TIME ORIENTATION AND ACTIVITY PATTERN 0F SELECTED HOMEMAKERS Thesis for the Degree of Ph. D. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DOHA ABDEL-RAHMAN ELGIDAILY 1971 I ”if: vn- ..' -‘- 8.. ngman‘u‘4ugmmh 5;“ z' ' "“vfliu 1“ (“- ”"sz'ti ". Michigan State at, University This is to certify that the I I thesis entitled ‘ TIME ORIENTATION AND ACTIVITY PATTERN OF SELECTED HOMEMAKERS presented by Doha Abdel—Rahman Elgidaily has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein Family Ecology WW W Major professor fl Date June 9, 1971 04639 ABSTRACT TIME ORIENTATION AND ACTIVITY PATTERN OF SELECTED HOMEMAKERS By Doha Abdel—Rahman Elgidaily’ This study attempted to; first, identify the pattern of activity and/or resource use of a selected group of home— makers--the extent to which the activity pattern involved conservation of resources——; second, to identify the tempor— al value—orientation of the homemaker, past, present or future time-oriented; third, to explore relationships between the time—orientation and the activity pattern of the home— maker. An accepted idea in home management literature is that values direct activities. The degree to which the home- maker's behavior in the home relates to her temporal values and/or time—orientation has been a comparatively untapped home management research area. The sample consisted of thirty rural non-farm families who reported income below the poverty level five years earlier. The homemaker in‘each family was observed for approximately four hours while performing her normal daily activities. Verbal conversation was mechanically recorded. Activity data were coded according to the nature of the activ— ity, area in which the activity occurred, and materials and Doha Abdel—Rahman Elgidaily equipment used in the activity. Distinction was made between activities which have a resource conserving function and those with the ultimate.purpOSe of consumption. Two addi- tional categories were identified as control activities, Which were purposeful but did not serve the immediate function either of resource conservation or consumption, and prepara— tion-construction activities, which have the function of augmentation or creation of resources. Conversations were transcribed and then divided into message units. Each message unit was coded on two dimensions, the time reference which appeared to be intended by the home— maker——past, present or future—-and the content of the message unit, whether the homemaker referred to self, other persons or materials. Activity pattern of each homemaker was identified as being higher or lower than the average mean frequency per minute of the thirty homemakers with respect to resource conserving activities (RCA) and the consumption activities (CA). Since the two categories of activities-—RCA and CA——are com— plementary to each other in the sense that a higher level of RCA facilitates or is necessitated by a higher level of CA, activity pattern was identified from both RCA and CA. Half of the homemakers had mixed activity patterns characterized by higher RCA and lower CA, or vice versa. Percentages of each of the three time categories in rela— tion to total number of message units were calculated and Doha Abdel—Rahman Elgidaily used as variables indicating the time—orientation of the homemaker. The predominance of the present time reference in the conversation of these homemakers was consistent with studies relating time-orientation to social class which found that lower class people are predominantly present time—oriented. When each time reference was looked at separately in the same manner as activities, thirteen home— makers were classified as being more future time-oriented, and sixteen and thirteen homemakers, respectively, as being more present and more past timeroriented. With respect to the content areas, these homemakers could be classified as person-centered rather than material centered people. The Chi Square Test of Independence indi— cated that time-content categories were dependent on each other. The homemakers were less future and less past time- oriented under the "materials" category of content and less present time—oriented under the "self" content category. The relationship between the time—orientation of the homemaker and her activity pattern was in the predicted direction. Future time—orientation was associated with higher mean frequency per minute of RCA, whereas past and present time—orientation WEre associated with lower mean frequency per minute of RCA. Several hypotheses were suggested by this exploratory investigation. Doha Abdel—Rahman Elgidaily Homemakers who are future time—oriented perform resource conserving activities more frequently than those who are past or present time-oriented. A higher economic position is positively related to fre— quency of performance of resource conserving activities because (a) performance of RCA contributes to economic im— provement and (b) families in a higher economic position have more material resources requiring conservation. A higher level of education is positively related to frequency of performance of resource conserving activities. Younger, more highly educated homemakers tend to be more future time-oriented than those who are older or have less education. Homemakers who are highly self—centered perform resource conserving activities more frequently than those who are other person—centered. Homemakers who are highly materials-centered perform control activities more frequently than those who are highly person-centered (self or other persons). Homemakers who are future time—oriented and/or highly other person—centered perform preparation—construction activi— ties more frequently than those who are present or past time-oriented and/or highly self—centered. TIME-ORIENTATION AND ACTIVITY PATTERN OF SELECTED HOMEMAKERS BY Doha Abdel—Rahman Elgidaily A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family Ecology 1971 47/? 3/ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer wishes to express the most sincere appreci— ation to Dr. Frances Magrabi, chairman of her guidance com— mittee whose assistance, suggestions and generous help throughout the study were a true learning experience. To her, the writer wishes to acknowledge her indebtedness. It has been very fruitful, as many previous Ph. D. candi— dates have found, to associate and work with her. Special thanks are also due to Dr. Beatrice Paolucci, Dr. Margaret Jacobson and Dr. Carol Shaffer, members of the committee for their interest and advice. Grateful appreci— ation is expressed to Mrs. Jacqueline Wood and Miss Beverly Braden who assisted in coding the data. Gratitude and acknowledgment are also expressed to Dr. Harry Hoppe, in the English department who gave of his time for editorial sug— gestions. A special note of thanks is due to the Egyptian Govern— ment for making my graduate study possible and for financial support. Sincere appreciation is also expressed to the Institute for International Education (IIE) for the finan— cial support. Grateful acknowledgments are due to the Agricultural Research Service, United States Department of Agriculture who supported the study. Last but not least, my husband and my parents deserve much credit for this presentation. Their faith and confi— dence in my abilities were a great sustaining influence. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER P age I. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Statement of the Problem. . . . . . . . . 3 II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 Resource Use in Family Activities . . . . 5 Role of resources in home management . 5 Family activity patterns . . . . . . . 7 Resource conserving activity: impor— tance of the concept. . . . . . ll Techniques for studying family activi— ties° . . . . . 12 Values Underlying Family Activities . . . 14 Role of values in home management. . . 15 Values as directors of activities. . . 18 Related research . . . . . . . . . . . 21 Techniques for probing values. . . . 24 Temporal Values and/or Time- orientations: 27 Time— orientation as a form of value orientation . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 Related research . . . . . . . . . . . 30 Time perspective. . . . . . . . . . 32 Time—orientation. . . . . . . . . . 36 Assumptions and Objectives. . . . . . . . 43 III. METHODOLOGY. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Definition of Terms . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Selection of Sample: Criteria and Rationale . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50 Mechanics of Contacting Families and Techniques for Obtaining Cooperation . 52 Collection of Data. . . . . . . . . . . 56 Means of establishing rapport. . . . . 56 Practice sessions. . . . . . . . . . . 56 Data collection. . . . . . . . . . . . 57 Interview schedules . . . . . . . . 57 Activity records. . . . . . . . . . 58 Verbal conversation . . . . . 62 Problems and difficulties encountered in collecting data. . . . . . . . . 62 TABLE OF CONTENTS-—C0ntinued CHAPTER IV. Coding of Data . . . Coding activity records . . . . . . . . Activity code. Material code. a u c - o o o a u o o n a a . a c n u . u . Space code . . . . . . . . . . . . . Intercoder agreement on activity coding. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Coding conversation . . . . . . . . . . Time- orientation . . . . . . . . . . Content. . . . a u n a o o Intercoder agreement on time refer— ence and content. . . . . . . . . Analysis of Data . . Activity data. 0 o a o . o c . u u . Conversation data. . . . . . . . . . Relationships. o o a o o a . u a o . CHARACTERISTICS‘OF THE SAMPLE . . . . . . . . Size and Composition of the Family . . . . Formal Education of Husbands and Wives . . Occupation of Husbands and Wives . . . . . Income of Families . . . . . . . . . Sources of Income in the Family. . . . . Amount of Money Owed Home Ownership . . Mobility of Families Years . . . . . . Meal Planning Before by and to the Family. in the Last Five Shopping. . Equipments and Facilities Available for the Family. . . . o a o 0 Condition of the Home. . . . . . . . . . . Summary. . . . . . . V. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION. 9 a o a - a o a o o o o a o o a a a o o a o Activity Pattern of the Homemaker. . . . . Distribution of activities. . . . . . Frequency of observed activities in ' relation to area of the home . . . . Frequency of observed activities in relation to material of central focus of the activity. . . . . . . . Relationships among activities. . . . . Identification of the homemaker's activity pattern . . . . . . . . . . iv 96 101 103 105 105 105 110 120 126 130 TABLE OF CONTENTS—~continued CHAPTER Time Orientation of the Homemaker. . . . . Distribution of message units . . . . . Identifying the time—orientation of the homemaker. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Content categories . . . . Test of independence of content— time categories . . . Relationship Between Activity Pattern and Time— orientation. . . . . . . . . . VI. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION. . . . . . . . . . . Limitations of the Study . . . . . . . . Implications for Future Research . . . . . LITERATURE CITED . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX A . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX B . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX D . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 138 139 141 147 147 151 156 162 163 168 175 177 209 213 LIST OF TABLES TABLE 1. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. Number of family members living at home five years before, and at the time, data were collected. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Distribution of family members by age, in thirty families, at the time data were collected. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Ages of husbands and wives of thirty families. Educational level of husbands and wives of thirty families. . . . . . . . . . . . Occupational level of husbands and wives of thirty families. . . . . . . . . . . . Income distribution of thirty families Number of sources of income in thirty families Amount of dollars owed by and to thirty families . . . Home ownership of thirty families. Number of moves in the last five years by thirty families. . . . . . . . . Meal planning before food shopping Types of equipment and facilities available for thirty families. . . . . . . . . . . Rating of thirty families on home condition with respect to five variables . . . . Distribution of observed activities performed by homemakers of thirty families . Percentage of resource conserving activities performed in different locations of the house by thirty families . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi Page 81 83 84 85 87 88 90 92 94 95 97 98 102 107 111 LIST OF TABLES——continued TABLE 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. Percentage of activities with ultimate purpose of consumption performed by thirty families in relation to places of performance. . . . . Percentage of control activities and activi— ties that augment or create new resources per— formed by thirty families in relation to place of performance . . . . . . . . . . Percentage of different activities performed by thirty families in relation to material of the central focus of activity. . . . . Percentage of total activities performed by thirty families in relation to material of the central focus of the activity. Correlation coefficients calculated among different types of activities. . . . . Number of homemakers identified for each of the four activity patterns . . . . . . . . Homemakers in the higher and lower groups of mean frequenciesfminute of resource Conserv1ng activities in relation to their educational level. . . . . . . . . . Homemakers in the higher and lower_groups of mean frequencies/minute of resource conserv1hg activities in relatibn to their occupations. Family incomes of homemakers in the higher and lower mean frequency/minute of resource con— serving activities . . . . . . . . . Percentage of message units spoken by thirty homemakers in each of the three time references . . . . . . . . . . Time orientation of thirty homemakers. . . Distribution of age of thirty homemakers in relation to their time—orientation . . Distribution of educational level of thirty homemakers in relation to their time— orientation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Page 115 121 127 129 132 135 135 137 140 143 143 146 LIST OF TABLES——continued TABLE 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34 35. 36. Percentages of message units spoken by thirty homemakers in each of the three content areas . Percentages of message units of thirty home— makers' conversations during the performance of daily household activities as related to time content categories. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Correlation coefficient calculated between types of activities and time content cate- gories. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Percentage of different kinds of activities performed in different locations of the house by thirty families. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Percentage of total activities performed in each location of the house by thirty families . Resource conserving activities in relation to material of central focus of the activity in order from highest to lowest percentages. . . . Utilization activities in relation to materials directly utilized in the activity in order from highest to lowest percentages . . . . . . . Control activities and activities which augment and/or create new resources in relation to_ material directly used in the activity in order frOm highest to lowest percentages. . . . . . . viii Page 148 149 153 214 215 217 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Time plays a crucial role in our lives, serving to integrate our behavior and mediate our adjustment to our surroundings (10). It has long been an area of concern to philosophers, biologists, physicists, and psychologists (64). In recent years, however, the nature of "time—orientation“ as a personality variable has been the focus of more atten— tion from psychologists in the form of either speculation or research about the manner in which people orient themselves to time. Evidence from several investigations (4,24,46,76,77) suggests that different individuals and groups do differ with respect to being past, present, or future time—oriented. According to Teahan (76) one can conceivably characterize individuals or groups of individuals by the kinds of temporal orientations which dominate their lives. The manner in which individuals perceive and orient them— selves to time is thought to be an important determinant of behavior. More specifically and as Fink (24) puts it, “The effects of an individual's time perspective on his behavior are dependent upon the differential importance he ascribes to his psychological past, present, and future." Temporal orientation thus seems to be an important area of study if one is to better understand individuals. A quote from Frank —§,, fir cited by Smeltzer (70) may illustrate this point: "... Probably the most important differentiating aspect of these private worlds (each individual's) is the value we give to the various constituents of the surrounding world, according to our individual time perspective.” If time perspective and time—orientation are of importance to the understanding of man, then professional people in the field of home management who are concerned about the homemaker and her managerial practices in the home must be interested in studying the homemaker's temporal value— orientation which underlies her behavior in the family setting. In current home management literature an accepted idea is that values direct activities and behavior. The de— gree to which the homemaker's behavior in the home relates to her temporal values and/or time—orientation, whether she is past, present, or future time—oriented, has been a compara- tively untapped home management research area, and thus is a question that remains largely unanswered. Very little is known regarding the nature of the time—orientation of the homemaker and its relation to her day—to—day activities and/or behavior in the home. Families achieve their goals through some type of be— havior or activity. Hence, many activities performed by the homemaker are directed toward goals. During the per— formance of the activity the homemaker acts upon or utilizes resources. It is through the use of resources in day—to—day activities that families reach their goals. As homemakers perform their normal daily activities, patterns which tend to conserve the family's available resources and serve a resource-conserving function, as well as patterns of activi— ties which utilize materials with the ultimate purpose of consumption, may be distinguished. Since the values which an individual holds determine or direct his activities, one expects a relationship between the temporal values of the homemaker and the activities she performs. The differences in the temporal value—orientations of homemakers may be related to differences in their activity patterns. For example, future time-orientation may be more strongly associ— ated than past time—orientation with the performance of activities which involve conservation of families' resources because a future time—oriented homemaker may be more inter— ested in ensuring that the material goods and resources she has available will continue to fulfill their function in the future, and therefore she may be inclined to perform activities which conserve these resources. Past time—orien— tation on the other hand implies that the homemaker lives in the past and repeats what she has done in the past° Statement of the Problem The present study is concerned with an investigation into the activity pattern of the homemaker, her time— orientation, and the relationship between the two. In more specific terms, the purpose of the study is three fold: First, to identify the pattern of activity and/or resource-use of the homemaker and the extent to which her activity pattern involves conservation of resources. Such identification of the homemaker's activity pattern in these terms may be useful in helping families improve their economic position and quality of life as dependent upon use of resources. Second, to identify temporal value—orientation of the homemaker, past, present, or future. Third, to explore relationships between time—orientation of the homemaker and her activity and/or resource—use pattern. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE This chapter is designed to present a review of litera— ture pertinent to the establishment of the theoretical orientation developed and used in this research. Areas of literature are presented in the following manner: 1) resource—use in family activities; 2) values underlying family activities; and 3) temporal values and/or time— orientations. Assumptions and objectives will be presented at the end of the chapter. Resource Use In Family Activities 1 Included in this section is a review of literature ‘ relevant to the role of resources in home management and the way in which the concept is viewed by different investiga— tors in the field, the concept of family activity patterns and resource—conserving activities, and techniques of study- ing family activities. Role of resources in home management Management is defined as the use of resources to achieve some goals. Thus, management by definition implies that one manages something, namely resources. This view is supported by many writers (18,19,68,69). Deacon and Bratton (18), for example, state that the functional nature of management is accepted in its definition as a process by which means (resources) are used to achieve ends. Paolucci and O'Brien (68), consider resources as the tools of management, indicating that increasing one's knowledge about resources facilitates management since management is the way one pro— ceeds from resources to goals. Resources are the tools which make end results and goals possible. Deacon and Maloch (19), in their proposed framework for management, define resources as being anything (knowledge; skills, abilities, or objects) which can be used or directed toward achieving demands. Nickell and Dorsey (65), in dis— cussing family resources, look at resources as consisting of tools, assets, capabilities and ways and means possessed. Several writers suggest the division of resources into human and non—human resources. Gross and Crandall (34), Paolucci and O'Brien (68) and Schlater (69), include energy, skills, knowledge and attitudes under the human resources, and tangible goods and facilities under the non—human resources. Gross and Crandall, however, consider time as a human resource. Whether time is a resource or utility has been questioned by Warren (78). Using as criteria that a resource must have alternate uses, be scarce, and be transferable, she points but that time meets only the first criterion of having alternate uses. On the other hand time is not scarce unless one believes that the world is coming to an end. Time is not transferable in the sense that one cannot sell or buy it, or give it to someone else. Time goes with each individual. From the previous discussion, one can see that decisions in management determine how the resources will be used. Family resources, whether human or non—human, are acted upon to achieve goals. It seems that the most important charac— teristic or quality of resources is their scarcity, and that is where the need for management arises. As Schlater (69) points out, resources of all families are limited and scarce; if otherwise, no managerial decisions would need to be made. The scarcity of resources implies that conservation and preservation of resources are important if the family is to use them efficiently to reach goals. Family activity patterns The focus of home management is on how families manage, in other words how they use and organize the resources they have in order to achieve their goals. In her study of ”Daily Activity Patterns of Peasant Homemakers," Nelson (63), explored the concept of activity pattern as an approach to understanding how time functions in home management. An activity pattern was defined as the ordering of tasks which is characteristic of a person or group of persons during some specified time span. Two types of activities were suggested as meaningful: behavioristic and ideational activity patterns. Behavioristic activity patterns were defined as those derived from what people do, while idea— tional activity patterns were defined’as those derived from what people say that they 6r others have done or will do. Within the selected time_span, the indices of presence, repetition, and sequence of activities were proposed as specific indicators of activity pattern. The most important result of this study was that it demonstrated the applic— ability of the concept of activity pattern in a specific cultura1.setting, namely Costa Rica. Nelson, however, did not discuss the possibility of the applicability of the concept “activity pattern“ in other cultures as well. In another research study (84), activity patterns and space needs of four types of American farm households were investigated. The four types of families were identified according to the age of children. The first type all had children under six. The second type had some children under six and some six or above. The third type had children 6—18 years of age and the fourth type had all children 18 or over or no children living at home. The activity patterns of individuals within the four family types were compared. Unlike Nelson's study, in the Northeastern study (84), activity patterns were identified for any individual in the family based upon the distribution of each class of indi— viduals' time. The patterns for each family type were not representative of any one family, yet the distinct differences between types gave evidence of changing housing needs through the life span of a family. The activity participation and the use of locations by the different family members in the different family types led the authors to make some generali— zations about the activity patterns which were identified in terms of the amount of time spent on each activity. In Baker's study of Costa Rican families (1), she was concerned with developing a conceptual framework and measure— ment model for family—resource patterns as they relate to the educability of preschool children. She classified family resource patterns by nine resource categories: space, move~ ment, care and appearance, play, task and work, child's learning, family learning, child's social contacts and family social contacts. Educability referred to the goal of having the child prepared for successful participation in school. Four resource dimensions were used for each resource cate— gory: availability—quantity, availability—quality, use— quantity, and use—quality. The study offered implications for theory of management in families, for cross—cultural research, and action programs with families. Although Baker‘s study was concerned with family resource patterns rather than activity patterns, the two concepts are related in the sense that resources are means of achieving goals and the use of resources usually is involved in any type of activity. Magrabi, Paolucci, and Heifner (58), developed a differ— ‘ent framework for studying family activity patterns. 10 These investigators conceptualized family activity patterns as being characterized by a set of dimensions: the time (duration and sequence) associated with the activity; participants; communications (content and identity of message receivers and sender); spacial areas in which the activity takes place (identity of the space and index of interactive space); and the materials utilized in the activity. If the homemaker's pattern of activity and/or resource use is to be viewed from the position of conservation of resources, such dimensions as the time associated with the activity, house— hold materials and equipment used in the activity may be useful in classifying the activity. There have been several studies done on the homemaker's use of time in the United States. These studies were pri— marily concerned with the amount of time allocated for dif— ferent types of family activities. Some studies of the use of time by homemakers showed that a full—time homemaker averages about eight hours on a weekday for household work (12,81). Data collected by Steidl (73) in a study of three groups of families (full—time farm and city homemakers, and employed city homemakers) provided some information about the distribution of activities between homemaking and non— homemaking activities. She found that the pattern was similar among the three types of homemakers. The propor— tions of time allocated to the broad areas of food, housing, 11 clothing, marketing, and personal activities were the same among them. However, the employed city homemakers allocated slightly less time to homemaking activities, as compared to the full—time farm and city homemakers. What Steidl. meant by pattern in her study is the percentage of time allocated for the activity. Some of the studies on the use of time report that activities may express the individual's values. Two studies reported by Dyer (20) and Ketchum (42) suggest that values of homemakers are expressed through the daily activities performed by them. These two studies will be revieWed later in the section on "values underlying family activities.“ Resource—conserving activities: importance of the concept The management of material goods in the household presents one of the most complicated problems today. Material goods, as Gross and Crandall (34) state, range from food mixers to safety pins, from fabric rugs to linoleum, from clothes to pens and pencils, and from highly perishable foods to books. Within the family, goods are made available for the life support of its members. In order for a family to obtain goods, this can be done through the acquisition of new goods from the market. Once these are made available for the family and in order to regulate acquisition of new goods from the market, the family needs to conserve these avail— able resources owing to the fact that resources are in w: fi=1== _—.. 