THE INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL NORMS ON KOREAN WIVES’ INTENTION TO TALK WITH THEIR HUSBANDS ABOUT THE EQUAL DIVISION OF HOUSEWORK By Sun Young Park A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Communication – Master of Arts 2017 ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL NORMS ON KOREAN WIVES’ INTENTION TO TALK WITH THEIR HUSBANDS ABOUT THE EQUAL DIVISION OF HOUSEWORK By Sun Young Park The perception in South Korea that housework is the primary duty of a wife, even when she is equally engaged in social activities as her husband, creates a conflict for female employees’ work-life balance. Nowadays, Korean males see the equal division of housework more positively than in the past. However, a large gap still exists between the attitude and the actual behavior of the equal amount of housework. The present paper assumes that interpersonal communication can be the channel to reduce this gap. The theory of planned behavior and social norms approach are the theoretical frameworks that guide the study. The purpose of the study is to examine how social norms are influencing Korean wives’ intention to talk to their husbands about the equal division of housework. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES iv INTRODUCTION 1 LITERATURE REVIEW Korean Women’s Domestic Duties Social Norms and Korean Wives’ Intention to Initiate Communication The Theory of Planned Behavior Social Norms Approach Hypotheses and Research Question 3 3 5 7 8 10 METHOD Participants and Procedure Measures 12 12 13 RESULTS Hypothesis 1 Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 3 Hypothesis 4 Research Question 15 15 15 15 17 18 DISCUSSION Implications Limitations Conclusion 19 21 22 23 APPENDIX 25 REFERENCES 28 iii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Demographic Information 12 Table 2. Descriptive Analysis 14 Table 3. Pearson Correlations Matrix 14 Table 4. Two-Step Regression Analysis for Moderation of Norms 17 Table 5. The Average Minutes of Housework Per a Day by Employment Status of Couples 19 iv INTRODUCTION The theory of planned behavior (TPB) and the social norms approach (SNA) have been useful tools to predict an individual’s behavioral intention and behavioral changes. In particular, subjective norms from TPB and descriptive and injunctive norms from SNA have been adapted to several studies to explain the mechanism of an individual’s behavior. While some researchers did not differentiate subjective norms and injunctive norms (Lapinski & Rimal, 2005), other research found that each norm is a distinctive concept and has a differential impact on behavioral intention (Park & Smith, 2007; Park, Klein, Smith, & Martell, 2009). The present study aims to test the previous findings of the TPB and SNA in a different and new context, the division of housework in South Korea (Korea). Korean males spend the least time in doing chores among countries from the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD, 2014). The prevalent perception in Korea that housework is a duty of female family members, even when they are engaged in the same work and social activities as men, causes conflict to female in their work-life balance. Although more Koreans have a more positive attitude toward the equal division of housework than in the past, very few people actually put this into action (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014a). Interpersonal communication between husbands and wives is one channel that could be used to resolve this issue. The purpose of the study is to examine how different social norms are influencing on Korean wives’ intention to talk to their husbands about the equal division of housework. The present paper is largely based on Park and Smith’s research (2007) which tested the separate influence of subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms in organ donation. This paper begins with background information about gendered division of household labor in Korea and how social norms can impact Korean 1 wives’ intention to initiate conversation with their husbands about the equal division of household labor. Two theoretical frameworks, the TPB and SNA, guide hypotheses and research questions. 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Korean Women’s Domestic Duties Korean women have the lowest glass ceiling in jobs among OECD countries. This indicates that women have the lowest employment rate, and fewer women are senior managers or board members. The gender wage gap is largest in Korea compared to other OECD countries (The Economist, 2016). This might be explained by the ‘gendered’ market design in Korea. The organizational culture in Korea is known for working late and heavy drinking, which forms informal relationships between peers, supervisors, and business associates (Kim, 2009; Lee, Park, Lee, Kim, & Kim, 2006). The design of this market assumes that employees have wives or other female family members who can relieve them from domestic duties, such as housework or childcare, so that they do not necessarily have to go home early. Thus, female employees are often regarded as incapable of coping with domestic duties and careers (Kim, 2009). While male employees engage in informal bonding with each other late at night, female employees are often considered as out-group members and have fewer chances to get promoted. Responsibility of household labor is not only a burden for working women. The unequal amount of housework is deleterious to women regardless of their employment status. Whereas the division of household labor is usually not related to married men’s personal well-being or marital satisfaction, the unequal amount of household labor could reduce marital satisfaction and increase depression for married women (Cotrane, 2000). There is no relationship between wives’ employment status and the degree of Korean husbands’ contribution to housework. Recent national census data (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014b) revealed that wives engaged in significantly more housework when a husband and a wife were equally engaged in economic activities, when only a husband was employed but wife was not, and even when only wife was 3 working but a husband stayed at a domestic sphere. For example, Korean husbands in dualearner couples spent only five less minutes per a day doing housework than working husbands whose wives are not employed (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014b). The reason why women take care of the majority of domestic responsibility regardless of their employment status is due to Confucian values that are deeply rooted in Korean culture. Confucianism