lllll"'ll1llli'vlmflilllmllmltl if _ 3 1293 10702 5169 . iiifiifitfi‘ Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled "Machines, Families, and Militaries: How Organizational Members Use Language to Construct Organizational Reality" presented by Catherine Lee Helms has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. degree in Communication W V avraca/ Major professor Date October 291 1985 0-7639 ‘ MS U is an Wank Action/Equal Opportunity Institution MSU LIBRARIES RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. ‘FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. MACHINES, FAMILIES, AND MILITARIES: HOW ORGANIZATIONAL MEMBERS USE LANGUAGE TO CONSTRUCT ORGANIZATIONAL REALITY BY Catherine Lee Helms A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Communication 1985 Accepted by the faculty of the Department of Communication, College of Communication Arts and Sciences, Michigan State University, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Arts degree. Guidance JQCZzfifc’L V flair/(IQ? Chairman Committee: 2 / 1 ,' ' \ “fl 6 / ;l/YLr/ M -.. MA? A7 @fiflflf ABSTRACT MACHINES, FAMILIES, AND MILITARIES: HOW ORGANIZATIONAL MEMBERS USE LANGUAGE TO CONSTRUCT ORGANIZATIONAL REALITY BY Catherine Lee Helms The influence of language usage upon the perceptions of organizational members and, subsequently, upon the or- ganizing process is explored. Organizing is viewed as a dynamic process in which members reduce equivocality by actively and consensually structuring their environment. Metaphor was selected as the pertinent linguistic device in this process for its ability to clarify unfamiliar sit- uations and still allow for diverse interpretations of an equivocal event. Contrary to the hypotheses, it was found that as communication among organizational members increased the similarity of the linguistic features appearing in their communication (e.g., metaphor) decreases. Furthermore, as communication increased, the accuracy with which they pre- dicted each others attitudes decreased. However, meta- phorical similarity was found to be positively related to the accuracy of members' attitudinal predictions. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to thank, first of all, Dr. Richard V. Farace for his unfailing patience and insightful guidance which, coupled with a wry wit, served to avert a number of poten— tial disasters during the course of my graduate years. I would also like to extend my appreciation to Dr. Gerald R. Miller and Dr. William Donahue for their astute counsel during the preparation of my Master's Thesis. I also wish to recognize the contributions of my parents, Leo and Martha Helms, who provided me with the emotional sup- port which made everything possible. And finally, I wish to thank my husband, Rashmi Kothari, for encouraging me at the low times, rejoicing with me at the high times, and laughing with me at all times. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page List of Tables . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v List of Figures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi Chapter I -- Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . l The Process of Organizing . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Definitional Processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Agreement and Perceived Agreement . . . . . . . 5 Stability v. Flexibility . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 The Role of Communication in the Organizing Process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 Chapter II -— Metaphor as a Powerful Linguistic Device . . . . . . . . 13 Definition of Metaphor . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 Metaphor and Homogeneity . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 Metaohor and Heterogeneity . . . . . . . . . . . 21 Chapter III -- Methodology . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 Operationalizations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 Chapter IV -- Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 Content Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 Statistical Analyses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 Chapter V -- Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 Future Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 iii TABLE OF CONTENTS (continued) References Notes.................. Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Appendix -- Example of Survey Questionnaire Completed by Respondents . . . iv LIST OF TABLES ‘Page Table 1 -- Frequency of Responses Within Each Thematic Category . . . . . . . 33 LIST OF FIGURES Figure l -- Illustration of the Theoretical Model to be Empirically Tested . Figure 2 -- Illustration of the Revised Model Based Upon the Data Collected . vi 42 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The Process of Organizing Organizing is a process composed of the patterned be- havioral interactions of a number of individuals collectively seeking to specify appropriate responses to their environ- ment (i.e., to reduce equivocality)l (Deetz, 1982; Hunt, 1972; Monge, 1977; Pacanowsky & O'Donnell—Trujillo, 1983; Weick, 1979). The process notion of organizing has two im- plications: (1) it is dynamic, constantly changing and developing, and (2) it is complex, with many elements which interact and influence one another simultaneously. The pat- terned nature of organizational members' interactions point to the presence of structured relationships (however flex- ible) that guide and coordinate these interactions. The search for efficient and effective responses to phenomena is the motivating factor behind organizing. The organizing process as outlined above often con- strains perceptions of stability and predictability by an organization's members. "Much of what happens in organiza- tions to individuals, formal units, and informal groups is chaotic. As members . . . communicate to set and achieve goals, organize themselves into cohesive units, and establish social ties and group identities, they often find their 2 here-and-now experiences confusing" (Bormann, 1983, p. 101). In reaction to the dynamism of the organizing process, or- ganizational members engage in sense-making behavior in- tended to impose structure and, consequently, predictability upon their world (Conrad, 1983). This structure is com- prised of rules and procedures mandated by the organization and its members either formally or informally. Implicit in this conceptualization of organizing is the idea that individuals actively structure and reinforce their 'environment'. Not only do they construct and enforce the rules and procedures enabling them to make sense of and deal with their environment, they also actively influence the perceptions (and evaluations) leading to these rules and procedures. Snyder, Berscheid, and Decker-Tanke (1977) claim that, "unbeknownst to the perceiver, the reality that he confidently perceives to exist in the social world has, in fact, been actively constructed by his own transactions with and operations upon the social world" (p. 658). In effect, people externalize their preconceived notions of what should exist in the world by recognizing only those events and objects which they've anticipated, and then be- having as if these are the only events and objects which actually exist (Berger & Luckmann, 1966; Morgan, 1983). Individuals not only select stimuli, they also manipulate them to fit into their interpretational frameworks. The meanings of phenomena are predetermined to a large extent. The result of the organizing process is a decrease in the equivocality perceived by organizational members. The resolution of equivocality leads to feelings of environ- mental stability (Festinger, 1954; Farace, Monge, & Russell, 1977), which further engenders perceptions of environmental control. Organizational members collectively structure organizational reality, and with definition comes a sense of control. Three key issues arise from the pro- cess notion of organizing which the following sections explore. Each of these issues involves a further breakdown of how organizational members organize. First, the definitional process engaged in by organi- zational members is socially enacted. Social construction of the environment is motivated by two underlying elements: (1) the need to conform, and (2) the need to incorporate 'outside' information in developing attitudes. Second, consensual definition of phenomena occurs on two levels: (1) actual definitional agreement between or- ganizational members, and (2) perceived definitional agree- ment between organizational members. Third, the definitional process, as outlined in the preceding two sections, enables the organization to maintain a sense of stability while retaining flexibility. These three issues are discussed further in the following section. Definitional Processes In this section a theoretical explanation for why social construction occurs is presented. Interpretation of equivocal input arises from the cooperative efforts of a number of individuals collectively musing upon the meaning and implications of an act or event (Salancik & Pfeffer, 1978; Weick, 1979; Deetz, 1982). Social Information Processing Theory, developed by Salancik and Pfeffer (1978), centers on the collective manner in which much equivocal stimuli are made sensible. The social information processing approach pro— ceeds from the fundamental premise that indivi- duals, as adaptive organisms, adapt attitudes, behavior, and beliefs to their social context and to the reality of their own past and pre- sent behavior and situation . . . The social context has two general effects on attitude and need statements: (1) it provides a direct cOnstruction of meaning through guides to socially acceptable reasons for action; (2) it focuses an individual's attention on cer- tain information, making that information more salient, and provides expectations con- cerning individual behavior and the logical consequences of such behavior. Individuals develop many of their attitudes and per- ceptions of the world on the basis of feedback from, and interaction with, those around them. Those with whom in- dividuals associate act as interpretational mediators, focusing their attention on particular stimuli and influ- encing the manner in which they attach meaning to that stimuli. Directed meaning is constructed, perpetuated, and modified via communicative interactions among members. They embark on collaborative