. .1...Hh...w- pas-.14? 735m?” . ... imfiwfi ...me .- [...-.VRLV. 9-. :2 . H :1 . ‘ by; ...-R VV ..V . . ..., a%- - . ....érhfi , - , _. .: 7.3%.,3 u: . g _ , - ... ”- Sin-.3. aumwwwu$u$§%‘ {RH-“Lawn Wag . _ ax 5x . i.» 119.. ....bmvvnmeri k. ..anvaufimeov . mw kahuna.” iv. , V . V v - F X. ~h. . 3‘ . V NJ?! .WMW-yww. %%J*vkiulmn \v..ham.m V . . V "O. . .. w :- . ...-e. n «vignhw mums-.3. ._ W ,m . 33?;- y...» WWW: .2 ., Z3. .1 . , - «gli, I WWW“ pm. ... . , imam-uh. . V \n 3 V4 #1 ‘ , V .1- 3 5) r5 . V V a V . (R ._W , . - a. 5%. 953%...ka V .3. , . a», . . .. . . L, x xnwfi nih- iv .1... .... I t V a K.” . 3n x 19? 34!. X. X. 45....3 5.... iv 1 \H. "tau; 3.. , .\ .v .. v.7 : . H. .. .RSKNPAWM. 1391...». mflim ... q. I“ ~ . 5 ~ A, .u V . s. )13 RV? \1. H .\ 1H J. . 9......- bfius- ...,u .3 ,swwk-Qvlub-m ma..- .flmfihnfm. .s-V. a an»... . . xh A. vikfit. .H\3.1.}wm in: ...-«V: \. ...? V l. ..s a . L .. ... 3.: ...: ..kmCVs-fi .. 3-1.»... .mfifix-ghhmaaxbfiuh Ln .. in .3... . .- mfiv x s a t... :5. k. ,.. 3V» \. I)? {a}... .xildfihw. )1. +15% 1 [.5 . . . V , - Lawns V ....a 33.3%. ...mu‘mmuua.$.mx $.03 ,3. u..- anx o... 5393, - . . . s ., -.\ «3 ,.7 .. V. “Rama-W. x VV - . ...». . 98am}:- Lwfimmfiwu5 amp him-g. . x. , a ‘ Hum-4.: n. . . a V 2.61.3. ...-:4: .13 - 9-b- zgnwmmfil $1...- . . 3.35.3. :9. v. ... 5;. . hi“ ‘3 \Rywmwomfiuua fihfi. K .4. -. 4-13.“...5 ...V flvgfihfifififlfififl» 7 ‘ “X ‘ .l‘.» y . L . .4 ‘ , ‘Ll SI 5y 1 V. \ \- .\. (17} ‘ \VI I at $3.3... V . Q». iifiklunzk. .a .meEkt}. £535.01 .3 1K. 12.1.43... 4! Jury?) \xukmuv , 1).. . Kr Hr? - . .. .. V LAN. 7.. End-«max L a .. 3:. xx w 3. ~ . 7-:- tamxfl.‘ 4a.! 3 L4 4. ., . r52. . . . - . ‘ .3 .1! .... 5. x. ssh... x; - 5. Hak- . . rk flax... .. 1 IN _ . - 5...: . V . I L. :1. It} .411... . 1‘0“: x r...” x\ 3 is}... . _ V, . . _ _ kfi-hhfiififiasw 3... «N»; . .. m £3 . ,V . . t V.. {.9 mm“... ...-a is{-me-nbmfiuwhxmmfimmwmwfimfiw. .V . . -, . V . hay-$32.. A0¢WnMx§axv IV. ~awnbwfiVLMuv-fim 1 I... . - v x s V. ...-.9 3.... u 51 2-. 1.13. u..V....x....n. . . 3;. ...-H1243: .. 4 \ raga}: . $.51.- ..V... A; . t» i V .. ’9.L§. ....(wr \ x \V ..3 \13 xmk. ...Hl..!\..~.\.3.\. 1 .\.w.. hwfikix. 1»... . t .r‘ a? - . $.21: .f‘¢b..3v . . 2 v) 1v . r . V. M , L 1 . i x)... axfisfi >13. V1. . X t . 34%.. .23. V ..4 u ... . . . Vz.r.n.........m..-.vu.. . Emu»? bum...» EV, 3.13me usxufiuwmum-Tfixi . . singufiiamkifi V V ...! 3.5.5. : Vt- n... ”brand-Ham: .2 ...:&§... .913 .mflfihfi ...-nu, $9.388»? $- - .. s n.3- nfifimme-xsbo- .V 1_uve..nvv...vvn.»..§lvr of” .u . ukkwxu. 4... n: 355-“...- n. 35...»? n...“ Khan-.3 33%.. .vsn 11‘.» a. w . rt ./ O . g - \Jifiarwa. \u1\51\\3v\fl.11\Hwfifiuhfihvx$xsu.3\ aflufiNVQEJflawfl/qu.“ .. rm. .V 131Whn.)\~m1flvxuwixxxUJWNHfihxx-ugl:{hh7 lg. . is: 32. .>.V..:::. as). .. 3.3.5.3}: . 53... -. V r; ‘3’ in ~ 4B§LMVAT 4.. (MN: - V I . - i1. : JENNW! «um? 25...: 5.3. V . an .1 1-. . fl-wvsfii. 0?\‘,\.m.s.flzV.?an. KM...- . - . :3 . x .3. 6%»... a .almmlw. ‘M11531- 11kt:- k. V .23. .fiefiaxg . FEM-L a .1 at. 1 is .. .qu. L v.4“ - - . 3535.3: ). .1...- «vsdfi- 3L! 1‘ «his huerMQRHHbWH-du ... unuhuw... Jan. 2.- :44. 3., {hr-nab? ;.. . X J. n 3%». «han- - mmmk A nth-W; . . . .- s V V . .. t V . . . - 5- b} .5 . .1. l... L153 [IS-dun sewn-..- , 34.11 L: ...; ...}... - ......fuflft . «173552? R. .2.” N Kamuw13§¥ 9 - $. ...LL - 1.... .\ V - x. . LR.“ _ .l . ...?A4‘LLV‘J..¢ - . . .. ..21.ka.mthmQWmHHWnfiWoflwfifinflflhi ...”ka V . ...x . . NM 1-. Mn .hfln . rummnmmwfl 3:.- .. a..y....\aaa3:‘\..?h.sazaaflx) ... .3... 1.32923... b. 5-H». ...-H..- . . . . J... . .. v1 . t 4?: 2i). .... ix}. 3 19.... £314.93 - x . :3 Va... . a. it V i 21.3.. -. x... 133- 23. ,J . >1 . V .4 4 .5.) u .39 2 x23. 3 .232 .xL. ire-.ifii .3... - 3‘33. \$.1.¢\ \\ - . . .u. .. . L . .1 31.3. I9 tflVs... 3 V .KgflHNVkVeur-k “DU“ \r‘r‘wahnflui. k". x5 .6234 . . . . -vmfiilwimfiluxssfl 13 .534. like - .,.u.-.\\(..$\\ , . .. wfifiwhauuflj........HNhCIUAuwuwwV-flkflnvhuua .\ .7 a $5.5... 1 . , .- V a} 3.?3-NHh-é.“ .5" ...-{.33. - .5 1a.. “nu-.1143... . . , . 3&ALKHKVIxrvlanL... .mfinm3‘3m his ‘32. a 6.53.. V - - - . nhfimnuTafiumavufi fifthfi Law‘s-,1. A, 33:; I... .V ,. 1 . . 1.-.?! _ {x p P I g V}. {12... x1~m~s§ankhnwfi 321‘}&a§% . . 3-. It?“ 9 . \ {2,132. >352. 1 Z. )Qnfiiaah'svsziaa S3... . ..l.¥;rgvlb {Ox-Oil V!!! i! )s.-W\.fl.\7«flfi.:\\s\1\?. . .Iht. z ..\ r a]. $19.»? I. 7.71%.: ... ‘Vfig vr? . 2.3.24 ..s-Qxdnixinwflvflnv \51-Nflxflyq‘o. - ”£52.11 urn-5.3.“..Nanw. 1.3.2511?! ...smnwhaN-wzflwun-snmfiflfl.‘ n2 {it'll}... -£_wfly+os% . . x 53....“ x “.2130. 2: hi . J .V.-.-...-.utV-..le..”..Tst.?:o{..J. . .23. , aova-bammbnh-‘stfiuufit .ssvyTbilr. '31:..- V .r\naIi1 -. 15)..)- 13.3. ...-412,6rhubwuv ..VJfiflfflum-wwgt «53‘ s . x tunudTvVMs - 3.2.: . V 1.. .V ,rfw.uv.%hurdv.1«3in% 7 u . tfltt‘.vh.’uv§ 3:01 - 7.. Ma}: - V 31... \. ..31§35£\+43,k\ .. .2 25 .. v.1\1».Vl.!....,lw\5 Iv . 5k.) . 1“} 1)) “S“rflfimt‘nax \ ..- 3....G-q-ééflthfl 1‘ 5 «as-hauls“; J V - - . 2th” . . . Z. S . :I.-4V.nthw.....m ~ V. . V iiifabohgq V...» 31.3.9" W51 1 any... i - . “Eaumwttrtfi ALMWWfi. \P..Iil$\ 35.1 At - V - 1-45...» (Iliz. .Lnxfifiwilau. . 9. .~. . .1 alman- fut-unwb.hu..!.nb.wtfi$hit-LL rhmwfifi MbvrIJLJnNnN: . - g has? fflzunrl.¢taipn.fr%fih7finfl£$rm fi 1:12.132}. .. , v\ . ¢ V V .u r ...-Hf!!!)- . 11...?»- (It, thfiflhfififlntixfifia353.0“??? - «1%., 1 ... .135 ...... bins-nam- .ue. fluid...»- V.. > . 1 ¢ 4.1 . V ,i I Shit... (y J3. up . 21)). 35.). 1.45.0: 1353‘ - . 7) .9... . .. t: I .....is.) )5 , 3km. . . .- . . - . .. met fit-ti .- $31.35.. 1v$shumvfl$aia .. x é «MK 1.. V .....uusljth-Zulfi 3-2..- ..urzf... . 3.. - a . ., Vs-x.uxnii-ufi.....wahwflv‘nhngafias}? t I... b. V ... Vault-«Q2 V nun-hump? - . - . . . .. . \.L.L.....h§1:.i..- - V. .. salil.2z..Vv-.s\s\azit1vlnv Ivtnfltbshcbfluu d5!»..r!v&5.,#._. . (5.5.2.1317 off . . . . , 52...;- . . ib...es-.-3W.Ifirl V fir“ ”u . . .1 r A...» 7. 72 $¢figwflvth 3 ~ .I r... ..- - .fi...=...u..$c..x.. 1 Pu . LAW-NF LVMACL4§2M>§LJMLL V a 7 insrgxgysfizfi I. V A.Itfi&.4 .m-PKLL...\.uW-3n1ucud~l£ .561 I:fi.~f.1.) 2mm”! .. 5; {hilt - . .- vvk‘f ...! 1 1.1.. 157.333..»5133 . . 34 Afilv. All, 1.1!..43513-9L22ngé32. Int-a... .quz . n - Einst- . .h-wrl litiluxiz‘h'113nn8fivfih. 5 hl. fi 5‘ . V - - 1‘ x .151 .. . .. 3T; Jud-{1.2.31} . 11¢ 3.. t, 2 . . 3N; no.3...“ V- .. ,. .Jvfikvhq‘.\\.n.hnllfi_u($smu31! s an. .\_.<.v Janus... \ fivfi rm V 11-733 ..ZKw Vat. ...v‘s. Av).537.w , . LU: l V. Lu»... . V . ~1\;KD.1fl110WMXMflIJn7 sgfiiaflfl. . . ‘05.. V. vufflflhn- 5M...- z . " I II. . l . .21I\v . . i x- ..whht? L . - - .. . . Atfii‘t-V-A - .91}? $1! FE. r VV ,7; .ugmwnuuuhmrflu _ V. . V . . h: ....J.);...... .L . . . . V .. . ..véhlut4irL . - w-t...L.-é..l V . . £4. if} - 5‘ V _ - ...:étatn .0... U! ma!) ....1 \LAV-fi‘1Lt‘g V 51 . d . ‘fikht n.71LLA114. d.l<.\ nu... x.\~..11..0...$1: :97 .6: “Ni; (fit-.34. - . . . V . . 341:1? . . \fl‘}. r1. . X . . .V a, tiff-«fig ,7...Lh b . . . . «rig. is).fluV-llunwgmmmwwbwzhfihfiunhflfi y , ... fufl».:du<..» a»... (Luau .mfiwwérinflwdf V - . .. vlifitivficc 7.3“... xx. 51 )3.“th 7.... \9:W\~HW.W~5V.\¢.J‘N..0.N)~V;N11- - - . \aK "WW”... . ufifxfi it! {7.- ua. .. .‘HN mitt-1.70 L ; 1.1a].- .ixgi 3a .§:\.\qu3. . - . 1"! at. 1), i \\ ‘fr‘. . i. ..u- . . LN! I .1; flfi?..rfl,lflvflyuu:lfl 1:02.37.’ ... ‘ . _ a if) V \.I\?\wfl4\$glfi.iflnmxxtx§ $.31... .nn-JY . 7v 4...? {.351 5.332....hbgylyfx1flx n35... ~ IZQQ-fluurfibnfiw : .5 . 11.1,...3! V : .3! r! .lfl}...v¢flw. ......gcf'huwv' %§ 12...... hihhxamazxi?13§ . r a 7 31".... : . -. V. x!- .» I V- . .....11. i _ 2...... . . .V..- r. >3...J...P in . .n . u 01: all! . .1 inhptp-nrbfla... fom-tfxnflfiuz. e 4 {5.522.333 y n ,r . ‘ . fifiuuzflhohfi-ailyhuflf - rrv’v.’?r. 1(a).- ...? (.21: w.H.uVHWnnMN1uvf..-\$)A$\ I... 313‘s. 6.. .. 3!... 13?.- IE1 is . 1 2:. a, 6V . .géfzfizfii. . hull... .. - . . » $§¥i¥$§tfi . _ V 3.2.4.5.”..1 , 715. . . ,. . , - ~I- Ito... , h» I 1.1.3 no...“ mod n... ~rbtktk..t um...» 332%; that V..- . 5:». rflwuflfiunn. r. AM.“ .J wy‘wf -1 :y 7‘ I (”L {firinzfiflnmwdnfi 33..---.me £1!in .. ~ A 7. 15.1.1. 15.5.}- .15: L. %.C {L .Lbs.v§l)1+.ith «A gfifihfl. .... u? a? . 5? .131?sz V2 7n. 3.. V Bflga....u§ . . - . tisfpni If: .5 7. a Riot”; I. Chic).- .7... . L! V . . u. arr-fl. :3. hmzuv via“ .... b~t&?v.v~ whi‘. ‘AL - . . .vzfiakmutrq. . “trik‘. .V‘ Vi: . taunt-z h..." t&.~l§..flu.u.$.fiqh Ju- . than; Ban-L1 luv-VII.» 3710., . fl 3 fr?! ... ... 11.... \. f V .x. . . g .93.“ . .flfiki.h IV...” . . . 7ft. 5 § , 4 a a - 9"” A} r «t v. E» L . ‘14}!7’71‘ ~n|a'\‘. - ". «Vii-1.73148. . 9|: . .9691. I}! 3... ..VTtV SI 7 {ilfi‘i-ifir («H-fixm a . ..D’ZEIVtibg ...!!! .. . 5 11.»... fin“... a... . .- . ‘17.. - {Ob-.t.t-3Nwtmw§.x.. . . . LA .02.... 5131» .3! 7".) .. ”gin-Ennnwmuifié .1 ..f... . ~ {fixflh ..oAmesPdflmmmuflLdflw‘t Vfl7r~umfivikatuhflwfum £1 . . - t. . A. 4A . 1 (k.- 3. V . V1 ...;;.fi..t{¥.hwmfi. zilwzwfifs. 1T.-&5 e .‘6 L)“.- N}Z.ffl.~flhfififlflhu.ffiu . V V I . . . h‘V‘ . z i v r K. C .. . d, 31...... «a... £831 , tux-... . ...-unmask»? Haunt. swfla... «um: 25.2.: 1+...» aquzwhumin. amp-mgu-nfi 2. . 3,. Int. ...-....ta. 392251 .1 J . 3: ... .6. . . V -. V... +2.» .3? .. +3 ......cuu. V : 3+ezuuzfigiflf-Vfifimrizm an»... ...-at” ...-trrV-aut $32}. , .2233"; a. V fig?- Vflfi.-5t..fi.mw tr: ...... . ...- - - b.» .A. 4 V. .319“... LV. V. ¢~5fNfintfixu ht». $41.9 y’iifim- lfi‘tm. 1"! a 9-"...4‘ JET—.— .. Nth Anita I... 1 +5.... 25...... 7% . 4.3% ...-kw“; .... V a}. . ...-3)... tin/.14.?! i' ,0 (Nut!!! -LGQ to: V. . N é ‘- f . h Vf‘ V f . .5 y 1 5% N .. t Y1! . .9. . affluxfldhgnoiuw. 2}- ?aufihuhmbi . a . a: . . t , :5 8.: 535.57. titaniufi. . “ugh...fiufnugrflfl{bu G. .3 1.. l - A . .I.. , 9 - .. n. . 2ft. fiufiuimmwg . 1?. .... -..-.43. V ...-:7: , V L .2 AL". V .6...“ Tau; «t Vaah 25"... ... L11: . .5922... ...-1.90175"... Vina“. ... . ... . ...Ci-Dglfli‘bbi H ....u - L... ...-....k.r..-.1v93$f. unfinnupls 5?...“ 4-2:! LTI. ..i -. .V!. . V V vyy...l5 .tv. . 5:53.37. it I... 7.5-7 ...-033.“ Va L-IIJVINII L . ~ 1th . ..- ‘ LE E K b: 19%. )5" ‘ Michigan State V‘ University This is to certify that the thesis entitled Mflw WM presented by W CG Don/MW‘ has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for WW8, WW @W Major professor Date 7' $¢ %L 0-7 639 \‘*-‘n 5.. \ umww 518! \w \..