A STUDY DESIGNED TO DErERM'INETHE EFFECTS oEf : ¥ , i E . -TWO TREATMENTSON‘THE-JNTERPERSONALNEEDrV-W” ‘ \ BEHAVIORS OF INCLUSION, CONTROL, AND AFFECTION] __ AS MEASURED BY THE. FIRO’-B j ’ V Dissertation for the Degree'of-PhLng' ~ - 'MICH'IGANfSTATE UNIVERSITY _ .4 g EGERALD'P. STURM ” ’ .1977 " ’ _.t.-Q1}37.;“g1 «s- ..e. . L [B P R Y fit/1155:3312: State University as. v IHE‘sztz This is to certifyithat the thesis entitled A STUDY DESIGNED TO DETERMINE THE EFFECTS OF TWO TREATMENTS ON THE INTERPERSONAL NEED BEHAVIORS 0F INCLUSION, CONTROL, AND AFFECTION AS MEASURED BY THE FIRO-B presented by GERALD P . STURM has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for degree in Secondary Ed. 8 Curriculum $5M MEQNM Major professor PH.D. 2‘ 25' 77 Date 0-7 639 - : -,. -..-u ‘t'm'flfl‘ - ‘\~"~‘ -l— — x~ I: ~. ~‘. - ~ - r . ABSTRACT A STUDY DESIGNED TO DETERMINE THE EFFECTS OF TWO TREATMENTS ON THE INTERPERSONAL NEED BEHAVIORS OF INCLUSION, CONTROL, AND AFFECTION AS MEASURED BY THE FIRO-B BY Gerald P. Sturm The purpose of this study was to determine if treatment A and/or treatment B would significantly reduce the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as measured by the FIRO-B. The following variables were investigated: the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection; and whether or not treatment A or treatment B was effective in (l) reducing the total number of incon- gruencies shown by teachers in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection, and (2) reducing the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in each of the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as measured by the FIRO-B. The participants in the study were all of the educators enrolled in the two sections of Education 601 at Wright State University during the fall quarter, 1975. There were fifty (50) enrolled in the section which met on Gerald P. Sturm Tuesdays and received treatment A. There were forty—seven (47) enrolled in the section which met on Thursdays and received treatment B. Therefore, there were ninety-seven (97) subjects who participated in the study. The FIRO theory provided the basis for this study. The FIRO theory suggests that interpersonal needs can be subsumed into the three areas of inclusion, control, and affection; and further that when individuals come together in groups these interpersonal needs represent the concerns of the individual group members with inclu- sion being the first concern, control the second concern, and affection the third and final concern. The FIRO-B, the instrument used in this study, measures the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection. The FIRO-B was administered to all partici- pants during their first class session and again at their last class session, thus the source of pre and post test data. To maintain consistency the FIRO-B was administered to all participants by the researcher. It is also noted here that the researcher was not involved in administering either treatment A or treatment B. The data were analyzed using a statistical tech- nique known as chi square (X2). All of the null hypo- theses were accepted; therefore, it was concluded that: (l) instruction did not make a significant difference in reducing the total number of incongruencies in the Gerald P. Sturm interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection; (2) treatment A and/or treatment B did not make a significant difference in reducing incongruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affec- tion; (3) treatment A and/or treatment B did not make a significant difference in reducing the number of incon- gruencies in the interpersonal need area of inclusion; (4) treatment A and/or treatment B did not make a signi— ficant difference in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of control; and (5) treat- ment A and/or treatment B did not make a significant difference in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of affection. A STUDY DESIGNED TO DETERMINE THE EFFECTS OF TWO TREATMENTS ON THE INTERPERSONAL NEED BEHAVIORS OF INCLUSION, CONTROL, AND AFFECTION AS MEASURED BY THE FIRO-B BY ,v“ Gerald Pf Sturm A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Secondary Education and Curriculum 1977 DEDICATION To Joann - my most valuable companion Deb, Dan, and Doug — my hope and joy Vickie - my mother and In Memory Of Si — my father ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS It is only appropriate to mention the following people, who certainly proved to me that people need people: To Dale Alam, my committee chairperson, for continued help and support, and who has given me more than I cangut into words. I will cherish that unselfish giving forever. To George Myers, Richard Gardner, and John Suehr, committee members who provided guidance, support, and encouragement which has given me new confidence and purpose in life. I will make every effort to be this to others as I continue my work. To Troy Stearns, my original committee chairperson, for sharing his wisdom and knowledge with me, while at the same time giving me complete trust and expert advice. To Charles Blackman who provided me with very unique and helpful kind of support, before and during my program. To James Dillehay for the extensive help given to me throughout the preparation of my dissertation. A person without whom I would have never finished. To Pat Gilbert for human and technical support. To Barb Borders for her special effort in helping with the finished product. To special friends, Art Tolley, Betty Cady, Ann Yezzi, Norm Sterchele, Judy and Jerry Wallace, Dennis Badaczewski, Eileen Fernandez, Joe Young, Tom Matczynski, Carl Benner, and Irene and Whitey May for the kind of support each one gave to me. To Gerald Poor, Maynard Hoeh, Richard Asiala, Lilburn Hoehn, and Paul Cochran for expressing belief ( and encouragement at various points in my professional life. To my mother and father who gave unconditional support which made this whole effort possible. To Deb, Dan, and Doug for giving me a purpose for continuing to grow and be. And to Joann who beyond belief and all possible expectations stood by me, encouraged me, helped me, and most of all put up with me, a special thanks of your own. Thanks to all of you. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . Chapter I. II. III. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . Background of the Problem to be Investigated . . . . . . . . Statement of the Problem . . . . . Questions for Study . . . . . . . Definitions of Terms . . . . . . Significance of the Study . . . . . Purpose of the Study . . . . . . . Limitations of the Study . . . . . Organization of the Study . . . . . THEORY AND RESEARCH . . . . . . . The FIRO Theory . . . . . . . Interpersonal Need . . . . . . Inclusion, Control, and Affection . Inclusion Behavior . . . . . . Control Behavior . . . . . . . Affection Behavior . . . Types of Interpersonal Behavior . . Inclusion Types . . . . . . Control Types . . . . . . Affection Types . . . . . . Summary of FIRO Theory . . . . . Research Related to FIRO Theory . . . Group Composition . . . . Individual Behavior Change . . Summary of Research Related to FIRO Theory . . . . . STUDY DESIGN AND INSTRUMENTATION . . . Questions for Study . . . . . Description of the Population . . . . V Page viii 43 44 45 Chapter Description of Treatments Treatment A Treatment B Instrumentation Validity Reliability Purpose of Instrument Data Collection Data Analysis IV. Summary of Findings V. SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND REFLECTIONS Summary Conclusions Recommendations Reflections APPENDICES BIBLIOGRAPHY 0 ANALYSIS OF DATA 0 vi RECOMMENDATIONS, LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Number of Incongruencies Identified by the Administration of the FIRO—B . . . 55 2. Analysis of Treatment Effects Related to Total Number of Incongruencies . . . . 56 3. Analysis of Treatment Effects Related to the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Inclusion . . . 57 4. Analysis of Treatment Effects Related to the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Control . . . 58 5. Analysis of Treatment Effects Related to the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Affection . . . 58 LIST OF APPENDICES Appendix A. Course Syllabus Used for Treatment A B. Course Syllabus Used for Treatment B C. Waiver Form for Research Involving Human Subjects . . . . . . . D. Instrumentation . . . . . FIRO— B Directions . . . . FIRO- B Instrument . . . . FIRO-B Tally Sheet . E. Statistical Analysis for Tables 2, 4 and 5 . . . . . . viii Page 69 74 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Background of the Problem to Be Investigated Groups are incalculably in the life of every human being. And skills in group functioning are vital to all of us. Belonging to groups is the most important aspect of your life. The quality of your life depends upon the effectiveness of the groups to which you belong, and this effectiveness is largely determined by your personal group skills and knowledge of group processes. Why are groups important? As humans we are social animals and have an inherent social nature. It is not our nature to live alone. We are born into a group called the family and would not survive the first few minutes, the first few years of our lives without membership in this group. Our personal survival as well as survival of our species has always been linked to the interrela— tionships formed among human beings. Our personal identity is derived from the way in which we are perceived and treated by other members of our groups. Almost all of our time is spent inter- acting in groups; we are educated in groups, we work in groups, we worship in groups, we play in groups. Our whole life is spent in a variety of group memberships. Even our species identity as a human is developed in our interactions with others within groups. What makes us human is the way in which we interact with other persons, and we learn how to interact within the groups in which we are socialized and educated. At all stages of our lives we need to belong to groups. 1 Teachers in our society find themselves members of a variety of groups, some of which may be their own 1David W. Johnson and Frank P. Johnson, Joining Together (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc., 1975), p. l. choosing (i.e., joining a professional association or community service club) and some which may be conditions of employment (i.e., a faculty member in a specific school building or a specified and required committee assignment). The field of school administration has incorporated a broader range of approaches to leadership, i.e., participatory management. Thus, the teachers may find themselves members of a group which is a component part of the operational procedure used in the organization where employed. The teacher may be called upon to func- tion in a group which is charged with responsibility for solving problems, making decisions, generating new ideas, and/or evaluating outcomes. Max Weber (1947) theorized that the expansion of bureaucracy has encroached on the more traditional forms of governance. As modern institutions increase in size and complexity individuals who are part of the organization tend to take on more specialized roles and functions. Weber argued that membership in these organizations becomes restricted to those who have specialized training for their positions. In recent years teachers have formally joined together to form an influential power base. Teachers' groups have increased their effectiveness so that the teacher now influences school organization and educational legislation to a much greater degree than ever before. Teachers, through this joining together, are increasingly 2Herbert J. Walberg, "Professional Role Discontin- uities in Educational Careers," Review of Educational Research, Vol. 40, No. 3, (AERA, June, 1970), p. 410. involved in making decisions related to the organization and operation of schools. "Because the American teacher has stopped being passive and has started reaching for a share in directing the process of education, their energies and ideas will have much more influence than they would 3 Teachers, then may find themselves have ten years ago." a part of a group which must pay attention to continually improving its effectiveness. Teachers might also be in a school that is using the team teaching concept in one form or another. Schools might be implementing IGE (Individually Guided Education), using interdisciplinary teaming, or might have a school within a school which finds teachers and students as members of the same group. The keys to the success of team teaching are cooperation, pre-planning, flexibility in sche- duling, variety of materials, and individualiza- tion. Pooling the professional and personal strengths of each of the team members offers richer opportunities to the pupils and stimulates professional growth of the teachers. Essential for smooth and effective operation is a team leader knowledgeable in curricular development with special skill in group dynamics.4 In terms of facilitating learning, teachers seldom find themselves placed in a situation which does not 3Harold Howe, Picking Up The Options (Washington D.C.: Department of Elementary School Principals, N.E.A., 1968), p. 206. 