COMPOSITION TECHNEQUES EMPLOYED IN THE FERST MOVEMENT 0F ELLfiOTT CARTER’S FIANO CONCERTO Mfg R}? the Degree of Ph. D. MICHGAN STATE UMVERSITY ERENE ROSENBERG GRAU 1973 “1.0T GAN This is to certify that the II II IIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIII IH3 |129|3 10683 thesis entitled “min-JV»? ' t BA .. ‘- its University Compositional Techniques Ehployod in the First Movement of Elliott Carter's Piano Concerto Irene Rosenberg Gram has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. presented by Date May 11’ 1973 0-7639 7%«z/zm Major professor I‘d/W9; .I'III‘Ig.J 01 $7140 HAY 13 Mill ABSTRACT COMPOSITIONAL TECHNIQUES EMPLOYED IN THE FIRST MOVEMENT OF ELLIOTT CARTER'S PIANO CONCERTO BY Irene Rosenberg Grau The first performance, recording, and publication of Elliott Carter's Piano Concerto aroused keen interest. This work, Carter's first composition in this form, was two years in creation. Despite much discussion in the press, however, a published technical analysis has not yet appeared. The present study investigates the compositional aspects of the first movement of the two-movement concerto. The findings indicate in a generalized way the creative impulse which prevails, however, throughout the entire work. An analysis in depth has been made of form, melody, harmony, den- sity, dynamics, rhythmic activity and rhythmic character, tempo, meter, and metrical modulation, and interpretation and articulation markings. These aspects offer a comprehensive frame of investigation, and although in this work they are treated by the composer in highly original and innovative Irene Rosenberg Grau ways they proved open to study by conventional analytic techniques, expanded and modified as the situation demanded. The first, and perhaps most significant point of the present study, is that a systematic and objective theoretical analysis must be combined with an attempt to View the philosophy of the composer as it is expressed in this composition. Carter's apparent purpose in the E1329 Concerto is the creation of an abstract form which experi- ments with time factors and with structural procedures. The development of form in this work is removed from traditional structural considerations of departure and return, repetition, reconciliation of antitheses in thematic material or tonality. The form grows out of the principle of continuous motivic metamorphosis, in this work of motives comprised of twelve different three-note combinations. Carter makes use of the transformation of time elements through frequent change of meter and tempo, notated as metrical modulation, a technique in which a designated note value retains a constant duration although it changes in its significance within the metrical pattern. Combined with extreme fluidity of tempo and meter is a prevailing use of fragmented, dense, and polyrhythmic patterns. The motivic three-note combinations which Carter creates for this composition are non-tertian and give rise Irene Rosenberg Grau to chordal sonorities which are predominantly quartal, secondal, or clusters. When tertian or expanded tertian chords appear it is usually in a polychordal setting. Both tertian and non-tertian sonorities are used, juxta- posed and superimposed. Figures arising from the kind of harmony employed permeate the melodic structure.' The voice lines are often plural and are usually polyphonically complex. The melodic ambitus is extremely wide. The melodic design is immensely varied in contour and in intervallic structure. Melody, harmony, and rhythm do not coordinate to create phrase structure, cadences, or sectional conclusions. Density and volume favor high levels, although both fluctu- ate widely and frequently. They, too, are disassociated from structural considerations. The independence of each of theSe compositional aspects, in combination with the techniques of motivic evolution and metrical modulation, tends to give the work a continual forward thrust which makes of the composition's total 348 measures an extra- ordinarily large one-part form. It is theorized that the aural challenge in each of these compositional aspects mabee intended to express an aesthetic of extreme concentration and intense emotion. Carter's use of emotive expression markings can be con- strued as a further indication of his concern for the communication of musical character. Irene Rosenberg Grau The first movement of the Piano Concerto is traditional in that it is non-electronic, non-aleatory, and conventionally notated in equal temperament. It can perhaps be called futuristic in its innovative compositional technique and above all, in its philosophy. COMPOSITIONAL TECHNIQUES EMPLOYED IN THE FIRST MOVEMENT OF ELLIOTT CARTER'S PIANO CONCERTO BY Irene Rosenberg Grau A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Music 1973 ws' ACKNOWLEDGMENTS GR My warm thanks go to the faculty of the Music Department of Michigan State University and especially to the members of my dissertation committee: Dr. Paul Harder, Chairman; Dr. Russell Friedewald; Mr. Richard Klausli; and Dr. Theodore Johnson. I wish to eXpress my particular gratitude to Dr. Paul Harder for his generous counselling and encouragement. His advice in matters of content, procedure, and style has been invaluable. Acknowledgments are made to Associated Music, Inc. for permission to include within my text their publication of the Piano Concerto by Elliott Carter. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . . . . . . . . . . ii PREFACE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv Chapter I. PURPOSE AND METHOD . . . . . . . . . . 1 II. FORM . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 III. HARMONY . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 IV. MELODY AND COUNTERPOINT . . . . . . . . 55 V. TEMPO, METER, AND METRICAL MODULATION . . . 76 VI. DYNAMIC MARKINGS . . . . . . . . . . 90 VII. DENSITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99 VIII. RHYTHMIC ACTIVITY AND RHYTHMIC CHARACTER A . . 106 IX. EXPRESSION AND ARTICULATION MARKINGS . . . . 119 x. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . 125 BIBLIOGRAPHY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 139 APPENDICES I. Graph of the Occurrence of Events . . . . . 142 II. Graph of Tempo and Meter Change . . . . . 144 III. Graph of Appearances of Eighth Note = 252 . . 155 IV. Classification of Volume Level and Volume Variation . . . . . . . . . . 157 V. Graph of Density . . . . . . . . . 159 VI. Graph of Rhythmic Activity . . . . . . . 165 VII. Graph of Rhythmic Character . . . . . . . 168 VIII. First Movement of Elliott Carter's ' Piano Concerto . . . . . . . . . . 170 iii and 1965. quarter of a century of fruitful creative work. PREFACE Eliott Carter wrote his Piano Concerto in 1964 Preceding this composition lay more than a Carter's published compositions prior to the Piano Concerto include: 1961 Double Concerto for Harpsichord and Piano with two Chamber Orchestras 1957 String Quartet #2 1956 Canonic Suite for four B Flat Clarinets 1955 Variations for Orchestra 1952 Elegy for String Orchestra 1952 Sonata for Flute, Oboe, Cello, and Harpsichord 1951 String Quartet #1 1950 Eight Etudes and a Fantasy for Woodwind Quartet 1950 Recitative and Improvisation for four Kettledrums 1948 Sonata for Violoncello and Piano 1948 Woodwind Quintet 1947 The Minotaur, Ballet and Suite from the Ballet 1947 Emblems (for Men's Chorus and Piano) 1946 Elegy for String Quartet 1946 Piano Sonata 1945 Musicians Wrestle Everywhere (SATB a cappella, or with string acc.) 1944 The Harmony of Morning (4 part Women's Chorus and Small Orch.) ' 1944 Holiday Overture (revised 1961) 1943 Dust of Snow (Medium Voice and Piano) 1943 Elegy for Viola (or Cello) and Piano 1943 The Line Gang (Baritone and Piano) 1943 Voyage (Medium Voice and Piano) 1943 Warble for Lilac Time (Soprano, or Tenor, and Piano or small group of instruments) 1942 Symphony #1 1942 The Rose Family (Medium Voice and Piano) 1941 The Defense of Corinth (Speaker, Men's Chorus, and Piano 4 hands) 1940 Pastorale for Viola (Eng. Horn or Clarinet) and Piano iv 1939 Canonic Suite for Quartet of Alto Saxophones 1939 Pocahontas, Ballet and Suite from the Ballet 1938 Heart No So Heavy As Mine (SATB a cappella) 1938 Incidental Music to "The Merchant of Venice" (guitar and alto voice) 1938 Prelude, Fanfare, and Polka for Orchestra 1937 Harvest Home (SATB a cappella) ‘ 1937 Let's be Gay (Women's Chorus and Piano) 1937 To Music (SATB a cappella) 1936 Incidental Music to "Mostellaria" (Baritone, Men's Chorus, & Small Orchestra) 1936 Tarantella (Men's Chorus and Piano 4 hands) 1933 Incidental Music for "Philoctetes" of Sophocles (Baritone, Men's Chorus, Oboe, and Percussion) Although there are works for large orchestra, Carter's attention seems to have been drawn particularly to smaller ensembles and to chamber music. He uses piano as a solo instrument only once; and piano in combination with orchestra, prior to the present concerto, only in the Double Concerto for Harpsichord, Piano and Two Chamber Orchestras. The Piano Concerto is a new line of thought for Carter in terms of instrumental sound, but upon review of his total output to date it becomes apparent that each of his works expresses an interest in a fresh kind of instrumental circumstance. A free line of inquiry into many aspects of musical composition is the hallmark of Carter's creation. As he himself notes: I began to question the familiar methods of presentation and continuation, of so-called "musical logic," based on the statement of themes and their development. . . . Too, I questioned the inner shape of "this" and "that" --of musical ideas-~as well as their degrees of linking or non-linking. .'. . I was pre-occupied with the time-memory patterns of music. The philosophic openness and intellectual strength which characterize Carter's career reach new expression in the Piano Concerto. The Piano Concerto was commissioned by the Ford Foundation. The selection of the composer to be commis- sioned on this occasion was given to pianist Jacob Lateiner, who turned to Elliott Carter. The first performances of the Concerto were given by Lateiner and the Boston Symphony, directed by Erich Leinsdorf, on January 6th and 7th, 1967, in Symphony Hall, Boston. It was recorded at these per- formances by RCA Victor (LSC-3001), with the joint subsidy of the Martha Baird Rockefeller Fund for Music, the Steinway Foundation, and the National Foundation for the Arts and Humanities. Although this event was attended by much publicity and was widely discussed in the press, a published technical examination of the compositional techniques employed in the Concerto has not yet appeared. This lack is curious in view of the composition's singularity. Both the Concerto itself and the apparent difficulty of approaching it represent tantalizing challenges. Hence, the present study. 1Elliott Carter, Jacket notes to Nonesuch Recording 'H-7134, of Carter's Sonata for Flute, Oboe, Cello, and Harpsichord, and Carter's Sonata for Cello and Piano. vi CHAPTER I PURPOSE AND METHOD The philosophy underlying this paper is that a work of art ideally should be understood on its own terms, as part of its culture, and within an historical perspec- tive. The sad gap which exists at present between the contemporary composer and the contemporary listener indicates that in all three regards understanding is weak. The personal, social, or historical raison d'étre of contemporary style and content fails to link many musical creators today to the general public. ‘Contemporary "art" music may fairly be reported to be considered widely as enigmatic, isolated, and irrelevant. It mabee suspectedthat the experimental or avant-garde composer today is misunderstood because his work lies in advance of the understanding of his time. Hopefully, our understanding of the musical trends leading into the future is being aided by the development of a methodology of appreciation, of a systematic technique of analysis, built upon the past but suited to the new material. Proceeding from this premise, the present study involves two main considerations. One is to examine a l significant new work, the Piano Concerto, by the highly esteemed American composer Elliott Carter. The second is to evaluate the applicability to this composition of the analytic techniques which have been useful up to the present, and to attempt to devise extensions of these techniques which may be particularly suitable to music of our own day. The Piano Concerto has two movements, the first consisting of 348 measures and the second of 330. Both movements employ full orchestra of 2 Flutes (Piccolos) 4 F Horns 2 Oboes ‘ 3 B-flat Trumpets 2 B-flat Clarinets 3 Trombones 2 Bassoons Tuba Contrabassoon Timpani (2 players) Strings (minimum: 7-6-5-4-3 stands) There is also a concertino group of Flute, English Horn, Bass Clarinet, Violin, Viola, Cello, and Bass. The concertino group sometimes plays alone, sometimes with the solo piano, and sometimes merges into the larger ensemble. In the second movement of the Concerto, the con- certino conducts an extensive dialogue with the solo piano; there is a dramatic confrontation of distinct musical personalities. In the first movement there is no comparable theatrical device. However, in all other major aspects the two movements are very similar,and the compositional pro- cedures in regard to rhythm, harmony, density, thematic process, and formal structure appear to derive from the same creative impulse. Both movements exhibit a unified and highly distinctive aesthetic, as well as a composi- tional technique which is readily identifiable in all parts of the work. For this reason, it was felt that the present study could deal specifically and in depth with an examina- tion of the first movement and still could be understood to validly represent in a more generalized sense a way of viewing the compositional procedures used in the second movement. The modus Operandum is to make as comprehensive a list as possible of those aspects of musical composition whose study has proven fruitful in enriching our under- standing of the music of earlier periods. This list will serve as a kind of cultural definition. Each of these aspects will then be investigated in connection with the first movement of the Concerto. In view of the complexity of the material, a minute and systematic categorization of data will be undertaken. All the information derived from the various avenues of study will be included in this report. Some information may, significantly, be of a negative character. It may appear, for example, in the early stages of various lines of inquiry, that the expected results will not be forthcoming; in some cases it may be that no conclusion can be