.TH E 9 v , I ? IIJAARY mchngan State University N This is to certify that the dissertation entitled AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIORS AND PERSONALITY FACTORS IN CHURCH MEMBERS AND ENVIRONMENTALISTS presented by Chris Henry Newhouse has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PhD degree in Fisheries and Wildlife fi/fiw %/ Major profeéstsr Date X—/df6 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 MSU LIBRARIES m RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. A AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIORS AND PERSONALITY FACTORS IN CHURCH MEMBERS AND ENVIRONMENTALISTS BY Chris Henry Newhouse A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Fisheries and Wildlife 1985 ABSTRACT AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIORS AND PERSONALITY FACTORS IN CHURCH MEMBERS AND ENVIRONMENTALISTS BY Chris Henry Newhouse To be most effective in bringing people to a state of environmental action, it would be valuable to understand 'their values and motivations. This information could allow more specific targeting of educational and communications efforts. This study surveyed two groups on the following factors: position on the Maslow personality hierarchy, ranking of Rokeach’s terminal values, type and amount of environmenal action, perceived amount of commitment necessary to take environmental action, and type and amount of social/political action. The groups were first, environmental activists (individuals from the Sierra Club, the West Michigan Environmental Action Council, and the Audubon Society), and second, members of Free Methodist Churches. Research questions involved relationships between Maslow level and action (environmental and social/political), between Maslow level and Rokeach values, Chris Henry Newhouse and between Rokeach values and action (environmental and social/political). Maslow level (as measured with Beer's Preference Inventory) was found have no significant relationship with environmental action, social/political action, or dominant Rokeach terminal values. Rokeach terminal values were significantly related to both environmental and social/political action. In almost every action, those listing "salvation" as their first priority value were significantly lower in level of action from every other value group. Values profiles of the two research groups showed that the top-ranking values of the environmental groups were peace, health, and family security (salvation ranked last). The top—ranking values of the church groups were salvation, health, and family security (peace ranked 11th of 18). The environmental group is not homogeneous in values profiles; each environmental action appears to have a unique values profile. Those most active in recycling differ from those most active in making donations, etc. Comparing the two study groups on the perceived commitment necessary to take environmental action, it was found that there were no significant differences between the two groups. Discussion included implications for communications and education, relationships to other literature on environmental action, and strategies for behavior change. To Andy, Kristi, and Cathey, since the time and energy I spent in achieving this rightfully belonged to them. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS A dissertation is not really the accomplishment of one person. Many others have had major input and deserve much of the credit for the culmination of this project. First, I thank my wife Cathey for giving so generously of her time, encouragement, understanding, expressions of confidence, and prayers. Andy and Kristi deserve special credit for being understanding the many times I had to go back to my office to work on the dissertation. Dr. Ben Peyton deserves much credit for the way he constantly helped with his insights, suggestions, questions, and revisions. Thanks to Ben also for sticking with me _through a prolonged timetable. To those others on my committee, Dr. Darrell King, Dr. Ted Ward, and Dr. Marty Hetherington, I give thanks for their input of time and effort. Garnet Hauger should be recognized far more than these few words can convey. Her statistical expertise, her computer wizardry, and especially her enthusiasm throughout the difficult days of this project were all critically important. Dr. Wayne Clugston was a strong supporter both personally and in his role as Academic Dean Of Spring Arbor College. The knowledge of his support was as important as the many details he expedited. My parents, Henry and Clara Newhouse, deserve commendation for their help and their love - more concretely, for their personal, financial, and prayer support. There are also many others, friends who encouraged, supported, believed, exhorted, and prayed me through this. They are too numerous to mention by name, but will always be appreciated for their individual contributions. Finally, I acknowledge that this accomplishment is the result of answered prayer. This is the actualization of Proverbs 3:5,6 ,"Trust in the Lord with all your heart and lean not on your own understanding. In all your ways acknowledge Him and He will make your paths straight." iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLESOOOOOOOOOO. 00000 ....OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO Vi LIST OF FIGURES ......................... . ............. viii CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTIONOOOOO....000........OOOOOOOOOO ..... .0 1 Statement of the Problem.... ..... ... ....... . ..... 8 Research Questions ............................... 8 Significance of the Problem .................. 10 Assumptions and Limitations of the Study......... 14 Definitions of Important Terms............. ..... . 17 2. LITERATURE REVIEW ......... . .............. . ....... 20 A Survey of Environmental Education Curricula with regard to Adults .......................... 21 Adult Environmental Action, including relationships to attitudes, knowledge, concern, and values ................ ... ......... 23 Adult Education .................. ......... ...... 29 Application: Adult Potential and Participation in Environmental Programs...................... 32 Recommendations of other Researchers for Increased Action .................. . ........ .... 35 summarYOOOOOOOOOOOOOO ............ O ...... .... ..... 38 3. METHODS OOOOOOOOOOOOOO ... ......... 0 ..... .... ...... 40 Personal Preference Inventory - Maslow's Needs Hierarchy ................................ 41 Environmental Action Survey ...................... 43 Environmental Commitment or Perception of Amount of Concern Necessary to Take Environmental Actions .......................... 46 Social or Political Action Survey ...... .. ..... ... 49 Terminal Values - Rokeach's Values Survey........ 52 Sample Population ................................ 54 Incentives to Increase Rate of Return ............ 55 Analysis of the Data. ..... ......... ....... ...,... 57 iv Chapter Page 4. RESULTS .................. ..... .......... ......... 64 Research Question 1. ............................. 67 Research Question 2 .............................. 77 Research Question 3 ............................. . 77 Other Pertinent Data ............................. 100 Summary of Results ......................... ...... 111 5. DISCUSSIONOOOOOOOOOOOOO 000000000000 0.0.0.... ..... 117 Relationships between Maslow level and environmental or social/political behavior (Research Question 1)... ...... ... ...... ........ 118 The relationship between Maslow level and Rokeach terminal values (Research Question 2).. 121 Relationships between Rokeach values and environmental or social/political behavior (Research Question 3)........ ....... . .......... 122 Suggestions for future research ............ . ..... 141 APPENDIX A. Research Survey...... ....................... 144 B. Credibility Letters ........ . ................ 160 C. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each environmental action when comparing mean environmental action frequencies among Maslow levels........... 165 D. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each social/political action when comparing mean social/political action frequencies among Maslow levels ........... 173 E. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each environmental action when comparing mean environmental action frequencies among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values..... ..... 177 BIBLIOGRAPHY ..................................... 187 Table 4.7 LIST OF TABLES Page Responses to selected demographic questions (by % Of respondent group church/environmental)...... .............. 66 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for environmental actions l.d and 17.c when comparing mean action frequency among all Maslow levels........ 69 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each Maslow level when comparing mean action frequencies among types of environmental action ...... 7O ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each Maslow level when comparing mean action frequencies among social/political actions ........... 75 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates comparing mean Maslow level among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values .......... . ...... 78 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates comparing weighted environmental score among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values .......................... 80 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each of the seven most common dominant Rokeach terminal values when comparing mean environmental action frequency among all 17 environmental action types ............... 84 vi Table 4.10 4.12 4.13 Page ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates comparing total social/ political score (level of social/ political action) among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values ................................... 90 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each social/political action when comparing mean action frequencies among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values.. 92 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each of the seven most common dominant Rokeach terminal values when comparing mean action frequencies among all social/political actions ....... 97 A comparison of mean scores for Section III (perceived concern or commitment necessary to take environmental action) between environmental groups and church groups.. ..... . ...... ... ........... 101 Ranking of dominant Rokeach terminal values of recyclers in this research..... 107 Simple and multiple stepwise linear regression of the effects of SPS, Rokeach dominant terminal value and Maslow level on WES ...................... 108 WES comparisons of all in each group vs those also claiming affiliation with the other group.. ...... ....... ...... 109 vii Figure 4.1 LIST OF FIGURES Priority rankings for the top six Rokeach Terminal Values of selected groups — arranged in descending order by median score. ................... 103 A comparison of the top six median to dominant Rokeach terminal value scores for environmental and Free Methodist groups - in order of decreasing priority ............. . ........ 105 Second priority values of those choosing salvation as their first priority value - for physiological level vs. self—actualizers......................... 110 Type of env. action vs Rokeach dominant terminal values .............. . ........... 113 Type of social/political action vs dominant Rokeach terminal values......... 115 Potentially effective relationships - to find most effective values to stress for specific actions or actions most likely given specific high priority values................................... 138 viii Chapter 1 I NTRODUCTION With each day's news comes the realization that there is more need and increasing potential for citizen participation in environmental action. This action may take one of many forms, from picking up litter (ecomanagement) to political involvement. This, in fact, is the major Objective of environmental education. As stated by Hungerford and Peyton (1976, p.11), the environmental educator works to "develop an environmentally literate citizenry that is both competent to take action on critical environmental issues and willing to take that action". Since it is a primary goal of environmental educators to work toward increased participation on the part of the citizenry (Stapp, 1979, and Hungerford and Peyton, 1976), it would be advantageous to have a better understanding of the reasons for a person's participation. More specifically, 1 environmental educators should have knowledge of the reasons for a person to initiate a positive environmental action (or behavioral set), as well for the person to complete the action and to continue in similar actions. This research was designed to investigate means to increase the efficiency of environmental communications and environmental education. Various researchers have attempted to relate an individual's environmental orientation to his social, physical, or personality characteristics. Socioeconomic 'status (SES), level of education, political orientation, locus of control, conventionalism, and androgyny are among those traits which have been studied (Arbuthnot, 1977; Burrus-Bammel, 1978; Van Liere and Dunlap, 1980; Borden & Powell, 1983). It is this author's contention that while these factors are important in describing correlations with an individual's environmental orientation, they are not sufficient to give an understanding of the reasons for the orientation. In the Arbuthnot study, psychological variables included personal control, political fascism, self-esteem, and superstition (among others). While this author saw the utility of each of these factors in specific situations, he felt that there was a need for an integrated analysis of personality to be applied to environmental activists (and their less active counterparts). Maddi (1968, p.10) defined personality as "a stable set Of characteristics and tendencies that determine those commonalities and differences in the psychological behavior (thoughts, feelings, actions) of people...". He distinguished between the core and the periphery of personality. To him, core denoted things common to all people, things which disclose the inherent attributes of persons. He stated that core personality exerts an extensive and pervasive influence on behavior. Periphery of personality was that part which was more generally learned than inherent, was more concrete, and was used to explain 'differences among people. One of the personality theories which, according to Maddi, stressed the core of personality was Maslow's basic needs hierarchy. In this study, Maslow's needs hierarchy supplied one of two broad theoretical frameworks for understanding the motivational aspects Of the subjects' personalities. Maslow's theory involves a six step hierarchy in which an individual must fulfill the levels in succession as he approaches self—actualization, the highest level. The second type of personality framework employed in this study was Rokeach’s Terminal Values survey. This was chosen as an example of a survey for important elements in periphery of personality. The elements, Rokeach's 18 terminal values, were more concrete and more specific than Maslow's six needs. Maslow's basic need hierarchy theorizes that a person must proceed in order from physiological necessities like food and shelter (level VI) to the next level, which involved maintaining the security of these necessities (level V). Above this was love — from family or other associates (level IV). Ascending from love, the next steps both deal with esteem. Level III was respect, the esteem of others. Level II was self-respect or self-esteem. Finally, a person might achieve the top level, level I — self-actualization. A further postulate of Maslow is that there is a growth need. Nobody, he stated, is satisfied with any level short of self—actualization. While a person's progress may be halted at any level, there is an overall need to continue to advance toward self—actualization. The self—actualizers are activists. Maslow himself stated that "Self-actualizing people are, without one single exception, involved in a cause outside their own skin, in something outside themselves" (1971, p.43). It could be deduced after reading the description of Maslow's self-actualizers, that environmental action is a very effective means of (or a consequence of) achieving self—actualization. In accord with this possibility, it is expected that some self-actualizers are engaged in positive environmental actions. But environmental action is by no means the only way to achieve self-actualization. Accordingly, any study of behaviors which lead to self—actualization would include the full range of those behaviors, not just the ones in the environmental realm. The persons currently employing other behaviors to achieve self—actualization are considered by this author to be potential environmental activists. Their interests and activities are extremely important. Environmental activism is not limited to self—actualizers. Even the physiological level is affected if an environmental problem causes a direct threat to one's health. The need to maintain one's health might lead to 'activism among even the lowest of Maslow's levels. Harry Miller, an adult educator, supplied the rationale for the other component Of the proposed motivational structure. His adaptation of Lewin's force field analysis (Miller, 1967) helps one to understand why an adult might become involved in an educational activity. This researcher felt that the recognition of forces for such participation was a prerequisite to understanding and using the knowledge in environmental education and environmental appeals. The contribution of Kurt Lewin (1947) was the depiction of a person's actions as a product of positive and negative forces in his/her environment. These forces may be psychological or situational. They may originate from within the person's mind, from the external environment, or from a combination of both. Just as in physics, the sum and direction of all forces on a person determine the resultant. According to Miller, Maslow's construct is essential, but not sufficient. Maslow, he intimates, provides an outline for motivation; Lewin helps fill in the details. Among Miller's conclusions are the following. Working class people, traditionally absent from adult education, are not a part of the educational establishment. In fact, they are represented in very few traditional associations. Labor unions and churches are the only two established organizations normally joined by these persons. Miller stated that the ego needs of middle and ‘upper-middle class persons often result in their enrollment in adult education classes, but they need to perceive progress or accomplishment in order to complete the class. Constructs of this sort are challenging and stimulating, but Miller's work is not accompanied by educational research. For application to environmental education, this should be documented more specifically. There is another instrument which is capable of assessing the same types of forces described by Miller, but which carries the advantages of being completed, validated, and widely used. This is the Rokeach Survey of Terminal Values (Rokeach, 1968). Overall, Rokeach's survey addresses the same constellation of motivators described by Miller, but has much more potential to be used in educational research. Rokeach's terminal values survey is a set of 18 values determined by Rokeach to be the smallest and best representative set of values which would explain a person's desired end states of existence. The derivation of this particular set of values is described more fully in the Methods chapter. Rokeach states that these values both "guide human action in daily situations" and "give expression to basic human needs" (Rokeach, 1968, p.14). They are integrally related with a person's needs and his actions. Another way Rokeach describes this relationship between values and needs is by stating that values are the cognitive representations and transformations of needs. ‘E.C. Tolman echoed this statement when he said that humans have the same set of inherent needs which are satisfied by individuals applying different values, these values being based on their experiences (Cofer and Appley, 1964). By rank ordering Rokeach's 18 values, a person allows an experimenter to better understand both motivation and behavior. There are currently theories of personality and motivation as well as instruments to measure some aspects of personality and motivation. Their application to environmental research shows promise to both understand reasons for a person's actions and to predict or increase these actions. Statement of the Problem This dissertation was an attempt to increase understanding of the relationship between personality and environmental action. More specifically it asked about relationships among Maslow's Needs Hierarchy, Rokeach's Terminal Values, socio—political action, and environmental action. 'Research Questions 1a. Is there a correlation between Maslow level and environmental action (level and type)? 1b. Is there a correlation between Maslow level and socio/political action (level and type)? 2. Is there a relationship between Maslow level and the presence and importance of specific elements of Rokeach's Terminal Values survey? 3a. Is there a relationship between Rokeach's Terminal Values and environmental action (level and type)? 3b. Is there a relationship between Rokeach's Terminal Values and social/political action (level and type)? Sample populations were drawn from two general populations. The first general population consisted of members Of three specific environmental groups - the Sierra Club, the Audubon Society, and the West Michigan Environmental Action Council. The second population was drawn from members of Free Methodist (conservative protestant) churches. This followed the precedent of Arbuthnot (1977) and resulted in a wide range of environmental scores. Administered to each person was a combined test battery including the following: Demographic information Preference Inventory — Maslow level Environmental Activism Scale Perceived Importance of Environmental Actions Social Activism Scale Rokeach's Terminal Values Survey 10 Significance of the Problem As stated previously, the primary reason for undertaking this project was to investigate means to increase the efficiency of environmental communications and environmental education. This study will have implications for the choice of topical material of environmental education for adults. A knowledge of the motivational priorities of certain groups 'of adults will help to determine the curriculum which can meet their most pressing needs in the most efficient manner. Conversely, it should also help to decrease instances of non—enrollment and dropping out due to insufficient motivation or interest. Similarly, teaching methodology can be benefited by a knowledge of the motivational structure of the adult students. Scheduling, study aids, choice of anecdotes, and application examples could all be optimized by such knowledge. Though not widely used in environmental action, Maslow's construct has been applied to the field of political participation quite effectively. Both Knutson (1973) and Renshon (1974) state the need for a unified theory of personality to be used as a basis of understanding, Knutson uses the following images to describe her view: trait 11 studies have provided many pieces of the jigsaw puzzle; Maslow's framework will provide an outline of the political picture. Together they may give significant advances in understanding political behavior. Political advisors use personality information to predict, among other things: 1) the type of leader or platform which will be supported in a particular campaign and by a certain group of voters. 2) the probability that certain individuals will join specified groups 3) the types Of political appeals which will be most effective in stimulating a person's action 4) the function of various political activities (campaigning, parade marching) in an individual's psychic economy. The transition from political to environmental activities is so slight that to restate each point in environmental terms seems redundant. As environmental issues continue to become more political, a motivational assessment of this sort will become increasingly important. Environmentalists, politicians, economists, and educators all are interested in projecting future trends for their respective fields. This study could aid their efforts by giving a unified structure as a basis for prediction. Further, this will allow the prediction of action or behaviors, not just inclination or attitude. Another significant aspect of this study was the fact that it was one of the first studies of its kind to use a 12 personality superstructure in a broad environmental action context. The Arbuthnot (1977) and Maloney & Ward (1973) studies used personality variables but not an integrated personality theory to attempt to categorize individuals in relation to the environment. Dunlap, Grieneeks and Rokeach (1983) used a Maslowian construct, but with only recycling as an indicator of the individual's environmental action. This study, in combination with existing data, will allow increasingly specific and effective communications with all of those persons affected by an environmental situation. Rather than use the same type of appeals for all 'persons, different groups could be targeted with very specific appeals. And, naturally, this should increase the potential for a positive response to the communication. Much of the previous work, then, lacked the combination of integration and specificity which is necessary for the work to be used for other than the initial researcher's intent. It was expected that this research would be the start of a series of projects which had the potential to be more widely applicable. In this study, environmental action was used as the criterion measure for a person's environmental orientation. The author realized that most other studies have employed environmental knowledge or environmental attitude or environmental values to measure environmental orientation. Despite these precedents, this author was convinced that the most valid measure of environmental orientation was its 13 manifestation as environmental action. The literature, abundant though lacking in consensus, suggested that neither knowledge, nor attitudes, nor values can predict environmental behavior with certainty. All of these are useful, but there are many other variables which affect a person's behavior. Since it is the environmental behaviors which were most specifically of interest to this study (and to many environmentalists), it was decided that this study would attempt to census those behaviors. The choice of this particular project also had significance for this researcher. His long—range goals 'include working to further the environmental education, and hopefully the environmental action, of previously non-environmentally-involved persons. Much of the impetus for this came from the paper by Miller (1967) examining adults' participation in education in light of sociologic force-fields. Miller's work was specifically in relation to education; the findings explain what has been observed in adult education programming. Miller's contention that very few public institutions influence the middle and lower—middle class means that the schools (and any educational effort connected with the schools) have very little appeal or credibility to them. Ultimately this author intends to use these institutions to initiate nonformal environmental education. The first step was to determine the personality and 14 motivation superstructures of some of these people in relation to environmental action, social action, and in comparison to a group traditionally found to be environmentally active. In summary, the author felt that currently environmental educators have the ability to ascribe various characteristics to citizens in relation to their environmental orientation, but lack the ability to fully understand (motivationally) or effectively communicate with several previously non-involved groups. The intent of this research is to increase the ability of environmental 'educators to accomplish the latter two tasks, aswell as to make optimal use of the groups which are already involved. Assumptions and Limitations of the Study Despite the enthusiasm with which the project was conceived, it was apparent that there were assumptions and limitations to the study. One assumption was that the information published about the validity and reliability of the Beer and Rokeach instruments is correct. Reliability and validity tests were done on portions of these surveys in the current research to ensure that this research setting did not make the tests invalid or unreliable. There was no assumption that either of the groups 15 tested would be representative of the U.S. population as a whole. Choice of groups was based on three factors. The first of these was precedent — the Arbuthnot study especially. The second was an attempt to census groups of people which would demonstrate a distinct difference in their environmental and personal assessments. And the third factor was the desire to do further work with persons in the environmentally non-involved group. It was also recognized that even among environmental and church groups, the current sample may not be representative. The financial and authority-based 'incentives may have elicited responses from a non—representative sample group. Other criticisms also might be directed toward the study. One involves the choice of the Maslowian theory of personality. The criticisms directed by those who do not subscribe to Maslow's construct cannot be refuted. Any personality theory, by its theoretic nature, has its proponents and opponents. Maslow does not adequately describe dysfunctional personalities; he is most directed toward those who are mentally healthy and those who are self-actualizers. Most persons, the author expects, will view the Maslowian needs hierarchy as at least an adequate model for explaining some aspects of personality. Others, this author included, feel that it is extremely useful as a construct for explaining many Of a person's actions. Miller (1967), Knutson (1973) 16 and Renshon (1974), as already noted, feel that Maslow's framework is valid and valuable for this purpose. Another caution should be added in this section. It involves the confusion of correlation and causation. Those strong correlations which might appear in the assessments do not prove that one element is the cause of another (belonging to a church or to a labor union does not cause one to be less involved in adult education). It is simply intended to convey the idea that in the presence of one element, an observer would have a high probability of finding the correlated element. Although there is the 'possibility that one might cause the other, it is just as likely that a common third element might cause both independently. The value of the correlation lies in its ability to predict one element given the presence or absence of another. Despite these potential limitations, the author felt that the study had great importance in increasing understanding of the interaction of personality and environmental action. From this understanding, the author expects, will come more effective environmental communications and environmental action. 17 Definitions of Important Terms Ambition — a status or condition strongly desired or sought after Appeal — an earnest request for some sort of thought or action, a solicitation; used especially in relation to communications or advertising Attitude — a hypothetical construct about a mental state which is inferred from verbal reports and behavioral observation (Heberlein, 1980) Belief - the cognitive component of an attitude (the other component is affective - an emotional one) (Herberlein, 1980) Environmentally noncommited (or environmentally noninvolved) — the state of not considering or valuing a positive environmental aspect in thoughts and actions Environmental orientation — the sum of an individual’s knowledge, perceptions, values, actions, beliefs, and attitudes toward the environment Formal education — schooling; that portion of education 18 which is deliberate, planned, staffed, financially supported, and has sequentially programmed coursework. It usually involves certification, prerequisites, and accreditation (see also: nonformal education) (Ward, 1982) Goals — something which a person wants, that for which an effort is made Knowledge — that which is known and understood, whether from books and teachers or from experience and Observation Motivation — the process by which the expression Of a behavior is determined, or the force which arouses activity, sustains the activity in progress, and regulates the pattern of the activity (Cofer and Appley, 1964) Motivational status - used specifically in this study to indicate the assessment of a person’s placement according to Maslow's hierarchy of needs Nonformal education - that portion of education which is deliberate, planned, staffed, financially supported, but which adds the qualifications of usually being short—term, highly functional, and unrestricted in time and place. In general, it is more responsive to change and need than is formal education (Ward, 1982) 19 Personality — a stable set of characteristics and tendencies that determine those commonalities and differences in the pshycological behavior (thoughts, feelings, actions) of people that have continuity in time and that may or may not be easily understood in terms of social and biological pressures of the immediate situation alone (Maddi,1968) Positive environmental action — any conscious action or behavior which benefits some aspect of the environment. This could include conservation as well as ameliorative actions. Social action — any behavior which is not consciously or directly designed to benefit the individual taking the action, but which instead benefits others Value - a hypothetical construct, an enduring belief about "desired end states of existence" such as freedom or equality, a standard for evaluating or guiding attitudes and actions. Values serve as the basis for attitudes and beliefs (Rokeach, 1968), and (Heberlein, 1980). Chapter 2 L I TERATURE REVI EW An analysis of literature pertaining to adult 'environmental action opens a diverse—appearing collection of information. Yet the many facets all may be related to the central theme: involvement of informed adults in positive environmental action. This echoes a superordinate goal of environmental education: "to aid citizens in becoming enviromentally knowledgeable and, above all, skilled and dedicated citizens who are willing to work, individually and collectively, toward achieving and/or maintaining a dynamic equilibrium between quality of life and quality of the environment."