4Jean Marie King and VonHerbert I. Haden, "Team Teaching," Innovations in Education: Pros & Cons (Worthington, Ohio: Charles A. Jones Publishing Co., 1971), PP. 157—159. involve a group of students. For the most part teachers find themselves developing and implementing instructional processes which will effect a group of students. Many times the effectiveness of an instructional strategy or a teacher behavior is related to the knowledge and understanding of group processes. Understanding group processes and group dynamics as they relate to instruction leads then, in the opinion of the researcher, to implica- tions in curriculum development. The area of curriculum development creates many opportunities and situations that demand decision making on the part of classroom teachers. Changes in the last few years have moved today's teacher into more complex group circumstances which require more understanding and expertise. Curricu- lum development processes more than occasionally involve groups of teachers who are asked to make decisions rela— tive to needs, goals and objectives, and resource materials. The teacher, while an individual with uniqueness, is also a part of something bigger than self. If this is true, then to expand by a process of growth, the ability to function within a group and to understand group develop- ment becomes a necessity. To be an effective group member, you need a conceptualization, or idea, of group effective- ness and an understanding of how your behavior can contribute to this effectiveness. Any effective group has three core activities: (1) accomplishing its goals, (2) maintaining itself internally, and (3) developing and changing in ways that improve its effectiveness. One theory of group development, the FIRO theory, suggests that three dimensions: inclusion, control, and affection, in this order, usually predominate in the development of a group. Inclusion problems, such as the decision to be in or out of the group, usually occur first and are followed by control issues and finally by affection problems. This order is not rigid, but it seems that the nature of group life is such that people tend first to determine whether they want to be in a group, then to find out what place of influence they have, and finally decide how personally close they will become. Further within each of the three phases individuals seem to concentrate first on their relations with leadership within the group and then on their relations to each other.6 Given that group effectiveness is predicated on the FIRO theory, that group members have the interpersonal needs of inclusion, control, and affection, the question with which this study deals is the effectiveness of each of two approaches used in helping teachers become more effective group members through the reduction of incon- gruencies7 in the interpersonal needs areas of inclusion, 5David W. Johnson and Frank P. Johnson, Joining Together (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc., 1975), p. 3. 6William C. Schutz, The Interpersonal Underworld (Palo Alto: Science and Behavior Books, Inc., 1966), pp. 169—171. 7Defined in Chapter I of this study, p. 10. control, and affection. This study intends to lend insight into the effectiveness of two different treatments as they affect the interpersonal need structure of indi— vidual participants. Statement of the Problem How do we encourage people to become more congruent? Does formalized instruction bring about a reduction of incongruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclu— sion, control, and affection? The independent variable in this study is Treatment A and Treatment B, and the dependent variable is interpersonal need (inclusion, con- trol, and affection ) structure of individual participants. Using the FIRO theory, that people have the interpersonal needs of inclusion, control, and affection, and that people with incongruencies in these areas can be helped to become more self-congruent and therefore more compatible with others and more effective group members, the problem focuses upon the approach used to assist people in the process of reducing incongruencies. Questions for Study 1. Does instruction in the area of interpersonal communication reduce the number of incongruencies shown by teachers as measured by the FIRO-B regardless of method? 2. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers, as measured by the FIRO-B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treatment B? 3. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the area of inclusion, as measuredlnrtheFTRO-B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treatment B? 4. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the area of control, as measured by the FIRO—B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treatment B? 5. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the area of affection, as measured by the FIRO-B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treat- ment B? Definitions of Terms Interpersonal: The term "interpersonal" refers to relations which occur between people as opposed to rela- tions in which at least one participant is inanimate. It is assumed that, owing to the psychological presence of other people, interpersonal situations lead to behavior in an individual that differs from the behavior of the individual when he is not in the presence of other persons.8 FIRQ: Fundamental Interpersonal Relations Orientation. It signifies the basic idea that every 8William C. Schutz, The Interpersonal Underworld (Palo Alto: Science and Behavior Books, Inc., 1966), p. 14. person orients himself in characteristic ways toward other people, and the basic belief that knowledge of these orientations allows for considerable understanding of individual behavior and the interaction of people. The word is pronounced to rhyme with "Cairo."9 FIRO-B: Stands for Fundamental Interpersonal Relations Orientation--Behavior. Is the key measuring instrument designed to measure an individual's orienta- tion to the interpersonal needs of inclusion, control, 10 and affection. Inclusion: The interpersonal need for inclusion is defined behaviorally as the need to establish and maintain a satisfactory relation with people with respect to interaction and association. "Satisfactory relation" includes (1) a psychologically comfortable relation with people somewhere on a dimension ranging from originating or initiating interaction with all people to not initiat— ing interaction with anyone, and (2) a psychologically comfortable relation with people with respect to elicit- ing behavior from them somewhere on a dimension ranging from always initiating interaction with the self to never initiating interaction with the self.11 91bid., p. ix. lOIbid., p. 57. llIbid., p. 18. Control: The interpersonal need for control is defined behaviorally as the need to establish and maintain a satisfactory relation with people with respect to con— trol and power. "Satisfactory relation" includes (1) a psychologically comfortable relation with people some- where on a dimension ranging from controlling all the behavior of other people to not controlling any behavior of others, and (2) psychologically comfortable relation with people with respect to eliciting behavior from them somewhere on a dimension ranging from always being con- trolled by them to never being controlled by them.12 Affection: The interpersonal need for affection is defined behaviorally as the need to establish and maintain a satisfactory relation with others with respect to love and affection. Affection always refers to a two-person (dyadic) relation. "Satisfactory relation" includes (1) a psychologically comfortable relation with others somewhere on a dimension ranging from initiating close, personal relations with everyone to originating close, personal relation with no one, and (2) a psycho- logically comfortable relation with people with respect to eliciting behavior from them on a dimension ranging from always originating close, personal relations 12Loc. cit. 10 toward the self to never originating close, personal relations with the self.13 e — Expressed: The behavior expressed to others in the interpersonal needsareasof inclusion, control, and affection. May also be synonymous with behavior "given" to others.14 w — Wanted: The behavior wanted from others in the interpersonal needs areas of inclusion, control, and affection. May also be synonymous with "desired" or "taken" behavior from others.15 Incongruence: A difference of more than two (2) between "e" (expressed) and "w" (wanted) on the nine (9) point scale of the FIRO—B. Congruence: The term congruence, when used in this study, represents a balance in any of the inter— personal need areas of inclusion, control, and affec- tion. This balance is shown mathematically by the dif- ference in the "e" (expressed) and "w" (wanted) score on the FIRO—B being less than two (2). Significance of the Study This study has significance for (1) those involved in teacher education both at preservice and l3Ibid., p. 20. 14J. William Pfeiffer and Richard Heslin, Instru- mentations in Human Relations Training (Iowa City: University Associates, 1973), p. 139. 15 Loc. cit. ll inservice levels, (2) those involved in curriculum development, (3) those involved in designing instructional alternatives for students, (4) those persons in adminis- trative roles who are concerned about the quality of life experienced by people within their organization, and (5) those concerned about improving human relations skills no matter who is involved. Particularly, this study has significance for those responsible for educating prospective teachers. The assumption is made in this study that it is impor- tant for teachers to develop understanding in group theory, dynamics, and techniques, and further that teachers are able to put those understandings into practice. The study specifically focuses on the approach used in attempting to help teachers become more congruent in the interpersonal needs areas of inclusion, control, and affection which are the basis for the group theory this study uses. The study is significant in that the effect of two instructional strategies, attempting to reduce incongruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection, will be determined. The results of this study will lend insight into the question, "What type of treatments should be utilized by those who are concerned about designing instructional 12 strategies which deal with the interpersonal needs of inclusion, control, and affection?" This study is also significant in that it makes application of the FIRO theory and therefore contributes awareness about the theory to those in the helping pro- fessions. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study is to investigate whether or not each of two different treatments used in a communications course will reduce incongruencies in the interpersonal needs areas of inclusion, control, and affection. While the FIRO theory identifies and pre- sents areas of interpersonal needs, the search continues for ways to help people (1) develop self-awareness in relationship to those needs, (2) look at the effect the needs of others have upon them, (3) look at the effect of their needs upon others, and (4) relate their needs to their behaviors. This study looks at two treatments to determine if one is more effective than the other or whether either is effective in helping teachers develop an improved balance (congruence) between (1) the amount of inclusion they desire from others and the amount of inclusion they give to others, (2) the amount they desire to control others and the amount they wish to be controlled by 13 others, and (3) the amount of affection they desire from others and the amount of affection they give to others. If better ways can be found to assist people in becoming more aware of their interpersonal needs and if treatments that result in a person's need structure being changed can be identified, then this study would be useful to anyone in the helping professions who chooses to apply the FIRO theory. Limitations of the Study As is the case in most studies, decisions con— cerning the scope and limitations have to be made based on the variables which can be controlled. The variables which affect persons' interpersonal needs structure are numerous and complex; in fact, it may be impossible to identify all of them. This study was done within the confines set forth by the following limitations: 1. The FIRO theory is the complete basis for describing interpersonal needs. 2. The FIRO—B is the only instrument used to determine whether or not interpersonal needs were changed. 3. That a balance (congruence) between "e" (expressed) and "w" (wanted) represents a more healthy person in relation to interpersonal needs. 4. That an incongruence, that is, an imbalance between "e" (expressed) and "w" (wanted), creates dis- order and anxiety which are undesirable. l4 5. The population used in the study was confined to teachers who were enrolled in two sections of Educa— tion 601 at Wright State University. 6. The total time was a period of eleven weeks representing one quarter (term) during which pretest data was collected, treatments A and B were applied, and posttest data was collected. 7. That two different treatments were designed to be utilized, one with the section of Education 601 which met Tuesday evenings and one with the section of Education 601 which met Thursday evenings. Organization of the Study Chapter I Introduction Chapter II Theory and Research Chapter III Study Design and Instrumentation Chapter IV Analysis of Data Chapter V Summary, Conclusions, Recommenda- tions, and Reflections CHAPTER II THEORY AND RESEARCH The purpose of this study is to determine if either of two treatments is effective in helping teachers develop an improved balance in the interper— sonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection. While the FIRO theory itself has been developed and supported through research, the results relative to instructional strategies designed to apply the theory are incomplete. The treatments utilized in this study were designed, in part, to apply the FIRO theory. The theory presented in this chapter is limited to the FIRO theory, and the research presented is limited to research which involves the FIRO theory and the FIRO-B instrument because of the uniqueness of the FIRO theory and the parameters of this study. This chapter is (l) a presentation of the FIRO theory, and (2) a review of the research related to the FIRO theory. As the reader will be able to see, there is a noticeable lack of research related to instructional strategies designed to apply the FIRO theory. 