~36}! MO? MAN I u ”If; fillll 'n 3 la; 1:... .I__ .- i . I ‘3 ABS TRACT A STUDY OF THE EXPRESSION OF TRADITIONAL JAPANESE CULTURE THROUGH SELECTED SOCIAL RITUALS As REVEALED IN POPULAR NOVELS By Midori Yamanouchi Banks There has been increased interest in the dramaturgi- cal approach to the study of interpersonal dynamics in recent; years. There also has been renewed interest on the study of rituals. Analysis of these studies often suggests the importance of everyday interaction rituals. The drama... turgical approach to the study of human behavior is applied h." to study the ritualization of symbolic behavior. The process of ritualization of everyday behavior interaction is seen from the perspective of Japanese culture and per- sonality. In Part II, a discussion of Japanese everyday social run“, is presented as manifested through contemporary Japanese popular novels. Reflecting history and tradition, Social rituals in Japanese interpersonal relations contain a hierarchy of principles which we might consider as the cultural basis for the manifested overt behavior patterns of the people. Japanese mental characteristics such as a naturalistic dom-to-earth orientation, the conditionality "hose 0 are 1 o v- .Qzue".° 38 t 5 the use of a .41.” t it :‘zzeirthinking. th! conce :nm identified and dis 2... principles {or inter means of these psycholer auction Jade between pu'r: mm of thinking regar tateexphasis on vertica; iittzcular as expressed ; Inadiition to this ‘ :t:'.s,an analysis of 1313' v . . la“ " «.3: 3 external aspects "Ruse of language. Pat'- 43.3 usage, (2) Ti tua l i '3 "I e I“. A \‘J'd‘ ”zed greetings for t In.‘ - ..c.anonships, and (1‘) ‘ N .. concept of face 4 "w 1: reviewed in the 1" “”3 Points developed in £1: ' nth include the gene ‘53:.2 .me context; var-i r... 335‘ . J'Ct. frequently e Q . Midori Yamanouchi Banks of their thinking, the concept of resignation, and emotion- alism are identified and discussed. Some of the major guiding principles for interpersonal relationships as ex- pressions of these psychological characteristics are the distinction made between public and private matters, the relativity of thinking regarding interpersonal relationships, and the emphasis on vertical relationships and honorifics in particular as expressed in Japanese language. In addition to this internal analysis of social rituals. an analysis of Japanese interaction rituals in term of various external aspects is also undertaken: (1) the proper use of language, particularly the relative nature of language usage, (2) ritualization of spatial pattern, (3) standardized greetings for different occasions and for differ- ent relationships, and (h) the social rituals of clothing. The concept of face and face-work in Japanese inter- action is reviewed in the last chapter and related to various points developed in the earlier chapters. Topics dealt with include the general concept of face and face-work in a Japanese context; various expressions used in referring to the subject; frequently mentioned embarrassing situations in Japanese novels; and a particular type of face-losing situation involving those who are able but fail in competi- tion, those who are less than lucky. The last section describes the use of a third party in settling differences in Japanese interpersonal relations. a.“ fluid-3| iSI'JDY 3F IKE ES: Nidori '1' A STUDY OF THE EXPRESSION OF TRADITIONAL JAPANESE CULTURE THROUGH SELECTED SOCIAL RITUALS AS REVEALED IN POPULAR NOVELS By Midori Yamanouchi Banks A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY College of Social Science 1973 If V- . . I 1? I ' "'Q m AZXKCO' I: has been my goc £29. Dr. Donald V. 0125 11911:: Dr. Ralph .\'ic‘:‘ 23:93. In most ind Jrspiration to 85.91“ Sigrity. I am grateful tinted And Mrs. Wall 1'1"!" and Dr. Clmstec .‘s ”in“ and com d 11k! a1; ’7 data” me and, 33t b hI A CKNOWLEDGEMENT It has been my good fortune to have Dr. James B. McKee, Dr. Donald W. Olmsted, Dr. Douglas Dunham, Dr. Walter R. Fee and Dr. Ralph Nicholas as the members of my advisory committee. I am most indebted to each one of them for the inspiration to aspire for academic excellence and integrity. I am grateful to Professors McKee, Fee, Dunham, and Olmsted and Mrs. Walter R. Fee for having read my draft. Dr. McKee and Dr. Olmsted have been the most wonderful thesis advisers, and their kindness and patience are deeply appreciated. My gratitude also goes to my husband, Dr. Valter R. Banks, for reading rough drafts, pointing out ambiguous points and coming forward with criticism. I should like also to express my appreciation for my bl‘Other's insistence on my mastery of Japanese language and °133810s, my sister's and my late father's never failing faith in me, and, last but not the least, to my mother for ha'ing given the opportunity to breathe the best of old Japan . ii L ”IX-31.3 “0-0-pr l he'd-deed Enter I. JEIEPAL EIECRISE Recent Dowel: Limitation o: Ethological E Ritualizatiox Human Ri Ethology N I «'. RIKALIZATICX 3 Dramaturgic ‘ Behavior Ritualizatio imitation o of Even'da Cultural bif ExProssion resentation °nc°Pt of INTRODUCTION Chapter I. II. III! TABLE OF CONTENTS 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 PART I GENERAL THEORIES OF RITUALIZATION . . . . . Recent Developments Limitation of Ethological Approach Ethological Studies Ritualization of Behavior in Man Human Ritual Elements Ethology and Human Ritualization RITUALIZATION OF SYMBOLIC BEHAVIOR . . e e Dramaturgic Approach to the Study of Behavior Ritualization of Everyday Behavior Limitation on Individual Variation of Everyday Rituals Cultural Differences in Standardized Expressions Presentation of Self Concept of Space in Interaction Rituals Good Manners as Social Rituals PART II CULTURAL SOURCES FOR JAPANESE INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS: GENERAL DISCUSSION ON JAPANESE MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS . . e e . . . e 0 General Observations The Tangible or Down-to-Earth Orienta- tion of Japanese Language Conditionality, Multi-Dimensionality and Tendency away from Exaggeration Resignation Bmotionalism iii Page 15 15 21 25 31 33 38 56 56 so 6’4 66 7h 93 10h 109 109 120 128 137 lh6 M '4- ¢ . ~'-t.«_ ‘1.e.r v.” .1. 332131 or man: ucfl't'fl' {fleeoe‘E-‘S . 0 0 Historical An; Interperson; Feudal Soc of Todd; Sir: and ,_L. . 11233 ani Lack of t Hibun $37 and It: Behatic hidapanese l IntergerSOI Vocabula Japanese Intern Honorif; Var; JaPanese ‘ Q"'\ I event}. RITUM‘S Jar-363.51% LIP: SOClal Ritu; sOcial Ritu; Rituals as ' 'ajof Catex Rituals U Relatln PTOper Chapter Page IV. REVIEW OF TRADITIONAL JAPANESE INTERACTION PATTERNS C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 151 Historical Analysis of Major Concepts on Interpersonal Relationship 151 Feudal Social Bond and 'Familialism" of Today 158 Reigi 16% Giri and Nin'B 169 Oyake and atakushi 174 Lack of the Concept "Privacy" 179 Mibun §§§ 186 Fragmentation of the Japanese Society and Its Impact on Interpersonal Behavior 190 On Japanese Language with regard to Interpersonal Relationship 192 Vocabulary on Good Social Behavior 192 Japanese Accent and Pattern of Social Interaction 196 Honorifics 199 Varieties of Honorifics 202 Japanese Language and Social Structure 206 V. SOCIAL RITUALS AND SOCIAL CRISIS IN EVERYDAY JAPANESE LIFE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 216 Social Rituals in a Traditional Society 218 Social Rituals in a Newer Society 220 Rituals as a Response to Danger 221 Major Categories of Japanese Social Rituals 222 Relating to the Proper Use of Language 225 PrOper Spatial Behavior 231 Place for Interaction 2&1 Standardized Greetings 2&9 Regarding Proper Attire to wear: Occasions and Seasons 262 Types of Kimono and Special Occasion for Them 266 Social Consequences of Violating Social Rituals 272 VI. ON JAPANESE CONCEPT OF FACE AND FACE-WORK. . 283 Japanese Expression Regarding "Face" 302 Embarrassing Situations 313 Face-work Rituals for Less Lucky Ones: Neutralizing Measures 318 Face-work by Employing a Third Party 325 CONCLUSION ‘ 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 339 APPENDIX eeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeeee 31‘s BIBLIOGRAPIIY 0 O 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 350 iv .v...\...|uflolm .n a... an»! PART I In recent ”“5 t? :1 iteraturgical approm finer, as Batman stat: sanction in natural 5 3|! lareorer, th 0 ana EL: unclear. . . ."1 As. relatively new ani "tn 4 .. trnclal importance I 9132:} :e of the expres :éate I my interaction wi « and real, but amps. .. .nal relationsh "‘“Hocoum to a pin it ' ' “dying social r INTRODUCTION In recent years there have been studies utilizing the dramaturgical approach to interpersonal relations. However, as Goffman states, "The study of face-to—face interaction in natural settings doesn't yet have an adequate name. Moreover, the analytical boundaries of the field remain unclear. . . ."1 This research field itself is, thus, relatively new and in its formative stage. Yet, it is of crucial importance. Bennis and others emphasize the importance of the expression of interpersonal feelings in our daily interaction with others. Not only such feelings are basic and real, but they do "have real effects tin interpersonal relationshipsn, and that they can be studied Vithout recourse to a physiological or instinctual theory."2 Studying social rituals is an important segment of “19 dramaturgical approach to the study of interpersonal dYnamic. and the quality of human relations. Also the ““derstanding of social rituals would be essential for “Mowing the quality of interpersonal relation in any given seeigty. \ N 1Erving Goffman, Interaction Ritual (Garden City, - Y. : Doubleday do Company, 1937’ e P. 1. 2Warren G. Bennis and others, eds., Inter ersonal %. rev. ed., (Homewood, Ill.: Dorsey Press, 1968’, Social rituals s recesses to specific 3 In; in a society to 2mm. Social ritus :srizas hey are on info: a people. “e”: m: such as law: an Emotion of bringin 1! :eea violated, or <1 mm: 0!" an offend 7"‘13 serve a society ‘ Xi’an- e....:s‘ai;>s among its Moreover, in con teasing urbanization can form that gen: :1:qu human relati: Uta? ‘5. called "tom; ""3713 great and it 2 Social rituals may be considered as standardized responses to specific situations which recur frequently enough in a society to warrent the canalization of behavior patterns. Social rituals represent normative behavior inasmuch as they are culturally built-in regulatory mecha— nisms for a people. While more formal types of social control such as laws and official regulations may serve well the function of bringing justice when a code of good conduct has been violated, or damage done to a party has to be corrected, or an offender punished, they do little to effec- tively serve a society as mechanisms for maintaining positive relationships among its members in their everyday life. Moreover, in contemporary Japanese society with its increasing urbanization and rapid social change, there are certain forces that generate toward a breakdown of the <1uality of human relationships in that: (l) the trend toward what may be called "temporary system" where geographic Inability is great and interpersonal relationships tend to 3 and (2) the density of population which be less permanent, a‘ff‘ects personal space or "territoriality" which is a 31éflrificant factor in interpersonal dynamics. and (3) a general disintegration of norms that accompanies a rapid social and cultural change. Sir Julian Huxley pointed out that when existing l”Whale are no longer adaptive and adequate to new types \ 0 3Usage by Philip E. Slater, in his "Some Social °“3°quences of Temporary Systems," in Bennis and others, eds., 02, cit.. pp. 139-53. i5;,7,;'_~,o.scoial situat; no social Changes, ‘ exactly added either :Lz'e: rituals which are With these cons; foo-secs: crucial in c 7-1113. Yet there i: .aznese ererydav socia '1' "I. as the findings of i! talysis of intera Eli‘s-i ‘ ..:...f:cance of s O 45.! are reputed to 5W! eta, - r «.3 reputatic xittl’. M to establisi' 3 of psycho-social situation which are the result of contem- porary social changes, new everyday rituals will have to be constantly added either to take over or to supplement these older rituals which are no longer adequate.