EEHESES ”am” I Hmflamu‘“ ,. , mama? Michigan State University \ This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Achievement in Similarity Tasks: ' Effect of Instruction, and Relationship with Achievement in Spatial Visualization at the Middle Grades Level presented by Alex Friedlander has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D . degree in Teacher Education aux/(2; m azmw Major professo/d \ Dr. William M. Fitzgerald Date June 27, 1984 MSU i: an Affirmative Action/Equal Oppurrmuly Inrlilun'on 0-12771 ll ll 3 129 « ll llll‘llmlllllllllll L )V1531_J RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to LlBRARlES gemove this checkout from Ali-[3-IIL. our record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. {Egi}. “J" i 200 031:7 7 '5 ML '1 W ; 5 , JAN 5 / " 2° man—sumo; ACHIEVEMENT IN SIMILARITY TASKS: EFFECT OF INSTRUCTION, AND RELATIONSHIP WITH ACHIEVEMENT IN SPATIAL VISUALIZATION AT THE MIDDLE GRADES LEVEL By Alex Friedlander A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Teacher Education 1984 ")Icl'r/V ,7 37"". ABSTRACT ACHIEVEMENT IN SIMILARITY TASKS: EFFECT OF INSTRUCTION AND RELATIONSHIP WITH ACHIEVEMENT IN SPATIAL VISUALIZATION AT THE MIDDLE GRADES LEVEL BY ALEX FRIEDLANDER Purpose This study had three related purposes: (1) to determine any existing differences in similarity achievement by grade level and by sex, prior to, and after instruction pertaining to similarity, (2) to ascertain any existing differences between a verbal and a ‘visual presentation of similarity tasks, and (3) to study the relationship between performance on similarity tasks and performance on spatial visualization tasks. The sample (N==67S) was drawn from five schools with a suburban, middle-class, midwestern, predominantly white population and with teachers that volunteered to teach the Similarity Unit--an instructional unit developed by the Middle Grades Mathematics Project for grades six, seven and eight. Analysis of Variance, and Analysis of Repeated Measures served as the main statistical tools to test the research hypotheses. Main.Finding§ Pre, and post instructional performance on four similarity-related topics were analyzed: (1) basic properties of similar shapes, (2) proportional reasoning, (3) area relationships of similar shapes, and (4) applications. ,At a significance level of .05: no sex differences in pre-instructional achievement or gains were observed; achievement increased as a function. of grade level; and seventh. graders gained significantly more when compared to the sixth and eighth graders. Pre- instructional performance on area growth tasks was uniformly poor (18-22 percent) and the gains of the sixth graders as a result of instruction were particularly low (5 percent). Performance on four similarity tasks presented verbally was compared with performance on four equivalent tasks accompanied by figures. Although no overall difference between the two presentation modes could be detected, different presentation modes seemed to be favored for different similarity-related topics. Student performance on a spatial visualization test before and after instruction on similarity indicated significant gains. For a restricted sample (N=161) performance in similarity tasks of students that underwent instruction in spatial visualization one year before this study was compared with performance of students that did not undergo this kind of instruction. In this case, instruction in spatial visualization did not have a significant effect on achievement in similarity tasks. Sara, Amit, Ronen -- my family Professors William Fitzgerald (Chair), Richard Houang, Glenda Lappan, Perry Lanier, and Bruce Mitchell -- my doctoral committee. David Ben-Haim and many other Israeli and American friends. Sharon Tice -- my typist. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES . . . .'. . . . . Chapter I. INTRODUCTION . II. III. IV. Background . The Study . REVIEW OF LITERATURE . Introduction . . . . . . . The Development of the Similarity Concept The Development of Proportional Reasoning The Concepts of Area and Area Growth . Geometry and Spatial Visualization . Sex Differences . . . . . . The Effect of Instruction DESIGN OF THE STUDY . . . . Purposes of the Study Population and Sample Instrumentation . . . Instructional Material . Procedure and Data Collection . . The Statistical Design of the Study ANALYSIS OF DATA AND RESULTS Introduction . . . . . . . . . . Ere-Instructional Performance in Four Similarity-Related Areas . Effect of Instruction in Four Similarity- Related Areas iii Page“ vii H 13 13 13 19 31 36 41 45 57 57 58 61 64 67 68 7O 7O 72 76 Chapter Pre-Instructional Performance on Tasks Presented in a Verbal, and a Visual Mode . . . . . Effect of Instruction on Performance in Verbal.and in Visual.Task . . . . . . . . . . . Effect of Instruction in Similarity on Spatial Visualization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Effect of Instruction in Spatial Visualization on Performance in Similarity Tasks . . . . . . . . V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS-. Significance of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . Design of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Main Findings and Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . Pedagogical Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . Recommendations for Further Research . . . . . . . . APPENDICES Appendix A. MGMP SIMILARITY TEST: ITEMS INCLUDED IN THE FOUR SUBTESTS: ITEMS INCLUDED IN THE TWO PRESENTATION MODES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . B. MGMP SPATIAL VISUALIZATION SUBTEST: SAMPLE ITEMS C. THE MGMP SIMILARITY TEST AND THE MGMP SPATIAL VISUALI- ZATION TEST: RELIABILITY COEFFICIENTS, AND PRETEST- POSTTEST CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS . . . . . . . D. THE MGMP SIMILARITY UNIT: OVERVIEW . E. MEAN SCORES, STANDARD DEVIATIONS AND CORRELATIONS RELATED TO THE RESEARCH QUESTIONS . . . REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . iv Page 82 86 92 98 104 104 105 107 119 121 124 133 135 137 139 146 ’ Table 2.1 3.1 3.2 3.3 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 4.8 4.9 4.10 LIST OF TABLES The Van Hiele Levels of Development in Geometry . Distribution of Participating Classes and Teachers by Grade Level and by School . . . . . . . Sample Ae-Distribution of Students by Grade Level and by S ex 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Sample B--Distribution of Students by Grade Level and by Experience in Spatial Visualization (SV) Instruction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Design of the 3 x 2 Multivariate Analysis of Variance for Hypothesis 1.1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of the 3 x 2 Multivariate and Univariate Analysis of Variance for Hypothesis 1.1 3 x 2 Multivariate Repeated Measures Design for Hypothesis 1.2 . . . . . . Summary of Multivariate and Univariate Analysis of Repeated Measures for Hypothesis 1.2 . . . . Design of the 3 x 2 Bivariate Analysis of Variance for Hypothesis 2.1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of the 3 x 2 Bivariate Analysis of Variance for Hypothesis 2.1 3 x 2 Multivariate Repeated Measures Design for Hypothesis 2.2 Summary of Analysis of Repeated Measures for Hypothesis 2.2 . . 3 x 2 Multivariate Repeated Measures Design for Hypothesis 3.1 . . Summary of Analysis of Repeated Measures for Hypothesis 3.1 Page 47 S9 60 60 73 74 77 79 83 84 87 89 93 96 Table 4.11 4.12 C.1 0.2 E.1 E.2 E.3 E.4 E.5 E.6 2 x 2 x 2 Univariate Repeated Measures Design for Hypothesis 3.2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of Analyses of Repeated Measures for Hypothesis 3.2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Reliability Coefficients--Cronbach a for the MGMP Similarity Test and for the MGMP Spatial Visualia zatiOn Subtest by Time, by Grade, and by-Sex Pearson Correlation Coefficients between Pretest and Posttest Scores on the MGM? Similarity Test and on the MGMP Spatial Visualization Subtest by Grade and by Sex . . . . . . . . . . . Mean Scores (in percent) and Standard Deviations on Four Similarity-Related Areas for Sample A by Grade Level and by Sex . . . . . . . . . . Correlations among Pre-instructional performance on Four Similarity-Related Areas for Sample A by Grade 0 O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O 0 Mean Scores (in percent) and Standard Deviations on Test Items Presented in a Verbal and a Visual Mude for Sample A by Grade Level and by Sex . Correlations among Pre, and Post-instructional Performance on Similarity and Spatial Visuali— zation Tasks for Sample A by Grade Mean Scores (in percent) and Standard Deviations on Similarity and on Spatial Visualization for Sample A by Grade Level and by Sex . . . . . . . Mean Scores (in percent) and Standard Deviations on Similarity and on Spatial Visualization for Sample B by Level of Instruction in Spatial Visualization by Grade and by Sex vi Page 100 101 135 136 139 140 141 142 143 144 Figure 4.1 4.2 4.3 LIST OF FIGURES Properties Invariant Under Transformation Group Performance by Grade Level in Four Similarity- Related Areas, Prior to, and After Instruction Performance by Grade Level in Tasks Presented in a Visual, and a Verbal Mode, Prior to, and After Instruction . . . . . . . . . . . . . Performance by Grade Level on the Similarity Test and on the Spatial Visualization Subtest, Prior to, and After Instruction . . . . . . . . . Gains in Four Similarity—Related Topics--Sample A vii Page 14 78 88 95 111 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background The Concgpt of Similarity The acquisition of the similarity concept is important to the development of children's geometrical understanding of their environment, and of prOportional reasoning. Phenomena that require familiarity with enlargement, scale factor, projection, area growth, indirect measurement and other similarity-related concepts are frequently encountered by children in their immediate environment and in their studies of natural and social sciences. Since Felix Klein's Erlanger Programm--a classification of geometrical transformations, similarity has been recognized as an important mathematical concept. Piaget's (1960) developmental theory of the child's understanding of geometrical concepts added a cognitive aspect to research on the concept of similarity. Piaget viewed the ability to make similarity judgments as an intermediate stage in children's developmental path from a topological to a Euclidean perception of the environment. Many researchers (e.g., Martin, 1976b; ‘Lesh, 1976; Schultz, 1978) raised serious questions about Piaget's hierarchical view of the child's construction of his spatial reality, and about the relevance of his theory to mathematics education. However, researchers seem to agree that around the age of 9, children may make perceptual judgments of similarity (as for example, in comparing two triangles) and that around the age of 11 children may have the mental ability to make a gradually increasing use of proportions (as expressed in judging similarity of rectangles). A poor performance on similarity tasks (Carpenter et a1., 1981) may indicate existing deficiencies in the instruction of geometry in general (Wirszup, 1976), and of the similarity concept in particular. Fuson (1978) points out the need for instruction in similarity: Similarity ideas are included in many parts of the school curriculum. Some models for rational number concepts are based on similarity; thus, part of student's difficulty with rationals may stem from problems with similarity ideas. Ratio and proportion are part of the school curriculum from at least the seventh grade on, and they present many difficulties to the student. Standardized tests include many proportion word problems. Verbal analogies (a:b::c:d) form major parts of many intelligence tests. Similar geometric shapes would seem to provide a helpful mental image for other types of proportion analogy situations. Training studies of teaching experiments concerning ways to teach geometric similarities and ways to generalize the solution of geometric proportions to other types of proportion would be valuable. (p. 259) Proportional Reasoning The concept of similarity is an instance of proportionality. Proportional reasoning is frequently required in mathematics, natural science and everyday life. Chiapetta and McBride (1978) for example, found among a sample of ninth graders a positive relationship between the ability to reason proportionally and knowledge and understanding of simple machines, concepts on the structure of matter, and applications of equivalent fractions. The importance of prOportional reasoning in a child's intellectual development can hardly be overemphasized. Inhelder and Piaget (1958) consider it one of the six abilities that characterize the formal- operational thinker. Due to its wide-spread use, the concept of proportionality has been investigated more systematically than similarity. The Karplus studies would be an instance of a thorough analysis of proportional reasoning as a function of age, sex, social status, and nationality. Besides attempts to verify Piaget's cognitive stages in the deve10pment of proportionality, more detailed studies suggest that task-related variables are equally important when one investigates performance in proportionality. The variables that have been recommended for consideration are: (1) level of abstractness (Wollman & Karplus, 1974; Portis, 1973), (2) ratio versus fraction (i.e., part-to-part versus part- to-whole) presentation (Wachsmuth, Behr & Post, 1980), (3) level of numerical difficulty (Abramowitz, 1975; Karplus, Pulos & Stage, 1980), (4) task sequencing (Karplus, 1978), and (5) irrelevant information (Collea, & Numadel 