originates from the teachings of the Chinese philosopher Confucius and has affected to Korean culture to a great extent since the last kingdom of Korea (Seo, Leather, & Coyne, 2012). Confucianism emphasizes traditional roles of a dominating husband and a wellcomplying wife to maintain balance and harmony in society (Choi, 1994; Croucher & Miles, 2009; Han, 2012). While Confucian manhood is characterized with active and honorable images such as moral leadership, social strictness, personal dignity, and family responsibility, Confucian womanhood is represented as self-sacrificing women who accept husband-father dictation, refrain from debate, do not shame their families with carelessness, and whose work is to serve in roles such as as daughter-in-law, wife, and mother (Han & Ling, 1998). Thus, the Korean culture, based on Confucianism, has formulated the perception that housework is a first priority for Korean women and the least priority for Korean men even when married couples are equally employed. The problem is that the cultural and social pressure on Korean wives to do a vast majority of the housework deters women’s entry into the labor force and equal treatment of male and female employees in the work place, as shown by the fact that South Korea has the lowest glass ceiling in jobs among OECD countries (The Economist, 2016). Furthermore, this gendered perception that housework is a duty for women solidifies patriarchy in Korean family in which a husband is a primary decision maker while a wife’s major role is limited to providing emotional 4 nurturance to a husband and children (Han, 2012). Consistent with this argument, recent research suggests that gender equality has to be established first to achieve the ideal division of housework in Korea since the perception of non-traditional gender roles is positively associated with the equal division of housework in dual income couples (Jung & Cho, 2015). Some may expect that these traditional values might be changed by now due to the country’s urbanization and a shift in generations. Although this might be true in terms of attitudes toward the equal division of household labor, a gap still exists between the attitude and the actual behaviors. According to a recent poll conducted by the Korean National Statistical Office, almost half of the male population endorse equal division of housework. However, only 16% of them actually report doing so (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014a). This paper assumes that interpersonal communication between a husband and a wife is one channel to reduce the gap between the attitude and the actual equal division of housework. As the data shows, the positive attitude of men toward doing more chores is not sufficient to change the traditional gender roles. A husband and a wife need to realize that both of them want equality, articulate their commitment to each other, and make an active effort together to achieve the equal division of housework (Askari, Liss, Erchull, Staebell, & Axelson, 2010). As the very first step of communication between a couple, a Korean wife’s intention to talk with her husband about the equal division of household labor is important. Social Norms and Korean Wives’ Intention to Initiate Communication Not every wife may negatively evaluate their contribution to chores even though they do much more housework than their husbands (Askari, Liss, Erchull, Staebell, & Axelson, 2010; Lennon & Rosenfield, 1994; Perales, Baxter, & Tai, 2015). Some researchers argue that women’s desire for the equality in housework is usually thwarted by their lower social status and 5 the threat of divorce, which will yield a lower standard of living. Thus, women are induced to define an objectively unequal situation as just (Hochschild, 1989; Lennon & Rosenfield, 1994). Although not every married woman perceives the needs to talk to husband about sharing housework, the role of social norms is expected to play a significant role in predicting Korean women’s intention to talk with their husbands about the equal division of household labor. This is because people tend to make decisions based on their perceptions of what others think or do (Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990). Perceptions of normative behaviors are associated with an individual’s behavior and yield intervention effects (Martens et al., 2006). Intervention effects of social norms have been tested in various contexts. For example, it has been shown that social norms are conducive to reduce college student’s heavy drinking by correcting prevalent misperceptions (Perkins, Haines, & Rice, 2005), and peer norms are significantly related to students’ drug use and sexual behavior (Martens et al, 2006). As shown in several studies, people consider what others in similar social positions would do or think in given situations when making decisions about their own behaviors (Kilmartin et al., 2008). In other words, people are influenced by their perception of other people’s thought and actions. Although most past research was conducted in the context of health-risk behaviors, it is conceivable to expect that perceived social norms will impact Korean wives’ decisions to initiate conversation with their husband about the equal division of housework. If married women who do not perceive a given amount of housework unfair find out that a majority of other similar women discuss how to achieve an equal division of housework with their husband, they will become aware that they are not engaged in normative behaviors, and they might intend to have a conversation with husbands as others do. Conversely, although some wives think their current division of housework is unfair, they might not be likely to initiate the conversation with their husbands when they 6 perceive others who are important to them would not think it is appropriate to negotiate the division of housework. Also, due to the collectivistic culture in Korea where others’ opinions are particularly influential to an individual’s decision, Korean wives are expected to be more sensitive to others’ opinions compared to women from other nations with individualistic culture (Hofstede, 1993; Seo, Leather, & Coyne, 2012). Thus, normative information about others’ communicational behavior pertaining to the equal division of housework would lead Korean wives to intend to initiate conversation with their husbands, even when they are not aware of the need to re-divide the division of household labor. Park and Smith’s (2007) research examined different types of social norms and found that each norm brings a separate impact to a behavioral intention to varying degrees. To investigate how these