive programs as a method for positively impacting adjustment to developmental crises, more research is necessary: (a) to better understand the process of adjustment, (b) to refine programs, and (c) to justify the continuation and eXpanSion of prevention efforts. Some suggestions for future research follow. First, large subject pools are preferable if statisti- cally Significant changes are sought. The use of a power test (Cohen, 1969) will inform the researcher of the numbers needed. Larger subject pools will further increase the success of random assignment in equalizing groups before treatment. If large samples are economically unfeasible, pretesting, if it is used at all, Should be applied to all subjects in order to moniter similarity of groups before starting treatment and also to provide increased statistical power through a repeated measures design. Furthermore, an argument can be made for the value of more intensive 114 observation of fewer subjects without aiming for statistical differences. In this case, trends within smaller samples might provide important information at less eXpense. Second, subject drop-out should be carefully considered in advance and efforts made to reduce it. These might include minimizing the amount of time required of subjects during poSttesting and/or maximizing the subjects' commitment to, or rewards for, completion of the research. Rewards might take the form of money, feedback, or subjective gains such as increased self-awareness or ability to talk to an interested person about an important topic. By fully informing subjects of the details of their participation, some drop—outs may be eliminated prior to participation. If very few people decline to participate in an extensive longitudinal study, perhaps the information being offered to them is insufficient for adequate evaluation of their commitment. In that case, information about participation should be eXpanded and/or the commitment involved emphasized. Assessment techniques must be carefully selected both for their ability to tap characteristics which directly reflect an effective intervention and for sensitivity to subtle differences. For example, if an intervention is aimed at increasing the amount and quality of interaction between a parent and child, a tool measuring the amount and quality of interpersonal interaction through direct observa- tion is preferable to one measuring possible secondary 115 effects such as changed parental attitudes or gains in the- child's developmental quotient. In the event that a new technique is to be compared to an old and established one, time should be allowed for the new technique to be piloted and to become comfortable for its administrator and its receiver. (This strategy might also reduce the magnitude of any Hawthorne effect.) In the present research, the new educational program was neither comfortable for the instructors nor eXpected by the parents. Had instructors been allowed to teach the material several times before running subjects, or had psychologically trained co-instructors been employed, the materials would likely have been presented more confidently. Also perspec- tive parents might have heard through informal or formal communication that the content of the class had been eXpanded, thus changing their eXpectations. Additionally, eXperi- mental studies might be aimed at finding means for modifying instructors' information and values and assessing the effect of instructor attitude change on teaching style. Finally, timing of an intervention so that the recip- ient is aware of its value is likely to enhance the intervention's success. This is particularly difficult in preventive programs Since, by their very nature, they often occur before a need is clear to the recipient. Thus, the patient may be more receptive to learning about dental floss after several painful hours in the dentist's chair. In the 116 case of the present research, postbirth education eXper- iences might have better met the above qualifications if parents had been approached as they were experiencing new difficulties and needs. The first step would be to identify timing and patterns of increased stress for parents. Then at a time when levels of stress are rising, neighborhood- based parent groups could be initiated or home visitors might call on the family. An alternative to changing the time of the intervention is to heighten the recipients' perceived need for it. In the present study, this might have been accomplished by increasing the expectant parents' awareness through contact with and observation of beginning parents and their emotional difficulties. Other potential tools would include presenting the findings of this and other research and utilizing related classroom role-plays. Modes of intensifying perceived needs might be assessed for their relative ability to impact subjects. The would-be researcher in this complex area should be forewarned of the massive amount of time and energy necessary and the likelihood of minimal immediate rewards. Even though the researcher may feel realistically prepared to face the difficulties, estimates of the necessary commitment are likely to fall short of reality. Perhaps the undertaking should be viewed as a developmental task Similar to beginning parenthood--even the informed parent is 117 still surprised by the task's enormity. On the other hand; just as parenthood is an inevitable part of biological continuation, so is longitudinal research invaluable to the understanding of psychological development. BIBLIOGRAPHY Abenheimer, K. M. A note on the couvade in modern England. British Journal pf Medical Psyphology. 1946, g0, 376-377. Argles, P. and Mackenzie, M. Crisis intervention with a multi- -problem family: A case study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiat_y and Allied Dis01plines. 1970. 11. 187-195. Bernstein, R. and Cyr, F. A study of interviews with husbands in a prenatal and child health program. Social Casework. 1957, 8, 473 -480. Blau, T. H. The professional in the community views the non-professional helper: psychology. Professional Psychology. 1969, 1, 25-31. Brigham Young University (Producer). The Miracle 9f Birth. Provo, Utah, 1974. (Film) Broussard, E. R. Neonatal prediction and outcome at 10/11 years. Revised version of a paper read at the 128th annual meeting of the American Psychiatric Association, 1975. Broussard, E. R. and Hartner, M. Maternal perception of the neonate as related to development. Child ngchiatry and Human Development, 1(1), 1970. Bruehl, R. G. The process of leaving home in a case of family postoral counseling. Journal pf Pastoral Care. 1971, _5, 241- 251. Campbell, D. T. and Stanley, J. C. EXperimental and quasi- eXperimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally College Publishing Company, 1963. Caplan G. Mental hypiene work with eXpectant mothers. Mental Hygiene. 1951, 35, 41-50. Caplan G. Ana approach to community mental health. London: Tavistock Publications, 1961. 118 119 Carkhuff, R. R. Differential functions of lay and pro- fessional helpers, Journal 9: Counselipg Psyphology, 1968, 15. 117-126. Chertok, L. Psychosomatic methods of preparation for childbirth: Spread of the methods, theory and research. American Journal quObstetrics and Gynecology. 1967, 9§C76981 Clark, L. Introducing mother and baby. American Journal 9f Nursing. 1974, 74, 1483-1484. Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press, 1969. Colman, A. and Colman, L. Pregnancy: The psychological eXperience. New York: The Seabury Press, 1973. Curtis, J. L. A. A psychiatric study of fifty-five eXpec- tant fathers. United States Armed Forces Medical Journal. 1955, 6, 937-950. Dyer, E. D. Parenthood as crisis: a re—study. Marriage and Family Liying. 1963, 25, 196-201. Edwards, A. L. EXperimental design ip psychological research. New York: Rinehart and Company, Inc., 1950. Eliot, T. D. Handling family strains and Shocks, in H. Becker and R. Hill (eds.). Family, Marriage and Parenthood. Boston: Heath, 1955. Erickson, E. H. Identity, Youth, and Crisis. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc., 1968. Feldman, H. Development pi the husband-wife relationship. Itgaca, New York: Cornell University, Mimeographed, 19 5. Feldman, H. The effects of children on the family. In A. Michel (ed.). Family issues 9: employed women ip Europg and America. Leiden: Brill, 1971. Feldman, H. and Feldman, M. The family life cycle: some suggestions for recycling. Journal q£_Marriage and the Famiiy. 1975, 32, 277-284. Figley, C. R. Child density and the marital relationship. Journal q£_Marriage and the Famiiyi 1973, 35, 272-282. 120 Freeman, T. Pregnancy as a precipitant of mental illness ‘ in men. British Journal Lf Medical Psychology. 1961, 24, 49 54. Gelles, R. J. The violent home: g study pi physical agression between husbands and wives. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, 1974. Gelles, R. J. Violence and pregnancy: a note on the extent of the roblem and needed services. Famiiy Coordinator. 1975. 2_. 81-86. Ginath, Y. Psychoses in males in relation to their wives' pregnancy and childbirth. Israel Annals Lf Psychiat_y and Related Disciplines. 1974,12, 227-237. Harrison, P. The making of a motherhood myth. American Baby. 1976, 3_, 26. Hartman, A. A. and Nicolay, R. Sexually deviant behavior in eXpectant fathers. Journal pi Abnormal Psyphology. 1966, z_, 232- 234. Hill, R. Families under stress. New York: Harper, 1949. Hill, R. Generic features of families under stress. In H. J. Parad (ed.). Crisis intervention. New York: Family Service Association of America, 1965. Hobbs, D. F. Parenthood as crisis: a third study. Journal pi Marriage and the Family. 1965, pg, 367-372. Hobbs, D. F. Transition to parenthood: a replication and extension. Journal pi Marriagg and the Family. 1968, 3p, 413—417. ““““ Hott, J. The crisis of expectant6 fatherhood. American Journal pi Nwwsing.1976,z_, 1436- 1440. Hurley, J. R. and Palonen, D. Marital satisfaction and child density among university student parents. Journal pi Marriage and the Family. 1967,2 _9, 483- 484. Huttel, F. A. A quantitative evaluation of psycho-prophy- laxis in childbirth. Journal pi Psychosomatic Research. 1972. lé. 81-92. ‘ Inman, W. S. The couvade in modern England. British Journal pi Medical Psychology. 1941, _9, 37- 55. 121 Jacoby, A. P. Transition to parenthood: a reassessment. Journal pi Marriage and the Family. 1960, 3i, 720-727. Jarvis, W. Some effects of pregnancy and childbirth in men. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association. 1962. 19. 689-659. Klaus, M. and Kennell, H. Maternal-infant bonding: the impact pi early separation pi loss pp family development. St. Louis: Mosby, 1976. LaCoursiere, R. B. Fatherhood and mental illness: a review and new material. Psychiatric Quarterly. 1972, fig, 109-124. Landers, Ann. Lansing, Michigan: The State Journal. January 22, 1976, D2. Landis, J. T. and Landis, M. G. Building p successful marriage. New York: Prentice-Hall, 1948. LeMasters, E. E. Parenthood as crisis. Marriage and Family Living. 1957. 12. 352-355. LeMasters, E. E. Parents ip modern America: p sociological analysis. Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey, 1970. Liebenberg, B. EXpectant fathers. American Journal pi Orthppsychiatry. 1967, 37, 358-359. Lindemann, E. Symptomatology and management of acute gL1:iefLl American Journal pi Psychiatry. 1944, 1 1, 1 1-1 8. Meyerowitz, J. H. and Feldman, H. Transition to parenthood. Psychiatric Research Reports. 1966, 29, 78-84. Miller, B. 0. Child density, marital satisfaction and conventionalization: a research note. Journal pi Marriagp and the Famiiy. 1975, 37, 345-347. Miller, L. C., Hampe, E., Barrett, C. L., and Noble, H. Children's deviant behavior within the general popula- tion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1972. 21. 16-22?‘ Mott Foundation (Producer). While you're waiting. Flint, Michigan, 1967. (Film) Obrzut, L. A. EXpectant fathers' perception of fathering. American Journal pi Nursing. 1976, 2p, 1440-1442. # 122 Panter, G. G. and Linde, S. M. Now that you've had your baby. David McKay Company, Inc., 1975. Parad, H. and Caplan, G. A framework for studying families in crisis. Social Work. 1960, 3, 3-15. Parkes, C. M. Bereavement and mental illness. British Journal pi Medical Psychology. 1965, 38, 1-12. Parkes, C. M. Psycho-socialtransition: A field for study. Social Science and Medicine. 1971, 5, 101-115. Pugh, T. F., Jerath, B. K., Schmidt, W. M., and Reed, R. B. Rates of mental disease related to childbearing. New England Journal pi Medicine. 1963, 268, 1224-1228 Rappaport, R. Normal crises, family structure and mental health. Family Process. 1963, p, 68-80. Rappaport, R. and Rappaport, R. Family transitions in contemporary society. Journal pi Psychosomatic Research. 1968, ii, 29-38. Rhodes, G. L. Attitudes ip parents and family and motiva- tion for parenthood: pp expioratory stud . Unpublished M.A. thesis, Michigan State University, 1974. Rollins, B. and Feldman, H. Marital satisfaction over the family life cycle. Journal pi Marriage and the Family. 