1978). Furthermore, sex differences on performance of proportionality tasks in form of male superiority are also indicated by some studies (Keating & Schaefer, 1975; Stage, Karplus & Pulos, 1980). Several studies indicate a generally poor performance on proportionality tasks at various ages and stress the need for instructional interventions (e.g., Lovell & Pumfrey, 1966; Renner & Paske, 1977; Pagni, 1983). Consequently, units on proportional reasoning have been designed and evaluated from the middle grades level (Wollman & Lawson, 1978) through high school (Kurz & Karplus, 1977), and even at the college level (Pagni, 1983). Research indicates that in most cases mastery of abstract proportional reasoning may be expected only above the age of 14-15, if at all (Lovell, 1972). Piagetian research (e.g., Piaget & Inhelder, 1967) shows that similarity tasks may be mastered two or three years earlier than other proportionality schemes. Therefore, the concept of geometrical similarity may be a step towards an understanding of proportionality and research findings in either of these fields should be relevant to the other. The Middle Grades Mathematics Project One of the four instructional units designed by the Middle Grades Mathematics Project (MGMP) is on the concept of similarity. The purpose of the MGMP Similarity Unit is to provide a response to the present deficiencies in the instruction of geometry. By offering a rich variety of experiences designed to fit the first two levels in the Van Hiele model of development in geometrical understanding, the unit attempts to build a solid base for further advance in the understanding of geometry in general, and of similarity and prOportionality in particular (Lappan, 1983). At the middle grades level, the importance of teaching informal geometry is not contested. Due to the present deficiencies in the understanding of the most basic geometrical concepts (Carpenter et al., 1981), and to the recommendations mentioned before, a problem-solving oriented, activity-based, informal geometry unit of instruction seems warranted. The Study The'Purpose This study is concerned with the performance of middle-grade students in similarity tasks prior to instruction with the MGMP Similarity Unit, and with the impact of instruction of the unit on their performance. The basic goal of the study is to detect the extent to which the MGMP instructional intervention helps to overcome the cognitive difficulties that exist at this critical transitional age. There are three purposes for this study. The first is to determine any existing differences in similarity achievement in general and in its four identified content components by grade level and by sex, prior to, and after instruction pertaining to similarity. Second, to ascertain any existing grade level and sex differences in performance on similarity tasks presented in a verbal or a visual mode. The third purpose is to study relationships between performance on similarity tasks and performance in spatial visualization. Accordingly, the study will examine performance in similarity tasks from three different aspects: 1. 2. A classification by content will distinguish among tasks requiring: (1) recognition of similar shapes and of their properties, (2) proportional reasoning, (3) use of the area relationship between similar shapes, and (4) applications of the similarity concept. Two research questions will be asked on the performance of sixth, seventh, and eighth graders in similarity tasks. The first question is concerned with grade level and sex differences in pre-instructional performance, whereas the second inquires about the effect of instruction (i.e., the existence of gains) on achievement in the four similarity-related topics mentioned above. A classification by presentation mode will distinguish between (1) visual tasks which include drawings of the involved geometric shapes, and (2) verbal tasks which contain only a word description of the geometric situation. In this study, two research questions are related to pre-instructional performance and gains on the verbal and the visual subtests. These questions are concerned with grade level and sex differences in achievement on the items presented in the two modes. 3. The relationship with spatial visualization will be examined by analyzing performance on spatial visualization tasks in parallel to performance in similarity tasks. Two research questions are concerned with the possibility of a relationship between achievement in similarity tasks and spatial visualization. The first question inquires about the effect of instruction in similarity on spatial visualization, whereas the second deals with the effect of instruction in spatial visualization on performance in similarity tasks. Significance of the Study One of the goals of this study is to determine patterns in the performance of middle-grade students in geometrical similarity tasks. Investigations on geometrical similarity that were conducted in school settings are rare. Grade level differences (i.e., level of performance increasing with age) would confirm the developmental nature of understanding the concept of similarity. This study may contribute to the present knowledge on the complex issue of sex differences in mathematical performance: male superiority among students of this age has been indicated in spatial visualization (e.g., Ben-Haim, 1983), and in proportional reasoning (e.g., Brendzel, 1978). In regard to geometry, the issue of sex differences is less clear: some studies tend to detect sex differences in relation to informal geometry tasks (Shonberger, 1976; Werdelin, 1961), whereas other studies indicate contradictory results (Olson, 1970; Thomas, 1977; G. D. Peterson, 1973). Another goal of this study is to determine the effect of instruction with the MGMP SimilaritygUnit on the performance of middle-grade students in similarity tasks . Significant gains in performance in similarity and/or spatial visualization would indicate the effectiveness of the instructional intervention and strengthen the claims that deficient geometrical understanding may be improved by using apprOpriate teaching strategies and materials . The use of concrete models and of a problem-solving orientation appears to be superior in the instruction of geometry (Bring, 1972; Buchert, 1980; Hempel, 1981). These observations support the educational implications of cognitive research on the acquisition of geometrical concepts. However, cognitive research also sets limitations on the effect of instruction: instructional interventions may accelerate cognitive development, but cannot substitute it (Montangero, 1976). Some studies support this view by indicating a lack of instructional effect as a result of their subjects' low cognitive level (Young, 1975; Wirszup, 1976). P.M. and Dina Van Hiele identified five cognitive levels in the development of geometrical understanding, and more importantly, showed how to adapt the instruction to the limitations set by these levels. Present deficiencies in the understanding of geometry in the United States are attributed by Wirszup (1976) to instructional, rather than cognitive limitations. He presents as evidence the significant results obtained by a Soviet reform in the geometry curriculum that relied on the Van Hiele model. The teaching of geometry in an informal manner is generally accepted at the elementary' and ‘middle grades level, and recommended. by' some mathematics educators even at the high school level (J. C. Peterson, 1973). Transformational geometry was also considered as an alternative for the traditional high school curriculum. However, the results of some related evaluation studies were disappointing (Olson, 1971; Usiskin, 1972; Durapau, 1979)--a fact that can be explained in part by observing that new ways of instruction were used without changing in any significant way the instructional goals or the evaluation tools of the traditional curriculum. A third goal of this study is to determine the effect of instruction with the MGMP Similarity Unit on the performance of middle-grade students in spatial visualization. If a relationship between spatial visualization and performance in similarity tasks is indicated by this study, a certain sequence between the MGMP Similarity Unit and Spatial Visualization Unit may be recommended. More general conclusions on the role of spatial visualization in informal geometry tasks may also be drawn. There are strong cognitive and pedagogical reasons to assume a close relationship between the ability of spatial visualization and the ability to acquire geometrical abilities (Piaget, 1964; Hoffer, 1977). Some studies investigated the relationship between Spatial visualization and geometrical abilities in general. In sharp contrast to spatial visualization, geometrical abilities do not have a clear definition, 10 and have not been widely investigated. Studies that analyzed achievement in plane Euclidean geometry (e.g., Holzinger & Swineford, 1946; Werdelin, 1961; Hanson, 1972) are more likely to find connections with verbal or reasoning abilities rather than with spatial visualization. On the other hand, studies that consider less formal aspects of geometry were able to show a connection between geometry and spatial visualization. This relationship reveals itself, at least from the middle grades level and on, through the improvement in spatial visualization that was shown to occur as a result of instruction in informal geometry (Van Voorhis, 1941; Brinkmann, 1966; Battista, Wheatley & Talsma, 1982). Population, Sample and Instrumentation The sample used in this study consisted of sixth, seventh, and eighth graders from five schools in the area of Lansing, Michigan. All five schools have a middle-class, predominatly white student pOpulation and may be considered as typical, midwestern suburban schools. The tests used in this study have been developed as evaluation tools for two MGMP units: The MGMP Similarity Test consists of 25 multiple choice items that assess a variety of similarity-related tasks (for sample items, see Appendix A); the MGMP Spatial Visualization Subtest (Appendix B) consists of 15 multiple choice items chosen from the original test that has been designed to evaluate the MGMP Spatial Visualization Unit. Both tests were administered to all the sampled students prior to, and after a two to three-week-long instruction with the MGMP Similarity Unit . 11 Limitations In order to allow the evaluation of relatively large number of students, this study used paper-and-pencil tests. Although the items are concerned with a variety of concepts related to similarity, the presented situations resemble closely the ones presented in the instructional unit. Consequently, tasks that require proportional reasoning in other than geometrical settings have not been considered. Research indicates that many task-related variables may influence the performance in prOportionality tasks. Not all of them can be controlled in a single study. The level of abstractness varies according to the visual or verbal presentation of items; manipulatory aids were not included in the process of testing. However, all tasks relate to geometrical situations that are considered more concrete than some other proportionality tasks (e.g., the Balance Task, numerical proportions, or some verbal story problems). The computations involved in the similarity tasks tested are of a numerically moderate level of difficulty. Sequencing of tasks according to level of difficulty or inclusion of irrelevant or redundant information has not been employed in the test items. Although socio-economical status (SES) was indicated as a variable that influences performance in proportionality tasks (Karplus & Peterson, 1970; Karplus, Karplus & Paulsen, 1977), the sample used by this study is limited mainly to middle-class students from suburban areas. 12 The conclusions that can be drawn from this study will be therefore limited to the effect of a specific instructional intervention (i.e., the MGMP SimilarigLUnit), at the middle-grades level, on middle-class students from a typical midwestern suburban area as expressed in two paper-and-pencil achievement tests: (1) the MGMP Similarity Test, and (2) a selection of items from the MGMP Spatial Visualization Test. . CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Introduction In this chapter, literature on different aspects of geometrical similarity will be reviewed. The first three sections examine the deveIOpment in children's understanding of the similarity concept itself, of proportional reasoning, and of the area relationship of similar shapes. Existing research on the relationship between geometrical ability and spatial visualization will be examined next. This study examines the influence of grade level and sex on performance in similarity tasks. From the reviewed literature on cognitive development in areas related to similarity, conclusions on grade level (i.e., age) differences may be drawn. » Literature on sex differences in performance on similarity-related fields will be reviewed in a separate section. The rationale behind this study's examination of the effect of instruction on similarity will be presented in the last section of this chapter. This section will review studies that have examined instructional interventions in fields connected to similarity. The Development of the Similarity Concept The Mathematical Aspect In 1872, Klein established a new structure for the analysis of geometrical concepts, which is known also as the Erlanger Program. 