THE INFLUENCE OF EARLY PARENT INVOLVEMENT AND DEPRESSION ON ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT, PSYCHOSOCIAL BEHAVIORS, AND MOTIVATION IN CHILDREN WITH LEARNING DISABILITIES ACROSS ELEMENTARY SCHOOL By Seema Mahdavi A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of School Psychology – Doctor of Philosophy 2017 ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF EARLY PARENT INVOLVEMENT AND DEPRESSION ON ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT, PSYCHOSOCIAL BEHAVIORS, AND MOTIVATION IN CHILDREN WITH LEARNING DISABILITIES ACROSS ELEMENTARY SCHOOL By Seema Mahdavi Children with learning disabilities represent the largest category of students served within special education systems in schools, and are at increased risk for academic and psychosocial problems in comparison to peers without learning disabilities. While much of clinical practice and research focus has been on academic interventions, understanding other risk and protective factors that hinder or promote academic and psychosocial development in children with and without learning disabilities is critical to inform other points of intervention. More specifically, parenting behavior in early childhood including, parent depression and parent school involvement, may hinder or promote outcomes for children with learning disabilities. The purpose of this study was to examine whether parent depression, a risk factor, and parent school involvement, a protective factor, predicted academic and psychosocial outcomes in children with subtypes of learning disabilities (i.e., reading disability, math disability, co-morbid reading and math disability) differentially as compared to children without learning disabilities at two time points in elementary school, kindergarten and fifth grade. Primary analyses used multi-sample Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) to evaluate the effects of parent depression and parent school involvement on math and reading academic achievement and psychosocial behavior at kindergarten, as well as on psychosocial behavior in fifth grade. Additionally, the long-term effects of academic achievement and psychosocial behavior in kindergarten on these outcomes in fifth grade were also examined. Data was drawn from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study – Kindergarten Cohort of 1998-1999 (ECLS-K) kindergarten and fifth grade data collection waves. The final study sample consisted of approximately 10,630 children. Results indicated that parent depression did not significantly predict parent school involvement, however direct effects of parent depression on kindergarten reading achievement and direct effects of parent school involvement on kindergarten outcomes were noted for children with and without learning disabilities. Additionally, complex relations between kindergarten and fifth grade academic and psychosocial outcomes arose. Finally, effects of gender, age, race, and socio-economic status were examined and significantly influenced both kindergarten and fifth grade outcomes. Findings point to the importance of promoting parent school involvement as a means to improve outcomes for children with and without learning disabilities, as well as the importance of promoting early academic achievement and psychosocial development. Copyright by SEEMA MAHDAVI 2017 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Graduate school, and the dissertation project specifically, is a long and at times, arduous journey. My graduate school journey would not have been possible without the resources and supports from several entities. I would like to acknowledge the National Center for Education Statistics for granting me access to a fantastically rich dataset and the many researchers and participants whom contributed so significantly to educational and psychological science in their various roles that culminated in the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study – Kindergarten Cohort of 1998-1999. I would also like to thank the Michigan State University School Psychology Program for fostering a diverse and bright academic environment. First, I am forever grateful to Dr. Jodene G. Fine, my dissertation chair, academic advisor, and wise mentor. Her years of guidance, support, and patience in all academic matters have helped shape, deepen, and refine my research and clinical skills. She has left a permanent impression on my being, pushing me to think more critically and modeling a type of caring and warmth that cannot be adequately expressed in words. I am lucky to have her in my circle. Second, I would like to thank the members of my dissertation committee, Drs. Ryan Bowles, Carol Sue Englert, and Evelyn Oka, for their interest, constructive feedback, and time. Dr. Bowles’s generosity in sharing his statistical expertise and his readiness to impart his knowledge about early child development was instrumental to completing this project. I am appreciative of Dr. Englert’s willingness to share her thoughtful analysis and interpretation through the eyes of a special education and literacy expert. I am thankful for Dr. Oka’s keen ability to guide my understanding of complex psychosocial behaviors and mold my discussion of equally complex results. v Third, to the friends and comrades who supported me through this journey, thank you! To my cohort at Michigan State University and Nationwide Children’s Hospital, especially Kara Constantine, Samanta Boddapati De Anda, Taylor Hicks-Hoste, and Mohammed Palejwala, thank you for providing a listening ear, endless cheerleading, thoughtful guidance, and comedic relief. To Hannah Sahin and Michelle Baker, thank you for being a part of another milestone in my life and for your constant support and love. Finally, this dissertation is dedicated to my parents, Anu and Ali, who have made my successes possible and who have supported me through all life’s challenges. Their tireless sacrifices, boundless encouragement, and unwavering love have allowed me to pursue my professional passions. My graduate degree is as much theirs as it is mine. To Shireen, your optimism, love, and silliness lifted me so many times throughout graduate school. There is no better sister than you. To Lee Ann, Roger, Hannah, Ivy, and Michael, thank you for always sharing your enthusiasm for my work. And Thomas, love of my life, without you I would not have reached this point with an ounce of sanity. Your devotion, patience, support, and humor have helped me tackle the hills and valleys of graduate school. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES...................................................................................................................... x LIST OF FIGURES..................................................................................................................... xii CHAPTER 1................................................................................................................................ 1 INTRODUCTION....................................................................................................................... 1 CHAPTER 2............................................................................................................................... 4 LITERATURE REVIEW...................... .................................................................................... 4 Learning Disabilities and Academic Achievement..................................................................... 4 Definitions of Learning Disability.............................................................................................. 6 Mathematics Disability....................................................................................... 9 Mathematics disability subtypes......................................................................... 15 Summary............................................................................................................. 16 Reading Disability.............................................................................................. 17 Co-morbid Mathematics and Reading Disability............................................... 22 Psychosocial Development in Children with Learning Disabilities........................................... 23 Externalizing Problems....................................................................................... 26 Internalizing Problems........................................................................................ 29 Interpersonal Problems....................................................................................... 33 Motivational Factors in Children with Learning Disabilities..................................................... 35 Self-Regulation................................................................................................... 35 Ability Beliefs..................................................................................................... 37 Intrinsic Motivation............................................................................................ 39 Summary............................................................................................................. 40 Parent Influences on Psychosocial Development, Motivation, and Academic Achievement.... 41 Parental Depression............................................................................................ 41 Parent School Involvement................................................................................. 44 Purpose of Present Study............................................................................................................ 48 Research Questions and Hypotheses.......................................................................................... 50 CHAPTER 3............................................................................................................................... METHOD................................................................................................................................... Study Model................................................................................................................................ Study Design............................................................................................................................... Data Collection................................................................................................. ECLS-K Original Sample........................................................................................................... Missing Data..................................................................................................... Final Sample for Current Study.................................................................................................. Exclusion Criteria............................................................................................... Learning Disabled Sample................................................................................. Sampling Weights.............................................................................................. vii 54 54 54 57 57 61 62 71 72 73 75 Variables and Measures.............................................................................................................. 80 Parent Depression................................................................................................ 81 Early Parent School Involvement....................................................................... 83 Academic Achievement...................................................................................... 85 Early academic achievement............................................................................... 85 Later academic achievement............................................................................... 86 Psychosocial Behaviors....................................................................................... 88 Kindergarten psychosocial behavior................................................................... 89 Fifth grade psychosocial behavior...................................................................... 91 Motivation........................................................................................................... 93 Covariates............................................................................................................ 95 Age at assessment................................................................................................ 95 Gender................................................................................................................. 96 Race-ethnicity..................................................................................................... 96 Socioeconomic status.......................................................................................... 97 Data Analyses............................................................................................................................. 98 Preliminary Analyses.......................................................................................... 99 Structural Equation Analyses.............................................................................. 100 Model Fit............................................................................................................. 104 CHAPTER 4............................................................................................................................... 122 RESULTS................................................................................................................................... 122 Preliminary Analyses.................................................................................................................. 122 Differences between LD and non-LD Groups.................................................... 122 Differences between Categories of LD............................................................... 124 Parent depression group differences................................................................... 125 Parent school involvement group differences..................................................... 125 Academic achievement group differences.......................................................... 126 Psychosocial behavior group differences............................................................ 127 Motivation group differences.............................................................................. 128 Analyzing the Structural Model.................................................................................................. 141 Establishing Factors by Testing the Measurement Model.................................. 141 Measurement Invariance.................................................................................... 142 Testing the Structural Latent Model................................................................... 142 Research Question One: Influence of Parent Depression and Parent Involvement on Outcomes for All Children........................................................................................................................... 147 Mediation Analysis............................................................................................. 147 Research question 1a.......................................................................................... 148 Research question 1c........................................................................................... 149 Research question 1d........................................................................................... 150 Influence of covariates........................................................................................ 150 Moderation Analysis........................................................................................... 156 Research question 1b........................................................................................... 156 Research Question Two: Comparison Between Children with LD and Children without LD... 157 Testing the Model Separately for LD and non-LD Samples............................... 158 Multi-group model............................................................................................... 158 viii Procedural steps.................................................................................................. 158 Research question 1a for multi-group sample.................................................... 159 Research question 1c for multi-group sample..................................................... 159 Research question 1d for multi-group sample..................................................... 160 Influence of covariates......................................................................................... 161 Multi-group Moderation Analysis....................................................................... 164 Research Question Three: Group Comparison Between Subtypes of LD................................... 165 Testing the Mediation Model Separately for LD Subtype Samples.................... 165 Testing the Moderation Model Separately for LD Subtype Samples.................. 166 CHAPTER 5................................................................................................................................ 167 DISCUSSION.............................................................................................................................. 167 Parent Depression and School Involvement as Predictors of Kindergarten Outcomes............... 170 Kindergarten Achievement and Psychosocial Behavior as Predictors of Fifth Grade Outcomes..................................................................................................................................... 177 Kindergarten Academic Achievement................................................................. 177 Kindergarten Psychosocial Behavior.................................................................. 181 Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior and Motivation as Predictors of Academic Achievement . 184 Conclusions and Clinical Implications........................................................................................ 187 Limitations and Future Research................................................................................................. 188 APPENDIX.................................................................................................................................. 190 REFERENCES............................................................................................................................ 192 ix LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Overview of Original ECLS-K Data Collection Sample ……………............... Table 2 Comparison of Demographic Information Between Included and Excluded Samples……………………………………………………................................ 65 Table 3 Comparison of Covariates and Predictor Variables for Included and Excluded Samples………………………………………………........................................ 66 Table 4 Comparison of Covariates and Predictor Variables for Included and Excluded Samples (Ordinal)………………………………………................................... 68 Table 5 Overview of Data Collection for Final Sample……………………................... 77 Table 6 Demographic Characteristics of Final Sample (n=10,630)................................. 78 Table 7 Summary of ECLS-K Variables Corresponding to Constructs and Covariates………………………………………………………….................... 80 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Parent Depression………………………………………………………....................... 83 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Parent School Involvement……………………………………………………………............ 85 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Kindergarten Psychosocial Behavior………………………………………………................ 93 Table 8 Table 9 Table 10 60 Table 11 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior………………………………………………................. 93 Table 12 Summary of Research Questions, Variables, and Analyses……………............ 103 Table 13 Descriptive Statistics for Full Sample…………………………………............. 105 Table 14 Unweighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples.............. 107 Table 15 Unweighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples (Categorical Variables)…………………………………………………............ 109 Table 16 Weighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples….............. Table 17 Weighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples x 112 (Categorical Variables)………………………………………………................ 114 Table 18 Correlation Matrix for LD (upper) and Non-LD (lower) Sample Part 1……..... 117 Table 19 Correlation Matrix for LD (upper) and Non-LD (lower) Sample Part 2………. 119 Table 20 Unweighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics……………………….......... Table 21 Unweighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics (Categorical Variables)……………………………………………………………................. 131 Table 22 Weighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics………………………….......... Table 23 Weighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics (Categorical Variables)……………………………………………………………................. 137 Table 24 Final Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a Four-Factor Model…………............ Table 25 Parameter Estimates for a Four-Factor Structural Regression Model (Mediation Model)............................................................................................... 154 Table 26 Parameter Estimates for a Four-Factor Structural Regression Multi-group Model Across Children with and without Learning Disability…....................... xi 129 135 152 162 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Simplified Conceptual Model…………………………………………............... 53 Figure 2. Detailed Conceptual Model (Reading)…………………………………............. 55 Figure 3. Detailed Conceptual Model (Math)…………………………………….............. 56 Figure 4. Modified Structural Model (Mediation Paths in red)……………………........... 145 Figure 5. Modified Structural Model (Moderation Paths in green)…………………….... 146 xii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Currently, students with a learning disability (LD) represent the largest category of disability in schools (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). By definition, children with LD experience more academic difficulties than their typically developing peers due to underlying neurological and psychological deficits that interfere with learning (APA, 2013; IDEA, 2004). Research also suggests that children with LD are at increased risk of experiencing social-emotional and behavioral problems (Willcutt et al., 2013), and diminished motivation in school (Bender & Wall, 1994; Graham & Harris, 2003; Grolnick & Ryan, 1990). Moreover, children with LD are a heterogeneous group. Research indicates that children with various subtypes of LD experience different academic and psychosocial problems, and children with multiple LD’s experience worse prognoses compared to children with one LD or no LD (Martinez & Semrud-Clikeman, 2004; Willcutt et al., 2013). Studies of long-term outcomes indicate that children with LD are more likely to drop out of grade school, have lower levels of post-secondary education, remain unemployed or receive fewer promotions, have low self-esteem, and have increased likelihood of mental health problems, behavioral problems, and juvenile delinquency (Boetsch, Green, & Pennington, 1996; Daniel et al., 2006; Goldston et al., 2007; McGee, Prior, Williams, Smart, & Sanson, 2002; Willcutt et al., 2013). Despite the multiple points of intervention, much of the extant research and practical efforts for children with LD have been focused on academic interventions in schools to target the primary academic problem. More recently, with the passage of special education legislation that reflects the need for earlier intervention and inclusion of parents in their children’s educational planning, educators and researchers have begun to pay more attention to early preventative 1 efforts that may mitigate the long-term academic problems for children with LD, including consideration of other psychological, motivational, and social-emotional factors. In order to develop interventions designed to target academic and non-academic factors, several areas must be investigated further. First, gaining an understanding of how academic and psychosocial factors develop and influence one another in children with LD over the course of time is important. It has been established that early academic performance is related to later academic performance, and that early psychosocial problems are related to later psychosocial problems (Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, & Sperling, 2008; Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2012). However, evidence regarding how academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors influence one another is mixed; some studies indicate that early psychosocial problems do not influence later academic achievement (Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, & Sperling, 2008), whereas others do (Hinshaw, 1992; Lepola, Poskiparta, Laakkonen, & Niemi, 2005; Trzesniewski, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Maughan, 2006). Finally, there is evidence that children experiencing academic problems demonstrate lower motivation to learn (Tabassam & Grainger, 2002). However, much of the research examining these relations in children with LD is confounded by methodological factors including various definitions of LD, no differentiation of subgroups of LD, small sample sizes, and lack of control groups. Thus, relatively little is known about whether these relationships hold for children with LD compared to typical populations, and more specifically for children with various subtypes of LD. Second, understanding the early risk and protective factors that exist in relation to developing an LD and associated problems, and how those risk and protective factors influence one another may lead to more targeted preventative efforts. Various risk factors for developing an LD have been researched including, genetic predisposition, prenatal and postnatal biological 2 insults, and environmental factors such as poverty (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., 2009; Bhate & Wilkinson, 2006; Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014; Gaddes, 2013; Piper et al., 2012). Additional risk factors such as parent depression can exacerbate academic and social-emotional problems (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2009). On the other hand, protective factors such as increased parent involvement have been related to more positive academic and behavioral outcomes, social-emotional well-being, and motivation to learn (Christenson, 2000; Epstein, 1994; Gonzalez-DeHass, Willems, & Holbein, 2005; McWayne et al., 2004; Pianta, Rimm-Kaufman, & Cox, 1999). Overall, the purpose of this study is to examine whether parent depression, a risk factor, and parent involvement, a protective factor, predict academic, psychosocial, and motivational outcomes in children with subtypes of LD compared to children without LD over the course of elementary school. The current study will test a hypothesized latent variable model to examine the relations between parent depression, parent involvement, early academic and psychosocial outcomes, and later academic, psychosocial, and motivation outcomes across elementary school in a sample of children with reading disability, math disability, reading-math disability, and without LD. By determining the magnitude by which parent variables influence child outcomes, conclusions about whether parent depression and parent involvement serve as risk and protective factors differentially for children with and without LD may be drawn. This study takes advantage of a large, nationally representative dataset in order to address methodological problems in other studies including small sample sizes and lack of control groups. This model also accounts for various environmental factors including, socioeconomic status and race-ethnicity that are associated with LD and various academic and psychosocial outcomes (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014; Coutinho, Oswald, & Best, 2002; O’Connor & Fernandez, 2006). 