- | LDEC‘ “\ 3gawm @W 41 5 2005 OVERDUE FINES: 25¢ per du per item RETURNING LIBRARY MATERIALS: Place in book return to remove charge from circulation records TOWARD A HUMAN ECOLOGICAL APPROACH OF ARCHITECTURE AND URBAN DESIGN BY Kamal El‘Din Beshir Awadallah A THESIS Submitted to l Michigan State University ‘ in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF URBAN PLANNING School of Urban Planning and Landscape Architecture 1982 ABSTRACT TOWARD A HUMAN ECOLOGICAL APPROACH OF ARCHITECTURE AND URBAN DESIGN BY Kamal El-Din Beshir Awadallah This thesis addresses the problem of the lack of cooperation between designers from the environmental design fields such as architecture and urban design, and researchers from the emerging behavioral science. This problem stems from a theoretical and professional gap between the two fields. By focusing on the theoretical gap which resulted in view of the relationship between human behavior and the physical environment, this thesis has suggested a reorientation of the designers determinis- tic attitude (i.e. the physical environment determines behavior) toward a human ecological approach. By redefining architecture and urban design as a means for designing adaptive spaces, a synthesis between the design fields and behavioral science has been proposed as a base for a conceptual framework that is aimed to close the theoretical gap. Although this thesis does not propose a closing of the existing professional gap between designers and researchers, it indicates the need for further attention to this problem. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS My sincere gratitude to all those special persons who contributed to the completion of this thesis. Specifically, I am grateful to Dr. Steven Orlick who served as my thesis chairperson, Dr. Ismet Kilincaslan and Professor Duane Mezga for their suggestions and constructive comments. Dr. Carl Goldschmidt and Professor Sanford Farness must also be thanked for their guidance and support offered me, whenever they were asked. My wife Josiane, and my son, Karim, deserve a special thanks for their love, patience, and understanding. Thanks to Jane Rice Blaine, the Urban Design librarian, for her cooperation whenever it was needed. Finally, thanks to Judy Gilroy who had to decipher my handwriting in order to type this manuscript. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Idea . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Problem . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Scope . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Methodology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Purpose . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Organization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Chapter I. HUMAN BEHAVIOR AND THE PHYSICAL ENVIRONMENT: THEORETICAL ORIENTATIONS . . . . . . . . . . Philosophical Orientations . . . . . . . . Empiricism and Environmental Determinism Rationalism and Nativism . . . . . . . . Interactionalism and Transactionalism . Theoretical Limitations . . . . . Aspects of Man-Built Environment Interactions . . . . . . . . . . . . Environmental Perception, Cognition, and Evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . The Ecological View . . . . . . . . . . . Three Ways to Define Culture . . . . . . . Summation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . II. DESIGNING THE BUILT-ENVIRONMENT: A THEORETICAL REORIENTATION . . . . . . . . The Meaning of the Rational Design Process The Architectural Design Bias . . . . . . The Theoretical Limitations . . . . . . The Professional Limitations . . . . Architecture as Cultural Adaptation . . . Redefining Architecture . . . . . . . . Summation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iii Page H mflflUTOJF-J 10 12 13 15 15 18 20 22 25 31 32 34 36 42 44 55 56 57 66 Page III. TOWARD A THEORY OF HUMAN ECOLOGICAL ARCHITECTURE AND URBAN DESIGN . . . . . . . . 67 General Theoretical Reorientation . . . . . 68 Specific Conceptual Reorientation . . . . . 77 Environmental Image . . . . . . . . . . . 77 Mental Mapping, Orientation and Meaning . 81 Spatial Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84 Privacy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84 Personal space . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85 Crowding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86 Territoriality . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Defensible space . . . . . . . . . . 89 Behavioral Units (Structured Behavior) . . 9O Behavioral setting . . . . . . . . . . . 9O Behavior circuits . . . . . . . . . . . 92 Conceptual Framework for Human Ecological Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 Theories and Models . . . . . . . . 94 A Conceptual Descriptive Model . . . . . . 100 A Design Process Model . . . . . . . . . . 107 Summation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111 VI. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION . . . . . . . . . . . . 115 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122 iv LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure Page 1. The Perception Continuum . . . . . . . . . . . 23 2. The Cognitive Process . . . . . . . . . . . . 24 3. Behavior-Environmental Inter—Relationships . . 27 4. An Ecologica1-Cu1tural—Behaviora1 Model . . . 29 5. Ascending Hierarchical Definition Structure for Nouns . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 6. Closed Hierarchical Definition Structure for Verbs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 7. Pruitt-Igoe Housing Scheme Blown Up in 1972 . 48 8. Confusion and Ugliness, Long Island . . . . . 51 9. Archology Hexahedron, A Scheme to House 170,000 Persons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 10. Environment and Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . 71 ll. Behavior/Perception Continuums . . . . . . . . 74 12. Three—way Classification System for Models . . 99 13. Ideal Process of Model-making in Relation to Reality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100 14. A Conceptual Model (1): The Contextual Scheme . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103 15. A Conceptual Model (2): The Social/Economic Organizational Scheme . . . . . . . . . . . 104 16. A Conceptual Model (3): The Behavioral/ Perception Scheme . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 17. A Conceptual Model (4): The Cultural/Design Scheme . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 Figure Page 18. A Design Process Model (1): Design/Research Cooperation Scheme . . . . . . . . . . . . . 112 19. A Design Process Model (2): Design With People . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113 vi INTRODUCTION Id_ea_ The idea of this thesis is derived from the emerging awareness of the study of the relationship between human behavior and the physical environment. This study has shown a rapid development in the last decades, particularly in the emerging field of behavior science. However, the implications of such a study lies in the hands of the fields of the environmental design, such as architecture, urban design, interior design, and industrial design. Therefore, there are two major fields for which, this study is impor- tant. On one hand, there is a group of researchers from social science fields such as psychology, anthropology, geography, and sociology, who, through the initiation of the multi-discipline behavior science, are concerned by human behavior and the physical environment. On the other hand there are the designers from the environmental design fields. Designers of the built-environment, although practicing in different fields, have one common thing that relate them to the study of behavior-physical environment. That is; they deal with changing a specific physical environment for meeting specific human activities. Therefore, it is quite important for the designer in general to know more about the relationship between the physical environment and behavior, if he wants to design the most appropriate environment. In this extent, the designer of the built-environment, whether he is an architect, urban designer, city planner, or interior designer, is equally responsible for considering the importance of the behavior—environment relations, particularly when a physical design is taking place. Although the study of behavior and the physical environment is important for both the behavior scientists and the environmental designers, they have contributed to it quite differently. While the behavioral scientists and researchers have been adjusting and shifting their theoretical attitudes and orientations, based on research findings, the environmental designers have been reluctant to consider such reorientations. In brief, the views of the relationship between human behavior and the physical environment can be summarized as follows: 1. Deterministic approach: That is the physical environment determines human behavior; in other words a specific behavior is expected to result in a specific builteenvironment. 2. Possibilistic approach: That is the physical environment provides possibilities and constraints, within which people make choices, the criteria for such choices are mainly cultural. 