IV " - " ""‘N ' " ””"T'T‘ " " “” '7 A , ”7-”. 7. 7 “’1 .......... PATFFRNING OF FAMILY RESOURCES FOR EDUCABILITY‘ , ', CONCEPTUALIZATION AND MEASUREMENT * IN COSTA RICAN FAN‘ILIES ~~~~~ MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY GEORGIANNE RUTH BAKER ‘,.. S I NIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 3129319 LIBRARY ,J MWS .Michigan State ‘ University This is to certify that the thesis entitled PATTERNING OF FAMILY RESOURCES FOR EDUCABILITY: CONCEPTUALIZATION AND MEASUREMENT IN_COSTA RICAN FAMILIES presented by 13r'~'~', Georgianne Ruth Baker has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. Family and Child Sciences /7 , p /7 Major professor degree in Date July 10. 1970 0-169 ABSTRACT PATTERNING OF FAMILY RESOURCES FOR EDUCABILITY: CONCEPTUALIZATION AND MEASUREMENT IN COSTA RICAN FAMILIES By Georgianne Ruth Baker The problem of the study was two-fold. First was refinement of a conceptual framework and measurement model for a managerial construct, family resource pat- terns, related to a developmental construct, educability of the preschool child. Family resource patterns were operationalized by nine resource categories: space, movement, care and appearance, play, task and work, child's learning, family learning, child's social contacts and family social contacts. Educability referred to the goal of having the child prepared for successful partici- pation in school. Second was investigation of hypo- thesized relationships between family resource patterns for educability and family status and structure charac- teristics, using a sample of 89 intact families with pre- school children in Costa Rica, Central America. For data collection in homes, original instruments were deve10ped including an Occupation and Income Survey, Georgianne Ruth Baker nine Resource Inventories, and nine sets of drawings of resource-related activities (involving preschool children and persons of their home environment) with focused ques- tions (Picture Questions Instrument). Sources of items for inventories and activities for drawings included those used in a previously constructed instrument, obser- vations in Costa Rican homes, consultation with inform- ants and researchers, and the pertinent literature. The Scoring system developed for the combined resource data provided numerical scores on four dimensions for each re- source category: Availability-Quantity, Availability- Quality, Use-Quantity, and Use-Quality. Dimension scores were transformed to comparable ratings, then summed to resource ratings, and finally to total ratings for sta- tistical and descriptive analyses. Major findings were the following: The managerial-developmental framework, originally conceptualized for, and applied in, another culture, demonstrated applicability in this cultural group of families. For scoring reliability, an approximation of 89 per cent consistency was found for 36 resource mea- sures, based on results of one-way analyses of variance for different scoring treatments on two randomly assigned groups. Evidence for construct validity included differ- ential meaningfulness for each of four resource -'~ “Irv“! J-.-enu)&4llu ’ re.aticns t fat-6A “fin ”'N‘V'v'l Shh». * L "2 1W" 4. . “It .1", Ella. , 21m coeffj :M‘ Ma \ a“! b“.‘I‘G “v. 52$ ‘ “ IQr Tr.‘¥ “Ayn“? A -‘V q ‘t. \"vrj’1 +- 3 Georgianne Ruth Baker dimensions, ascertained by examination of significant par- tial correlation coefficients with total resource ratings (TRR). The fourth dimension (UQL) seemed unique in com- parison with the other three because: first, its meaning was derived from the mother's verbalized perception of relations between current resource activities and future school success, based solely on the Picture Question Instrument; second, there was only one partial correla- tion coefficient with TRR significant at the .01 level; and third, the multiple correlation coefficient with TRR was smaller than those for the other three dimensions. The UQL dimension merits further study to decide whether it was poorly conceptualized, or irrelevant, in terms of the culture, or inadequately measured. Validation for the interdependent—interrelated as- pect of resource patterns was evident in the clustering of 33 of 36 intercorrelation coefficients of the resource measures, significant at the .01 level. Chi square analysis and contingency coefficients showed: first, a significant, substantial relationship existing between TRR and range of individual resource ratings, inter- preted as indicating that extent and consistency of re- sources available were related in the child's environment; second, that the level of family resources for educa- bility was significantly related to family status char- acteristics (education, income, residence), but not to In an 9x121: I 5" (I) H LJ. ; 1 p.» (‘4‘ (D Georgianne Ruth Baker family structure characteristics (nuclearity, size, age and sex of preschool children). There appeared to be potential for use of the conceptual-measurement system in descriptive family analy- sis, demonstrated by a profile of family resource ratings. In an exploration of concurrent validity, the predictive contribution of the construct family resource patterns was limited; however, no conclusion was warranted due to the empirical, exploratory nature of the problem. The study offered implications for theory of management in families, for cross-cultural research and action programs with families. PATH" PATTERNING OF FAMILY RESOURCES FOR EDUCABILITY: CONCEPTUALIZATION AND MEASUREMENT IN COSTA RICAN FAMILIES By Georgianne Ruth Baker A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Sciences 1970 /-. 9:1"? -- “7/ @DCopyright by GEORG IANNE RUTH BAKER 1970 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Especial appreciation is expressed to the members of my advisory committee: to Dr. Beatrice Paolucci, chairman, for her unique insight in guiding the study and whole- hearted support of its interdisciplinary and cross-cultural aspects, to Dr. Francis M. Magrabi, for her assistance in computer programming, and who, with Dr. Jean D. Schlater and Dr. Armin Grams, offered many valuable criticisms. Certain phases of the study were completed while working at the Center for Teaching and Research, Inter- american Institute of Agricultural Sciences (O.A.S.), Turrialba, Costa Rica. Gratitude is extended for the interest and support of the Institute. It is especially expressed to Dr. Linda J. Nelson, colleague at the Insti- tute, and to Mrs. Maria Eugenia D. Vargas of the Uni- versity of Costa Rica, both of whom offered helpful advice and encouragement. For technical help the efforts of Mrs. Inés S. Santisteban and Mrs. Barbara B. Zfifiiga are appreciated. Gratitude is extended also to the Institute of Nutrition of Central America and Panama, and to the Costa Rican Ministry of Public Health for assistance in obtain- ing the study sample. It was a privilege to collect data 11 in the homes of the participating families and I am grate- ful for their contribution. To Dr. Robert D. Hess and Dr. Virginia C. Shipman are extended my appreciation for the opportunity to work under their stimulating guidance at the University of Chicago as part of my graduate study. Gratitude is also expressed for financial assistance during graduate study from the General Foods Corporation, the American Home Economics Association, and at Michigan State University--alumni of the College of Human Ecology, the Graduate School, and Office of International Programs. Finally, I am thankful to family and friends for their generous interest in the research reported here. 111 TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. . . . . LIST OF LIST OF LIST OF Chapter I. II. III. IV. TABLES . . . . . FIGURES O O O O C APPENDICES. . . INTRODUCTION . . Objectives . . Assumptions. . Hypotheses . . Definitions. . REVIEW OF LITERATURE Management in the Family Resources: Management . . Family as Environment PROCEDURE . . . . Fundamental Concept Selection and Description of Sample. ”Selection and Description of Instruments Data Collection . Data Analysis . FINDINGS . . . . Method of Measurement Dimensions of Resourcefulness. Interrelated Patterning of Resources Hypothesis 1 . . Hypothesis 2 . . Hypothesis 3 . . Relations of Individual Resources to Status and Structure Descriptive Use of Family Resource Patterns . . . iv Page ii vi viii ix «cunt» l—' 12 12 2L: 1‘7 60 6O 75 92 98 102 102 , lOA//’ 107 107 110 112 112 127 Chapter LITERATUR APPENDICES Chapter Page V. DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS . 13H Discussion of Findings. . . . . . . 134 Limitations . . . . . . . . . . 146 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . 1A7 Implications . . . . . . . . . . 150 LITERATURE CITED . . . . . . . . . . . . 153 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I6“ Table 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. LIST OF TABLES Summary definitions of dimensions by resource categories . . . . . . . Correlation of home resources factor with mother's behaviors and child's performance by working class group and sex of pre- school child . . . . . . . . . Status characteristics of sample (N = 89) . Family characteristics of sample (N a 89) . Characteristics of preschool children in sample (N = 89) . . . . . . . . . Content of pictures by order of presentation and relevant resource category . . . . Typology of structure of ratings: Resource patterns for educability . . . . . . Methods used in analysis of data . . . Summary of significant differences between resource scores in two scoring situations. Partial and multiple correlations for scores of each of four resource dimensions with total resource rating . . . . . . . Intercorrelation matrix for nine resource ratings . . . . . . . . . . . Relation between level of total resource rating and range of individual resource ratings . . . . . . . . . . . . Relations between level of total resource rating and selected family characteristics Relations between space level and selected family characteristics . . . . . . . vi Page 10 56 7O 73 76 86 93 100 103 105 108 109 111 114 Table Page 15. Relation between child's movement level and father's educational status . . . . . 116 16. Relations between care and appearance level and selected family characteristics. . . 118 17. Relations between child's play level and selected family characteristics . . . . 119 18. Relation between child's task and work level and mother's educational status . . . . 121 19. Relations between level of child's learning opportunities and selected family characteristics . . . . . . . . . 122 20. Relations between level of family learning opportunities and selected family characteristics . . . . . . . . . 12A 21. Relations between level of child's contacts and selected family characteristics. . . 126 22. Relations between level of family contacts and selected family characteristics. . . 128 23. Procedure for first transformation and illustrations: from Resource scores to ratings . . . . . . . . . . . . 250 24. Procedure for second transformation and illustration using scores from one family: from Resource ratings to levels of relation to educability. . . . . . . 251 25. Basic statistics for resource dimension scores . . . . . . . . . . . . 257 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. COnceptual framework and measurement model for study of family resource patterns for educability of preschool child, and relations to family characteristics. . . A 2. I.N.C.A.P. survey and study communities in the Meseta Central of Costa Rica. . . . 68 3. A family profile: pattern of variability in resources for education. . . . . . . 129 viii LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix Page - A. Instruments. . . . . . . . . . . 165 B. Scoring Manual. . . . . . . . . . 217 C. Introductory Materials . . . . . . . 252 D. Table on Basic Statistics . . . . . . 256 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Organizing resources for goal achievement is a pro- cess examined and discussed in home management as well as in many social science areas concerned with education, change and deveIOpment. This capacity for arranging means to meet ends, when it operates within the family where the "ends" sought affect individual growth and potential, may play a significant intervening role in societal change. Hagan suggests that, viewed over long periods of time, families in their managerial-developmental capacities may add impulse or impediment to national social and economic goals (1). For this reason there is need for increased scientific understanding, prediction, and finally application of knowledge about resource organization at the family level in both developing countries and among subcultural groups in industrialized countries. Home management focuses on how families manage, that is, organize or pattern, resources to mediate their specific goals and values. In this study this focus is oriented to an area of wide concern, that of family environmental influences upon children's successful par- ticipation in the educational system. The study follows current emphases in home manage- ment pointed out by Paolucci and others: concept defini- tion, structural relationships and identification of behavioral and environmental variables related to family decision-making situations (2, 3, A, 5). The theoretic orientation combines a managerial and a developmental point of view in support of the philosophy that optimal human development should be the criterion for family management, and that individual members actively partic— ipate in family managerial efforts (6, 7). When the family environment is unsupportive of human development, imaginative solutions may be required. However, in order to suggest action, there is need for theoretic and analyti— cal orientations that help structure family observations and guide inter-family comparisons. This study explores such an orientation. Problems of conceptual and measurement equivalence, as Straus has discussed them, are considered and a method of measurement proposed as a procedure which may index the construct "family resource patterns for educability" in various cultural groups or societies, although phe- nomenal identity (through use of the same questions and items as well as method of quantification) may not be completely possible (8). Obiectives First, the study attempts to construct a conceptual framework and method of measurement and, second, explores relationships between family resource measures and selected family characteristics (see Figure l on the following page). Families rating high on resource pat- terns for educability are compared with those rating at middle and low levels. Specifically, answers are sought to the following questions: 1. Beginning from a managerial-developmental orientation, what generalized properties of family environment indicative of high educative capacity might one specify and attempt to operationalize? Can conceptual and measurement equivalence of family resource patterns be approached byfirst develOping operational definitions from data and then comparable measures through a procedure of transforming heterogeneous scores to standardized ratings? What evidence can be found for both distinctive dimensions and interrelated patterning of re- sources? What relation is there between how a family rates on a total resource measure and the variability in ratings it exhibits? EDUCABILITY Capacities of Child Educative Capacities of Environment Objectives: To construct conceptual framework and measurement model for relating family resource patterns to educability. FAMILY RESOURCE PATTERNS To relate family resource patterns to family char- j Family Contacts acteristics. Physical Constriction Mental Stimulation Resourcefulness ‘1. _ FAMILY CHARACTERISTICS Social Interaction RESOURCE RESOURCE QM no I CATEGORIES DIMENSIONS “iATVQ STRUCTLRE Space Nuclear- Child's Movement Availability- Education Extended Care-Appearance Quantity Occupation Family Size Child's Play Definitions Income No. of Sib- Child's Task-Work Availability- Residence lings !Child's Learning Quality Preschool Family Learning Child: QChild's Contacts Use-Quantity Age & Sex I I I Use-Quality V Scores l RATINGS Measures ‘7 ‘P 17 GROUP FREQUENCIES Figure 1.--Conceptual framework and measurement model for study of family resource patterns for educability of preschool child, and relations to family characteristics. Do family resource patterns differ significantly with respect to certain status characteristics? Do family resource patterns differ significantly with respect to certain structural characteris- tics? What possibilities are there for descriptive and predictive use of the resource measures developed in the study? Answers to the first three questions are explored through development of the conceptual framework and measurement model. The next three questions are explored by means of tests of hypotheses, and the last question by two additional analyses: a family profile using the framework of the study, and a regression problem using the resource and status measures as predictors to a criterion measure of family educational success. Assumptions Families in all cultures and at all status levels may organize available resources in ways that promote (or interfere with) the goal of the deveIOpment of potential capacities of their members. These organizational activities and resources present possibilities of observation and measurement. Among Costa Rican families, educability is a relevant motivating goal for resource organi— zation. Mothers of preschool children are the persons most familiar with activities involving the children and can give accurate reports of such activities by means of enumeration, description and response to visual stimuli. Hypotheses Families at three resource levels (low, middle and high with respect to educability) will differ significantly in range of resource rat- ings. The proportion of families at three resource levels (low, middle and high with respect to educability) differs significantly by certain status characteristics: education of parents, occupation, income and residence. The proportion of families at three resource levels (low, middle and high with respect to educability) differs significantly by certain family structure characteristics: nuclearity, size, number of siblings, sex and age of pre- school child. Definitions 1. Educability. The concept as explicated by Hess and his research group (9, 10, ll) refers to preparation of the preschool child to participate in later school learning activities. Educability is a dynamic concept: it may represent not only the young child's performance and abilities, but also certain educative capacities of his environment as inferred from family characteristics, or from a combination of properties which seem to have special developmental importance at the preschool age (12, l3, 1“). Also, educability may be viewed as a value and goal which depends upon the property of family resource- fulness for attainment. This study, then, focuses upon environmental as- pects of educability, and considers it to be a family goal mediated through family resource patterns. 2. Properties of special developmental importance for theppreschool child. Physical-spatial constriction .refers to crowding, movement levels and physical routines which might interfere with either learning and attention Or activity and manipulative experiences. Cognitive stimulation refers to preparatory experiences and skills for later learning, such as problem solving tasks, dramatic play, culturally relevant knowledge, adult guidance, and Opportunities for listening, remembering, and copying. In the social-emotional or interpersonal realm, interaction refers to general non-cognitive ex- periences and exchanges which provide the child with opportunities for attention, feedback and encouragement from adults, older children and age-mates. 3. Resources. These are objects, events, activi- ties or human beings within the child's environment that are available and that might function to influence educa- bility. Operationally, resources are those present in the data for this sample of families, and which are sum- marized by means of nine resource categories and the above-mentioned properties of developmental importance: Resource Categories Properties Space, Child's Movement, Care and Constriction Appearance Child's Play, Child's Task and Work, Stimulation Child's Learning, Family Learning Child's Contacts, Family Contacts Interaction A. Resourcefulness. This managerial property .represents a combination of dimensions considered in arriving at resource measures. The following typology indicates how the dimensions are combined: Resourcefulness Dimensions Quantity Quality Availability AQN AQL Use UQN UQL y 01‘ 1‘. .A-A“ 1" .rc ,2 IA 4.. L. .3; '1, even I.'. n4 Aid .icted a -§ 3 nay .... . H Irv-AA Ce v 01‘ P9 Q _ ”VI?! .1.“ 'J -.-" “H ... orf~ HA hrs. D» A\H he V In.“ ‘A I'Wn '4‘. Q » cat . t“. - Vv Pd 9» 5.: .1 Kb. Availability-Quantipy refers to resource amounts actually or potentially accessible to the child and/or family. "Potentially available" indicates that the mother verbalizes (in response to questions about Picture stimuli) an awareness that certain objects, activities, or persons could be considered resources in a specific situa- tion, even though she may not list them as presently accessible to the child or family in her Inventory re- sponses. Availabilipy-Quality indicates particular, re- stricted and pervasive variety by which available re- sources may be described, categorized or delimited. Use-Quantity refers to the amounts of actual, cus- tomary or regular, or expected involvement activities of child or family with resources. Use-Quality indicates mother's verbalized perception of relations (or non-relations) between each resource category and the child's future participation (successful or unsuccessful) in school activities (as indicated by responses to Picture stimuli). Definitions of the four dimensions as they relate to resource categories in this study are summarized in Table 1, page 10. 5. Family resource patterns for educability. Ap- plied to the family as environment, this analytical 77;: . u . I 1 .I. ..- .l- 1.... .autlu a —;\.4:Iu...r\.- ‘(C—an «2...: a...,»....w 2:.23 .CIZ >.-~...Z,}I.....*~7...; ~1>< .A.._~ SCSINJIR...~H_-..-:>< 1.7:.3211 .11....(‘1 .3... .s...~...._...~.. >... ...:..cu..:...:u—. L... ~..:.._ euzrulf >,.-.. :2.:.....|| ~ .1. sv~ Huom Hmfioom can mpomp mofiufi>fipom HmfiOOm cam aafiemm mmfiufi>auom muodpcoo Icoo m.»afiemu mo cowumflom mpompcoo Hmsm: m.>HHEmm on» cfinufiz cogoucmo Hmfioom cam muoducoo AHHEmm wmooosm Hoonom change 0» mofipfi>fiuom cam wuomp wmflufi>fiuom Hmwoom zHHEmu mmfipfi>fipom wuowpcoo Icoo m.cflfino ho cofiumamm new wuompcoo Hmswz m.ofifizo on» Cacufiz cmpmpcoo HmfioOm can mucoucoo m.vHHco mmmoozm Hoocom onsusu m.p~fino mmwpfi>fiuom on mmfiufi>fipom wcficpmoa mcfiCLmoH CH coaumq mcficsmoa SHHEmd wcficpmoa Lou wcficnmoq m.>H«Emu no cofipmaom IHoHuLmd Hmzw: m.>HfiEmm do mmmcnoumHmnIHoocom onuH>Hpow cam muoonno SHHEmm mmooosm Hoocom manage ou mcfipfi>fipom mcficnmma CH mcficnmoa m.cHH:o wcficpmoa MCHcsmoq Icnwofi m.uHHco co cofiumamm cofiumqfiofiusmq Hmsm: m.pafico CH mfifismm do ucwso>ao>cH Log mmfiufi>fipom new mpoonno m.cafico . mmmoosw xnoz Hoozow opsusa o» xnos cam xnoz use mxmmu CH cofiumo xcoz ecm meme» m.vfifico xpos new mxmmu cam mxmme mxmmu m.nHHno no cofiumaom Ifiofipnmd Hmsm: m.naficu CH mawemd do pcceo>ao>cH HmCOmLoq Log mofipfiafinfimmOm m.cHH£o mmooosm Hoocom onzuzu mofipfi>aoom swam 0» swag m.eHHco co :ofiumfimm swag Hana: m.eafico new massage co zsmfitm> mmHSH>fipom new muomwpo Amfii w.eHHso mmooosm Hoonom mpzuzp o» mofipa>fiuom cocmnmoqam new :ofiudssmcoo mood new mocmcmoaom new came mocmnmmqo< opmo m.cafico uo cofiumaom tcficuoHo Hash: m.cHHLo :H ocfipson do maficmfizmmm mEmpH wcfizp0Ho cam ooom can undo meUOSm Hoocom mpfi::EEoo Lo change on ucoem>os mucmsoao Scoso>oe wcfi caocomzoc oh .coocconcmfioc .caonomzo: :H acmEo>oz m.cafico no coauwamm I>Ho>CH mcofiuom m.nHH:o mucosmao do :oHSOfispwmm mpcmsoflo copmfimpupcmeo>oz m.uHHno mmooosm Hoocom caoccmzo: cfi muoonno opspzu o» mofipfi>fioom new moomom wcfi>ao>CH mucoEmHm mcomsmq Op cofipmHop m.cH«£o do cowumflom mmfiufi>fiuom Hmzm: m.cHH:o mcfimzoc 2H mumfipm> :H moomom caocmmso: comqm Agape Azeav Agg< asascmsouspfiflfipmafim>< moszommm “INK. l .l I, «mmfipowoumo oossommp an mcoflwcmefic no mcofipfichoc ancessmlu.a mqm<9 «we: :‘icusly deff i;eraticna1] :5;3 ate res :iiiarized i 11 construct summarizes resource organization based upon pre- viously defined properties, resource categories and dimen- sions, and variability in presumed relation to educability. Operationally, family resource patterns are measured by separate resource scores transformed into ratings, then summarized into high, middle and low levels of relation to educability. The lit general head af'mdatental rent. Both ~resented be: instructs a ‘ on.n~ ”:53 I CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE The literature is reviewed under the following general headings: management in the family, resources as a fundamental concept in management, and family as envir— onment. Both theory-oriented and research literature are presented because both are pertinent to this study which constructs a theory-related framework for research pur- poses. Management in the Family A theoretical model of the managerial process, of propositions more or less accepted within the field of home management, and with some basis in empirical obser— vation, has been summarized by Paolucci. This framework: . . . assumes that management is a particular kind of behavior (managerial) consisting of some basic processes that relate to each other in discernable patterns. The components of this framework in- clude the following processes: (1) formulating and selecting home—centered goals; (2) recogniz- ing within the situation the presence of an op- portunity or problem, and (3) achieving a goal by specifying and analyzing feasible alternative means, choosing the means to the goal, carrying out these means, and guiding the action toward the goal (2:339)- 12 13 One of several important ideas, discussed in rela- tion to the framework, is that ". . . if a particular goal is to be managed, it must be conscious" (2:3A0). This idea of awareness, or of a perception which could be verbalized, is found repeatedly in the work of others in the management field. Another idea pertains to assessment of management: its effectiveness can be measured by the degree to which desired family activities and relationships are attained. To provide the knowledge needed for esti- mation of the consequences of particular activities for specific ends, measurement of means and ends is required. Lastly, Paolucci stresses the ". . . interplay and significance of a single family's private management on other families and social units such as schools, corpora- tions, government" (2:3A2). This view of management suggests that the framework is not solely oriented to the family's internal life, but that at least equally signifi- cant are the consequences of family managerial activities for the larger society. Liston has defined management similarly to Paolucci, as a cognitive process involving perception, of making resources productive to accomplish a family's discretely selected goals. In a dynamic formulation, management is viewed in the context of social process, originating in change and resulting in change: ‘0" ("f T (D .. b—q) 0) *nt for iruolve 5 Scarce Fe to Paint resources . . . appl vities in may also 1 . . intI (6: ESI 23.x gPOwtu emhtnzents s “1 states 5 is productivit tie- productive Listen has ct s .... mana e qualit .en cha JCCCUP not relat 1 v‘ “.53 °~~AEty Tl kt. ‘ ‘~*enced b lu Perception of . . . internal or external change by the family may bring awareness of a problematic gap between the current situation and what is deemed necessary or desirable--a gap which calls for some form of adjustment. This gap may be per- ceived mainly as one involving . . . goal priori- ties or . . . kinds of resources considered rele- . vant for the situation. The problematic gap may involve special attention to . . . reallocation of scarce resources among alternative uses in order to maintain or to improve productivity of these resources. It may be concerned primarily with . . . appraisal and reorganization . . . of acti- vities involved in resource use. Or, finally, it may also be a problem of one or more forms of . . . interaction among family members . (6:65). H Taking a developmental stance, Liston and Paolucci see management in the family as creating situations to foster growth and maturation of its members, setting up environments so that its members may move from less de- sired states such as poverty to more desired states such as productivity. Looking upon management as related to the productive functioning of the family in society, Liston has observed that: . . . management by the family is much involved in the quality of give and take between the family and the other subsystems of its social environ- ment--economic, political, community, cultural, and physical (6:66). When changes occur in the family's environment, they occur not only within the family, but also in the family's relationships to the needs of other subsystems 0f society. The family exerts influence upon, and is influenced by, the larger society. Liston believes that: Too litt and pate in the f Liiited frat: :anagezent. tasic to her as; «Vn or in“ :4 sv‘vnzl Uni: Q .I b 1 Odnaed .r‘H 15 Too little attention has been given to the actual and potential impact of the quality of management in the family on various aspects of the general welfare of our nation (6:61). For research purposes, Deacon suggests a more limited framework, emphasizing functional aspects of home management. The rationale for home economics proposed as basic to her conceptualization is: . . . to study and interpret for families and for the larger society the interrelatedness and al- ternative possibilities for effective use of the available human and material resources in meeting needs (15:760). Viewed within this context home management concentrates upon ". . . the means by which the resources of indi- viduals and families may be measured and managed purpose- fully," while other areas in home economics deal with nurture and personal development (15:760). In this framework attention is given to understanding decision- making processes by which resources are channelled to reach goals, and to values and goals as the motivating forces for effective management and bases for evaluation. In a later conceptualization, Deacon and Maloch utilize a systems approach to focus on regulation or con- trol aspects of management, presenting an organized col- lection of interrelated elements which have a boundary and functional unity (16). The family is viewed as a social system bounded by its membership, values and resources. In this system, inputs are demands (the goals and events rquiring ac fineness of afthe relat ‘I a {at make "“hzing he ~eies (su l6 requiring action), and outputs are resource-use. Effec- tiveness of management is to be indicated by examination of the relation of output to input within the system. Functional unity of the system is provided by management (by which goals are responded to through use of resources). Internal managerial components of the system include planning, controlling, evaluation and decision- making. Planning includes the ability to perceive, or to foresee future needs or events, as well as objective ap- praisal of related current situations. Schlater has suggested reducing the managerial pro- cess, or what may be viewed as Deacon's internal mana- gerial components, to two major subprocesses: decision- making (linking values, alternative perception and selection) and decision-implementing (synonomous with organizing, and requiring non-mental activity as well as mental activity) (A). In viewing decision-implementing as organizing, Bell's definition is helpful. Organiza- tion is: . . . the ordering of family life brought about by balancing means, techniques, and activities that are important to the family and to individuals that make up the family (17:65). Organizing means coordinating, integrating or arranging strategies (such as planning, goal-setting, appraising, adjusting) and resources into patterns appropriate to the I 1 I . _‘ .L-o-b .rn‘ tI and “1 1”,"; Val; circular Upon it choice 0 attrOpriI an stra‘ gies ava: chosen. interdepe and yet c :ilprealcts 'I-n‘ \ \ y'vu en and ti :ganize its . '.r. In rese 17 family's value system. Bell notes the interrelatedness and circularity between family values and organization: Upon it [value system of the family] rests the choice of appropriate pattern of family life. The appropriate pattern will help determine resources and strategies to be used. Resources and strate- gies available may help determine the pattern chosen. . . . So, we have interrelationships and interdependencies. . . . One part leading into, and yet dependent upon, every other part (17:65). Bell predicts that a family which is aware of its value system and the resources and strategies at its command can organize its life pattern effectively. In research related to organizational patterns Walker reports a pilot study of a proposed classification for self-imposed standards for household tasks (18). More than one component of a task was considered and a scheme for relating standards to components was con- structed. She says: In the past, analysis of household work has focused upon a certain aspect of the task, hold— ing other aspects as constant. The concept of self-imposed standards attempts to identify the many components of a task that influence the outcome and to study the way in which these com- ponents are combined. . . . . . . a study of standards may be a means of helping . . . evaluate the manner in which they [homemakers] allocate their resources to house- hold work . . . [and] recognize possibilities for change (l8zu60). Here, "self-imposed" means that the person estab- lishes the degree to which he will seek to incorporate standards into his efforts. Standards are explicit cri- teria which support evaluation of the extent of goal :sievenent flfiffi‘pp w-wn aspe: tynappears . N.