:v F. 3 : whu L ma. .w 5,7,. .1? If”. . art; 1‘1. ‘ fer. ;7_.b‘xw:l. . A . v; , Ew.n,u...~..lzii.32. ' ax. VItffi)-.ll 2:1: . xfltt1 x »31 a 6v Iv O4. ‘u:2.t1?..1y7 , zrrur ; ¢.r‘w .11.. .é; 5 .6 .353 ' 33.5: :31 fl? ' § c ‘ - 3:32 v.10?! ‘ 1 , Q 9 s51 L: «2% fir 5: 1g: "t’ ‘ . :3. 3‘ I V? .'3 :1 y . ”=1“ l,: 5,: :ie 3‘ ' ‘ “t- . ,7 . "N ~ n’fihh‘a'fih he. 9,1,!J, , v v 71 “r3“; .1,1.x,v , '2‘. ’v' I.’ - r! 5 '7’ '73! z‘u‘: “‘7’ ‘0‘ ' c595?” » ‘ f ., - 139% ‘ , , . i v. w r15, #1:. 7 Traps V y“? . ‘ ' ‘ F V. I i If‘tl;1"1q. 9"4'3 , 1 gy,: .p 0?: ' u 2 17.7' ‘R a ,1 '7 :3 . _ 2:? , s . xw‘mvfimfim :«u wwquherhrmmnfiwr twili 7a.... afi 31.. ‘w '7 9s 37 w? r: «3’. 32.321111 "$1 _ , . 4d: .1 fir». $5 . «wave 3.} _ 4%.“; 2;»... :1, a.» am: a v la- “9:: - ' w‘flé L‘L'LI, rs +§LU r. ’3 4‘ fig m5 3-... n ;1' 'ugagagj‘s L2: II t L 1 b . . agitaaa‘ 4 q . 313 3;"; S .- _‘. ;, 3 W11 (-2 ,. . . wag...‘ . :.?..2..:; {5:2 :53. a: :11.- 7: .a v u r." a; 1:“ “z: 2 ‘ :c: NJ 3 . .75... u; q in y a. 1: 3 .31.?! .551 {if fitmi“ffil‘_flaif ,WIA‘? . 1‘15? ”damn? Ruhr... ... Eza 1.. i}? :g.:.a - 915.)? v; R . ti ,. ‘. “ 7”, LIN-g: w“?’= y 51,: v i g . t... .3113}: 5. 177193.. ‘ than 4.... v I 11:» . I , L .1..1.\..v..r. muslin. . w» y 1 r :5: ‘v 32 s . . . 1;? ,(,l., I~37¢ HRH: “I"; 3.: an 33.3? t7} ..u.:.é_:.n.1.;h .vizf . svaaxv4ni1ni.rel-13:12.5...1. 31.53.; .21 a“: flirt-lira _ w.v\..:,\an:§>¢ :0: .v‘ 7.1.4.2... >yalv'vv’1wiayxa :2 7'2): rrvaiz... 3151;133:717 l>13>1afi§w3 3,. ..zzcatfl..!...1u¢'cir:_;v7a‘15; .. :21} £12.. .1, 3.: V._. :1). : 3.17.353.3.5.3.1117 .\ r a: at . i,“ :1): 22.1: I: ,4. $235 .ry>v¢.... pik'? 2-x . ‘1 L 1 V « Hz .1: 41.3... 31:2? ‘13. .y’:in...1pé;ar;..:11 .r $.11 4 . I b 4.91.? . «iv: w i? .k s 30.....vézu $5? . , 3 :fiiizisi r .Hafi..t.,.!w r. (yin-L ”3‘ 5.9,.2}... 3.22.1.3: .3: .m ‘ 2‘5. ‘1 l2 s ‘5 r”. {I ‘ E. A #9 A? 3*“ v" A; ‘ 3: a‘i‘g‘a' Evy-é E ‘l , 3.7 fi . I L! {'3 A... E A 3391 szxc. .'. a 2%.: ~ ”Wt . 1;. ‘- a: as :; 4’ A: n. {35.4 Ft». 3.." , I. K'é '1". L 369% an .‘5‘ |§€ :, A ”a"; 1: .3 .xabnefigianl attalhl. 15.: 5.215.! at Lotnfilxbik! l 4.1:... c...>)at.)«|s. 51%))...3 nuittf-hax. Fin. .Atiiltiit 4 ll“llllllllllllllll » LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the ‘ l thesis entitled BROKEN PLURALS IN MODERN IRAQI ARABIC \ presented by Theresa McLaughlin Al—Azzawi has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in Linguistics Maijfo “39 Datew 0-7 639 MK“; I 04'8‘73 0 A 124 1 llOKEl l‘ne prl lemma my p11 unever ‘ file. l ABSTRACT BROKEN PLURALS IN MODERN IRAQI ARABIC By Theresa McLaughlin Al-Azzawi The problem of broken plurals (i.e.. internal or interdigitated plurals, as opposed to the so~ca11ed 'sound' plurals. realized as inflectional suffixes) has never previously been solved satisfactorily for Arabic. Mary M. Levy (The Plural of the Noun in Modern Standard Aggbic, Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Univer- sity of Michigan. 1971). while predicting the correct Plural(s) for any given singular item via phonological rules and devices to handle exceptions, does not offer explanation for the peculiarities of the system (e.g., multiple plural association). In this work'the hypothesis posited by Talmy Givén ("Some Historical Changes in the Noun Class System or Bantu: Their Possible Causes and Wider Implications” in W, Chun Wu Kim and Herbert Stahlke, eds., Urbana, 1971) to account for the noun class system of Bantu is used as a basis for the research into the apparently chaotic pluralization system of Arabic. The study is based on one subdialect of Arabic, namely Moslem Baghdadi Iraqi, which is a Bedouin dialect. ill: at minted 1 author of fall on malaria in, non- :a-to-one Plural 015 ill! of 1 morale] lie the tampon Elle 'g llie W— 7 Theresa McLaughlin Al-Azzawi This study finds that classes of Arabic nouns are associated with particular broken plurals on two bases. A number of plural classes are associated with broken plurals on the basis of culturally perceived semantic characteristics. This phenomenon reflects an older n-ary. non-anthropocentric. non-hierarchical system of one-to-one semanto-morphological correspondence. A few plural classes. are associated with broken plurals; on the basis of phonological shape, or canonical form. This phenomenon reflects a partial rearrangement of the system since the time of the hypothesized change to a binary, anthropocentric, hierarchical structure. which gave rise to the 'sound' grammatical masculine/feminine dichotomy. The theory on which the study is based is that of stratificational grammar. One of the requirements of the theory. that ”units" on one level (or stratum) are re- lated only to “units" on immediately contiguous level(s) is found to be too stringent. A less rigid model is adopted in order to account for the direct relationships which seem to exist between the gnostemic and the morphemic strata of Iraqi Arabic. BROKEN PLURALS IN MODERN IRAQI ARABIC By Theresa McLaughlin Al-Azzawi A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in.partia1 fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Linguistics and Oriental and African Languages 1973 Committee Members: Dr. David G. Lockwood. Chairman Dr. Ruth M. Brend Dr. Julia S. Falk Dr. Ernest N. McCarus Dr. Irvine Richardson Copyright by THERESA MCLAUGHLIN AL—AZZAWI 1973 llth uh: [ht DEDICATED especially to my husband ASIM HASHIM AL-AZZAHI without whose continual encouragement and mature understanding I would never have accomplished this end and to JULIA SABLESKI FALK who unknowingly caused me to grow in maturity and understanding and to WILLARD AND MARGUERITE BEECHEB whose book, Beyond Success and Failure, inspired me to accept academia for what it is and myself for what I am and to the chairman of my dissertation committee DAVID GEORGE LOCKWOOD a learned teacher and a friend and ultimately to my mother and father KELEM MAKER AND ROBERT DORSEY MOLAUGHLIN 11 So oft in theologic wars. The disputants. I ween, Rail on in utter ignorance or what each other mean And prate about an Elephant Not one of them has seen: from the poem The Blind Men and the Elephant by John Godfrey Saxe 111 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to thank the members of my thesis committee Dr. Ruth Brend. Dr. Julia Falk. Dr. David Lockwood. and Dr. Irvine Richardson of Michigan State University and Dr. Ernest N. McCarus of the Department of Near Eastern Languages and Literature of the University of Michigan for their spirit of cOOperation in the prompt return of comments and sug— gestions. iv :st of lab? ll of Fig ‘1:th Syi' his: I. llll TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables . List of Figures 0 e e e e e O O ' ist of Symbols and Abbreviations Chapter I. INTRODUCTION 1.1 The Topic 1.2 Previous Scholarship . e 1-3 Modern Iraqi Arabic . . 1.“ Data 0 e e o o e c o e 0 II. PRELIMINARIES . 2-1 Thioietical Approach to the Topic 0 0 Background . e e 2.1.2 Axioms and Primitives . 2-1.3 Types of Relationships 2.1.4 The Simplicity Measure 2.1.5 Major Goal 0 e e 2.2 Semology . . 2.2.2 2.2.1 The Gnostemic and Sememic Componential Analysis . . 2020201 Definition 0 e 0 2.2.2.2 Universal Status 2.2.2.3 Related Phenomena 2.2.2.h Cognitive Validity Markedness . . . 2.2.2 O O C . 2.2. The Gnostotactics . . . e . . 202.5 .0... Strata The Semotactics . . . . e - . 2-3 Strata Intervening Between the Phonemic . . . . . . o . 2.3.1 Lexemic . O 20302 Morphemic e e a e e 2.3.3 The Relationship of System to the Morphemic 2.3.h The Relationship Of the the Sememic Gnostemic System .000.- Morphemic System to the Phonemic System . . tic 2.u Ph°n°108¥ e o e e e e e e O 2.h.1 The Phonemic and Phone 2.h.2 Componential Analysis . 2.“.3 Markedness . . . o . . 2.“.h The Phonotactics . . - 2.u.5 The Phonetic Tactics . V Strata xiii xix \OCDOHd‘ H A GENERAL DESCRIPTION OF IRAQI BROKEN PLURALS 301 G3 302 Th neral Remarks -- Language in Flux . o ' e Old Semantic Classification 3.2.1 Semantic Taxonomy (Arransed by Phonological Shape) 3.2.1.1 3.2.1.2 3.2.1.3 3.2.1.14“ 3.2.1.5 3.2.1.6 3.2.1.7 3.2.1.8 3.2.1.9 vi FMaaL Plural Class 3.2.1.101 FVVL o 3.2.1.1.2 FvML(a) 1.3 FvaL . O O O O C O 0201. 02.1.1.“ FVMVVL MaaLa Plural Class . . . 02. 02.1 FVVL c o o o I 02. 02.2 FVML(&) o o o a 020 02.3 FVLV o o o o O 020 92.4 FVMVL . o o o I 3.1 FVMLV o o o a 3.2 FVVMLa . . . 3.3 Fvai(yya) . MaaMiL/FuMaaMiL Plural i Plural Class . 2.1.u.1 F(i)hvaa . . . 2.1.4.2 FUMMVL tot-4P SD co m M knuJo*d \nanoEg unaxau>w on» HHHh HHHH iMaaMiiL Plural Class . . 3.2.1.5.1 FvMMaaL(a) Round 0 o o o o 3.2.1.5.2 FvMMaaL(a) Male FuMaaMiiL Plural Class . . 3.2.1.601 FVMMVVL Round 0 3.2.1. .2 FvMMva Square 1 3 (“POBJNHVFQAJNTOAJg Hz HHHHHHHHHHz Plural Class . . . l FWWWMa)Rmmd 2 FvavL(a) Round 3 FMva(a) Round h FvaLa Round . 5 FvaML(a) Round 6 FvavL(a) Round 7 FvMMvL(a) Square 8 FvavL(a) Square 9 FMva(a) Square .10 FvavL(a) I o o o. o so 0 o no .0 o oo o. o o o o. o o. o o. b ‘TQ‘Q‘TQ‘Q\TQ‘QN3h O\ o o. 0 on o 0. Square u Plural C1883 o o o 0801 FUMLV(V)L o o o a)? 9 Plural Class . . . . 1.9.1 F(v)Maaya . . - h3W M)M MMJ\»UMJ\JHHJ\DM3w 3.2.1.10 3.2.1.11 3.2.1.12 30201.13 30201.1“ 30201015 30201016 3.2.1.17 30201018 3.2.1.19 302.1020 3.201021 vii FiMi Plural Class . . . . . 3.2.1.10.l FaML(aaya) FaMLaat Plural Class FuMuuL Plural Class . . . FvaL Strong 3.2.1.12.1 iMaaL Plural Class 1.1h.1 2H1 1h. 2 FvaL Bulk FvML Bulk FvML Strong . . 0 FvaL Abstract FvML Abstract . F: H '21 E. <3. E...- Arm 4 a you .0. FVMVL o FVMIIVOOO FiMaaLa Plural Class FaMaaMuL Plural Class FaMaaMiL Plural Class 3. 2 .1.17. 1 M17 2 .2 1.1 uuwuwm. NNNN O O O O O HHHH C HHHH vvvvq \10\U\1='Cn 3.2.1.17.8 3.2.1.17.9 ?aFMiLaa? Plural Class FBMi o o o 3.2.1.18.1 3.2.1.18.2 FvMMvL(a) FvavL(a) FanvL(a) FvMMvL(a) FvavL(a) FanvL(a) FvMMvL(a) Instrument FvavL(a) O 0 0 Strong Strong Strong Place Place Place Instrument . . FanvL(a) Instrument F3M11L o FiMMaL Plural Class . FuMMaL Plural Class 3.2.1.20.1 3.2.1.20.2 i i F M L 3.2 1.21.1 3. 2 1. 21. 2 3. 2 1. FvMiiL F88M1L o o Plural Class . FMva Weak ?aFMaL Weak . 21. 3 ?aFMaL Color 79 80 80 83 83 83 8h 8h 8h 85 85 . 85 86 86 86 86 87 87 30201022 30201023 3.2.1.2“ 3.201025 3.201026 3.2.1.27 30201028 30201.29 30201.30 30201.31 V111 FuMuL Plural Class . . . 3. 2. 1. 22. 1 ?aFMaL Weak 2.1.22.2 ?aFMaL Color Plural Class . . Fva Weak . Fva(v) Weak FvMLv Weak . FVMva Weak Fva Close . Fva(v) Close FvavL Close 1MLaa O O. NNNNNNN: O HHHHHHH NHUDONHVDOA> no a on no 'QCBU1CNQRJF' 3- F 3. 3 3 3 3 3 3 Fa 'U Ufidkdhnflkflh) 5-; La lural Class . . 3. 2. 1. 24. 1 FaaM1L . . . FaM11L Plural Class . . FMaaMLa Plural Class 0 o FMUUL Plural Class 0 o 0 3020102701 FVMVL BOdy Part 0 o o 0 3020102702 FVML(V) Body Part 0 o o o 302 1 2703 FVMVL craCks 3 ? 1 27 u FvML(v) Cracks 0 o o 3.2.1.27.5 FvaL Death 3 2 1.27 6 FvML(v) Death 3o2.1.27.7 FVMVL Elo ated . 3020102708 FVML V) Elongated . 3.2.1.27o9 FVMVL Plant FMuuLa Plural Class . 3.2.1.2801 FVMVL o 0 3020102802 FVML o o 0 FvMLaan Plural Class 3. 2. 1. 29. 1 FvavL . 3. 2. 1. 29. 2 Fva(v)L . . FVIMaaLaat Plural Class 3.2.1.30.1 FvaL Paired Body Part . 3020103002 FVMLB Plant FMaaLaat Plural Class . 96 99 99 99 30201.32 3.2.1.33 30201.34 30201.35 3.2.1.36 30201.37 3.2.1.38 3.2.1.39 3'2010u0 3.2 01 on]. 3.2.1.142 ix 99 ?aFMaaL Plural Class . . 3020103201 FVMVL ' ' . 100 3.2.1.32.2 FvML . . . . 101 3020103203 FVMV o o 0 ' 102 302010 32.“ FVVL o o O 0 102 3.2.1. 32.5 FvavL . . . 102 FaMaaLL Plural Class . . 102 FaMaLaat Plural Class . 102 F1MMaaL Plural Class . . 103 3020103501 FaaMlL o o o 103 FuMMaaL Plural Class . . 103 3020103601 F88M1L o O O 10“ FuMaLaa? Plural Class . 104 3020103701 F8M11L Agent 105 3920103702 FBM11L StronglOS 3020103703 F3M11L Weak 106 FuMaat Plural Class . . 106 3020103801 FaaMi o o o 107 FaMaaMiLa Plural Class . 107 3.2.1.39.l FvMMva(v) Agent 0 o o 107 3020103902 FVMMVLV Agent 0 o o 10? 3020103903 FVMMVVL(V) Strong . . . 107 3.2.1.39.“ FvMMva Strong . . . 108 FaMaaLi Plural Class . . 108 BozolouOol FVML(V) Place 0 o o 109 3020104002 FVML(V) Time 109 302010u003 FVMVL Time 0 109 3.2.1.h0.4 FvML(v) Weak 109 ?aFM1LarV?aF1MLa Plural Class 0 o o o o o o 110 Bogolohlol F(V)MVVL A Cover 0 o o 110 3.2.1.u1.2 F(v)Mva Statement . 110 3.2.1.hl.3 F(V)MVVL Instrument . 110 Questionable Plural Class- 88 o O O O O O O O O O 11 1. MW 3. h. 2. 1 Multiple Plural Association . . The Change in Semantic Classification The Newer Model -- A Conflation . . 3. h. l Diachronic Hypothesis . 3. h. 2 Synchronic Evidence . . 3.0.2.2 Borrowed Items . . . . “Ll A.Classification of the Data . . . . 17.2 The GnOBtOtactics o o o o o o o o 0 “0:3 The “orphOtacticB o o o o o o o h. 3.1 Semantically-based Plurals . 4. 3.1.1 0.3.1.2 “030103 0.3.1.0 usBoloS “030106 “030107 ATHEOBETICAL DESCRIPTION . . . o . . . . . . Round Plurals o o 0. 3.1.1.1 FMaaL-type 4. 3.1.1.2 FvMaaM1(i)L- type 0 o o 0 ”03010103 FVML-type o 0 Square Plurals . . . . u.3. 1. 2.1 FvMaaMi(i)L- “03010202 FMaleln-type n.3.1.2.3 FiMaL-type . Paired Body Part Plurals M 3.1.3.1 FvMaaLaat-type Abstract Plurals . . . . M 3.1.M 1 FaMaaLL-type h.3. .2 FaMaLaat-type .3 ?aFMaaL-type .lt FVMuuL-type . a? u) Lu4;4: O O O o Mnflkflp \0 o 818 o o . o o o FaMaLa-type . FvML-type . . FquLaa7-type FaMaaLi-type FVMMaL-type . FMaaMLa-type FaMiiL-type o .E o oo 01- o 01.0 o o HHHHHHHH o oo o co 0pseudo-phonemes P// Pi; pseudo-morphemes R PHONEMES // Figure 5 Hockett's Model of the Relationship of Morphemes to Phonemes The pseudo-morphemes may be rte-termed allomorphs and the PSEUdo-phonemes morphophonemes. The relationships are those of representation (R) and coupesition (C). Althousfih 1’0 is clear that a level exists between classical morphemes and classical phonemes, Hockett gave this level no linguis- tic status. One familiar with stratificational theory can easily see the development from this to the morphemic level in the stratificational framework. "Units" the size of the traditional morpheme were re-termed lexons (i.e.. the components of lexemes). "Units" the size of traditional alllo‘nm‘Phs were re-termed morphemes (1.13.. the t3°t1°al units of the morphemic stratum). and "units" the size of the morPhOphoneme were re-termed morphons (i.e.. the components of morphemes). The phoneme became the stratifica- tion“- phoneme which was the same size level as the traditional hen but L: L bhni mar are tactics L‘ Lama} sy LL htrod ih'phEheB. level hen Ehhehe, hit hm: than l7 phoneme but at a higher level of abstraction (i.e.. it was not a biunique level). The relationships in stratificational grammar are those of realization and composition. Since a tactics defines a stratal system, or, in other words. a stratal system cannot exist without a tactics, a tactics was introduced to generate the well-formed combinations of morphemes. The stratum so defined formed an intervening level between the classical morpheme and the classical Phoneme, thus solving the anomaly emerging from Bloomfield that A morpheme is composed of phonemes. and at the same time. A morpheme has allomorphs. The stratificational restatement of Hockett's model is Riven below . FM 18 ‘ MM HOCKEIT ‘3 TERMS LEKONS MORiTH EME‘S E. \ y 4-- Mer‘vkemes Pseuio‘ — mo emes e A90 c 5635/ Pseu (1.0" PM “W5- 50 MORPHONS R, - Fiuaems PHON Ems-s ‘WKJII; PURRES. Figure 6‘_ A Stratificational Restatement of Hockett's Model This was only the beginning, however. Since Hockett's article appeared, linguists working within the stratifica- tional framework have found uple evidence to warrant ‘the hypothesis of still further levels of structure, each level being structured in a similar manner. The present levels Posited for language are listed below. 19 Gnostemic Sememic Lexemic . GRAMMAR LANGUAGE Morphemic L Phonemi c PHONOLOGY Phonetic Figure 7 The Present Stratificational Model of Language This shows a neat division between grammar and. phonology butcnm_must not forget that the system is an interwoven whole. The gnostemic and phonetic strata are presently thought to be, if not outside language entirely, at 133315 at the periphery . 