13 14 The Program includes a classification of geometrical transformations according to the transformation groups involved and the invariant prOperties under that group (Fig. 2.1). The similarity transformation determines one of the "middle geometries", becoming thus an important stage in Klein's hierarchy of geometrical concepts. Invariant Properties Group: - . r‘ p a F . openness (closedness) of curves 8 ,3 . interior, exterior, boundary point <5 0 linear order, cyclic order 0 . straightness of lines convexity of figures . parallelism of lines 8. ratios of distance 9. measure of angles '__—__—_'10. length 1 2 3 4. connectedness 5 6 7 Similitude Affine Euclidean Transformation Figure 2.1 Properties invariant under transformation group. (adapted from Martin, 1976b, p.95) Piagetian Research Piaget's research on the child's perception of space fits Klein's model of classification quite smoothly. Both, Klein and Piaget emphasized invariability under transformation. Piaget (1970) opposed the view that knowledge is a passive copy of reality. According to him, to know reality, one must assimilate reality into a system of transformations which attempts to model isomorphically the transformations of reality. ihi his space books, Piaget (1960, 1967) asserts that projective and Euclidean concepts develop concurrently. However, the design of 15 the chapters, the research reported, and other comments suggest that after the deveIOpment of certain basic topological notions, spatial concepts tend to develOp from projective, to affine, to similarity, to Euclidean. Fuson (1978) observes that most of Piaget's analyses focus on topological concepts, or on Euclidean concepts, leaving thus the "middle geometries" relatively neglected. Nevertheless, Piaget (1967) describes five experiments related to the concept of similarity: (1) drawing similar triangles, (2) sorting similar cardboard triangles, (3) choosing and drawing similar rectangles, (4) drawing a similar configuration of line segments, and (5) c0pying supplementary angles. Piaget's categorization of results indicates that at the age of 9-11, his subjects were able to perform perceptual comparisons (considering slopes, parallelism, or angles), began to use simple proportions (1:2), and made measurements, whereas they could use proportions at the age of 11 or above. The failure to perform well in these tasks at earlier ages is attributed to the child's perceptually centered nature of preoperational thought, and later to the inability to use mental constructs (i.e., ratios and proportions) which have no direct concrete- empirical representation. Mathematics educators generally recognize both the advantages and the limitations of Piaget's research in the field of mathematics education. In a mathematical analysis of some of Piaget's topologi'cal tasks, Martin (1976a) concludes: Piaget's objective is not to study the development of mathematical concepts of the child but to study the 16 development of the child's concept of space. The models provided by mathematics are merely the means to an end. As a developmental psychologist, Piaget does not always use mathematical language as precisely as the mathematician might desire. (p.24) The issue of topological-to-Euclidean spatial development was also questioned. Martin (1976b) considers the evidence presented by Piaget's research and states that "it is premature to claim that any particular hierarchy models the sequence or the structure of the child's construction of his spatial reality" (p.112). Martin further suggests that "on the basis of evidence now available" it seems that topological, projective, and Euclidean concepts develop in parallel rather than sequentially. Relating to the concept of similarity, Martin asks whether it is psychologically sound to expect a child to develOp the concept of a variable constant of proportion (i.e., similarity) before the concept of a fixed constant of proportion of one (i.e., congruence). Variables Related to the Acquisition of Similarity Concepts Schultz (1978) showed in her experiments that operational structure is not the only determining factor in tasks that involved geometrical transformations. Attributes of fixed states (e.g., familiarity and size of the involved figures) and the features of the operation itself tend to have a significant influence. ‘Martin (1976a) suggests that the child's ability to order, organize and coordinate his actions might offer a better framework for his spatial development. 17 The model built by Pascual-Leone (1970) considers the informational content and the processing load required by a task as determinant variables in the logical thinking of a child. He defines the number of schemes, rules, or ideas that a child can handle simultaneously as M-capacity. According 1x) him, this capacity increases regularly in an all-or-none manner from age 3 through 16 at a linear rate of one chunk every two years. Thus, the derivation of the proportionality rule that requires simultaneous manipulation of four variables, is not possible before the age of 9. Leon (1982) applied the information processing theory to the child's development of some geometrical concepts such as similarity, and area. He reexamined Piaget's experiment on similarity judgments of rectangles and suggests that the emphasis should be not on the cognitive structures necessary to use prOportions, but rather on the prerequisite quantitative concepts, i.e., the logically implicit response rule requiring proportions. Another limitation on Piaget's theory lies in observed differences in ages at which children come to master operationally equivalent tasks. Piaget calls this phenomenon decalage. According to many researchers (e.g, Laurendeau and Pinard, 1970; Fuson, 1978), Piaget makes a too frequent use of the decalage as an explanation of arising difficulties. Referring to the "topological to projective to Euclidean" developmental path, Lesh (1976) states: The most important challenge to Piagetian theory is not the possibility of 21 different hierarchy-but that often 18 operationally isomorphic tasks vary so much in difficulty that it may be meaningless to classify concepts on the basis of operational structure. (p.235) To summarize, not much research has been done on the growth of the similarity concept. There is disagreement on the exact nature or the sequential order of the cognitive development of geometrical concepts in general, and of similarity concepts in particular. However, researchers agree that at the age of 11, a more general and conceptual use of proportions (as expressed in judgments on similar rectangles) occurs. Perceptual judgments of similarity (e.g., judgments on similar triangles) is possible about two years earlier. Performance on Similarity Tasks Most of the above mentioned studies were based on intensive interviews conducted with a small number of children. Statistical support for many of these claims is even scarcer. Young (1975) examined the performance of 791 children from grades K through 3 on tasks related to ten geometrical concepts--one of them being similarity. In view of the poor results on this specific task, Young recommends introducing the concept of similarity "above the third grade". Carpenter et al. (1981) analyze the results of the 1977-78 second mathematics assessment of the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). The answers of 70,000 students, ages 9,13, and 17 (Hi the similarity items follow the general pattern of performance in geometry: Students have some knowledge of certain basic concepts, but have little knowledge of the properties associated with these concepts, and little 19 abililty to apply these prOperties. In the case of similarity, 94 percent of the 13-year-olds recognized two similar triangles. However, only about 33 percent of the 13 year-olds, and 44 percent of the 17—year- olds knew that similarity does not require congruent sides, and less than 33 percent of the subjects knew that the angles of similar shapes must be congruent; a third of the 13-year-olds and half of the 17-year- olds could use indirect (shadow) measurement to determine the height of a tree. This gap between the potential achievements shown by cognitive research and the observed deficiencies in the acquisiton of geometrical concepts implies that instruction may play a crucial role, particularly if it is conducted at the transitional stages. Research on cognition shows that children at the middle grades level are at such a transitional stage in the growth of concepts related to similarity. This suggests that more detailed research, particularly at this age, is needed. The Development of Proportional Reasoning Similarity and Proportional Reasoning The ability to handle metric proportions and the concept of geometric similarity are related to each other: As mentioned in the previous section, at the mastery level of the similarity concept, proportions are recognized as the inherent rule that governs the situations in which similarity is encountered. Piaget, Inhelder and Szeminska (1960) argue that it is easier to study the growth of the concept of proportion 20 in geometric, rather than in non-geometric form, since before a child can think about proportions, he can perceive whether two different shapes are similar or not. On the other hand, Lunzer and Pumfrey (1966) showed in their study, that success in geometrical situations that require proportional reasoning (i.e., building a "wall" of a given length with Cuisinaire rods, or using a pantograph) should not be interpreted as mastery of the underlying proportional rule. Fifty percent of the nine- year-olds could handle the Pantograph Task with simple pr0portions, whereas less than 50 percent of the 15-year-old subjects were successful in the Balance Task that required the recognition and the application of proportionality in an abstract way. The section on the development of proportional reasoning will follow the same path as the review of the similarity studies. Piagetian research that analyzed mental Operational structures needed in proportional reasoning will be followed by studies that consider other variables that may influence performance on proportionality tasks. Studies on children's level of performance in proportional reasoning will be reviewed next, whereas attempts to improve proportional reasoning through instruction will be presented in a different section. Piagetian Research Inhelder and Piaget (1958) identify the understanding of proportionality with the stage of formal operational reasoning, which according to them emerges at 12 to 13 years of age. They investigated the child's acquisition of proportionality by examining children's reactions to situations such as equilibrium on a balance and prediction 21 of shadow size. 0n the basis of this inquiry the claim was made that pr0portional reasoning appears at the age of 11-12 years, but initially only in a qualitative form, and later (ages 13-17) it evolves into abstract reasoning involving the formulation of the law of proportions and the ability to operate on this law in a quantitative form. They identified proportional reasoning as one of the eight schemes that develop at the stage of formal thought. Martorano (1974) tested six out of these eight schemes and found that the proportionality scheme was more difficult than all others, except mechanical equilibrium. Linn and Swiney (1978) found a: strong relationship between proportional reasoning and general ability. Lunzer (1968) argued that the equality of two ratios always constitutes a second order relation (i.e., a relation between relations), and even a system of such relations (i.e., a/b = c/d implies ad 8 be and a/c = b/d). Studies with British children (Lovell, 1961; Lunzer, 1965; Lovell & Butterworth, 1966) and with American children (Steffe & Parr, 1968; Gray, 1972) confirmed basically the developmental categories identified by Piaget, but noticed a difference in age distribution. Lovell (1972) sums up these findings as following: Apart from very able twelve-years-olds, it is from 12 years of age onwards, the actual age depending on the ability of the pupil, that facility is acquired in handling metric proportion. Many pupils may not be able to do this until 14 or 15 years of age, and some never. (p.8) 22 In order to check the linkage between proportional reasoning and the formal operational stage, Chapman (1975) compared the performance of first, third, and fifth graders on conservation of ratio tasks (presented in a probabilistic manner) with that of college students. He concluded that even 10, or ll-year-old children do not discriminate proportions, whereas most of the items were anwered correctly by college students. Proportional reasoning has been widely investigated by Robert Karplus, who devised a test to determine the level of reasoning children use (Karplus & Peterson, 1970). In his Mr. Tall/Mr. Short task, the subject is presented two drawings: Mr. Short, whose height is measured in large and in small paper clips, and Mr. Tall, measured in small paper clips only. The subject is asked to predict Mr. Tall's height as expressed in large paper clips. Later, Karplus modified the task, by eliminating the drawing of Mr. Tall in order to prevent children from relying on perceptual cues. Karplus and Peterson (1970) categorized students' explanations (strategies) given in solving the Mr. Tall/Mr. Short proportional reasoning problem in the following way: N. no explanation or statement "I can't explain" given. I. intuition. Referring to estimates, guesses, appearances or extraneous factors, without data. IC. Intuitive computation. Use of data haphazardly and in an illogical way. A. addition. Applying the difference rather than the ratio. 