findings can be applied to Korean housewives’ behavioral intentions, the theory of planned behavior (TPB) and social norms approach (SNA) provide theoretical frameworks to guide the present study. The Theory of Planned Behavior The theory of planned behavior (TPB) is largely based on the theory of reasoned action (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). The TPB explains what leads to people’s behaviors using four constructs; attitude toward the behavior, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and behavioral intention (Ajzen, 1991). Behavioral intention is strongly associated with the actual behavior and is predicted by other three constructs. Attitudes toward a specific behavior reflect an individual’s evaluation of the behavior. Subjective norms indicate the perception of what other people who are important to an individual think he or she should do in a given situation. Perceived behavioral control (PBC) refers to an individual’s perceived difficulty of performing a specific behavior (Ajzen, 1991). The TPB has provided a theoretical framework to understand behavioral intention and behaviors of people from non-Western cultures (Park & 7 Lee, 2009). The difference between the TRA and the TPB is the inclusion of PBC. PBC is an important construct in the present study. Some Korean wives may feel a lack of self-efficacy to talk to their husbands about housework because of cultural values, or they might have a lack of skill and knowledge of how to start the conversation about the division of labor to their husbands. For this reason, PBC is included in the present study in order for the TPB to be used instead of the TRA. However, TPB only accounts for one type of social norm. Another theoretical framework is needed to uncover how other social norms influence Korean housewives’ intention to discuss the amount of household labor with their husbands. Social Norms Approach The social norms approach (SNA) was first proposed by Perkins and Berkowitz (1986) to analyze students’ alcohol consumption pattern. According to the SNA, relevant norms can be categorized into two types: descriptive norms and injunctive norms (Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990). Descriptive norms refer to the perception of prevalence of the act among people who are important to individuals. Injunctive norms refer to an individual’s belief regarding the approval of the act by people who are important to the individual (Park & Smith, 2007; Yun & Silk, 2011). People in different cultures might react differently to norms. It is possible that Korean cultural orientation strengthens or weakens the impact of each norm (Paek, Lee, & Hove, 2014). Injunctive norms are expected to be a strong predictor of Korean wives’ behavioral intention. Previous research found that injunctive norms may lead people to think that they are being pushed in a certain direction by the norms (Jacobson, Mortensen, & Cialdini, 2011; Melnyk, Van Herpen, Fischer, & Van Trijp, 2011; De Wit, De Ridder, Stok, & De Vet, 2014). People might perceive the injunctive norm approval as an attempt to limit their freedom of thinking and acting 8 (Silvia, 2006; De Wit, De Ridder, Stok, & De Vet, 2014). Due to this reason, some might expect that injunctive norms can have a negative impact on wives’ behavioral intention to talk with their husbands. This is plausible when one lives in an individualistic culture where an individual’s autonomy is a key value (Hofstede, 1991; Kagitcibasi, 2005). However, cultural orientation in Korea may strengthen the effect of perceived injunctive norms. Korean culture is characterized as collectivism where interdependence and social harmony are highly appreciated (Hofstede, 2001; Paek, Lee, & Hove, 2014). Others’ approval is perceived more significant in collectivistic culture than in individualistic culture. Collectivistic orientation emphasizes harmonization within a society, which promotes social acceptance and social approval (Tafarodi & Walters, 1999). Injunctive norms have normative pressures (Yun & Silk, 2011) and are likely to be associated with a collectivistic orientation. Therefore, it is predicted that injunctive norms positively impact a Korean wife’s intention to talk about the division of labor more than other norms. 9 Hypotheses and Research Question Previous studies found distinctive contributions of subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms in the contexts of organ donation and college students’ drinking habits (Park & Smith, 2007; Park, Klein, Smith, & Martell, 2009). The current study proposes that these different types of norms exist in the context of Korean wives’ intention to talk with their husbands about the equal division of labor: a reference group’s expectation of a wife (subjective norms) can be differentiated from perceptions of a group’s approval of the act (injunctive norms) and prevalence of the act among members in a reference group (descriptive norms) (Park & Smith, 2007). Reference groups are crucial to deliver social norms to individuals because people are affected by others’ behaviors who are in a same social position when making a behavioral decision (Kilmartin et al, 2008). For married women female friends who are also married can be a reference group to decide whether to talk with their husbands about the equal division of labor or not. It is expected that although some of norms may be directly relevant to Korean wives’ behavioral intention, others may also moderate the effect of attitudes and PBC on their behavioral intentions. Also, findings here will answer the question of whether the amount of time husbands spent in doing housework is influenced by wives’ employment status to a great extent. Even when only a wife is employed and a husband is unemployed, a wife still spends one more hour on domestic chores than a husband (The National Korean Statistical Office, 2014b). It is questionable whether married women’s employment status is influential to their behavioral intentions or not. Thus, the following hypotheses and research questions are posed: H1: Subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms will represent three separate dimensions of norms. 10 H2: TPB components and norms will be directly related to behavioral intentions. H3: Norms will moderate the effects of the TPB components of attitude and PBC on behavioral intentions. H4: Injunctive norms will be the stronger predictor of Korean women’s behavioral intention than subjective norms and descriptive norms. RQ1: Will employment status of couples affect Korean wives’ behavioral intentions to talk with their husbands about the equal division of housework? 