H THESIS “' \ LIERARY Michigan 5mm University This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE DEVELOPMENTAL RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PERCEIVED AND ACTUAL COMPETENCE IN MOTOR ABILITY AND THE RELATIONSHIP OF EACH TO MOTIVATION TO PARTICIPATE IN SPORT AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY presented by Beverly Dianne Ulrich has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for ___Eh_.JL_dcgree in W Physical Education Major professor Datemoxemhew 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution lllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllll 310672 1438 MSU RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to LJBRARJES remove this checkout from w your record. F_I______NES W‘Ill be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. Jim"33375 9062 l ’ .9 4;! ,1 APlRZNZE’ZBU 13‘ FEB 0 6 100:; JUN t 2‘9” 3E“? :1 5 35325" ABSTRACT THE DEVELOPMENTAL RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PERCEIVED AND ACTUAL COMPETENCE IN MOTOR ABILITY AND THE RELATIONSHIP OF EACH TO MOTIVATION TO PARTICIPATE IN SPORT AND PHYSICAL ACTIVITY BY Beverly Dianne Ulrich Perceived competence has been theorized as having an important affect on onehs level of motivation (Griffin & Keogh, 1982; Harter, 19813; Nicholls, 1978). Actual competence purportedly indirectly affects level of motivation by influencing cumPs perceptions (Harter, 19813; Bandura, 1977). Recently investigators have begun to examine the interrelationships among these variables within the motor domain. Critical years in the development of these interrelationships, however, have been virtually unexplored, (inen the childhood years). This investigation examined the developmental relationship between perceived and actual competence in motor ability and the relationship of each to motivation to participate in sport and physical activity. Twenty-five males and 25 females at each grade level, kindergarten through fourth, participated as subjects in this study. Perceived competence in motor ability was measured via two psychometric scales, the Perceived Competence Scale for Children (Harter, 1979b) and the Beverly Dianne Ulrich Pictoral Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Acceptance for Young Children (Harter, Pike, Efron, Chao, & Bierer, 1983). Actual motor ability was measured by performance on a nine—item motor abilities/sport skills assessment battery; Participation motivation was reflected by the childfs participation in organized sports. Univariate and multivariate analyses suggested that for children in these grade levels, perceptions of competence in motor ability were not significantly related to participation in organized sports. Multivariate analysis indicated that actual ability was significantly related to participation in organized sports. Post hoc univariate E tests and discriminant function analysis indicated that the difference in favor of the participants was primarily due to performance on the sport skill items. ldultivariate analysis and correlational procedures suggested a significant relationship between perceived competence.and actual competence in the motor domain. Again, subjects} Scores on the sport skill items were most important in distinguishing among subjects with high, average, and low perceptions Of their competence. None of the major relationships examined changed significantly across these five grade levels. Fbr young children, perceived competence may not be an important variable affecting participation in sport. Those who do participate, however, may develop greater levels of skill which, as age and accuracy of self perceptions increase may become important to onefis choice to be involved in sport. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS My doctoral program has been a long one but certainly filled with constant stimulation and growth. I would like to acknowledge several individuals who, through their perseverance, encouragement, and assistance, were instrumental in helping me complete this stage of my career. Iku Vern Seefeldt, for his unwaivering faith in my capabilities and encouragement throughout my program. The high standards he sets for himself and his dedication to research and teaching will be standards of performance toward which I will strive. Dr. Deb Feltz, whose research efforts and professional attitude I highly respect, and whose advice as a statistician and scholar was particularly helpful throughout my dissertation. Inn Joe Varro, for his valued feedback and interest in my program and dissertation. Dr. Crystal Branta, whom I have known as a committee member, teacher, mentor, colleague, and friend. In each of these roles she has demonstrated the wisdom to know when to advise and when to respect my need to grow and learn on my own. I will always be grateful for her contribution to my years as a graduate student. Crystal and Jack, for providing me with a home away from home, whenever needed. Their unconditional friendship throughout the last few years has enhanced a relationship which Dale and I will always treasure . ii Joy Kiger, for her willingness to help in any way possible, no matter how busy she was. The principals, teachers, and children of Carterville Elementary School, Carterville, Illinois and Winkler and Parrish Elementary Schools, Carbondale, Illinois, for their interest and participation in this study. And Dale, for sharing this experience with me as a colleague and husband, for his support, patience, and love. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES .................................................. vi LIST OF FIGURES..... ............................................ vii CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION ........................................ 1 Motivation: Theories and Mediating Constructs Attribution Theory .................................... 5 Self Perception as a Mediator in Motivation ........... 5 Empirical Evidence for the Relationship Between Perceived Competence and Motivation.. .............. 10 Actual Ability Relative to Perceived Ability and Motivation ......... . ....... . ........ . .............. 17 Development of Perceptions of Competence Descriptive Data ............... ..... .................. 23 Relationships of Cognitive Processes to the Accuracy of One's Perceptions of Self ....................... 24 Harter's Model of Competence Motivation ............... 29 Gender Differences ......................................... 36 Young Children's Extent of Involvement in Sport and Games.. 41 Overview of the Study ...................................... 43 Research Hypotheses ........................................ 43 Definitions ................................................ 44 CHAPTER II: METHOD Overview ................................................... 45 Subjects ................................................... 46 Data Collection ............................................ 47 Instrumentation Perceived Competence Scales ........................... 51 Assessment Battery for Motor Abilities and Sport Skills ............................................. 57 Questionnaire Regarding Participation in Physical Activity and Sport ................................. 62 iv CHAPTER III: RESULTS Relationship #1: Perceived Competence to Participation Motivation .............................................. 63 Relationship #2: Motor Abilities to Participation Motivation .............................................. 70 Relationship #3: Perceived Competence in Motor Ability to Demonstrated Motor Ability ........................... 78 Summary of Results ......................................... 89 CHAPTER IV: DISCUSSION ......................................... 91 APPENDIX A: PILOT STUDY ........................................ 105 APPENDIX B: PILOT STUDY - SUMMARY STATISTICS ................... 113 APPENDIX C: INFORMATION LETTER FOR PARENTS AND CONSENT FORM.... 118 APPENDIX D: MOTOR ABILITIES/SPORT SKILLS ASSESSMENT BATTERY: PROTOCOL AND SCORESHEET .................................... 121 APPENDIX E: QUESTIONNAIRE REGARDING PARTICIPATION IN PHYSICAL ACTIVITY AND SPORT ......................................... 127 APPENDIX F: SAMPLE PICTURE PLATES FROM THE PICTORAL SCALE OF PERCEIVED COMPETENCE AND SOCIAL ACCEPTANCE FOR YOUNG CHILDREN, AND FROM PLATES ADDED BY THE INVESTIGATOR........ 133 LIST OF REFERENCES .............................................. 138 LIST OF TABLES TABLE 1. Perceived Competence in Motor Ability: Cell Means and Standard Deviations for Participants and Nonparticipants by Gender and Grade Level ............................... 2. Univariate E Values and Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficients for the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Test Items ....................................... 3. Means and Standard Deviations for Main Effects Participation, Gender, and Grade Level on the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Battery Items .................... 4. Intercorrelation Matrix for Items on the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Assessment Battery ............... S. Univariate Flvalues for the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Test Items .............................................. 6. Univariate E_Values and Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficients for the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Test Items ........... ........ ............ . ....... 72 Means and Standard Deviations for Groups (Top, Middle, and Bottom‘Third on Perceived Competence) on the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Test Items ........ ....... ........ 8. Canonical Loadings for Perceived Competence Items and Motor Ability/Sport Skill Items on the Canonical Variate ......... . ...................................... vi Page 66 72 73 75 79 83 84 87 LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE Page 1‘ Effectance motivation, adapted from White, 1959 (Harter, 19818, p. 6) ooooooooo 000000.00 ooooooooooo ooooooo oooooooo 3O 2. Phase II of the development of effectance motivation (adapted from Harter, 1981a, p. 7) ............ . ......... 32 3. Phase III of the development of effectance motivation (adapted from Harter, 19813, p. 10 and Harter & Connell, in press) ................................ . ....... . ...... 34 4. Number of males and females who were classified as participants within each grade level..... ............... 