313 2 an .2309 . ,,_ - - .. -. an-.- " ' n. . " ...,." . _ " —Il-._.. ~ .- _. _ 2 . _v~ r‘ ..... "“'--v . -, .. .v‘ ‘2 .. n s... .H‘ .,_ ~-.._' . 2- — r 1 ‘;---o -.. I _'~ ' 4 ‘ D ~11 .. . .. _... > H . ~ -- o, . .. .o-.“' .‘t.._ t ‘.“~ ‘ ‘_-- .."'4 b»._ C .y‘. .~. 3 'v. : . -. on..'.‘ ‘ ,; .._ ~ ‘. .. . ~' ._ ‘ ‘|.‘ ':,~ . ‘. ‘ .L .' h. 2 .A "on .- _ .. ' v s.‘ . -._. . . _. ‘ l‘c, - " '0 . _- \‘. ‘0‘! "-.‘_ -, ._- .. - AA . . ~_) .‘ ‘- .“‘ . . 'n ‘ ‘ ~ . ‘v‘ -. I-. . u, I ‘ s,_ 3 .M 5., 3 s ._>. \. -< .'-. ‘v. u.“ ,_ , “ ‘ N. ' 2 'A - “I ~ ”A ,.‘ . ABSTRACT MATERNAL ATTITUDES AND VALUES IN RESPECT TO EMOTIONALLY DISTURBED AND PHYSICALLY DISABLED PERSONS by Birendra Kumar Sinha The study was designed primarily to ascertain the atti- tudes of mothers toward emotionally disturbed and physically disabled persons. "Personal contact" with the emotionally disturbed and the physically disabled was the basic variable investigated in the present study. Precisely, the influence of frequency and nature of contact on both content and inten- sity of attitudes was systematically explored. In addition, the present research aimed to examine evidence for predicted relationships between value systems of the subjects and attitudes toward the emotionally disturbed and the physically disabled. Two other dimensions investigated in this study were related to progressive versus traditional attitudes toward education and change orientations. Finally, an attempt was made to determine whether attitudes toward the disturbed and the disabled stemmed from popular stereotypes prevalent in the culture about mental illness and physical disability. Three groups (two experimental and one control) were employed in this research. One experimental group was com- posed of mothers of emotionally disturbed children and the u-.. .. . u .. .3... . I'I‘ ..¢w' - . ... .... ...., .. ..- .... - '-~ . . . ... . — ... . .._ V .‘ .- . .. -‘.l "-1. ..-_ _ "i‘- 3.. _ . C A .I . ‘-. ' '2' s- .I '..-.. a. ‘ .,_.. ‘— . 'I‘ -- v._ ~_'. ‘ .. .."‘ " ~. . o, .. u. - ‘A. .’ 4. ~_ n.. ‘ ‘ c,_ u -_ o ’- v. . ' - \. . ' o “. c d s.- 'v a. -, I ‘ a- . -,‘~ -."..’ - .- s g _ "‘ . . s -4 . ‘5 - ‘vl , ‘ ‘. I. -'_ .‘ . ‘- y ~ BIRENDRA KUMAR SINHA other experimental group of mothers of physically disabled children. These mothers were utilizing professional help for their children in.a.nmntal health clinic and a physical rehabilitation center. Since "personal contact" with emotion- ally disturbed and physically disabled persons was the main concern for the study, it was considered important to select mothers whose contact with the emotionally disturbed and the physically disabled would be most intimate and frequent. Such a population was ideally available at a comprehensive community mental health, and a rehabilitation center located on the campus of the Betty Jane Memorial Center in Tiffin, Ohio, a.typical midwestern, middle-sized community. The out-patient nmntal health services were provided by the Sandusky_Valley Guidance Center, and the rehabilitation services for the physically handicapped were rendered by the Betty Jane Rehabili— tation Center. Mothers of "normal," that is, non-handicapped children served as the control group. They were drawn from the city of Mount Pleasant, Michigan which is comparable to luffin in demographic composition. There was no statistically Eflgnificant difference between the three groups in respect to education, income, and age. The sample consisted of 60 mothers of emotionally disturbed children, 48 mothers of physically disabled children, and 69 mothers of normal children. The sampling, however, was not fully random on account of non-availability of apprOpriate subjects in the two experimental groups. BIRENDRA KUMAR SINHA A battery of research instruments were used to measure ammtudes, values, change orientations, and various demographic (Karacteristics. They were: (a) the Handicapped Persons Scale, (b) the Emotionally Disturbed Persons Scale, (0) the Education Scale, (d) the Gordon Survey of Interpersonal Values, (e) the Personal Questionnaire (general), (f) the Personal Question— naire: HP, and (g) the Personal Questionnaire: EDP. The ques- tionnaires were self—administered in all cases, and the estimated time to complete the questionnaires was approximately three hours. Unlike the psychoanalytic orientation of most studies on parent-child interactions, the theoretical framework of the present research was mainly social psychological. Within the purview of the social psychological framework, the nature of intergroup attitudes was viewed as relating to interpersonal values and contact variables such as frequency, enjoyment, and ease of avoidance of the contact. In keeping with the above theoretical orientation, 26 specific hypotheses were formulated which were classified into four major categories: (a) contact-intensity and contact—frequency interactions, (b) attitude-value interactions, (c) change orientation and atti- tude, and (d) general differences in attitudes reflecting cultural stereotypes. The main hypothesis relating to contact—intensity inter- action was that higher frequency of contact with the disturbed and the disabled produced greater intensity of attitude regard— less of attitude content. In respect to contact-frequency “. \ I¢ .- _ '- ‘n 1.. . ._.~ .. In. u.‘ - , -..' In .‘ ‘ . ~ .'-I - .- . . .' u. h. . .. . ¥’_ .I. s - .'.. ‘ --. ‘b .’ .'. v. _. ._. 'I . - 0 ~ - ‘~. _,. . ' a —‘ -. . ‘0 '5 _v_ o '.~ ~, ‘ .V . \. . .‘x . .— ..- .. . ,. . up... .. ' - ' .4 , '_ '- .v- ‘. ’V h --n “ ,. , . v; . _ — r..._,_. v ‘_.. l.- ‘4‘ ..’. -, ~.. '- - - ~_-. v. » . I . .- . BIRENDRA KUMAR SINHA hmeraction, the major hypothesis was that high frequency of cmfiact with the disturbed and the disabled is associated with fawnebleness of attitudes if, (a) there are other rewarding mmortunities to engage, (b) the contact was enjoyable, and (c)the interaction could be easily avoided. The hypotheses pmwaining to attitude-value interactions attempted to deter— mhm the role of "asset" and "comparative" value orientation hithe maintenance of favorable or unfavorable attitudes. The flange orientations selected for the study were: health prac- Utes, birth control practices, automation, political leader- Mup, and self-change. Finally, the main hypothesis in the 1mm category was that mothers of normal children would express nmre favorable attitudes toward physically disabled persons Hmn toward emotionally disturbed persons. The hypotheses were tested by means of analysis of vmflance, tetest, zero—order correlation, and multiple and gmrtial correlations. The results were analyzed for the three @mmps of mothers, and the total sample, regardless of treatment. The analyses of the results confirmed, in general, the hmmct of personal contact in the maintenance of favorable audtudes toward emotionally disturbed and physically handi- Cmmed persons. Amount of contact, however, was not the only flumor which produced favorable attitudes. The nature of contact, such as enjoyment and avoidance of contact, was ob- Ewrved to be associated in some manner with favorableness of mmdtudes, although the results did not indicate any clear and defluflte correlational pattern between contact variables and . .. "- \ ‘_ an. .“H o _ F -. . ‘...o ‘ .aV—uourl “‘ - .. . _’..-r v', I I ..-.""" ‘- . - - .' .y'r " “ . .-.- o. ‘ . . . .. y. ..- .‘X. “" '- ,07."’ A .. .. ud-""' .. ‘0' I..‘ 5 .- .-- I. ‘I'. ’ r ...o--. .. .- I . .. .ga- ' .. . _-..' _ »-. .CC‘IQ'II' - u .- ‘. p -' o~ ‘- c .. .. . -u.. . I '- ‘O'uoo‘ . ' .,. - __' u- _ -1 a — "F . 'n.‘. _ o a-. - . . ._ >L .—. .-..n .oh- . .. . ,_ n~ “ a, . \-' ‘ ‘~“ " .- < .'~. _ . .. _v .._ 'In- . or. .- v ‘ ‘— BIRENDRA KUMAR SINHA amitudes toward the disturbed and the disabled. Contact 'Mth education also did not per se produce favorable attitudes award education. Contrary to expectation, more frequent contact appeared Maproduce low intensity of attitudes toward emotionally dis- mubed and physically disabled persons. However, high fre- mmncy of contact with education did not result in lower knensity of attitude. No significant relationship was obtained unween contact with education and intensity of attitudes ‘Mmard education. It was hypothesized that mothers of emotionally disturbed mm.physically handicapped children would be characterized by miasset value orientation rather than a comparative value mientation. The Benevolence sub—scale of the Gordon scale of wflues was used as a measure of asset value orientation while Hm Leadership and Recognition sub-scales were employed to nmasure comparative value orientation. But the analyses of ‘Hm data did not yield consistent results to permit meaningful gnmralizations. However, it was found that mothers of emotion— aUy disturbed children had significantly higher scores on ‘Um value of Support when compared to mothers of physically fmndicapped or normal children. On the otherhand, mothers of Physically handicapped children expressed greater Conformity \wlue orientation than did the mothers of emotionally disturbed or normal children. The majority of the hypotheses relating to change orien- tatnxm, and attitudes toward education were not confirmed BIRENDRA KUMAR SINHA umsistently, and as such no definite conclusions can be made mithe basis of the present investigation. One interesting finding of the study was that mothers ofnormal children had more favorable attitudes toward the mwsically disabled than toward emotionally disturbed persons. fins was regarded as a reflection of the cultural stereotypes flmut mental illness as compared to physical disability. The present research raised many further questions mmarding theoretical and methodological issues inherent in astudy of attitudes and values about highly complex social dflects like emotional disturbance and physical disability. Anmjor problem demanding serious consideration was noted to bethe scaling technique. It was recommended that Guttman- Idngoes Multiple Scale Analysis (MSA I), which allows for mflti-dimensional analysis of data in addition to multi- mudimensional analysis, be used in future studies. Guttman's .fiwet theory (1959, 1961) appeared to be impressive in resolv- ing problems related to dimensionality of attitude, concept muuvalence, and item sampling. This theory suggests that the- amfltude universe represented by the item content can be sub— Mumctured into components which are systematically related tothe number of identical conceptual elements held in common. 'Nm substructuring of relationships between various components dfthe attitude universe thus allows for meaningful intergroup comparisons. It was also pointed out that a different conceptual frammmHWIWas needed to measure values in View of the failure D P "4.-" .- ..l ‘ .. - ’ .hl. . A .‘1.» .. . . ..,-~.. U ..--‘--~ .. ..[. V 4' -..¢¢. .. . o >...4_ ’~ 5'. O‘ u.... 'u r‘.- . - BIRENDRA KUMAR SINHA of the present approach (in terms of asset and comparative value orientations) to provide consistent results. The findings of the present research indicate a need for longitudinal studies in this area. In the least, a com- parison of groups before and after exposure to the attitude objects was considered a preferable design for making mean— ingful generali zati ons . -u o MATERNAL ATTITUDES AND VALUES IN RESPECT TO EMOTIONALLY DISTURBED AND PHYSICALLY DISABLED PERSONS By Birendra Kumar Sinha A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services, and Educational Psychology 1966 -. II "‘ u ‘. , . p I- ‘0’. -v ‘v- .- | I 'v.1 5.1 v -v- -p- n ’ v . - an. ufiu U. or .. . ~._... :. ." - .,_' N. ' - ' n ,o -‘ .. . ._' a 9. ' ‘Q. ~ . l .n, 4" ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Since the research was mainly carried out at the Sandusky Valley Guidance Center in Tiffin, Ohio, where I was working as a Clinical Psychologist, the cooper- ation and support of both the professional and secre- tarial staff were indispensable for the successful com- pletion of the research project. I am especially grate- ful to Dr. William Moore, Acting Director of the mental health clinic, and Mr. Paul Brown, Executive Director of the Betty Jane Memorial Rehabilitation Center, who pro- vided all possible help in encouraging mothers of the patient-children to participate in the research. My other professional colleagues at the clinic—-Mr. Donald Haliburton, Director of Psychiatric Social Service, Mrs. Ruth Chapman and Mrs. Vivian Sebeste, Psychiatric Social Workers--were of great assistance in enlisting cooper— ation of the subjects who volunteered to complete the time-consuming research instruments. A respected friend, Dr. Maurice Newberger, Director of the Psychology De- Dartment at the Rehabilitation Center, provided distin- S'Jished encouragement and help for which I am deeply Oblised. I am highly appreciative of the invaluable ii ...oo. 'v' —......4.-. . h ’0' I. u y . .’ \ -"" v. . .... . ¢.- . - ~ ‘.....‘._ U . . . ‘0‘ 1". < n . "o. _“-. . v ' tI-p. - ‘ ‘1‘. u- ' “1.. .- .. . ‘ ‘ ‘.-.' .'~. I‘ x ': 1'. 'c . r. . ‘- -'. .‘ u.. .'-‘ ‘ ._. ' .fl. : h .‘ . u. . ‘ . u. ‘ a - :' '* ‘0 V‘. . I . . o ~ secretarial help rendered by Mrs. Lloyd Kelbley, Mrs. mun Kieffer, Mrs. w. L. Fullerton, and Mrs. William Tuthill. Because the research was partly conducted in Mount Neasant, Michigan, I have incurred further obligations. Iam particularly indebted to Mr. Carlo Barberi, Superin- 1mndent of the Mount Pleasant School System for providing mmjects for the control group. I also wish to express aw gratefulness to Dr. Wilbur Moore, Vice-President of Mmdemic Affairs at Central Michigan University for financial support in the form of a small research grant. Dr. Karl Pratt, Head of the Department of Psychology at (kntral Michigan University was very kind in arranging nw class schedule conveniently so that I could make fre- mmnt visits to Michigan State University for computer mmlysis of the data. I want to thank Miss Katherine Morris for efficient mm.accurate scoring of the raw data, and Miss Susan finer for helping in the computer analyses. I am also indebted to Dr. Norman Abeles, Dr. (hegory Miller, and Dr. Cole Brembeck who served as nwmbers of the advisement committee for this dissertation. Dr.Abeles' critical and insightful suggestions have been a.great asset to this research. My greatest gratitude is to Dr. John E. Jordan, the committee chairman, who has 111 .a n -' ‘ . .n- ' '. '-T; u no. “" ‘. -r- ‘- n» n- O r. - .. '_‘ \. ~ ’ O "“ -.—... .‘I ' . " v.¢ ' 5—. ~ ~ ‘ ‘E' I-‘ -. ".A v . ’ - . -.--. .,... ‘ accorded unstinted support, encouragement, and inspir- ation at every stage of this research project. He has shown unusual understanding and sympathy to a foreign student who must encounter a multitude of problems in a field research work. Beyond these, I owe much to my wife, Asha, and to my sons, Ani and Neel. My wife has endured a long and arduous period of deprivations and hardships, for she had to perform multiple and often conflicting roles as mother, father, housewife, and student. Also, I greatly admire the exemplary patience with which my six- year old son, Ani, has looked forward to the completion of the research so that he could again play his favorite word-game with his daddy. iv -. --o..,—.--. . . -. .ch-‘c-h a- u . \“. . a ”in. - A ..‘ - U... a. Ina, -,' ..._ .\ - ‘ .Nv. ‘ I .‘ - TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS IJST OF TABLES IJST OF FIGURES . IJST OF APPENDICES Chapter I. INTRODUCTION Nature of the Problem. . Statement of the Problem. Definition of Terms . . Research Hypotheses . Hypotheses Related to Contact Fre- quency, Intensity and Attitude Scores. Hypotheses Related to Attitude-Value Interactions. . Hypotheses Related to Change Orien— tation and Attitude Scores . Hypotheses Related to General Dif— ferences Between Mothers of Dis— turbed, Handicapped, and Non- Handicapped Children . II. REVIEW OF RELATED RESEARCH AND THEORY Attitudes Toward Emotionally Disturbed Persons . . . . Parental Attitudes. General Studies. Page ii xi xxii xxiii 10 15 15 17 19 19 21 21 30 - D. _ out. o o - . ~ .- - .. . i -. 9-. - .'Iv- I I e . I I .§ - 'II n '1 '5 1- "' I \ ‘."-~, .‘ ‘_ -. .“ u. I‘ wp b ‘ O - -.. u- '- a V ~ Chapter Attitudes Toward Physically Disabled Persons . Parental Attitudes General Studies The Relationship of Values, Personal Con- tact, and Intensity to Attitudes. The Role of Values . Classification of Values Personal Contact . Attitude Intensity Theoretical Considerations. Psychoanalytic Model. . Social-psychological Approach. Attitude Organization and Measurement of Attitudes . . . . . . . Attitude Organization Attitude Scales Methodological Considerations. Control Group . . Selection of Variables Measurement Techniques Longitudinal Studies. III. METHODOLOGY AND PROCEDURE Research Population Selection of Samples. The Experimental Groups. The Control Group. Demographic Characteristics of Experi- mental and Control Samples. Selection of Variables Description of Instruments. Handicapped Persons Scale Emotionally Disturbed Persons Scale. Education Scale . . vi Page 51 51 5A 57 60 61 62 63 66 66 68 72 72 7A 75 77 79 79 81 83 86 87 88 89 .v . . .. :3 a 0. o 4‘ . I. 3. T. o. .. v- I- ‘- “o . a Q‘v . o .b- .u- .u- .b- 0: o a... or. ... .p. . . s . v . pp‘ p. o u a u ‘8 v I ‘-‘.p.: . -O~‘- C n v . v0 -'.’ , s . ,‘h-. Chapter Page Gordon' 3 Survey of Interpersonal Values . . . . 91 Personal Questionnaire (General) . . 93 Personal Questionnaire: HP . . . . 97 Personal Questionnaire: EDP. . . . 98 Collection of Data . . . . . . . . 98 Statistical Procedures. . . . . . . 101 Descriptive Statistics. . . . . . 101 Inferential Statistics. . . 102 Relational and Predictive Statistics . 10A Measurement of Attitude Intensity . . 106 Limitations of the Study . . . . . . 107 The Problem of Research Design . . . 107 The Problem of Sample Selection. . . 109 The Problem of Test Administration. . 109 The Problem of Statistical Analysis . 110 Research Hypotheses, Rationale, and Instrumentation . . . . . . . . 112 Hypotheses Related to Contact Fre- quency, Intensity, and Attitude Scores . . . . . 112 H:1: Contact-Intensity Inter- actions. . . . 112 Hypotheses Derivation (H: 1a1 — H: lau) . . . 112 Instrumentation (H: lal - H: lau). . 113 Hypotheses Derivation (H: lbl - H: lbg) . . . 113 Instrumentation (H: lbl - H: 1b2). . 113 H: 2: Contact-Frequency Inter- actions. . . . 11A Hypotheses Derivation (H: 2a1 - H: 2a”) . . 11A Instrumentation (H: 2a1 - H: 2au). . 115 Hypothesis Derivation (H: 2b). . . 115 Instrumentation (H:2b). . . . . 116 Hypotheses Related to Attitude—Value Interactions . . . . . . 116 vii Chapter Page Hypotheses Derivation (H:3a — H: 3b) . . . 116 Instrumentation (H: 3a. — H: 3b) . . 117 Hypotheses Derivation (H: Aa - H:Ac) . . . . . . . . . 117 Instrumentation (qua - H:Ac) . . 118 Hypotheses Derivation (H:5a - H: 5c) . . . 118 Instrumentation (H. 5a. - H: So) 118 Hypotheses Derivation (H: 6 - H:7). 119 Instrumentation (H: 6 - H: 7). . . 120 Hypotheses Related to Change Orien- tation and Attitude Scores . . . 120 Hypotheses Derivation (H:8a - H: 9) . . . 120 Instrumentation (H: 8a. - H: 9) . . 121 Hypotheses Related to General Differ— ences Between Mothers of Dis- turbed, Handicapped, and Non- Handicapped Children . . . . 121 Hypotheses Derivation (H:10a — H: 10b) . . 121 Instrumentation (H: 10a - H: 10b) . 121 IV. ANALYSIS OF THE DATA Section 1: Descriptive Data . . . . 122 Differences in Education, Income, and Age Between Respondent Groups. . . . 125 Differences in Some Other Demo- graphic Characteristics Between Respondent Groups . . . . 130 Summary of Descriptive Data in Tables 3- 12 . . . . . . . . 13“ Section 2: Hypotheses Testing, Mean Differences, and Correlational Analyses . . . . . . . . . . 136 Hypotheses Related to Contact Fre- quency, Intensity, and Attitude Scores. . . . . . 136 viii ; mmpter Page Hzl: Contact- -Intensity Inter- actions . . . . . 136 H1131 . . . . . . . . . 136 H2132. . . . . . . . . . 138 Hzla3. . . . . . . . . . 143 H:lau: . . . . . . . . . 145 Hzlbl. . . . . . . . . . 148 Htlbg. . . . . . . . . . 148 Summary of Contact and Attitude Intensity Scores . . . . . . 153 H:2: Contact-Frequency Inter— actions . . . . . . . . 153 H:2a1. . . . . . . . . . 153 H:2a2. . . . . . . . . . 157 H:2a . . . . . . . . . . 157 H:2a . . . . . . . . . . 164 H:2b . . . . . . . . . . 164 Summary of Contact and Attitude Content Variables. . . . 171 Hypotheses Related to Attitude- Value Interactions . . . . 172 H:3a . . . . . . . . . . 172 Hz3b . . . . . . . . . . 177 H:4a . . . . . . . . . . 180 H:4b . . . . . . . . . . 180 H:4c . . . . . . . . . . 187 H:5a . . . . . . . . . . 191 H:5b . . . . . . . . . . 193 H:5c . . . . . . . . . . 197 H:6 . . . . . . . 197 H:7 201 Summary of Attitude and Value Vari- ables. . . 204 Hypotheses Related to Change Orien- tation and Attitude Scores. . . 206 H:8a . . . . . . . . . . 206 H:8b . . . . . . . . . . 210 H:9 . . . . . . . . . . 213 Summary of Attitude and Change Orientation Variables . . . . 216 ix no.g...-. onuov‘.-. . '.V .- ._‘ - ,1 ‘1 . "- ‘5. .‘ _ . ‘v :‘ . .‘ Chapter Hypotheses Related to General Differ- ences Between Mothers of Disturbed, Handicapped, and Non- -Handicapped Children. . . H:10a. H:10b. 'V. DISCUSSION, RECOMMENDATIONS, AND SUMMARY Part I: Discussion of Results Hypotheses Relating to Contact Fre- quency and Intensity. . Contact Variables and Content of Attitude. . Value Variables in Relation to Attitudes Change Variables as Related to Attitude Scores . General Differences in the Attitudes of the Respondent Groups . Part II: Theoretical and Methodological Issues, and Recommendations Theoretical Issues Methodological Issues Recommendations Research Design and Sampling Selection and Analysis of Scales . . Statistical Analysis. Part III: Concluding Summary. REFERENCES APPENDICES Page 219 219 220 223 223 226 232 238 239 241 242 248 253 253 254 258 258 261 281 Din-UIOvII . . - .‘~; .. ’ ..-. ,. . ' \l ’r- . . find .._ ‘5..,.'-, _ ~......‘ .. .. v. 0-- ...__ n - " p... - ...‘__.. . .‘p. § '."v.’ . . __ . l In-n...-‘ — . -. _\n .'l." a I a A'Dr . .. a“ v...__‘ . I p..' u_‘ . I...” . . ~ . o_"v: . ' ‘ v _~‘- . - u;._ . -fi. ‘ ' I .-'.' Iv.‘_-‘ . .. a... c- “-i ‘Q 'C ’v- ._.. . ..' ‘ ' - v ,_ 0 ‘l _ -., - I - a . -~‘- ' .— .,. r- -. -. o... . I . ‘l‘ .-.' a- ‘ n... - u. s . . . a, ’- v . ‘fit - u ‘l . ‘L. 'm - \ . Table 10. ll. 12. 13. LIST OF TABLES Selection of Samples Size of Population and Income in Tiffin and Mount Pleasant. . . . . Distribution of Subjects According to "Treatments". . . . Occupational Composition of the Total Sample by Respondent Groups. Comparison of the Respondent Groups in Terms of Actual Educational Attainment Comparison of the Respondent Groups in Respect to Yearly Income (Self-Family) Comparison of the Respondent Groups in Respect to Age . . . . Comparison of Mean Differences, Standard Deviations, and F Statistics in Respect to Three Demographic Variables for the Three Respondent Groups . . . Comparison of the Respondent Groups in Respect to Number of Children Comparison of the Respondent Groups in Respect to Marital Status Comparison of the Respondent Groups in Respect to Recent Residence. Comparison of the Respondent Groups in Respect to Current Length of Residence (Mobility) . . . . . . . . . Means and Standard Deviations of Intensity Scores on the Attitude-Toward-Emotion— ally-Disturbed-Persons (EDP) Scale Com- paring High and Low Frequency of Contact with Emotionally Disturbed Persons for the Total Sample . . . . . xi Page 84 86 123 124 126 127 128 129 131 132 132 133 137 rm; ‘“ king. ', a. a- . _ '. u ' ..‘. y‘- ‘ ' ..'- ‘ :- .vu-v .‘ n a. . u. v- - r;V‘:' av.u. . u . .'. - a. I . n..c v.4 ’r- I p ‘II‘ ’v n U-.. .7- u... Table 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. Analysis of Variance of the EDP Intensity Scores Comparing High and Low Frequency of Contact with Emotionally Disturbed Persons for the Total Sample . Zero-order Correlations Between Contact and Intensity Scores on the Attitude Scales for the Three Respondent Groups and the Total Sample. . Means and Standard Deviations of Intensity Scores on the Attitude-Toward-Emotion- ally-Disturbed-Persons (EDP) Scale for the Three Respondent Groups Analysis of Variance of EDP Intensity Scores for the Three Respondent Groups. Duncan's New Multiple Range Test Applied to Means of EDP Intensity Scale for the Three Respondent Groups . Means and Standard Deviations of Intensity Scores on the Attitude-Toward—Handi- capped-Persons (HP) Scale Comparing High and Low Frequency of Contact with Physically Handicapped Persons for the Total Sample . . . . Analysis of Variance of HP Intensity Scores Comparing High and Low Frequency of Contact with Physically Handicapped Persons for the Total Sample . Means and Standard Deviations of Intensity Scores on the Attitude-Toward-Handi- capped-Persons (HP) Scale for the Three Respondent Groups. . Analysis of Variance of HP Intensity Scores for the Three Respondent Groups. Duncan's New Multiple Range Test Applied to Means of HP Intensity Scale for the Three Respondent Groups. . . xii Page 137 139 142 144 144 146 146 147 Table Page 24. Means and Standard Deviations of Intensity Scores on the Progressive-Attitude- Toward—Education Scale Comparing High and Low Frequency of Contact with Education for the Total Sample . . . . 149 ET Analysis of Variance of Intensity Scores on the Progressive-Attitude—Toward-Education Scale Comparing High and Low Frequency of Contact with Education for the Total Sample . . . . . . . . . . . . 149 26. Means and Standard Deviations of Intensity Scores on the Traditional-Attitude— Toward—Education Scale Comparing High and Low Frequency of Contact with Edu— cation for the Total Sample . . . . . 150 27. Analysis of Variance of Intensity Scores on the Traditional-Attitude—Toward—Edu— cation Scale Comparing High and Low Fre— quency of Contact with Education for the Total Sample . . . . . . . . . . 150 28. Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude- Toward-Education Scale for the Three Respondent Groups . . . . . . . . 151 29. Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude-Toward-Edu— cation Scale for the Three Respondent Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . 151 “4 Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Traditional-Attitude- Toward-Education Scale for the Three Respondent Groups . . . . . . . . 152 31- Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Traditional-Attitude-Toward-Edu— cation Scale for the Three Respondent Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . 152 32- Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to the Emotionally Disturbed) and Content of Attitude Toward Emotionally Disturbed Persons (EDP Scale) in the Total Sample . 154 xiii nu . 'r v-.. n" ”4.. '5. A t-v- . a U.‘ ... "v ‘ a» o . ‘1 - o. .. -_' "v. .- .-- Table Page 33. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to the Emotionally Disturbed) and Content of Attitude Toward Physically Handicapped Persons (HP Scale) in the Total Sample . 155 34. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to the Emotionally Disturbed) and Content of Attitude Toward Physically Handicapped Persons (HP Scale) in the Three Re- spondent Groups. . . . . . . . . 156 35. Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude—Toward-Emotion— ally-Disturbed-Persons (EDP) Scale for the Three Respondent Groups. . . . . 158 36. Analysis of Variance of EDP Content Scores for the Three Respondent Groups . . . 158 37. Duncan's New Multiple Range Test Applied to Means of EDP Content Scale for the Three Respondent Groups . . . . . . 159 38. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to the Physically Handicapped) and Content of Attitude Toward Physically Handicapped Persons (HP Scale) in the Total Sample . 160 39. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to the Physically Handicapped) and Content of Attitude Toward Emotionally Disturbed Persons (EDP Scale) in the Total Sample. 161 40. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to the Physically Handicapped) and Content of Attitude Toward Physically Handicapped Persons (HP Scale) in the Three Re- spondent Groups. . . . . . . . . 162 41. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to the Physically Handicapped) and Content of Attitude Toward Emotionally Disturbed Persons (EDP Scale) in the Three Re- spondent Groups. . . . . . . . . 163 xiv . 1,... 9" .‘ \ a 10"” fl" ' . - \"“J--. Uvuhc- < . . A"'-. . . :- oat-OJ I .. . .-,..- 'u. u..u--.-o. U l - l ,. ”a..- o‘ . , . ‘ U.‘ C. . u 0"... s...- O'. O... . . n 7", a ‘ ou....... . .' u D. .., u- -. ..‘. .. r A- “on- " .-.‘.. 4 - nu ’ a a o ,' ' II n ""O‘. ‘.. a p,o... v......__ 0.1,, ’--n...‘ ‘-.1‘ ‘ ""01“ - a... ‘ n "'-. s. . . g. . '4‘ 2 or ‘fioc at .‘O ‘b- ‘rb-,‘ .- v....‘-' e-‘l . ’v 9". .- 5“. I § .- “a .v . ‘- ‘ ’v . G' - ,_.- “v. I- .v I‘- II n. ‘ --.‘ I 5 II- . » 'I_~- . A n ,. ~ ‘D ‘- 'Q - , c ’. y‘a". “" h“- J Table 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude-Toward-Handi- capped Persons (HP Scale for the Three Respondent Groups. . . . . . . Analysis of Variance of HP Content Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Duncan's New Multiple Range Test Applied to Means of HP Content Scale for the Three Respondent Groups. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to Edu- cation) and Content of Progressive Attitudes Toward Education in the Total Sample . . . . . Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to Edu- cation) and Content of Traditional— Attitudes-Toward-Education in the Total Sample . . . . . Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to Edu- cation) and Content of Progressive- Attitudes-Toward-Education in the Three Respondent Groups. . Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Contact Variables (in Respect to Edu- cation) and Content of Traditional- Attitudes-Toward-Education in the Three Respondent Groups. . Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude—Toward-Emotion- ally—Disturbed-Persons (EDP) Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the Total Sample Analysis of Variance of EDP Content Scores Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the Total Sample XV Page 165 165 166 167 168 169 170 173 173 .o' .. a. , _ .. - . ‘ .- u ~‘dov .- u - . _ .- . - “-a. .__ . I- ,. _ .~ ‘ ‘ ‘u-n... . . I - . “A " u ' _v- .u.. _.‘-. - I ...."~ ." - .' -o- - .n" - ‘vn...... ‘p- n ’v' 0'. ._.. - . ~ .5. p ‘v.‘- fin 4 :a p. \ on '. (H I ‘ hble Page 51. Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude-Toward—Handi- capped-Persons (HP) Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the Total Sample . . . . . 174 52. Analysis of Variance of HP Content Scores Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the Total Sample . 174 53. Zero-order Correlations Between Attitude- Toward-Emotionally—Disturbed-Persons Scale (EDP Content) and the Gordon Value Scale for the Three Respondent Groups and the Total Sample. . . . . 175 TL Zero-order Correlations Between Attitude- Toward-Handicapped-Persons Scale (HP Content) and the Gordon Value Scale for the Three Respondent Groups and the Total Sample. . . . . . . . . . 176 EL Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude- Toward—Education Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the Total Sample . . . . . . . . 178 55. Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude—Toward-Edu- cation Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the ' Total Sample. . . . . . . . . . 178 57. Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Traditional-Attitude— Toward-Education Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the Total Sample . . . . . . . 179 58- Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Traditional-Attitude-Toward-Edu— cation Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Leadership Value for the Total Sample. . . . . . . . . . 179 xvi I" .I. - ' ’ C a... u g... ~ .n' by,- p I--. 5,.» .- u... v .- .-. n s: I ‘r on ‘7 u» 4 u ..'_ h- ~ ---.... n V. - .- II. o o. l n u- a .h -- n v V ~. -_’ H" \ « 1 u v. ‘ — H..- 'v -I ... r Table Page 59. Zero-order Correlations Between Progres- sive-Attitudes-Toward—Education (Con- tent) and the Gordon Value Scale for the Three Respondent Groups and the Total Sample . . . . . . . . . . 181 ML Zero-order Correlations Between Traditional- Attitudes-Toward-Education (Content) and the Gordon Value Scale for the Three Re— spondent Groups and the Total Sample . . 182 EL Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude-Toward-Emotion- ally—Disturbed- -Persons (EDP) Scale Com- paring High and Low Scores on Recognition Value for the Total Sample . . . . 183 62. Analysis of Variance of EDP Content Scores Comparing High and Low Scores on Recog— nition Value for the Total Sample . . . 183 61 Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude-Toward-Handi- capped-Persons (HP) Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Recognition Value for the Total Sample . . . . . 184 64. Analysis of Variance of HP Content Scores Comparing High and Low Scores on Recog- nition for the Total Sample . . . . . 184 55. Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude- Toward—Education Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Recognition Value for the Total Sample . . . . . . . 185 66- Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude—Toward-Edu- cation Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Recognition Value for the Total Sample . . . . . . . . . . 185 67- Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Traditional-Attitude— Toward-Education Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Recognition Value for the Total Sample. . . . . . . . . 186 xvii if u' U u . a , .- - a u- ‘ ‘finlv ~ A e r .“ ‘ 4 .‘vu o . . . .‘ ,. u-.- .. I ‘ § \ ---¢o'v-u a. o \q.“.- o“ u ., ’1--,r , A "O'cuon t‘ ' . Pa "_"'v‘-;r ""leu‘-'. t . V‘ n... p. . \ ' s ‘ u...~_-d v. ‘a.“‘~ .» -\ V. ‘0“ . ., . TT'F'Q " 'va ‘ K ‘ "dun-u . .‘ l l"¢. ‘ ‘ . “-o..- v r .. '- 75-: d 0‘..-‘ '1 . .. . '-".' :7. .-"" no... g -V' to 3,! L). Table 68. 69. 70. 71. 72. 73. 74. 75. 76. 77. 78. Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Traditional-Attitude—Toward—Edu- cation Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Recognition Value for the Total Sample Means and Standard Deviations of Leadership Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups . . Analysis of Variance of Leadership Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Means and Standard Deviations of Recog- nition Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Analysis of Variance of Recognition Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Duncan's New Multiple Range Test Applied to Means of Recognition Value Scale for the Three Respondent Groups Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude-Toward—Emotion- ally-Disturbed-Persons (EDP) Scale Com- paring High and Low Scores on Benevolence Value for the Total Sample Analysis of Variance of EDP Content Scores Comparing High and Low Scores for Benevolence Value for the Total Sample Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Attitude—Toward-Handi- capped—Persons (HP) Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Benevolence Value for the Total Sample Analysis of Variance of HP Content Scores Comparing High and Low Scores on Benevolence Value for the Total Sample Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude- Toward-Education Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Benevolence Value for the Total Sample xviii Page 186 188 188 189 189 190 192 192 194 194 195 Table 79. 80. 81. 82. 83. 84. 85. 86. 87. 88. 89. Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Progressive-Attitude-Toward-Edu- cation Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Benevolence Value for the Total Sample Means and Standard Deviations of Content Scores on the Traditional—Attitude— Toward-Education Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Benevolence Value for the Total Sample Analysis of Variance of Content Scores on the Traditional-Attitude-Toward-Edu- cation Scale Comparing High and Low Scores on Benevolence Value for the Total Sample Means and Standard Deviations of Benevolence Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups . . Analysis of Variance of Benevolence Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Means and Standard Deviations of Support Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups . Analysis of Variance of Support Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Duncan's New Multiple Range Test Applied to Means of Support Value Scale for the Three Respondent Groups Means and Standard Deviations of Conformity Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Analysis of Variance of Conformity Value Scores for the Three Respondent Groups Duncan's New Multiple Range Test Applied to Means of Conformity Value Scale for the Three Respondent Groups xix Page 195 196 196 198 198 199 199 200 202 202 203 Jn‘ Cl] [II 'I ' l I (,0) Table 90. 91. 92. 93. 94. ‘95. 96. 97. 98. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Atti- tudes Toward Emotionally Disturbed Per- sons (EDP Scale) for the Total Sample. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Attitude Toward Physically Handicapped Persons (HP Scale) for the Total Sample. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Atti- tudes Toward Emotionally Disturbed Per- sons (EDP Scale) for the Three Re- spondent Groups . . . . Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Atti- tude Toward Physically Handicapped Per- sons (HP Scale) for the Three Re- spondent Groups . . . Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Pro- gressive—Attitude-Toward—Education for the Total Sample. Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Tradi— tional-Attitude—Toward—Education for the Total Sample. . . . . Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Pro— gressive-Attitude-Toward—Education for the Three Respondent Groups Partial and Multiple Correlations Between Change Variables and Content of Tradi— tional-Attitude—Toward-Education for the Three Respondent Groups Comparison of Mean Differences and Standard Deviations in Respect to Five Change Orientation Variables for the Three Respondent Groups . . . XX Page 207 208 209 211 212 214 215 Table 99. Analysis of Variance of Change Orientation Scores for the Three Respondent Groups lML Means, Standard Deviations, and Critical Ratio of Content Scores on the EDP and HP Scales for Mothers of Non- Handicapped (i. e., normal) Children lmL Means, Standard Deviations and Critical Ratio of HP Content Scores for Mothers of Physically Handicapped and Non- Handicapped (i.e., normal) Children xxi Page 218 220 221 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. A Mapping Sentence for the Facet Analysis of Attitudes Toward Education. . . . 256 2. A Mapping Sentence for the Facet Analysis of a Research Project on Cross Cultural Attitudes Toward Education. . . . . 257 xxii -” n... .c- -... .1- -¢¢ ~ 'I ..,' . '- .- n -. .- 'u 0 '1 n. - .— I'. s _ '- "- s -. a —‘ «- - . g. 1 -. n n s .— Appendix A-l A-2 A-3 A-4 A-5 A-6 A-7 A-8 B-6 LIST OF APPENDICES Page Handicapped Persons Scale . . . . . . 282 Emotionally Disturbed Persons Scale. . . 290 Education Scale . . . . . . . . . 297 Gordon Survey of Interpersonal Values . . 305 Personal Questionnaire (general). . . . 307 Personal Questionnaire: HP . . . . . 329 Personal Questionnaire: EDP . . . . . 336 Definitions of Physical Handicap. . . . 342 Definition of Emotional Disturbance. . . 344 Administration Procedures . . . . . . 346 Basic Variables of the Study . . . . . 350 Rationale and Procedures for Producing Item Directionality . . . . . . . 355 Code Book . . . . . . . . . . . 360 (a) International Study. . 361 (b) Special Instructions for Tiffin Study. 407 FCC I and II Variable-Computer Print-Out Code Forms . . . . . . . . . . 412 Data Transcription Sheet . . . . . . 419 xxiii uh o.-.- ‘ . .. . ... q. . v . ... ~ CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The field of mental health and rehabilitation has bemawitnessing a gradual but steady shift from treat— mmuzof illness to preventive intervention by the com- mmflty. Although the traditional psychiatric methods of panent care have not proved to be completely unprofitable, ymauu'faith is currently being placed on new approaches to:h&erpersona1 behavior encompassing a host of social pmyduflogical variables (Adams, 1964; Bandura, 1961; Mary,1957; Rees, 1957; Szaz, 1960). This current trend in flmeprofessional and academic approaches to emotional cflsmumance and physical disability has manifested itself in huneasing emphasis on the community mental health move— ment. Nature of the Problem The comprehensive community mental health and rehabi— Mtathx1programs have become the most exciting and worth— Whtkaendeavors of the present decade in the fields of both behavioral sciences and medicine. The concern for the . '~ 7. . l‘~'\ . '1 ‘ ’- .. ‘ s v.‘ “ C.‘-‘ ~ v s .. . ‘.- .‘c ‘. n 12 Scale, comparative values by the Recognition and Leadership Scales. These scales were judged by the researcher to have acceptable construct validity for the measurement of the values proposed by Wright. Additional value orientations measured by the Gordon Survey of Interpersonal Values are labeled Support, Conformity, and Independence. Emotional disturbance.--This refers to those children or adults whose behaviors, feelings or emotions cause them to have difficulties with every day problems which they are unable to solve. Impairment.--This term signifies a defect in tissue or in body structure; and as such it has no particular functional connotations. Handicap.--This refers to the social disadvantages placed upon a physically impaired person as a result of impairment. A handicap is a consequence of culturally rmld values and attitudes which serve to define the gflwsically impaired person socially. Physical disability.--This is a functional term vduch denotes some loss of the tool function of the body. Anemproximate synonym for this term is "physically in- cmpmfltated." The technical distinction between "handicap" ami"disability" is, however, not very meaningful to the mnhers used in this research. Therefore, throughout the “mearch the term handicap was used with the mothers to dflmte"what has been technically defined herein as Eisabi lity. c . . ~ O;- -I '_I‘ .g. - .....~ .- .. '0'... h ‘ O .- .-.. .cacgu- . . '4 ---4- .. .V. .- I-.. - .‘ ‘5.’-'-.l ., .~ .....’.. o-n,"- a a .v-‘:‘J‘r . . .. "‘1 ‘ , "Ov..-‘ 1 ' s.-. .. .-~ - . ,4 , ' fi' ‘-. ' rpm '5 H... . n... ' v5 . '--..T a“; a . ‘.‘ U . 'v...‘ - v i . ..-. s '. ‘ "s I I. ’ ‘ns‘, . . -V-o . "-_ ‘\ -r. - .- “-— ,y’ . . .‘_, ~~- '- ._.' — ~.‘, ‘o‘ I»! ‘v ‘d‘ ~ .‘~ ‘_‘ . "u I V V.' r ‘ ‘0 a- I - - -" 2 ‘ ‘s. 0’ '. ‘ .5. u ~ . l .- Tn‘I- 's' - \ .‘ ' -u - ..’ . ‘ 1‘4 . . . ’ ~ .- .- .‘ \ " s ‘u p .‘ . l3 Rehabilitation.-~As defined by Jordan (1964), this term refers to "restoration of the disabled to the fullest physical, mental, social, and vocational usefulness possible." Educational progressivism.--Kerlinger (1958) has developed a ten-item scale of progressive attitudes toward education. Educational traditionalism.—-Another ten-item scale of traditional attitudes toward education developed by Kerlinger (1958) has been used in this study. These measures do not constitute scales in the Guttman sense, but rather are constituted of items which appeared in factor-analytic studies, and which were characterized by the terms that identify the scales. Special education.--As defined by Kirk (1962, p. 29), this term characterizes educational practices "that are Lnnque, uncommon, of unusual quality, and in particular are in addition to the organization and instructional pro- cmhues used with the majority of children." Jordan (HM4, p. 1) has elucidated: "The basic aims of special eduundon is to prevent a disability from becoming a handicap." Demographic variables.--Certain statistical data fiequently employed in sociological studies will be used 1nthe present investigation. These variables are age, eflmatuion, income, rental, occupation, number of siblings, Oacupational and residential mobility, and whether the -r v no. A a .. V‘s ; n... 9‘ .- .u‘- ._.. nus fir v“ v -.... .... ‘ \ V l4 respondent spent his youth in a rural or urban setting. Information on these variables were secured through responses of subjects on questionnaire items. Institutional satisfaction.--This term is used to describe a set of variables on which the respondents were asked to indicate how well they felt that various kinds of local institutions were doing their job in the community. Specifically, the institutions were schools, business, labor, government, health services, and churches. Interest group.--Any group that, on the basis of one or more shared attitudes, makes certain claims upon other groups in the society to engage in particular forms of behavior. Associational interest groups work as collect- ivities to exert influence (e.g., Almond and Coleman, 1960). Occupational personalism.--This term is operationally defined by questionnaire items designed to ascertain: first, about what per cent of the time people work with cmhers with whom they feel personally involved; second, how important it is to work with people with whom one is pmmonally involved. A personalistic orientation to life issometimes considered as a distinguishing characteristic oftraditional social patterns (e.g., Loomis, 1960). Relational diffusion.--This term is operationally duhned by a questionnaire item designed to determine the enent to which personal relations on the job diffuse into aperson's non-job social milieu. A personalistic diffusion MMWeeza the social milieu and occupational milieu is $1: . 1. ..¢..-- n ... _ ;‘ ..u ,‘_‘ fl... . . --..,‘ ." ’- .lc....\ , I.. ‘ u .. .- .'- . n— '_ " 'O a..- m u. — >. _ .‘c- . ‘ v, . ~ . "’.. ‘- Us --‘. 1 H'.‘ ~.; . . - ‘ -.. » ~.“ 1 . _. -- .‘-~" 'V. . ‘ ' o '- ' §_ ._-- - -., _ ‘ e. v-__ - ' 5‘— Q ‘ ‘l ‘ 5 - a. ‘ . .“- - I- 's -. ‘—. e ~ ‘-' .§ “ n - . ‘- - .- “ .- ‘- \.._ ‘ ‘ -- - , - c. ‘ l5 sometimes considered as a distinguishing characteristic of traditional social patterns (e.g., Loomis, 1960). Religiosity.--A term signifying orientation to religion. Operationally, it refers to three aspects: first, religious preference; second, the importance of religion; third, the extent to which the rules and regula— tions of the religion are followed. ResearchWHypotheses The research hypptheseélpresented hereunder are con- cerned with attitudes and values toward emotionally dis- turbed and physically disabled persons. Although it was recognized that additional questions and hypotheses would emerge in the course of investigation, the major hypotheses of this research were framed as follows: Hypotheses Related to Contact Frequency, Intensity and Attitude Scores . l. The more frequent the contact with emotionally disturbed persons, the higher will be the scores on the intensity statements of the attitude-toward-emotionally- chsturbed-persons (EDP) scale, regardless of whether amfltude content is favorable or unfavorable. 2. Mothers of emotionally disturbed children will Imve greater intensity of attitude toward emotionally fisturbed persons than will the mothers of physically handi- ‘meed or non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. \ 1. a For all hypotheses in which tests of significance It irltnolved, the statement of the hypothesis is in the Pseazrcfla form rather than the null form for purposes of Clarity. . - " l I- l a... c p u . .0” -.n .'.. . . '0' bad l .. ... _. -.. ..-.-.. v. -...'.. . n .- < - o..h.‘ ‘ “l_‘ ‘ . ‘ c.-. “' 1.....v '. .- .' "~av-. "9.- ' ...,l 'v ‘-c.... .. ' - - O- .— " a.._ 0' \D“ .. “-L. 1‘ w 7. - s ,' .- | c._ ' ‘.’-, ~ ‘-.‘ _~ -“-‘ '. . . . «A '0 — 9. ’c ‘- o a J‘- ‘ I s u.‘ c '» '. “. u.“ M, Q s v. N.’ ‘_ ‘n -- ‘. - . -~'-._ ... 's .' " '- C‘- ‘ - I '. .h‘ . '. ,~ ~ L 16 3. The more frequent the contact with physically handicapped persons, the higher will be the scores on the intensity statements of the handicapped persons (HP) scale, regardless of whether attitude content is favorable or unfavorable. 4. Mothers of physically handicapped children will have greater intensity of attitude toward physically handicapped persons than will the mothers of emotionally disturbed or non—handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 5. The more frequent the contact with education, the higher will be the scores on the intensity statements of the Education scale,regardless of whether attitude is traditional or progressive. 6. Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children will have greater intensity of attitude toward education (traditional and progressive) than will the mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 7. High frequency of contact with emotionally dis- meed persons will lead to favorable attitudes if high fiequency is concurrent with (a) alternative rewarding mmwrtunities, (b) enjoyment of contact, and (c) ease of ggddance of contact. 8. Mothers of emotionally disturbed children-will bane more positive attitudes toward emotionally disturbed Imrscnis than will the mothers of physically handicapped W’nCUI-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. : ..- ‘p A_‘ , u "" III 1. .-«-—— .. \l V \ ‘ a c" I. .. '.‘v' u . . ' u 'u- . ._. . v... g._ . . ‘ . -. ,_‘ .- H“ ’_II ,. a, — ' >.. ._. Q ..- ‘- .. 'a ._ ' ‘r- ..u.' .z - .__- ' . .- ._ a ‘ ~‘ - ‘ . ‘- - ‘ .,... , , - .._.. .. j .. \-.“. I. . ‘ _ __ ~-_ - \ .._ ‘ \ - v. “n. v._ ‘n.. _ ‘- R I g.‘ .,_ 7). u. ' — .‘ '_ *- '. . ~\. ., - 'o a ‘l . I u e .. -, ~ . - .‘ ~ , u ." . ‘ s ‘- v - Q \ . .-, l7 9. High frequency of contact with physically handi- capped persons will lead to favorable attitudes if high frequency is concurrent with (a) alternative rewarding opportunities, (b) enjoyhent of contact, and (c) ease of avoidance of contact. 10. Mothers of physically handicapped children will ham more positive attitudes toward physically handicapped pamons than will the mothers of emotionally disturbed ornon-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. ll. High frequency of contact with education will leaito favorable attitudes if high frequency is concurrent with (a) alternative rewarding opportunities, (b) enjoyment of contact, and (c) ease of'avoidance of contact. Pmpmfimses Related to Attitude- value Interacti ons l2. Mothers who score high_in need for power and armrol over others will tend to score $23.1“ acceptance ofemmtionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. 13. Mothers who score high in need for power and cmuxbl over others will tend to score £93 in progressive amfltudes toward education and high in traditional attitudes toward e ducat i on . 14. Mothers who score high_in need for recognition amiachievement will tend to score l2!.1n acceptance of mmmionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. 15. Mothers who score high_ in need for recognition amiachievement will tend to score low in progressive ‘ I .‘,..,,.'ao-,, v L. .an-o-o‘- . Vans“: . I .uv-s-u. ~ 9- . F ‘V' I find a . 5|:.-,.,~ 1 r . I u- .- Din-1". «I ‘. I v n. .q \. ’.'._V' 0' Aa-u..». . . .- - ,u .. ‘ '- A.....,‘. ' Q ’ .. ‘ .4... H ”J- ... F ‘ ..n....: ‘-‘ ‘I o I “§,. I- at! ..__..' ....‘.__.‘ . J ' Q ~ fr a... ’;“‘. - J, ”r“; 5'- '-~.." in. "u. fir: . . . '“‘A ”v 18 attitudes toward education and high in traditional attitudes toward education. 16. Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children will score lgggg on the values of leadership and Recognition than will the mothers of non- handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 17. Mothers who score high in need to help others, nabe generous, will tend to score high in acceptance of amnionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. 18. Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically hmmncapped children will score higher on the value of mnmvolence than will the mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children . l9. Mothers who score high in need to help others, ‘mabe generous, will tend to score high in progressive andtudes toward education and 12! in traditional attitudes toward e due at i on . 20. Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically hauflcapped children will score higher on the value of §§§5E§_than will the mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children . 21. Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically hauflcapped children will score lower on the value of Con- flflfl!