11 METHOD Participants and Procedure Two hundred and sixteen Korean wives were recruited to participate in the study. They were selected from a nationally representative panel of survey respondents administered by Focus Company Co., LTD., Seoul, Korea. Nine participants reported more than twelve hours on average time of daily housework. They were dropped because of an excessive number of hours. As a result, two hundred and nine wives’ responses are reported with a mean age of 34.33 years (SD=3.26). These participants’ employment status proportionally matched with the recent census in Korea (dual income couples = 42.9% (N=91), only wife employed = 6% (N=12), only husband employed = 50.1% (N=106)) (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014b). Participants filled out questionnaire via online surveys and received approximately one US dollar after the survey. Summary of the demographic information of participants is reported in Table 1. Table 1. Demographic Information N Valid Missing Mean Median Mode Std. Deviation Range Minimum Maximum N Mean Median Valid Missing Age (Wife) 209 0 34.33 35.00 35.00 3.26 14.00 26.00 40.00 Number Age of (Husband) Children 209 209 0 0 36.83 1.26 37.00 1.00 35.00 1.00 4.24 .97 25.00 7.00 25.00 .00 50.00 7.00 Education Education (Wife) (Husband) 209 209 0 0 - 12 Marriage Duration (years) 209 0 6.28 6.00 1.00 3.93 19.00 1.00 20.00 Place 209 0 - Wife's Housework (minutes/ day) 209 0 197.29 171.43 77.14 147.46 634.29 .00 634.29 Couple’s Working Status 209 0 - Husband's Housework (minutes/ day) 209 0 63.30 38.57 .00 94.66 634.29 .00 634.29 Perceived Fairness of Current Division 209 0 4.17 4.00 Table 1 (cont’d). Mode 5 5 1 2 4 Std. Deviation 1.72 Range 3 3 1 2 6 Minimum 3 3 1 1 1 Maximum 6 6 2 3 7 *Education: 3=high school graduate, 4=community college diploma, 5=bachelor degree, 6=graduate school degree *Place: 1=urban, 2=rural area *Couple’s working status: 1=dual income couple, 2=only husband is working, 3=only wife is working *perceived fairness of current division: 1=not fair to me at all, 7=very fair to me Measures As recommended by Ajzen, who first proposed the TPB (Ajzen, 1991), seven-point Likert scales were used to measure TPB components (subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, attitude toward the behavior, behavioral intention), as well as attitude toward the equal division of housework, descriptive norms and injunctive norms. Questionnaire items were adapted from Park and Smith’s study (2007) and edited accordingly to this context (Appendix B). The original English questionnaires were translated into Korean first by the author who is fluent in both English and Korean. Then, another person, who is also fluent in both languages and not involved with the current study carefully compared two questionnaires to make sure that there was no gap between the Korean version and the original English version. Responses within each subscale were analyzed and resulted in measures of attitude toward the equal division of housework (⍺=0.86, M=5.49, SD=1.41), attitude toward the behavior of speaking to husband about the equal division of housework (⍺=0.95, M=5.43, SD=1.39), behavioral intention (⍺=0.92, M=4.99, SD=1.25), PBC (⍺=0.91, M=5.12, SD=1.27), subjective norms (⍺=0.86, M=5.05, SD=1.15), descriptive norms (⍺=0.88, M=5.09, SD=1.13), and injunctive norms (⍺=0.89, M=5.41, SD=1.04). Descriptive analysis and Zero-order correlations are reported in Table 2 and Table 3 respectively. 13 Table 2. Descriptive Analysis Mean Median Mode Std. Deviation Range Minimum AE 209 0 5.49 5.50 7.00 1.41 6.00 1.00 AB 209 0 5.43 5.75 7.00 1.39 6.00 1.00 BI 209 0 4.99 5.00 5.00 1.25 6.00 1.00 PBC 209 0 5.12 5.25 5.50 1.27 6.00 1.00 SN 209 0 5.05 5.00 5.00 1.15 6.00 1.00 DN 209 0 5.09 5.00 4.67 1.13 6.00 1.00 IN 209 0 5.41 5.33 5.00 1.04 6.00 1.00 Perceived DN (%) 209 0 55.11 50.00 50.00 26.81 98.00 2.00 Maximum 7.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 7.00 100.00 N Valid Missing Perceived IN (%) 209 0 74.01 80.00 100.00 25.86 100.00 .00 100.00 Note. AE=Attitude toward Equal division of housework, AB= Attitude toward behavior of speaking to husband about equal division of housework, BI=Behavioral intention, PBC=Perceived Behavioral Control, SN=Subjective norms, DN=Descriptive norms Table 3. Pearson Correlations Matrix AE AB PBC SN DN IN AE 1 AB .64** 1 PBC .26** .50** 1 SN .20** .34** .61** 1 DN .17* .25** .52** .77** 1 IN .34** .46** .57** .75** .72** 1 BI .25** .47** .61** .67** .58** .67** BI 1 **. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). Note. AE=Attitude toward Equal division of housework, AB= Attitude toward behavior of speaking to husband about equal division of housework, BI=Behavioral intention, PBC=Perceived Behavioral Control, SN=Subjective norms, DN=Descriptive norms 14 RESULTS Hypothesis 1 Hypothesis 1 predicted that subjective norm, descriptive norm, and injunctive norm represent separate dimensions of norms. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted via MPLUS. In addition to three different norms, measurements of attitude toward the equal division of housework, attitude toward the behavior of speaking to husbands about the equal division of housework, and PBC were included. The seven-factor model was a much better fit (Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = .92, Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR)=.06, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) = .09), than a one-factor model (CFI = .54, SRMR= .15, RMSEA = .20). Therefore, hypothesis 1 was supported. Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 2 predicted that the TPB components, descriptive, and subjective norms would be directly related to behavioral intentions. Regression analysis revealed that TPB components of subjective norms, attitude toward the behavior, and PBC, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms significantly predicted behavioral intention, F(5, 203)=53.48, p < .01. 