2.1.2 Axioms and Primitives There are certain.primitives out of which a stratifica- tional description is constructed and certain axioms upon Which the theory is based. The first of these axioms is A.1 All human knowledge and thus all human activities including language may be described by means Of a few logical relationships. It is the ability to structure his universe by means of these relationships that is innate in man. ehjehs? Line to r in h and eoch hoe these 10 hi “ 20 As Hjelmslev (19611 p.127) has stated: Linguistic theory is led by an inner necessity to recognize not merely the linguistic system, in its schema and in its usage, in its totality and in its individuality, but also man and human society behind language, and all man's sphere of knowledge through language. These logical relationships are two of the primitives of the linguistic system. ‘ They are "and" and "or." The second axiom is A.2 Language, as a mode of communication, functions in two directions. During encoding it functions in the direction from meaning toward sound. During decoding it functions in the opposite direction (i.e.. from sound to meaning). Therefore, a theory should not impose a partiality, either intended or 1mplied, for either direction. Stratificational grammar accepts this axiom by its introduc- tion of the two primitives of directionality "upward" and. "dQWnWard." The third axiom is A03 Ordering is often crucial within the linguistic System. There are many cases of crucial syntagmatic ordering. In En'Ehlish the subject-verb-object word order is a good GXamp1e, ' Stratificational grammar accepts this third he by 'hordere LL. the he nee] hence L h’ee pre in hotel 1 While the 1m threet h. hhlc h“ Leer; Ll hen 21 axiom by introducing 1) the primitives "ordered” and I ”unordered” and 2) the notion of stratal precedence, i.e., the notion that the lower stratal tactics (the ones nearer sound) take precedence over the higher stratal tactics during encoding and the higher stratal tactics take precedence over the lower during decoding. An additional type of ordering extant in stratifica- z. tional theory, termed tactical ordering , characterizes variable ordering, not dependent on any ordering within the linguistic system. This type of ordering rather characterizes the realization of elements in the order in which they occur, e.g., the order of the realization of morphons composing any given morpheme. Lamb (1972: pp.675-6) gives the example from Monachi: Gloss: to haul water bucket a) Morphonic: pa noo 78' na P8 noo 'nu hE Phonemi c : panoo 2a ' napanoo ' noho Gloss: gold specifically gold ‘0) Morphonic: ?oono ?E ?oono 7E 'su Phonemic: ?oono?o 70011070 ' 8° Given the following two realization formulae: R1 E/VlK ] V1 32 u/oK J 0 Where K =2 any consonant or consonant cluster. "9 can account for the apparent need for reversed ordering ‘70 handle examples Far LL and b) h emp‘ hLthch hreelie h exam} Lie reef 2.1.3 h The hLel he hated L heneee :heee ' [of one )TPBSL 5’1! or -‘-—| ”PM " fl - 22 a) and b) above with stratificational tactical ordering. For example, in a) the morphons are realized in the order in which they occur in the morpheme. In 'nu hE, first 3; is realized as p, then 3; is realized as p yielding 29113. In example b) (?E 'su) first I}; is realized as p, then 2 is realized as p yielding ?o'so. 2.1.3 Types of Relationships The Justification for establishing an intervening level between two linguistic "units" such as that illus- trated above between the classical morpheme and the classical phoneme lies with the fact that the relationship between these "units” is not a simple relationship (i.e., it is not one-to-one). Relationships may be divided into two types: simple (or one-to-one) and complex (i.e., many-to- one or one-to-many) . The types of complex relationships existing within the linguistic system may be shown to be composed of-the primitives of the system of stratificational grammar (i.e., £112. 93;. u ward, 19.11.439.21: ordered, unordered). Following 18 a table of complex relationships and their primitive Comp onents . hhhtiw lhersifiL Seetreli: hpeer hits 23 TABLE I Primitive Components and Complex Relationships in Stratificational Notation COMPLEX RELATIONSHIP PRIMITIVE COMPONENTS STRATIFICATIONAL _N_________OTATION Diversification pp, downward, ordered 22: We p___________ncrdered Neutralization 9_r_, u ward, ordered pp, u ward, unordered and, downward, uno dered Portmanteau and, upwapd, ordered and, u ward, unord_______e____red Zero Simple or pp, downward, ordered or unordered T‘T 7T LL Composite Id downward o dered ‘yl 29" ”’ fi 7% 9 ¥ )1 Empty Simple or tactically specified and, u ward, opdezed or unordered :25 l 21+ The following is an explanation, with examples, of how to read the stratificational notation outlined in Table I. _ 1) a is realized as b or c, but b is TT chosen in preference whenever'possible. b C. LN/SooJ/ For example, the stratificational morpheme M/b:T/ (traditional allom'orph) is chosen only in the environment of comparative or superlative. Elsewhere (the unmarked meT/ “96:44] or rightmost branch) the stratificational morpheme M/gud/ is realized. a 2) 1* | a is realized as b or c (or a is a class b consisting of the members b and c). C. degedfi veg - For example , Pratt). <3 ream. 0. b 3) I l Either a or b is realized as c but a takes precedence. This occurs rather C. u 1 e a_ b infreq ent y h) ac Either a or b is realized as c. . S - L/bes/ For examp1e, eitherb big (in the sense of elder) or ~18 (1n the sense of large) can be realized as the lexeme L/big/, causing the ambiguity in a Phrase like 'my big sister ' for em For ex For L ‘—_ III-m” ‘ - 25 L 0 ’ 5) E a is composed of b followed by c. b. C, Ms/matm/ For example, EZn/'/QE/ /$w/' Cl , 6) a is composed of b and c simul- taneously (or without regard to order). 5 (L For exam 1e LN * LN P . /¢meye the lexcn [men/ (actually man + plural) is com- posed of the morpheme M/m3£'.n/ simultaneous A1 .+ with the plural pre- /maen/ /E / emptive morpheme M /5+ /. 7) (2 k) a and b occurring simultaneously are realized as c. eo" V/S/L/ s z-example, horse ena.e the sememes /horse/ and S/male/ occurring simul- taneously are realized as the “em” SN/stalli 011/. sW/SLLu/Izon/ 8) C: b a followed by b is realized as c. M/a/ “/Le/ C. I‘d/zh/ ‘ Ithrench, the morphemes "/3/ followed by M/1e/ are realized 3‘3 the morphon MN/o/. her eh} hem 5 held! heath 26 9) X x is realized as zero. For I example, one of the realizations of the plural morpheme in English ID 13 as “pl the morphotactics generates a determined element (”empty morph”) z in the environment of child and plupal. This 1 has no connections M, to higher levels. [zaylcl/ V‘ 10) (9 fl is realized as x. For example, X Lamb's Outlipe of Stpapizicapiopg; Grma; and Lockwood's I roduct on to St at icational L s 10 contain fur- ther elaboration on the notational system of stratifica- tional grammar. Reich's ”Symbols, Relations, and Structural Complexity” includes an explanation of their 1081081 1m- Dlications and dynamic interpretations. 2.1.14 The Simplicity Measupe One of the best features of stratificational grammar is its simplicity measure. The same measure can be used to measure a grammar as a whole or to measure any part of it: Thus. stratificational grammar can decide if a sim- P11fication in one part of the grammar has led to an over- all complicauon or is really a simplification in terms °f the grammar as a whole. The need for a simplicity measure stems from the fact that 81mFelicity and generality go hand in hand, and meefioatic 1e keeping K5 meme to a We to 0 equally desc ee.e.. the n Thus a1 ehe empirice A desc (self- as pos centre of ex} exhaue the re Lee (1966‘ “0 aeooun 55011 (1 .e. We 1y [Wire WE ee lines Elements Wally ‘3 subs t! 2? stratificational theory wishes to make use of this fact in keeping with the philosophy of science. Any theory wishing to achieve explanatory adequacy must be able to offer a process for deciding which of two equally descriptively adequate grammars is the simplest (i.e., the most general) (of. Chomsky, 1965: pp. 30-?) Thus an explanatorily adequate theory must include the empirical principle (Hjelmslev: 1961, p. 11 ) A description shall be free of contradiction (self-consistent), exhaustive. and as simple as possible. The requirement of freedom from contradiction takes precedence over the requirement of exhaustive description. The requirement of exhaustive description takes precedence over the requirement of simplicity. Lamb (1966b) has suggested a measure for deciding between two accounts which are equivalent in effective informa- tion (i.e., equally exhaustive) but which differ in surface information (i.e., differ in simplicity). This measure was later refined by Reich (1968). It amounts to what has been termed "counting lines and nodes.” But one must remember that nodes simply define relationships and lines the ”elements” taking part in the relationship. (Elements exist only in substance. Counting 111193 13 “Wally counting the number of times ultimate connections t30 substantive elements are involved in a particular relationship.) Thus counting lines and nodes is equivalent to cduntins +'s and -'s and letters in algebraic descrip- tions. For exam ettement the t) (a 11) a n he method c “he member e member of ti are stated. the and s the state it) is sim; Mum Hhile We toll item des fitted the he into e math men a ‘ Mean 28 For example, it is obvious that ii) is a simpler statement than 1). yet it imparts the same information. 1) (a-b)+(a-c)+(a-d) ii)a.(b+c+d) One method of measuring the simplicity would be to count the number of times a relationship is stated and the number of times elements entering into those relationships are stated. Thus 1) is shown to have five stated relation- ships and six stated elements. while ii) is shown to have three stated relationships and four stated elements. So ii) is simpler or. in linguistic terms, it captures more general izat i one . While this example does not mirror exactly the tech- nique followed in determining the simplicity of a stratifica- tional description. it serves to eXplain the rationale behind the simplicity measure. Actually. not every element enter- ing into each relationship is counted -- only those in excess of three. For example, X ii 5 (a) (b) Figure 8 Relationships of Composition and Neutralization given a common relationship such as composition (a) or neutralization (b), the normal situation is to have hee cone etelloeeee‘ a. tech meet me me et le mate the ease (th is the extra on 29 three connecting items. That is, in (a) x is composed of y followed by 2, while in (b) either q or r is realized as s. A node is defined in terms of its connections upward toward meaning and downward toward sound -- one incoming and at least two outgoing. Less than two outgoing would make the relationship vacuous, i.e., simple. The limiting case (i.e., the case where three such connections exist) is thus taken as defining the relationship or ppgg. Only extra outgoing lines are counted. For example,- C e 3 h Figure 9 Node with Extra Lines Figure, 9 would be counted as one node (relationship), defined by e and any two of f. g. h. and one extra line (one extra "item" entering into the relationship). Since a total stratificational description is given in terms of nodes’and lines (i.e., in terms of relationships and their connections) the same simplicity measure may be used throughout . 2.1.5 Majo; Goal The major goal of stratificational theory is a description of man's "competence to W his language" (Lamb: 1971a, p.103). That is, the competence and hemmee mm. the mot ‘ete eppmoe :ement mod teeelete 1 :e all of “ empe than he been me teetell g meet pote The e title are the team teetehc ‘5 it me: It I )3 E mode heme 30 performance models proposed by the theory are closely linked. ' - The model may be called cognitive since its aim . is to approach a working model which is compatible with current models of the brain and its functions. It attempts to relate language to other forms of human activity and to all of "knowledge." Thus the model has an even broader scope than the description of language. It can be and has been used to describe other human activities such as baseball games-Sand dance gesturesé. It obviously possesses great potential. The way in which the competence and performance models are related is simply that the performance model is the dynamic (or innervated) counterpart of the static competence model -- orthe competence model ”plugged in“ as it were. It must be pointed out that this performance model is a model of ideal performance. That is, it does not account for the variables of actual performance such as memory limits. physiological malfunctions, etc. Stratificational grammar aims at achieving an isomorphic model. A graphic illustration might be helpful. The goal of stratificational grammar might be compared to the goal 01‘ setting up a model of the inner workings of the computer (often called the hardware). Even though one cannot see the inside of a functioning brain as one can the inside Of a functioning computer so that the actual isomorphism the model may be c [stemmed about ho“ :hmi model will an 2.? 8pm 2.2.1 The Gnostemic The semology of imteteo strata -- t imie system is our] teem and to contaf ledge will my free hetemeehich impose heart to form dif imethemeeords. dii I‘m'r‘ntle. or one Iieee the world. The tactics 0 70? the taxonomic )1“) W161. etc. Meets" of .feleI. These: gt eeheepondence Wi 1.5.. the “lam the reasons f0! heed oi Enostom l 51mph r ela t1 0) 31 of the model may be compared, it is hOped that when more is learned about how the brain functions, the stratifica- tional model will approach this isomorphism. 2 .2 Semologx 2.2.1 The Gnostemic and Sememic Strata The semology of stratificational grammar is divided into two strata -- the gnostemic and sememic. The gnos- temic system is currently thought to be outside of language mower and to contain all of human knowledge. This know- ledge will vary from culture to culture as it is this system which imposes differing divisions on the content purport to form differing divisions of content substance. In other words. different cultures tend to view the world differently. or one's culture places a bias on how one views the world. The tactics of the gnostemic system, besides providing for the taxonomic hierarchies of the animal world, the plant world, etc. provides for well-formed combinations of the ”units” of meaning called gposgemes, e.g., G/go/, G/male/. These gnostemes are usually in one-to-one correspondence with gnostons, the components of gnostemes (i.e., the relationship‘is usually a simple one). Some- maJor reasons for positing a structure of gnostemes com... ' Posed of gnostons when the "composition” appears to be a simple relationship are: 1) There are c331 hpa,e.g.. certain i hemlly pessivized manta at this level la) Muskie 1b) Hi§ ha‘ ha above example is following which cenh Whittle interpret; 2!) The Cl 2b) The h 33) The c 3b) Us 1"” 13 also the I \ reheen “0mm“ ! 2) This Struc lsanalogous t 0 th W” Pmper. This second, llh-llhgmstic : in“ ell), , mm, 139031 mture , B‘ “d at thi ’il 32 1) There are cases of complex compositional relation- ships,e.g., certain idiomatic expressions which can be internally passivized need to be broken down into com- ponents at this level. An example follows. 1a) Muskie threw his hat in the ring. lb) His hat was thrown in the ring. The above example is in contrast to such examples as their following which cannot be passivized and still retain the idiomatic interpretations 2a) The cow kicked the bucket. (ambiguous) 2b) The bucket was kicked by the cow. (unambiguous) 3a) The child threw a fit. 3b) A {it was thrown by the child. (unacceptable) There is also the fact that pormanteau realizations exist between gnostemes and gnostons. 2) This structure bf gnostemes composed of gnostons is analogous to that posited for a stratal system of language proper. This second reason is not particularly convincing since extra-linguistic structure need not mirror exactly linguis- tic structure. But since there exist examples such as that in 1b), a relationship 01‘ composition between ~emes and -ons is posited at this level, even though most of the relation... Ships will indeed be simple. l‘ne aememic strat this within imam tlewel are realized teamemic level. I w? to separate strat t necessary to show ll that complex mtaememes or. in c we not one-to-one. 2) that the (101 ll tlcstemic level llferent from, tha It is commonl: he is the propos “m“ is the tex Mount for tel mm“. howevew hr. To 1unattat Elm between the mtctntlng “I p! l 33 The sememic stratum is the first level of the semology which is within language prOper. The gnostons of the gnostem- ic level are realized as sememes. the tactic ”units” of the sememic level. In order to Justify the existence of two separate strata -- the gnostemic and sememic -- it is necessary to show 1) that complex relationships exist between gnostemes end sememes or. in other words, that the relationships are not one-to-one, and '2) that the domain of the gnostotactics, which defines the gnostemic level, is greater than, or, in some sense, different from, that of the semotactics. It is commonly held that the domain of the semotac- tics is the proposition, while the domain of the gnosto- tactics is the text, or discourse. (Relations necessary to account for texts are presumed to include all of human knowledge, however.) Thus part 2) [above is accounted for. To illustrate the fact that complex relationships OXist between the gnostemic and sememic strata. thereby accounting for part 1) above. an example from Ikegami (1970. p.6Le) will be used. Hg/jounfi/ Ike 3am! we Wreememes mstemes cf the Mitt “Vera“. it all and that th! 1th “an‘ (hum l 31+ H‘S/molle/ HS/kuman/ S/humah / O S/Man/ e §£u31r/I Figure 10 Ikegami's Hypersememic Diagram The hypersememes (HS) of Ikegami's diagram are the gnostemes of the present discussion. Note that there exists diversification. in the realizations of young and male and that the sememe S/boy/ can be interpreted as _'y0un8 man' (human 4- male + young) or as a 'male child' (“man + young + male). hillustration of mic and sememic s C C MORSE 0E 7‘47 ’ tow Inte the GnOat: 35 An illustration of the integrated structure of the gnostemic and sememic strata is given below. CONTENT PURWRV aim : DIS COURSE 0 Tax";- .> Q’ *_ GNOSTEMES Vt ‘I‘ACTIC. PLAN ES ‘ bfiMi K : Rowan-cog z 2 E all) 3 5%: Figure 11 Integrated Structure of the Gnostemic and Sememic Strata 22.2 W 2.2.2.1 293220.121. Lyons (1963' P ‘eteehnique for th eenntic relations sentences containir eminentlal analye 19%) consisted of hints of amine Proportions 3 Mill “Omani c 222 the featureS ‘ thieease). (male were extract“1 as Katz and W1 ewtheabove. 