apt-L 0, . - ; . I Anyapny: M 11h? , . . “ ; 1 / g a r ll HI Hill “I llll HI llllll llll II "II“ . 3 1293 103gflnlwu8fluyfl" f, v." I L IB R A R Y Michigan State University . H This is to certify that the thesis entitled A Study of Teacher Planning: Description and Theory Development Using Ethnographic and Information Processing Methods presented by Robert Johnston Yinger has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in Education are, firm Major professor Ij/x / 77 Date 0-7 639 ® Copyright by ROBERT JOHNSTON YINGER 1977 A STUDY OF TEACHER PLANNING: DESCRIPTION AND THEORY DEVELOPMENT USING ETHNOGRAPHIC AND INFORMATION PROCESSING METHODS BY Robert Johnston Yinger A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY v Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1977 ABSTRACT A STUDY OF TEACHER PLANNING: DESCRIPTION AND THEORY DEVELOPMENT USING ETHNOGRAPHIC AND INFORMATION PROCESSING METHODS BY Robert Johnston Yinger The major purpose of this study was to investigate teacher plan- ning by means of a detailed case study of the processes involved in one elementary (first-second grade) teacher's planning decisions during a five month period of instruction. The study was designed to address a need for descriptions and theoretical models of teacher planning and to examine the usefulness of certain decision modeling methods for describing complex decisions as they occur in field settings. To ac- complish this, the study used the perspectives and methods of both ethnography and information processing psychology. The study involved two phases of data collection. In the first twelve weeks of the study, approximately forty full days were spent observing and recording the teacher's activities in both the preactive and interactive phases of teaching. Also during this phase, the teach- er's planning decisions were recorded as she "thought aloud" during her planning sessions. The second phase of the data collection further in- vestigated the teacher's planning by observing her behavior in the Teacher Planning Shell (a simulation task developed for this study) and in three judgment tasks examining the teacher's perceptions of her Robert Johnston Yinger students and instructional activities. Additional classroom observa- tions and interviews were also conducted during this phase. Two central aspects of the teacher's planning and instruction that emerged in this study were planning for instructional activities and the use of teaching routines. Activities were described as the basic structural units of planning and action in the classroom and were por- trayed as "controlled behavior settings" which were shaped and molded by the teacher to conform to her perceptions and purposes. Seven fea- tures of instructional activity were identified (location, structure and sequence, duration, participants, acceptable student behavior, in- structional moves or routines, and content and materials) and used as a basis for analyzing and describing the teacher's planning and in- struction. Teaching routines emerged as another distinctive feature of the teacher's planning "technology." Four types of teaching routines were described in this study: activity routines, instructional routines, management routines, and executive planning routines. Functionally, routines were characterized as methods the teacher used to reduce the complexity and increase the predictability of classroom activities, thus increasing her flexibility and effectiveness. Two models of teacher planning were developed in this study. The first was a structural model of preactive planning describing planning at five levels: (1) yearly planning, (2) term planning, (3) unit plan- ning, (4) weekly planning, and (5) daily planning. The second model of planning generated in this study was a theoretical model of teacher planning based on data collected in this study and on studies of plan- ning in the areas of chess thinking, musical composition, art, and Robert Johnston Yinger architectural design. Planning decisions were characterized by pro- cesses emphasizing problem finding and problem formulation as well as problem solving. In contrast to traditional models of planning emphasi- zing the statements of goals, the specification of alternatives, and the choice among alternatives, this model placed greater emphasis on finding and developing the planning problem and on the "design" process. Three stages of planning were represented in the planning model. The first stage, problem finding, was portrayed as a "discovery cycle" where the teacher's goal conceptions, her knowledge and experience, her notion of the planning dilemmaland the materials available for planning interact to produce an initial problem conception worthy of further ex- ploration. The second stage in the planning process was problem formu- lation and solution, where problem solving was characterized as a de- sign process involving progressive elaboration of plans over time. The third stage of the planning model involved implementation of the plan, its evaluation, and its eventual routinization. Dedicated to My parents, who have loved me and encouraged me throughout all my endeavors ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS A study of this magnitude would not have been possible except for the support and guidance provided by my doctoral guidance committee and the resources and support furnished by the Institute for Research on Teaching. I want to express my appreciation to the members of my guidance committee, Dr. Robert Bridgham, Dr. Joe Byers, Dr. Christopher Clark, and Dr. Edward Smith, who had the confidence in my abilities to approve this project and the continuing interest in the work to help see it through to its completion. To my chairman, Dr. Lee Shulman, a special thanks. He has been a continuous source of ideas, support, and chal- lenge throughout this project and has served as an inspiration and a model for scholarship throughout my graduate studies. This study was conducted while serving as a Research Intern at the Institute for Research on Teaching at Michigan State University. The Institute is funded primarily by the Teaching Division of the National Institute of Education, United States Department of Health, Education, and Welfare (Contract No. 400-76-0073). The Institute provided the support, resources, and research environment that enabled me to pursue a study of this intensity and duration. The many individuals who reacted to my ideas and findings throughout the study are too numerous to mention. Thank you all. I want to especially thank Debbie, Sheba, iii and Lynn for the many hours spent transcribing the field notes, proto- ~cols, and early drafts. Their help was invaluable. When one decides to do a study of only one individual, a great deal is riding on his or her participation and cooperation. I want to express my greatest appreciation to Mrs. Lisa who was willing to open up her professional life for five months and put up with an outsider's attempts to understand what was going on in her head. Her friend- ship is an important outcome of this study. Finally, I want to thank my wife Janet for her companionship and love. She has been a never ending source of patience, support, and encouragement and a joy to live with. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter Page. LIST OF TABLES o o o o o o O C a 0 o O o o o o o o o o o x LIST OF FIGURES o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o Xi I. INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Problem Purpose . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Overview of the Study . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 11 12 II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 Organization . . . . . . . A Definition of Planning . Planning as Design . Planning as Process . Planning Theory . . . Planning in Education . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Prescriptive Planning Literature Teacher Planning Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 14 15 15 18 24 25 29 III 0 METIIIOD . O O O O O O O O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O 36 Scope of the Study . . Selecting the Teacher . . . . . . . . Naturalistic Data Collection . . . . . . . . . . . . . An Information Processing Approach An Ethnographic Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of the Naturalistic Data Collection . The Teacher Planning Shell . Judgment Tasks . Pupil Sort . . . Activity Sort . . . . Sociogram Analysis Analysis . . . . Chapter Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 37 38 39 44 47 48 50 50 51 51 52 54 Chapter IV. Page THE SETTING 0 O O O 0 O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O O O O O 56 The Stage: Byron School . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 The Set and Props for Learning: Mrs. Lisa's Classroom . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Main Characters: Teacher and Students . The Teacher . The Students . . . . . . . . . . . . The Script: The Curriculum . . . . . . . . . A Typical Plot: A Day at School . The Drama: Life in the Classroom Classroom Organization The Weekly Schedule . . Grouping for Instruction Instructional Goals . Classroom Discipline . . Creativity in Teaching . . . . . . . . . . . Communication with Parents Summary of Classroom Life . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Set and the Props for Planning . Planning at School Planning at Home Chapter Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 63 63 64 67 .70 84 85 86 92 93 94 95 97 99 99 . 100 . 103 . 105 ACTIVITIES AND ROUTINES: A TECHNOLOGY OF TEACHING . . . 106 Part I: Teaching by Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 Activities as Structural Units of Planning and Action . . . . . . Types of Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Activity Frequency and Distribution . . . Activities as Controlled Behavior Settings Features of Instructional Activities Location . . . . . . . . Structure and Sequence . Duration . . . Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Acceptable Student Behavior Instructional Moves . Content and Materials Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Part II: Routines in Teaching . Types of Teaching Routines Activity Routines . . . Instructional Routines . Management Routines . . . . . . Executive Planning Routines Teaching Routines and Planning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Teaching Routines and Interactive Teaching Chapter Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 . 109 . 111 . 116 . 120 . 121 . 123 . 129 . 135 . 135 . 137 . 140 . 141 . 142 . 143 . 143 . 153 . 155 . 159 . 160 . 162 . 16S vi Chapter Page V VI. A STRUCTURAL MODEL OF PREACTIVE PLANNING . . . . . . . . 167 Overview of the Model Rationale . . . . . Levels of Planning . . . Descriptive Dimensions Level V - Yearly Planning Planning Goals . . . Information Sources . Form of the Plan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness . Level IV - Term Planning . Planning Goals . . . Information Sources . Form of the Plan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness . Level III - Unit Planning Planning Goals . . . Information Sources . Form of the Plan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness . Level II - Weekly Planning . Planning Goals . . . Information Sources . Form of the Plan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness . Level I - Daily Planning . Planning Goals . . . Infbrmation Sources . Form of the Plan . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness . Connections Between Levels of Planning . Chapter Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167 167 169 172 174 174 176 179 180 181 182 184 185 186 187 187 188 189 190 191 191 193 194 195 196 196 198 199 199 200 203 . VII. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF PLANNING I: Data from Other Fields 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 205 Reproductive Components of Thought . The Theory of Selz General Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Most General Solving Methods . . . . . . . . . Means Finding in Directed Productive Thought . . . . . . . Linking of Solving Methods . . . . . . . . Phase Structure and Bahle's Work on . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Musical Composition . . . . . . . . . . Thought and Choice in Chess: DeGroot's Theory Phase Structure of Chess Thinking General Methods of Chess Thinking Plan formation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 206 207 208 209 210 217 218 221 222 224 225 vii Chapter Page Progressive Deepening . Trying Out Summary . . . . . . . Problem Finding in Art: Problem Situations Problem Finding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Creative Process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Problem Finding and Creativity Reasoning in Architectural Design Introduction . . . . Problem Formulation . Solving Methods . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . VIII. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF PLANNING: A Process Model of Teacher Planning . . . . . . . . . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Toward a Theory of Teacher Planning: The Model's Purpose . Grounding of the Model . . . . General Features of the Model The Model's Focus . . . . . . . . . Purposefulness and Rationality Intelligence . Design . Choice . . . . . . . . . . . . . Design or Decision? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Outline of the Planning Model . Problem Finding Introduction . . . . . . . . . . General Teaching Dilemma The Discovery Cycle . Planning Dilemma . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Teaching Goal Conceptions Knowledge and Experience . Materials . . . . . . . Initial Problem Conception Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Problem Formulation and Solution . Introduction . . The Design Cycle . . . . . . . . The Elaboration Phase The Investigation Phase The Adaptation Phase . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Implementation, Evaluation, and Routinization Introduction . . . . . . . . . Implementation and Evaluation . Routinization . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 226 227 229 230 232 235 236 239 239 243 245 250 252 252 252 254 258 258 259 261 262 262 263 264 266 266 267 270 270 271 275 279 281 282 284 284 288 291 295 300 305 306 306 309 312 314 Chapter Page IX. DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . 317 Comparison of Results with other Planning Studies Discussion of Method . . . . . The Ethnographic Approach . . . . . . . . . . . The Information Processing Approach . The Teacher Planning Shell and the Judgment Tasks . . . . . Conclusions About the Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Implications and Directions for Future Research The Technology of the Teaching Process . . The Structural Model of Preactive Planning The Process Model of Teacher Planning . Problem Finding . . . . . . . . . Problem Formulation and Solution . Implementation, Evaluation, and Routinization . . . . . A Model for Planning Research . Implications for Teacher Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . The Current Status of Training for Instructional Planning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A "Via Media": Planning as Intuitive Design Suggestions for Teacher Training Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . smary O O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPmDIX A O O O O O O C O O O O O l O O O O O O O O O O 0 APPENDIX B . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX C . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPme D O O O O O O O O O O, O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 APPENDIX E . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . APPEND IX F O O O O O O C I O O O O O O O O O O O I O O O 0 APPENDIX G O O O O O O O O O O O O O C C O O O O O O O O . LIST OF REFERENCES . , 317 324 324 327 328 330 330 331 332 333 333 334 336 336 339 339 340 344 348 349 349 356 361 381 385 395 400 404 416 425 ix LIST OF TABLES Table 4.1 Number of students in Mrs. Lisa's classroom during the year . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 66 Page Table 5.1 Mean number of activities per day in each subject matter area during a twelve week period of instruction . . . . . . . . . . . 113 Table 5.2 Teacher-roles observed in activities during a representative three week period in winter term . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128 Table 5.3 Frequency of complex and simple activities during a twelve week period of instruction in winter term . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 130 Table 5.4 Mean duration of activities by subject matter area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 134 Table 5.5 Frequency of Mrs. Lisa's teacher-roles during a three week period of instruction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 138 Table 5.6 Routinization of activities in the weekly schedule for winter term . . . . . . . . . 147 Table 7.1 Types of problem situations and related cognitive functions . . . . . . . . . . . . 233 Table 7.2 Correlations between ratings on three dimensions of product evaluation and the problem-finding variable scores (Getzels and Csickzentmihalyi, 1976) . . . . . . . . 237 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1 The missing element in studies of teaching Figure 4.1 Floor plan of Byron School . . . . . . . . Figure 4.2 Mrs. Lisa's classroom . . . . . . . . . . . Figure 4.3 Divisions in the school day . . . . . . . . Figure 4.4 Winter term schedule . . . . . . . . . . . Figure 4.5 Spring term schedule . . . . . . . . . . . Page 59 62 88 90 91 Figure 5.1 Activities in Mrs. Lisa's classroom . . . . 110 Figure 5.2 Frequency of instructional activities in Mrs. Lisa's classroomrduring a twelvedweek period of instruction . . . . . . . . . . . 112 Figure 5.3 Mean percentage of daily activities in each subject matter area during a twelve-week period of instruction . . . . . . . . . . . 114 Figure 5.4 Reshaping the task environment by planning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119 Figure 5.5 Locations of activities in Mrs. Lisa's classroom . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122 Figure 5.6 Definitions for teacher role categories (After Gump, 1967) . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 Figure 5.7 Approximate allocated times for activities in Mrs. Lisa's classroom . . . . . . . . . 133 Figure 5.8 Effects of complexity and routinization on the structure and duration of an act iVi ty 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 150 Figure 5.9 Distribution of Mrs. Lisa's activities on dimensions of complexity and routinization 151 Figure 6.1 A structural model of preactive planning . 170 xi Page Figure 6.2 Interaction between levels of planning (illustrated for fall term) . . . . . . . . . . . . 202 Figure 8.1 Stages of the planning process . . . . . . . . . . . 265 Figure 8.2 The problem finding stage of teacher planning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 268 Figure 8.3 The problem formulation and solution (design) stage of teacher planning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 289 Figure 8.4 The implementation, evaluation, and routinization stage of teacher planning . . . . . . . 308 Figure 9.1 A model for planning research . . . . . . . . . . . 338 xii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY Problem Much of the research on teaching in the last twenty years has in- volved the search for those teaching behaviors that are related to teaching effectiveness. The results of these efforts have been some- what disappointing in that there have been few teaching behaviors that have been strongly and consistently related to student achievement or student attitudes. A general characteristic of most of these studies is their focus on teaching behavior that occurs when students are in the classroom. Jackson (1965) has referred to these face-to- face encounters between teacher and students as "interactive" teaching and has contrasted these behaviors with "preactive" teaching. Pre- active teaching includes behavior that occurs before and after school, during recess, and at other times when the teacher is alone in the c1as$room. This behavior in the "empty classroom" may include such things as preparing lesson plans, marking papers, setting up equipment, making and running dittos, thinking about how to deal with certain be- havior or learning problems, and so forth. Although this distinction between preactive and interactive teaching has been popular for many years, there have been few studies that have set out to examine the world of teaching when the students are absent. Recently it has become popular to characterize teachers as problem solvers and decision makers (Shulman and Elstein, 1975; Lanier and Shulman, 1975). Many educational researchers have contended that the most important teaching skill is decision making (e.g., Shavelson, 1973), or have gone on to assert "in teaching it's the thought that counts. "1 One consequence of this view is the temptation to portray the teacher as a rational information processor who is chiefly involved in making diagnoses, testing hypotheses, and making decisons all day long. It is much more likely that this conceptualization of teaching more accurately describes some moments of teaching than others. Jack- son argues that although there may be some advantage to using logical and rational models to describe the teacher's in-class activities, op- portunities for this type of behavior during interactive teaching are "few and far between" (Jackson, 1965). The rapidity and immediacy of the teacher's interaction with pupils in the classroom, he argues, pre- cludes the rational-purposeful kind of thinking that is normally asso- ciated with problem solving and decision making. To understand teaching as a purposeful, reflective activity, it is necessary to look in those places where this type of behavior is most likely to occur. Jackson argues that the preactive phase of teaching is one place where the notion of the teacher as problem solver and de- cision maker may have the most descriptive power. He goes on to say: As the teacher goes about deciding what textbook to use, how to group the children for reading, or whether to notify Billy's parents of his poor performance in arithmetic, his behavior is at least analyzable in terms that describe the 1This saying has been attributed to Dr. Perry Lanier and has be- come the informal motto of the Institute for Research on Teaching at Michigan State University. rational problem solver. At such moments concepts such as evidence, evaluation, prediction, and feedback have real meaning for understanding what the teacher is doing. It is doubtful that they have the same meaning in the interactive setting. (Jackson, 1965, p. 15) Of the many different things that teachers do in the preactive phase of teaching, one of the most important may be planning. It may be a rare teacher and classroom that would be able to function effec- tively without some kind of planning by the teacher. The wealth and variety of instructional materials available for teaching, the emphasis on meeting school or district objectives, and the wide range of student aptitudes in most classrooms are but a few of the demands on teachers that virtually necessitate thinking and planning for the term, coming weeks, or even the next day. The importance of teacher planning has been further emphasized in recent ecological studies of the classroom (Kounin, 1970; Gump, 1969; Doyle, 1977 a,b). In a study of beginning teachers, Doyle (1977 a) found the most salient characteristics of the classroom environment for those teachers were: (1) multidimensionality, (2) simultaneity, and (3) unpredictability. By multidimensionality, Doyle means that class- rooms serve a variety of purposes not all of which are compatible. Classrooms are simultaneous in that significant events often occur at the same time rather than following each other in serial fashion. Un- predictability refers to the degree to which the complexity of ebb and flow in classroom events prevents the teacher from accurately predict- ing the outcome of a planned activity. By adding to these characteris- tics those of urgency and spontaneity or, as Jackson (1968) refers to it, the "immediacy" of the classroom, one arrives at a picture of the teaching environment dominated by two features: complexity and un- predictability. In addition to characterizing the environment in which teachers are required to operate, ecological psychology acknowledges and empha- sizes the subtle yet complex interdependencies between behavior and en- vironment. As a way of looking at classrooms, ecological studies are based on the premise that the environmental demands of the classroom both shape observed behavior and establish limits to the range of re- sponse options available to the actors (Doyle, 1977 b). In other words, "settings have plans for their inhabitants' behavior, and inputs are achieved within the limits of the settings' control system to produce the planned behavior" (Barker, 1963). What this means for teachers is that not only is the classroom environment complex and unpredictable, but teaching behavior in the classroom may be to a large degree "con- trolled" or "planned" by the environment itself. Jackson illustrates how one central feature of the environment, the students, may affect teaching by suggesting that: ...the students to some extent control what the teacher does. When they are present, much of the teacher's be- havior is in response to their requests and questions and could not have been planned in detail ahead of time. In effect, the students "tell" the teacher what to do, and he simply does it without much thought. Much of what goes on during a teaching session (or for that matter during almost any kind of an interpersonal encounter) is pre- dictable in a broad sense only; the specifics must be dealt with as they happen. (Jackson, 1965, p. 13) If it is true that teaching behavior in the classroom is, to a large degree, a function of the features of the environment, then it becomes an important question to ask how the teacher can influence the environment so that behavior within the interactive setting conforms as closely as possible to the teacher's goals. It may be that teacher planning becomes the major tool by which teachers manipulate teaching environments to shape and control settings that may later shape and control their own behavior. To date, there have been no studies of teacher planning that have explored the role that planning plays either psychologically or func- tionally in teaching. The ecological studies have been concerned with identifying the salient features of teaching environments and describ- ing teaching behavior that is "adaptive" to the environment. Kounin's (1970) study of classroom management (much of whose findings have since been replicated by Brophy and Evertson (1974, a,b,c) suggests that the behavior of successful classroom managers is characterized by "withit- ness," or the awareness of what is going on at all times in the class- room; "overlappingness," or the ability to sustain one activity while doing something else at the same time; "smoothness," or the ability to maintain classroom continuity without unnecessary interruptions or con- fusion; and "momentum," or the ability to maintain proper lesson pacing and flow. Similarly, Doyle (1977 a) found five teaching "skills" that were successful for reducing environmental complexity for beginning teachers: 1. Chunking, or the ability to group discrete events into larger units. 2. Timing, or the ability to monitor and control the duration of events. 3. Overlap, or the ability to handle two or more events at once (borrowed from the Kounin). 4. Differentiation, or the ability to discriminate among units in terms of their immediate and long term signifi- cance. 5. Rapid judgment, or the ability to interpret events with a minimum of delay. The picture of teaching that these studies paint is of the envi- ronment on the one hand, characterized by such things as complexity and unpredictability, and on the other hand, successful or "adaptive" teaching behavior such as "withitness," "Chunking," "smoothness," and "timing." What they fail to portray are those elements of teaching that allow or facilitate a teacher's coping with these demands of teaching. It is suggested here that it is largely through planning that teachers simplify and shape the teaching environment so that they can successfully deal with classroom life. This portrayal of planning as the missing yet important element in these studies is illustrated in Figure 1.1. To summarize the argument thus far, most of the studies that have examined teaching have ignored that part of teaching that occurs be- fore students arrive at and after they leave school; and if one is interested in portraying the teacher as a problem solver or decision maker, it is probably more likely that this type of deliberation will occur in these "preactive" phases of teaching. Furthermore, of the many different things that teachers do in the "empty classroom," one of the most important may be planning. Ecological studies have pointed out the demanding characteristics of the environment and suc- cessful behavior for coping with the complexity and unpredictability of the classroom. It is posited that planning may play an important role in helping teachers to function effectively and efficiently in the classroom by allowing them to manipulate and shape behavior set- tings. The study of planning, therefore, becomes important because of M U T N E M O M , 5 2 2 5 3 7 1 3 3 1 3 1 3 G N I K N U H C P A L R E V O G N ' M ' T , R E H C A E T G N I N N A L P \ Y C A I D E M M I Y T I X E L P M O C \ Y T I L I B A T C I D E R P N U ' ' Y T I E N A T L U M I S ‘ Y T I L A N O I S N E M I D I T L U M S R O I V A H E B G N I H C A E T E V I T P A D A T N E M N O R I V N E M O O R S S A L C g n i h c a e t f o s e i d u t s n i t n e m e l e g n i s s i m e h T 1 . 1 E R U G I F its role in teaching and because it may be one of the important teach- ing activities where the teacher can and does function in a more ra- tional and deliberative manner. If this thesis is warranted, it may also have important conse- quences for the training of teachers. If effective and efficient methods for making planning decisions can be described and developed, they could become an important feature of teacher training and, thus, a powerful way to improve teacher effectiveness. Much of traditional teacher training has focused on those aspects of teaching important to teacher-pupil interaction in the classroom. Since the early sixties, an emphasis has been put on planning and in- struction that Jackson (1968) has referred to as the "engineering" point of view. This viewpoint includes both a set of values about the educational process and a set of instructional procedures by which the values can be implemented. Jackson summarizes the "engineering" view- point as follows: The core of values to be discussed here--the "goods," so to speak, of the engineering point of view--comprise the standards by which one might judge a piece of machinery or the plans for achieving a military objective. The first question, of course, is: Will it work? Will it get the job'done? This question, which entails the criterion of effectiveness, implies a clear idea of what job is to be done or what objectives are to be reached. Next comes a series of secondary questions having to do with the effi- ciency of the procedure under consideration. After ascer- taining that it will work, the critic is next interested in knowing whether it will do so speedily, accurately, precisely, and economically. These questions are chiefly concerned with the conservation of energy and expense. In the best of all possible worlds, according to this view, jobs should be done as cheaply and as quickly as possible, with a minimum amount of wasted motion. (Jackson, 1968, p. 164) When this viewpoint is applied to education, it usually takes on some variation of the following form. The first requirement is pre- paration of a detailed statement of objectives, preferably in "behav- ioral" form. Next one must analyze the skills and knowledge that a student will need to attain the objectives. This analysis is often carried out by a process called "task analysis." After the task is analyzed, the skills and knowledge that the student already possesses are measured. After instructional materials and techniques are se- lected, based on where the student is in relation to the objectives, the rest of the instruction process proceeds by the following sequence: teaching, evaluation of performance relative to the objectives, and finally, if the student has failed to master the objectives on the first try, re-teaching the lesson. This specific cycle of instruction has become known as the Basic Teaching Model of Glaser and includes the essential features of most goal-referenced instructional models (e.g., Popham and Baker, 1970). In addition to instruction, the "engineering" point of view has been prescribed for curriculum and classroom planning. For example, Taba (1962) recommends the following eight steps for planning a unit: 1. Diagnose needs. 2. Formulate specific objectives. 3. Select content. 4. Organize content. 5. Select learning experiences. 6. OrganiZe learning experiences. 7. Evaluate. 8. Check balance and sequence. 10 Such prescriptive theories of planning are common in education, al- though relatively little is known about how teachers actually plan, There has been much debate in educational circles about elements of the "engineering“ point of view such as "behavioral objectives," but there have been few empirical investigations of classroom teachers to examine whether this point of view is an accurate portrayal of how teachers gag_and dg_function in the classroom. One of the benefits of a detailed analysis of the planning pro- cess may be a better understanding of methods and procedures that classroom teachers can realistically use. Critics of the engineering view argue that one of its major weaknesses is its oversimplified image of what actually goes on in the classroom. They argue that it ignores features of the classroom environment mentioned above such as simultaneity, immediacy, multidimensionality, and unpredictability. They also argue that the engineering view overlooks such potentially important aspects of teaching as the teacher's interest in the sty- listic and artistic qualities of his or her own performance. An important outcome of investigations focusing on the planning process may be models of teacher planning that are known to reflect the teaching environment and the abilities and concerns of teachers. One might argue against this procedure by stating that what "is" in teaching is not necessarily what "should be." The approach taken in this study is not in opposition to this statement, but rather is based on the belief that it is important to examine and describe the behav- ior of experienced and successful practitioners who have developed strategies and methods for functioning effectively in the teaching environment. It is argued that models of planning based on what is 11 possible in the classroom will in the long run he more effective than more efficient models "borrowed" from other fields that are too diffi- cult and complex for most classroom teachers to implement. In this manner, descriptive models of teacher planning can lead to prescrip- tive models for preservice and inservice teacher training. The potential importance of teacher planning for effective teach- ing, the potential for portraying teacher planning as decision making, the lack of knowledge about how experienced teachers actually plan, and the potential importance of more accurate and effective planning models for teacher training are the central reasons for pursuing this study of teacher planning. The primary problem of the present study is to investigate and describe the decision processes involved in planning for instruction. Purpose The major purpose of this study is to investigate teacher plan- ning by a detailed examination and description of the decision pro- cesses involved in one elementary teacher's planning during a five month period of instruction. The specific objectives of the study fall under two major categories. The first category includes ques- tions about the planning process itself: 1. What different kinds of planning decisions does the teacher make? 2. What are the relationships, if any, among these decisions? 3. What information or cues are gathered and proCessed by the teacher to make planning decisions? 4. What is the major "unit" or element that the teacher thinks about when planning? 12 5. What are the general strategies and processes of the teacher's planning, and how might these processes be represented in a decision model? The second set of questions addresses those special problems and concerns related to conducting descriptive information processing re- search in complex naturalistic settings: 6. How useful are ethnographic methods for studying classroom decision making? 7. What is the usefulness of "process tracing" methods for modeling teacher decisions? 8. What are the advantages and limitations of various structured and unstructured methods for collecting planning data in the preactive phase of teaching? 9. How successfully can a "moderate fidelity" judgment or In-Basket type task be used to supplement pre- active and interactive classroom data collection strategies? In addition to these questions, a goal of this study is to gener- ate further questions about teacher planning and indicate potentially fruitful areas for future study. It is the intent of this study not to test specific research hypotheses, but rather to generate hypothe- ses about teacher planning and, if possible, weave them into a model of teacher planning that might be tested and elaborated by further study. Because of the huge gaps in our knowledge about teacher de- cision making, there is no way in which this study can be much more than a first step towards understanding the determinants and processes of teacher planning decisions. Overview of the Study Chapter II consists of a review of the pertinent literature on planning theory in fields outside of teaching as well as a comprehen- sive review of the literature on curriculum and teacher planning in 13 education. In Chapter III, method is discussed including the scope of the study, the selection of the teacher, data collection, and analysis. Chapter IV presents a description of the setting in which the study took place to provide a backdrop for later descriptive analysis. In Chapter V, two features of the teacher's "technology" of teaching, instructional activities, andiroutines are discussed; and in Chapter VI, a structural model of preactive planning is described. Chapters VII and VIII represent an attempt to develop a theoretical model of the planning process. Chapter VII discusses psychological research on deliberative, planning-related tasks in fields other than education. Chapter VIII presents a theoretical model consistent with the data discussed in Chapter VII and with the data collected in this study. Chapter IX includes a comparison of the findings of this study with those of previous planning studies, questions for future research, and implications for teacher training. A summary and conclusions are pre- sented in Chapter x. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Organization It has only been within the last ten years that planning has be- come an area for empirical study in research on teaching. Prior to this, those studying planning in education were primarily concerned with curriculum planning. The process of planning has been, however, a subject of serious study in areas such as economics, business, city planning, and national planning since the mid-1930's. This literature will be reviewed because of the volume of theory and research on plan- ning in these other areas and their influence on theories of educa- tional planning. The chapter begins with a discussion of the efforts to arrive at a definition of planning in areas outside of education and then pre- sents the definition of planning that will be used in this study. Se- cond, the planning literature in other areas is discussed, focusing on theory and research that have viewed planning as rational decision making. Third, the planning literature in education is reviewed and discussed. A Definition of Planning Interest in planning is as old as man's interest in relating knowledge to action. This can be traced back to Roman, Greek, and Middle Eastern cultures and might historically be found in the study 14 15 of government, law, administration, and public works. The systematic theory of the relation of knowledge and action has been traced back to the beginnings of urban civilization when a priesthood schooled in magic and astronomy sought to mediate between heaven and earth by ex- tending heaven's mandate into the affairs of men (Wheatly, 1971). The modern study of planning, however, has had more modest intentions (in most cases) and has been an object of theory and inquiry since the Great Depression. The myriad definitions of planning that have sprung up in the literature of planning will be discussed under two general headings: those approaches that define planning as design, and those that define planning as process. Planninggas Design The word plan_is derived from the Latin word planum, or level ground, and is commonly thought of as a drawing or diagram drawn up on a flat surface, such as a chart or blueprint. In this sense, the planner draws a blueprint, the design of which is completed before any steps are taken to realize its intentions. This is the kind of plan- ning characteristic of architecture and is the source of such defini- tions as, "Planning results in blueprints for future development; it recommends courses of action for achievement of desired goalS" (Bol- lens and Schmandt, 1965). Planning as Process Because of the tendency of deductive plans to decompose into smaller, more manageable parts or to distort reality when applied to complex, dynamic systems, those who study planning have become more interested in process than in states. Planning has come to pertain 16 not to some idealized future, but to the mode of moving from the pre- sent (Peterson, 1965). Thus, most planning theories of today are pro- cedural theories or theories of process. When viewed in this manner, planning can be generally defined as an activity centrally concerned with the linkage between knowledge and organized action (Friedmann and Hudson, 1974). One school of thought within planning theory characterizes the link between knowledge and action as rational decision making. This viewpoint is illustrated by the following definitions: Planning may be simply regarded as reason acting on a net- work of ongoing activities through the intervention of certain decision structures and processes (Friedmann, 1967). We define planning as a process for determining appropriate future action through a sequence of choices...Each of these choices requires the exercise of judgment; judgment permeates planning (Davidoff and Reiner, 1962). Planning is rational, adaptive thought applied to the future and to matters over which the planners or the administrative organizations which they are associated have some degree of control (Simon, Smithburg, and Thompson, 1950). Planning is more and more regarded as equivalent to rational social action, that is, as a social process for reaching a rational decision (Dahl, 1959). (Planning is) careful, deliberate, systematic and formal decision making (Argenti, 1974). Planning is the process of preparing a set of decisions for action in the future, directed at achieving goals by opti- mal means (Dror, 1963). A second school of thought has arisen in opposition to the "rational-comprehensive" (Lindblom, 1959) view of planning that the above definitions portray. This position has develOped largely as a result of the difficulties and inadequacies of the pure rational model for accurately describing planning behavior. This viewpoint will be 17 referred to in this chapter as "practical rationalism" and is illus- trated by the following definitions: (Planning is) pragmatic and piecemeal and never compre- hensive and complete, (and usually constitutes) compro- mise solutions of pressing practical issues (Myrdal, 1960). ...the planner may see himself as reasonably extending the domain of bounded rationality, as opportunity affords. If so, he is not a practical man, at least not enough to pride himself on it; he is not a rationalist, at least not enough to have rationalized too much of himself or others; perhaps he is a practical rationalist, or better, a rationalistic practitioner (Seely, 1962). The plan initiates a course of action which produces events experienced by the agent, in the light of which he modifies the plan; so that, in a sequence of phases, the plan is con- tinuously initiating action or being modified by the results of action; and this modification is not merely a more ef- ficacious employment of means to an originally intended end..., but also a modification of the end of view, a re- vision of intention, a recasting of desires, a development in understanding (Blackham, 1961). (Planners) might be better defined as practical artists, artists with reference to purpose, rational artists..., or artists of rationality with reference to human activity (Seeley, 1962). The main interest of this study is to examine the mental pro- cesses involved in teacher planning. Since there is little empirical evidence to support specific deliberation processes in planning, most of the preceeding definitions are inadequate as a starting point for this investigation. There are few psychological definitions of plan- ning available. One of the oldest and most well known definitions of a plan is that of Miller, Galanter, and Pribram (1960). "A plan is any hierarchical process in the organism that can control the order in which a sequence of operations is to be performed." Recently Bobrow (1975) has referred to planning as "a search for a series of actions to bring about a particular world-state." The definition that will be 18 initially used in this research study is similar to the definition of planning suggested bdee Groot (1965L For the purposes of this inves- tigation, planning is referred to as a process of preparing a frame- work for guiding future action. It can be seen from this definition that planning is regarded as a process strongly oriented towards future action. It is oriented towards action rather than, say, knowl- edge or self-development, and the fact that this action is in the future introduces the problem of uncertainty and unpredictability. It is assumed that the planning process involves decision making and judgment. At this time, however, no claim is made regarding the "ra- tionality" of either the process or the resulting framework. Planning Theory It is not the intention of this section to attempt a comprehen- sive review of the planning literature outside of education. This has been amply and effectively done by others.2 The purpose here is to discuss that part of planning theory that Friedmann and Hudson (1974) refer to as "the tradition of rationalism." This school of planning has had as its primary concern the problem of how decisions can be made more rationally. Until recently, this has been the predominant tradition in planning theory, so much so that, for many, planning has become synonymous with rational decision making. This tradition is of special interest to the study of teaching since it has been concerned with individual decision making in complex social situations such as economics, management, and city planning. 2For example, Dykman (1961) and Friedmann and Hudson (1974). 19 Rational decision models are rooted in the Utilitarian founda- tions of modern economics. Economics begins with the premise that people act rationally with a goal to optimize something. Historically, economics has been concerned with such things as optimum allocative efficiency, optimum product factor relationships, and Optimum degrees of specialization. Much of modern economic theory has been involved with the search for the rule that would locate the position of optimal decisions. The paradigm for decision making that has grown out of this work requires: 1. The setting of goals, 2. The formulation of alternatives, 3. The prediction of outcomes for each alternative, and 4. The evaluation of each alternative in relation to the goals and outcomes. This view of "economic man" was first challenged by Herbert Simon in his now classic book Administrative Behavior (1957 a). His think- ing was influenced by Chester Barnard's (1938) work and was chiefly concerned with the limits of rationality in human social behavior. Simon proposed a model of rational choice that differed from previous notions on two specific points: 1. While economic man maximizes — selects the best alter- native from among all those available to him; his cousin, whom we shall call administrative man, sacri- fices - looks for a course of action that is satis- factory or "good enough"... 2. Economic man deals with the "real world" in all its complexity. Administrative man recognizes that the world he perceives is a drastically simplified model of the buzzing, blooming confusion that constitutes the real world. He is content with this gross simpli— fication because he believes that the real world is mostly empty — that most of the facts of the real world have no great relevance to any particular 20 situation he is facing, and that most significant chains of causes and consequences are short and simple...He makes his choices using a simple picture of the situation that takes into account just a few of the factors that he regards as most relevant and crucial (Simon, 1957 a, pp. xxv-xxvi). Thus, Simon retained the general paradigm mentioned above with two major modifications. First, the criterion by which decision are made was changed. Rational man became a satisfier who makes choices without specifying and examining all the alternatives rather than a maximizer searching for an optimal or maximal solution. Second, the decision maker no longer had to deal with the real world in its totality and interrelatedness but could just deal with those "strategic factors" (Barnard, 1938) of a situation, enabling him to reduce the demands on his capacity for thought and use relatively simple rules of thumb. These ideas were further elaborated by Simon in a book on organization behavior co-authored by James March (March and Simon, 1959). Here, they elaborated the notions of "satisficing" and "bounded rationality." March and Simon identified planning as decision making but failed ade- quately to distinguish planning from decision making in general. The first practical application of Simon's model was undertaken by Jan Tinbergen (1952, 1964). He devised an econometric model for economic policy analysis that was used for short range planning in the Netherlands and for National Planning under the directions of the United Nations. While this model of planning was gaining wide support, several empirical studies focusing on the application of the rational model in city and national planning began to reveal some gross defi- ciencies of the method. Meyerson and Banfield's (1955) case study of public housing in Chicago found the realities of urban planning 21 practice to fall far short of the rational-comprehensive model of planning. Also, a large number of empirical studies of national plan- ning (e.g., Gross, 1965; Hirschmann, 1967; Faber and Sears, 1972) found little success in using a rational planning model to generate signifi- cant developmental actionflat the national level. The impact of these studies on rational planning theory has been summarized by Friedmann and Hudson (1974): Rationalism had largely ignored the experience of planning practice, as indeed it had ignored the work of organiza- tion development theories. And for good reason, for the recorded experience with planning threatened the very foundations of rationalist thinking. While rationalism argued a thin logic from the first principles to derive the basis for rational choice, empiricists labored to discover how "real" planning worked or did not work - and why. And what they discovered - if nothing else - was that planning-in-practice did not look at all like planning-in-theory (p. 12). Proponents of the rational planning method have had to come to grips with three major problems--reasons that planning-in-theory breaks down in practice. The first reason is the complexity and "tur- bulence" of most planning environments. Rational models were developed on simple problems or assumed that alternative courses of action were either given or obtainable and that the outcomes or consequences of each alternative were either specified or calculatable. Most of these assumptions are unrepresentative of social settings because of the complexity, uncertainty, and multidimensionality of social interaction. It rarely is possible for a decision maker to determine all possible courses of action (or even known when one has done so) or to predict the consequences of various alternative actions. The second reason rational planning theory breaks down in prac- tice involves the limits on knowledge available during planning. In 22 any given situation, there are limits to the information available to a decision maker. The complexity of most planning situations makes extraction of knowledge from the environment difficult. Even enormous data banks do not reduce the problem since they create further prob- lems of classification, search, and aggregation. The problem of knowledge is compounded by the difficulty of untangling means from ends and the difficulty of establishing values for various alterna- tives. The non-repetitiveness of many planning situations and changes brought on by the passage of time are additional factors that place limitations on knowledge available during planning. The third difficulty rational planning has failed to face is that of limited human information processing capabilities. Rational models require that decisions be based on optimal or maximal criteria that use as information probabilities and utility functions. Models such as Simon's reduce this demand somewhat, but still assume a comprehen- sive approach to the problem. This presents the difficulty of deter- mining goals at the outset and evaluating alternatives in light of goals and outcomes. In effect, what this implies is that decision makers when confronted with a decision will start from fundamentals anew each time and build from the ground up. These difficulties of the rational-comprehensive model of plan- ning were quickly recognized by several theorists. One of the earli- est and best known alternatives proposed was Lindblom's (1959) "suc- cessive limited comparisons" presented in a paper entitled "The Science of Muddling Through." He argues that, in practice, policy makers consider relatively few values and relatively few alternative policies and proceed in decision making by "building out of the 23 current situation, step-by—step and by small degrees" (Lindblom, 1959). Lindblom proposes that in most situations means-ends analysis is of limited value or even inappropriate, since the selection of goals and the empirical analysis of alternatives are often tightly intertwined. He argues that in deciding between two policies, a decision maker focuses his attention on the marginal or incremental values, being concerned only with those increments by which two alter- natives differ. Finally, Lindblom's method proceeds not by a compre- hensive analysis of the situation in hand, but by a chronological series of successive approximations towards a desired objective, that is, a series of "successive limited comparisons." Lindblom proposed a modification of this method in his book The Intelligence of Demo- gragy_(l965). Here he argues that the preferred social good is de- termined by an incremental decision process he refers to as "mutual adjustment." Lindblom was criticized for his failure to acknowledge the im- portance of structural decisions in planning and this criticism led to much debate about the relative importance of "big" and "little" de- cisions. This debate was put to rest by Etzioni's (1967, 1968) notion of "mixed scanning." Here effective decision making is based on a strategy that combines synoptic and incremental decision strategies. Etzioni proposes that effective decisions combine what he calls "con- textuating" decisions with "bit" decisions. Contextuating decisions are those fundamental decisions that are made through an exploration of the main alternatives of a situation omitting details so that an overview is pdssible. Bit decisions are made incrementally within the 24 contexts set by the fundamental decisions. The relationship that he suggests between the two strategies is that: 1. Most incremental decisions specify or anticipate fundamental decisions, and 2. The cumulative value of the incremental decisions is greatly affected by the underlying fundamental decisions (Etzioni, 1968, p. 289). There have been no published empirical studies of planners using either Lindblom's "successive limited comparisons" method or Etzioni's "mixed-scanning" strategy. Most of the empirical work in planning theory has been limited to case studies showing the inadequacies of rational-comprehensive planning methods. It is a purpose of this study to throw further light on these strategies, not by testing them speci- fically, but by describing planning decisions in an environment not unlike those that have been studied outside of education. Teacher planning probably contains elements common with the planning conducted by architects and designers, public and private administrators, policy analysts, and urban and city planners. Planning in Education Until recently, the literature on planning in education has been mainly prescriptive. Many volumes have been written recommending spe- cific principles for curriculum planning3 and most recent methods textbooks include at least one chapter on teacher planning. Within the last several years, researchers have become interested in how teachers plan in the classroom and have begun investigating the relation 3See, for example: Anderson (1956), Caswell and Campbell (1935), Gwynn (1943), Krug (1950), Sayior and Alexander (1974), and Varduin (1967). 