𝑅" for the analysis was .57 and the adjusted 𝑅" was .56. Attitude toward behavior (B=.15, p<.05), PBC (B=.20, p<.01), subjective norms (B=.28, p<.01), and injunctive norms (B=.23, p<.01) significantly predicted wives’ behavioral intention of talking to husband about the equal division of housework. However, descriptive norms were not a significant predictor of behavioral intention (B=.06, p>.05). Thus, hypothesis 2 was partially supported. Hypothesis 3 Hypothesis 3 predicted that norms will moderate the effects of the TPB components of attitude and PBC on behavioral intentions. To reveal how the norms impact the relationship 15 between the TPB components and wives’ behavioral intentions of speaking to their husbands about the equal division of housework, regression analysis was conducted. Using a two-step regression analysis, the TPB components of attitude toward the behavior of speaking to the husbands and PBC were included in the first block. Subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms were entered to the second block in order to examine the variance in the behavioral intentions explained by different types of norms. When the TPB components of attitude toward behavior and PBC were entered to the first block of the regression analysis, attitudes toward behavior (B = .21, p<.01) and PBC (B = .50, p<.01) were statistically significant. When the three different types of norms were entered to the second block, they together explained an additional 16% of the variance in behavioral intention. Although descriptive norms were not a statistically significant predictor of behavioral intention (B = .06, p>.05), subjective norms (B = .28, p<.01) and injunctive norms (B = .23, p<.01) were significantly related to behavioral intentions to talk with husbands about the equal division of housework. As shown in Table 4, when three types of norms were entered to the second block, the regression coefficients of attitudes toward behavior and PBC were readjusted, taking into account the three norm variables entered to the second block. Both attitudes toward behavior and PBC remained significant even after including the three norms. Although descriptive norms alone are not a significant predictor of behavioral intentions, the three norms together moderated the relationship between TPB components of attitudes and PBC, and behavioral intentions. Thus, hypothesis 3 was supported. 16 Table 4. Two-Step Regression Analysis for Moderation of Norms Model 1 2 Unstandardized Coefficients Std. B Error 5.67 1.25 .19 .06 .49 .06 (Constant) AB PBC (Constant) AB PBC SN DN IN F(2,206)=70.84 -.50 .14 .20 .40 .09 .37 Standardized Coefficients Beta t .21 .50 Sig. 4.53 3.46 8.09 .00 .00 .00 -.38 2.69 3.13 3.31 .79 2.92 .70 .01 .00 .00 .43 .00 Adjusted 𝑅 " =.40 1.32 .05 .06 .12 .11 .13 .15 .20 .28 .06 .23 Adjusted 𝑅 " =.56 F(5,203)=53.48 a. Dependent Variable: Behavioral Intention Note. AE=Attitude toward Equal division of housework, AB= Attitude toward behavior of speaking to husband about equal division of housework, BI=Behavioral intention, PBC=Perceived Behavioral Control, SN=Subjective norms, DN=Descriptive norms As shown in Table 4, when three types of norms were entered to the second block, the regression coefficients of attitudes toward behavior and PBC were readjusted, taking into account the three norm variables entered to the second block. Both attitudes toward behavior and PBC remained significant even after including the three norms. Although descriptive norms alone are not a significant predictor of behavioral intentions, the three norms together moderated the relationship between TPB components of attitudes and PBC, and behavioral intentions. Thus, hypothesis 3 was supported. Hypothesis 4 Hypothesis 4 predicted injunctive norms will be the strongest predictor of Korean wives’ behavioral intention over subjective norms and descriptive norms. Regression analysis was conducted to reveal which is the strongest predictor among three different norms, F(5, 17 203)=71.67, p < .01. 𝑅" and the adjusted 𝑅" for the analysis were .51. The analysis confirmed that subjective and injunctive norms are equally significant predictor of behavioral intention (B=.37, p<.01). Consistent with the result of hypothesis 2, descriptive norms did not significantly predict behavioral intention (B=.03, p>.05). Since injunctive norms were not stronger predictor than subjective norms, hypothesis 4 was not supported. Research Question Research question was posed to determine how employment status of couples affects Korean wives’ behavioral intentions to talk with their husbands about the equal division of housework. In this study, participants’ employment status proportionally matched with the recent census in Korea as 42.9% of wives from dual income couples (N=91), 50.1% of them whose husbands were employed but they were not (N=106) and 6% of wives who were employed but their husbands were not (N=12) (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014b). A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) did not indicate significant differences in behavioral intentions across three types of employment status of couples (F=1.08, df=2/206, η" =.00, p>.05). Mean scores of behavioral intention were similar across three different conditions of when wives and husbands were equally employed (M=5.00, SD=1.16), when wives were not employed but husbands were working (M=5.04, SD=1.24), and when husbands were not employed but wives were working (M=4.48, SD=1.86). Each mean of behavioral intention was not significantly different from the total mean across three groups (M=4.99, SD=1.25). ANOVA indicated that Korean wives’ behavioral intention of speaking to their husbands about the equal division of housework were not affected by employment status of couples. 18 DISCUSSION Although Korean wives spent more time on doing housework than do their husbands regardless of employment status (Table 5), their perceived fairness toward the current division of housework was at the midpoint (M=4.17, SD=1.72) of a seven-point Likert scale. Table 5. The Average Minutes of Housework Per a Day by Employment Status of Couples Wife's Housework Husband’s Housework (minutes per a day) (minutes per a day) Dual Income Couple (N=91) 148.27 (SD=106.10) 66.78 (SD=92.54) When only husband is hired (N=106) 250.39 (SD=163.52) 59.81 (SD=97.97) When only wife is hired (N=12) 100.00 (SD=80.32) 67.74 (SD=86.62) Total (N=209) 197.29 (SD=147.46) 63.30 (SD=94.66) As Koreans hold a favorable attitude toward the equal division of housework but are not actually doing so (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014a), Korean wives did not perceive the unfair division as unfair although they have favorable attitude toward the equal division of housework (M=5.49, SD=1.41). The present study found that proximal peer norms can influence Korean wives’ intention to talk about the division of housework with their husbands to solve this issue. This study confirmed the previous findings (Park & Smith, 2007; Park, Klein, Smith, & Martell, 2009) that subjective norms, descriptive norms, and injunctive norms represent separate dimensions and each dimension has a unique role to influence individual’s behavioral intentions. Although subjective and injunctive norms directly predicted behavioral intention, descriptive norms were not a statistically significant predictor of Korean wives’ intention to talk with their