48 5. Subsets of children (classified by grade level) who were not significantly different on perceptions of competence in motor abilities ..................... . ............ .... 6S 6. Mean values for perceived competence by hours of play ...... 69 7. Performance in motor abilities/sport skills items by hours per week spent in gross motor activity ........... . ...... 80 8. Possible relationship among three elements of Harter's Model of Competence Motivation, within the physical domain, and for young children......... ................. 102 vii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Today more than ever, accountability is an issue in physical education, as in all eduCational programs. For years public school administrators, teachers, and coaches have promoted physical education and sport programs based on a variety of benefits such as physical fitness, motor skill development, positive self-concept, sportsmanship, leadership, cooperation, and moral development (Dauer, 1971; Martens & Seefeldt, 1979; Smoll & Lefebvre, 1979). .Apparently the public has not been convinced of either the importance or the ability of the schools to produce such benefits within the school setting. In the past few decadescunnmunity sponsored sport programs for youth and young children have continually expanded, while funding for physical education is in constant danger of being reduced or eliminated. In addition, the "accountability" era in education is forcing physical educators to produce empirical evidence to support the claims they have made. In Illinois for example, one of the few states which mandates physical education five days a week in the elementary schools, a strong lobby recently attempted to reduce the extent of this mandate. 'This specific effort was unsuccessful. 'The establishment.of a comufittee to document justification for physical education, however, by two national associations (Seefeldt, 1984) accentuates the critical nature of this issue. Several studies have demonstrated that significant changes can be made in the gymnasium, that is, attributes such as fitness (Shephard, Lavellée, Jequier, Rajic, & LaBarre, 1980), cooperation (Rogers, Miller, & Hennigan, 1981),1and motor skills (Miller, 1978; Masche, 1970) can be affected. The importance of some of the objectives listed above and the fact that the gymnasium is the most appropriate place in which to promote them seems obvious. For others, their value as specific attributes to be developed via physical activity is not so clear. Should cooperation, development of positive self-concept, and leadership_qualities apply more to physical education than to any other academic discipline, or do conditions unique to the gymnasium hold special promise for nurturing such attributes? Of these attributes, the development of a positive self-concept has recently been a popular source of investigation by researchers in many educational disciplines (Carlson, 1970; Davis, 1981; Pate, 1978). ReSults of experimental studies have been equivocal. Studies involving physical education and physical activity have resulted in as many supportive as non—supportive conclusions regarding the affectance of a positive change in self-concept (Hughes, 1973; Martinek, Zaichkowsky, & Cheffers, 1977; Schempp, Cheffers, & Zaichkowsky, 1983; Smith, 1982). The inconsistency in the results of self—perception studies may be attributed to several sources. Byrne (1983) conducted a thorough review of the literature and concluded that a universally accepted and operational definition of self—concept does not exist. lflany terms are used interchangeably, such as self—esteem, self-confidence, self-image, body image, self—efficacy, reflected self, and self-concept. In addition, contradictions frequently exist between theories espoused and measurements used in such research (Sonstroem, 1982). Wylie (1974), in her classic review of the state of the art in self-concept research, recommended either abandoning self-concept theory or making theoretical and methodological changes to make it more scientifically acceptable. She went on to support the latter. One of the major criticisms of the methodology used in self— concept research relates to the use of a single score for self-concept. This score is based on the non—weighted sum of items reflecting self— perceptions in several domains, such as cognitive, physical, and social. It suggests that people perceive themselves equally in all areas. Sufficient evidence exists indicating that people may feel positive about themselves in one or more domains and negative about their abilities in another. Further, these feelings and the relative importance of each domain may change with age (Sonstroem, 1982). The most significant and productive self relationships to be investigated involve the areas of a specific domain. A curriculum specialist might investigate the effect of open classrooms versus traditional settings on self-perceptions in the academic area. In physical activity and sport one might investigate the effect of a free play situation versus instruction in motor skills on childrenks perceptions of themselves in the domain of physical activity. One might ask why perceptions of onefs abilities are important. If one feels positive about oneself in the area of physical skills, will one be more likely to choose to become involved in sport and physical activity? Are perceptions of ability reflective of actual ability? Limited data exist from which we might answer these questions. However, studies involving upper elementary grade level students suggest that these relationships exist and are positive (Boling & Kirk, 1983; Guyot, Fairchild, & Hill, 1981; Roberts, Kleiber, & Duda, 1981). A second question which relates to those above is Thaw do perceptions of competence relate to the primary goals of elementary physical education, iJL, development of motor skills and improvement of fitness?" Ostensibly, these are promoted so that children are competent enough to participate in the common games and sports of childhood and through these games maintain a healthy level of fitness. Little is known about why children choose to become involved. Young children seem to have an incessant desire to be active, but as age increases many become less active. If the skills and concepts learned in the gymnasium are only used during that period of required involvement, the results of physical education would seem to fall short of the objectives of the educational system in general. The investigation of the relationship of perceptions of ability to actual ability and the relationship of each to participation motivation seems relevant to providing an optimal movement or motor skill development program for children. By understanding the interrelationships of these variables we may determine which ones can be affected within the movement setting. Further, we may better understand how such variables may affect childrenfls desires to become involved in and persist in the games and sports of our culture. Examining theories of motivation seems essential if we are to understand these interrelationships. Motivation: Theories and Mediating Constructs Attribution Theory The most influential approach to motivation theory relative to sport and physical activity has been attribution theory. In this, the basic assumption is that actions are based on a search for understanding (Weiner, 1972). Attribution theory assumes that we judge our successes and failures based on several causal elements, to determine why we succeeded or failed. The major causal categories used are ability, effort, luck, and task difficulty. Weiner (1974) placed these into a two-dimensional model in which causality (internal or external) and stability (stable or unstable) were the dimensions. Each of the four causal elements fell into one of the two levels of each dimension. .Ability and effort were internal factors, luck and task difficulty were external. Ability and task difficulty were stable factors, while luck and effort changed, or were unstable; One may legitimately desire to include additional factors in the model, particularly in the sport domain, because the theoretical predictions derive from the dimensions and not the causal factors per se (Roberts & Pascuzzi, 1979). Self-Perception as a Mediator in Motivation While attribution theory may remain useful in the investigation of certain aspects of motivation, a growing number of researchers are producing support for perception of ability as the most important determinant of achievement behavior (Covington & Omelich, 1979; Nicholls, 1978; Roberts & Pascuzzi, 1979; Spink & Roberts, 1980). Several investigators have theorized that attributions of ability and the self—concept of ability play the central role in mediating motivation (Bandura, 1977; Griffin & Keogh, 1982; Harter, 1981a; Kukla, 1978; White, 1959). The concept of competence was first introduced by White, in 1959, as a psychological construct mediating intrinsically motivated behavior. In his paper, "Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence." he proposed "effectance" motivation to explain why an individual feels impelled to engage in mastery attempts. .He suggested that individuals act because they feel a need to have an effect. If these performance attempts are satisfying, one feels competent, which provides a feeling of efficacy, inherent pleasure and joy. One is also likely to want to repeat that performance. However, White's explanations for such motivational concepts were very general. He viewed competence motivation as a global motive which directed the organism in all achievement-oriented tasks. .As such, it did not lend itself to empirical investigation. In 1978, Harter proposed a framework which expanded WhiteRs theory, providing a more specific model of motivation which could be empirically tested. Harter (1981b) concentrated on identifying the specific domains in which competence may be measured, and viewed onefis perceptions of competence as the central mediator of:ouo£ .o gauge—co ”:11 .n .- -- 5:02.05 I _-l 3c2onEoU .o 3.0.0.:8 _ .o oacom 5.53 .2393; .o 02.3 _o>ocaaon_o co _o>ocaa< oucoioooxx .8050 £050 >4 8:32.. .o co_.o:.o>w m0 —O>Od =030>=02 eocofiozw 053=O&\u00093m 8:39... .coEco.._>cw £3.50 9.303 093.225 29:32 fotoi .cotcoaevc. 8390.... 