‘ A ‘-:“H ' C ,.e to wk In a f 321,151. V8111 flit-ems 0f FfiiECtS a I. .‘ r “I; 1 ccrlnp, .2 «a ‘ “an of l8 achievement: they are considered with respect to either the means of attainment (resources) or the accomplishment itself. The number of possible standards can be reduced by placing all tasks into three groups which differ in their major contribution to goal achievement. A pattern for a task, according to Walker, is a per- son's individualistic approach. Underlying pattern is a common thread, or similarities, in groups of tasks. These common aspects or dimensions, when combined in some way, lead to a standard for that group of tasks. "Individual— ity" appears in how one orders these dimensions, and the degree to which one strives to achieve each (18zu56). In a factor-analytic study, Mumaw examined a par- ticular value predisposition in relation to organizational patterns of families (19). She assumed that organization reflects a person's value of order (order need). Follow- ing a conceptualization by Nichols (5), organization was regarded in this study as a pattern, or structure of re- lationships, which results from design and implementation of plans for task completion at either the individual or group level of effort. In order to identify organizational patterns of each person, Mumaw factor-analyzed their responses to an activities index, ending with four princi- pal factors. Individuals were classified low, moderate or high on each factor.' The researcher observed forty-six different patterns of organizational components of task These .»r‘ _‘.u I e yr 8‘ OJ. a‘fla . :«t . .-wd of er than t v 5- ‘ ~v- war». tezperscns » .a vurxv“ :‘nc' ‘l' P u “ .egulariz I . ‘V‘V ‘¢ ion t TIA-u! ‘V PV~4t L . In“ ‘ I In is! V '68 par CDC 1 A‘Dro \ I h; “Cpk, a1 V l9 standardization, assignment, regularization and arrange- ment. These patterns were then examined by situational characteristics and order need (as measured by a check list of adjectives designed to serve as an index of the strength of the value). Differences in organizational patterns were ex- plained by a difference in value orientation. Low order need persons tended to be oriented to creativity in task organization, to be less conservative and more urbanized; high order need persons tended to be more conservative and rural. The high order need people scored significantly higher than the other group on the task standardization and regularization factors, both of which involved the person in his own activities, not with other persons. Mumaw suggests that this finding indicates that the pre- disposition towards order may be impeded when organizing involves participation of others in completion of tasks (19:3). Likert has presented what he terms a newer theory of management based upon industrial research and which has isolated characteristics and organizational patterns of "high-producing" managers (20). Productivity, or perform— ance, was measured in various ways including time stand- ards met, job satisfactions, turn-over, costs and losses. His work, along with some thoughts of Fromm's which . ~ in :acer‘JAQe a —nb:‘v'~+.r\ P .mvl I‘dvllund 5' f 73 me fun: tr A .. .He-ry. tI] Pecole wor . ‘fi EPOUp 4_t:V-lt 20 follow, summarize some central concerns apparent in the thinking and research interests of home management. One of Likert's findings is that high producers supervise according to a different philosophy and set of motivational assumptions. They avoid what he describes as the fundamental deficiency of traditional management theory: the inadequate motivational assumption that people work only for economic goals, and that a manager controls those under him who in turn have the duty to obey (20:59-60). The role of the work group is found to be of central importance. This is the group in which a person spends much time, the one in which the individual is most eager to achieve and maintain a sense of personal worth. The greater the skill of the person in the manager role in using group methods of supervision, the greater are pro- ductivity and satisfactions of the group. For example, findings indicate that freedom or independence in doing one's work or in making decisions leads to high perform- ance only when there is much interaction between the individual, his peers, and superior (20:2A). In addition, the power of group goals is revealed in that they can push production down or up, depending upon the level of goals set by the group. Evidence of the in- fluence of the group is further shown by the small devia- tions by members from goals set by the group Q’O:A3). gerforxance gressures (2 Favors} 7:313 must he Gary/2..“ r..-uu.t1't'1t:}' . t‘ha .. l‘ I: iOI‘CQ a C s 'cvl’u'ES Ii}! “ .EI‘t E ‘HALs 21 Quantity and quality of productivity correspond to the re- sponsibility felt by the group. Thus, the high-producing managers make widespread use of participation, whereas traditional managers get compliance with their established performance goals by use of hierarchical and economic pressures (20:100). Favorable attitudes as well as high performance goals must be present if organization is to achieve high productivity. Those managers whose pattern of leadership yields favorable attitudes think of those they supervise as human beings rather than just as workers. Characteris- tic of their management is a highly coordinated, highly motivated, COOperative social system. The different motivational forces in each person have coalesced into a strong force aimed at accomplishing mutually established objectives. Likert summarizes many findings by stating an inte— grative principle of supportive relationships in which the individual is central: The leadership and other processes of the organi- zation must be such as to ensure a maximum prob- ability that in all interactions and all rela- tionships with the organization each member will, in the light of his background, values, and expec- tations, view the experience as supportive and one which builds and maintains his sense of per- sonal worth and importance (20:103). Fromm has formulated a basic principle of humanistic management, a principle which is recognized in the views :f‘ Likert a1 :elieves tit; negate the I to cons‘ Hal It: r,‘ r. Viuc group w: .e ECIFW m. 29 fr: 2.“ -““ . vu CE» ‘3’», '~..lfl .3 t,‘ vSt iftpr . -I~ “file re f1? 22 of Likert and the preceding writers, although with varying emphases: that the individual asserts himself and takes an active part in the managerial process (7:105). He believes that the principles of the technological society negate the humanist tradition; for example: Another general practice in organizing work is to constantly remove elements of creativity (in— volving an element of risk or uncertainty) and group work by dividing and subdividing tasks to the point where no judgment or inter—personal contact remains or is required (7:36). He considers the present system (what Likert has called the traditional management orientation would be part of this) to be pathogenic and believes that the principle of maximum efficiency can be given up, if only for a time: "That is to say, man, not technique, must become the ul- timate source of values; optimal human development and not maximal production the criterion for all planning" (7:100). It would not be necessary to polarize or dichotomize management and participation; optimal cen- tralized decision—making and decision-implementing, gag optimal participation could be done. This is the pro- cedure of humanistic management. Summary In a family, managing is a process of deciding what is most important to the family and using selectively available means to achieve this. It is a control system for goal attainment. Such concepts as decision-making, ‘ \ n p“: In de: ves 3 .L 7v. IshV‘ ‘anrs‘i 0397‘, .‘ul‘, W CO‘JI‘SE 0.. In CC iout. an, i ‘h 'I". L ..&PS to fol 23 organization, values and goals, and resources, apply to families at all status levels. However, management is likely to differ in control styles, in resource organiza- tion, in participation, and in outcome. In decision-making, a family may either order al- ternatives to best maximize a goal and then select be- tween alternative ways to do this, or mediate or create a new course of action out of conflicting or uncertain alternatives. The latter course of action may be the more realistic situation for many families at lower status levels. Decision processes may then be more of an ordering and directioning of change than a selecting and rejecting process. Because resources are the means for making change, they are also ordered and directed toward outcome. This balancing, adjusting feature ap- pears to follow from actual decision situations families face. The home environment is the setting for managerial activities, and within the families the parents (some- times one parent only, or another person altogether) are the principal managers in that they organize resources and control situations and participation so certain out— comes result. From this framework, we turn to a more detailed explication of the concept of resource (5, 17, 21, 22, 23). k 0“ inn Vn:““ ..v..\‘r“ Q J‘U .s4.i ’. ,A'- .AV“ .4¢¢“" .10 0 ‘Q >A..aa""‘ to .- ‘ Q“, a .r,‘ .H‘ 1.4 r“r ... Ck FA! Pvlnt G 2A Resources: Fundamental Concept in Management This section surveys varying definitions of the con— cept of resources, economic and social psychological points of view about resources, and finally the concep- tualization of resourcefulness. Meaning of Resources Dealing with concepts of family finance, Magrabi and McHugh discuss resources as a class concept which in- cludes different properties held in common by a group of objects (3). In this view, resources may be identified in varying degrees of specificity: for example, resources Icould be defined to include all various forms of income and wealth available to the family. Discussing family resources, Nickell and Dorsey say that they consist of tools, assets, capabilities, and ways and means possessed (24:82—85). With Gross and Crandall (25:12A), they have suggested the division into nonhuman and human resources. Nonhuman resources would be tangible goods and facilities, while human resources would include time, energy, skills, knowledge, attitudes. The point is stressed that there may be potential human resources (potentially increased capability in a skill for example) which may not be presently available to the family. Deacon an Ing hone manage . .. means their poten represented satisfying ; reaching of A resource ties and sk; which has of derands. Re of the soluI p. re; point out 1 .ation: ". . . ':e instrumental the self-suffici feats function a in is self-suf investigat f3e€pindlers de Deeeon and IIaloc Trials (26). I communities ‘ finned "instrurr. 3: Individual en‘ It. ; In .md‘VICe or a lit. CSAME?! he is a IDStrunem ~33? III em Certain 25 Deacon and Maloch, in a systems approach to research- ing home management, define resources of the family as: means which are available and recognized for their potential in meeting demands. Means are represented by those things which have "want- satisfying power" and are instrumental in the reaching of desired ends. A resource . . . [is] anything - knowledge, abili- ties and skills, objects — which can be used or which has direct application in the meeting of demands. Resources are, therefore, the content of the solution (16:32). F They point out that the resources are specific to the sit- uation: ". . . In one managerial situation an object may be instrumental [a resource] while in another it may be the self-sufficient end desired" (16:32). That is, ob— jects function as values if the nature of the satisfac- tion is self-sufficient instead of instrumental. Investigating perception of cultural alternatives, the Spindlers developed a conceptualization related to Deacon and Maloch's distinction of resources as instru- mentals (26). In their anthropological study of acculurat- ing communities‘among the Blood Indians, the Spindlers defined "instrumental" activities as those ". . . that an individual engages in for the achievement and main- tenance of a life style and status in the social groups of which he is a member or aspires to be a member" (26: 312). Instrumental roles of the Indians have prescribed for them certain activities related to fulfillment of specific and V5 roles Could be In BlOOd occupations 83 contribute ’60 '5 self-respect ° activities mOSt tetaves as he 8 environment. T title Operation nonnatively in I In their I sitiations, WhiI aresource simpl I27, 28). In ec EaKing that a re :etisfying power resources within NI “Er, sex I , temp reedom from res :Ich as love and limited resource ifitors and :HA ~P~Dersonal fc £05 PCES HOt DPE 26 specific and valued social goals. Thus, activities and roles could be added to the list of family resources. In Blood society, instrumental activities are not occupations as usually thought of, but activities which contribute to earning specific rewards, subsistence and self-respect. The Spindlers develOped an inventory of activities most relevant to the way in which a Blood behaves as he attempts to cope with everyday life in his environment. The instrument is intended to elicit spe- cific Operational perceptions of social reality organized normatively in means-end relationships. In their work on resource mediation in interaction situations, Whiting and Longabaugh have chosen to define a resource simply as ". . . anything that a person wants" (27, 28). In economic terms this would be analogous to saying that a resource is anything that has want- satisfying power. In their conceptualization, common resources within the family and home would include food, water, sex, temperature, rest, privilege, information, freedom from restriction and pain, and derived resources such as love and praise. Operationally, Longabaugh limited resources ". . . to things of value to the actors and . . . likely to be salient (brought into inter—personal focus by one or another of the inter— actors) in the interaction" (28:322). In this view, resources not presently possessed by a particular ' ‘- :Eties and L -'r~"I£L AL-“_-':] and V" I“...- “ A. n . ,1 v AIJfl' Concl A '\”"‘F"I .L‘ -.' V“ESCY econ * ~‘ A c. y'p's rega ’: J, J)' The. Jnu Pes‘l‘l‘ :1:an P9“: 4» 3— - tLOn . .7 Ce Paw ‘~« 0 .‘ r1 ease neasu .3“ch -V’ and .5; 27 individual (a child, for instance) are desired and thus acquire motivational force. In general we might say that a comprehensive descrip- tion of resources in the family includes such factors as: the means-end relationship or instrumentality for goal achievement; consideration of possession, availability and potentiality; specificity with regard to common pro- perties and the managerial situation; awareness, desir- ability and value; and both human and nonhuman elements. We now consider more specifically the economic and social psychological orientations toward resources. Economic Point of View This orientation emphasizes the processes of con- tinuous adjustment, substitution and combination of re— sources. In addition, Deacon and Maloch have cited scarcity, measurability and alternative uses as attri— butes of economic resources, and Magrabi and McHugh have discussed value, efficiency and control as relational concepts regarding resources in the economic context (16, 3). The latter authors point out that: One result of establishing value relationships among resources is that less-valued resources tend to be substituted into the use process for those having higher values (3:108). A value measure imposes an ordering among goals or re- sources, and it is partly determined by the decision—maker and partly by environmental factors. Efficienc res:urces and E If two r683 source is 37 obtains a I. be evidence is achieved sources. . [A coriiini the more e1 And conce 3'03“ ....;.ences his then r t ‘ “lientione: ” D. in hang of res- :11i3ally, con ' o o the ' able and w goals or t accordance 28 Efficiency is the essential relationship between resources and goals: If two resources are equally valued, the one re- source is more efficient than the other if it obtains a higher goal. Or greater efficiency may be evidenced when one of two equally valued goals is achieved through the use of lesser valued re- sources. [A] combination of resources would be said to be the more efficient (3:108-109). And concerning control, Magrabi and McHugh indicate that it implies a two-way relationship: a decision maker controls resources but in turn is likely to be controlled by them due to the fact that possession or nonpossession influences his attitudes towards them. (This point has been mentioned before under Whiting's and Longabaugh's meaning of resources, and will be discussed again.) Spe- cifically, control is: . the degree to which the decision—maker is able and willing to expend resources to attain goals or to adjust his valuation of goals in accordance with his resources (3:109). A decision in the economic area involves choice of re- sources and goals having the best balance in value rela- tionships as measured by efficiency, and this may change over time and space. The utility function in terms of combinations of resources has been discussed by Clarkson, who indicates that any particular level of utility is derived by com- bining different amounts of goods, and that a large Faiety of < greater uti] criinations theory). Hicks teeen corple sauces (30: nation where ialattainre “Mort ..l.,,,,,s of a thepresence 31°15 EXplair . . . the for X, X for corp; ”Oticed t than this FOP Sub-st and COULII it is Suf. be the s a: 29 variety of combinations is possible (29). It is this greater utility which is of interest, not the different combinations of goods which yield it (indifference theory). Hicks has stated the distinction and relation be- tween complementarity and substitution applied to re- sources (30:AA-A6). Substitution is operative in a sit- uation where one or more resources may replace another in goal attainment. Complementarity is operative when the effects of a resource are reinforced or supplemented by the presence of other resources. Concerning reciprocity Hicks explains that: . . . the relation of substitution (and comple- mentarity) is reciprocal. If Y is a substitute for X, X is a substitute for Y; and similarly for complements. Nevertheless it should be noticed that the theorem says something more than this mere reciprocity by classification. For substitutes to be substitutes both ways, and complements to be complements both ways, it is sufficient that the cross-effect should be the same in sign; they do not have to be equal, as the reciprocity theorem tells us that they are. The conditions for reciprocity by classification will clearly be less stringent than those for the equality of the cross— effects. So long as the degree of substitution (or complementarity) is considerable, we can relax the linearity condition quite consider- ably, and the reciprocity by classification will still hold. . . . (31:128) Underlying individual economic conduct in handling resources is an assumed stability, according to er'S low b . . nr‘""" ~I-H““I'e .F I‘" ...::I‘ f CL “rs t hi can. no a E “a f f nu .J S C 0 to 0 e .1 s C t VJ mu. 9 n. Y“ .T. n. U at .. . no 5 . 1 . .nu T.» r“ a: r“ a». a . .4 IA. ‘ A A. v ~ l. C “I U 1‘ “y 01" I." .I "« n prod‘ nsions 0: Jet“? ‘;I S» i...‘3 & 3O Schumpeter's explanation of the theory of circularity and equilibrium (32). Based on the conditions surrounding the individual, there is an equilibrium between means on hand and wants to be satisfied. Economic life is a cir- cular flow: there is continuity and constancy of values and resource-exchange processes. Individual valuations of resources are interdependent and interrelated. In the context of this theory, availability and use, quantity and quality of resources may be examined. Fitzsimmons and Lancaster refer to the relation be- tween production and consumption, as apparent in resource dimensions of availability and use (33, 3A). Fitzsimmons says that production is the making of goods available (the creating of utilities at the time, in the place, and form desired) for consumption (which is the utilization of goods directly for satisfaction of human wants) (33: l6A-l72). In Lancaster's View of consumption, "Goods, as such, are not the immediate objects of preference or utility or welfare, but have associated with them char- acteristics which are directly relevant to the consumer" (3A:18). He uses the more neutral term "characteristics" instead of "satisfactions." The consumer wishes to attain the most desired bundle of characteristics subject to situational constraints, and goods are needed to obtain these characteristics.) He points out the analogy to production theory: goods or resources are viewed as imuts into :utplts. Til Ranch framei sated earlie In cons ons for re igncr .cnsuxer . pessesses EGOds may Eive a Sp. 3 0’ (n ffinain refe; 31 inputs into a process in which the characteristics are outputs. This is an analogy similar to the Deacon— Maloch framework for researching home management pre- sented earlier. Regarding efficient use of resources, Lancaster believes that: In consumption, as in production, the prime rea— sons for inefficient use of the existing technology are ignorance and lack of managerial skill. The consumer may not be aware that a certain good possesses certain characteristics or that certain goods may be used in a particular combination to give a specified bundle of characteristics (34:18). Then, in reference to efficiency and the state of tech- nological development of the society, he comments: A relatively static technology, in consumption as in production, will . . . probably lead to a situation in which the efficient activities be- come generally known and traditional. Traditional consumption patterns will be efficient only within a relatively unchanging choice situation and only optimal for consumers whose preferences on char- acteristics approximate the society mode. Tradi- tion will be less useful when the technology is changing rapidly . . . or when the consumer's preferences diverge from the mode (3A219). In the economic view of resources, the power of wealth and services to satisfy wants depends upon two things, according to Fitzsimmons (33). The productiv- ity of goods and wealth depends upon their availability and possession, and the qualities they have or that are attributed to them. Burk has defined quality in rela— tion to food as ". . . the combination of attributes Of a product that have significance in determining the :Iesree of a c a: 19's . "semence arr 4\ e. "‘5 Pesch‘r‘cef‘t It'll-Or ‘si.l VPni “E an O IF: K 'Venifin 32 degree of acceptability of the product to the user" (35:117). Such quality is either composed of inherent attributes or may be developed in a commodity. Burk suggests that changes in quality may occur in one of three ways: more of a particular attribute, more con- sistency of an attribute (more standardization), or more availability over time. Concerning quality and quantity, Nichell and Dorsey have said that: The quality and amounts of the resources that every individual or family has at its disposal are different from those of every individual or family. Each is constantly forced to work with different amounts and combinations of resources (2A:8U). Certain factors related to the economic view of re- sources--usefu1ness, limitation, alternativeness, inter— dependence and assessment--will be mentioned again under the resourcefulness concept. Social Psychological View of Resources Those who view resources in the family from a social psychological stance are interested in such factors as: competence, perception and empathy as human resources; resource mediation and exchange as determinants of role learning and growth in children; and the motivational, reinforcing and qualitative properties of human resources. Several investigators have been concerned with com- petence in the family setting, especially with regard to 33 optimal functioning of family members in large, urbanized, industrial or industrializing societies (36, 37, 38, 39). Competence includes the dimensions of problem—solving and of skill in mediating activities, as well as awareness and understanding of available alternatives and how to attain them. Bronfenbrenner has commented that in order for children to function as productive, cooperative members of the community they need competence not only in cognitive functioning but also in the areas of a sense of control over the environment, capacity to defer immediate gratifi— cation, skills in working c00peratively with others, socially responsible patterns of behavior, and techniques for non-destructive resolution of personal and inter- personal problems (36:l-2). Scheinfeld has called for a shift in family culture from a primary emphasis on con- trol in child-rearing to development of competence and emphasis on internal experiences in order to help child- ren grow in it (37). There is complexity and circularity in viewing competence as a resource: Scheinfeld sees it as a needed resource input in optimal family functioning, while Sussman stresses it as output: The degree of competence exhibited by any indi- vidual or by a closely interacting primary group such as the family is an outcome of the totality of formal and informal socializing experiences to which he (or it) has been exposed (38:5). _Iparentlyr cor itensional re exponents, ar :acurce for E that the: “hp .. . opp. cakes avai Options in individual tence to r Percepti angnificant I rehrence. Ho; Perception (or lack oi “aerial af’ him. It 12' 3A Apparently competence may be regarded as a multi- dimensional resource with both managerial and perceptual components, and in Sussman's view, its importance as a resource for goal attainment would be due to the fact that the: . opportunities which a modernizing society makes available to its members become viable options for a given individual only when that individual demonstrates the requisite compe- tence to recognize and pursue them (38:3). Perception, as a component of competence, becomes a significant human resource in the managerial frame of reference. Hoyt offers the following definition: Perception is the recognition of the significance (or lack of significance) to man of what his material and his non-material environment offer him. It includes not only his perception of separate things and ideas but of their relation- ships. . . . So for a man to perceive the significance of the various parts of his environ- ment in relation to himself is the first step not only toward the solution of his problems but toward knowing what his problems are (“0:76). There are two aspects of perception: the mental-emotional set for something or