12 general scarce, and therefore need conservation and care. For each article in the household, appropriate treat— ment and care after use are necessary to preserve it if this material good is to continue to function properly in the future during a normally expected time period. By the same token, avoiding waste will conserve the family's resources. Family activities such as caring for durable goods and avoiding waste tend to conserve and preserve the family's resources and hence may be called resource— conserving activities. Helping families conserve their resources may be a useful means of improving their economic position and environmental quality through organizing and regulating their acquisition of new goods. Other activities seem to utilize and use up the family's resources and may be termed consumption activities, for example, moving things away from storage for the purpose of consumption. Techniques for studying family activities Family activities studies within home management have tended to be quantitative, i.e., focusing on the amount of time spent in each activity. Studies of homemakers' activi- ties and/or time use have usually been made using chrono— logical records of activities for one or two days, more specifically by means of personal diaries. Diary studies have proven to be time—consuming and require cooperative and intelligent subjects, and thus do not adequately represent those homemakers with little 13 education, or with young children or other demanding responsibilities. In studying daily activity patterns of homemakers, Nelson (63), utilized the observational tech— nique as a more accurate method for obtaining detailed data. In the Northeastern study of household activities home observations were also utilized (84). Warren, back in 1940 (79), developed a method designed to eliminate the limited representation which occurred in diary studies. In Warren's study, selected homemakers were asked to recall their activities for some days prior to the interview. This method was based on the assumption that the homemakers' recall was accurate enough so that results were a valid representation of time use in the period which was recalled. Its limitation is that recall may be a vari— able factor, thus the validity of such data may be ques— tioned. Such factors as the interval between the occurrence of the event and the recall, homemaker variation in ability to remember, and the relative importance of the events themselves to the homemaker, can operate to make recall data quite different from the actual time use which the method aimed to reveal. Ketchum (42) and Dyer (20) in their studies of the homemakers' values as reflected in family activities both used the open—ended question of how the homemaker used her time the day before, and why. The homemakers recalled yesterday's activities with reasons for doing each. The two 14 researchers were primarily interested in the value content of the activities rather than the activities themselves. Therefore, the recall method did not seem inappropriate, since the reasons for doing activities were more important than an accurate detailed record of the activities. If activities performed by the homemaker are to be analyzed in terms of whether or not they involve a resource— conserving function, a detailed record of what the home— maker does minute by minute for a certain period of time is important so that each activity can be classified with respect to whether it is a resource—conserving activity or not. For example, while the homemaker is preparing food, she may possibly turn on the stove, then turn it off, move things from and to storage. If the homemaker kept a record of her daily activities, she would probably include all these activities under food preparation, since it is the major activity going on. On the other hand if the home— maker is observed while doing the same activities, the observer can record each activity in detail so that activi— ties such as moving things to storage which clearly imply resource conservation, may be classified as such. Values Underlying Family Activities Included in this section is a review of literature pertinent to the role of values in home management, values 15 as directors of activities, reIated research, and techniques for probing values. Role of values in home management The importance of values in home management has been perceived by several investigators. Malone and Malone (59) strongly emphasize the idea that management is guided by family values and goals, ."It cannot be emphasized too strong— ly that good management strives to reach the largest total of family desires but is guided by family values and goals.” Hill (37) points out the fact that values are of first concern to all of us. He emphasizes that this must be true if we accept the fact that the purpose of home economics is to promote the well being of individuals and families. Paolucci (67) in pointing out the importance of values I!" in home management says ... becoming aware of the value content in family living, recognizing that values can be expressed and perpetuated through the performance of mundane household tasks, and the ability to define those values that underlie the choices and decisiOns individuals make is the first concern of home management.” In this connection, Frank (28) thinks that the most effective opportunity to modify the practices of home man— agement is to be found in the school and college, where young people who are to be hememakers can be helped to dis— cover not merely efficient practices and skills but more importantly the ideas and concepts and the values which they 16 carry into their adult lives and attempt to realize as members of a family and parents of children. Emphasizing the responsibility of home management as a source of human values, Frank believes that home management is not merely a question of skills and standardized equip— ment which are important to the conservation of human energy and time. More specifically, he looks at home management as a way of life for which the homemaker needs clarification of aims and purposes, aspriations and values, a faith in the supreme importance of the human relations that alone give the home social justification. It seems that the interest in and the importance of val~ ues in home management stem from their relation to decision and goal formation. To emphasize the importance of values in decision—making, Mockmore (61) says that in almost any situ— ation which calls for decision, one is forced to evaluate alternatives. What this means is simply to weigh alterna— tives in terms of one's identified values. Gross (33) in her article on "Impact of Certain Disciplines on Home Manage— ment" also points out the important relationship of values and decision—making. She says that in selecting one alterna— tive out of several, one aspect is its probability of attain— ing certain goals and the other is the desirability of those goals and the latter comes from the decision—maker value system. In Jacob, Flink, and Schuchman's (39) opinion, ”A value is the standard for decision—making." Geiger (30), 17 expresses the idea that man makes his decisions on the basis of his values. The relationship between values and decision—making seems to be based on the process that involves among other things, evaluation, choice, and elimination of possible alternative courses of action in favor of that alternative which in the mind of the decision—maker is the most desirable course of action. Once the decision is made, a goal is reached, since each alternative is made up of two elements, resource and goal. The decision involves choosing specific means (use of resources) to reach a goal. As values play a vital role in the decision—making process in management they also are the underlying force in setting our goals. The three sources of motivation in management reported by Gross and Crandall (34) are values, ; goals, and standards. Although the three are closely related, ivalues is the key term of the trilogy. From values stem the function as the most basic underlying force in directing two other aspects, goals and standards (34)° That values managerial activity is an accepted proposition in management, ' Schlater (69) states. Examination of the underlying values that prompt a family to select goals and make particular decisions to effect these goals increases understanding of management and should ultimately result in more effective management (69). In this connection, Deacon and Bratton (18), believe *that individuals or families who have developed or recognized 18 a system of values that provides a basis for selection among goals will experience more satisfaction in management of activities than those who have not. It seems that regardless of the facility required in the other aspects of management, satisfaction would be limited if there were too much vacillation on goals or uncertainty about relative importance of the values one holds. Schlater (69) also elaborates on this point by stating that the success of the family in making satisfying decisions depends upon under- standing the values its members are attempting to realize through decisions made. When values are consciously identi— fied and expressed, they can more easily be translated into goals and then through decision—making to realization. Lee (51) thinks that one way to aid the individual in his world of change is to help him become aware of the value (content of his everyday life and to be aware of the values 1 l at the base of his choices and decisions. ‘ I 1 Values as directors of activities The idea that values are directors of behavior and activities is reflected in the literature. From the previous discussion one can see the role of values in selecting among alternatives in a decision—making process and the role of values in setting goals. The following review of litera— ture illustrates and clarifies the relationship of values to action, behavior and/or activity through their relation to decisions and goal formation. The point of view of a 19 number of writers from different fields is presented; many and varied interpretations are given. Heenan (36), points out that the person's needs, motives and values determine the choices he makes, steer his behavior and give him a sense of direction. According to Kluckhohn (45), "A value is a conception, explicit or implicit, distinctive of an individual or characteristic of a group, of the desirable which influences the selection from available modes, means, and ends of action.“ Williams (82), looks at values as ”... observable variables in human conduct.... They (men) select particular objects and courses of action out of a range of possibilities present in a situ— ation. Some human behavior at least, is purposive: people can and do state their goals in advance and can then be observed to act as if they were in fact pursuing those ends.” Williams (83) in another one of his writings also in- cludes the idea that values serve as a standard for action by pointing out that values are meaningful and affectively invested pattern principles that guide the human conduct. Jacob, Flink, and Schuchman (39) also state that values are the normative standards by which human beings are influenced in their choice among the alternative courses of action Which they perceive. Linton (56) illustrated the relation— ship of values to behavior by defining the value as any element common to a series of situations which is capable of evoking overt respOnse in the individual. 20 While some of the writers discussed above see values as standards for action, Fitcher (26) has expressed the view that values influence actions as well as goals. He believes people use values as norms and criteria that point the way to goals and objectives. Stein and Cloward (74), define values and their rela— tionship to action in the following statements: "Values determine the choices men make, and the ends they live by. What is considered good and what is evil; what is right and wrong; success and failure; what is important and unim— portant; desirable and undesirable; beautiful and ugly, all are value questions. Whether the values lie in the realm of ethics, economics, aesthetics, or religion, they exist as they are experienced in human minds and translated into human action.“ In presenting these different points of views, one can conclude that values are concepts of the desirable which are important to the individual and assist him in selecting (modes, means and ends of actions. They are the underlying ‘forces directing activities. From values stem goals which in their turn are translated to some type of behavior or ‘activity. "Values are the original horizon from which stem our peculiar pattern of goals through which we view the Potentialities of our resources and even the management ‘process" (57). Each activity, therefore, is directed toward a goal which is formulated according values. Values thus 21 underlie-behavior and direct activities through the use of resources. The relationship between values and activities can be illustrated in the following diagram. F VALUES ""— M GOALS M ACTIVITIES Related research Several studies in home management reported by Dyer (20), Engebretson (22), Kohlmann (48) and Ketchum (42), have focused on the homemaker's values, with homemakers as respondents. And others have focused on family's values, with parents and their children between the ages of 12 and 18 (60), or with parents and one or more of their children of varying ages as respondents (13,25). Although much has been written concern— ing the idea that values underlie behavior and are directors of activities, very little research is reported in this regard. The two studies conducted by Dyer and Ketchum have focused on the homemaker's values as they are reflected in personal and family activities. Only these two studies will 22 be reviewed since they are more relevant to the present study in that they focus on values in relation to home activities. Ketchum (42) studied the values of homemakers as they were reflected in family activities. She compared the home- makers' values as identified by the reasons given for the daily home activities with their ranking of twelve selected values. After making the comparison, she tried to see whether the hierarchical arrangement is the same. The author thought of values as the directors of the managerial choices. The 50 homemakers who constituted her sample were volun— teers from a Home Demonstration group. The data consisted of background information, answers to open—ended questions about activities performed and reasons for their performance, the ranking of a list of 12 values, and forced—choice test of the most important value in each of 66 pairings of values. In each of the pairs, the homemakers selected the statement representing the most important value. In order to determine the relative importance of the various values to each homemaker, the number of times each value represented was chosen over the other values repre— sented, was computed. The relationships between values reflected in the reasons, and the rank order and the forced— choice test were compared. A high correlation was found between the rank order and the forced—choice test which made the investigator assume that they may be used interchangeably. 23 The correlation coefficient calculated between values reflected through reasons given for activities and values selected in the forced choice test was much lower. This perhaps may be due to the difficulty that Ketchum mentioned in her classifying reasons according to the selected value categories, and also to the limited ability of the home— makers to verbalize reasons for performing activities. It is possible that a higher correlation could have been obtained between the categorized reasons and the other rank— ings, if the homemakers had categorized them instead of the researcher doing the categorization of reasons herself. However, Ketchum's study was valuable in the sense that it contributed knowledge and research techniques concerning the study of family values. Looking at Dyer's study (20), one finds it very similar to Ketchum's. It upholds the idea that activities have value COntent and that homemakers are to a certain degree aware of the value an activity mediates. She attempts to identify the values of homemakers through their expressed reasons for their daily activities, and to compare the hierarchy of their values by other research techniques. She defines values in much the same way as Ketchum. Different from Ketchum's study, however, the sample consisted of a group of student wives. She had the home— makers categorize the reasons they expressed for performing activities. This perhaps overcame the limitation reported by Ketchum. Dyer used 9 projective stories representing 24 each value in place of the forced choice test used by Ketchum; she also used the ranking order of a set of 9 values, both tests being constructed around the same values. The home~ makers categorized the reasons also around the same 9 values. As the homemaker was asked to record her activities with reasons for doing each, 9 envelopes were provided for the 9 values and she was asked to place her activities and their accompanying reasons in the envelope she thought each belonged. The highest correlation was found between projec— tive stories and categorized reasons, and the lowest corre— lation between the rank order test and categorized reasons. The researcher accordingly concluded that the more projective the instrument is the more likely it is to reveal values underlying the homemaker's behavior. Techniques for probing values Values are not tangible or observable. They have to be inferred by means of some kind of research technique. No two individuals or families hold the same values. Value identification becomes a real problem because people have difficulty verbalizing their values and goals, and they are [not usually aware whether their actions or decisions are achieving desirable ends. According to Straus (75) ”Unfortunately most people are rather non—plussed when (asked what their goals are." 1 Different means for probing the values which particular :groups hold have been suggested and used by researchers. 25 The idea that values can be effectively identified and inferred through observed behavior is found in the litera- ture (45,83). Kluckhohn (45) contended that ”Values can be appraised by studying choice, preference or inference from overt behavior, but the study of choice—behavior seems to offer the nearest approach to a research method uniquely adapted to the study of values." Williams (83) also suggests looking at the choices made in order to identify values. He points out that direct observation, testimony of witnesses, self—reporting and other indirect means can be used. Clough‘s (11) classification of values is based upon the aspect of human behavior to which they apply. He identi— fies seven areas of activity in which values are apparent. His classification of values according behavioral activity lends support to the idea that a relationship exists between values and behavior. The rank—order test, the projective—story technique and the forced-choice test fer identifying values were used by several researchers in hOme management. Beyer (7), and Kimball (43) used the rank order test to identify values related to housing, values related to farm and home practices, respectively. Dyer (20) and Ketchum (42) utilized the same technique for studying homemakers' values. Values related to housing needs were researched by Cutler (13), and values of a group of homemakers were studied 26' by Ketchum (42), and Kohlmann (48). All these researchers used the forced—choice tests. Dyer (20), Engebretson (22), and Martin (60), in probing homemakers' values utilized the projective—story technique. Summarizing the techniques used in studies conducted on values, one can say that rank—order test seems to be effective in bringing values to the level of awareness and in identifying a hierarchy among values. Families need to be made sufficiently aware of their values which underlie their decisions and goals at least to know what they are working toward. This awareness increases the managerial ability of family members. The forced—choice tests suggest the establishment of a priority of values. One needs to keep in mind, however, that the choices must always be made between two values even though neither of them may be held to a very important degree. The projective story technique showed effectiveness in the studies where it was used. In the previous three ways of studying values, sets of values are established by the researchers. This, however, probably has the disadvantage that these pre—established sets of values may not be all inclusive. In other words the selection of the range of possible values may not fit into a set of categories. This danger can prdbably be reduced to some extent if each researcher is very careful in select— ing and delineating those values that are to be studied. Aside from the previous techniques, values may also be inferred by other means. For example, what people say 27 in their words can reflect their values. Kluckhohn (45) wrote under the heading “Operations For The Study of Values“ "... what a person says about his values is truer from a long term viewpoint than inferences drawn from his actions under specific conditions ... people often lie by their acts and tell the truth with words." Language used by people can also be an expression and reflection of these people. According to Berlo (6), language is one of the ways in which we express ourselves and our ideas, and messages are the expression of what we think of. One usually speaks as he thinks. What people value or see as important is expressed in their vocabulary during their conversations. All humans have language. The language any group uses is an expression of that particular group. What is important in a culture, and in a family is shown in the vo— cabulary. Language reflects attitudes, customs, feelings and values of the group from which language originates. One of the assumptions of this present paper therefore, is that values are expressed through language and hence can be inferred from the conversation of people. Temporal Values and/or Time Orientations Included in this section is a review of literature relevant to: time—orientation as a form of value—orienta— tion, and related research. 28 Time—orientation as a form of value— orientation A major study of value-orientation was conducted by Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck (46). They argue that the basic values of people have direct effect upon their behavior and thought. They classify value orientation into five cate— gories, 1) Human nature (innate goodness and badness), 2) Man—nature (subjugation—to—nature, harmony-with—nature, mastery—over—nature), 3) Time dimension (present, past, future), 4) Activity (being, being—in-becoming, doing), and 5) Relational (lineality, collaterality, individualism). The researchers used this framework for the study of dif— ferent cultural group communities in southwest U.S.A., namely a Spanish—American village, a Navaho Indian band, and Anglo—Saxon. Primarily the study was concerned with the exposition of a theory of variation in value—orientations. The researchers defined the value—orientation as "patterned principles resulting from transactional interplay of three analytically distinguishable elements of the evaluative process, the cognitive, the affective, and the directive elements——which give order and direction to the ever—flowing stream of human acts and thoughts as they relate to the solution of common human problems.“ Time—orientation, as mentioned above, was one of the five forms of value—orientation studied by them. They state that the temporal orientation can break easily into the three-point range of past, present and future. The three 29 fold division proposed by Kluckhohn and Strddtbeck for the time value orientation schema has a similarity to Spengler's (71) conception in its distinction between a relatively timeless, traditionless, future—ignoring present and realizable future, but it also differentiates from these an orientation which places primary emphasis upon the main— tenance, or the restoration of the traditions of the past. Variations in temporal focus were illustrated by Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck. They point out that Spanish Americans who have been described as taking the view that man is a victim of natural forces are also peOple who place the present time alternative in first order position. They pay little attention to what has happened in the past and regard the future as both vague and unpredictable. Planning for the future or hoping that the future will be better than either the present or the past simply is not ‘their way of life (46). Historical China, these researchers ‘argue, was a society which gave first—order value preference to the past time-orientation. Ancestor worship and a strong family tradition were both expressions of this preference. So also was the Chinese attitude that nothing new ever happened in the present or would happen in the future; it had all happened before in the far distant past. Some of the chief differences between the people of the ’United States and England, Kluckhohn and Strodtbeck argue, derive from somewhat varying attitudes toward time. 30 Americans have difficulty understanding the respect the English have for tradition, and the English do not apprEci— ate the typical American's disregard for it (46). Americans, more strongly than most people of the world, place an empha— sis upon the future——a future which is anticipated to be 1 bigger and better. This does not mean they have no regard for the past or no thought of the present. But it certainly is true that no current generation of Americans ever wants to be called "old fashioned." The previous discussion reveals that time—orientation is a form of value—orientation. Kluckhohn's definition of value—orientation, that it gives order and direction to E actions and thought in solving the human problems, is con— sistent with the earlier—stated definitions of values which affirm that values are directors of activities. The temporal value—orientation accordingly may be defined as lthe direction——past, present, or future—-of the individuals' (thoughts and feelings that affect their overt action. Related research The temporal value—orientation of the homemaker whether lshe is past, present, or future time—oriented has not yet \. lbeen fully explored, but a review of pertinent psychological (literature related to the concept of time—orientation is 1 iincluded. In a study of decision—making styles of Mexican 1 homemakers, Bustrillos (9), identifies the style as the lcombination of the dimensions of the three elements, mode, 31 time reference, and decision making rule. This time refer— ence base has three dimensions, past, present, and future. The responses of the homemakers to three hypothetical decision problems were examined and classified according to the three elements of decision—making style and their dimensions. The criteria for coding statements under each of the time—reference categories were as follows: For future reference the criteria were predictive statements or explicit expectations of fulfillment or a future state of affairs. For past reference the criteria were statements about the traditional, the habitual, or a reference to what had gone before. For present reference the criteria were statements which denoted the current conditions, that which was on— going but not habitual, what was felt, or what was universal. These are consistent with definition of time—orientation as value. Bustrillos's results showed that the situational characteristics as the homemaker perceived them at the time she made the decision were most frequently the present time reference. The past and future time references were domi— nant in very few decisions. Although Bustrillos considered time reference as an element in decision—making styles, this so—called time reference appears to represent in fact the temporal value— orientation. For the present study, Bustrillos's study is Valuable in the sense that it gives some clues about the way time reference can be examined and coded. 32 Some work on the nature of time-orientation appears in psychological literature. Several definitions and studies of true—orientation have been reviewed and presented. A number of workers have shown that different individuals and groups do differ with respect to time—orientation. Some researchers used the two terms time—orientation and time perspective synonymously in the sense that their definitions are almost the same. Some authors gave the two terms varied definitions. It seems appropriate therefore, to review the two terms separately. Time perspective. Barndt and Johnson (5) studied time- orientation in delinquents. To them the two terms time- orientation and time perspective meant the same thing. This study will be discussed later in the time—orientation section. Wallace (77) on the other hand, in investigating the (phenomenon of time perspective in schizophrenia, differen— (tiated between the two concepts of time—orientation and :time perspective. Time—perspective referred to the span of itime covered by the individual's thoughts, fantasies, feel— (ings, etc., into the past and into the future, while time— {orientation referred to the direction in time of the ‘individual's thoughts, fantasies, and feelings, into past “present, or future (77). To him time—perspective, means the ldistance into the future that the individual is able to :project himself, and past—time perspective signifies the time span U7). menti or fl that orga his ofr reL and log fut em 33 span into the past that the individual can conceptualize (77). The term that Wallace has suggested for the distance mentioned above is extension, which can refer to either past or future time perspective. By using the story completion technique Wallace found that the length of future time span and the degree of organization of its contents are significantly reduced for his sample of schizophrenic patients as compared with a group of normal controls. According to Merrell (24) “time perspective is the relative degree of extensiveness, specificity, clarity, and emotional involvement with events (physical and psycho— logical) occurring differentially in the past, present, and future.“ This definition is somewhat similar to yet differ— ent from Wallace's definition of time perspective. The two definitions are similar in the sense that they both involve extensiveness and/or distance reflected in the individual's feelings. But they are different since Wallace considers time perspective with respect to past and future only, while Merrell looks at it in the three time dimensions, past, present, and future. Wallace's definition seems to be more precise and clearly stated. Stated by Lewin (54), time perspective is defined as "the totality of an individual's views of his psychological future and psychological past existing at a given time.