husbands about the equal division of housework. Korean wives perceived that their close female friends who are also married approve of talking with husbands about the equal division of 19 housework. However, they did not perceive that many of their friends actually had conversation with their husbands. In addition, in spite of moderation effects of three norms, descriptive norms alone did not moderate the relationship between the TPB components of attitudes and PBC and behavioral intention. The present study also finds that employment status of couples did not influence on wives’ intention to talk with their husbands about the equal division of housework. The non-significant effect of descriptive norms might be explained by the fact that participants did not strongly perceive descriptive norms toward interpersonal communication with husbands about the equal division of housework. In the online survey, participants were asked to report their perceived descriptive norms (What percentage of your female married friends have talked to their husbands about the equal division of housework?) and perceived injunctive norms (What percentage of your female married friends approve of wives’ talking to husbands about the equal division of housework?). The mean of perceived descriptive norms was 55.11% (SD=26.81) while the mean of perceived injunctive norms was 74.01% (SD=25.86). Korean wives thought that roughly above half of wives’ proximal peers were actually engaged in the behavior whereas almost 3/4 of proximal peers approve of the behavior. This result suggests that an individual’s perception of social norms is closely related to the impact of those norms. The result might be interpreted as descriptive norms do not influence on wives’ behavioral intention to initiate the conversation with husbands about the equal division of housework. However, it may be one possibility to interpret the result as the reason why descriptive norms did not predict behavioral intention in the present study is because descriptive norms were not firmly established among proximal peers of Korean wives. Another interesting finding of the present study is that employment status of couples did not influence on wives’ intention to talk with their husbands about the equal division of 20 housework. It is conceivable to assume that wives are more likely to negotiate the equal division of housework when they are employed but their husbands are not. Unlike this expectation, the analysis for research question revealed that none of the variance in behavioral intention was explained by different types of employment status (dual income couple, when only husband is employed, when only wife is employed). This seemingly surprising result is consistent with the recent census results in Korea. According to the national poll conducted by the Korean National Statistical Office (2014), the wife always spent more time on housework than husband across three types of employment status of couples (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014b). A similar result was reported in the present study as shown in Table 5. The wives spent more time on housework regardless of employment status of couples. Consistent with this result, Korean wives’ intention to initiate conversation with husbands about the equal division of housework was similar across the employment status of couples since the employment status did not impact the division of housework. Implications This study is meaningful for three reasons. First, this is the first study in the communication field to look into the division of household labor in Korea. The topic has not attracted much attention so far even though it is directly related to daily lives and social issues. This study could bring attention to how interpersonal communication impacts traditional gender roles in Korea, which is relatively understudied. Second, the validity of the existing finding was tested in the present study. Replicating Park and Smith’s study (2007) reveals previous findings can be applied in the new context of the division of housework in Korea. Particularly, the present study limited the scope of a referent group to a proximal peer group (Lewis & Thombs, 2005; Park, Klein, Smith, & Martell, 2009) whereas previous studies focused on distal or global norms 21 (Park & Smith, 2007; Park, Smith, & Martell, 2009). The different level of norms induced the meaningful finding that norms established among proximal peers matter before Korean wives make a decision as whether to initiate a conversation regarding the division of housework with their husbands. Last, this paper also provides a practical implication to achieve the equal division of household labor in Korea. The paper shows that interpersonal communication between a husband and a wife can contribute to reduce the gap between attitude and the actual behavior so that the equal division of household labor can be achieved. A wife’s intention to talk about the equal division of housework is important to initiate the conversation about housework. But it may be hard to initiate the conversation when a wife does not evaluate the division of housework as unjust, which is commonly happening to housewives (Coltrane, 2000; Johnson, Galambos, & Anderson, 2016). The present paper emphasizes the importance of proximal peer norms in predicting Korean wives’ intention to talk about the equal division of housework. This leads to the conclusion that there are more chances to achieve the equal and equitable division of household labor when many people are talking about the topic. This is a useful piece of information in order to design a campaign related to the division of household labor in Korea in the future. Limitations The sample recruited for the study is nationally representative. Although the working status of couple is proportionally matched with national census, the number of wives who have jobs but their spouses do not was very small (N= 12) compared to other working statuses (dual income couple (N=91), only husband is working (N=106)). This is partly because it was hard to recruit wives who were employed but their husbands were not as they are only 6% of the Korean population (The Korean National Statistical Office, 2014b). If available, future study could be 22 conducted which recruits samples that are evenly divided across the three different working statuses of couples. The present study assumes that Korean wives’ behavior of speaking to husbands about the equal division of housework is a deliberative decision. As the TRA (Fishbein& Ajzen, 1975) and the TPB (Ajzen, 1991) imply, people fully consider and assess information when they deliberatively make