against something which supports "insensitivity." Examples are wishful thinking, pre- judices, and fears, as apparent in attitudes and expecta— tions. The other less recognized but more important aspect is sensitivity: when a person fails to perceive he is not attending and listening. "Because of insensi- tivity, man simply does not notice at one period what, at a later period, is as obvious as the sun in the heavens" (“0:77). Hoyt illustrates failure to perceive or insensitivit oijects of n environment 3;;when ide are either I". scious level are not seen urns but en heart 01“ the .0 1* Percentiox E'KaI‘eI‘IESS th: 2? " , Fr *1 JpERneSS Risa; repeated] SCIIEIja Or in: BPSst be} a: ‘093 a: or diSC0~J 35 insensitivity in three choice situations: when natural objects of major potential usefulness are present in the environment but not recognized as useful and not taken up; when ideas of major potential value in conducting life are either not consciously recognized or are at the con- scious level but not used; or when consequences of actions are not seen (“0:80). Hagen has observed that one's value system may limit one's perception of alternatives--because of it one may just not be aware of any (1:117). He suggests that the heart of the human resource he calls creativity is a set of perceptions in a person which includes not only an awareness that the world one knows is understandable, but an "openness to experience" or a "capacity to be sur~ prised." He says: It is a perception that as life proceeds one will repeatedly meet phenomena that do not fit in the schema one knows to that point, and that if they interest one, one can count dependably on being able both to deduce the system in which they do belong and on finding satisfaction in the process of discovering and exploring it (1:138). Hagen refers also to empathy as another index of creativity in discussing Lerner's research on modern, transitional and traditional types of individuals (1: 253). Empathy is described as one's ability to project himself into another's position and express a judgment concerning what he would do or feel if he were the other person. It is a personality trait which includes need :;Mrny, the I “:‘ferently I‘I‘I ’erner asked 9' " wha‘ :T.. . , Another I hciern, on! "mart .w and re: first”? by Me F~rte :a‘ 36 autonomy, the perception that others may act alone and differently from oneself. To Operationalize empathy, Lerner asked questions in the mode: "If you were head of . . . , what would you do?" Another example of research into the perception component of competence is the Spindler instrumental acti- vities inventory described on page 25 (26). It was ap- plied to situations where two different social systems, one modern, one traditional, converged, and both were present and perceived by the Blood Indians. The IAI is a way of cataloguing perceptual and cognitive dimensions of behavior as the Indians move into an industrialized, urbanized world by asking them to make value-oriented choices between pairs of instrumental activities (26:321). There is another social psychological view of re- sources also related to perception, although this com- ponent of competence is not treated specifically as a resource. Whiting has proposed a framework utilizing value, control and administration of resources as a set of con- cepts by which social interaction may be described (27). These concepts, suggested as crucial determinants of learning by identification in parent-child interactions, were later operationalized by Longabaugh (28). Whiting's assumptions about resource value are stated in this way: 37 First, a resource is valued to the degree that its absence increases motivation and consumption pro— vides satisfaction. Second, the value of any re- source may be increased or enhanced by insecurity - threats that a resource already available may be taken away or that desired resources may be with- held. . . . A resource that is available whenever one wants it is perceived to be of little value compared to one that is sometimes available and sometimes not (27:11A-115). He cites the economic principle of the value of scarce goods (scarcity increases value) as an example of value enhanced by insecurity with respect to availability. Persons and resources are related by control. Al- though one may have direct access and control over some resources there are many, especially for young children, that one can get only through others by such actions as begging, bargaining, attacking, obeying. Parents have direct control over many resources which children control only indirectly through them. Socializers are resource mediators or administrators. In mediating activities, a socializer (a parent) gives, withholds, or deprives a person (a child) of a resource, for example: provides food, restricts freedom, withholds love, takes away a privilege. He may also threaten such actions, thus enhancing resource value by arousing in- security, or he may promise a resource, thus creating reinforcement by reducing insecurity. In Whiting's framework, the process of identifica- tion begins with socialization when the mother trains and ::n:rols her c rediate so 73;: specify th interction, 0 resources. A between social ing by identif‘ status with re. core will he p; 38 controls her child by withholding resources. The mother may mediate some resources, the father others; the culture may specify the differences. Then, in adulthood social interaction, one both mediates, and accepts mediation, of resources. A major postulate is offered about the relation between social interaction as resource—mediation and learn- ing by identification: the more a child envies another's status with respect to control of a given resource, the more will he practice that role, fantasizing himself as the person he envies controlling and consuming the valued resource of which he feels deprived. In a pilot study of mother-child dyads, Longabaugh coded face-to-face interpersonal behavior, calling his category system "resource-process coding" (28). Acts were coded on two dimensions: the resource salient (of value to the interactors and prominent in their inter- action), and the modes or modalities (ways in which re- sources become salient and are dealt with by the inter- actors: seeking, offering, depriving, accepting, not accepting). He assumed that all interpersonal behavior could thus be categorized. Three resources, information, support, and control, were examined in terms of the extent of resource exchange occurring and the comparative control of the recipients over the resources. "Resource exchange" referred to the quantity of value passing between two interactors. 39 "Resource distribution" referred to the relative posses- sion of salient resources by the two persons. Inter- correlations of these two factors revealed differential cluster patterns, suggesting two separate dimensions of interaction. Concerning methodology in a study of this type, Longabaugh comments that resources may be narrowly and specifically defined (mother's smile) or broadly defined (information). However, there are minimal criteria for including resources: that they correspond to some ex- tent to resources actually valued and exchanged by per- sons, and that the transmission of these resources be capable of being measured in observed situations. Con- ceptualization of resources is inadequate if either cri- teria is not met: empirical validity and reliable measurement. Inferential ability of the investigator plays an important role: he decides the salient re- sources after preliminary observations. Longabaugh sug- gests that it would be inefficient to rely solely on empirical measures when dealing with human subjects and to ignore purpose or meaning, perhaps the most signifi— cant variable in behavior. In his view, reliable in- ferences about interpersonal meanings based upon knowledge Of the situation and the people make use of the experience and knowledge of the investigator about what he is study- ing: resource mediation actions in families. NO Other researchers have defined factors which operate within the family as means to optimal growth and develop- ment. Baldwin, Kalhorn and Breese devised rating scales to appraise parental warmth, intellectual objectivity in attitudes towards children, and control (Al). These were the three variables which formed a framework for describ- ing and summarizing parent-child interaction. In research fundamental to this study, Hess and Shipman studied urban black families in order to specify linkages between social structure and individual behavior. They investigated a particular cluster of human resources, a characteristic developing in the preschool years and of instrumental importance in attainment of the goal of successful school participation of the child (9, 10, 11). This concept, educability, referred to the child's ". readiness to use the teaching and learning re- sources of a formal school situation" (11:4), and to: . . a cluster of cognitive skills (e.g., langu— age, concept formation, visual and auditory dis- crimination), to the child's motivation to learn in a classroom setting (curiosity, need for achievement, etc.), and to his acceptance of the role of pupil (11:171). ' Gordon recently discussed demographic factors in- strumental in children's development which have been isolated by various investigators, such as the degree of Crowding in the home, quality of housing, ethnic member- ship, father absence, level of income, social class (A2zl). Al He also described parental cognitive factors which, when consistently available and utilized, have given evidence of functioning in achievement of goals related to intel— lectual and personality develOpment of children. Many of these factors are included in the studies by Dave and Wolf, described under the section of this review on family as environment (page 52). Lastly he mentioned a set of emo— tional resources: consistency of behavior control, order in work habits, and perception of another as a separate person. Bronfenbrenner has pointed out examples of social psychological resources which have specific quantity— quality limitations (36). The problems which a child may have when he begins school may be traced back to lack of stimulation, both cognitive and motivational, in his home. It is not necessarily true that the child has not had sufficient attention from the family; however, it has been so generalized and diffused, instead of appropriately discriminating, in quality, that it has not had an impact in selective reinforcement of school-related behaviors. He questions whether the parents lack ability, or motiva— tion, to deal with children appropriately to their needs. In addition, he also points out that there is re- search evidence that it is the quality of a child's school peers and/or older children with whom he interacts, and not the quality of the school itself, which may be more iicsrtant to n 4» 'ana "8.6)?5 LI‘ :tnstructive I :s“:"nnn“nlhpc ":c ' V‘Il mllrl I “‘"nqq P .eiulness" TC say the 35. that h It I188 to "iiple. '1': things W ”Th other fiche . ~17-llar 1., ‘t-r‘ 4 “melons f. t-al‘e~ v- . “ 0 an EC} F. A2 important to academic gains or losses (36:2). "Quality" here refers to social class standing of peers, and the constructive or destructive behaviors of older neighbor- hood children who serve as models for young children. Resourcefulness Within the framework of this study, people, objects, events and activities may possess the property of "re- sourcefulness" if they function as contributors or as means to obtaining some desired outcome. At a conference on values and decision making, Liston described this property: To say that a person is resourceful is simply to say that he has a wide horizon as to--the things he has to work with. Take recreation, for ex- ample. You can get your recreation by doing things by yourself, by doing things in the home with other people with things you have in the home . . . or you can get recreation by going to a free movie or to an expensive concert. Similar interpretations could be given of various horizons for a long list of resources. [Values are] . . . the original horizons from which stem our peculiar patterns of goals and standards and through which we view the potential- ities of our resources . . . (A3z6A-65). 'With especial recognition to discussions of family re- sources by Gross and Crandall and Paolucci and O'Brien, resourcefulness includes the following characteristics (25:126-lu7, 21). Usefulness.--Utility is capacity to satisfy human wants. Without usefulness, or utility in action, pos- session of resources is insufficient for goal mediation. “3 That is, only when used, are so-called resources com- pletely "resourceful." Usefulness seems to require a kind of balancing between conserving things and meeting demands people place upon them. Usefulness is a creative charac- teristic, a unique combination of perceiving demand and using means to meet demand (A, 23). In this connection, Deacon suggests that resources can be discovered, that uses for them can be expanded, and Schlater that resources can be created (15, A). Limitation.-—Restrictions within a given time and space may cause some resources to become more important. There are always some limits on availability and use. Absence, or too little, of any single resource may be crucial in determining success or failure in goal attain- ment, even when there are other means available. In an affluent society, limitation is a changeable characteris- tic, and some resources appear endlessly plentiful. In a traditional or a modernizing society, however, resources may appear endlessly restricted. In either society, the actual situation is likely to be that there is both scarcity and abundance of resources. As limits upon some resources become less, they become more apparent on others. Amounts of some (income, capacity to work) vary greatly from family to family, from time to time, and are restricted by inherent capacity, by training, by status. new“? 3.“ sore “the sfiict work “the? Pestri A'- 11 excellence’ ‘ o n 7,’ “n 4".“ nEUn/Lll lye 1 Di). witty of P ajilele qua ”L‘P.l “sources 5‘49 txns: It is a I“ co." 8 MC? greater t rocels hp .avoratle (‘u :t‘” .Iacgeatle an a." n I. er OI‘ Cfi aers are co' «to re” .-nlngs f :‘I AU Resources may he further limited by unavailability of some others (lack of education, for instance, may re- strict work opportunities which in turn restrict income). Other restrictions are imposed by limits of quality, of excellence, of meeting standards in the makeup of the resource itself. Hagen has given an example of how increase in quantity of personal resourcefulness may be related to available quantity and quality in other contributing resources such as social contacts and supportive situa- tions: It is a reasonable speculation that the child be- comes more resourceful and more creative the greater the number of differing understandable models he comes in contact with in emotionally favorable contexts (1:139). Substitution, complementarity.—-Resources are inter- changeable and transferable: one may serve in place of another or offer possibility of a course of action when others are committed elsewhere or nonexistent. There are two meanings for alternativeness: alternative uses of a given resource (it may be used for different ends), and use of alternate resources to reach a goal (if some means is missing, one finds or creates out of the possibilities at hand a substitute that will do in its place). Also, resources can reinforce or supplement one another in goal attainment. us ‘ Interrelatedness and interdependence.--According to Cross and Crandall: A philosophy which appears suitable for today is one which highlights the interrelatedness of resources . . . maintains that materials should be subordinate to people in importance. . . . Conservation of things should be balanced against the demands such conservation makes upon other resources, particularly the human resources (25: 132). This element is suggested as a kind of summary with all preceding characteristics seeming to point to this aspect of resourcefulness. If availability and utilization can be categorized as two dimensions of resources, they appear to function in an interrelated manner, although concep- tually distinct. Interrelatedness implies a reciproc— ity among means relevant to a specific outcome. Inter— dependence implies a mutual dependence: one resource does not function without another or others. The sum of re- sources available for a goal usually determines whether or not it will be mediated. Yet mediation is also de- pendent upon total interrelated use of resources. This combined pattern may be so complex that it is not easy to conceptualize and to operationalize problems from the point of view of resource-interrelatedness and -inter- dependence. Yet as separate resources are examined for their influence on a particular goal, this patterning aSpect is likely to become apparent both to the families involved and to researchers. A6 Measurability.—-Gross and Crandall have indicated that: Resources are similar in that they are all useful; they are all limited; their use is interrelated; the managerial process is applicable to all re- sources; and finally, most important, the quality of life an individual achieves is dependent upon his use of them (25:125). If the course of a person's or of a family's life may be determined by resource—availability and utilization, then both dimensions €puld be carefully determined, and assess- ment based upon observed indicants. Limits, like uses, would need to be assessed in relation to specific goals, with recognition that resources vary in how they are limited and in the accuracy with which such differences can be measured. The nearest to objective evaluation of use of spe- cific material resources may be through inventories to determine length of life, but this is only part of effective utilization. Qualitative differences are par- ticularly difficult to measure and require inferences by an experienced observer. Checks on use requiring infer- ences include: quality and quantity of imagination em- ployed in using resources, continuing satisfactions people express from use, effect upon persons involved in resource exchange. Some measurements (such as time used, total income, income allocation) are objective measures and common Sandards a. lease leave -* *et at. t} .g {V 5;;rces: ti Other he . . . cc are to Fl relaticri compare 7.8 SUPG a: _ r ‘H .m 3 n r ”es 5 g V i“90€nit1 .i1ldiffellc 35:111th ..:1 , ~- tI...l°e kn tgelar ‘1‘, 130.3”; “7 standards against which resource use has been compared. These leave much to be desired, perhaps because they fail to get at the interrelated-interdependent aspects of re- sources: their patterning for goal achievement. In this regard, Deacon has commented: Other measures yet to be developed should have . . . comparative and additive attributes if they are to have significance for clarifying inter— relationships. . . . In addition to being able to compare and add resource potential within the measure under consideration is the need to be able to do so between and among the available measures (15:761). Some studies already mentioned have attempted to develop measures of resources within the family environment and to assess resource patterning for goal achievement in different ways. In the last section we examine family as environment. Family as Environment This section presents several views of family as en— vironment, and relates them to management and development within the family. In this regard, Paolucci has said: Home management today deals with husbanding re- sources so that the more intangible as well as the tangible goals of the family are reached. Recognition that child rearing practices result in different personality types and that the pos- sibilities for growth are enhanced if one acquires skills, knowledge and attitudes valued by a par- ticular culture, obligates adults to so arrange the home environment for children so that it offers the best chances for optimum growth (an: 5-6). “8 To which Liston adds: The degree to which the needs of family members are met will determine the character of the human resources on which the family may capitalize in its next round along the process of its develop- ment (6:66). Sussman appears to be in agreement with the above premise when he discusses reciprocity, or some form of coopera- tion, in relation to superordinate goals in the family (U5). The family can be viewed as an environment for both emotional exchange and socialization in the area of mental functioning, with the goal of developing ability to handle frustrations and competition in its members: The family provides the psychological and physical territory in which one can be emotional, express one's feelings and both give and receive affective response unabashedly (US:7). However, family needs and objectives also create linkages with bureaucratic organizations external to the family, and cooperation or reciprocity is again suggested as a guiding principle (instead of conflict) for these relationships. Thus, the family serves as an environment supporting linkages between its members and society. Societal conditions in turn influence the family environ- ment and individual members' development. An example of this process may be the research of Barry, Child, and Bacon into relations between occupation and child training (H6). They compared societies, using Ethnographic data, with different types of economies and I19 with differences in child rearing practices in the areas of obedience, achievement, and self-reliance pressures. Resulting associations led them to infer that how a family trains its children tends to be a suitable adaptation to the economy. Families in subsistence societies tended to insure their survival by arranging their child-rearing practices around the objectives of developing independent, venturesome adults, while in high accumulation economies (which stress animal husbandry and commercial agriculture) the families socialized children into responsibility and obedience because their goal was that of ensuring the continuity and protection of an assured food supply. These findings seem supportive of Liston's View that: ". management in the family is much involved in the quality of give and take between the family and the other subsystems of its social environment--economic, political, community, cultural, and physical" (6:66). The family as a specific environment mediating be- tween individuals and the larger society, as suggested above, has not been widely studied in the past. Pre- liminary to offering his framework for viewing the family as a mediating environment, Hagen discusses this point: "The analysis of effects of conditions in the society at large on personality formation has been a no man's land of the social sciences" (1:200). In his attempt to syn- thesize knowledge from several disciplines into a theory 50 of social change, Hagen developed a model of the impact of the home environment on personality (1:200-217). He indi— cated specific causal sequences by which it seems possible that social pressures upon parents would lead them to create an environment conducive to personality change in their children, and that this would continue over suc— cessive generations. The historical sequence in the family, of progressive changes in personality over the generations, appears to be loss of status respect by authoritarian parents leading to retreatism and finally to creativity. As retreatism deepens over the generations, it creates circumstances of family life leading to develop- ment of innovational and creative personalities. Family as environment for human development may be viewed also in the context of Bloom's proposition that: Variations in the environment have greatest quantitative effect on a characteristic at its most rapid period of change and least effect on the characteristic during the least rapid period of change (u7zvii). Following this thesis, the family might be considered as an environment which represents a set of powerful persist- ing forces affecting human characteristics, such as the child's educability, which are undergoing rapid develop— ment at a particular period, in this case, at the pre- school age. Then, located within the home, the neighbor- hood and the community in which the young child carries 51 out his activities, would be found the resources or means for helping him to learn new concepts or discriminations preparatory to school experience. Operationally the environment would be bounded by the child's direct and indirect involvement with these resources, whether they be persons, activities or objects. The studies to be mentioned have taken this point of view. Constancy, consistency, powerfulness and pervasive— ness are suggested by Bloom as the principal environ- mental factors which converge to influence human char— acteristics (u7:19u—212). Constancy refers to the idea that the environment's effect becomes stabilized and rein— forced only when it is relatively constant over a period of time. Consistency suggests that various contemporary aspects of the environment are similar and mutually rein- forcing. Consistency is the element which distinguishes a powerful environment from one only moderately so, or one ineffectual in its consequences. Bloom specifies the relation between powerful environments and develop- ment thus: . powerful environments represent rather ex- treme instances of abundance or deprivation and apparently involve most individuals in them in very similar ways. That is, they are relatively uniform in preventing individuals from securing the necessary nutriments, learning experiences, or stimulation necessary for growth, or they are so powerful in reaching all with the appropriate nutriments, experiences, and stimulation that all (or almost all) individuals are affected in similar ways and to a similar extent (“7:212). 52 A major characteristic of such environments is their pervasiveness: an individual is engulfed in the situation which presses him from every side toward a particular development or outcome. Young children are especially affected by such extreme, sustained environments, while older, more experienced persons, dissatisfied with such surroundings, may produce apprOpriate modifications in the environment. Wolf and Dave, students of Bloom's, were concerned with characteristics within homes which indicate educational environments related to general intelligence on the one hand and to educational achievement on the other, of fifth grade children (U8, “9). Wolf identified three environmental process variables contributing to intelligence develOpment: Press for Achievement Motivation, Language DevelOpment and Provision for General Learning (U8). His measurement scales appeared to represent particular patternings of resources within homes for the specific goal. He predicted positive and significant correlations between environmental measures and children's I.Q. scores, and found correlations between them of .69 to .76. Multiple correlations and a factor analysis showed even higher commonality among the three variables than predicted, perhaps indicating interdependence Of the measured resources as well as an environment operat- ing to develop intelligence. 53 Other findings included support for a prediction of a greater relationship between environmental variables and I.Q. than between social class and I.Q. He found that his scaling instrument appeared to be more sensitive in upper class homes than in lower class homes. Following Bloom, Wolf suggested the idea of a pervasive environment affect- ing all persons within it, as well as a specific one affect— ing a particular individual, indicating that perhaps some resources influence the goal (general intelligence develop— ment) directly for some family members, and indirectly for others. Dave constructed an Educational Environment Index composed of six variables: Achievement Press, Language Model, Academic Guidance, Activeness of Family, Intel— lectuality in Home, and Work Habits in Family (“9). As indicants of the variables, his rating scales assessed such aspects as parental aspiration for the child's educa- tion as reflected in goals and activities, materials in the home related to school learning, family activities and possessions which expose the child to a variety of experiences, thought-provoking situations related to toys and daily activities, habits of punctuality and perseverence as shown by family routines. Dave predicted that he would find a greater relation- ship between his Educational Environment Index and a measure Of the children's educational achievement than he would 5“ between class status and the achievement measure. He reported correlations of .79 between the Index and achievement, and -.02 to .27 between four class measures and achievement. He predicted a positive but moderate relationship among the environment measures and showed that eleven of the fifteen values were interrelated at levels higher than .50 at the .05 level of significance. This finding suggests that there is an interdependence of these environmental dimensions. Hess has commented that one of the most familiar findings in research related to education and develOpment has been the association between learning and environment, conceptualized as social class and culture (50). Social class of itself is a probability statement of the likeli— hood that certain experiences will occur, and that they will have predictable effects upon behavior; however, this concept and that of ethnic culture and related behavior needs to be further refined and examined in detail. The involvement of family and community reference groups in the successful socialization and education of dis- advantaged children is an issue under current investigation. He suggests that at early age levels the effects of both cognitive and noncognitive experiences of young children are likely to be diffuse and general, and that "the concept of the family as a socializer of cognitive behavior seems 55 likely to become one of the most thoroughly explored areas of early education in the next few years" (12:1—2). Approaches to these questions have taken varied focus, such as the developmental—cognitive approach, the focus on social structure and socialization, the focus on deficits and lack of specific skills in children from disadvantaged families, or an approach centered on restructuring the environment of the child. In the "underdeveIOped re- sources" model, the environment of the child is considered relatively restricted and his behavior lacks the qualities of complexity needed to allow him to participate fully in other parts of society, although his family culture may equip him to function within sub-cultural limits (50:35). A question for investigation would be: what are the mechanisms of exchange that mediate environmental pres- sures into cognitive behavior? Concerning mediated con- tacts, Hess says that: . . . it does seem we have underestimated the ex- tent to which direct (though diffuse) experience with the environment (interaction with peers, TV, newspapers, music, observation of community life, etc.) directly shapes the child's cognitive and behavioral strategies and resources (50:2“). The following table represents the influence of what may be called a measure of family as environment from the investigations of Hess, Shipman and their research group at Chicago (11:32). In the table are correlations be— tween this variable and some measures of mothers' 56 TABLE 2.