“ In another article Lewin (55) expresses the same idea, but refe: says the futu give con: Alt? W017 amt fm Es) 34 refers to both time orientation and time perspective. He says that an individual's behavior may encompass more than the present situation, and the psychological past and future are part of the total life space existing at any given moment. The individual's views of the past and future constitute his time perspective or time orientation (55). Although Lewin equated the two terms time perspective and time orientation, his definition lacks clarity. The use of words psychological past and future in the definition seem ambiguous because these words need definition themselves. The Lewinian concept of time perspective has been the focus of several investigations, namely those reported by Eson (23), and Fink (24). Eson in his analysis of time perspective at five different age levels, was primarily concerned with “temporal emphasis" or the stress given in thought or conversation to the past and the future. To him, time perspective means ”the emphasis given to the various time dimensions.” His method was to have his subjects list items that they had talked or thought about in the previous two week period. Then, he analyzed these items according to whether they referred to the past or future. Eson's findings showed that all his group representing the five age levels places more emphasis on the future. Contrary to the common assumption that old people place less emphasis on the future and more on the past, Lewin found that his 65 year old group place more emphasis on the future than on the past. spec ques orie shi} Act 35 3 According to the definition Eson used for time per— spective and the methodology he employed, it may be questionable whether his results revealed the type of orientation which characterized his subjects. Fink (24), on the other hand in his thesis "The Relation— ship of Time Perspective to Age, Institutionalization, and Activity," focused on the concept of time perspective which he defined as "The psychological past, present and future as they exist for and influence the individual." Fink's primary tool for assessing time perspective was the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) from which he used pictures. He also analyzed the subjects' daily conversations in terms of the past and future references in their thoughts. Fink found that aged people were more concerned with the past and less with the future than younger people. However, he also found that aged institutiOnalized people were more past oriented than were their non-institutionalized age equals. A related but somewhat modified View of time perspec— tive is given by Krech and Crutchfield (49), who suggest that time perspective is ”the psychological past and future as they exist for and influence the individual at the present moment.” In the original formulation by Lewin (54) as mentioned above, time perspective referred to ”The totality of the individual’s views of his psychological past and psychological future existing at a given time." 36 In fact the previous conceptions and definitions of time perspective for the most part are confusing. One would not know exactly whether each author was studying time perspective or time—orientation since the authors did not clearly define each concept. To a large extent, the difficulty and confusion stem from the fact that some of the terms included in the above definitions themselves need definitions and clarification. For example, concepts such as psychological past are subject to many interpre— tations if used in a definition. Therefore the inclusion of such terms in definitions lends ambiguity to the obtained findings. It seems that Wallace’s definition of time per— spective avoids much of the confusion since it precisely differentiates between time—orientation and time perspective. Time—orientation. Barndt (4), compared extensiveness of time—orientation of two groups of 16 year old boys, one of institutionalized delinquents and one of public school pupils. He defined time-orientation as ”psychologically experienced relationship with the time continuum.“ Story completion method was used. He measured time—orientation by having the subjects complete a story and then tell them how long a time had elapsed from beginning to end of the story. The results were placed into categories ranging from "under an hour" to "three months or more.". Holding social class constant, Barndt found that his delinquents projected less future time into their stories and were more pres e1 deiin who i adols their ver tiv ext 37 present—oriented than their more future—oriented non— delinquent brethren. This was also true of the subjects of Davids et al.'(l4) who found that male and female juvenile delinquents of adolescent age were present time—oriented as compared to their normal group who were future time—oriented. Some probing has been done into the relationship between social class and time—orientation. Lewin for example, thinks that the social environment in which an individual lives affects his time—orientation. LeShan (53), attempted to investigate the relationship between social class and time orientation. His definition of time—orientation is very similar to the previous definitions of time perspec— tive. He suggests that time—orientation refers to the extensiveness of the time span with which the individual is concerned, the crucial time limit during which he will frustrate himself in order to attain a goal. His subjects were 74 lower class and 43 middle class children eight to ten years old. Using the story projective technique, stories were told by the subjects in response to the stimulus “Tell me a story.” The stories were scored in terms of the length of time covered by the action of the stories. LeShan found statistically significant differences in the length of time covered in the stories told by children of these two social classes. The middle class children were more future time- oriented than their lower class peers who seemed to be I'.—";’7€vS;E“—‘gkrvr~j; 3.443;??? ...... .xl. , 1 7 38 predominantly present oriented. The middle class children also told stories which involved a longer time span. Thus, LeShan substantiated his hypothesis that there are different time—orientations for different social classes. According to LeShan's definition and method of scoring the data, one is not quite sure whether LeShan measured time perspective or time—orientation. Fraisse (27) supports LeShan's findings by pointing out that the temporal perspectives and orientations bear a relationship to each of the groups to which one belongs and in which his experiences have their origin. Davis (17) and Kinsey (44) have also remarked on dif— ferences in temporal orientations between members of dif- ferent social classes. They both point out that lower class groups are less likely to postpone present gratification in favor of future gains. They believe that middle class families place greater stress upon the future in the train— ing of their children. Davis (16), while discussing the socialization process, suggests that the class culture largely determines the extent and kind of future orientation for a given adolescent. Murphy (62) speaks of differences in time perspective or orientation between Hindus and Moslems. He attributes these differences to differences in cultural backgrounds. Frank (29) says "Whole social classes may be described by the time perspective that dominates their lives." 39 Other workers report differences in time—orientation according to socio—economic subcultural experiences. Gist and Halbert (31) who worked in Australia, Davis (17) in America, and Spinley (72) in England, agree that lower socio—economic class members tend to be preoccupied with the present and its problems and pay relatively little attention to the future. Teahan (76) studied the time—orientation of sixty seventh and eighth grade parochial school boys, thirty of whom were high achievers and thirty were low achievers. He used pictures from the TAT to assess time orientation. His results substantiated his main hypothesis that high achievers are more future time—oriented than low achievers. This c0nclusion was true for a number of other studies relating time—orientation to achievement (15,32,4l,47). Israeli (38) in a study concerned with the feelings of undergraduate students about the various time—orientations, found that while 13 percent of the students worried about their past, 94 percent were hopeful about the future. He used what now appears to be a rather naive questionnaire. He simply has his subjects fill out a questionnaire with statements such as ”Is the past more important than the present?“I "Is the future more important than the past?" and so on through the various combinations of past, present and future. He also developed the future-biography tech— nique which asks the subject to write on his personal eXpectations on the future. ‘40 Smeltzer (70) studied time-orientation and time per— pective in psychotherapy. The purpose of his research was 0 determine the changes in time—orientation and future time erspective in the course of psychotherapy. His subjects are nineteen (ten female and nine male) students who had eceived treatment for emotional problems at Michigan State niversity Counseling center. Time—orientation was deter— ined by noting the time reference of each of the verbs used y the subject (past, present, or future). According to his findings, Smeltzer (70) concluded that the entire pattern of decrease in past, increase in present, and increase in future time—orientation may be the important factors in successful psychotherapy. Balken and Masserman (3) and Laffey (50) used the verb tenses in their subjects’ TAT stories to indicate the domi— nant time—orientation. Laffey, however, found that his subjects (male prisoners) tended to use the present tense predominantly even when referring to the past and the future. He explained that the tendency was due to the social class and status of his subjects. This would cause a difficulty i-f the verb tense was used as an indicator to the time reference. Laffey observed that this would probably not be a problem in middle class or college subjects who have been taught "proper" grammar. Eldred et a1. (21), Grummon (35), Iaffe (40) and Weintraub and.Arronson(80) also have used Jerb tenses as an indicator of time reference, in therapy—like and t ing t subje meth< pitf; avoi ers bet sta wri ori int 41 and therapy situations, with success. The method of indicat— ing time-orientation by noting the verb tense used by the subjects in their conversations seems to be a satisfactory method if conversation of subjects is available and if the pitfall that Laffey noted can be taken into consideration to avoid bias due to improper uSe of grammar. From the previous discussion, we perceive that research- ers Who studied time-orientation did not neceSsarily define it as different from time perspective. To differentiate between the two concepts, time-orientation as Wallace (77) stated and as the previous discussion of Kluckhohn's (46), writings on the temporal value-orientation included, time— orientation could refer to the direction in time of the individual's thoughts and feelings—~past, present, and future-~Which directs his actions. It is important if time— orientation or time-perspective is to be studied that the ‘ definition be clear in the researcher's mind. He has to select the apprOpriate definition and methodology for investi— gating any of the two concepts. The great variety of techniques used and discussed above for studying time—orien— tation may raise some questions about the findings-reported. The theoretical and empirical evidence presented sug— gests the importance of the concept of temporal orientation as a determinant of behavior. It also suggests that some individuals are predisposed to "live in the present" and to have either little appreciation for or awareness of the future 42 or the past. Such people may have a proclivity to act with little apparent regard for the consequences of their behavior. Others seem to be able to think ahead before acting and to modify their behavior in terms of possible consequences. People in this group are more concerned with the future as the most important time period. They could be said to have future time—orientation. People who are past time—oriented always seem to glance backwards. They shut themselves and live only with reference to the past, they are tied to the past, and prefer to with— draw into the past. This group of people seems to be unable to meet realities of the present and despairs of the future. To them, the future is an uncertain area of life and hence they rarely look in that direction. They try to relive the past and repeat what they have done in the past. In light of the discussion presented in this chapter, the author hypothesized the following relationships of the homemaker's pattern of resource use and the three-point range variation in her temporal value—orientation. Homemakers who are future time—oriented see an objec- tive and a purpose in what they are doing. Therefore, it seems possible that the performance of activities which involve conservation of the family's available resources imply future time—orientation. More specifically, home— makers who are future time—oriented, are likely to perform activities which tend to conserve the family‘s resources i i t i t i more oriei sis ‘ as a and and) act car: the fut ori 43 more frequently than do both the past and the present time— oriented homemakers. This can be supported by the hypothe— sis that future versus past and present time—oriented people, as a result of their ability to think ahead before acting and to consider the possible consequences of their behavior and/or activity might be expected to be more concerned with activities which involve conservation of resources such as caring for durable goods, since they are interested that these goods continue to fulfill their function in the future. Whereas people who have past or present time— orientation, are less concerned with such activities since present time—oriented people live in the present and have little thought to the consequences of their behavior. Likewise people who have past time—orientation are tied to the past, and may be expected to repeat what they have done in the past. The following diagram shows the possible relationships between the time—orientation of the homemaker and her pattern of resource use. Assumptions and Objectives Underlying the overall design of the study are the following assumptions: 1. The homemaker uses all three time references in normal conversation; that is, her conversation may refer sometime to the past, present and future. Yet, the most frequent time reference shows her time—orientation. 44 DIAGRAM SHOWING POSSIBLE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN THE TIME—ORIENTATION OF THE HOMEMAKER AND HER PATTERN OF RESOURCE USE PATTERN OF RESOURCE USE HIGHER MEAN FREQUENCY/MINUTE ON RESOURCE-CONSERVING ACTIVITIES L FUTURE PRESENT LOWER MEAN FREQUENCY/MINUTE OF RESOURCE—CONSERVING ACTIVITIES PATTERN OF RESOURCE USE 45 2. Values are expressed through and inferred from language;-therefore, they can be identified through conversation. The homemaker has an established pattern of daily family activities which can be inferred from observing her using the available family resources during selected time periods. Sufficient rapport can be established to reduce excessive change of behavior during the presence of the observer . To give direction to the study, three objectives were formulated: 1. N a To identify from observed activities of the home— maker, patterns of resource use involving conserva— tion of resources with respect to use of equipment and materials. To identify the nature of the value orientation of the hOmemaker with respect to the three—point range of time—orientation, whether she is predominantly past, present or future time—oriented. To identify the relationships between the time orientation of the homemaker and her activity pattern of resource use, including conservation of resources. Since the original project, data of which were used in the present study, was not designed to test any hypotheses, no statistical hypotheses were postulated. This area of 46 istudy is exploratory in nature and the research is expected jto generate hypotheses as an outcome. The author, however, Tis raising the following research question regarding the ‘relationship between the time—orientation of the homemaker and her activity pattern: Homemakers who are future time-oriented perform activi— ties which have a resource conserving functionymore frequently than do homemakers who are past and/or present time—oriented. Additional exploratory questions were raised. Fraisse (27), and Frank (29), have noted the possibility that an individual can have more than one time orientation, each applicable to a different aspect of living, or he may develop a well—integrated time orientation applicable to all areas of life. Since this possibility exists, it is necessary to note the content with regard to time references. If differ— (ences do exist, then some indication of these is available. Time orientation, therefore, will be studied in relation to gtwo main content areas developed by the author: person— iversus material—centeredness. The content of the homemaker‘s iconversation could predominantly center around either persons ior materials. If the homemaker is predominantly person— :centered in her conversation, she possibly can be either Eself— or others-centered. "Thus, the analysis of the home— imaker's conversation will be in terms of time—orientation, :as well as the content of the conversation with respect to iself—, others— and materials—centeredness. inte coni the 2‘PP see hYE 47 It may be possible that the homemaker has just one integrated time—orientation with respect to these three content areas. On the other hand it is also possible that the homemaker may have different time-orientations, each applicable to one of the content areas. Therefore, it seems worthwhile to explore this possibility. No specific hypotheses were formulated in this concern since the idea is exploratory in nature. However, it is possible that if time—orientation showed differences when considered under the three content areas, these differences will be reflected in the relation- ship of time-orientation and activity pattern. Consequently, a relationship might be expected between the activity pattern and the three content areas, which could be illus— trated in the following diagram: ACTIVITY PATTERN P TIME-ORIENTATION (PAST, PRESENT, FUTURE) CONTENT AREAS (SELF, OTHERS, MATERIALS) .___ 48 Lennard and Bernstein (52) were found to have developed similar content areas for analyzing the therapist's inter— views with the patients in psychotherapy. Their content areas were: a) Therapist—client relationships, b) Family relationships, c) Other Social relationships, d) Self. The last three content areas which are relevant to the present study were defined below and the statements of the patients were classified according them as follows: Family relationships represented propositions regarding the client's status and interactions in his family of origin and, if he is married, with his present family. In—laws and other relatives were also considered to be a part of this category. Social relationships represented all other social rela- tionships outside the family. Self represented reference to life experiences that did not refer to other content than the person himself. Smeltzer (70) in his study of time—orientation and time perspective adapted the same content areas of Lennard and Bernstein. At the same time his subjects' statements were ‘scored for time-orientation, they were also scored for con— itent according to Lennard and Bernstein's system. i The three content areas of the present study, self, jother, and materials are defined and the coding system is iexplained in the next chapter. i CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY In this chapten, terms are defined and explained. Also included are the following sections: selection of sample; mechanics of contacting families and techniques for obtaining cooperation; the collection, coding and analysis of data. Definitions of Terms For the purpose of this study, the terms were defined in this manner: Temporal value-orientation and/or time—orientation: the direction—-past, present, or future—~of the individual's thoughts and feelings which affects his overt action and is reflected in his conversation. Content areap: the three categories of self, others, and materials, which are expressed in the individual's conversation. Activity: the overt action which a person performs alone or with others. Pattern: a combination of COnsistent, distinctive styles, forms or types of activities. Resources: durable and non—durable goods which are available for the family to act upon to reach desirable ends. 49 50 hey include all material things from pins to refrigerators. .distinctive characteristic of resources is their scarcity. The temn in this study refers only to nonhuman resources. Resource conservation: the act of the preservation of resources from loss, injury, decay, or waste. ) Homemaker: the wife—mother who has the responsibility to manage the household and care for the rest of the family. Selection of Sample: Criteria and Rationale h The data analyzed in this study were collected in a % Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station Project 3029.* E The present study has its origins in the author's experience i in working as a research assistant on that project. The sample of families selected for the study was a non— probability sample. It consisted of thirty rural-non—farm families from an area in the vicinity of Lansing, Michigan. The sample met the following specifications: At the time data were collected, all families had both husband and wife present and one or more children ten years of age or older living at home. This decreased variability in this regard, :hus increasing the homogeneity of the sample. 'The project was designed to investigate relationships between family activities which conserve or augment re— sources and patterns of communication among family members. The project was directed by Dr. F. Magrabi, professor in the department of Family Ecology, and supported by the Agricultural Research Service USDA, Grant No. 12—14—100—9153 (62) administered by the Consumer and Food Economics Research Division, Federal Center Building, Hyattsville, Maryland. 51 The total sample of thirty families selected also reported income below the poverty level five years before the data were collected and half of them reported that their economic position had since improved. Daily activities which tend to conserve the family's available resources can be thought of as being part Of the process by which families may improve their economic position. Definition of poverty used in this study is consistent with that used by Orshansky (66) at low cost levels. Her definition of poverty was based on the income which was less than three times the cost of the low—cost food plan for families of three or more persons. She considered the average income for poor rural non—farm families of different sizes headed by the male as follows: Number of Average Income Family_Members Low Cost Level 3 $3,170 4 $4,010 5 $4,680 6 $5,255 7 or more $6,405 The thirty families of the sample were considered to be at the poverty level 5 years before data were collected. The poverty lines develoPed by Orshansky served to classify a Bureau of the Census population sample as of March 1964 in terms of 1963 money income. Data collected for the present study started Fall 1967 and lasted for two years. Average incomes in families of different sizes were: 52 NUmber of Number of Average Family Member* Families Income* 3 4 $3,412 4 3 $3,365 5 5** $3,936 6 5** $5,349 7 or more 11 $5,247 8 It may be observed that some of the averages of income for the families are slightly higher than those considered by Orshansky. Michigan prices and incomes may be somewhat higher than other states. Therefore, Orshansky's definition was considered to be too restrictive for the state of Michigan. Rural non—farm families were studied because rural America affords a home for a substantial number of the nation's poor. Rural non—farm residents outnumber those of farms by about four to one (2). Because of the serious problems in the rural non-farm population, focusing research on this group was important. Mechanics of Contacting Families and Techniques for Obtaining Cooperation Interviewers, all graduate students, canvased the three- county area surrounding Lansing (Ingham, Eaton, Clinton) to contact families and interview homemakers to identify the families which would fit the requirements for inclusion in T *These figures are five years before data were collected. **One family is excluded because of lack of data on income. 53 the sample for the study. Names and addresses of families were obtained in different ways, e.g., by asking informants in the community such as the editor of local newspaper or others who knew about the community for names and addresses of families who met the criteria, and by receiving sugges— tions from some of the visited families. Without any earlier appointments, the interviewer stopped by the residence where the family lived. She intro— duced herself to the homemaker, and explained that the pur— pose of the study was to help families by providing teachers with information about how different homemakers perform' household activities. She handed the homemaker a letter of introduction to read in order to confirm the validity of the interviewer's presence. (See Appendix A, page 176 for letter of introduction.) If the homemaker agreed to cooperate, the interviewer immediately obtained basic information from her about the family on a two—page interview form. The basic information was necessary to check whether the family met the required specification of the study and to promote future interest, for the sharing of information with the interviewer led to a sort of commitment from the side of the homemaker. At this point, no future date was set for collection of data and this omission was later judged to be a weakness, for the time was ripest when cooperation was elicited and the com— mitment was made. However, when the respondents were 54 contacted 2 to 6 months later, many intervening family disturbances made it impossible for some families to help with the project, e.g., husbands“ attitudes toward the study, personal illness, children's schedules. The two—page form of the initial interview is included in Appendix B, pages 178—180 (interview fOrm I). One of the questions included in this form was ”How well off are you compared with five years ago as far as your net worth is concerned? a) Better off b) Worse off c) About the same“ According to the homemaker's answer and how she per- ceived her family's economic position, the family was classified as either better off or not better off. About 70 of the families who were first contacted met the criteria required for the study and were again approached several months after the initial interview. As the inter— viewer did the first time, she introduced herself, showed letter of introduction, and, if asked explained that the study was to obtain first—hand information about families which was possible only by visiting in homes and seeing how homemaking activities; were performed. The objective was to collect information which would help other families, not to inspect, judge or criticize. The homemaker was told what information was sought, how much time would be involved and what inconvenience would be experienced, if any. Assurances were made that all information obtained would be confidential d n 5 AV r a C 0 c w a m 55 and not be identified with one's name but would be computed as part of the study. If the respondent appeared to hesitate in agreeing to cooperate, the interviewer tried to keep her talking in order to obtain her confidence, to make her feel that her cooperation was needed, and to assure that her contribution would be significant. If the homemaker agreed to cooperate, a time was set to begin collection of data. Efforts were made to fit into the homemaker's schedule and to make her the interviewer's first consideration. The interviewer became sensitive to each homemaker's moods and feelings and uncomplainingly accepted unexpected schedule changes and delays. If the homemaker decided to withdraw from the study at any time, the interviewer respected this wish and thanked her for allowing the interviewer to spend some time in her home. Some of the 70 families who permitted the initial inter— view and who met the required criteria, refused when con— ,tacted again, to permit additional interviews and observations. Data were collected from the rest of the families, and addi— ,tional families were contacted until complete data were ,obtained from 32 families out of which the 30 families in— tcluded in the study were selected on the basis of having more ‘complete data. r i esta? bsfo fict @ 56 Collection of Data Included in this section is discussion of means of establishing rapport with families, practice sessions before actual collection of data, data collection, and dif— ficulties encountered in collecting the data. Means of establishing rapport It was important that efforts be made to establish rapport with families if they were to be observed while performing their normal daily activities. This would help families behave in the normal way without excessive altera— tion due to the observer's presence. The previous tech— niques for obtaining cooperation and confidence of the homemaker helped to accomplish the establishment of rapport, besides the several preliminary visits and interviews. Practice sessions Some trial observations in two outside families were carried out before the actual research data were collected, for the purpose of providing practice in observing and recording activities. These practice sessions were helpful in familiarizing the interviewers with the equipment used, e-g., clipboard and pencil for writing down activities iobserved, tape recorder for recording the conversation among family members. gafiifrf 74.? a ..r . _ ‘_~ =é—l—‘w V iri¥xpfm,u1 1: 57 Data collection Five trained graduate students collected the data over a two—year period. Data collected consisted of the follow- ing: I. Interview schedules. Interview appointments were made with the homemaker of each family by one of the graduate students in order to get background data from the homemaker concerning biographical information and information about household routine and equipment. While filling out the forms with the needed infbrmation, the interviewer tried to create a friendly atmosphere so that the homemaker would not feel embarrassed. The four interview forms are included in Appendix B, pages 181—207 (Interview Forms II, III, IV and V). Interview form II included questions which elicited information about family composition, educational and occupa— tional status of both homemaker and husband, the different communities in which the family lived for 6 months or more, some of the hobbies and activities in which the family members participated, and the physical condition and the general health of the homemaker. Questions included in interview form III concerned information about buying and use of different types of food and planning meals before food shopping. In the last few pages of the form an equip— ment checklist was filled out including major appliances, small appliances, utensils, etc. Interview form IV was filled out by the interviewer after the visits were completed. 