drawn. However, all the relevant data will be assembled and reviewed in the hope of permitting the information to express its own meaning without suffering from the bias of preconception. It will then remain to be seen whether these lines of inquiry have succeeded in providing a viable analytic framework for the music analyzed, and possibly for other contemporary works of similar characteristics and prOperties. The musical aspects investigated include: Formal structure Sectional organization Phrases Delineation of small units Motives Motivic uses Developmental techniques Tonality Key relationships Harmonic function of the bass Chord progression Cadences Functional harmony Chromatic harmony Impressionism Concepts of Consonance and dissonance Chord structure Non-harmonic tones Twelve-tone technique Contrapuntal devices Utilization of pitch area Melodic structure Melodic recurrence Melodic intervallic relationships Density Rhythmic intensity Types of rhythmic motion Tempo Meter Metrical modulation Dynamics Interpretation directions Articulation Repeats Bridges and transitions Interconnectedness of compositional aspects Use of folk material These lines of inquiry, with their concomitant offshoots, will be grouped under the chapter headings of: Form Harmony Melody and Counterpoint Tempo, Meter and Metrical Modulation Dynamics Density Rhythmic Activity and Rhythmic Character Expression and Articulation Markings. CHAPTER I I FORM The first movement of Elliott Carter's Piano Concerto is a striking example of an extraordinarily large one—part form. Referring to his Variations for Orchestra, Carter said, "As in all my works, I conceived this one as 2 a large, unified musical action or gesture." This concept appears to be applicable to the Piano Concerto also. The question of the feasibility of extending the concept of unity through a composition of three hundred and forty-eight measures duration must be solved in terms of the special and inherent logic of the structure. In the Piano Concerto, First Movement, the structural logic does not depend upon the ordering of the structural parts in either classic or innovative ways, or upon comparisons of differences or reconciliation of antitheses. The logic depends rather upon an unfolding continuity of development which seems to make manifest a sub-stratum of emotive continuity. In the large one-part form the original tonal material presented at or near the beginning of the 2Elliott Carter, "Shop Talk by an American Composer," Problems of Modern Music (New York: W. W. Norton, 19655, p. 54. composition must be capable of providing for a great deal of manipulation. It is also necessary that the emotional tone of the composition as a whole be rich, varied, and_ withal coherent. Carter's own comments on his compositional pro- cedure are highly relevant. The characteristic effort of the serious composer, as I see it, is not so much in the invention of musical ideas in themselves, as in the invention of interesting ideas that will also fill certain compositional requirements and allow for imaginative continuations. Serious music appeals to a longer span of attention and to a more highly developed auditory memory than do the more popular kinds of music. In making this appeal, it uses many contrasts, coherences and contexts that give it a wide sc0pe of expression, great emotional power, and variety, direction, ' uniqueness, and a fascination of design with many shadings and qualities far beyond the range of popular or folk music. Every moment must count somehow, as must every detail. For a composer it is not always easy to find a passage that fits the particular situation and moment at which it appears in the composition, that carries to a further point some idea previously stated, that has had the apprOpriate expressive quality motivated by what has been heard and yet is a passage that sounds fresh and alive. As far as I am concerned, I am always inter- ested in a composer's phrases and their shape and content, the way he joins them, the type of articulation he uses, as well as the general drift or continuity of a large section, and the con- struction of the whole work. The small details _of harmony, rhythm, and texture fall naturally into place when one has interesting conceptions of these larger shapes.3 Carter's usage in the Piano Concerto of composi- tional faCtors such as dynamic markings, density, rhythmic 31bid. activity and rhythmic character, and Some aspects of tempo, meter, and metrical modulation, all indicate an awareness of the necessity of generating emotional tone as a structural principle in an expanded one-part form. In addition, these factors, although they do not delineate structure in a traditional sense, do combine with tonal elaboration and instrumentation to present a patterning of musical events which gives dimension to the form. The thrust of these musical events is usually in the direction of change of some kind. Following is a list, rather like a kind of relief map, representing some of the more important configuration changes, or, as Carter states it above, "general drift," with brief descriptions pointing out the salient characteristics of each area. 1. From the beginning to m. 18. The original thematic material of a perfect 4th, diminished 5th, minor 2nd, and diminished 8th, using the pitches G, F#-C, C#, is presented in a completely dodecaphonic matrix. (In this paper the term "dodecaphonic" will mean totally chromatic.) Piano solo, joined by the concertino, provides adistinctive orchestration, in contrast to the more usual employment of full orchestra. Meter is 3/4, quarter note 126.. (See page 10). 10 1 1.. h I . I E & II I I8 2 .E :25 29.6 A8! 1158: "$8 K To... 8.1.880 inhmfiwwzo "I | I 7.. 7I .1 . I . \ 1 a A n a r I CE. 88$ 0231.38 {i I null; 1. . + I 1151: 1: up {EW\wI\Hmnm I 1 a - .1/11/I.\.I\ 1.0th 22.3 «A...» who... ouxrnmxcmxaaea .3 3. 1. _. 839:0... of .3 cocoa—com otoucoU ccoE ll 2. M. 18-32. Here occur changes of meter and texture. The linear writing refocuses into voice leading in pairs. The orchestra enters. This area tends to be lyrical in the orchestral part and figural in the piano part. (Duration: 14 bars.) 12 3. M. 32-40. At this point there begins a series of melodic phrases in thematic entrances in the piano part and in clarinet and strings, with thinning of texture and lessening of rhythmic activity. (Duration: 8 bars.) M. 32 AM ”I 3P“ » AJ- 7 ”thw- M. M. 40-68. Significant here is the appearance of a rhythmic figure of triplet 16th notes which will be pervasive in this area of the work. The use of timpani makes this figure immediately recognizable as a compositional event. (Duration: 28 bars.) (See page 13.) l3 «my -'0 b0...- 14 r a. 1L 68-111. Rhythmic activity and texture diminish. The triplet 16th figure disappears for two measures. A new thought emerges: a motive in clusters with glissando. This area builds upon the accumulation of everything pre- ceding, and pushes forward for a longer period than any of the preceding areas. (Duration: 43 bars.) 6. M. 111-124. The meter and tempo change, activity lessens, concertino reappears, piano part dissolves. Sonorities of 3rds and 6ths can be perceived within the lZ-tone material. (Duration: 13 bars.) (See page 15.) Jul-183.75 L.1f===: _1. (ac1)/-—>. C 14... «If! 15 > A g l #— "f i 7P 1 All I I I A 'I7 :— _--.>_ I _""""""I re“: - - U l :1 < 2 f < -A 0;-.--.-_-. ‘E E n I l l u r 1 1 Tm) “TIT” 1:: ') ‘8 Wm- § «1, ' ' f 1: ’£\ & FAF. “Ef- f‘ In is o “a “‘5 33g... 1!?" (0. II.“ . "L—_‘ .4- P' as _ _ 14““ q . . 'i—I- ' -- ‘g’ :c.‘ :E-I 16 7. M. 124-152. The meter and tempo change. Oboe makes a prominent entrance with a subject consisting of melodic intervals of. 3rds and 6ths, followed by imitative counterpoint. (Duration: 28 bars.) M. 124 -‘- b 0—,LJIJsflO.25-o L)-J-tmu?&h ($3) m» U. .I-nozs» " Ob 8. M. 152-167 The distinctive feature here is the absence of piano for fifteen measures. (Duration: 15 bars.) (See page 17.) .1 M. 152 17 .J: 183.75 3.31 18 9. M. 167-188 The piano, abSent since m. 150, makes a vigorous re-entry. (Duration: 21 bars.) «he JAIN {—745 "I“! “- Oh-DJ.'70 10. M. 188-223 ‘ Volume, density, and rhythmic activity drop low. (Clusters, noted at m. 68, are again the main sonority. (Duration: 35 bars.) (See page 19.) l9 L ~ “I - N" 1 . .v r a: J 1%: .fl.“ Th. F _ nfl M F A I n _ u ~ ~ / m ~ ~ I. h ~ . \- h f- I. II v _ _ i a I. I: ~ / I\(\ 3.! 1’ a ~I NP it u in" ‘- 1 f if I. In -I _ I «— F < [ML 20 ll. Upbeat to m. 223-256 At this point, marked un poco espressivo, p decrescendo to RBI the piano, in a solo entrance, unfolds a series of 4ths. (Duration: 33 bars.) M. 222 21 12. Upbeat to m. 256-270 These orchestral entrances use material related to the firSt statement of the composition, the augmented 4th, minor 2nd, and diminished 8th. The original pitches C-F# are conspicuous in the cello entrance. (Duration: 14 bars.) M. 256 22 13. M. 276-313. The meter and tempo change. The concertino reappears in a series of imitative entrances, initiating the composition's propulsion to its conclusion. (Duration: 43 bars.) (See page 23.) 14. M. 313-328 A distinctive event here is the crashing chord which embraces the totality of pitches from sub-contra C to five-line b, an effect used only in this area of the composition. (Duration: 15 bars.) (See page 24.) 15. M. 328 to the conclusion This event is noteworthy as an extremely rare use of recall within the work. The original thematic material is re-used on the original pitches: G, G-flat (= F#)-C, D-flat (= C#). Further mnemonic reinforcement is offered by the original tempo, re-affirmed, at m. 329, at quarter note = 126. The psychological rounding off and sense of completion, caused by the return of characteristics of the I beginning, signal the approaching end. (Duration: 21 bars.) (See page 25.) 23 I. 25 2m .2 26 The landmarks included on the foregoing "map" are only the main and most apparent ones. Other parts of the landscape will of course be perceived and subjectively evaluated. The compositional landscape preSents a picture of extreme fluidity and organic self-determination, free from.arbitrary structuring through the imposition of historical mores or mathematically oriented aesthetics. The spacing of events in terms of the numbers of measures within each area is: 17, 14, 8, 28, 43, 13, 28, 15, 21, 35, 33, 14, 43, 15, 21. The spacing in terms of time- duration must surely be equally varied, since the tempo continually changes and the measure lengths are unequal. It is worth noting that the graph (see Appendix I) of this study of form-happenings reveals an absence of meaningful correlation with the graphs of other compo- sitional factors. The formal aSpect, like all the other aspects of this movement, is completely independent. The nature of the events within the work is also varied. In some cases only a brief effect is noted. In other instances a change in the on-going situation is perceived and different emphases will be apparent in the entire new area. I The study of the form brings out clearly this significant finding: ‘it does not seem possible to designate any of the areas of this composition by pre- established formal terms such as theme groups, transitions, 27 developments, recapitulations, or the like. Most of the time it is clear that, while some elements provide change, other, simultaneous, elements offer such strong lines of continuity that the elements of change do not bring about a separation of distinct parts from the structural whole. The factor which is probably strongest in pro- ducing unity is the tonal material itself, which is totally chromatic throughout and which makes use of tightly organized motivic interval patterning. The independent functioning of each compositional aspect, as will be demonstrated further along in this study, also provides unity through overall characterization. Finally, the fact that the evolution of thematic, rhythmic--indeed, all compositional aspects--is pervasive and continual is in itself a unifying principle. CHAPTER I I I HARMONY Harmony-may be understood in this Study to refer to three different concepts: (1) harmonic texture or chordal sonority, (2) cadential forms, and (3) harmony in relation to formal organization. The harmonic texture of the first movement of Elliott Carter's Piano Concerto includes a vast array of chordal sonorities. Some of these sonorities are pre- sented simultaneously; some occur as arpeggios or chordal figuration; some can becomprehended vertically or diagonally as chords accumulated by extension on the time continuum. Careful analysis indicates that some of the classic sonorities used in the common-practice period are included among Carter's chord types. But more prevalent are innovative sonorities resulting from the intervallic aggregations typical of dodecaphonic manipulation. As the variety of chordal sonorities arising in the Piano Concerto is so great, it may be well to define further the concept of "chord" as it will be used in this paper: the co-existence of three or more tones within a time span sufficiently compact so that their relationship 28 29 may be perceived. This definition permits chordal sonorities to be classified into eight types. triads (tertian) seventh chords (tertian) polychords, or juxtaposed tertian sonorities expanded tertian (9ths, 11ths, 13ths, lSths) tertian with added tones tertian with cross relations or split chord members . quartal-quintal . secondal-clusters CWU'IUbbJNH o cox! There is of courSe some overlapping of chord types and some variation in analysis does occur. It should be noted that two-note sonorities occur only rarely in this work. A study of the density aspect (see Appendix IV) reveals only seven measures which contain one to two simultaneous pitches, these arising only in the first third of the composition. Chord type I, the tertian triad, hardly exists as a complete, self-sufficient entity within this work. When tertian triads or seventh chord structures appear they almost always form a part of polychords. ICarter's technique in regard to polychords is highly original and can be directly linked to the very distinctive textures prevailing in this composition. The usage of polychords in the past has often relied upon wide, juxtaposed spacing. Separation of the constituent chords by pitch area has been an effective means by which independent chordal sonorities have been 30 held in aural counterpoise. Carter's use of polychords departs almost entirely from this traditional method. Most often his polychords are not distinctly juxtaposed but are crowded together in a dense and rapidly changing texture. The existence of tertian triads and seventh chords can be traced, but their power and distinctiveness as functioning entities are sacrificed in favor of a sub- ordination into a richer, more complex web of interrelation- ships. A further distinction is that traditional methods usually deal with clearly recognizable tertian structures, whereas, through the use of added tones and cross relations, as well as secondal and quartal sonorities, Carter's chords often lose the customary means of identifi- cation as polychords and merge into other categories. An interesting and typical example of the compli- cated use of polychords occurs in the orchestral score at m. 41: the first 16th of the measure is an e minor triad (C-flat = B), the third 16th is e minor plus a major seventh (diminished octave), the seventh 16th is E major plus a major seventh with F major implied on the upper staff. At the beginning of m. 42 there are three levels: C-flat major in the middle, A major arpeggio with a sustained B-flat on the tOp level, and F contra entering on the lowest level in the piano part at the second 8th. 31 32 Diminished triads are rejected. The augmented triad occurs occasionally, as at m. 328, last beat, where it appears in conjunction with the interval of a major third D-flat-F. M. 328 11.11". THMthI > . {SI/3 (£11; f )7 l'-"sz Seventh chords and expanded tertian sonorities can be found frequently, as in m. 36 and 37 (see page 33) and m. 231, 232, and 233 (page 34). 