efforts to (1) ensure conformity for themselves in relation to others and of deviates (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955; Newcomb, 1953) and (2) obtain ad- ditional attitudinal and perceptual input from others with which to devise their interpretation of equivocal phenomena (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). Further, the more equivocal events are, the more im- perative socially processed definitions become (Salancik & Pfeffer, 1978). Festinger (1954) concluded that the greater ambiguity of job characteristics, the more the worker will rely on social comparisons to assess them. Deutsch and Gerard (1955) discovered that "the more un- certain the individual is about the correctness of his judgement, the more likely he is to be susceptible to both normative and information social influences in making his judgement" (p. 630). White and Mitchell (1979) found that in the absence of objective information, employees relied more heavily on social cues in evaluating aspects of their work environment. This brief review of the prevailing literature on social construction of reality focuses on two components: (1) individuals' need to conform to their social environment, and (2) individuals' reliance on others form the trans- actional activity of sense-making and interpretational re- inforcement. The following section provides an explanation for how social consensus occurs. Agreement and Perceived Agreement Scheff (1967) developed a two-element model of con- sensus. He defines social consensus in terms of the levels of co-orientation achieved. The zero-order level repre- sents actual agreement or disagreement. The first-order level refers to perceived agreement or disagreement. So, for example, if two groups of assembly line workers agree that a new company policy is fair and equitable then there is a zero-order co-orientation. If each also believes that the other is in agreement with them, then there is first— order co-orientation. The model incorporates higher levels of consensus (for instance, at the second level groups recognize each others' perception of the other's position); however, the focus of this paper will be on the zero— and first-order levels. Earlier, the dichotomy organizations face between order and adaptability was noted. It was posited that organiza— tional members collectively arrive at a response to this issue. One mechanism used in organizations is to focus on the attainment of perceived agreement rather than on actual agreement; on first-order rather than zero—order consensus (Farace, Monge, & Russell, 1977; Eisenberg, 1983). If mem- bers believe they are in agreement with one another, regard— less of their actual position, then the perceptions of stability and control so necessary to organizational sur- vival are fostered. Tangentially, on the occasions where members perceive themselves to be in agreement when they are not, each will confidently act upon his or her inter- pretation of the situation. Rather than resulting in chaos, there is the potential for the organization to reap the benefits and select among a number of successfully carried through plans of action instead of becoming fixed to one standard operating procedure. Organizations are thus able to retain multiple options in their response to equivocal stimuli and at the same time encourage perceptions of stability by organizational members. Weick (1979) calls this process 'split decision'. "It is the split usage of retained content that enables the organization or organism to fit into a particular environment, generate immediate activity, and still detect the necessity for altered actions to improve that fit" (p. 219). It's easy to assume that adaptation is promoted consistent acts emitted by a tightly run or- ganization. If people make that assumption, then evidence of ambivalence and hypocrisy is treated as a threat to adaptation and survrval . . . the opposite is true. Ambivalence may guarantee adaptation in the short run and sur- vival in the long run. If some faction of the organization wants to act on the basis of pat wisdom and some other faction wants to act in ways that Oppose the past, both factions are partially correct. More importantly, both factions should act on behalf of their beliefs. What is being observed here is simply another instance of the split decision pattern that allows the organization to retain both flexi- bility and stability (p. 244). The next section introduces the function of multiple-order consensus in the attainment of an effective stable/flexible balance. Stability v. Flexibility An organization's struggle to mesh idiosyncratic responses to equivocality invariably results in an attempt to institutionalize the various attributional systems of its members, making them more predictable and, therefore, more controllable (Berger & Luckmann, 1966). If this auto- matic response continues unchecked, an organization can find itself so protectively padded by rules and regulations that it is unable to sense or adjust to external changes. This insularity works to retard internal growth as well. In spite of the overwhelming strain toward the security of tight control, organizations must somehow maintain a balance between stability and adaptability, between control and coordination, and between collective and individual sense-making. They must develop and maintain a 'sense' of themselves, their raison d'etre, so as not to be subsumed by the larger social order or torn apart from within, and to aid interpretational processes. Yet at the same time they must remain sensitive enough to detect and react to exceeded tolerance levels, both externally and internally. It is through communication (e.g., how language is used) that this is accomplished. It is the distance between zero- and first-order consensus which largely determines an organ- ization's ability to accomplish the above. An organization which supports diverse attitudes (i.e., low zero-order con- sensus) allows for a wide range of responses to its environ- ment. An organization which maintains a strong sense of purpose and cohesion (i.e., high first-order consensus) en- genders feelings of stability and predictability. An or- ganization which incorporates both (low zero-order and high first-order consensus) has achieved a balance between stability and flexibility. The Role of Communication in the Organizing Process The nature of an organization's sense-making agenda determines the success of the organizing process. Communicative acts are the means by which sense-making agendas are constructed, diffused, and transformed. People are motivated to communicate with others, and through this communication, to develop stable, socially-derived inter— pretations of events and their meanings (Festinger, 1954; Jacobson & Seashore, 1951; Frost & Morgan, 1983). Social reality is defined by the language used by the members of the social system. Language does more than communicate infor- mation and more than enable the members to make sense. Language creates the reality. The 'organization' has no objective reality (in a positivistic sense), but rather is created daily by the linguistic enactmEfits of its members in the course of their every— day communications between eadh other; that is, by the way in which its members talk, hold discourse, share meanings (Evered, 1983, p. 126). The definition of a phenomenon is little more than the culmination of terms used to describe it and names pre- viously applied to it. "When people collectively try to shrink the possible meanings attached to an equivocal in- put, they essentially are negotiating issues of naming and connection" (Weick, 1979). Referring to previous passages, organizations and their members have a tendency to 'over' define and 'under"re- define. It is to be expected that the labelling procedure activated by ambiguous stimuli consumes time and effort, particularly when the variable interpretations of many are involved. Moreover, once something has been satisfactorily defined, it is no longer perceived of as uncertain and, sub- sequently, a sense of control is generated. Consequently, 10 if unchallenged, many social perceptions (i.e., meanings attached to particular phenomena) would remain relatively stable. However, the world is not stable and unchanging. For an organization to survive, its members must also be able to gauge and respond to needed changes in their previously satisfactory definitions. An emphasis on attaining high first-order and low zero-order consensus of definitions is one mechanism that facilitates organizational sensitivity and flexibility. One effective strategy for fostering perceptions of definitional agreement (regardless of whether or not actual definitional agreement exists) is to use unclear language. The more ambiguous utterances are, the more room is left to infer the meaning which is in line with their own attitudes and values. "Strategic ambiguity is essential to organizing be- cause it allows for multiple interpretations to exist among people who content they are attending to the same message (i.e., perceive the message to be clear) . . . Ambiguity is used strategically to foster agreement without limiting the number of possible interpretations" (Eisenberg, 1983, p. 9). "[Purposeful ambiguity] is a potentially useful message strategy for situations in which either the source has little information about the attributes of the audience or he is aware that the receivers have heterogeneous at- titudes toward the message issue" (Wilson, 1971, p. 3). In one sense, the normative function of social consensus is 11 being fulfilled. Everyone perceives their attitude as being reinforced simultaneously with the belief that every— one else agrees with it. In another sense, the informa— tive function of social consensus is satisfied. If a group doesn't have a solidified attitude toward a subject, yet is predisposed toward a general position, an ambiguous message will provide them with enough information to develop an attitude, albeit biased by the original per— ceptual framework of the group. Steinfatt, Miller, and Bettinghaus' (1974) research supports the idea that the greater the ambiguity of a statement, the greater the chance that one or more of the interpretations of it will not occur to the receivers. They "would not necessarily be reasoning with the intended materials, but rather with only those re— lationships which have occurred to [them]" (p. 317). The relationships and implications that become apparent to indi— viduals are influenced by those with whom they communicate frequently (i.e., the group of which they're a member). Given an ambiguous statement, some will interpret it to encourage a move toward change, while others will inter- pret it as reinforcement for the existing order; all the while believing themselves to be in agreement (Eisenberg, 1984). At one level, there is the freedom for individual group interpretation which allows for organizational flex- ibility. At another level, the perceptions of agreement lead to a more cohesive organization. 