30.32 2: .o 2cecoafiou 2.. S .85 9.030300 30:02 33 caregivers lay the foundation for the development of childrenfis sense of competence during infancy and early childhood. Presumably climbing fences, running down hills, and chasing a friend still bring inherent pleasure, but the purity of the perceptions begins to be tempered by the reactions of significant others. Phase III (See Figure 3). The hypothesized relationship of the major correlates affecting competence/effectance motivation in Phase III has been characterized by Harter (Harter & Connell, in press) as a chain. Four primary correlates are now identified (actual competence, perception of competence, competence affect, and understanding what controls performance outcome). ‘The first three of these appeared in Phase II; however, their relationship to each other and to motivation becomes attenuated somewhat. In this phase, the effect of children's actions on their environment may be interpreted as their actual level of competence. As in Phase II, significant others respond with both evaluative and acceptance/rejection information. Each continues to have an effect on perceptions of competence and affect, respectively; However, older children begin to perceive themselves in a more complex manner as the capacity for logical thought and appreciation of the relationship between cause and effect emerges. Harter referred to this new aspect of information processing as the internalization of cognitive- informational structures (labelled in Figure 3 as Internalized Set of Mastery Goals and Criteria for Success). Internalized mastery goals and criteria for success begin to be formed when children adopt the performance standards of the significant Others in their world. This 34 Unknown Control Actual Competence Effect on the environment: If Known Control Success or failure ( internal or external) T; ‘ t' ---------------- .1 : Evaluation by whenJi 1 I b.---------------J Domain Specific * Competence/Effectance r----"-----’-"-""fi' Motivation , Internalized set of Mastery ' : Goals and Criteria Lo: Success : L ................ J Eerceived M l l ‘ l' -------------- H : Acceptance or Reiection l: : by others : L ............ _.| (Signifies most important FCompetence Affect ]/ relationships in the model) (signifies correlates to motivation) [:1 l l Liz—.11 (signifies antecedents to motivation) Figure 3: Phase 1]]: of thedevelopment of effectance motivation (adapted from Harter. 19810. p. 10 and Harter 8. Connell, in press). _ 35 process allows children to judge how much they value a specific domain and what level of performance constitutes success or competence. The consistency and relevancy of the evaluative feedback children receive from others influences the degree to which goals and criteria for success are internalized. This, in turn, affects perceptions of who or what controls performance outcome. If children are given clear, consistent, and relevant evaluations about their performances they will develop consistent and realistic internalization structures and understand who controls performance outcomes, iJL, internal or external/powerful others. Inconsistent evaluations lead to ambiguities about the source of control (unknown control). Harter (Harter & Connell, in press) contended that'the driving force for the "motivation chain" is the child's perception of who controls the outcome of performance situations. Those who understand who controls it are the high level performers; those without a clear understanding perform less competently. Competence affect results from cxuys perceptions of competence and the acceptance or rejection of that performance level by others. ‘It also affects motivation level but not as directly as perceived competence. Support for the directionality of the variables in Harter’s model comes from a study involving upperelementary and junior high school level subjects (Connell, 1981). First, subjects were tested on all elements in her model. Second, structural equation modeling procedures (a type of path analysis) were used to test four possible models or directional relationships among the motivational correlates proposed. 36 The model presented was the one determined to best fit the data collected. Most of the resultant relationships seem plausible; However, her reference to the unknown control element as being directly related to performance seems overzealous, based on her cross-sectional data and correlational procedures. Further, Connell (1981) from whose dissertation these results were derived, refers to the understanding of control element as "setting the stage" for the cognitive and affective processing of performance information. Intuitively this seems to be a more reasonable interpretation. In summary, Harter (Harter & Connell, in press) proposed that a clear understanding of who controls performance outcomes (one's self or powerful others) leads to demonstration of higher levels of actual competence, which leads to higher perceptions of competence and to a strong motivation to have an effect on the environment or demonstrate mastery of tasks in that domain. Conversely, children who do not know why they are successful or unsuccessful perform less competently, perceive themselves as less competent and lack motivation to achieve in that domain. Gender Differences Evidence is accumulating that there are gender differences in the domain of physical activity for the three central variables to be investigated in this study, iJL, perceived competence, actual motor ability, and motivation to participate. Based on her own research, Harter (1978) noted a trend toward gender differences in motivation levels for tasks in which a gender difference was noted in skill level. 37 For tasks in which males demonstrated an advantage over females, either in measures of true ability or those which are stereotyped as masculine, males reported higher levels of motivation. However, for tasks in which females demonstrated equal or higher skill levels compared to males, level of motivation appeared to be equal. .Although Harter's investigations have dealt primarily with cognitive skills, a similar trend may be expected in motor skills. Males appear to have an.advantage in level of skill when compared to females both in true ability and as stereotyped by society. Prior to adolescence males have a slight, although in most practical situations a non—significant advantage in actual motor ability. ldhile minor anatomical differences may account for this disparity in skill level, strong cultural expectations are almost certain to play a part in fostering this difference (Herkowitz, 1978). The stereotype that sport and game participation is more appropriate for males than for females, while changing, certainly still exists. Parents demonstrate a greater response to the involvement in sport of their sons than to their daughters' involvement (Lamb, 1976). Children also display gender differences regarding their opinion of the appropriateness of participation by females in sport. In a study involving third through sixth grade boys and girls, Selby and Lewko (1976) found males less favorable toward female participation in sport than were females, regardless of their own level of participation. Motivation level in sport and physical activity may be measured by participation versus nonparticipation or persistence in a task. When motivation is measured via percent of each gender participating in 38 sport, males continue to dominate, even though the number of female participants is increasing (Kleiber & Roberts, 1983; Lever, 1976; Youth Sports Study, Phase I, 1976). ldhen persistence in a task was analyzed, a gender difference was not found (Roberts et ed., 1981). IHowever, this measure was of a childls anticipated persistence, not demonstrated persistence. Phase one of a study of sport participation by youth (State of Michigan, 1976) found that one out of four boys participated in and completed a season in baseball while only one in ten girls did ex» Additional research is necessary to determine if females who choose to participate will try just as hard to succeed as their male counterparts. Males typically perform sport skills better than females, and they also perceive their general level of athletic ability to be higher than do females (Duquin, 1978; Wiggins, 1973). 'This difference in predicted ability may not be true for all tasks, however, Evidence suggests that for those tasks categorized as having "masculine" components such as speed or strength, males perceive their ability to be higher than females, but for "neutral" tasks a gender difference is not found (Corbin, Landers, Feltz, & Senior, 1983; Corbin & Nix, 1979; Corbin, Stewart, & Blair, 1981). Gender differences in self perceptions may be masked by the use of global self-concept scores. The assessment of global self traits or attributes usually fails to reveal gender differences (Magill & Ash, 1979; Martinek & Zaichkowsky, 1977; Piers, 1969). However, such measurements tend to mask differences which may be occurring within separate domains (e49, physical, social, and cognitive) by utilizing a 39 composite score for all assessment items which weights each domain equally. In reality, children are likely to value some competencies more than others, or feel significantly more competent in one area than in others (Rosenberg, 1979). Ability in physical skills is generally considered to be an important source of status for children (Smoll, 1974), however, athletic inVolvement may be a more salient source of pride and status for males than for females (Kleiber, 1979; Kleiber & Hemmer, 1981). Harter (1981) did not find a gender difference for the cognitive, social, and global subscales across Grades 3 through 6. Only on the physical subscale did males report significantly higher perceptions of ability than did females. The reactions and evaluations of significant others to young childrenksattempts to perform movement tasks are important to the development of perceptions of competenceu Gender differences in perceived competence, particularly in the physical activity domain, may begin as a result of differential responses by parents. There are several ways in which significant others may have a differential effect. Stereotyped attitudes are likely to be reflected in parents' reactions to and evaluations of the movement performances of their offspring, ioe" more encouraging for boys, less encouraging for girls. This may influence developing perceptions of competence and motivate some to repeat such movement‘s more often than others. The task experiences one has are further expected to influence onefis attitudes (Breer & Locke, 1965). If young boys are experiencing movement activities more frequently, §2d_if they are positive, a more positive perception of competence should develop. Further, the increased 4O participation would provide more opportunities for comparison of abilities whereby a more accurate conception of motor ability may result. With accuracy may come the realization for some boys that they are not as good as they thought they were, thereby discouraging participation. 