1970! 29 20'28. Russell, C. S. Transition to parenthood: problems and gratifications. Journal pi Marriage and the Family. 1974, pp, 294-301. Ryder, R. G. Longitudinal data relating marital satisfac- tion and having a child. Journal pi Marriage and the Family. 1973, 32, 604-606. Scherz, F. H. Theory and practice of family therapy. In R. W. Roberts and R. H. Nee (eds.). Theories pi social casework. Chicago, Illionis: University of Chicago Press, 1969. Schwartz, M. C. Casework implications of a worker's pregnancy. Social Casework, 1975, pp, 27-34. Shapiro, I. S. Is group parent education worthwhile? A research report. Marriage and Family Living. 1956, i8, 154. 123 Sheehy, G. Passages: predictable crises pi adult life. New York: E. P. Dutton and Company, 1976. Sifneos, P. B. Short-term psyphotherapy and emotional crisis. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press, 1972 Silverman, A. and Silverman A. The case against having children. New York: David McKay Co., Inc. 1971. Spock, B. How much should a first baby change a marriage? -Redbook Magazine. March, 1976, 22-28. Thomas, A., Chess, 8., and Birch, H. G. Temperament and behavior disorders ip children. New York: University Press, 1968. Tiedge, L. B. An analysis of the expressed concerns of a selected group of eXpectant parent organization prenatal graduates at six and twelve weeks postpartum. Lansing, Michigan: EXpectant Parent Organization, Mimeographed, 1979. Tinker, R. H. Dominance ip marital interaction. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Michigan State University, 1972. Trethowan, W. H. and Conlon, M. F. The couvade syndrome. British Journal pi Psyehiatpy. 1965, 111, 57-66. Trethowan, W. H. The couvade syndrome: some further observations. Journal pi Psychosomatic Research. 1968, ii, 107-115. Wainwright, W. M. Fatherhood as a precipient of mental 4114383. American Journal pi Psychiatry. 1966, 123, O- . Weiner, R. Adolescent problems: a symptom of family disfunction. Social Casework. 1966, 4 , 373-377. Wenner, N. K. and Ohaneson, E. M. Motivations for preg- nancy. American Journal pi Orthopsychiatry. 1967, 22: 357’358- Wurger, M. K. The young adult stepping into parenthood. American Journal of Nursing. 1976, 1p, 1283-1285. Zemlick M. and Watson, R. Maternal attitudes of acceptance and rejection during and after pregnancy. American Journal pi_0rthopsychiatpy. 1953, pg, 570. APPENDIX A Introductory Letter 124 APPENDIX A $3 X y'A ' 9 as: expectant parents 32%;“ orgamzation August 5, 1976 Dear Expectant Parents, He are currently revising our program and assessing its ability to help parents adjust to the pregnancy and birth of their first child. In order to do this, we will be surveying and interviewing a number of parents who are enrolled in our program. On the basis of the parents‘ responses. we will be able to modify our present program so that it better meets the needs of couples who are expecting a child. As you are no doubt aware by now, pregnancy and beginning parenthood are times of many feelings and changes. Your participation in this survey will help us to better understand the adjustments necessary in becoming parents so our program can provide information which will be helpful in dealing with the first few months of parenthood. Those parents who participate will be contacted on separate occasions before and after their child is born. The survey should require only 20 to 30 minutes to fill out a questionnaire and 15 to 20 minutes in a telephone interview for each of the contacts. This assessment is being conducted through the cooperation of Expectant Parents’ Organization and the Michigan State University Cooperative Extension Service. It has been funded by the Ingham County Board of Comissioners. Before your assignment to an expectant parent class, you will be contacted concerning your willingness to participate in the survey. This will allow your questions about the study to be answered and will aid us in assigning you to a parent education group. Your participation should not only prove helpful and thought-provoking for you as a family but also will serve to help later couples in their pregnancy and parenthood. Results of this survey will be made available to you upon its completion if you wish to see them. Your cooperation is vital to the success of this program. All responses will, of course, be confidential. Naturally, you are free to choose not to par- ticipate; however, if you do so decide, we ask you to indicate that decision when you are first contacted within the next couple weeks. Thank you for your cooperation, Carol Ducat 2620 montego o lensing, Michigan 48912 APPENDIX B ‘Research Contract 125 APPENDIX B *4 % *A Research: Contract , see expectant parents Vim)? orgamzatIon Expectant Parents' Organization maintains a philosophy of parent participation in its programs and planning. The cooperation of expectant and new parents in honestly sharing their experiences and reactions is essential to the success of this philosophy. Those parents participating in this feedback procedure can expect to experience the satisfaction of helping later beginning parents. More importantly, as a result of the interviews and questionnaires used, participants should experience an increased awareness of themselves, their families, their feelings and their adjustments to pregnancy and parenthood. This study is designed to assess expectant parent education classes. Two new classroom approaches are to be tried. Parents who agree to be available to give feedback in the program will be assigned by a lottery process to one of the new programs. The study will obtain feedback from parents about the effectiveness of the approaches and also provide Expectant Parents' Organization with additional information about the process of become a parent so that the program may be further revised to include additional relevant information. This information will, of course, be shared with other expectant parent education programs for their incorporation. In short, the over-all goal of this assessment is the improvement of expectant parent education. The feedback process is aimed at getting your input in the following areas of family life: (l) your perceptions and expectations of your child, (2) the effects of parenthood on your marital relationship, 3) your family's decision- making procedures, (4) your communication patterns, 5) family changes and adjustments necessary for you to adapt to pregnancy and the addition of a child, (6) rewards and costs of family life. In order to clarify our expectations of participants and so that you can better understand what to expect of us, the following agreement is made. The expectant couple agrees to the following: (1) Each parent will complete a questionnaire within prescribed time limits on two or three different occasions (before attending class, 3-4 weeks after birth, and 6-7 months after birth). (2) Each parent will participate in a phone interview on two or three different occasions (before attending class, 3-4 weeks after birth and 6-7 months after birth). 126 (3) The expectant couple will provide relevant background information (see attached sheet). (4) The couple will allow anonymous use of the pooled information volunteered by all couples for the purpose of providing information to other parent education programs . (5) Each parent will complete brief classroom evaluations at the end of each class period. (6) The expectant couple will fill out and return the enclosed postage paid card within one week of the baby's birth. (7) The expectant couple will provide accurate and honest information. The Expectant Parents' Organization agrees to the following: (1) Classroom notebooks will be provided free of charge for participating parents. (2) Information provided by expectant couples will be kept confidential and anonymous in tabulating results. (3) Evaluation results will be provided within six months of the completion of the study to those parents who desire them. (4) Twenty hours of classroom training will be provided for participating couples at the usual charge. (5) Every effort will be made to schedule phone interviews at a convenient time. The undersigned have read the above conditions and agree to them. DATE Expectant Mother DATE Expectant Father DATE Project Coordinator Phone: B4l-6845 Please fill out the enclosed Background Information sheet and sign and date this fonm. Keep one copy of this agreement and the post card for your files. Return one copy of this agreement and the Background Information form to us in the enclosed envelop. Any questions may be directed to the Project Coordinator. APPENDIX C Background Information 127 APPENDIX C Background'Information BACKGROUND INFORMATION ' Wife's Name Wife‘s Age Hife’s Occupation wife's Race Hife's level of education (circle highest year completed) school grades college 6 7 B 9 l0 ll 12 l 2 3 4 MA PhD Hife's mother's level of education (circle highest grade completed) school grades college 6 7 B 9 l0 ll 12 ,l 2 3 4 MA PhD Hife's father's level of education (circle highest grade completed) school grades college 6 7 8 9 10 ll 12 l 2 3 4 MA PhD Has wife ever been divorced? (circle one) YES NO If Yes, when? Has wife had any previous children? (circle one) YES NO First letter and first three digits of wife‘s drivers license number * Husband's Name Husband's Age Husband's Occupation ' Husband's Racep______ Husband's level of education (circle highest year completed) school grades college 6 7 B 9 10 ll l2 1 2 3 4 MA PhD Husband's mother's level of education (circle highest grade completed) school grades college 6 7 8 9 l0 ll l2 1 2 3 4 HA PhD Husband's father's level of education (circle highest grade completed) school grades college 6 7 8 9 10 ll l2 1 2 3 4 MA PhD Has husband ever been divorced? (circle one) YES NO If Yes, when? Has husband had any previous children? (circle One) YES NO First letter and first three digits of husband's drivers license number * Your approximate combined yearly income: How long have you been married? Did you live together prior to marriage? (Circle one) YES NO If yes, how long? 'This is the code number which will be used to anonymously identify your responses in the future. 0 APPENDIX D Course Content for T and B Class 1 OBJECTIVES: 1. To acquaint parents with other class members. 2. To acquaint parents with total program and import- ance of prenatal instruction. 3. To dispell superstitions and anxieties regarding development of the baby by providing factual inform- ation. 4. To give information of the maternal changes and fetal development of the first trimester of preg- nancy. 5. To help expectant parents deal effectively with their feelings concerning this period of pregnancy. 6. To help expectant parents understand the importance and meaning of prenatal care. TOPICS: l. Introductions 2. Film - While You're Waiting 3. Conception and birth control 4. The first trimester of pregnancy Class 2 OBJECTIVES: I. To enable expectant parents to understand the phys- ical changes that occur in the mother and the devel-_ mental growth of the fetus during the second and third trimester. 2. To provide information on the natural pattern of weight gain based on fetal and maternal changes. 3. To help expectant parents identify common feelings during this stage of pregnancy and explore ways of dealing with these feelings. 4. To promote understanding of the importance of nutri- tion. TOPICS: l. The second and third trimester 2. Nutrition 3. Film - Great Expectations 4. Exercises to relieve physical discomfort during pregnancy. Class 3 OBJECTIVES: 1. To provide information relevant to the parents' choice to breast or bottle feed. 2. To encourage creativity in the choice made. 3. To encourage confidence in the choice made for 128 APPENDIX D Course Content for T infant feeding. 129 Class 3 cont. TOPICS: l. Breastfeeding 2. Bottle feeding Class 4 OBJECTIVES: - 1. To assist the expectant parents to prepare for both the physical and emotional experience of childbirth. 2. To lessen anxiety and fear of the unknown through emphasis on the normal processes of labor and delivery. 3. To alert each couple to the individuality of each labor and delivery. TOPICS: l. Pre-labor and plans for hospital admittance 2. First stage of labor 3. Abdominal breathing Class 5 OBJECTIVES: 1. To assist parents in preparing for the emotional and physical experience of childbirth. 2. To lessen anxiety and fear of the unknown. 3. To practice breathing techniques for labor and delivery. TOPICS: 1. Active phase of labor 2. Chest breathing 3. Transition phase 4. Second stage of labor 5. Third stage of labor 6. Pushing technique 7. Summary of all stages of labor and delivery Class 6 OBJECTIVES: l. To allow discussion of parents' concerns about labor and delivery. 