12 Strategic ambiguity, then, is an important mechanism for coping with the following organizational paradoxes: stability v. predictability, and organizational v. factional interpretive frameworks. Metaphorical language is parti- cularly useful in attaining strategic ambiguity. Metaphor has long been the exclusive domain of poets and rhetoricians. Recently, however, social scientists have recognized the power and pervasiveness of metaphorical language in everyday communication, especially in the appli- cation of connotative meaning to equivocal episodes or be- havior. "When people talk about [metaphor] they usually talk about its role as a decorative figure of speech, How— ever, one of the main contributions of modern linguists is to show that once we penetrate beneath the taken—for-granted meanings associated with these words, we quickly see that their role and significance in language and thought is much deeper and more pervasive . . . Metaphor makes meaning in a primal way; its role is not just embellishment" (Morgan, 1983, p. 602). Metaphor will be shown to be a potentially powerful communicative device in the organizing process. Its char- acteristics enable it to facilitate maintenance of organi- zations' stability/flexibility balance. CHAPTER II METAPHOR AS A POWERFUL LINGUISTIC DEVICE Definition of Metaphor Through the years, theorists have debated the defini— tion, functions, and components of metaphor. A theoretical continuum has arisen which ranges from contentions that all language is metaphorical (that anything symbolic qualifies as a metaphor) (Carlyle, 1927) to claim that only those linguistic configurations that conform to rigorous standards are metaphoric (Searle, 1979). Whatever the stance one adopts, there seems to be general agreement that a primary purpose of metaphor is to equate two literally unlike things so that additional understanding and comprehension are brought to at least one of them (Webster's New Collegiate Dictionary, 1979; Fraser, 1979; Kreftling & Frost, 1984; Black, 1979; Ortony, 1975; Pondy, 1983). Richards (1936) devised a succinct and comprehensive 'language' for discussing metaphors: 'Tenor' is the tOpic under discussion; 'Vehicle' is the metaphorical term being used; 'Ground' refers to what the tenor and vehicle have in common; and 'Tension' refers to the dissimilar char— acteristics of the two. 13 14 Searle (1979) discusses metaphor from a linguistic van- tage. He distinguishes between the speaker's intention (what he calls the utterance meaning) and the literal meaning of the words, sentences, and expressions (termed the word mean— ing). Based on these two concepts he devised a strategy for interpreting metaphorical statements: (1) Where the utterance is defective if taken literally, look for an utterance mean- ing that differs from sentence meaning, (2) when you hear ' tenor ' is ' vehiCle ', to find.possible utterance meanings look for ways in which ' tenor ' might be like ' vehicle ', and to fill in the respect in which ' tenor ' might be like ' vehicle ', look for salient, well-known, and distinctive features of ' vehicle ' things, and (3) go back to the ' tenor ' and see which of the many candidates for the ground are likely or even possible properties of ' tenor ' (p. 114). In other words, people exposed to a metaphorical statement first decipher it literally. If the literal sentence is nonsensical, then they search for metaphorical meaning by comparing the tenor and vehicle for insight into the ground and, consequently, comprehension of the figurative, or ut- terance, meaning. This above position has theoretical support, but little empirical support (Fraser, 1979; Glucksberg, Gildea, & Bookin, 1982). An alternative explanation is presented by Black (1979) who adopts an interaction view of metaphor. In context of a particular metaphorical state- ment, the tenor and vehicle 'interact' in the following ways: (a) the presence of the primary subject the tenor incites the hearer to select some of the secondary subject's the vehicle properties; (b) invites him to 15 construct a parallel implication—complex that can fit the primary subject; and (c) recipro- cally induces parallel changes in the secondary subject (p. 29). Black's view has gained both empirical and theoretical sup- port (Levin, 1979; Shon, 1979; Glucksberg, Gildea, & Bookin, 1982; Gardner & Winner, 1979). The interaction view suggests two things: One, that the metaphorical influence is bi- directional. That is, the tenor affects the meaning attri— buted to the vehicle and is affected by the use of that vehicle. Second, rather than determining figurative meaning only after exhausting the plausible literal meanings, people simultaneously take into account both the utterance and word meanings of a sentence. While attempting to conceptualize metaphors, many ty- pologies and metaphor models have been deduced. Ortony (1975) proposed a functional typology of metaphors. He claimed that metaphorical language performed three functions: (1) Metaphor enables the prediction of a chunk of characteristics in a word or two that would otherwise require a long list of characteristics individually predicated the Compactness thesis . . .(2) Metaphor enables the predication by transfer of characteristics which are unnameable the Inexpressibility thesis . . .(3) Because of a metaphor's greater proximity to per— ceived experience and consequently its greater vividness, the emotive as well as the sensory and cognitive aspects are more available, for they have been left intact in the transferred chunk the Vividness thesis (PP. 47-50). Lakoff and Johnson (1980) have devised a metaphorical typoloqy in which (1) orientational metaphors are abstract 16 phenomenon conceptualized as bodily experiences of the physical world. Concepts are structured linearly (e.g., 'his spirits rose'); (2) ontological metaphors involve the projection of physical characteristics onto areas of experience which have a non-physical status (e.g., 'the idea was hard to swallow'); and (3) structural metaphors lay characteristics of one structured experience or activity onto another (e.g., 'time is money'). Fernandez (1972) developed a typology which focuses on aspects similar to Lakoff and Johnson's. According to Fernandez, (l) structural metaphors encourage translation be— tween realms on the basis of some structural isomorphism or relational similarity of parts (e.g., 'rain of blows'); and (2) textual metaphors facilitate assimilation on the basis of similarity in feeling tone (e.g., 'inflamed passion'). Finally, a typology has been constructed which divides up metaphors on the basis of their novelty (Black, 1979; Paivio, 1979; Cohen, 1979). Black (1979), for instance, distinguishes between dormant metaphors (e.g., 'falling in love') and active metaphors (e.g., 'he fought like a tiger'). Dormant metaphors have become cliches while active metaphors are relatively unique. He contends that an active metaphor has two important variables: (1) emphasis; the degree to which the metaphor cannot be adequately replaced (similar to Ortony's Inexpressibility thesis); and (2) resonance; the degree to which the metaphor encourages implicative elaboration (parallel to Ortony's Vividness thesis). The 17 greater the emphasis and resonance of a metaphor, the stronger and more active it is. There have been writings on both active and dormant/ dead metaphors. Berggren (1962) warns against the abuse of metaphor (i.e., the creation of dead metaphors), defining it as the loss of tension between the tenor and vehicle. The metaphor is turned into, not only a literal truth, but the literal truth about the principal subject in question. For instance, the mechanistic metaphor of organizations qualifies as a dead, or dormant, metaphor because many of its users no longer recognize or acknowledge the dissimilarities between mechanical parts comprising a machine and human parts com— prising an organization (i.e., the tension between the tenor (organization) and the vehicle (mechanical device) has dis— appeared). In essence, advocates of this metaphorical view of organizations perceive organizational members as unfeeling, replacable components of a machine; employees don't just resemble mechanical parts, they are mechanical parts. Searle (1979), however, contends that dead metaphors "have become dead through continual use, but their continual use is a clue that they satisfy some semantic need" (p. 98). Black (1979) supports this with the statement that a purpose of metaphor is to "remedy a gap in the vocabulary" (p. 33). If the metaphor is apt then “the new sense introduced will quickly become part of the literal sense" (p. 33). With regard to active metaphors, Paivio (1979) claims that there are three important concepts which determine the 18 strength of what he calls novel metaphors (to be used inter— changeably with active metaphors); (1) the similarity of the attributes of the tenor and the vehicle; (2) the propor« tional relation of a number of elements in the statement; and (3) the perception of a new entity which is distinct from the related elements considered separately. Krefting and Frost (1984) write that "novelty appears to depend on both frequency of use and tension between vehicle and topic . . . richness of implications is a function of the com— plexity of the ground shared by vehicle and tOpic . . . the more extended the metaphor, the more ground vehicle and tOpic are likely to share" (p. 8). In acknowledgement of the numerous diSparate concepts ualizations of metaphor, the definition to be used focuses on the major integrating feature of metaphors. Metaphor is the description of some phenomenon in terms of another, where there are shared as well as distinct characteristics between the two phenomena.2 Moreover, for the purposes of this paper, the interaction View of metaphor will be adopted. Additionally, attention will be focused on the dormant/active distinction of metaphors. Metaphor and Homogeneity There are many theorists who argue that the use of meta- phorical language enables organizational members to success- fully reduce equivocality by facilitating the social develop- ment and perpetuation of a cohesive sense of organizational reality (Asch, 1958; Ortony, 1975; Pondy, 1983). 