23 S. scaling. Not relating to the scale inherent in the data, thereby failing to see the whole problem-displays a tentative attitude toward his/her estimate. AS. addition and scaling. Focuses on the difference rather than ratio, but scales it up. P. proportional reasoning. Uses proportionality. May or may not use word ratio. In a later follow-up study, Karplus and Karplus (1972) found it convenient to collapse these seven categories into three categories representing conceptual levels: Intuitive Level I = I + IC Concrete-operational Level II = A + S Formal-Operational Level III = AS + P In this latter study, the performance of 153 students was compared to their performance on the same task, two years earlier. It was found that during this period, 65 percent of those who were at level I moved up, whereas more than one third of the subjects showed no change in category. In the investigators' view the results prove the developmental nature of the acquisition of the concept of proportionality. (No clear order could be established between the substages of level II.) Further Karplus studies reveal that the additive strategy (defined above as A) is used systematically by many seventh and eighth graders (Wollman & Karplus, 1974), and even by 20 percents of the college students that participated in another experiment (Karplus, Adi & Lawson, 1980). The use of this strategy is attributed to a concrete-operational way 24 of thinking combined with inadequate instruction. Variables Related to PrOportional Reasoning Many alternative factors in the development of proportional reasoning have been suggested in addition to or instead of Piaget's scheme of cognitive development. A comprehensive study in this direction has been conducted by Pulos, Stage and Karplus (1980). Eighty-seven sixth and eighth graders were tested for: (1) proportional reasoning (measured by their Lemonade Task that involved ratios of sugar to concentrated lemon juice), (2) M-capacity (the number of schemes that a child is able to handle simultaneously), (3) fluid intelligence (measured by a wide range series completion test), (4) crystallized intelligence (measured by a wide range vocabulary test), (5) cognitive restructuring (measured by the FASP Embedded Figures Test) (6) field dependency (measured by Pascual-Leone's Water Level Task), (7) formal reasoning (measured by Piaget's Conservation of Volume Task), and (8) divergent thinking (measured by an. alternative uses test). Stepwise multiple regression analyses were employed to measure the relationship between proportional reasoning and the other seven cognitive variables. The results of the study suggest that proportional reasoning is significantly related to only two of the variables: (1) information processing (M) capacity (as suggested also by de Ribaupierre and Pascual- Leone, 1979; Case, 1979; and Furman, 1980), and (2) encoding, or cognitive restructuring (as suggested also in Siegler, 1978). In view of the lack of relationship between proportional reasoning and conservation of volume, Pulos, Stage and Karplus conclude that "the results do not 25 seem to support the hypothesis that a formal reasoning structure, as defined by Inhelder and Piaget, is a necessary prerequisite for proportional reasoning" (p.148). Other variables that seem to influence performance in proportional reasoning tasks are: 1. Level of abstractness. In a comparison of six proportionality tasks, Wollman and Karplus (1974) report that seventh and eighth graders (N=450) were more successful in concrete tasks (Mr. Tall/Mr. Short, Ruler, Shading Fractions) as opposed to more abstract ones (Candy, Numerical). Similarly, Portis (1973) indicates a significantly better performance of fourth, fifth, and sixth graders (N=138) in a proportionality test using physical and pictorial aids, as compared to an equivalent test that used only symbols. Lunzer and Pumfrey (1966) report a much better performance on the concrete Cuisenaire Rods, and Pantograph Tasks than in the Balance Task. They also remark that in the first two tasks, their subjects rarely' applied. specifically' the proportionality rule. Moreover, Wollman and Karplus (1974) found that in abstract tasks most subjects who reason proportionally in an incomplete manner (i.e., incorrectly or concretely) "regressed to additive reasoning". 0n the other hand, increasing the level of abstraction may have also :a positive effect: when Karplus's subjects were denied the opportunity to see Mr. Tall in the Mr. Tall/Mr. Short Task, the number of intuitive-perceptual guesses dropped significantly. 2. Ratio versus fraction. Wachsmuth, Behr and Post (1980) contrasted performance of fifth grade students (N=15) on Ink-Mixture 26 Tasks presented in a ratio (i.e., part-to-part) format with the performance in the same tasks presented in a fraction (i.e., part-whole) format. They indicated a greater success (72 percent) for the ratio format than for fractions (58 percent). Noelting and Gagne (1980) found low correlations between the Orange Juice Experiment bearing on ratios and the Sharing Cookies Experiment bearing on fractions. 3. Numerical context. Abramowitz (1975) raised the possibility of a relationship between the numbers employed in a proportion and the rate of success, since children may "have an intuitive understanding of proportionality without concurrently having the mathematical facility to solve proportion problems "(p.25). She checked three numerical characteristics: (1) equal or unequal differences (i.e., presence or absence of a repeated difference among the three given numbers), (2) size of the unknown number (i.e., whether the unknown number is larger or smaller than the three given numbers), and (3) types of ratio (i.e., whole numbers or fractions). The results obtained from 32 seventh graders indicate that type of ratio and size of the unknown number have a significant effect, but the difference between numbers does not. Abramowitz (1975) also observed that subjects, especially those transitional between concrete and formal Operational thinking...may be quite capable of reasoning through proportions of moderate difficulty. However, when faced with a more demanding task, these same subjects may revert to the use of patterned concrete strategies. (p.26) 27 Karplus, Karplus, and Wollman (1974) also report that the value of the ratio influences performance: a ratio between 1 and 2 attracts more errors (specifically, more additive responses) as compared to ratios smaller than 1 or greater than 2. Quintero (1983) tested the performance of 36 fifth, sixth, and seventh grade Puerto-Rican children on verbal problems involving the basic proportion a/b = c/x. She found that one third of her subjects could solve problems in which both a/b and a/ c are integers, whereas 20 percent could handle problems in which only one of these ratios is integer. The same phenomenon was reported by Karplus, Pulos and Stage (1980) with 120 sixth and eighth graders: a success rate of about 60 percent was observed in the Lemonade Task when both a/b and a/c or only a/b were integers, but the rate of success was much lower when only a/c or no ratio were integers (38 and 18 percents respectively). The British project "Concepts in Secondary Mathematics and Science" (CSMS) administered a Ratio and Proportion Test to over 2200 eleven to sixteen-year-old students. In analyzing the results, Hart (1982) identified five levels of performance: At level zero, the student is unable to make a coherent attempt at any of the questions. At the next four levels, the student is able to solve items of increasing difficulty. Her categorization of the items was: I. No rate is needed or given; the answer may be obtained through multiplication by 2, 3 or taking half. II. Rate is easy to find or answer can be obtained by taking an amount and then add half again as much. 28 III. Rate must be found and is harder to find than above; fraction Operation may also be needed. IV. Ratio is needed; the questions are complex in either numbers needed or setting. 4. Sequencing. Karplus (1978) reported a 51 percent rate of success when a prOportional reasoning task was preceeded by a simple ratio task, while only 12 percent answered correctly when the first task was more difficult. 5. Irrelevant and redundant information. Collea and Numadel (1978) found that when a problem contained both irrelevant and redundant information, a significantly poorer performance resulted. All these findings suggest that "consistent use of proportional reasoning is not a developmental outcome, but depends instead on overcoming task related obstacles" (Karplus, Pulos & Stage, 1980, p.141). Reaching the same conclusions, Abramowitz (1975) contemplates that it may be that "the use of proportionality occurs in.a developmental sequence across a certain set of tasks." This argument "has implications for when it would be best to teach various concepts requiring an understanding of proportionality" (p.27). These instructional implications will be discussed in a separate section. Socio-economic status (SES) and sex are other variables that have to be considered in performance on prOportionality tasks. Particularly poor proportional reasoning by low SES children was shown in two Karplus studies: Karplus and Peterson (1970) indicate that although urban and suburban children perform equally low on the Mr. Tall/Mr. Short Task 29 at the sixth grade level, by the end of high school most of the suburban subjects (80%) had mastered the task, whereas only 9 percent of their urban counterparts did so. 'Thirty-six hundred seventh and eighth graders from seven countries were tested on the same task (Karplus, Karplus & Paulsen, 1977), and only small differences in overall achievement among countries were detected. However, the results show that groups of low SES American students performed at the lowest level (i.e., primarily intuitive). Difficulties in Progortional Reasoning In the section on the concept of similarity, it was shown that generally, at the age of 11 or 12 years, children are able to handle proportion tasks if they are accompanied by physical action. Studies that used paper-and-pencil tests or tasks that required the use of the prOportional rule in a generalized form, report that the ages at which a significant portion of the population could master the tasks is higher- -about 15 years. Lunzer and Pumfrey (1966) report a success rate of less than 50 percent for his 15-year-Old subjects in the Balance Task (see also Lovell & Butterworth, 1966). Similar trends are reported by the Karplus studies: Karplus and Peterson (1970) found that 32 percent of the suburban 8-10th graders and 80 percent of the suburban 11-12th graders could reason proportionally on the Mr. Tall/Mr. Short Task. The corresponding numbers for their urban peers were respectively five and nine percent. Wollman and Karplus (1974) found that only 20 percent of the 450 seventh and eighth graders tested on five proportionality tasks applied proportional 30 reasoning in a consistent way. In their survey of seven countries, Karplus, Karplus and Paulsen (1977) discovered that 25 percent of their subjects (N =- 3600, grades 7-8) used proportional reasoning. The same rate of success among American seventh graders was reported by Abramowitz (1975). Moreover, the rate of develOpment seems to be very slow, at least before the age of 15 years (Karplus & Karplus, 1972). Sudden "jumps" in the level of performance were noticed later in high school (Karplus, Adi & Lawson, 1980; Karplus & Peterson, 1970). Studies report a poor level of proportional reasoning even at the college level. According to Lawson and Wollman (1976), Fuller and Thornton (reported in Pagni, 1983), and Renner and Paske (1977), only about 50 percent of their samples of college students were able to reason proportionally in a formal way. The figures reported by Karplus, Adi and Lawson (1980) for college freshmen, and by Karplus and Peterson (1970) for suburban 12th graders are higher: 74 and 80 percent respectively. Conclusion There are various development models for the acquistion of the concepts of geometrical similarity and of proportional reasoning; many of them are complementary rather than contradictory. By attempting to superimpose the literature on similarity and on prOportionality, it may be concluded that, the developmental stages in the acquisition of the similarity concept and of proportional reasoning are quite similar, and any explanation that seems valid in one field can be applied in the other. Chronologically, however, the concept of similarity (and possibly 31 some other concrete applications of proportionality) seem to precede a more general and abstract use of prOportionality. The Concepts of Area and Area Growth Definition of the Problem The area. growth. of similar shapes, requires the recognition. of the fact that the enlargement of a figure by a scale factor of n will increase its area by n2. The concepts of similarity and of area are, therefore, prerequisites to an understanding of area growth. In this section, the literature on the concept of area will precede the description of a few studies that can be related directly to the topic of area growth. Concept of Area Conservation of area. According to Piaget, Inhelder, and Szeminska (1960), area conservation (i.e., considering area as a stable attribute independently of the shape of a figure) is attained at the early concrete- Operational stage--around the age of 7 1/2 years. Beilin (1964), however, objected to the fact that in Piaget's experiments on area conservation, the transformations of the figures were performed in front of the subjects. He defines "quasiconservation" as the ability to determine that two noncongruent regions have equal areas when the child is presented with the end product of a rearrangement. Beilin tested 316 children in grades K through 4 and found a correct response level of less than 50 percent of his fourth-grade subjects. Two other larger-scale studies 32 (Renner, 1971; Hademenos, 1974) indicate that area conservation as defined by Piaget is generally achieved at a later age of about ten years. Similarly, in studying the conception of area measure with 75 eight, ten, and eleven-year-old children, Wagman (1975) concludes that "about a third of the ten, and 11-year-olds in the sample either failed to apply at least one of the neglected axioms (area, congruence, and additivity) even in perceptually easy cases, or did not conserve area" (p.109). Calculation of area. The quantification skills necessary to derive the area (e.g., height x width for rectangles) seem to evolve also at a later age than Piaget's theory of cognitive develOpment indicates. Anderson and Cuneo (1978) showed that the adding rule (i.e., height + width) describes five, and six-year-Olds' judgment of rectangular area. Leon (1982) shows that seven-year-old children use the "linear extent" rule to judge area--which in the case of rectangles would be the length of the diagonal, and that only by the ages 8-9 is the linear extent rule replaced by the multiplicative rule. The difficulties in the calculation of area seem to persist even at a later age. Carpenter et a1. (1981) analyzed the results on the area items of the second mathematics assessment of the NAEP and Observed that (1) few nine-year-olds have any knowledge of even basic area concepts (28 percent could find the area of a rectangle divided in square units); (2) among the 13-year-olds, 51 percent could find the area Of a rectangle by the dimension of its sides, 12 percent could find the area of a square with a given side, and even fewer could find the area Of a 33 right triangle; (3) 74 percent of the 17-year-olds in the sample calculated correctly the area of a rectangle, 42 percent--the area of a square, and 20 percent--the area of a parallelogram or of a right triangle; (4) the success rate in application exercises was even lower- -16 percent of the 17-year-olds could find the area of a region made up of two rectangles. Carpenter et a1. (1981) conclude: Performance Of perimeter, area, and volume exercises was among the poorest of any content area on the assessment. Not only was performance extremely low on exercises at the application level, but many students at all ages appeared to have no understanding of the most basic concepts Of perimeter, area, and volume. (p.98) In a survey of geometric concepts possessed by 198 sixth graders on leaving elementary school, Schnur and Callahan (1973) report similar results: finding areas of rectangles was "marginally easy" (i.e., a difficulty index between .50 and .69), areas of squares and parallelograms were "marginally difficult" (i.e., a difficulty index between .30 and .49), and calculating the area of a triangle was "very difficult"(i.e., a difficulty level of less than .10). Perimeter and area. The confusion of area and perimeter has been Observed in all the studies that related to the acqusition of these concepts. Piaget, Inhelder, and Szeminska (1960) observe that in their area tasks, some subjects related to the perimeters of the two regions, and consider the reliance on linear terms characteristic of the concrete level (III A). Wagman (1975) reports that approximately one third of 34 their subjects (ages 8, 10, and 11 years) confused area and perimeter at some time during their interview. In their analysis Of the second NAEP results, Carpenter et a1. (1981) indicate that 23 percent of the 13-year-Olds and 12 percent of the 17-year-olds calculated the perimeter of a rectangle instead of its area, whereas half of the 13-year-olds, and a third of the 17-year-olds did the same in the case of a square and a right triangle. Area Growth of Similar Shapes After three weeks of instruction that focused on growth and shrinking of figures and objects, Fitzgerald and Shroyer (1979) report a low level of performance among their sample of 350 sixth graders: 35 percent could master a task (Hi the growth of a square presented in a concrete mode, but only half of these answered correctly when the task was presented in an abstract mode; the rate of success for tasks that required the application of the area growth principle varied between 3 and 13 percent. McGillicudy-DeLisi (1977) related the performance of six through 13-year-old children on tasks that involved enlarging rubberband figures on a pegboard with the cognitive level of her subjects (N=75). By a qualitative analysis of her subjects' strategies and types of movement, she found that the rate of success increased with operative level. An explanation for the difficulties encountered in area growth tasks can be deduced from the research conducted by Bang in France (reported in English by Montangero, 1976) and by Lunzer (1968, 1973) in England. The tasks involved inquiries about area and perimeter under two kinds of transformations: (1) a series Of rectangles with a fixed perimeter 35 (but a decreasing area) created from an initial square, and (2) a series of figures with a fixed area (but an increasing perimeter) created by cutting a triangular section from a lower corner of a square and transferring it vertically until it is attached to the upper corner of the original figure. Lunzer (1968) argues that at the concrete-operational level of reasoning, children employ "false conservation": realizing that something was preserved, they resist the evidence of perception, and regard the conservation of both area and perimeter as logically necessary in both cases (1) and (2). They reason about the figures as Objects and about area and perimeter as essential characteristics of the object that must "go together". According to Lunzer, only at the formal level, area and perimeter are clearly disassociated, and both are assumed to vary according to some (but not necessarily the same) law. He argues that since area snd perimeter are well-defined relations existing in figures, the discrimination between them requires the ability to use second-order relations--an ability that can be found only at the formal level of reasoning. This argument may provide a valid explanation to the reported difficulties in the acquisiton Of the concepts of perimeter, area, and area growth. The understanding of the area growth requires the recognition of the fact that for similar shapes linear dimensions and area vary according to different rules. 36 Geometry and Spatial Visualization Spatial ability Although descriptions and measurements of aspects of intelligence started earlier, only in the 1940's did spatial ability start to gain more attention. Wolfle (1940) reviewed the factorial studies up to 1940, and stated that verbal ability and space ability were the two most frequently identified factors. Factor analyses conducted by a group of American psychologists in the Aviation Psychology Program (Fruchter, 1954; Zimmerman, 1954; Michael, 1954) identified in spatial ability at least two factors: (1) Spatial Relations and Orientation, and (2) Spatial Visualization. Ekstrom et al. (1976) observe that there has been some difficulty in explaining the difference between these two factors. They suggest that the figure is perceived as a whole in spatial orientation, but must be mentally restructured into components for manipulation in spatial visualization. More specifically, they define spatial visualization as "the ability to manipulate or transform the image of spatial patterns into other arrangements" (p.173). In his survey of studies on spatial visualization, Ben-Haim (1983) argues that the nature of this ability is still a controversial issue--especially for children younger than 11 years. He points out that "the picture in the eighties is still unclear,‘ and quotes from Harris (1981): Our attempts to identify the critical components of various "spatial" tests are still part guess work, particularly where we lack factor analyses involving both standard and 37 nonstandard tasks...Furthermore, consensus is still lacking on the meaning of the two factors--orientation and visualization...factor analysts continue to have difficulty in differentiating and interpreting these factors. (p.23) There is an abundance of spatial visualization tests (see for example Smith, 1964; Ben-Haim, 1983), but only two will be mentioned here: (1) the Spatial Relations of the Differential Aptitude Test (DAT) which requires the subject to identify the figure of a solid that can be Obtained when a given pattern is folded (Bennett et al., 1966), and (2) the Middle Grades Mathematics Projects (MGMP) Spatial Visualization Test which requires the subject to rotate cube constructions mentally and to identify them from a different perspective (Lappan, 1983). The DAT test was frequently used in studies that compare spatial visualization with geometric abilities, whereas data from the MGMP test will be used in this study to assess the relationship between spatial visualiztion and achievement in similarity concepts. Geometry and spatial visualization One of the goals of this study is to examine the relationship between the acquisition of similarity concepts and spatial visualization. Since studies on this specific topic have not been reported, the issue will be extended to include the relationship between spatial visualization and geometrical abilities in general. Research in this area, however, has limitations of its own. In his study on the relationship between geometrical, and spatial ability, Werdelin (1961) observes: 38 Very few studies have dealt with the structure of geometrical ability, especially in comparison with the number of investigations of, for example, the verbal and the visual- perceptual field. With a few exceptions, the investigators of the field seem to have considered it to be an unitary part Of mathematics, or devised only one test to cover it. Quite often, geometrical ability and spatial ability have been confused with each other, which of course is easily done, but which does not facilitate the solution of the problem. (p.33) The source Of this confusion is the fact that there is no clear definition of geometrical ability. Most of the relevant factor analyses and regression studies relate to achievement in plane Euclidean geometry at the high school level. Euclidean geometry is a clearly defined field, but on the other hand, it also requires a great deal of formal knowledge and of logical and verbal ability. Whether conclusions can be drawn from these studies to the relationship between spatial visualization and a more informal geometrical ability, is a debatable issue. There are strong cognitive and pedagogical reasons to assume a close relationship between informal geometry and spatial ability. Some of the visual aspects of similarity and prOportional reasoning tasks have been already discussed. From a pedagogical point of view, Hoffer (1977) lists seven aspects of the visual perception ability that are essential in the geometrical development of a child at the elementary and middle grades level: (1) visual-motor coordination (i.e., 39 coordinating vision with movement of the body), (2) figure-ground perception (i.e., distinguishing foreground from background), (3) perceptual constancy (i.e., recognizing an object out of its original context or from a different vieWpoint), (4) position in space (i.e., determining the relationship of an object as compared to the observer), (5) spatial relationship (i.e., perceiving the position of two or more objects both in relation to the observer and in relation to each other), (6) visual discrimination (i.e., distinguishing similarities and differences between Objects), and (7) visual memory (i.e., recalling accurately an Object no longer in view and relating its similarities and differences to other items in view or not in view). The possibility of a connection between the acquisition of similarity concepts and Spatial visualization is strengthened 'by results from a study that revealed a relationship between prOportional reasoning and spatial ability: Brendzel (1981) used a sample of 400 ninth and eleventh graders, and by conducting an analysis of covariance with the 1.0. as a covariant, she found that ability in spatial visualization accounted for 62 percent of the variation in achievement in proportional reasoning (as measured by the Karplus, Rund, and Piaget's tasks). The findings of studies on the relationship between high school geometry achievement and Spatial visualization are inconclusive. Holzinger and Swineford (1946) report a high multiple correlation (.71) for predicting plane geometry achievement by the best three predictors out Of an initial battery of nine tests: one of them was connected to the general factor (as measured by a series completion test) and 40 two others to spatial ability (as measured by the Visual Imagery, and Punched Holes-Verbal tests). Werdelin (1961) conducted a comprehensive factorial analysis of mathematical abilities in three highly selective Swedish schools, and concludes that "the Space factor or factors are of essential importance to the different aspects of geometry; primarily to geometrical construction and abstraction and also to problem solving" (p.122). In an earlier study, Werdelin (1958) reanalyzed the data published by Rogers (1918), and showed that contrary to the initial findings of this study, geometrical ability depends on spatial ability and also, but to a smaller degree on verbal ability. Werdelin (1958) notes that the geometrical tests in Rogers' study were "quite similar" to space tests. This Similarity may be considered a potential source of confusion between geometrical and spatial abilities, but on the other hand, it may be considered a valid evaluation mean of a less formal geometrical ability. In an investigation of the informal aspects of geometrical abilities at the college level, Blade and Watson (1955) indicate that achievement in spatial visualization among engineering freshmen correlates positively with grades in descriptive geometry (r=.54) and in engineering drawing (r=.34). 