£1_than will the mothers of non—handicapped (i.e., rmrmal) children. I A " V»FP:H‘ :1. ......v... '1 ' n. I- . .. .-.“6 P 2‘ ' ‘ y gnu... .r-- 'o-vvd . . _ -~ ”g. :rn rru .un-vd nan-i :na.' A \ O hovvcnbgu :: ,4“an ‘Ar ‘Wf;:uu' p "" it‘s-a... 'n V"’;n: " itvdn.—.~ . ""‘Ip. - ~~ fi':-.: u..-- ‘ >o..‘.." ., '01.. “ 1 ‘ a...“ Cu.“ ., O‘- - .; - -:.‘"':*-'~r , ..,...,,__m’ l .. .- 1. .' ' ‘ 0‘s 4' "y .lv'..: . . i _‘ . N 't .‘ ‘. ‘r- '- ‘ 'v F." .." p.‘- F V “‘1 :V'A: . v . I _“ 9.. r .. .,_ ‘ ._~. -r r' ‘ \‘. If‘r .‘v 5..»- C..- . o‘ c 6. a ‘3... \‘.-N a . u“‘ i“. v. a ‘ ~- ~‘.‘ '51.:- ‘- l9 Hypotheses Related to Change Orientation and Attitude Scores 22. Mothers who score high on change orientation will also score high on positive attitudes toward emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. 23. Mothers who score high on change orientation will also score high on progressive attitudes toward education amd $2! on traditional attitudes toward education. 2h. Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically lumdicapped children will have higher mean scores than will Immhers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children on the fifllowing change orientation measures: (a) health practices, UH child rearing practices, (0) birth control practices, RD automation, and (e) self change. Eflxmheses Related to General Differences Between Mothers of Distrubedl Handicapped and Non-Handicapped Children 25. Mothers of non—handicapped (i.e., normal) children wtfl.tend to have more favorable attitudes toward mwsically handicapped than toward emotionally disturbed persons . 26. Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children WLU.have less favorable attitudes toward physically handi- owned persons than will the mothers of physically handi- Capped Chi ldren . t-v-Iooo—. . ,- ‘O -“ .u—I-.. ‘ ‘. -”"VP sf.- a ~-o---d‘:~.. ,_,_ v . ' .' '. . .. . _ ' .‘ _I ‘ p'\ ,. -n.-o.-~--'~ " ‘ o - . -.\ :rh y.....,:, ._‘~- ~“ ‘-. H- - \. ~' ‘ Q ‘ "“ as- _ ‘ a -5 t; ,. _., . .~v.-,_‘ «KN». . C . ...“.—: . ‘ s C '. ~_"- u'“. n.- __ ¢-.: V..-" "u a‘§ -' ... u. U a 'Q“" —‘,—‘ . ‘ . d .--.. u. -.‘ - ~.. ._ I‘ .- vo— ' .‘4._ ‘4. 3 - ~- _‘ fl \ '3' pa ‘v u— 1‘-'; .0 “;.‘- ~ \ -._ h. ‘0 h n..- .- .‘_ _ ‘ Q ’\~ I ‘~' -—.. v- ‘- ." v O. ‘.‘ ‘o ‘I l .5 .- ~ ' ~Plp -\ “v- _ u.‘ . ‘ : y s ‘v— o. 4 . '0 .‘<“ 'I' .D" Q V. I“: 5‘ “ § “ * V d '0 s \1‘ I- I , u h . ‘a- ~v ' " an ‘0 :‘ A a. u“ H, a ., “‘- . - ~ ~ I 4“ s ‘ u ‘\ '9 . A ‘1 ' U n‘ s . ;.‘ o -A_ CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED RESEARCH AND THEORY Although there have been many studies exploring the dmracteristics of parent-child relationships in mental illness and physical disability, most of them have focused Lmon isolated aspects such as relationship of selected path- wmnic parental traits to psychopathological or undesirable mnsonality development of the child (Barker, 1948; Bolles, Imnger, and Pitts, 1941; Field, 1940; Kasanin, Knight and $fim, 1934: Pintner, Eisenson, and Stanton, 1941; Putnam, lWfih Radke, 1946; Tietze, 1949; Wood, 1946).1 In addition totflm restricted nature of the variables investigated, these cannrstudies have suffered from serious methodological mfiects. The methodological problems will be discussed at mmw length in a later section of this chapter. During the hmt two decades, still greater attention was given to .mmearch in the area of attitudes and values as related to mmmional disturbance and physical handicap. In recent years ahug the impact of the social environment on the devel— ment, stability and changes in attitudes toward For an extensive review of parent—child interactions, ‘Um reader is referred to Spiegel and Bell (1959)- 20 .,--"‘,L‘ r_,“\_ . ._' _ I .-J Gd av 9"'.- ' I ...qr- P“": ‘ ..,4-.'—-~ vb. .. v . '..o r‘b‘l- a .4 l-a‘. ‘v'-" ..-~~ p-na a 4 .—~ .\ \ au-‘u u... .0... I '\ no r. r- a... l U Q g -""' fl“»«finr ‘ ‘ fl ,0..." v ‘v--- . "' ~.... ,"._'_‘ n — .. _ “ "‘-.us...—...‘ ‘49.: ~ ' a " 'r—u a' coo-n...‘, P ‘ . -4 ., ‘ ‘Q -.‘I- 6“.- , h. A . Inn._ ,— ‘. .‘v 7“ ... ~\ "'I-‘.~' p. _‘ u. n_ -._ a... A _ "-.A._ a fi- - v- - "- ~. 9...“.- '-v.. - v.4... .- p -o-“ ’r- n. r c. ‘ .""-~ .I' I._ \ u .“h. 7'. a ‘ a- ' " . A ~l I ‘P- - - —. -.. v . . 'b-b ‘ . .‘ , ~ u .‘v\ ‘- ‘I‘ '4 u c. .- ‘ \, '- Q...- ‘1 u ‘ -.I u‘; v Nu. ~ — .-. . .t »‘ y‘ap‘ ‘I "v. ' '2“, ~ \ , _ ~— ~,.— ‘ .- ‘ ~" u .- g ’ r I; ‘_‘ ‘._ . .. - < ‘- a4 ‘w: -.~ .- . “ .- I“ ‘_. , :-. ~ on 1 '- 4 ~ 21 handicapped persons have been studied rather extensively by psychologists, sociologists, and rehabilitation experts. That attitudes and values toward various social objects are instilled early in life needs no documentation. Interest has shifted from the study of the chronological development of attitudes and values to a consideration of certain crucial factors in the individual's life affecting that develOpment. In general, social contact, exposure to education, and interpersonal values have been found to be amh crucial factors responsible for producing changes in attitudes. The related research to be presented in this section wiILbe concerned primarily with reviewing those studies thm:bear upon (a) attitudes toward emotionally disturbed masons, (b) attitudes toward physically disabled persons, mn1(c) the relationships of values, personal contact, and intensity to attitudes. Attitudes Toward Emotionally Disturbed Persons Egental Attitudes The significance of parental attitudes in the accep- taum of emotionally disturbed persons has been suggested ID’many researchers and practitioners in the field of Wflmal health and child development. The findings of early IBychological researches have indicated that parental atti- mums produce enduring impressions on the personality of . q l‘rr'v‘ A“ .- . ‘ ‘ - ‘ h I Q... -.-..‘.d. . . :-.. .9 ., ';" u . -.!.v do w ’ 0 ~‘. .-.-‘.p;“: or ‘ “Oo"‘-.U .-I y . . . <‘5. . .1 , o“ . ‘ r n ';n Aar- "‘ “'I vv-.¢ «1. ~ a o .. , _'_ ' ‘- ’\ ,. r a‘ ’o.-.... -.¢-' L “ I . '.;‘--'., vr ’ ” .“" .0. : I- r: ‘......-..-. L‘ . n ’ ’ huh“: ' hl."‘ov’ ~‘K— . . . ' Q .- a -:‘;, 3 ~ I ‘v v u.‘~‘-‘- ‘ l .. ‘ I ‘ P s, -.. . \ .I- "v . .. ~v.,-~'. - ..‘ _: .' .‘ H'a-‘ ' s F»u\ . "~b~ “ - ' A , x: ”I" 0....» v ‘u. ‘- "- n " . 2". f g.‘ '- ‘o ’- a 'L'b. ‘ ~,_-.' sh" ~~ ‘I .— ' Irv " V .“: .. ‘ . ‘..I I. HT»: ‘g.: V ‘u. ‘- - I“ . . ....s,v~p n. 1“ ~ I‘Qll \ 'o c , .,,.' P5 - “.o '-» ‘5” “‘v' [‘0 v v‘ H ‘. .h ‘P ~ .. . h‘. a cl “'. .' l.’~ . Q ‘1‘ .u :- .‘ — "- un“ ,. “.1 'l— .N . ."; a ‘.‘ ‘n' . ' c, -m, ‘- 22 the child. however, as mentioned earlier, most previous researchers were interested in determining the influence of specific pathogenic characteristics in the parents on later behavior symptoms in children. Review of the literature reveals the preference of researchers in studying parents of schizophrenic patients. As early as 1934, Kasanin, Knight, and Sage observed that sflxty per cent of the parents of a group of forty-five schizophrenic parents showed parental overprotection and mflection. In a similar study of twenty-five mothers of sdflzophrenic patients, Tietze (1949) found the mothers to beinsecure, superficial, rigid, and domineering. With a ‘wew to establishing patterns of parent-child relation- flups in schizophrenia, Riechard and Tillman (1950) analyzed sflxty-six cases from the literature plus thirteen of their own. Three categories of schizophrenogenic parents were idmfiflifled: (a) overtly rejecting type mother, (b) covertly mflecting type mother, and (c) schizophrenogenic father eflflbiting domineering, sadistic attitudes toward the child. Afactor-analytic study of attitudes of mothers of schizo— muenic patients by Shepherd and Guthrie (1959) indicated the pmxmbility of several schizophrenogenic patterns of mother- homL. The author was able to delineate five factors: (a) «mached authoritarianism, (b) inadequacy and inconsistency, H” pervasive control, (d) s0phisticated denial of inadequate Immhering, and (e) annoyance and rejection. The data also ...--g .. _ a .,..---‘ . er. .'.- ,p..-vu c . u-n Or» in. “I. .‘- '9'" a y"... , fr ‘ n *.J v.n~ 'f1 'I «b . - q ‘ V a D“ fill-“-VU unnufi 5. v if 0 In ”‘6'- ‘I‘ m.'.., '.. _: -..'-v. p-.." "“ 'A -... n V'- ""“v‘F .. _. p van a..- -..’ . _V\A “‘ ‘ ...,’. ‘ ‘ “fir‘fia .. ‘ 'Uoo" ‘ u.. I ‘thfin‘.~ "~»...: h.“ A ~ '~ ‘ G ... r “" pn‘ag.’ ..,. __ v.- V" ‘3 ..-A‘ ‘ u \ . Q, ' Afr—r ’4. V v.." III J , a. I 23 indicated some relationships between maternal attitudes and self-concept and social-perception of the schizophrenic sons. From the point of view of research design, the above mentioned studies were inadequate in that none of them used a control group. A controlled study of personality rela- tionships in mothers of twenty-five male, hospitalized schizophrenic patients was conducted by Prout and White (1951). Although very little difference was found between the experimental and control groups, a more frankly critical attitude toward their children was expressed by mothers of the normal control group. Also, mothers of normal children ckmonstrated a more gregarious and outgoing attitude toward life. Interestingly enough, a similar investigation by lurk (1953) comparing mothers of normal children with those of hospitalized schiZOphrenics found contradictory results. The mothers of schizophrenic children manifested restric— tive attitudes in controlling their sons, and exhibited both excessive devotion and cool detachment. Zuckerman, Oltean, and Monashkin (1958) used the Parental Attitude Research Instrument (PARI), a currently popular research instrument developed by Shaefer and Bell (1958), to retest the familiar hypothesis that mothers of schizophrenics exhibit more controlling and rejecting attitudes in compar- ison to nmthers of normal children. In spite of the fact that significant interactions were observed between two groups and levels of education, the study failed to : .- g. . u - l I q . - ... . a . “u. 24 substantiate the main hypothesis. The results of this study are very similar to the one mentioned earlier by Prout and White (1951). From a methodological standpoint, studies comparing different types of mental disorders including a control group of parents of normal children should enable more reliable and valid conclusions. In this connection, it is worthwhile to mention two studies reporting reSults in the opposite direction. McKeown (1950) compared parental be- haviors of schizophrenic, neurotic, and normal children, and found statistically significant differences among the three groups. The parents of schizophrenics having the same sex showed demanding antagonistic behavior more fre- quently. The same type of behavior pattern was exhibited by both parents of neurotic children. But in the case of the . parents of normal children, encouraging behavior was pre- dominant. Klebanoff (1959) made a comparative study of parental attitudes of mothers of schizophrenic, brain-injured and retarded, and normal children with the help of Parental Attitude Research Instrument (PARI). Interestingly, mothers of schizophrenic children showed less rather than more patho- logical attitudes in contrast to the mothersof brain-damaged and retarded children. The author, quite legitimately, ex- presses grave doubts regarding the commonly accepted hypo- thesis that schiZOphrenia is caused by pathogenic parental attitudes. However, it must be pointed out that the samples - .u a ’. ..< o u .4. I. o-wl' 25 in this research were woefully small-fifteen mothers each in the two experimental groups and twenty-six mothers in the normal control group. One of the few studies which used MMPI scales in an attempt to establish definite relationships between parental pathology and the kinds of behavior problems exhib- ited by their children met with failure (Liverant, 1959). Nevertheless, significant MMPI differences between parents of disturbed (schizophrenic, neurotic, acting-out, and physically complaining) and non-disturbed children were observed and these supported the general clinical observa- tion. The pervasive influence of maternal attitudes has been shown in studies investigating genesis of autistic behavior and mental retardation. Despert's (1951) analysis of case material led him to conclude that mothers of autistic chil- dren were compulsive, perfectionistic, narcissistic, immature, frigid, emotionally detached, and frightened by bodily con- tact. Certain types of mental retardation have been consid- ered by one researcher to be a by-product of neurotic maternal attitudes (Goshen, 1963). Parental attitudes have also been shown to be associated with neurotic behavior in children. Field (1940), who studied maternal.attitudes of twenty-five children with conduct dis- orders and neurotic traits, found that inadequate parental role, rejecting attitude toward children, poor marital ad— justment, and infantile neurotic traits characterized these ... , . ,7.- . - 26 mothers. Similar results were obtained by Bolles, Metzger, and Pitts (1941) in a study where one hundred forty-two neurotic patients were compared with a normal control group. Probably, one of the better studies in this area is the one carried out by Shoben (1949) who develOped a questionnaire to measure dominant, possessive, and ignoring attitudes cm parents in relation to child adjustment. When the ques- tflonnaire was administered to the mothers of emotionally (fisturbed children, whose children had been identified as cflinical cases, juvenile offenders, or considered by mothers .wsbeing problem and normal children, a significant differ— ence was found in regard to the above-mentioned parental attitudes. There have been numerous studies that have shown some sort of relationship between pathological parental attitudes and school phobia (Estes, Hylett, and Johnson, 1956; Johnson, et al., 1941; Van Houten, 1948). Although such studies indicate the existence of neurotic parental traits only vaguely in cases of school phobia, a "careful EWudy rmight define more sharply the unique features of the 'phObOgenic' mothers" (Kessler, 1966, p. 239). Tunere appears to be fewer studies of parental atti- MMes jJi the area of juvenile delinquency and acting-out di~°i<>r'ders. At the Michigan Child Guidance Institute, a larger- Study involving five hundred cases was conducted with aviewr t<3 establishing relationships between behavior - we 1. fly“... ,l, _"... 27 patterns in the child and parental behaviors. The correla- tional analysis of the data revealed three patterns: (a) parental rejection correlated with the child's unsocialized aggression characterized by violence, cruelty, malicious mischief,and Open defiance of authority, (b) parental negligence and exposure to delinquency patterns correlated with socialized delinquent behavior in which the child was on good terms with a delinquent gang but Opposed the norms of adult society, and (c) parental overcontrol correlated with overinhibited behavior in the child marked by shyness, zmathy, and seclusiveness. As expected, these correlational patterns again, were not completely supported in another similar study where mothers of twenty-one adolescent social- ized delinquents displayed excessive control and very inadequate understanding of child's feelings (Cass, 1952). thing the Parental Attitude Research Instrument (PARI), Fwdoff (1959) attempted to measure maternal attitudes of the nmmhers of institutionalized delinquents and healthy adoles— cents. More pathogenic attitudes and authoritarian demands were expressed by mothers of delinquents. Winder and Rau (1962) found that parental attitudes of ambivalence, puni- tiveness, restrictiveness, and low maternal self-esteem correlated with peer evaluations of preadolescent boys Judged to be socially deviant. Of continuing interest to many researchers is the StUdY' of the influence of parental attitudes on children's 28 adjustment and behavior, in general. As is characteristic of the literature, no consistent results were obtained by the investigators on this tOpic. This is best illustrated in two early studies reported the same year. Lewis (1945), for example, found that more desirable scores were obtained by the children on BPC Personal Inventory Scores whose parents were rated as showing a "superior" attitude toward the child and home. On the other hand, Read's (1945) con- clusion was that no relationship exists between child behavior and parents' views in respect of desirable child behavior. However, child behavior was found to be positively related to liberalism in views on parental control. Other smudies, although differing in methodology, selection of samples, and use of research instruments, arrived at similar results regarding parental attitudes and child adjustment Uthman, Eyman, and Windle, 1963; Peterson, Becker, and Ikfllmer, 1959; Stern, 1964; Tamkin, 1964; Tolor and Rafferty, 1963) with the exception of one important study using PARI where the author concluded that there was no invariable r81ation between certain parental attitudes toward child rearing and parental acceptance of the child (Medinnus, 1963). A recent research explored some social-psychological Variables influencing parental acceptance of residential treatment for their emotionally disturbed children (Schuh- mmi, Ccm, and Rae-Grant, 1964). Mothers who accepted luacevnent differed significantly from mothers rejecting . 1 - A “H‘.~ 29 institutional placement. The latter showed feeling of alienation and guilt about the child, and tended to be rejecting of the child. 0n the dimension of authoritar- ianism interpreted as "an inability to take on child's role," there was a significant difference between parents of emotionally disturbed children and parents of normal children (Adams, 1965). However, when authoritarianism was taken to mean fascism—conservatism, no difference was found between the two groups of parents. The studies reviewed thus far give the impression that there is some kind of basic, perhaps qualitative, difference tmtween the parental attitudes of normal and emotionally cfisturbed children. But, as Law (1954) has pointed out, nmthers of normal children are not completely free of tensions nor do they possess infinite love and patience. In fact, the difference between the two groups of mothers may becxm of degree, rather than kind. It may be noted that although the literature abounds with research on maternal attitudes, this reviewer encount- ered only one study which surveyed common parental attitudes and reactions toward the "emotionally disturbed child" in Salt Lake City (Cole, Shaw, Steneck, and Taboroff, 1957). However, the researcher had a rather general purpose of dehermining the assimilation of recent psychiatric concepts incfllild-rearing practices. He found that the parents enuixited a fairly good knowledge of symptoms and cause of 30 amnional disturbance; but this was not correlated with umir desire to seek professional help. (kneral Studies Insofar as the public's attitudes toward emotionally <flsturbed persons are concerned, the entire program of cmmuehensive mental health planning and community psychi- aMw rests upon the positive attitudes of the community. {Mm following comment by Davis about the necessity of assessing peoples' attitudes and values still hold true in thepuesent decade: Since mental health is obviously connected with the social environment, to promote such health is to treat not only particular minds but also the customs and institutions in which the minds function (Davis, 1938, p. 55). Chamberlain and DeSchweinltz(l955) have suggested several facuns in community acceptance of the mental health prob— ikmm, such as, good public relations through news media, aiVoidance of technical terminology, and helpful and under- Stmuung person-to-person contacts. It would be no exagger- ation to state that "by now mental health has become a Sc>Cial goal and cultural value" (Ginsburg, 1955, p. 3). Moreover, there have been several theoretical and e'Tlpirical attempts to study attitudes of the public, in BNWTal, and various categories of professionals, in Daticular. The spurt of studies in recent years emphasiz- 1ngsocial-psychological aspects of the public attitude 31 toward emotional disturbance is particularly noteworthy. A closer look at the network of influences operating on those associated with the emotionally disturbed provided new insight into the whole problem of change in attitudes and beliefs, and pointed to new directions for further research in the area. Some of the most significant recent research have been concerned with the measurement of attitudes and opinions of the workers in the field of mental health. A series of statistically sophisticated studies have been published isolating factors responsible for differential attitudes of professionals and non-technical personnel in mental hospitals toward psychiatric patients (Cohen and Struening, 1962, 1963; Struening and Cohen, 1963). A SDecially constructed attitude scale, Opinions about Mental Illness (OMI), was used which yielded five major factors reflecting: (a) stress on patients' difference and inferi- OI‘ity to normals, (b) desire to place strong social restric- tions on them both during and after socialization, (c) moral Sense of obligation to help unfortunates, (d) more profes- S'ional attitudes toward their treatability, and (e) atti- Wkdes toward etiology of psychiatric illness. Apparently, the first two factors indicated negative attitudes and the neXt two, positive attitudes toward mental patients. In addition, these researchers have been able to determine Occupational profiles and profile clusters for nineteen - .. ..a1. «a.. - 32 occupational groups, and have investigated factorial invariance and other psychometric characteristics. Thus, their conclusion was that attitudes toward mental illness vary with the respondents' occupations. Furthermore, atti— tudinal responses were influenced not only by occupation, but by amount of education as well. These studies, there- fore, strongly suggest that different attitudinal patterns can be traced even in those who are most intimately con- nected with mental patients as voluntary workers. A similar attempt at exposing the factors involved in the organization of physicians' attitude towards the emotionally disturbed patients was made by Taylor (1965). The five factors discovered were: (a) self-confidence in treating the emotionally disturbed patients, (b) general dogmatism,and an authoritarian-repressive attitude, (0) perceived status of psychology and psychiatrists, (d) acceptance of the counseling role in general practice, and (e) an unidentified factor with main loadings on questions about psychiatric institutions. However, Taylor could not establish any direct relationship between the personality variables and negative feelings about the emotionally dis- turbed patients. But in a most recent study by Wright and Klein (1966), there are discernible indications that formal education and training and experience with mentally ill persons are conducive to favorable attitudes toward them. An interesting comparison of a random sample of the general public and psychiatrists concerning their conceptions 33 of mental illness was made by Manis, Hunt, Brewer, and Krecher (1965). There was no significant difference be- tween the views of the public and psychiatrists regarding the acceptance of troublesome behavior as an indication of mental illness compared to non-troublesome behavior. This study might suggest that the public has become more sophisticated in their knowledge of the symptomatology of mental diseases, and are not apt to consider only trouble- some behaviors as signs of mental disorder. 0n the other hand, a random sample of two hundred forty-seven white housewives revealed ignorance of contemporary ideas about mental illness (Stewart, 1959). The author, therefore, pointed to the need for education of the community by appropriate agencies. An ambitious project at the University of Michigan Survey Research Center undertook to study a representative Sample of two thousand four hundred and sixty Americans OVer twenty-one and living at home with the help of exten- Sive interviews regarding various facets of the mental hcalth problems (Gurin, Veroff, and Feld, 1960). The findings, however, quite expectedly, do not provide any Clear-cut picture of the situation. In his recent work, Nunnally (1961) has discovered a new dimension of "incon- Sistency" in the public attitude. People seem to either agree with inconsistent opinion statements or disagree With apparently consistent statements. 34 Apparently taking cues from the oft-quoted study by Ibllingshead and Redlich (1958) which attempted to corre— late social class with the incidence, development, diagnosis and treatment of mental illness, Stewart (1959) intended to establish relationships between opinions about mental illness and positions in the social class hierarchy among mute housewives. The results were inconclusive in that the pattern of responses failed to show any definite rela— tionship. Another relevant research with a restricted mmective of determining how rural residents differ from Intan dwellers in their attitude toward mental illness found no basic difference between the two groups insofar mathe utilization of mass communication media was con- cerned (Crawford, Rollins, and Sutherland, 1961). The (fissemination of mental health information was able to improve their knowledge of mental problems. New insights into our understanding of the public image of mental illness and mental health professionals fmve come from a rather comprehensive cross-cultural research consisting of two independent but complementary Studies on samples drawn from the continental United Skates, Hawaii, and England (Askenasy, 1963; Zavollani and (mkenasy, 1963). The first study investigated the views cn'mental health personnel whereas the second one dealt lath attitudes of the general public. Socio-cultural factors were found to be significantly related to attitudes .u‘ o I o . " “an a - 35 toward mental illnesses, notwithstanding the training and background of the respondents. Clusters of acceptance and rejections of mentally ill persons were also found in each cfi‘the three countries studied by these researchers. For example, degree of favorableness of the perception of mental patients was correlated with the degree of occupa— tional trust. A somewhat similar, but less sophisticated, cross- cultural survey of popular reactions to mental illness in the United States, England, and France was made earlier by the Commission de la Ligue Francaise d'Hygiene Mentale (1959). The results of this survey provided clear indica- tdons of stigma attached to mental patients which carries cwer even after their recovery. In summary, there does not appear to be general uneement as to the nature of popular attitudes toward mental patients. While a number of researchers suggest the prevalance of more or less positive attitude toward emotional disturbance and mental health personnel (e.g., Iemkau and Corcetti, 1962; Nunnally and Kittross, 1958), there are studies which have obtained contradictory results (e.g., Cumming and Cumming, 1957; Joint Commission, 1961; Star, 1956). Perhaps, we might agree with Erikson's Mmervations: It appears that on the surface the public has developed reasonably tolerant attitudes toward the mentally ill and even a hesitant respect for the practice of psychiatry. People understand t" F up" I‘..‘ ’a ..~-- .-r 1“. ...- u H... ,,,. - . ,, -. .. 36 the need for increased psychiatric facilities, appreciate the enormity of the mental health problem, and agree that mental illness is a condition requiring specialized treatment and competently trained help. Yet underneath the pleasant surface of these enlightened prin- ciples, people have little idea how to recog- nize the concrete problems that these principles encompass (Erikson, 1957, p. 270). Thus the only irrefragable conclusion in the present confusing situation that can be made at this time is that methodologically superior research under a comprehensive theoretical framework is desperately needed. Attitudes Toward Physically ‘Disabled Persons Earental Attitudes Research reports on the physically handicapped have shown assuredly the pervasive influence of parental atti- tudes. Consistent with many theoretical approaches to child psychopathology, the research findings in this field SUgg'est strongly that parental attitudes of hostility, nglt and self-recrimination produce a feeling of differ- ence auud inadequacy in the physically handicapped child. IWost systematic studies, although less comprehensive 1n5n3<>;3e, have been conducted in the area of visual dis- ahtt11;5,. Sommers (1944) who studied the influence of Damnat;51l attitudes and social environment on the personality dewfil<>lbrnent of the adolescent blind stated, ". . . the ma“ JOITj-133r of mothers studied experienced frustration or ..m -, l .o 37 feelings of conflict because of having given birth to a child." The attitudinal reactions of the mothers of blind children fell into five categories: (a) genuine acceptance, (b) an attitude of denial that either parent or child is affected by the handicap, (c) overprotectiveness and exces- sive pity, (d) disguised rejection, and (e) overt rejection. Significant relationship was observed between parental attitudes and the adjustment of visually handicapped and sighted individuals by Verillo (1958). Parents' attitudes of acceptance and rejection had a marked influence on their social and emotional adjustment. Additionally, verillo (1958) also found that persons of high socio- ecmkmuc status exhibited attitudes of overprotection, ckmunance, anti-minority, and authoritarianism. In4a similar vein, Underberg (1958), and Underberg, et a1. (1961) noted that there was less understanding in the parents of partially-seeing children than what was usually the case with ‘the parents of normally seeing children. The research mxuzlJided that this was due to the lack of proper under- standing of the emotional factors of partially-seeing children on the part of their parents. Much the same “Stilts were obtained earlier in another study probing the DSyCh(Dilogical problems of the congenitally blind (Cole and Taboroff, 1956). 1kn.illuminating study in many ways was carried out b y CC>C>1<' (1963) regarding mothers' attitudes of children with 38 one of the following handicaps: blindness, deafness, mongolism, cerebral palsy, and organicity. Their atti- tudes differed significantly depending upon the type of handicap of their children. The mothers of deaf children and children having organicity were overindulgent, whereas the mothers of mongloid and cerebral palsy children, were punitive. Toward their blind children, the mothers were found to be overprotective. In general, they expressed the attitude of rejection for the mildly handicapped and the attitude of overprotection for the severely handi— capped. In contrast to the studies reviewed above where the reeearch problem was the determination of existing parental attitudes toward the handicapped, Barclay and Vaught (1964) were interested in maternal estimates of future achievement in cerebral palsied children. The findings indicated that wulxiren having low intellectual potential as rated by ex— Derts were typically overestimated by the mothers regard- lefis le the child's age or degree of physical handicap. Ddore convincing results have been obtained in regard tothee relationship of parental attitudes to speech defects. wOmj C 3.946), in his investigation of parents of children Suf’f’e‘lT’flLrlg from stuttering or other articulatory defects, “Mnd t3hat the mothers were neurotic, submissive, and self- “msci-C3118. The fathers of these children, however, did not diff e17 iffirom the fathers of normal children. Differences in . 0" ". .0: l nil-t -'"‘-'.n .. _ Doc... .1‘- ‘ I ..o-- n“: "' —~v-.~. _ . ' . bus o-ns "‘0- . _ . R's cit-1.. .... " 0- I m, - .;~ I ""--‘ 1 .‘1 'I .. ._ ,r 'A.~‘I- h.‘ .,‘ \ v .I. o._: \ ’a., °~ «.: -‘ J u o.,‘ " -- '1' I '_ ..- \..._ ‘J I. . ! ‘¢“ :5. . - -v..'. c ",v. ‘. \‘o u.-. g is" ‘ a ‘u ‘ I “V ’ n . .'-, a..- “ ‘. o . - “..: _ U ... c ‘_ \ n D "- “n - ._. '- .5 . ' _\ A § ‘ ‘v C u '4 ._ .. . I w s... s ‘ . U \ . , .._~ . ' - ‘ .!'~ - '. n u h‘ I Q 39 attitudes and personality characteristics of mothers and fathers of stuttering children were studied by Holliday (1958) in a matchedecontrol experiment. The experimental- group fathers were matched with the control-group fathers on the variables of age, education and occupation, and the experimental-group mothers were matched with the control- group mothers on the variables of age and education. The data showed that the fathers of stutters were more compul- sive and less exhibitionistic or outgoing than the fathers of normal children. The mothers of stuttering children also tended to be more abasing in their attitudes toward tfiemselves than the mothers of children who do not stutter. Thus, Wood's (1946) conclusion that mothers and fathers of stmtters have similar personality characteristics was not corroborated by Holliday (1958). The influence of parental attitudes on the behavioral merfestations of the handicapped child has been indicated by.mau1y other investigators in reference to facial deformity (MacGregor, et a1. 1953), acquired physical deformities (Watscari and Johnson, 1958), cerebral palsy (Haring, 1959), andmixedorganic handicaps (Carter and Chess, 1951). In s°me manner, these physical disabilities "symbolize to the Parents 8. transmission of 'poor biologic inheritence'," according to Rome and Robinson (1959, p. 1267). The above mentioned studies including several others (B Mme 3 et a1. 1960; Denhoff and Holden. 1954; Fliesler - .- _‘..-.-o‘ . ,i 40 and Hebeler, 1960; Gurney, 1958; Reeves, 1962; Shere, 1956; Worchel and Worchel, 1961; Wortis and Cooper, 1957) have demonstrated in a general manner that parental attitudes are responsible, to a great extent, for the successful ad- justment Of the handicapped children and their acceptance by the community at large. Parental acceptance appears to enhance the self-concept of the disabled which, in turn, motivates them to make efforts for successful adjustment in the social life and in the world Of work. The interpretations that have been advanced by various researchers of the data on the relationship of parental attitudes to the manifest psychological problems (fl'disabled children clearly smack of a general bias in favor of psychoanalytic approach. That is, less emphasis has been placed on factors associated with ego-functioning at conscious and reality levels than on psychOpathological Luencesses Operating in the disabled and their parents at the leverl of unconscious fantasy and feeling. In their review of paycliiatric conditions associated with metabolic, endocrine, and rutitritional disorders, Rome and Robinson have surmised: In the presence Of gross genital anomalies (:fllntersex, cryptorchism, infantilism, pubertas E>IPaecox), the parents' response is in unconscious Elc:cord with the success or failure with which they have handled their own psychosexual problem and ‘Vfifth the repurcussions and consequences of this in heir marital relationship. The threat to mature psbfchogenitality is so realistically witnessed in tzriesse syndromes that parents rarely are able to §T<3<3ept with equanimity a deficiency of this sort r1 their progeny (Rome and Robinson, 1959, p. 1268). , . ,_0 P079 ‘ . n . ....u..--At . v u, .. o _, s. .... e .- I~.~‘ - .....' a. . ‘\ >-~"II ‘V . . -’O .- . _ ~o.o....- . l .- .' u-- a a . "“oovov. ' . ‘4>-- . . v. - _ "I.~.‘... ‘ >- O... . ’ --~..‘ " l , c a.. . ' n-c. - _\-.- I-~“ 8"‘09. ., . _ '- "--:_ . I . ._ a ‘ '§ .'Q - n.‘ ‘- . ' T‘-. ..,~.‘ . v‘ -‘ -A. ’II- ' d I 1 . . .‘D . — . a ~. - w. . . i a. "‘ '1 .,.. ‘n . ‘ '.. c "' - - C ‘t .I-_ .' - . .“ . F- .5- ‘ f-‘ _ . s, "s u . . . .' Q h . \ . n “ ‘ s .. u . '. ‘\'- 41 Whatever theoretical orientation one might adOpt for explaining the data, review of the literature points to the fact that parental attitudes toward handicapped chil— dren tend to be extreme with a preponderance of overpro— tection as opposed to overt rejection (Wright, 1960). Most frequently Observed attitudinal patterns are rejection, oversolicitude, inconsistency, and inflated estimation of accomplishments beyond the child's abilities (Barker and Wright, 1954). Thus, there appears to be general concord among researchers that the handicap Egg gg plays a less decisive role on emotional adjustment than the personali- ties and attitudes of persons to which the disabled child is exposed continually. However, we must not underestimate the role of other psychological variables in the personality develOpment of the disabled which might possibly prove to be far more unisequential than attitudes of the parents. As Kessler Puts it: But it would be a mistake to conclude that the Emersonality of the handicapped is determined solely buy parental attitudes. A handicapped child, like .auqy other child, observes himself and compares him- Esealf with other children (Kessler, 1966, p. 342). W Studies lFor the most part, empirical research in the field OfIDkllVESical handicap have been confined to the study of spec:i.f?j_c physical disability in a Specific setting in the 42 United States. This has posited serious restrictions on the generalizability of the findings. However, the impor- tance of such studies cannot be attenuated. The earliest psychological studies of physical handicap reflected the view that each physical disability gave rise to specific mental and personality characteris- tics. Such a psychobiological approach is best illustrated in the classic work, Psychology of the Physically Handi- caphed by Pintner, Eisenson, and Stanton (1941). In recent studies, however, greater emphasis has been placed on all those factors which might function as intervening variables between physical disability and its psychological mani- festations. Wright (1960) who has reviewed the entire anea Of attitudes toward physical handicap in her standard reference work, Physical Disability: A Psychologigal ggproach states that "somatic abnormality as a physical fact is not linked in a direct or simple way to psycholog- ical. behavior" (p. 373). The author further points out' that: "there are far fewer psychological experiences pecu- liar' 1:0 persons with physical disabilities than an offhand guess might indicate" (p. 3). In the literature on physical handicap, one frequently encomllifters the term "somatOpsychology" which has been de- flnec1 Ely Barker and Wright as referring to: ‘ . . those features of physique that affect the psychological situation of a person via his body :353 a.tool for behavior and as an Object with social wFl-Eglnificance to himself and others (Barker and right, 1954, p. 419). 43 Thus, in order to conceptualize the psychological aspects of physical disability, differing experiences resulting from differing disability should be emphasized. Barker and his associates (1953), in an early study, attempted a content analysis of attitudes expressed in Considerable religion, fiction and humor (pp. 74-76). variation in attitude was revealed by the religious and literary analyses. Generally Speaking, jokes relating to the physically handicapped were more deprecating than Similar jokes about salesmen and farmers, for example. tendency in the people to mask their negative attitudes toward disability with the help of jokes were observed by Barker and Wright (1955). Of all the physical disabilities, visual impairment A seems to have been most systematically investigated. series of studies regarding attitudes toward blindness have been reported by Whiteman and Lukoff (1962, 1964, They have been concerned particularly with the ex- 1965). Dior-ation of attitudinal components, and their relation- shiEDES with different personal values and differential Sen551gtivity to methods of attitude change (Whiteman and Lukcxfifg 1962). With respect tO attitude structure, the autrICDJRS found that for a Specific component, correlations are higher between disability groups. They also made attempts to trace the self-concept of the blind person and hi S (D‘VTl attitudes to the sighted. r- ‘ ' _ _ . ._ —d-.- We " . .___.,__r v 44 In their factorial study of sighted peOple's attitudes toward blindness, Whiteman and Lukoff (1964) were able to identify five factors: (a) the degree to which the respon- dmne.have a negative view of the emotional life and general mmquacy of blind people, (b) the degree to which the respon- dmfie see blind peOple as socially competent, (c) the degree nawhich blindness is perceived as potentially threatening mruniquely frustrating, (d) tendencies to be protective ofblind people, and (e) readiness for personal interaction luth blind people. The area of employer attitude toward the blind was reviewed by Clunk (1947). He discovered that many employers mud negative attitudes in complete disregard to the true mmfloyment potential of the blind. The socio-economic level of the respondents was also finmd to have a relationship with verbalized attitudes Mmerd the physically handicapped. In a paper presented to I\Ia‘cional Psychological Research Council on Blindness, Raskin (HEM) set forth a cogent analysis of the attitudes of sighed people towards blindness suggesting multiple deter— minsults of such attitudes. The possible operation of psy- Cr10dynamic, situational, socio-cultural, and historical deBterminants were hypothesized by the author. A search for deeper motivations underlying attitudes onthe part of the seeing toward blindness was made by Stmauer (1951) within the purview of psychoanalytic theory v ,v.'- .' ':....L - - .. _ .c' o.- - ..- .I 'f 4.. ‘0‘.- . .“’>. a, I-‘I ..,... .... . . . . .., R., v“:- . a On. - - . q a. . l ' ‘ph- . N 5,. .— ' u... -,. .- _ - .- “ mu,‘ . .. . I ‘ v e . . r 'h- -- ..M ‘Q — 45 relating to intrapsychic or fantasy aspects of behavior. &me Of the overt attitudes toward the blind as suggested kw the author are: child like curiosity without restraint, general fear to look at strange sights, and apprehension cfi‘loss of one's own identity for having a feeling of oneness with the blind person. Threat to the bodily integrity and loss of identity were also found to be present in peer group attitudes toward the amputee child resulting in greater rejection of the amputee by his classmates (Centers and Centers, 1963). An investigation of uniformity and cultural varia- bility of preference rankings of pictures of different kinds of physical deviation revealed: . . . remarkable uniformity in the hierarchy of preference which the children exhibited for pictured children with or without various visible physical handicaps (Richardson, et al., 1961, p. 246). It may be noted that the samples of the study included b0th disabled and nondisabled of various ethnic and social class groupings. Some sex variations were also found, in 'Hmat the girls tended to deprecate children with more '%“3cial" impairments than the boys who seemed to have g1‘eater concern for "functional" impairments. An elaboration of this investigation by Goodman, SE El;_(l963) concerned itself with the question of acquisi- tion of the value pattern noted in the above mentioned 46 study (Richardson, et al., 1961). The children and adults selected for the study came from subcultures with differ— ent value organizations about visible impairments. These groups were Jewish and Italian (because of hypothesized variant values for facial characteristics and body weight), and-retarded and emotionally disturbed (because of hypo- thesized inadequate or distorted internalization Of social norms). The data indicated that adults showed the same preference pattern as the dominant children's pattern, whereas the Jewish children gave higher ranking to both facially disfigured and obese than others. In addition, both retarded and disturbed children exhibited deviant patterns. The authors suggest that differential response patterns are acquired largely in the absence of contact with disabled persons, and have an implicit character c0mmunicated from parents to children without explicit r'ules or awareness. The cultural values with respect to disability, thus, appear to be related to cultural uniformity. A number of social-psychological variables were found t“) be at the root of attitudes of non-handicapped persons t(Ward the orthopedically handicapped (Nash, 1962). The Subjects who manifested favorable attitudes most, as a grOup, were younger, currently married, and of higher edu- cational level. 47 Using some standard psychological tests (such as ATDP, MMPI, and others), Siller (1964) found a general con- firmation of the view that the acceptance of the disabled is related to a positive self-image and stable object relationships on the part of non-handicapped persons. Preferences for different types Of physical disability rmve been investigated by many researchers. Insofar as lneference for teaching particular groups over others is cmncerned, it was shown that the gifted were most preferred, mentally retarded and emotionally disturbed children were least preferred, whereas physically handicapped children were placed in the middle (Badt, 1957; Dickstein and Dripps, 1958; Kvaraceus, 1956; Murphy, 1960). But, generally, there yum a tendency to prefer to work with those best known to the subjects. Force (1956), and Haring, et a1. (1958) cmserved that cerebral palsied children are most difficult ‘Ubinteract with. Force has hypothesized an acceptance— Iejection continuum based on "visibility," that is, obvious- ness of the impairment. When attitudes of different socio-economic groups inward blindness were compared with other physical disa- lfllities, blindness was overwhelmingly selected as the least Eupferred disability (Gowman, 1957). Other physical disa— btUties in order of perceived seriousness were leg amputa- thnn deafness, arm amputation, and severe facial burns. Ion- n-av» I .. R‘Ar \- 4“... . .-.v a A a - .'l “I- x ’ -'-b-v-~ ea-.. . I ‘dn'rba p.‘ 7 \ ' V ..~.,..... d. b. . . 9 . --. . g p, '_f '2. r: —......‘ u“... L I O~.~.' ““ . '81... ‘g v ' , .v. . ‘ a 1 0 .\ " ‘R‘;-. . ....._‘_"“ . n}. u“ v' . ‘u‘v. -~“ . .- n . ~ .Q n. x 2! ‘ "' a ‘ . OP . ”“1 :H ‘ cu.-. . . -P~ r -.‘ .. ~ “‘5 ‘ 5" ".- " ‘- a _ ‘ - .~._.J 5““ .1 ' ‘c, i I l. ."n. . 1 -,~ "I F\ b "Is. 5‘ ‘ . § r- V-.‘fi- . ‘- .u; ._ , ‘ 5 ‘.I ’u . ,. .._ -‘\.. \ -‘ : ~.-. v- .- "- .- U 1 .._ . s n. “. 2‘. ‘ a fi ‘.‘\ s. l» deg, .. ;_ .‘un .. a p I I. - Q ‘ ," a .‘ ‘. .fin" v \ 48 Unlike the fortuitous theoretical considerations evident in most previous studies on attitudes toward the physically disabled, recently the researchers have attempted uneXplore such attitudes in terms of basic psychOlOgical mxmepts of "prejudice" and "ethocentrism." It may be emsumed that physically handicapped persons represent an ~~ I.» \ ‘M a-” I Q‘ "1: e.,. § .p “ g in a"" I. ~‘. ~' . aa‘ \ J \ t ~ ~ ‘ \ 1‘ A, A- .‘v“ . .‘. Q‘ . . ‘-‘\ ‘» 5 Fh v~ n..- ‘- 52 ofnmdern clinical and counseling psychologists in examining vahmasystems of their clients as well as of those closely amsociated with them (e.g., Lowe, 1959; Samler, 1960; hulliamson, 1958). The measurement of values has been central to a \mriety of cross-cultural studies (Morris, 1956; Watts, 1962), studies of individual differences (Allport, Vernon, and Lindzey, 1951), societal characteristics (Morris, 1956), aspects of counseling (Rogers, 1951; Stefflre, 1958; Super, 1961), interpersonal relationships (Gardner and Thompson, 1963), and the impact of education (Jacob, 1957). Gordon Allport (1951, 1955, 1958) has been one of the most vocal advocates of the study of values. Prejudice and negative stereotypes are most intimately related to values. In his book, The Nature of Prejudice, Allport (1958) states that "the most important categories a man has are Ins own personal set of values" (p. 2“). He further asserts: Man has a propensity to prejudice. This prOpensity lies in his normal and natural tendency to form generalizations, concepts, categories, whose content represents an oversimplification of his world of eXperience (Allport, 1958, p. 26). And Allport believes that "one type of categorization that Iflfidisposes us to make unwarranted prejudgments is our personal values" (p. 27). Perhaps, the utility of the value concept is a function Ofijs close ties with behavioral and motivational aspects of Wndous theories of both society and personality. For :- nh o . r” r” . . a. J . 1. .\y o. e. .. 3: r” Tn L. "I. J. L. . . ru .. a: .3 ”a n. n. a. u . g. s. 1 . z . .a. .1. .u ",1 a a. . 3 C. a . r” . . ... . I . .3 .1 .3 Z. c . 3 :t n “ rum W.“ mu” 1 . ".1. Sm . u Cy r: ,. ~ ‘ . Q h. J ‘ A: Q. »: V“ s a h r.. a. n: . A; a. r. 1. .7. .4. 7. .. c. .v i».. .... u... 1‘ a. . AV ‘0 n :u A-- r. .l .v I A . fi u .Iu - N s . asu “. fly ~x~ G v I. \ ~fi‘ ’- u. .. A... r“ .-u . .' .1 .m :» 4v Av no . . . ran 3 ¢ ruv F a u ..v. up‘ :— 0. u." . . u .a n. '. J. . . «1.. ..v.. c 3.. .u. .«d L Q. A. 6. Es pn- a p40 “a .h» .. & n.. :u u :- - § . . p—n Ni A U . :u a u u 3 o . u. 3 . 9‘ .: v» ~ 3. u. ‘ :- slv . s i. .. . n... .. . . .mv .u. 5.: u a t a . . . s ;. ..a u.” .... .... .5. 1. i. p... .5. . c .. .. . . . . n .V. . Q Q. . . t ‘ ~\\ . . . . . . v... ..;. .. . ha ;.. L. . . . . s... -\ I. k. —‘ 53 emmmle, values are central to Talcott Parsons' "theory cfi‘action" in which "value-orientations" are seen as the bans of "attitudes" taken toward various social objects (Parsons and Shils, 1951). Recent theoretical models of attitude formation and change have attempted to incorporate values rather system— atically. Katz assumes in his theory that attitudes are related to a given value system manifesting a vglgg— expressive function. . . in which the individual derives satisfactions from expressing attitudes appropriate to his personal values and to his concept of himself. This function is central to doctrines of ego psychology which stress the importance of self-expression, self—develOpment, and self-realization (Katz, 1960, p- 173). Katz (1960) has also suggested that it is easier to change isolated attitudes, whereas an attitude that is closely related to a value system is highly resistant to change. According to Rosenberg (1956, 1960), an instrumental Islationship between attitudes and values exists. He demon- strated that stable positive attitudes were perceived as instrumental to positive value attainment and the blocking Of'negative values, whereas stable negative values were perceived as instrumental to negative value attainment and the blocking of positive values. This is also illustrated in the fact that moderate attitudes as compared to intense ones were related to less important values. W ‘ -']< Or“°'__ gr. nah Unto-y : C H ' l v" ‘.I;y|7: I... r {1‘ no -uc-~hub Ucv cu - 'P .lr~ Jrq'vy’ ‘ '1... .' .ng.‘ K...” In» .. '\ A 'OHV' vn.~.-v ‘. .o .. OIV '- r I \. ’ o . . .. ‘ ‘ : ‘ I . ‘-: .P 5‘" Frvu sn. uvh c..'.. v» " o I' ‘ 1“ n. c» “:W‘ 2. . ‘ a "‘ ‘vl--|\ .‘ *F ‘ .. . “a F“ '— v’.~clt 6.5“. V. .- "'~. ‘ . C': ”:31 ‘r tr 0. ‘h- ‘.‘ ... .mo...‘ ' p ~-...:. a“; sung, n.» 1-. -‘ H a. H‘ ‘ I 'it':’. ‘ \...‘:1 «:2 ‘.‘~ " '— c ‘ ‘ .__ 41:“ gr; ‘ . . A ‘- ‘. > ‘ «,fitfi ‘ I u‘l ‘ '1‘ “1"! my.“ ‘ A‘ ‘ .~ in I 72:... ‘I A“. u ~ "Vv ‘s P _ K‘ - V. .._ ‘ ‘x « u . , n \‘a" I . NA. fir“: .. ". v . ' ‘.' n\:‘ ‘~,~ “‘:; § -. . .‘V'm. “‘ -"q .‘ ‘ x ‘s. § ’ v .H: II .0 v ‘p, ‘ Q 'A “‘ . . '*-\“'a. .\\ Al. . Q ‘ . ‘-,_ ‘- ‘ n I . R v,- . [_ - u g. ‘ ‘4_ H- " ' -- L :- . “I 3 . ' ' I ‘ § I, 1 " 4' v- - 5“ In his "three-process theory," Kelman (1961) considers "internalization" as the most developed attitudinal system in which the individual absorbs the attitude as part of his value system.' Since there are many other theoretical models (e.g. Festinger, 1957; Heider, 1958; Newcomb, 1963; Osgood and Tannenbaum, 1955) presenting structural and functional 6 analyses which obviously include values, no attempt will be made to reView this aspect of the literature in the present report. However, some of the major classificatory schemes used in the study of values will be presented here, for they are directly related to the present research. Classification Of Values (Various classificatory systems have been provided by researchers of different disciplines. Philosophers have classified values in such broad and vague categories as higher and lower, mental and physical, permanent and trans— ient, intrinsic and extrinsic, instrumental and inherent. A sociologist has suggested that there are "dominant," "variant" (choice-values),and "deviant" (rejected or anti- social) values (Kluckhohn, 1953). Perhaps, the most comprehensive psychological classi- fication of values was proposed by Spranger (1928). In his classification, human beings are categorized into six basic value types: (a) theoretical, (b) economic, (c) esthetic, (d) political, (e) social, and (f) religious. '.,.:::...:N. S. ' A ,.