2. To acquaint expectant parents with initial medical treatment of their baby. 3. To allow discussion of feelings about the father in the delivery room. 4. To acquaint couples with recovery room procedures. TOPICS: 1. Film - Miracle of Birth 2. Fathers in the delivery room 3. Care of the infant in the delivery room 4. Recovery room 130 Class 7 OBJECTIVES: 1. To give parents a realistic picture of the newborn infant's appearance. 2. To identify the needs of the new mother and infant particular to the post partum period. TOPICS: l. The appearance of the newborn 2. The physical needs of the newborn 3. The physical needs of the new mother 4. Film — The Newborn Class 8 OBJECTIVES: 1. To familiarize the couple with the hospital sur- roundings in which the birth will occur. TOPICS: 1. Physical aspects of the hospital 2. Tour Class 9 OBJECTIVES: 1. To provide basic information to parents regarding clothing and equipment that is needed in the care of an infant. 2. To provide information to parents as to the basic needs of an infant in the first year of life. 3. To provide information to parents as to growth and development during the first year. 4. To help parents recognize phases of adjustment of both parent and child. TOPICS: 1. Equipment and clothing needs 2. Basic needs of the infant 3. Phases of adjustment 4. Attachment 5. Safety 6. Babysitters Class 10 OBJECTIVES: 1. To motivate parents to explore various philosophies of parenting 2. To enable parents to cope with the stresses of leaving the hospital and assuming the roles of parents. TOPICS: 1. Baby temperament 2. Crying babies 3. Philosophies of parenthood 4. Child discipline 131 — Course Content for E Class 1 OBJECTIVES: 1. To help couples recognize that pregnancy, birth and parenthood are stressful events which are represent- ative of the many normal developmental events occur- ring in the family life cycle. 2. To encourage the identification of the needs and expectations of the class participants. 3. To provide a rationale and overview of the class. TOPICS: l. Introductions 2. Film - Adapting pp Parenthood 3. The Family Change Model Class 2 OBJECTIVES: 1. To illustrate the dimensions of the second trimester in relation to the family change model. 2. To understand that exercise is an essential element in health maintenance for mother and father. 3. To provide information and practice exercises that will help the mother reduce the discomforts of pregnancy and prepare her body for the birth pro- cess. TOPICS: l. The second trimester 2. Care of the pregnant body 3. Exercises Class 3 OBJECTIVES: 1. To understand the relationship between stress, tension and pain. 2. To learn a procedure for reducing tension in the delivery room and elsewhere. 3. To learn constructive communication skills for use in preventing or reducing stress. TOPICS: 1. Stress 2. Communications skills 3. Relaxation techniques Class 4 OBJECTIVES: 1. To provide information relevant to breast and bottle feeding. 2. To provide practice in a decision-making model. 3. To promote the use of decision-making process when appropriate. 132 Class 4 cont. TOPICS: 1. Breast feeding 2. Bottle feeding 3. Film - Breastfeedipg 4. Decision-making Class 5 OBJECTIVES: 1. To develop a better understanding of the labor pro- cess. 2. To reduce anxiety of the unknown by providing inform- ation. TOPICS: 1. Preparation for birth 2. Plans for hospital admission 3. Signs and symptoms of labor 4. Stages of labor and delivery 5. Coaching role of the husband Class 6 OBJECTIVES: I. To develop a better understanding of the birth pro- cess thereby reducing anxiety of the unknown. 2. To develop skills in breathing techniques that will facilitate the birth process and also alter the mother's perception of discomfort. TOPICS: 1. Second state of labor 2. Anesthesia 3. Forceps, fetal monitors, c/sections 4. Chest breathing, panting 5. Film - Miracle pi Birth 6. Baby in the delivery room 7. Recovery room Class 7 OBJECTIVES: 1. To facilitate an understanding of the physical, emotional, and relational changes occurring during the postpartum period. 2. To promote a positive relationship between parents and their infant through an understanding of the attachment process and knowledge of the infant's needs. 3. To provide an opportunity to review and practice breathing and relaxation skills for childbirth. TOPICS: l. The attachment process 2. Physical chan es in the mother following delivery 3. Emotional and relational dimensions after birth 4. Preparation for childbirth skills 133 Class 8 OBJECTIVES: 1. To familiarize the couple with the hospital sur- roundings in which the birth will occur. TOPICS: 1. Physical aspects of the hospital 2. Tour Class 9 OBJECTIVES: 1. To sort out expectations of baby and his behavior. 2. To identify what behaviors are expected for the role of mother and father. 3. To practice decision making skills in coping with the crying baby. TOPICS: 1. What is life with baby like? 2. What is my role as mother/father/parent? 3. Coping with crying Class 10 OBJECTIVES: 1. To recognize that the marriage relationship is an important factor in the health growth and develop- ment of the family, including the child. 2. To promote awareness of common problems expressed by new parents. 3. To review some methods of coping with these prob— lems through re-establishing closeness between the parents and identifying support systems outside the family. TOPICS: 1. Common concerns of new parents 2. Re-establishing closeness 3. Outside sources of support 4. Course summary APPENDIX E Curriculum Workshop Friday, Sept. 10 7:30-8:00 p.m. 8:00-8:30 p.m. 8:30-10:00 p.m. Saturday, Sept. 8:00-8:45 a.m. 8:45-9:30 a.m. 9:30-9:45 a.m. 9:45-11:15 a.m. 11:15-11:45 a.m. ll:45-l:OO p.m. 1:00-2:00 p.m. 134 APPENDIX E Curriculum Workshop Sept. 10, 11, 12, 1976 Demonstration on how to use new projectors Introduction, ground rules, schedule Carol Ducat Class 1 Introductions, "Adjusting to Parenthood" film, Family Change Model, Research methods and approach Class 2 How to relate homework assignments to second trimester Communication and Stress Break Continue with Communication and Stress Lunch Decision Making Classes 5 and 6 Labor and Delivery 2:00 p.m. (optional) preview of film "Great Expectations" Sunday, Sept. 12 8:00-9:00 a.m. 9:00-9:30 a.m. 9:30-9:45 a.m. 9:45-11:15 a.m. ll:l5-ll:45 a.m. Class 7 Hospital Stay, Attachment Relationship with Child Break Continue Relationship with Child Role Playing Lunch 135 llz45-lz30 p.m. Relationship with Spouse 1:30-2:00 p.m. Summary APPENDIX F Class Topic Checklist 136 Class Topic Checklist ID # Male Female (circle one) Following is a list of tOpics which may or may not have been discussed in your class. Please check the topics which you remember covering during this ten—week series. physical aSpects of pregnancy emotional aspects of pregnancy effects of pregnancy on the family family change stress diapering a baby feeding a baby bottle preparation breast feeding family decision-making stages of labor and delivery exercises for labor and delivery exercises for pregnancy newborn characteristics baby temperament or disposition parenthood effects of babies on the husband-wife relationship maintaining closeness with your spouse after birth infant schedutes postpartum changes in the mother nutrition choosing a pediatrician effective communication disciplining children coping with a crying baby pediatrician in class as guest lecturer equipping the nursery child safety APPENDIX G Instructor Evaluation Form 137 APPENDIX G Instructor Evaluation Form CODE: ' CLASS: INSTRUCTOR: NEEKLY EVALUATION For purposes of our survey, at the end of each class. you will be asked to quickly rate your instructor based on how she presented the materials for that specific class period. Please indicate your response to the following statements by circling the word or phrase which best describes your assessment. If you were absent for the class, please check this blank and turn in the form. Circle one: Male Female The instructor's knowledge about today's subject was: EXCELLENT GOOD ADEQUATE POOR The instructor's enthusiasm for today's subject was: VERY HIGH HIGHER THAN ABOUT LOWER THAN VERY LON AVERAGE AVERAGE AVERAGE In dealing with today's topics. the instructor appeared: VERY SOHEHHAT SLIGHTLY VERY ILL AT COMFORTABLE COMFORTABLE UNCOHFDRTABLE EASE The instructor's ability to present ideas about today's topics was: EXCEPTIONAL GOOD ABOUT AVERAGE POOR EXCEPTIONALLY POOR I 0--------'---'-------------'--------------"---"-.,3.“'------2 ............................ CODE: CLASS: INSTRUCTOR: __HEEKLY EVALUATION For the purposes of our survey, at the end of each class. you will be asked to quickly rate your instructor based on how she presented the materials for that specific class period. Please indicate your response to the following statements by circling the word or phrase which best describes your assessment. If you were absent for the class, please check this blank and turn in the form. Circle one: Male Female The instructor's knowledge about today's subject was: EXCELLENT GOOD ADEQUATE POOR The instructor's enthusiasm for today's subject was: a VERY HIGH HIGHER THAN ABOUT LONER THAN VERY LOH AVERAGE AVERAGE AVERAGE In dealing with today's topics, the instructor appeared: VERY SOHEHHAT SLIGHTLY VERY ILL AT COMFORTABLE COMFORTABLE UNCOMFORTABLE EASE The instructor's ability to present ideas about today's topics was: EXCEPTIONAL GOOD ABOUT AVERAGE POOR EXCESgégNALLY APPENDIX H Instructor Reports Date: 138 APPENDIX H Instructor Reports Instructor's name: Educational approach being used at present time: Traditional program Modified program Please indicate your feelings of satisfaction with each of the two educational approaches by checking the best answer to complete the statements below: At the present time, I believe the traditional educational approach to be very satisfactory. somewhat satisfactory. neutral. somewhat unsatisfactory. very unsatisfactory. At the present time, I believe the modified educational approach to be very satisfactory. somewhat satisfactory. neutral. somewhat unsatisfactory. very unsatisfactory. If I were to compare the two programs, I would say the modified program is far superior to the traditional one. the modified program is somewhat better than the traditional one. both the modified and the traditional programs have equal merit. the traditional program is somewhat better than the modified one. the traditional program is far superior to the modified one. Please make any comments about your feelings or eXperiences with these two programs below. APPENDIX I Questionnaires 139 APPENDIX I Questionnaires GENERAL DIRECTIONS FOR QUESTIONNAIRES Enclosed are two questionnaires, one for the wife and one for the husband. It should take about half an hour apiece to fill out the questionnaires. Specific directions are included in the questionnaires themselves when needed. It is important that you try to answer each question even though it may sometimes be difficult to decide on an answer. If you should have difficulty with a question, answer it as best you can; and 11 you wish, write a comment explaining your answer. Please fill out your own questionnaire before talking with your spouse about‘how he/she would answer. After you have both finished your question- naires, feel free to discuss your answers with each other if you wish. However, do ppp_change your original answers on the questionnaire after talking with your spouse. Return the completed questionnaires separately in the two envelopes which are provided. Thank you 140 APPENDIX I Questionnaires ID i l ' oars pre-husb ' HUSBAND'S quesTIoaNAxns murr* Host persons have disagreements in their relationships. Please indicate below the approximate extent of agreement or disagreement between you and your partner for each item on the following list. - Almost Occa- Fre- Almost - Always Always sionally , quently Always Always .Agree Agree Disagree Disagree Disagree Disagree 1. Handling family finances 2. Matters of recreation 3. Religious matters 4. Demonstration of affec- ‘ tion 5. Friends 6. Sex relations 7. Conventionality (correct or proper behavior) 8. Philosophy of life 9. Hays of dealing with parents or in-laws 10. Aims. goals. and things believed important ll. Amount of time spent together - ' 12. Making major decisions 13. Household tasks 14. Leisure time interests and activities -15. Career decisions 16. Child raising an ' hue - the Host of often Occa- ' time the time than not sionally Rarelz Never ll. How often do you discuss ’ - or have you considered divorce. separation or terminating your relao tionship? ‘ ‘ 18. How often do you or your mate leave the house after a fight? 19. In general, how often do ~ you think that things between you and your partner are going well? Over * Part I is mainly comprised of the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment»Scale. 141 ID i pre-husb All More , the Host of often Occa- time the time than not sionallv Rarely, Never 20. Do you confide in your note? 21. Do you ever regret that you married? , ' 22. How often do you and your partner quarrel? ' 23. How often do you and your mate 'get on each other's nerves?