1301 features. termed temple being th' emetic markers lltitan) and (meal titers in the f it [young heist Elwht] . The stratii lot. It rec 081 36 2.2.2 Componential Analysis 2.2.2.1 Definition Lyons (1968: p.1e76) defines componential analysis as "a technique for the economical statement of certain semantic relations between lexical items and between sentences containing them”. Previous work in semantic componential analysis (of. Goodenough, 1956 and Lounsbury, 1956) consisted of identifying distinctive features on the basis of analogy , or proportional relatedness. Proportions such as the following were set up man: woman: child we stallions mare: foal and the features (human) vs. (non-human, or equine in this case). (male) vs. (female), (adult) vs. (non-adult) were extracted as components. Katz and Fodor (196M distinguish components such as the above, termed 'semantic markers' from item-specific features, termed 'semantic distinguishers', the classic example being the lexical item bachelor with two of its semantic markers (i.e., features or components) being (human) and (male) and two distinguishers under these markers in the feature hierarchy [who has never married] and [young knight serving under the standard of another knight] . The stratificational approach makes yet another distinc- tion. It recognizes the fact that "the meaning of some enie can be analyz ‘ hielso that some investiga the mistake of can be analyze for example. t components in plus a coupon (Ismbe 1969 , lecrucisl state intent is one whi anemic signs. T lithe taxonomic lactic distingu lithe structure w hie they are pat hose semantic fe ll.e., which occu llms) are consié 3.2.2.2 Univerg Although it lollies are lens Mine given E (knowledge on 37 words can be analyzed into components" (Lamb: 1969, pp.(+6-7) but also that some investigators of semantic structure have made the mistake of supposing that taxonomic structure can be analyzed in terms of sememic caponents: for example, that m should be analyzed into components including all the components of evergreen plus a component which distinguishes pines from other evergreens. But such a component would occur only as a component of pine. A true sememic component occurs in two or more different sememic signs. For example, femalg occurs not only in mare, but also in 33;, ow, vixen, h_e_n_. sisteg, Queen, and so forth. In addit on, true semantic components are sememes and as such they articipate in the sememic syntax. (Lamb: 1969. pp.leg-7) The crucial statement here is that a true sememic com- ponent is one which occurs in two or more different sememic signs. This in effect says that those features of the taxonomic hierarchy which are item specific (the semantic distinguishers of Katz and Fodor) are not part of the structure of language ( 1.e.. in the stratificational sense they are part of the gnostemic stratum). Only those semantic features which function in the semotactics ‘ (i.e., which occur as components of two or more sememic signs) are considered components in the linguistic sense. 2 .2 .2 .2 Univergal Status Although it seems obvious that the taxonomic hier- archies are language specific (and probably even speaker 8Decific given each speaker's greater or lesser degree 01‘ knowledge on a particular subject). whether or not meet the cmponen lot so obvious. one. Lyons (1968s 1: hot empirical avid oeeeent time would 1 the hypothesis . ' osmotic components on Isle indefinite insole aware that she new distincti taxonomic hierarc there exists a oer [whoops a subset levels of the war eeeos that every olmlflcant disti hose distinction: lie is only hypo into which, if a lo the arguments 3.2.2.3 Related in interest healer has the Matter how 1‘! liferentiate 1‘ ‘39 speaker 1180‘ ‘-—.--1I|.-|'" IIIII 38 some of the components of these hierarchies are universal is not so obvious. Katz and Postal (1961+) suggest that they are. Lyons (1968: p.1e73). on the other hand, says that ”such empirical evidence as there is available at the present time would tend to refute, rather than confirm. this hypothesis.” lntuitively it seems that the set of semantic components cannot be a closed set since a speaker can make indefinitely7finer distinctions once he or she is made aware that a distinction exists, and in order to make new distinctions one must add new features to the taxonomic hierarchy. But intuitively also, it seems that there exists a certain functional subset of these components (perhaps a subset of those which function on the sememic levels of the world's languages) which is universal. It seems that every human language would make a linguistically significant distinction between (male) and (female) since these distinctions are overtly manifested in the universe. This is only hypothetical, however. Actually the decision as to which, if any, components are universal is not crucial to the arguments herein presented. 2.2.2.3 Related Phenomena An interesting fact about semantics is that each speaker has the capacity to describe any item or phenomenon no matter how fine or gross a distinction is needed to differentiate it from another and no matter what language the speaker uses . heoontext to incl nominee appears hinolined to ask ill-defined struct hing able to comp should not prohibi oitheir structur 2.2.2.1) Co itiv is far as th «ponents of m ninetificaticnali Possess correlat 2.2.3 linkages: The notion o of simplicity wi The is, the nor he required to Inked a portion Verbal pai‘ line As useful Mrs are simi] the" towards 39 Another phenomenon which makes describing the entire gnostemic and sememic strata for any given language utter- ly futile is the fact that given any utterance interpreted as nonsense under normal circumstances. one can extend the context to include, say. the surreal, and then the utterance appears semantically well--formed.8 One might be inclined to ask "Why bother to try to study such an ill-defined structure?" The answer is that fear of not being able to completely describe these stratal systems should not prohibit onefrom trying to describe parts of their structure . 2.2.2.4 'Cosnitive Validity As far as the cognitive validity (or reality) of components of meaning, it is quite safe to say that stratificationalists presume that such components actually possess correlates in the mind of the speaker. 2.2.3 Markedness The notion of markedness has to do with the notion of simplicity with respect to the relationship of composition. That is, the more marked a unit is, the more components are required to define it. And conversely, the less marked a particular item is, the fewer its components. Verbal pairs such as gaging and 3933, 99m; and 52 serve as useful illustrations. It is obvious that these Pairs are similarly related by the direction they imply (i.e., towards or away from the speaker). It is also hlons that they i the more marked considering the fac tone, which cover olonientation' ( oonteininoe the cam otlll more sdequat this composed hoeen to be the hrkedness i tional descriptio termed 'antoms‘ him like 93mg 8 enlyzed componen lower linguistic lid girl are clear heather. The C lMair for Eng: one 'liow gig is “she?“ unless With of the per his m can 1 “he lexemes 5 5m 'ant onyms ‘—_' r—rr..-c’l-OI ' no obvious that they involve motion. The decision as to which is the more marked member of each pair is arrived at by considering the fact that 52 and take ”are more general terms. which cover movement without a specific direction- al orientation" (Lamb: 1969. p. 1m ). Thus 993;; may be analyzed as a sememic sign composed of the sememes 59 and direction toward. while lying may be analyzed as containing the components tag plus direction toward. A still more adequate analysis might be found in describing Elie. as composed of go + carrying (something). so 59 can be seen to be the least marked of the four. Markedness is relevant to each level in a stratifica- tional description. Pairs which have been traditionally termed 'antonyms' (and which were grouped together with pairs like 29g; and 32) such as mg and old may be analyzed componentially in stratificational terms on a lower linguistic stratum, namely the lexemic. 193mg and pig are clearly related by the fact that one is 3:12; the other. The question as to which is the unmarked of the pair for English may be found in questions of the sort “How 9_l__d is she?” where one does not ask "How m5 is she?" unless the speaker is focusing attention on the Youth of the person about whom he is asking the question. Thus mpg can be analyzed as a lexemic sign composed of the lexemes 9;}; and 319;. The rationale for considering these 'antonyms' on a lower level than the verbal 'antonyms' that The native sp opposite of bi the opposite This knovledg for according serve equally he have thus lithe units of th hvenentionerl not truth. It might be hononio hierarc mused of fever he set to which mud daisies. p Thus the ter than the term fo has supportive he child genera] learns (whether 5 «the general t1 Maps even to m. 3M The Ghost No real pii liven. lamb ha; Sch as the tax 1+1 is that The native speaker of English knows that the opposite of m is little and not small; that the opposite of large is small. not little. This knowledge clearly does not involve meaning, for according to meaning. small and little could serve equally well as opposites of b 5. (Lamb: 1969, p.145 ) We have thus far discussed markedness with respect to the units of the sememic and lexemic strata. but we have mentioned nothing about markedness on the gnostemic stratum. It might be that the unit flower in the gnostotactic taxonomic hierarchy of vegetation might be said to be composed of fewer components than any of the members of the set to which it is related by extension.9 namely annual daisies. perennial chrysanthemums, etc. Thus the term flower might be said to be less marked than the term for any specific flower. It might be mention- ed as supportive evidence that in language acquisition, the child generalizes the word for the first flower he learns (whether it be a particular flower. say, daisy. or the general term ;:lower) to include all flowers and Perhaps even to include the still less marked category plant I 2.2.14 The Gnostotactics No real picture of the gnostotactics has ever been given. Lamb has alluded to the structures it contains such as the taxonomic hierarchies mentioned before. But hue the gnostotac munchiedge, it hlspoiut in the :1 lies. and probably test my overall heme in partic lush of the gnos but and the struc ledge oi referents The present lulled subportio puiions which ar hecrphotactio 0 hi provide for sashes (i.e., 11 he semotactics Stratum and the enemies may b! Prism the dos} he at this 1 936 described. his of ghost! ”Mal and top Wesponding 1+2 since the gnostotactics is thought to contain all of human knowledge, it is rather presumptuous. at least at this point in the development of the science of linguis- tics, and probably also for a good while to come. to at- tempt any overall precise formalization even for one language in particular. Topics which come under the domain of the gnostotactics are such things as the con- text and the structure of discourse and mutual foreknow— ledge of referents. The present study will concern itself with very limited subportions of the gnostotactics. only those portions which are relevant to the ultimate definition of morphotactic classes. 2.1.5 The Semotactics The semotactics, whose domain is the proposition, must provide for possible well-formed combinations of sememes (i.e., linguistically functional units of meaning). The semotactics is the structure which defines the sememic stratum and the syntagmatic configurations which it Renerates may be roughly compared to the sentences com- prising the deep structure10 of transformational grammar. It is at this level where such notions as deep level case11 are described. Semotactic realizations of the different types of gnostemic focus (predicate. propositional. func... tional and topic) ultimately insure their corresponding surface structure (i.e., lexemic) realizations, The semotactic hheen certain pro Maine. This re inersnnple. the di sentences is whethe in to one analysis in non-topicelize. been generated by thin; generated 2.) Strata Intern 2.}.1 Lexeuic The stratum train of the la: tactics nust prov hpsndent clauses hostotectic prop he senotsctics T Marked as int fluoride the v: TIlnslizstion. The lexotet intimate levn ‘3' transformsti TMinute-obj“ We realized “1.7818, if n 43 The semotactics also accounts for relationships between certain propositions which share a common semantic structure. This relationship has been termed agnation.12 Fer example. the difference between active and passive sentences is whether or not the goal is in "focus" (accord- ing to one analysis) and the difference between topicalized and non-topicalized sentences is whether "topic“ has been generated by the gnostotactics and is thus capable of being generated by the semotactics. 2.3 Strata Intervening Between the Sememic and Phonemic 2.3.1 Lexemic The stratum below the sememic is the lexemic. The domain of the lexotactics is the clause and thus this tactics must provide for various realizations of dependency. Dependent clauses are the ultimate realizations of the gnostotactic propositions unmarked for "propositional" focus. The semotactics has generated as dependent any predication not marked as independent. It is left to the lexotactics to provide the various permissable realizations such as nominalization, relativization and subordination. The lexotactics. in generating structures at the approximate level of abstraction of the surface structures 01’ transformational grammar. accounts for the subject- Predicate-object surface functions. Subject is the lore. tactic realization of functional focus. According to one analysis, if no other deep (or gnostemic) level function his focus, then is slim of agent as W @9212 The domain of precisely the morp nlngicrd word). I sisters and affix hm that the tee hi of the morph elenents but also lexenic: norphemic u this study we M and its 1.3.3 The Relgti Hogphgnic is suggests: tin cases which tortotactic oon till they defir he more usua‘. hlized as uni the being re [Mb (1977 Wm on of ' ML is in focus, then the lexotactics provides for the reali- zation of agent as subject if there is an agent. 2 .3 .2 Morphgmic The domain of the morphotactics is the word (more precisely the morphological word, in contrast to the phon- ological word). It provides for well-formed combinations of stems and affixes. An illustration from Lamb (l9?la.p.ll9) shows that the tactics of the lexemic stratal system and that of the morphemic account not only for different elements but also for different ordering of these elements. lexemic: 3rd sg. erfect assive take -z + be -en + take - morphemi c s have en In this study we will be concerned with the lexemic element 113;; and its various morphological realizations. 2.3.3 ‘lhe Relationship 9: the Gnostemic System to the Mor h m c S tem As suggested by Lamb (1971:!) there appear to be cer- tain cases which call for a direct link between certain ghastotactic configurations and the morphological classes which they define. This is not the usual case, however. It is more usual for units on the gnostemic level to be realized as units on the sememic and then lexemic levels before being realized as morphemes. Lamb (1971;) posits a new device to handle the direct connection of morphemes to meaning while guaranteeing heir participation ullethie device 1: ludiancnd node 0 cede into the tact more locations leer level are (11 lllterent connecti their different level. Wt The Relati Phonemic S The morphemi end thus more clc lurphemic. The d l the syllable. include the phon 1lllchl clause, e lle have seer lithe morpholog: Marlee. for tile boundaries lit the tactic: "not. (or. Se 3 the syllable The units dPhonemes. M 2'1“]- e) 45 their participation in higher level configurations. He calls this device the 'representative'. The representative is a diamond node on a higher level which connects up- wards into the tactics of that level and downward to one or more locations at the level below. These -emes on the lower level are distinguished from one another by their different connections to the gnostemic system and sometimes by their different connections to the tactics of the lower level. 2.3.4 The Relationship of the Morphemic System to the Phonemic System The morphemic and phonemic systems are contiguous and thus more closely related than are the gnostemic and morphemic. The domain of the phonotactics is most common- ly the syllable. However, the domain may be expanded to include the phonological word, phonological phrase, phono- logical clause, etc. We have seen that the domain of the morphotactics is the morphological word. It is quite clear that morpheme boundaries, for the most part, do not coincide with syl- lable boundaries in any given language. Thus. it is clear that the tactics of the two levels are separate and dis- tinct. (Cf. Section 2.4.Le for a more detailed discussion 01‘ the syllable.) The units defined by the phonotactics are stratification- 81 phonemes. (For more on the S—phoneme, of. Sections 2.1.1 and 2.4.1,) :_ i The norphemic r uetel to expressi thought to be t then the morph The elements telephones of lee-phone. liorpho Inlte or the phon it heals: Md The Phone The phonolo eltlo strata -- stratum is a par stratum is thong eiratel systems The phonemic sys environments of hencrphonic 31 tried stratific lute ambiguity tional phoneme ‘ mi. The tactic that of stratf let there is ' llent in level 1+6 The morphemic system is the closest grammatical system to expression substance. The phonological system is thought to be the only linguistic stratum intervening between the morphemic system and expression substance. The elements on the two levels are related as follows. The morphemes of the morphemic stratum are composed of morphons. Morphons are in turn realized as phonemes (the units of the phonotactics). which are composed of phonons. 