58 The interviewer rated the condition of home as she saw it according to 5 factors——cleanliness, state of repair of building and permanent fixtures, state of repair of fur- nishings and equipment, and shabbiness. She used the l to 5 scale for rating, with 1 indicating extremely poor and 5 indicating extremely good. Economic information was elicited by questions in inter— view form V. Such questions as numbers of sources of income in the family, jobs held by husband and homemaker or any other family member during the past five years prior to the interview, as well as income associated with each job were included. Other incomes received from any other source, e.g., bonus, overtime, or tips were also listed. Other questions elicited information about whether the family owed money on any loans or mortgages, the assets and savings of the family and whether the home was owned or rented. II. Activity records. 'The observational teChnique was utilized for collecting data on activities. Like any other research method, the observation technique has some problems encountered in using it. A major problem that must be taken into consideration is that the presence of the Observer usually changes the situation being observed. This prOblem, however, can be reduced if sufficient rapport is developed by visiting the family a number of times before recording any actual observational data. This may help the family members become accustomed to the presence of the Observer, 59 and presumably behave normally when the data are actually collected. The main concern was that the Observer's presence would not cause excessive alteration of the homemakers' pattern of resource use. It was assumed in this paper that the procedures stated earlier, which were carried out for establishing rapport, were sufficient to accustom the family members to the presence of the observer, and did not them— selves change the behavior of the family. Another problem encountered in planning and carrying out observational studies as Stated by Borg (8) is obtaining both objective and valid information from the Observation. Usually in the process of Objectification there is some loss of validity. This means that the experimenter has the altern— ative that he can either objeCtively observe, in which case -his observation will be slightly related to the complex behavior he wishes to study, in other words the data will be less valid; or he can get data more closely related to the complex behavior he is studying but will find it of more limited value because of its subjectivity. The homemaker in each family was observed While perform— ing the normal daily activities by the same graduate student who did the interview. If any other person participated in or performed any activity, it was also reported, but the home— maker was the primary focus of the observation. The following data were recorded minute by minute by the observer for each sperson present during the observation: activity in which engaged, person or materials acted upon, area of the home, and other equipmental materials used. For recording activi- ties the observer had a clipboard and a pencil for writing down activities as she was observing. She had also a stop watch by means of which records of activities were divided into one minute segments while she was observing. Forms for recording observations are presented in Appendix B, page 208 (activity record sheet). Observers were instructed to give special attention to activities which have implications for resource conservation, e.g., turning lights or water on and off, storage of materials and the like. The homemaker as a person performing the activity received primary attention of the observer. Some homemakers spent some of the observation time visiting with neighbors or watching TV which was probably what they would normally have done at that time, but this did not provide much data about resource conserving activities. The observations were performed at times convenient to the family and were usually arranged in advance. The fre- quency distribution of the number of minutes observed was as follows: Time in Minutes Number of Families Less than 200 1 201—250 15 251-300 11 Over 300 3 30 The aggregate time period ranged from 156 to 350 minutes with an average of 255 minutes. In one instance the entire period 61 of observation, which was four hours, was done in one day. But in other families the observations were dOne on 1, 2, 3 or 4 different days with varying intervals between and with varying number of minutes in each observation. No attempt was made to specify a uniform time of the day or a uniform number of minutes for observing each family. Instead, arrangements with homemakers were made to allow for observations in which other family members, particularly the children, were at home engaged in activity in the same area as the wife—mother, and a variety of major household activi— ties were taking place, namely food preparation, cleaning, and laundry. This seemed reasonable, not only because the flexibility made it easier to obtain cooperation from families, but because it placed the emphasis on obtaining data about a variety of typical activities. The daily and weekly schedules for performing activities varied from house— hold to household, hence observing at the same times of the day and week in each family might have resulted in less c0mplete coverage of activities than the plan actually followed. The actual activities observed included food preparation, cleaning, laundry, ironing, tidying, eating, resting and watching TV, and caring for children. These activities were performed by nearly all families. However, the frequency of the performance of an activity varied from family to family. act bat th FT).— 62 III. Verbal conversation. The conversation going on in each family during the observation period and while the activity was being performed was tape recorded by using a battery-powered tape recorder supported by a strap from the observer's shoulder. The equipment allowed the observer to be very mobile. It meant that the homemaker could be followed throughout the house as she engaged in the different activities. Problemsand difficulties encountered in collectinggdata In the course of data collection, there were some prob— lems and difficulties encountered. Occasionally the equipment did not function properly. It was disappointing to both the homemaker and the observer to begin observation and discover it could not be completed because of improperly working equipment or because the observer had forgotten some needed materials. In some cases the tape ran out while the observer was busy writing the activities she was observing. In such instances the observer tried to exclude that part of the observation when the tape ran out and set a new time for another observation. Sometimes the quality of the conversation recorded was impaired by a loud radio or TV or by the sound of the vacuum cleaner While the mother was using it. This resulted in recordings of poor quality, which were difficult to trans— cribe. Another difficulty encountered was that after setting 63 i i appointments with some homemakers for data collection, ‘interviewers were faced with schedule changes and delays which were not accounted for and which each interviewer iuncomplainingly accepted. In some cases some homemakers iwithdrew from the study after part of the data was collected. , Coding of Data In this section the coding system used for classifying activities and materials in order to determine the pattern of activity and/or resource use of the homemaker and pro— cedures for coding conversation to determine the time— orientation of the homemaker will be explained. Data on activity records and conversation were coded by different persons from those who collected them. Coding activity records Data concerning activities observed while being per- formed were coded by the writer and another graduate student. With each activity were coded the participants in the activ— ity, the equipment and materials or persons acted upon, other materials used, area of the home, and time of occurrence. The activity record code'sheet as well as the activity record code form are found in Appendix C, pages 210,211. The activity record code sheet contains activities, materials, and space codes. Each of the three categories will be explained. 64 Activity code. Activity categories for coding were developed to satisfy the following requirements: a) that the categories distinguish resource conserving activities from thOSe that utilize materials with the ultimate purpose of consumption, b) that the categories be independent of materials utilized in the activity. The classification system developed, was not a comprehensive one in the sense that it included only home activities. The classification system distinguished four categories of activities which were considered to serve a resource— conserving function: Cleaning: included activities such as removing dirt or waste, sweeping, etc. Applying protective materials: included such activities as floor waxing, placing hot pad under pans, etc. Conditioning—for—storage: included activities such as wrapping food before storage, folding clothes, unplugging equipment, etc. Moving—to—storage: included such activities as putting cans away in cabinet, turning off water, lights or stove, etc. The previous four categories of activities seemed to satisfy the definition of resource conservation. Each activ— ity in one way or another involves preservation of resources either from loss, decay or waste. The following three categories of activities were used in distinguishing activities which had consumption as an ultimate purpose: ym‘r' 65 Moving—from—storage: included such activities as taking things from storage place to place of use, turning on water, lights or stove, etc. Conditioning—for—use: included such activities as laundry, ironing, plugging in equipment, etc. Service: this category included activities such as eating, feeding a child, smoking, etc. Activities coded under service were service to self or to others. The previous three types of activities each seemed to use up some kind of resource. Therefore, they can be com— bined under one single category knOWn as consumption activi— ties. Two categories of activities——preparation and con— struction——seem to result in an increase in the family's resources and the creation of new resources from available ones. Household production, e.g., activities involving fOOd preparation from ingredients or construction of cloth— ing from yard goods, could be termed as activities which augment resources or create new resources from available ones. These two types of activities were: Preparation or construction from basic materials: included such activities as food preparation from raw 1n- gredients or construction of clothing from yard goods, etc. Preparation with partially prepared materials: similar, to the previous category except that partially prepared ma- terials, e.g., preparing cake from cake mix, etc., are used. 66 Control: One additional category, control, was used to classify activities which appeared to be purposeful but did not serve the immediate functions either of consumption or of resource conservation. This included activities such as moving materials from one location to another, checking on food being prepared or checking on children or mail, looking in storage areas. Such activities were known as control activities. Material code. Materials commonly used in households were classified into categories which were roughly similar in terms of function, Operation, and/or structure. They were grouped in 14 categories. Built-in or large equipment: such as refrigerator, stove, sink, etc. Portable equipment: such as vacuum cleaner, stand mixer or hand mixer, etc. Mixing or measuring utensils: tools, dishes, pans, ironing board, ash tray, etc. Ingredients: such as any food substance, etc. House supplies: such as paper towels, water, electric- ity, gas, etc. Containers: such as rack, suit case, waste can, hamper, etc. Furniture: such as tables, chairs, dresser, etc. Bedding and linen: such as table cloth towels, pillow cases, blanket, etc. 67 Other household textiles: such as curtains, rugs, pillows, etc. House structure and structure storage: such as doors, windows, walls, built—in cabinets, etc. Clothes: included all garments, shoes and accessories, etc. Personal supplies: such as hand lotion, hair brush, cigarettes, toys, etc. Miscellaneous: included letters, scissors, sewing supplies, etc. One final code was used for any type of material that was unknown. Space code. Different codes were assigned to different areas of the home (see Appendix C, page 211, activity record code form). Examples of coding activity records: (In'Minute 1 the following activities were recorded as performed in the kitchen: "peeling potato at sink, stove turn on, add salt and pepper"; in Minute 2 the following activities were re— corded in the kitchen also: ”Iron shirt sleeve, go to stove, turn potatos with spatula." Coding was as follows (see the following page). 8 6 m>oum Aemv o>oum you shamans looao no mom Ammv com tcm . manpmmm AMNV toom Aamv Hoxmsoaon Aomv .mmum poom AHNNV coroner AHHHV N oumon anemone Ammo Acouflv on: Suaofluuooao Ammv nonpoHo Aaov Hoxmewaor Aomv [Howl.tcou Aammv conouex AHHHV m can Ammo coon lems umxmsmsos lems .mmum eooo AHNNV assures lease a huflofluuomao ommmoum o>oum lems so was lame Harnesses lems usonwimce>os Amemv scrapes lease a scam lems com com omecx Ammv poom Aamv Hormeoson Aomv .moum coom Aammv coflouflx AHHHV H mauoouep, mcwuom mamwuoums Honuo com: pouum GOmHom mua>flpom oommm ouscflz Hmeuoums 69 The numbers between brackets represent the apprOpriate code number according the code sheet in the Appendix. Every activity coded was considered a separate codable activity. There was always one material coded for the material acted upon directly, while there was one, or more or no material coded for other materials. If any two materials were acted upon and both had the same code number they were coded as one material, such as knife and pan in the previous example. If any activity started in Minute-l for instance and was still going on in Minute 2, it was coded in Minute 1 as well as in Minute 2 and was counted as two separate codable activities (see activity record cede sheet and aetivity record code form in Appendix C, pages 210, 211). Although the observers were instructed to pay special attention to activities which involve conservation of resources, the categories for coding activities and materials were not developed until after the data collection was com— plete. No actual coding of data was done until the two coders through some training sessions reached 75 percent agreement. This was important to insure some degree of reli— ability in the coding. Intercoder agreement on activity coding. Percentage agreement between coders was continually calculated to assure reliability of the coding system. Segments of activities coded by one coder were randomly selected for a period of 10 minutes and coded by the other coder. If the item checked __,_7 70 was coded by each coder and in the same way, it was con— sidered to be an instance of agreement. Intercoder agreement was calculated as follows: percent intercoder agreement on activity = number of agreements between the two coders number of disagreements + number of agreements between coders x 100 The previous formula was applied for checking percent agree— ment for each of the dimensions coded: activity, material acted upon, other materials used, and space. The averages of percentages agreement, respectively, were 79, 83.5, 73.2, and 92.1. As each dimension was checked for agreement separately, agreement on all dimensions was also checked; that is, whether all categories were coded by the two coders and in the same way. Perfect agreement for all categories averaged 65.8 percent. All the previous figures were averages of per— centage agreement done on samples from thirteen families. Coding conversation Before coding the taped—recorded conversations the data were transcribed from the tapes to coding sheets by members of the research team, some of whom had collected the data. In the communication transcriptions, the person speaking and the person or persons spoken to, were identified if possible. After tapes were transcribed they were replayed and by using 71 a stop watch the conversation was divided into one minute segments. The activities record was used sometimes as a check on the timing of conversation (see conversation code sheet in Appendix C, page 212). Some of the recorded tapes were of good quality and much of the conversation could be transcribed easily from them. Conversation in other tapes was not clear, sometimes because of a sound of loud TV or radio. Thus the conversa— tion could hardly be heard and transcribed. Attempts were made to transcribe these tapes more than once to get all that could be heard as accurately as possible, however, all tapes included portions which could not be transcribed or coded. Most transcribed conversations were checked against tapes by a person other than the one who transcribed it and any missing conversation that could be understood and was not transcribed was added. After all tapes were transcribed and checked, the transcribed data were divided into small message units for purpose of coding and analysis by means of a slash mark. The message unit was defined as a word or a group of words stating or expressing an independent idea and usually containing a subject and predicate. The message unit could consist of one word, e.g., "look," ”No," or several words, e.g., "I‘ll spank you if you do that.” The following are additional examples: This is a conversation between the mother and the child. 72 Mother: "School bus was late today/Wasn't it?/" Child: "Yea"/ Mother: "Look"/ Child: "What?"/ Mother: I'Do I have some stars?"/ Child: "Look/picturesf/ The data were then typewritten and the typewritten data were proofread against the original data. All conversation message units were coded in terms of the time reference each message unit referred to——past, present, or future, and in terms of the content of the message unit--self, others or materials. Only the homemaker's conversation was analyzed in the present study regardless of to whom she was directing the conversation. Coding was done by the writer on the same typewritten forms which are found in Appendix C, page 212. Time-orientation. The method of assessing the time orientation by noting the verb tense used by the homemaker was used. However, due to the possible colloquial usage of the subjects of this study and the possibility that a wrong present tense for example could be used by the homemaker when she was referring to or intending the future, the main verb of each message unit was coded in the time reference which fippeared to be intended by the homemaker. In order to determine the time reference intended, the following were the criteria: Message units about habitual things "I used to" or re— ferring to things happened or gone in the past, or words 73' like yesterday, two weeks ago, were considered past refer— ence intended. Message units about what is felt, or when the home— maker orders a child to do something, the time reference intended was the present. Message units about the future state of affairs, pre- dictions or expectations in the future like ”I hope,” conditional statements such as "If you . . . I will . . .“ or "When . . . then...," all words like tomorrow, next week, were presumed to be intended as future reference. Some conditional message units were used in the context of the past. In such a case they were coded past reference, e.g., "You should have done so and so.” Each of the message units was coded according to the time reference intended of the main verb in the message unit using the appropriate code. In many cases the tense of the main verb and the time reference intended of the main verb of the message unit were the same. However, in cases where they are not in agreement, the time reference intended was coded. Examples of coding time reference intended: "As I look back I think_of the happy days” (past time reference). ”If you do that again, I spank you.” (future reference intended) "You should have been there." (past reference intended) 74 "We hope we will make it." (future reference intended) "As I look back I remember the old days." (past reference intended) "When I was a child I g§g§_to spend a lot of time playing." (verb and reference intended past) ”Shut the door." (verb and reference intended present) A message unit was considered unclassified and coded as "9" if it did not include a verb to determine the time reference intended. The following and similar message units were coded as ”9": ”Allright," "Yes," ”OK," "No.” Some homemakers used the expression "You know" frequently in their conversation, such an expression was coded as "9” also. Percentages of time reference for each homemaker was calcu- lated in the following manner: A frequency count of all message units of the homemaker with past reference . percent past reference = Total number of all message units (of the homemaker) coded for the 3 time references x 100 Present and future reference percentages were computed in a similar manner. Content. Each message unit which was coded for time reference was also coded for content, whether the message unit centered around self, others or materials. Coding message unit for content was according to the following definitions: 75 Self: message units referring to self or self experi— ences were coded under self category. Others: message units referring to any other person than the self, e.g., relatives, friends, boss, etc. are . coded under this category. Materials: message units referring to any material good of the household were coded under this category. Message units which did not fit in one of the previous areas were considered unclassified and coded as "9". In cases when two content categories were in the homemaker's message unit, the dominant category was coded. Which cate- gory was dominant was judged by the structure of the sentence with the subject given first priority. Examples: "I forgot to do the dishes." (Self, verb and reference intended past) In a message unit where the subject was only implied the rest of the message was coded. Example: "Take the ball.“ (Materials, present reference intended) In cases when the subject is implied and there was more than one content cate— gories, only one category is coded, with "Self" given first priority, followed by "others." Example: "Give m§_the book.‘ (Self, verb and time reference intended present) Shown on the next page are the 9 possible time-content categories resulting from coding each message unit with regard to the time reference intended and the content. Message units which were coded for the time reference but were unclassified and coded as "9" with respect to content were left out and not analyzed, and vice versa. 76 Time Reference Past Present Future Self p C o Other 4J C. 8 Materials Intercoder agreement on time reference and content. Intercoder reliability was checked with the assistance of an independent coder. The coding procedures were explained to the second coder (a graduate student) and the terms were defined and discussed. Percentage agreement between the two coders on time reference and content was calculated on randOmly selected segments of approximately 5 minutes. The same formula used for calculating percent of agreement on activities was used. The average percentage agreement done for 10 families for time reference and content was 87 and 88, respectively. Analysis of Data Activity data. The sum of frequencies was found for each activity performed in relation to area of the home where the activity took place, person performing the act1V1ty, material acted upon as the central focus of the activity, and other materials used in the activity. The frequencies of the 77 appropriate categories of activities performed by the home— maker were combined to make up more general categories: resource conserving activities (RCA), activities with ulti- mate purpose of consumption (CA), and activities which aug— ment or create new resources (Aug. Act.). Since the length of time covered in the observations varied from family to family, frequencies were converted to mean frequencies/minute for each homemaker. Obviously, homemakers who were observed for a longer period of time had more opportunity to perform any type of activity, simply because they were observed longer than others. Hence, using the mean frequency/minute seemed to be a fairer measure for comparison among homemakers. Mean frequency/minute of any activity performed by the homemaker was computed as follows: Sum of frequencies of formed b Mean frequency/minute of RCA RCA per Y = homemaker performed by the homemaker Number of minutes of the observation time of the homemaker Mean frequency/minute was computed for each individual activ— ity performed by the homemaker and for each of the combined categories known as RCA, CA and Aug.Acts. Mean frequencies/ minute were used as indicators of the activity pattern of the homemaker. Conversation data: A frequency count of each home— maker's message units of her conversation with respect to time reference categories, content categories and the 9 pos— sible content—time categories was done. The Chi Square Test 78 of Independence was done in order to test for relationships between content and time categories in order to determine whether the categories were dependent or independent of one another. As mentioned earlier, some of the recording tapes were hard to transcribe, and hence not all the conversations on them were transcribed. Therefore, the percent of each cate— gory in relation to the total number of message units spoken by the homemaker and transcribed was calculated to give a fair measure to compare homemakers in terms of the different time content categories. Mean frequency/minute would not be a very appropriate measure since the amount of time could not be estimated accurately, because some time counted was not transcribed owing to the poor quality of some recordings. A frequency count of message units spoken by the homemaker that were coded in the past time reference Total number of message units spoken by the homemaker in the three time references Percent time reference = A frequency count of mes- sage units spoken by the homemaker that were coded in the self content category Total number of message units spoken by the home— maker in the three content areas Percent of self content category = Percentage time references were used as variables indicating the time-orientation of the homemaker. 79 Relationships. Correlation,coefficients were calcu- lated to determine the relationship between the time— orientation of the homemaker and her activity pattern, and to detect any other relationships of different types of activities and the 9 content-time categories (85). CHAPTER IV CHARACTERISTICS OF THE SAMPLE A general description of the thirty families which com— prised the sample of the study is presented in this chapter, including selected aspects of family characteristics. Whenever data are available, the family's situation will be compared at the time data were collected and five years before that. Tables of means, ranges, and percentages accom— PanY the discussion. Sizeand Composition of the Family The number of family members living at home in the house— hold is shown in Table l. The families varied in size from four to fifteen persons living at home at the time data were collected, with an average of 7.0. Five years before that time, the average number of persons living at home was 6.1 persons, while the size of the family ranged from three to eleven persons. Over a five year period these families had slightly increased in number of members living at home. The smallest family at the time data were collected had two children, while the largest family had thirteen children living at hmme. In contrast, five years before the data were collected, there were one child and nine children at 80 81 Table 1. Number of family members living at home five years before, and at the time, data were collected. _ - , Number of Families Size Range 5 years before data At the time data were collected were collected 3-5 13 9 6-8 12 15 9-11 5 4 12—14 —- l 15 —— " 1 Total 30 30 Mean number of family members 6.1 7.0 82 home in the smallest and largest families, respectively. The majority of families (about two—thirds) had both pre- school and school age children. The ages of the children living at hoquat the time data were collected, ranged from under two years to over nineteen years. Distribution of family members in the sample according to age and family composition is presented in Table 2. The first three age groups which include from O to 18 years old, all represent children. However, the fourth age group-— nineteen years old and over—~represents primarily the parents. Also, any nineteen or more year old child or family relative living with the family is included in this~category. Only two households included a person outside the nuclear family-—the father-in—law in one family, and the sister—in— law in the other. In at least one other family, a non— relative was living in the household. As Table 2 indicates, most of the children in the sample of the thirty families are between seven and twelve years of age. The majority of the homemakers and their husbands were between the ages of thirty and forty With an average of 36 and 39, respectively (Table 3)- FOrmal Education of Husbands and Wives The highest schooling grade completed by both the home— makers and their husbands is presented in Table 4. The range of the educational level for the sample was wider for 83 Table 2. Distribution of family members by age, in thirty families, at the time data were collected. Age Groups Item 0-6 7—12 13—18 19+ Total Total number of members in each age group 35 63 37 74 209 Average number of members in each group per family 1.2 2.1 1.2 2.5 7.0 Percent composition by age group 16.7 30.2 17.7 35.4 100.0 84 Table 3. Ages of husbands and wives of thirty families. Age range Number of husbands . Numberlof wives 29 or less 30-35 36—40 41-45 46e50 51-55 ‘Over 55 5 10 Total Mean age 30 38.9 30 36.1 85 Table 4. Educational level of husbands and wives of thirty families. Educational level Number of hubbands Number of wives Less than 8th gradb 2 —— 8th grade 4 4 Some high school 11 6 High sdhool graduate 8 19 Technical or business school 1 1 Some college 3 —— College degree 1 —— 86 the husbands than for the wives. Two husbands had less than 8th grade education, but no wife had that little. Conversely, three husbands had some college education and one had a college degree, while no wife had any college education at all. Even though the range of educational level was wider for the husbands than for the Wives, as a group the husbands seem to have more formal education. Occupation of Husbands and Wives The occupation of both husbands and wives is shown in Table 5. The highest number of the husbands was in laboring occupations, while the majority of wives were full—time home— makers. Only eight wives were employed outside the home, the majority of whom were doing clerical or sales type of work. Only one husband in the whole sample who had the college degree was in the professional class of occupations, he was a civil engineer. Income of Families Review of the income data indicates that incomes at the time data were collected, and five years earlier ranged from $6,000 to approximately $19,000 and from $1,950 to 7,000, respectively. Income distribution for the sample of thirty families is shown in Table 6. The majority of families received income between $3,000 and $5,999 annually five 87 Table 5. Occupational level of husbands and wives of thirty families. fir Occupation Number of husbands Number of wives Professional 1 -— Clerical or sales 1 4 Labor 20 1 Farm labor 2 1 Service 5 2 Hememaker - 22 Doesn‘t apply 1 -— Total 30 3O 88 Table 6. Income distribution of thirty families. Number of familiesw Income range in At the time data 5 years before data dollars were collected were collected Below $2,999 —— 2 3,000-5,999 l 24 6,000-9,999 23 . 