'The actual point at which this may begin to occur, and whether it does occur earlier for males than for females is unknown, but warrants further investigation. Nicholls (1978) hypothesized that an understanding that ability and effort are distinct causal factors should lead to more accurate assessment of one's own competence and thus affect motivation. He studied children ages 5 through 13, and developed four stages of reasoning which he believes children go through regarding reasoning about their achievements in tasks. He found that boys were more advanced in their reasoning in an academic task situation, and the older males (ages 9 to 13) chose more difficult tasks to perform than did females of the same age. However, they were no more accurate in estimating their own level of competence. Accuracy improved with age for both genders. Some support for Nicholls} theories was produced in a study of fourth and fifth grade participants and nonparticipants in sport (Roberts et al., 1981). Participants believed ability was more important than effort as compared to nonparticipants, but a gender difference in perception of the role of effort and ability was not found. Participants demonstrated higher perceptions of competence, reported that they would persist longer in difficult tasks, and had higher expectations of future success. ldhile admitting the topic is 41 debatable, these researchers suggested that " ."sport selects out those who perceive themselves as more competent to begin with rather than contributing in a substantial way to the development of perceptions of competence" (p. 213). However, this study was cross-sectional in design and only identified participants and nonparticipants. Developmental variables may be examined with greater scientific validity by utilizing a longitudinal design. By following the same subjects over several years, one may find that participation does not increase perceptions of ability for those who find success. For those who are eliminated from teams, are not given an opportunity to participate in games, or are unsuccessful in other ways, organized sport participation may decrease perceptions of ability. Therefore, some of Roberts et al.s nonparticipants may have been drop-outs whose perceptions of competence were higher at an earlier time in their lives but were negatively affected by participation in organized sport. Younnghildren's Extent of Involvement in Sport and Games The number of children participating in organized sport and games has been rising in the last few decades (Kleiber & Roberts, 1983; Martens & Seefeldt, 1979). In addition to the probable trickle-down effect of increased societal interest in fitness and activity, Seefeldt (Seefeldt & Gould, 1980) suggested several reasons for the increase in numbers: (a) lowering of the age at which children may enter organized sport programs, (b) increase in the number of sport situations available to children, (c) greater participation by females, and (d) improved public transportation, providing greater access to sport facilities. 42 The number of children participating in sport rises during the elementary school years and peaks during the middle school years. Magill and Ash (1979) studied first through fifth grade children in Texas and found an increase in participation by grade level. By third grade over half of their subjects had participated on at least one organized team during the past year. The second graders reported close to 50% participation, while many first grade children were already involved. Results of Phase I of the Michigan Youth Sports Study (State of Michigan, 1976) revealed that most participants entered organized sport at approximately eight or nine although some began as early as age three or four. By 11 or 12 years of age a peak and subsequent drop-off in participation was observed for males and females. In addition to time spent in organized sport activities, participation in non-agency sponsored physical games is frequent. Kleiber and Roberts (1983) studied the free-time activity patterns of children between the ages of 9 and 11. They reported that the most frequently chosen activity for both boys and girls was physical games, such as ping pong, basketball, and tag, with percent of participation greater for males than females. Other categories into which childrenks after-school activities were classified included physical free-form activities (playing catch, frisbee, climbing), watching TV, lessons (music, dance, gymnastics), reading, doing homework, etc. We must confront the facts that (a).89. Total scale reliability for the preschool/kindergarten items was .88; for first/second grade items, .87. As with the other perceived competence instrument utilized in this study, additional items were added to the subscale for physical ability. Each subscale currently contains six items, therefore six additional items were added bringing the total to 12. The internal consistency of the items in the expanded subscale for perceived competence in physical ability was examined with reliability analysis using coefficient alpha procedures. An acceptable standardized alpha of .80 was obtained which approximated that obtained by the test developer. 'The range of alpha values obtained by item, if deleted from the test, was from .76 to .81. Items included ip_Harter.§. Items added £2 the scale: Pike's physical activity subscale: 1. good at swinging items for 1. good at jumping 2. good at climbing pre-school 2. good at bouncing a ball 57 3. can tie shoes & kinder— 3. good at kicking a ball 4. good at skipping garten level 4. good at throwing a ball 5. good at running children 5. good at catching a ball 6. good at hopping 6. am strong Items included _i_rl Harter _&_ Items added _t_:_g_ Elle_§§§1_e_: Pike's physical activipy subscale: 1. good at swinging items for 1. good at jumping 2. good at climbing first and 2. good at batting a ball 3. good-at bouncing ball second grade 3. good at kicking a ball 4. good at skipping level 4. good at throwing a ball 5. good at running children 5. good at catching 6. good at jumping a rope 6. am strong Examples of the picture plates used to assess perceived competence in motor domain tasks may be found in Appendix F. Assessment Batterypfor Motor Abilities and Sport Skills The items included in the motor assessment battery were chosen to represent (a) specific motor abilities as identified by factor analysis studies and (b) motor skills common to the most popular organized sports of children in the community being studied. Test items had to meet several criteria, as listed below. Each item had to: 1. be appropriate for the age range of 5 to 10 years (appropriate to children's cognitive as well as motor abilities), 2. be reliable, 3. be valid, 58 4. in conjunction with other items in this assessment battery, not require a total administration time in excess of 30 minutes, 5. be administerable in a typical elementary school gymnasium or multi—purpose room, and 6. not require elaborate or non—portable equipment. Preliminary items were chosen by this investigator and reviewed by three content experts. ‘Following minor revisions, nine items were agreed upon as representative of the two content areas specified, motor abilities and sport skills. Subsequently, a pilot study was conducted to determine if all items could meet criteria one through four as indicated above. One hundred ninety-four children in Grades K through 4 were assessed with the battery of task items. (Fbr details of the pilot study see Appendix AJ Based on the results of this preliminary study all of the original items were maintained, four of which required revisions in protocol. Test-retest reliability coefficients for a sample of 10 first grade subjects ranged from .57 tol.95 for the nine items. Specific item reliabilities may be found in Appendix B. Face validity of the items was based on the judgment of three content experts, as noted previously. The validity of the battery of items as a measure of general motor ability was examined in the pilot study. All children who were assessed in the pilot study were also rank ordered within grade level on the basis of demonstrated motor performance by their physical education specialist. Rank order scores were converted to z scores and used in multiple regression and discriminant function analyses. Results of the multiple regression analyses suggested that the amount of variance that could be accounted 59 for by the nine items, within each grade level ranged from 56% to 77% (M_ = 66.4%). Results of the discriminant function analyses suggested that when used in combination, subjects' scores on the nine items could successfully classify them as belonging in the top, middle, or lower third of their grade level in motor performance, as indicated by rank order scores assigned to them. Percent of correct classification ranged from .69 to .88 (ll = .79). A brief description of each item included in the battery, how it was scored, and the ability or skill which it measured follows. Complete protocol information is located in Appendix D. Item Motor ability 3; skill tested 1. Standing Broad Jump (power of legs) The broad jump test required subjects to stand with toes behind a take—off line on the floor and jump as far as possible onto a tumbling mat. Take-off and landing were on two feet. Score was measured in inches, calculated to the nearest 1/2 inch. 2. Flexed Arm Hang (muscular endurance of upper arms and shoulder girdle) Subjects grasped the 1 1/2 inch diameter chinning bar with a pronated grip and maintained as long as possible, a position in which the chin was above the bar and arms fully flexed. Score was number of seconds position was maintained, scored to the nearest 1/10th of a second. 