19 The use of metaphors in organizational dialog plays a necessary role in helping organization participants to infuse their organizational ex— periences with meaning and to resolve apparent paradoxes and contradictions, and that this in— fusion of meaning or resolution of paradox is a form of organizing. In this sense, the use of metaphors helps to couple the organization, to tie its parts together into some kind of mean— ingful whole; that is, metaphors help to or— ganize the objective facts of the situation in the minds of the participants (Pondy, 1983, p. 157). Metaphors facilitate the organizing process in two ways. First, they are a useful device for moving from the known to the unknown. They allow the transfer of coherent chunks of characteristics -- perceptual, cognitive, emo— tional, and experiential -- from a vehicle which is known to a topic which is less so. In doing so they circumvent the problem of specifying one by one each of the often unnameable and innumerable characteristics; they avoid dis- cretizing the perceived continuity of experience and are thus closer to experience and con- sequently more vivid and memorable (Ortony, 1975, p. 53). Second, just as other communication forms serve to socially construct reality, so metaphors perform the same service more economically. As organizational members sift through behavioral patterns, social and political situations, and environmental events, they collectively enact parti- cular sequences of stimuli. Together they search for a sensible interpretation of these to reduce experienced equi- vocality and enhance perceived stability. It is during this process of searching that metaphors are especially powerful. Salient characteristics of the phenomenon are metaphorically articulated (Asch, 1958); a suitable encompassing metaphor 20 arises from this cumulative process; is seized upon (as- suming it successfully alleviates perceived equivocality); and a snowball effect ensues (i.e., the diffusion of the appropriated metaphor). For example, a popular organiza- tional metaphor is the military (Cooney, 1978; Weick, 1979). 'Military' organizations "do battle" rather than compete. Instead of hiring employees, they "recruit them". Events and situations are defined by the major metaphor; in this case, the military. When the stimulus is encountered in the future, it is recognized in the terms previously predicated and, as such, is easily identified and interpreted. Moreover, organiza- tional members are much more comfortable encountering the phenomenon because they can infer unobserved characteristics and categorize similar yet previously unknown phenomena, thereby increasing predictability and a sense of security in their now more structured world. Given these characteristics, it logically follows that if unfamiliar phenomena are talked about via the original perceptual metaphor (Berggren, 1972), other organizational members will be provided with the same mechanism for inter- pretation; easing and homogenizing their comprehension of the unknown phenomenon. In essence, theorists emphasize the role of metaphor in producing a clear and homogenous perception of equivocal events in organizations. Billow (1977) represents this posi- tion when he writes, "the creation of metaphors may lead to 21 greater intellectual clarity . . . unfamiliar or abstruse ideas can be more readily comprehended when expressed in terms of the familiar and more concrete" (p. 83). In sum, the organization and its component groups de- fine and are defined via the descriptive and evaluative com- ments being made about the situation. In this way organiza- tional stability is established, and metaphor provides a facile means of accomplishing this. Metaphor and Heterogeneity Although metaphorical assertions induce organizational cohesiveness in the sense that everyone is attending to the same analogy, it can be superficial cohesiveness. (It is superficial in instances of perceived agreement and actual disagreement.) It is its very superficiality which simul- taneously promotes organizational stability and organiza- tional flexibility. Metaphors are strategically ambiguous in that they establish perceptions of agreement while at the same time preserve multiple interpretations. The various individual interpretations are heavily influenced by group beliefs and norms. Diversification is furtherd by the dif- fering degrees of interest a group displays toward a topic (Newcomb, 1953). Pondy (1983) claims that "because of its inherent ambivalence of meaning, metaphor can fulfill the dual function of enabling change and preserving continuity" (p. 164). Along the same lines, Berggren (1962) holds that "metaphor constitutes the indispensable principle for inte- grating diverse phenomena and perspectives without sacrificing their diversity" (p. 237). 22 The conceptualization of metaphor's role in the or- ganizing process as an arbiter between stability and flex- ibility fits in well with the interaction view of metaphor. Metaphor acts as a perceptual filter. It does not merely compare obvious similarities between the tenor and the vehicle, but introduces new meaning to both by construing the one in terms of the other (Black, 1979; Berggren, 1962). Taken one step further, metaphor interacts with the prin- ciple subject to produce new meaning, and also interacts with the individual's cognitive framework to flavor that new mean- ing. Moreover, as stated previously, a major determinant of an individual's perceptual framework, especially in an or- ganizational context, is an individual's group membership(s). In other words, construction and interpretation of metaphor are influenced by the organization and the major metaphor it promotes, as well as by the composition and character of the group(s) to which individuals belong. Social consensus enacted by intimates is more compelling than that of strangers or acquaintances (Newcomb, 1953). As much as a major metaphor organizes perceptions of phenomenon there still exists per- sonal proclivities, primarily structured through group ties, which react to that metaphorical organizing in different ways. Metaphor, by its very ambiguity allows for a range of interpretations, which isn't possible in clear, literal speech. In doing so, it defines for individuals the socially accepted version as well as giving new information, both tempered by group perceptual frameworks. This, in turn, 23 facilitates diverse perceptions which lead to organizational flexibility. Metaphorical allusion precludes and abets perceptual consensus (i.e., social conformity) which further leads to confidence in the organization's definition of itself (i.e., stability). It is the diverse, group-influenced interpre- tation of metaphor which allows for organizational adaptive responses. Furthermore, diversity is enhanced as groups go about 'acting' on the basis of their perceptions, thereby rein- forcing their particular interpretation of the metaphorical analogy. People attend to external stimuli to define and reinforce internal states (i.e., emotions, perceptions, at— titudes, etc.). Therefore, individuals' own behavior serves, at least partially, as an indication and reinforcement of the perception which leads them to act in that way. As such, metaphor is more than an effective method of arriving at diverse consensus. External stimuli determine metaphorical use which, in turn, determines the recognition of external stimuli. "When we describe the workings of emo- tions, ideas, or trends of character, we almost invariably employ terms that also denote properties and processes al- ready observable in the world of nature . . . We say that a man thinks 'straight'; that he faces a 'hard' decision; that his feelings have 'cooled'" (Asch, 1958, p. 86). Sub- sequently, "the metaphoric assertions men make about them- selves or about others influence their behavior" (Fernandez, 24 1972, p. 42). In other words, the objects or activities from which metaphorical terms are chosen are bounded by what actually exists in the environment. In turn, the metaphorical terms adopted restrict and guide what is per- ceived in the environment, how it's interpreted, and what behavior follows in reaction. The issue arising from this discussion, one which needs to be empirically tested, is whether metaphors in fact do contribute to the forming and processing of percep- tions and attitudes in a group setting. If so, do they operate in accordance with the following theoretically pre- scribed model? Similarity of Amount of Discrepancy + . . - metaphors (———> communication 6% between actual and perceived 1\ consensus 4‘ Figure 1. Illustration of the Theoretical Model to be Empirically Tested In the next section, the proposed model is broken down into hypotheses and the specified variables are operation- alized. Also included is an explication of the statistical analyses to be performed upon the data and a description of the subject pool from which data is to be gathered. CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY Hypotheses Assuming that metaphorical language functions within the organizing process as theorized, the following three relationships are hypothesized. The first two hypothesis deal with amount of communication and metaphoric similarity: Hypothesis #1: The degree of similarity between members' perceptions of metaphors will increase as the amount of communication between members increases Hypothesis #2: The degree of similarity will increase more for active metaphors than for dor- mant metaphors as communication increases The relationship between amount of communication and simi- larity of active and dormant metaphorical language points up the ambiguous nature of metaphors. Dormant metaphors have become cliches through their continual usage. This usage points to members' perceptions of these metaphors as particularly apt expressions of salient aspects of their organizational environment. Dormant metaphors are typically organizations' central metaphor(s). Becuase of their embed- dedness, dormant metaphors aren't expected to vary dramati- cally across group boundaries.3 On the other hand, active metaphors would seem to be a response to specific and relatively idiosyncratic events 25 26 within the organizational environment. The 'uniqueness' of the event produces perceptions of equivocality in the members facing them. The equivocality causes the enact- ment of the definitional process. As members integrate new information, they talk about it. Their talk is influ— enced not only by already existing major metaphors of the organization, but also by the unique characteristics of the focal event. This uniqueness prompts the creation of new and unique metaphors. Due to the consensual nature of the defining process, these active metaphors will be shared by those who have participated in the talk. Additionally, those who communicate with one another more frequently are likely to engage in defining activities with one another more fre— quently. They are thus exposed to the same sets of active metaphors. The coexistence of relatively similar sets of dormant metaphors and diverse sets of active metaphors illustrates the accomodating nature of metaphors. Dormant metaphors interact with the various group environments resulting in differential focusing and interpretation of an organiza- tion's major metaphors. This in turn leads to a branching effect apparent in the different active metaphors the groups develop to cope with the events occurring around and within them. Hypothesis three focuses on the relationship between amount of communication and the discrepancy between zero- and first-order consensus. 