3. Probablistic approach: That is the physical environment does affect people's behavior not in terms of determining or generating it, rather in terms of choices, based on criteria attributed more to the physical setting. The designers attitude reflects, to a great extent, the deterministic approach, in that whenever a space or a structure is designed there is always the assumption of expecting, certain, desired, behavior to result. Moreover, whether this assumption happens or not, the designers do not bear responsibility for evaluation or follow up. This task is left to researchers, who on the other hand, have extended their theoretical views to the probabilistic approach which has led to the emerging of what is called the ecological approach. In other words, in viewing the behavior-environment relations, it is important to consider the context of this relationship, including culture. Problem The problem this thesis addresses is the lack of cooperation between designers from the environmental design fields--such as architecture, urban design, and city planning——and researchers from behavior science, particularly in regarding the relationship between behavior and the built-environment. This problem has two dimensions. First, there is a theoretical gap between designers and researchers in those fields. Second, there is a professional gap. The theoretical gap, as indicated in the previous discussion, stem from the designers' reluctance to change the deterministic attitude toward the built-environment behavior relations. On the other hand, the professional gap is a result of the difference in the intellectual styles and goals of the designers and researchers. While researchers are seeking the knowledge of the relationship between behavior and the built—environment, for example, designers are seeking the control of this environment. In other words, designers, in general, are action-oriented, and behavior scientists are research—oriented. This difference in itself does not mean that there is a gap, unless if there is no cooperation between the two professions. This lack of cooperation between the designers and the researchers, particularly at the profes- sional level, can also be attributed to the kind of the social, political, and economic environment within which the two types of professions operate. This is what one may call the "real world". Although the "real world" differs from one designer to another, the problem is always the same, in that there is the gap between the designer of a specific environment and the actual users. The designers' employer (who pays for the service) is in most cases, a developer, who is looking forward to making a certain profit, that is not necessary by meeting the needs of the actual users. It is a typical case for a designer not to meet or even know any of the users of his designed setting, and in extreme cases he might not see the physical setting at all. The researchers, on the other hand, are employed, in most cases by academic or public institutions whose goal is to increase the body of knowledge of the particular area of research conducted, regardless of whether or not the suggested implication of the findings are actually considered by designers of the built—environment. Another way to state the problem with its different, although related, dimensions is, in terms of the relation- ship between the physical-environment and human behavior, where there is a gap between the designers in the environ- mental design fields, and researchers in the behavior science fields. This gap has two dimensions; one is theoretical and the other is professional. This has led to a lack of cooperation between the two professions. Scope The scope of this thesis is extending throughout two major fields-—environmental design and behavioral science. While the environmental design includes a range of disciplines such as architecture, urban design, and city planning, the focus within this field is on the aspect of the physical design of the built—environment. In this extent, the different design disciplines are used through— out the thesis as to indicate to this design aspect. On the other hand, the behavioral science includes another range of disciplines from the social science, particularly those disciplines which deal with human behavior by one way or another. Such disciplines are, for example, geography, anthropology, psychology, and sociology. The particular focus on the behavior science, in this thesis, is on its contribution to the study of the relationship between behavior and the physical environment. The key focus, then, is on design and the issue or relationship between behavior and the built—environment. In other words, as was indicated above, the environmental design fields have a deterministic attitude in viewing this relationship. On the other hand the behavioral sciences' current approach is an ecological one. Therefore by reconsidering the design deterministic approach a conceptual framework can be developed between the two fields. The purpose of this conceptual framework is to close the theoretical gap between the designers and the behavioral scientists, particularly, in View of the behavior—built- environment relation. This gap is not the only reason for the problem of lack of cooperation between the designers and the behavioral scientists. The professional gap accounts, as well, for the same problem. However, the thesis is limiting itself to the closing of the theoretical gap. In the mean time some dimensions of the professional limitations and gap within and between the design and behavior science will be discussed. Methodology The methodology used in this thesis is an extensive review of the literature pertaining to the problem of the theoretical gap between behavioral scientists and environ— mental designers, particularly in view of the relationship between behavior and the physical environment. This review has even extended within each discipline to include any related issue and concept that may help develop the thesis. This has already enabled the research of this thesis to lay out a variety of theoretical and philosophical orientations which resulted in the development of other ideas and concepts used in the following chapters. Without doing this, a synthesis between various disciplines of the behavior science field and the environmental design field, would never have been feasible. Purpose The purpose of this thesis is to propose a possi- bility for an alternative, ecological, approach to the deterministic attitude of the environmental design fields toward the issue of behavior- built-environment relation. This approach will, possibly, bridge the theoretical gap between the design and the behavior science fields. In turn, this will contribute to the study of the behavior— environment in general by considering the implications of the current View of the behavior science fields which is an ecological, humanistic one. In addition, as the researchers will have an access to the initial design problems and questions the designers will be aware of the possible implication of the research findings in this area of study. This will help designers to design better physical-environment for people. Organization This thesis is devided into three chapters. The first chapter will be looking at the question of how does the built-environment relate to human behavior? The answer of this will be a review of the different theoretical attitudes and philosophical orientations that led to them. The chapter will then extend the review with focusing on the current View of this relationship. This theoretical review, although will use a range of different disciplines, will particularly focus on the emerging behavioral science field. The second chapter will, then, shift the focus to the design fields in an attempt to reconsider the deter- ministic attitude held by designers, by looking at the theoretical limitations. In doing this the chapter will discuss the possibilities of reconsidering the meaning of rationality in the design process. The chapter then will discuss the possibility for defining architecture and urban design in terms of cultural adaptation. The third chapter will attempt to develop a synthesis between design and behavioral science, in the light of architecture and urban design defined as cultural adaptation. The synthesis will be at a general theoretical as well as a specific conceptual level. The chapter will end by an attempt to develop two models. One is a concep- tual framework model aimed to close the theoretical gap between the environmental design fields and behavior science. The second model is a design process one that has two schemes. The first scheme will represent occasions for cooperation between designers and researchers in the design process. The second scheme will indicate the possibility for designing with people. CHAPTER I HUMAN BEHAVIOR AND THE PHYSICAL ENVIRONMENT: THEORETICAL ORIENTATIONS This chapter will commence the thesis by intro- ducing an overview of the different theoretical orientations concerning the relationship between human behavior and the physical environment. The chapter will begin with a discussion of the philosophical orientations which has led to the various theoretical attitudes in viewing this relationship. This will lead to the general theoretical limitations within the disciplines undertaking behavior environment study, which are integrated in the emerging behavioral sciences. The discussion will then focus on some specific concepts of man-built environment relation which are interpreted to be the bases of the behavior physical environment relation. This will be followed by introducing the ecological View as it represents an impor— tant emerging theme of viewing the whole notion of behavior—environment interactions. Finally, the chapter will end by indicating the importance of culture particularly when viewing behavior in an ecological sense. One of the fundamental questions, to start with, is; in which way does the physical environment relate to human behavior? This question is: 10 11 . . . an extremely difficult question to answer since the evidence is often difficult to compare, is con- tradictory and there is no consenses of generally accepted theoretical position. (Rapoport, 77:p2). According also to Rapoport, there is a range of theoretical attitudes towards the answer of the question, from; (1) Environmental Determinism; (2) Possibilism to (3) Probabilism. The environmental determinism attitude is, briefly, that the physical environment determines human behavior. This attitude is stronger in the field of planning and design, particularly in architecture, however, it also existed in cultural geography and anthropology. The possibilism approach is, "that the physical environment provides possibilities and constraints within which people make choices based on other, mainly cultural, criteria" (Ibid., 77:p2). This attitude is, much as in geography, a reaction to the determinism one. The current View, the probabilism, is stronger in the field of environmental design research. It is a view of that the physical environment does affect the people's behavior but not in terms of determining it rather in terms of choices based on criteria attributed to the physical setting. In essence this View rejects the idea of that the physical environment could generate or determine activities or behavior. 