25 between planning and classroom teaching and learning. Both of these areas of discourse and inquiry will be discussed in this section, be- ginning with the prescriptive planning literature and followed by the research literature on teacher planning. Prescriptive Planning Literature Most of the work in curriculum planning to date has focused on a model of curriculum planning first proposed by Tyler (1950) and later elaborated by Taba (1962) and Popham (Popham and Baker, 1970). This model recommends four essential steps for effective planning: 1. Specify objectives. 2. Select learning activities. 3. Organize learning activities. 4. Specify evaluation procedures. This model is basically a rational means-ends model in which a plan- ner's first task is to decide on the desired ends, or what is to be accomplished, and then select the appropriate learning activities to accomplish them. Curriculum planning is characterized as a task that requires orderly and careful thinking, and this model is proposed as a rational and scientific method for accomplishing this task (Taba, 1962). The central decisions in this model are choices concerning objec- tives and choices concerning content and activities. The formulation of clear and comprehensive objectives has been considered by many to be the "essential platform for the curriculum" (Taba, 1962) which may be the reason behind the extensive debate among proponents of various methods for stating instructional objectives (see, for example, 26 Popham, Eisner, Sullivan, and Tyler, 1969). Although the statement of objectives is advocated by nearly all curriculum theorists, the state- ment of objectives in behavioral form has dominated most planning models. Within this model, theorists seem to be in greatest disagree- ment in relation to the specificity and observability of the behavior included in the objective. Whereas earlier theorists portrayed the formulation of objectives in a more general, developmental, and fluid context, more recent proponents of behavioral objectives (e.g., Mager, 1962; Gagné, 1965) advocate a very specific, linear, and operationally oriented process of objective development. Choices concerning content and activities are becoming more impor- tant with the enormous increase in available materials and activities and with increased public interest in what is being taught in the schools. The major criterion for selection of activities that is im— plied in the rational model of curriculum planning is the degree to which various content and activities lead to the accomplishment of the desired objectives. Additional criteria for this decision have been proposed such as validity and significance of content, consistency with social realities, balance of breadth and depth, provision for wide range of objectives, learnability and adaptability to experiences of students, and appropriateness to the needs and interests of the stu- dents (Taba, 1962). These additional criteria are proposed as a screen through which each activity would be sifted, but how each of these po- tentially complicated judgments are to be made by the planner is not Specified. In general, Curriculum planning is currently viewed as an orderly, rational, deliberative process that can be most effectively 27 accomplished by using a rational, "ends-means" type of model (Zahorik, 1975). It is interesting that this type of model, which may be appro- priate for planning at the more general curricular level, is the same model that is most often prescribed for teacher planning at the class- room level. In most elementary methods textbooks, planning for teaching is defined within the following framework: A plan for teaching, like a plan for a house, for losing twenty pounds of weight, for winning the conference championship, or for a vacation serves as a guide to selecting activities, actions, and decisions as one attempts to move from where he is to where he wants to be - to his goal(s)...The plan developed by the teacher is his best estimate of the most direct route to reach educational objectives and is made prior to embarking on a lesson (Collier, Houston, Schmatz, and Walsh, 1967, p. 115). Although various types of long term and short term planning are usu- ally discussed in textbooks, the process of planning proposed always takes the form of (1) state objectives, (2) select and organize the materials, procedures, and techniques to be used to achieve the objec- tives, and (3) specify evaluation procedures to determine whether ob- jectives have been attained. When discussing planning for instruc- tional units, Beauchamp (1965) presents four plans developed by others for organizing a unit, all of which contain the above components. Peterson (1964) presents the same procedures for planning specific lessons. The only departure from this rational model of teacher planning that has been advocated is the "integrated ends-means model" (Zahorik, 1975) suggested by McDonald (McDonald, 1965; McDonald, WOlfson, and Zaret, 1973) and Eisner (1967). They propose that teachers do not 28 begin their planning by thinking about objectives and then proceeding to decisions about activities, evaluation, and so forth; rather, teachers first focus on the type of learning activity that will be pro- vided for the students. They argue that objectives arise and exist only in the context of an activity, as a result of students' choosing their own learning experiences and pursuing their own objectives. Thus, in this model, ends for learning become integrated with means for learning and the specification of goals prior to an activity becomes meaningless. With the exceptions of McDonald and Eisner, the steps that are ad- vocated for rational curriculum planning and rational instructional planning are the same. The same model has been proposed for decision making from the most comprehensive curriculum planning project down to planning for tomorrow's social studies lesson. There has been little attention paid to the process other than its proper sequence, result- ing in few guidelines for making the complex judgments and decisions at each point in the model. In effect, what appears to have happened is that education has borrowed the rational ends-means decision making model from other fields and has prescribed this procedure as the most effective and efficient method for all_educational planning. Because of the improved efficiency that this model has brought to curriculum planning, fiscal planning, and so forth, educators have not questioned the appropriateness of this procedure for classroom planning. The question of whether teachers d2 plan in this manner has been over- looked, along with the more important question of whether teachers gap. or should plan in this manner. 29 The answers to these questions have immediate and practical impor- tance since the rational planning model is accepted by most teacher educators. It has become part of most materials directed at improving teacher education and has become central to most materials directed at improving teacher planning skills (e.g., Joyce, egial,, 1971; Taba, 1966). Morine (1973) has aptly summarized the state of current train- ing for classroom planning: Teacher educators may agree that planning is an essential part of teaching effectiveness. Judging from the scar- city of training materials, it is questionable whether the educational world at large concurs with this view. Train- ing materials for planning skills begin and end with diag- nosis and the writing of behavioral objectives...The ma- terials may be systematic, but they are hardly comprehen- sive (p. 135). Teacher Planning Research Although researchers such as Philip Jackson have long pointed to the importance of looking at teacher behavior in the preactive setting, relatively few studies have ventured into this domain. Empirical studies of teacher planning have only been conducted since 1970, and, to date, the published studies can still be counted on one hand. There are basically two types of research on teacher planning. The first type has focused on testing the adequacy of the rational plan- ning model for describing what teachers do and has examined the effect of using this model on teacher classroom behavior. The second type of study has attempted to describe how teachers actually plan, free from the constraints of any recommended procedure. Zahorik (1970) did the first empirical study of classroom plan- ning when he examined the effect of structured planning on teacher classroom behavior. He provided six of his sample of twelve teachers \ 30 with a partial lesson plan containing behavioral objectives and a de- tailed outline of content to be covered two weeks hence. He requested the remaining six teachers to reserve an hour of instructional time to carry out a task for the researchers, not telling them that they were going to be asked to teach a lesson on credit cards until just before the appointed time. Zahorik analyzed recorded protocols of the twelve lessons focusing on "teacher behavior that is sensitive to students." He defined this behavior as "verbal acts of the teacher that permit, encourage, and develop pupils' ideas, thoughts, and actions." Upon examining the protocols of the planners and non-planners, Zahorik noted that teachers who planned exhibited less honest or authentic use of the pupils' ideas during the lesson. He concluded from this that "the typical planning model--goals, activities, and their organization, and evaluation--result in insensitivity to pupils on the part of the teacher." These results must be interpreted cautiously because of the size of the sample, the length of observation, and the lack of control for or description of the content taught or of the teacher's normal teaching style. The study does, however, raise interesting questions about the effect of a teacher's planning method on the way one reacts in the classroom. Zahorik (1975) continued this line of inquiry by examining the use of behavioral objectives and the "separate means-ends" model of planning as well as the use of the "integrated means-ends" model pro- posed by McDonald and Eisner. He asked 194 teachers (primarily grades K-12) to list in writing the decisions that they make prior to teach- ing and the order in which they make them. He classified these de- cisions into the following categories: objectives, content, 31 activities, materials, diagnosis, evaluation, instruction, and organi- zation. He found that the kind of decision used by the greatest num- ber of teachers concerned pupil activities (indicated by 81% of the teachers). The decision most frequently made first was content (51%) followed at a distant second by objectives (28%). Zahorik concluded from this study that teacher planning decisions do not always follow logically from a specification of objectives, and that, in fact, objectives are not a particularly important planning de- cision in terms of quantity of use. He also argued, however, that the integrated ends-means model does not appear to be a functioning real- ity because of the relatively few teachers (only 3%) who began their planning by making decisions about activities. Several things should be kept in mind when interpreting the re- sults of this study. First, the data are based on teacher self- reports. Second, there is no indication of what Zahorik meant by planning decisions or how the teachers interpreted its meaning. Third, there is no evidence that the classification system used by Zahorik reflects the types of decisions actually made by teachers. For example, it may not be true that decisions concerning activities and decisions concerning content are easily separated in the teacher's mind. It may be that content is an essential component of a learning activity and if the content is changed the nature of the activity is changed. Only recently has research on teacher planning begun focusing on describing teacher decision making in actual planning situations. Peterson, Marx, and Clark (1977) examined planning in a laboratory situation as twelve teachers prepared to teach an unfamiliar 32 instructional unit to a group of eight students (also new to the teachers). During the planning period, teachers were instructed to "think aloud" and their verbal protocols were later coded by means of decision categories such as objectives, materials, subject matter, and process. As in Zahorik's (1975) study, there was no indication whether these categories accurately captured the teachers' actual perceptions of the planning situation. The following results were obtained from this study based on protocols from the planning periods prior to three days of teaching (fifty-minute periods): (1) teachers spent the largest prOportion of their planning time dealing with the content (subject matter) to be taught, (2) after subject matter, teachers con- centrated their planning efforts on instructional processes (strate- gies and activities), and (3) the smallest proportion of their plan- ning time was spent on objectives. All three of these findings were consistent with those by Zahorik (1975) and Goodlad, Klein, and others (1974). The third finding is also similar to results reported by Joyce and Hartoonian (1965) and by Popham and Baker (1970). Also reported in this study were results suggesting individual differences in cognitive processing styles and abilities in planning that were relatively stable during the experimental period. A major weakness of the study is that it was conducted in a labo- ratory situation, with students and materials that the teachers were dealing with for the first time, and planning was only observed for ninety minutes prior to the lesson. Also, planning was only observed over a three-day period. It is possible that this type of task may offer little information about how planning might proceed when the teacher is in his or her own classroom, with familiar students, 33 materials, and resources. Peterson, Marx, and Clark did, however, point to the richness of descriptive data that may be obtained by observing actual planning and also suggested the usefulness of thinking aloud as a method to elucidate the teacher's thought processes. Morine (1976) avoided some of these problems by studying planning in a semi-controlled classroom setting and in a simulated planning situation. Teacher written plans were collected for two experimenter- prescribed lessons (one in mathematics and one in reading) and ana— 1yzed according to (l) specificity of written plans, (2) general for- mat of plans, (3) statement of goals, (4) source of goal statements, (5) attention to pupil background and preparation, (6) identification of evaluation procedures, and (7) indication of possible alternative procedures. Morine found that teachers tended to be fairly specific and use outline forms in their plans, yet paid little attention to behavioral goals, diagnosis of student needs, evaluation procedures, and alternative courses of action. In the simulated setting, Morine had teachers plan a reading pro- gram for fourteen new students. The task was designed to identify the kind of information teachers consider important for planning a program for a school year. Information was available from cumulative records for each student, and the resulting plans were analyzed according to the types of information about pupils that teachers requested, group- ing procedures used, and the differential use of materials and support services. Morine found that a majority of the teachers indicated that there were major differences between the way that the simulation was constructed and the way that they normally planned. The major dif- ference reported was that they normally avoided so much preliminary 34 information about students or try to get first hand information (e.g., give their own tests). Morine found that as a group the teachers tended to ask for the same kind of information, were fairly accurate in identifying the pupils' reading levels, and differed little in grouping practices and use of support services. Currently, Morine and Joyce (1976) are involved in.a study inves- tigating, among other things, the preactive planning of a group of teachers at different times during the school year using four different methods of data collection. Planning will be examined using thinking aloud technique, a post teaching interview, and structured and un- structured written lesson plans. Although decision making has been a concern of nearly all the em- pirical studies that have been discussed thus far, they are all char- acterized by the following weakness. None of the studies has examined teacher planning in the classroom over long periods of time. Recent investigators such as Morine and Joyce have begun examining planning in naturalistic settings, but their observation and description have been limited to one or two days per month for each teacher. To date, no one has taken on the task of describing teacher planning in detail over an extended time period. Moreover, there has been no attempt to describe the process of teacher planning decisions as they occur. Typically, an analysis has been made from the product of teacher plan- ning, that is, lesson plans. Peterson, Marx, and Clark (1977) have conducted the only study of teacher planning as it happens and then only for three ninety-minute periods. Finally, although Zahorik has compared teacher planning to prescribed models of teacher planning, no 35 one has attempted to develop a model of teacher planning from actual planning behavior in a naturalistic setting. It is because of these weaknesses in research on teacher plan- ning and decision making that the present study has been undertaken. Teacher planning was examined as it naturally occurred in the class- room setting over a period of several months. The resulting behavior was described in detail and models of teacher planning were developed. CHAPTER III METHOD Scope of the Study The primary objective of this study was to describe those mental processes involved in teacher planning decisions made prior to teach- ing. This objective was addressed within the context of a case study focusing on a detailed examination and description of one elementary teacher's planning decisions during a five month period of instruc- tion. The central thrust of the study was descriptive with a major purpose of providing a "thick—description" of the teacher and the classroom setting in which she functions. There is no attempt in this study to examine the relationship between teacher planning and either classroom interaction or student outcomes. These process- product relationships (Dunkin and Biddle, 1974) were ignored because it was felt that before the effects of planning decisions on classroom interaction and student learning can be understood, we need to de- scribe and understand what actually occurs during the planning pro— cess. Although major interest was in describing planning behavior as it naturally occurs, teacher planning was also examined in a simulated setting in which certain aspects of the teacher's present environment could be manipulated. The study began in January, 1977, and was concluded in June, 1977. During the first three months of the study, the investigator was pre- sent in the classroom on the average of four days per week. During 36 37 these days he observed the teacher from 8:30 in the morning until she left school at 4:45 in the afternoon. The fourth month of the study involved only occasional visits to the classroom while the data were being analyzed and while the theory and models were being developed. The fifth month of the study involved several more weeks of observation (usually half days) and the teacher's participation in the planning simulation. Selecting the Teacher Before the study began, four criteria were established to guide the search for a participant in this study. First, an experienced teacher was sought so that planning could be observed that had proved workable and effective for the teacher over a period of several years. Second, a teacher who did a great deal of overt planning was desired, since it would be easier in an initial study of teacher planning to begin with a teacher whose planning activities were more readily ob- servable. The third criteria was to look for a teacher who was re- garded as being successful by other professionals--a "good" teacher. This was thought to be important since good planning was of primary interest at this time and would be of more value if one wanted to make prescriptions to other teachers' planning behavior. Finally, a self- contained classroom was sought so that the teacher would not be shar- ing planning responsibilities with other teachers. In the first week of January, several potential teachers were identified through the recommendations of teachers currently working at the Institute for Research on Teaching. Two teachers were con- tacted and appointments were arranged to discuss the study. The first 38 teacher expressed interest in participating in the study and agreed that a final decision on her participation would be contingent on both the investigator's and her willingness to proceed with the study after three days of classroom observation and interaction. The second teacher turned out to be in a team-teaching situation, and it was de- cided to make a decision about the first teacher before contacting others. After the observation period, the teacher agreed to participate in the study, and it became apparent to the researcher during this time that she met all of the specified criteria for a participant in the research. The teacher was teaching in a combined first and second grade classroom in a local school district. She had had six years of teaching experience, three years in a special education classroom and three years in a first and second grade "split" classroom in the pre- sent building. She appeared to be a very organized and creative teacher who spent much time in planning activities and was highly re- garded by her colleagues. She was in her early thirties and had earned a bachelor's degree in social work and a master's degree in special education prior to teaching. Naturalistic Data Collection A central purpose of this study was to examine and describe plan- ning decisions as they were made in the classroom setting. Two meth— ods of inquiry were chosen to accomplish this. The first method, pro- cess tracing, is an approach to describing decision making that has proven successful in relatively simple laboratory and field settings but has never been tested in a complex naturalistic setting. The 39 second method was micro—ethnography (Smith and Geoffrey, 1968) which has been widely used to examine the social environment of groups but has only once been used as a tool to examine teacher decision making. Each of these methods will be discussed in turn. An Information Processing Approach While the major purpose of this study is to describe the mental processes involved in teacher planning decisions, it is also concerned with examining the usefulness of a particular mode of inquiry that has proven successful in studies of decision making in other fields. This approach, called process tracing, begins with a complex representation of decision making in the form of verbal protocols and attempts to simplify the processes by representing the decisions in the form of decision trees or flow diagrams. Most researchers using this methodology subscribe to the notion that humans are adaptive, goal oriented systems. Simon (1969), elabo- rating on this view, states that: A man, viewed as a behaving system, is quite simple. The apparent complexity of his behavior over time is largely a reflection of the complexity of the environment in which finds himself (p. 25). It is this view of "thinking man" that provides both the theoretical and methodological foundation for this study of teacher decision mak- ing. Viewing man as a relatively simple, adaptive system has certain implications for the ways in which human information processing should be studied. Newell (1973) has suggested that to predict a person's behavior, one must have information about (1) his goals, (2) the struc- ture of the task environment in which he is functioning, and (3) the 4O invariant structure of his processing mechanisms. Using this informa- tion, one can fairly accurately predict what "methods" are available to the subject, and from the method one can predict what the subject will do. In order to utilize Newell's formula, the researcher must take pains to describe the subject's goals and the structure of the task environment for each new situation. One assumes, however, that there are certain invariant structures of the human processing mechanism across situations, and that only a gross knowledge of these processes is needed. Simon (1969) has summarized these processes as follows: The evidence is overwhelming that the system is basically serial in its operation: that it can produce only a few symbols at a time and that the symbols being processed must be held in special, limited memory structures whose content can be changed rapidly. The most striking limits on subject's capacities to employ efficient strategies arise from the very small capacity of the short-term memory structure (seven chunks4) and from the relatively long time (five seconds) required to transfer a chunk of information from short-term memory to long term memory (p. 53). The process tracing approach has proven manageable and effective for understanding behavior during relatively simple tasks such as cryptarithmetic or chess problems (de Groot, 1965; Newell and Simon, 1972). In these situations both the goals of the subject and the task environment are well specified, and the researchers can focus their efforts on determining the "methods" or "programs" that the subject is using to solve the problems. The investigator's job becomes more difficult, however, as the task environment increases in complexity. 4This estimate was based on Miller's (1956) estimate of short- term memory (STM) of approximately seven chunks, plus or minus two chunks. More recent work on STM (Craik, 1971) has lowered this esti- mate to three chunks, plus or minus one. 41 This has been shown to be true in studies of physicians' diagnostic problem solving (Elstein, eg_al,, in press) and in attempts to describe the decision making of bank trust investment officers in naturalistic settings (Clarkson, 1962). Elstein and his colleagues set out to describe the cognitive pro- cesses of physicians from their initial encounter with a patient to the final diagnostic decision. Physicians were confronted with simu- lated patients in a simulated office situation,and data were collected on their "thinking aloud" during the encounter as well as their com- ments during simulated recall (Bloom, 1954; Kagan, et_al,, 1967) of the session. By repeating the same simulations on twenty-four physicians, the major features of the task environment became well understood. The protocols and stimulated recall provided information about the physicians' goals and intentions. Thus, they were able to complete Newell's formula and their study provided interesting and unexpected information about the methods that the physicians were using to solve these problems. Clarkson examined the decisions of a bank trust investment offi- cer as he selected investment portfolios during a period of several months. He used a variety of data collection methods, illustrating "the slow, painstaking, and frequently artful manner in which 'think- ing aloud' and other process data are collected and transformed into information processing models" (Shulman and Elstein, 1975). He began his study by interviewing the bank's trust investment officers and by attending departmental meetings that reveiwed past and future trust investment decisions. He then focused on the deliberations of one investment officer, examining the history of several accounts and 42 attempting to develop "naive behavioral models" of those decision pro- cesses that seemed to be invariant across accounts. In an attempt to test these models, "think aloud" protocols were made of the trust officer's successive deliberations during the course of his work. Inspection of these protocols indicated that many of the decisions pertaining to expectations and valuation of stocks, indus- tries, etc., were made prior to portfolio selection. To discover how these prior decision were made, protocols were gathered while the trust officer read and commented on articles from financial journals and on analysts' reports to which he subscribed. From these protocols and those collected earlier, a model was constructed of the trust offi- cer's decision processes, eventually being developed into a computer simulation. Over a validation period of six months the computer simulation successfully matched the predictions of the trust officer, and appeared to be using approximately the same deliberative processes as used by the human decision maker. These two studies illustrate well the information processing ap- proach to examining decision making. Here the subject is viewed as a processor of information capable of being simulated by a sequential decision making model. There are a number of benefits from this in- formation approach: (1) it provides a formal language for dealing with complex problems, and as such, (2) it requires a precise formula- tion of the theory, and (3) it allows for a direct test of the model by running the program and comparing its output and processes with that of a human decision maker performing the same task (Wortman, 1972). 43 The application of information processing methodology to the de- scription of teacher decision making processes promises to be a chal- lenging and formidable task. The environment in which teachers, es- pecially elementary teachers, daily operate is many times more complex than that of the physician or trust investment officer. This is true largely because of the fact that teachers deal with many people at one time and are in contact with them in a variety of different settings every day, over long periods of time. One way to address the complexity of the teacher's environment is to characterize it as being composed of many different task environ- ments in a process of constant fluctuation and change. According to Simon's earlier statements, it makes sense to think of the teacher as being effective in certain situations and with certain tasks rather than as the possesser of global or generic traits and abilities. In other words, the appropriate question to be asked in research on teaching is not "What is an effective teacher?" but rather "In what situations is a teacher effective?" If one wishes to use Newell's "formula" to describe and predict teacher behavior, a large amount of effort must be devoted to describ- ing and characterizing the task environment and the teacher's goals for decision making. This is one reason that this study takes the form of an in-depth process tracing of one teacher over a long period of time. As mentioned earlier, the goal of information processing ap- proaches such as process tracing is to describe the processes or meth- ods by which problems are solved and decisions are made. To obtain this information, the researchers have generally relied on introspec- tive reports gathered as participants think aloud while performing a 44 task. These protocols are then formalized into models representing the mental processes used by the participants while engaged in the task. A primary source of data for this study is the teacher's verbali- zations of her thought processes during instructional planning. There is much debate in psychology about how to deal with verbal data. This study takes the same approach to the issue as do Newell and Simon (1972): The prOtocol is a record of the subject's ongoing behav- ior, and an utterance of time t is taken to indicate knowl- edge or operation at time t. Retrospective accounts leave much more opportunity for the subject to mix current knowl- edge with past knowledge, making reliable inference from the protocol difficult. Nor, in the thinking-aloud proto- col, is the subject asked to theorize about his own behav- ior - only to report the information and intentions that are within his current sphere of conscious awareness. All theorizing about the causes and consequences of the sub- ject's knowledge state is carried out and validated by the experimenters, not the subject (p. 184). In this study, the teacher was requested to think aloud through out most of her preactive teaching activities. For those deliberative planning periods such as writing down next week's schedule in her plan book or planning for a new instructional unit, her thinking aloud was recorded on audio tape. During other preactive activities, a written description was kept on her behavior, making special note of verbali- zations related to planning decisions. An Ethnograhic Approach A second and equally important set of data collected during this study was detailed written descriptions of the teacher's behavior in both the preactive and interactive phases of teaching which provided 45 detailed descriptions of the environmental "context" in which plan- ning decisions were made. One reason for using this approach is that research on teaching has provided few in-depth descriptions of actual teaching behavior in classroom settings. The exceptions to this are the ethnographic studies done by participant observers who have spent long periods of time examining one educational setting (e.g., Cusick, 1973; Smith and Geoffrey, 1968). These studies, along with less extensive observations by researchers such as Jackson (1968), have provided valuable knowl- edge and insights into the nature of teaching that has been for the most part overlooked in the short and infrequent forays into the classroom that characterize research on teaching. Given this dearth of information about life in classrooms and the promise of the descriptive—ethnographic studies, it seemed that to describe adequate- ly and understand teacher decision making, one would want to spend an extended period of time in the classroom observing the day to day activities of teaching. Ethnographic descriptions of teaching in this study were col- lected as the investigator functioned as a "participant observer" in the classroom. "Participant" does not mean here that the investigator took on the role of either the teacher or a student, but that he par- ticipated in the social structure of the classroom developing signifi- cant relationships with the teacher and students. The observer's role most frequently took the form of sitting quietly at a spot in the classroom offering full view of all activities, taking written notes and recording as much of the action of the classroom (focusing on the teacher) as possible. Interactions with students were limited 46 primarily to friendly and informal conversations and to occasional questions about what was being written down by the observer. At times when the students were not in the classroom, the investigator func- tioned as the teacher's "shadow," following the teacher throughout the day and recording her behaviors and statements. During these times, the teacher was engaged in an on-going "thinking aloud" process where she attempted to verbalize her thoughts regarding the activities in whcih she was involved. Notes were kept throughout this process and questions were often asked to clarify or elaborate her statements. During more deliberative instructional planning sessions, thinking aloud was also tape recorded. By using these approaches, a detailed written description of the teacher's behavior was obtained that por- trayed her planning decisions within the context of days, weeks, and months of teaching. Another advantage of the ethnographic method is the detailed de- scription it provides of the teaching environment. The importance of carefully describing the environment in which behavior occurs has been pointed out by both the ecological and information processing psycho- 1ogists. As mentioned in Chapter I, the ecological approach to exam- ining teaching underlines the subtle and complex interdependencies between behavior and the environment. Their notion that settings es- tablish limits on the response Options of participants suggests that an understanding of behavior in any given situation will require an understanding of the environment in which it occurs. This view is supported by information processing theory and has been most widely discussed by Newell and Simon. In their theory of thinking, they set forth the following propositions: 47 1. A few, and only a few, gross characteristics of the information processing system (IPS) are invariant over task and problem solver. 2. These characteristics are sufficient to determine that a task environment is represented (in the IPS) as a problem space, and that problem solving takes place in a problem space. 3. The structure of the task environment determines the possible structures of the problem space. 4. The structure of the problem space determines the possible programs that can be used for problem solving (Newell and Simon, 1972, pp. 778-779). The third and fourth propositions of this theory suggest the important role of the task environment in shaping decision making. They suggest that the task itself must be considered a major determinant of human behavior, as Opposed to attributing behavior to a stable trait or ability of the decision maker across situations. Thus, if decision making is largely a function of the task envi- ronment, a crucial task of research becomes that of describing the situation in which the behavior occurs. These descriptions contribute to an understanding of information processing by making possible an understanding Of the task requirements which, in turn, yields a better understanding of the person who performs in a more-or-less successful manner (Dawes, 1974). Summary of the Naturalistic Data Collection Two strategies of naturalistic data collection were used in this study. A process tracing approach was used to collect detailed infor- mation about the teacher's specific planning decisions with an empha- sis on describing the decision process. In addition, a micro-ethno- graphic approach was used to describe the behavior and context of 48 teaching with a goal of providing a background from which to better understand the specific planning decisons. The Teacher Planning Shell During the course of this investigation, teacher planning behavior was Observed in several artificial situations. The first Of these situations that will be discussed has been named the Teacher Planning Shell. The Teacher Planning Shell is a moderate fidelity simulation resembling an In-Basket, although differing in one major sense. Clas- sical teacher In-Baskets (Shulman, 1965; Shulman, e£_al,, 1968) gener- ally represent total task simulations. In these simulations, the participant is usually confronted with a familiar task in an unfamil- iar situation. One of the major tasks confronting the participant is to familiarize oneself with the new environment and then manipulate it in some manner to achieve the desired goal(s). A weakness of these simulations is that it is nearly impossible to duplicate the informa- tion that would be internally available in real situations as a re- sult of previous experience. (Efforts have been made to provide re- ference memories representing this experience, but the process of re- trieving this information is often cumbersome and time-consuming.) The Teacher Planning Shell attempts to circumvent these problems by providing a simulated situation in which a participant can use his or her current knowledge about most aspects of the task environment. This is achieved by manipulating one or two components of the teach- er's environment while leaving the rest undisturbed. For instance, a teacher may be asked to plan lessons for a new curriculum component that would be implemented in his present classroom with his current 49 students at the present time of year. Although situation factors are not standardized and controlled as in classical In-Baskets, informa- tion on these factors can be collected by a questionnaire administered prior to the planning task. This approach has several advantages over total-task simulations. First, the Teacher Planning Shell provides a format for planning in a semi-controlled setting that has greater task validity. By allowing the teacher to remain, for the most part, in the present teaching situation, the psychological fidelity of the task should also be in- creased. Second, the Teacher Planning Shell should be less difficult than most In-Baskets, in that there is less learning and searching for information that must occur prior to planning. Third, the "shell" format provides for flexibility by allowing different kinds of plan- ning tasks and potential problems to be presented to the teacher. For the purpose of this study, five different problems were de- veloped that would be appropriate for use in late spring. Four of the problems were used with the teacher in this study. The instructions and problems for the Teacher Planning Shell are presented in Appendix A along with the Teacher Planning Shell Questionnaire administered prior to the simulation. The Planning Shell was primarily designed for this study, but is constructed so that it may be used as a tool to examine teacher plan- ning decisions outside the framework of an in-depth case study. Since an essential element of the Teacher Planning Shell is that the parti- cipant deal with the problems at the current time of the year, addi- tional problems are being developed that would be appropriate for be- ginning of the year, fall, and winter term simulations. 50 In this study, the Teacher Planning Shell was used primarily as a tool to examine teacher planning in situations that had not been na- turally observed during the Observation period. The teacher was in- structed to think aloud during the task and the verbalizations were recorded on audio tape. Since the Teacher Planning Shell was adminis- tered late in the study, the data were used to modify or confirm hy- potheses and models that had been developed from earlier descriptions. Judgment Tasks During the third and fourth months of the study, the teacher was asked to participate in several small scale experiments that were de— signed to examine more closely the teacher's thinking about students and activities by providing information about the characteristics or cues to which the teacher paid attention. Initially, only two tasks were planned, a pupil sort task and an activity sort task. As a re- sult of the teacher's interest in doing a sociogram on the class, a third task was added to provide additional information about teacher perceptions of students. Pupil Sort This task consisted of presenting to the teacher names of her students written on separate index cards and instructing her to group the pupils according to any dimension or characteristic that she could think of. No guidance was given as to what characteristics to use nor about the number of groups. The task lasted approximately forty-five minutes and the teacher was instructed to think aloud during the task. If the teacher had not done so during the task, she was requested at the end of each sort to explain how the groups differed from each 51 other and how the children in each group were similar. Within the forty-five minute period, twenty-two different sorts were accomplished. A list of the sort dimensions used is presented in Appendix B. The protocols were examined to determine the number of dimensions used by examining the similarity of group composition in the various sorts. Activity Sort The activity sort used the same procedure as the pupil sort using the names of the fifty-one instructional activities used in the class- room that had been identified by classroom observation. During the forty-five minutes set aside for this task, twenty dimensions were used as a basis for sorting. These dimensions as well as the fifty-one activities are presented in Appendix B. The analysis procedure was the same as that used in the pupil sort task. Sociogpam Analysis During the third month of the study, the teacher expressed in- terest in doing a sociogram of the class. She had informally done this before and had found it to be an interesting source of information about social relations in the classroom. The investigator offered to construct the sociograms if she would decide on the questions that she would ask the students and be willing to discuss her predictions about the outcome of each sociogram prior to seeing the results. To this she agreed, and a period was set aside during class to collect the students' responses to each of the twenty questions that she had constructed. Over the weekend, the investigator constructed a socio- gram for each of the questions. During the next week, these were pre- sented to the teacher and her reactions recorded. Prior to showing 52 each sociogram to the teacher, the question was read and she predicted how the diagram would look. While she examined each sociogram, her reactions and the accuracy of predictions were discussed. These data were used to supplement and elaborate the information on her thinking about pupils collected in the pupil sort task. (The questions for which the sociograms were developed may be found in Appendix B.) Analysis Basically, two types Of data were generated and analyzed in this study. During the first three months of the study detailed field notes were taken during both the preactive and interactive teaching activities that occurred on the Observation days. In addition, de- tailed notes or audio recordings were made during the teacher's plan- ning and during her participation in the Teacher Planning Shell and the judgment tasks. The field notes were taken with the purpose of providing a run- ning account of the teacher's behavior. As incidents occurred, as many features of the behavior and situation were recorded as possible. Attempts were made to record what was said, who said it, the nature and location of the activity, the participants, the noise level, tone of voice, posture, facial expression, and so forth. Obviously, in quick interchanges and fast moving activity sequences, certain things were missed. The strategy used to sort out complex situations was to focus on the teacher's behavior, only recording students' behavior as they interacted with her. The analysis of the field notes proceeded in the following manner. At the end of each observation day, the field notes were recorded onto 53 cassette tapes to be later transcribed. Putting the field notes into a form that could be easily transcribed served as a review of the day's activities and provided a further stimulus to thinking about the teacher's planning in relation to classroom activities. When in typed form, the field notes were reread with the purpose of looking for broad patterns of behavior in the interactive setting that seemed re- lated to planning decisions. As the study progressed, the field notes became the background for interpreting planning behavior, since they illustrated the various factors that seemed to be influencing the plan- ning of classroom activities as well as shedding light on the factors that effected the implementation of the activities in the interactive setting. The notes and tape recordings of the naturalistic and simulated planning activities were analyzed in a manner similar to that used with the field notes. Because of the difficulty of transcribing the audio tapes, they were analyzed by repeatedly listening to the deci- sion protocols and summarizing their content, making special note of decision components and processes. The variety and complexity of the different planning situations precluded an analysis of the protocols at a level similar to those used in previous process tracing analyses, but a model of the planning process was constructed that reflected the process at a more meaningful level. (This point will be discussed in more detail in Chapter IX.) The data analysis and model development proceeded by alternation between data collection and conceptualization. The general procedure followed the steps in qualitative analysis initially advocated by Becker (1958) and used by Smith and Geoffrey (1968). They include: 54 1. Selection and definition of problems, concepts, and indices. 2. Checking the frequency and distribution of phenomena. 3. Construction of social system models. 4. Final analysis and presentation of results (Smith and Geoffrey, 1968, p. 14). Time became an important tool in the analysis. Concepts, methods, and processes gradually surfaced in the data as a result of spending ex- tended amounts of time observing and describing the teacher's decision behavior. As process elements became apparent, they were formulated into working hypotheses to be examined in future situations as well as in previous field notes. As models were further developed, they were discussed with colleagues at the Institute, many of whom were or had been classroom teachers. Thus, over time, concepts were defined and tested against classroom observations and a process model of teacher planning gradually took form. Chapter Summary The primary purpose of this study was to investigate teacher planning by a detailed examination and description Of the decision pro- .cess involved in one elementary teacher's planning decisions during a five month period of instruction. Data were collected by observation and recording of the teacher's activities in both the preactive and interactive phases of teaching and by recording the teacher's thinking aloud during planning in naturalistic situations in a new simulation task called the Teacher Planning Shell and in three judgment tasks. Data were analyzed by examining field notes and taped protocols as the study proceeded, and by generating and testing concepts and hypotheses 55 through interaction with previous data and ongoing teacher behavior. As a result, descriptions of the teacher's planning were developed along with models of the planning process. These descriptions and models will be elaborated and discussed in the following chapters. CHAPTER IV THE SETTING The purpose of this chapter is to describe the character of the setting in which this study was conducted. The analogy that will be used is that Of a play. Ecological psychologists have drawn on this analogy when they talk about behavior settings and props. The picture of the teaching environment that will be painted here will necessarily lack the richness and detail of the real setting, but like the oversim- plified and Often stylized stage backdrop, it will contain enough of the features of the real setting to convey the proper context. And, as such, it may serve to enhance the visual appreciation of the set- ting, and to facilitate the audience's perception and understanding of the action. The chapter will follow a dramaturgical or theatrical outline. It begins by describing the stage and set on which teaching occurs. Next the characters and the major props in the classroom are discussed. Subsequent to that, a typical plot is unfolded followed by a more general discussion of the daily drama in the classroom. The final section discusses the set and props for planning. 56 57 The Stage: Byron School Byron School5 is located in a medium sized community adjacent to a large midwestern university. It is one of nine elementary schools in a rather progressive school district. The surrounding neighborhood is primarily made up of single family dwellings along with some rental property. The neighborhood is mostly upper middle class and many of its inhabitants are faculty, staff, or students at the university. Because of these university affiliations, the neighborhood is approxi- mately 92% white with 8% minority inhabitants. There is a very active and influential community organization in the area that is concerned with any city policy that would affect the neighborhood. Recently, they have been involved in issues such as building and zoning codes, parking and traffic control, and rental housing standards. The school also has a very active Parent Teacher Organization that sponsors work days several times a year to clear up the school grounds and activities like flea markets to raise money for the school. Community participation is very prominent in the life of the school, and there are always many more volunteers than needed for parent aides or room mothers. Byron School was built in 1922 and is the second oldest building in the district. The building and playground take up a square city block, bounded on the south and west sides by primarily student rental housing and on the nOrth and east sides by single family dwellings. The building is a three story, brick structure with a newer wing (built in 1946) that contains the lower grade classrooms. There are 5The names of the school, teacher, and students have all been changed to insure anonymity. 58 twelve classrooms in the building, eight of which are currently being used for general instruction. The other four classrooms function as a music room, art room, reading lab, and science room. The school also contains, in the older part of the building, a library and a gymnasium that also serves as an auditorium. The kindergarten, first grade, first-second grade, and second-third grade classrooms are on the first floor in the newer wing of the building. The third grade, fourth grade, and the two fourth-fifth grade classrooms are on the second and third floors of the building. A floor plan of the building is pre- sented in Figure 4.1. The building uses steam heating that keeps the newer wing much too hot and the second and third floors much too cold throughout the winter. The older classrooms are characterized by very high ceilings and room for litte more than the teacher's and students' desks. The rooms in the newer wing are much larger than the older classrooms. In this wing there are two rooms on each side of the hall connected by a workroom containing restrooms, the teachers' desks, and storage space. The Set and Props for Learning: Mrs. Lisa's Classroom Mrs. Lisa's classroom is located on the north side of the new wing and adjoins the second-third grade classroom. This was the third year that Mrs. Lisa has had her first-second grade class in this room. Her three previous years were spent teaching special education in what is now the music room. (The special education classroom has been moved to another school in the district.) The classroom is approximately twenty feet wide and thirty-five feet long. The north wall is made up of windows affording a view of 59 6V!“ _ MKSLISA‘S r2 ML 3CD cussm 60-03 1‘!" Cum“ '1 in: ‘ Tuners News «m m names» . A ISTFLOOR ._ Woe E _ ° ”NOW-‘5 , oWIcc _J_. I. .— TsuME MUSIC W Room [—1 , M Willi)” “ " r % 9551300!“ 330 mm mm (Ia-em cassava“ 41" [m Guns whom | . .‘w ' $3"me 2?...“ “WI m 22:: ‘r- — “‘6 OASSM ’ .i M1— . u L Liam‘t , I 41251" CW1? 56% g l o o h c S n o r y B f o n a l P r o o l F 1 . 4 E R U G I F 2'” flaofi 3” FLOOR. 