“ . . ' ‘ Almost Occa- Everv an Every Day sionally, Rarely Never 24. Do you kiss your mate? All of Host of Some of Very few None of them them them of them then 2S. Do you and your mate en- gage in outside interests together? - How often would you say the following events occur between you and your mate? Less than Once or Once or once a Twice a Twice a Once a More Honth Month week day often if < 0 ‘ 25. Have a stimulating exchange of ideas 27. Laugh together 28. Calmly discuss something 29. Work together on a project There are some things about which couples sometimes agree and sometimes disagree. Indicate if either item below caused differences of opinions or were problems in your relationship during the past few weeks. (Check yes or no) Yes No 30. .__ Being too tired for sex.. 3l. Not showing love. The dots on the following line represent different degrees of happiness in your elationship. The middle point, "happy," represents the degree of happiness of most relationships. Please circle the dot which best describes the degree of happiness. all things considergd, of your rglationshipa S 6 O Extremely Fairly A Little Happy Very Extremely Perfect ’ yghepay . Qghaapy U_nhappy - Happy Haney 11:2 In I pro-hush 32. which or the following statements best describes how you feel about the future o! your relationship? _I want desperately tor my relationship to succssd. and would go to almost a; length to son that it does. _I want very much for my relationship to succeed, and will do all I can —to see that it does. _I want very much for my relationship to succeed, and will do n: fair —shars to see that it does. _It would bo nice it my relationship succeeded. but I can't do much more —§hgg I am doing now to help it succeed. It would be nice i! it succeeded. but! refuse to do gag more than I so doing now to keep the relationship going. Hy relat -onship can never succeed. and there is no more that I can do to keep the relationship going. PAR! II 1. Check the response below'which best describes what changss'havs occurred in your total family income within the last six months. Total income decreased 36, 000 or more. Total income decreased less than 3e,OOO. Total income remained the same. Total incoma increased only by a cost of living raise. Total incoma increased by more than cost of living but less than 3‘, 000. Total income increased by more than $4,000. 2. Has your wife's employment situation changed as a result of the pregnancy? (circle one) Yes No If yes, describe how below. 3. During this pregnancy. how often has your wife seemed to experience physical or mental discomfort due to the pregnancy? (check best answer) all of the time most of the time more often than not occasionally rarely never l ‘ I 4. Are there regularly scheduled or usual times in your daily routine when you and your wife discuss things with each other? (circle one) Yes No If your answer was yes. when are those times? If your answer was yes. how often do they occur? (check best answer) less than once a month l-Z times per month l-Z times per week _ once a day ____more than once a day ever 11+} In f pre-husb 6. What is the average anount of tine.you and your wife spend in discussions each day? (check best answer) less than 15 ninutes between 15 ninutss and 30 minutes between 30 ninutes and 1 hour between 1 and 2 hours ‘ note than 2 hours 7. When you are faced with a decision, how often do you consult with your wife? (check best answer) - all of the tine sons of the tine nest of the ties occasionally Never 8. Please list below any recent decisions you have nsde about the toning baby. PART 111 Please indicate the degree of your agree-ens or disagree-set with each of the following statenents by circling the best answer according to the key below. III: &A - Strongly Agree d - disagree a - agree SD - Strongly Disagree 3 - Neither agree nor disagree 1. sa a a a so Housework is the wonan's job. 2. SA a h d SD A husband should feel obligated to help his wife if he has the tine. '3. SA a N id SD housework should be done before the husband cones hone. A. SA' a N d '50 The husband and wife should share in decisions concerning expenditures of money. 5. SA a H d SD All in all, ny own parents did a good job raising ne. 6. SA a N d SD All in all. ny wife's parents did a good job raising her. 7. SA a N d so At the present tine ny wife seess generally pleased with our sexual relationship. 8. SA a N d SD Adjusting to this pregnancyis a difficult experience for ne. 9. SA a N .d 80 Adjusting to this pregnancyis a difficult experience for ' ny wife. - _ 1.0..” a a d 50 After conpleting this questionnaire. ny wife and I are likely to discuss our answers to it. Continue to next page 144 In I pro-hush 2A2! IV Generally at the present tine, how satisfied are you with each of the following? (check one answer for each iten) very Somewhat Soaewhat -Very satisfied satisfied dissatisfied dissatisfied a. You and your wife's decisions about work b. You and your wife's decisions about parenting c. You and your wife's decisions about nosey d. You and your wife's . - decisions about leisure tine activities .e. You and your wife's. sexual relationship f. You and your wife's conversations g. The anount of ties you and your wife spend in consersations h. Your present job situation i. Your wife's present job situation - 1. Your participation in faeily decision nahing k. Your wife's participation in fanily decision raking ’ Paar v IRSTRUCTIOSS: For the following items, please check the answer which best conpletes the sentence. 1. As a result of ny.wife's pregnancy, ny wife and I now feel ouch closer to each other. feel sonewhat closer to each other. feel sonewhat about the sane degree of closeness as before the pregnancy. feel sonewnat less close to each other. feel such less close to each other. 2. Conparad with before ny wife's pregnancy. ny wife and I now have easy note discussions than before. a few note discussions than before. about the sans nunber of discussions. slightly fewer discussions than before. nany fewer discussions than before. Over 145 In I pta-husb 3. Compared "ifih 0“! 3031138! 5010‘! my wife became pregnant. my wife and I can now be described as 1____nuch less satisfied with our present situation. _ slightly less satisfied with our present situation. ____about as satisfied as we were before the pregnancy. ____nore satisfied with our present situation. much more satisfied with our present situation. ‘e M ‘ :‘3‘11: 0‘ my "1:". r. nc .. I have assumed many diff:::ntybehaviors and tasks from those I previously performed in the family. _ . , I have assumed some new behaviors and tasks within the family. I heve kept the same behaviors and tasks-within the family. I have performed fewer of my usual behaviors and tasks within the family. I have stopped many routine family tasks and behaviors. ated with how I felt before.ny wife became pregnant. I now feel much more unsure of my ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. somewhat less sure of my ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. about the same amount of confidence in my ability to handle difficulties. somewhat more confident of my ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. - ‘ ____euch more confident of my ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. 6. Cfllpetfid 91:5 how I felt b£f°!| my wife became pregnant. I'now feel much happier. - somewhat happier. ' about the same amount of happiness. somewhat less happy. much less happy. 7. when most decisions occur in our family. __g_one partner makes the final decision with little or no consulting of the other. ____one partner makes the final decision after discussion with the other. ____both partners reach the decision together through discussion and comprmnise. our decisions are not our own but are often forced on us by outside circumstances. Ellll u s PAR: VI 1. You probably have given some thought to what you think having a baby in the . family will be like. How much do you expect the coming baby to change each of the following in your family? (Check the best blank for each item.) A A moderate ' Very hot at great deal amount Somewhat little all a. Your relationship with your wife b. Your life style c. Your leisure time activities d. Your sleeping and eating habits e. Your sexual relationship f. Your family finances Continue to next page In! preehusb 2. Following is a partial list of new parents' tasks. Indicate how you think each of these tasks should be divided in your family. Check the appropriate blank for each item as you think your family should divide it. husband .Busband Husband was was should should do and wife should do should always do more than should share more than always do wife equally husband a. Change baby's - diapers b. Launder baby's clothes c. Quiet baby when it tries d. Arrange for care for baby when wife must be away s. Get up in the night with baby f. feed baby g. Play with baby b. Other tasks (please Ivscifr): ‘PAII VII' to: the following questions, make an I on the percentage line in the place which best represents your answer or opinion. . 1. All things taken into account. what percentage of the time have things been going well during the past month? (Mark the line below.) I ._n[ I I ~ I I I I I gI I 1002 so: so: 70: so: so: «or 30: 20: to: or 2. All things taken into account. what percentage of the time have you felt satis- fied with your marriage relationship during the past month? (Hark the line below.) I I I j I I I I I_ I' I 1002 901 802 702 b0: 50: 601 302 202 10! OZ 3. when all family decisions for the past month are taken into account, mark - the line below to indicate what percentage of those decisions you agree with. I '1" I I I I_ I I I I I 100: 902 802 702 602 50% 60: 301 201 10: 0: Over 147 APPENDIX I Questionnaires 10 f DATE post l-husb HUSSAND‘S QUESTIOflflAIRE emu: Host persons have disagreements in their relationships. Please indicate below the approximate extent of agreement or disagreement between you and your partner for each item on the following list. Almost Occa- Fro- ‘ Almost Always Always sionally quently Always Always Agree Aggree Disagree Disagree Disagree Disagree 1. Handling family finances 2. Matters of recreation 3. Religious matters 4. Demonstration of affec- ‘ tion 5. Friends 6. Sex relations 7. Conventionality (correct ' , or proper behavior) 8. Philosophy of life 9. Rays of dealing with parents or in-laws lD. Aims, goals. and things believed important ll. Amount of time spent together .olZ. Making major decisions l3. Household tasks . l4. Leisure time interests and activities 15. Career decisions _l§. Child raising All More . the Host of bften Occa- time the time than not sionallz» ggrely Hever l7. How often do you discuss or have you considered divorce. separation or . terminating your rela- tionship? . - l8. How often do you or your mate leave the house after a fight? 19. In general. how often do . you think that things between you and your partner are going well? Over * Part I is mainly comprised of the S anier D d' Adjustment Scale. p ya it 11i8 .ID i post 1 All ' More _ the Most of often Occa- time the time than not sionally, Rarely Never 20. Do you confide in your mate? Zl. Do you ever regret that you married? 22. How often do you and your partner quarrel? 23. How often do you and your . mate "get on each other's nerves?” ‘ "" Almost Occa- Eve Da Ever: Oaz sionallz Rarely Never 24. Do you kiss your mate? All of Host of Some of Very few None of them them them of them them 25. Do you and your mate en- gage in outside interests together? -' How often would you say the following events occur between you and your mate? Less than Once or Once or once a Twice a Twice a Once a More flgggg’ Month Month week day often 26. Have a stimulating exchange of ideas 27. Laugh together 28. Calmly discuss something 29. Work together on a project There are some things about which couples sometimes agree and sometimes disagree. Indicate if either item below caused differences of opinions or were problems in your relationship during the past few week . (Check yes or no) Yes he 30. Being too tired for sex. 31. Not showing love. The dots on the following line represent different degrees of happiness in your elationship. The middle point, ”happy.” represents the degree of happlness 0* most relationships. Please circle the dot which best describes the degree of happaness. all things considergd. of your rglationshipa 5 6 Extremely Fairly A Little Happy Very Extremely Perfect Qghappy unhappy_ Enhappy Happy H399! Continue to next page 149 ID I poot l-hnob 32. 1. 2. 3. 5. 