“ With these considerations, a study of social rituals seems most crucial in our attempt to understand interpersonal _..‘J dynamics. Yet there is no systematic study regarding a.“ Japanese everyday social rituals available as of date. Hence the findings of this study will be a contribution to the analysis of interaction in Japanese everyday life, and the significance of social rituals in general. The Japanese Imople are reputed to be very polite and most courteous, however this reputation may be a result of their close adherence to established social rituals. Possibly it is this adherence to social rituals that contributes to the Prevention of open hostilities, which are rare in Japanese e179:l:‘yday interaction. Specifically social rituals may help iJuiividuals in working situation in that they can better distinguish personal relationships and instrumental relation- Shiqaa. If so, a certain degree of harmony can be maintained “'91! when two people who are not fond of each other have to '°rki itith each other. Possibly also the existence of social 1Rituals may give individuals a sense of security and comfort, i.e - e not having to suffer from the anxiety of not knowing if one is being prOPBro \ lo B Julian Huxley, "A Discussion on Ritualization of ahaViemr in Animals and Men: Introduction," Philosophical Transaction s 23; the Re a1 Society 23 London, Ser. B, No. 772, 01. 251, 29 Dec. 19665, p. 2 . —‘—‘— Based on th' PI unison the "11 be: 23;: social rituals 1 only ingrained in S inane are usually aural" responses t trait, a selective refer this study 1 can; rituals are :11 3:33:31 social ritua 13km eyes of nor: titzuon and accep‘ dime life. 01‘ c. “a ten the novel ‘5 lentioned ‘ New} "Y social ritu 35.3w. Lu . uSAlp throng; R Based on the presumed importance that social rituals have upon the well being of a society as a whole, and that basic social rituals in everyday interaction are usually deeply ingrained in social customs and that such behavior patterns are usually taken for granted and viewed as "natural" responses to a given situation not requiring much thought, a selective list of contemporary popular novels are used for this study to see how various elements of Japanese social rituals are dramatized in them. In other words, inter- personal social rituals as observed and reproduced through the keen eyes of novelists will serve here as data as to the common and accepted expressions of rituals in everyday Japanese life. Of course obvious exceptions can also be noted when the novelist deals with a character that is conspicuously deviant. As mentioned before, this study describes Japanese everyday social rituals with an emphasis on face-to-face relationship through popular novels. It is not concerned '31131 the general impact of specific social rituals upon the Quality of interaction or upon Japanese culture as such, a1though its significance is fully recognized and it is hoped that such a study will be undertaken in a future. It is also not concerned with a comparative Study of Japan use everyday rituals of today with any other period °f JEI-pamese history or with other societies. The degree of impasrt that Vesternization has upon Japanese culture and Pfirsonality and upon everyday interaction patterns are 4.. _ . 1,, _' " “Ell-(«1 m framing and challen: 2.7221 the soc." °f “‘1 :g‘nien Japan are int mica-ding Japanese 5 cm the continuing Iggreater emphasis. is: and other such ism involved in t‘ni its“ upon various c “‘34:? significant swam-p. It .pp. .333 is far gnate; 5122 '.. -:'. . “Md ‘1" Scope O “So. this Stu ”Care“ '1" "My or 11:3; status differ. his . 111ml; theSis 9'22: : 18 th. 8 x ublec "Riser lce t C 0. ‘ina‘. ooJCtUral nife 1,. is d‘aroa 3 of t‘ - u. T11 . . 8 Hm ignirica & ritual ~; 0 \lt'. Ut t 5 fascinating and challenging topics for study, but it is beyond the scope of this thesis. While most Western scholars of modern Japan are intrigued by such comparison, there are some leading Japanese scholars, such as Prof. Chie Nakane, who see the continuing elements of Japanese culture deserv- ing greater emphasis. We are inclined to agree with Prof. Nakane and other such scholars, but we do not intend to become involved in this dispute. Rather, our analysis focuses upon various cultural and ritual elements which are seemingly significant in the Japanese everyday interpersonal relationship. It appears that the importance of social rituals is far greater than normally so recognized by most laymen. In the future we wish to extend this study to the issue of the relative importance of rituals, but this too is beyond the scope of this thesis. Also, this study is not a psychological study nor a comparative study of various social variables influencing social status differentiation. Rather, what we intend to pursue in this thesis. because of some presumed social impact, is the subject of everyday social rituals in Japanese fece-to-face interaction (and their cultural basis) and analyzing the content of literary works. It is hoped the structural manifestation of these rituals can be studied. Some sub-areas of this topic could also be studied in depth, such as the significance of laughter as a component of social rituals. but these too would be outside the scope of this thesis. 1' E ‘a me.um' -"‘“" ‘ ‘ Ibis study is no me: Mamie a qu 3:391: consulted. I :I’Jepmse scholars it How the novels 1 2mm in the {0110' 11:33:31) bat“ an 1 ' l "M It. um this introd J c " Tue .‘(et'zr \ In order to go1 k..‘ 7 M: .apaneso no"? :1?an -‘ ~ \ g 9:53“ (Fuel; mam, TokYO) '35 selling‘aooks from th iifietitles, transla imls, novels dealin iii-fiction titles ‘16 emphasis in {5:9 '0 ”‘3 have t 6 This study is not a quantitative study. Hence, it does not undertake a quantitative analysis of the content of novels consulted. For supportive materials, the works of Japanese scholars and personal observations are used. How the novels were chosen for this study will be discussed in the following section. Theories on the relationship between literature and social life will conclude this introduction. The Method of Selecting Novels In order to get a fair representation of contemporary popular Japanese novels dealing with Japanese everyday life, Shuppan Nenkan (Publication Yearbook, compiled by Shuppan Nyfisu-Sha, Tokyo) was consulted to obtain the list of best selling books from the end of WWII to the present. Of these titles, translations of foreign works, historical novels, novels dealing with non-Japanese societies, and non-fiction titles were excluded. An emphasis was placed upon those authors whose complete works have been included in the list of best sellers, and those who have received various literary awards as well. These titles which appeared in the list more than once received particularly close attention. Those novels which were initially published as shimbun sthetsu ("news- paper novels" which appear in a newspaper as a series) of major newspapers also received careful considerations. This selection is based on the fact that (1) the most file I b.- e ee\ ‘n x, 7 popular fiction tends to portray more common topics rather than eccentric and outrageous characters, and, in fact, often portray everyday life of various categories of people, and that (2) there seems to be an un-written understanding among authors to stay with a reasonably moderate and some- what educational orientation when writing shimbun sthetsu because of its presumed impact on general public, and that (3) those novels, i.e., shimbun sthetsu, ought to be rather simple, straight forward and not too difficult, for the readers of these novels are most inclusive, i.e., women at home and children, young and old, highly educated and not so well educated, and so on. Hence, they are aimed at the most general reading public.5 Moreover, the authors selected to write a shimbun shosetsu for one of the major papers are usually already well established novelists, because the primary function of the inclusion of a novel in a newspaper series is to increase the newspaper subscrip- tion.6 In addition to the novels so selected, some titles in the pre-WWII best selling list, which are considered classics, are also included when it seemed appropriate to do so.7 These various authors' novels were used qualitatively and not quantitatively. In other words, some works had 5Seiseien Ihara, "Shimbun Shfisetsu," Waseda Bungaku (February 1906). Cited in Senichi Hisamatsu, Nihon Bungaku Shi: Kindai (Tokyo: Shibun D5, 1966), p. 79. 6Circulation figure of about U,000,000 or over. 7See Appendix, "Annotated List of Novels Used." "I Onl‘ he I" 35.! I In,‘ «1... In“. E . l‘.‘.” “u. . I "M. |._ 8 very little to contribute toward the particular interests of this thesis, and hence are not even reflected in footnotes. Thus, the footnotes do not necessarily reflect all the titles consulted for this study. It might be added that the background of the authors whose works contributed most toward my study tend to have relatively strong educational background although their original family backgrounds and the route they took to have become literary figures differed considerably. It may be that the more educated tend to have the type of sensitivity which interests and enables them to describe in detail intricate social interaction scenes. The differences in treatment of subject by the authors of various background, such as educational, social or philosophical difference, in itself would be a topic worthy of further study. Theories of Relationship Between Literature and Social Life Not only novelists, but most literary artists have endeavored to attempt to catch the color of life itself. As Coser stated, literature, "though it may also be many other things, is social evidence and testimony. It is a continuous commentary on manners and morals . . . ."8 And, literary works present for us the precious record of modes of response to peculiar social and cultural conditions of the time the story takes place. Han Suyin's §_Many Splendored Thing is therefore a literary piece more so than 8Lewis A. Coser, ed., Sociology Through Literature (New York: Prentice-Hall, 1963), p. 2. 