12 Although there is a large number of disciplines involved in these attitudes, the most crucial of all is the environmental design in general and architecture and planning in particular. They are the disciplines that actually determine the final product of the built- environment-—whether it is assumed to affect behavior or not. Therefore, before extending the View over these disciplines, it is important to lay out the state-of—the- art of the philosophical grounds of the above attitudes. By doing that, in the following section, the current evolved view will then address itself. Philosophical Orientations Confronting to the question of the ways people know or image their environment, I will try to go back to more general context of how knowledge in general is attained, in other words how an object is known by a subject. In this case, it is important to broaden the focus, for a while, to the general philosophical positions concerning the object-subject question. This overview of theoretical or rather philosophical positions can be discerned into "three fundamentally different ways in which the relationship between environment and behavior has been conceptualized" (Moor and Golledge, 76:p12). These three directions are; (1) empiricism and environmental; determinism (2) rationalism and nativism, and (3) interactionalism and constructivism 13 Each of these classes of theory is based on different assumptions about the "organism, what factors influence behavior, the nature of reality, and for those which include treatments of knowledge, the way in which knowledge is attained" (Ibid., 76:p12). According to Moore and Golledge (76), there are two extremes concerning behavior and environment between which all these theories are stretched. One extreme is that external environment entirely determines behavior; and the other is that behavior are entirely determined by internal biological and hereditary forces. Empiricism and Environmental Determinism The empiricism was first articulated by the eighteenth century British philosophers Locke, Berkeley, and Hume. While the environmental determinism is a twentieth century school, it is based on the positivism and neopositivism expressed in the work of Comte, Mill, Carnap, Wittgenstien, Reichenbach and others (see Bochenski, 1966) (Moore and Golledge, 76:p12). Both the empiricism and the environmental determinism agreed upon that behavior in general, and knowledge in particular, are Strictly under the control of the environment and that the ‘miiin.and only source of knowledge is sensation through Vfltich "one only can grasp singular and material events" (IIDid., 76:p12). They, on the other hand, disagree upon 14 the issue of the laws; the empiricism defined them as posteriori and the neopositivism as periori to experience (Ibid., 76:p12). In this theoretical context, extended to the twentieth-century environmental determinism, behavior is defined as consisting of patterns of responses and is assumed to be determined by the environment impinging on a passive organism and selecting and reinforcing particular responses. In other words, the environment acts on people and we, then, are the product of the environment. There are, in fact, sub-classes of theories that disagree on defining other consequential issues such as knowledge and representations. These are three major schools: strict stimulus-response (S-R); mediational S-R; and cognitive behaviorist theories (Moore and Golledge, 76:p12). In this View, the environment is treated as real, objective and normative existance totally independent of the observer. This View was also, categorized as "man reducible to environ- ment" (Overton and Reese, 77:p13), in which the basic metaphysical assumption of the primacy of the material world is the grounds. This is, thought and actions of man are held to be determined by and hence to derive from an independent, stable material reality. (Ibid., 77:p13). 15 Rationalism and Nativism The alternative Opposition to empiricism and determinism is the position of rationalist and idealist like Plato, Descartes, Spinozo, and Leibniz, which started by the nativist contention that; . . . knowledge is given immediately as an innate idea, before experience; it is a basic act of pure thought that opens all reality to us. Sensation in this view, gives us nothing but images of individual, particular things; that which is designed by them is given not by sensation but by innate ideas arising from our inherent powers of intellection. (Moore and Golledge, 76:pl3). This View can also be categorized as "environment reducible to man" (Overton and Reese, 77:p15), that what is commonly assumed that the environment is held to be itself merely a construction of man. As such, behavior is defined as the expression or projection of inherent (genetic and biological) factors in the contest of a specific environment. This second trend of theories is, therefore, resting on the other extreme, opposite to the first environmental deter- minism one. Interactionalism and Transactionalism .As expected from.theoretical streams, the third alternative to both Opposite extremes is one which tries t1) synthesize, and bridge the gap, between the two polarized Viiews on the subject—object question. This position is that of the interactionalism and treansactionalism. A landmark of this position was Kant's 16 fundamental distinction between the matter (or content) of knowledge and the form of knowledge. The first corresponds to sensation and the second causes the matter to be arranged in a certain order. The "matter" of knowledge is given through experience (as believed in the empiricism), while the form of knowledge, parallel to the rationalism, "is given a priori" (Moore and Golledge, 76:p13). The form of knowledge, as independent from the environment, is assumed by Kant to be universal and constant. For Kant, there were only two pure forms of intitution - space for the outer senses and time for the inner. All other contents of reality are experienced in the context of space and time. (Ibid., 76:pl4). Interestingly, while both empiricism and rationalism, although fundamentally opposite, agreed upon the premise that one can understand the ultimate nature of reality, Kant argued that, since there is no way for us to apprehend the nature of ‘reality' except through man, it is impossible to completely seperate the process of knowing from the result and knowledge . . . that there can be no complete understanding of truth in either sense or reason; thus, instead of knowledge ever representing exactly what is real, what we take to be real is a product of the act of knowing - that is, a 'construction of thought'. (Ibid., 76:p14). In this respect, the idealism, neo-Kantians represented One pole of assumptions based upon denial of knowledge tflnrough empirical methods and complete primacy of mind CKnntrary to both the empiricism and rationalism with their COqasequential theoretical echoes. 17 However, neo-Kantian theories of both phiIOSOphy and psychology moved to a rather interactive position, in xvhich the key word "grasp" was changed into "active con- sstruction" of objects regarding knowledge of reality in {general and environment in particular. In this View, the :subject (man) plays an active role with the object (environment). The result is that knowledge is the extent to which the subject can construct the object. This view, in this respect, is also parallel to that of the inter- actional and transactional positions on environment- behavior relations. Experience and behavior are assumed to be influenced by intraorganismic and extraorganismic factors operating in the context of ongoing transactions of the organism-in-environment. Behavior can then be attributed as a function of either biological or of environ- mental factors, and it is more than being the summation of them. A definition of behavior, therefore, is that it is an interaction of biological (sensation), personality. Socio-cultural and environmental factors each in the context of the other and the context of their mutual interactions. This View, in general, can also be called, "man and environ- ment as interdependent systems" (Overton and Reese, 77:p15) in which "two interdependent systems-~man and environment—- reciprocally interact and exert formative influences on each other" (Ibid., 77:p15). 18 Theoretical Limitations These three philosophical orientations of the object subject question, as briefly layed out above, to a certain extent, represent the walls of the context of behavior, cognition, and perception theory. The underlying point of the subject-object question in this context is the concen- trating on how one object is perceived, or known, by a subject. This point, until very recently, was the obstacle and limitation for an environmental perception study to be undertaken. A great example of this limitation is the Gestalt perceptual theory, which while having explicit interest in the context, was primarily developed through the study of form and object perception (Ittelson, 73zp2). Ittelson (73) attributed this theoretical limitation to that although some interest in the larger environment is expressed in some opposing theories, their extremely narrow definition of behavior and rigid concept of experimental procedures, . . . has driven the study of perception almost exclusively into the highly contrived laboratory situation, where object perception is virtually the only avenue open for study. Thus, in the history of experimental psychology and overwhelming bulk of perception research has been carried out in the context of object perception, rather than environment perception, with the findings of the former providing the basis for understanding the latter. (Ibid., 73:p3). This was a major phenomena, in the past 100 years, in the psychological theories and views of perception. 