2 10,000-13,999 5 —— Over 14,000 1 —— Total 30 28* Mean income $8,148** $4,581* Per capita income I,164** 746 * l-i *Income data on two families are missing. **These figures exclude one family whose present income is 818,928/year, 420% increase over five year period. 89 years before data were collected, whereas almost the same majority figure of families received from $6,000 to $9,999/ year at the time data were collected. Although two families had income below $2,999 five years earlier, no family was receiving that small an incOme after five years. One family was actually receiving $18,928 annually, an increase of 420 percent over the five year period. Interesting to note, however, is that the husband of that family receiving that amount was not the one in the professional occupation, but rather he was working in farm labor and his highest grade of schooling was teChnicaI or business school. This figure $18,928 was excluded in computing the average income per family to avoid its excessive effect on the mean. The average income per family has increased over the five year period from $4{581 to $8,148. Sources of Income in The Family Presented in Table 7, is the number of sources of in— come in the sample of thirty families. Any person living in the household and contributing to its income was con— sidered as a source of income in that household. For example, in a family Where the father, mother, daughter, and son each was working even part time and earning money, four sources of income were counted for that family. Also any income coming from bonds, dividends or interest on saving was a source of income. Most of the sons who were working had ‘E-HI 90 Table 7. Number of sources of income in thirty families. Number of sources Number of of income Families l.source 9 2 sources 10 3 sources 6 4 sources 5 Total 30 Mean number of sources of income 2.2 91 only part time jobs during the summer vacation. Daughters who were considered sources of income were mostly baby sitters. As Table 7 indiCates, the majority of families derived income from one or two sources, while six of the families had three sources and five had four sources. The average number of sources of income per family‘for the total sample, however, was 2.2. The husband was the primary source of income in all families. What is important, it seems, is the amount of dollars each source adds to the family's income rather than the number of sources of income in the family. For example, the family Who was receiving the high— est income of $18,928 per year had it only from one source, that was the.husband, while themajority of families who were getting the least annual incomes, had them from two sources. More interesting to note, however, is that among families who received the least annual income, two families derived their income from four sources. One would conclude that more family members are employed in low income families than in high income ones because of economic pressure. Amount of Money Owed by and to the Family While four families had no debt (Table 8), the amount of money owed by two families exceeded $10,000 in an actual figure of $12,500 and $16,760.. Most of the families' debt was due primarily to home mortgages since a large number of the families were buying their homes. 92 Table 8. Amount of dollars owed by and to thirty families. ,— Amount of dollars owed Number of families by or to the family . Owing money Owed Money 0 4 25 Less than $999 8 2 l,000—3,999 9 - 4,000-6,999 1 l 7,000—9,999 6 2 10,000 and over A ' 2 — _——------~-_—-F_---—-—‘__—-—————_———_-——-—-———_---—--——_—- Total 30 30 93 The majority of families-~25 of them-—had no money owed to them. Two families had $400 and $30 owed to them while" three families had the amount of money ranging from $4,500 to $7,500 owed to them. Home Ownership Only one family in the whole sample was renting their home, while the majority of families were owners of their homes, although some of these families still bad debt on their homes (Table 9). Three families in the group had rent free homes from the husbandsr employers. Two of these husbands were doing farm labor while one was employed as laborer. Mobility of Families in the Last Five Years The homemakers of the thirty families were asked to list the different communities where they lived at least for six months or more during the previous five years. Table 10 shows the number of moves in different communities. It may be observed from the table that the majority of families were settled in their present Community for at least five years. Six families, however, reported that they moved once, twihe, and four times in different EOmmunities over the preceding five years, Where they stayed six months or more in each community. One homemaker reported seven moves of her family in seven different communities in the last five years. 94 Table 9. Home ownership of thirty families. Home ownership 9 Number of families' Renters ‘ 1 Owners, still paying 11 Owners, fully paid 14 Rent free from employer 3 Total 29* *Data were missing for one family. 95’ Table 10. Number of moves in the last five years by thirty families. Number of moves in different communities . Number.of.fami1ies None 23 1 move 3 2 moves 2 4 moves 1 7 moves 1 96 Meal Planning Before Shopping The homemaker in each family was asked whether She plans-her meals before she goes to buy the food (Table 11). While equal numbers of homemakers responded that they always or sometimes plan meals before shopping, the majority of the homemakers indicated that they never do any meal planning before buying food. ,It seems possible, however, that each of these homemakers perceived the questionof meal planning differently. One homemaker could have perceived planning in terms of dollars, while another homemaker thought of planning in terms of nutrition. This difference might have affected the responses, although the overall concept of planning was evident. Equipment and Facilities Available for the Family Selected equipment and facilities available for families were placed in two categories. As Table 12 shows, they were categorized under major applicances and small appliances. With respect to the major appliances, the majority of families seem to have them available in their homes. Although all the homes had heating systems, hot water heaters, stoves, and refrigerators, these facilities and equipment varied in type Of fuel used. All thirty families had central heating sys— tems, nine of which were operated with oil, five with wood and coal, six with gas, nine with electricity; and two 97 Table 11. Meal planning before food shopping. Meal planning Number of homemakers Always 8 Sometimes 8 Never 14 ————————-———-_——————————-—————_————————————————_-—_————————— 98 Table 12. Types of equipment and facilities available for thirty families. Equipment and facilities Number of families Major appliances: Television set 30 Heating system 30 Hot water heater 3O Stove 30 Refrigerator 30 Freezer 22 Washing machine 27 Dryer 23 Vacuum cleaner y- 23 Radio 30 Small appliances: Iron 30 Electric frying pan” 21 Mixer 25 Electric knife 8 Blender 4 Popcorn popper 8 Waffle iron 16 Griddle 11 Coffee maker 21 Toaster 29 Roaster 8 Electric can opener 11 Electric fan 24 99 families did not specify the tYpe of fuel. The majority of families——eighteen--had the hot water"'“ heater of the electric type, while the rest of the families had the gas or oil—Operated types. About half of the stoves found in the homes of the thirty homemakers were gas stoves, and the other half were electric ones. One home- maker reported that their stove needed some repair and was not frequently in use. Only 22 families had freezers in their home, seven of which were the chest type of freezer. There was a washing machine in every house of the thirty families with the exception of three families who did not have one. However, one homemaker reported that their washing machine was broken and another homemaker reported it was not working. These two families therefore, did their laundry outside. The 25 families who had washing machines and in good condition did their laundry at home in different locations, e.g., in the kitchen, utility room, basement, garage, or bathroom. One of the families had the semi—automatic type of washing machine, fourteen had the fully—automatic types and ten had the wringer type of washing machine. Fifteen of the 23 homemakers who reported having dryers did not specify the type they had, while four had the elec= tric type, and three had the gas type. The dryer in one family was reported as broken and not in use; therefore, in this family the clothes were usually dried either on clothes line or in the launderette. 100 The ownership of vacuum cleaners was reported by 23 families, howeven in one familythe vacuum cleaner was not. working and needed repair. The families who did not own vacuum cleaners used various cleaning equipment such as sweepers, brooms, and mops for cleaning. With respect to the small appliances which are listed in Table 12, not all the families had them except the iron and the ironing board which were found in each house. Also all but one family owned a toaster. One family among the 21 who had an electric frying pan reported having two of them, and one other family reported that theirs was not working. Mixers and electric coffee makers were reported to be ‘ owned by the majority of families. The other small appli- ances listed in Table 12 were reported owned only by few families. The ownership of four cars was reported by one family, but only two of these cars were in running condition. Two of the families who reported the ownership of one car admitted that their car was not running, while three of the families who reported having two cars admitted that only one was in running condition. One of the two families who had three cars reported that two of them were not running, and' the other family had only one car in running condition. 101 The families who reported having one car were the families receiving the higher incomes, while those families owning two or more cars were the families with the lower incomes. Practically speaking, it is the cars that were in running condition which could only be considered. Looking at the sample as a whole, the majority of families had one car in running conditiOn. Exactly half of the families reported the ownership of other vehicles, besides cars, owned and used by the family. Among such vehicles were bikes mainly used by the children, tractors, trucks, and trailers. Condition of the Home Table 13 summarizes rating on home condition. In the majority of cases those homes rated extremely poor or good on one variable were alsO‘rated the same on the other vari— ables. There were seven homes which were rated extremely good on all characteristics, in three of which the wife was, full-time homemaker, and in the remaining four families the wife was employed. The number of children in these seven families ranged from three to seven with an average of 6.2 children. One would have expected that when the wife is full—time homemaker and there are smaller number of children in the family the home would be rated high with respect to cleanli— ness, orderliness, etc. However the rating results do not 102 Table 13. Rating of thirty families on home condition. with respect to five variables. Number of families Home condition Extremely Extremely Total* poor 2 3 4 good Cleanliness —— V 4 7 9 8 28 Orderliness l 5 4 l4 4 28 State of repair of building and permanent fix— tures l 4 12 4 7 28 State of repair of furnishings and equipment 1 4 10 9 4 28 Shabbiness 2 6 9 7 4 28 *Two families have missing data on rating. 103 support this conclusion. It is possible that there may be other factors involved such as age of Children and time devoted for cleaning and tidying. Summary All families were in early stages of the family life cycle. At the time of the study, the homemakers of these families took care of a varying number of children ranging from two to thirteen. Most of the children were between seven and twelve years of age. The homemakers and their husbands were comparatively young with an average age Of 37.5 years. Both husbands and wives could be considered to have little schooling with the exception of four husbands, three of whom had some college and one with a college degree. The majority of both hus- bands and wives are high school graduates. Most of the wives were full—time homemakers and almost all of the hus— bands with the exception of one who had a professional job, had unskilled types of employment. Five-years before data were collected these families were considered below the poverty level with respect to their incomes. However; five years later and at the time of the study the average percentage increase in their incomes reached approximately 94 percent. The majority of families generally derived their income from more than one source; however, the husband was a primary source of income in all families. 104 Each of these families had several means of getting information on home and family life, e.g., magazines, TV, newspapers, relatives, etc., or by writing to extension services. All thirty facilies were found to have the major appliances and facilities in their homes such as heating systems, stoves, refrigerators. The majority of them reported the possession of a freezer, a washing machine, a dryer, and a vacuum cleaner. These families comparatively had a stock of equipment that is likely to need special care to function efficiently for an expected time period. The majority of the families had one car in a running condition. Although some families owned more than one car, practically speaking, they were not all in running condition. CHAPTER V RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Discussion of the findings will be divided into three sections. Each section pertains to one of the three objec— tives of the study in the following order: 1) Activity pat— tern of the homemaker; 2) Time—orientation of the homemaker; 3) Relationship between activity pattern and time-orienta— tion of the homemaker. The study does not test any hypothe— ses statistically; however, reports of statistics and levels of significance are included as criteria to aid in identify— ing possible relationships to be tested in further studies. The Activity Pattern of the Homemaker Included in this section will be discussion of the data about observed activities including distribution of activi- ties, frequencies of observed activities in relation to both area of the home and material directly acted upon, relations ships among activities, and identification of the homemaker’s activity pattern. Distribution of activities A total of 12,131 recorded, codable activities was performed by thirty families during an average observation 105 106 time of 255 minutes per family. The mean number of codable activities performed by each family was 404.4. Since the homemaker in each family was the person of primary focus of the observation, the majority of these recorded activities (11,533, which accounted for 95.2 percent of total) were performed by her. Other members of the family, mainly the children, performed the remaining 4.8 percent which were observed and recorded. It seems reasonable therefore, to restrict the analysis to activities performed by the home— makers of these families. The 4.8 percent of activities observed as being performed by other members of the family was probably not a fair representation of activities per— formed by these members, because little attention was given during the observation period to any person otherthan the homemaker of each family. Table 14 presents activities performed by homemakers of thirty families, analyzed in terms of their distribution of percentages and mean frequencies per minute. The cate- gories of cleaning, conditioning—for—storage and moving—to— storage, are presented each separately and all combined into a single category of resource conserving activities (RCA). This single category of RCA also includes applying pro— tective-materials category of activities which was defined as having a resource conserving function. Because of the small frequencies of applying protective materials category of activities due to the fact that few homemakers performed 107 Table 14. Distribution of observed activities performed by homemakers of thirty families. Total Mean .' number of Percentage frequency/ Type of activity activities of total minute Cleaning 2,125 18.4* .28** Conditioning for storage 441 3.8 .06 Moving to storage 1,080 9.3 .14 Total RCA*** 3,720 32.1 .49 Conditioning for use 1,795 15.6 .25 Moving from storage 1,332 11.5 .18 Service 2,108 18.2 .28 Total CA 5,235 45.3 .71 Aug. acts. 1,239 10.7 .16 Control activities 1,359 11.8 .18 Total 11,553 99.9 Mean number Of activities performed by each homemaker 385 *Each value represents an average percentage of thirty homemakers. **Each value represents an average mean frequency/minute of thirty homemakers. ***RCA include the category of applying protective materials. 108 any activities of that type during the observation time, this category was excluded from the table as separate cate— gory but was added to total RCA. The total 74 applyingr protective~materials~codable>activities were performed by homemakers of only nine families which accounted for .6 percent of total codable activities performed by all home— makers of thirty families. The applying protective materi— als category of activities was not recorded as being per— formed in 21 families during the observation period. The categories of conditioning-for-use, moving—from— storage and service were presented each as a separate type of activity, then they were combined into a single category of consumption activities (CA). The augmenting and/or creat— ing new resources category of activities (Aug. Acts) combines the two activities of preparation and construction with basic materials and with partially prepared materials, since the subdivision seemed to be not very reliable. The reason that the subdivision was not very reliable was in the diffi— culty of coding this type of activity. If a preparation activity was started before the observation period the coder had no way to know whether the preparation started from raw or from partially prepared materials. Therefore, in such cases, it was assumed that partially prepared materials were used, though it could very possibly be that the preparation started with basic materials. The previous categorization of activities is found in Table 14 and the following tables. 109 Review of Table 14 shows that there were differences in the frequencies of activities performed by thirty homemakers. Apparently these homemakers performed cleaning and service activities proportionately and by mean frequency per minute more than any other activity during observations, and condi— tioning-for-storage activity less frequently than any other activity. Looking at the four categories RCA, CA, Aug. Acts and control activities, consumption activities ranked the highest in terms of frequency of performance and control activities ranked the lowest. This, however, can be explained in several ways. It could possibly be that these homemakers had a tendency to perform a specific type of activity more often than other types simply because they liked it or preferred it. On the other hand, it may have been necessary for these homemakers to perform specific activity more frequently than others. For example, since they had a number of children to take care of, service activity was necessary for these children. It could also be possible that only during the observation time, they performed one activity more than the others, probably because the observation time. was not long enough to discloSe or represent these homemaker’s dominant type of activity. However, according to the results of the study and keeping in mind the possible explanations, one would say that activities which had an ultimate purpose of consumption were more frequently performed by this group of homemakers than any other activity. 110 Frequency of observed activities in relation to area of the home The frequencies of observed activities were analyzed in terms of their relation to different locations in the house. A general examination was given to the total codable activi— ties performed in each of the thirty families as they relate to place of performance in the house. The percentage number of Codable activities performed within certain locations in the house varied according to the different types of activities although the kitchen was the most common place for each activity. These differences are illustrated for the activities observed, in Tables 15 through~17. ‘ Table 15 shows percentages of resource conserving activities observed in relation to area of the home where performed. Six of the families performed 100 percent of the observed cleaning activity in the kitchen. This does not i mean, however, that these six families usually do their cleaning only in the kitchen, but this is what actually happened during the observation time, i.e., all their clean: ing happened to be in the kitchen. The highest percentages of the conditioning—for—storage and moving—to—storage activities were performed in the kitchen. Conditioning—forlstorage activities consisted of folding clothes before storage, and preparing food before storage. However, some families used the living room, dining room, the basement or utility room for performing condition— ing—for-storage activities. 111 Table 15. Percentage of resource conserving activities performed in different locations of the house by thirty families.' Item Percent Cleaning activity performed in kitchen living room bath ‘ other* Conditioning-for—storage activity performed in kitchen living room dining room H basement or utility room other* Moving-to-storage activity perfOrmed in kitchen living room bedroom basement or utility room dining room other* Total RCA** performed in kitchen living room bath dining room bedroom other* 62.0 15.4 12.8 *A composite of locations in which less than 10 percent of activities were performed. **RCA include the category of applying protective materials. '112 Moving-to—storage activity was mostly done in the kitchen possibly because the majority of activities coded in this category were such activities as turning off water, turning off stove, putting kitchen articles and food con— tainers back to storage place and the like. Other locations in the house—-living room, bedroom, basement or utility room, and dining room——a1so were used. These locations included such activities as moving clothes or any article‘ to its storage place. Presented in Table 16*are percentages of consumption activities as they relate to place of performance. Review of the table shows that the kitchen was observed to be the most common place for approximately 75 percent of the moving— from—storage activities. Moving—to—storage activities as discussed above covered just the reverse actiVities of moving- from-storage. Such activities as turning on water, elec— tricity or gas, moving things and articles from the storage place for the purpose of consumption and use were included in the moving-from—storage activity category. Although this activity was also performed in other locations in the house, these locations accounted for less than 25 percent of the moving-from;storagenactivity. Conditioning—for—use activity, which for the most part was ironing and laundry, was done in the kitchen most fre— quently, but the living room, dining room, basement or utility room were also used, perhaps to relieve congestion in the kitchen. 113 Table 160 Percentage of aCtiVities with ultimate purpose of consumption performed by thirty families in relation to places of performance. Item Percent Conditioning-for-use activity performed in: kitchen 57.2 living room 14.6 dining room 12.0 basement or utility room 7.8 other? 8.4 Moving-from-storage activity performed in: kitchen - 7 basement or utility room living room bedroom other* \Jhnn01o H+a¢>©Ln Service activity performed in: kitchen living room dining room bedroom other* LOU"! OAIbOWNO kONwl—IU'! All CA performed in: kitchen living room dining room basement or utility room bedroom other* HLD UNA-Axiom (nor—10mm —_— *A composite of locations in which less than 10 percent of activities were performed 114 Service activities were mostly performed in the kitChen; however, other locations such as the living room, dining room, basement or utility room, and bedroom were also used for this activity. The activity included service to self, others, or pets. Such activities as eating, smoking, watch— ing TV, were considered in this category and were for the most part service to self, Such as family members eating or watching TV. Probably the small percentage of the service activity which was done in the-bedroom was accounted for by a mother feeding the infant in this location. Most control activities occurred in the kitchen, because for the most part this consisted of such activities as checking on food while it cooked. The 25 percent of the control activity performed in the living room was probably accounted for by the mother spanking children or practicing other kinds of control over them. Activities observed as preparation or construction were primarily food preparation activities and therefore, almost exclusively done in the kitchen (Table 17). One family simply did not perform any preparation—construction'activi— ties at all during the obServation period, and 25 families performed 100 percent of them in the kitchen. The remaining‘ very small percentage of this activity was performed in the‘ basement or utility room either for cooking food or con— structing clothing from yard goods. -— 115 Table 17. Percentage of control activities and activities that augment or create new resources, performed by thirty families in relation to place of performance. Item Percent Control activity performed in: kitchen living room dining room bedroom outside other* Nib \leWKOONO‘ Ohmi-‘CDEO Preparation or construction activity performed in: kitchen 95.8 basement or utility room ‘ 3.8 other* 0.4 *A composite of locations in which less than 10 percent of activities were performed. 116 As the foregoing discussion indicated, each type of activity occurred in a variety of locations in the house. It is desirable, therefore, to look directly at certain locations individually to see how each was used for the different types of activities (see summary Tables 32 and 33 in Appendix D, pages 214, 215). Kitchen: Living room: The kitchen was used for the highest percent— age of each type of activity and more than any other single location in the house. The kitchen could, therefore, be considered the central location of the house during the observation time of the thirty families. The living room had a wide variety of uses for the thirty families of this study, nevertheless, there was little concentrated use for any one activity except for the service activity which took place more often in the living room than anywhere else in the house. The second highest percentage of almost all types of activities except moving—from—storage and preparation, construction activities, for which the living room ranked the third, was performed in the living room.' While most of the service activity was performed in the living room, preparation and construction activities were performed 117 less frequently in this location of the house; other activities ranged from 5 to 15 percent of them done in the living room. Dining room: Of the total codable activities observed, only 5.5 percent were performed in the dining room. Actually not all families had a sepa— rate room which was known and used as dining room. The majority of families——about three fourths of theme—had a dining room which was part of the kitchen or the living room and, therefore, any activity performed in the dine ing room of these families was actually con— sidered in the kitchen. The few families which had a separate dining room used it as such, primarily for eating, which was con— sidered service activity. Others used this location as a place for folding clothes, which activity was considered as conditioning—for— storage. The two activities,eating and fold— ing clothes,accounted for the 5.5 percent of activities performed in the dining room. Basement or utility room, bedroom, bathroom and other locations were observed in use for about 13 percent of the total codable activities. The division of these small per— centages made further analysis not very meaningful. 