3. Side-step Test (agility without running) From a standing position astride a center line, subjects were required to slide 6 feet to the right, touching the right foot to the 60 floor outside the right side line. Without turning, subjects had to slide in the opposite direction to touch left foot outside the left side line. Sliding continued to alternate side lines until 10 seconds had passed. IPoints were scored for each line crossed within the 10 seconds. 4. Sixty Yard Shuttle Run (speed and agility while running) Two cones were placed 15 yards apart with a starting line on the floor, even with the outer edge of one cone. Subjects ran two laps around the cones. Score was the number of seconds which elapsed 'from start to finish, scored to the nearest 1/10th of a second. 5. Sit-up Test (strength and endurance of abdominal muscles) Bent-knee sit-ups were performed, on a tumbling mat. Feet were held by the subject's testing partner. Subject's score was the number of sit-ups completed within 30 seconds. 6. Playground Ball Dribble Test (eye-hand coordination and a sport skill) In this test subjects were asked to dribble a 9 inch red playground ball in and out of a row of six traffic cones placed 8 feet apart. Score was the number of cones the subject dribbled past within 30 seconds. 7. Soccer Ball Dribble Test (eye-foot coordination and a sport skill) Subjects were asked to dribble CLe., move the ball with their feet only) a size five indoor soccer ball in and out of a row of traffic cones. The row consisted of five cones placed 10 feet apart. Subjects received one point each time their ball passed between two cones within 30 seconds. 61 8. Soccer Ball Throw Test (power of upper body and a sport skill) From behind a restraining line, subjects used a two-handed overhead throwing motion to throw a size five outdoor soccer ball for distance. A step forward was taken as the subject threw the ball. Score was the distance the ball traveled in the air, measured to the nearest inch. 9. Softball Repeated Throws Test (eye-hand coordination and a sport skill) In this test subjects were asked to throw an indoor softball at a large target on the wall as often as possible within 30 seconds. Subjects were required to catch (or retrieve) each rebound. The ball had to be thrown from within a designated throwing area; catching was not restricted to that area. Score was the number of times the subject hit the target within 30 seconds. Test-retest reliability of the items as revised from the pilot study was assessed during this study for 10 subjects in each of three grade levels. Five male and five female kindergarten, second, and fourth grade level subjects were tested a second time on the motor ability/sport skill assessment battery within approximately two weeks of initial testing. The Pearson product correlations for the kindergarten grade level subjects ranged from .53 to .89 (ll of .72). Values for second grade level subjects ranged from .57 to .98 (M_of .79) while the range of values for fourth grade level subjects was from .65 to .91 (ll of .77). 62 Questionnaire Regarding Participation in Physical Activity and Sport In order to examine the level of participation in sport by subjects and to classify each child as a participant or nonparticipant in organized sport, a questionnaire was developed. The questionnaire was sent home with each child with the request that parents or guardians assist their child in its completion. Of primary interest was whether or not the child had participated in and completed the season in at least one organized sport during the past year. Those who answered this question with a yes were classified as participants for the purposes of this study. Those who did not complete at least one season of participation in an organized sport program were classified as nonparticipants. Additional information was requested of the subjects which related to reasons for participating or not participating, amount of time spent at practice, the sport in which the subject participated, how important being good at sports was to them, their choices of activities when given free time, and the amount of time spent in gross motor activity during a week. Such information was used to describe this sample and to understand better why these children were or were not participants in sport. A copy of the Questionnaire Regarding Participation in Physical Activity and Sport is located in Appendix E. CHAPTER III RESULTS Three major relationships were examined, via univariate as well as multivariate procedures. First, analysis of variance (ANOVA) and multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) were used to examine the relationship between childrenfls perceptions of their competence in motor ability and their participation motivation. Second, a MANOVA was used to examine the relationship between motor abilities and participation motivation. “Third, the relationship between perceptions of competence in motor abilities and actual motor performance was analyzed via a MANOVA, as well as correlational techniques. The analyses and results obtained will be discussed separately for each major relationship. Relationship #1: Perceived Competence to Participation Motivation AEQXA,‘ a 2 X 2 X 5 (Participation by Gender by Grade Level) ANOVA was performed to compare group differences on perceptions of competence in motor ability. Due to unequal numbers of participants and nonparticipants (participation main effect) the general linear model for regression analysis technique was utilized. This method allowed for the examination of each effect holding it orthogonal to all other effects in the model. The main effect of participation (participant versus nonpartici- pant in organized sport) was of primary concern. lhawever, sufficient 63 64 research suggests that age and gender are related to perceptions of ability. Therefore, these variables and all possible interactions were included in the analysis to investigate the possibility that the relationship between perceptions of competence and involvement may vary in relation to gender and or age (identified as grade level in this study). Results revealed that the participation main effect, FIl, 230) = 2.69, p < .10 was not significant. The main effect for gender, [(1, 230) =- 11.16, p< .001, and the grade level main effect, 11(4, 230) = 9.94, p < .0001 were significant. None of the interactions (participation by gender, participation by grade, gender by grade, participation by gender by grade) were found to be signiicant. Follow-up for determining how the genders differed on perceptions of competence was accomplished by inspection of mean values. The mean for males was 3.36 (SD 2 .48) while females had a mean of 3.13 (SD = .57). ZPerceived competence mean scores could range from one to four with one indicating low perceived competence and four reflecting high perceptions of competence. 'The mean values suggested that males perceived themselves to be more competent in motor skills than did females. To investigate the differences among grade levels in perceptions of competence in motor abilities, mean scores were subjected to tests for linear and quadratic trends and Scheffé tests of significant differences between and among groups. A significant linear effect was found [(1, 245) = 43.34, p < .0001, with mean values decreasing as grade level increased. The test for a quadratic effect was not 65 significant. Results of the Scheffé procedures suggested three homogeneous subsets of groups (Figure 5). Third and fourth grade level Grade Level 4 3 2 1 K Subset One Subset Two Subset Three Figure 5. Subsets of children (classified by grade level) who were not significantly different on perceptions of competence in motor abilities. subjects were not significantly different; first, second, and third grade level subjects did not differ significantly; and kindergarten, first, and second grade level subjects were not significantly different. Table 1 contains all cell means and standard deviations. MANOVA — A second analysis examined the relationship between perceived competence in motor ability and participation, gender, and grade level. In this MANOVA procedure subjects' item scores on the motor ability subscale of the perceived competence scale were used as the dependent variables. .Although such a multivariate analysis would conceptually provide a more powerful analysis of the relationship between the independent and dependent variables than the original ANOVA, it was not chosen as the primary model for analysis. 'Fhis decision was made for two reasons. First, only half of the total number of items used in the motor ability subscale were similar across all grade levels and therefore half of the available information would 66 NHum was a no oHum an: m mu “HHoo mom muaoaHOHuuoaao: Ho sonaazss mHum HHuz N no oHum H nu: no oHnm NH": 3 HQ mHnm ON": a ow oum 0H": m uu NHum cum cum «Ha: wHuz mu: N ha H no M no "HHou pom muamaHuHuuma Ho poneazs Ho>oa ocouu Ho. oo.m mm. mo.m ow. om.m no. Hm.m om. mm.m Hooch No. OH.m mo. Hm.m oc. No.m 0e. wo.m mm. HN.m Hm. mm.m moHoEom we. mm.m Hm. wo.m em. mo.m mm. m¢.m om. mm.m mm. N©.m mOHmz mm. HN.m Ho. mc.m mm. oo.~ 0o. em.m om. cm.m Ne. mm.m *smuamQHOHuumacoz me. 0H.m mm. ow.m oo. cm.m om. em.m He. mH.m mm. ha.m mOHmsom mm. mm.m ac. m~.m mm. cH.m He. mq.m ma. eq.m em. oo.m mOHmz om. mm.m mm. oH.m on. ON.m He. em.m as. om.m mm. mm.m smucoaHOHuumm mm .2. NW H... mm .2. mm H... mm n W m 3838 Hmuoa N cOHuoaHowuumm maOHumH>on cumcamuw new name: HHoo H OHan Ho>oq Dunno can nouaoo A; muaoaHOHuuoaaoz pom mucoaHOHuumm How "auHHHn< uouoz aH ooaouoqeoo oo>Hoouom 67 be ignored. Second, most of the items that were similar across the grades were added to the subscale by this investigator and were therefore not original items on the scale. Results of this analysis were similar to those obtained in the ANOVA analysis. The main effect of participation was not significant, Wilks' Lambda 3 .95, [(7, 224) = 1.65, p < .12. The main effects for gender, Wilks' Lambda a .91, [(7, 224) = 3.26, p_ < .003 and grade, Wilks' Lambda 8 .69, {(28, 809) =- 3.13, p< .0001 were significant. In addition, one interaction effect was found to be significant, gender by grade, Wilks' Lambda =- .83, F_(28, 809) = 1.49, p< .05. This suggested that for one or more items the difference between males'anul females' perceptions of ability in motor skills was not the same across all grade levels. As a follow-up to the significant gender by grade level interaction effect the univariate E_values were examined for each dependent variable. At the univariate level only one of the perceived competence items, ability to dribble a ball, demonstrated a significant interaction effect, _F_(4, 230) = 3.25, p < .01. Newman—Keuls' procedures were used to examine differences between all pairs of cell means involved in the interaction. ‘The primary source of this interaction appeared to be the fact that males and females did not differ significantly in their perceptions of competence on this item at any grade level except fourth. At this grade level the mean for females (2.68) was significantly lower than for males (3.24). Further, the mean value for females was significantly lower than for all other groups except the third grade level males. 