decisions (Rhodes, Ewoldsen, Shen, Monahan, & Eno, 2014). However, it is also possible enough that Korean wives spontaneously decide to talk with their spouses about the equal division of housework. Unlike deliberative decision making, spontaneous decisions proceed from the consideration of readily accessible information from memory (Fazio, 1990; Gerrard et al., 2008; Olson & Fazio, 2009; Rhodes, Ewoldsen, Shen, Monahan, & Eno, 2014). Although it is reasonable to expect that Korean wives will thoroughly consider information before they decide to talk about the division of housework with their husbands given the traditional gender norms in the culture, some of them might automatically decide to speak to their husbands about the issue without cognitive process of carefully weighing information. Future studies can look into how different social norms and attitudes can play roles in Korean wives’ spontaneous decision making of speaking to their husbands about the equal division of housework by measuring accessibility of three different norms and accessibility of attitude toward the behavior of speaking about the equal division of household labor. Conclusion These results provide important evidence that proximal peer norms are particularly salient when Korean housewives make the decision to talk with their husbands about the equal division of housework. Both proximal peers’ subjective and injunctive norms influenced behavioral intention of talking to a husband about the equal division of housework as well as 23 attitudes and PBC of the behavior. The findings suggest that interventions designed for the equal division of housework have to focus on norms that are shared among wives’ proximal peers. According to the results, it would be more effective to inform Korean wives about other married female friends’ approval or expectations toward having a conversation with a husband about the division of household labor, rather than to simply figure of how many of their friends have ever engaged in the similar conversation. 24 APPENDIX 25 Measures for Talking with a Husband about the Division of Housework Demographic information What is your age? What is your husband’s age? What is your highest level of education? What is your husbands’ highest level of education? What is your working status? What is your husband’s working status? employed How many children do you have? In which area do you reside? 1. Urban 2. Rural How many years have you been married? Attitude toward the equal division of housework The equal division of household labor is good–bad, wise–unwise, favorable–unfavorable, beneficial–not beneficial. Perceived fairness in housework How do you feel about the fairness of housework in your relationship? (very unfair to me-very fair to me) Actual division of housework On average, how much time do you spend on doing housework per a day? On average, how much time does your husband spend on doing housework per a day? Behavioral Intention I intend to talk with my husband about the equal division of housework in the near future. I mean to talk with my husband about the equal division of housework in the near future. I have it in my mind to talk with my husband about the equal division of housework in the near future. I will talk with my husband about the equal division of housework in the near future. Direct measure of attitudes toward behavior Talking with my husband about the equal division of housework is good–bad, wise–unwise, favorable–unfavorable, beneficial–not beneficial. Perceived behavioral control It is easy to talk with my husband about the equal division of housework. I am confident about talking with my husband about the equal division of housework. I can talk with my husband about the equal division of housework. I know how to talk with my husband about the equal division of housework. Subjective norms My female friends who are married expect me to talk with my husband about the equal division of housework 26 My married female friends whose opinion I value expect me to talk with my husband about the equal division of housework. My married female friends who are important to me expect me to talk with my husbands about the equal division of housework. Descriptive norms My female friends who are married have talked with their husbands about the division of housework. Most of my female friends who are married have talked with their husbands about the equal division of housework. Most of my female friends who are married have had discussions about the division of housework with their husbands. What percentage of your female married friends have talked to their husbands about the equal division of housework? (0-100%) Injunctive norms Most of my female friends who are married would approve of my talking with my husband about the equal division of housework. Most of my female friends who are married would endorse my talking with my husband about the equal division of housework. Most of my female friends who are married would support that I express to my husband my opinions about the equal division of housework. What percentage of your female married friends approve of wives talking to husbands about the equal division of housework? (0-100%) 27 REFERENCES 28 REFERENCES Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211. Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood-Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Askari, S. F., Liss, M., Erchull, M. J., Staebell, S. E., & Axelson, S. J. (2010). Men want equality, but women don’t expect it: Young adults' expectations for participation in household and child care chores. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 34, 243–252. Choi, E. (1994). Status of the family and motherhood for Korean women. In J. Gelb & M. Palley (Eds.). Women of Japan and Korea. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1015-1026. Coltrane, S. (2000). Research on household labor: Modeling and measuring the social embeddedness of routine family work. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 1208 –1233. Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior: A Review and Avenues for Further Research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429–1464. Croucher, R., & Miles, L. (2009). The legal structuring of women’s collective voice in the South Korean workplace. Journal of Contemporary Asia, 39, 231-246. De Wit, J. B. F., De Ridder, D. T. D., Stok, F. M., & De Vet, E. (2014). Don't tell me what I should do, but what others do: The influence of descriptive and injunctive peer norms on fruit consumption in adolescents. British Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 52-64. The Economist (2016). The glass ceiling index (Data file). Retrieved from http://www.economist.com/blogs/graphicdetail/2016/03/daily-chart-0 Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 75-109. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. 29 Gerrard, M., Gibbons, F. X., Houlihan, A. E., Stock, M. L., & Pomery, E. A. (2008). A dualprocess approach to health risk decision making: The prototype willingness model. Developmental Review, 28, 29-61. Han, S. M. (2012). Today's working women in South Korea. Career Planning and Adult Development Journal, 28, 37-47. Han, J., & Ling, L. H. M. (1998). Authoritarianism in the hyper- masculinized state: Hybridity, patriarchy, and capitalism in Korea. International Studies Quarterly, 42, 53–78. Hochschild, A. R. (1989). The second shift: Working parents and the revolution at home. New York, N.Y: Viking. Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind. London: McGraw Hill.