(Harvey, 1976) This review of the literature will focus on the following topics: 1) a survey of environmental education curricula with regard to adults. 2) adult environmental action, including relationships 20 21 to attitudes, knowledge, concern, and values 3) adult education 4) applications: adult participation and potential in environmental programs A Survey Of Environmental Education Curricula with regard to Adults Any survey Of the literature would show that the majority of environmental education programming has taken place in schools, with the programming directed toward 'youth. Stapp et al. (1971) described the major effort of environmental education as being in the realm of formal elementary and secondary education. They reported that several colleges and universities have also adopted environmental education, but that all of these formal programs taken together "reach only a small percentage of the general public." The majority of potential target audiences are not in schools — they are in the nonformal sector. Johnson et al (1980) defined the issue more clearly. Using the analogy of a marketplace, they stated that youth has been the primary consumer. The logistic structure Of the whole elementary, secondary, and post—secondary educational system has been oriented toward youth — in timing, location, and even the placement of summer vacation. According to their marketplace analogy, the field Of adult 22 learning is an “untapped market". In numbers of potential students, resources available to students, and need for education, adults are becoming and will become increasingly important to the educational system (Johnson et al,1980) In an inventory of environmental education curricula, Childress (1978) determined that the students primarily affected by environmental education programming were in two groups. The first group was from grades 5 and 6, the second group from grades 10-12. No mention was made of adult curricula. Clearly environmental education has been characterized 'by the emphasis on formal education and young learners. Yet Harvey's superordinate goal of environmental education (as stated previously) suggests a broader emphasis. Age is not mentioned. Instead the emphasis is on the development of knowledgeable, skilled, and dedicated citizens. While this does not preclude youth, it implies the inclusion of adults especially. Further, an analysis of the conceptual level of the specific goals of environmental education (Hungerford, Peyton, and Wilke, 1980) makes it questionable whether younger students could even comprehend all of the goals of environmental education. 23 Adult Environmental Action, includingirelationships to attitudes, knowledge, concern, and values Since the rationale for this dissertation is to increase adult environmental action, the following section will analyze the status of environmental action, especially in relation to adults. To document the need for environmental action might seem simplistic, but it is no less essential. Jean Matthews, a coordinator for the U.S. Department of the ’Interior (1974), stated the need for a populace that not only understands ecology and environmental problems, but which is also able to act on the basis of that understanding. Educational institutions can have a role in bringing about informed environmental action by "organizing and conducting educational programs for adults which keep them informed before controversy sets in, to offer unbiased information when it is needed, and to referee conservation conflicts when appropriate" (Dambach, 1969). But, as stated by Stapp (1971), there are "few programs (which) emphasize the role Of the citizen in working both individually and collectively, toward the solution of problems...." in the environment. A more recent review of environmental literature shows that the situation has not changed significantly. ,As reviewed by Johnson et a1 (1980), the environmental 24 awareness and action programs are few; the successful ones are even fewer. There seems to be a common problem in attracting and maintaining community participation in any environmental program. It should be noted, however, that the same review found that those programs emphasizing awareness rather than action were more plentiful, more diverse, and more successful. A review of "Research in Enviromental Education" 1971-1980 showed that less than 9% of the 429 environmental studies used overt behavior as a criterion (Hungerford et al, 1983). Of the papers which did report behavioral 'analyses, more than a third Of them used littering as the only behavior studied. The authors Of the review lamented that dissertation topics seldom include citizenship behaviors. One of the most concise descriptions of environmental action was offered by Hungerford and Peyton (1976) and later modified by Champeau (1982). They divided the realm of environmental action into 6 categories, each with an operational definition. The six categories are the following: Persuasion, Consumerism, Political Action, Legal Action, Ecomanagement, and Interaction (involving any two or more of the preceding action modes). Champeau (1982) modified "Consumerism", making it "Economic Action" in order to allow a more comprehensive treatment of potentially influential monetary actions. Means to bring this type of commitment into public and 25 educational reality are not as widespread as one would hope. Peyton and Hungerford (1980) showed that school teachers in three midwestern states perceived that they had "limited abilities to use" environmental action skills. While they stated that they felt confident to teach environmental action skills, few teachers reported that they had used them. Environmental action often has been a studied variable in combination with one or several of the following: attitudes, values, beliefs, and knowledge. The literature relating behaviors, attitudes, values, beliefs, and 'knowledge can be described most charitably as "complex“. Because of the lack of operationalized and standardized definitions of terms, results of similar types of studies are often interpreted to give what appear to be conflicting conclusions. The following section will review some of the more closely related studies. Perhaps the most common conclusion is that there is some relation of attitude and knowledge. Burrus-Bammel (1978) and Moore (1981), report that attitude and knowledge are directly correlated. In two separate studies, (1976, 1978) Weigel documents his contention that there is a strong correlation between attitudes and behavior. Heberlein and Black (1976) corroborate this, at least to the point of stating that specific attitudes correlate with behaviors better than general attitudes. Heberlein and Black, in a 1981 paper, show that when attitudes are supported within a consistent more likelj Buttel and stating th of attitud both know] in an inde concludes knowledge (Ramsey a: 0the between k result of Significa attitudes affected an envir< led to a Problem) Environm landmark With the StUdies attitude overt b‘ t0 am. in the 26 consistent framework of other related attitudes, they are more likely to be significant predictors of behavior. Buttel and Flinn (1978) also support this contention, stating that education is the primary factor in development of attitudes. Borden and Schettino (1979) conclude that both knowledge and attitudes are predictors of behavior, but in an independent, additive, sense. Yet another study concludes that knowledge moderates attitudes, with increased knowledge preventing extreme attitudes in either direction (Ramsey and Rickson, 1976). Other researchers have failed to find a correlation 'between knowledge and attitudes. Horsley reported that as a result of a role—playing experiment his subjects showed a significant behavior change with no similar change in attitudes (1977). A study of environmental issues in highly affected communities found that knowledge of all facets of an environmental issue and its possible solutions most often led to a resistance to change (resistance to solving the problem), indicating that knowledge is unrelated to positive environmental attitudes (Tichenor et al, 1971). Wicker's landmark 1969 review of attitudes and actions summarizes with the following statement, "Taken as a whole these studies suggest that it is considerably more likely that attitudes will be unrelated or only slightly related to overt behaviors than that attitudes will be closely related to actions." For these reasons, "attitude" was not included in the current study. Anothe “environmer for "envir< findings i1 variables, locus of c environmen Age and so correlated studied tt that age, environmer and occup: with cone. Envi Concerns, few resea to any 0f Social 0! The! enVironme COIIEge : betWeen . queStion environm Van 27 Another well-researched, but usually undefined term is "environmental concern". The lack of a standard definition for "environmental concern" may be related to divergent findings in research. Tucker (1978) found that among social variables, income, social class, and internal vs. external locus of control were most highly correlated with I environmental concern (although he did not define concern). Age and social responsibility were not significantly correlated with concern. But Van Liere and Dunlap (1980) studied the social bases of environmental concern and found that age, education and ideology were the best predictors of 'environmental concern. Social class, place of residence, and occupational prestige were not significantly correlated with concern. Environmental behaviors are the translation of beliefs, concerns, attitudes, or values into reality. Yet relatively few researchers have studied behaviors of adults in relation to any of the previously mentioned variables or any other social or personal criteria. There is one study linking environmental concern with environmental action. Dunlap et al (1973) found among college students a direct and significant correlation between their concern measure and positive response to a question on whether the subject had ever taken any environmentally positive action. Van Liere and Dunlap (1978) reported that both awareness Of consequences and ascription of responsibility are import: behavior, : Attit environmen (1977). I quality we the verbal relations] of pro—en' knowledge control ( Sia literatul influencq the most Compare environm Sensitiv action. Predict: Classif; technoL Du framewO were pr Values‘ "highs! 28 are important in being able to predict a pro—environmental behavior, i.e. refraining from burning yard wastes. Attitude and personality variables which predict environmental behaviors were directly studied by Arbuthnot (1977). It was found that commitment to environmental quality was more verbal and affective than behavioral. Both the verbal and the affective commitments had a moderate relationship to environmental behaviors. Other predictors of pro—environmental behaviors were education, environmental knowledge, conservatism (negative), and lack of personal control (negative). Sia et a1 (1983) surveyed the environmental action literature for all variables which had been shown to influence environmental action. In order to find which were the most powerful predictors, they designed a study to compare them. They found that perceived skill in using environmental action strategies and level of environmental sensitivity were the best predictors of environmental action. Those variables found to have less importance as predictors were locus of control, psychological sex role classification, and belief in/attitude towards pollution and technology. Dunlap et al (1983) used a Maslowian personality framework as their theoretic perspective. Maslow's levels were proposed to be related to some of Rokeach's Terminal Values. Certain Rokeach terminal values were categorized as "higher order" in the Maslow hierarchy while other terminal values were Their expe values in and a Nati chosen adu recyclers with theil shows the environme: EM Tl First, 9‘ esPecial The next if and w been env toward a analYZe especia: The IaS' recomme adults 0n adult 1 29 values were classed as "lower order" in the hierarchy. Their experimental design involved a comparison of Rokeach's values in members of two groups - a known group of recyclers and a National Opinion Research Center group of randomly chosen adults. The results supported their hypothesis that recyclers would show "higher order" values when compared with their randomly chosen counterparts. Their research shows the efficacy of Rokeach's Values Instrument in an environmental action context. Adult Education The organization of this section will be as follows. First, general characteristics of adults will be examined, especially in relation to the environment and to education. The next topic will be the political potential of adults — if and why they participate. Recognizing that there have been environmental education programs oriented directly toward adults in the past, the author will next describe and analyze some of these programs. The focus will be directed especially toward the success or failure of the programs. The last portion of this section will examine recommendations of the State of Michigan in relation to adults and the environment. One of the earliest modern educators to rediscover the adult learner was Malcom Knowles. In the time since the adult-orier Confucious word "scho' Knowles ap children, adults. A applicatic rewarded 1 the educa' In " Education adults, I QIOUP whi Immediatt intended Which an environm administ adults. Th1 K—lz cu Educati This we schooli descrit Studies "skill: 30 adult-oriented education of Aristotle, Jesus, and Confucious, education has become almost synonymous with the word "school", which nearly invariably implies children. Knowles applied the term "pedagogy" to the education of children, contrasting this with andragogy — the education of adults. Adults, he said, prefer short—term, specific, application-based learning. He recommended that they be rewarded for those experiences and abilities they bring to the education system (Knowles, 1978). In "Toward a National Strategy for Environmental Education", Stapp et a1 (1971) do not write only about ladults, but they do mention adults very specifically as a group which should be targeted by environmental education. Immediately after this recommendation, they identify another intended target — specific occupational or social groups which are especially influential in relation to the environment, such as engineers, policy makers, and administrators. This group is specifically limited to adults. The emphasis on adult life is even reflected in the K—12 curriculum. In 1978, the Michigan State Department of Education published the Life Role Competencies document. This was the culmination of an attempt to base formal schooling on the needs of the adult world. Rather than describe learning outcomes as "reading skills" or "social studies skills", the skills listed in the document.are the "skills that we all presumably need to have to function well" in a vocational demonstrat 1) a basis for 2) a environmel and 3) p« envrronme' When there wil then, adu influenti basis of Educatiol Th Environm Pertaini a lifelc System, enVironr Islated Permeat. (Berry 31 well" in all the areas of personal, family, social, and vocational life. For example, students should be able to demonstrate the following abilities: 1) an ability to use the natural environment as a basis for creative expression, 2) an awareness of alternative solutions to major environmental problems, and 3) personal responsibility for the quality of the environment. When and if this type of curriculum is implemented, .there will be less need for remediation among adults. Until then, adults will remain in most cases as a large group of influential citizens making environmental decisions on the basis of limited information (Michigan Department of Education, 1978). The 1977 Tbilisi Intergovernmental Conference on Environmental Education made the following recommendations pertaining to adult education: "Environmental education is a lifelong process, not confined to the formal education system, integrating education concerning the work environment, education for the consumer, and education related to economic development; its subject matter should permeate every part of the formal and non-formal programs." (Berry and Lowe, 1978). Applicatio Env1ronmen Anotl participai adults cal address t Education that the had matur quality, System (I Lar 0f Madis Citizens environ“ responde factors Politic: is high. Re Partici be effe e“Viror "Potent elevat: the po. 32 Application: Adult Potential and Participation In Environmental Prggrams Another reason for the emphasis on adults is their participation in politics. By any of a number of means, adults can influence the legislative process. In his address to the 1980 National Association of Environmental Education conference, Clay Schoenfeld shared his optimism that the environmental activists of the 1960's and 1970's had matured and, while remaining committed to environmental quality, were working more effectively through the political 'system (1980). Larson et a1 (1981) polled a random sample of residents of Madison, Wisconsin and found a significant number of citizens had participated in some form of political environmental activism. Approximately half of the respondents indicated that they had used environmental factors as at least one,criterion in their choice of political candidates. As the authors stated, however, this is higher than the national norm. Regardless of current levels of political participation, any increase in political participation would be effective in helping to accomplish the ends of environmentalists. The key word in this case is "potential". Voting along environmental lines, or even elevating environmental concerns to an "issue" status, has the potential to show positive results. A re‘ helped to undertake The a college local prc solutions college/< publicat: could ha‘ particip citizens Programm in great trust ir college and Suri A : Kupchel (with a environ 5998 on inSpira progran titles CYClesl 33 A review of three adult-oriented environmental programs helped to understand both the type of programs already undertaken and the role of motivation in those programs. The goals of the first program (which was presented as a college course) were to focus the community's attention on local problems and to involve the community in the problems' solutions. The project's results were positive in terms of college/community relations and with regard to the resulting publications and proposals, but there were some areas which could have been improved. The first of these was community 'participation. The authors described the attendance by citizens as "poor", recommending that in the future programming be worked through local groups. This may result in greater participation due to citizens having greater trust in the organization, less anxiety than being in the college setting, and more community input to design (Davis and Surls, 1975). A second project of a similar nature was completed by Kupchella and Levy (1975). In this case, it was a course (with a syllabus) designed to further educate environmentalists about environmental problems. This author sees one noteworthy criticism and a motivational stroke of inspiration which should be considered for future adult programming. In the published course schedule, lectures had titles like "Fundamental Principles Governing Biogeochemical Cycles" and "Fundamental Principles of Interspecies Interactic that sort non-colle« descripti sessions. Ther device us the parti had gotte scholars] grant, e cover th scholars the obli all have partici; In environ1 were he (genera TWO wor attenda The aut c0mmuni in Par‘ M8. by thi 34 Interaction in Nature". In this writer's Opinion, titles of that sort would cause high anxiety levels among non—college—trained ecologists. Subtitles or simplified descriptions may have led to higher attendance at those sessions. There was, however, a very effective motivational device used in this study. This dealt with program costs to the participant. A tuition-based program the previous year had gotten little response. The newer program offered scholarships. Since the project was funded by a federal grant, each student was "given a scholarship" to totally 'cover the program costs. The prestige of getting a scholarship, the knowledge of the cost of the course, and the obligation engendered by receiving a scholarship would all have worked together to maintain a high motivation for participation. In the third project, a different approach was used by environmental educators in southern Ohio. Here, workshOps were held dealing with the local air quality problems (generated in large measure by the local steel industry). Two workshops were offered, but a significant decrease in attendance was evident in the second one - from 66 to 29. The authors did not speculate on reasons for a poor community response, but it is suspected that it may be due in part to the nature of the program - to increase understanding of the air pollution problem (emphasis added by this writer). A problem/solution orientation may have been more It s optimum u learners. and learn factors. recommenc interact: that mort educatio motivati conclusi environn W Th bringin environ enlists Sectior enlistj While l POssib P0ssib I EnViro 35 been more effective (Force et al, 1977). It seems that none of the preceding programs makes Optimum use of what was already known about motivating adult learners. Gallo (1971) points out that adult participation and learning depend to a great extent on motivational factors. He analyzed drop—outs, then devised a set of recommendations dealing with areas like scheduling, costs, interaction style, and curriculum design. His thesis was that more adults would begin and successfully finish adult education programming if more attention were paid to motivation. This coincides with this author’s own 'conclusions based on the preceding review of selected environmental education programs. Recommendations of other Researchers for Increased Action The environmental educator's primary tasks include bringing adults to a state where their positive environmental actions can be predicted, encouraged, or enlisted. Various predictors were discussed in the previous section; this section deals with methods for encouraging or enlisting the support of adults in environmental programs. While the literature tells only of previous programs, it is possible to make some inferences about what might be possible in the future and how it might be accomplished. In "Notes on Coordinating a Community Program in Environmental Education", the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (1977) desc education p environment Recommendat 1. ma formalizing 2. de 3. se by all 4. pr All of thee 0f educatic mOtivationa Miller dealing Wit interPretat include non non-school pr°9ramming There f°r eff8ctj review Papa that Writte onsubjECts ratiOnaleS 36 (1977) describes all aspects of adult environmental education programming for people desiring to become environmental education program coordinators. Recommendations include the following: 1. make every effort to "legitimize" the program by formalizing it into local organizations 2. design the program to meet specific needs 3. set short-range goals so that results will be seen by all 4. publicize the program and its results. All of these recommendations reflect a practical synthesis of educational techniques, communications theory, and motivational psychology. Miller (1967) included several recommendations for dealing with people in educational settings. Based on his interpretation of force fields, these recommendations include non-formal approaches to education and possible non—school institutional vehicles for educational programming. There is very little literature analyzing the reasons for effectiveness of environmental communications. Hines’ review paper (1983) reports eight studies which all reveal that written or verbal appeals have had little or no effect on subjects' behaviors. Analysis of the appeals and their rationales has not been reported. Some programs described in the lite rudimentary A Wisc local high into local from univer to inform a The Abeles included re integration complete su The Ku response ra known to be for Environ 9r°uPS, the In a broade thr°ugh ins These dearth of s that thEre supporting c°1lllllunicati design text BIOOks, 197 would haVe rIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII‘:____________________—f 37 in the literature include enough information that a rudimentary analysis is possible. A Wisconsin program described by Abeles (1973) used local high school students to bring the university program into local reality. The high school students (with help from university students and faculty) undertook a campaign to inform and involve the community of the proposed program. The Abeles report, however, was a progress report and included recommendations for even better communication and integration with the community. Evidently it was not a complete success in the communications realm. The Kupchella-Levy paper (1975) had one of the best response rates, but did so by enlisting people previously known to be ideologically similar. By offering an "Ecology for Environmentalists" course through local environmental groups, they maximized their program's chance for success. In a broader sense they showed again the efficacy of working through institutions already established in the community. These studies have used communications tools, but the dearth of specific references in the literature suggests that there has not been a strong focused approach toward supporting environmental programs by means of a strong communicatione/motivation basis. Even a general message design text like Public Communications (Hart, Friedrich and Brooks, 1975) or Persuasion (Karlins and Abelson, 1970) would have been a great asset to a program designer. Summary From t it was seen action—orie literature predictors realm of tr There personality however, ge environment 'h~-.person in environm When a analyzed, 1 Principles in their be Was demonst and more sp knowledge t action. All of need to 11nd relation to framewOrk. ability to 38 Summary From the survey of environmental education curricula, it was seen that there is a scarcity of and a need for more action-oriented adult environmental education programs. The literature on environmental action showed some correlative predictors of environmental action, but all were in the realm of trait studies rather than personality frameworks. There exist useful frameworks for understanding adult personality and motivation. These frameworks are not, however, generally used for environmental action or lenvironmental education. As stated in Hines (1983, p. 28) "....personality factors have not been widely investigated in environmental action studies in nonformal settings." When adult environmental education programs were analyzed, it was seen that in general they did not apply the principles of adult education or adult motivation, resulting in their being less than optimally effective. The potential was demonstrated for more efficient educational programming and more specific use of personality and motivation knowledge to make more effective appeals for environmental action. All of these observations, taken together, indicate a need to understand environmental action or non—action in relation to an individual's personality and motivation framework. With the increased understanding comes the ability to use that knowledge in the most efficient way — increasing pa involved and involved. Tl understanding personality a 39 increasing participation levels among those not currently involved and maximizing effectiveness for those already involved. This study is an attempt to provide the increased understanding of environmental action or non-action in a personality and motivation context. Thi instrume well as measure in detai Th: Tl respom incone Chapter 3 METHODS This research necessitated the development of some new instruments to assess environmental and social action, as well as adaptation and use of two other instruments to measure personality variables. All of these are described in detail in this chapter. The survey consisted of the following six sections: Demographic Information Preference Inventory - Maslow's Needs Hierarchy Environmental Action Concern or Commitment necessary to take Environmental Action Social and Political Action Rokeach's Terminal Values The Demographic section (six questions) asked respondents to indicate level of education, occupation type, income level, membership in an environmental organization, 40 residence remaining A copy 01 Personal The responds adaptati Ohio St: the phy: exclusi industr researc that th well. constrt researc B: aPProx litera reCOgn PSYCho into N de£im‘ in a , retail 41 residence category (urban, suburban, rural), and sex. The remaining five sections are next described in more detail. A copy of the entire survey is included as Appendix 1. Personal Preference Inventory - Maslow's Needs Hierarchy The part of the survey designed to assess the respondent's level on Maslow's needs hierarchy was an adaptation of one used by Michael Beer (1966, Copyright, The Ohio State University). Beer's inventory did not measure the physiological level of Maslow's model, probably a safe ‘exclusion since he was surveying middle management in an industrial setting. It was expected that the current research would include all Maslow levels, so it was decided that the physiological level would have to be censused as well. The following paragraphs will deal first with Beer's construction of the instrument, then with the current researcher's addition of physiological level responses. Beer designed the survey by first compiling a list of approximately 100 needs from psychological and sociological literature. Examples of such needs are the need for social recognition or the need to express one's creativity. Seven psychologists were asked to group the items from the list into Maslow categories, according to Maslow's own definitions of the categories. Each item which was classed in a category by at least six of the seven judges was retained for the final form of the inventory. The final 42 form was then submitted to eight other judges for grouping according to item desirability. Inter—item reliabilities were computed by Kuder-Richardson Formula 8 for each of the levels, and ranged from .67 to .74. In general, items at the extremes of the scale showed the greatest negative correlation (Beer, 1966). Beer also reported evidence that the instrument showed construct validity. When "autonomy" was correlated with "freedom of action" as a perceived job need, the results were significant (P<.01). When "self-actualization" was correlated with the perceived job need "initiative", the 'results were significant (P<.02) (Beer, 1966). By showing the strong correlation between Maslow's theoretic categories and workers' perceived job needs (which would have been predicted by the Maslow model), Beer strengthened his contention that his instrument was a valuable predictor of Maslow's theoretic needs hierarchy. The addition of physiological level responses was done in a similar manner. The researcher first found information about the physiological level and its characteristics. Next, this researcher and one other social science researcher composed statements to use as examples of physiological level needs (e.g. having to be concerned with whether there will be food available for supper). These statements were submitted to a panel of three psychologists for revision. Their revisions were edited to form.the final part of the Maslow level survey and were added to the 43 existing instrument. Scoring was done as on Beer's original survey. Respondents were asked to indicate their preferences in order by using numbers one (most preferred) to six (least preferred). The values assigned to each of the questions representing a given level (e.g. physiological level) were summed. When scores had been computed for each level, the level having the lowest score was considered