33 n .30.; 34 The tertian sonorities are never organized into successions which resemble functional harmony. The major-minor seventh chord (dominant seventh structure) occurs only rarely among the other types of _seventh chords, as in m. 105, where it appears fleetingly in a polychordal context (see page 35). Major-minor seVenth chords have neither the priority of occurrence which they enjoyed in the music of the past nor the predictability of usage found in earlier styles. Most often the interval of a minor seventh will be left empty, or will be placed upon an interval of a third, forming a ninth chord (both illustrated in m. 184, page 35). 35 V .J—z I: 73.5 ”f5 M. 1848'"1 fl' 36 If the interval of the minor seventh is filled, it is most likely to contain two fourths, as in m. 227. This is one of Carter's favorite sonorities in the , Piano Concerto, as illustrated in m. 3392340 (p. 37). The diminished seventh chord, like the diminished triad, is rejected. I Parallel chord streams and alternations between polarized harmonies, suggestive of impressionistic tech- niques, are also absent in this composition. Passages in which consonant harmony or consonant intervals are prominent_do sometimes contain streams within the texture, .for instance in m. 95 throughrm.104 where the lower score of the piano part is, with one exception, a series of twenty-four 6ths,while simultaneously the lower score of 37 2593 A11 Hi ‘hflI IfiIb 38 the orchestral part presents a series of fifths (see pp. 39-40). M. 156 through m. 165 shows two simultaneous independent lines, each of which is compoSed almost exclusively of the melodic and harmonic intervals of the 3rd and the 6th. The relationship between the two lines is, however, highly dissonant (see p. 41). There seems to be no concern in the presentation of any of the chords for such matters as root re-inforce- ment or sonorous doublings. Registration and voicing. over the pitCh range is immensely varied, as is shown in the study of melody (please see Chapter 3), but the guiding thought does not seem to be the play of sonorities per se. Tertian sonorities are most frequently blurred by the addition of non-chord tones, by cross relations at the chord root as in m. 111, first six notes, or m. 261, downbeat (see page 42), or by split chord members within the chord structure as in m. 134 (p. 42). IThe most prevalent sonorities within the 21332 Concerto are those which contain seconds, sevenths, or ninths, often in conjunction with perfect or augmented fourths.. The interval of the fourth is motivic and often .occurs as quartal harmony, in the form of interlocking 39 4...; a: as Ed ’6 13w. 9:? qu . n . la 5: seat a: a “ END. 2.: as. as 1. L a 5 \ 1 aIIHILau 11.... I I 1. a L: \I ur» w .111 I as. 1.1.11 IQI. rllwllu raw... ill-M _ VHIIQLDRHUMQMIH< (1\ .A no... )1 Ell/3A.. .Mox. . N “In as A A A I Au! m. A. 8 so“ Alfinv 40 iv“ an .lnm A .4 I h 3. HI. '1 IS. 4‘: I ‘5 hrI h H P 3 _1 Fan- A 7 I mart. ° ' :5nd ,1 fig 41 n. 156' ,_.I. . It..J-isa.n 111,1. «’84 J.-'m r71! -J.-1 42 '9» J'183.75 - IL. J-183.75 M. 134 43 fourths, m. 329, or m. 2 and 3, J=126 - '42.).fanfasflco M. 2 "3f superimposed fourths, m. 9, or m. 223, 44 or juxtaposed fourths, m. 334. T AH P #37 I IV Another favored sonority is clusters, as in m. 50, 68, 181, sometimes spread apart into sevenths or ninths, m. 72-74 (see page 45), ’N :N um & D— 1.!» 45 41: 1'“ W. C . in Euxgoxm _ _ _(1 icgm II Ell u _«IL 4‘ ”'31:? I 46 sometimes tightly bunched with a fourth as the outer parameter and a third contained, m. 193-194. gp 1 .1 1.. Clusters are sometimes used in expanded form as fistfuls of sound containingas many as all twelve tones, m. 245- 246 (see page 47). Carter's harmonic texture in this composition is immensely varied. There are brief areas where a particular sonority may prevail, for instance in m. 18 through m. 22, which is predOminantly quartal, or m. 188 through m. 194, predominantly clusters, or m. 106 through m. 110, totally chromatic. Usually, however,the harmonic vocabulary is displayed in such richness that classification of the area or even of a group of specific measures by chord type is impossible. This observation becomes increasingly valid Lee the composition progresses. An example of a measure 47 T. . _MM.11~mI./m\I/.-nw§ .r u F1 .1. IL \ ‘ — ‘- — a N a“ n L h n d.’ v . r v o HEN N C I“ a H n ’ a N uh . on. yr _- u! u A a RF : O a . *1 - 48 with a characteristic wealth of harmonic suggestion is m. 294. This measure is an extreme but typical example ‘ of the aesthetic of dodecaphonic saturation, containing elements which are atonal, polychordal, triadic and tertian, secondal and quartal. Secondal Quartal. 2f "P Quartal fl Tertian Triadic Elliott Carter wrote in the program notes to the first performance of the Piano Concerto: Technically the work is based on twelve dif- ferent three-note groups (triads):_ six, used exclusively by the piano and concertino and six, by the orchestra. Each triad is associated with one or more tempi and expressive characters. Musical ideas are formed out of constantly chang- ing uses of these fundamental materials. 49 At the outset, the piano and concertino state ideas derived from the triad shape exemplified by C, F sharp, G and the three speeds and characters associated with it. This is answered briefly by the orchestra using material from one of its triads (shape: G, D flat, F). Later these two triads, which are the primary ones for the orchestra and the piano, each join three others from their respective instrumental groups to form one twelve- note chord for the soloists and another of dif- ferent character for the orchestra, the alternation of which constitutes the conclusion of the first movement, summarizing in brief all the various expressive characters that have been brought into increasingly sharp focus and contrast.4 Nearly any Small section of this work will reveal the presence of the two basic three-note groups or their metamorphoses. In fact it is usually impossible to proceed beyond any fourteen to sixteen note sequence without having included therein all twelve tones. M. 1 to 5, or M. 138 to 139, or m. 223 to 224, or the single measures 235 or 302 all illustrate Carter's characteristic technique of accumulating twelve tones (see page 50). In view of this typical saturation it becomes highly debatable whether it can be perceived that the metamorphosis of these two basic groups into six assigned to piano and concertino and six assigned to orchestra, as outlined by Carter, does result in giving clarification to the texture or dimension to the tonal characterization. The interrelationship of the basic "triads" is so close and the means of presentation so dense that a lucid dif- ferentiation of usage does not seem to result. This 4Elliott Carter, "The Piano Concerto." Boston Symphon Program Notes (Boston, Mass.: The Boston Symphony, anuary , 67). 50 A VI. d r “A lhI no I . mo... .2. t to: m dunks. Plum... 1...: 11w L . . in 1 . 8 NA .1 he 1M. a it mull! .1I... l.]nflflfl.WMmWM 1. E 31.1.1.4... .. E IN: w. . . \I 1 . . .c w . 51 conclusion, however, in no way negates the fact that the concept of such differentiation may have been very significant in the creative process. It may be that the framework within which the composer creates and the framework within which the listener functions, although agreeing in the main, need not be congruent in all their parts. ' Cadential patterns do not appear to exist within the work. Functional harmony employing root progressions by fifths or fourths is totally absent, as are mediant progressions. The utilization of harmonic patterns in 'terms of root movement is completely discarded. On the contrary, roots, where they can be found to exist at all, are ignored in terms of chord weight. I Phrase organization is not generated directly by the harmony. 'It is true, of course, that harmonic usage is among those compositional factors in thistork which create increasing or lessening dynamism, but rather than phrases in the conventional sense there exist undulations within the texture, such as may be described as fluctua- tions within the formal scheme. These events are structured by tonal manipulations, variations in density, rhythmic activity, changes in rhythmic character, dynamics, expression markings, and orchestration. They flow very coherently into one another. There is no consistent use 52 of harmony to produce section endings. The conclusion of the entire work is brought about by a process of summation in which the expectation of concluding is partly engendered by the total duration. I The absence of clearly recognizable cadences results in a generally high level of tension throughout the work. The wish to relax and even to rest is consistently frustrated; instead the listener is challenged to expand his concentration span. Tension and extended concentration are part of the aesthetic of the composition. This aesthetic concept follows naturally from Carter's remarks, quoted in Chapter II, £25m, p. 8, about the distinguishing character of "serious music." The structural uses of harmony, organizing a large work and clarifying the framework, can be found in some instances Within the work; for example,in those areas mentioned above as having a recognizable type of chordal sonority for a perceivable duration. However, there is no master-network of harmonic relationships upon which the Structure of the entire movement is engineered, no utilization of relative positions within an harmonic scheme (either diatonic or chromatic) for sectional organization, no reference to an underlying focal point which recurs either as a tonal degree or as a pitch area. 53 There is a significant, albeit slight, tonal recall in m. 328, where the middle of the measure uses the pitches G-flat, C, and in m. 330 over the bar line to m. 331, where the original motivic pitches C, F-Sharp, C-sharp, are used again in prominent positions. These pitches also occur in a cello entrance at m. 256 downbeat and are sustained in a tremolo until m. 258 downbeat, where they are repeated as the beginning of a figural pattern. M. 256 54 [1. U) H“ Fl a“ (I) «‘1 ‘ CHAPTER IV MELODY AND COUNTERPOINT Fundamental issues emerge at once in the examination of melody. Can a definition of melody be formulated which will be valid for the entire history of music, up to and including the present? It is in fact the most basic definition of melody as motion and rhythm which is applicable to Carter's music. It may be that the best term to define contemporary usage of the emotive concept of melody would be simply this univerSal definition: "A linear succession of tones.“ The new melody or linear succession of tones may be con- stituted of single notes or of groupings of notes; beyond this nothing may be specified with validity. However it may be constructed, the essential quality, and the one which designates it as a legitimate expression of the melody concept, is that the constituents are not filler- tones or color-tones but sequential carriers of an expressive content. Viewed from this standpoint, even the most unusual of Carter's lines may be considered melody. 55 56 In the first movement of the Piano Concerto, Carter's lines tend to consist of a multiple strand, analogous to a bundle of fibers within a muscle sheath, sometimes bunched into a muscle knot, sometimes thin as a lean sinew. His "voices" are treated as orchestral or choral voices, that is, as aisection capable of divisi. Polyphony within one voice is common and an abundantly "freistimmig" texture prevails. The single line (monophony) occurs only twice in the entire composition, at m. 17 and at m. 130-133. Several voices are present almost all the time, resulting in a very complex poly— phonic web, within which each strand is woven with equality and independence. The homOphonic concept in which melody floats upon a chordal support never appears in this work. The technique used is simultaneous voice- leading of lines of varying thickness, range, activity, design, and interval selection, sometimes co-ordinating, sometimes differing. An important characteristic is the clouding of the melodic line through simultaneous major or minor seconds in the same voice, as in m. 70 (page 57). As classical melodies were often doubled in thirds or sixths, or medieval chant in fourths and fifths, so Carter's lines tend to be adorned by simultaneous 57 seconds. Other frequent doublings are p. 4ths, aug. 4ths, 7ths, and 9ths, as in m. 56-58 (page 58).' Some doublings in 3rds and 6ths do exist in this work, for example at m. 157-165 (page 59), usually however in a generally dissonant context. In places where clusters adorn the melodic line it is not possible to speak of melodic interval progression but rather of relative placement within the pitch-space and relative density in filling a pitch-space area, as in m. 65-71 (page 60). In areas of thick texture the lines are often a series of harmonies of fluctuating density, made up pre- dominantly of quartal chords with enclosed third, quartal chords with ambitus of a seventh, quartal and quintal inter- locked, or quintal with ambitus of a ninth, as in m. 170-175 (page 61). 58 59 r4. fL 2.. a 2... .5: “Fe F~w .. (I 4111 (1 [‘13 I-.- 1% 1 alum/1 11.. 1.7.. . ngmmu. (\me .3 (HIMwan - - u. :m =3”??? {if iififlflf .5“: '7: UV I it >w p 6O 61 . . HI \Illlu/ Sinus .. A H 1 I . 1‘. link“. \ .IIM n x a .