‘,::....*‘v.vu '5‘;n Anv-q‘ “.va ‘vno~- "’ ‘ I (‘1 A ::: r. or - v.‘~~‘h-~ ‘ 1 V I I - ‘. "V‘ 'Y" q'n . -\ n 0-4-~ .u .4gvtn ... .y-o‘ . I ' ,A r_n "On our“... - '- :: “:99“ +» "‘ ohgw. VV .. . . ':”:.~ m- M. ,. ~"““ ~' Q.. a U , "tn. ‘ : . I". '5 Hrv-b 4 ....--_,_ "may “”fi1= I‘— 'I'..-~ \‘J ‘I- we .IIA ~ -.~::\'hg fir-r“. Q .l s 'h -. .I::Q“IF‘1 .-.~..-~ ,. V.“ e . A \ F I 'l..~:‘ rt. ‘.‘. 'In. , . . ‘1. 2:." , ~,~“ v, [‘1'- ' yv ~- V 'V-A. w" I": a "r . 'VA..;;EI - “ '..._ I.‘:; ~r."~ ‘\‘- . 'A .'. _ ‘t. ~“i‘. “'- ~C'D “\-..., A v Q'- ‘uti‘l‘ ' h.‘..v-,:‘" . ‘~: ‘ 7 «‘4- 55 The claSsic instrument for measuring values developed by Allport, Vernon, and Lindzey (1951) is based on Spranger's classificationgsystem. After considering the various usage of values, Morris (1956) claSsified them into three categories: (a) operative values in which preference for one kind of object rather than another is expressed by living beings, (b) conceived ~values refer to those preferential behaviors which are directed by an anticipation of the consequence, and (c) _§jeCt values where the emphasis is upon the objects which determine what is preferable for the individual. Morris (1956) has used his well-known instrument, Ways to Live to measure "conceived values." Further attempts to meaSure conceived values have been made recently by Gardner and Thompson (1963), who label these as "ought to" or "prescription" values. Other interesting classification systems have been proposed by counseling psychologists in the field of voca— tional counseling (Stefflre, 1958; Super, 1961). The seven values measured by Stefflre's Vocational Values Inventory are altruism, control, job freedom, money, prestige, security, and self-realization. In a recent work, A Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: Affective Domain (Krathwohl, et al., 1964), attempts have been made to provide a conceptual framework for classifying affective variables including values in education. g 3.. .ol‘O. 1’" a, ,. 3': .0 u: “a: .u-m u; "A‘V‘u, " ‘ Fr“ -vav H...‘ .‘A -\..,1 ' , . "*H’r‘vr‘ r. “v. V‘A'~Q.‘ ._-...A vfl.':’f {dc—av, at 1'..‘ - '5 fur": “‘4 ugh.-. c“ Hm”. V "‘V:t .L, - fl ”:5; , "' P s.‘ I... V ‘. A, 'v \ ' C~ ‘ ‘H. A 'V Q“! 5 ('4 us_ “I . 9A \ \ '~ I -2 . v“. a a“ 1. - ‘.“: ‘WrV—v—‘w—"— _ —‘__—»— ‘h‘ 56 Clyde Kluckhohn (1951), in his comprehensive classi- fication, conceives of values as having several dimensions IMke modality, content, generality, specificity, intensity, emd SO forth. Catton (1959) also refers to several dimen- sions of values, for example, distance of the object-—spacial, social, and temporal. A worthwhile conceptual framework has been suggested by Dumbo, Leviton, and Wright (1956); and Wright (1960) in respect to dominant value characteristics for studying value systems of those associated with handicapped persons. Values cmlbe clustered according to whether they are derived from U” comparisons or from (b) intrinsic assets. Wright elaborates: ' If the evaluation is based on comparison with a standard, the person is said to be invoking com— parative values. . . .:On the other hand,.if the evaluation arises from the qualities inherent in the object of judgment itself, the person is said to be invoking asset values.- What matters is the object of judgment in a setting that has its own . intrinsic purposes and demands. The person's ' reaction is then based upon how appropriately the situational demands are fulfilled rather than on comparison with a predetermined standard (Wright, 1960, p. 29). It is recognized that there are many situations in Ihfe, such as job selection, where comparative valuing inhavior may be inescapable. Nevertheless, a humanistic \dew of life based on intrinsic asset values would direct armrson to evaluate the disabled for his unique character- istics as a human being. Care for the disabled, elderly, all 'n'Edr. a n ‘- ':' .g. .a- n-" ‘ 0“ ...‘.|““‘A’ Q‘ : ‘ u L. bind-nova. - . . . 1 “I" rA‘R‘v-w F E .- ,_,:. avail-1.5 on- _ . u u..!;: ‘A'n.;”3 u..1i»~ aid at. n...,.‘..‘ ‘,.:'. +r . ._ ‘ ""“~- inn-An: e”. 2": no u... ,_ u‘-, ~._Lv~,:,_'“‘ I'ayd“: fifl‘ .. ,. § U‘J“': . Q . a ‘ I" . ‘Qh'c‘\ r» N» I” v g," n \ fly. 0 “ v“ r.. ‘u‘ ‘. ‘r- we 57 poor and weak appear to be the direct expression of asset values. In historical perspective, an important criterion of a civilized society has been the achievement of the above goals, regardless of the differences in socioeconomic and political systems. Thus, a reasonable assumption that can be made on the basis of asset-value framework, is that those holding higher asset values would have more favorable attitudes toward the physically handicapped and emotionally disturbed than those expressing comparative valuing behavior. Personalicontact Many suggestions have been made in regard to the im- portance of personal contact in changing attitudes and reducing prejudice. 'Individuals have been found to modify their neutral or negative attitudes in a positive direction as a consequence of contact with members of a given group over a period of time. This fact is demonstrated in educa- tion, religion, the armed forces, and cultural exchange programs. An experiment in modifying attitudes toward the Negro by Smith (19N3) showed that a group of graduate students changed their attitudes in a favorable direction as a result of personal contact with various aspects of Harlem life. A large-scale research by Williams (196“) and his colleagues on four communities found that work situation provides the most favorable environment for the Negroes and p 1' { - o we!" r- . ' - v ’ I vglljil" I Q Q " \ luv: ,- ,- . ':"", ‘11- A c ..,...‘.. ‘r , .‘ ‘ u .‘ A a... I“ H" ‘ '9 Q. . .. \ F7 .- .~ “: - ' "nn .3. ‘ ‘ u .0 .“~‘-' v. n . ' .~ ‘59. ’ . ‘ F .n. ..-D~.. a... v u... . a “:7 "v.‘~- d.‘ ny.‘ ‘~ ..' ‘ fl ‘ . v.-"v ‘v rr 4D I I :v (N I. 58 whites to interact positively. Similar results have been obtained in other studies on attitudes toward Negroes (Brophy, 1946; Deutsch and Collins, 1951; Harding and Hogrefe, 1952). Allport (1958) has presented a brilliant exposition of-various kinds of intergroup contact (pp. 250—268). He observes that more favorable attitudes are created when there is an "equal status contact," and when the contact is in pursuit of common goals. Also, those having contact with high status or high occupational group Negroes held more favorable attitudes than those having contact with lower status Negroes (pp. 254, 261-262). Close personal contact with an equal-status member of the minority group in question was also found by Watson (1950) to be a stimu- lant for more favorable attitudes. However, Jacobson and his associates (1960, pp. 210- 213) point out that equal status contacts are more likely to result in unfavorable attitudes if one group does not fully accept the equality of the other. Contact on the basis of status equality promises to be a very important dimension via which attitudes of parents can be interpreted toward emotional disturbance and physi- cal handicap. According to Zetterberg (1963), two other variables seem to be involved in the intergroup contact: "cost of avoiding interaction" and "availability of alternative u‘..l' ..... V'- L... ~-; . . .‘ '0 .O‘Iv.‘ ‘a.. ‘ I \ o ' n v 59 rewards." The author explains: if the costs of avoiding interaction are low, and if there are available alternative sources of reward, the more frequent the interaction, the greater the mutual liking (Zetterberg, 1963, p. 13). In summary, frequent contact with a person or group is likely to lead to more favorable attitudes if: (a) the contact is between status equals in pursuit of common 'goals (Allport, 1958, p. 267), (b) the contact is perceived as instrumental to the realization of a desired goal value (Rosenberg, 1960, p. 521), (c) contact is with members of a higher status group (Allport, 1958, pp. 25A, 261-262), (d) the contact is among status equals and the basis of status is unquestioned (Jacobson, et al., 1960, pp. 210-213), '(e) the contact is volitional (Zetterberg, 1963, p. 13), and (f) the contact is selected over other rewards (Zetter— berg, p. 13). .Although the social psychological research, as noted above, indicate clearly the importance of personal contact as one of the most effective agents in themitigation of prejudice and development of positive attitudes, very few researchers have studied this aspect in the field of mental health. Only casual references have been made by some clinicians about the role of group therapy, especially with children, in diminishing prejudicial attitudes (Konopka, 19u7; Rosenbaum and Berger, 1960). . nlfiuv r*;r\ " .AntUAol-o . v 0 P ' _. ....'1Oevva a. .. u'LCOQUO'V, a ‘.'... 1..., .y,« zrb ~L." .144:- v. -.~.v «was we .. . :Ji::vvu 'Uu a L ~ I ‘ ~>v¢ ., 7"“ '.' l a' mu...) .u'u- ' .. . ' I; ‘lcbb‘..‘~~ ... :IUOU“:S \l . . . e r “‘FQV‘. v“.-. "2":3 6- .. .o-u...~-' ' U I-: :.‘Hfl“‘h“ a. nu.".‘ 2‘, ""~ .- 4, 'H.‘ *- soc.:..E: “ U ‘ ~\_ ”a .\ , "a A‘. U V U“ h .e -.. \q ‘ a ._ _ ’~ ‘.‘ \~ ‘V‘& 0' f‘ V 6O Attitude Intensity Intensity of attitudes, which refers to the strength of cognitive, affective and motivational aspects, is partic- ularly important to the problem of measurement. It has been suggested that the degree to which an individual is per- sonally involved in an issue will determine the strength of his attitudes (Sherif, Sherif, and Nebergall, 1965, pp. 91, 1142). In other words, this dimension of attitudes deals with the motivational and emotional aspects of the problem. Intensity is not only an important component of atti- tude structure, but it is also considered as an action predictor (Rosenberg, 1960, p. 336). Because of motivational and emotional involvements, intense attitudes have been found to be highly resistant to change (Carlson, 1956, p. 259). Considering the question of relationships between attitudes and action, Rosenberg (1960) states that "the 'stronger' the attitude, the more likely it will be that the SubJect will take consistent action toward the attitude. obJect" (p. 336). Guttman and Foa (1951) have shown that intensity is related to amount of social contact with the attitude. object. Thus, intensity has been established as an important attitude COmponent, increasing predictability. It apparently varies With 90th related value intensity (Rosenberg, 1960) and w1th amount of contact (Foa, 1950; Guttman and Foa, 1951). . Q ~‘l ... u:~v‘\l" " v . u, ,u'vuv. . I 'I F ,I‘.qu’Ar a p.:¢¢ovu (V'e ' - "H‘O'Fv ' fl .L‘u, J‘iun-ouu ~ a >l: "'3":‘~' 'lI' .0..-l0~- i. '°"I‘ 1;: 25“... :-...;a. do. «I. .qu-‘u' ‘IIV-v‘vu ' 0 . "in 'l . “Lt‘fig' -:...'w 4..-. ‘. ' ‘an- - a u ‘ N.‘ L' ‘o U‘A. ‘l I '5 " Ml ' “y- d 1‘14..:..’ *'r . .‘.."l." .. _ I”: 'I'WSA“ ’N‘ u..'. J ., | . '5 ... IH‘V A F ‘v‘ ‘5 ""4 :5“ " '., :-:~- -.':—H’y ‘f‘ ‘9‘ "J 0 a ['\ b |~ c n...‘ cl! \1 ' .,- : u-~ P‘ n “~ A ' ‘e '- ‘I; "u." " v v o “‘J h. t.” U .K‘ ‘1. " ~ 61 Moreover, considerable research has been done to determine the "zero point" of a scale that discriminates the psychologically "true" positive from negative attitude direction (e.g., Edwards, 1957; Guttman, 19u7, 1950, 1954a, 195ub; Guttman and Suchman, 19u7; Suchman, 1950). Atti- tude intensity is regarded as an important component of attitude structure in determining the "zero point" of a scale. Locating a true zero—point appears to have the highly desirable characteristic of elimination of question bias, which often confounds cross—lingual studies. In other words, the location of a true zero-point on a scale HEKGS it possible to compare responses between different language groups without further concern for question word- ing (Guttman, 195Aa; Suchman and Guttman, 19A7). Another usefulness of intensity analysis is to re- chme error (Guttman and Suchman, 19U7; Foa, 1950) caused tWhabitual overstatement or understatement of feelings. Ikmever, Foa (1950) has shown that is is not usually necessary to compute this factor. Theoretical Considerations As mentioned before, the various studies of parental andPublic attitudes toward the emotionally disturbed and prW'Sically handicapped that have been executed hitherto, are "metly limited in scope and theoretical significance inas- "mfih as they are primarily concerned with the descriptive, aDplied, and practical aspects of the problem. The instru— Ments and techniques utilized in these investigations have ‘ r n‘r “ Q .;;r “A . V.” a,” 1"" .. .5 C :u-“9 . ”.‘uvb V. ' I a “V ..,. ..: -r\ d ",_..4.-v.eu - ”V '-" -v" G. .. '3‘- ., - - Pa "VT“. Olav JAL~ v ‘ Q "”32“ ‘rgnn; No.0vo in.» V. . an --.—t.' - A "i ' "Oo-u.v-y“ . ,. u. ‘I- . A 1.. J' o... -’ ’) ‘/ tl) ‘ I (I) (f s .1. ~. r A “o: “QQ . «v \— I |.o ‘ . .. ‘ 5'5“ :y; m.‘ n. V. ‘ ._ . u A ~ D'P‘JR‘ U I. s m p: n . J -T 'w ““b n 9 Q r '1. O: I .. . A. v .f‘q n ‘2': "~‘l ‘r U U 3. _. .':‘:‘l ‘ “6‘ . . b” ‘. u \ U. Q): R “ ‘-‘l:‘\~‘ n v- ‘ ‘I 62 tmen rather specific to the particular situation. In the amence of a sound, comprehensive theoretical base, the uwmlusions are frequently lacking in wider applicability, gynerality, and theoretical relevance. The urgent need for research studies generated by broader theoretical bases, especially in the field of re- habilitation have been emphasized by Levine (1961), Meyer- son (l9u8, 1963), and Wright (1960). A trenchant criticism cfi‘the problem has come from O'Connor and Goldberg (1959) who state that most studies in this area are characterized by ". . . isolation without relationship to theories and findings of other studies" (p. A87). ‘They further accuse: There is a tendency to neglect theoretical research and to concentrate on immediate practical problems. Too often the findings are too inconclusive to war- rant wide application; seldom are they repeated and related to each other (O'Connor and Goldberg, 1959, p. N87). LSYChoanalytic Model The research pursuits in the field of clinical psy- Chology and child psychopathology emit a distinct psycho- amilytic flavor.l But the increasing evidence accruing frmnsocial psych°1°3y’ anthropology, and sociology regarding mueimportance of environmental influences on human behavior asopposed to psychobiological dominance has rendered the theor'Etical model of psychoanalysis controvertible and equivocal. \ 1 tow See the review of research on Parental Attitudes ard Emotionally Disturbed Persons. - ' nfi:"’ - . #IQ. \ : q ..~r-n: ” .— ~.§.- we . a...“ ...-v. F, '\ ‘ v - ,..,.... v. :rv‘v . .‘. :foc g‘ . n . ..v .o-o‘n vow . . ’“P _.,.,.: h" .- , - n u . . .... ~,,..e.-: ...~I‘ “Va-4-.. U . "'"'.‘.'; 9“,... r --.-'--I .. v... u . 0' Q 5‘ ""‘v~-r~ .- . , " ‘ _ . ~"u-..--§-- . ‘ .' .‘yoo Q.- . a H;\_; ‘- .4-.v ._~“‘ - A!» \——"‘ -" ~~vo>n ‘ra. . ’- A;f 0..., v - I. u- a. \ r, \ '5 o. ..v ~‘v ‘.1 e - 'N _ -\ Q.._ \P‘; VV‘ ‘*-...~ » ‘ 'Ie‘, .- ‘ " I‘— § ' 1-- 2, “~— 1 . - ‘u. ‘5;-._ “c \—."t. ~‘o.' \ ~-' ' ‘Lu. '-._ 1..»w‘ ‘- h a og-H\ " . .‘ h21 o. ‘ A. ; ‘ ‘~. P‘ s ‘I r. 1... . 3.- ‘s. ‘- \ d— 's v . . ' 3 . I a, ’ . 4" -‘ 4 ,v ;~:. 1 . .‘ ‘1 “ \ KEN ”i S ~I~ y‘“ ’ ‘ ‘3’... , -'- q .“l . l‘. \: \"".“ '-. “O 63 Frank (1965) has presented an extensive review of forty years of research on the role of family in the devel- opment of psychopathology "without being able to feel that we are any closer to an answer than was Freud" (p. 201). Parental characteristics of "overprotectiveness," "rejec— tion," "domination," and "deprivation and frustration" derived from psychoanalysis have not been found to be responsible for pathogenic behavior in children, according to many research reports. Frank remarks: It seems apparent that the major conclusion that can be drawn from these data is that there is no such thing as a schizophrenic or a neuroto— genic mother or family. At least these data do not permit of the description of a particular con- stellation of psychological events within the home and, in particular, between mother and child that can be isolated as a unique factor in the development of one or the other kind of personality disorder (Frank, 1965, p. 198). Socialepsychological,Approach The social-psychological approach to mental health and physical disability is the major theoretical orientation of the present study. More specifically, this orientation has close resemblance to the field theoretical paradigm of interpersonal relationship as developed by Lewin (1936). The field theoretical approach has been used by Barker, gt El; (1953); Dumbo, et a1. (1956); Meyerson (1948, 1963); and Wright (1960) in studying attitudes toward physical dis- abilities. Barker and co-workers (1953) have conceptualized the position of the physically handicapped in the modern western A 3“..- PP V‘— t.- .- ' ;~ ~- .vv"'¢ : fl.- .,~,- H’.- A ‘- u— ..,;--~ .1- l .‘ 7‘" _ "F-'l“ A ~ .,.:-...~-’ W' .. t F- .. .pA ‘?' a." ‘...d-v~-4.“ -- v -- -.-. p .. ,1. h ’ ~— 1.’ ‘ '-V~‘ . - . .. ,,...- ’1“ . \ A “""~-vufi‘- . - " ~- ' . \ ' ..-—-VA~ a.-. ~-._ «r- — .- .— ~._,_-: ':"; A-‘18'Yr-u‘ . -.‘. :..’...Vfi‘-l " o. s 7"“ ‘~-?...,~ a: ‘- ICH‘ ..,‘_'~ .' -.“.;‘ S‘- I v, V a c \ : "FA ”fl . ‘ I ~ ‘(W—w .1- d I I‘\ a}, - b 'u :- '-‘1‘U:‘ ’ a 'u-‘ \‘l‘ "A L u. 'i 'J b;‘ ‘- . .’-_.‘_ . 'U - ‘ “'N. r n . g- , I . I 1‘- . ‘g 31"] I ' J I ‘n ' s ,. fl‘_u ,rg ‘.' .‘uvr‘; I o \v ‘N . “a. 4 v4.- - “.‘ ‘ ‘I ris- - .. ~ki an“ . . ‘. v 0-- Q '..: J ”.5 2‘. F ‘b i A .,‘_ We “.— V. .5 a a.» yvfi“ '. g -‘_ l: l I! i. 64 society as being characterized by three significant psycho- dynamic factors: (a) it is underpriviledged, (b) it is marginal, and (c) it involves exposure to more frequent psychological situations. As a matter of fact, there seems to be a close resemblance between the emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped on the one hand, and racial and religious minorities, on the other (e.gu Chesler, 1965; Pundel, 1960; Himes, 1960). g Psychiatrists and clinical psychologists appear to rave acknowledged the role of social and ecological factors in mental health (e.g., Caplan, 1964; Cobb, et al., 1963; Ikmning, et al., 1964; Meerloo, 1959). In a paper presented m:the 73rd Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,.B1ackman and his colleagues (1965) have ven— tured to build a "community mental health theory" based on sociometric approaches. In this theory: it is postulated that a crucial aspect of the environment involves the interaction of an individ- ual with the members of his community. One impor— tant type of interaction is with the people to whom the individual feels close and with whom he has regular, face-to—face contact. . . . One function of the interaction of these individuals is the exchange of emotional support and services (Blackman, et al., 1965, p. 191). The above mentioned theoretical framework, thus, brings out the central constructs of self, other, reference group, social role, attitude, and value. These constructs may be subsumed under a more general, but basic dimension of "interpersonal relationship." Within this framework, .AI ’l n: a ‘ Vnofiv \. a - ":-:.: "a: . .u-u—oh-voiv .- :n. Indrtz‘rlr: la. n. ~.-OO'--OI- ‘ u '9' ..,c“:q .y E '— -'-v.ov.‘.-~' ‘ v .. u,‘...,; “h“; ‘ a u, ‘ ' ""c‘v‘ “‘4“. ~- u'.“ "‘0 ' e ‘ 1 F: _ y. . ..-,. .».,JA‘” .. ‘ _ .- --.' ~“va I 0....1. ‘! “"2. ng neg. ." .. 1 ~ ‘ 5:: ’7' P: :V‘fl; . I ~“"“v~ ~: Jr (r: “7“ ’I D .0. a..‘ ““ . .. ~ \."":a.. h .g..."-:.’ .,_ . ~ . . ._ "H .-.'~ fl ‘.‘A' r ..; ,. “H ‘ul~.' A ‘ is v- I’- '1. . " A “v- ... v. 2"“ ..~" . .3, 1‘ . I G 1" . fie. P. 'I ‘V' ~ . s.’ I F ‘l ‘M h ,. ""v ”V1 . ‘.. e‘ 3. ~ ‘~ '~. 0" ~ ~ : n" I.. a. a“ ‘ A ' o 5‘ g .' ~r‘ Q V‘.' ‘ ‘. U ~.‘ .3 o ",R “l “0.. ‘ 3.. ‘» a“ ',-‘.s ‘ ' K‘ ‘V “t ““' ’I . :Q‘ a u \‘_ Q.: , 65 mental illness and physical handicap may be considered a social value judgment rather than some objective phenomena in themselves. Certain roles in society have high value for maintaining the contemporary social system; and peOple are generally esteemed according to how they are perceived to valued social roles. Hence, attitudes toward disability should vary according to the kinds of social roles per- ceived to be important to the individual, or collectively to the society (Goodman, et al., 1963; Richardson, et al., 1961). More recent approaches to social-psychological pheno- mena by balance theorists might also provide a new perspec- tive in the understanding of the problem under investigation. In particular, Festinger's (1957) theory of cognitive dis- sonance would suggest that attitudes that are dissonant to a value orientation would tend to be abandoned, whereas consonant attitudes would be maintained (see also Rosenberg, 1960). In consonance with the social-psychological orienta- tion, it may be construed that actual contact with others is an important determinant of attitudinal evaluations of them (e.g., Allport, 1958; Homans, 1950). That is, the nwre frequent the contact between persons or groups, the more favorable is the attitude. However, frequency of contact is not related to evaluation in any simple sense. Contact frequency has been observed to be related directly q .__r.:.,, -o to ’V . .tu:o"' V. ‘ . -" "‘ eke" -- a”; r- "‘l ,,'unn‘.. . ._.',.o:pr P“ ‘r ::.‘:u-Uhu U. «~- ' . a r '- .,. . “A ‘:' a ;\ \ c ace-.vu bdvoo "' "'91‘5—‘7-1 ‘ 3 s. ' n I f m. n".-‘U--‘ .. no. .- ‘ . -‘R _r~ ‘. M" ' ...o.;, .- AF- '5‘ ‘u I”. v. “:‘v- "V--‘ .\_ u. ."".'. .. ' r“~"7‘ I- ~ .' "¢.‘.:. .— .. . k.-- --.. ‘ "‘ C- "w- ‘ lyn' pr fig}.‘.'.‘- 'v-,.' - W’Yr--v_‘ v... ,_, ‘- v...,.‘ .. 66 to intensity of attitude (Guttman and Foa, 1951; Foa, 1958). Whether it is also related directly to a positive evaluation of the person seems to depend on intermediate variables such as the social status of the persons con- tacted, the absence of coercion in the interaction, and the availability of alternate reinforcing behaviors (e.g., Zetterberg, 1963). We might conclude our theoretical discussion with a note of caution from Murray and Kluckhohn who stated tersely: A human being does not grow up in vacuum: His development is determined not only by the physical environment as the biologist proved, and by family environment as Freud proved, but, as the massive data collected by the cultural anthropologists showed by the larger societal and cultural institu- tions that are extolled, preached, and practiced not only by parent "carriers" but by the leading minority (authority figures), if not by the majority, of the group in which the individual is reared (Murray and Kluckhohn, 1953, p. 4). Attitude Organization and Measurement of Attitudes Attitude Organization The nature of attitudes has been conceived diversely by different authors. A frequently quoted definition is that of Gordon Allport (1935). He defines: An attitude is a mental and neural state of readiness, organized through experience, exert— ing a directive or dynamic influence upon the individual's response to all objects and situa— tiogs with which it is related (Allport, 1935, p. 10 . J.“p. nrwr-V ,.. ‘ A4 ‘ utth _. .e...‘ ‘1‘! . u‘vd"-‘..§ ,1.....,.,., v. .. .ccnvliv, -I ' I- - ... e.,, ”s. . .. -— 4-1 haven ‘vo‘- ‘ u .......;_H "r n s A h "“"‘-v-- u... “CL" ’P "no-«w -...4 .H 4"“. ‘» N . _ ‘ I-Q ‘ - “ 2 "I... “' “' —- 4'. K ‘ " n . ._ '- "ra v- .. ~..,_‘: u. o A. .. -':,: . N. I .' g. '..'-“Y s) v v..' w.'_.: h""' "';v... '-.‘_‘ c. Q‘.' ‘ ‘c. ‘1‘ .. ~4 "t; In. . ufiu. -PC“; .._- 1‘ '1", ‘n‘ fith-N' '-. c- 1 -. . ‘..‘~".F .‘ ~ ~_ . ‘V-_ E .' Ad, . ~: 1‘ ‘n. ' . a. ‘ v ~' L. . ‘I ‘. '- n -.i ~ ~. ._. A o h lu~ p . ~. Kr, c u‘ r.‘ I- ‘1 .- h g vs ‘ " “r- *v I. ‘ ' a " ’9, «A ‘ \ -: Hv ~~ .. ’ ) p.»- k‘u .. i - . -K; \ ‘1‘”v‘. ...'-,"V V“ y. \ A 1‘. . ~‘ ~_->l‘. “ -F -.. 67 Recent approaches focus on affective, cognitive, and be- havioral components of attitudes (Secord and Backman, 1965). Notwithstanding the various explanatory definitions of attitude, the definition formulated by Guttman (1950) has been utilized in the present research inasmuch as the statistical analyses of the attitudinal responses will be based on Guttman's approach. Guttman (1950) defines atti- tude as "a delimited totality of behavior with respect to something" (p. 51). This would include belief (cognitive component), overt action (behavioral component), and implicitly, evaluation and intensity (affective component). According to Guttman (1950), responses on an attitude scale are one form of delimited behavior; but many kinds of behavior which are more or less intercorrelated forming separate subuniverses may be embodied in an attitude uni- verse. Therefore, an adequate attitude abstraction from this universe should include sampling from each of the possible subuniverses, a task of uncertain empirical possi- bility. A limited sampling of behavior, on the other hand, would render the inferences quite restricted in range and scope. We proceed to measure attitudes on the assumption that a relationship exists between the statements made about a social object (for example, emotionally disturbed persons), and overt behavior toward that object. But this relationship needs to be substantiated by adequate empirical research. “,. ”rag" ‘0‘: ‘,,u.. .5 '.».¢~.rlr ‘f‘ n ‘ My ,'_‘_:.-e..uu-:r -ap . '2 rr (.>-_. ' ' "5‘ r .4 '-‘ 3 .. J- ..-~»A,l:‘ are; ..::-u-‘- V9. — r5 (1. , - o. _r ‘ ~Fnr "~,. on have. n..-_.~ """I‘r~»~ J - ‘ ‘~\ . °"~kv-.~.~, “ l "H; nus . &.ue.-'-..: U “ c ‘ 'i‘lrgn ru 1 “‘-‘Ov‘ ‘_ q C." 4., "‘v¥o., - ' F r...,_ “Fcr‘rj‘; ,"""-"---- "‘ "sI‘ ~ .g‘a . a: 1‘ ' ‘fv‘V‘J- ‘ .5; ., " .~,, ‘ ‘r-<: . ,. ‘ on.-v ‘: .,.‘ -\. .._. I‘ J * e :- 6.._‘_I ..". h—I d“ 1" r.‘u ‘I - ."O o. e‘ 5. n- ‘ ‘-“ ) 4‘, F‘ A ‘. .. .... ‘ ‘_ 4 F: .“ Ct. ‘2“6 e-,; ‘ _ hr A , u... g " . .k ‘r: 0' v..- -75.,“ ‘~' _. .¥ . 5... “‘ ~~-:V". ~ ‘ -. [‘0‘ ‘v V. 7. ‘~ .7 V- ‘~~ I r .o‘v‘ n,— V‘.. ‘-."‘ 5-, ‘ .~v~, ~‘4 Nh. - Q -‘_ ‘ .\_ ."4- . at: «. . V- V kg s" a“ ' oP~ \‘ ‘ V‘O "__ v. —~. .w .t '.l . . . . "-kn .. ‘vtx Ii \- . . U 68 The underlying characteristics of attitudes and their relationship to other variables have been analyzed by Green (1954, pp. 335-336). Consistency of responses with respect to a social object is the most important characteristic. Secondly, the attitude itself is an abstraction from a set of consistent, or covarying, reSponses. Green states: In each measurement method, Covariation among responses is related to the variation of an underlying variable. The latent attitude is defined by the correlations among responses (Green, 1954, p. 336). Hence, responses in themselves are not attitudes; rather, the attitude is defined by the latent variable. The detec— tion of this latent variable requires certain scale proper— ties. Attitude and related constructs, for example, value and belief, differ from other psychological variables, because they are always in terms of a referant class of social objects. Scalogram analysis employed by Guttman (1950, ch. 3), and other workers in the field is consistent with the above considerations of attitude organization. Measurement of attitudes in the present study is based on this approach. Attitude Scales The general framework under which the scale analysis is intended to be performed in this research, is derived prim- arily from the works of Guttman (1954a, 1954b) and his associates (Guttman and Foa, 1951; Guttman and Suchman, ‘ , Q - '- . r 3.2:." q" n c ur-n ., I _ , 1 A A F . ,.. . Uto--e-- V ‘ Q ”was - l - . . »;,-..'D. “ v 2 ‘ ‘. ‘i. I ' . ‘3 "Y‘ ‘ r n...,“- VA; “.1 I, § ‘ in. . R “ ~— . ‘ N” a ‘ “nuns. --~.... fl! \ ' ’ - . L""‘~ I-F‘ ‘ , ... - "‘ . ..-\..‘ ‘~ ‘4 h 'r .. .-..o “1"-- ~I . , a — a. , u- my..~ -., . "' >- -.-' ; ..~ -. ‘ "~ . 1‘- .‘ ." ' ‘p. ., _" ,. I. v» “| 4 'u— '5- ‘. ‘. v I .;~ . .‘ w L, _ "c h.- -’ ‘ ‘. r,‘ ‘ _ o. . I..‘ .- fl - a 4.. ~. . ‘.- - ~ ’7 ‘~- - p, ‘g. A N, ‘-‘ s. ‘ _a '- ". . h a. ~_-_ . "A“ "\‘ y‘- ‘\ 5.. ‘- ‘~ A. ._. ‘ A .— _. v p.’ ' . o "..k ‘r'~ o " § ‘ i. ‘u_ I.“ . A" “. V." 5 x ' n ‘ d x s x -'.-' ._ _ - : U.. \ a; .. ._ ~h .‘h- '.~ c h \"- ”c . .v‘y‘v - k . . . -~‘Zr_, \‘fi ‘ un‘rn \. R~ w‘ '. . '»‘ ‘u N» -\p “ ‘,-'_‘, V .. 9.. '2 ‘ r . 7O unidimensional, the pattern of responses must be 90 per cent reproducible. The next question which arises at this point is how to tackle a situation when the reproducibility of a scale is lower than the desired 90 per cent. Such a scale is described as quasi-scale, if the errors occur in a random fashion. Stouffer comments: The correlation of the quasi-scale with an outside criterion is the same as the multiple correlation between responses to the individual item forming that scale and the outside criterion which Justi- fies the use of sets of items from an area not scalable in the strictest sense (Stouffer, 1950, p. 5). The criteria suggested by Guttman, when applied to the scales used in the present research would indicate that they are more or less quasi-scales rather than proper scales, in the strict Guttman sense. It may be noted, how— ever, that the criterion of 90 per cent reproducibility is rm more an absolute standard than is the selection of an alpha of .05 for the test of significance. In those areas cfi‘social research where previous studies do not provide enough comparison data, even lower limits of reproduci- bility may prove to be valuable. But the important criterion in regard to the scale error appears to be the :andomenss of the errors. Suchman explains this problem as follows: The error pattern of the quasi-scale question is recognizable from the manner in which the fairly large number of errors that occur gradually de- crease in number as one moves further and further " anpu .- -?z-s t-r- ‘ ’ J 'vc‘rgn avioov. l nu-“n‘pi ‘\ 3 s" ..-n. ¢~$.- 5...! A . u 1‘ ’.;.‘A“:r :— -- u..-'g.‘p-- “ V‘v- .cm ”u " A .\ I v4..~ ‘ . A ’ Q A _ . nr-" I .— "' H..- ' ‘3'" *r- .tn' a “'0 v.. u.‘ ' 1"" ‘A ‘- ...¢u.d.. v.4 v. ,3 ‘ r b .¢ob “. l n’ “ c. , i. \ . ‘ 1 ‘ N‘“. V'fi, A ‘A‘ V...‘ V. . .- \«~_‘: ‘Ir V v G A .‘¥ ~‘O. \- 71 away from the cutting point. These errors . . . do not group together like non~scale errors (Suchman, 1950, pp. 16o-161).l Another issue which merits some consideration is the comparability of respondents on the basis of favorableness or unfavorableness of responses. What point on the scale marks this division? It has been shown by Foa (1950) and Suchman (1950) that slight changes of question wording nught alter the response patterns considerably. The best solution to this problem is to-achieve an objective "zero" point, independent of the content of the items, which will demarcate the favorable responses from the unfavorable ones. The intensity component of attitude has been found to have the characteristic of solving the problem of ques— tion bias by setting zero point along the evaluative dimen— sion of the attitude scale.. Several experiments have shown that intensity will usually form a quasi—scale which“ when plotted against the content dimension, will reveal the point mxthe content scale of the.lowest intensity of response (Foa, 1950, 1961; Guttman, 1947, 1950, 1954a, 1954b; Gutt- man and Foa, 1951; Guttman and Suchman, 19u7; Suchman, 1950; Suchman and Guttman, 1947). That is, the point of nunimum intensity actually marks the point of indifference along the evaluative continuum, in respect to the item 1The "cutting poinfl'refers to the point at which the "favorable" (or, e.g.,"Yes") responses to an item, can be divided with the least amount of error from the "unfavorable" (or, e.g.,"No") responses to an item, when the respondents have been ordered on the basis of total score for all items in the scale. 1". . (I... 72 cmfient. This has been shown to mark a division between favorable and unfavorable responses. This would, then, mumar to relate directly to the question of validity, since it determines with considerable certainty that a favorable response (in relation to the zero point) actually represents a favorable attitude to the content in question. It is, therefore, posSible to state in respect to a particular gnpup as to what per cent of the respondents are actually favorable, neutral, or unfavorable, as defined by an objec- tive and invariant referent point. Methodological Considerations Any research undertaking in the area of attitudes 2mm values must encounter a multitude of technical and methodological difficulties. The problems become exceed- ingly complex when the studies are carried out in the field, that is, outside the laboratory setting. Neverthe- less, with the refinement of statistical tools and sophisti- cated research designs, we are now better equipped to tackle the problem in a scientific manner. gymrol Group In historical perspective, it is no wonder what earlier studies measuring attitudes adopted somewhat naive research designs. The general strategy followed by those investigators was to administer some kind of a scale at one time to group of people drawn from a specific social u u. U- .,.....50 ... p~ A“ ‘r‘ ‘ g... L..u~~ v. . ' o .. .A < 5 -'€' ‘- V‘ u.-uvv -00 Ii ‘ u. up I .1 -n n--.‘ v. . V': --.‘.~. kk "“ “‘ Vi. . . .I-V: .fr‘a "'ou...‘ ‘4‘; ‘. .. Ac. ‘ O._ a r:- " in-" u_ . ;;~ ‘r ‘r- _ " ‘1. a..: 73 setting. Differences between the means of the various gxmps of the total sample were then construed by the researchers to indicate whether or not there was any dif- finence in the attitudes of the groups compared. The con- cflusions thus obtained from such studies were definitely rum valid since there was a conspicuous lack of control groups. Our review of the literature on parent—child inter- mfidons has shown that the findings are highly inconsistent. fins may be partly due to the variation in research methods Lmed in the studies. Three dimensions of variation in the investigations have been identified by Spiegel and Bell. They are: ”(l) the use of large numbers of cases (30 or more) versus the study of the single case; (2) the use of subjective impressions derived from clinical case studies versus the use of objective or test measures; (3) the use of selected control groups versus studies in Which no control group was re- ported (Spiegel and Bell, 1959, p. 121). The authors have taken great pains in tabulating 85 parent-child studies published since 1930 with a View to comparing the methodological characteristics mentioned above. Their conclusion which has special bearing on the Inesent research may be quoted here: in the whole sample, clinical studies far outnumber objective methods of study, and the Studies lacking a control group are much more numerous than those in which a control group is used (Speigel and Bell, 1959. p. 121). hp '0 c C ; . «40'5" '_ '.:',.v’no V .. r“,— '3“0 r‘: .AIUOV O ,, ,. ’1’: on46r~ h . .4 -...» v. C: cg :60. H, v.0 V. ~ ' ‘t‘ A h .I.‘ .gy. v U ' h ' r2? a "" .'d~v’ .- n, ‘ ..‘ Q‘; "‘,: f ...““' Ulny 'vflw _ 7!; Selection of Variab les —— There are many studies which deal with one variable at a time with the result that contextual relationship of that particular variable with other related variables is lost sight of. Admittedly, it is almost impossible to control all other significant variables, yet it is possi- ble to set up a multivariate eXperimental design which would not be accused of yielding spurious results. In the least, a multiple regression analysis can be used to evaluate the contribution of related variables. Measurement Techniques Traditionally, attitudes and values have been meas- tued by questionnaires. But clinical research seem to favor interviews over objective measuring devices. Although the controversy surrounding clinical versus statistical prediction (Meehl, 1954) is not yet settled, researchers seem to favor the use of questionnaires for correlational Emalyses of the variables in question. However, some in- \estigators believe that interview studies allow us to Penetrate beyond the data provided by such psychometric‘ hnnruments as questionnaires or scales. Sears (1965) made a comparison of interviews with quEStionnaires for measuring mothers' attitudes toward sex andaggression. Interview measures were found to be more Smasfactory in regard to independently obtained measures ..~ .H‘jngr's ': DI ,. n... o - -'-'--~"~vg 4r v1; ' '...o» ‘u . ‘1 I« deb-In {—— n 'A “in-.15 a .4“- . u. ' I .nuy- A,“ .'...1! .d. I . " ’ an. : 1:P‘r‘£ :u' . :funyu‘4, ‘4 ' a . . A I".‘l"‘lv‘fl \_ -v‘.‘-..f‘ L, K ., _ 'i‘ ”as A“ .. a“: ‘. O .‘r‘.’ ‘vle . Ila. ' ....4u.‘na‘ s ;'; 'QA‘. ‘ m. ,anl ”’2‘; U“, .. . J 'c “ 7 v, V‘ . 5"eii Q24 ' ".. '1. '7“: l . vvc‘, “. :‘v". ‘ — . ‘- ' - ... 7,: ”I“ ‘ “IA\.D fi:,. “as. :y. . . 1‘. ..‘ P6392". -‘. : | , '9 a.“ ‘-"v ‘ “‘ .5 1A,. ' \I -V..‘ "t: . ‘ ,4c‘. »“\_: arr; a,‘ .... "H534: p, ‘ Av "\.fi~ ' “M4 .- ' us .,I 3. ‘ A V J ‘~CL.‘“ 75 of children's behavior. But attitude scale proved more effective in replicating group differences diScovered in previous studies and in measuring mothers' observed behavior. It has also been suggested that Q-sort techniques ‘should be used instead of interviews or questionnaires for making accurate appraisals of parental attitudes (e. g., Babbit, 196A). longitudinal Studies Most studies of parental attitudes are cross~ sectional, in the main, even though control groups may have been added. If attitudes and values are indicators of consistency of behavior and thought patterns, then longitudinal studies are best suited to provide reliable and valid data as to the nature of attitude with respect to a given social object. Furthermore, the role of various factors in producing and sustaining changes in attitudes and values can also be measured effectively with longi— tudinal research. It is not surprising, therefore, to find many longitudinal studies for measuring changes in attitudes and values of students as a consequence of college education (e.g., Lehmann, Sinha, and Hartnett, 1966; Plant, 1963; Sanford, 1962). Johnson (1963) has made a fervent appeal for a great expansion of research in the field of disability which 1 I ‘u-i .l-nn ”.54 , . . .r. um; go-I-U‘\‘\‘ (Ix ‘ . . r “A”, '— y..--\l ‘1 u; no‘rfl an-r " 9‘ a. "ac-UQV-h» L. o . 76 would include systematic and comprehensive studies of the broad spectrum of the disabled. Such studies would begin in early childhood and would continue through the life of' the handicapped.- CHAPTER III METHODOLOGY AND PROCEDURE The primary purpose of the present research was to investigate the attitudes of mothers toward emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. "Personal contact" was the basic variable on which the mothers' responses were compared. Two other dimensions emphasized in this research were "value system" and "educational experience" of the mothers selected in the study. It was also presumed that some general ecological character- istics such as, change orientation, institutional satis- faction, and popular stereotype about mental illness would influence the mothers' attitude scores. The subjects' attitudes, values, educational exposure, and other rele— vant aspects were measured by a set of attitude and value scales, and questionnaires eliciting demographic infor- mation. Research Population Since "personal contact" with emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons was the main concern of the present research, it was considered important to select mothers.whose contact with emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped would be most intimate and 77 C“ frequent. cu. able at a cor; ughflyl‘llfi' u r .maL&$'J U*Jc O. ‘ g g. 3.5.? Magma. tile-sized. arises are 1 Igyfin‘ w‘j‘ nun ‘0 I Jun-4.‘ OV“ "0‘0“. entoa Inova4fl- ‘ «wk-41.)! ..a :{n'l J -3151 +a+4n “.'v v-' I ‘7 ”v ‘( n... e'dQV‘C: :" '1. 9". Va ~ sgd. bL‘ “an 7'; A‘ ""‘OSESt: . "“3"? wit‘r m 33‘? $5 ‘I- K “A (F) 78 frequent. Such a pepulation of mothers was ideally avail~ able at a comprehensive community mental health, and a rehabilitation center located on the campus of The Betty Jane Memorial Center in Tiffin, Ohio, a typical midwestern, middle-sized community. The out-patient mental health services are provided by the Sandusky‘Valley Guidance Center which caters patients ranging from preschool children to adults. The rehabilitation services for the physically handicapped are rendered by the Betty_Jane Rehabilitation Center. The rationale for selection of mothers who were using the services of these two out—patient centers for their emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children was based on the assumption that these mothers had closest, most frequent as well as different kinds of contact with such children. The selective population for the two experimental groups, therefore, was mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children who were using the facilities and services of their local mental health clinic and physical rehabilitation center. The group of mothers who had neither emotionally disturbed nor physically handicapped children served as the population for the control group. These mothers were selected from Mount Pleasant, Michigan, another mid- western, middle-sized community. VA" 7‘5“: U .cvdnst «“- mug:- o ‘- ‘ '5'“: :c ”vi-‘v‘ . . :94 lpl‘n‘: ' cub .-“."lv.‘ { -._.' 'I ' 5 rs.) 'A~.,“r "V‘ . ‘vo ‘V‘. .0 ‘ u'F‘cfl " 'vug,_‘ V. A . “. .v ‘A q u' .a"“ I ~ fl“: -r c. ‘ v . u. 7 a . ‘a Cr; u f... '4‘ ._' ‘4 \‘dPGv- u, r ' 79 Mothers of non—disturbed and non-handicapped children were presumed to have little or no close personal contact with the emotionally disturbed or the physically handi- capped as compared to mothers in the two experimental groups. Selection of Samples Since the samples could not be selected on a random basis because of the specific nature of the population and limited availability of subjects, selection of the samples was made according to specified criteria. Careful exploration of the possible selective factors was at- tempted in order that permissible generalizations could be made in the light of these selective factors. The Experimental Groups The first and foremost consideration was that the mothers selected for the study should have either an emotionally disturbed child or a physically handicapped child undergoing treatment in one of the two centers. The children who were new patients or ex-patients were excluded. That is, only the mothers of children under active psychotherapy or physical therapy were acceptable. The overlapping cases where the child was receiving both kinds of services at the Sandusky Valley Guidance Center and at the Betty Jane Rehabilitation Center were also excluded from the samples. Those mothers who had two children, one under psychotherapy and the other receiving "lvflfid‘- thv . v .. u .n..J| n- u':;-warb Or- voaaou-nvncv .v ”3'35... "C.“ - u-o-ovJ v. v (“a ) 3 ) (I: (I) w KL“) k I D I 1- F- a .-"._ ‘92‘; -Un‘ ' 1' Vi‘-‘ . ... .4 's '33», . (Q..‘ “‘A: ‘ . O.. - A‘ ' #- ... HQ. fir Vi " "'hrnn "" v ‘\ ANNE '*¢-' r '. do“; Say! " s - LV‘Qe“ NV-.. ‘ - q... -. v5. 7 'v.. EWV‘ .H ~ :"~V" t 80 physical therapy for the disability were screened care- fully. Also, the mothers who were themselves undergoing treatment for either any mental problem or physical dis- ability were not selected for the study. The age range of children was between 5 and 15 years. Moreover, only those children were considered whose I. Q.'s fell within the average range of intelligence (i.e., 90—109). Administration of a standard intelligence test such as the Stanford-Binet or the Wechsler Intellih I gence Scale for Children to the incoming patients is a routine diagnostic procedure at both centers. As such, the I.Q. scores of the children were easily available for the purpose of sample selection. Also, the children were of both sexes in both the groups. Insofar as the diagnostic categories of the children are concerned, all patients in the emotionally disturbed group belonged to the category of "adjustment reaction" of childhood or adolescence. The diagnostic categories in the physically handicapped group were, however, variegated. Most common disability cases were speech defectives, blind, deaf, and cripples or amputees. The mothers thus selected came primarily from the city of Tiffin. All of them were of white race. How- ever, there were some mothers who belonged to neighboring suburb or rural areas. "5}...” h-4¢¢v‘4! ~~ :I".I.O '7 ‘A ‘ 0v Oct-UQ‘Q.J ~ I war - :c;u&~4, o-tvv- .o-.,_ " . 0n“... “.IQ|I "‘J g c... ““ v)- .1.._~ ”a F s ‘ _ F v ‘ v. M... . II-“: ‘- -..u..-..2. - ' v th _ ~ '3 ‘ .-‘~ " v on. ‘te/u ‘- ; C. 'fano f h...‘ ‘v. v .; «WC-“JP? ““ “Q"CV—Hc I. . a C Pcth" .~.' ‘U'.-V‘ M’s.“ n'uu" - 'n I'.‘ v.“-‘ a ..; -c\ m— . Q i“, V.“ n: ‘ s .. {a “‘2‘“. I Q‘ .5" . .'.. I " ~‘:.1=:= ’_‘ VVvu ‘1‘ d 3. ’ a “ s ‘ r- \II 11.5 El! ‘ v‘. . . a "\ ".Vee ~75» , «‘V‘ a; — . t V. ‘ JJ .,.V’FQ ‘- ._‘ .h‘A ' . p “‘ ugh t‘rch U .“ s ‘v- I‘.F:‘: ~‘ \“ ‘. q :_‘ ‘ \V . I; “~ 5 ‘ ... . I. .- i u.~'\ H Va‘ 'h 81 Although exhaustive demographic data were obtained with the help of a questionnaire, no attempt was made initially to control age, income, education, marital status, mobility, residence, number of children, and so forth. The experimental groups were originally to have con— sisted of all those mothers who met the above criteria of sampling. It was soon obvious, however, that many mothers 'were not willing to complete the various questionnaires. 'There were others who, although volunteering to answer tune questionnaires, did not return them within a reason- able period of waiting. Of 75 mothers of emotionally dis- tnxrbed children who were initially given the question- rutires, only 6U mothers returned the packet. Since four r .numudnrtaumu L C :3 fi.u..o.mv,~ maul! . N. .SQ:<.~. U. 8 .cmnpafino Aameaocv Umaamoficcmnlcoc mo mnmnpoz u ozono Honpcoo .cmnpafizo Umoamoapcmn maamofimmna no mnmnpoz u HH ozone Hmucmsfinmoxm .cmnpafico eanSpmfiv mHHMQOHpoEm mo mnmzpoz u H axons Hmpcmefinoaxm H Ro.mm ow :H mm ooa Ammlcozv Honucoo em.sm m: m om mm Ammv HH Hmpcmsfinmoxm eo.om oo e as me “some H Hmccceflcccxm Wm .02 Hmfinmpmz Hmfinmomz umme Hmfinmpmz pmme Hmfinmpmz pmme ozone pmme nmuomnmm msficaSpmm wcfi>fimomm H macaw: mo amnezz amnesz amnssz .mmHQEmw ho soapomaomll.a mqm;r'vl H ua-v‘v.5, x... . U U m (I) F-v. A‘: A -—~4. . . -"'" 'v»..~ ‘ M N """' "VA- J a P'r ‘ . lov‘- _.‘ . ‘7‘ b‘ \ H L (I) 1‘) '9 (1‘ (d 7‘ ’(1 85 Seneca County. Generally speaking, it is a farm area. The city has a modest amount of industry: glassware, plottery, electrical and toolmaking machinery. Heidelberg College, a private and coeducational institution, caters to the needs of the community for higher education. In 1960, the Bureau of the Census (1963b) reported that the city has a population of 21,478. Considering the rate of population growth, the estimated population at the present ‘time would be in the neighborhood of 24,000. The mental health and physical rehabilitation services are provided by a.complex of agencies located on the campus of the Betty Jane Center. Mount Pleasant is located in the central part of the state of Michigan. It serves as the county seat of Isabella County. According to the census (1963a), popu- lation of the city was 14,875. Presently, the estimated Size of population is approximately 18,000. The growth or the city is credited to the discovery of oil in 1928. It now supplies the oil fields and refines oil. There is a state university called Central Michigan University whictl;is coeducational and draws student population from Varidous parts of Michigan. The city also has a Child Guidance Clinic for the diagnosis and treatment of emotionally disturbed children. The following table derived from the United States Cefmsus of Population, 1960, provides a comparison of the . . ya’y. ,9. vvbuouv- O ‘ an; .' ' r -. “do-.. Uguy . R.“ ’1 “'--a- ~u.. ”1}: ‘ A. p ‘0... \v- .- v--.- ‘I\ .‘Jhg |~ h. ‘- '0 0.,“ ‘.“J Vin." 5 I n . V; “. ._ . I ‘..‘~ ‘a‘.. '7‘.“ l t v 5 v ”1:”. . ._ ‘ - K “A...“ ~ . § . “ - .‘.. W‘q‘ ' v ‘ ‘ ‘ LI. _ ‘ ’. ' . ~‘ng . "v ~ ‘ .- V.” ." o 'b p “. . .2 . I - F e _ ~ '. -.I_ . ~ ‘~ y 'u‘ . . \ " .'; a, “ ~. _ . h— p .‘ ‘\ ‘. .. ‘. 3 «r .‘~ ‘I \_ .‘ y‘ . .' _. -"~ I.. -‘ ,.' .— 86 tvvo cities in relation to some general characteristics of? the population (1963a, 1963b): TABLE 2.--Size of population and income in Tiffin and Mount Pleasant. Per cent With Total Population Median Incomes of ijty Popu— by Sex Income lation Under $10,000 Male Female $3,000 and Over 'Tigffin 21,478 10,330 11,148 5,759 15.6 11.7 Mount EfiLeasant 14,875 7,374 7,501 6,229 15.6 17.6 ‘ Selection of Variables The selection of variables for the present research was primarily stimulated by an ambitious cross-cultural research project being carried out at Michigan State University (see page 9, Chapter 1). This international research project has hypothesized a relationship between attitudes toward physical handicap and personal contact, value structure, educational orientation, and certain demographic variables. Moreover, the research reports, and theoretical and methodological considerations already reviewed in Chapter II pointed to the paucity of research data based on the variables mentioned above, particularly in the field of mental health. Thus the theoretically—dictated and empirically supported variables were those suspected to have some . n ..--.l r ‘ ' ‘ .' ”H 91., ’_ .,...v‘o¢o bi-..- v I ‘ .- ‘-l r neo- , In .- uouu' 1] iv..- . . . q. p p‘.~. 1‘ . g .— ..c U] aunts-Aa— il . ‘ 2““? uelugcgg... :1“ n. n._ H‘ t A" ‘ u "1 .‘- r ’Dy unnot. «D . V_. I ‘ u.- ‘ ., _ - - . 5.: , - — FV' ‘. , . . " Iva. . .5 —.. . ‘0'! ‘C - 87 rerticular relationship to the two main criterion vari- mfles: (a) attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons, and (b) attitudes toward physically handicapped persons. The other two important variables were concerned with the value system of the subjects and their attitudes toward education. Additional variables that were selected for the study related to the respondents' change orientation. Description of Instruments A number of instruments were employed to measure the subjects' attitudes toward emotional disturbance and physical handicap, their value systems, their attitudes toward education, and so forth. With the exception of one, most of the measures were adapted after suitable modifications from already available tests, while some were locally constructed specifically for the study. The battery of tests consisted of the Handicapped Persons Scale, the Emotionally Disturbed Persons Scale, the Edu- cation Scale, and Survey of Interpersonal Values. In addition, a series of personal questionnaires were given They were: Personal Questionnaire (general), Personal Quandonnaire--HP, and Personal Questionnaire--EDP. All the scales and personal questionnaires have been attached in APpendix A . 88 PMndicapped Persons Scale The items used in this scale were taken from the Attitudes Toward Disability Scale (Yuker, gt_a1., 1960). Test-retest reliability scores were reported to range from .67 to .78. Various construct validity data (Yuker, et;§£., 1960, pp. 5—8) that were collected from disabled employees of Abilities, Inc., a light manufacturing com- pany which employs disabled workers, also indicated the adequacy of the scale. Among these employees, the test was found to be negatively related to age and anxiety, and positively related to verbal intelligence and job satisfaction. Females and those with low absentee rates made higher scores. But the validating group itself has questionable generality and the rationale for item selection is not clear. A recent study attempted to determine the factorial structure and correlates of the Attitudes Toward Disabled Persons Scale (Siller and Chipman, 1964). The obtained data indicated acceptable reliability and comparability over age and educational levels. But the author questioned the use of a single overall score. However, the test repre- sents a major attempt to fill a gap in the field and herrants further study. The Handicapped Persons Scale used in the present r‘esearch was modified so as to make provisions for re- flmmdent scoring. The Likert-type format was retained, Muzthe response categories for each item were reduced r , .. ‘r JO. ' .' “Aw .yhfiv “A‘I“‘ 7‘ w _,. ovd‘-. "' . .a “ Uvibld, .o‘ .. .po .- iv. It... A I c. .5 ... . . a. .: .I .. . r. r. .4 .1 c. x. a. ..~ so f. T~ .2 n. . a . c. : . a .. A a 2» . . : . ~ . .. .v ... a .. a y ‘ cc. .. ... v” .»u 1. a. 4.. .3 C. 4‘ to to r“ G» A. a. re 2. h. n... .»~ :~ 5. L. c. L. . . -. a . . 2. .. .1 ... .3 L . :~ I. ~ . 2. s . 2‘ Te 4 ..I. ...u n. L. u. \ “a 9: Ce .; p . .1 Z. n. . 1‘- 1 v r . f. .r.. .2. g u “.4” «.u .s- ..