Irlllllllllllll I I I I I y 5m Ill/ll l ll?/l7’/Wl’//ii77ll‘f//l 7 u . 3 293 10671 6370 322?}; ' E‘ééahggaa 3222mm $22"??? 33 ms; 9“ {h 36;) This is to certify that the dissertation entitled The Cognitive Processes Of Competent Third Grade Writers: A Descriptive Study presented by Barbara J. Diamond has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PhD degree in Education Major professor Laura Roehler Date January 21, 1985 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 35‘3“ “.3312? ‘0 u-.. u o MSU LlBRARlES RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. THE COGNITIVE PROCESSES OF COMPETENT THIRD GRADE WRITERS: A DESCRIPTIVE STUDY By Barbara J. Diamond A DISSERTATION submitted to Michigan State University in partiaI fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Teacher Education I985 ABSTRACT THE COGNITIVE PROCESSES OF COMPETENT THIRD GRADE WRITERS: A DESCRIPTIVE STUDY BY Barbara J. Diamond The purpose of this study was to describe and explain the task environmental influences of three competent third grade writers and to describe the underlying cognitive processes involved in their writing. Key elements of the task environment identified for examination were the students' socio-cultural background, the teachers instruction, the student's interactions, and their oral language use. The writing processes of the students were examined for descriptions of the planning, transcribing and revising processes. This ethnographic study was conducted by the classroom teacher in her combination third/fourth grade classroom. While the teacher directed the study, observations were made by a research assistant, who was the investigator's teaching partner. The data consisted of field notes of writing instruction and student writing, student and parent interviews, student writing samples, and the teacher's journal. Two units of analysis, the group literacy episode and the individual literacy episode were structures in the observational data from which inductively reached categories were formed and charted. Within the individual literacy episodes writing behaviors of the students were analyzed by examining their "problem solving stops". The major findings revealed that the following elements of the task environment influenced the writing proceses of the students: I) the writing content of organization and structure that was emphasized by the teacher, and her de-emphasis on mechanics; 2) the formality of the group literacy episode; 3) the time restrictions imposed upon subjects and upon the school day; it) the Barbara J. Diamond literate socio-cultural backgrounds of the target students; and 5) the nature of student's interactions with peers, which were both distracting and helpful. The findings related to cognitive processes revealed that: I) these competent students were able to balance the cognitive constraints so that they could effectively express the meaning desired; 2) they each planned for writing, but in different ways; 3) they revised during the process of writing, two primarily for orthographics, structure, and handwriting, and one for meaning and audience; and it) they could readily express meaning because of the wide store of available knowledge. DEDICATION To Mom and Dad, John, Kim and Johnny 1'1' ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to thank the many people who offered help and support during the writing of this dissertation. I am especially grateful to: Dr. Laura Roehler, chairperson, who displayed a skillful combination of wisdom, patience, and encouragement in guiding this dissertation. She was always available; a valuable resource and a special friend. Dr. Frederick Erickson, Dr. Sheila Fitzgerald, and Dr. George Sherman, the members of my committee, for their expertise and advice they shared. Barbara Reeves, who was an extremely competent typist and who never expressed frustration or impatience in spite of occasional pressure filled circumstances. The children and staff at Riverton School who were wonderful to work with and whom I value so much as colleagues and friends. Mom, Dad, and my sister Pat, who have given so unquestionably of themselves in helping me achieve this goal. Kim and Johnny, my children, whose enthusiasm, humor and belief in me helped me to keep things in perspective. John, my husband, whose love, understanding and encouragement was always there, and without whom this dissertation and degree could not have been achieved. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Figures ..................... vi CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION .................... I The Problem ............................ 2 Significance ............................ 3 Overview ............................. 7 CHAPTER II: REVIEW OF LITERATURE ............ 8 Introduction ............................ 8 Composing Processes in the Classroom . . ............... 9 Cognitive Processes in Writing .................... [4 Writers Interacting Within the Classroom Community .......... 20 Summary .............................. 25 CHAPTER III: THE DESIGN OF THE STUDY, PROCEDURES, AND METHOD OF ANALYSIS . ............... 27 Research Method ................... - 27 Procedures ............................ 29 Selling .............................. 32 Data Analysis .......................... 4| Analysis of Group Literacy Episodes ............... 42 Analysis of Individual Literacy Episodes ........ - 46 Planning ......................... 49 Tronscribing ........................ 50 Reviewing/Revision .................... 50 Social Interaction ..................... 5| Other Considerations in the Data Analysis ............. 5| Summary .............................. 52 CHAPTER IV: FINDINGS ...................... 51; TOSk EHVironmeni .......................... 55 Socio-Cultural Background ................... 55 Mindy's Background .................... 55 Kathy's Background .................... 57 Anne's Background .................... 58 Teacher'slnstruction - - . What--Tasks Assigned/Content Emphasized How the Episodes Developed . . . How Students' Verbal Behaviors Related to Instruction. Summary of Teacher's Instructions ...... . . Student Interactions - - . . . . . Mindy's Interactions ----- . . Kathy's Interactions . - . . Anne's Interactions Oral Language and Its Relationship to Writing Students' Verbal Behaviors During Group Literacy Episodes Students' Oral Language During Interactions In Individual Literacy Episodes. Sub-vocalization During Individual Literacy Episodes . Summary of Task Environment . . . . . Cognitive Processes of the Target Students. Mindy. . . . . . Planning- - . . . . . . Transcribing - . . . . . . Review/Revisions- - . . . . Summary. . . .. . . . Kathy. . . . . Planning. . . . . Transcribing . '. Review/ Revision Summary - Anne . . . . . . Planning . Transcribing . . . Review/Revision Summary. . Summary of Findings . . . CHAPTER V: CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE AND RESEARCH - The Task Environment for Writing . Cognitive Processes of the Writers - ° Implications for Practice - Implications for Further Research ° A Final Thought Appendices. . . . Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . O O O O O O O O O O O O O o o O o .IIS .Il6 . I20 I20 I24 I28 I33 I34 I36 224 \lChU‘IJ-‘WN 00 LIST OF FIGURES Physical layout of the classroom Charting of group literacy episode for analysis . Charting of individual literacy episode for analysis . . Synoptic chart of group literacy episodes . . . . . Chart showing hierarchical and temporal organization . . . Organization for autobiographical sketch Functional typology of student verbal behavior during literacy episodes ................ Task environment for writing. . . . . ..... Excerpts of Mindy's story with teacher's comments - Excerpts of Kathy's story ..... . . . . . . . Excerpts of Anne's story, showing insertions - - . Planning, Transcribing, Reviewing/Revising Processes of Target Students ............... group 34 as . 147,48 . 62,63 64 65 74 90 - 96 |06, l07 -lllI -II7 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION One of the major goals of research on writing should be to impart information that can help teachers be more effective in providing writing instruction. In an effort to achieve this goal, research on writing has begun to flourish in the decade of the l970$ and has continued in the I980s. Researchers have looked at specific kinds of knowledge that writers possess and bring to bear as they formulate meanings and express these meanings in writing (Flower & Hayes, l98l; Nold, l98l; Scardamalia, I98I). These researchers have looked at the cognitive processes that enable the generation of meaning. In doing this, they have contributed to the understanding of writing as a cognitive activity. This knowledge in isolation, however, cannot readily be translated into implications for effective writing instruction. Other researchers have conducted investigations which examine the context or situation in which a person writes and its influence on writing processes (Graves, I982; Florio & Clark, I979). These situations and goals can be closely associated with instructional writing tasks and affect to a great extent how these tasks are structured. These contexts also include specific functions of written language that are defined within a writer's social and cultural group and his/her classroom community. Additionally, oral language functions as a part of social context of students' lives and is shown to influence written language in yet another way. Cook-Gumperz & Gumperz (I982) have contributed to the understanding of writing as a contextual activity. While the studies of context of writing are crucial to an understanding of the writing process, in order to actualize the goal of effective writing instruction this perspective alone is also inadequate. One approach to achieving this goal of providing information which can help teachers increase their effectiveness is to study the cognitive processes of students as they are involved in different kinds of writing activities in the classroom setting. Such an approach reflects the interdependences of the two perspectives--the cognitive and the contextual--on the process of writing. In conducting such an investigation, an attempt is made to bring together the internal and cognitive processes the writer uses to construct meaning with the external environment that shapes and constrains this meaning. The research undertaken here takes this approach. The Problem The purpose of this study is to describe and explain the task environmental influences of three competent third grade writers and to describe the underlying cognitive processes involved in their writing. The task environment includes the context of writing—instruction, situations in which these students write, and their purpose for writing (Frederiksen & Dominic, l98l). It also includes the socio-cultural environment and the social interaction that can influence writing as a cognitive activity. Cognitive irocesses are those mental activities employed in activating different kinds of knowledge that the writer has and can elicit in constructing meanings and expressing them in writing. m in this investigation is defined as the process of selecting, combining, arranging, and developing ideas in effective sentences, paragraphs, and often longer units of discourse (NCTE, l98l). The two major research questions entailed in the study are as follows. I. In what ways does the task environment constrain (influence) the writing process of three competent third grade writers? --How does each student's socio-cultural background affect the process of writing? «How does the teacher's instruction affect the process? -What influences do students' interactions with each other have on these students as they write? -What is the relationship between the children's oral language and what they do during the writing process? 2. What cognitive processes do these three competent third grade writers use in generating written text? --Do students evidence planning or Ere-writing activities? «What does the transcribing process (the written representation of ideas) look like? --Do students evidence the revision process? What is the nature of this process? --How do these processes interact? Significance The study is of special interest to practitioners and researchers because the findings (0) have practical and theoretical application, (b) will extend and clarify existing information, and (c) will answer the need to contribute added meaning to knowledge about writing. The findings will have practical application for teachers through the identification and description of environmental influences that can affect the writing process. Teachers are often faced with decisions regarding the amount of social interaction to allow in the classroom while providing an appropriate environment for learning. It is often the view held by both teachers and administrators that a quiet classroom is the most productive. The literature also shows a positive correlation between the time students spend on task and academic achievement (Fisher, Filby, Marliave, I977; Smyth, I979). Recently, however, in the area of writing, social interaction has been viewed as a means of enhancing students' writing, and findings show that classrooms of this type may provide an environment that is more conducive for producing writing (Britton, I970; Dyson 8. Genishi, I98l; Rosen 8. Rosen, I974). An analytic description of the task environment and how it affects the writing process can help teachers in making informed decisions that pertain to social interaction and writing. Findings that identify and describe cognitive processes of writers can also have practical applications. With the identification of these processes, a model can be developed which specifies what competent writers do. This model can serve as a guide to writing instruction and to diagnosis of writing difficulties (Hayes 8. Flower, l98l). Teachers, faced with the charge to teach writing, have often implemented instruction based on their knowledge of themselves as writers and conventional wisdom about what is important to teach. Indeed, processes such as planning, organizing, and editing have been identified as important by teachers for some time. However, investigation into the nature and use of these processes and the possible identification of other processes, particularly in young children, can lend support to already established practices and serve as a guide to the development of new practices. The identification of writing processes in children can help in establishing a theory of writing. Several researchers have worked toward the development of such a theory and have called for more data that can aid in its development (Collins & Gentner, I980; Shuy, l98l). Collins and Gentner envision a theory that would be both prescriptive and descriptive and one that would explicate underlying principles of the writing process. An ethnographic study, as is the one proposed, can be particularly effective in theoretical formulations because it is discovery-oriented, concerned with the particulars of context, the dynamics of social interactions, and the construction of meanings. It places the researcher in classrooms and allows him/her to develop a grounded theory (Glaser 8. Strauss, I967) based on what occurs in those settings (Kantor, Kirby, 8. Goetz, I98I). Information from the study can be significant to teachers and researchers in clarifying existing knowledge regarding the type of instructional support that is most appropriate for the development of competent writers. The literature offers essentially two views regarding the development of writing abilities and the roles that teachers should play. The first view held by some researchers is that learning to write is an extension of language development. These researchers view children's writing as a natural state in the overall development of language (Emig, l98l; Britton, I970; Loban, I976). This view sets forth the belief that although writing is natural, it is activated by enabling environments-- safe, structured, private, unobtrusive, and literate. Adults in these environments do not directly and explicitly teach information about writing; rather, they are fellow practitioners and providers of possible content and experiences. Bereiter (I980), on the other hand, holds that it is important for educational purposes to look at and identify the distinctive features of writing, rather than to treat it as an extension of other kinds of language development. By identifying these distinctive features of writing, Bereiter maintains that researchers and others can identify whatever is potentially susceptible to direct influence. One of the distinctive features identified, for example, is that because written utterances can be shaped and reshaped, writing lends itself to development of craftsmanlike skills not normally found in speaking. Educators feel that direct instruction may serve a useful purpose in helping students acquire these skills. These two views of writing development and teacher intervention reflect the types of decisions about instruction that teachers must make. They must decide when to directly intervene in students' writing development and/or how best to provide an "enabling environment" for the maximum growth of students. They must determine how much intervention is good and necessary in what is considered by some to be a naturally occurring process? The proposed study can aid in clarifying knowledge related to this issue. The study can also help to clarify conflicting findings regarding the identification of writing processes in young children. For example, many researchers and theorists hold the view that the planning and reviewing processes are late-blooming processes, not found in young children (Bereiter, I980; Flavell, I977). In fact, some experiments have shown that necessary activities for both planning and reviewing, such as examining the message for faults and taking the role of the audience are late-developing activities (Markman, I977). The research shows that children simply write down thoughts as they come to mind, giving no thought to clarity of message and reader understanding. Other researchers have reported conflicting findings. Scardamalia (I98l) has found that children displayed a sense of audience in an essay writing task by addressing the reader. Graves (I980) has identified the process of rehearsal for writing in first graders. These students use drawing and talking as a means of planning topic and story content. This rehearsal process in children is analogous to the planning process identified in older students and adults. Graves (I979) has also identified the beginnings of the reviewing process in young writers, and Calkins (l98l) has found that third graders review their writing frequently and in different ways. With the conflicting evidence among researchers about the existence of given processes in children, there is a clear need for clarification as to what processes are evidenced in young children and an extension of knowledge about the nature of these processes. Finally, and significantly, there is a need to contribute added meaning to knowledge about writing. This study proposes to identify and describe the cognitive nature of processes in competent third grade writers. This analytic description will add support and meaning to information about processes that has been identified by other researchers. Graves and his colleagues have conducted extensive research on writing processes of children over a two year period. They have stated, however, that More needs to be learned about what occurs within the writing episode in the 805. We are just beginning to get a sense of the ingredients in the process, but far more data are needed to explain how children function (Graves, l98l, p. I99). This study can provide a portion of the needed data. Overview The chapters which follow are arranged in the following manner: Chapter II presents a review of the literature related to the study; Chapter III gives the design of the study, the procedures used, and the method of data analysis; Chapter IV presents findings which answer the questions raised at the outset; and Chapter V provides discussion of the findings and implications of the study for instruction and future research. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Introduction In recent years much of the research on writing has shifted from product- oriented research from which processes were inferred to research directed toward actual behavior of writers throughout the composing process. Studies which sought to determine the relative efficacy of one treatment over another have become less frequent. Rather, descriptive investigations and case studies from which relevant hypotheses can be generated and a theoretical framework about written discourse can emerge have become prevalent (Cooper 8. Odell, I978). The type of inquiry which looked at the conditions under which students wrote became prominent as an outgrowth of basic research in the last two decades on syntactic development (Hunt, I965; O'Donnel, Griffin, 8. Norris, I967; Smith 8. Swan, I978; Loban, I976), rhetorical development (Britton et aI., I975; Kroll, I978; Moffett, I968), and composing processes (Emig, l97l; Stallard, I974; Graves, I975; Flowers & Hayes, I977). Because of findings from this body of research on processes involved in producing written discourse, it became a natural next step to ask questions about how the observed phenomena occurred in the natural setting of the school and the classroom. This literature review reflects the view advanced in this study that writing occurs (and thus should be studied) within a task environment in the classroom which interfaces with the cognitive activities and processes in which the writer engages. Therefore, the literature related to the study addresses the area of interest by categorizing the research about (a) the composing process in the classroom, primarily reflecting the orientation of English and language arts' educators; (b) the more detailed cognitive processes in writing, reflecting the views of cognitive psychologists; and (c) writers' social and academic interactions within the classroom environment, reflecting the views of educational anthropologists and sociolinguists. Because literature is being assimilated that will help to construct a "story of writing" and because research on writing processes is relatively new and somewhat limited, the literature bearing on this study does not always deal with child writers as subjects. In studies in which adult or older student writers are used as subjects, the studies are discussed because of the systematic and detailed identification and description of cognitive processes they provide. In these cases, care has been taken to clearly identify the age of the sample group or individual. Composing Processes in the Classroom During the last six years, Graves and his colleagues have done extensive research in the classroom setting with young children, which, in addition to providing insight into behaviors children engage in during the process of writing, raises questions about specific cognitive activities in which students engage during writing. Graves' initial study (I975) involved the use of the case study method, analysis of broad samples of children's writing, and the naturalistic observations of children while writing. He identified characteristic behaviors of students as they wrote, which categorized them into two types: I. reactive writers: these children showed erratic problem solving strategies, the use of overt language strategies, the use of overt language to accompany pre—writing and composing phases, isolation that evolved in action-reaction couplets, proof reading at the word unit level, a need for immediate rehearsal in order to write, rare contemplation or reviewing of products, characterizations that exhibited general behaviors similar to This Graves study was the first of elementary aged children as they actually engaged characteristic behaviors gave subsequent researchers a provocative point of departure from which to investigate more deeply. In fact, Graves (I979) himself engaged in further research, looking at revision as an important aspect of writing. He examined and documented how and in what order primary children change their composing, spelling, and motor behavior during the writing process. In this study of three students in a first grade classroom, several findings related their own, a lack of a sense of audience when writing, and an inability to use reasons beyond the affective domain in evaluating their writing; and reflective writers: these children exhibited little rehearsal before writing, little overt language to accompany writing, periodic rereadings to adjust small units of writing at the word or phrase level, growing sense of audience connected with their writing, characterizations that exhibit general behaviors similar to their own in the expression of feelings, and the ability to give examples to support their reasons for evaluating writing. to revision were presented. These included the following: children revise in other media forms such as block building, drawing, and painting before they revise in writing. Children who demonstrate an overall learning stance toward revision in another area are more likely to demonstrate it in an area such as writing; when children try a new approach to writing, other areas in which they might have been competent may suffer temporarily; beginning writers do not revise; toward the end of the primary years, many children reach a point of equilibrium when handwriting and spelling problems are behind them and messages flow easily onto the paper. Children do not revise these messages; peer audiences have an effect on children's revision and their use of new approaches to the writing process; and when children no longer erase but cross out, draw lines and arrows for new information arrangements, or change their handwriting to a scrawl, they indicate a changed view toward words. in writing in the classroom. Identification of these In another research report related to revision, Calkins (I979), a colleague of Graves', discussed findings based on her study of one advanced third grader and the sequence of problems she faced as she changed her approach to revision. The student found that writing consisted initially of choosing a topic and putting down ideas. The writer's words at this time were pre-determined and final. Calkins documents the change that occurred over a three month period as the writer made her first revision, became less bound to one neat paper and made several drafts, and depended less on teacher intervention for approval. For this student the process of revision was gradually internalized. The revisions made by the subject in the Calkins study were made initially as a result of teacher intervention in the writing process. This leaves the question unanswered as to when in the process children see the need to revise on their own and what knowledge or mental activity brings this to bear. This study of cognitive process can address this issue. Another phase of the writing process that has been investigated