3 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Learning Disabilities and Academic Achievement Learning disability (LD) is an umbrella term used to describe a range of neurologicallybased disorders that affect a person’s ability to process, think about, and perform verbal and/or non-verbal tasks. Over the past several decades, researchers have identified specific psychological and neurological processes that characterize specific types of LD (e.g., dyslexia, dyscalculia). Additionally, researchers have also examined the genetic and environmental factors that influence the development of learning disabilities, including prenatal and postnatal risk factors. For example, maternal illness, malnutrition, and exposure to teratogens in utero, low birth weight, premature birth, postnatal traumatic injuries, exposure to environmental toxins, and nutritional or environmental deprivation are risk-factors for developing an LD (AarnoudseMoens et al., 2009; Bhate & Wilkinson, 2006; Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014; Gaddes, 2013; Piper et al., 2012). Environmental factors, including poverty, have also been shown to affect the likelihood of developing an LD. For example, children from low-income families are overidentified with LD’s in schools (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). The effects of poverty (e.g., malnutrition, lower levels of early academic enrichment, greater likelihood of exposure to toxins such as lead) may account for the fact that there is a higher incidence of LD’s for children living in poverty (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). Similarly, children from African-American and Hispanic backgrounds tend to be diagnosed with LD’s at a disproportionate rate. Research suggests that differences in diagnostic practices, and racial biases or stereotypes may play a part in over-identification of minority children with an LD (Harris-Murri, King, & Rostenberg, 2006). 4 One way to conceptualize how genetic and environmental factors influence development of an LD and outcomes for children with LD is through a risk-resiliency model. Zimmerman and Arunkumar (1994) described a risk-resiliency model that posits that positive child outcomes can occur despite risk factors through a process by which the interaction between protective factors and risk factors alters the influence of risk factors and decreases the likelihood of negative outcomes. Following this model, if a child has multiple genetic and environmental risk factors for developing an LD (e.g., familial history of LD, cognitive deficits), protective factors including parent involvement, access to education, and positive parent mental health, may improve the prognosis of the child who does develop an LD. Understanding the nature of LD, as well as the risk and protective factors that may influence the development of an LD and associated academic and psychosocial problems is imperative to designing assessment and intervention practices that target more than just the symptoms of the LD (e.g., academic interventions). In general, LD’s are persistent and permanent (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). According to the National Center for Learning Disabilities (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014), students with a learning disability account for approximately 42% of all children with disabilities served by the special education system in public schools across the country (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). It is estimated that an additional 15% of students struggle academically due to unidentified learning disabilities or lack of intervention (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). Thus, children with learning disabilities represent the largest group of students with a disability in schools. Children with LD’s are at-risk for poorer prognosis in life including, low academic achievement, higher rates of high school drop-out, lower level of higher education attainment and future employment, increased behavioral problems and juvenile delinquency, poorer social skills and self-esteem, 5 and increased likelihood of mental health problems including depression and anxiety (Boetsch, Green, & Pennington, 1996; Daniel et al., 2006; Goldston et al., 2007; McGee, Prior, Williams, Smart, & Sanson, 2002; Reschly & Christenson, 2006; Robinson & Oppenheim, 1998; Willcutt et al., 2013). The following sections will provide an overview of how LD’s are defined and diagnosed, the prominent features associated with LD, and the extant research on academic outcomes for children with LD. Definitions of Learning Disability There are two widely accepted definitions of and guides to diagnosing an LD. First, the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA, 2004) states that a specific learning disability (SLD), another term for LD, is “a disorder in one or more of the basic psychological processes involved in understanding or in using language, spoken or written, that may manifest itself in the imperfect ability to listen, think, speak, read, write, spell, or do mathematical calculations, including conditions such as perceptual disabilities, brain injury, minimal brain dysfunction, dyslexia, and developmental aphasia. Such term does not include a learning problem that is primarily the result of visual, hearing, or motor disabilities, of mental retardation, of emotional disturbance, or of environmental, cultural, or economic disadvantage (pg. 118).” According to IDEA, there are eight categories of SLD: oral expression, listening comprehension, written expression, basic reading skills, reading fluency skills, reading comprehension, mathematics calculation, and mathematics problem solving. IDEA stipulates two main ways to diagnose an SLD. The first method is based on a pattern of strengths and weaknesses in the student’s academic performance and/or intellectual ability in relation to their 6 age, intellectual development, or state age- or grade-level standards, and consideration of quality of instruction received. If a student exhibits significant weaknesses in one of the eight areas and strengths in other areas, they may be diagnosed with an LD. The second method is based on the student’s progress towards state age- or grade-level standards after receiving evidence-based interventions (EBI), typically through a process known as Response-to-Intervention (RtI). RtI involves a systematic process of assessing the student’s deficits, applying EBI’s, and monitoring the student’s progress or growth following intervention. If a student does not make adequate progress after a certain amount of time receiving intervention, the student may be diagnosed with an LD. The second definition of LD is from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; APA, 2013). The DSM-5 defines a specific learning disorder, another term for LD, as a “persistent difficulty in reading, writing, arithmetic, or mathematical reasoning skills during formal years of schooling. Symptoms may include inaccurate or slow and effortful reading, poor written expression that lacks clarity, difficulties remembering number facts, or inaccurate mathematical reasoning. Current academic skills must be well below the average range of scores in culturally and linguistically appropriate tests of reading, writing, or mathematics. The individual’s difficulties must not be better explained by developmental, neurological, sensory (vision or hearing), or motor disorders and must significantly interfere with academic achievement, occupational performance, or activities of daily living (APA, 2013b, pg. 1).” 7 According to the DSM-5, a diagnosis should be based on a review of a person’s developmental, medical, educational, and familial history, previous academic reports, observations from teachers, and a student’s progress after academic interventions have been conducted. Despite the definitions and guidelines provided by the IDEA and DSM-5, there is still considerable variability in diagnosis of LD in both research and practice. Individual school districts and practitioners may set more specific criteria to determine whether a student is making “adequate” progress or “demonstrates a pattern of strengths and weaknesses.” For example, a school might implement an intervention for 6 weeks, whereas another school may implement an intervention for 8 weeks before determining whether a student’s progress (or lack thereof) is indicative of an LD. Likewise, a school may stipulate that a student must show at least two or three areas of strength in comparison to a weakness. In research, studies may use more liberal (e.g., performing at or below the 30th percentile) or more conservative (e.g., performing at or below the 10th percentile) criteria to determine whether a student demonstrates an LD (Murphy, Mazzocco, & Hanich, 2007). Another source of variability stems from the changing definitions of LD. Currently, the DSM-5 and IDEA models of identification both highlight the importance of considering student’s academic growth based on the application of EBI’s; however this is a fairly new consideration. Previous models of LD identification recommended diagnosis of LD based on a severe discrepancy between a student’s achievement scores and their intellectual ability (i.e., IQ score). Research has demonstrated that a severe discrepancy model may be biased in identification of students with LD (Fletcher, Coulter, Reschly, & Vaughn, 2004). Thus, previous research may inaccurately identify students with an LD. 8 Finally, previous research has often studied children with LD as a homogeneous group, irrespective of the specific types of academic struggles that they may have (e.g., reading, math). Rourke and Fuerst (1992) noted that there is “an almost lack of sensitivity to the notion that there may be subtypes of children with LD for whom various kinds of academic and social learning may be more or less difficult to achieve.” (p. 362). Thus, greater specificity and attention to LD subtypes is warranted in both clinical and research efforts aimed at understanding the risk and protective factors that influence important outcomes and providing the most effective and appropriate intervention. More recent research has begun to explore the differences and similarities among subtypes of LD, although more work is needed in this area. The remainder of this section will focus on math disability and reading disability, two of the most common subtypes of learning disability. Mathematics Disability. Historically, research on academic achievement has centered on reading disabilities; however the number of children who show deficits in math is reportedly equal to, if not greater, than those with reading problems (Mazzocco, 2001). Mathematics learning disability (MD) refers to a number of neuropsychological deficits that interfere with learning of concepts and/or procedures in one or more domains of math, such as algebra or geometry (Geary, 2004; Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Bailey, 2012; Mazzocco, 2007). Estimates of the prevalence of MD vary; some report between 5%-8% of children have an MD, which is comparable to the number of children with reading disabilities (Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Bailey, 2012; Judge & Watson, 2011). Berch and Mazzocco (2007) report that up to 10% of children have persistent math difficulties, indicating that there may be more children who are low achieving in math or not identified with an LD. As with reading disabilities, research indicates both genetic and environmental factors contribute to the development of MD (Light & DeFries, 9 1995; Shalev & Gross-Tsur, 2001). In one study, parents and siblings of children with arithmetic-based subtypes of MD were ten times more likely to be diagnosed with MD compared to the general population (Shalev & Gross-Tsur, 2001). A major barrier to understanding the true prevalence of MD is the lack of consensus on the definition of MD across research studies and clinical practice, as well as few specific screening or diagnostic protocols to evaluate a student for a MD (Geary, 2004; Gersten, Jordan, & Flojo, 2005). According to some researchers, children who perform at or below the 35 th percentile on a standardized assessment of math are characterized as having math difficulties (Clements & Sarama, 2009). Researchers argue that this more liberal definition allows for identification of children who are performing in the low average range and may be at greater risk for developing more serious math difficulties or MD in the future to receive earlier intervention (Gersten, Jordan, & Flojo, 2005). However, liberal criterion may conflate research on students with MD with low achievement in general. Other researchers have used a cut-point of performance below the 25th percentile (1st quartile) on a standardized assessment (Geary, Hamson, & Hoard, 2000; Gross-Tsur, Manor, & Shalev, 1996; Morgan, Farkas, Hillemeier, & Maczuga, 2014; Murphy et al, 2007). However, Geary (2004) points out that a cut-point at the 25th percentile (or higher) does not align with the estimation of 5%-8% of students having a true MD. The most conservative criterion of performance that has been used by researchers to study children with MD is performance at or below the 10th percentile (Mazzocco & Myers, 2003). Overall, the standardized achievement tests used to gauge performance capture a wide range of mathematical procedures and concepts, whereas children with MD often have more severe deficits in some areas and not others (Geary, 2004). Thus, scores on standardized tests may over or underestimate the student’s competencies in specific procedures and domains of mathematics. 10 Researchers indicate that true MD is generally persistent, and reliance on one test score at one point in time does not indicate a MD in and of itself. Consideration of academic history and response to evidence-based interventions is an important component of diagnosis (Geary, 2004). For example, some research indicates that children who do not respond to intensive intervention in fact retrieval may have more complex cognitive and memory deficits, and the persistence of retrieval problems despite intervention is a strong indicator of MD (Howell, Sidorenko, & Jurica, 1987). Overall, when examining the research on MD, it is important to consider how the disability is defined. Throughout this literature review, children with MD will refer to children who demonstrate low achievement (e.g., below 35th percentile) to very low achievement (e.g., below the 10th percentile) in mathematics. Children with mathematics learning disability do not follow a typical developmental trajectory when learning math. A typical developmental pattern for most children begins with numerical knowledge and builds into conceptual knowledge of mathematics, and finally to computational mechanisms involved in solving more complex problems. In infancy, children begin to develop an understanding of numbers and quantity. Investigation of infant ability using dishabituation paradigms indicates that infants discriminate small quantities when presented with arrays of dots or objects (Starkey & Cooper, 1980). Mathematics research in infancy suggests that there is some inherent drive to understand basic numerical principles and counting (Geary, 2006), although there is little doubt that experience and observational learning account for much of mathematical development. For children with MD, research suggests that the ability to identify numbers and discriminate quantities is generally intact, although these skills are often delayed (Badian, 1983; Geary, Hoard, & Hamson, 1999; Gross-Tsur, Manor, & Shalev, 1996). 11 As children begin preschool and kindergarten they are exposed to more formal numberrelated activities. During this time, foundational skills of math, including counting knowledge and strategy development, are reinforced. Counting knowledge begins as children learn to make associations between written numbers, number names, and mental representations of quantities. By around age four, most typically developing children have the ability to sequence numbers from one to ten (Geary, 2006). There are several principles of counting knowledge that continue to develop during early childhood, and with application and error correction become more automatized and accurate. These principles include learning one-to-one correspondence (only one word for each object), stable order (the order of number words remains the same despite the set), cardinality (the final number word represents the total quantity of objects), abstraction (any object(s) can be grouped and counted), order-irrelevance (items can be counted in any order), and ordinality (successive number words signify sequentially larger quantities) (Geary, 2004). These principles form the basis for understanding of arithmetic operations. In regards to counting knowledge, studies indicate that children with MD are able to accurately identify some counting principles, but not all. In a study by Gelman and Meck (1983), first grade children were asked to watch a puppet count objects and identify if there were errors in counting. Children with MD and reading disability (RD) accurately identified one-to-one correspondence errors when they occurred on the last item (i.e., double counting last item), but not when they occurred on the first item, indicating difficulties with working memory. Also, these children identified violations of non-essential counting principles, such as counting adjacent items consecutively, as errors. These findings were replicated in a subsequent study, which also found that children with MD and RD-MD persist with these same errors in second grade, and only children with reading disabilities did not show these same counting errors 12 (Geary, Bow-Thomas, & Yao, 1992). Thus, many children with MD show difficulty understanding counting principles, particularly principles of order irrelevance. These difficulties likely delay their ability to learn and complete higher-order arithmetic problems (Ohlsson & Rees, 1991). Typical development of arithmetic concepts, including addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division, develop through several mechanisms. Applying counting knowledge, use of manipulatives to aid counting, and successively more complex strategy use evolves with practice. For example, children typically begin to solve addition problems using a “count all” strategy in which they count all the objects in a given problem set starting with “one.” As children develop memory skills and fluidity with counting, they may begin with the larger addend and count on, also known as the min strategy (e.g. to solve 5 + 3, a child would begin with “five” and count up three numbers until they reach “eight”) (Geary, 2006). As children amass repeated practice with solving basic operations, they develop associations between the problem and answer. These associations are stored in long-term memory and can be retrieved relatively effortlessly and quickly. Siegler (1989) noted that children use multiple strategies to solve math problems at any point in time and that strategy use depends on the accuracy and speed associated with using a given strategy and overall efficiency. Retrieval is the most efficient process, thus math fluency is characterized by accurate and quick retrieval of basic math facts. Development of math calculation skills involves not only fluency of basic math facts but also deep conceptual knowledge. Deep conceptual knowledge, often taught and practiced in elementary and secondary school, may include but is not limited to learning properties of basic operations such as the associative property (order of addition or multiplication of multiple numbers is irrelevant), understanding ratios (parts of a whole), and estimation. Many of these 13 concepts are learned through physical manipulation of objects, applications to other concepts, and continual maturation of memory, attention, and other executive functioning capacities (Geary, 2004; Geary 2006). For children with MD, research suggests that the types of math strategies used and change in strategy use over time is less developed (Geary, Hoard, & Hamson, 1999; Hanich, Jordan, Kaplan, & Dick, 2001; Jordan & Montani, 1997; Gross-Tsur, Manor, & Shalev, 1996). In a study by Jordan and colleagues (2003), children with MD used finger counting in second and third grade, whereas their peers without MD used primarily verbal counting and automatic retrieval of math facts. Children with RD and typically achieving children showed maturation in strategy use between first and second grade, from finger counting to verbal counting and automatic retrieval. Some studies indicate that by the end of elementary school children with MD do become more proficient at verbal counting strategies and demonstrate fewer counting errors (Geary & Brown, 1991). In terms of using the most advanced strategy, retrieval, to solve arithmetic and word problems, children with MD and RD-MD demonstrate significantly more errors and variable response times compared to typically achieving peers, and do not show the same improvements in fact retrieval over time, as with verbal counting (Barrouillet, Fayol, & Lathuliere, 1997; Fayol, Barrouillet, & Marinthe, 1998; Geary & Brown, 1991; Gersten, Jordan, & Flojo, 2005; Rasanen & Ahonen, 1995). Similar deficits in fact retrieval have also been noted in children who have a lesion to the left hemisphere and associated subcortical regions early in life (Ashcraft, Yamashita, & Aram, 1992). Another study found that children with poor automaticity of fact retrieval performed similarly to children who are fluent in fact retrieval on untimed story problems, but performed significantly worse when the problems were timed (Jordan, Hanich, & Kaplan, 2003). Thus, cognitive processes including, working memory and 14 long-term storage and retrieval, as well as development of more efficient strategies differ in children with MD compared to typically achieving peers or peers with RD only. The use of inefficient strategies uses additional attention and working memory resources, and further hinders the development of automaticity of fact retrieval and understanding of more abstract concepts. Mathematics disability subtypes. As previously mentioned, children with MD are a heterogeneous group, and may show strengths and weaknesses in different mathematical domains influenced by their developmental trajectory. There is evidence to support several subtypes of MD including, procedural, semantic, and visual-spatial subtypes. Characteristics of children with procedural subtype of MD include more counting errors, immature strategy use (e.g., finger counting, counting all), misalignment of numbers, and errors with regrouping (e.g., carrying or borrowing). These features may be related to working memory and attention deficits that make it difficult for children to hold and manipulate number words and counting procedures mentally (Hitch & McAuley, 1991). Additionally, problems with the phonetic-articulatory system may also hinder a child’s ability to process, hold, and attend to verbal representations of numbers (Geary, 1993; McLean & Hitch, 1999). Characteristics of children with semantic subtype of MD include difficulty retrieving basic facts from long-term memory, more errors with fact retrieval, and inconsistent reaction time patterns with fact retrieval. It is hypothesized that problems with the language system responsible for encoding long-term memories representations of phonetic (the number word) and semantic (the quantity of the number) properties associated with arithmetic problems are at play (Geary, 2004). Another possibility is that difficulties inhibiting irrelevant associations lead to difficulties with fact retrieval (Barrouillet, Fayol, & Lathuiere, 1997; Hanich et al., 2001). For example, in a study by Geary and colleagues (2000) 15 children with MD and children with RD made more counting-string errors (6 + 2 = 7 or 3, because 7 comes after 6, and 3 comes after 2), indicating that they had difficulty inhibiting the irrelevant information in working memory related to the problem addends. Finally, characteristics of children with visual-spatial subtype of MD include difficulties with estimation tasks and misunderstanding or errors in representing numerical information spatially (e.g., on a number line or graph). Summary. Given the developmental nature of mathematics learning, it would not be unreasonable to assume that early math difficulties subsume later math difficulties, and that in general MD is a persistent disability. Several studies indicate that math difficulties do persist across elementary school grades (Morgan, Farkas, Hillemeier, & Maczuga, 2014; Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2009). In one study, students performing below the 10 th percentile in fall and spring of kindergarten had the slowest growth rates in math from first through fifth grade, and approximately 65%-70% of students with MD in kindergarten continued to experience MD in first, third, or fifth grade (Morgan, Farskas, & Wu, 2009). Morgan, Faskas, Hillemeier, and Maczuga (2014) found that children with who were performing in the lowest 25 th percentile in mathematics at the end of kindergarten were seventeen times more likely to have math disability throughout elementary and middle school. These studies not only demonstrate the stability of MD, but also highlight the importance of early academic skills in predicting later academic problems. Early math skills have been shown to predict reading, math, and science achievement from kindergarten through eighth grade, including for students who enter school with lower math skills (Claessens & Engle, 2013). Additionally, general math achievement in kindergarten entry has been shown to be highly predictive of math achievement in third grade (Claessens, Duncan, & Engel, 2009; Duncan et al., 2007; Jordan, Kaplan, Ramineni, & Locuniak, 2009), and studies 16 indicate that the gap between children with and without math difficulties tends to remain stable or increase over time (Shin, Davison, Long, Chan, & Heistad, 2013). In sum, children with MD represent a heterogeneous group of students with neuropsychological deficits that interfere with learning and performance of procedures and concepts in various mathematical domains. These children often show a different developmental trajectory that is characterized by poor counting knowledge, problems with fact retrieval, and inefficient strategy use. Although researchers have begun to examine the defining features, and neuropsychological and biological correlates of subtypes of MD, there is a lack of consensus on the diagnostic criteria for MD and various definitions of MD complicate the interpretation of many research findings. Nevertheless, it is well established that children with MD have lower academic achievement compared to peers, greater social and emotional problems, and face poorer prognosis in later life for employment and social-emotional adjustment in adulthood (Reschly & Christenson, 2006; Martinez & Semrud-Clikeman, 2004). Reading Disability. Literacy is one of the most important skills in today’s society, required for success in school, the work force, and daily living. However, many children and adults struggle to read. In fact, the majority of students with an LD have a RD (Lerner & Johns, 2009; Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2003). Reading disability (RD) is characterized by neuropsychological deficits that lead to difficulty reading, not otherwise attributable to low intellectual functioning or environmental factors such as cultural considerations, poverty, socialemotional problems, or lack of instruction or exposure to language and text (Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2005). The predominant features of RD include inaccurate or dysfluent word recognition, poor decoding skills, and poor spelling (Judge & Watson, 2011; Pennington, 2009). RD is typically diagnosed based on a persistent pattern of difficulty in reading despite 17 intervention and performance in the lowest 10th percentile on standardized assessment of reading. The prevalence of RD in children