68 Because many of the subjects in this study were younger than the age at which most children become participants in organized sport, an additional variable, which may also reflect participation motivation, hours per week spent in gross motor activity, was examined. The general linear model for regression analysis was used with perceived competence in motor ability as the dependent variable. Independent variables were hours of play (a continuous variable) and gender and grade level (each identified as a categorical variable). Results suggested that the hours of play main effect was significant, F_(1, 230) a 3.78, p < .05. A plot of mean values for this main effect (Figure 6) suggested that as the number of hours in a week spent in gross motor activity increased, perceptions of motor ability also increased. The amount of variance in perceptions of competence for which the full model could account was .26. Hours of play specifically accounted for only .014 of the total variance. This appears to be a very small proportion when compared to that accounted for by the other significant main effects in the model, gender and grade level, which accounted for .043 and .174, respectively. In Figure 6, the mean for 20 hours per week conspicuously departs from the positive relationship between perceived competence and hours per week spent in gross motor activity. The severity of the drop may result from the fact that 1 of the 11 subjects had a mean score for perceived competence which was almost three standard deviations below the mean for the subject's grade level. A specific reason why this subject had such a low score could not be identified. lVithout this 3.554 3.50- 3.45- 3.40- 3.354 3.30- 3.25- 3.204 3.15- 69 3.10: PERCEIVED COMPETENCE IN MOTOR ABILITY cf I I I I T r 05 IO 15 20 25 30 N: (121) (62) (34) (ll) (8) (14) Figure 6. Mean values HOURS PER WEEK SPENT IN GROSS MOTOR ACTIVITY for perceived competence by hours of play. 70 score the mean value for 20 hours per week would have been 3JH. providing a less severe drOp in the graph. As noted above, the main effects gender F(1, 230) = 13.44, p < .0003 and grade level, _F_(4, 230) = 13.57, p < .0001 were also significant. The relationships between these variables and perceived competence in motor ability were examined previously in a model with participation as the additional independent variable. Results were similar. 'Further interpretation of the results relative to these main effects would therefore be redundant (see pages 64-65). None of the interactions included in this model were significant. Relationship #2: Motor Abilities to Participation Motivation MANOVA - A 2 X 2 X 5 (Participation by Gender by Grade Level) MANOVA was applied to examine the relationship between childrens' choices to be participants or non-participants in organized sport and their performance on nine selected motor ability and sport skill items. As in the first relationship examined, participation was the effect of primary interest. Gender and grade level were also included because ample research indicates that each has a significant relationship to motor performance. ZFurther, the relationship between participation and motor ability may change with different levels on one or both of these variables. The results revealed a significant main effect for participation, Wilks' Lambda = .89, [(9, 222) = 3.01, p< .002; gender, Wilks' Lambda = .71, _F_(9, 222)= 9.91, p_< .0001; and grade, Wilks' Lambda = .26, £136, 833) a ILLOZ, p_< .0001. None of the interactions (participation by gender, participation by grade, gender by grade, participation by 71 gender by grade) were significant. Univariate Fltests, as well as discriminant function analyses were used as follow-up procedures when significant multivariate results were obtained to determine which dependent variables contributed most to differentiating the groups. Results of the univariate E tests for the involvement main effects showed significant differences between participants and nonparticipants on four of the nine items in the motor assessment battery (Table 2). These items were the soccer ball dribble, basketball dribble, softball repeated throws, and the soccer ball throw items, ith, the sport skill items. Inspection of the means for these two groups (Table 3) indicated that participants performed better than nonparticipants on all of these items. The discriminant function analysis suggested that five items could be used to discriminate significantly between these two groups (Wilks' Lambda = .7950, p.< .0001). These items included the soccer ball dribble, soccer ball throw, situp, sidestep, and flexed arm hang test items. However, the discriminant function coefficients associated with each of these items clearly indicated that the soccer ball dribble item was the most powerful discriminator. The discrepancy between the results of the univariate gland discriminant function analyses may reflect the difference in the mathematical techniques involved. Univariate {_values reflect separate group comparisons for each dependent variable. Therefore, they do not account for intercorrelations among the variables. Discriminant function procedures (when using the stepwise method).attempt to identify in a stepwise manner, the most parsimonious subset of 72 Table 2 Univariate §_Values and Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficients for the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Test Items . Standardized Discriminant Dependent Variable Univariate F Function Coefficient Participation Main Effect (Wilks' Lambda = .7950 pK.0001) 1. Soccer dribble 22.69** 1.00 2. Basketball dribble 10.56** XX 3. Softball repeated throws 5.20* XX 4. Soccer ball throw 4.28* .30 S. Broad jump 0.46 XX 6. 60 yard shuttle run 2.85 XX 7. Situp 0.05 -.36 8. Sidestep 0.35 -.25 9. Flexed arm hang 2.54 .16 Gender Main Effect (Wilks' Lambda = .7352 23.0001) 1. Soccer dribble 47.47** .81 2. Basketball dribble 40.04** .52 3. Softball repeated throws 32.27** XX 4. Soccer ball throw 17.29** XX S. Broad jump 11.96** .40 6. 60 yard shuttle run 5.93* .28 7. Situp 0.77 -.41 8. Sidestep 2.73 -.80 9. Flexed arm hang 6.63** .22 (Wilks' Lambda = .2483 pfi.00001) Grade Main Effect (Wilks' Lambda = .7811 p§.0001) 1. Soccer dribble 33.93** -.18 -.70 2. Basketball dribble 79.77** .40 .30 3. Softball repeated throws 80.78** .41 .38 4. Soccer ball throw 52.17** .20 .59 5. Broad jump 36.08** .07 .08 6. 60 yard shuttle run 30.49** -.07 .70 7. Situp 28.24** .22 -.36 8. Sidestep 27.67** .28 -.27 9. Flexed arm hang 1.29 -.23 .39 *p_< .05. *fp < .01. XX 2 item not included in the discriminant function equation 73 Table 3 Means and Standard Deviations for Main Effects Participation. Gender, and Grade Level on the Motor Abilities/Sport Skills Battery Items Dependent Variable W Participggts Nonparticipants 14. fl 1‘. .52 1. Soccer dribble 7.52 2.30 5.59 1.80 2. Basketball dribble 16.59 6.15 12.24 5.75 3. Softball throw 10.88 3.77 8.45 4.04 4. Soccer ball throw 196.98 74.61 156.06 64.11 S. Broad jump 47.38 9.68 43.75 8.92 6. 60 yard shuttle run 18.00 1.72 18.82 1.93 7. Situp 14.42 5.30 13.01 4.81 8. Sidestep 15.15 3.35 14.34 3.13 9. Flexed arm hang 10.03 7.78 7.37 6.38 Gender Males Females 9. a M 5.9 1. Soccer dribble 7.41 2.18 5.67 2.02 2. Basketball dribble 16.32 6.38 12.41 5.64 3. Softball throw 10.78 3.91 8.51 3.97 4. Soccer ball throw 192.18 75.82 159.88 64.89 S. Broad jump 47.44 9.43 43 60 9.12 6. 60 yard shuttle run 18.14 1.76 18.70 1.95 7. Situp 14.10 5.25 13.29 4.91 8. Sidestep 14.58 3.44 14.90 3.07 9. Flexed arm hang 10.04 7.25 7.29 6.92 Grade 9. 9 .2. 9. 5.9 9. 5.9 M 59 1. Soccer dribble 7.94 2.22 7.86 2 01 6.75 1.54 2. Basketball dribble 20.56 3.62 18.18 4 76 14.43 4.30 3. Softball throw 13.75 2.76 12.32 3 12 9.66 2 44 4. Soccer ball throw 251.35 66.56 212.32 64.87 173.24 43.04 S. Broad jump 52.82 7.72 51.59 7.86 44.61 6.28 6. 60 yard shuttle run 17.47 1.13 17.06 1 30 18.14 1.50 7. Situp 16.80 3.98 16.80 3 79 14.66 3.32 8. Sidestep 16.95 2.58 16.27 2 53 15.41 2.31 9. Flexed arm hang 10.65 9.49 9.24 6 97 9.36 6.81 .1. 9 M 59 9. 5.9 1. Soccer dribble 5.81 1.94 4.31 1.29 2. Basketball dribble 11.47 4.84 7.19 3.21 3. Softball throw 7.12 2.45 5.32 2.40 4. Soccer ball throw 135.04 37.70 108.19 32.70 S. Broad jump 41.83 6.62 36.16 8.03 6. 60 yard shuttle run 19.26 1.76 20.20 1.65 7. Situp 10.82 5.12 9.40 4.03 8. Sidestep 13.62 2.64 11.44 2.88 9. Flexed arm hang 7.13 5.41 6.94 6.29 74 groups (Klecka, 1975). The first step involves choosing one variable which has the highest value on the selection criterion. At each subsequent step the variable added to the equation is the one which will yield the best criterion score (maximize the statistical difference between the groups) given the variable(s) already in the equation. Of the dependent variables included in these analyses, the soccer ball dribble, basketball dribble, and softball repeated throw items were highly correlated (Table 4). The fourth sport skill item, soccer throw, also demonstrated a moderate correlation to two of those three items. Therefore, when the strongest of these items was entered into the discriminant equation little of the remaining variance could be accounted for by these related variables. Post hoc univariate E_tests for the main effect gender showed a significant difference between males and females for seven of the nine items (Table 2). ‘These included all four sport skill items (soccer ball dribble, basketball dribble, softball repeated throws, and soccer ball throw) and three of the five motor ability items (broad jump, 60 yard shuttle run, and flexed arm hang). Examination of mean values indicated that males performed better than females on all of these items. The discriminant function analysis also identified seven of the nine items as significantly related to discriminating between the groups (Wilks' Lambda 2 .7352, p_< .0001). Specific items varied somewhat from the univariate results. 