is what has been identified as the pre-writing phase. Sowers (I979) looked at the writing of a beginning first grader over a five month period. The pre-writing phase was marked by activities of talking and drawing. Before the subject wrote, she would draw a scene, then talk about it. As she progressed through the writing, she continued to compose aloud. Sower's findings about children's behaviors during the pre—writing period are consistent with those of other researchers (Britton, I970; Graves, I975; Rosen, I979). Gundlach (l98l) used compositions by primary children to make inferences about the nature of writing processes. He, too, saw a connection between writing and speaking. He stated the following about one incident: We have observed children occasionally producing a soundtrack as they compose--as in the case of the second grade boy who made the sounds of a revving engine and squealing tires as he draw a picture of a racing car, then wrote about the new toy racing car he had recently acquired (Gundlach 8. Moses, I976, p. I40). Dyson and Genishi (I982) studied the writing processes of two six-year old first graders to determine (a) the purposes for which each child used oral language when composing and (b) how the writing was affected by social context. The findings showed that both children used oral language chiefly to encode words to print, to transform their messages from something spoken to something written. The results showed further that the "muttering" of young children to themselves and their "chattering" with each other can be valuable and, for some children, critical factors in the process of learning to write. Dyson (I983) used participant observation to examine the role of oral language in early writing. In a writing corner set up in a kindergarten classroom, children were simply asked to write according to their own definitions of writing. The researcher observed and interacted with the children to gain insight into their perceptions of writing and their reasoning about writing behaviors. Dyson found that the writing center was a hub of both oral and graphic activity. The oral language was an integral part of the early writing processes. Talk provided both meaning and, for some children, the systematic means for getting that meaning on paper. Any thematic content of the writing product often evolved in the task preceding writing. Talk was also used to elaborate on the full meaning of products to audiences. Further, oral language was a tool for seeking needed information, assisting self in encoding and decoding, and, finally, distancing self from work (i.e., expressing evaluation of completed work). This study strongly suggests a relationship of oral language and written language. It would appear that it is a means of assisting in encoding for young writers and perhaps is useful in helping the student understand and incorporate meaning from his/her external environment into his/her writing. Literature which contributes to an understanding of the latter area will be presented in the category of "Writers Interacting Within the Classroom Community." The behavior that the writer shows from the time first words are put on paper until all drafts are completed is referred to by Graves (I983) as composing. He enlists data from three writers in pointing out the complexity and variance in how writers compose. Each writer has an image of what s/he is trying to say, and each begins by laboring to write the correct words to portray the image. Of himself, Graves writes, "I write a line. It doesn't fit. I try another line. A dead end. I clean my study, make phone calls, eat, return and write some more . . . . I wonder when the great breakthrough will come." Graves contention that all writers follow a simple pattern is not easily documented in the literature. What is clear is that composing is a process in which several different behaviors can occur. Two other studies are worthy of mention here. The first, Emig's (l97l), is important not only because it was the first to examine writing behaviors as students engaged in writing, but because it looked at nine components of the composing process. While this study does not deal with young children as subjects, the components of the composing process and behaviors identified may be useful as points of comparisons between young writers and older writers. Emig examined the writing behavior of eight l2th grade student writers using case study procedures. Each subject met four times with the investigator in an experimental setting in which s/he was asked to compose aloud and/or generate a written selection. Nine components of the composing process were identified: (a) context, (b) nature of stimuli, (c) pre—writing and planning, (d) starting, (e) composing aloud, (f) stopping and contemplating the product, (9) reformulation, and (h) seeming influence on writing by teachers of composition. Emig also identified two modes of composing that students exhibited. These are: I. reflexive: this writing was most often poetry, characterized as having a long pre-writing period in which starting, stopping, and contemplating the product occurred and reformulation occurred frequently. The self is the chief audience in this mode; and 2. extensive: this writing was chiefly prose and characterized by writing discourse that is often detached and reportorial. Audience is adult others, notably teachers. The second study, Stallard's (I974), also looked at senior high school writers who were asked to write on a preselected assignment topic area. Student behaviors observed included the following: (a) planning behavior, (b) revision, (c) rate of writing, (d) audience awareness, (e) consideration of purpose, (f) stylistic concerns, (9) attitudes toward writing, and (h) the practice of stopping to read during the composing process. Stallard found that good writers spent more time both contemplating and completing the writing task than did randomly selected students. He also found that good writers made more revisions and made them during the process of re-reading their papers. The next category of research presents literature that looks more deeply into the mental activities which can account for some of the student writing behaviors identified here. Cognitive Processes in Writing Studies which look in greater depth at some of the processes cited above can provide additional information from the theoretical perspective of cognitive and developmental psychology. Flowers and Hayes (l98l) and Nold (l98l) identified component processes of writing that, at a gross level, can be segmented into the following categories: I. pre-writing or planning during which the writer plans, organizes, and discovers information; 2. articulating/translatigg/transcribigq when the writer puts ideas into words; and 3. post-writing/reviewing/revisiLg when the writer evaluates and modifies text. These processes were more finely delineated by Hayes and Flowers (I980) through a technique of protocol analysis, a technique typically used by cognitive psychologists in problem solving tasks, in which novice college writers were asked to think aloud into a tape recorder as they wrote. Through the analysis of a number of protocols over a two year period, the authors proposed a tentative model of the writing process. The authors divided the writer's world into three major parts: (a) the task environment, (b) the writer's long-term memory, and (c) the writing process. The model describes the writing process. The task environment and the writer's long-term memory are the context in which the model operates. A brief description of the model follows: I. task environment: includes everything outside the writer's skin that influences the performance of the task-~writing assignment, intended audience, motivation, and, once writing has begun, the text which the writer has produced; 2. writer's long-term memory: includes writer's world knowledge (about people, games, story, characters, and many audiences); also includes generalized writing plans, perhaps in the form of a story grammar; and 3. the writing process: consists of three major processes: planning, translating, and reviewing. The planning process consists of sub- processes of generating, organizing, and goal setting. The reviewing process consists of reading and editing. The authors feel that "although the model is provisional, it provides a first approximate description of normal composition that can guide research and afford a valuable starting point in the search for more refined models" (p. l0). Nold (I98I) advanced a model of the writing process which is similar to that of Hayes and Flowers. Her model of plan/transcribe/review describes these component processes of writing as recursive and occurring in irregular patterns. She asserts, for example, that planning occurs regularly in the middle of draft writing, not only before transcribing, and rewriting actually occurs throughout the writing of the draft. Nold identified the products of the planning sub-process as a representation of (a) the writer's intended meaning, (b) the writer's intended audience, and (c) the persona the writer wishes to project through his/her writing. She identified two kinds of constraints that influence the transcribing process: I. glgbal plans that reflect a writer's conception of meaning, audience, and persona; and 2. local plans that reflect the writer's knowledge about language such as syntax, vocabulary, and orthography. The reviewing sub-process is bounded by the planning sub-process that has preceded it. Consequently, young writers who do not make elaborate plans about meaning, audience, and persona can be expected to review their texts only against the conventions of written English they hold in memory. Nold cautions against the assumption that review and revision are one-time processes that occur at the end of a writing session. Rather, revising and rewriting occur throughout the writing of a draft. The development of a model of writing which has much in common with models of speech production was advanced by Bereiter (I979). It includes: I. high level executive schema which directs the whole operation (of writing? 2. genre schemas which consist of knowledge available for directing certain kinds of writing; 3. content processor which draws semantic material from memory and organizes it according to instruction from genre schema. Its output is called a gist; and 4. language processor which receives the gist and puts it into explicit language. With this model in mind, Bereiter feels that one interpretation of writing development is that it consists of the gradual elaboration and refinement of relevant schemes at different processing levels. The above models of the writing process are based on data obtained from adult skilled writers. However, the models can provide a body of information against which to examine and understand data obtained in this study and to see which part(s) of a model fit child writers. Scardamalia, Bereiter, and Goelman (l98l) have examined and advanced their views about ways in which the conditions of text production may influence cognitive processes in composition. Two of the conditions are particularly relevant here because of the focus of this study on both the internal (cognitive) and the external (environmental) processes of writing. The first condition was based on the hypothesis that interference from the mechanical requirements of the written language interferes with high level concerns of composition. This belief is firmly embedded in conventional wisdom. In an experimental procedure with fourth and sixth graders, the researchers were able to separate results of the effects of mechanical (spelling, punctuation) interference from effects of rate. This was done by introducing a third production mode in addition to writing and normal dictation. This third mode, slow dictation, was dictated by the child to an experimenter who transcribed according to each child's previously- determined writing rate. Thus it was possible to isolate the effects of mechanical interference by comparing writing with slow dictation, since they were equivalent in speed. The results indicated that the low level requirements of writing do make a difference to children. If these are removed, children produce more and do it a great deal faster. With respect to quality, however, the differences were small (significant at the .06 level) and indicated that they had only a weak effect, if any, on higher level components of the writing process. In fact, after getting students to increase their quantity by production signaling, the researchers found that writing was the superior medium for producing coherent and well-expressed composition. The second condition was based on the hypothesis that there is discoordination in processing resulting from lack of external signals. This presupposes a model of the composing process consisting of sub-processes governed by an executive system which orchestrates or regulates the sub- processes of writing. Scardamalia et al. state that "it is probably this factor more than any other that limits the novice's ability to profit from being taught the strategies of the expert." The novice is so involved in mastering the sub- processes that s/he has little spare capacity for implementing more sophisticated executive procedures. The theory behind this second investigation stems from a theory of speech production. The executive system controls speech production, and this same executive system is adapted to writing. The authors contend that because the system is initially oriented to speech production and thus designed to respond to signals from the external environment, without such input the system has trouble functioning. Thus, in their experiment, the authors provided an external cue (normally missing in writing) to increase quantity of text produced by fourth and sixth grade subjects. The results showed that (0) children added more material by elaborating on points they had made previously and (b) this intervention led children to extend a coherent string of text units. How children learn to sustain language production without external signals is a question raised by this study. Scardamalia et al. give a more refined view of the writing process in looking at component processes and two of the production factors that impinge upon them. Matsuhashi (I982) gave yet another close view of the writing process as she looked at the writing process "in motion; the writer engaged." She held that the temporal aspect of writing can give clues to the writing process; specifically, the BLUE between words. Her purpose was to locate some patterns in observational pause data that could suggest how planning requirements for generalizing and reporting (writing) differ. Although her subjects were adult writers, Matsuhashi's method of analysis and her findings could have implications for this study. She found that: l. whatever range of plans--whether local or global--the writer was operating under, schematic representation of knowledge and experience inform text production at all times; and 2. when subjects paused to re—read, they were planning for new content that would move the discourse forward. Matsuhashi further made two broad claims about planning: I. planning does not correspond to grammatical units; rather it corresponds to psychological processing and is based on underlying conceptual content; and 2. long pauses accompanied by gazing or re-reading activity and by removing the pen from the page corresponded to multiple decisions, generally ones which encompass global issues as well as local ones. It is apparent that there are many constraints and cognitive demands placed on the writer as s/he plans, transcribes, and reviews his/her writing (Bereiter, I980). Scardamalia (I982) looked at one aspect of how children cope with the cognitive demands of writing specifically in their ability to coordinate an increasing number of ideas in writing. Children of different ages (IO-I4) were asked to (a) write about the information contained in a four-celled matrix and (b) defend a thesis. In the first task children were taught how to read matrices and instructed to write a paragraph or sentence containing all the information in the matrix. The results of student writing were classified into four levels of integration from low to high. In the second task students were asked to express 20 a position on an issue. Scardamalia found that students had great difficulty in putting thought together in writing. In fact, she found that writing presented obstacles to complex idea coordination. In further discussing the findings, she stated There is a paradox underlying children's difficulties with idea integration in writing. Young children who are particularly constrained by limited resources do less planning, less notetaking, less revising, and less reading than adults. Thus, the very activities adults use to break up this complex activity into manageable sub- routines are what children fail to take advantage of. (p. l00) While Scardamalia explained the "demandingness" of writing and held that there were no easy solutions, she offered, "The challenge to researchers and educators is to understand the various cognitive processes that accompany writing, their demand characteristics and how the multiple processes involved in writing get coordinated" (p. l0l). Thus far, processes at the gross levels of planning, transcribing, and reviewing have been identified. Researchers have offered evidence and conceptual discussion about cognitive activities that occur when the planning, transcribing, and reviewing, are being actualized. Although many of the studies discussed conceptions of how adult writers use cognitive activities as they write, they are important here because they (a) increase the state of knowledge about these processes and (b) underscore the need for this study in helping to describe these processes in children. Research which looks at the environment in which these processes occur can give added information and insight into writing in the classroom. Writers Interacting Within the Classroom Community The third category of research that has implications for this study is the research reflecting the perspectives of the sociolinguists and anthropologists. Researchers in this group provide a richer understanding of life in classrooms, 2l revealing the diversity of students and the complexity of communicating in context. These descriptions serve as a source of new ideas for investigating the processes of teaching and learning. The fact that writing is a process of communications and that it is learned/developed in a context dominated by oral interactions make it important for this study to examine findings from these perspectives. Cook-Gumperz and Gumperz (l98l) point out the primacy of oral language in the child's culture in the early years and that the attribution of meaning, both cognitive and social must be mediated through the oral medium during this time. When children enter school, however, adults study and evaluate their performance and competence from a written literacy perspective. Students must not only learn to write but learn to read and get meaning from the written word. They must learn to move from the contextually-oriented oral tradition of the home to the decontextualized, literate tradition of the school (Cook- Gumperz 8. Gumperz, l98l; Olson 8. Torrence, l98l). There is a differential distribution of ability to move from the oral to the literate tradition which may have an effect on acquisition of written competence in students. Scollon and Scollon (I978), in studying their daughter Rachel's socialization into literacy, found that before she reached the age of three, she had adopted many of the important literacy skills necessary to success in school that her parents possessed. Examples of socialization in the child's life before school were continuous with the school preparation of literacy in many segments of Western society. Scollon and Scollon express the view that children from orientations other than those of the majority culture might find it difficult to develop the literate orientation in spite of heavy teaching of grammar, spelling, and oral display of reading. 22 Collins and Michaels (I980) in studying oral narratives of first and fourth grade students were able to discover distinct differences in discourse strategies among the students. The oral narratives of some of the children showed consistent use of lexical and syntactic devices which resembled the discourse style of middle class, literate adults. Other children relied more on prosodic cues which reflected an oral discourse style. Further, in the written version of two fourth grade narratives, the same stylistic dichotomy was evident. Because children first learn to communicate through oral language, it might be expected that students' background plays an important part in literacy learning and in the processes involved in that learning. The interaction between teacher and student and the context in which children are exposed to literacy instruction has an effect in determining student competency in both receiving and producing language and can influence cognitive processes involved in both modes. DeStefano, Pepinsky, and Sanders (I982) described rules of discourse operating within the framework of literacy instruction (in a desegregated classroom) of first graders. The researchers examined (a) teacher-initiated talk and their replies to talk initiated by others, concentrating on academic rather than social interaction; (b) cohesion in text to determine whether student talk tended to become more related to teacher talk; and (c) grammatical structuring of student talk to determine structural changes in patterns of talk over time. The results of the various analyses showed that there were relatively few open bids on the teacher's part for initiating talk in the classroom. Most student discourse consisted of student responses to nominations by their teacher to have them talk. Spontaneous bids on the students' part tended to be relatively infrequent as the teacher, for the most part, demanded, commanded, questioned, and exhorted, eliciting largely single words in response from students. There appeared to be no mutually devised "social construction of reality"--no 23 reciprocity between teacher and students. The teacher clearly controlled the discourse. The study points out the teacher's influence in shaping and structuring the processes of literacy learning. Mosenthal and Jin Na (I98l), in investigating the way students compose text, found that differences are related to the type of speech register or verbal response pattern students most often adopted in verbally interacting with a teacher. The subjects were first classified into those who employed either a non-contingent, imitative, or contingent response register. Students were then asked to compose a description of a picture stimulus. Noncontingent students tended to write more interpretive essays and use more interpretive propositions in devising their compositions than did the other two register groups. The authors felt that one interpretation of these findings is that in addition to the writer attending to the form and meaning of external and internal events, the writer attends to a third type of meaning which is social or situated meaning (Cook-Gumperz, I977). The manner in which writers integrate external form and meaning with schemata is determined, in part, by the way the writers understand the social context in which they are writing (Doyle, I979; Winograd, I977; Freedle 8. Duran, I979). Erickson (I982) views talk in lessons and interactions between teacher and student "as the collective improvisation of meaning and social organization from moment to moment." He views school lessons in terms of an academic task structure and a social participation structure and sees both aspects of organization as mutually constitutive. Successful participation in a lesson requires the student to have knowledge of both subject matter organization and its logical organization as well as knowledge of discourse and its social organization. 24 In examining a number of instances of the performance of a small lesson sequence, Erickson discovers an underlying ideal model of a lesson. He finds, however, in looking at an instance of the lesson sequence, that some features of specific organization deviate from the general inferred model. These discrepancies represent "adoptive" action taken by teacher and student (in one instance) and have consequences for both the academic task structure and the social participation structure. As students gain meaning for writing, these two structures interact. The nature of the interaction can influence processes in writing. The inseparability of the academic-cognitive and social-interpersonal dimensions of classroom life are underscored in a study by Florio and Clark (I982) in which they examine children's use of writing in the classroom. The study was conducted over a two year period in a second/third grade classroom. In this open-space type classroom, students were encouraged to make choices about timing and format of their academic and social activities. They also worked in a variety of interactional arrangements, including small groups, teacher-led groups, mixed-age tutorials, and learning centers. The organization of the classroom in this way encouraged and legitimized writing. Through such an organization, teacher and children interacted and negotiation of choice in learning was possible. Further, the organization helped to "ensure that more than formal, teacher-generated, written monologues were undertaken." Florio and Clark identified the following four functions of writing: I. writing to participate in community, 2. writing to know oneself and others, 3. writing to occupy free time, and l; . writing to demonstrate academic competence. 