is estimated between 5% and 17.5% (Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2005). Reading disabilities are equally prevalent among boys and girls, although some studies indicate that boys are more likely to be identified with LD in reading in school and receive special education services (Shaywitz, 2003). This phenomenon may be due to different behavioral manifestations of learning problems (e.g., more hyperactivity, behavioral problems) between boys and girls that leads to differential attention from teachers regarding the severity of the learning problems. Etiological studies indicate that RD is highly heritable; 23% to 65% of children with RD have a parent with RD (Scarborough, 1990), and approximately 40% of their siblings have RD (Pennington & Gilger, 1996). For typically developing children, there are two general routes (dual-route model) by which they learn to read: the phonological and orthographic routes (Ehri, 2015; Woollams, 2015). Infants and toddlers pick up on language sounds from their environment (Hart & Risley, 1995). As children begin formal instruction in reading, they learn to associate orthographic representations (print) of letters with their respective language sounds, also known as phonemic awareness (Ehri, 2015). Through the process of decoding, or segmenting words into their individual phonemes and stringing phonemes together, children use the phonetic code (lettersound associations) to read words. Meaning is assigned to the word once the child decodes the word. This process is known as the phonological route (Ehri, 2015). The phonological route is an indirect method of reading, used by beginning readers and by advanced readers when faced with a complex, unfamiliar word. It is a slow, deliberate, and effortful process that requires attention, working memory, and visual, auditory, and language-processing. On the other hand, the orthographic route (also known as the word-form route) is a more direct method of reading used 18 by skilled readers (Ehri, 2015). The orthographic route involves automatic recognition of whole words associated with their semantic meaning, and is the most efficient and fastest method of reading. When a child uses the phonological route to read words multiple times, they commit words and their associated meanings to memory. As a child progresses from using the phonological route to the orthographic route to read, they develop greater reading fluency, or the ability to read accurately, quickly, and with little effort. As children develop reading fluency, they begin to read to learn, or in other words, read to comprehend new information. Reading comprehension is the ability to understand, remember, and think about the text that is being read. Reading fluency and comprehension influence one another, such that children who are fluent readers are better able to understand what they read and children who have strong comprehension skills become more fluent readers (Klauda, & Guthrie, 2008). There is considerable research that supports the dual-route model of reading. Functional neuroimaging research indicates that three main areas of the brain are involved in reading (Shaywitz, 2003; Pugh et al., 2000). Two posterior systems in the left hemisphere, the dorsal temporo-parietal circuit and the ventral occipito-temporal circuit, are involved in automatic word recognition. The temporo-parietal region (also known as the word analysis system) contains Wernicke’s area, and is implicated in phonological awareness (Pugh et al., 2000). The occipitotemporal circuit (also known as the word form system) is involved in rapid recognition of whole words (Pugh et al., 2000). Finally, the inferior frontal gyrus in the anterior portion of the brain, which contains Broca’s area, is involved in articulation and has been associated with silent reading (Pugh et al., 2000). Thus, beginning readers rely on the temporo-parietal circuit to decode words via the phonological route, but as they practice decoding the word multiple times it 19 becomes stored in the occipito-temporal word form area and is retrieved via the orthographic route. Children with RD have core deficits in phonological processing that impact their decoding skills, reading fluency, and/or reading comprehension (Brady, 1991; Bus & Ijzendoorn, 1999; Cain, Oakhill, & Bryant, 2000; Olson, Forsberg, & Wise, 1994; Pennington, 2009; Snowling, 2001; Torgesen, 2000; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1994; Yuill & Oakhill, 1991). Although several models of RD point to deficits in auditory perceptual processing and naming speed (e.g., Tallal, 1980; Wolf & Bowers, 1999; Wolf, Bally, & Morris, 1986), a substantial amount of research supports the hypothesis that RD is related to core problems with phonological processing. Pennington (2009) found that individuals with RD showed underactivation in the temporo-parietal regions of the brain associated with phonological awareness and decoding. Consequently, underactiviation in the occipito-temporal word form system was also found given that storage of known words is dependent on accurate decoding (Pennington, 2009). Several studies have also indicated individuals with RD demonstrate weaknesses in other neuropsychological and cognitive domains after controlling for performance on phonological measures such as, verbal comprehension and speech and language processing (Pennington & Bishop, 2009), slower verbal naming speed and general processing speed (Compton, DeFries, & Olson, 2001; Landerl, Fussenegger, Moll, & Willburger, 2009), and impairments in executive functions including verbal working memory, response inhibition, and attention (Ebejer et al., 2010; Purvis & Tannock, 2000; Roodenrys, Koloski, & Grainger, 2001; Willcutt, Pennington, Olson, Chhabildas, & Hulslander, 2005). Similar to MD, studies have indicated that RD is persistent over time, and that student’s early reading achievement and skills are predictive of later reading achievement (Algozzine, 20 Wang, & Violette, 2011; Froiland, Powell, Diamond, & Son, 2013; Judge & Bell, 2010; Lin, Morgan, Hillemeier, Cook, Maczuga, & Farkas, 2013). For example, McNamara, Scissons, and Dahleu (2005) found that measures of phonemic awareness and alphabetic principle in kindergarten predicted first grade reading achievement. The results of this study also revealed that children who were poor readers in kindergarten were more likely to have reading difficulties in first grade. This trend is true in later grades as well; Lin and colleagues (2013) found that children with RD in third grade (performing in the lowest 10% on reading achievement test) were almost twenty times more likely to still have an RD in fifth grade. Similarly, Judge and Bell (2010) found that children with reading disabilities made less growth in reading across kindergarten through fifth grade compared to children without LD. Despite the evidence indicating that RD is stable over time, and children with RD typically demonstrate problems early on, a study by Lipka and colleagues (2006) found a small subset of children who did not demonstrate reading or phonological deficits until third and fourth grade. Thus, RD is a heterogeneous category, and there may be some children who develop reading problems later in life, particularly in regards to problems with reading comprehension but not word reading. Overall, children with RD have marked impairments in their ability to decode, read fluently, and comprehend text. These features are based in core deficits in phonological processing, and are associated with individual neurological differences and neuropsychological impairments such as verbal working memory, processing speed, and attention deficits. Similar to children with other disabilities, children with RD are at-risk for poor academic achievement, higher levels of academic frustration, school dropout, low self-esteem, and poorer prognosis in adulthood (e.g., unemployment) (Boetsch, Green, & Pennington, 1996; Daniel et al., 2006; Goldston et al., 2007; McGee, Prior, Williams, Smart, & Sanson, 2002; Willcutt et al., 2013). 21 Co-morbid Mathematics and Reading Disability. Research indicates that math and reading disabilities co-occur (RD-MD) in approximately 30% to 70% of individuals with either disorder (Kovas et al., 2007; Landerl & Moll, 2010; Willcutt et al., 2013). One proposed model of co-morbidity with empirical support is the correlated liabilities model (Willcutt et al., 2013). This model suggests that RD and MD co-occur more frequently than expected by chance because they share etiological factors, whereas additional etiological influences specific to RD or MD lead to distinct disabilities occurring alone. Twin analyses indicate that there are shared genetic influences between RD and MD, as well as separate genetic and environmental influences that uniquely influence the development of RD or MD alone (Knopik, Alarcon, & DeFries, 1997; Kovas et al., 2007; Willcutt et al., 2010). In concordance with the correlated liabilities model, several research studies suggest that children with RD-MD think and perform differently from children with RD or MD alone (Jordan, Hanich, & Kaplan, 2003b; Jordan & Montani, 1997; Martinez & Semrud-Clikeman; 2004; Willcutt et al., 2013). In a comprehensive study examining academic, neuropsychological, and psychosocial functioning of children with RD, MD, RD-MD, and children without LD, results indicated that children with RD-MD demonstrated significantly more impairments than children with one LD or without LD, and that children with LD demonstrated more impairments than children without LD (Willcutt et al., 2013). Children with RD-MD demonstrated greater inattention symptoms, higher internalizing symptoms, lower grades in school, lower global functioning in daily responsibilities, greater social isolation, and greater neuropsychological impairments in verbal comprehension, phoneme awareness, working memory, naming speed, processing speed, vigilance, and response variability compared to children with one LD. Further analyses also revealed that verbal comprehension, working memory, and processing speed were associated 22 with both math and reading deficits, whereas phoneme awareness and naming speed were associated independently with reading deficits and set shifting was associated with math deficits. These findings remained after controlling for performance IQ, ADHD symptoms, and comorbidity. Overall, this study suggests that RD-MD is characterized by multiple neuropsychological deficits that are distinct, but related to RD and MD. Other studies have also found that children with RD-MD perform differently from children with one LD. For example, on math calculation tasks, children with RD-MD demonstrated more errors with finger counting and performed worse overall than children with MD only (Jordan, Hanich, & Kaplan, 2003b). Additionally, children with RD-MD showed more stability in math and reading problems throughout second and third grade compared to children with MD only. Interestingly, in a study by Jordan, Kaplan, and Hanich (2002) children with RD performed similarly to children with MD in mathematics at the end of third grade. This finding may be indicative of a negative influence of reading difficulties on mathematics performance, and possibly, more globally across multiple academic subjects (Grimm, 2008; Jordan, Kaplan, & Hanich, 2002). Overall, children with multiple LD’s tend to have greater academic and psychosocial problems and neuropsychological deficits compared to children with one LD and children without LD. Psychosocial Development in Children with Learning Disabilities Psychosocial development describes the interactions between social factors (e.g., interpersonal relationships, school environment) and psychological factors (e.g., cognitions, behaviors, and affect) that influence an individual’s daily functioning. Although the main focus of attention in regards to identification, intervention, and research of children with LD’s is the neuropsychological and academic profile, attention to non-academic outcomes, such as 23 psychosocial development, is equally important (Moore, Lippman, & Ryberg, 2015). Research indicates that although academic outcomes are strong predictors of occupation, health, income, and other important life success indices, non-academic outcomes such as social competence (e.g., interpersonal skills), emotional well-being (e.g., internalizing problems), and maladaptive behaviors (e.g., externalizing problems) all serve important roles in supporting or hindering academic achievement, and child development broadly (Moore, Lippman, & Ryberg, 2015). For children who are already at-risk for academic underachievement, such as children with LD, understanding how psychosocial factors may serve as protective factors may inform interventions that improve their overall success in school. Research suggests that early academic problems influence children’s behavior and mood (Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2012). Experiencing academic problems in reading and/or math early on may lead to increased feelings of frustration, anxiety, depressed mood, and negative behaviors (e.g., interpersonal problems, aggression), triggering a cycle of reciprocal influences between poor academic performance and negative mood and behavior (Fleming, Harachi, Cortes, Abbott, & Catalano, 2004; Lane, Beebe-Frankenberger, Lambros, & Pierson, 2001; Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2012; Wehby, Falk, Barton-Arwood, Lane, & Cooley, 2003). Furthermore, as children experience academic failure in reading and/or math, they are more likely to actively avoid reading or math activities at home or school thereby reducing the opportunities they have for academic practice and growth (Griffiths & Snowling, 2002; Guthrie, Schafer, & Huang, 2001; Senechal, LeFevre, Hudson, & Lawson, 1996). Given that children with LD are, by definition, more likely to experience school failure, it is not surprising that studies have found that children with LD are more vulnerable to psychosocial problems (Gadeyne, Ghesquiere, & Onghena, 2004), even in comparison to generally low-achieving children (La Greca & Stone, 1990; 24 Maughan, Pickles, Hagell, Rutter, & Yule, 1996; Ochoa & Palmer, 1995; Roeser, Eccles, & Strobel, 1998). Although a link between academic achievement and psychosocial factors has been established, the exact mechanisms by which problems develop are unclear. Some researchers have posited that increased psychosocial problems in children with academic difficulties may be related to a “common cause” variable, such as inattention, which leads to problems in academics, behavior, and social-emotional well-being (Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, & Sperling, 2008). Deficits in higher-order executive functioning skills such as, attention and inhibition, are common features in children with LD, as well as children with behavior and social-emotional problems. Another possibility is that academic and behavioral problems are reciprocally causative over time, indicating that interventions targeting both academics and psychosocial factors are necessary. Hinshaw (1992) examined seventeen studies that suggested an overall bidirectional causal model between reading and behavior problems (see also Lepola, Poskiparta, Laakkonen, & Niemi, 2005; Onatsu-Arvilommi & Nurmi, 2000; Trzesniewski, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Maughan, 2006), however several of these studies do not control for earlier reading problems as predictors. Although the current study is unable to address whether academic problems lead to psychosocial adjustment issues, whether psychosocial adjustment issues lead to academic failure, or whether academic problems and psychosocial adjustment issues are reciprocally causative, this study aims to understand the patterns in academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors across kindergarten to fifth grade and may be able to shed light on whether children with academic problems and/or psychosocial adjustment issues in kindergarten continue on a negative trajectory in one or both areas. 25 Overall, more studies are needed to determine how early academic and psychosocial problems influence the academic and psychosocial trajectory of children with LD’s. Much of the research on psychosocial problems in children with LD’s has focused on behaviors, cognitions, and emotions related to academic task such as, attitudes towards reading (Chapman, Tunmer, & Prochnow, 2000; Lepola, Salonen, & Vauras, 2000; Viljaranta, Lerkkanen, Poikkeus, Aunola, & Nurmi, 2009), in comparison to research on more general internalizing, externalizing, and interpersonal problems. The following sections will review the current literature on externalizing problems, internalizing problems, and interpersonal problems in children with LD. Externalizing Problems. Externalizing problems describe a range of maladaptive behaviors including, aggression, disruptiveness, and poor emotional control (e.g., anger), as well as inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. McIntosh (2005) hypothesized that a “coercive cycle of education failure” leads students to view academic demands as aversive and engage in maladaptive behaviors that are maintained by escaping academic tasks such as, receiving a timeout or being sent to the principal’s office. The loss of instruction time increases the academic gap between what is expected in the classroom and what the student can do, which leads to increased aversion and escape-maintained problem behaviors. Several studies have demonstrated a relationship between low academic achievement and behavior problems in children with LD (Algozzine, Wang, & Violette, 2011; Algozzine, Horner, & Putnam, 2008; Beitchman & Young, 1997; Bender & Smith, 1990; Konstantareas & Homatidis, 1989). Other studies indicate that externalizing problems tend to be stable and have negative long-term impacts (Bub, McCartney, & Willett, 2007; Kwon, Kim, & Sheridan, 2012). For example, children with high levels of externalizing problems in kindergarten were more at-risk for continued externalizing problems in third and fifth grade (Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2009). 26 Some studies estimate that nearly half of individuals with RD meet criteria for at least one behavioral or emotional disorder (Goldston et al, 2007; Maughan, Rowe, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2003). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most frequent co-occurring externalizing problems, particularly inattentive and combined subtypes that are characterized by significant inattention (Willcutt et al., 2010). Individuals with RD are also at a higher risk for externalizing disorders such as, ODD and CD, but several studies suggest that associations between RD and antisocial behavior may be restricted to the subset of individuals with comorbid ADHD (Frick et al., 1991; Maughan et al., 2003; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000). Other studies have found that poor readers are more likely to act out, be aggressive and argumentative, and have higher rates of externalizing pathology than children without reading difficulties (Fleming et al., 2004; Hinshaw, 1992; Lin, Morgan, Hillemeier, Cook, Maczuga, & Farkas, 2013; Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2009; Trzesniewski, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Maughan, 2006). Academic problems have been found to be long-term predictors of externalizing pathology. Morgan, Farkas, and Wu (2012) found that students who identified as poor readers in third grade were twice as likely to identify themselves as angry in fifth grade, compared to those who were not identified as poor readers. Another study found that children in first grade with RD (bottom 10% on standardized reading test) were more likely to have externalizing and internalizing problems in third grade than children without RD. Interestingly, children with poor task engagement in first grade were more likely to experience reading problems (bottom 10% on standardized reading test) in third grade, however this was not true for children who demonstrated severe externalizing behaviors in first grade. Analyses included an autoregressor 27 variable for reading or behavioral problems in first grade to account for the influence of existing problems (Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, & Sperling, 2008). In regards to children with MD, there are no previous studies that have reported the prevalence of co-morbid behavioral or emotional disorders (Willcutt et al., 2013); however some studies have found elevated attentional problems, and internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children with MD compared to children without LD (Auerbach, Gross-Tsur, Manor, & Shalev, 2008; Cirino, Fletcher, Ewing-Cobbs, Barnes, & Fuchs, 2007; Lin, Morgan, Hillemeier, Cook, Maczuga, & Farkas, 2013; Shalev, Auerbach, & Gross-Tsur, 1995; Willcutt et al., 2011). Scarborough and Parker (2003) found that children with MD and MD+RD had more behavioral problems in second grade compared to children with RD and without LD. Additionally, some studies have found that elevated inattention and externalizing symptoms are more prevalent in children with RD+MD compared to children with MD only, indicating that multiple learning challenges increase the prevalence of externalizing psychopathology (Badian, 1999; Shalev, Auerbach, & Gross-Tsur, 1995; White, Moffitt, & Silva, 1992). Greenham (1999) conducted a review of psychosocial adjustment in children with LD and concluded “the literature regarding externalizing behavior problems in individuals with LD is fairly consistent,” (p.179) with the majority of studies indicating increased aggression, disruptive behaviors, and/or hyperactiveinattentive behaviors in children with LD compared to typically developing children, as rated by parents and/or teachers. Overall, there is strong evidence to suggest that children with reading challenges are atrisk for greater externalizing psychopathology concurrently and in the future compared to children without LD. Levels of externalizing problems in children with LD tend to be at the subclinical level, but are still more problematic compared to children without LD (Greenham, 28 1999). Although there is less research on children with MD, the preliminary results also point to increased risk of externalizing pathology in elementary and middle school grades. In addition to externalizing behaviors, children with LD also seem to be challenged with internalizing disorders disproportionate to their non-LD peers. Internalizing Problems. Internalizing problems describe a range of emotional difficulties including, anxiety and depression. Several studies suggest that children with LD experience more internalizing problems than their peers without LD (Arthur, 2003; Margalit, 1998). Earlier studies have found that middle school students with LD are more likely to report loneliness, victimization, less social satisfaction (Bender & Wall, 1994; Sabornie, 1994), and chronic depression and anxiety (Cohen, 1986; Goldstein, Paul, & Sanfilippo-Cohn, 1985), compared to their non-LD peers. These findings suggest that internalizing problems in children with LD may be even more prevalent in later elementary and middle school grades. Studies also indicate that higher levels of stress and anxiety in older students with LD are related to lower academic self-efficacy, self-confidence, and increased self-doubt and self-criticism, compared to students without LD (Sparks & Lovett, 2009). Studies examining children with RD have found that elevated levels of anxiety and depression (Arnold et al., 2005; Carroll, Maughan, Goodman, & Meltzer, 2005) was related to increased suicidal ideation or suicide attempts in adolescents with RD compared to their neurotypical peers (Daniel et al., 2006). These findings remain even after controlling for other symptoms of psychopathology including, ADHD and conduct disorders (Daniel et al., 2006, Goldston et al., 2007; Maughan, Rowe, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2003; Willcutt & Pennington, 2000; Willcutt et al., 2013). Some researchers have found that as children with RD become more aware of their learning challenges in comparison to their typically achieving peers, 29 they report feeling more depression (Maughan, Rowe, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2003). Children with RD and RD+MD have also self-reported more impairment on the Sense of Inadequacy scale on the Behavior Assessment System for Children, 2nd Edition (BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2006) compared to typically achieving children (Martinez & SemrudClikeman, 2004). As with externalizing problems, there appears to be a longitudinal effect of academic problems on later internalizing psychopathology. For example, students with RD in third grade were twice as likely to identify themselves as sad, lonely, and distractible in fifth grade (Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2012). There are very few studies that examine internalizing problems in children with MD. One study examined the differences in emotional adjustment and school functioning in a group of 120 middle school (6th to 8th grade) students with RD, MD, RD-MD, and students who were typically achieving. They found that students with RD-MD reported the poorest functioning on the School Maladjustment, Clinical Maladjustment, and Emotional Symptoms Index on the BASC-2 selfreport, compared to the typically achieving students. Additionally, the MD group reported poorer functioning on the School Maladjustment Composite compared to typically achieving children. Further analysis revealed that the RD-MD group showed greater impairment on Atypicality scale (e.g., odd and immature social behaviors) and Depression scale compared to the typically achieving group (Martinez & Semrud-Clikeman, 2004). Literature on children with nonverbal learning disorder (NVLD), a disorder characterized by weak math skills, intact word decoding, and deficits in motor coordination, spatial organization, and social competence, indicates higher levels of social isolation, elevated symptoms of withdrawal from social engagement, and more internalizing problems (Greenham, 1999; Osman, 2000; Rourke, 1989; White, Moffitt, & Silva, 1992). These studies may provide additional support for the hypothesis that children with MD 30 and associated social deficits are also at-risk for greater internalizing psychopathology; although more research to clarify the psychosocial correlates of MD with and without RD is needed. Despite findings of increased internalizing problems in children with one or more LD, some studies have found that children with one LD do not show a discernable increase in internalizing psychopathology (Barkauskiene, 2009; Jorm, Share, Matthews, & McLean, 1986; Miller, Hynd, & Miller, 2005; Sideridis, Mouzaki, Simos, & Protopapas, 2006). For example, Barkaukiene (2009) examined 204 children in second to fourth grade, of which 102 children were diagnosed with one or more LD through a school psycho-educational evaluation. Results indicated that only children with multiple LD demonstrated higher internalizing problems, in comparison to children with single LD or no LD. This finding extended the findings of a study conducted by Miller, Hynd, and Miller (2005) indicating that children with RD did not show significant internalizing psychopathology based on parent, teacher, and self-report of internalizing symptoms on the Behavioral Assessment System for Children (BASC; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2002) and Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1985). It is important to note that this study classified children as RD based on two models: a 20-point discrepancy between a reading achievement score and their IQ, or based on a reading achievement standard score below 85. Thus, findings may be conflated based on inclusion of children with low average reading skills or systematic biases or errors involved in using a severe discrepancy model to diagnose a true LD. Along with differences in diagnostic criteria, the mixed findings in the literature may be due to differences in rater’s perceptions (e.g., parent, teacher, or