\. A «(\\ c - /I\\ Q < (9 “l‘ (l‘ I" I use . a I4I _H_ A . an A . T “W H "II ¢ I I \/ IId I u 1 - . < {I A “(We . 2.1%. A I . _ b. m) .111 1. I - A In 1 1 1. _ 1.1 PMQ n53... fl IAJ 5.0 mm. uneI a\ .I 1 , [MATI I 1 . .m x. I . . . . V . . ... \- Mfie INA fllnlmv mail."wul . \i i l I I I 11'21 7. m I. .T II .infll Immiflww .1 4 HIIIII .I--I-. A am. 37 .aI A K .3. we. 1 . m 1 «affine a II NV. ”1.. ma. 1 . .1 .III II\ (N ( . . . . 855 c!\. A A A A. . w... , 1 n% 1.. 1.1 “W. 1 w <1 I 1.1: .111 .1 an. .1 I - - - ‘V a. .371. I 4. 62 The traditional allocation of voices by pitch area, historically derived from vocal ranges, does not” seem applicable in the least. On the contrary, a voice, indicated as a line by the continuity of its note stems, may freely range from the highest to lowest pitches and may proceed by wide jumps, using the G or F clefs without regard to placement on upper or lower staves. A typical example of free-ranging writing is m. 238-240. Here both piano staves, written as two voices (and in one instance, upbeat of m. 239, three voices), using both treble and bass clefs, range from contra F-sharp to three-line a and back to contra E. The three-line a is given to the left hand. During the same three bars the orchestra, also written in the reduction as two lines with one extra note (downbeat of m. 240) as a third voice, ranges from.contra B—flat to two-line a-flat (see page 63). Occasional differentiation between voices may be found in the tendency of the bass line to move more slowly at times than the upper parts, for example in m. 12-15, or m. 243-244. However this is rare (see page 64). Generally, differentiation based upon rhythmic activity and rhythmic character is difficult to discern within this Work. There is, however, continual differen- tiation of voices in dynamics, articulation, and expression markings, as well as in instrumentation. in. o- o- ’ \T f 1? ! \ :J—o I5 4,99% W may“ I? l'"'l {I >f 64 65 The ambitus of almost any area fluctuates rapidly and tends to be enormously expansive. Unusual instances of narrow ambitus occur in m. 188-192 and m. 68-71 (see page 66). 3 When narrow ambitus occurs it is of relatively short duration, whereas the expansive ambitus may continue for considerable time. A typical picture is p. 37, m. 329-334 (see page 67). The fullness resulting from active polyphonic motion over a wide ambitus is examined in Chapter 7, Density. A brief résumé of the data revealed by that examination is that two-note sonorities predominate in sixty-four bars; fouréto five-note sonorities, one hundred and four bars; five— to six-note sonorities, sixty-nine bars; and "extremely Opaque" sonorities in thirty-nine bars. Fluctuations in density are continual, but there appears to be a general increase in fullness as the work progresses. The prevailing rapid motion, as well as the fluid, diversified rhythms, tend to obliterate any temporal distinction between the concepts of melody, melodic figuration, and "passagio." The unification or blurring of these concepts is furthered by Carter's tendency to use similar intervallic motion and "freistimmig" texture throughout. An example is m. 231-237 (page 68). J——‘I 3‘ ,3 I. m .-I W A o 2 iI‘V r o hm °° \ m IHIJ. OHM/i 1‘ I. .1. L ~ ‘I ) )V I J IV g I. . a 1% fig ‘ .idu I f L 2$34 ‘f :2 h, I; “it: A- N .. ‘7‘. “- 67 ___,i__ ____q ._ =23 — =— 5“. _. .___ if : m____. r. ___ .____ _: I: p __ __ .5" 4:: (i _ I l v- 623 \‘i i o -E Inmv, tL I.u= » eruuuum 4mm I a xv a xrummunanmwc an I, w Mr» w" .mx aw» I up. av\ . Ix . a\\.. I\ “II' SI .fififlflfl rIIInnlllL 3 . U «a “NH/uni a .b. 53.9.1.6 ) k S- .m- ) Ale «we I) ) t 'f L? It Ofluuwuflm coaunuamam macaw: 69 The specific intervallic structure of the melodic lines is usually difficult to calculate, due to the vari- ability of texture within the line. It is apparent, however, that although the interval choices are highly varied, there is a distinct preference in many areas for ' certain intervals and a rejection of others. An interval count was made of all of page 1, chosen for its leading, and hence probably motivic, position (please see p. 70). The calculations of interval motion in "freistimmig" places, for instance m. 4, beat 2, were made according to the Vii“ rafifion ofthe note stems. In places where the single line doubles on the same stem the motion was calculated into and from both of the notes on the stem. Where a new voice enters, the motion was calculated from all the existent notes on the same staff to the new note. Augmented 2nds were calculated as minor 3rds, diminished 8ths as major 7ths, and augmented 8ths as minor 9ths. Intervals larger than the major 9th were reduced to within the compass of the octave. Rests were ignored; the calculations are from the note preceding the rest to the one following. i The frequency of occurrence of intervals on page 1 is as follows: per. 4 35 per. 5 2 aug. 4 27 Maj. 2 l min. 2 10 Maj. 6 l min. 7 9 Maj. 3 none min. 3 8- per. 8 none min. 9 6 Maj. 9 none min. 7 3 70 J - 126 (J- 73‘9"! J - 12w. - 42.) fanfash'ca ORCHESY RAL REDUCTION bgg/ira .L. J.-as 71 The high preponderance of melodic 4ths is signifi- cant and proves to be motivic. The relatively high count of minor seconds relates to the opening motive. The absence of M. 3rds, eSpecially in conjunction with the low count of p. 5ths, indicates the rejection of triad formations. A similar study was made of melodic interval fre- quency on p. 37, m. 332, in a line of 16th notes including two clusters. In this case the results are: per. 4 6 Maj. 7 1 aug. 4 2 min. 2 none min. 7 2 Maj. 3 none min. 9 2 per. 5 none min. 6 2 Maj. 6 none min. 3 2 per. 8 none Maj. 2 1 72 Although the second study deals with a much smaller area than the first study, the results are corroborative. A third study, of m. 231-237, referred to above (please see p. 68), shows a somewhat different pattern of interval preference: per. 4 12 min. 9 4 per. 5 12 min. 2 3 Maj. 7 12 Maj. 2 3 min. 7 11 min. 6 3 aug. 4 9 Maj. 3 2 min. 3 7 per. 8 none Maj. 6 6 Among the significant differences emerging from the third study are the prevalence of 5ths, as well as the higher proportion of minor 3rds and major sixths. The higher use of fifths and sevenths suggests an expanded tertian analysis in m. 23l-234. The principal characteristics which emerge from Carterls interval choice are a preference for intervals of high tension, a disregard for traditional resolutions of these tensions, and a preference for disjunct motion. The melodic line does not reveal any tendency toward the use of chains of similar intervals or symmetrical construc- tions, except for areas where the preponderance of p. 4ths, augmented 4ths, and minor 7ths is so great that these. intervals do appear in sequence. Often the melody reverses direction, returning to the previous pitch. This‘ accounts for some sequential pairs, for instance in m. 99, piano part (page 73). Often this technique leads into sbcc. ; bmhmw b M. 156 n.n.ID- 333- it J-Ifl.” ~ . r_.,.J.I.J...r.o.:. 3 . 1:. J - 13% 7 7° "2.542 I , I 7 ‘59:. r 5 , =_,qf :i=—nf The relationship between tempo and meter in this work is a fascinating one. In many places where the duration of the small unit, for instance the eighth note, remains constant but the meter changes, there is a kind of rhythmic phrasing, a play of stress, sometimes duple, sometimes triple, sometimes asymmetrical, which gives to the composition a naturalistic motion, a spontaneous swaying to and fro comparable to the movement of natural objects, rooted but rarely still. When a graph is made on which the duration of the eighth note is projected for each tempo and each meter throughout the entire work, including those sections where eighths are not used as an actual note value, it is found that for long stretches the eighth remains constant while 87 the meter plays around it; and it is further discovered that after disturbances within the middle section of the work, the eighth returns to its original duration for most of the final thirty-seven bars. The eighth remains at 252 for the first forty-nine bars, touches 252 again at m. 84, m. 101-102, m. 205-221, m. 246-251, m. 311-319,' and m. 329-348 (the end). (Please see Appendix III, Graph of Eighth Note = 252.) No other constant tempo factor has this frequency or duration. It may therefore be said that in this work Tempo Primo at eighth note = 252 is the basic tempo. The composition recalls this Tempo Primo numerous times and concludes with a recognizable return to Tempo Primo. Most of the variations away from the constant eighth at 252 occur in the middle section, while the beginning and ending sections vary in meter rather than in the eighth note duration. It appears that an arched structural form and architectonic unity centered upon the eighth note underlie the extremely varied metrical modulations. Although Carter makes use of metrical stress, he is by no means the slave of the meter. Beats are often replaced by rests. While in many cases this means only that the beat is felt rather than heard (as in those measures characterized in Chapter VIII, Rhythmic Activity and Rhythmic Character, as "jagged"), in other instances 88 it does result in obliterating the beat and obscuring the metrical arrangement (as in other measures characterized in Chapter VIII as "diverse"). Furthermore, off-beat and irregular dynamic accents are used frequently. Carter's manipulation of stress is anything but simplistic. Tempo and meter change combine with dynamics, density, and rhythmic activity to offer an aural picture of the utmost variety. Motoric rhythm marked by asymmetrical measures and off-beat accents, such as occurs for instance in £2 Sacre du Printemps and other works by Stravinsky, never appears in Carter's Piano Concerto. The technique of metrical modulation has no room for rubato, accelerando, or ritardando. Carter may make his tempo changes by gradual successive stages of one or two measures. One such series of gradual changes is m. 100, marked quarter = 126, going through m. 101-104 marked dotted quarter = 88, and arriving at m. 105 marked quarter = 73.5. Another example can be found at m. 279 marked quarter = 60, going through m. 285-286 marked quarter = 90, arriving at m. 287 marked eighth = 180. On seven occasions a short intermediate measure is employed. Since the intermediate measure is so brief, there is no feeling of establishing a level tempo. The effect in metrical modulation is analogous to a transient modulation in tonal analysis. 89 Change within the individual measure, on the minute basis of increasing prolongation from one note to the next, for instance, is never seen in this work. It may be, however, that rubato of a very subtle kind is called for by Carter's frequent use of the term "espressivo." The possible meanings of "eSpressivo" are discussed in Chapter IX, Expression and Articulation Markings. CHAPTER VI DYNAMIC MARKINGS Carter's Piano Concerto has an extreme profusion of markings relating to the control of dynamics. A random sampling shows that page 9 of the two-piano score has fifty-one markings pertaining to volume level or volume change. Page 19 has forty-seven; page 29, forty- seven, page 39, fiftyeeight. Further examination indi- cates that this wealth of dynamic markings is character- istic of the entire work. Most often, a single measure will have several simultaneous or successive markings, of the same or of contrasting nature. The dynamic range is extensive and the gradations subtle. The parameters of volume are ff: and ppp. The indication ppp occurs four times (m. 194, 198, 225, and 227)?.£££ is used seldom (m. 311 is one instance). Volume gradations are indicated by ff, f, mi, mp, p, pp, piuéf, pig_p, poco f, by the words "crescendo" and "diminuendo, and by Opening and closing wedges. Although traditionally the words "crescendo" and "diminuendo" are used for changes from one volume level to another and wedges are used for small variations of volume within one 90 91 pervasive level, Carter uses these markings interchangeably. The marking §£_appears frequently and if: occasionally. It may be expected, in the use of dynamics in general, that they serve not only to provide variety of volume but also function as indicators of musical character and structure, especially if there are correlations between dynamics and density, tempo, instrumentation, motivic use, and other compositional factors. In order to comprehend the meaning of the voluminous dynamic usage in this work, three criteria were formulated. One is the volume level during any time period, the second is the stability of volume during any time period, and the third is the kind of change which occurs. Six categories emerge from the application of these criteria to the work: 1. relatively stable, with fluctuations in the range from pp_to mf, ' 2. relatively stable, with fluctuations in the range from E£.t° ff, 3. very diverse, ranging more widely than (1) or (2) I 4. predominantly level at p, 5. predominantly level at f, 6. predominantly crescendo. A category for relative stability within the middle volume range of p to E, which would complete the classification system, was not used as this kind of dynamic usage did not seem to exist within this work. 92 There also seemed to be no place for a category for "predominantly diminuendo." Since both of these kinds of dynamic usage are encountered frequently in most music, their absence in this situation should be noted as two significantly empty, or non-applicable, categories. (Please see Appendix IV, Classification of Volume Level and Volume Variation.) The validity of this kind of categorizing would seem to be directly related to the length of the time period to which the categories are applied. A time period of one measure or less turned out, upon experimentation, to be too brief for general use. On the other hand, three or four bars tended to be insufficiently specific. Two measures were found to be the smallest unit which was usable for this investigation. The two-measure unit is not of uniform size in terms of time elapsed nor of pulses encompassed, due to frequent changes of both tempo and meter, but it does have the advantage of being a constant factor which could be used in measurements of other musical data as well. All the data derived were placed on a graph in which the horizontal axis represented one-measure units. This was done to facilitate comparison with other musical events which were similarly graphed. Category 3, "Very Diverse," was not scanned solely in two-measure units but also represents on the graph various one-measure units between other categories. Category 6, "Predominantly 93 Crescendo," also represents a single measure in two instances which seem particularly significant (one is the final measure). Subjective judgment could not be removed entirely, but, in order to objectify as much as possible, various considerations were carefully held in mind in decision making. These include the number of volume changes and the frequency of changes within the framework of each category. Both could be very high without disqualifying the two- measure unit for placement within either of the "relative stability" categories, unless the range of fluctuations exceeded the volume boundaries of the category. In that case the unit was placed in "Very Diverse." Sections of low volume punctuated by loud interjections were placed in Category 1 unless the interjections seemed to prevail by reason of their duration, frequency, or rhythmic position. This would have made the section more suitable for 2 or 3. Rests were considered as tending to reduce the volume level, unless they were very brief. The number of notes present at any one time (density), and the rhythmic activity were studied and graphed separately. Although,in actual performance, volume, intensity, and density may frequently be found to interrelate, the separate investigation of each of these aspects revealed that a planned relationship among them is rejected by the composer. 