25 These functions reflect both the oral tradition and literate tradition and allow for practice of discourse functions which differ from those normally associated with school writing. Summary The purpose of this study is to describe and explain the task environmental influence of three competent third grade writers and to describe and explain the underlying cognitive processes involved in writing. The literature review has presented the research in three categories that are relevant to this study in an attempt to provide insight into the state of the knowledge in the area of writing process research. The literature in the first category provides a good background of the composing processes but, at the same time, offers support for the need for this study. While the literature makes clear that children can make revisions in their writing when teachers intervene, for example, it raises the question of the presence of revision in writers without teacher intervention. It also leaves the question of the nature of this revision unanswered. The importance of oral language and writing is identified in studies of beginning writers. However, the relationship of orality and writing in older children needs further explication. Pre-writing as a component process of writing is established and explained in younger writers, but what and how older elementary writers plan and rehearse for writing needs further clarification. Finally, certain patterns of behavior in composing are advanced, but there is clearly a need to look more deeply into the mental activities involved in composing, especially in light of information provided by more detailed studies of older writers. The second category of research, that related to cognitive processes of writers, provides models of the writing process which identify three sub- 26 processes (planning, transcribing, and reviewing). The literature further addresses the influences on cognitive processes that the conditions of text production may have. Researchers also examine pause data and the implication of pauses between words for the planning and transcribing sub-processes. The thrust of the literature presented points out the number of demands placed on writers, particularly young writers, and the importance of attempting to understand how processes involved in writing get coordinated by these young writers. This study of competent writers will further examine these processes. The third category of research relates the importance of oral language as the basis of written language for young writers. The literature also suggests oral language is important because it is the medium through which communication between students and teachers and between students and students occurs and that this communicative interaction has implications for what writers understand as meaning and how these writers use this meaning as they engage in the writing process. Studies presented in this category point out the fact that learning occurs within a task environment that includes academic-cognitive and social- interpersonal structures. This fact makes a strong case for the study of writing processes within a framework that takes these two structures into account. CHAPTER III THE DESIGN OF THE STUDY, PROCEDURES, AND METHOD OF ANALYSIS The major questions of this study related to the issues of task environment for writing and the cognitive processes in which competent students engage as they write within the task environment. Specifically, the major questions are: I. In what ways does the task environment constrain (influence) the writing process of three competent third grade writers? -How does each student's socio-cultural background affect the process of writing? «How does the teacher's instruction affect the process? —-What influences do students' interactions with each other have on these students as they write? -What is the relationship between the children's oral language and what they do during the writing process? 2. What cognitive processes do these three competent third grade writers use in generating written text? --Do students evidence planning or pre-writing activities? -What does the transcribin process (the written representation of ideas) look like? --Do students evidence the revision process? What is the nature of this process? --How do these processes interact? Research Method The investigation of cognitive processes of competent third grade writers was conducted using ethnographic methodology with the teacher as principal investigator. Ethnography was found to be most suitable because it (0) supports 27 28 the notion of researcher as participant, (b) is discovery-oriented, and (c) is concerned with the particulars of context and social interactions. In this study all of these factors are vital. The first factor, support for researcher as participant was important because this researcher served a dual role--that of researcher gm teacher in the classroom. Therefore, she was a participant in the fullest sense of the word. It was important that the teacher role not be viewed as intrusive to the research process, but as a useful and even necessary part of the inquiry. As teacher, certain background knowledge, experiences, and predispositions were present and bound to be continually farming as the study proceeded. Ethnography supports the subjective judgments of the researcher in a way that allows her to combine her responses with a systematic scrutiny of participants' behavior. Wax (l97l) maintains that the successful ethnographer develops the ability to maintain a dual identity, insider and outsider, and to present authentically the experiences of people being studied. This methodology, more than any other, lends itself to teacher as investigator and holds that there can be advantages to the dual role. This study is concerned with process. Because processes are dynamic, continually developing, and occur in real time, it was important to use a method that could be sensitive to the sometimes subtle changes in behavior that signal the occurrences of cognitive processes in writing. Implicit in most ethnographic inquiry is a discovery process in which a variety of questions are addressed, possible answers proposed, and alternative explanations suggested as the study proceeds. Because of the probing, discovery-oriented process, ethnography provides the most appropriate method of inquiry of a study of the cognitive processes in writing. The fact that this investigation is concerned with the task environment in which writing occurs underscores the importance of the third characteristic of 29 the ethnographic method cited above. Contextual elements in ethnographic inquiry are deeply rooted and reflective of the research model developed by an anthropologists and fieldwork sociologists. Human behavior and values as manifested in group interaction are viewed as significant in both disciplines. It is the view advanced in this investigation that a study of processes in which the physical, natural, and socio-cultural environments are also studied is crucial to understanding and explaining these writing processes. Ethnography is highly compatible with this aspect of the study. Procedures This teacher-investigator was concerned with capturing the context in which target students wrote and the cognitive processes they used within this context. It was important then to focus on both the student and the wider social context of the environment. As teacher, it was impossible to conduct systematic observations and to effectively carry on normal teaching duties. For this reason, observation of the classroom procedures and individual students were made by a research assistant who was also the afternoon teacher of the students, the person with whom the teacher-investigator shared teaching responsibilities. Prior to data collection, the research assistant was trained in data collection techniques. VTraining was conducted during two Saturday afternoon sessions and two after school sessions in which literature on ethnographic techniques was read and discussed. In addition, fieldnotes taken by the investigator and other ethnographic researchers during other research studies were examined by the research assistant. The investigator pointed out salient features of note taking and observation during this time. Two practice sessions were conducted in the classroom to further train the research assistant and to de-sensitize all the 30 students to her presence in the room as participant observer rather than as teacher. During note-taking and observations, the research assistant moved to several different locations in the room. When making observations of the whole class, she distanced herself from the group and sat or stood in the back of the room. At other times, the research assistant was interested in observing two or three writers at once. During these observations, she positioned herself in the aisle between the writers. Finally, when observing one writer only, the observer sat to the writer's side and slightly behind him/her. The writers seemed comfortable with this arrangement; and from this vantage point, the research assistant was able to observe in detail when necessary and also interact and informally question students about their writing behaviors as the writing progressed. The teacher-investigator made every attempt to teach writing as she had before the study began, as she made a conscious effort to maintain consistency in teaching style and delivery. Procedurally, writing was conducted in the same way, also, with a pre-writing experience designed to provide background and information about writing before the students began to write, followed by independent writing with the teacher's providing help as needed. This help was with idea development, language usage, word choice, and other writing skills. The only conscious change was that writing was scheduled at least three times a week rather than the usual two times. Students were informed that a study was being conducted, the purpose of which was to learn more about students' writing. Because the study was not an experiment in which one group's performance was measured against or compared to another's, the effect of subject contamination or the Hawthorne effect was not an issue. Rather, the fact that all writers were observed during the study 3| would make the possibility of effects of increased writing performance, if any, likely to occur across all writers. Further, the most important factor in maintaining the validity of the study in regard to subjects is the intensity of the observations. As the amount of direct observation increased, the students become increasingly desensitized to the fact that they were being observed. Therefore, the likelihood of the subjects "faking" or "putting on an act" was decreased. The observations were conducted first with a broad focus to capture the physical and social .context in which learners Operate. The focus was then narrowed to target students as they engaged in writing. What was ultimately required were changes and shifts in focus as individuals were intently involved in writing, in social interactions, and in the overall learning environment. These observational focus decisions were guided by the teacher-investigator when feasible. The data were collected during the months including the end of January through April, I983, and consisted of the following: I. observation and field notes of classroom writing activities and target individuals, gathered two mornings a week; 2. student journals written in three or four mornings a week, which provided a data source of unassigned, uncorrected writing, representative of students' feelings, concerns, and daily experiences; 3. student writing samples including letters, creative writing, expository writing, and informal notes generated by teacher request and by the students themselves, both in school and at home; 4. student interviews of target students (one formal and several informal) by which to capture the students' thoughts and feelings about writing. These were both formal and informal; and 5. teacher journal kept two or three days a week and including plans for and reflections on the teaching of writing, observations of the students' reactions to their writing, and theoretical notes as insights occurred. 32 Field notes provided the most extensive source of data and were reviewed daily with the research assistant. The student journals provided data about how students write when given a free rein. Student interviews were useful as students reflected on their writing and shared their perceptions of themselves as writers. The interviews also served as triangulationI with the fieldnotes, teacher journals, and writing samples. The teacher journal provided an important link between the field notes taken by the research assistant and the teacher's perceptions of what was occurring. Three videotape recordings were made and were to be a part of the data source. However, due to the quality of the tapes, analysis was difficult and the tapes did not contribute enough significant additional information to warrant their use. Setting Riverton School2 was the site of the study. This school is the oldest school in Riverton, a mid-Michigan community which is the home of a large university. There are two stories to the school, with primary classrooms housed on the first floor and intermediate classrooms (grades 3-5) housed on the second floor. The rooms are large and airy, and most rooms have a small room adjoining the large room which provides additional work space. The student population of Riverton School is 2l0. The population is predominantly white, with black, Hispanic, and Asian students comprising approximately l8% of the student enrollment. The attendance area includes three distinct neighborhoods. One-third of the students come from an area of middle income residences that surround the school. Many of the parents of these lThe process of comparing different types of data with one another to test working hypotheses. 2Pseudonyms are used for the school, town, and students. 33 students are employed by the nearby University. Another one-third come from the student population that is bussed from a middle income area of single family homes. The final one-third reside in a low-middle income housing development of town houses; they are also bussed to the school. The racial composition from the latter areas reflects the racial composition of the general school population. The classroom was a third/fourth grade combination with 24 students: two Asians, four blocks, and l8 Caucasian students. The students represented a range of academic achievement from slightly below grade level equivalent in reading/language arts and mathematics to well above grade level in these areas, as measured by standardized achievement tests. In the teacher's judgment, about three-fourths of the class was highly motivated in all academic areas. The other one-fourth often had to be motivated by external means, such as small rewards. The three target students were highly motivated. The room was arranged so that students were seated in rows facing the chalkboard as noted in Figure l. Earlier in the year the desks had been arranged in clusters of four and a horseshoe arrangement. While this facilitated group interaction, the students' attentiveness and completion of assignments declined. A discussion between the teacher-investigator and her teaching partner resulted in the decision to use the arrangement described in the study which helped increase attentiveness and still allowed for student interaction in other areas. The room was bright and airy, with tall windows on one wall of the room. There was student work displayed on the bulletin board, along the walls under the chalkboard, and in the small adjoining room. The carpeted area in the back of the room was used for class discussion and small informal group meetings. The table in the back was used for individual and group meetings of a more formal nature. 34 T‘llldlbo If... I CI. 9 n — - . 00:31:... . I 0 . I s o .0. I . . n I 5 .. s o . n I . a . O u I .«l '4 '1 '2'.'.'.' 1'3". '1 '. . ' 3“ ”If-liafi'.‘ifirt'll'ds'l‘ wk shell F I u D ' . ' > ‘ . . I .u I. . . ' l ’. ‘ l r - ' ' S ' n . . .l IIL‘: .5 L . , , 1 .. m 6 I III I =, Physical layout of the classroom. Figure l. 35 In the small room, students worked in pairs or in groups on a variety of activities such as practicing a play or writing a group report. This room had the advantage of allowing students to talk more freely without disturbing others. The following example highlights this point. Mindy, Kathy, Kate, Yvette, Shelly are all working in small room. Teacher working in other room helping students individually. Mary and Kathy working together on George Washington Carver report. Exchanging ideas. There is disharmony; arguing over who would write and who would look the information up in the book. (Fieldnotes, 2/l7/83) The general atmosphere in the room was of "relaxed control." The teacher controlled the ebb and flow of classroom activity, giving students explicit cues as to what behavior was expected and when. During the beginning of one writing period, for example, after passing out papers the teacher told the students, "You have l0 minutes until recess-either use that time to write or think of an idea for writing." A few minutes later, she said, "The people in the front of the room are off to a good start." And later in the same IO-minute period, "Many of you should be working a lot quieter" (Fieldnotes, l/27/83). With the exception of discussion times, students were generally able to move freely around the room. In addition they could go to the library to read, write, or complete assignments. The student did not have to ask permission, but would take a pass and place it on his/her desk so the teacher would know where the student was. During independent writing time, students were allowed to talk quietly and move around if they were not disturbing others. In fact, peer discussion about their writing was encouraged. For example, students were told that they could read portions of their text to each other and ask others for help with ideas. During class discussion periods the teacher stood at the front of the room to conduct the lesson. Students sat at their seats and were usually quiet, 36 directing verbalizations to the class as a whole. Roles and behaviors were well understood and firmly established by this time of year. Further and specific elaboration on the environment as it relates to the writing processes will be discussed in Chapter IV, Findings. The elementary school curriculum was prescribed by the district and set forth guidelines in the form of objectives in the subject areas of science, social studies, reading, and math for grades one thorugh five. The writing curriculum was prescribed in the Common Writings Program. This program specified a group of writing objectives for each of the grades; two of the objectives were minimum objectives specified for mastery. The objectives for the first, second and third grade students are appended (Appendix A). These objectives were guidelines, but teachers typically went beyond them in teaching writing. What occurred was that each teacher taught writing in the way that she saw appropriate, as long as the objectives were achieved. Each student was to have two samples of his/her writing in a Common Writings Folder to be passed on as part of the student's permanent record. This was the only link with the previous year's writing instruction other than informal conversations with teachers. Writing in this third/fourth grade classroom was usually done twice a week, but varied depending on emphases on other areas of the curriculum. Sometimes it was done more frequently, sometimes less frequently. The teacher had noted in her plan book the types of writing activities that she would have students engage in. These were autobiography and biography, journal writing, creative writing--poetry and stories, report writing, and classroom newspaper publications. The teacher embedded the teaching of skills in the specific genre in which the students were writing. Typically, instruction centered around either structure, content, or purpose. For example, when concentrating on content, emphasis 37 would be on what ideas were to be expressed in the given genre and how they were related. While all three emphases were important in writing instruction, one element was usually made the object of primary focus. Students wrote drafts of writing when it was going to be published (in book form or newspaper format for others to read). When writing in journals and for other assignments (i.e., some stories or essays not to be published), they were not 9MB required to write a first and then final draft. The techer made her decision based on her perception of the benefit to the students. In fact, sometimes the student's expectation that s/he would have to write a paper over, limited the amount s/he would write and negatively affected his/her willingness to write. Oral language development was encouraged through announcements at the beginning of each morning during which students would bring to the class' attention items of interest (an incident, possession, poem). Class elections were held three times a year, and during this time campaign speeches were written and delivered by supporters of candidates and the candidates themselves. A microphone was avilable and was used during speech time. The teacher had taught for Ill years, maintaining a one-half time teaching position for the last four years. During these last four years, she has been in the unique position of being able to (a) engage in writing instruction in her classroom, (b) work on a research project focusing on language arts--particularly writing, and (c) study the literature on writing pursuant to her academic studies. The teacher brings to this study teaching that reflects the practical knowledge that comes from many years of teaching and the theoretical knowledge gained from several years of research and study. As a result, teaching decisions, strategies, and procedures in the study reflect the blending of knowledge gained in these three areas. 38 The three target students selected for primary focus exhibited a high level of competence in writing as determined by the following criteria: I. a modified, focused, holistic evaluation (Greenhalgh & Townsend, l98l) of current writing samples. Included in the evaluation criteria were attention to purpose and audience, ability to organize ideas, use of varied and rich vocabulary, and overall ideational content and development; 2. teacher evaluation of in-class performance during writing assignments. This included appropriate contributions to oral discussion and questions related to writing tasks and task completion; and 3. evaluation of samples from previous years' writing folder. The focused, holistic evaluation was chosen because it could effectively evaluate writing based on specific criteria set in a classroom and not by some general criteria established elsewhere. The teacher-investigator and research assistant (afternoon teacher) made the evaluations. Each student had a Common Writings folder, required by the school system, that followed her/him through her/his school career. Several writing samples were specified to be included in the folder. Evaluation of these samples, using the criteria of ability of organize ideas and overall ideational content, was made. This, again, was done by the teacher investigator and research assistant. After evaluation of the students' writing samples and in-class performance, three students were chosen to be the target students: Mindy, Kathy, and Anne. The fact that they were all female was based solely on the fact that these three best fit the selection criteria. It is acknowledged that male students exhibit differences in writing behavior from females (Graves, I975), and an examination of male students could have proved fruitful. However, it was decided that including male students who did not meet the criteria would compromise the purpose of the study. 39 In addition to their writing competence, each of the target students had qualities, noted below, which helped with the analysis and understanding of their behavior as it related to the task environment and cognitive processes in writing. Profiles of each target student follow. Mindy Mindy lived in the area of Riverton which had rental townhouses. She was an only child, living with her mother who was a college graduate and worked as a secretary at the nearby university. This is what Mindy's mother said about Mindy's early years: Well, when she was a little baby, about six or seven months, I gave her books, and I let her tear them up just so that she would be exposed to handling them. They were old books. I just let her play around with them and tear them up . . . . Then afterwards I started reading and since the father was not in the home, even at that young age I talked to her a lot and she was the only one with me . . . . We were on welfare at the time. (Interview with mother, 4/ 9/ 83) Mindy was bubbly, enthusiastic and eager to perform well, always oriented toward completing a task well. She was a capable student, an avid reader who scored above average on standardized tests. She was an extremely informative subject because of her ability to be in tune with her feelings and express her thoughts and intentions. Mindy was well liked by her fellow students. She was elected president during one of the three class election cycles. She was able to lead and be a leader without alienating others. Kathy Kathy lived in the iddle class neighborhood of older homes surrounding the school. The students in this area all walk to school and knew each other both in and out of school. Her father was a fiscal analyst and her mother was a college graduate not employed outside the home. Kathy's mother was active in school activities such as the PTA and school carnival. Academic achievement was important to Kathy, much of which seemed to be self-imposed. While her mother seemed to want her to do well, she seemed to prefer that she make an overall good adjustment in school. Learning come easy for Kathy, and she scored extremely well on standardized tests. Kathy had a traditional, middle class family background with a strong family orientation. Her cultural sketch stated: 40 My family celebrates all of our holidays at home, but later on in the day we visit one of our grandparents' homes. On Easter we go to my grandparents' house that we live closer to so that we can also go to our church. We go to church every Sunday, and we are Catholic. I like Christmas because we get presents to help entertain us, and also because Jesus was born on Christmas. Our family spends a lot of time together, and I am proud that our family is happy most of the time. (Writing sample, cultural sketch, 2/3/83). Personally, Kathy was friendly but somewhat reserved. She was not likely to be Spontaneous in her relationships with others, although she would speak up once she had established a level of trust. She was an honest, dependable subject-—consistent in her responses, if not prolific in words. In a group, she was a strong contributing member but seldom the leader. Aux Anne lived in a an area of middle income housing in Riverton in which students were bussed to school. Her mother was a nurse and her father a CPA. Her parents were born in China, and Anne and her sister were born in a town near Riverton. Her family appeared to have a strong bond, and she seemed especially fond of her small sister who was in kindergarten. The family made a conscious effort to maintain their Chinese-American heritage. Anne wrote: Since I am Chinese, l have to eat Chinese food and soon it gets boring. So on special days we have American food instead . . . . My mother and father just love Chinese food. They don't let us have American food very often. Anne was somewhat of a "loner" and did not make friends easily. She seemed to want to be friends with the other students, but said that they rejected her. In one of her writing samples, she closed by saying, Sometimes people put me down on purpose by calling me "Chinese eyes" or "Wang Tang eyes." (Cultural sketch, 2/ 3/83) Anne was a very bright student who scored well on standardized tests. As a subject, she was not given to detailed verbalizations and was somewhat constrained in her descriptions. She was an extremely warm and affectionate student, however, giving hugs to the teacher and stating "I love you." 4| Data Analysis Because data were collected in a way that attempted to capture the naturally occurring classroom events, the field notes and writing samples contained facts of classroom life that might not directly pertain to the major questions advanced in the study. While all the data were useful in formulating general impressions of classroom life and writing in the classroom, there also needed to be units of the data which could be closely analyzed in order to answer the questions entailed in the study. These units of data were an outgrowth of two major social participation structures3 in the classroom that related to writing. The first was the whole group social participation structure. Within this structure, certain writing-related events continuously appeared in which the teacher discussed writing with the class in general. The unit of analysis had the following features: I. a writing content focus initiated by the teacher and addressed to the entire class, 2. student-teacher verbal exchange centered around this writing content, and 3. a connected sequence of time bounded by teacher beginning statements and ending statements about writing time. These units were designated group literacy episodes. Group literacy episode is used here rather than group writing episode because both oral and written language behaviors are observed and analyzed. These units of analysis yielded information about the general task environment for writing-~the teacher‘s instruction, the purpose for writing, and the situation and context in which students wrote. There were six group literacy episodes that centered around the following topics: (a) trip narrative, (b) cultural sketches (autobiographical), (c) 3The teacher made arrangements for the presentation of content which determined how the students may acceptably interact during the lesson (Phillips, I972; Mehan, I979). 