~ . c a u .A‘ ~.- ~ e -.H ..v s v p \ 89 from seven to four. Another modification was that instead of requiring the respondent to transfer a number from a set of coded categories at the top of the page to indicate his response, the item alternatives were stated following each question. Since it is intended to submit the items to scale analysis rather than follow the suggested scoring system, there is no need to follow the same numerical SCOI‘GS . ggnotionally Disturbed Persons Scale The scale attempts to measure the respondents' atti— tleeS toward emotionally disturbed persons. This scale mnas specially constructed for the present research. Ehssentially the scale under consideration is based on the Séime item content provided in the Handicapped Persons E5<2a1e discussed in the previous section. Suitable modifi- cations were made in the scale by substituting mental h’Ec'ilth concepts for the concepts pertaining to physical disability. Fm&cation Scale This scale is an adaptation of Kerlinger's Attitudes [PCJWard Education Scale (Kerlinger, 1958, 1961; Kerlinger and Kaya, 1959). Modifications similar to those de- SCBribed in the Handicapped Persons Scale were made for tlle Education Scale. It would not be out of order here to present a brief description of the Attitudes Toward Education Scale I I a . .. .. 4.. ’v- ‘ n‘f ‘ '-—n-d-y o-Avnov. -,.c o." i. h. .p. . a a. d . v s v n v s u . .- vv,-.. I. 90 as developed by Kerlinger. The complete instrument con— sists of 20 items, of which 10 are "progressive," and the other 10 are "traditional." Educational attitudes have been conceptualized by Kerlinger as hinging on two relatively independent underlying factors or ideologies. They are: "traditionalism" and "progressivism." The author has shown that traditionalism can be conceived as the affirmation of a stand which emphasizes a conservative- traditional approach to educational issues. Progressivism, on the other hand, is not just the Opposite of traditional- ism in education. It has an existence of its own where education has a much wider connotation. Precisely, the restrictive-traditional factor has been defined by Kerlinger (1958, p. 112) as that which emphasizes subject-matter for its own sake. The hier- archical nature of impersonal superior-inferior relation- ships is considered important; and external discipline is a matter of great concern. Social beliefs are pre- served through the maintenance of the status quo. In contrast, the permissive-progressive factor emphasizes problem—solving with a minimum concern for the subject- matter. In this perspective, education is seen as growth and the child's interests and needs are seen as basic to education. Equality and warmth in interpersonal relationship are valued. Internal rather than external discipline is considered important. Social beliefs tend 91 to be liberal, and education is viewed as an instrument of change. The Attitudes Toward Education Scale represents a factor analysis of a set of 40 items given to 598 sub- jects of varying backgrounds, but all apparently of above average education. The scale has been found to hold up under cross-validation; however, there is no indication that persons of lower educational attainment have been adequately represented in the studies. In fact, a sur- face examination of the items (see Appendix A-3) suggests that some of them may be somewhat overly complex and diffi- cult for many people. As employed in the present study, the "progressive" and "traditional" items will be analyzed independently as two separate scales. The Education Scale, a modified version of the Kerlinger's Attitudes Toward Education Scale, was included in the present study for several reasons. Firstly, there is some justification in hypothesizing a relationship between progressive attitudes and attitudes toward emotional disturbance and physical disability. Secondly, in a study so closely interwoven with educational con— cerns, the findings are interesting in their own right. Gordon's Survey of Interpersonal Values The selection of the Gordon Survey of Interpersonal Values (Gordon, 1960) was based on two considerations. First, an instrument was needed which would yield scores “'9‘“ vial-- ' 1 92 on items that seemed logically related to the values under test in the hypotheses. These values are: "asset" orientation to others and "comparative" orientation to others. Of the six sub—scales in the instrument, the one for Benevolence is described as: "Doing things for other Ipeople, sharing with others, helping the unfortunate, loeing generous” (Gordon, 1960, p. 3). In a subsequent leesearch, Benevolence was found to correlate .49 with the bhlrturance score on the Edwards Personal Preference Sche- dllle and negatively with Achievement (-.24) and Aggression (—.28), according to Gordon (1963, p. 22). It was decided CH1 the basis of the description, the item content, and the intercorrelations with the EPPS that the Benevolence Value prOposed by Gordon would be an adequate operationalization of "asset value." The second value to be operationalized was that of a "comparative" orientation toward others. The Gordon Manual offers the follwoing definition for Recognition Value: "Being looked up to and admired, be- ing considered important, attracting favorable notice, achieving recognition" (Gordon, 1960, p. 3). The defi- nition for Conformity Value is: "Doing what is socially correct, following regulations closely, doing what is accepted and prOper, being a conformist" (Gordon, 1960, p- 3). The value of Leadership was defined as: "Being in Charge of other people, having authority over others, heing in a position of leadership or power" (Gordon, 1960, p. 3). 93 All three of these values would appear to involve ranking of others in some kind of absolute scale, either of social acceptability (Conformity), achievement (Recog- nition), or power (Leadership). On the basis of surface consideration of item content, the Recognition items were judged to be most representative of Comparative values. Moreover, a correlation of .58 was found between Aggres- sion on EPPS and Leadership. This would indicate that Leadership also is a good indicator of comparative values. A special feature of Gordon's value scale is that it utilizes forced-choice technique. Apparently, the pur- pose of a forced-choice format is to control the factor of social desirability. Whether or not ipsative measures are as valid as normative measures is still a vexing problem for psychometricians. For example, "Knapp (1964) made an empirical investigation of the concurrent and ob- servational validity of Gordon's Survey of Interpersonal values. He concluded that "yes-no" response format differentiated between the two groups (Navy offenders and non—offenders) better than the forced-choice format. However, for the purposes of this study, it was con- sidered useful to employ a measure which would curb the subjects' tendency to fake good on value questions. Personal Questionnaire (General) This questionnaire had two parts in it. The first part was concerned with the respondents' contacts with 94 school and education. It also attempted to elicit their knowledge about education. The second part of the questionnaire was intended to provide personal information about the subjects such as, age, income, education, marital status, number of children, mobility, and so forth (see Appendix A-5). Contact with education was, thus, measured by four items (PQ 4—7) where the respondents were required to indicate: (a) how much they had worked in schools or educational settings; (b) what per cent of their income was derived from such work; (c) how they generally felt about such work; and (d) what other work opportunities they could have chosen alternatively. In addition, three Other questions were asked to determine various kinds or levels of education experienced, and varieties of contact With education. Change Orientation items contained a number of Statements which purported to elicit subjects' attitudes tDward change in such areas as health practices, child- rearing practices, birth control practices, automation, and political leadership (PQ 39-43). It was assumed that peOpIe expressing positive attitudes toward emotional diSturbance and physical handicap would reveal greater flexibility and openness toward change. Self change (PQ 47—49), and future orientation (PQ 52—54) of the Subjects were also included in the Questionnaire for measuring attitudes toward change. 'Aaocavan-U J I..." ‘7 - .vnd", -Jq - u .‘A- ‘7'";Y‘ .."" ""‘D‘oto V -"-".A a \ . Uv-uv¢-:, . ‘ . v D'.‘ fir -“.'f .n‘ ..“. «‘- ' l \ .w“, "."~v I We... dig-V ’. -. "nmr ‘._ r- i ‘v‘ . ~ . h _, w \ p. .w‘ v. V... F’- "‘ s ‘ ‘ ‘E‘Ctfi o. ‘ C ’. .~ .‘1 \' &' V r- U .- .._' -‘L'l u‘ ‘1 AV w ‘ r . i h - c “q ‘ U ‘\ \ I u n‘: .. ._ ‘ ' g "_ Q i - 95 These questions were adapted from Programa Inter- americano de Informacion Popular (PIIP) in Costa Rica (Felty, 1965). Institutional satisfaction was measured by a set of nine questions (PQ3lA—31I). These measures were adapted from Hyman (1955, p. 400). The institutions selected (schools, business, labor, government, health services, and churches) were listed in the question. The respon- dents were asked to indicate whether they judged these institutions as: excellent, good, fair, or poor in re- spect to how well they perform their particular job in the community. It was hypothesized that mothers who hold favorable attitudes toward the emotionally disturbed and physically disabled would be less satisfied with the insti— tutions, generally speaking, than those mothers who express negative attitides. Preferences for personal relationships were measured with the help of a set of three items (PQ 21-23) in Section 2 of the Questionnaire asking personal information. These three items were devised to help identify respondents or groups of respondents along a traditional-modern di- mension. The predominance of affective relationships as opposed to affectively neutral relationships is supposedly one of the distinguishing characteristics of the "Gemein- shaft” or traditional orientation (e.g., Loomis, 1960, p. 61ff). One question (PQ 21) asked the respondent to indicate the approximate per cent of personal interactions *v'flf~.- .' o , \ :— coo—noiui .. ~nv in.-. I J ‘ F‘ ... :crcze M O‘UvotvUU v- .': "'V‘”"rc_ I... ""3" vo-y a ‘V v- a. ‘5 ‘5 ... t‘. J yv 4'; .,. . Lhzqu. A -u“‘~‘v4-v ‘e‘ ‘50 - :3- h "v..- ‘2: “V v‘.’ 5 file i III- “"0: r'fi V ...v. N . I a ‘~ “~ :- * v- '\ ‘d .5 .K c. ‘ v \ ’7. . o‘ ”A ‘Q w—w—u— 96 on the job which were with persons who were close personal friends. Another question (PQ 22) asked how important it was to work with persons who were close personal friends. The third question (PQ 23) was intended to signify dif— fuseness of specificity of personal interactions under the hypothesis that the traditionally oriented person is more likely to have personal interactions which are dif- fused between job and family, or other affective non-job interactions. Loomis comments: Members of the Gemeinshaft-like system are likely to know each other well; their relationships are functionally diffuse in that most of the facets of human personality are revealed in the prolonged and intimate associations common to such systems (Loomis, 1960, p. 72). In accordance with our hypotheses about values, then, those respondents who are committed to "asset" values, being more concerned with intrinsic valuation of the per- Son rather than valuing him for his absolute achievements, should also express a greater need for personal inter- aCtions generally, and a greater diffuseness of inter— personal relationships. Religiosity of the subjects were measured with the help of three questions (PQ 19, 20 and 38). Specifically, the questions asked were: (a) religious preference, (b) the felt importance of religion to the respondent, and (C) conformity to the rules and regulations of the religion. "Religiosity” also seemed to be related to the traditional-modern dimension, and as such higher — op-ran vyflu fl .~ ‘ :H... “VJ‘J . . . ppa I-oo ‘r'lflv- :u‘ av“ fits-v9.1 C‘.”" av” Jen-4». _. ___1__— fl H‘n-td'. V . {A u“. A‘s 7*r "' A ' It 7“ *‘V at; U . Kr «a 41 y-.,,,, '9 .7 ‘4 Y“ Y.r “ a I, .a-: I :7" 3"“r nu IAVU -‘Ad:.. 1? c.‘ u. 7‘ rhcn, in. U. ”C.- . v 'v. ‘14:“ Q‘wé. v. UV“ I '7. n R.. ‘ N I‘ “F a '.~“:’\ 7‘- u NJ ‘ . \ fi.‘ -‘ . u. x I. 'UE.‘ ‘- ‘ v .- .‘ . K 7.. r g \.H “0. “.r- c ‘ v.‘ s_- u I - A ‘I s “‘VS‘ ’ v r- u ‘. p 'uh ”fl ‘ .itv- 3... .‘ n~4‘ us.“ o. “ l‘ ‘A i '. ‘ .v“~-) A “s ( "I ‘L - Q ~.' ‘I. a q»: ‘M A ~— ‘H ‘ . >" I 'v__\ ‘r .. n '1‘ ‘: . ‘* “ac; v" A ,"- v. ~r: - .~ Q. Q. - ‘c \- ' J in“ “ o c :- .1?— 5 ‘. -:~ 97 scores would be expected among persons with less education and low income. Demographic Characteristics were ascertained by a number of questions in the Personal Questionnaire. They were age (PQ 8), marital status (PQ 12), number of children (PQ 13), number of siblings (PQ 16, 17), education (PQ 26, 27), occupation (PQ 37), home ownership (PQ 29), rental (PQ 30), rural-urban youth (PQ 9), and income (PQ 14). It is not intended to use all of these demographic variables in the present data analysis because of time and space limitations. They will be utilized more fully in the larger study described on page 9. Egggonal Questionnaire: HP Contact with physically handicapped persons was measured by nine questions. The items (PQ—HP) were con— Structed to determine: (a) the kind of physical disability With which the mothers had had the most contact, or knew the most about (PQ—HP l, 2); (b) the type of relationship the respondents had had with physically disabled persons ‘-family, friends, working relationships, casual, etc. (PQ-HP 3); and (c) the approximate number of encounters the subjects had had with physically handicapped per— Sons (PQ—HP U). Other significant questions were de- Signed to explore alternative opportunities (PQ-HP 9), enjoyment of contact.with handicapped persons (PQ-HP 8), ease of avoidance of such contacts (PQ-HP 5), material I ogu 55's.“. Chav‘i'q '- pnhw fi"7 .avul v.1- IN. ‘lv 0 . . .prqfir‘ZE .- ."."“V I ':r:_-: ~v-sr . ~-.»—.‘ h- -.. “ .- 5,... 7‘ n..,. -.u~.¢4-., , ' I t ”:1. "‘ry~r~< ,. J""'-vso..,.‘_ a s.‘ . .. ~ ‘\ 'V‘f;n "“~"‘JIAA.‘a 0. _ .y...‘_‘c~ :' :..q-"‘ . v.5 ‘ ’ H»- , “c. v . .I . . ‘ .1. Vhfic" ‘.~.“Vv‘ . . by "‘ ’CY. v “‘vu‘l' 98 gain from contact with the handicapped (PQ-HP 6), and percentage of income derived from working with the dis- abled (PQ—HP 7). Personal Questionnaire: EDP Contact with emotionally disturbed persons was deter— mined with the help of this questionnaire which was specially constructed for the present research. The items in this questionnaire were comparable to the ones in the Personal Questionnaire: HP described above. Thus the questions provided information about the following: (a) whether or not the respondents had had any contact with emotionally disturbed persons (PQ—EDP l); (b) the kinds of experiences the mothers had had with emotionally disturbed persons-- family, friends, working relationships, etc. (PQ-EDP 2); and (c) approximate number of encounters these mothers had had with emotionally disturbed persons (PQ-EDP 3). Other important areas explored were: ease of avoidance of con- tact with emotionally disturbed persons (PQ-EDP u), material gain from such contacts (PQ—EDP 5), per cent of income de- l"ived from contact while working with emotionally disturbed persons (PQ—EDP 6), enjoyment of contact with emotionally dis- turbed persons (PQ-EDP 7), and alternative opportunities (PQ-EDP 8). Collection of Data Ideally, there should have been a "testing" session in which the data could be collected in a single session. But, the composition of subjects in both control group and ; it- ~ ---;r. w;v ,4 3...... -u-.. .. .. . . . . A». "H 3" “..-\ ..vH 0:.mo mm Hmm>Hzmwsozv xhoz 02 5w me.o H o o Tm.m H o o .umzucHumazucoz aw oo.a : o o :H.e m mm.H H mHnmcsoucoz-maz mm ca.m o o o cm.HH a ma.H H mHnmnsolwuz mm ms.o H o o o c ma.H H .mczucoz Hm mm; a $6 2 2 .m H £5 m 333$ .03: C. no.0 H o o c : mm.H H mo>Hpmtmqo Ho mo.o H o o rm.m H o o moHcmcooz mm mo.c H o o mm.m H o o coEmLom Ho mm.H m o o ;\.3 x mm.H H coEmummpo ow em.H m so.H H o o mo.H H .opm .wpmHHm: mm mm.H m Ho.H H cg.: m o o cmammxaocm mm m9: e SH H H.m.m H 85 m mhmxhoz $me Hm mo.mH om mm.mm 2H cH.H m ss.m m HmoHHmHo om 56 H o o “m x. H o o m$5.6 5.3m 3 om.H m o o im.m H mm.H H monope HHmbom m: mo.c H o o n : mm.H H whoxpoz HmHoom mm 56 H o o 2. .m H o o mcmcohmmmom mm mo.c H o o Hm.m H o o .ooZIzomelcoz wm om.H m o o pm.m H mm.H H .somBI.Umz om mc.o H em.H H o o o o mhmcomme HempcmEon OH omemmmmwm mocmscmcm mmmwmmwmwm mocosvcbm owwwmmwmmm mosescohm mmwwmwwmmm accsvmpm coHuthome mooo Hmpoe m:|:oz mm mzm quopu HCOHumasooo i. ‘l .2 I .II‘ 1. .mdsohm Hconcoouoe an deEmm Hwoou on» go :oHuHmoqeoo HmCOHBMonoouu.a mHm1; 4% ;h.— 1.2.... .. HHH H..H~:.~.~...- :71. —‘.:.J..u_.......~ A»: .. (Nae -ava~ N-N w...&E..v rv u~sN ad (VF 126 .cmLUHHLo HHmEHoc ..o.HV voodooHocmzlco: Ho whosooz u mzlcoz .cmpUHHco nodomoHocmc zHHMonmna Ho mpocuo: u a: .choHHLo oanSHmHo hHHmcoHuoEo Ho whmcuoz u mom H 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 Locuo m 00.0 H co.o o 00.0 o mm.H H mmtwmo smocm>o< 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 hogan oohwoonpmom a mm.m 0H 03.: m HH.HH m Ho.m m omnmoc HHHm ILm>Hcs Lo monHoo o 0H.mH mm am.0m 0H 0a.eH m Hm.oH 0 HHHmcm>Hcs ho oonHOO mEom m 50.30 mcH 0H.em a: HH.Hm mm mg.mp o: nmumHasoo mpmom HH 0» 0H thm : mm.m :H m:.H H 00.0m 0 Fm.~ : ompmHQEoo mpmmm m on 5 Song m cm.H m 00.0 o 00.0 o :0.m m umpmHQEOO munch m o» : Eonm m 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 omumHano munch : can» mmmH H ammocmohom mocmzoopm mwm»coohom mucosvohm swapcmopmm mocmsomhm owmpcmohmm mucosoopm COHQmosom nmpmsqn< ompmsmu< cocmswoa nopwsnu< co unsoe< muoo Hmuoe mzacoz m: mam azopo H .pcoechupm HmcoHpmosoo Hmsoow Ho mspou CH mosopw pcoocoammh 0:» mo comHquEoonu.m mHm unvfis ~..r~ 3. HJH~d~ (w 3. Q 3.1.. nv~kr0.~ thL h —» «HiwmyH: .k a; aka... 3. ~ .~ ETNZHHHhHI.‘ . 3v .axWN MN <.\. 129 .cmHUHHno HHmEHoc ..o.HV UmQQMOHccmnlcoc mo mhmnpoz mmlcoz .cothHno woodonvcmn HHHmOHmHQQ Ho mhmspoz .cmHvHHco umnHSHmHv mHHmcoHpoEm Ho mpmnpoz n mom mm H mmm.m www.mm mmH Hmuoe H00.» HOH.mm no mmlzoz Hm0.H 0H0.mm mH mm wm.o HHmmm.o mom.m HmH.Hm mm Ham mmH HOH.m mmH.m mmH Hmpoe mmm.m 050.0 Hm muscoz HHH.H 00H.m 0H mm 0m.0 H000H.0 0mH.H mH0.H Hm mom mEOocH HmH.0 HNN.H HmH Hmpoa m0m.0 mHm.H no mmlcoz Ham.0 00N.H mH mm mm.0 meoH.H mH0.0 00H.H mm mom coHpmoswm M mo m QoHpmH>mQ saw: 2 Q2090 oHanHm> mocmonchHm ohmocmHm H .masohw unmacommmh woman on» How moHanhm> OHHQMHonou boys» on pooomoh CH moHpmempm m 0cm .mcoHumHaoo unoccupm .moocohmmmHo same no comHhmoEooll.m mHmHB ‘r ?)‘p 3r a“ UoAv Act . .1 ‘I- R n g: dub usu‘-‘ “'9 A“ Vic-4' V; wan-.3 gr .0 "“oG-.d - ”C- tam-- UV. I u... A“ F‘P'vml u r..d'b' ‘WI‘H‘: 5‘V“.~'u o A." gnhn J. ‘1." ‘. ”p: ”:4 lqg ‘- “L 4‘ FA .‘2 an -' Ch s- h v. N in "a‘ha’, v A thn.‘- a H‘v‘ ; 130 The average income of the total sample was $8,500 approximately, although there is a considerable dispersion in the income of individual mothers. When compared with the median income of the residents in Tiffin and Mount Pleasant, these mothers have somewhat higher income. As reported in Table 2, the city of Tiffin has a median in- come of $5,759, whereas Mount Pleasant has $6,229 as its median income. This would suggest that those who seek the services of professional agencies for mental problems or physical disability, generally come from higher income groups. The average age of the mothers in the research samples was 35.28 years. However the standard deviation of the total sample in respect to age data indicates that the majority of mothers fell in the age range of 28.64 to “1.92 years. Differences in Some Other Demographic Characteristics Between Respondent Groups Descriptive data pertaining to number of children, marital status, recent residence, and length of residence of the control group and the two experimental groups are presented in Tables 9-12. Although these variables were not of direct concern for the study, the frequencies and percentages help in providing a general comprehensive picture of the samples. On the average, mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children E1 Hm”. . C+U.HUH.H£ 0 L 0 rnmwaHEHHC [:11.... 1.-- .- n‘flhku’. ..II. I n" 30 DUOQE3L Cfi 1r... ’. .. 3330wa HEDCHHOQEQL my: a .. H3 CC m. N L .3th CL .1 C .uL.f- .CvaHHCo AHMECOC ..m.HV omQQMoHUCmCICoC Ho mhmnuoz u mmICoz .CmCUHHCo woodonchC HHHMonmCQ no mCmeoz u mm .CoCUHHCo UmnpspmHv HHHmCoHpoEm Ho mHmCuoz u mom 131 H mm.H mm.H mm.m H mm.H H ewe OH NH.H mm.H 00.0 o mm.H H mcHz a NH.N 00.0 Ho.0 m mm.H H Hame H Hm.H 00.0 00.0 o OH.m m cm>mm H Ho.m mo.m mm.m H mm.m m me o om.mH H0.OH oo.om m Hm.om HH m>Hm m Hm.Hm OH.mH HH.HN m mm.mH OH Hsom H Hm.Hm HH.HN mm.mH H mm.mm HH mmnae m HH.mH HH.HH HH.HH m HH.HH m 039 m HH.H HH.mH mm.m H mm.H H mco H H .HHH .umpm H .HHH .umHH H .HHH .amhm H .HHH .umHm gmHHHHHo mcoo Hmpoa Hm-coz mm mom Ho HmQEHZ asohc H .CmHoHHCo no hmnECC ou woodman CH mQCOHM quoCoammh mCu Ho ComHHMQEoonu.m mqm - mhj-vfls-rflu .H Mu J u L it: 51V .w \w “J h- A& an flu in, a.“ 1N. EQHJO.HM~ U nhfly five-Onwnu mvru. .u .CoHUHHCo HHmECoC «.m.HV coaamochmCICOC mo mHmeoz n mmuCoz .CmHUHHCo UmdeoHUCmC HHHmOHmzCQ Ho mCmeoz H mm .CoHoHHCo umnhspmHo HHHmCOHuoEm Ho mCmCuoz u mam H 00.0 mH H0.HH 0 HH.HH m H0.m m gaspsm HHHo H 00.0m H0 H0.m0 HH HH.HH 0m 00.00 mm HHHo m mm.mH Hm 00.0 0 mm.mm 0H 00.0w HH ego» thcsoo m HH.HH 0m MH.mH 0 mm.mm 0H 0m.0H 0 thcsoo H H .HHH .umhm H .HHH .umhm H .HHH .Hth H .HHH .umhm mocmnHmmm maoo Hmpoe HH-HOz Hm mom Hcmomm HQCOHo 132 .ooCoonmC pCoomH on poodmmh CH masonw pCmoCoammH on» Ho ComHCMQEooan.HH mqmH0 m 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 00.0 0 mech m HH.m0 HmH NH.m0 H0 HH.HH 0m 0H.00 Hm HmHHHmz H H .HHH .omHm H .HHH .uth H .HHH .umhm H .HHH .umhm mzpmpm 0000 Hmpoa Hm-coz Hm Ham HHHHHHH ozone H .mspwum prHhms op uomdwoh CH masohw pcmucoamma 0:» mo ComHCMQEooII.0H mHmHB .AzufiHfinoEv DUCQUHEQL LO LJECOH JC@LLSO Op uOQQQQL :fi EQZOL$ JCQUZOQQ$L 03¢ g0 COQfiLQQEOUll.NN N43<5 133 .cmHUHfino AHmEpoc ..m.Hv UmQQMOHUcmSICOC no mgmnpoz u mmlcoz .cmpvafino Umaamofivcmn zHHmQHmch Ho mamcuoz u mm .cmHUHHSO Umnazpmfio zHHmCOHpoEm mo mnmnpoz u mam H m0.H0 00H H0.00 :3 00.0m 0m HH.HH mm mummm amp pm>0 m mm.mH mm NH.mH 0 :0.mH 0 HH.HH 0 mumma amp on cw>mm : HH.HH mm NH.mH 0 0H.0H 0 0m.0H 0 mammm xfim on mmpce m m0.m 0 mm.: m mm.0 m Hm.H : mummm ozu 0p mco m :m.: H mm.: m mm.: m zm.m m Ham» m can» mmmq H H .H0H .0mam H .00H .0mum H .H0H .wmpm H .noH .0mum mocmonmm 0000 Hmpoe mmucoz mm mom Ho npwcmH Q50 H aw .HHHHHHpoeV mocmvfimmh mo £pMCmH meHMSO 0p poQOmh CH masonw pcmncoammh map mo comfiamm5oonu.mH mqm¢e --l fi rd: Qv .3 L9 ~»*‘r*o” “ginnv. Hang ‘ “v. v . nAr‘vv' 'UV‘od. ‘ ‘2'. b b. ..._4 € 7 ...6 p4 LEA "HV' FA Ll- *t 13“ had four children, whereas mothers of the control group had three children. A large majority of mothers were living with their husbands. Less than 8 per cent of total mothers were divorced, widowed, or separated. In regard to residence, approximately 60 per cent of the mothers were living in the city, only 9 per cent of the respondents lived in city suburbs. Since mobility has been considered by some as a contributing factor in mental illness, current length of residence of mothers of emotionally disturbed children may be noted. Nearly 73 per cent of these mothers had lived at their present residence for more than seven years. The percentage of mothers in the total sample living at one place over seven years was 75.35 per cent. Summary of Descriptive Data in Tables 3-12 The sample size of 60 and 69 for the mothers of emotionally disturbed and non-handicapped children re- spectively was considered adequate for the study. However, there were only 48 mothers in the group having physically handicapped children. In the total sample, a substantial majority of mothers were housewives. The three samples were comparable in education, income, and age as indicated by statistically non-significant E statistics. The aver- age educational level of mothers was high school or a little more. Very few mothers (about 6 per cent) had a college or university degree. Average yearly income, .ot" oto‘s , were 4 “l p Vonb v 11: the ‘ \ ecz' R Us; Col A or «a 01. .EU 135 from self-employment or from family earnings, was found to be in the range of $7,000 to $8,000 for the total sample. Most mothers were a little over 35 Years old and had three or four children. None of the mothers were separated, while only 3.01 per cent were widowed and 4.21 per cent were divorced. Thus, approximately 93 per cent of the total respondents were living with their husbands. Furthermore, the demo- graphic data reveal that 60 per cent of the mothers of emotionally disturbed children and 65.67 per cent of the mothers of non—handicapped children were living within the limits of the city. However, only HH.HH per cent of the mothers of physically handicapped children lived in the city proper, and HH.HH per cent resided in country or country town. In regard to current length of resi— dence, a majority of the respondents had not changed residence during the past seven years. The foregoing description of sample characteristics must be interpreted with caution inasmuch as the data based on self-report may be somewhat inflated by "social desirability" factors. This would be especially true in respect to education and income. Lacking objective ex- ternal criteria, the extent or presence of a directional bias cannot be assessed. a F V‘.' r { H‘s .MM '4... _ 33 n u. . H H c. Me. a: . 136 Section 2: Hypotheses Testing, Mean Differences, and Correlational Analyses Hypotheses Related to Contact Frequency, Intensity, and Attitude Scores H:1z Contact-Intensity Interactions Hzlal: The more frequent the contact with emotionally disturbedgpersons, the higher will be the scores on the intensity statements of the attitude-toward-emotionally- disturbedgpersons (EDP) scale, regardless of whether attitude content is favorable or unfavorable. In testing this hypothesis, intensity scores on the attitude-toward-emotionally-disturbed-persons were regarded as the dependent variable, and contact frequency scores as the independent variable. Tables 13 and 14 present sta— tistics for all subjects regardless of treatment. In other words, all mothers were considered as one group; and then approximately 25 per cent of the total sample who had the highest intensity scores were compared with approximately 25 per cent of the mothers who had the lowest intensity scores on the EDP scale. The highly significant P (P-< .005) indicates that frequency of contact with emotionally dis- turbed persons does contribute to real differences in in- tensity of attitudes. But high frequency of contact did not produce higher intensity scores. The mean of EDP in- tensity scores for the low frequency contact group is larger than the one obtained by the high frequency contact or vu- «‘5‘ . A .Vv..- (U (D ('1‘ :5 (D ( 11) (“f (D (D ) 137 TABLE l3.--Means and standard deviations of intensity scores on the attitude-toward-emotionally-disturbed- persons (EDP) scale comparing high and low frequency of contact with emotionally disturbed persons for the total sample.1 Mean of EDP Standard Variable N Intensity Scale Deviation High frequency of contact 35 50.171 4.098 Low frequency of contact 44 52.818 3.405 Total 79 51.646 3.932 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. TABLE l4.--Ana1ysis of variance of the EDP intensity scores comparing high and low frequency of contact wit? emotionally disturbed persons for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square E of E Between categories 136.559 1 136.559 9.832 0.005 Within categories 1069.517 77 13.890 Total 1206.076 78 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. QfiAHB - Luau.“ ., a t b . arr .A 2 \f audio Ufi~ hr.» by. :AAV‘iQQ :v IUV‘UU “- "“f‘ *K O-! z....y Hevfi 2...“, \ 3'... ou‘v'uu- v 85"“H‘r "d4‘vy ' "n 1: . in» .K“ : ‘JF'A. A 0¢v..1‘ c- 138 group. Hence, Hzlal cannot be considered confirmed for the total sample. Zero-order correlations between contact and intensity scores are presented in Table 15. No significant relation- ship between contact with emotionally disturbed persons and intensity of attitudes toward them is observed in the total sample; yet the direction of relationship is negative. This contact with EDP is also not related to intensity scores on the HP scale, or intensity scales of progressive and tradi- tional attitudes toward education. H:1a2: Mothers of emotionally disturbed children will have greater intensity of attitude toward emotionally disturbed persons than will the mothers of physically handicapped or non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. The intensity scores of the three groups of mothers (EDP, HP, and Non—HP) were compared and analyzed. Mean differences, standard deviations, and 3 statistics are reported in Tables 16 and 17. There exists a highly significant difference (P < .005) between the three re- spondent groups. Mean rankings, however, are not con— sistent with the hypothesis in that the EDP group has a lower mean score than the Non—HP group. Although the significant 3 statistic supports the hypothesis that the treatment means do not come from a common population, it is still necessary to consider the relationships between pairs of means to conclude which -.-.¢- .. _‘ . . _- pr.-.— 0, - m:— ... , 1 0.- 0 . ‘ —p 0 .H. 139 TABLE 15.--Zero-order correlations between contact and intensity scores on the attitude scales for the three respondent groups and the total sample. EDP hP ED ED Intensity Intensity Progressive uraditional Group1 Variab1e2 scale 0ca1e Ingensity Intensity cale scale N r 1 r N r 1 r EDP EDP contact 48 -0.189 48 -0.091 47 0.040 47 -0.011 HP contact 41 0.064 41 -0.302 41 -0.151 41 -0.138 ED contact 16 0.237 16 0.162 16 0.456 16 0.174 HP EDP contact 18 0.306 18 0.344 16 0.701** 16 0.242 HP contact 36 -0.346* 36 -0.038 34 -0.227 34 -0.260 ED contact 34 0.099 14 0.235 14 0.268 14 0.020 Non-HP EDP contact 30 0.266 31 0.267 31 0.215 31 0.101 HP contact 51 —0.010 51 -0.049 52 -0.073 52 -0.022 ED contact 40 -0.160 40 0.070 41 -0.016 41 0.015 Total EDP contact 96 -0.062 97 0.074 94 0.110 94 0.031 HP contact 128 -0.175* 128 —0.222* 127 -0.094 127 -0.0th ED contact 70 0.024 70 0.154 71 0.138 71 0.043 1 EDP - Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP - Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP I Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 2EDP contact - contact with emotionally disturbed persons. HP contact I contact with physically handicapped persons. ED contact = contact with education. *P < .05 "P < .01 :w —- #- VI-AV ' d . H. :v «N. : . .H .. M... I. . Cw . . g .H H w». n H H 140 TABLE 16.-—Means and standard deviations of intensity scores on the attitude-toward-emotionally—disturbed— persons (EDP) scale for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of EDP Variable Group N Intensity Standard Scale Deviation Intensity of EDP 55 60.454 7.042 attitude toward HP 45 55.822 5.730 emotionally Non-HP 66 63.667 6.316 disturbed persons Total 166 60.476 7.119 Ranking of means: Non-HP (63.667) > EDP (60.454) > HP (55.822) Duncan's Test Results: Non-HP > HP; Non-HP > EDP; EDP > HP lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. TABLE l7.--Ana1ysis of variance of EDP intensity scores for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 1646.523 2 823.261 19.984 0.005 Within categories 6714.880 163 41.196 Total 8361.403 165 ‘ o ‘r A: e .4 '1‘. 4t“ ‘3 1) ‘ 3 A Irv “30‘-- d” v ._ v." 3,”. . .I‘JA “a-.. .. w p‘n'. ~Y,!\ a any.‘ ‘. ‘ VJ A.'£h a, V'u.. -. ‘ I A: ’3 1‘. “-J ‘- . .. ~ In “C. v.“‘\‘. u "2* a: 30“. V. I”, "v“- v.“ 141 of the treatment means is the primary source of variance. For this a test of multiple mean comparisons seems de- sirable. Several posteriori tests have been developed to analyze the variance between three or more means where the overall F is significant. In this study, Duncan's New Multiple Range Test, as modified by Kramer (1956) for unequal replications is used. Table 18 reports the findings of the Duncan's test. Each group differs significantly (P . .01) from every other in respect to intensity of attitude toward emotion- ally disturbed persons. Mothers of emotionally disturbed children do not have greater intensity of attitude than that of the mothers of normal children. However, the EDP mothers do score higher than the HP mothers who obtained lowest intensity score on the EDP scale. Therefore, H:1a2 is not considered to be confirmed. The correlational analysis shown in Table 15 also does not permit the inference that there is a linear relationship between EDP contact and intensity since the correlation coefficients in all the three groups are non-significant. However, the correlation between con- tact with emotionally disturbed persons and intensity of progressive attitude toward education is found to be highly significant (P < .01) in mothers of handicapped children. 0.04 r” Q 3 _ . _ G. L. _ . . . ~03 .dJ fl 0 M . - — r£ rd. Y.” ”a n.v A v — C. .- H—‘v .A.‘ . A. fl - D . 0 fl 0 3m pox». . . o . . 3“ . n p. .i}. .d. ‘ t“ J 142 TABLE 18.--Duncan's New Multiple Range Test applied to means of EDP intensity scale for the three respondent groups. Range of Means (p) 2 3 df=l63 Studentized ranges1 for a = .05 (Zp, df=l63) 2.772 2.918 for a = .01 (Zp, df=l63) 3.643 3.796 -----—-—-----~—-—------_---—------------——-_-------_m----- Mean Differences3 :- __ = *‘H‘ XNon—HP XHP (p 3) 57.423 ‘ _ — = xx XNon-HP XEDP (p 2) 24.888 — _ ‘ = ** XEDP XHP (p 2) 32.591 lSignificant studentized ranges for Duncan's new multiple range test with a equal to .05 and .01 taken from Edwards (1965, pp. 373—74). 2p = the range of means (2 and 3). s the square root of the error mean square of the analysis of variance of Table 17. Thus, s=/Hl.196 = 6.42 3Mean differences of columns 2 and 3 have been transformed into the equivalent of t - scores for multiple means. To be significant, the figure must exceed the R'p value of the same column. The formula given by Kramer (1956) is: _ _ — __X__£_ , z ' (Xy XZ) n + n szp, error df of A. of V. ( R \ ,\ I P . ~ rffalfi;v naudgw a- ”'8‘: ”Fr "4- UV:- "i. r "a. “53::“fir “"V‘ou.. Cd" “07“:- V‘ vvug’: .3 § ~ 0 ‘ \ QR ,- ".UJ.‘ ‘ 1: V‘n . a“ g: o. “a ‘tu. y'f’ns‘ ‘1 ‘Ur‘icg‘ 9“ Ii'V-v. ’1‘} .. r'oi '_I— ' ‘ l f 143 H:1a3: The more frequent the contact with physically handicapped persons, the higher will be the scores on the intensity statements of the attitude-toward-handi— cappedgpersons (HP) scale, regardless of whether atti- tude content is favorable or unfavorable. This hypothesis was tested by dividing the entire research sample into two groups-—one having high frequency of contact with handicapped persons and the other con- sisted of mothers who had low frequency of contact. There is no significant difference between the HP intensity means of the two groups as indicated in Tables 19 and 20. This hypothesis is not supported. It may be pointed out that the absolute value of the mean of the low contact group is higher than the high contact group, which is similar to the findings of Hzlal. This difference was significant at the .10 level of confi- dence suggesting thereby a further verification of the hypothesis with the help of a large sample. In the entire sample, a significant relationship (P < .05) exists between contact with handicapped persons and intensity statements on the EDP and HP scales. The direction of relationship is, however, negative (see Table 15). Thus the correlational analysis supports this hYpothesis in an inverse manner. High frequency of con- tact with physically handicapped persons produces less intense attitudes toward both disabled and disturbed persons. -.-on 1 _i u ‘_ cud-lad ‘4 :‘Mo: ' HIV-Uh: H . g 1 (In I ,. p O f" .08 Vt ‘ I “'IIAI 5,- \ t- u..l~-v.~- a U _. ———H 0A.. fi'n - uvh i. '“ flhy ) D0) ~l“ v. .- .“HHU ‘ Gr», 5 l H'.n~;-“ d H- 144 TABLE 19.—-Means and standard deviations of intensity scores on the attitude-toward—handicapped—persons (HP) scale comparing high and low frequency of contact with physically handicapped persons for the total sample.1 Mean of EDP Standard Variable N Intensity Scale Deviation High frequency of contact 55 60.364 6.174 Low frequency of contact 73 62.493 8.180 Total 128 61.578 7.434 1Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. TABLE 20.--Analysis of variance of HP intensity scores comparing high and low frequency of contact with physically handicapped persons for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. of Variance Squares Freedom Square F F Between categories 142.245 1 142.245 2.606 0.10 Within categories 6876.974 126 54.579 Total 7019.219 127 1Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. a rnr r — A coil" .3 ~ :5 u i.- .F‘ng‘ Vvtov. w 1.3 —fi\0r. bung“. p ,- CE.‘ 9. a. 2.. ‘1 Ah“. _ 5‘ Var V . E "P'Ws. 'VA._' ‘.' c‘xv“ Grp v . 145 H:1au: Mothers of physically handicapped children will have greater intensity of attitude toward physical handi- cap than will the mothers of emotionally disturbed or non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. As indicated in Tables 21 and 22, the means of HP intensity scores for the three groups of mothers differ significantly (P < .005). The highly significant F suggests that the group means do not come from a common population. But the direction of the difference does not support the hypothesis. Ranking of means reveal that mothers of normal children have the highest intensity scores followed by the mothers of emotionally disturbed children. Mothers of handicapped children express least amount of intensity in respect to attitudes toward physi- cally handicapped persons. However, the Duncan's test (Table 23) does not indicate a statistically significant difference between the control group and the EDP mothers' group. No prediction can be made about intensity of atti- tude toward the handicapped on the basis of closeness of contact (see Table 15), since there is a non-significant correlation between these two variables. Interestingly enough, a significant negative relationship is noted be- tween HP contact and EDP intensity scores. E“ ‘1. or} ‘ ““-u 146 TABLE 21.--Means and standard deviations of intensity scores on the attitude-toward-handicapped-persons (HP) scale for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of HP Variable Group N Intensity Standard Scale Deviation (Total) Intensity of EDP 55 62.818 9.284 attitude toward HP 45 56.955 5.013 physically Non-HP 66 63.000 6.187 handicapped persons Total 166 61.301 7.554 Ranking of means: Duncans test results: Non—HP(63.000) > EDP(62.818) > (HP(56.955) Non-HP > HP; EDP . HP lEDP = HP = children. Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non—HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) TABLE 22.--Ana1ysis of variance of HP intensity scores for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of 3 Between categories 1166.847 2 583.423 11.530 0.005 Within categories 8248.093 163 50.602 Total 9414.940 165 _ all-lou- .- ‘I‘ l / IJIU‘“ 6 AA A A1 'u.” “in”: v. . .Fv'oc A( H “ans. V] ‘ y. ..y \ "’v “"Ivcsv. ' - v- -1. _ .. y o o h . ’:;V‘ H “'1... ‘ u .. V 2.. .. V‘ AQV‘.-:: ‘ ‘y 3 no A, ‘_ "'y.‘-‘ ‘ w 1 A “q“ ' .1 H" 147 TABLE 23.--Duncan's New Multiple Range Test applied to means of HP intensity scale for the three respondent groups. Range of Means (p) 2 3 df=l63 Studentized ranges1 for a = .05 (Zp,df=163) 2.772 2.918 for a = .01 (Zp,df=l63) 3.643 3.796 R'p )R'= szp, error df of A. of V. (=R'p) W ‘ u.-u- ' ‘pz alt-Q“ ' .‘pr-It'- . —- fife Ln du-v Hw‘ dict .‘ b “- Q our)“ C. v “0 1 148 Hzlbl: The more frequent the contact with education,_the higher will be the scores on the intensity statements of the Education Scale, regardless of whether attitude is traditional or progressive. The statistical analyses presented in Tables 24-27 indicate that the mean differences between persons with high and low contact with education, are not significantly different on either progressive or traditional intensity scores. Thus the data do not support the hypothesis. The zero-order correlations given in Table 15 also suggest that there is no significant relationship between educational contact and intensity of attitudes toward education or toward emotionally disturbed or handicapped persons . H:1b2: Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children will have greater intensity of atti- tude toward education (traditional andprogressive) than will the mothers of non—handicapped (i.e., normal) children. The non-significant F statistics (Tables 28-31) do not support the hypothesis that close contact with dis- turbed and disabled persons tends to enhance the intensity of attitude toward education. This finding is consistent with the results obtained in correlational analysis (Table 15) in that the correlation coefficients for con- tact with education and intensity scores on the attitude scales are not significant. ".5“ l. .‘a Mr 4. yg.l ~ ’ A A v r“: "a ... "1'“ AA' J. Vv. Afi, .I/d:; \ r? 7‘s V. '.~ 149 TABLE 24.--Means and standard deviations of intensity scores on the progressive-attitude-toward-education scale comparing high and low frequency of contact with education for the total sample.1 Mean of Variable N Progressive Standard Intensity Scale Deviation High frequency of contact 46 32.652 2.906 Low frequency of contact 25 32.240 3.515 Total 71 32.507 3.116 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. TABLE 25.--Analysis of variance of intensity scores on the progressive-attitude-toward-education scale comparing high and low frequency of contact with education for the total sample.l Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 2.752 1 2.752 0.280 0.60 Within categories 676.995 69 9.811 Total 679.647 70 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless Of treatment. gut-on ‘! .4 u “may nqA 5 \: UtVov fl (\I‘f V we”. ’fi'v 9,,1 av“ ‘1’: “0 fiAy— ‘e V‘v.. - .Ao.‘ g. vi‘.‘ - «'0 37%;» V‘ U.~: In.“ ;~'? . .A.-~_ ‘ U I" T“, 0‘ U. c ‘u 150 TABLE 26.--Means and standard deviations of intensity scores on the traditional-attitude-toward-education scale comparing high and low frequency of contact with education for the total sample. Mean of Variable N Traditional Standard Intensity Scale Deviation High frequency of contact 46 32.978 3.249 Low frequency of contact 25 33.640 3.108 Total 71 33.211 3.193 1 of treatment. Total sample refers to all respondents regardless TABLE 27.--Analysis of variance of intensity scores on the traditional-attitude-toward-education scale comparing high and low frequency of contact w total sample. ith education for the Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 7.093 1 7.093 0.692 0.41 Within categories 706.738 69 10.243 Total 713.831 70 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless Of treatment. I In " ““qu C- I- r nag-”9‘ II. we . "A“: :1. L F . ‘08 .au- . .‘ a s .. P cr- vVuv".g Ffia‘.n;: gotta. VH n..{o.. cuu‘.‘ . ‘fi'vfifin avid. ,‘ 94“,,ggg vuu.u,- fr ._ ’0’ fir-1 .‘an v..--.‘. '0. V‘J‘TCG '- "r lzrn 151 TABLE 28.--Means and standard deviations of content scores on the progressive-attitude-toward-education scale for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of Variable Group‘ N Progressive Standard Content Scale Deviation Content of EDP 55 28.436 3.120 progressive HP 43 28.744 3.001 attitude Non—HP 68 28.176 3.507 toward education Total 166 28.410 3.244 1 EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. ‘TABLE 29.--Ana1ysis of variance of content scores on the progressive-attitude-toward-education scale for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 8.549 2 4.274 0.403 0.67 Within categories 1727.596 163 10.599 Total 1736.145 165 .‘I'H n in Jun“; - . (5r ‘ no \n- a... A 9" r5 “Juan... . U‘"n:# vi 5.. ‘h . “J r "% ~ ‘.A“~h § n .‘;W:I 152 TABLE 30.-—Means and standard deviations of content scores on the traditional-attitude-toward-education scale for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of Variable Group N Traditional Standard Content Scale Deviation Content of EDP 55 29.436 3.414 traditional HP 43 28.512 3.418 attitude Non-HP 68 29.176 3.494 toward education Total 166 29.090 3.446 lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = children. Mothers of non—handicapped (i.e., normal) TABLE 31.--Ana1ysis of variance of content scores on the traditional-attitude-toward-education scale for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. “Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 21.491 2 10.745 0.904 0.41 Within categories 1938.154 163 11.891 Total 1959.645 166 . "F7 " 7' fl \ UA-‘L' 00A- 4 I .. . :5 P)‘" it. 1 V O \ ‘ A .Ouvgyln ring- .- “GA-c . k I ‘v . Q9,- \- '- V‘. ’53 153 Summary of Contact and Attitude Intensity Variables The analyses of the data do not provide results in the direction of the hypotheses. Neither of the included hypotheses was confirmed. In fact, some hypotheses were confirmed inversely. Less frequent contact rather than high frequency of contact with emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons produced significantly greater intensity of attitude on the EDP and HP scales. This would appear a special case requiring further exami- nation and will be discussed in the following chapter. H:2: Contact—Frequency Interactions H:2al: High frequency of contact with emotionally dis— turbedgperscns will lead to favorable attitudes if high frequency is concurrent with (a) alternative rewarding gpportunities, (b) enjoyment of contact,_and (c) ease of avoidance of contact. Total Sample.--As indicated by Table 32, the multiple correlation relating to the combined contact variables and favorableness of attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons is significant at the .01 level of confidence in 'the total sample. Because of a very small number, 'alter- native to contact' was not included in this analysis. Table 32 also points out that amount of contact and enjoyment of contact when partialled out contribute mOst to predicting attitudes toward disturbed persons. Since high scores on the EDP content and HP content scales . .d-_ 1r.»4 «5‘ uni-VIII AI mud vvu I 1.H\ny.h lttdy‘... Po; ‘ N v . i 154 indicate negative attitudes, the direction of the partial correlations have negative signs. H:2a is, thus, con- 1 sidered confirmed for the total sample. TABLE 32.--Partia1 and multiple correlations between con— tact variables (in respect to the emotionally disturbed) and content of attitude toward emotionally disturbed per- sons (EDP scale)1 in the total sample.2 EDP Contact Variab1e3 N = 90 Partial Correlation Amount of contact -0.264** Avoidance of contact 0.191 Enjoyment of contact -0.296** Multiple correlation R = 0.497**' 1 Low scores on EDP content scale indicate positive attitudes. 2Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. 3EDP contact = Contact with emotionally disturbed persons. ** P < .01 Partial and multiple correlations were also obtained between combined EDP contact variables and attitudes toward physically handicapped persons (Table 33). Consistent with the above findings, the multiple correlation is highly Significant (P < .01). However, unlike the previous case, enJoyment of contact did not contribute to the prediction “”5"?" C...“ in.“ any-Uh, “I vutodav I'MA-le. ‘1‘“V-Andt P'VAJ 4’ ”Hum.“ ---~~ "- “| c.“" ag‘:‘- H u"‘~ (I) (1) 'r1 ( p *1 m 155 of favorableness of attitude. On the otherhand, besides amount of contact, the significant variable associated with attitude on the HP content scale was avoidance of contact in the total sample. TABLE 33.--Partia1 and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to the emotionally dis- turbed) and content of attitude toward physically handi— capped persons (HP scale)1 in the total sample.2 EDP Contact Variable3 N=90 Partial Correlation Amount of contact -O.279** Avoidance of contact -0.307** Enjoyment of contact ~0.l60 Multiple correlation R=0.369** lLow scores on HP Content scale indicate positive attitudes. 2Total sample refers to all respondents regard— less of treatment. 3EDP contact = contact with emotionally disturbed persons. **P < .01 Respondent Groups.--When the three respondent groups are Compared with each other in respect to combined EDP contact Variables and attitudes toward the handicapped, signifi- Cant multiple and partial correlations are found only for the mothers of emotionally disturbed children (Table 34). :wt 44" nfir'" v'vt' "‘ FY. van-I L V‘ re... .- udu'v ‘. . 0"“, 1 v..;la 156 But avoidance of contact when partialled out does not contribute significantly to the multiple correlation. TABLE 34.-—Partial and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to the emotionally disturbed) and con— tent of attitude toward physically handicapped persons (HP Scale)l in the three respondent groups. Partial Correlation EDP Contact Variables EDP HP Non—HP N=45 N=l7 N=28 Amount of contact —0.476** —0.354 -0.230 Avoidance of contact —0.218 -0.239 -0.275 Enjoyment of contact —0.381* -0.231 0.184 Multiple Correlation R=0.532** R=0.4OO R=O.353 lLow scores on HP content scale indicate positive attitudes. 2EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal children. 3EDP contact = Contact with emotionally disturbed persons. *P < .05 **P < .01 -'/2 ' nit-a ' . Frn v 1'. .1 S 5.... I»: u "c. $..' P . ..:v~ ‘¢“ V1. '4). P. Arc. 157 H:2a2: Mothers of emotionally disturbed children will have more positive attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons than will the mothers of physically handicapped or non—handicapped (i.e., normal) children. This hypothesis is confirmed. The mean differences and standard deviations are reported in Table 35. Ana- lysis of variance and Duncan's test results are presented in Tables 36 and 37. The absolute value of the mean for the EDP group is the lowest which indicates that mothers of emotionally disturbed children have more positive atti- tudes toward the disturbed compared to the other two groups of mothers. However, Duncan's test reveals that there is no significant difference between mothers of handicapped and normal children in this regard. H:2a3: High frequency of contact withpphysically handi— capped persons will lead to favorable attitudes if high fpequency is concurrent with (a) alternative rewarding gpportunities, (b) enjoyment of contact, and (c) ease of avoidance of contact. Total Sample.--The relationship between the com- bined contact variables and favorable attitudes toward .handicapped persons is found to be highly significant (P < .01), as shown by the multiple correlation (Table 38). However, none of the individual contact variables contributed significantly to this correlation. One con— tact variable, alternative rewarding Opportunities could "Av-b V. v‘v‘uv... .OJ.. 0 c..l. 5Acvr y “4'41. ppafi . vm‘v ‘v - list. ”Onto 5'08» '1' 3 ( Q . . Ari v..‘-‘. “Us. ‘vv‘.’ hl‘. - “.~"‘H 158 TABLE 35.--Means and standard deviations of content scores on the attitude-toward-emotionally-disturbed-persons (EDP) scale for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of Variable Group N EDP2 Standard Content Scale Deviation Content of EDP 55 50.818 3.806 attitude HP 45 52.689 4.106 toward Non—HP 66 52.576 3.823 emotionally disturbed Total 166 52.024 persons Ranking of Means: HP(52.689) > Non-HP(52.576) > EDP(50.818) Duncan's test results: HP > EDP; Non—HP > EDP lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 2Low scores on EDP content scale indicate positive attitudes. TABLE 36.-—Analysis of variance of EDP content scores for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 119.956 2 59.978 3.952 0.02 Within categories 2473.947 163 15.178 Total 2593.903 165 P.0lvfi .1 II .a‘n-Jud .F F": u» .4. and! r fl . ‘fi..nero UV“ 0. “on iv. a .v. 3 H A . '9'." fl “3...; u 159 TABLE 37.--Duncan's New Multiple Range Test applied to means of EDP content scale for the three respondent groups. Range of Means (p) 2 3 df=l63 Studentized ranges1 for a = .05 (Zp, df=163> 2.772 2.918 for a = .01 (Zp’ df=l53) 3.643 3 795 Mean differences XHP — XEDP (p=3) 13.164* XHP ‘ XNon—HP (p=2) '827 XNon—HP ‘ XEDP (p=2) 13’617* lSignificant studentized ranges for Duncan's new multiple range test with a equal to .05 and .01 taken from Edwards (1965, pp. 373-74). 