94 In this work, multiple layers, with some strands marked loud and others soft, occur very frequently. These sections were placed in the "Very Diverse" category, unless the dynamic spread did not exceed pp to EE or mg to ff. Accents and sforzandi were considered to function with the surrounding volume level. A‘s; in piano, for instance, was not considered the equivalent of subito fgrtg, as in this work subito forte, or even £2552 by itself uses the heavy line over a note to indicate greater emphasis upon that note. This was assumed to be only a slight volume modification. Carter does not use the staccatissimo (') sign to indicate a particularly sharp, short accent. The word, "marcato," which occurs thirty- one times throughout the movement, is dealt with under expression markings rather than volume indications. When the criteria and categories had been defined and established, the entire work was scanned repeatedly and each definable unit was placed on a graph (please see Appendix IV, p. 158). It then became apparent that the initial impression Of very profuse and extreme dynamics is in fact true for one hundred and forty-eight. Category 3, "Very Diverse," was the one which appeared most often on the graph. It is also the one which includes some of the longest stretches of any one ' 95 kind. It includes one stretch of twenty-four measures, one of fifteen, one of thirteen, five of twelve, as well as various shorter occurrences. Category 3 definitely prevails in the last third of the work and particularly at the end. Category 5, "Predominantly f," is the next most prevalent category, with fifty-two measures. However, it is filled by only two appearances, one of twenty-eight measures and one of fourteen. The twenty-eight measure stretch is indicated principally in the solo Piano part. Category 4, "Predominantly p," is least filled, with only one appearance of thirteen measures at approxi- mately mid-point in the composition, m. 222. It is evident that the sections of comparatively level dynamics are the least frequent, but have relatively long duration.v Perhaps the implication here is that sufficient time is a requisite of quietude. Although Category 4, "Predominantly p," is the meagerest, Category 1, "Relatively stable from pp to mg," is the next in frequency to Category 3. The lower volume range is indeed used a great deal, although with fluctua- tions. Category l includes sixty-four measures in twelve appearances. Significantly, this category begins the composition with eight bars which offer a particularly clear-cut example of the type of sonority of that category. 96 Its next appearance is for seven measures, then two, three, ten, four, six, two, and eight, with the graph indicating that Category 1 dwindles in importance from the mid-point of the work. Category 2, "Relatively stable from mf to if," contains only nineteen measures, in five scattered appearances. Activity in this volume range tends to be of the upward thrusting type, as Category 6, "Predominantly crescendo,‘ contains thirty-six measures, in eight appearances. ’During the first third of the work there is a five-measure stretch and a six-measure stretch, but starting at m. 202 there is a five-measure appearance, then three measures, and then, clustered toward the end, five measures, ten, one, and one more for the final measure of the composition. Both Category 3 and the Category 6 become increasingly important as the end of the work approaches. . Summary of the data regarding dynamics: 1. Only one place steady at piano (fourteen measures beginning at m. 223) 2. Two places steady at forte (forty-two measures beginning at m. 123 and m. 208) 3. More general stability in the lower range (sixty-four measures) than in the upper range (nineteen measures) 4. No category for diminuendo, whereas there are thirty-six measures of crescendo 5. No category for stability in middle range 97 6. Most prevalent, one hundred and seventy-four measures of very wide and frequent dynamic fluctuation. Analysis of dynamics does not reveal that dynamics are used in this work to illuminate the formal structure or to indicate any kind of sectional organization. The pro- gression of the work is marked only very generally by the appearance at the beginning of "softer stable" and the rise in "diverse" and "crescendo" toward the end, while both the piano level and the forte level occur only in the central portion. Neither does the analysis reveal any identification by dynamics of thematic structure, characteristic motives, or instrumental usage. No significant correlations between dynamic usage and other compositional aspects were dis— covered by comparison of graphs. The ready conclusion is that the use of dynamics in this work is merely decorative. Further consideration, however, indicates another, far more SOphisticated, function. Decorative dynamics, whose main purpose is to alleviate tonal boredom, certainly does not require the immense variety demonstrated by this work. Rather than comforting the listener's ear, the range, diversity, and frequency of dynamic change in this work offer an aural challenge of the highest difficulty. Many listeners will not be able to meet this challenge. The willingness to meet the challenge and to function within_such an 98 elaborate framework forces upon the listener an attitude of the highest concentration. It implies a discipline requiring a tense emotional state lasting for three hundred and forty-eight measures. It may be that the dynamic usage is a means of sensitizing the listener to all aspects of the composition. Possibly this is the principal meaning of Carter's dynamics in this work: the tension level forced upon the participating listener is itself a vital part of the message communicated. Voluminous dynamic markings of the kind found here are not rare in twentieth-century music, especially in the works of Schonberg, Webern, and Berg. (See, for instance, Six Little Piano Pieces, Op. 19, by Schonberg, the Piano Sonata, Op. 1, or the Chamber Concerto for Piano, Violin, and Thirteen Wind Instruments by Berg, or Webern's Klavierstuck, Op. Posthumous.) Carter's link to the Viennese atonalists and expressionists, which is also suggested by other aspects of his compositional technique, may be further bolstered by his usage of dynamics. An analogy can be made between Carter's dynamics and the use of color in certain contemporary paintings, such as those of Jackson Pollack; non-structural, non- representational, large scale, often pointillist, ranging over the entire palette. CHAPTER VII DENSITY f Density, or the "thickness" of sound, is a compo- sitional factor which has grown greatly in significance during the late 19th and 20th centuries. Density is related to harmonic texture and chord structure, contra- puntal delineation and linear integrity, acoustical reinforcement and tonal diversification, spacing and utilization of pitch areas. In orchestral writing, density is related to tone color. Density may be consistent, stable in the number of voices used andperhaps also in their assigned ranges, or fluctuating in number and pointillist in allocation of pitch area. The organization of density can be useful in establishing formal structure. The deliberate evocation of well-established hiStorical associations in density usage, such as four-part chorale writing or melody with figural accompaniment, for example, may offer clues to thematic characterization and interpretation. In a concerto or concerto grosso, density is concerned with the massing of instrumental forces into tutti, solo, or concertato groups. 99 100 In view of the striking complexity of material in Carter's Piano Concerto, a minute and systematic classifi- cation of data regarding density may be essential. In this study, density is investigated in regard to both (1) the degree of thickness and (2) the consistency of texture. First it is necessary to define density in such a way as to eliminate confusion from related consideration of volume (acoustical reinforcement), rhythmic activity, and orchestration. Orchestral doublings of the same pitch ‘\ do not come under consideration because the composer's piano{\, 3 reduction of the orchestral score is used for this study. Kéhfifl' Carter states in a prefatory note to the published two- piano score that his reduction of the orchestration is fairly literal except for a few octave transpositions and some very densely composed passages. Volume is considered separately in Chapter VI, Dynamic Markings, and rhythmic activity in Chapter VIII, Rhythmic Activity and Rhythmic Character. Those instances within this work where density seems to have increased simultaneously with the building up of volume are studied from both vieWpoints. Similarly, closely arpeggiated figures are measured under both Density and Rhythmic Activity. The strictest and most precise definition of density would be simply the number of pitches present at any one time. A problem immediately arises in the interpretation ' of "one time." Does this mean occurring simultaneously 101 (beginning together), or perceived as related and compre- hended as a chordal structure which is extended over a relatively brief duration, as an arpeggio or an accumula- tion of chord tones? Another approach would be from the durational aspect, without, if possible, infringing upon the concept of rhythmic activity. This approach would require choosing a suitable time unit and measuring the thickest and thinnest of sonorities during that time span. Should priority be given to the most prevalent density within the time unit, or to the density of rhythmically significant chords? If the time unit is not precise simultaneity, how minute should it be? A suitable, although often rather large, time unit is the measure. This choice facilitates graphing in the same way as is used for other compositional aspects and offers ready comparison with other data by making use of the measure as the same constant factor in each case. A sliding scale of density measurement was devised, with overlapping of boundaries. Subjective judgment was used to determine those sonorities which were likely to be perceived as chordal although not simultaneous. Rhythmic placement of thick sonorities, and the most prevalent density within the measure were taken into consideration as tending to raise the density level. Ambitus was also considered; sonorities which were either very broad and expanded or very narrow and concentrated were rated higher 102 where it was feasible to do so. To illustrate, let us take a measure where the highest number of pitches which seemed to form one sonority was four, but where only two notes were also present at one time. According to the scale of classifications below, this measure could be placed in one of three categories, depending upon the concomitant considerations. In all decisions, aural perception was taken as the principal guide. In this connection it is noteworthy that Carter's dodecaphonic tonal matrix has nothing to do with the traditions of doublings and spacings for full sonority within a chord. The classifications which developed as useful categories in this work are: l. l to 2 notes 2. 2 to 4 notes 3. 4 to 5 notes 4. 5 to 6 notes 5. 6 or more notes 6. extremely opaque. The spread within each category is required by the length of the one-measure time unit and is adjusted in the way which seems suitable for this particular composition. Category 1 is significant for its meagerness and its placement. The composition begins with one of the rare appearances of the single note, in this instance a note lasting for an entire measure, which is a unique occurrence 103 within this work. In all, the single note occurs only three times: at the beginning, marked piano espressivo, for a whole measure of the piano part; in m. 223, last quarter, again in the piano part, marked piano un poco espressivo; and in m. 285, downbeat, the lowest C, in this instance actually a doubling between the piano and the double bass, marked fortissimo. The single line is also extremely rare. It occurs in m. 17 in the piano part and in m. 130-133 in the orchestral part. The other appearances of Category 1 are predomi- nantly two-note sonorities: m. 17, 113, 130, 131, 132, 160, and 166, seven in all. They are conspicuously absent from the second and third sections of the composition. Category 2, "2 to 4 notes," has sixty-four measures, scattered throughout the work but appearing, least in the third section. Category 3, "4 to 5 notes," has one hundred and four measures, beginning in m. 4 and appearing without apparent pattern throughout. Category 4, "5 to 6 notes," does not begin until m. 31, and contains sixty-nine measures. Category 5, "6 or more notes," begins in m. 28 and has sixty-four measures. This category is absent -from m. 107 to m. 173. A lowering of density results as it is replaced by sparser categories, Category 3 in particular. 