19 As a result the investigation of perception has lost the essential esthetic unity without which any pursuit leads to chaos, rather than resolution. (Ibid., 73: p3). Therefore, the philosophical question of the object-subject with all its multi-dimensions and assumptions can be seen as the dilemma of the psychological theories of perception. For centuries sensation, only, was considered as a base for perception in what Lang (74:p99) called sensation based theories. In contrast, what he called information-based perception theory as developed by psychologists such as James J. Gibson and Eleanor J. Gibson (Lang, 74) which focuses not only on how the data identified with the object of perception are delivered in an organized way but on the discovery of how we process phenomenal information and the relationships that come to exist within it. It is not a denial of sensory experience, . . . but patterns within the entire context provided by ambient light is said to be the basis of visual perception rather than the defining qualities of particular sensation. (Ibid., 74:p99). Whether sensory based theory, or information-based theory, architects have been reluctant to take any avenue of theories which can be useful as to change their assump— tions of one way causal relationship from environment to man or object-on—subject effects (Ibid., 74:p100). This, in turn, has not encouraged the researchers to investigate 20 and test their assumptions and hypothesis about this relationship the thing that accounts for another kind of theoretical limitations. In analytical research work it is of the utmost importance to know, at an early stage of the investi- gation, whether a subordinate element stands in a relation of mutual causal influence with all other parts of the system, or whether it is an unchangeable independent structure, influencing the whole by 'one—way' causation. (Lorenz, 68:p127). Aspects of Man-Built Environment Interactions The previous sections discussed the philosophical orientation of the theory of behavior-environment in the behavioral science fields. If one considers that these are the general contributions of these fields to the study of behavior environment, this section, by looking at some aspects of the current trend, can be specific contribu— tions. The focus in this section will be on some of these specific concepts of the man-environmental interaction. In the last section it was concluded that the current trend, despite the limitations in the behavioral science is the transactional, interactional approach which supports the importance of the context (larger environment) in its broad definition of behavior as the process of mutual interaction between man and the environment. Human aspects interacting in this process was synthesized to be attributed to biological (sensation) and personality 21 (socio-cultural) factors. It is also important to note that the fundamental assumptions, upon which this current trend is based, according to Rapoport's review, are; (1) Knowing, perceiving, and evaluating the environment is a dynamic process whereby information from the external environment is constantly being received, perceived (selected), and organized (based on evaluation) and used to help individuals operate. This process with its three facets, is what can be broadly called perception (Rapoport, 77:p30). In this sense, perception, with its different meaning, is inseparable component of behavior. (2) Subjective conceptions of the environment are "the ways in which people understand, structure and learn the environment . . . this might better be called ENVIRONMENTAL COGNITION (Ibid., 77:p3l). The variations of these subjective conceptions between different individ- uals are due to differences in past experiences about the environment and also due to differences in their socio— cultural environments, but not due to biological differences (Moore and Golledge, 76:pll). (3) The biological differences account, then, for the different perceptual conceptions among different individuals at a given time. These are merely sensory information perceived differently by different (biologically) individuals. This is, simply, to assert that we perceive the environment differently. 22 (4) The combination of both the perceptual and cognitive conceptions are accounting for the process by which people make choice and select. "A better term might be ENVIRONMENTAL EVALUATION OR PREFERENCE" (Rapoport, 77:p3l). (5) Finally, information extracted from large- scale external environments exist in some type of psycholog- ical space, not like the geometrical one, but can help to reproduce the same physical space (by training overtime), (Moore and Galledge, 76:p10). "This is the space in which configurations, characteristics, and meanings about the world are held as mental images or cognitive representa- tion" (Ibid., 76:p10). Environmental Perception, Cognition, and Evaluation The above assumptions underline Rapoport's (77:p37) position about the importance separating the different, confusing, meanings of perception. The three meanings, as presented above, while they form a continuum, as in Figure 1, "it seems possible to separate them so that a particular process belongs more or less in one of these categories" (Ibid., 77:p31). There are two, relating, although conceptually different, meanings of the term environmental cognition (Rapoport, 77:p108). One is based on psychological 23 __ . .wufikv 4h- . :.. liliesnar., é‘efifléfifbw‘éx . '» scoeN mos EvA wanes; <1 J> CONTINUUM Figure l.--The Perception Continuum SOURCE: Rapoport, 77:p3l premises which stress the knowledge of the environment, while the other is based on anthropological views which tend to stress that "cognitive processes are concerned with making the world meaningful and that there are different ways in which meaning can be given to the world" (Ibid., 77:p108). In other words, the underlying objective of the two views is investigating two fundamental sets of variables, the first is biological (for psychology) and the second is cultural (for anthropology). Cognition, from the latin word for 'getting to know' refers both to the process of knowing and understanding and the product - the thing known. (Ibid., 77:p109). The concept of cognition then, as a way of mediating information about the environment, is very useful for the behavior research. Figure 2 shows a diagram suggested by Rapoport (77:p109), to illustrate the cognitive process. 24 ‘ DECODED fl , OR READ I Understand 1M: _ Figure 2.——The Cognitive Process SOURCE: Rapoport, 77:p109 The process is assuming that the physical or the built- environment is the physical expression of the cultural cognitive categories "which, if the environment is meaning- ful, produce the appropriate and intended cognitive schemata" (Ibid., 77:p109). These cognitive representations or schemata are the ones known as mental maps which in general terms governs the process of imaging and since they involve both spatial and temporal schemes, they, thus guide one's orientation. The term perception, in most of the social science literature, is the most confusing one. However, as shown above, Rapoport (77) seems to be aware of this confusion and accordingly made the distinction between perception and cognition as follows; It is possible to distinguish between perceptual cognition, or knowledge of environment, and symbolic cognition or knowledge about the environment (Gibson, 25 1968). The former is perception, the direct sensory response to things and places, while in the latter, cognition, the information is preceded and may come from indirect sources. While all people see the world more or less the same way (Gibson 1968, p. 321) they structure it and evaluate it quite differently. (Rapoport, 77:pl78). As such, perception is experiencing the environment through, mainly, sensory, direct, interactions, while cognition is knowing and comprehending the environment by relaying, in most cases, on pre—coded information and past experience. Both concepts are involved in everyone's daily interactions with the environment. While people want to understand (comprehend) the built-environment cognitively, they also wish to experience it. By doing this, although seeming contradictive but are complementary to one another, criteria are used in the process of evaluating the environment. One is clarity (based on cognition) and the other is complexity (based on percep— tion). The nature of perception, therefore, is a complex multi-sensory one. It also depends, to a certain extent, upon memory, cognitive representation, and culture. The Ecological View The previous sections of this chapter have already indicated some theoretical concepts of the nature of man- environment interactions. This section will deal with another dimension, that is how can the relationship between behavior and the physical environment be viewed in an 26 ecological context. In this respect, Roger Barker (69) has developed his famous concept "behavior settings". An important point to note, as Barker indicated in his work, is that both the physical and the social environment impinge on behavior. Barker summarized his attributes of the relationship between behavior and environment by defining the concept of behavior setting as follows: 1. A behavior setting consists of one or more standing patterns of behavior, which uniquely define the behavior pattern in terms of both time and space. Both man—made parts of a town and natural features can comprise the milieu of, and thus affect, the behavior setting. The milieu encompasses, environs, and encloses behavior and, hence, can be used to describe that behavior setting. The milieu (environment) is similar in structure to the behavior, hence they affect each other in an essential way. The environment-behavior parts are so relating to each other, if called synomorphs, structurally a behavior setting is a set of such synomorphs. While behavior and environment are related to, and affecting each other they maintain a specific degree of independence. The underlying message Barker tried to convey was 'that while behavior is an inescapable component interrelated unith the environment (the milieu), it has not been the £311hdect of the physically oriented environmental designers. E’eezrhaps one, admitted, difficulty in considering behavior, iLss that the environment affects behavior in an important.way. EBIJnt: this environment is experienced by each individual di fferently. Thus, predicting the environment-behavior L 27 relation is quite a complex task--that will involve basic theoretical consensus as well as methodological research cooperation among a range of different disciplines. Bonni Morrison (74) pointed out at the importance of looking at the behavior—environment relation in terms of three main environments; (1) Natural Environment, (2) Man-Made Environment, (3) Behavioral Environment. Figure 13 summarizes the inter-relationships between these three environments. ENVNMMENTAL lNT‘E-KFACE agriculi'uv’ai—v induSIH‘ . Planning 0 5(6)): . designing . fedmoioaq 0 Production NATURAL . MAN- BUILT ENVIRONMENT ENVIRONMENT .. ...-fag, ’. ( M B E J . P’fi‘S‘G‘ . $000- m$\d 0 50(30- bu‘oloai .l .oigzo'. ..... ..... . . . ,. . .. .. MAN/ MAN/ BEHAVIORAL Euvm— BUM ENVIRONMENT NATURrL Emm . r MENT ( fDE) W..— ' P35¢h°‘°3:ca‘ m EACE ’ 5° ‘5" - 'mFomaf'ion - L) sf 0 ‘Mrvi’dl’W‘ 66613! on . ‘P'O‘kch‘m \_/ o CansunPIion ' ”Maw-vent -1?Li.gure 3.