42 story writing for publication, (d) newspaper publication. Two of the episodes covered the some topics. The second type of social participation structure was characterized by the students' individually and independently engaging in writing activities (thinking, transcribingupencil to paper, conversing about ideas) that were initiated during the group literacy episodes. The teacher was not directly involved with all the students at any one time, but available to them on an individual basis as needed. What emerged from this social participation structure was a second unit of analysis having the following features: I. a period of writing time that extended over at least two separate class periods and usually more, 2. focus on the target students' engagement in writing (paper-to- pencil as well as pauses in writing), from the initiation of an idea for writing to production of a finished writing piece, and 3. social interaction from a given target student's perspective. These units collectively yielded information about the target students and their cognitive processes as well as the task environment in which they wrote. They provided information from the inception of an idea to the completion of the written product. These events were termed individual literacy episodes. Each individual target student began the writing process in the whole group literacy episode, often getting their ideas during this time. Therefore, the individual literacy episode was often a direct extension of the whole group episode. The separation of the two types of episodes was for analytic purposes only. These types of episodes are discussed in detail below. Analysis of Group Literacy Episodes Three specific categories were used in the analysis of the group literacy episodes. These categories were (0) academic tasks, (b) teacher verbal behaviors, and (c) student verbal behaviors. 43 Academic tasks were those specified by the teacher and set the direction for the lesson. In one episode, for example, the teacher's journal entry specifies the following, "I want to help the students organize their ideas into sequential units and to identify how to develop main ideas and supportive details in paragraphs. By using the trip sequence, they should be able to accomplish this" (Journal entry, I/2h/83). Here we can see that the teacher had a definite planned task for students to accomplish. Doyle (I982) supported the importance of identification of task, asserting that "students' academic work in school is defined by academic tasks that are embedded in the context that they encounter on a daily basis." Tasks regulate the selection of information and the choice of strategies for processing information. Clearly the identification of academic tasks is requisite to an understanding of how students gather and process information. Teacher behavior is the category in which an examination of verbal interaction reveals steps and strategies that teachers and students use in completion of the literacy episode. This was done through connected sequences of verbal interaction. These verbal interactions were coded in such a way that identified teacher-initiated questions and statements and the resultant student responses and comments. Figure 2 details an example of a coded episode using this category. Once the episodes were coded, certain patterns become apparent. These patterns become important in determining how the lesson was shaped, who determined changes in the direction of the lesson, and generally how learnigg occurred. From these data, a narrative description of the lesson was written. 41+ Included in the episode was the what of instruction--a second step in the analysis in this category. Each teacher statement about content was identified, read, and re-read before elements of content emerged. These categories were: I. idea production which involved simulation of background experiences and world knowledge, 2. overall text structure which involved the organization and grouping of ideas to form a meaningful and coherent text, 3. paragraph structure involving the identification and development of a single idea through use of related details, and ll. orthographics which involves concerns about spelling, punctuation, and capitalization. These writing content elements provided detailed information about the type of content emphasized as the teacher worked to meet the task objectives. Key to CodingDesiggtions T: Teacher S: Student Tex: Teacher explanation 5,: Student response TqU: Teacher question Squ: Student question Tr: Teacher response SC: Student comment TB: Teacher request for SB: Student behavior* behavior A sequence was comprised of (a) a comment initiated about a topic and (b) resultant responses related to that topic. A change of topic signalled a new sequence. Interrupted sequence occurred when unrelated or inappropriate questions occurred or disturbing behavior occurred (that was noted by teacher). shows connected sequence ----------- shows interruption of sequence * indicates inappropriate behavior [TBI Tex .TqU 45 Group Literacy Episode #I All right, you should be sitting like K, K, and A. Boys and girls-- I'm waiting for J. The spelling test will be after our writing exercise so put away your spelling lists. Yesterday, we took a trip to the MSU Museum. Who can tell me what we said about writing that was related to our trip? You need to think about the things that happened and get the main idea and details and the order that things happened. We write what happened first, second, and like that. Yes. We start with one main idea from the trip . . . the thing that happened first, then fill in the details. Who has an idea about a way to begin? (looking at the outline on the board) Do we write it like that? No. I'll explain that in a minute. (T starts calling on students as they raise their hands to give the teacher ideas for starting their stories.) We came into the room . . . One day . . . (looking at Ki) Once upon a time . . . (she giggles) (Yo is bothering Er. Teacher told her to stop.) (Writing on the board) One day our class took a field trip to the Museum. It was a cold day when we came upstairs and started a discussion about our trip. (Teacher explains how to develop this with other sentences that tell what the discussion included.) Okay, if you start your story with the bus as the main idea of your first paragraph, you fill in with details about what happened once you were on the bus. You wrote "It was cold day" (corrected the teacher). (inserts 9) Okay, are there other ways that you can think of for starting? It started out with our class or "Back in the I9th century . . . " (She laughs.) Figure 2. Example of group literacy episode. 46 The student behavior category identifies salient student verbal behaviors including student questions of the teacher, student responses to the teacher's questions, and student-initiated comments. The nature of these responses were specifically defined. From these data it was possible to determine whether students tended to ask procedural questions (concentrating on local concerns) or content-related questions (concentrating on global concerns); whether they initiated comments about content or commented primarily on ways to complete the task; whether they expressed themselves orally at length and in detail, or in short phrases and in a cursory way. Data from each literacy episode was charted using the categories specified above. A narrative description of each episode was written based on this information. Additionally, a narrative description of instruction across these episodes was made. Analysis of Individual Literacy Episodes In each individual literacy episode, the range of behaviors in which target students engaged was examined and charted on a continuum from the beginning of the written selection initiated in the group literacy episode to its completion several days later; Figure 3 is an excerpt from the charting of an episode. These behaviors were all-inclusive, from non-verbal behaviors dealing with the position of the pencil when pausing, to walking around the room to relieve tension. m behaviors documented the teacher-student interaction and student- student interaction (both social and academic-related). Also documented and analyzed were instances of vocalizations and sub-vocalization by students while writing. Additional information was gained from student and parent interviews. After extensive readings and re-readings of the episodes, salient dimensions of the students' writing behaviors and social interactions were categorized under 7 4 .Auacmuxmv mv0mwam xomcmuwF sznw>wv:m we ufi nufis vmuafimsuamlou umw paw .c3ow ua usm .%u0um :uHS cmumuumsum umw mmEHuoEom vac H mmwsw mxooaa mo> “um>umm mcwucmnu mo m—Qsmxm .m mesmwa unwwmuum a: wafiuuwm m.m:m “Eoou vcsoum :amuwmuma m.uonuamllmmcfi Ino wmmcom wcfiumu cam mumu mo xcfisu Ou Moo: mxume soap wme macu mmov lama H um Iamso mmoozo wamnlmco we xOOu Imuosv cam :ofiumau "mom "uo>uwmno a: vmocmaw Ou m>cs 0» avg .mfizu wmuumum lasso usonm mxmm whuasm wumzz mwcfiuwus wafiow E.H 30: 3o:& u.cow momma «\m cmuuHHB mamme acmom> mmsn .2 x s x x a; x x mwma comm so and Ou uxou Loss 30m wcHHHmam usonm uw>uwm Ibo cmxmm ":COfim lame: wuoz MOM >u0uw mcoamfi>mu usonm xoon :pcu xumn kvmmpfim haw mcfimaaxm um>hmmno maona ovlwu ou coxooa m.xowuumm .um “xoon mHon3 wcwoc m>ms usn mCOfiu xoon Hmcuaofi “xoonmuo: ma 3ouuoEOu lop cam nuwcwfl Imaww5m 3mm m we cm :a uo>o m3: u>mfiaan ”:ch usonm vmcumocoo 98w muumnom .mz 9.5.7.3 magi»: x x x 9 :Ofimfi> x .9 x x mufip3 ou oufiu3 on non usona Hmcomuwa nmum moscfiucoo moscfiucou wcfixHMu H hum> xoon mmum uozommu ou wafixamu uamwsum umwumu u unmvSum ou wcfixamu ucmcoum uwcuo no quBSum ou wcfixamu Hw>pmmno u 48 monoH unocsum Ou wsfimeu umsomou n soaumNfiHmnuw> uamcaum uowumu u mmmuwouo CH wcfiufip3 u vmzcwpcou .m muswfim "mcoaumcwfimmn wcfivou Ou kmM uH mo xafisu u.:mo usn .commmu m m>mzllumuam£u an on mason»: mo unwsocu Ilummh umma vofip mcoox smaoumo “cowumu lacmwuo m.3mua wowuoum mocmz mpw>wp coop mefiu can muopa Ou Bo: socx llum>uwmno mafia umEESm "cao hm3m unwfiu u.:oc "Awfiv Auo>ummno .ufi um mumw% “DOM namuumw :3ow mufipa u.cow momma mucsou Ouv womso: woow m.EoE ks n3muv Inaccfix a“ uuosm ma umwuom uLwHE 2 man G30 mzmm cu waH u.aop "z hum> u moEaumEom vfimumm maaoc mmmvfi kHOum vaaosm umsu «puma “paumuumsaafi voow m mwcfiov m.w£m 30: now Ou ucwuuoasw wcoa 30; HHmo umns wu.:o% "mvcmaow 3o: mxmm mccmfiow m.uH mamm EoE "um>uomno 9 A, a x av qaamaunl x x x x ca :30c wowwon umw u.aon .um:u aoao>mc oufips ou cam wCHuHHB mu.=om wcfinquom usoam xcfise mmum mmsafiuaoo mm m 49 the sub-processes of writing: winning, transcribing, and revision. A fourth category of social interaction was added to capture the task environmental aspects of writing. While ascribing to the literature that points out that these processes do not occur in discreetly defined stages, for purposes of analysis, such categories seemed the most effective way to provide insight. These categories are discussed in further detail below. Planning Even though the literature clearly identifies planning as a key part of the writing process (Graves, I982; Hayes & Flower, l98l), the degree to which the planning is evident in children's writing and what the process looks like is still unclear. In fact, planning and prewriting are sometimes confused. In this analysis, an adherence is to the distinction made by Emig (l97l) which states that Prewriting is that part of the composing process that extends from the time a writer begins to perceive selectively certain features of his inner and/or outer environment with a view toward writing about them . . . planning refers to any oral and written establishment of elements and parameters before or during a discursive formulation. (p. 38) For the purposes of this analysis, prewriting is considered as a part of the group lecture/discussion episode and occurs but once in a writing period. Planning, on the other hand, can occur many times during the writing episodes. In this category, behaviors relative to planning before the transcribing process such as notes and oral discussion were recorded. The relative ease or difficulty of the planning in target students is also documented. Additionally, any prohibitions to planning and the role that the teacher plays are the types of information that are included in this category. 50 Transcribing The student writes--formulates ideas and meanings, may think about spelling and punctuation--and may consider audience during the transcribing process. What is ultimately transcribed reflects the juggling of all these several concerns and decisions about what to attend to and at what point in the process to deal with them. Thus, planning and transcribing are intricately bound. In this analytic category certain behaviors tended to recur. Pauses were examined, for example, and what was written after the pause seemed to be indicative of the student's thoughts and decisions about writing at this point. What writers said as they wrote was also a way to capture their cognitive processes. While the degree to which target students verbalized as they wrote varied, each of the subjects yielded some data in this area. The ability to monitor one's writing is another behavior that is manifested during the transcribing process. Student decisions about getting started, where to go next, and keeping track of where they have been is necessary for the control over the writing process itself. While the literature shows that most young writers lack this ability, certain behaviors that seemed to reflect this ability were recorded and analyzed when they were seen. ReviewirLg/Revision Reviewing is the reading and evaluating of a text for some elements perceived to be present. It has been said that younger children generally review their text for punctuation and spelling rather than for meaning and audience awareness (Bereiter, I980). Instances in which target writers went back to read the text, upon analysis, give information about how sophisticated their review and revisions can be and the degree to which they engage in review. SI In revising, writers add or delete elements of the text--letters, punctuation, words, phrases, clauses, sentences, paragraphs--because they have examined them as faulty ggg think of a good way to change them. This is not a one time only process that occurs at the end of a written episode. Examples of changes in text as the student went along were recorded and the nature of changes made were analyzed in this category. Social Interaction This final category, more than any other, is reflective of behaviors that are occurring throughout the writing process. The social interaction that occurs is intricately a part of the task environment. What was observed was overt behavior and the nuances of behavior between students, the student and the teacher, and student and the participant observer. This category provided information about how the students' cognitive processes were affected by the social interaction behaviors. While the use of analytic categories are linear and appear to lack depth, it was felt that certain elements and moments within the writing process need particular attention and need to be isolated in order to ultimately be described most fully and clearly. The purpose here is to establish a category system against which the literacy episodes can be examined. The narratives of the collective episodes of each student, in contrast to the analytic categories, convey the richness, density, and the interconnectedness of the writing process. Other Considerations in the Data Analysis Important to this analysis was the written product. This was considered in the analysis in two ways. When the target student was the primary focus for an extended length of time, the student behaviors and actual writing of text were documented at the same time. In other words, written text and student behavior 52 were recorded simultaneously. Thus pauses, re-scanning, erasures were noted, and the thought processes contributing to these behaviors could be determined either by what the student wrote next or by questioning from the observer. In these instances, the product became a dynamic part of the task environment and was analyzed as being integrally related to the range of behavior exhibited by the students. A second consideration was the written product is its completed form. In this type of analysis, student behaviors were documented but were not linked with the specific wording of the text in progress. Thus the product was analyzed after completion by examining it for reflection of contextual constraints (purpose, audience, personna) and textual constraints (grammatical and thematic cohesion). Through this analysis, inferences about processes could be made. Summary In this chapter, the ethnographic method was purported to be particularly useful as the method of inquiry for the study of students' cognitive processes in writing. Three main reasons are that ethnography supports the notion of researcher as participant, is discovery-oriented, and is concerned with the environment and interactions of participants in the environment. Procedures for conducting the study were presented. These procedures were non-traditional in the sense that the teacher in the study was the principal investigator. Advantages of this situation were reported. Additionally, precautions taken because of the teacher's close involvement in the situation were explained. One of the primary procedural adjustments was that observations were made by a research assistant. Data were collected from several sources-~field notes (the primary source), student journals and writing samples, student and parent interviews, and the teacher's journal. 53 The setting for the study, a classroom of 24 third/fourth graders, yielded information about how three target students interacted with the teacher and peers as they engaged in writing. Two distinct units of analysis were identified and were used to analyze the data. The group literacy episode was the first unit of analysis, and the individual literacy episode was the second. These became broad organizers from which analytic categories emerged. From these data, charts and narrative accounts were developed which, when assimilated, produced answers to the major research questions entailed in the study. These findings are reported in Chapter IV. CHAPTER IV FINDINGS The major questions of this study are: I. In what ways does the task environment constrain (influence) the writing process? «How does the student's socio-cultural background affect the process of writing? «How does the teacher's instruction affect the process? --What influences do student interactions with each other have on students as they write? -What is the relationship between children's oral language and what they do during the writing process? 2. What cognitive processes do competent third grade writers use in generating written text? «Do students evidence planning or pre—writing activities? «What does the transcribirm process (the written representation of ideas) look like? --Do students evidence the revision process? What is the nature of this process? «How do these processes interact? These questions reflect the approach taken in this study, which looks at two perspectives on writing. The first perspective, addressed in question one, looks at the external contextual environment for writing (influences outside the writer's skin). The second perspective, addressed in question two, looks at the internal and cognitive processes (influences within the writer's head). As the findings are reported, these two perspectives can provide a point of reference. 54 55 The findings, 0 result of the analyses described in Chapter III, first answer the question of the task environment by (a) describing the students' socio- cultural background as it relates to writing, (b) describing and explaining the teacher's instruction and the students' verbal behavior as it related to this instruction, (c) describing student interactions and their relationship to writing, and (d) describing the target students' oral language and its relationship to the writing process. Findings which are related to the cggnitivegrocesses of the students are reported next by describing the mental activities of the target students as they wrote during the three component phases of pre- writing/planning, transcribing, and review/revision. TASK ENVIRONMENT Socio-Cultural Background The findings related to the backgrounds of each of the students identify aspects of this background that can influence students' writing. These are (0) parents' interests in writing and discussions centered around students' writing, (b) the presence of books in the home and students' interest in reading, (c) students' actual writing in the home, and (d) the presence of a variety of experiences. The findings regarding each of the students' backgrounds are presented below. Mindy's Background Mindy lived with her mother who spent a lot of time with her. Mindy talked about the influence that her mother had on her writing. She discussed her story that was to be published: "I don't know how I started this, but it took me just a half hour to think of this idea. Mom said I had author's block. I guess I did" (Fieldnotes, 3/I6/83). Then, later, as she continued to write, Mindy said, "Mom said it's important to get the ideas down--I'm afraid I might forget my ideas if I don't write them down right away" (Fieldnotes, 3/l6/83). Mindy's S6 mother took time to react to her writing and to Mindy as a writer. Her mother explained the relationship between her economic plight and the value she placed on writing. But I have always, Ms. 0., I've always encouraged her what she can do herself, like all her math papers and reading assignments from grade one I threw out. I kept what she wrote, what she has done, what she has made, because I felt that when I was on welfare if I had resources I would have lived well if I could sew, fix the heater, fix the refrigerator. I felt that our own inner resources were more important than money. So this is where I got the idea of encouraging her to do things on her own, you know? To see if she could make up a play or if she could think of things herself. (Interview, 4/l8/83) Mindy's mother valued the self-expression and creativity that writing encourages. She also felt it was important to expose Mindy to children and to all kinds of children, as she explained: . . . I always had a lot of children in our room. I always brought in a lot of kids because she was alone. So I wanted her exposed to all kinds of children. So I brought her all kinds of kids. Kids that had parents who were on drugs. All kinds . . . . (Interview, 4/I8/33) This emphasis on exposure to all kinds of children again underscored the feeling that Mindy's mother had about her child's exposure to the "real world" and her ability to cope as a result of this exposure. Finally, discussions of emotions at home seemed to be reflected in Mindy's writing at school. Her mother stated: We try to straighten out our emotional problems by discussions; and if she gets angry, I tell her don't be made at your own anger. Anger is all right if it's not destructive, and we have discussions on anger because I feel that anger is an important emotion, not something to be discarded and put away. That's too big of an emotion, and l have anger. (Interview, ll/l8/83) Mindy spent a good deal of time having her characters react with just the right emotions. this emphasis probably stems, in part, from reactions to emotions in her home. Mindy's family was not a traditional one, but a single parent family. Her mother made a great effort to give Mindy support and security and took an 57 active interest in what she was doing in school, particularly things that enabled her to call on her inner resources to produce. Thus, she had stimulated Mindy to write and to value writing. Kathy's Background Kathy lived with her mother, father, and brother, and she got help with her schoolwork and with writing from her family. Kathy's mother was not employed outside the home but was active in school activities as a volunteer. Kathy did writing at home and stated that she was writing a book. When asked what kind it was, she said, "Well, it's not the kind that we learn in school. It's like just a little book, because I'm trying to help my brother learn to read" (Interview, ll/Il/83). Kathy stated that a neighbor helped her learn to read, "During the summer before I went into kindergarten we had a neighbor who had a bunch of books and I'd go over there and he'd help me read." The fact that Kathy's neighbor and not Kathy's mother taught her to read is indicative of the fact that although Kathy was bright, her mother and father did not exert excessive pressure on her to excel academically. Conversations with her mother pointed out that she was as concerned with Kathy's social adjustment as well as her academic adjustment. In an interview Kathy was asked if her mother and father helped her with writing. She stated that they did and that they helped her if she was having trouble wording a sentence or having trouble spelling a word. Kathy's cultural sketch indicated that the family enjoyed holidays and birthdays together and that they went to church every Sunday. An example of the influence of the family Christian values came out in the statement that Kathy made as she was writing a paragraph about her favorite holiday. Kathy started writing. "My favorite holiday is . . . " Kathy: I can't write that Christmas is my favorite holiday because of the presents or else my mom will be mad. 58 Kathy continued writing ". . . because Jesus was born." (Fieldnotes, 4/2/83). Kathy's family values and background influenced her writing directly as shown in the excerpt above. They also indirectly affected her writing as she had experiences that built a knowledge source from which to draw as she wrote. Anne's Background Anne lived with her mother, father, and sister in a close-knit family situation. Her parents were interested in what occurred in school and came to conferences and other school activities. When asked if her mother and father helped her with her writing, Anne said that her mother helped because her father was so busy. She stated, "When I finish a rough draft of a letter, she will correct it and then I'll go write it over . . . " (Interview, 4/I3/83). Anne also indicated that she wrote letters to her grandfather and her babysitter who moved to California. She also said that "Usually I end up writing in Chinese rather than English. Writing in Chinese is something different. It's a character for each word so it is totally different." Anne's mother indicated that Anne has to practice her Chinese at the sitter's after school before she can watch television. (This was revealed when Anne's mother stopped by the school to leave something for Anne. The teacher had been concerned that Anne was doing her Chinese writing rather than other assigned work. Anne's mother and teacher discovered that Anne had decided the more Chinese she could get done in school, the more television she could watch at her sitter's.) Anne said that her mother also read her writing and told her that " . . . she doesn't know what that means." Then Anne did rewriting. Anne was encouraged to read. She stated that she liked Carolyn Keene books and that is her favorite author. In the interview she said, ". . . My mom S9 hates those books 'cause I never look at the books she buys. I keep on checking them out and keep reading for hours." When asked what kind of books her mother bought, she said, "She buys stuff like the Wizard of Oz and I've seen the movie . . . Pinnochio and Robin Hood, and so they are kind of boring." It is apparent that Anne's mother wanted her to engage in activities at home that would reinforce the literate environment of the school. Anne's cultural sketch indicated that the family engaged in many activities together, both cultural and recreational. Those experiences provided her with a strong background from which to draw when writing. In summary, the socio-cultural backgrounds of the students show that they were given support and encouragement for learning and for academic success. The students were from literate environments in which exposure to books, reading, and writing were the norm. Writing served a functional purpose in the homes of two of the students, Kathy and Anne. In Mindy's home, writing was also encouraged because it stimulated creativity and the development of critical thinking that could be useful in life. All three students received critical feedback from their parents on their written products. Teacher's Instruction The teacher's instruction comprised an important part of the context for writing and, therefore, could provide valuable information about the relationship of instruction to cognitive processes in writing. Three aspects of the instruction ‘ were examined and are reported here. These were (a) m was included--the tasks assigned and the content emphasized, (b) M the instruction was delivered, and (c) how the students verbal behavior related to this instruction and the cognitive operations necessary for participation in instruction. 