2:215 an autobiograth‘ :e 3:;19 and manner! It is said that :53: our sense percei 311i: is so it vould acquire data on V318 111:! ncvelists to b ire-sass. In other \ tiedobecauso of 1 utpecple do withou‘ 1% reason for the po; 163mg out many t1 i459“ ”put their Hark Schorer d 313918 the croativ 232E” impoud Upon at: ' If the artist 9 merely an autobiography, for the scenes, the customs of the people and manners are so vividly portrayed. It is said that subliminal perceptions are something beyond our sense perception and even our cognitive level. If this is so it would be impossible for ordinary people to acquire data on what is subliminal. It takes the acute mind of novelists to bring this phenomena into the level of awareness. In other words, literature condenses what people do because of the author's ability to describe what most peOple do without really being aware of it. Moreover, the reason for the popularity that many novels enjoy is that they bring out many things which readers feel that they could not "put their finger on" before. Mark Schorer discussed the need of realization, as it impels the creative artist. The necessary limitations that are imposed upon every creative being is his environ- ment. If the artist inhabits "such barren ground cas in America: . . . it would be difficult to fulfill himself."9 Regarding the work of ten well-known twentieth century American writers, T. K. Vhipple concluded as follows: Most of their work has a touch of that gauntness or emaciation, a deficiency in variety, body, mass, which springs from underfeeding of the imagination - from having lived, that is, in a world which affords inadequate experience. 9Introduction to Leon Surmelian, Technigues‘gg Fiction Writin s Measure and Madness (Garden City, N. Y.s Doubleday, 1963, Anchor Books, 1969), p. x. 10Quoted in Surmelian, op: cit., p. x. r' 'In" 9...: A‘ _ ts". e l‘.‘ 10 The world that the author creates, therefore, depends in large part on the world that he inhabits. Thus, "even the greatest genius has a collaborator in his environment, and if that collaborator is sluggish, even the greatest genius may fail in the full realization of himself."11 It follows, then, that if the creative process of writing itself is a movement from the unrealized to the realized, it is a process that in part brings the unconscious quality of the habitual into the realm of consciousness, and in this way it endeavors to know and to make known the previously unknown. There has been considerable controversy over the question of unconscious behavior, however. Some insist that men are often unaware of the most important things they do, and others are equally adamant that any talk of "un- conscious" or "subconscious" processes is fraudulent. Regardless, it would be reasonable to say that much of our everyday behavior escapes our conscious awareness, some are natural responses to a recurring situation and some are characterized as habits whether one is aware of them or not. Much of our everyday behavior and expressions are, in fact, not a result of deliberate thinking. These are the behavior areas the novelist is able to observe and describe through his perceptivity and sensitivity, as discussed before. In his Language 222 Literature in Society, Hugh D. Duncan discusses literature as equipment for action: 11Mark Schorer, Introduction, in Surmelian, op, cit., p. xi. to}: 5:5 out that wt; :espell, Burke uses s::;:l:;:cal function iris certain social 1'? oust have a way c to. £211.. ’ The detelo; Zitmlitation of ‘31 Ii’fiv'i': ‘ ‘ ...,....g mud n 2“ a.‘apropriat e . Concerning cro 11 and points out that whereas Freud uses the dream, Malinowski the spell, Burke uses the proverb as an illustration of the sociological function of verbal symbolism.12 When people find a certain social relationship recurring so frequently they must have a way of naming it to begin dealing with it at all.13 The deve10pment of social ritual involves both the canalization of behavior patterns and language. Thus, in studying social rituals, the use of literary works seems most appropriate. Concerning cross-cultural studies of behavior, Al- Issa and Dennis wrote: One of the many ways of comparing cultures is to compare the content of the successful plays produced by each culture. This method can be criticized, as the authors acknowledge. Perhaps authors, not nations, are being compared. But plays are produced for profit, and it seems likely that over the years the producefls have learned to give the audiences what they want.1 Our assumption is that the psychological constella- tions in a literary work, as in a dramatic work, "indicate sensitive areas in the personalities of those for whom the work has appeal" and that "their needs, assumptions and values are expressed ('projected')" in it.15 It may be reasonable to state, as McGranahan and Wayne did, that 12(New'York: The Bedminster Press, 1961), p. 8b. 13Cf. Kenneth Burke, The Philoso h 2; Literagy Form: Studies in Symbolic Action (Baton Rouge: Louisiana State University Press, 1931), p. 293. 1’"Ihsan Al-Issa and Wayne Dennis, eds., Cross- Cultural Studies 2; Behavior (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1970), p. 196. cEditors' note). 15Donald V. McGranahan and Ivor Wayne, "German and American Traits Reflected in Popular Drama," in Al-Issa and Dennis, eds., op, cit., p. 197. I... ‘ | an! I 5"" s . A ,v. 12 analysis of popular songs, poetry, art, humor, novels and short stories, and so forth, may well reveal other psycho- logical facets of a historical population that are not accessible to direct study.16 In order to create a desired sensation in the mind of reader, the writer has to reproduce situations portraying the characteristic movements of a given group, reflecting geographic, social, age, sexual and other differences. The author provides the reader with scenes, and so forth. so that the reader can build up his own sensations of space, scene and atmosphere. In this regard, Hall in his Th2 Hidden Dimension discusses literature as a key to perception, Although he is specifically talking with regard to social Space, the same principle may be applied to the study of social rituals. A writer's "success in communicating perception depends upon the use of visual and other clues to convey different degree of closeness." Not only the spatial relationship of interactants, their concern for non-verbal communication, particularly the subliminal level of awareness of the interactants themselves, would be most helpful in fully creating scenes of interpersonal interaction. The question that Hall asked himself was "whether one could use literary texts as data rather than simply as descriptions."18 "It 16Loc. cit. 17Edward T. Hall, The Hidden Dimension (Garden City, N. Y.: Doubleday, 1966), p. 95. . 18Loca cit. p.’ :stiermz'cered." Hal :21; levels; what is :Ltcttsr. . . . [the some kid communicat 551?»:frtant cultural 3915!. it is reason; tints-verbal express- an: 1'.“ some purpgse ’ Stifting 0f po si ‘. a M.- mscious level. I Li tital interaCtioq 1:112; . irate seems am W“ rit‘tals. .n Ct"W-lllsion ’ rig-e1 , . .t y DEG“ Comte Ln'ltinn trim“ “e t. n tde bess F‘. 01c state to use ‘ 31:7- 51:0: «lffs' \‘Su SQ]. e . J 2L '3 13 must be remembered," Hall states, "that communications are on many levelsz'what is relevant on one level may not be on another, . . . [the passages: reveal how great writers perceive and communicate the meaning and uses of distance as a significant cultural factor in interpersonal relations."19 Likewise, it is reasonable to say that the study of gestures and non-verbal expressions as well as verbal exchange will serve the same purpose, as much of body movement, change of tone, shifting of position, et cetera, is done at the subconscious level. In fact, space is but one component of the total interaction process. And, what he says about social space seems applicable also to the study of everyday social rituals. In conclusion, as Shibutani commented, "it has frequently been contended that literary men present a more convincing depiction of human life than social psychologists," and in "even the best of their [social psychologists'n 20 And that studies, something seems to be missing." although these phenomena that novelists and playwrights deal with as central theme are unquestionably a central part of the drama of life, social psychologists have until quite recently avoided their study.21 Thus, it seems most appropriate to use literary works as sources of data in this study of social rituals. 19 Loc: cit. 20Tamotsu Shibutani, Societ and Personality (Engle- wood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1961), p. 323. 21Loc: cit.' 1U In the two following chapters, we review Western scholars' scientific works on the subjects most relevant to the topic of this thesis. Part II deals with the Japanese everyday social rituals in face-to-face inter- personal relations. Inasmuch as social rituals are products of tradition and hence reflect the basic ways of thinking of a people, Japanese scholars' works on Japanese culture and personality are reviewed in Chapter III. The major concepts on interpersonal relationship in Japanese everyday life from historical point of view is specifically dealt in Chapter IV. 3mm 13301 Recen ~— Loren: defines 21:5 of behavior .1 b ..::=.lization as ti 1, a massing amount 0 i‘salso among anthr i11‘~$tience disci p1 "'iilitation. In {as 3.33% 3 Symposium, CHAPTER I GENERAL.THEORIES 0F RITUALIZATION Recent Developments Lorenz defines the young science of ethology as the biology of behavior,1 and identifies the subject of ritual and ritualization as the principal concern of ethologists.2 An increasing amount of interest has been seen in recent years also among anthropologists, psychiatrists, and other social science disciplines in the subject of ritual and ritualization. In fact, in 1965 the Royal Society of London sponsored a symposium, the first one in 150 years, under the leadership of Sir Julian Huxley with ritual as the central topic for discussion. Each member presented his findings as to how ritualization or formalized activities take place among animals, and how the process of evolution is related to ritualization. 1Konrad z. Lorenz, "Ritualization in the Psycho-social Evolution of Human Culture," Philoso hical Transactions of the Re a1 Societ 2£_London, er. B, No. 7727 v61. 251 (BBB. I536}. p. 275: ' ' 2Sir Julian Huxley, who himself is credited with being the founder of ethology, acknowledges Charles Darwin as the forerunner of ethology, notably the publication, Expression 2; the Emotions ig.§3g‘§gg.Animals (1881). The ‘work of elous-TTQOl), entitled Bird Watching, is also of great importance. More contemporary ethologists include such scholars as Lorenz and Tinbergen. For a brief review of the subject, see "A Discussion on Ritualization of Behaviour in Animals and Men: Introduction," Philosophical Transactions of the Re al Societ 22 London, Ser. B, No. 772, Vol. 251 'Tfiec. 193%). Pp. 259-71. 5? Huxley. 15 n...‘ . e 01".. . ".“el v~ ‘- -.‘~4 I.,‘ 16 Referring to ritualization in man, and being fully aware of the existence of wide disagreement as to the use of terms like ritual in anthropological and psychological literature, Huxley states that he continues to use the term ritualization "for simplicity's sake . . . in a broad sense to denote the adaptive formalization and canalization of motivated human activities so as to secure more effective communicatory ('signalling') function, reduction of intra- group damage, or better intra—group bonding."3 Not only do approaches to the study of ritual and ritualization of behavior differ according to discipline, there is no uniformity in the use of the term ritual and ritualization between disciplines.“ Erikson mentions that 3Huxley, op. cit., p. 258. 