self). For example, one study found that children with RD did not self-report higher internalizing problems, although parents and teachers did (Heiervang, Stevenson, Lund, & Hugdahl, 2001). A recent meta-analysis 31 indicated that approximately 70% of children with LD have higher levels of anxiety compared to children without LD (Nelson & Harwood, 2011), however, levels of anxiety in the LD population were not clinically significant and several important factors moderated the relationship between LD status and anxiety level including, type of rater and setting of LD diagnosis (e.g., clinic/hospital or school). This study found that there was low agreement between types of raters (parents, teachers, child), and that children diagnosed in clinic/hospital settings were regarded as having higher internalizing symptomology. Overall, the research is inconclusive regarding whether children with LD, and more specifically certain subtypes of LD, experience increased internalizing symptoms compared to children without LD. It seems that children with LD tend to experience more internalizing symptoms dependent on the type of informant and diagnostic criteria. However, this relationship may not always be clinically significant and may hold true for children with multiple LD compared to children with one LD. As with externalizing problems, children with high levels of internalizing problems in kindergarten are more likely to have persistent internalizing problems in third and fifth grade (Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2009). Greater risk for internalizing problems in school is likely influenced by academic failure and peer comparisons. This study will add to the current literature by exploring whether children with LD experience more internalizing symptomology compared to children without LD, based on teacher-reports of LD within a school-diagnosed sample. Additionally, differences between sub-groups of children will be explored, potentially adding more clarity to this issue. Finally, given that internalizing and externalizing problems often negatively influence children’s social competence, with some research suggests that children with LD demonstrate more deficits in interpersonal skills, this 32 study will explore the relations between internalizing and externalizing problems and interpersonal skills. Interpersonal Problems. Interpersonal problems involve difficulties getting along with others, making or maintaining friendships, and experiencing social isolation or rejection. Across several studies, children with LD consistently demonstrate more interpersonal problems. For example, studies have reported increased social isolation (Mishna, 1996), peer rejection (Kuhne & Weiner, 2000; Ochoa & Olivarez, 1995), and difficulties with social integration (Gadeyne, Ghesquiere, & Onghena, 2004) in children with LD from first grade through adolescence. More specifically, children with RD tend to experience more peer rejection and lower social status (Lopes, Cruz, & Rutherford, 2002). Estimates from meta-analyses and review studies indicate that approximately 25% to 30% of children with LD are socially rejected compared to 8% to 16% in the typical population (Greenham, 1999). However, several studies have reported that 40% to 70% of children with LD are rated within the average range in terms of acceptance by peers (Conderman, 1995; Ochoa & Palmer, 1991). Thus, there is evidence to support that children with LD tend to be rejected by peers more than children without LD but, similar proportions of children with and without LD are rated within the average range for peer acceptance. Regarding social skills, the extant literature reveals a similar trend to those findings on peer acceptance/rejection. Namely, although children with LD are typically rated has having poorer social skills in comparison to children without LD, a similar proportion of children with and without LD are rated as having average social skills (Greenham, 1999). For example, a metaanalysis examining 152 studies, found that approximately 75% of children with LD demonstrate social skills deficits, and that these deficits differentiated children with LD from children without 33 LD (Kavale & Forness, 1996). However, other researchers have found that children with LD and low-achieving children without LD received similar, low ratings on social skills and demonstrated fewer social interactions in comparison to typically-achieving children (Tur-Kaspa & Bryan, 1995; Vaughn & Haager, 1994). Thus, the research suggests that children with LD tend to experience more social skills problems; however there may be a common factor of academic underachievement that underlies interpersonal skill deficits. In terms of subtypes of LD, several studies report small, but significant, weaknesses in social functioning for children with RD (Goldston et al., 2007; Willcutt, Pennington, Olson, & DeFries, 2007; Willcutt et al., 2011). Children with RD in third grade are twice as likely to selfreport as unpopular in fifth grade compared to children without RD (Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2012). For children with MD, research indicates similar findings, including more problems with social awareness, social anxiety, social isolation, and social withdrawal compared to children without MD (Auerbach, Gross-Tsur, Manor, & Shalev, 2008; Shalev, Auerbach, & Gross-Tsur, 1995; Shalev, Auerbach, Manor, & Gross-Tsur, 2000; Willcutt et al., 2011). In general, research supports the notion that children with LD tend to experience more interpersonal problems than children without LD, although there appears to be equivalent numbers of children with and without LD who possess average social skills. Given that interpersonal problems are often associated with other externalizing and internalizing problems this pattern is not unexpected. However, there is limited research that examines the relation between externalizing, internalizing and interpersonal problems in children with LD. Research examining the protective and risk factors related to development of psychosocial problems in children with LD, particularly subtypes of LD, is warranted and will better inform the types of interventions that may be implemented both at home and in the classroom. 34 Motivational Factors in Children with Learning Disabilities Motivation can be defined as the desire and decision to engage in goal-oriented behaviors. This broad definition encompasses multiple constructs of motivation including, an individual’s ability to demonstrate self-regulated behavior, show effort, persistence, and interest, and evaluate thoughts about their behavior. Compared to the literature on typically achieving children, there is relatively little research on the motivational profile of children with LD, especially for subtypes of LD, and for motivational constructs including intrinsic motivation. The following sections review the extant literature on three aspects of motivation in children with LD: self-regulated learning, ability beliefs, and intrinsic motivation. Self-Regulation. Self-regulation, or self-regulated learning, is a cornerstone of goaldirected behavior, and is an important component of academic performance (Pintrich, 2000; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Pintrich & Zusho, 2002). Self-regulation involves an individual’s ability to engage in a recursive process of self-monitoring their thoughts, behaviors, and emotions (e.g., What am I doing? What can I do? How do I feel about this?), self-instructing or guiding their behavior (e.g., What do I have to do? How do I do it?), and self-reinforcing their behavior based on performance (e.g., How did I do? What do I need to do the same or differently?) (Bandura, 1991). Self-regulated learning requires three components. First, a student must possess metacognitive skills to monitor, plan (e.g., set goals), evaluate, and change their thoughts and behaviors. Second, a student must persist and engage in a task in order to evaluate their performance, use feedback, and make modifications. Finally, a student must possess strategies to engage cognitively with the task (e.g., memory and comprehension strategies). This model of self-regulated behavior is rooted in social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1991), which 35 posits that behavior is influenced by one’s own perceptions of their abilities, thoughts, behaviors, and emotions within their social context (e.g., social comparisons to peer groups). Several studies have found that children with lower academic achievement demonstrate greater difficulty with self-regulated learning tasks. For example, children with academic difficulties are more impulsive, set lower academic goals, have difficulty evaluating their performance accurately, and are more critical of themselves (Borkowski & Thorpe, 1994). These problems with self-regulated learning have negative effects on their psychosocial development including greater levels of anxiety, lower self-esteem, and lower levels of intrinsic motivation (Sideridis, Morgan, Botas, Padeliadu, & Fuchs, 2006; Zimmerman & Risemberg, 1997). Children with identified LD’s demonstrate less academic self-regulated learning and are less motivated to engage or persist in on-task behaviors in class (Bender & Wall, 1994; Graham & Harris, 2003; Grolnick & Ryan, 1990; Wilson & David, 1994; Wong, Harris, Graham, & Butler, 2003). Using the ECLS-K, 1998-1999 Kindergarten Cohort data, Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, and Sperling (2008) investigated trends in task engagement and LD across first through third grade, and found that children with RD in first grade were more likely to demonstrate poor task engagement in school in third grade. Several components of self-regulated learning including goal-directed behavior and persistent task engagement have been associated with increased academic achievement in reading and math (Bodovski & Youn, 2011; DiPerna, Lei, & Reid, 2007; Fantuzzo et al, 2007; McClelland, Acock, & Morrison, 2006; McClelland, Morrison, & Holmes, 2000; McWayne, Fantuzzo, & McDermott, 2004). Researchers have hypothesized why children with LD exhibit lower levels of selfregulation. First, students with LD often struggle with self-awareness and self-assessment (Butler, 1998, 1999), which may impede their ability to evaluate their performance accurately 36 and make use of feedback to employ more efficient cognitive strategies and persist on tasks. Second, self-regulated learning relies on several executive functioning abilities, including attention, working memory, planning, organization, and the ability to work independently (i.e., initiation). Given that children with LD demonstrate deficits in executive functioning including, working memory and attention regulation (Fuchs, Compton, Fuchs, Bryant, Hamlett, & Lambert, 2012; Fuchs et al., 2006; Geary, Hoard, Byrd-Craven, Nugent, Numtree, 2007; Geary, Hoard, & Hamson, 1999; Goldston et al., 2007; Passolunghi & Siegel, 2001, 2004), it is plausible that core executive functioning deficits impede self-regulated learning in children with LD. Issues with self-regulated learning, including executive functioning and self-evaluation, are also likely related to one’s beliefs about their skillset and ability. Ability Beliefs. Ability beliefs (also known as perceived competence), includes a students’ belief about their ability to perform a given task. In other words, ability beliefs encompass a student’s answer to the question, “Can I do this task?” More positive ability beliefs have been associated with increased task persistence, use of more advanced cognitive strategies, better metacognitive skills, lower levels of anxiety, increased intrinsic motivation, mastery learning goals, and increased achievement (Bouffard-Bouchard, Parent, & Larivee, 1991; Fincham & Cain, 1986; Paris & Oka, 1986; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Schunk, 1985; Wong, Wiest, & Cusick, 2002). Several studies have shown that students with LD possess more negative ability beliefs. In one study of elementary school children with school-identified LD, students with LD reported lower academic ability beliefs and academic self-concept compared to their typically achieving peers (Tabassam & Grainger, 2002). In a more recent study, sixth through twelfth grade students with and without LD were assessed on measures of academic self-efficacy, theories of 37 intelligence, academic goal preferences, and attributions for task persistence (Baird, Scott, Dearing, & Hamill, 2009). Students with LD were more likely to report low academic selfefficacy, and attributed more effort as a sign of low academic ability. Mediational models suggested that these findings were influenced by the tendency of students with LD to view intelligence as fixed (i.e., entity view instead of malleable). Negative ability beliefs may arise in children with LD, in part, due to attribution of failure to internal causes such as their ability or their effort, and attribution of academic successes to external causes such as help from the teacher or good luck (Bender & Wall, 1994; Butkowsky & Willows, 1980; Cooley & Ayres, 1988; Fincham & Cain, 1986; Kistner, Osborne, & LeVerrier, 1988; Lewis & LawrencePatterson, 1989; Pearl, 1982, 1992; Rogers & Saklofske, 1985). However, negative ability beliefs seem to be restricted to the academic domain for children with LD; non-academic self-concept has not been found to differ from typically achieving peers (Chapman, 1988; Harter, Whitesell, & Junkin, 1998; Kloomok & Cosden, 1994). Chapman (1988) argued that negative school attitudes develop earlier in the lives of children who experience academic failure and persist through high school. Motivational constructs are often highly related to one another. For example, ability beliefs play a role in self-regulatory mechanisms that influence a student’s behavior, cognitions (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Pajares, 1996; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000) and affect, such as, anxiety that can hinder achievement (Bandura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Gerbino, & Pastorelli, 2003; Cervone, Kopp, Schaumann, & Scott, 1994). Several studies suggest that more positive ability beliefs are related to self-regulated learning including, increased persistence and use of cognitive strategies (Chapman, 1988). Meltzer and colleagues (2004) found that students with LD who held positive academic ability beliefs were more likely to engage and persist in academic tasks, 38 and to use cognitive strategies. Additionally, academic ability beliefs are related to intrinsic motivation; as children feel more academically competent they become more intrinsically motivated to learn and vice-versa (Guay, Boggiano, & Vallerand, 2001; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Intrinsic Motivation. Intrinsic motivation encompasses a student’s desire and value towards a goal and/or tasks to reach a goal. Research indicates that students who are interested in a task and value the task for its importance and for the purposes of learning and challenging themselves are more likely to engage in self-regulated learning and have improved academic performance (Ames & Archer, 1988; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Dweck & Elliott, 1983; Greene, Miller, Crowson, Duke, Akey, 2004; Meece, Blumenfeld, & Hoyle, 1988; Nolen, 1988; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Paris & Oka, 1986). Froiland and Oros (2014) found that student’s self-rating of interest in reading in fifth grade positively predicted 8th grade reading achievement, even after controlling for SES, ethnicity, gender, and prior reading achievement. Thus, when students enjoy and value a task, they are more likely to be engaged in school work which increases the likelihood of academic achievement (Lepper, Corpus, & Iyengar, 2005; Unrau & Schlackman, 2006). Several studies have also shown that intrinsic motivation is related to academic achievement in children with LD (Adelman, 1978; Adelman & Taylor, 1986; Deci & Chandler, 1986; Deci, Hodges, Pierson, & Tomassone, 1992; Switzky & Schultz, 1988) However, studies examining levels of intrinsic motivation in children with LD are mixed. Some studies have found that children with LD demonstrate less intrinsic motivation than students without LD. In a study of fifth and sixth grade students in Greece, children with LD demonstrated lower intrinsic motivation and academic ability beliefs compared to children without LD (Zisimopoulos & Galanaki, 2009). In a study of 89 students with LD in fourth through eighth grade, children with 39 LD had lower academic intrinsic motivation, as well as more negative self-perceptions of their academic ability in comparison to children without LD (Wilson & David, 1994). However, in a study by Pintrich, Anderman, and Klobucar (1994), fifth grade children with LD did not differ on measures of intrinsic motivation or ability beliefs from children without LD, but did have lower levels of metacognitive skills. Inconsistent findings may be due to study confounds including grouping children with different subtypes of LD and different conceptualizations of motivational constructs. Further research is needed to examine how these motivational constructs develop and influence academic achievement. Despite the mixed results regarding levels of intrinsic motivation in children with LD, studies do indicate that intrinsic motivation fosters persistence on difficult tasks, such as a challenging reading passage (Guthrie et al., 2007). Similar to findings regarding attribution style and academic ability beliefs, children with LD who attribute academic success to their effort instead of to other external factors are more likely to show greater levels of academic intrinsic motivation (Adelman, 1978; Black, 1974). Intrinsic motivation is associated with several positive academic and social-emotional outcomes including lower rates of school dropout (Deci & Ryan, 2008; Grant, 2008; Hardre & Reeve, 2003). Summary. Overall, research suggests that children with LD may have more difficulty with self-regulated learning, lower academic ability beliefs, and lower academic intrinsic motivation. Some research suggests that deficits in self-awareness and executive functioning such as, attention, planning, and organization, may contribute to these motivational problems. Additionally, social comparisons with peers and academic failure in school also contribute to poorer academic ability beliefs which in turn affect intrinsic motivation. However, it is still unclear whether motivation differs among children with different subtypes of LD or multiple 40 LDs. Additionally, it is unclear how psychosocial problems, particularly internalizing and interpersonal problems, may influence motivation in children with LD. Parent Influences on Psychosocial Development, Motivation, and Academic Achievement It is well-established that parenting effects children’s development. Several parenting variables have been studied including, parent psychological well-being, parenting style, family structure, and parent involvement in their child’s education and development. Within the literature, more attention has been paid to the relation between parenting variables and child outcomes in early infancy and toddlerhood or in adolescence, with relatively fewer studies examining outcomes in elementary school-age children (Bodovski & Youn, 2010). This section will review the extant literature on two dimensions of parenting, parent depression and parent school involvement, in relation to children’s academic and psychosocial development, with particular attention to research on elementary school-age children with and without LD. Parental Depression. According to the DSM-5, depression in adults is characterized by episodic or persistent symptoms of depressed mood signified by feelings or observations of sadness, emptiness, and hopelessness, loss of interest or pleasure in daily activities, maladaptive changes in sleeping and eating patterns, fatigue, restlessness or psychomotor retardation, feelings of worthlessness or excessive guilt, difficulty thinking or concentrating, and/or suicidal ideation or attempts (APA, 2013). These symptoms impede an individual’s ability to function in daily life, and are not due to a medical condition or the use of substances. Parent depression, particularly maternal depression, has been widely studied in relation to child outcomes (Goodman, 2007; Goodman et al., 2011; National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2009). Many studies indicate that high levels of parent depression are associated with increased behavioral problems in toddlers (Coyne, Low, Miller, Seifer, & Dickstein, 2007; Dietz, Jennings, 41 Kelley, & Marshal, 2009; Gross, Shaw, Burwell, & Nagin, 2009). Elementary school-age children with highly depressed mothers have been shown to have more internalizing and externalizing problems and lower reading skills in kindergarten and first grade (Ashman, Dawson, & Panagiotides, 2008; Kurstjens & Wolke, 2001; Sohr-Preston & Scaramella, 2006). Maternal depression has also been linked to increased risk-taking behavior (e.g., substance use and sexual risk behavior), increased likelihood of internalizing problems including depression, increased problem behaviors, social impairments, and lower academic achievement in adolescents (Champion et al., 2009; Cortes et al., 2009; Goosby, 2007; Gross, Shaw, & Moilanen, 2008; Gross, Shaw, Moilanen, Dishion, & Wilson, 2008; Martins & Gaffan, 2000; Mazza et al., 2009; Rice, Lifford, Thomas, & Thapar, 2007). Researchers have hypothesized about the mechanisms by which parental depression negatively influences child development. Several researchers have found that higher parental depression is associated with decreased parent engagement in their children’s development and education (McLoyd, 1998; National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2009). Some researchers have noted that parental depression is associated with fewer positive interactions between parent and child, more disengaged parenting practices, and more inconsistent and punitive parenting. For example, Bodovski and Youn (2010) found that parental depression was associated with decreased parental warmth and increased use of physical punishment, and those children with more depressed parents in kindergarten had lower reading and math achievement in fifth grade using the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten dataset. Other researchers have found that mothers living in poverty are more likely to experience depression, and that maternal depression mediates the negative effects of poverty on cognitive, behavioral, and social-emotional outcomes for children (Belle, 1990; Carlson & Corcoran, 2001; Johnson & 42 Flake, 2007; McLoyd, 1990; Pachter et al., 2006). Moreover, some research suggests that maternal depression is associated with increased behavioral problems across racial-ethnic groups. For example, Pachter and colleagues (2006) found that maternal depression is directly related to increased behavioral problems in African American children age six to nine, and this effect was partially mediated by parenting practices in Caucasian and Latino children. Thus, parental depression may influence children’s development via direct and indirect pathways. Despite research examining the influence of parent depression on child outcomes, no studies have examined the influence on parental depression in children with LD. Several studies have examined other aspects of parent internalizing psychopathology including, anxiety and parenting stress, in relation to child outcomes for children with other chronic conditions. One study examined symptoms of anxiety in mothers of children in middle school with specific LD, which included children with dyslexia, dysgraphia, and dyscalculia (83% of children in the sample were diagnosed with all three LD’s) (Karande, Kumbhare, Kulkarni, & Shah, 2009). They found that mothers of children with LD had high levels of anxiety, with concerns mainly related to their child’s academic performance, behavior and prognosis in life. Despite a lack of control group, this study provides some evidence that parents of children with LD experience increased internalizing psychopathology. Another study examining parenting stress found that mothers of children with LD reported higher levels of parenting stress compared to mothers of children without LD (Antshel & Joseph, 2006). In this study, mothers of children with RD had the highest level of stress. High parenting stress has been associated with increased maternal depression (Hassiotis, 1997; Lipman, Boyle, Dooley, & Offord, 2002; Moes, Koegel, Schreibman, & Loos, 1992), thus, parenting stress may mediate the effects of maternal depression on child outcomes. Overall, research suggests that parental depression and stress are 43 higher among parents of children with chronic conditions, such as ADHD or other health problems (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2009), however little is known about the influence of parent depression on children with LD. In sum, research suggests parental depression is related to a variety of negative outcomes in children from infancy through adolescence, including lower academic achievement, increased internalizing and externalizing problems, and social impairments. Research also suggests that parental depression is related to socioeconomic status, and that the negative effects of parental depression on child development may operate differentially across racial-ethnic groups. Finally, research suggests that parental depression is associated with parenting practices, including lower levels of parental warmth, increased punitive punishment, and lower levels of parent involvement in children’s development and learning. Further research is warranted to investigate how parent depression influences the academic and psychosocial outcomes in children with LD. Parent School Involvement. Parent school involvement is a broad term that encompasses parent behaviors and perceptions that are related to their child’s development, cognitively, educationally, behaviorally, socially, and emotionally. Parent involvement has been conceptualized in several ways including, parents’ discussion about schooling and their aspirations for their child (Bloom, 1980), parents’ participation in school and educational activities (Stevenson & Baker, 1987), and parents’ rules and monitoring at home related to schooling and education (Marjoribanks, 1983). Given the wide range of behaviors that constitute parent involvement, it is important for studies to define the parameters. In this study, parent involvement is defined as parents’ participation in school activities such as, contact with teachers and attending school events. It is important to recognize that this type of parent school involvement may not represent all parents 44 equally. For example, Hill and colleagues (2004) found that parent school involvement, as broadly defined as attending school events, knowing about their child’s school performance, and having communication with their child’s teacher, was related to lower rates of behavioral problems for students whose parents achieved higher educational levels (a proxy for socioeconomic status). Additionally, Grolnick and colleagues (1997) found family SES was a strong predictor of parent’s involvement in school and at home, however it was not associated with personal involvement such as sharing aspirations for their child’s education were not significantly related to SES. Regarding ethnic differences, studies have also found that parent school involvement was related to improved achievement for African-American students (Hill et al., 2004). Overall, studies have indicated that effects of parent school involvement, as defined by school-based activities in comparison to home-based activities, on academic and psychosocial outcomes vary. Relations may differ by socio-demographic variables including SES and race (Hill & Craft, 2003), and differences may be mediated by the student’s level of psychosocial competence and academic skills. Parental involvement has been associated with a variety of positive child outcomes. Early on, parent involvement helps