to be the one most preferred by the respondent. This was interpreted as that individual's Maslow level. Environmental Action Survey The survey of environmental action behaviors was based on a model proposed by Hungerford and Peyton, (1976), and later refined by Champeau (1982). Using the categories of action from Peyton/Champeau and some of their examples, the author designed more questions with the intent of including examples of actions in each category. The rationale for assigning sublevels to each category was based on the premise that one action might have several different possible motivational bases. In the example which follows, it was seen as a possibility that picking up litter at home might be motivated by a sense of civic pride, domestic duty, peer pressure, financial incentive,.or commitment to the environment. Addition of sublevels - 44 other litter pickup situations — was intended to allow the researcher to separate someone who picks up litter out of environmental commitment from someone who does so due to other motivations. A person motivated by peer pressure might pick up litter around his home and neighborhood, but not as consistently in public or commercial areas. The sublevels were intended to reduce the likelihood that non—environmentally-motivated persons would be classed with those strongly environmentally motivated. An example of one such question (in the "ecomanagement" Icategory) with sublevels follows. Within the past year, how Often have you 1. Picked up any type of litter - a. at home b. in your neighborhood __ not at all __ not at all __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally __ occasionally frequently __ frequently 45 c. in public areas d. in commercial areas (parks, etc.) (parking lots, shopping centers) __ not at all __ not at all __ seldom __ seldOm __ occasionally __ occasionally __ frequently ___frequently Scoring of the environmental action questions was done with a four-response scale (Not at all = 0; Seldom = 1; ' Occasionally = 2; Frequently = 3). This allowed summation of the scores for total environmental action — Total Environmental Score or TES. To obtain evidence of content validity for the environmental action part of the survey, the researcher used an accepted outline (Hungerford and Peyton, 1976) to define the domain of the environmental action content as well as topics from three other research surveys for specific questions (Sia et a1, 1983; Champeau, 1982; and Larson, 1981). This researcher and the major professor were the final authorities on the content and syntax for each of the survey items. From the theoretic basis of the environmental action survey it was expected that members of environmental organizations would score higher on environmental action items than persons who were not members of such 46 organizations. The pilot study supported this expectation; when TES was compared between members of environmental organizations and members of conservative churches, the results showed a significant difference (t = 4.50, d.f. = 21, P<.01). A Cronbach's alpha computation for inter-item reliability of the survey results showed an alpha value of .946. Together, these factors indicate that the environmental action part of the survey both represents the existing knowledge of environmental action and shows a positive relationship with membership in environmental action groups. This supports the contention that the Environmental ' Action Survey does measure degree of environmental action. Environmental Commitment or Perception of Amount of Concern Necessary to Take Environmental Actions As the construction of the survey became more defined, it was noted that it would not be possible to determine with certainty a respondent's commitment to environmental action. Despite the request that only those actions done "at least pgrtially because of your concern for the environment" be included in the survey, it was expected that some respondents might include actions done for non—environmental reasons. This expectation was supported by Hines' review of environmental action research (1983). To better determine the commitment, another section, the 47 Environmental Commitment/Concern Scale asked each respondent to indicate on a three—part scale how much environmental commitment it would take for an individual to complete any of the actions in the preceding section. Responses were scored as follows: very much = 1, a moderate amount = 2, and very little = 3. Evidence for reliability and validity of this section came from several sources. Reliability was measured by Cronbach's alpha computation for inter-item variability. The alpha value for this section of the survey was .80, indicating a high level of reliability. The environmental education literature has precedent for asking persons to indicate their concern. Dillman and Christenson (1972) asked respondents to rank community problems according to their perceptions of the extent of the problem. Dunlap et a1 (1973) used a "Concern for Environmental Rights" scale asking respondents to indicate in selected environmental situations their perceptions of primacy of interests e.g. the good of the environment, individual rights, industrial needs. In these cases, persons answered reliably and consistently with other theoretic constructs when asked about environmental concern. Validity of this section comes from this precedent as well as the fact that this form of question was recommended by a panel of professionals in environmental education and adult education. This researcher and the major professor concluded that, within the limitations which apply to any % self-report of attitudes, asking persons to report on their 48 perceptions of amount of commitment necessary to take such actions was a valid question. Validity of each of the "content" portions of the questions (e.g. recycling, petitioning) has already been established in an earlier section. On the bases of these factors, it was decided that the section measuring perceived commitment or concern necessary to take environmental actions was both reliable and valid. Weighted Environmental Score The environmental commitment measure was used to compute a weighted score for environmental action (Weighted Environmental Score or WES). The value for WES was obtained by multiplying the commitment score for an action category by the environmental action score for that category, then summing all categories. The following is an example of weighting: a person who felt that picking up litter required little commitment to the environment would receive a lower WES than a person who felt that it took a great deal of commitment, even if they had picked up the same amount of litter in the same situations. In effect, this weighted more heavily the actions of the individual who felt committed to an environmental action. 49 Social or Political Action Survey In designing this study, it was understood that non—environmental actions might account for a large part of the variance which explains how a person invests motivational energies. The general motivational and personality surveys may indicate one of several types of behaviors, among them environmental action and other social/political actions. Maslow (1954, p. 211) writes that self—actualizers are "strongly focused on problems outside themselves". ' Environmental problems would be one example of a focal point of this type, but persons involved in other (non-environmental) social or political actions would be another group who might have a similar motivational and personality profile. For these reasons, another part of the research survey was designed which would allow the researcher to determine levels or types of non-environmental social or political action. This portion of the survey specifically excluded any such actions in the environmental realm. Understanding a person's social/political activism will help in two ways. First, it will explain a part of the variance in predicting the behavioral results of a person's personality profile. And second, it will help in either motivating a non-involved person toward action, or in moving someone who is socially or politically active but not 50 currently involved in environmental action toward being more environmentally active. Since no model for social and political action categories was found, the Peyton/Champeau model for categories of environmental action was applied to other social or political actions. Using questions generated by the use of this model, the researcher asked three professional social scientists to independently review and edit the list of possible actions. When this was finished, it was compiled, revised, and sent to four other researchers in the social science area for their evaluation. On the ' basis of their comments, the form of the social/political part of the survey was finalized. This process was designed to achieve content validity. The researcher attempted to establish construct validity by beginning with the fact that those at high Maslow levels and at the "higher" Rokeach levels are concerned with non-self issues such as the "good of mankind in general"(Maslow, 1954, p.211). They would be expected to show a high degree of commitment to such causes. While either environmental or social/political action would be capable of satisfying such concerns, it is likely that such individuals would not confine their actions to any single realm. Many of these persons would be likely to be involved in both types of actions, in which case involvement in one type would be related to involvement in the other. One line of 51 support for this comes from Tucker (1978), who found a significant positive relationship between a social responsibility scale and membership in an environmental organization. Analysis of the current research showed that when persons in environmental groups were compared to those from conservative churches, the environmental groups were significantly more involved in social/political action (t = 3.88, d.f. = 162, P<.01). This helps establish construct validity for the social/political action survey. Inter—item reliability of the instrument was tested with Cronbach's alpha test. The value of alpha was .876, ' indicating a high level of reliability. The preceding paragraphs have shown that the social/political action survey was based on an accepted action model (from the environmental realm), that it was revised and edited by professionals in the field, that it corroborated Tucker’s finding about persons being involved in at least two general altruistic behavior sets, and that it showed high inter-item reliability. These indicate that the instrument had sufficiently high reliability and validity to be used in the current study. Scoring of the social/political action survey also used a four—part scale (never, seldom, Often, frequently). Values from 0 (never) to 3 (frequently) were assigned in tabulating results. As with the environmental action portion, the total social/political action score for all actions or scores for individual actions could be used in analysis. 52 The social/political action survey was not designed to be used in a weighted sense as was the environmental action survey. There was no corresponding commitment/concern survey. The first reason for this is the similarity between the environmental action and the social/political action portions of the survey. Of the 19 items on the social/political action survey, 11 had counterparts on the environmental action survey. These matched items could be used if one wished to learn of perceived amounts of concern necessary to undertake the actions in question. A second reason was the desire to keep the size of the total survey as ' small as possible; a separate concern/commitment section for the other social/political realms was not judged to be a critical variable. Further support for not weighting the social/political items came with the results and will be explained more in the discussion section. Terminal Values — Rokeach's Values Survey The final portion of the survey was designed to census personality variables. The form used in this study was the Rokeach Terminal Values Survey (1972). It required respondents to assign priorities to 18 different terminal values, such as Freedom, Self-respect, Wisdom, and A Sense of Accomplishment. These values are a person's "conception of 53 something that is personally or socially preferable" (Rokeach, 1973, p. 10). This forced rank procedure discerned the respondent's preeminent value, and allowed analysis for constellations of very important values. In ranking, respondents were asked to assign first priority to the most important value, eighteenth to the least important value. The Rokeach instrument was designed with both theoretical and empirical bases. Noting that theorists have postulated that between two and twenty-eight terminal values exist, Rokeach derived a list of values within that range I using sociological and psychological values literature. He then increased the validity of his list by interviewing a group of 30 graduate students in psychology and a random sample of 100 adults in Lansing (MI) about their values. The two lists were matched, then reduced by eliminating those judged to be synonymous (e.g. peace of mind and inner harmony) or highly correlated (the correlation between ratings of salvation and unity with God was over .80). The resulting list, composed of values which appeared in the literature and in interviews, was reduced to 18 items. These items compose the values list that is both smallest in number and most inclusive (Robinson and Shaver, 1973). Several of the values measured by the Rokeach survey correlate highly with the behaviors they would be expected to indicate. Equality is the value most predictive of becoming involved in civil rights activities. Salvation is the value 54 which best predicts church attendance. And World of Beauty best discriminates between artists and those in other professional groups (Rokeach, 1973). Rokeach also reported a median test-retest reliability score of .76 after two to four months, and .69 after fourteen to sixteen months (1973). The instrument appears to be reliable, even over a relatively long time span. Distribution of the Survey Sample Population The sample populations were drawn from members of Michigan citizens' environmental organizations (N - 171) and from members of Free Methodist (conservative protestant) churches (N = 232) in Michigan. The church group was from churches in Monroe (n=36), Davison (n=88), Boyne City (n=26), Watervliet (n=16), and Troy (n=66). There were three different environmental groups sampled, the Sierra Club, the Audubon Society, and the West Michigan Environmental Action Council (WMEAC). Specifically, surveys were sent to officers of the Michigan Sierra Club (n=17), and to selected members of the Saginaw Valley (n27), Traverse Bay (n=19), and Wakelin—McNeil (n=15) Groups. Other surveys were 55 sent to members of the Jackson (MI) Audubon Society (n=68), and to members of the WMEAC (n=45). In none of these organizations would the charter or bylaws allow wholesale distribution of the mailing lists. In order to circumvent this rule, the researcher contacted the head of the group involved. These individuals were persuaded to send a list of acquaintances who belonged to the organization. The only selection criterion was that the individual be active in the organization (as opposed to being only a name on a mailing list). This served the purpose of gaining the researcher access to names and addresses of ' people known to be active in the desired organizations. An exception to this procedure was the case of WMEAC. Here, no addresSes were permitted to be released, but the Chair volunteered to send the surveys to individuals in the organization if such surveys were sent to the organization in bulk. It is understood that these procedures introduced some bias into the surveyed population, but the researcher felt that the bias served to include those persons who better conform to the ideals of each of the groups - to have members who are actively pursuing the goals of the organization. Incentives to Increase Rate of Return In order to achieve the highest possible rate of return for the survey, several different strategies were 56 incorporated. The cover letter (first page of the survey, Appendix 1) included several direct personal appeals as well as an implicit association with both Spring Arbor College and Michigan State University. There was also a financial incentive, but for the benefit of the group with which the individual was associated. As explained in the cover letter, each returned survey would result in a donation of $.50 to the group. In this way, large numbers of surveys being returned could result in a significant contribution to the group's treasury. It was expected that the group financial incentive would encourage participation, but would avoid some I of the potential difficulties encountered in offering direct personal financial incentives. It should be noted here that this procedure might also influence the character of the respondent group. It might, because Of the group incentive, lead to a group more heavily composed of those strongly motivated to support the group financially. Since this is another means of actively pursuing the goals of the organization, it would not be undesirable to have this group well represented. As can be seen on the survey data from both social/political action and environmental action, it clearly did not eliminate those who are not active in donations of money to environmental or social/political causes. Also included were two introductory letters from well—known individuals urging participation in the study. The first letter was from Harry Whitely, the chairman of the 57 Michigan Natural Resources Commission (Appendix 2). The second letter was one of several designed to be more specific to the group of which the individual was a member. For various groups, letter number two was from the chair of the state chapter of the Sierra Club (Mackinac Chapter), the president of the Michigan Audubon Society, the Superintendent of the North Michigan Conference of the Free Methodist Church, or the pastor of the surveyed individual's local Free Methodist Church (Appendix 2). In addition, the pastors of the churches involved in the study were asked to announce at least once from the pulpit ' that completion of the survey had their approval. Individuals who had not returned the survey within two weeks were mailed a reminder postcard. They were not sent a second survey with the postcard. Non—respondents were not censused to determine possible response bias. Analysis of the Data This section will describe the procedures by which the data were examined in answering the research questions described in the introductory chapter. Each question will be restated, then the methods of analysis will be described. 58 Research Question 1a. Is there a relationship between Maslow level and environmental action (level and type)? Maslow level was determined by the adaptation of Beer's survey as was previously described. pgygl of environmental action was estimated by using the weighted environmental score (WES). Pearson's r was used to determine the correlation between these two variables. Type of environmental action was examined by observing the total environmental score (TES) for each of the questions of the environmental action survey. ANOVA was used to I determine whether there was a relationship between each question's TES and Maslow level Whether to use WES or TES was determined by the type of analysis intended. When the researcher was interested in overall action levels which showed environmental concern, WES was used. When more specific actions or motivations were being analyzed, TES was used. The use of TES was preferred in these cases to avoid having two different motivational constructs measured with the same specific behavior. For example, when Rokeach's health terminal value was being analyzed with specific environmental actions, the researcher wanted to be able to state the relationship between concern for health and the environmental action, without also having to deal with the possible intervening effects of the environmental concern variable. 59 Analysis for the relationship between Maslow level and pypg of environmental action was done in two different ways. The first way was designed to discern whether any Maslow level was more or less active in a Specific action. For each of the 45 actions,it used ANOVA to find whether there were differences in amount of action among the five different Maslow levels. The safety/security level was eliminated since there was only one individual classed in this group. If the data were thought of as a matrix with the five Maslow levels as the X axis and the 45 actions as the Y axis, this analysis ' would be horizontal, one row at a time. With the same data matrix, the second analysis was vertical. For each Maslow level, it compared amounts of all types of environmental action, again using ANOVA. There was one additional manipulation of the data in this second analysis. In order to facilitate data manipulation, the environmental actions were collapsed into general types of actions. Thus, picking up litter at home, in the neighborhood, in public areas, and in commercial areas, would be reduced to a single score for litter pick-up. This resulted in a reduction from 45 to 17 environmental action scores. These scores were then changed to percent scores, allowing comparison of the litter question (possible raw score range 0 - 12) with the monetary donation question (possible raw score range 0 - 9) etc. With this conversion, a person who had a score of 8 out of a possible 12 points on T____________ 444, 60 the litter questions (67%) would be able to be compared with someone who had a score of 6 out of 9 on the monetary donation questions (also 67%). They were both active in proportionally the same amounts. Research Question 1b. Is there a relationship between Maslow level and socio/political action (level and type)? First, Maslow level was correlated with lpygl of social/political action. The level of social/political action was computed by summing the scores for all of the ‘ social/political action questions (SP5). The possible correlation between Maslow level and level of social/political action was examined with the Pearson's r statistic. The relationship between Maslow level and the type of social/political action used frequency scores for each action as the indicator of type of action. The social/political action data was placed in matrix form with Maslow categories on the X axis, and again, both horizontal and vertical analyses were performed with ANOVA. The horizontal analyses were to determine whether there were differences among Maslow groups for amount of each type of action. The vertical analyses were to determine whether there were differences among amounts of the various types of actions at each Maslow level. 61 Research Question 2. Is there a relationship between Maslow level and the presence and importance of specific elements of Rokeach's Terminal Values survey? Surveys were placed into categories according to which of Rokeach’s 18 terminal values was classified as first priority. Seven of the 18 terminal values were found to account for 146 of the 164 surveys returned (89%). The n for these values ranged from 72 to 6. Analysis of variance was used to compare mean Maslow scores among these seven terminal ' value groups. Research Question 3a. Is there a relationship between environmental activism (level and type) and the dominance of specific elements of the Terminal Values survey? For level of action, survey scores were grouped according to the Terminal Value element given priority #1. By using ANOVA the most commonly occurring groups were examined for their relationship with WES. Analysis for Eypg of environmental action was done in a manner analogous with research question 1. The data matrix for this analysis had the seven most commonly occurring dominant Rokeach terminal values on the X axis and the seventeen different general action types on the Y 62 axis. These 17 action types were the ones derived by collapsing the original 45 situation—specific actions. ANOVA was used to first find whether there were differences in amount among types of actions for each dominant Rokeach terminal value (vertical analysis). Next, ANOVA was used to find whether there were differences among the dominant Rokeach terminal values for amount of each type of action (horizontal analysis). Research Question 3b. Is there a relationship between social/political activism (level and type) and the dominance of specific elements of the Terminal Values survey? Level of social/political activism was analyzed using the same seven Terminal Value groups as in question 3a. Analysis of variance was applied to total social/political action scores versus each of the seven most common dominant Rokeach terminal values. Typg of social/political action was examined as in 3a, using ANOVA. The data matrix in this case had the seven most commonly occurring dominant Rokeach terminal values on the X axis and the 19 different social/political actions on the Y axis. Both horizontal and vertical analyses were performed, determining whether there were significant differences among 63 amounts of each type of action for each dominant Rokeach terminal value, as well as whether there were significant differences among Rokeach terminal values for amounts of each type of action. All analysis was done on a PDP—ll computer using the Minitab statistical analysis package. Chapter 4 RESULTS In addition to presentation and analysis of data, this chapter will present demographic information and other 'post—hoc observations pertinent to the study questions. Description of Respondents Members of environmental groups returned 52% (89) of their surveys; members of Free Methodist churches returned 32% (74) of their surveys. Most respondents reported having at least some college education (Table 1). The occupation most often listed by respondents from both groups was "official/professional" (Table 4.1). Income levels were distributed evenly among all respondents from both groups (X2 = 6.67 for environmental groups, 4.32 for church groups, each with df = 4; Table 1). Six of the respondents from Free Methodist churches (8%) indicated that they also belong to some environmental 64 65 organization. There was an even distribution of urban, suburban, and rural respondents among the environmental groups (X2=.427, df=2). The church group was not evenly distributed (X2=8.742, df=2, p<.02), being less well represented in urban areas. There were no significant differences in numbers of males and females responding to the survey from either group. When the three environmental groups were compared for amount of environmental action (using WES), it was found ’that there were no significant differences among the groups (F = 2.27, d.f. = 2,86). 66 Table 4.1. Responses to selected demographic questions (by O s of respondent group — church/environmental) Education Some college College degree or Graduate degree some graduate school 36% / 13% 23% / 38% 12% / 38% Occupation Official or professional Homemaker 36% / 63% 28% / 12% Home site Urban Suburban Rural 18% / 36% 45% / 34% 38% / 30% Family Income (in units Of $1,000) <10 10—20 20—30 30—40 >40 11% / 15% 23% / 20% 19% / 24% 17% / 13% 13% / 25% 67 Research Questions Research Question #1 was "Is there a relationship between Maslow level and environmental action (level and type); between Maslow level and social/political action (level and type)?" This question was subdivided into the following relationships. Maslow level and level of environmental action Using the Maslow score (determined with Beer's Preference Inventory) as the independent variable and the weighted environmental score (WES) as the dependent variable, analysis with Pearson's r showed that there was very little correlation between the two variables (r=.07, P=.49). Maslow level and type of environmental action Possible relationships between Maslow level and type of environmental action were investigated in two ways. The first analysis was designed to determine whether individuals at different Maslow levels were more or less active with regard to specific environmental behaviors. 68 Each of the 45 environmental behaviors was tested by ANOVA to find whether there were frequency differences among the various Maslow levels (Appendix C). Results were statistically significant for only two of the 45 actions. For action #1.d (litter pick—up — commercial areas), F = 2.90, d.f. = 4,158, P<.05. Observation of the confidence interval estimates shows that those persons at Maslow levels II and VI appeared to be lower in frequency of litter pickup in commercial areas than were the other groups. The second action showing a statistically significant difference (F = 2.82, d.f. = 4,158, P<.05) was action #17.c (donations — ’national) where those at Maslow level II were higher than all other groups (Table 4.2). Although not statistically significant in most cases, it was seen that individuals at Maslow level II were quantitatively higher than any other group in 33 of the 45 (73%) environmental action questions (Appendix C). : The second means of analysis looked at each Maslow group individually, examining action scores on the 17 environmental action types. Significant differences were found among environmental action scores in every one of the five Maslow categories (the safety/security level was eliminated because of extremely low sample size). The analysis may be seen in Table 4.3. Generalizations may be made from this table if the percentage scores of >60% and (20% are used as arbitrary (not statistically Table 4.2. 69 ANOVA and 95%confidence interval estimates for environmental actions l.d and 17.c when comparing mean action frequency among all Maslow levels. * Action l.d (pick up litter - commercial) Maslow level I II III IV VI N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN 1.3377 0.8696 1.4545 1.4375 0.9722 POOLED STDEV - 0.8014 Action 17.c (donate $ - Maslow level I II III IV VI N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN 1.1039 1.7391 0.9091 0.7500 0.8056 POOLED STDEV - 1.1450 * all other actions are included as Appendix C STDEV 0.7365 0.8689 0.6876 0.9639 0.8447 national) STDEV 1.1190 1.2869 1.0445 1.1255 1.1419 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV Tr ( t ( ( (i-~----) ) t uh <---*-----> 030 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN LL 1' 1.20 BASED ON POOLED STDEV A v 1.60 2.90 df- 158 P<.05 P- 2.82 df- 158 P<.05 Table 4.3. '70 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each Maslow level when comparing mean action frequencies among types of environmental action. Maslow level I (self-actualization) Action Mme N 1 77 3 77 4 77 5 77 6 77 7 77 a 77 9 77 10 77 12 77 13 77 14 77 15 77 16 77 POOLED STDEV = MEAN 0.67623 0.26727 0.36377 0.13870 0.26701 0.17727 0.54610 0.69377 0.60948 0.35468 0.48714 0.09338 0.12338 0.03481 0.05831 0.62896, 0.36662 0.26929 Maslow level II (esteem Action N Type 23 1 23 2 23 3 2. s 23 6 23 7 23 e 23 9 23 10 fig 11 :- 12 :3 13 * 14 33 15 :g 16 e 17 23 POOLED STDEU = MEAN 0.61957 0.33696 0.47130 0.23174 0.40130 0.26087 0.69696 0.70652 0.70609 0.45652 0.67348 0.11522 0.13000 0.04348 0.09391 0.74043 0.58087 0.27944 81020 0.17658 0.27124 0.32577 0.24584 0.30168 0.29159 0.38344 0.23654 0.22573 0.32129 0.32191 0.18210 0.19755 0.10246 0.14983 0.33368 0.32890 self) STDEV 0.17071 0.36894 0.31663 0.28705 0.33575 0.35282 0.33861 0.20621 0.23754 0.34082 0.32723 0.23027 0.18725 0.10272 0.13954 0.26510 0.34224 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN .BASED ON POOLED STUEU --+ --------- + --------- + --------- +--- (-t—-) (-*--> (-#--) (--x-) <-x--) (-r--) <--x-) <--x-) <-x--) <--x-) <-r--) (--x-) c-x--) <-x--) <-t--) (-s--) (-¥--) --+ --------- + --------- + --------- + 0.00 0.24 0.48 0.72 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEU “‘+ -‘ T '+ --------- +-" ("-t--) (---X'--) (-?‘*'--) ("’*"-) (“‘3-‘-) ("-*---) (--‘*--‘) (-"*‘-) (—"*1f) (“‘t---) (--*'--) (“‘*‘--) (--*-'-) ("3"‘) (‘--¥---) (-"*---) (-'*---) --‘+ -------- + ------- + ------- +-‘ 0.00 0.30 0.60 0.90 P- 54.23 di- 16, 1292 P<.01 F- 17.85 di- 16, 374 P<.01 7]. Table 4.3 (continued). Maslow level III (esteem - others) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Action BASED ON POOLED 81080 P- Typ. N MEAN srnrv ----- + --------- +-—- e + 10-52 1 11 0.72727 0.17470 (----4----) 2 11 0.31727 0.28107 < ----- *----> df- 3 11 0.36364 0.30605 <--——x———-) 15. 4 11 0.03000 0.09950 (--—-x----) 170 s 11 0.13091 0.19781 <----x ----- ) 5 11 0.09000 0.13755 <----x----) P<.01 7 11 0.46545 0.34500 < ----- x----> a 11 0.70909 0.20681 ( ..... .----. 9 11 0046182 0034438 (‘-'-¥ ----- ) 10 11 0.27273 0.33577 (----t----) 11 11 0.56091 0.34405 ( ----- xe---) 12 11 0.05000 0.09022 < ----- t----) 13 11 0.09182 0.20346 (----x----) 14 11 0 0 <----x----) ’ 15 11 0 0 (---—x-——-) 16 11 0.45455 0.30959 <----x----) 17 11 0.33273 0.33003 <----x----) ----- +--- s + —-+- POOLED STDEV = 0.26293 -_ ,-_ _ 0.00 g0.30‘”_", 0.60 _0.90_ "9'10" 1°V°1 IV (1°V°) runrvrnunt 95 PCT-CI’S FOR MEAN . BASED 0w POOLED sroev p- ActiOn N MEAN STDEV ----+ --------- + --------- + --------- +--8.63 TYPO 16 0.66125 0.20966 (----r----) 1 16 0.28125 0.35193 c----x----) d1. 2 16 0.36563 0.38171 (----r----> 15, 3 16 0.13563 0.29353 < ----- x----) 255 4 16 0.18750 0.28375 <----x----) 5 16 0.19500 0.26907 < ----- x-—--) p<.01 5 16 0.50063 0.47164 <----x----) 7 16 0.67500 0.25560 <-----:----) 3 16 0.57313 0.32780 <----x----) 9 16 0.28188 0.36907 <----r----> 1° 16 0.53188 0.35626 <----x----> 11 16 0.06938 0.22201 (----t----) 12 16 0.07313 0.25078 (----x----) 13 16 0.05188 0.14511 <—---s----> 14 16 0.06250 0.14726 (----x----) 15 16 -0.62625 0.31979 (----t----) 15 16 0.30563 0.36026 <----x---—) . 