--Corre1ation of home resources factor with mother's behaviors and child's performance, by working class group and sex of preschool child.l Working Class Group Correlation of Home Resource Total Group by Sex of Factor with: Group Preschool Child (H 1A3) Boys Girls Mother's Behaviors Expresses attitude of personal optimism to— .33 .10 .5u wards the future (low score=high optimism). Expresses attitude of powerlessness in rela- -.39 —.27 -.51 tion to the school (low score=low feeling of powerlessness). Exerts pressure for achievement upon her .38 .23 .52 child (low score=high pressure). Controls her child through giving him impera- .23 .13 .3u tive commands and asserting authority. Controls her child through encouraging his -.27 - 23 -.3U subjective consideration of his own or others' feelings. Child's Performance Sorts blocks correctly into groups Ly color, -.23 -.lU -.30 shape, height and mark. Shows behavior problems during mother's teach- .18 .02 .32 ing of block sorting (resistance, non— response). Explains his reason for sorting pictures -.25 -.38 —.IU in a scoreable manner. Unable to verbalize a reason for his picture- .29 .31 26 sorting choice. Performance on Stanford—Binet IQ. -.31 —.06 —.53 1Table adapted from Table III-6, page 32, in: Robert D. Hess, et a1” The Cognitive Environments of Urban Preschool Children (Chicago: The Graduate School of Education, University.of Chicago, 1968). For Home Re- sources Factor, a low score indicates high availability and use of re— sources for educability of the child. Levels of significance are: for totad.group: ri.l9: p=.05, ri.2U: p=.01; for boys: ri.27: p=.05, ri.32: D=.Ol; for girls: rt.26: p=.05, rt.3U: p=.Ol. 57 behaviors and preschool children's performance relative to educability. The Home Resources Factor is a score de- rived from a principal factor analysis of nine ratings of (availability and use of family and child resources: space, physical movement, care and appearance, play, task and work, learning and social contacts. Basic methodology was developed and the scoring carried out by Baker, using data from the Chicago study (23; 11:229-2U6; 51:131—147). There are significant relationships apparent between this family environment measure and mother-child measures for the total group and by sex groups of children (11: 3343“). Families which scored high on resource avail- ability and use (the Home Resources Factor) were those in which mothers were optimistically oriented towards the future, expected achievement from their children, and did not feel powerless towards the school. These mothers also showed a significant tendency towards controlling their children's behavior by appeals to their feelings and in- ternal states instead of by emphasis on their own author- ity and status, which would tend to cut off reflection over alternatives by children. In addition, there are differential relationships for boys and girls. For girls, there are more significant relationships in total, suggesting that the Home Resource .Factor may indicate a more pervasive, supportive family pSm .m.¢.o.z.Hnu.m madman d m\k v m . >m>mam .a.<.o.z._ zomu / omEUMJmm >oahm mo mmuhnznzzou O N V 68-... 0.0 can can To -M. »m>m:m .m.<.u.z.~ 2— v A\\ . o.U $3.65 oz< mzoaumm 5.2m: .r . x 2 8:63.58 3:23:28 0 // m «z u¢,«a.z=.a ,x\ J w 7/. ,J s v .2523 <53: 9:. mo mEzSIII /_/ J x..\.\ s./ 0‘ v /./- l ./. o 323 32935 I .I .l/ \) .\ /./ .1... /.L ././ é ) \ I. ./. v .... mix: $2225.55 1...! A}. \.a If... /./ / I / /.I..!I ...u .IL \ (f m m o a z < ././ 0 \oo<»¢asmo mao>mq oucfi ooasosu mwsfipmm mo oflmom pcprSmom mwcfipmm mo mcmopumm manfimmom .zpfiafinmospo pom mesmppwd oopsomom ”mmcfipwm mo mHSHosspm no awoaoamenl.w mqmde 9U Department of Nutrition, a supervisor introduced the re— searcher by letter to staff of the health clinics in the 12 study communities and requested that they help in making initial contacts in their areas. The supervisor also made similar arrangements in the community where pre— testing of the instruments was done. At the initial visit, the researcher was introduced by the community nurse, the study was explained, as well as the COOperation desired. If the family agreed to participate and met eligibility criteria, a letter, signed by the researcher, was left with them as a reminder. It contained a statement of the purpose of the study and some information about the investigator (see Appendix C, page 253). Sections of the Preliminary Questionnaire were re- vised or completed at the time of this initial contact with the family. Before leaving, the family was informed about when to expect a second visit from the investigator. The second visit, a few weeks to several months later, was a time when some more refusals were encountered, and some cautiousness and reluctance to participate began to appear. In one family, the researcher finally secured complete cooperation after several discussions with the head of the household. He agreed to let his family par- ticipate but only when he was present. In other families, too, cooperation was allowed only when the head of the household had been contacted personally, and only when others were present to hear what the mother and researcher were discussing. It is difficult to know what differentiated eligible families who dropped out of the study from those who finished. There are several possible explanations. That some had negative feelings or misunderstandings about re— search could be deduced from comments made about participa- tion in the earlier nutrition survey. Other families ex- pressed fears related to tax collection, loss of children, or recent law enforcement activities in the area. Several women mentioned an alcoholic husband as the reason for not participating; others would only say their husband refused them permission. In succeeding visits, a set order was usually fol— lowed in presenting the instruments. First, Part I and II of the Resources Inventories were completed. Then followed Part III of the Resources Inventories and the Occupation and Income Survey, the latter filled out with the father if it could be arranged. At the last visit the Picture Questions were presented to the mothers, under the assump- tion that by this time the maximum level of rapport had been established. When data collection was terminated, the researcher presented the preschool child in the study a picture story book as a remembrance of his family's participation. 96 Presentation of the Picture Questions was sometimes made difficult because of the good rapport with the family. When the picture stimuli were shown to the mother, others present sometimes responded to the questions. It was usually possible to control this interruption unless it was caused by the woman's husband. She was often reluctant to express herself with him present, or looked to him to tell her what to say. In this case the researcher some- times asked him to leave the room. Although use of a tape recorder was a novel experience for the women, most ap- peared to enjoy it. Visits were made at the family's convenience on all seven days of the week, as early as 7 a.m., and as late as 7 p.m. Field work began in April and finished at the end of August, 1968, covering almost 5 months' time. This was about double the time estimated on the basis of pre—testing the instruments. That procedure, with four families, required about three visits and three hours per family. For the 89 families for whom the instruments were completed, there were U66 visits, averaging 5.2U visits and 3.73 hours per family. In addition, for the 15 fami— lies who either proved to be ineligible or did not com- plete the interviews, there were 37 visits made, averaging 2.U6 visits and half an hour per family. There were other factors contributing to the length of time required for the field work. One was travel time 97 in and between communities and the researcher's place of residence. For work in two of the more distant communi- ties, the researcher lived with a family. It was the rainy season of the year, complicating travel on good roads, and making it difficult on the more rural roads, paths, and in the mountains because of mud and fog. How- ever, there was another unforeseen and important addition to the field work which increased the time, but which was of great assistance in its successful completion. The first woman visited alone by the researcher, after initial contacts were completed, refused to co— Operate in the study, although she had expressed no hesi- tation previously. This situation was discussed with an Institute sociologist, available for consultation through— out this phase. He recommended that letters be requested from the director of the Institute and the parish priest in a community near the Institute introducing the re- searcher and attesting to her character. Copies of these letters are in Appendix C, page 25U and 255. Thereafter, the director's letter was presented in each community to the director of the primary school while explaining to him the nature of the study, the data sought from each family, and which families were to be visited. His help was asked in dealing with any problems which might arise in the course of work in the community. In a similar fashion, the letter from the parish priest was presented 98 during a visit to the priest in each community, at which time his support was requested. That this step was beneficial is shown in the following illustrations. A school director personally visited and arranged for the investigator to interview a family which could not be located on first coming into the community. A family which met the investigator with polite evasiveness in repeated visits proved to be very cooperative after talking with the priest of the com- munity. In a rural community, the first visit made was to the most isolated family who expressed suspicion of the study. A letter brought from the local priest con- vinced them that this was a worthwhile project and there was nothing to fear. Word of the letter spread to other families not yet visited and possibly accounted for their receptiveness. In another example, the priest was able to secure needed income data when a wife expressed con- cern about having her husband questioned. Data Analysis After scoring, data from collection instruments were transferred to data processing cards. The Control Data Corporation 3600 model computer was used to perform mul— tiple correlation and regression problems, and the 6500 model computer for the remaining computations. Table 8 shows the complete analysis of the data including 99 statistical procedures used and the instrument associated with each of them. 100 a meo H »x uofim mmucmHonumov co mmmmmmm Hucofiofiuuooo mocmwcfiucoo .osmscw H20 mucofiofiupooo coaumHmLLOo oHQEHm -mucoHoneooo :oHpmHosmoo m oHdeHze paw Hmflpcms :mo. m : pcsx .mmoczmxm .COHpmH>op psmpcmuw .owcms .cmmz MOHpmHseew e momcms .cmoE pcoo Lou .pcsoo accosoopm Empwomm Lopzqeoo paw oaumfipmpw wcfipms mossomos Hmpoe mxmn I an + m u a coapwsco :onmopme ofipmspmso mcfiomn mopzomoc Hmpo» no Ho>oH .owcmL momsommm mwcfipmp monsommh pr0p .mmcfiumh mossommh opmhwqom mcoHu nemeHo an soasosw nomOom monsonoi cocoom oohzomox wmsoom monsomom a new < mqsosm CH mmsoom momzomom necessc Eoocms mo oHome .mmfiaficmu mfiqfimw mo LooEJC osoo mcofiumwso mpzuofim op msozmcm .mwfipoucm>cH oomsomom mo>ssw osoocH pom cofiomQSooo .zmH>hoch zsmcfisfiaopm ”some 0HcomnuoEoQ mama .zpfiHHnmosoo o» cofiumaop mo mam>ma one“ madame moLSOmmn Hmuou mo coHmeHOMmcmme .wCHpmp oopzomop Hmsop ocm omcmp mossommp cmozpmn coHpmHos nmmcfiHH>sso mo wcfiupOHm uhhmcfiefiammm H meoLuoomn mo umoe .mwcfiump momSOmoc mo coHumHmLLoch .mcwpmp ooL30mos Hess» OH anHmcoEHo whoom oomzomop mo cofipmamm .mmcfipmp wULJOmOL Cu DQLOOU GOLZOWQL MO COfiQMELOMmCGLB .m®LOUm wohjomwh “O COHUQHLOWQD .wssoooopa wcfihoom On one mooco Isomewv mo mocopmfixo mumefiumm o» mosopm go wfiwzawc< .wCHLOom Log m new < nozosw 0p moHHfiEmm oflqfiMm no ucoEcmHmwm Eopcmm .osspooosq wcfimoom cwHHnmumo on mfimzamcm ucmucoo .oHoEwm mo mofipmfipouommmco zfifiEmm mo coapqfipomwo mfiwmflwc< so mmoahsm .cump mo mfimmHmcm :H pom: mpocumen|.w mqmee Jill .Hmme .spHmtm>H:: mpesm cemHeOHz .Aomma .hpampm>fic: mumpm cmeonz "cmmficofiz .mcfimcmq umwmv coHpmpm pcoEHpoqu HmLSpHSOHLm< .wa .oz .cofipmfimomoo wmfipmm Bpso new wcfiupoam mama .ccfizzom .2 hmummm .ofiumfipmpm pmuwfisoamolpcmmm .HmmmH .HpHmem>Hco msesm cmwficofiz “cmwficofiz .wcfimcmq pmmmv cofipmHm ucoefisooxm HmLSpHSofiLw< .m .02 .cofiumasomoo mmfipom B go mfimhamc< mmzlmco .Hmfix .m camcoam .HmomH .HSHmtm>H:: spasm cewHeeHz ”:mwfinofiz .mcfimcmq umwmv coaumpm ucmsfipmaxm Hasspazofipw< .s .02 .coHuchome mofipmm BHc: esesm cecheHz "cecheHz .wchcmH seems :H .02 .upomom HmOHccome mocofiom Hmfloom sou mpsufipncH Lousmeou .moHome mocowcfiucoo go mamaHmc< .oaowmmq .E cmH< H mkwpvmmfi mwmo mucoHonmmOo cofiumaonpoo loam Hmcofipmosom mHHEmm .wcfipms m oHaHuHSE pcm Hmfiusmm mopsowop HmuOH .mump OHQQmeoEoQ mmcfipms mossomoc mpwsmomm .mcHums oopsommc Hmpou .mpmp ofizqmcmoEmQ mwcfipmp mopsomop mumsmaom wx ppHm mmucmHOHmmooo co mmoemom mCOHpmzdo COHmmomes ofipmppmza mmcflumn mopsomos Hucofiofimgooo mumpmoom mo new madcap monsOmmc mocowCHpcoo .ommsum “no kuou mo maw>oH .wump cacawpmoEoQ .cofipmscm COHpOHpoLQ m CH mchmp oomsomop HmHOp mo :oHumLOHaxm .mHmmHmcw Apsum wmmo cumspmsHHH ou zHHEmu m mo soapoHsome .seHHHneesee 0p cofipmamn go wHw>mH oucfi mmcfipmp moszomop mumpwowm do cofippromwche pHfico Hoocomopa go xom new omm .wmcfianfim mo ponezc .mNHm zfifiemm .cofiumosco .mEoocH “moanmfinm> msoscHucoo pcm wcfiuwp Hapou Coma» non wcofipmHms smoCaHH>pso go wcfiupoam "hmmcfiEHHmhm m new m monocuoazc mo munch CHAPTER IV FINDINGS Results are presented in relation to stated objec— tives and specific questions under the following headings: method of measurement, dimensions of resourcefulness, interrelated patterning of resources, tests of hypotheses, relation of individual resources to status and structure, descriptive use of family resource patterns, and explora- tion of the prediction potential of the construct. Method of Measurement Significant differences between resource scores en- countered in two scoring treatments, computed by one—way analyses of variance, are summarized in Table 9 (76; 77: 200). A non—directional hypothesis of no difference was specified and a probability level of .05 selected for support of the hypothesis. For 32 of the analyses, no significant differences were found. There were four in- stances in which the hypothesis could not be supported. Findings indicate that for scoring two groups, a maximum of 89 per cent consistency could be claimed. Significant differences were encountered in three resource categories: Child's Movement, Care and 102 103 TABLE 9.--Summary of significant differences between re— source scores in two scoring situations. Resource Variance Level of Category Dimension Ratio (F) Significance Child's AQN .29 N.S. Movement AQL .30 N.S. UQN 7.79 <.O2 UQL 3.73 N.S. Care— AQN 7.0U <.02 Appearance AQL .09 N.S. UQN U.9l <.10 UQL 1.25 N.S. Child's AQN 9.30 <.01 Learning AQL .58 N.S. UQN 2.16 N.S. UQL 7.18 <.02 l Two—tailed non—directional test, df = 1/87, N.S. not significant. 10U Appearance, and Child's Learning. In 2 of the U cases, the inconsistency in scoring was observed for the dimension of Availability-Quantity (AQN). Availability—Quality (AQL) did not present any scoring inconsistencies. Each of the other dimensions, Use—Quantity and Use-Quality (UQN and UQL), had one significant difference. In the following presentation of findings these abbreviations for the dimensions will be used. Dimensions of Resourcefulness Table 10 presents information relevant to the dimen— sionality of the resourcefulness property of family en- vironment. It shows partial correlation coefficients for each set of nine resource category scores relating to each dimension (78; 79:165). Each coefficient is that be- tween a resource score of a particular dimension and a measure called Total Resource Rating, with the influence of the other eight scores held constant or partialed out. Total Resource Rating is used here as an internal cri— terion. An index of validity is suggested by how well individual scores correlated with this criterion, abbre— viated TRR (80:Ul7). Also in the table are multiple correlation coefficients computed between TRR and the combined effects of the nine scores of each dimension (79:131). 105 TABLE 10.——Partial and multiple correlations for scores of each of four resource dimensions with total resource rating. Category of Resource Dimensions Resource AQN AQL UQN UQL Partial Correlations Space .31 .52 .27 .08 Child's Movement .11 -.26 .28 .16 Care—Appearance .26 .2U .U5 .18 Child's Play .35 .U0 .35 -.09 Child's Task—Work .20 .38 .28 .29 Child's Learning .22 .UU .22 .23 Family Learning .U6 .27 .55 .1U Child's Contacts .30 -.19 .25 .23 Family Contacts .23 -.23 .20 .08 Multiple Correlations =.93 R =.90 R =.93 R =.66 R R2=.87 R2=.80 R =.86 R =.UU Levels of Significance r i .22: p = .05 r .001 l+ U) U“! '0 ll r i .29: p = .01 r H- l:- O *0 A .0005 106 Twenty—six of the 36 partial correlations (72 per cent) for the dimension scores were significant. By individual dimension, there were 7 significant relations for AQN, 8 for AQL, 8 for UQN, and 3 for the last, UQL. Examined by type of resource category, Child's Learning was the only one significantly related to TRR on all U dimensions. Seven categories showed significant rela— tions for 3 of U dimensions. Movement and Family Con- tacts presented the fewest significant relations. If .01 is selected as a conservative significance level, each dimension may be examined for the resource scores related to TRR with all others held constant. For AQN dimension, the resources would be Space, Child's Play and Contacts, and Family Learning; for AQL, Space, Child's Play, Task and Work, and Child's Learning. For UQN, the resources would be Care and Appearance, Child's Play, and Family Learning; for the last dimension, UQL, only Task and Work is significantly related to TRR. Child's Play emerges as the only resource category with significant relations on three dimensions. Multiple correlation coefficients were highly significant. Coefficients of the first three dimensions were almost identical. The fourth dimension, UQL, ac- counted for the least amount of variation in TRR. The R2 coefficient of the later dimension indicates that the 107 prOportion of variation accounted for was only about one- half that accounted for by any of the others. Interrelated Patterning of Resources As evidence of the interdependent, interrelated patterning aspect, the intercorrelations of the nine individual resource ratings (IRR) are presented in Table 11 (78). There was found a significant clustering of 33 of the 36 coefficients. Ten of these intercorrelations were values of .50 or higher. All IRR shared in the latter cluster of values except Child's Movement. Child's Learn- ing was the IRR most often associated with the others: it was so interrelated with six measures at .50 or more. Space and Child's Contacts each showed three significant interrelations at this level or above. Hypothesis 1 Families at three resource levels, low, middle, and high with respect to educability, will differ significantly in range of resource ratings. For all three hypotheses, relationships were determined by the chi square tests of independence with .01 the chosen level of significance, and the degree of association by the contingency coefficient (82, 83, 8U, 79). Table 12 reports findings for hypothesis one. TRR indicates the extent to which family resource patterns were oriented to educability of the preschool child. 108 em. e s cH Ho. u a oo.H whomucoo mafiewm Hz. oo.H mpompcoo m.oHH£o em. ms. oo.H wcHeHemH sHHsem mm. mm. mm. oo.H weHetemH m.eHHeo on. em. as. Hm. oo.H xtozuxmee m.eHHeo em. me. me. om. em. oo.H seHm m.eHHee em. mm. am. we. em. em. oo.H eeeeeeteeaumteo em. we. mm. on. mm. mm. mm. oo.H setEe>oz m.eHHee an. em. 0:. mm. Hm. em. mm. mm. oo.H eoeem m m s e m s m m H .wWCHpmcH mOeHSOmmeH mCfiC ..HOQ XHcHUUME COHQMHMLQOOL®UCHII.HH Mdmdfifi 109 Range of IRR suggests the consistency, as described by Bloom, with which resources of the family environment were oriented to educability (U5:212). TRR varied from a low of 8 to a high of 71 among the families; the range of IRR from 1 to 8. TABLE l2.-—Relation between level of total resource rating and range of individual resource ratings. Range of Individual Resource Ratings Total Rgzggiée Low High Total Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Low U6 l2 5U 1U 100 26 Average 10 U 90 38 100 U2 High 62 13 38 8 100 21 II A x2 20.57, p .001, C = .U3 Guided by visual inspection of the plotted relation between TRR and range, sample families were divided into three levels of association to educability: those with TRR of 2U or less; those with TRR of 25 to U8; and those with TRR of U9 and above (81). For range of IRR, there were 2 groups of families: 1 with scores of U or less (low) and the other with scores above U (high). Results show that hypothesis one was supported: within the family environment, the extent of 110 resourcefulness was significantly and substantially associated with the consistency of this property. Within-category differences show that families with low TRR levels were associated with either low or high IRR variation with about the same frequency. However, 90 per cent of the families in the middle TRR level showed high range scores. This group represented U3 per cent of the sample of 89 families. For families at high TRR level, almost two-thirds had low IRR range scores. Hypothesis 2 The proportion of families at three resource levels, low, middle, and high with respect to educability, differs significantly by certain status character— istics: education of parents, occupation, income, and residence. Table 13 shows that significant relationships were found between TRR levels and U of 5 family status mea- sures. Contingency coefficients indicated a high degree of association. Only the relation with occupational sta— tus was not significant. Therefore, hypothesis oneiwas supported with this exception. Of interest are the associations within the indi- vidual contingency tables. The association with mother's educational status was highest. Low TRR families were most often those in which she had less education, and high TRR families were those in which she had higher education. For fathers, less education tended to be related with both 111 TABLE 13r—Relations between level of total resource rating and selected family characteristics. Total Resources Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Rating Father's Educational Statusl U Years More than or Less U Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 77 20 23 6 100 26 Middle 69 29 31 13 100 U2 High 19 U 81 17 100 21 Mother's Educational Status2 3 Years More than or Less 3 Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 69 18 31 8 100 26 Middle U3 18 57 2U 100 U2 High 0 0 100 21 100 21 Income per Capita3 Less than More than $700 $700 Total Low 77 20 23 6 100 26 Middle 60 25 U0 17 100 U2 High 10 2 90 19 100 21 Residence” Urban Rural Total Low 12 3 88 23 100 26 Middle 36 15 6U 27 100 U2 High 76 16 2U 5 100 21 1 - - 14 X2 - 19.13, p <.001, C - . 2 2x2 = 23.31, p <.001, c = .U6 3x2 = 22.62, p <.001, c = .U5 ”x = 20.78, p <.001, c = .uu 112 low and middle TRR levels, and more education for him again with high TRR levels. For economic status, less than $100 ($700) per capita per year of family income appeared related to either low or middle levels of TRR (77 and 60 per cent, respectively, of the cases). Income above $100 was re- lated to high TRR (90 per cent of the cases at this level). The contingency table for residence suggests a relationship between urbanization and high TRR, and be— tween rural residence and low to middle TRR. Hypothesis 3 The proportion of families at three resource levels, low, middle and high with respect to educability, differs significantly by certain family structure characteristics: nuclearity, size, number of siblings, sex and age of pre- school child. No significant relationships were found between TRR and any of the structure measures. Furthermore, there were no trends to significance at probability levels greater than .01. Therefore, the analyses offered no support for an hypothesized association between total family resource patterns for educability and family struc— ture. Relations of Individual Resources to Status and Structure Secondary analyses were undertaken in order to in- vestigate relations between individual resource categories 113 and status and structure measures. These are summarized in Tables 1U to 22. 21222 More significant relationships were encountered for this resource category than for any other (Table 1U). There were significant associations between space levels and all five status measures, offering specific support for hypothesis two. Of interest are the occupational categories. Low space families tended to be more agricultural (60 per cent of families at this level); families at the middle space level tended to be more mixed in occupations (U5 per cent of the families); and high space families to be more non—agricultural in work orientation (UU per cent of those at this level). Children from families with low space levels had parents with less educational attainment (90 per cent of fathers, 80 per cent of mothers of such children). When space levels were high, parental educational levels tended to be high also, although more frequently so for mothers than for fathers (91 to 62.5 per cent at this level). Low income and rural residence most frequently ac- companied low space resources (87 per cent and 93 per cent, respectively, of the cases at this level). llU mm OOH 3H s: OH Hm O mm Ome Om OOH O mm mH me O mm oHOOHz om OOH : mH w Om OH OO 304 Hmpoe oLsOHSOHLwmH woman cmmsuon mCOHpmHomII4H mqmqe 115 mm.uO mm.uO .mOO.AHO.va .OH.OHume Om.uO .HOO.AmOO.va .em.mHumxm m as.uO .HOO.va .OO.Hmumxm mm.uO .HOO.va .OO.mmumxm .mOO.AHO.vO .sm.OHu He Oa.uO .HOO.va .OH.Omu x: ma.uO .HOO.va .sO.OHu xH mm. OOH O mm mm ms emHm em OOH OH Om HH Ha eHOOHz Om OOH ON HO OH mm 30H Hmpoe : cmnp who: wme so : eOHHhO Hooeehpaa Op nmeHHnHm so appeaz mm OOH H mm mm Os OmHm em OOH 1H mm MH O: pHOOHz om OOH OH OO NH O: 30H Hmuoe w cmnp who: mmmq co m omNHm zHHEmm mm OOH NH m.sm Om m.mO anm em OOH mH Om OH H: mHOOHz Om OOH mm mm m s 30H Hmpoe Hmssm amps: moocmpHmmm mm OOH mm ON H mm OmHm em OOH mH OH aH mm mHOOHz Om OOH a mH OH HO 30H Hepoe OOHO can» the: OOHO seep mmeH zepHQmO moo mEoOcH 116 Additionally, the space resource measure offered some evidence concerning relations with family structure characteristics. There were significant associations with family size and number of siblings of the preschool child. The relation was apparently a negative one: the lower these families rated on space the larger they were. Movement The only significant relationship found was with father's educational level (Table 15). When the preschool child's movement was held to a low or middle level, he apparently had a father with education of four years or less, in some two-thirds of the cases. If the child ex— perienced high movement, his father (about 76 per cent of the cases at this level) had more than four years of edu— cation. TABLE 15.--Re1ation between child's movement level and father's educational status. Father's Educational Status Child's U Years or Less More than U Years' Movement Schooling Schooling Total Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Low 69 20 31 9 100 29 Middle 67 29 33 1U 100 U3 High 2a A 76 13 100 17 x = 11.3u, p = <.005>.001, C = .3u 2 117 Care and Appearance Table 16 shows that levels of this resource were significantly associated with education, income and residence. Some evidence related to hypothesis three is the fact that number of siblings of the preschool child showed significant association with care and appearance. Low levels of the resource were in families with the larger number of siblings of the preschool child (71 per cent of the cases at this level); high levels were more often maintained in families with the smaller number of siblings (72 per cent frequency at this level). Also in relation to hypothesis three was a trend, non-significant at the .02 level, to an association be— tween care and appearance and age of female preschool children. Low ratings tended to be most frequently associated with younger-age girls and high ratings with older—age girls. Child's Play Family measures related to the child's level of play are presented in Table 17. Percentages of children with low to middle play levels and with fathers of lower edu— cational attainment tended to be about three-quarters of those at these two levels. For children accustomed to high play levels, about two-thirds of their fathers had higher educational attainment. TAHLE Mi--Relations between care and appearance level and ’1 Q Iamily 118 characteristics. selected Care and Appearance Per cent Per cent H Per cent H Fatlmnc's r k1031113nal thatLusl J .' . , . U . e21 s 1' r‘ , \ LES" echo «'3' . . .. L' r. 1'.(_/:)t: uzlilrl " fly -1. ‘, V' " lea. LI ( Schooling Total Low 77 2U 23 7 100 31 Middle 65 17 35 E 100 26 High 37.5 12 '2.5 20 ‘00 32 V ' ‘£ 1 Mother 3 .JO.11 nal tatuSL H u Here than 3 zears or 2 U j , m .. < tears '..c Less Schooling i , .