6O What--Tasks Assigned/ Content Emphasized The tasks assigned and the content emphasized were closely related. The tasks specify in a general way the work that the student was to do. These tasks were delineated in the teacher's journal and were determined before the episode began. The content or the "what" of instruction was the specific, substantive portion of instruction that the student needed to understand and internalize in order that learning occur. This was taken from the fieldnotes and was the content that was actually taught. Figure 3 gives a synoptic view of the six literacy episodes. In looking down the first two columns for each episode (task set and content emphasized), it was clear that the content emphasis and the tasks were primarily focused on the organizational and structural components of writing. The first two episodes dealt with the organization of a paragraph, while the third, fourth, and fifth dealt with overall text organization. In the first episode, the teacher built on the common experience of a trip to the museum to show temporal organization as well as hierarchical organization. Figure 4 shows a chart used to help students understand this organization. The students were first asked to tell what the major events of the trip were. The teacher explained that these were the main idea of each paragraph. Then the details about each paragraph were elicited by the questions who? what? where? and how? (as seen in Figure 4). This type of main idea structure demonstrated to students how ideas were arranged hierarchicall . It was then pointed out that each major event was ordered according to what happened first, second, third, and fourth. Therefore, a second type of structure was developed because each event could be ordered temarally. In Episode 3 (Figure 3), the task emphasis was again on structure although a part of the task emphasizes cultural background. The students were to write autobiographical sketches using cultural information and using the concept of 6| paragraphing--the use of main ideas and detail-~which was developed in the two previous episodes. The teacher again used an organizational framework (see Figure 5), this time in the form of an outline. The structure developed was hierarchical only, since the main ideas were in categories such as family traditions, foods, music/dance, and did not need to be arranged temporally. The fieldnotes show how the content was developed. 62 Task Set by Content How Teacher Student Verbal Teacher Emphasized Taught Behaviors Episode Organization Paragraph Asks questions; Responds to l of ideas into development gives explana- teacher's ques- (story sequential units (main idea, tion; uses tions; questions about for writing; detail, idea chart (Frome); teacher about trip) identifying development; writes on procedures, main idea of ordering of board; answers length; makes paragraph-- events in questions. comments that elaborating sequence; teacher cor- on ideas and use of ditto rects; concern adding details marks (inci- about doing to logically dental teach- what teacher expand idea). ing); main wants. idea of para- graph; logical ordering of events. Episode Same as Reads example Response to 2 Episode I of student teacher ques- (story (review). paper; ques- tions. about tions about trip) main idea; explains how to achieve coherent text. Episode Student under- Cultural back- "Lecturette"; Comments about 3 standing of ground of dif- explanation of culture; re- (auto- concept of ferent groups organization sponse to ques- bio— culture and organization with chart; tions and con- graphi- identification of information response to tent (about cal of practices about back- student ques— content). sketch unique to his/ grounds; con- tions. her own cul- tent-family ture; write tradition, holi- autobiographi- days celebrated, cal sketch in- etc.; nothing cluding cul- about mechan- tural informa- ics. tion 951g cor- rect para- graphing. Episode Identification Elements of Explanation Comments re- ll of story struc- story struc- with trans- sponse to (prepara- ture that is ture; what's parency; ques- teacher ques- 63 Task Set by Content How Teacher Student Verbal Teacher Emphasized Taught Behaviors tion for embedded in included in tions; reads tions; student story text to aid in each element student ex- reads. writing) reading com- in the setting; ample; re- prehension initiating views. and story event. writing. Episode Same as epi- Same as epi- Questions; Responds, com- S sode story sode 4 records stu- ments (ini- (story structure to (review). dents' re- tiates); ques- to be be used in sponse; tions proce- published writing teacher re- dures; clari- in book (review). views; ex- fies content. form) plains by example. Episode Identification Headline, by- Explanation Respond; read 6 of how news- line, lead with article volunteer (article paper article paragraph of as example; information. for class is written; news article; questions; news- write news- how these calls on stu- paper) paper article function; dent to read for class what's in- responds to newspaper. cluded; nouns, student ques- verbs as used in headlines; definitions of words. tions. Figure 3. Synoptic chart of group literacy episodes 64 TEMPORAL ORGANIZATION HIERARCHICAL ORGANIZATION Questions Main Idea Details What happened We came in and talked Who? Ms. D., Ms. M., class first? about the trip. What? talked about behavior Where? in the room How? whole class discussion What happened We got on the bus. Who? our class and Ms. K's next? How? noisy/quiet What? did you talk? about what? who did you sit by? What happened We arrived at the Who? tour guides met us next? museum. Where? entrance to museum What? took off coats, went to lecture room, passed around rocks and tools What happened We took a tour of the Who? two classes and tour next? museum. guides What? saw things from pioneer life; saw dif- ferent kinds of dinosaur bones. Figure 4. Chart showing hierarchical and temporal organization. 65 Introduction name American heritage or culture Family traditions holidays special celebrations things we do as a group Foods examples: Greeks--baklava Church what is it like? Music and dance People like me that I am proud of Figure 5. Organization for autobiographical sketch. The teacher uses the chart on the board. T: This is the introduction. This will be your first paragraph. I want you to do it in sentences, expressing the main idea first. Then add details. Your next paragraph will be about family traditions with a sentence as the main idea, then details about these traditions. As the teacher passes out paper for writing after the discussion, she again stresses the organizational structure. T: I want you to think of the main idea and some examples. Use specific details (Fieldnotes, 2/3/83). In Episode 4 (Figure 3), the emphasis was on the underlying story structure that was embedded in text. The teacher presents this structure on the board: Setting Initiating Event Reaction Action Conclusion (Fieldnotes, 2/24/83). She explained what each part meant, and then used a short story on a transparency, asking the class to select the parts of the story that correspond to the story structure elements. 66 It can be seen from the episodes described above that the organizational and structural elements of writing were a major content emphasis of the teacher's writing instruction. A second emphasis of instruction was idea production. The fieldnotes in several episodes showed the teacher eliciting ideas of what to write about from the students. In Episode I, for example, the fieldnotes show the following: T: . . . who has an idea about starting? The students started to give the teacher ideas for starting their stories: Mindy: We came into the room . . . Alice: One day . . . Shelly: Once upon a time . . . The teacher wrote these on the board. Later in the episode, the teacher asked for ideas for topics to write about. Students generated many topics which the teacher wrote on the board. As students began to write independently, the fieldnotes showed: The teacher is asking Alice questions to stimulate ideas for her story (Fieldnotes, l/27). In Episode 3, a discussion about culture was used to stimulate students to think about their own backgrounds. The teacher asked the students, "How do you celebrate? What are your family traditions?" They shared their experiences, drawing on their personal backgrounds to respond. While these are examples of initial generation of ideas, they do not show the teacher explaining ways that these ideas can be developed. The teacher encouraged students to write details about the idem, but there was no documentation that they were shown how to do this. In the discussion of story structure, however (Episode 5), there was an approximation to this idea development. The teacher and students together generated ideas that could be 67 used in given parts of the story structure. The teacher began by asking why it was important to learn about story structure and then used the chart on the board that outlined story structure to have students generate types of information that could be included in each part of the structure. In this episode, the students were lead in thinking about ways of developing a setting by including certain information. During the discussion, some students were also stimulated to talk about books they had read in which the author either enhanced the story by adding certain ideas or made the story less interesting for the reader by including seemingly unnecessary information. It has been pointed out thus far that emphasis on structure and idea generation formed the primary focus of the teacher's instructional content in writing. In further analysis of the data, it became apparent that the teacher de- emphasized orthographics (spelling, sentence structure, and grammar) during writing. Several statements were made during the instruction which showed this. T: Boys and girls, if you don't know how to spell a word, leave a space for it and come back to it later. And later: T: Don't be overly concerned about handwriting and spelling. In another episode, story structure and development were emphasized and spelling was de-emphasized. T: If you have a burning idea, then let me know. Include characters, setting, plot, what will be happening, and a good conclusion. Remember the film that we saw about story endings. I'd like a well—developed story. First copy; don't worry about 5 llin . You will do this after on your final copy (Fieldnotes, 15(3793') It must be noted here that spelling was taught as a separate subject in this classroom, however. The students were divided into three spelling ability groups, and there was a weekly list and a daily assignment for each group. The words were taken from a commercial spelling series, and the lessons were developed 68 from the suggestions in the series and the teacher's judgment about what was important for the students to know. One spelling lesson was developed from the information below that was on the chalkboard. On Board: 9:30: noun: the name of a person, place, or thing toes potatoes/s calves calf: change the f to v and add gs fairy: change the y toiand add g singular plural child children foot feet boys keys toys The teacher is discussing plural and singular endings to nouns in preparation for spelling assignment to be done. This is what I want you to do now. (I) finish yesterday's spelling assignment (2) do today's assignment (3) work on reports on black Americans I will check them in the back of the room (F ieldnotes, 2/23/83). This excerpt documents the type of isolated spelling lesson that is a part of the instruction in the classroom. The teacher explained the lesson which dealt with adding g or as to nouns and had written the words on the board as she explained. During the lesson, accuracy was stressed. However, when students wrote during writing time, they were told not to concern themselves with correct spelling, at least for the first draft. There was a change in behavior expected on the part of the students during spelling time and during writing time. 69 Grammar was taught during one episode in which the use of nouns and verbs was explained in the context of writing newspaper articles. Here the teacher discusses the succinct way that headlines are written. T: All right, the headline consists of just two words. Think of the parts of speech they are. What is hijacker? Tammy: Verb? T: No. (She calls on Sandy.) Sandy: A noun. T: Yes, hijacker is a noun. 21319 is a verb, Tammy, an action word. The important thing is that a noun and a verb only are used here. The articles, or small words like g and the; are not important here. The headline is made up of action words; they should catch attention (Fieldnotes, 4/27/83). While grammar was taught in this example, it was in the context of a specific use in writing. In summary, the content emphasis from the analysis of the episodes and other data was strongly geared toward the way students organize their writing at the paragraph and whole text levels. Secondly, an emphasis was put on the generation of different ideas and, to a very limited degree, development of these ideas within a logical structure. Orthographics were de-emphasized during the writing instruction, but spelling was emphasized and taught as a subject separate from writing instruction. How the Episodes Developed As data from each of the individual episodes were analyzed, patterns of behavior for teacher and student became apparent. The verbal interaction coding in every episode was marked by teacher questions/student responses. .1312 teacher controlled the direction of the episodes through use of strategically placed questions. The following example shows a student asking a question about 70 procedure during a time in the lesson when the focus was on the content of idea generation. The teacher answered the student's question, then got back to content by asking a question herself. Anne: Can we write more than four paragraphs? T: Yes. Don't be overly concerned about handwriting. Who has other ideas for writing? (Fieldnotes, l/27/83) In another exchange the teacher had been eliciting responses from the students about elements of story structure. They discussed the setting and what was included, and the teacher asked them what was included in the initiating event. Several students responded, then one went back to a comment about the setting which moved the direction backwards rather than forward. The teacher used a directive to get back to the immediate focus as the excerpt shows. Mindy: I read some authors do put more in the setting. If Nancy Drew talks to dad, it tells what she said. Sometimes authors will say what her face looked like and lots of details. T: Right. Mitchener's Hawaii had lots of details in the setting. He went on and on for many pages describing the land and how it looked and how people came to live there. Alice: In a book I read, it was boring for a while because it took so long to get to the interesting part. T: Yes, sometimes there are a lot of details. Back to the initiating event. As your story develops, many times there is more than one initiating event (Fieldnotes, 3/3 /83). Immediately after this exchange, the student who changed the direction initially asked another question which was off the focus. Mindy: We're going to get an author, aren't we? T: Yes. Next Wednesday. Sheila Roberts. OK, let's see what's next (teacher is referring to next element in the lesson). The teacher asked a question to get students focused again. What seemed to have occurred in all of the episodes was the allowance of a temporary and short 7I deviation from the content focus, then the teacher drew students back in the direction she wanted them to go. In addition to the questioning pattern and its use, findings from the data show that guestions served important functions for the teacher in guiding student thinking. These functions were: I. to have students focus at the beginning of a lesson, 2. to have students retrieve information from memory and apply it to text, 3. to have students spontaneously generate ideas, and 4. to elicit from students their knowledge and understanding of word meanings used in context. The first function was used at the outset of the episode to have students focus on what the lesson was to be about. In Episode I, for example, after the teacher stated that the day before the class took a fieldtrip to a museum, she asked, "Who can tell me what we said about writing that was related to the trip?" In another episode, the teacher said, "Before you start your books, let's review what we said about story structure. Who remembers why we use a structure?" In instances such as these, questions focus and alert students to the emphasis of the lesson. Another function of the questions was to have students retrieve and apply knowledge. As the teacher read a story that a student had written, she asked students to identify the main idea. Here the students must (a) retrieve from memory the definition of a main idea and (b) select from the information in the paragraph that which corresponds to the definition of a main idea. This type of processing was repeated several times in the data. In another episode, for example, students were asked to identify story elements from a story that was projected on a transparency and match it with the corresponding element in the story structure (Episode 4). 72 Another function of questions was shown by the teacher when she used them to elicit students' ideas and had them brainstorm topics for writing. These ideas were listed and form a reference for all students to use. In these cases the teacher took all the ideas that students had without condition or judgment. Finally, students were questioned to see if they had specific word knowledge for understanding of the teacher's talk. The teacher might use a word in an explanation and then question to see if students knew what the word meant. It almost seemed as if it were a deliberate attempt to "stretch" students' vocabularies while in the process of teaching about something else or giving an explanation. An example follows: Mary had told the class about finding several books about George Washington Carver in the library and using these to do her report. T: (enthusiastically and smiling) You'll have information overload. Sometimes there are discrepancies among books in the information given. Does anyone know what discreEncy means? Several students responded with the wrong answer before teacher explained. Teacher put example of a discrepancy on board . . . George Washington Carver resource books give two dates for his birth . . . She pointed out the fact that there was a discrepancy because he could only have one birth date. Pointed out the fact that they didn't keep accurate records then (Fieldnotes, 2/l/83). The teacher might not expect the students to know the answer, but she encouraged their use of context to come up with an educated guess. This use of questions was seen several times in the data. Explanations of concgpts were usually given after a questioning/response sequence. In Episode 2, for example, the students were asked to identify the main idea of each of the paragraphs in Mary's writing. At the end of the reading and the identification of the paragraphs, the teacher said, "You see how Mary took the idea of a trip to Ann Arbor and told about several things? She then developed each one and arranged them in the order that they occurred" 73 (Fieldnotes, 2/I/83). The explanations were usually brief and to the point as is the one above. This was the teacher's way of bringing closure to the content focus and of preparing students to begin to write. In only one episode did the teacher lecture to the students. This was a short Iecture—-"lecturette"--in 'which the teacher talked about different ethnic traditions in an attempt to give students an understanding of things that make up a person's cultural background. In this case she spoke for a somewhat prolonged period of time without eliciting questions or having students initiate questions (Fieldnotes, 2/3/83). How Students' Verbal Behaviors Related to Instruction There were three major categories of students' verbal behavior that provided data about instruction. The three major categories were (a) student responses to teacher questions, (b) questions asked by students, and (c) comments initiated by students. As is shown in Figure 7, student responses to teacher guestions made up the largest category of student verbal behavior, as might be expected. The recall of specific information was observed most frequently as students responded to teacher questions. Students answered in this category by selecting from their knowledge in a specific area. In the following example the student was asked about the parts of speech. T: Think of the parts of speech they are ("Hijacker Seized"). What's hijacker? Student: A noun? This response involved recall of specific information. Another example of a response in this category: 74 Student Responses to Teacher Questions recall of information free production of ideas (brainstorming) generation of experiences generation and application of knowledge sew Questions Asked by Students l. procedural 2. writing content/subject matter related 3. general informational Comments Initiated by Students l. extension/elaboration of concept 2. concerns about task completion 3. application of previous reading experiences Figure 7. Functional typology of student verbal behavior during group literacy episodes. T: OK, what's an accomplice? Lolita: A companion? Teacher explained that it is a little more than a companion. Here the student drew on knowledge of word meaning. A second response type was "freely produced (brainstormed) ideas in response to the teacher's questions." In this category, students' ideas could flow in an uninhibited manner. This was in contrast to the specific selection of knowledge needed to generate answers in the previous category. Students' responses in giving ideas of ways to start a story showed the nature of these responses: 75 Mary: We came into the room . . . Alice: Once upon a time . . . Shelly: One day . . . Mindy: It started out with our class . . . Lolita: Twas the date of January 20 . . . Curtis: It was a cold day . . . Another response similar to brainstorming was the "generation of experiences that related to a specific topic." In response to teacher questions about trips to write about, students responded by generating several. In a sense students were engaging in brainstorming, but there was a greater condition placed on the ideas that they came up with because students had experienced the trip. The fourth response, and the least frequently seen, was the "generation of specific knowledge and its application into a content-specific text situation." In these responses, student not only generated knowledge, but were called upon to apply this knowledge. These response types made greater demand on the students' cognitive abilities. Students also asked questions of the teacher, although this was seen for less frequently than the "response to teacher questions" category. When asking questions, students generally asked "procedural questions," wanting to know how to do a writing assignment that the teacher had made. Specifically, questions were about such things as how long their writing had to be and if they had to write the headings. Students sometimes wanted information of a more general nature, asking questions such as "We're going to get an author, aren't we?" or "Will we get to write adventure stories?" These were "general informational questions" and were asked considerably less frequently than procedural questions. A third type of question was "related to the writing content/subject matter focus" of the episode. For example, when discussing story structure, one student was confused about the story elements of action and reaction and said, "I got a 76 question with action and reaction. It seems you're skipping something between action and conclusion" (Fieldnotes, 3/3/83). Another question was about the focus on racial and ethnic differences. One student asked, "Is it true that black people couldn't sit where they wanted to at the movies?" Only a small number of verbal behaviors were questions initiated by students, and only a small percentage of those were related to content. Students sometimes initiated comments or shared their ideas that were related to a given topic being discussed. At times this would take the form of extensior or elaboration of a concept--as when a student told about a book she had read that had a long setting that was boring. Another student told about things she had learned by viewing 8221s on television and how she learned things about her cultural background. This showed a greater level of involvement of the student and longer duration of speaking. Summarj of Teacher's Instruction In summary, the teacher orchestrated the episodes by determining the topic to be discussed and by employing questions in an effort to achieve the purposes for the lesson and for the students. Students responded by giving the teacher the types of responses she desired. These appeared to be short and to the point, primarily responding with the naming of specific information or concepts. There was only limited discourse from the students as they did not volunteer information or elaborate on ideas to any extent. Rather, while allowing students to initiate comments, the teacher did not encourage them, as she would soon get back to the topic and the direction she had in mind for the lesson. One notable exception to this pattern was the episode on culture. In this episode, although the teacher led the discussion, students volunteered 77 information and initiated comments and opinions. The teacher not only allowed this to develop during the episode, but seemed to encourage it. Because of the nature of the interactions in the group literacy episodes, certain cognitive situations were most relevant. Students were primarily retrieving information from long term memory and sorting this information in order to select a specific information requested by the teacher. They were not involved in making planning decisions directed at goal setting and purpose setting because this was done by the teacher in the episodes. Providing the organizational frames, in particular, precluded students' need to make planning decisions to set purpose. Student Interactions Students interacted with each other primarily during the individual literacy episodes, inasmuch as the teacher controlled the interactions during the group literacy episodes. The nature of these interactions and how they affected the writing process (either helping students write or hindering the writing process) are reported here. These interactions were analyzed from the perspective of the individual target students; therefore, findings will be reported from the perspective of each of the three students. Mindy's Interactions Mindy showed several types of interactions with other students. Several instances were seen where students approached Mindy for help and she assisted them. Sometimes these were short exchanges in which she helped another student spell a word. Other instances were for longer periods as when she volunteered to have a "conference" to help another student as described below. 