2p = the range of means (2 and 3). s = the square root of the error mean square of the analysis of variance of Table 36. Thus, 3 = JIETI7E = 3.90 3Mean differences of columns 2 and 3 have been transformed into the equivalent of £-score for multiple means. To be significant, the figure must exceed the R'p Value of the same column. The formula given by Kramer (1956) is: n — _ _ ._L_z__ = ' (Xy XZ) n + n > szp, error df of A. of V. ( R p) c' ('7‘ (I- Z) F's. F ; I 0.1.14“ h, 4- ‘ ,0. 'avv . gr; A. an; ‘VV i”"“r- oudy“." !'~'f\‘! ;. -H — uvv_‘“ H - AY'w u'H‘Ji.‘ "“u 7", I."‘|¥-~ . 160 not be included in the analysis on account of insufficient number of cases. H:2a3 is considered confirmed for the total sample. TABLE 38.--Partial and multiple correlations between con— tact variables (in respect to the physically handicapped) and content of attitude toward physically handicapped persons (HP scale)1 in the total sample.2 HP Contact Variable3 N=1l9 Partial Correlation Amount of contact -0.179 Avoidance of contact —0.119 Enjoyment of contact -0.180 Multiple Correlation R=O.279** lLow scores on HP content scale indicate positive attitudes. 2Total sample refers to all respondents regard- less of treatment. 3HP Contact = Contact with physically handicapped persons. **P < .01 Correlational analysis was also done to determine relationship between HP contact variables and attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons. Table 39 which presents this analysis reveals that no such relationship exists in the total sample. 161 TABLE 39.--Partia1 and multiple correlations between con— tact variables (in respect to the physically handicapped) and content of attitude toward emotionally disturbed per- sons (EDP Scale)1 in the total sample.2 HP Contact Variable3 N=119 Partial Correlation Amount of contact 0.019 Avoidance of contact 0.022 Enjoyment of contact 0.077 Multiple correlation R=0.086 lLow scores on EDP content scale indicate positive attitudes. 2Total sample refers to all respondents regard— less of treatment. 3HP Contact = contact with physically handicapped persons. Respondent Groups.--The multiple correlation be- tween the combined contact variables and attitudes toward physically handicapped persons is significant (P < .01) for the mothers of emotionally disturbed and handicapped children (Table 40). H:2a3 is, therefore, partially con- firmed in the three respondent groups. A further analysis exploring relationship between combined HP contact variables and attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons yielded no significant results in any of the three groups of mothers (Table 41). 2g r“ run so F. 0 Av w .. HS 0. e r H. a; n . 1,1". 1L; a 0.- al. A 5.] hard. 162 TABLE 40.--Partial and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to the physically handi- capped) and content of attitude toward physically handi— capped persons (HP Scale)1 in the three respondent groups.2 Partial Correlation giriifliifit EDP HP Non-HP N=60 N=48 N=69 Amount of contact —0.l33 —O.l86 —0.058 Avoidance of contact -O.236 -0.055 -0.102 Enjoyment of contact -0.320* -0.050 -0.044 Multiple Correlation R=0.452** R=O.335* R=0.127 1Low scores on HP content scale indicate positive attitudes. 2EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 3HP contact = contact with physically handicapped persons. *P < .05 **P < .01 1r] anolu '- 0". Vu‘ . : R§‘1‘. C...“ ‘51-} s ‘ H..gi‘ 163 TABLE 4l.--Partial and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to the physically handi- capped) and content of attitude toward emotionally dis- turbed persons (EDP scale)1 in the three respondent groups. Partial Correlation HP Contact Variab1e3 EDP HP Non-HP N=60 N=48 N=69 Amount of contact -0.159 -0.049 0.220 Avoidance of contact 0.045 0.104 -0.l29 Enjoyment of contact -0.080 0.126 0.009 JMultiple Correlation R=0.179 R=0.24l R=0.268 lLow scores on EDP content scale indicate positive attitudes. 2EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non—handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 3HP contact = contact with physically handicapped persons. I (n v... -‘y “An- .‘ V“; w't'» ‘v‘. r-& “I".I" flag ‘ F vc“ g i: "n H “‘ PA . ‘ N. ‘ \ 13 y.‘ . (2‘1" - ~ — m H Hod; 0‘ . (U 317 ‘3 (D 164 H:2a”: Mothers ofpppysically handicapped children will have more positive attitudes towardpphysically handicapped pprsons than will the mothers of emotionally disturbed or non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. The data presented in Tables 42-44 support the hy- pothesis. Mothers of physically handicapped children have the lowest mean score which indicates that they ex- pressed the greatest amount of positive attitude toward the handicapped. The mean differences between the three groups are significant at the .03 level of confidence (Table 43). As revealed by Duncan's test (Table 44), attitudes of the control group mothers do not differ from those of the mothers of handicapped children. H:2b: High frequency of contact with education will lead to favorable attitudes if high frequenpy is concurrent with (a) alternative rewarding opportunities, (b) enjoy-l ment of the contact, and (C) ease of avoidance of contact. Total Sample.--The multiple correlation (Table 45) indicates that the correlation between progressive edu- cational attitudes and the combined contact variables is not significant. Similarly, Table 46 shows there is 110 significant correlation between the combined contact ‘variables and traditional attitudes toward education. fiz2b is not confirmed for the total sample. Respondent Groups.—-When mothers of three groups arercompared in terms of the relationship between combined I“. F H ‘ “Fun-“.4 AAA“; vvc 0 loan: P la. ‘0- 0 F1. “1. H 5“:. navy" 165 TABLE 42.--Means and standard deviations of content scores on the attitude-toward-handicapped persons (HP) scale for the three respondent groups. 1 2 Variable Group N Mean of HP Standard Content Scale Deviation Content of EDP 55 46.582 4.475 attitude HP 45 44.022 6.218 toward Non—HP 66 44.651 5.027 physically handicapped Total 166 45.036 5.383 persons Ranking of means: EDP(46.582) > Non-HP(44.651) > HP(44.022) Duncan's test results: EDP > HP; EDP > Non-HP 1EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non—HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 2Low scores on HP content scale indicate positive attitudes. TABLE 43.—-Ana1ysis of variance of HP content scores for the three respondent groups. .Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. 'Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 186.246 2 93.123 3.430 0.03 Within categories 4425.344 163 27.149 Total 4611.590 165 ‘1 J n 1 F. v.‘ f! E .fl-h‘ -----1 r'“’ Q0” Qg‘dvl q u . . I o.IU S S a. to H. . .rn rim mu C a /( a: 3‘. «04/ 3. H A T. . v . I I: u 166 TABLE 44.--Duncan's New Multiple Range Test applied to means of HP content scale for the three respondent groups. Range of Means (p) 2 3 df=l63 Studentized ranges1 for d = .05 (Zp, df=l63) 2.772 2.918 for a = .01 (zp, df=l63) 3.643 3.796 _---_-______-_-----__-___g_; _______________________________ R'p [R' = (s)(zp,df=l63)] = .05 14.442 15.203 a = .01 18.980 19.777 Mean differences3 EEDP - XHP (p=3) 18.012* ifEDP ' ifNon—HP (p=2) 14.958* iHon—HP ' 3EHP (p=2) “'601 1 Significant studentized ranges for Duncan's new multiple range test with a equal to .05 and .01 taken from Edwards (1965. pp. 373-74). 2p the range of means (2 and 3). s the square root of the error mean square of the analysis of variance Table 43. Thus, 8 = V27.149 = 5.210 3Mean differences of columns 2 and 3 have been 'transformed into the equivalent of p - scores, for multiple Ineans. To be significant, the figure must exceed the R' ‘Value of the same column. The formula given by Kramer (1956) is: 2n n — _ — __y__§_. = ' (Xy XZ) ny + nz > szp, error df of A. of V. ( R p) *P < .05 phfi" 7‘ I R - ‘I-ou-g'. ”fly-‘- \\\ .., a“ {1‘ “‘1 v‘. 167 contact variables and progressive attitudes toward edu— cation, significant multiple correlation is observed for the HP and Non-HP groups (Table 47). Although the multiple correlation is significant at the .01 level of confidence for the HP group, none of the three contact variables contributed significantly individually to this multiple correlation. In the control group, enjoyment of contact, when partialled out contributed significantly to the multiple correlation. TABLE 45.--Partial and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to education) and content of progressive attitudes toward education in the total sample.l ED Contact Variable2 N=42 Partial Correlation Amount of contact 0.260 Enjoyment of contact -0.233 Alternatives to contact -0.l27 Multiple Correlation R=O.300 ¥ lTotal Sample refers to all respondents regard- less of treatment. 2ED Contact = Contact with education. H... ."l‘ .114...“ my“ ; Hunt.- at ‘r. 434»: I. -U‘ 2? ‘.'n v. V“ ‘5 4. v I :‘m‘ v-5” (9"; C...“ 8“" Hive P. A 168 TABLE 46.--Partial and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to education) and content of traditional-attitudes—toward-education in the total sample. ED Contact Variable2 N=42 Partial Correlation Amount of contact 0.106 Enjoyment of contact 0.028 Alternatives to contact -0.098 Multiple Correlation R=0.154 lTotal Sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. 2ED Contact = Contact with education. Insofar as relationship of combined contact variables to traditional attitudes toward education is concerned, significant multiple correlations are found for the EDP and HP groups (Table 48). When partialled out, alterna- tives to contact, contributed most to this relationship in the EDP group. Notwithstanding the fact that significant multiple correlations are obtained for the respondent groups, the directions of partial correlations are not consistent With the hypothesis. In reSpect to progressive attitudes toward education, H:2b cannot be considered confirmed, Since the majority of the partial correlation coefficients are negative. However, large amount of negative partial IIIr—~r———————————————————______________________________________________________________________________________________________________________' i —.—v* .544“ ’) (in I» L) T1 (f ' S H‘ 1wfi‘ln v’ CA..¢J.A.. \.f..‘1 ‘- : "ide— \ i 1; t... ,_ 169 TABLE 47.--Partia1 and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to education) and content of progressive—attitudes-toward-education in the three respondent groups. Partial Correlation Egagiggigt EDP HP Non—HP N=60 N=48 N=69 Amount of contact -0.031 0.133 0.202 Enjoyment of contact 0.102 —0.187 —0.251* Alternatives to contact -0.l24 0.157 -0.172 Multiple Correlation R=0.l46 R=O.388** R=O.303* lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children Non—HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 2ED contact = contact with education. *P < .05 **P < .01 170 TABLE 48.--Partial and multiple correlations between contact variables (in respect to education) and content of traditional-attitudes-toward-education in the three respondent groups. Partial Correlation ED Contact Variables2 EDP HP Non-HP N=6O N=48 N=69 Amount of contact 0.077 —0.279 0.078 Enjoyment of contact -0.039 0.053 0.063 Alternatives to contact -0.332* 0.098 0.050 Multiple Correlation R=O.348** R=O.309* R=0.l38 lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. 2ED Contact = Contact with education. *P < .05 **P < .01 a a. a. B a. w w e 4 3 VJ ”H w. r.“ n . . . 4a ”4 fly I. 0. By 2.. all. «0 . »1 RD .3 V. facts; flflr‘h Vilnova» . 0..“ r . "Q Ve: t: We 4} n*‘ c.““h ‘ » h v 171 coefficients obtained for traditional attitudes toward education lend some support to this hypothesis. Summary of Contact and Attitude Content Variables Most hypotheses under this category have been con- firmed by the data. Total Sample.--High frequency of contact with emotion- ally disturbed persons is noted to be related to favorable- ness of attitudes toward the disturbed. Amount of contact and enjoyment of contact appear to be the most significant factors responsible for positive attitudes toward emotion- ally disturbed persons. The combined contact variables in respect to emotionally disturbed are also found to be re- lated to favorableness of attitudes toward physically handicapped persons. In similar vein, the combined HP contact variables are related to favorable attitudes toward the disabled. Positive attitudes toward emotionally dis- turbed persons are not associated with the combined HP contact variables. No significant relationship is observed between progressive and traditional attitudes toward education and combined contact variables concerning education in the total sample. Respondent Groups.--Mothers of emotionally disturbed children are found to have more favorable attitudes toward the disturbed than those of mothers of physically handi- capped or normal children. Similarly more positive . .4" ‘ Vufia- I- z 5".“ Jung. u 3th m 172 attitudes toward the disabled were expressed by HP mothers in comparison to EDP mothers. But mothers of normal children did not differ significantly with HP mothers in their attitudes toward handicapped persons. As revealed by multiple and partial correlations, the combined EDP contact variables are observed to be related to favorable attitudes toward the handicapped only in the group of mothers of emotionally disturbed Children. Except the control group, mothers of emotionally disturbed and handicapped children revealed significant correlations between combined HP contact variables and positive atti- tudes toward the handicapped. However, no significant relationship is found between combined HP contact vari- ables and positive attitudes toward the emotionally disturbed in any of the three groups of mothers. Hypotheses Related to Attitude- ‘Value Interactions Iiz3a: Mothers who score high in need for power and control over others will tend to score low in acceptance. of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. This hypothesis was tested by means of analysis of ‘Variance for the entire sample which was divided into high arm.low groups on the basis of value scores on the Leader- Ship sub-scale. The results are reported in Tables 49—52. Zero-order correlations between the two variables have also been presented in Tables 53 and 54. - ‘F.‘ 0y. (1" NE A F“. a. .,, ”'0‘. .4 ‘nu. Ctr; \A.. (1’) 11’ 173 TABLE 49.-—Means and standard deviations of content scores on the attitude-toward-emotionally-disturbed- persons (EDP) scale comparing high and low scores on Leadership value for the total sample.1 Mean of EDP2 Standard Variable N Content Scale Deviation High scores on Leadership value 42 50.857 4.100 Low scores on Leadership value 73 52.726 4.032 Total 115 52.043 4.139 1Total sample refers to all respondents re- gardless of treatment. 2Low scores on EDP content scale indicate positive attitudes. TABLE 50.--Analysis of variance of EDP content scores comparing high and low scores on Leadership value for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 93.119 1 93.119 5.658 0.02 Within categories 1859.663 113 16.457 Total 1952.782 114 1 Total sample refers to all respondents re- gardless of treatment. n ’4)- (h (111 ‘\') t "X (D 3‘ $|‘ T‘ . owe... p 1 l 174 TABLE 51.--Means and standard deviations of content scores on the attitude—toward-handicapped-persons(HP) scale comparing high and low scores on Leadership value for the total sample.1 Mean of HP2 Standard Variable N Content Scale Deviation High scores on Leadership value 42 45.643 5.026 Low scores on Leadership value 73 44.753 5.484 Total 115 45.079 5.316 lTotal sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. 2Low scores on HP content scale indicate positive attitudes. TABLE 52.-—Analysis of variance of HP content scores comparing high and low scores on Leadership value for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 21.091 1 21.091 0.744 0.39 Within categories 3201.205 113 28.329 'Total 3222.296 114 chHCEHHHTA .and 4..£.+ “21:4 fiQaJOtnni JCCUCOQQOL .00..qu 03...» eHOLH meadwuomw mwBHuuxw £00403 02¢ 0C3 «.mdcwuHHOO £3va mulfiMwOmu Muconluuwdhhldvfle.-~ru.~29.4...HHJlnLAH.HMZLOH_JO:.0|mUnH.$30nwln.ufio~DJH-\w.u.m.~ nhfleflwSJWQ munkfivlfidmfimyHrHOU .HMUU.HOIIO.NUNII.MI».\. £1“.qu .. r. r 175 mo. v ma. .pcoEpmonu mo mmoHonmwon meCUCOQmop HH< n Hmuoem .cmnoHfizo AHmenoc «.m.HV voodoofiocmzlcoc mo mnocpoz u mmlcoz .smnoHano woodwofiocmz zHHmonzzd mo mnonpoz u mm .conoHano oonpzpmHo HHHmCOHpoEo mo mnonuoz n mam m .moosufippm o>fipmwoc DHMoHocH mopoom peopcoo mom zmfima mo0.0 mmH em0.0u m0 H0H.0 0m *e0N.0 Hm soHsooccoo 0H0.0 mmH 000.0 mm 00m.0 0m m0~.0: Hm cnooosm Hmo.on mmH moo.o| mm mmm.o: mm wao.o Hm mocoao>ocom 0HH.0 mmH mm0.0- m0 *mmm.0 0m 0HH.0 Hm coHpchooom HomH.OI mma :Hm.o| mm *Hmm;01 mm mmH.oI Hm magnetomoq L z a z u z m z mzlcoz mm mam Hopoe moam> m mQSOHw .oHdEmm Hmpou on» one masonw psoocoomop mops» on» now CHMom mzam> coopoo on» one HApzopnoo mmmv datum mcomnoQIUCDHSpmHUIHHHMCOHuoEmlpgmzoulmbzufiupm cmmzumn mCOHpmHmnaoo hmn&01ommwln.mm mqm COUCHOU mwgu USN...» HfiUZQQCOU .HHHV £|TL$30$I$UJGH¢OQ :003J03 QCOHQQHQLLOU EQULOIOLQNII.QA SQWQW 0.6.0.00» nnnhnvnu..HWQ'UUQQQC.r HUCQ 176 .conoHHno AHmEpoc «.o.HV woodwoaocmnlcoc no mponpoz .conoHHno omdomofiocmn zaamOHmmnd mo mnmnpoz .conoHfino omen3pmfio zHHMQOHpoEm Ho whonuoz .pcoEpwopp Ho mmoaopmwon meDUQOQmop HH< vm* Hoooem mmlcoz H mm momm .moozpaupm o>fiumwoc opmofiocfi mopoom pcopcoo mm nwfim H *zma.0 mma 020.0 mm *mmm.0 0m #:0N.0 0m zpfifihomcoo 0H0.01 NmH 0m0.0l m0 000.0! mm mmN.01 0m phoaasm mmo.0| mmH ©NH.0 mm *omm.0I mm 020.0 0m mocofio>mcmm 000.0 NmH 000.0I mm 0HH.0 mm w:a.0 0m Coapficmooom HH0.0 mmH 00H.0 mm 000.0 mm N0H.0I 0m Qanmhmcmoq n z A z A z a z mHmcoe dmucoz NMMoco mom osHo> .oHQsmm HMHOH mzu 0cm masonw qupCoamon woman can now canon moam> 200900 one 0cm HApnopcoo mmv mamom mcompoalooaamo“CatalohmzopICUSHHppm coozpon mcoapmHomHoo noonononowll.:m mqm m Hazonu .oHdsmm Hmuop on» one mononw unmoCOQmon omega on» pom onom osam> coopow can and Hocopcoov Coapmosoonopmsoulmoosufippmno>fimmopmopd coospon mcofiumfionpoo hoonononownn.mm mqm Hasono .oagsmm H0000 one 000 masonw 0000000000 00020 on» pom onom osHm> 000000 on» 000 Apcopcoov coapmoz0on0nmzounmo0suH0001H0000000mnp 0003000 mQOHpmaopnoo po0pouop0an.00 mam¢e 183 TABLE 61.--Means and standard deviations of content scores on the attitude—toward-emotionally—disturbed— persons (EDP) scale comparing high and low scores on Recognition value for the total sample.1 2 Mean of EDP Standard Variable N Content Scale Deviation High scores on Recognition value “9 52.143 H.067 Low scores on Recognition value 68 51.u71 4.005 Total 117 51.752 “.028 lTotal sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. 2Low scores on EDP content scale indicate positive attitudes. TABLE 62.--Analysis of variance of EDP content scores comparing high and low scores on Recognition value for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square 5 of E Between categories 12.871 1 12.871 0.792 0.38 Within categories 1868.9ul 115 16.252 Total 1881.812 116 A“ V . .z-. C nv‘ v- 5. h .v 184 TABLE 63.-—Means and standard deviations of content scores on the attitude-toward-handicapped-persons (HP) scale comparing high and low scores on Recognition value for the total sample. Mean of HP2 Standard Variable N Content Scale Deviation High scores on Recognition value 50 45.760 5.061 Low scores on Recognition value 68 45.073 4.614 Total 118 45.364 4.800 lTotal sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. 2Low scores on HP content scale indicate positive attitudes. TABLE 64.—-Analysis of variance of HP content scores comparing high and low scores on Recognition for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square E of 3 Between categories 13.578 1 13.578 0.587 0.45 Within ‘ categories 2681.752 116 23.119 Total 2695.330 117 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. 185 TABLE 65.--Means and standard deviations of content scores on the progressive-attitude-toward-education scale comparing high and low scores on Recognition value for the total sample. Mean of Variable N Progressive Standard Content Scale Deviation High scores on Recognition value 50 28.960 3.428 Low scores on Recognition value 66 28.000 2.845 Total 116 28.414 3.132 1 of treatment. Total sample refers to all respondents regardless TABLE 66.--Ana1ysis of variance of content scores on the progressive-attitude-toward-education scale comparing high and low scores on Recognition value for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 26.218 1 26.218 2.712 0.10 Within categories 1101.920 114 9.666 Total 1128.138 115 '17" v 'u. A... ‘v. (j '49) J (u (D (3 186 TABLE 67.--Means and standard deviations of content scores on the traditional-attitude—toward-education scale comparing high and low scores on Reiognition value for the total sample. Mean of Variable N Traditional Standard Content Scale Deviation High scores on Recognition value 50 29.260 2.754 Low scores on Recognition value 66 28.924 3.085 Total 116 29.069 2.939 1 of treatment. TABLE 68.--Ana1ysis of variance of content scores on the Total sample refers to all respondents regardless traditional-attitude-toward-education scale comparing high and low scores on Recognition value for the total sample. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square E of 3 Between categories 3.207 1 3.207 0.369 0. Within categories 990.241 8.686 Total 993.448 115 187 scored low on Recognition value compared with either progressive attitudes or traditional-attitudes-toward- education. It may be noted, however, that in case of progressive-attitudes-toward education, the difference between high and low scores on Recognition value is significant at the .10 level of confidence (Table 66). This would suggest a further verification of the hy- pothesis on another sample. H:4b is not supported by the data. The correlation coefficients for the value variables in question also indicate a lack of statistically signifi— cant relationship for the total sample (Tables 59 and 60). H:4c: Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children will score lower on the values of Leadership and Recognition than will the mothers of non- handicapped (i.e., normal) children. Mothers of emotionally disturbed, physically handi- capped, and normal children did not obtain significantly different scores on Leadership value as indicated by Tables 69 and 70. In respect to scores on Recognition value, significant differences (P < .02) exist between the three groups of mothers (Tables 71 and 72). But the hypothesis is not supported in the predicted direction. Ranking of means reveal that mothers of emotionally dis- turbed children scored the highest while HP mothers scored the lowest. Duncan's test (Table 73) indicate that 188 TABLE 69.--Means and standard deviations of Leadership value scores for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of Variable Group N Leadership Standard Value Scale Deviation Leadership EDP 52 7.269 4.366 value HP 39 7.128 4.969 Non—HP 64 8.140 5.055 Total 155 7.593 4.803 lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. TABLE 70.--Ana1ysis of variance of Leadership value scores for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of 5 Between categories 33.069 2 16.535 0.714 0.50 Within categories 3520.324 152 23.160 Total 3535.393 154 189 TABLE 71.--Means and standard deviations of Recognition value scores for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of Variable Group N Recognition Standard Value Scale Deviation Recognition EDP 52 10.000 4.106 value HP 39 7.692 3.419 Non-HP 64 9.766 4.679 Total 155 9.323 4.282 Ranking of means: EDP(10.000) > Non-HP(9.766) > HP(7.692) Duncan's test results: EDP > HP; Non-HP > HP lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP 8 Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. TABLE 72.--Analysis of variance of Recognition value scores for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between categories 140.079 2 70.039 3.967 0.02 Within categories 2683.792 152 17.656 Total 2823.871 154 ¥ 190 TABLE 73.--Duncan's New Multiple Range Test applied to means of Recognition value scale for the three respondent groups. Range of Means (p) 2 3 df=152 Studentized ranges1 for a = .05 (Zp, df=152) 2.772 2.918 for a = .01 (Zp, df=152) 3.643 3.796 R'p [:R' = (8) (2p, df=152)]2 a = .05 11.648 12.261 0 8 .01 15.308 15.951 Mean differences3 RED? - ifip (p=3) 15.408* 23D? _ iN0n_HP (p=2) 1.772 X' Y (p=2) 14.439* Non-HP ‘ HP lSignificant studentized ranges for Duncan's new multiple range test with 0 equal to .05 and .01 taken from Edwards (1965, pp. 373-74). 2p = the range of means (2 and 3). s = the square root of the error mean square of the analysis of variance of Table 72. Thus, S = “17.656 = 4.202 3Mean differences of columns 2 and 3 have been transformed into the equivalent of E - scores for multiple Ineans. To be significant, the figure must exceed the R' 'value of the same column. The formula given by Kramer (1956) is: /2n n - _ - __1__E_ = v (Xy Xz) my + nz > szp, error df of A. of V. ( R p) *P < .05 mg F 30,: i s z :8 heads is , mc‘ ound tc l 1 v H 0 o” A ‘79": «NJ V -,fi"’h ”LUVtée .I‘V' Imus n'n’ '. Vol.03- sturbe O‘l‘ c red ,1 \g nan ‘1 low 5 O 0:23" u. u. la y”. l“ ‘ 0 am Leac ‘0 191 there is no significant difference between control group mothers and the mothers of emotionally disturbed children. Thus, mothers of Non—HP and EDP groups scored higher on Recognition value than did the mothers of handicapped children. The hypothesis is considered confirmed only for the HP group. Zero-order correlations between Leadership and Recognition value scores, and EDP content scores are found to be significant only in the group of mothers having physically handicapped children (Table 53). Where- as Leadership value correlated positively (contrary to the predicted relationship) with attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons, a negative correlation supporting the hypothesized relationship is noted in case of Recognition value for the HP group (Table 53). None of the groups indicate significant relationship between HP content scores and Leadership or Recognition value (Table 54). I{:5a: Mothers who score high in need to help others, 1x3 be generous, will tend to score high in acceptance of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. Tables 74 and 75 reveal that there are no signifi- caxn: differences between the means of mothers who scored jhigh and those who scored low on Benevolence value when compared with scores on the EDP scale. Similarly, high and low scoring mothers on Benevolence value did not Non-HP(17.125) > HP(15-359) Duncan's test results: EDP > HP lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. TABLE 85.--Ana1ysis of variance of Support value scores for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square F of F Between groups 242.450 2 121.225 4.975 0.01 Within groups. 3703.744 152 24.367 Total 3946.194 154 200 TABLE 86.—~Duncan's New Multiple Range Test applied to means of Support value scale for the three respondent groups. Range of Means (p) 2 3 df=152 Studentized ranges1 for a = .05 (zp, df=152) 2.772 2.918 for a = .01 (Zp, df=152) 3.643 3.796 R'p [R' = (s)(zp,df=152)]2 a = 05 13 683 14 403 d = 01 17.982 18 737 Mean differences3 EEDP - EH? (p=3) 21.997** XEDP - XNon—HP (p=2) 11'582 XNon—HP ’ XHP (p=2) 12‘295 1 Significant studentized ranges for Duncan's new multiple range test with 0 equal to .05 and .01 taken from Edwards (1965, pp. 373—74). 2p = the range of means (2 and 3) s the square root of the error mean square of the analysis of variance of Table 85. Thus, S = V24.367 = 4.936 3Mean differences of columns 2 and 3 have been transformed into the equivalent of t - scores for multiple mearus. 'To be significant, the figure must exceed the R' VEJJle of the same column. The formula given by Kramer (1956) is: —_ 2n n :— _ — 2 Z = v (xy XZ) my + nz >szp, error df of A. of V. ( R p) **P < .01 201 value of Support than the mothers of normal or handi— capped children. This hypothesis is considered only partially confirmed. The zero—order correlations between the value of Support and attitudes toward the disturbed and the dis- abled are not significant either in the total sample or the three respondent groups (Tables 53 and 54). H:7: Mothers of emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped children will score low on the value of con- formity than will the mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. The ranking of means, standard deviations and F statistic are presented in Tables 87 and 88. There are significant differences (P < .05) between mothers of the control group and experimental groups. Nevertheless, the hypothesis is not supported as shown by Duncan's test (Table 89). Mothers of emotionally disturbed children expressed lowest Conformity value; but these mothers are not statistically different from control group mothers. Contrary to the hypothesis, HP mothers obtained highest Conformity mean, and are statistically different from Non—HP and EDP mothers. H:7 is not re- garded to be confirmed. Table 53 presenting zero-order correlations reveals that Conformity value is positively correlated with the EDP’content scale in the mothers of emotionally disturbed 202 TABLE 87.--Means and standard deviations of Conformity value scores for the three respondent groups. 1 Mean of Variable Group N Conformity Standard Value Scale Deviation Conformity EDP 52 18.615 5.623 value HP 39 21.461 4.999 Non-HP 64 19.125 6.119 Total 155 19.542 5.766 Ranking of means: HP(21.461) > Non—HP(19.125) > EDP(18.615) Duncan's test results: HP > EDP; HP > Non—HP lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non—HP = Mothers of non—handicapped (i.e., normal) children. TABLE 88.—-Analysis of variance of Conformity value scores for the three respondent groups. Source of Sum of Degrees of Mean Sig. Variance Squares Freedom Square 3 of F Between groups 199.477 2 99.739 3.081 0.05 Within groups 4921.000 152 32.375 'Total 5120.477 154 203 TABLE 89.-—Duncan's New Multiple Range Test applied to means of Conformity value scale for the three respondent groups. Range of means (p) 2 3 df=152 Studentized ranges1 for a = .05 (Zp,df=152) 2.772 2.918 for a = .01 (Zp,df=152) 3.643 3.796 R'p[ ' = (s)(Zp,df=152)]2 a — .05 15.773 16 603 Mean differences3 “ _ ’ = * XHP XEDP (p 3) 19.000 _ _ _ = * XHP XNon—HP (p 2) 16.263 XNon—HP ‘ XEDP (p=2) 3'863 lSignificant studentized ranges for Duncan's new multiple range test with 0 equal to .05 and .01 taken from Edwards (1965, pp. 373-74)- 2p = the range of means (2 and 3). s the square root of the error mean square of the analysis of variance of Table 88. Thus, 8 = /32.375 = 5.690 3Mean differences of columns 2 and 3 have been transformed into the equivalent of t - scores for multiple means. To be significant, the figure must exceed the R' value of the same column. The formula given by Kramer (1956) is: _ _ 2n nz (Xy - XZ) 5—11—H—->szp, error df of A. of V. (=R' ) y z p *P < .05 204 children. The value of Conformity is also found to be positively associated with attitudes toward the handi— capped in the total sample as well as in the EDP and HP groups. Summary of Attitude and Value Variables The F tests and correlational analyses of the various hypotheses pertaining to attitude—value interaction did not yield consistent results. Very few hypotheses were fully confirmed. Total Sample.——Mothers scoring high in need for power and control over others (Leadership value) scored high in acceptance of emotionally disturbed persons. But they did not score high in acceptance of physically handi- capped persons. Low scores on Recognition value did not result in greater acceptance of emotionally disturbed or physically handicapped persons. Similarly, mothers who scored high in need to help others, to be generous (Bene— volence value) were not different from low scoring mothers in their attitudes toward emotionally disturbed or handi- capped persons. The correlational analyses of the total sample re- vealed that high scores on Leadership value are signifi— <3ant1y related to favorable attitudes toward emotionally ciisturbed persons. The values of Recognition and Bene- ‘volence had no correlation with EDP and HP scales. 205 Consistent with the above findings, attitudes to- ward education (progressive or traditional) were not found to be significantly different in high and low scoring mothers on the value scales of Leadership, Recognition, and Benevolence. Except for the value of Benevolence which suggested significant positive correlation with traditional attitudes toward education, none of the re— maining value variables were observed to indicate signifi- cant relationships with either progressive or traditional attitudes toward education in the total sample. Respondent Groups.--When value scores of the three groups of mothers were compared, mothers of emotionally disturbed children as well as mothers of normal children did indeed score significantly higher on Recognition value than did mothers of handicapped children. Also, EDP mothers had significantly higher scores on the value of Support compared to mothers of physically handicapped or normal children. Conformity value was found to be highest in mothers of physically handicapped children, while there was no significant difference between EDP and Non—HP mothers in this regard. The zero-order correlations between value variables and attitude scales for the three groups of mothers re- 'vea1ed few significant correlations. The amount of correlations in all cases were too low to permit general- izations. Low positive correlation was observed between .Leadership and EDP scores in mothers of handicapped 206 children. Recognition and EDP scores correlated nega- tively in mothers of handicapped children. Some signifi- cant negative relationships were noted between Conformity value and attitude scores on the EDP scale in mothers of emotionally disturbed children. The value of Conformity was also found to have a slight negative association with attitude scores on the HP scale in EDP and HP groups. But Benevolence did have a significant positive corre- lation with HP scores in mothers of handicapped children. The relationship between progressive-attitudes- toward-education and Conformity value was detected to be significant in a positive direction for both HP and con- trol groups. Conformity value had significant negative correlation with traditional-attitudes-toward-education for the control group only. Again, the control group mothers indicated a negative correlation between Benevo- lence and traditional-attitudes—toward-education. Hypotheses Related to Change Orientation and Attitude Scores H:8a: Mothers who score high on change orientation will also score high on positive attitudes toward emotionally disturbed and physically handicappedgpersons. Total Sample.--According to Table 90, the multiple correlation between change orientation and attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons is highly signifi- cant (P < .01). When the six change variables are 207 TABLE 90.--Partia1 and multiple correlations between change variables and content of attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons (EDP scale)1 for the total sample.2 Change Variable N=158 Partial Correlation Health practices 0.046 Child rearing practices -0.078 Birth control practices 0.186* Automation 0.034 Political leadership -0.078 Self change 0.054 Multiple correlation R=0.223** 1 High scores on EDP content scale indicate negative attitudes. 2Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. *P < .05 **P < .01 partialled out, only one variable, namely, birth control practices makes most differential contribution to the multiple correlation. But the direction is reversed. In regard to the relationship between attitude scores on the HP scale and change orientation, the multiple correlation is noted to be significant at the .01 level of confidence (Table 91). No single variable contributes significantly to the multiple correlation. Moreover, 208 TABLE 91. --Partia1 and multiple correlations between change variables and content of attitude toward physi- cally handicapped persons (HP scale)1 for the total sample.2 Change Variable N=158 Partial Correlation Health practices -0.058. Child rearing practices 0.077 Birth control practices 0.122 Automation 0.020 Political leadership 0.105 Self change —0.137 Multiple correlation R=0.229** lHigh scores on HP content scale indicate negative attitudes. 2Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. **P < .01 only two correlation coefficients between the HP criterion variable and self-change and health practices are nega- tive, which are consistent with the hypothesis. Thus the hypothesis is not considered confirmed for the total sample. Respondent Groups.—-Significant multiple correlations are observed in the EDP and HP groups in respect to atti- tudes toward the disturbed and change variables. However, there is no consistency in the direction of relationship (Table 92). The partial correlation reveals that except 209 TABLE 92.--Partial and multiple correlations between change variables and content of attitu es toward emo- tionally disturbed persons (EDP scale) for the three respondent groups.2 Partial Correlation Change Variables EDP HP Non-HP N=52 N=44 N=62 Health practices 0.048 0.070 0.049 Child rearing practices -0.099 0.241 ”0.164 Birth control practices 0.234 0.385* -0.016 Automation -0.009 0.076 -0.052 Political leadership -0.221 -0.081 0.047 Self change —0.009 -0.037 0.012 Multiple correlation R=O.34l* R=0.426** R=0.196 1 High scores on EDP content scale indicate negative attitudes. 2EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. *P < .05 **P < .01 210 two variables, namely, health practices and birth control practices, most are associated with the criterion (EDP content scale) in the expected direction. This is not true in case of HP mothers where only two variables-- political leadership and self change have negative coeffi- cients. Concerning the relationship between attitudes to- ward the handicapped and change variables, significant multiple correlations were obtained for all the three groups of mothers (Table 93). But a look at the partial correlations indicate that self change is the only vari- able contributing significantly to the multiple corre- lation in the EDP group. H:8a cannot be considered fully confirmed by the results. H:8b: Mothers who score high on change orientation will also score high on progressive attitudes toward education and low on traditional attitudes toward education. Total Sample.--As indicated by Table 94, a highly significant relationship (P < .01) exists between pro- gressive attitudes toward education and change variables, although no individual change variable contributes signi- ficantly to this multiple correlation. In View of the Ineager size of negative partial correlation coefficients, ‘the hypothesis can be considered supported for the total seunple as far as progressive attitudes toward education arms concerned. There is no statistically significant 211 TABLE 93.—-Partia1 and multiple correlations between change variables and content of attitude toward physi- cally handicapped persons (HP scale)1 for the three respondent groups.2 Partial Correlation Change Variables EDP HP Non—HP N=60 N=48 N=69 Health practices -0.118 0.005 -0.l68 Child rearing practices 0.154 -0.012 0.120 Birth control practices -0.118 0.299* 0.136 Automation —0.222 0.064 0.138 Political leadership -0.004 0.164 0.014 Self change -0.328* 0.005 -0.092 Multiple correlation R=O.395** R=O.384* R=0.271* lHigh scores on HP content scale indicate negative attitudes. 2EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. *P < .05 **P < .01 212 TABLE 94.—-Partial and multiple correlations between change variables and content of progressive-attitude- toward-education for the total sample.1 Change Variable N=156 Partial Correlation Health practices 0.137 Child rearing practices 0.116 Birth control practices 0.043 Automation -0.040 Political leadership -0.005 Self change 0.055 Multiple correlation R=O.220** 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. **P < .01 relationship between traditional attitudes and change orientation (Table 95). Respondent Groups.--The multiple correlations be- tween the change orientation variables and progressive attitudes toward education are found to be significant in all the three groups of mothers (Table 96). However, only in the control group, is the partial correlation pertinaing to health practices significant in the pre- dicted direction. If the direction of partial corre- lation coefficients is considered, then H:8b can be considered only partially supported by the data for the respondent groups. 213 TABLE 95.—-Partia1 and multiple correlations between change variables and content of traditional-attitude- toward-education for the total sample.1 Change Variable N=156 Partial Correlation Health practices 0.095 Child rearing practices 0.011 Birth control practices -0.000 Automation 0.024 Political leadership 0.097 Self change -0.030 Multiple correlation R=0.l45 1 Total sample refers to all respondents regardless of treatment. Table 97 does not indicate any relationship between the change orientation variables and traditional atti- tudes toward education in any of the three groups of mothers. H:8b in respect to traditional education is not supported. H:9: Mothers of emotionally disturbed and_physically handicapped children will have higher mean scores than will mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children on the following change orientation measures: (a) health practices, (b) child rearing practices, (0) 'birth control practices, (d) automation, and (e) self change. 214 TABLE 96.--Partial and multiple correlations between change variables and content of progressive-attitgde— toward-education for the three respondent groups. Partial Correlation Change Variable EDP HP Non-HP N=60 N=48 N=69 Health practices -0.020 0.122 0.313* Child rearing practices 0.226 -0.196 0.168 Birth control practices 0.058 -0.110 -0.053 Automation —0.118 0.142 -O.176 Political leadership -0.155 0.166 0.034 Self change -0.006 0.287 -0.123 Multiple correlation R=O.318* R=0.415H R=O.370** lEDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. *P < .05 **P < .01 215 TABLE 97.--Partial and multiple correlations between change variables and content of traditionaleattitude- toward-education for the three respondent groups.l Partial Correlation Change Variable EDP HP Non-HP N=60 N=48 N=69 Health practices 0.226 -0.021 0.038 Child rearing practices -0.084 0.099 -0.024 Birth control practices 0.227 0.222 -0.154 Automation 0.204 -0.010 -0.137 Political leadership 0.039 0.145 0.099 Self change 0.024 0.023 -0.136 Multiple correlation R=0.327* R=0.216 R=0.236 l EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. *P < .05 216 Comparisons of mean differences, standard deviations, and F statistics are presented in Tables 98 and 99. There is no statistically significant difference between the three groups in any of the five change orientation vari~ ables. This hypothesis is not considered to be confirmed. Summary of Attitude and Change Orientation Variables The use of multiple and partial correlations to test the hypotheses revealed very few statistically significant correlations. Moreover, the size of correlations were also very low. Total Sample.--The combined change variables, and attitudes toward the disturbed and disabled were found to have significant relationships, although the direction of most partial correlation coefficients were inconsistent with the hypotheses. A significant but low multiple correlation was noted between the combined change orien- tation variables and progressive attitudes toward edu- cation. No such relationship was evident in the case of traditional attitudes toward education for the total sample. Respondent Groups.--The multiple correlation be- tween change orientation variables and attitudes toward emotionally disturbed persons was significant for EDP and HP mothers. The partial correlation revealed that high scores on the variable of birth control practices produced negative attitudes toward the emotionally 217 TABLE 98.--Comparison of mean differences and standard deviations in respect to five change orientation vari- ables for the three respondent groups.1 . 1 Standard Variable Group N Mean Deviation Health EDP 54 3.315 0.865 practices HP 45 3.289 0.815 Non-HP 67 3.492 0.704 Total 166 3.379 0.790 Child rearing EDP 55 3.145 0.803 practices HP 45 3.822 0.747 Non-HP 65 2.938 0.789 Total 165 2 976 0 788 Birth control EDP 55 1.800 0.911 practices HP 45 1.956 0.903 Non-HP 65 1.677 O 664 Total 165 1.794 0.823 Automation EDP 54 3.130 0 802 HP 44 3.045 0.888 Non-HP 65 3.185 0.726 Total 163 3.129 0 795 Self change EDP 55 2.327 ‘0.883 HP 45 2.378 0.806 Non-HP 65 2.554 0.613 Total 165 2.430 0 767 l EDP = Mothers of emotionally disturbed children. HP = Mothers of physically handicapped children. Non-HP = Mothers of non-handicapped (i.e., normal) children. :00 000.00 00000 000.0 000 000.00 000000 000003 00.0 000.0 000.0 0 000.0 000000 0003000 000000 0000 000 000.000 00000 000.0 000 000.000 000000 000003 00.0 000.0 000.0 0 000.0 000000 0003000 0000000000 000 000.000 00000 000.0 000 000.000 000000 000003 00.0 000.0 000.0 0 000.0 000000 0003000 0000000 00000 000 000.000 00000 000.0 000 000.00 000000 000003 00.0 000.0 000.0 0 000.0 000000 0003000 0000000 00000 000 000.000 00000 000.0 000 000.000 000000 000003 mm.o 200.0 000.0 m Hm:.H 00000w 000300m 000000000 000000 m 00 M 00000m 5000000 000000m 0000000> 0000000> .wfim 000: 00 0000w00 no 50m 00 00000m .00000& 0000000000 00000 000 000 000000 00000000000 0&0000 00 00000003 00 0000000<|I.mm m0mwca madam clownow: cannon .m anon d I.IIIIIIH.H I 0 h I Q 0 fl ‘ K .3. NBS—cg 5M)\A5 ELEV—330.60 cal-on gnu. A3 a §u\fi5 g new \5. 923.28 uo\ .2 5 E53\ 8. 358 uo\.u. 859:: 35a.- uo\§ Era Hum .55 uo\ .2 Egg sou .5 g 9.385. «o \ .5 33.56 017.2562... «x 33.23 153- on 30.— nay N. ”Flange .783!- an .033 :93 55? a . 3:8. 355:... 5? Huang ~35?! x All 9:22. a mo .28 05 guano» :3... ab « giant»! guano? n .3383 x a a“ NMAHNAHN u «lionlbulludon mmmummw 3. a so .2 n we uso and :4 Ma cowuco>oun «a unqua- n .850 no ; a «0 among? 05 new €38 a no 83:10 on» .3 fig 83 and.) «3.58 acofiouno>va « ~15u00~ufiucw z ova-flu Am «.35 g E .5 .2 oqanuo on You? ~d. n vlcuduou hue-unol o0 Quechua o 30: nu Inseam «a coda-nun handed-ham no .— Eouu nHOOu mafia: nucogul N. ha Emu—HG .5058 9.123136 1 guacauwvauu u n~l90w>u664 do ecu-unuaddl hAuI«OOI u . uuonunou a. n mummmmw MNNHIu-dulmdafi noon 0 Au vauuoonIn: do a m nunovaau 0‘ A0. a on on ous can an A undocumm Handwuanuducw ma a an as v a. u «a: n caduceus ounequm v: a a 000 a on n .IIIIJWIII you .32.: 9.38:2" u v: a u. n as cofia zoo 539m ual—Egon. ~23qu no 05>... a g 3.5 gonna-o.— N hunucouo o "J acoacuofiom ~IOO~ «I mun cal. o u . a n nuaouam no wgfl Manama Hofiummam A5 3.: .p. 3352. 5:33. 1:93 93.19. «a. bazaauupfi -auu~ Gide—.0 nag nonlIII-vflnl‘llnnlunnno-dhm— .aug-ugqc... ouaum nag—~03- § 8 IE: no is E 0| :Ivuohc .w :50». .— NI 7 ohfloguggi A“ all gun-lino“ I madly-Dwain a: Gallo Ho 8" no g- ; ml . at. g g Dunno ~148- OI a0. .3 Saul-.800 ml N n1 loan VI 8.— u N! Al can ha 030460.:- ouu Ema-Imam a Mlle—law»: 0.05 .- .ulolu nun. gut “ a * in Enl- lqu a as... 35.0.. 83.080 nl no: a Cu A! 8'. «I g a to. ml .5 8 a? .5 . .3328 1:3. 4 n no «I. o :gnga Ngdfla Mn Sada—i Nd % N‘ I a N. 93:... unto—lac! «Inez-uni «a no .28 I3 val-bu 0a uu * «o g 05 new :3 wan-500: an“ no guano In» Slain u g Pal a. g. 7 a g. g a! I eel-8261:30- n 5 i N 2 a . . .5 .2 .3 \ 015. no 38% 9n. A o gala-Irani— an 0.2.32. >238- o v .331 no a Hunk-Ii a 3!. «u a 1:85 «v _ 33 313-3.. 0 a a g 8.: £8» 2:3 ~ 3.8.35- . ~ .3 g: 3.05.... 9.13... w u v 3a g g in... a an in: nails—33 u a 03‘? «Q viola- Aug 0 A N on; C no i .- dug a noun .hv g as E N0 ”3 HI all! 5 f u a! In! luau-lulu! u .3 g a. 3.399». 0: .I.D o .3800. ugquzusflq ml «0.qu nu END 33...... 8.5!. v. uni—8!.» on 3.. 3 bag «u ago-52.8 1:83! nu 9.1.3 13.13. g .2 3 8:38".- !«uu! .8: .388 a... n» .1. 5 w 3 an” Ill-I- f. 3 1838.. 3 unencu— i In anal-23. aloo— nl .8353 50?»: ah i cannul- ad 3035550 .3 and «a .5 a a 2: .5 258 determination of attitude content, sampling of the items, and length of the scales may be resolved on the basis of this model. With reference to the scaling of values, it is recommended that the dimensions and classification schemes of values relevant to attitudes toward the mentally ill, the disabled, and education should be re-examined. If scaling of value items with Guttman's technique is de- sired, then the forced-choiced format should not be used. It is also suggested that some other value instruments, such as, The Differential Value Inventory(Prince, 1957) or Ways to Live (Morris, 1956) be employed in order to determine which value scale differentiates the groups significantly in respect to the criteria. Statistical Analysis.--Recommendations regarding scale analysis have already been mentioned in the previous section. In addition, factor analysis appears to be of greater potential value in determining predictor vari- ables for subsequent multiple regression analysis. Future research should also use chi-square statistics to test goodness of fit of the sample results with the normal probability model or some other theoretical distributions of the parent population. Part III: Concluding Summary The present research has confirmed, in general, the impact of personal contact in the maintenance of favorable attitudes toward emotionally disturbed and physically 259 handicapped persons. It has furthermore demonstrated that amount of contact alone does not always produce favorableness of attitudes; rather enjoyment and avoidance of contact are also involved in some manner. However, no consistent pattern can be determined between contact vari- ables and attitudes from the present research. The data also point to the fact that mothers of normal children have more favorable attitudes toward the disabled than toward emotionally disturbed persons and that the amount of attitudes toward the disabled is the same in both the mothers of normal and the mothers of physically handi- capped children. This gives the impression that cultural stereotypes about mental illness and physical disability play a significant role in attitudes even in the absence of personal contact. Contrary to expectation, more fre- quent contact is seen to reduce the intensity of atti- tudes toward emotionally disturbed and physically handi- capped persons. The majority of the hypotheses relating to values, change orientations, and attitudes toward education are not confirmed consistently, and as such no definite conclusions can be made on the basis of the present investigation. Although several specific hypotheses remain clearly unsubstantiated in the study, it does not necessarily warrant rejection or a major reformulation of the hy- potheses in question at the present state of our know- ledge about the constructs of attitude and values and 260 their relationships to other variables. Consideration of theoretical and methodological problems would suggest further examination of the hypotheses with the help of improved research design, more adequately formulated measuring instruments, and more appropriate statistical techniques. A major implication of the research findings is that future studies of attitudes about important social objects such as the mentally ill and the disabled must encounter the complexity of attitude composition under a sound and logically consistent theoretical system. Only then it is possible to derive a meaningful and predictable relationship between specific attitudes and relevant interactive variables. 11.1.14. III! i I'll REFERENCES 261 REFERENCES Adams, H. B. Mental illness or interpersonal behavior. Amer. Psychologist, 196A, 19, 191—197. P. L., Schwab, J. J., & Aponte, J. F. Authoritarian parents and disturbed children. Amer. J. Psychiat., 1965, 121, 1162-1167. Allport, G. W. Attitudes. In C. Murchison (ed.), Handbook pf social psychology. Worcester, Mass.: Clark University Press, 1935. Allport, G. W. Becoming. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1955. Allport, G. W. The nature of prejudice. New York: Doubleday-Anchor, 1953. Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. A study of values: A scale for measuring the dominant interests ip personality. (Rev. ed.) Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1951. Almond, G., & Coleman, J. S. (eds.). The politics of the developing areas. Princeton: Princeton UniVErsity Press, 1960. American Psychiatric Association. Training the psychiatrist pp meet changing needs. Washington: Amer. Psychiat. Ass., 1963. Askenasy, A. R. Attitudes toward mental patients and their rehabilitation: A study pf mental health personnel £2 éfl Atlantic boarder state ip Hawaii and ip England. Part one of a two part final report, Office of Voca- tional Rehabilitation Project RD 426, World Federation for Mental Health: U. 8. Committee, New York, 1963. Babbit, P. H. Appraisal of parental attitudes. J. Rehabilit., 196a, 20—21. Badt, Margit I. Attitudes of university students toward exceptional children and special education“ Except. Child., April 1957, 23, 287. 262 263 Bandura, A. Psychotherapy as a learning process. Psychol. Bull., 1961, 68, 1u3-159. Barclay, A. & Vaught, G. Maternal estimates of future achievement in cerebral palsied children. Amer. A. ment. Defic., 196b,.69, 62-65. Barker, R. G. The social psychology of physical disability. A. App. Issues, 19U8, 6, 28-35. Barker, R. G., & Wright, Beatrice A. Disablement: The somatopsychological problem. In E. D. Witthower & R. A. Cleghorn (eds.), Recent developments l2 psycho- somatic medicine. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott, 195D. Barker, R. G., Wright, Beatrice A., Meyerson, L., & Gonich, M. N. Adjustment to physical handicap and illness: A survey of the social psychology of physique and disability. Bulletin 2: the Social Science Research Council, 1953, No. 55. Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. Midwest and its children, the psychological ecology 9: AA American town. Evanston, 111.: Row, Peterson, 1955. Barton, A. H. Studyipg the effects pf é college education: A methodological examination pf changing values AA college. New Haven, Conn.: Edward W. Hazen Found— ation, 1959. Barton, W. Foreword. In The American Psychiatric Associ- ation (ed.), Planning for mental health: A report pp the nation. Washington: Amer. Psychiat. Ass., 1965. Bastide, R., & van den Berghe, P. Stereotypes, norms and interracial behavior Sao Paulo, Brazil. Amer. sociol. Rev., 1957, 22, 689-694. Bell, A. H. Attitudes of selected rehabilitation workers and other hospital employees toward the physically disabled. Psychol. Rep., 1962, 10 (1), 183-186. Bennet, C. C. Community psychology: Impressions of the Boston conference on the education of psychologists for community mental health. Amer. Psychologist, 1965, 20, 832—835. Blackman, 8., Mandell, W., Goldstein, K. M., & Silberstein, R. M. An approach to a community mental health theory. Proceedings pf the 73rd Annual Convention 93 3A3 American Psychological Association, 1965, 191-192. 264 Bolles, M. M., Metzger, H. F., & Pitts, M. W. Early home background and personality adjustment. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1941, 20, 530-535. Brayfield, A. H. Community mental health centers "staffing" legislation. Amer. Psychologist, 1965, 20, 429-430. Brophy, I. N. The luxury of anti-Negro prejudice. Pub. Opin. Quart., 1946, 9, 456-466. ’ Browne, W., Mally, M., & Kane, R. Psychosocial aspects of hemophilia: A study of twenty—eight hemophilic children and their families. Amer. A. Orthopgychiat., 1960,30, 730—740. Caplan, G. Principles 9: preventive psychiatry, New York: Basic Books, 1964. Carlson, E. R. Attitude change through modification of attitude structure. A. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1956, 52, 256—261. Carter, V., & Chess, Stella. Factors influencing the adaptations of organically handicapped children. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1951, 21, 827-837. Cass, L. K. Parent-child relationships and delinquency. A. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1952, 47, 101-103. Catton, W. R., Jr. A theory of value. Amer. sociol. Rev., 1959, 24, 311. Centers, Louise, & Centers R. Peer group attitudes toward. the amputee child. A. soc. Psychol., 1963, 61, 127— 132. Chamberlain, H. E., & De Schweinitz, E. Nine programs for the promotion of mental health. In R. Kotinsky & H. L. Witmer (eds.), Community programs for mental health. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1955- Chesler, M. A. Ethnocentrism and attitudes toward the physically disabled. A. Pers. soc. Psychol., 1965, 2, 877-882. Clark, J. Manual of computer programs. Research Services, Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1964 (mimeo). Clunk, J. Employer attitudes and the adjustment of the blind. In Donahm2& Donahue (eds.), Psychological diagnosis and counseling of the adult blind. New York: American Foundation for tfie blind, 1947. 265 Cobb, 8., French, J. R. P., Jr., Kahn, R. L., & Mann, F. C. An environmental approach to mental health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1963, 107, 5963606. Cohen, J., & Struenning, E. L. Opinions about mental illness in the personnel of two large mental hospitals. 1. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1962, 64, 349—360. Cohen, J., & Struenning, E. L. Opinions about mental illness: Mental hospital occupational profiles and profile clusters. Psychol. Rep., 1963, 12 (1), 111—124. Cole, N. J., Shaw, O. M., Steneck, J., & Taboroff, L. H. A survey assessment of current parental attitudes and practices in child rearing. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1957, 27, 815-822. Cole, N. J., & Taboroff, L. H. The psychological problems of the congenitally blind child. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1956, 26, 165-178. Commission de la Ligue Francaise d'Hygiene Mentale. Attitude de la societé a 1’ égard des maladies mentales. [Public opinion in regard to mental illnesses.] Hyg. ment., 1959, 48, 14-43. Cook, J. Dimensional analysis of child-rearing attitudes of parents of handicapped children. Amer. A. ment. Defic., 1963, 68,_354-361. Cowen, E. L., Underberg, R. P., & Verillo, R. T. The development and structure of an attitude to blindness scale. A. soc. Psychol., 1958, 48, 297-304. Crawford, F. R., Rollins, G. W., & Sutherland, R. L. Variations in the evaluation of the mentally ill: 11 The vieWpoint of the rural dweller. l. Hlth. hum. Behav., 1961, 2, 267—275. Cumming, Elaine, & Cumming, J. Closed ranks: AA experi- ment l2 mental health education. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1957. Davis, K. Mental hygiene and the class structure. Psychiatry, 1938, 1, 55-56. 266 Denhoff, E., & Holden, R. Family influence on successful school adjustment of cerebral palsied children. Excep_. Child., 1954, 21, 5-75. Despert, J. L. Some considerations relating to the genesis of autistic behavior in children. Amer. A. Ortho- ppychiat., 1951, 21, 335-350. Deutsch, M., & Collins, M. E. Interracial housipg. Minn— eapolis: Univer. of Minnesota Press, 1951. Dickstein, Joan, & Dripps, Elaine. An analysis of attitudes of acceptance-rejection towards exceptional children. Master of Education Thesis, Boston University, 1958. Dingman, H. F., Eyman, R. K., & Windle, C. D. An investi- gation of some child-rearing attitudes of mothers with retarded children. Amer. A. ment. Defic., 1963, 67, 899-908. Downing, M., Rickels, K., Downing R., & Robinson, F. Personality and attitudinal correlates of response to drug treatment in psychiatric out-patients: III. Neurotic medical clinic patients of lower socio- economic status-—a demographic study. A. Psychol., 1964, 57, 165-188. Dumbo, T., Leviton, G. R., & Wright, Beatrice A. Adjustment to misfortune--a problem of social-psychological rehabilitation. Artificial Limbs, 1956, 3, 4-6. Edwards, A. L. Eyperimental design £3 psychological research. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. Edwards, A. L. Techniques pf attitude scale construction. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957. Erikson, K. T. Patient role and social uncertainty: A dilemma of the mentally ill. Psychiatry, 1957, 20, 263—274. Estes, H. R., Haylett, C. H., & Johnson, E. M. Separation anxiety. Amer. A. Psychother., 1956, 10, 682-695. Felty, F. E. Attitudes toward physical disability in Costa Rica and their determinants: A pilot study. Unpubv lished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1965. Festinger, L. The theory 9: cognitive dissonance. New York: Harper & Row, 1957. 267 Field, M. Maternal attitudes found in twenty-five cases of children with primary behavior disorders. Amer. l. Orthopsychiat., 1940, 10, 293-296. Fliegler, L., & Hebeler, T. A study of the structure of attitudes of parents of educable mentally retarded children and a study of a change in attitude structure. U. S. Dept. of HEW, Office of Education, Cooperative Research Program, Project No. 018. Vol. I & II. Syracuse, N. Y.: Syracuse University Research Institute, 1960. Foa, U. G. Scale and intensity analysis in opinion research. Int. A. Opin. Att. Res., 1950, 4, 192—208. Foa, U. G. The contiguity principle in the structure of interpersonal relations. Human Relat., 1958, 2, 229- 238. Foa, U. G. Convergence in the analysis of the structure of interpersonal behavior. Psychol. Rev., 1961, 68, 341- 353. Foa, U. G. A facet approach to the prediction of communal- ities. Behav. Sci., 1963, 8, 220-226. Force, D. G. Social status of physically handicapped children. Except. Child., 1956, 26, 104-107, 132. Frank, G. H. The role of the family in the development of psychopathology. Psychol. Bull., 1965, 64, 191—205. Frank, J. D. Persuation and Healing. Baltimore: John HOpkins Press, 1961. Friesen, E. W. Nature and determinants of attitudes toward education and toward physically disabled persons in Colombia, Peru, and the United States. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1966. Gardner, E. F., & Thompson, G. G. Investigation and measure— ment pg the social values governing interpersonal relations amon adolescent youth and their teachers, (Parts I & II . Cooperative Research Project No. 259A (8418) and 259B (8418), Syracuse University, and Ohio State University, 1963. Genskow, J. K., & Maglione, F. D. Familiarity, dogmatism, and reported student attitudes toward the disabled. A. soc. Psychol., 1956, 67, 329-341. 268 Ginsburg, S. W. The mental health movement and its theoretical assumptions. In R. Kotinsky & H. L. Witmer (eds.), Community programs for mental health. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1955. Goode, W. J., & Hatt, P. K. Methods Ap_socia1 research. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1952. Goodman, N., Dornbusch, S. M., Richardson, S. A., & Hastorf, A. H. Varient reactions to physical disabilities. Amer. sociol. Rev., 1963, 28, 429—435. Gordon, L. V. Manual for survpy pfinterpersonal values. (No. 7-2761). Chicago, 111.: Science Research Associates, 1960. Gordon, L. V. Research briefs pp survey 9: interpersonal values. Chicago, 111.: Science Research Associates, 1953. Goshen, C. E. Mental retardation and neurotic maternal attitudes. A research report. Arch. gen. Psychiat., 1963, 9, 168-174. Gowman, A. G. The war blind AA American social structure. New York: Amer. Found. for the Blind, 1957. Green, B. F. Attitude measurement. In G. Lindzey (ed.), Handbook 9: social psychology. Vol. 1. Theory and method. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1954. Gurin, G., Veroff, J., & Feld, Sheila. Americans view their mental health: A nationwide interview survyy, New York: Basic Books, 1960. Gurney, Wilma. Parents of children with congenital computa- tion. Children, 1958, 5, 95-100. Guttman, L. The cornell technique for intensity and scale analysis. Educ. psychol. Measmt., 1947, 7, 247—280. Guttman, L. The problem of attitude and opinion measurement. In S. A. Stouffer (ed.), Measurement and prediction. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press, 1950. Guttman, L. An outline of some new methodology for social research. Pub. Opin. Quart., Winter 1954, 18, 395— 404 (a). Guttman, L. The principal components of scalable attitudes. in P. F. Lazarsfeld (ed.), Mathematical thinkipg_lp the social sciences. Glencoe, 111.: Free Press, 1954 (b). , 269 Guttman, L. A structural theory for the intergroup beliefs and action. Amer. sociol. Rev., 1959, 24, 318-328. Guttman, L. The structuring p; sociological s aces. Tech- nical Note No. 3, Contract No. AF 61 (0525-121, Dec., 1961, p. 14. Guttman, L., & Foa, U. G. Social contact and an intergroup attitude. Pub. Opin. Quart., Spring 1951, 43-53. Guttman, L., & Suchman, E. A. Intensity and a zero-point for attitude analysis. Amer. sociol. Rev., 1947, 12, 57-67. Hafterson, J. M. Multiple scalogram analysis (MSA) on the CDC 3600. Michigan State University Computer Institute for Social Science Research, Technical Report 6, Feb. 10, 1964. Hafterson, J. M. The "CUT" program for Guttman scalogram analysis. Computer Center, Michigan State University, 1965. (Mimeo) Handel, A. F. Community attitudes and adjustment to dis— ability. Outlook for the Blind, 1960, 54, 361-367. Harding, J., & Hogrefe, R. Attitudes of white department store employees toward Negro co-workers. A. soc. Issues, 1952, 8, 18-28. Haring, N. G. A review of research on cerebral palsy and emotional adjustment. Except. Child., 1959, 26, 191- 194. Haring, N. G., Stern, G. G., & Cruickshank, W. M. Attitude educators toward exceptional children. Syracuse, Y.: Syracuse University Press, 1958. 9.: N. Heider, F. The psychology 9: interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley, 195 - Himes, J. B. Measuring social distance in relations with the blind. Outlook for the Blind, 1960, 54, 54-58. Hobbs, N. Statement on mental illness and retardation. Amer. Psychologist, 1963, 18, 295-299. Holliday, A. R. An empirical investigation of the personality characteristics and attitude of the parents of children who stutter. Dissertation Abstr., 1958, 19, 569-570. Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. Social class and mental illness: A communipy study. New York: Wiley, 1958. 270 Homans, G. C. The human group. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1950. Hyman, H. Survey design and analysis. Glencoe: 111.: Free Press, 1955- Jacob, P. E. Changipg values 13 college. New York: Harper & Bros., 1957. Jacobson, E., Kumata, H., & Gullahorn, Jeanne B. Cross- cultural contributions to attitude research. Pub. Opin. Quart., Summer 1960, 24, 205—223. Johnson, W. A broader and bolder rehabilitation program. A. Rehabilit., 1963, 2, 13-14, 41-46. Johnson, A. M., Falstein, E., Szurek, S. A., & Svendsen, M. School phobia. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1941, 11, 702-711. Joint Commission on Mental Illness and Health. Action for mental health. New York: Basic Books, 1961. Jordan, J. E. Problems and promises of education in Latin America. Special education in Latin America. Phi Delta Kappan, January, 1964. Jordan, 8. The disadvantaged group: A concept applicable to the handicapped. A. Psychol., 1963, 55, 313-322. Kasanin, J., Knight, E., & Sage, P. The parent—child rela- tionship in schizophrenia. A. nerv. ment. Dis}, 1934, 79, 249-263. Katz, D. The functional approach to the study of attitudes. Pub. Opin. Quart., Summer 1960, 24, 163-204. Kelman, H. C. Processes of opinion change. Pub. Opin. Quart., 1961, 25, 57-78. Kennedy, J. F. Mental illness and mental retardation: Message from the President of the United States relative to mental illness and mental retardation. Amer. Psychologist, 1963, 18, 280-289. Kerlinger, F. N. The attitude structure of the individual: a Q-study of the educational attitudes of professors and laymen. Genetic Psychology, 1956, 53, 283-329. Kerlinger, F. N. Progressiveness and Traditionalism: Basic factors of educational attitudes, A. soc. Psychol., 1958, 68, 111-135. Kerlinger, F. N. Factor invariance in the measurement of attitudes toward education. Educ. psychol. Measmt., 1961, 21, 273-285. 271 Kerlinger, F. N., & Kaya, E. The construction and factor analytic validation of scales to measure attitudes toward education. Educ. psychol. Measmt., 1959, 19, 13—29 0 Kessler, Jane W. Psychopathology 2: childhood. Englewood, Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1966. Kirk, S. A. Educating exceptional children. Boston, Mass.: Houghton-Mifflin, 1962. Klebanoff, L. W. Parents of schizophrenic children: 1. Parental attitudes of mothers of schizophrenic, brain- injured and retarded, and normal children. Workshop, 1958. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1959, 29, 445-454. Kluckhohn, C. Values and value orientations in the theory of action. In T. Parsons & E. A. Shils (eds.), Toward 2 general theory pf action. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1951. Kluckhohn, Florence R. Dominant and variant value orienta- tions. In C. Kluckhohn, H. A. Murray & D. M. Schneider (eds.), Personality: Ip_nature, society and culture. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1953. Knapp, R. R. An empirical investigation of the concurrent and observational validity of an ipsative versus a normative measure of six interpersonal values. Educ. psychol. Measmt., 1964, 24, 65-73. Konopka, C. Group therapy in overcoming racial and cultural tensions. Amer. A. Orthppsychiat., 1947, 17, 693-699. Kramer, C. Y. Extension of multiple range tests to group means with unequal numbers of replications. Biometrics, 1956, 12, 307-310. Krathwohl, D. R., Bloom, B. S., & Masia, B. B. A taxonomy pf educational objectives: Handbook IA: The affective domain. New York: David McKay, 1964. Kvaraceus, W. C. Acceptance-rejection and exceptionality. Except. Child., 1956, 22, 328-331. Law, Shirley. The mother of the happy child. Smith Coll. Stud. soc. [A., 1954, 25, 1-27. Leary, T. Interpersonal diagnosis pf personality. New York: Ronald Press, 1957. Lehmann, I. J., Sinha, B. K., & Hartnett, R. T. Changes in attitudes and values associated with college attendance. A. educ.Psychol., 1966, 57,89—98. 272 Lemkau, P., & Corcetti, G. An urban population's Opinion and knowledge about mental illness. Amer. A. Psychiat., February, 1962, 118, 8. Levine, S. A proposed conceptual framework for special education. Except. Child., 1961, 28, 83-90. Lewin, K. Principles of topological psychology. New York: McGraw—Hill, 1936? Lewis, W. D. Influence of parental attitudes on children's personal inventory scores. 1. genet. Psychol., 1945, 67, 195-201. Likert, R. A technique for the measurement of attitudes. Arch. Psychol., 1932, No. 140. Lingoes, J. C. Multiple scalogram analysis: A set theoret- ical model for analyzing dichotomous items. Educ. ppychol. Measmt., 1963, 23, 501-524. Lingoes, J. C. Ap IBM — 7090 program for Guttman—Lingoes multi-dimensional scalogram analysis--I. The Univer. of Michigan, 1965. Lingoes, J. C. New computer developments in pattern analysis and nonmetric techniques. I.B.M. Journal, 1966 (in press). Liverant, S. MMPI differences between parents of disturbed and non-disturbed children. A. consult. Psychol., 1959, 23, 256—260. Loomis, C. P. Social systems. Princeton, N. J.: Van Nostrand, 1960. Lowe, C. M. Value orientations--an ethical dilemma. Amer. Psychologist, 1959, 14, 687-693. MacGregor, F. C., Abel, T. M., Bryt, A., Laner, E., & Weissman, S. Facial deformities and plastic surgery: A psychosocial study. Springfield, 111.: Chalres C. Thomas, 1953. McKeown, J. E. The behavior of parents of schizophrenics, neurotic, and normal children. Amer. A. Sociol., 1950, 56, 175-179. Madoff, J. M. The attitudes of mothers of juvenile delin- quents toward child—rearing. {. consult. Psychol., 273 Manis, J. G., Hunt, C. L., Brawer, M. J., & Krecher, L. C. Public and psychiatric conceptions of mental illness. A. Hlth. Human Relat., 1965, 6, 48-55. Mark, J. C. Attitudes of mothers of male schizophrenics toward child behavior. A. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1953, 48, 185-189. Maslow, A. H. (ed.). New knowledge Ap human values. New York: Harper, 1959. Medinnus, G. R. The relation between parent attitudes and parental acceptance of the child. A. genet. Psychol., 1963, 103, 117-121. Meehl, P° E. Clinical versus statistical prediction. Menneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1954. Meerloo, J. A. M. Psychiatric ecology. A. nerv. ment. Dis., 1959 3 129 , 385-390 . Meyerson, L. Physical disability as a social psychological problem. A. soc. Issues, 1948, 4 (4), 2-10. Meyerson, L. Somatopsychology of physical disability. In W. M. Cruickshank (ed.), Psychology p: exceptional children and youth. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1963, pp. 1—52. Morris, C. Varieties pi human value. Chicago, 111.: University of Chicago Press, 1956. Murphy, A. T. Attitudes of educators toward the visually handicapped. Sight—Saving Rev., Fall 1960, 30 (3), 157-161. Murray, H. A.. & Kouckhohn, C. Outline of a conception of personality. In C. Kluckhohn, H. A. Murray, & D. M. Schneider (eds.), Personality; Ap_nature, society) and culture. New York: Alfred Knopf, 1953. Nash, M. V. A study of attitudes of a group of non- handicapped people toward the orthopedically handi- capped. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, 1962. Newcomb, T. M. Stabilities underlying changes in inter- personal attraction. A. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1963, 66, 376-386. Nunnally, J. C., Jr. Popular conceptions pi mental health. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston, 1961. 274 Nunnally, J., & Kittross, J. M. Public attitudes toward mental health professions. Amer. Psycholpgist, 1958, 13: 589'594- O'Connor, F., & Goldberg, 1. Children with crippling condi- tions and special health problems. Rev. Educ. Res., December 1959, 29, 471—496. Osgood, C. E., & Tannenbaum, P. H. The principle of congruity in the prediction of attitude change. Psychol. Rev., .1955, 62, “2‘55. Parsons, T., & Shils, E. A. (eds.), Toward A general theory p: action. Chambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1951. Peterson, D. R., Becker, W. C., Hellmer, L. A.,Shoemaker, D. J., & Quay, H. C. Parental attitudes and child adjustment. Child Develpm., 1959, 30, 119-130. Pintner, R., Eisenson, J., & Stanton, M. The psychology pg the physicalAy handicppped. New York: Appleton- Century-Crofts, 1941. Plant, W. T. The psychological impact p: the public two- year college pp_certain non-intellectual functions. San Jose, Calif.: San Jose State College, 1963. Prince, R. A study of the relationship between individual values and administrative effectiveness in the school situation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1957. Prout, C. T., & White, Mary A. A controlled study of per— sonality relationships in mothers of schizophrenic male patients. Amer. A. Ppychiat., 1951, 107, 251-256. Putnam, M. C. Case study of an atypical two-and-a-half-year— old. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1948, 18, 6-30. Radke, M. J. The relation of parental authority to children's behavior and attitudes. Univer. of Minnesota Institute of Child Welfare Monograph Series, No. 22. Minneapolis: The University of Minnesota Press, 1946. Raskin, N. The attitudes of sighted people towards blindness. Paper read at National Psychol. Res. Council, 1956. Read, K. H. Parents' expressed attitudes and children's behavior. A. consult. Psychol., 1945, 9, 95-100. Rees, T. P. Back to moral treatment and community care. A. ment. Sci., 1957, 103, 303-313. 275 Reeves, J. K. Some parental and social attitudes towards children using hearing aids. Volta Rev., 1962, 6A, 314-316, 331. Reichard, Suzanne, & Tillman, C. Patterns of parent-child relationships in schizophrenia. Psychiatpy, 1950, 13, 247-257. Richardson, 8. A., Goodman, N., Hastorf, A. H., & Dornbusch, S. M. Cultural uniformity in reaction to physical disabilities. Amer. sociol. Rev., 1961, 26, 2u1—2u7. Riley, Matilda W., Riley, J. W., & Jackson, T. Sociological studies Ap_scale analysis. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press, 1954. Roeber, G. A. A study of certain public attitudes toward the orthopedically disabled. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University, 1959. Rogers, C. R. Client-centered therapy. Boston, Mass.: Houghton-Mifflin, 1951. Rogers, C. R. A note on the nature of man. A. counsel. Psychol., 1957, A, 199-203. Rome, H. P., & Robinson, D. B. Psychiatric conditions associated with metabolic, endocrine, and nutritional disorders. In S. Arieti (ed.), American handbook p; psychiatry. Vol. I. New York: Basic Books, 1959. Rosenbaum, M., & Berger, M. Group_p§ychotherapy and group function. New York: Basic Books, 1960. Rosenberg, M. J. Cognitive structure and attitudinal affect. A. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1956, 53, 367-372. Rosenberg, M. J. A structural theory of attitude dynamics. Pub. Qpin. Quart., Summer 1960, 24, 319-340. Ruble, W. L., Kiel, D. F., & Rafter, Mary E. Calculation of least squares (regression) problems on the LS Routine. Stat. Series Description No. 7, Agricultural Experiment Station, Michigan State University, 1966. (a) Ruble, W. L., Kiel, D. F., & Rafter, Mary E. One-way analysis 9: variance with unequal number p: replications permitted. (UNEQI Routine.) Stat. Series Description No. 13, Agricultural Experiment Station, Michigan State University, 1966. (b) 276 Ruble, W. L., & Rafter, Mary E. Calculation 9: basic statistics when missing data are involved. (The MDSTAT Routine.)‘*Stat. Series Description No.6, Agriculture Experiment Station, Michigan State University, 1966. Rusalem, H. The environmental supports of public attitudes toward the blind. Outlook for the Blind, 1950, HA, 277—288. Samler, J. Change in Vlaues: A goal in counseling. A. counsel. Psychol., 1960, 7, 32—39. Sanford, N. (ed.). The American college. New York: Wiley, 1962. Schauer, G. Motivation of attitudes toward blindness. Outlook for the Blind, 1951, 45, 39-A2. Schuhman, A. I., Coe, R. M., & Rae-Grant, N. I. Some social-psychological variables influencing parental acceptance of residential treatment for their emotionally disturbed children. A. Child psychol. Psychiat., 1964, 5, 251-261. Sears, R. R. Comparison of interviews with questionnaires for measuring mothers' attitudes toward sex and aggression. A. pers. soc. Psychol., 1965, 2, 37—A4. Secord, P. F., & Backman, C. W. Social psychology, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1965. Shaefer, E. S., & Bell, R. Q. Development of a parental attitude research instrument. Child Develpm., 1958, 29: 339‘3610 Shepherd, Irma L., & Guthrie, G. M. Attitudes of mothers of schizophrenic patients. A. clin. Psychol., 1959, 15, 212-215. Shere, Marie. Socio—emotional factors in family of the twin with cerebral palsy. Except. Child., 1956, 22, 196-199, 206-208. Sherif, Carolyn W., Sherif, M., & Nebergall, R. E. Attitude and attitude change: The social Judgment- involvement approach. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders, 1965. Shoben, E. J., Jr. The assessment of parental attitudes in relation to child adjustment. Genet. Psychol. Monogr., 277 Siller, J. Personality determinants of reactions to the physically disabled. Amer. Found. for the Blind, 1964, NO. 7’ 37-520 Siller, J., & Chipman, A. Factorial structure and correlates of the attitudes toward disabled persons scale. Educ. psychol. Measmt., 1964, 24, 831-840. - Smith, F. T. Ap_experiment Ap_modifying attitudes toward the Negro. New York: Columbia University, Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 887, 1943. Smith, M. B., & Hobbs, N. The community and the community mental health center. Amer. Psychologist, 1966, 21, 499-509- Sommers, Vita S. The influences p: parental attitudes and social environment pp the personality development 2: the adolescent blind. New York: Amer. Found. for the Blind, 1944. Spiegel, J. P., & Bell, N. W. The family of the psychiatric patient. In Arieti, S. (ed.), American handbook p: psychiatgy, Vol. I. New York: Basic Books, 1959. Spranger, E. Types p£_men. (Trans. by P. J. W. Pigors.) Halle: Niemyer, 1928. Star, Shirley A. Public attitudes--a measure of the dif- ficulties. In APA (ed.), Psychiatry, the press and the public. Washington: Amer. Psychol. Ass., 1956. Stefflre, B. Vocational value inventory. Tentative Manual. Michigan State University, 1958. Stern, Nancy W. Maternal personality, attitudes, and child- rearing practices, and their relation to child adjust- ment. Dissertation Abstr., 1964, 25 (1), 635. Stewart, C. A. A study of opinions regarding mental ill- nesses and facilities for their care as realted to social class membership. Dissertation Abstr., 1959, 19 (8), 2173. Stouffer, S. A. An overview of the contributions to scaling and scale theory. In S. A. Stouffer, L. Guttman, E. A. Suchman, P. F. Lazaresfeld, Shirley A.Star, & J. A. Clausen (eds.), Measurement and prediction. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press, 1950. Struening, E. L., & Cohen, J. Factorial invariance and other psychometric characteristics of five Opinions about mental illness factors. Educ. psychol. Measmt., 1963, 23, 289-298. W . W h a n a .3 r: C. Cu Co Q» r . a l Uh Uh m- 278 Suchman, E. A. The intensity component in attitude and opinion research. In S. A. Stouffer, L. Guttman, E. A. Suchman, P. F. Lazaresfeld, Shirley A. Star, & J. A. Clausen (eds.), Measurement and ppediction. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press, 1950. Suchman, E. A., & Guttman, L. A solution to the problem of question bias. Pub. Opin. Quart., 1947, 2, 445— 455. Super, D. E. Work value inventory. Form D. Teachers College, Columbia University, 1961. Szaz, T. S. The myth of mental illness. Amer. Psycho- logist, 1960, 15, 113-118. Tamkin, A. S. Survey of pathogenic parental attitudes in parents of emotionally disturbed children. Psychol. Rep., 1964, 15, 36. Taylor, J. B. The organization of physician attitudes towards the emotionally disturbed patient. A, Hlth. Human Behav., 1965, 2, 99-104. Tietze, Trude. A study of mothers of schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry, 1949, 12, 55-65. Tolor, A., & Rafferty, W. The attitudes of mothers of hospitalized patients. A. nerv. ment. Dis., 1963, 136, 76-81. Underberg, Rita. The relationship between parental under- standing and child adjustment in the visually disabled adolescent. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester, 1958. Underberg, Rita, & Others. Factors relating to adjustment to visual disability in adolescence. New Outlook for the Blind, 1961, 55, 253-259. U. S. Bureau of the Census. A. A. census 9: population: 1960. Vol. I. Characteristics Q; the pOpulation. Part 24, Michigan. Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1963. (a) U. S. Bureau of Census. A. S. census 9: population: 1960. Vol. I. Characteristics 9: the population. Part 37, Ohio. Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1963. (b) Van Houten, Janny. Mother-child relationship in twelve cases of school phobias. Smith Coll. Stud. soc. WA., 1948, 18, 161—180. 279 Verillo, R. T. A study of adjustment and the relationship between parental attitudes and adjustment in visually impaired and sighted adolescents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester, 1958. Waisanen, F. B. A notation technique for scalogram analysis. The Sociol. Quart., October, 1960, 1 (4), 245-252. Ward, J. H., Jr. Multiple linear regression models. In H. Borko (ed.), Computer applications Ag the behavioral sciences. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1962. Watson, J. Some social and psychological situations related to change in attitude. Human Relat., 1950, No. 3, 15-56. Watson, Jane E., & Johnson, Adelaide. The emotional signifi- cance of acquired physical disfigurement in children. Amer. A. Orthopsychiat., 1958, 28, 85—98. Watts, H. J. Methodological problems in the measurement of values. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1962. Whiteman, M., & Lukoff, I. F. Public attitudes toward blindness. New Outlook for the Blind, 1962, 56, 153-158. Whiteman, M., & Lukoff, I. F. A factorial study of sighted people's attitude toward blindness. A. soc. Psychol., 196“: 6“: 339‘353- Whiteman, M., & Lukoff, I. F. Attitudes toward blindness and other physical handicaps. A. soc. Psychol., 1965, 66, 135-145. Williams, R. M., Jr. Strangers next door: Ethnic relations Ag American communities. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1964. Williamson, E. G. Value orientation in counseling. Personnel Guid. A., 1958, 36, 520-528. ‘Winder, C. L., & Rau, Lucy. Parental attitudes associated with social deviance in preadolescent boys. A. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 1962, 64, 418-424. VWood, K. S. Parental maladjustment and functional articula- tory defects in children. A. Speech A Hearing Dis- orders, 1946, 11, 255—260. 280 Worchel, T., & Worchel, P. The parental concept of the mentally retarded child. Amer. A. ment. Defic., 1961, 65 , 782-788. Wortis, H., & Cooper, W. The life experience of persons with cerebral palsy: A study of sixty-three histories. Amer. A. Phys. Med., 1957, 36, 328-344. Wright, Beatrice A. Physical disabilipy: A psychological approach. New York: Harper, 1960. Wright, F. H., & Klein, R. A. Attitudes of hospital personnel and the community regarding mental illness. A. counsel. Psychol., 1966, 13, 106-107. Yolles, S. F. The role of the psychologist in comprehensive community mental health centers: The national insti— tute of mental health view. Amer. Psychologist, 1966, 21, 39-41. Yuker, H. E., Block, J. R., & Campbell, W. J. A scale to measure attitudes toward disabled persons. Human resources Stud., 1960, No. 5, l4. Zavalloni, M., & Askenasy, A. Attitudes toward mental illness: A cross—cultural study. New York: World Federation for Mental Health: U. S. Committee, 1963. Zetterberg, H. L. 9g theory and verification Ag sociology. Totawa, N. J.: Bedminster Press, 1963. Zuckerman, M., Oltean, Mary, & Monashkin, I. The parental attitudes of mothers of schizophrenics. A. consult. Psychol., 1958, 22, 307-310. APPENDICES 281 APPENDIX A—l Handicapped Persons Scale 282 PL-ipb [3,1nt- YSEtt- ] No. Location Male Group Female I Date HANDICAPPED PERSONS SCALE Instructions: Given below are 20 statements of opinion about physically handicapped persons. We all think dif- ferently about persons with physical handicaps. Here you may express how you think by choosing one of the four possible answers following each statement. These answers indicate how much you agree or disagree with the state- ment. Please mark your answer by placing a circle around the number in front of the answer you select. You are also asked to indicate for each statement how strongly you feel about your marking of the statement. Please mark this part of your answer in the same way as before, by placing a circle around the number in front of the answer you selects _Y' 1. Parents of handicapped children should be less strict than other parents. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly 2. Physically handicapped persons are just as intelligent as non-handicapped ones. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 2 ATDP Handicapped people are usually easier to get along with than other peOple. l. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Most physically handicapped people feel sorry for themselves. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Physically handicapped people are the same as anyone else. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 3 ATDP There shouldn't be special schools for physically handicapped children. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly It would be best for physically handicapped persons to live and work in special communities. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly It is up to the government to take care of physically handicapped persons. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 10. ll. 4 ATDP Most physically handicapped people worry a great deal. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Physically handicapped people should not be expected to meet the same standards as non-handicapped people. 1. Strongly Disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Physically handicapped people are as happy as non- handicapped ones. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. l2. 13. 14. 5 ATDP Severely physically handicapped people are no harder to get along with than those with minor handicapps. l. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly It is almost impossible for a handicapped person to lead a normal life. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly You should not expect too much from physically handi— capped people. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 15. l6. l7. 6 ATDP Physically handicapped people tend to keep to them- selves much of the time. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Physically handicapped people are more easily upset than non-handicapped people. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Physically handicapped persons cannot have a normal social life. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 18. 190 20. 7 ATDP Most physically handicapped people feel that they are not as good as other people. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly You have to be careful of what you say when you are with physically handicapped people. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 3. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Physically handicapped people are often grouchy. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly APPENDIX A-2 Emotionally Disturbed Persons Scale 290 No. Location Male Group Female Date y EMOTIONALLY DISTURBED PERSONS SCALE Instructions: Given below are 20 statements of opinion about emotionally disturbed persons. We all think differently about persons with emotional disturbances. Here you may ex— press how you think by choosing one of the four possible answers following each statement. Please mark your answer by placing a circle around the number in front of the answer ypu select. You are also asked to indicate for each statement how strongly you feel about your marking of the statement. Please mark this part of your answer in the same way as before, by placing a circle around the number in front of the answer you select. 1. Parents of emotionally distrubed children should be less strict than other parents. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly 2. Emotionally disturbed persons are just as intelligent as emotionally stable ones. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. EDPS Disturbed people are usually easier to get along with than other poeple. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Most emotionally disturbed people feel sorry for themselves. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Emotionally disturbed peOple are the same as anyone else. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly There shouldn't be special schools for emotionally dis- turbed children. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly \ I No. 10. EDPS It would be best for emotionally distrubed persons to live and work in special communities. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly It is up to the government to take care of emotionally disturbed persons. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Most emotionally disturbed people worry a great deal. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Emotionally distrubed people should not be expected to meet the same standards as emotionally stable people. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. ll. l2. 13. 14. EDPS Emotionally disturbed people are as happy as emotionally stable ones. 1. Strongly disagree 2. Disagree About how strongly do you feel 1. Not strongly at all 2. Not very strongly Severely emotionally disturbed get along with than those with l. Strongly disagree 2. Disagree About how strongly do you feel 1. Not strongly at all 2. Not very strongly It is almost impossible for an person to lead a normal life. 1. Strongly disagree 2. Disagree About how strongly do you feel 1. Not strongly at all 2. Not very strongly You should not expect too much turbed people. 1. Strongly disagree 2. Disagree About how strongly do you feel 1. Not strongly at all 2. Not very strongly Agree Strongly agree about your answer? Fairly strongly Very strongly people are no harder to minor disturbances. Agree Strongly agree about your answer? Fairly strongly Very strongly emotionally disturbed Agree Strongly agree about your answer? Fairly strongly Very strongly from emotionally dis- Agree Strongly agree about your answers? Fairly strongly Very strongly No. 15. 16. 17. 18. EDPS Emotionally disturbed people tend to keep to themselves much of the time. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Emotionally disturbed people are more easily upset than emotionally stable ones. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Emotionally disturbed persons cannot have a normal social life. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Most emotionally disturbed people feel that they are not as good as other people. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree .About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 19. 20. EDPS You have to be careful of what you say when you are with emotionally disturbed people. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly ’ Emotionally disturbed people are often grouchy. l. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly APPENDIX A-3 Education Scale 297 NO. Location Male y, Group Female Date EDUCATION SCALE Instructions: Given below are 20 statements of opinion about education. We all think differently about schools and education. Here you may express how you think by choosing one of the four possible answers following each statement. These answers indicate how much you agree or disagree with the statement. Please mark your answer by pggcigg,a circle around the number in front of the answer youfiselect. You are also asked to indicate for each statement how strongly you feel about your marking of the statement. Please mark this part of your answer in the same way as before, by placing a circle around the number in front of the answer ygu select. _L l. The goals of education should be dictated by children's interests and needs as well as by the larger demands of society. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly 2. No subject is more important than the personalities of the pupils. ‘ 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 4. 2 E.D. Schools of today are neglecting reading, writing, and arithmetic; the three 3'5. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree . 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly The pupil-teacher relationship is the relationship be— tween a child who needs direction, guidance, and control and a teacher who is an expert supplying direction, guidance, and control. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strOngly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Teachers, like university professors, should have academic freedom-—freedom to teach what they think is right and best. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. .Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly .1 ,1 r, I, ( {I No. 3 E.D. The backbone of the school curriculum is subject matter; activities are useful mainly to facilitate the learning of subject matter. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly disagree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Teachers should encourage pupils to study and criticize our own and other economic systems and practices. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly The traditional moral standards of our culture should not just be accepted; they should be examined and tested in solving the present problems of students. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly arm-’1' ('I'! .3 a No. 9. 10. 11. u E.D. Learning is experimental; the child should be taught to test alternatives before accepting any of them. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly The Curriculum consists of subject matter to be learned and skills to be acquired. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly The true view of education is so arranging learning that the child gradually builds up a storehouse of knowledge that he can use in the future. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 12. l3. l4. 5 E.D. One of the big diffidulties with modern schools is that diséipline is often sacrificed to the interests of children. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all I 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly The curriculum should be made up of an orderly sequence of subjects that teach to all students the best of our cultural heritage. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Discipline should be governed by long-range interests and well-established standards. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About hos strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 15. l6. l7. 6 E.D. Education and educational institutions must be sources of social ideas; education must be a social program undergoing continual reconstruction. l. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Right from the very first grade, teachers must teach the child at his own level and not at the level of the grade he is in. l. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About now strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly Children should be allowed more freedom than they usually get in the execution of learning activities. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly No. 7 E.D. 18. Children need and should have more supervision and discipline than they usually get. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly 19. Learning is essentially a process of increasing one's store of information about the various fields of knowledge. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly 20. In a democracy, teachers should help students under- stand not only the meaning of democracy but also the meaning of the ideologies of other political systems. 1. Strongly disagree 3. Agree 2. Disagree 4. Strongly agree .About how strongly do you feel about your answer? 1. Not strongly at all 3. Fairly strongly 2. Not very strongly 4. Very strongly APPENDIX A-4 Gordon Survey of Interpersonal Values 305 %I %I %l %I %I .4 RAW SCORE Date . . Grade or Occupation PERCENTILE l NORM GROUP .1 44.~ Marital Status School or Firmi 1 .. Mark your answers in column A —-—-> EE- SURVEY OF INTERPERSONAL VALUES By LEONARD V. GORDON DIRECTIONS In this booklet are statements representing things that people consider to be important to their way of life. These statements are grouped into sets of three. This is what you are asked to do: Examine each set. Within each set, find the one statement of the three which represents what you consider to be most important to you. Blacken the space beside that statement in the column headed M (for most). Next, examine the remaining two statements in the set. Decide which one of these statements represents what you consider to be least important to you. Blacken the space beside that statement in the column headed L (for least). For every set you will mark one statement as representing what is most important to you, one statement as representing what is least important to you. and you will leave one state- ment unmarked. Example M L To have a hot meal at noon , , . 7 . :::::: — To get a good night’s sleep ,, , :::::: :::::: To get plenty of fresh air , , , , N _ :::::: Suppose that you have examined the three statements in the example, and although all three of the statements may represent things that are important to you, you feel that “To get plenty of fresh air" is the most important to you. You would blacken the space in the column headed M (for most) beside the statement. Notice that this has been done in the example. You would then examine the remaining two statements to decide which of these represents something that is least important to you. Suppose that “To have a hot meal at noon” is the least important to you. You would blacken the space in the column headed L (for least) next to this statement. Notice that this has been done in the example. You would leave the remaining statement unmarked. In some cases it may be difficult to decide which statement to mark. Make the best decision that you can. This is not a test; there are no right or wrong answers. Be sure to mark only one M (most) choice and only one L (least) choice in a set. Do not skip any sets. Answer every set. Turn this booklet over and begin. SCIENCE RESEARCH ASSOCIATES, INC. 8 259 EAST ERIE STREET, CHICAGO, ILLINOIS 60611 Copyright I960 © Science Research Associates, Inc. All rights reserved. Printed In U.$.A. Reorder No. 7-2760 6789/I-98765432 To be free to do as I choose To have others agree with me To make friends with the unfortunate To be in a position of not having to follow orders__._,__.______.__. To follow rules and regulations closely ........ . ...... . ...................... To have people notice what I do To hold an important job or office ____________________________________________ To treat everyone with extreme kindness To do what is accepted and proper ____________________________________________ To have people think of me as being important ,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,, To have complete personal freedom To know that people are on my side, ,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,,, To follow social standards of conduct ....................................... To have people interested in my well being ............................ To take the lead in making group decisions ............................ To be able to do pretty much as I please...__...,,,,,,..._.._________._ To be in charge of some important project,,,....,.. To work for the good of other people ........................................ To associate with people who are well known .......................... To attend strictly to the business at hand .............. , ................. To have a great deal of influence ............................................ To be known by name to a great many people ........................ To do things for other people To work 011 my own without direction To follow a strict code of conduct ............................................ To be in a position of authority ............................................ To have people around who will encourage me ........................ To be friends with the friendless . ........................................... To have people do good turns for me. , .................................. To be known by people who are important .............................. To be the one who is in charge... .............................................. T0 conform strictly to the rules . ._ To have otheis show me that they like me ____________________________ To be able to live my life exactly as I wish .............................. To do my duty ........................................................................... To have others treat me with understanding.........,_,_.,..._ To be the leader of the group I’m 1n To have people admire what I do ............................................. To be independent in my work ......... . ........................................ To have people act considerately toward me“ To have other people work under my direction,,.._._._',,_.__,._... T 0 spend my time doing things for others ................ \ ________________ To be able to lead my own life ............................. ............ To contribute a great deal to charity ....................................... To have people make favorable remarks about me. ,7. 