- lCtdl ‘ chcelinr Low 61 19 39 12 1C0 31 Middle U6 12 EU 1U 00 26 High 16 5 CU 27 100 32 lnccre per Capita3 Less than V700 Torr than V730 Total Low 8U 26 l( 5 100 31 Middle 5U 1U fit 12 1:- 26 High 22 7 76 25 0- an I n ': 1 ‘1 {Hus—L (Pri(“ 'rk 'ur’l .Ctal Low 23 7 77 AU 100 3- Middle 19 5 81 21 100 2t High 69 22 31 10 100 32 a . n\ o [— 1umter of a t11n*s (f Preschool czildJ Low Middle High mrH Ujme \ .1 N 0.) HP‘ (\JCNH LA.) 0.) LA} x2 = 10.9u, 2x2 = 1u.13, 3x2 = 2U.30, “x, = 19.82, 5x = 11.72, <.005>.001, <.001, C <.001, C <.001, 0 <.005>.001, c = .38 : .116 = .143 .3U TABLE 1L-—Relations between child's 119 play level and selected family characteristics. Cgiig's Per cent N Per cent H Per cent N Per cent N Father's Educational Statusl U Years More than or Less U Years’ Total Schooling Schooling Low 71 20 29 8 100 23 Middle 7U 20 26 7 100 27 High 33 13 62 21 100 gm Household Occupation2 Agriculture Fixed Agriddlture Total Low 50 1U 39 11 ll 3 100 28 Riddle U1 ll 37 10 22 C 100 2' High 21 7 LL 3 5 13 160 ‘4 Incone per Car1t13 Less than More than H ,q, 2700 2700 ‘8“t* Low 71 20 29 8 100 PL Middle 63 17 37 10 100 2? High 29 10 71 SU 100 U Residence“ Urban Rural Total Low 18 5 82 23‘ 100 2 Middle 30 8 70 19 100 27 High 62 21 38 13 100 ~,U l _ _ X2 - 10.U2, p <.01>.005, .32 2x2 = 1U.68, p <.01>.005, = .38 3X2 = 12.U8, p <.005>.001, C = .35 “X = 13.75, p <.005>.001, C = .37 120 Children at low to middle play levels appeared to come more often from agricultural or mixed occupation families, while those with high play levels apparently came more often from non-agricultural families. Seventy-one per cent of children at low, and 63 per cent at middle play levels, came from families with lower incomes; while at the upper level 71 per cent of the children came from higher income homes. Associations with residence tended to follow those with income. Children with either low or middle play levels appeared to live most frequently in rural areas (82 and 70 per cent, respectively), and those observed to have high play levels lived in urban communities (62 per cent of the children at this level). Task and Work Only mother's educational status was significantly related to child's involvement in personal tasks and small work efforts about the home (Table 18). Some two— thirds of the children with low involvement had mothers with low educational status; from two-thirds to almost three-fourths of the children with middle to high task and work participation had mothers with higher educa- tional status. It is also noted with reference to the hypothesized relation to family structure that slight trends to 121 significance at the .10 level were found between this re- source category and the nuclear — modified-extended family characteristic, and with children's age, and age of male preschool children. No consistent trends were suggested by the data. TABLE l8.--Relation between child's task and work level and mother's educational status. Mother's Educational Status Ch' 's Tagidand 3 Years or Less More than 3 Years' Work Schooling Schooling Total Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Low 67 16 33 8 100 2U Middle 33 13 67 26 100 39 High 27 7 73 19 100 26 X2 = 9.6U, p = <.01>.005, C = .31 Child's Learning Oppprtunities Status categories of parental education, income, and residence were significantly related to this resource (Table 19). Children who had low to middle levels of learning Opportunities apparently most often had fathers with lower educational attainment (76 and 70 per cent of the children at these two levels). Two-thirds of children with high learning opportunities in their home 122 TABLEIM1--Relations between level of child's learning opportunities and selected family characteristics. Child's Learning Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Father's Educational Statusl U Years More than or Less U Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 76 22 2U 7 100 29 Middle 70 21 30 9 100 30 High 33 10 67 20 100 30 Mother's Educational Status2 3 Years More than or Less 3 Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 59 17 U1 12 100 29 Middle 50 15 50 15 100 30 High 13 U 87 26 100 30 Income per Capita3 Less than More than Z700 $700 Total Low 66 19 3U 10 100 29 Middle 63 19 37 11 100 30 High 30 9 70 21 100 30 Residence“ Urban Rural Total Low 21 6 79 23 100 29 Middle 27 8 73 22 100 30 High 67 20 33 10 100 30 1x2 = 13.12, p <.005>.001, c = .36 2x2 = 1u.27, p <.001, c = .37 3x2 = 9.UB, p <.01>.005, c = .31 “x = 15.75, p < 001, C = .39 123 environments had fathers with higher educational status, and also mothers with similar status in 9 of 10 cases. Within-category differences for income and residence indicate that low to middle amounts of learning opportuni— ties for children were related to less income and rural residence, and higher opportunities associated with higher income and urbanization. A slight trend (at .10 significance level) was noted for an association between this resource and age of the girls among the preschool children. Low learning oppor— tunities seemed related more frequently to younger age among the girls and high opportunities to older age. Family Learning Opportunities Contingency tables for this resource Show signifi- cant relations between family learning and mother's edu— cation, family income, and residence (Table 20). In 65 per cent of the families with low learning opportunities, the mothers had less education. Evidence at the middle level was mixed, but at the upper level, 90 per cent of those in this category had mothers with more education. Families with both low and middle levels of learn- ing opportunities were in the lower income category with 67 per cent frequency, and those with higher levels were in the upper income group with 71 per cent frequency. 12U TABLE 20.—-Relations between level of family learning opportunities and selected family characteristics. nggiizg Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Mother's Educational Status1 3 Years More than or Less 3 Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 65 22 35 12 100 3U Middle U6 ll 5U 13 100 2U High 10 3 90 28 100 31 Income per Capita2 Leégoghan Mog$oghan Total Low 67 16 33 8 100 3U Middle 67 16 33 8 100 2U High 29 9 71 22 100 31 Residence3 Urban Rural Total Low 15 5 85 29 100 3U Middle U2 10 58 1U 100 2U High 61 19 39 12 100 31 3x, = 20.78, p < 001, C = .uu 2x2 = 10.81, p <.005>.001, c = .33 3x = 15.07, p <.001, c = .38 125 Rural residence was apparently more often associated with low family learning Opportunities (85 per cent of families at this level) than was urban residence with high family learning opportunities (61 per cent of those at this higher level). Child's Contacts The extent of social contacts maintained by pre— school children in the study was associated with family status in the areas of parental education, per capita income, and residence, as reported in Table 21. Eighty— one per cent of children with low social contacts and 60 per cent of those with middle levels had fathers with low educational status. Some 67 per cent of those who ex- perienced high social interaction had fathers who had achieved higher educational status. Low contacts were frequently related to having a mother of minimal education (for 62.5 per cent of child- ren at the low level). Moving from middle to higher levels was associated with increasing frequency to hav- ing mothers with more education (from 53 per cent of the middle level group of children to 93 per cent of child- ren with high contacts). Trends observed for relationships with income and residence were similar: low to middle levels of social contacts for preschool children were linked with less 126 TABLEEfls——Relations between level of child's contacts and selected family characteristics. 1 ggitgcis Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Father's Educational Statusl U Years More than or Less U Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 81 26 19 6- 100 32 Middle 60 18 U0 12 100 30 High 33 9 67 18 100 27 Mother's Educational Status2 3 Years More than or Less 3 Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 62.5 20 37.5 12 100 32 Middle U7 1U 53 16 100 30 High 7 2 93 25 100 27 Income per Capita3 Less than More than 2700 2700 ’TOtal Low ‘72 23 28 9 100 32 Middle 63 19 37 11 100 30 High 19 5 81 22 100 27 Residence“ Urban Rural Total Low 19 6 81 26 100 32 Middle 33 10' 67 20 100 30 High 67 18 33 9 100 27 1x - 1 6 — 2 - 3.9 , p <.001, C - .37 2x2 = 19.18, p <.001, c = .A2 3x2 = 18.7U, p <.001, c = .U2 “X = 1U.70, p <.001, C = .38 127 income and rural living; high contacts with more family income per capita and urban residence. Additionally, social contacts of the child tended to relate to his age (level of significance was .10). It appeared that older children more frequently had high levels of social interaction. Family Contacts This resource category was significantly associated only with status measures of parental education (Table 22). In comparison with the social contacts of the child, the strength of the associations as expressed in the contin- gency coefficients was not as pronounced. The degree of relation was reversed: that is, level of family contacts was more strongly related to the father's educational status, and the child's contacts to the mother's educa- tional status. Trends in percentages at the various levels and within the two educational categories (of father and mother) appeared to be somewhat similar for both family and child's social contacts. Descrlptive Use of Family Resource Patterns On page 129 is a profile of individual resource ratings for one household in the study, fictitiously named the Arias-Salazar family. It may be studied with reference to Table 2U in the Scoring Manual, Appendix C, 128 TABLE 22.-—Relations between level of family contacts and selected family characteristics. Family - Contacts Per cent N Per cent N Per cent N Father's Educational Statusl U Years More than or Less U Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 88 23 12 3 100 26 Middle 51 20 U9 19 100 39 High U2 10 58 lU 100 2U Mother's Educational Status2 3 Years More than or Less 3 Years' Total Schooling Schooling Low 62 16 38 10 100 26 Middle U1 16 59 23 100 39 High 17 U 83 20 100 2U 1x2 = 13.32, p <.005>.001, c = .36 2x = 10.UU, p <.01>.005, c = .32 129 family contacts pom moonsomop CH mpHHHanpm> mo appease child's 0 contacts family 9 learning child's 0 learning moonsommm k P SO 8 'w 0. d — d 11k «iny {is 513 hua hul nut CnP .COHpmospo ”oHHmonQ zHHEmm oq w .0 O l. H 30H 11m .1 m .1 : pHOOHs m cmHm 130 page 251. In that table are ratings which this family received on each of four resource dimensions for all re— source categories, then summed to the IRR in the profile. Patterning of resources for educability in this family is evidenced by graphing IRR achieved and then connecting these ratings to illustrate the consistency, or lack of consistency, among them. Considered separately, 3 IRR were in the area of low relation to educability, 5 in the middle level, and l, child's learning, at the high level. The range of IRR varied from 0 for care and ap— pearance, and task and work, to 6 for child's learning. While IRR range for the family was 6, the TRR, Total Re— source Rating, achieved was 28. Two of 3 environmental properties related to edu— cability (which guided selection of resource categories for the study), constriction and cognitive stimulation, were most affected by high inconsistency in range of IRR. Only the property of social interaction, indicated by social contacts, presented an appearance of consistency, although only the middle level of relation to educa- bility was reached. This family, with a middle level TRR and a high range of IRR, was representative of the group of fami- lies mentioned under hypothesis one, that group which made up U3 per cent of the total sample. This was a rural family, living in an agricultural workers' 131 community on a large coffee farm. It was a mixed occupa- tion family. José Alberto, father of the preschool child around whom the study questions were focused, and head of the household, worked as a "jornalero," or contracted day worker on the farm. A son, Manuel, worked with him; another, Rafael, worked in a factory. Other older child- ren contributed to family income by working during coffee harvest. Total family income for 1967 was 06936. With 1U members at the time, family income per capita amounted to ¢U95 or $7U.72 per year. The father had never attended school; his wife, Clara Luz, completed one year. Theirs was a nuclear family; although they had relatives on the same and nearby farms, they did not see them often enough to be classified as a modified-extended group. At the time of study, there was a new baby in the family; therefore, Oscar, the pre- school child, had 12 siblings at home. He was U years and 5 months of age when family observation began. Two siblings, Maria and Luisa, were also in the 3-to-6 pre- school age group; another in this age category had died recently. One older brother, Rafael, had completed six years of primary education at the elementary school on the "finca" grounds where the family lived. 132 Exploration of Prediction Potential of Family Resource Construct A tentative exploration was made of the use of TRR as a general indicator of family resources for educa- bility, and other measures, in a regression equation to predict a criterion measure (78; 83:315). The latter measure, called "family educational success," was opera- tionalized by presence or absence in each family of at least one sibling of the preschool child of the study who was old enough for the sixth grade of primary school, and who was presently in that grade (or who had already completed it or gone beyond). The exploratory hypothesis was that, given informa- tion about TRR and some family characteristics, perhaps one could predict family educational success with a fair degree of accuracy. In addition to TRR, variables selected were parental education, residence, income, and occupation. Together, these were used as independent variables in calculating coefficients for the linear re- gression equation by the method of least squares, with the educational success measure as dependent variable. A special question was whether the TRR measure would make a larger contribution to the equation than the others. Results were a multiple correlation coefficient of .509, significant at the .01 level. The R2 coefficient 133 was .259, indicating that the equation accounted for about 26 per cent of the variability in family educational suc- cess, leaving the rest due to other factors. Partial cor— relation coefficients were very low, varying from -.308 for father's education to .001 for mixed occupation. That for TRR was very small--.l67. Father's education, resi- dence, and mother's education were the measures which con- tributed most. An idea of the success of prediction was obtained by using the regression equation to estimate educational suc- cess for families in the sample with at least one child old enough to have reached the sixth grade. For the UU families who met this criteria, predictions were made in- correctly for seven, a 16 per cent error. On the basis of the findings, the tentative hypothesis was considered to have received some support; however, it was evident that the TRR measure did not contribute more to the pre- diction than the status measures under conditions of the experiment. CHAPTER V DISCUSSION, CONCLUSIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS The chapter discusses the findings and presents limitations, conclusions and implications of the re- search. Discussion of Findings Reliability In relation to scoring reliability an approximation of 89 per cent consistency was claimed for 36 resource measures, based on results of one-way analyses of vari- ance for two scoring treatments. In addition, there were also four resource scores for which significance of the difference of means for the scoring groups was less than the .10 level. One of these, UQN Care and Appearance, appears in Table 9. The other three were UQN dimension Child's Contacts, and AQN dimension scores for Child's and Family Contacts. If these trends are accepted as evidences of fur— ther possible inconsistencies in scoring, then 8 of 36 measures were unstable in two scoring treatments (which differed superficially). This indicates that a more 13U 135 conservative estimate of reliability of scoring would be 78 per cent. If more work is done with the conceptual and measurement system developed here, the inconsistencies for AQN and UQN dimensions should be examined closely. The resource categories which appear to require refine— ment are Child's Learning, Care and Appearance, and Child's Contacts. The task would be aided by study of the measures of central tendency and dispersion computed for each score for evidences of non-normal or curtailed distributions in Table 25, Appendix D (85). Dimensionality Given the caution needed with regard to interpreta— tion of results, tentative ideas are offered. From exami- nation of partial correlation coefficients of the four sets of scores with TRR (Table 10, page 105), the dimen— sionality of family resourcefulness for early educability may be specified to a degree. There appeared to be some evidence of differential composition of the dimensions, that is, of differential meanings for availability and utilization, and quantity and quality aspects of resource- fulness. Thus, support appeared for this property as described in the conceptual framework of the study (page 8, and U2 to U7). For interpretive purposes, the convention might be adopted to consider only the three highest partial 136 correlations in order of their magnitude of relation to TRR. In this context, quantity of family resources avail— able for educability might more rigorously be described as composed of, first of all, amount of family learning opportunities (excluding the preschool child); then amount of objects and activities for the child's play; then household space and the objects which are present within it. Quality of resources available within the home en- vironment in relation to attainment of educability prob— ably ought to include particular attention to variety of household spaces and objects first, and then to variety of the young child's learning and play opportunities. For quantity of resource utilization for educability, one would consider, first, aspects of the family's (not the child's) actual involvement in learning activities; then the child's usual clothing and food consumption; and finally the child's usual play activities. Quality of Resource Use Some observations are offered specifically about the fourth dimension of resourcefulness. Partial correla— tions of these scores with TRR need interpretation keeping in mind that the source of data was different from the other three dimensions. It is based exclusively on picture stimuli with focused questions. The resulting measure is 137 one of the mother's verbalized perceptions of means—end relationships, inferred as indicating qualitative aspects of resource utilization. Size of partial correlation coefficients suggests that these mothers defined the dimension in terms of task and work efforts of the child, an emphasis indicating possibly a primary cultural stress upon goals of early independence and responsible partici- pation. They then defined the dimension in terms of the child's learning and social contacts, indicating perhaps secondary cultural goals of early cognitive and social- emotional stimulation in the family. The multiple correlation coefficients were smallest for UQL, indicating that this perception—oriented dimen- sion may contribute less to family resource patterns for educability than any of the other three. However, it may be a most unique and important dimension, but one either inadequately conceptualized or measured, or one which had less importance in the particular families investigated than assumed. The data came exclusively from use of an instrument which may have been subject to unknown sources of non-validity or unreliability. It is pointed out that another investigator's ability to reproduce the scores was not ascertained. On the other hand, researchers have offered evidence that perception of means-end rela— tions is an established managerial concept (2, 6, U0), and that it is, in turn, commonly viewed as a component 138 of competence, a human resource of particular value in linking individuals and families to society (1, 26, 38). In research and discussions about the goal of early educability, this investigator noted a common reference to "the" preschool child of the family. As mentioned in the profile of the Arias-Salazar family, families may have more than one child in the preschool stage (even though only one is designated for research purposes). While the study concept is of "the preschool child," the family perception may be of "the children," with the mother holding a generalized, diffuse orientation towards ‘them as a group. With the Picture Question data, it was otaserved (but no significance attached to the fact at the tcime) that, although the investigator carefully and con- s:istently referred to the resource activities of the pic- tllres in terms of a specific child, even naming him and this age to his mother, she often spoke of "them" in her ariswers, not of "him" or "her." The usual pronouns were plural. Therefore, it may be that the concept of relations bei:ween the quality of a child's home activities and fatnire school success was either poorly specified and Opexrationalized in this study, or that it was not per— CEixred as important to many Costa Rican families: that 13, educability was not a culturally relevant goal. WltfljJfi larger family groups, parents may have diffuse, 139 non-specific orientations toward a child: he is only one of a group, and this impedes perception of him as needing access to certain amounts and qualities of resources at critical periods of development. Interdependent Functioning While the partial correlation technique was explored for evidence about the meaning of distinctive dimensions of resourcefulness, another aspect was also examined by correlational analysis. This was the interdependent patterning, or interrelated functioning of resources, discussed by many in the field of home management (15, 17, 18, 21, 25, 33). In this study, the patterning or clustering nature of family resources related to educa- bility was apparent from the matrix of resource ratings in Table 11. This finding is in agreement with the high commonality reported by Hess on similar resource measures, where all nine resources clustered on one principal fac— tor (11). That this is a differential patterning, with some resources contributing more strongly than others to the overall cluster of means in the family environment for the educability goal, is inferred from the sizes of the coefficients, and the fact that Child's Learning, a most specific resource category associated with future school 1U0 success for a child, was most often related to the other resource categories at .50 or above. Extent and Consistency A particular way of looking at resource quantity and quality is highlighted by findings relevant to hypothesis one, which supported the belief that level of resourceful- ness attained would relate to consistency in amounts of resources present for educability. There appeared to be several qualitatively distinct family environments when both extent and consistency of resources were examined: families in which the resource environment was very con- sistent but either very low or high on total resources; and families in which the resource environment was highly inconsistent but not high in total resources. This inter— pretation might be studied in light of the assumption of the economic view of resources that there is continuity and stability in handling resources and that there is an equilibrium between means and wants to be satisfied (32, 3U). This possible distinction about family environments could be explored further in light of Bloom's hypothesis about powerful environments being those which have a pervasive stability (U7). The effects of the first two environments may be more lasting than that of the third. Measurements on goal attainment, in this case, of 1U1 educability—related behaviors of the child himself, would be required both at the preschool age and after exposure to school influences, before a prediction could be sup— ported. Developmental Properties and Resources Physical constriction, cognitive stimulation, and social interaction, the three developmental properties proposed in the conceptual framework of the study for guiding selection of resource categories for educability (page 7 ), might be examined with reference to the re- sults of the chi square analyses. If resource categories are eliminated which showed three or less significant relations with family characteristics, then the five re— maining ones may be organized according to these proper- ties to judge whether, and how, each property was opera- tionalized in the families studied. In this context, the results provide evidence that constriction would refer to household space, and physical care and appearance; stimu— lation to child's play and learning, and interaction would mean child's social contacts. Resources, Status and Structure Chi square analysis supported the hypothesized rela— tionship between family resource patterns, measured by TRR, and status characteristics, measured by parental 1U2 education, income, and residence, but not occupation. The analysis did not support the hypothesized relationship with family structure measures, however. When the 9 re- source categories were considered separately, there were 28 significant relationships with the various status measures, and only 3 with structure measures. Categories showing the greatest number of significant relations, and therefore highest discriminating ability, with the family characteristics measures were: Space with 7 significant relations; Care and Appearance with 5; Child's Play, Learning and Contacts, all with U. The least discriminat— ing resource measures proved to be Child's Movement and Task and Work, and Family Contacts and Learning. Resource ratings in the contingency tables can be examined for information about the concept of levels of relation to educability. In addition, this would be necessary for the significant chi square tables in order ‘to take the next analytical step which is to collapse the tables to 2 by 2 size for determination of the source of clifference in each. As a guide to making decisions about know to combine the three resources levels, an inspection of trends is revealing. For all resource categories the evidence seemed to be that low and middle levels of re- sources were associated with larger family size, rural residence, less education for the father, and low income. .At‘the high resources levels, the evidence suggested the lU3 cluster of family characteristics of smaller family size, urban residence, more education for both parents, and higher income. There are two family characteristics for which these level trends apparently do not hold true. For mother's education, divided slightly lower than for father's educa- tion, there was some evidence of relation between the middle resources level and higher education. This may be a function of how the group was dichotomized for analysis. For occupation, the low and middle levels of resources seemed to be either agricultural or mixed occupation groups; while high resources levels related to non- agricultural occupations. The findings suggest that the resource measures may have use in specifying a more generalized concept such as the influence of family environment, defined as social status, upon human development. This specification is zin urgent current need, according to other researchers (1, 6, UU, U7, 50). Egadly Characteristics The chi square results could also be examined from the point of view of the family characteristics. Parental education measures were both related significantly at the Chosen .01 level with resource measures 8 times; residence and income had 7 such relationships. Occupation was lUU least important among the status measures with two cases of being significantly related to resources. Among family structure measures, only family size and number of sib— lings, similar measures, showed any significant relations, and the total was only three. It is of interest that little evidence was found in this study of preschool child- ren and the family environment of relations between age and sex of the child and the resource measures. Either the measures were too gross to detect differences or the culture placed little emphasis on this developmental stage, a speculation related to the previous discussion about the UQN dimension. This question is of importance in view of the emphasis in theory and research currently being placed upon stage- and sex-related influences in the fields of human development and education (9, ll, l2, l3, 1U). Also, it may be possible that the prediction of family educational success would be improved by addition. of significant structure measures, if such could be found. FamilylProfile What might be said about the profile of resources for the goal of educability in Oscar's environment? That there was physical constriction present is evident from the extreme rating (in comparison with other children of the sample) on his usual food and clothing consumption and routine physical care. Lack of stimulation was 1U5 suggested in the extreme rating (again compared to other children of the sample) for doing things for himself and helping with little tasks at home. Influencing factors might be found in the pattern of family characteristics (for example, very low family income and educational attainments of parents, measures found to relate to low levels of resources for educability). Others might be such factors as family size or number of similar age siblings, indicating competition for scarce resources with Oscar. In some ways, Oscar fared better compared to other children of the sample. He experienced only moderate physical constriction as far as level of household space and his own movements were concerned. He also experienced moderate amounts of stimulation through play, and social interaction through contacts with others. The ratings suggest that Oscar's environment for educability varied from low to moderately pervasive. It was neither highly supportive nor consistently negative in relation to attainment of the goal of his educability. Its effects upon the child might be controlled or mediated either by improvement in status levels of the family, by direct efforts in relation to the low and moderately rated resource categories, or by efforts with the child but totally outside the family, or a combination of Inethods. The status characteristics of most influence 1U6 and possibly ammenable to societal alteration might be parental education levels or family income. Perhaps some resource ratings could be examined for improvement pos- sibilities, fOr example, care and appearance for physical constriction, and either task and work or family learning for stimulation. These are especially suggested as all others were moderate to high in level compared with other families. Limitations The sample families were purposively selected. It is not known to what extent results based on this sample can be generalized to any pOpulation beyond it. Level of measurement attained was ordinal; however, analyses were carried out under the assumption of at least equal—appearing intervals. It is not known to what ex— tent the assumption was met. Other assumptions relevant to statistical models used were recognized but no asser- tion is made of rigorous compliance with them. The process of combining items into scores and transforming them into ratings meant loss of information from the data; however, this may have been balanced by a gain in comparability through development of ipsatized ratings, that is, normalized ratings for intra-societal use . 