5. Hhich at tho following ototononto boot'doocziboo how you fool about tho fututo o! you: tolotionohip? _I wont dooporotoly tot oy rolotionohip to ouccood, ond would go to olnoot —ggz_lggggg.to ooo thot it dooo. I wont voty ouch for oy tolotionohip to ouccood, ond will do all I too to too thot it dooo. I wont voty ouch for oy tolotionohip to ouccood, ond will do 31 to it ggg§:,to ooo thot it dooo. _It would bo nico it oy tolotiooohip ouccoodod, but I con' t do ouch ooto —§hon l on dgigg now to holp it ouccood. It would bo nico it it ouctoodod. but I tofuoo to do on: noto thou I on doigg now to hoop tho tolotionohip going. fly tolotionohip con novor ouccood, ond thoro is no noto thot 1 con do to hoop tho tolotionohip going. PAR! 11 Chock tho tooponoo bolow which boot dooctihoo who: thongoo hovo occuttod in you: totol tonily incnoo within tho loot oio noutho. Total incono doctooood $5,000 or onto. Iotol incooo doctooood looo thon $6,000. Totol incooo tonoinod tho oolo. rotol incono inctooood only by o toot o! living toioo. ' totol incooo incxooood by noto thoo toot of living but looo thou $6,000. _Iotol incooo inczooood by onto thon $6, 000. lovo you oovod oo o tooult at tho now boby? (cirtlo ono) to: lo If yon, dooctibo how your living ottongooonto hovo choogod (o.g., oowod tron l-bodrooo.opo:toont ond bought houoo). PLEASE NOT! NEH 12183808! HUHBER: During thio ptognoncy, how ottoo did you: wiio oooo to ooporionco phyoicol or . oll at tho tino octooionolly ooot of tho tino __ ttrIlY noto ofton thon not novor how would your wito'o lohot ond dolivory boot bo doocribed? voty ditticult oooy difficult ‘ voty oooy obout tho oooo oo ooot Arc thoto togulotly ochodulod or uouol tinoo in you: doily toutino whon you and your wito diocuoo things with ooch otho:2 (circlo ooo) to: No ' It you: onowo: woo yoo. whon oro thooo.tiooo? 11 you: onowot woo yoo, how otton do thoy occur? (chock boot ooowot) looo thon onco o nonth ooco o doy l-Z tinoo pot oonth _ooto thon onto o doy l—Z tinoo pot wook Ovor 150 ID ! poot l-huob 6. Shot in tho ovorogo anount of tinoyouond yourwito opond indiocuooiono ooch doy? (chock boot ono'wor) , ' looothoolininutoo , bomon 13 oinutoo and 30 oinutoo botnoon 30 oinutoo and 1 hour botwoonlondlhouro ' norothonlhouro 7. Hhon you oro tocod with o docioion. how otton do you conoult with your wito? (chock boot onowor) ollotthotino ';_ooooo£thotiao . _ooot o1 tho tino . '_occooionolly Novor 8. Ploooo liot bolow ony rocont docioiono you hovo oodo obout tho now boby. an 111 Ploooo indicoto tho dogroo o1 your ogroaont or dioogronont with ooch of tho following ototononto by circling tho boot onowor according to tho koy bolow. In: so - Strongly Agroo ’d - dioogroo o - ogroo SD - Strongly Dioogroo l - noithor ogroo not dioogroo 1. SA o l d 30 louoowork io tho wooon'o job. 2. SA o R d 50 A huobond should fool obligotod to-holp hio wiio i: ho hoo tho tino. 3. 8A o. N '_d 81) Bouooworh should bo dono bototo tho huobond coooohooo. 6. SA o R d 50 "rho huobond ond wito ohould ohoro in docioiono concorning upondituroo o1 oonoy. 5.8A o s d 51) Allinoll,nyownporontodidogoodjobroioingno. 6. so .‘ x a so Allinoll.oywi£o'o poronto didogood job roioing hot. 7. SA o N d SD At tho prooont tioo oy wifo oo-no gonorolly plooood with our ooansol rolotionohip. 8. SA o H d 81) ddjuoting to thio now boby in o difiicult ooporiooco {or no. 9. 8A o I d 81) Adjuoting to thio now boby io o difficult ooporionco for. any wizo. 10..“ o a d $0 “to: cooploting thio quootionnoiro. oy wito ond I oro 7 likoly to diocuoo our onoworo to it. Continuo to non: pogo 1151 lb I poo: l-huob an IV Gonorollyotthoproaont tino. hooootiotiodoaroyouwithoochot thotollowingl (chockonoanowortoroochitu) _ Yor'y Solowho: Sol-aha: Vory ootiotiod ootiotiod dioootiotiod dioootiotiod o. You and your wito'o dociaiono about uork b. You ond your wifo'o docioiono about porootin; t.'You and your wito'o docioiono obout oonoy d. You and your wifo'o docioiono obout loiouro :ioo octivitioo .o. You-‘ond your wilo'o. ooouol rolotionohip 1. You and your wifo'a convorootiooa g.‘!hoooounto£tiooyouoodyour witoopondinconvorootiooo h. Your prooon: job oituotion 1. Your wiio'o prooont job oituation j..Your participotiou in tonily docioion noting h. Your wilo' o puticipotion in looily docioion noting rmv 1331313611038: Yo: tho following itooo, ploooo chock tho atom which but cooplotoo tho oontonco. l.dootooulto£ thiobirth,nywi£oandlnoo tool nth clooor to ooch othor. tool oouowhot clooor to oach othor. tool aouowhot obout tho “so dogroo of clooocooo oo boron-o tho prepaid-y. ‘5 fool oooouhot looo clooo to ooc'a othor. foolnuch loco clooo to oochothor. 2. Connorod with hotoro tho birth of our baby, :1 21:. cod 2. now hovo ‘ oony noto diacuooiono than boforo. o tow aoro diocuooiono than boforo. . about tho oano nuobor o1 diocuooions. _ olightly towor diocuooiono than botoro. oony fowor diocuooiono thon boforo. 152 In I post l-huob 3. Cooparod with our foolings boforo tho birth of our baby. ny wifo and I can now bo doscribod as ____nuch loss satisfiod with our prosont situation. ___.slightly loss satisfiod with our prosont situation. ___.about as satisfiad as wo wora boforo tho prognoncy. ooro satisfiod with our prasont situation. _ouch noro aatisfiad with our prosont situation. A. As a rasult of this birth. ____I hovo assunod nony difforont bohoviors and tasks tron thooo I prawiously porfornod in tho faoily. I hovo assunod soon now bohowiors and tasks within tho faoily. _ ____I hows hop: tho saoo bohowiors and tasks within tho faoily. I hovo porfotnod favor of ny usual bohavioro and tasks within tho fanily. _I hovo stoppod nony routino faoily tasks and bohawiors. S. Conoarod with how I fol: boforo tho birth of our baby, I now fool _ouch ooro unouro of oy ability to handlo difficultios than I fol: boforo. _sonowha: lass aura of ny ability to bandlo difficultios than I fol: boforo. about tho saoo aooun: of confidonco in ny ability to hondlo difficultios. sooowho: ooro confidant of oy ability to hondlo difficultios than I fol: bofoto. ____ouch ooro confidant of oy ability to handlo difficultias than I fol: bofora. 6. Coooarod with how I fol: boforo tho birth, I now fool ouch hoopior. aooowhot hoppior. about tho saoo anoun: of happinoso. sooowhat loos happy. ouch loss happy. soot docisiono occur in our fanily. ono partnor aakos tho final docision with littla or no consulting of tho othor. mpartnar nokos tho final docision aftor discussion with tho othor. _bo:h partnors roach tho docision togothor through discussion and conproniso. :our docisions aro not our own but aro oftan forcod on us by outsida —circuostoncos. 7 IA]: VI 1. A now baby always cousas sooo changes in tho family. Chock tho blank which .-boo: doscrihoo how such your now baby has changod oath of tho following. A A nodarato ' Vary Not a: grant doal amount Sonowhot littla all a. Your rolationahip with your wifo b. Your lifo stylo c. Your laisuro tins activitios d. Your slooping and oating habits o. Your sandal rolationship f. Your fanily financoa g. 0thor (plooao opocify): llllll llllll llllll Continua to nor: pogo ' 153 ID I . post l-husb 2. following is a partial list of now paronta' tasks. Placo an ”S” in tho blank which indicatas how you think oach of thoso tasks should bo dividod in your family. Plato an "A" in tho blonk which indicatss how this task actually i_s_ dividod in your family. . You will has two answors for oath itan. husband Husband Husband Wifo "ifs should should do and wifo should do should always do unto than should shora noro than always do wifo squally husband a. Chango baby' a diopors b. Laundor baby's clothas c. Quiot baby whon it crios d. Arrango for cars for baby whon wifo oust bo away .o 6.: up ‘3 eh. m: with baby ' f. toad baby g. Play with baby h. ' 0thor tasks (plooao spocify) : rm VII' For tho following quostions. oaks an x on tho portontago‘ lino in tho placo which boot roprosonts your answor or opinion.. , 1. All things takon into account. what parcantago of tho tins hows things boon going wall during tho pas: nonth‘! (Hark tho lino bolow.) L I I I « I I I - J J I ' I max 90: so: 70: . 60: so: 40: 30: 20: 10: oz ' 2. All things takon into account, what parcantago of tho tins hows you fol: satis- If’itd with your norriago rolstionship during tho pas: nonth? ,(hark tho lino o ow. l I Ifi I f / I It I I / 1001 901 80: 701 60: 502 401 302 201 10: oz 3. "hon all fanily docisions for tho past month aro takon into account, nork tho lino bolow to indicato who: porcontago of thoao docision: you agroo with. . I / / - / I / l I 1002 902 802 701 602 502 40% 301 201 1.02 OZ Ovor 154 In I post-l 1mm: 8mm. Cooploto tho following sontoncas as quickly as you too. Don't worry about tartbook answors; cooploto th- ths way m fool about tho issuoo involwod. l. 1. 3. 6. 3. 7. 9. 1‘. Who: poronts want nos: of thair childron Sooowooondon'twaottnbowocbildranbocouso Hynothor hon wont childran bocauso Largo fanilias fathor oopocts his child birth control A childlaso Iarriago Conorally, tho rooson for having childron I want to boys childron bocauso Sooooondon': wan: to howo childron bocauoo hoot fanilios Hononwantchildranbocauso Although having childran is natural Continua to nor: pogo. 16. 17. 21. 22. 30. 155 In planning for a child's futuro Rho: childron won: ans: of thoir parants Ibo only child hnthor axpocts hor child Ibo roason I want to born a son o "ban a child is ya: unborn, tho paronts In considoring oorriago. a poroon Childron who aro not plannod I would oopoct Iy son to I would liko to hawo a daughtar bocauoo hy fathor Early norriogoo Plannod paronthnod logarding childron, tho wifo wants hor husband to I would oopoc: ny daughtar to I hops to hows 0, l, 2, 3, 5+ childran. (Circlo ono) 156 APPENDIX I Questionnaires ID I DATE post g-husb HUSBAND‘S QUESTIONNAIRE PART I Must persons have disagreements in their relationShips. Please indicate below the approximate extent of agreement or disagreement between you and your partner for each item on the following list. Almost Octa- Fre- Almost lweys Always sionally quently Always Always Am Am Disagree Disagree Disaggg Disagree > l. handling fhnily finances 2. Matters of recreation 3. Religious setters 4. Demonstration of affeco tion 5. Friends - 6. Sex relations 7. Conventionalitxmicomct or proper behavior) 8. Philosophy of life 9. Hays of dealing with parents or in-laws l0. Aims. goals. and things believed important ll. Amount of tine spent together l2. Making najor decisions l3. Household tasks l4. Leisure time interests and activities 15. Career decisions 16. Child raising I III III III HI I I MI More _ Host of often Occa- the time than not gionally Rarely . Never |§~£§lll l7. How often do you discuss or have you considered divorce. separation or terminating your rela- tionship? - l8. How often do you or your note leave the house after a fight? l9. In general. how often do you think that things between you and your partner are going well? 157 ID! , you: All ‘yore the Host of often Occa- time the time than not sionally Rarely Never 20. Do you confide in your mate? Zl. Do you ever regret that you married? 22. Haw often do you and your partner quarrel? 23. How often do you and your mate ”get on each other's nerves?“ Almost Occa- Evegz Day Eves: Day sionally Rarely» Never _ 24. Do you kiss your mate? All of Host of Some of, Very few-hone of them them than . of them them 25. Do you and your mate en- gage in outside interests together? .. __ __ How often would you say the following events occur between you and your mate? Less than. Once or Once or -once a Twice a Twice a Once a More Never, Month Honth 9 week ..day gften 26. Have a stimulating exchange _ of ideas _ - Z7. Laugh together 28. Calmly discuss something 29. Work together on a project There are some things about which couples sometimes agree and sometimes disagree indicate if either item below caused differences of opinions or were problems in your relationship during the gag: few necks. (Check yes or no) Yes No 30. __ _ Being too tired for sex. 31. __ __ Not showing love. The dots on the following line represent different degrees of happiness in your clationship. The middle point.-"htppy." represents the degree of happiness of most relationships. Please circle the dot which best describes the degree of happiness, all things considergd, of your rglationshipa 5 0 Extremely Fairly A Little _ Happy Very Extremely Perfect 911mm game)! when Happy - Happy 158 In! post Z-husb 32. Which of the following statements best describes how you feel about the future of your relationship? I want desperately for my relationship to succeed. and would go to almost any lgggth to see that it does. I went very much for my relationship to succeed. and will do all I can to see that it does. I went very much for my relationship to succeed. and will do 5: fair share to see that it does. It would be nice if my relationship succeeded, but §_ggg___gg_ggg§_gggg than I am doing now’to help it succeed. It would be nice if it succeeded. but I refuse to do any more than I «doing now to keep the relationship going. fly relationship can never succeed. and there is no more that I can do to keep the relationship going. Illll ‘ ' ‘ {All 11 1. Check the response below which best describes what changes have occurred in your total family income within the last year. tbtal income decreased $6,000 or more. total income decreased less than 36,000. total income remained the same. Total income increased only by a cost of living raise. Total income increased by more than cost of living but less than $6, 000. Total income increased by more than $6,000. 