children transition from home to formal schooling in preschool or kindergarten, and connections between parents and school help children develop early skills vital to later school success (Christenson, 2000; Epstein, 1994; Pianta, Rimm-Kaufman, & Cox, 1999). Studies have found that parent involvement is positively related to early literacy and math skills (Dickinson & DeTemple, 1998; Gauvain, Savage, & McCollum, 2000). In a study by McWayne and colleagues (2004), children in kindergarten whose parents showed higher levels of involvement (e.g., providing enrichment at home) had higher social skills, self-regulation skills, and were more cooperative in school compared to children whose parents were less 45 involved. Several studies have also suggested that parental involvement is positively related to children’s academic achievement across grades and within families of varying socioeconomic statuses and racial-ethnic backgrounds (Christenson, Rounds, & Gorney, 1992; Fan & Chen, 2001; Fehrmann, Keith, & Reimers, 1987; Izzo, Weissberg, Kasprow, & Fendrich, 1999; Jeynes 2005, 2007; Keith, Keith, Quirk, Cohen-Rosenthal, & Franzese, 1996; Keith et al., 1998; Miedel & Reynolds, 2000; Shaver & Walls, 1998; Stevenson & Baker, 1987). In contrast, some studies have not found any significant relations between parent involvement and children’s academic achievement (Sheldon, 2002; Watkins, 1997). Mixed results may be due to varying definitions of parental involvement, as well as grouping heterogeneous populations. Parental involvement has also been linked to children’s motivation in school (GonzalezDeHass, Willems, & Holbein, 2005; Grolnick & Slowiaczek, 1994; Sanders, 1998). For example, Ames and colleagues (1993) found that when second through fourth grade children perceived their parents are more involved in their education (e.g., knowledge of class and teacher), they had more positive academic competency beliefs and intrinsic motivation. In another study, Izzo and colleagues (1999) found that parent participation in school activities was positively related to children’s engagement in school in kindergarten through third grade; however increased parent-teacher contact was related to lower school engagement because the nature of parent-teacher contacts was primarily due to behavioral concerns. On the other hand, some studies indicate that early parent involvement, including contact with teachers, may actually serve as a protective factor because parents are more aware of their child’s academic difficulty early on which may lead to early intervention (Miedel & Reynolds, 2000). The extant literature on the influence of parent involvement on outcomes for children with LD specifically is relatively sparse in comparison to the literature examining parent 46 involvement in typically developing samples or children with other risk factors such as, low socio-economic status. One of the first articles to examine parental involvement in relation to children with LD highlighted the importance of interactions between parents and educational staff, stating “when parents are actively involved with professionals, three processes occur: the parents and professional exchange information, the parents are encouraged to grow in their role, and a trusting, productive relationship between parents and teachers is built.” (McLoughlin, Edge, & Strenecky, 1978, p.291-292). This article also identified several stages and ways in which parents may be involved in promoting the academic success of their child with an LD. For example, monitoring and being aware of early signs of difficulty, participating in school, home, and community activities that promote development of children’s skills, and advocating for early intervention. The importance of parental involvement in the academic and behavioral development of children with disabilities is now reflected in special education law which encourages and specifies the rights of parents to be involved in their children’s educational planning (IDEA, 2004). Given that parental involvement increases school readiness, and school readiness has been found to positively influence later academic achievement, it is reasonable to suggest that early parental involvement may help set children on a more positive academic and psychosocial trajectory. In a more recent study using the ECLS-K data, parents’ engagement in home literacy activities in kindergarten positively predicted academic achievement in eighth grade indirectly via children’s kindergarten achievement (Froiland, Peterson, & Davison, 2012). Children who develop LD’s may have a variety of early risk factors including, neurobiological and environmental vulnerabilities (e.g., genetic predisposition, poverty, parental depression). Parental involvement, especially early on in a child’s education, may serve a protective factor in children 47 with LD that alters the influence of risk factors and promotes better psychosocial and academic outcomes. Thus, research suggests that parental involvement leads to improved academic, socialemotional, behavioral, and motivational outcomes in children; however, it is unclear how these mechanisms operate in children with LD and what factors may hinder or promote parent involvement in school-based activities. Purpose of Present Study There is a plethora of evidence suggesting that children with LD experience more academic difficulties and are more likely to experience academic failure and be at risk for school dropout (Reschly & Christenson, 2006). Additionally, there is strong research suggesting that children with LD experience more psychosocial problems, including internalizing and externalizing problems and poor interpersonal skills (Willcutt et al., 2013). These problems tend to persist, and put children with LD at greater risk for increased mental health problems in adulthood and juvenile delinquency (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014). However, there are several gaps and methodological issues in the existing literature. First, many studies use small samples that are not representative of the population at large (La Greca & Stone, 1990; Martinez & Semrud-Clikeman, 2004; Maughan, Rowe, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2003). Second, several studies do not use control samples thereby limiting the conclusions that can be drawn in relation to children with LD (Karande, Kumbhare, Kulkarni, & Shah, 2009; Trzesniewski, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Maughan, 2006). Third, several studies group children with various subtypes of LD into one category thereby ignoring the differences among these heterogeneous groups and limiting the ability to understand how children defined by different academic problems function (Gadeyne, Ghesquiere, & Onghena, 2004; Rourke & Fuerst, 1992). This study aims to address these issues by using a large, nationally representative dataset, and examining 48 group differences among children with subtypes of LD including, math disability, reading disability, co-morbid math and reading disability, and children without LD. These same methodological issues plague the small number of studies that have examined motivational factors including, self-regulated learning, ability beliefs, and intrinsic motivation in children with LD. Some studies have demonstrated that children with LD have more difficulty with self-regulated learning, more negative ability beliefs, and lower intrinsic motivation for learning, but results are far from conclusive. Moreover, despite research indicating the relation between low academic performance, increased psychosocial problems, and lower motivation for learning, it is unclear how these three factors influence one another in children with subtypes of LD. Very little research has examined the specific role of parent involvement as a protective factor and parent depression as a risk factor in children with LD. Research suggests that increased parent involvement in beneficial for children with LD, but much of the research is theoretical in nature. Likewise, research examining the effects of parental depression on child outcomes mainly focus on typically developing children and does not specifically examine children who develop learning disabilities. Finally, some of the existing research studies do not account for the effects of socio-economic status and race-ethnicity on academic and psychosocial outcomes for children with LD. This study uses statistical methods that can account for these potential influences within a complex model. The purpose of the current study is to test a structural equation model examining the relations between parent depression, parent involvement, early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten, and later psychosocial behaviors, motivation, and academic achievement in fifth grade among a sample of children with and without LD. Using 49 special education eligibility in fifth grade to identify students who have reading disability, math disability, co-morbid reading and math disability, and no LD, the current study examines whether parent depression served as a risk factor and parent involvement served as a protective factor, and differentially predicted improved outcomes for children with and without LD. Structural equation modeling (SEM) will be used test the proposed model (see Figure 2 and Figure 3). Research Questions and Hypotheses The following research questions were proposed. A simplified conceptual model is presented in Figure 1. The full conceptual model is explained in detail in the following chapter (colored lines represent pathways examined in different research questions). Research question one focuses on specifying relations between constructs in the conceptual model. Research question two and three focus on testing whether the specified relations are the same or different for children with and without LD. Research Question 1. What are the relations between parent depression and parent school involvement on early child academic achievement and psychosocial in kindergarten, and motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement outcomes in fifth grade? 1a. Does parent depression indirectly predict early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten via parent school involvement? This question is depicted by red paths in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Hypothesis 1a. Lower levels of parent depression will indirectly predict higher levels of academic achievement and more positive psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten via parent school involvement. Lower levels of parent depression will be directly related to higher levels of 50 parent school involvement, which in turn, will predict higher levels of academic achievement and more positive psychosocial behaviors. Parent school involvement is a mediating variable. 1b. Does parent school involvement in kindergarten predict children’s motivation and psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade given their level of academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten? This question is depicted by green paths in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Hypothesis 1b. As the level of academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten increases (e.g., more positive performance) higher levels of parent school involvement will predict higher levels of motivation and more positive psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade. Early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors are moderating variables. 1c. Does early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten predict academic achievement in fifth grade? This question is depicted by blue paths in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Hypothesis 1c. Higher levels of academic achievement and more positive prosocial behaviors in kindergarten will predict higher levels of academic achievement in fifth grade. 1d. Does motivation and psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade predict academic achievement in fifth grade? This question is depicted by yellow paths in Figure 2 and Figure 3. Hypothesis 1d. Higher levels of motivation and more positive prosocial behaviors in fifth grade will predict higher levels of academic achievement in fifth grade. Previous academic and psychosocial performance will be controlled given that the model is run with all variables simultaneously. Research Question 2. Do the relations between parent depression and parent school involvement on early child academic achievement and psychosocial in kindergarten, and 51 motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement outcomes in fifth grade differ for children identified with learning disabilities in 5th grade and children without learning disabilities? Hypothesis 2. The relations between parenting variables and academic, psychosocial, and motivation outcomes hypothesized in research question 1 will be stronger for high-risk children with LD compared to children without LD. In other words, the magnitude of the relations will be greater for children with LD compared to children without LD. Thus, parent depression will serve as a greater risk factor, parent school involvement will serve as a greater protective factor, and the relations between academic achievement, motivation, and psychosocial behavior will be greater for children with LD compared to children without LD. Research Question 3. Does type of learning disability (math disability, reading disability, math-reading disability) moderate the relations between parent depression and parent school involvement on early child academic achievement and psychosocial in kindergarten, and motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement outcomes in fifth grade? Hypothesis 3. The relations between the parenting variables and academic, psychosocial, and motivation outcomes hypothesized in research question 1 will be strongest for children with the highest level of risk, children with RD-MD, then for children with one LD (MD or RD), then for children with no LD. In other words, the magnitude of the relations will be the least for children without LD, greater for children with one LD, and the greatest for children with comorbid RD-MD. 52 Figure 1. Simplified Conceptual Model Parent Mental Health and Parenting Behavior Early Academic Achievement and Psychosocial Behaviors Covariates: • Gender • Race • SES • Age at Assessment Motivation and Psychosocial Behaviors Academic Achievement 53 CHAPTER 3 METHOD Study Model The current study aimed to test a structural equation model examining the relations between early parenting variables, namely parent depression and parent school involvement, and outcomes in kindergarten and fifth grade. Outcomes of interest included early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten, and motivation, academic achievement, and psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade among a sample of children with and without LD. A structural equation model specified four latent constructs and five observed variables. The latent variables included parent depression, parent school involvement, early psychosocial behaviors (kindergarten), and psychological behaviors in fifth grade. The observed variables included standard scores on assessments of reading and math in kindergarten and fifth grade, and a mean score on a measure of motivation. Covariates included the child’s gender, race-ethnicity, SES, and age at kindergarten assessment. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the proposed model (see Figure 2 and Figure 3). The model in Figure 2 depicts relations for academic achievement in reading and the model in Figure 3 depicts relations for academic achievement in mathematics. The models are presented separately for simplicity; however academic achievement will be run simultaneously. 54 Figure 2. Detailed Conceptual Model (Reading) Parent Depression Motivation Early Academic Achievement: Reading Academic Achievement: Reading Early Parent School Involvement Early Psychosocial Behavior Psychosocial Behavior Note: Latent variables are shown in circles. The dashed lines represent the interaction between the endogenous variable (parent school involvement) and the moderator variables. Covariates: • Gender • Race • SES • Age at Assessment 55 Figure 3. Detailed Conceptual Model (Math) Parent Depression Motivation Early Academic Achievement: Math Academic Achievement: Math Early Parent School Involvement Early Psychosocial Behavior Psychosocial Behavior Note: Latent variables are shown in circles. The dashed lines represent the interaction between the endogenous variable (parent school involvement) and the moderator variables. Covariates: • Gender • Race • SES • Age at Assessment 56 Study Design Data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998-1999 (ECLS-K) kindergarten and fifth grade data collection waves were used to examine the hypothesized relation between parental depression and early parent school involvement, early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors, and motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement outcomes in children with and without learning disabilities in mathematics and/or reading. The U.S. Department of Education’s (DOE) National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) within the Institute of Education Science (IES) conducted the ECLS-K by following a nationally representative sample of children entering kindergarten in the fall of 1998 through the eighth grade. This study took advantage of data at two time points to understand the influence of early parenting variables on child motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement within a large sample of children identified with and without learning disabilities. Data Collection. Data were collected using a multi-informant and multi-method design, allowing for in-depth analysis of child, family, school, and other environmental variables related to child development and education. Data were collected at multiple time points: the fall and spring of kindergarten (also known as base year; 1998-1999), the fall and spring of first grade (1999-2000), the spring of third grade (2002), the spring of fifth grade (2004), and the spring of eighth grade (2007) (see Table 1). Children were sampled in the fall of 1998 if they were entering kindergarten. Approximately 81% of children sampled in the fall of kindergarten attended kindergarten for the first time, approximately 4% of children repeated kindergarten, and approximately 15% of children were missing data on kindergarten entry status. 57 Several sampling procedures were used in subsequent rounds of data collection to address issues of representativeness of the sample to the population and attrition. In the spring of 1999 (kindergarten spring), an additional 1,430a children were sampled for the study via efforts to convert schools that had originally refused to participate in the fall. In the fall of 1999 (first grade fall), data were collected for children who had been sampled in kindergarten and stayed in the same school for first grade (i.e., non-movers), and for a random 50% of students who were sampled in kindergarten but transferred to another school for first grade (i.e., movers). However, given that many children do change schools between kindergarten and first grade schools, a high number of students moving to the same first grade location were treated as non-movers. This procedure was done to limit costs associated with data collection. Finally, a half-open interval sampling procedure was implemented in the same 50 percent subsample of schools used in the fall first grade data collection time frame to “freshen” the spring 2000 first grade sample. This was done to include first grade students who had not been enrolled in kindergarten in 1998-1999, and thus, would have not had a chance to be included in the base year sample. A total of 170a students were added to the first grade sample via the freshening procedures. For this study, children added during the first grade freshening procedures were not included because they are missing data collected during the spring of kindergarten (see Missing Data and Exclusion Criteria sections below). The primary methods of data collection involved interviews, questionnaires, and direct child assessments. Computer-assisted telephone or in-person interviewing was used to conduct parent interviews and questionnaires. The parent interview lasted approximately 65 minutes in the spring kindergarten round. Participating parents were sent a thank you letter and educational activity booklet upon completion of the interview. Computer-assisted interviewing and one-on- 58 one untimed administrations of hard-copy instruments were used to conduct direct child assessments. Self-administered questionnaires were distributed to teachers and school administrators to complete. Field staff received training prior to conducting assessments. Assessors were selected based on previous experience with other large research studies (e.g., NAEP, TIMSS), and received intensive training on interviewing techniques and assessments via home training modules and in-person training sessions. Spanish-bilingual assessors also received additional training to practice Spanish versions of assessments and interviewing techniques. For the data collection periods used in this study assessors attended five days of training and field supervisors attended an additional two days of training prior to the fall kindergarten data collection. Prior to spring kindergarten data collection assessors attended an additional review of procedures over a four-week period. Prior to the fifth grade data collection, assessors attended at least eight hours of home-study training and five to eight days of in-person training. During the fifth grade data collection period, assessors also completed a certification process involving written exercises and performing a live, observed assessment. Overall, 99% of assessors scored above an 85% on the certification procedures (considered passing), and 1% were offered remedial training and completed a second observed assessment before being cleared. The primary investigator was granted permission to use the restricted-use ECLS-K data for this project under a restricted-data license agreement provided to her dissertation faculty chair. A waiver from the Michigan State University Institutional Review Board was obtained for completion of this project (see Appendix A). 59 Table 1 Overview of Original ECLS-K Data Collection Sample Data Collection Unweighted Unweighted 1,a Wave Sample Size Number of Respondents2,a Age of Study Child (months) Data Collection Period Kindergarten F 21,390 19,680 Mean 65.79 SD 14.80 Fall 1998 Kindergarten S 22,8103 20,580 74.63 4.87 Spring 1999 First Grade F 6,5104 5,420 79.94 4.97 Fall 1999 First Grade S 21,3605 17,320 86.94 4.84 Spring 2000 Third Grade S 21,360 15,300 111.06 4.77 Spring 2002 Fifth Grade S 16,1406 11,820 134.63 4.93 Spring 2004 Eighth Grade S 12,130 9,720 171.25 6.69 Spring 2007 Note. F = Fall; S = Spring; a All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement; 1 Sample size represents total number of children sampled for the study. Some children were non-responders or ineligible; 2 Represents total number of children with child and/or parent data; 3 approximately 1,430 children were sampled from schools that were converted from refusals in the fall; 4 Data were collected from only 30% of sampled schools containing about 27% of base year students; 5 approximately 21,190 children remained eligible after the base year, and 165 children were sample via the sample freshening procedure; 6 approximately 5,210 children were excluded due to ineligibility (e.g., moved, hard refusals, no first or third grade data) in fifth grade. 60 ECLS-K Original Sample The ECLS-K used a multistage, stratified, clustered, equal probability systematic sampling design. The following information is obtained from the publicly available U.S. Department of Education National Center of Education Statistics Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998 –99: User’s manual for the ECLS-K base year public-use data files and electronic codebook (2001). The primary sampling units (PSU) consisted of geographic areas based on groups of counties. The 1990 county-level population data and 1994 population estimates of five-year-olds by race-ethnicity were used to determine a total of 1,335 PSUs (U.S. DOE NCES, 2001). One hundred PSUs were selected for the ECLS-K based on census region, race-ethnicity, and 1988 per capita income. Within each PSU, public schools with a minimum of 24 kindergarteners and private schools with a minimum of 12 kindergarteners were selected (schools with fewer students were clustered together) (U.S. DOE NCES, 2001). The target number of schools sampled per PSU was proportional to the weighted measure of size of each PSU, with a minimum of one school per PSU for any PSU that was so small that it would not otherwise have been allocated a school. Of the school districts with sampled schools, 75% of public school districts and 93% of private school districts cooperated with the study. Overall, a total of 1,280 schools (934 public) were selected for the ECLS-K (U.S. DOE NCES, 2001). Children that identified as Asian or Pacific Islander (API) were the only groups that were oversampled to meet the nationally representative sample size criteria. Two independent sampling strata were created within each school, one for API students and another for the remaining students. Within each stratum, students were selected using equal probability systematic sampling. Twins were sampled as a unit (both included), rather than separate. Once the sample was finalized, parental information was obtained from the schools, and parental 61 consent for each child was obtained. The ECLS-K study followed the consent procedures specified by participating school districts. Implicit consent was obtained from about half of the schools via a form sent to each of the selected student’s home. The first wave of data collection in the fall of 1998 and spring of 1999 (kindergarten year) included a total of 21,260 children across the country. Within the base year sample, children were White, non-Hispanic (55.2%), Black or African-American (15.1%), Hispanic, Race Specified (8.3%), Hispanic, Race not Specified (9.4%), Asian (6.4%), Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander (1.0%), Native American or Alaskan Native (1.8%), and Multi-racial (2.4%); 51.1% of children were male (n=10,866); the average age of children was 65.8 months (SD = 14.8). In the spring of 2004 (fifth grade), a total of 11,820 children participated in the study. Demographic characteristics of the fifth grade sample remained roughly the same: White, non-Hispanic (57.0%), Black or African-American (11.4%), Hispanic, Race Specified (9.3%), Hispanic, Race not Specified (9.8%), Asian (6.9%), Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander (1.3%), Native American or Alaskan Native (1.9%), and Multi-racial (2.4%); 50.7% of children were male (n= 5,987); the average age of children was 134.6 months (SD = 4.9). Missing Data. Given that longitudinal research studies often have missing data due to attrition (e.g., move school, decline participation, death), the data were analyzed to determine the number of children missing data at kindergarten and fifth grade time points (approximately 9,620 children did not have any data at either the kindergarten spring or fifth grade spring time point; after exclusionary criteria were applied approximately 10,720 children did not have any data at one of the time points of interest). To better understand the nature of the missing data and the exclusionary factors, comparison tests were calculated to determine if there were significant differences on demographic variables and predictor variables between children with and without 62 missing data (see Table 2, Table 3, and Table 4). Given that large sample sizes often yield a higher rate of significant group differences, effect sizes and odds ratios were examined as well. Although several significant differences emerged between the two groups, only a few yielded medium to large effect sizes suggesting a more meaningful statistically significant difference across the groups. There were very small to small effect sizes (Cohen’s d ranged from .02 to .31) for differences in SES, age at fifth grade, teacher-rated early psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten, and student-rated motivation in fifth grade. There were medium effect sizes for differences in academic achievement (Cohen’s d ranged from .42 to .44), with the included group having higher scores, on average of 4 T-score points, on reading and math achievement at both time points. Medium to large effect sizes were noted for fifth grade psychosocial behavior (Cohen’s d ranged from .55 to .81), with the approaches to learning scale having the most significant difference across groups. On average, students included in the study were rated has having higher approaches to learning behavior by approximately .5 points. Differences in the academic achievement and fifth grade psychosocial behavior between the included and excluded study group is a limitation of the study. However, these findings are not entirely unexpected as students with more pervasive disabilities and impairments (e.g., cognitive disability, autism spectrum disorder) were excluded from the study and have a higher likelihood of lower academic and psychosocial skills as they may mature at a slower rate than their typical peers as the demands of school increase over time. Also, the excluded sample had far fewer children with learning disabilities; students with learning disability were 3.11 times more likely to be included in the study. However, the inclusion of children with learning disabilities is important for the aim of this study and the sample of children without learning disabilities is similar in both groups. Instead of imputing a large amount of missing data, sample weights were applied to all analyses 63 to account for non-response biases and maintain generalizability of the final results to the population. Robust weighted least square and maximum likelihood estimation options within the MPlus program were used to test the factor structure of each latent variable and the full structural model. The final sample used in the structural model consisted of 10,630 children. 