2.1+ Phonology 2.“.1 The Phonemic and Phonetic Strata The phonology of stratificational grammar consists of two strata -- the phonemic and phonetic. The phonemic stratum is a part of language proper, while the phonetic stratum is thought to be outside of language. These two stratal systems handle essentially different phenomena. The phonemic system handles the realizations and tactic environments of the "segmental" alternations extant in the morphonic alternation pattern. The realizations are termed stratificational phonemes, a term coined to elim- inate ambiguity with classical phonemes. The stratifica- tional phoneme is more abstract than its classical counter- part. The tactics of stratificational phonology is stated in terms of stratificational phonemes. This is not to say that there is no place in the theory for a biunique level equiv- alent in level of abstraction to the classical phonemic. fl the contrary. the re oi the points hcglv adhered t here to be a link «lame and th have event be act 1 eetitiee and the p r The ctratif level -- the phcn viretiriceticml tie the limguis language and exp every phonemic s llee voicing ever the uorphon m/d vlth [e] and vic try dietributic llroment. 1 Currently “ he phonetic 10 Interface. Th! umterpart. h! “phenomena If "inflation 1| Manage. and internaticallz it? On the contrary, the maintenance of a biunique level is one of the points which most stratificationalists have strongly adhered to. They would argue that in order for there to be a link between the phonic correlates outside of language and the phonological units inside language. there must be mutual predictability between the phonetic entities and the phonemic. Thus stratificational theory provides for such a level -- the phonemic sign level -- which is below the stratificational phonemic level in the realizational plane. It is the linguistic level closest to the interface of language and expression. It is here that. for example. every phonemic sign P8/t/ in a language with non-distinc- tive voicing word finally, whether it was related to, say. the morphon MN/d/ or the morphon MN/t/. would be associated with [t] and vice versa. except in the case of complemen- tary distribution where the variant is predictable by en- vironment. Currently the connections between the phonemic and the phonetic levels are thought to be across the language interface. The phonetic stratum. like its gnostemic counterpart, has not been studied a great deal. The types 0f phenomena which it is thought to handle are phonetic assimilation within the domain relevant for the particular language, and specification of phonetic components not 8S’stematically distinctive. it is general tullv analyzed. nlmticn. In a componen uehpheeic is n elpninte of arti lieelf. That is. reticular coupon in. In a strati ertnally play a mi only these 1 in example in llcdern Iraqi (See the List of Componentialc Lb b8 2Jh2 ngpogential Analysig It is generally agreed that phonemes may be componen- tnally analyzed, and that these components play a role in explanation. In a component theory such as that used in S-grammar the emphasis is not on the contrast along the dimension §® MdeQ \ I +>+8+Q mezdnw // §E® II maria (I I/ l // ‘I ’l/ ¢12>+ a women 4@ era @ wemswmw ‘ Figure 21+ r‘MaaL Plurals (Semantically-based) Bound 159 (:) The type of singular characterized by the item p 'tube' is generally described as having a weak le radical. i.e.. either 1 or 1. Its characteris- surface form is Fva. e.g.. Euub 'tube'. 2112 'barrel'. forms have here been analyzed as lexical nouns (i.e., derived) of the form FVWflL. where w signifies a weak cal. V signifies a vowel alternation uoii. whose ace alternants are contextually predictable (in this 1 before 1. 2 before 1). fl indicates a vowel ”place er” considered a vowel by the CVCV... pattern of the atactics. but given no phonic substantive realization. 160 The plural vowel pattern (giau+V+) will be given :edence over the singular pattern 12 in the following er. If the plural pattern has been selected by the hotactics then the phonotactics will receive the singu- form and the plural pattern simultaneously. 3ngb . + + fl+at V e the phonotactics imposes the order CVCV...(C). it t selects c. then the precedence-taking ¢+ (in pref- ce to E. which is realized as fl). then 1. then aa+ preference to fl). then b. In case there are only three consonants (as is the case here). the last preemptive L (V+) will be realized as h. Thus nouns related in to £ngb (cuwbucuub) in the singular will have the ll form §Qwaxbfl (cwaab). given abstractly as FMaaL. :2) - (:) Forms such as tall 'hilr and filis 'fils'. I having dissimilar surface forms are related to forms tuub at a more abstract level. These forms too may alyzed as having the vowel pattern _Q. Forms such as _are here analyzed as having the singular form rv1gs e morphemic sign level. The 1 here is the same mor- X (hvu) present in forms like chub and 2112. The nt is predictable here also by context. (Actually the underlying (morphonic and phonemic) vowel to which phoneme” "labial" is added in the appropriate en- nents of labials. velars. emphatics. and r in the nic alternation pattern.) It is here posited then :he stratificational phonemic sign (C-phonemic) form 161 filis is file (the same shape as tall above) and that e phonetic tactics inserts an epenthetic copy of the rst vowel between the last two consonants of a stem if ey are not the same and if they occur word finally f. filim 'film'. a borrowing from English). At the unique phonemic sign level where i in the environment _C# is not distinctive. all stratificational phonemic s in this position are realized as fl. The phonetic :tics may then insert the anaptyctic vowel everywhere this environment. Thus items such as tall (tglfll) and ;;§ (illflg) have their plurals realized in the manner flained for £2122 under C) . (:) There are certain nominals which take the plural LaL. but which have a final vowel in the singular, e.g.. fig 'narghile‘ (garfisa. i.e.. a zfl_a pattern). In order handle the plural mechanism for such nouns it is nec- ary that the final singular vowel which denotes the mmatically feminine be suppressed (i.e., not be realized) "plural" is chosen. This is done in Figure 2n by having feminine suffix as a possible suffix only in the singu- configuration (of. the right branch of the topmost down- 5 ordered 2;). The morphon T is realized as P/t/ before uffix beginning with a consonant. Elsewhere it is lized as fl. For example. the phonotactics. according ?1%ure 2b would receive agrflg (since the feminine n+3 1+V+ ?ix -3 would not have been allowed to be realized) and 162 i generate g¢+rax+§. with V+ being realized as n since S were only three stem consonants. Thus the plural Sass. This guarantees that. whether the singular forms l plural is FMaaL end in a vowel (garsa) or not (cuub). ; is no final vowel in the plural pattern. g) There are certain other nominals which end in a I and which keep this vowel in the plural. These als represented by gamflin 'hose for narghile' pose blem since their final vowels must somehow be kept not from the final vowels of nouns like ggréa above are suppressed. In order to keep them distinct only one difference :ded: the suffix -; attached to some forms as a deter- sign is allowed to be realized in the environment lral. The phonotactics generates the plural vowel 'ns of such nouns in the following manner. Receiving iguration such as gamg§ 1 . the phonotactics would fut-v4- a: the order gflfmaa+ci. V+ being realized as 5 since are only three stem consonants. Thus the emerging _form is gmaal; (FMaaLi). ) - There is one additional problem here. how- kThere are nominals whose plurals are of the form . but whose singulars not only have an i after the Foper but also an additional a (ggzgjiiiacgaajxg ttion channel') or sometimes also an additional ya 163 QO+i+yastquizxa 'nightstick‘). This additional materi- lso signifies ”grammatically feminine”. The only addition- estriction needed is a subclass governing the realiza- . of 1 (cf. ). The subclass of nominals represented quizxa 'nightstick' would enable the additional I to ealized in the singular. (:) and (:) Nominals of the form hayfiya 'snake' and gigggg 'vulva' whose plurals are of the form FMaaLa a different kind of problem. The singular form may ay not have a suffix yet the plural form always does. resolution of this problem lies in making separate lessee of these nominals as in Figure 25. ,4! 16h .3“ ha‘ A . G .i 4‘. 4r \ \ \\ — / / / / /// A /// / ./ A V / / ‘ / a ‘ (:) ¢fi-‘/+ @ a” 8 Figure 25 Subclasses of FMaaL-type Plurals Phonotactics on receiving the configuration + supplies the order kfligaa+§a. As before V is as d yielding the surface form ksaasa. 165 The final form classified as a FMaaL-type plural epresented by thflbag (mutbag) 'double-tubed flute'. __a____ __a___ form actually represents two phonologically predictable l1 alternants FMaaHiL and FHaaMuL. The phonotactics. receiving thflbag yields the form m¢+tat+bV+g (mtaabug). __AL___— ——e4 .—L ¢+as+V+ time because there are more than three stem consonants r ultimately realized as one of the alternants 1 or g. Lding on environment. Exactly how the derivational vowel patterns (see the tst left unordered 91) are assigned is not dealt with is treatment. Examples of adjectives in this plural are shown. Their simultaneously realized vowel rn is 251:. where V1 is a morphon denoting the alterna- aF°i, g in the environment k__ and 1 elsewhere. The remainder of the semantically defined [round] 18 (eight in number) may be generalized to two ional round] broken plural classes. They are: Ls: (FaMaaMiiL, FiMaaMiiL, FMaaMiiL). M aMiL, FuMaaMiL) and yjfl (FuMi, FiMi, FaMIaat). 3 26 (see u.3.1.1.2) shows how the first of these 3] allomorphs (i.e., morphemic sisHS) 1to the overall structure. Figure 27 (see u.3.1.1.3) Lbes the second. 166 .1.1.2 We We have reduced thirteen broken plural classes. ntically defined by the component GN/round/, to ee allomorphic (stratificational morphemic) classes L, FvMaaMi(i)L. and FVML. Figure 26 describes the aaMi(i)L plurals. 167 6”/mu we) / SVda 7‘15" \ \ 9V5 i 74>) \ '\ MV$¢M‘ \ n&?¢‘m :r H7¢wa 1w ,1; V” 0+ a:+ 1'" Figure 26 aMi(i)L Plurals (Semantically-based) Round 168 lownward unordered and labeled (:) defines the class .ngulars which take FvMaaMi(i)L plurals. The enabler :hed to this plural class enables this particular L1 choice (FvMaaMi(i)L) to be realized. (:) is the subclass of singulars whose plural is .Mi(i)L FuMaaMi(i)L where the alternation inJu (V+) onologically predictable. The generalization that h of the final vowel of the plural is predictable e occurrence of length with the final vowel of the lar is captured in the following manner. Length ice the final consonant in the morphotactics and is ned thereto. so that length is present in both singular Lural forms. The phonotactics will assign this length : syllable nucleus (v) as a constituent. Thus, in arphotactics the configuration is V :C while in the :actics the configuration is !;_9. (Actually two of abstract length will have to be set up in order sure their separate treatment in the phonology. That :ngth originally associated with a consonant must be Listinot from length originally associated with a vowel, :hough both are ultimately associated with a vowel on mface.) here are both masculine and feminine nouns taking lural. An example is sVda?g; (sidaara 'common Iraqi ar'). (The feminine suffix is realized as fl in the nment of plural.) Its simultaneously realized plural 169 V+as+i+. The phonotactics would interdigitate the emptive vowels to yield §V+danfzi+z (81958112)- Glottal has several realizations in Iraqi. The singular fonm his noun at the stratificational phononic level would dat . The phonotactics enables ? to be realized Length (I) in the environment before a consonant in morphzmc alternation pattern. The plural form at the utificational phononic level would be gidaszig since the lotactics would enable ? to be realizad as l in the wronment before (or after) 1 in themonmxnic alternation ern. (It may also be realized as 1, En i, g in various r environments.) One thing which became evident in dealing with this lass of plurals is that glottal stop (7) will have to its alternants specified in the morphonic alternation Ern above the phonotactics (i.e., before the phono- ics assigns a point of articulation to X or !f(i.e.. e i is realized as g (in some environments) or i). h + zample, na? 'ua 'water wheel'. pl: nv+?a: iar. a1 stop must first be realized as g in an ”elsewhere” onment. Then V+ (neutralized with 1) can be ctly predicted as 3 before 1 in the phonemic nation pattern (nuwaa'iir). Nouns in this subclass take the plural FMaaMiiL. her words the first vowel is 9+. The plural of 3k (mankuuk ‘bobbin') is mnaakiik. 170 Class (:) is the ”elsewhere” class (i.e., the class hose members take a: as the first vowel of the plural. he plural of eranss (gurgggsa 'blister') is faraagiis. ouns in all three of these subclasses ( (:) . (:) . (:) ) ave similar underlying shapes so there is no way to pre- Lct class membership on such a basis. ,3010103 FVML‘tRBe Figure 27 describes the FvML-plurals defined by the toston GN/round/. (In general discussions defining :mantic components are referred to as [round] . for ex- ple; in theoretical descriptions these components are ferred to as gnostons. for example, GN/round/.) 171 LN/Pl/ 4‘... - v/ - / N ® ‘ P—baSeA \ GHA-b‘SCJ/ G’éeh'owd/ \ O \ C)bwnn2n \ .na \/ A / \ . 9 as) , \ , A A @A CqunJ/\ . / QT \ \\ ' /- k1 \ \ /% \l \ \\ //' /// \a§¢? \ \///¢/:} —\ an.\ A._{ ,,”’./”’ -_ - / \ / 7 \ If A /\ Uh¢ha?\$im?kd\ali3 bam¢7q?\\ A ‘\\:‘\\\\ .__ 4 _.,__\ ‘ 3" (1* a*' 55+ Figure 27 FvML Plurals (Semantically-based) Bound 172 <:) Here we see that the broken or internal (simulfixed) .rals. which we have been so far discussing, form a di- tomy with the sound or external (suffixed) plurals. broken plurals are defined by the downward unordered ,indicating simultaneity, while the sound plurals are ined by the downward ordered and indicating consecu- ity. (:) This node defines the class of FvML—plurals as e 1e. All of the members of this class take a plural h FvML as a factor. Here we have an instance of an bling line connecting to an enabler within the the tics rather than below. Currently in stratificational Dry, enabling lines are allowed to connect to enablers V below the tactics in the ”emic" alternation pattern. a restriction was found to be too stringent in such as as (:> . The Iraqi data requires both semantic (or Itotactic) conditioning (see the diamond node connected :he gnoston GN/round/) and morphological conditioning. urder to adequately account for the data. conditioning in the morphotactics was necessary. In other words. e not all items characterized by the gnoston GN/round/ any one particular plural shape, but are further itioned by the morphotactic c1ass(es) of the singulars which they are associated. it proved necessary to v conditioning within the tactics of the morphology. 173 The members of the class defined by the node designated (:) form a subclass. The surface shape of the plural is i. As an example take the item £2123 (mragyg 'mirror'). shape of the singular with the feminine ending ya (the sing allowed to be realized in this case) is mfl;a?z§. phonotactics receives the 1; after the stem proper. 5 beginning again the CV sequence and allowing the sonant cluster 31 (mfira? and ya). Glottal stop is VCVC CV [ized as length before a consonant. yielding the form flag. The plural is 32:33 which the phonotactics ‘1“ u ardigitates to yield gu+rfli . The surface form (£321) 1chieved when glottal stop is realized as 1 in the en- )nment u.iC #. In other words, mur? is realized as 174 (:) defines the subclass FiMi. The usually predictable i (l) alternation is not the case here. Thus two .arate subclasses had to be set up (FuMi and FiMi). 3 the item L222: (Lag; 'aba'). The singular is 12223 :h glottal stop being realized as a in the environment __#. In other words 'ab? is realized as :222. The Lral is $2222 . The interdigitated form is 'i+bg!?. 1+9? . >tta1 stop is realized as i in the environment u.iC #. 