1(Ritual as an adjective is defined by $23 Shorter Oxford Dictionapz as "pertaining or relating to . . . rites: of the nature of . . . a rite or rites": and rite is defined as "a formal procedure in a religious or other solemn obser- vance; a custom or practice of a formal kind (e.g. rites of hospitality) the general or normal custom, habit or practice of a country, now especially in relation to religion or worship (e.g. the Roman rite)." Ritual as a noun is defined as "a prescribed order of performing religious or other devotional service; a book containing the order to be observed in the celebration of religious or other solemn service . . . the performance of ritual acts." (Quoted in Huxley, op. cit., p. 258n.) Leach suggests that, for the ethologists, Hinde s definition seems to serve for all: "ritualization refers to the evolutionary changes which the signal movements of lower vertebrates have undergone in adaptation to their function in communication," Philoso hical Transactions 331392 Re a1 Society of London, er. B, o. 772, Vol. 251 (Dec. 19 p. #03.) However, for the purposes of this paper, Huxley's general-definition seems to fit better. Leach also notes that: "tor the ethologist, ritual is adaptive repetitive behaviour which is characteristic of a whole species; for the anthropologist, ritual is occasional behaviour by par- ticular members of a single culture . . . . It cannot be too strongly emphasized that ritual, in the anthropologist's sense, is in no way whatsoever a genetic endowment of the species." (Leach, 02: cit., p. #03.) V :r:'.iost of a nunbe 23;: the conventio :32: and rituals time-tines Vitnes 7;:5' for the purpc tenses of the yes Fifi: my much it tons-ritual by so 15341105, not in 1 ii‘i‘tes and belie: 1:2: tolationshi; gr.” .5398 thg P051 17 the oldest of a number of connotations concerning ritualiza- tion is the conventional anthropological one, "which ties it to rites and rituals conducted by communities of adults (and sometimes witnessed by children or participated in by youths) for the purpose of marking such recurring events as the phases of the year or of the stages of life."5 Fortes' usage is very much in this line. He distinguishes ritual from non-ritual by saying that "the distinguishing feature of ritual lies, not in the internal constitution of actions, utterances and beliefs, but in their external signification, in their relationship with the total cultural environment."6 rortes takes the position that "ritual is distinguished from non-ritual by the fact that it is aimed at the occult," and accordingly defines ritual as "procedure for prehending the occult, that is first, for grasping what is, for a particular culture, occult in the events and incidents of people's lives, secondly, for binding what is so grasped by means of the ritual resources and beliefs available in that culture, and thirdly, for thus incorporating what is grasped and bound into the normal existence of individuals and groups."7 Leach, on the other hand, proposes that ritual be . viewed in broader terms. 0f the three types of distinctively human behaviors, i.e., (1) "rational technical" behavior, 5B. Erikson, "Ontogeny of Ritualization in Man," Philosoppical Transactions pg’pgp Ro a1 Societ pg’London, er. B, No. 772, Vol. 251 (Dec. 19 , p. 357. 6M. Fortes, "Religious Premisses and Logical Technique in Divinatory Ritual," Philoso hical Transactions of'gpg Rozal Sociegz pg London, Ser. B, No. 772, Vol. 251, p. #15. 71bid., p. #11. ."::mnitire" behav 25in convention in tail for to behavior 53155 {2) by some 01 trial. Leach sees .ftea'me tvo categ' ztrthenakes the t. hteTallensi cust L'ioiebaby (i.eu :T’SCETUMS a cocf 15213171139“ they 83 iiazalabash dish) is ‘izrlfzereas in the (1 “91mm? to ”Wish nical one, u s‘fl -. w contepmali z 0 “ e sGlitter-y . inf” I“ the 1 a; 18 (2) "communitive" behavior, and (3) "magical" behavior, the orthodox convention in anthropology is to reserve the term ritual for the behaviors of class (3), and call behaviors of class (2) by some other term, such as etiquette or ceremonial. Leach sees the distinction between behaviors of the above two categories as either illusory or trivial so that he makes the term ritual embrace both categories.8 Thus the Tallensi custom where a messenger announcing the birth of a baby (i.e., if the baby is a boy, the messenger arrives carrying a cockerel and a throwing stick of the kind men carry when they go hunting, and if it is a girl, a pullet and a calabash dish) would be ritualization in this defini- tion, whereas in the definition of Fortes it would not be.9 A relatively recent connotation of "ritualization" is the clinical one, where "the term 'private ritual' is. used to conceptualize obsessional behaviour consisting of repetitive solitary acts with highly idiosyncratic meanings."10 In the light of more recent findings this connotation is no longer popularly used. Erikson suggests this clinical connotation be set aside in order to take account of newer insights both in ethology and in psycho- analysis.11 There is now a trend in the ethological literature12 which follows the original suggestion of Sir 8Leach, op. cit., p. hoh 90f., Fortes, op. cit., p. #12. 1onrikson, op. cit., p. 337. 11Loc. cit. 12Summarized in Konrad Lorenz's Das So enannte Boese, 196b, later translated in English and published as 2E.Aggression (New York: Harcourt, Brace a Worhi,l966). 1,;1: Juli? t° use tigeoetically P” manuals.” B insulating that 1 cast consist of a is: no persons It .‘ncurring context exits value f or b 1m conditions are iinn lather and h “in that mum r3Ere-verbal uper (1's ‘ on in grand '3'.th rituals" as “to. 19 Julian Juxley to use the word "ritualization" for certain phylogenetically preformed ceremonial acts in the so-called social animals.13 Erikson thus suggests that we should begin by postulating that behavior to be called ritualization in man must consist of an agreed-upon interplay between at least two persons who repeat it at meaningful intervals and in recurring contexts; and that this interplay should have adaptive value for both participants.1u According to him, these conditions are already fully met by the way in which a human mother and her baby greet each other in the morning. He says that ritualization in man seems to be grounded in the pre-verbal experience of infants and reaches its full elaboration in grand public ceremonies.15 Erikson regards "private rituals" as non-communicative and non-functional ritualization of behavior characteristic of animals in captivity. So far I have discussed rituals principally from the cultural viewpoint. Let me focus our attention now on the 13Erikson, op, cit., p. 337. In this regard, Lorenz comments on Huxley s usage and states: "If Sir Julian Huxley, when first describing ritualization, used the term without inverted commas, he did so on purpose, to emphasize that it denoted a purely functional concept equally applicable to a phyletic and a cultural process." Lorenz, "Ritualization in the Psycho-social Evolution of Human Culture," p. 278. 1"lerzuwon, op, cit., p. 337. Interestingly, v. w. Turner in his "Anthropological Epilogue" to the Royal Society of London symposium on ritual points out that "non-human 'ritualized' behaviour tends to occur most frequently in‘ dyadic or triadic social situations," while many forms of human ritual or ceremonial behavior often involve large groups of participants. Philosophical Transaction 23.332 Rozal Sociegz.p£_London, Ser. B, No. 772, Vol. 251, pp. 521-22. 15Erikson, op, cit.. p. 337. ' gel '4 "‘ nee. e e ...:,, e.. IV a” IH"‘AI 1': ”I ‘w‘ 20 evolutionary viewpoint in order to illustrate the significance of combining the cultural and ethological approaches for better understanding of the subject. Clearly from the evolutionary viewpoint, Huxley points out that: Two divergent tendencies seem to be operating in animal ritualization. One is for ritualized behaviour to evolve in the direction of an ethological reflex, by pro- ducing signals which release appropriate action with the minimum of delay: the other is to produce long-continued ceremonies with a sexual or social bonding function. During vertebrate phylogeny, ritualization has tended increasingly towards more efficient bonding, with more elaborate ceremonies, in which individual learning plays an increasing role, notably in the primates. Their ritualized behaviour foreshadows various human factors, especially in regard to rank organization, play, and, in chimpanzees, primitive dances. Similarly, primate characters used in threat and sexual ritualized behaviour foreshadow aspects of human dress, adornment, scarifica- tion which perform the same functions.15 Although when anthropologists talk about ritual they are usually thinking, primarily, of behaviors of a non- verbal kind, but speech itself is a form of ritual. As Leach puts it, "non-verbal ritual is simply a signal system of a different, less specialized, kind."17 Turner believes a review of findings regarding ritual as communication improvement will contribute to our understanding of the evolution of behavior patterns.18 In other words studying the relationships between verbal and non-verbal rituals may provide the link between the ethological and cultural approaches to ritualization. Accordingly, a number of social 16Huxley, op. cit.. Pp. 257-58. 17Leach, op: cit., p. #0“ 18V. W. Turner, "Anthropological Epilogue," Philo- sopgical Transactions p£_the Royal Sociepz p£,London, Ser. B, No. 772, Vol. 251, p. 521. artists are M" ‘1 :acie: to human ignoral line of . lith regard t 3:521, Sirdvtiste fit: and comparativ shroud us to re- :9 relationship bet and the York 01 1533. it has been: “(“1513 an adaptai 7' ion-t» «man Spacj 11‘. '- 1' "olutiona; ' W the soci. Cm" "1 5's? ugGStS. ' ‘e N "Jar, WStula EQOIOgiCall 21 scientists are now developing kinesic and dramaturgic approaches to human interactions.19 They seem to follow in the general line of development of the ethologists. With regard to the significance of this broader approach, Birdwhistell states that the work of the etholo- gists and comparative psychologists in the last few years has forced us to re-evaluate our previous conceptions of the relationship between human and animal behavior.20 That is, with the work of Tinbergen, Hess, Lorenz, Blauvelt, and others, it has become increasingly evident that social living is an adaptational imperative for the membership of many non-human species.21 And, if we are willing to concede that the evolutionary ladder runs from the inorganic to the organic to the social and, finally to the human, as Bird- whistell suggests, we must also be willing to re-evaluate our primary postulates as to the nature of man himself.22 Limitation of Ethological Approach Ethologically Sir Julian Huxley defines ritualization as: . . . the adaptive formalization or canalization of emotionally motivated behaviour, under the teleonomic pressure of natural selection so as: (a) to promote better and more unambiguous signal function, both 19Notab1y Goffman's Evepzdaz Rituals and other works, and Ray L. Birdwhistell, Kinesics and Context (Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1970). Cf., Birdwhistell, P. 79. 2Osirdwhisten, op, cit., p. u. 21Loc, cit. 221e1a.. p. 5. :ffici at St: grams of a thra-5P°°if1 32:15.1 bendi: is: so finding! menial bah“ use: or unit! _:::;::‘.3, ‘hich exactly. the“ axons have be! lcccr'iing «1.1 be broad‘ ....=.;zed displé lien: in natuz ~ treat activi‘ -~ 33:1”: a cor [I so correspsr 4“". 25 «s. A3 L "Mar occur- ' 'e “E- t.’ ““m‘itti tn: 2: ’Huxl : n 0? a“I SEDPa ‘ \ 22 intra- and inter-Specifically; (p) to serve as more efficient stimulators or releasers of more efficient patterns of action in other individuals; (p) to reduce intra-specific damage; andzgd) to serve as sexual or social bonding mechanisms. Based on findings in the field of ethology he concludes that animal behavior consists of a number of organized entities or units, each with a distinct function or set of functions, which may be called behavior-organs. And, more importantly, that the great majority of animal behavior— patterns have been subjected to a process of ritualization. According to Huxley, ritualized behavior-patterns can all be broadly characterized as displays. The simplest ritualized displays being monovalent, and the more complex, bivalent in nature.2u The latter is more frequent, notably in threat activities between rivals of the same sex, where it involves a combination of aggression and escape motives, and the corresponding intention-movements for attack and fleeing.25 As Lorenz, Tinbergen and others illustrate, these may occur in alternation, often combined in a compromise intention-attitude or threat.26 23Huxley, o cit., p. 250. His definition on ritual in man, supra, p. 16. 2“Monovalent ritualized displays is the simplest, and arises from singly-motivated intention-movements. 2 SHuxley, op. cit., p. 251. . 26Cf.,K. Lorenz, "Ritualized Fighting," in J. D. Carthy and r. J. Ebling (eds.). 223 Natural Histor of A ression (London and New York: Academic Press, 1965). ‘See also N. Tinbergen, "Behaviour and Natural Selection," in J. A. Moore (ed.). Ideas ppDModern Biologz (Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Zoology, New York, 1963). vol. 6, p. 521. See also his "Aggression and Fear in the Normal Sexual Behaviour of Some Animals," in I. Rosen (ed.). 223 Pathology ‘ppg Treatment p£_Sexua1 Deviation (London: Oxford University Press, 19 _ . .‘ . -l 1:!” are 593“ other ritualizat‘ ‘silly. it '35 bel‘ 33:21:de their 3:, outside of the a: from studying lfiratsry situatior our have been : fat-ting human bah. ts racial behavi o r termini: in th '41".- tey comunic Masses are been ”3!. They are ; ”idence that r i no. bulli‘: 23 There are some striking similarities between animal and human ritualization and canalization of behavior. Tradi- tionally, it was believed that animal behavior was solely controlled by their instinct. Some, in fact, even believed that, outside of the field of biology, we have nothing to learn from studying animal behavior, particularly in the laboratory situation. However, findings regarding animal behavior have been most helpful in generating hypotheses regarding human behavior.27 Not only studies dealing with the social behavior of primates but ethological studies of other animals in their natural havitat, e.g., the ways in which they communicate with each other, and their bonding processes are becoming more and more significant and note- worthy. They are particularly significant in the light of new evidence that not all animal ritualizations are geneticly based, e.g., bullfinches learning their song only from their father, or father-figure. J. Nicholai's case of a bullfinch raised by canary father-figure is analogous to human compul- sion to stick to habit and the fidelity to an acquired custom 27In this regard A. H. Maslow, H. Rand and s. Newman, in "Some Parallels Between Sexual and Dominance Behavior of Infra-human Primates and the Fantasies of Patients in Psycho- therapy," states: ese parallels have certainly enriched our perception, giving another dimension to many human psychological problems, enabling us to see much that we had not noticed before . . . ." (In Bennis and others, o . cit., p. 105). See also, Harlow and Harlow, "Social DeprivaEion in Monkeys," Scientific America, Vol. 207, No. 5 (1962), pp. 137-lh6; and. Harry Harlow, "The Heterosexual Affectional System in Monkeys," American ngchologgst (January 1962) and also in Bennis and . others, op. cit., pp. “3-60. For a systematic compilation of findings on animal social behavior, see also Robert B. Zajonc, Animal Social Psychologz (New York: John Wiley, 1969). no ion from one round that, in car ozone {£33323 fig: cars. leading to es riaparticular rar Infocds may be inc-~ nrituals, Special ‘Timlop in role: he role play ltlztion and mainte (5213.15. of tours T'hililation. In t in: a“ 30“ instl "135131: at '0 fit. can”. Doria ti more well as 1: G. I. can“: H th Mg] 1 fplon of her: 39 e Ri «oi Cul tur 31. n T "q. ' Carst I: :‘allng'h “ *‘ 3‘an _ 2h handed down from one generation to the next.28 It has been reported that, in certain primates, e.g., the Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata), what may be called proto-tradition occurs, leading to each group having its own food tradition with a particular range of acceptable foods. Now and again new foods may be incorporated in the traditional menu, and new rituals, special food washing ceremonies for example, may develop in relation to them.29 The role played by genetic inheritance in the evolution and maintenance of phylogenetically evolved rituals is, of course, taken over by learning in cultural ritualization. In the process of learning itself, however, there are some instinctive, or phylogenetically programmed, mechanisms at work, such as so-called creature habit - compulsion. Deviation from it is said to cause anxiety, in man as well as in animals.30 G. M. Carstairs, when discussing rituals as social 31 releasers, said that he was chary of analogies. Lorenz too agrees and says that an erroneous assumption of homology 28In Nicolai's study, a male bullfinch which had been reared by canary birds learned to sing canary song. His canary-like song was handed down in its pure form, without admixture, through four generations bred in aviaries in which plenty of normally singing bullfinches were present. (Lorgnzi "Ritualization in the Psycho-social Evolution," p.20. 290f. C. A. Southwick, ed., Primate Social Behaviour (London and New York: Van Nostrand, 1963). See K. Imanishi (p. 68) and J. Itani (p. 91) for their findings. Cited in Huxley, op, cit., p. 258n. 30Lorenz, "Ritualization in the Psycho-social Evolu- tion of Human Culture," p. 280. 31G. M. Carstairs, "Ritualization of Roles in Sickness and Healing," Philosoppical Transactions pp the Rozal Sociepz 25 £23133. Ser. B, No. 772, Vol. 251, p. 306. “use, of mysiol gurcasly nisleadin mtat the statener :to trap.32 "91‘0‘ suns plainly COW.- rnclude the possil fitted in assumin Zurich-patterns d ‘34, “have to be ftitlinally analogo ‘1‘: tat caution in 3531 findings n1 3:" ' 33m :11 tn) 33$. . p z 25 or, worse, of physiological identity, can indeed be dangerously misleading. However, he adds that he does not see that the statement of obvious analogies can lead us into a trap.32 "Provided," he says, "that an analogy concerns plainly comparable details sufficiently numerous to exclude the possibility of coincidence, we are fully justified in assuming that the two analogous structures or bchaviour-patterns do indeed have the same function."33 Thus, wo have to be cautious about not confusing being functionally analogous with being phylogenetically homologous. With that caution in mind more recent and relevant etho- logical findings will be discussed first, and then the discussion of ritualization in man will follow. Ethological Studies Much of the ritualization of animal behavior is in conjunction with the fourth function that Huxley discusses, 3h namely, to serve as sexual or social bonding mechanisms. Among territorial animals, for example, ritualized adver- 35 tisement of territory dominance is important. 0. R. 32Lorenz, "Ritualization in the Psycho-social Evolu- tion of Human Culture," p. 278. 331oid.. p. 279; 3“Supra, p. 20. 35Many important functions are expressed in territo- riality, and new ones are constantly being discovered. According to H. Hediger, animal psychologist, territoriality insures the propagation of the species by regulating density. It provides a frame in which things are done. Thus it co- ordinates the activities of the group and holds the group together. It also helps prepare ready responses against danger. (Cf. Hall, op: cit., p. 8). See also Niko Tinbergen, “The Curious Behavior of the Stickleback," Scientific American, Vol. 187, No. 6 (Dec. 1952), pp. 22-26, for a well known study of territoriality. “cater, a psycho. :fxrjeys in a nat; fztticas of terrih 3mm md evol' may be neither rem, his findin tzality as e beha "J? nth the same t1! territoriali t m“ to th“ tho W I territory, 22am in select L'Em' “as“ vim Sta “archer able to ‘1! Signals, “hi Mariam, has 3m . "5 agaUISt 0‘ 2" I. "'“Ient on “11¢ 36\ '23“! CR. Cal” \2 d r Q and g °_b1 \ a e O 38 Q ( I l sir” ”112% \‘ CD it. ”91113 We 31‘; “Va. 196 rkeo O 26 Carpenter, a psychologist who pioneered in the observation of monkeys in a native setting, came up with thirty-two functions of territoriality including those relating to the protection and evolution of the species.36 Although the list may be neither complete nor representative of all species, his findings indicate the crucial nature of terri- toriality as a behavioral system, a system that evolved in very much the same way as anatomical systems evolved.37 While territoriality offers protection from predators, it exposes to them these unfit ones too weak to establish and defend a territory, and. hence, territoriality reinforces dominance in selective breeding. Territoriality is also associated with status,38 inasmuch as that a dominant animal is somehow able to acquire personal Space by displaying threat signals, while the subdominant animal cannot.39 Also territoriality has the function of pr0per spacing, which protects against over-exploitation of that part of the environment on which a species depends for its living.”o 360. R. Carpenter, "Territoriality: A Review of Concepts and Problems," in.A. Roe and G. G. Simpson, eds., Behavior and Evolution.(New Haven: Yale University Press, 1958). 3711.113.. op. cit., p. 9. 38Cf.. A. D. Bain, "Dominance in the Great Tit, Parus Major," Scottish Naturalist, Vol. 61 (19h9), pp. 369-h72. Cited in Hall, opI cit., p. 9. For interesting discussion on this general topic, see J. N. Bleibtreu, The Parable 2£.the Beast, (New‘York: Hacmillan Company, 1968). Collier Books edition, 1969, ch. 6. 39Bleibtreu, op. cit., p. 203. uoHall, op, cit., p. 9. m,“ I 0"“!- ' 'DeIHI '.'.l 3235' 27 Among animals, especially so in non-human primates, grooming is important as a social and sexual bonding ritual.“1 Various modified forms are also seen in man. In birds, as ”2 on Redthroated Falconet shows that grooming Sparks' study takes the form of allo-preening, which is derived from the monovalent ritualized or at least highly formalized activity of auto-preening.“3 It might be mentioned here that highly ritualized inter-specific grooming has been observed to occur in some fishes. For example, Wickler reported that Labrcides dimidiatus, a Tropical Wrasse, removes ectoparasites from the bodies of other fish, and thus benefiting them. These fish often show special invitation postures, and the groomer occasionally displays in a sort of dance. And Labroides is never eaten even by large predators. Some groomers, such as Labrcides, are mimicked both visually and behaviorally by quite unrelated species, which take advantage of the "appeasement" attitude induced in the groomed species to bite pieces of flesh from them.uu Similar examples of “ICf., K. R. L. Hall and I. Dchre , "Baboon Social Behaviour," in Primate Behaviour, ed. by Irvin Dchre. (New York: Holt, Reinhart a Winston, 1965). Also C. R. Carpenter, "Soziologie und Verhalten freilebender nichtmenschlicher Primaten," Handb. 