7‘ Turn the page and go on. Mark your answers in column B ——> B A To be a person of influence ,, ,, . . , ,, ,, , ,, ,, ;;;;;: ;:;;;; 33;; 33;; To be neared with kindness ,,,,,, ,, , H To always maintain the highest moral standaids , , ,,,,,,,, To be p1aised by other people , , To be re l11tivel1 unbound by social conventions , ,,,,, To work for the good of society To have the affection of other people To do things 111 the approved manner go around doing favors for other people :‘I To be allowed to do whatever I want to do To be regarded as the leader To do what is socially correct fl , have others approve of what I do To make decisions for the group To share my belongings with other people To be free to come and go as I want to ‘11 help the poor and needy To show respect 10 my superiors ‘ ,— To be given compliments by other people To be in a very responsible position ' ‘0 do what is considered conventional To be 111 charge of a group of people To make all of my own decisions To receive encouragement from others , ,, To be looked up to by other people To be quick in accepting others as friends To direct others in their work To be generous toward other people , , ,,,,, To be my own boss To have understanding friends To be selected for 11 leadership position , , ,,,,,,,, , To be treated as a person of some importance To have things pretty much my own way To have other people interested in me To ha1e p1ope1 and c01rect social manners , , ,,,,,,, To be sympathetic with those who me in trouble [‘0 be very popular with other people , _ . To be free from having to obey rules ,,,,,,, , , , ........... To be in a position to tell others what to do ....................... To alwavs do what is morally right , , N , . To go out 01 m1 wav to help otheis To have people willing to o tfe1 me a helping hand To h 1111e people) eadmiie me , , ,_ ,,,,,,,,, , To alwa1 s do the approved thing T o be 11bl c to Ie111e things Iving a1ound if I wish ,,,,, , ..... APPENDIX A-5 Personal Questionnaire (general) 307 No. Location Male , Group Female Date PERSONAL QUEST IONNA 113E This questionnaire has two parts to it. The first part has to do with your contacts with schools and education, and what you know about education. You may have had considerable contact with schools and education, or you may know a great deal about education. On the other hand, you may have had little or no contact with schools or education and may have never thought much about it at all. For the purposes of this investigation, the answers of all per- sons are important. If you know very little or nothing about schools or education, your answers are important. If you know a great deal about them, your answers are important. The second part of the questionnaire has to do with personal information about you. Since the Questionnaire is completely anonymous, you may answer all of the questions freely without any concern about being identified. It is important to the study to obtain your answer to every question. 165 NO. PERSONAL_QUESTIONNAIRE Please read each question carefully and do not omit any questions. Please answer by circling the correct answer (or answers) or fill in the answer as requested. 165 SECTION 1: Experiences with Schools and Education Below are listed several different kinds of schools or edu- cational divisions. In respect to these various kinds or levels of education, which one have you had the most profes- sional or work experience with, or dogyou have the most knowledge about? This does not refer to your own education. Please answer by circling the number of the group you select. Circle only one. Elementary School (Grade School) ........... .. 1 Secondary School (High School) ............... 2 College or University ... ................ ..... 3 Other Types (Please Specify) 4 I have had no such experience ... ............ . 5 Which other groups, in addition to the one indicated above, have you also had some professional or work experience with? Please circle the number of each additional group with which you have had some experience. Elementary School (Grade School) ............. 1 Secondary School (High School) ............... 2 College or University ........................ 3 Other Types (Please Specify) 4 I have had no such eXperience ................ S NO. . 2 P.Q. 3. The following questions have to do with additional kinds of contracts you have had with schools or education. Please girolg the number of each experience that applies to you. Be sure and circle the number of every experience that applies to you. I know little or nothing about education .. ....... ..... l I have read or heard a little about schools and education .... .......... ....................... ..... ... 2 I have studied about schools and education through reading, movies, lectures, or observations .... ........ 3 A neighbor of mine works in education ................. 4 A friend of mine works in education ................... 5 Some relative works in education . ............ . ...... .. 6 My father, mother, brother, sister, wife (husband), or child works in education (in any position, pro— fessional or non-professional) ............... ......... 7 I have worked in education, as a teacher, adminis— trator, counselor, volunteer, etc. . ....... ............ 8 Other (Please Specify) 9 -——.._—-.-_7 ...... . ___._. If on the preceding three questions you indicated that you have had no personal experience with any kind of education, please skip Questions #4 through #7. If you indicated that you have had experience with one or more of the levels of education listed, please answer Questions #4 through #7. -.. .M .. w. 165 No. 3 P.Q. 4. About how much have you worked in schools or educational settings? Please circle the number of the one best answer. Less than three months ......... ..... . ...... ........... 1 Between three and six months .................... . ..... 2 Between six months and one year ....................... 3 Between one and three years ... ........................ 4 Between three and five years .... ....... ...... ..... .... 5 Between five and ten years ............................ 6 Over ten years ........ . ....................... ........ 7 Over fifteen years .... ..... ......... ..... . ........ .... 8 5. If you have ever worked in education, about what per cent of your income was derived from such work? Less than 10% ... ............... . ..................... . 1 Between 10 and 25% ... ........ . .......... ... ..... . ..... 2 Between 25 and 50% ... ..................... . ........... 3 Between 50 and 75% .......................... ... ....... 4 Between 75 and 100% ......... . ......................... S 6. If you have ever worked in education, how have you generally felt about it? I definitely have disliked it ......... .... ..... ....... l I have not liked it very much ......................... 2 I have liked it somewhat ............ ..... ........ ..... 3 I have definitely enjoyed it ............ ...... . ....... 4 165 No. 4 P.Q. 7. If you have ever worked in education for personl gain, (for example, for money or some other gain), what opportunities did you have (or do you have) to work at something else instead, that is, something else that was (or is) acceptable to you as a job? I do not know what other jobs were available or accept- able OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO .... ..... O .............. 0...... 1 No other job was available ........... . ............ .... 2 Other jobs available were not at all acceptable to me . 3 Other jobs available were not quite acceptable to me .. 4 Other jobs available were fully acceptable to me ...... 5 “'1 8. How old are you? (Write age in box) ... ................. {— .-.J. 9. ,Where were you mainly reared or "brought up" in your youth (that is, up to the age of 15 or 16)? Country .......................... . ........... . ........ 1 Country Town .......................................... 2 City .................................................. 3 City Suburb ......................................... .. 4 10. Where have you (or the main bread winner in your family) been employed during the past three years? Country ............... . ..... ..... ............ ......... 1 Country Town ......................... ... ....... ....... 2 City .. .................. . ..................... . ....... 3 City Suburb ........................................... 4 165 NO. 11. 12. 13. 14. 165 5 P.Q. Where you have mainly lived during the past three years? Country ......................... ............... ...... . 1 Country Town ........ ..... .... ........... . ..... ........ 2 City ............................. . ...... ....... ..... .. 3 City Suburb .................. . ...... .......... ..... ... 4 What is your marital status? Married ........................................ . ...... 1 Single ........................... .. ....... .. ....... ... 2 Divorced .................. ............................ 3 Widowed ..... ........ . ..................... ... ..... .... 4 Separated ............................................. 5 How many children do you have? (Please write number in box). Please answer either A or B, which applies best to your present situation. Please read both choices, than answer only one. A. If you are self-supporting, about what is your total yearly income before taxes (or, if you are married, the total yearly income in the family). Include extra income from any regular sources such as divi- dends, insurance, etc. Please write the total in the box . i If you are not self-supporting (or, if you are married, if your family is not self-supporting), what is the approximate total yearly income before taxes of the persons who mainly provide your sup- port (that is, parents, relatives or others). Make the best estimate you can. ’ ('25 No. 6 P.Q. 15. According to your answer to Question 14, about how does your income compare with that of most people in the total commun- ity where you live? Much lower .................. .......... . ..... 1 Lower ............... . ............ . ....... . ........... . 2 About the same ......................... .......... ..... 3 Higher ..... . ........................ . ......... . ....... 4 Much higher .. .................... ... .................. 5 16. How many brothers have you? (Please write number in box). 17. How many sisters have you? (Please write number in box). 18. About how does (or did) your father's income compare with that of most peOple in the community in which he lives (or lived)? Much lower ...................................... . ..... 1 Lower ................................................. 2 About the same ..................................... . . 3 Higher ........ . ......... ............ .................. 4 Much higher ......... . ............ . .................... 5 165 19. What is your religion? Catholic ............................,........ ..... .... l Protestant ................. ..... .............. . ....... 2 Jewish ...... . ........ ............ ....... ........ ...... 3 None ....... .................. .... ..... ................ 4 Other (Please Specify) j S 20. About how important is your religion to you in your daily life? I have no religion ........ .......... .............. .... 1 Not very important . ....... .... ..... .... ............... 2 Fairly important ................................. ..... 3 Very important .......... ........ ...................... 4 21. During an"average" work day, you probably have occasion to talk and make contact with other adult persons where you are employed. Estimate about what per cent of these contacts and conversations are with people you feel personally close to, whom you consider to be close friends, or that are rela- tives of yours. I do not usually talk or make contact with other adult persons where I am employed ......... ...... . ..... 2 Less than 10% .... ............. ...... .................. 3 Between 10 and 30% ..... ................... ....... ..... 4 Between 30 and 50% ....... ............ ........ ......... 5 Between 50 and 70% ................................ .... 6 Between 70 and 90% ...... .... ..................... ..... 7 More than 90% ..... . ............... .... ................ 8 165 No. 8 P.Q. 22. How important is it to you to work with people you feel personally close to? Not at all important ...... .. ...... ..... ..... .......... 1 Not very important ...... . ......... . ................ ... 2 Fairly important .......... .............. ........... ... 3 Very important ........ ................................ 4 23. Now please consider all of the personal contacts you have with people when you are not at work. Would you estimate about what per cent of your contacts apart from working hours are spent with peOple whom you know because of your jgbg that is, those who work at the same job, trade, or profession, or in the same place that you do, or that you otherwise contact in the pursuit of your job. None ........ . ............. . ............. . ...... ....... 1 Less than 10% ................................. . ....... 2 Between 10 and 30% ...................... ..... ..... .... 3 Between 30 and 50% .................. .................. 4 Between 50 and 70% ................................ .... 5 Between 70 and 90% .................................... 6 More than 90% .............. ... ........... ... ........... 7 24. What social class do you believe you are in? Lower .. .................... . ..................... ..... 1 Lower Middle ......................... . ................ 2 Middle ........................ . ....................... 3 Upper Middle ............ . ........... . ..... . ........... 4 Upper ..... ........................................ .... 5 Upper Upper ............. .. ..... . ...... .. ............ .. 6 165 NO. 25. 26. 165 9 P.Q. Which social class do you believe your father is (or was) in? Lower .............. ..... .............................. 1 Lower Middle .......................................... 2 Middle .. .............. ............ ............ . ....... 3 Upper Middle ................ .......................... 4 Upper ....... . ............ ....... ........ . ...... ....... 5 Upper Upper ............... ...... ........... ... ....... 6 About how much education do you have (Circle only one). 3 years of school or less .. ........ . ..... .... ......... l 6 years of school or less .. ........................... 2 9 years of school or less ............................. 3 12 years of school or less .. ........ .................. 4 Some college or university ........................ .... 5 A college or university degree ..... ..... ........ . ..... 6 Some graduate work beyond the first degree ............ 7 One or more advanced degrees .......................... 8 Other (Please note number of years of study or diploma obtained) _ 9 NO. 10 P.Q. 27. About how does your education compare with that of most people? Much less than most ................................. 1 Less than most ...................................... 2 About average ....................................... 3 More than most ...................................... 4 Much more than most ................................. 5 28. About how does (or did) your father's education compare with that of most peOple in his time? Much less than most ................................. 1 Less than most ...................................... 2 About average ..... ........ .......................... 3 More than most ... ........... ........................ 4 MuCh more than mOSt 00.000.000.000...00.000.00.000... 5 29. What type of living arrangement do you have? Rent a house .......... ..... ...... ...... ....... ...... 1 Rent an apartment ................ ..... .............. 2 Rent a room (meals in a restaurant, etc.) ........... 3 Purchase a room and board (rooming house, etc.) ..... 4 Own an apartment .... ..... ........................... 5 Own a house .......... ............................... 6 Other (Please Specify) ' . 7 165 NO. 30. 31. 165 ll P.Q. Please answer either A or B. Please reaggboth before answering. A. If you are renting the house in which you live, about how much money per month do you pay for rent? (Writgmmw amount in boxL ............. ..... ............ ...... ! —-t-———v. ...-.— B. If you own the house in which you live (house, apart— ment, or other), about how much money per month do you believe you could rent the house for? (Write amount in box). ....................... ..... ....... In every community each group (for example, schools, busi- nessmen, labor, the local government) has a different job to do for the community. In your community, would you say that the schools are doing an excellent, good, fair, or pgor job? How about businessmen? Labor? The local government? The doctors and hospitals? The church? (Please circle the appropriate number to indicate how you feel each job is being done). Please answer for each group. A. Elementary Schools Do not know .................................. ..... 1 Poor .............................................. 2 Fair .................................... .......... 3 Good .. ......... . ..... ........ ........ ............. 4 Excellent ..... ....... ...... .... ....... ............. 5 B. Secondary Schools Do not know ...................... . ............ .... 1 Poor ....... ...... ................................. 2 Fair ......... ..................................... 3 Good .. ....... . ..... ........ . ...... ......... ...... 4 Excellent .... ..... ............. ......... .......... S NO. 31. 165 12 P.Q. Continued from Page 11. The instructions on the previous page apply to the following sections, C through E. C. Universities Do notknow 000000000000000000000000.000000000 ..... 1 Poor 00'. 000000000000 00000000000000.000000000 ...... 2 Fair 00 0000000000000 00.000000000000000000000.0000003 GOOd 0000.0..00.0.0000000000000...00000000000000...4 Excellent ...... .............................. ..... 5 Businessmen Do not know ....................................... 1 Poor 0. ....... 00.00000000000000000000... 00000 00.0.. 2 Fair 000.0000.00.000000000000000. 00000 0000 0000000 00 3 GOOd 000000000000000000000000.0000.00000000000000.04 Excellent 00000000000000.0000.000.000.000.000000000 5 Labor DO not know 000000000 00000 0.00 000000000000 0000000.. 1 Poor 00000000 00000000000...0000000000000000000000002 Fair 00000.0000000000000000...000.00.00.000000000003 Good O.O.000.000.000000000000000...00000000000000004 Excellent ... ......... ............................. S NO. 31. 165 13 P.Q. Continued from Page 12. The instructions on Page 11 apply to the following sections, F through I. - F . Local Government DO not know. 000000000 00‘00000000000 0000000 0000 00000 1 Excellent ......................................... 5 National Government Excellent ........ ....... .... ....... . .......... .... 5 Health Services (Doctors and Hospitals) Donotknow~000000.000....00000000000000000.000000. 1 Poor 000000000000000000000000000 000000000..000000002 Excellent ...................... ............ ....... 5 Churches Do nOt know 00000000 000000.00000000000000000000000. 1 Poor 00000000000000.0000000000000.000.000.000000...2 600d 0000 000000 00000000000000.0000... 00000000 00000.4 Excellent ................ ............ ......... .... 5 No. 14 P.Q. 32. How long have you lived in your present community? Less than 1 year . .......... ......... ..... ........... 1 From 1 to 2 years ................................... 2 From 3 to 6 years ................................... 3 From 7 to 10 years .................................. 4 Over 10 years ....................................... S 33. Have you changed your residency (from one community to another) during the past two years? Please cigcle the correct number. 34. Have you changed your employment during the past tWo years? Please circle the correct number. 35. About how many times have you changed residency (communities) during the past 10 years? Please circle the correct number. None ........... ............... ...... ................ l 1 Time ...... ..... . ............... . ........... ....... 2 2 — 3 Times ................ ........... . ..... ........ 3 4 - 6 Times ....... ...... ......... ................... 4 7 - 10 Times .... ..... .. ........ . ......... ........... 5 Over 10 Times ......................... ..... ... ..... . 6 165 No. 15 P.Q. 36. About how many times have you changed jobs during the past 10 years? Please circle the correct number. None 0000.00.00.000000000.00.00000000000000000.000000 l lTime 00000.000000000000000000000000000000000.0000002 2 - 3 Times ......................................... 3‘ 4 — 6 Times ......................................... 4 7 - 10 Times ........................................ 5 Over 10 Times ........... ..... .......... ..... ........ 6 37. Please state your occupation. Briefly state the title or name of your job and the nature of your work. 38. In respect to your religion, about to what extent do you observe the rules and regulations of your religion? Please circle the correct number. I have no religion ............ .... ..... . ..... ....... I seldom 0000 00000 000000000000000000000000000.0000000002 sometimes 000000000 00000 0 00000 0.000000000000000000000 3 usually 00000000000000000000.000000000000000... 000000 4 Almost always 00000000000000.0.0000000000000000...... 5 165 NO. 39. 40. 41. 165 16 P.Q. Health experts say adding certain chemicals to drinking water results in less decay in people's teeth. If you could add these chemicals to your water with little cost to you, would you be willing to have the chemicals added? Please circle the correct number. Probably not ..... ..... ................................ 1 No ... ............. .................................... 2 Yes 00000000000000000000 000000000000000000000000000000.4 Some peOple feel that in bringing up children, new ways and methods should be tried whenever possible. Others feel that trying out new methods is dangerous. What is your feeling about the following statement? "New methods of raising children should be tried out whenever possible." Strongly disagree ..... ..... ............... .......... 1 Slightly disagree .......................... ......... 2 Slightly agree .............. . ........ .. ......... ..... 3 Strongly agree ...................................... 4 Family planning on birth control has been discussed by many people. What is your feeling about a married couple prac— ticing birth control? Do you think they are doing something good or bad? If you had to decide, would you say they are doing wrong, or rather, that they are doing right? It is always right ......... . ............ .............. 1 It is probably all right ....... ....... . ...... ......... 2 It is usually wrong .. ........ . ..... . ..... ............. 3 It is always wrong 000000000000000000000000000.000000004 NO. 42. 43. 44. 165 17 P.Q. Pe0p1e have different ideas about what should be done con- cerning automation and other new ways of doing things. How do you feel about the following statement? "Automation and similar new procedures should be encouraged (in government, business, and industry) since eventually it creates new jobs and raises the standard of living." Disagree Strongly .......... .. ........ . ........... ... 1 Disagree Slightly .......... ..... ..............,..... 2 Agree Slightly ........... ........................... 3 Agree Strongly ........ .. ........... . ........... ..... 4 Running a village, city, town, or any governmental organiza- tion is an important job. What is your feeling on the following statement? ‘"Politica1 leaders should be changed regularly, even if they are doing a good job." Strongly disagree ................................... 1 Slightly disagree .................................. 2 Slightly agree . .................. . .............. .... 3 Strongly agree ............................ . ...... ... 4 Some peOple believe that more local government income should be used for education even if doing so means raising the amount you pay in taxes. What are your feeling on this? Strongly disagree ................. . ................... 1 Slightly disagree ....... .. ........ .. ....... . ......... . 2 Slightly agree ... ..................................... 3 Strongly agree .............. . ................ ......... 4 No. 18 P.Q. 46. PeOple have different ideas about planning for education in. their nation. Which one of the following do you believe is the best way? Answergnly one. Planning for education should be left entirely to the parents 0000000000000000000000000000000.000000000000000 1 Educational planning should be primarily directed by the individual city or other local governmental unit .. 2 Education planning should be primarily directed by the national government ..... .......... .......... ...... 3 47. Some peOple are more set in their ways than others. How ’ would you rate yourself? Please circle the number of your ch01ce. I find it very difficult to change .... ..... ..... ..... . 1 I find it slightly difficult to change ........... . ..... 2 I find it somewhat easy to change my ways ........ ..... 3 I find it very easy to change my ways ................. 4 48. I find it easier to follow rules than to do things on my own. Agree strongly ................................... i ..... 1 Agree slightly ......................... ............... 2 Disagree slightly .............. ...... ................. 3 Disagree strongly ............... . ....... ..... ..... .... 4 165 No. 19 P.Q. 49. I like the kind of work that lets me do things about the same way from one week to the next. Circle the number of your choice. Agree strongly ........... .................. ....... .... 1 Agree slightly ... ....... ... ..... .......... ..... ....... 2 Disagree slightly .............. ................... .... 3 Disagree strongly ............. ....................... . 4 50. A good son will try to find work that keeps him near his parents even though it means giving up a good job in another part of the country. ' Agree strongly ... ..... ..... ............ . ......... ..... 1 Agree slightly ................ ..... . .................. 2 Disagree slightly .......... . ...... ...... .............. 3 Disagree strongly ......... .... ........................ 4 51. We should be as helpful to people we do not know as we are to our friends. Disagree strongly ......... .... ........................ l Disagree slightly .................. . .............. .... 2 Agree slightly ............ ... ......................... 3 Agree strongly .............. ......... ....... .......... 4 165 No. 20 P.Q. 52. Planning only makes a person unhappy because your plans hardly ever work out anyway. Agree strongly ........ ....................... ........ . 1 Agree slightly ................. ...... . ........ ........ 2 Disagree Slightly 0000000000000000000000000000.00000000 3 Disagree strongly ......... ........... . ...... .. ...... .. 4 53. Which one of the following requisities do you consider mogt important to make your life more happy and satisfactory in the future? Circle the single, most important choice. Nothing 0000000000000000000000000000.000000000000000000 1 More money ......... ...... ................ ...... . ...... 2 More friends ...... ....... .... ........... . ..... . ....... 3 Better job ....... ...... ............................... 4 Good health .. ........... ..... ....... ...... ..... ....... 5 Other (Please Specify) ‘ 6 54. What do you think you can do to make this possible? Please answer one of the two alternatives below. Nothing Please Specify 165 APPENDIX A-6 Personal Questionnaire: HP 329 No. Location Male Group Female Date PERSONAL QUESTIONNAIRE: HP This questionnaire deals with your contacts with physically handi- capped persons, and what you know about them. Perhaps you have had much contact with physically handicapped persons, or you may have studied abOut them. On the other hand, you may have~hadi little or no contact with physically handicapped persons, and may have never thought much about them at all. For the purposes of this investigation, the answers of all per- sons are important, so even if you know very little or nothing about physically handicapped persons your answers are important. 165 PERSONAL QUESTIONNAIRE: HP Please read each question carefully and do not omit any questions. Please answer by circling the correct answer (or answers) or fill in the answer as requested. 1. Some physically handicapping conditions are listed below. In respect to these various handicaps, which have you had the most actual experience with. Please answer by circling the number of the group you select. Circle only one. l. blind 6. disfigured (such as severe burns or scars on face) 2. partially blind 7. spastic (or cerebral 3. deaf (and deaf-mute) palsy) 4. partially deaf 8. speech disorders 5. crippled or amputated 9. none limbs 2. 'Which other groups have you also had some experience with? Please circle the number of each additional group with which you have had some experience. 1. blind 6. disfigured (such as severe burns or scars on face) 2. partially blind 7. spastic (or cerebral 3. deaf (and deaf—mute) palsy) 4. partially deaf 8. speech disorders 5. crippled or amputated 9. none limbs If on the preceding question you indicated that you have had no personal experience with physically handicapped per- sons (by circling response No. 9, please skip questions #3 through #9. If you indicated that you have had the exper- ience with one or more of the above handicapping conditions, please answer questions #3 through #9. 1165 NO. 165 2 P.Q.-'HP The following questions have to do with the kinds of exper- iences you have had with physically handicapped persons. Please circle the number of each experience that applies to ‘ypg. If more than one experience applies, please circle a number for each experience that applies. I have read or heard a little about physically handicapped persons ................................. l I have studied about physically handicapped persons through reading, movies, lectures, or observations .. 2 A friend is physically handicapped .................. 3 Some relative is physically handicapped ............. 4 I have personally worked with physically handicapped persons, as a teacher, counselor, volunteer, child care, etc. 0.000000000000000000.000000000000000000000 5 My father, mother, brother, sister, wife (husband) or child is physically handicapped .................. 6 I, myself, have a physical handicap. (Briefly, 7 please indicate the kind of handicap) Considering all of the times you have talked, worked, or in some other way had personal contact with physically handi- capped persons, about how many times has it been altogether? Please circle the number of the single best answer. Less than 10 occasions .......... .................... 1 Between 10 and 50 OCCaSions 000.000000000000000000000 2 Between 50 and 100 occasions ........................ 3 Between 100 and 500 occasions ....................... 4 More than 500 OCCaSionS 0000000.000000000000000000000 5 NO. 6. 165 3 P.Q.-HP When you have been in contact with physicially handicapped people, how easy for you, in general, would it have been 39. have avoided being with these handicapped persons? I could generally have avoided these personal contacts only at great cost or difficulty ...................... 1 I could generally have avoided these personal contacts only with considerable difficulty ..................... 2 I could generally have avoided these personal contacts but with some inconvenience 0............OOOOOQOOOOOOOO 3 I could generally have avoided these personal contacts without any difficulty or inconvenience ............... 4 During your contact with physically handicapped persons, did yougainmaterially in any way through these contacts, such as being paid, or gaining academic credit, or some such gain? No, I have never received money, credit, or any other material gain ......... ..... . ....... ................... 1 Yes, I have been paid for working with handicapped persons .0. .............. .0... ........... ......OCQOOOOO 2 Yes, I have received academic credit or other material ga in 0000000000000000 Q ..... O ........ C OOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOOO 3 Yes, I have both been paid and received academic credit 4 NO. 8. 165 4 P.Q.-HP If you have never been paid for working with handicapped persons, go on to the next question. pryou have been paid, about what per cent of your income was derived from contact with physically handicapped persons during the actual period when working with them? Less than 10% . ......... . ....... ... ........ ............ 1 Between 10 and 25% ........................ ............ 2 Between 25 and 50%......OOOOOOOOO......COOOOOOOQOOOOOO 3 Between 50 and 75% .......... ....... ...... ............. 4 More than 75% .............................. ....... .... 5 How have you generally felt about your experience with handi- capped persons? I definitely have disliked it .................... ..... l I have not liked it very much .......................... 2 I have liked it somewhat ... ..... . ................. .... 3 I have definitely enjoyed it .......................... 4 If you have ever worked with the physically handicapped for personal gain (for example, for money, or some other gain), what Opportunities did you have (or do you have) to work at something else instead: that is, something else that was (or is) acceptable to you as a job? I do not know what other jobs were available or acceptable ......OOOOOOCCOOO......QOO0.0.000.000.0000.0 1 No other jOb was available 0.0.0..........OOOOOOOOOOOO. 2 Other jobs available were not at all acceptable to me . 3 Other jobs available were not quite acceptable to me .. 4 Other jobs available were fully acceptable to me ...... 5 NO. 5 P.Q.-HP The following questions should be answered ' by all persons, regardless of whether or ' ! not they have had any personal contact with ' persons who are physically handicapped. 10. Have you had any experience with mentally retarded persons? Considering all of the times you have talked, worked, or in some other way had personal contact with mentally retarded persons, about how many times has it been altogether? Please circle the number of the single best answer. Less than 10 occasions ................................ 1 Between 10 and 50 occasions .............. ............. 2 Between 50 and 100 occasions ...... ......... ........... 3 Between 100 and 500 occasions ......................... 4 More than 500 occasions ................... ............ 5 11. Have you had any experience with emotionally ill persons? Considering all of the times you have talked, worked, or in some other way had personal contact with emotionally ill persons, about how many times has it been altogether? Please circle the number of the single best answer. Less than 10 occasions ................................ 1 Between 10 and 50 occasions ................. .......... 2 Between 50 and 100 occasions .... ...................... 3 Between 100 and 500 occasions .............. ........ ... 4 More than 500 occasions ..... .. ....... ................. 5 165 APPENDIX A—7 Personal Questionnaire: EDP 336 NO. LOCATION MALE GROUP FEMALE DATE PERSONAL QUESTIONNAIRE: EDP This questionnaire deals with your contacts with emotionally disturbed persons, and what you know about them. Perhaps you have had much contact with emotionally disturbed persons, or you may have studied about them. On the other hand, you may have had little or no contact with emotionally disturbed persons, and may have never thought much about them at all. For the purposes of this investigation, the answers of all persons are important, so even if you know very little or nothing about emotionally disturbed persons your answers are important. NO. PERSONAL QUESTIONNAIRE: EDP Please read each question carefully and do not omit any questions. Please answer by circling the correct answer (or answers) or fillixlthe answer as requested. 1. Please indicate below whether or not you have had any experience with emotionally disturbed persons. Be sure to read the definition of emotionally disturbed persons which is enclosed. Please circle only one of the follow- ing choices. Yes, I have had experience with emotionally disturbed persons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 N2, I have not had experience with emotionally disturbed persons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 If on the preceding question you indicated that you have had no personal experience with emotionally disturbed persons (by circling response No. 2), please skip Questions 2 through 8. If you indicat- ed that you had eXperience with emotionally dis- turbed persons, please answer Questions 2 - 8. The following questions have to do with the kinds of experiences you have had with emotionally disturbed persons. Please circle the number of each experience that applies to you. If more than one experience applies, please circle a number for each experience that applies. I have read or heard a little about emotionally disturbed persons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . l I have studied about emotionally disturbed persons through reading, movies, lectures, or observations. 2 A friend is emotionally disturbed . . . . . . . . . 3 Some relative is emotionally disturbed. . . . . . . A I have personally worked with emotionally disturbed persons, as a teacher, counselor, volunteer, child care, etc.. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 NO. PQ-EDP My father, mother, brother, sister, wife (husband), or child is emotionally disturbed . . . . . . . . . . 6 I, myself, have an emotional disturbance. (Briefly, please indicate the kind of emotional disturbance) 7 Considering all of the times you have talked, worked, or in some other way had personal contact with emotionally disturbed persons, about how many times has it been alto— gether? Please circle the number of the single best answer. Less than 10 occations Between 10 and 50 occasions. . . . . . . . . . . . . Between 50 and 100 occasions Between 100 and 500 occasions. UltLUNF’ More than 500 occasions. When you have been in contact with emotionally disturbed people how easy for you, in general, would it have been to have avoided being with these disturbed persons? I could generally have avoided these personal contacts only at great cost and difficulty . . . . . l I could generally have avoided these personal contacts only with considerable difficulty . . . . . 2 I could generally have avoided these personal contacts but with some inconvenience . . . . . . . . 3 I could generally have avoided these personal contacts without any difficulty or inconvenience . . A During your contact with emotionally disturbed persons, did you ggin materially in any way through these con- tacts, such as being paid, or gaining academic credit, or some gain? No, I have never'received money, credit, or any other material gain. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I Yes, I have been paid for working with disturbed persons. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Yes, I have received academic credit or other material gain. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Yes, I have both been paid and received academic credit . . . . . . A NO. PQ-EDP If you have never been paid for working with distrubed persons, go on to the next question. If you have been paid, about what percent of your income was derived from contact with emotionally disturbed persons during the actual period when working with them? Less than 10% Between 10 and 25%. Between 25 and 50%. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Between 50 and 75%. More than 75% UlthH How have you generally felt about your experience with disturbed persons? I have definitely disliked it I have not liked it very much I have liked it somewhat. I have definitely enjoyed it. . . . . . . . .1:me If you have ever worked with the emotionally disturbed for personal gain (for example, for money or some other gain), what gpportunities did you have (or do you have) to work at something else instead; that is, something else that was (or is) acceptable to you as a job? I do not know what other jobs were available or acceptable. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I No other job was available. Other jobs available were not at all acceptable to me . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Other jobs available were not quite acceptable to me Other jobs available were fully acceptable to me. . 5 The following questions should be answered by all persons, regardless of whether or not they have had any personal contact with persons who are emotionally disturbed. NO. 10. ll. PQ-EDP Have you had any experience with physically handi- capped persons? Considering all of the times you have talked, worked, or in some other way had personal contact with physically handicapped persons, about how many times has it been altogether? Please circle the number of the single best answer. Less than 10 occasions. Between 10 and 50 occasions Between 50 and 100 occasions. Between 100 and 500 occasions mil—DUMP More than 500 occasions Have you had any experience with mentally retarded persons? Considering all of the times you have talked, worked, or in some other way had personal contact with mentally retarded persons, about how many times has it been altogether? Please circle the number of the single best answer. Less than 10 occasions. Between 10 and 50 occasions . . . . . . . . . . . Between 50 and 100 occasions. Between 100 and 500 occasions . . . . . U‘l-IZ'WNI-J More than 500 occasions . . . . . . . . . Please indicate below if any of your own children are/or have been physically handicapped or emotionally disturbed? (Circle only one.) Yes, I have, or have had, a child who was physically handicapped . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I Yes, I have, or have had, a child who was emotionally disturbed . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Yes, I have had a child who was both physically handicapped and emotionally disturbed . . . . . 3 None of my children have been physically handicapped or emotionally disturbed. APPENDIX A-8 Definitions of Physical Handicap 3A2 DEFINITIONS What is meant by "physical handicap." The words "physically handicapped" will be used often in the questions and statements that follow. Where these words are used, they will include persons with any of the following handicaps: 1. blind persons--those who have no useful sight at all. partly blind persons--those who have some sight but have trouble reading and getting about even with glasses. deaf persons--those who have no useful hearin: at all. partly deaf persons--those who have some hearing but have trouble understanding other persons even with a hearing aid. cripples or amputees—-those who have arms or legs that have been paralyzed or removed even though they may be of some use with artificial hands or legs. spastic (or cerebral palsy)--those who have poor control and coordination of their leg, arm, and head movements. Movements are often Jerky and speech hard to under- stand. disfigured--those who have been obviously damaged about the face, such as with burns or scars, so that the face has been changed. APPENDIX A-9 Definition of Emotional Disturbance 3AA 3A5 DEFINITION What is meant by "emotional disturbance?" The words "emotionally disturbed" will be used often in the questions and statements that follow. Where these words are used, they will include persons with the following disturbance: Those children or adults whose behaviors, feelings or emotions cause them to have difficulties with everyday problems which they are unable to solve. APPENDIX B-l Administration Procedures 3A6 3A7 ATTITUDES AND VALUES TOWARD MENTAL AND PHYSICAL DISABILITIES The purpose of this study is to investigate attitudes and values of parents toward physically handicapped and emo— tionally disturbed persons. The problems of mental and physical handicaps have assumed much greater importance in the life of a modern community. The Betty Jane Memorial Center is a unique place providing facilities for the treatment of various kinds of disabilities. This is a growing institution particularly geared to the local needs of the community. Its service facilities are constantly expanding with a view to meeting the needs of the community in the best possible manner. The future planning of these facilities, however, should depend upon a proper evaluation of the parents' attitude and values. The present investigation attempts to deal with the attitudes and values of the parents who are using the services at both Betty Jane Rehabilitation Center and Sandusky Valley Guidance Center. (Tiffin) 3A8 ATTITUDES AND VALUES TOWARD MENTAL AND PHYSICAL DISABILITIES The purpose of this study is to investigate attitudes and values of parents toward emotionally disturbed and physically handicapped persons. The problems of mental illness and physical disability have assumed much greater importance in the life of a modern community. Both the government and the civic leaders have helped establish mental health clinics and rehabilitation centers in order to provide professional services to the public. These service facilities are constantly expanding with a view to meeting the needs of the community more effectively. However, the future planning of these facilities should depend upon a proper evaluation of the parents' attitudes and values in respect to the emotionally disturbed and.the physically disabled. The present investigation, tflius, attempts to determine the various factors that might bee associated with attitudes toward emotionally disturbed avid physically handicapped persons. (Mount Pleasant) v 3A9 INSTRUCTIONS FOR ANSWERING THE QUESTIONS The enclosed questionnaires attempt to determine the parents' attitude toward physically handicapped and emotionally disturbed persons. Please note, in a study like this, there are no right or wrong answers. We want you to answer how you feel about certain things. Therefore, we do not want your name on the questionnaire. In this manner no one will know your answers. Please answer quickly, with your first idea, and do not spend a lot of time thinking about each item. If, however, there is no answer that exactly fits what you would like to answer, please choose the alternative nearest to your desired answer. Order of Administration of Questionnaires Page of Definitions(Physical Handicap) Education Scale Survey of Interpersonal Values Personal Questionnaire Handicapped Persons Scale Personal Questionnaire: HP (Handicapped Persons) Page of Definition (Emotional Disturbance) Emotionally Disturbed Persons Scale Personal Questionnaire: EDP (Emotionally Disturbed Persons) \DCDNQU‘IEWNH The questionnaires have been arranged in the order men— tioned above. Please answer them in the same order. For your convenience and to avoid confusion, the questionnaires have been numbered (l-2—3—A-5—6-7-8-9) in the upper left hand cor- ner of each set of questions. Take each set one at a time and when you have answered all questions, move on to the next set. Please do not look at them ahead of time. Your cooperation is greatly appreciated! APPENDIX B—2 Basic Variables of the Study 350 BASIC VARIABLES - INTERNATIONAL A. Attitudes Toward Education 1 Traditional attitudes, Items 3, 4, 6, 10, ll, 12, 13, 14, 18, 19 - Content Raw Score total Adjusted total score (dichotomized) 2 Traditional attitudes, Items 3, 4, 6, 10, ll, 12, 13, 14, 18, 19 - Intensity Raw Score total Adjusted total score (diChotomized) 3 Progressive attitudes, Items 1, 2, 5, 7, 8, 9, 15, l6, 17, 20 - Content Raw Score total Adjusted total score (dichotomized) 4 Progressive attitudes, Items 1, 2, 5, 7, 8, 9, 15, 16, 17. 20 - Intensity Raw Score total Adjusted total score (dichotomized) B. Contact with Education (Q'aire) 1 Levels of education experienced Q'aire, Item 1 (primary contact) Q'aire, Item 2 (additional contacts - no. kinds of) 2 Varieties of contact with education Q'aire, Item 3 3 Amount of contact (work) with education Q'aire, Item 4 4 Personal gain through working in education Q'aire, Item 5 (%.of income) 5 Alternative Opportunities available Q'aire, Item 7 (refers to other possible employment) 6 Enjoyment of contact Q'aire, Item 6 (2. Aid to Education - Financial (Q'aire) Item 44 (local) Item 45 (federal or national) £565 G. 565 2 BASIC VARIABLES r INTERNATIONAL Education Plannigg (Q'aire) Item 46 Interpersonal Values - Gordon Scale mUltthl-J scores: Support scores: Conformity scores: Recognition (comparative score) scores: Independence scores: Benevolence (asset score) scores: Leadership (comparative score) t‘U'Jl-ISUOUI Demographic, S.E.S., Other Control Data (All from Q'aire) .WN (DVOU‘ID ll 12 Education (self-amount), Item 26 Occupation (specific), Item 37 Income and rental (S. E. Class) Item 14 (income - yearly, self-family) Item 30 (rental) Age: Item 8 Sex: Front sheet of questionnaire Marital status: Item 12 Number of children: Item 13 Size of family: Item 16 (brothers - do not use) Item 17 (sisters - do not use) Items 16 and 17 (siblings) Housing (type of), Item 29 Mobility: Residency, Items 32, 33 and 35 Card 4, Col. 25 Occupational, Items 34 and 36 Rural-Urban Status: Items 9, 10 and 11 Employment status - current: Item 37 Satisfaction with institutions (Q'aire) 1 2 Satisfaction with elementary schools Item Bl-A Satisfaction with secondary schools Item 31—B Satisfaction with universities Item 31-C 565 3 BASIC VARIABLES - INTERNATIONAL 4 Satisfaction with businessmen Item 31-D 5 Satisfaction with labor Item 31-E 6 Satisfaction with local government Item 3l—F 7 Satisfaction with national government Item 31-G 8 Satisfaction with health services Item 31-H 9 Satisfaction with churches Item 31—I Self-Statements (Q'aire) Comparative income status - self: Item 15 Comparative income - father: Item 18 Comparative social class - self: Item 24 Comparative social class - father: Item 25 Comparative education - self: Item 27 Comparative education - father: Item 28 O‘U'IDWNH Religiousity Questionnaire (Q'aire) 1 Religious affiliation: Item 19 2 Perceived importance: Item 20 3 Perceived norm conformity: Item 38 Personalism Questionnaire (Q'aire) l Orientation toward job personalism a Statement of extent of personalism on job: Item 21 b Perceived importance of personal relations: Item 22 2 Diffusion of personal relationships Percent of job-social overlap: Item 23 3 Familialism: Item 50, (Son's work) 4 Other orientation: Altruism: Item 51 Attitudes Toward Change (Q'aire) 1 Health practices (water): Item 29 2 Child-rearing practices: Item 40 3 Birth control practices: Item 41 565 4 BASIC VARIABLES - INTERNATIONAL 4 Political leadership change: Item 43 5 Automation: Item 42 6 Self Conception Item 47 (Perceived self-rigidity) Item 48 (Adherence to rules) Item 49 (Job regularity and rigidity) 7 Future orientation Item 52 (Planning - personal) Item 53 (Requisites for happiness) Item 54 (Achievement of happiness) Attitudes Toward Handicapped Persons 1 Handicapped Persons Scale, Items 1-20 - Content Raw Score total Adjusted total score (dichotomized) 2 Handicapped Persons Scale, Items 1-20 - Intensity Raw Score total Adjusted total score (dichotomized) Contact with Handicapped Persons 1 Kinds of handicapped persons experienced P.Q.-HP, Item 1 (most contact) P.Q.-HP, Item 2 (additional contacts - no. of) 2 Varieties of relationship with handicapped P.Q.-HP, Item 3 3 Frequency of contact with physically handicapped P.Q.-HP, Item 4 4 Ease of avoidance of contacts with handicapped P.Q.-HP, Item 5 5 Personal gain through working with handicapped persons P.Q.-HP, Item 6 (experienced gain) P.Q.-HP, Item 7 (% of income) 6 Alternative opportunities available P.Q.—HP, Item 9 (refers to other possible employment) 7 Enjoyment of contact with physically handicapped P.Q.-HP, Item 8 8 Frequency of contact with mentally retarded persons P.Q.-HP, Item 10 9 Frequency of contact with emotionally disabled persons P.Q.