'ib? is realized as 'ibi. (:) This subclass has the plural shape FaMLaat ~- t is. the internal preemptive vowel sequence 2:g_: and suffix -aat. Take bamgya? (baglaaxa 'okra') as an mple. The singular is straightforward at this point. plural is (pgggzgl a'é). The interdigitated form is *T a ngfya?aat. It here becomes apparent that we need a n boundary between the stem proper and suffixed material. that this boundary must function as an environment Lvalent to that of a consonant for the realization of 1. >ther words, if we posit a stem boundary (+) between 1:123 and sit. we can say that Z is realized as L in the ronment ___C,+. Then we would get the form bamxasas . phonotactics or the phonetic tactics could insure that one of a sequence of two long vowels is realized. A nd choice for the solution of this problem is to con- :t the phonotactics in such a manner as to have all 175 aterial after the last consonant able to be interdigitated lth a preemptive vowel truncated (i.e., realized as t). n this case the a: of ba+mgfya? would be truncated yielding lth the addition of the sound plural suffix all the correct arm bamyaa . The second solution seems to be the more eneral case, i.e.. it seems to account for more data. or example bantalawn 'trousers' p1: banggtiig. The ggg are seems to have been truncated. (:) The item here rahfiha? (rahhaaya 'grinder') is O A I. robably derived but it is the only feminine example aVaiI- ale for this plural subclass. 176 3.1.2 Square Plurals 3.1.2.1 FvMaaMi‘izL-type The semantically based plurals defined by the gnoston ’square/ are four in number: FMaaLiin. FiMaL. FMaaMiiL 1 FaMaaMiiL. The last two are also defined by the gnoston ’round/ and were discussed above. Actually there are tare items in all of the FvMaaMiiL plurals. Figure 28 shows r the component GN/square/ is interrelated with the haMi(i)L plural system. A Gu/guare/ 6%und/ ' Q in» A.» ‘5 :umffi-i.k : ‘ dsqgum 1r \ \ ’ FMmL RML FIMQL FMaaLiin \ \ : \ I 55.41.41 \ampazb X SOJ9$JQ :d\ \\ \\ \\ \ AIL \ \ \ \\ \ \ \ 4 Figure 28 'MaaMl(1)L Plurals (Semantically-based) Square 177 h3.1.2.2 FMaaLiin-type The square plural FMaaLiin is morphologically similar :0 the FhaaL round plurals and thus must be integrated ith them. Figure 29 details this integration. 43.1: ; ‘Ii L dumfaKam F MQL FVMMM bdzyit Figure 29 FMaaLiin Plural (Semantically-based) Square r' 178 The noun dukkaan 'shop' pl: dkaakiin is subject to a dual interpretation. It may be analyzed as belonging to the plural class FHaaMiiL or to the class FMaaLiin. If the second analysis is correct than dukkaan provides another example of truncation of final singular material. The noun haayit 'wall' p1: hyaatiin also presents a____m :___4___ an interpretation problem. Normally one would assign it an underlying shape of ha?¢yit. In that case the plural H should be hwaayiin. To obtain the correct plural (hyaatiin) w %- one must analyze the underlying form as halyit. M These nouns form a separate class and enable the Qfas+ alural (related to the round plurals) to be simultaneously realized with the noun stem which is obligatorily followed >y the sound masculine plural -ii . For example: halyit is realized as h¢+yas+tisn ;:-:& isn 4_.____4___ as >r hyaatiin. 4...a___ L.3.1.2.3 FlMaL-txpe The last plural class characterized by the component 'N/square/ is FiMaL. This particular morphological shape ;as not been introduced yet. Figure 30 shows this plural. he plural of ga§g§ (g§§§g 'ledge'L for example is g;§§§. s usual the :a feminine ending of the singular is not ealized in the environment of plural. All of these nouns ave similar phonological shapes and they are all feminine. 179 Panama, Fv MaaMli L H (1" Figure 30 FiMaL Plural (Semantically-based) Square 14.3.1.3 Paired Body Parts Plurals The next semantic class to be considered is that of [paired body parts]. There are only two morphological classes so related: FMaaLaat and FVMaaLaat where V a UN u). The only problem is with the plural of ?ubut ___c. 'armpit'. The singular is predictable. Its underlying Phonemic shape is ?Vbt. 1 would then be correctly pro- “ vided as g in the phonemic alternation pattern. and would also be copied by the phonetic tactics to prevent dissimilar 180 consonants from surfacing contiguously. However. the underlying phonemic shape of the plural is ?V+ban+talt. 1 would then be incorrectly predicted to bepi since the emphatic (t) is not in the immediate environment. I can see no way at the present time to resolve this conflict without assigning the surface vowel to the underlying singular forms and have the plural use that vowel as its first vowel. It seems that such an analysis would lose much in.generality. l1'0302l.03e1 FmfiaLa-aE-EIB: Figure 31 details a context sensitive solution to the [paired body parts] plurals. It captures the general- ization that both plurals share the constituents 3;: and Eli: and differ only with respect to their initial preemptive vowels. On the other hand, it misses the generalization that one of these plurals is related in shape to the FMaaL ( [round] ) plurals. 181 Figure 31 FMaaLaat and FVMaaLaat Plurals (Semantically-based) Paired Body Parts ':n 182 Figure 32 details another context-sensitive solution which captures the generalization the first solution failed to capture but misses the generalization the first solution made (i.e., that both paired body parts plurals share 2l£)- The final Judge is the simplicity meaSure which prefers the first solution. A A‘L . A 6" use: (’N/rbu . 4M9 N \ a \ ¢ . s \\ D' \ \\\ \ A A 7MJ¢7\ rVJ4w1 ‘\\ \\ _ 51h mpg? due: a“ A.» ‘L v+ \\\ I We? A a‘“ ¢+ai+ Figure 32 Alternate Diagram to Figure 31 183 b.3.1.h Abstract Plurals The next semantic class described is [abstract]. There are four morphOIOgical shapes associated with this class: FaMaaLL, FaMaLaat. ?aFHaaL and FVMuuL. 4.3.1.“.1 FaMaaLL-txge xa7y5§¢5 \\ 4—0.4" :§:3¢s¢s \\ 'a ?¢M¢W\ 4+ a:* Figure 33 FaMaaLL P1ura1 (Semantically-based) Abstract — ’ 18h As an example take the item 'a7QQO (zagmgi' common man' ). The plural is 'a?flmflm . The -L (nisba suffix3) a Hat is not realized. because in the environment of plural the speaker does not generate the nisba suffix for this item. It is a determined constituent of the singular form. however. This is a different -i than that encountered in the FMaaLi plurals. Actually this (Lgamml) is the same surface form used for 'gz (paternal) uncle' (where the -i is a pronominal suffix). It is also used as a term of respect for any man. The phonotactics would interdigitate the preemptive vowels to yield the form Leilalimflim. The phononic shape would be iagagmn. “-3-1-“-2 Esflséss£:£xss Another [abstract] plural is FaHaLaat. Figure 3a describes this plural. The results are fairly straight- forward and need no further explanation. The plural of Zlflflfl (algae 'a service') is xadamaat. 185 A‘L A‘L N “Alma, A \ O \\ ‘0’ \ l / \ A Xialoém \ \ A ‘ 5 J0” ¢k :zn cut A 060." Figure 34 FaMaLaat Plural (Semantically-based) Abstract 186 403010403 7aFHaaL-tzge A third abstract] plural is ?aFMaaL. It is described in Figure 350 Figure 35 ?aFMaaL Plural (Semantically-based) Abstract 187 This plural class enables a prefix 3a- to be realized followed by the simultaneous realization of the singular item and the relevant part (fliéli) of the ‘round] plural pattern. The plural of ryggg (£1211 'livelihood'). for example, is (}a :¥£2% or ?arzaag. “03.10“.“ FWU‘JL‘txpe The last of the [abstract] plurals is FVMuuL. This is described in Figure 36. Figure 36 FVMuuL Plurals (Semantically-based) Abstract 188 Most abstract nouns belong to the derivational system. 1.e., they have their singular vowel pattern specified separately and simultaneously by the derivational system of the morphology. For example. Egg; 'doubt' is of the form _' §KK a9! Figure 37 An Example of a Noun in the Derivational System where the root Egg can be interdigitated with other vowel patterns to achieve verbal or adjectival stems, e.g.. 'to doubt' and 'doubt'. and afi is the vowel pattern for abstract nouns. Nouns like 32:29 'promise' still have the vowel pattern 22.“ However, whether one treats them as derivational or lexical (for simplicity of description) Preempting and interdigitation still function in the same manner 0 189 4.3.1.5 weak Plurals There are many [wean] plurals: FaMaLa, FuMuL. FiMiL, FuMuLaa?, FuMaLaa?. FiHLaan, FaMaaLi, FaMiiL, FaHaaMiiL, FiMMaL. FuMMaL, and FMaaMLa. These twelve classes can be generalized to eight stratificational morphemic signs. however. Some of them may be integrated with plurals we have encountered previously or will encounter on the following pages. Figure 38 describes the plural FaMaLa. h.3.1.5.1 FaMaLa-tzpe 0" (1* Figure 38 FaMaLa(at) Plurals (Semantically-based) Weak 190 The previous figure shows how the weak plural FaMaLa is integrated with the abstract plural FaMaLaat. The noun kaafiz and the class it represents have a derivation- ally determined singular vowel pattern a:i. The plural of 52; (kaafi; 'infidel') is kfr . The same inter- a:g 8'1 ++B a a digitation and preemption as before takes place yielding the form kafara. h.3.1.5.2 FvML-type Figure 39 shows the generalization of three plurals FuMuL. FiMiL. and FiMLaan to one stratificational morphemic pattern: i‘g_fl. Sometimes the i‘g can be generalized to a phonologically predictable alternation. but not in this case. 191 A O A O 1.. AL Gwaou/ \ \ . N A out im \ ,. .l C) .Ala 6i)‘.t \\ //.1‘ ’3’ / o A @ um n's @ a M‘ ? a 75 Sip“ 4”- A ~£: : (:Z“ if; \\ m‘é \\ \ an: El q H U|+ 95+ Figure 39 FvML Plurals (Semantically-based) Weak 192 (:> The nominals in this subclass take either the plural FiMiL or the plural FuMuL. Both plurals have a IQ pattern since the second vowel is inserted as a copy of the first Just in case different consonants occur contiguously word finally. (:) signifies a subclass under (:) which enables the preemptive vowel 2? to be realized. Both subclasses (:) and (:) have a simultaneous vowel sequence ea in the singular which is derivationally specified. (:) specifies the subclass which enables the FiMLaan plural pattern to be realized. The FiMLaan pattern has the same vowel sequence as the FiML pattern followed by a suffix gig. This suffix has the same shape as the ending on certain adjectives represented by the item (n's am) a5 (na'saan 'sleepy'). The subclass @ comprises items like which are derivational Lay (‘amya 'blind’). (we are using the a!!! feminine derivational vowel pattern here; the masculine vowel sequence (23) bears an anatactic relationship to the feminine (22): the masculine form is preceded by an affix 3a-) as well as items like (:) 2922; (Lag; 'mouse') which are lexical in nature. (Notice that they both have the same vowel pattern. however.) 193 h.3.1.5.3 FquLaa?-type Figure no generalizes two plural classes to one stratifica- tional morphemic vowel pattern. The second vowel of FuhuLaa? and FuMaLaa? is phonologically predictable. If the middle and last radical provide the proper environment a u is selected. otherwise an a is selected. For example. the plural of haqisr 'low, base' is huqura:? (W is realized as 2 between 1 and 1) and the plural of zabino 'troublesome' is xubaGafl’ (where V is realized as e)- .‘dl‘IIII-—- \\ AL A.» \ .. \ a? \ W" A b x9 A a,h u*9;+ Figure no FquLaa? Plurals (Semantically-based) Weak 191+ 4.3.1.5.4 FgJiaaLi-type Even though it appears at first glance that the plural FaMaaLi fits neatly into the FaMaaMi(i)L plural group. it proves simpler to set up a separate class at this point and integrate it with plural classes encounter- ed on the following pages, e.g., FaMaaLa, FaMaaL. FiMaaL, FiMaaLa. FuMaaLi: and the previously described FVMaaLaat. P‘umlg Sinfiu‘ws G (“v/clue! Pp. ; ® ‘ \\o g / / CD A \ \ / 0 \ / \ // ‘ / @A \ / % f, A r aT‘ been“ «\/ . l 4 wt da?¢k': \ V* W 07 az+ Figure 1&1 FaMaaLi Plural (Semantically-based) Weak ———————————————————————————————————————————____________T:::]IIIIIIIII" 195 Figure 41 illustrates the integration of the FaMaaLi [weak] plural with the FVMaaLaat [paired body part] plural encountered previously. (:) and.(:> define the FaMaaLi class as a whole with the general internal vowel sequence v+a:+ and the suffix 1i. (:> signifies the subclass represented by balflg (balwa 'affliction'). This subclass enables the vowel a: to be realized as the first vowel of the plural vowel sequence. The plural of balwa is Mlflg :)orm1§awi. 8H8! (:) signifies the subclass represented by da?gh; (daahiya 'disaster'). This subclass presents a small problem. In order to capture the generalization that the i is preserved in the plural. it is necessary to have this subclass not allow the suffix -1 to be realized. The 81nsular is @72hiza where y is permitted to be realized. The plural 18 \\ \\ \\ . (1+ 2+ u+ a+a:+ i”: Figure 52 FaMaaMVL Plurals (Semantically-based) Strong While all the members of the FaHaaMuL plural class Provide the correct environment for the choice of g: as the final vowel of the plural vowel sequence. this vowel cannot be 3: since many of the items taking the FaMaaMiL plural also provide the correct environment. 209 Thus the if—u+ dichotomy was set up. The plural of na7¢de (naadum 'dam') is nawaadum of 'as he ' ks ' ' ,_3 _£_s Les—r. army ) I 0 O agaakizg of qudsain (qursaan pirate ) qaraasina: of masfihusr (mashuur 'celebrity’) masaahiir. “03.1.8 Agent P1E3;8 There are six [agent] plural classes: FuMMaaL. FiMMaaL, FuMaat. FuMaLaa7, FaMaaMiLa and FMaaMiiL. Since the FuMMaaL and FiHMaaL plurals are phonologically pre- dictable alternants. there are actually only five different morphological shapes represented. 4.3.1.8.1 FVEEEQL-txgg Figure 53 describes this plural. The plural of W' (waa'id-' reacher') is " while that of syh 3% . P M an (saayih 'traveler') is siyyaah. The morphon MN/*/ enables “ g the C (reduplicated consonant) construction to be realized in the phonology. (See Figure 43. p. 197.) 210 Figure 53 FVMMaaL Plurals (Semantically-based) Agent “03.1.8.2 FuMaaE-tyge The FuMaat plural is another [agent] plural. The roots belonging to this class are biconsonanta1.5 The Singular items all have a determined -; suffix. while the Plural has a determined -§at suffix. The description is given in Figure 54. 211 Plurals A A C’Veuf/ A\ d 41 0 : \\_._.__ /’ 1“ \\ \ 5‘9 a- ' “1' Figure 5h FuMaat Plural (Semantically-based) Agent The plural of Jasbi (Jaabi 'collector') is jgp2§_. h.3.1.8.3 FuMaLaa'I-tme This plural is defined also by the gnostons GN/strong/ and GN/weak/. Figure 55 shows the integration. 212 “lager/1 A \\ 9 e, \ — \ \i A \ \ \ A; WZJ‘ A a i. 0:? A ur"+ Figure 55 FuMaLaa? Plural (Semantically-based) Agent The plural of gazing (waziir 'minister') is 122a3327. “ad-8.1+ mm It is evident that this plural can be integrated with the FaMaaMVL strong plurals and also with the FaMaaL- 213 type plurals. (We have only discussed the FaMaaLi weak plurals in this category). Figure 56 details this in- tegration. uv¢g¢1zk. \\ Wu1|¢’7¢K; fligiwgés CV06r V+ Figure 56 FaMaaMiLa Plural (Semantically-based) Agent 21a The plural of qurflsaan (qursaan 'pirate') is qaraasina. Q 0 : The glottal stop in gallg 'angel' and giglg 'clergyman' is peculiar. It is needed in order to insure the realization of E: (1) in the plural forms (of. malaa71ka and gasggwisa) but where one predicts a x realization. a 1 and 1 appear. In the singular the realization is neutralized with d and the phonetic tactics produces the correct results by inserting the epenthetic capy vowel for gala; and by not inserting anything where the final consonants are the same (of. giss). “0301.805 F M 111-1'; 0 This plural. the last of the [agent] plurals. is also characterized by squar§.[roung, and Enstrumeng. It has previously been integrated under b.3.1.2.1. Here another diagram is unnecessary since the only addition to the diagram cited above would be a class of items including muxgtaaz (mgtaar 'mukhtar') and rawguzs (:aaggug 'dancer') enabling the g: as the first preemptive vowel. This class is a subclass of those items enabling length (3) to be realized. h.3.1.9 Instrument Plurals The instrument plurals are three in numbers FaMaaMiL, FMaaMiiL and ?aFMiIam The first two have already been discussed in some detail under u.3.1.7 and 4.3.1.8. 215 “-3-1-9-1 W The FaMaaMiL-type includes such items as xanflja; (xanjaz 'dagger') and gargbas (£3122; 'reel'). The FMaaMiiL—type includes such items as migflgass (mingaag 'tweezers') and maggkuik (mankuuk 'bobbin'). Note that length is predictably preserved in the final vowel of the plural. “03.1.9.3 7aFM3La-tlpe The 7aFMiLa plural is diagrammed in Figure 57. It is integrated with the 7aFMiLaa? strong plural. The plural of aflnash (Jnaah 'wing’) is 7ajniha. —_—__L .____a .____L_ A ¢+i+ 0:? Figure 57 ?aFMiLa Plural (Semantically-based) Instrument 216 h.3.1.10 Location Plurals The two location plurals are FaMaaLi and FaMaaMiL both of which have been discussed with respect to other plural classes. They are detailed below. b.3.1.10.1 FaMaaLi-type Both FaMaaLi and FaMaaMiL can be integrated using the same technique as was used with the FMaaL-type round plurals. Figure 58 details the integration. ma +al¢laxa: \ A A Kanns / '1 A // Gib ‘I’ w— " . A ‘ \\ A.L tabs 9“ sat.» \\ 0* w [Jr Figure 58 FaMaaLi Plurals (Semantically-based) Location 217 The a and 2 forms appear on the surface as the same plural shape . The gnoston GN/location/ enables both the may prefix. and the particular plural class to be realized. (2&- does not occur with all location items. however.) Bi- (with gay) and tri-consonantal surface forms take the FaMaaLi plural (a subtype of FaMaaHiL) while tri- (with gay) and quadri—consonantal surface forms take the FaMaaMiL plural. It seems that perhaps the general ”locative“ singular pattern was 22 making the biconsonantal stems (i.e., underlying triconsonantal with final weak radical) appear to have a feminine suffix. and the general plural pattern was g;i_making the biconsonantal stems appear to have the suffix ~L. The ma- locative prefix was constant. While it would be simpler if one were analyzing only this one plural to treat it as described above. it proves simpler when viewing the total plural system to integrate this plural with previous analyses. such as FaMaaMuL, FMaaMiL. etc. 4.3.1.11 hale Plurals There are four [male] plurals: FuhLaan. FuMLa. FMuuLa and FiMaaMiiL. The morphological shape of the first three has not previously been discussed. The first two (FuMLaan and FuMLa) are morphologically related. All of these plurals are discussed below. 