1&7 Size of multiple correlation coefficients must be cautiously interpreted because they may be spurious due to use of TRR as internal criterion. The items them- selves contributed indirectly to TRR. Without’further analysis sources of differences appearing in the con- tingency tables could not be specified with confidence. No attempt was made to directly measure and relate to the resource variables the educability-linked be— haviors of the children, which are output behavior vari— ables necessary for further specification of the input role of the resource categories as means for goal attain- ment. Although the conceptual framework and measurement model appeared to be applicable cross-culturally, no claim of this nature is made for all of the individual resource scores because they represent culturally modi- fied definitions which may be limited to the particular group of families studied. Conclusions . Within the confines of this study utilizing a pur- posive sample of 89 intact families with preschool children, distributed heterogeneously by family status and structure characteristics, and geographically located within a recognized developing region of Costa Rica, the following conclusions are warranted. 1148 The managerial—developmental framework for the con— struct family resource patterns for educability, ori— ginally conceptualized for, and applied in, one group of families, demonstrated applicability in another cultural group of families. There was evidence for content validity. It was provided by a process of establishing an adequate repre— sentative sampling of items for the resource categories, based upon practices observed in families, suggested by cultural informants and researchers, or the developmental- managerial literature reviewed. Evidence for construct validity included differ- ential meaningfulness for each of the four dimensions in terms of the resource categories which showed significant partial correlation coefficients. The evidence also indi- cated that the fourth dimension, UQL, may be unique in comparison with the others, AQN, AQL, UQN, because its multiple correlation coefficient with the Total Resource Rating was the smallest. It requires further study for refinement of the definition in terms of the culture and measurement. Additional evidence for construct validity indi- cated, by means of inspection of the intercorrelation matrix of nine resource ratings, that there was a pattern- ing or clustering of the measures, significant at the .01 level. When only substantial coefficients were 1&9 considered (over .50), all resource categories expect movement contributed to the cluster. Validation of the construct of family resource pat— terns for educability was further investigated by means of chi square analyses and the contingency coefficients for study of significant and substantial associations. An hypothesized relation between extent (Operationalized by levels of TRR) and consistency (measured by the range of IRR) of resource patterns was supported. Analyses using the general resource measure (TRR) and the individual resource measures revealed many signifi- cant relations with family status but very few with family structure characteristics. On the basis of the evidence the hypothesized association with status was considered supported; that with structure was not. An ordering of importance of the resources was suggested by secondary analyses: space, and care and appearance showed the most significant relationships; play, child's learning and con- tacts were intermediate; family learning and contacts were low; movement and task and work were lowest. There appeared to be potential for use of the conceptual—measurement system in descriptive family analy- sis, demonstrated by an illustrative profiling of a family in order to be able to describe it in comparison with other families. 150 In an investigation of concurrent validity, contribu- tion of the construct in a prediction system was found to be limited; however, due to the empirical and exploratory nature of the problem, no conclusion is warranted about the predictive potential of the construct without further investigation. Implications Theory Resource patterns for goal attainment appears to be a fruitful construct for a theory of management in the family, especially the aspects of dimensionality, inter- dependent patterning directed to specific family goals, and the qualities of extent and consistency of resources at critical individual development periods experienced within the family environment. Research The study tests a framework developed within one culture and finds it meaningful in terms of another. It suggests a possible approach to use in constructing a system for comparison within, and perhaps, between, cul— tural groups of families. Reliability and validity have been major foci of the study. The evidence for these criteria is supportive of the conceptual and measurement models; however, on the 151 basis of the present findings, refinement would be needed if the system is contemplated for other studies. The more significant measures, perhaps, were those better defined (space, learning, play) and not necessarily those most important for educability. Measurements on the children would be required for analysis in order to evaluate em— pirically the significance of the resources for educa- bility. Additionally, measurements of managerial be- haviors and value orientations might add significantly to predictability of goal achievement in the area of educability. Extension and/or refinement of the meaning of family structure is needed in order to reexamine its associations with TRR. It is suggested that birth order of the pre— school child, or age of parents, might be measures worthy of investigation. The results indicated that status over- whelms structure as a significant variable in relation to resources for educability; it may truly be most important, or there may be an interaction between status and struc- ture not uncovered in the study. Action Programs For possible application in programs carried out by (fiiange agents, there are suggested in the study both a cormeptual orientation, or way of looking at a problem, and a system of data collection, which emphasize the 152 relation between the family system and a societal institu— tion, the schools. Perhaps a simplified but more repre— sentative study might uncover some guidelines for action programs aimed at development of the parental resource as instrumental in guiding or mediating a goal such as edu- cability, within the family environment. An outcome of this type would be of benefit to families and significant to societal goals. If educability is a viable and important societal goal, then efforts in relation to it may need to be focused more on rural, lower income and less educated families, based on results of this study. It is a task which might receive the attention of extension workers or rural school teachers. Some inputs to families may be needed from outside institutions in order to raise overall TRR to moderate or high levels, for categories of resources such as space and family learning opportunities, for example. If the direction of causality can be assumed to be from status to family resource levels for educability, then limited improvements only can be made by families without inputs in the areas of parental education and family income. LITERATURE CITED 153 LITERATURE CITED Hagen Everett E. On the Theory of Social Change. Homewood, Illinois: Dorsey Press, Inc., 1962. Paolucci, Beatrice. "Contributions of a Framework of Home Management to the Teaching of Family Re- lationships." Journal of Marriage and the Family, XXVIII (August, 1966), 338-3A2. Magrabi, Frances M., and McHugh, Helen F. "Concepts in Teaching Family Finance." Journal of Home Economics, LV (February, 1963), 107-110. Schlater, Jean D. "The Management Process and Its Core Concepts." Journal of Home Economics, LIX (February, 1967), 93-98. Nichols, Addreen. "Person-Centered and Task- Centered Styles of Organization." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 196A. Liston, Margaret I. "Management in the Family as Social Process." Proceedings of Conference: Conceptual Frameworks: Process of Home Management. East Lansing: Michigan State' University, 196“. Fromm, Erich. The Revolution of Hope: Toward a Humanized Technology. New York: Harper and Row, 1968. ‘ Straus, Murray A. "Phenomenal Identity and Concep- tual Equivalence of Measurement in Cross- National Comparative Research." Journal of Marriage and the Family, XXXI (May, 1969), 233-239. Hess, Robert D.; Shipman, Virginia C.; and Jackson, David. "Early Experience and the Socializa— tion of Cognitive Modes in Children." Child Development, XXXVI (December, 1965), 869-886. 15A 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 155 Hess, Robert D., and Shipman, Virginia C. "Maternal Attitude Toward the School and the Role of Pupil: Some Social Class Comparisons." Teaching and Learning in Depressed Areas. Edited by A. H. Passow. New York: Teacher's College Press, 1967. Hess, Robert D.; Shipman, Virginia C.; Brophy, Jere E.; and Bear, Roberta Meyer. The Cognitive Environments of Urban Preschool Children. Chicago: Graduate School of Education, Uni— versity of Chicago, 1968. Hess, Robert D., and Bear, Roberta Meyer, eds. Early Education: Current Theory, Research, and Action. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company, 1968. Grotberg, Edith, ed. Proceedings of Research Semi- nars: Critical Issues in Research Related to Disadvantaged Children. Office of Economic Opportunity, Washington, D.C., 1968—1969. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1969. Schrimshaw, Nevin S., and Gordon, John E., eds. Pro— ceedings of International Conference on Malnu- trition, Learning, and Behavior. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Mass., 1967. Cambridge, Mass.: The M.I.T. Press, 1968. Deacon, Ruth E. "Home Management: Focus and Func— tion." Journal of Home Economics, LIV (November, 1962), 760-762. Deacon, Ruth E., and Maloch, Francille. "Proposed Framework for Home Management." Journal of Home Economics, LVIII (January, 1966), 31-35. Bell, Helen. "One Home Management Concept - Organi- zation." Proceedings of Conference on Teaching Home Management. Purdue University, Lafayette, Indiana, 1962. Washington, D.C.: American Home Economics Association, 1962. Walker, Florence S. "A Proposal for Classifying Self-Imposed Housekeeping Standards." Journal of Home Economics, LX (June, 1968), 456-560. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 2A. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 156 Mumaw, Catherine R. "Researching Organizational Patterns." Paper presented at Annual Meeting of the American Home Economics Association, Boston, Mass., June, 1969. Likert, Rensis. New Patterns of Management. New York: McGraw—Hill Book Co., 1961. Paolucci, Beatrice, and O'Brien, Carol. "Resources: the Tools of Management." Forecast for Home Economists, LXXVI (March, 1960), 21—22. Paolucci, Beatrice. "Plans for Strengthening Family Life: Implications for Home Management Re: Housing." Proceedings of Fifth Annual Land— Grant HousinggConference. University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minn., 1962. Baker, Georgianne R. "Patterning of Home Resources for Children's Early Cognitive Growth." East Lansing, Michigan, 1965. (Typewritten.) Nickell, Paulena, and Dorsey, Jean M. Management in Family Living. New York: John Wiley, 1968. Gross, Irma, and Crandall, Elizabeth W. Management for Modern Families. 2d ed. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, 1963. Spindler, George, and Spindler, Louise. "Research— ing the Perception of Cultural Alternatives: the Instrumental Activities Inventory." Con— text and Meaning in Cultural Anthropology. Edited by M. S. Spiro. New York: Free Press, 1965. Whiting, John W. M. "Resource Mediation and Learn- ing by Identification." Personality Development in Children. Edited by Ira Iscoe and Harold W. Stevenson. Austin, Texas: University of Texas Press, 1960. Longabaugh, Richard. "A Category System for Coding Interpersonal Behavior as Social Exchange." Sociometry, XXVI (September, 1963), 319-3AA. Clarkson, Geoffrey P. E. The Theory of Consumer Demand: A Critical Appraisal. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1963. 157 30. Hicks, J. R. Value and Capital. London: Oxford Uni— versity Press, 1957. 31. . A Revision of Demand Theory. London: Oxford University Press, 1956. 32. Schumpeter, Joseph A. The Theory of Economic Develop— ment. Translated by Redvers Opie. New York: Oxford University Press, 1961. 33. Fitzsimmons, Cleo. The Management of Family Re- sources. San Francisco: W. R. Freeman and Co., 1951. 3A. Lancaster, Kelvin. "Allocation and Distribution Theory: Technological Innovation and Progress: Change and Innovation in the Technology of Consumption." American Economic Review, LVI (May, 1966), lA-23. 35. Burk, Marguerite C. "Implications for Agriculture of Qualitative Changes in Food Consumption: U.S.A. Experience." Agr. Exp. Sta. Misc. Journal Series. Paper No. 1233. St. Paul: University of Minnesota, 1965. 36. Bronfenbrenner, Urie. "Motivational and Social Com- ponents in Compensatory Education Programs: Suggested Principles, Practices, and Research Designs." Proceedings of Research Seminars: Critical Issues in Research Related to Dis- advantaged Children. Edited by Edith Grotberg. Office of Economic Opportunity, Washington, D.C., 1968-1969. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1969. 37. Scheinfeld, Daniel R. "On Developing Developmental Families." Proceedings of Research Seminars: Critical Issues in Research Related to Dis- advantaged Children. Edited by Edith Grotberg. Office of Economic Opportunity, Washington, D.C., 1968-1969. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1969. 38. Sussman, Marvin B., and Brooks, Margaret W. "The Competence Concept." Case Western Reserve University, October, 1969. (Mimeographed.) 39. A0. A1. A2. A3. AA. “5. A6. A7. 158 Foote, Nelson N., and Cottrell, Leonard S. Identity and Interpersonal Competence: a New Direction in Family Research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1955. Hoyt, Elizabeth E. Choice and the Destiny of Nations. New York: Philos0phical Library, 1969. Baldwin, Alfred L.; Kalhorn, Joan; and Breese, Fay H. "The Appraisal of Parent Behavior." Psy— chological Monogpaphs, No. 299. LXIII, 1949. Gordon, Ira J. "Developing Parent Power." Proceed- ings of Research Seminars: Critical Issues in Research Related to Disadvantaged Children. Editied by Edith Grotberg. Office of Economic Opportunity, Washington, D.C., 1968-1969. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1969. Liston, Margaret I. "Interrelationship of Values and Decision-Making in Home Management." Proceedings of Conference on Values and Decision-Making in Home Management. Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 1955. Paolucci, Beatrice. "Home Management Education." Paper presented at XIV Conferencia Internacional Da Familia, International Union of Family Organizations, Rio De Janiero, Brazil, July, 1963. (Mimeographed.) Sussman, Marvin B. "Cross-National Family Studies: Some Conceptual Issues in Family-Organizational Linkages." Paper presented at Family Bureau— cracy session, 64th meeting of the American Sociological Association, San Francisco, Calif., November, 1969. Barry, Herbert, III; Child, Irvin L.; and Bacon, Margaret K. "Relation of Child Training to Subsistance Economy." American Anthrppolo- gist, LXI (February, 1959), 51—63. Bloom, Benjamin S. Stability and Change in Human Characteristics. New York: Wiley and Sons, Inc., 196A. H8. “9. 50. 51. 52. 53. 5A. 159 Wolf, Richard M. "The Identification and Measurement of Environmental Process Variables Related to Intelligence." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, 1964. Dave, Ravindrakumar H. "The Identification and Measurement of Environmental Process Variables that are Related to Educational Achievement." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, 1963. Hess, Robert D. "Parental Behavior and Children's School Achievement: Implications for Head Start." Proceedings of Research Seminars: Critical Issues in Research Related to Dis- advantaged Children. Edited by Edith Grotberg. Office of Economic Opportunity, Washington, D.C., 1968-1969. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1969. Hess, Robert D.; Shipman, Virginia C.; Brophy, Jere E.; and Bear, Roberta Meyer. Egg Cognitive Environments of Urban Preschool Children: Follow-Up Phase. Chicago: Graduate School of Education, University of Chicago, 1969. Baker, Georgianne, and Paolucci, Beatrice. "Family Resource Patterns for Early Educability: a Measurement Model Proposed for Cross-Cultural Applications." Proceedings of Conference on Imppoving Our Competencies in International- Intercultural Home Economics. Cornell Uni— versity, Ithaca, New York, 1969. Roy, Prodipto; Waisanen, Frederick E.; and Rogers, Everett M. The Impact of Communication on Rural Development. Paris: UNESCO, and India: National Institute of Community Development, 1969. Secretaria General de la OEA. Comité Interamericano de Desarrollo Agricola. Inventario de la Informacion Basica para la Programaci6n del Desarrolo Agricola en la América Latina: Centroamérica. Washington, D.C.: Uni6n Panamericana, 1965. 56. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 62. 160 United Nations, Economic Commission for Latin America. Education, Human Resources and De— velopment in Latin America. New York: United Nations (E/CN.12/800), 1968. Lombardo, Heraclio A. Analisis de una Economia Agricola Dentro de la Meseta Central de Costa Rica. Serie Planeamiento No. A. San José, Costa Rica: Instituto Interamericano de Ciencias Agricolas de la O.E.A., 1965. International Monetary Fund. Statistics Bureau. International Financial Statistics, XXIII (May, 1970), 92. Inter—American Development Bank. Social Progress Trust Fund Eighth Annual Report. Socio— Economic Progress in Latin America. Washington, D.C.: Inter-American Development Bank, 1968. Waisanen, Frederick B., and Durlak, Jerome A. Estudio Sobre Actitudes Hacia 1a Dinamica de Poblacion. Programa Inter-Americano de In— formaci6n Popular. San José, Costa Rica: American International Association for Economic and Social Development, 1966. Jordan, John W. Attitudes Toward Education and Phy- sicallnyisabled Persons in Eleven Nations. Research Report No. 1. East Lansing, Michi— gan: Latin American Studies Center, Michigan State University, 1968. Céspedes, Francisco S. "The Contemporary Educa- tional Scene in Latin America." Report of Eastern Regional Conference of Comparative Education Society: Challenges and Achieve- ments of Education in Latin America. Pan American Union, Washington, D.C., 196A. Chacon Jinesta, Oscar. Codigo de Educacion. San José, Costa Rica: Imprenta Trejos Hermanos, 1965. 63. 6A. 65. 66. 67. 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 161 Arroyave, Guillermo; Ascoli, Werner; and Schaefer, Arnold E. "The Nutrition Survey of Central America and Panama." Institute of Nutrition of Central America and Panama, Guatemala City, Guatemala, and Nutrition Section, Office of International Research, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md., 1967. (Mimeographed.) "Informe Preliminar Sobre 1a Encuesta Nutritional de Costa Rica." Institute of Nutrition of Central America and Panama, Guatemala City, Guatemala, 1967. (Mimeographed.) Williamson, Robert C. "Some Variables of Middle and Lower Class in Two Central American Cities." Social Forces, XLI (December, 1962), 195-207. Bock, E. Wilbur, and Iutaka, Sugiyama. "Maintenance of the Extended Family in Urban Areas: A Com— parative Study of Argentina, Brazil, and Chile." Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Council on Family Relations, Washington, D.C., October, 1969. Survey of Family Income and Expenditures, 1961. San José, Costa Rica: General Office of Statistics and Census, 1965. Household Income and Expenditure Statistics, 1950— 196A, No. 1. Geneva, Switzerland: Inter- national Labour Office, 1967. Fried, Jacob. "Picture Testing: An Aid to Ethno— logical Field Work." American Anthropologist, LVI (February, 195A), 95-99. Goldschmidt, Walter, and Edgerton, Robert B. "A Picture Technique for the Study of Values." American Anthropologist, LXIII (February, 1961), 26-A7. Hunt, Raymond G., and Smith, M. Estellie. "Cultural Symbols and Response to Thematic Test Mate- rials." Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality Assessment, XXX (December, 1966), 587—590. Kaplan, Bert. Studying Personality Cross—Culturally. New York: Harper and Row, 1961. 73. 7A. 75. 76. 77. 78. 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 162 Lindzey, Gardner. Projective Techniques and Cross- Cultural Research. New York: Appleton— Century-Crofts, 1961. Murstein, Bernard 1. "New Thoughts About Ambiguity and the TAT." Journal of Projective Techni— ques and Personality Assessment, XXIX (June, 19657. 219—225. Parker, Seymour. "Ethnic Identity and Accultura— tion in Two Eskimo Villages." American Anthrppologist, va1 (April, 196M), 325—3u0. Kiel, Donald F. "One—Way Analysis of Variance with Unequal Number of Replications Permitted (UNEQl Routine)." STAT Series Description, No. 13, Agricultural Experiment Station. East Lagsing, Michigan: Michigan State University, 19 7. Kerlinger, Fred N. Foundations of Behavioral Re- search. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Ruble, William L.; Kiel, Donald; and Rafter, Mary E. "Calculation of Least Squares (Regression) Problems on the LS Routine." STAT Series Description, No. 7, Agricultural Experiment Station. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, 1969. McNemar, Quinn. Psychological Statistics. 3rd ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1962. Guilford, J. P. Psychometric Methods. 2d ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., Inc., 195“. Schwinn, Peter M. "Data Plotting and Curve Drawing on the Calcomp Plotter (PLOTXY Routine)." STAT Series Description, No. 16, Agricultural Experiment Station. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, 1966. Lesgold, Alan M. "Analysis of Contingency Tables." Computer Institute for Social Science, Techni- cal Report No. IA. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, 1968. Walker, Helen M., and Lev, Joseph. Statistical In- ference. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1953. 8A. 85. 163 Siegel, Sidney. Nonparametric Statistics. New York: Ball, McGraw-Hill Book Co., Inc., 1956. Frederick J.; Ruble, William L.; and Kiel, Donald F. "Calculation of Basic Statistics on the BASTAT Routine." STAT Series Descrip— tion, No. 5, Agricultural Experiment Station. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State Uni- versity, 1969. APPENDICES 16A APPENDIX A INSTRUMENTS Preliminary Questionnaire Occupation and Income Survey Resources Inventories Picture Questions 165 Codigo de la Familia Nifio Sexo Instituto Interamericano de Ciencias Agricolas de la O.E.A. Centro de Ensefianza e Investigacién Turrialba, Costa Rica INVESTIGACION SOBRE RECURSOS FAMILIARES Y ACTITUDBS EDUCATIVAS -0- Cédigo del INCAP Provincia Canton Comunidad Direccién VISITAS A LA FAMILIA Fecha Hora Tiempo Notas PREGUNTAS PRELIMINARES: (A algfin adulto, miembro de la familia. Llene las preguntas con asterisco (t) antes de la primera entrevista). t1. Estoy buscando a (Padres del nifio prescoiar) Usted es ... .--... .........-.._..... '.—--I—— -... ~ . .. . .. . .. . -.. o- - . .- .~ . — . -~ av. ... - .- ....... VA . . . . . -... ls- .. .. . .. . . - . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . ... .. - .. . . . . .. . .. .. ...-.. - .- - .—...... 1 . . .-. .. . . . --..--- .... . _.- —-. ... ..-. - . ... a» - ..." . .. . -..-. .... . ..--- -» . .... .- .... .. - .». — -- . ,-. . .. -.. . v ... . u ... - .q.. -. .... -. -.-» - ...... ...- .. .... . . .. - . ..e. —- I . ., -... . . - v . ... ..... . . .... . ..-». — - -— ‘ . . .. . . ‘ .a- . ... .- . . H -v . -.. A . ..- - _ ~ 7-- ...-.... .. .. . ..-.-. -..a- -.-... . .....- . --.. .. .... - ... ,.... . . .- .. . ur— . .__. . ,. .‘ ..- . ~ . . .. ,. . ... .. - _ » .. .. .— , . 1 .. . . . .... . , . . . . .. . H. . -.. . - --- . ...... ... . .. . V- . . 4 .. ... .. w " .. , . . . . .. , . . . . . . . _ ..... .. _ .....- . .. . . .... . . . ‘ u ' _ h. .. , .... .. , . . ...- . .. .. - ... .. .. .. . . . . ... . . .. . . . . .. .,, . .- .. . - .. . ‘ A _ , .- .. . . , . .. .... . .. -. . . . . . . w... . .. ., . . 7 . .. .. . . . . .. ... . . .. . - .... .. . . . -.. . . . . . .. . - .-.-.. .A . . . . . . . . .— . -_ . ... . ‘ I. ~-- . ... . . .. . .. . - .. .-. . .. . . .- .. ... .1 . . .. . _ . - . .... _. ..4 ..-... . . — .....-u- .-..l- ...... .. ... .. —. .. u ., , .. .. ._ _. .. ..- ‘ .— . ..--s .- ..“... .. ~5...-—~<~. 1- .. --&- - . -~ - - -- - ~» . ... -- - “ . , . I , ~ .. . . . . .~ 4 . 7. - l' ‘ . . ‘ . . r , ,: , ‘ ’ . . . . ... . . . .....-‘...- . . ... .... . -- ‘ . ' .. . ' ~ . . I . ‘7 . . . J . ‘_. .1 J \ x . \ . .. ..-...... ., . - , . . .. .... . .... .... .... .. .. ... .... . _ .. .. . ....-. - a ~w‘-‘.-.‘ ...~--- ' “ - . ¢ — ._.- .. ... ....-. - . -... ... - ‘ ...--. - .-. .... -. . v.‘ - u— .. . ~ —- -. o . ... - - . ~ Vive en esta casa e1 nifio (o nifios): Si No Si No Si No (De el nombre (o nombres) del nifio (nifios) que tiene entre tres y seis afios de edad, segun e1 INCAP). Si dice que NO, pregfintele: Dénde vive e1 nifio? Ahora, continue con las preguntas hasta el punto que se pueda y después busque a1 nifio en la otra direccién. Ahora este nifio tiene afios? Si No (De la edad segfin e1 INCAP, pero para este afio). Si dice NO, preguntele: Cuéntos afios tiene e1 nifio? (Si no tiene entre 3-6 afios, tormine 1a entrevista). Cuél es la fecha de nacimiento de ? Los padres de son: ? Si No ? Si No 9 Si No (De los nombres de los padres segun 1a lista del INCAP). Si responde NO, preguntele: Quiénes son los padres? Viven ambos padres toda la semana, en esta casa, con Si No '0 . a u..-- " -.. ...—p .. . _.. . 0 . .. .. \ -». . . -. ... --. . .- ... .. , ... ...-- --.—.....—.. h... «s ...-..-- .u .- -. ...... 4......“ -- . . . 1 - . v...- s . . 1 . ‘ , u . . -‘ . . 4 onw‘..- — ..- , . , . . . .- .. m. _ . -. . .1. . a . . .. -.- ~§M _ ... 0.» -~ 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. l6. 17. 18. 19. (NOTA: Si responde NO, pregunte si ellos viven parte de la semana en la casa. Si responde que uno u otro, o ambos de los padres no viven ni un dia en la casa, termine la entrevista.) Ya empezé a ir a1 kinder? Si No A la escuela? Si No (NOTA: Si responde SI a la escuela, termine 1a ontrevista). Ahora, si queda mas de un sujeto para el estudio, seleccione el que tenga la edad mas aproximada de 4 a 5 afios para enfocar e1 estudio y 1as preguntas siguientes. CONTINUAR LA ENTREVISTA CON LA MADRE 0 EL PADRE DE ESTE NINO Y PRESENTE LA CARTA QUE EXPLICA EL ESTUDIO. Cual es el lugar de nacimiento de ustedes, los padres de ? Padre Madre Durante cuénto tiempo han vivido ustedes y en esta casa? Dénde Vivian ustedes antes de vivir en esta casa? Por cuénto tiempo? Para e1 padre de , cuél fue el primer trabajo que tuvo? Cuél es la ocupacién actual de él? Desde cuéndo? Cuél fue 1a ocupacién anterior a la presents? Se considera que el padre de es el jefe de esta familia? Si No Si dice NO, preguntele: Quién es el jefe? Qué ocupacién tuvieron los abuelos de ? (NOTA: Abuelo, no abuela). De parte del padre De parte de la madre Los abuelos, saben (sabian) leer y escribir? De parte del padre: Si No De parte de la madre: Si No - r- . - o, ‘ . , 1 .. I" ...»-.. . . . - .- . . .... . .... ~ ‘ . . . . . 7 . x , . . ‘ .. . . " ' . . .... .... . . - »—. .... - . . .-.. .. . . .... . .. ..- . -.. "....-. ~ "- -..... -........-_.» 0’ .--. . ... “...... .. -.. _ ._ . ' . ..... _ ... .. ..... .. ... o-«--........_.~-~.¢ ...-. ....--. . ._ ... .. . - - .—~ . . - .... - - - . . - -- .- . . \ . . ~ ‘ ‘ . , ’ ' . ...--.‘-- ..- ... ... -.—-.....--...- _ q¢Vv-'~-.> y 1 -.~- ...... ... .- ...- >- -..... ...-...- .,. - A . 4r .-- . -... . . ‘ . ‘ ' ' . 1 . ‘ . l 7 a . ‘ D - . . . ...... . . .. .. -.-..u .... .-.. -. - a. . ., - -.. ... . - -. . - .- . . .. .. . .. , .. . . . .. ...-.. ... ..-—- ..-... .. - .-- . m. ...... \ l , , , . .. . ..- --- . ~ , -.~'- . .-.“-..rv. .... ..-._ .... ... ,_ ..__.. - .. ' . - ...-- .. ..-... ~ - . .~. - ... .a.-. .... . - - . . . ...... - . . .--.A-.——.. . -.-.. - . 3 . ‘ . .. - ~ '. . ' n . ‘ - .. . ) . . ~- ‘ . ”cu .‘.--.-m ..y 4 . - .. u - . . .. .o .- .. . . ’ ‘. . - ' ' u.-. .-- . -.. ...-..-..71. ., .. ...-- -- ..- ,- .. -..-- . , ~ _ . . n . . , , -..n... .---....-. ..—.....7. .-..... ' ' . s ’ ' . ‘ . . . t V. ~ -- ...— .-~...a—.. . . . . . .1 . ‘ . ' . . .. ,. -- -. . ..--.. . .. - -... ..k‘.-. _.___,__, .. . . «...—.- .-.... u.“ . \ I v ' ‘ . ' ‘ / ' l . ‘ P . .. . I - _,, . . . _ .. . . ' . ' " ""‘ " ' " -.-... a . .-‘ .V.» 20. 21. 22. 