64 Table 2 Comparison of Demographic Information Between Included and Excluded Samples Characteristics Included Sample Excluded Sample Chi-Square (df) Na (%) Na (%) Odds Ratio (95% CI) Total 10,630 10,720 Child Gender 50.67 (1), p<.001*** 1.22 (1.15-1.28) Male 5,180 (48.7%) 5,690 (53.1%) Female 5,450 (51.3%) 4,930 (46.0%) Child 297.60 (8), p<.001*** Race/Ethnicity 1.04 (1.03-1.06) White/Non5,930 (55.7%) 5810 (54.2%) Hispanic Black/African1,220 (11.5%) 1,980 (18.5%) American Hispanic, Race 960 (9.0%) 800 (7.5%) Specified Hispanic, No 1,120 (10.5%) 890 (8.3%) Race Specified Asian, Non800 (7.5%) 570 (5.3%) Hispanic Native Hawaiian 140 (1.3%) 80 (0.7%) or Other Pacific Islander Native 210 (2.0%) 170 (1.6%) American or Alaskan Native More Than One 230 (2.2%) 280 (2.7%) Race Learning 144.12 (1), p<.001*** Disability Status 3.11 (2.56-3.78) With Learning 410 (3.9%) 140 (1.3%) Disability Without 10,220 (96.1%) 10,580 (98.7%) Learning Disability Note: a All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. 65 Table 3 Comparison of Covariates and Predictor Variables for Included and Excluded Samples Variables Included Excluded Independent Samples tCohen’s d Sample Sample test (df) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Kindergarten SES .04 (.80) -.03 (.80) -6.96 (20098), p<.001*** .09 Age at 68.41 (4.32) 68.51 (4.66) 1.52 (18894), p=.13 .02 Assessment (K, 1999) Age at 134.54 (4.38) 135.81(5.75) 7.11 (1229), p<.001*** .25 Assessment (5th grade, 2004) Kindergarten Achievement Direct Math 51.63 (9.69) 47.64 (9.78) -12.64 (1270), p<.001*** .41 Assessment .42 Direct 51.56 (9.58) 47.50(9.92) -12.60 (1251), p<.001*** Reading Assessment Fifth Grade Achievement Direct Math 51.54 (9.52) 47.40 (10.31) -12.68 (1292), p<.001*** .42 Assessment Direct 51.46 (9.56) 47.20 (10.01) -13.27 (1300), p<.001*** .44 Reading Assessment Early Psychosocial Behaviors 3.20 (.65) 2.99 (.71) -21.18 (18141), .31 Teacher-Rated p<.001*** .29 Approaches to .27 Learning 3.25 (.59) 3.07 (.66) Teacher-Rated -19.28 (17916), .31 Self-Control 3.19 (.61) 3.02 (.66) p<.001*** .23 Teacher-Rated Interpersonal -18.07 (17983), Skills 1.58 (.58) 1.78 (.70) p<.001*** Teacher-Rated Externalizing 1.52 (.48) 1.64 (.55) Teacher-Rated 21.79 (17209), p<.001*** Internalizing 15.87 (17735), p<.001*** Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior 66 Table 3 (cont’d) Teacher-Rated 3.11 (.66) 2.58 (.65) Approaches to Learning Teacher-Rated 3.27 (.58) 2.90 (.66) Self-Control Teacher-Rated 3.12 (.62) 2.71 (.67) Interpersonal Skills Teacher-Rated 1.61 (.56) 1.96 (.70) Externalizing Teacher-Rated 1.60 (.51) 1.98 (.64) Internalizing Motivation Student-Rated 2.75 (.63) 2.56 (.69) School Note: Equal variance not assumed for all variables. 67 -25.65 (1317), p<.001*** .81 -17.40 (1226), p<.001*** .60 -18.71 (1245), p<.001*** .64 16.11 (1209), p<.001*** .55 18.59 (1200), p<.001*** .66 -8.67 (1291), p<.001*** .29 Table 4 Comparison of Covariates and Predictor Variables for Included and Excluded Samples (Ordinal) Variables Included Sample Excluded Chi-Square (df) Na (%) Sample Odds Ratio (95% CI) Na (%) Parent Depression Felt bothered 76.75(3), p<.001*** Never 5260 (49.5%) 4390 (41.0%) .96 (.92-1.00) Some Time 3490 (32.8%) 3140 (29.3%) Moderate Amount 770 (7.2%) 770 (7.2%) Most of Time 260 (2.4%) 420 (3.9%) Poor appetite 102.21(3), p<.001*** Never 7090 (66.7%) 5850 (54.6%) .89 (.86-.93) Some Time 2010 (18.9%) 1980 (18.4%) Moderate Amount 390 (3.7%) 450 (4.2%) Most of Time 300 (2.8%) 470 (4.3%) Felt the “blues” 69.91(3), p<.001*** Never 7880 (74.1%) 6660 (62.2%) .99 (.93-1.05) Some Time 1460 (13.8%) 1510 (14.1%) Moderate Amount 250 (2.3%) 280 (2.6%) Most of Time 170 (1.6%) 270 (2.5%) Trouble focusing 56.43(3), p<.001*** Never 5750 (54.1%) 4790 (44.7%) .98 (.93-1.03) Some Time 3300 (31.0%) 3070 (28.7%) Moderate Amount 450 (4.2%) 490 (4.6%) Most of Time 270 (2.6%) 380 (3.5%) Felt depressed 76.30(3), p<.001*** Never 7210 (67.8%) 5960 (55.6%) .97 (.91-1.04) Some Time 2090 (19.6%) 2170 (20.3%) Moderate Amount 250 (2.3%) 300 (2.8%) Most of Time 230 (2.1%) 300 (2.8%) Felt things were effortful 86.06(3), p<.001*** Never 5590 (52.6%) 4530 (42.2%) .93 (.90-.96) Some Time 2960 (27.9%) 2760 (25.7%) Moderate Amount 500 (4.7%) 530 (5.0%) Most of Time 700 (6.6%) 900 (8.4%) Felt fearful 15.04(3), p=.03** Never 8320 (78.3%) 7270 (67.8%) 1.04 (.98-1.10) Some Time 1160 (10.9%) 1130 (10.5%) Moderate Amount 160 (1.5%) 190 (1.7%) Most of Time 130 (1.2%) 140 (1.3%) Difficulty sleeping 75.14(3), p<.001*** Never 4870 (45.8%) 4020 (37.5%) .95 (.92-.99) Some Time 3420 (32.2%) 3020 (28.1%) Moderate Amount 750 (7.1%) 750 (7.0%) 68 Table 4 (cont’d) Most of Time Talked less Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt lonely Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt sad Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Difficulty “to get going” Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Parent School Involvement Contact school Yes No Attended open house Yes No Attended PTO Yes No Attended parent group Yes No Attended teacher conference Yes No Attended school event Yes No Volunteered at school Yes No 730 (6.9%) 940 (8.8%) 7930 (74.6%) 1440 (13.5%) 260 (2.5%) 150 (1.4%) 6820 (63.6%) 1450 (13.6%) 250 (2.4%) 210 (2.0%) 7910 (74.4%) 1480 (13.9%) 210 (2.0%) 170 (1.6%) 6680 (62.3%) 1510 (14.1%) 250 (2.4%) 300 (2.8%) 6130 (57.7%) 3130 (29.4%) 330 (3.1%) 190 (1.7%) 5110 (47.7%) 2960 (27.6%) 400 (3.7%) 270 (2.5%) 5830 (54.9%) 3210 (30.2%) 440 (4.2%) 280 (2.7%) 4900 (45.7%) 2980 (27.8%) 460 (4.3%) 400 (3.7%) 5160 (48.6) 4800 (45.2) 5190 (48.4) 3800 (35.4) 7520 (70.7) 2430 (22.9) 6310 (58.9) 2650 (24.7) 3680 (34.7) 6270 (59.0) 2790 (26.1) 6170 (57.6) 920 (8.6) 9040 (85.0) 700 (6.5) 8270 (77.2) 33.98(3), p<.001*** 1.06 (1.00-1.12) 83.35(3), p<.001*** .89 (.84-.95) 60.88(3), p<.001*** 1.00 (.94-1.06) 51.49(3), p<.001*** 1.00 (.95-1.04) 66.56(1), p<.001*** 1.27 (1.20-1.35) 63.33(1), p<.001*** .77 (.72-.82) 71.74(1), p<.001*** .77 (.73-.82) 12.75(1), p<.001*** .83 (.75-.92) 27.64(1), p<.001*** .81 (.75-.88) 8570 (80.6) 1400 (13.1) 7470 (69.7) 1500 (14.0) 6870 (64.6) 3090 (29.0) 5750 (53.7) 3220 (30.0) 5140 (48.4) 4820 (45.3) 4020 (37.5) 4950 (46.2) 69 50.18(1), p<.001*** .80 (.76-.85) 87.65(1), p<.001*** .76 (.72-.81) Table 4 (cont’d) Participated in fundraiser 43.87(1), p<.001*** Yes 6140 (57.8) 5110 (47.6) .82 (.78-.87) No 3810 (35.9) 3860 (36.0) Note: a All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. 70 Final Sample for Current Study The final sample for the current study included children from the ECLS-K data set who had parent and teacher reports of psychosocial behavior, direct assessment of math and reading skills in kindergarten and fifth grade, and self-report of motivation in fifth grade. School staff, namely the primary special education teacher, also completed a questionnaire for each child with an IEP which indicated their primary disability in the spring of fifth grade. This information was used to determine students identified with a primary learning disability in math, reading, or both math and reading in fifth grade. All unweighted sample sizes reported in this document were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. Participants assigned the longitudinal sample weight C2_6FP0 were retained for the current study. The longitudinal sample weight C2_6FP0 was assigned to children who had parent interview data across four rounds of data collection (spring kindergarten, spring first grade, spring third grade, and spring fifth grade) in conjunction with child assessment, school, teacher or classroom data. This sample weight is more restrictive than a weight that would account for just spring kindergarten and spring fifth grade data, but it is the weight that best fits the analysis based on the available weights published in the ECLS-K restricted dataset. Using a more restrictive weight still adjusts for the current studies intended analyses, and will allow for generalizability to the kindergarten class of 1998-1999. The final sample consisted of 10,630 children. Mean child age at each time point is reported in Table 5. Children in the final sample were approximately 56% White, non-Hispanic, 11% Black or African-American, 9% Hispanic, race specified, 10% Hispanic, no race specified, 7% Asian, 1% Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander, 2% Native American or Alaskan Native, and 2% Multi-racial; 5,712 were male (50.3%). The majority of children entered 71 kindergarten in the fall of 1998 for the first time (n=8,900; 83.7%). Approximately 3% of children repeated kindergarten and 13% of children did not have data on kindergarten entry status. By the fifth grade data collection period (spring 2004), a small percentage of students had been retained and were in a grade below fifth grade (n=910; 8.6%) and an even smaller percentage of students had been promoted to a grade above fifth grade (n=20; 0.2%). Within this study, children’s age at assessment was used as a covariate to control for effects due to maturation (see Covariates section below for further discussion on retention). See Table 6 for child demographic characteristics. Exclusion Criteria. Given the interest in the relations between parent involvement, parental depression, motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement in children with learning disabilities, children with intellectual disability, neurological conditions (e.g., traumatic brain injury, cerebral palsy), neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., pervasive developmental disorder, autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit and hyperactivity disorder), and mental health issues were excluded from the sample so as not to confound results. Exclusion was based on parent and/or school report of a diagnosis of developmental delay, intellectual disability, cerebral palsy, pervasive developmental disorder, autism spectrum disorder, visual or hearing impairment, attention-deficit and hyperactivity disorder, anxiety or depression disorder, or “other disability” during kindergarten and/or fifth grade; 1,100 children met these exclusion criteria. Participants who are missing data entirely across one of the two waves of data collection of interest (kindergarten or fifth grade) were also excluded (9,620 children did not have any data at either the kindergarten spring or fifth grade spring time point). No other exclusionary criteria were applied. 72 Learning Disabled Sample. Overall, a total of 410 children with a learning disability were included in the current study, of which 180 children had a reading disability, 20 children had a math disability, and 220 children had a reading and math disability. Field supervisors were provided the name of all fifth grade students who received special education services and their primary special education teacher (e.g., teacher who managed the child’s Individualized Education Plan (IEP) or spent the most time providing services to the child). Identification of learning disability was based on special education records and report of the child’s main disability category by the child’s fifth grade primary special education teacher. Thus, in this study diagnosis of a learning disability is presumed to follow IDEA definition of specific learning disability. Schools likely used a pattern of strengths or weaknesses model, response-tointervention, or discrepancy paradigm to identify a learning disability. Direct information about the child’s learning disability category (e.g., reading, math, or both) was unavailable, so LD type was determined based on the special education teacher’s report of the child’s primary IEP goals. If the child had primary reading goals, it was assumed that they demonstrated academic difficulty in reading and thus, were categorized as LD-Reading. If the child had primary math goals, it was assumed that they demonstrated academic difficulties in math, and were categorized as LD-math. If the child had primary goals in both reading and math, it was assumed that they had academic difficulties in both subjects and were categorized as LD-Co-morbid Math and Reading. Of the 410 children identified with LD, less than 10 children did not have primary reading or math academic goals specified in their IEP, thus they were not included in the categorized analysis. This study examined how early parenting variables influenced motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement in children diagnosed with learning disabilities in fifth 73 grade, rather than children diagnosed with learning disabilities in kindergarten or children who were diagnosed with learning disabilities at any point in time between kindergarten and fourth grade for two primary reasons. First, research indicates that identification of learning disabilities in very young children (i.e., preschool/early kindergarten) is often unreliable and inaccurate (Rathvon, 2004; Scarborough, 1998). Instead of having a true learning disability, children in kindergarten may be struggling academically because they have not received formal education prior to kindergarten (e.g., instruction in basic reading principles in preschool or at home), children may be adapting to classroom expectations and behavioral or motivational issues may be more salient in screening/testing, and because there is a wide range of natural variation in the rate at which children begin to learn to read or do math (e.g., some children in kindergarten may be “fast learners” compared to other children who take a longer time to grasp reading or math principles). Second, by examining children who were identified with a learning disability in fifth grade, it is presumed that these children have either had persistent academic problems throughout elementary school and continue to qualify for special education services or they have been recently diagnosed with a learning disability. Moreover, the stability (e.g., persistence and permanence) of learning disabilities is a cornerstone of the diagnosis. Children who have been diagnosed with a learning disability prior to fifth grade, and no longer qualified for special education as a child with a learning disability in fifth grade, presumably were no longer experiencing impairment in academic functioning, and may have received support earlier in elementary school. Given that this study aims to answer questions about how early parenting variables influence outcomes for children with and without persistent academic problems it is 74 rational to examine students who are identified or continue to be identified as a child with a learning disability at the point at which outcomes are measured (fifth grade). Overall, 3.9% of the study sample had an LD, 1.7% had an RD, 0.2% had an MD, and 2.1% had a RD-MD. This represents a lower prevalence of children with LD in the study sample as compared to general population estimates. Extant literature indicates 5-10% of the population has a MD (Berch & Mazzocco, 2007; Geary, Hoard, Nugent, & Bailey, 2012; Judge & Watson, 2011), 5% and 17.5% of the population has a reading disability (Shaywitz & Shaywitz, 2005), and approximately 30% to 70% of individuals with either disorder have a co-morbid RD or MD (Kovas et al., 2007; Landerl & Moll, 2010; Willcutt et al., 2013). There are several reasons that the study sample may have a lower LD sample size including, overlap in the presence of LD in children who were excluded from the study, particularly children with identified ADHD and social-emotional and behavior problems. Researchers suggest that approximately 24% to 38% of children with RD and/or MD have co-morbid ADHD (Capano, Minden, Chen, Schachar, & Ickowicz, 2008; DuPaul, Gormley, & Laracy, 2013; Langberg, Vaughn, Brinkman, Froehlich, & Epstein, 2010), and between 18%-19% of children with oppositional-defiant disorder, anxiety, and/or depression have an LD (Mayes & Calhoun, 2006). Additionally, students with LD were identified based on report of LD status from the child’s IEP, thus children who may receive a clinical diagnosis of LD and who are not yet being served in the school and children who may have an LD but are being served via undocumented accommodations are not able to be accounted for. The relatively lower prevalence of LD compared to the general population is a limitation of the study and conclusions were interpreted with caution. Sampling Weights. Descriptive analyses on the final sample include unweighted and weighted means. Unweighted descriptive statistics represent the ECLS-K population where each 75 case was counted equally. Weighted descriptive statistics represents the entire population of children attending kindergarten in 1998, and accounts for those children who were not sampled. The weighted sample reflects population estimates from the 1994 U.S. Census. Full sample weights and nesting variables (based on the Taylor Series method for computing standard errors) were applied to structural equation analyses to adjust for differential selection probabilities, reduce non-response bias and standard error, and allow for results that are generalizable to the population. 76 Table 5 Overview of Data Collection for Final Sample Data Collection Wave Age of Study Child (months) Data Collection Period Kindergarten Spring Mean 68.41 SD 4.32 Spring 1999 Fifth Grade Spring 134.54 4.38 Spring 2004 77 Table 6 Demographic Characteristics of Final Sample (n = 10,630) Characteristic Sample Na Sample % Weighted N Weighted % Child Gender Male 5,180 48.7% 1,644,099 49.0% Female 5,450 51.3% 1,709,317 51.0% White/NonHispanic 5,930 55.7% 1,880,891 56.1% Black/AfricanAmerican 1,220 11.5% 553,007 16.5% Hispanic, Race Specified 960 9.0% 306,944 9.2% 1,120 10.5% 352,309 10.5% Asian 800 7.5% 100,708 3.0% Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander 140 1.3% 27,480 0.8% Native American or Alaskan Native 210 2.0% 61,841 1.8% More Than One Race 230 2.2% 68,676 2.0% 410 3.9% 123,453 3.7% 10,220 96.1% 3,229,963 96.3% Child Race/Ethnicity Hispanic, No Race Specified Learning Disability Status Dichotomized With Learning Disability Without Learning Disability Categorized 78 Table 6 (cont’d) Math Disability 20 0.2% 9,052 0.3% Reading Disability 180 1.7% 40,126 1.2% Co-morbid Math and Reading Disability 220 2.0% 72,492 2.2% Note: a All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. 79 Variables and Measures A description of the measures for each of the predictor and outcome variables are described below in relation to the conceptual model, including parent depression, early parent school involvement, academic achievement, psychosocial behaviors, and motivation. Refer to Table 7 for a list of latent constructs and variables. Table 7 Summary of ECLS-K Variables Corresponding to Constructs and Covariates Construct ECLS-K Variables ECLS-K Data Source 1. Parent Depression* a. Parent report of symptoms of depression (Kindergarten Spring) a. Parent Interview Questionnaire 2. Parent School Involvement* a. Parent report of school involvement (Kindergarten Spring) a. Parent Interview Questionnaire 3. Early Psychosocial Behavior* a. Teacher report on Externalizing Problem Behaviors Scale (Kindergarten Spring) b. Teacher report on Internalizing Problem Behaviors Scale (Kindergarten Spring) c. Teacher report on Interpersonal Scale (Kindergarten Spring) d. Teacher report on Self-Control Scale (Kindergarten Spring) e. Teacher report on Approaches to Learning Scale (Kindergarten Spring) a/b/c/d/e. Teacher SRS 4. Psychosocial Behavior* a. Teacher report on Externalizing Problem a/b/c/d/e. Teacher Behaviors Scale (5th Grade) SRS b. Teacher report on Internalizing Problem Behaviors Scale (5th Grade) c. Teacher report on Interpersonal Scale (5th Grade) d. Teacher report on Self-Control Scale (5th Grade) e. Teacher report on Approaches to Learning Scale (5th Grade) 5. Motivation a. Child report SDQ School scale (5th grade) 80 a. Child SDQ. Teacher SRS Table 7 (cont’d) 6. Early Academic Achievement a. Math T-Score (Kindergarten Spring) b. Reading T-Score (Kindergarten Spring) a/b. Direct Child Cognitive Assessment 7. Later Academic Achievement a. Math T-Score (5th Grade) b. Reading T-Score (5th Grade) a/b. Direct Child Cognitive Assessment a/b/c/d. Parent Interview Questionnaire 8. Covariates a. Gender b. Race c. Age (Kindergarten Spring) d. SES Note: * indicates a latent construct. Confirmatory Factor Analysis was conducted on individual items to identify constructs. Parent Depression. Parent symptoms of depression were measured using information obtained from the parent interview during the spring of kindergarten. The respondent was typically the child’s mother, although if the mother was unavailable, the respondent was another parent, guardian, or household member (in that order of preference). In the current sample, the majority of the parent interviews were completed by biological mothers (n=7,030; 66%) and female guardians such as adoptive mothers, step-mothers, or foster mothers (n= 100; 1%). In each wave of data collection, attempts were made to collect information from the same respondent (e.g., the mother at each data collection point), however in some cases this was not possible and another parent, guardian, or household member served as the respondent. Parents answered the following twelve items about their own psychological well-being using a four-point Likert scale (1=Never, 2=Some of the time, 3=A moderate amount of the time, 4=Most of the time). These items map closely to the criteria for depression listed in the DSM-5 (APA, 2013). - How often during the past week have you felt that you were bothered by things that don’t usually bother you? 81 - How often during the past week have you felt that you did not feel like eating, that your appetite was poor? - How often during the past week have you felt that you could not shake off the blues even with help from your family? - How often during the past week have you felt that you had trouble keeping your mind on what you were doing? - How often during the past week have you felt depressed? - How often during the past week have you felt that everything you did was effortful? - How often during the past week have you felt fearful? - How often during the past week have you felt that your sleep was restless? - How often during the past week have you felt that you talked less than usual? - How often during the past week have you felt lonely? - How often during the past week have you felt sad? - How often during the past week have you felt that you could not get going? Parent responses on these items were used as indicators of their psychological well-being. Since these individual items were provided in the ECLS-K data set and no composites were created, factor analysis was conducted in order to combine items to represent the specific construct of interest. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted using weighted least square parameter estimates (estimator WLSMV) specifying all twelve items representing the construct of parent depression using MPlus student version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012) with the current sample of approximately 10,630 participants. CFA identifies sets of variables that reliably measure a hypothesized construct (Kline, 2011). All the items loaded substantially on the hypothesized factor of parent depression. The model had a close fit with the data. See Table 8 82 for the final factor loadings and their standardized and unstandardized coefficients. Fit indices included a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) equal to .031 (CI = .028-.034), CFI =.957, and TLI=.947. Higher scores on this latent variable are indicative of higher rates of depressive symptomology. Table 8 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Parent Depression Items Unstandardized Standard Error (SE) Felt bothered 1.000 0.000 Poor appetite 1.002 0.040 Felt the “blues” 1.361 0.037 Trouble focusing 1.080 0.031 Felt depressed 1.360 0.034 Felt things were effortful 1.061 0.036 Felt fearful 0.960 0.050 Difficulty sleeping 0.970 0.038 Talked less 1.164 0.041 Felt lonely 1.206 0.039 Felt sad 1.311 0.032 Difficulty “to get going” 1.058 0.035 Note: All factor loadings were significant (p<.001). Standardized 0.612 0.613 0.832 0.661 0.832 0.649 0.587 0.593 0.712 0.737 0.802 0.647 Early Parent School Involvement. Parent school involvement was measured using the parent interview data from the spring of kindergarten. Parents responded to the following eight questions about their involvement in their child’s schooling. Parents responded “1=Yes” or “2=No” to the following items: - During this school year, have you or another adult in your household taken it upon yourself to contact your child’s teacher or school for any reason having to do with your child? - Since the beginning of this school year, have you or the other adults in your household attended an open house or a back-to-school night? 