218 “03.1.1101 FuML-txge This type comprises two plurals FuMLaan and FuMLa. They are described in Figure 59. ¢ .‘L 1.; ‘ma/ 4 \\\\ " \ ‘ / / / / / A A / / / / \ __ \~ / AI. 73&? A V b ~0r3 EEE 2;;— 0:1 a3; G'Tl a u" 95+ Figure 59 FuML Plurals (Semantically-based) Male 219 The class of items enabling the FuMLaan plural to be realized is represented by 2;; (faaris 'knight'). Its plural is £2;saag. An examgl: of the items representing the FuMLa plural is 3; ? (15; 'brother'). The glottal stop is realized as fiainthasingular in the environment C___# and as g in the plural in the environment before the -g suffix. The plural is 3213;. “030101102 quuLa-txae This male plural type is morphologically related to one of the female types and will be integrated under 4.3.1.12.2. Figure 60 describes this plural. {6* uzf Figure 60 FMuuLa Plural (Semantically-based) Male 220 This plural is enabled to be realized by singular items of the general form FdeL. e.g.. gibgb (zibb 'penis') and M (m; 'bastard‘ ). The expected epenthetic insertion occurs when the final consonants are not the same. The plural of 21be is zbuuba. 4.3.1.11.3 FiflfliiL-tne This plural class has been described previously under the round plurals (of. 4.3.1.1.2). Figure 61 details the integration of the male variant. The plural of 17m (aflgasnsfiigaan 'neighbor') is Jiyaariin. a: or j?!” :H 5an with no Cond'ctionin5 en 43am») lenaJrh fl} Vf an!“ if : Figure 61 FiMaaMiiL Plural (Semantically-based) Male j 221 4.3.1.12 Female Plurals There is only one [femalel plural: FMuuL. It can be integrated with one of the [male plurals (FMuuLa) which is morphologically similar. Figure 62 shows the integration. 4.3.1.1201 FMuuL¢the Figure 62 FMuuL Plural (Semantically-based) Female 222 This If poses the same problem as encountered before. Sometimes the phonetic tactics inserts the expected copy of the first vowel in the correct environment: sometimes a non-alternating i, and sometimes 1 whose alternants are .1233- The plural of 9.51119 (La—1359. 'female breast') is nhuud. There are two [plant] plurals u FVMaaLaat and FMuuL . both of which can be integrated with previously encountered plurals. e.g.. FMaaLaat and FMuuLa. “0301.13e1 FVMBaIaat-tzae The FVMaaLaat-type is described in Figure 63. The plural of :2th (nabta ’plant') is papaataa . \ 91./lam” “fr- bon \ ' ’ par +/ A \ \ - \ “~ nab/t \*~., A‘L a: t A f— Figure 63 FVMaaLaat Plural (Semantically-based) Plant 4.3.1.13“? FHuuL-tzpe The second of the [plant] plurals and the final semantically based plural is FMuuL. It is the same moI'Phological shape as the female plural. Figure 64 224 shows the integration. The plural of hirfls (hiris 'plant') is hguus. ¢V'u:* Figure 64 FMuuL Plural (Semantically-based) Plant The plurals not discussed were either of an undeter- mined common semantic characteristic or did not comprise a major semantic class. These plurals would be integrated 44_____________fIIlIlllllllllllllllll'iiiifiw 225 and diagrammed in the same manner as the plurals presented herein in a fuller treatment. 4.3.1.14 Semantically-based Plurals -- Integrated This diagram describes not only an integration of all the semantically based plurals detailed above. but also the phenomenon of multiple plural association. That is. some items will be members of more than one class. Figures 65 - 75 present the overall morphological des- cription of semantically based broken plurals in Iraqi Arabic. Due to the size and complexity of the overall diagram. it had to be presented in sections. End points (or terminals) of one diagram can be matched with begin- ning points on succeeding diagrams. Figure 65 S-based Plurals Integrated 6”” o ca+i on/ I . Figure 66 Prefixes 228 . s ageij 01136 as ms. flame? \ A+$+ 52‘. I A¢$+u6 233V 352/1 . . A#,:+> 136? A+ +556 w\m&mfix +us$w£dwzz +uE+mc_svr/ r. 3.19:. 3. ts”. < «+61. > ”as“? “(Magi ‘ r s +6 . sassy. $«+....LS Listed. ‘ 016.7% XN>L Musttseva. §*1.X , .3, $3., 4 3, +33? ‘ { ‘ { ‘ numb Anne, 1‘ ‘ ‘ AoPcL .4qu sum.» Figure 67 Morphotactic Classes of Singulars (a) «1&s. "6* «1 +s2+sei :,x> (3v. him? fiSSoLV I ,. £33, a+>+u +a fisspwz r... 35 9 as. 9 matey, 9.3%.»8. & 4 , n+ia§oz 4. {a are; are $1.52 Q‘s/{Oz « > s .3. . WNW» e was at is , l, c, / +5§<£+nt~vl { m «afiwr as? m50 L% km . r. a > 3.37... 4 Ce, as a are, $3 4 Sewage {$539+ 9s.“ 4 ++.s+6 c.5300 I 4 tarsv/ 4 4 3, 4 4‘ means. / 9:43?“— 03V Sufi Figure 68 Morphotactic Classes of Singulars (b) a a. . sec m > x was A+ ..+:.< + if”) dQJ‘ns. M «+. V 5* as», vflJdi are: +01%; A+.:+> «to +HW>eu { A+ns+> fa, fer, a i .31 < .3 V, y, fl+>¥5 $03 ‘ ‘ A vus :. 9.th .3.” 1 memes ”A ”3% sumas‘ { AA u:++ Sissies. Aivu 3+$ { +s+.. 2 Wusswmv/ «26V / ++e oivxoiuwx +£s1w/ «Y! a U .4 14‘ p¢ws aiucl ‘ ‘ ‘ Anudv / AWNV { Figure 68a Morphotactie Classes of Singulars (c) +:.+U+us+> Jeans its .6 “3; “‘(‘y 231 +9 +5 #&.to+w +6.L I .‘ + . +6 +6 +> +$ +> +u6+§ ‘ +fl +> \L c\8\20 Figure 69 Corresponding Plural Classes (a) 232 ia.meie+>.e “ .ts+w +3 1, +.. .45 +is+e +~ +5 x +6 11“. 11“ As , _ >+5 +fl+3 +>+5+6 +U +> «T +& +: L“: +fi+u6+> 1.23 +> +6 in +K+HQ+Q 1‘ +§ dfil.______4[ ‘\\\\\ Figure 74 Pronominal Suffixes Figure 75 Feminine Suffix 44______—___________:III-IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII" 238 4.3.2 Phonologicglly-based Plurals In almost every plural class there are items which do not possess the semantic characteristic common to the majority of the members. They appear rather to have been assigned on the basis of phonological shape. This section details such items grouping them in order that predic- tions might be made based on the canonical form of the singular. Singular Plurals Freguency (i.e., number of items in FvMMvL FaMaaMiL 126 the data as- mm. 9 arms? FaMaaMiiL 3 FaMaaMuL 3 FMaaMiiL l FvMaaMiiL 1 FvML (fem) FvMaL 153 (mass) 7aFMaaL 95 FMuuL 29 FMaaL 19 FaMaaLi 25 FMaaLa 12 FuMuL 12 FMaaLi 12 FuMuuL 8 FiMLaan 4 FiMiL 4 FaMaaLi 4 ?aFM1La 3 FMuuLa 3 ?aFMuL 3 FaMaaMiL 3 Singglar FvML. FvavL (i.e., (i.e., (1080. (1080. FanL cont'd WsV?) WBV?) vvsv?) WSV?) Plurgg FiMaaL FiMuuL ?aFMiLaa? FiMLaat FMaaMiL FiMi FuMaaLi FaMaaMiL ?aFMiLa FuMuL ?aFiMLa FiMaaMiL FiMLaan FuMuLaa? 7aFMaaL FuMaaMiL FMaaMiL ?aFMiLaa? FaMaLa FuMLaan FiMLaat FMaaLa ?aFMaaL FMuuL FaMaaLa FuMuuL FMaaL FuMuuLaat 7aFMuL FiMuuL Freguencr .p ta H r4 H +4 N nit» N M)-P s'xacn N H r4 H thv N H '\) H t4 t4 c--: U\tm . maids; FvMaL FvavL (1090' WSV?) (1.90, WxV?) FVMMVVL (i.e., last 0 omitted) .FMva (1000. Vii-2V?) (lees. W3V?) FMVMMiL FvaL (i.e., VVaV?) 240 P1ura1§ ?aFMaaL FiMaaL FMaaLa FMaaMiL FaMaLa FiMLaan FaMaaMiiL ?aFMaaL FaMaaMiiL FMaaMiiL FiMaaMiiL FaMaaHiL FMaaMLa FuMaaMiiL FaMaaLa FaMaaLi FMaaLa FMaaMiL FuMuL FuMLaan FiMaaMiL FiMLaan FMaaMLa FMaaL FaMaaMiL Ereguencz 4 1 1 Id H h‘lv a 22 16 HHNNme‘ wawfl H 241 The above facts would be integrated into the broken plurals in the following manner. The less frequent of the plurals for a particular singular phonological shape will be the marked preemptive pattern. enabled to be realized by the few items comprising each class which takes a par- ticular marked plural. The most frequently occurring (i.e., the expected) plural for a particular phonological shape will be the unmarked plural for that phonological shape. Figures 76-78 detail the phonologically-based plurals. 4.3.2.1 Plurals of the ghonological Shape EzMMvL 4.3.2.1.1 FaMaaMiL and FaMaaMiiL-trpeg What Figure 76 details is the unmarked nature of the plural FaHaaMiL. thus explaining its relatively frequent occurrence as the plural for the singular shape thMvL. The choice of Q: or I: as the first preemptive vowel in this series is more marked than the choice of 5:. The final vowel is either the marked choice 3: or the un- marked 1: with "long" r being more marked than "short” l- If one takes the least marked path of the plurals for the singulars of the shape FvMMvL ( ) the resultant plural is FaMaaMiL. For example, the plural of laglag 'stork' is lagaalig. 242 A A 601W A“. \\ '\ \ \ \ 10> .‘L \\ \\ (:> \\ .‘L \\ \ ’ \ \\ \ \ \ e to \ \\ . \ \ x, \\ \+/ u+AI \\_${/V*a+ 49‘ < ‘+ I Figure 76 FaMaaMiL Plural (Phonologically-based) FvMMvL 213 4.3.2.2 Plurals for the Phonological Shape FvML 4.3.2.2.1 FvMaL-tme This plural type poses a slight problem since the first vowel of the plural is the same as the first vowel of the particular singular item taking this plural. Figure 77 details this plural type. Figure 77 FvMaL Plural (Phonologically-based) FvML 244 The blank (_) is not a preemptive. Instead the normal vowel takes precedence over it. This is detailed in the phonotactics. The plural of nisgb (nigba 'relation- ship') is nisab. All of the singular items in this class are grammatically feminine. i.e.. they have a suffix -3. “0302.202 7aFMaaL-txpe The second most frequent plural type for the singular phonological shape FvML is ?aFMaaL. This plural is de- tailed in Figure 78. All of the items entering into this plural type are grammatically masculine. A II . 1‘ C”é+xsa£/ 1.. “L ‘ii I' A \ \_ 1 \ P‘") \ qu¢§ ? a his¢t> 4.. AL ¢+a:+ O\+ Figure 78 7aFMaaL Plural (Phonologically-based) FvML 2l&5 The remainder of the plurals for the phonological shape FvML are diverse but may be grouped under four major plural types: FaMaaMiL, FvMuuL. FiML and FHvL. These are the marked types for this particular singular phonological shape and would be integrated as choices under the first line of a downward ordered 2;. Particular nominal subclasses would enable certain plural subclasses within these types to be realized. 4.3.2.3 0the ho 010 all -ba ed P1 ls The remainder of the phonologically-based plurals would be integrated as above with the most frequently occurring being the unmarked choice for the particular singular shape. Since they are quite straightforward they need not be detailed here. 4.3.3 Summary This section (4.3). in dealing with the morphology of both the semantically-based and phonologically-based broken plurals of Iraqi Arabic, has been complete insofar as possible and insofar as the data studied are concerned. Due to time and space limitations a few of the descriptions were succinct. leaving minor details and previously ex- plained material to be filled in by the reader. 4.4 The Phonotactics The phonotactics must account for the following phenomena in order to be compatable with the suggested treatment of plurals: interdigitation of consonants and vowels. preemption of singular vowels by plural vowels. and 246 syllable structure. Since there are no consonant clusters at the phonological level. the phonotactics does not have to account for them. Consonant clusters may however appear on the phonemic sign level as a result of the empty realiza- tion of t vowels. For example. PS/bflussha/ 'with her money' from gg 'with' followed by 351313 'money' followed by fig ‘her'. CV A preview of the general structure of the phonotactics has been given in discussions in the section of morphology (of. 4.3.1.5.5) Since it is the responsibility of the phonotactics of Iraqi Arabic to account for syllable structure. interdigitation and preempting. we must structure our model so as to include these phenomena. Figures 79-84 detail the phonotactics in general. 24? I Q (a) o t ' Lu Yo @ O 0 ~ (u) (a) Lb Fr Figure 79 The Phonotactics Figure 80 Liquids and Glides 249 Figure 81 Vowels (Preemptive. Long and "Normal") 250 @ o O 0 I I- o ’ I 4' 4r 0 0 0 (e a) 01> <9) . ’ Lb Fr C" SF ‘7 «’ E2. Af’ 7>kr Figure 82 Fricatives 251 Figure 83 Stops (M) 252 (K) Figure 84 Nasals N5 253 The diagram above (Figure 81) accounts for two different types of syllable structure: a long vowel alone, e.g.. 2; ‘and'. 1; 'yes'. or the now familiar CV(CV)...(C) pattern. With the CV(CV)...(C) pattern it thereby accounts also for interdigitation since it can only accept a series of consonants and vowels in that order. Thus. receiving simultaneously the sequences £22 and 15;, the phonotactics can only supply the order kitasb 'book'. The phenomenon of preemption is accounted for by the downward ordered 2r over the vowels. The marked series are the preemptive vowels. That is. first the tactics checks to see of any preemptive vowels have been generated by the morphotactics. If so. then the preemptive vowel is realized and the vowel it preempts is realized as fl (in the morphonic alternation pattern above the phonotactics). Only if there is no preemptive vowel does the “normal" vowel get realized. The preemptive vowels neutralize with their ”normal" counterparts below the phonotactics in the phonemic alternation pattern. The case of non-preemption (i.e.. the blank (_)) is handled as in Figure 85. 254 MN/f/ Preamf+th *0 to normal " voWels Figure 85 Phonotactic Realization of the Morphon MN/_/ The tactics first checks to see if MN/__/ has been generated: if so. instead of a preemptive vowel. the "normal" vowel takes precedence. Otherwise the tactics functions as described above. An example is the FVMaL 255 plural. The plural of xidma 'a service' is xidflm . + _a The phonotactics. on receiving this configuration. allows xrda+m. The blank insures that the singular vowel takes precedence. Thus the plural r;___. The phenomenon of consonantal reduplication (or gemination) has been discussed previously (cf. 4.3.1.5.5). but we will reiterate it here. If the morphology has generated MN/*/, then a reduplicative construction in the phonology is allowed to be realized. See page 197 for figure and examples. This is also illustrated in Figure 79 where the MN/*/ enables the reduplicated consonant construction to be realized. 4.5 Intervenrgg Realrzatronal Phenomegg Some of the realizational phenomena which were mentioned informally in morphological discussions are formalized here. He mentioned before that the preemptive vowels neutralize with their "normal" counterparts. This configuration is part of the phonemic alternation pattern (below the phonotactics). Figure 86 details this. 256 _ . \ \ o a a“. .9' ‘ LhVo / a u I Figure 86 Alternation and Neutralization of the "Normal" and Preemptive Vowels Figure 86 also shows that if the phonotactics does not accomodate y: (in case there are less than four radicals). then it is realized as t in the morphonic alternation pattern. That is, y: in the plural pattern £211: is only realized when there are four radicals. There are three additional alternation phenomena. One of these occurs in the morphonic alternation pattern (above the phonotactics) and is detailed in Figure 8?. This figure describes the alternate realizations of glottal 257 6 stop: length (s), frontality (y). labiality (w), and 7 glottality (?). The environments are stated rather than diagrammed: they would occur as part of the phonotactics. MN/7/ marl—e; 0.8 # m, excefhoml l a,» PHoumvmtns Figure 87 Alternate Bealizations of Glottal Stop The second alternation is that of‘ggii or 1 described in Figure 88. It occurs in the phonemic alternation pat- tern. The environment forms a ”natural class" in the sense that all of the items enabling £2 (labial) to be realized are performed with a raiSed tongue back as is the vowel g. [ 258 This alternation must occur in the phonemic alternation pattern (below the phonotactics) since the specification of 1 as i or 2,must succeed u.e.. occur lower in the realizational chain than) the realization of glottal stOp as x or g. (If this were not the case we could handle the alternation in the morphonic alternation pattern as in Figure 89. PuoucrrACchs Pfiouevurc. ALTsauAnou PA'n'eRN Figure 88 Alternate Realizations of 1 Figure 89 Possible Alternate Analysis of 1 The third alternation phenomenon occurs in the phonemic alternation pattern. This phenomenon. the realiza- tion of glottal stop as 19 (a). must be spec- ified as occurring in the environment Of ¢C #, as in the phonotactics consonants do not occur contiguously. 260 / WONEMtC. / / / ' ALTERNA‘I‘XON PATTERN v0 (5' Figure 90 Realization of Glottal Stop as a Another phenomenon which occurs in the phonemic alternation pattern is the related phenomenon of the realization of 1 and 1 as g and 1. respectively, in the environment ¢C____#. This entails only the generation of 12 as a determined element in this environment. Note that all the so-called weak radicals (1.1.1) are specified as Vo(calic) in this environment (¢C___#). An example of this is P/filw/ 'colt'. The phonemic sign realiza- tion of this item is zilu. 261 u.6 The Phonetic Tactics Since the phonetic tactics has never been formalized, the types of phenomena it handles with respect to our analysis of broken plurals will only be explained verbally. There are two related phenomena which it is assumed that the phonetic tactics handles. One is the insertion of a determined vowel (1) between the first two consonants of a three consonant cluster. The other is the insertion of a determined vowel (usually ;, sometimes 3 or a) between two different consonants which occur phonological word finally. The first is a quite general Arabic phenomenon (i.e.. it occurs throughout the dialects). An example of it is PS/bf1ussak/ 'with your money'. realized on the sur- face as bifluusak. Often speakers of Arabic as a native language insert this vowel in English words containing triconsonantal clusters. for example, [lipistik] ‘lipstick'. The second phenomenon is a characteristic of Iraqi Arabic but not of all the dialects. e.g.. not of Egyptian Arabic. It is the vowel insertion between dissimilar consonants phonological word finally. for example, filim (Iraqi) versus film (Egyptian). -—-_- The phonetic tactics also handles phenomena like phonetic assimilation, but this has no real bearing on the general problem of broken plurals. 