—-Behavior—Environmental Inter—Relationships SOURCE: Morison, 74:p176 28 This Inodel, although a very general framework that does not propose any specific issue, is useful to look at as a "conr2eptual tool". Its primary premises are: (l) the need 'to use a systematic approach that is a complete human ecoltngical perceptive in order to provide a broad and holirstic view of a total system; (2) this View is important to cxonsider in times of great specialization and fragmenta- 'tior1 of knowledge; and (3) no part of the man—behavior- envdrronment system, as a complex framework, is an independent entity. Edwin Willems wrote, supporting flfis ecological aEHProach of dealing with behavior as behavior—environment urlits , that , . . . without this percepective, the bits and pieces which we study so frequently in experiments and with which we tinker so indiscriminately in psychotherapy, behavior modification, and behavior pharmacology are all abstractions that have lost much of their scientific and practical usefulness because they are seperated from the contextual interdependencies of everyday life. (Willems, 77:p25). This ecological approach emerged in both the field 0f psychology and of anthropology. The "ecological Psychology has emphasized the need to study behavior in more molar and natunflistic context" (Berry, 80:p83). While, "a similar movement within anthropology was develop— ing the point of view that the forms which a culture evolves can and must be understood as adaptation to its habitat" (Ibid., 80:p83). This movement is called cultural ecology. 29 The ecological psychology, as Barker's work discussed above, was pushed further to a cross-cultural psychology, so to increase "the range of independent variables". In fact this shift toward accounting cultural variable in an ecoloqical context indicates the failure of the traditional psychology that looks at behavior from a narrow perspective. As a response to this shift, Berry (80) has introduced a model of "ecocultural psychology" in an attempt to incor- porate ecological, cultural, acculturational, and behavioral Variables into a single model, Figure 4. EE°°.'°9y t Traditional Traditional. Organisms Culture Behavior Accultural \ / Influence Contact Accultural l Culture Behavior Figure 4.--An Ecological-Cultura1-Behavioral Model SOURCE: Berry, 80:p86 30 The underlying idea of this model is to bring the cultural.and ecological elements together, as they both are related to behavioral elements. These three basic elements were ciefined by Berry (80), as follows: (1) Ecological: interactions between human organisms and their habitat (2) Cultural elements: group-shared patterns of behavior which are adapted to the group's habitat (3) Behavioral elements In gnarallel, there is another set of elements, . O . which is introduced through major contact with 1:echnologically dominant societies, includes the aacculturative influences themselves (operating mainly ‘through urbanization and education), the contact culture (a culture no longer simply in adaptation to its habitat, but now also under these acculturative influences), and acculturated behavior (consisting of 'shifts' in behavior from previous levels, and ‘acculturative stress' behaviors which are novel and mildly pathological). (Ibid., 80:p85). 1318 relationships between all these elements are probabil- ‘isitic (rather than deterministic) and correlational (rather 1111an causal). This model also, according to Berry, espouses a tiefinition of the concept of culture as an adaptation t 4—4 [.— 0 £2 < m 03 l- “J z " 0 Z o .1 SE w 8 , _J (.0 individual “"pindividual g ‘ o Figure 15.--A Conceptual Model (2): The Social/ Economic Organizational Scheme. SOURCE: The Author 105 4- UJ 9: i—D: 9 > LU < é’r LU (rm 3 a. 4a! m égw 2 -.J a) E22” ocrE < ID< 4"” 0 Z 2 $3 5 F3 o. UJ O E 8 ‘a Lu“ 0 0 tr ZJ Figure l6.--A Conceptual Model (3): The Behavioral/ Perception Scheme. SOURCE: The Author 106 4- 8 mi E m w a < L: D O 0 Z 2 < LU El 4- 4"“ $555? w iii: m ,:—-= LIJ O O (I 0- 2 Q 5 {3 O 8 O 0: SJ Figure l7.—-A Conceptual Model (4): The Cultural/ Design Scheme. SOURCE: The Author 107 and design. 12. Therefore, this model is aimed primarily toward two purposes. One is to set a pool of concepts from which hypotheses and continual research can extend. Second, is to serve as a frame of reference for architects and planners who want to develop design criteria in the light of human ecological perspective. 13. If these two purposes are achieved a closing of the gap between social scientists, and architects and planners, will result as they both espouse a common theoretical ground. This model then serves as a descriptive tool of the conceptual synthesis which is geared toward the suggested reorientation toward a human ecological theory of architec- ture. By using the same descriptive mechanism a design process model can be, also, developed. A Design Process Model The above discussion has already suggested a closing of the gap between researcher (behavioral scientist) and the designer (architect or planner). This can happen by integrating the information and finding of the research into the design process itself. By doing this, the designer can scientifically develOp hypothesis and witnesses then tested throughout the process. This suggestion is based on assuming that the design process is already a continual 108 one (open-ended). The researcher on the other hand, can use the design process to "empirically" examine his hypothesis and theories. This can happen if the actual design problems and questions are made explicit by the designers. Unless designers have interest in assuming and considering the importance of the relationship between behavior and the built-environment, they will be reluctant to provide researchers with useful research questions and problems. For this reason, this thesis has focused on the possibility for designers to reorient their view of the built-environment behavior relations, as a basic step toward possible cooperation with researchers from behavior science. This is the first fundamental step toward the development of a design process that can be the operational framework of the human ecological perspective which is shown in Figure 18 as suggested by Zeisel (81). This first scheme of the design process, in the form of cooperation between researchers and designers, is one which allows for the development of empirical knowledge about man in his built-environment. The means to achieve this scheme require that both designers and behavior scientist understand each other's problems and professional nature and characteristics. On one hand the design process traditionally has three elementary activities, these are: imaging, presenting, and testing (Zeisel, 81:p6). 109 Imaging is similar to the concept of image that everyone experiences, but in this case the designer image a solution to his problem in terms of mental picture of both the context and the form. At this point the researcher should be able to supply the designer with the type of information about human behavior that can be useful for the designer's images. This design activity (imaging) is the key one, that if changed and shifted to be "human activities" and "behavior" oriented instead of "geometric forms" and "space" oriented, the processes of representation and testing will accordingly follow these criteria. This can be done by developing behavioral units and criteria in an image form. By doing this the designer will be able to image the activities within the space, instead of using stereotyped images, like land use and building categories and names. They could then create a new system of images that view the design elements in terms of what actually "it is people do in the environment" (Perin, 70:p97). This can be the responsibility of the researchers to be able to provide their findings and information in such a way that designers can use for their imaging process. The major problem that actually can maintain the gap between researchers and designers is their clients and the problems of who pays for the service? and why? This problem, although, varies from one situation to another, it stems from the political, economic, and social 110 environment within which both researchers and designers operate. However, the key solution to this problem is in the hand of both professions, in terms of how much they commit themselves to this approach. Another professional dilemma still faces both the designers and researchers; that is the gap between them and the real users of the built-environment. A "citizen participation" concept, then, addresses itself to close this gap. However, this is not quite enough, because, every design decision has political implications. Middle class home-owners are in favor of public housing - somewhere else. They believe there should be a place where teen-agers can obtain help for drug problems as long as it is located - somewhere else. A new factory in town will be good for business but it is going to take over the last unused bit of river bank. A new city hall will help give the town a better image but it may not be worth those extra taxes. (Sommer, 72:pl33). Therefore, what Sommer (72) has called "design awareness," should extend beyond the designers and researchers to the users. This can be done by informing people about the latest research findings and about the different steps of the design process as it develops different options and alternatives. This basic step is what Sommer (72) called "environmental workshops." The professional limitations facing architects and designers can be overcome if this kind of awareness among the users is promoted. By this "awareness," the process of getting peOple involved in the design process (citizen participation) can be meaningful, 111 and useful. This step can also be represented in a model shown in Figure 19. The realization of the above two schemes of the design process (shown in both Figure 18 and Figure 19), hence, can be "rational." On one hand, it can attain the analytical, critical sense of rationality by integrating a research process into its cycle, and on the other hand by facing the users and their real needs by having them aware and involved in the design process. Summation This final chapter of this thesis has developed a possibility for steps toward a theory of human ecological architecture. The following points describe these possibilities: (1) In a general conceptual level, the concept of architecture as adaptation with its developed two "purposive" variables--freedom and efficiency of the human activity relate to behavior, especially viewed by the current trends of the behavioral science. (2) Also a synthesis between the concept of architecture and design as adaptation and behavior at a specific conceptual level was possible to make. This involved two main categories of behavior; structured behavior (units) such as behavioral setting and behavioral circuit; and unstructured behavior (spatial) such as 112 emanam .Hmmamm "momaom .wEmnom coflummmmooo noummmmm\cmflmoo AHV Hmpoz mmmooum cmwmmo ¢||.wa muzmflm :0 an 3% 75358 \: ‘51... \m\ \\ c‘.