104 Category 6, "extremely opaque," has only thirty- nine measures, but as it merges into Category 5 we do not get the impression of meagerness in that category as we do in Category 1. On the contrary, the combination of 5 and 6 reveals the very important usage of thick density. The denser categories begin later and occur more frequently as the composition progresses. Category 6 occurs for longer stretches later in the work and is the concluding sonority. In the entire movement, only seventy-two measures contain sonorities of less than 4-5 notes and these measures occur mostly in the earlier sections, while the denser sonorities, first appearing in m. 25, increase in significance until the end. Some of the sonorities of Category 6 are tone clusters including almost all the, notes throughout the pitch range of the piano, as at m. 316 (see page 105). Summary of findings regarding density: There does not appear to be a systematic use of different leVels of density, different concepts of con- sistency or variety of texture, or different kinds of density change, such as the addition of voices. Neither is density used for sectional organization or thematic characterization. The soloistic role of the piano is not marked by a particular kind of texture. In studying the 105 fir), graph of density, for which a classification decision has been made for each measure, it proved impossible to discover any meaningful pattern. It would seem that the artistic purpose is the expression of extreme variety and freedom, with much use of very thick sonOrities challenging the perceptual capabilities of the listener. The aesthetic is of non-structuring and fantasy. The general growth in density throughout the work tends to reinforce the onward progression in much the same way as was found true for other compositional aspects. This growth is rendered less than obvious by the wide and continual fluctuations. Please see Appendix V, Graph of Density, p. 159. CHAPTER VIII RHYTHMIC ACTIVITY AND RHYTHMIC CHARACTER Elliott Carter's Piano Concerto is extremely complex in regard to rhythm. The spacing of the sounds in time and in relation to one another displays fantastic variation. In the hope of achieving greater awareness in this area, a classification and graphing system was attempted along the lines of previous chapterS. (Please see Appendix VI, Graph of Rhythmic Activity, p. 165). V The concept of rhythmic activity may be described as a horizontal view of density. Instead of measuring the number of simultaneous sounds, the measurement applies to 'the number of successive sounds within a given duration. Again, in this study as in others, one measure was taken as the unit of duration and each measure was classified and graphed on a scale of l. l to 3 notes per measure 2. 4 to 7 notes 3. 8 to 11 notes 4. 12 to 20 notes 5. more than 20 notes. 106 107 Although the disparity in the lengths of measures within the work is in this case a factor which may tend to lessen the validity of this means of considering the data, some conclusions can be drawn. Rhythmic practices throughout the work appear to adhere to a fairly uniform concept. A careful perusal of the entire movement indicated that the first 120 measures of the composition could, in fact, offer a sampling which would be ample, characteristic, and representative of the method of investigation. This in itself constitutes one of the most important findings: stability of usage. Undoubtedly this stability functions as a significant factor in creating unity in this composition, a very help- ful consideration in such an extraordinarily large one- part form. 1 Immediately apparent is the correlative fact that rhythmic activity, like "dynamics" and "density," is extremely free, fluid, and wide-ranging. There is no observable pattern. Complications of notation often obscure the true number of successive musical occurrences, for instance when a note tied to the next beat must be written twice. In cross rhythms, some notes share time slots. For example, a five-note group juxtaposed upon a two-note group results in only six sequential sounds. The third important conclusion, drawn from examining both the music itself and the graph, is that in this 108 composition rhythmic activity is a completely independent aspect. It is free from either positive or negative cor- relations with other compositional factors. Increased rhythmic activity may be used at times to aid in building volume, but in general this is not the case. Thicker sonorities may be relatively more widely spaced in time, as in the last few bars, but sparser spacing also occurs in areas of meager density. Rhythmic activity in this work is used to express scope, variety, frequent change, high levels of rhythmic density, conceptual independence, and dynamic propulsion. In listening along a time continuum, it is not only the relative frequency and duration of successive notes which is perceived, but also, and more importantly, the kind of rhythmic motion which prevails in any area. The use of gradations of rhythmic density offers one means of expression; the kind of rhythmic motion employed by the composer offers another. The rhythmic character of any area depends upon the total number of notes, their fre- quency or relative speed, and their relative placement. In this work, rhythmic character types can be sorted into five categories: 1. smooth 2. uneven 3. jagged 109 4. diversified '5. extremely diversified. It is of interest to note that in the study of this compositional aspect, as well as of several others, it is necessary to include a superlative classification. The classifications are also appropriate to the work analyzed in that the homophonic and polyphonic considerations are studied together. The study of rhythmic character types proved rewarding in clarifying the concept in general and the specific usage within this work. "Smooth" implies that most notes are of equal duration or similar subdivision, or fit easily upon the meter. Examples are m. 17, 23, 232, 258, 282-284 (see page 110). It is not necessary that these measures contain relatively few notes. On the contrary, if the rhythmic activity is very high the impression may be "smooth" because all the time slots are filled. Polyrhythms may therefore lose their effect, as for instance, in m. 109 (page 111), unless the integrity of each rhythmic strand is clearly delineated in other ways, as in m. 207 (page 111), where the orchestration clarifies the rhythmic counterpoint. Carter's technique in dealing with this sensitive concern is one of the more debatable aspects of this composition. 110 M.r_1_7__-7’. = J fl*J 111 M. 109 finance fiss. *Tpt I—a-L?‘ “final/am ’9' 112 "Uneven" describes those places where notes are of similar but not equal duration, where the subdivision changes, but where the metrical placement remains undisturbed, as in m. 6, 22, 45, 76-78. M. 76 f Tp.‘ "Jagged" refers to an irregular rhythm that sur- prises by missing beats. Some kinds of syncopations and anticipations would be included in this category, especially those which place events narrowly off the beat. The dotted eighth followed by the sixteenth sometimes falls into this category, as does the pattern which places very disparate note values in sequence. This type of rhythmic character is of particular interest, because, although it may seem very active due to the alertness required of both listener and performer in perceiving and effectively playing the disproportionate values, it does not necessarily contain many notes. Rather, it is of importance to guard 113 the significant space between the notes. From this view- point, "jagged" may be considered an expansion of the concept of Monteverdi's "stile concitato." Examples are m. 49, 114, 106-107, 145, 150, 189, 251, and 339. -h_.,J.-73.s M. «339461 VA -" 114 The classification "diversified" requires many notes--12 to 20 or even more than 20, comparable to Categories 4 and 5 of Rhythmic Activity.’ There is usually great variety of note values and of subdivisions within the single line, as well as a high degree of contrapuntal complexity. "Smooth," "uneven," or "jagged" lines may be contained within the texture; however, the overall impression is of rhythmically independent lines made up of different note values. Examples are m. 75, 212, 230, and 327. M. 212' ~ I‘": "Extremely diversified" will probably contain. polyrhythms and irregular groupings. In this study the term is reserved for measures which are so complex that they tend to be interpreted as a sound effect, similar to 115 the "special effect" interpretation of the measures of highest density. These saturated areas occur with con- spicuous frequency, as in m. 81, 99, 215, 248, 249, 313-315, and 343. u. 215’" memn '..-.-..---00. ill :7 E [:1 «J L1 3___.I I.” 116 The first ninety measures of the Piano Concerto ' were systematically classified and graphed in terms of rhythmic character. (Please see Appendix VII, Graph Of Rhythmic Character, p. 168.) As in the graphing of rhythmic activity, this sampling offered sufficient data to serve as a visual representation of the kind of use of this compositional factor in the entire work. Carter's ingenuity in devising and manipulating rhythms seems inexhaustible. Rhythmically identical measure-groupings hardly exist. Although repeated figures, chains of triplets, for example, do sometimes recur, Carter usually manages to eschew the use of repetition or sequential patterning. There is no rhythmic motive. Rhythmic recall is not practised. There is no correlation of rhythmic character with any other compositional aspect. Interpretive mood is evinced by rhythmic character only in an oblique way. Significantly, ostinato is not used. Rhythmic patterning of an organizational nature, such as augmentation, diminution, or "divisions," is also completely absent. Carter's rhythmic aesthetic is striking in its non- affiliation with other considerations. Perhaps its most important quality, in addition to the astOnishing level of its imagination, is that it is a selfécontained means of expression. 117 So many areas within this work can be described as "diversified" or "extremely diversified," with more than twelve or even more than twenty successive sounds per measure, that questions of aural judgement arise. Are the more complex measures intended to be clearly and accurately perceived, or is a sound effect of overwhelming activity the desired objective? In Chapter VI, Dynamics, aural strain in regard to the perception of fluctuating dynamics was presumed to be part of Carter's aesthetic in this composition. Perhaps the rhythmic activity and complexity, amounting at times to a shock wave, may be adjudged similarly. If confusion, as an expressive phenomenon, is in fact part of the conceptual framework of mid-twentieth- century music, it can be observed in measures such as 239-242. Confusion certainly must be admitted to its place on the extreme Dionysian side of the aesthetic continuum. In this work Carter demonstrates that if con- fusion is to be communicated purposefully, it cannot be assigned with surety to the possibly obvious happenings of aleatory means, but requires meticulous structuring by the composer. 118 CHAPTER IX EXPRESSION AND ARTICULATION MARKINGS Carter is extremely specific in every aspect of the music analyzed. Dynamic markings are precise, tempo is measured to minute metronomic calibrations. It would not seem as if there were need for many interpretive directions or "expression markings." However, Carter does make use of an extensive vocabulary of "expression markings," and, surprisingly, many are of a subjective, non-specific nature. A complete list of expression markings found in the first movement of the Piano Concerto and the places where they are used follows (when the marking occurs at an upbeat, the numbering is of the following measure): 1. espressivo, m. 1, 8, 28, 35, 50, 94, 124, 133, —_I38T—ITU, 141, 144, 146, 162, 228, 236, 258, 259, 264, 270, 271, 277, 279, 322, 329, 330, 331, 339 (this is the final marking) 2. un poco expressivo, m. 223 3. espressivo sempre, m. 281 4. molto expressivo, m. 54, 105, 125, 126, 276, 299,‘3l8 5. molto espressivo e ben legato, m. 301 6. cantabile espressivo, m. 5, 175, 179, 231 119 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16.’ 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 120 espressivo ben cantando, m. 32 cantabile, m. 24, 49, 216 ben cantando, m. 140, 304 ben sentito, m. 319 delicato, m. 69 1e iero, m. 6, 54, 72, 80, 98, 128, 130, 199, 20I, 225, 253, 329 sempre leggiero, m. 81 marcato, m. 22, 23, 24, 30, 34, 37, 39, 61, 102, 147, 168, 169, 174, 178, 184, 204, 205, 212, 215, 219, 247, 248, 249, 253, 293, 308, 327. Marcato mano sinistra, m. 27 ben marcato, m. 209 marcatissimo, m. 207, 310 sonoro, m. 216 brillante, m. 290 sempre staccato e pesante, m. 302 giocoso, m. 123, 185 accompagnato, m. 105 Very often, different strands within the texture have different markings (m. 252 is one instance among many), presumably to indicate linear balance in terms of tonal pre-eminence.or placement within simultaneous independent phrases. Sometimes Carter will repeat an expression marking as if to reaffirm its validity in the face of conflicting markings for simultaneous parts. Often he will meticulously place the same marking in simultaneous parts. In general, 121 expression markings occur at the beginning of phrases and indicate tonal character or solo position. Articulation markings are also highly specific and numerous. The word "legato" appears often and there are many legato slurs as well as longer, phrase, slurs. Carter 'uses staccato dots, but not under slurs. Sometimes he will pair staccato dots with a heavy line over each note, achieving the same kind of "portato" effect which is often the interpretation of dots under a slur. .In m. 17 he uses the word "détaché." . It is clear that the expression markings which Carter considers most important in this work are "marcato," "espressivo," and "cantabile." These are all terms of considerable ambiguity. If "marcato" is interpreted in its literal sense, one may wonder why a slightly higher dynamic level was not Specified.e The implication is either that "marcato" is too subtle a volume gradation to be named as such, or that the meaning is principally of psychological import, referring to the portentious presenta- tion of each note within the phrase. "Cantabile" may be related to "marcato," referring to tonal prominence or to similarity of sound within a phrase. The familiar term "dolce" is conspiCuously absent. Enigmatic, too, is the use of the term "espressivo." Possibly the usage goes beyond the exact definition and means a flexibility in phraSe dynamics and phrase rubato. 