 Hofstede, G. (1993). Cultural constraints in management theories. The Executive, 7, 84–91. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Jacobson, R., Mortensen, C., & Cialdini, R. (2011). Bodies obliged and unbound: Differentiated response tendencies for injunctive and descriptive social norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 433-448. Johnson, M. D., Galambos, N. L., & Anderson, J. R. (2016). Skip the dishes? Not so fast! Sex and housework revisited. Journal of Family Psychology, 30, 203-213. Jung, J., & Cho, S. Y. (2015). Factors influencing household work sharing and perceptions of equitable and unequitable household work sharing in dual-earner households. The Korean Journal of Community Living Science, 26, 717-729. Kagitcibasi, C. (2005). Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context: Implications for self and family. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 403-422. Kilmartin, C., Smith, T., Green, A., Heinzen, H., Kuchler, M., & Kolar, D. (2008). A real time social norms intervention to reduce male sexism. Sex Roles, 59, 264-273. Kim, H. K. (2009). Analyzing the gender division of labor: The cases of the United States and South Korea. Asian Perspective, 33, 181–229. The Korean National Statistical Office (2014a). The opinion and the reality of the division of household labor in South Korea (Data file). Retrieved from http://www.index.go.kr/potal/main/EachDtlPageDetail.do?idx_cd=3029 30 The Korean National Statistical Office (2014b). Regional employment report (Data file). Retrieved from http://kostat.go.kr/portal/korea/kor_nw/2/1/index.board?bmode=read&aSeq=346897 Lapinski, M. K., & Rimal, R. N. (2005). An explication of social norms. Communication Theory, 15, 127–147. Lee, D. W., Park, H. S., Lee, T. S., Kim, M. K., & Kim, Y. H. (2006). Korean working adults’ and undergraduates' attitudes toward, and self-efficacy in, joining drinking parties. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 34, 487–497. Lennon, M. C., & Rosenfield, S. (1994). Relative fairness and the division of housework: The importance of options. American Journal of Sociology, 100, 506-531. Lewis, T. F., & Thombs D. L. (2005). Perceived risks and normative beliefs as explanatory models for college student alcohol involvement: An assessment of a campus with conventional alcohol control policies and enforcement practices. NASPA Journal, 42, 202– 222. Martens, M. P., Page, J. C., Mowry, E. S., Damann, K. M., Taylor, K. K., & Cimini, M. D. (2006). Differences between actual and perceived student norms: An examination of alcohol use, drug use, and sexual behavior. Journal of American College Health, 54, 295-300. Melnyk, V., Van Herpen, E., Fischer, A. R. H., & Van Trijp, H. C. M. (2011). To think or not to think: The effect of cognitive deliberation on the influence of injunctive versus descriptive norms. Psychology & Marketing, 28, 709–729 Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures (pp. 19-63). New York, NY: Psychology Press. The Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (2014). Balancing paid work, unpaid work and leisure (Data file). Retrieved from https://www.oecd.org/gender/data/balancingpaidworkunpaidworkandleisure.htm Paek, H., Lee, H., & Hove, T. (2014). The role of collectivism orientation in differential normative mechanisms: A cross-national study of anti-smoking public service announcement effectiveness. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 173-183. Park, H. S., & Smith, S. W. (2007). Distinctiveness and influence of subjective norms, personal descriptive and injunctive norms, and societal descriptive and injunctive norms on behavioral intent: A case of two behaviors critical to organ donation. Human Communication Research, 33, 194–218. 31 Park, H. S., Klein, K. A, Smith, S., & Martell, D. (2009). Separating subjective norms, university descriptive and injunctive norms, and U.S. descriptive and injunctive norms for drinking behavior intentions. Health Communication, 24, 746–751. Park, H. S., & Lee, D. W. (2009). A test of theory of planned behavior in Korea: participation in alcohol-related social gatherings. International Journal of Psychology: Journal International de Psychologie, 44, 418–33. Perales, F., Baxter, J., & Tai, T. (2015). Gender, justice and work: A distributive approach to perceptions of housework fairness. Social Science Research, 51, 51-63. Perkins, H. W., & Berkowitz, A. D. (1986). Using student alcohol surveys: Notes on clinical and educational program applications. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 31, 44-51. Perkins, H. W., Haines, M. P., & Rice, R. (2005). Misperceiving the college drinking norm and related problems: a nationwide study of exposure to prevention information, perceived norms and student alcohol misuse. Journal of studies on alcohol, 66, 470-478. Rhodes, N., Ewoldsen, D. R., Shen, L., Monahan, J. L., & Eno, C. (2014). The accessibility of family and peer norms in young adolescent risk behavior. Communication Research, 41, 3– 26. Seo, Y., Leather, P., & Coyne, I. (2012). South Korean culture and history: The implications for workplace bullying. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17, 419-422. Silvia, P. J. (2006). Reactance and the dynamics of disagreement: Multiple paths from threatened freedom to resistance to persuasion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 673–685. Tafarodi, R. W., & Walters, P. (1999). Individualism–collectivism, life events, and self-esteem: A test of two trade-offs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 797-814. Yun, D., & Silk, K. J. (2011). Social norms, self-identity, and attention to social comparison information in the context of exercise and healthy diet behavior. Health Communication, 26, 275-285. 32