15 16 The FIRO Theory "FIRO" stands for "Fundamental Interpersonal Relations Orientation." It was chosen because it indicated the basic concept that every person orients himself or herself in characteristic ways toward other people, and the basic belief that knowledge of these orientations allows for considerable understanding of individual behavior and the interaction of people. The word in pronounced to rhyme with "Cairo."l The FIRO theory is an axiomatic theory. The theory is based upon four specific postulates which were developed through the testing and researching of various theorems. The four postulates are the proposi— tions expressing what are taken to be the most funda- mental ideas embodied in the theory. The four postulates contained in the FIRO theory are: Postulate l - The Postulate of Interpersonal Needs. (a) Every individual has three interpersonal needs: inclusion, control, and affection. (b) Inclusion, control, and affection consti- tute a sufficient set of interpersonal behavior for the prediction and explanation of interpersonal phenomena. Postulate 2 - The Postulate of Relational Continuity. An individual's expressed interpersonal behavior will be similar to the behavior he experienced in his earliest interpersonal relations in the following way: Principle 1William C. Schutz, The Interpersonal Underworld (Palo Alto: Science and Behavior Books, Inc., 1966), p. ix. 17 of Constancy. When he perceives his adult position in an interpersonal situa— tion to be similar to his own position in his parent—child relation, his adult behavior positively covaries with his childhood behavior toward his parents (or significant others). Principle of Identification. When he per- ceives his adult position in an inter— personal situation to be similar to his parents' position in his parent-child relation, his adult behavior positively covaries with the behavior of his parents (or significant others). Postulate 3 - The Postulate of Compatibility. If the compatibility of one group, h, is greater than that of another group, m, then the goal achievement of h will exceed that of m. Postulate 4 — The Postulate of Group Development. The formation and development of two or more people into an interpersonal relation (that is, a group) always proceeds in the same sequence, as follows: Principle of Group Integration. For the time period starting with the group's termination, the predominant area of interpersonal behavior begins with inclusion, is followed by control, and finally by affection. This cycle may recur. Principle of Group Resolution. The last three intervals prior to a group's anti- cipated termination follow the opposite sequence in that the predominant area of interpersonal behavior is first affection, then control, and finally inclusion. In summary the FIRO theory states that there are three interpersonal need areas, inclusion, control, and affection, sufficient for the prediction of inter— personal behavior. Orientations which an individual acquires toward behavior in these areas are relatively 2Ibid., pp. 196-199. l8 invariant over time. All interpersonal relationships follow the same general developmental sequence. It begins with inclusion behavior, is followed by control behavior, and finally, affection behavior. This cycle may recur. When the relation approaches termination it reverses direction, and investment from the relation is withdrawn in the order of affection, control, and inclusion. From these postulates it is theoretically possible to predict the course of a relation if we know the interpersonal orientations of the individual members of the relation and the interpersonal description of the circumstances under which they will interact.3 While this study has as its basis the FIRO theory, it more specifically uses the propositions expressed in Postulate One and Postulate Four. Therefore, those two components of the FIRO theory will be further described. Interpersonal Need The term "interpersonal" refers to relations that occur between people as opposed to relations in which at least one participant is inanimate. It is assumed that, owing to the psychological presence of other people, interpersonal situations lead to behavior in an individual that differs from the behavior of the individual when he is not in the presence of other persons. 3Ibid., p. 200. 19 An optimally useful definition of "interpersonal" is one such that all situations classified as interpersonal have important properties in common--properties that are in general different from those of noninterpersonal situations. An interpersonal situation is one involving two or more persons in which these individuals take account of each other for some purpose or decision. Further, it is often useful to consider situations as interpersonal in which behavior is determined by expecta- tions of the behavior of others, even if the others are not physically present.4 The other term in the phrase under discussion is "need." A "need" is defined in terms of a situation or condition of an individual the nonrealization of which leads to undesirable consequences. An interpersonal need is one that may be satisfied only through the attainment of a satisfactory relation with other people. The satisfaction of a need is a necessary condition for the avoidance of undesirable consequences of illness and death. A discrepancy between the satisfaction of an interpersonal need and the present state of an organism engenders a feeling in the organism called anxiety.5 4Ibid., pp. 14—15. 51bid., p. 15. 20 Inclusion, Control, and Affection Inclusion behavior is a behavior aimed at satis- fying the interpersonal need for inclusion. It means a comfortableness with (1) being interested in other people somewhere from originating or initiating all interaction to doing no originating or initiating of interaction with anyone, and (2) having others interested in self somewhere between always wanting others to focus on self to never wanting others to focus on self. In relationship to self image the need for inclusion is the need to feel significant and worthwhile. Control behavior is a behavior aimed at satis- fying the interpersonal need for control. it means a comfortableness with (1) being able to respect others somewhere from controlling all the behavior of other people to not controlling any behavior of others, and (2) having others respect self somewhere from wanting others to always control self to wanting others to never control self. In relationship to self-image, the need for control is the need to feel competent and responsible as a person. Affection behavior is a behavior aimed at satis- fying the interpersonal need for affection. It means a comfortableness with (1) being able to love others somewhere from initiating close, personal relations with everyone to originating close, personal relations with 21 no one, and (2) eliciting behavior from others somewhere from always wanting others to initiate close, personal relationships with self to never wanting others to initiate close, personal relationships toward self. In relationship to self-image, the need for affection is the need to feel the self is lovable. Inclusion Behavior To generalize, inclusion behavior refers to association between people. There are many terms which can mean the same as positive or negative inclusion behavior. Some terms which may mean positive inclusion behavior are: "mingle,' "communicate,' "comrade," "member,' "togetherness,' "join," "belong," "associate," "companion," and "extrovert." Some terms which may mean negative inclusion behavior are: "exclusion," "outsider," "outcast," "detached," "withdrawn," "isolate," "aban— doned," or "ignored."6 The need to be included manifests itself as wanting to be attended to, to attract atten— tion and interest. The classroom hellion who throws erasers is often objecting mostly to the lack of attention paid him. Even if he is given negative affection he is partially satisfied, because at least someone is pay— ing attention to him. Many times people, when in groups, talk a lot to gain a prominence. They may not be interested in 61bid., pp. 18—21. 71bid., p. 21. 22 power or dominance, but merely wanting an inclusion identity. The person who is the laugh of the party is another example of a prominence seeker. Joining a fraternal or service organization is primarily an inclusion behavior because it represents belonging and togetherness while it by no means neces- sarily indicates a liking for other members or a defi- nite desire for power. Identity is an essential part of inclusion. The person is primarily concerned that he is recognized and paid attention to separate and apart from others, he needs a particular identity. If a person is not seen as an individual with a particular identity, others are interested enough in him to find out his particular and individual characteristics. This does not necessarily mean that others have affection for him or that they respect him. Others may be exploring his particular and individual characteristics to find points of vulnerability. At the outset of interpersonal relations a common issue is that of commitment, the decision to become involved in a given relation or activity. Usually, in the initial testing of a relation, individuals try to identify them- selves to one another to find out which facet of themselves others will be interested in. Frequently a member is silent for awhile because he is not sure that people are inter- in him. These behaviors, too, are pr1mar11y in the inclusion area. This, then is the flavor of inclusion. It has to do with inter— acting with peOple, with attention, acknow- ledgment, being known, prominence, recognition, prestige, status, and fame; with identity, individuality, understanding, interest, 23 commitment, and participation. It is unlike affection in that it does not involve strong emotional attachments to individual persons. It is unlike control in that the preoccupation is with prominence, not dominance.8 Control Behavior Control behavior basically makes reference to the decision—making process which occurs between people. Some terms which indicate that control exists, and there- fore are known as primarily positive control are: "influence," "dominance,' "ruler,' "authority,' "superior officer," "power," and "leader." Some terms which indicate lack of control, and therefore are labeled primary negative control are: "follower," "submissive," "henpecked," "milquetoast," "anarchy," "rebellion," and "resistance." The need for control manifests itself as the desire for power, authority, and control over others and therefore over one's future. At the other end is the need to be controlled, to have responsibility taken away. Manifesta- tions of the power drive are very clear. A more subtle form is exemplified by the current magazine advertising campaign featuring the influential. This is a person who controls others through the power he has to influence their behavior. A direct way of gaining control over others is to acquire wealth or come to power in a political role. Many times these types of control involve coercion and 81bid., p. 22. 9Loc. cit. 24 bargaining versus a more subtle approach like persuasion. In groups the desire to gain leader role status or to have ideas influence direction and operation of the group are manifestations of control behavior. If there is an argument in the group the inclusion seeker can be distinguished from the control seeker: the inclusion seeker, person seeking prominence, wants to be a parti- cipant in the argument while the control seeker, person seeking dominance, wants to win the argument. Both of these roles are different from affectional wishes of the group members. There are many forms of control behavior, such as, walking to the blackboard and taking the chalk may be a form of competitive success, using intellectual superior- ity over others may represent a motivation to control others, or rebelling against the existing structure may represent a no-one—is-going-to-control—me stance which is a "I will control" behavior. Control is also manifested in behavior toward others controlling the self. Expressions of independence and rebellion exemplify lack of willingness to be controlled, while compliance, submission, and taking orders indicate various degrees of accepting the control of others. There is no necessary relation between an indi- vidual's behavior toward controlling others and his behavior toward being controlled. The domineering sergeant may accept orders from the lieutenant with pleasure and gratefulness, while the neighborhood bully may also rebel against his parents; two persons who control others differ in the degree to which they allow others to control them. Thus the flavor on control is transmitted by behavior involving influence, 25 leadership, power, coercion, authority, accom— plishment, intellectual superiority, high achievement, and independence, as well as depend— ency (for decision making), rebellion, resistance, and submission. It differs from inclusion beha- vior in that it does not require prominence. The concept of the "power behind the throne" is an excellent example of a role that would fill a high control need and a low need for inclusion. The "joker" exemplifies the oppo— site. Control behavior differs from affection behavior in that it has to do with power rela- tions rather than emotional closeness. The frequent difficulties between those who want to get down to business and those who want to know one another illustrate a situation in which control behavior is more important for some and affection behavior for others. Affection Behavior Affection behavior generally refers to close personal feelings between two people. Affection can only occur between two people at any one time, whereas inclusion and control relations may occur either in dyads or between one person and a group of persons. Some terms which mean affection and are primarily positive are: "like," "personal," "positive feelings," "emotionally close," "friendship," and "love." Some terms which mean lack of affection and are primarily negative are "dislike," "cool," "emotionally distant," and "hate." The need for affection is fulfilled through behavior which leads to becoming emotionally close. In addition to being emotionally close, an affectional 26 relationship includes the element of confiding innermost wishes, anxieties, and feelings. The strong affectional relationship is usually accompanied by a high degree sharing of these emotions. Affection behavior in groups can be character- ized by overtures of friendship and differentiation between members. Affection behavior may be avoided by being equally friendly to all group members, thus an example of inclusion behavior rather than affection behavior. A difference between affection behavior, inclu- sion behavior, and control behavior is illus- trated by the different feelings a man has in being turned down by a fraternity, failed in a course by a professor, and rejected by his girl. The fraternity excludes him and tells him, in effect, that they as a group don't have suffi- cient interest in him. The professor fails him and says, in effect, that he finds him incom— petent in his field. His girl rejects him, and tells him, in effect, that she doesn't find him lovable. Thus, the flavor of affection is embodied in situations of love, emotional closeness, personal confidences, and intimacy. Negative affection is characterized by hate, hostility, and emotional rejection.l Types of Interpersonal Behavior12 In each of the interpersonal need areas, inclu- sion, control, and affection there are three types of interpersonal behavior: (1) deficient--indicating that llIbid., p. 24. lzIbid., pp. 25-31. 