2001. vol. 8 (18). 10 (11). pp. 1-32. Cited in Huxley, op, cit., p. 257. th. H. Sparks“ "Clumping and Allopreening in the Redthroated Falconet, Ibis, Vol. 107, p. 2H7. “BHuxley, o cit., p. 257. Here a distinction is made between forma ze and ritualized behavior patterns. The former is more generic and broader, and it may not have necessary characteristics of adaptive quality, etc.. which are expected of ritualized behaviors. “nu. Vickler, "Mimicry in Tropical Fishes," Philoso- ppical Transactions 2! the Rozal Society 23 London, Ser. B, N0. 772. Vol. 551. PP. 73-7 . l ::;zliud,1.0n ad mun also in ins rally being that C ze- nd wasp-mimic} tel: tee to gain an they scavenge Huxley point iii-order") of g 12:236. in most bi 1:13.“ The stuc ‘1'. mm. In pa: ecu "4 b'haYio r I 28 ritualized, i.e., adaptively formalized, mimetic behavior are seen also in insects, with the biological advantage usually being that of immunity from predation, but the bee- and wasp-mimicry of flies like Volucella appears to enable them to gain unmolested entry into their hosts' nests, where they scavenge for food.“5 Huxley points out that a hierarchical rank-order ("peck-order") of graded dominance superiority and inferiority is found in most birds and mammals which live in small groups.“6 The studies of pecking order among monkeys are well known. In particular, Maslow and others studied the symbolic bdhavior of establishing dominance and subordinate positions among primates.u7 Here, interestingly, sexual bonding rituals were used as symbolic gestures in such cases. A similar point is also brought about in Harlow's study, where the young monkey who recognizes the other's dominance makes the gesture of mock-presentation.“8 To establish a peck-order among them works to eliminate intra-group fighting so that the group would have more time for feeding and other basic functions as a result.“9 As a consequence of submission or appeasing rituals which are adapted by weaker animals in uij., A. D. Imus, Insect Natural Histopz, 2nd ed. (London: Collins, 1956), Chapter 9. Cited in Huxley, op, cit., p. 257. 6 Huxley, op, cit., p. 252. l”Maslow, Rand and Newman, op, cit., pp. 105-20. 1‘8Harlow, op. cit., pp. “3-60. u9Cf., K. Lorenz, "Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels,‘ Journal far ornitholo 1e,.Vol 85, 137. And also his Qp ression lNew‘York: Harcourt, Brace A World, 1966). Cited in Huxley, op, cit., p. 252 as examples. ' ‘- I; 7 . I 1 ‘ U ne.‘ ~d‘ eo. we. lib 13W ‘L I.‘ 29 has been almost totally prevented. Such appeasement gestures are believed to release a built-in inhibitory mechanism in the strong animal, who is automatically stopped from actually attacking the other.50 In other words, among non-human animals, the use of ritualized symbolic behavior, as seen in the course of establishing pecking orders, reduces intra- specific killing; and, therefore, this ritualized behavior is rather crucial for the well being of the species. With regard to reducing inter-specific dangers, an interesting example among the ritualized behavior of animals is that of Stoats. The extraordinary behavior, which appears to be based on ritualized play activity, enable them to first attract the attention and then cause the immobiliza- tion of Rabbits. In turn, it is suggested, that the Rabbit's non-adaptive induced immobility appears to be analogous to the ritualized, i.e., adaptively formalized,"freezing" and death-faigning of many animals, including the Opossum.51 Huxley suggests that the most autaesthetio rituals are 52 possibly those of play and sports. In young mammals with long active juvenile periods, in particular, play-learning 5°In human situations. a similar function can be observed with regard to appeasement gesture, such as in an attempt to escape the consequences of traffic violations. Huxley, op, °1§n p. 252. 5ICf.. I. Bourliere, The Natural Histopz of Mammals (New‘York: Knopf, 195E). Also, D. Morris, The Mammals (London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1965). Cited inH uIey,o _p. .Elfioo PP. 255-57. 52Vocal mimicry, however, has an autaesthetio as well as an allaesthetic function. Huxley, op, cit., p. 256. LA" its: an :." :m: q. . :‘:e1.;z: ." as. h 2... ""e':: M. ‘ I‘I: ' ‘1 __,____,‘._-. -—-‘;— 77., . ~‘_ _‘7_,, ,m ,fi,—- 30 occurs and provides a valuable preparation for adult life, e.g., young lions learning hunting methods, as Schenkel reported, through repeated exercise of adaptive motor patterns of behavior supervised by adult females.53 Play is also a socializing activity in many species: in monkeys groups of young participate together},4 whereas in lions several adults and their cubs join together.55 It has been mentioned that repetitive pacing in captivity may be an example of pathological rituals which are resulted from restriction of activity, and are not func- tional.56 In agile species, new and individually ritualized acrobatic activities adapted to the cage environment may be developed similarly. In addition to restriction of activity, when crowding is added, "the resultant stress may result in anti-adaptive deritualization and disorganization of behaviour. Obvious analogies occur in man."57 In addition, at times, mal-adaptive and non-adaptive survivals of originally adaptive ritualization may occur in new environment. For example, the domestic dog's turning round on a rug is a non-functional relic of a ritualization which was useful in the wild.58 53R. Schenkel, "Play, Exploration and Territoriality in Wild Lions." Szpposium: Zoological Societz, 1966. 5bCf., H. I. Harlow and M. K. Harlow, "The Effect of Rearing Conditions on Behaviour." Menninger Clinic Bulletin, Vol. 26. 213. . "" "" _— 550f., Schenkel, op, cit. 56Cf., H. Hediger, Vild Animals pp Captivigz (London: Butterworths, 1965). 57Hux1ey, op. cit., p. 257. L00: Cite These ethologicz dinner with regari mm, are very i' 112g mi survival of 1:: :f behavior which my! captured ani mals of originali “Mum both ad at." survivals o f 31 These ethological studies suggests that ritualization of behavior with regard to grooming, pecking order, and play activities, are very important contributors to the well being and survival of the species. There are some ritualiza- tion of behavior which may not be functional, such as the pacing of captured animals and even mal- or non-adaptive survivals of originally adaptive ritualization. Similarly, we see in man both adaptive ritualization and mal- or non- adaptive survivals of ritualization. Ritualization of Behavior in Man In ritualization of animal behavior it has been observed that there are two divergent trends operating, simplification and complexification. Reviewing the evidence from recent ethological, psychological, psychiatrical and anthropological studies on ritualization of behavior in man, Huxley points out that one would recognize these divergent trends, (1) toward simplification of communication and immediate almost automatic action on the one hand, as in the mechanization of prayer into prayer-wheels or mere repetition of paternosters; and (2) to complexification, delayed action, long continuance, and bonding function on the other, as in the Mass or the initiation rites of almost all cultures.59 As Huxley points out the "peculiarities of human ritualization . . . care: caused by the much greater speed of psychosocial than of biological evolution. This leads 59Ibid.,.p. 266. VEIIfm'"' much higher {requ auxin survival of r i‘lier psycho-social "2:: earthquake of 1 sun“ mg example of are grimy hindered intercession .61 ] inWrath tend to “the age. h the same 1 3%; traditional 1533M 531001 Tie ;;ece of n’CkVBar Irisa Coma prae Heat a “in C01 372155“ a“ diSti 13:31, “53°01 tra his '1" a: .4 all: th 9 1‘. l 1‘! " “ carl‘yin 3:: got "at“ 3% 33.11 I -f. 32 to a much higher frequency of non-adaptive or even mal- adaptive survival of rituals originating in relation to an earlier psycho-social environment."60 The case of the Lisbon earthquake of 1755, as reported by Kendrick, provides a striking example of this sort where rescue operations were gravely hindered by religious insistence on ceremonies 61 of intercession. Prayers for rain and ceremonies to avert divine wrath tend to be regarded as non-adaptive in this scientific age. At the same time, adaptive time-binding and socially bonding traditional rituals in institutions are widespread. The "Old School Tie" is a real bonding mechanism, as well as a piece of neckwear acting as a personal recognition signal. It is a common practice in Japan for members of a company to wear a small company badge on their suit lapels or hats. Students are distinguished by their school uniform and other visible school traditions. Thus, in addition to simplification and complexification of rituals there also may be survivals of vestigial rituals, e.g., the carrying of nosegays, originally to prevent catching contagious diseases, by English judges, and the pouring of water over a tombstone, a custom in Japan, whose original function has been long forgotten. As to the function of rituals in man. generally speaking, "whether phylogenetically or culturally evolved. 6°Loo. cit. 611‘. n. Kendrick, The Lisbon Earthquake (London: Methuon. 1956). u . ~ . . . ~ _(. . ' . ‘ mm I mils: do in f ac :z, or canalizina fair: or groupS. :emessee of Pb in their differe tamerocesses a :mlzgeent by eac‘: In em, tit: “many speai to than in other Ficus distinctiv 333:1". his be 2"3? 0f the 1am “mung“ lea lit-natal develoz in“ team th, 535.33 hike of 1 ‘-' -. , . n: g, D (‘7 :5" 5' . y, ‘9 I l: "9 O M H J .. JJ ‘1 ‘1'! ~ I ~-.-- H' ~——' A' W a, :1 H- 0 av A 33 (rituals: do in fact perform the same functions of communica- tion, or canalizing aggression, and of effecting the cohesion of pairs or groups."62 There are obvious analogies between the processes of phylogenetic and cultural ritualization, while their differences concern the mechanisms underlying the two processes and the amounts of time required for development by each.63 In man, ritualization and canalization of behavior is, generally speaking, far more complex, elastic and varies more than in other animals. Erikson attributes this to the various distinctive properties of man's mental life and capacities. his behavior and development. Namely, the com- plexity of the language, man's great capacity to learn, and also prolonged learning process, together with very slow post-natal development of man reaching his maturity, con- tribute toward the complexification of ritualization in man. Erikson talks of these in term of affecting psycho-social evolution with regard to ritualization of man.6u Human ritual elements Erikson discusses various basic elements in human ritualization, each representing each particular stage of life.65 First, he isolated two elements in human rituals 62Lorenz, "Ritualization in the Psycho-social Evolu- tion of Human Culture." p. 279. 