27 Hypothesis #3: As the amount of communication among members increases, the distance between their actual and perceived (zero— and first-order) consensus decreases Previous research has indicated that as individuals com— municate more their attitudes coalesce, either as a function of attraction, intensity of attitude, or both (Newcomb, 1953; Scheff, 1967). As individuals communicate they are also being exposed to additional information about the other person and his or her attitudes. It follows from these two premises that attributional accuracy (e.g., a low degree of discrepancy between actual and perceived consensus) will in- crease as communication increases. The final two hypotheses concern the relationship of metaphoric similarity and the distance between actual and perceived consensus. Hypothesis #4: As the degree of similarity of members' perceptions of metaphors increases, the distance between their actual and per— ceived consenses decreases Hypothesis #5: The degree of similarity will increase more for active than for dormant meta- phors as distance decreases Assuming that similarity of active metaphors varies closely with amount of communication, then those that are frequently communicating with one another are exchanging and sharing the same active metaphors. They are not only focusing upon the same phenomena, they are also interpreting it in the same manner. Given these similar orientations, the accuracy with which they ascribe attitudes to one another will in- crease . 28 Conversely, those individuals who seldom communicate have different sets of peculiar experiences, and use dif— ferent active metaphors to describe them. They attribute attitudes to one another using more general information. They base these attributions upon stereotypes which may or may not be accurate when applied to specific individuals. So their perceptions of the other's attitudes, being grounded in more general level information, do not system— atically correspond with the actual degree of consensus. Organizational cliches (i.e., dormant metaphors) are shared by all organizational members and, because of their deep-seatedness within the organizational culture, they are relatively stable over time and across situations. There- fore, the sharing of a dormant metaphor does not differen- tiate individuals and groups from each other, and does not indicate that additional information is selectively avail- able to particular individuals and groups with which to improve attributional accuracy. Operationalizations To test the proposed model, the four variables need to be measured and their relationships analyzed. The fol- lowing operationalizations were used in this process. Operationalization of the first two variables (simi- larity of metaphors and degree of novelty) was accomplished in two stages. In the first stage, approximately ten per- cent of the organization was asked to describe the informa- tion flow within their organization in interviews. 29 The interviews were transcribed and the metaphors occurring in the interviewees' talk identified. In the second stage a set of questionnaire items were constructed which asked the respondents to indicate their perceptions of each metaphor identified in the interviews as either new and unique (active) or old and well—worn (dormant). (All members of the organization participated in the second stage.) This section of the questionnaire was presented in a matrix format, where the list of identi- fied metaphors appeared down the side and the categorical headings ('novel' and 'cliche', respectively) were placed across the top. The respondents were charged with indi- cating (1) whether each metaphor actually occurred in con- versations among housing staff, and if so, (2) if they per— ceived the term to be a creative description (active) or an old and much used description (dormant). The variable 'degree of metaphorical similarity' was then calculated by tabulating both the number of metaphors with a checkmark in the 'novel' column and the number of metaphors with a checkmark in the 'cliche' column. The variable 'degree of novelty' was computed by taking the dif- ference between the number of checkmarked dormant metaphors and the number of checkmarked active metaphors; the higher the value, the greater the degree of novelty (or activity). In both cases, the task was performed only for those pairs of respondents who had identified each other as being fellow communicators in the communication section of the questionnaire. 30 For Operationalization of the 'amount of communica- tion' variable, all members of the organization were asked to list the seven staff members with whom they talked the most.5 They were then asked to indicate the number of hours per month they communicated with each person on the topic in question (e.g., information flow). The number of com- munication hours specified served as the 'amount of com— munication' variable value. The fourth variable (discrepancy between actual and per- ceived consensus) was operationalized in the following manner: All employees were asked (1) to indicate their attitude toward the topic on a 7-point Likert scale, and (2) to predict the topical attitude then by each communi- catee listed on their communication roster (also on a 7- point Likert scale).6 Actual consensus was computed by subtracting a respondent's own attitude score from the attitude score indicated by that respondent's communicatee on her/his own survey. Perceived consensus was computed by subtracting a respondent's communicatee prediction score from that communicatee's prediction score for the respondent. Discrepancy was calculated by subtracting the 'actual' dif- ference from the 'perceived' difference. A visual demon- stration of this process is below:7 (other attitude score) — (respondent attitude score) Actual consensus 31 (other prediction score) — (respondent prediction score) Actual consensus (perceived consensus) — (actual consensus) Discrepancy "Degree of novelty" also appeared in the fifth hypothesis and was operationalized in the same manner as in the second hypothesis. (See Appendix A for an illustration of the questionnaire.) Sample Data were gathered from 36 full-time and part—time employees in the University Housing Department of a large Midwestern university. They were the staff of two dormi- tories; one large (27 employees) and one small (9 employees). Occupations covered included Resident Director, Graduate Advisor, and Resident Assistant. Procedure Two staff people (a Resident Director and a Resident Assistant) were interviewed from the smaller dormitory and three staff people (a Resident Director, a Graduate Advisor, and a Resident Assistant) were interviewed from the larger dormitory. These individuals were chosen by their superiors on the vasis of their volubility. The objective of the in- terviews was to elicit talk about the information flow with- in the organization. The metaphors of the interviewee oc- curring during this talk were noted and later incorporated into the survey questionnaire. 32 The questionnaire was composed of three main sections. The first section was the communication roster. The second section consisted of the attitude items. The final section included the list of metaphors and required their identifi— cation by respondents. A set of surveys were sent to the Resident Director of each hall who then distributed them to the Graduate Advisors and Resident Assistants at their weekly staff meeting. Col- lection of the completed surveys occurred the following week at the next staff meeting. The data coding process began with the assignment of identification codes to each respondent. Then, for each communicatee listed by the respondent, the number of similar active and dormant metaphors was calculated. The two atti- tude items and the communication item were in numerical form initially so no transformations of calculations were needed prior to data entry. The data were entered onto floppy diskettes with a Radio Shack Model IV microcomputer using a data entry soft- ware program developed by Jim Resh and Richard V. Farace (copyright 1983). Once all of the data for both halls were entered and their entry-accuracy verified, the files were uploaded to a mainframe computer. The statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences). CHAPTER IV RESULTS Content Analysis Although the actual metaphors generated in the inter- views were not directly used in testing the hypothesis, it is interesting to review them. A number of themes were apparent in the metaphorical language occurring in the in- terviews. (In the sense used here, 'themes' refer to areas of concern and/or attitudes toward information flow.) TABLE 1 Frequency of Responses Within Each Thematic Category Theme Novel Cliche Obtaining/Relaying 148 212 Way it Was Used 108 130 Structuring 85 73 Overload 55 90 Necessary Info. 50 80 Useless Info. 23 50 Underload 18 21 The most popular theme was Obtaining and/or Relaying Information. It was reflected by such terms as 'grapevine' 33 34 'shoot the bull', 'spoon-feeding', and 'get straight from the horse's mouth'. Note that many more of these terms were considered cliches than were considered novel. This may be due, in part, to the frequency with which this con- cern arises in people's talk. It is also interesting to note that the two Resident Directors' speech was where the majority of these types of metaphors were generated. Is the metaphor's popularity, then, a reflection of 'manage- ment's values and concerns or vice-versa? The next most popular theme was the Manner in Which Information was Used once obtained. This concern was ap— parent in terms like 'stay on track', 'evaluative tool', 'enforce rules', and 'down the road'. Here, too, there is a discrepancy between the frequency with which these terms were considered novel and the frequency with which they were considered cliche. Again, the amount of time spent com- municating this concern may be responsible for the greater number of cliches. The generation of these terms was fairly equally shared by all the interviewees. The three themes which followed next in preference are, respectively (1) the Way Information is Structured, (2) the Presence of Too Much Information (i.e., information over- load), and (3) the Necessity and Usefulness of Certain In- formation. Terms reflecting this first theme were 'see things as black and white', 'chain of command', and 'filtered information'. Interestingly, the number of terms considered novel and the number of terms considered cliche were similar. 