60 the asphalt play area just outside and the playground beyond. The floor is covered with a grey tile and the walls are light blue. walking into the room, one is struck by the brightness and neatness of the room. Along the south wall there are brightly decorated bulletin boards on the doors of the built-in storage cupboards. Other bulletin boards around the room are covered with bright paper and are either decorated or used for such things as displaying today's weather and date, indicating job assignments, and displaying newspaper articles of interest. There are many posters and signs above the windows and around the room. The student desks, work tables, and bookcases are neatly arranged, and one gets the impression that everything is in its place. The student desks are located toward the front of the room and the large chalkboard. The students sit in pairs at small tables that are rearranged several times a term. They are often grouped into three or four major seating clusters. On top of each table are rec- tangular plastic wash tubs (one per student) containing parapher- nalia such as pencils, erasers, and rulers. Texts and workbooks are kept in a vertical file rack where each pupil's books are displayed in the slot behind his or her name. There is a large (9 x 12 feet) rug toward the back of the room that is used as a gathering place for the whole class at the beginning of the day, after recess, and at other times when the whole group gets together. Near the rug, there is a small carpeted platform raised a foot Off the floor that Mrs. Lisa sits on to direct activities. Around the edge of the rug are a half dozen brightly painted cement blocks that the students can use as seats. In addition, the room contains 61 two tables that are used with small groups, Mrs. Lisa's worktable which serves as her desk during the day, and several other worktables and cupboards. See Figure 4.2 for a basic floor plan of the classroom. The "backroom" that connects Mrs. Lisa's classroom with the class- room next door is approximately eighteen feet long and twelve feet wide. In the middle of the room are Mrs. Lisa's and the other teach- er's desks and file cabinets. Along the south wall there are two rest rooms (one for each classroom) and a storage closet. Windows run the length of the north wall and beneath the windows and along the west wall are storage cabinets with a low counter top on them. This area is used as an art lab where Mrs. Lisa sets out various materials that the students may use during their "choice time" activities. Each year, Mrs. Lisa decorates the room and names activities fol— lowing a central theme. This year the room is set up around a "Pea- nuts" cartoon theme, and there are many posters and signs around the room depicting various characters from this pOpular comic strip. Last year the room had a bicentennial theme, and the year before the theme was signs of the zodiac. The room is very well furnished with enough room for an informal meeting area, four places where small groups can meet, chalkboards for each small group area, adequate storage space, a record and cassette tape player, and easy accessibility to film strip and movie projectors. The main inconveniences of this classroom are the noise from the adja- cent playground, the small number of electrical outlets in the room, and the heat problem in the winter. The heat problem seemed to be the most troublesome. Since the new wing has radiant floor heating, it caused the floor tile adhesive to bubble up between the tiles, Often 62 Lcwmi cwmos] $03K | Sum “as ._,, *53a t 0 FIGURE 4.2 Mrs. Lisa's Classroom 63 making the floor too messy for sitting. Also, since all the heat for the wing went through Mrs. Lisa's room first, the temperature in her room often was as high as eighty degrees. This required Opening win- dows and having the students wear lighter clothes in the room. After much complaining, several letters to the superintendent of schools, and several months of inconvenience, this problem was finally cor— rected. The Main Characters: the Teacher and Students The Teacher When one first meets Mrs. Lisa, one gets the impression of an or- ganized, energetic person. She is in her early thirties, a little over five feet tall, and has short blond hair. She makes most of her own clothes, and at school slacks or a jumper accompanied by brightly colored tennis shoes were her usual outfit. Mrs. Lisa has been teaching for six years. Prior to teaching, she worked as a school social worker then returned to school and earned a Master's in special education. Her first three years of teaching were spent in a special education room in Bryon School working with emotionally impaired and learning disabled students. This was the third year that she has been teaching in a regular classroom, all of which have been first-second grade classes in Byron School. Mrs. Lisa is well respected by other teachers who call upon her for advice in handling difficult students and who are constantly amazed at the pace that she keeps up in her planning and teaching. Mrs. Lisa has described herself as a compulsive organizer and planner which is reflected in the amount of time that she spends preparing for teaching. 64 Mrs. Lisa designs many of the games and activities that she uses in the classroom. Much time is spent creating new materials to use with the students,and she is constantly on the lookout for new ideas that might be useful in her teaching. Mrs. Lisa is married with no children, and she and her husband Bill live across town in a large townhouse complex. In her spare time she enjoys sewing and swimming, and she says that she has a passion for traveling that is too often left unfulfilled. The Students Because of declining enrollment in the district in the last few years, many teachers have been assigned combined or split classes. This is the third year that Mrs. Lisa has taught in a first-second grade split classroom in Byron School. The students that Mrs. Lisa had this year were assigned or selected mainly on the basis of teacher recommendations and parent requests. The kindergarten and first grade teachers recommended students that they thought would get along well both socially and academically in a split classroom. Some students who had created problems together were purposely split up. This also involved recommending the assignment of several fast first graders and several slow second graders to the room. Eight of the thirteen second graders had been first graders in Mrs. Lisa's room last year, and they and their parents requested that they return to the room. Several parents of first graders also requested Mrs. Lisa's room since the other first grade teacher was not assigned until late in the summer. The rest of the students ended up in Mrs. Lisa's classroom as a result of the need to assign specific numbers of students to each room. 65 For most of this study, there were twenty-seven students in Mrs. Lisa's classroom--fourteen first graders and thirteen second graders. This fluctuated throughout the year, however, and the slightly dif- ferent composition of the class as the year progressed is listed in Table 4.1. When asked to describe her class as a whole, Mrs. Lisa gave the following response: The group as a whole is on the immature side, dependent on me for many easy decisions they could make. (They) cry easily, (are) unable to create things on their own (in art or letter writing), (and) they tend, as a whole, to copy or imitate my work. They are louder than other groups I have had (and) are very talkative. On the whole, in comparing them to other (city) children, they are of average bright- ness with a couple of outstanding exceptions. They are difficult to manage as a class, as they tend to be very "I-me" oriented--not a sharing, work-together type group. They tend to take things for granted--are less appreciative. We have spent a lot of time on manners, cooperativeness, and consideration. There was a wide range of ability among the students. At one extreme, a group of students was reading at a pre-primer level, and at the other extreme, a group was reading at a fourth and fifth grade level. In math the students ranged from a first to a fourth grade ability level. In most subject matter areas, the achievement of approximately half of the class was clustered just above grade level with the other half distributed somewhere above or below. A richer view of the class can be presented by listing some of Mrs. Lisa's descriptions of individual students. When asked to de- scribe several "good" or successful students in her class, Mrs. Lisa offered the following descriptions: Lana - hard worker, neat, consistent behavior, follows rules, very independent, sincere, very straight, far above grade level in academics. 66 Table 4.1 Number of students in Mrs. Lisa's classroom during the year First Grade Second Grade Total September January March May 6 5 5 5 9 9 9 8 7 8 9 8 5 5 5 5 27 27 28 26 67 Jim - sincere, sympathetic, good worker, follows rules, independent, well liked by classmates, responsible, tolerant. Allan independent, good sense of humor, kind to others, good worker, bright. Elaine well liked, athletic, independent, hard and neat worker, very responsible, follows rules, sympa- thetic to others, COOperative. Bobby - popular, excellent judgment, good, neat worker, very independent, empathetic towards peers, co- operative. Susan - independent, cooperative, considerate, good worker, bright. Tommy - good worker; a bit immature, yet responsible; very, very bright; creative. The following descriptions are of those students who gave her the most difficulty in the classroom--her "problem" students: Tim - very insecure, unhappy, non-accepted, stubborn, responds negatively to many things, has many small annoying habits, tends to steal. Billy - pushy, unkind, grabby, mean to other children, constantly talks. Jan - very immature, very dependent, unable to make a decision, cries easily, bright. Diane - emotional problems causing learning problems, immature, insecure, emotional block to listening. Roy - severe learning problems, tries hard, reversals, poor reader, repeating first grade. Anna - severe learning problem, immature, highly distrac- ted, talks a lot, repeating first grade. The Script: the Curriculum The city school district influences the classroom curriculum in two major ways. It exercises a major influence by specifying perfor- mance objectives for all elementary grade levels in five subject 68 matter areas: reading, mathematics, writing skills, social studies, and science. These objectives were developed over a five year period with the cooperation of a group of classroom teachers, and are pub- lished in a handbook that is organized by subject matter topics. General objectives are provided for each area as well as enabling ob- jectives for each grade level to work towards meeting the general ob- jectives. Both affective and cognitive performance objectives are specified and examples are provided for each objective. The objectives are not presented as mastery objectives; rather, the emphasis is placed on specifying the grade level at which acquisition should begin. In the preface to the handbook, the following statement about its use is made: It (the handbook) has been developed by teachers with teach- ers as its key targets. As such, it is hoped that Objectives are in a form that will be most helpful to teachers in (1) providing an overall picture of a subject area in total and by grade level, (2) establishing their own classroom and stu- dent objectives, and (3) sharing Objectives with parents. An outline of the objectives by subject matter is presented in Appen- dix C. The Objectives exercise an additional influence through the for— mat Of the student evaluation and reporting forms. At each grade level, performance statements based on enabling objectives are listed for reading, communication skills, math, science, and social studies. The teacher is to report the student's progress by marking for each statement whether the student is (1) doing well, (2) needs to improve, (3) is working in an area, or (4) whether the statement does not apply. The performance statements listed in the first and second grade eval- uation and reporting forms are presented in Appendix C. 69 The second major influence the district exercises on the curricu- lum is through curriculum programs that have been adopted district- wide. The district has adopted programs in two subject matter areas. The Science Curriculum Improvement Study (SCIS) program has been chosen for elementary science instruction and the Man A Course of Study (MACOS)--Family of Man program--is being used for social studies. The complete SCIS kits are available in each school, and the Family of Man kits circulate among the schools. Within the last year, the district has also developed and adopted a standardized spelling check list for grades one to five. This check list is to become part of each stu- dent's permanent file, and at the end of the year, the teacher is to check those words that the student is able to spell. In the classroom, Mrs. Lisa used all three of these programs with modifications. Since she is teaching a combined class, she has com- bined the first and second grade science objectives and selected activities from the Level 1 and Level 2 SCIS program to use with her children. She omits most of the beginning lessons in the Level 1 pro- gram since she feels the lessons are too repetitive. She mainly draws upon the second half of Level 1 and the first half of Level 2 so that there will be minimal repetition for those students who go into a second-third grade classroom next year. In addition to the SCIS ma- terials, Mrs. Lisa also teaches additional units on such things as nutrition, dinosaurs, and the solar system. For social studies, Mrs. Lisa uses three or four of the Family of Man kits during the year. This year she taught units on Mexico, Mos- cow, and Japan. In the fall, she began a unit on the states in the union, but discontinued it after several weeks because it was too 70 difficult for this class. Most of the social studies time in the fall is spent using a socio-emotional program called Developing Understand- ing of Self and Others (DUSO). The program contains records, posters, and puppets, and focuses on skills such as listening, cooperation, manners, and so forth. In addition to these programs, Mrs. Lisa has developed additional social studies units such as a unit on economics that she integrated with math. Mrs. Lisa uses the district spelling checklists, but has developed several lists of words that the students are taught prior to beginning the first grade words. At the beginning of the year, each child was tested on the spelling words and was as- signed to each group depending on how far along he is in the list. There is no reading series specified by either the district or the school. The school and district have available basal readers from several publishing companies. This year Mrs. Lisa has used three dif- ferent basal reading series, upper level readers from three other series, and a phonics workbook series. She uses ditto masters for two of the series, and has developed comprehension worksheets for many Of the texts. In math, Mrs. Lisa uses the mathematics series adopted by all of the teachers in the building. She uses the workbooks for grades one through four and has developed a recording system for the workbooks to keep track Of each student'sprogress during the year. She has also developed a series of math quizzes that are given several times a week. The quizzes are timed mastery quizzes designed to provide practice in addition and subtraction and move students towards meeting the computa- tion objectives. 71 For handwriting a second grade workbook is used from a series that was adopted by several other teachers in the building. There is no language arts or writing skills program available to Mrs. Lisa, so most of this instruction is based on activities that she has designed. They include such things as board work, teacher-made dittos, writing to pen- cil pals, writing thank you notes and recipes, finding mistakes in written passages, and teacher-made games. Special teachers are provided for gym, music, and art. The stu- dents leave the room for these activities, and Mrs. Lisa is not re- quired to teach in these areas. The students spend one-half hour per week with the music teacher, one hour every other week with the art teacher, and thirty minutes three times per week with the gym teacher. They also spend an hour per week at the library and forty-five minutes per week with the reading consultant in the reading lab. Activities that are not subsumed under the above subject matter areas include perceptual and psychomotor skill training, listening skill development, reading aloud to classmates, art projects in the room, and field trips. Several times during the year, the class plans and cooks a meal together in the room. Mrs. Lisa uses this activity to integrate math and writing and to give the students a realistic ap- plication exercise for these academic areas. This year she also had the students publish a class newspaper in order to integrate various aspects of language arts. A Typical Plot: a Day at School Before describing the more general characteristics of Mrs. Lisa's teaching and planning, it may be helpful to describe a typical school 72 day. It is difficult to isolate and describe a typical day in Mrs. Lisa's classroom since the scheduled activities vary widely from one day to another. The purpose here is not to describe what is typical of or common to all school days, but rather it is to provide a picture of one representative school day. The day that has been chosen is a Monday in February, the week before Valentine's day. Mrs. Lisa arrives at 8:00 a.m. This morning she spends the time before school running off dittos and beginning her weekly letter to parents that she will send home on Friday. The first bell rings at 8:55 and the students come in from out- side. They go immediately to their lockers and begin removing their coats and boots. Mrs. Lisa is in the hall greeting the students and collecting the empty envelopes in which she sends the week's work home on Fridays. The students come into the room and most go over to sit on the rug. One of the students, Allan, has brought in his valentines and begins putting them in the students' mailboxes at the "post of- fice." Several other students gather around him. Mrs. Lisa enters the room and tells them that they should be on the rug unless they are dis- tributing valentines. At 9:00 the second bell rings, and Mrs. Lisa comments to the class, now sitting on the rug, that they are not doing their jobs. She then asks who are the Today Is and Weather Report persons. Tim and Diane get up to do their jobs. Jan, who has also forgotten her job, gets up to mark off the day on the calendar. Mrs. Lisa begins calling the roll. The students respond to their names as they finish putting on their shoes. When Mrs. Lisa finishes the roll, she remarks that there are five people absent this morning. She then asks Tim and 73 Diane who are standing by the date and weather boards to give their reports. Tim does all right, but Diane has up the wrong month and day of the week. Mrs. Lisa corrects her mistake, then has her say the correct day and date. Mrs. Lisa then asks whose job it is to take the attendance slip to the office. Bobby jumps up, takes the slip, chooses one of his classmates to go with him, and leaves for the Office. Mrs. Lisa then asks the Snoopy Snews people to come up to the platform. Debbie comes up and Mrs. Lisa asks who the other Snews per- son is. Jim says that he is, but has forgotten to bring in a news ar- ticle. Debbie shares a story about down filled clothing. Mrs. Lisa asks the students if anyone has an article of clothing containing down insulation. She then reads exerpts from the article to the students, explaing as she goes along. Several students then comment about down clothing that they have. At 9:10, Mrs. Lisa finishes the discussion, thanks Debbie, and walks over to the small blackbOard near the rug on which the morning schedule is written. The schedule has been written with mistakes in spelling, grammar, and punctuation; and Mrs. Lisa says, "OK, let's find the mistakes." Before she begins calling on the students, she points out this week's Break-the-WOrd Contest word that the students may try to unscramble before Thursday. She then begins calling on the students and correcting the mistakes that they point out. When done, she goes overthe morning schedule with them. At 9:17, Mrs. Lisa picks up the handwriting folders and tells the students to come up and get them as she calls their names. The students get their folders and pencils and return to their seats. min“ iv 74 At 9:20, Sandy, the aide, and Mrs. Pat, a parent volunteer, enter the room. Mrs. Lisa calls on one student from each of the seating areas to get the dictionary for his group. When done, she goes up to the front board and asks, "Who knows what the Mindbender is for this week?" A student says "consideratiOn." Mrs. Lisa writes it on the board, and the students copy it onto their sheets. Mrs. Lisa then has the class find the word in the dictionary, read the definition, write down a short definition, then write a sentence using the word. Sandy and Mrs. Pat circulate around the room aiding those students who have questions or need help. At 9:40, Mrs. Lisa hurries along several students who are not finished and tells the "Nowadays"6 reading group to get their books and pencils and go to the back table (Mrs. Lisa's table) to read with Mrs. Pat. She calls the students in the low reading group by name and tells them to get their books and go up to the round table at the front of the room. The students in the "Highways" group (the top group) are instructed to go to the rug to read with Sandy. She then explains the assignment on the board to the remaining group ("Seven Is"). By 9:45, Mrs. Lisa is sitting down and reading with the pre- primer group. She has them turn to a specific page and begin reading. Each student reads aloud one page as he is called on. If a reader has difficulty with a word, Mrs. Lisa helps him or her sound out the word. Periodically, she comments on the pictures or'On what is happening in the story. 6The reading groups are named after the title or part of the title of their current reading books. 75 At 9:57, the "Nowadays" group that has been reading with Mrs. Pat finishes reading. The students put away their books in the book file cabinet, get their reading folders, and return to the table. When the pre-primer group finishes their story, Mrs. Lisa has them open their phonics books. She tells them to put their names and date on the as- signed pages, and tells them that today they are going to work on the short p_sound. She then works through several pages with the group. At 10:07, Mrs. Lisa looks up at the clock and tells the group that they page they are working on will be the last page that she will do with them. The "Highways" group has just broken up, and the students are working at their seats on dittos in their reading folders. The "Nowadays" group is still working with Mrs. Pat at Mrs. Lisa's table. Mrs. Lisa then tells the pre-primer group to put circles around the numbers on the next three pages and then explains how to work each page. At 10:13, Mrs. Lisa announces, "It's recess time," and tells the class that they can leave their materials where they are working. She tells them that she has recess duty this morning and that they may head outside as soon as they are ready. By 10:16, Mrs. Lisa is hurrying along those students still in the hall and goes out for recess duty. At 10:25, Mrs. Lisa blows her whistle, and the students come in from recess. She helps several students with their coats and boots in the hall. The students come into the room and go over to sit down on the rug, talking and putting on their tennis shoes. Mrs. Lisa enters the room after several minutes and tells those still putting on their shoes to hurry since they must leave for gym. She asks if anyone has seen Mike's other mitten. No one responds. 76 At 10:30, Mrs. Lisa again tells them to be sure to have their gym shoes on and tells the class that when they come back from gym, they can finish their phonics or folder work and then do their station work. She then calls the Line Leader who jumps up and heads out of the room and the Tapper who excuses each student by tapping him or her on the head. The following excerpts from field notes describe Mrs. Lisa's ac- tivities while the students are at gym: Mrs. Lisa says that she's going to ask in several other rooms if they've found a mitten. She then goes down to the office to see why so many kids were absent today. When she returns to the room, Mrs. Lisa pulls out the folder work for those who were absent. She puts their names on the folders and then the word "absent" on top of each. She says she sends them home with their work done on Friday and puts notes at the bottom of the weekly letter to their parents. She isn't that concerned that they do it. She won't take the time in class for make- up, but she says many parents are real good about it and have their kids do it at home and bring it back to school. She next grades the other work turned in this morning. Mrs. Lisa writes down the pre-primer group reading pages for this week. She mentions that she tries to have them read four stories per session, and it goes down to three as the stories get longer since she wants to be able to spend more time on comprehension. She plans for Wednesday, Thursday, and next Monday in a notebook that she keeps with the reading materials for the pre-primer group. She looks at the next primer in the series. She then looks through the "Nowadays" reading books to see what work to put in their comprehension folders for next Monday. She checks to see if she has a worksheet for a story several pages away. She does, and says that this will work out just right since there are two days' reading before it. At 11:05, the students return from gym. Mrs. Lisa tells them to get a drink, then start in on their work. She tells the "Seven Is" group to get their books and meet her at the round table. She gets her book and walks over, sits down with the group, and starts them on their story. Several times during the reading, students come up to 77 Mrs. Lisa with questions about their seat work. She lets them wait. until there is a break in the reading, then quickly answers their ques- tions. At 11:20, Mrs. Lisa finishes with the reading group and tells the students to put their books away. She stands up and says to the class, "Anyone who has a question, put your hand up and I'll come and answer it." Mrs. Lisa spends several minutes answering questions and circu- lating to inspect the work that the students are doing at their seats. As the students finish their seat work and begin on their station work, the following incident is recorded: 11:25 Several kids are complaining that Karen is copy- ing and telling the others the answers to the station work. Mrs. Lisa tells Karen that if that's what she wants to do, she'd rather have her throw it away than do the work. Mrs. Lisa then puts up on the windows several ditto sheets that the students have colored, then continues pulling the morning work from the students' work-done folders. She comes to an empty folder and asks Debbie if she has completed her boardwork. Debbie says "no," and Mrs. Lisa tells her that it has to be done before she goes to lunch. By 11:30, most of the students are on "choice time" although sev- eral are still working on their folders or on station work. Mrs. Lisa continues checking the students' work until 11:35 when she tells the class to begin cleaning up for lunch. She asks the "boardbuffers" to erase the front board. Several students come up to tell her that their station work is not done. Mrs. Lisa replies, "No sweat, you can do it this afternoon." She then walks over and writes the afternoon schedule on the small blackboard as the students finish cleaning up and stand behind their seats. When everyone is at his or her seat, 78 Mrs. Lisa calls the Line Leader and Tapper, and the students go out to their lockers. When they have gotten their coats or lunches (some stay for lunch and some go home), the students return to the room and go over to the rug. At 11:40, the bell rings and Mrs. Lisa calls the Line Leader and Tapper. As the students are tapped, they leave for lunch. Before leaving for lunch, Mrs. Lisa spends approximately forty minutes finish- ing grading the morning's work. Mrs. Lisa pulls work out of the file folders and sees that Jim has turned in no work. She checks and finds his phonics book still in the book file cabined. She puts it on his desk and then walks over to the small blackboard and writes, "Jim W. - no recess." She then can't find Tom and Tim's phonics books. She marks them down on the point record sheet and marks "re-do" on sloppy sentences from the pre- primer group's board work. As she pulls the work from the "Nowadays" comprehension folder, she writes in next week's assignment on the attached assignment sheet. She then checks the pre-primer group's phonics books and marks how each student did on the record sheet attached to the inside of the cover. She then grades this morning's work in the B phonics book and the C phonics book. Mrs. Lisa leaves at 12:20 to eat her lunch in the teachers' lounge and returns to the room at 12:50. At 12:55, the first bell rings. Mrs. Lisa stands at the door of the classroom and watches the students in the hall. The students come in and sit on the rug; some put valentines into the mail boxes. At 1:00, Mrs. Lisa comes into the room, tells those delivering valentines to stop now, and then sits on the platform and calls the roll. When done, Alice and Tristin take the attendance slip to the office. Mrs. Lisa then goes over the afternoon schedule with the class, and then tells them that they will need pencils for the math quiz and that they can return to their seats. She passes out a quiz sheet to each 79 student, telling them to keep it face down. She then goes to the front of the room and explains to the class that they will have one minute to do as many problems as they can. She gets the class ready, then has them begin. After a minute, she calls time, and then has the students exchange papers. She reads off the answers, lets the students see how they did, then has the afternoon helpers collect the papers. Mrs. Lisa then reads a letter to the class from a previous classmate who is now living in Germany. She then puts the letter up on the Snoopy Snews Board so that the students may later look at the writing and the Ger- man stamps. At 1:15, Mrs. Lisa begins a lesson on metric measurement. Ex- cerpts from the field notes describe the activity: 1:15 Mrs. Lisa asks the students to get out their math rulers and asks Eric and Lucille to pass out a worksheet to everyone. She tells everyone that he or she should have two different sheets. She reminds them to put their names on them and then walks around making sure everyone has both sheets ...she asks the kids to make sure that they have a front and a back on each sheet. 1:20 Mrs. Lisa tells the kids to take out their rulers before they start, and for each to measure a hair on the person next to him or her. She then asks the kids how long their hairs were. Then she has the kids measure their hands from the palm to the tip of the middle finger. She circulates to check on them. She then asks for the different lengths that they've found and tallies the numbers on the front board. 1:25 When through tallying, she asks which number had the most tallies and which had the 1east...she then gives instructions for the next worksheet (on measuring objects) and circulates to check on who is measuring properly and who isn't. Mrs. Lisa pulls a few shades because of the sun's re- flecting off the snow outside. 1:30 Mrs. Lisa goes back to the front and tells them how to do the other worksheet. She then checks 80 the work of several kids and tells the class that when they're done, they can turn the sheet over and begin on the other side. 1:35 Mrs. Lisa draws an object on the board and demon- strates how to measure an object that isn't straight, and how to add up the sides to get the length. She tells the kids that when done, they , should put both sheets together, and asks Eric, Tom and Michelle to pick up the Sheets as the kids finish. At 1:40, Mrs. Lisa tells Bill and Eddy, who are far ahead of the other students, to get their workbooks and start working where they left off. She then calls students who have just completed a unit and passes out the unit test for them to complete at their seats. Next she calls by name the seven students who are in her lowest math group and tells them to get their notebooks and sit on the rug. Mrs. Lisa sits down with them on the rug and passes out a pretest for the next unit that they will be doing. She explains the sheet to them, works a few problems with them, and tells them that they may return to their seats and work on the pretest. At 1:50, Mrs. Lisa calls those students in the top math group over to the rug. Before beginning with them, she says to the class, "Any- one with a question, now is the time." Two students come up, and she answers their questions. Mrs. Lisa then begins working with the group. Mrs. Lisa hands out slips of paper to those on the rug and tells them she wants to start on the concept of carrying. She writes a problem on her sheet and tells them to copy it onto theirs. She gives them the problem 238 + 111. She stops Bobby who is starting to add the numbers with the hun- dreds column. She tells them that they should al- ways, always, always start with the ones column; "otherwise, you'll get all your carrying problems wrong." 1:55. Mrs. Lisa gives them another addition problem with carrying to try., Mrs. Lisa calls Diane over and 81 tells her that all the work is wrong that she's just handed in to her. She explains what she's to do and tells her to re-do the problems. Mrs. Lisa then gives the group two problems, one that involves carrying and one that doesn't. She asks the group if they can tell the difference. She then explains that they can't leave a two-digit number in the ones' column. It has to be carried over to the tens' column. 2:00 Art comes up to the rug with a question. Mrs. Lisa gives the group two more problems and has them figure out which one needs carrying. She then answers Art's question. She then shows the group how to do a carrying problem and tells them to try one on their own. When they've tried, she works it with them. Fred brings up his work; Mrs. Lisa says it's okay and that he can do some- thing on his own. Fred gets out the Blockhead game and begins playing with it. Mrs. Lisa works through another carrying problem with the group. 2:05 After a few minutes, Mrs. Lisa tells the group that they'll continue working on this later this week, but it's now time for recess. Mrs. Lisa tells the whole class that it's time to clean up for recess. She calls Jim, Debbie, Art, and Mike over to the round table. She tells Jim that he has to complete the work he didn't do this morning and hands back to the others some work from this morning that has to be re-done. At 2:10, Mrs. Lisa calls the Line Leader and the Tapper, and the rest of the class leaves for recess. She returns to her table and begins stapling together the metric work sheets done earlier. After about five minutes, Art and Mike finish their sheets and give them to Mrs. Lisa. She says, "That's better," and the boys leave for recess. Mrs. Lisa spends the rest of the recess looking over the math quizzes. At 2:25, the students begin coming in from recess. Debbie gives Mrs. Lisa her re-done work, and Mrs. Lisa says that she has done a much better job. The students sit down on the rug. Mrs. Lisa then 82 passes out the spelling folders and reminds the class that they should each have a sheet of paper, a pencil, and their folders when they go to their seats. When everyone is seated, Mrs. Lisa goes to the front of the room and reads this week's spelling words for each of the five spelling groups. The words have been previously marked on the spelling lists that are stapled to the inside of their folders. The students follow along as the words are read for their group. When she is done, she goes over the headings they should have on their sheets of paper, and then tells everyone to write all of their spelling words once and to write two sentences using two of the words. As the stu- dents work, Mrs. Lisa moves around the room answering questions and checking their work. She picks up the folders as the students finish. Those who are finished sit and talk to those seated around them. At 2:45, Mrs. Lisa puts away all of the spelling folders and walks over to a table near the window that has a box of science materi- als on it. She tells the students that they are to come up to the table and get a tray of science materials, and they are to work with partners until 3:00 trying to invent a "system" using the pulleys, rubber bands, and other materials on the tray. (This is a lesson from the SCIS unit on systems.) When all the materials are distributed, Mrs. Lisa walks around the room making comments on the students' ef- forts. The students are working intently on their systems, talking and showing to others what they have created. At 3:00, Mrs. Lisa tells each pair to decide on one system and put it together so they can demonstrate it to the class. She also tells them to think of names for their systems while they are waiting 83 for everyone to get ready. The remainder of the lesson proceeds as follows: 3:05 The students start demonstrating their systems at the front of the room. Mrs. Lisa writes the names of the systems on the front board. Some of the names they tell her are: the putt-putt, the blast-off system, the typewriter noise system, the train system, tic-tac-toe, the fan, the cuckoo system, and the break system. She tells those not demonstrating to be quiet and not to play with their systems. She warns Paul once and then takes his tray away from him. 3:15 Mrs. Lisa tells the class that the other person in each pair should now make a system. She also tells them to begin to fill out the sheets that she gave them earlier. Each student is going to put the name of the system and the objects of interaction on it. She lists the possible objects on the board that they may have used. 3:20 Tom says that he feels as if he's going to be sick. Mrs. Lisa asks him if he can wait until school is out, and he says no. She then sends him down to the office to call his mother. Mrs. Lisa then tells the class to draw their first system at the bottom of the sheet. She draws an example on the board and circulates to check on how they're doing. She then tells them that if they can't remember, to put the system back together again and then draw it. 3:25 Mrs. Lisa tells the kids to listen closely to the instructions so they can start where they leave off next time. She tells them to put their ini- tials on the bottom of the tray that their materials came on and to circle the initials of the person who is supposed to demonstrate next time. They are to take the systems apart; otherwise, she won't accept them. She goes around the room and picks up the system description sheets from those who are finished. 3:30 She tells those who are ready to bring their trays up to her. When the systems are turned in, the students put their chairs up on their desks and begin cleaning up the room. Those who do not have a clean up job stand by their desks. Mrs. Lisa reminds the class that 84 there are two times that they may pass out valentines: five minutes before school starts and during choice time. I At 3:35, Mrs. Lisa tells the class to get behind their seats if they are ready. She inspects the room and thanks the class for being quiet today. She reminds the students to check the "nameless bucket" (where nameless papers go) before leaving, and then calls the Line Leader and the Tapper. The students file out to the hall to get their coats and boots on. When they are dressed, the students come back to the rug and wait for the bell to ring. At 3:41, the bell rings. Mrs. Lisa calls the Line Leader and the Tapper, and the students leave for home. After school, Mrs. Lisa sits down at her table and writes the reading assignments into the "Highways" group folders for Wednesday. She then looks over the math quiz from this afternoon and separates the perfect papers from the rest. When done, she gets out the handwriting workbook and plans the handwriting lessons for the rest of the week. After that, she checks the math work from this afternoon. At 4:40, the teacher with whom she rides home comes into the room, and she packs up to go home for the day. The Drama: Life in the Classroom Mrs. Lisa has described her teaching style as traditional, orga- nized, structured, flexible, routine, consistent, creative, and demand- ing. These adjectives well portray the organization of her teaching, and may serve as an outline for describing life in her classroom. 85 Classroom Organization Mrs. Lisa's classroom is traditional in the sense that she is in charge at all times. She does all of the planning for and organization of instructional activities, thus providing a well structured learning environment for the students. She requires that for most activities, noise be kept at a minimum, that the students be polite and respectful to her as the teacher, and that they be courteous and well behaved to- wards each other. All activities are planned and organized well in advance of teach- ing. Materials are checked and rechecked before class; and instruc- tions for activities, the passing out of materials, and the activities themselves run smoothly once initiated. Much of Mrs. Lisa's work be- fore school and during breaks involves preparing and organizing ma- terials for activities later in the day. Once an activity begins, little or no time is needed to locate and organize materials for the students. At the beginning of fall term, and prior to the beginning of win- ter and spring terms,7 Mrs. Lisa draws up a weekly schedule for the class. This schedule is tentative at first and may be modified some- what before its establishment by the third week of the term. Mrs. Lisa establishes a classroom schedule for several reasons. Primarily, she does so since she feels that children of this age need a schedule and routine to reduce anxiety. She feels that too much change is disorienting for the students and that they learn better if 7The "term" is not an official calendar designation, but is a di- vision that Mrs. Lisa uses in her planning since winter and spring breaks divide the school year into three sections. 86 they can predict what is coming next in the day. She tries to be flexible in her teaching, but tries to avoid abrupt or frequent changes in routine. A weekly schedule also aids Mrs. Lisa in her planning so that she can count on a specific number of lessons with a fairly set duration. A schedule also makes it easier to schedule her teacher aide and parent volunteers on a regular basis. The Weekly Schedule The weekly schedule becomes the major time allocation mechanism in Mrs. Lisa's classroom, and each term the schedule is changed to re- flect her instructional priorities. Most of the fall is spent getting to know the students, organizing the class socially, and sorting out the students by ability levels. Much student time is spent on listen- ing to instructions, following directions, and on other basic class- room skills. The winter term is the heaviest academically, and much emphasis is placed on reading and mathematics. Spring term is slower paced, with a continued emphasis on reading and math skills, but with more time devoted to writing skills, social studies, and science. Throughout the year, Mrs. Lisa places a premium on developing basic reading and math skills, and, as a result, science and social studies are often given a less prominent position in her curriculum. Mrs. Lisa strives to meet several criteria when establishing her schedule. She tries to balance each day between the more intense aca- demic activities such asreading and math, and more active large group activities such as art, science, and social studies. She tries to schedule a more rigorous academic regimen for earlier in the week when the students are fresh, and taper it off on Thursday and Friday as the 87 weekend approaches. Mrs. Lisa also tries to schedule activities to take into account times when the students will be more active such as after gym and recess, and times like the end of the day when the stu- dents will be more fatigued. Her second major concern is to schedule small group and individual activities when she will have help in the classroom. There is a full time aide for the four classrooms in the newer wing of the building, so Mrs. Lisa must schedule her in advance on a regular basis. She uses her to help with reading groups three mornings a week, for math one morning, and to help give spelling tests to the spelling groups on Friday morning. She also has four parent volunteers, two of whom help with reading groups once a week, one who helps with math one morning a week, and one who serves as a reader dur- ing library. During most terms, there is a student aide who is in the classroom one full day per week. Each day is divided by lunch and recesses into four instructional blocks, as shown in Figure 4.3. There is an hour and ten minutes from the beginning of school to the morning recess, an hour and fifteen minutes between recess and lunch, an hour and ten minutes between lunch and the afternoon recess, and an hour and fifteen minutes from recess to the end of the school day. Each recess is fifteen minutes long, and the lunch period lasts for an hour and fifteen minutes. School begins at 9:00 a.m. and is over at 3:40 p.m. At the beginning of the year, times for music, art, library, reading lab, and gym are chosen on the basis of teacher seniority or assigned. Once these times are set, Mrs. Lisa is free to arrange her schedule as she desires. Once aide times are set, they are not very flexible because of the need to coordinate with other teachers, but 88 H T R U O F G N I H C A E T V N O I S S E S RECESS D R I H T G N I H C A E T N O I S S E S LUNCH PERIOD D N O C E S G N I H C A E T N O I S S E S RECESS T S R I F G N I H C A E T N O I S S E S 0 4 : 3 5 2 : 2 0 1 : 2 0 0 : 1 0 4 : 1 1 5 2 : 0 1 0 1 : 0 1 , 0 0 : 9 y a d l o o h c s e h t n i s n o i s i v i D 3 . 4 E R U G I F 89 parent volunteer times are more easily changed. Reading and math are for the most part scheduled for the morning when help is available, and large group activities such as science, writing skills, and social studies are typically scheduled for the afternoon. Mrs. Lisa's winter term and spring term weekly schedules are presented in Figures 4.4 and 4.5. The daily schedule provides adequate time between activities to allow for transition as a group and to take into account individual differences in student work rates. Mrs. Lisa has developed two mecha- nisms to handle these differences. The first is station work. This year there were two stations in the classroom (there have been as many as five in previous years) to which students are variously assigned on Monday, Wednesday, and Friday. The station work consisted primarily of dittos that were oriented towards perceptual and psychomotor de— velOpment in one station and more academically oriented in the other. When students finished their morning work on these days,they would determine their work station work assignments by their initials that Mrs. Lisa had placed in a slot over the station, and then complete the assignment. If this work was completed before the period ended, the student was generally free to sign up for the second controlling mecha- nism, choice time. Choice time consists of free time activities that Mrs. Lisa has provided for the students. These activities include the art lab in the back room, games, a science center, the record player, construc- tion blocks, books, and so forth. The number of students participating in each activity is regulated by a board with nails on which students may put tags with their initials. The choice time activities are used t s e t n o C d r o W s l l i k S g n i t i r W s r e d l o F g n i d a e R 90 S S E C E R S S E C E R R S S E C E R S S E C E R S S E C E s t s e T g n i l l e p S 0 0 : 1 1 m y G 0 3 : 0 1 y r o t S h t a M p u h s i n i F 0 0 : 1 1 m y G 0 3 : 0 1 ) g n i d a e R ( y r o t S 0 3 : 0 1 h t a M p u { h s i n i F 0 0 : 1 1 m y G 0 3 : 0 1 ) g n i d a e R ( H C N U L H C N U L L H C N U L H C N U L H C N U s e i d u t S l a i c o S l l o R 0 0 : 1 . R . S . S . U z i u Q h t a M 0 0 : 1 r e d a e R y l k e e W s l l i k S g n i t i r W s e i d u t S l a i c o S r o l l o R 0 0 : 1 g n i d o c e D h t a M s l l i k S g n i t i r W / t r A l l o R 0 0 : 1 l l o R 0 0 : 1 g n i l l e p S e c n e i c S 0 3 : 2 s b o i e e B 0 3 : 2 0 0 : 3 p u n a e l C t a e r T t u o s s a P s r e p a p s e i v o M e g n a h C y r a r b i L g n i l l e p S S S E C E R S S E C E R . S S E C E R S S E C E R S S E C E R e c n e i c S s e i d u t S z i u Q h t a M l a i c o S r o h t a M 0 3 : 2 0 3 : 2 e l u d e h c S m r e T r e t n i W 4 . 4 E R U G I F s w e n S y p o o n S g n i t i r w d n a H r e d n e b d n i M g n i d a e R b a L g n i d a e R d n a s k o o B s c i n o h P l l o R 0 0 : 9 s w e n S y p o o n S k r o w d r a o B s k o o B y p o o n S 0 4 : 9 g n i d a e R s w e n S y p o o n S s w e n S y p o o n S c i s u M 0 1 : 9 g n i t i r w d n a H s w e n S y p o o n S g n i t i r w d n a H l l o R 0 0 : 9 g n i d a e R l l o R 0 0 : 9 u o k r x x é a d i r F y a d s r u h T y 9 y a d s e n d e W y a d s e u T y a d n o M l l o R 0 0 : 91 C N U L H C N U L , H H C N U L H C N U L H C N U L y r o t S 0 3 : 0 1 s t s e T g n i l l e p S 0 0 : 1 1 s k o o b k r o W g n i d a e R g n i t i r w d n a H 0 0 : 1 1 z i u Q h t a M 0 4 : 0 1 r e d n e b d n i M 0 0 : 1 1 ' s t e e h s r o p u h s i n i F h t a M p u h s i n i F m y G 0 3 : 0 1 g n i d a e R 0 4 : 0 1 . m y G 0 3 : 0 1 y r o t S 0 3 : 0 1 m y G 0 3 : 0 1 A s e i d u t S l a i c o S 0 3 : 1 e c n e i c S r o l l o R 0 0 : 1 . R . S . S . U s e i d u t S l a i c o S g n i d a e R l l o R 0 0 : 1 l l o R 0 0 : 1 e c n e i c S r o k o o B s c i n o h P s l l i k S g n i t i r W / t r A l l o R 0 0 : 1 l l o R 0 0 : 1 z i u Q h t a M . h t a M y a d s r u h T a d i r F y y a d s e n d e W y a d s e u T y a d n o M t s e t n o C d r o W 0 4 : 9 z i u Q h t a M z i u Q h t a M g n i t i r w d n a H g n i d a e R s w e n S y p o o n S c i s u M 0 1 : 9 l l o R 0 0 : 9 s w e n S y p o o n S s w e n S y p o o n S s w e n S y p o o n S l l o R 0 0 : 9 l l o R 0 0 : 9 l l o R 0 0 : 9 s w e n S y p o o n S l l o R 0 0 : 9 h t a M S E C E R S S E C E R n i t i r w d n a H g R S S S E C E R S S E C E R S S E C E s t r o p e R k o o B h t a M b a L g n i d a e R 0 2 : 9 S E C E R S S E C E R S S E C E R R S S S E C E R S S E C E y r o t S - A - d a e R 0 3 : 2 s e i v o M 0 0 : 3 p u n a e l C t a e r T s r e p a p t u o s s a P 5 4 : 2 y r o t S - A - d a e R 0 3 : 2 e e B g n i l l e p S 5 1 : 3 p u n a e l C 0 1 : 3 y r a r b i L 0 3 2 g n i l l e p S 5 1 : 3 r e d a e R y l k e e W 0 3 : 2 g n i l l e p S 0 5 : 2 ‘ y r o t S - A - d a e R 0 3 : 2 s e i d u t S l a i c o S 5 4 : 2 g n i l l e p S 0 2 : 3 e l u d e h c S m r e T g n i r p S 5 . 4 E R U G I F b 92 to provide the students with a change of pace as well as to provide for learning and skill develOpment in a more informal play situation. Grouping for Instruction Instruction in Mrs. Lisa's classroom is primarily conducted in large and small groups. Students who are having problems that require extra help are worked with individually, although time is rarely taken away from group instruction for this purpose. Individual help is most often given at times when the remainder of the class is doing work at their seats. When Mrs. Lisa came from a special education classroom to the regular classroom, one of the beggest errors she remembers making was trying to individualize all of her teaching. She said it was easy for nine to twelve special education students, but it was a "disaster" for a split classroom of twenty-five to thirty students. Now, Mrs. Lisa tries to group students in those subjects where there will be wide variation in basic skills and where attention to individual differences is most crucial at this age. This normally includes subjects such as reading, mathematics, and spelling. Most writing skills activities were conducted as a large group (whole class) since they mostly in- volved skill development and practice, and she felt she could ade- quately monitor performance through turned-in assignments. Social studies and science were taught as a large group since grouping was not perceived as necessary, and planning for these subjects involved enough time and effort as it were. This year, Mrs. Lisa also grouped the students for reading, mathe- matics, and spelling. Activities were never divided by grade level, 93 and Mrs. Lisa never referred to the students as first or second graders. Reading groups were established around the fourth week of fall term. Group membership changes were common in the fall as students improved, but became less frequent in winter and spring terms. There were four reading groups this year: a group of first graders reading at the pre- primer level, a group of second grade boys reading at the first grade level, a group of first and second graders reading at the second grade level, and a group reading at the third and fourth grade levels. Math groups, usually three in number, were set up for specific units based on pretesting. Within math groups, students often worked at different paces in their workbooks. Spelling groups, as mentioned earlier, were set up by pretesting the students on the district spelling list in the fall. Spelling activities were usually done in a large group, with each student using the words assigned to his or her group. Social studies, science, and writing skills were always taught to the whole class. Instructional Goals Many of Mrs. Lisa's goals for the year are concerned with develOp- ing responsibility, neatness, independence, and c00perativeness in the students. When academic assignments are made, she expects each student to complete his or her work in a neat manner and turn it in to the pro- per place. Each student has a "work-done" file near Mrs. Lisa's table, and worksheets and workbooks are to be turned in when completed. Re- sponsibility is further fostered by the assignment of jobs in the classroom. Each week jobs are chosen by the students that include such things as Boardbuffer (board eraser), Light Person (responsible for 94 turning off and on lights), Stagehand (pulls down shades and projection screen for films), Helpers (help Mrs. Lisa pass out or collect materi- als), and Tapper (taps students on the head when they are quiet and ready to leave the room for recess, lunch, going to the library, and so forth). There are enough jobs so that everyone can have one each week. Besides developing responsibility, Mrs. Lisa has found that the jobs provide a mechanism for keeping the room neat and clean and activities running smoothly. Mrs. Lisa's academic goals are much more loosely defined than her social objectives. She uses the district objectives as a general guideline for what should be presented, but does not formulate beha- vioral or other objectives for the students. She has expectations for the students, although she does not feel that every student should get to the same point by the end of the year. She tries to monitor closely each student's learning progress, especially in reading, math, and language arts, and then uses grouping and sometimes individual in- struction to allow the students to progress as far and fast as they can. Mrs. Lisa spends most of her instruction on fundamentals in reading, writing, and math since she feels strongly that a solid foun- dation in these areas will be the basis for later progress. Classroom Discipline Disciplining was rarely observed in Mrs. Lisa's room. When it occurred, it was smooth and rapid and rarely interfered with class ac- tivities. Mrs. Lisa clearly specified appropriate student behavior in various situations. Contingencies for misbehavior were well laid out, and she was consistent in following through on her promises. She said 95 that this approach to discipline resulted from her special education experience and seemed to be very effective in the regular classroom. Creativity in Teaching The creative aspect of Mrs. Lisa's teaching was apparent in most all of her instructional activities. Potentially dull and routine tasks were usually framed in activities that resulted in a high level of student interest and involvement. Many activities and groups were named after Peanuts characters--a technique that heightened interest and involvement. Mrs. Lisa's creativity was apparent in her develop- ment of science and social studies activities, but was most obvious in language arts. A few examples might best illustrate this point. Each Monday morning, the handwriting lesson consisted of spelling, looking up in a dictionary, and writing the "mindbender" word for the week. These were words such as vociferous, sympathetic, procrastina- tion, and scrumptious that were used as vocabulary builders. They were tied into social studies later in the week when the students did their "Snoopy books." These were folders for which each week the students were given a ditto containing a Peanuts character engaged in an ac- tivity related to the mindbender word. For instance, the students were given a picture of Snoopy dressed as a detective ("Super-Sleuth") when the mindbender word was investigate. The students were asked to write the mindbender word on the sheet, and then Mrs. Lisa directed a short discussion about the meaning of the picture and the mindbender word with an emphasis on social and affective development. Another example of creativity was the decoding activity. Each student was given a sheet with a symbol code for each letter of the alphabet. 