23. Qué afio o grado escolar mas alto terminaron los abuelos? De parte del padre De parte de la madre Piensa enviar a a la escuela primaria? Si No Si dice SI: Qué distancia hay entre la escuela y la casa? Como esta la carretera 0 via (material y condicién) entre la escuela y la casa? .n._.-~.-- - . U o....- . . ”.... ...-~~¢-*- y» . .-.-.o ...... s»-‘ .... ..., .-.- mr. firm» Hem szzwwom bm H» fisSHbe K oewom saw m>u zosuwm mm vs wmsmonm mmwmowmb con mH ammo mmxo mama pcmnm mmonm UHHQ psmam Poona cameo o mmo mmOOHmH 5mm mpwo mbuoammow wHHBmHHm mmozuamHHm dcwamowmu mm vs omosmHm mmcomowmb mawowoumw L I u ,l 11...; 11" .l .I. 1.700 "I ... HP. Hm. PM“ Hr. Hm. Gowgmfl\uvmmo| figmmupmm q amuse Hmmm Instituto Interamericano de Ciencias Agricolas de la O.E.A. Centro de Ensefianza e Investigacién Turrialba, Costa Rica INVESTIGACION SOBRE RECURSOS FAMILIARES Y ACTITUDES EDUCATIVAS OCUPACION E INGRESOS FAMILIARES Georgianne Baker Educadora para el Hogar Economia y Ciencias Sociales FAMILIA INSTRUCCIONES Si es posible, hable con el jefe de la familia sobre 1as siguientes preguntas. Tenga cuidado de obtener la informacién no solamente de la ocupacién e ingresos de él, sino de todas 1as demés personas que contri- buyeron trabajo, pagado o no, durante e1 afio pasado; es decir, 1a esposa, los hijos, y cualesquiera otras personas que vivan en la casa. El periodo que cubren 1as preguntas es entre enero y fines de diciembre de 1967. No olvide averiguar si e1 trabajo actual de ellos es igual o diferente 31 del afio pasado. Lea 1a siguiente introduccién a la persona que con— testaré 1as preguntas: QUIERO CONVERSAR CON USTED SOBRE LAS PERSONAS DE LA FAMILIA QUE TRABAJARON DURANTE EL ANO PASADO Y LAS ENTRADAS (INGRESOS) QUE DIERON A LA FAMILIA. LE AGRADECERE MUCHO QUE ME CONTESTE ALGUNAS PREGUNTAS PARA QUE YO PUEDA ENFOCAR MEJOR LOS RECURSOS DE SU FAMILIA, QUE PROBABLEMENTE CONTRIBUYERON A HACER POSIBLES LAS ACTIVIDADES DE SU NINO, . TENGO INTERES PRINCI- PALMENTE EN LOS TRABAJOS DEL AND 1967, DE USTED Y DE CUALES- QUIERA OTRAS PERSONAS QUE TRABAJARON. NADIE QUE USTED CONOZCA VERA SUS CONTESTACIONES.- Ewes H $3931 ewbwbmo anbzem H0©N ponObmm no Hm waHHHm ammo H. mo Hm HWBHHHm. cam vcumobmm «HmdmumHOb mecbm 20 Hmmn Hw. onmwmm Hcmwos Hom Hr. ocmuwo Um >ZHZ>HMm cdwbzam mew Hw. mo. NH. mm. mw. NA. mm. om mw0bcoaom szzshmm mm. mo. ma. mo. mo. wH. mm. M>2HEH> m2 Mb >20 Homq Us osmH cwaHBo won osmuawm eoamH awwo wonmoumw ammo pus «comm won Hmowuwnow Wouwoaow won mmow mmo ww. bppswwmu ur. pHpcwme mm. bpnswpms wm. bwncwwmw wu. wousHomoHObmm Abo Huowcwamm oh wow memHmomv M mmcHumHaom Amwmawwwomowowomv wm. mmsmbowmm no somoowom Awwmmwmsom. mm muHBmHmm am Bmpcwbmawm mm «Mmuawm «\o maHHHoHom Hmowuwao was How ocmsaom mecwwwaom no Hm -. ...- .- . . . I . .. . . . . . . I ' .I‘ . .. _. . - ----.... .-. .....--. -.. .. . ,. . .. ...... , . , .. . . . - I ....- a.... >- -.. - - - l . . . . . . ., . . ll 7. Qué juguetes hay que el nifio disfruta especialmente? 12 INVENTARIO DE RECURSOS: Parte I: EL NINo .8. PATRON DE CONTnCTOS SOCIALES DIRECTOS RESUMEN: Disponibilidad: Cantidad Utilizacién: Cantidad FAMILIA CLASIFICACION: Calidad 123l+5 Calidad Pregunta ' En qué actividades y celebracio- nes de la familia participa e1 nifio? Qué hace el nifio en estas actividades? Con qué frecuencia participa e1 nifio? Qué actividades hace e1 nifio con nifios pequefios o bebés? Con qué frecuencia? Qué actividades hace el nifio con otros de su edad (sus iguales)? Hay algo especial que le gusta a1 nifio hacer con ellos? Con qué frecuencia lo hace? Tiene el nifio contactos con a1- gunos adultos extrafios? Qué? Con qué frecuencia? 5. Participa e1 nifio en algunos gru- pos grandee (como la iglesia)? Cuél es la participacién de él? Con qué frecuencia? 13 FAMILIAfiV r- r . INVENTARIO DE RECURSOS: Parte II: LA FAMILIA CL“SIFIC“CION- 1 2 3 4 5 7. PATRON DE APRENDIZAJE INDIRECTO RESUMEN: Disponibilidad: Cantidad Calidad Utilizacién: Cantidad Calidad Pregunta PARA USTED Y EL RESTO DE LA FAMILIA: 1. Le gusta a alguien en la familia leer algunas cosas? Qué? Quién? Con qué frecuencia? 2. Le gusta a alguien de la familia oir e1 radio 0 mirar el televisor? Qué cosas eye 0 mira regu- larmente durante la semana? Quién lo hace? Tiene un radio 0 tele- visor siempre en la casa? 3. Le gusta a alguien la mfisica? A quién? Oye la mfisica? Toca algfin instrumento? Cuél y con qué frecuencia? #. Va alguien a lugares Como el mu- seo, biblioteca, zoolégico, cine, parque o la plaza? A cuél? Quién? Con qué frecuencia? 5. Ademés de éstos, ha hecho alguien viajes fuera de la comunidad? A dénde? Quién? Con qué fre- cuencia? 6. Usa alguien los servicios del Banco? Para qué? Qnién? Con qué frecuencia? .. ...-.. ... .. —. --.- a n . 4 .... u 4 ..-. ... . do... - . —.-.- ...... ...- -~- - _ - n -. .- .- .. ... -A . “A . .l . ..-. w- < :- .. » . . .c~..——- -.——-_—-. .- . .- 0A9- ...—... -... i..— . .-.-.... n.- .. . ...,, .—--. --- “..-... . ~— ‘- ~n .. - - - ...- . . .__.. .. .-- -.. . A ....-n-.-._.. .7 ». 00‘.- . w-o.-. ‘ no ......uu ”..-, .~.n .... ..-..-..,.. ,-- - .-i. ...-.— . ....— 21 '7. Qué distancia recorre el nifio fuera del hogar y el vecindario inmediato? Con qué frecuencia? 8. Qué uso de autobfis, automévil, bicicleta, caballo, reccrrido a pie, hay por parte del nifio? AID/EE—427/68 GBaker/iss 1 April 1968 ».....a... .-.v‘..-, -. _ ~w< 4......” . ._ . -. . . ...-.........- ...a...._,r..-.--....-.. V, . . .-.-.- ....._.._.. . . r . - z A ,. _ l ‘ . . r.. - v ' ' -“ ‘ ‘ ‘ G . u, .v' I ,. -‘ . . ~ ‘ , P , . .o , . c . o .‘ ' 1 o .. . . a . . . .. .......- . A a .i .- . _. ......-.. . .i .M . . . . - . .W .. .-_-. V -.- . . . ‘ F -.....r - ..-. . - .c .-.. ...... “.-..-. .... .. W... ‘. .-..-- .m‘.....-‘-. .... .. . .. .. ....r .. 198 Instituto Interamericano de Ciencias Agricolas de la OEA Centro de Ensefianza e Investigacion Turrialba, Costa Rica INVESTIGACION SOBRE RECURSOS FAMILIARES Y ACTITUDES EDUCATIVAS PREGUNTAS PARA LOS DIBUJOS DE ACTIVIDADES SOBRE EL USO DE LOS RECUBSOS FAMILIARES Georgianne Baker Educadora para el Hogar Economia y Ciencias Sociales INSTRUCCIONES Hay nueve (9) Juegos de dibujos. Cada uno cor- responde a cada uno de los patrones de recursos (véase e1 Inventario de Recursos). El proposito de los dibujos es usarlos como estimulos visuales para obtener ideas adicionales y opiniones personales de la madre, ademas de sus respuestas directas a1 Inventario. El intercambio entre el entrevistador y la madre se graba en una cinta magnetofonica. Asi, se puede considerar que estas respuestas verbales a material semejante del Inventario pueden servir como control 0 "check" de la com- prensién por parte del entrevistador del idioma y palabras de la madre. Se sugiere que se use este instrumento con la madre antes del Inventario 0 al final de la recoleccion de todos los datos, pero no immediatamente después del Inventario. ' Primero, lea 1as ideas de la Introduccion a la madre. Después, muéstrele el primer Juego de dibujos y expliquele la accion o acciones que se ven. Hagale 1as preguntas que pertenecen a este Juego de dibuJos en particular. No agregue mas preguntas especificas, pero puede preguntar "Y- qué mas?" o algo asi. Continue asi con los otros grupos de dibujos. Tenga cuidado de grabar toda la conversacion y. revise después para saber si la cinta esta bien. INTRODUCCION AQUI TENEMOS DIBUJOS EN QUE HAY NINOS PEQUENOS, QUE TIENEN MAS O MENOS LA MISMA EDAD QUE ; ES DECIR, ANOS. EN LOS DIBUJOS VEMOS AL NINO, ALGUNAS VECES A SU MAMA, A SUS AMIGOS U OTROS MIEMBROS DE LA FAMILIA, U OTROS ADULTOS. VEMOS ACTIVIDADES COMO LAS QUE PUEDEN OCURRIR EN CUALQUIER FAMILIA, EAL COMO LA~SUYA. YO QUIERO QUE USTED, COMO MADRE DE UN NINO DE ___ ANOS, ME DIGA SUS IDEAS Y REACCIONES A LAS ACTIVIDADES PRESENTADAS EN LOS DIBUJOS. USAMOS ESTA GRABADORA DURANTE NUESTRA CONVERSACION PARA QUE YO PUEDA RECORDAR MEJOR SUS IDEAS DESPUES. 199 NINO ACOSTANDOSE, COMIENDO (Patron No. 3: Cuidado y Apariencia Personal) A la madre: "HAY ALGUNAS ACTIVIDADES QUE LOS NINOS HACEN DIARIAMENTE; ACTIVIDADES QUE CONTRIBUYEN A SU CREDIMIENTO Y SALUD FISICA Y QUE DEMANDAN LA ATENCION DE LA MADRE MUCHAS VECES. POR EJEMPLO, AQUI HAY UN NINO ACOSTANDOSE Y OTRO EMPEZANDO A COMER." Preguntas: l. Qué pensamientos o preocupaciones podria la madre tener con las comidas, el descanso, la limpieza 0 el vestuario de su nifio? 2. Qué esta diciendo la mama a su nifio en estos dibujos? 3. Le parece que nifios que actuan como estos dos tendran éxito en la escuela cuando empiecen? Por qué? 201 NINO TRABAJANDO, HACIENDO TAREAS (Patron No. 5: Tarea y Trabajo) A la madre: "AQUI HAY NINOS HACIENDO TRABAJOS o TAREAS PEQUENAS PARA LA FAMILIA. RECUERDE QUE SON NINOS DE MAS o MENOS ____ ANOS, COMO . ESTE NINO ESTA CUIDANDO A SU HERMANITA QUE ESTA LLORANDO: ESTE OTRO ESTA COMPRANDO PAN. ESTE ESTA COMIENDO -- QUE ES UNA TAREA QUE TODOS LOS NINOS DEBEN DOMINAR, PERO AQUI LA NINA HA TENIDO UN ACCIDENTE." Preguntas: l. Qué otras tareas o trabajos pueden hacer los nifios pequenos? 2. Qué hace.la mama cuando su niho pequefio tiene problemas o accidentes durante suS intentos de aprender 1as tareas? 3. Piensa usted que nifios como éstos van a tener éxito en las actividades futuras en la escuela? Por qué? 203 MOVIMIENTOS DEL NINO (Patron No. 2: Movimiento Fisico) A la madre: "VEMOS AHORA A UNOS NINOS REALIZANDO ACCIONES FISICAS: UNO ESTA CORRIENDO Y JUGANDO VIGOROSAMENTE AL FUTBOL. OTRA ESTA SENTADA DENTRO DE LA CASA, SIN HACER NADA. Y OTRA NINA ESTA SALIENDO DE LA CASA HACIA UN LUGAR EN EL VECINDARIO." Preguntas: 1. Piensa usted que estos nifios actfian en forma muy parecida a nihos de edad prescolar? Por qué? 2. De estos nifios, cual esta actuando mas parecido a ? Como? 3. De estos tres nifios, cual piensa usted que podré ir a la escuela con buen éxito? Por qué? 2014 ,\ VIII I {ml/0’1" ;//,:/, U 205 NINOS USANDO EL ESPACIO Y OBJETOS (Patron No. l: EspaciOIC ' A la madre: "AQUI HAY DIBUJOS QUE REPRESENTAN A NINOS PRESCOLARES USANDO ALGUNAS COSAS DE LA CASA. HAY UN NINO EN LA SALA ARREGLANDO EL RADIO. HAY UNA NINA EN LA COCINA Y ELLA YA HA PUESTO ALGO EN EL REFRIGERADOR." Preguntas: l. Qué cosas de la casa 1e gusta usar o trata de usar el nifio pequefio? 2. La mama de estos nifios esta contenta de que sus nifios realicen actividades como éstasdentro de la casa? Por qué? 3. Le parece que los nifios queestan acostumbrados a usar las cosas de la casa son aquellos que tendran éxito cuando vayan a la escuela? Por que? 206 207 APRENDIZAJE DEL NIfio (Patron No. 6: Aprendizaje Directo) A'la madre: "TENEMOS AHORA UNOS NINOS QUE NO TIENEN EDAD SUFICIENTE PARA IR A LA ESCUELA PERO QUE, SIN EMBARGO, ESTAN TRATANDO DE "APPRENDER": UN NINO QUE QUIERE "ESCRIBIR"; UNA NINA QUE PREGUNTA ALGO A SU MAMA; Y UN NINO TRATANDO DE PINTAR Y QUE HA DEJADO CAER LA PINTURA?" Preguntas: 1. En la familia, hacen los pequehos usualmente cosas para aprender, como en la escuela, por ejemplo? 2. Qué hace la mama de la familia sobre estas actividades de su nifio? 3. Cree usted que actividades como éstas tendran importancia para nifios der3 hasta 6 afios, que todavia son demasiado pequefios para ir a la escuela? Por qué? 209 JUEGOS DEL NINO (Patron No. A: Juego) A la madre: "HABLEMOS AHORA DE NINQS JUGANDO. COMO EJEMPLOS, AQUI TENEMOS NINOS DE ANOS DE EDAD, COMO , JUGANDO DE CASITA. Y AQUI VEMOS A LA MAMA DANDOLE A SU NINA ALGO PARA USAR COMO JUGUETE." Preguntas: l. Qué actividades de Juego son mas comunes para los nihos pequefios? 2. Qué cosas podria ofrecerles la mama para Jugar? 3. Le parece que a los nifios que Juegan mucho cuando son pequefios les gustara la vida escolar? 211 CONTACTOS SOCIALES DE LOS NINOS (Patrén No. 8: Contactoerociales Directos) A la madre: "AHORA PENSEMOS EN LAS OPORTUNIDADES QUE TIENEN LOS NINOS PARA CONOCER, Y HACER ACTIVIDADES, CON OTRAS PERSONAS. HASTA AHORA HEMOS VISTO A ALGUNOS NINOS PRESCOLARES CON SU MAMA, CON EL PANADERO, CON UNA HERMANITA Y CON SUS AMIGOS. Y AQUI HAY: UNA NINA CON SU PAPA, UN NINO CON SU TIO Y ABUELOS, Y OTRO CON UN EXTRANO - TAL VEZ UN MAESTRO." Preguntas: l. Hay ocasiones especiales cuando el niho pequeho podria tener oportunidad de contacto con otras personas? 2. De los contactos sociales, cuales serian mas parecidos a las actividades de nifios de afios de edad? 3. Podrian contribuir las experiencias sociales del nifio a su éxito en la escuela, mas tarde? Por qué? 213 OTRAS PERSONAS DE LA FAMILIA: APRENDIZAJE (Patrén 7: Aprendizaje IndirectO) A la madre: "HEMOS TERMINADO DE DISCUTIR LAS ACTIVIDADES DE LOS NINOS. PERO QUIERO TENER SU REACCION A DOS GRUPOS DE DIBUJOS QUE REPRESENTAN A OTRAS PERSONAS DE LA FAMILIA. EN PRIMER LUGAR, AQUI HAY MIEMBROS DE UNA FAMILIA ENTRANDO A UN EDIFICIO PARA VISITARLO; Y AQUI ELLOS ESTAN LEYENDO Y ESCRIBIENDO. TAL VEZ ELLOS ESTEN APRENDIENDO MUCHAS COSAS." Preguntas: l. Puede nombrar otras actividades que esta familia podria realizar Si quiere aprender? 2. Con qué frecuencia piensa usted que esta familia realiza actividades que podemos llamar "educativas"? 3. Cuando los nifios de esta familia vayan a la escuela, cree usted que seran mejores alumnos que los nifios de una familia que tiene pocas oportunidades de aprender? 215 OTRAS PERSONAS DE LA FAMILIA: CONTACTOS SOCIALES (Patr6n 9: Contactos Sociales Indirectos) A la madre: "OTRA VEZ VEMOS A OTRAS PERSONAS DE UNA FAMILIA, PARTICIPANDO AHORA EN ALGUNOS ACTOS SOCIALES. AQUI ESTAN LOS HERMANOS DEL NINO PRESCOLAR HABLANDO CON UN AMIGO. Y TAMBIEN HAY MIEMBROS DE LA FAMILIA ESCUCHANDO UNA CHARLA EN UNA REUNION." Preguntas: l. En-qué otras actividades sociales puede participar esta familia? 2. Con qué otras personas de la comunidad podria tener contactos la familia? 3. Los nifios de familias con muchos contactos sociales tendran major o menor éxito en la vida escolar? APPENDIX B SCORING MANUAL 217 SCORING MANUAL Purpose: The manual provides instructions for com- puting scores and subsequent ratings for nine Resource categories. The measures are derived from a variety of data: mothers' anSwers to questions on Resource Inven- tories, to questions about visual stimuli in the form of drawings of Resource-related activities (called Picture Questions), as well as written observations of the inves- tigator. Illustrations of scoring are provided where they contribute to a general understanding of procedures. Preparation of data: To increase objectivity in scoring, all family identification is removed from both Resource Inventory and Picture protocals, code numbers assigned, and then protocals are assembled into groups by code number and Resource category. Order of scoring: Because the scoring procedure is developed by examination of the data, it is essential to identify ambiguities in operational definitions and inter- pretations early in the scoring process. Therefore, two groups of ten randomly selected families are first scored and problems discussed and resolved by the scorer and another person familiar with the data and scoring 218 219 procedure. Then all families are scored on each Resource category, one category at a time. The scores: This procedure results in thirty—six scores divided into groups of four scores which pertain to nine Resource categories. Each group of scores is in— tended to operationalize four dimensions of a Resource: Availability-Quantity (AQN), Availability-Quality (AQL), Use-Quantity (UQN) and Use-Quality (UQL). Each score is named by the Resource category and dimension to which it refers. A standardized procedure is used to score one of these dimensions, UQL, for all Resource categories. It is discussed immediately following the first Resource category, Space, on page 225. Transformations: The scores do not sum to a total Resource score. However, they may be transformed to ratings by means of a standardized logical procedure, such as that explained on page 2A9° Then the ratings can be used for comparative family analysis. RESOURCE CATEGORY 1: SPACE I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers to household spaces available in relation to the number of persons present. Sources of data are the house floor plan and number 3 of the Space Resource Inventory. The score is obtained by summing items under A and then dividing by B. 220 A. Count the total number of individual house— hold spaces. Such a space is defined as an area for family-related activities or use which is "roomlike" in that it has a roof and is divided off from sur- rounding spaces by means of walls, half—walls, a doorway or archway. The space may be attached to the main house or separate, but it must be used for a family-related purpose. Thus count a storage area used for kitchen firewood, household tools, or the family jeep, but not one used for animals such as a chickenhouse. Count each of the following as indi— vidual spaces: A covered porch, An attached enclosure separate bath and/or toilet space, An enclosed toilet or bath facility detached from the house but on the house grounds, A roofed-over sink area if detached from the house but on the house grounds, A hall or inside stairway. B. Count total number of persons who are mem— bers of the group living in and using the house: all eat and sleep there. Include roomers or ser- vants, and adolescent children who may board away during the week but return on weekends. II. Availability—Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to total variety available in five kinds of hous- ing elements. The score is obtained by summing items under A, l to 5, and then subtracting negative descrip- tors under B. 221 A. Count the different kinds, or amounts if more than one, of these variety elements. "Kind" refers, for example, to electric light bulb: "amount" refers to 5 electric light bulbs. 1. Materials and surfaces. Data are the house floor plan plus 13, IA, 15, 16 and 17 of the Inventory. Some illustrations are: plastic curtains, chicken wire, glass windows, cement floor, red painted wall, china dishes, aluminum pan. The underlined words (red, painted, etc.) represent variety in metals, fibers, colors or other substances present in the house. 2. Machines, tools and food containers. Sources of data are the house floor plan and 10, 12, and 13 of the Inventory. Some illus- trations are: television set, radio, car, washing machine, "machete," corn grinder, cook- ing stove, forks, set of dishes. 3. Storage facilities. Sources of data are the house floor plan and number l5 of the Inventory. Illustrations include: dish cabinet, clothes cabinet, trunk, basket for clothes, nails in walls for hanging clothes or tools, shelf. Do not count tables as storage facili- ties. 222 A. Utilities and fuels. Sources of data are the house floor plan and 8, 9, 10 and 11 of the Inventory. Illustrations are: water source(s) within the house, electric light bulbs, flourescent light fixture, cooking oil or keroscene, firewood, flush toilet. Count each different item only once. 5. Adornments. Sources of data are the house floor plan and number 14 of the Inventory. Illustrations are: small figurines, a doll used as decoration (not for play), flowers, fancy doilies, photographs, claendars, diplomas displayed or the wall. B. Count descriptors which indicate limits on quality of the variety elements. These will be found in the observer's notes on the house floor plan and 8 to 19 of the Inventory, for example: Light bulbs are all burned out. Sanitary facility is far away from house. There are no ceilings in the rooms. The paint is peeling (faded). The windows do not have glass. There is no floor in the kitchen. The walls are unpainted. There are chickens (insects) in the house. III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to objects and spaces of the house which the child uses, and his use-actions involving these Objects and spaces, minus constraints or limits upon either of these two elements 223 by his mother or others. Sources of data are Picture Ques- tions 1 and 2, and 6, 7 and 11 of the Inventory. The score is obtained by summing items A through B. A. 'Objects of the house used and actions re- lated to use, minus constraints. From Picture Ques- tions 1 and 2, count response units which indicate objects the child may use. Count and add response units which indicate child's actions involving the objects. Count and subtract response units which indicate constraints on either the objects or ac- tions. A "response unit" refers to the presence of the element being scored: in this instance, of an object, a use-action, or a constraint on either of these two. One sentence may contain several scoreable response units. Some illustrations are: Response of Family A Unit Score "Well, he is interested in Objects: radio, A everything, like the radio, typewriter, the typewriter, and tools knives, hammer like knives and hammers. Use-actions: 1 He tries to use every- tries to use thing. Constraints: Total: Response of Family B Unit Score "Sometimes I let her turn Objects: radio 1 on the radio if she is careful, but also I have to be very careful because she could get a shock Use-actions: turn on, carry— ing it around 22A Response of Family B, cont. Unit Score from the current, or be- Constraints: -2 cause Of carrying it around one related to she might drop it, so I shock for child; don't let her for fear of one to damage damage." for radio Total: 1 Response of Family C Unit Score "The only thing he plays Objects: boxes 2 with are boxes or benches benches because that's all there " Use-actions: l is in the house, inside. plays with Constraints: -1 that's all there is Total: 2 B. Child's sleeping. Count weighted items which pertain. Source of data is number 7 of the Inventory. Item Score Child has a bed to himself 2 Child sleeps with one other person in the bed 1 Child sleeps with more than one other person in bed —1 Child and no more than two other persons sleep in the same room 1 More than two persons sleep in the same room with the child —1 C. Child's use Of sanitary facilities. Count the following weighted items which apply. Source of data is number 11 of the Inventory. Item Score Child uses facilities with no problem. 1 Child has problem in using facilities. -1 ThEre are no facilities available for child to use at the family's house. -1 225 D. Child's use of play spaces within the house only. Source of data is number 6 of the Inventory. Count spaces which the child is permitted to use for playing in the house. If mother says he plays "everywhere" or "anywhere," count this (3); if she says "only out-of—doors," count this a (—1) re- Sponse. IV. Use—Quality (UQL). Consult general scoring instructions and illustrations which follow. USE—QUALITY (UQL) Scoring Instructions A basic procedure was used to score this dimension for all Resource categories except for Child's Movement, where it forms part of this dimension score. Illustrations of the procedure are listed where necessary for consistent interpretation under each Resource category, except for Child's Learning, Family Learning, Child's Contacts and Family Contacts included here. The UQL dimension refers to mother's perceptions of the relation of the Resource category to probable success of the child when he goes to school. It allows the re- spondent to verbalize the quality of school-relatedness for actions involving each of the Resource categories. Source of data is the mother's response to Picture Ques- tion 3, of the general form: 226 "Do you think that a child who (whose family) acts like the ones we see in these pictures (does these things, uses things like these) will be successful in the activities of school when he (she) goes? Why?" To score, count and add or subtract the five re sponse units described below. A response unit refers to the presence of the particular Resource category, resource-actions and school relationship qualities being scored. Such a unit might be a sentence, a single word, a phrase or a paragraph. I. Positive. Score each response (1) and sum the total. Three common units occur: A. (SP) Simple positive, with or without an immediately following phrase, such as: "Yes, I think so," or "I hope so." Be careful in scoring because the mother may say "No," but the response is actually positive because she proceeds to give an illustration which indicates that she does see a relation between the Resource and future school success for the child. B. (GR) A general relation is expressed be— tween the Resource Of the question and future school success. C. (SR) The response gives a specific illus— tration of the simple positive response (SP). It may be one word or a whole paragraph, but it clearly relates to the Resource category under discussion 227 and is neither vague nor repetitious. It may en- tail a characteristic of the child. II. Not Positive. Score each such response unit (-l), and sum the total. Three common units appear: A. (SN) A simple negative response which is of the order: "I don't think so," or simply: "No." B. (NR) The unit states a reason that is ng£_ related to the Resource category of the question even though it may indicate a favorable relation to school success or a favorable characteristic of the child. C. (NR) The response unit states a reason why the relationship would not exist between the Resource category and school success, or it ex- pands the reason already given for the non- relationship by giving a specific example. III. Vague (V). Score each illustration (0). Common vague responses do not arrive at a generalization or a bringing together of several concepts or ideas in terms of the Resource category of the question. The unit repre— sents a series of incomplete phrases which could be either positive or negative if they were completed or which can— not be clearly interpreted in the context of the total response given. Illustrations are: ". . . with the Same teachers . . . ," and "Well . . . at times." 228 IV. Repetitious (R). Score each response (0). A repetitious unit says the same thing more than once, using slightly altered words; it does not add a new specific idea to illustrate a preceding statement. V. Inability to respond (I). Score these forms of response as (0). Two illustrations of (I) are: A. Statements like: "I don't know," "Who knows?" or "One can't really tell." B. No response is made after several explana- tions of the question and general probes have been offered. Illustrations The following are illustrations of response units and scoring decisions for four Resource categories. 1. Child's Learning (and future school success) Response of Family A Unit Score "Yes, they (child's learning actions) SP 1 can have much importance." "That is, reading, and drawing can SR, SR 3 have much importance, and the same SR with writing . . ." "Because from the time they are small GR 1 they like to study." Total: '6 229 Response of Family B Unit Score "Yes, it's important." SP 1 "This one likes to paint and, SR 1 already, all those things." "It's a good thing for them." R 0 "When they enter school they'll already . . . " V 0 Total: 2 Family Learning (and future school success) Response of Family C Unit Score "Yes, it could be. . . .‘" SP 1 "The small child . . . it has to be the big one, who has studied much, and already has more experience than the smaller . . . NR -1 "All that she (the big one) is learn- ing, it will serve her more because she has already studied more." R 0 Total: 0 Response of Family D Unit Score "This . . maybe." V 0 "It's that I don't know, I don't I, R O, 0 know." Total: 0 Child's Contacts (and future school success) Response of Family E Unit Score "Well, there are children, perhaps, GR 1 already educated, they are cultured . . . "It's like this child she is SR 1 this way: comes to the house and doesn't talk at all. She'll go to school and do the same." 230 Response of Family E, cont. Unit Score "They are that way as older children. R 0 They go to school this way, and I think they are this way the same as in the house." Total: 2 Family Contacts (and future school success) Response of Family F Unit Score "If he'll have more success if he sees R 0 these things, you ask me?" "Well, I think that always the child who has more chances to go out, SR 1 to contact other people, SR 1 has more chances to develop himself GR 1 than the child who is always kept closed in." Total: 3 Response of Family G Unit Score "It seems to me that he should have GR 1 greater success because of every- thing for learning . . . " "The children are intelligent and SR 1 they pay much attention to what another person says and explains." "When they go to school perhaps that GR 1 will serve them." "One day the teacher will ask if they SR 1 have ever been in a discussion and they can say, yes, that they have heard a doctor explain that fruits have to be washed before eating; that if they have fallen from a tree the fruits then have to be washed so they can be eaten; that before eating they should wash their hands and brush their teeth. Well, many things " Total: A 231 RESOURCE CATEGORY 2: CHILD'S MOVEMENT I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers to movement-related elements present within the house, neighborhood or community that are listed under 1 to 8 of the Movement Resource Inventory. The score is obtained by summing the items. Include "traffic" as a movement-related element unless the respondent has said that there is none. Include as an element such items as river, train crossing, cliff or hill if mentioned. II. Availability-Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to movement elements which are "housebound." Sources of data are l to 8 of the Inventory, and Dimension AQN. The score is obtained by summing items under A and then dividing by B. The higher the score is, the mgrg "housebound" are the movement elements, and therefore the lgrgr is the movement variety available. A. Count total number of elements which are restricted to the house and the immediately sur- rounding household property. Do not count a "cafetal" (coffee field) as part of household property because it is often not possible to know if it pertains to the household. B. Use the score from Dimension AQN, (total movement elements present within the house, neighborhood or community). 