17 ----+ --------- + ------- --+ --------- +-- pooreu 37089 0.30659 0.00 0.30 0.60 0.90 Q Maslow level VI (physiological) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Action BASED ON POOLED STDEV :- Typ. N MEAN STDEV -‘-+ --------- + ------- + w +--- 23.51 1 36 0059778 0019595 (“"“"") 2 36 0.30778 0.34589 (---¥---) df- 3 36 0.24528 0.28070 (---¥---) égé 4 36 0.08833 0.18067 <---x--) 5 36 0.31500 0.32582 (‘--!---) 01 6 36 0.19750 0.30013 (---¥---) P<. 7 36 0.52528 0.38621 (---¥---) 8 36 0.69583 .20350 (---¥--‘) 9 36 0.61111 0.26996 (--¥---) 10 36 0.29611 0.35020 (--¥---) 11 36 0.51389 0.33928 (--i---) 12 36 0.06139 0.14367 (---t--) 13 36 0.17583 0.24204 (--*‘--) 14 36 0.03694 0.09789 (---¥--) 15 36 0.04167 0.11503 (---*--) 16 36 0.62139 0.33065 (---#---) 17 36 0.34250 0.32219 (--*---) ---+ --------- + --------- + --------- +--- POOLED STDEV = 0.27462 0.00 .24 0.48 0.72 72 significant) cut-off points. Actions 1 (litter pick—up), 8 (turn lights off), and 16 (stop buying a potentially harmful product) appear to uniformly be the highest frequency actions — at all Maslow levels. The actions appearing at uniformly lowest frequencies are 4 (distribute a petition), 6 (campaign), 12 (make a speech), 13 (report a violation), 14 (lawsuit), and 15 (injunction). These items are not discriminators of certain Maslow levels; they are actions consistent at all Maslow levels. Actions which can be associated with specific Maslow levels are more difficult to determine. Examination of the means and confidence interval estimates resulted in the following observations. Maslow level I was found to have no actions which distinguished it from any of the others. Maslow level II was distinguished by being the only level to have actions 7 (vote) and 11 (discussion) at levels above 60%. Maslow levels III and IV were seen to be the only levels in which action 5 (attempt to influence elected officials) was less than 20%, as well as the only levels in which action 9 (purchase for environmental reasons) was ggt above 60%. There were no unique discriminators of Maslow level VI. While these observations are not statistical analyses, the patterns are important in comparison with other data. 73 Maslow level and level of social and/or political action Social/political action was measured by Part IV of the survey (Appendix A). Scores for each action were summed to result in a social/political action total score (SPS). When Pearson’s r was used to correlate Maslow level with SP5, the result was r=.l48. There was no significant correlation between the two sets of scores. ~Maslow level and type of social and/or political action This relationship was examined in two ways. The first was on the basis of actions. Each of the 19 actions was examined in turn, using ANOVA to determine whether there were differences in amount of action among the Maslow levels. None of the ANOVAs showed any significant differences (Appendix D). One relationship which did appear on the basis of this analysis was that Maslow level II had the highest action levels in 7 of the 19 actions. Though the differences between Maslow level II and the other levels are not statistically significant, this is a pattern similar to the results of the comparison of Maslow level and environmental action. 74 The second way in which this data was examined was on the basis of Maslow level. Each Maslow level was singled out and ANOVA was used to determine whether there were differences in amount of action among all 19 of the social/political actions (Table 4.4). For every one of the five Maslow levels tested the values of F were significant at P<.Ol. There are differences among frequencies of social/political actions for each Maslow group. Some generalizations emerged from obesrvations of the data. Maslow levels I, II, and III showed the highest 'frequencies of action for those items involving education, with voting behaviors being the second most frequent. The highest action frequencies for Maslow levels IV and VI were for voting behaviors, with education activities ranking second in frequency. Closer examination shows that the frequency means for voting behaviors are quite similar among all Maslow levels. It appears that the frequency of education actions are higher than voting in the higher Maslow levels and lower than voting in the lower Maslow levels. While these differences in frequency of social/political action may not be statistically significant, the pattern should be noted for comparison with other research questions. The behavior appearing at the lowest frequency appears to be filing lawsuits, at all Maslow levels. Table 4.4. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each Maslow level when comparing mean action '75 frequencies among social/political actions. \ Maslow level I (self-actualization) Soc/pol action NEAN 0.8961 0.2468 1.2338 1.3247 0.4286 0.4805 0.2727 0.8701 1.2987 1.6623 0.7273 0.3506 0.2078 0.2208 1.1039 1.3377 1.7922 1.6883 1.2857 STDEU 0.9260 0.5656 0.8870 0.8651 0.7332 0.7714 0.6617 1.0046 1.0521 0.9544 0.9547 0.6838 0.6353 0.6412 1.0206 1.0338 0.9081 1.1033 1.0495 POOLED STDEU = 0.8813 Maslow level II (esteem - self) Soc/pol action N 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 23 MEAN 1.08696 0.47826 1.08696 1.21739 0.47826 0.60870 0.39130 1.34783 1.65217 1.65217 0.47826 0.30435 0.08696 0.26087 1.00000 1.13043 1.56522 1.65217 0.95652 POOLED STDEV = 0.90315 STDEU 0.94931 0.66535 1.08347 0.85048 0.66535 0.94094 0.78272 1.26522 1.02730 1.19121 0.84582 0.47047 0.28810 0.68870 0.95346 1.05763 0.94514 0.93462 0.97600 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FBR MEAN 29.28 df- 18, 1444 P<.01 BASED ON POOLED STDEU ---------- +---------+-------—-+------ (--x--) (--4--) <--—x--) (--t--) <--x——) (—-x--) (---x--) <—--x~-) <---x--) (---t--) <--x--> (--x--) (--x—--) (---4--) (--t---) <-—4---> <--x--> <--x--) (--4---> ---------- +--—------+---------+------ 0.60 .20 1.80 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR HEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEU ----+—-- + — + --------- +-- <---t----) (----¥~---) (—--x----) (----t----) (-—--x----) <----:-?--) (-—--t----) (----t----) (-—--:----> <----t----) <----t----) (~---:----> (----x—---) <---x----) (----x----> (-—--#----) <----x-—--) <-—--x----) (-—--t----) ----+ --------- + --------- + --------- +—- 0.00 0.75 1.50 2.25 P- 7.63 df- 18, 418 P<.01 Maslow level III (esteem - others) Table 4.4 Soc/pol action N 1 11 2 11 3 11 4 11 5 11 6 11 7 11 3 11 9 11 10 11 11 11 12 11 13 11 14 11 15 11 16 11 17 11 18 11 19 11 POOLED STDEV = Hallow level Soc/pol action N 1 16 2 16 3 16 4 16 S 16 6 16 7 16 8 16 9 16 10 16 ll 16 12 16 13 16 14 16 15 16 16 16 17 16 18 16 19 16 MEAN 0.81818 0.27273 0.90909 1.36364 0.45455 0.45455 0.18182 1.00000 1.27273 1.63636 0.72727 0.36364 0.09091 0.63636 1.00000 1.27273 1.81818 1.90909 1.54545 0.93676 IV (love) 1.4375 1.1250 1.0625 POOLED sTDEv = 0.7914 Hallow level Soc/pol action N 1 36 2 36 3 36 4 36 5 36 g 36 36 3 36 9 36 1° 36 11 36 12 36 13 36 1‘ 36 15 36 15 36 17 36 18 36 19 36 POOLED STDEV = MEAN 0.86111 0.13889 0.86111 1.00000 0.22222 0.36111 0.05556 0.63889 1.02778 1.69444 0.66667 0.44444 0.05556 0.11111 0.86111 1.02778 1.55556 1.55556 1.16667 0.78772 (continued). STDEV 1.16775 0.46710 0.83121 0.92442 0.82020 0.68755 0.40452 1.09545 1.19087 1.02691 0.90453 0.50453 0.30151 1.02691 1.18322 1.00905 1.16775 1.04447 1.21356 VI (physiological) 1.02663 0.81064 76 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI‘S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STUEV p- : 1 : 1 3.95 ( ------ x ------ ) ( ------ I ------ ) df- ( —————— x ------ 1 13, < ------ x ------ > no ( ------ x —————— 1 < ------ x ------ ) P<.Ol < ------ x ------ ) < ------ x ----- > < ------ x ------ > ( ------ x ------ ) < ------ x ------ 1 < ------ x ------ > < ------ x ------ ) < ------ x----~-) ( —————— x ————— ) < ------ x ------ 1 ( ------ x ------ 1 < ----- ~t------) ( ------ x ------ ) 0.00 0.80 1.60 2.40 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT c1 8 FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV :- 5.67 df- 18, 285 P<.01 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT cr's FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV F- : f , 1 15.81 (---t----) <-—-x---—) df- (---¥—---) 18, 1-~—-t---) 665 (-—-—t-—-) (___‘___) P<.01 <--—x---) (---—x—--) (—--t---) <---:-——-) (---x---> <——-x----) (---x---) <——-*---) (---x--—-) (-——x—--) (---t~--) (——-x---) (-—-x~—--) 0.00 0.60 1.20 1.80 77 Research Question 2 Are there relationships between an individual's Maslow level and the priority given to specific elements of Rokeach's terminal values survey? Respondents were grouped according to which terminal values they had ranked as priority #1 on Rokeach's survey. A set of seven of the eighteen values accounted for first priority (dominant) position on 146 of the 164 returned surveys (89%). The "n" values ranged from 72 to 6. ANOVA revealed that there were no significant relationships Ibetween mean Maslow levels and the seven most common Rokeach values (F = 1.25, d.f. = 6,139). Table 4.5 shows the mean Maslow level (with 95% confidence intervals) of the seven most common Rokeach terminal values. Research Question 3 Is there a relationship between the priority assigned to specific elements of Rokeach's terminal values survey and environmental activism (level and type); between priority assigned to specific elements of Rokeach's terminal values survey and social/political activism (level and type)? For analysis, this was broken into more concise subquestions as follows. 78 Table 4.5. ANOVA and 95%confidence interval estimates comparing mean Maslow level among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values. * INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Pokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEU ?31Ue N HEAN STDEV -‘-+ --------- +---~-::::;:::::;---+--~ {325 4 27 2.407 1.886 <- ___) 0 13 2.722 2.024 ( ------- x—--- ) g 11 72 3.014 2.191 <--::-- 6' 3 9 1.667 1.000 < ---------- x ---------- ‘139 15 a 2.625 2.200 < ----------- t ---------- . 3 6 2.333 2.160 <-—-—; ------- x-—---; ------- ) N.5 9 6 ( -------------------------- 13 6 1.333 0 81 ___+ _________ + _________ + _________ +___ POOLED STDEv = 20028 000 102 204 306 * Rokeach values 14 salvation peace health freedom self—respect sense of accomplishment 8 = wisdom 5 4 9 8 1 3 l 79 First priority Rokeach terminal value and level of environmental action The persons having the seven most frequently chosen dominant terminal values were examined. These ranged from n = 72 to n = 6. For each of these seven groups of individuals, a mean was computed for the sums of each individual's 45 weighted environmental action scores. These means were compared by ANOVA among the seven terminal value groups, resulting in a highly significant value for F (F = 11.22, d.f. = 6,139, P<.01). Table 4.6 shows 95% confidence 'intervals for the mean WES of each of the most common dominant terminal values. On the basis of Table 4.6, it appeared that the "salvation" terminal value (#14) had a lower action score than did any of the other terminal values. In order to test whether it was the salvation group which was the source of the differences found in the previously described ANOVA, a second ANOVA was run on the top six Rokeach values which remained when the salvation variable was removed. In this case, there was no significant difference among the other groups (F = 1.86, d.f. = 5,68). Those people listing Rokeach's #3 value (a sense of accomplishment) as their first priority value appear to be higher in level of environmental action (WES) than the other 80 Table 4.6. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates comparing weighted environmental score among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values. * INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED srnev value N HEAN swnrv ---- +—- 4 ——- — + —-——F= 4 27 123.00 48.07 (---t--) 11.22 .3 18 107.00 43.49 (---t----) i4 72 62.71 40.45 (--x-1 gr: 3 9 106.22 61.96 t ----- t ----- 1 b- 15 8 114.75 26.71 ( ----- l ----- ) 139 3 6 166.67 . 61.11 ( ------ x ------ 1 15 6 107.00 46.01 < ------ x ------- ) P<~°l ---------- +---------+-—-------+------ F‘OOLED STDEU = 44.35 100 150 200 * Rokeach values 14 = salvation 4 a peace 9 a health 15 = self—respect sense of accomplishment 18 = wisdom 3 81 groups, but the difference is not significant. This shows that there is a difference among environmental activism levels when persons having different dominant Rokeach terminal values are compared. This difference comes from the significantly lower WES scores of those who rank the Rokeach value "salvation" as dominant. First priority Rokeach terminal value and type of environmental action The seven most common Rokeach terminal values were again selected for analysis in this section. As in the first research question, the analysis was done in two different ways. The first type of analysis used each of the 45 environmental actions. For each action, ANOVA was employed to find whether there were differences in level of action among the seven different Rokeach terminal values. Most actions appeared at significantly different levels among the dominant Rokeach terminal values (Appendix E). Those actions which did not show significant differences among the dominant Rokeach terminal values were the following: l.a, l.b, l.d (pick up litter) 82 8.a, 8.b, 8.c (turn off lights) 9.b, 9.d (environmentally based purchases) 13.a, 13.b (report violations) 14.b (group lawsuit) 15.a (individual filing an injunction) Those actions which did show significant differences among the dominant Rokeach terminal values were categorized by which of the terminal values groups showed the highest and lowest levels of action. In every case showing a significant difference, the 'difference appeared to be based on a lower score by those choosing salvation as their dominant terminal value, thus other comparisons are based on variations which are in most cases not significantly different. When those actions showing significant differences among Rokeach terminal value groups were examined for which terminal value group showed the highest score, some patterns were seen. Action frequencies for questions dealing with voting (7.b,c), and with recycling (2.a,b,c,d) were highest for those individuals choosing self-respect (Rokeach value #15) as their dominant terminal value. This same group also had the largest number of items for which they had a higher mean action frequency than any other group (10/45). The questions about attempting to influence elected officials (5.a,b,c), purchasing for environmental reasons 83 (9.a,c,d), stop buying potentially harmful products (16), and donating money (l7.b,c) showed highest response frequencies among those choosing a sense of accomplishment (Rokeach #3) as their dominant terminal value. Questions dealing with campaigning (6.b,c) received the highest action frequency from those choosing peace (Rokeach #4) as their dominant terminal value. Action frequencies for questions about making a public appearance (12.b,c) showed the highest frequency of action from individuals in the group choosing health (Rokeach #9) as their first priority value. The second type of analysis grouped data in categories corresponding to the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values. For each of these groups, ANOVA was used to determine whether there were differences in amount among all environmental actions. In order to reduce the size of the data matrix, the 45 actions were collapsed into 17 action types. This eliminated the possibility of learning about litter pick-up in the neighborhood versus at home, but kept the general action type of litter pick—up. Another change was required to allow the comparison of action types — the scores had to be changed to percent. This allowed comparisons of scores with different ranges e.g. 0-3 with 0-9, but did not change the overall distribution. The seven ANOVAs done in this second analysis were all significant at P<.Ol (Table 4.7). Examination of the data shows that there are some 84 Table 4.7. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each of the seven most common dominant Rokeach terminal values when comparing mean environmental action frequency among all 17 environmental action types. Rokeach dominant terminal value 14 (salvation) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Action 94959 0N POOLED 57090 P- type N MEAN 97950 --+ --------- + --------- + --------- +---- 58-03 1 72 0.60625 0.19536 <-x-> 2 72 0.15961 0.20235 (--4-> df- 3 72 0.19931 0.29235 (-t--) 16. 4 72 0.04191 0.15793 1—1-1 1207 5 72 0.10931 0.21246 (--t-) 5 72 0.04000 0.13356 <--1-) P<-°1 7 72 0.32167 0.34019 (-:—-> 8 72 0.69496 0.24574 <--4-> 9 72 0.50709 0.26925 <-x-) 10 72 0.15750 0.27702 (--x-> 11 72 0.39056 0.30122 <-x-) 12 72 0.02597 0.07499 <-t-> 13 72 0.11347 0.20902 <-x-) 14 72 0.01153 0.05779 <-x-—) 15 72 0.02093 0.10459 <-4-) 16 72 0.54709 0.35156 <-:-) 17 72 0.14917 0.23667 1-1-1 -—+ —+- — ---+ --------- +---- POOLED 57099 = 0.23079 0.00 0.24 0.49 0.72 Rokeach dominant terminal value 4 (peace) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT cx's FOR menu Action 94950 0N POOLED 91099 p. type N MEAN 97950 -+ --------- + --------- + --------- + ----- 26.91 1 27 0.71630 0.19073 <--x--> 2 27 0.40741 0.36610 <-—-x--) df- 3 27 0.43222 0.30110 (-—:---) 16, 4 27 0.17296 0.22991 <--x--> 442 5 27 0.45741 0.31764 (--:--1 6 27 0.39519 0.36330 <--4--) p<.01 7 27 0.75370 0.29675 (--4--> 9 27 0.70444 .22555 <--4---> 9 27 0.76519 0.15550 <-——4--) 10 27 0.45741 0.31515 (--:--1 11 27 0.64704 0.29727 <---4--> 12 27 0010667 0017072 ("-"-) 13 27 0.12926 0.20299 <--:---) 14 27 0.06915 0.13272 (--x---) 15 27 0.11074 0.15920 (---x--> 16 27 0.74296 0.21271 (~-x-~) 17 27 0.65963 0.29796 <—-x--1 -+ --------- + --------- + --------- + ----- POOLED 97090 - 0.25771 0.00 0.30 0.60 0.90 85 Table 4.7 (continued). Rokeach dominant terminal value 9 (health) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR HEAN Action 94950 0N POOLED 37090 3- tYpe N HEAN 97090 ----+-- 4 —— 4 ---+—- 13.97 1 19 0.70399 0.09519 1~--x----1 2 19 0.39333 0.32700 <—--x-—-1 df- 3 19 0.55556 0.29219 1----x---1 16. 4 19 0.29611 0.30504 1---x---1 289 5 19 0.40167 0.32711 <---x----1 6 19 0.39500 0.37069 1---x---1 P<.01 7 19 0.70444 0.37359 1---4----1 9 19 0.71222 0.15761 1---x—--1 9 19 0.61167 0.22566 1---1----1 10 18 0047167 028755 (""*"'-) 11 19 0.64779 0.31231 1----1---1 12 19 0.19056 0.26994 1-—-4--—1 13 19 0.10167 0.14064 1---x----1 14 19 0.02779 0.09537 1---4---1 15 19 0.02779 0.09537 <---4---1 16 19 0.63056 0.32196 1---1---1 17 19 0.45611 0.25971 1---4---1 ----+ --------- + --------- + --------- +-- 900190 97099 4 0.26701 0.00 0.30 0.60 0.90 (Rokeach dominant terminal value 8 (freedom) INDIUIDUAL 95 PCT 01's FOR HEAN Action 94990 0N 900190 57090 :- typo N HEAN 97050 - 4 4 4 4— 5,32 1 9 0.62999 0.13176 1-----x ----- 1 2 9 0038000 003639‘ ( ---- ‘ ..... ) df- 3 9 0.46333 0.33211 1 ----- 4 ------ 1 15' 4 9 0.27999 0.33479 1 ----- t ------ 1 135 5 9 0.34556 0.23431 1 ------ t ----- 1 6 9 0.21000 0.29720 1 ----- 4 ----- 1 p<,01 7 9 0153222 0038068 ( """ ‘ """ ) 9 9 0.77999 0.20109 1 ----- t ----- 1 9 9 0.69556 0.24991 1-----4 ----- 1 1o 9 0.49000 0.39601 1 ----- x ----- 1 11 9 0.61111 0.35435 1 ----- x ------ 1 12 9 0.22111 0.32371 1 ----- x ------ 1 13 9 0.11111 0.19597 1 ------ x ----- 1 14 9 0.05556 0.11727 1 ----- x ----- 1 15 9 0.09222 0.14594 1 ----- x ----- 1 16 9 0.70556 0.30909 1 ----- ~x ----- 1 17 9 0.49556 0.23939 1 ------ t--—--) ----- +----~----+----— 4 — +— 900150 97090 = 0.29331 0.00 0.30 0.60 0.90 Rokeach dominant terminal value 18 (wisdom) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT c115 FOR MEAN Action BASED ON POOLED STDEU P- type N HEAN STDEU ----- + --------- + --------- + --------- +- 5.99 1 6 0.76500 0.09670 <----9 ----- 1 2 6 0.36167 0.22996 1-—--4-~--1 at. 3 6 0.39000 0.44399 1 ----- 1—-—-1 16, 4 6 0.27933 0.31115 (----t—---) 95 5 6 0.37167 0.34365 1----x ----- 1 6 6 0.20333 0.29514 (----3 ----- 1 9<.01 7 6 0.35333, 0.17963 1—--x-—--1 9 6 0.74167 0.27029 1 ----- x--~-1 9 6 0.54000 0.33154 1 ----- x----1 10 6 0.59167 0.22999 1 ----- x----1 11 6 0.75000 0.17355 1 ----- x----1 12 6 0.12933 0.20193 1---—x ----- 1 13 6 0.09333 0.20412 1—--—4 ----- 1 14 6 0.05500 0.13472 1—---x ----- 1 15 6 0.05500 0.13472 1-—--4 ----- 1 16 6 0.61167 0.39035 1----4 ----- 1 17 6 0.53667 0.31123 1----x ----- 1 ----- +---~-----+~--------+-------—-+- POOLED 97090 = 0.26902 0.00 0.40 0.90 1.20 Table 4.7 (continued). £36 Rokeach dominant terminal value 15 (self-respect) (01110011111100 00001311711703 63(00me HEAN 0.70875 0.57250 0.50125 0.20875 0.34625 0.08250 0.89000 0.69625 0.65875 0.66750 0.68750 0.04125 0.08375 0.06375 0.08375 0.62625 0.65375 POOLED STDEV = 0.19511 STDEV 0.13410 0.34628 0.29575 0.26406 0.20184 0.15276 0.14402 0.16587 0.15085 0.17774 0.25695 0.08184 0.12569 0.08798 0.12569 0.21540 0.16405 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Rokeach dominant terminal value 3 (sense of accomplishment) Action type O~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~0~z POOLED STDEV HEAN 0.77667 0.45667 0.38833 0.11000 0.63167 0.33333 0.85167 0.63000 0.79000 0.52667 0.72167 0.16667 0.19500 ‘0.09333 0.11167 0.89000 0.66833 .25536 STDEV 0.06532 0.33255 .22886 0.17041 0.25087 0.21295 0.27029 0.24166 0.13755 0.41263 0.40351 0.26181 0.26779 0.20412 0.27353 0.17041 0.18649 94990 0N 900190 97090 p- ---+ --------- + --------- + --------- +--- 17.69 1---t---1 <---*-—-1 dz- 1----x---1 15, <----‘---> 119 . (—--x---1 <'-'~*‘--> 94.01 (---1--—1 1----x---1 1----1---1 <---x---1 1—--x----1 1---4---—1 1----x---1 <—-—x---1 1-—--1-—-1 (----t---) <---x----1 ---+ --------- +-— -- 4 4 -- 0.00 0.32 0.64 0.96 INDIVIDUAL 95 901 01's 909 MEAN 94990 0N 900190 97090 9- —---+---- 4 4 -+—- 7.30 (----t ----- 1 (----t ----- 1 d:- (--—-x----1 15, (----x----1 35 1----t---—1 <----4 ----- 1 p<.01 (----t----) (---—t----1 1---—x----1 1----s----1 (----x----1 1---—4----1 1----x----1 1---—t----1 <----x-—--1 1----1----1 1-—--t-—--1 ~--—+ --------- + --------- + --------- +-- 0.00 0.40 0.90 1.20 * Environmental action types ‘OQQQUIAWNH rAHrarAH14reH QCBUI§1»B)HCD turn lights off purchase for env. . distribute information discussion public appearance report violation file lawsuit file injunction stop buying product donate S d officials pick up litter recycling sign petition distribute petition influence electe campaign vote reasons 87 actions which are uniformly high or low regardless of the terminal values of the group. Actions 1 (litter-pickup) and 8 (turning off lights) consistently occur at mean frequencies between 60% and 80%. Actions 12—15 (public appearance, reporting violations, filing lawsuit, filing injunction) consistently occur at mean frequencies below 20%. Certain groups had distinctive elements to their scores. Those choosing "salvation" as their dominant terminal value were distinct from other groups in their consistently low scores. In 11 of the 17 action types '(65%), their scores were below 20%. Among all the other groups, between 2 and 5 of the 17 action types showed scores below 20%. On the other end of the scale, this group had no scores above 70%. The other groups ranged from 2 to 5 items with scores above 70%. The group of those indicating that Rokeach #4 (peace) was their dominant terminal value had the highest number of behavior types with frequencies above 70% (5). This group showed a low frequency for action #4 (distribute a petition) when compared with the other action frequencies within the group. The individuals who placed Rokeach #15 (self—respect) as their highest priority value were distinctive in several ways. Their mean score for frequency of voting for an environmental candidate was 88%, the highest frequency for any action among any group. This was the only group in 88 which action #10 (distribute information about environmental issues) was above 60%. Other than actions 12-15 (lowest for all groups), the action type showing the lowest frequency of action was #6 (campaigning for a person due to his/her environmental stance), with a frequency of 8%. The group choosing "a sense of accomplishment" (Rokeach #3) as highest priority was the only one to have a mean score above 60% for action #5 (attempt to influence elected officials). The lowest score for this group (not considering actions 12-15) was for distributing a petition (11%). Those people who chose Rokeach #18 (wisdom) as their dominant terminal value had their highest scores in action #7 (voting). As seen above, significant differences were found among groups and among action types when Rokeach's terminal values were used in analyzing amount of environmental action. In addition, many nonsignificant differences were also observed. Social/political action (level) and highest priority Rokeach terminal values For each of the seven most frequently occurring Rokeach terminal values, the mean was computed for total social/political action score. When means of 89 social/political action scores for each of the seven selected Rokeach values were compared, ANOVA showed significant differences among the groups (F = 4.38, df = 6,139, P<.01) (Table 4.8). The social/political action scores for those ranking salvation highest appear to be significantly lower than those of most other Rokeach values (Table 4.8). In order to test whether this is statistically significant, another ANOVA was run with the same Rokeach values, but excluding the "salvation" variable. The results show that the "salvation" value was the largest single source of the Ivariance, since F = .77 (d.f.= 7,75). With the removal of the salvation group, there was no longer any significant difference among the remaining groups. Table 4.8 also shows that those people ranking Rokeach #3 (a sense of accomplishment) as their primary value had a higher mean social/political score than the other groups. The difference between this and the other scores is not statistically significant. This shows that there is a relationship between the social/political action score and the dominance of certain of Rokeach's values. The relationship is that those persons ranking salvation as the most important value are less likely to show high levels of social or political action. 90 Table 4.8. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates comparing total social/political score (level of social/political action) among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values. * INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN - h BASED ON PODLED STDEV F- $§§3:° N MEAN 97090 --+ --------- + --------- + 777777777 +----4:39 4 27 19.99 9.99 1----4----—1___) 9 19 20.93 9.12 < ------ *- df= 14 72 13.28 7.61 (-(u~t--) 1‘ ) 6. ' 9 17.22 8060 ---------------- I5 9 16.87 4.95 1- -— 1 ~ ----) 139 2 . 25°°° 3~1° . ";::::::::i:; """" ’ p<.01 19 6 19.93 13.00 __+ _________ + _________ + _________ +____ POOLED STDE‘) = 8.15 12.0 18.0 24.0 30.0 * Rokeach values 14 4 9 8 15 3 l8 salvation peace health freedom self—reSpect sense of accomplishment wisdom 91 Social/political action (type) and highest priority Rokeach terminal values Analysis of these factors was again done in two different ways. The first way involved isolating each of the 19 social/political actions. For each action, ANOVA was used to find whether there were differences in amount of the action among the seven most common dominant Rokeach terminal values. It was found that seven of the 19 actions showed significant differences in amount of action among the Rokeach values (Table 4.9). The seven actions showing significant differences in action frequency among different Rokeach values were the following: write to or call an elected official (P<.Ol) . participate in a peaceful demonstration (P<.01) write a "letter to the editor" (P<.01) call a radio or TV station (P<.Ol) join an organization (P<.Ol) donate money (P<.01) 1. campaign (P<.05) l—‘KOQQONNI‘ Patterns of highest and lowest scores may be helpful for comparisons with other types of actions. In six of the seven preceding actions, the terminal value group having the lowest action score was #14 (salvation). The exception to this pattern was calling a radio or TV station, for which the lowest scoring terminal value group was Rokeach #15 (self—respect). For five of the above seven actions (#1,6,7,8,11), the Table 4.9. 92 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each social/political action when comparing mean action frequencies among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values. Action 1 (write to or call official) Rokeach value N 14 72 4 27 9 18 3 9 1s 8 3 6 19 6 MEAN 0.5833 1.1481 1.1667 0.8889 0.8750 1.6667 1.5000 POOLED STDEV - 0.8650 Action 2 (demonstration) Rokeach value N 14 72 4 27 9 18 8 9 15 8 3 6 19 6 MEAN 0.09722 0.51852 0.38889 0.11111 0.50000 0.33333 0.33333 POOLED STDEV - 0.50103 Action 3 (stop buying) Rokeach value N 14 72 4 27 9 18 3 9 15 3 3 6 19 5 MEAN 0.9861 1.2963 1.1667 1.2222 0.8750 1.3333 1.5000 POOLED STDEV - 0.8830 Action 4 (sign petition) Rokeach value N 14 72 4 27 9 8 15 3 18 H maxmsom MEAN 1.125 1.296 1.500 1.111 1.000 1.500 1.167 -POOLED STDEV - 0.862 STDEV 0.8005 0.9488 0.9852 0.7817 0.6409 0.8165 1.2247 STDEV 0.29834 0.64273 0.77754 0.33333 0.75593 0.51640 0.51640 STDEV 0.8639 0.8689 0.8575 0.9718 0.3536 1.0328 1.3784 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV T T (---;--) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV w INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV ~19 E'- 3.14 df- 139 P<.01 F- -‘- 0.83 df- 139 N's. F- 0.72 df- 139 )N.S. Table 4.9 (continued). Action 5 (distribute petition) Rokeach value N MEAN srosv 14 72 0.2222 0.5097 4 27 0.4444 0.6405 9 18 0.5556 0.9218 3 9 0.4444 0.5270 15 8 0.3750 0.5175 3 6 0.3333 0.5164 18 6 1.0000 1.2649 POOLED STDEV - 0.6403 Action 6 (letter to editor) Rokeach value N MEAN STDEV 14 72 0.2639 0.5812 4 27 0.6667 0.9608 9 18 0.7778 1.1144 3 9 0.4444 0.5270 15 8 0.5000 0.5345 3 6 1.3333 0.8165 18 6 0.5000 0.8367 POOLED STDEV - 0.7594 Action 7 (call radio or TV) Rokeach valu. N MEAN STDEV 14 72 0.06944 0.30611 4 27 0.18519 0.48334 9 18 0.50000 0.85749 3 9 0.55556 1.01379 15 8 0 0 3 6 0.83333 0.98319 18 6 0.33333 0.51640 POOLED grozv - 0.54793 Action 8 (join organization) Rokeach value N MEAN STDEV 14 72 0.4722 0.7686 4 27 1.0741 1.1068 9 18 1.1667 1.1504 3 9 1.1111 1.0541 15 8 1.3750 0.9161 3 6 1.6667 0.5164 13 6 1.5000 1.3784 POOLED STDEV - 0.9364 Action 9 (donate $) Rokeach value N MEAN STDEV 14 72 0.7778 0.9378 4 27 1.7407 1.0225 9 18 1.7778 0.8782 8 9 1.6667 1.1180 15 8 1.7500 0.7071 3 6 1.6667 0.5164 13 6 1.8333 1.1690 POOLED STDEV - 0.9461 93 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV T (--*--) 2L T I‘ (----*--~-) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV 1' (~--*---) 7 f 4 1.94 df- 139 N05. E'- 2.97 df- 139 P<.01 F- 3.98 df- 139 P<.01 E'- 4.33 df- 6. 139 ) P<.01 6.36 df- 139 P<.01 944 Table 4.9 (continued). Action 15 (charity campaign volunteer) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV 3.99 value N MEAN STDEV ¢ ¢ i 14 72 0.9722 0.9717 (--—*-—-) dr- 4 27 1.0370 1.2855 ( ----- * ------ ) 6 9 18 1.5000 1.0981 ( * ) 139 3 9 0.6667 0.7071 ( * ) 15 9 1.0000 0.7559 ( * ) 3 6 1.3333 1.2111 ( w ) N95- _13 6 1.1667 1.1690 ( . ) 900199 STDEV - 1.0021 0.60 1.20 1.90 Action 16 (serve at home) ' INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S 909 MEAN 9 Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV 1'34 value N MEAN STDEV 9 ¢ 9 ¢ - 14 72 .1.3194 0.9468 (--*--) df 4 27 1.0370 0.9398 (----*----) ' 9 19 1.3999 1.0922 ( ----- *----) 6: 9 9 0.9999 1.1667 ( r ) 139 15 9 1.3750 0.5175 ( * ) 3 6 1.8333 0.7528 ( ' )N.S. 13 6 0.6667 0.9165 ( * )- . POOLED STDEV - 0.9514 0.00 0.75 1.50 2.25 Action 17 (join education program) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED oN POOLED STDEV P- value N MEAN STDEV 9 ¢ 9 4 0.71 14 72 1.569 0.990 (---*---) 4 27 1.852 1.027 (-----*-----) 55' 9 19 1.999 0.932 ( * ) 6. a 9 2.000 0.500 ( * ) 139 15 9 1.625 0.916 ( * ) 3 6 2.000 0.632 ( * ) N.S. 19 6 1.667 1.211 ( * -’ - pooLgp srpgv . 0.952 1.20 1.80 2.40 3.00 Action 18 (educate someone) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT cr's POR MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV P- value N MEAN STDEV 9 5 f ¢ 1.46 14 72 1.399 1.069 (---.---) 4 27 1.779 0.934 ( ...... . ..... ) dz. 9 18 2.000 0.970 ( ' ) 67 9 9 1.999 1.054 ( . ) 139 15 9 1.975 0.641 ( * ) 3 6 2.000 0.994 (— . ; N.S. 19 6 1.667 1.366 ( . ) 900190 swnrv - 1.021 1.20 1.90 2.40 3.00 Action 19 (attend meeting) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT cr's ron MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV F' value N MEAN STDEV 5 9 9 I 1.97 14 72 0.9961 0.9268 (--.