78 Mindy: (reading Debra's story, playing with hair braid): OK, let's see; well, maybe like Garfield, I don't know. Garfield could be planning something on his birthday that Jon didn't know about (Mindy is helping Debra with ideas). (to Debra) Did Garfield open all his presents? Debra: Only the teddy bear. Mindy: (giving suggestions to Debra for what Garfield could do, questioning her about comic strip) What is your Garfield? Different? shy? nice? Debra: Funny. Mindy: Funny Garfield goes with a funny teddy bear. What do you think? (moving her arms as she talks to Debra; many hand gestures) Debra: (moves away, seemingly satisfied with the help she received from Mindy). (Fieldnotes, 3/8/83) Another kind of interaction was noted when Mindy left her writing completely and walked around. This occurred after she had been working for a sustained period of time. She had been writing a report on Ethel Waters in the classroom; she got up and took a walk to the small room. As she approached Kathy and Mary, she said, "Teacher said I could walk around since I've been working so hard" (Fieldnotes, 2/l7/83). She then went back to her seat and continued reading and talking notes for her report. In another instance she said she needed to "walk off tension." A third type of interaction with Mindy and other students occurred when students come over just to talk--usually the conversation centered on writing, however. Two examples follow. Jendy came over to Mindy who was working on the cover of her report. Mindy asked Jendy what she did her report on. Jendy: O. J. Simpson, but I'm not through yet. (Fieldnotes, 3/l/83). In another episode: 79 Yvette began conversing with Mindy. Mindy: Yvette: Mindy: Sometimes chapters end up being very short. Once in kindergarten, I wrote a story about a princess who wanted a toy. It was very long, and I guess kind of boring. When I was four years old, I wrote "The Mischievous Mouse." I couldn't pronounce it, though. My printing was big and lopsided. In summertime, I wrote "Bedtime Stories" which is a collection of my short stores. You're a good illustrator. I don't like to draw. My mom is good at it. (Fieldnotes, 3/I6/83). Mindy was able to interact with other students, and in some cases it seemed to move her writing forward as many of the interactions were centered around writing. Mindy would sometimes test her characters' reactions by asking her friends how they would react to an incident. Mindy's response in an interview pointed out her she questioned her friends. Often in parts there'd be a part and I wouldn't know how to explain it . . . I had to ask my friends about their emotions or the reaction to something because if I had her just smile when her grandmother was kidnapped, you know, so I asked, "What would you do?" you know, and then took those ideas and put them down. (Interview, 4/2l/83) Mindy talked further about the importance of having a "kid" read her story. . . . I know you read it and I took some of your suggestions, but a kid, you know, because it's really based on a kid's story, and I wanted to know what a kid would think about it. So I tried reading it to Jackie and she's six years old and it's kind of hard when you're reading in cursivenyou get impatient; so I'm asking people if, do you want to read my story? But it's awfully long to read. (Interview, 4/2l/83) Finally, an exchange between Mindy and Kathy seemed to be counter to the characteristic behaviors that Mindy exhibited in other interactions. In the following exchange, Mindy and Kathy were discussing how they would start their 80 writing. Kathy asked Mindy to tell her what she was writing about. The following conversation ensued. Mindy: OK, but not all (reluctant to show Kathy). Kathy: You can tell me the initiating event. On her paper, in I984 . . . . Mindy: (shows Kathy what she's written) What are you writing about? Kathy: I don't know. Mindy: (asks observer how hot it is in China) Kathy: (writes) In I984 there was a new school being . . . (she covers up her page as Mindy tries to look) Mindy: I told you now, so tell me. Kathy: I'll tell you at recess. Observer note: social problems between the two girls. After recess the class continued writing for about l5 minutes. The fieldnotes note that Kathy and Mindy wrote notes back and forth-~something about being sorry and playing together. In this exchange there seemed to be competitiveness between the two girls. However, in the interview, Mindy explained her reluctance to share her story with Kathy in this way, "I wasn't feeling good about Kathy reading my story, not so much that I didn't want her to because I was afraid she was going to copy, but I wanted it to be a surprise . . . " (Interview, 4/2l/83). There is definite tension between the two girls which may be one of the unintended outcomes of school writing. It does not seem to be constructive in helping Mindy's writing. Kathy's Interactions In reading the interactions that Kathy engaged in during the individual literacy episodes, certain patterns emerged. Unlike her behavior in the whole group episodes, Kathy initiated interactions frequently, was assertive and 8l sometimes competitive as a writer. The two major behaviors seen were interactions that showed her monitoring the work of others and a general awareness and curiosity about what was occurring in the room. These behaviors were interspersed throughout the writing process. This inclination to monitor the work of others was noted several times in the fieldnotes. In one episode in which she was working on her book, the f ieldnotes read: ”:03 Page 9 in her writing . . . "During the night Jean . . . " pencil point on paper . . . pauses for two seconds to see if Mindy's done with her story yet . . . asks her. Mindy: no. Mindy and Mary are adjacent to Kathy. Mindy is finishing her story while Mary is copying it over for her. Mindy is questioning Mary as to how she'd feel (see page 40 of draft) in a situation. “:07: Kathy playing with her pencil. Continues to write, interrupted easily. Glances over to Mary and Mindy to help out . . . . (Fieldnotes, 4/2l/83) Several instances were noted when Kathy asked other students if they were finished already. Another episode showed Kathy again looking at Mindy's paper. She said, "Mindy, you have to do those" (pointing to questions on the front page of a spelling assignment) (Fieldnotes, 4/28/83). In another instance, she asked Mary if she wrote on the back. These and other exchanges pointed out that Kathy was constantly aware of what Mindy, in particular, was doing. She appeared to feel that it was important to know where she was (whether finished or not) and how she was going about completing the task. It is difficult to tell whether this was a concern for Mindy's doing the work correctly or whether she wanted to make sure that others didn't complete the work before she did. In the interaction described earlier between Mindy and Kathy as they discussed how they would begin their writing and their reluctance to share, 82 Kathy was eager to find out what Mindy had written but reluctant to tell what she herself had written. While her behavior could be indicative of insecurity about the value of what she had written, it could also indicate a desire to have a story that was better. More important than the motivation of the behavior, however, was the result. Her preoccupation with what Mindy was doing was not productive in helping Kathy write. Kathy was also aware of what was occurring in the classroom. In the middle of her writing, she would stop as this notation suggests: Kathy: Mary, I want my story back. (puts in desk; writes name, date, language arts) Kathy looks at paper; writes "one day during school . . ." Kathy turns around to look at the class (five seconds), whispers to Mindy that Anne's seat has been moved. (Fieldnotes, 4/l7/83) In another instance while she was writing, she stopped and commented: Kathy: (to Ray, laughing) You're putting all the states in the world . . . (Ray was at the map in the front of the room) Ray: No, I'm just trying to locate the states I need for my story. Kathy started talking about math homework to Ray. (Fieldnotes, 4/I7/83) The observer commented in the fieldnotes that Kathy was easily distracted. She always returned to her writing, however, and she was able to continue and to complete it. In an interview Kathy stated that she preferred to "write alone" because she could get more work done. It was apparent that most of her interactions with other students were not centered around the content of what she was writing. Anne's Interactions Anne had few interactions with other students while writing. She tended to write alone and stated in an interview, when asked if she liked to write with 83 friends or alone in a quiet place, that she liked a quiet place because she could "concentrate and get better ideas." Although she interacted very little with the other students, she was aware of what was occurring in the room, particularly what the other students were doing. In an interview, for example, she knew the types of stories her classmates had written and some of the details. Instances when she did interact with other students were only tangentially related to writing. She was asked by other students to illustrate their books, report covers, and other work. In these instances Anne would write her own story gpg illustrate for someone else simultaneously. An example illustrates how she worked. 9:53 Continues writing, looking intently at paper. 9:55 Sits back in chair, stretches, sighs. Anne asked the observer if she should start a new paragraph of a quotation "Mother knows . . . " Continued to write. 9:57 Got up to get another sheet of paper. 9:59 Started talking about how she liked her new pants. IO:OO Left seat to talk to Shelly and Yvette about their stories (which she is illustrating). l0:02 Rearranges her pages in order. l0:03 Yvette came over to read Anne a poem ("The Circus") and watched her illustrate it by drawing a horse. Note that in this sequence Anne was involved in three different activities-- writing her story, illustrating her poem, and planning illustrations for her classmates. Another example is further illustrative of Anne's interactions with other students. The students were writing cinquains; Anne had written a poem that was not a cinquain, and she was decorating the background. 84 II:I4 Anne finished with decoration. Pencil in hand, she looked up at the blackboard (one second), write "Mother," said to herself "adjective-—let's see . . . " (four second pause). Asked "how do you spell lovable? Let's see . . . " Scratched head, looked up at Blackboard, right board. "I want one with two syllables . . . " (paused l6 seconds). Jumped up, "Yeechl" Went to wash glue off of hands. II:I7 Sitting back in chair, leg bent, elbow resting on knee. Then wrote, "sweet, kind." II:I9 Julie asked Anne if she could draw a butterfly. Anne: I don't know; you mean like this? (She drew.) Julie: "Yes. Thanks, Anne." (Fieldnotes, 4/5/83) Julie went back to her seat, and Anne went back to writing her poem. Then another student came over and interrupted Anne, asking her to draw a butterfly. Anne again complied, then went back to her writing. While preferring to work alone, Anne seemed able to interact with others, to do several things simultaneously and still get back to her writing. It was through her artistic ability that she seemed to be able to interact with the other students. Her writing did not appear to be negatively affected by these interactions. In summary, on examination of the interactions of the three target students indicated that they responded and initiated contacts with other students in a variety of ways and for a variety of purposes. What seems to be common to all the students, however, is the fact that these interactions were a distraction, but they were able to handle the distractions in their own ways and successfully get back to the writing at hand. In Mindy's case, she was able to use the interactions to help her with decisions about her writing. Kathy's interactions showed her concern for completing the task at hand and how other students sometimes distracted her. Anne was able to concentrate on her writing but her 85 interactions point to her love for art, its importance for her, and how she "made room for it" in her writing. Oral Language and Its Relationship to Writing There were three ways that oral language and writing were examined in this study. These were in looking at (0) students' verbal behavior during group literacy episodes; (b) target students' verbalizations with other students, teachers, and observers during individual literacy episodes; and (c) target students' sub-vocalizations while writing. Students' Verbal Behaviors During Group Literacy Episodes The oral language behaviors of students in the classroom were reported in some detail under "Teacher's Instruction" and "Students' Verbal Behaviors." The primary oral behaviors seen, as reported, were responses to teacher's questions and these were of short duration. Students did not engage in extensive discourse or in exchanges of ideas to any great extent. Specifically, during group literacy episodes, the amount and type of oral language behavior varied among the tamet students. Mindy was the ;most verbally involved in the group episodes. Of the IO types of verbal behaviors identified (Figure 7), she exhibited seven of these behaviors. The major categories of verbal behavior for Mindy were questions asked of the teacher and initiation of comments. In asking procedural questions, Mindy primarily showed concern that the task be done correctly and that the expectations of the task be understood. A typical example occurred after the teacher and students had discussed story structure and had been told that they would be writing their own stories. Mindy asked, "How long are you going to let us work on these things?" (Fieldnotes, 3/3/83) and "Does it have to be one of those topics?" In adding to 86 and initiating comments about concepts being discussed, Mindy demonstrated involvement in the episode beyond a simple recall of information and showed a degree of interest in and knowledge about what was being discussed. At one point in the group literacy episode, the teacher was discussing different cultures. Mindy offered, "Here are some Indian legends," and she opened her desk to show the book. Another time she stated, "I read some authors do put more in the setting. If Nancy Drew talks to Dad, it tells what she said; sometimes authors will say what her face looked like and lots of details" (Fieldnotes, 3/3/83). Kathy's verbalizations during the group literacy episodes were few. She responded to questions by recalling specific information. In fact this was her primary way of responding. The teacher asked, for example, "What is the next element of story structure after the setting?" Kathy responded, "The initiating event" (Fieldnotes, 3/3/83). When she answered questions, most were answered with short phrases. Kathy had only one verbalization in which she initiated a comment. At that time, she elaborated on a point made by the teacher. She volunteered the following, "Sometimes you put weather in the setting" (Fieldnotes, 3/3/83. Again, the comment was short and direct. Anne showed two types of behavior. She asked procedural questions and responded to the teacher's questions by recalling specific information. A typical procedural question was asked by Anne during the group literacy episode when they were discussing the trip essays when she asked, "Can we write more than four paragraphs?" (Fieldnotes, l/27/83). Her primary concern expressed by her verbal behavior seemed to be in making the teacher's expectations of the task clear. A second type of verbal behavior was responses to questions asked, which required recall of information. Anne responded correctly and her answers were 87 short and to the point. She responded only twice in this category. In looking at these responses, it is clear that there was a difference in the number and type of behavior displayed by the verbal target students. Mindy and Anne used language to gain information about how to proceed with the writing ask. Mindy, however, engaged more in extensive talk about writing concepts than did the other two students. Kathy and Anne's verbalizations were primarily responses rather than generations of ideas or talk about concepts. Students' Oral Language During Interactions in Individual Literacy Episodes The students' oral language during individual literacy episodes, as they interacted with the teacher, observer, and peers, was shown to be related to writing in three major ways. Students used oral language to clarifj meanings and stimulate ideas. they asked their peers and others about whether a portion of text made sense or whether a character's reaction was the way they would react if they were that character. Other examples show students using language to question other students in an effort to stimulate their thinking, to help them generate ideas. In writing an article about a carnival, for example, Kathy stated, "I was thinking about how I should put this; I want to tell about the people who make the carnival a success" (F ieldnotes, 4/28/83). Oral language was used to share writing experiences, to verbalize how their writing was progressing, and what they were writing about. These sharings were outlets for both positive and negative feelings about their writing. Anne, in writing her autobiographical sketch, stated that it was easy to write about her culture and showed a great deal of enthusiasm (Fieldnotes, 2/3/83). On the other hand, when writing a report on Martin Luther King, she stated, "I wish we didn't have to do this--this is so stupid." 88 Students occasionally went to one another's desks and asked what they were writing about and shared with each other more specific information about their writing. Jendy came over to Mindy's desk, and Mindy asked what her report was one. Jendy replied, "O. J. Simpson." Mindy went on to talk about her report on Ethel Waters, that "Ethel started out by singing songs that she knew. I think that's really good she went so far" (Fieldnotes, 3/l/83). Students used oral language as a means of seeking approval for their writing. They asked the teacher, observer, and other students to react to what they had written or wanted to know if their work were neat. This approval seeking occurred while the writing was in progress and seemed to encourage students to continue with more confidence. Finally, oral language was used in a way that showed no relationship to writing. Students engaged in general conversation about school, play, home, reading or spelling, or relationships with each other. While these conversations did not seem to directly aid writing, they may have helped indirectly in that they provided a "break" from writing. In Mindy's case, this oral change of pace served to relieve tension. In other cases it appeared to be a distraction for students. Oral language then appears to be an integral part of writing as students used it as a way to test their understanding and perceptions of the environment. Language appeared to be a bridge between meanings and experiences in the environment and students' ability to internally represent meaning as they wrote. Sub-vocalization During Individual Literacy Episodes Careful examination of the individual literacy episodes for the three target students showed surprisingly few instances of sub-vocalization of students while writing. In fact there were only three instances noted in the data: two of Mindy and one of Kathy. The literature suggested that sub-vocalization among young 89 children (six year olds) was quite prevalent and that talk provided both meaning and, for some children, a systematic means for getting that meaning on paper (Dyson, l98l; Dyson & Genishi, I982). The target students in this study showed similar behaviors to those of the reflective writers described by Graves (I975). These reflective writers were more similar to competent adult writers-«exhibiting little overt language to accompany writing, a growing sense of audience, and periodic re=readings of their text. The absence of sub-vocalization suggested that the target students had completed the transition from the contextually oriented oral tradition of early home environment to the de-contextualized literate tradition of the school. Summary of Task Environment The findings which describe the task environment and its influence on the writing process were reported from four perspectives: (a) the socio-cultural background of the students, (b) the teacher's instruction and students' verbal behavior, (c) the social interaction of the students with each other, and (d) the oral language and its relationship to writing. Figure 8 summarizes these findings. In examining the aspects of task environment related to the socio-cultural backgrounds, Figure 8 shows that the target students had somewhat similar home environments which supported the goals of the teacher for written literacy. Both reading and writing were done in the home of each student, and parents offered support for student achievement and writing. 