63Loo. cit. “or. E. Erikson, "The Roots of Virtue," in J. Huxley, ed., The Humanist Frame (London: Allen and Unwin, 1961), and also.f§T Erikson, Insight 222 Responsibility, (New York: Norton, 196“). , 65 E. Erikson, "Ontogeny of Ritualization," Philoso- M Transaction pf the Re a1 Societ: pf London, Ser. B, N°- 772» Vol. 251. pp. 337-59. "_"" ‘eruz eel—‘6” time clearly 87°“ :mlsmt: the "numint Ihe first elemen stand on the apparen tariffimation and {mites that its abet mating or extingu mill verify his infirm this firs1 1.34%: 1 “3’51" Olment whi :tiil‘ ' it? 15 based on 4)., ”334115“ , me ‘e‘ _u H i become; indi 5,: f‘ ift£liti,,.n58 Rulitet' 1 H 3h which seem clearly grounded in ontogenetic stages of development: the "numinous" and "judicial" elements. The first element, the numinous element in rituals, is based on the apparent need that man is born with for regular affirmation and certification of self by others. He states that its absence can harm an infant radically, by diminishing or extinguishing his search for impressions which will verify his senses.66 So Erikson suggests, there- fore. "that this first and dimmest affirmation, the sense of a hallowed presence, contributes to man's ritual-making a pervasive element which we will call the 'numinous'."67 Mutuality is based on the reciprocal needs of interacting human organisms. This ritualized affirmation, once insti- tuted, "becomes indispensable as a periodical experience and must find new forms in the context of new developmental actualities."68 Erikson calls the second basic element in human ritualization, "judicial," and places its ontological source in the second stage of life, i.e., early childhood. During this stage of growth, we have a growing psychosocial autonomy with rapid advances in level of psychosocial awareness, and 66Once aroused, Erikson states, "this need will reassert itself in every stage of life as a hunger for over now, ever more formalized and more widely shared ritualizations and rituals which repeat such face-to-face "recognition' of the hoped-for. Such ritualizations range from the regular exchange of greetings affirming a strong emotional bond, to singular encounters of mutual fusion in love or inspiration, or in a leader's 'charisma'." (Ibid.. p. 338). 671bid.. p. 358439. “gig. . p. 339. h ”'2". i n. “my" M 5:52 :52215 '3} :I‘ .e L. in!» v ‘5‘! {I} 0‘. I ,. A 1‘” I, e.H 33‘» a.“ 35 hence socializing pressures are developed to transmit the methods by which the discrimination between right and wrong are to be made. This second element of ritualization is differentiated from the numinous element primarily by an emphasis on the presumed existence of the child's free will. Unlike ritual- izations in the infancy stage, now the child is trained to "watch himself." In this process, he learns of negative role models which furnish explicit images of pseudo-species which he must not resemble in order to maintain the accep- tance of his teachers. These numinous and judicial elements can dominate a particular stage of life or a particular institution, but cannot produce ritualization all by themselves, other elements must combine with them. Erikson adds the elements " of "competence of performance," of "dramatic elaboration, and of "ideological commitment," each representing his next three stages of human psychosocial development, play age, school age, and adolescence, and leading to "generational sanction" as the last of the basic elements in adult rituals. The dramatic element (representing "play age") is "grounded in the maturational advances of the play age which permits the child to create with available objects (and then in games with cooperative adults and peers) a coherent ElEE Iiflfl,§£gpgpip turns and some form of climatic conclusion."69 691bid., p. 3th. A I l in. "He'a .fi“.~: "N we &‘ x522: .1! 37!. II ' , 36 Perfection of performance is the element added in the school-age. The sense of detail, seriously attended to within a meaningful context, characterizes this element added in the school-age. In fact it is because the mental and emotional capacity for such accuracy arises only with school-age that children are sent to school.70 Rules of performance become important and thus one may refer to this element as the "formal" element in the ritualization process. By adding all those above mentioned elements, Erikson says, we feel closer to an understanding of the dimensions of any true ritual. "But we also perceive the danger of over-formalization, perfectionism, and empty ceremonialism, not to speak of the neurotic 'ritual' marked by total isolation . . . ."71 Hence the next two, "ideological" and "generational" elements in ritualization are significant. Tb be fully grown in the human sense means "the readiness to join not only the technology but also certain irreversible commitments to one's pseudo-species; which also means to exclude (by moral repudiation, fanatic rebel- lion, or warfare) inimical identities and outworn or foreign ideologies."72 It is this ideological commitment with associated ritualizations that Erikson identifies with the adolescent stage of maturation. L 70For a general discussion on rituals in education, see B. Bernstein and others, "Ritual in Education," Philo- ical Transactions of the Rozal Socie§x_ of London, Ser. B, No. 772. Vol. 251. pP.-E29- 36. ' 71 Erikson, "Ontogeny of Ritualization in Man," p. 3&6. 72 ' ' Loo. cit. lit regard to ‘ elite of the adult, 4: earliest ritual artisans time act I tiecho and reaffi '1‘. zlzois. Hence. n ‘41:! outogehesis 01 with: the sancti Tannin} lh U km" the core arsenal incest: “"7 Spirits or g. them“: for ‘ 37 With regard to the dominant function of ritual in the life of the adult, Erikson emphasizes the fact parents as the earliest ritualizors in their children's lives must at the same time act as participants in instituted rituals which echo and reaffirm the ritualizations of their own childhoods. Hence, "the prime contribution of adult ritual to the ontogenesis of ritualization . . . :13 that: ritual reaffirms the sanction needed by adults to be convincing ritualizers."73 Thus, Whether the ceremonies of the adult years call on personal ancestors in the beyond or on culture heroes, on spirits or gods, on kings or leaders, they sanction the adult; for his mature needs include the need to pp periodicallz reinforced in his role of ritualizer, which means ... to become a numinous model in his children's minds, and to act as a judge and the transmitter of tradi- tional ideals. This last element in the ontogenetic series I would call the ggnerational which includes parental and instruct ve, productive, creative ang:curative endeavours. Table 1. Ontogeny of Ritualization75 muhmflity influmy 0f recognition early disgiiming- on 0 childhood ‘, good & bad play i dramatic ... J. “is:- school- 1 l 1 rule? of er or- 38° , f p mance adoles— l 1 solggarity _cence J, , J, conviction alumnus - GEMHmh in adult NUMINOUS JUDIC DRAMA RMAL IDEO rituals m TIC F0 LOGICAL sfigflhm 7322151. . p. 3&7 . 7uLoc cit. 75Ibid.. p. 38h. 532:3 iud human 3 In other word: :syicsocial erolr .iy;:genetic basis simian, not by th it's relatively ex 321:: instead of i “3131.75 Also for .téta a high degre rL“nonhuman ri tu iitetic or self-re ”mm Huxley e 53‘s .a1 latiox 'l‘umst be 38 Ethology and human ritualization In other words, the process of human ritualization in psychosocial evolution has a primarily ontogenetic, not a phylogenetic basis; is directed mainly by psychosocial selection, not by the genetic mechanism of natural selection; and is relatively extremely rapid, operating in terms of decades instead of in millennial periods, and shows accel- oration.76 Also for the same reason ritualization in man lead to a high degree of individuation.77 In addition, many more human ritualized activities have a strong auto- aesthetic or self-rewarding component than those in animals. In summary, Huxley enumerates as follows: In man, we find not only the adaptive canalization and ritualization of overt behaviour, but also that of thought or 'inner behaviour', resulting in motivated idea-systems and in internal (psychological or mental) organizations concerned with guilt and conscience, anxiety and ’oceanic’ feelings. Secondly, every human being has to construct his private ritualizations, his own canalization systems, to guide the chaos and conflict of infantile thought and feeling into individually adap- tive as well as socially acceptable channels. Thirdly, ritualization in the broad sense may spread from the emotional into the intellectual sphere. there resulting in adaptive canalizations of thought, like concepts and formulas, ethical and legal principles, scientific laws and theological doctrines. Such extended ritualization is no longer directly comparable with ritualization in animals and should perhaps be more strictly categorized as formalization, but both have similar functions, and both must be considered together in any gengral survey of the adaptive canalization of behaviour.7 It is evident that ritualization and canalization of behavior in man influence the organization of thought and behavior 760f., Huxley, op. cit., p. 257. 77ibid.; p. 259. 78Loc, cit. 5'; :‘9 . .p’ "‘ fin, and thus the :i::ssologies as V arssnies, architec Enan rituals bail functions: 5 :2, e.g., scapegoa :siizss. Rituals :Liziry rituals; ri Ital rituals; p53: lit; rituals; lega finals; nouming ri titimittee ritual 36! d. passage" I 5.39311er to man. Homer, hUInE 9:131”. thus becc “‘3 at Particu] 3,2... ...th revolutmr ““53““ to a at its; . , edmh agri, a: . . 'mn'ins hunt the: 0' ‘. . n runCtion ‘Q _ mm: than r 39 of man, and thus the formation of kinship systems, religious and cosmologies as well as symbolic expression in rites and ceremonies, architecture and art. Human rituals can be categorized by principal psycho- logical functions: some ritual involve psychological projec- tion, e.g., scapegoat rituals, and others such as masochism or sadism. Rituals may be grouped by social function: military rituals; rituals of national prestige and bonding; medical rituals; psychiatric rituals; greeting and hospi- tality rituals; legal rituals; appeasement rituals; moral rituals; mourning rituals; liturgical rituals; parliamentary and committee rituals; oath-taking rituals; the universal "rites de passage" rituals; seasonal rituals. Humor which is peculiar to man, is also ritualized and canalized.79 However, human rituals evolve through psychosocial evolution, thus becoming ecologically adapted to particular habitats at particular times. For example, after the neolithic revolution, hunting rituals had to be abandoned or relegated to a secondary position, and new rituals concerned with agriculture had to be evolved. And, not only that, surviving hunting rituals underwent considerable change in function. Thus, it is more confusing to classify human rituals than rituals of other animals. As has been mentioned before, there is a radical difference between biological evolution in animals, which 79cr., A. R. Radcliffe-Brown, Structure and Function in Primitive Socie (New York: Free Press, 1965f, particu- larT—_—— y Chapter and 5_ on joking relationship. abused on E’Mti' this his“! °n I 33:103. his d izioecity I“ m c; itself i! Pr Iron an “h mined, the 5:31: the d" shtiouery d0"1 fzrneech took P] hing: of canton ring on the na‘ terstand ritual: than between Litell postulat ho is based on genetic transmission, and cultural evolution, which is based on non-genetic cultural transmission and tradition. This difference is explained largely in terms of capacity for what may be called true (symbolic) language,80 which itself is properly called ritualized behavior. From an ethologist's point of view, an example of ritualized, i.e., adaptively formalized, adaptation in H222 sapiens is the development of speech. The psychosocial evolutionary developments which resulted in the capacity for speech took place a very long time ago, and the findings of contemporary anthropology have absolutely no bearing on the matter.81 Leach urges that in order to understand rituals in man, it is essential to study the relation between speech and non-verbal rituals.82 Bird- whistell postulates that kinesic structure is parallel to 80The only well-formalized animal language is that of bees. (K. von Frisch, 22: Dancing Bees. London: Methuen, 195E). Referred to in Huxley,pp.'ppp., p. 258. Man is unique, however, in developing language that can be written down and recorded. Margaret Mead wrote with regard to language in its broad sense that, . . . all natural languages that survive are sufficiently redundant that they can be learned by the members of any Homo sapiens group. (We: . . . would say that any language developed by a human society can be learned by the members of another human society, and that this learning is possible both because of the redundancy, which provides for a whole range of individual differ- ences in sensory modalities, memory and intelligence, and because language has been conceptualized the world over as a part of culture that can be learned by members of different cultures. (Margaret Mead, Continuities in Cultural Evolution, Tbrry Lectures (New Haven: Yale T—nivcr'—sity_‘rrc"ss'._196u). p. 1+5). Quoted in Birdwhistell, op, cit., p. 108. 