262 u.7 Implications for the getatheory The major implication for the metatheory which this study has made is that language is a much more integrated and much less well stratified system of relationships than it was previously thought to be. While stratification is still an important concept upon which to base the theory, the requirement that units on one level relate only to the units of the 1evel(s) immediately contiguous is too strong. It needs to be made less stringent, in order to allow relationships between units on non-contiguous levels. This study has utilized the less stringent requirement in the analysis of Iraqi broken plurals. It has posited a direct relation- ship between portions of the gnostotactics and the mor- photactics in accounting for the semantically-based broken plurals. The less stringent requirement was used in this analysis in order to achieve descriptive adequacy. In other words, the data itself requires the analyst to posit direct links between semantics and morphology. It must necessarily follow that the theory, in order to maintain explanatory adequacy. must allow this less stringent requirement (i.e., that direct relationships may exist between non-contiguous levels). For, in order for a theory to be explanatorily adequate, it must first allow descriptively adequate grammars to be written and secondly, it must present a means of choosing the simplest 263 of n descriptively adequate grammars. The empirical evidence presented by the data from Modern Iraqi Arabic thus requires the theory of stratifica- tional grammar to incorporate a less stringent requirement regarding the existence of direct relationships between non-contiguous levels. That is. the theory must change requirement 1 (Hg 1) to requirement la (Rq la): Rq 1: Direct relationships only exist between ”units“ on immediately contiguous levels. Bq lat Direct relationships usually exist between “units" on immediately contiguous levels. In some cases (as required by the particular description) direct relationships may exist between ”units“ on non-contiguous levels. FOOTNOTES Chapter h 1. In these and all following examples V represents the vowel alternation ifl’u. while v represents any vowels V1 - the same vowel as in the singular pattern. 2. A problem arose in constructing Figure 15 in that the fact that one member of each dichotomy is a marked choice could not be reconciled with the fact that un- marked choices also have connections to the predication structures. Therefore the downward gas appear as unor- dered. If this problem could be resolved, then an analysis in which the downward 25s were ordered would be preferred. 3. The nisba suffix is a suffix indicating relationship, for example. someone from. of. or in the ”relationship" of nation to national with the country of Iraq is termed Iragi. where the -; is the nisba suffix. #. It should be recalled that the phonetic tactics has a restriction on dissimilar consonants finally. and in- serts an epenthetic copy of the first vowel in such cases. 5. These are traditionally analyzed as based on ”defective roots according to Ernest N. McCarus, and so are considered triconsonantal according to such an analysis. 26h 265 6. The present analysis uses glottal stop (1) as the under- lying representation in many instances where other analyses have used underlying g. z or (g, i). (Cf. Brame (1970) and Levy (1971)). 7. This analysis misses the generalization that g is more "marked” than i. In order to capture this. the analysis would have to show 3 as having more components :3 than _1_, e.g.. ;_ - P/Hi/ and ‘_ = P/Hi /. Since the / Lb/ component 31 is not necessary elsewhere in the phonology and the component E; is, the analysis given (; 2 £1: E = _Q) was chosen. CHAPTER V Residue 5.1 Theory of Exceptions Levy's (1971) dissertation on noun plurals in Modern Standard Arabic provides a good example of the use of the theory of exceptions as posited by Lakoff (1965). Nouns which prove "exceptions“ are accounted for in one of four ways: 1) ordering. i.e.. by ordering two rules so that after the first has applied to an item. the item no longer meets the structural description of the rule following. (The later rule is the more general). 2) -rule i] . i.e.. minus rule lexical features. or marking lexical items so that they do not undergo a partic— ular rule for which they meet the structural description. (They may undergo a later more general rule for which they also meet the SD.) 3) suppletive. i.e.. truly exceptional items which are spelled out idiosyncratically in the lexicon. h) minor rules. i.e.. [} qatal Pl] . These mark exceptional plurals in that they are not the expected plural for the particular semantic and/or canonical form. These apply optionally if they are an alternate plural for the given item. 266 267 5.2 A Stratificational Treatment Since there are no rules (but only relationships) in a stratificational description. none of these notions on the handling of syntactic irregularity is apropos in the case of the present analysis. Markedness in the sense of downward ordered gas does play a role in stratificational grammar. The most frequent (or the expected) plural for a particular semantic or canonical subclass is interrelated in an unmarked manner as the last branch of a downward ordered 21. The less frequent plurals for such a class are related via the more marked (leftmost) branches and only occur when they are enabled to be realized by the small subclass of items which takes them. Multiple (or optional alternate) plurals are handled by multiple class membership. That is, an item taking more than one plural will simultaneously be a member of several morphOIOgical classes. The plural which is actually generated will depend on several factors (i.e., whether the speaker is basing his plural on semantic grounds or on canonical shape. or whether the speaker chooses a more marked alternant as opposed to a less marked one with respect to various broken alternants or with respect to a broken versus a sound alternant). There are a few items whose plurals are truly exceptional in that they serve as the plural for Just the given item. These plurals will be shown to be "exceptions" by the fact that the class which enables their realization contains only vr:::lIIllIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIiIiIIIIIIIIIIIIiiiiF? f 268 one member. They are an integral part of the system of relationships nevertheless. The following nominals were found to be classifiable as exceptions. Singular Plural ?ustaad ?asatida ?umm 7ummahaat badir bduuraat *xara xaryaan saa?il saa?il “saffaar safafiir sana siniin *‘adu 'aadaa? *‘amya (f.) 'imyiin *qiss qissiisiin *qasmar qasaamra qanaat qanawaat mara niswaan Glgsg professor mother' seed feces liquid coppersmith year enemy blind clergyman fool canal woman Starred items have alternant non-exceptional plurals. An attempt is made to describe the treatment of each. The plural of 7us§aad 'professor' will be related to the FaMaaMiLa agent plural but will enable length following the second vowel to be realized as fl as illus- trated in Figure 91 yielding 7agatida. 269 \ \ Figure 91 Exceptional Plural 7asatida The plural of ?umm 'mother' is a sound feminine plural. The only idiosyncracy is that ah must be enabled to be realized as a determined non-final suffix in the environment of plural for this particular item as in Figure 92 yielding 7gmmahaat. 270 Figure 92 Exceptional Plural ?ummah§at The plural of bgdiz 'seed' (also baziz) is predictable inasmuch as it belongs to the gun plant plural type (FMuuL). It is exceptional in that it requires the suffix -aat. This is shown in Figure 93. 271 Gu/P lau+/ \___'_’l: // / / dti (>445? ¢ u: Figure 93 Exceptional Plural bdugraat The exceptional plural of gar; 'feces' is xagyaan. The non-exceptional broken plural is xiryaag (weak, low). (It also has a sound plural xaryaat.) The plural 22125;; can be viewed as exceptional in the choice of the first plural vowel as described in Figure 94. Figure 9h Exceptional Plural xazygag The plural of gaggi;_‘liquid' is the same form as the singular. It can be handled the same way as the English plural of bee , i.e.. by letting the singular item enable the plural morpheme to be realized as t as illustrated in Figure 95. 273 GN/S Joasei/ P—ba: eJ Figure 95 Exceptional Plural saa?;1 The exceptional plural of saffaar 'coppersmith' can ‘ a_____. be integrated with the FaMaaMiiL plural type. The singular enables length following the second vowel to be realized as I! as in Figure 96 yielding safafiir. / .I———— 274 Figure 96 Exceptional Plural safafiir a_______ The plural of gap; 'year' is detailed below. This plural is unusual in that it takes the plural suffix -iin and simultaneously fails to have final consonantal material realized in the plural stem. This second phenomenon was detailed previously (see pp.l7H-5). The plural is siniin. P~bas Cc, GN 4b“ eel / / / .d/ _i:|’\ Figure 97 Exceptional Plural siniin The exceptional plural of 'adu 'enemy' is 'aadaa?. It is similar in exceptionality to gang above in that it not only takes a suffix but fails to realize a final stem consonant. Figure 98 describes this plural. FLBaseA C%'bqsed/ Figure 98 Exceptional Plural 'aadaa? The exceptional plural of lgmxg (f.) 'blind' may be integrated with the FiML-type weak plural and is ex- ceptional only in that it takes the suffix -iin. Figure 99 details the integration. The plural generated is :igyiig. Figure 99 Exceptional Plural ‘imxiin The exceptional plural of gigs 'clergyman' may be analyzed as a sound masculine plural with an exceptional affix -;i§r. FigurelOO illustrates this. The plural generated is gigsiigi; . Figure 100 Exceptional Plural gisgiigiin The exceptional plural of gaémag 'fool' may be integrated with the FMaaMLa-type weak. low plural. It is exceptional only in that it has a as the first vowel rather than E. Figure loldetails this yielding the plural g a éaamra a 6%eak/ ¢7ag¢nM1r \ \ \ \ \ \ a a; Q! Figure 101 Exceptional Plural gasaamra The plural of ganaat 'canal', ganagaat. is open to several alternate analyses. Firstfi it is possible that it is a sound feminine plural: singular. gana? i t (fem- inine suffix): plural. gana? + sat (feminine plural suffix) in which case it would not be exceptional. Second. it is possible that it is unexceptionally associated with the broken plural FaMaLaat in which case the final 3 of the singular fails to get realized. Third, it is possible to analyze it as an exceptional plural of the form FaMaMaaL. In case it is analyzed as an exception. it would be handled as in Figure 102. 280 P~bA§eJ 6%“:ks eJ / Figure 102 Exceptional Plural ganagaat The plural of £212 'woman'. piswaan. is interesting. Synchronically it appears as a suppletive plural completely unrelated to the singular form. If one looks at the underlying radicals (2:1 and Egg) it is not too difficult to construct a plausible diachronic history given the strong relationship between the nasals and the affinity of z and g in historical linguistics in general. However. in a synchronic study such as this. one is compelled to treat the forms as unrelated. The plural of mar; would be 444-______‘_______________________________:3IIIIIIEEEEEEFT'" 281 handled as in Figure 103. Thus in the environment of plural the morpheme alternant gigflz is realized. This particular morpheme enables the exceptional plural _aun to be realized. thus yielding pigfll asn for the plural. The blank insures that nisg? is realized and the suffix ash is then appended. LN/PI / (ginfluhrs) LEX mm, A MOWCYKS ALTan-ms PmrEEN , Figure 103 Exceptional Plural niswaan FOOTNOTES Chapter 5 1. It is this interpretation which is most commonly given in Arabic grammars. The suffix -t is posited as the feminine suffix (rather than -;t as is commonly given for underlying forms using E and 1 (or g and i). for example, 9355! -;t) on the grounds that -t is used elsewhere in the system to designate the grammatically feminine, for example. compare 222 'son' and 212 -3 'daughter'. 282 CHAPTER VI Conclusion This dissertation. in substantiating the hypothesis that the morphology of plural formation in Arabic re- veals an underlying multi-gender system upon which the more recent morphological/agreement genders seem to have been superimposed. makes wider claims for the Semitic language family in general. Talmy Givén in suggesting the extension of his hypothesis from the Bantu language family to the Semitic acted as the catalyst for this study. The research and reported results are original. The study has made implications for the analysis of Arabic both synchronic and diachronic in substantiating the semanticemorphological corres- pondence of the broken plural system. It has also made implications for the theory of stratificational grammar in positing a less rigidly stratified model to achieve a more descriptively adequate analysis of the Iraqi Arabic broken plural system. Levy (1971). while presenting a thorough phonologically- based description of Modern Standard Arabic broken plurals, failed to note the semanto-morphological relation- ships which exist within the system. The present study improves on a treatment such as Levy‘s 1) by noting the semanto—morphological relationships which exist and 2) by 283 28h explaining some of the peculiarities of the system (e.g.. multiple plural association) by means of culturally- perceived semantic characteristics. There seems to be a partial meaning/form correspondence extractable from the system. For example. tri-consonantal plural patterns FHaaL are all associated with the FMaaLa FMaaLi semantic component GN/round/. Thus one might say that the broken plural pattern 25; signifies rotundity. Also. triconsonantal plural patterns FMuuL are FMuuLa both associated with the semantic component GN/genitalia. body parts/. Similarly one might say that the broken plural pattern 22; signifies procreativity. No attempt was made in the present study to discover to what extent such meaning/form correspondences could be said to exist. 6.1 Unresolved Problems There are several generalizations which, while easily stateable in an informal manner. were difficult to formalize due to the nature and amount of the data. One is the fact that a number of the broken plurals share a or a; as a final plural preemptive vowel. It was decided in this presentation to keep the diagrams readable rather than to try to capture this generaliza- tion formally. Several anatactic relationships were also not dealt with satisfactorily: the £2 [round] singulars and their flat plural counterparts. and the fig masculine adjectives ‘W 285 of color and defect with their a! feminine counterparts. Anataxis in its usual sense is used to describe different or opposite ordering of constituents between one stratum and another. Both of the anatactic phenomena described above. however. appear on the same stratum, and it is not clear Just how such Jixtaposition is handled within the same stratum. 6.2 Suggestions for Further Research There needs to be much more work along the lines developed in this study using other dialects of Arabic and other Semitic languages to verify and perhaps broaden the hypotheses made herein. When more information is gathered from other dialects. perhaps a more fully detailed hypothesis can be made on the semantic-morphological structure of broken plurals in the Proto-language. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Azzawi. Theresa. 1969. ”A Stratificational Discussion of Stress in Modern Iraqi Arabic.” MS. Bateson, Mary Catherine. 1967. Apabic Language Handbook. Washington, D.C. Bennett, David C. 1969. Spapial and Temporal Uges of English Prepositions. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. ale University. Blanc, Haim. 196U. Commuppl Dialects of Baghdad. Boston. Mass. Brame, Michael K. 1970. Arabic Phonology. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, M.I.T. Chafe, Wallace. 1970. Meaning and the Stpucture of Langppg . Chicago. Chomsky. Noam. 1965. Agpects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge. Mass. and Morris Halls. 1968. The Sound Paptern pf Engligh. New York. Clarity, B.E.. Karl Stowasser. and Ronald G. Wolfe, eds. 196D. A Dictionary 0: Iragi Ayabics English-Ayabio. Washington, D.C. Erwin, Wallace M. 1963. Referenge Gpammap o: Iragi Apabig. Washington, D.C. 1969. A Basic Course in Iraqi Apabic. Washington. D.C. Fillmore, Charles. 1968. "The Case for Case" in Univepsals in Linguistic Theory, Bach and Harms. eds. New York. Fleisch, Henri. 1961. T aité de hilolo ie a abe. Vol. I (Preliminaires. phonLtique, morphologie nominale). Beirut. 286 287 Givén, Talmy. 