35 This could be because certain sets of peOple are much more concerned with this aspect of information flow than other sets. The people for whom this theme was more of a con- cern would discuss it more which would account for greater integration of certain metaphorical items into their vo- cabulary (e.g., a high percentage of cliches). Those people for whom this theme wasn't a great concern may still have regular contact with their 'concerned' peers which is why they've heard the terms before, but don't consider them to be common terms. This reasoning is somewhat supported in that only one interviewed individual provided the majority of the metaphors appearing in the questionnaire. Metaphorical terms in which the fourth theme is ap- parent are as follows: 'keeping head above water', 'burn- out', and 'flooded'. Terms reflecting the fifth theme were 'practical how-to', 'know what's up', and 'what's the scoop'. The difference between the amount of novel and cliche re- sponses were similar to the amount in the second most popular theme, probably for much the same reason. The metaphors displaying the fourth theme were generated equally by all five interviewees, while metaphors reflecting the fifth theme were generated in a fairly concentrated manner by two of the interviewed individuals. The final two themes are somewhat similar in content. They are (1) Information Which is Useless, and (2) the Absence of Information (i.e., information underload). Ex- amples of metaphorical language displaying the former theme 36 are 'garbage mail', 'waste of time', and 'bible that no- body reads'. The discrepancy between the number of novel and cliche responses follows the pattern of the first, second, fourth and fifth themes. Just as there were rela- tively few responses to this theme's metaphors, so there were very few metaphors reflecting this theme appearing in the interviews. (Note that the one condition may have ex— cerbated the other' that the fewer metaphors appearing in the interviews, the lesser the opportunity to respond to this theme in the questionnaire.) The least popular theme was represented in such words as 'information gaps' and 'cut-off from things'. Here, too, there's little discre- pancy between the number of novel and cliche responses along with the generation of these metaphors by only a few indi— viduals. Overall, there were consistently more metaphorical terms checkmarked as cliche than as novel. Also, there was a propensity for some respondents to checkmark a very large proportion of the listed metaphors as occurring in their talk and for others to checkmark only a very few. The ques— tion then arises whether, given the questionnaire format used, the responses were actual representations of amount of metaphorical usage; whether individuals differ that markedly in their use of figurative language. Another in— teresting avenue of questioning is whether the popular meta— phors and themes are dictated by the upper levels of the or- ganization; whether they originate at the lower levels and spread up; or whether they spread horizontally. 37 Statistical Analyses Scatterplots were constructed for each relationship and it was apparent from these that all of the relation— ships shared the following qualities: (1) they are linear (which validates usage of regression techniques) and (2) there are several outliers (which depress the correlational values for each relationship). Alpha was set at p j .10 because of the exploratory nature of this study. The hypotheses were tested using re- gression techniques. This analytical method was chosen for two reasons: (1) Regression analysis is more appropriate for survey research where there are no manipulated variables, and (2) The variables are continuous rather than categorical and so are more compatible with the regression technique (Pedhazur, 1982). In the first hypothesis a positive relationship between similarity of metaphor sets and amount of communication was proposed. This was not supported in the data. The correla- tion between the two variables is -.08 (p=.24). When a par~ tial correlation is calculated, controlling for the influence of the interaction between amount of communication and degree of metaphorical novelty, the correlational value is -.31 (p=.003) for the hypothesized relationship. In other words, as the degree of similarity between members perceptions of metaphors increases, the amount of communication between members decreases to a statistically significant degree. The effect size of the similarity and communication 38 relationship, to be reported in terms of R2, is .09 (i.e., 9% of the variance in metaphorical similarity is explained by degree of communication). The confidence interval (set at 95% confidence level) around the correlational value is P (-.61 :_r:f.01). The second hypothesis is an interaction hypothesis; predicting that as communication among members increases the degree of active metaphorical similarity will increase at a faster pace than dormant metaphorical similarity. Ac- cording to this hypothesis, degree of metaphorical similarity is dictated by the interaction of two variables: amount of communication and degree of activity. As the amount of com- munication increases, the distance between the degree of active and dormant metaphorical similarity becomes more pro- nounced. This hypothesis' prediction of a positive relation- ship between degree of overall metaphorical similarity and the interaction variable (amount of communication by degree of activity) was not supported. The correlation between the two variables is -.42 (p=.001). This means that as communi- cation increased and the degree of metaphorical novelty in- creased, the degree of overall metaphorical similarity de- creased. The effect size of this relationship is R2=.17. The confidence interval around this value is P (-.69 itif.15). In the third hypothesized relationship, amount of com- munication is negatively related to the distance between actual and perceived consensus. The calculated correlation of .01 (p=.465) does not support this hypothesis. However, 39 when the two interaction variables are controlled for through partial correlation (the interaction between com— munication and activity variable, and the interaction be- tween discrepancy and activity variable), the hypothesized relationship is significant with a correlation of .32 (p= .002), although not in the hypothesized direction. So, as communication increases, the distance between members' actual and perceived consensus also increases. The effect size for the communication and discrepancy relationship is .10 and the confidence interval around this value is P (.02 :1“: .62). Hypothesis four predicts a negative relationship between metaphorical similarity and consensual discrepancy. This relationship is strongly supported in the data with a cor— relation of -.34 (p=.001) and an effect size of .11. Thus, as hypothesized, as the degree of metaphorical similarity increases the distance between members' actual and perceived consensus decreases. The confidence interval around the correlational value is P (-.63 :_r: —.05). The last hypothesis proposed the following relation— ship: as the degree of consensual discrepancy decreases the increase of active metaphorical similarity will be more dramatic than the increase of dormant metaphorical similarity. Initially, the data did not support this hypothesis. The correlation between overall metaphorical similarity and the interaction between discrepancy and activity was -.28, contrary to the positive relationship expected. However, 40 with degree of metaphorical activity controlled for, the partial correlation is .19 (p=.04). In other words, as degree of metaphorical similarity increases, the size of the interaction between consensual discrepancy and meta— phorical activity also increases. The effect size of this relationship is .04 and the confidence interval around the correlation is P (-.13 j_r: .51). It wasn't possible to correct for correlational atten— uation due to measurement error in any of the relational values because three of the variables (communication, active metaphor sets, and dormant metaphor sets) are measured with single items and the correction equation requires multiple item measurements. It also wasn't possible to calculate reliability scores for three of the operationalized variables, again because they were single item measurements. The reliability coef— ficient for the two-item attitude scale is .28. CHAPTER V DISCUSSION This study was conducted in an effort to uncover some factors influencing the way in which organizational members construct and act out organizational reality. It was pro- posed that as communication among members increases the similarity between the metaphors that they use in their com- munication also increases, particularly for active metaphors. Furthermore, as communication increases the discrepancy between how members perceive each others' attitudes and what those attitudes actually are diminishes. Finally, the magnitude of this discrepancy is also negatively influenced by the similarity of members' metaphor sets, especially active metaphor sets. Revising this model to fit the data, it appears that as communication among members increases their perceptions of metaphorical similarity decrease, particularly for dor- mant metaphors. Moreover, as communication among members increases the distance between actual and perceived atti- tudinal advocacy also increases. The degree of this dis— crepancy decreases as metaphorical similarity increases, especially for active metaphors. The modified model is pre— sented next. 41 42 Similarity Amount of I + \ Discrepancy _ ‘\ of metaphors .