---) 4 27 1.2593 1.0225 ( ----- t ..... ) df- 9 18 1.4444 1.0416 ( t ) 6, 8 9 1.2222 1.0929 ( * ) 139 15 9 1.3750 0.517s ( . ) 3 6 1.9333 0.7529 ( - ) N.S. 19 6 2.0000 1.2649 7 . , - POOLED STDEV - 0.9629 0.60 1.20 1.90 2.40 Table 4.9 (cont Action 10 (vote) inued). Rokeach N MEAN STDEV Z21“‘ 72 1.472 0 949 4 27 1.052 1 167 9 19 1.944 0.973 3 9 1.556 1.130 15 9 1.975 0.641 6 2.500 0.549 is 6 1.333 1.033 POOLED STDEV - 0.975 Action 11 (Campaign) Rokeach value 14 N MEAN STDEV 4 72 0.06944 0.30611 9 27 0 19519 0 49334 a 19 0.50000 0.95749 15 9 0.55556 1.01379 3 6 0.93333 0.99319 13 6 0.33333 0.51640 POOLED STDEV - 0.54783 Action 12 (report lawbreaker) Rokeach value i4 N MEAN srnsv 72 0.06944 0.25599 9 27 0.22222 0.69798 8 9 22222 0.66667 15 6 0.50000 0.83666 3 6 0 16667 0.40925 POOLED STDEV - 0.46819 Action 13 (file lawsuit) Rokeach value N MEAN STDEV 14 72 0.06944 0.25599 4 27 0.22222 0.69799 9 19 0 9 9 0.22222 0.66667 15 a 0 0 3 6 0.50000 0.83666 5 0.16667 0.40925 19 . POOLED STDEV ‘ 0.46819 Action 14 (run/serve in office) Rokeach value N MEAN STDEV 4 72 0.1111 0 4613 27 0.2963 0 7240 9 19 0.611 1.0369 3 3 0.2222 0.4410 15 3 6 0.3333 0.8165 19 5 POOLED STDEV - 0.6393 95 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV + + + 1.81 (—-~--) ( -------- ) df- ( ————— * ----- ) 6, ( -------- . ------- ) 139 ( -------- ~ ------ -—) ( --------- * ---------- )N.s. ( ---------- * --------- ) 0.75 1.50 2.25 3.00 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON 900199 STDEV F- . - 2.65 ——*—— ( _____ )____) d£_ ( ----- t ----- ) 6 v 1 -------- ~ -------- ) 139. (---- —————— ~ ---------- ) ( ---------- * ---------- ) P<.05 -0.00 0.40 0.90 1.20 . INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN E- BASED ON POOLED STDEV 0.74 <--*--—> df- ————— fi—_—--) ) 6 I ( """"" * """"" 139 ( ------------ ~ ----------- ) ‘ ‘‘‘‘‘‘‘‘‘ “7": 777777 ’ N.s. 0.00 0.30 0.60 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDE F- 1.67 (--~--) (""*7--) df- (£::': 6. ________ ._______) ‘ ‘7) 139 ( --------- .- ....... ( ________ . _________ > ) N.s. 0.00 0 75 1.50 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI's FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV 5'01 (---~:—) 1 96 group showing the highest level of action was Rokeach #3 (a sense of accomplishment). Question #2 (demonstration) showed that the group ranking peace highest had the highest action level, followed very closely by the group ranking self—respect highest. Question #9 (donate $) had the highest level of action from the group stating that wisdom was the highest priority value. When all 19 social/political questions were considered (regardless of level of significance) it was found that the group ranking a sense of accomplishment most highly was the most active group in 12 of the 19 questions (63%). The second means of analysis separated the data by means of the Rokeach dominant terminal value ranked highest priority. Each of these seven groups was analyzed by ANOVA to find whether there were significant differences among the levels of the 19 actions (Table 4.10). In every case, there were significant differences among the frequencies of actions. The groups with Rokeach dominant terminal values 3,4,8,9,14, and 15 had values of F high enough that P<.01. Rokeach dominant terminal value 18 was significant at P<.05. Although there were differences among the frequencies of social/political actions within each group, the profiles of the different groups were quite similar. In almost all groups, the lowest frequency actions were file a lawsuit, report a violation of the law, and run for 9'7 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each of the seven most common dominant Rokeach terminal values when comparing mean action frequencies among all social/political actions. Table 4.10. Dominant terminal Rokeach value 14 (salvation) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Social/political 34560 on 900150 STDEU F- action N MEAN 57050 --+ --------- + --------- + --------- +----32.99 1 72 0058333 0480053 ("t-‘) 2 72 0.09722 0.29934 <--4--> df- 3 72 0.99611 0.96399 <~-4--> 18. 4 72 1.12500 0.93913 (--:--) 1349 5 72 0.22222 0.50969 (--4--) 5 72 0.26399 0.59123 (--t--) P<.01 7 72 0.06944 0.30611 (--x--> 8 72 0.47222 0.76961 <-~4~-> 9 72 0.77779 . 0.93792 <--4--) 1° 72 1.47222 0.94901 <--4-> 11 72 0.44444 0.74950 (--x--> 12 72 0.34722 0.60995 <--4~-) 13 72 0.06944 0.25599 <--x--) 14 72 0.11111 0.46134 <--4--1 15 72 0.97222 0.97165 (--4--> 16 72 1.31944 0.94695 (--:--> 17 72 1.56944 0.99047 <--4—-1 18 72 1.39999 1.06994 <--4--> 19 72 0.99611 0.92680 <-~9--> . --+ --------- + ------ ~--+ --------- +---- 900150 57050 = 0.76395 0.00 0.60 1.20 1.90 Dominant terainal Rokeach value 4 (peace) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT 01's FOR MEAN Social/political BASED ON POOLED STDEv 3' action N MEAN 57050 -——+ 4 ‘4 — -+--- 10-62 1 27 1.1491 0.9499 <---a----) 2 27 0.5195 0.6427 <----4----) df- 3 27 1.2963 0.9699 <---x----> 18' 4 27 1.2963 0.9699 <---4----) ‘94 5 27 0.4444 0.6405 <----4----) 6 27 0.6667 0.9609 <---~4----) P<°°1 7 27 0.1952 0.4933 <---4----) 8 27 1.0741 1.1068 (---4----> 9 27 1.7407 1.0225 <---4----) 10 27 1.9519 1.1670 <----4---) 11 27 0.9630 1.0554 <----4---) 12 27 0.2222 0.6990 <~---0----> 13 27 0.2222 0.6990 <---—4-—--) 14 27 0.2963 0.7240 (—---4---~) 15 27 1.0370 1.2955 c--—-4---) 16 27 1.0370 0.9399 , (----6---, 17 27 1.9519 1.0267 (---—x---) 19 27 1.7779 0.9337 <-~--x---) 19 27 1.2593 1.0225 (----1---, --~+ --------- + --------- + --------- +--- POOLED 57050 = 0.9217 0.00 0.75 1.50 2.25 Table 4.10 (continued). 9E3 Dominant terminal Rokeach value 9 (health) Social/political action N g 1. 3 18 4 l8 5 18 6 18 7 18 18 3 19 9 19 10 13 11 1a 12 13 13 13 14 13 15 1a 16 13 17 13 18 13 19 18 MEAN 1.1667 0.3889 1.1667 1.5000 0.5556 0.7778 0.5000 1.1667 1.7778 1.9444 0.7778 0.2778 0 0.6111 1.5000 1.3889 1.8889 2.0000 1.4444 200120 smnrv - 0.0572 STDEV 0.9852 0.7775 0.8575 0.9852 0.9218 1.1144 0.8575 1.1504 0.8782 0.8726 1.0603 0.4609 0 1.0369 1.0981 1.0922 0.8324 0.9701 1.0416 Dominant terminal Rokeach value 8 Social/political action 0‘0‘0‘0‘0‘09‘00‘0‘0‘0‘070‘0‘00‘0‘02 MEAN 0.9999 0.1111 1.2222 1.1111 0.4444 0.4444 0.5556 1.1111 1.6667 1.5556 0.9999 0.1111 0.2222 0.2222 0.6667 0.9999 2.0000 1.9999 1.2222 POOLED STDEU = 0.8438 Dominant terminal Rokeach value 15 (self-respect) Social/political action ommmmmm OOQOOQQQODOOZ MEAN 0.8750 0.5000 0.8750 1.0000 0.3750 0.5000 0 1.3750 1.7500 1.8750 0.1250 0.3750 0 0 1.0000 1.3750 1.6250 1.8750 1.3750 '900120 92029 - 016347 STDEU 0.7817 0.3333 0.9718 0.6009 0.5270 0.5270 1.0138 1.0541 1.1180 1.1304 1.0541 0.3333 0.6667 0.4410 0.7071 1.1667 0.5000 1.0541 1.0929 STDEV 0.6409 0.7559 0.3536 0.5345 0.5175 0.5345 0 0.9161 0.6409 0.3536 0.5175 0 0 0.7559 0.5175 0.9161' 0.6409' 0.5175 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI 5 FOR HEAN BASED ON 900150 97020 F- ...... +--------_+ +-----_---+-__------+ 7. 64 < ----- 9----> < ----- x—---) df- ( “““ *‘“‘*) 187 1 ----- x----> 323 (----t~--~) 1 ----- 4----> P<.01 <--—~x ----- ) < ----- r~---) <----x ----- ) (---—x ----- ) 1 ----- 4----) (----4 ----- 1 (----t----) < ----- 4----) < ----- 4----) (--—-4 ------ ) ( ----- a----) (----4----) <----r~-~-> ------ +---------+—--------+~---—----+ 0.00 0.90 1.60 2.40 (freedom) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI 5 FOR MEAN BASED ON 900150 STDEV F' ..... +------ T +-- _+_4.39 1 ----- t----) (----t ----- ) . di- (--—-9 ----- 1 13' (--—-x ----- 7 152 (-~9-8 ----- ) (----3 ..... ) P<.01 1 ----- 4----) <—---1 ----- 1 < ----- t----) c ----- 4---—) ( ----- x----) <—-~-t ----- 1 1-~-~x ----- ) (----t ----- 1 ( ----- 4----> < ----- x----) < ----- 4 ----- ) < ----- l----) <-—--4 ----- ) ----- +~--------+ 4 —-+- 0.0 1.0 2.0 3.0 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN 94990 on 900190 91090 F' ...... +---------+---------+---------+ 10.15 1 ----- x----> ( ----- 4----> df- 1 ----- x----> 18: (----4 ----- 1 133 (-—--x----) ( ..... *----) P<.01 <---—x----> (-~--* ----- 1 <----x ----- 1 <----4----> 1 ----- x—---> <-~--x—---> (----4----> 1-—--x----) 1~-~-x ----- 1 <----4 ----- 1 < ----- x----> (----4----> <----4 ----- 1 ------ +--—------+- — 4 — -+ 99) Table 4.10 (continued). Dominant terminal Rokeach value 3 (sense of accomplishment) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’ 8 FOR MEAN Social/political BASED ON 900190 STDEU 7' action N HEAN 91090 ----+ --------- +--- 4 —+-- 3'73 1 6 1.6667 0.9165 1 ...... 1 ..... ) 2 6 0.3333 0.5164 1 ----- 4 ------ 1 df- 3 6 1.3333 1.0329 1 ----- 4 ------ > 18. 4 ‘6 1.5000 1.0499 1 ------ 4 —————— ) 95 5 6 0.3333 0.5164 1 ----- 4 ------ > 5 6 1.3333 0.9165 < ----- 4 ------ ) P<~°1 7 6 0.9333 0.9932 1 ----- 4 ------ > 3 6 1.6667 0.5164 < ------ 4 ----- > 9 6 1.6667 0.5164 1 ------ 4 ----- > 10 6 2.5000 0.5477 ( ...... 4 ...... , 11 6 1.3333 1.0329 c ----- 4 ------ > 12 6 0.6667 0.9165 1 ------ 4 ----- ) 13 6 0.5000 0.9367 1 ------ 4 ------ 1 14 6 0.3333 0.9166 < ----- 4 ------ 1 15 6 1.3333 1.2111 1 ----- 4 ------ ) 15 6 1.9333 0.7529 1 ----- 4 ------ 1 17 6 2.0000 0.6325 1 ------ 4 ------ > 13 6 2.0000 0.9944 1 ------ 4 ------ ) 19 6 1.9333 0.7529 1 ----- 4 ------ 1 . ‘ ----+—- —-—+ - 4 —— -+-- 900190 91090 = 0.9196 0.0 1.0 2.0 3.0 Dominant terminal Rokeach value 18 (wisdom) INDIvIDUAL 95 901 CI 3 FOR MEAN Social/political . BASED ON 900190 91090 9- action N 5253 srngv -------------------- % --- 2.05 1 6 1.5000 1.2247 ( ------- t ------- ) 2 6 0.3333 0.5164 1 ------- 4 -------- > a:- 3 6 1.5000 1.3794. 1 ------- 4 ------- 1 13, 4 6 1.1667 1.1690 < -------- 4 ------- ) 95 s 6 1.0000 1.2649 1 ------- 4 ------- 1 6 6 0.5000 0.8367 ( ------- t ------- ) p<,os 7 6 0.3333 0.5164 1 ------- 4 -------- ) 9 6 1.5000 1.3794 ( ....... . ....... , 9 6 1.9333 1.1690 ( ....... . ________ , 10 6 1.3333 1.0329 ( ....... . ........ , 11 6 1.0000 0.9944 ( ....... 4 ....... , 12 6 0.5000 0.9367 ( _______ 1 _______ , 13 6 0.1667 0.4092. ( ________ 4 _______ , 14 6 ° . 4 0 < ------- 4 ------- ) 15 6 1.1667 1.1690 ( ........ ‘ ....... , 15 6 0.6667 0.9165 ( ........ , _______ , 17 6 1.6667 1.2111 ( ........ , _______ , 13 6 1.6667 1.3663 ( ........ 1 ....... , 19 6 2.0000 1.2649 . .............. , 900190 srnzv -.1.0699_ 100 or serve in public office. In five of the seven groups (those ranking peace, health, freedom, self-respect, and salvation highest), the action type reported most frequently involved education. Note that this is not to say that the frequencies of educational actions were the same among these groups. The highest levels of action were in the group choosing Rokeach value #3 (a sense of accomplishment) as first priority. Here voting behaviors were highest in frequency, with educational behaviors ranking second. Despite the fact that educational behaviors were ranked second here, they 'were at approximately the same levels that they were in the preceding five groups. The high frequencies of voting behavior distinguished this group from the other groups. For those ranking wisdom (#18) as the highest priority, the most frequent actions involved attending meetings and donating money. Other pertinent data A comparison of Section III (perceived concern or commitment necessary to take environmental action) between environmental groups and church groups When mean data for Section III was compared between the environmental groups and the church groups, it was seen to be very similar (Table 4.11). Analysis of variance to IIIII......IIIIIIIIl-III-nnr— lOl compare mean scores for the importance of each environmental action showed that none of the differences were significant. Table 4.11. A comparison of mean scores for Section III (perceived concern or commitment necessary to take environmental action) between environmental groups and church groups. Item Description Mean for Mean for church env. F: 1 sign petition 1.43 1.41 0.02 2 distribute petition 2.44 2.33 1.06 3 write/call official 2.27 2.29 0.06 4 campaign 2.60 2.73 2.23 5 vote 1.54 1.59 0.19 6 pick up litter 1.75 1.61 1.64 7 recycling 2.00 2.01 0.02 8 turn off lights 1.49 1.47 0.07 9 purchase - environmental 1.92 1.91 0.02 10 distribute info 2.34 2.39 0.24 11 discuss environment 1.90 1.88 0.04 12 speech or appearance 2.78 2.81 0.22 13 report violation 2.44 2.40 0.11 14 file lawsuit 2.78 2.84 0.58 15 file injunction 2.76 2.80 0.16 16 stop buying harmful 1.82 1.68 1.67 17 donate money 1.83 2.01 3.44 102 Rokeach comparisons : Median values and dominant values In some cases, it was advantageous to compare data from this studywith that from Rokeach's other books or papers. The most common form of ordering data done by Rokeach himself (e.g. 1973) was to use ranking of median scores. He assigned a score to each value, based on the priority ranking given to the value by the respondent, then computed the median score for that value. Next he ranked the values by median score. This allows the use of non—parametric tests to compare the groups, but does not allow the 'experimenter to use the individuals whose responses went into the median score for any other comparisons. Figure 4.1 shows the comparisons between median scores of the six top-ranking terminal values from Rokeach's national survey, from his survey of recyclers, from this study's environmental groups, from Rokeach's 1968 Baptist church subsample,and from this study's church groups. This does not include the 12 less important scores. It can be seen that there are many differences among these groups. This study used the value ranked as first priority by the respondent for ordering data. This allowed the data to be grouped according to which of Rokeach’s terminal values was most important to the respondent. The researcher recognized that the two procedures are different, but needed to be able to group the data without sacrificing the ability to form other groups based on the individual's responses. 103 Rokeach national sample (1968) 1. peace 2. family security 3. freedom 4. equality 5.se1f-respect 6. happiness * (salvation 9th) Rokeach recyclers (1973) l. wisdom 2. self-respect 3. family security ,4. inner harmony 5. freedom 6. love (peace 10th) (salvation 18th) Rokeach Baptist churches (1968) peace family security salvation freedom self—respect happiness * O 0 O O (DUTQUUNH O 0 Environmental groups (1984) 1 peace 2 health ** 3. family security 4. freedom 5 inner harmony 6. self— respect (salvation 18th) Free Methodist churches (1984) salvation health ** family security inner harmony self— respect freedom (peace 11th) fhmbWNH O O O O O * deleted by Rokeach from current version of survey ** added by Rokeach to current version of survey Figure 4.1. Priority rankings for the t0p six Rokeach Terminal Values of selected groups — arranged in descending order by median score. 104 Use of median score rank in this study would preclude recognizing individuals and forming new groups to study the actions of those individuals (e.g. to compare levels of environmental action among the groups with the most commonly occurring Rokeach dominant terminal values). One consequence of the decision to rank values by the dominant method rather than by the median method is that it is not possible to use nonparametric analysis to find whether there are statistically significant differences between the groups. Comparisons were made of the results of the two methods 'of ranking. Figure 4.2 compares the different methods of ranking — by median score and by dominant score using the current study groups. Included in Table 4.13 are only the tOp six scores from the set of 18 terminal values. These two methods appear relatively similar, at least when comparing the six values which are given highest priority. Finally, this researcher attempted to take a subsample from this research which would compare to Dunlap et al (1983). Respondents who had made any positive response to either of the questions on recycling paper or glass were grouped and analyzed. This was to simulate the population which might have been encountered at a glass and paper recycling center. The frequency scores for each dominant Rokeach terminal value were summed for those who had answered "seldom", "occasionally", or "frequently" to either 105 Environmental groups by median score by dominant score peace peace health health family security salvation freedom self—respect inner harmony freedom self-respect accomplishment Free Methodist groups by median score by second priority score peace family security health inner harmony accomplishment freedom * self—respect * by dominant score ** salvation salvation health family security family security freedom inner harmony health self—respect inner harmony freedom wisdom * tied for sixth position ** numbers 2~6 here are based on the frequency of the second priority score, since "salvation" was virtually always first priority for this group Figure 4.2. A comparison of the top six median to dominant Rokeach terminal value scores for environmental and Free Methodist groups — in order of decreasing priority. 106 of these questions. These numbers were then divided by the number of individuals in the entire survey having those dominant terminal values, to result in a score representing the proportion of individuals with a given dominant terminal value who were recyclers. These proportions were ranked from highest to lowest (Table 4.12). It was seen that the three dominant terminal values groups most often recycling paper or glass are self—respect, wisdom, and sense of accomplishment. These are different from the three dominant terminal values most often seen in environmental groups as a whole. These are also all representative of Maslow's higher ‘order values, according to Dunlap et a1 (1983). 107 Table 4.12. Ranking of dominant Rokeach terminal values of recyclers in this research. Rokeach number of total n proportion of value recyclers of group recyclers 4 14 27 52% 14 13 72 18% 9 9 18 50% 15 6 8 75% 8 5 9 55% 18 4 6 67% 3 4 6 67% Ranked proportions (by group) % active Rokeach Rokeach recyclers number value 75% 15 self—respect 67% 18 wisdom 67% 3 sense of accomplishment 55% 8 freedom 52% 4 peace 50% 9 health 108 Predictors of environmental action Simple and multiple stepwise linear regression were used to attempt to find the predictive relationship of each part of the survey by itself and in combination on WES. Social-political score and Rokeach dominant terminal value were found to be the best predictors of WES. Their independent contributions and their joint contribution to the variance within WES can be seen in Table 4.13. Table 4.13. Simple and multiple stepwise linear regression of the effects of SPS, Rokeach dominant terminal value and Maslow level on WES. factor R2 F= P= SPS .275 61.42 (.01 Rokeach dominant term. value .155 29.74 (.01 Maslow level .003 0.49 N.S. SPS + Rokeach .342 SPS + Maslow level .275 Rokeach + Maslow level .156 SPS + Rokeach + Maslow level .343 109 Environmentally oriented church peOple There was a small number of people who would qualify for both the environmental and the conservative church groups. From among the environmental groups, nine persons ranked salvation as their primary terminal value. From among those in the church groups, six stated that they were also in some type of environmental group. Comparative data for these people are seen in Table 4.14. The means for the subgroups are very similar to the means of the whole group. Table 4.14. WES comparisons of all in each group vs those also claiming affiliation with the other group. Mean WES for env. group Mean WES for env. subgroup (all) with salvation #1 priority mean = 123.85 mean = 125.22 range = 328 — 31 range 212 — 66 Mean WES for church Mean WES for church group (all) group also in env. group mean = 53.85 mean = 49.67 range = 150 - 12 range 107 — 15 110 Maslow levels of those ranking salvation first priority It was seen that those giving salvation first priority had great heterogeneity of values. When Maslow levels were used to further categorize these persons, some differences became apparent. Fifty-three of the 72 persons responding that salvation was their primary value were from either the self—actualizers (34 at Maslow I) or the physiological level (19 at Maslow VI). Their presence at the extremes of the hierarchy led to further examination of their values. When the frequency of the value they placed second was examined, 'it was found that these two groups were more different than might have been expected (Figure 4.3). The relative positions of freedom and family security should be noted, as should the presence of the wisdom value only in the self-actualizing group. Physiological level Self—actualizers family security freedom health inner harmony freedom wisdom self-respect family security Figure 4.3. Second priority values of those choosing salvation as their first priority value — for physiological level vs. self—actualizers. 111 Summary of results Research Question 1 Is there a relationship between Maslow level and environmental action (level and type); between Maslow level and social/political action (level and type)? Very few important or statistically significant relationships were found using Beer's Maslow survey. ’The top three Maslow levels (self—actualization, self—esteem, and esteem of others) showed highest frequencies of social/political action for the "education" item. Voting behaviors were the second most frequent type item. Maslow levels IV and VI placed voting behaviors as their highest frequency activity. Since the frequency of voting behaviors was at approximately the same level in all groups, it can be seen that the frequency of participation in education was less in the lower Maslow levels. Research Question 2 Are there relationships between an individual's Maslow level and priorities on Rokeach's terminal values survey? When the seven terminal values most frequently chosen 112 as first priority were analyzed for mean Maslow level, no significant relationships were found. Research Question 3 Is there a relationship between the priority of specific elements of Rokeach's terminal values survey and environmental activism (level and type); between priority of specific elements of Rokeach's terminal values survey and social/political activism (level and type)? .1. Among the seven most common dominant Rokeach terminal value groups, those ranking salvation dominant are significantly less environmentally active than the other groups. 2. There were many significant relationships between type of environmental action and Rokeach dominant terminal values (Figure 4.4). a. When each of the 45 environmental actions was analyzed for differences in action frequency among the most common seven Rokeach dominant terminal values, the following patterns emerged: the self—respect group had highest levels of action in voting and recycling. the sense of accomplishment group had the highest levels of actions in influencing elected officials, 113 Env. sig. diff. among Rokeach sig. diff. among action Rokeach values value env. action type 1.a 14 (salvation) x 1.b 4 (peace) x 1.0 x 9 (health) x l.d 8 (freedom) x 2.a x 18 (wisdom) x 2.b x 15 (self—respect) x 2.c x 3 (sense of x 2.d x accomplishment) 3.a x 3.b x 4.a x 4.b x 5.a x 5.b x 5.c x 6.a x 6.b x 6.0 x 7.a x 7.b x 7.c x 8.a 8.b 8.c 9.a x 9.b 9.c x 9.d 10.a x 10.b x 11.a X 11.b x 12.a x 12.b x 12.c x 13.a 13.b 14.a x 14.b 15.a 15.b x 16 x l7.a x 17.b x 17.c x Type of env. action vs Rokeach dominant Figure 4.4. terminal values. 114 purchasing with environmnental considerations, refraining from purchasing a harmful product, and donations. b. When each of the seven most commonly occurring Rokeach dominant terminal values was analyzed, comparing frequencies among the 17 types of environmental actions, every ANOVA was statistically significant. Patterns emerging were the following: the salvation group was lowest in action frequency in most questions. the peace group had the greatest number of behaviors occurring at frequencies above 70%. the self-respect group had the highest frequency of environmentally motivated voting behavior. 3. When each of the seven most commonly occurring Rokeach dominant terminal values was analyzed by comparing frequencies among social/political actions, lower frequencies of action by the salvation group caused significant differences among the groups. 4. There were many significant relationships between type of social/political action and Rokeach dominant terminal values (Figure 4.5). a. When each of the 19 social/political actions was analyzed for differences in action frequency among the most common seven Rokeach dominant terminal values, seven of the analyses were statistically significant, showing the 115 soc/pol sig. diff. among Rokeach sig. diff. among action Rokeach values value soc/pol actions 1 x 14 (salvation) x 2 x 4 (peace) x 3 9 (health) x 4 8 (freedom) x 5 15 (self—respect) x 6 x 18 (wisdom) x 7 x 3 (sense of x 8 x accomplishment) 9 x 10 x 11 x 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 Figure 4.5. Type of social/political action vs dominant Rokeach terminal values. 116 following patterns: for five of the seven significant analyses, the lowest frequency of action was found in the salvation group. for five of the seven significant analyses, the highest frequency of action was found in the sense of accomplishment group. b. When each of the seven most commonly occurring Rokeach dominant terminal values was analyzed, comparing frequencies among the 19 social/political actions, every ANOVA had statistically significant results. Some of the 'patterns to emerge from the data were the following: for five of the seven value groups, the action having the highest frequency was education. the sense of accomplishment group had voting as its most frequent action (although education was ranked as highly as in the previous five groups). the wisdom group had attending meetings as its highest frequency action. Chapter 5 DISCUSSION This chapter will first summarize findings for each research question, and then describe implications of the findings for the future of environmental education and 'environmental communications. It will also consider relationships within the results which influence validity and applicability to other studies. Included with this is a section entitled "Environmental groups vs. Rokeach's recyclers" which explores possible reasons for the findings that the personality profiles of this study’s recyclers are nearly opposite those of the study performed by Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach (1983). Demographic data The members of the two organizations are not similar. Observation of demographic data shows the largest_ differences between groups are in the areas of education, 117 118 occupation, and home site. The church groups are not as highly educated, more likely to answer "housewife" for occupation, and less likely to live in an urban environment. Response bias Because of the lack of a survey of non—respondents to determine possible bias, it will not be known whether the respondent groups are completely representative of the general populations from which they were drawn. Relationships between Maslow level and environmental or social/political behavior (Research Question 1) In general, Maslow level (as measured by the modified Beer instrument) does not appear to be a good predictor of either environmental or social/political behavior. Despite this poor predictive value, some relationships did appear. The group giving self-esteem (Maslow level II) highest priority appears to be more environmentally active than any other Maslow level group. These individuals reported higher levels of activity than the other Maslow levels in 33 of the 45 actions, but differences were not always significant. For this group, high frequency involvement was primarily in donations, voting, and environmental discussions. 119 Though statistically significant differences among Maslow levels were not observed for any of the social/political actions tested, the self—esteem group (Maslow level II) was seen to be highest in social/political action frequency in 7 of the 19 possible social/political actions. The relationship between voting behaviors and involvement in education appeared to be important when Maslow level was analyzed to find whether there were differences among social/political behavior frequencies. Voting was one of the uniformly high frequency behaviors. At the top three Maslow levels (self—actualization, self—esteem, and esteem of others), education activities were reported even more frequently than was voting. For the lower Maslow levels (need for love, physiological - IV and VI), the frequency of participation in social/political education activities was below the frequency of participation in voting activities. Education appears to be less important to the lower Maslow levels. This supports the conclusions of Harry Miller (1971). Based on the results (table 12), this researcher would not recommend using Beer's measure of Maslow’s personality hierarchy to explain environmental or social/political action. The reason that this measure of Maslow level was not a significant predictor of action in the current study may be 120 one or a combination of several. One may be that the Maslow framework is simply too broad to use as a predictive basis for specific actions. Support for this possibility comes from Edward C. Tolman, a behaviorist with force~field theory beliefs. He agreed that needs exist, feeling that needs are inherent in being human. Whether they are physical, social, or psychological, they exist. But, he states, it is an individual’s values which are applied to situations that might satisfy the needs. The individual’s values are based on that individual’s total life experiences. Thus Tolman states that a given need may have several value/action sets which might satisfy it in different persons (Cofer and Appley, 1964). A second reason for the lack of Maslow's needs being a predictor of action may be that despite the results of initial validity tests, this tool for measuring Maslow level was not valid. Maslow’s levels are not meant to be static or exclusive. Maslow saw persons working their way, in general, up the hierarchy. But a person could be working on several levels at one time, leading to an invalid assessment of Maslow level when the test reports just one level. Further, Maslow projected that about one per—cent of the population were self—actualized. The Beer instrument’s results were that nearly half (74/164) of the respondents were self—actualizing. A part of the discrepancy may come from the tense of "self—actualize". It is expected that there would be many more persons working on 121 self-actualization than had already achieved it. Another reason for the difference may come from the non—random sample. The groups sampled, and the respondents from those groups, may have varied significantly from a random sample of the general population. The relationship between Maslow level and Rokeach terminal values (Research Question 2) Maslow level, as indicated by Beer's survey, was shown 'to be an ineffective predictor of Rokeach Terminal Values. When ANOVA was used to compare the two, no relationships were found among dominant Rokeach values and Maslow levels. This is in contrast to the findings of Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach (1983), who stated that higher Maslow levels would be characterized by certain Rokeach values being ranked very highly. A further discussion of the differences between this research and their paper will follow in the section titled "Environmental groups vs. Rokeach's recyclers" One relationship which did emerge from analyzing this question was that those who ranked family security as the highest value had a mean Maslow level of 4.6. This corresponds quite well to Maslow level V which is safety/security, providing more evidence of construct validity. 122 Relationships between Rokeach values and environmental or social/political behavior (Research Question 3) Rokeach's Terminal Values showed a greater predictive power than Maslow's hierarchy. Table 4 showed that the salvation variable was the best predictor of the total amount of environmental action. Those people placing salvation as their first priority value were significantly less likely to have a high WES. It was also found that among the terminal values, there was significance to amounts of various actions. These values will be examined in more detail in the following paragraphs. Profiles of respondents - Rokeach Terminal Values In order to better understand the groups responding to the survey, the data were examined by separating the church respondents from those in environmental groups. For the church groups the dominant terminal value, almost without exception, was salvation. In order of frequency, the second most important values were family security, freedom, health, inner harmony, and wisdom (Table 0" 13). The first priority values of the environmental groups 123 were peace, health, salvation, self—respect, freedom, and a sense of accomplishment (in order of decreasing frequency). When second priority scores were included for examination, family security was seen to be important to this group as well. Second priority scores reinforce the set shown in first priority except for salvation. Salvation appears to show a pattern of either being first priority or being relatively unimportant. The primary difference between the two groups is the identity of the first priority value. Salvation is extremely important to the church group, but is relatively unimportant to most persons in the environmental group. Peace is most important to those in environmental groups, but is relatively unimportant to those in church groups. Normative Data — Rokeach’s terminal values As may be seen in Rokeach’s assessments of 1409 American men and women (Figure 1), neither of the groups surveyed in the current study is completely representative of the 1968 national norms. The 1968 national survey reported the first three values in importance to be peace, family security, and freedom When the national survey was made, the value "health" was not a part of the survey. When using either median scores or dominant scores in comparing Rokeach’s recyclers to this study’s 124 environmentalists, or when comparing Rokeach's Baptists to this study's Free Methodists, differences were far more evident than similarities. Comparing the columns of Figure 1 allows the differences between the church and environmental groups to be seen in another way. These median comparisons show again that peace and salvation are again the best discriminators between the two groups. The church groups rank salvation first and peace eleventh. The environmental groups rank peace first and salvation last among the eighteen values. In this study, most of the other values are at approximately 'the same rank. There are important differences among even the most comparable groups. Environmentally oriented church people It was seen that there are environmentally active persons who also rank salvation as their highest priority terminal value, and that there are church people who also belong to environmental groups. These data are encouraging in that they show it is not impossible for members of church groups to be also involved with environmental groups. Conversely, it is not impossible for environmentalists to be religious. The two groups are not mutually exclusive. One additional consideration in this comparison is that the demographic section of the survey did not ask the 125 respondent to identify the nature of the environmental organization. The church people who responded that they belonged to an environmental organization may have belonged to a garden club or to the Sierra Club. Membership in the former type of organization might result in a much lower environmental action score (as was seen) than membership in the latter. Heterogeneity of those ranking salvation first priority In the section on utility of Maslow's hierarchy, it was 'seen that Beer's assessment of Maslow level was not a strong predictive tool. The possibility that it was too broad was discussed. That possibility was borne out when it was used in combination with Rokeach's values - as in the section "Maslow levels of those ranking salvation first priority". The difference in second priority values between the self-actualizing group (Maslow I) and the physiological group (Maslow VI) conforms exactly with the predictions which would be based on Maslow's hierarchy. Freedom and wisdom are characterized as "higher order" values, while family security is classed as a "lower order" value by Rokeach himself (Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach, 1983). Maslow level may have value as one of several possible secondary importance influences which would further explain variance among those who are or are not environmentally active. 