0n the other hand, there are many studies (Murray, 1949; Weiss, 1955; Werdelin, 1958; French, 1964; Bennet et al., 1966) that seem to indicate that geometrical ability is independent of the space factors. The ambivalence of these findings is reflected also in a study by Hanson (1972): he found some connection between visual perception 41 ability and geometry for his ninth-grade subjects (11 percent of the variance in geometry achievement could be accounted for), but did not find a Significant correlation for the tenth graders. Werdelin (1961) observes that "the question whether mathematical (geometrical) ability is dependent on the visual factor(s) has not been definitely answered" (p.38). This Observation seems to be valid in view of the contradictory findings in the case of Euclidean geometry, and of lack of evidence in the case of informal geometry. Sex Differences Sex Differences in Geometric ability Sex differences in mathematical abilities is a widely debated issue. For many years it was accepted that sex differences in mathematical abilities, favoring males emerge at adolescence (Maccoby and Jacklin, 1974). More recently, counter-evidence has been provided by Fennema and Sherman (1978) who investigated the achievement Of high school students in light of their mathematical backgrounds and found that males and females with similar course enrollment had Similar achievements. As compared with studies on sex differences in spatial visualization, the research related to geometric abilities is Significantly scarcer. Moreover, the two fields are sometimes hardly distinguishable. For example, a study on the effect of instruction in Tangram puzzles (Smith & Shroeder, 1979; Smith & Litman, 1979) is discussed as a study in Spatial visualization, but could be related to informal geometry as 42 well. The issue of sex differences related to the concept of similarity has not been considered as a separate tOpic. An indirect attempt to address it was made in 1925 by Cameron. By using rather rudimentary statistical methods and a battery of nine tests, she analyzed sex differences in mathematical aptitudes in a sample of 13 through 17-year- oldS. One of her tasks required the completion of a construction of a rectangle similar to a given one, when one of its Sides is also a given. Cameron (1925) concludes that the performance of boys and girls was slightly in the boys' favor , but "fairly equal". Due to the lack of systematic research on the concept of similarity, studies on sex differences in geometric abilities in general and in proportional reasoning may be considered relevant. Three studies that relate to informal, or to transformational geometry have detected no sex differences in performance: Olson (1970) at high school level, and Thomas (1977) at the grade levels 1, 3, 6, 9, and 11, reached this conclusion in studies on transformation geometry; G.D. Peterson (1973) made this Observation after administrating a 50-item geometry achievement test to 725 fourth, fifth, and sixth graders. When the connection with spatial ability is made, the results become less clear. Shonberger (1976) indicates that practical and geometrical problems involving Spatial components are the source for sex differences in mathematical abilities. In his study on geometric and spatial abilities Of Swedish boys and girls at the high school level, Werdelin 43 (1961) found a male superiority in less formal aspects of geometry such as geometrical construction and abstraction, and geometrical problem solving, whereas girls had a better ability to prove theorems. These findings remained basically unchanged, when he compared 143 boys with the same number of girls matched by general reasoning, numerical ability, and age. Werdelin also refers to another Swedish study conducted in 1944 by Siegwald. This study indicated male superiority in dynamic visualization (i.e., mental imagery manipulations), space relations, geometrical imagination, construction of difficult, unfamiliar geometrical figures, and in geometrical problems, but not in perception of form, construction of simple, familiar shapes, and in static visualization. With regard to the effect of geometry instruction on spatial abilities as a function of sex, Brinkmann (1966) found that eighth- grade boys and girls performed and gained similarly on a spatial visualization test after an intervention of ten programmed units in informal geometry . A four-hour-long instruction based on Tangram puzzles caused Similarly good results for both sexes at the fourth grade level (Smith & Schroeder, 1979). However, the same intervention erased the pre- interventional female superiority at the sixth and seventh grade level implying that at the Stage of early adolescence, girls profit less from this kind of instruction (Smith & Litman, 1979). Most of the studies on sex differences in high school Euclidean geometry imply that as compared to boys, girls have similar or higher 44 abilities in tasks that require proving theorems (Touton, 1924; Perry, 1929; Werdelin, 1961; Senk.& Usiskin, 1982). To conclude, the findings in geometry seem to confirm a trend that has been Observed in other mathematical fields: when experience is controlled (as for example, in.Euclidean geometry or in a tranformational but formal approach to geometry) girls and boys perform equally well. On the other side, in mathematical tasks that also rely on out-of- School experience (e.g., some spatial tasks) sex differences tend to Show up. Sex Differences in Proportional Reasonipg Studies on sex differences in proportionality tasks suggest that boys tend to outperform girls at any age. Chapman (1975) showed that even at the third and fifth grade levels, boys scored significantly (p<.01) higher than girls in ratio comparison tasks and exhibited greater frequency of verbal explanations referring to proportions. In his prOportional reasoning study of seven countries, Karplus et al. (1977) Observe that eighth and ninth grade female subjects tended to use additive responses (which are of a lower order) more Often than males. Brendzel (1981) refers to a study conducted in 1977 by Piburn at Rutgers University. Piburn detected a male superiority in proportional reasoning tasks among his subjects ranging in age from 13 tO 23. 45 Keating and Schaefer (1975) found that fifth and seventh-grade psychometrically bright boys outperformed Sixth and eighth-grade bright girls on three Piagetian tasks--one of them.being the Balance Task. Some studies also indicated sex differences as a function of task- related variables. Besides a general male superiority, Stage, Karplus, and Pulos (1980) found, that among their Sixth, and eight-grade subjects, younger and female students have additional difficulties with structural changes that make tasks more difficult for all students (such as a more difficult numerical context). At the ninth grade level, Brendzel (1978) found evidence of sex differences favoring males in proportional reasoning tasks, particularly when unfamiliar measuring units were employed. Garrard (1982) concludes that at the eighth grade level, visual adjuncts were more effective (p<.10) for girls in the solution of twenty non-routine proportionality tasks. The male superiority in proportional reasoning, and the inconclusive findings on sex differences in geometrical tasks, do not allow for predictions on the existence of, or lack of sex differences in geometrical Similarity tasks that combine these two aptitudes. The Effect of Instruction Influencing Cognitive Development Relatively few attempts have been made to study the influence of instruction on the acquisition of geometrical concepts, and among these studies few could Show positive results. Since no studies have 46 concentrated specifically on instruction related to the concept of similarity, three related tOpics will be discussed: (1) the teaching of geometrical concepts in general, (2) the teaching of proportional reasoning, and (3) the effect of instruction in geometry on spatial ability. The Piagetian research on these topics is mainly descriptive, and its instructional implications are usually stated as speculations in separate studies. More recently, Piagetian researchers recognize the potential of instruction as an accelerator of cognitive processes. Montangero (1976) states: Learning must be subordinated to the laws of development Since operational structures do not derive from structures that might exist outside the child, but stem from the coordination of internalized actions...In brief, training, according to the Genevan conception, consists in trying to accelerate cognitive develOpment. (p. 126) Some empirical studies showed that instruction on a certain concept at a certain age failed to bring the desired results, and the failure was related to the subjects' functioning at a lower mental-Operational stage than the level requested by the presented task. Young (1975) for example, examined the relationship between performance on tasks related to ten geometrical concepts and instruction. By use of Chi- Square scores in a sample of 791 kindergarten through third graders, Young concluded that performance was independent from received instruction. Boulanger (1974) attempted to teach prOportionality to 51 47 third graders that were at the concrete-operational level, and concluded that the training was neither retained over time, nor did it transfer to other Simple proportion tasks. Lawson and Wollman (1980) also experimented with teaching proportional reasoning in a class of seventh graders, and concluded that the extent of success depended on each subject's operational level, and that formal operational level is a prerequisite for teaching proportional reasoning. An important model that takes into account both the cognitive and the instructional aspects in children's understanding of geometry, was designed by P.M. and Dina van Hiele (for a good description in English, see Wirszup, 1976). The Van Hieles identified five stages of geometrical development (Table 2.1) and propose that the levels cannot be skipped. Unlike Piaget, they argue that the levels develop primarily under the influence of school instruction. Therefore, instruction should be Table 2.1 THE VAN HIELE LEVELS OF DEVELOPMENT IN GEOMETRY (adapted from Burger et al. 1981) LEVEL CONTEXT FORM OF REASONING 0 Basic shapes and figures in Visual identification and comparison some geometrical space of shapes I Properties of shapes in the Informal analysis of shapes in terms space of their properties II Relationships among proper— Logical partial ordering of pro- ties of shapes perties III An abstract geometrical system Formal deduction (proof) of theorems within the system IV Various geometries Rigorous mathematical study of the geometries 48 geared to lead students deliberately from one level to the next. Wirszup (1976) reports on two Soviet researchers who adapted a program of geometry instruction to the'Van Hiele model with striking success. Wirszup (1976) also uses the Van Hiele model to explain the present deficiencies in the teaching of geometry in the United States: Only a very small number of the elementary schools offer any organized studies in visual geometry, and where they are done, they begin with measurements and other concepts which correspond to Levels II and III of thought develOpment in geometry. Since Level I is passed over, the material that is taught even in these schools does not promote any deeper understanding and is soon completely forgotten, Then, in the 10th grade, 15 and 16 year old youngsters are confronted with geometry for almost the first time in their lives. The whole unknown and complex world of plane and Space is given to them in a passive axiomatic or pseudo- axiomatic treatment. The majority of our lflgh school students are at the figgp level of develOpment in geometry, while the course they take demands the fourth. level of thought. It is no wonder that high school graduates have hardly any knowledge of geometry, and that this irreparable deficiency haunts them continually later on. (p.96) Thus, according to this view, instructional, rather than cognitive deficiencies are the main reason for a poor performance in geometrical 49 tasks. Instructional Interventions Geometry. Most of the studies about the effect of instruction on achievement in geometry compare the effect of different modes of instruction on a wide variety of concepts, rather than examine the effect of instruction on the acquisition of a specific concept. The topics of reported comparisons are: (1) concrete-activity orientation versus paper-and-pencil approach, (2) problem-solving orientation versus presentation of the end-product, and (3) formal (i.e., Euclidean) versus informal geometry . 1. Concreteness. In a survey of Studies that compared the use of concrete materials with reliance on symbolic representations in the teaching of mathematics, Fennema (1972) concludes: Learning of mathematical ideas is likely to be facilitated by a predominance of concrete models in the early grades and a gradually increasing proportion Of symbolic models as children move through the elementary school...Piaget placed children up to twelve years of age in the concrete- operational stage of cognitive development. Children in this stage are capable of learning with symbols, but only if these symbols represent actions the learners have done previously. (p. 637) Relating specifically to geometry, Bring (1972) showed that a unit for fifth and sixth graders on volume, congruence, symmetry and isometrics 50 gave significantly better results when presented in a concrete-activity mode as compared to a control group that used only paper and pencil. 