2. Have you moved as a result of the baby? (circle one) Yes no If yes, describe how your living arrangements have changed (e.g.. moved from lmbedroon apartment and bought house). PLEASE HOT! NEW TILEPBON! NURSER: 3. have you changed your Job situation as a result of the baby? (circle one) Yes no If yes, describe how. é. Are there regularly scheduled or usual times in your daily routine when you and your wife discuss things with each other? (circle one) Yes No If your answer was yes, when are those tines? 5. What is the average amount of time you and your wife spend in discussions each day? (check best answer) Lass than 15 minutes Between 15 minutes and 30 minutes Between JO minutes and 1 hour Between 1 and 2 hours More than 2 hours (Continue to next page) 159 to: post Z-husb 6. When you are faced with a decision, how often do you consult with your wife? All of the time ‘ . Some of the time Never Most of the time Occasionally 7. Please list any recent decisions you have made regarding your chiid. [All 111 Pleaae indicate the degree of your agreement or dieagreeeent with each of the following atatenente by circling the beat enewer according to the key below. III: SA - Strongly Agree d - dieagree a - agree SD - Strongly Dieagree U - Neither agree not disagree 1. SA a K d SD Housework ie the wouen'a.job. 2. SA a N d SD A hueband should feel obligated to help hie wite it he has (MU-IO. A '3..SA a I d SD Housework ahould be done before the huebend tonne hole. 4.34 a x a so TheMebandendwiieehouldahareindeoiaioneconoerning .eapenditurea ot noney. 5. SA. a N d '80 All in all. ly own perente did a good job raieing ae. 6. SA a N d SD All in all. ay wife'a parents did a good job raining her. 7. SA a N d so At the preaent tine my wife aeene generally pleaaed with our aexual relationahio. 8. SA a h d SD Adjusting to thin new baby in a difficult experience for ne. 9. SA a h .d SD Adjusting to thin new baby in a diificult experienee for ‘ Iy wile. ‘ . 10. SA a N d SD Alter toupleting thio questionnaire. lay wife and I are likely to diituea our anewere to it. I1.SA a N d SD Our friends and relativea think I as doing a good job raining our child. 12. SA a N d SD Our friends and relativea like our baby. (Continue to next page) 160 ID! poet Z-husb PART IV Generally at the present tine. how satisfied are you with each of the following? (check one answer for each itee) ‘ v... , 3...... Sonewhat Vary aetisfied satisfied dissatisfied dissatisfied a. You and your wife's decisions about work b. You and your wife's decisions about parenting c. You and your wife's decisions about honey d. 'You and your wife's decisions about leisure tine activities a. You and.your wife's _ sexual relationship f. You and your wife's conversations . . . g. The anount of tine you and . your wife spend in conversations h. Your present job situation 1. Your wife’s present job situation 1. Your participation in family decision making k. Your wife's participation in family decision making 1. Your wife's ability to be a good parent n. Your own ability to be a good parent 4 PAR! V INSTRUCTIONS: For the following items, please check the answer which best ‘ coepletns the sentence. . 1. As a result of our child, my wife and I now feel nuch closer to each other. feel somewhat closer to each other. feel sonewhat about the same degree of closeness as before the pregnancy. feel somewhat less close to each other. feel ouch less close to each other. 2. Compared with before our baby arrived. my wife and I now have nany more discussions than before. a few more discussions than before. ‘about the sane number of discussions. slightly fewer discussions than before. many fewer discussions than before. ID 161 I post Z-husb 3. A. 5. 6. 7. f. g. Compared with our feelings before the birth of our baby, my wife and I can now be described as ____much less satisfied with our present situation. slightly less satisfied with our present situation. ___.about as satisfied as we were before the pregnancy. _mors satisfied with our present situation. :much more satisfied with our present situation. As a result of this birth, *1 have assumed many different behaviors and tasks froe those I previously *performed in the family. I have assumed sole new behaviors and tasks within the family. I have kept the ease behaviors and tasks within the fanily. I have performed fewer of my usual behaviors and tasks within the fanily. I have stopped many routine family tasks and behaviors. Compared with how I felt before the birth of our baby. I now feel much sore unsure of my ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. somewhat less sure of ay ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. about the same amount of confidence in my ability to handle difficulties. somewhat more confident ofw ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. _auch more confident of my ability to handle difficulties than I felt before. Compared with how I felt before the birth. I now feel much happier. somewhat happier. about the same amount of happiness. somewhat less happy. much less happy. the other. one partner makes the final decision after discussion with the other. _ _both partners reach the decision together through discussion and compromise. :our decisions are not our own but are often forced on us by outside —circumstances. . PAR! VI A baby always causes some changes in the family. Check.the blank which best describes how much your baby has changed each of the following. A A moderate . Very' Hot at great deal amount Somewhat little all 'Your relationship with your wife Your life style Your leisure time activities Your sleeping and eating habits Your sexual relationship Your family finances Other (please specify): Continue to next page 162 ID 0 post l-husb 2. following is a partial list of new parents' tasks. Place an "S" in the blank which indicates how you thinkzeach of these tasks should be divided in your family. Place an "A" in the blank which indicates how this task actually ig_divided in your‘family. ,You will have two answers for each item. Husband ' "Husband Husband nu. nu. should should do and wife should do should always do more than should share more than always do wife equally ‘husband a. Change baby’s diapers b. Launder baby's clothes c. Quiet.baby when it an». d. Arrange for care for baby when wife must be away s. Get up in the night with baby 1. feed baby g. Play with baby b. Other tasks (please specify): 2A2: VII Yor the following questions, make an x on the percentage line in the place which best represents your answer or opinion. l. All things taken.into account. what percentage of the time have things been going well during the past month? (Mark the line below.) I - I I I I I ' / I I I «100: ~ 90: so: 70: 602 502 «or 30: 20: 102 oz 2. All things taken into account, what percentage of the time have you felt satis- {1:4 with your marriage relationship during the past month? (hark the line ow. " g I I I I I - I / ' I /' I 100: 90: 80: 70: so: 502 act 30: 20: . 101 oz 3. when all family decisions for the past month are taken into account, mark the line below to indicate what percentage of those decisions you agree with. I " I I' I I I j I I I I 1001 so: so: 70:: . 602 50?. «oz 30: 20: 101 oz Over 163 ID! post Z-husb PAR! VIII A. What method of feeding did you choose for your child? (Please check one and answer any questions which follow the answer you checked.) breast only How long was baby breastfed? Bottle only Combination of breast and bottle How long was breastfeeding used? How did you combine breast and bottle? 3. Of all the times you may want a babysitter. how easy is it for you to get one? (Circle a number.) 1 2 3 6 S Sitter Sitter Available Sitter Sitter never always available usually available about half usually not available the time available When you do get a babysitter, how confident do you feel about your sitters' ability to cars for your child? (Circle a number.) 1 ‘ 2 3 5 5 Extremely ' neutral Extremely un- confident in comfortable aitter's ability with aitter’s ability C. Following are four reasons that people may wish to have children. First, rank these items from I to 6. placing a 1 next to the reason that is most important to you personally, 2 next to the second must important reason, etc. Next rank these items as you think most people in this country would rank them. Your personal ranking How you think others would rank them A. An affection and liking for children; a concern for children; a desire to take care of ‘or nurture a child. 3. A feeling that it is part of the purpose or destiny of people, a desire to continue the human race. C. A desire to create someone in his own image; the child's ability to follow in his/her parents' footsteps or to reflect well on his parents. D. A child's ability to provide comfort or support for a parent, such as companionship, affection. cement for a marriage or help around the house. ID! 164 post 2 PAII II From birth. different babies have different ways of reacting to experiences. This section asks you to describe your baby' a style of reacting along several dimensions. Circle the number on the scale which best describes your baby. (There are no right or wrong answers: rather this is an attempt to describe the l. 3. temperament with which your child was born.) Activity level: Does your baby move around a lot. twist, squirm or kick while you dress him/her or does s/he lie quietly awake for long periods? Very active very quiet l _2 3 _ 4 ,' . ' 5' ' Regularity: Is your baby predictable with respect to eating. sleeping and bowel movements or is s/he unscheduled and erratic? . Very ~ very unscheduled Predictable 1 2 3 b 3 ’Adaptability to change in routine: low easily does your .baby adapt to changes in schedule or to new situations? Does it take many experiences with a new situation for your baby to adapt to it? Do you find yourself adapting your schedule to the baby or does your baby readily adjust to,a schedule which is convenient to you? Adapts very easily Experiences great die- to new situations comfort with new situs and schedules ' ' ations and schedules 1 . 2‘ ,3 6 S - Reaction to new experiences: How does your baby usually'react the first time s/he encounters new people. foods. situations, etc.? Does s/he act pleased and curious or tend to act somewhat frightened and shy away? (Bots: It is 'devulopmentally normal for a child to begin showing a fear of strange people . sometime between 6 S 12 months. If your baby has reached this stage, dis- regard his/her reactions to strange people when answering this question.) Very pleased S . Very easily frightened curious about a new in strange & new situation ' situationa' l 2 3 6 5 Level of sensitivity to sensations: Does your baby startle easily at loud noises? Become annoyed by bright lights, rough clothes, hot or cold tempera- tures? Does pain (a bump or scratch) bring a howl of discomfort or barely a whimper? Very sensitive Pays very little to sensations attention to sensations l 2 3 6 5 (Continue to next page) 165 ID! post 2 6. Positive or negative mood: Is your beby generally pleased and contented or frequently fussy and dissatisfied even after a nap or feeding? Does slhe'cry a lot upon awakening? Almost always .Almost always dis- contented satisfied 8 unhappy l 2 3 A 5 7. Intensity of response: when your baby expresses feelings. bow intensely does a/he do it? Does s/ha cry loudly or fairly quietly in comparison to other babies? when happy, does s/he squeal with excitement or simply- smile? How much energy does s/he seem.to have for eating, fussing and generally moving about? leacta very leects very mildly intensely l 2 3 b S 8. Distractibility: How easy is it to draw your child's attention away from an activity? When crying, can s/bo be easily distracted with.a toy or change of position? when eating..is s/ha easily distracted by-other activities? Very easily Persists in face of distracted. most distractions . l 2 3 A 5 9. Persistence and attention span: How long will your baby stick with an activity. even if difficult or interrupted? For example. the persistent child keeps trying to reach a toy,out of reach; the nonpersistont one tries only once or twice. ,A persistent child may keep fightins ¢8P0=1r ences s/he dislikes. like having his/her face washed while the nonpersis- tent child accepts without protest. A persistent child may return again and again to a forbidden activity, while the nonpersistsnt child may stop almost immediately. ' Extremely usually accepts persistent without protest l 2 3 6 3 ' APPENDIX J Interviewer Training II. 166 APPENDIX J Interviewer Training Introduction of interview outline to be