122 What does "sensibile" (upbeat of m. 257), mean in regard to a single sound, the diminished third A - C-flat? "Giocoso (subito)" at the upbeat of m. 185 is correlated significantly with a change of articulation from "legato" to "staccato." "Giocoso" at m. 123 is _particularly interesting. M. 123 ~J-no.25-. - J-uo.25-¥ (“99) (as) 123 It is not a performance direction, but a descrip- tion of the musical character at that moment, an evocation of the empathy and psychological involvement of the player (oboe). The specific verbal communication of this joyful mood serves to indicate Carter's awareness of the inten- sity of feeling and emotional participation of the per- former. It also makes explicit his wish and expectation in terms of human communication between composer, performer, and listener. It may be taken as an expression of Carter's philosophy that he makes use,in this work,of terms which indicate to the player not only what he should do, or what he should try to make the listener perceive, but also what he himself might feel if he were internalizing the emo- tional progression of the composition. The emotive con- tinuity which is the life line of a free form (as this work appears to be) may well call upon the sympathetic partnership of the performer to a degree analogous to that practised by some schools of acting. The freedom of the lines of internal coherence within this composition are signalled by the use of the term "fantastico" at the beginning. This word appears above the staff, next to the metronome indication in the. solo piano part and in the same position above the orchestral reduction, which is tacit at this point. There is no indication in the positionusually allocated to the 124 title of the moVement or to the principal tempo. Although "fantastico" is written in lower case italics without an initial capital, it may be that the meaning of the term extends in a general sense to all aspects of the composition. Certainly there is no specific meaning which could apply at this point. CHAPTER X SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Elliott Carter (b. 1908) has had a long career as composer, teacher, and lecturer. His critical success and frequent performances place him in a position of leadership in the world of music. He not only influences the progres- sive movement in contemporary music, but in his personal and unique way he may be considered one of its true representatives. Each of Carter's new works is therefore' extremely interesting in itself, and also may prove helpful in illuminating some of the conceptual trends and composi- tional techniques of today. The first movement of the Piano Concerto poses serious challenges to the listener and analyst; hopefully, in studying these challenges, per- ceptual skills may be developed which will be rewarding in understanding not only Carter but other composers of our time. The principal difficulty in undertaking the present study lay in finding a way to begin (a far from rare problem in analysis and of significance in itself). The first impression is of a work of great complexity and innovation, very active, thick, long, aurally overwhelming, 125 126 apparently formless, the creation of which may have been governed to a high degree by an intuitive process. It was helpful at first to define the composition by determining what it is not. It is a work without themes or tunes, with no distinction between statements and transitions, no scenario even in the sense of area-distinction by emotional content, no tonality, no modulation, no identification by. pitch-space area, no returns or repeats, no cultural or folkish allusions. Casting aside false expectations in regard to these considerations brought out the fact that some traditional and even conservative criteria remained. The work is non- aleatory, non-electronic, and makes no use of mixed media or experimental acoustical effects. There are no micro- tones or deviations away from the locked-in pitches of the tempered scale. Traditional instruments are employed and ’used in traditional, albeit sometimes very virtuosic, ways. (The virtuosic element may also be regarded as traditional in a concerto.) Pitch notation, rhythmic notation, expression markings, and dynamics are employed in the classic tradition. The six basic attributes which define most Western music are all present in some way--rhythm, structure, melody, harmony, timbre, and volume. In addition there are directions to the player. These basic attributes turned out to be quite sufficient as points of 127 departure for a comprehensive analysis. All the investigable elements of composition listed earlier in Chapter I, Purpose and Method, could be grouped into these attributes. The study of rhythm required two kinds of inquiry, one into rhythmic activity and rhythmic character, and another into tempo, meter, and metrical modulation. The study of melody included a consideration of the counter- point. Timbre was deemed a Specialized tOpic somewhat outside the area of this paper; however, the thickness of sound was investigated under Density. Volume comes under Dynamic Markings, and directions to the player under Expres- sion and Articulation Markings. Harmony, in its three aspects of chordal sonority, cadential form, and structural organization, formed a chapter in itself. Form, although it is presented first in this paper, was actually investigated last, as it required bringing together sufficient knowledge of all the other attributes or aspects of the composition. Procedure, in the sense of evolution over a time- span, is the particular philosophical inquiry in which we, together with Carter, are involved in this work. In studying each attribute it seemed important to consider both the character of the element itself and the kind of prosedure in which it was engaged, whether, for instance, the melodic lines were of the same type throughout or, if they varied, in which way they did so. Wherever possible a statistical method was employed, measuring the attribute 128 in its various presentations and measuring the procedure in which it was involved. Each new aspect studied was scrutinized for possible clues regarding other aspects. A brief recapitulation of findings follows. In form, the first movement of the Piano Concerto is less like a structure with members, connecting tissue, and organized parts expressing a genetic lineage than it is like protoplasm moving toward its fulfillment. The concept of structure in this work is not one of "being" but "becoming." Apparently one long evolving form, its forward movement is marked by a series of interconnected changes of various kinds, some relating to the harmonic texture, or to the rhythm, density, orchestration, or motivic usage. There are no pauses. The 12-tone matrix is always used and the motivic three-note chords of C, F—sharp, G and G, D-flat, F are always present in their original form or metamorphosed. There is a slight recall of the original pitches toward the end of the work, but in no sense a recapitulation. The movement is headed by the indication "fantastico, which is perhaps a reference to an intuitive approach to the unfolding of the motivic possibilities. The emotional level is high throughout, due to the absence of sectionalization, obvious phrase endings or cadences, as well as to the particular kind of usage of other compo- sitional aspects. The overall impression is of an 129' immensely dynamic forward propulsion in one Sweep from beginning to end. The harmonic texture is based upon the use of the two motivic three-note chords. As these three-note chords are not tertian triads but are atonal in implication, it is therefore not surprising that even in their furthest metamorphosis they rarely exhibit tertian formation. The diminished triad never appears; neither does the diminished seventh chord. ”The augmented triad is seen seldom, but major and minor triads do appear as part of polychords, as do sevenths, ninths, elevenths, thirteenths, and fifteenths. Tertian sonorities with cross relations or added tones are frequent. Most prevalent, again a natural consequence of the motivic material, are quartal and secondal harmony. Although the harmonic vocabulary is proquely varied, the favorite and characteristic _sonorities are sevenths or ninths combined with fourths, or clusters which may be expanded to include all twelve tones. - 7 Movement in block chords is not used, nor are sustained harmonies. Chordal figuration, free expansion 'of the number of voices present, extension of some pitches within afigure to form a harmonic constellation are the techniques prevailing.' Chord streams are not used, but occasionally a Stream of a particular interval may appear within the texture. 130 Although Carter writes (see p. 48) of tonal organization by the method of allocating the three-note chords to soloists or to orchestra, the aural impression is of dodecaphonic saturation throughout.‘ The harmonies are not generated serially, but all twelve tones are always present in close juxtaposition. Cadences in the usual sense of the word cannot be discerned. The tension level of the harmony is consistently high. Although there are some fluctuations to a lower tension level, these periods are brief and do not appear to have Significance in defining the structure of the entire composition. Organization by pitch-Space area, or by tonal reference, for example to a special pitch emphasized by frequent repetition or prolongation or rhythmic placement, are techniques unused by Carter in this work. The figural writing which is employed almost continually tends to bring together the concepts of harmony and melody. The harmony is definitely implied by the melodic movement, and the prevailing contrapuntal treatment of the melodic lines causes the harmonic sonorities in most instances. Melodic voices are treated as choral or instru- mental "sections" which divide and reunite continually. The fluctuation of texture is one of the most identifiable characteristics of the work. Voices frequently move in paths of seconds, or in clusters. Occasionally streams of 131 parallel sixths or thirds appear, or sometimes a stream of fifths. The twinning of the melodic line into seconds may be motivic in that the original three-note chords contain seconds. Since the line is often thickened in this way into two or more concurrent notes, it is usually not possible to speak of the precise intervallic motion which comprises the melodic movement. In the few instances where the linear flow is of single notes, the intervals chosen clearly indicate a preference for leaps of fourths, sevenths, and fifths. This correlates with the harmonic analysis of the most prevalent chordal sonorities. In design, the lines often reverse direction, repeating the previous pitch. Chains of identical or similar intervals appear rarely. Small interval pattern- ing is used, but expansive movement, noted above as fourths, sevenths, and fifths, is preferred. There seems to be no preference for direction of movement, but swift and fre- quently reversing motion, ranging over a very wide ambitus, is a conspicuous style-characteristic, as is rhythmic fragmentation. Short rests are often interjected and there seems to be no deliberate manipulation of phrase length in terms of the duration of melodic continuity. Legato slurs and staccato markings are used as articulation, not as phrasing indications. Variety of phrase length and definition of phrase character through articulation are 132 not utilized as deliberate techniques in this work. Melodic phrasing is not supported by harmonic usage. Although bunches of sound frequently group upon the line, the prevailing texture is polyphonic. All the voices share the characteristics mentioned above, but they are differentiated by rhythmic patterning, dynamics, articulation, and by the occasional tendency of the lowest voice to move more Slowly. The clarity of the voice leading, however, is obscured by the "freistimmigkeit," by the frequent crossing, fragmentation, and the pointillism in the orchestration. There is little, if any, polarization of soprano and basses. "Companioning" of two or three voices within the texture is rare. The counterpoint is freely dissonant at all times and the lines are completely independent. The impression is of varying degrees of concurrent activity, usually rapid and dense. Elliott Carter's writing is frequently associated with a new technique of tempo change called metrical modu- lation. A considerable amount of publicity has been given to this innovation. In the composition analyzed, metrical modulation is evidenced by Sixty metronome markings and a comparable number of meter changes, indicating that Carter is deeply involved in the procedure of shaping events in time. This concern is extremely enlightening as to Carter's meaning in creative circles today, for it is both sophisticated in its technical application and profound 133 in its confrontation with basic musical truths. The philosophy of music conceived as aural experiences on a time continuum creates the vieWpoint which illuminates all the aspects of the first movement of the Piano Concerto. Carter's continual manipulation of the time element expresses his aesthetic involvement with matters of procedure and "becoming." All the compositional aspects of the work are in flux and the time element itself is extremely fluid. The time element in music is by nature dual, both objective and subjective, lying within the twin concepts of duration and pulse or tempo and meter. Carter plays with the time element by varying the duration of his beat, the subdivisions of his beat, and the distribution of accent over the group of beats.v He uses bar lines and meter indications, but, going further, he controls the time element by means of a specific interchange unit which may be a beat or may be a portion of a beat. The interchange unit functions rather as a coin whose buying power or "real value" changes in altered cirCumstances, although its assigned numerical value is consistent. A large array of coins may be used by Carter; their meaning changes. To expedite this fluidity, Carter makes use of extremely specific directionsas to both the particular 'interchange unit, or metrical value which "modulates," and also the objective duration of the beat. The 134 interchange unit is indicated as a note value over the bar line at the point of tempo change: the duration of the beat is indicated by a metronome indication in each instance. The technique of metrical modulation results in precise control over the time element. Presumably, there- fore, extreme finesse over the Shaping of aural events on the time continuum can be achieved. .On the other hand, this system disregards two time concepts which are of limitless interest: (1) rubato, accelerando, and ritardando (that is, progressive change within minute units), and (2) complete change Without a ready interchange unit (as, for instance, from one system of coinage to another totally unrelated). It would seem, then, that metrical modulation alone does not provide complete control of all the elements that pertain to time. Carter's further explorations along these important lines will probably be of the highest interest. It may be that a change in his entire aesthetic would reSult from either greater control over the time element, or renuncia- tion of the presumed present objective of