2. Problem solving, Mathematics educators agree on the advantages of emphasizing the evolution of mathematical models from real-life situations, as opposed to an expository teaching that presents a ready- made product. Two studies that addressed this issue in relation to the teaching of informal geometry were conducted by Scott, Fryer, and Klausmeyer (1971), and by Hempel (1981). Both indicate that expository teaching gives better short-term results, but the discovery approach proved superior in retention and transfer. Buchert (1980) also compared the two 'modes of instruction. of informal geometry ‘with. 108 seventh graders, and found that the "mathematized" (i.e., process-oriented) instructhmn was clearly superior (p<.0001) for students of all arithmetical abilities. 3. Informalitl. In the last eighty years, many committees recommended that geometry be taught informally at the K-8 grade levels. J. C. Peterson (1973) suggests to extend this recommendation to the high school level as well. Several studies conducted in high schools compared the teaching of formal Euclidean geometry with a less formal approach, but their findings are not clear. From a pedagogical point of view, Peterson presents the advantages of teaching informal geometry at 33.31 grade level. According to him, "informal geometry is more than a list of topicsr-it is a method of teaching geometry. Informal geometry at its best makes use of discovery methods of teaching, inductive reasoning and the student's 51 inquisitiveness" (p.60). However, difficulties arose ‘when.:more rigorous studies compared achievement of students who learned an experimental informal geometry course for the 10th, and 11th grade level with a control group that studied the traditional high school geometry course. Cox (1980) concludes that his results are "not clear": the control group did better on the ETS Cooperative Test in Geometry-Part 1 and also on problems in solving multiple concepts and properties, and more complex constructions. 0n the other side, the experimental group showed better attitudes towards geometry. Three studies experimented with interventions based on transformation geometry at the high school level: Olson (1971) reports disappointing results-especially on a four-problem proof test; Usiskin (1972) found that his control group outperformed (p<.01) the experimental group in standard geometry content; Durapau (1979) indicates similar achievements for his control and experimental groups, but a: better performance of the latter (p<.01) on transfer questions. A possible explanation of these results is the fact that in order to allow for comparisons, new experimental approaches have been evaluated with tools that measured Skills characteristic to the content and the goals of a traditional geometry course. Proportional reasoning. Lunzer (1973) tries to strike a balance between the limitations set by' the cognitive-Operational. ability' of a: child in terms of proportionality and the need for instruction. 'He states: 52 It has not been shown that the realization of proportionality comes about spontaneously as a result of maturing logic...It is more probable that these relations need to be taught. But the evidence suggests that even given good teaching, they will not be applied spontaneously to new problems until the student's powers of reasoning have reached an advanced level of development. (p.13) After reporting SES- rather than nation-related differences of performance in proportional reasoning, Karplus et al. (1977) conclude their international survey by arguing that "teaching makes a difference at this age" (i.e., 13-15 years), and recommend an instruction that provides concrete experiences and emphasizes active participation in learning. Similar conclusions are drawn by Wollman and Lawson (1978) in a comparison of a verbal approach with a physical-action orientation in teaching seventh graders to reason proportionally. By using a three- week-long instruction unit, Kurz and Karplus (1977) showed that proportional reasoning can be taught effectively at the high school prealgebra level: after instruction, more than 50 percent of the experimental students, but less than ten percent of the control group scored highly on proportional reasoning tasks. However, in a comparison of two instructional modes for the same unit, they could not detect a significant difference in achievement: the rate of success in the "manipulative" group went up as a result of instruction from 15 percent to 70 percent, whereas the "paper-and-pencil" group advanced from 17 to 62 percent. 53 Spatial ability. Presently there is no evidence that instruction in geometrical Similarity can cause improvement in spatial visualization, the issue can be addressed indirectly by analyzing the studies that attempted to relate instruction in geometry in general to Spatial visualization. The studies that deal with transformational geometry are particularly relevant, since the concept of similarity fits naturally into a transformational approach to geometry. Theoretically, the connection between transformational geometry and spatial ability is quite straightforward: the study of Slides, flips, and turns on objects or drawings should increase the ability to perform mental manipulations of figures. Piaget (1964) argues that imagery manipulations can be described in terms of geometrical transformation. However, findings of some empirical studies, do not support this argumentation. Perham (1977) reports that after an ll-session-long instruction in transformation geometry, his experimental group of first graders gained significantly' more than. the control group (N==72) in transformation geometry but not in spatial visualization (as measured by a test on horizontial-vertical and left-right orientation, and on figure folding). A similar failure to achieve transfer from transformation geometry to mental imagery manipulation after a 12-session-long instruction was reported by Williford (1972) at the second and the third grade level. Contrary results were reported by Ekman (1968), who detected significant improvement in spatial ability during the summer, as result of instruction in geometrical measurement and calculations conducted towards the end 54 of the fourth grade. This study, however, has no control group that would assure that the improvement is not a result of maturation. Brinkmann (1966) experimented at the eighth grade level with a 10-session-long programmed course in geometry that emphasized figure differentiation (n: discrimination, pattern folding, and object manipulation in a problem-solving approach. Wolfe (1970) basically replicated Brinkmann's study at the grade levels seven, eight, and nine. Both studies Show significant gains in spatial visualization (as measured by the DAT Space Relations Test). Wolfe, however notes that little transfer was observed in tasks that were different from the ones encountered during instruction. At high school level, Ranucci (1952) did not detect any Significant differences in space perception abilities between a group that studied a course in solid geometry and a control group that did not take the solid geometry course. Similarly, Brown (1955) compared performance in spatial visualization (as measured by the DAT Space Relations Test) of a control group that studied one year of plane geometry, with a group that studied a combined course of plane and solid geometry. He concluded that the addition of solid geometry did not prove itself as a facilitator for spatial visualization. Studies by Myers (1958) and Sedgwick (1961) indicate respectively that neither a high school course in mechanical drawing nor a college course in descriptive geometry could cause a significantly different performance in Spatial visualization tasks as compared to control groups that did not study these courses. 55 However, instruction that uses a less formal approach to geometry has a stronger influence on spatial visualization. Van Voorhis (1941) conducted a first-year college course that included estimating linear extent, angles and areas, three-dimensional tick-tack-toe, and various other visualizing experiences. A study by Battista, Wheatley, and Talsma (1982) involved a geometry course for prospective elementary teachers that included manipulating concrete models, paper folding, tracing, using Miras, constructing polyhedra, and transformational geometry. Both studies report a significantly better performance in spatial visualization: the first as compared to a control group (on the "Cards" and "Figures" sections of the Thurstone Test for Primary Mental Abilities), and the second as compared to pre-interventional performance (on the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test: Rotations). To summarize the presented evidence, at the elementary level, instruction in geometry did not have a Significant effect on spatial visualization. Starting from the middle grades level, this effect manifests itself in interventions that emphasize the visual aspect of geometry. Studies on traditional courses in geometry could not Show similar effects. Brinkmann (1966) offers an explanation for this phemomenon: When one realizes that the emphasis in the teaching of geometry is usually on develOpment of formal proofs based on a certain type of "givens", the failure to add to the performance on spatial visualization should not be surprising. The behaviors demanded are simply different. (p. 180) PLEASE NOTE: Page 56 is lacking in number only per author. NO text is missing. Filmed as received. UNIVERSITY MICROFILMS INTERNATIONAL. CHAPTER III DESIGN OF THE STUDY Purposes of the Study This study was designed to investigate three areas that are related to the concept of similarity: (1) performance in four Similarity- related areas, (2) performance on tasks presented in a visual or a verbal mode, and (3) relationship between performance on similarity and Spatial visualization. 1. Performance in four similarity-related areas. 1.1 To investigate prior to instruction in similarity the possibility of grade level, or sex differences in performance of sixth, seventh, and eighth graders in tasks that require the use of (1) 'basic properties of similar Shapes, (2) proportional reasoning related to similarity, (3) the principle of area growth, and (4) applications of similarity in scaled drawings and in indirect measurement. Performance on tasks presented in a visual or a verbal mode. 1.2 To investigate the effect of instruction of the MGMP Similarity Unit on the performance in the four topics mentioned above. 2.1 To investigate grade level enui sex differences in pre- instructional performance on Similarity tasks presented in a verbal mode, and on equivalent tasks accompanied by drawings. 2.2 To investigate the effect of instruction pertaining to similarity 57 58 on similarity tasks presented in a verbal mode, and on their visual counterparts. 3. Relationship between performance on Similarity tasks and spatial visualization. 3.1 To explore any possible effect of instruction in similarity on performance in Spatial visualization tasks. 3.2 To explore any possible effect of instruction of the MGMP Spatial Visualization (one year before this study) on performance in similarity tasks. Population and Sample The participant subjects in this study were students from five suburban middle schools :hi the Lansing, Michigan area. All five schools have a middle-class, predominantly white student pOpulation. The selection of the schools was made according to the teachers' willingness to participate in the experimental instruction of the MGMP Similarity Unit: schools that had more than one volunteering teacher have been selected to participate in the study. Table 3.1 presents the distribution of classes and teachers by school and by grade level. Since School 5 participated a year before this study' (Winter, 1982-83) in the instruction of the MGMP Spatial Visualization Unit, the sample was divided into two parts: 59 Sample A consisted of Sixth, seventh and eighth-grade students from Schools 1, 2, 3, and 4. This sample was used to explore issues related to students who did not undergo an instruction in spatial visualization. Although most of the questions were concerned with performance in similarity, School 5, has been omitted from this sample to prevent a contamination of the results due to a potential influence of instruction in spatial visualization on performance in Similarity tasks. Table 3.2 presents the distribution of the subjects in Sample A by grade and by sex. Sample B consisted of seventh and eigtht-grade students from School 5. This sample was used to explore issues related to the comparison of students who learned the Sgatial Visualization Unit and those who did not. Students of both kinds could be found in this sample. The distribution of the subjects from this sample by grade level and by experience in instruction in spatial visualization is presented in Table 3.3. Table 3.1 DISTRIBUTION OF PARTICIPATING CLASSES AND TEACHERS BY GRADE LEVEL AND BY SCHOOL N N Classes Teachers Gr. 6 Cr. 7 Cr. 8 Cr. 6 Cr. 7 Gr. 8 School 1 6 2 - 2 2 - School 2 3 3 - 3 3 - School 3 - - 3 - - 2 School 4 2 2 1 1 1 1 School 5 - 3 5 - 2 2 Total 11 10 9 6 8 5 60 Table 3.2 SAMPLE A--DISTRIBUTION OF STUDENTS BY GRADE LEVEL AND BY SEX N N N Male Female Total Grade 6 137 111 248 Grade 7 87 83 170 Grade 8 54 42 96 Total 278 236 514 Table 3.3 SAMPLE B-—DISTRIBUTION OF STUDENTS BY GRADE LEVEL AND BY EXPERIENCE IN SPATIAL VISUALIZATION (SV) INSTRUCTION N N N Students with Students without Total SV instruction SV instruction Grade 7 29 29 58 Grade 8 69 34 103 Total 98 63 161 61 Instrumentation The instruments