27 the individual is not trying directly to satisfy the need, (2) excessive-—indicating that the individual is constantly trying to satisfy the need, and (3) ideal—— indicating satisfaction of the need. Inclusion Types The Undersocia1--This interpersonal behavior is seen as introverted and withdrawn. The undersocial person avoids associating with others and most frequently rejects invitations to join others. This person con— sciously keeps a distance between self and others and doesn't want involvement with others because of a fear of losing privacy. In most cases the undersocial person unconsciously wants attention and recognition from others, but harbors a fear that others will ignore and have no interest in him. There is a strong drive toward self— sufficiency as a method for existence without others. This person then directs much energy toward self— preservation and therefore creates a world of his own in which his existence is more secure. This withdrawal is usually accompanied with anxiety and hostility, and often results in feelings of superiority or the feeling that others don't understand me. A direct way of expressing withdrawal is non- association with others and lack of involvement and commitment. A subtle expression of withdrawal might be 28 a behavior pattern of always being late or not keeping engagements. The reference that is being made to self-image is that of worthlessness. Many times the undersocial person has a low motivation to live. The degree the individual is committed to living probably is reflected in his level of enthusiasm, perserverance, and involvement. The Oversocial—- This interpersonal behavior is expressed in the form of extroversion. This person seeks people incessantly and wants others to seek him out. Like the undersocial person, he is also afraid others will ignore him. The interpersonal dynamics are the same as those of the withdrawn person, but the overt behavior is the opposite. The oversocial is the type that can't be alone; most of the activities in which he participates are designed to be done together. The interpersonal behavior" then, is designed to focus attention on self, to make others notice self, to be prominent, and to be listened to. The direct avenue to achievement is intensive, exhibitionistic behavior. A more subtle approach is to acquire status through name dropping or to ask startling questions. The Socia1--The social person is comfortable being with people and also comfortable being alone. This person can be a high, low, or moderate participator 29 in a group without anxiety. He is capable of making a strong commitment to certain groups or persons as well as being comfortable with withholding commitment in situa- tions where it is felt to be appropriate. In reference to self-image, there is a feeling of worth and significance as a person. There is a capa- bility of being genuinely interested in others and a feel- ing that others are interested in self. There is an identity and an individuality. Usually the social person has integrated aspects of a large number of individuals into a configuration which can be identified as self. Control Types The Abdicrat-- The interpersonal behavior of the abdicrat person tends toward submission and the avoidance of power and responsibility. Most usually this person moves into subordinate positions which are removed from decision—making. He consciously wants others to relieve him of obligations. Many times he will not take control of a situation in which he is needed, such as an accident on the playground. This kind of person is usually a follower. Unconsciously, he has the feeling that he is incapable of responsible adult behavior and that others know it. He is most comfortable respond— ing to situations with avoidance. Behind this feeling is 30 anxiety, hostility, and lack of trust toward those who might be of help. These feelings are usually expressed in the form of pasSive resistance. The Autocrat-—The interpersonal behavior of the autocrat tends to be dominating. Usually the autocrat desires a power hierarchy with self at thettop. He is the seeker of power and the competer. There is a fear that if others are not dominated they will dominate him. This need to control others usually is displaced into other areas such as intellectual superiority, athletic superiority, or by gaining political power. The basic feelings of the autocrat are that others think he can't make decisions, therefore he makes them for others as often as possible to prove his capability. There is also a strong feeling that others don't trust him. The Democrat—-The interpersonal behavior of the democrat is a comfortableness with both giving orders or not giving orders, and a comfortableness with both taking orders or not taking orders. There is a feeling of being a capable and responsible person which results in assuming responsibility with confidence. He is not worried about being incapable or incompetent. There is a feeling that others are respectful and will be realistic in trusting him and his decision-making. A“. 31 Affection Types The Underpersona1--The interpersonal behavior of the underpersonal person tends toward avoiding close personal relationships with others. Most relationships are kept on a superficial, distant level. He con- sciously wishes to maintain an emotional distance, and frequently expresses a desire to not get involved. His fear is that no one loves him. In groups he is afraid of not being liked and has difficulty liking people because of distrusting their feelings toward him. The direct method of maintaining emotional dis— tance is to reject and avoid people, sometimes even being antagonistic. The subtle approach is to be super- ficially friendly to everyone. Keeping everyone at the same distance removes responsibility for giving any one person warmth and affection. In reference to self- image there is a feeling that the self is unlovable. Others won't like the self because the self doesn't deserve it. If others are allowed to get too close, they will discover the unlovable traits of the self. The Overpersonal-—The interpersonal behavior of the overpersonal person is to become extremely close to others. There is also a need to have others treat the self in a close, personal way. The main goal of personal relationships is being liked. Being liked is very 32 important and helps reduce the fear of being rejected and unloved. The direct method used by the overpersonal person is to be extremely personal, intimate, and con- fiding. The subtle approach is to be manipulative and react to any attempts by others to establish relation- ships with others, in other words, to become possessive. The Personal-- The interpersonal behavior of the personal type is to be comfortable in close relationships and also being comfortable in emotionally distant relationships. It is important to be liked, but not being liked can be accepted. Being disliked does not result in feeling unlovable. There is a feeling that the self is lovable and a capability for giving genuine affection. Summary of FIRO Theory The FIRO theory is that there are three inter- personal need areas, inclusion, control, and affection, which are sufficient for predicting interpersonal beha- vior. Each of these interpersonal need areas has two dimensions: (1) "expressed"--the inclusion, control, and affection given to others, and (2) "wanted"--the inclusion, control, and affection taken from others. The first part of this chapter has presented the postu- lates upon which the theory is based, the definition of interpersonal needs, the descriptions of inclusion 33 behavior, control behavior, and affection behavior, and types of behavior related to each interpersonal area of inclusion, control, and affection. Research Related to the FIRO Theory The following part of this chapter contains a review of the research related to the FIRO theory. The research related to strategies using the FIRO-B as a measure for inclusion, control, and affection is also included. In surveying the research related to the FIRO theory it was found that a number of studies utilizing the theory have been conducted. In most cases the theory was used through administering the FIRO-B to evaluate change. In some instances the FIRO-B was used to deter- mine the interpersonal orientation of trainers and group participants, and then various hypotheses were tested in relationship to group composition. Composition here refers to interpersonal structures brought to a group by individual members. For the purpose of reporting the related research two sub—headings were created: (1) group composition, which includes group climate, trainer style, and participant orientation; and (2) individual behavior change, which utilizes the FIRO theory to assess change in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, con— trol and affection. 34 Group Composition The FIRO theory was the basis for doing a study which looked at the effects of matching trainers with orientations the same as the orientations of group parti- cipants. The results indicated that this type of homo- geneous matching was more effective in providing a laboratory learning climate than other matching. This study was done in 1965.13 In 1966 a complex model of laboratory training was used to test a theory of learning based on Kelman's three processes of influence-~compliance, identifica- 3 tion, and internalization. Differences in group climate determined by the members' relative orientation to authority (control), data (feedback), and people (affec— tion and intimacy) were hypothesized to interact with different kinds of training styles, measured on the same three dimensions to produce different kinds of learnings. Four different criteria of learning were measured: (1) changes on the FIRO-B questionnaire, measuring attitudes toward control and affection behavior; (2) the amount of personal awareness achieved by the group; (3) the trainer's evaluation of the group's success; and (4) a test of the diagnostic ability of group members. The data were collected from thirty on T-Groups conducted in differing situations over a period of three years. l3J.R. Powers, "Trainer Orientation and Group Com— position in Laboratory Training, Unpublished Doctoral Dis- sertation, Case Institute of Technology, 1965. 35 The results support the conclusion that dif- ferences in climate and style lead to quite different training outcomes. Kleman's trichotomy of influence mechanisms formed the basis from which predictions were derived. The "compliant" learning pattern, found among groups with authority—oriented trainer style and group climate, showed the highest diagnostic ability scores The "internalizing" learning pattern, found among groups with a data-oriented climate and a people—oriented trainer style, showed the greatest favorable change on FIRO-B and the highest interpersonal awareness. In addition to identifying the importance of these variables and supporting the theoretical linkages between them, this study demonstrated that a "laboratory" is not a unitary phenomenon; differences within a laboratory are important enough to create measurably different kinds of experi— ences and outcomes.14 In 1961 William C. Schutz composed groups homo— geneously with respect to members' expressed behavior and the behavior they wanted from others in interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection. He predicted that such compositions would lead to a high degree of recognition of within—group behavior styles. He found that homogeneous groups could identify l4P.B. Smith, "T—Group Climate, Trainer Style, and Some Tests of Learning," Unpublished Manuscript, School of Social Studies, University of Sussex, England, 1966. 36 characteristics of their own group significantly better than chance. Another finding was that there were marked behavioral differences between groups. Each seemed to settle on particular topic and each went into the topic to a greater depth than usual in groups of this type. Schutz concluded the possibility thus emerges of compos- ing groups with certain characteristics for a given purpose. For example, a training group to be intro- duced to a variety of group phenomena might better be made heterogeneous on the FIRO—B scales while the ther- apy or other type of grouping in which it is desirable to explore a single area in some detail could be composed homogeneously.15 H.B. Pollack also studied group composition using the FIRO-B and focused on the interpersonal behavior dimension of control. He composed four homogeneous groups around control: (1) high expressed, high wanted; (2) high expressed, low wanted; (3) low expressed, high wanted; and (4) low expressed, low wanted. Twelve hetero- geneous groups were formed by combining subjects who scored high, moderate or low on expressed and wanted control. The subjects of heterogeneous groups showed more positive change than subjects of homogeneous groups on the reduction of differences between expressed and wanted 15William C. Schutz, "On Group Compositionf'Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, p. 62. 