‘~ .\ \ fiwwfli Pfifiz 34.5 mom ... .. .3 gummy wzfcduodw __ ii 3.53%» .\ . m, .. . . . . . \\ ~\. \\\.\ \\ . \ ...... ...‘C ,. .i z I .\..i t .. , .V . ~ . . .. .I o.... u . 5 | . \\.. .\\ ... \xx..q.. V i ‘ O .41... - i . m. . -. \ . . \o\ .... 2 am as». «EA. 7. AU . . , i . . \ . \\ , \ . . Qmwfiu ‘\\ a...\\ .U u i“ i \.L\\\ \ .\.\ \,.\.\.\.\\\\\ x \\ fl. . b I . \ .. V \ \ \ . . v. i i .\ w. x,.....«.n....r \. \ , , o..w .Efiw , . . .i . a; \I \.\ “a. \ ‘ 00’ unmet/.4 .i ) . N ‘ \\..\ . \o‘. n\ \ \ \ .....lw I s so“ \\ \. .\ I/\. \.\ “u v \\ \ u , ..m.........M....,.,_....- v v. ...on... ....“mm... k\I I 5‘4, r / . / I I / I / ......) / ll .40.”. .'\‘\ . .0 Q / / 0... . 4 I b 33.0... . \ o .‘l.. / .... ......»a ....a......u . I ‘ ll.\r . {I .05 ‘I t 1". Q \ I c d... ..........~C. . ..i 0...... n..... \ I I n A. Iona... : . fi......... *5.“ 29341;“ 113 PROBLEM é US$35 é. CONTEXT ‘ ’1 W i A DATK fflwmmnflu' (JKHEMHWS’E;;YWRNSVP ANN¥55i W. 4% . AUU$W*WW5 3 ffiTUN. WT i P ARTIClmT' 14me suntan»: gas-finch! ‘ w M ML DiSGN % PRESENTATION mm? Figure 19.--A Design Process Model (2): Design With People. SOURCE: Author 114 personal space, privacy, crowding, territoriality, and defensible space. (3) This synthesis has revealed a diversity between efficiency and freedom (of human activity) if viewed at different contextual levels (individual, group, society). (4) A conceptual descriptive model was then intro- duced in an attempt to show this diversity between efficiency and freedom as purposive variables and a range of different concepts and ideas. Basically, contextual synthesis was represented by two schemes of the model and behavior/culture/design synthesis was represented by two other schemes. This conceptual model, by indicating how different concepts relate to each other, can be a bases for closing the theoretical gap between the designers in the environmental design fields and researchers in the behavior science fields. (5) In the light of this approach, a proposed model of the design process was introduced. This model, indicates two consequences or implications of the human ecological design approach, represented in two schemes. First scheme is a possibility for occasions of cooperation between designers and researchers in the design process, as proposed by Zeisel (81). Second scheme is indicating the importance of the users participation in the design process. CHAPTER IV SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The impetus for this thesis was a concern of our urban environment, particularly the built-environment and how it relates to people. This concern is emerging in a variety of academic disciplines in corresponding to the fast technological and social change shaping the contem— porary world. This thesis, therefore, has lent itself toward searching for steps to take in order to bring a desired and crucial awareness to the milieu of design, planning and architecture. This awareness was thought of to be a revision of the theoretical premises of the deter- ministic approach, often, used by urban designers and architects. This has, thus, called for the research to go beyond the boundaries of these fields into the realm of the emerging behavior science that, already, includes psychology, anthropology, sociology and philosophy. The discussions that were presented focused on the theoretical gap between the environmental design fields and the behavior science fields in View of the relationship between human behavior and the built—environment. This focus has two main topics. 115 116 The first topic was the human behavior built- environmental relations, in which an extensive review of the current related theoretical attitudes and concepts toward it is laid out. This allowed for providing a reason to reconsider the architectural determinism. The second topic was steps toward a formulation of a new theoretical attitude in architecture and design that is parallel to the current view of behavioral science; that is human ecological. These steps were not in themselves methodologies or practical tools, rather they were conceptual synthesis on different levels of analysis that can lead to further methodological development in the design process. There- fore, the work that has been completed up to this point should be considered as a conceptual framework model of how the planner, the designer and the architect may View man in his built-environment. Moreover, this conceptual frame- work is aimed to bridge the theoretical gap between designers and researchers (as indicated). Finally, it should be noted that bridging this theoretical gap between the two fields (design and behavior science) is only one side of the problem of the lack of cooperation between them. The other side of this problem is a professional gap which can be bridged, maybe by a different approach that will deal with the social, political and economic factors of this problem. 117 The first chapter of the thesis has lent itself to the question of how the environment in general and the built-environment in particular relates to people? By doing so, it has raised and concluded the following points. (1) There are three major philosophical/theoretical attitudes toward man—environment interactions. One is that the environment controls and determines man's behavior; the second is that behavior determines and constructs the environment; and the third is that both environment and human behavior are mutually interrelated. (2) The third attitude is representing the current trend in the emerging behavior science. However, the limitation facing this trend is the narrow scope of analysis that does not go beyond the individual level. (3) Therefore, the ecological approach that calls for multi-level analysis is gaining a larger creditability in that it tries to remove the persisting theoretical limitations. This can be achieved by the use of the concept of behavioral settings developed by Barker (69). (4) Culture is also a major factor to consider within this View, the thing that calls for the need for cross-cultural analysis at all the different levels. (5) Culture has three dimensions to look at. These are as manifestation, as systems, or as adaptation. The important point is that these dimensions complement each others and without considering their importance 118 a cultural-ecological approach will have a short coming. Finally, this section has developed an overview of the possible relations between man and his built- environment. The underlying message this overview has developed was the need for a corresponding response and reorientation of the architectural and design theory. In response to this message developed above, the second chapter has concentrated on reconsidering the architectural and urban design theory. In doing this the following conclusions were arrived at. (1) In order for a "rational design process" to cope with the scientific rationality, and as it delivers means of communication (non-verbal communication) it should be based on considering people's behavior and interactions as its basic criteria. Its goal should be the fitness of man and his needs into the context of the design form, and its means are scientific or critical (analytical) methods of understanding both the human behavior and its context. (2) The deterministic approach of the design and architectural theory has resulted theoretical and profes— sional limitations. (3) The theoretical limitations stem from the reluctance of architects to question what they are doing. This has resulted no advances in their theory. Also, by 119 not getting involved in research activities they isolated themselves from the social and behavioral scientists. (4) The professional limitations are best described by the gap between the designer and the user. (5) Therefore, in order for the architectural limitations to be removed, and the design process to be "rational," a reorientation in its theory is inevitable. This reorientation can start by redefining architecture in terms of cultural adaptation. (6) By doing this, a bridge between the architect, designer, and planner and the social scientist could be built which also will result advances in the theoretical and technical knowledge of the two fields. It will, also, result a better built-environment. (7) For this reorientation in the theory of architecture, chapter four has suggested a synthesis between architecture defined as: art-science of design-constructing adaptive spaces and structures that maximize the efficiency and freedom of human activities in a human ecological context! that is defined by the particular environment, in terms of social, technological, and political and economical organization of that environment and its linkage to other local, regional, state, national or international levels of interactions, and the different theoretical premises of the ecological approach developed in the field 120 of behavioral science. This can be done by using the two "purposive variables" freedom and efficiency of human activities. The third chapter of the thesis has, thus, attempted to draw this synthesis at two different levels. The first is at the general theoretical level and the second is the specific conceptual one. The following are the findings of this chapter: (1) There is a possibility of a conceptual synthesis between architecture as cultural adaptive tool using the two "purposive" variables freedom and efficiency, and behavioral science at two levels; general and specific. (2) A conceptual descriptive model was possible to develop in order to lay out this synthesis in four schemes. The first and second schemes indicated a contex- tual synthesis while the third and fourth schemes demon- strated behavior/culture/design synthesis. (3) According to these possibilities of a synthesis and also according to the recommendations derived from the meaning of rationality, a design process model was developed to illustrate the possibility to apply these concepts in a practical way. This was not a methodology of design, rather it was descriptive mechanism of the cooperation between designers and the social scientists at one scheme and people at another. 