very tight control. The entire movement is shaped by time considerations. The original tempo is quarter note 126. This can be sub- divided into eighth note = 252. Carter calls attention to the eighth value at m. 10 by shifting from 4/4 meter to 6/8 meter, keeping the eighth constant. If we grasp 135 this clue and keep in mind the impliedvalue of the eighth note throughout the work, it can be seen that, despite many wide fluctuations, the eighth often re-asserts itself at 252. This metronome marking provides a touching- down place at intervals through the work and becomes a settling-down place at the end. This circumstance may, of course, be unintentional. It does provide a thread, however tenuous, through what appears at first to be a maze of changes of both tempo and meter. The first movement of Carter's Piano Concerto offers a challenge to the listener's comprehension through the sheer number of sounds employed. Extreme density and extreme activity prevail. A statistical method was employed in the hope of giving definition to the somewhat overwhelming impression of the ear and eye. Each measure was counted, classified, and graphed in terms of the number of simultaneous sounds (density). Large areas of the work were graphed in terms of the number of successive sounds (rhythmic activity). It emerged that prevailing density is so high that only seventy-two measures out of the total of three hundred and forty-eight contain con- sistently fewer than four to five notes at the same time. Density increases through the middle of the work and is 'highest at the end. Except for the gradual upward shift, no purposeful patterning appeared. Similar findings emerged from a note-count of successive sounds. Rhythmic 136 activity, like density, is generally very high, very varied, and tends to increase slightly toward the con- clusion of the composition. Here again, a purposeful patterning, related perhaps to other compositional aspects, could not be found. The rhythms employed were classified in terms of character implications such as smooth, jagged, and the like. Here, too, the findings were, typically, of great variety, ingenuity, and complexity, tending in many instances, due to the activity of each line and to the contrapuntal diversity, toward aural saturation and hyperactivity. The meaning of the challenging thickness is uncertain. One hypothesis is that the listener of the future may be expected to reach higher comprehension levels than at present. Or it may be that the listener today, by effort and careful attention, can outreach his present self. Or it may be that the listener's effort and careful attention is in itself the goal of Carter's com- positional thrust in these areas. The aural saturation and hyperactivity may be designed to sensitize the listener to higher emotional and intellectual states. Or it may be that turbulence is indeed the prevailing message in this composition. If the last two hypotheses are correct, they supply a raison d'étre to the profusion of dynamic markings and 137 expression markings. A typical page of Carter's score may include forty to sixty such markings, some concurrent, some sequential. No doubt many of these markings are intended to give clarity to the texture, but many others are probably designed to provide variety and richness of expression to the on-going aural experience and to challenge the listener's involvement with the composition. The dynamics do not identify structural areas. The findings for each measure in terms of density, rhythmic activity, dynamics, and tempo and meter were- placed on a graph to facilitate comparison. It is note- worthy that purposeful correlations do not appear. Perhaps the two most significant findings which emerge from the study of all the compositional aspects of the work analyzed are the intensity of functioning of each aspect and the apparent disassociation between them. It may be fruitful to look for analogies between ' the techniques of this composition and some of the techniques of twentieth-century painting. In the use of dynamics, harmony, and rhythm, we have a brilliant, profuse, and diversified palette of color. In density, a generally thick application of paint, with the surface in high relief. In rhythmic activity and rhythmic character, the uSe of a complex web of design with many planes of super-imposed and conflicting perspective. 138 Harmony and melody both emerge from compositional choices predetermined by the creator and motivic in the sense that they can be perceived by the listener and also in the sense, more important, that they provided guidance in creation. These choices are innovative and, to a high degree, intuitive. In his concern for the time element, perhaps the most sensitive matter of all, Carter may be compared to the painter who realizes that his picture may be seen as a whole, as a Gestalt, but may also be viewed in part: and as the eye peruses the picture and makes its own path between the parts, a subjective reproduction of the picture is created by the viewer. So, the pulling together of the parts of Carter's composition by the memory of the listener creates the form. It may therefore be possible in music, as in painting, to create a totally abstract form. From this vieWpoint it can be seen that while many of the techniques and honored traditions of the past are discarded in Carter's Piano Concerto, the basic purpose of the art-work remains the same, and eternally true: the communication of an emotional state through the ordering of sound. BIBLIOGRAPHY 139 Carter, BIBLIOGRAPHY Elliott. Jacket notes to Nonesuch Recording H-7134, Sonata for Flute, Oboe, Cello, and Harpsichord, and Sonata for Cello and Piano. . "Shop Talk by an American Composer." Problems of Modern Music. New York: W. W. Norton, I960. . "The Piano Concerto." Boston Symphony Program. Notes. Boston, Mass.: The Boston Symphony, January 6, 1967. 140 APPENDICES 141 APPENDIX I GRAPH OF THE OCCURRENCE OF EVENTS 142 APPENDIX II GRAPH OF TEMPO AND METER CHANGE 144 APPENDIX II GRAPH OF TEMPO AND METER CHANGE This graph offers a visual representation of the tempo and the meter used in each measure throughout the work analyzed. The numbers running from left to right across the top of the graph are measure numbers. The numbers running from bottom to top along the left side of the graph are metronome indications. Numbers in paren- theses within the graph area are metronome indications which differ from the customary calibrations. The note values drawn on the graph are those which equal the metri- nome indication. The numbers which appear as fractions on the graph area indicate the meter at that point. The duration of any tempo can be determined by position on the measure number line. The longest duration is twenty-three measures, which occurs only once. The next longest durations are eighteen measures, once; seventeen measures, once; fifteen measures, once: twelve measures, four times, eleven measures, once; and ten measures, five times. The longest durations occur in the final third of the work; and the largest number of changes of tempo and meter occur in the middle third. The metronome indication of 126 occurs at the beginning and at the end of the work. 145 146 .H .o «mozmso mass: oze omsme so seems 147 .m .o .5245 mass: 52 omzme mo seams 148 _ «E .m .d .3258 seems 92 0.55. so seams 149 .v .m smogu mam: Q5 0%.”: .mO flaw 150 .m .m .555 mass: oza oases so was a; m. we .62 151 .m .o .3245 mmems 9,2 058.. so mmamo 152 .b .m .MGZ¢MU MMBHE QZ< OmSMB m0 mmdmu 153 in m m m won .m .o .mozamo seems ozm omsme no shame QQ 154 .m .a .5520 -mmBNZ QZd.OmZHB m0 mmflmw APPENDIX III GRAPH OF APPEARANCES OF EIGHTH NOTE 155 252 156 Nmm mBOZ mBmUHm ho mMUZfimflmmmd m0 mmdmu APPENDIX IV CLASSIFICATION OF VOLUME LEVEL AND VOLUME VARIATION 157 158 nvmlwmm mmm mmmlmmm Hmm ommtmmm bmmlmam hHMIMHm NHMImom NOMImmN ommlohm mmmlamm mmmlvmm thIOBN oomlmmm mwmlaom mmmlvmm MNN NNmImON Nomlomd homImom omalmhd mmHIHmH , thIOBH boa moalmoa mmH moalmma mma hmalwma NmH vaImmH MNHImHH HmHIvHH HHHIMOH NHHINHH NOHIhm omlmm hmlmh HmImm vmlmm vamm vmlmm hmImm nmlmm .E molmm Hmtom mm mNInH Hmtom .E oatm .E FHIHH mIH .E Hooded use us Houmom pom ms oosoomouu ouuom oases mmuw>wo >Ho> mo omsmm sflsuflz mo mmsmm ceases sausmsaeosssa sersssHEOsmss sersssasosmss spaeansrm m>asmasm erasessum m>arsamm. .COHDMflHm> mEsHo> was Hm>mq mEsHo> mo QOHDMOAMMmmMHU APPENDIX V GRAPH OF DENSITY 159 160 .huflommo osmuuxm osHu m um mono: whoa Ho m oEHu m as mmuos mum was“ s um mopos mus mafia m um mono: «IN 05H» m um mmuos NIH .H .a .seHmzma mo reams 161 .N .d .weHmzmo mo reams 162 - “I .m .o .seHmzmo mo reams 163 I... .1 .v .d .seHmzmo mo reams 164 A "I - of“ .m .m .MBHmsz .mO ~15de APPENDIX VI GRAPH OF RHYTHMIC ACTIVITY 165 166 .wusmmos Hum moosmunsooo om son» who: .musmmms Hum moosmuusooo omlma mufimmofi Hum noosmuusooo,HHIm chances use moosouusooo nIv assumes Hoe mmocouusooo MIH Smart; .H .d .ESHSBN 3815mm mo madame .m .v «m .N .H 167 .m .m .seH>Heo¢ oHsmesmm mo seams vaLW d flp hamsmnuxm .m . pmwmwmum>wa .v mmeoamamo 325me so reams common cm>msa spoofim .m .N .H APPENDIX VIII FIRST MOVEMENT OF ELLIOTT CARTER'S PIANO CONCERTO 170 Note The reduction of the orchestra for a second piano is fairly literal except for a few octave transpositions and some very densely composed passages. A compromise between playability and cueing to give the main orchestral material was sought. In many passages the piano part should be performed not emphasizing the upper line of a series of chords but as a sequence of chords or intervals evenly balanced. This is particularly true of mm. 1224-139, Fifi-151, l79-18l, 223-2'25, 25l-269. 321-327, 339-340, 543-607 (wherever simultaneous groups of two or more notes occur), and in 669 to the end. As is indicated in the score, the two passages that emphasize the perfect fifth —- mm. 393-410 and 505-521 ——- depend for their character on this sonority. Pedalling is suggested in many passages to clarify the chordal intent. but need not be followed literally, and may be used in many other passages at the soloist’s discretion. Pasagos marked A‘ “‘x or R’” “x (which occur only in the con- certino) are to be played as a continuous accelerando or ritardando; the note values indicated are only approximate. A note to mm. 522-529: Beginning at m. 522 the soloist should accelerate gradually while the orchestra maintains a strict, regular tempo. The dotted arrows indicate approximately what points the piano should have reached in its relation with the orchestra. This relationship can be somewhat free, but at m. 529 the soloist must reach the beginning of his tremolo somewhere between the limits indicated. Once there, he must continue his accelerating and fading tremolo. reaching the maximum speed and lowest dynamic level at the point indicated at the end of the bar. The length of this tremolo is determined entirely by the time it takes the orchestra to arrive at this point playing in strict tempo. Elliott Carter 171 'K—i' Piano Concerto Reduction by the composer “0’ Elliott Carter J - 116 (J- '42),fanfasfieo ,9; SOLO PIANO J - t26(J- - 47.), fanfash'ca ORCHESTRAL do... p (cramp. J REDUCTION until In. 19 [El] the J- '8‘ --==::f : NB: Boxed instrument names refer only to the Concertina IOU. AMP-6715 © Copyright 1967:, Associated Music Publishers, he, New York Printed in U.S.A. All Rights Reserved 172 173 ('9’) > mare. F—3_1 H's—i r—z—w . ’ P Fla (pp) 1 ____ b 4 184 “L. J-183.75 = I)» J-183.75 AMP-6715 185 _L -—,JVJ-J-u025—. —L .— y JvJ . J I no. —. "lam wr-m J *vJJ- J-noas-o as f C—fi» J-' 73.5 AMP-6715 186 Q- ) $(J lm.u) = .bsiunu -b» J.-7s.s AMP-6715 187 . . E w___ a: _ .__ . ._ ...m_____ : . ., ._ _ £17 ,_. ____ E, a. . s .....r _,____.. _=.s . = E... 5.. Lu“ __ ‘ “_w =. .. . . .1“... 1mm. _, E .. . a. .. _. ._.____.~. E E. .5. .. ____ ____ = .m ____ .__.__ . l: , ‘ W m a '" .... m E _ ... __ ___._... . 3 J m. Na. ____ . 5%.. _p . - .. r. ... . _._. : ,___. .5 1 ..u s. ..u“ a. @ 188 __ .,.. . .1, .1 m ma= ...: i t. _E a” s , s..___ w, ____ a: as .___ :3” ... .. an... o . _m__ ., ____ .._. _.,_ ... :. s... g _ L e 4: 1... M . .__ 3 up. . mm. ..vum v—- m... r___._ E m ,1 __.___. 105-e *‘fiJ-‘IJ- Nil-6715 189 Concertina < ..l‘. _. J.-1os> _ a , 2“] > —=: A A A >h >v- "5’ > > > :fnonfnppo A .5 >7 . . ' ‘ - . < ’V (accent! I , $ ‘ . ' m (1808' I ’0’ i 5 f > . ~— - J? _.J. -1ol [E]? >_ > a AMP-67 l 5 190 ...J-so A .i ..h. AMP-671 5 191 ..bs 11.139 AMP-67] 5 192 w m m. Min Dill l I Miblowm]. any AMP-6715 193 . a... u . . ... .h. = . ... . _— un‘u ...“ nn-L h ‘v > -. . . 77 7:7 7 _ =— A" < . ;" ‘“ _— “ .‘ . . .31.. 7. _— .. t .n. a m a a 194 b J AMP-6715 195 . . .5 II! o 1.. A .___ a . _. ...x; J an”: r...“ ...; E... ,"5 .5. can Ur... 1s '72 f fflf—c AMP-6715 196 ‘um-v .11 .1 11 _1___ ___1_ 1: 1111111 1y 111111111111 =1— 1. 111 __._u.l.r Vim“ _.__m~ 1 11.1.1 :1. 1 1 1 11 1.1.111“ ___1..1 :— ...,m ._/|‘l\’f,\.._ ..mm 11,111.11 .111 . 11111 111.11 _ =2 P AMP-67H 197 «Lfi'Jbg-9,J-'“ _ _1 _ _ a . .1 Q bnm_ \‘h In!) t 11 1.. «1 ”....“ ... ..Hp Tam 1— 1"- a-.. l.- _- n . . D 1. 111, 11.1 1.11 1111 "l, VA {35 0..-----.... .. AMP-67] 5 198 1111.1. .1. 11.1 .1 _1 _ m._.— tr m _1 11. "gm .1... ...u . .1... =11“... .1 .1. I ...... . _.= . 111.1 . 1 1. m." ..1, ... 1.... M . 2 u .. .u21ku” - 3." f 1 1.. 4...... 1 .111 __1_ 1 1 1 ... 1 . 1 ... =1 1 J, .: . .... __ . 1 . m 1 .2 . a... ,m. 1 ... 7__=1.1 .. _ 1 =1... 1 .2 ” ..um. > 1 _5 m 9.: I 1 =13... 11... 1 1 1 1 .... . ____1 _. a 1 w 1 , .... ...—,1 . 1.11.11, . —= P m ._ _ . . "7 a = m :1: . _ 1. == ..1 1 1. 1 .. 1 I” ”mum. .mmm _ m: . .mn 1 ... . 1 .1”. “...”: 1 .— ....1. J .. m“ m ...... E... F .1 . 1: ...m L.» .1 w” .w w m... a. 1. . 11.1-1... . 1.1.11- ..__....1 1 L J = 110.25 AMP-67 l 5 199 9)., J..13.5 E .1...__ E .1111 . E a firm” * ..1 .. .... , .... =1. .4 — . . _ . ~ .‘ v I‘- . - .. a ._ =1 ...... A...”... _1_._ ... ... ., ._ 1...... AMP-6715 200 mums _ *5" 201 7 - J-J ”41,4. - 53.3 ‘ fi-TJ‘J' a“ ’ ‘ f mum AMP-6715 u- . 81in: out be“: AMP-671 5 203 15 1 11JII""""""]!""5.. 1.1111.- ...1111. ..un 4 .__... 1.2m 1111 .u_u 11... .1 111 .1._m_“_ ___ 1.... .__. I”: my 1...... 204 51 ’ 8 .0; J f\——/ g. i J ‘1 >A E D o—l- J+,JI12‘ h A a"1 q N,\ urnf ’ ”If ( A a 1 r—‘—’—‘ "93 > 6 I 0—: AMP-671 5 205 .7. j...——: .1 .v. 11.1 1.1 1. .111. ......--. m 111.1111 11.1111...1 = .._> .11 .- == __._._ r. —= E“. r _u . .. x 111. m .1 v. --—- ----~-_--.-—....-_-.------— --_._-- - ‘—\ ....- _....._._.-......-J 1‘ 11 v EE (pad! 1.1/ /\ man? ...}. P AMI-67H 207 “lull“ l ' 41'“ ’ - 11.71% .. 1. 111.1... 111.1 111 1111, . .11. .1... 11111 .11. _ .__11 111 11 1.. .1. ? “h: ',1 o-—--~. E I 208 (ahsm p espr. —. J ... flab. ORR { b p>w 9"” < ”4715 209 c—J» J-m .J-os n ‘ I J 5 J « 4" - AMP-67] 5 "‘TIE@fiflfljflli'ilelflwlfiififlfl“