37 behavior. While the confrontation variable was not examined, the author speculated that heterogeneity did lead to more confrontation among members. Participants involved in heterogeneous interpersonal groupings demonstrated greater learning or change than those in homogeneous interpersonal groupings.l6 Using the FIRO-B, J.R. Powers (1965) found that groups which were homogeneous with respect to a high desire to give and to a high desire to receive showed I a higher level of learning than groups with a low desire to give or receive.17 In 1971 C.W. Vraa found that the strengths of members' interpersonal needs, as measured by the FIRO—B, was a major factor in group membership. When the need to be included by others was low to moderate the group process was enhanced and when the inclusion need became high, the group process was inhibited. Vraa in 1974, again using the FIRO—B, confirmed a hypothesis that emotional climate differed as result of the level of the need to be included.18 l6H.B. Pollack, "Change in Homogeneous and Hetero- geneous Sensitivity Training Groups," Journal of Consult- ing and Clinical PsycholOgy, 1971, pp. 60-66. l7Cary L. Cooper, Theories of Group Processes (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1975), p. 191. lerid. 38 The Managerial Behavior Questionnaire (MBQ) developed in 1965 by F.I. Steele to measure four inter— personal values dimensions utilized the FIRO theory in determining one of the values dimensions, that of con— trol.19 There are a number of studies which look at the compatible—incompatible dimension of group composition and have used the FIRO-B as instrumentation. Some of them are as follows: W. Shalinsky's 1969 study predicted that groups made up of compatible members would perform better and perceive each other as more attractive and cooperative than groups made up of incompatible members. The FIRO-B was used to establish compatibility, specifically the affection score. The hypotheses were supported. Com- patibility around affection led to a higher level of cooperation and production among members. In 1973 W.J. Underwood and L.J. Krafft tested compatibility in a context that emphasized rational, non- personal processes. They concluded that interpersonal compatibility does not have effect in the work context that it does in the interpersonal learning context. In the same year, 1973, W.B. Reddy found that groups which 19C.L. Cooper, and I.L. Mangham, T-Groups: A Survey of Research (New York: John T. Wiley & Sons, Ltd., 1971), P. 177. 39 were compatible worked together more effectively than those groups which were incompatible. Reddy's study contradicts the findings of Underwood and Krafft; however, the Reddy study was conducted in a conference setting versus the "real work" setting of the Underwood and Krafft study. Therefore, it is possible that the major conference focus was on interpersonal relation and not on "real work." In 1972 Reddy using the FIRO-B found the greatest gains toward self—actualization were made by group members whose affectional compatibility style, whether high or low, opposed the groups' norms. P.B. Smith and M.J. Linton in 1974 used FIRO-B instrumentation to form ten groups, both compatible and found changes in self-actualization in all groups.20 Individual Behavior Change Using the FIRO—B before and after laboratory training in human relations to measure expressed and desired inclusion, control, and affection, participants were changed during the experience. Administration of the instrument six months after the experience showed that participants continued to change. Persons entering the laboratory training who were overly dominant became less 20Cary L. Cooper, Theories of Group Processes (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1975), p. 191. 40 dominant and those who were overly submissive became . 21 more assert1ve. In another study changes in attitudes toward social behavior were assessed for members of human relations training groups and for control groups which discussed social psychology in management. Scores on the FIRO-B were compared before and after training. The results showed that the experimental subjects change significantly more than the control subjects in the direction of a better match between expected and wanted scores both in the areas of control and affection. Additional data showed that the FIRO—B scores will pre- dict how subjects will be rated by other members on these two behavior dimensions.22 In 1968 L. Cureton used the FIRO—B to determine and compare behavior change between students in a T-group and students in a conventional human relations course. The study concluded that participants in T—groups showed significant change while those in the conventional course did not.23 21William C. Schutz, and V.S. Allen, "The Effects of a T-Group Laboratory on Interpersonal Behavior," Jour- nal of Applied Behavioral Science, Vol. 2, No. 3, 1966, pp. 265—286. 22P.B. Smith, "Attitude Changes Associated With Training in Human Relations," British Journal of Social Clinical Psychology, No. 3, 1964, pp. 104-112. 23C.L. Cooper and I .L. Mangham, T-Groups: A Survey of Research (New York: John T. Wiley & Sons, Ltd. , 1971) , p. 27. 41 In 1961 R. Meigniez used a French translation of the FIRO-B with three French T—groups. There were no sig- nificant changes and it was concluded that it might be attributed to cultural differences in attitudes toward control and affection.24 In 1967 H. Baumgartel and J.W. Goldstein used the FIRO-B along with the Allport-Vernon Study of Values to test the prediction that participants would become more like the highly valued members of their group in their attitudes over the course of the group. The highly valued group members had high expressed control scores on the pretests. On the posttests there was no signi- ficant change in the expressed control scores. However, two other FIRO scores did show significant changes; participants increased their wanted control scores and decreased in their wanted affection scores.25 In 1969 C.L. Cooper used the FIRO—B instrument in measuring the social influence of the T-Group trainer on T-group members. He found that when the trainer was seen to be attractive, the participant became more like the trainer in his attitudes.26 24Ibid. 25Ibid., p. 28. 26Ibid., p. 122. 42 Summary of Research Related to FIRO Theory The research related to the FIRO theory contri- butes to the understanding of the theory and supports its application to groups. However, evidence related to instructional strategies is noticeably lacking. Cureton (1968) did compare the effect of two strategies, and Meignez and French (1961) looked for significant changes as the result of a single strategy. Only the two studies mentioned above relate to instructional strategies; therefore, there is a need to determine if Treatment A and/or Treatment B bring about changes in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection. CHAPTER III STUDY DESIGN AND INSTRUMENTATION This chapter contains a description of the study by presenting questions for study, a description of the population studied, a description of the treatments used with the population, a description of the FIRO—B instru— ment, and the procedures used in data collection and W analysis. As can be found in Chapter II of this study, the FIRO theory has been used as a basis for many studies, and the FIRO—B has frequently been utilized to determine individual interpersonal need change. The FIRO-B has also been used in many studies to determine group com- position and to look at the effect that groups and trainers have on the interpersonal need balance (congruency) of group members. Only Cureton's studyl looked at the type of treatment and how it affected participants. None of the studies compared treatments which (1) were designed to meet the same general goals, (2) incorporated different instructional modes, and (3) used different instructional materials to facilitate interpersonal growth lL. Cureton, quoted in C.L. Cooper, and I.L. Mangham, T-Groups: A Survey of Research (New York: John T. Wiley & Sons, Ltd., 1971), p. 27. 43 44 and learning. This study does compare the effects of two different treatments and also looks at the effect of each of the treatments on the interpersonal needs of inclusion, control, and affection. Also, the two treat— ments are both applied in graduate college course offer- ings, therefore making the results of this study more applicable to conventional approaches used in education today. Questions for Study Using the FIRO-B instrument to identify incon- gruencies previous to treatment and again following treatment, the effects that each of the two treatments had on reducing incongruencies will be investigated. To conduct the investigation the following questions were put forth to establish a basic direction for the study. 1. Does instruction in the area of interpersonal communication reduce the number of incongruencies, as shown by teachers, as measured by the FIRO-B regardless of method? 2. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the area of inclusion, as measured by the FIRO-B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treat- ment B? 3. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the area of inclusion, as measured by the 45 FIRO—B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treat— ment B? 4. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the area of control, as measured by the FIRO—B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treat- ment B? 5. Were the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the area of affection, as measured by the FIRO-B, significantly affected by Treatment A or Treat- ment B? Description of the Population The population utilized in this study represents all of the students who were enrolled in Education 601 during the Fall term of 1975-76 school year at Wright State University in Dayton, Ohio. Education 601 is a graduate course required of all students pursuing a Master's degree in Education. Education 601 is titled Communication in Education. Through random self-selection students were assigned to one of the two sections of Education 601 without knowing the specific treatment to be used in their section. Most students selected the section based on time and convenience versus being assigned. One of the sections met on Tuesday from 7:00 to 9:30 p.m. and had fifty students enrolled, and the other section met 46 on Thursday from 7:00 to 9:30 p.m. and had forty-seven students enrolled. The population for this study came from a regular enrollment process and was not established through any special procedure. No subset grouping was created because all of the students in both sections of the course were used in the study. The population was approximately sixty percent female and forty percent male. Most were married and working full time in addition to being part time graduate students. Only five percent were full time graduate students. All possessed undergraduate degrees and were entering various concentrations (elementary education, curriculum and supervision, educational administration, etc.) in pursuit of an advanced degree in education. Description of Treatments In designing research the researcher has the option of testing the effect of treatment against a control group or testing the difference in the effect of two treatments. In that Education 601 emphasizes cognitive as well as affective domains, it was deemed necessary to use two treatments rather than treatment and control group. It is worthy to mention here that the researcher was not involved in administering either Treatment A or Treatment B. 47 The emphasis then in this particular description will be on the uniqueness of Treatment A and Treatment B. However, in addition to the differences which prevailed, there were some basic commonalities which existed. Those 2 commonalities were: (1) the basic assumptions upon which Education 601 operates: (a) communication is a type of human behavior that can be studied, analyzed, and changed; (b) communication is purposive and therefore necessary in problem-solving, controlling behavior, and understanding people; (c) communication is listener oriented, and so the theory and techniques of message sending and receiving are central features of its study; (d) the study of communication involves verbal, non- verbal and symbolic factors in message transmission and reception; (e) the process of changing is not terminal—— it is continuous and ongoing; (f) what is communicated is affected by the experiential background (e.g., needs, values, attitudes, beliefs) of the sender and the receiver—-meanings are in people not words; (g) the pro- cess of purposeful change and personal growth depends upon an awareness of behavior and its effects——feedback is essential; and (h) effective communication is essen- tial for mental health and personal adjustment; (2) the general goals for all students enrolled in Education 601: (a) understand, appreciate, and apply the various 2Course syllabus, Education 601, Wright State University, Dayton, Ohio, 1975, pp. 1-2. 