232 III. Use-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers to the child's actual movements: his actions involving the movement elements minus constrictions he experiences. Sources of data are numbers l—3 and 5—8 of the Inventory and Picture Question 2. The score is obtained by summing items under A and then subtracting those under B. A. Count each movement which is performed, allowed, or encouraged for the child. Sources of data are those listed above. Illustrations are: Child uses all entrances and exgfig of the house. Child's movements are not endangered by traffic. Child uses public transportation, rides a horse, goes for a walk often. Child plays vigorously, goes out of the house and around the neighborhood, and also sits quietly at times. B. Subtract number of different constrictions placed upon the child's movement. These may be evidences of movements hindered, prevented or dis— couraged. Sources of data are all those listed above. Some illustrations, with indicants of constriction underlined, are: Child is afraid of stairs. Child does not go to the other side of the fence; he stays within. Child does not use a neighborhood play area which is available. Traffic is a danger; it stops or limits the child's movements. Child seldom goes away_from home, or £22 alone, or he is afraid of animals or a bicycle. Mother prevents a certain action by the child. 233 IV. Use-Quality (UQL). This dimension refers to mother's perception of the school—success—relatednesS of the child's movement. Source of data is her response to Picture Question 3. The score is obtained by summing A and B. A. Score the mother's weighted estimation of movement relevant to school success according to the following scheme: Response Score "I don't know." 0 "Only the seated child." 1 "Either the child playing ball or the one going out of the house." 2 Any two examples, such as: "both the child playing ball and the one going out of the house." 3 "All three children." A B. Count each reason given for the choice under A and subtract each reason given against other possible choices, against the one actually chosen, or unrelated reSponses. Follow the basic procedure set up for the UQL dimension. Due to the way this question is stated: "Of these three children we see here, which do you think will be able to go to school with good success? Why?" count as positive responses general qualities of a child such as "bigger," or "more lively." 23A RESOURCE CATEGORY 3: CARE AND APPEARANCE I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers to food and clothing items usually available for child and mother. Source of data is the Care and Appear— ance Resource Inventory. The score is obtained by summing A, B, and C. A. Count whatever items mother mentions as food in 2, 3 and A of the Inventory. Include "agua dulce" (sugar water), coffee, bread and tortillas. B. Count items of clothing for the child in 10 and 11 of the Inventory. These are items used every day, at night, and "for good." If socks and shoes have not been mentioned but they are found listed under clothing observed on the child (13), count them also. However, do not include the clothes which the child is observed to be wearing; these will be counted under the UQN dimension. C.‘ Count items of the mother's clothing which are listed under number 15. Include two observa- tions only. If there is no second observation, repeat the first one. II. Availability—Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to available daily routines in household activi- ties of eating, personal care, sleeping, and changing 235 clothes. The score is obtained by summing items A through D. A. Count the number of meals plus the number of regular snack times (small meals often called "coffee" times) which mother says the child has everyday. Data is number 1 of the Inventory. B. Count and sum the following weighted items pertaining to personal care. Sources are numbers 6 and 6 of the Inventory. Item 1 Score Daily bath 2 Bath every other day, or 3—A, 2—3 times per week 1 Bath less than the above 0 Item 2 Score Teeth brushed every meal, at meals, or several times a day, or twice a day 2 Teeth brushed once per day, or "everyday" 1 Teeth brushed hardly ever, very little, sometimes, never 0 Item 3 Score Hands washed before eating, or at every meal 2 Hands washed several times, often, twice per day, "when dirty" 1 Hands washed once per day, hardly ever 0 Item A Score Hair is washed daily 2 Hair is washed every other day, or 3-A, 2-3 times per week 1 Hair is washed less than the above 0 C. Count and sum the following items which refer to the child's sleep. Sources of data are 7 and 8 of the Inventory. 236 Items Score Sleeps "well" or a regular number of hours each night Naps usually during the day Goes to bed by 7 p.m. at night Sleeps 10 hours or more each night None of the above occur OHI—‘i—‘H D. Count number of regular changes of clothes provided, using these weighted items from 10 and 11 of the Inventory. Items Score Clothes changed both for going to bed and for "good" or other reasons Clothes changed for going to bed. Clothes changed for "good" or other reasons Same clothes worn all the time OHNUO III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to the child's use of foods and clothing minus limits placed upon use. Score by summing items under A and B and then subtract items under C. A. Sum only the foods which the child eats from the food groups of milk (cheese, ice cream), meat (fish, poultry, eggs), fruits and vegetables. Sources of data are 2, 3, and A of the Inventory. Count illustrations given, not general terms such as meat, unless no illustrations are given. B. Sum items of clothing observed on the child. Source of data is number 13 of the In— ventory. If the child is observed to wear shoes and socks on more than one visit, count them twice 237 in order to allow for cases where the child does not wear them each time, or at all. Count only two observations of the child's clothing. C. Count number of illustrations of limits on the child's use of food and clothing in the Inventory. Illustrations of limits are: Signs of soil on child or his clothing No shoes worn Some foods are not liked or not eaten at least weekly, or only irregularly Clothes are ragged There are problems in getting clothes or food for the child IV. Use Quality (UQL). Follow general scoring in- structions set up for the dimension. Note that a char- acteristic response of the mother to the question of the relation between the Resource category and later school success of the child is: "No, this child will not be successful." Then she gives illustrations which are characteristics of the child ("lazy about getting dressed," "not paying attention to his eating") specifically re- lated to both the Resource category and to school success, thus indicating that she sees a relationship. In this case, her response is scored positively. RESOURCE CATEGORY A: CHILD'S PLAY ' I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers to elements and situations for play, either avail- able or possible as indicated by the mother's perceptions. Score by summing A and B. 238 A. Number of play objects and situations men— ioned in numbers 1-7 of the Play Resource In— ventory. Be careful not to repeat items. B. Number of common play situations, and objects which mother might offer the child for play. Count here only those items which are dif- ferent from those counted under A. Sources of this data are Picture Questions 1 and 2. II. Availability-Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to variety of play usually available, that is, the presence of building, role—playing and creative play things and situations for the child at home. Sources of data are 1-3 of the Inventory. Do not count such items as mud, water, stones and sticks here unless the mother has mentioned them. Score by summing A, B, and C. A. Count number of items mentioned under the building category of number 1 of the Inventory. B. Count number of items mentioned under the rolejplaying category of number 2 of the Inventory. C. Count number Of items mentioned under the creative category of number 3 of the Inventory. III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to the child's usual play (objects and actions) minus any restrictions on his play. Sources of data are A-7 of the Inventory and Picture Questions 1-2. Score by.summing 239 A, B and C, and subtracting D. Do not repeat items once they have been mentioned under A or B. A. Count play substitutes which mother gives the child or which he encounters by himself, such as sticks, water, and mud. B. Count number of outdoor and indoor play objects which the child has. C. Count number of limits on his play, either mentioned under the above three items, or in the mother's responses to Picture Questions 1 and 2. Illustrations are: Child uses no play substitutes or has no favorite toy. Mother will not let him use substitutes for toys. Child has nothing to play with out-of- doors, or nothing for indoor play. Toys are sex-typed by mother: only boys play with certain toys or only girls play with certain toys. IV. Use-Quality (UQL). Follow general scoring in- structions set up for the dimension (page 225). RESOURCE CATEGORY 5: CHILD'S TASK AND WORK I. Availabilitnyuantity (AQN). This dimension refers to situations at home which might allow the child to have experiences in personal tasks or work efforts of the family. Not only the child, but mother or other family members may be engaged in these activities. Source of data are the Task and Work Resource Inventory, 2A0 especially 1, A and 7, and Picture Questions 1 and 2 for additional illustrations. Score by summing A and B. A. Count personal tasks which mother mentions such as the child getting himself dressed or being dressed by his sister. B. Count work experiences available in the home, whether or not the child himself participates. II. Availability—Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to family involvement in children's task and work experiences leading to press for or against the child's participation. Sources of data are the Inventory and Picture Questions 1 and 2. The score is obtained by summing items under A and then subtracting items under B. A. Count number of illustrations of family involvement, support, allowance for error, help Offered, expecting or allowing the child to do (try) something in the way of a task or small job for himself or the family. Do not count here an illustration of someone doing a task instead of the child. B. Count number of illustrations of punishment, warnings to the child, stOpping him from engaging in a task, or deprecating his efforts at doing a task. l\.) J“? F" III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to the child's usual participation in task and work. Score by subtracting B and C from A. A. Total number of task and work available in the home: use score from Dimension AQN. B. Count number of problems which the child himself encounters in his efforts to do a task, such as not being able to put his shirt on right, or tie his shoes, or having an accident when he does something for his father. Sources of data are 2 and 5 of the Inventory, plus Picture Question l and 2. C. Count illustrations in the Inventory and Picture answers of someone doing a task instead of the Child. IV. Use-Quality (UQL). Follow general scoring in- {\o R) U1 structions and illustrations for the dimension, page ’ RESOURCE CATEGORY 6: CHILD'S LEARNING I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension indicates objects and activities for learning which are available or possible for a preschool child. Sources of data are the Child's Learning Resource Inventory and Picture Questions 1 and 2. The score is obtained by summing A and B. A. Count number of Objects available, such as books, paper for drawing, pencils. 2A2 B. Count number of activities the mother men- tions which the child might do "to learn" before going to school, such as "writing," making lines, drawing, being read to, reciting. II. Availability-Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to family's involvement in learning for the child before school. Inventory and Picture Questions 1 and 2 are data sources. Count items under A and then count and subtract the items under B. A. Sum number of evidences of family's (and others') active participation in learning oppor- tunities for the child, as well as evidences of the family "allowing" these opportunities. Illustrations are: the mother reading to the child or letting him take a page from a notebook to use for writing. B. Sum and subtract evidences of family's and others' non—involvement or negative involve- ment, such as preventing the child from looking at his older brother's school books. III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to amount of learning activities in which the child actually engages. Data is from the Inventory except number 9. The score is the sum of each weighted activity times the score assigned to the frequency of its occurrence,.under A and B. 2A3 A. Activities assigned (2) are those most directly related to school activities, such as draw- ing, plays school, looks at pictures, is read to- B. Activities assigned (1) are those less directly related to school activities, such as dances to music, watches TV, uses musical instru- ment. For each of the above activities, multiply its assigned weight by the score assigned to the frequency with which it occurs: Frequency Score Daily, all the time A Weekly, several times a week, almost every day, less than daily 3 Less than weekly, every 15 days, 1-2 times a month, very often 2 All others (every 3 months, 1-2 a year, sometimes, once in a while, in the past) 1 Never, almost never, very little 0 IV. Use-Quality (UQL). Follow general scoring in- structions and illustrations for this dimension, page 225. RESOURCE CATEGORY 7: FAMILY LEARNING I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers not to the young child, but to family learning activities: all those which the mother mentions in her responses to the Family Learning Resource Inventory and Picture Questions 1 and 2. The score is the sum of A, B and C. 2AA A. Count number of activities mentioned which refer to school-related learning, such as reading, writing, arithmetical transactions (such as use of bank services), informal study. Do not include regular school attendance for the purpose of secur- ing a grade school, high school or university diploma or degree. B. Count number of activities mentioned which refer to travels or visits to places such as to the zoo, museum, park, library, school, or other towns or parts of the country or a foreign country. C. Count number of activities mentioned which indicate entertainment facilities, such as tele- vision, radio, music or movies. II. Availability-Quality (AQL). This dimension re- fers to the extent that family learning activities are primarily school-related. The score is obtained by divid- ing A of AQN by the total score of AQN. III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to regularity with which the family actually engages in learning activities. To score it, sum the weighted fre- quencies of the activities under A, B and C of AQN. Fre- quency categories are the same as those listed under UQN of Child's Learning. IV. Use-Quality (UQL). Follow general scoring in- structions and illustrations for this dimension, page 225. 2A5 The question is worded in a slightly different way for this Resource category and the following one; it asks the mother not about the preschool child, but about other family members and then how their activities might influ- ence the child's later school success. An illustration showing how mother might respond is the following: "Because the older ones have learned they teach those who are moving up. I find it has to be so. The older person teaches the younger; be- cause the family has helped him (the child); he has developed " RESOURCE CATEGORY 8: CHILD'S CONTACTS I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers to total possible social contacts and events in which the child might be able to participate. Sources Of data are the Child's Contacts Resource Inventory and Picture Questions 1 and 2. The score is the total of A and B. A. Count the contacts which a preschool child might have, for example, with his parents, his brothers and sisters, an adult friend of his father, a teacher who comes to visit, a cousin with whom he plays, uncles and aunts. B. Count events in which the child might be included, such as a children's birthday party, conversation with grandparents, going for a walk with father, going to church with a sister, shopping in town, attending a party at the school. 2A6 II. Availability-Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to the extent to which the child's social contacts and activities are centered within the family. Sources of data are the same as for dimension AQN. The score is the sum of A and B, divided by C. The higher the score which results the more restricted is the variety of the child's contacts. A. Count number of persons with whom the child interacts who are within the family. "Padrinos" (godparents) are not listed as family members. B. Count number of activities which involve the child and other family members. Each activity encountered in the data is scored only once: either as involving family members or non-family persons. An activity involving the child with a family member is scored here even though it may also occur with non-family persons. To illustrate, if the child usually "plays with his brother," play as an activity is scored here even though the child also plays with a neighbor. However, "goes to church with mother" is scored as a non—family activity and is not counted here. C. Divide by the total score from dimension AQN. III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to amount of social activity in which the child usually 2A7 participates. The Resource Inventory only is the source of data. The score is the sum of the weighted frequencies for activities in which the child engages from the follow— ing categories; Family celebrations and activities Activities with smaller children Activities with the peer group Contacts with non-family adults Participation in large groups The frequencies are the following: Frequency Score Activity done every day A Activity done weekly, several times a week, almost every day 3 Activity done less than weekly, every 15 days, 1-2 a month, very often 2 Activity done less than monthly; every three months, 1-2 a year, sometimes, once in‘a while, in the past 1 Activity never done, almost never, very little, hardly ever 0 IV. Use-Quality (UQL). Follow the general scoring instructions and illustrations for this dimension, page 225. RESOURCE CATEGORY 9: FAMILY CONTACTS I. Availability-Quantity (AQN). This dimension refers not to the preschool child, but to the total pos— sible social contacts and events in which other family members might be able to participate, either by themselves or with others. Sources of data are the Family Contacts Resource Inventory and Picture Questions 1 and 2. The score is the total of A, B and C. 2A8 A. Count family members who engage in social activities, for example, mother and father, brothers and sisters, grandparents. B. Count persons with whom the family has con- tacts, such as friends of the parents or brothers and sisters, teachers, employer. C. Count the events, organizations and acti- vities in which the family participates, such as meetings in the community, visits with friends, the village cooperative, picnics or trips on weekends. II. Availability-Quality (AQL). This dimension refers to the extent to which the family's social con- tacts and activities are centered within the family. The score represents the total number of family members en- gaging in social activities plus the family activities engaged in. Source of data are the same as for dimension AQN. The score is the sum of A and B, divided by C. The higher the score the more restricted is the variety of family contacts. A. Count number of persons in the family par— ticipating in social contacts. B. Count number of social activities or con- tacts which occur within the family group. C. Divide the sum of A and B by the total score of dimension AQN. 2A9 III. Use-Quantity (UQN). This dimension refers to amount of social activity in which the family usually participates. Source of data are l to A of the Inventory only. The score is the sum of the weighted frequencies for activities in which the family engages from the follow— ing categories: Community contacts Contacts outside the community Contacts with friends who are not extended family members. Contacts with family members. The frequencies are the same as those listed under dimen- sion UQN of Child's Contacts. IV. Use-Quality (UQL). Follow the general instruc- tions and illustrations for scoring this dimension, page 225. TRANSFORMATION OF SCORES The investigator may desire to compute a summary value for all or any of the Resource categories explained in the Scoring Manual and then use this value for com- parative family analysis, similar to that undertaken or suggested in this study. If so, it is necessary to trans- form the scores according to some set of logical rules or a statistical procedure in order to permit arithmetic operations and comparisons. As one approach, the trans- formation procedures used in this study are summarized in Tables 23 and 2A. 250 o H H a 3 on C m I. H Hum 36 o» and n om. « mm.m mm.m go: 0 an H m 3 on 3 m o H ....Hsm SH 3 and a On. A ism :m.m 20: - .o mH wchmL .HmsaoacH came cm:» 0 ms. acumopm mH oncom 8H .H nH mcHuon .Hm>L0o:H H mm. :mm& mHmzro oncom aH .4 m w . o H :Huaa Hm> unmocH cmwu can» wan m mm. wH whoom mm “m oHom em. 0» as. n mo. H mm. mm. HO< s H m HmH 0H HHV pcoco>oz m sH H anon os.HH 0» om.oH n on. « om.HH om.HH ZG< m.UHHLo O 1| H H Hm o» H“ u . H DH.1 ..w 0» mm.H u in. u -m.H mm.H 403 a a H ... S U.. S m HH . ems“ om.m oh om.: u 3m. u 0:.m 0:.m :02 O OH H A... :m 0.. .... m ...... Hod-.. .-...mm 3 me...“ u on. u M.I.T.. Hnmnzv 3.... 49‘ .o mH mzHomL . annoucH Cap: 0 mm. saga xuoH mH choow OH .H n nuHunL .Hnuaoucfl H .-. uni: nHM1wm on on M” .w wH manna; .anawa ucH camL snip Lcem.au n om.H nH oaoom an H 6H3; um. On mu. u Em. a ma. Ammuzv mm. 2a< oomam .Amm can» ”mHoa oHrHaa meH MH omumo "mascooo nonaam or“ on usmoaoo ”mon» “cannons nchH xv ”noaoow wcHoaL ”mH whoon now mnHum; as cacom a acHuomLunum Him mchHm an no CCsziHLHmHU on» concosHQ apomo w. :oanmEHQ coanaeHo pH moaoomom mucmru .m Hm>a0ncH :mo; ocHn.onmm .m mo came monaiou .H monzomom u o co meao moms Hu 9 H whomoooaa go mcoHpmpoasHHH .mucHumL Ou nopoon mopzowom Echo uncaHquunsHHH mom smHomnaommcmLp umLHu Lou opsooooamnt.mm wqmoz x m H m H H m.oHHco x q H H m o woman Ho: 20: H®< 20¢ Hmuov Hmnmv Hmuov nCOanoEHQ oopsomom cmHm mHeeHz zoo ”meanness ”mpHHHDMozpm OocH mHoEommm 0cm maowoumo zaowoumo o» COHumHoa mo mHo>mH “mchma oopsomom m moaoommm some now mconcoeHo HHm cu mmchmp mopsomom :mem< .m o» campuma some Eon .N no wmaoom pom mwcHump chuoo .H mascoooam .szHHanopo ou :oHumea mo mHm>mH o» mwchmL moasomwm anm ”HHHEmm moo anw mmaoom wch: COHumaHmsHHH pow COprEHomwcmap UCOomm How manpoooamul.zm mHm¢B . 11¢ 41:11:11]...“ .11 253 INSTITUTO INTERAMERICANO DE CIENCIAS AGRICOLAS El Insdtuto es un Organismo cspcciali- zado de la Organizacio’n de los Estados Americanos. Fue establecido por los 00- TURRIALDA. COSTA RICA bicmos dc las Repuiblicas Americanas para promovcr el adelanto dc la: cien- cias agrfcolas por medio de la investi- gacién. la educacién y el desarrollo dc scrvicios tegionales. Marzo de 1968 Estimados senor y senora: Desde hace casi dos afios tengo e1 placer de vivir en Costa Rica, donde estoy trabajando como educadora para el hogar y profesora en el Departamento de E02 nomia y Ciencias Sociales del Centro de Ensefianza e Investigacién de Turrialba. Durante este tiempo he tenido oportunidades muy valiosas para aprender algo de la vida costarricense, tanto en las areas rurales como en 1as urbanas. He vi- sitado varias families, algunas escuelas y he oonversado con padres, maestros y personal de algunos ministerios y de la Universidad sobre el crecimiento,la educacién y el futuro de nuestros pequefios nifios de Costa Rica. Ahora quisiera solicitar 1a ayuda de su familia en mi estudio de las familias de doce comunidades de la Meseta Central. Estoy pidiéndoles muy especialmente su cooperacién por el interés demostrado por ustedes y a la vez su amable pa- ciencia durante la encuesta nutricional llevada a cabo e1 afio antepasado, 0 sea en 1966, por el Ministerio de Salubridad Publica y el Instituto de Nutri- ci6n de Centro America y Panama (INCAP). Desearia solicitar la cooperacién del senor jefe de la familia en cuanto a al- guna informacién sobre la ocupacién actual y los ingresos de la familia. Tam- bién solicito cooperaciOn a la sefiora madre del nifio "pre-escolar" en cuanto a algunas informaciones e ideas sobre el nifio joven y sus actividades, y el sistema educativo en esta érea. Como soy extranjera, ustedes me podrian ensefiar muchas cosas durante nuestras conversaciones, que serén de mucha importancia en mi futura labor como profe- sora a nivel universitario, y ojala también muchas cosas importantes para tan- tas otras familias, asi como instituciones dedicadas a1 desarrollo social en Costa Rica. Les agradezco mucho su participacién en este proyecto de investigacién cienti- fica y suscribo de ustedes, , ente, . WM 27% Georgiann Baker Educador para el Hagar Economia y Ciencias Sociales uto administra el Proyecto 39 del Programa de Cooperacién Te'cnica de la O.E.A. Este Proyecto procura e1 mcioxamiento de la agricultura y de la vida tu- [as Americas, pox medlo de la ensenanza :ecmca. Pata facilitar la eiecucion dcl Proyecto se ha dividido alas Américas en (res Zonas. donde se hau esta- ) oficinas regionales: l) Zona None, con OflClna en La Habana, Cuba; 2) Zona Andina, con Oficina en Lima, Peni; y 3) Zone Sur, con Oficinaen Montevi - rugu3Y- -1: it IIII‘IA 25A \'\ :gfi- :)— msnww INTERAMERICANO DE CIENCIAS AGRICOLAS DE LA on a El Instituto es un organismo ospccialimdo do In Organivacion do los Estados Americanos. Fuc osta- TURRIALBA, COSTA RICA blocido por los Gobiomos do [as Repdblicas Amori- CINC: "CA- Mllbt cams on 1942 para promovor en desarrollo ocondnuco y social 3 craves do In oducacldn y la Investigacldn. CT/DT-575 2 do mayo do 1968 Estimados sofioros: Por este medio quioro presentarlos a la sefiorita Georgianne Baker, profesora on nuestro Centro de Ensofianza o Invostigaoién aqui on Turrialba. Como parte de sus responsabilidades, 1a sefiorita Baker esta encor- gada do una investigacion do la familia oostarrioonse: sus recursos y sus actividades, y los opinionos do la familia on cuanto a la edu- caoion. Esperanos que los resultados do esta investigacién, aunque no beneficiarén direotamento a code familia participanto, ayudarén a que los estudiantos do cioncias sociales del Centro y los profosio- nalos do Costa Rice en el desarrollo comunal, 1a extonsién agricola y la oducaoién, conozcan la vida real do la familia costarrioonso. Le danos todo nuestro apoyo a1 desarrollo do esta investigaci6n y solicitamos la cooperacién do ustedes durante los dies en que la sofiorita Baker ostaré trabajando on esa comunidad. Aunque coda familia no recibiré nada concroto por su participaoion, esperamos que sontiré una satisfaccién profunda por habor contribui- do a un trabajo que puede ayudar a mejorar los siotomas do oduoacion y de servicios rurales on Costa Rica. Agradeconos anticipadamente toda la colabo»-o)'. que/gzrproste a sofiorita Baker y suscribinoo atontamont: I // / / Dirnando Sudroz do Castro ‘ubdirootor del Centro do Ensefianza o Invostigaoion Anya.) tiene $1! Dirczxg 'u’m (it-I-uzol n ( 5!) Pi: n, .\}!.-II‘1;ldv «I 9181?, Sw jun!” Sus Eros Oficinas Regionalos abarcan los siguiontos parses: Zena Andinn, “do 478, Lima, Peril (I‘Dlivizz, Colombia, Ecuador, Pom ', \I'onczzlcia): ”Iona None, Apartado 1815, Guatemala, Guatemala (Mexico, Istmo Centro- rim-mo y Antillus Maw 13$); tom Sur, CIsiIIa dc Comm 1'2: 7, Mum IdCC , UmIgII'..y(1\rgentint, Brasi1,CbIIo, Paraguay yUmguay). Mantlono dos ros: Centro de Ens-flu '.I In: u Invostigacién, TurII Hm, Cso ‘Gl RI: :;I 3." CLIVIIIH‘ III Invc filigacién y Enscnama para 11 Zona Tcmplada Como parte del Centro wescigscionos Agri‘vsolz Is de' III loamy, la I): rm aw Ia, Colon”, UI'Iig : ,.Im1m.vw. eI Proyccto 206 (Re 10mm .zAgraI-ii) del Programa do Cooperacldu no... do Id OER. ;-::'.I~'-I.I’-.I.-.I'.v pm e! (Ton git. [I v. r.. Hiczxno 12.....«31 Ru 3: {Oginl (CD’S). Mzzntwne mmbién mlcloos do invostigacidn y enseflanza para CanoIJOIBNofiowulo hunnflgodén 4‘. .T. 'D'mr ’5. 5"A-1- 255 1 de Mayo de 1968 Para el que concierne: Como sacerdote de la Parroquia de me es grato reconocer y recomendar a la Srta. Profesora Georgianne Baker, Educadora para el Hogar y Economia y Ciencias Sociales. Ella es catolica y practica amplia— mente su credo, siempre se ha mostrado con muy buena voluntad para ayudar en la comunidad, en todos los campos, religioso, social,etc. Por dicho motivo la reoomiendo para que su labor pueda ser efectiva y para que todo lo que Uds. puedan hacer para ayudarla en su trabajo. Reciba la mas amplia aprobacfon de este humilde servidor y amigo. Agradeciéndoles anticipadamente todo lo que hagan en este respecto, y con sumo gusto reciban una bendicién sacerdotal de quién les estima y quiere. Padre Coadjutor - APPENDIX D TABLE ON BASIC STATISTICS 256 TABLE 25.--Basic statistics for resource dimension scores. Basic Statistics Resource Category Dimension Scores Migiflzm Msgiflgm Mean 3:322:1gn Skewness Kurtosis Space AQN 0.29 3.00 0.96 o.h6 1.5a 6.83 AQL 8.00 65.00 33.15 11.97 0.30 2.60 UQN -U.OO 16.00 5.u1 “.16 0.2“ 2.72 UQL -5.00 7.00 1.88 2.06 -0.77 “.30 Child's AQN 5.00 19.00 11.20 3.09 0.36 2.59 Movement AQL 0.11 0.88 0.52 0.1“ -O.18 3.18 UQN —5.00 11.00 3.3“ 2.03 -0.12 3.30 UQL -1.00 8.00 3.39 1.93 0.2“ 2.85 Care- AQN 12.00 35.00 22.19 5.03 0.20 2.u6 Appear- AQL 5.00 20.00 13.55 2.92 -o.us 2.97 ance UQN —3.00 20.00 9.51 5.11 -0.06 2.83 UQL -1.00 7.00 1.74 1.83 0.98 2.99 Child's AQN A 00 38.00 18.91 6.51 0.59 3.23 Play AQL 1.00 20 oo 8.99 n.01 o.u1 2.99 UQN 0.00 18.00 6.60 3.20 0.62 “.17 UQL -3.00 6.00 0.60 2.12 —O.23 2.22 Child's AQN 2.00 16.00 9.23 2.70 0.11 3.52 Task- AQL -5.00 8.00 1.91 2.31 0.12 3.30 Work UQN 0.00 1U.OO 6.73 2.85 -0.06 2.96 UQL -3.00 5.00 1.53 1.73 -O.25 2.72 Child's AQN “.00 26.00 19.65 “.85 0.20 2.65 Learning AQL -2.00 10.00 3.30 2.91 —0.19 2.76 UQN 1.00 49.00 22.97 9.99 0.17 2.95 UQL —3.00 7.00 2.60 1.90 -0.59 3.83 Family AQN 5.00 33.00 19.65 5.6“ 0.52 3.02 Learning AQL 0.00 0.60 0.33 0.12 -0.05 2.99 UQN 5.00 95.00 22.18 8.90 0.95 2.8“ UQL -3.00 6.00 1.89 1.89 -0.26 2.70 Child's AQN 2.00 29.00 13.91 5.42 0.51 2.75 Contacts AQL 0.20 1.00 0.58 0.18 -0.07 2.51 . UQN ' 2.00 27.00 12.90 “.96 0.86 3.03 UQL -3.00 8.00 2.UU 2.19 -0.39 3.2“ Family AQN 7.00 36.00 19.97 5.2“ 1.2” 5.17 Contacts AQL 0.19 1 00 0.55 0.15 0.99 3.28 UQN 1.00 26.00 8.70 u.u0 0.80 “.60 UQL -N.OO 9.00 1.71 2.33 -0.03 3.6“ 257 "I111111171111111111s