118 However, the storage activities, moving to and from storage place and the conditioning-for—use activities were the three activities which accounted for the most of this per— centage. These results are for the most part in agreement with what was reported in the Northeastern study on household activity data and space needs related to design (84). In this study the authors, in order to analyze the relation between activities and place Of performance, calculated the percentage of time expended on each of the fourteen classes of activities studied, which were performed within certain locations in the house. They concluded that the kitchen was in use more than any other location in the house by all the four types of families who constituted the sample of the study, although the percentage of time spent in the kitchen for a specific activity varied according to family type. The following were activities performed in the kitchen: food preparation, eating, leisure, care of individuals and per4 sonal care. These activities are very similar to activities of the present study performed in the kitchen, if the four latter activities would be considered under the service cate— gory of activities in the present study. The living room was second in rank order of daytime use in Northeastern study as well as in the present study. This location of the house was primarily used in the Northeastern' study for such activities as leisure, care of individuals, 119 feeding infants, reading and sewing. These activities with the exception of sewing would be included under the category of service activities in the present study, the highest percentage of which was done in the living room° The bedroom ranked third In daytime use, where the preponderance of time was oCcupied by pre—school children who took naps. Daytime use of bedrooms varied greatly with the family type, depending on the presence of small children. The Northeastern study focused the observation on all family members, father, mother and children, which is why probably the bedrooms ranked third in percentage of daytime use owing to children taking naps. It is possible that the same thing was happening in the present study since there was a number of pre—school children in the families. But since the observation was primarily focused on the homemaker, the bedroom was observed only in use when the homemaker was performing something there, i.e., moving clothes to storage place, which accounted for arsmall percentage of that activity but was the highest percentage with respect to an activity performed in such a location. If children had been observed more continuously in the present study, the results might have showed the bedrOOm ranking high in order as a location used in the house. According to the results of the present study, the bedroom was in use for only 4 percent of the total recorded codable activities. The Northeastern study results are thus very similar to the present study. The differences may be attributed to the 120 fact that in the Northeastern study all family members were observed during the observation time with equal attention given to each member in the family rather than just to the' homemaker as in the present study. Frequency of observed activities in relation to material of central focus of the activity The frequencies of observed activities were analyzed to determine their relation to different materials acted upon and/or utilized in the activity. These materials were coded in two separate categories, the material or person of the central focus of the activity, and other materials which were utilized or acted upon in the same activity. For example, When the homemaker was peeling potatoes at the sink, the activity was food preparation, the material of central focus of the activity was the potato, and other materials acted upon in the same activity would be the knife and the sink. The percentage of total codable activities performed by thirty families in relation to different materials of direct focus of the activity, are displayed. Table 18 summarizes the results and indicates the differences in materials acted upon according to the different types of activities. No material was coded as the central focus in the service cate— gory of activities. This is due to the fact that the service category was defined as either service to self, others or pets, which were not classified as a material. Within the confines of this study and the definition of resources given, aumeflnm may mum3 mcowumm wmsmomb .hbfl>fibom many mo bcmoumm H.mm as coma pmpom mcflnu OOH HMbOb bog 0p mwflbfl>flbom Houbcoo mo doabcouummee .mcprDOH mo omsmomn OOH annoy boa op mommpsmoumm mEomx 121 m.m III: m.o H.o o.m IIII III: QBOGMCD N.ma IIII N.m m.H m.n m.a 0.0 . msomcmaamomflz m.v «III m.m m.H h.m m.H lull mmflammsm Hmcomuwm o.m III: are H.mm 6.ma m.om N.o mmsuoHo o.m nus: v.0 ,, H.o H.H I--- 4.0m mmmuopm Hanan Iosuum .mmHSbosnum omsom s.m I--- m.o m.m e.a o.m o.m mmaflsxmu caosmmsos smhno o.a I--- m.m m,m H.m s.m 4.0 sagas 6cm mqaeemm m.m all: m.o m.H .c.o Ilil o.m mHSbchdm H.N N.o o.m _ 0.4 H.m s.~ m.m mumcumuaoo N.H o.H H.HN «.6 m.ma ---- s.o mwuammsm eaoswmsom m.o m.nm m.m~ m.oa c.ma m.ma w.o boom m.m m.o s.om. .e.oa m.mm m.o s.mm .usm.mcmm .mmsmfle .masmgmu: 6.H nun: m.m o.a e.m m.m m.m pcmsmssum mahmhuom e.m IIII N.o m.H g.o IIII ¢.NH pcmamflsgw momma Ho sflluaflsm o.ooa 0.00H 0.00H o.ooH o.ooa o.ooa o.ooa. emamsumbme Hag Aucmoaomv . «e.uo¢ .Hbmcoo womuObm mm: wUMHObm mUMHObm mce Honucoo .Ho Eoum now Oh How IcmmHO .mmum gcfl>02 .tcou mcH>OE .tcoo mmousommu >bfl>wuum MpH>Hbom Hmflnmbme mo mmwe 3mg coeumuflawbb mcfl>ummcov monsommm mcflbmmho , ..OD¢ >bH>Hbom mo wmmfi .mufl>fluom mo msoom Hmuucmo mbb mo Hafiumbme 0p Goebmamu QH mwflaflfimm hpuflflb mg pwfihomnmm wmflufl>fluum ucmnmmmet mo mmmunmoumm .ma OHQMB 122 non—human resources were considered for identifying the activity as having ultimate purpose of consumption. When service activity was related to other materials acted upon, it was found that the four most frequently utilized materials were food, household supplies, utensils, dishes and pans, and furniture. This could probably be explained by the fact that service activity for the most part was eating or drinking coffee while sitting on some kind of furniture. Also watching TV while sitting on a chair or sofa constituted much of the service activity. The household supplies utilized in the service activity were primarily napkins used while eating, and electricity which was on while watching TV, both materials of which were considered aS'household supplies. Combined activities knOWn as RCA, CA, Aug. Acts. and control activities in relation to material of direct focus of the activity are found in Tables 34, 35 and 36 in Appen— dix D, pages 216—218. Kitchen utensils, pans and dishes were most frequently employed in cleaning, moving—to—storage, and all RCA. Since the kitchen was mainly observed in use for the cleaning and the moving—to—storage activities as discussed above, it would obviously follow that the material acted upon should be something located in the kitchen. The thirty homemakers were mostly occupied in cleaning utensils, pans and dishes. House structure and structural storage which included doors, windows, and built—in cabinets, were the main 123 materials acted upon for 20 percent of the cleaning activity. Built-in and large equipment such as refrigerator and stove, the furniture category of materials, and containers were the central focus material of only 12, 8 and 3 percent, respec— tively, of the cleaning activity. Cleaning needed for any article in the household usually depends on how much it is used. Since utensils, pans and dishes, were the materials most used in cleaning activity, they would be most frequently used in much of the moving—to-Storage activity and that was in fact the case. These homemakers washed utensils, dishes and pans, then stored them in their storage place. Following utensils, food and household supplies were next in use as the material acted upon for 19 percent and 14 percent, respectively, of the moving—to—storage activities. Food was actually stored either in the refrigerator or in containers on shelves or in cabinets. Moving household supplies to storage for the most part included shutting off electricity, gas or water, each of which was considered the direct materi— al acted upon in such activity. Clothes were the material acted upon for 14 percent of the moving—to—storage activity, usually storing clothes after being folded or placed on hangers. Clothes were the central focus material acted upon for most of the activities involving conditioning—for—storage, although, in a small percentage of the folding activities, 124 bedding rather than clothes were the material acted upon. Ranking the second as material acted upon for the moving—to- storage activity, food also ranked the second for the con— ditioning—for—storage activities, probably because food waS' wrapped before being stored in the refrigerator. Some families after washing utensils, pans and dishes, wiped them off before storage. This prObably explains the 6.5 percent of the conditioning-for-storage activity in which utensils, pans and dishes, were the central focus of the activity. Looking at the activities combined as a single cate— gory RCA, one can say that the highest percentage of these activities focused on kitchen utensils, dishes and pans. House structure and structural storage, and clothes were the central focus of 17 and 12 percent of this category of activities, respectively. As mentioned earlier, laundry and ironing for the most' part constituted the conditioning—for—use activity. It was expected then that clothes would rank first as the material of direct focus of this activity. Clothes accounted for more than 50 percent of the conditioning-for—use activity. Bedding were also the central focus of this activity, but in only 4 percent of the activity. Other materials were the central focus for about 50 percent of the conditioning-for-use activity. Utensils, dishes and pans were materials acted upon for the highest percentage of moving-from-storage activities. 125 This means that these materials were frequently in use and thus serves to explain why these materials were also fre— quently the focus of the cleaning activity. Food and house— hold supplies were the materials used for the second and third highest percentages of the moving-from-storage activity, respectively. When the two activities, condition— ing—for—use and moving—from—storage activities were com- bined as a single utilization category of activities, clothes were the focus of the highest percentage of this activity, followed by food, utensils, dishes and pans and household supplies. The remaining materials were the focus of ohly about 15 percent of the utilization activities. In control activities a variety of materials was acted upon. However, the highest share (35 percent) of these activities focused on persons rather than materials. When mothers practiced any kind of control over their children, the children were considered as the direct focus of the activity. Checking on mail,which was considered in the miscel- laneous category of materials, accounted for 15 percent of the control activity. Tidying which was not cleaning was considered a control activity. Household textiles were the material acted upon in about 4 percent of the control activities. Since food preparation for the most part constituted preparation-construction activities, obviously food was the 126 direct material acted upon for the highest share of these activities. Very little sewing and constructing clothing from yard goods were ..Observve’d. Clothes were the materials acted upon for only 3 percent of the preparation-construc— tion category of activities. In summary, all categories of materials were used in some way or other, although some were used more than others in performing the different activities (Table-19). Built-in or large equipment were the central focus of the highest portion (29 percent) of total codable activities while personal supplies were the central focus of the smallest part (1 percent) of all activities. This does not mean, however, that built-in or large equipment were in use more than, for example, utensils, clothes and therest, since these percentages present all activities combined, some of which were utilization activities while others had resource— conserving function, others had resource—augmenting function, and some were purposeful but did not serve the immediate function of consumption or reSource conservation or augmen— tation. Relationships among_activities From the figures and percentages in the previous dis— cussion, one is in a position to deduce some relationships among the different types of activities. Using the average mean frequency per minute of each activity, simple correla- tions among the activities were calculated. The 10 percent 127 Table 19. Percentage of total activities performed by thirty families in relation to material of the central focus of the activity. Percent of Type of material activity All materials* 100 Built—in or large equipment 29.3 Food 19.1 Utensils, dishes 13.7 Clothes 13.0 Household supplies 5.0 House structure or structural storage 4.5 Miscellaneous 4.2 Containers 2.2 Furniture 2.1 Bedding 1.8 Unknown type 1.8 Other household textiles 1.7 Portable equipment 1.4 1.4 Personal supplies *Percentage does not total 100 because of rounding. 128 level of probability was taken to denote significant rela— tionships. However, any correlation significant at the lper— cent leve1_of probability was pointed out. The existence or non—existence of relationships among the actiVities was taken to indicate whether the activities were associated with one another or independent of each other. Table 20 includes the correlation coefficient calcu- lated among activities. The mean frequency per minute of moving-from-storage increased as the mean frequency per minute of moving—to—storage activities increased. Investigating the raw data showed that in most cases, as families moved things away from storage, they tended to move these same things back to their storage place again. Significant positive relationships appeared between cleaning and moving to and from storage and a negative relationship between cleaning and the service activity (Table 20). It seems logical to suppose, that as things are being cleaned, materials used in the cleaning activity need to be moved from and to storage. After things are cleaned, these families tended to put thenlback in their storage place. While these families were engaged more in cleaning they did not have time to sit around relaxing or watching TV. Moving—from—storage activity seems to be associated with preparation-construction activities. Since preparation— construction activities were for the most part food 129 Table 20. Correlation coefficients calculated among different types of activities. CorrelatiOn Relationships coefficient Moving—from—storage/moving-to-storage O.55** Cleaning/moving—from-storage 0.38*’ Cleaning/moving-to—storage O.60** Cleaning/service —0.46* Moving-from-storage/preparation:—construction 0.4l* Preparationr-construction/control 0.27* *Significant at the 10 percent level of probability. **Significant at the 1 percent level of probability. 130 preparation as mentioned earlier, this relationship can be explained by the fact that as fbod was being prepared, families obviously needed to move food materials and other articles necessary for the preparation from their storage place. A significant positive relationship was found between preparation—construction activities and control activities, such as checking on food while it was being cooked. It is obvious that during the food preparation process, several checks on food are necessary. Identification of the homemaker's activityppattern As mentioned earlier in the methodology chapter, activity categories for coding were developed to satisfy the requirement that the categories distinguish resource- conserving activities from those that utilize materials with the ultimate purpose of consumption. To identify the activity pattern of the homemaker with respect to conserva— tion of the family's available resources and utilization of materials with the ultimate purpose of consumption, the mean frequency per minute for each of the two categories of activities was computed for each of the thirty homemakers, and the average of the mean frequencies per minute for the thirty homemakers in each category was calculated. Each homemaker's mean frequency per minute was compared with the average to determine whether the homemaker was above or below 131 average. Thirteen homemakers were classified above the average mean frequency per minute of RCA, and twelve home- makers ranked above the average in CA. Seventeen and eighteen homemakers were below average in RCA and CA, respectively. In order to determine her activity pattern each'hbme— maker was classified according to her standing with respect to the two categories of activities RCA and CA. The combi- nation of her standing in both categories formed her activity pattern. The following were the four possible activity patterns: HH HL LHandLL It may be observed in Table 21 that each of the four patterns was identified as being the pattern of at least five homemakers. On superficial consideration one would think that RCA are opposed to CA because the function of each category is the contrary of the other. One category of activities is to conserve resources, while the other is to consume them. Therefore; one might expect that homemakers who had a tendency to perform one category of activities, would be less inclined to perform the other. But this is not the case. In fact, the two categories of activities are complementary to each other in the sense that a higher level of RCA facilitates or is necessitated by a higher level of CA. Therefore, one might consider that homemakers with an HH or LL pattern have a balance of RCA and CA. One usually 132 Table 21. Number of homemakers identified for each of the four activity patterns. Activity pattern Number of RCA CA homemakers H H 5 H L 8 L H 7 L L 10 Total 30 133 conserves his resources in order to use them. The distinc- tion made between the two activity categories was arbitrary in that a single motive was attributed to the homemaker for each activity. For example, it was assumed that the pur— pose of the activity of moving things from storage was to consume, whereas, it could very possibly be that the home— maker moved the thing from storage for some other reason. Also while the moving-from—storage activity was distinguished as a CA and the moving—to—storate activity as a RCA, one activity necessitated the performance of the other. This is reflected in the significant positive relationship found between the two activities. As Table 21 shows, homemakers with the activity patterns H H and L L combined are equal to those with the activity patterns H L and L H combined. Thus, half of the homemakers have mixed activity patterns characterized by higher mean frequency per minute in one category of activities and lower mean frequency per minute in the other. According to the previous discussion, these homemakers are of special interest as representing a possible imbalance of RCA and CA. The other half of the homemakers have uniform activity patterns in the sense that either both mean frequencies per minute of the two categories of activities were higher or lower. It would appear that these homemakers were simply more or less active than the average. Investigating families whose homemakers‘ activity pat— terns were higher in RCA, it was found that eight out of 134 thirteen of them reported or perceived themselves as being better off than they were five years ago. In contrast seven out of seventeen of the families whose homemakers' activity pattern were lower in RCA reported themselves as being better off than they were five years ago. This might sug— gest the possibility of helping families improve their economic position and welfare through the identification and the understanding of the importance of RCA. In order to determine whether any interrelationships existed, homemakers in the two groups of higher and lower mean frequency per minute of RCA were compared with their educational level, occupations and income. Table 22 indi— cates that although the range of education was almost the same for the two groups, hOmemakers in the higher group seem to be better educated than those in the lower group. As Table 22 shows, the majority of homemakers in the higher group were high school graduates, while in the lower group only nine out of seventeen homemakers were so- One home- maker in the higher group had some technical and business school after high school, while none in the other group had any education beyond high school. In this case, one can think of the education of the homemaker as being a factor related to her knowledge about conservation of the family‘s available resources. It could be the topic for future in— vestigation'in'the3home management research area. 135 Table 22. Homemakers in the higher and lower groups of mean frequencies/minute of Resource Conserv— ing Activities in relation to their educational level. Educational level Number of homemakers Higher group Lower group 8th grade 1 3 Some high school 1 5 High school graduate 10 9 Technical or business school 1 — Total 13 17 Table 23. Homemakers in the higher and lowerggroups Of frequencies/minute of resource conserving activ— ities in relation to their occupations. Occupation Number of homemakers Higher group Lower group Homemaker 8 14 Service 1 1 Farm labor ' 1 Labor . l 1 Clerical or sales 3 l Total 13 17 136 Presented in Table 23 (on the preceding page) is the occupational level of homemakers in the two groups of homemakers. Fourteen out of seventeen of the homemakers in the lower group were fulltime homemakers while only eight out of thirteen of the homemakers in the other group were fulltime homemakers. Thus there appears to be a relation- ship between the homemaker being employed and the frequency of her performing RCA. Going out of the house to work and meeting with people and talking to them may broaden the homemaker's knowledge about the importance of conserving and the means by which one can conserve the family's avail— able resources. Income and mean frequenCy per minute of performing RCA may be somewhat related. As Table 24 shows, families of homemakers who performed higher mean frequency per minute of RCA tended to receive higher incomes than those of home— makers with lower mean frequehCy per minute of RCA. While the majority of families in the lower group were receiving incomes below $8,999 annually, only about half of the home- makers in the other group received that amount. One home— maker's family annual income reached $18,928 in the higher group while none in the other group had any income exceeding $11,999 per year. Oh investigating equipment owned by the thirty families, it was found that the majority of homemakers in both groups had the major appliances in their homes such as stove, 137 Table 24. Family incomes of homemakers in the higher and lower mean frequency/minute of resource conserving activities. Income range Number of homemakers (in dollars) Higher group Lower group Less than $5,999 — l 6,000—8,999 7 l3 9,000—1l,999 4 3 12,000—14,999 l — Over 15,000 1 — Total 13 17 Mean income 9,028* 7,533 Per capita income 1,616* 941 *These figures exclude the family with the annual income of $18,928 which income increased 420% over the last five years. 138 refrigerator, TV, vacuum cleaner, etc. On the other hand, the majority of homemakers who tended to perform higher mean frequency per minute of RCA had the small appliances such as mixer, blender, coffee maker, roaster and the like. In contrast only a few of the group of homemakers with lower mean frequency per minute of RCA had these small appliances. As families receive more income, they become able to get more equipment and appliances. Consequently homemakers of these families can perform activities which tend to conserve their family's resources more frequently than homemakers with less income who don't have as many appliances to uti— lize and care for. It seems Clear that as caring for dur- able goods was considered RCA, the performance of such an activity is associated with the equipment that any family has. LThi57+at least, is What this study showed. .It raises the question, however, of the possibility that a family may have enough equipment and materials and yet give no care t0- these things either because of lack of knowledge about conservation of resources or’because these articles are simply not used very often and therefore don't need much care. Time Orientation of the Homemaker Included in this section will be discussion of data concerned with the conversation.of the homemaker in the. following manner: Distribution of message units, identifying 139 the time orientation of the homemaker, content categories, and test of independence of content—time categories. Distribution of message units The total number of message units transcribed from tapes as being spoken by homemakers of thirty families and which were coded and analyzed was 16,614 with a mean of 553.8 mes— sage units per homemaker. As presented in Table 25 each time reference was composed of the three content areas self, other persons, and materials. While conversation message units of the present time reference were found to comprise the highest proportion (63 percent) of total message units, future time reference message units accounted for the lowest, 10 percent, of total message units. The past time reference accounted for 27 percent of the total with a mean of 151 message units per homemaker. The predominance of the present time reference in the conversation of these homemakers is consistent with studies relating time—orientation to social class. The total sample of this study was rural non-farm families, who reported earning incomes below the poverty level five years before data were collected. By other measures (other than income) of socio-economic class such as the educational level and occupation of both husband and wife, these families were in lower socio—economic levels. 140 Table 25. Percentage of message units spoken by thirty homemakers in each of the three time references. Mean number of message Time reference Percent Number units/homemaker a Past 27.3 4,534 151 Present 62.8 10,435 348 Future 9.9 1,645 55 Total 100.0 16,614 141 According to Lewin (54) the social environment in which the individual lives influences his time-orientation. Davis (17) and Kinsey (44) have remarked on the difference in temporal orientation between members of different sOCial classes. The two authors pointed out that people of lower class group are less likely to postpone-present gratifica- tion in favor of future gain. LeShan (53) in his investiga— tion of the relationship between time-orientation and social class, found that lower class children eight to ten years old were predominantly present time—oriented. Identifying the time-orientation of the homemaker Since present time references predominated the conversa— tion of homemakers, each time reference was looked at sepa— rately to determine where eaCh homemaker stands with respect to the average percentage of each time reference. Therefore, the average of percentages for the thirty homemakers in each time reference was calculated. Each homemaker's percentage of any time reference was compared with the average percent of that time reference in order to decide whether the home- maker was average, above average or below average. Homemakers showing percentages of past time references which were average or above average were considered more past time— oriented than those who fell below the average, who were con— sidered less past time—oriented. Likewise, homemakers who were more future and more present time—oriented were 142 identified., Thirteen homemakers were identified as being more future time—oriented and sixteen and thirteen home— makers, respectively, were identified as being more present and more past time—oriented. Table 26 presents the five combinations of time orien— tations identified for the thirty homemakers. The majority of those (eight out of thirteen) who were more future time— oriented were less past time-oriented. Since future and past are the two extremes of the time reference Continuum, it was expected that those homemakers who were more future time-oriented would show less past time-orientation. However, five of the homemakers who were more future time—oriented were also more past time—oriented. It is possible that a person may have a good experience, about which he likes to talk, and at the same time is anticipating a similar future experience. AnalOgous explanation might account for homemakers who were more present time-oriented and less past time-oriented and either more or less future time-oriented. There was some evidence in the literature of a relation— ship between timeworientation and age. Therefore, the group of homemakers who were more future time—oriented was compared with those who were less future time—oriented with respect to age. Table 27 shows that the majority of homemakers who were more future time—oriented were between the age of 30 and 35 years, while most homemakers who were less future time— oriented were older than 35. The range of the age of the less future time—oriented group of homemakers was 27—55 which 143 Table 26. Time orientation of thirty homemakers. Number of Future time— Present time— Past time- homemakers oriented oriented oriented l more less less 7 more more less 5 more less more 9 less more less 8 less less more Total 30 Table 27. Distribution of age of thirty homemakers in relation to their time—orientation. Number of homemakers Age range Future time—oriented Past time—oriented More Less More Less Under 30 V 2 3 l 4 30—35 6 4 5 5 36—40 3 6 4 5 41—45 1 2 l 2 46—50 x 1 1 1 1 51-55 — 1 l — Total 13 l7 13 17 Mean age 35 37 37 35 144 was wider than that of the other group who were more future time—oriented. Similarly, it seems that more past time- orientation is associated with older age (Table 27). These results are in accord with those reported by Fink (24) who found that aged peOple were more concerned with the past and less with the future than younger people. The two different age groups Fink investigated were in the age ranges of 50—60 and 61—70. The 50—60 year old group may not represent young people, generally speaking, but relative to the other group they are less old. Fink found differences in their time—orientation, the less old (50—60 years old) group which he called the young group being more future and less past time—oriented than the older group (61—70 years 61d) . The relationship between age and time orientation did not seem to be very clear in Eson's (23) study of temporal emphasis of five different age levels. His groups were 10 year old males and females, 16 year old males and females, 25 year old males, 35 year old males and 65 year old males. Although his sample presented a wide range of ages, he found that a greater stress was given to the future than to the past by each of his five age level group. One would expect, for example, that the 65 year old group would show more emphasis on the past than on the future. The picture, however, was the reverse. Eson's results also showed that the 25—year—old group had the highest mean percentage of future responses and the T’f lowest mean percentage of paSt responses indicating that this 145 group was the most future time—oriented and the least past time-oriented of all other grdups studied. The results also indicated the mean percentage of future responses of the 65-year—old group, was higher than that of the 35—year—old, 16—year—old and 10-year-old males groups. The issue of the relationship between age and time-orientation is unresolved and more research is needed in this area. Table 28 shows the distribution of educational level of homemakers of different time—orientations. It may be ob— served that homemakers who were more future time-oriented seem to have a higher level of education than less future time-oriented homemakers, although the highest educational level of both groups was graduation from high school with the exception of one homemaker Who got some technical and business schooling after high school. By the same token, homemakers who were less:past time- oriented seem to be more educated than those who were more past time—oriented (Table 28). However, since the educa— tional level of young persons nowadays is generally higherv than that of older persons, it is not clear whether time— orientation is related to education, age or both. Relation— ships between education and time—orientation of homemakers who have received very little education and homemakers with more education such as college degrees should be explored. 