used in this study were a Similarity performance test and a spatial visualization performance test. Similarity The similarity performance was measured by the Middle Grades Mathematics Project Similarity Test (MGMP SIMT). This test was developed by the Middle Grades Mathematics Project staff, including this investigator, in the Mathematics Department of Michigan State University. During Fall 1982, a pilot test was administered to approximately one hundred Sixth and seventh grade students in two schools, prior to, and after instruction with the first draft of the MGMP Similarity Unit. Test validity and reliability were considered: the test was revised in view of student results and observations made by the project's staff and the participating teachers. The MGMP Similarity Test consists of 25 multiple choice items with five Options for each item. The presented Options have been selected according to the most popular misconceptions indicated by cognitive research on similarity concepts and on proportional reasoning. The items were scored by assigning a 1 for a correct response and a 0 Otherwise; no correction was made for guessing. The test was not timed, but usually did not exceed 25-30 minutes. The total score on the test was considered as a general indicator of the level of performance in similarity tasks. The Cronbach reliability coefficients calculated for the test ranged from .53 to .82. Table C.1 in Appendix C includes the reliability 62 coefficients for the MGMP SIMT by grade and by sex. Another indicator of the quality of the instrument was the significant high correlations (P < .001) between the pretest and the postest scores. The corresponding Pearson correlation coefficients (Table C.2 in Appendix C) ranged from .46 to .79. Twenty items out of the 25 were clustered into four subtests (Appendix A), in order to analyze student performance on four tOpics: (1) basic properties of similar shapes (i.e., recognition of similar shapes and their properties), (2) prOportional reasoning (i.e., finding the fourth number in a proportion that represents lengths of sides of similar shapes), (3) the principle of area growth in similar shapes, and (4) applications of similarity in scaled drawings and in indirect measurement. Subtests (1) and (4) include six items each, and subtests (2) and (3) include four items each. In the original test, the items were not presented in a clustered form; the item numbers in Appendix A represent their original sequence. The scores on these subtests were used as indicators of student pre, and post-instructional performance on the considered similarity-related topics. In order to examine student performance in similarity tasks presented in a verbal or in a visual mode, eight items were clustered into a visual and a verbal subtest of four items each (Appendix A). Although twenty out of the 25 items were accompanied by drawings, only four of these were selected so that the selected visual items had strictly equivalent counterparts stated in a purely verbal form. The four pairs of equivalent items included one pair on the concept of similarity, 63 one pair on indirect (shadow) measurement, and two pairs on area growth. Spatial Visualization The performance in spatial visualization was measured by a 15-item subtest of the MGMP Spatial Visualization Test. The original 32-item test was developed by the MGMP staff during summer and fall 1981 and was administered to about 1500 students in two evaluation studies of the MGMP Spatial Visualization Unit (Ben-Haim, 1983 ; Lappan, 1983). Fifteen representative items have been selected to form the MGMP Spatial Visualization Subtest (MGMP SVST) used in this study. The SVST consists of items requiring three-dimensional visualization and is unrelated to topics or notations developed in the Similarity Unit. Appendix B presents a representative sample of six items from the SVST. The items of this test are also multiple choice, with five options for each item, and with a 0/1 scoring. The total score on the test was considered as an indicator of the level of performance in spatial visualization. The Cronbach OL reliability coefficients of the SVST ranged from .59 to .80, whereas the pre-post Pearson correlation coefficients ranged from .66 to .80 (Tables C.1 and C.2 in Appendix C). The SIMT and the SVST were both administered during the same class period, and the total administration time did not exceed 45-50 minutes. In order to neutralize a possible effect due to a certain sequencing of the two tests, about half of the classes received the SIMT first, and the SVST second, while the other half received the two tests in 64 reverse order. The order of testing was randomly attributed to whole classes. Instructional‘Material The MGMP Instructional Model The Middle Grades Mathematics Project developed four instructional units. Each of these units requires two to three weeks of instructional time, and concentrates on a specific concept that is considered to be important in the mathematical develOpment of the middle grades level student. Two MGMP units will be briefly described here: (1) the Similarity Unit that formed the basis for the instructional intervention in this study, and (2) the Spptial Visualization Unit that was taught in several experimental classes in School 5 one year before this study. Each of the MGMP instructional units provides a carefully sequenced set of challenging and exploratory activities designed to lead the student to the understanding of a mathematical concept, and a carefully developed and very detailed instructional guide for the teacher. The units utilize an instructional model developed by Fitzgerald and Shroyer (1979). The model consists of three phases: launch (introducing new concepts, clarifying definitions, reviewing old concpets, and issuing a challenge), exploration (individual work in gathering data, sharing ideas looking for patterns, making conjectures, or developing other types of problem- solving strategies), and summary (demonstrating ways to organize data discussing the used strategies, and refining these strategies into efficient pmoblem-solving techniques). The launch and the summary 65 are conducted in a whole-class mode, while at the exploration Stage, the teacher becomes a fellow investigator who helps individual students to follow their own exploratory path of thinking. The MGM Similarithnit The concrete-activity orientation of the MGM Similarity Unit follows recommendations of the Piagetian research on geometrical development, and the instructional implications of the Van Hiele studies. The unit presents the concept of similarity at the Van Hiele levels I (properties of shapes) and II (relationships among properties of shapes) of geometrical development. The present neglect in the consideration of the Van Hiele levels in the teaching of geometry in the United States is believed to be a reason for present deficiencies in many children's understanding of geometrical concepts (Wirszup, 1976; Carpenter et al. , 1981). The MGM Similarity Unit is a response to deficiencies in the teaching of geometry. By offering a rich variety of experiences designed to fit the Van Hiele levels I and II, the unit contributes to the building of a solid base for further advance in the understanding of geometrical concepts. Moreover, the launching-exploration-summary model corresponds to Van Hiele's three steps required to help the child move to a higher developmental level: information, directed orientation, explanation. The unit contains nine activities that require 1-1 1/2 class periods each: 66 -figure enlargement by rubber bands -figure enlargement by point coordinates ~comparing rectangles -families of similar rectangles -comparing triangles -creating similar figures with repeating tiles -figure enlargement by point projections -enlarging pictures -applications of similar triangles. A more detailed overview of the unit is presented in Appendix D. The nine activities use concrete manipulatives to help provide the transition from the student's concrete to abstract thinking. The four Similarity-related topics of this study (basic properties, proportional reasoning, area growth, and applications) are encountered throughout the unit in a Spiral way. The MGM Spatial Visualization Unit The Spatial. Visualization. Unit includes. ten. carefully' sequenced activities on the representation of three-dimensional objects in two dimensional drawings, and vice versa, on the construction of three- dimensional objects with blocks from their two dimensional representations. The activities deal with the flat views of buildings as well as with the isometric drawings on dot paper (paper with dots arranged on diagonals rather than rows). In most of the activities the students are asked to perform some fairly demanding orientation and visualization tasks. They are asked to mentally rotate a building 67 and draw either flat views of the other sides or isometric drawings from other corners. Cubes are always available to help a student who needs to see the concrete object to be successful. Procedure and Data Collection The experimental instruction of the Similarity Unit and the data collection were conducted from January to May 1982. The whole sample of 695 students was tested on the Similarity Test and on the Spatial Visualization Subtest both before, and after instruction. The classroom teachers were provided with all the instructional materials, the testing material and written instructions concerning the administration of the tests. The tests were administered during the regular school day by the classroom math teachers. For all the teachers, this was the first time that they taught the Similarity Unit, although many of them have already taught another MGM instructional unit. Prior to instruction, a two-hour workShOp for the teachers was conducted in each of the five participating school, but no direct intervention by the staff of the project has been provided during instruction. The workshop acquainted the teachers with the nine activities in the unit, recommended instructional strategies, and presented a more in-depth view of the geometrical concepts involved. 68 The Statistical Design of the Study To analyze the data collected during the study, analyses of means, standard deviations, correlations, Multivariate and Univariate Analyses of Variance and ANOVA with Repeated Measures were conducted. A detailed description of the hypotheses tested and the statistical procedures used for each purpose of the study will be given in the next chapter, where they will be followed by a description of the findings. For the significance tests, the assumptions were that the subjects were responding independently of one another and that the error vectors had a multivariate normal distribution with mean zero and a common variance covariance matrix. The fact that this study was conducted in a regular school setting made the assumption of independence and normality feasible. The effects of instructional intervention were examined by using design 6 from Campbell and Stanley (1963): 01 X 02, where 01 and 02 are the scores collected before and after instruction, and is the treatment (instructional intervention). A control (no-intervention) group was not employed in this study, due to (1) physical difficulties of recruiting and testing a group of equivalent size and structure, and (2) the relatively small possibility of confounding effects as a result of the Short duration of instruction and the wide variety of sampled subjects. The collapsing of students from four different school buildings into one sample would probably increase to a certain degree the 69 heterogeneity of the pOpulation. However, the purpose of the study was to examine the effect of instruction pertaining to similarity in heterogeneous, middle-class suburban school conditions, rather than in an artifically homogeneous environment. There are two alternative units of analysis for this study: teacher versus student. Hopkins (1982) compares these two possibilities and concludes that taking the teacher as a unit ignores the fact that the observational unit is the student and actually only exchanges the assumption of independence among students with another not necessarily more true assumption of independence among teachers. Moreover, post- hoc examinations of the results did not reveal any outstanding discrepancies in the results by school building or by teacher. Thus, this study will consider the student as unit of analysis. All the analyses were carried out on the CYBER 170, Model 750 computer at the Michigan State University Computer Center, using SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) programs. CHAPTER.IV ANALYSIS OF DATA AND RESULTS Introduction This chapter presents the findings that correspond to the six research questions of this study. Each of the six questions that were posed in the introductory chapter will be restated here as a hypothesis. Each hypothesis will be followed by a description of: (1) the statistical design and procedures used to examine the data, (2) a description of the obtained results, and (3) a short summary of results related to the initial question. Basically, the Six hypotheses stated in this chapter have been tested by employing Analysis of Variance. The means, standard deviations and correlation coefficients computed allowed for further interpretations of the results. The Analyses uwumawafim HSOM a“ Hm>ma momum mo mocmauoWHOm ~.q wuawwm . mezomu oszowalleat. (A) 2 (8) 3 (C) 4 (D) 6 (EH/4 130 VERBAL PRESENTATION MODE 2. A 1x5 rectangle grows into a 4x20 rectangle. The area of the new rectangle is how many times larger than the area of the small rectangle? (A) 3 times (3) 4 times (C) 5 times (D) 15 times (E) 16 times 6. If the lengths