1971. "Some Historical Changes in the Noun Class System of Bantu; Their Possible Causes and Wider Implications" in Papers in African Lingpis- tics. Urbana. Gleason, H. A. 1965. Linguistics and English Grammar. New York. Goodenough, Ward H. 1956. "Componential Analysis and the Study of Meaning." Language 32.195-216. Greenberg, Joseph. 1955. "Internal p—Plurals in Afro- Asiatic (Hamito-Semitic)." Verbffentlichun en des Instituts fur Orientforschung, No. 26. Berlin. 1966. "Language Universals" in Cpppgpp Tpends in Linguistips III. pp. 61-112. The Hague. Hjelmslev. Louis. 1961. Prolegomena to a Theory of Langpage. (Translated by Francis J. Whitfield . Madison. Wis. Hockett, Charles F. 1961. ”Linguistic Elements and Their Relations.” Langpage 37.29-53. 1968. The State of the App. The Hague. Hyman. Larry. 1970. ”How Concrete is Phonology?" 11.!!Eulge “'6 a 58-76 a Ikegami, Yoshihiko. 1970. The Semological Structure of the En lish Verbs of Motion: A Stratificational Approach. Tokyo. 1971. "A Stratificational Analysis of the Hand Gestures of Indian Classical Dancing." Semiotica b.365-91. Katz, Jerrold J. and Jerry A. Fodor. 1963. ”The Structure of a Semantic Theory." Langpgge 39.170-210. Katz. Jerrold J. and Paul Postal. 196k. An Integrated Theor of Lin uistic Descri tions. Research Monograph No. 2%. Cambridge, Mass. Killean, Mary Carolyn G. n.d. ”Interesting Featuresnof Gender-Number Concord in Modern Literary Arabic. MS. Kiparsky, Paul. 1968a. ”How Abstract is Phonology?" MS. 1968b. "Linguistic Universals and Linguistic Change" in Universals in Linguistic Theory, Bach and Harms, eds. New York. Kurylowicz, Jerzy. 1961. L'Apophonie en Sémitigue. Wroclaw, POl‘ndp 288 Lakoff. George. 1965. On the Nat e of S ntactic I e lar t . Cambridge ' M188 0 e 1969. "Empiricism without Facts." Foundations of Language 5.518-27. Lamb, Sydney M. 1966a. ”Prolegomena to a Theory of Phonology.” Langpage “2.536-73. 1966b. Outline of Stpatificatiopgl Grammag. Washington. D.C. 1969. "Lexicology and Semantics" in Linguistics Today, Archibald A. Hill. ed. New York. 1971a. "The Crooked Path of Progress in I Cognitive Linguistics." Geor eto Mono ra h S es on Langpages and Lipguisticg, 2‘. 1971b. "Linguistic and Cognitive Networks" in Cognition: A Multiple View. Paul Garvin. ed. New York. 1972. "Some Types of Ordering" in Phonological Theopy: Evolution and prrent Practicg. V. B. Makkai. ed. New York. Lekiaschwili, A. S. n.d. ”Zur Bildung des gebrochenen Plurals im Arabischen.” 25th International Congress of Orientalists, Proceedings. Vol. II. 136-Ul. Levy, Mary M. 1971. The Plural of the Noun in Mpdern Standard Arabic. npublished Ph. . Dissertation, University of Michigan. Lockwood, David G. 1969. "Markedness in Stratificational Phonology." Language h5.300-8. 1972a. Iptroduction to Stratificational Linguistics. New York. 1972b. "Neutralization, Biuniqueness, and Stratificational Phonology” in Phonological Theory: Evolution and Current Practice. V. B. Mak 1. ed. New York. 1973. ”Replacives without Process" in A Reader in Stpatificational Linguistics. A. Makkai and D.G. Lockwood. eds. University. Ala.. Lounsbury, Floyd G. 1956. "A Semantic Analysis of the Pawnee Kinship Usage." Language 32.158-19h. 289 Lyons, John. 1968. Introduction to Theoretical Lipgpistics. Cambridge. Makkai, Adam. 1972. Idiom Structure in Epglish. The Hague. McCarthy, R.J.. S.J. and Faraj Raffouli. eds. 1969. Spoken Apabic o: Baghdad: An Apthology of Texts. Part Two A . Baghdad. Murtonen. A. 1964. Broken Plurals: Origin and Development of the Sygtem. Leiden. Postal. Paul M. 1968. Aspects of Phonological Theopy. New York. Reich, Peter A. 1968. "Symbols, Relations, and Structural Complexity." Linguistics Automation Project. Yale University, New Haven. Conn.. Rice, Frank A. and Majed F. Sa'id. 1966. Eastern Arabic. Beirute Sampson, Geoffrey. 1970a. "On the Necessity for a Phonological Base Component.” Langppge 46. 586 -626. 1970b. Stratifipgtional Gpammar: A Definition and an Example. Janus Linguarum Series Minor 88. The Hague e Saussure. Ferdinand de. 1959. Course in General Lingpistics. New York. Sobleman, Harvey. 1962. Arabic Dialect Spudies: Bibliogyaphy. Washington, D.C. Sullivan. William J. 1969. A Stratificational Description of the Phonolo and In leot onal Mor hole of Russ an. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. Yale University. Trubetzkoy, N. S. 1969. Principles of Phonolpgy. Berkeley. Van Ess, John. 1938. The Spoken Arabic of Ipag. London. (2nd edition). Weinreich, Uriel, William Labov and Marvin Herzog. 1968. "Empirical Foundations for a Theory of Language Change“ in Directions for Histopical Linguistics. Austin. Tex. Woodhead, D. B. and Wayne Beene, eds. 1967. A Dictionapy of Iraqi Arabic: Arabic-Epglish. Washington, D.C. 44‘_‘__________TIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIEVwn 290 Wright, William W. 1896. A Grammgr of the Arabic Language. 3rd edition. 2 Volumes. Cambridge. APPENDIX Questionsble Plural Classes APPENDIX Questionable Plural Classes These plural classes are questionable either 1) because their defining semantic characteristics are too numerous or 2) because the number of items com- prising a particular plural class are too few. ?aFMuL Sem Phop ? FvaL This class does not seem to have a semantic character- istic and its P-shape for the singular is a quite common one. All the items of the class are listed here. saham 'share' bahar 'sea' sahsr 'month' nafis 'soul' FaMaaLa em Phon ? FaMiyya FaMLaan These items seem to be grouped on the basis of P-shape. There is no apparent reason for gunuch to be in this class. All the items of this class appear here. 291 292 xaliyya 'cell' sariyya ’company' dahiyya 'blood sacrifice’ qadiyya 'legal case' maziyya 'merit' kaslaan 'lazy' nasraani ’Christian' ‘xisi 'eunuch' FuMuLaa? §sm 2mm 7 (weak) FaMi iL There does not appear to be any semantic character- istic for the two members of this class. The plural P- shape is morphologically related to other [weak] plural classes. however. The singular P-shape FaMiiL is common to both members of this class. hakiim 'wise' haqiir 'low, base' FuMaaLi Sem Phon ? (strong) ? These are the only two members of this class. There does not seem to be any semantic or phonological Justifica- tion for the class. The plural P-shape is morphologically related to other {strong} classes. however. 293 mulls 'tutor' (in Islam) fuxtiyya 'turtledove' FuMLa Sem Phon 7 (male) ? The same remarks apply to this class as to the one above. The plural P—shape of this class is morphologically related to other [male plural classes, though. ?ax 'brother' gariib 'strange' FuMuuLaat Sem Phon ? FvaL These items have no semantic characteristic in common. They have no other plural. wasil 'receipt' dasam 'fat' FiMLaat Sem Phon [meal] Fva This appears to be a mixed class. There are five members total and these are the only two related items. They form a minor class [meal] . 'aga 'dinner' gada 'lunch' FaMaaL a FaMaat Sem Phon [female 7 relative ‘- or<] This at first appeared to be a broken plural class. but on closer inspection. it seems to be a regular fem- inine plural (-aat). Both hint and ?uxut have related masculine forms: 2% 'son' 2% t 'daughter' 2% 'brother' 2% t 'sister' where -p is a feminine suffix. The plural of the feminine is the masculine stem plus the regular suffix -352. In the singular there is a predictable epenthetic vowel in- sertion between dissimilar consonants in final position and a predictable vowel alternation inJu (V). The only problem arises with the plural of 3; t 'sister' which is xawpap. The radicals are in an anaZactic relationship with those of the singular. In order to achieve the cor- rect plural, the underlying form for the feminine plural must be ;3 instead of 2; . Glottal stop is normally real- a a ized as p in the environment given. bint 'daughter' 7uxut 'sister' Sg. minus -1 m Phon [nationality] ? This is probably a fairly recent plural class and it cannot be called a broken plural at all. for the plural is merely the singular minus the (nisba or "relationship") suffix -;. There is no different vowel interdigitation. It is strictly a semantically- based plural class. ?armani 'Armenian' ?a1maani 'German' ?amriikaani 'American' ?ingiliizi 'Englishman' sruugi 'person from the rural areas of Southern Iraq' baanyaani 'Hindu' turmaani 'Turkoman' ruusi 'Russian' 'arabi 'Arab' FiMaL Sem Phon [square] FvMLa This may be a subclass of FVMaL. The fourth member of this class is button. sayla “woman's long head scarf' xayma 'tent' daéca 'low hedge' Afl____________________________::::1IIIIIIIIIIIIIEIIEEEIIIIIIIIEEEEEEFT 296 FMaaLiin [square] FvMMva(a) FvavL All of the items of this class are here listed. Two of the members may also be analyzed as belonging to the FMaaMiiL plural class. leaving haayit as an a____a exceptional plural. haayit 'wall' dukkaan 'shop' darbuuna 'alley' FaMaaMiiL Sem Phon [round] FvMMva(a) Esq uare] FvMMvL ( a ) Eow, base, wean] Etrong, high] Etatement] This is a plural class which gives evidence of more than a few characteristic semantic features and which is therefore probably a P—shape plural class. The possibility of having morphological overlapping of separate corresponding semantic classes was considered in cases of two or three defining semantic characteristics. Given the fact that this class has more than several and the fact that there are large numbers of items associated with this plural 297 which are not defined by the given semantic characteris- tics but which do correspond to the given canonical forms. it seems more probable that this class is defined by phonological characteristics. Five-sixths of the total number of items are accounted for here. Round -- 1/3; square. weak. strong -- 1/7 each: statement. a minor class -- l/lh. Notice that the body parts here are the coccyx -- the lowest point of the spine and the pharynx -- the lowest point (exclusive of the vocal cords) in the vocal tract 0 298 The humans in this class are looked on with dis- The animals contained herein are the common. or insignificant insects and birds. The items in the [strong] S-class denote respected humans and institutions or a "strength-in-numbers“ concept. [statementlappears to be characteristic of a significant minor class. related. [round] haawan migrafa girbiil mashuuf zanbiil mirhaad xaasuuga cafciir naaquus furgaasa ?idfir mihraae mihraab burnayta burkaan taawuus naafuura minxaar ?ikliil firjaal miftaah mintaad taahuuna purgaal The items listed under it are definitely 'mortar' 'large spoon' 'coarse sieve' 'long boat' 'large basket' 'toilet' 'spoon' 'concave spatula' 'church bell' 'blister' 'fingernail' 'plow' 'prayer niche' Chat. 'volcano' 'peacock' 'fountain' 'nostril’ 'wreath' 'compass' Ikeyo 'balloon' 'windmill' 'compass' mingaar ?ubuub karkuusa qurbaac 'amuud tarbuus saaruux siryaan mazriib mirzaab dambuus xaazuuq xartuum mihraar mijdaaf pirdaag barmiil bardaag ?ibr11g tinniin jarbuu' xanziir 'irbiid timsaah miidaan [square] paskiir tirbaas jizdaan carcuuba marjiiha tasWiir tafsiir qaamuus 299 'telescope' 'tube' 'tassel' 'whip' 'post' 'fez' 'rocket' 'artery' 'drainspout' 'drainspout' op1no 'shaft' 'elephant's trunk' 'thermometer' 'oar. 'glass' 'barrel' 'glass' 'pitcher? ’dragon' 'kangaroo rat' 'pig' 'large snake' 'crocodile' 'square (Open place)' 'hand towel' 'door bolt' 'wallet‘ 'picture frame' 'swing' 'photograph' 'book interpreting the Koran‘ 'dictionary' taQriir taqwiim kuuliis cirdaag haanuut dihliiz diiwaan santuur qaawuus *dbaara *faargawn [weak ] 'as'uus O zarduum Ja‘muus JarOuum taa'uun ba'buus qaazuug miskiin miflis bartiil bahluul majnuun gaZguuza mamluuk tartuur O O sa'luuk darwiis barguuGa bargasa zarzuur 'asfuur *mxannaO 'ifriit 300 'official report' 'calendar' 'opening at the side of a stage' 'summer cabin' 'canteen' 'narrow passage' 'guest house of a village' 'dulcimer' 'hospital ward' 'file' 'railroad car' 'coccyx' 'pharynx' 'feces' ogemo 'plague' ‘act of lewd poking' 'the shaft' 'poor' 'bankrupt' 'bribe‘ 'clown‘ 'madman' 'well-proportioned girl' 'white slave' 'braggart' 'pauper' 'dervish' 'flea' 'small insects' 'starling' 'sparrow' 'effeminate man' 'clever' *Items preceded by an asterisk do not correspond to the established P—shape(s) for the given plural class. [strong] bilyawn zangiin tilmiid Jiiraan ma'miil m'azzib 'irriis mashuur 'anguud Jamhuur ?unbuub ?ustuul tadbiir miiOaaq miiraaG maw'id qaanuun dastuur {statement} ?inbiiq ?injiil tasriih matluub ta'biir ta'riif FV1MaL Sem [abstract] [round] [sq uare] [strong] Phon 'billion' 'wealthy' 'student' 'neighbor' 'cuetomer' 'guest' 'bridegroom' 'celebrity' ‘bunch' 'crowd' 'tribe' 'fleet' 'organization' 'pact' 'inheritance' 'appointment' 'law' 'constitution' 'retort' ‘gospel' 'declaration' 'wish' 'expression‘ 'definition' FVMLa 302 This is another plural class which is characterized by more than one semantic feature. Three-fifths of the total number of items are accounted for semantically by means of these S-classes. Abstract accounts for 1/6; round. 14; square. 1/6: strong. 1/25. One minor class. [elongated]. is a subclass of [round] . Due to the number of defining semantic characteris- tics, this is probably better described as a P-shape plural class. (See note under Egflggflii_. p. 296.) [abstract] Jumla 'total' tuhfa 'rarity' bid'a 'innovation' bu?ra 'focus' junha 'misdemeanor' hiJBa 'excuse' hikma 'wisdom' hiila 'trick' xibra 'experience' xidma 'a service' xitta 'plan' dimma 'conscience' zubda 'essence' zarga 'blue' sil'a 'commodity' siira 'behavior sudfa 'chance' :ibra 'warning' 'uqda 'quirk' fursa 'opportunity' nag. ~1/2- giima 'price' mihna mudda ni'ma mihna nisba hudna [round] birka nugta nuqta wusla luula kutla giisa gulla guffa gursa girba qus'a qubba fiina r11§a 'uuda 'ilba 'ukra 'ugda 'uqca tawba tug'a gum siisa sibha xuuda dugma 303 'tribulation' 'period of time' 'benefaction' 'profession' 'relationships' 'armistice' 'puddle' 'SPOt.} ”free" variants ’dot' 'piece' 'cylinder' 'lump' 'date container' 'cannonball' 'large basket' 'flat round loaf' 'water bag' 'large bowl for carrying food to the troops' 'dome' 'fez' 'token' 'amulet' 'wooden container' 'bump' 'knct' 'heel (of shoe)‘ 'ball' 'stain' 'navel' 'glass bottle' 'prayer beads' 'helmet' 'button' 30h xirza 'bead' hilma 'nipple' hufra 'hole' hidba 'hump on the back’ 31113 'basket of palm leaves' jufra 'hole' tunga ‘clay water Jug' badra ‘spool' ‘ liifa 'pad of plant fibers used as scouring pad' Qumma 'peak' *kawm 'heap' [elongated] firca 'brush' 'unda 'splinter' 'usba 'tendon' diixa 'date stalk' dings 'column' Gurma 'shred' pulka 'spangle' tides 'drawstring' tufga 'rifle' tikma 'pillar' ?ubra 'needle' liita 'thin, flexible stick' *hiim 'crowbar' [square] hizza 'large cloth slung over the shoulder by workmen to carry sand' xirga 'rag' hujra 'room' Suwwa 'patch' juuma 'loom' Junta 'suitcase' Jinda 'porter's back pad' 305 Jufta 'dominoes piece' tiiga 'wall around roof' buqca ‘large cloth' ?awda 'room' durga 'shield' ruzma 'parcel' rug'a 'patch' sufra 'woven mat' su'ba 'section' ¥1qqa 'apartment' gutra 'headcloth' gurfa 'room' gutra 'headcloth' fijja 'piece of cloth' fuuta 'woman's head scarf' girma 'pleat' qisla 'barracks' gubba 'room' kulla 'mosquito net' [strong] hujja ‘authoritative source' Tumma 'people' ?usra 'clan' xulfa 'offspring' zumra 'gang' firqa 'team' FMaaMiiL §sm 2:122 [agent] FvMMva(a) [instrument] [square] [round] 306 The items listed here account for about 3/5 of the total number of items in this class -- agent. 1/5: instru- ment, l/5; round. l/lh and square, 1/8. The semantic classes which characterize this plural class are all major categories whose members require no further explanation. There are other [agent] classes. three of which are char- acterized by different P-shapes for the singular. viz. FaaMi (p1: FuMaat). FaaMiL (p1: FuMMaaL). FaMiiL (p1: FuMalaa?) and one of which has the same P-shape as this class FvMMva(v) (p1: FaMaaMiLa). There is also another plural class for [instrument], FamaaMiL. whose character- istic singular P-shape is FvMMvL(a). Because of the number of semantic features needed to define this class it is probably better analyzed as a P- shape class. (See note under FaMaaMiiL, p.296.) [agentfi baggaal 'grocer' bayyaad ’tinner' hammaal 'porter' xabbaaz 'baker' raaguus 'dancer' muxtaar ‘mukhtar' raggaa' 'shoe repairman' zabbaal 'garbage collector' sabbaag 'painter' saffaar 'coppersmith' fallaah 'farmer' gassaab 'butcher' gawwaad 'pimp' kannaas 'sweeper' 307 mallaah 'sailor' naddaaf 'man who renovates mattresses' nazzaah 'man who empties septic tanks' naatuur 'watchman' laa'uub 'player' saa'uul 'fire tender' saabuuh 'expert swimmer' caawuus 'foreman' Jaasuus 'spy' [instrument] saatuur 'cleaver' mingaas 'tweezer' maakuuk 'bobbin' mi'caal 'sling' maaxuud 'club, bat' faddaan 'animal-drawn plow' faanuus 'lantern' Eaahuul 'plumb line' sicciin 'knife' zanjiil 'chain' cingaal 'fastener' cillaab 'hook' Xaakuuc ’hammer‘ tannuur 'outdoor oven' bismaar 'nail' baapuur 'long cigarette holder' *mhaffa 'hand fan' *mugas 'scissors' *jaawan 'large mortar' [round] lakluuka 'ball. lump' karuuk 'baby cradle' 'stone' salbuux O saahuud xarmuus mincaasa maa'uun finjaan [square] taabuut daaguur diilaab raazuuna sijjaada sirdaab sibbaac sanduuq sanduug taabuuga qur?aan kaagada 308 'large bead on the end of a string of prayer beads' ’bunch of dates' 'bowl' 'plate' '(small porcelaid cup' 'coffin' 'removable center door post' 'wardrobe' 'shelf' 'prayer rug' 'cellar‘ 'window' 'money box:}"free” variants 'box 'brick' 'Koran' 'sheet'