48 processes of human communication; (b) acquaint students with the effect of their own communication input and how this either facilitates or interferes with the self- expression of others; (0) promote greater interaction and intellectual contact and stimulation among peers and college faculty; (d) impress upon students the responsi— bility and the power that he/she has for managing their personal and professional growth; (3) present and model the self-directive process of using others for feedback, stating objectives and obtaining further feedback speci— fic to those objectives; and (f) emphasize the importance of the affective component of learning and communication and the necessity for attention to this if cognitive learning and communication is to progreSs satisfactorily; and (3) all students experienced seven to eight hours of large group instruction which were didactic input ses— sions. Following are descriptions of Treatment A and B which emphasize differences. Treatment A Treatment A was utilized with the section of Education 601 which met on Tuesday evenings from 7:00 to 9:30 p.m. during the 1975 Fall term at Wright State University in Dayton, Ohio. The characteristics unique to Treatment A were: (1) an informal approach; subjects elected to participate in a personal growth group or a 49 discussion group, (2) there was no defined level of performance, (3) there were no predetermined outcomes, (4) expectations were unlimited, and (5) subjects received their pretest FIRO—B scores. Course syllabus used for Treatment A is Appendix A. Treatment B Treatment B was utilized with the section of Education 601 which met on Thursday evenings from 7:00 to 9:30 p.m. during the 1975 Fall term at Wright State University in Dayton, Ohio. The characteristics unique to Treatment B were: (1) a structured approach; skill areas were identified and structured experiences were conducted with the subjects receiving immediate feed- back relative to their performance level, (2) a formal assessment cycle was used, pretest—performance—posttest, (3) there were predetermined outcomes, (4) there were limited expectations, and (5) subjects did not receive their pretest FIRO—B scores. Course syllabus used for Treatment B is Appendix B. Instrumentation Validity Content validity is determined by showing how well the content of the test item samples the class of situations or the subject matter about which conclusions 50 are to be drawn. If the theory underlying the use of Guttman scales is accepted, then content validity is a property of all legitimate cumulative scales, and there- fore, of all FIRO-B scales. If all the items are measuring the same dimen— sion, and if they are of descending popularity, then they must represent a sample of items from that dimension. Any other item in that dimension fits between or beyond scale items according to the percentage accepting the item, and an individual's response to the new item is at least ninety percent predictable from his scale score. This implied that any sample of items in this dimension would rank respondents in essentially the same way; therefore, the sampling of the universe of items yields a satisfactory content validity.3 Reliability The scales of the FIRO—B are all Guttman scales and reproducibility is the appropriate measure of internal consistency. According to Guttman, reproducibility is a more stringent criterion than the usual internal consis- tency measures since it requires not only that all items measure the same dimension (i.e., that they be uni- dimensional) but also that the items occur in a certain 3William C. Schutz, The FIRO Scales Manual (Palo Alto, Calif.: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., 1967), p. 6. 51 order. The usual criterion for reproducibility is that ninety percent of all responses are predictable from knowledge of scale scores. The FIRO-B scales were deve- loped on about one thousand subjects and the reproduci- bility computed at ninety-four percent.4 Purpose of Instrument The primary purposes of the FIRO-B are: (1) to measure how an individual acts or behaves in interpersonal situations, and (2) to provide an instrument that will facilitate the prediction of interaction between people. The FIRO—B produces six scores: three on behavior expressed towards others and three on behavior wanted from others in the areas of inclusion, control, and affection. It measures the expression of orientations by the degree to which one joins and includes others, controls and leads others, and is friendly and personal with others. It also measures the desire for such beha- vior from others by an indication of the extent to which one wants others to include him and invite him to join them, influence and lead him, and express friendly and affectionate feelings toward him. The following illus- tration summarizes the six FIRO—B scores: 41bid., p. 5. Expressed "e" (toward others) Wanted "w" (from others) The procedures used to collect data I Inclusion 52 C Control A Affection I join other people, and I include others. I take charge; I influence people. I get close to people. I want peo- I want people I want peo— ple to to lead me. ple to get include me. close and personal with me. Data Collection for this study are as follows: 1. Permission was obtained from Wright State University Research Committee to use human subjects for research. 2. The subjects (students) enrolled in Education 601 were requested to participate in the study and a waiver form was provided (see Appendix C) for those who agreed to participate. released collected data for use in this study. The waiver form, when signed, to participate in this study was made totally by the subjects. 3. The FIRO-B (see Appendix D) was administered to all subjects in two sections of Education 601 at their first class session. data. There was anonymous collection of The choice 53 4. All subjects who participated were assured anonymity through collection procedure and identification process. The subjects did not identify themselves on the instrument; however, the last four digits of the social security number were used with those subjects in Treat- ment A so that scores could be returned to them. 5. The FIRO-B was administered at the last class session of both sections of Education 601. 6. The researcher administered the FIRO—B at all times to insure consistency in directions and pro- cedures. Data Analysis Tally sheets (see Appendix D) were utilized to identify the number of incongruencies for each section of Education 601. The total number of incongruencies were found for each section of Education 601. Also the number of incongruencies in each of the three interpersonal need areas (inclusion, control, and affection) were found for each section of Education 601. The resultant frequencies were subjected to a statistical test known as chi square (X2).5 All hypotheses were tested for significance at the .05 level. 5Robert R. Johnson, Elementary Statistics (North Scituate, Massachusetts: Duxbury Press, 1976), pp. 422-424. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF DATA The purpose of this study was to determine whether there were changes in the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affec— tion of the educators enrolled in the two sections of (I Education 601 during the Fall quarter of 1975 at Wright State University. The following variables were investi- gated: the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, con- trol, and affection, and whether or not Treatment A or Treatment B was effective in (l) reducing the total number incongruencies shown by teachers in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection, and (2) reducing the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in each of the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as measured by the FIRO-B during the time the treatments were utilized with the subjects participating in this study. The dependent variables mentioned were analyzed using chi square (X2). The reader will recall that the operational definition of incongruence is a difference of more than 54 55 TABLE l.--Number of Incongruencies Identified By The Administration of The FIRO-B. Data Showing the Total Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Areas of Inclusion, Control, and Affection Number of No. of Incongruencies Group Subjects Pre Post ,Treatment1\(Tuesday Section) 50 63 55 Treatment B(Thursday Section) 47 59 6O Data Showing the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Inclusion Number of No. of Incongruencies Group Subjects Pre Post : Treatment A (Tuesday Section) 50 27 23 ‘ Treatment B (Thursday Section) 47 26 24 Data Showing the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Control Number of No. of Incongruencies Group Subjects Pre Post Treatment A (Tuesday Section) 50 20 19 Treatment B (Thursday Section) 47 20 25 Date Showing the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Affection Number of No. of Incongruencies Group Subjects Pre Post Treatment A (Tuesday Section) 50 16 13 Treatment B (Thursday Section) 47 13 ll two (2) between the "e" (expressed) score and the "w (wanted) score on the nine (9) point scale of the FIRO—B. Table 1 shows, while not significant, that there was a reduction in the total number of incongruencies, and that 56 there were reductions of incongruencies in five of six subgroupings at the end of the applied treatments. TABLE 2.-—Ana1ysis of Treatment Effects Related to Total Number of Incongruencies. Pre Post Treatment A 63 55 118 Treatment B 59 60 119 122 115 237 x2 = .34 The null hypothesis stated that there was no difference in the reduction of the total number of incon- gruencies shown by teachers, regardless of treatment, as measured by the FIRO—B. The X2 value computed for this analysis was .34 and since this value was less than the required value of 3.84, the null hypothesis was accepted. Therefore, it can be concluded that instruction did not make a difference in the number of reductions in incon- gruencies. Further the null hypotheses stated that there was no difference in the number of incongruencies shown by teachers as measured by the FIRO—B as the result of Treatment A or Treatment B did not make a difference in the number of reductions in incongruencies. Calculations for Table 2 are found in Appendix E on page 91. 57 TABLE 3.—-Analysis of Treatment Effects Related to the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Inclusion. Pre Post Treatment A 27 23 50 Treatment B 26 24 50 53 47 100 x2 = .04 The null hypothesis stated that there was no difference in the reduction of the number of incongru- encies shown by teachers, as measured by the FIRO—B, in the interpersonal need area of inclusion as a result of Treatment A or Treatment B. The X2 value computed for this analysis was .04, and since this value was less than the required value of 3.84, the null hypothesis was accepted. Therefore, it can be concluded that Treatment A and that Treatment B did not make a significant differ- ence in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of inclusion. Calculations for Table 3 are found in Appendix E on page 91. The null hypothesis stated that there was no difference in the reduction of the number of incongru- encies shown by teachers, as measured by the FIRO-B in the interpersonal need area of control as a result of 58 TABLE 4.——Analysis of Treatment Effects Related to the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Control. Pre Post Treatment A 20 19 39 Treatment B 20 25 45 40 44 84 X2=fi Treatment A or Treatment B. The X2 value computed for this analysis was .40, and since this value was less than the required value of 3.84, the null hypothesis was accepted. Therefore, it can be concluded that Treatment A and that Treatment B did not make a significant dif— ference in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of control. Calculations for Table 4 are found in Appendix E on page 92. TABLE 5.-—Analysis of Treatment Effects Related to the Number of Incongruencies in the Interpersonal Need Area of Affection. Pre Post Treatment A 16 13 29 Treatment B 13 ll 24 29 24 53 .2 59 The null hypothesis stated that there was no difference in the reduction of the number of incongruen- cies shown by teachers, as measured by the FIRO—B, in the interpersonal need area of affection as a result of Treatment A or Treatment B. The X2 value computed for this analysis was .12, and since this value was less than the required value of 3.84, the null hypothesis was accepted. Therefore, it can be concluded that Treatment A and that Treatment B did not make a signi- ficant difference in reducing the number of incongruen- cies in the interpersonal need area for affection. Calculations for Table 5 are found in Appendix E on page 92. Summary of Findings In this chapter the data collected in this study were presented and analyzed. The data were treated in accordance with the data collection and analysis pro- cedures previously determined. As the reader can see, while the number of incongruencies changed from begin- ning to end of the treatment period, none of the changes were found to be significant. All of the null hypotheses stated in this study were accepted; therefore, the data analysis presented in this chapter resulted in the following conclusions: 60 1. That instruction did not reduce the total number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection shown by teachers as measured by the FIRO-B. 2. That neither Treatment A nor Treatment B was effective and that neither treatment was more effective than the other in reducing the total number of incon— gruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as measured by the FIRO-B. 3. That neither TreatmentZXnor Treatment B was effective nor was one treatment more effective than the other in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of inclusion as measured by the FIRO-B. 4. That neither Treatment A nor Treatment B was effective nor was one treatment more effective than the other in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of control as measured by the FIRO-B. 5. That neither Treatment A nor Treatment B was effective nor was one treatment more effective than the other in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of affection as measured by the FIRO-B. CHAPTER V SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS RECOMMENDATIONS, AND