126 Environmental groups vs. Rokeach's Recyclers It was instructive to compare the results of the current research to that done by Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach in 1973 (published in 1983) surveying users of a recycling center. Recycling glass or paper was used by Dunlap et a1 as the indicator of environmental action. Terminal values profiles of Rokeach's recyclers showed that their top five values (ranked in order of decreasing importance on the basis of median scores) were wisdom, 'self-respect, family security, inner harmony, and freedom. World at peace ranked 10th. Compared with the environmental groups in the current survey, there are several differences. Here the top five terminal values ,in decreasing order of median scores, are the following: peace, health (not included in the 1973 version of the survey), family security, freedom, and inner harmony. Self-respect ranked sixth and wisdom ranked seventh (Figure 2). The values ranked three through five are essentially the same in both studies, but those ranked at the top are quite different. Dunlap's survey placed wisdom as most important; the current survey placed it seventh (of eighteen). Dunlap's survey placed self-respect second; the current survey placed it sixth. And peace, ranked first in this survey, was ranked tenth in Dunlap's survey. 127 These differences are extremely important — more than just a few isolated factors. They form a pattern essentially opposite what Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach (1983) concluded was representative of environmentally active persons. The theoretic basis of their paper was that higher order values (in the Maslowian sense) would characterize environmental activists. This study shows that the three values ranked highest by median score for environmental groups are peace, health, and family security. All three of these are characterized as lower order values by Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach. The hypothesis that 'higher order values are uniformly found among environmentally active persons is not supported by this study. The current study showed that each type of environmental action has its own best Rokeach dominant terminal value predictor. Environmentally active persons vary in their values and in their frequency of taking different actions. There is no single best value predictor or value profile of all types of environmental actions. The differences between this survey and that of Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach may have come from a number of factors. The first might be temporal. In the eleven years between the two surveys, many values may have changed. But given Rokeach’s own definition of a value as "an enduring belief", and his .69 reliability score after 14 to 16 months, and given the fact that the Dunlap, Grieneeks and 128 Rokeach paper was published only one year prior to the current research, this seems less likely than the second alternative. This researcher felt that the difference is due to the nature of the surveyed groups. Defining a person as environmentally active on the basis of one action (recycling in Dunlap’s research) might well have resulted in a different group than one based on any other action, or on active membership in an environmental organization. This research confirms that from among those active in environmental groups, the people most active in recycling 'have different value priorities than those who are most active in some other environmental activities. The recycler subgroup of the current study had as its three top dominant values self—respect, wisdom, and sense of accomplishment - very similar to the recyclers of Dunlap et a1 (1983). Other actions have their own values profiles, for example, attempting to influence elected officials (sense of accomplishment is the best predictor of this action), or campaigning at the state and national level for an environmentally concerned candidate (world at peace is the best predictor of this action). Environmental activists are not a homogeneous group; they are segmented into many subgroups according to their values and their action patterns. They cannot all be described by the same values profile. Any past study which used a single action as a basis for estimating total 129 environmental action or as a basis for understanding environmental action should be examined in this regard. This finding supports the intent of the current research — to find values important to specific segments of the population and use those particular values to most efficiently bring about an increase in environmental action or in environmental education. Comparisons of Concern/commitment Weights When the values assigned by respondent groups to the ("concern or commitment it would take to do certain actions" section (Section III of the survey) were analyzed, some unexpected results were seen. Table 11 shows that when each action was compared between the church groups and the environmental groups, the mean weights for perception of concern or commitment to do each of the environmental actions were nearly the same. None of the differences were statistically significant. Evidently the church people and those in environmental organizations both feel the same about the commitment it would take to do specific actions. The difference between the two groups comes in whether they do the actions. Implications for environmental communications or environmental education curriculum are that it is not necessary to educate someone about the amount of commitment to the environment necessary to take a particular action. °eInorxxno uorgeonpa Iejuamuoxrnua xo steadde [eguamuoxrnua xog sxxomamexg burzruebxo se pasn uaqm aouegxodmr geaxfi fitterguagod go axe Kaqg gnq 'asuas aArgarpaxd e u; uorgoe restarted/teroos uqu xuexxodmr seat axe SGHIPA Teurmxag gueurmop qaeaxoa °drqsuorgetax aqg go asn axem og xapxo u; aArgoe Atteorngod xo KIIeroos aq ox umoux asoqx on passaxppe aq steadde IeguamuoxrAua axngng gqu papuammooax s; gr 'xoqarpaxd e se anIeA agexapom go gsaq 3e 31 srqq qbnoqxtv °Kpngs quaxxna aqq u; uorgoe IeguamuoxrAua go xogorpaxd gsaq aqx sem uorgae Ieorgrtod/Ieroos go ISAGI gqu uaas S: gr 'sgrnsax aux go at anem moxa axons Ieguamuoxrnua go sxogarpaxd go aauegxodmr aAggeIau °gueargrufirs Buraq auou qgrm 'Items {1e exam (uorgrgad e ubrs °6°a) Kanns uorgoe Ieorgrrod/teroos aqq ur paxxnooo oste qarqm suorgae IeguamuoxrAua go sgqbram aouegxodmr go suosrxedmoa xog SBDIQA “a“ °suorgoe {eorgrtod/Ieraos axe: ox guamgrmmoo paAraoxad Kanns gou og uorsroap aqg sgxoddns osTe Burpurg srqm °uorgoe sq: axe: pInoqs auo qu pue xaqgaqm — IeuorgeArqom aq ptnoqs suorgearunmmoa IeguamuoxrAua pue uorgeanpa IeguamuoxrAua go gsnxqg aux gen: anbxe pInom xaqoxeasax srqm °uorgoe IeguamuoxrAua qnoqe aseq afipaImoux e aAeq qgoq neg: aouaxagur egg on spear guamgrmmoo paAraaxad uo IeAaI ames aux ge axons sdnoxfi qgoq gen: gaeg aqm OEI 131 Importance of knowledge of the relationship between values and environmental action While having modest value as current predictors of environmental action, knowledge of a group's values might have extreme importance when applied in a curricular sense or in an action campaign. Designing appeals or education to best meet the first (or very high) priority values of certain groups would maximize potential for involving the 'group. This would allow maximum efficiency of resource allocation, depending on the knowledge of the desired action or the specific value and action profile of the group. Implications for behavior change Rokeach described instances of using the values survey in a program of behavioral change (e.g. Penner, 1971, in Ball-Rokeach, 1984). This survey could be used to initiate change in the same way. In the studies Rokeach described, the results of the survey were used to generate dissatisfaction within the subjects. Rokeach (1973) and Ball—Rokeach et a1 (1984) have both reported that behavior change can result from experimental application of knowledge of a person's values. They write that self—dissatisfaction 132 is often the primary motivator for such change. When a person is confronted with an incongruence between his values and behavior,he acts to reduce the incongruence. The studies reported show that behaviors change to allign more completely with the person’s overall values. PeOple who were made aware that their values were inconsistent with their behaviors showed significant behavior change (Penner, 1971, in Ball-Rokeach et al, 1984). Penner's study dealt with inter-racial behavioral interactions and the importance of the value "equality". For the current study, it is expected that showing 'inconsistencies between a person's environmental actions and values will generate the same self—dissatisfaction. It is further expected that people will show change in behaviors if it can be demonstrated to them that such a change will both reduce the self—dissatisfaction and also increase self—esteem. According to Rokeach, the more specific the self-dissatisfaction, the better it serves to change the behavior. As he states,"...even persons who are believed to be extremely resistant to change can be induced to change their basic outlooks and actions" (p. 329, 1973). Implications for education and communications The results of the current study may be used in three ways. The first involves using groups similar to those surveyed - those for which this study might be a predictive 133 tool, such as other Free Methodist churches or Sierra Club chapters. When making appeals or designing and presenting educational materials, the personality and action information might be used directly. Those values most important to the individuals being served would be those best stressed in order to achieve action or learning. Figure 8 shows some specific value/action relationships which might be used in this application for environmental groups. When these particular groups are used, it may also be possible to use this research to generalize about the types 'of actions these persons are or are not involved in. This allows the communicator or educator to be more specific in whether he will provide detailed rationale for the action or simply describe the current situation and provide tools for dealing with the situation. The second way in which the study might be used involves understanding the values structures of a different group. It is possible to make inferences about values which are important to those people who make up an organization by bringing together information from diverse sources. The group charter or mission statement, interviews with leaders and members, press reports, and histories of the group will all lead to knowledge of the values most likely important to the members of the group. Knowledge of those values allows the practitioner to design the best communications or 134 curriculum to most directly affect and serve the intended audience. The third use involves first priority values and inferences based on those values. Inferences derived from examination of each of the first—priority values and other knowledge of the group can help understand other differences among the groups and can help in applying the values to education and communications. The first-priority value of the environmental group was peace. Dunlap et a1 (1983) state that peace is a lower ’order value, related to safety and security, but this author feels that it is a higher order value - one with a strong altruistic basis. Support for this contention comes from Ball-Rokeach et a1 (1984), who describe three experiments which raised the priority of the higher order values "equality" and "freedom". The priority of the value "peace" rose consistently in all three, even though it was not a goal of the experiments. This strong relationship to the two other altruistic, higher order values leads to the inference that peace is more likely one of them than one of the lower order values. A common element to both the value "peace" and environmental action is an other-directedness, also known as altruism. An altruistic action is not primarily or directly something which will advance the individual; rather it is done for others or for all mankind. 135 The first-priority value of the church group was salvation. Salvation is a personal and individual phenomenon, one which might be labelled "egoistic". This research suggests that persons who rank salvation as first priority are less environmentally active. Other research in progress (Harold Darling, Professor of Psychology, Spring Arbor College, personal communication) seems to indicate that persons with salvation ranked as first priority are less socially active. It is paradoxic that the church people seem to value helping others, but that they are actually found to be less ‘active than their non-church counterparts. Rokeach found that those who rank salvation highly think of themselves as more concerned with welfare of their fellow man than those who do not share their religious beliefs. But he reports that three separate studies using independent measures of social compassion all contradict that impression. Though the Bible is permeated by the exhortation to show love, care, and concern for others, the research projects just mentioned and the observations of other social scientists indicate that the exhortation is not yet completely actualized. This paradoxic finding lends itself perfectly to the cognitive dissonance model of behavior change which was mentioned earlier with reference to values. While there is a great deal of literature on the altruism—egoism continuum, very little of it deals with the 136 continuum as an organizing scheme for motivation and the resultant behaviors. It seems that the topic of altruism is most often reported in theoretical, philosophical, or mathematical articles. Survey scales to measure altruism per se either do not exist or were not discovered. Research showing relationships of the general characteristic of altruism to specific actions has not been undertaken or reported. There are some aspects of altruism which have been reported in behavioral terms. One of these is willingness to help. Factors seen to be important in the development of ‘helping behaviors include emotional security and self-confidence (Block, 1972, in Likona, 1976). Also important are a personal state of well-being and a need for fulfillment. Harden (1977) feels that these are important in increasing prosocial behaviors because of the reduced pressures toward egoistic self-concern. He further states that help gviving increases with higher in Kohlberg moral reasoning levels. Other studies have found that religion, humanitarianism, and social responsibility were not as important as knowledge of how to help and experience in helping in the past (Huston et al, 1981) and (Bateson and Gray, 1981). Apparently the development of an altruistic approach to life is a product of one's personal well-being, moral development, and knowledge. The relative importance and timing of each of these factors is still unknown. 137 The recommendation of this author is to more directly find whether a person is self— or other—centered. A semantic differential scale could be composed for several terms in the self/other or egoism/altruism continuum. The person's outcome could be compared with level and type of environmental activism. This would efficiently settle the question of whether the inference about self/other from the values "salvation" and "peace" is justified. The preceding paragraphs lead to two sets of recommendations for dealing with the groups in this study. 'The first recommendation involves the first priority values and their inferred corollaries. Appeals or curriculum directed toward environmental groups should incorporate the theme of "peace", but should also integrate the more general motivator of other-directed action which already exists within the population. This population may need direction more than motivation. Examples of such appeals might take the form of "You can best help future generations by ....... ", or "The most effective way you can do your part for the environment is ....... ", or possibly by using a slogan like "Declare 'Peace' on the Environment". The relationships of Figure 5.1 might also be applied very profitably in this regard. Using the matrix of Figure 5.1, it is possible to find which actions are most likely for an individual with a given value ranking highly, or conversely, it is possible to find which values ought to be 138 Action Rokeach Terminal Value I wisdom I peace I sense of I self— I healthI I I I accompl. I respect I I I vote I X I I X I X I I | I I I I I J I | I I I I I I I I I donate S I X I I X I I I I I I I I I I I I I L I I I I I I I influence I I I x I I I officials I I I I I I I I I I I I I | I I I l .Campaign I I X I I I I I I | I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I Public I I I I I X I appearance I I | I I I I I I I I I I I recycle I I I I X I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I purchase I I I X I I | I I I I I l I I I I I I I I I I I l boycott I I I X I I I I I I I | I I__ | I .1 I | Figure 5.1. Potentially effective relationships - to find most effective values to stress for specific actions or actions most likely given specific high priority values. 139 stressed to be most efficient in enlisting people to take a given action. While these relationships are for the most part not based on statistically significant differences in the data, it is felt that they are important and that they provide a good starting point for both action and further research. A similar type of model used for making appeals to those in church groups would emphasize both the individual nature of the primary value and the high value placed on belief in the Bible. The inward or individual orientation of the primary value might be used by showing direct 'personal gains to be had from specific environmental actions. An example of this is an appeal like the following: "Wilderness - will it be there to enjoy when you retire?". The second part of this inferred values constellation involves belief in the Bible. An example of a Biblical appeal would be "Would Noah have taken a pair of snail darters?". Biblical references to care for the environment will carry much more influence than would exhortations from other sources, especially others who are not in the same confidence group (in the Miller sense). If the first priority values are not going to be used directly, they should at least be used indirectly, as a checklist to make sure the curriculum or communications do not violate highly valued factors for either group, 140 The cognitive dissonance mentioned earlier might also be used very successfully with the church group. As was seen in previous paragraphs, the generalizations from this study and others are that church people are significantly less environmentally active than environmentalists. If this fact is made known to the church people, along with the reminder of their belief in the Bible, and teachings of the Bible about concern for the environment as well as for other people, they should quickly see the incongruity of their values, beliefs and actions. The incongruity model suggests that they will act to reduce the incongruity or dissonance. 'It is expected that this would involve becoming more environmentally active. One caution should be added here concerning source credibility. As Miller mentioned and as is intuitively understood, the conservative church group is not as trusting of the educational system as are some other groups. Despite the best of intentions, an environmental educator who attempted to communicate with such a group might find the attempts futile. A lack of credibility with the group might allow the group to ignore his or her message. Cognitive dissonance is a strong force; one of the easiest means to resolve it is to find fault with the source of the information which caused the dissonance. This prevents having to admit one's own irreconcilable beliefs, values, and actions. The environmental educator should take all possible steps to ensure source credibility. 141 Both the church groups and the environmental groups showed a very high priority for health and for family security. It would be wise to use these values in communications and curriculum which would be less specifically targeted. While the first priority value of each group is not perceived as extremely important to the other, the second two values are seen as important to both groups and might be most effectively used as the basis for a general action campaign. An example of such a campaign might involve the slogan "Protect your . family's health - stop pollution". When additional groups are surveyed and the results are more widely applicable, some of the other values less important in this study might be seen to be common among other groups and increase in their communicative value. Suggestions for future research 1. A study should be done to find whether the application of these results and recommendations is useful in increased response to communications or in increased participation and learning in educational programs. 142 2. It is suggested that similar surveys be used to find the values and action profiles of labor groups, the other traditionally non-involved group according to Miller (1971). 3. This instrument should be refined or combined with others already in existence (e.g. Sia, 1983) to account for more of the variance in motivation for environmental action. 4. A broader range of environmental groups and environmental behaviors should be sampled to present a fuller values profile of environmentally active persons. This would help to better resolve the differences between this research and that of Dunlap, Grieneeks, and Rokeach (1983). 5. A semantic differential scale should be constructed to test for a person's placement on the self/other continuum. 6. Factor analysis of the current data could find whether there was a self/other cluster within Rokeach's values, as well as whether the church and environmental groups differed on this factor. 143 7. These populations or others similar to them should be studied more intensively, possibly as a substudy of approximately 10 people in which interviews could discern more about the personality and environmental action factors of specific people. 8. Age may be a factor in personality and environmental action. This possibility should be investigated in regard to level and type of environmental action, as well as to the individual's placement on the self/other continuum. APPENDIX 144 Appendix A. Research Survey. MICHIGAN CITIZENS’ ENVIRONMENTAL ACTION AND ' PERSONAL PREFERENCE SURVEY A Joint Research Project Michigan State University Spring Arbor College Department of Fisheries Department of Biology and Wildlife Natural Science Building Natural Resource Building Spring Arbor, Michigan 49283 East Lansing, Michigan 48824 145 Appendix A (continued). MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DEPARTMENT OF FISHERIES AND WILDLIFE EAST L’INSING 0 MICHIGAN ' 488244222 NATURAL RESOURCES BUILDING l5") 535-4477 MICHIGAN SURVEY OF ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIOR AND ?ERSONALITY A Joint Project of Michigan State University Spring Arbor College Department of Fisheries & Wildlife and Department of Biology Natural Resources Building Natural Science Building East Lansing, MI 48824 Spring Arbor. MI 49283 Dear Participant: Would you please help in this research project? It is designed to learn more about the types of behaviors people do with regard to the environment as well as personality information to try and understand "why". The results of this study should guide educators and policy makers in future efforts to increase effective public involvement in environmental issues. Your input does matter. We appreciate the value of your time and effort. If we did not think this project to be extremely worthwhile, we would not request your valuable cooperation. While we cannot pay you for the value of your time. we have arranged to make a donation of 50¢ for each completed questionnaire. This donation will be given to your church to be used where needed most. When added to donations from the many others completing these forms. it can become a Significant amount. There are no "right" or "wrong" answers. There are only yeur answers. Your truthful replies to evegy question are what will make this project successful. Notice that there is no place for a name on the answer sheets. The name on the return mailing is designed to prevent duplicate mailings. You are promised complete confidentiality. Thank you in advance for your help on this research project. 79‘ a; 2% fltw Mr. Chris Newhouse Assistant Professor Biology Spring Arbor College Spring Arbor. MI 49283 Dr. 9. Ben Peyton Assistant Professor Fisheries and Wildlife Michigan State University MS U is an Allin-cum Action/Equal Opponmmy [summo- 146 Appendix A (continued). MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DEPARTMENT Of FISHERIES AND WILDLIFE EAST LANSING 0 MICHIGAN 0 48824-1222 NAIUIAL RESOURCES BUILDING (Sm 555-4477 MICHIGAN SURVEY OF ENVIRONMENTAL BEHAVIOR AND PERSONALITY A Joint Project - or Michigan State University Spring Arbor College Department of Fisheries & Wildlife and Department of Biology Natural Resources Building Natural Science Building East Lansing, MI 48824 Spring Arbor, MI 49283 Dear Participant: WOuld you please help in this research project? It is designed to learn more about the types of behaviors people do with regard to the environment as well as personality information to try and understand "why". The results of this study should guide educators and policy makers in future efforts to increase effective public involvement in environmental issues. Your input does matter. It is expecially important for us to gather data from people like you who are more active and informed about environmental issues. While we cannot pay you for the value of your time, we have arranged to make a donation of 50¢ for each completed questionnaire. This donation will be given to your environmental organization to be used where needed most. When added to the donations given for the many others completing this form, it can become a significant amount. There are no "right" or "wrong" answers. There are only ybur answers. Your truthful replies to evegz question are what will make this project successful. Notice that there is no place for a name on the answer sheets. The name on the return mailing is designed to prevent duplicate mailings. You are promised complete confidentiality. We appreciate the value of your time and effort. If we did not think this project to be extremely worthwhile, we would not request your valuable cooperation. Thank you in advance for your help on this research project. Sincerely, rash ahmgai Dr. R. Ben Peyton Chris Newhouse Assistant Professor Assistant Professor Fisheries and Wildlife 310109? Michigan State University :Pr§39 2:22: CEIIEESBS pring r, .WSU 4': m (II/Moe Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 147 Appendix A (continued). Directions: Please answer all questions. Do not write your name on the form. When you finish, please fold the heavy cover over the question booklet and staple or tape securely. Please return the booklet as indicated. Please start by circling the number in front of the following questions. Remember. we do not have or want your name. 1. What is your highest level of education? (circle one) 1. 2. 3. 4. S. 6. 7. some grade school some high school completed high school some college completed college some graduate work a graduate degree 2. What is your occupation? (circle the most appropriate category) 1. 2. 3. 4. S. 6. 7. clerical or sales or service manager or proprietor homemaker (if this, please also check the occupation of the family breadwinner) . official or professional farmeowner or manager laborer student 3. What was your 1983 family income? 1. 2. 3. 4. S. 4. Me 1. 2. less than $10,000 from 10.000 to 19.999 from 20,000 to 29,999 from 30.000 to 39.999 above 40.000 you a member of an environmental organization? yes no 5. Where do you live? 1. 2. 3. 6. Sex 1. 2. urban area - city suburban area - town. small community rural area - country male female 148 Appendix A (continued). PART I PREFERENCE INVENTORY Purpose of Inventory: Below you will find six sets of statements that describe various aspects of life. These factors are of differing importance to different people. The purpose of this inventory is to determine the relative importance to you of each of the six statements in each set. Although some items in the various sets may be similar they express different aspects of the life situation and are necessary to a complete description of what is important to you. This is not a test of ability or consistency in making answers. Its only purpose is to make it possible for you to rank each set of items in terms of their importance to you. DIRECTIONS: a. READ all the statements in each set. b. THINK about how important each statement is to you. c. RANK the statements in order of their importance to you. Do this by placing a "if next to the statement that is most important to you, a "2” next to the statement that is second most important to you and so on through number "6" which would be the statement of least importance to you. d RANK ALL STATEMENTS in a set even when this is difficult. EXAMPLE: The opportunity in my life to work together with other people. .I... Having sufficient authority for the role expected of me. 5 Having knowledge of others' plans that affect me and my life. 1 Credit given me by others for doing a good job. 3 The opportunity to utilize all of my abilities. Not having to worry where the next meal will come from. '0 RANK the statements in each set in order of their importance to you. 4 The sense of worth that my life gives me. Relative freedom from supervision. Knowing that my physical needs (hunger. thirst. etc.) are satisfied. Being told what I am.supposed to do and how I am to do it. The opportunity to develop my full potential. The opportunity to develop close friendships. 149 Appendix A (continued). The opportunity to give help to other people. NOT having to make decisions. Having adequate clothes to protect me from the elements. The opportunity to come up with my own solutions to problems. The opportunity for personal growth and development. The importance of a role in life. Having a source of income to provide for survival needs. The chance to achieve as much as I want in my life. Receiving adequate information about plans and policies that influence me. Freedom to express my opinion and suggestions to others. Being liked by others. The feeling that what I do is regarded as important. Freedom to use my own judgement. Getting as far ahead as my abilities will allow. Having others recognize the importance of what I do. An Opportunity to show my liking and friendship for others. A warm, dry place to live. Knowing someone who will help me out when I get into a jam. Freedom to make decisions. The Opportunity to participate in activities such as picnics. bowling leagues, etc. A routine where I always know what is expected of me. The prestige and regard I receive from others. The opportunity to advance in responsibility as far as I am able to. The fact that I have all the food I need. A sense of belonging to a group. The fact that I am bee now seriously hungry or thirsty. A definite set of rules and procedures that I can follow. NOT having my work interfered with. Credit given me for doing good work. The feeling of self-fulfillment from being able to use my own unique capabilities and realizing my potential. 