146 Table 28. Distribution of educational level of thirty home— makers in relation to their time—orientation. Number of homemakers Educational Level Future time-oriented Past time—oriented More» Less More Less 8th grade 1 4 2 2 Some high school 1 4 2 4 High school graduate 10 9 9 10 Technical or busi— ness school 1 — - l Total 13 17 13 17 147 Content Categories Each content area refering to self, other persons, and materials, includes the three time references, past, present, and future. The "other persons” content area dominated the content of the message units spoken by the thirty homemakers, which accounted for half of the total conversation message units (Table 29). For the "self" and "materials” content areas, there was a mean of 155 and 122 message units, respectively, per homemaker and the percentages of approxi— mately 28 and 22 of the total of message units. These home— makers' conversations were centered around persons more than around materials. They prefer to talk about other people and themselves more frequently than they do about material things. Judging from their conversation alone, they could be classified as person—centered rather than material—centered people. Test of independence of content— time categories In order to determine whether messages should be classi— fied by content before being analyzed for time reference, the Chi—Square Test of Independence was performed. The signifi— cant value (718.85) indicated that time—content categories were dependent on each other. ”Therefore, message units were analyzed in terms of the nine time—content categories. As Table 30 indicates, all the nine categories were used by the homemakers. Investigation, however, of the raw data 148 Table 29. Percentages of message units spoken by thirty homemakers in each of the three content areas. Mean number of Percent of message units Content area total Total per homemaker Self 27.9 4,641 155 Other persons 50.0 8,306 277 Materials 22.1 3,667 122 —-———--————— _-—‘———~—-————.————————~-—_——_————————————_—_— Total 100.0 16,614 I'll. lll' [lull-III lllllllllln IIIIII'|I ll'll'lIII.llllllll'llll'lllIIIIIIIUIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII mm mm mm Aha ow 149 ON mm mm m.bH m.om ¢.va .m .ga .m VI‘NKO ¢H0.N mNH.m mmm.m ooo mom.m mom.H mocmummmu mEHuImHobsm mamaumbmz mocmummou meflblmHSbom mcomumm Hobuo mocmummou oeflblmunbom meow mocmummmm mafiplbcmmmum namehmumz mucoummmu mEHblucmmmHm mcomumm Hmnuo mocmummmu maflblucmmmum mamm mocmummmu mEHblbmmm nameumbmz mocmnmmmu meflblbmmm mcomumm Hobbo mocmummmu mEHprmmm wawm HmMmEoEo; Hoe wows: mommwofi mo Monaco com: moduommbmo Ham CH Hobos mo bcmoumm muflcs mommmoe mo Monaco Hoboe whommbmo bambooolmEHB .mmwuommumo bambcoolmEHu ou Umbmaon mm mmflbfl>flbom UHOflmmoOS maflmp mo mocmEHomuom Ono mcfluot mcoflpmmum>coo .mHmMMEmEOS muuflgb mo woes: mommmme mo movabcoouom .om mHQMB 150 showed that five homemakers said nothing in the material future reference. The highest percentage of all message units spoken by the homemakers was in the present time reference under the "other persons" content category, while the lowest percentage was in the future time reference under the "materials" category of content. Comparing the three time references, each consisting of the three content areas, we find that "other persons" content category accounted for the highest percentage, regardless of the time reference. In talking about other people, these homemakers used the present time reference more frequently than either the past or the future. In using the past and the future time references, they tended to refer least to materials in their conversation, while in the present reference they referred least to themselves. Looking at the three content areas, each composed of the three time references, we perceive that present time references still accounted for the highest percentage of message units. The future time reference in all content areas accounted for the lowest percentage. These homemakers appeared to be less future and less past time—oriented under the “materials" content category and less present time— oriented under the content category of “self." Smeltzer (70), in his study of time-orientation in psychotherapy adapted the following four content areas from Lennard and Bernstein (52): family relationships, other social relationships, self, and therapist—client relationships. In order to test for a relationship, between the time orientation of the client and these content areas, the changes in past, present and future-time-orientation in the first half to the last half of psychotherapy under each of these areas were determined. He tested for'differences between means of percentages of these categories under each of the three time references. His results showed more similarities than differences when he looked at the time—orientation in the context of the four content areas. The differences were only under the self content category in the three time— orientations, and in the past time—orientation under the therapist—client category. This, like the present study, indicates some relationships between time and content cate— gories. The Relationship Between Activity Pattern and Time Orientation The research question that was raised in this study concerns the relationship between time—orientation of the homemaker and her activity pattern. It was expected that homemakers who are future time—oriented will perform activ— ities which have a resource conserving function more frequently than do homemakers who are past and/or present time-oriented. In order to determine the relationship exist— ing, simple correlation coefficients were calculated between average percentage of time-orientation and the over—all 152 average mean frequency per minute of activity categories. A table of simple correlations accompanies the discussion (Table 31). When each of the three time-orientations, past, present, and future was correlated with the category of RCA, a nega— tive correlation of -0.23 and -O.21 and a positive correla— tion of 0.13 were found, respectively. These relationships are in the predicted direction although the correlations were not significant. .Homemakers who were more past or present time-oriented performed fewer activities which have resource conserving function. On the other hand, the posi— tive correlation found between future time—orientation and the category of RCA suggests that future time—orientations is associated with higher mean frequency per minute of RCA. A negative correlation, significant at the 10 percent level of probability was found between present time—orienta— tion under the ”self" category of content and activities which have a resource—conserving function. The past time orientation under "other persons" category of content showed a significant negative correlation with RCA, indicating that homemakers who were more past time—oriented under "other persons" category of content, performed RCA less frequently than those who were less past time-oriented. Other correla— tions between RCA and time—orientations under the self, others, and materials content categories were in the pre- dicted direction although they were not significant (Table 31). 153 Table 31. Correlation coefficient calculated between types of activities and time content categories. fi—V. Resource conserving activities/ Past time—orientation —0.23 Present time-orientation -0.21 Future time-orientation 0.13 Self past time-orientation —0.16 Self present time—orientation —0.33* Self future time-orientation 0.26 Other persons past time—orientation -0.43* Other persons present time—orientation —O.15 Other persons future time—orientation 0.04 Materials past time—orientation —0.14 Materials present time-orientation —0.02 Materials future time-orientation 0.02 Self content category ' 0.38* Other persons content category —0.43* Consumption activities/ Self content category —0.37* Preparation, construction activities/ Future time-orientation O.46* Self present time-orientation —0.38* Other persons future time-orientation 0.55** Self content category -0.33* Control activities/ Self content category —0.34* Other persons content category —0.33* Materials content category 0.63** *Significant at the 10 percent level of probability. **Significant at the 1 percent level of probability. 154 Correlations between categories of activities and time— content categories may reflect personality characteristics of homemakers which could be explored in future research. Significant positive and negative relationships, respectively, were found between the category of RCA and the ”self“ and “other persons” content categories (Table 31). Performing RCA was positively related to referring to self in conversa— tion but negatively related to referring to other persons. In contrast, consumptiOn activities were found to be nega— tively related to the self content category. Frequencies of performing preparation—construction activities were found to be positively related to future time—orientation. Present time—orientation under the ”self” content category and future time orientation under the "other persons" category content were negatively and positively related to preparation construction activities, respectively. This indicates that activities which tend to create new resources or augment the available resources appeared to be associated with future time—orientation. The negative correlation between preparation—construction activities and the self category of content raises some questions about personality characteristics of these home~ makers. As they performed more of the preparation construc— tion activities they tended to talk less about themselves. Correlations between control activities and each of the COntent categories self, other persons, and materials, were 155 significant. Homemakers who tended to perform more of this type of activity tended also to be material—centered in their conversations rather than person-centered. They talked more about material things and less about themselves or other people. Some significant relationships, either positive or nega— tive, appeared to exist between the different types of activities and time and content categories. The correlations between activities and time-orientation were usually in the direction which was expected. Relationships between the types of activities and the three content areas seemed to be related to personality characteristics and raised questions that need to be further investigated to better understand the homemaker and her behavior in the home. CHAPTER VI SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION The purpose of this study was threefold: first, to identify the pattern of activity and/or resource use of a selected group of homemakers—~the extent to which their activity patterns involved conservation of resources; second, to identify the temporal value orientation of the homemakers, whether past, present or future time—oriented; and third, to explore relationships between time—orientation of the homemaker and her activity pattern. The assumptions underlying the overall design of the study were that temporal value-orientation can be identified from language used in conversation, and that an established pattern of family activities can be inferred by observation of the use of available resources. It was hypothesized that future time—orientation versus past and present time- orientations is associated with frequencies of performing activities that have a resource conserving function. The word resources refers to non-human resources, including all durable and non—durable goods available for the family. A non-probability sample of thirty rural families living in the vicinity of Lansing was selected for the study. The criteria applied in the selection of the families were: families were to include both husband and wife and one or 156 157 more children ten years of age or older living at hOme, all families reported income below the poverty level five years before data were collected, and half of them reported their economic position had since improved. Interviews were performed to get background information about the family, especially over the previous five year period. To obtain as accurate and complete information as possible on the homemaker's activities she was observed for approximately four hours while performing her normal daily activities. Observations were scheduled at the homemaker's convenience and were planned to include major household activities; namely, food preparation, cleaning and laundry. Data were recorded minute by minute by the observer for each activity performed. Verbal conversation was mechanically recorded. Activity data were coded according to the nature of the activity, individual(s) involved, house area in which activ— ity occurred, and materials and equipments used in the activity. Distinction was made between activities which have implications for resource conservation, and these with the ultimate purpose of consumption. Two additional cate— gories were identified as control activities, which were purposeful but did not serve the immediate function either of consumption or of resource conservation, and preparation— construction activities which have resource augmentation and creation function. 158 Conversations were transcribed, and then divided into message units for purpose of coding and analysis. Each message unit was coded on two dimensions, the time reference—— past, present, or future--and the content of the message unit, whether the speaker referred to self, other persons, or materials. The time reference which appeared to be intended by the homemaker was coded. The sample included a group of homemakers with ages ranging from 27 to 55 years. Family income ranged from below $6,000 to over $10,000. The majority of these homemakers were not employed. Their educational level ranged from 8th grade to high school graduate. The majority of husbands had relatively unskilled jobs. Their education ranged from .less than 8th grade to some college, with the exception of one husband who had a college degree and worked as a civil engineer. All families were in early stages of the family life cycle. The number of children in the family ranged from two to thirteen, most of whom were seven to twelve years of age. The majority of families were found to have a stock of the major appliances in their homes. A total of 12,131 recorded, codable activities was per— formed by the thirty families with an average of 404 recorded activities per family during an average observa— tion time of 255 minutes‘per family. The majority of these recorded activities wasperformed by the homemaker since she was the person of primary focus of the observation. 159 During the observation period, the kitchen was con— sidered the central location of the house since it accounted for the highest proportion of each of the four types of activities more than any other single location in the house. Built—in or large equipment was the central focus of the highest percentage of total codable activities while personal supplies accounted for the smallest percentage. However, with respect to the four types of activities, utensils and dishes, clothes, miscellaneous, and food, respectively, were the central focus of the resource conserving activities, consumption activities, control activities and resource aug— menting activities. Activity pattern of the homemaker was identified as being higher or lower than the average mean frequency per minute of each of the resource conserving activities and the consumption activities. Since the two categories of activi— ties, RCA and CA are complementary to each other in the . sense that a higher level of RCA facilitates or is necessi- tated by a higher level of CA, activity pattern was identi— fied from both RCA-and CA. Half of the homemakers had mixed activity patterns characterized by higher mean fre— quency per minute in one category of activities (RCA or CA) and lower mean frequency per minute in the other. These homemakers are of special interest because they represent a possible imbalance of RCA and CA. The other half of the homemakers was found to have a uniform activity pattern 160 characterized by higher or lower mean frequency per minute in both categories of activities, thus representing a pos- sible balance between the two activities, although some might have been more or less active than the average. It was found that the majority of the homemakers whose activity pattern was higher in RCA reported their families as being better off than they were five years ago while only a few of the homemakers whose activity pattern was lower in RCA did so. This suggests the possibility of helping families improve their economic position and welfare through the identification and the understanding of the importance of RCA. Homemakers with higher mean frequency per minute of RCA were compared with those with lower mean frequency per minute with respect to their educational level and occupation. The results showed that education and occupation could be factors related to the homemaker's conservation of family's available resources. Income also may be related to the performance of RCA perhaps because families with more income can afford to have more materials and equipment which are used and cared for. The total number of message units transcribed from tapes was 16,614 with the mean of 554 per homemaker. The predomi— nance of the present—time reference in the conversation of these homemakers was consistent with studies relating time— orientation to social class in the sense that low class people are predominantly present time—oriented. 161 Percentage of each category in relation to total number of message units was calculated and used as variables indi— cating the time—orientation of the homemaker. When each time reference was looked at separately, thirteen homemakers were classified as being more future time—oriented, and sixteen and thirteen homemakers, respectively, as being more present and more past time—oriented. Comparisonsamong homemakers of different time—orientation revealed possible relationshipsto exist between time—orientation and both age and education, a matter which needs further exploration. With respect to the content areas and judging from their conversation alone, these homemakers could be classified as person—centered rather than material—centered people. The Chi—Square Test of Independence was performed to test for relationship between content and time. The results indicated that time—content categories were dependent on each other. The homemakers were less future and less past time—oriented under the "materials“ category of content and less present time—oriented under the ”self" COntent category. Correlation coefficients were computed to determine the relationship between the time—orientation of the home— maker and her activity pattern. The relationships were in the predicted direction although the correlations were not significant. Future time—orientation was associated with higher mean frequency per minute of RCA, whereas past and present time—orientation were associated with lower mean 162 frequency per minute. Significant correlations found between categories of activities and time—content categories may reflect personality characteristics of the homemakers which should be explored in future research. Limitations of the Study This study had some limitations which ought to be kept in mind in the interpretation of its results: 1) The families selected consisted of a non—probability sample. Therefore the two major assumptions of randomiza— tion and normality were violated. Consequently, generaliza— tions must be limited to this sample. 2) When compared much of the real differences found be— tween means could not be distinguished from chance differences. 3) An estimate of inter—observer reliability was not available since each family was observed by one observer only. Thus, no check was made on the records which the observer wrote. Observers may have oVerlooked some behavior or activ— ity so commonplace as to esCape attention. Some activities were definitely lost. If the observation had been done by two observers, reliability could have been checked and this limitation could have possibly been reduced. 4) The limited length of time of the observation, approximately four hours, may not have given an adequate, sample of normal daily activities. 5) The distinction made between the two categories or RCA and CA was arbitrary in that a single motive was attri— buted to the homemaker for each activity. 163 6) The poor quality of some of the recordings inter- ‘ferred with the transcription of these parts of the recordings and thus reduced the amount of conversation to be analyzed. 7) The present study utilized one technique for probing the temporal value orientation, that was analyzing the home- maker's conversation in terms of the time referred to in the message unit. Since the identification of the time—orienta— tion of the homemaker is a relatively untapped home management research area, this part ofthe study was exploratory in nature. Other techniques should be used with the present one to assess time—orientation in order to perfect the technique used in the present study. Implications for Future Research This study contributed new data concerning the homemak— er's time-orientation and her activity pattern. Although conclusions are limited to the sample studied, the new data raise questions and suggest areas for further research. Seven hypotheses which emerged from the findings will be stated with accompanying discussion. The field of home management has a responsibility for studying values. Although several'home management researches have focused on values, the temporal values have been a com— paratively untapped home management research area. The method used in this study to assess the time~ orientation by analyzing the conversation of the homemaker 164 in terms of referring to the present, past or future seemed to be an appropriate method. .However, it is-highly desir— able to check its validity. The development and use of other techniques in studying time—orientation will help perfect the present method. The present study begins to explore the homemaker's time—orientation. Because of its important role in the determination of behavior the concept should be studied fur— ther. The_following is a major hypothesis that needs to be vigorously tested. .Hypothesis 1: Homemakers who are future time-oriented (perform resource conserving activities more frequently than those who are past or present time-oriented. The results of the present study showed differences in homemakers' time—orientations. Some were more future, more present or more past time—oriented than others. This orien— tation appeared to be related to their activity pattern. Future research studies should focus on time-orientation of the different family members to see whether there is a common temporal value-orientation for the whole family or whether members have individually held time—orientations. Does the size of the family influence the commonality or variation in time-orientation among members? What are the consequences of differences in time—orientation of family members? What are the implications for home management in terms of goal forma— tion, family relationships, and decision-making in the family, of family members holding similar or different time— orientation? These are questions for future study. 165 Relationships of time—orientation to several variables such as age, education and social class are not very clear and need additional investigations. Is time-orientation re— -lated toeducation, age or both? Or is one or the other an intervening variable? The following hypothesis should be tested. Hypothesis 2: Younger, more highly educated homemakers tend to be more future time- oriented than those who are older and have less education. Identification of activities Which conserve resourceS“ may be useful in helping families improve their economic position and the quality of their life. The following hypothesis should be tested. Hypothesis 3: A higher economic position is positively related to frequency of performance of resource con— serving activities because (a) performance of resource conserving activities contributes to economic improve- ment and (b) families in a higher economic position have more material resources requiring conservation. Is performance of resource conserving activities associ— ated with education? This is a question which the present study raised. Hypothesis 4: A higher level of education is positively related to frequency of performing resource conserving activities. The concept of resource conservation has cross—cultural implications. In the developing countries where the resources- are more limited and scarce than in the United States, such a concept is important.‘ Studies directed toward the identifi— cation of activities which tend to conserve the families‘ available resources are highly desirable. Relationships found beeween activities and time content categories may reflect personality characteristics of the homemakers which should be explored in future research. The following hypotheses are suggested. Hypothesis 5: Homemakers who are highly self—centered perform resource conserving activities more frequently than those who are other persons—centered. Hypothesis 6: Homemakers who are highly materials— centered perform control activities more frequently than those who are highly person—centered (self or other persons). ' Hypothesis 7: Homemakers who are future time—oriented and/or highly other persons—centered perform preparation— construction activities more frequently than those who are present or past~time—oriented and/or highly self— centered. Educational Implications. The present study showed that future time—orientation is associated with frequency of per— forming resource conserving activities. Past and present time—orientations showed a reverse relationship. This may have some educational implications concerning the modifica— tion of time—orientation toward the future. The question now is: Is it possible to modify or change the time-orientation and how can this be done? The present study indicated that homemakers who re— ported improvement of their economic position over a five year period were found to be performing resource conserving activities more frequently than those who did not improve economically. If this is true, it implies that educatiOnal programs can be designed to improve homemakers' understanding of the importance of performing resource conserving activi- ties and the means to conserve families'-resources. No previous researches have specifically addressed them— selves to exploring the homemaker's temporal value—orientation and its relationship to her day—to—day activities. The present study has identified'and clarified questions and implications for future research. Further study of this area is important because it may offer clues for better understanding management in the home and enhance the‘pOSSi‘ bilities for imparting knowledge about home management. LITERATURE CITED .l|.|||| ill I‘ll i 10. 11. LITERATURE CITED Baker, G. R. "Patterning of Family Resources for Educa— bility: Conceptualization and Measurement in Costa Rican Families." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Baker, J. "Rural America Needs Home Economists,” Agricultural Science Review, No. 2, Fall 1964, p. 150. Balken, E. R., and Masserman, J. H. "The Language of Phantasy: III. The Language of the Phantasies of Patients with Conversion Hysteria, Anxiety States, and Obsessive-compulsive Neuroses," Journal of Psychology, Vol. 10, 1940, pp. 75—86. Barndt, R. J. "Time—Orientation in Delinquents." Unpub— lished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1953. Barndt, R. J., and Johnson, D. M. "Time-Orientation in Delinquents," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Vol. 51, 1955, pp. 343-345. Berlo, D. K. The Process of Communication, An Introduc- tion to Theory and Practice. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston Inc., 1960. Beyer, G. H. "Housing and Personal Values." Agricul-. tural Experiment Station Memoir 364, Cornell Univer51ty, Ithaca, New York, 1959;‘ Borg, W. R. Educational Research, An Introduction. New York: David McKay Company Inc., 1965, p. 237. Bustrillos, N. R. "Decision Making Styles of Selected Homemakers." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1963. Campos, L. P. "The Relationship Between "Anal" Person— ality Traits and TemporalExperience." .Unpublishéd Master's thesis, Michigan State UniverSity, 1959. Clough, S. B. Basic Values of Western Civilization. New York: Columbia University Press, 1960, pp. 7—8. 168 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 169 Cowles, M. L., and Dietz, R. P. ”Time Spent in Home— making Activities by a Selected Group of Wisconsin Farm Homemakers," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 48, No. 1, 1956, pp. 29L35. Cutler, V. F. "Personal and Family Values in the Choice of a Home."" AgriCultural Experiment Station Bulletin 840, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, 1947. Davids, A., Kidder, C., and Feich, M. “Time—Orienta— tion in Male and Female Juvenile Delinquents," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Vol. 64, 1962, pp. 239—240. Davids, A., and Sidman, J. "A Pilot Study——Impulsivity, Time—orientation and Delayed Gratification in Future Scientists and in Underachieving High School Students, Exceptional Children, Vol. 29, 1963, pp. 170—174. Davis, A. ”Socialization and Adolescent Personality," 43rd Yearbook Part I, National Society of the Study of Education, University of Chicago Press, 1944, pp. 198—216. Davis, A. Social Class Influence Upon Learning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1958. Deacon, R. E., and Bratton, E. C. ”Home Management—— Focus and Function," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 54, No. 9, 1962, p. 763. Deacon, R. E. and Maloch, F. ”Proposed Framework for Home Management," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 58, No. l, 1966, pp. 31-35. Dyer, D. M. ”Students' Wives Values as Reflected in Personal and Family Activities.” Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1962. Eldred, S. H., et g1. "A Procedure for the Systematic Analysis of Psychotherapeutic Interviews," Psychiatry, Vol. 17, 1954, pp. 337—345. Engebretson, C. L. "Analysis by a Constructed Typology of Wives' Values Evident in Managerial Dec1sion Situ— ations.” Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965. Eson, M. E. "An Analysis of Time Perspective at Five Year Levels." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Univer— sity of Chicago, 1951. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 170 Fink, H. H. "The Relationship of Time Perspective to Age, Institutionalization and Activity." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1953. Fisher, S. C. Relationships in Attitudes, Opinions, and Values Among Family Members. (University of California Publication in Culture and Society 11.) Berkeley, California: University of California Press, 1948. Fitcher, J. H. Sociology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1957, p. 301. Fraisse, P. The Psychology of Time. New York: Harper & Row, 1963. Frank, L. K. The Philosophyyof Home Management. Proceedings from Seventh International Management Congress. Washington, D. C.: National Management Council, 1938. Frank, L. K. Society as the Patient. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press, 1948. Geiger, G. Philosophy and The Social Order. Boston: Houghton—Mifflin Co., 1947, p. 167. Gist, N., and Halbert, L. Urban Society. New York: Crowell, 1956. Green, H., and Knapp, R. "Time Judgment, Aesthetic Preference and Need for Achievement," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Vol. 58, 1959, pp. 140-142. Gross, I. H. "Impact of Certain Basic Disciplines on Home Management in Family Living," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 58, No. 6, 1966, p. 448. Gross, I. H., and Crandall, E. W. Management for Modern Families, 2nd ed. New York: Appleton Century-Crofts, 1963. Grummon, D. L. "The Use of Language as a Method for the Study of Personality“and Psychotherapy." Unpub— lished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, 1950. Heenan, D. K. "Evaluation in the Humanities." Evalua— tion in Higher Education by Dressel, P. L., and Associates. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin Co., 1961, pp. 157-191. 171 37. Hill, E. A. "Human Values in Home Economics," Journal of Home Economics, V01. 47, No. 8, 1955, p. 592. 38. Israeli, N. "The Social Psychology of Time, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Vol. 27, 1932, pp. 209-213. 39. Jacob, P. E., Flink, J. J., and Schuchman, H. L. "Values and Their Function in Decision Making,“ The American Behavioral Scientist, Vol. 9, No. 9, May, 1962, p. 10. 40. Jaffe, J. "Dyadic Analysis of Two Psychotherapeutic Interviews." Comparative Psycholinguistic Analysis of Two Psychotherapeutic Interviews. Edited by ‘ Gottschalk, L. A. New York: International Univerr sities PreSs, 1961. ' 41. Kahn, P. "Time-orientation and Reading Achievement," Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol. 21, 1965, pp. 157— ,158. ' 42. Ketchum, F. N. "A Study of Homemaker's Values as Reflected in Time Used for Family and Personal Activi— ties." Unpublished Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1961. 43. Kimball, W J. "The Relationship Between Personal Values and The Adoption of Recommended Farm and Home Practices.” UnpubliShed Ph.D. dissertation, Univer— sity of Chicago, 1960. 44. Kinsey, A. C. Sexual Behavior in the Human Male. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders Co., 1948. 45. Kluckhohn, C., and others. "Values and Value-Orienta— tion in the Theory of Action." Toward a General Theory of Action. 'Edited by Parsons, T. and Shels, E. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1954, p. 395. 46. Kluckhohn, F. R., and Strodtbeck, F. L. Variation in Value-Orientations. ”Evanston: Row, Peterson ‘ ” and Company, 1961, pp. 1—20. 47. Knapp, R. H., and Garbutt, J. T. "Time Imagery and the Achievement Motive," Journal of Personality, Vol. 26, 1958, pp. 426-434. 48. Kohlmann, E. L. "Development of an Instrument to Deter— mine Values of Homemakers." Unpublished Ph.D. disser— tation, Iowa State University of Science and Technology. 1961. liiiliilfil I. I. i 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. S6. 57. 58. 59. 60. 61. 172 Krech, D., and CrutChfield, R. S. Theory and Problems in Psychology. New York? McGraw—Hill Company Inc., 1938. ' Laffey, J. J. "Impulsivity and Temporal Experience in Prisoners." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Mighigan State University, 1963. Lee, D. "Individuals in a Changing Society," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 52, No. 2, 1960, p. 79. Lennard, H. L., and Bernstein, A. The Anatomy of Psychotherapy. New York: Columbia University Press, .1960. ” LeShan, L. "Time—Orientation and Social Class," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psyphology, Vol. 47, 1952, pp. 539—592. Lewin, K. Field Theory in Social Science. New York: Harper and Brothers,1951. Lewin, K. "Time Perspective and Morale." Civilian Morale. Edited by Watson, G. New York: Houghton— Mifflin, 1942. Linton, R. Cultural Background of Personality. New York: D. Appleton Century Co., 1945, p. 11. Liston, M. Interrelationship of Values and Decision Making in Home Management. Proceedings from Confer— ence on Values and Decision Making in Home Management. Michigan State University, 1955, p. 65. Magrabi, F. M., Paolucci, B., and Heifner, M. E. "Framework for Studying Family Activity Patterns," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 59, No. 9, 1967, pp. 714-719. Malone, C. C., and Malone, L. H. Decision Making and Management for Farm and Home. Ames: The Iowa State College Press, 1958, p. 56. Martin, E. A. "Analysis by a Constructed Typology of Family Members' Values Evident in Managerial Decision Situations." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965. Mockmore, B. M. "Lasting Values in a Changing World," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 50, No. 10, 1958, p. 751. T 'llllllllli 1.-.- 11J 62. 63. 64. 65. 66. 67 68. 69. 70. 71. 73. 74. 75. 173 Murphy, G. “Social Tensions in India, American Academy of Political and Social Science, The Annuals, Vol. 276, 1951, pp. 35—42. Nelson, L. J. ”Daily Activity Patterns of Peasant Home— makers." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1963. Nichols, H. "The Psychology of Time," American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 3, 1891, pp. 451—529. Nickell, P., and Dorsey, J. M. Management in Family Living. 3rd ed., New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1959. Orshansky, M. ”Counting the Poor: Another Look at the Poverty Profile," Social Security Bulletin, Vol. 28, Jan. 1965, pp. 3—29. Paolucci, B. Home Management. A report prepared in the Golden Anniversary White House Conference on Children and Youth Conference. Washington, D. C., 1960, p. 146. Paolucci, B., and O'Brien, C. "Resources: The Tools of Management," Forecast for Home Economics, Vol. 76, March 1960, pp. 21-22. Schlater, J. D. "The Management Process and Its Core Concepts,“ Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 59, No. 2, 1967, pp- 93—98. Smeltzer, W. E. "Time-Orientation and Time Perspective in Psychotherapy.” Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1968. Spengler, O. The Decline of the West, Vol. I. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1926. Spinley, B. F. The Dsprived and the Privileged. London: Routledge and Keagan Paul, 1956. Steidl, R. E. "Continuity of Household Work.“ Agri— cultural Experiment Station Memoir 383, Cornell Univer— sity, Ithaca, New YOrk, 1963. Stein, H. D., and Cloward, R. A. (eds.) Social Perspectives on Behavior. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1958, p. 263. Straus, M. A. "Direct, Indirect, and Disguised Measure- ment in Rural Sociology." Agricultural Experiment Station Technical Bulletin 26, Washington State Univer— sity, Pullman, Washington, 1957. T lllllI].|||||l|z-JI|. .. J 174 76. Teahan, J. E. "Future Time Perspective and Academic Achievement." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1956. 77. Wallace, M. “An Investigation of Future Time Perspective in Schizophrenia." Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Michigan State University, 1954. 78. Warren, J. "Time: Resource or Utility," Journal of Home Economics, Vol. 49, No. l, 1957, pp. 20—22. 79. Warren, J. "Use of Time in Its Relation to Home Manage— Agricultural Experiment Station Bulletin 734, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, 1940. 80. Weintraub, W., and Arronson, H. "The Application of Verbal Behavior Analysis to the Study of Psychological Defense Mechanisms: Methodology and Preliminary Report," Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, Vol. 134, 1962, pp. 169—181. 81. Wiegand, E. "Use of Time by Full—time and Part—time Homemakers in Relation to Home Management." Agri— cultural Experiment Station Memoir 330, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, 1954. 82. Williams, R M. American Societyy A Sociological Inter— pretation. New York: Alfred Knopf, 1960, pp. 399—401. 83 Williams, R. M. “Value—Orientations in American Society.” Social Perspectives on Behavior. Edited by Stein, H. D., and Cloward, R. A. Glencoe: The Free Press, 1958, p. 289. 84. "Farmhouse Planning Guides, Household Activity Data and Space Needs Related to Design." Agricultural Experiment Station, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, 1959. 85. "STAT Series Description," No. 4. Agricultural Experi— ment Station, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 1969. Il.\|ll|| l J] APPENDIX A 175 u” llli J] I 176 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY mnxman-mmmnqwms COLLEGE OF HOME ECONOMICS - OFFICE OF THE DEAN ' HOME ECONOMICS BUILDING September 26, 1967 Dear Homemaker: The College of Home Economics and the Agricultural Experiment Station at Michigan State University want to study families so we can be of more help to all families. Miss Mabel Merrell would like to tell you about the study and give you an opportunity to take part in this study. If you have some questions about the study that are not answered or would like to verify information, call Dr. Frances Magrabi who is in charge of this project. Her phone number at Michigan State University is 355—7702. Sincerely yours, Pearl J. Aldrich Associate Dean, Research PJAsjm 176 T14 lil.| ‘Ill III..|J APPENDIX B 177 178 INTERVIEW FORM I Family ID Number Interviewer MICHIGAN AGRICULTURAL EXPERIMENT STATION SURVEY COLLEGE OF HOME ECONOMICS RESPONDENT Last Name First Husband's Name or Initials ADDRESS House Number or Box Street or R.R. Apt. Number CITY OR TOWN ZIP CODE TELEPHONE NUMBER Record of Contacts Date and Time Person Interviewed Reason for Non—Interview Location of Residence: Condition of Roads: General Impression of Interview: , . ‘ .. I ~ .. . .. -.,.._ I.~* ‘4. ,. ‘ I 1. N Residence: 179 Unincorporated Town: Open Country: Over 10 acres Yes No Population Value of Farm Produce Sold: Under $50.00 $50.00—$250.00 Over $250.00 Would you tell me, please, who in your family are living at home? Ages Sex HUSBAND WIFE CHILDREN Are you (the wife) employed outside the home? Yes No Uhat kind of work does your husband do? What was the last grade completed in school by: Your husband? Less than 8th Grade 8th Grade Some High School High School Graduate Technical or Business College some College College Degree 3325222 Less than 8th Grade 8th Grade Some High School High School Graduate Technical or Business College Some College College Degree . ,3 . ...." , . v ,.l .u. ‘ ‘ . . '1 . I. L‘ I. .‘ I .1 ._ ‘f -" n, r ' , ‘ . 8. 180 Taking into account your general financial situation—-~savings, assets, and debts-~how well off are you compared with five years ago as far as your net worth is concerned? (a) Better off (b) Worse off (c) About the same (If a or b) By about what amount are you better (or worse) off in: Income? Savings and Property? Debts? Why are you better (or worse) off? About what is your total family income now? (Use card) l About what was your total family income five years ago (in 1961?) . . ‘iv' . -" . c‘ 7 ., ‘ ._ . \ . . “an. r, L.__.__.i 181 ID Number Interviewer MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY AES PROJECT NO. 3029 Interview Form II 1. Family composition: (Include all who regard this as their permanent address). \ Name Sex Age Relationship Current marital status 2. Are there family members who live elsewhere? (college student, sales- man, grandparent). Proportion of Where they What periods each month on average Name spend time of time during previous year 3. Are you (the wife) employed outside of the home? Yes 1 (Go to 6) No No response \ON 4, What kind of work do you do? Professional Business professional Clerical or sales Labor (except farm) Farm labor .Agribuéiness Service Worker Other \onUIwar—IO \0 Doesn't apply L_____.__.i 182 ID Number 5. How many hours are you usually away from home on workdays, or is your employment for pay done at home? 4 or less 4+ to 8+ 9+ to 10+ 11 or more Not away Varies doesn't apply 6. Is your husband employed? (Go to 8) Yes No No response 7. Why is he not employed? School Health Retired Between jobs Out of work doesn't apply 8. What kind of work does your husband do? Professional Business professional Clerical or sales Labor (including agribusiness but 325 farm labor) Farm owner or manager Farm labor Service worker Other (Go to 12) Doesn't apply 9. Does your husband have more than one job? Yes No doesn't apply 10- Is your husband employed full-time or part-time? Full-time Part-time doesn't apply 11. Is your husband's employment at home away from home doesn't apply Ln-PLO Nl—‘O NH \O-PUJNHO NHO Ulww \l 183 ID Number 12. How many hours is your husband away from home on workdays? Less than 6 hours (50) 6 to 9+ hours 10 to 11+ 12 or more Doesn't apply (Omit if no children over 12 in household. Go to 14) 13. Are any children in your household employed even part-time? Yes No Doesn't apply (Omit if no other adults in household. Go to 15) 14. Or any other adults employed? Yes No Doesn't apply HO J in 1&4 I“- I "1.4 mofiumm umSD mcwusm 50% nufl3 msw>wa mcomumm wcw>aoq Mow commom news “now mo>fiuu< “mow oumum zucaoo ‘ no c308 nonesz oH .mefluhmE uzoz moch ONCE Mm.m£ucoz xam eo>aa m>me 30% £0w£3 Cw mowuwcsfifioo uconommww msu uwwa mmmmfim .HN r———_-=—.'-'P' 223 ACTIVITIES OF FAMILY MEMBERS 185 ID Number ~What are some of the activities or hobbies in which you or members of your family participate. Family Member Type of Activity Where it occurs Time Frequency ID Number 2 . 23. Where do you get some of the ideas on home and family life that are helpful to you. (Buying, household hints, recipes, entertain- ing ideas, crafts) Magazines Relatives Newspaper Friends or neighbors Television Home adviser Radio Doctor Books Church Bulletins Clubs and organizations Other 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. YOUR HEALTH tall 1 Height average 2 short 3 Weight thin 1 average 2 overweight 3 extremely overweight 4 ID Number How many days of work have you missed in the past year because of illness? Have you gone to a doctor during the past year? yes no On what occasions have you gone to the doctor during the past year? Do you use any physical aids? Check-up Major surgery Physical illness Accident Emotional distur- bance advice Other Doesn't apply Crutches Canes Braces Supports Wheelchair Artificial limbs Glasses Hearing aid Other Doesn't apply NH war-d \IO‘UI OGD‘IOUIJ-‘UJNH ...] VD 1 AA-4inniii Interview Form III MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY AES Project No. 3029 Food Buying and Use Column 1. How often do you buy food 2. Who usually buys the food for the family? 3. Before you buy food, do you plan the meals? 4, Do you prepare a sh0p- ing 113:? 5. Fresh potatoes ID Number During the past week did you buy or use any of the following: Interviewer Daily 0 Weekly 1 Several times weekly 2 Every two weeks 3 Monthly 4 Homemaker O Husband 1 Homemaker and Husband 2 Someone else 3 Homemaker 25 husband 4 Homemaker & someone else 5 Homemaker 2E someone else 6 Always 0 Sometimes 1 Never 2 Always 0 Sometimes 1 Never 2 Prepare but don't fol-5 ' low 3 prepare but buy others 4 Yes 1 No 0 .—~.—- "‘1 ~ \ n 'wm' ’1. 189 Colin: '6' 9c 10. 11 i 12- 13. 14- 154 16, l7 18. 19, 20.. Potato chips . Frozen, canned, or dried potatoes Frozen vegetables Frozen meat, poultry, or fis Frozen fruits or juices Frozen pie, cake, or cookies Frozen rolls or doughnuts Frozen dinners Cake mix Pudding or pie mix Quick bread mix Cooky mix Casserole mix Do you can or freeze extra food for future use? Does your family have the evening meal together? If not, why not? ID Number h Always Usually Seldom Never Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes _No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes Yes No Yes No Always except husband, who is working I—I H OH OH OH I-' v—A r—I v-I I—I OH OH OH r—I LDNI-‘O L”.___ Column 21. About how much time do you usually spend pre- paring breakfast? Who usually does this? 22» Packed lunches Who usually does this? 23. Noon meal Who usually does this? 24. Evening meal ‘Who usually does this? LAUNDRY ID Number 0 None less than 15 min. 15-30 min. 30-60 min. over 1 hr. None less than 15 min. 15-30 min. 30—60 min. over 1 hr. None less than 15 min. 15-30 min. 30-60 min. over 1 hr. None less than 15 min. 15-30 min. 30-60 min. over 1 hr. 25. Where do yOu do the laundry? a Kitchen Utility or laundry room Garage or lower level Bathroom or hall Outside room or porch Launderette Other 0 -D(» N!‘ <3 .biploea <3 a~u>n>~ «L‘LnNr-J ONUIJ-‘Lan—‘O 191 ID Number 26, On which day or days is the wash usually done? Sunday Monday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday Saturday Varies Everyday 27-28. About how many loads of clothes were washed in the last 7 days? 29. Where are the clothes usually dried? Clothesline Dryer, automatic Dryer and clothesline Launderette CLOTHING AND HOME SEWING 30. Who shops for most of the major clothing items for you? Homemaker Husband Other adult 31. Who shops for most of the major clothing for your husband? Homemaker Husband Other adult (Omit if no girls) 32. Who shops for the girls over 12? Homemaker Father Other adult Girl Combination 'Does not apply CONC‘U‘IDLONl-‘O LONI-‘O 0 l 2 I" \obwwn—IO 192 ID Number 33. Who shops for the girls under 12? Homemaker Father Other adult Girl Combination Does not apply (Omit if no boys) 34. Who shops for the boys over 12? 35. 36. 37. Homemaker Father Other adult Boy Combination Does not apply Who shops for the boys under 12? Homemaker Father Other adult Boy Combination Does not apply And for any other family members: Homemaker Husband Children Other adults Combination Does not apply Do you sew for your family? Yes 1 No 0 Patch & mend 3 . Does anyone else sew for members of your family? If yes, who? Yes 1 No 0 Patch & mend 3 \o-b-:> \O-Pldl90d o \DJ—‘ri-IO i l I 39. 40 41-42. 193 ID Number TRANSPORTATION How many cars do you have? How many of these are in running condition? Are there any other vehicles owned & used by the family. 43. How many members of the family drive? 44. Do you drive? Yes 1 No 0 EQUIPMENT CHECK LIST Card 3 Major appliances Column 1. Stove 7. Heating system None 0 None 0 Elec. or central 1 gas 1 space ' 2 other 2 other 3 (portable) 2. Refrigerator 8. Dryer None 0 None 0 mechani— has 1 cal 1 ~ natural 2 9. Vacuum 3. Freezer (separate) None 0 has ’~1 None 0 ‘ ‘ has 1 10. Television 4. Sink None 0 None 0 has 1 has 1 11. Hot water heater 5. Dishwasher None 0 None 0 gas or has 1 elec. 1 other 2 6. Washer None 0 Wringer 1 Automatic ’ . T'. r. —-—-—s——_—_——._ Small appliances (electric) Column 12. Iron None 0 has 1 13. Ironing board None 0 has 1 14. Electric fry pan None 0 has 1 15. Mixer None 0 has 1 16. Electric knife None 0 has 1 l7. Blender None 0 has 1 18. Popcorn popper 19. 20. 194 ID Number EQUIPMENT CHECK LIST (Cont'd) None 0 has 1 Waffle iron None 0 has 1 Griddle None 0 has 1 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. Coffeemaker None has Toaster None has Roaster None has Can opener None has Fan None has 1119.83.12 26. 27. 28. 29. Pots & pans None has Bread pans None has Cake pans None has Muffin tins None has H I—‘O ,_a HO 1 w i 195 ID Number Utensils (cont'd) 30. Pie pans 40. Dish pan None 0 None 0 has 1 has 1 31. Cookie sheets 41. Dish drainer None 0 None 0 has 1 has 1 32. Skillets 42. Canister None 0 None 0 has 1 has 1 33. Tea kettle Table service None 0 43. Dishes has 1 None 0 34. Can opener has 1 None 0 44. Glasses has 1 None 0 35. Knives has 1 None 0 45. Silverware has 1 None 0 36. Measuring cups has 1 None 0 46. Tableclothes has 1 Dish towels, etc. 37. Measuring spoons None 0 has 1 None 0 has 1 Miscellaneous 38. Mixing bowls 47. Cupboards 1 None 0 None 0 1 has 1 has 1 l 39. Egg beater 48. Garbage cans ‘ None 0 None 0 has 1 has 1 196 ID Number HOUSEHOLD ROUTINE We know that care of your house requires some of your time. Could you estimate the average time on a weekday that you Spend on (read list). Does not '49. 50. -51. 52. Cleaning, floorsmél: Does some other person frequently do or help with the following tasks? If so, who? (Read list of tasks) Mark only one. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 9 Other house- Daugh— hold Hired Combi-1 Doesn't No Husband Sons . ters member help nation" apply I l 53, Dishwashing Meal 54. Pre aration Family 55 . laundry 55. Troninz Daily 57._Ql.e§_niaau Weekly 58 ‘ cleaning Chauffeuring 59.family Gardening & ‘iawh mow- 60.~ ing — Caring for family 61.members 52,‘Bedmaking r—r lil‘ll..|‘|l|.l.l I!" ..I ll .1 J 197 ID Number 63. What household tasks do you like to do? dislike doing? 64. Do you have any seasonal jobs that you feel are important? canning, cleaning IIIII' l|.l| 1.1.1.1.].1 1.1 II IIJ 198 FORM IV ID Number CONDITION OF HOME Rate home with respect to each factor, on a scale ranging from 1 to 5 as follows: l 2 4 5 Extremely Extremely poor good A. Cleanliness W B. Orderliness C. State of repair of building & permanent fixtures D. State of repair of furnishings and equipment M E. Shabbiness 199 Amwoa Bot mumowmcwv Ammumozwummfiaoamfim u.xom3 use mowumm mamas mmouuxmemms mmmnm>< mo mummmg .pmuswm .pn..woumrm .ALOH ucwmomm umouxmv .mmwzoz momma comp ymwmoa mom was: 05 Lemma mp0“ emu mass umwa .moaunwa ones 50% moamm no mumm> m umma emu wsflusm was; won 924mmpm Mbow some? mQOm emu HHw swam menu so umflg sowumsuomcw ofiaofioom a “sowhoamam » > amom 3mw>uoueH T "US$00“ “mom. maw>mou wewusmum memo» Hmuow 0H Scum .x3\mmso£ mofludm mauflw >uoflmm Hmsnsm oumEonnnnm undemoHQEm we momma we .ou .Mosamm .mmHZQE momma meme umeOH How mam: 50% seems mQOm on» mauo umflg I 11; I01 )ll...‘ +3 ID Number What is your present take-home pay? Husband $ Wife 5 What deductions aremade by your employer on your present job? Husband Wife Social Security 1 Insurance 2 Savings 3 Union 4 Taxes 5 Other Did any other members of the family have any income? Yes 1 No 2 If yes ' Length & Who Job Amount of Pay Frequency In addition to his salary, did your husband receive any income from bonuses, overtime or tips? $ Yes 1 No 2 Doesn't apply 9 Did you receive any income from bonuses, overtime or tips? $ Yes 1 No 2 Doesn't apply 9 202 ID Number id your family receive income from other sources? Yes 1 No 2 f yes, what was your income from each source? trade, or second job $ arming or market gardenidg $ oomers, boarders, or foster children $ my other self-employment $ ent $ nterest $ eteran's pension, veteran's sthoobnallotment $ nemployment compensation, union benefits $ acial security $ limony or regular contributions from public welfare, ther government aid $ ther (Specify or describe) 3 a you carry any life insurance? asband (Face value) ife ther family members a you own or rent your home? Per month Rent Current market value H“ W Home owner you own other real property? Yes 1 No 2 pproximate market value? $ ID Number you own your home or other property, do you have a mortage any of these properties? Yes, on one Yes, on both No Doesn't apply >roximately how much is your present mortage? first mortage Second mortggg $_“ $ ments per month First mortage Second mortage $ $ ut how much would you receive if you were to trade in or sell your omobile or other vehicles? lst S 2nd $ 3rd S W 4th S t is the total charge for car or truck insurance for your family? $ you own other major items of equipment (boat, camping equipment, ting or fishing equipment, power tools, lawn mower)? Condition Wm— M 204 ID Number ince you moved here have you made any additions or improvements 0 your home? Explain: $W toes your family have any savings program (including what the employer leducts from your check)? (banks, bonds, credit unions) Yes 1 No 2 Type Amount Invested 30 you or others in your family have any checking accounts at banks? Yes 1 No 2 $__________._______ iave you inherited any money or property in the last five years? H Yes (amount or value) $_~_____“_____~_ No 2 Do you have any other money owed to you? $ 205 ID Number Do you owe money on any personal loans not already discussed? From what source did you borrow? How much are the payments? How many payments do you have left to make? Bank per per week month per per week month Credit Union per per week month per per week month Small Loan Company per per week month per per week month Friend or relative per per week month per per week month Other (please specify). per per per per week month week month 25. 206 ID Number Do you owe any money now on anything you have bought on the installment plan, with revolving accounts, or for anything on which you make regular monthly or weekly payments? 26. If yes, please fill in the following information: Yes 1 No 2 2% did you buy, or what e you making payments on? How much are the payments? How many payments do you hrve left to make? per week per month__u_~ week _ month week month week month ‘ per per week month per per week month per per week month per per week month 2,07 ID Number Each family has certain expenses which occur regularly. What do you spend on each? __§§pg§§iture flgngy Time Period Food $ Clothing $ Medical $ Auto expenses $ Utilities (heating, water, $ electricity, telephone) Other Specify) 208 ACTIVITY RECORD SHEET Persons present: Codes: Page: Time Activity and Materials Space Activity Family ID No.: l . . ' N .: Situation : Observation 0 ‘ Date : Observer : C X I D m P P A 9 O 2 210 ACTIVITY RECORD CODE SHEET Family No. Observation No. Coder Date Min. Space Activ- Person Mat'l or Other Description of ity 1—10 11-20 21—30 Acting Per.Acted Mtl's Activity On 31-40 41—50 51—66 Notezcolumn numbers refer to location on IBM cards. .ovoo pamHUIHo3OH on: ,omwu co>am m defines omomuom woo QMSD mHOE mo>umm Eoou m MH . . . . . * ozonx Ins cofluoooa iBmfl> GH #02 omsom woflmuoo mun I>fluom mo GQMp woo soap oHoE mo>uom I Soon omomusmIHuaoz .ouo .suucm ,Hamm Eoou mmMHOpm soom snaaabo Ho quEmwmm HmH mend oue>nom mad Hma HmH Hoa Nma apom Ava mou< wuou HMGOmHom 211 Eoonvmm Hma mwu< mcfimwoam coo ~ooflwwo MNH Eoom mcfi>flq HmEHom NNH Eoom waflEom HNH mon< mQH>Hq soom menses «Ha i marches HHH mood moom *oUOU oUMMm mo\om\aa e Czocxub omwa I Hmflumumz m moflammdm Honouuom meDOHU ommuowm Honsuosuum ,mouduosupm omsom wmsm imuo>oo meam imcflmuuso I moafiuxme maoaomsom Hosoo mHoBOB omcwcflq can mnflwwwm musuflcusm tflwoau OHQMB muocflmuooo mom tmufloflupomam tHoDMB ~mHoBOB Hommm ,moeHQQSm omsom moom I muqmfiUonGH .opo bosom twoflmflm .maooE .mHHmeDD mcHHSmooz Ho moaxflz unofimflswm mHQoDHom prEmfldvm mmHMH Ho CH Daflsm ms _ msoocmaaoomflz HsAmHMflnwumE Ho wcomuom mov HOHDGOO Hem mm Aameflom Ho comuom m opvmofi>nom Hmm He manned use nouns co GHDB ommuoum Eonm w>oz New Hm mpflmflq Ugo Mmgmz wwo CHSB wmmuowm OD ®>OS Hum mu HomMHOQm Ne ommHObm nom mmm an own now Hmm "mcflcoflufiwcou mm maneuopmz moummwum mm IhHHMHpHom apes coabmnmmmnm NNN Hm mamauopoz venom SDH3 COHumummem ANN ”coauosupucoo I coflumnmmoum mm mm HMHHmeE m>HuomDOHm mcflwaamm Nam Hm DHHQ mca>oeom I moflcmoau Ham "mcflcmoao owoo mHoHHmDmZ_ mooo mmaun>auua zmom MQOU Qmoomm MBH>HBU¢ 212 CONVERSATION CODE SHEET Persons (code, names, age, sex) Page Family ID No. Observation No, Coder Date 1—3 10—11 12-13 14—15 16-17 Conversation Message 43 44 Time + — — — Units Content Time Note: column numbers refer to location on IBM cards. APPENDIX D .mmH#H>Hpom mo muomwpmo mHMHngmE w>Hoompoum OCHhHQO mQHODHUxm mo mmsmomn vow rmcHOCSOH mo mmdmumfl OOH Hmqu poo ow mommpcmuumm mEom* II II II H.O I: II II II c30c¥n5 GoHpmN |oH 3wH> CH #02 ¢.m II m.N m.O O.m h.H II h.m wmdon wOHmuoo II II I: II II II II II Econ omomusmeHoE o.H I- m.o 0.0 H.o ¢.N N.o m.o wuucm ‘Hamm m.o n- I- N.H «.0 ¢.H «.0 I- goo“ mmmnoum m.m m.m N.O m.m w.h O.m m.o N.N Eoou thHde Ho ucmfiwmmm ¢.H II m.m H.N n.O O.m m.O w.v Eoouflpmm m.o II N.¢ H.¢ m.m N.w H.N H.H Eoouwmm 4 m.O II 0.0 m.O II woo woo II com ronmwO 1 2 m.ON m.O H.mm ¢.m O.¢H H.OH v.mH O.¢H EOOH OGH>HH H.O I: m.O II N.O N.O II II Eoou hHHEmm H.m II m.o n.H O.NH m.¢ m.NH O.H Eoou OGHGHQ 0.0% m.mm m.Om m.h> N.>m O.NO ODNO ¢.Nh cmnopHm OOH OOH OOH OOH OOH OOH OOH OOH smoonm00H HH4 R $ N fi X X X X OCH wmsom mflw CH mmeH>Hpo< coHvosupmqou wUH>Hmm wmmyouw mms wmmuogm.®mmuo#m ICmMHo GOHpmooq Houpcoo flaw Scum Mom op How , GOHpMmeme mQH>OS .Ucoo OQH>02 ovcoo moUHSOme >HH>HQUM COHomESmcoo >pH>HuUm 3w: wpmwno OGH>gwmnoo wouoomwm Ho unwemd< ! >uH>Huo¢ mo ma>a .mmHHHamm sauflsu an mmsoa map mo mCOHuwooH #cmHmMMHU cw UmEHOmumm mmeH>Hoom mo mOCHM pawHwMMHU Mo omwocmouwm .Nm anmB 215 Table 33. Percentage of fiotal activities performed in each location of the house by thirty families. Percent of Location total activity All locations* 100 Kitchen 64.3 Living room 16.8 Dining room 5.5 Basement or utility room 3.8 Bedroom 3.7 Bath 2.2 Outside 2.2 Hall, entry 0.8 Office, den 0.3 Family room 0.1 Unknown “" *Percentage does not total 100 because of rounding. 216 Table 34. Resource conserving activities in relation to material of central focus of the activity in order from highest to lowest percentages. Percent of Item activity Cleaning activities with the following materials acted upon: Utensils, dishes, pans 32.7 House structure and structural storage 20.4 Built—in or large equipment 12.4 Furniture 8.0 Containers 3.3 Other household textiles 3.0 Portable equipment 3.2 Other* 9-0 Conditioning—forestorage activities with the following materials acted upon: Clothes 56 8 Food 18 8 Utensils, dishes, pans 6.5 Bedding 5 6 Portable equipment 3 8 Other* 8 5 Moving-to—storage activities with the following materials acted upon: Utensils, dishes 22.8 Food 19.4 Household supplies 13.9 Clothes 13.6 Miscellaneous 7.6 l Containers 5.1 , Bedding 5.1 Personal supplies 3.7 1 Other* 8. 8 l Total RCA with the following materials acted ‘ upon: % Utensils, dishes 30.0 House structure and structural storage 16.9 Clothes 11.8 Food 8.2 Built—in or large equipment 7.3 1 Furniture 5.0 Household supplies 4.6 Containers 3.6 ; Miscellaneous 2.8 1 Other* 9,8 *A composite of materials upon which less than 10 percent of activities were acted. 217 Table 35. Utilization activities in relation to materials directly utilized in the activity in order from highest to lowest percentages. Percent of Item activity Conditioning—for—use activity with the following materials directly utilized: Clothes 52.1 Food 16.9 Utensils, dishes, etc. ' 10.4 Household supplies 6.2 Bedding 3.9 Other household textiles 2.8 Other* 7.7 Moving—from—storage activity with the following materials directly utilized: Utensils, dishes 30.7 Food 25.9 Household supplies 21.1 Miscellaneous 5.2 Clothes 4.4 Bedding 3.8 Other* 8.9 Total utilization activity with the following materials directly utilized: Clothes 31.5 Food 20.8 ‘ Dishes 19.1 1 Household supplies 12.6 ‘ Bedding 3.8 Miscellaneous 3.7 Other* 8.5 *A composite of material upon which less than 10 percent of activities were acted. Table 36. Control activities and activities which augment and/or create new resources in relation to material directly used in the activity in order from highest to lowest percentages. Percent of Item activity Control activities with the following material directly used: Miscellaneous 15.2 Unknown material 9.8 Clothes 8.6 Food 6.8 Personal supplies 4.3 Other household textiles 3.7 Other* 51.6 Preparation and construction activities with the following material directly acted upon: Food 97.5 Other** 2.5 *Includes 35.1% in which persons were acted upon. The rest is a composite of materials. **A composite of materials which less than 10 percent of activities were acted upon them. , 1," vii Wm