83 - Since the beginning of this school year, have you or the other adults in your household attended a meeting of a PTA, PTO, or Parent-Teacher Student Organization? - Since the beginning of this school year, have you gone to a meeting of a parent advisory group or policy counsel? - Since the beginning of this school year, have you or another adult in your household gone to a regularly-scheduled parent-teacher conference with your child’s teacher or meeting with your child’s teacher? - Since the beginning of this school year, have you or another adult in your household attended a school or class event, such as a play, sports event, or science fair? - Since the beginning of this school year, have you or another adult in your household acted as a volunteer at the school or served on a committee? - Since the beginning of this school year, have you or another adult in your household participated in fundraising for your child’s school? Parent responses on these eight items were used as indicators of early parent involvement in children’s schooling and psychosocial development. Since these individual items were provided in the ECLS-K data set and no composites were created, factor analyses were conducted in order to combine items to represent specific constructs of interest. CFA using weighted least square parameter estimates (estimator WLSMV) specifying all eight items representing the construct of interest using MPlus student version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012) was conducted with the current sample of approximately 10,630. All the items loaded onto the hypothesized construct of parent school involvement. The model fit the data closely. Fit indices included a RMSEA equal to .017 (CI = .013-.022), CFI =.962, and TLI=.947. However, one item, “Contact School,” had a low standardized factor loading of 0.235 and communality below 84 0.1, thus it was removed from the final analysis as it did not significantly contribute to the overall construct. See Table 9 for the final factor loadings and their standardized and unstandardized coefficients. All the items loaded onto the hypothesized construct of parent school involvement. The model fit the data closely. Fit indices included a RMSEA equal to .020 (CI = .015-.025), CFI =.964, and TLI=.946. Higher scores on this latent variable are indicative of less school involvement. Table 9 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Parent School Involvement Items Unstandardized Standard Error (SE) Standardized Attend Open House 1.000 0.000 0.659 Attend PTA/PTO 0.747 0.067 0.492 Attend Parent Advisory Group 0.618 0.060 0.407 Attend Parent-Teacher Conference 0.613 0.072 0.404 Attend School Event 0.872 0.053 0.574 Volunteer at School 1.104 0.060 0.727 Participate in School Fundraising 0.792 0.072 0.522 Note: All factor loadings were significant (p<.001). Academic Achievement. For the current study, early academic achievement is defined as the child’s performance on direct cognitive assessments of mathematics and reading skills in the spring of their kindergarten year. Later academic achievement is defined as the child’s performance on direct cognitive assessments of mathematics and reading skills in the spring of 5th grade. Early academic achievement. In the spring of kindergarten, the full ECLS-K direct child cognitive assessment took approximately 50 to 70 minutes to administer per child in a oneon-one setting. Using a two-stage assessment design, children first completed a routing section, consisting of 12 to 20 items of varying difficulty, to determine the appropriate form for each of the subject areas (reading, mathematics, and general knowledge). The assessment included multiple choice and open-ended items. All items were answered with pointing or verbal 85 responses. The reading assessment included questions to measure print familiarity, letter recognition, beginning and ending sounds, rhyming sounds, word recognition, receptive vocabulary, listening comprehension, and comprehension of words in context. The mathematics assessment included questions to measure number sense, number properties, basic operations, and geometry and spatial sense. Manipulatives and paper and pencil were available from some questions. Children with disabilities with documented Individualized Education Plans (IEP), Individualized Family Service Plans (IFSP), or 504 Plans on file with the school were permitted accommodations during direct assessment. Permitted accommodations included modified setting (e.g., special lighting, quiet room, adaptive chair or table), scheduling testing during a particular time of day or for shorter periods at a time, and assistive devices to improve access to the assessments (e.g., hearing aid, cane, brace, voice synthesizer). Item-response theory (IRT) standardized scores (T-scores) were calculated for each subject area. For this study, the standardized scores for math and reading will be used in the structural model separately (i.e., not combined average or latent factors). Standardized scores have a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. IRT standardized scores allow for comparison of child performance to the population and direct comparisons of academic achievement across time points. The range of IRT standardized scores on the reading assessment and math assessment were 0.0 to 90.0. The reliability for standardized scores was 0.95 for the reading assessment and 0.94 for the math assessment. Later academic achievement. Children were assessed using the ECLS-K direct cognitive assessment covering three subject areas (mathematics, reading, and science). The assessment was created with input from child development, elementary education, and content area experts, and validity of the assessment was established via convergent validity with the 86 Woodcock-McGrew-Werder Mini-Battery of Achievement (MBA) (alpha = 0.73 for reading assessment and 0.80 for math assessment). The content of the assessments extended upon the scales developed for assessments in prior years. For example, in the kindergarten reading assessment, children were asked to read short sentences, whereas in the fifth grade assessment, children were asked to read passages that were more complex. The assessment took approximately 96 minutes to administer and was administered using hard-copy assessments and computer-assisted interviewing. Similar to the kindergarten assessment, a two-stage assessment approach was used to determine which form the student would complete. The routing test included 18 to 25 items, in each subject area. The reading assessment included questions that measured children’s ability to make literal inferences from text (using key words in the text to answer comprehension questions), extrapolate from cues in text to make inferences, use background knowledge and context cues in a sentence to understand homonyms, evaluate connections between problems in narrative and real-life problems, and comprehension of nonfiction text (e.g., biographical and expository text) through questions requiring students to identify the “tone” of the remark, infer the author’s intent behind a character or selection in the text, and identify evidence to support or refute ideas presented in the text. The math assessment included questions that measured conceptual, procedural, and problem-solving knowledge in relation to number sense, numerical operations, measurement, geometry and spatial sense, data analysis, statistics, probability, patterns, algebra, and functions. Item-response theory (IRT) standardized scores (T-scores) were calculated for each subject area. Similar to the early academic achievement variable, the standardized scores for math and reading will be used in the structural model separately (i.e., not combined average or latent factors). The range of IRT standardized scores on the reading assessment and math 87 assessment were 0.0 to 96.0. The reliability for standardized scores was 0.93 for the reading assessment and 0.94 for the math assessment. Psychosocial Behaviors. For the current study, early psychosocial behaviors were measured using teacher’s ratings on a self-administered social rating scale (SRS) in the spring of kindergarten. Later psychosocial behaviors were measured using teacher’s ratings on the SRS in the spring of fifth grade. The SRS is a modified version of the Social Skills Rating System (Graham & Elliott, 1990). The SSRS has been shown to discriminate between students with and without disabilities (Demaray et al., 1995; Bramlett, Smith, & Edmonds, 1994). For each child, their primary teacher in kindergarten and their reading teacher in fifth grade rated the frequency of specific behaviors using a Likert scale from one (Never) to four (Very Often). The SRS included five scales total. The Approaches to Learning (ATL) Scale consisted of six items in the spring of kindergarten and seven items in the spring of fifth grade. The kindergarten items measured the child’s attentiveness, task persistence, eagerness to learn, learning independence, flexibility, and organization. In the spring of fifth grade, a seventh item regarding the child’s ability to follow classroom rules was added. The Self-Control (SC) scale consisted of four items relating to the child’s ability to respect the property rights of others, control their temper, accept peer ideas for group activities, and respond appropriately to peer pressure. The Interpersonal Skills (IP) scale consisted of five items related to the child’s skills in forming and maintaining friendships, getting along with people who are different, comforting or helping other children, expressing feelings, expressing ideas and opinions (and feelings in the kindergarten survey) in a positive way, and showing sensitivity to the feelings of others. The Externalizing Problem Behaviors (EB) scale consisted of five items in the spring of kindergarten and six items in the spring of fifth grade related to the frequency with which a child displayed problem behaviors 88 including, arguing, fighting, getting angry, acting impulsively, and disturbing ongoing activities. In the spring of fifth grade a sixth item regarding the frequency with which a child talks during quiet study was added. Finally, the Internalizing Problem Behavior (IB) scale consisted of four items related to the presence of symptoms of anxiety, loneliness, low self-esteem, and sadness. Mean scores for each scale were reported on a scale from one to four. Higher scores on the ATL, SC, and IP scales indicated more positive behavior, whereas lower scores on the EB and IB scales indicated less problem behavior. Thus, the EB and IB scales were reverse coded, so that higher scores were indicative of less problem behavior and more positive functioning. Thus, higher scores on these latent variables are indicative of more positive psychosocial behavior. The split-half reliability for teacher SRS scale scores in the spring of kindergarten was 0.89 for ATL, 0.80 for SC, 0.89 for IP, 0.90 for EB, and 0.78 for IB. In the spring of fifth grade, split-half reliability for teacher SRS scale scores was 0.91 for ATL, 0.79 for SC, 0.88 for IP, 0.89 for EB, and 0.77 for IB, indicating moderate to high reliability. Kindergarten psychosocial behavior. Although composites were created, factor analyses were conducted in order to combine items to represent the specific construct of interest, psychosocial behaviors. First, Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) specifying all five mean scores for the spring of kindergarten representing the construct of interest was conducted with the current sample using MPlus student version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). The model did not fit the data adequately. Results of an Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) specifying the five mean scores indicated that the score for the Self-Control scale and the Externalizing Behaviors scale loaded on a secondfactor. Based on the factor loadings from the EFA and theoretical considerations, two distinct constructs emerged: learning and social-emotional skills, and maladaptive behaviors. The first 89 construct, learning and social-emotional skills, consisted of the Approaches to Learning, Interpersonal Skills, and Internalizing Behaviors scales. These scales address self-regulated learning (i.e., attentiveness, task persistence, eagerness to learn, learning independence, flexibility, organization) and social-emotional characteristics including relational skills (i.e., forming and maintaining friendships, getting along with people who are different, comforting or helping other children, expressing feelings, expressing feelings, ideas, and opinions in a positive way, and showing sensitivity to the feelings of others) and emotional well-being (i.e., presence of anxiety, loneliness, low self-esteem, and sadness). The second construct, maladaptive behaviors, consisted of the Self-Control and Externalizing Behaviors scale. These scales address emotional regulation (e.g., ability to respect the property rights of others, control their temper, accept peer ideas for group activities, and respond appropriately to peer pressure) and externalizing problems (e.g., arguing, fighting, getting angry, acting impulsively, and disturbing ongoing activities). Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) representing the two constructs of interest was conducted with the current sample using MPlus student version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). All the items loaded substantially onto the hypothesized factors of psychosocial behavior. The model fit the data adequately. Fit indices included a RMSEA equal to .044 (CI = .035-.053), CFI =.987, and TLI=.966. However, the two second-order factors are highly correlated (r =.857). Given the high correlation between the two-second order factors, and problems associated with identification of latent constructs with only two or three observed variables, the error variances between the SelfControl and Externalizing Behaviors scales were correlated. This allowed for examination of a more parsimonious latent variable identified by five observed scores. See Table 10 for the final factor loadings and their standardized and unstandardized coefficients. All the items loaded 90 significantly onto the hypothesized construct of psychosocial behavior. The model fit the data adequately. Fit indices included a RMSEA equal to .044 (CI = .035-.053), CFI =.987, and TLI=.966. Fifth grade psychosocial behavior. Given that there were several different items added to the fifth grade SRS survey, it was hypothesized that all five scales would load onto the hypothesized construct of psychosocial behaviors: Approaches to Learning, Self-Control, Interpersonal Skills, Externalizing Behavior, and Internalizing Behavior. Theoretically, the Approaches to Learning scale consists of items that may closely align with the construct of motivation. For example, task persistence, learning independence, eagerness to learn, and executive functioning skills such as organization, are all related to aspects of self-regulated learning and intrinsic motivation. Since additional data was available to examine motivation as a separate construct at the fifth grade time point, the ATL scale score was examined in context of that construct of motivation, as well as the construct of psychosocial behavior to establish discriminative validity and ensure that it would be most appropriate to include the ATL variable within the construct of psychosocial behavior. It should be noted that this procedure was not done at the kindergarten time point. For the kindergarten construct of psychosocial behavior, the ATL scale was included. This was done for several reasons. First, unlike the fifth grade data, there is no self-report data of student’s motivation in kindergarten. Thus, a separate construct for motivation in kindergarten was not examined within the model. Second, although some of the items comprising the SRS, and in particular the ATL scale, were similar in kindergarten and fifth grade, there were additional items in fifth grade. The differential information collected at the two time points contributes to different conceptualizations and combinations of the scales. Thus, in 91 kindergarten, the ATL scale was examined within the broader construct of psychosocial behavior in order to capture important information related to prosocial and motivation behaviors. To examine the fit of the ATL scale in fifth grade, correlations were examined first. The correlations between the ATL scale and six observed motivation variables (see Motivation section below for more detailed explanation) were compared to the correlations between the ATL scale and the remaining four observed psychosocial variables (SC, IP, EB, IB). ATL was more strongly correlated with the four observed psychosocial variables (values of r ranged from .38 to .72) than with the six observed motivation variables (values of r ranged from .13 to .42). Next an exploratory factor analysis specifying all 11 variables (five psychosocial variables and six motivation variables) was conducted. The correlation between two theoretical second-order factors, psychosocial behavior and motivation, was low (r=.24) indicating a shared variance of only 5%. Additionally, although the ATL scale loaded significantly (p<.05) on both second-order factors, ATL loaded much higher on the psychosocial factor (standardized factor loading = 0.768) than on the motivation factor (standardized factor loading = 0.121). Thus, it was concluded that the ATL scale more appropriately aligns with the construct of psychosocial behavior rather than motivation. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) specifying the five hypothesized SRS mean scores for the spring of fifth grade representing the construct of interest was conducted with the current sample using MPlus student version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). See Table 11 for the final factor loadings and their standardized and unstandardized coefficients. All the items loaded significantly onto the hypothesized construct of psychosocial behavior. The model fit the data adequately. Fit indices included a RMSEA equal to .057 (CI = .049 -.065), CFI =.980, and TLI=.959. 92 Table 10 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Kindergarten Psychosocial Behavior Items Unstandardized Standard Error (SE) Standardized Approaches to Learning 1.000 0.000 0.754 Self-Control 1.002 0.023 0.839 Interpersonal Skills 1.134 0.025 0.919 Externalizing Behavior 0.778 0.030 0.653 Internalizing Behavior 0.389 0.026 0.395 Note: All factor loadings were significant (p<.001). Table 11 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a One-Factor Model of Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior Items Unstandardized Standard Error (SE) Standardized Approaches to Learning 1.000 0.000 0.793 Self-Control 1.009 0.025 0.903 Interpersonal Skills 1.063 0.021 0.898 Externalizing Behavior 0.852 0.026 0.770 Internalizing Behavior 0.421 0.026 0.414 Note: All factor loadings were significant (p<.001). Motivation. Children’s level of motivation in school was measured in 5th grade using a self-report scale, Self-Description Questionnaire (SDQ), and a teacher report of self-regulation and intrinsic motivation from the Social Rating Scale (SRS). For the SDQ, assessors read all questions to children, and took approximately 5 minutes to administer all items. Children answered 42 questions, across six scales, about their perceptions of their competence and interest in peer relations, externalizing behaviors, emotions, and school subjects, including reading and math. The items on the academic and peer scales of the SDQ were adapted from a validated tool, the Self-Description Questionnaire II (Marsh, 1992). The internalizing and externalizing scales were created for the ECLS-K study. The SDQ School scale (reliability α=.83) consists of six items regarding how well children feel they do in all school subjects, and their enjoyment of all school subjects. The SDQ Reading scale (reliability α=.90) and Math scale (reliability α=.92) each consisted of eight items regarding children’s perceptions of their reading grades, difficulty 93 of reading work, and interest in and enjoyment of reading and math, respectively. The SDQ Peer (reliability α=.82) scale consisted of six items regarding children’s perceptions of their popularity, and ability to make and get along with friends/other children. The Anger/Distractibility (Externalizing) scale (reliability α=.78) included six items regarding children’s perceptions of their externalizing problem behaviors including fighting, arguing, talking and disturbing others, and distractibility. The SDQ Sad/Lonely/Anxious (Internalizing) scale (reliability α=.79) consisted of eight items regarding children’s perceptions of internalizing problems including, feeling sad, lonely, ashamed of mistakes, frustrated, and worried about school and friendships. Children rated each item using a four-point response scale: 1=not at all true, 2=a little bit true, 3=mostly true, 4=very true. Mean scores for each scale were reported on a scale from one to four. Higher scores on the School, Reading, Math, and Peer scales indicated higher levels of motivation, whereas higher scores on the Externalizing and Internalizing scales indicated perception of more problems and lower levels of motivation. The Externalizing and Internalizing scales were reverse coded, so that higher scores were indicative of less problems and higher levels of motivation. Since this study aims to understand children’s motivation in regards to their academics, the three academic scales (Math, Reading, School) were analyzed for underlying commonality. Exploratory Factor Analyses and Confirmatory Factor Analyses were run on the three academic scales and then on all six scales, however no theoretically meaningful combination of scales arose as significant and adequately fitting the data (e.g., one factor of the three academic scales, two factors of the three academic scales and the three psychosocial scales, or all six scales on one construct of motivation). For the current study, children’s mean score the SDQ School scale comprised the construct of motivation. The School scale was chosen over the Math and Reading 94 scales, because the goal of the study is to examine how early parent variables, academic performance, and psychosocial behaviors influence a child’s motivation in school broadly, and the items within the School scale subsume math and reading. Moreover, it is likely that children who struggle in reading and/or math will have lower motivation in those subject areas, however, the effects of academic underperformance in math or reading on general school motivation may be more nuanced. Covariates. The child’s age at assessment (Spring of Kindergarten), socioeconomic status (SES), gender, and race were included in the model to control for their differential influence on parent depression, parent school involvement, child academic achievement, motivation, and psychosocial behaviors. Grade retention was not used as a control variable for two reasons. First, the primary goal of this study is to examine relations between early parenting variables and academic and psychosocial outcomes in children with and without LD, not the effects of grade retention on these outcomes. Second, research has established that children who are retained in grade school have a higher likelihood of academic failure and more psychosocial problems (Cortiella & Horowitz, 2014), thus controlling for retention may remove important variance in the outcomes for children with academic underachievement including children with LD. Finally, a small percentage of students (n= 910; 8.6%) in the study sample were ever retained in grade, thus, the effects of retention are likely minimal within such a large sample. Age at assessment. A composite of the child’s age at the time of assessment in the fall of their kindergarten year was included. This composite was derived by determining the number of days between the child’s assessment date and date of birth, and dividing by 30 to obtain age in months. Given that children may start kindergarten at different ages (e.g., five or six), it is 95 important to control for age in order to remove variance related to age-related maturity or influences of age on the variables of interest. Gender. A composite of the child’s gender was included based on information from the kindergarten fall parent interview. Given that some studies have found gender differences in academic achievement (Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; Matthews, Ponitz, & Morrison, 2009; Nowell & Hedges, 1998; Weaver-Hightower, 2003), motivation and learning behaviors in school (Cvencek, Meltzoff, & Greenwald, 2011; McGeown, Goodwin, Henderson, & Wright, 2011; Silverman, 2003), externalizing problems and internalizing problems (Chaplin & Aldao, 2013; Leadbeater, Kuperminc, Blatt, & Hertzog, 1999), and interpersonal skills (Crombie, 1988), it is important to control for these potential influences within the current study. Additionally, some studies show that female students are more at-risk for math disabilities than their male peers (Fryer & Levitt, 2010; Jordan, Kaplan, Nabor Olah, & Locuniak, 2006) whereas other studies show no gender differences (Lachance & Mazzocco, 2006). Other research indicates that boys are four to five times more likely to be diagnosed with an LD (Shapiro, Church, & Lewis, 2002). Therefore, it is important to control for gender effects that may arise due to disproportional identification of LD. Race-ethnicity. A composite of the child’s race-ethnicity was included based on information gathered through the parent interview. This composite included the following categories: White, non-Hispanic; Black or African-American, non-Hispanic; Hispanic, race specified; Hispanic, no race specified; Asian; Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander; Native American or Alaskan Native; and more than one race specified, non-Hispanic. Given that some studies show racial differences in academic achievement (Blackorby & Wagner, 2005; Chatterji, 2006) it is important to control for these potential influences within the study. Additionally, 96 research has indicated that race-ethnicity, along with gender and socioeconomic status, influence the likelihood of developing and being identified as having a learning disability in school (Judge & Bell, 2010), thus it is important to understand how these factors operate within the proposed model, and account for these influences. Socioeconomic status. Socioeconomic status (SES) is a measure of social standing. Given that research has shown negative effects of poverty on academic achievement (Chatterji, 2006; Denton & West, 2002; Lee, Grigg, & Donahue, 2007; Morgan, Farkas, Hillemeier, & Maczuga, 2014), psychosocial problems (McLoyd, 1998), and parent school involvement (Evans, 2004), it is important to consider the influence of SES within the hypothesized model. The SES variable used in the current study was a composite variable of three indicators, income, parent education, and parent occupation, and reflects the family’s SES in the spring of kindergarten. The SES variable is a continuous variable that ranges from -4.75 to 2.75, with higher values corresponding to higher SES. Only the kindergarten SES variable was used in the current study, because it was highly correlated with the SES variable in the spring of fifth grade (unweighted r= .859, weighted r=.839). The specific components of the SES variable include Father/male guardian’s education, Mother/female guardian’s education, Father/male guardian’s occupation, Mother/female guardian’s occupation, and household income. Since not all parents responded to all questions in both rounds, missing values were present for some of the SES indicators (28.2%). Hot deck imputation was used to impute missing values, which involves using the value reported for an item by a respondent that is similar to the non-respondent on other characteristics. A categorical search algorithm, CHAID (Chi-squared Automatic Interaction Detector), was used to analyze and determine the best predictors for respondents and non-respondents. Additionally, for households with one parent, SES was computed by averaging 97 the available components. Each indicator (income, parent education, and parent occupation) was standardized to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. The specific indicators are outlined below. Household income was collected from the parent interview in the spring of kindergarten. Income represents the primary caregiver’s annual reported household income. Parent education was collected from the parent interview during the fall of kindergarten (or spring if the household did not participate in the fall). The respondent parent reported the highest level of education attained. Education level was recoded from 1 to 9 (1 = 8th grade or below; 2 = 9th to 12th grade, 3 = high school diploma/equivalent, 4= vocational/technical program, 5 = some college, 6 = Bachelor’s degree, 7 = graduate/professional school/no degree, 8 = Master’s degree, 9 = Doctorate or professional degree). Parent occupation was collected via the parent interview in the fall of kindergarten. Parent occupation was recoded to reflect the average of the 1989 General Social Survey (GSS; Nakao & Treas, 1992) prestige score of the occupation. This was done by averaging the corresponding prestige score of the 1980 Census occupational category codes covered by the ECLS-K occupation (NCES, 2001). The 22-position scale ranks occupation according to their relative prestige. Data Analyses Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Premium version 24 was used to prepare data and run descriptive and preliminary analyses. MPlus student version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012) was used to model the data. The sample of interest was selected following the application of exclusionary criteria, and variables of interest were imported into SPSS to run preliminary analyses. Analyses were conducted using sample and nesting weights, and compared to results without sample and nesting weights. All data analyses were conducted on a freestanding computer in Erickson Hall at Michigan State University, as required by the 98 restricted-use license agreement from the IES. Investigators obtained approval for use of the ECLS-K data set for this study by the Institute of Education Sciences (IES) and Michigan State University Institutional Review Board (IRB; see Appendix A). Preliminary Analyses. Prior to testing the structural equation model, preliminary analyses were conducted. Data screening (e.g., plots) and descriptive statistics (see Table 13) using all variables of interest were conducted to better understand the ECLS-K population and constructs of interest. Correlation matrices (see Table 18 and Table 19) for all variables of interest were run to screen for multicollinearity and the distribution of variables were examined using graphs to address potential problems with outliers and account for skewed distribution. No study variables were highly correlated (r >.85), indicating no potential problems with multicollinearity (Kline, 2011). Outliers were included in the study given that the research questions aim to examine group differences among children with LD and it was expected that some parents and children would fall in the extreme ends of the spectrum on measures of depression, academic performance, and psychosocial behaviors. Additional preliminary analyses were conducted without latent variable modeling prior to testing the structural equation model. Means and standard deviations of continuous variables (see Table 14 and Table 16) and frequencies and percentages of categorical variables (see Table 15 and Table 17) were examined, with and without weights. Independent samples t-test were conducted to determine whether there were significant differences on mean scores for all continuous variables of interest between the LD and non-LD groups. Chi-square test of independence was conducted to determine whether there were significant differences on mean scores for all categorical variables of interest between the LD and non-LD groups. 