96 They were required to write all of their spelling words in code, and then to decode a message that Mrs. Lisa had written on the board. This provided a review of the spelling words as well as practice in hand- writing. Other activities were framed in semi-competitive situations that allowed the students to display their academic abilities. One of the favorites was the weekly Spelling Bee. An excerpt from the field notes illustrates how this activity was conducted. 2:53 Mrs. Lisa numbers off the kids by two's and tells them to sit down on the floor in two lines down the middle of the room. She then tells them the ground rules: "no drinks, no getting up for any- thing except for marking points on the board." She also reminds them of their promise made last week to be quiet if she continues with the spelling bees. She tells them she will stop immediately if they are noisy. She begins the spelling bee, cal- ling kids from alternate rows, using words from each student's spelling list. (Note: She has to look at the student, remember what group he or she is in, and pick a word from the list that hasn't been used.) She pronounces the word and uses it in a sentence before asking for an answer. If the spelling is correct, the student gets up and marks a point for his or her team. If it is wrong, Mrs. Lisa skips to the next person. 3:05 They finish one round and start down the rows again. J. raises his hand and Mrs. Lisa calls on him. He says, "D. made two marks (on the board)." Mrs. Lisa tells him that she is keeping score on her paper, so she will know if he does. She comes to D. and tells him that she is going to ask him a word and if he misses it, she will give him another one. She asks him a word off of another spelling list (I assume more difficult). He spells the word correctly and, she says, "Good,"1?u1going to move you to the other group." 3:10 She continues down the rows. T. and D. are talking. Mrs. Lisa sends them immediately back to their seats. 3:14 They finish the bee. Mrs. Lisa walks to the front board and counts the points. 97- Another competitive activity was the Word Contest. This invOlved the students' writing as many words as they could based on the letters in the mindbender word within a specified period of time. In the fall, the class tried to see how many words they could generate as a group, and by spring Mrs. Lisa was giving an award to the person who could generate the most words by him- or herself. Communication with Parents Mrs. Lisa feels strongly that it is important to communicate to the parents what is going on in her classroom. The primary way that she accomplishes this is by writing a letter to the parents that is sent home each Friday. In this letter, she comments on special ac- tivities that the students did during the week and notifies the parents of anything special that the students will be doing next week. She also uses the newsletter to remind the parents of special days such as professional development days when school is not in session all day, and of times when the students need to bring in money for field trips, book club orders, and so forth. Appendix D contains the first letter that Mrs. Lisa sent out in the fall and.lexamples of letters sent home during winter and spring term. Another way that Mrs. Lisa communicates with parents is through the take-home envelopes that she sends home on Fridays. This contains all of the work that the student did during the week. As Mrs. Lisa grades the students' work during the week, she puts it into a large tray. On Thursday afternoon, she sits down on the rug with the class and passes back the graded work. The students can then see how they did on each assignment and ask any questions that they have. Mrs. Lisa then has them pass in their work, and she puts it in their large 98 manilla envelopes. On Fridays, she puts her letter and the school newsletter in the envelopes and sends them home. At the beginning of the next week, the students are to bring back their envelopes with their parents' signatures and comments in the space provided on the outside of the envelope. This way she makes sure that the parents have an opportunity to see their children's work and respond with any questions or comments. The school district requires only two grading reports and parent conferences each year. Mrs. Lisa feels that there is too much time between them, so she has developed a feedback form that she sends home in March. This provides the parents with more specific information about where their children are in reading, math, and writing skills than they receive by seeing their children's work or through the per- sonal notes that Mrs. Lisa often writes on the bottom of her weekly letters. To follow up comments or questions that parents have sent to her and to keep certain parents appraised of their children's special pro- blems or unusual progress, Mrs. lisa spends on the average of four hours a week on the phone talking with parents. This usually takes place at night or on weekends, and is a major portion of the school related work that she does at home. Mrs. Lisa spends so much time on this and other activities because she feels that the home is a major and crucial influence on her students. She believes that life at home directly affects both the academic and social performance of the stu- dents. Because of this, she tries to keep abreast of things that are happening at home in a student's life, and tries to let the parents know what is happening at school. She also feels that the parents can 99 play a more active role in their children's learning if they know what activities are going on at school, and if they can see samples of their children's work. In short, she sees the home and the classroom as ex- tensions of each other and feels that the children's school experiences will be more meaningful if the teacher and the parents know what is going on in the other portion of the children's lives. Summary of Classroom Life To summarize, Mrs. Lisa's classroom is run in a fairly traditional manner centering around well planned, organized, and structured acti- vities. Much effort is spent on developing responsibility, neatness, independence, and cooperativeness in her students. Daily and weekly activities are guided by a well structured schedule that has as its goal the provision of well balanced, interesting, and effective learn- ing activities. Students are grouped for reading, math, and spelling, and aides are used to facilitate small group instruction. Many crea- tive activities are used for teaching language arts and writing skills, and semi-competitive situations are used to create an enjoyable atmos— phere for learning and for performance testing. Mrs. Lisa makes many efforts to communicate these activities to the parents of her students through a weekly letter, by sending work home on Fridays, and through phone conversations. The Set and the Props for Planning The previous sections have focused on those aspects of the setting most closely related to Mrs. Lisa's interactive teaching. This has been necessary to provide a comprehensive view of the environment in which this study of planning was conducted. Since this was a study of 100 planning, the setting in which Mrs. Lisa's preactive decision making occurs becomes of special interest. The purpose of this section is to provide a description of where, when, and with what Mrs. Lisa does her planning. The major description of the "how" of her planning will be reserved for later chapters. Planning at School The planning that Mrs. Lisa does at school is usually of a short term nature. This mainly includes weekly and daily planning. The dis- trict requires that each teacher keep a plan book with daily lessons filled in one week in advance. They are to keep these plan books on their desks so as to be available to substitutes if they have to be called on short notice. There are several principals in the district who require their teachers to submit their plans for the next week by Friday afternoon, but Mrs. Lisa has never had to do this since she has been at Byron School. Mrs. Lisa lays out her weekly plans two weeks in advance and then makes any necessary modifications on a weekly or daily basis. The other teachers in the lower wing use the standard plan book in much the same way, although Mrs. Lisa's planning is usually in more detail and done farther in advance of teaching. Mrs. Lisa does most of her in-school planning at her work table near the door. This is a low round table that serves as her base of operations during the day. On the table is a plastic filing bin in which she keeps a clip board with frequently used record or scoring sheets and several file folders and teacher manuals. Also on the table are a can of pencils and pens, a tape dispenser, and a stapler, plus a 101 stack or two of teacher manuals, workbooks, dittos to be used later in (the day or week, and miscellaneous notes that Mrs. Lisa writes as re- minders to herself. The round table is Mrs. Lisa's area and is only oc- casionally used by the students when they read in small groups. Mrs. Lisa uses this table rather than her desk in the backroom since it gives her more room to spread out whatever materials she is using. It also is easily accessible to the students' work-done folders and the filing rack where the students' books and her teacher manuals are kept. If her table is too cluttered, Mrs. Lisa may move to one of the other tables in the room where she has more room. When doing her weekly and daily planning, Mrs. Lisa usually has present her plan book and the notebooks in which she has done her more long range planning. Often she will get up and check a text or work- book for page numbers or will look in her file cabinet for a worksheet that she might want. There are basically three times during the day that planning may occur: in the morning before school, during breaks in the day, and after school. Mrs. Lisa says that she is not a "morning person," and as a result, does not function at her best early in the morning. Be- fore she leaves school in the evening, Mrs. Lisa tries to make sure that there is nothing left undone that is essential to tomorrow's ac- tivities. Typical morning activities, because of this, usually in— volve such things as running off dittos that will be used in several days, writing her weekly letter, and running necessary errands such as checking the film schedule in the teachers' lounge, returning a book to the library, or talking with another teacher. 102 Mrs. Lisa has typically three breaks in the day, and sometimes four, when she can do some planning. These are the two recess periods and the lunch period, and the times when the students leave the room for gym, art, and music. Most of this time is taken up by marking the_ students' papers from the morning or afternoon, although occasionally, as time allows, Mrs. Lisa will do some weekly or daily planning then. For some activities, her planning is entwined with her evaluation. If an activity is based in a folder, as are some spelling, handwriting, and reading activities, the folder usually includes an assignment and comment sheet attached to the inside cover. When Mrs. Lisa grades the work from today, she will also make next week's assignment and enclose the appropriate worksheets. In this manner, she does not have to deal again with the folders until she passes them out for the activity the next week. Several excerpts from the field notes illustrate this prac- tice: As she pulls the work from the "Nowadays" comprehension folders, she writes in next week's assignment on the assignment sheet. (2/7) (As she begins grading the comprehension sheets) she re- members that the kids won't be here next Monday to do the folder work because of Presidents' day. She decides to do the folder work next Wednesday because she says that it's more important than phonics work at this point. She gets the "Nowadays" reading book and looks through it to see what story they will be reading this Wednesday so she can get the comprehension worksheet to go with the story. (2/14) Mrs. Lisa does most of her daily planning after school. Since the schedule and the weekly planning usually already specified what acti- vities will occur each day, most of this planning is intertwined with setting up activities, writing assignments on the board, and so forth. This is especially true for well routinized activities such as reading, 103 where the only major daily decision that she has to make is about con- tent. This selection from the field notes shows a typical afternoon's work. After school, Mrs. Lisa asks the student aide to finish grading the other side of the pretest that she started on earlier. Mrs. Lisa then sets up the station work (for Friday) and then writes tomorrow's lessons. She remem- bers that she has a new aide coming tomorrow (Jenny's mom) and starts to assign her to the "Nowadays" group, but re- members that Jenny is in that group. She reassigns Sandy to this group and then goes up to the front board and writes the reading assignments for tomorrow. She then walks over to the small blackboard and writes the word "due" and underneath (l) word contest, (2) library book, (3) home- work. She sets up the memory blocks and pulls the spelling work from the spelling folders. She gets the math board work out of the work-done folders and sorts it by groups. She then looks through the metric pretest and begins plan- ning the next steps that she's going to take with the unit. She then grades today's spelling lessons. (2/2) Planning at Home Most of Mrs. Lisa's long term planning is done at home. The pri- mary reason for this is that at home, there is much more room to spread out the materials and she can leave them out until she is finished. At school, things must be put away to make room for daily activities. Also, home is a more comfortable atmosphere for long periods of plan- ning. Mrs. Lisa does her planning at home in the basement. One half of the basement is carpeted and furnished with several bean bag chairs, her sewing machine, and a television set. When Mrs. Lisa is planning for the year (as she does in the summer) or for specific units, she brings home all the materials that she has gathered that might pertain to the planning task and then sits down on the rug or in a bean bag chair and spreads her materials around her. In this manner, she can 104 visualize all of her materials at once and experiment with different patterns of organization and sequencing. If she wants to see how pos- ters or maps look, she tapes them to the walls. When she previews films or movies, she shows them on the wall and listens to tapes or records on her record player and tape recorder. Sometimes in the even- ing, she will watch a television show if she is doing a task not re- quiring constant attention. Other planning activities which Mrs. Lisa typically does at home include scheduling films from the media cata- logue, previewing new instructional materials that have been purchased, and in the spring,ordering materials for next year. Not all of Mrs. Lisa's school related activities involve planning. Planning is most emphasized in the summer, during breaks and at times during the year when she is developing an instructional unit. During two months of this study, Mrs. Lisa kept a diary of her activities at home. She spent on the average of two hours per night working on school related activities. This is similar to the average out-of- school work time reported in a study of English elementary teachers (;'= 130.7 minutes per day) by Hilsum and Cane (1971). The most com- mon activities that Mrs. Lisa did at home, listed in order of fre- quency of occurrence, include correcting papers, talking with parents on the phone, planning, and general organizational tasks. This is com- parable to the English study where they found that most of the teach- ers' at home time was used for marking (26.2%), for lesson planning (15.3%), and for mechanical jobs (13.5%). The complete entries from Mrs. Lisa's diary are listed in Appendix E. 105 Chapter Summary This chapter has attempted to describe those aspects of the class- room, curriculum, and students that shape life in Mrs. Lisa's class- room. The importance of such a description is based on a conviction that understanding the context of behavior is essential to understand- ing the behavior itself. The outline of the chapter was based on a dramaturgical or theatrical outline, where the classroom, teachers, students, and so forth, were characterized as elements essential to a good drama. The chapter begins by describing the most general characteristics of the drama, the stage set by the school and the set and props that make up Mrs. Lisa's classroom. Next, the main characters--the teacher and the students--were described. A description of the script used in the classroom (the curriculum) was presented next, followed by a pre- sentation of one school day as a typical plot. A large part of the chapter was devoted to describing general characteristics of Mrs. Lisa's classroom. Discussed in this section were classroom organization, the weekly schedule, grouping for instruc- tion, instructional goals, classroom discipline, creativity in teach— ing, and communication with parents. Finally, the setting and props that Mrs. Lisa uses for planning were discussed. The following two chapters will present some hypotheses and models about teaching and planning in Mrs. Lisa's classroom. It is hoped that this chapter will serve as a useful backdrop for under- standing and interpreting what is presented subsequently. CHAPTER V ACTIVITIES AND ROUTINES: A TECHNOLOGY OF TEACHING The purpose of this chapter is to discuss two aspects of Mrs. Lisa's teaching that emerged as central factors in her thinking and in her planning for instruction. The chapter begins by discussing acti- vities, their function as structural units of planning and action, their role as controlled behavior settings, and their major structural components. The second part of the chapter deals with routines in teaching, including the different types of routines observed in Mrs. Lisa's teaching and their role in planning and interactive teaching. Part I: Teaching by Activities Activities as Structural Units of Planning and Action Activities may be broadly defined as self—contained organizational units for performing a specific function. Educational activities are designed to stimulate learning by firsthand experience--hence, educa- tors usually refer to them as instructional or learning activities. Educational activities involve transactions between students and their environment-~the teacher, other students, materials, and so forth. Homans (1950) has suggested that the activity of the group is an important part of the sociological structure of a group. Smith and Geoffrey (1968) argued that the structure of activities is a major and often neglected part of classroom social-system analysis and devoted a full chapter to discussing activities, focusing primarily on daily 106 107 lessons. It is proposed here that activities not only play an impor- tant role in classroom organization and interaction, but are also a basic structural element in the teacher's decision making. There are several reasons for suggesting that teachers use activi- ties as the basic unit for decision making and planning. The first reason is that activities permit the organization of classroom action at a manageable level. Organizing instruction at the level of activi- ties is much more specific and manageable than thinking about a subject matter or whole units of instruction, and is not as detailed and com- plex as thinking about and trying to organize statements of behavioral outcomes of instruction. Second, activities have as their focus some action--some "work" that is to be done. Since planning is chiefly con- cerned with future action, activities are a natural "action unit" for teachers to work with. Jackson (1968) suggests that teachers are more concerned with immediate behavioral outcomes such as enthusiasm and involvement in the classroom than they are with more long-term learning outcomes. If this is so, then the activity becomes a unit that can be used as a device to create enthusiasm and involvement as well as learn- ing. Third, since activities are self-contained units, they can be tailored to fit into a specific time slot, and because of this somewhat independent characteristic, they may be arranged and sequenced into larger units of instruction. In short, it is because of their manage- ability, their orientation towards action, and their independence, that it is suggested that activities are a basic structural unit of both preactive and interactive teacher decision making. Early in this study it became apparent that activities played a central role in Mrs. Lisa's thinking about instruction. Activities 108 first became visible in the classroom as the basis for organizing the day. Nearly all action and interaction in the classroom took place within the boundaries of an activity, and, for the most part, the re- maining time was used for preparation for or transition between acti- vities. This organization was communicated to the students by way of the daily schedule. The first thing that Mrs. Lisa did each morning and afternoon after calling the roll was to go over the schedule. It was presented as a list of activities, some of which were punctuated by times. Two excerpts from the field notes serve as examples: Mrs. Lisa then goes over the afternoon schedule. It reads: Roll, Sharing, Change Jobs, Mindbender, Recess - 2:10, Read-a-Story, Weekly Reader, Spelling. (4/11) After school Mrs. Lisa erases the small blackboard and puts up tomorrow's schedule. It reads: Snews, Graphing, John's Shells, Weight and Height, Recess - 10:10, Field Trip - 10:30, Pass Out Papers, Clean up. (4/13) As a result of experience, the activity names became for both the teacher and students a sufficient source of knowledge about what was to occur and how it would be carried out. The students often came over to look at the schedule to see what was coming up next, and the names of the activities served as an adequate cue to evoke Mrs. Lisa's plans for the activities. It soon became apparent that activities also played an important role in Mrs. Lisa's planning decisions. In addition to her daily plan- ning, Mrs. Lisa's weekly and unit planning was largely made up or or- ganizing and sequencing activities. Her weekly planning as it was laid out in her plan book looked much like the schedules that she put on the blackboard each day. For some activities, page numbers and 109 additional notes were listed along with the activity name, though for most activities, the activity name alone was sufficient to indicate what would occur during the allotted time. When Mrs. Lisa planned units for science or social studies, the first step in her planning was to gather all the materials she could find on the topic, look through the materials, and then list activities that might be carried out as part of the unit. These activities were either based on the materials themselves or on ideas developed from the materials. Once a general sequence for the unit had been decided, Mrs. Lisa's planning largely involved the selection and sequencing of activities. This process can be seen during Mrs. Lisa's planning for a unit on dinosaurs for science: Mrs. Lisa begins by thinking out loud about where she wants to begin the unit. She decides that she wants to begin in the pre-dinosaur era and begins looking for a film strip on this era and appropriate activity to go with it. As she checks through the filmstrips, she relies on her preview comments. She also keeps the general sequence of the unit in mind. It is a time line following the evolution of rep- tiles and dinosaurs. She then begins writing activities in her plan book. She does one week at a time, first looking to see how many periods she had previously set aside for science and then fits in activities into each slot. (3/9) Types of activity. Fifty-three activities8 were identified in Mrs. Lisa's schedule. A complete listing of these activities grouped by subject matter is presented ianigure 5.1. Some activities such as roll, Snoopy Snews, finding mistakes in the schedule, and recess took place every day, while others such as cooking, holiday parties, and field trips 8"Activity: as used here refers to the generic activity type. A specific instance of an activity type will be referred to as activity occurrence. ActivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroom Figure5.1 # wordContest SpellingTest SpellingFolders SpellingBee RemedialHandwritingGroup PencilPals OrangeSuitcase(Grammar) Newspaper(ClassNewspaper) Mindbender PostOffice) Treat Roll Recess Movies(Fridayp.m.) LetterWriting(Classroom HolidayParty HandwritingWOrkbooks BucketCheck(DeskClean-up) FindingMistakesinSchedule Non-Instructional Decoding(Spelling) CreativeWriting. StationWOrk Calendars(FirstofMonth) GradedPapers) WritingSkills PassoutPapers(Returning MusicwithMusicTeacher MathWorkbooks Gym FieldTrip Directions(Listening) MathUnits MathQuizzes MathGames Cooking Math ChoiceTime ChangeJobs LibraryBooks) BlockPatterns(Memory) tainedSilentReadingin ArtwithArtTeacher toClass) Assembly U.S.S.R.(UninterruptedSus- Miscellaneous SilentReading(inReaders) ArtinRoom StoryReading(TeacherReading ScienceUnits ReadingGroups Science ReadingFolders WeeklyReader Read-a-Story(StudentReadingto Class) ReadingLab SnoopySnews SnoopyBooks SocialStudiesUnit SocialStudies PhonicsWOrkbooks Library BookReports Reading 110 Writingskills morethanonemathactivityonMondaysandWednesdays. mathactivityeverydayexceptFriday,anditwasmorecommontohave Therewasatleastone eachdayexceptFridaywhichhadonlyone. Therewereontheaverageatleasttworeading-relatedactivities ulewasillustratedinFigure4.4(p.90)anddiscussedinChapterIV. Thissched- lowedfairlycloselyherweeklyscheduleforwinterterm. Figure5.2andTable5.1generallyindicatethatMrs.Lisafol- graphicallyinFigure5.3. Thissameinformationisportrayed activitytotals(Table5.1). eachdayoftheweekwerecalculatedalongwithdailyandweekly ofactivitiesbysubjectmatter,themeannumberofactivitiesfor Toprovideabreakdown activityduringthisperiodofinstruction. Figure5.2illustratesthefrequencyofeach occurreddaily. activitiessuchasSnoopySnewsandfindingmistakessincethey Omittedfromthesecalculationswere inMrs.Lisa'sclassroom. tabulatedtodeterminethefrequencyanddistributionofactivities activitiesoccurringoverthetwelve-weekperiodofwintertermwas Thenumberofacademic Activityfrequencyanddistribution. wereapartofthescheduleonlyduringspringterm. Read-a-Story,silentreading,bookreports,andremedialhandwriting Thenewspaper, readingfolderswereonlyusedduringwinterterm. Activitiessuchaspost-office,letterwriting,mathgames,and groupartactivitiesintheroomdidnothappenonaregularbasis. occurredeveryotherweek,andactivitieslikepencilpalsandlarge tookplaceatleastonceaweek,thoughartwiththeartteacher Mostoftheotheractivities happenedonlyonceinseveralmonths. 111 S p e l l i n g F o l d e r s S p e l l i n g T e s t W o r d C o n t e s t O r a n g e S u i t c a s e L e t t e r W r i t i n g M i n d b e n d e r S p e l l i n g B e e P e n c i l P a l s ‘ H a n d w r i t i n g W o r k b o o k s ~Lq °£Irep burzznooo serqrnraoe buraqrmoP °sAep Iooqos euru—qurgq q'uornonxasur go porxad xean‘antemq e Burznp mooxsseto 3.9511 °sxw ur eserqrnraoe Ieuorqonxasur go Aouenbezg 2 ° 9 amfim sede qunrqov P a s s O u t P a p e r s S t a t i o n W o r k F i e l d T r i p D i r e c t i o n s C o o k i n g C h o i c e T i m e M u s i c G y m A r t w i t h A r t T e a c h e r B l o c k P a t t e r n s A s s e m b l y S c i e n c e U n i t W e e k l y R e a d e r A r t i n R o o m S n o o p y B o o k s S o c i a l S t u d i e s U n i t C r e a t i v e W r i t i n g M a t h W o r k b o o k s C a l e n d a r s D e c o d i n g M a t h Q u i z e a M a t h U n i t s M a t h G a m e s U . S . S . R . R e a d i n g F o l d e r s R e a d i n g G r o u p s S t o r y R e a d i n g R e a d i n g L a b P h o n i c s W o r k b o o k s . l.— U L i b r a r y ) 1 :E >9 ’5 '9 ’L '8 ’6 >01 '11 he. 2! >51 '91 '91 -91 C L1 81 ~61 >02 >12 ~22 P53 ’92 hSZ >9Z >LZ ~82 r62 Lor. >15 bZE ~€£ “97$ saouazxnooo 33 zeqmnu l— Ll ZTT T o t a l 1 0 . 2 9 ( 6 . 0 0 ) 7 . 6 6 9 . 9 9 8 . 5 7 9 . 8 7 ( 4 . 5 0 ) 4 6 . 3 8 s c h e d u l e . a O m i t t i n g a c t i v i t i e s o c c u r r i n g d a i l y ( S n o o p y S n e w s a n d f i n d i n g m i s t a k e s i n m e n t ) . H a l f — d a y s i n c l u d e d t w o M o n d a y s ( p . m . o n l y ) a n d f o u r F r i d a y s ( a . m . o n l y ) . b 5 9 S C h O O l d a y s , 6 h a l f - d a y s ( o t h e r h a l f - d a y d e s i g n a t e d f o r p r o f e s s i o n a l d e v e l o p - M i s c e l l a n e o u s S c i e n c e a n d S o c i a l S t u d i e s 3 . 2 0 ( 2 . 5 0 ) . 7 7 ( . 5 0 ) 1 . 3 3 . 7 5 3 . 0 8 . 2 5 2 . 2 5 . 8 3 6 . 1 2 ( 3 . 2 5 ) 1 . 0 0 ( . 2 5 ) 1 5 . 9 8 3 . 6 0 R e a d i n g 2 . 6 7 ( . 5 0 ) 2 . 0 0 2 . 6 6 2 . 0 0 1 . 1 2 ( . 0 0 ) 1 0 . 4 5 i n s t r u c t i o n . b M e a n n u m b e r o f a c t i v i t i e é ’ p e r d a y i n e a c h s u b j e c t m a t t e r a r e a d u r i n g a t w e l v e - w e e k p e r i o d o f T a b l e 5 . 1 f u l l ( h a l f ) f u l l ( h a l f ) M o n d a y T u e s d a y W e d n e s d a y , T h u r s d a y F r i d a y w e e k M a t h 1 . 8 8 ( . 0 0 ) 1 . 5 8 2 . 2 5 1 . 3 3 1 . 1 2 ( . 0 0 ) 7 . 0 4 W r i t i n g S k i l l s “ 1 . 7 7 ( 2 . 5 0 ) 2 . 0 0 1 . 7 5 2 . 1 6 1 . 6 3 ( . 5 0 ) 9 . 3 1 ETT areaduringatwelve-weekperiodofinstruction. Meanpercentageofdailyactivitiesineachsubjectmatter Figure5.3 -Misce11aneousActivities -ScienceandSocialStudies m.-WritingSkills x\\\‘-Math -Reading Week Friday Thursday Wednesday Tuesday Monday Activities Of Percent 114 Alsodescribedinthissectionwerethetypes,frequency,and discussingthefunctionthatactivitiesplayasbehaviorsettings. Thenextsectionwillelaborateonthisnotionby forinstruction. hasdiscussedthenotionofactivitiesasstructual"buildingblocks" Thissection tionmightbebestdescribedas"teachingbyactivities." andinstructioninMrs.Lisa'sclassroom,somuchso,thatherinstruc- Tosummarize,activitieswereasalientfeatureoftheplanning latersection. Thetimespentinvariousactivitieswillbecoveredina sameday. minutesthatMrs.Lisamightspendreadingastorytotheclassonthe everyotherTuesdayisrepresentedasoneactivityasisthefifteen Thus,thehourspentwiththeartteacher oftimespentineacharea. totalactivitiesoccurringineachsubjectmatterareangt_theamount tiousininterpretingFigure5.3sinceitrepresentsthepercentageof Onemustbecau- tieswhileFridayhadthelargestnumber(over60%). Tuesdayaveragedthesmallestnumberofthese"miscellaneous"activi- takenupbyactivitiesnotsubsumedunderthesubjectmattergroupings. Ontheaverageaboutthirtypercentoftheday'sactivitywas oftheschedule. herprioritiesthesetwosubjectswouldbethefirsttobesqueezedout scienceandsocialstudies,butMrs.Lisaacknowledgedthatintermsof Therewerefourperiodssetasideinthescheduleeachweekfor week). theentiretwelveweekperiod(justoveroneofeachtypeoflessonper teenscienceunitlessonsandfourteensocialstudiesunitlessonsfor Figure5.2showsonlyfour- occurredlessfrequentlythanscheduled. Scienceandsocialstudiesactivities dayoccurringquitefrequently. activitiesoccurredmorefrequentlythanmathactivities,withtwoper 115 ofsignalingandelicitingbehaviorthatconformstohisorher theteacherisabletostructureactivitiestoincreasetheprobability Throughplanning, createdandcontrolledbytheteacheraheadoftime. andcontrolledbythesetting,butthesettingitselfhasbeenlargely Notonlyisthebehavioroftheteachersignaled behaviorsettings. settings,althoughtheymaybemoreaccuratelydescribedascontrolled Activities,asdefinedinthisstudy,areequivalenttobehavior type. mentisconsistentwithbehaviorinotherenvironmentsofthesame son'sperceptionandthatbehaviortakingplaceinacertainenviron- Hefurtherstatesthatbehaviorsettingsexistindependentofaper- betweenthephysicalcomponentsandthestandingpatternofbehavior. soforth),(3)astandingpatternofbehavior,and(4)interaction tialboundaries,(2)aphysicalmilieuwithprops(books,pencils,and (1)definitetemporalandspa- settingshavefourdistinctfeatures: Kounin(InstituteforResearchonTeaching,1977)statesthatbehavior behavioroccursinecologicalunitsreferredtoasbehaviorsettings. Theyfurtherarguethat thatshapeandsetlimitsontheirbehavior. Doyleproposethattheenvironmentmakescertaindemandsoninhabitants ItwasstatedthatresearcherssuchasBarkerand discussedbriefly. gardingtheinterdependencebetweenbehaviorandtheenvironmentwas InChapterI,thepositiontakenbyecologicalpsychologistsre- ActivitiesasControlledBehaviorSettings ticsoftheseactivitieswillbediscussedinthesectiononfeatures Thecharacteris- distributionofMrs.Lisa'sinstructionalactivities. ofinstructionalactivity. 116 Thoughhisstudyexamineddecisionmakingoveralong inChapterIII. Clarkson's(1962)studyofabanktrustinvestmentofficerdiscussed Theexceptiontothispatternwas periodsoftime(daysorweeks). abletorepresentisproblemsolvingordecisionmakingoverextended Whatthesesimpleproblemshavebeenun- amanageableproblemspace. tailproceduresandmethodsusedt9simplifythetaskenvironmentinto focusingontasksofthistype,theyhavebeenabletodescribeinde- By inafairlyshortperiodoftime(oratleastatonesitting). moveorsolvingacryptarithematicproblem,allofwhichcanbesolved logyhavebeenrelativelysimpleproblemssuchasdecidingonachess Mostofthetasksinvestigatedbyinformationprocessingpsycho- solutionwithinthatspace. tionorcertaincharacteristicsoftheenvironmentandsearchesfora lemsolverordecisionmakercreatesamentalrepresentationofapor- ratherthanconfrontingtheenvironmentandallitscomplexity,aprob- Thus, aresimplifiedandrepresentedinthemindasproblemspaces. humaninformationprocessingcapabilities,complextaskenvironments Informationprocessingtheoryassumesthatbecauseoflimitationsin decisionmakingbetweenthetaskenvironmentandtheproblemspace. tionthathastraditionallybeenmadeinstudiesofproblemsolvingor ignoredbyinformationprocessingpsychologyandobscuresthedistinc- Thistypeofinteractionwiththeenvironmenthasgenerallybeen throughpreactiveplanning. boundariesandguidelineshavealreadybeenestablishedforbehavior islargelyareactiontothepupil'sactionsinthesetting,general Therefore,eveniftheteacher'sbehaviorintheactivity purposes. 117 Throughtherearrangementofthephysical tohavewiththestudents. lyinfluencedthetypeandqualityofinteractionMrs.Lisawasable cludedthewholeclassorspecificgroupsofchildren,groupinggreat- Whetheranactivityin- forinstructioninMrs.Lisa'sclassroom. Variousformsofgroupingalsoinfluencedthetaskenvironment eachactivity. eachweektoactivitiesandbysettingtheamountoftimeprovidedfor subjectinhercurriculumbyallocatingaspecificnumberofperiods ningofeachterm,Mrs.Lisadeterminedtherelativeemphasisoneach Bysettingaweeklyscheduleatthebegin- teachingenvironment-time. Schedulingdealswithoneofthemajorfeaturesofthe classroom. Lisa'sclassroomwerethroughscheduling,grouping,andrearrangingthe ThreecommonwaysthisoccurredinMrs. throughteacherplanning. conceptualizationofthesituationtakesplaceinavarietyofways Thisreshapingofthetaskenvironmentbasedonone'sinitial processisillustratedinFigure5.4. This theexternalenvironmentbasedonthisinternalrepresentation. ficproblemspace,butalsogoesontoactuallychangeandsimplify simplifiesthetaskenvironmentinternallybythinkingwithinaspeci- Inotherwords,theplannernotonly withinwhichheisfunctioning. tomoldthetaskenvironmenttoconformbettertotheproblemspace Whenthishappens,theplannerisattempting inwhichitwilloccur. toshape,accordingtoone'sperceptionsandpurposes,theenvironment Onewaytoguidefutureactionis frameworkforguidingfutureaction. AsdefinedinChapterI,planningisconcernedwithdevelopinga periodoftime(severalmonths),hewasalwayslookingatthesame welldefinedtask. 118 5 . 4 i g u r e F R e s h a p i n g t h e t a s k e n v i r o n m e n t b y p l a n n i n g . T A S K E N V I R O N M E N T T A S K E N V I R O N M E N T S P A C E P R O B L E M . / P L A N N I N G \ . S P A C E P R O B L E M 611 Duration 3. StructureandSequence 2. Location 1. Thesecomponentsare: ininstructionalsettings. ofbehaviorsettingsexpandedtoincludecomponentsespeciallysalient featuresarebasicallyanelaborationofBarker'sandKounin'sfeatures These characterizeinstructionalactivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroom. Inthecourseofthestudy,sevenfeatureswereidentifiedthat analysisofactivityfeatureswasundertaken. standbetterMrs.Lisa'sthinkingaboutinstructionalactivities,an Tounder- theirfunctionascontrolledbehaviorsettingswasproposed. structuralunitofplanningandactioninMrs.Lisa'sclassroom,and Intheprevioussections,activitieswereportrayedasthebasic FeaturesofInstructionalActivities identifiedandwillbediscussedinthenextsection. tionalactivitiessubjecttomanipulationthroughplanninghavebeen withinthecontextofinstructionalactivities,componentsofinstruc- SincemostofMrs.Lisa'sinstructiontakesplace teacherbehavior. mentthatmaybemanipulatedbytheteachertoinfluencestudentand Theseexampleshaveshownthreefeaturesoftheclassroomenviron- geographicboundariesinwhichtheseactivitiestookplace. learningsettingsforvariousactivitiesinfluencedthespatialand theartlabareexamplesofhowherconceptualizationofappropriate differentsettingssuchnastherug,thetablesforsmallgroups,and Herprovisionfor oftaskenvironmentsavailableforinstruction. structureoftheclassroom,Mrs.Lisawasabletoinfluencethetypes 120 activities. and,forsomeactivities,secondarylocationsforeachofMrs.Lisa's Figure5.5listsprimary atvariouslocationsaroundthecommunity. Fieldtripstookplace readinglab,andintheclassroomnextdoor. occurredoutsidetheroomatthelibrary,artroom,gym,musicroom, Activities tivities,thestudentscouldpickwheretheymightsit. Forseveralac- roundtablesoratotherlocationsaroundtheroom. Activitiesalsotookplaceatthe thestudents'seatsandontherug. InMrs.Lisa'sclassroom,themostcommonlocationsforactivitieswere theactivitymighttakeplaceatmanydifferentplacesintheroom. althoughforsomeactivitiessuchaschoicetimeorthewordcontest, activities,locationwasspecifiedthefirsttimetheactivityoccurred Formost oneofthetables,orinanotherlocationinthebuilding. Theactivitymighttakeplaceontherug,atthestudents'seats,at Locationreferstothephysicallocationofanactivity. Location. boratedfurtherbydiscussingeachofthesevenfeatureslistedabove. characteristicsofMrs.Lisa'sinstructionalactivitieswillbeela- Beforeproceedingtothat,however,the secondpartofthischapter. Thisroutinizationofactivitieswillbediscussedinthe routinized. decisionswereonlymadeonceortwiceandtheactivitybecamefixedor Insomeactivities,thesedecisionsweremadequiteoften,butinmost, classroom,planningdecisionsweremadeabouteachofthesefeatures. ItisprOposedthatforeachinstructionalactivityinMrs.Lisa's ContentandMaterials 7. InstructionalMoves 6. AcceptableStudentBehavior 5. Participants ,4. 121 IR-InRoom ‘ R-Rug OR-OutofRoom S-Seats aSecondarylocationsareinparentheses RT-RoundTable LocationsofactivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroom Figure5.5 -ClassroomNextDoor -Playground(IRwhenraining) Movies Recess ArtRoom“' ArtwithArtTeacher -Gym Gym -ReadingLab ReadingLab -MusicRoom -Library Music Library -Gym Assembly OutsidetheClassroom SilentReading BucketCheck StationWOrk ArtinRoom ScienceUnits WeeklyReader SnoopyBooks MathGames WordContest ChoiceTime HolidayParty U.S.S.R. StudentChoice jobboard ChangeJobs-at chalkboard SocialStudiesUnits(R) linesnearfront Cooking SpellingFolders SpellingBee-two ReadingGroups PencilPals OtherLocations AtRoundTables Newspaper(R,RT,OR) OrangeSuitcase SpellingTest Treat Roll PassOutPapers Directions(S) BlockPatterns SnoopySnews RemedialHandwriting Mindbender LetterWriting HandwritingWOrkbooks Decoding CreativeWriting Calendars MathWOrkbooks MathUnits,(R,RT) MathQuizzes FindingMistakes PhonicsWOrkbooks(RT) StoryReading Read-a-Story OntheRug ReadingFolders(RT) BookReports(RT)a AtSeats IntheClassroom 122 terialsandgroupingsinMrs.Lisa'sclassroomgenerallynecessitated tionopeningalessoninGeoffrey'sclassroomsincethevarietyofma- Set-uphereinvolvedmorethanthedirectiveorillustra- musicroom. orevengoingtoanotherplaceinthebuildinglikethelibraryor theroom,rearrangingdesks,pullingdownshadesorprojectionscreens, aspassingoutmaterials,directingstudentstocertainlocationsin Thisincludedsuchthings timewasalmostalwaysinvolvedinset-up. Inbeginninganactivity, classroomincludedthreemajorcomponents. ThegeneralstructureorsequenceofanactivityinMrs.Lisa's ferentfromthestructureofGeoffrey'slessons. ferencesrequireastructureforMrs.Lisa'sactivitiesthatisdif- Thesedif- students,Mrs.Lisawasteachingfirstandsecondgraders. Third,whereasGeoffreywasteachingjuniorhigh usedbyMrs.Lisa. oriented,asopposedtothemanysmallgrouporindividualactivities Second,Geoffrey'slessonsappearedtobelargegroup heractivities. teachingfromtexts,whereasMrs.Lisausedmorevariedmaterialsin First,Geoffreydidmostofhis Geoffrey'sclassroominseveralways. ActivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroomdifferedfromlessonsin "epilogue"occurredintheformofastudyperiodorhomework. ofsummaryorwithanassignmentoftextquestions,andfinally,an Third,thelessonclosedwithsometype ing,orteacherquestioning. sontookplace,involvingsuchthingsasreadingfromthetext,recit- Next,thebodyoftheles- anillustrationbasedoncommonexperience. entry,involvingsometypeofdirectivesuchas"turntopage254"or First,alessoninitiallyinvolvedsometypeofopeningor lesson. activities,SmithandGeoffreyproposefourphasesorepisodesofa Intheiranalysisofdailylessonsas Structureandsequence. 123 Althoughmostactivitiesfollowedthis set-up,lesson,andtake-down. NearlyallofMrs.Lisa'sactivitiesincludedthethreephasesof abreaksuchasrecessorlunch. orparticipantsleadingintotheset-upphaseofanotheractivityorto take-downphaseinvolvedsomerearrangementorregroupingofmaterials Inallactivities,the sheets,folders,orbooksandputthemaway. activities,thisalsoinvolvedhavingoneortwostudentscollectwork- Inmostgroup orworkbooktothebookrackorreturningtoone'sseat. Forothers,itmightonlyinvolvereturningone'stext undertaking. activitiessuchaS'cooking,art,orchoicetime,thiswasoftenamajor Forsome Thethirdphaseofanactivitymaybecalledtake-down. toGeoffrey'slessons. Alsoabsentwastheassignmentofquestionsorhomeworkcommon son. vityratherthanbysummarizingorotherwiseformallyclosingtheles- directivefromMrs.Lisatoproceedintothethirdphaseoftheacti- Moreoftenthannot,thelessonwasclosedbya inreadingormath. mightbemonitoringthegroupasawholeorworkingwithonegroupas Duringthesesessions,Mrs.Lisa sessionsinworkbooksorfolders. Often,however,thelessoninvolvedprivatework Geoffrey'slessons. haviorsuchasreading,reciting,andquestioningthatdominated Forwholeclassorteacher-runactivities,thelessonmayinvolvebe- Oncetheactivityissetup,thesecondorlessonphaseoccurs. activity. theproceduresthatthestudentswereaccustomedtofollowinginan thatthestudentshadbeengivenortoprovideaddendaorchangesin set-upalsoinvolvedgivingdirectionsfortheuseofthematerials Inmanyactivities, morecomplexpreparationsthanOpeningatextbook. 124 Itwillbediscussedinmoredetailin measureactivitycomplexity. 9Teacherroleisintroducedhereasawaytoconceptualizeand thesectiononinstructionalmoves. signedforuseinclassroomobservationinstruments. analysisoffieldnotesinthisstudythancategoriesprimarilyde- Itwasfeltthatitwouldbeeasiertousethistypeofsystemforthe toanalyzefieldnotesandwrittenaccountsofclassroominteraction. nitionswerechosenprimarilybecausetheyweredevelopedforanused 1972;MorineandMorine,1973;Perkins,1964),Gump'scategorydesig- proposed(e.g.,AmidonandHunter,1967:Fishburn,1966;Lundgren, Althoughmanyotherconceptionsofteacherrolehavebeen Figure5.6. Definitionsofthesecategoriesareprovidedin andnon—in-activity. director,recitationleader,instructor-demonstrator,reader,tester, Theyincludewatcher-helper,participant,action tern"(Gump,1967). vityaremodeledafterGump'scategoriesof"teacherleadershippat- ThecategoriesusedtodefineMrs.Lisa'srole(s)duringanacti- rolesdemandedbytheactivityincreases. thatactivitiesbecomemorecomplexfortheteacherasthenumberof Itwashypothesized Lisawouldneedtotakeonduringanactivity. ororientations,thatMrs. 9 intermsofthenumberofdifferentroles, plexityaffectedMrs.Lisa'splanningdecisions,complexitywasdefined Sincethisstudywasconcernedabouthowcom- terials,andsoforth. typesandcomplexityofstudent-teacherinteractions,complexityofma— numbersofteachermoves,numberofstudentbehaviors, differentways: Thecomplexityofanactivitymightbecharacterizedinmany complex. basicsequence,somehadverysimplestructureswhileothersweremore 125 Definitionsforteacher—rolecategories(afterGump,1967) Figure5.6 Tusuallybusywithanotheractivity. lessonphase;Tnotkeytopupilaction. tentlyattendingtoactivityduring Tnothelping,notclearlyandconsis- NotinActivity 8. testingiscoded"tester." "watching"butallteachersupervised Monitoringmayinvolve writtenform. Tcangivequestionseitherorallyorin Tester 7. Treadstopupils. Reader 6. onlyasmallpartoftheteachingeffort. brieflytocheckthemout,butthisis pupilquestions;mayquestionpupils Mayanswer notaskforcontributions. Does Doesnotuserecitationformat. something,whatthefactsare,etc. Ttellsorshowsstudentshowtomake Instructor-Demonstrator 5. modeofteaching. askingforstudentinputisprominent inrecitationformatifquestioningor Codeactivitiesnot swers,mayquiz. Tasksforreciters,commentsonan- RecitationLeader 4. isnotsupplyingthecoreaction. action,ismakingdemandsfordoing,but Tiskeyto asmasterofceremonies. throughoutlessonphase,canleadoract Tgivesdirectionsormanagesactivity ActionDirector 3. pateswithstudentsinactivity. Tisnotleadingactivity,butpartici- Participator 2. callyinvolvedwithactivityaffairs. Ifatdesk,isatleastperiodi- desk. Maycirculate,standatback,orbeat helpingaworkinggroupofindividuals. Tiswatchingover(monitoring)or watcher-Helper 1. (T=Teacher) 126 role)aremoredemandingontheteacherduringplanningandduringthe thatcomplexactivities(activitiesinvolvingmorethanoneteacher- Itseemsreasonabletopostulate ofrolesobservedineachactivity. SeeTable5.2foracompletelistingofthenumber oneteacherrole. duringtheactivity;theremainingtwenty-sixactivitiesinvolvedonly roles,andonlyeightactivitiesinvolvedtwodifferentteacherroles involvedthree occurredinthisperiod,onlythreeactivitiesever Ofthethirty-seventypesofinstructionalactivitythat Figure5.6. periodinwintertermwerecodedusingthecategoriesdefinedin room,thelessonphasesofactivitiesduringarepresentativethreeweek ToassessthecomplexityoftheactivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclass- (1967)analysisofclassroombehaviorsettings. tionofstudentbehaviorinteacherbehaviorwasillustratedinGump's Thisreflec- individualorgroupworknotcenteredaroundtheteacher. thenonecanassumethatthestudentsareinvolvedinsomesortof andinthesamemanner,iftheteacher-roleisthatofawatcher-helper moststudentsarefunctioningasparticipantsinarecitationsession; functioningasarecitationleader,itisreasonabletoassumethat Forinstance,iftheteacheris mirrorimagetotheteacher'srole. Onecanconceiveoftypesofstudentbehaviorasakindof sametime. tionofteacher-rolealsoreflectsstudentbehavioroccurringatthe Second,theno- andonethatcanbeeasilyidentifiedinfieldnotes. Teacher-roleinvolvesamorewholisticunitofbehavior ferentunits.) andBiddle,1974,forareviewofthesedif- (SeeDunkin orventures. unitofteacherbehaviorthanverbalorpedagogicalmoves,utterances, First,teacher-roleseemedtobeamoremeaningful severalreasons. Teacher-rolewaschosenasameasureofactivitycomplexityfor 127 5 623 8 Totals-15 R—Reader -WatcherHelper WH NA-NotinActivity -RecitationLeader RL ID-Instructor/Demonstrator ActionDirector AD— T-Tester P-Participant WH/WH/WH/WH/WH/WH/WH/WH/WH/WH/WH/WH RL/RL/RL/RL/RL/RL/RL/RL/RL/RL/RL/RL SnoopySnews FindMistakes NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/ StationWork AD/AD/AD/ PassOutPapers NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/ Gym NA/NA/NA/ Music ADpWH/ FieldTrip Directions . IDPAD/ Cooking NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/NA/ ChoiceTime AD/ ChangeJobs BlockPatterns Assembly ArtwithArtTeacher ArtinRoom IDRL/RLID/ ScienceUnit RL/ WeeklyReader SnoopyBooks ADRLID/RLWHID/IDRLAD/ SocialStudiesUnit P/P/ WordContest T/T/ SpellingTest wu/wn/ SpellingFolders AD/AD/AD/ SpellingBee IDwu/ PencilPals OrangeSuitcase RL/RL/RL/ Mindbender WH/WH/ LetterWriting ID/ID/ID/ HandwritingWorkbooks wnRL/ Decoding ADWH/WH/ CreativeWriting Calendars WH/WH/ MathWorkbooks ADWH/RLAD/RLT/WHID/ MathUnits T/T/T/T/T/T/T/T/ MathQuizzes ADWH/ MathGames P/P/ U.S.S.R. R/R/R/R/R/R/R/R/R/R/R/ StoryReading WHAD/WHAD/WHAD/ ReadingLab RLR/RL/RL/RL/ ReadingGroups ReadingFolders PhonicsWorkbooks Library I l 1 H H + + H H H H w w H H H H m H w H H H H N H H H N N H N H N H H N H H H 2 + + + + + + + WHPADRLIDRTNA___ andRoles Activity Roles F of TypicalNumber Teacher-rolesobservedinactivitiesduringarepresentativethree weekperiodinwinterterm. .Table5.2 feger,1974;HarnischfegerandWiley,1976:Berliner,§E_31,,1976). (See,forinstance,WileyandHarnisch- gagedtimeortimeontask. discussionsofclassroomlearningtofocusonallocatedtimeanden— Recently,ithasbecomepopularin lengthoftimeanactivitylasts. Thedurationofanactivitysimplyreferstothe Duration. tworoles,andonlyfourpercentinvolvedthreeormore. onlyinvolvedoneteacher-rolewhileonlynineteenpercentinvolved thatapproximatelyseventy-sevenpercentofMrs.Lisa'sactivities Usingthisdefinitionofcomplexity,itwasfound ofteacher-roles. ityofanactivitywasdefinedbythedemandsputonMrs.Lisainterms Thecomplex- set-up,lesson,andtake-down. madeupofthreephases: Tosummarize,activitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroomusuallywere thistwelve-weekperiodweresimpleactivities. Thus,seventy-sevenpercentoftheactivityoccurrencesduring cent. activitieswithtworolesonlybringsthetotaltotwenty-threeper- Includingcomplex complexactivitiesinvolvingthreeteacherroles. vityoccurrencesduringthisperiod,onlytwentyorfourpercentwere Ofthe481acti- placeontheaverageofbetweensixandseventimes. ofeleventimes,andactivitiesinvolvingthreeteacher-rolestook Activitiesusuallyinvolvingtwoteacher-rolesoccurredontheaverage teacher-roleoccurredontheaverageoffourteentimesduringtheterm. Ascanbeseen,activitiesthatinvolvedonlyone showninTable5.3. Theresultsofthesecalculationsare weekperiodofwinterterm. quencyofcomplexandsimpleactivitieswascalculatedforthetwelve- Totestthis,thefre- lessfrequentlythanmoresimpleactivities. thesedemands,thenonemightpredictthatcomplexactivitiesoccur Ifoneoftheaimsofclassroomorganizationistosimplify activity. 129 thethree-weekperiodusedforidentifyingactivitiesassimpleor acalculatedonlyonthethirty-sevenactivitiesthatoccurredduring complex(seeTable 14.26 371 26 Simple 11.25 90 20 complex Complex Activity Occurrences Activities ofRoles role per of of Number Teacher- Occurrences Number Number of Total MeanNumber Frequencyofcomplexandsimpleactivitiesduringatwelve-weekperiod ofinstructioninwinterterm. a Table5.3 130 Ifoneusesthe notaccountedforintheteachers'allocations. Thiserrorisoftenrelegatedtotransitiontime partoftheteacher. timeandactualactivitytimeisduetopoorplanningorerroronthe ontaskbehavior,theyassumethatthedifferencebetweentheallocated Whenresearchersexaminetime theset-upandtake-downtimeinvolved. areallocatingtimeforanactivity,theyareincludingsomenotionof beendocumented,itseemsreasonabletoconjecturethatwhenteachers Thoughithasnot thathasbeenreferredtohereasthelessonphase. notmakethedistinctionbetweenthecompleteactivityandtheportion Mostcontemporarystudiesoftimeallocationandtimeontaskdo Mrs.Lisa'sactivitiesrarelydeviatedfromthescheduledtime. Forthesereasons,thedurationof carriedacrosstheseboundaries. forMrs.Lisa'sactivities,anditwasrareforanactivitytobe framedbytherecessandthelunchbreakprovidedfairlyrigidframes Also,thefourinstructionalblocks herexperiencehadtaughther.) timesandnotcrowdingactivitieshadbeenoneofthemajorthingsthat timesincasualconversations,sheremarkedthatprovidingforthese (Several tivitiestofallshortofortogooverthescheduledtime. Becauseofthis,itwasrareforac- wasbuiltintotheactivities. schedule,itwasarrangedsothatadequateset-upandtake-downtime Second,insettingupthe IVforadescriptionofthisprocess.) (SeeChapter whenMrs.Lisasetupherweeklyschedulefortheterm. First,timeallocationsforactivitiesweremadetoalargedegree Twofactorsmakethispossible. agetimespentineachactivity. servedinthisstudy,althoughitispossibletoapproximatetheaver- reportacomparisonofallocatedandengagedtimeforeachactivityob- Thefieldnotesrecordedinthisstudywerenotofsufficientdetailto 131 rolesweremathunits,creativewritinganddecoding(twentyminutes). Theshortestactivitiesinvolvingtwoteacher previousorientation. Mrs.Lisashiftedorientations,sheusuallydidnotswitchbacktoa Once ashiftingbackandforthbetweenroleswasrarelyencountered. Incodingteacher-rolesforTable5.2, shifttoanotherteaching-role. OnceMrs.Lisawasorientedacertainway,shedidnotquickly ing. inactivitiesinvolvingmultipleroles,therewasnotrapidroleshift- Thisseemstoindicatethatwith- simpleaveragedonlytwentyminutes. tionofforty-fivemdnutes,whilethetwenty-sixactivitieslabeled Thetwelveactivitieslabeledcomplexhadanaveragedura- Table5.2. theaveragedurationofsimpleactivitiesusingthedesignationsin Theaveragedurationofcomplexactivitieswascontrastedwith downbysubjectmatters. Table5.4providesasummaryofactivitydurationsbroken minutes. fifteentothirtyminuterange,averagingalittleovertwenty-five MostofMrs.Lisa'sactivitieswereinthe andfromthefieldnotes. timeswerecomputedfromMrs.Lisa'sweeklyschedules,herplanbook, These allocatedtimeforeachactivityispresentedinFigure5.7, Theapproximate Mrs.Lisaadheredcloselytoherplannedactivities. equipmentfailure,surpriseassemblies,andsoforth,butgenerally Ofcourse,therewereunexpectedoccurrencessuchasfiredrills, boardrarelydeviatedfromactualtimespentinMrs.Lisa'steaching. vitiesintheweeklyscheduleorinthedailyscheduleontheblack- Usingthisbroadernotionofanactivity,timeallocatedtoacti- transitiontimeisabsorbedbytheset-upandtake-downphases. Thisisbecausewhathasbeentraditionallycalled almosttozero. broadernotionofanactivityproposedhere,transitiontimedrops 132 ApproximateallocatedtimesforactivitiesinMrs.Lisa's Figure5.7 classroom 15min. WordContest -10min. SpellingTest 15min. SpellingFolders -15min. SpellingBee -20min. writing RemedialHand- -30min. PencilPals -15min. Treat -15min. OrangeSuitcase -10min. StationWork -20min. Mindbender 2min. - -15min. Roll -15min. LetterWriting Recess -20min. WOrkbooks -15min. PassOutPapers Handwriting -30min. -20min. Music Movies min. U I inSchedule FindingMistakes -20min. HolidayParty -20min. Decoding -30min. Gym 15-30min. CreativeWriting 2-4hours - FieldTrip -20min. Calendars -15min. Directions -40-90min. Cooking WritingSkills 5-20min. - ChoiceTime -15min. ChangeJobs 5min. - BlockPatterns -30min. MathWorkbooks -20—40min. Assembly -20-30min. MathUnits ArtwithArtTeacher-60min. -10min. MathQuizzes -20-50min. ArtinRoom -50min. MathGames Miscellaneous Math -50min. ScienceUnits -30min. U.S.S.R. Science 30min. ReadingLab -20min. SilentReading 15min. ReadingGroups 20min. ReadingFolders -20min. WeeklyReader -15min. Read-a-Story 5min. - SnoopySnews 20min. PhonicsWorkbook -20min. SnoopyBooks -45min. Library 50min. SocialStudiesUnits -30min. BookReports SocialStudies Reading 133 bNotincludingcookingandfieldtrips aRoundedtonearestminute min. 26 AllActivitiesb min. 20 min. 50 min. 24 min. 18 min. 27 min. 25 Miscellaneousb Science SocialStudies WritingSkills Math Reading MeanDuration a SubjectMatter Meandurationofactivitiesbysubjectmatterarea. Table5.4 134 Lisaspecifiedthisbehaviorbeforeanactivitybegan,althoughin Insomeactivities,Mrs. behaviorwasspecifiedineveryactivity. Acceptableandunacceptablestudent Acceptablestudentbehavior. teachingtimesscheduled,andtherestofheractivitieswereconducted asawholeclass. Mrs.Lisahadnoindividual forremedialhandwriting(onegroup). writingskillsactivitygroupingwasforspelling(fourgroups)and Theonly (samegrouping)andformathquizzes(differentgrouping). Inmath,studentsweregroupedformathunitsandworkbooks ders. groups,readinglab,bookreports,phonicsworkbooksandreadingfol- Studentsweregroupedforreadinginreading tainskillsorconcepts. fromonegrouptoanotherastheyimprovedorhaddifficultywithcer- thefirstsessionofanactivity,althoughMrs.Lisadidmovestudents dividedintoseveraldifferentgroups,thiswasusuallydonepriorto Whenstudentswere wholeclassandsomeincludedonlyafewstudents. Someactivitiesincludedthe largelybydecisionsaboutgrouping. Theparticipantsinanactivityweredetermined Participants. wordstoagroupdecodingofasentencewrittenontheboard. accompaniedthetaskshiftfromindividualcodingofone'sownspelling Indecoding,theroleshift fromworkbook-tapetaskstoreadinggames. teacher-rolesoccurringinreadinglabaccompaniedtheshiftintask Forinstance,theregularshiftin sameuntilthetaskwasover. Rolesshiftedwithtasksandremainedthe taskswithinactivities. rolesisthatMrs.Lisa'sroleshiftsseemtoberelatedtoshiftsin durationandcomplexityandthelackofshiftingbackandforthbetween Onereasonforthispossiblerelationshipbetween (fiftyminutes). Theshortestactivityinvolvingthreeroleswassocialstudiesunits 135 wasindicatedbytherarityofhavingtodisruptanactivityto term,Mrs.Lisa'ssuccessinelicitingappropriate(desired)behavior Bywinter someoneisspeaking,andpayingattentiontoinstructions. freedomtoleaveone'sseat,talkinginturn,listeningpolitelywhen Thesewerethegeneralnoiseleveloftheclassduringanactivity, entinthebehaviorsMrs.Lisamostoftenmentionedtothestudents. Theseemphasesareappar- beingpolite,cooperative,andindependent. Thisinvolvessuchthingsas "responsiblecitizens"intheroom. goalsforherstudentsrevolvearoundhelpingthestudentstobecome ManyofMrs.Lisa's somethingwiththeminthewinterandspring." herownwords,"Ittakesallfalltogetthekidstowhereyoucando In ingthestudentstheirrightsandobligationsineachactivity. Muchofhertimeandenergyduringfalltermwaswrappedupinteach- andappropriateformsofinteractionwasacknowledgedbyMrs.Lisa. theimportanceofteachingorlettingthestudentslearnacceptable Interactionwithinactivitieswasnotafocusofthisstudy,but behaviorindifferentculturalgroups. turnsatspeakingandhowdifferentteacherrolesaffectappropriate work(Phillips,1972;Schultz,1976)hasfocusedonhowchildrenget Muchofthis volvedinsocialinteractionssuchasgroupdiscussion. terpretandpredicttheshiftingrightsandobligationsofthosein- Thishasinvolvedanexaminationofhowchildrenlearntoin- room. contextof"participationstructures"(Phillips,1972)intheclass- Ineducation,thishasbeenexaminedinthe petence"(Hymes,1974). bysociolinguistsandanthropologistsasataskofgaining"socialcom- acceptablebehaviorforspecificsocialsituationshasbeenportrayed Thislearningof othersstudentslearneditthroughtrialanderror. 