90 UZHBHmS mom BZMZZOmH>2m xm49 .0 mmDUHh Deana»: nan Ha>oummw mcfixaom uo mamas no can: amosmcmH Hanoi anomdmm usona mcfiufiuz and: >azu umc3 can .vq>wvcH mewmmmumoum mm: mewufiu3 30: unwamnum> ou .maucawuwmxu medumuz ouanm 0» com: amosmcaa Hake: avmswcaq mama“ auaHSEwum can mmcflcmws aufiuaaa on cam: awasmcma Hanoi Hugo unawumazv >uoum an» no vaguuum usona muaaucou usono abomwmm anaconda ou paccommmul ucaEEoo a naumfiuwcfil mucaeeou amunduM:al mzouo panama» unawumusv nanummu amasmcaq uo mcoflumadv pmxman panama» ou omocommaul uo mcofiumusv omxmdl mono eoou azu cw cadmcau ucfiuusuuo mm: umnz Eoou u>ufiauu ou Ocsoua oaxaaau puuouficoe xaucmawml an» :« mcfiuuauuo ma: van: mcofiuummu m.umuuouacu umau many venous usonm qumDOAMsu can uo ou mumnuo ou vaxamul panaucau mc0wuaauaucfi mmocmuaza ucaumcoal xamu 0» menu mucmnsum mucapsum now mua>oa mucapzum uunuo uo xuo: mam: wow mCOMuumumuCH .mxoon poumuumsHHHI on» pauouficosl canoaoumma mucwnsuml acupsum .maoomflma naaaouucou panacea .mmusoumwc ucwnsum omuwsfln .mmmcommau ucmpsum uuocm .mpazm:a\mcoMumaso auscmaoum moacmzume co mMmMcQEMIwa mmapfi uo sawuaumcao Ho>aa uxwu sawuusuumcH m.ua£ama9 waons can nmmumaumm um uxau uo wusuusuum can mama“ can mucu>u no coHuMNMCMUuo ucaucou mean no auou3l nacuamou mmwua>wuua Hausuaso uwzuamou oasaa> ma) unguauzl new Hawuom c“ Ummmmca >stmmu mafiufi>wuua acma ado cm>qv ma: mauuw>wuom mowmmwfiu unason so: can Immwu xaasau mcouuml Hoonum new uuommsml can commusoucu mm: mewpmmmn mcfiufiuz um: pummusouca :mfiamcm ma Lufl3 um: panda: mucwuaml mcofiuoeu uo cowmmaumxal Ham: mm amacfizu ea auouzl ous~a> can mean cozaa> xuw>wummuo omQHa: uuzuos .meo: um muons Ca ucawuum mum: mxoom can :ofimmuumxa afloat muuaspmum ammHHoa mauaaaaum avaaaoo ucauma «Hmcfim nanumu can umcuozl umnumu can uazuozl uuascaum amuuaou panuozl mzz< Vikki >O-HZ accoumxaam Hausuasu Iofioom 9I In the teacher's instruction, the content for writing stressed organization and structure at the paragraph and whole text level. Idea development was also a part of the writing content, whereas a de-emphasis was placed on orthography. The teacher orchestrated the whole group episodes by determining the topic to be discussed and by asking questions in an effort to achieve the purposes for the lesson and for the students. The findings showed that there was only limited discourse from the students as they did not volunteer information or elaborate on their ideas to any great extent. The interactions of the students indicated that some were helpful in moving the writing forward, and other interactions hindered the process. For all three students, there were instances in which the interactions were a distraction. The target students in the group episodes varied somewhat in their verbal behavior, with Mindy being more assertive and asking questions of the teacher and initiating comments. Anne and Kathy did more responding to the teacher's questions. Finally, the oral language of the students was an important part of writing as it was used to clarify meanings and stimulate ideas. It was also used to share writing experiences and to assess how writing was progressing. The findings thus far have addressed the first perspective on writing with which the study deals, that of the contextual environment for writing. The second perspective on writing, that of the cognitive processes of competent writers, is reported below. In this section, we look at the three target students and their writing processes. COGNITIVE PROCESSES OF THE TARGET STUDENTS As the students wrote, they each exhibited ways that they employed 92 mental activities to bring forth different kinds of knowledge that they possessed and how they used this knowledge to express themselves in writing. Because data were collected from writers in action, the dynamics of composing were reported. There were no neat packages of behavior that were necessarily common to all three students, as they experienced writing as individuals. The behaviors of the students were reported under the three major processes of writing, those of planning, transcribing, and reviewing/revising. The planning process consists of the generation of ideas, organizing of these ideas, and setting of goals in order to establish a writing plan to guide the production of text to meet these goals. In the target students, behaviors which were indicators of these subprocesses of planning were observed to varying degrees. These behaviors indicated (a) ways that the students come up with ideas; (b) the kind of specific framework or schema, if any, that they had in place to guide organization; and (c) whether there were an internal criterion for judging the text. The transcribing process acts under the guidance of the writing plan to produce language that corresponds to the information and ideas that the writer attempts to convey. What has been done is to directly observe competent writers at work. As they wrote, they made stops or "surfacings" which allowed a view of problem solving attempts. These surfacings or problem solving stops are the glimpses of the underlying mental processes of the writer. These problem solving stops revealed the writer's attention to constraints that are imposed on the writing process. These constraints reflect global plans (meaning audience, persona) and local plans (orthography, grammar, syntax) and reveal pg! the writer gets to her goal. The constraints that the writers attend to during these problem solving stops are reported in the transcribing section. 93 The reviewing/revising process consists of reading and editing of what was transcribed to detect and correct perceived inaccuracies in expression of meaning and/or linguistic and orthographic conventions. The findings about the rewriting/revising report the target students' incidences of and purposes for reading what they have written and the types of changes (revisions) made as a result of this reading. The planning, transcribing, and reviewing processes of individual students are reported in narrative form for each student with emphasis on the writing strategies and problem solving stops that they exhibited while writing. A figure at the end of the narrative further elucidates these processes in graphic form. Planning It's like a square frame; it starts here and I've got my idea all planned out and the details go around it; and if I want to change a place, I've got to work those details in, but I've still got to have that basic idea in there somewhere. (Interview, 4/2l/83) Mindy's statement about planning was supported in the observational data. She seemed to have a definite idea of what she wanted to write about and wanted to adhere closely to this plan. In fact, in writing a story to be published in book form, she resisted suggestions to make some changes in her text by both the teacher and an author who visited the classroom. On one occasion, the teacher suggested that she might want to leave out a portion of text (note teacher's comments encircled page 20 of her draft, Figure 9). Mindy stated, however, that her characters' conversation and involvement with her teaching (Mrs. Longing) was important to the plot. The author who visited the classroom took Mindy's story, along with others, to read outside of class and to critique. Mindy stated, "Ms. Roberts told me a few suggestions; but to fol low those, I'd 94 have to change my whole book around, and I don't have a whole year to do that!" (smiled). Here, again, she resisted deviating from her plan, partly because of the time involved, but she also seemed committed to her original plan. When having to make an initial decision about what to write, Mindy had difficulty. In the writing of their stories, Mindy and Kathy sat together talking for a while. Mindy was heard stating, "I don't know how to start." She began to write about seven minutes later. She and Kathy were involved in a controversy about sharing what they had written which added little to Mindy's productivity in getting off to a good start. She continued writing after recess, however, but was not "overly enthusiastic" as the observer noted. Mindy discussed her difficulty with the teacher the next day as the journal excerpt notes, " . . . Mindy wanted to talk to me in private and waited until the others . . . " In this conference, she was very concerned that her story be "right" for her and that it make sense. Here she was concerned in her planning with audience. In the interview, too, she also expressed concern for sufficient time to plan. I feel that when you write, like I should be given some time because when I write and just all of a sudden write, and you've got, let's say--okay, Mindy, you've got a week to do it, like it's rushing me and I can't put down all the ideas like I want them, so I think I like it better when I've got the idea and have time to think about it and time to plan my story because I plan my story in my head, you know; because I've got my story and then I add details and what I want as I go along. (Interview, 4/2I/83) Three points about Mindy's planning were brought out in the excerpts. First, it was clear that she herself had to feel that what she had come up with was "right." She seemed to have criteria for what was an acceptable piece of writing and that influenced her planning. The second thing that came out was that Mindy did not want to be restricted by time in making decisions about her writing, as was usually the case in school. She talked about its taking her a week 95 or two to think about her ideas. Finally, she was concerned that the text make sense, that it seem "right" to the reader who would see it. Here she considered the audience in her planning. 96 Figure 9. Excerpt of Mindy's story with teacher's comments. 97 Planning was not a one-time process of Mindy; rather, it occurred over and over as her writing progressed. It had to occur each time a decision was made. This type of planning will be considered in the discussion of the transcribing process. Transcribing While Mindy actually engaged in writing as a paper/pencil process, it was possible to observe the several cognitive processes that were used as she constructed meaning. One way to observe these processes was to examine her behavior when she paused during writing. As these pauses were examined closely, it became clear that most of them were centered around global concerns (expression of meaning, audience response). For example, she focused on meaning when she stopped frequently to plan the actions of her characters. Typical of this was the instance in which she decided on the course of the "breakfast conversation" between two of the main characters in her story. She gave considerable thought to who would initiate the conversation and the course that the conversation would take. During another pause she deliberated on a character reaction. She's writing (pauses one second). Looked up, continued writing . . . (paused 30 seconds), put pencil down. Huddles; seemed to sense something was wrong. Then she said to the observer, "Do you think a cat could know that?" (Fieldnotes, 3/22/83) Here as Mindy wrote, her attention was focused on the authenticity of meaning that is conveyed to her audience. Again, Mindy stopped to consider the action of her main character, Tassie, discussing how she would react to the "tragedy" of her grandmother's kidnapping: "Well, when something tragic happens, people usually hug their pets so that could be Tassie's reaction-~right?" (Fieldnotes, 3/ 22/ 83) 98 These examples underscore Mindy's concern for meaning and audience. she drew information from her memory and selected that which was correct and appropriate to the concept she was trying to express to her audience. During transcribing, other pauses were illustrative of Mindy's attempts to use language in a way that appropriately expressed the meaning that she was working to express. In writing her text, Mindy had to draw on her knowledge of language forms and how to use these forms correctly in written language. In the example below, while writing her story, she stopped to think about how to rephrase a sentence that she had just written, explaining that Tassie had no relatives. "Mindy paused. 'Tassie didn't have many uncles and aunts, let alone (really bothered her).' Mindy said this didn't sound right. Changed to " . . . aunts that lived nearby'" (Fieldnotes, 3/I6/83). She had probably used the expression let alone in oral language, but seemed to draw on her knowledge of written language usage to realize that this was not appropriate. Mindy gave attention to choice of words as she wrote. She tried to use words that showed excitement or created interest. This seemed to reflect a definite influence of instruction. In one example she explained to the observer that "Mrs. D. said the words should be more interesting, exciting." Mindy then went on to show her an example in which she used the words, "' . . . a hidden message,‘ gasped Tassie" instead of the less interesting version "' . . . so that's the note' said Tassie." She also used "Tassie responded wearily" instead of "Tassie said." When transcribing, Mindy become concerned about the physical layout of her story. This occurred after the teacher was overheard discussing chapter organization with other students. Mindy said aloud to the observer, "I'm going to have to choose chapters and paragraphs!" Up to this time, Mindy had not been concerned with how the overall text would be structured. She was organizing 99 and expressing thoughts into cohesive units, but physically there was no indication of where one paragraph ended and another began. In other writing that was not as long (report, trip essay, newspaper essay), Mindy was able to structure her paragraphs, although much of this was done by the teacher when she provided the organizational framework during the whole group episodes. In an interview, when asked about whether she was aware of thinking of such things as paragraphing and punctuation, Mindy said, "I put them aside . . . I know it's awfully hard to read, but I think I'd rather write than worry about the paragraphs. As for Kathy, she writes and paragraphs and automatically it comes down; she can't help it." In terms of spelling and punctuation, Mindy was able to put these on hold. She felt it was more important to concentrate on "getting her ideas down." Indeed in examining the draft of her story, it's clear that she had not given attention to spelling. It is noteworthy, too, that of the questions and comments to the observer and teacher, not one of them involved concern about how to spell a word. Review/Revisions Mindy did much of her revision as she went along. She would stop to evaluate what she had written, then she would make a change. Sometimes she would ask the observer or teacher how her writing was going. Other times she would ask a classmate when writing her story. The fieldnotes give this account of Mindy's changing a portion of text: "Paused, pencil eraser to mouth, 'When Tas got downstairs she found breakfast ready . . . ' Paused to change sentence structure; wanted two ideas instead of just one" (Fieldnotes, 3/l7/83). Much of Mindy's writing was characterized by these types of changes as the writing progressed. In a sample page of her story (Appendix E), she inserted a part at IOO the asterisk to keep the story line in tact. She realized that she left out a part of the story that was important to the credibility of the plot. She reviewed her text often to see where she left off and to check to see if something made sense. When she hadn't written for a while, she had to review to "reacquaint herself with the story." When she reviewed her text the final time after it was completed, she tried to check for spelling. The draft shows how she wrote over words to correct spelling errors. This was how she addressed spelling concerns and grammatical concerns. She inserted words that were missing and added correct punctuation and capitalization (see Appendix E). Some revision was not observed by the observer. In the interview, however, the question was asked: Q: So, do you go back a lot while you're writing and read something over and then see if it really makes sense? M: Well, if it combines, like if I read a part of it, and I stopped, and I skipped some, and then I read what I had written, and I want to see if those two have some sense of connection; because if they don't, then; I've got to change something. (Interview, 4/2I/83) The observation data supported this statement and showed that Mindy attempted to make changes which would keep the meaning in tact. Summary Mindy's writing seemed to be guided by global concerns, and that was where the primary cognitive processes seemed to focus. The planning process was marked by Mindy, beginning with a basic idea and then adding to it as she continued to write. Planning was adversely affected by imposition of time restrictions. When Mindy was engaged in the transcribing process, global plans guided cognitive activities. She searched for meaning and authenticity in character reactions and story line. She also expended effort in searching for language that expressed the desired meaning and specific words that not only IOI were functional but aesthetically pleasing in that they needed to be interesting and exciting. The local concerns (physical layout, orthography) were put aside during transcribing. For example, words were frequently misspelled and handwriting was often messy. Revisions were made in progress (as the writing occurred), particularly those having to do with meaning. Other revisions related to spelling, paragraphing, punctuation, and chapter organization were done after the text was completed. The final draft of the story to be published was typed, based on the revisions of the first draft. Kathy Planning The first thing I do is think about if you have a choice, what will be easier to write about, and I get my characters. Usually, I think about what they will be doing . . . . Usually, I get to my plot and then that's when I start thinking about how my story is going to end . . . . It is hard to have a good ending to a story you have if it is really complicated or it is really a big scene or something so it's kind of hard to have a good ending. . . . The story could build up to that. Sometimes I do know what my ending will be, and I just build my story up to that. (Interview, 4/l/83) Initial planning began for Kathy during the whole group episodes so that when she got her paper, she did, indeed, have an idea and a plan of sorts to write about. She stated in the interview that she started thinking while the teacher was talking and explaining the writing assignment. However, when writing her story initially, Kathy seemed to have difficulty getting started. She stated that while usually she could start right away, sometimes she waited because she had to think awhile. There was evidence in the observation data that she and Mindy did sit and discuss their stories and hesitantly share the beginnings as they slowly developed. At the second writing session, Kathy asked the teacher for help with her story, stating that she was having trouble getting from the setting to the initiating event. She also stated that she wasn't satisfied with her setting. I02 It is significant here that in her planning, Kathy used the elements of story structure given by the teacher in whole group episode #3. she seemed to use these elements as an organizational guide for her writing. This type of organization framework seemed to help Kathy in all her writing and eliminate her need to do her own planning related to organizing. In examining Kathy's story (see Appendix E), it was clear that she began her planning with her characters. The characters were a projection of her friends, and she used some of their personality traits in her story. She drew on her knowledge of very familiar behavior patterns and gave them to her characters. Her emphasis was primarily on character development and less on the development of her plot. This explained part of her difficulty in getting from the setting to the initiating event. In her planning, Kathy did not seem very restricted by time for planning, and she stated that she started getting her ideas as the teacher was talking. " sometimes, I'll wait, though (until the teacher finishes talking), 'cuz, like when we were writing our books, I waited, then I started. I had to think awhile. Usually, I think while you're talking" (Interview, 4/l/83). Here it seemed that Kathy could do her initial planning rather quickly. Again, this underscores her planning for characters and working her way to the plot as she clarified in her mind what her characters would be "doing." Transcribing As Kathy wrote, like Mindy, it was possible to observe the type of mental activities in which she engaged. In closely examining her behaviors, it appeared that most of her activities were centered around local concerns (physical layout, orthographics, and word choice). She seemed particularly concerned with physical aspects of paragraphing. In one instance, Kathy was waiting for the l03 teacher to read her paper and began looking it over, realizing she forgot to indent. She said to Mindy, "'Oops, I forgot to indent.‘ Quickly corrects this" (Fieldnotes, 2/3/83). In another example, as Kathy was writing, the following observation appeared in the fieldnotes: "Wrote one line, looked over again to Mindy and Mary (two seconds); continued writing, turned back to page 9 to see how long her paragraph was. She told me then she'd know when to start a new one" (F ieldnotes, 4/2l/83). Her statement to the observer indicated again that there was a formula for Kathy which told her how long her paragraphs should be, based not on the cohesiveness of ideational content, but on physical length. As she wrote, Kathy took note of her choice of words several times. In some cases it seemed to be motivated by a sense that a given word would simply be good to use. In the draft of her story, for example, Kathy said, "I'm going to use the word passageuthat's one of our spelling words" (F ieldnotes, 4/2l/83). On other occasions, she was concerned about whether the word was aesthetically appropriate. In a portion of her writing, she was concerned about the repeated use of the pronoun M. "It's funny; I have eight characters, so I always use the word Iggy. It's kind of boring. I have to use the girls or something" (Fieldnotes, 4/2l/83). This also showed a sense of awareness of audience and what was pleasing to them. Spelling concerns were very evident during the time when Kathy was transcribing. In fact, most of Kathy's interactions with the observer were centered around the spelling of words. Among those that the observer noted were the following: l0:2l Out at r ; Kathy pausing (six seconds), looked around room. She asked me if "recess" is correct spelling . . . I0:27 Kathy erased. "I was spelling from. I always put an g instead of an m. Kathy asked me how to spell amazement. I04 ”:05 Kathy paused for five seconds and yawned, looked at fingernails and pencil in hand; started to write. She asked me how to spell introduce. (F ieldnotes, 4/l7/83) Several other examples show the emphasis that Kathy placed on the spelling of words correctly. This emphasis on correctness along with her concern for the physical layout of her writing underscored Kathy's orientation toward local rather than global concerns. Only one occasion was noted in which Kathy paused to concentrate on the meaning conveyed. When Kathy was writing an article for the class newspaper, the fieldnotes report, "Kathy is working on carnival article . . . pause (three seconds); new paragraph. She says, 'I was thinking about how I should put this. I want to tell about the people who make the carnival a success'" (F ieldnotes, 4/2l/83). Her concern for meaning here may have been due to a personal interest in the carnival and who helped because both of her parents were very much involved. A statement Kathy made as she was writing seemed to express her feeling about writing and the lack of time she devoted to attending to meaning. As she wrote, the fieldnotes state, " . . . Kathy went back up to the word haunted on page 9 and made it neater. She turned the page over. 'Finally, I'm in the lens!"' (Fieldnotes, 4/2l/83). In response to an interview question, when asked about punctuation, paragraphing, spelling, Kathy stated, "Sometimes like if I want to get done quickly, I then . . . I'll put the punctuation in later; but most of the time, I'll put them in as I go. Then I don't have to do it later." For Kathy, the punctuation, paragraphing, and even spelling did not appear difficult. Rather, it seemed that she was doing much of this automatically. She stated this, "Yup, all of it is automatic . . . " (Interview, 4/2I/83). IOS Review/Revision The reviewing of the text that Kathy did was done intermittently throughout the time she was writing. The primary reason for reviewing the text was to see where she left off. Other reasons were nearly always to indent for a new paragraph which she forgot to do several times and to make her writing neater. Because of the care she took with spelling and neatness, in her mind there was very little need to revise her text after she completed it. The first two pages from the first draft (Figure 9) of her story graphically demonstrate why Kathy felt little need to revise. The final draft, that was typed, bore close resemblance to her initial writing because of the very few revisions made. In view of the fact that she seemed to be content with the initial meaning and plot development of the story as it evolved and her original copy was accurate and neat, she revised very little after her writing was completed. In an attempt to get Kathy to think more about global issues in her writing, the teacher suggested that Kathy add more elaboration in one of her stories. She resisted, as the following excerpt shows. Kathy: Mrs. D., first of all, the way my story is, I don't want to do that. Teacher: Why not? Kathy: Just because I don't want that in there. Teacher: Well, by writing "the week was over," you really left the reader hanging. Maybe if you added what went on during the week, it would make it more interesting. Kathy: Smiles and agrees. 9:55 Kathy starts writing about the week. She giggles at first as she writes. How'd you think of ideas to start writing so soon? Kathy: I started thinking when Mrs. D. was talking . . . I told her during my interview that I start to think while she's talking (smiles). l06 l -' l . I ‘7‘“ Q G ‘ F ‘5 6 lg 6H €56 \l'\,€l (f {'1‘6-6/57 ‘ 6) C672 6 C66 ‘6 / 6 M1 Acme/s a, flaw“ “la/.6006 ,660/L //U__ 6/ 6 ,M’J/ /6§uf / 6 6 we», 6:; 6/1 76240“ 2‘; dcfl/ ya 62 fl/éa’ 6/66 fl/W ‘ 100166 , 66/ ///76 06/66 .663 6M 6646 w ,cxxv/ / 666‘ 1766‘ f.» t6/62 11.611671 /6L2. 44/ Laval}. ( 9146/6 ,z.o'n..L, 6647616444. Lmbgcl, lit/#6 6f 6 6 1L 2,606: .667 /.‘-/ L6 01/16} 077 a, 10-096 6/ m 6.6 7‘ 643:7 7661/ 0" 6.4»; 04/1036. :1” 6 A4- "‘ 0764. 7666/25 £66414 am 6637—966 6‘ ”6165/ ,m wag; 662,6 6: w max. 07:. n. / 19%... 0/6614 /(¢/6c(lé .466 V/.-/ 6721. ’6/6 ”£43656? A (6/60 0 6 défllflfl" (@414 f {/66/ ‘//x)., .W a A/JLZ //7(16d6/;€/. ,8 (/:f".4_l/I/ I {/x’g. '/- 0/ Abl‘wlrb Z6011 /C<.»(6Lz_ Z2060“. 6? 61/6” / I 66 (m 66 0mm! 261.0164 .. 2956/; 25456», Luau. flew/xi ,60‘ a. GIG/gm. .166le 06 cgwl. Max/1.. MM: ‘ 6 M t 74/ , Aim/.46 6 zzaw ”66%. mm;- ‘./J/‘/(mv(." x604 .6 K66“- {ygafig}; 10116 Figure l0. Excerpts from the final draft of Kathy's story. 107 [Wm/MA (j 6 / fl 6 6/)72/6 N0.) WM 6. 764" «'4‘ 60.310 ,/«68//‘6 6666/ Wé /(’f’ - _ #662: -,.)/,'/W J . 360,. .666.” Ml. 66(64/ '6 fuzz/47’ fly a" a W x166 «0736 0.404)». 7(2440/ W ’3: 06/644, % 0 M45644 ,6" /{/0& 4x.- 42 '6 Mac/L/ ’ZI’J/fl, 4%de ,60/1/4. 0748. 6‘44: £6546”- M26 61/66 .Zz'xM M91466 £66647. a» ad 162/?) 442644 Awe M44 ,4. a. .22: /60‘66/6«6/wé‘: <91 Q6666. W606 aaé/U 6%: .6424fi 766/ .6 44¢ (.42- 0/257» i I /6/’.Z \f-fl/LC'OKI 766v " \ [62% I ./I.’-‘, i 0446f“ ,. .ar 6W[// .6 (£66 426246744 52/2146 66/1/64 (KI/6:6 [/2634 1/. if /6 0/4 74:7, 604on a! . ’ .IéI/g, ' ,6}, 0,0,6 1/ 2.1.. .662. °J6MAIW #6666 M L" 4?“ 4446 a. 4L JI I‘£06 ,0// 6641 .W 06.666 ,P/M’fll. a ./6/, 66/ W6 .6. . 