'Two of the sport skill items were not included in the discriminant equation while the two motor 75 om. amummwflm mm. Om. asufim mm.l Hm. om.| mauuszm coma 00 mm. mm. mo. qo.l gash cocoa mm. «m. om. mm.1 00. zomnu Hams pmuuom om. mm. on. oc.u 0“. mm. mzonnu cmummamu Hamnumom mm. cm. 00. 50.: 00. mm. ow. mannwuc Hamnumxmmm mm. we. mm. No.u on. me. cm. on. massage umuuom mam: aoummcfim asuwm manusnm neon zousu mzomnu massage sum new» vmomm Hana commoqmu Hamnumxmmm cmxmam oo umouom Hamnumom >mouumm acmEmmmmm< mHwam uuoam\mmfiuwawn< nouoz ecu :o a «Hams WEQUH How Xflhumz GOfiumHmHHOUHmu—CH 76 ability items which were not significant in the univariate analysis provided some discriminating power. In the discriminant equation the soccer dribble and sidestep items were weighted most heavily. Mean scores (Table 3) indicated that males performed better on the first of these items while the second one represents the only item on which females scored better than males. The five additional items reflected motor ability and sport skill items. Their coefficients were moderate to low for this equation and may be inspected in Table 2. Significant univariate Es were obtained for eight of the nine motor items for the main effect grade level (Table 2). {dean values for these items generally improved as grade level increased (Table 3). Subjects"performance scores on one item, the flexed.arm hang, did not differ significantly by grade level. The discriminant function analysis for grade level resulted in three statistically significant discriminant functions, at the .05 level of significance. However, the third function was very weak, with an associated Wilks' Lambda of .90. Additionally, the subsets of homogeneous grade levels produced by this function provided little meaningful information. Therefore, only the first two functions will be discussed further. The first discriminant equation was associated with a Wilks' Lambda of .2483 with p_:>:u< copes. 39.0 2. pzmmm 2mm; 5.. £50: .1. .8 2: $3 88 cacuz on ma ca .3 0— no - - . . . 23¢ math—.5 Oz<> co \ JIOK— <5. 52 (SPUODGS) awu Ind— 00 mm on n— 0— no . . p p - _ m<<3q O;a or: :23? 602023.: 8:2an3 *o _oooE roots: *0 «$556.6 out: 9350 3:30:29. 233.com .m 0.5mm oucoaanou .3285; ’ l oucoaanOU .oeau< Aoogmznoamo am.» *0: cozooccofl ’ I I 1., r cozofiozcom : 820262 103 competence and motivation becomes established, although at different rates for different children. The possibility that the direction of the relationship which may become established between perceived competence and participation motivation may be bidirectional should also be investigated. In older children perceived competence may affect choice to become involved in organized sport. But participation may also affect perceptions of competence. iFor example, being relegated to sitting on the bench or receiving primarily negative feedback from the coach may lower perceived competence. Perceptions of competence may increase for those who are stars on the team or who receive primarily positive feedback from coaches and peers. Certainly, further research is warranted to establish such relationships, the nature of their cause and effect status, and the timing of such events. The need for a longitudinal approach is evident. In conclusion, this research effort may be considered exploratory in nature. Until recently, little effort had been made to theorize regarding the developmental relationships among the variables investigated here. Research involving young children within the physical domain is limited and usually was indirectly related to the major research questions being tested. The psychological characteristics and motor abilities children bring with them to situations which involve motor skill instruction are important. Each affects how the teacher or coach should structure the session. (Ruldren who want to learn and expect to enjoy the situation may require the instructor to attend minimally to motivational 104 techniques, allowing greater emphasis on development of skill. Conversely, children who expect to have little success and to find little pleasure in the situation may require the teacher to place greater emphasis on changing perceptions. Knowledge of childrenks levels of motor skill provides information the instructor can use to prescribe successful and challenging experiences. Learning experiences need to be appropriately matched to childrenfis levels of skill to maximize improvement. Understanding how children's psychological characteristics and motor abilities change, which may be changed by means of specific treatments, and how each may affect the subsequent use of skills learned in a movement setting have theoretical as well as practical importance. Such information would further our understanding of development in general and the interrelationships among various domains. In applied situations this information could influence the selection of instructional goals for children at various developmental levels and the expectations of what may be reasonable improvement. Specific learning tasks may also be prescribed more appropriately. At some levels of development change may be facilitated more easily in one domain than another. .At other levels we may provide for the development of skills in more than one domain via tasks that are mutually beneficial. Continued research investigating these interrelationships seems warranted. Many questions are still to be answered, and more to be raised, particularly from a developmental approach. APPENDICES APPENDIX A PILOT STUDY 105 PILOT STUDY The items in the Assessment Battery for Motor Abilities and Sport Skills were chosen to represent general motor ability in the investigation of the relationships among actual motor competence, perceived motor competence, and participation motivation. ‘The literature clearly suggests that a general attribute of motor ability does not exist, per se (Singer, 1975; Sage, 1977). Therefore, it was deemed necessary to measure a number of tasks representative of several more specific motor abilities. Specific motor abilities which have been identified by factor analysis studies involving children include: speed; power; agility; eye-hand, eye-foot, and general body coordination; flexibility; and balance (Rarick & Dobbins, 1975; Peterson, Reuschlein, & Seefeldt, 1974). Items such as the 30 to 50 yard dash, broad jump, shuttle run, etc., have commonly been used to represent these factors (Smoll, Schutz, & Keeney, 1976; Glassow, Halverson, & Rarick, 1965). Safrit (1973) noted that a problem communi to many motor ability tests used to judge motor performance is that they do not relate well to actual sport and game situations. Kirkendall, Gruber, and Johnson (1980) noted that although individual items (measuring speed, agility, etc) are not very predictive of demonstrated skill in sports, when tested in ways more similar to game situations, correlations increase substantially. Many single tasks may be judged as measuring more than one component (e49, basketball dribble test measures a skill specific to a sport as well as eye-hand coordination; soccer ball dribble test measures a skill specific to a sport, agility, and eye—foot coordination). A battery composed of 106 items representing motor ability factors and sport skills was therefore developed to be representative of many, though certainly not all, components of general motor ability. The items included in the preliminary assessment battery were chosen with six additional criteria in mind. The purpose of the pilot study was to determine how well each item could meet four of the six criteria. There were as follows. Each item had to: 1. be appropriate for the age range of 5 to 10 years (appropriate to children's cognitive as well as motor abilities), 2. be reliable, 3. be valid, and 4. in conjunction with other items in the assessment battery, not require a total administration time in excess of 30 minutes. The nine items included in the preliminary test battery were: 1. standing broad jump 2. flexed arm hang 3. side-step test 4. sixty yard shuttle run 5. sit-up test 6. basketball dribble test 7. soccer ball dribble test 8. soccer ball throw test 9. softball repeated throw test Methods: Subjects: A total of 88 females and 106 males were tested on all nine items of the test battery. All were volunteers from an elementary 107 school in Carterville, Illinois, enrolled in grades kindergarten through four. Ages ranged from a mean from kindergarten subjects of 67.5 months to a mean of 119.5 months for fourth grade level children. Procedures: During the months of December and January (1983-1984 school year) children were tested on the battery items as part of their physical education classes. Each day two stations were set up in the gymnasium. The physical education specialist assisted in the testing by administering one item while this investigator administered the other (NHL Upon entering the gym children were asked to remove their shoes and socks and to report to the station at which they were to begin testing that day. Assignment to stations was random and changed daily. After being tested in one station the student reported to the other station for a second test. .All physical education classes met two to three times per week for 30 minutes each class period. Testing for the pilot study was accomplished for each grade level over a period of approximately 2 l/2 weeks. Ten first grade level children who were members of a class not included in the total pilot study were chosen randomly as subjects in an examination of the test—retest reliability of the items. ‘These children were taken in pairs to the gymnasium during free time and tested on all items by this investigator. The order of presentation of the items was counterbalanced and randomized for each pair of subjects. Subsequent to initial testing a retest was administered one week later. In addition to the actual testing, all subjects were given a rank order score based on their general level of motor performance as 108 demonstrated in their school physical education class. Rankings were generated within each grade level by the childrenhs physical education specialist. Results: The results of the pilot study will be addressed as they related to compliance of the test items with the criteria noted previously. All data referred to are included in Appendix C. 1. Appropriateness of items across age range of 5 to 10 years a. Cognitive requirements: For the sit-up test, directions appeared to be somewhat difficult for kindergarten level subjects to comprehend. Some children did not seem to understand the concept of doing as many sit-ups as possible in 60 seconds with a rest within that period of time, as needed. Protocol for this item was adjusted, as noted below in section 1c. None of the other items appeared to be too difficult for the youngest children to understand. In addition to verbal directions given, all tasks were demonstrated as well for the kindergarten and lst grade level children. Demonstrations were supplemental for children in grades 2 through 4 as deemed necessary. 