136 Itisimportanttonote recitationleaderorinstructor-demonstrator. helperoraction-directorthanamoreteacher-centeredrole,suchas Mrs.Lisamoreoftentookamorestudent-centeredroleasawatcher- Thefrequencyoftheteacher-rolesinTable5.5indicatesthat notesduringotherweeksrevealedalmostnodeviationsintheseroles.) (Aspot-checkofthefield herorientationsthroughoutwinterterm. ingthisperiod,onecanconfidentlyassumethattheserolesrepresent BecauseoftheapparentstabilityofMrs.Lisa'srolesdur- Table5.5. teacherrolesinactivitiesduringthisperiodissummarizedin Thefrequencyofvarious thatMrs.Lisahadinvariousactivities. sibletogetsomepictureofthevarietyandfrequencyoforientations rolescalculatedtodeterminethecomplexityofanactivity,itispos- Fromthetabulationsofteacher- onherinteractiveteachingbehavior. instructionalstrategiesorstyle,nodetailedanalysiswasconducted SincethisstudywasnotprimarilyinterestedinMrs.Lisa's roles. instructionalbehaviorwasrepresentedearlierintermsofteacher— cludedacombinationofseveralormanyofthesemoves,Mrs.Lisa's Sincemostactivitiesin— tion,monitoring,evaluation,andfeedback. instructionweregivinginstructions,questioning,presentinginforma- SomeofthemovesmostapparentinMrs.Lisa's inmanydifferentways. Withinanactivity,theteachermayperform atachievingsomeoutcome. Thisbehaviorischaracterizedasinstructionalmovesdirected tivity. theothercomponentnecessarytotheparticipationstructureofanac- Mrs.Lisa'sbehaviorinanactivityprovided Instructionalmoves. look,orawordwouldterminateundesirablebehavior. understoodbythestudentssothatsimplecuessuchasagesture,a Appropriatebehaviorseemedtobewellenough disciplineastudent. 137 aSeetextforanexplanationofhowthiswasestimated. 22.7 20 2.7 3.1 4 7 3.2 10 27.8 23.1 0.2 17.2 13 17 11 18 5 3 1 6 8 11 6 14 Not-in-Activity Tester Reader Demonstrator Instructor- RecitationLeader ActionDirector Participant Watcher-Helper (Gump,1967) Time Teacher-Role Role ofTime of of Percent Percenta Frequency FrequencyofMrs.Lisa'steacher-rolesduringathree-weekperiodof instruction. Table5.5 138 timesreportedforGump'steachersandmaybeasmuchafunctionof Thisisfairlycloseto structingorleadingarecitationsection. studentactioninanactivityasoppostedto23%spentactivelyin- Thirty-fivepercentofMrs.Lisa'stimewasspentdirectingorwatching asaparticipant,instructor,demonstrator,reader,andtester. Mrs.Lisaspentlesstimeasarecitationleader,butspentmoretime fourroles,whereasMrs.Lisa'srolesaremuchmoreevenlydistributed. isthatGump'steachershave91%oftheiractivitiesconcentratedin AnobviousdifferencebetweenGump'steachersandMrs.Lisa Table5.5. teachersspentintheteacher-rolesislistedinthethirdcolumnof Thepercentoftimethese classroomdayandteacherandpupil"acts." graphs,andamultifacetedcategorysystemobserving"segments"ofthe teachersfortwo-dayseach,usingspecimenrecords,time-lapsephoto- Gump(1967)observedsixthird-grade similarnotionsofteacher-role. ingthisperiod,acomparisoncanbemadewithanotherstudyusing Byestimatingthepercentoftimespentineachteacher-roledur- anaveragetimewasused. Foractivitydurationswherearangeoftimewasgiven, rateenough.) timeintheactivity,butforestimationpurposes,itshouldbeaccu- (Thisassumessomewhatincorrectlythateachrolehadequal ofroles. involvedmorethanonerole,thetotaltimewasdividedbythenumber Foractivitiesthat rencesduringthethree-weekperiod(Table5.2). propriatedurationoftheactivity(Figure5.7)bythenumberofoccur- teacher-role,eachroleoccurrencewasweightedbymultiplyingtheap- Toprovidearoughestimateofthepercentoftimespentineach butonlythenumberoftimeseachrolewasidentifiedinanactivity. thatthesecountsdonotindicateamountoftimespentineachactivity 139 themorpagescouldbetornout-afrequentconsiderationwith notthematerialswereconsumable(whetherthestudentscouldwriteon theattractivenessandworkmanshipofthematerials,(3)whetheror text,ditto,workbook,manipulatables,games,films,orkits),(2) Thesefactorsare(l)theformatofmaterials(e.g., ingaloud.") offactorsthatrepeatedly"surfaced"indiscussionsandinher"think- orweightingofthesecues,andthislistisbasedonconsiderations (Nodesignwasimplementedthatproducedanestimateoftheimportance werefrequentlyusedwhenMrs.Lisajudgedtheusefulnessofmaterials. TeacherPlanningShell,eightfactorsor"cues"wereidentifiedthat teachingmaterialsandthroughmorecontrolledobservationinthe ThroughdailyobservationsofMrs.Lisa'sjudgmentandselectionof junctionwithsomematerials,thesedecisionswerecloselyintertwined. tentofherinstructionwasnearlyalwaysdeliveredthroughorincon- Sincecon- activity-relateddecisionsMrs.Lisamadeinherplanning. .Decisionsaboutcontentandmaterialswerethemostfrequent taughtandhowitwouldbepresented. periodsofinstructionnecessitatednewdecisionsaboutwhatwouldbe majorcomponentofMrs.Lisa'sweeklyanddailyplanningsincemost Thesetypesofdecisionswerea shewouldprovideforthestudents. contentthatwouldbecoveredduringalessonanddecidewhatmaterials Withinthisframework,Mrs.Lisahadtodeterminespecific vities. ofanactivitywassetbythesubjectmatterorientationofmostacti- Thegeneralcontent tifiedinthisanalysiswascontentandmaterials. Theseventhactivitycharacteristiciden- Contentandmaterials. thenatureofteachinginearlyprimarygradesasaparticularteach- ingstyle. 140 Seven functionascontrolledbehaviorsettingsinherteaching. asstructuralunitsinplanninghasbeendescribedaswellastheir Theroleofactivities structionaltechnology-teachingbyactivities. Thusfar,thischapterhasdescribedoneaspectofMrs.Lisa'sin- Summagy mostprominentinMrs.Lisa'splanningisprovidedinthenextchapter. Asomewhatmorecompletepictureofwhenthesecueswere described. study,therelativefrequencyandimportanceofthesefactorswerenot Becauseofthescopeandnatureofthis studyofteacherjudgment. mentsofcontentandmaterialscouldbeasubjectofamoredetailed Eachofthecuesidentifiedasbeingrelatedtojudg- andactivities. tialthatthecontenthadforintegrationwithothercontents,lessons, tentwithhergoals(andthedistrictobjectives),and(7)thepoten- presentation(e.g.,lesson,film,ortask),(6)the"fit"ofthecon- knowledge,strategy,etc.),(4)meaningfulness,(5)lengthofcontent orderofcontentpresentation,(3)emphasisofthecontent(skill, (1)difficultylevel,(2)sequenceand Theseinclude: ofMrs.Lisa. aboutcontent,sevenmorefactorswereidentifiedasfrequentconcerns Inmakingdecisions materialsortoborrowideasandmakeherown. thenitwasnotuncommonforMrs.Lisatocombineseveraldifferent Ifmaterialsfailedtomeetherstandardsintoomanyareas, content. wouldbeenoughmaterialsforagrouporforthewholeclass,and(9) terials,(7)theset-uptimeinvolvedforher,(8)whetherornotthere (6)whetherarecordingorevaluationsystemwasprovidedwiththema- serveherpurposes,(5)theclarityofinstructionsforthestudents, workbooks),(4)whethertheycouldbeeasilymodifiedoradaptedto 141 performanceprograms. undertakenbyMarchandSimon(1959)wheretheyrefertoroutinesas Themostextensivetreatmentofroutineswas businessorganizations. routineshavebeenrarelystudiedandusuallyonlyinthecontextof However,theroleandfunctionof actionandbehaviorarerepetitive. tinesareanefficientandcommonmodeofOperationinsituationswhere Rou- tionistocontrolandcoordinatespecificsequencesofbehavior. Routinesareestablishedprocedures,orprograms,whosemainfunc- tines. todealwiththemanyactivitiesusedintheclassroom,namely,rou- ThepurposeofthissectionistodiscussamechanismMrs.Lisaused fiedasthebasicstructuralunitofMrs.Lisa'splanningandteaching. Inthefirstpartofthischapter,activitieswereidenti- vironment. thecomplexityandtoincreasethepredictabilityoftheteachingen- ItwassuggestedinChapterIthatplanningfunctionstoreduce RoutinesinTeaching PartII: secondpartofthischapter. activitiesandtosimplifytheplanningtaskwillbediscussedinthe AmechanismusedbyMrs.Lisatoestablishandregulatethese ning. orterm,andnotallofthesedecisionswerepartofheron-goingplan— aboutsomefeaturesweremoreprominentatcertaintimesoftheyear Decisions abouteachofthefeaturesofanactivitymentionedabove. Mrs.Lisa'splanningforinstructioninvolvedmakingdecisions scribedasconcernsinMrs.Lisa'splanning. featuresofinstructionalactivitieshavebeenidentifiedandde- 142 wasdevotedtoteachingstudentsthestructureandsequenceof Mrs.Lisasaidthatmuchoftheclassroomtimeinthefall degree. AllactivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroomwereroutinizedtosome componentsofanactivityaspossible. ofactivitiesusedinherclassroomwasbyroutinizingasmanyofthe ThewayMrs.Lisawasabletomanagethelargenumber imposingtask. componentsforeveryactivityduringagivendayorweekwouldbean Tomakeadecisionabouteachofthese (7)contentandmaterials. pants,(5)acceptablestudentbehavior,(6)instructionalmoves,and (1)location,(2)structureandsequence,(3)duration,(4)partici- componentsofactivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroomwereidentified: Earlierinthechapter,seven planningandteachinginherclassroom. establishingactivityroutinesthatMrs.Lisasimplifiedthetasksof Itwasprimarilyby ordinatethecomponentsofclassroomactivities. Activityroutinesfunctiontocontrolandco- Activityroutines. Eachoftheseroutineswillbediscussedinturn. tines,(3)managementroutines,and(4)executiveplanningroutines. (1)activityroutines,(2)instructionrou- planningandteaching: wereidentifiedinMrs.Lisa's Fourdifferenttypesofroutine TypesofTeachiggRoutine lection,organizationandsequencingofroutines. planningforMrs.Lisalargelyinvolveddecisionmakingaboutthese- Thus, abilityandreducingthecomplexityoftheteachingenvironment. routinesfunctionedasthemajormechanismforincreasingthepredict- Inthisclassroom, teacher'sthinkingandplanningforinstruction. Routinesplayamajorroleinclassroomorganizationandinthe 143 workbookandthenwalkstothefrontoftheclass. tellsthemthatthey'reonpagefourteenintheir She gettoyourseats,quietforachange." saystothewholeclass,"Let'sseeifyoucan eralkidsareshss-ingeachother,andMrs.Lisa Sev- Thekidsgobacktotheirseats. itup." beginstotalkandMrs.Lisatellshimto"zip Ed namesandhandsoutthehandwritingbooks. Mrs.Lisagoestotheplatform,callsthekids' 9:15 2/2/77 instructionalmoveswerepartofanestablishedroutine. structure,theduration,acceptablestudentbehavior,andtheteacher's mosttwomonthsapartillustratethedegreetowhichthesequenceand Thefollowingdescriptionsoftwohandwritinglessonsdoneal- book. groupactivitydoneatthestudents'seatsusingthehandwritingwork- Thiswasalarge 9:30duringwinterterm,handwritingtookplace. Tuesday,wednesday,andThursdaymorningsfromapproximately9:15to On Anexampleofawellroutinizedactivityishandwriting. teaching. littleornotimewasspentdealingwiththemduringplanningor establishedandunderstoodbyboththeteacherandthestudents,and Theothercomponentsoftheactivitieswere contentandmaterials. Lisa'splanningandinteractivedecisionmakingdealtprimarilywith sothat,formostreading,math,andwritingskillsactivities,Mrs. theactivitiesinMrs.Lisa'sclassroomwerewellroutinized-50much Bythebeginningofwintertermwhenthisstudybegan,mostof standingofthesefeatures. ofanactivitywasnotdependentonthestudent'sawarenessorunder- instructionalmovesweretheteacher'sresponsibility,andthesuccess Componentssuchasduration,contentandmaterials,and changed. andparticipantsforactivitieswereeasilylearnedandoftenwere Location activitiesandacceptablestudentbehaviorineachsetting. 144 Lisafinishedandheadstothefrontoftheroom. Mrs. theirseatsandbeginopeningtheirbooks. Thekidsreturnto turntopagethirty-eight. thehandwritingbooksandtellsthekidstheycan kids'namesandastheycomeup,shepassesout Mrs.Lisathencallsthe onSuzie'sexplanation. Mrs.Lisaskimsthroughthearticleandelaborates Universityisusingsolarheatingtoheatit. andshowsapictureabouthowthehatcheryatState Shecomesuptotheplatform uptotheplatform. ThekidsfinishandMrs.LisaasksSuzietocome 9:04 3/29/77 ished,shewillpickuptheirfolders. Mrs.Lisatellsthekidsthatwhenthey'refin- throughthefoldersthatMrs.Lisahadgivenher. Sandysitsdownontheplatformandglances 9:28 onthelineandonlytwoiftheycannotfitthree. Shethentellsthemtowritethreethings swerit. question,andthencallsonvariouskidstoan- ShecallsonDonatoreadthesecond ofthem.) shesaidthattherewasn'troomtowriteallfour (InlookingovertheseonMonday, choosetwo. swers(fouranimals)andMrs.Lisatellsthemto Thekidsgivefourcorrectan- toanswerit. Thensheasksthekids readthefirstquestion. ShecallsonTomto havetoansweronthispage. therearetwoquestionsthatthey'regoingto Shetellsthekidsthat thefrontoftheclass. Mrs.Lisathenwalksbackto' withthemtoday. folderstoSandyandexplainswhatshe'sdoing Shegetsthereading towriteoneoftheirown. andtellsthekidstotracethelettersandthen Mrs.Lisawalkstothebackoftheroom room. Sandy(theaide)entersthe onthenextpage. Shetellsthemtoputtheirinitialsandthedate 9:23 theairisnotblowingstraightintotheroom. shadesonthewindowsthatshe'dopened,sothat Shethenwalkstothewindowandpullsdownthe ontowrite"akangaroo,afox,andazebra." Shegoes they'regoingtowritesomethingelse. remindsthemtoputacomma,notaperiod,since Shewrites,"Wesawawalrus,"and workbooks. Thekidscopyitintotheir tenceontheboard. linestheblackboardandthenwritesthesen- She Thensheexplainshowtodotheexercises. askstheclasstoreadthesentenceonthepage. She malsinthepicturesintheirworkbook. Mrs.Lisaasksquestionsabouttheani- class. overandopenstwowindowsatthefrontofthe Shethenwalks ofthepageandthedate2/2. Shetellsthemtoputtheirinitialsonthetop 145 Thistableindicates Lisa'sclassroomisillustratedinTable5.6. TheextenttowhichotheractivitieswereroutinizedinMrs. thekidsfinish,SuzieandDonapickupthebooks. As seewhattheycomeupwithfortheirendings. kidsthatshe'llbeinterestedtoreadtheseto Mrs.Lisatellsthe saidthatshe'sfinished). finished,tohandtheirbookstoSuzie(whojust Mrs.Lisatellsthekidsthatifthey're spelling. she'llhelpthemiftheyneedhelpwiththeir andpenciloffherdeskandtellsthekidsthat Shethengetsapad tenceshastoendwith23. tellsthekidsthatthelastwordintheirsen- Mrs.Lisa withthelastwordinthephrase(cat). theboardandthenwritessomewordsthatrhyme Shewritesitupon canfinishitontheirown. partoftherhymeonpagethirty-nineandthenthey Shetellsthekidstocopythefirst thenextpage. onestheywantandthenthey'llwritearhymeon Whendone,Mrs.Lisatellsthekidstochoosethe 9:20 andalsotwelvetorhymewiththelastword(lake). Thekidsgeneratetwelvetorhymewiththisword theirlinessincethiswordislonger(light). togettwowordsrhymingwiththefourthwordon Shetellsthemthattheymayonlybeable words. Forthenextword(tree)thekidslisttwelve thewordsontheboardforeachwordinthebook. shewouldlikeforthemtowriteatleastthreeof Shetellsthemthat goesontothenextword. Shewritesdownsixofthem,andthen herthese. word(sun)andthekidsraisetheirhandstogive Shethengoestothenext havequiteachoice. she'sgoingtostopthereandsaysthatthekids wordsforthefirstone(day),Mrs.Lisasays Afterseventeenrhyming providedintheworkbook. Thekidswritetwoorthreeinthespace board. thekidssaythem,Mrs.Lisawritesthemonthe they'redoingtodayasksforrhymingwordsandas Thelessonthat thengoesuptothefrontboard. She oftheotherworkbooksandgivesittoher. backtothebookcaseandtearsthepagefromone Shegoes withherlastweekwhenshewasgone. thatitwasprobablyintheworkthatshesent Mrs.Lisagoesoverandlooksatitandsays page. Loissaysthatshedoesn'thavethat blackboard. Mrs.Lisagoesupandlinesthefront tohim. Andysaysthatit'shis,andMrs.Lisagivesit Afteraboutthreeseconds, lostawholesection. Someoneseemstohave theyhavepagesixty-one. andasksthekidstochecktheirbookstoseeif Mrs.Lisaseessomeworkbookpagesonthefloor 146 470045l4 TotalBlanks + + + + + + + + + + + O ‘ N w N N J ‘ N I N I U ‘ I N ‘ Q N M ‘ J V N I M N N O ‘ N N O ‘ N N U N O ‘ M N V N N Q O ‘ O ‘ O ‘ N + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + +++ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + ‘ + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + **-activitiesdesignatedas non-routine established. oncetheweeklyscheduleis thatareroutinizedor"set" +-indicatesfeaturesofactivities StationWork PassoutPapers **FieldTrip **Cooking ChoiceTime ChangeJobs BlockPattern ArtinRoom **Science ++ WeeklyReader **SocialStudies + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + ¥ SnoopySnews SnOOpyBooks WordContest SpellingTest SpellingFolders SpellingBee PencilPals OrangeSuitcase Mindbender LetterWriting HandwritingWorkbooks FindMistakes Decoding CreativeWriting Calendars MathWOrkbooks **MathUnit MathQuizes MathGames U.S.S.R. StoryReading ReadingLab ReadingGroups ReadingFolders PhonicsWorkbooks Library f i x e d f e a t u r e s T o t a l n u m b e r o f D u r a t i o n S t r u c t u r e L o c a t i o n P a r t i c i p a n t s S t u d e n t B e h a v i o r C o n t e n t & M a t e r i a l s I n s t r u c t i o n a l M o v e s Routinizationofactivitiesintheweeklyscheduleforwinterterm. Tadile55.6 147 flectedlessoftheroutinizationoftheactivityandmoreofits Thelessonphaseofanactivityre- longerforMrs.Lisatosetup. Non-routineactivities,asaresult,took materialstobeused. location,structure,acceptablestudentbehaviors,andthecontentand hand,moretimehadtobespentspecifyingandexplaingsuchthingsas turesofnon-routinedactivitieswerenotknowntothestudentsbefore- Sinceasmanyasfivefea- ing)wereestablishedinallactivities. ataminimum,thedurationofanactivityandtheparticipants(group— Table5.6showsthat noveltyorlackofroutinizationofanactivity. numberofteacherandstudentbehaviorsinthisphasereflectedthe Anincreaseinthe visibleduringtheset-upphaseofanactivity. Routinizationorlackofroutinizationofanactivitywasmost thatwouldbecoveredduringanactivity. wereused,planningdecisionsmightinvolvetheselectionofpages Incasesinwhichtextsorworkbooks thatthestudentsmightneed. uallyonlyhadtodealwiththecontentofthelessonandanymaterials tinizedactivitiesrequiringweeklyordailyplanning,Mrs.Lisaus- Whatthesepatternsindicateisthatforrou- vitiesscoredfive). terialsandstructureoftheactivitytobeleftopen(thetwoacti- leftopen(thesixteenactivitiesscoredsix)orforcontentandma- 5.6),themostcommonpatternwasforthecontentandmaterialstobe whosefeatureswerenotcompletely"set"(ascoreofseveninTable Forroutinizedactivities unitactivities,cooking,andfieldtrips. cludemathunitactivities,socialstudiesunitactivities,science Thesein- activities. fewersetfeaturesaredesignedasnon-routine Activitieswiththreeor oncetheweeklyschedulewasestablished. foreachactivitythenumberoffeaturesthatwereroutinizedor"set" 148 mainedagroupofcomplexactivitiesthatwasstillnotroutinized However,bywinterterm,therere- soweremanycomplexactivities. Allsimpleactivitieswereeasilyroutinizedand goriestwoandfour. astheschedulebecameestablished,mostactivitiesmovedintocate- year,allofheractivitiesfellintocategoriesoneandthree,and, Thiswouldmeanthatatthebeginningofthe teachingaspossible. mandsonherplanning,andteachingbyroutinizationasmuchofher sizeabasicgoalinMrs.Lisa'splanningofstrivingtoreducede- OnewaytoaccountfortheemptycellinFigure5.9istohypothe- simple/non-routine. Bywinterterm,noactivitiescouldbeclassifiedas plex/routine). vitiesfallintocategoriesthree(complex/non-routine)andfour(com- Theremaining30%oftheacti- activitiesfallintothiscategory. Twenty-sixor70%ofthe thesimple/routinecategory(categorytwo). tivitiesillustrated,amajorityofMrs.Lisa'sactivitiesfallinto Ofthefourtypesofac- byFigure5.8ispresentedinFigure5.9. inwhichMrs.Lisa'sactivitiesfallintothefourcategoriescreated Theway activitiesarecharacterizedbytheirshorterset-upphase. characterizedbylongerandmoreelaboratelessonphaseswhileroutine Complexactivitiesare onthestructureanddurationofanactivity. Figure5.8illustratestheeffectsofroutinizationandcomplexity itsspecialfeatureswereneededaftertheset-upphase. activitywasbetterunderstoodbythestudents,fewerremindersabout Asan ingsomefunctionsoftheset-upphaseintothelessonphase. Mrs.Lisaoftenhadtorepeatinstructionsduringthelesson,carry- Thiswasduetothefactthat theactivitybecamemoreroutinized. However,therewassomeshrinkageinthelessonphaseas complexity. 149 F i g u r e 5 . 8 E f f e c t s o f c o m p l e x i t y a n d r o u t i n i z a t i o n o n t h e s t r u c t u r e a n d d u r a t i o n o f a n a c t i v i t y . T - — T a k e - d o w n p h a s e L - L e s s o n p h a s e S - S e t - u p p h a s e C O M P L E X I T Y C o m p l e x S L T i s L T 051 S i m p l e s L ' 1 ' S L T N o n - r o u t i n e R o u t i n e R O U T I N I Z A T I O N DistributionofMrs.LiSa'sactivitiesondimensionsof Figure5.9 complexityandroutinization (16%oftotal) Pencilpals‘ Decoding Creativewriting Mathgames (14%oftotal) Fieldtrips Cooking Mathunitactivities activities‘ Readinglab Socialstudiesunit Library Scienceunitactivities Complex/Non-routine Complex/Routine 4. 3. (70%oftotal) codedinTable6 Restofactivities Noactivities Simple/Non-routine Simple/Routine 2. 1. 151 planningforandteachingunitsinmath,socialstudies,andscience thisamountedtowasthatbythemiddleofwinterterm,Mrs.Lisawas What werequicklymetinthefallandthefirstpartofwinterterm. Also,mostofthescienceandsocialstudiesobjectives thematerials. andthoughtherewerekitsandmaterialsavailable,thereweregapsin IVdiscussedthematerialsusedinscience,socialstudies,andmath, Chapter lished,preparedmaterialsthatwouldmeettheseobjectives. posedbyhavingobjectivessetbythedistrictandnothavingpub- saidthattheunitformatwasbasicallyinresponsetothedilemma She taughtinthisformat,Mrs.Lisagavethefollowingrationale. Whenaskedwhyscience,socialstudiesandmathwere andspringterms. decisioncommittedhertomoreplanningfortheseareasduringwinter This tinized,butMrs.Lisachosetoteachthemusingaunitformat. Alloftheseactivitiescouldhavebeenrou- arealltaughtinunits. Onefactorthatthesethreeactivitieshaveincommonisthatthey Lisa'steaching,mathwashighonherlist. AlthoughscienceandsocialstudieswereofalowerpriorityinMrs. andmathunitactivitiesoccurringalittleovertwotimesaweek. studiesactivitieseachaveragingalittleoveroneoccurrenceaweek wereregularlyoccurringscheduledactivitieswithscienceandsocial Figure5.2showsthattheseactivities ismoredifficulttoexplain. Thepresenceofscience,socialstudies,andmathunitactivities highcomplexityseemedtokeeptheseactivitiesoutofcategoryfour. Thus,lowfrequencycoupledwith needoreasywaytoroutinizethem. isdifferent,andtheseactivitieshappensorarelythatthereisno Eachcookingactivityandfieldtrip categoryareeasytoexplain. Thepresenceofcookingandfieldtripsinthis (categorythree). 152 Further,itislikelythat andvarietyofmovesforanyoneteacher. Itmakessensetohypothesizethatthereisafinitenumber (moves.) Withineachrole,thereareseveraldifferentbehaviors behavior. rolewasofferedasameaningfulunitinwhichtocharacterizeteacher Inthefirstpartofthischapter,teacher- figurationsandsequences. teachingthatovertimehavebeendevelopedandoccurinregularcon— Theseroutinesareineffectstrategiesorstylesof tionalmoves. proceduresestablishedbytheteachertocarryoutspecificinstruc- Instructionalroutinesaremethodsand Instructionalroutines. andthestudents. preparingunitsofspecialimportanceorofspecialinteresttoher objectivesandbecauseofMrs.Lisa'swillingnesstospendextratime becauseofthelackofpreparedmaterialsthatwouldmeetallofthe Science,socialstudies,andmathunitactivitieswerenotroutinized Theonlyfairlystandardcomponentswereparticipantsandduration. units,Mrs.Lisahadtomakedecisonsaboutmostactivitycomponents. Inplanningactivitiesforthese socialstudies,andmathunits. Thiswasprimarilyplanningforscience, thatwerenotroutinized. duringwinterandspringtermswastakenupbyplanningforactivites MostofMrs.Lisa'splanningtime andmanagementwhenbeingtaught. wereroutinizedandrequiredlittleplanningandlittletimeinset-up Tosumup,bywintertermmostofMrs.Lisa'sactivities(86%) timeinplanningfortheseareas. sonswerethebasicreasonsforMrs.Lisa'swillingnesstoinvestmore Thiscommitmenttotheimportanceorinterestoftheseles- teaching. thoughtthestudentswereespeciallyinterestedin,and(3)sheenjoyed that(1)shethoughtthestudentsespeciallyneededtoknow,(2)she 153 groups,instructionsweregenerallygivenonlyonce,andMrs.Lisa Insmall andoftenoneofthestudentswasaskedtorepeatthem. Inlargegroups,instructionswereusuallygiventwoorthreetimes Mrs.Lisa'smethodforgivingisntructionsdifferedwithgroupsize. Asanotherexample, minuteorsoandglanceatthestudents'papers. tenceortwoontheboard,shewouldwalkaroundtheclassroomfora AfterMrs.Lisahadwrittenasen- theblackboardontotheirpapers. andSnoopybookswherethestudentswereoftencopyingsomethingoff largegroupactivitiessuchashandwritingbooks,spellingfolders, Forinstance,Mrs.Lisausedthesamemonitoringroutinefor similar. Thiswasmostcommonacrossactivitiesthatwere ferentactivities. ManyofMrs.Lisa'sinstructionalroutineswereusedwithindif- androleswereroutinizedaswell. Thus,astheactivitywasroutinized,theteacher'smoves modified. toring,reviewing,andquestioningthatwereestablishedandrarely movessuchasgivinginstructions,demonstrating,instructing,moni- Mrs.Lisahadalargerepertoireofinstructional activityroutines. Lisa'sclassroom,instructionalroutineswereestablishedcomponentsof SinceinstructiontookplacemainlywithinactivitiesinMrs. instructionalactivities. teacher-rolesthatareroutinelyemployedinconjunctionwithspecific routinescanbecharacterizedaspatternsofmovesforcarryingout Giventhesebasicconceptions,instructional somemovesoverothers. rolescanbeportrayedascollectionsofteachingmovesemphasizing Thus,teacher- repeatedwithinmanydifferentactivitiesorroles. tosimplifyplanningandpreparation,successfulbehaviorsormovesare 154 Sincemostmanagementroutinesdidnotinvolvethewholeclass, somecases,thetimeandlocationatwhichitistobecarriedout. accomplished,thestepsandsequenceinwhichitistobedone,andin Proceduresspecifywhatistobe nents:proceduresandparticipants. Managementroutinesincludetwomajorcompo- morningorafterlunch. leavingtheroom,cleaninguptheroom,andstartingschoolinthe transitionbetweenactivities,passingoutorcollectingmaterials, room,managementroutinescontrolledandcoordinatedsuchthingsas InMrs.Lisa'sclass- haviornotassociatedwithspecificactivities. duresforcontrollingandcoordinatingclassroomorganizationandbe- Managementroutinesareestablishedproce- Managementroutines. carriedoutaspartoftheinstructor-demonstratorrole. instructing,demonstrating,monitoring,andevaluatingwereroutinely strate,thendoseveralfiguresasaclass),andthespecificmovesof somebasicdeliberationaboutherinstructionalstrategy(e.g.,demon- paperfolding)activityforhersocialstudiesunit,therewasonly Forinstance,whenMrs.Lisaplannedtheorigami(Japanese task. formedasanestablishedresponsetoaspecifictypeofinstructional Mrs.Lisa'sinstructionalmoveswerehighlyroutinizedandwereper- Basedonthesetwoobservations,itseemsthat moveswereapparent. Whennewactivitieswereimplemented,fewneworunusualinstructional theinstructionaltaskitselfratherthanhowitwouldbetaught. foranewactivity,mostofMrs.Lisa'sattentionwasdirectedtowards Whenplanning Lisa,usuallyonlywhenanactivitywasnotgoingwell. DecisionsaboutinstructionalmoveswererarelymentionedbyMrs. reliedonwatchingeachstudent'sworktodeterminewhetherthein- structionshadbeenunderstood. 155 Toavoidthetalking,shoving,and ortogohomeaftershcool. roomtogotoanotherroominthebuilding,togotorecessorlunch, usedsinceitwassetintomotionwhenevertheclasswasleavingthe Thiswasfrequently amanagementroutineinMrs.Lisa'sclassroom. Theroutineestablishedforleavingtheroomisagoodexampleof setting. tributedtothesmoothfunctioningoftheclassroomasalearning ThisnotonlysimplifiedtheteachingtaskforMrs.Lisa,butalsocon- complexbehaviorsequencesoftenbyonlythementionofseveralwords. setupbythejobsystemenabledMrs.Lisatosetinmotionrather Themanagementstructure toastudenttheprocedurefordoingajob. havingtodomanythingsherselfandfromhavingtostopandexplain ThisroutinizationofbasicmanagementneedsfreedMrs.Lisafrom timesduringtheday,butotherswerecarriedoutasneeded. Somejobswereonlydoneatset tingthestudentstothispoint. However,Mrs.Lisasaidthatmuchoffalltermwasspentget- Lisa. jobentailedandcouldcarryitoutwithlittleguidancefromMrs. Bythetimeofthisstudy,nearlyeverystudentknewwhateach als. attendancesliptotheofficeorhelpingcollectandpassoutmateri- chairsonthesmallgrouptablestohelpingjobssuchastakingthe rangedfromclean-upjobssuchaserasingtheboardorputtingupthe These forajobinvolvingsomemanagementaspectoftheclassroom. cussedinthelastchapter,eachweekeverystudentwasresponsible Asdis- Lisa'sclassroomwastheassignmentofjobstothestudents. ThemostprominentframeworkformanagementroutinesinMrs. tocarryouttheroutine. individualsorgroupsofindividualswerespecifiedasparticipants 156 couldquicklychecktoseeifeveryonehadturnedinhisorherwork. tem,Mrs.Lisahadallofthecompletedassignmentsinoneplaceand Asaresultofthissys- filesthatMrs.Lisakeptnearhertable. studentswererequiredtoturntheirworkintotheir"work-done" Tosimplifyhergradingprocedures,forinstance, activesetting. minimizedtheworkshehadtodoinboththepreactiveandinter- Lisaestablishedmanyroutinesnotassignedtospecificstudentsthat Mrs. Notallmanagementroutinesinvolvedthestudents'jobs. dentsinaline. outtheadditionaldemandsbroughtonbyhavingtomonitorthestu- Moreimportant,thisprocedureaccomplishedMrs.Lisa'spurposewith- popularjobs,andthestudentsenjoyedbeingexcusedinthismanner. BoththeLineLeaderandTapperwere ratherthanasalargegroup. filteredoutoftheroomoneatatimeoringroupsoftwoorthree Theeffectofthiswasthatthestudents sheappearedtobeready. ingtoeachofthestudentsandtappingeachontheheadwhenheor Thisinvolvedwalk- leavetheroom,theTapperbeganhisorherjob. AftertheLineLeaderhadbegunto LineLeaderwasfirstinline. stanceswhentheclassdidlineup(forexample,onfieldtrips),the Leaderwasthefirstpersonwhocouldleavetheroom,andinrarein- TheLine Thesetwowordssettheroutineinmotion. later"Tapper." door,Mrs.Lisawouldfirstsay"LineLeader"andthenseveralseconds classwastoleavetheroom,ratherthanaskingthemtolineupatthe Whenthe LeaderandExcuser(morecommonlyreferredtoasTapper). EachweektwostudentschoseorwereassignedtothejobsofLine uptoleavetheroom,Mrs.Lisadevelopedthefollowingprocedure. pushingthatsheusuallyhadtocontendwithwhenthestudentslined 157 Whengoingoutside,Mrs.Lisawouldexcusethestudents ingtheroom. Therugalsoservedasa"staging"areaforleav- overtheschedule. gularfocusforintroducingactivitiessuchascallingrollandgoing Inthismanner,therugprovidedaninformalandre- tobeginclass. Bythetimeofthisstudy,studentsautomaticallywentovertotherug thatinthemorningandafterlunch,schoolwouldbeginontherug. Itwasestablishedatthebeginningoftheyear dinatedactivity. Therugwasanothermanagementroutinethatcontrolledandcoor- glanceattheboard,Mrs.Lisacouldquicklydeterminewhichstudents wereinvolvedinthevariousactivities. ciatedwithhavingtoomanystudentsdoingoneactivity;andwitha Thisprocedureminimizedtheproblemsasso- choosesomethingelse. Ifallthenailswereoccupied,astudenthadto theirnametags. tivitybyputtingupnailsonwhichstudentswererequiredtoput Lisausedthisboardtoregulatethenumberofstudentsdoingeachac- Mrs. tiesthatwereavailabletothestudentsduringchoicetime. timeboardwasabulletinboardneartherugthatlistedtheactivi- Thechoice interactivesettingarethechoicetimeboardandtherug. TwoexamplesofroutinesthatsimplifiedMrs.Lisa'sjobinthe butethemtothedesks. students,shedidnothavetocarrythemaroundtheroomanddistri- andeasilyaccessible:andwhenworkbookshadtobereturnedtothe lookthroughaparticularbookforplanningpurposes,itwasquickly Whensheneededto aroundtheroomtoobtainorreturnstudentbooks. studentsgetandreturnbookstothisrack,shedidnothavetowalk Byhaving filecabinetwherestudenttextsandworkbookswerekept. Apracticethatsimplifiedplanningandgradinginvolvedthebook 158 executiveplanningroutines. determinedtoagreatextentbytheefficiencyandappropriatenessof Thesoundnessandoriginalityofplanningmaybe processofplanning. theplannerdealwithcertainplanningtasksbyhelpingtomanagethe Executiveplanningroutineshelp Gagné's(1970)modeloflearning. samewaythatcognitivestrategiesactivateandguidelearningin Theyactivateandguideplanningprocessesinthe 1972;Gagné,1974). tivecontrolstructuresinmodelsoflearning(LindsayandNorman, Executiveplanningroutinesfunctioninasimilarwaytoexecu- zingofotherroutines. tinesserveasmeta-routinesformanagingthesequencingandorgani- Inotherwords,executiveplanningrou- trolandcoordinateplanning. Theseroutinesoccurwithintheteacher'sheadandfunctiontocon- tasksandresultsfromexperienceinnumeroussimilarsituations. systemofestablishedthoughtpatternssetoffbyspecificplanning Executiveplanningroutinesarea Executiveplanningroutines. teachingsettings. ducesthedemandsontheteacherinboththepreactiveandinteractive dinatesstudents'behavioroutsideofactivities,and,assuch,re- managementroutinesfunctionasthegluethatholdstogetherandcoor- Theseexamplesillustratehow andregularlyinMrs.Lisa'sclassroom. ThereweremanyothermanagementroutinesthatOperatedsmoothly ments. thestudentsweretogetherforanylastminuteremindersorannounce- Thisminimizedwanderingaroundtheroom,and untilthebellrang. theircoats,theyweretocomebackintotheroomandwaitontherug Whentheyhadgotten severalminutesearlytogototheirlockers. 159 otheractivitiesforthistime,theuseofroutinesreducesthetime andintheeveningsoronweekends-andsinceplanningcompeteswith necessarilytakeplaceontheteacher'stime-—beforeandafterschool Sincemostplanningmust evaluated,decidedupon,andmanipulated. haviorandthusreducesthenumberofcharacteristicsthatmustbe Theroutinizationofactionfixescertainaspectsofbe- routines. Onemethodtocopewiththesedemandsisbydeveloping anyonetime. findmethodstodecreasetheamountofinformationtobeprocessedat mentimposemanydemandsontheteacher,anditbecomesnecessaryto plexityandunpredictabilitythatcharacterizetheteachingenviron- Thecom- andenergyfrommanyplanningandimplementationdecisions. teacherflexibilityandeffectivenessbyfreeingtheteacher'stime Itisproposedherethatwhenusedproperly,routinesincrease usingsuchaplanningstrategywillbediscussedinthissection. Theresultsof selection,organization,andsequencingofroutines. thatMrs.Lisa'splanningchieflyinvolveddecisionmakingaboutthe Itwasproposed tinesinMrs.Lisa'steachinghasbeendiscussed. Thusfarinthischapter,theprominanceofactivitiesandrou- TeachingfiRoutinesandPlanning tersVIandVIII. andthedecisionprocessesthattheyinvolveisundertakeninChap- Furtherdescriptionoftheseroutines toanestablishedprocedure. planning,althougheachweekthisplanningwascarriedoutaccording forweeklyplanningwasdifferentfromtheroutinefordailyorunit Theroutine differentplanningtasksinconsistentandregularways. TheseroutinesbecamevisibleasMrs.Lisarepeatedlyapproached 160 teachingroutinecangreatlyreducetheneedforplanning. inghasmanypotentialpitfalls,itservesasanexampleofhowa Thoughtextbookteach- oftheteachertothatofthetextbookauthors. proachtoteachingsubordinatestheautonomy,initiative,andjudgment Third,thisap- dayperspectivesregardingthegoalsofinstruction. Second,textbookteachingtendstoresultinday-to- toinstruction. forsignificantinnovationsormodificationsoftraditionalapproaches containedinthematerialsthemselves,thusreducingtheopportunities theteacher'srolebecomesmainlythatofaprocessorofinformation First, warnsofthreepossibleconsequencesofusingthisroutine. However,inhisanalysisoftextbookteaching,Hudgins alltimes. ceduralclarityforthestudentsandafirmfocusonwhereoneisat Furthermore,itprovidespro- materialsareonhandandspelledout. Iteasesthepreparationrequiredoftheteachersincethe teaching. SmithandGeoffreypresentseveraladvantages.ofthistypeof someclassrooms. teachingcouldvirtuallyeliminatetheneedforpreactiveplanningin Coupledwithsomewelltestedmanagementroutines,textbook lesson. thematerialstodictatethestructure,sequence,anddurationofthe closelyatextbookandteachers'manual,ateachermightalsorelyon Byfollowing textorsimilarmaterialtoprovidecontentandmethod. byHudgins(1971)andreferstothepracticeofrelyingheavilyona TextbookteachinghasbeendiscussedbySmithandGeoffrey(1968)and canbeillustratedbyageneralroutinecalledtextbookteaching. Thedegreetowhicharoutinecanreducethedemandsonplanning andenergyexpendedforplanning,thusfreeingtimeandenergyfor otheractivites. 161 Onewaythey effectivenessintheinteractivesettingaswell. managepreactiveteaching,buttheyincreaseteacherflexibilityand Teachingroutinesarenotonlyanefficientandeffectivewayto TeachingRoutinesandInteractiveTeaching effort. evaluatingstudentworkandformaintainingafocusonindividual Inthesamemanner,routinesmadepossibleadequatetimeforquickly vitiesthatincreasedtheinterestandinvolvementofthestudents. allowedhertospendmoretimedevelopingcreativematerialsandacti- boundandrestrictedbyestablishedwaysofdoingthings,routines Ratherthanbecoming flexibilityandeffectivenessofherteaching. Tosummarize,Mrs.Lisausedroutinesasatooltoincreasethe mance. devotemoretimeandenergytoevaluatingindividualstudentperfor- theactivitiesthemselveswerewellroutinized,Mrs.Lisawasableto Since inkeepingtrackofindividualprogressinmathandreading. needs,andbywinterterm,muchpreactiveteachingtimewasinvolved tiesalsofreedtimefordiagnosingandprescribingforindividual Routinizationofactivi- fordevelopingcreativewaystopresentit. ofactivities,moretimewasavailablefordecidingoncontentand Becauseoftheroutinization tion,andacceptablestudentbehavior. routinization,butrathersuchthingsasparticipants,sequence,dura- Asdiscussedearlier,contentwasnotthefocusofher similarways. teaching,althoughactivityroutinesdidreducedemandsonteachingin TextbookteachingasdescribedabovewasnotapartofMrs.Lisa's 162 overtheintercom,firedrills,mishapsintheclassroom,andby terruptionsarecausedbymessengers,otherteachers,announcements In- theclassroomandtheteacher'sabilitytocontroltheclassroom. lengestothestabilityofthesocialandorganizationalstructureof Interruptionsarecommoninmostclassroomsandareconstantchal- islarger,lesstimeandeffortisrequired. fortmaybeexpandedtoregainthelostground;butifthatsubsystem Ifthatsubsystemissmall,thenmuchtimeandef- stablesubsystem. cedureisinterrupted,thesystemwillusuallybreakdownintoitsmost Ifacomplexpro- plexsystems,especiallythosepronetointerruption. Thisparableillustratestheimportanceofstablesubassembliesincom- tionoftheman-hoursittookTempus(pp.90-91). partsofhiswork,andheassembledhiswatchesinonlyafrac- sembledwatchinordertoanswerthephone,helostonlysmall Hence,whenHorahadtoputdownapartlyas- thewholewatch. bly;andasystemoftenofthelattersubassembliesconstituted assemblies,again,couldbeputtogetherintoalargersubassem- ersubassembliesofabouttenelementseach.Tenofthesesub- Buthehaddesignedthemsothathecouldputtogeth- ofTempus. ThewatchesthatHoramadewerenolesscomplexthanthose tofindenoughuninterruptedtimetofinishawatch. moretheyphonedhimandthemoredifficultitbecameforhim Thebetterthecustomerslikedhiswatches,the theelements. itimmediatelyfelltopiecesandhadtobereassembledfrom assembledandhadtoputitdown-toanswerthephone,say- Tempushadsoconstructedhisthatifhehadonepartly each. Thewatchesthemenmadeconsistedofabout1,000parts Whatwasthereason? hisshop. pered,whileTempusbecamepoorerandpoorerandfinallylost However,Horapros- customerswereconstantlycallingthem. garded,andthephonesintheirworkshopsrangfrequently-new Bothofthemwerehighlyre- manufacturedveryfinewatches. Thereonceweretwowatchmakers,namedHoraandTempus,who latesthefollowingparable: Whentalkingaboutevolutionofcomplexsystems,Simon(1969)re- roomorganization. accomplishthisisbyfunctioningasstablesub-assembliesofclass- 163 broughttotheclassroomwasfreedomandflexibilityforMrs.Lisa. Amajorresultofthestabilityandpredictabilitythatroutines timewasspentinset-upandmanagement. Anotherwayofstatingisisthatmoretimewasspentontaskandless ofthiswasmoretimespentoncontentandlesstimespentonprocedure. Theresult supposedtobedoingandhowtheyweresupposedtodoit. thatreducedthedemandsonthestudentstofigureoutwhattheywere Routinesprovidedanorderandstabilitytotheclassroom activity. dentstopredictwhatwouldbeexpectedofthemasparticipantsinan Routinesalsoallowedthestu- wouldbedoingforthenextfewhours. tiesformorningorafternoon,studentshadagoodideaofwhatthey Byknowingthescheduleofactivi- roomenvironmentforthestudents. increasingthepredictabilityandreducingthecomplexityoftheclass- Anotherfunctionofroutinesintheinteractivesettingisthatof studentstoldthesubstitutewhattheyweresupposedtodonext. Lisaonceremarkedthatthelasttimeasubstitutetooktheclass,the TheroutinesweresowellknownthatMrs. alsoknewwhatwastocome. forthewholeactivity,andtheynotonlyknewwhathadbeendone,but Theyhadasufficientoverviewoftheprocedure quenceoftheactivity. hadterminatedbecauseoftheirknowledgeaboutthestructureandse- Mrs.Lisaandthestudentswereabletopickuptheactivitywhereit Afteraninterruptionwasover,both bliesofclassroomorganization. Thiswaslargelyduetothestabilityofactivityroutinesassubassem- InMrs.Lisa'sclassroom,littletimewaslostduetointerruptions. directthestudentsbacktotheirs. tialformuchlosttimewhiletheteacherreturnstohisorhertaskto Afteraninterruption,thereisthepoten- innumerableotherintrusions. 164 165 Since most activities were stable and predictable, she was able to make impromptu changes or deviations to topics of interest to the students without destroying the orderly function of the activity. Dif- ferent groups could be assigned to aides or parent volunteers, more time could be spent with individuals as problems arose, and new teach- ing methods could be explored within the context of otherwise stable activities. Many routinized activities no longer required continuous monitoring or supervision and Mrs. Lisa could use the time for giving feedback to individual students or could run more than one activity at once. In short, the less time that was tied up in management, su- pervision, or monitoring of the group, the more time Mrs. Lisa could devote to individualized attention or instruction. Chapter Summary In this chapter, activities and routines were presented as two central aspects of Mrs. Lisa's planning and instruction. Activities were first discussed as basic structural units of planning and action in Mrs. Lisa's classroom. The role activities played as controlled behavior settings was discussed next, including the way future task environments were shaped to conform with the teacher's perceptions and purposes. Seven features of instructional activities were discussed that were presented as foci for teacher decision making and planning. The second half of the chapter was devoted to teaching routines. It was proposed that for Mrs. Lisa, planning chiefly involved the se- lection, organization, and sequencing of routines. Activity routines, instructional routines, management routines, and executive planning routines were each described and illustrated, and the function of 166 routines as means for increasing teacher flexibility and effectiveness in planning and instruction was discussed. CHAPTER VI A STRUCTURAL MODEL OF PREACTIVE PLANNING The purpose of this chapter is to present a structural description of Mrs. Lisa's preactive planning. This description is "structural" to the extent that it attempts to discern the arrangement of basic ele- ments underlying Mrs. Lisa's preactive planning. The chapter will focus on a model depicting five different levels of planning, corree sponding to five different time periods for which planning is conducted. Planning at each level will be discussed in terms of four descriptive aspects of planning: (1) planning goals, (2) information sources, (3) form of the plan, and (4) criteria for judging the plan's effective- ness. Finally, the connections and interactions between planning levels will be described. This chapter adds another important piece to the description of Mrs. Lisa's planning. Chapter IV described the environment in which planning and teaching took place. Chapter V discussed activities and routines as the basic units or elements of her planning. This chapter will undertake a description of the structure of Mrs. Lisa's planning. Overview of the Model Rationale It is assumed that planning is a rational, that is, an orderly, intelligible activity, and, as such, that it can be described and understood. The focus of this study was on describing the decision 167 168 processes that are regular features of Mrs. Lisa's planning. As the study progressed, it became apparent that Mrs. Lisa's planning had sig- nificant structural as well as process characteristics. The nature of the teaching task requires planning in a variety of forms and circumstances. The complexity of teaching requires that some organizing and sequencing take place ahead of time. The unpre- dictability of the task makes the elaboration and revision of plans over time a necessary and unavoidable job. To accomodate these demands, Mrs. Lisa uses several different types of planning. The identification and description of these different types of planning are important for several reasons. First, by identifying dif- ferent types of planning and how they interact, it may be possible to identify strategic research sites for further and more detailed study. This may allow future research to be more efficient and productive by specifying times during teaching when certain types of planning most consequential fbr educational programs are likely to be observed. Second, a structural analysis of planning may provide insight into decision processes in planning. It is likely that the way in which planning decisions are made reflects the configurations and re- lationships of different planning types. Demands imposed by the struc- ture of planning may also be exposed in this type of analysis. One possible structural problem created by different types of planning is the coordination of planning activity across levels or types of plan- ning. A third reason a structural analysis of planning is important is that if different levels of planning can be identified, the nature of planning should be different at each level. If this is true, then it 169 may have important implications for teacher training. If planning is no longer characterized as a unitary skill or as a set of skills for a specific purpose or setting, then it is possible that teachers may be poor planners at one level but not another, or may even ignore one type of planning. Awareness of the different tasks that make up the total planning activity may increase the efficiency and productivity of teacher planning. Levels of Planning Five basic types of planning activity were identified in Mrs. Lisa's work. Because of their hierarchical organization and f6cus on different spans of classroom activity, they are referred to as levels of planning. The five planning levels portrayed in the model are (l) yearly planning, (2) term planning, (3) unit planning, (4) weekly plan- ning, and (5) daily planning. Although planning may occur in the in- teractive teaching setting, it was not a focus of this study and is not a part of the model.10 Figure 6.1 illustrates the five basic levels of the model plus two other levels--institutional planning and planning for next year--which interact with preactive planning and which will be mentioned briefly. 10Planning has been defined in this study as a process of prepar- ing a framework for guiding future action. The focus of this study has been on the preparation of this framework prior to teaching when the students are not in the classroom. It is reasonable to assume that this framework can is often modified as a result of things that take place once the students are in class. Thus, "interactive planning" most likely involves revision of plans rather than planning dg_novo. The study of interactive planning is also complicated by methodo- logical problems. These problems are common to the study of any inter- active teaching decision and involve such issues as the consciousness of the decision, its observation, the inconvenience to the teacher of reporting the decision as it occurs, and so forth. For these reasons, 170 7? 7? x ‘ I 7T \ / \ R A E Y T X E N R O F G N I N N A L P IF > - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - l G N I N N A L P L A N O I T U T I T S N I H C R A M S R E B M E T P E l R E B M E C E D 5 L E V E L ) G N I N N A L P Y L R A E Y ( 4 L E V E L ) G N I N N A L P M R E T ( 3 L E V E L ) G N I N N A L P T I N U ( 2 L E V E L ) G N I N N A L P Y L K E E W ( 1 L E V E L ) G N I N N A L P Y L I A D ( . g n i n n a l p e v i t c a e r p f o l e d o m l a r u t c u r t s A 1 . 6 e r u g i F 171 As mentioned above, each level in the model has as its focus a different scope or span of activity. Yearly planning is concerned with general materials, pupil placement, and sequencing and organizing teach- ing for the whole school year. Term planning centers on activities that will occur within the twelve weeks before the next vacation. Unit plan- ning refers to the planning involved in developing an instructional unit for a specific subject matter. This unit will take place over a period of several weeks within a term. Weekly planning focuses on activity that will occur as part of the schedule on Monday through Friday, while daily planning involves the last minute modifications or preparations to be made during the day or before school starts the next day. Four of the five levels of planning in this model were directly observed during this study. Since this research was conducted during the winter and spring terms, there was no opportunity to observe plan- ning at the beginning of the school year. Information on this type of planning was obtained through interviews and through recall stimulated by using Mrs. Lisa's plan book to recreate her planning before school started and at the beginning of the year. The basic structure of the model was developed through observation and interviews during the study. This was further corroborated by Mrs. Lisa's description of her own planning. Towards the end of the study, Mrs. Lisa was asked to list and characterize the types of planning she does. The following list and description of planning types was gener- ated without any knowledge of the model being developed: it was felt that preactive planning might initially be a more fruitful and less difficult area to study. 172 1. long range--basic ideas for social studies, science units--some fOr math and reading--basic structure of what will be done but not specific time. 2. Term-~planning on a term basis for social studies and science and for movies. 3. Monthly--deciding on basic units for social studies, science, and math. I decide on what I need librarian to get or what movies I need. 4. Weekly--use teacher's plan book--specific units and ' time element added-~mOre detailed. 5. Daily--put schedule on board, getting actual materials out. 6. Immediate--on the spot planning--changes if needed, time fillers. Descriptive Dimensions To describe and differentiate planning at each of the five levels in the model, four dimensions of the planning process will be discussed. These include (1) planning goals, (2) information sources, (3) form of the plan, and (4) criteria fer judging planning effectiveness. These dimensions have been adopted from a study of the planning process (Dror, 1963) aimed at identifying the main facets of planning in the areas of administration and policy science. Four primary facets of planning were identified by Dror: (l) the general environment of the planning process, (2) the subject matter of planning, (3) the planning unit, and (4) the form of the plan. Dror's planning facets were designed to describe planning in an organizational framework (usually towards the top) and by groups of in- dividuals. Since Mrs. Lisa's planning was done individually and with close contact with the "subject matter" of planning (materials, stu- dents), many of Dror's categories were inappropriate for a description 173 of her planning. For this reason, the most relevant aspects of Dror's taxonomy were selected and grouped into the four categories of goals, information sources, form of the plan, and judgment criteria. The description of these four aspects of Mrs. Lisa's planning at each level is based on several data sources. For the most part, Mrs. Lisa's planning goals were obtained through discussion and interview and through observation of her on-going planning. Infbrmation about the sources of information used in her planning were obtained through observation and through the pupil and activity judgment tasks explained in Chapter III. The form of Mrs. Lisa's plans were observed during the study, and the description of her criteria for judging planning effec- tiveness was based on observation and interview as well as on the analysis of past plans. To summarize, the structural model to be presented and discussed in this chapter represents Mrs. Lisa's preactive planning at five levels: yearly planning, term planning, unit planning, weekly plan- ning, and daily planning. The planning at each level will be charac- terized by five descriptive dimensions of planning. First, Mrs. Lisa's goals for planning will be discussed. Second, the primary information sources used in planning will be described. Third, the discussion will attend to the form the plan takes: and finally, the criteria that Mrs. Lisa uses to judge the effectiveness of her planning will be described. The next five sections are devoted to discussing the five levels of activity observed in Mrs. Lisa's planning focusing on these four di- mensions. 