(—————fi> communication I — 4‘ Figure 2. Illustration of the Revised Model Based Upon the Data Collected Limitations There are a number of limitations in the manner in which this study was constructed and implemented which should be considered before the implications of the findings are dis- cussed. First and foremost is the relative lack of general- izability due to (l) non-random sampling (all staff members of each dormitory were asked to participate), (2) a small subject pool, and (3) non-representativeness of the subject pool (unlike typical organizational members, the respondents were all under 25, the majority were full-time students and worked part-time, and because of the nature of their job, they lived at their place of employment). Because of the small sample size, which reduces statistical power, the probability of a Type II error is increased (i.e., of not finding an effect when one actually exists). Another potential problem is that the people inter- viewed were selected by the two Resident Directors as being especially articulate and influential individuals. It would have strengthened the study if the interviewees had been randomly selected. It might have provided a more representative set of metaphors. 43 Additionally, there was some indication that the complexity of the questionnaire confused some respondents and prevented them from completing it accurately. Implications Amount of communication was expected to be a signifi- cant factor in the proposed theoretical model. This was the case, but not in the predicted relational directions; the expected positive relationship with metaphorical simi- larity was negative and the expected negative relationship with discrepancy was positive. The cause for this could be located in the methodology. For instance, the responses to the communication item were prompted by a request to specify the number of hours spent communicating on informa- tion flow per month. The definition given for information flow was considered by a number of respondents to be vague and ambiguous. The amount of room evidently left in the provided definition for individual interpretation could have influenced the hypotheses in which communication was a variable. Also influencing the communication variable value was the fact that it was measured with one item, and if that item was flawed in any way, the value obtained is inaccu- rate . However, due to the strength of the relationships of which communication is a part, the cause is probably theor— etical in nature. For instance, rather than there being an increase in metaphorical similarity as communication in- creases, it might be the case that the less individuals 44 speak to one another the greater reliance upon, and so attention to, the metaphor as an orientation device (e.g., a way of discovering another's topical position and/or establishing some common ground). This interpretation re- ceives support from the results of the second hypothesis; the less individuals communicate, the more dormant metaphorical similarity, rather than active metaphorical similarity, be- comes prevalent. In other words, not only is there greater reliance on the metaphor as a linguistic device for reducing uncertainty when speaking with someone infrequently communi- cated with, but the metaphors used are the cliched ones; the ones in which there's more of a chance for recognition and familiarity by one's communication partner. When speak- ing with someone communicated with frequently there is a greater pre-established base of similar knowledge and ex- perience. One does not need to fall back on, or even pay attention to, figurative language to communicate effectively. And new or unique metaphors, unless very powerful, either escape notice or are forgotten because of their relative infrequency. Thus, the reduction in importance and acknow- ledgement of metaphorical language. It is also possible that idiosyncratic events, rather than impelling members to create new metaphors, cause heavier reliance upon the old metaphors, or that by the time active metaphors are diffused throughout the group, they are no longer considered active by group members. 45 It was predicted that as communication between indi- viduals increased, the more accurate would be their pre~ dictions of each others' attitudes. According to the data, the opposite is true. The cause for this could be due to the methodology employed, as discussed earlier, but it is more likely due to the variables' relational conceptualiza- tion. For instance, it has been shown in previous research that similar individuals are more attracted to each other and so communicate more. If so, then it's possible that as people communicate more and more with those they consider attractive, they over-assume their similarity. They may focus on attitudes they share, generalizing to both the in- tensity of similarity and to the content of attitudinal simi- larity (i.e., assume that they have similar attitudes on other topics as well). If this is true then the respondents' prediction of each others' attitudes on information flow (a topic which may not be commonly discussed) may be more of a reflection of what they think the others' attitudes are due to their interpersonal attractiveness than an objective evaluation based on others' known characteristics. The relationship between attitudinal discrepancy and metaphorical similarity (relative to the amount of metaphorical activity) was as hypothesized. At this stage in the investigation into the types of language constructs occurring in peoples' communication (such as active and dormant metaphors), it may be more appropriate to adopt a research method which utilizes respondents 46 perceptions rather than providing the investigator with actual conversational transcripts and giving him/her the responsibility to distinguish between the types of language constructs appearing in peoples' talk. Future Research Further research into the influence of various language constructs upon reality construction is valuable and worth— while. The alternative variable relationships discussed in the previous section need to be further investigated. Do unfamiliar communicators rely upon figurative language to establish a common orientational base? Do familiar communi— cators ignore the figurative language that ideas are couched in and attend more to content? Do familiar communicators over-generalize attitudinal similarity? Comparative studies could include investigation into a number of different linguistic constructs to determine not only their relative influence but also how they interact, counteract, and are embedded within each other. For in- stance, one could devise a study about the influence of myths, stories, and metaphors upon perceptions of organizational culture where metaphors are abbreviated references to stories and stories are outward manifestations of myths. Another example is a study which investigates whether story-telling and metaphor-making are primarily individual traits, group traits, or organizational traits. There are strong relationships between the linguistic and communication behavior constructs apparent in this study. 47 Not only do these relationships need to be further explored, but so do relationships between other linguistic and com- munication behavior constructs. REFERENCES 48 NOTES 1Equivocality, in this context, refers to the number and complexity of the available interpretations of and responses to a phenomenon or an event. As the amount and complexity of potential responses increases, equivocality increases. th should be noted that other figures of speech are encompassed in this definition, specifically, simile and analogy. This is a deliberate and not unprecedented inclusion (see Morgan, 1983, pp. 602, 605). 3'Groups' is loosely defined as sets of individuals who communicate frequently with one another. 4'Management style' is an example topic. The actual topic used will be determined by its perceived importance to organizational members. 5Rogers and Kincaid (1981) found that a 7—person roster was the most effective length for a sociometric survey. It is short enough to prevent respondent fatigue, but long enough to adequately represent the respondent's personal network. 6The 'attitude' scale will consist of only two items: one item aimed at measuring the perceived importance of the topic, and the other item a general attitude measure. The scale is brief because each respondent will be using it re- peatedly (e.g., for every listed communicatee). The inclu- sion of a perceived salience item allows the researcher to statistically control for any confounding effects of per- ceived topical importance. 7For example, suppose Respondent A specified an atti— tude score of 5 for himself and predicted an attitude score of 6 for Respondent B. Respondent B, on the other hand, in- dicated an attitude score of 3 for herself and predicted an attitude score of 7 for Respondent A. The actual consensus 49 between Respondents' A and B is '5 - 3', or '2'. The per- ceived consensus between them is '6 - 7'. or '1' (the absolute value is used). The discrepancy variable value is '2 - 1'. or '1' (again, the absolute value is used). 50 BIBLIOGRAPHY Asch, S.E. "The Metaphor: A Psychological Inquiry." In Person, Perception and Interpersonal Behavior, pp. 86- 94. Edited by R. Taguiri and L. Petrullo. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 1958. Berger, P. and Luckmann, T. The Social Construction of Reality. Garden City, NY: Doubleday and Co., Inc., I966. Berggren, D. "The Use and Abuse of Metaphor." Review of Metaphysics 16 (1962): 237-257. Billow, R.M. "Metaphor: A Review of the Psychological Literature." Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 84, No. l (1977): 81—92. Black, M. Models and Metaphors. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 1962. Black, M. "More About Metaphor." In Metaphor and Thought, pp. 26-37. Edited by A. Ortony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Bormann, E.G. "Symbolic Convergence." In Communication and Organizations: An Interpretive Approach. Edited by L.L. Putnam and M.E. PacanowSki. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, 1983. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd Edition. New York: Academic Press, 1977. Conrad, C. "Organizational Power: Faces and Symbolic Forms." In Communication and Organizations" An Interpretive Approach, pp. 173-194. Edited by L.L. Putnam and M.E. Pacanowsky. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, 1983. Cooney, J.E. "'Marketing Warfare' Seeks Military Link for Sales Campaigns." Wall Street Journal (April 28, 1978): 25. Deetz, S.A. "Critical Interpretive Research in Organizational Communication." The Western Journal of Speech Communica- tion 46 (Spring, 1982): l3l~l49. 51 Deutsch, M. and Gerard, H.B. "A Study of Normative and In— formational Social Influences Upon Individual Judgement." Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 51 (1955): 629-636. Eisenberg, E.M. "Ambiguity as a Strategy in Organizational Communication." 'Communication Monographs, Vol. 51, No. 3 (September, 1984): 277-290. Eisenberg, E.M. "Strategic Ambiguity in Organizations." Paper presented at the May 1983 Annual Meeting of the International Communication Association (Dallas, Texas). Evered, R. "The Language of Organizations." In Organiza- tional Symbolism. Edited by L.R. Pondy, P.J. Frost, G. Morgan, and T.C. Dandridge. Greenwich, Connecticut: JAI Press, Inc., 1983. Farace, R.V., P.R. Monge, and E.M. Russell. Communicating in Organizing. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, 1977. Fernandez, J.W. "Persuasions and Performances: Of the Beast In Every Body . . . And the Metaphor of Everyman." (1972): 39-60. Festinger, L. "A Theory of Social Comparison Processes." Human Relations 7 (1954): 114-140. Fraser, B. "Interpretation of Novel Metaphors." In Metaphor and Thought, pp. 173-187. Edited by A. Ortony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Frost, P.J. and G. Morgan. "Symbols and Sensemaking: The Real-ization of a Framework." In Organizational Symbolism. Edited by L.R. Pondy, P.J. Frost, G. Morgan, and T.C. Dandridge. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc., 1983. Gardner, H. and E. Winner. "The Development of Metaphoric Competence: Implications for Humanistic Disciplines." In On Metaphor, pp. 121-140. Edited by S. Sacks. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1979. Glucksberg, S., P. Gildea, and H.B. Bookin. "On Under- standing Nonliteral Speech: Can People Ignore Metaphors?" Journal of Verbal Learningpand Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 85-980 Jacobson, E. and S.E. Seashore. "Communication Practices in Complex Organizations." Human Relations Research in Large Organizations #3 (1951). 52 Krefting, L. and P.J. Frost. "Unravelling Webs, Surfing Waves, Wildcatting: A Multiple Metaphor Perspective on Managing Organizational Culture." (In press) Lakoff, G. and M. Johnson. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Pfess, 1980. Levin, S.R. "Standard Approaches to Metaphor and a Proposal for Literary Metaphor." In Metaphor and Thought, pp. 129-135. Edited by A. Ortony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Morgan, G. "More on Metaphor: Why We Cannot Control Tropes in Administrative Science." Administrative Science Quarterly 28 (1983): 601-607. Newcomb, T.M. "An Approach to the Study of Communicative Acts." Psychological Review, Vol. 60, No. 6 (1953): 393-399. Ortony, A. "Why Metaphors are Necessary and Not Just Nice." Educational Theory, Vol. 25, No. l (1975). Pacanowsky, M.E. and N. O'Donnell-Trujillo. "Organizational Communication as Cultural Performance." Communication Monographs, Vol. 50 (June, 1983): 126-147. Paivio, A. "Psychological Processes in Comprehension." In Metaphor and Thopght, pp. 151-157. Edited by A. Ortony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Pedhazur, E.J. Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research. 2nd Edition. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1982. Pondy, L.R. "The Role of Metaphors and Myths in Organization and in the Facilitation of Change." In Organizational Symbolism, pp. 157-170. Edited by L.R. Pondy, P.J. Frost, G. Morgan, and T.C. Dandridge. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc., 1983. Richards, I.A. The Philosophy of Rhetoric. London: Oxford University Press, 1936. Rogers, E.M. and D.L. Kincaid. Communication Networks. New York: The Free Press, 1981. Salancik, G.R., and J. Pfeffer. "A Social Information Pro- cessing Approach to Job Attitudes and Task Design." Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 23 (June, 1978): 225-246. 53 Scheff, T.J. "Toward a Sociological Model of Consensus." 'American Sociological Review, Vol. 32 (1967): 33-46. Schon, D.A. "Generative Metaphor and Social Policy." In Metaphor and Thought, pp. 254—270. Edited by A. Ortony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Searle, J.R. "Metaphpr." In Metaphor and Thought, pp. 93- 128. Edited by A. Ortony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1979. Snyder, M., E. Berscheid, and E. Decker-Tanke. "Social Per- ception and Interpersonal Behavior: On the Self-Fulfil- ling Nature of Social Stereotypes." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 35, No. 9 (1977): 656—666. Steinfatt, T.M., G.R. Miller, and E.P. Bettinghaus, "The Concept of Logical Ambiguity and Judgements of Syllo- gistic Validity." Speech Monographs, Vol. 41 (November, 1974): 317-328. Webster's New Collegiate Dictionary. Springfield, MA: G & C Merriam Co., 1979. Weick, K.E. The Social Psychology of Organizing. 2nd Edition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley PubliShing Co., 1979. White, S.E. and T.R. Mitchell. "Job Enrichment Versus Social Cues: A Comparison and Competitive Test." Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 64, No. l (1979): 1-9. Wilson, G.B. "Purposeful Ambiguity as a Persuasive Message Strategy." Ph.D. Thesis, Michigan State University, 1971. APPENDIX 54 APPENDIX A Example of the Survey Questionnaire Completed by Respondents In the following pages you will be questioned on the topic of the information flow system as it exists among the hall staff and University Housing administrators. As used in this survey, information flow refers to (1) where a piece of knowledge originates, (2) how it is passed along (i.e., speed of transmission, accuracy of relayed information, mode of transfer, telephone, face—to-face, staff meetings, etc.), and (3) how it is used or implemented. Below is a set of directions to follow in completing the survey: I. Communication Section A. Write your name on line 1 under the first column. On the same line, under the second column, indicate the approximate total num— ber of hours you spend talking about infor- mation flow (per month). B. Then, on lines a - g, list the seven indi- viduals you speak with most frequently, and the amount of time you spend per month talking with each of them on information flow. II. Attitude Section A. On line 1, indicate your own responses to the two statements (placed at the top of the two columns) using the coding scheme provided. 55 B. Then, on lines a - 9, indicate your pre- diction of the listed individuals responses to the same two statements. III. Descriptive Language Section A. The instructions for this section are on the first page of the section. 56 III. Below are a list of terms used to describe the information flow among the staff of the University Housing Program. Please indicate (by a checkmark in one of the two columns) the descriptive terms that you are familiar with (i.e., that either you use when you talk about information flow or that others use when they talk about aspects of the information flow system). The column with the heading "Novel" refers to the uniqueness or inventiveness of the terms. Place a checkmark in this column (1) if the term(s) on that line actually occur in con- versations among staff, and (2) if you think the term(s) are creative descriptions. The column with the heading "Cliche" refers to the triteness of the term(s) on that line (i.e., they are well-worn). Place a checkmark in this column (1) if the term(s) actually occur in conversations among staff, and (2) if you think the term(s) are 91g and much used descriptions. If the term(s) on a line don't occur in conversations about the information flow in the University Housing Program, don't place a checkmark in either column. NOTE: These terms are all taken out of context so if you aren't familiar with them they may not make sense. If this is the case, simply skip that term and go on to the next one. TERM(S) REFERRING TO THE INFORMATION FLOW NOVEL CLICHE l. garbage mail 2. grapgvine 3. up to speed 4. evolution of procedures 5. fluke 6. best thing since sliced bread 7. gaps 8. pick up on information 9. formal information gathering/‘ dissemination 57 TERM(S) REFERRING TO THE INFORMATION FLOW NOVEL' CLICHE 10. informal information gathering/ dissemination ‘ ' “ 11. one-on-one's 12. keeping your head above water 13. cut-off from things 14. under public scrutiny 15. boxes everything up 16. important to pass on 17. see things as black and white 18. shoot the bull 19. break things down to their common denominator 20. stay on track 21. sticking by closely 22. evaluative tool 23. bible that nobody reads 24. bottom line 25. slip by/slipped my mind 26. going behind peoples' backs 27. off the wall 28. chain of command 29. agenda 30. gripes 31. spoon feeding 32. get straight from the horse's mouth‘ 33.‘ liaison 58 TERM(S) REFERRING TO THE INFORMATION FLOW CLICHE NOVEL 34. the big picture 35.‘ cost ofmistakeS'd 36. telephone tree 37. waste of time 38. running hall like a business 39. practical how—to 40. know what's up 41. trial and error 42. carried out 43. screwed up 44. what the scoop is 45. in a bind 46. keeping up 47. bounding peOple 48. down the road 49. upeto-date 50. what's going on 51. burn-out 52. enforce rules/procedures 53. word of mouth 54. information spreading 55. junk mail 56. step over the edge 57. ramming down peoples' throats 58. filtered information 59 TERM(S) REFERRING TO THE INFORMATION FLOW NOVEL CLICHE 59. flooded 60. breakdown 61. channeling information HH cos.w woos mdnosoH< venom yahoo u UHmHsanomnmm\wsoHBUH