used OLD?) C) PHD Discussion of each subject area Memorization of subject areas and subtopics Discussion of how to introduce self at beginning of interview Keeping a log of hours istening skills Paraphrasing 1. Definition of paraphrase 2. Demonstration of paraphrase 3. Practice of paraphrase (interviewers use each other to practice skill) Empathy 1. Definition of empathy 2. Demonstration of empathy 3. Practice of empathy and paraphrasing together Asking questions 1. Open-ended questions a. Definition of open—ended questions b. Purpose (to expand on topic) c. Creation of questions relevant to interview outline d. Practice (1) Each other (role plays) (2) Actual parent interview 2. Pursuit questions a. Definition of pursuit questions b. Purpose (to track down more specific inform- ation 0. Creation of pursuit-type questions relevant to questionnaire (1) Introduction of closed questions-— specifically the ones included as part of the interview (2) Pursuit questions aimed at answering the closed questions d. Practice (1) l-2 actual parent interviews listened to and critiqued by trainer and other interviewer (2) l-2 parent interviews to be completed in free time at home Transitions 1. What makes a good transition 2. What kinds of natural transitions may occur in this particular subject matter 3. What to do when transition isn't there (genera- tion of ways to smoothly introduce each topic) 167 n. Continued practice I. Begin recording answers a. For interviews done as practice at home, go over forms in class b. For interviews done in class, both inter- viewers fill out the interview form to check for reliability c. Practice continues to criterion reliablity F. Dealing with interviewee's questions (Total training time estimated at 15-20 classroom hours.) APPENDIX K Outline of Telephone Interviews 168 APPENDIX K Outline of Telephone Interviews Prebirth interview I. II. III. IV. VI. VII. VIII. Introduction A. Identification of interviewer B. Establishment of convenient interview time C. Investigation of privacy and degree of comfort of interviewee Reactions to pregnancy A. Discovery of pregnancy B. Interviewee's reaction C. Spouse's reaction D. Pregnancy planning Changes in life due to pregnancy A. How is being pregnant different from before pregnancy B. What have been changes with pregnancy C. How long taken to adjust to changes Support structure A. Who is contacted for problems or concerns B. How often contacted G. How comfortable does interviewee feel contacting resource Expectations of parenthood and coming baby A. What changes are anticipated B. What rewards are expected C. What difficulties are anticipated Communication with spouse A. Topics currently discussed .3. Differences between current topics and pre-pregnancy topics Other reactions or feelings Interviewer's assessment Degree of comfort for interviewee Degree of honesty of interviewee Inconsistencies Additional reactions to interviewee or interview process bow» 169 Postbirth interview--1 month I. II. III. IV. VI. Introduction A. Identification of interviewer B. Establishment of convenient interview time C. Investigation of privacy and degree of comfort of interviewee Labor and delivery A. Description of the labor and delivery B. Description of reaction at first sight of baby 1. Interviewee's reaction 2. Spouse's reaction C. Efficacy of classes in preparing for labor and delivery Baby's name A. When chosen .B. Why chosen C. Who was primarily responsible for choice The baby at home A. Feelings and reactions to having baby at home 1. How is it as eXpected 2. How is it different from eXpected B. Changes in family routine and roles 1. New activities or chores 2. Changes in old routines 3. Most difficult changes C. Return of family routine to a predictable pattern Marital relationship A. Effect of new baby on marital relationship 1. Changes 2. Positive aSpects 3. Negative aspects B. Effect of new baby on communication 1. How conversations are different from before birth , 2. How satisfied interviewee is with conversations and frequency Support sturcture A. Who is contacted for problems or concerns B. How often contacted C. How comfortable does interviewee feel contacting resource D Has there been contact with other prenatal classmates 170 VII. Xpectant parent classes feedback What stands out . What was most helpful . What would interviewee leave out . What additional reactions VIII. terviewer' 8 assessment Degree of comfort for interviewee Degree of honesty of interviewee Inconsistencies Additional reactions to interviewee or interview precess E A. B C D In A. B. C. D. Postbirth interview--1 year I. Introduction A. Identification of interviewer B. Establishment of convenient interview time C. Investigation of privacy and degree of comfort of interviewee II. Description of child A. Personality of child B. Characteristics which parent might like to change C. Special problems in growth or health D. Hopes for child III. Decision to have more children A. Does couple plan additional children B. What factors influence decision C. Who would be primarily responsible for decision IV. Changes in life due to baby A. Changes in lifestyle and relationships to other people Use of leisure . Attitudes toward job Relationships with friends Relationships with relatives Spontaneity Personal goals B. Age of child when household routine settled into a predictable and comfortable pattern C. Readjustment of marital relationship mmthr-A V. Relationship with spouse A. Ways in which baby has affected relationship B. Effects on sexual relationship C. View of spouse in parental role VI. VII. VIII. IX. 171 Attitude toward self as a parent A. View of self in parental role B. Effects of parenting on self-concept C. Strengths and weaknesses as a parent D. Relationship between parenting role and life goals Parenthood A. How is parenthood as expected B. How is parenthood different from eXpected C. What are rewards and costs Advice or thoughts for parents—to-be Reactions to participation in survey Interviewer's assessment . Degree of comfort for interviewee . Degree of honesty of interviewee Inconsistencies Additional reactions to interviewee and interview process UOtUID APPENDIX L Interscale Correlations for Original Communication, Decision-making and Marital Adjustment Subscales Before Creating Combined CDMA Scale 172 APPENDIX L Interscale Correlations for Original Communication, Decision-making and Marital Adjustment Subscales Before Creating Combined CDMA Scale Pre-birth observation (N = 66) MA DM Communication (COM) .93* .79* Marital Adjustment (MA)‘ .73* Decision-making (DM) 1-month observation (N = 93) MA DM COM .61* .69* MA , .56* l-year observation (N = 95) MA DM COM .66* .60* MA .72* * significant at p _<_ .001 APPENDIX M Inter Scale Correlations at Each Time of Observation 173 APPENDIX M Inter Scale Correlations at Each Time of Observation Pre-birth observation (N = 66) CRADJ SEXADJ CDMA — .35 .47 CRADJ* - .38 l-month observation (N = 93) CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR CDMA — .08 .44 .37 CRADJ* .09 - .19 SEXADJ .OO DISCR* l-year observation (N = 95) CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR ATTBA CDMA - .35 .43 .13 .20 CRADJ* - .33 - .10 - .39 SEXADJ .05 .33 DISCR* ' ' .17 * The reader is reminded that for these scales, low values are a positive score while high values are poor. KEY: CDMA - Communication/Decision-making/Marital Adjustment CRADJ - Crisis Adjustment SEXADJ - Sexual Adjustment DISCR - Discrepancy Between Should and Actual in Division of Tasks . ATTBA - Attitude Toward Baby APPENDIX N Item Content for Scales 174 APPENDIX N Item Content for Scales Communication/Decision-makinEZMarital Adjustment (CDMA) Scale' ITEM CONTENT (Questionnaire location of items is designated 10 11 12 13 in parentheses. See Appendix I, p. 139 for actual questionnaires.) Presence or absence of regularly scheduled or usual times in daily routine when husband and wife discuss things with each other (II 4) Average amount of time couple spends in discussions each day (II 5) Likelihood of consulting with spouse when faced with a decision (II 6) Belief that the husband and wife should share in decisions concerning eXpenditures of money (III 4) Degree of satisfaction with own and spouse's decisions about work (IV a) Degree of satisfaction with own and spouse's decisions about parenting (IV b) Degree of satisfaction with own and spouse's decisions about money (IV c) Degree of satisfaction with conversations with spouse (IV f) Degree of satisfaction with the amount of time Spent in conversations with Spouse (IV g) Degree of satisfaction with participation in family decision-making (IV j) Degree of satisfaction with spouse's participation in family decision-making (IV k) Degree of consulting between spouses when making family decisions (V 7) Percentage of time respondent has felt satisfied with the marriage relationship during the past month (VII 2) 175 14 Percentage of all family decisions during the past month with which respondent agrees (VII 3) 15 Total score for the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale (I) 176 APPENDIX N Item Content for Scales Crisis Adjustment (CRADJ) Scale ITEM CONTENT (Questionnaire location is designated in parentheses. See Appendix I, p. 139 for copies of questionnaires.) \) O‘xU‘t (rm Degree to which adjusting to pregnancy/birth has been a difficult eXperience for the respondent (III 8) Degree to which adjusting to pregnancy/birth has been a difficult eXperience for the spouse (III 9) Degree of change in relationship with spouse (VI 1 a) Degree Degree Degree Degree of of of of change change change change in life style (VI 1 b) in leisure time activities (VI 1 c) in sexual relationship (VI 1 e) in family finances (VI 1 f) 177 APPENDIX N Item Content for Scales Sexual Adjustment (SEXADJ) Scale ITEM CONTENT (Questionnaire location for each item is designated in parentheses. See Appendix I, p. 139, for copies of questionnaires.) 1 Degree of couple's agreement about sex relations (I 6) 2 Degree to which spouse seems generally pleased with' present sexual relationship (III 7) 3 Respondent's satisfaction with couple's sexual relationship (IV e) A Subscale score for Affective EXpression from the Spanier Dyadic Adjustment Scale (I) 178 APPENDIX N Item Content for Scales Discrepancy_Between Actual and Expected Division of Tasks (DISCR) Scale For each item, the difference was calculated between the respondent's view of who should do as opposed to who actually does the following tasks. ITEM CONTENT (Questionnaire location is designated in parentheses. See 1-month or 1-year question- naires in Appendix I, p. 139.) 1 Change baby's diapers (VI 2 a) 2 Quiet baby when it cries (VI 2 c) 3 Arrange for care for baby when wife must be away (VI 2 d) A Get up in night with baby (VI 2 e) 5 Feed baby (VI 2 f) 6 Play with baby (VI 2 g) 179 APPENDIX N Item Content for Scales Attitude Toward Baby (ATTBA) Scale ITEM CONTENT (Questionnaire location for item is designated 10 11 12 in parentheses. See l-year questionnaire in Appendix I, p. 139. Items 6-15 are from the 1-year interview.) Parental rating of baby's activity level (X 1) Parental rating of baby's regularity (X 2) Parzntal rating of baby's reaction to new eXperiences (X) Parental rating of baby's level of sensitivity to sensation (X 5) Parental rating of baby's positive or negative mood (X 6) Whether the couple has decided to have another child Number of costs of parenthood listed in response to an open—ended question Three-point rating of how rewarding parenthood has been for the respondent Three-point rating of how costly parenthood has been for the respondent Three-point rating of the degree to which a subject answered with fatalistic responses when asked open- ended questions about rewards and costs of parenthood Three-point rating of the degree to which a subject answered with instrumental responses when asked open-ended questions about rewards and cost of parenthood Three-point rating of the degree to which a subject answered with physical responses when asked open-ended questions about rewards and costs of parenthood 13 1A 15 180 Three-point rating of the degree to which a subject answered with economic responses when asked open-ended questions about rewards and costs of parenthood Three—point rating of whether parenthood has been easier than the parent expected. Three-point rating of whether parenthood has been harder than the parent eXpected. APPENDIX O Scale Items' Reliabilities and Correlations with Own and Other Scales 181 APPENDIX 0 Scale Items' Reliabilities and Correlations with Own and Other Scales Communication/Decision-making/Marital Adjustment (CDMA) Scale Pre-birth observation (N = 66) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ RELIABILITY 1 .35* -.10 -.16 .12 2 .27* .11 -.02 .08 3 .55** -.39* .41** .30 4 .29* -.10 .04 .08 5 .63** -.21 .38** .40 6 .44** —.18 .16 .19 7 .61** —.31* .34* .37 8 .56** -.22 .24 .31 9 .49** -.28* .23 .24 10 .72** -.20 .28* .52 11 .71** -.O9 .23 .51 12 .50** -.13 .16 .25 13 .57** -.17 .32* -32 14 .74** -.22 .25* .54 15 .74** -.33 .70** .55 * p é .05 ** p 9. .001 a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 171+. 