-HP, Item 11 APPENDIX B-3 Rationale and Procedures for Producing Item Directionality 356 Rationale and Procedures for Producing Item "Directionaly" in the Following Scales: 1. Handicapped Persons Scale 2. Hearing Handicapped Persons Scale 3. Blind Persons Scale John E. Jordan John E. Felty September 30, 1965 357 The rationale for reversing content scoring on the HP scale items 2, 5, 6, ll, 12. a. All of the other items of the scale state either a difference between HP's and others, or a negative characteristic--therefore, ggreement with these items indicates less acceptance (according to Yuker—Block). b. The 5 items mentioned above are statements of similarity between HP’s and others, therefore, ggreement indicates more acceptance. In order to make the "direction" of acceptance the same for all items, the scoring was reversed on these 5, so that peOple who disagreed with statements of similar- ity would get a higher score. c. After this reversal, high scores on each of the items is supposed to indicate less acceptance. d. In the dichtomoization procedure (Felty, by hand) there was a final reversal of scoring on all items in order to make a high (1) score be favorable, and a low (9) score unfavorable for each item. It is, of course, not necessary to make this final step, but it is more convenient for my thinking, and a more usual procedure, to make more favorable scores higher. For Dickie and Weir, the positively-stated items are not all precise statements of similarity, but the items can be divided into those in which agreement with the item indicates unfavorable attitudes, and those in which agreement indicates favorable attitudes. This is by inspection, of course, and it is possible that empirical test could indicate that a given item was placed in the wrong category. Such an item would probably scale nega— tively with the others, and scoring would have to be reversed for this item in computing total scores for each subject. This question is independent of the question of whether a high total score indicates favorable or unfavorable attitudes, which is a question of item content. If you want a high total score to indicate favorable attitudes, (see l,d above), one way would be to follow Felty's procedure on the H—P scale (as outlined above and in the code book). However, if the computer dichotomization is used, it will be necessary to reverse the total scores after the dichotomized total scores have been computed for each person for scale items (this is a hand procedure based on new dichotomized totals--either machine or hand- dichotomized--and takes place as the last two operations 358 in the "scale and intensity analysis" subsection of the "flow and control chart." That is, after scaling, even by computer, someone still has to figure out the new total scores for each respondent for each "scale," enter these into unused columns of the data sheet, and then have them punched into Deck 1 for further analysis.) If after dichotomization, total scores ranged from g to gg (possible with 20 dichtomoized statements scored gll) and high scores indicate unfavorable attitudes, the scoring can be reversed by making up an equivalence table to transpose the scores; e.g., Total Scores Dichotomized Reversed "Unfavorable" "Favorable" 2O 0 19 l 18 2 17 3 etc. etc. Another way of doing this would avoid the necessity of making two sets of reversals; i.e., instead of reversing the similarity-type items (see above, 1.b), reverse the others. This means many more items have to be reversed initially in the scoring (Kathy's job) but that no further reversal is necessary since a high score for each item would then presumedly indicate a favorable or accepting response. Although this would be more time— consuming for Kathy, it would save time later and is not as complicated. (Note: it will still be necessary to obtain new scale item total scores by a hand procedure after dichotomization and scaling as indicated on p. 2). For the Blind Persons Scale (Dickie) a high score (strong agreement) indicates favorable attitude for items 2, 101 13, 11LL l7, l9. For the Hearing Handicapped Persons Scale (Weir) a high score (strong agreement) indicates favorable attitude for items 1, 73,10; 15. If the scores are reversed for these items, a high total score will indicate unfavorable or unaccepting attitudes, and a further reversal following dichotomization would 359 be advisable (as on pages 1 and 2). If scores are reversed for all other items, a high total score will indicate favorable or accepting attitudes, and no further reversal will be necessary. 3. For Sinha (Emotionally Disturbed Persons Scale-EDP) the procedures follow exactly those Of Felty for the HP scale. (See pages 1-10 of codebook number 865). A. Following is a summary of the above procedures to be used by all studies: a. in initial scoring, reverse favorably stated items (usual procedure) i.e., those items mentioned specifically by number. b. submit for dichotomization and scale analysis by computer. c. for scale items obtain new total scores for each respondent. d. convert these total scores by inverting the order (e.g., bottom of page 2).High score now indicates favorable attitude. e.' enter scale scores (converted) onto data sheets in Open columns. f. have scale scores punched into Deck 2 at data processing. g. use new scale score totals in subsequent analyses (Anova, MRA, etc.). h. since the intensity items are all clearly directional, from low to high intensity, there would be no reason for making any reversals. 1As mentioned before, a possible complication can arise with items which scale negatively with the other items in the Lingoes procedure. This would seem to indicate that the prejudgment about whether the item was "favorable" or "unfav- orable" was in error, and would require a reversal of scoring for this item in obtaining a total scale score. That is, all "Q's" would be scored as "l's" and vice versa (as Lingoes states it, the item has been "reflected"). John E. Jordan John E. Felty APPENDIX B—A (a) International Study (b) Special Instructions for Tiffin Study 360 CODE BOOK CROSS CULTURAL ATTITUDES TOWARD EDUCATION: THEIR NATURE AND DETERMINANTS INTERNATIONAL STUDY* John E. Jordan College of Education Michigan State University August 25, 1965 INSTRUCTIONS FOR THE USE OF THIS CODE BOOK 1. Code Q_Or 9Q will always mean Not Applicable or Nothing, except as noted. 2. Code i_for a one column no response, or —9 for a two column no response, or —99 for a three column no response will mean there was NO Information or Respondent did not answer. 3. In each case in the following pages the column to the left con- tains the column number of the IBM card; the second column con- tains the question number from the questionnaire; the third column (item detail) contains an abbreviated form Of the item; and the fourth column contains the code within each column Of the IBM card with an explanation Of the code. The fifth COIL .222 (recode) is reserved to later indicate recoding after the item count is finished; i.e., after all data is key punched, run the data through the M.S.U° computer (ACT II, FCC, and/or Single-Column Frequency Distributions) to determine the pat- terns Of response alternatives to a question. This will indi— cate if regrouping, etc., need to be considered for the item. 4. Coder instructions always follow a line across the page and are clearly indicated. 5. In some cases when codes are equal to others already used, they are not repeated each time, but reference is made to a previous code or the immediately previous code with "same". 6. Under Code, the first number is the questionnaire question alternative and the second number is the actual code which is entered on the data sheets (i.e., 1—4; one l is the question- naire question alternative and g is the code). * This code book is specifically for the United States sample thru Card 4. Limited modifications and/Or additions are made in certain nations and/or states. Special instructions are appended for each study before scoring that sample. 865 /<,' \ 865 Column-Ques. 1,2,3 Face Sheet 8655 CARD 1 Item Detail SEES Nation and UNITED Location 001 - 002 - 003 - 004 - 005 ~ 006 — 007 — 008 — 009 - 010 - 011 - 012 - 013 — Page 1—1 Recode* STATES Mich., Mt. Pleasant Mich., Cadillac Mich., Ann Arbor Mich., Port Huron Mich., Lansing Mich., Walden Woods Mich., Flint Mich., Misc., Ka1., Mid. Kansas, Wichita Ohio, Tiffin West Virginia Kentucky Georgia LATIN AMERICA 101 - 102 - 103 - 104 - 105 - 106 — EUROPE 201 - 202 - 203 - 204 - 205 - 206 - 207 — says. 301 - 302 — 303 _ 304 - AFRICA 401 - 402 — 403 - Costa Rica Colombia Peru Argentina Mexico Surinam England Holland Belgium France Yugoslavia Denmark Germany Israel Japan India Formosa Kenya Rhodesia South Africa 34a Columnfgues. 4,5 Face Sheet 6,7 Face Sheet 8 Face Sheet 9 (Code derived from Col's T22,‘23, Card 1) 10 New 11,12 Face Sheet 13,14 Face Sheet Item Detail Group Number (adminis- tration) Respondent Number Sex of Respondent Occupational Recode anterest group) Occupational Recode (Spec. Ed., Rehab. SER)* Deck or Card Number Project Director, location and con- tent area * If respondent is not an SER "educational person", he received a i, 865 Page 1-2 Code Recode* 01 - 99 Check Special Instructions 01 - 99 l - Masculine 2 - Feminine l - Code 01 — O9, Rehab., Spec. Ed. 2 - Code 10 - 19, Education 3 - Code 20 — 45, Profes- sional, Business, Medical 4 - Code 50 - 86, White Col- lar, Blue Collar, Laborer 1 - Teacher, Educable Retarded, (Type A and Type C) 2 - Teacher, Trainable Retarded (Type B) 3 - Teacher, Hearing 4 - Teacher, Vision 5 - Speech Correction 6 - Visiting Teacher (Also Social Worker) 7 - Diagnostician 8 - Other (Professors, Supts., Administrators, etc.) + - Non-teacher 01 LATIN AMERICA 01 Felty: Costa Rica (total — pilot study) 02 Friesen: Peru and Colombia (total) 03 Taylor: Costa Rica (country study) CARD 1 Page 1-3 Column—Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 13,14 Face Sheet UNITED STATES (continued) 31 Sinha: Ohio (parents- M. R., emot. dist. and normal) 32 Dickie: Kansas (total and blind scale) 33 Weir: Kansas (total and deaf scale) 34 Mader: Michigan (spec- ial educ. - intra) 35 Jordan: Michigan — Mt. Pleasant (Spec. Ed.) ASIA 51 Cessna: Japan (total plus university stu— dents and government employees) EUROPE 7l Boric: Yugoslavia (total) 72 Fabia: France (total) 73 Hansen: Denmark (total) 74 Loring: England (total) 75 RObaye: Belgium (total) 76 Schweizer: Netherlands (total) 77 Kreider: Europe (total) 15,16 Face Sheet Day Of Admin- 01 to 31 istration (Use the actual day) 17,18 Face Sheet Month Of 01 - January Adminis- 02 - February tration 03 — March 865 OJ Column:gues. 17,18 Face Sheet (continued) 1. 19,20 Face Sheet 21 Face Sheet 22,23 37 Q'aire * See page 4-2 865 Item Detail Year Of Adminis- tration Type Of .Adminis- stration Occupation of Respon- dent* (Spe- cific) Page 1-4 Code Recode* 10 — October 11 - November 12 - December 64 - 1964 65 - 1965 66 - 1966 70 - 1970 1 Group 2 Self-administered 3 Interview, individual + NO information 401 - 09) Rehab. & Spec. Ed. 01 - A11 administrative persons, public and private schools or agencies 02 - Teachers, elem. and secondary academic and vocational 03 - School Special Services (Psych., soc. work, speech, etc.) 04 - University teachers, 05 06 07 08 09 professors, researchers, specialists, etc. - Medical (Doctors, Den- tists, etc.) - Other professional (Psych., Soc. worker, Speech, etc., not pri- marily in public or private schools) - Para-medical (Nurse, O.T., R.T., P.T., ect.) - Unskilled Help (Hospital aide, janitor, any non- prof., non-tech. role) - Other Columnfiguesa‘ Item Detail 22,23 37 Q'aire Occupation (continued) of Respon- dent* (Spe- cific) *See page 4-2 865 £2.d_e Page 1-5 Recode* (10 - 19) Educationalgpersonnel other than Rehab. and Spec. Ed. 10 - Elementary teachers, (include elem. v.p.'s, counselors, etc.) 11 - Secondary teachers 12 - Guidance and personnel workers (psych., social work, counselor if not elementary) 13 - Other special services (Speech, spec. teacher, audiometric, etc.) 14 - Administrative (elem., sec., central office adm., including elem. principal, sec. v.p. and princ., etc., in non-teach.) 15 - University teachers, professors, researchers, specialists, etc. 16 - 19 Open (20 — 29) Medical, other than Rehab. and Spec. Ed. 20 - General practitioners 21 - Surgeons 22 - Psychiatrists or psycho- analysts 23 - Dentists 24 - All other medical spec- ialties 25 - Open 26 - Tech. and Prof.: Nurse, O.T., P.T., R.T., Audio, etc. 27 - Non-tech. and non-prof.: aide, janitor, attendant, etc. 28 - 29 Open CARD 1 Page 1-6 Columnjgues. Item Detail Code Recode* 22,23 37 Q'aire Occupation (30 - 39) Professional and (continued) Of Respon- Technical, not Spec. Ed. and dent* (Spe- Rehab. or Medical or Educ. cific) 30 - Engineers (degrees): civil, electrical, mechanical, etc. 31 - Lawyers, attorneys, public accountants 32 - Ministers, clergymen 33 - Musicians 34 - Clinical psychologist 35 - Researchers, scientists, not primarily in education 36 - Social workers, etc. 37 - 39 Other (40 - 45) Business and Industry, Managers, Officials, prop.'s 4O - Gov't and other bureau- cratic Officials: public administrators and Offi- cers, union Officials, stage inspectors, public utility, telephone Offic- ials, etc. 41 - Manufacturing, industrial officials, exec's, etc. 42 - Non-mfg., service, indus- try: bankers, brokers, insurance, real estate 43 - Retail trades: food, clothing, furniture, gaso- line, vehicle sales, etc. 44 - General: i.e., manager executive, etc., no other qualifications 45 - Open (46 - 49) Farm ownersL,Operators and managers Of large farms, e.g., heavy equipment andZor many empl. * See page 4-2 865 CARD 1 . Page 1-7 Columnjgues. Item Detail Code Recode* 22,23 37 Q'aire Occupation 46 - Farm owner (continued) Of Respon- 47 - Farm Operator (renter) dent* (Spe- 48 - Farm manager cific) 49 - Open (50 - 59) White Collar: Office, clerical, etc. 50 - Clerical and similar: tellers, bookkeepers, cashiers, secretaries, shipping clerks, attend- ants, telephone Operators, library asst's, mail clerks and carriers, file clerks, etc. 51 - Sales workers: advertising, sales clerks, all mfg., wholesale, retail and other 52 - Small shopkeeper or dealer 54 - 59 Open (60 - 69)7B1ue Collar: crafts- men, foremen, and kindred work 60 - Craftsmen: carpenters, bakers, electricians, plumbers, machinists, tailors, toolmakers, photographers, etc. 61 - Foremen: all construc- tion, mfg., transporta- tion and communication, and other industries 62 - Servicemen: telegraph, telephone, etc. 63 - Mechanics and repairmen 64 - Shoemakers, roofers, painters, and plasterers 65 - Merchant marine, sailors (non-military) *See page 4—2 865 /(rul CARD 1 Page 1-8 Column:gues. Item Detail Code Recode* 22,23 37 Q'aire Occupation 66 - Bus and cab drivers, (continued) of Respon— motormen, deliverymen, dent* (Spe- chauffeurs, truck and cific) tractor drivers 67 - Operatives Of all other mech. equipment (machine, vehicle, misc. mfg.) 68 — 69 Open (10 - 74) Serivce and Private Household workers) 70 - Private household: laun- dress, housekeeper, cook 71 - Firemen and policemen, sheriffs, and baliffs 72 - Attendents, professional and personal (valet, mas- seur, misc. mfg.) 73 - Misc. attendents and services: hospital attendents, bootblacks, cooks 74 - Open (75 - 79) Military Personhel 75 - Ranking Officers, all services (Navy Commander and up, Army and Marines Colonel“and'up) 76 - Junior Officers,‘Army and Air 77 - Junior Officers, Navy and Marines 78 - Non-commissioned personnel, Army and Air 79 - Non-commissioned personnel, Navy and Marines (80 - 86) Laborers * See page 4-2 865 Page l-9 Recode* Small farm owners, renters, and farm laborers (small farm has no heavy equipment, provides minimal income and substance, employs 3 or less persons, full or part time, except for migrant help) Non-mfg., non-industrial: fishermen, hunters, lumber- men, miners, gardeners, teamsters, garage laborers, etc. Manufacturing Of durable goods: wood, clay, stone (stonecutter), metal, glass plastic,. machinery, of all kinds Mfg. of non-durable goods: food (bakery, beverages, etc.). tobacco, clothing, cloth, paper, printing, chemicals, rubber, leather, etc. Non-mfg. industries: rail- road, construction, trans- portation, workers, etc. 86 Open No employment gplumnegpes. gpemypetail Code 22,23 37 Q'aire Occupation 8O - (continued) of Respon- dent* (Spe- cific) 81 - 82 — 83 - 84 - 85 - 187) 87 - * Instructions for Coder: OCCUPATIONS, Persons that haven't worked, such as housewives, students or others who have never had a regular occupation COLUMNS 22-23. Coding information is derived from two sources: 1. Occupational description Of groups as listed by the administrator. 2. Personal statements by the respondents in Question 37 of the questionnaire. Question 37 is the primary source Of information. If vague or from notes Of administrator. * See page 4-2 865 incomplete, score entirely Columntgues. Item Detail 24 37 Q'aire Current Employment Status* 25 1 thru A11 ques- thru 20 H-P tions in 44 Content** handicap- ped per- sons scale are to be scored from 33 data . See instruc- tions below. btur0k4 I Code .5me I Page 1-10 Recode* Employed or self-employed Retired Temporarily out Of work Housewife, but formerly employed Unable to work (other than retired or housewife) but formerly employed Student or persons trained for employment but not work- ing for various reasons , strongly disagree disagree agree strongly agree bUNI—J Q. ~ * Instructions for Coder: EMPLOYMENT STATUSL COLUMN 24. Code from questionnaire Question 31_if person clearly states employ— ment status. If nO employment stated, and no indication with certainty from the administrator, score‘1. ** Instructions for Coder: HANDICAPPED PERSONS SCALE SCORING, COLUMNS 25-44. INOTE: CERTAIN STEPS AND PROCEDURES ARE THE SAME FOR THE EDUCATION SCALE AS FOR THE HANDICAPPED PERSONS SCALE. THESE PROCE- DURES WILL BE WRITTEN IN CAPITAL LETTERS. The content part Of the question is the first half of the question (i.e., the first score). 1“. Reverse the content response numbering for the Handicgpped Persons Scale (NOT the intensity response number) for items — The number of response l on data sheets., .2 .3. 5.. £365 2,_§,_§,.ll, and l2, as follows: is changed to I and scored directly .4. .2 Z .1. CARD 1 Page l-ll Column-Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 2. Special instructions for §Q_RESPONSE. Count the number Of NO RESPONSE items, if more than §_Occur, do not score respondent for this scale. If there are p 9£_less in total, and 3_9£_less in sequence, the NO RESPONSE statement is to be scored either l_or 2.by the random procedure of coin flipping. If a head is Obtained, the sCOre assigned will be l. If a tail is obtained, the score assigned will be 2, 3. TOTAL THE RAW SCORES FOR EACH RESPONDENT AND WRITE THE TOTALS ON THE TRANSCRIPTION DATA SHEET DIRECTLY BELOW THE COLUMN TOTALED.* 4. INTENSITY RAW SCORES FOR EACH STATEMENT ARE TO BE SCORED ON THE DATA SHEET EXACTLY AS THEY APPEAR ON THE QUESTIONNAIRE: i.e., IF.l IS CIRCLED IN THE INTENSITY SECTION OF QUESTION ONE, SCORE IT AS l_ON THE CORRESPONDING SECTION OF THE TRANSCRIPTION SHEET. 5. Dichtomization Procedures (i.e., forfMSA - applied to all scales). a) Using raw data scores (i.e., the/actual number circled by the respondent) via the HaftersOn pg: Program on the M.S.U. CDC 3600, determine the point éf_least error for each item on the content scales. b) Using this point (i.e., between l_and g, or between g_and §_Or between 3_and 4) rescore the items, via recode cards, as Q, l_via the Hafterson MSA Program on the M.S.U. CDC 3600 to determine which items form §_scale. Run at both .01 and .05 level. c) For Handicapped Persons ScaleA items are scored 9 above the column break, l below the column break. For edugation Spale scoring, the reverse is true: items are scored l above the column break, 9 below the column break. d) Using the same procedure in point 5—a above, determine the CUT points for the intensity component nyeach item. * By this procedure, the possible range of scores is from Q to 89. Doubling the Obtained score will approximate scores Obtained by the method of Yuker, 33 gl., (1960, p. 10) 1.HP scale, blind scale, and deaf scale. 865 CARD 1 Page 1-12 Column-gues. Item Detail Code Recode* 5. e) Enter the MSA Program with the CUT points for the intensity component and scale as in Point NO. 5-b for content. f) Adjusted total scores for content and intensity. Sum the didhfiomized content and intensity scores (i.e., 9,.l) Obtained by the above procedure for each respondent on these items that scaled for both content and intensity. Maximum score will be l §_the number gf_the same items that scaled gp_both content and intensity. g) Zero Point. Using only the items that scaled for both con- tent and intensity, plot and determine the ”zero point" for each cultural group (or other desired groupings) via the method detailed on pages 221-234 by Guttman (1950). 6. Dichotomization Procedure (alternative to no. 5 above). Attempt to program the CUT Program into the MSA so that both procedures under 55a and b are conducted jointly. 45 1 thru Handicapped l - 1, not strongly at all thru 20 §;g Persons 2 - 2, not very strongly 64 Intensity* Scale 3 - 3, fairly strongly Intensity 4 - 4, very strongly 1. Except for NO RESPONSE, intensity scores are to be determined as noted in the preceding section regarding Content. 2. Those scales which are rejected because Of an excess Of NO RESPONSE items in respect to content will Of course also be rejected for intensity. Intensity questions which are unscored, but which occur when the content part Of the ques- tion is scored, will be scored as follows: If content score is l_or 4, score intensity 4, If content score is 2_Or 3, score intensity just below the mean intensity score for that item: i.e. mean intensity of the group. * Instructions for Coder: HANDICAPPED PERSONS SCALE, INTENSITY, COLUMNS 45—64.‘ See instructions 1 and 2 above and 3 on the next page. 865 CARD 1 Page 1-13 Column—Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 3. Intensity questions which are unscored, and which occur when the content part Of the question is also unscored, will be scored at the highest point below the respondent's own median on the other intensity questions in the questionnaire; i.e., if respondent generally scored intensity questions either 4 or 3, so that the median was in between_3 and 4, score NO RESPONSE_§, and so forth. 65 3,4,6, Education 1 - 1, strongly disagree thru 10,11 Scale Tradi-. 2 - 2, disagree 74 12,13 tional, Con- 3 - 3, agree 14,18 tent Respon- 4 - 4, strongly agree 19* .ggs ** 1. Items are to be scored on the transcription sheet as circled by the respondent. 2. Follow the procedures outlined in caps on Pages 1-10, 1-11, and 1-12 for the Handicapped Persons Scale. Be sure to score only those items indicated above as applying to the education traditional scale, content. * The traditional and the progressive scales are both in the Kerlinger education scale but the responses are scored separ- ately on the transcription sheet. ** ipstructions for Coder: EDUCATION SCALE, TRAPITIONALL CONTENT, COLUMNS 65-74. See instructions 1 and 2 on page 1-13. 865 Column:Ques. 1,2,3 10 11,12 13,14 15,16 17,18 19,20 21 865 Face Face Face Face 37 Q 37 Q Face Face Face Face Face Face Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet 'aire 'aire Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet Item Detail Nation and Location Group Number Respondent Number Sex of Respondent Occupational Recode (Interest group) Occupational Recode (Spec. Ed.- Rehab. SER) Deck or Card Number Project Director Day of Adminis- tration Month of Adminis- tration Year of Adminis~ tration Type of Adminis- tration Code Same Ol - 01 - Same Same Same 02 Same and 01-31 01-12 Same as Card 1, Same as 99 99 as as as as 1-3 as Card 1, Card Card Card Card Card Page 2-1 Recode* page 1-1 page page 1-2 page 1-2 pages 1-2 page 1-4 page 1-4 CARD 2 Page 2-2 Column-Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 22.23 Face Sheet Occupation Same as Card 1. pages of Respond- 1-4 through 1-9 ent 24 Face Sheet Current Same as Card 1. page 1—10 Employment Status 25 3.4.6.10. Education 1 - 1. not strongly at all thru 11.12.13. Scale. Tra- 2 - 2. not very strongly 34 14.18.19 ditional. 3 - 3. fairly strongly Intensity 4 - 4. very strongly Responses* 35 1.2.5.7. Education 1 - 1. strongly disagree thru 8.9.15. Scale. Pro- 2 - 2. disagree 44 16.17.20 _gressive. 3 - 3. agree Content 4 - 4. strongly agree Responses** * Instructions for coder: EDHQAIIQN.EQALE. TRADITIONAL. INTENs SIT . COLUMNS 24-33. Intensity questions are scored as indica- ted in caps on pages 1-11. 1-12 and 1-13 and as noted before. Handicapped Persons Scale. pages 1-10. 1-11 and 1-12. instruc- tions 1 through 5. **_;Q§tructions for Coder: EDUCATION SCALE. PROGRESSIVE. CONTENT. 1. Items are to be scored exactly as circled. 2. Follow the procedures outlined in caps on pages 1-11. 1-12 and 1-13. Handicapped Persons ggale. Be sure to score only those items indicated above as belonging to the education progressive scale content. 865 Column-Ques. 22.23 Face Sheet 24 Face Sheet 25 thru 34 35 thru 44 3.4.6.10. 11.12.13. 14.18.19 1.2.5.7. 8.9.15. 16.17.20 * Instructions for coder: SIT . QQLUMN§ 24-33. ted in caps on pages 1-11. Item Detail Occupation of Respond- ent Current Employment Status Education Scale.lgrae ditional. Intensity Responses* Education Scale..g£g- _gressive. Content Responses** Page 2-2 Code Recode* Same as Card 1. pages 1-4 through 1-9 Same as Card 1. page 1-10 Q not strongly at all not very strongly fairly strongly very strongly ‘ othH I bcyrord ‘ § 1 - 1. strongly disagree 2 - 2. disagree 3 - 3. agree 4 4 strongly agree Q .EDHCAIIQNHSQALE..ZBADLILQNALo.lEI§Hf Intensity questions are scored as indica- 1-12 and 1-13 and as noted before. Handicapped Persons Scale. pages 1-10. 1-11 and 1-12. instruc- tions 1 through 5. ** Instructions for Coder: 1. 2. 865 EDUCATION SCALE. PROGRESSIVE. CONTENT. Items are to be scored exactly as circled. Follow the procedures outlined in caps on pages 1-11. l-12 and 1-13. Handicapped Persons Scale. Be sure to score only those items indicated above as belonging to the education progressive scale content. CARD 2 Page 2-3 Columnjgpes. Item Detail Code Recode* 45 1.2.5.7. Education 1 - 1. not strongly at all thru 8.9.15. Scale. Pro— 2 - 2. not very strongly 54 16.17.20 _gressive 3 - 3. fairly strongly Intensity 4 - 4. very strongly Responses* 55-56 Raw g Value scale. score Support 01 32 score** 57-58 Raw 9 Value scale. 01 32 score Conformity score** 59-60 Raw 3 Value scale. 01 32 score Recognition score** (comparative) 61-62 Raw I Value scale. 01 32 score Indepen- dence score** 63-64 Raw B. Value scale. 01 32 score Benevolence score**(asset) 65-66 Raw L Value scale. 01 32 score Leadership score** (comparative) * Instructions for Coder: COLUMNS 44-53. gressive content. ** EDUCATION SCALE. PROGRESSIVE. Same as instructions for Education Scale. see page 2-2. INTENSITY. Pro- Entries for columns 63-74 are obtained through scoring accord- ing to SRA Manual for Survey of Interpersonal Values. Science Research Associates. Inc., 259 East Erie Street. Chicago. Illi— nois. 1960. For scoring. coders should use the special keys adapted from the SRA English edition of the scale. Although the summed scores of the six value scales should total 90. scores Ibetween 84 and 95 are “acceptable." 865 Column-Ques. 67-68 Sum of item scores. 1-20. Content 69-70 Sum of item scores. 1-20. Intensity 71-72 Sum of item scores. 3. 4.6.10.11. 12.13.14. 18.19 73-74 Sum of item scores. 3. 4.6.10.11. 12.13.14. 18.19 Item Detail Code Adjusted (Check totals based here) on item :2_to dichotomiza- tion. H.P. §cale. Con- tent* Adjusted (Check totals based here) on item ‘12 to dichotomiza- tion. H.P. Scale. Inten- sity* Adjusted (Check totals based here) on item .12 to dichotomiza- tion Educa- tion Tradi- tional Scale. Content*“ Adjusted (Check totals based here) on item .19 to dichotomiza- tion Educa- tion Tradi- tional Scale. Intensity*' * See Card 1. page 1-12. adjusted total scores are obtained. 865 instruction no. Page 2-4 Recode* dich. for no. to use Code will be: 99 or obtained score dich. for no. to use Code will be: QQ_or obtained score dich. for no. to use Code will be: ,QQ or obtained score dich. for no. to use Code will be: .99 or obtained score 5-f. to ascertain how Column-Ques. 75-76 Sum of item scores. 1. 2.5.7.8.9. 15.16.17.20 77-78 Sum of item scores. 1. 2050708I9I 15.16.17.20 Item Detail Code Adjusted (Check totals based here) on item .32 to dichotomiza- tion Educa- tion Progres- sive Scale. M* Adjusted (Check totals based here) on item .12 to dichotomiza- tion Educa- tion Progres- sive Scale. Intensity* * See Card 1. page 1-12. instruction No. adjusted total scores are obtained. 865 Page 2-5 Recode* dich. for no. to use Code will be: IQQ_or obtained score dich. for no. to use Code will be: .99 or obtained score 5-f. to ascertain how u.._-—-- Column:gues. 1.2.3 Face Sheet 4.5 Face Sheet 6.7 Face Sheet 8 Face Sheet 9 37 Q'aire 10 New 11.12 Face Sheet 13.14 Face Sheet 15.16 Face Sheet 17.18 Face Sheet 19.20 Face Sheet 21 Face Sheet 865 Item Detail Nation and Location Group Number Respondent Number Sex of Respondent Occupational Recode (Interest group) Occupational Recode (Spec. Ed.- Rehab. SER) Deck or Card Number Project Director Day of Admin- istration Month of Adminis— tration Year of .Adminis- tration Type of Adminis- tration Code Same as 01-99 01-99 Same as Same as Same as 03 Same as and 1-3 01-31 01-12 Card Card Card Card Card Same as Card 1. Same as Card 1. Page 3—1 Recode* page 1-1 page 1-2 page 1-2 page 1-2 pages 1-2 page 1-4 page 1-4 CARD 3 Page 3-2 Column-gges. _ Item Detail Code Recode* 22.23 Face Sheet Occupation Same as Card 1. pages of Respond- 1-4 through 1-9 ent 24 Face Sheet Current Same as Card 1. page 1-10 employment status 25.26 1 Q'aire Contact Primary group 1 - 01. Elem. School (Educ.) 2 - 02. Sec. School 3 - 03. University 4 - 04. Other as specified 5 - 05. No experience 27.28 2 Q'aire Contact Secondary group 1 - 01 (Educ.) 2 - 02 3 - 03 SAME 4 - 04 5v- 05 29.30 3 Q'aire Educational 1 - 01 Know nothing about Ed Contact 2 - 02 Read little about Ed (Varieties) 3 - 03, Studied about Ed 4 - 04 Neighbor works 5 - 05 Friend works 6 - 06 Relative works 7 - 07 Family works 8 - 08 I work in Ed 9 - 09 Other (1) If any combination of alternatives 1. 2 and 3 are circled. code (2) (3) 865 as 10. Impersonal Contact If any combination of alternatives 4-8 are circled. code as 11. Personal Contact. If alternatives are circled in both division. code as 12. Both Impersonal and Personal Contact. This requires coding alterna- tive OTHER (i.e.. alternative 9) as either personal or imper- sonal contact; i.e.. according to its content. Column:gues. 31 4 Q'aire 32 5 Q'aire 33 6 Q'aire 34 7 Q'aire 35.36 8 Q'aire 37 9 Q'aire 865 .— CARD 3 Item Detail Amount of Contact (Educ.) Percent of income from Education Enjoyment of Educational Work Alternative work (to educ.) Age Community in which reared. If more than one is checked try to determine in which one the respond- ent spent most of the time. If 2221.2 mflmUl-fiWNl-J I .tht—I m-waH I I U'ltfiwNH I Q Q Q Q Q mflmUI-bWNl-d Q Q Q UlnbUJNH sssss U1I§OJN §~§~ Q Q Q UTIbUNH s Q Page 3-3 Recode* less than 3 months 3 months to 6 months 6 months to 1 year 1 year to 3 years 3 years to 5 years 5 years to 10 years over 10 years over 15 years less than 10% 10 to 25% 25 to 50% 50 to 75% 75 to 100% disliked not much somewhat enjoyed no information unavailable not acceptable not quite acceptable acceptable years years country country town city city suburb CARD 3 Page 3-4 Columnjgpes. Item Detail Code Recode* 37 9 Q'aire (continued) impossible. try to choose a median (i.e. country. city. score country town) 38 10 Q'aire Employment 1 - 1. country community 2 - 2. country town (recent) 3 - 3. city 4 - 4. city suburb 39 ll Q'aire Recent Resi— 1 — 1. country dence 2 - 2. country town 3 - 3. city 4 - 4. city suburb 40 12 Q'aire Marital 1 - 1. married Status 2 - 2. single 3 - 3. divorced 4 - 4. widowed 5 - 5. separated 41.42 13 Q'aire Number of l - 01 children. 2 - 02 If blank. 3 - 03 check Ques. ° ° 13. If 10 - 10 single. score 00; if married. score -9. 43.44 14 Q'aire Yearly Income UNITED STATES ‘ (self-family) 01 - less than $1.000 (for other 02 - $1.000 to $1.999 nations see 03 - $2.000 to $2.999 Special ° Instructions) 10 - $9.000 to $9.999 865 Column-Ques. 45 15 Q'aire 46.47 16 Q'aire 48.49 17 Q'aire 51.51 NOneT 52 18 Q'aire 53 19 Q'aire 865 Item Detail Comparative Income (self-fam- ily) U'lnPWNH H Brothers. If the respondent 3 answers - only one 10 question (17 or 18) and other is blank. assume it to be zero. N Sisters Siblings - 1 Obtain by ° summing 15 above Ques- tions 16 and 17. Col's 45. 46 and 47. 48 Fathers' Income: Comparative UlthNI-d Religious Affiliation O‘U‘lwal-J I UlvaJNl-J Q Q Same .9292 Q Q Q - 01 - 03 - 10 as - 01 - 15 Q Q Q I mbWNH Q Q Q Q I I U'thJNl—l Q I Page 3-5 Recode* much lower lower about the same higher much higher number of brothers much lower lower about the same higher much higher Roman Catholic Protestant Jewish None Other to 9. Other major religions CARD 3 Page 3-6 Column-Ques. Item Detail Code Recode*. 54 20 Q'aire Religion 1 - 1. No religion (Import- 2 - 2. Not very ance) 3 - 3. Fairly 4 - 4. Very 55 21 Q'aire Personaliam 1 - 1. none (job-amount) 2 - 2. no contact 3 - 3. less than 10% 4 - 4. 10 to 30% 5 - 5. 30 to 50% 6 - 6. 50 to 70% 7 - 7. 70 to 90% 8 - 8. over 90% 56 22 Q'aire Personalism 1 - 1. not at all (job-impor- 2 - 2. not very tance of) 3 - 3. fairly 4 - 4. very 57 23 Q'aire Personalism 1 - 1. none (job-diffu- 2 - 2. less than 10% sion) 3 - 3. 10 to 30% 4 - 4. 30 to 50% 5 - 5. 50 to 70% 6 - 6. 70 to 90% 7 - 7. over 90% 58 24 Q'aire Social Class 1 - 1. lower Position 2 - 2. lower middle (Self) 3 - 3. middle 4 - 4. upper middle 5 - 5. upper 59 25 Q'aire Social Class Same as above Position (Father) 865 Column-gues. 60 26 Q'aire 61 27 Q'aire 62 28 Q'aire 63 29 Q'aire 64 30 Q'aire 865 Item Detail Education (Self- amount). If more than one is circled. choose the highest amount or determine the approp- riate an answer. Education (Self-com- parative) Education (Father - comparative) Housing (type of) Housing (rental- month) (for other nations see Special Instructions) Code (DQOUIDUJNH I UTbUJNl-J mph-JR)!“ l I thNl-J i mm II 7.. Page 3-7 Recode* three years or less six years or less nine years or less twelve years or less some college degree work beyond degree advanced degree Q Q Q Q Q Q (DQOWUIDNNH Q Q much less less average more much more Q Q Q Ul-wai-J Q Q much less less average more much more Q Q Q U"l.bLa\Jl\.)l--"I Q Q rent house rent apartment rent room purchase room and board 5. own apartment 6. own house 7. other IP-le—i QQQ Q UNITED STATES $20 or less 21 - 40 (dollars) 41 - 75 76 - 125 126 - 200 201 - 300 300 or more Column-Ques. 65 3l-A Q'aire 66 31-B Q'aire 67 31-C Q'aire 68 31-D Q'aire 69 31-E Q'aire 70 31-F Q'aire 71 31-G Q'aire 72 3l-H Q'aire 73 31-I Q'aire 865 Item Detail Institutional Satisfaction Elementary Schools Institutional Satisfaction Secondary Schools Institutional Satisfaction Universities Institutional Satisfaction Businessmen Institutional Satisfaction Labor Institutional Satisfaction Government (local) Institutional Satisfaction Government (National) Institutional Satisfaction Health Services Institutional Satisfaction Churches £95.12 U'lfiUJNl-J I Same Same Same Same Same Same Same Same m-th-‘w Page 3-8 Recode* do not know poor fair good excellent Column-Ques. 74 75 865 32 Q'aire 33 Q'aire CARD 3 Item Detail Code Residency 1 - (current 2 - length) 3 - 4 _ 5 - Residency l - (change- 2 - recent) mprH QQQQQ H Q Page 3—9 Recode* less than a year one to two years three to six years seven to ten years over ten years yes no Column-Ques. 1.2.3 4.5 10 11.12 13.14 15.16 17.18 19.20 21 865 Face Face Face Face Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet 37 Q‘aire New Face Face Face Face Face Face Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet Sheet Item Detail Nation and Location Group Number Respondent Number Sex of Respondent Occupational Recode (Interest group) Occupational Recode (Spec. Ed.- Rehab. SER) Deck or Card Number Project Director Day of Adminis- tration Month of (Adminis- tration Year of Adminis- tration Type of Adminis- tration 9221.2 Same 01 - Same Same Same 04 Same 1-3 01-31 01-12 Same Same as Card 1. page 1-4 as Card 1. 99 99 Card 1. as as Card 1. as Card 1. as Card 1. and 1-3 as Card 1. Page 4-1 Recode* page 1-1 page 1-2 page 1-2 page 1-2 pages page 1-4 CARD 4 Page 4-2 Column:Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 22.23 Face Sheet Occupation Same as Card 1. pages of Respond- 1-4 through 1-9 ent 24 Face Sheet Current Same as Card 1. page 1-10 Employment Status 25 34 Q'aire Job change 1 - 1. yes (recent) 2 - 2. no 26 35 Q'aire Residency - . none (change fre- - . one time two to three times four to six times seven to ten times over ten times quency) (i. e.. last ten years) Q Q O‘U’ltPUJNH I O‘U‘IthH Q Q 27 36 Q'aire Job (change frequency) (i.e.. last ten years) none one time two to three times four to six times seven to ten times over ten times Q Q Q Q O‘U'ItPUJNI-J I mmbwwr—a Q Q 28.29 37 Q'aire Occupation Same as Card 1. pages (Specific) 1-4 through 1-9 30 38 Q'aire Religiousity 1 - 1. no religion (norm con- 2 - 2. seldom formity) 3 - 3. sometimes 4 — 4. usually 5 - 5. almost always 31 39 Q'aire Change Ori— 1 - 1. no entation 2 - 2. probably not (Health 3 - 3. maybe Practices) 4 - 4. yes 32 40 Q'aire Change Ori- l - 1. strongly disagree entation 2 - 2. slightly disagree (Child 3 - 3. slightly agree Rearing) 4 — 4. strongly agree 865 CARD 4 Page 4-3 Column-Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 33 41 Q'aire Change Ori- l - 1. always right entation 2 - 2. usually right (Birth con- 3 — 3. probably wrong trol Prac- 4 - 4. always wrong tices) 34 42 Q'aire Change Ori- l - 1. strongly disagree entation 2 - 2. slightly disagree (Automation) 3 - 3. slightly agree 4 - 4. strongly agree 35 43 Q'aire Change Ori— l - 1. strongly disagree entation 2 - 2. slightly disagree (Political 3 - 3. slightly agree Leaders) 4 - 4. strongly agree 36 44 Q'aire Education 1 - 1. strongly disagree (aid to - 2 - 2. slightly disagree local) 3 - 3. slightly agree 4 - 4. strongly agree 37 45 Q'aire Education 1 - 1. strongly disagree (aid to - 2 - 2. slightly disagree federal) 3 - 3. slightly agree 4 — 4. strongly agree 38 46 Q'aire Education 1 - 1. only parents (planning 2 - 2. only city or local responsi- government bility) 3 - 3. primarily federal government 39 47 Q'aire Change Ori- 1 - 1. very difficult entation 2 - 2. somewhat difficult (self) 3 — 3. slightly easy 4 - 4. very easy 40 48 Q'aire Change Ori— 1 - 1. agree strongly entation 2 - 2. agree slightly (self-role 3 - 3. disagree slightly adherence) 4 - 4. disagree strongly 865 QC Column-gues. 41 49 Q'aire 42 50 Q'aire 43 51 Q'aire 44 52 Q'aire 45 53 Q'aire 46.47 54 Q'aire 865 Item Detail Change Ori- entation (self- routine job) Personalism (Famialism- Parental ties) Personalism (Other ori- entation) Future Ori- entation (Planning) Future Ori- entation (Happiness) Future Ori- entation (Happiness possibility) Code tthH I Same waI-l bWNH I I OAU'I-D-WNH I 06 07 08 09 10 -9 htubOPJ bWNH kWNH s Q mU'IuPUNI-J Page 4-4 Recode* agree strongly agree slightly disagree slightly disagree strongly Q Q Q disagree strongly disagree slightly agree slightly agree strongly Q Q Q agree strongly agree slightly disagree slightly disagree strongly Q Q Q nothing money friends job health . other Q Q Q Q Q Nothing Marriage Divorce Friends Religion (Satisfaction with life) Money Job Education Health (Mental) Health (Physical) No response Column-Ques. Item Detail 9992 Page 4-5 Recode* HANDICAPPED PERSONS QUESTIONNAIRE 48 l-Q-HP HP Contact Group (Pri- mary) 49.50 2-Q-HP HP Contact Group (Sec- ondary) 51,52 3—Q-HP HP Contact (varieties) 53 4—Q-HP HP Contact (amount) * NOTE: QmflmU‘I-FWNH 00 to 08 \ImU'l-bWNI" LJ'l-PWNH If either or both alternatives 1 and 2 are circled. as gs - Impersonal contact. tives 3-7 are circled. code as 92 - Personal contact. If alternatives from both preceding divisions are circled. code as lQ.- Impersonal and Personal contact. 865 blind partially blind deaf (and mute) partially deaf crippled disfigured spastic speech none Q Q Q Q Q Q Q OWQO‘U‘I-fiwNAI—J Q If there was no contact and questions are not answered score 9, The score for this question is the score of the response alternatives circled. i.e.. scores can range from Q_to 8, 01. 02. 03. 04. 05. 06. O7. 08) 09)* See note below 10) Minimum knowledge Studied about HP Friend HP Relative HP Worked with HP Family HP Self is HP 1. less than ten 2. ten to fifty 3. fifty to 100 4. 100 to 500 5. over 500 code If either or all alterna- Columnegpes..v Item Detail 54 5-Q—HP HP Contact (ease of avoidance) 55 6-Q-HP HP Contact (gain from) 56 7—Q-HP HP Contact (% income) 57 8-Q-HP HP Contact (enjoyment) 58 9-Q-HP HP Contact (alterna- tives to) 59 lO-Q-HP Contact (amount- M.R.) 60 ll-Q-HP Contact (amount- EDP) 865 QUNI‘“ bWNI‘" QWNH punt” u s s s s Q s s s s Q Q U'IuthH UI-PoUJNH Q PLUM?“ thH QQ Q Q Q Q Q Q U'l-thINI-J U'IPUJNI—J Q Page 4-6 Recode* great difficulty considerable difficulty some inconvenience no inconvenience no rewards paid credit paid and credit less than 10% 10 to 25% 25 to 50% 50 to 75% over 75% disliked. great disliked. little liked. some definitely enjoyed No information on alternatives No other job available Other available job N91 acceptable Other available job acceptable less than 10 10 to 50 50 to 100 100 to 500 over 500 Column:Ques. 61.62 Sum of item scores 1-20 Content 63.64 Sum of item scores 1-20 Intensity 65.66 Sum of item scores 3. 4.6.10.11. 12.13.14. 18.19 67.68 Sum of item scores 3. 4.6.10.11. 12.13.14. 18.19 865 Item Detail Code Handicapped 00-80 Persons Scale Total Content Raw Score. entry on trans- cription sheet Handicapped 00-80 Persons Scale Total Intensity E! Score. entry on transcrip- tion sheet Education 00-40 Scale. Tra- ditional Total Raw Content score entry on transcrip- tion sheet Education 00-40 Scale..g£ag ditional Total Raw Intensity. score entry on transcrip- tion sheet Page 4—7 Recode* Column:gues. 69.70 Sum of 71.72 865 item scores 1. 2.5.7.8. 9.15.16. 17.20 Sum of item scores 1. 2.5.7.8. 9.15.16. 17.20 Item Detail Code Education 00-40 Scale..g£9- (gressive Total 3331 Content score entry on transcrip- tion sheet Education 00-40 Scale..g£g¢ gressive Total M. Intensity score entry on transcrip- tion sheet Page 4-8 Recode* I '1 39:2,!" 40 7 l Tiffin-Ohio (010) (SPECIAL INSTRUCTIONS) Card 5 Page S-S-O-l Column-Ques. 1—9 Face Sheet Item Detail Nation and Location Same as Card 1 Code Recode* 1 page 1-1 10 Face Sheet Special Education —1, mothers (SVGC) of Q'aire (Occupational EDP Recode) -2, mothers (BJRC) HP —3, mothers (Mt. Pleasant)Non-SER 11,12 Face Sheet Deck or Card Number 05 13-24 Face Sheet Nation and Location Same as Card 1, page 1-1 25 1 thru All questions in EDP l—l strongly agree thru 20 EDP Scale are to be 2-2 disagree 44 Content scored from raw data.3—3 agree Instructions parallelu-U strongly agree exactly those of the H-P scale. (Note especially reversals in items 2, 2, 6, II, and 12. US 1 thru Instructions for 1—1 not strongly thru 20 EDP scoring parallel at all 64 Intensity those of H-P scale. 2—2 not very See Card l-ll. strongly 3-3 fairly strongly u—u very strongly 65-66 Sum of Emotionally Distur- 00-80 item scores bed Persons Scale. 1-20, Total Content Raw content score. (EDP) 865 Card 5 Column-Ques. 67-68 Sum of item scores 1-20 Content (EDP) 69-70 Sum of ad- Justed item scores. Content We: 71-72 Sum of ad- Justed item scores, Intensity (m) 408 Tiffin-Ohio (010) (SPECIAL INSTRUCTIONS) Item Detail EDP Scale. Total Intensity raw score. Adjusted totals based on item dichtomoziation, EDP-Content (l) Adjusted totals based on item dichtomization EDP—Intensity_(l) Page S-5-0—2 Code Recode* 00—80 00- (Check Dich. for no. to use here). See pp 1-11 for instructions. (1) See Card 1, Page 1-11, instructions No. S-f, to ascertain how adjusted total scores are obtained. 865 ' *pM-wmfl A n 1:09 Tiffin-Ohio (010) (SPECIAL INSTRUCTIONS) Card 6 Page S-6—0—1 Column-Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 1-9 Face Sheet Nation and Location Same as Card 1, page 1—1 10 Face Sheet Special Education —1, mothers (Occupational (SVGC) EDP Recode) -2, mothers (BJRC) HP -3, mothers (Mt. Pleasant) Non-SER 11,12 Face Sheet Deck or Card Number 06 13-24 Face Sheet Nation and Location Same as Card 1 page 1—1 25 1 EDP Contact with EDP 1—1 yes Q'aire 2—2 no 26-27 2 EDP Varieties of l—Ol, Minimum Q'aire Contact with EDP knowledge 2—02, Studied about EDP 3—03, Friend-EDP 4—04, Relative—EDP 5—05, Worked with EDP 6-06, Family EDP 7—07, Self is EDP -08) —09) See note below -10) IVote: If either or both alternatives 1 and 2 are circled, code as 08 - Impersonal contact. If either or all alternatives 3-7 are circled, code as 09 - Personal contact. If alternatives from both preceding divisions are circles, code as 10 - Impersonal and Personal Contact. 8655 “L ‘ Iv: , 410 Tiffin-Ohio (010) (SPECIAL INSTRUCTIONS Card 6 Page S—6-0-2 Column-Ques. Item Detail Code Recode* 28 3 EDP Amount of Contact l-l, less than ten Q'aire with EDP 2—2, ten to fifty 3—3, fifty to 100 4-4, 100 to 500 5-5, over 530 29 A EDP Ease of avoidance l—l, great difficulty Q'aire of EDP 2—2, considerable difficulty 3—3, some inconven- ience A-A, no inconvenience 3O 5 EDP Material gain from l-l, paid Q'aire contact with EDP 2-2, credit 3-3, no rewards A—A, paid and credit 31 6 EDP Per cent of income l-l, less than 10% Q'aire 2-2, 10 to 25% 3-33 25 to 50% A—A, 50 to 75% 5-5, over 75% -6, if i is circled in No. 6 or if they have never worked with EDP 32 7 EDP Feeling about con— l—l, disliked, great Q'aire taCt with EDP 2-2, disliked a little 3-3, liked, some A—A, definitely enjoyed 33 8 EDP Alternatives to l-l, no information on Q'aire contact with EDP alternatives (i.e. employment) 2-2, no other job available 3-3, available-not acceptable A-A, available-not quite acceptable 5—5, available- acceptable L :qu Card 6 Column-Ques. 3A 9 EDP Q'aire 35 10 EDP Q'aire (I) Ch U7 All Tiffin—Ohio (010) (SPECIAL INSTRUCTIONS) Item Detail Amount of Contact with physically handicapped Amount of Contact with mentally retarded WtWNI—J U’ILTWRJI—J I I I I mummy—v U'IJZ‘WRJI—J IIII Page S—6—0-3 as: UUUUU UUUUU less than 10 10 to 50 50 to 100 100 to 500 over 500 less than 10 10 to 50 50 to 100 100 to 500 over 500 Recode* APPENDIX B-5 FCCI and II Variable-Computer Print-Out Code Forms 412 :L~ A13 Tiffin 010 FCC I and 2 Variable-computer printout code form John E. Jordan College of Education Michigan State University Wu—umugu p . . - . .. v . . . . s :" . - I. AlA Tiffin 1 of 5 FCC I Field No. Question Variable Name Col. Card I 1 Face Sheet Nation and Location 3 2 Face Sheet Sex 8 3 37 Q'aire Interest Group 9 A None Occup. recoder (mothers) 10 5 Face Sheet Admin. type 21 6 Face Sheet Employment status 2A 7-26 HP Scale HP Content 25—AA 27-A6 HP Scale HP Intensity A6—6A A7-56 Ed Scale Ed. Trad Content 65—7A Card 2 First 2A Columns SAME as Card 1 except for Col. 11,12 (i.e. Deck or Card No.) 57-66 Education Scale Trad. Education—Intensity 25-3A 67-66 Education Scale Prog. Education-Content 35—AA 77-86 Education Scale Prog. Education~lntensity A5-5A Card 3 First 2A Columns SAME as Card 1 except for Col. 11,12 (i.e. Deck or Card No.) 87 A Q'aire Contact (amount-education) 31 88 5 Q'aire Contact (gain from education) 32 89 6 Q'aire Contact (enjoyment—education) 33 90 7 Q'aire Contact (alternatives to ed) 3A 91 9 Q'aire Early Youth Community 37 92 10 Q'aire Employment Community (recent) 38 93 11 Q'aire Residence Community (recent) 3 9A 12 Q'aire Marital Status A0 95 15 Q'aire Income (comparative—self fam.)A5 96 18 Q'aire Income (father's comparative) 52 97 19 Q'aire Religious affiliation 53 98 20 Q'aire Religion (Importance) 5A 99 21 Q'air Personalism (job—amount) 55 100 22 Q'aire Personalism (job-importance) 56 101 23 Q'aire Personalism (job-diffusion) 57 102 2A Q'aire Social class position (self) 58 103 25 Q'aire Social class position(father) 59 10A 26 Q'aire Education (self-amount) 60 105 27 Q'aire Education (self-comparative) 61 106 28 Q'aire Education (father-comparative)62 107 29 Q'aire Housing (type of) 63 108 30 Q'aire Housing (rental-mouth) 6A 109 31A Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 65 (elementary schools) 110 318 Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 66 (secondary schools) 111 31C Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 67 (universities) A15 Tiffin 2 of 5 Field No. Question Variable Name Col. 112 31D Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 68 (businessmen) 113 31E Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 69 (labor) 11A 31F Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 70 (local government) 115 310 Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 71 (national government) 116 31H Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 72 (health service) 117 311 Q'aire Institutional Satisfaction 73 (churches) 118 32 Q'aire Residing (current length) 7A 119 33 Q'aire Residing (change-recent) 75 Card A irst 2A columns SAME except for Columns 11-12 (i.e. deck or Card No.) 120 3A Q'aire Job (change-recent) 25 121 35 Q'aire Residing (change-frequency) 26 122 36 Q'aire Job (change—frequency) 27 123 38 Q'aire Religiousity (norm-conformity)30 12A 39 Q'aire Change orientation (health pr)31 125 A0 Q'aire Change orientation(child rear)32 126 A1 Q'aire Change orientation(birth cont)33 127 A2 Q'aire Change orientation(automation)3A 128 A3 Q'aire Change orientation 35 (political leaders) 129 AA Q'aire Education (aid to-local) 36 130 A5 Q'aire Education (aid to—federal) 37 131 A6 Q'aire Education (planning respon— sibility) 38 132 A7 Q'aire Change orientation (self) 39 133 A8 Q'aire Change orientation (self- rule adherence) A0 13A A9 Q'aire Change orientation (self- routine job) A1 135 50 Q'aire Personalism (familialism- parental ties) A2 135 51 Q'aire Personalism(other orientation)A3 137 52 Q'aire Future orientation (planning) AA 138 52 Q'aire Future orientation (happiness prerequisites) A5 139 l-Q-HP Contact group (primary HP) A8 1A0 A-Q-HP Contact (amount of HP) 53 1A1 5-Q-HP Contact (ease of avoidance) 5A 1A2 6-Q-HP Contact (gain from—HP) 55 1A3 7—Q-HP Contact (% income from HP) 56 1AA 8-Q-HP Contact (enjoyment-HP) 57 1165 A16 Tiffin 3 of 5 Field No. Question Variable Name Col. 1A5 9-Q-HP Contact (alternative to HP) 58 1A6 lO-Q-HP Contact (amount - MR) 59 1A7 ll-Q-HP Contact (amount-emotional i11)60 Card 5 First 2A columns SAME except for Col. 11,12 (i.e. Deck or Card No.) 1A8—l67 EDP Scale EDP total content 25-AA 168-187 EDP Scale EDP total intensity A5-6A Card 6 188 1-EDP Q'aire Contact with EDP 25 189 3-EDP Q'aire Contact (EDP, amount) 28 190 A-EDP Q'aire Contact (EDP, avoidance) 29 191 5-EDP Q'aire Contact (EDP, gain) 30 192 6-EDP Q'aire Contact (EDP, % income) 31 193 7-EDP Q'aire Contact (EDP, enjoyment) 32 19A 8-EDP Q'aire Contact (EDP, alternatives) 33 195 9-EDP Q'aire Contact (HP, amount) 3A 196 lO-EDP Q'aire Contact (MR, amount) 35 1165 M17 Tiffin A of 5 FCC 11 Field No. Question Variable Name Col. Card 1 1 Face Sheet Group Number A,5 2 37 Q'aire Specific Occupation 22,23 semis First 2A Columns SAME as Card 1 except for Col. 11,12 (i.e. CDN ONW zoo First 2A (i.e. 9 10 11 12 13 1A 15 l6 17 Value Value Value Value Value Value Columns SAME as Deck or Card No.) Deck or Card No. Scale Scale Scale Scale Scale Scale 1 Q'aire 2 Q'aire 3 Q'aire 8 Q'aire 13 Q'aire 1A Q'aire 16 Q'aire 17 Q'aire None ) Support Value 55,56 Conformity Value 57,58 Recognition Value(comparative)59,60 Independent Value 61,62 Benevolence Value 63,6A Leadership Value (comparative)65,66 Card 3 Card 1 except for Col. 11,12 Ed. Contact group (primary) 25,26 Ed. Contact group (secondary) 27,28 Ed. Contact (varieties) 29,30 Age 35.36 Number of children A1,A2 Income(yearly,self,family) A3.AA Brothers(do not use in FAN etc)A6,A7 Sisters(do not use in FAN,etc)A8,A9 First 2A Columns SAME as Card 1 except for Col. 11, 12 (i.e. 18 19 20 21 22 23 2A 25 26 27 1165 Deck or Card No.) 37 Q'aire 5A Q'aire 2-Q-HP 3-Q-HP HP HP ED ED ED ED Scale Scale Scale Scale Scale Scale I ' a ' T “a“; Siblings 50,51 Card A Occupation (specific) 28.29 Future orientation (happiness possibility) A6,A7 HP Contact group (secondary) A9,50 HP Contact (varieties) 51,52 HP — Total content 61,62 HP - Total intensity 63,6A Ed. Trad. Content 65,66 Ed. Trad. Intensity 67,68 Ed. Prog. Content 69,70 Ed. Prog. Intensity 71,72 A18 Tiffin 5 of 5 Field No. Question Variable Name Col. Card 5 First 2A Columns SAME as Card 1 except for 001. 11,12 (i.e. Deck or Card No.) 28 EDP Scale EDP - Total Content 65,66 29 EDP Scale EDP - Total Intensity 67,68 Card 6 First 2A Columns SAME as Card 1 except for Col. 11,12 (i.e. Deck or Card No.) 30 Contact - EDP (Varieties) 26,27 1165 a: ’L .39 .. 7." .; .‘~:_’ J . ~er ‘nmw APPENDIX B-6 Data Transcription Sheet A19 I Attitudes Toward Education: International Study . o v v o ......... 865 Handicapped Persons Edugation Scale - ~ Education Scale - Scale (Card 1) raditional Progregsive = Card 1 Card 2 Card 1 Card 2 COntent Intensity Content Intensity Content Intensity (C01) (C01) (Col) (C01) (C01) (Col) 1” ____(25)_____j45) 3. (65)_____125) 1. (35)_____j45) 2. _____(26) ______(46) An (66) ______(26) 2. (36) ______(46) 3. _____ ______ 6. (67) _____(27) 5. (37) _____(47) 4. __ __ 10. __(68) ____(28) 7. __(38) __(48) 5. __ __ 11. ___(69)I ____(29) 8. __(39) ___(49) 6. __ __ 12.____(70) ___(30) 9. __(40) ___(50) 7. _____ ______ 13.____f71)‘_____(3l)]5._____(41)._____(51) 8. __ __ 14.___(72) ___(32) 16. _(42) _____(52) 9._____ ______ 18._____(73)______(33) ll ____(43) _____(53) "10. ___(-34)I ____(54) 19.____(74) ____(34) 20. (44) ____(54) ll._____ ______ 12._____ ______ _____ ______ _____ ______ 13._____ ______ 14. ._____ 15.___(39) ____(59) 16._____ ______ 172____ ______ Location 18°——-—- -———- Group W_ A “; fl“ A l9._____ ______ 20. (44) (64) Respondent No. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES INIWINHII(I”I“I)"I)”I)(I)NIHIIWIIWIWHI 31293500260058 .A