174 Level V - Yearlnglanning Yearly planning involves the development of plans whose scope is the whole school year. Mrs. Lisa does most of her yearly planning dur- ing the summer vacation and in the first few weeks of school. This planning is done mainly at home, but since Mrs. Lisa tutors students in her own classroom for about half the summer, she does have easy access to all of her resource materials and does some of her planning there. Planning Goals Mrs. Lisa's major goals in yearly planning are to establish the general content of her instruction for the school year and to outline the basic sequence in which the content will be presented. Establish- ing the content involves generating ideas for activities and coordina- ting them with the district performance objectives and the materials and resources potentially available for teaching. At this point in her planning, Mrs. Lisa's academic goals are fairly general and are framed by the district objectives. (These ob- jectives were briefly discussed in Chapter IV and are outlined in Ap- pendix C.) In Mrs. Lisa's words, "Objectives are a framework for teachers to feel their way through teaching." For nearly all subject matter areas, she tries to teach so that as many objectives as possible are met. Mrs. Lisa tries to hit all the objectives in science and so- cial studies. She used the parent conference form objectives rather than the more detailed statements in the objectives handbook. Quite a bit of effort was spent in the summer combining the objectives in these areas for both grades and then deciding which objectives should be covered without repeating too much or covering the second-third 175 grade teachers' materials. In writing skills, Mrs. Lisa feels that many of the objectives are too difficult for her age level and cannot pos- sibly be mastered. Because of this, she is more concerned with intro- ducing topics than pressing for mastery. Mrs. Lisa uses the reading objectives only to "give her a feeling" where the students should be by the end of the year. Overall, Mrs. Lisa's teaching is not strictly guided by the district objectives, but she does want to be in a posi- tion where she can say that she is teaching the content specified for students at those grade levels. As a result, the district objectives act as a "frame" for her planning. Yearly planning is important for ordering or reserving certain instructional resources. This is especially true fbr films. The county has a media center that supplies films and other resources to all districts in the area. Reservations for films must be made in the late spring or early summer for the next year because of the amount of coordination involved in serving all of the schools. Mrs. Lisa uses many films in her social studies, science, and writing skills instruc- tion. Thus, in order to coordinate the arrival of the films with her teaching, she must have a general notion of when specific units or les- sons will occur during the year. Early sequencing also becomes impor- tant for reserving the kits for social studies that are shared by the schools in the district. A consequence of not ordering films early (and a pattern followed by some teachers at Byron School) is that films have to be used when they arrive for other teachers. This checking the film list and using what is in makes it hard to tie films into subject or unit being studied and was often used by some teachers to fill time. The consequence of not reserving social studies kits early was not 176 getting them when wanted and having to schedule units around the avail- ability of specific kits. The necessity for scheduling or ordering materials in the spring is not the sole motivator for yearly planning, but it is a major one. It is certainly the reason that Mrs. Lisa's yearly planning takes place so early. Without these restrictions, it is likely that yearly plan- ning would be done later in the summer. A major factor that seems to motivate Mrs. Lisa's yearly planning is her "need" to have an overview of the year to provide her with a picture of how both her instructional objectives and the district objectives will be covered during the school year. This overview pro- vides a feeling for the overall balance and sequence of the year's instruction as well as pointing out times and instances where more specific planning will be needed. Information Sources There are four basic sources of information used by Mrs. Lisa in her planning. Three sources are external and one is internal. The external sources are pupils, materials and resources, and the teaching environment. The internal source is her teaching experience. There are two major pieces of information about her students that Mrs. Lisa uses in her yearly planning. The first is the general num- ber of first graders and the number of second graders she will have in the fall. She receives this number late in the spring and it affects her yearly planning in the following manner. If the count she receives shows that she has more students at one level than the other, she has a general idea about the general age, maturity, and academic level of 177 the majority of her students. Mrs. Lisa may then decide to spend more or less time on specific subject matter areas or units as she begins to anticipate the student interest and needs. This impression is modified, if need be, in the first few weeks of school, but beforehand it may in- fluence her general selection and sequencing of instruction. The second major piece of information about pupils that influences her yearly planning is the number of last year's first graders returning to her classroom as second graders. This affects her planning a great deal in areas such as science, social studies, and writing skills, since Mrs. Lisa does not want to repeat the same units or activities if a large number of her students participated in them the year before. This year Mrs. Lisa had eight first graders return, and next year she will have twelve. This repetition of students forces her to develop what amounts to a two-year curriculum in the subject-matter areas af- fected. The major cue for Mrs. Lisa regarding materials and resources in her yearly planning is availability. When selecting and sequencing con- tent, she is concerned whether materials such as kits, texts, workbooks and films are available that are related to the content to be taught. In considering availability, Mrs. Lisa considers how adequately materi- als meet her general goals for presenting the content, the durability and workmanship of the materials, and whether the number of texts or workbooks available is sufficient. Further aspects of materials to which Mrs. Lisa attends are difficulty, attractiveness, specific skills taught, and interest value for the students. In addition, she checks out whether the materials are consumable (not to be reused next year-- so pages can be written on or torn out) and whether they contain some 178 type of recording or evaluation system. Many of these decisions are made late in the spring when materials such as workbooks, ditto mas- ters, and texts are being ordered as well as during the summer when she considers the resources available in the classroom or in the school. When planning for the year, Mrs. Lisa considers one primary source of information from the general teaching environment: the curriculum guidelines set by the district. As mentioned in Chapter IV, the dis- trict influences Mrs. Lisa's planning through setting performance ob- jectives for each grade level and through the adOption of specific curriculum packages to be used in certain subject matter areas. The manner in which the performance objectives influence Mrs. Lisa's yearly planning was mentioned earlier when talking about yearly planning goals. These objectives serve basically as a framework for selecting content to be taught and rarely represent a more detailed influence even at lower levels. Programs adOpted by the district in science, social studies, and spelling are major influences since they present the available content to be taught, and Mrs. Lisa must decide what and when specific activities or units are to be presented in the classroom. The final major source of information Mrs. Lisa uses in her yearly planning is experience. Mrs. Lisa draws heavily on past experience both for selecting content and for sequencing. Knowledge about what content has proved to be influential or important in student learning affects her choices, and knowledge based on sequencing content in various ways in the past influences her decisions about the general sequence of instruction for the year. At this level of planning, ex- perience becomes an important influence, since the students, one of the most important sources for modifying plans, are not yet present. 179 Meanwhile her knowledge based on teaching at the same grade level in the same school for several years provides the best estimate of what content and sequence will be appropriate for her new students. Form of the Plan At the yearly level, Mrs. Lisa's plans take the form of very gener- al outlines and lists of content and activities for each subject mat- ter area. For each major subject matter area--reading, math, science, social studies, and writing skills--a spiral notebook is used for planning. In each notebook, Mrs. Lisa lists the content she wants to cover during the year and then begins to outline on a weekly basis the content and activities she would like to use in her instruction. The detail of the outlines varies widely across subject matter areas; ex- amples of these plans are in Appendix F. Since much of the content and-sequence of reading is dictated by readers and workbooks, Mrs. Lisa's early planning consisted solely of a list of possible reading activities such as reading lab, phonics books, book reports, and silent reading. In writing skills, Mrs. Lisa first listed the content to be covered for each grade level based on the district performance objectives. Next, she listed for the first thirty-three weeks, word sounds and parts of speech to be covered each week. The next page in the notebook consisted of a list of activity ideas such as "word of the week" ("Mindbender"), verbal directions, "code," "rhyming," and "make small words" (later named "word contest"). In science, Mrs. Lisa listed the general science objectives for first and second grades. She then recorded the lessons from both the first and second grade manuals that would cover these objectives. 180 After that, she entered activities for the first thirteen weeks and the units to be covered in winter and spring terms. Mrs. Lisa's early social studies planning included a weekly list of mindbender words and topics to discuss when doing Snoopy books. Next, movies were listed and earmarked for certain months under the general topics of feelings, economics, cities, others, and countries. After that, lessons were laid out on a weekly basis including such activities as Duso, Snoopy books, states to be discussed as part of the United States unit, and other units such as nutrition, Japan, eco- nomics, and mapping. For winter and spring terms only, unit names were indicated every few weeks. The math planning at this level took the form of listing the units to be undertaken on a monthly basis. Cuisennaire rods were used for math instruction from September to December. Topics to be covered were greater than-less than, grouping, addition, subtraction, missing ad- dends, fractions, and patterns. For winter and spring terms, units were entered by month: January--time, February--metrics, March--graph- ing, April--money, and May--review. For each unit, several possible activities were listed. Also recorded were movies that might be ap- propriate in certain months. Criteris for JudginggPlanning Effectiveness At this level of planning, Mrs. Lisa's main criterion for judging planning effectiveness is the comprehensiveness of her resulting plans. This criterion is more structural than functional and involves her impression of the completeness of the overall "picture" of the year in each subject matter area. Comprehensiveness involves the degree to 181 which Mrs. Lisa's content or instruction covers the district objectives in each area as well as the extent to which the general sequence of content and activities is specified. This level of planning is in- tended to bring Mrs. Lisa to the point of having a general idea of what content she will be teaching in each month of the school year for each subject matter area. It also provides her with an idea of what special materials or resources she will need to arrange for or reserve early in the year. In short, Mrs. Lisa strives to have a general yet comprehensive idea of what content she will be teaching, the sequence in which it will be taught, and some ideas about activities that she may use to teach it. Level IV - Term Planning Term planning is the next level of planning Mrs. Lisa engages in after yearly planning. Based on Mrs. Lisa's self report, fall term planning did not really begin until the third week of school when she had a more complete picture of her students. The first week of school (just Thursday and Friday) was spent orienting students to the school and classroom by involving students in many different short activities such as art projects (e.g., making locker decorations), story reading, movies, and doing some simple handwriting dittos. During these acti- vities, Mrs. Lisa began observing and trying to determine student abilities on basic skills such as following directions, working as a group, physical coordination, drawing, cutting, and so forth. She was also trying to identify students with certain fears and the pupils' general ability to make decisions for themselves. The second week of school continued this general assessment, but Mrs. Lisa began testing 182 the students in more detail. On Monday, Tuesday, and Wednesday morn- ings, Mrs. Lisa tested the students using testing packets she had de- veloped herself. On Monday, she tested the students on handwriting (capitals and lower case letters), knowledge of beginning and ending word sounds and sight word knowledge (using Dolch words). On Tuesday mornings, she tested general reading abilities, more handwriting, per- ception and eye-hand coordination, and basic math skills (simple addi- tion and subtraction, missing addends, time, money, and measurement). On Wednesday, she tested for discrimination ability and perception (figure-ground discrimination, matching figures). Each of these pack- ets took about thirty minutes to administer. Also, during the second week, Mrs. Lisa observed the students' memory ability (using memory blocks), their ability to describe objects, themselves, and their families, coordination skills (during gym), and listening skills. She also gave some individual students the Boetell Reading Test and listened to every student read orally. Based on these two weeks of observation and assessment, Mrs. Lisa was ready by the third week of school to begin the students on a regular schedule and begin assigning them to groups. It was at this point that term planning for the fall really began. Planning for winter term took place in December and over the winter break. Planning for spring term was completed just before or during spring break. Plannigg Goals Mrs. Lisa's term planning involves two major activities. First, she lays out in more detail the content she wants to cover in the next three months. This is especially important in science, social 183 studies, and math where much of her teaching will be done by units. Since more than one unit is taught during the term, the approximate length of each unit must be determined and dates must be specified to arrange for any special materials she may need. For those subject matters such as reading and writing skills where teaching is done on a daily and weekly rather than a unit basis, content priorities are established and activities are generated. Most of this is accomplished as part of the second major activity of Mrs. Lisa's term planning, the establishment of a weekly schedule. The purpose and function of a weekly schedule in Mrs. Lisa's teaching was discussed in Chapter IV and will not be repeated here. To summarize, the weekly schedule as it is established each term re- flects Mrs. Lisa's subject matter priorities for the term, content priorities within subject matters, and the basic methods that will be used in teaching. This is done primarily by allocating time to various instructional activities on a weekly basis. It is at this level of planning that the activity ideas generated earlier are elaborated to determine whether they can function adequately to present Mrs. Lisa's subject matter content and achieve her skill development goals. If so, then the activity is given a slot in the schedule. The weekly schedule for the term is not fixed prior to the begin- ning of the term. After two or three weeks when the students have had time to adjust to the schedule, Mrs. Lisa reevaluates the format and may make some revisions. Most of these revisions involve moving acti- vities to different time periods, rather than adding or deleting acti- vities. This is due to the fact that most problems that occur in the 184 schedule are timing and scheduling problems rather than the "failure" of an activity. In the fall, an additional task of term planning is the grouping that Mrs. Lisa does for reading and spelling. After testing student reading abilities, Mrs. Lisa decides how many reading groups she will have and initially assigns students to groups. Group composition may change as she obtains a more complete picture of individual abilities by working directly with the groups or as students rapidly improve or fall behind the other group members. Once reading groups were estab- lished, their composition rarely changed. This seemed primarily due to the large differences in reading groups. (Each group was reading at least a grade level apart.) The grouping for spelling was based on a pretest on the district spelling list. Since differences between spelling groups was not as great as between reading groups, students were moved more frequently from one group to another. By the beginning of March, one group caught up with the group just ahead of it, and Mrs. Lisa combined them into a new larger group. Grouping in math was done on a unit basis and will be discussed under unit planning. Information Sources As opposed to yearly planning, term planning benefits from direct ' experience with the students. It is for this reason that Mrs. Lisa waits until the third week of school to do her fall term planning. The first two weeks involve mainly orientation to the classroom, pre- testing, and group projects to provide Mrs. Lisa with the information she will need to group students properly and to decide which learning 185 activities will be most effective. Knowledge about the students' aca— demic abilities provides Mrs. Lisa with the information she needs to make decisions about content and sequencing for the term. To decide on activities, she uses information she has gathered about attention spans, listening and perceptual abilities, cooperation, and likes and dislikes of the students during the first two weeks of school. When making decisions about activities and the weekly schedule for the term, Mrs. Lisa gathers several pieces of information from the more general teaching environment. As the schedule is worked out, she must consider times when the students will be out of the room for gym, reading lab, music, art, and library. Times for these activities are determined during the first week of school and Mrs. Lisa's schedule must work around them. Also Mrs. Lisa must consider times when her aide or parent volunteers will be available to help her in the class- room. Another general restriction she must consider is the amount of time she has for teaching between breaks in the day caused by recesses and the lunch period. Form of the Plan The two major activities of term planning each take a different form. The elaboration of content for the term is done in the same spiral notebooks in which the basic yearly planning is done. This in- volves filling in the content that will be presented each week or specifying the beginning and ending dates for specific units. The weekly schedule for the term is worked out on a sheet of paper that Mrs. Lisa will put in her plan book and use as a guide for her weekly planning. Examples of those plans can be viewed in Appendix F. Prior 186 to making up the schedule, Mrs. Lisa has listed the activities she wants to fit into this term's schedule. When she sits down to arrange the activities, she also has available last term's activities and a list of times when aides are available. She then arranges and rear- ranges activities on her worksheets until the schedule conforms to her expectations. The steps taken in Mrs. Lisa's spring term schedule planning this year are presented in Appendix G. Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness The effectiveness of her elaboration of the content lists is judged in the same way as was the more general yearly planning. Mrs. Lisa is mainly concerned at this point with comprehensiveness of her list and that she has a basic idea of what content she will be teach- ing each week. This aspect of term planning is successful if it re- sults in a more detailed specification of what will be taught during the next three months. The schedule is initially judged using several criteria. First, Mrs. Lisa wants to include the activities she thinks should be a part of her weekly routine. Second, the schedule should be so arranged that she can have help from aides for those activities needing more than one adult. Third, she wants to develop a schedule which includes a balance of academic and non-academic activities as well as times in the classroom and times out of the classroom. Finally, she wants a schedule that accounts for the age, maturity, and stamina of her cur- rent students. 187 Level III - Unit Planning Nearly all of the major planning decisions in science, social studies, and math are made at this level of planning. Prior planning in these areas had consisted mainly of deciding which topics might be good to treat at various times during the year. It is not until this level that decisions are made about specific content, materials, and activities. After the term schedule is established and most activities are functioning as routines, unit planning becomes a major focus of Mrs. Lisa's planning and the most time-consuming planning activity in which she is engaged. Mrs. Lisa begins planning for a unit during the month prior to its scheduled beginning. Most unit planning is done at home where materials can be spread out and left undisturbed until the unit is pieced together. PlanninggGoals Mrs. Lisa's primary goal in planning units for social studies and science is the development of a sequence of well organized learning experiences that present the subject matter content in a comprehensive manner and at an appropriate level for students of this age. As a re- sult, much of her planning time is involved in gathering all the ma- terials available to her on a specific topic and then generating as many activities as she can based on the materials. These activities are then organized and sequenced to present the content of the unit in a meaningful manner. In unit planning for math, Mrs. Lisa's focus shifts from activities to content and grouping. Though special large group or whole class activities may be used to present certain concepts, most math content 188 is presented on an individual or small group basis. Prior to begin- ning a unit, Mrs. Lisa usually gives a pretest to assess each student's knowledge of the topic. Based on these results, she then arranges the students into homogeneous groups. Once the groups are established, Mrs. Lisa lists the content that each group will cover. In most cases, this chiefly involves listing worksheets or pages in the math workbooks that each group will do. A secondary goal of Mrs. Lisa in much of her unit planning is the development of activities to integrate more than one subject matter area. Nearly all social studies and science activities integrate an aspect of writing skills such as spelling or creative writing. Ex- amples of such activities include cutting out pictures of foods from various food groups, glueing them on a sheet of paper, and labeling them as part of the nutrition unit in social studies or drawing a dino- saur of one's own creation and writing a story about it as part of the dinosaur unit in science. Social studies and math were integrated, for example, in a unit on money. Students learned about economics and practiced their math by comparative shopping for groceries and by keeping individual checking accounts at a fictitious bank. Information Source By the time Mrs. Lisa begins any unit planning, she has a good idea of the abilities of her students in specific subject matter areas. This information allows her to set the difficulty of the content to be presented at a fairly accurate level for the group. Since all ac- tivities in science and social studies units are done as a large group, Mrs. Lisa also pays attention to the students' attention spans, their 189 ability to work together as a group, and their interests when she selects or creates activities. In addition to the information she generally gathers about instruc- tional materials, Mrs. Lisa pays special attention in unit planning to the length of a lesson, the time required for set-up, the format (media) of the material, the degree to which materials or activities are modifiable, and the "integrativeness" of materials and activities. Since programs and kits are available in science and social studies, they provide a major source of information and materials. The district performance objectives are an influential source of information since they serve as a framework for deciding on what skills and content should be covered in a unit. The objectives supplement Mrs. Lisa's knowledge about the students' ability levels when she is deciding on the content for the unit. Mrs. Lisa does not, however, get out the objectives and match her activities to them. Rather, her knowledge about their general content serves as a general influence in her planning. The spatial arrangement of the room and the facilities available become an additional factor to consider when doing large group activities or when considering activities requiring that materi- als be left out for a long period of time or that have special light or heat requirements as in some science activities. Form of the Plan Unit plans for science and social studies usually take the form of activity or content lists and outlines. When Mrs. Lisa previews potential materials for a unit, she keeps notes on the content covered and possible activities that come to mind. She may also have an 190 outline of the organization and sequencing of topics within the unit. After all the materials have been previewed, Mrs. Lisa selects activi- ties and begins sequencing them. Often, the sequencing is done as she begins fitting the specific activities into the times set aside in the weekly schedule for science and social studies. Math unit plans are usually in the form of lists of page numbers or dittos to be pre- sented. When the students are grouped, Mrs. Lisa makes a separate list for each group. Sometimes topics and activities are specified in Mrs. Lisa's plan book. Examples of these plans are presented in Appendix F. Criteria for JudgigggPlanning Effectiveness Since a unit is a smaller piece of the curriculum than is a term or a year, Mrs. Lisa can have a more detailed picture of the organiza- tion, sequence, balance, and flow of instruction than at more general levels of planning. These characteristics become more detailed and tangible and are major cues in Mrs. Lisa's evaluation of her plans. The comprehensiveness of the unit and the fit with her general concep- tions of the district objectives also function as judgment criteria at this level, but potential student reactions and performance begin to play a more influential role than at higher, more abstract levels. When activities are specified on a lesson by lesson basis in her plan book, she is able to visualize the unit in the most detailed form it will take until it is taught in the classroom. As such, Mrs. Lisa can get an impression of each activity. Before becoming a part of the unit, these activities have usually been judged to be balanced between passive experiences such as watching a film or listening to Mrs. Lisa 191 and more active experiences such as drawing, writing, and discussing. They also have been judged meaningful and interesting to the students and seem capable of presenting several basic ideas to the students in a manner that allows them to practice using the concepts or principles. In short, Mrs. Lisa uses potential student involvement and enthusiasm as often as she uses potential learning outcomes as criteria for judg- ing planned activities at the level. Level II - Weeklnglanning Life in Mrs. Lisa's classroom is most visible on a weekly basis. This is largely due to the weekly schedule she establishes and the resulting routines. Planning also functions most visibly on a weekly basis in Mrs. Lisa's classroom. This may be because weekly planning is recorded in the teacher plan book--where one might first look for signs of teacher planning. It may be due also to the fact that weekly planning is done at school, whereas more long-range planning is done at home or during breaks. Since Mrs. Lisa's classroom functions most regularly on a weekly level, weekly planning becomes the "nuts and bolts" planning that maintains regularity and continuity while being responsive to day-to-day changes. Weekly planning begins two weeks before teaching and is elaborated and modified up to the day of in- struction. Planning Goals The major goal of weekly planning is to lay out the week's activi- ties within the framework of the established weekly schedule while adjusting for interruptions to the schedule and responding to specific needs of the students. Mrs. Lisa is dedicated to the importance of 192 regularity and predictability of daily and weekly activities in which the students are involved. The schedule is established with great care to provide an optimal organization, sequence, and balance, and to reflect her instructional priorities for the term. Because of this effort, Mrs. Lisa strives to adhere to the schedule as closely as pos- sible. Since the schedule rarely runs as planned, a large part of weekly planning involves modifying the week's activities while main- taining continuity and regularity in activities judged most crucial to student progress. This may involve rescheduling an important activity to another time or dropping a scheduled activity to insert a special activity. Since weekly planning is done two weeks in advance, the schedule can usually be rearranged to accomodate special activities or interruptions without losing an important piece of the curriculum. When rearrangement must occur, the goals of balance, variety, and so forth that were important in her original planning remain just as im- portant in any replanning that is done. Since most activities occurring on a weekly basis become well routinized after several weeks in the schedule, Mrs. Lisa's weekly planning rarely has to be concerned with components of activities other than content and materials. Advanced weekly planning allows her to prepare or collect needed materials well in advance of teaching and to specify basic content in order to maintain continuity and momentum in a certain area. To maintain this continuity while addressing any special or individual student needs, Mrs. Lisa must closely monitor student performance. This becomes most important for reading, writing skills, and those math activities not taught on a unit basis, since they run throughout the whole term without the closure provided by 193 unit organization. Mrs. Lisa is most concerned with monitoring and checking the students' understanding of materials and mastery of any skills being taught in "fundamental" areas such as reading, writing, and math. If she finds that an individual or group of students is having consistent difficulties, she will either spend some individual time with them or schedule another lesson to review the same topic. It was not uncommon for Mrs. Lisa to set back the lessons she had scheduled for the following week to make room for this type of acti- vity. A secondary though important purpose of weekly planning is to facilitate Mrs. Lisa's advanced planning for special activities in the room. Mrs. Lisa keeps a note card in her planning book to keep track of information that should be included in her weekly letter to parents. When she does her weekly planning, she often jots down notices or re- minders about special activities that she will include in her next letter. In this manner, advanced weekly planning allows her to com- municate special information to the parents which will facilitate the smooth functioning of activities in the classroom. To summarize, weekly planning presents the challenge to Mrs. Lisa of maintaining the "status quo" of the schedule while at the same time being sensitive to and adapting to constraints on time as well as to special needs of the students. Information Sources The main source of information about the students to which Mrs. Lisa attends in her weekly planning is their performance in activities in the preceding days and weeks. This influences her decisions about 194 what activities may be passed over as a result of time constraints and what subject matter areas need increased attention. Institutional constraints are important cues in weekly planning because of the in— fluence they have on the schedule. When Mrs. Lisa begins arranging the activities for a week, the first thing she looks for is whether there are any scheduled interruptions in the week caused by professional development days (for which the students are absent), holidays, assem- blies, field trips, and so forth. Two other factors she considers are whether there is a special day such as Saint Patrick's Day or Valen- tine's Day occuring during the week and the position of the week with- in the term. If, for instance, the week is toward the end of the term, the schedule may have to be mofified to accomodate the comple- tion of units or special activities that should not be carried over to the beginning of the next term. The major aspect of materials and resources that Mrs. Lisa attends to in her weekly planning is their availability. She is mainly con- cerned about the curtailment of a certain resource such as an aide or one of the resource teachers and whether or not scheduled materials such as films or kits have arrived. One reason that Mrs. Lisa plans in advance is so that these factors can be taken into account and any necessary schedule adjustments can be made. Form of the Plan Weekly planning is done in a standard plan book provided to all the teachers in the district. The book supplies enough pages so that each week can be laid out on two facing pages. Each page provides a row for each day of the week and three blocks for each day. If two facing 195 pages are used, there are three blocks available for the morning schedule and three blocks available for the afternoon schedule. Mrs. Lisa uses the middle blocks of each page for indicating recess: thus, she arranges her schedule into four instructional blocks analogous to the four teaching periods in a school day. (Refer to Figure 4.3 on page 88 for an illustration of these four periods.) Mrs. Lisa's weekly plans consist of listing the activities which will occur in the teaching periods for each day. For some activities times are indicated and occasionally notes will be included specifying page numbers or special activities. The activities are written in pencil so that as things come up which influence the schedule, acti- vities may be added or deleted or moved to another place in the sched- ule. Several of Mrs. Lisa's weekly plans are presented in Appendix F. Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness The effectiveness of weekly plans is judged according to the com- pleteness of the plans, the degree to which the weekly schedule has been adhered to, and the flexibility of the plan to allow for time constraints or for interruptions and special activities. Mrs. Lisa also looks for the continuity that scheduled activities provide with unit lessons and on-going activities and the degree to which the schedule is responsive to special student needs and to opportunities for special activities presented by holidays or special community or school days. In short, the effectiveness of Mrs. Lisa's weekly plan- ning is judged in the same manner as planning at other levels, by the fit between her goals for planning and the actual products and out- comes . 196 Level I - Daily Planning Daily planning refers to the planning that is chiefly concerned with the next day's activities. Mrs. Lisa's daily planning takes place for the most part after school on the preceding day, although some preparatory activities may have been carried out over the last few days. Last minute preparations or revisions may also take place in the morning before school or during breaks in the day. In Chapter II, planning was defined as a process of preparing a framework for guiding future action. To this level this framework has consisted of lists, outlines, and schedules. At the daily level, the framework produced by planning also becomes the physical arrangements and preparations made by the teacher. This physical framework in- fluences action by increasing the efficiency of the teacher's instruc- tion by reducing the number of things the teacher has to deal with prior to initiating an activity. Planning Goals Mrs. Lisa's daily planning seeks to accomplish four major pur- poses. The first is to arrange the classroom in order to facilitate the smooth running of the next day's activities and in order to reduce the number of things she will have to worry about or be involved with during her teaching. This involves activities like writing the sched- ule on the small chalkboard, setting up the station work assignments, gathering together and setting out materials, and writing assignments or problems on the chalkboard. The second purpose of daily planning is to specify those components of activities not yet decided upon. Because of the routinization of 197 of classroom activities, the only decisions that are commonly made on a daily basis are those pertaining to content and materials. In cases where materials are closely linked to content, decisions about both of these components are often made in the several preceding days. This is because of the time involved in creating or arranging for many materials. The most frequent content decisions made at the daily level are in reading and in handwriting. Since most of the instruc- tion in these areas is done in texts and workbooks, the only decision left to be made at the daily level is the selection of pages to be covered in tomorrow's lesson. The third goal of Mrs. Lisa's daily planning is to fit the day's schedule to last minute intrusions and to the attitudes and working abilities of the students. This last minute fitting and adjusting of the schedule is necessarily done before school or at breaks during the day. This may be done as a result of a notice about a fire drill or assembly cancellation received in her box in the morning or as a result of Mrs. Lisa's judgment about potential student interest or involvement in a specific activity. The fourth purpose of Mrs. Lisa's daily planning is to prepare the students for the day's activities. Mrs. Lisa's belief that it is im- portant that the students are able to predict what is going to happen during the day was mentioned earlier, and it is largely for this rea- son that the schedule for the morning and afternoon activities is put on the small chalkboard and reviewed with the students soon after calling roll. 198 Information Sources Two pieces of information that Mrs. Lisa is especially sensitive to at the daily level are clarity of instructions and set-up time. Clarity of instructions in texts or workbooks will make a big dif- ference in the smoothness of activities, especially when the instruc- tions refer to seat work to be done while Mrs. Lisa is with other stu- dents. This is usually checked closely so that she can make additional comments before the activity begins. The set-up time of an activity also influences the smoothness of transition between activities. Mrs. Lisa tries to assess this as accurately as possible so that advanced preparations can be made and so that as little class time as possible is consumed by this chore. The student cues Mrs. Lisa attends to during the day also influence her daily planning. A rough evaluation of what the day will be like is often made based largely on the way the students come into the classroom in the morning. Mrs. Lisa feels that this is tied closely to the weather and often said, "When the barometer is up, so are the children: and when the barometer is down, the children are, too." In other words, when the weather is clear and sunny, Mrs. Lisa expects to receive less work from the students: and when it is cloudy or rainy, the children are usually quieter and work better. In addition to this initial judgment in the morning, Mrs. Lisa pays attention throughout the day to the students' interest, involvement, and enthu- siasm related to activities. If these factors are low in the morning and a heavy academic activity is scheduled for the afternoon, Mrs. Lisa may substitute a less demanding activity. Mrs. Lisa tries to anticipate the attention levels and change-of—pace needs of the 199 students in her normal schedule, but occasionally last minute revi- sions are necessary. Form of the Plan Daily planning takes two forms. The most obvious form is the schedule that is written on the small chalkboard. This schedule is put up every morning and afternoon as a list of the day's activities. Examples and more detailed discussion of the daily schedule were pre- sented in Chapter IV. The second form that daily planning takes in Mrs. Lisa's classroom is the preparation and arrangement of materials and facilities in the room that guide and facilitate teacher and student actions during activities. These planning activities were mentioned earlier and in- clude writing assignments on the board, rounding up materials for an activity, setting up the room in a certain manner, and so on. Criteria for Judging Planning Effectiveness Unlike higher levels of planning, Mrs. Lisa's judgment of the effectiveness of daily planning includes both structural and functional criteria. The structural criteria apply to the planning that occurs in the afternoon of the preceding school day or in the morning before school begins. Planning is judged as effective if all the last minute pre- parations and decisions about content, materials, facilities and schedules have been made. Mrs. Lisa's almost compulsive need to have everything prepared before the students enter the room is the reason for the long hours that she keeps after school and the amount of work that she does at home. When talking about her daily planning, Mrs. 200 Lisa said, "If I don't get it done at night (in the afternoon), I'm afraid I won't have it ready in the morning, so I tend to take a lot (pf materials) home." The functional criteria used by Mrs. Lisa relate largely to "how the day went." The major cues that she uses to make this judgment are how noisy the students were during the day, and the involvement, in- terest, and enthusiasm communicated to her as the students participated in various activities. Mrs. Lisa feels it is possible to influence the day by either overplanning or underplanning. She feels overplanning can often restrict her spontaneity and flexibility in teaching, and that underplanning leads to long transition times and to unnecessary chaos in activities. Although higher levels of planning play an in- fluential role in how activities come across in the classroom, it is only at the daily level that planning is judged by its outcomes. The evaluation of activities in the classroom are eventually stored and fed back to higher levels of planning, but the effectiveness of plan- ning at these levels is rarely judged as a result of the outcomes pro- duced by activities later in the year, term, unit, or week. Connections Between Levels of Plannigg The previous sections of this chapter have described Mrs. Lisa's planning at five different levels. These descriptions have portrayed aspects of her planning that are unique to each level, but have not been concerned with an important feature of the model, the connections and interactions of different planning levels. It is the purpose of this section to describe these connections and pinpoint times at which interaction is most likely to occur. Rather than examining each level 201 with its connections to the other levels separately, the discussion will focus on various times during the school year when interaction is most likely to be seen. Six planning times have been identified that involved the most visible interaction among the different levels of Mrs. Lisa's planning. They are: (1) the beginning of the year, (2) the beginning of the term, (3) the third week in the term, (4) the beginning of unit planning, (5) when the weekly schedules are planned. and (6) at the end of the school day. These connections are illus- trated for fall term in Figure 6.2. The beginning of the year provides opportunity for observing interaction among several levels. As Mrs. Lisa gets to know the stu- dents better in the first few weeks of school, her yearly planning is elaborated and modified. When this is taking place, fall term planning is also being laid out and a weekly schedule is developed. This plan- ning does not react significantly with daily and weekly planning until the third or fourth week since the activities of the first few weeks are fairly standard orientation and diagnostic activities that Mrs. Lisa always uses in the first few weeks of school. At the beginning of the term, one is most likely to see interaction between yearly and term planning. As the next term is laid out, yearly plans are consulted for general sequence and content. Since the week- ly schedule for the term is only tentatively arranged at the beginning of the term, the third week in the term provides interaction among daily, weekly, and term planning. At this time, the schedule may be modified since Mrs. Lisa feels the students have had enough time to adjust to it and problems in the schedule are not merely related to student adjustment. Modifications are based on actual classroom 202 YEARLY "” ”"— 4:1 PLANNING PLANNING UNIT PLANNING WEEKLY PLANNING DAILY PLANNING \ “ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ “ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ \ I ' ' I I' 1 ' —TI""'T‘ i ; SEPT. @ ® ® § OCT. ' NOV. TDEC. INTERACTION BETWEEN YEARLY, TERM, AND WEEKLY PLANNING AT THE BEGINNING OF THE TERM AND BEFORE THE BEGINNING OF THE NEXT TERM INTERACTION BETWEEN WEEKLY. UNIT. AND TERM PLANNING AT THE BEGINNING OF EACH WEEK INTERACTION BETWEEN UNIT AND TERM PLANNING AT THE BEGINNING OF EACH UNIT INTERACTION BETWEEN WEEKLY AND DAILY PLANNING DURING DAILY PLANNING \ \ \ _ . ‘7‘ L Figure 6.2 Interaction between levels of planning (illustrated for fall term). 203 outcomes and on problems Mrs. Lisa may have encountered in her weekly or daily planning. When unit planning is beginning, interaction often occurs with term planning. The planned length of the unit and the number of peri- ods set aside during a week influence the scope and length of the unit. Unit planning also interacts with weekly planning as the unit activi- ties are being fitted into the weekly schedule. Weekly planning also requires connections with term planning as Mrs. Lisa integrates the week's activities with her goals and priorities for the term. At the end of the school day, when most daily planning occurs, one is most likely to observe interaction between daily and weekly plan- ning. Daily planning is usually a function of what has been specified for the week. It is also possible that the plans for later in the week may be modified as a result of the day's activities. Each of these six times that have been briefly characterized may be thought of as potential research sites at which to examine Mrs. Lisa's planning in more detail. It is at these times that planning is most explicit since several planning levels are interacting. Although it would be unreasonable to assume that other teachers plan in identi- cal ways as Mrs. Lisa, these times when her planning is most active and visible might serve as guides to strategic research sites in other teachers' planning. Chgpter Summary The purpose of this chapter was to outline and discuss a structur- al model of preactive teaching. This type of analysis is important as a means to identify strategic research sites, to provide additional 204 information about decision processes, and to describe the nature and the degree to which planning activities differ. The structural model of Mrs. Lisa's planning identified planning at five levels: (1) yearly planning, (2) term planning, (3) unit planning, (4) weekly plan- ning, and (5) daily planning. Each of these levels was discussed based on goals of planning, sources of information, form of the plan, and criteria for judging planning effectiveness. Finally, the connec- tions and interactions among levels were discussed for six strategic times during the year. "ll‘ulllllilllm L I131? A H Y Michigan State University .A..-,~.rv- 3 "H514! ' ' EaWH 'T'gllcsr—I Mal—4%— " <5“) x\'" \ . ~ 1 3 0 0 , 7 / ( . \ . k O CHAPTER VII THE PSYCHOLOGY OF PLANNING I: DATA FROM OTHER FIELDS Simon (1957 b) distinguishes three phases of decision: intelli- gence, design, and choice. Intelligence includes processes for scanning the environment to see what matters require decision. Design involves processes for developing and examining possible courses of action. Choice includes processes for choosing among courses of action. Rational models of planning have as a rule concentrated heavily on Simon's third phase of decision and ignored the other two phases. Observations of Mrs. Lisa's planning decisions indicated processes related to choice, but these processes did not seem to be as prominent as processes related to problem formulation (intelligence) and design. Because of this, theories and research related to the functioning of these latter processes were examined and eventually served as a concep- tual basis for developing a theoretical model of teacher planning. This model will be presented and discussed in the next chapter. The main purpose of this chapter is to present and discuss research from fields other than education that bear on the question of what psycholo- gical processes underly deliberative thinking. This chapter reviews three areas of research and theory which in- fluenced the final form of the planning model. The prominence of rou- tines in Mrs. Lisa's planning suggested the importance of reproductive thought components in planning. The theory of Selz and de Groot's ap- plication of his ideas to chess thinking acknowledge the role that 205 206 reproductive thought plays in complex problem solving and decision acti- vities and has provided many apparent parallels to Mrs. Lisa's planning processes. The prominence of efforts concerned with designing and creating materials and activities in Mrs. Lisa's planning prompted an examination of studies of designers and artists in other fields. Get- zels and Csikszentmihalyi's (1976) work analyzing creativity in artists and the recent work on problem formulation and work strategies in archi- tectural design provided additional insights into the decision pro- cesses observed in this study. Reproductive Components of Thought In this study, planning is conceived of as the process of develop- ing a framework to guide future action by means of problem solving and decision making. Planning, like other forms of problem solving, is by no means a unitary process. It involves various components, materials, and operations. One component of human problem solving that has long been a subject for experimentation and discussion is the role played by past experience (Birch and Rabinowitz, 1951; Maier, 1930, 1931, 1945; Wertheimer, 1959). The classical distinction between reproduc- tive and productive thinking is based in part on the differing roles that past experience may play in each type of deliberation. On the one hand, reproductive thinking is characterized as the solution of pro- blems by means of the existence of stimulus equivalences in the pro- blem situation and in the previously mastered situation (Maier, 1945). In other words, reproductive thinking is the process of arriving at a solution by directly applying previous learning. On the other hand, 207 productive thinking (Wertheimer, 1959) involves the repatterning and restructuring of past experience is meet current demands. The planning process primarily involves productive thinking; that is, most of the situations or problems to which planning is applied are new and unique. However, it may be that reproductive factors such as knowledge and intuitive experience play an important role in informa- tion processing related to planning. Two researchers who have directly addressed the function of reproductive methods in problem solving are Otto Selz and Adriaan de Groot. Each of their theories will be dis- cussed focusing on the contribution of past experience to their charac- terizations of human problem solving and decision making.11 The Theory of Selz Otto Selz was a member of the Denkpsyghologie group led by Kulpe at the University of Wurzburg at the turn of the century. He has been judged by some as the most important yet most generally underrated psychologist of the wurzburg school, and only small portions of his work have been translated from the German (see Mandler and Mandler, 1964, for a portion). Because of this dearth of information on Selz's work, the following discussion relies heavily on de Groot's interpreta- tion of Selz's theory and research. 11The work of Selz and de Groot is discussed in some detail in this chapter for the following reasons. No substantial translations of Selz's work exist in English, and the German versions are out of print. De Groot remains the primary interpreter of Selz; however, the work that contains his review of Selz's theory as well as his own theory of chess thinking (de Groot, 1963) is also out of print. 