81Leach, op, cit., p. #0“. 82Loc, cit. ‘17." lung "'z £11 Lugsge structure, a to are astonishin Generally we u re use ordinary the anti interact E te behavioral “abducts of Q, Will in .an in I‘ Winch ”3.1135 ‘111 in man 1‘ or :‘lm'literat. s‘ tats of inform 2mg t° d«semi -‘~ ri . . it (lacuna 0 f #1 language structure, and that the kinesic system has forms which are astonishingly like words in language.83 Generally speaking, ritual is a communication system, and we use ordinary speech and non-verbal ritual to commu- nicate and interact with others. In fact, the verbal part and the behavioral part are not separable. Both of these are products of the given culture.8u Discussing rituali- zation in man in relation to conceptual and social develop- ment, Leach emphasizes the importance of the "language" or ritual in man for the survival of society. The abilities of non-literate societies to transmit extremely elaborate amounts of information from generation to generation without having to depend on written record Leach attributes to the existence of verbal and non-verbal rituals.85 Thus, studying primitive society would serve us in understanding the function of language as ritual toward survival of society, and Leach's analysis, therefore, would be appro- priate to be reviewed here. 83Birdwhistell states: ”. . . at least as far as English, American and German .kinesic systems are concerned. . . . it has become clear that there are body behaviors which function like significant sounds, that combine into simple or relatively complex units like words, which are combined into much longer stretches of structured behavior like sengences or even paragraphs." (Birdwhistell, op, cit., p. O . . He found no gesture or body motion which has the same social meaning in all societies, That is, as he puts it, "Insofar as we know, there is no body motion or gesture that can be regarded as a universal symbol." (Ibid.. p. 81). 8“For an insightful discussion on the elements of ritualization in various stages of life of man, see Erikson, op, cit., and also supra, pp.33-36. 85Infra. p. 12h. AIIIIIIIIIIIlIIIllIIIIllIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII-----::____________ .' I ...-mum" Studies of 3‘ mi“! have adaP xiiitions in vhi ch 73112:, the lustra ileiiaged to surv tiers vould find i zfniays or hours tm people were 3 attrition to gen er Emotion about t 'Jiteay best be hence of any Writ “333?. Leach :ritnal "no, tc 3““ survival of Pumps 80-; Hillbut not no‘ a.“ more verba; '42:: 30! today. v #2 Studies of some non-literate societies indicate that they have adapted to the particular environmental conditions in which they existed. For instances, the Eskimos, the Australian Aborigines, and the Kalahari Bushman all managed to survive in environments in which most out- siders would find it impossible to stay alive for more than a few days or hours. They could not have survived unless these people were somehow capable of transmitting from generation to generation an extremely elaborate body of information about the local topography, its contents and how it may best be utilized. "How is this achieved in the absence of any written documents or of any kind of formal schooling?" Leach asks. According to him, the performance of ritual serves to perpetuate knowledge which is essential for the survival of the performers.86 Perhaps so-called modern languages may be more complex but not necessarily more advanced: they merely take up more verbal space. In comparison to our own speech habits of today, which contains a vast amount of redundancy. in non-literate societies the whole of knowledge has to be encapsulated into a memorizable set of formalized actions and associated phrases. Quite analogous to our contemporary manner in which we store information on punched cards or computer tape, primitive people use the objects which they employ in ritual in a similar way: the message is not 86Loach, op, cit., p. 1:05. I I m_ awed by the obj augment and seg Information ' {regeneration to a; be categorize information she It other Ien. mm PGOples e that"tories of h t: they use to c mm"? of burr 5111b. thought “7 e. U “:7. “So, if. “an. t0 lake ar ttoathich 1, “i t: is em,“ 6d ruling , clear ‘ tihtuu on the ' 0: :ters, may be 1&3 conveyed by the objects as such but by the patterned arrangement and segmental order. Information which must be stored and transmitted from generation to generation for the survival of society might be categorized as (1) information about nature and (2) information about society, that is, the relations of men to other men. As it is evident in Australian totemism, primitive peoples economized words by using and classifying the categories of human society by means of the same words which they use to classify the categories of nature so that a group of human being and a class of natural objects may all be thought of as representations of the same entity.87 Also, in primitive societies, it is hardly possible to make any clear cut distinction between infor- mation which is expressed in verbal form and information which is expressed in non-verbal action.88 Thus. the idea of making a clear distinction between myth on the one hand and ritual on the other, as did Jane Harrison, Malinowski and others. may be also challenged. As one observes ritual in primitive communities, it is a complex of words and actions. It is not the case that the words are one thing and the rite another. The uttering of the words is itself a ritual. For ordinary non-literate people there are many kinds of information which are never verbalized but only expressed in action. In our ordinary conversation among 87Ibid., p. uo6. 881bid.. p. #07. ...-no— .. ‘ mates also, tr 7;: action, in i1 raring, but the I car already abou :fritsal sequenc In any ritual to be notices in the ‘same to stability 1‘ d”indent 'f the actor: ”355 it p05: littl. “rial ttafiination , car-.enscd Vi FusiSOly as 3:: his "I? to It: . 4 “high pr nu intimates also, the totality of the behavior, namely words plus action, in its proper context, conveys more complete meaning, but the meaning is conveyed because of what we know already about the context. This is particularly true of ritual sequences. In Leach's words, In any ritual performance some of the actors are likely to be novices but the majority will have participated in the 'same kind' of ritual many times before; indeed the stability of‘the form of the ritual through time is dependent on the fact that it is familiar to most of the actors. But while the familiarity of the actors makes it possible to reproduce past performances with little variation this same familiarity allows the combination of words and actions to be drastically condensed without final loss of communication value ..8 precisely as happens in the conversation of intimates. For this very reason that Leach warns against attempts to interpret the meaning of ritual by anthropological intuition, the kind of interpretation which has been quite common in the past and still seen today. He states that "no inter- pretation of ritual sequences in man is possible unless the interpreter has a really detailed knowledge of the cultural matrix which provides the context for the rite under discussion."90 As many anthropological studies on body motions in the recent years indicate, it would be reasonable 89Loc. cit. Moreover, unlike when we wish to convey information at a distance by means of speech alone without reference to context, where redundancy as in writable speech is valuable for clarity, the more condensed message forms, which are characteristic of ritual action, are generally rappropriate to all forms of communication in which speaker and listener are in face to face relations and share a common body of knowledge about the context of the situation. Thus, the condensed and multi-faced concepts as in primitive society do not lead to ambiguity. "In any event in ritual sequences the ambiguity latentin the symbolic condensation tends to be eliminated again by the device of thematic repe- tition and variation." (Leach, op. cit., p. #08). 9°Ibid., p. #07.” :szy mt 695“” a: in meaning 091 mm! of mat i Edvard Bali hatter how ha' tweszhinself of tor-acts of his 1m: in world. may control manufgz v ul Lin are 010? A" at.“ . not onlv Zibccauso peat 13‘5ng ray 1 Thus, as f§ JE‘ but it c "QB-ailing b .5319.“ “5 30ciet1 it. ‘Qg‘qagES c {h ' 1 "i a: 1‘5 to say that gestures are culturally linked both in shape and in meaning.91 Both are products of the given culture, and that of what Erikson calls pseudo-species. Edward Hall, in his Ihg.Hidden Dimension, says that "no matter how hard man tries it is impossible for him to divest himself of his own culture, for it has penetrated to the roots of his nervous system and determines how he per- ceives the world. Most of culture lies hidden and is outside voluntary control, making up the warp and weft of human existence."92 Moreover, "even when small fragments of culture are elevated to awareness, they are difficult to change, not only because they are so personally experienced but because pe0ple cannot act or interact at all in any :meaningful way except through the medium of culture."93 Thus, as Birdwhistell states, "not only can we dispense with so-called 'natural' gestures as being single-culture bound, but we can be prepared to discover that the methods of organizing body motion into communicative behavior by various societies may be as variable as the structures of the languages of these societies."9u La Barre too questions the universality of any gesture, even seemingly "natural" auui universal smiles and laughter, and says that many of 91"Communication without Words," in lWAventure Humaine,” Encyclopedic 51.2.3; Sciences 51_e_ l'Homme, Vol. 5 (Geneva; Sister; Paris: De la Grange Bateliere, 1933’, pp. 157-66. Cited in Birdwhistell, o cit., p. 79. 923. Hall, p. 188. 93Lbc,.git. guBirdwhistell, 02. cit., pp. 81-82. :55: motor habits :iorstanding in :11 tile gestu 2:31 and meanings ;:is also P055151 :fzis m. mat zooming Specia tifmot fro- ma {Especial idiolc moral is a pr i:nrso.96 Insc 'fl :zmoicationz “PM ranges o: t4‘.adivi