1 Appendix A (continued). PART II 50 This set of questions deals with actions you might have taken at least pertially becau_se it; your concern done one of the following actions (for example, only to save money) for the environment. If you have . but only for a non-environmental reason . please respond "not at all". Please check how often you have done each of the following (with consideration of the environment being at least the past year. one important part of the decision) within 1. Picked up any type of litter - a. at home b. in your neighborhood _ not at all _ not at all __ seldom _ seldom ____occasionally occasionally ____ frequently frequently c. in public areas d. in commercial areas (parks. etc.) (parking lots, shopping centers) __ not at all _ not at all __ seldom __ seldom .___ occasionally occasionally .___ frequently .___ frequently 2. Taken the following items to a recycling center - a. paper b. glass __ not at all _ not at all __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally __ occasionally __ frequently _ frequently c. metal d. motor oil ‘ __ not at all __ not at all __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally __ occasionally __ frequently ___' frequently 151 Appendix A (continued). Signed a petition aimed at improving environmental conditions - a. among friends and b. in public places __ not at all _ not at all __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally _ occasionally __ frequently _ frequently Distributed a petition aimed at improving environmental conditions - a. among friends and b. in public places __ not at all _ not at all _ seldom __ seldom _ occasionally __ occasionally _ frequently __ frequently Written to or called elected officials to try and influence their position on an environmental issue - a. local officials and b. state officials and c. national officials __ not at all _ not at all __ not at all _ seldom __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally _ occasionally __ occasionally _ frequently __ frequently __ frequently Campaigned for somebody seeking public office at least partly because of his/her environmental stance - a. local level and b. state level and c. national level __ not at all _ not at all _ not at all __ seldom __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally __ occasionally __ occasionally __ frequently _ frequently __ frequently Voted for somebody seeking public office at least partly because of his/her environmental stance - a. local level and b. state level and c. national level _ not at all __ not at all __ not at all __ seldom __ seldom _ seldom __ occasionally __ occasionally __ occasionally __ frequently __ frequently __ frequently 152 Appendix A (continued). 8. lo. 11. Turned off lights for energy conservation - a. at home and b. at a friend or relative's home __ not at all __ not at all __ seldom __ seldom ___.occasionally occasionally .___ frequently frequently and c. in a public building(school, church, restaurant) not at all seldom occasionally frequently Purchased some particular brand or type of object at least partially for environmental reasons - a. because of its recyclable container ‘___ not at all ____ seldom ____occasionally ___'frequently to minimize harmful effects caused by similar products ____not at all seldom ‘____occasionally frequently C. b. because it uses less energy not at all seldom occasionally frequently because the company was known to be environmentally concerned not at all seldom occasionally frequently Distributed information about environmental issues - to strangers ___ not at all ____ seldom a. to friends not at all ‘___ seldom occasionally frequently b. occasionally frequently Engaged in an informal discussion to encourage someone to consider some aspect of an environmental issue - with someone having views similar to your own not at all seldom occasionally a. frequently b. with someone having views different from your own not at all __ seldom occasionally _____frequently 153 Appendix A (continued). l2. 13. 1m 15. 16. Made a public appearance or spoken "formally" about an environmental issue - a. at the local level b. at the state level c. at the national level __ not at all __ not at all _ not at all __ seldom _ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally __ occasionally __ occasionally _ frequently frequently frequently Reported to authorities an individual or organization violating an environmental law - (pollution. hunting violations) a. an individual you know well b. an individual you don‘ t know well _ not at all __ not at all __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally __ occasionally __ frequently __ frequently Filed a lawsuit against an individual or organization violating enviromental laws - a. by yourself b. as a part of a group lawsuit _ not at all __ not at all __ seldom __ seldom __ occasionally _ occasionally __ frequently __ frequently Filed an injunction to prevent a threat to the environment - a. filed by yourself b. filed as part of a group action __ not at all _ not at all _ seldom __ seldom occasionally __ occasionally frequently __ frequently Stopped buying a product due to its potentially harmful environmental effects - a. not at all seldom occasionally frequently Given donations for an environmental project - a. at the local level b. at the state level c. at the national level not at all not at all not at all seldom seldom seldom occasionally occasionally occasionally frequently frequently frequently 154 Appendix A (continued). PART III Please respond to the following items by stating how much environmental concern or environmental commitment you feel it would take to do the following actions. Assume that each is in an environmental context. Example: I, feel it would take (circle a number) of environmental commitment to (for each item) § .5? .555? :é’f $5.? 1 2 3 Sign a petition l 2 3 Distribute a petition l 2 3 write to or call an official 1 2 3 Campaign for somebody l 2 3 vote for somebody l 2 3 Pick up litter l 2 3 Use a recycling center 1 2 3 Turn off lights l 2 3 Make environmentally sound purchases 1 2 3 Distribute information l 2 3 Discuss the environment with somebody l 2 3 Make a public appearance or a formal speech 1 2 3 Report a violation of environmental law 1 2 3 File a lawsuit 1 2 3 File an injunction l 2 3 Stop buying a harmful product l 2 3 Give donations for an environmental project 155 Appendix A (continued). PART IV Inventory of Social and/or Political Attions (not including environmental. actions) this series of questions deals with action in other social or political realms. Social or political ac- hon may be concerned with poverty 3 rights. peace. education. or discrimination. there are. of course. many other possible topics. Remember - this set excludes environmental actions. Regardless of which issues or topics may be of concern to you. please answer ' «following questions about how you have attempted to deal with the issue. For each action. report how often you have taken this type of action during the past year. Please respond to every item. :3 the past year. how many times. if any. have you. ...... ......because of a social or political issue? {Please circle the appropriate letter) .3' . f sides v :9“ n.1Vg S’s’a‘v- N s 3 P l. written to or called an elected official 2 m 0 m ?' participated in a peaceful demonstration (march. :al.y. picket) .n order .ohring the issue to the public's attention 8 S o F 3. stopped buying from or using the services of that company .4 u- L) ~u ‘- . signed a petition N S 3 F 5. distributed a petition V S o F 6. written a "letter to the editor" of a newspaper or magazine v 3 3 P 7 called a radio or rv station to share your views N s o F 3. joined an organization which was organized to deal with social or political issues N S o F 9. donated money to an organization or individual in order to help in the resolution of your particular issue v 3 0 P -0. voted for an indi'-dual at least partially v s o E ll. campaigned for an indiViduel at least partially N s D F l2. reported to the authorities cases where an individual or organization has been breaking the law 3 s o 5 l3. filed or actively and directly supported a lawsuit N s o P l4. run for or served in public office at least partially N s o P ls. been a volunteer in a charity campaign. such as making holiday food boxes or working for a Red cross drive N S o P 16. gone to someone’s home or had someone into your home primarily to do something to improve that person's well- being (not as a social or business Visit) N S o F 17. participated in some type of educational program at least partly for the purpose of learning more N s o F 18. attempted to educate somebody N s o P l9. attended a meeting at least partly for the purpose of finding out how to be more effective in your efforts to solve a social or political problem 156 Appendix A (continued). PART V INSTRUCTIONS On the next page are is values listed in alphabetical order. Your task is to arrange them in order of their importance to YOU, as guiding principles in YOUR life. Each value is printed on a abel which can be easily peeled off and pasted in the boxes on the left-hand side of the page. tudy the list carefully and pick out the one value which is the most important for you. Peel it off and paste it in Box 1 on the left Then pick out the value which is second most important tfor you. it off and paste it in Box 2. Then do the values. Peel same for each of the renaining The value which is least important goes in Box 18. Work slowly and think carefully. If you to change your answers. The labels peel off place to place. The end result should truly change your mind. feel free easily and can be moved from show how you really feel. 157 Appendix A (continued). 15; Iéé f I7! I8i A COMFORTABLE LIFE (o procporous (Ito) AN EXCITING LIFE (o stimulating. octIvo Ilfo) A SENSE OF ACCOMPLISHMBNT (lostIng contribution) A WORLD AT PEACE (ftoo of war and conflIct) A WORLD OF EIAUTY (boouty ot noturo ond tho arts) EQUALITY (brothorhood. oquol opportunity for all) FAMILY SECURITY (toklng coro oi lovod onoo) FREEOM (Indopondonco. froo choico) HEALTH (physical and nlontoI roll-Hug) INN. HARMONY (froodom from Innor contIIct) MATUII LOVE (usual and spiritual IntImocy) NATIONAL SECURITY (protocflon from ottock) PLEASURE (on onloyoblo. IoIouron llto) SALVATION (sovod. otornol lilo) SELF-RESPECT (colt-octootn) SOCIAL RECOGNITION (mpoct. odmirotlon) TRUE FRIENDSHIP (cIooo comoonIonoIIIp) WISDOM (o moturo ondoutondlng ot Ilto) WHEN YOU HAVE FINISHED. GO TO THE NEXT PAGE. 158 Appendix A (continued). Thank you very much for your participation in this study. Your responses will help our research effort which we expect will benefit all the citizens of Michigan. Please be sure to flip the heavy sheet over to serve as a cover, to staple or tape the booklet, then to return the booklet. 159 Appendix A (continued). mmmmv H2 .nons< mafinom omoHHoo Moons msasmm monon Mo nowmomoum p.mm¢ omdoaaoz mango .hs new? 525.: .392 Siam omoon contra. macaw 160 Appendix B. Credibility Letters. STATE OF MICHIGAN 92‘" . Q 133’; EEZE' autumn. nesounccs CMON ‘ "3” rsgntasnmrécenson JAMES J. BLANCHARD. Governor a , A $525; 33;" DEPARTMENT OF NATURAL RESOURCES 3 STEWART MYERS STEVENS T. MASON BUILDING =AYMONO poupone BOX 30028 «mm H NHII'ELEY LANSING. Ml 48909 RONALD O. SKOOG. Oioctor ANlZ 1”” Mr. Chris Newhouse Assistant Professor of Biology Spring Arbor College Spring Arbor, Michigan 49283 Dear Professor Newhouse: The Department of Natural Resources is most interested in reviewing the results from your research project for the purpose of improving communica- tions with Michigan citizens. The Department is undertaking a number of management initiatives in public affairs and social resource management which could be enhanced by obtaining the results from your survey. Dr. Ben Peyton is aware of these efforts and can be of considerable assistance to us as he oversees your efforts in completing your doctoral dissertation on these matters. I certainly encourage the cooperation of members from the Michigan Sierra Club and the Audubon Society Chapters in responding to your research endeavor. Sinc rely, W .. . Whiteley. Ch irman Natur 1 Resources Camission cc: Natural Resources Commission Dr. Skoog ~ Dr. Peyton 91025 £53.» ‘ ’3‘ Ian- D 161 Appendix B (continued). Michigan Audubon dociety 7000. North Westnodge, Kalamazoo, Michigan 49007 616 344-8648 May 10, 1984 Mr. Chris Rewhouse . Assistant Professor of Biology Spring Arbor College Spring Arbor, Michigan 49283 Dear Professor Nevhouse: The Michigan Audubon Society is extremely interested in research that evaluates motivational and personality correlates of environmental action. The state of Michigan and the Michigan Audubon Society has been fortunate since it's founding in 1904 in having many individuals motivated to advancing the cause of environmental action. I strongly encourage Michigan Audubon Society menbers to cooperate in this research project. Sincerely, ' . , _ fl '. V I V icfliL/r%dl‘ ‘““’ Roger Sutherland president "This Is Roovcioa poser. use It ooein." 162 Appendix B (continued). I ...—_— lull")!!! Trumlulmg concur: IlllU “(Moll West Michigan Environmental Action Council 152-l LIkL' l)r1vc,S E. - Grand Rapids. Michigan 49‘00 - olb-wildOSl October 12, 1984 Dear WMEAC Member You might be asking yourself why a WMEAC cover letter is in an envelope with a Spring Arbor College return address. The answer is simple: the WMEAC Board of Directors is cooperating with a Michigan State University Ph. D. Candidate. Assistant Professor Chris Newhouse, in his research on environmental activists. As is more fully explained in the first page of the enclosed survey form, the information gained in this project will help increase environmental activism. That is why the Board of Directors has decided to support this project and why the Board encourages you to complete and return the survey. Pro- fessor Newhouse has proVided explicit instructions on how this can be done. Please be assured that the Board of Directors respects your privacy. We HAVE NOT released our mailing list. or your name, in conjunction with this research. Rather, as is Board policy, Professor Newhouse has provided all materials (surveys and envelopes) to us and WE HAVE ADDRESSED THE ENVEL- OPES. In effect, we have acted only as the mailing house. In return for this service by WMEAC, in addition to the knowledge gained through the research, WMEAC will receive a donation for each survey re- turned to Professor Newhouse. For these reasons we encourage your support of this worthwhile project. I would be pleased to answer any questions you might have regarding Board policy on mailings to our members. Sincerely yours. %\ T‘K/L Frank Ruswick, Jr. Executive Director 163 Appendix B (continued). / “.— Lfi'h ‘- a 0' '5 x‘ '- 1 g n} p , North Michigan Conference OF THE FREE METHODIST CHURCH SUPERINTENDENT: TED JOHNSONJSIOS SHAMROCK BLVD.. BIG RAPIDS. MI ‘9307 (618) 796-3147 iuwmutrwucmwnm: I hereby urge your participation in Professor Houhouse's Fh.D. project by filling in the enclosed questionnaire to the best or your abilities. This project can no doubt be of a great service to us all as it seeks for practical applications of Qua-infinity to every day lining. Sincerely, l‘\ ' . ’ I .. "."_-_Jc;,l\_', toa's. Johnson, Superintenth ‘ North Michigan Conference of the Free Methodist cdun-ch I 164 Appendix B (continued). "For Zion 's sake I wdl not keep silent, for Jerusalem 's sake I will not lemam ouzer. all her ngnreousness runes Out like the dawn, her salvation like .3 During torch. " //—\ lsarah 62: ,/ .f . “g,,—c\/ ‘/,”r’;;;;;;;ET;RYDERMAN:;;;;‘\\\ . ’ ‘ . , __ " 3724 Horse Shoe Drive . Trov, Michigan 48384 .I‘K‘t‘ -.\ It‘ll l( x 1151 ( .1 ll Il‘( 'h g ' eafTih.mTTT.----m -.mi. ‘\c “-——- (fifiafirééd-l707 Dear Friend, I was recently approached by Chris Newhouse, a personal friend and a professor at Spring Arbor College, with the request to involve the members and friends of our church fellowship in a research project he is currently pursuing in his graduate studies. Knowing him.and his commitment to higher Christian education and knowing you and your interest in and support of Spring Arbor College, I was sure that you would want to assist him. He is especially interested in polling the Christian community as to their response to several critical issues. Please take time to add your responses to his research. Again, we both appreciate your support and interest. This is one way we can feel a part of the growth and adventure which goes on and springs from our campus at Spring Arbor. In His service, with joy and enthusiasm, Rik Cryderman, Pastor and friend JC/rc Appendix C. 165 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each environmental action when comparing mean environmental action frequencies among Maslow levels. Action 1.a (pick up litter - home) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow BASED ON POOLED STDEV P- lqvgl N MEAN STDEV ¢ a t t 0.49 1 77 2.805 0.514 (---*---) 11 23 2.696 0.470 ( ' ) df- III ll 2.727 0.905 ( ‘ l 4, IV 16 2.075 0.342 ( * ) 150 VI 36 2.694 0.668 ( ' ) ¢ t f # N.S. POOLED STDEV - 0.565 2.40 2.64 2.88 3 12 Action 1.b (pick up litter - neighborhood) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN a..1°w BASED ON 900050 srnzv p. 1 1 N urns STDEV 5 ¢ 9 t 1.25 I'V' 77 2.065 0.079 (.....-..) III 11 2.364 0.809 ( * ) 4, Iv 16 1.938 0.854 ( * ) 153 v: 36 1.770 0.929 ( r ) Poorrn srosv - 0.054 1.60 2.00 2.20 2.00 "'5‘ Action 1. ' k l'tter - arks) c (Pic up 3 p INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR'MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV F. ?:::2' N MEAN STDEV ¢ t i 4e 1.02 I 77 1.909 0.729 (~---‘--) II 23 1.913 0.900 ( ‘ ) df- III 11 2.102 0.603 < 0 ) 4, Iv 16 1.607 0.073 ( 0 ) 153 VI 36 1.722 0.849 (-- ‘----) POOLED sroev - 0.790 1.50 2.00 2.10 ”'3' Action Maslow level I II III Iv vI POOLED Action flaslow level I II III IV VI POOLED STDEV - 1.297 l.d (pick up litter - commercial) N 77 23 ll 16 36 STDEV - 0.8014 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S EOR MEAN 2.a (recycle - paper) BASED ON POOLED STDEV 5‘ MEAN STDEV t t t 2.90 1.3377 0.7365 (---.----) 0.0696 0.8689 ( . ) a:- 1.4545 0.6076 ( . — ) 4, 1.4375 0.9639 ( . ) 153 ..... * ------ 0.9722 0.0447 < i ¢) ¢ P<‘os 0.00 1.20 1.60 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN aaszn 0N poorao srnzv p- aras srnsv t s e e 0.13 1.286 1.202 ( ----- . ----- ) 1.304 1.329 ( ' J df- 1.545 1.500 ( ~ ) 4, 1.375 1.455 ( ‘ ) . 153 1.250 1.339 ( * - ) - + 1.00 1.50 2.00 2.50 “'5' 1(56 Appendix C (continued). Action 2.b (recycle - glass) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow BASED ON POOLED STDEV e- 190,; N MEAN STDEV e 5 i 0.54 I 77 0.0961 1.1905 ( ----- ....--) II 23 1.1304 1.3917 ( 8 ) (If. 1:: 11 0.0102 1.2505 ( . r 4 Iv 16 0.6075 1.2500 ( . ) lga VI 36 1.1309 1.3970 ( * POOLED STDEV - 1.2007 0.50 1 00 1 50 “'5' Action 2.c (recycle - metal) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow BASED ON POOLED STDEV :- level N MEAN STDEV e I 1 p 0.50 I 77 0.6003 1.0792 ( ----- 9----) II 23 0.0696 1.3247 ( df- III 11 1.1010 1.0707 ( ~ ) 4 IV 16 1.0000 1.3156 ( * ) 153 VI 36 0.0611 1.3126 ( ) eooLED STDEV - 1.1935 0.50 1 00 1 50 2.00 ”'5' Action 2.d (recycle - motor oil) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S r Maslow BASED ON POOLED STDEV OR MEAN 5- 19991 N MEAN STOW i *r + 0 e 97 I 77 0.3377 0.7002 (----t----—) . I; 23 0.7391 1.2511 ( . , d£_ III 11. 0.2727 0.5467 ( t ) 4 Iv 16 0.3125 0.7932 ( . ) 150 VI 36 0.4444 1.0541 ( . ) POOLED SIDEV - 0.9215 0.00 0 40 0.00 Action 3.a (sign petition - friends) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow BASED ON POOLED SIDEV P- 1°V°1 N NEAN SIDEV : 9 I s 1.70 77 1.1039 1.0506 (---'—--) I 23 1.3913 1.0331 < . II 11 1.2727 0.9045 ( * ) df- III 16 1.0625 1.1015 . ) 4' IV 36 0.7222 0.9445 (-----‘------l 158 VI ' 5 3 ¢ 3 POOLED STDEV - 1.0342 0.50 1.00 1.50 2.00 N's. Action 3.b (sign petition - public) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN n..1ow BASED ON POOLED STDEV :- level N MEAN STDEV I i f f 1.64 I 77 1.0779 1.0100 (----*---) II 123 1.4348 1.0369 ( * ) dg. VI 36 0.7500 0.9373 ( t ) poonzn srozv . 1.0330 0.50 1.00 1.50 2.00 “'3' Action 4.a (distribute petition - friends) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S son MEAN Maslow BASED ON POOLED STDEV P- level N MEAN SIDEV ¢ ¢ 3 2.11 I 77 0.4005 0.0360 (--*--) II 23 0.8696 1.0137 (---*------> d1- :1; 11 0.1010 0.6030 ( * .. ) 4, IV 16 0.3750 0.8851 ( ‘ ) 158 v1 36 0.3056 0.6604 (----*-----) N s POOLED STDEV - 0.0224 0.00 0.50 1-00 Action S.a Maslow level I II III IV VI Appendix C (continued). ction 4.b (distribute petit Maslow level N MEAN STDEV I 77 0.3506 0.7027 II 23 0.5217 0.8458 III 11 0 0 IV 16 0.4375 0.9639 VI 36 0.2222 0.4847 POOLED STDEV - 0.7139 (write/call official - N MEAN 0.7532 1.0000 0.3636 0.4375 0.8611 POOLED STDEV - 0.9247 Action 5.b (write/call official - state) Maslow level I II III IV VI MEAN 0.8052 1.2174 0.4545 0.6250 1.0278 eooLED srnrv - 1.0294 STDBV 0.9618 0.9535 0.5045 0.7274 0.9900 STDEV 0.9873 1.1661 0.6876 1.0247 1.1081 1657 on - public) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN A. T local) T k ( BASED ON POOLED STDEV r (-—-~---) ‘( ) t ( ----~--- 1 0.00 0.40 k L 1‘ ) ) 0.00 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S POR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV Y (---~--> t ( ) L r t ( ---------- ) A r 0.50 1. 00 1.50 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV 4L ( r I' (...... ( t A. T ) ) Action 5.c (write/call official - national) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLBD STDBV Haelow level I II III IV VI MEAN 0.8442 1.3913 0.3636 0.6250 0.9444 POOLED SIDEV - 1.0727 STDEV 1.0395 1.2336 0.6742 1.0247 1.1450 Action 6.a (campaign - local) Maslow level I II III IV VI -N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN STDEV 0.5584 0.6087 0.4545 0.5000 0.5278 POOLED STDEV - 0.9215 Action 6.b (campaign - mallow level I II III IV V? MEAN 0.5195 0.7391 0.1818 0.6250 0.5833 POOLED STDEV I 0.9271 0.9527 1.0762 0.6876 0.7303 0.8779 state) STDEV 0.8975 1.0962 0.4045 1.0247 0.9373 ( v— .00 ) (.......-...., 0.50 0. f 1.00 A. r <---9--) ) ) (-.....--..-)~ 60 1.20 1.50 1.80 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S POR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV A T INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV 0.00 ‘7 (--t----> ( fl ) J. r <---——*---—-) 0.50 1.00 0 L T E'- 1.40 df- 150 N.S. F- 1.49 df- 158 N.S. E'- 1.52 Hf- 158 N.S. F. 2.22 df- 158 N.S. P- 0.07 df- 158 N.S. ’- 0.73 d!- 158 N.S. Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Meelow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Meslow level I II III IV VI Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action haslow level I II III IV VI Appendix C (continued). 168 6.c (campaign - national) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S 500 MEAN BASED ON POOLBD STDEV F- N MEAN STDEV + 1 1 1.62 77 0.5195 0.9121 (---.-_-_) 23 1.0000 1.3143 ( . )df- 11 0.1010 0.4045 ( 3 ) 4, 16 0.6250 0.0051 ( 3 ) 150 36 0.6667 1.0420 ( ..... ...--._; STDEV - 0.9020 0.00 0.50 1.00 “'3 7.a (vote - local) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S ron MEAN r BASED ON POOLED SIDEV 1'32 N MEAN STDBV ¢ t t 5 ° 77 1.550 1.200 (--3---) 23 2.007 1.041 ( 3 df‘ 11 1.273 1.009 ( 3 ) 4. 16 1.500 1.414 ( . ) 158 36 1.472 1.103 ( ----- 3---) 1 1 1 1 N.S. STDEV - 1.191 0.60 1.20 1.00 2.40 7.b (vote - state) . INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV F' N MEAN STDBV 3 5 e 0.73 77 1.675 1.106 (-—-3--) 23 2.000 1.120 ( . ) di- 11 1.364 1.027 ( 3 ) 4. 16 1.500 1.414 (------3-------) 150 36 1.503 1.273 (----3-—--) t t t N.S. PDDLED STDZV - 1.212 1.20 1.00 2.40 7.c (vote - national) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S son MEAN BASED ON BUDLED STDEV ?' N MEAN SIDEV : : : 0-93 77 1.675 1.240 (----3----) 23 2.174 1.072 ( 3 1 df' 11 1.545 1.214 ( 3 ) 4. 16 1.500 1.414 ( 3 ) 150 36 1.667 1.207 ( 3 ) 5 t t N.S. SIDEV - 1.245 1.20 1.00 2.40 8.a (lights off - home) INDIVIDUAL 95 9cm CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLBD SIDEV {-86 N MEAN SIDEV e 6 s 5 - 77 2.779 0.553 (---3--) df_ 23 2.913 0.200 3 ) ‘ 11 2.909 0.302 ( 3 I50 16 2.625 0.005 ( 3 ( ) * ) 36 2.972 0.167 ; i ; ¢ ".5. srnev - 0.495 2.40 2.64 2.00 3.12 8.b (lights off - friend/relative) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Boa MEAN r- BASED ON POOLED STDEV 0 46 N MEAN STDEV . : 3 3 ° 77 1.532 1.003 (----3----) d:_ 23 1.565 1.121 ( 3 ) 4 11 1.102 1.079 ( ) 150 16 1.375 1.147 ( ( .7 ) ) 36 1.333 1.069 N.S. 1.00 1.50 2.00 POOLED STDEV - 1.091 Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Haelow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Maslow level I II III IV VI 0 .9: ((19333; 295119 (11311511 c) MEAN 1.922 1.870 2.273 2.062 1.944 STDEV - 1.088 9.a (purchase - recyclable) MEAN 1.779 2.087 1.273 1.500 1.972 STDEV - 1.044 STDEV 1.073 1.100 1.009 1.181 1.094 STDEV 0.995 1.041 1.104 1.317 1.000 169 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV ( 3 ) 9.b (purchase - energy efficient) MEAN 2.169 2.348 1.545 2.312 2.167 STDEV - 0.854 STDEV 0.818 0.647 1.293 1.078 0.775 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED 0N POOLED STDEV ' (--3--) ( ——3 ) ( ‘ ) ( ----..“ .-.-.m- ) ( ----fl.“ ) 1.50 2.00 2.50 9.c (purchase - minimize harm) N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN 1.974 2.261 1.545 1.938 1.944 POOLED STDEV I 1.025 POOLED Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED STDEV 0.986 0.964 1.128 1.124 1.068 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT 013s roe MEAN BASED ON BDDLED STDEV ..l_ L A r v— V' r (--3--) ( ‘ ) 9.d (purchase - concerned company) N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN 1.390 1.783 1.182 1.125 1.250 STDEV - 1.120 STDEV 1.066 1.126 1.079 1.258 1.180 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV T T T (-~--3----) < ------- --3-------) 10.a (distribute info - friends) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV N MEAN STDEV 3 : t 3 77 1.351 1.144 (----'---l 23 1.609 1.196 ( 11 1.000 1.095 ( * ) 16 1.000 1.155 ( * ) 36 1.056 1.170 1* )1 1 STDEV - 1.155 0.50 1.00 1.50 2.00 E'- 0.33 df- 158 N.S. E'- 1.72 df- 158 N.S. E'- 1.83 df- 158 N.S. P- 0.94 df- 158 N.S. P- 1.15 df- 150 N.S. P- 1.27 df- 158 N.S. Appendix C (continued). Action 10.b (distribute info - strangers) Maslow level N I 77 II 23 III 11 IV 16 VI 36 MEAN 0.7792 1.1304 0.6364 0.6875 0.7222 POOLED STDEV I 1.0096 Action 11.a (discussion 1370 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Maslow levcl N MEAN I 77 1.571 II 23 2.304 III 11 1.727 IV 16 1.687 VI 36 1.694 POOLED STDEV I 1.034 Action 11.b (discussion Maslow llVOl N MEAN I 77 1.351 11 23 1.739 III 11 1.636 IV 16 1.500 VI 36 1.389 POOLED STDIV I 1.065 Action 12.a (public appearance - local) naslov level N MEAN I 77 0.5325 II 23 0.6957 III 11 0.3636 IV 16 0.3125 VI 36 0.3889 POOLED STDEV I 0.8736 Action 12.b (public appearance - state) Maslow level N MEAN I 77 0.18182 II 23 0.21739 III 11 0.09091 IV 16 0.18750 v: 36 0.13889 POOLED STDEV I 0.61118 Action 12.c (public appearance - national) Maslow level N MEAN :I 77 0.12987 I 23 0.13043 11 11 0 ;V 16 0.12500 1 36 0.02778 POOLED STDEV I 0.49342 BASED ON POOLED STDEV 84 STDEV I I I 0.80 0.9407 (-..-e---) 100576 ( t ) df- 1.0269 ( * ) 4. 1.1383 ( t ) 153 1.0586 ( ..... e ..... ) t I ¢ N.S. 0.50 1 00 1.50 - similar views) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDBV F- STDEV i I I 2.24 1.031 (---e..--) 0.926 ( e ) df- l.104 ( ‘ ) 4. 1.078 ( . ) 158 1.064 ( * ) f t : _.N.S. 1.50 2.00 2.50 — different views) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S ron MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDBV ?' STOW t i ‘f i °e71 1.036 ( ..... . ..... ) 1.137 ( - ; dr- l.027 ( t ) 4, 1.155 ( ' ) 158 1.050 ( ' ) :— ¢ 4' 4: Nos. 1.20 1.60 2.00 2.40 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S ron MEAN BASED ON POOLED sroev P- 0.8673 (.....----) 1.0632 ( ... ) - 0.6742 ( t ) if 0.7932 ( - ) 0.8376 ( - ) 15° 0.00 0.30 0.80 1.20 "'3' INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Eon MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV P- STDEV ¢ ¢ f 5 0.11 0.62254 (---*----) 0.73587 ( ‘ ) d£- 0.30151 ( * ) 4. 0.75000 ( * ) 158 0.48714 ( * —) i # 4“ i N.S. -0024 0.00 0024 0e48 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN F. BASED ON POOLED STDBV 0.44 STDEV ¢ i i —# 0.54654 (“""“") df- 0.62554 ( ( ~ ) ) 4' 0 . * . 0.50000 . ( * ) 15° 0.16667 ( * ) N‘s. -o.20 0.00 0.20 0.10 Appendix Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Meslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Maslow level I II III Iv VI POOLED Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Meslow level I II III IV VI POOLED Action Maslow level I II III IV VI POOLED 1371 C (continued). 13.a (report violation - acquaintance) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN F- BASED ON POOLED STDEV 0 17 N MEAN STDEV 5 5 5 . 77 0.2468 0.5884 (----*-----) 23 0.3043 0.6350 ( . ) df- 11 0.1818 0.6030 < . ) 4. 16 0.1875 0.7500 ( . ) 153 36 0.3056 0.7491 ( * ) 5 5 5 N.S. SIDEV - 0.6507 0.00 0.24 0 48 13.b (report violation - stranger) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV F- N MEAN STDEV 5 5 t 5 1-39 77 0.4935 0.7543 (---~-----) 23 0.4783 0.7305 ( . ) dr- 11 0.3636 0.6742 ( a ) 4. 16 0.2500 0.7746 ( * ) 158 36 0.7500 0.9063 ( 5 ) 5 i 5 5 N.S. SIDZV . 0.7847 0.00 - 0.32 0.64 0.96 14.a (lawsuit - yourself) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON BooLED STDEV P- N MEAN SIDEV 5 5 5 5 1.05 77 0.01299 0.11396 (-——---.--) 23 0 0 ( w ) di- 11 0 0 ( . ) 4, 16 0.06250 0.25000 ( * )158 36 .> 0 0 ( * ) ¢ t 5 5 N.S. SIDEV - 0.14543 -0.050 0.000 0.050 0.100 14.b (lawsuit - group) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV ?' N MEAN STDEV 5 5 5 5 0-43 77 0.1948 0.5629 (----*---—-) 23 0.2609 0.6192 ( * ) 4" 11 0 0 ( 5 ) 4! 16 0.2500 0.7746 ( . >158 36 0.2222 0.5909 ( * t t 1*— :— N.S. SIDEV - 0.6026 -0.24 0 00 0.24 0.48 15.a (injunction - yourself) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S roa MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV 7' N MEAN STDEV 5 5 5 5 0-44 77 0.06494 0.29637 (---*----) 23 O O ( t d:- 11 0 0 ( a ) 4. 16 0.06250 0.25000 ( . ) 158 36 0.05556 0.23231 ( . ) ¢ ¢ i 2‘: N.Se STDEV - 0.27461 -0.10 0.00 0.10 0.20 lS.b (injunction - group) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED 0N 900LED STDEV F' N MEAN STDBV 5 5 5 5 1.59 77 0.2857 0.7229 (....---) 23 0.5652 0.8435 . ) df- 11 o o ( e ) 4, 16 0.3125 0.7932 ( - ) 158 36 0.1944 0.5248 ( ----- * ----- ) 5 5 5 5 N.S. srpgv . 0.7092 40 0.00 0.40 0 80 1372 Appendix C (continued). Action 16 (stop buying - harmful) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN ““1°' BASED ON POOLED STDEV F“ l‘V'l N MEAN SIDEV 5 5 5 5 1.50 I 77 1.883 1.000 (----*---) II 23 2.217 0.795 5 )dt- III 11 1.364 0.924 ( r ) 4, IV 16 1.875 0.957 < t ) 158 VI 36 1.861 0.990 (----*----) 5 5 5 5 N.S. POOLED SIDBV - 0.963 _ 1.00 .’ 1.50 2.00 2.50 Action l7.a (donate 5 - local) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN ”A'1°' BASED ON POOLED SIDBV P- 1°V‘1 N MEAN STDEV 5 5 5 1.67 I 77 1.143 1.097 (----~---) 11 23 1.826 1.072 ( ~ )6:- III 11 1.182 1.079 ( r 4, IV 16 1.187 1.223 ( a ) 158 VI 36 1.306 1.215 ( t ) 5 5 5 N.S. POOLED srngv . 1.132 1.00 1.50 2.00 Action 17.b (donate 5 - state) INDIVIDUAL 95 per CI'S FOR MEAN "861°" BASED 0N BoOLED STDEV F- lavol N MEAN SIDEV 5 5 5 1 1.98 I 77 1.0519 1.0374 (----*---) II 23 1.6522 1.1912 ( 8 )d1- III 11 0.9091 1.0445 ( 4 ) 4, IV 16 0.8125 1.1673 ( - ) 158 VI 36 0.9722 1.1335 ( . ) t 5 5 5 N.S. pooLED srnsv - 1.0947. 0.50 1.00 1.50 2 00 Action 17.c (donate 5 — national) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Eon MEAN 1:311” BASED ON POOLED SIDEV 5'82 I N MEAN STDEV 5 5 5 ' 77 1.1039 1.1190 (---~---) d: 1‘ 23 1.7391 1.2869 5 ) - III 11 0.9091 1.0445 ( . ) 4. IV 16 0.7500 1.1255 ( . ) 158 VI 36 0.8056 1.1419 (-----*---) 1 5 5 P<.05 pOOLED STDZV - 1.1450 0.60 1.20 1.80 1373 ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each social/political action when comparing mean social/political action frequencies among Maslow levels. Appendix D. Action 1 (write or call an official) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow BASED ON 900LED SIDEV F- level N MEAN STDEV 5 5 5 5 0.61 z 77 0.8961 0.9260 (-....-....) I: 23 1.0870 0.9493 ( 3 ) d:- III 11 0.8182 1.1677 ( 9 ) 4, Iv 16 0.6250 0.8062 ( ‘ ) 153 v: 36 0.8611 0.8993 ( . ) I I I 5 N. . BODLED STDEV - 0.9301 0.40 0.80 1.20 1.60 S Ac“°“ 2 (d‘“°“‘t“‘i°“’ INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S roe MEAN na‘law BASED ON POOLED SIDEV !_ ‘ N MEAN SIDEV 5 5 5 I‘V‘l 77 0.2468 0.5656 (--------) 1'79 I 23 0.4783 0.6653 < = ) ‘1 11 0.2727 0.4671 ( *~ 5) df- II‘ 16 0.4375 0.6292 ( . ) 4. 5g 36 0.1389 0.3507 ( 5* ——) 158 poongp srpzv . 0.5424 0.00 0.24 0.48 0.72 N.S. Action 3 (sign petition) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow BASED ON POOLED STDEV :- level N MEAN STDEV —5 5 5 5 1.27 I 77 1.2338 0.8870 (----.----, II 23 1.0870 1.0835 ( * ) 6:- III 11 0.9091 0.8312 ( —* ) 4, IV 16 1.1875 0.7500 ( . __, 158 VI 36 0.8611 0.7983 ( *- ) POOLED STDEV - 0.8833 0.40 0.80 1 20 1.60 “'5' Action 4 (sign petition) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S 80B MEAN Maslow BASED ON POOLED SIDEV ' P- level N MEAN STDEV 5 5 5 0.93 I 77 1.325 0.865 (-----*---—-) 11 23 1.217 0.850 ( 5 ) d2- III 11 1.364 0.924 ( 5 e__)‘ Iv 16 1.312 0.946 ( . ) 158 VI 36 1.000 0.793 ( -~ -) ' * * N.S. POOLED STDEV I 0.860 Action Maslow level I II III IV VI 'BOOLED Action Maslow level I II III IV VI BooLED Action Maslow level I II III IV VI Appendix D (continued). 