121 The chapter can then be a proposed solution to the problem of the increasing unbalance between man and his built-environment that is resulted from the lack of architectural theory that consider human behavior and needs as its basic criteria. This solution can also contribute to our body of knowledge (technical/theoretical) about man-built-environment relations. Finally, it should be again noted that this thesis is a step toward more awareness of not only our environment but also our knowledge about the environment that should be enhanced and increased for this awareness to exist. This awareness can start with not only the architecture design or planning students it should be at a larger scale to include everyone in a way that C. Allexander (77) called "A Pattern Language." REFERENCES REFERENCES Abel, Chris. 1980. Meaning and Rationality in Design. In Meaning and Behavior in the Built-Environment. eds. Geoffrey Broadbent. Richard Bunt. and Tomas Llorens. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Ackoff, Rusell L. 1974. Redesigning the Future. New York: John Wiley 8 Sons. Alexander, Christopher, et. al. 1977. A Pattern Language. New York: Oxford University Press. Alexander, Christopher. 1964. "Notes on the Synthesis of Form." Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. Alexander, Theron. 1973. Human Development in an Urban Age. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc. AllsOpp, Bruce. 1974. Towards a Human Architecture. London: Fredrick Muller Limited. Arnheim, Rudolf. 1971. Gestalt Psychology and Artistic Form. In Aspects of Form. ed. Lancelot Law Whyte. New York: American Elsevier Publishing Company, Inc. Banham, Reyner. 1976. Megastructure, Urban Futures of the Recent Past. London: Thames and Hudson. Banham, Reyner. 1960. Theory and Design in the First Machine Agg. New York: Praeger Publishers. Baron, Reuben M. and Rodin, Judith. 1978. Personal Control as a Mediator of Crowding. In Advances in Environmental Psychology! Volume 1 The Urban Environment. eds. Andrew Baum. Jerome Singer. and Stuart Valins. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Barker, Roger G. 1968. Ecological Psychology. Stanford, Cal.: Stanford University Press. 122 123 Berry, John W. 1980. Cultural Ecology and Individual Behavior. In Human Behavior and Environment. eds. Irwin Altman. Amos Rapoport. and Joachim F. Wohlwill. (Advances in Theory and Research, Vol. 4, Environment and Culture.) New York: Plenum Press. Blake, Peter. 1969. Forward. In Arcology: The City in the Image of Man. by Soleri, Paolo. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press. Boulding, Kenneth. 1956. The Image. Ann Arbor: The Univer- sity of Michigan Press. Chemayeff, Serge. and Alexander, Christopher. 1963. Community and Privacy. New York: Anchor Books Doubleday & Company, Inc. Deasy, C. M. 1974. Design for Human Affairs. New York: A Schenkman Publication, Halsted Press Division. Doxiadis, C. A. et. a1. 1974. Anthropolisngity for Human Development. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc. Echenique, Marcial. 1972. Models: A Discussion. In Urban Space and Structures. eds. Leslie Martin. and Lionel March. London: Cambridge University Press. Ficker, Victor B. and Graves, Herbert S. 1978. Social Science and Urban Crises. New York: Macmillan Publishing Co., Inc. French, Jere Stuart. 1978. Urban Space. Iowa: Kendall-Hunt Publishing Company. Gallion, Arthur B. and Eisner, Simon. 1980. The Urban Pattern. New York: D. Van Nostrand Company. Goodman, Robert. 1971. After the Planners. New York: Simon and Schuster. Hall, Edward T. 1966. The Hidden Dimension. Garden City, New York: Doubleday & Company, Inc. Heimsath, Clovis. 1977. Behavioral Architecture. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Holahan, Charles J. 1978. Environment and Behavior. (The Plenum Social Ecology Series. Series editor Rudolf Moos.) New York: Plenum Press. 124 Ittelson, William H. ed. 1973. Environment Perception and Contemporary Perceptual Theory. In Environment and Cognition. New York: Seminar Press. Jarvie, I. C. 1968. Utopian Thinking and the Architect. In Planning for Diversity and Chang_. ed. Stanford Anderson. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press. Jenks, Charles A. 1977. The Lapguage of Post-Modern Architecture. New York: Rizzoli International Publications, Inc. Kaplan, Stephen and Kaplan, Rachel. 1978. Coping Strategies: Choice and Control. In Human-scape, Environments for Pegple. eds. Stephen Kaplan and Rachel Kaplan. North Scituate, Mass.: Duxbury Press. Kaplan, Stephen. 1976. Adaptation Structure, and Knowledge. In Environmental Knowing Theories, Research, and Methods. eds. Gary T. Moore and Reginald G. Golledge. (In Community Development Series, Vol. 23, ed. Richard P. Dober.) Stroudsburg, Penn.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, Inc. Lang, Jon et. al. ed. 1974. Emerging Issues in Architec- ture. In Designing for Human Behavior: Architecture and the Behavioral Sciences. eds. Jon Lang. Charles Burnette. Walter Moleski. and David Vachon. (Community Development Series, Vol. 6, series ed. Richard Dober.) Stroudsburg, Penn.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, Inc. LeCorbusier. 1954. The Modular. Cambridge: Harvard Univer- sity Press. LeCorbusier. 1927. Towards a New Architecture. London: Architectural Press. Lorenz, Konrad Z. 1968. The Role of Gestalt Perception in Animal and Human. In Aspects of Form. ed. Lancelot Law Whyte. New York: American Elsevier Publishing Company, Inc. Lynch, Kevin. 1960. The Image of The City. Cambridge, Mass.: The Technology Press & Harvard University Press. Moholy-Nagy, Sibyl. 1968. Matrix of Man. New York: Frederick A. Praeger, Publishers. 125 Moore, G. T. 1976. Theory and Research on the Development of Environmental Knowing. In Environmental Knowing. eds. Gary T. Moore. and Reginald G. Golledge. (In Community Development Series, Vol. 23, ed. Richard P. Dober.) Stroudsburg, Penn.: Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross, Inc. Moore, G. T. and Golledge, R. G. 1976. Environmental Knowing: Concepts and Theories. In Environmental Knowing. eds. Gary T. Moore. and Reginald G. Golledge. (In Community Development Series, Vol. 23, ed. Richard P. Dober.) Stroudsburg, Penn.: Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross, Inc. Morris, A. E. J. 1972. History of Urban Form. London: George Godwin Ltd. Morrison, Bonnie Maas. 1974. The Importance of a Balanced Perspective: The Environment of Man. Man-Environment Systems. Vol. 4, No. 3, pp. 171-178. Mumford, Lewis. 1961. The City in History. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World, Inc. Newman, Oscar. 1972. Defensible Space. New York: The Macmillan Company. Overton, Willis F. and Reese, Hayne W. 1977. General Models for Man-Environment Relations. In Ecological Factors in Human Development. ed. Harry McGurk. New York: North-Holland Publishing Company. Palen, J. John. 1975. The Urban World. New York: McGraw- Hill Book Company. Parr, A. E. 1970. In Search of Theory. In Environmental Psychology. eds. Harold M. Proshansky. William H. Ittelson. and Leanne G. Rivlin. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc. Perin, Constance. 1970. With Man In Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press. Polanyi, Michael. 1959. The Study of Man- Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Pond, Irving K. 1918. The Meaning of Architecture. Boston: Marshall Jones Company. 126 Porteous, Douglas J. 1977. Environment and Behavior: Planning and Everyday Urban Life. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company. Proshansky, Harold M. Ittelson, William H. and Rivlin, Leanne G. 1970. The Influence of the Physical Environment on Behavior: Some Basic Assumptions. In Environmental Psychology: Man and His Physical Setting. eds. Harold M. Proshansky. et. al. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc. Prokop, Dieter. 1967. Image and Function of the City. In Urban Core and Inner City. ed. Michel H. M. von Huten. Leiden, Netherlands: E. J. Brill. Quantrill, Malcolm. 1974. Ritual and Resppnse in Architecture. London: Lund Humphries. Rapoport, Amos. 1980. Cross-Cultural Aspects of Environ- mental Design. In Human Behavior and Environment. eds. Irwin Altman. Amos Rapoport. and Joachim F. Wohlwill. (In Advances in Theory and Research, Vol. 4, Environment and Culture.) New York: Plenum Press. Rapoport, Amos. 1977. Human Aspects of Urban Form. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Rapoport, Amos. 1976. Environmental Cognition in Cross— Cultural Perspective. In Environmental Knowingy Theories, Research, and Methods. eds. Gary T. Moore. and Reginald G. Golledge. (In Community Development Series, Vol. , ed. Richard P. Dober.) Stroudsburg, Penn.: Dowden, Hutchinson and Ross, Inc. Schwartz, Barry. 1972. The Social Psychology of Privacy. In People and Building_. ed. Robert Gutman. New York: Basic Books, Inc., Publishers. Sjoberg, Gideon. 1980. The Preindustrial City: In Urban Life. eds. George Gmelch. and Walter P. Zenner. New York: St. Martin's Press. Smith, Peter F. 1979. Architecture and the Human Dimension. London: George Godwin Limited. Soleri, Paolo. 1969. Arcology, the City in the Image of Man. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press. 127 Sommer, Robert. 1969. Personal Space: The Behavioral Basis of Desigp. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Sommer, Robert. 1972. Design Awareness. San Francisco: Rinehart Press. Sorokin, Pitirim A. 1962. Society, Culture and Personality. New York: Cooper Square Publishers, Inc. Spreiregen, Paul D. 1965. Urban Design: The Architecture of Towns and Cities. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company. Stea, David. 1974. Architecture in the Head: Cognitive Mapping. In Designing for Human Behavior: Architecture and the Behavioral Sciences. eds. Jon Lang. Charles Burnette. Walter Moleski. and David Vachon. (Community Development Series, Vol. 6, series ed. Richard Dober.) Stroudsburg, Penn.: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, Inc. Stea, David. 1965. Space, Territoriality, and Human Movement. Landscape. 15:13-16. Steiss, Alan Walter. 1974. Urban System Dynamics. Lexington, Mass.: Lexington Books, D.C. Heath and Company. Thorndyke, Perry W. 1980. Performance Models for Spatial and Locational Cognition. Santa Monica, Cal.: Rand Corporation. Willems, Edwin P. 1977. Relations of Models to Methods in Behavioral Ecology. In Ecological Factors in Human Development. ed. Harry McGurk. New York: North-Holland Publishing Company. Zeisel, John. 1981. Inguiry by Design. Tools for Environmental-Behavior Research. Monterey, Cal.: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. MICHIGAN STQTE UNIV, LIB illMINIHillliHIHWIHHI!\lilHilHHlWHlil 312931073982 R Q RIES 44