REFLECTIONS Summary The purpose of this study was to determine if treatment A and/or treatment B would significantly reduce the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as measured by the FIRO-B. The following variables were investigated: the interpersonal need areas of inclu- sion, control, and affection; and whether or not treat- ment A or treatment B was effective in (l) reducing the total number of incongruencies shown by teachers in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection, and (2) reducing the number of incongruencies shown by teachers in each of the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as measured by the FIRO-B. The participants in the study were all of the educators enrolled in the two sections of Education 601 at Wright State University during the fall quarter, 1975. There were fifty (50) enrolled in the section which met on Tuesdays and received treatment A. There were 61 62 forty—seven (47) enrolled in the section which met on Thursdays and received treatment B. Therefore, there were ninety seven (97) subjects who participated in the study. The FIRO theory provided the basis for this study. The FIRO theory suggests that interpersonal needs can be subsumed into the three areas of inclusion, control, and affection; and further that when individuals come together in groups these interpersonal needs , represent the concerns of the individual group members with inclusion being the first concern, control the second concern, and affection the third and final concern. The FIRO-B, the instrument used in this study, measures the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, con— trol, and affection. The FIRO-B was administered to all participants during their first class session and again at their last class session, thus the source of pre and post test data. To maintain consistency the FIRO-B was administered to all participants by the researcher. It is also noted here that the researcher was not involved in administering either treatment A or treatment B. The data were analyzed using a statistical technique known as chi square (X2). All of the null hypotheses were accepted; therefore, it was concluded that: (l) instruction did not make a significant dif- ference in reducing the total number of incongruencies in 63 the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection; (2) treatment A and/or treatment B did not make a significant difference in reducing incon— gruencies in the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection; (3) treatment A and/or treat- ment B did not make a significant difference in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of inclusion; (4) treatment A and/or treatment B did not make a significant difference in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of control; and (5) treatment A and/or treatment B did not make a significant difference in reducing the number of incongruencies in the interpersonal need area of affection. Conclusions Using the FIRO theory as a basis, the purpose of this study was to determine if either or both of two treatments would affect the interpersonal need structure of the participants. The findings of this study show that significant changes did not result in the inter— personal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as a result of the two treatments. There are a number of speculations which can be made and questions which can be raised related to this study, some of which follow: 64 Why didn't instruction make a difference? It may be that instruction in a formalized traditional time frame just cannot have a significant effect on inter- personal need behavior because such a time frame is too short. Changes in human behavior and attitude do not occur in short periods of time. A person is a product of all the experiences and influences, both direct and indirect, of their lifetime. Any number of studies related to human behavior Show that individual behavior change is an ongoing, long term process. Therefore, it is highly possible that the relatively short time period during which instruction was applied for this study is a major factor contributing to the insignificant results. If instruction in and of itself did not signi- ficantly reduce the number of incongruencies this means that it would be impossible to determine the effectiveness of treatments. Why wasn't there a significant number of reduc— tions in incongruencies as a result of treatment A or treatment B? While the reader can see by referring to Table 2 in Chapter IV of this study, there was no signi— ficant reduction in the number of incongruencies, the reader can also see by referring to Table l in Chapter IV that there were fewer incongruencies at the end of the treatment period than there were at the beginning of the treatment period. Therefore, incongruencies were 65 reduced during the treatment period. It is possible that given more time to apply treatment, there would have been significant changes in incongruencies shown by subjects of the study. Of the two treatments, A and B, neither proved significantly more effective than the other, however there were more reductions in incongruencies as the result of treatment A than there were as a result of treatment B. This might be attributed to the fact that subjects in treatment A were informed of their pretest scores on the FIRO—B. And, further that in setting their personal growth objectives for the course, the information they received relative to their interpersonal needs of inclu- sion, control, and affection was taken into consideration when setting these objectives. Also, those subjects involved in treatment A had fewer specifically identified outcomes than did those subjects involved in treatment B, so it is possible that the FIRO theory having less competition, in terms of outcomes, made more of an impact on the subjects in treatment A. Why wasn't there a significant number of reduc- tions in incongruencies in any one of the interpersonal need areas of inclusion, control, and affection as the result of treatment A or treatment B? The reader can confirm the "no significance" finding of this component of the study by referring to Tables 3, 4, and 5 in 66 Chapter IV. The reader can also see, by referring to Table 1 in Chapter IV that as a result of treatment A subjects had fewer incongruencies in all three inter— personal need areas, and as a result of treatment B subjects had fewer incongruencies in two of the three interpersonal need areas. As in the case of overall incongruencies the number was reduced during the treat- ment period, therefore, it is possible that given more time, the number of reductions in incongruencies might have been significant. It Can be concluded that the general pattern developed by the subjects of the study was toward reduc— ing incongruencies shown in the interpersonal need areas identified in the FIRO theory. Recommendations As a result of conducting this study the researcher makes the following recommendations: 1. That those persons concerned about helping others develop a more balanced (congruent) interpersonal need behavior, who are utilizing the traditional time block given to University courses, give consideration to the constraints which such a situation has. 2. That further research involving the appli- cation of the FIRO theory in conventional educational 67 settings should make a greater distinction between instructional strategies developed for that purpose; 3. That further research related to the appli— cation of FIRO theory is needed if the theory into practice concept is to be achieved; 4. That further research to check out rival hypotheses, such as, were the subjects alike or dif- ferent? (sex, age, background, etc.), would lend insight to application of the FIRO theory; and 5. That further research related to application of the FIRO theory should involve a more rigorous research design. Reflections Upon completing this study the researcher would like to state that much insight has been gained into the FIRO theory, and while learning about the theory much was also learned about self. The researcher has become more aware of the effect of his behavior on others and more aware of his own interpersonal need behavior, which has resulted in some personal behavior change. The researcher can also see that doing research is a valuable learning process. The feeling of worth, capability, and significance has been enhanced for the researcher. APPENDICES 68 '1; ~_ APPENDIX A COURSE SYLLABUS USED FOR TREATMENT A 69 APPENDIX A COMMUNICATION Education 601 Instructors: Joe Young Mary Ann Jones Overview This course is designed to acquaint graduate students with the effect of their own inputs in communication — interpersonal transactions with the objective of developing self—directed behavior and the ability to facilitate self—expression in others. Small groups will be used as learning laboratories for this purpose. Course Objectives As a result of this course, students will: 1. Develop meaningful personal contacts/relationships with fellow students. 2. Formulate observable objectives for personal growth and improvement of personal communication. 3. Develop a more balanced interpersonal need structure in the areas of inclusion, control, and affection based on the FIRO theory. ‘ 4. Recognize and use a variety of feedback sources for assessing movement toward these objectives. 5. Keep a running log to indicate sequential progress toward achieving these objectives. 6. Describe personal communication behavior patterns and their probable effect upon associates. 7. Demonstrate skill in facilitating the self expression of others. 8. Describe the basic dynamics of the communicative process in terms of concepts for the "Concept List.“ Suggested Texts or References 1. Powell, John, 1969, Why Am I Afraid to Tell You Who I Am, Chicago: Argus Communication. 2. Mortensen, C. David (ed), 1973, Basic Readings in Communication Theory, New York: Harper and Row. Course Outline Sept. 23 Introduction to course. Class cards. FIRO-B administration, short lecturette Readings, 1 handout 70 71 Sept. 30 FIRO-B results, summary Discussion of types of groups Active listening in groups 3 Minute Test Group selection - sign ups - housekeeping Contract behaviors Brief meeting of small groups for room numbers and meeting group leaders. Oct. 7 Meet with small groups — 1.5 hours 1/2 hour - discussion of Logs 1/2 hour - overview — use of Logs Oct. 14 Small group meeting - 1 1/2 hour Large group - lecturette Topic: to be announced Oct. 21 Lecturette: Interpersonal Communication (first half) Reading 1 due Small group meeting — 1 1/2 hours Nov. 4 Midterm Exam as a large group. Lecturette: Interpersonal Communication (second half) Nov. 11 Return Midterm Exam Limited 2-way discussion Film Nov. 18 Small group meeting - 1 1/2 hours. Lecturette: To be announced *Nov. 25 No formal class meeting. However, Reading II due before Wednesday at 4:00 p.m. Dec. 2 Reading II assignment returned. Small group meeting (2 hours) Tasks to be performed: a) Evaluation of contract behaviors. b) Evaluation of group member participation. 1) individual 2) group 3) leader Dec. 9 Final Examination Wrap—up 1) Student Feedback — Instructor evaluation 2) Evaluation of Group Leader Point System Evaluation , no. of points Small group Logs 5 Midterm Exam 50 Final Exam Reading I 20 Reading II Contracted Behaviors 20 Group Participation 25 Total 120 Grade Determination A = 110 - 120 B = 90 - 109 C = 70 - 89 F = Below 70 72 Readings Evaluation Each of the 2 reading assignments are worth 10 points each. 5 points - for accurate interpretation of author's meaning. 5 points - for relating each of the questions to your group experience and/or to your own personal or professional experience. Log Evaluation You must turn in a log every time your group has meeting in order to score the 5 points. Legitimate absences must be explained (if possible in advance) to the instructor(s). These logs will consist of forms (to be later discussed) and may show that you are working toward the contracted behaviors. They may show implementation of communication skills within the small group process. Or, they may summarize personal, as well as, group member, communication. Group Participation This will include, in part: 1. Your evaluation of yourself. 2. Your group leader's evaluation of your participation within the small group setting. 3. Your group members' evaluation of your participation within the small group setting. Exams Examinations will be of the short answer (identification) and multiple choice variety. The midterm and final exams will be worth approximately 25 points each. Contracted Behaviors Each individual will contract 3 communication behaviors on which he/she will work/improve in this course. By September 30th, these should be typed out and given to the instructor for approval. Once approval is granted, a copy should also be given to your group leader. Remember that your log should reflect your progress toward achieving these goals. Small Group Selection Criteria 1. N9 curriculum groups. 2. No close friends especially within Personal Growth groups. 3. It is recommended that counseling majors elect Personal Growth Groups. 4. Task oriented groups decide on their own tasks. 5. Group membership should be limited to 5 or 6 members. 