1—month observation ITEMa O \O (I)\) O\U\ C'Kl) N H H H H H H Kn PK») N H *- *4!- a 182 ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE p £2.05 5:.001 Item content (N = 93) CMDA CRADJ SEXADJ .12 .09 .09 .32* .09 .01 .25* .01 33* .25* .27* 02 .60** .05 .39** .55** .24* .31* .59** .06 .09 .61** .22* .10 .71** .10 .13 .61** .03 .22 .66** .02 .28* .90** .09 .19 .62** .19 .43** .70** .00 .26* .70** .01 .57** DISCR .02 .23* .21* .00 .20 .21* .15 .27* .20 .35* .33* .27* .22* .30* ITEM RELIABILITY .02 .10 .06 .05 .36 .30 .35 .38 .50 .37 .AA .16 .39 .50 .A9 can be found in Appendix N, p. 174. 183 1-year observation (N = 95) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR ATTBA RELIABILITY 1 .09 .12 .05 -.14 —.07 .01 2 .32* -.12 .05 .19 -.09 .11 3 .30* .12 .05 .06 -.27* .09 4 .54** -.18 .14 .12 .02 .29 5 .52** -.19 .19 -.04 .15 .27 6 .65** -.19 .28* .10 .19 .42 7 .64** -.30* .34** -.O7 .29* .41 8 .68** -.24* .34** .04 .17 .46 9 .67** -.23* .27* .08 .14 .45 10 .70** -.23* .18 -.01 .20 .49 11 .66** —.O7 .20 .06 .15 .44 12 .30*- -.13 .10 .44** .05 .09 13 .37** -.26* .62** .15 .34* .14 14 .24* -.19 -.09 -.11 -.04 .06 15 .73** -.49** .45** .10 .27* .53 * p 5E.05 ** p s. .001 a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 174. 184 APPENDIX 0 Scale Items' Reliabilities and Correlations with Own and Other Scales Crisis Adjgstment (CRADJ)_Scale Pre-birth observation (N = 66) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ RELIABILITY 1 -.21 .39* -.24 .15 2 -.28* .50** -.17 .25 3 -.34* .57** —.27* .32 4 -.01 .78** -.22 .61 5 -.02 .62** —.22 .39 6 -.44** .65** -.38* .43 7 -.16 .74** -.14 .55 l-month observation (N = 93) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR RELIABILITY 1 -.16 .54** -.21* -.05 .29 2 -.17 .63** -.13 -.21* .40 3 -.14 .54** -.05 -.29* .30 4 .08 .64** .01 .04 .41 5 .11 .56** —.06 .06 .32 6 .07 .62** -.06 -.11 .39 7 ~.11 .46** .15 —.18 .21 * p 5.05 ** p s .001 a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 176. 185 ljyear observation (N = 95) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR ATTBA RELIABILITY 1 -.12 .51** -.18 —.14 —.41** .27 2 -.25* .52** -.23* -.22* -.46** .27 3 -.25* .47** -.12 .01 -.05 .22 4 —.07 .75** -.27* -.05 -.06 .56 5 -.15 .68** -.28* -.01 -.24* .46 6 -.26* .48** -.18 .05 -.23* .23 7 -.22* .30* .05 .00 .01 .09 * 2 5.05 ** 5:. .001 a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 176. APPENDIX 0 Scale Items' Reliabilities and Correlations with Own and Other Scales Sexual Adjustment (SEXADJ) Scale Pre-birth observation (N = 66) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ RELIABILITY 1 .27* -.28* .86** .73 2 .46** -.26* .66** .44 3 .29* -.35* .74** .55 4 .44** —.29* .84** .70 1-month observation (N = 93) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR RELIABILITY 1 .21* .07 .78** -.1O .60 2 .27* -.O5 .55** .12 .31 3 .40** -.20 .63** .03 .40 4 .29* -.05 .69** -.04 .48 * p éi.O5 ** p :é.OOl a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 177. 187 1-year observation (N = 95) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR ATTBA RELIABILITY 1 .29* -.19 .81** —.08 .18 .65 2 .26* -.28* .79** .08 .29* .62 3 .40** -.29* .75** .10 .36** .57 4 .43** -.27* .84** .06 .22* .71 * p 5.05 ** p é.- .001 a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 177. 188 APPENDIX 0 Scale Items' Reliabilities and Correlations with Own and Other Scales Discrepgncy in Task Division (DISCR) Scale 1-month observation (N = 93) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR RELIABILITY 1 .33* -.23 -.04 .77** .58 2 .24 -.18 -.04 .59** .35 3 .15 -.10 .36* ' .17 .03 4 .14 .03 -.21 .63** .40 5 .04 .00 -.13 .59** .35 6 .28* -.13 .07 .50** .25 1-year observation (N = 95) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR ATTBA RELIABILITY 1 -.01 .00 -.19 .56** -.O2 .31 2 .05 -.13 .10 .73** .16 .54 3 .11 .03 .13 .46** .21* .22 4 -.10 -.02 -.O7 .68** .15 .47 5 .04 -.09 .07 .70** .10 .49 6 .36** -.12 .14 .28* -.03 .08 * p 5;.05 ** p 5 .001 a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 178. 189 APPENDIX 0 Scale Items' Reliabilities and Correlations with Own and Other Scales Attitude Toward Bapy (ATTBA) Scale 1-year Observation (N = 95) ITEMa ITEM CORRELATION WITH EACH SCALE ITEM CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR ATTBA RELIABILITY 1 -.16 .09 .05 —.16 .17 .03 2 .01 -.13 -.08 —.19 .30* .09 3 .19 .12 -.09 .01 .24* .06 4 -.07 -.17 .07 .22* .12 .02 5 .08 .10 .06 .09 .17 .03 6 .08 —.29* .34** .35** .60** .35 7 .01 —.35** .29* .19 .39** .15 8 .08 -.19 .19 .05 .45** .20 9 .25* -.29* .30* -.09 .63** .40 10 .20 -.02 .20 .07 .28* .08 11 .11 .09 .17 -.02 .22* .05 12 .01 —.15 .18 —.06 .40** .10 13 .03 .04 -.08 -.02 .27* .07 14 .05 -.35* —.13 .19 .31* .10 15 .18 -.48** .22 .19 .56** .32 * 2 5;.05 ** p {1.001 a Item content can be found in Appendix N, p. 179. APPENDIX P Summary of Alpha Coefficients for each Scale at each Observation 190 APPENDIX P Summary Of Alpha Coefficients for each Scale at each Observation Pre-birth 1-month l-year Average CDMA .86 .84 .83 .84 CRADJ .80 .77 .73 .77 DISCRa .71 .74 .73 ATTBAa’b .66 .66 SEXADJ .86 .76 .87 .83 a Scale not administered pre-birth b Scale not administered at 1—month APPENDIX Q Correlations Between Husbands' and Wives' Scale Scores at each Time of Observation 191 APPENDIX Q Correlations Between Husbands' and Wives' Scale Scores at each Time of Observation Prebirth Observation (N = 33 couples) CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ CDMA .69** DRADJ .53** SEXADJ .61** 1-month observation (N = 38 couples) CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR CDMA .48** CRADJ .22 SEXADJ .45** DISCR .27* 1-year observation (N = 39 couples) HUSBANDS’ SCALES CDMA CRADJ SEXADJ DISCR ATTBA CDMA .49** CRADJ .28* SEXADJ .54** DISCR .31* ATTBA .36* * p 22.05 ** p .4;- .001 APPENDIX R Regression Analysis Summary Tables .mmH .Q :o pcsom on coo Pampcoo EOPH 21“ OMO. mam. mam.fi ONH.I HOO. :Nm. mwm. Oww. ONO. w Hes. San. 6R6.H H6H. 606. mam. R66. sR6. was. A Ans. RNA. 66m.fi skfi.- 666. man. :66. R66. can. 6 NMN. who. :mN.N mwfi.l HMO. Mam. mwm. NON. NHH.H m smm.- m6o. smm.m ooH.- 6H6. mam. Hmm. 66:. 6:6. 3 wwwdmm ..... amazmmm ..... mm ...... Maw ...... mm ..... 1.1mm ....... was ...... m:: man. smo. omm.a 6mm. oofi. mmm. :w:. 660. mm6.m m 6am. mso. mas.s 66m.- Ems. smfi. 66m. mso. mas.s A BOZ¢OHm m m muzOEmm mO Dammmezm O MHQEHm mmH3 m moanme andsssm mamzamcg Coammonwom m xHDzmmfl< 193 APPENDIX R Regression Analysis Summary Tables Wives' l-month Changg Score VARIABLE CONTENT (Questionnaire location is provided in parentheses. See Appendix I, p.139.for questionnaires.) 1 Wife's indication that she (as opposed to her husband) should be more responsible for arranging care for the baby when she must be away (VI 2 d) 2 Composite score for couple including wife's indication of percentage of time things have been going well, both parents' indications Of the per- centage of time each has felt satisfied with the marriage relationship recently, and each parent's indication of the percentage Of recent family decisions with which S/he has agreed (VII)* 3 Husband's indication of how likely he is to consult with his wife when making a decision (II 6) 4 Husband's combined score on two items indicating that Since the pregnancy he has felt happier and closer to his wife (V 1 & 6)** 5 Wife's level Of education (Background Information Form, Appendix C, p. 127) 6 Composite score for both husband and wife indicating the degree to which each eXpected the coming baby to change activities in their lives such as their relationship with each other, life style, sleeping and eating patterns, their sexual relationship and family finances (VI)*** 7 Wife's indication Of how likely She is to consult with her husband when making a decision (II 6) 8 Husband's indication Of the degree to which he eXpectS the coming baby to change his leisure time (VI 0) * Inter—item correlations range from .681 to .917. All are significant at p éi.001. ** Items correlate .683 which is significant at p s .001. *** Inter-item correlations range from .321 to .783. All but one are signigicant at p 5&.05. 194 .Aom .mmfi .Q So USSO% on coo pSopcoo SopH u zv npnflnpmom mmozna ow npnanonm Q Scum oadom Homepm3wo< OHOMEQ moacmmm an coconowwflo one mpcomonmon onoom owsmno m moo. 6mm. mam. omH. moo. 6mm. 6om. omo. Ram. m moo. 6R3. mum. 66s.- 6oo. :mm. son. H66. sun. A man. R6m. 66H.H mmm. oHo. mam. mos. 6o6. sum. 6 own. mum. mam.a 56m. smo. 6mm. moo. mas. mo6. m 66H.. Rom. 66m.H mam.- moo. :Hm. m6o. one. om6. s 66H.- mmn. moo.fi omo.- omo. moo. one. mom. moo. m 66H. mos. mm:.m mom. oso. N6H. mos. sow. oom.H m mam.- 66o. 6H6.m o:m.- NNH. mmfi. mom. 66o. 6H6.m H mozoon a m mozozmm mo ommmezm 69mm -Honm oaamm>o mqmsz amooom m m manoaaoz -Honm mmezm on a ameH onoom omcmzo mmohua .mo>fi3 .m madame anMSSSm mamzamc< scammonwom m xHszmm¢ VARIABL t 11 195 APPENDIX R Regression Analysis Summary Tables Wives' l-year Change Score CONTENT (Questionnaire location of items is pro- vided in parentheses. See Appendix I, p. 139, for questionnaires.) Wife's indication of how much change she expects the coming baby to cause in her relationship with her husband and their sexual relationship (VI a & e)* Composite score for couple including wife's indi- cation of percentage of time things have been going well, both parents' indications of the percentage of time each has felt satisfied with the marriage relationship recently, and each parent's indication of the percentage of recent family decisions with which s/he has agreed (VII)** Husband's indication of the average amount of time he Spends in conversations with his wife each day (II 5) Whether husband's employment has changed as a result of the pregnancy (II 2) Husband's indication of how likely he is to consult with her husband when making a decision (II 6) Wife's indication of how likely She is to consult with her husband when making a decision (II 6) Husband's indication of the degree to which he eXpects the coming baby to change his sexual relationship with his wife (VI e) Husband's indication Of how pleased he thinks his wife is with their present sexual relationship (III 7) * Items correlate .683 which is Significant at 2 as .001. ** Inter-item correlations range from .681 to .917. All are significant at p 42.001. 196 cpnfloomm Eonm manom p:oEem:o< omen»o hoacmmm EH .mma .m :o ossom on coo pCOPCOO SopH .Aom n my nonnnpmon cocoa-H on Q ooSOMOOMHo one mesomonmom monoom owsmno m on.- was. wmA.H o6o.- :oo. Hmm. mos. mHA. Ema. NH m6N.- 6mo. mno.fi mom.- oHo. mam. oos. HAS. man. no omm. vo. ssfl.m mmo.- moo. 2mm. omm. mmn. own. on Am”----mm-..--A.A.mm.:-mmm ..... “Am ...... Am”---:qm..mm ..... AAA”. ....... mm... ....... A-:- moo.- mmo. mmm.m on.- moo. Amm. 6mm. 6:6. Rum. m ASA.- mfio. Hom.m 63o.- moo. mam. omA. one. mm6. A mmH. moo. mmm.m son. AHo. mom. mos. mom. omm. 6 mom. moo. 336.: NNH.- Hmo. 6E3. moo. New. mms.s m o6m. moo. mmm.m Nmm. omo. 66:. 6A6. Non. Hom.m o mam. moo. mom.m son. Hmo. moo. Am6. mso. omm.m m oo6.- moo. oo:.6 oofi.- moo. mmm. oAm. moo. mHH.m m omm. moo. mAH.m Ame. mom. Rom. moo. moo. mAH.o A aozooHa a m mozo2mm mo nommmezm «mam -Honm. gao mnmsz mmozmm mo ommmezm 69mm -Honm aaamm>o mqozom mmooom m m maoHeooa -Honm mmezm on a zoom w omoom owcmno anemia .mocmnmsz mofipme homessm mmmzamcg :ommmonmom m xHszmm< 200 APPENDIX R Regression Analysis Summary Tables Husbands' 1-year Change Score VARIABLE CONTENT (Questionnaire location is provided in parentheses. See Appendix I, p. 139, for questionnaires.) 1 Composite score for both husband and wife indicating the extent to which each felt satis- fied with his/her own and the Spouse's partici- pation in family decision-making (IV j & k)* 2 Composite score for couple including wife's indication of percentage of time things have been going well, both parents' indications of the percentage of time each has felt satisfied with the marriage relationship recently, and each parent's indication of the percentage of recent family decisions with which S/he has agreed (VII)** 3 Wife's level of satisfaction with her own employ- ment situation (IV h) 4 Husband's indication of the extent to which he expects the coming baby to change his relationship with his wife (VI a) 5 Husband's indication of how pleased he thinks his wife is with their present sexual relationship (III 7) 6 Wife's combined score indicating how likely she is to consult with her husband when making a decision and how satisfied She has been with family decisions regarding work and parenting (II 6, IV a & b)*** * Inter-item correlations range from .662 to .832. All are significant at 2 £- .05. ** Inter-item correlations range from .681 to .917. All are significant at p éi.001. *** Inter-item correlations range from .337 to .462. All but one are significant at p £2.05.