1% Physical requirements: Inspection of the means and standard deviations for each item across the 5 grade levels suggested an appropriate age related increase for all items. Idinimum and.maximum values obtained suggested the lack of a floor or ceiling effect for most items. For one item, the flexed arm hang, several subjects at each grade level were unable to maintain the starting position, and mean values were low. The possiblility that this occurrence was sample specific was suggested when these scores were compared to scores of 109 children the same age who were part of a larger group to whom the same test was administered. Since mean values and standard deviations were acceptable, this item was maintained. Examination of scores for this item within the major study suggested that the low values obtained in the pilot study may have been specific to that sample. c. Protocol: Throughout the pilot study several aspects of administration and scoring were examined. For the ball dribble (with hands) three different balls were tried; a junior basketball, fully inflated 9 inch playground ball, and a semi-deflated 9 inch playground ball. The 9 inch playground ball (inflated to a pressure of 2 1/2 lbs) provided most control and did not hurt the hands of young children while still offering a challenge for older children. The sit-up test allowed children to perform as many sit-ups as possible within 60 seconds. However, two scores were recorded, (a) number done before a rest was taken, if taken and (b) total number done in 60 seconds. 'This was done to examine a possible recovery factor which may interfere with the strength and endurance factors desired. A significant difference was not found between the two, although some children did stop, rest, and then continue. Additionally, some younger children did not seem to understand the concept of doing as many sit- ups as possible within 60 seconds with a rest within the test as needed. Therefore to alleviate the possibility of confounding the recovery effect with strength and endurance and to eliminate confusion, a 30 second sit-up test was used to replace the 60 second test. When test-retest reliabilities were obtained, that for the 60 yard shuttle was relatively low, .57. Since the distance covered was 110 judged as being short enough to be considered more of a speed than an endurance measure, a second trial was added (as is typically done in 30 and 50 yard dashes) to increase this value. The protocol for the side-step test indicated penalties for cross- over steps. During the pilot study some younger children, although demonstrating appropriate use of sliding steps during practice, sometimes reverted to galloping, particularly to one of the two directions. Therefore, verbal reminders were given when such movements occurred and lines crossed with such movements were not be counted. One change was made in the scoring of the soccer ball dribble test to increase objectivity. Pilot procedures called for awarding a point for each cone dribbled past, as in the basketball dribble. However, younger children sometimes skipped cones due to a lack of control, which made judgments regarding awarding points difficult. Scoring procedures were therefore adjusted to award a point each time the ball crossed an imaginary line formed by any two adjacent cones in the row of cones. Procedures for all items not specifically addressed above were maintained as originally formulated. Protocol for all items may be found in Appendix B. 2. Test-retest reliability Scores for five male and five female first grade level children resulted in generally moderate to high Pearson product correlation values. The range was from .57 to .95 with only two scores below .79. 111 The average across all nine items was XXL From this information it was judged that the items chosen demonstrated sufficient reliability. 3. Validity The validity of this battery of items to provide objective values for motor abilities and sport skills was examined by comparing the scores that subjects attained on the test items to their rank order score within their grade level. Rank order scores were converted to percentiles and then to Z scores and used as the dependent measure in a series of multiple regression analyses. These analyses suggested that at each grade level some combination of items was able to account for a significant amount of the variance in rank order scores. The average R2 a .664, while individual grade level values were .56, .66, .77, .59, and .75 for Grades K, 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively. Discriminant function analyses were also performed on these data. Subjects} 2 scores were used to classify children into one of three categories, top, middle, or bottom one-third of their class relative to overall motor performance. Classification (Group 1, 2, or 3) was the criterion variable, while the nine test items were the predictors in the model. One significant (p_ starting line Description: Two cones are placed so their outer edges are 15 yards apart. A starting line is placed on the floor, even with the outer edge of one cone. Subject runs two laps around the cones. Runner is asked to keep turns close to but not touching markers. Scoring: Time is recorded to the nearest 1/10th of a second. Two trials are given; subjectds score is the mean of both trials. 5. Basketball Dribble Test Equipment: 9 inch red playground ball inflated to 2 1/2 pounds of pressure, six cones, masking tape, stOpwatch A 8' A 8' A 8' A 8' é 8' A 5' <--suba‘ect A? 3/ III \/ fl. _——-— begins here 123 Description: Six cones are placed in a straight line, first one is 5 feet from the starting line, rest are 8 feet apart. On "go" subject dribbles with either hand in and out of cones, continuing for 30 seconds. (Cones are placed in a line parallel to and 6 feet from a wall. Tester moves in a line parallel to and 6 feet from the subject as subject dribbles through the course, on the side of the cones which is opposite to the wall. Tester insures that a loose ball does not get farther away from the subject than 6 feet, by stopping it.) Scoring: One point is awarded for each cone passed in 30 seconds. A cone is considered passed when the subjectds body is even with it. Two trials are given, with score being the mean of both trials. Soccer-ball Dribble Test Equipment: One semi-deflated indoor soccer ball (size five, inflated to 4 pounds of pressure), five cones, masking tape, stopwatch Description: Five cones are placed in a straight line, 10 feet apart with the first one 5 feet from starting line. on "go" subject dribbles soccer ball with feet in and out of the cones for 30 seconds. As noted above for the basketball dribble, cones are placed 6 feet from a wall, and tester runs parallel and along the side of the subject to keep "loose" balls from getting too far away. Scoring: Subject receives one point each time the ball passes between two cones in 30 seconds. 'Three trials are given, and subject's _s___core is the mean for all three trials. A 10' Q 10' & 10'/&\10' A 5' .. <—-subject begins here 124 7. Softball Repeated Throws Test Equipment: Two indoor softballs, masking tape for wall target and throwing area, table turned on its side for a backstOp target F\\\\\‘\\\\\\\ backstOp I throwing area ~‘\\\\‘\J 15 feet ‘\\\\ 9 feet Description: a. Target area is outlined with masking tape on the wall as shown (10 feet X 5 1/2 feet). IL Throwing area is marked on floor, 9 feet from the target (5 1/2 feet X 5 1/2 feet). c.‘Table is placed on its side, 15 feet from the throwing area to serve as a backstop. d. Child stands in throwing area with softball in hand. On "go" child throws ball overhand at target, attempts to catch rebound in air or after bouncing, repeating as often as possible in 30 seconds. Child may leave throwing area to catch but must return to execute throws. .All throws must hit within target to score a point. A practice of 10 seconds preceeds two 30 second trials. Scoring: One point is scored for each throw in which the ball hits within the target boundaries. Subjectds score is the mean of two trials. 8. Soccer Throw—in for Distance Equipment: one size five soccer ball, tape measure, masking tape Description: From behind the restraining line, subject utilizes a two-handed overhead throw for distance» One step is taken prior to throwing, but restraining line may not be stepped on or over until after the ball is released. Scoring: Two practice trials, followed by three test trials are given. Distance is measured from a line perpendicular to the restraining line, to the nearest inch. Subjectds score is the mean of the three test trials. 125 9. Sit-up Test Equipment: mat, stopwatch Description: To assume the starting position, the subjects lie on their backs with knees flexed, feet on the mat approximately 12 to 15 inches from the buttocks. Hands are clasped behind the head. Feet are held by a partner to keep them in constant contact with the mat. The subject, by tightening his or her abdominal muscles, curls to a sitting position, touching both elbows to knees. Chin should remain tucked into chest. One sit-up is completed when the subject returns to the original starting position (midback must make contact with the mat). Testing begins when tester says "go". Scoring: One 30 second trial is given. Subjectfs score is the total number of sit—ups completed within that time period. 126 .H m. NH as m. me NH HE M. me me He m. «a He amok Bough sash songs soummowm umooom cmoum Hamnumom _ a z .N _ m m, «d w. my we as as as ms ma H8 m. up He osmz manafiun maaziau< was «Hosanm «Hanson to umuoom emxmam -usm as as magnumxmmm mum: < Enos Hmmmmmmoom I wmmeh