208 General theory. Most of Selz's work aimed at developing a con- ceptual model of directed thought. In his theory, thinking is charac- terized as one continuous activity and described as a "linear chain of operations." In this mdoel, each succeeding operation is determined by the outcome of the proceeding operation. In general, the course of thought process is determined by three factors: (1) the repertoire of "solving operation dispositions" available to the subject, (2) the features of the problem (including a "goal") as perceived and conceived by the subject, and (3) the subject's "determination" or intention to solve the problem. This third factor is the motivational factor that initiates and sustains the process and Selz's model is assumed to be valid only when this conscious determination exists. The mechanism that maintains the chain of mental operations in Selz's theory is based on the concepts of "general linkings" and the "schematic anticipation." General linkings are Selz's replacement for classical notions of association. An association is supposed to link a mental disposition to concrete images. Selz's general linkings are of a more abstract nature and are supposed to link problem situations with general solving methods. Since these linkings are acquired for the most part by learning and are of an individual nature, Selz refers to them as thought habit dispositions. These abstract general linkings direct the thought processes. Each next step is determined by the situation resulting from the pre- ceeding step. The mechanism that "fuels" the process and keeps it going is referred to as schematic anticipation. A determined solution process begins with the starting of a goal. In setting this goal, Selz 209 assumes that there is an accompanying schematic anticipation of the consequence of reaching the goal. In other words, the goal-as- attained is anticipated in some incomplete, schematic form. Further- more, this schema is accompanied by some gap that is to be completed 'and an experience of tension corresponding to the open gap. Thus, "This schematic anticipation, with its gap and tension, is considered to be the starting point for the further course of the thought pro- cess" (de Groot, 1965, p. 58). The concept of schematic anticipation is very general and fundamen- tal to Selz's analyses. The presence and dynamic function of antici- patory schemata are as much a part of complex problem solving activi- ties in productive and creative goal settings as they are of simple, reproductive tasks. This will become more evident in the following discussion of those methods of problem solving proposed by Selz to be "the most general" or the most applicable to the largest variety of problems. The most general solving methods. The distinction between reproductive thought and productive thought was apparently of funda- mental importance to Selz since he published a separate book on each topic (Selz, 1913, 1922). Although de Groot says that the distinctions between the two modes of thinking were not laid out clearly, Selz's use of the two terms is similar to later use by Maier, Wertheimer, and others. Reproductive thinking, according to Selz, refers to those methods by which relational facts that are present in memory are ac- tivated and reproduced to solve a problem. Productive thought occurs in situations where no previously 210 formed knowledge complex can provide an immediate answer to a problem. Selz's "most general solving methods" refer to the latter mode of thinking. Selz distinguishes between two types of methods within productive thought. Methods used for finding of means (Mittelfindung) refer to the most general heuristics used in problem solving, invention, crea- tion, and so forth. Methods for means application (Mittelanwendung) include such general solving methods as ordering, trying out, and checking. Since the understanding of means application methods is less problematic and will be mentioned later, the focus here will be on those methods used for means finding. Means finding in directed productive thought. The simplest way to solve a problem is to actualize or call up means-ends connections already existing in memory. This reproductive process is very impor- tant in productive thought, according to Selz, and is characteristic of processes he refers to as the "method of determined means~ actualization." Three cases of means actualization are differentiated by Selz: (l) purely cognitive actualization, (2) automatic actuali- zation of means, and (3) routine actualization of means. Purely cog- nitive actualization occurs in those instances where one has the Eggf scious know-how to proceed but lacks application experience using those means. In this case, one can at any time actualize a solution to a problem, but only on the basis of cognitive acquaintance with the means-ends relationships. Automatic actualization of means occurs when one possesses an auto- matic solution complex for solving the problem. These complexes are 211 are learned "automatically" as a result of experience, and when a ques- tion such as "How do you do it?" is asked soon after the behavior, it cannot be easily answered. The best examples of automatic solution complexes are motor skills. If, for example, a baseball player is asked to explain how a fly ball is caught, he is usually at a loss for a verbal explanation of the perceptual and motor skills involved in the task. This also holds true for some intellectual skills, and the same response may be encountered when asking similar questions to an ex- perienced chess player, physician, or teacher. The third method of means actualization that Selz discusses is routine actualization of means. This refers to situations where one has not only the knowledge of a method for a solution but also has direct application experience. Although also based in experience, this method is different from automatic means actualization in that one is able consciously to report the method used. If one does not possess the necessary know-how to solve a problem, then it becomes necessary to find new means to produce new results. Methods for finding new means in productive thought are discussed by Selz under the heading of "determined abstrac- tion of means" and are the second main type of directed productive thought activity. In general, means abstraction refers to methods used for constructing new means-ends connections previously unknown to the problem solver. Selz is unclear in his explanation of how this abstraction occurs, although the direction of the process is supposed to be determined by a corresponding schematic anticipation of the re- sult of the abstraction, that is, of a solving method that may be successfully applied to the problem. The three subtypes of determined 212 means abstraction enumerated by Selz are (1) reproductive abstraction of means, (2) coincidental abstraction of means, and (3) immediate abstraction of means. Reproductive abstraction of means occurs in situations where one knows or has experienced a specific means-ends relationship but has never really hit upon the idea of using that means as a way to reach that specific end. This is often difficult to distinguish from purely cognitive means actualization. De Groot explains the difference in the following manner. The individual is cognizant of a relationship such as "A leads to B" (A --9 B) but the serviceability of using A to attain B has never dawned on him. The schematic anticipation in the form of "a means x is sought in order to attain B" activates the com- plex A --) B and at the same time activates the complex's property-- "A can be used as a means to attain B." De Groot says that this men- tal operation is reproductive since it is based on the actualization of a memory complex, but the realization that A leads to B is new knowledge. In later situations where this knowledge is needed, it would most likely be produced as a result of routine means actualiza- tion since the previously acquired awareness of the means-ends rela- tionship is retained. The second method of means abstraction is coincidental means ab- straction. This occurs in situations where one has searched for means to reach a certain goal with no success. At some later time when one is still under the influence of the uncompleted task, an accidental or coincidental perception of the relationship may lead to the realiza- tion that a certain means may be used to reach the temporarily for- gotten goal. Selz says that this type of actualization is most common 213 in situations where one is strongly involved, if not obsessed, with solving the problem. Even though there is no active motivation or determination functioning at the moment, there is a relatively strong "determination disposition" that is easily activated. As a result of perceiving the partially related event or means-ends structure, the problem and the uncompleted schematic anticipation are activated and the means is seen as a way to reach the desired goal. Selz explains this phenomenon by suggesting that once a task has been temporarily abandoned and then reactualized, the schematic anticipation appears in a less detailed and more general form. If the specificity of ori- ginal schematic anticipation had prevented the discovery of a specific solution and the specifying conditions are not all necessary, then the reduction of restrictions and the dropping out of unnecessary details and conditions increases the space in which a workable solution can be found. This notion of "inner coincidences" was used by one of Selz's stu- dents, Julius Bahle, to explain the phenomenon of inspiration, speci- fically musical composition. Bahle (1939) showed that in composers, there often exists an extraordinarily strong motivation or disposition to produce works according to a certain style. This disposition may often generalize into a "plan" of expressing some particular feeling or fundamental experience in music. These "productive experiences" as Bahle called them produce very strong determination dispositions that may be easily activated by some chance thought, perception, or image. In addition to setting the stage for these internal coincidences, Bahle also found that composers often tried to arrange their outer environ- ment to encourage such fruitful occurrences. This was done by keeping 214 to certain schedules, following special food and drink habits, working in certain climates and locales, and so on. Thus, by "living with a problem" and by arranging the outer environment to facilitate certain perceptions and mental experiences, it may be possible to increase the probability of inspirational and creative moments. The third method of finding new means to solve a problem is called immediate means abstraction. This occurs as a result of a "structural insight" into the nature of the task. Insight here is used in the true Gestalt sense of the word since the means are "read" from the structure of the task either during the original perception or after some immediate restructuring. De Groot says that this type of actuali- zation is most frequent in situations such as practical, geometric, or chess problems where visual structures or schema play a strong role. He does concede, however, that this also can occur in the absence of these visual structures where one is able to perceive some logical, causal, or other structure in the task. He also states that the de- gree to which this method may be applied by an individual is a func- tion of the amount of experience. "Experts" in some field who imme- diately see how to proceed after inspecting the problem provide one example of this method. In this case, the materials structure them- selves, so to speak, and this immediate abstraction process may play an important role in influencing the direction of further problem solving. One of the major questions arising from Selz's mechanistic con- ception of productive thought is how does it account for creativity? If thought is conceived of as a determined chain of solving methods, each of which is reproductively actualized and set off by the results 215 of the proceeding method, how are new results produced? The first part of Selz's answer relates to the methods of means actualization and abstraction. He makes the point that even though the methods of means actualization are using old means, they lead to new results simply as a result of applying old means to new materials. And even though the three methods of means abstraction are actualized by repro- ductive processes, their application to the immediate problem is new to the problem solver. The previous discussion of composer inspira- tion is an example of this process working. The second part to Selz's answer about how his theory accounts for creativity is‘related to his third main method for directed productive thought--"productive utilization of previously abstracted means-ends relationships." This method draws further attention to the activities of a creative individual when not at work solving a specific problem. Selz found that these individuals often methodically build up their creative power. De Groot discusses the way in which mental producti- vity is developed outside of specific problem solving situations. First, of course, the repertoire of solving methods in a par- ticular field is built up by learning processes, both from books and by experience. Theoretical study leads to cogni- tive mastery of means that can then be applied through "cog- nitive means actualization,” while experience "in the trade" continually extends and differentiates the subject's arsenal of automatic and routine actualization methods (p. 68). A "creative" individual may go beyond these steps, according to Selz, by imposing on himself a permanent motivation or "set" to build up and improve his repertoire of solving methods. This set may permeate all aspects of the person's activities, so that means are collected and stored from every context and situation in which the person finds himself. De Groot proposes that his process may become particularly 216 important to artists who often draw on personal experience for a source of inspiration since, "They are liable to develop the set to look for and then to store materials and expressive means which they may find potentially useful, regardless of whether specific creative plans are in mind" (de Groot, 1965, p. 68)- To summarize, Selz portrays productive thought as the task of find- ing appropriate means to provide solutions to problems for which one has no immediate solution. The heuristics used to accomplish this are grouped under the rubric of methods for the finding of means. The two most general methods for means finding are means actualization and means abstraction. Means actualization refers to methods where means- ends relationship already exist in memory and are merely actualized or called up to solve the problem. In this case, the solution itself is not immediately available, but means to reach the goal are available in some form. The second general method, means abstraction, occurs in situations where one does not possess the know how to solve a problem and must search for new means. Under each of these methods, three sub— types of methods are proposed: purely cognitive, automatic, and rou- tine actualization of means and reproductive, coincidental, and imme- diate abstraction of means. The third general method for means find- ing is called "productive utilization of previously abstracted means- ends relationships," and refers to the activity of collecting and storing means for possible use at some later, undetermined time. This latter method along with the method of coincidental abstraction of means were used to illustrate how Selz's theory can account for crea- tive thought. 217 Linking of solving methods. According to Selz, the complete thought process is comprised of a sequence of operations resulting from strictly lawful combinations and linkings of solving methods. One might then ask what initiates, controls, and terminates this process? In a general form, problem solving is conceived of as a series of alternating phases of "abstraction" (the finding of subgoals in a task) and "realization" (the application of appropriate means). Transitions from "a-phases" to "r-phases" (a -—9 r) are simply determined by finding a and then applying means r. The transition from r to a (r --9 a) does not, hwoever, automatically proceed from the completion of the previous operation. To explain this r --9 a transition, Selz offers the notion of a "determination to realize the composite opera- tion." According to this concept, there is a higher order motivation to complete the task which activates a new abstraction and its corres- ponding realization phase. This return to higher order goal or to the main problem is proposed as the mechanism for stimulating comple- tion of the whole task. The process just described where the sequence of operations is determined by previous successful operations signalling following op- erations is referred to as "cumulative linking." The other major form of linking that occurs is called "subsidiary linking." Subsidiary linking occurs when the next solving method to be activated takes the place of one that has just failed to obtain the goal or subgoal. This Operation implies a return to the original task where the schematic anticipation of the goal-as-attained will necessarily be modified based on the knowledge one now possesses about how not to proceed. 218 Although Selz speaks about trying to reach the same goal, the goal set- ting is in reality somewhat modified as a result of experience. The question of which solving methods will be actualized at a given point in the problem solving process is based on a strict deter- ministic formulation. First, it is determined by the specific system of general linkings--both cumulative and subsidiary--in the individual. Second, it is based on cues in the current stage of the problem situa- tion. General linkings between solving methods and specific problems are determined by individually acquired reaction dispositions each of which is linked to specific types of internal and external situational stimuli. Phase structure and Bahle's work with musical composition. The alternating phase structure mentioned in the previous section becomes especially important in more complex situations and with more complex tasks. It also becomes more visible. De Groot reports that such a phase structure has been reported by all investigators who have worked with protocols of actual thought processes. Although this type of phase alternation was traceable in Selz's relatively simple tasks, this motion was more thoroughly elaborated and illustrated in Bahle's work with musical composition. As mentioned earlier, Julius Bahle was a student of selz in the early thirties. Although Selz's influence on his work is rarely ac- knowledged in his writings, Bahle's studies are viewed as a direct outgrowth of Selz‘s work. Bahle chose to analyze musical composition, more specifically song writing, using fascinating and ambitious experi- mental method that is described by de Groot: 219 After some highly interesting laboratory experiments, Bahle (1930) sent eight carefully selected poems to a large num- ber of well known composers. He then requested them to put one or more of the poems to music and, additionally, to make daily protocols on the progress of their work. In order to facilitate the latter task and, in particular, in order to furnish the composers with the point of view from which such an uncommon task had to be carried out, he enclosed an ela- boarate instruction...as well as a questionnaire. The ques- tions referred to the composer's motives in choosing a cer- tain text, possible phases in his developing the composition, etc. Moreover, the composers were requested to turn over all sketches and notes which they had made during their work. As a result of the skillful choice of the poems in addition to the carefully worded questions and instructions...Bahle's experiment by correspondence (Fern Experiment) became a great success. No less than 32 composers cooperated,together pro— ducing a wealth of valuable data. Some of the participants, it is true, could not be inspired by any of the eight poems, so they instead reported on a song composition using a text of their own choice. The materials collected in this way were compared with historical data from letters and bio- graphies and worked into an integral conception of musical composition (pp. 79-80). Bahle used a more relaxed theoretical model than did Selz and did not insist on the same mechanical, "reflexoidal" characteristic of pro- ductive thought processes. The creative process is described as a "methodical activity structure" that stressed a high degree of organi- zation but was not as mechanistic as Selz's "system of specific rela- tions.“ Bahle (1936, 1939) showed that the process of composing a piece of music begins with a schematic anticipation of the completed work (the goal-as-attained) that is very abstract and general and often includes hardly any musical notes. As a result of attempts at solving subpro- blems (or "specific work problems," as Bahle calls them) or the main problem, the total goal conception (de Groot's term) is gradually modi— fied until the anticipatory schema applied in successive goal settings is gradually filled out. In this manner the outcomes--both successes 220 and failures--of each solving method not only contribute to the com- pletion of the problem, but also to the shaping of the goal as concep- tualized. De Groot states that, "The characteristic feature of this process is alternation and interaction between the anticipations on the one hand and the results of attempts at subproblems on the other" (p. 75). Bahle confirmed Selz's notion of the influence of the anticipation on the realization or elaboration of the subproblem (the a --9 r transi- tion mentioned earlier). He showed that the specific work problems established by the composer directed and determined the following ela- borative phase. He also showed the reverse influence; that is, the effect on the outcomes of specific elaborations of specific work pro- blems, or in Selz's terms, the schematic anticipation that is to govern the next phase. Bahle's work illustrated the pronounced in- fluence of successful and unsuccessful elaborations and completions on changing the structure of the goal conception to such a degree that he labelled this interaction between anticipations and results "the principle of creative form-making." By observing a task which occurs over a much longer period than those examined by Selz and one for which the goal is self set, Bahle was able to highlight the importance of goal changes as a result of subproblem elaborations. This notion of problem development was also reflected in the work of Karl Dunker (1945) when he said that, "What is really done in any solution of problems consists in formulating the problem.more productively." In addition, Bahle also showed that linkings between solving me- thods do not always function as immediately as in Selz's tasks. This 221 is especially true for subsidiary linkings. Bahle found that pauses were quite common during creative activity setting the stage for pro- cesses such as coincidental means abstraction. This notion of "crea- tive pauses," as Bahle called them, became important elements in his theory to the extent that he considered them regular parts of the pro- cess as a whole, forming "meaningful links in the methodological acti- vity structure." Thought and Choice in Chess; de Groot's Theory In the late 1930's, Adriaan de Groot, a student at the University of Amsterdam, who, in addition, was an international class chess play- er, took up the problem of describing the thought processes under- lying the skilled chess player's choice of a move. In his own words, "Why do masters find the good moves that patzers overlook?" The sim- ple experiemental method that he used was to present his subjects with an unfamiliar board position taken from an actual tournament game and ask them to find and play the next move as if they were participating in the game. While doing this, he asked them to "think aloud" during the process, giving as complete a rendering of their thought processes as possible. Twenty-two subjects were used in his studies ranging from grand masters (including two world champions) down to "second class"12 players. De Groot's goal was to arrive at a general descrip- tion of the structure and dynamics of chess thinking as a basis for theory formation. 12Equivalent to a class C rating on the United States Chess Federa- tion scale. 222 The basic framework adopted for de Groot's research was Selz's con- ception of thinking as a hierarchically organized, linear series of directed operations. De Groot's work also served as a test of Selz's theory in a more elaborate and complex task situation. Two aspects of de Groot's findings will be discussed in this section as a way of sum— marizing his theory. First, the phase structure that was found to be prominent in chess thinking will be described, and, second, general methods of chess thinking will be discussed. Phase structure of chess thinking. De Groot found that the basic structure of chess thinking is made up of the alternation be- ‘tween phases of direct analysis (elaboration) and phases involving integration, abstraction, and restructuring. There are three general phases that he discusses: phases of orientation, elaborative move phases, and trasitional phases. The orientation phase is the first phase to be visible in de Groot's move selection problems. This is the subject's first ex- posure to the board position where he considers the position, orients himself to it, and lets the problem "sink in" and become structured for him. De Groot has also called this the phase of problem formula— tion since it is at this point when the subjective "problem" is formed and concretized by the subject. Once the subject begins exploring possible moves, he enters an elaborative move phase. This phase is characterized by the working through and analyzing of the move to some degree until a preliminary result regarding the feasibility of the move can be reached. After the orientation phase, chess thinking is characterized by the subject's 223 examining a large number of moves one after another. That is, there is a string of elaborative move phases involved in each choice of move problem. It is also possible that there is more than one elaborative move phase devoted to examining one possible move. This is especially true in processes of longer duration which de Groot calls "reinvesti- gation of the same solving prOposition." A move possibility is rarely selected in isolation from a more general goal or plan that a player has in mind. It is usually a part of or a means for reaching a more comprehensive "board goal." These general goals direct the player's strategy for a certain period of time and, for this reason, are referred to as plans. Plans are part of the "total goal conception" that the subject develops early in the orientation phase. The total goal conception is the complete formula- tion of the problem developed by the subject that includes not only the problem structure, but also notions of difficulty and solvability, methods that may be applicable, dispositions, intuitive and emotional preferences, and so on. Like Selz's schematic anticipations, the total goal conception is characterized by incompleteness since it refers to notion of the goal-as-attained. De Groot found that the transitions from one elaborative phase to another do not always occur immediately, nor are all elaborative phases devoted to calculations and direct investigations of move pos- sibilities. For this reason, he talks about a third aspect of chess thinking called "transitional phases." In this phase, thought is less rigid and more "open-minded" and is characterized by reflecting, inte- grating, and abstracting. The function of these phases is to inte- grate the operations that have preceeded and to prepare for the next 224 investigations. Transitional phases are most common, according to de Groot, when there is no longer available any pre-established, di- rect connection from the result of an investigation to the next ela- borative method or when as a result of continuous negative results one runs out of subsidiary methods at one level. When this occurs, one is forced to return to a more fundamental problem and reevaluate further action. It is during this transition phase that the problem (goal con- ception) may be reformulated as a result of abstracting new possibili- ties and relationships from results thus far and by integrating and restructuring this new information. In short, transitional phases perform the same functional purpose as Selz'a Mittelfindung methods; that is, they are involved in finding new means or in this case new moves, new plans, new approaches to and views of the problem, new sub- programs, and so forth. General methods of chess thinking. De Groot defines a solving method as a "typical problem transformation." In other words, he felt that, Whenever a particular transformation is found to occur predictably in a number of solving processes (protocols) that result from Aufgabenl3 of a distinct, definable type, the transformation can be called a solving method (p. 280). De Groot agreed with Selz (1924) that Every directed thought process up to the attainment of the goal or to the subject's abandoning his solving attempts can be described as a chain of sequentially corresponding solv- ing methods that are cumulatively or subsidiarily linked (p. 11). 13Aufgaben refers to the general task in a problem situation. 225 Three frequent and general methods of chess thinking that will be dis- cussed in this section include "plan formation," "progressive deep- ening of the investigation," and "trying out." Plan formation. De Groot defines plans as incompletely defined long-term schemes for action. These schemes serve as concrete, stra- tegic goals to guide the following investigations, in contrast to the subject's general mating or drawing purpose. Plan formation generally takes place in the first phase of chess thinking and has mainly a directive and selective function. A plan functions as an aid to the choice of move decision by organizing and delimiting the group of "considerable moves" (moves stated by the subject as worth consider— ing). In this manner, the problem is transformed by narrowing the moves to be considered. Another problem transformation that occurs in the plan forma- tion is grouping changes. What begins as a multiple choice problem is reduced to a binary choice problem by dividing the "considerable moves" into two groups: those moves fitting into the plan, and those moves are not fitting into the plan (or possibly fitting into another plan). As a result, the problem is simplified and transformed since only what fits into the plan need be investigated. A third transformation that may occur as a part of plan formation in a more general sense is "stipulating the order of investigation." This refers to ordering of Operations in advance on either a condi- tional or unconditional basis. This ordering is, however, to be dis- tinguished from Other "ordering" operations that proceed routinely and 226 automatically as a way of investigating possibilities during elabora- tive move phases. Progressive deepening. Progressive deepening refers to a process of investigation that is characteristic of many lengthy and difficult choice-of-move problems. It refers to the analysis of a certain move or plan that takes place in a series of successive phases of reinvesti- gation. This process includes two aspects. First, the investigation broadens itself by growing new branches containing new considerable moves or countermoves. Second, it deepens itself by repeatedly taking up the move or plan and calculating its results more thoroughly. The exact form of the investigation is not predetermined, but rather it is what de Groot callls a "conditionally composite opera- tion." These types of operations usually proceed by the following format: If a calculation leads to satisfying results, other varia- tions are tried out (attempts at strengthening) while the same plan and first move are retained; if the variations fail, other first moves are tried out; if those also fail, the entire direction of the investigation is changed (an- other plan) (p. 263). This process can be illustrated in de Groot's four phases of deepening: l. The First Phase of Orientation orients the play to pos- siblities for moves in a general direction and to general consequences of moves. 2. In the Phase of Exploration, the player beings trying out (rather than investigating) possibilities by cal- culating a few sample variations a few moves deep. ' If these prove unsatisfactory, then they are tempo- rarily set aside. 3. The Phase of Investigation involves a deeper search for possibilities that are more sharply defined. Move 227 variants are calculated and they are calculated more deeply and exhaustively. 4. The Final Phase of Proof is made up of checking and recapitulating as the player strives to develop a sub- jectively convincing argument to support or reject a certain move or plan. Progressive deepening is not always a voluntary or pre-planned process. De Groot has called progressive deepening a subsidiary method since the subject hopes that a satisfactory move will be found in the first or second investigation, and he will not have to investigate further. This can be seen in early phases of deepening where the sub- ject quickly circulates several promising moves hoping to make further investigation unnecessary. It is usually the case in more difficult problems that further investigation must be carried out, but sometimes early calculations will succeed. The importance of progressive deep- ening and its prominence in difficult problems may, therefore, be due to its orientation towards economical thought. Trying out. In chess thinking, trying out as a mental operation is a very important method in thinking and decision making. If the entire thought process is envisioned as an "empirical though investigation" (de Groot, p. 293), then trying out serves as a general method to de- termine the worth of various moves or plans. In some decision making situations, variations and alternatives may be tried out by actually doing them. In chess, however, this luxury does not exist, and trying out can take place only in thought. Trying out as a method can be distinguished from the process of trial-and-error in several ways. When using the method of trying out, 228 one always exhibits a clear sense of direction. The process is usually put forward in the format of "If...is tried, then ...will result." The word i£_implies that one is working within a well defined goal setting (schematic anticipation) trying to check out fairly specific expecta- tions. This specific orientation towards direction, selection, and evaluation that is set up by anticipations does not exist in trial- and—error processes. Another basis for distinction is that trying out takes place in constant interaction with problem development. Therefore, as a result of trying out, much more specific types of information flow back to the subject than in trial-and-error. De Groot states that in trial-and- error processes, there is only room for good and bad experiences; but in trying out, the subject gains information about the problem in the form of qualitative results and generalizations, evaluative refine- ments, and structural and causal "insights." Trying out takes place in the form of a complex, organized process where every next move must be chosen from a group of considerable moves based on an idea or plan that is, in turn, part of a specific goal conception. Thus, trying out as a method is based on partial insight into the problem and occurs in cases where the subject has certain vague anticipations about the result. De Groot specifies three possible functions of trying out. The first function of this method is to aid the subject in hitting on a solution for a main or subproblem. If an attempt is successful, then the method can lead directly or indirectly to a solution. Second, trying out can function to increase the information on the problem. It does this by specifying and concretizing the problem, by adding new 229 information and experiences to the total goal conception, and by fur- ther defining the group of considerable moves. The third function of trying out is the discovery of new means. It is in this capacity that Selz describes trying out: The trying out of several solution possibilities is a gen- eral operation for the finding of means. It must be ap- plied by (animal and) man whenever it is impossible via determined immediate means-abstraction to discover struc- turally based solving methods and where firm empirically based correspondences are not yet at hand (Selz, 1922, p. 645). ‘ Trying out also favors the occurrence of insight (coincidental means abstraction) and serendipity through trying various calculations and modifications. Summary This section has focused on two theories of productive thinking that emphasize the importance of reproductive thought components. Selz depicts problem solving as a hierarchically organized, linear series of directed thought operations energized and directed by a schematic anticipation of the goal-as-attained. The most general solv- ing methods were described as various ways to actualize or abstract means from.memory. In this manner, productive thought is portrayed as a determined chain of solving methods each of which is reproductively actualized or abstracted and set off by the results of the proceeding method. This theory was elaborated by Bahle's work on thought in musical composition and by de Groot's work on chess. By looking at longer, more complicated processes, a characteristic phase structure was de- scribed in which thought progresses as an alternation between phases of 230 direct analysis and elaboration and phases involving integration, ab- stracting, and restructuring of the schematic anticipation (Selz), specific work problem (Bahle) or total goal conception (de Groot). Thus, problem solving is portrayed as a process that involves problem formulation and development as well as problem solution. These general solving methods--p1an formation, progressive deepening and trying out—- that de Groot had found important in chess thinking were then dis— cussed. Each method was described as involving a transformation of the problem as well as reproductive thought processes. Problem Finding in Art: the Creative Process Thinking, especially decision making, is too often equated with rationality and characterized as systematic deliberation based on specific laws of logic and inference. This is particularly true of rational models of planning portraying the process as one primarily involving choice among specifiable alternatives based on the outcomes of each alternative and the objectives of the decision maker. In this study, Mrs. Lisa's planning did not function according to this descrip- tion. It followed much more along the lines of a creative process, where efforts were not aimed at solving a structured problem, but rather focused on fashioning new problems. For this reason, planning was viewed as a process of problem finding and development, in addi- tion to a process of problem solving and decision making. In the previous sections of this chapter, problem formulation re- peatedly appeared as an important process in productive thinking. The reworking and reformulation of schematic anticipations and goal conceptions were shown to be crucial aspects of problem solving in 231 tasks as apparently divergent as selecting a move in chess and creat- ing a musical composition. What was overlooked in these studies was how the problem was first discovered or found. This is primarily due to the fact that these researchers were concerned with problem solv- ing and thus supplied their subjects with the problem. There are many situations where no problem exists or where the problem is so generally stated that one's first task becomes that of "finding" the problem. This is especially common to areas more gener- ally regarded as creative, such as art and music, but it is common to science as well. In his text on physics, Albert Einstein makes the following point: The formulation of a problem is often more essential than its solution, which may be merely a matter of mathetical or experimental skill. To raise new questions, new pro- blems, to regard old problems from a new angle, requires creative imagination and marks real advance in science (Einstein and Infeld, 1938, p. 92). The only systematic investigation of problem finding that exists today has been done by Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi (1976). In this study, they chose to investigate creativity in artists by examining how they discovered and formulated the problems on which they chose to work. This work will be discussed here in three parts. First, Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi's general theoretical framework and the problem situation used for the study will be presented. Next, the behavior observed in the problem finding situation will be described; and fin- ally, the relationships between problem finding and creativity will be discussed. 232 Problem Situations The general conceptual framework adopted by Getzels and Csikszent- mihalyi for this study is that of viewing the creative process as a response to a problematic situation. As such, the main elements of creativity are the same as those of other problem situations and in- clude the phases of (1) problem formulation, (2) adoption of a solu- tion method, and (3) reaching a solution. Based on these three struc- tural elements of problem situations, Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi de- veloped an analytical model of various problem situations and their related cognitive functions. This model is illustrated in Table 7.1. At one extreme of possible problem situations are "presented pro- blem situations" (type-case 1). In this situation, the problem has a known formulation, a known (routine) method for solution, and a recog- nized solution. The only task remaining is for the individual to follow the established steps for finding the solution. The primary mode of thought is memory and retrieval, since "all the problem solver has to do is plug the given data into a known formula to find the solution that is already known" (p. 81). In type-case 2, the situation is the same except that the method of solution is also unknown to the problem solver. The primary task of the individual becomes that of reaching a solution matching the one already known by others. Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi illustrate this type-case with Wertheimer's classic problem, "How would you go about finding the area of a parallelogram?" Since the answer is not known by the individual, his mode of thought must switch from memory to reasoning. e v i t i n g o C y r a m i r P f o d o h t e M 1 . 7 e l b a T s n o i t c n u F e v i t i n g o C d e t a l e R d n a s n o i t a u t i S m e l b o r P f o s e p y T 233 n o i t a n i g a m i n w o n k n u n w o n k n u n w o n k n u d e r e v o c s i D ( ) s m e l b o r p 3 e s a c - e p y T y r o m e m n w o n k n u n w o n k n w o n k d e t n e s e r P ( ) s m e l b o r p 1 e s a c - e p y T n o s a e r n w o n k n u n w o n k n u n w o n k 2 e s a c - e p y T e v l o v n I n o i t u l o S n o i t c n u F d n o i t a l u m r o F n o i t u l o S m e l b o r P n o i t a u t i S m e l b o r P 234 The third case type, at the other extreme of problem situations, is what is called "discovery problem situations." Here the problem has neither a known formulation, a routine method of solution, nor a re- cognized solution. At this extreme, only the "dilemma" is presented, and the problem to be solved must be discovered and identified by the individual. It is in this situation that the individual must become a "problem finder." Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi specified three steps involved in becoming a problem finder: (1) one must feel that there is a challenge needing resolution in the environment, (2) one must formulate his feeling as a problem, and (3) one must attempt to devise appropriate methods for solving the problem (p. 81). An additional difficulty occurs in this type of problem situation. Since the problem has been discovered and solved by the individual, there is no predetermined standard to which the solution may be compared. The only way that the solution can be accepted or rejected is on the basis of a critical, relativistic analysis. It is this type of analysis that is at the heart of art criticism. The central question that Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi formulated for study was: "Is there a significant relationship between the pro- blem finding behavior of fine arts students and the aesthetic value, especially the originality, of the drawings they produce?" The speci- fic situation in which they chose to examine problem finding was the preparation for and drawing of a still life. The study was conducted at the School of the Art Institute of Chicago with thirty-one fine arts students. Each was asked to select from a constant arrangement of twenty-seven objects, arrange them any 235 way he wanted, and then draw the arrangement. The study was conducted in an empty room at the Institute that had been fitted with all the materials and paraphernalia normally available in an artist's studio. During the selection, arrangement, and drawing processes, an observer kept a running record of the students' behaviors, and photographs of the work were taken periodically. At the end of the period, the stu- dent was interviewed to assess the reality of the situation and to get a subjective account of the artist's experience during the experiment. Problem Finding Problem finding in this study was observed in two stages: the problem formulation stage which refers to the student's behavior be- fore beginning his drawing, and the problem solution stage that begins once a drawing is started. During the problem formulation stage of the experiment, three types of behavior were observed and quantified: (1) number of objects manipulated (out of twenty-seven available), (2) uniqueness of objects chosen (based on a score corresponding to the rank order frequency of which each object had been used by the sample as a whole), and (3) exploratory behavior during selection and arrangement (based on the amount of active manipulation and experi- mentation with an object before putting it into the arrangement). Three behavior dimensions were also observed during the problem solution stage of the study. The first dimension was the openness of the initial structure of the problem. This was measured by calcula- ting (based on timed photographs) the percentage of the elapsed time for the whole exercise used to composed the final structure with its essential elements. The lower this time, the sooner the structure of 236 the problem is set or "closed." In other words, "high openness" was characterized by a large proportion of time devoted to problem composi- tion. The second behavior observed was the amount of exploratory acti- vity during the drawing stage. A higher score was given on this di- mension if the subject changed paper or medium, or changed or rear— ranged the objects. The third behavior observed at this stage was the number of changes introduced in the structure or content of the pro- blem while drawing. Higher scores resulted from behavior such as changing perspective, magnitude, or position and adding or deleting visual elements or objects in the drawing. In addition to the two stages of observation, information was gathered at what was called the "solution evaluation stage" by ques- tioning the artists when their drawings were finished. This focused on their concern for problem finding in general and each of the stages of formulation, solution, and evaluation. Problem Finding and Creativity At the conclusion of the study, each student's drawing was rated by five selected artists based on judgments of overall aesthetic value, originality, and craftmanship. The correlations between these ratings and the scores assigned to the problem finding variables observed in the experiment are reported in Table 7.2. As can be seen in Table 7.2, judgments of overall aesthetic value and originality of the drawings are significantly related to problem finding behavior observed at the problem formulation stage and to the artist's reported concern with problem finding. Drawings of students 237 Table 7.2 Correlations between ratings on three dimensions of product evaluation and the problem-finding variable scores (Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi, 1976). Process Aesthetic Overall Dimensions of Product Evaluation Variables Value Originality Craftsmanship Problem Formulation - (total score) .40C b .54 .28 Problem Solution (total score) .26 .38C .12 Concern with Problem Finding (total score) .41C Total Problem Finding Score (grand total) .47b b .56 a .65 d 31 d .31 ap‘<.0005 bp4<.oos Cp < .025 dp (.05 238 who prior to drawing handled many objects, selected unique objects, and explored them in great detail were judged higher in originality and overall aesthetic value. The work of students who explored ob- jects in greater detail was also judged higher in craftmanship. Since consistent relationships were found between behavior during the problem formulation and problem solving stages and the judgment of the quality of the product, the time spent on the arrangement and drawing task was calculated to estimate the effects of spending more time on the task. The results of this analysis showed that time spent on the problem formulation stage of the experiment was significantly related to the judged quality of the product (correlation with overall aesthetic value equals .46 ( p (.01) and correlation with originality equals .53 (p‘<.005». Time spent working on the drawing, however, was not significantly related to its judged worth. Two general conclusions were put forth as a result of this study. First, problem finding (including the discovery, formulation, and en- visagement of the problem that proceeds problem solving) is central to the creative process and is related systematically to the quality of the creative product. Second, a subjective concern for problem finding, especially at the problem formulation stage, is related to the originality of the product. The results of this study support the notions of problem solving that ascribe importance to discovery, formulation, development of the problem. The influence of these processes has been shown in the work of Selz, wertheimer, de Groot and others and has now been shown to be of central importance to more open, creative problem situations. 239 Teaching might be characterized as an instance of this latter problem situation (type-case 3 in Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi's pro- blem situation classification), where only the general "dilemma" is passed to the teacher and he or she is responsible for formulating and developing the problem situation to the point where it can be dealt with by problem solving methods. If this is so, then planning becomes the major site for this process, and the characterization of planning as merely problem solving or decision making may overlook problem finding as a crucial and central function of the planning process. Reasoning in Architectural Design Introduction In the introduction to this chapter, Simon's (1957 b) three phases of decision-~intelligence, design, and choice-~were described. It was said that most models of planning have emphasized the choice phase of decision to the neglect of the other two phases. Because of this, successive sections of this chapter have reviewed theories and re- search that have’placed greater emphasis on the aspects of problem finding, problem formulation, and design. It is the purpose of this section to review research on problem solving and decision process in a task environment where design is the primary purpose. Architectural design has been chosen since a growing body of research has been de- veloping since the early nineteen sixties to analyze and externalize the thinking processes of designers. There is no clear distinction between the act of designing and the act of planning. Design has been defined as "to conceive the idea for and prepare a description of a proposed system, artifact, or 240 aggregation of artifacts" (Archer, 1970). This definition, though not mentioning planning, sounds remarkably similar to the definition of planning used in the present study. Other definitions are more ex- plicit in their connections. For instance, design is "the act or art of making plans, schemes, patterns, (and) models" (Nadler, 1970). In architecture, design has been traditionally associated with the pro- cess of drawing up plans for a building. This was also the source for the traditional definitions of planning discussed in Chapter II. For all practical purposes.the words pl§g_and design may be used inter- changeably,and the only basis for distinction may be that design is preferred in architecture because of its popular connotation of being related to visual drawings, sketches, patterns, and blueprints. The conception of the design process has changed radically with progress in the building industry. Traditionally, it was accepted that architectural design was composed of three design processes: (1) "disposition" (generally called composition), (2) proportion (i.e., study), and (3) construction (the execution of the design) (Gaudet, 1904). Even by the beginning of the twentieth century, the developments in building technology and programmatic requirements for building made the notion of developing and completing detailed "composition" prior to interaction and collaboration with engineers and contractors impractical and nearly impossible. More recently, the design process has been portrayed as a morphological and evolutionary process characterized by iterative and cyclical features.14 14See, for instance, the Royal Institute of British Architects' Handbook of Architectural Practice and Management, 1967. 241 As the research and theoretical focus has turned to the indivi- dual designer, design has been portrayed as a process of problem solv- ing and decision making. The degree of rationality attributed to this process, however, differs widely across theorists. Jones (1970) de- scribes these differences by grouping theorists according to three con— ceptions of the designer: the designer as magician, the designer as computer, and the designer as self-organizing system. The designer as magician refers to “black box" theorists who characterize the design process as a creative, "irrational" and partly unconscious process (e.g., Osborn, 1957; Gordon, 1961; Matchett, 1968; Broadbent, 1966). The conception of designer as computer is based on the assumption that the design process is rational and explicit and that the human designer is able to operate with full knowledge of what he is doing and why he is doing it. In other words, the designer is portrayed as a human computer, ...a person who operates only on the information that is fed to him and who follows through a planned sequence of analytical, synthetic, and evaluative steps and cycles until he recognizes the best of all possible solutions (Jones, 1970, p. 6). It is this approach that underlies nearly all of the computer simula- tions of design process, but it is also the process assumed by many to be operating in human designers (e.g., Archer, 1970). The third approach, the designer as self-organizing system, is one proposed by Jones that falls between the "black box" and "glass box" theories. Jones sees both of these models as having the effect of widening the area of search for the designer beyond his capabilities and purposes. He replaces "blind" search of the alternatives with an "intelligent" search that "uses both external criteria and the results 242 of partial search to find shortcuts across unknown territory." This process is based on a notion of strategy control that accurately models the strategy itself and the external situation that the design is in- tended to fit. It is this last approach (designer as self-organizing system) coupled with elements of the first approach (designer as magi- cian) that will be discussed in this section. The peculiar mix of creativity and reason necessary for effective designing or planning is perhaps accurately captured by defining the process as a series of rationalizations, or as the father of modern architectural rationalism called them, "inspirations rigorously ana- lyzed by reason" (Viollet-le-Duc, 1863). Thus, the designer first visualizes some relationship of forms (a problem) intuitively and then strives to justify it in relationship to the constraints imposed on the finished design. Collins (1971) characterizes this process as analogous to playing the role of all the participants in a legal trial: All the time design—decisions are being made, the archi- tect must be the advocate supporting a particular solu- tion, the advocate opposing this particular solution, the witnesses testifying both for and against its adop- tion, and the judge who makes the final decision (p. 81). If this analysis of the design process is correct, then the crucial decision process in design is not that of choosing among alternative designs, but rather that of deciding whether or not to reject the de- sign that one has visualized and formulated. In other words, the quality of creative talent may be measured by the variety of formula- tions that one is capable of thinking of, but the quality of decision making depends on the criteria that are used for rejecting an idea as undesirable or unworkable. 243 This formulation of the design process points to two basic ac- tivities whose description is crucial to an understanding of planning and design. The first is the "inspiration" or problem formulation pro- cess, and the second is the evaluation and solution process. Re- search and theory related to each of these processes will now be de- scribed. Problem Formulations Most problems confronting designers are what are called ill- defined problems (Eastman, 1969; Reitman, 1964; Simon, 1973). In these types of problems, no problem specification is given or agreed upon, no formal language with precise solution operations are avail- able, and the goals to be achieved and the restrictions on the problem are open to interpretation. Moreover, no systematic test is available for deciding whether a form is acceptable as a solution. This type of problem is the same at Getzels and Csikszentmihalyi's (1976) "discov- ered" problem situations (type-case 3). In these situations, only the general dilemma for the design process is supplied, and it is up to the designer to discover and formulate the specific design problems. The formulation of the problem provides three needed elements for the problem solver. First, it provides a design goal. This is usual- ly in an ambiguous form similar to what de Groot calls a goal concep- tion. This may involve general criteria that may be met by more than one design state or form. Second, the problem formulation defines the problem space in which the problem solver will search for a solution. The third element the problem formulation provides is the set of 244 criteria for judging a solution as workable. This is largely due to a specification of a "goal-as-attained" (a part of the goal conception). In one of the few experimental studies of problem formulation, Baer (1976) defined the process as "mappings of an amorphous collec- tion of different types of statements given by the task into a parti- cular internal representation chosen at an early moment." The main task of problem formulation as he described it was to encode the goals of the task into an internal representation through processes of "re- cognition," "construction" and "adaptation." These processes take place in the following pattern. First, the designers search the pro- blem to recognize possible goals. These goals may then be transformed to goals of a specific type (for example, goals expressed in spatial terms) and may then be ordered. Next, the primary goals are evaluated as suitable candidates for action (some type of initial design) and any preconditions, restrictions, and returns on action are calculated. If these are met and seem sufficient, initial designing ("construc- tion”) takes place. The results of the action are then incorporated into the problem space and a new state of the problem is present. Subsequent steps involve another cycle of defining goals, acting on them, and further transforming the problem. In this manner, the pro- blem formulation process is made up of a step-wise filtering process followed by intermediate solving processes to delineate the problem further. Baer also found that designers "chunked" the problem to deal with parts of the design and used hierarchies in representations grad- ually to increase the detail of the formulation. Overall, this process sounds very similar to the phase structure prOposed by de Groot to describe problem solving, where thinking is 245 made up of an alternation between phases of direct analysis (elabora- tion) and phases involving integration, abstraction, and restructuring of the problem (goal). It may be that the processes involved in goal transformation and development during the problem solution stage are merely continuations fo the processes responsible for goal formulation in ill—defined problems. This argument will be pursued further in the next section on solving methods. Solving Methods There have been many more studies of the problem solution pro- cess in architectural design than of the problem formulation process (see Eastman, 1968, 1969, 1970 a, 1970 b, 1972, 1973; Henrion, 1974; Krauss and Myer, 1968; Mohr, 1972; Moran, 1970). Most of these studies have been detailed analyses of one or two designers solving architectural design problems or case studies of actual deliberations. Most of these studies of the design process have emphasized pro- cesses of problem formulation and transformation. Eastman's (1970 a) study of space planning (arranging fixtures within a limited space to meet certain constraints) found design proceeding through steps of problem definition (including both goals and constraints) and problem transformation. This process was described as the production of a series of information states, where each new transformation requires a new operation. In another article (Eastman, 1970 b), these operations were described as search strategies incorporating either "generate-and -test" processes, "means-ends analysis," or "planning." These strat- egies will be taken up in more detail later in this discussion. 