5 (distribute petition) N 77 23 ll 16 36 MEAN 0.4286 0.4783 0.4545 0.3750 0.2222 STDEV I 0.6623 6 (letter to editor) N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN 0.4805 0.6087 0.4545 0.4375 0.3611 STDEV I 0.7781 7 (call radio or TV station) N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN 0.27273 0.39130 0.18182 0.37500 0.05556 POOLED STDEV I 0.60602 OOOOO STDEV 0.7332 0.6653 0.8202 0.6191 0.4216 STDEV 0.7714 0.9409 0.6876 0.7274 0.7232 STDEV .66166 .78272 .40452 .71880 .23231 Action 8 (join organization) Maslow level I II III IV VI MEAN 0.8701 1.3478 1.0000 0.6875 0.6389 POOLED STDEV I 1.0052 Action 9 (donate S) Maslow level I II III IV VI N 77 23 11 16 36 MEAN 1.299 1.652 1.273 1.187 1.028 POOLED STDEV I 1.040 STDEV 1.0046 1.2652 1.0954 0.7932 0.8669 STDEV 1.052 1'74 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED 0N 900LED STDEV .F. 5 ( . 3 ) 0.77 ( ( a. ) df- ( ' ) 158 ( t ) 0.24 0.48 0.72 "‘5‘ INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Eon MEAN’ BASED 0N POOLED SIDEv :- 5 5 5 5 0.37 < a-----) ( ‘ ) df- ( 5 ) 4, ‘( y ’ ) ) 158 .00 0.30 0.60 0.90 "'5' INDIVIDUAL 95 It? CI'S Eon MEAN BASED 0N EoDLED SIDEV :- ; i i 1 e 47 ( t d:- ( ‘ ) 4, ( ) 7 )158 ( 2 5 I I N.S. 0.00 0.24 0.48 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Eon MEAN BASED ON POOLED STDEV " ¢ ¢ 5'1: l e 95 (—--5-—-—) ( * > 4' ( . ) 158 0.50 1 00 1.50 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Eon MEAN :- BASED oN POOLED‘ SIDEV 1 . 30 ‘ * -.L-.---.. ( ( ) ‘.__ _) d:- _a 1 4' (I * ‘ _) 158 ( ..- ...... ...-T---) 2 ¢-— N e S e _ 0.80 1.20 1.60 ' 2.00 1775 Appendix D (continued). Action 10 (vote) Maslow level N MEAN I 77 1.662 II 23 1.652 IV 16 1.750 V! 36 1.694 POOLED STDEV I 0.992 Action 11 (campaign) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow level N MEAN I 77 0.7273 II 23 0.4783 III 11 0.7273 IV 16 0.6250 VI 36 0.6667 POOLED SIDEV I 0.9310 Action 12 (lawbreaker reported) naslow N MEAN §‘V'1 77 0.3506 II 23 0.3043 III 11 0.3636 Iv 16 0.1875 VI 36 0.4444 POOLED STDEV I 0.6477 Action 13 (lawsuit) Maslow level N MEAN I 77 0.20779 II 23 0.08696 :11 11 0.09091 IV 16 0.31250 VI 36 0.05566 POOLED STDEV I 0.50777 Action 14 (run/serve in Maslow level N MEAN I 77 0.2208 11 23 0.2609 111 11 0.6364 IV 15 0.1675 VI 35 0.1111 POOLED STDEV I 0.6474 BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV 8- STDEV i i I - 0.04 0.954 (--..-e--_-) r 1.191 ( I - 1.027 ( . ) ) if 0.856 ( . 0.980 ( . , ’ 153 I I I t N.S. 1.20 1.60 2.00 2.40 INDIVIDUAL 95 BCI CI'S FOR MEAN BASED 0N BooLED SIDEV E- SIDEV i ; . - 0.34 0.9547 (.-—-~---) 0.8458 (-----~----—-) 0:- o.904s ( . ) 4, 0.9574 (---—---*------) 158 0.9258 ( . ) t ; : _NOSO 0.40 0.80 1.20 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV E- SIDEV i i i I 0.47 0e6838 (..---.-.--) 0.4705 ( . ) 0.5045 ( t ) df- 0.S439 ( a ) 4, 0.7346 ( a ) 155 0.00 0.24 0.48 0.72 N.S. INDIVIDUAL 95 per CI'S FOR NEAN BASED 0N PooLED SIDEV 8- STDEV — : ¢ : — 1.08 0.63531 (----*-—-) 0.28810 ( --------- . ------- ) df- o.30151 (— ‘-'—— ) 4. 0.60208 (— —* ) 158 0.23231 ( -~ -, _ : I I "N.S. 0.00 0.24 0.48 public office) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN ?_ BASED ON POOLED SIDEV L *_ 1.42 SIDEV -— r . I - 0.6412 (--- ;‘-1____) d:_ 0.6887 ('7““‘ ‘ __________ . ........... )4, 1.0269 . ( _______ ) 153 0.5439 (----- ----- ) . 0.5225 (“:’-*'.‘---. r +— —— 0.00 0.32 0.64 0.96 Appendix D (continued). 1376 Action 15 (volunteer in charity campaign) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Maslow BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F- level N MEAN SIDEV I I I 0 55 I 77 1.1039 1.0206 (---....._-, ° II 23 1.0000 0.9535 ( . ) dz. III 11 1.0000 1.1832 ( * 4 Iv 16 1.2500 1.0000 ( * 158 v1 36 0.8611 0.9607 ( . ) POOLED STDEV - 1.0078 0.40 0.80 1.20 1.60 "'5' Action 16 (serve in bone) INDIVIDUAL 95 per CI'S FOR MEAN T“1§" BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F' °V° N MEAN SIDEV I I I I 0°79 i1 77 1.338 1.034 (-------) III 23 .1.130 1.058 ( * ) Iv 11 1.273 1.009 ( ~ ) df' VI 16 1.062 0.854 ( . ) 4. 36 1.028 0.810 ( ‘ ) 153 pooLED sIDEV - 0.974 0.50 1.20 1.60 2.00N-8. Aeti°n 17 (°d“°‘ti°n‘l p‘Ogtam) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S r05 MEAN E'- Maslow BASED ON POOLED STDEV L 0.81 N MEAN SIDEV I I i'v'l 77 1.792 0.908 (---*--) II 23 1.565 0.945 ( - 6:- III 11 1.818 1.158 ( ‘ 4' Iv 16 1.437 0.964 ( . ) 153 VI 36 1.556 0.998 ( I ) :2 15 A; N.S. POOLED SIDEV - 0.957 1 20 1.60 2.00 2.40 e sonebod ) Aeti°n 18 (educat Y INDIVIDUAL 95 per CI'S Ion MEAN F BASED ON POOLED STDEV ' T:::$w N MEAN SIDEV I I I 1.25 I 77 1.688 1.103 (---~--—) I: 23 1.652 0.935 ( ~ ) 6:- III 11 1.909 1.044 ( w 4, 16 1.125 0.806 < - ) 158 IV 36 1.556 1.027 (-----*-----) VI I I I N.S. POOLED SIDEV - 1.035 1.20 1.80 2.40 Ac“°“ 19 (att°nd m'°tlng) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCI CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED SIDEV P- "“1°' N MEAN SIDEV I I I 0.92 1‘V‘1 77 1.2857 1.0495 (----*--) I 23 0.9565 0.9760 ( . ) , 6:- II 11 1.5455 1.2136 ( . 4, III 16 1.0625 0.8539 ( . ) 158 IV 36 1.1667 0.8106 ( ----- - ------ ) VI I I I N.S. 1.00 1.50 2.00 POOLED SIDEV I 0.9851 177 Appendix E. ANOVA and 95% confidence interval estimates for each environmental action when comparing mean env1ronmental action frequencies among the seven most common Rokeach dominant terminal values. * Action 1.8 (pick up litter - home) Rokoach INDIVIDUAL 95 per CI'S 808 MEAN F. valu. BASED ON POOLED SIDEV 0.51 14 N MEAN SIDEV I I I I 4 72 0.08333 0.40246 (---*---) dr- 9 27 0.74074 1.19591 ( ) 5' a 18 0.66667 1.08465 ( . ) 139 15 9 0.77778 1 30171 ( 3 8 1.00000 1.41421 ( . § ”.5. 18 6 0.66667 1.21106 ( . ) . POOLED SIDEV - 0.87917 00 0.50 00 1.50 Action 1.b (pick up litter - neighborhood) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Eon MEAN Rokeach BASED 0N PooLED SIDEV ’- value N MEAN SIDEV I I I I 2.10 14 72 1.778 0.953 (---5--) 4 27 2.185 0.622 (----5--—-) di- 9 18 2.167 0.618 ( . ) 6. 8 9 1.889 0.782 . 139 15 8 2.375 0.518 ( ) 3 6 2.500 0.548 ( . ) N.S. 13 6 2.333 0.816 ( ) POOLED SIDEV - 0.816 1.50 2.00 2.50 3.00 Action 1.: (pick up litter - parks) INDIVIDUAL 95 pcr CI'S r03 MEAN Rokeach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F- valu. N MEAN SIDEV I I I I 4-07 14 2 1.639 0.737 (--*--) 4 27 2.222 0.698 (-—--I ----- ) di- 9 18 2.167 0.514 (---*----) 6. 3 9 1.889 0.601 ( 5 ) 139 15 8 1.875 0.835 ( * ) 3 6 2.167 0.408 ( t ) P<.01 18 6 2.500 0.548 ( * ) POOLED SIDEV - 0.688 1.50 2.00 2.50 3.00 . - t r - commercial) 3°“°“ l°d (F‘Ck up lit ° INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S ron MEAN '_ Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV L L 1.15 Vilfl. N MEAN STDEV I .:-) I I 14 72 1.1250 0.7680 (-- _-_-) d£_ 4 27 1.4444 1.0127 (-: ______ ) 6' 9 18 1.2778 0.5745 £ ------ 139 a 9 3-2223 . , 8 1.37 o . ___ _-_.__-___. . . 15 6 1.6667 0.5164 (----:::-—;:_---) ) N S is 6 1.3333 0.8165 .( ------ -:— ¢ ¢ 0.60 1.20 1.80 2.40 POOLED STDEV I 0.8028 Appendix E (continued). Action 2.a (recycle - paper) Rokeach value N MEAN 14 72 0.9167 4 27 1.6296 9 18 1.3889 8 9 1.7778 15 8 2.2500 3 6 2.1667 18 6 2.0000 POOLED SIDEV - 1.2173 STDEV 1.1598 1.3053 1.1950 1.3944 1.1650 1.3292 1.2649 Action 2.b (recycle - glass) Rokeach value N MEAN 14 72 0.4306 4 27 1.4444 9 18 1.3333 8 9 1.4444 15 8 1.8750 3 6 1.6667 18 6 1.1667 POOLED STDEV I 1.1679 STDEV 0.9319 1.3960 1.3284 1.4240 1.3562 1.5055 1.1690 Action 2.c (recycle - metal) Rokeach value N MEAN 14 72 0.4722 4 27 1.0741 9 18 1.3333 8 9 0.5556 1s 8 1.7500 3 6 1.0000 13 6 1.1667 POOLED STDEV I 1.1327 STDEV 0.9490 1.3566 1.2834 1.1304 1.2817 1.0954 1.4720 Action 2.d (recycle - motor oil) 1378 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach MEAN STDEV N 121“. 72 0.08333 0.40246 4 27 0.74074 1.19591 9 18 0.66667 1.08465 8 9 0.77778 1.30171 15 8 1.00000 1.41421 3 5 0.66667 1.21106 13 6 0 0 POOLED STDEV I 0.37917 Action 3.a (Sign petitio Rokeach N MEAN STDEV {:1“' 72 0.6111 0.8649 4 27 1.3333 1.0000 13 1.7222 1.0178 9 9 1.3333 1.1180 3 a 1.3750 0.9161 :5 5 1.0000 0.8944 13 6 1.3333 1.5055 POOLED STDEV - 0.9594 n - friends) BASED 0N POOLED STDEV E- : i ¢ ¢ 3 . 4 0 (---*---) ( ----- *---) df- ( * ) 6, ( . ) 139 ( t ) ( * ) P<.01 ( i ) 0 75 1.50 2.25 3 00 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCI CI'S Eon MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F- I I I 5.02 (---~-> ( ..... a-----) df- ( 5 ) 6. ( a ) 139 ( * ) ( ‘ ) P<.01 ( . ) 0.75 1.50 2.25 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCI CI'S FOR MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F- I I I I 3.07 (--~--) (----I ----- ) d:- ( ‘ ) 6. ( . ) 139 ( t ( ' ) P<.01 ( ' ) 0.00 0 75 1.50 2.25 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Ion MEAN ‘ BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV g-84 (-:'--) <—------) 6:- (----~----) 6, ( ' > 139 ( * ) ( * ) P<.01 ( * ) L o 00 0.75 1 50 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN ?_ BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV - 4.72 ---;---) T _ ( (___..._____) _ g: t ) I ( -“M---*-------‘-'.' ) ( y ) P< o 01 < ‘ 1 o 60 1.20 1.80 Appendix E (continued). 1'79 Action 3.b (sign petition - public) Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 N 72 27 18 9 8 6 6 MEAN 0.5833 1.2593 1.6111 1.4444 1.6250 1.3333 1.0000 POOLED STDEV I 0.9685 STDEV 0.8999 0.9842 1.0369 1.0138 1.0607 1.0328 1.2649 Action 4.a (distribute petition - Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 N 72 27 18 9 8 6 6 MEAN 0.1667 0.6296 0.9444 0.8889 0.6250 0.3333 1.0000 POOLED STDEV I 0.7743 STDEV 0.5566 0.8389 1.1100 1.0541 0.9161 0.5164 1.0954 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR.MEAN Action 4.b (distribute petition - public) Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 MEAN 0.08333 0.40741 0.83333 0.77778 0.62500 0.33333 0.66667 POOLED STDEV I 0.63821 STDEV 0.43605 0.74726 0.85749 0.97183 0.74402 0.51640 0.81650 Action 5.a (write/call official - Rokeach Value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 72 27 H 00003900 MEAN 0.3611 1.2222 1.0556 1.1111 0.6250 1.8333 0.6667 POOLED STDEV I 0.8360 STDEV 0.7181 0.9740 1.0556 0.9280 0.5175 0.7528 1.0328 Action 5.0 (write/call official - state) Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 MEAN 0.2778 1.4815 1.1111 1.0000 1.3750 2.1667 1.1667 POOLED STDEV I 0.8418 STDEV 0.6548 (..n-) 0.9755 1.0786 0.8660 0.9161 0.7528 1.3292 BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV P- I I I 4.73 (--~--) (.....n--...) df- ( ) 5: ( ) 139 ( ) ( ‘ ) P<.01 ( ‘ ) 0.60 1.20 1.80 friends) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Eon MEAN BASED ON POOLED SIDEV P- : I I I 4.15 (--~--) (...-....) df- (m......) 6' ( ( ‘ ) 139 (--.-0......m) P< .01 0.00 0.60 1.20 1.80 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S ton MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV P- I I I I 5.02 (-----) (-...-a...—-) df- ( I ) 5 , ( ) 139 ( * ) P<.01 ( ) 0.00 0.30 0.80 1.20 local) INDIVIDUAL 95 BCI CI'S FOR MEAN BASED ON POOLED SIst :-43 <-—~-) 7 (---~----) df- (..--..‘n’ 6' ( I ) 139 ( a ) 6 (-----'--“--) P< .01 < ~ 1 - 0.00 0.75 1.50 2.25 INDIVIDUAL 95 Bcr CI'S ton MEAN !_ BASED 0N PooLED SIDEV . . 11.49 ("""“") 139 ( __. “""' ' ( "" . ) 94.01 (-----—-'-----> Appendix Action Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 ?OOLED Action Rokeec value 14 POOLE! Action Rokeac value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 POOLE! Actim Rokel Vllu 14 9001 Aetia ROkeI P001 1(30 -Appendix E (continued). Action S.c (write/call official - national) 35k..¢h INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN 5,1“. BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F- 14 N MEAN SIDEV I I I 7-23 4 72 0.3472 0.8077 (--..., 9 27 1.4074 1.1522 (-....--.., df- 8 18 1.4444 1.1490 (........---, 6, 15 9 1.0000 1.0000 4 139 3 8 1.1250 0.8345 18 6 1.6667 1.0328 3<.01 6 1.5000 1.2247 ( ) POOLED SIDEV - 0.9633 0.75 1.50 2.55 Action 6.a (campaign - local) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S roa MEAN ' Rokeach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV '- value N MEAN SIDEV I I I I 6.70 14 72 0.1250 0.4421 (--'-) 4 27 0.9630 1.1596 (-----~-----) 01- 9 18 1.2222 1.1660 6. 8 9 0.5556 0.8819 139 15 8 0.5000 0.7559 3 6 0.8333 0.7528 P<-°1 18 6 0.6667 1.0328 ( POOLED STDEV - 0.8062 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50 Action 6.b (campaign - state) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV 5' Value N MEAN SIDEV I I I I 7'7° 14 72 0.1111 0.4297 (--t--) 4 27 1.1481 1.1670 (.........) 9" 9 18 1.0556 1.1618 (-----~---) 5' 8 9 0.7778 1.2019 ‘ 139 15 8 0.2500 0.7071 3 6 1.0000 0.6325 P<.01 1a 6 0.6667 0.8165 POOLED STDEV I 0.8129 Action 6.0 (campaign - national) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rah-sch BASED ON POOLED SIDEV !- value N 333” srpzv I ~ 11.90 14 72 0.1250 0.5018 4 27 1.4444 1.2506 d:- 9 18 1.2778 1.1785 (--'--) 6, 8 9 0.5556 0.8819 139 15 8 0 0 3 6 ‘ 1.1667 0.7528 P<.01 13 6 0.5000 0.8367 A - POOLED STDEV I 0.8470 0.00 0.75 1.50 Action 7.a (vote - local) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN '- Rokeach 8A5ED ON POOLED STDEV 1 - 9.72 value N MEAN STDEV I I I I 14 72 0.9028 1.0768 (--‘--) d:- 4 27 2.1481 0.9488 (III-‘----) 6 9 18 2.0556 1.1618 (---'---) 159 a 9 1.5556 0.5325 8 2.5000 . :5 6 2.3333 1.0328 P<-°1 13 6 2.6667 0.5164 POOLED STDEV I 1.0373 Appendix Action Rokeac! value 14 POOLED Actim Rokoa value 14 ._-AAH¢~ Appendix E (continued). Action 7.b (vote - state) 1&31 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN BORODCh v‘lu. N MEAN STDEV BASED 0N 800LED SIDEVL L 10 47 14 7 0.9722 1.0873 (--t..) f r ' 4 27 2.2963 0.9121 (-...-..... 4: 9 18 2.1111 1.1318 (-—-_.-..-...)) c - a 9 1.5556 1.2360 ( . ) °' 15 a 2.7500 0.4629 ( , 139 3 6 2.6667 0.8165 ( , ’ 18 6 2.3333 0.8165 ( - . ) ’P<-°1 900LED SIDEV . 1.0310 0.75 1.50 2.55 3,30 Action 7.0 (vote - national) INDIVIDUAL 95 ICI CI'S Ion NEAN Roieach BASED ON POOLED SIDEV 5'83 va no N NEAN SIDEV I I I . 14 72 1.014 1.144 (-—-*--) 9 18 2.167 1.150 ( - ) 6. 3 9 1.667 1.323 ( . ) 139 15 8 2.750 0.463 (---------—--) 3 6 2.667 0.816 ( - )P<.Ol 13 6 2.667 0.816 (------'-—----) 200LED SIDEV - 1.078 1.50 2.25 3.00 Action 8.a (lights of: - home) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S roa NEAN Rokeach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV p. value N NEAN STDEV I I I 1 53 :4 :3 I333 3°33: “7'77 ' . . ( ‘ ) . g 13 2.333 0.323 ( - 1 2‘ . 0.333 ( - > ' 15 8 3.000 0.000 ( - ) 139 3 6 3.000 0.000 < * ) 18 6 2.833 0.408 (—— . ) “~5- BooLED SIDEV - 0.514 2.70 3.00 3.50 Action 8.b (1190:: at: - triand/relativo) INDIVIDUAL 95 BCI CI'S FOR NEAN '. Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDBV L 1__ 0.75 value N MEAN STDEV -—— i I ' 14 72 41.431 1.059 (7"") dz- 4 27 1 778 1.155 (---"“') 5 9 18 1.167 0.985 (----~---) ' 8 9 1 556 1 014 (-—------'-----) 139 a 12375 1.188 (--------------) 15 .5 1 333 1.211 (--------'----'7-") "-5° is 6 1.833 1.329 - (--—--f---'---:----) ¢__ " ' v 2.25 3.00 EooLED SIDEV - 1.090 O-7$. , 1:5°,,_ Action 8.6 (lights of: 7 Public’ . INDIVIDUAL 95 BCI CI'S Ion NEAN P_ k n BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV 1__ 1 97 no eac __ . 3 . T . NEAN SIDEV - __ value N 2.028 1.043 (--' ) d:- 72 ......) 14 1.556 1.155 (““" -- 4 27 (--.—......- ) 6' 18 2.333 00840 (-“-_“_*-”--O--)139 9 9 2.556 1.014 .-....--) a 1" .....u‘dO-O.) N.S. 15 5 1.333 1.211 (---'—“ _....-.......—-) 3 5 2.000 1.265 A (----f“ y 1-- 18 '—0.75 1.50 2.25 3.00 POOLED SIDEV . 1.042 Appendix Actim Rokea value 14 POO: Act 80k 182 Appendix E (continued). Action 9.8 (purchase - recyclable) INDIVIDUAL 95 PC'! CI’S FOR MEAN STDEV Rokeach 3A5 : value N NEAN SIDEV ED 0“ ‘OOLED :‘46 14 72 1.417 1.045 (--.__, - 4 27 2.630 0.492 __._ a _ 9 18 1.722 0.826 (---__.-___.,( ' ’ 6‘ I 3 9 1.889 1.167 (--.-...--e_....__) 139 15 g 2.000 0.926 (-----_--._-__-__) 2.667 0.516 -....--... .......__ 18 6 1.667 1.366 (-...._.__._.£______) ' ) P<.01 POOLED SIDEV - 0.941 1.50 2.25 3.00 Action 9.b (purchase - energy efficient) . INDIVIDUAL 9s Bcr CI'S POR MEAN Rokeach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV P- VIluO N MEAN SIDEV I 1.39 14 72 2.042 0.926 (--..., . 4 27 2.407 0.636 (.........-) dz. 9 18 2.000 0.907 (..-._-......) 5, B 9 2.556 0.527 (---—---—-~---—--—-) 139 15 8 2.000 0.756 (----—-----—- 3 6 2.333 0.816 (----—-----—---) N.S. 18 6 1.667 1.366 (---—--——-~—--—......) POOLED SIDEV - 0.866 _ _ ..1.2g __ 1:80 _ 2.40 3.00 Action 9.: (purchase - minimize harm) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S r0: AEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED SIDEV P- N NEAN SIDEV I 1_ 3.42 14 72 1.625 1.067 <-----) 4 27 2.444 0.847 (-....._--) dg- 9 18 2.111 0.900 (--—--~-—---) . 3 9 2.111 0.601 (---—---~----—--—) 139 IS 8 2.375 0.518 (----_._..__._____, 3 6 2.667 0.516 (---—---------) P<.01 18 6 2.000 1.549 (---—..----.-__._.__.) POOLED SIDEV - 0.972 1.50 2.25 3.00 3.75 Action 9.0 (purchase - concerned company) INDIVIDUAL 95 Bcr CI'S FOR MEAN ?_ Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV 2 14 value N MEAN STDEV - 14 72 1.000 1.101 (------ at- 4 27 1.704 0.993 <---—------) 6 9 18 1.500 1.043 (----...-..___) , 8 9 1.667 1.1%: ( (-d-—-—:------—-7-) l3 8 1.500 0.7 ------—- -------- :5 6 1.833 1.169 (----—---------«---) N.S. 13 6 1.167 1.472 ( ------ —-—--------—----) POOLED SIDEV - 1.079 0.75 1.50 2.25 Action 10.a (into - to friends) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S Ion MEAN r BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV - a k n V:I::C N NEAN SIDEV - I 9.28 14 72 0.5972 0.9881 (-'--) 4 27 1.6296 1.0795 (.......-) d1- 9 18 1.7778 1.0603 (..-....___-) 5' 3 9 1.7778 1.0929 (-...._-.-_._.__ 13 8 2.1250 0.6409 (-----........_._) 15 6 1.6667 1.2111 <-------_-._-..-_...) 9<.01 is 6 2.3333 0.5164 (---...---.-....._.-) 0 80 1.60 2.40 3.20 POOLED STDEV - 1.0030 Appendix Action 17.c Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 POOLED SID! 183 Appendix E (continued). Action 17.c (donate 5 - national) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV P- value N MEAN STDEV 14.81 14 72 0.3333 0.7506 (-*--) 4 27 1.8889 1.1209 (-----..-) di- 9 18 1.2778 1.0741 ( ..... e ..... ) 8 9 1.6667 0.7071 ( ----- t ....... ) 139 15 8 1.7500 1.0351 ( _______ e ________ ) 3 6 2.0000 0.8944 ( ....... 4 ________ ) p< 01 13 6 1 8333 1.1690 ( ........ e ......... POOLED SIDEV - 0.9080 0.75 1.50 2.25 - Rokeach values salvation peace health Ereedom sell-respect sense 01 accomplishment wisdom Appendix -Action I Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 100180 Action Rokeac value 14 POO! Act: Act val 14 15 18 PO( Appendix E (continued). -Action 10.: (info — to strangers) Rokeach value N 14 72 4 27 9 18 3 9 8 :5 . 18 5 MEAN 0.3472 1.1111 1.0556 1.1111 1. 5 1.5000 1.1667 ’OOLED STDBV - 0.9057 STDEV 0.7720 0.9740 0.8725 1.4530 0.6409 1.3784 0.9832 1184 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Action 11.a (discussion - sim11ar views) Rokeach value N 14 72 4 27 9 18 a 9 15 g 3 18 5 2.333 2.000 POOLED STDEV - 0.941 STDEV 0.918 1.001 0.938 0.928 0.744 1.211 0.894 Action 11.b (discussion - ditterent views) Rokeach value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 l-‘NQ mmcnsomxluz POOLED STDEV - 0.989 Action 12.a (public appearance — local) Rokeach value 14 7 4 2 9 1 8 15 3 N 2 7 8 9 8 6 18 6 POOLED‘STDEV . 0.8165 1.3292 Action 12.b (public appearance - state) Rokeach value N 14 72 4 27 9 18 8 9 15 8 3 6 18 6 POOLED STDEV I 0.33333 0.33333 0.58126 0 . m 4 a m m 1.00000 0 0.81650 0.81650 BASED ON POOLED SIDEV P- 6.58 (---) <----~--) d:- (-—--«-) 6. (-----------) 139 (-------~-------) (--—----—------- P<.01 ( ----«.-- ---—--- ) 0.75 1.50 2.25 INDIVIDUAL 95 BCI CI'S Ion MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV g"3 (--~--) .1.....) d1- (----------) (--...._..1-_._._..._) 139 (--—--——--——--—----) (__---_......n-....-_.——-) P<.01 t--—--—---—'--—--—---) 1.20 . 1.80 2.40 3.00 INDIVIDUAL 95 Ice CI’S FOR NEAN BASED ON BooLtD SIDEV 4.10 (-'—-) (---'- ) d1- ..---e-..__.) (------~-----) 139 _____ ...._..-....-) (---—---—------) P<.01 (---—-----------) 1.50 2.25 3.00 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI’S FOR MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV " 3.96 (--~---) (----e....) df- -—--— I ----- ( ..--l---e----._-_) 139 (---—----e------—--) --... ..... e-—_-—._-—-) P<.01 ( ------ -«---—--—-—-—) 0 00 0.60 1.20 1.80 INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S eon MEAN BASED 0N POOLED SID [- 3.18 (...---) (-—---~—---) 8:- ( --.... ......— ) I (------------------J 139 ..----_..e....-.-—) (-------'------‘--) ' P<.01 (----------——------) 4.. ...-- --- 0.00 0.40 0.80 Appendix 1 Action Rokeac! value 14 4 9 8 15 3 18 POOLE! Actioi Rokee value 14 Hmwmmbwfg Appendix E (continued). 1235 Action 12.: (public appearance - national) not..ch INDIVIDUAL 9s ecr CI'S 808 MEAN v.1“. N 12A" BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F- l‘ ‘ STOW 4? i “ 2 43 4 72 0.01389 0.11785 (--.-..) * ° 9 27 0.03704 0.19245 (....-._. ) d£_ 8 18 0.38889 0.97853 ( , 6 9 0.33333 1.00000 ( . ' 3 6 0.33333 0.81650 ( 1 ?OO - i t i : LED SIDEV 0.48245 -0.30 0.00 0.30 0.50 Action 13.a (report violation - acquaintance) INDIVIDUAL 95 BCI CI'S 808 MEAN 52:3:ch BASED 0N BooLED SIDEV P- 1‘ N MEAN STDEV ¢ , I I I 0.57 4 72 0.2361 0.6165 (---~--) 9 27 0.2222 0.5774 (---a---) d:- 8 . 18 0.1111 0.3234 ( - ) 6. 9 0.4444 0.8819 ( . ) 139 15 8 0 0 ( - ) 3 6 0.3333 0.8165 ( - ) N.S. 13 6 0.1667 0.4082 ( a ) POOLED SIDEV - 0.5019 0.20 0.00 0.40 0.80 Action 13.b (report violation - stranger) INDIVIDUAL 95 ecr CI'S FOR MEAN fi°ksach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV ’- thue N 333” sfpgv I I ; ¢ 0.50 14 72 0.4444 0.7485 (--*--) 4 27 0.5556 0.8473 (-------) di- 9 18 0.5000 0.7071 ( a ) 6. 8 9 0.2222 0.4410 ( . ) 139 15 8 0.5000 0.7559 ( a ) ‘ 3 6 0.8333 0.9832 ( s ) N.S. 18 6 0.3333 0.8165 ( - ) POOLED SIDEV - 0.7618 0.00 0.50 1.00 1.50 Action 14.a (lawsuit - yourselfi) P- Rokeach value 4'44 14 INDIVIDUAL 95 per CI'S r08 MEAN d:— 4 BASED ON POOLED SIDEV 6' 9 N MEAN SIDEV I I 139 3 , 72 0.02778 0.16549 (.......) 15 27 0.07407 0.26688 (—---*--') __. e<.01 is 6 0.33333 0.81650 - (-f - POOLED SIDEV g“9;3§438 °~°° °'15 °°32 0'48 Action 14.b (lawsuit - group) INDIVIDUAL 95 BCI CI'S Ion MEAN Rokeach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV I- value N MEAN STDEV —— I I I 1.66 14 72 0.06944 0.34910 (..-.....) 4 27 0.40741 0.79707 (......._....., d:- 9 18 0.16667 0.51450 (...............-) 5, 8 9 0.33333 0.70711 4—--—-----—---:---------) 139 15 8 0.37500 0.51755 (-—~ . ) 3 6 0.33333 0.81650 (— - -) N.S. 18 6 0.33333 0.81650 (— 1 —:e—— - -—) - 0.00 0.30 0.60 0 90 POOLED SIDEV - 0.55108 1136 Appendix E (continued). Action 15.a (injunction - yourself) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S 80R MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV P- value N MEAN SIDEV I I I 2.03 14 72 0.02778 0.16549 (-a-) 4 27 0.07407 0.26688 (----....) .81. 9 18 0 0 (---‘-.-) 6, 8 9 0 0 ( ‘ ) 139 15 a 0 0 ( ‘ ) 3 6 0.33333 0.81650 ( - ) “.5. 18 6 0 0 ( c—‘ ) POOLED STDEV - 3.;:-:; 0.00 0.20 0.40 Action lS.b (injunction - group) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED 0N POOLED SIDEV F' value N MEAN STDEV I I I I 2.53 14 72 0.09722 0.47943 (--‘---) 4 27 0.59259 0.88835 ( e ) di- 9 18 0.16667 0.51450 ( * ) 6. 8 9 0.55556 0.88192 ( = )139 15 8 0.50000 0.75593 ( e ) 3 6 0.33333 0.81650 ( - ) p<.os 13 6 0.33333 0.81650 ( e ) POOLED STDEV - 0.64722 0.00 0.32 0.64 0.96 Action 16 (stop buying — harmful) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV P- value N MEAN STDEV I I I I 2.17 14 72 1.639 1.052 (-'--) 4 27 2.222 0.641 (------) d:- 9 18 1.889 0.963 (---'---) 6. a 9 2.111 0.928 ( - ) 139 15 8 1.875 0.641 < - ) 3 6 2.667 0.516 ( - ) P<.05 13 5 1.333 1.159 ( A . ) POOLED STDEV - 0.941 1.50 2.25 3.00 3.75 Action l7.a (donate 5 - local) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED STDEV ’- value N MEAN STDEV I I I I 12.74 14 72 0.5833 0.8999 (~-*--) 4 27 2.1481 0 .9488 (...—.9..-) d!- 9 18 1.5000 1.1504 (----'---) 6. a 9 1.4444 1.1304 I ) 139 1 15 8 2.1250 0.3536 ( I ) 3 6 2.0000 0.6325 ( 5 ) P<.01 13 6 1.8333 0.9832 4 1 - - ) 1 poogzp srpgv . 0.9343 0.75 1.50 2.25 3.00 Action 17.b (donate 5 - state) INDIVIDUAL 95 PCT CI'S FOR MEAN Rokeach BASED ON POOLED SIDEV F- 6 va1u. N MEAN STDEV I I I 13-0 14 72 0.4306 0.8192 (-*-) 4 27 1.8889 1.0127 (--e--) di- 9 18 1.3333 1.0847 (---a--) 6. a 9 1.3333 0.8660 ( . ) 139 15 8 2.0000 0.5345 ( : ) 3 6 2.0000 0.6325 ( ) P<.01 18 6 1.1667 0.9832 ( t 1 ) - 0.75 1.50 2.25 POOLED SIDEV - 0.8862 BIBLIOGRAPHY Abelm ArbuU Balla Beer, Berry Borde Borde Burru Butte Champ Child 187 BIBLIOGRAPHY Abeles, T. "Environmental Education: A Community/University Approach". Journal of College Science Teaching. October 1973. Arbuthnot, J. "The Roles of Attitude and Personality Variables in the Prediction of Environmental Behavior and Knowledge." Environment and Behavior. Vol. 9. N0. 2. 1977. Ball—Rokeach, S.J., M. Rokeach, J.W. Grube. The Great American Values Test, The Free Press, New York, 1984. 'Beer, M. The Ohio State University Bureau of Business Research, 1966. Berry, M. and G. Lowe. "The Challenge of Environmental Education". EPA Journal. April 1978. Borden, R. and P. Powell. "Androgyny and Environmental Orientation". Current Issues in Environmnental Education. ed. A. Sacks, L. Iozzi, and R. Wilke. Vol. 8. 1983. Borden, R. and A. Schettino. "Determinants of Environmentally Responsible Behavior". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 10. No. 4. Spring, 1979. Burrus-Bammel, L. "Information's Effect on Attitude - a Longitudinal Study to Investigate Immediate and Long—Term Effects in a Camp". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 9. No. 3. Spring 1978. Buttel, F. and W. Flinn. "Social Class and Mass Environmental Beliefs". Environment and Behavior. Vol. 10. No. 3. September 1978. Champeau, R. "Development of a Perceived Locus of Control Instrument for Environmental Action", unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1982. Childress, R. "Public School Environmental Education Curricula: A National Profile". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 9. No. 3. Spring 1978. Cofer, Dambaci Davis, Dillma Dunla] Dunla Force Gallc Hart Harv Hebe Hebe Heb 188 Cofer, C. and Appley, M. Motivation: Theory and Research, John Wiley and Sons, New York, 1964. Dambach, C. "Conservation Through Adult Education". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 1. No. 2. Winter 1969. Davis, C. and F. Surls. "Community-Oriented Environmental Education". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 6. No. Summer 1975. Dillman, D. and J. Christenson, "The Public Value for Pollution Control", in W.R. Burch, N.H. Cheek, and L. Taylor, ed. Social Behavoir, Natural Resources, and the Environment, New York, Harper and Row, 1972. Dunlap, R.; R. Gale; and B. Rutherford. "Concern for Environmental Rights Among College Students". American Journal of Economics and Sociology. Vol. 32. 1973. .Dunlap, R.; J. Grieneeks; and M. Rokeach. "Human Values and Pro—Environmental Behavior". AAAS Selected Symposium 75. Westview Press. Boulder, Colorado. 1983. Force, J; T. Seliga; and R. Swain. "A Community Environmental Education Program on Air Quality". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 8. No. 3. Spring 1977. Gallo, V., "Adult Learning: Motivation Key to Success". Clearing House. Vol. 7. December, 1971. Hart, R.; G. Friedrich; and W. Brooks. Public Communication. New York. Harper and Row. 1975. Harvey, G. "Environmental Education: a Delineation of Substantive Structure". Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Southern Illinois University. 1976. Found in H. Hungerford; B. Peyton; and R. Wilke. "A Framework for Environmental Education Curriculum Planning and Development". Heberlein, T. "Environmental Attitudes". Zeitschrift fur Umweltpolitik. 1980 Heberlein, T. and J. Black. "Attitudinal Specificity and the Prediction of Behavior in a Field Setting". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol. 33. No. 4. 1976. Heberlein, T. and J. Black. "Cognitive Consistency and Environmental Action". Environment and Behavior. Vol. 13. No. 6. November 1981. Hines Horsl Hunge Hunge Hunge John: Karl Know Knut KUpc Lars Lewi Made 189 Hines, J. "Synthesis Report of Research in Environmental Education 1971—1982 Part V: Environmental Action", Presentation at National Association for Environmental Education meeting, Fall 1983. Horsley, A. "Effects of a Social Learning Experiment on Attitudes and Behavior Toward Environmental Conservation". Environment and Behavior. September 1977. Hungerford, H. and R. Peyton. Teaching Environmental Education. Portland ME. J. Weston Walch. 1976. Hungerford, R.; R. Peyton; and R. Wilke. "A Framework for Environmental Education Curriculum Planning and Development". Current Issues in Environmental Education. Ed. A. Sacks et al. Vol. 6. 1980. Hungerford, H.; A. Tomera; and R. J. Wilson. "An Analysis of (Intervention) and Allied Variables in Studies Abstracted in RIEE 1971—1980". Environmental Education. Vol. 8. ed the Emphasis Placed on Overt Environmental Behavior Current Issues in . by A. Sacks, L Iozzi, and R. Wilke. Johnson, D.; R. Champeau; 1983. and C. Newhouse. "Perspectives on the Status and Success of Environmental Education for Adults“. Current Issues in Environmental Education. Ed. A. Sacks et al. Vol. 6. 1980. Karlins, M. and H. Abelson. Persuasion. New York. Springer Publishing Co. Inc. 1970. Knowles, M. The Adult Learner: A Neglected Species. 2 ed. Gulf Publishing Co. Houston. 1978. Knutson, J. The Human Basis of the Polity. Chicago. Aldine-Atherton. 1973. Kupchella and Levy. “Basis Principles in the Education of Environmentalists". Education. Vol. 6. Journal of Environmental Spring 1975. Larson, M.; M. Forrest; and L. Bostian. "Participation in Pro—Environmental Behavior“ Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 12. No. 3. 1981. Lewin, K. "Frontiers in Group Dynamics“. Human Relations. Vol. 1. No. 2. 1947. Maddi, 8. Personality Theories a comparative analysis, The Dorsey Press, Homewood, Ill. 1968. V‘- Ma Ma Ma Ma Mi Mi Ra RI 190 Maloney, M. and M. Ward. "An Objective Scale for Measurement of Ecologic Attitudes and Knowledge". American Psychologist. Vol. 28. July 1973. Maslow, A. Motivation and Personality, New York, Harper and Bros., 1954. Maslow, A. Motivation and Personality (2 ed.). New York Harper and Row. 1970. Matthews, J. "Environmental Education and Community Action". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 6. No. 2. Winter 1974. Michigan Department of Education. "Life Role Competencies". 1978. Miller, H. "Participation of Adults in Education: A Force—Field Analysis". Center for Studies of Liberal Education for Adults. Occasional Papers No. 14. Brookline, Mass. ERIC # ED011996. 1967. 'Moore, H. "Energy—Related Information/Attitude Measures of College—Age Students". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 12. No. 4. Summer 1981. Peyton, R. B., and H. Hungerford, "An Assessment for Teachers' Abilities to Identify, Teach, and Implement Environmental Action Skills.", Current Issues VI, 1980. Ramsey, C. and R. Rickson. "Environmental Knowledge and Attitudes". Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 8. No. 1. Fall 1976. Renshon, S. Psychological Needs and Political Behavior. New York. The Free Press. 1974. Robinson, J.P., and P.R. Shaver, Measures of Social Psychological Attitudes. Ann Arbor, MI. Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, 1973. Rokeach, M. Beliefs, Attitudes, and Values. San Francisco. Jossey—Bass. 1968. Rokeach, M. “Terminal Values Survey", copyright, Halgren Tests, San Francisco, CA 1972 Rokeach, M. The Nature of Human Values. New York. The Free Press. 1973. Schoenfeld, C. “President's Address to the 1980 NAEE Conference“. Current Issues in Environmental Education. Ed. A. Sacks et al. Vol. 6. 1980. 191 Sia, A., Hungerford, H., and Tomera, A. "An Analysis of Selected Predictors of Pro—Environmental Behavior: A Preliminary Study presented at National Association for EnVironmental Education conference, Fall 1983. Stapp, W. et. al.(12 others), "Toward a National Strategy or EnVironmental Education". Environmental Education Current Issues in Vol. 5. 1979. Tichenor, P.; G. Donohoe; C. Olien; and J. "Environment and Public Opinion Journal of Environmental Education. Vol. 2. No. 4. Summer 1971. Tucker, L. "The Enviromnentally Concerned Citizen, Correlates“. Environment and Behavior September 1978 Bowers. Some Vol. 10. U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service "Notes on Coordinating a Community Program in Environmental Education" Minneapolis. Jenny Publishing 1977. Van Liere, K and R. Dunlap. Behavior: "Moral Norms and Environmental An Application of Schwartz’s Norm-Activation Model to Yard—Burning" Journal of Applied Behavioral Psychology. Vol. 8. No. 2. 1978. Van Liere, K. and R. Dunlap. "The Social Bases of EnVironmental Concern. A Review of Hypotheses, Explanations, and Empirical Evidence". Public Opinion Quarterly. Vol. 44. No. 2. 1980. Ward, T. "Non Formal Education — What Is It?". Class Handout. Education 821 E. Michigan State Univer51ty Winter 1982. Weigel, R. and L. Newman, “Increasing Attitude-Behavior Correspondence by Broadening the Scope of the Behavioral Measure" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, No. 6, 1976 Weigel, R. and J. Weigel, "Environmental Concern the Development of a Measure" Environment and BehaVior, V 10 ,No. 1, 1978. Wicker, A. “Attitudes vs Actions The Relationship of Verbal and Overt Behavioral Responses to Attitude Ob3ects" The Journal of Social Issues. Vol. 25. No. 4. 1969.