73 Suggested Group Activity Since the majority of meeting time will be spent in small groups, the bulk of information for which you are responsible will, of necessity, be through your outside reading. Not all of the concepts, about which you will read, will be covered through the one-way lecturettes. Thus, it might be profitable to discuss some of the concepts within your small groups. It should be noted, however, that the small groups to which you are assigned are not to be devoted to this activity. It may only serve as, perhaps, a "jumping off" point into other group activity. Concept List By Midterm know: Trust Methods of Communication Sharing Problem ownership Listening Roadblacks to Communication Feedback Firo—B Communication Process Feedforward Communication Modes Meta communication By Final know: Active Listening Values Conflicts Focusing Conflict Resolution Clarifying Descriptive Feedback Accepting Judgemental Feedback - Reading Time Table Sept. 30 Part I - Mortensen Oct. 7 Part II Oct. 14 Part III Oct. 21 Part IV Oct. 28 Part V Nov. 4 Part VI Nov. 11 All of Powell Nov. 18 You will also be responsible for information distri- buted in handout form and for information gathered through the lecturettes. Readings 1. Describe briefly and give examples of "Every perception is necessarily a private and incomplete one." 2. Describe briefly and give examples of the three fears or threats present - a sensitive topic must be discussed with a powerful person in an emotionally charged atmosphere. Define and give examples of "meta-communication." 4. Describe briefly and give examples of "The medium is the message." 5. Define and give example of "Feedforward." U) o APPENDIX B COURSE SYLLABUS USED FOR TREATMENT B 74 APPENDIX B COURSE OUTLINE Ed. 601 - Fall 1975 Fernandez - Hoehn Sept. 25 1. Overview of Course Pre—Tests a. Firo—B Release forms Input on small groups Meet with facilitators (rotate through each facilitator) Assignment a. Communication Probes, "Problems in Active Listening“ (pg. 527—31) b. Human Relations Development, pgs. 22-26, 70-77, 79-86, 95—102, 113-117. N mmusw October 2 . Comm. Behavior Inventory Input on Levels of Listening Input on Carkhuff's Global Scale . Input on Carkhuff's Empathy & Respect Scales Assignment a. Communication Probes, "From Monologue to Dialogue" (pgs. 90-95) b. Human Relations Development, pgs. 119-126, 127-133 U‘IDWNH . . October 7 1. Lab Secction 5:00 - 6:50 (Empathy & Respect) Room 263 Room 158 Room 128 owns I I October 9 1. Lab Sessions 5:00 — 6:50 (Empathy & Respect) Room 263 Room 158 Room 128 '11th l 75 2. 3. October 1. October 1. October 1. October 1. 76 Input on Carkhuff's Concreteness & Genuineness Scales Assignments a. Communication Probes, "Self—Disclosure" (pgs. 19-23) "Human Interaction in the Small Group Setting (pgs. 142-47) ' "Group Climate" (pgs. 153-55) “A Way of Thinking About Leadership and Groups" (pgs. 155-158) "You Can Be A Better Leader" (pgs. 162—65) "Group Problem Solving" (pgs. 165-71) b. Human Relations Development, (pgs. 87-94) Lab Sessions 5:00 — 6:50 (Group Observation and Feedback) A - Room 263 B - Room 158 C - Room 128 Lab Sessions 5:00 - 6:50 (Group Observation & Feedback) D - Room 263 E — Room 158 F — Room 128 Input on Non—Verbal Assignment a. Communication Probes, "An Overview of Transactional Analysis" (pgs. 108-115) Lab Sessions 5:00 — 6:50 (Non—Verbal) A — Room 263 B - Room 158 C — Room 128 Lab Sessions 5:00 — 6:50 (Non—Verbal) D — Room 263 E — Room 158 F Room 128 Input on Interaction Systems 77 November 4 1. Lab Sessions 5:00 - 6:50 (Interaction Systems) A — Room 263 B - Room 158 C — Room 128 November 6 1. Lab Session 5:00 — 6:50 (Interaction Systems) D - Room 263 E - Room 158 F - Room 128 2. Mid Term 3. Assignment a. Communication Probes, "Communication Shock" (pgs. 64—69) "Patterns of Swearing" (pgs. 84-87) "The Language of Love: Communication Fights" (pgs. 103-107) November 11 1. Lab Session 5:00 — 6:50 (Make-up Session) November 13 1. Lab Session 5:00 - 6:50 (Make—up Session) 2. Input on Conflict Management November 20 Various Communication Activities December 4 Total class time is given over to small groups. December 11 Final Additional Reading Assignments: Communication Probes "The Role of Human Values in Communication" (Pgs. 6-8) "Perception and Human Understanding" (pg. 24—26) 78 "The Fine Art of Conversation" (pgs. 116—120) "Serial Communication of Information in Organization" (pgs. 125-133) "The Public Communication Perspective" (pgs. 174—179) Human Relations Development pgs. 42—44 pgs. 62—65 APPENDIX C WAIVER FORM FOR RESEARCH INVOLVING HUMAN SUBJECTS 79 APPENDIX C RESEARCH INVOLVING HUMAN SUBJECTS CONSENT TO PARTICIPATE AS A SUBJECT IN RESEARCH (SHORT FORM) Project Title: Grant or Contract No.: Principal Investigator: Department: I consent to participate as a subject in this research investi- gation. The nature and general purpose of the experimental procedure have been explained to be my . I understand my identity will not be revealed in any publication or document resulting from this research. This authorization is given with the understanding that I may terminate my service as a subject at any time. Signed (Subject) Date 80 APPENDIX D INSTRUMENTATION 81 APPENDIX D FIRO-B DIRECTIONS: This questionnaire is designed to explore the typical ways you interact with people. There are, of course, no right or wrong answers; each person has his own ways of behaving. . Sometimes people are tempted to answer questions like these in terms of what they think a person should do. This is ESE what is wanted here. We would like to know how you actually behave. Some items may seem similar to others. However, each item is different so please answer each one without regard to the others. There is no time limit, but do not debate long over any item. 82 83 H+H, Th. Y Last 4 Digits of Soc. Sec. # FIRO—B Section — Circle One; Tue., Th. Please place number of the answer that best applies to you in the box at the left of the statement. 1. I try to be with people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never I let other people decide what to do. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never I join social groups. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never I try to have close relationships with people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never I tend to join social organizations when I have an opportunity. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never I let other people strongly influence my actions. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never I try to be included in informal social activities. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never Please be as honest as you can. rarely rarely rarely rarely rarely rarely rarely I try to have close, personal relationships with people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never I try to include other people in my plans. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) 6) never rarely rarely 10. ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. l6 l7. l8. 19. 20. 84 I let other people control my actions. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 6) never I try to have people around me. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 6) never I try to get close and personal with people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 6) never When people are doing things together, I tend to 5) rarely 5) rarely 5) rarely join them. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I am easily led by people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I try to avoid being alone. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I try to participate in group activities. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never PLEASE BE AS HONEST AS YOU CAN I try to be friendly to people. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I let other people decide what to do. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody My personal relations with people are cool distant. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I let other people take charge of things. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 85 21. I try to have close relationships with people. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 22. I let other people strongly influence my actions. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 23. I try to get close and personal with people. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 24. I let other people control my actions. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 25. I act cool and distant with people. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 26. I am easily led by people. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 27. I try to have close, personal relationships with people 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 28 I like people to invite me to things. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 29. I like people to act close and personal with me. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 30. I try to influence strongly other people's actions. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody PLEASE BE AS HONEST AS YOU CAN 31. 32. 33 34 35. 36. 37. 39. 86 I like people to invite me to join their activities. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I like people to act close toward me. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I try to take charge of things when I am with people. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I like people to include me in their activies. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I like people to act cool and distant toward me. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I try to have other people do things the way I want them done. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I like people to ask me to participate in their discus- sions. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I like people to act friendly toward me. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody I like people to invite me to participate in their activities. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody 40. 41. 42 43. 44. 45 46. 47. 48. 49 50 51 o 87 I like people to act distant toward me. 1) most 2) many 3) some 4) a few 5) one or two people people people people people 6) nobody PLEASE REMEMBER TO BE AS HONEST AS YOU CAN I try to be the dominant person when I am with people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I like people to invite me to things. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I like people to act close toward me. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I try to have other people do things I want done. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I like people to invite me to join their activities. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I like people to act cool and distant toward me. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I try to influence strongly other people's actions. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I like people to include me in their activities. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I like people to act close and personal with me. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I try to take charge of things when I'm with people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I like people to invite me to participate in their activities. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never 52. 53. 54. 88 I like people to act distant toward me. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I try to have other people do things the way I want them done. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never I take charge of things when I'm with people. 1) usually 2) often 3) sometimes 4) occasionally 5) rarely 6) never 89 FIRO-B Summation Chart GROUP Wants to Wants to Wants to Wants to Wants to Wants to Include be Control be ' Receive Give Others Included Others Controlled Affection Affection 11.4.; .. .. APPENDIX E STATISTICAL ANALYSIS FOR TABLES 2, 3, 4 AND 5 90 APPENDIX E STATISTICAL ANALYSIS Calculations for x2 -- Table 2 Observed Expected Pre Post Pre Post Treatment A 63 55 118 Treatment A 60. 74 57. 27 118 Treatment B 59 60 119 T reatment B 61. 26 57. 74 119 122 115 237 122 115 237 2 2.262+ 2.262+ 2282+ 2.262 60.74 57.26 61.26 57.74 x?- = .08+.09+.08+.O9 2 Table value: x (.05)df : l = 3.84 Calculations for x2 -- Table 3 Observed Expected Pre Post Pre Post Treatment A 27 23 50 Treatment A 26. 5 23. 5 50 Treatment B 26 24 50 Treatment B 26. 5 23. 3 50 53 47 100 53 47 100 2 = .52 .52 .52 .52 —— +—+ —— +— 26. 5 23. 5 26. 5 23. 5 x : .01+.01+.01+.01 x : .04 2 Table value. x (.05)df = l : 3.84 91 92 Calculations for x2 -- Table 4 Observed Expected Pre Post Pre Post Treatment A l 20 I 19 39 Treatment A 18. 57 20. 43 39 Treatment B I 20 l 25 45 Treatment B 21.43 23. 57 45 40 44 84 4O 44 84 2 1.432 1.432 1.432 1.43‘2 x — + 18.57 20.43 21.43 23.57 2 X H .11+.10+.10+.09 x2 = .40 Table value: 342,05) df = 1 = 3.84 Calculations for x2 -- Table 5 Observed Expected Pre Post Pre Post Treatment A 16 l 13 29 Treatment A [15. 87 13.13 29 Treatment B 131 11 24 Treatment B [13. 13 10. 87 24 29 24 53 29 24 53 2 .132 .132 .132 .132 X : 15.87 + 13.13 + 13.13 + 10.87 x2 = .00 + .06 + .06 + .00 x2 = .12 2 Table value: x (.05) (if : 1 = 3.84 BIBLIOGRAPHY 93 BIBLIOGRAPHY Cooper, C.L., and I.F. Mangham, T-Groups: A Survey of Research, New York: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., 1971. Cooper, Cary L., Theories of Group Processes, New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1975. Howe, Harold, Picking Up The Options, Elementary School Principals, N.E.A., Washington, D.C., 1968. Johnson, David W. and Frank P. Johnson, Joining Together, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1975. King, Jean Marie, and VonHerbert I. Haden, "Team Teaching," Innovations In Education: Pros and Cons, Worthington, Ohio: Charles A. Jones Publishing Co., 1971. Pfeiffer, William J., and Richard Heslin, Instrumentations In Human Relations Training, Iowa City: University Associates, 1973. Pollack, H.B., "Change in Homogeneous and Heterogeneous Sensitivity Training Groups," Journal of Consult- ing and Clinical Psychology, 1971. Powers, J.R., "Trainer Orientation and Group Composition in Laboratory Training,"UnpublishedDoctoral Dissertation, Case Institute of Technology, 1965. Schutz, William C., The FIRO Scales Manual, Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., 1967. . The Interpersonal Underworld, Palo Alto, California: Science and Behavior Books, Inc., 1966. . "On Group Composition," Journal of Abnormal and Social PsycholOgy, 1961. Schutz, William C. and V.S. Allen, "The Effects of T-Group Laboratory