66% 70-6 "(m 0.06% W 6/2/14 claw/i @464 40/! a. 66622 l08 9:57 Kathy is writing diligently . . . "thought she would make it and she did/I" Kathy: Started to talk about Mary's birthday party and roller skating. IO:00 Kathy continues with her story. LEft hand holding paper down. She erased. I asked if she spelled something wrong. "Whenever I write a line thinking ahead, so I wrote the next word--ahead." Kathy asked me how to spell committee. Paumree seconds); turned to see what Was were doing. Kathy with a smile, "I hope she's (Mrs. D) satisfied!" |O:Oll Kathy got up to show Mrs. D. Here the teacher's attempt to have Kathy look more closely at the meaning expressed in her writing had little positive affect in terms of changing her attitude about the need for more concern in this area. In response to a question about revision, Kathy said, "Sometimes I'll look back to see if it makes sense or should I put something else in later in the story. Like I was reading a book and they didn't even look back or anything and they put . . . there was . . ." She gave an example of a book she read that didn't make sense because of conflicting information given by the author. There was inconsistency in the observation data and the interview data on the revision process for Kathy. She showed very little evidence of making substantive changes, although she articulated that she did in the interview data. In the book written for publication, she showed no evidence of revisions in the longhand version other than spelling and punctuation changes. Summary Kathy was a confident writer who was guided by local concerns and plans for completing the writing tasks. The planning process was relatively easy for her when the teacher provided an organizational framework. There were times when she wrote about a trip, on autobiographical sketch, a newspaper article, |09 and a story to be published that Kathy responded attentively and welcomed the "schema" for writing provided by the teacher. Transcribing for Kathy was marked by cognitive processes dealing with orthographic concerns and about the physical layout of the text. She was able to use the language in clearly expressing meaning. She did not concentrate on this aspect of writing to refine her meaning, but, rather, it seemed less important than completing the task. Kathy made only minor revisions because of the careful attention she gave to spelling, neatness, and paragraphing as she transcribed. She showed a satisfaction for the meaning conveyed the first time and saw no need to change if. Anne Planning The first thing I do is think of an idea--something to write about . . . then I think how I'm going to put it into words. I have all my ideas, then go along, figure out that doesn't make sense, so change it . . . . I usually know how it's going to end. (Interview, 4/l3/83) At first glance, Anne's first two sentences seemed to be an obvious assertion. However, the statement showed a great deal of insight. She presented the fact that not only did she need to come up with an idea, but putting this idea into words required thought. She realized that writing required a specific way of connecting words and sentences and that she had to deal with that. Anne stated that she thought about how she was going to put her ideas into words at the beginning. It is reasonable to assume that she thought about this as she got into her text as well. In fact, she stated that she went along and made determinations that something didn't make sense, so she changed it. Based on the interview statement then, it can be said that Anne planned for producing a llO paper by generating specific ideas and by planning how to translate those ideas into language. In planning for writing, Anne also showed that she was aware of the need for procedural plans. In some cases these plans were specified by the teacher. Anne was usually quick to ask questions about ho_w_ the writing should be done. For example, the teacher had an outline on the board and was discussing the content of the paper when, referring to the outline, Anne asked, "Do we do it like that?" (Fieldnotes, l/27/83). At this point, she was thinking ahead, planning while the teacher was explaining what to include in the paper. She realized that she needed to plan her format in terms of sequencing and clumping together of ideas. Later, she also asked about length, asking if she could write more than four paragraphs. When the plan (topic and format) was not provided by the teacher, Anne wrestled with coming up with an idea. In writing her story to be published in a book, she stated that she didn't know what to write about. She eventually came up with the idea to write a story entitled "The Jungle." When asked how she came up with that idea, she stated, "That's all I can think about now" (F ieldnotes, 3/3/83). Anne seemed limited by time as she said 92’1- The implication was that if she had longer to think, she might possibly come up with something different. Beyond the initial planning, Anne seemed to sense the need to have what she was writing make sense to the reader. She stated this in an interview. Other planning during writing was focused on physical features of the text. Anne was quite preoccupied with this; in fact, this concern is graphically illustrated in the findings reported during the transcribing process which follows. Transcribing Anne's transcribing behavior was analyzed through observational data, through analysis of her written products, and through interview data. Her behavior, more than that of either of the other two students, seemed to be most affected by the particular genre in which she was writing. When writing the autobiographical sketch, she was very enthusiastic and wrote extensively without many pauses. The fieldnotes state, "Anne has written one side of a page, is diligently working on the back sheet." Later in the episode, the following note is made, "Anne still working neatly. I asked her if it's easy to write about her culture. 'Yes.' She was very enthused." The text of that paper (see Appendix E) demonstrated that Anne, on her first draft, was able to attend to the physical and organizational aspects of paragraphing as she indented appropriately for the most part and had appropriate information included within each paragraph. She considered and used punctuation as she transcribed, using not only periods but colons as well. While the colons were not always used properly, in most instances they were. Anne also used commas when separating things in a series which is a fairly advanced skill. Additionally, Anne capitalized holidays appropriately and only failed to capitalize Chinese and American. What is evident here is that Anne was able to employ cognitive processes necessary to attend to orthographic concerns while conveying the meaning that was necessary for the reader to have. This was probably possible because of the freedom Anne had from planning and organizing of informational content. The outline that the teacher provided gave Anne an external framework to use in organizing and selecting information about which to write. This point was clearly made when Anne's writing behavior when writing her story is contrasted with the writing above. In the writing of a story, Anne had to choose her own topic, plan for the organization of the content, and make l|2 decisions about what to include. In other words, she had to engage in more complex cognitive processes. While the physical layout and orthography were correct, the meaning and logical were somewhat inconsistent (see Appendix E). To examine this more closely, the most consistently observed type of cognitive activity was related to physical layout and orthographic concerns. She was especially concerned about handwriting as the following excerpt shows, "Anne is being very careful about her handwriting. She erases almost every six or seven words because she doesn't think they're neat enough" (Fieldnotes, 11/20/83). And, if Anne stopped writing after two minutes, the fieldnotes pointed out her attending to paragraphing: "Anne sat back in chair, stretched and sighed. Anne asked me if she could start a new paragraph for a quotation 'Mother knows . . . ' (p. 6 of copy)" (F ieldnotes, 4/20/83). As Anne attended to the physical layout of her text, she was at the same time constructing meaning for the reader. At only one time was it noted that Anne interrupted her writing to consider global concerns (meaning, audience, personna). This was written in the fieldnotes in the following manner. "Anne said, 'Gosh, I was thinking about the part of the story that doesn't make sense."' The observer responded, "Why not?" Anne said, "I wrote a part that mother told Joy 0 story and wanted her to pass it down. It doesn't make sense to me." The observer asked Anne, "Why leave it in if it doesn't make sense?" Anne told the observer that she might take it out. Anne never revised or deleted that part of the story, however. It appears then that Anne's cognitive processes were involved most in local concerns and that attention to global details was secondary. It also appears that when having to decide on topics and organization as well as content of her writing, she faced the problem of excessive constraints on her as a writer. She appeared to have set priorities and "satisficed" on meanings and concentrate on |l3 organization and orthographics. She made a decision about where she would concentrate her efforts. Review/Revision Anne's review of her writing involved looking at her text to see if it were neatly done, as in the case when she erased an e to "make it fatter" (Fieldnotes, 4/20/83). She also went back to read where she left off so that she would know where to begin again. The fieldnotes document no revisions such as the one above in which she was attending to letter formation and neatness. But examination of the draft of Anne's story showed places where Anne went back and made changes in the text in preparation for having the story typed. These changes give information about the nature of the revisions. Several revisions were designed to give added information to the reader. Note in Figure ll, portions of the text from pages I and 2 showed insertions that Anne made. On page one she wrote, "She had no brothers and sisters (but she had a cat)." It is not clear what the importance of this revision is to the story since the significance of the cat was never mentioned again. It seems as if Anne wanted to give more detail to the reader about her characters and place. Similarly, she added the name of the hotel where her characters slept when she inserted "called the Holly Hotel." This name was not mentioned again in the story. Anne made other revisions that demonstrated her awareness that she needed a referent that the audience could identify without ambiguity. In writing about a conversation between two female characters, Anne had initially used she referring to both. After re-reading, she realized that she had to identify each by Ilh s 20%;". ; win {i/W/ Figure ll: Excerpts from pages I and 2 of Anne's text, showing insertions. ”5 name so she could be understood. This revision helped to clarify meaning for the reader. These reflect the only two types of revision that were related to meaning concerns. Anne also read through the story and added phrases that had been left out but needed to be added to clarify the meaning of the sentence. For example, she had written, "The time probably wouldn't be till morning and came to her mother's room." She inserted "at morning she" after 933 which gave the sentence meaning. Although changes were made, there were no major revisions in content of the story as far as plot and characterization were concerned. Her one reference to concern about the story's making sense was never addressed as she decided to leave it as it was. Summary Anne's writing was guided by a determination to efficiently complete the task at hand. This was reflected in her planning, transcribing, and revision. The planning process saw Anne's major cognitive activity directed toward deciding on an idea and addressing the linguistic concerns necessary to put these ideas in writing. She also planned procedurally how she was going to complete a task (i.e., use teacher's outline, write several paragraphs). Transcribing varied depending on the genre in which Anne was writing. Her primary cognitive processes were directed toward fine motor concerns of handwriting and orthographic concerns of spelling and punctuation. Attention to global concerns were minimal as Anne seemed content to go with her first expression of meaning. Revisions made by Anne were primarily concerned with handwriting and neatness. She made some changes in wording to decrease ambiguity. ||6 Summary of Findings Related to Specific Cognitive Processes of the Target Students The findings are summarized in Figure l2 and show strategies and problem solving stops observed in the three students. During planning, Mindy seemed to have the most developed schema of the three students. She would fill in and modify as she wrote. Kathy's writing was less dependent on a general plan than on character development and actions. She would also use elements of story structure in guiding her planning, switching to this when she got lost. Anne started with general ideas and added to them and modified as she went along. She was also guided by procedural concerns such as length and format. Both Mindy and Anne were aware of audience and sense making. The transcribing process (Figure l2) shows that Mindy was most concerned with global plans (meaning and audience) and less concerned with local concerns such as physical layout. On the other hand, Kathy and Anne were making "stops" that placed attention on local concerns such as physical layout and spelling with few stops that focused on global concerns such as meaning and audience. The review and revising followed along the same lines as the transcribing. Mindy reviewed her text frequently as she wrote to find her place and to check to see if what she had written made sense. She revised as she went along, primarily for meaning. Kathy reviewed her text frequently, checking on paragraphing and neatness of her writing. She made revisions as she was writing. These were primarily punctuation revisions. Anne reviewed frequently for neatness and to find her place. She revised in progress for word meaning on only two occasions. pd“ ll7 mazmnahm Bm0m<fi ho mmmmmuomm qumH>mm\UzH3mH>mm .UszHmUm24mB .UZHZZdAm .NH NKDUHh mownmmumonuuoi mowumfinsou umumm pomw>mm mewcmwei :Owunauucsmi mcficmmfii wwwumoum Cw cwwwwwm mmwuuoum Cw nwmw>mm wwmuooum Cw comfi>mm qumH>Nm woman pawn: mmmcummCI mewcmwa uOu oz< mmwcumwci onwnmmummummu xumnu ou munam new“: oszmH>um Naucmzumuu ooaom>mm MausoSUoum cm3om>mm Nflwcwaowuu om3ww>mm cumucoo H~Ew£afii Amflmmnmam HnEwcfiavi mmmnxmmldmwwmfimm no mucmuuomfiw may mcflcmmz uo mmmcmum3m Umuoumi mcflmmmaa aafimUwumzummmi ummumucw\ucmewuwuxmi x umflmmwm .xuoz mcfiHHmmmi muwoco buoz mmflmmwlmmmm Ammmcummc. mewufiuzocmnu c300 mmocw :ofiumsuucsai mcumucou mafiaammmi newuumm Haw mumfiumoquMi Nmmmmmmmwmm Mmmmmmmmwmm wmmmmqmm. oszHmomzeme amcofiummnfl Houzuuauumi mcuoucou Hmuzuushumi mucmfinsui unwnmmummumai mcflcmmumnummi mewcmofi uo >u«0mucm£uDMI u=o>mq Hmufim>nm uzo>mq HwUMm>cm mdwmmwfl unwxmzimmcwm oucmwnsw ou mcwxMZimmcmm unEuOu\:umcwHi mcflccmHm nmowsm musu mmfidmmdm nusuuw >uoum uo mucwawdm mewccnam umnwsm mcumucou Hmuadwmmmm van mcfifluso m.um£ummh Umcflzm chancmum accumucH mmmuoum mcfiuac ou wuououi quzzaqm ommsmcnfl o» 09 mumflmcouu op 30:: Immmcm Heuwcmu mzz¢ mafia cmnw uwcumu mumuuaumcu ocsoum pawsm moon a=B/ (V\ '. .. O \. £2737“ '1 . 3 :- N\ ‘95 “{\ \M‘W A C\\7/ X r. \A N‘ CA, VEGA } M‘Mxx/ 53x7; :—-: Wmd / /\:‘ 3! ) WW"";W'V~1V'~VUUV- - .. - - .8 I . 0 U I -. .0 I .0 r 216 /, /L:€.‘Z/‘/ f/ “L. 1 ) Q/LW {:17 «LawL/LQ -LC‘Y/(Ld'f 711%; 11/ CV71, d Vac/~21: .41 ,9 . ‘ C77 \‘ 217 1mm 12W * gimp“ Xwfioi’, Jva/LW ,UC ,gx'z AMI/:31 ZiLCi /i)1/y-. /’/“"v'5"//'Z - ./ ¢ WW . _, VLCL ’24? are) 022191 #2 6% M10141, 1%wa v.1 / .15 /"’ U‘L. OJ .4 Ufi/ 7.2171. 32:96 (.31...’-...*_: 11-1! 21:1/1 J/Aru/v’i 61/17. .1 41:21:: :7ch #07157 2111171 ,4 v .1 $4,631.60 5'va film/17 11“ '- -1.qu t/ /?_ - 4m- and/d / ' 1 val ,4 5’10??? Q1 0'“: five, ;C/l .. ”j a Z6462, 7 4.11:1” i 2’ IL /’L .19: «awflc/l , v’/ z, nefl CA .; :1“ “2'1 :40 «fizftffi 10* (UL .322 1.0/1 I‘flzj/.. / ( Lam—d '11,: - ’4'?“ . «y Mil/1‘ /}4’L¢L,c7/d 12,414 2 §_ -m.—. -)-'--..— .—_ m-- _— / 7.1 /L CY bfW CELL/)1. jJ'L on 'n/rLa’ :Q Zia/I: 7?mech LL; W/‘YLLJJ 3., l 218 _n— ———_.._ -_-—-— " M- CL-ZJZW :71/ L/‘LL/ flvé/ZL/ /-’J'L’/ /C C CL [ALL LLLL'J _, ./ LLY‘ ../'L WW v / ;L/LOZLO 1 j A - A/ 27.7 1 . I d, - w-’Lh-v ” .10“, NJ ;1/ _.- l/: /0 1K1). j—X’J» U -‘.’L‘.’7_J ((0-0- LW‘L/J w :0; ~ . I gull“) ,CC/23-7 _JQ‘ cm I m:fi mya/IT/gfc/ AU'D‘LW/ .- ”77/671an Milk mad --—-.—._-.—_ -- ¢- o——_-— h--O..--_ . .. ELL/224:1 Wfi (La/mi ,LJ’ -é/ii/ , mum/>7. L n .Lm; 765:1}? -Lw’wn t/w Aflwmda ciééfnnby flag/W %W Ava. Viz/LOLLLCL/TL if; OLLLQI/O' DRAFT OF ANNE'S STORY MIDDLE 3 OF 12 PAGES jm //1/./ “Lt- ”7.; W724 L .2127; - wa 1.6702456 .10- j?» “with 1601256, 1}le J «la-{LU ......."'.:..1: — in “(j .vIL.-- Lil-53.2 ’40." 77.1% _iQ’.---._-_ 00/41/77 "/ 44/70/1466 ,. gal/Lt J46: .a/‘fkl. -.__-./.w.17L2,my 4,33” Jf/w _- -u’1.a..VL/J;‘Cz.~z.1--- 6621.01». 26 00727.2 -2- u. V6.66 5.7.); <(. gm? ”44261.1. _ /L./Z 2% PLL‘C- ’c/bw'?’ C 1/1.”: 1 77.00 PLOW -Lc—azj ”mu éfiwar [1251224. -l/Zé/Z" ”632% CL 0.9:... W024. .-__-. _- flu: Jam/11.01. “hm/C61. a/XlL—j %/ é.) .. .. % ...QCLM ....... 1mm $2?ng Mf- 3:! ._—a 220 ® 1 _———.".—-— ——- _. ..— . . ...“A' a; __.-—-.__ _' -.— at ,2uL/‘213JJ‘.U .///‘-./51..7L 2.1- - __JLL/‘ZU [.-L‘Lvflull (”flit (-61.. _LL‘rC-‘j/ .- OQ/IM 10".- _ Cu’i/ 1...?) ...2/21; Am. /2<21/"{7’ z 3:1 ..éié 22.2 17 -610-ng ’It/1' '7 (1:27 C1 79 14.612375}; 2.75 1’ {LC/1.2:." . n . A" ‘4 .a/ -/X/ } 7/ '2], ,' 3 ? Jx/ Ll.- «(7:64 v/J-J'tu .-4. I / ./--g.:: 2 X ..r". A f .- v. . “I ‘ . I / ' .3 l .‘ g . i - H ~ 0 - O . . on! v \ .1'2/ % .- Ca C‘ld" - - 1 ~ A .4 CLO/d 4.11.6.0 .0” 33.5772 .3132" "‘ :— ‘ "Ll wwc/co/L: L5. 01‘} 477/ J ;7 /mo —- ””4 /Z- Z"; id; -352; 271/ Mir-1-629 2 /2" :2 JJ-e/ 1226 /fl.&J2/'22.Z>2.2 Q/V-LZ _ 3.. Ud/l 292/“ j; ,K’LLZ - /,/71<‘Z/LZ/ZA ‘ ._ )7 23,-rm -QU-Vma .6va cm “(3 [W -_._Zé./21'2 770 J/r/Lowl W'LL %0L7LZ . 7.72m}: .-.-_ 1 W ' hf/Wd. .5... £ - .... -1.-- -Wm '1 . MmoZ/u z._6.:~*’.- -. 2721-4. _- ”£2;ch “Jun-U.- .. 221 mtjfiz. 2:.‘1/‘11 ..Muk/l ' rf/ ”/ /I’,_" . . %/ {—11 ”(2 -\./I).L/‘/"\' :7/32/ :"I ‘L" .2'112/1-1'1/ L.‘..7:;‘f:.»}/.Ii . it: 22.4.2 .y.‘. 2 ) -;-. 2‘ ;‘_:t./._ 21.42.24 22 n- <22. --.424-.. by .2042. Z? 40.. -2- flua. .424M .1/ . JEZZ/ZZZ 0774/.) / 41.214 .__- 04147-4374 074 44/ .UL/M'VL 0. 7224th. #10 4. 44% “2.4.; ' .24th 0.4m. MA 414/ r f2 Q. Jet-442.22; (“4.212141 ) -hL/lw- 1.2.2442! - -- 4:002:4/ 440.42. 2:0 .44 (2.22.2. .1fl72‘x’7” glut/12.2.24 471.6421 Z// /Z‘: 22/ ' EAA/Z/L/ .00?"- (LIZ/.1 Q 1 [4214. 41/sz . flag 2% MUZU.ZL. ”Ar“; ZZZ? .002 Z _. 200/447 .40?" AMA J. / 222 Q) _ .M3-L>C31fl7/1 ml /,.-A.L 7/L2%67 wfla‘ulQ @LLLL- LL - :_LL,-L /L% _6 (2 7.6514670 c diam / . . . .1 0‘ I , [15?” 4241‘- ' GYM/{imi— and 2 ;- , JIM 2462-] MM XML/LEM it? (XL/L3 #6101 223 BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Bereiter, C. (I980). Development in writing. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Britton, J. (I970). Language and learning. Miami: University of Miami Press. Britton, J., Burgess, T., Martin, N., McLeod, A., 8. Rosen, H. (I975). LIE development of writingabilities. London: MacMillan. Calkins, L. M. (I980). Research update: children learn the writer's craft. Language Arts, 5_7. Collins, A., <3: Gentner, D. A. (I980). A framework for a cognitive theory of writing. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing. Hillsdale, , NJ: Erlbaum. Collins, J., 6. Michaels, 5. (I980). The importance of conversational discourse strategies in the acquisition of literacy. Proceedings of the Sixth Annual Meetings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Cook-Gumperz, J. (I977). Situated instruction: Language: Language socialization of school age children. In S. Ervin.Trips & A. Mitchell- Kernan (Eds.), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press. Cook-Gumperz, J., 8. Gumperz, J. (I98I). From oral to written culture: The transition to literacy. In M. F. Whiteman (Ed.), Writing: The nature development and teachinLof writing: Writing communication, Vol. I. Variation in writing: Functional and- linguistics-~cultural differences. Hillsdale, NJ: . Cooper, C. R., & Odell, l. (I978). Research on composing: Points of departure. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. DeStefano, J., Pepinsky, H., & Sanders, T. (I982). Discourse rules for literacy learning in a classroom. In L. C. Wilkinson (Ed.), Communicating in the classroom. New York: Academic Press. Doyle, W. (I982) Academic Work Research and Development Center for Teacher Education, The University of Texas at Austin. Dyson, A. H. (l98l, October). Oral language: The rooting system for learning to write. Language Arts, 5_8(7). Dyson, A., & Genishi, C. (I982). Whatta ya tryin to write? Writing as an interactive process. Language Arts, 2(2). Erickson, F. (I982). Classroom discourse as improvisation: Relationship 224 225 between academic task structure and social participation structure in lessons. In L. C. Wilkinson (Ed.), Communicating in the classroom. New York: Academic Press. Erickson, F. (I982). Taught cognitive learning in its immediate environments: A neglected topic in the anthropology of education. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, XIII(2). Emig, J. (l98l). Nonzmagical thinking: Presenting writing developmentally in schools. In C. H. Frederiksen & J. F. Dominic (Eds.), The naturg, development and teaching of written communication--volume 2, Writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Emig, J. (I970. The composing process of twelfth graders. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Fisher, C. W., Filby, N. N., Marliave, R. 5. (I977, April). Instructional time and student achievement in second grade reading and mathematics. Paper presented at the meetings of the American Educational Research Association, New York. Flavell, J. H. (I977). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Florio, S. (I979-80). The problem of dead letters: Social perspectives on the teaching of writing. The Elementary School Journal. Florio, 5., & Clark, C. (I979, December). Occasions for writing in the classroom: Toward a description of written literacy in school. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association, Cincinati. Florio, 5., & Clark, C. (I982). What is writing for? Writing in the first weeks of school in a second-third grade classroom. In L. C. Wilkinson (Ed.), Communicating in the classroom. New York: Academic Press. Flower, L. S., & Hayes, J. R. (I977). Problem-solving strategies and the writing process. College English, _3_9, 4149-46l. Flowers, L., & Hayes, J. (I980). The Dynamics of Composing: Making Plans and Juggling Constraints. In L. Gregg & E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive Processes in Writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Frederickson, C. H., 8. Dominic, J. F. (Eds.) (l98l). Writing: The nature, development, and teaching of written communication, volume 2. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Freedle, R., & Duran, R. P. (I979). Sociological approaches to dialogue with suggestive applications to cognitive science. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing, vol. 2. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Gordon, R. (I969). Interviewing: Strategy, techniques and tactics. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. Graves, D. (I975, Winter). An examination of the writing processes of seven year old children. Research in the Teaching of Emlish. 226 Graves, D. (I979). What children show us about revision. Language Arts, _5_6(5). Graves, D. (l98l). Research update: Writing research for the eighties: What is needed. Language Arts, 5&2). Graves, D.. H. (l979b). Research update: A six-year-old's writing process: The first half of first grade. Language Arts, 26(7). Graves, D. H. (I983). Writing: Teachers and children at work. Exeter, NH: Hunemann Educational Books. Greenhalgh, C., 8. Townsend, D. (I98I). Evaluating students writing holistically-- an alternative approach. Language Arts, 5_8(7). Gundlach, R., 8. Moses, R. (I976, October). Developmental issues in the study of children's written language. Paper presented at the First Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Gundlach, R. (l98l). On the nature and development of children's writing. In C. H. Frederiksen 8. J. F. Dominic (Eds.), The nature, development and teaching of written communication--volume 2, Writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hayes, J., 8. Flower, L. S. (I980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. Gregg 8. E. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hunt, K. (I965). Grammatical structures written at three grade levels. Urbana, IL: National council of Teachers of English. Kantor, K., Kirby, D., 8. Goetz, J. (I98l, December). Research in context: Ethnographic studies in English education. Research in the Teaching of English, EM). Kroll, B. M. (I978). Cognitive egocentrism and the problem of audience awareness in written discourse. Research in the Teaching of English, l_2_, 269-28l. Loban, W. (I976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English, Research Report l5. Markman, E. (I977). Realizing you don't understand: A preliminary investigation. Child Development, 4_8, 986-992. Matsuhashi, A. (I982). Explorations in the real-time production of written discourse. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know. New York: Academic Press. 227 Mehan, H. (I979). Learning lessons: Social organization in the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Howard University Press. Moffett, J. (I968). Teachinthhe universe of discourse. New York: Houghton Mifflin. Mosenthal, P., 8. Na, T. J. Classroom competence and children's individual differences in writing. Journal of Educational Pfiychology, 220), l06-l2l. NCTE Committee Forum. (I979). Standards for basic skills writing program 836:838. Language Arts, g”). Newell, A., 8. Simon, H., Human Problem Solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, I972. Nold, E. W. (l98l). In C. H. Frederiksen 8. J. F. Dominic (Eds.), The nature, development and teachigg of written communication--vo|ume 2, Writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Olson, D., 8. Torrance, N. (l98l). Learning to meet the requirements of written text: Language development in the school years. In C. H. Frederiksen 8. J. F. Dominic (Eds.), The nature, development and teaching of written communication--volume7, Writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Rosen, C., 8. Rosen, H. (I974). The language of primary school children. Baltimore: Penguin Books. Scardamalia, M. (l98l). How children cope with the cognitive demands of writing. In C. H. Frederiksen 8. J. F. Dominic (Eds.), The nature, development and teaching of written communicatiom-volume 2, Writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., 8. Goelman, H. (I8l). The role of production factors in writing ability. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know: The language, processl and structure of writing discourse. New York: Academic Press. Schatzman, L., 8. Strauss, A. (I974). Field research: Strategies of a natural sociolggy. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Scollon, R., 8. Scollon, S. G. D. (l98l). Narrative literacy and face in inter-ethnic communication, R. O. Freedle (Em? Norwood, NJ: . Shuy, R. (l98l). Toward a developmental theory of writing. In C. H. F rederiksen 8. J. F. Dominic (Eds.), The nature, develg>ment and teaching of written communication-~volume 2, Writing. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Smyth, J. (I979). Pupil engaged learning time: Concepts, findings and implications. Research report #79-l-4, occasional paper series. Victoria, Australia: School of Education, Deakin University. 228 Stallard, C. K. (I974). Analysis of the writing of good student writers. Research in the Teaching of English, 8. Wax, Rosalie. Doing Fieldwork: Warnings and Advice, The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, I97I. Winograd, T. A. (I977). Framework for understanding discourse. In M. A. Just 8. P. A. Carpenter (Eds.), Cognitive processes in comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.