246 In a case study of design, Krauss and Myer (1970) found a similar process structure. They propose that the essence of the design pro- cess involves (l) the construction of forms relating to the relevant data and constraints, (2) the reevaluation of the problem and possible solutions in light of several different sets of criteria, and (3) re- formulating the problem to correspond to this changing program (set of constraints). Rusch (1970) argues that this interplay of problem for— mulation and transformation can be explained in terms of productive and reproductive thought. By content analysis of a series of graphic representations (a series of lithographs of bulls by Picasso), he pro- poses that these two modes of thought play different roles in the de- sign activity. Decisions derived through incremental activity (pro- ductive thought) function to clarify the form of the design (problem formulation) and decisions derived through reorganization (reproduc- tive thought) function to keep the problem solver on course towards a final solution (problem transformation). Processes of problem formulation and transformation have also been incorporated into computerized models of the design process. Moran (1970) proposes a model of the designer that uses different kinds of representation or language (systems of symbols and syntactic rules for generating well-formed statements) to express different as- pects of the problem. The assumption is that as a designer thinks of a problem in different ways, different slants or problem formulations may be generated. To be useful, these different languages must be based on information already in memory so that there can be a mapping of symbols onto concepts in memory. An additional assumption of the model is that problem representations are primary influences on 247 problem solution since operations performed on different representa- tions are the primary sources of new information. In this manner, the problem formulation stage is integrated with the problem solution stage by processes involving representation --9 operation --9 reformu- lation. In his PSP (Problem Solving Process) model, Manheim (1970) dis- tinguishes between processes that generate actions and processes that choose among them. This first process is called search, and the se- cond process is grouped into three operations--prediction, evaluation, and choice. At the heart of his model is a goal formulation and revi— sion process similar to those mentioned earlier. As new actions are generated (through search), they are examined and evaluated usually resulting in a modification of the goal. Thus, the analysis proceeds as a parallel evolution of a set of actions and a set of goals until a satisfactory design is reached. Three search strategies are proposed by Eastman (1970 b) as characteristic of problem solving in architectural design. The first, "generate-and-test," involves trying every possible alternative until one is found that is satisfactory. This is considered to be an algori- thmic method (that is, it guarantees an eventual solution) and has been shown to model the decision processes of some designers in sim- ple situations or at the beginning of a solution sequence (Eastman, 1969). Means-ends analysis is a common decision process which relates possible operations (means) to criteria or goals to be achieved (ends). In its most common variant, means-ends analysis is applied to an al- ternative generated by some other strategy. It can also be used to 248 identify appropriate operations for generating the initial operation (see Eastman, 1970 b, for further discussion of these two variants). Means-ends analysis has been found to be a common heuristic process in design problems and is included as part of more complex search strat- egies. One such strategy, and the third strategy discussed by Eastman (1970 b) is "planning." Planning as a search strategy has the follow- ing qualities: 1. The problem structure is analyzed to find those ele- ments that are most closely related (subsets of the problem). 2. Planning involves search of an abstract problem space to provide a guide for problem solving. This guide functions by structuring the problem based on one type of relation (problem representation). 3. The search strategies for solving the limited rela- tionships of the problem considered in planning are the same as those used in other apsects of design. This includes generate-and-test, means-ends analysis, and even planning (itself). 4. Once the planned aspect of the problem is resolved, it is used as a guide to generate the fully specified problem (paraphrased from Eastman, 1970 b, pp. 145-147). Eastman also discusses two types of planning. "Bottom-up" plan- ning involves the aggregation of the basic elements of the design until the single design unit at the top is reached. In most cases of design, however, this situation does not exist. What is usually known is the complete design unit (the general problem) at the top of the theoretical hierarchy. The seCond mode of planning thus becomes the decomposition of the problem from the top-down. "Top-down" planning is the typical situation facing designers. It is primarily based on past experience, and, because of this, it has been criticized. Since 249 in reality, experience is not based on a complete analysis of aggrega- tions of activities, the basis for a specific composition cannot be validated. To remedy this problem, Alexander (1964) offers an alter- native procedure for decomposition and Eastman (1970 b) argues that for any well defined system, there are procedures available for aggre- gating basic elements in a "bottom-up" manner. To summarize, planning as a search strategy Provides a method for delimiting the combina- torial possibilities of a problem. By factoring out for preliminary analysis critical aspects of the problem, it focuses on particular problem representations that are most likely to provide a solution. In concluding this section, two additional aspects of the design process that have shown up in research should be mentioned. Through- out this discussion, the crucial role that problem representation and formulation plays in the design process has been emphasized. It has been repeatedly implied that the greater the ability to create dif- ferent representation the more productive is the design process. The importance of multiple representations of the probelm has been further illustrated by Eastman's (1970 a) study of space planning. There he found a clear correspondence between the kinds of constraints consid- ered by a designer and the representations used. Thus, more success- ful designing was accompanied by a greater variety of ways of repre- senting the problem. This implies that more effective design (or planning) may be facilitated by representing the problem in a variety of ways such as words, numbers, flow diagrams, sections, perspectives, and plans. It is still unknown, however, whether this use of multiple representations may be facilitative in design activities that are not of such a visual nature as architectural design. 250 Another aspect of the design process touched on in this section is the effect of past experience (or reproductive thinking). It was im- plied that aspects of the design process such as representation have a base in memory. The exact role that experience plays in the effective- ness of the process has not been thoroughly studied. One finding that supports the importance of experience was also reported by Eastman (1970 a). He found that those designers who relied upon direct retriev- al from memory for generating problem constraints were more effective ("far superior," in his words) designers than those who relied solely upon external cues for generating constraints. Although most of the designers seemed to generate constraints randomly from memory, some were found to have highly organized lists that were applied to speci- fic aspects of the problem. These results seem to indicate that the reason more experienced individauls are more effective in this type of task is because they bring more information into the situation and are less at the mercy of the task for providing cues. Summary This section has looked to research and theory in the area of architectural design to provide further insight into the aspects of problem solving referred to as design or planning. Design has been portrayed as an evolutionary process characterized by interative and cyclical features. The design process has been described as a peculiar mix of creativity and reason involving a series of "inspirations rig- orously analyzed by reason." It has been proposed that the crucial decision or judgment process is rejection of unworkable alternatives rather than choice among several alternatives. 251 Two basic processes of problem formulation and problem solution have been discussed. The research seems to indicate that the distinc- tion between the two may not be psychologically meaningful since both processes seem to be characterized by a series of problem formulations, solutions, and problem transformations. As such, the design process is quite similar to the problem solving processes proposed by Selz, Bahle, and de Groot. Three search strategies that are common in de- sign were described with special emphasis on planning as a search strategy. Finally, research findings were reported fortifying the theorized importance of multiple representations of design problems and of information, representation, and strategies brought to design problems by experienced practitioners. In the next chapter the theory and research in the areas of pro- blem solving, problem finding, and design will be combined with empiri- cal observations from this study to formulate a theoretical model of the planning process. CHAPTER VIII THE PSYCHOLOGY OF PLANNING II: A PROCESS MODEL OF TEACHER PLANNING Introduction Toward a Theory of Teacher Planning: the Model's Purpose The ultimate aim of research on teaching should be the develop- ment of models and theories to: 1. Provide the most parsimonious summary of actual or anticipated research findings, 2. Coordinate research so that many separate findings support each other, 3. Locate the most strategic or manageable propositions for testing, and 4. Provide a limited area in which to locate false pro- positions, when a hypothesis fails to meet an empirical test (Zetterberg, 1965, p . 161-166). In addition to the description of one teacher's planning, it is a purpose of this study to formulate a general model of teacher plan- ning that may serve functions similar to those mentioned by Zetterberg. As Snow (1973) suggests, there appear to be almost as many defi- nitions of theory as there are people concerned with theory. Snow defines six different levels of theory ranging from formal axiomatic theories to the development of formative hypotheses. For the purposes of this study, theory will be defined it its simplest sense--as a symbolic construction designed to bring generalizable facts into 252 253 systematic connection (Snow, 1973) or as Zetterberg (1965) defines it--an interrelated set of propositions. There are almost as many definitions of model as there are of theory. The word m9g§1_has been used interchangeably with theory at one definitional extreme and has been used to refer to a deductive sys- tem constructed to interpret the calculus of a theory at the other. In this chapter, the term will be used to refer to a system of rela- tionships used to promote theorietical development. To be more speci- fic, a model will be defined as a well developed descriptive analogy that functions to help visualize phenomena that cannot be easily or directly observed (after Snow, 1973). Using these definitions, the possibility of developing a model of teacher planning seemed within closer reach than a theoretical repre- sentation. Given the lack of previous theorizing on teacher informa- tion processing, a sufficient set of propositions about planning or decision making is not yet available to attempt the development of a formal theory. In light of this situation, a model of teacher plan- ning is the first step towards theory development. The purpose of this chapter is to set forth a general model of teacher planning. This constitutes a first attempt at relating the various components of the planning process, and its purpose is to promote theory development by generating hypotheses about teacher planning. One way to characterize this model further is to indicate what it is not intended to be. First, it is not intended to be a specific representation of Mrs. Lisa's planning. Her planning was one source of data for developing the model, but not the sole basis. Her plan- ning can be represented by the model, but the specifics of her 254 deliberation are not represented in detail. Thus, rather than being a model of an individual teacher, speculations about teacher planning in general are presented. Second, the model is not intended to be a complete and detailed picture of the planning process. The major components of the plan- ning process indicated in this and other planning studies are included in the model; however, it is possible that there are other important factors that have been overlooked. In the same manner, the temporal and interactive relationships between components (indicated by connect- ting arrows in the illustrations) are not meant to exhaust all the im- portant relationships in the planning model. Also, no effort has been made in many cases to describe in detail the processes indicated by all the arrows in the model. In short, the following discussion is the result of efforts to generate a general model of teacher planning for the purpose of moving from descirptions of planning toward a theory of the teacher planning process. Grounding of the Model Glaser and Strauss (1965, 1967) distinguish between formal theory based on logical specualation and "grounded" formal theory based on data. Before describing the process model of teacher planning, this section will describe the data base or the "grounding" of this model. Three sources of data serve as the basis for this model. The first source is the description of Mrs. Lisa's planning put forth in this study. This includes descriptions of the task environment for her planning and teaching, her goals, and her general behavior in the teaching environment. This information is similar to that suggested 255 by Newell (1973) for studying human information processing. He pro- poses that to describe the methods a person is using to solve a pro— blem'( finish--Apr. Graphing--late Apr. Money 2 column - & + -—Apr. Missing addends--115, 151, 155, 193, 194, 198 412 FA Weekly Planning Examples (from plan book) GRADE OR CLASS March 21-25, 1977 A.M. Subject, Class or Section >4 é g 91’012355 5W4“ Mdbww ' £14de [Anfh A00; C: /0,'5 0 7’” . . ”:00 16*“! ‘7’ 5M” fave K! W00 led“ My anew '3’" . /o:Jd 53917 . ¢ ”to“. v ’0' a W” [7 flaJuJ [-646 M Claw“-—2 13-13“ 173-21; 2 8 “3 2 7mafia“ 6 Awe/e 2 9: ‘g‘ S gfins‘d ””3"” dad” 7" MW 0.7076 m o.’ 30 I ”:04 1M4 9‘ 7 . 5.4%... 0 m 3", u m, i i “ah; 31,37 ;°‘" véd wk /o.'.70 filmy >c 3 é B N g E tummy Iowa. Mw,0pu-wa>/J 517-4604;“~K€‘prl"...4’ 0‘0“” ”PM" 5W , 2.70 ”W’a 9:40 dc/Jarf‘ffi Wfld [0140+ /0.'/o A lozlo g /'-"’° ”“4 5W . [/{00 SM #414» Ski/(kw 413 WEEK BEGINNING March 21-25, 1977 P.M. [:00 2nd, M M ”'3" ,4 ,3, #I-bdo 4- - H. ;(cg:1u) r . . 3.53.3 .5532 . . Ma. 4. re s... “mm—2r. (5%" “0 Jim‘aw M £ 9“: 7"" . ' 90W / MM‘IJ- we as M 4W gmfl g s 1:00 m MC? ' LA. W- 3... 4:30 W aw 5a,. 33.54. W' ' W33.“ ‘2“ u «44%.:- [W 4“” If” g 3 g Mac {on M #JJMMJ did.“ 4.1-1.2 paf'Juh- a: W‘fl 4:34: MD 375- Marni ' ' 5‘7? E E g .Jtlof moo M, mm. 3w») WU.) NW DM-errd 1:30 PM guuprf‘h’ spell—(.5 14.4.— 2 g "" (zfihdfoaaufic) E -feat ruhfla 4d“ skydgozu‘lrd% 3 :02: 32W 5W3 2:51) shed E '25 [:00 find '10; 2mm. 414 GRADE OR CLASS January l7-21, 1977 A.M. SubjectLgC1ass or Section g‘ifizfl . /o 30 6’7». 3 g WW‘T‘J' ‘— vfiuuiuy' (Jauyr: ° 4dr ”,5; 1215- ‘9 Ad. Wk. . . / m M? / 0.70 )6 9100 rod 5; wageifit (we? 1" g :3 B ”'30 5.977 /0 $40 Ifizvfit , ’4‘ 243.1. {m e 7: 00 M E SW! ' ”:33" 65‘” 2 WM E 5’4“} 7" MM MW ”27;" 5.4.ka /0.'/0 fl : Q 52 E 9" ad 'e’a ”M'fi‘j flea ' $332.4 ' . /0 {34 J79»? /o.'¢a maad (NW/ H a” 1 0 .’ I” K «2 21¢ r515 nal/0,6 Saree /o «’30 6'?m 7"00 (A! 5’3/o‘flulflaflb' g; '9Q9.2240W7{7 4/am9.%;3222éf E a F84 Adv/c: 415 WEEK BEGINNING January l7-21, 1977 ”cow -W . ' 4.30 M1V£~¢Wj 319‘ng .:.e-— was... 1:461:49”. 7'. >. W “ .4» g D05M7M7 MW .7016 MM Eat! .2'30 mud. 49a} ’4” a W W (”on E cm" B rm: no M" /300%-D’q a! 30m5¢¢afi ”coo/4545“ '2 yo ”A"? /'20 SW IOQtfiz‘é?hb¢L’ >4 g S E aux/«£5 .'/0 Z KIM/IW‘M m- MW A’— 4‘ ,9... MM 3 .W E + mm a“ mam/8. Q .70 ,6 1 ”MW 'ofl U!” Ida/6(4) Ml Pal 51:17“ Snow/ow 06 ’ . 53,30 flew-“v 3,30 #:4- 190 Add “IO/'1’“ [-i >u E E 56”?” APPENDIX G SPRING TERM SCHEDULE PLANNING PROTOCOL SPRING TERM SCHEDULE PLANNING PROTOCOL 3/11/77 STEPS: 1. Gets out 1 sheet of paper for a.m. and l for p.m. and puts days of weeks across the top (sheets turned sideways). Q - "So this will be a typical weekly format?” A - "Yes, right now I'm.just putting it in to play around with it. I have no idea what it will look like...except that I know that I'd like to try reading for a little bit in the afternoon and...these are the things I want to put in it." story hour remedial handwriting group tongue twisters and Bill Martin Books mapping Snoopy Books revising reading for high kids Finds list of things that she made up when she was doing winter schedule to make sure she had all the things in the schedule. She also gets out list of aide times. Checks through activities: mindbender - will continue handwriting books - will continue math quizzes - maybe a little extra so 2nd graders can get down math facts silent reading - USSR plus one other activity (may incorporate into reading groups) group math - money and graphing stations vowel sounds, quizzes, orange suitcase post office - out make works - out (will be done as a spelling - will try to change contest) routine of deriting wores; T-decoding; 416 417 Th-spelling bee. Will leave in spelling bee since kids really like it. decoding - may do it differently snoopy books - wants to get back to rhyming - ? them Weekly readers - still has them coming 5. Writes in activities she has to work around MTWTH 10:10 - 10:30 - recess MWF F 10:30 gym 9:10 - 9:40 - music MTWTH 2:10 - 2:30 — recess W T T 2:30 9:20 2:00 library reading lab every other week - art Looks at math and reading ("the biggest things") Puts down USSR silent reading on Friday after lunch. Looks at current schedule for reading times M, T, W, Th. a.m. - reading. Says, "Monday is a good day almost all day since they aren't so extremely exhausted. Tuesdays they aren't too bad. By Wednesday, they start pooping out in the afternoon. That's something you have to remember and having any neatly planned, hard academic activities on Friday is really dumb, so that probably won't change a lot." Says she could have reading Monday afternoon first thing. Then says, "The other thing I have to keep in mind when I start figuring that, is planning time for me. Does it mean that I'm going to have to spend my entire lunch hour preparing for that? Also, correcting. Am I going to spend a lot of time at night then correcting this sort of stuff that I usually correct at noon?" 10. Initially tries reading Monday afternoon, Tuesday morning (reading lab), Wednesday afternoon and Thursday morning. 11. Next checks her aide schedule to see how that fits in: Monday a.m. - Sandy, Mrs. P. Weds. a.m. - Sandy, (Plus student aide all day) Thurs. a.m. - Sandy, Mrs. S. 12. Tries to decide whether she could handle it all on her own Monday p.m. and Wednesday p.m. Remembers she's supposed to have a student aide all day Wednesday. 418 13. Puts down reading as such: Monday - p.m. Tuesday a.m. Wed. - p.m. Thurs. - a.m. 14. Next decides what reading to do on each day. Thursday, because of all the help (aide and parent volunteer) it would be to her advantage to do book reports - puts it down on schedule. Wednesdays - phonics books (tentatively); Mondays - wonders if parent volunteer could come Monday p.m. rather than Monday a.m. - puts it down anyway. 15. Looks for second spot for silent reading - ponders Monday as a time but decides to go on. 16. Looks to fit in math; looks at Monday a.m. Then looks at 30 minute slot after gym on Monday, Weds., and Fridays. Puts down math for Monday, Tues., Weds., Thurs. for morning. 17. Then looks at 30 minute slots after gym (11:00 - 11:30) on Monday, Wednesday and Fridays so that she can have spelling the same time every day but notices that she has four blocks on Tuesday and Thursday. "You don't want to use a half hour for one thing and a half hour for another necessarily. It usually doesn't work out that well." Q - "Why do you.want to have spelling the same time every day?" A - "So it is consistent. The more consistent it is, the better I feel the kids know what is coming off and the better they learn." 18. Says the one time she does have open, except for Friday, the period first thing in the morning. That would be okay since she says she's going to keep the spelling test where it is on Friday (11:00 - after gym) because for one thing her aide is here to help give the tests. 19. Says all year she has had handwriting first thing every morning since it is so important (especially Sept. to Dec., where they often spent 45 minutes every morning on handwriting). "Without the handwriting they can't do anything else." But she feels this is less important now and decides to write in spelling each morning. 20. She starts writing it in and says "there's only one problem - we have a spelling bee." She says that would be on Thursday morning and she has down book reports for that morning. "We can't get the book reports done and have a spelling bee. There isn't enough time." 419 21. Looks at having it the first thing in the afternoon but says that they have art every other week and throws Tuesday off, and library on Wednesdays. Keeps her from having it right after recess in the afternoon. 22. Decides to put spelling bee down for first thing in morning anyway and see what she comes up with. 23. She notices hour after recess on Thursday and changes book reports to there, since she'll have Sandy then also. 24. She next looks at half hour blocks on M, W, and Friday and puts down handwriting for that period. 25. Remembers one other thing that she is "tied into” - on Thursday afternoon she has to pass out papers. "Because I'm not about to do it (separate the week's work to go home) myself" (brackets are mine). 26. She checks her list to see what she still has: vowel sounds and quizzes says, rhyming "all reading type things" weekly reader She looks at placing these after spelling bee on Thursday, then says "Um boy, being locked into aides (times) really hurts too. That's what got me all messed up last time." 27. She next looks for a time to have the kinds read to the class - after recess. The best time for her is the after— noon. Sees they could do it Monday, Tuesday, and Friday or M, T, Th, and Fri. 28. She looks to see how many weeks they have left and counts 9. 9 times 4 is 36 but she then counts the number of interfering activities such as p.d. days and counts up 32 possible days. She says that would be enough time for everyone to read once and for some to have a second chance so she decides to do it 4 times a week (M, T, Th, F) right after recess. 29. She then puts down writing skills for Tuesday afternoon when they don't have art. This includes the Bill Martin Books, Pencil pals and newspaper. She says "That's an awful lot for every other week for what's left. Out of nine weeks that means we only get 4 times, so I need another time besides that. I'll have to have it twice." 30. She says Friday afternoon could be the other time and looks to see how many Fridays they'll miss before end of school. She sees they only miss two so after silent reading on Friday may be a good time. 420 31. She checks list again and puts mindbender word down for Monday during handwriting. 32. She says she still has some big blocks of time because she doesn't have much math in yet. 33. She next says that she is uneasy about "wasting" Monday morning on Math when they are so ready to settle down and reading with so much more sense at that (time in the) morning." 34. She starts to put reading on Monday morning but says she still has spelling there. Says spelling, depending on what she does for an activity is 15 minutes long. That would make it 9:20 and until 10:10 that would not make it (reading) very long. She says though that's about all the time she has now, she has after recess to finish up. 35. She says, "Oh boy, this trying to squeeze in spelling is really a problem." She then considers the possibility of having spelling not all at the same time: Monday - p.m. first thing Tuesday - a.m. first thing Weds. - a.m. first thing Thurs. spelling bee in afternoon again. She then says "I should probably figure out what kind of activities I want to do for spelling before I figure out when I want it." 36. She says "The spelling bee takes the longest.“ Then says, "I still think Monday morning is going to have to be reading time...Anyway I look at it, it's the only thing that makes sense since then I have Sandy and Mrs. Pat for aides." So she moves reading with book reports to Monday morning starting at 9:05. 37. She then looks at Monday morning schedule which would have reading, recess, gym, then handwriting. She says "hand- writing would be a good activity (after gym) since it is kind of quiet and settling." She says from 9:05 to recess would give her an hour and 5 minutes until recess and she thinks she could get all of the reading and book reports done in that period. 38. She then goes to Monday afternoon. She says she could have spelling first thing, then she has two blocks of time left. She considers putting math in after spelling, but says, "My problem is that I always think of spelling as being a writing skills or reading type thing...and I wonder how much (of a) transition problem it would be to go from spelling into math. One thing, I can anticipate the fact that they're not all going to be done (with their spelling) at the same time. So, it will mean some of then having to wait after they finish their spelling to start their math, if I wanted to do anything 421 as a large group or even in small groups." 39. She then looks at last period in afternoon. She says on Wednesdays she has 15 minutes after library and looks at putting spelling at 3:10 every day and come back from library on Wednesdays 5 minutes early. She then says, "That has possibilities. It depends what I want to put in here (pointing to open periods on Monday and Tuesday afternoons after recess). Maybe I finally found a place for it." 40. She says that would leave her those two open times for social studies and science...or for something else. 41. She then goes through complete Monday schedule: reading (book reports), gym, handwriting (mindbender), math, story, blank, spelling. She then puts down a math quiz and math facts before math and then says "Is there anything else besides science auui social studies that I'd rather fit in Monday because Monday is usually such a good day for them?" 42. She considers weekly reader then looks to see how much time she really has there. She says the story reading will probably go until 2:45 since they are such slow readers, and it would be 2:50 before they could start on an activity. "Twenty minutes would be all I have here, so that's too short for any big lesson." So she decides to put weekly reader in there, saying, "Let's see if that makes sense as we go along." 43. She then looks at Tuesday morning and says she has a time open before reading lab since she refuses to go up there for that long of a time. Then reading lab, blank, art or writing skills, story, blank, spelling. 44. She then looks for something to do before reading lab. Considers decoding, rhyming, vowel sounds. She then glances over Wednesday's schedule, then finally puts in handwriting books for that period. 45. Lisa then says that she feels she can cut math back to 3 times a week since there is not a whole lot that the kids need to learn that they don't already know. Since they "hit it so hard" this winter (4 times/week). She says, "We can cover everything we need to 3 periods and still have time left over" and says "when I'm thinking about math, I'm not putting it as a top priority anymore; I'm putting more emphasis on where they lack in reading and writing skills now." She says they need work on creative writing, vowels and sounding out words rather than just reading in workbooks. "So I'm feeling the need to have reading a little bit more often and math a little less.“ 46. She says silent reading after story on Tuesday would help fulfill this need. She says it's a completely different type 422 of reading activity from reading lab in the morning (where they listen to tapes) and says "I don't want to have two of the same type of activity in the same day." 47. She decides to leave period open for Tuesday morning after recess. 48. She then looks at Wednesday's schedule. She says she has Sandy and an aide in the morning and could easily have reading in the afternoon and have math in the morning and a short activity after gym. 49. She writes in math quiz then math (Sandy would be here for a math group); then gym and leaves period after gym blank. 50. She then goes to Thursday schedule. She has reading after recess and Sandy could read with a group and she could show the orange suitcase before she arrives. 51. She says that before recess may be a good time for math and that would put two math periods back to back on Wednesday and Thursdays. She says that that makes sense to her since it would give her the carry over from Wednesday to Thursday that she doesn't have if the periods are far apart. 52. She says that leaves a big block of time on Thursday afternoon "which I need to get for science or social studies. I haven't gotten any in here yet." She then sets aside time for doing either science or social studies. 53. She then reviews Thursday schedule as math quiz, math, recess, orange suitcase and reading. She says the one group she is concerned about is the bottom reading group and this gives her Monday morning, Wednesday afternoon and Thursday morning to read with them. "I want to be able to read with them at least three times a week. Because without that kind of constant reading with them, they are going to forget a lot. They don't know it well enough to go off on their own. The 5 boys in 'Surprises' book still need to be read with at least twice a week but I would like to give them the opportunity to read once a week on their own because I know they're feeling that everyone else is better than they are since everyone else gets to read on their own and they aren't." 54. She reviews Thursday afternoon as it is set: Science or social studies, recess, story, pass out papers and spelling bee. 55. She then moves to Friday's schedule. She puts down music, add up points, blank (since they won't be doing word contest in class), recess, gym, spelling tests; p.m. - silent reading, writing skills, recess, treat, clean up and movies. "I really feel that movie time is good for the kinds and it gives me time 423 also to put my head back together Friday afternoon." She says it also gives them a time to be with the class next door (since they see them together). 56. She then goes back to see where the gaps are: Monday is full Tuesday - after morning recess Weds. - after gym Thurs. - full Fri. - 15 minutes not full (in a.m. after adding up points) 57. She says she has 3 gaps and then looks to see what she has left on her list. 58. She sees Snoopy books and says "perfect" and puts in the 15 minute slot on Friday morning. 59. She says she has left: remedial handwriting group rhyming (if she wants) decoding She says she could stick with using decoding in spelling. 60. She says, "Here (Weds. morning) is half an hour and here (Tuesday morning) is more like an hour." She looks at them for a minute and then says "Priorities. I only have science and social studies once. Shame on me. (you) see how much priority I give that." 61. She thinks about putting handwriting group in the slot after gym on Wednesdays since she could take that group and the student aide could take the rest of the group and do something with them. She also notices she has no station work in yet and other kids could do station work or rhyming. She says "It has to be something the other kids won't miss out on doing. It can't be something particularly enthusiastically fun cause then they won't want to work with me. Because they won't want to work with me anyway since we really work." 62. Decides to put down station work and keep in mind that she has an aide since she might be able to do something completely different in their stations. 63. She then says that this could be a good time for the kids to practice their math facts - do flash cards. "No matter how bad the aide would be she could handle that." Lisa then checks through her class list to see how many kids she'll have in the handwriting group - she counts 9 or possibly 10. That would leave the aide with 16 or 17 kids. 424 65. Lisa then says the block of time on Tuesday morning could be filled in with science or social studies. 66. She then says "that will do it. It will get in everything in that I had planned and be stressing the things I feel need stressing." She then checks her aide times and says she'll only have to change one of her parent volunteers from Tuesday morning before reading lab. 67. She then checks over reading periods again and switches orange suitcase to Wednesdays and puts folders on Thursday. 68. She then says "That looks pretty good to me. You have to try it out for 3 weeks to find out...since it takes a while for the kids to adjust to a new schedule." TOTAL TIME INVOLVED - 75 MINUTES LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Alexander, C. Notes on the synthesis of form. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1964. Anddon, E.J. and Hunter, E. Verbal interaction in the classroom: The verbal interaction category system. In Amidon, E.J. and Rough, J.B. (eds), Interaction analysis: theory, research and application. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1967. Anderson, R.L. and Faust, G.W. Educational psychology. New York: Dodd, Mead, and Company, 1974. Anderson, V.W. Principles and.procedures for curriculum improvement. New York: The Ronald Press Company, 1956. Archer, L.B. An overview of the structure of the design process. In Moore, G. (ed), Emerging methods in environmental design and planning. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970. Argenti, J. Systematic corporate planning. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1974. Baer, A.E. A cognitive model of problem formation in design. Carnegie-Mellon University, Institute of Physical Planning, Research Report No. 62, 1976. Bahle, J. Zur psychologie des musikalischen gestaltens, eine untersuchung fiber das komponieven auf experimenteller und historischer grundlage. Archiv ffir die gestamte Psychologie, 1930, 14, 289-390. Bahle, J. Der musikalische schaffungsprozess. Psychologie der Schdpferischen erlebniss und antriebformen. Leipzig: Hirzel, 1936. Bahle, J. Eingebung und tat im musikalischen schaffen. Ein beitrag zur psychologie dur entwicklungs-und schaffensgesetze Schdpferischer menschen. Leipzig: Hirzel, 1939. Barker, R.G. 0n the nature of the environment. Journal of Social Issues, 1963, 12, 17-38. Barnard, C.I. The functions of the executive. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1938. 425 426 Beauchamp, G.A. Basic dimensions of elementary methods. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1965. Becker, H.S. Problems of inference and proof in participant observation. American Sociological Review, 1958, 38, 652-660. Berliner, D.C., Cahen, L., Filby, N., Fisher, C., Marlave, R., Moore, J. Proposal for Phase III-B of the BTES. San Francisco: Far West Laboratory for Educational Research and Development, 1976. Birch, H.G. and Rabinowitz, 8.8. The negative effect of previous experience on productive thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1951, 41, 121—125. Blackham, H.J. Political discipline in a free Society. London: Allen and Unwin, 1961. Bloom, B.S. The thought processes of students in discussion. In French, S.J. (ed), Accent on teaching: Experiments in general education. New York: Harper, 1954. Bobrow, D.G. Dimensions of representation. In Bobrow, D.G. and Collins, A. (eds), Representation and understanding. New York: Academic Press, Inc., 1975. Bollens, J.C. and Schmandt, H.G. The metropolis: Its people, politics, and economic life. New York: Harper, 1965. Broadbent, G.H. Creativity. In Gregory, S.A. (ed), The design method. London: Butterworths, and New York: Plenum Press, 1966. Brophy, J. and Evertson, C. The teacher effectiveness project. In T. Good (ed), Trends in the study of the classroom. New York: APS Publications, 1974. (a) Brophy, J. and Evertson, C. The Texas teacher effectiveness project: Summary report. Research report, the Research and Development Center for Teacher Education. The University of Texas at Austin, 1974. (b) Brophy, J. and Evertson, C. The Texas teacher effectiveness project: Presentation of non-linear relationships and summary discussion. Research report, the Research and Development Center for Teacher Education. The University of Texas at Austin, 1974. (c) Caswell, H.L. and Cambell, D.S. Curriculum develgpment. New York: American Book Company, 1935. Chase, W.G. and Simon, H.A. The mind's eye in chess. In Chase, W.G. (ed), Visual information processing. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1973. 427 Clark, C.M. and Peterson, P.L. Teacher stimulated recall of inter- active decisions. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. San Francisco, 1976. Clarkson, G.P.E. Portfolio selection: A simulation of trust investment. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall, 1962. Collier, C.C., Houston, W.R., Schmatz, R.R. and Walsh, W.J. Teaching in the modern elementaryischool. New York: The MacMillan Co., 1967. Collins, Peter. Architectural judgment. Mbntreal: McGill-Queen's University Press, 1971. Craik, F.I.M. Primary memory. British Medical Bulletin, 1971, 21! 232-236. Cusick, P.A. Inside high school. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Dahl, R.A. The policies of planning. International Social Science Journal, 1959, 3! 340. Davidoff, P. and Reiner, T.A. A choice theory of planning. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 1962, fig, 103-115. Davis, R.H., Alexander, L.T., and Yelon, S.L. Learning system design. New York: McGraw Hill, 1974. Dawes, R.M. The mind, the model, and the task. Paper presented at the 7th Annual Indiana Theoretical and Cognitive Psychological Conference. Bloomington, Indiana, April, 1974. de Groot, A.D. Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton, 1965. Dewey, J. Human nature and conduct. New York: H. Holt, & Co., 1922. Doyle, W. Learning the classroom environment: An ecological analysis of induction into teaching. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. New York, April, 1977. (a) Doyle, W. The uses of nonverbal behaviors: Toward an ecological model of classrooms. Merrill-Palmer Quaterly, July, 1977. (b) Dror, Y. The planning process: A facet design. International Review of Administrative Sciences, 1963, 22, 46-58. Dunker, K. On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 1945, 5Q, whole no. 270. 428 Dunkin, M.J. and Biddle, B.J. The study of teaching. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1974. Dyckman, J.W. Planinng and decisions theory. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 1961, 21, 335-345. Eastman, C.M. Explorations in the cognitive process of design. Carnegie-Mellon University, Department of Computer Science ARPA Report, DDC No. 671-158, 1968. Eastman, C.M. Cognitive processes and ill-defined problems: A case study from design. Proceedings of the Joint International Conference on Artificial Intelligence. Washington, D.C., 1969. Eastman, C.M. On the analysis of intuitive design processes. In Moore, G. (ed), Emerging methods in environmental design and planning. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970. (a) Eastman, C.M. Problem solving strategies in design. Carnegie- Mellon University, School of Urban and Public Affairs, Reprint No. 23, 1970. (b) Eastman, C.M. General space planner: A system of computer aided architectural design, user documentation. In Mitchell, W.J. (ed), Proceedings of the EDRA3/AR8 conference. Berkeley, 1972. Eastman, C.M. Automated space planning. Artificial Intelligence, 1973 p 1' 41-640 Eisner, E.W. Educational objectives: Help or hinderance. School Review, 1967, 25, 250-266. Elstein, A., Shulman, L.S., Sprafka, S.A., Jason, H., Kagan, H., Allal, L., Gordon, M., Loupe, M.J., and Jordon, R.D. Medical problem solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, in press. Etzioni, A. Mixed scanning: A "third" approach to decision-making. Public Administrative Review, 1967, 32, 385-392. Etzioni, A. The active society: A theory of societal and political processes. New York: The Free Press, 1968. Faber, M. and Sears, D. (eds). The crisis in planning. vol. 1: The issues; V61. 2 : The experience. London: Chattos and Windus for the Sussex University Press, 1972. Fishburn, C.E. Learning the role of the teacher. Journal of Teacher Education, 1966, 12, 329-331. Friedmann, J. A conceptual model for the analysis of planning behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly 1967, 2, 225-252. 429 Friedman, J. and Hudson, B. Knowledge and action: A guide to planning theory. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 1974, 49, 2-16. Gagne, R.M. The analysis of instructional objectives for the design of instruction. In Glaser, R. (ed), Teaching machines and programmed learninggAVol 2. Data and directions. Washington, D.C.: National Education Association, 1965. Gagne, R-M. The conditions of learning, (2nd ed). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1970. Gagne, R.M. Essentials of learningifor instruction. Hinsdale, 111.: The Dryden Press, 1974. Gaudet, J. Elements et theorie de l'architecture, Vol. 1., Librarairie de la Construction Moderne, 1904. Getzels, J.W. and Csickszentmihalyi, M. The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem finding in art. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1976. Glaser, B. and Strauss, A. The discovery of substantive theory: A basic strategy underlying qualitative research. The American Behavioral Scientist, 1965, 8, 5-12. Glaser, B. and Strauss, A. The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Chicago: Aldine, 1967. Glaser, R. Implications of training research for education. In Hilgard, E.R. (ed), Theories of learning and instruction. The 63rd Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1964. Gordon, W.J.J. Synectics: The development of creative capacity. New York: Harper and Row, 1961. Gross, B.M. National planning: Findings and falicies. Public Administration Review, 1965, 25, 263-273. Gump, P.V. The classroom behavior setting: Its nature and relation to student behavior. Final Report, Contract No. OE—4-10-107, U.S. Bureau of Research, H.E.W., 1967. Gump, P.V. Intra-setting analysis: The third grade classroom as a special but instructive case. In Willems, E.P. and Raush, H.L. (eds), Naturalistic viewpoints in psychological research. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1969. Gwynn, J.M. Curriculum principles and social trends. New York: MacMillan Company, 1943. 430 Harnischfeger, A. and Wiley, D. Conceptual policy issues in elementary school teaching-learning processes. Research proposal submitted to N.I.E. Basic Skills Group, 1976. Henrion, M. NOtes on the synthesis of problems: An exploration of problem formulations used by human designers and automated systems. Unpublished Masters Thesis, Royal College of Arts. London, 1974. Hilsom, S. and Cane, 3.5. The teacher's day, London: National Foundation for Educational Research in England and Wales, 1971. Hirschman, A.O. Developmental projects observed. Washington, D.C.: The Brookings Institute, 1967. Homans, G.C. The human group. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1950. Hudgins. B.B. The instructional process. Chicago: Rand McNally and Company, 1971. Hymes, D. Foundations in sociolirguistics. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press, 1974. Institute for Research on Teaching. Proceedings of the research on teaching mathematics conference. Conference Series No. 3. East Lansing: Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State University, in press. Jackson, P.W. The way teaching is. Washington: National Education Association, 1965. Jackson, P.W. Life in classrooms. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1968. Jacobson, M.D., Stimart, R.P., and Wren, G.T. Models and educational research. American Educational Research Journal, Jones, J. The state of the art in design methods. In Moore, G. (ed), Emerging methods in enviornmental design and planning. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970: Joyce, B., Morine, G., Weil, M., and Wald, R. (eds), Materials for modules: A classification of competency oriented tools for teacher education. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1971. Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D.R., Goldberg, A.D., and Cambell, R. Studies in human interaction; Interpersonal process recall stimulated by videotape. East Lansing: Michigan State University, 1967. 431 Kounin, J.S. Discipline and group management in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1970. Krauss, R. and Myer, J. Design: A case history. Center for Building Research, MIT, 1968. Krug, B.A. Curriculum planning. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1950. Lanier, J.E. and Shulman, L.S. Technical proposal, Institute for Research on Teaching. Michigan State University, 1975. Lewin, K. Intention, will, and need. In Rapaport, D. (ed), Organization and pathology of thought. New York: Columbia University Press, 1951. Lindblom, C.E. The science of "muddling through". Public Administrative Review, 1959, 12, 79-88. Lindblom, C. The intelligence of democracy. New York: Free Press, 1965. Lindsay, P.H., and Norman, D.A. Human information processing: An introduction to psychology. New York: Academic Press, 1972. Lundgren, U.P. Frame factors and the teaching process: A contribution to curriculum theory and theory on teaching. Stockholm: Almgrist and Wiksell, 1972. MacDonald, J.B. Myths about instruction. Educational Leadership, 1965, 22, 571-576, 609-617. MacDonald, J.B., Wolfson, B.J., and Zaret, E. Reschooling society: A conceptual model. Washington, D.C.: Association for Super- vision and Curriculum Development, 1973. Mager, R.F. Preparing objectives forrprogrammed instruction. San Francisco: Fearon, 1962. Maier, N.R.F. Reasoning in humans. 1. On direction. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1930, ll, 115-143. Maier, N.R.F. Reasoning in humans. 2. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Cgrparative Psychology, 1931, l}, 181-194. Maier, N.R.F. Reasoning in humans. 3. The mechanisms of equivalent stimuli and of reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1945, 35, 349-360. Mandler, J.M. and Mandler, G. Thinking: from association to gestalt. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1964. 432 Newell, A. You can't play 20 questions with nature and win: Projective comments on the papers of this symposium. In Chase, W.G. (ed), Visual information processing. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1973. Newell, A. and Simon, H.A. Human problem solvingr, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1972. Nisbett, R.E. and Wilson, T.D. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 1977, 82, 231-259. Osborn, A.F. Applied imagination: Principles and procedures of creative problem solving. New York: Scribners, 1957. Peterson, D.G. The elementary school teacher. New York: Appleton-Century-Cro fts , 1964 . Peterson, P.L., Marx, R.W. and Clark, C.M. Teacher planning, teacher behavior, and student achievement. Unpublished manuscript, 1977. Peterson, W. On some meanings of "planning". Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 1965, El, 130-142. Philips, S.U. Participant structures and communicative competence: Warm springs children in community and classroom. In Cazden, C., Hymes, D., and John, V.C. (eds), Functions of langpage in the classroom. New York: Teachers College Press, 1972. Popham, W.J. and Baker, E.L. Systematic instruction. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall, Inc., 1970. Popham, W.J., Eisner, E.W., Sullivan, H.J., and Tyler, L.L. Instructional objectives. American Educational Research Association Monograph Series on Curriculum Evaluation, No. 3, Chicago: Rand McNally, 1969. Reitman, W.R. Heuristic decisions procedures, open constraints, and the structure of ill-defined problems. In Shelly, M.W. and Bryan, G.L. (eds), Human judgment and optimality. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1964. Royal Institute of British Architects, Handbook of architectural practice and management., 1967. Rusch, C.W. The role of graphic activity in the design process. In Moore, G. (ed), Emergingimethods in environmental design and planning. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970. Sayior, J.G. and Alexander, W.M. Planning curriculum for schools. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1974. 433 Manheim, M.L. A design process model: Theory and applications to transportation planning. In Moore, G. (ed), Emerging methods in environmental design and planning. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970. March, J. and Simon, H.A. Organizations. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1959. Matchett, E. Control of thought in creative work. Chartered Mechanical Engineer, 1968, l5, 163-166. Meyerson, M. and Bonfield, E.L. Politics, planning and the public interest. Glencoe, 111.: The Free Press, 1955. Miller, G.A. The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 1956, g, 81-97. Miller, G.A., Galanter, E. and Pribram, K.H. Plans and structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1960. Mohr, M. A computer model of the design process that uses a concept of an apartment floorplan to solve layout problems. In Mitchell, W.J. (ed), Proceedings of ERDA3/AR8 conference, UCLA, Los Angeles, 1972. Moran, T. A model of a multilingual designer. In Moore, G. (ed), Emerging methods in environmental design and plannipg. MIT Press, 1970. Morine, G. Planning skills: Paradoxes and parodies. Journal of Teacher Education, 1973, l2, 135-143. Morine, G. Special study L: A study of teacher planning (BTES Technical Report 76-3-1). San Francisco: Far West Laboratory for Educational Research and Development, 1976. Morine, G. and Joyce, E. Variables that effect teacher's decision making: A proposal. Proposal submitted to Institute for Research on Teaching. Michigan State University, 1976. Morine, G. and Morine, H. Teaching. In Goodlad J.I. and Shane, H.G. (eds), The elementary school in the United States. The Seventy-second Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part 2. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1973. Myrdal, G. Beyond the welfare state: Economic planning and its international implications. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1970. Nadler, G. Engineering research and design in socioeconomic systems. In Moore, G. (ed), Emergingimethods in environmental design and planning. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1970. 434 Seely, J.R. What is planning? Definition and strategy. Journal of the American Institute of Planners, 1962, 35, 91-97. Selz, O. Ueber die gesetz des geordneten denkverlaufs. Stuttgart: Spemann, 1913. Selz, O. Zur psychologie des produktiven denkens und des irrtums. Bonn: Friedrich Cohen, 1922. Selz, 0. Die gesetze de produktiven und reproduktiven geistestatigkeit. Kurzgefasste darstellung. Bonn: Fredrich Cohen, 1924. Shavelson, R.J. What is the basic teaching skill? Journal of Teacher Education, 1973, 32, 144-151 Shulman, L.S. Seeking styles and individual differences in patterns of inquiry. School Review, 1965, 25, 258-266. Shulman, L.S. and Elstein, A.S. Studies of problem solving, judgment, and decision making. In Kerlinger, F.N. (ed), Review of Research in Education. vo1. 3. Itaska, Illinois: F.E. Peacock and Co., 1975. Shulman, L.S., Loupe, M.J. and Piper, R.M. Studies of the inquiry process: Inquiry patterns of students in teacher training programs. East Lansing: Final Report, Project No. 5-0597, Office of Education. Michigan State University, 1968. Shultz, J. It's not whether you win or loose or how you play the game: A microethnographic analysis of game playing in a kindergarten/first grade classroom. Working paper, Newton Classroom Interaction Project, Harvard Graduate School of Education, 1976. Simon, H.A. Administrative behavior: A study of decision-making processes in administrative organizations (2nd ed.) New York: The MacMillan Co., 1957. (a) Simon, H.A. Models of man. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1957. (b) Simon, H.A. The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 1969. Simon, H.A. The structure of ill structured problems. Artificial Intelligence, 1973, 2, 181-201. Simon, H.A., Smithburg, D.W., and Thompson, V.A. Public Administration. New York: Knopf, 1950. 435 Snow, R.E. Theory construction for research on teaching. In Travers, R.M.W. (ed), Second handbook of research on teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1973. Smith, L.M. and Geoffrey, W. The complexities of an urban class- room. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1968. Taba, H. Curriculum development, theory and practice. New York: Harcourt, Brace and WOrld, Inc., 1962. Taba, H. Teaching strategies and cognitive in elementary school children. San Francisco: San Francisco State College, 1966. Tinbergen, J. On the theory of economic policy. Amsterdam: North Holland, 1952. Tinbergen, J. Economiclpolicy: Principles and design. Amsterdam: North Holland, 1964. Tyler, R.W. Basic principles of curriculum and instruction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1950. varduin, J.R. Cooperative curriculum development. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall, Inc., 1967. Viollet-le-Duz, E.E. Entretiens, 1863. Wertheimer, M. Productive thinking. Tavistock, 1959. Wheatly, P. The pivot of the four corners: A preliminary inquiry into the origins and character of the ancient Chinese city. Chicago: Aldine, 1971. Wiley, D.E. and Harnishfeger, A. Explosion of a myth: Quantity of schooling and exposure to instruction, major educational vehicles. Educational Researcher, 1974, 5, 7-12. Wortmen, P.M. Medical diagnosis: An information-processing approach. Computers and Biomedical Research, 1972, 5, 315-328. ‘ Zahorik, J.A. The effect of planning on teaching. Elementary School Journal, 1970, ll, 143-151. Zahorik, J.A. Teachers' planning models. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Washington, D.C., April, 1975. Zetterberg, H.L. On theory and verification in sociology. Totowa, N.J.: Bedminster Press, 1965.