99 Structural Equation Analyses. Prior to testing the proposed conceptual model, the proposed latent factor structure was tested using Confirmatory Factor Analysis using MPlus student version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). Brown (2015) recommends testing the measurement model for each construct separately prior to testing the full structural model. The five specified latent variables in the model are described under the variables and measures section above. Each latent variable is measured by several observed variables or indicators, and each indicator loaded on one specified factor. The latent factors included parent depression, parent school involvement, early psychosocial behaviors, and fifth grade psychosocial behaviors using the full study sample (n=10,630). The four latent factors in conjunction with five additional observed variables compose the measurement model. Given that the full structural model will be tested with different groups, group invariance testing was conducted to determine whether the model measures similar constructs across groups (e.g., children with LD and children without LD; children with MD, RD, MD-RD, and no LD). Group invariance was tested by examining the difference between the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) for configural, metric, and scalar invariance tests for each latent variable. Cheung & Rensvold (2002) recommend a change in CFI between configural, metric, and scalar invariances that is equal or less than .01 to indicate adequate fit across groups. For parent depression, results indicated adequate fit for LD and no-LD groups (CFI=.968 for configural model, CFI=.971 for metric model, CFI=.980 for scalar model, and CFI=.012). For parent involvement, results indicated adequate fit for LD and no-LD groups (CFI=.968 for configural model, CFI=.967 for scalar model, and CFI=-.001; metric model is not available with categorical data). For kindergarten psychosocial behavior, results indicated adequate fit for LD and no-LD groups (CFI=.987 for configural model, CFI=.985 for metric model, CFI=.977 for scalar model, and 100 CFI=-.01). For fifth grade psychosocial behavior, results indicated adequate fit for LD and noLD groups (CFI=.975 for configural model, CFI=.972 for metric model, CFI=.968 for scalar model, and CFI=-.007). Overall, all latent variables passed tests for group invariance. The proposed model was tested using latent variable structural equation modeling (SEM) techniques that allow for unobserved latent variables in the model to be tested in relation to the hypotheses developed using the ECLS-K non-experimental data. Latent variable SEM allows for simultaneous confirmatory factor analysis of all latent variables and the magnitude of the relations between constructs (Kline, 2011). First, it was hypothesized that lower levels of parent depression will indirectly predict better early academic achievement and early psychosocial behavior in kindergarten, via higher parent involvement (research question 1a). Second, it was hypothesized that higher levels of parent school involvement will predict higher academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade dependent on levels of early academic achievement and early psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten (research question 1b). Third, it was hypothesized that increased early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten will positively predict academic achievement in fifth grade, where early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors are accounted for (research question 1c). Fourth, it was hypothesized that higher motivation and psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade will positively predict academic achievement in fifth grade (research question 1d). Next, multiple-group SEM was explored to answer the research questions pertaining to group differences in the specified relationships of the conceptual model. First, children with an LD were compared to children without an LD by simultaneously fitting the model to both groups to determine whether the magnitude of relations between parent depression and school involvement and child academic, psychosocial, and motivational outcomes differ. It was 101 hypothesized that parent depression and school involvement in kindergarten would be more strongly predictive of motivation, psychosocial behaviors, and academic achievement in children with LD compared to children without LD (research question 2). Thus, low levels of parent depression and increased parent involvement were hypothesized to serve as greater protective factors for children who are academically underachieving compared to children who are not. Finally, in order to determine whether subtype of LD moderates the effects of the conceptual model, a three group SEM was explored by comparing children with RD, children with RD-MD, and children without LD. Children with MD cannot be included as an independent subgroup in this analysis due to inadequate sample size. However, given that children with MD often experience co-morbid reading problems, regardless of whether they are identified for services through a formal identification process, this study will group children with MD and children with RD-MD for the proposed multi-group analysis (Research Question 3). Significant findings were interpreted with caution, as this was a limitation of the study. Moreover, this analysis was exploratory in nature and may be under-identified given that the sample sizes between the categorical groups are uneven (e.g., n=20 MD, n=180 RD, n=220 RD-MD, n=10,220 non-LD). It was hypothesized that the magnitude of relations between parent depression and school involvement on academic achievement, psychosocial behaviors, and motivation outcomes would be strongest for children with combined LD (e.g., RD-MD), then children with one LD, then children without LD (research question 3); thereby keeping in line with the hypothesis that low levels of parent depression and high levels of parent school involvement serve as a greater protective factor for children who are most academically at-risk (e.g., RD-MD group). In order to compare the groups, it was proposed that cross-group equality were used to derive unstandardized estimates of the parameters in all groups (Kline, 2011). Then, 102 the parameters would be freely estimated across all groups and a comparison between the constrained and unconstrained (e.g., freely estimated) model fit would be examined using absolute and relative model fit statistics (Kline, 2011). This comparison would determine whether the fit of the model worsened under constrained parameters which would indicate group differences (e.g., unequal direct effects). Overall, multiple-group SEM was used to determine whether parent depression and parent involvement have a different effect on outcomes for children with subtypes of LD (i.e., RD, RD-MD) and without LD. Weighted least squares and maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors were used to estimate model parameters. Table 12 Summary of Research Questions, Variables, and Analyses Research Analyses Variables Question 1. Model Fit Latent Variable Structural Equation Modeling 2/3. Model Fit Multiplefor Group SEM Subsamples of Learning Disabilities Covariates Parent Depression* Early Parent School Involvement* Early Academic Achievement Early Psychosocial Behavior* Motivation Psychosocial Behavior* Academic Achievement Gender Race SES Age Parent Depression* Early Parent School Involvement* Early Academic Achievement Early Psychosocial Behavior* Motivation Psychosocial Behavior* Academic Achievement Gender Race SES Age *Note: See Table 7 for specific indicators corresponding with latent variables. 103 Model Fit. To determine model fit, absolute and relative fit indices were used. The chisquare statistic, an absolute fit index, was examined in analyses comparing mediation paths. A non-significant chi-square index (p > 0.01) signifies that the null hypothesis is not rejected, where the null hypothesis is a perfect fit in the population. Thus, a non-significant chi-square statistic indicates an adequate model fit. However, the chi-square statistic is affected by sample size, which may lead to rejection of the null hypothesis even when there are differences between the observed and predicted covariances (Kline, 2011). Thus, additional fit indices were considered due to the study’s large sample size. Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Fit Index (TLI) are relative fit indices which both account for sample size (Kline, 2011). The CFI and TLI values greater than or equal to 0.95 indicate an adequate fit (Hooper, Coughlan, & Mullen, 2008). Finally, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) was used as an indicator of absolute fit. The RMSEA provides an estimation of the model with optimal parameter estimates fits the covariance matrix for the population. An RMSEA value less than or equal to 0.07 and with a 90% confidence interval with an upper limit less than or equal to 0.08 is considered an adequate model fit (Hooper, Coughlan, & Mullen, 2008). For multiple-group SEM analyzing mediation paths, chi-square significance testing was conducted by calculating the chisquare difference statistic between the constrained and unconstrained models. If significant, this suggested the model parameters are unequal across groups. 104 Table 13 Descriptive Statistics for Full Sample Variables Mean Covariates Kindergarten SES .04 Age at Assessment 68.41 (K, 1999) Age at Assessment 134.54 th (5 grade, 2004) Parent Depression Felt bothered 1.59 Poor appetite 1.38 Felt the “blues” 1.25 Trouble focusing 1.51 Felt depressed 1.33 Felt things were effortful 1.62 Felt fearful 1.19 Difficulty sleeping 1.73 Talked less 1.25 Felt lonely 1.25 Felt sad 1.44 Difficulty “to get going” 1.51 Parent School Involvement Contact school 1.48 Attended open house 1.24 Attended PTO 1.63 Attended parent group 1.91 Attended teacher conference 1.14 Attended school event 1.31 Volunteered at school 1.48 Participated in fundraiser 1.38 Kindergarten Achievement Direct Math Assessment 51.63 Direct Reading Assessment 51.56 Fifth Grade Achievement Direct Math Assessment 51.54 Direct Reading Assessment 51.46 Early Psychosocial Behaviors Teacher-Rated Approaches 3.20 to Learning 3.25 Teacher-Rated Self-Control Teacher-Rated Interpersonal 3.19 Skills 1.58 Teacher-Rated Externalizing S.D. Range Weighted Mean Weighted S.D. .80 4.32 -4.75-2.75 45.77-96.30 -.03 68.31 .78 4.29 4.38 110.87-153.53 134.53 4.38 .75 .71 .59 .72 .64 .88 .52 .90 .58 .58 .65 .72 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-4 1.60 1.42 1.27 1.55 1.36 1.70 1.21 1.78 1.27 1.27 1.47 1.55 .74 .74 .61 .76 .69 .95 .54 .93 .63 .63 .68 .75 .50 .43 .48 .29 .35 .46 .50 .49 1-2 1-2 1-2 1-2 1-2 1-2 1-2 1-2 1.46 1.28 1.66 1.92 1.16 1.34 1.52 1.41 .50 .45 .47 .28 .37 .47 .50 .49 9.69 9.58 14.39-85.84 17.08-87.73 50.85 50.99 9.68 9.70 9.52 9.56 21.90-80.61 16.61-81.02 51.11 51.16 9.61 9.52 .65 1-4 3.15 .67 .59 .61 1-4 1-4 3.22 3.15 .61 .62 .58 1-4 1.62 .60 105 Table 13 (cont’d) Teacher-Rated Internalizing Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior Teacher-Rated Approaches to Learning Teacher-Rated Self-Control Teacher-Rated Interpersonal Skills Teacher-Rated Externalizing Teacher-Rated Internalizing Motivation Student-Rated School 1.52 .48 1-4 1.55 .50 3.12 .66 1-4 3.09 .67 3.27 3.12 .58 .62 1-4 1-4 3.25 3.10 .59 .63 1.61 .56 1-4 1.64 .58 1.60 .51 1-4 1.62 .53 2.75 .63 1-4 2.72 .64 106 Table 14 Unweighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples Variables Learning No Learning Disability Disability Independent Samples t-test (df) a a N Mean N Mean (SD) Cohen’s d (SD) Covariates Kindergarten SES 390 -.32 (.71) 9880 .06 (.80) 10.38(428), p<.001*** .50 Age at Assessment (K, 1999) 380 69.18 (4.94) 9330 68.38 (4.30) -3.13(404), p=.002** .17 400 135.23 (5.00) 9790 134.51 (4.35) -2.85(423), p=.005** .15 390 40.81 (9.06) 9930 52.06 (9.47) 23.89(418), p<.001*** 1.21 350 40.32 (7.52) 9500 51.98 (9.40) 28.34(394), p<.001*** 400 41.41 (8.86) 9790 51.95 (9.31) 23.29(435), p<.001*** Age at Assessment (5th grade, 2004) Kindergarten Achievement Direct Math Assessment Direct Reading Assessment Fifth Grade Achievement Direct Math Assessment Direct Reading Assessment Early Psychosocial Behaviors TeacherRated Approaches to Learning TeacherRated SelfControl TeacherRated Interpersonal Skills 1.37 1.16 1.46 390 39.17 (8.15) 9790 51.95 (9.28) 30.33(433), p<.001*** 390 2.56 (.67) 9700 3.23 (.64) 19.21(415), p<.001*** 390 3.03 (.64) 9630 3.26 (.59) 7.06(412), p<.001*** .37 380 2.86 (.65) 9610 3.20 (.61) 9.87(407), p<.001*** .54 107 1.02 Table 14 (cont’d) TeacherRated 380 1.78 (.70) 9660 1.57 (.57) -5.80(403), Externalizing p<.001*** Teacher380 1.71 (.55) 9610 1.51 (.48) Rated -6.97(404), Internalizing p<.001*** Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior Teacher410 2.67 (.61) 9310 3.14 (.65) 15.14(449), Rated p<.001*** Approaches to Learning Teacher400 3.04 (.63) 9230 3.28 (.58) 7.58(434), p<.001*** Rated SelfControl Teacher400 2.84 (.66) 9110 3.13 (.62) 8.95(434), p<.001*** Rated Interpersonal Skills Teacher410 1.80 (.61) 9250 1.60 (.55) -6.28(436), Rated p<.001*** Externalizing Teacher410 1.79 (.54) 9150 1.59 (.51) -7.29(437), Rated p<.001*** Internalizing Motivation Student400 2.56 (.69) 9790 2.76 (.62) 5.70(426), p<.001*** Rated School Note: Equal variance not assumed for all variables. a All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. 108 .34 .39 .75 .40 .45 .34 .38 .30 Table 15 Unweighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples (Categorical Variables) Variables Learning Disability No Learning Disability Chi-Square (df) Na (%) Na (%) Parent Depression Felt bothered 6.36(3), p=.10 Never 190 (47.1%) 5070 (49.6%) Some Time 120 (30.2%) 3360 (32.9%) Moderate Amount 30 (6.3%) 740 (7.3%) Most of Time 20 (4.1%) 240 (2.4%) Poor appetite 13.94(3), p=.003** Never 240 (58.0%) 6850 (67.1%) Some Time 80 (19.8%) 1920 (18.8%) Moderate Amount 20 (5.8%) 370 (3.6%) Most of Time 20 (4.3%) 280 (2.7%) Felt the “blues” 11.64(3), p=.009** Never 270 (65.5%) 7610 (74.5%) Some Time 60 (15.7%) 1400 (13.7%) Moderate Amount 10 (3.4%) 230 (2.3%) Most of Time 10 (2.9%) 160 (1.6%) Trouble focusing 9.69(3), p=.021** Never 210 (50.2%) 5550 (54.3%) Some Time 110 (27.3%) 3180 (31.2%) Moderate Amount 30 (6.3%) 420 (4.2%) Most of Time 20 (3.9%) 260 (2.5%) Felt depressed 8.80(3), p=.032** Never 260 (62.6%) 6950 (68.0%) Some Time 80 (18.4%) 2010 (19.7%) Moderate Amount 20 (4.1%) 230 (2.3%) Most of Time 10 (2.9%) 210 (2.1%) Felt things were 3.05(3), p=.38 effortful 200 (49.3%) 5390 (52.7%) Never 100 (24.9%) 2860 (28.0%) Some Time 20 (5.8%) 480 (4.7%) Moderate Amount 30 (7.5%) 670 (6.6%) Most of Time 11.24(3), p=.01** Felt fearful 310 (74.9%) 8010 (78.4%) Never 30 (8.0%) 1130 (11.1%) Some Time 10 (2.4%) 150 (1.5%) Moderate Amount 10 (2.4%) 120 (1.1%) Most of Time 7.13(3), p=.07 Difficulty sleeping 170 (40.1%) 4700 (46.0%) Never 130 (30.4%) 3290 (32.2%) Some Time 30 (8.2%) 720 (7.0%) Moderate Amount 40 (9.2%) 690 (6.8%) 109 Table 15 (cont’d) Most of Time Talked less Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt lonely Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt sad Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Difficulty “to get going” Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Parent School Involvement Contact school Yes No Attended open house Yes No Attended PTO Yes No Attended parent group Yes No Attended teacher conference Yes No Attended school event Yes No Volunteered at school Yes 13.51(3), p=.004** 280 (68.6%) 60 (13.8%) 10 (1.9%) 10 (3.4%) 7640 (74.8%) 1380 (13.5%) 250 (2.5%) 140 (1.4%) 2.66(3), p=.45 290 (69.3%) 60 (13.8%) 10 (2.7%) 10 (2.2%) 7620 (74.6%) 1420 (13.9%) 200 (1.9%) 160 (1.6%) 3.27(3), p=.35 230 (55.1%) 110 (26.3%) 10 (3.4%) 10 (2.7%) 5910 (57.8%) 3020 (29.5%) 310 (3.1%) 180 (1.7%) 210 (51.4%) 120 (28.7%) 20 (4.6%) 10 (2.9%) 5620 (55.0%) 3090 (30.3%) 420 (4.2%) 270 (2.7%) 210 (51.0%) 160 (39.6%) 4950 (48.5%) 4640 (45.4%) .69(3), p=.88 3.08(1), p=.08 11.40(1), p=.001*** 250 (61.6%) 120 (28.7%) 7260 (71.1%) 2310 (22.7%) 10.29(1), p=.001*** 110 (26.3%) 260 (64.0%) 3580 (35.0%) 6010 (58.8%) 40 (9.2%) 340 (81.4%) 880 (8.6%) 8700 (85.2%) .40(1), p=.53 2.56(1), p=.11 330 (80.4%) 40 (10.1%) 8230 (80.6%) 1350 (13.3%) 230 (55.3%) 140 (35.0%) 6640 (65.0%) 2940 (28.8%) 10.97(1), p=.001*** 16.94(1), p<.001*** 150 (37.2%) 110 4990 (48.8%) Table 15 (cont’d) No 220 (53.1%) 4600 (45.0%) Participated in 17.16(1), p<.001*** fundraiser Yes 190 (46.4%) 5950 (58.2%) No 180 (43.7%) 3630 (35.5%) a Note: All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. 111 Table 16 Weighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples Variables Learning Disability No Learning Disability N Covariates Kindergarten SES Mean (SD) -.44 Age at Assessment (K, 1999) Age at Assessment (5th grade, 2004) Kindergarten Achievement Direct Math Assessment 116,267 69.56 117,019 41.00 8.69 3,128,922 51.22 9.51 Direct Reading Assessment 107,695 39.86 7.35 2,992,525 51.39 9.53 Direct Reading Assessment Early Psychosocial Behaviors TeacherRated Approaches to Learning TeacherRated SelfControl TeacherRated 3,229,963 Mean (SD) 123,453 Fifth Grade Achievement Direct Math Assessment .87 N 5.45 2,995,631 Independent Samples t-test (df) Cohen’s d -.01 .78 68.27 4.23 117,910 135.48 5.44 2,810,864 134.49 4.33 170.59(131,117), p<.001*** .52 -79.95(121,779), p<.001*** .26 -61.60(124,247), p<.001*** .20 393.64(127,748), p<.001*** 1.21 499.98(121,110), p<.001*** 1.35 118,237 40.85 8.71 2,810,503 51.54 9.40 115,974 39.69 7.60 2,804,928 51.63 9.29 412.05(130,101), p<.001*** 1.18 519.08(130,705), p<.001*** 1.41 116,739 2.53 .68 3,019,827 3.17 .66 320.16(125,521), p<.001*** .96 116,783 3.04 .64 3,003,567 3.22 .60 114,804 2.83 .64 2,991,553 3.16 .62 94.50(125,036), p<.001*** .29 172.19(123,251), p<.001*** 112 Table 16 (cont’d) .52 Interpersonal Skills TeacherRated Externalizing TeacherRated Internalizing Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior TeacherRated Approaches to Learning TeacherRated SelfControl TeacherRated Interpersonal Skills TeacherRated Externalizing TeacherRated Internalizing Motivation Student-Rated School 115,625 1.83 .76 3,010,906 1.61 .59 115,557 1.71 .56 2,988,811 1.54 .50 121,416 2.67 .63 2,577,957 3.11 .67 120,939 3.07 .57 2,551,266 3.26 .59 120,671 2.81 .63 2,503,546 3.11 .63 121,233 1.79 .59 2,553,381 1.63 .58 121,416 1.76 .48 2,525,138 1.61 .53 -96.62(121,071), p<.001*** .32 -99.38(122,667), p<.001*** .32 238.27(134,658), p<.001*** .68 116.34(133,427), p<.001*** .33 160.60(132,447), p<.001*** .48 -93.33(132,475), p<.001*** .27 -103.67(135,808), p<.001*** .30 118,237 2.59 .78 2,810,151 Note: Equal variance not assumed for all variables. 113 2.73 .64 59.60(124,952), p<.001*** .20 Table 17 Weighted Descriptive Statistics between LD and Non-LD Samples (Categorical Variables) Variables Learning Disability No Learning Disability Chi-Square N (%) N (%) (df) Parent Depression Felt bothered 1802.44(3), Never 67,928 (56.4%) 1,678,074 (52.9%) p<.001*** Some Time 40,785 (33.8%) 1,149,172 (36.2%) Moderate Amount 7,557 (6.3%) 270,662 (8.5%) Most of Time 4227(3.5%) 74,467 (2.3%) Poor appetite 8182.52(3), Never 76,606 (63.6%) 2,240,349 (70.5%) p<.001*** Some Time 26,806 (22.2%) 680,528 (21.4%) Moderate Amount 11,686 (9.7%) 139,603 (4.4%) Most of Time 5399 (4.5%) 115,078 (3.6%) Felt the “blues” 3086.84(3), Never 89,679 (74.4%) 2,539,143 (80.0%) p<.001*** Some Time 25,628 (21.3%) 487,456 (15.4%) Moderate Amount 2959 (2.5%) 82,716 (2.6%) Most of Time 2230 (1.9%) 64,737 (2.0%) Trouble focusing 11576.32(3), Never 62,341 (51.7%) 1,849,482 (58.2%) p<.001*** Some Time 38,569 (32.0%) 1,046,561 (32.9%) Moderate Amount 8,629 (7.2%) 171,109 (5.4%) Most of Time 10,958 (9.1%) 110,021 (3.5%) Felt depressed 13355.76(3), Never 85,769 (71.2%) 2,306,057 (72.6%) p<.001*** Some Time 24,381 (20.2%) 695,835 (21.9%) Moderate Amount 8,649 (7.2%) 70,261 (2.2%) Most of Time 1,698 (1.4%) 102,708 (3.2%) Felt things were 1704.75(3), effortful 72,607 (60.4%) 1,730,262 (54.6%) p<.001*** Never 30,606 (25.5%) 961,967 (30.3%) Some Time 5,950 (4.9%) 166,363 (5.2%) Moderate Amount 11,058 (9.2%) 312,837 (9.9%) Most of Time 24168.65(3), Felt fearful 98,819 (82.0%) 2,677,019 (84.2%) p<.001*** Never 11,696 (9.7%) 403,675 (12.7%) Some Time 1,723 (1.4%) 54,854 (1.7%) Moderate Amount 8,259 (6.9%) 42,146 (1.3%) Most of Time Difficulty sleeping 3906.93(3), Never 55,487 (46.0%) 1,520,760 (47.9%) p<.001*** Some Time 44,091 (36.6%) 1,109,936 (34.9%) Moderate Amount 5,821 (4.8%) 269,732 (8.5%) 114 Table 17 (cont’d) Most of Time Talked less Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt lonely Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt sad Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Difficulty “to get going” Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Parent School Involvement Contact school Yes No Attended open house Yes No 15,097 (12.5%) 277,753 (8.7%) 90,992 (75.5%) 16,565 (13.7%) 4,911 (4.1%) 8,029 (6.7%) 2,550,184 (80.2%) 469,415 (14.8%) 96,061 (3.0%) 63,820 (2.0%) 91,892 (76.3%) 21,557 (17.9%) 5,424 (4.5%) 1,624 (1.3%) 2,549,165 (80.3%) 471,399 (14.8%) 78,789 (2.5%) 76,233 (2.4%) 75,437 (62.8%) 37,218 (31.0%) 5,480 (4.6%) 2,007 (1.7%) 1,950,599 (61.4%) 1,038,623 (32.7%) 115,544 (3.6%) 71,723 (2.3%) 63,205 (52.5%) 47,986 (39.8%) 5,360 (4.4%) 3,946 (3.3%) 1,832,568 (57.7%) 1,073,920 (33.8%) 152,764 (4.8%) 116,180 (3.7%) 69,276 (56.3%) 53,769 (43.7%) 72,137 (59.1%) 49,874 (40.9%) 1,741,317 (53.9%) 1,488,311(46.1%) 12382.25(3), p<.001*** 3352.69(3), p<.001*** 588.51(3), p<.001*** 1865.93(3), p<.001*** 271.29(1), p<.001*** .91 (.90-.92) 2,348,497 (72.8%) 877,730 (27.2%) 10969.61(1), p<.001*** 1.85 (1.831.87) Attended PTO Yes No 31,396 (25.7%) 90,615 (74.3%) 1,111,307 (34.4%) 2,116,551 (65.6%) 3956.06(1), p<.001*** 1.52 (1.501.54) Attended parent group Yes No 9,884 (8.0%) 113,569 (92.0%) 270,813 (8.4%) 2,955,952 (91.6%) 23.13(1), p<.001*** 1.05 (1.031.08) Attended teacher conference Yes 109,988 (89.1%) 115 2,706,336 (83.8%) 2465.32(1), p<.001*** .63 (.62-.65) Table 17 (cont’d) No Attended school event Yes No Volunteered at school Yes No Participated in fundraiser Yes No 13,464 (10.9%) 522,551 (16.2%) 86,347 (69.9%) 37,105 (30.1%) 2,138,755 (66.2%) 1,090,376 (33.8%) 47,701 (38.6%) 75,752 (61.4%) 60,403 (48.9%) 63,050 (51.1%) 116 1,559,005 (48.3%) 1,670,182 (51.7%) 1,924,517 (59.7%) 1,301,651 (40.3%) 733.50(1), p<.001*** .84 (.83-.85) 4427.23(1), p<.001*** 1.48 (1.471.50) 5665.41(1), p<.001*** 1.54 (1.631.56) Table 18 Correlation Matrix for LD (upper) and Non-LD (lower) Sample Part 1 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 1 - -.04 .17 .03 .09 .02 -.03 -.01 .05 .12 .02 .07 -.07 .05 .03 .03 -.09 -.07 .04 -.05 2 .01 - .16 .09 .15 -.06 -.07 -.15 -.32 -.08 -.08 -.17 -.32 -.32 -.24 -.01 -.13 -.12 -.05 -.12 3 .16 .19 - .59 .65 -.46 -.31 .02 -.16 .00 .05 -.09 -.09 -.08 -.06 .07 -.00 -.05 -.01 -.00 4 .06 .13 .64 - .74 -.71 -.25 .02 -.11 .08 -.06 -.10 -.06 -.08 -.01 -.01 -.01 -.00 -.01 .01 5 .07 .15 .68 .79 - -.54 -.33 -.00 -.17 -.02 -.06 -.08 -.16 -.15 -.13 .11 -.04 -.00 .02 .02 6 -.03 -.09 -.49 -.70 -.57 - .29 -.06 .15 .05 .06 .09 .04 .10 .01 -.06 -.04 -.10 -.04 -.10 7 -.05 -.09 -.37 -.28 -.34 .26 - -.12 .18 .04 -.03 .05 .10 .15 .02 -.07 -.04 -.05 -.01 -.04 8 -.01 -.11 .06 .07 .06 -.09 -.07 - .09 -.02 .06 .06 .22 .08 .12 -.05 .02 .01 -.01 .08 9 -.03 -.25 -.10 -.07 -.09 .06 .05 .08 - .24 .10 .36 .31 .35 .23 .01 .03 .05 .09 .12 10 .00 -.12 -.10 -.06 -.089 .06 .06 .04 .18 - .31 .08 .11 .26 .16 .04 -.02 -.03 .00 .05 11 -.02 -.09 -.03 -.02 -.02 .02 .02 .07 .08 .17 - .09 .10 .17 .08 -.04 .02 -.01 .02 .06 12 .00 -.16 -.03 -.02 -.02 .01 .02 .05 .16 .05 .04 - .29 .21 .07 .01 .06 .05 -.00 -.04 13 -.04 -.27 -.10 -.07 -.08 .04 .04 .11 .26 .12 .06 .14 - .32 .22 -.02 .10 .12 -.07 .09 14 -.03 -.35 -.15 -.12 -.14 .09 .08 .11 .26 .21 .12 .16 .30 - .11 .05 .06 .04 .01 .02 15 -.02 -.26 -.12 -.07 -.10 .06 .06 .08 .20 .19 .07 .09 .21 .28 - .04 .01 .07 -.01 .06 16 .02 -.05 -.02 -.03 -.02 .04 .02 -.06 .00 .02 .01 .01 .01 .02 .01 - .34 .41 .38 .37 17 .01 -.17 -.06 -.05 -.05 .05 .06 -.04 .07 .03 .02 .05 .05 .07 .04 .30 - .39 .33 .31 18 -.01 -.14 -.05 -.06 -.06 .06 .03 -.03 .06 .03 .00 .06 .06 .07 .05 .38 .38 - .34 .61 19 .03 -.04 -.01 -.03 -.02 .03 .02 -.10 -.01 .01 -.00 .01 -.01 -.01 -.01 .37 .29 .41 - .41 20 -.00 -.16 -.05 -.04 -.05 .04 .04 -.02 .07 .03 .00 .06 .07 .07 .07 .38 .33 .60 .39 - 21 .01 -.15 -.07 -.07 -.06 .07 .03 -.04 .05 .02 .02 .05 .04 .08 .02 .31 .30 .35 .33 .35 22 .02 -.07 -.01 -.03 -.02 .03 .02 -.03 .03 -.01 -.01 .01 .04 .03 .05 .24 .20 .31 .27 .34 23 .03 -.03 -.02 -.04 -.03 .04 .03 -.08 .00 .01 .01 .00 -.02 -.01 .00 .27 .29 .29 .34 .30 117 Table 18 (cont’d) 24 -.01 -.11 -.04 25 -.01 -.14 -.06 26 .00 -.06 27 .03 28 -.05 -.06 .06 .02 -.03 .04 .02 .01 .05 .06 .06 .05 .31 .35 .40 .32 .41 -.06 -.07 .06 .05 -.04 .06 .02 .00 .05 .05 .07 .07 .28 .27 .45 .32 .50 -.03 -.05 -.05 .05 .03 -.07 .03 -.00 -.02 .02 .00 .01 .00 .37 .30 .50 .38 .58 -.10 -.04 -.04 -.04 .05 .03 -.06 -.00 .01 .01 .03 -.00 .03 .01 .29 .29 .36 .39 .36 .16 .37 .42 .20 .23 -.15 -.17 -.03 -.16 -.11 -.03 -.05 -.15 -.22 -.15 -.04 -.11 -.09 -.03 -.10 29 .23 .42 .42 .20 .23 -.15 -.19 -.05 -.20 -.10 -.05 -.08 -.20 -.25 -.18 -.05 -.12 -.10 -.02 -.12 30 .10 .46 .36 .20 .22 -.15 -.14 -.07 -.20 -.10 -.06 -.10 -.20 -.26 -.20 -.05 -.13 -.11 -.00 -.11 31 .08 .43 .37 .20 .22 -.16 -.17 -.05 -.18 -.09 -.05 -.11 -.16 -.24 -.15 -.05 -.12 -.09 -.02 -.10 32 -.01 .08 .17 .08 .10 -.10 -.07 .02 -.05 -.04 -.01 -.03 -.02 -.05 -.03 -.04 -.04 -.06 -.04 -.05 33 .02 .20 .39 .31 .30 -.33 -.13 .04 -.09 -.07 -.02 -.03 -.07 -.14 -.09 -.04 -.09 -.08 -.04 -.08 34 -.01 .16 .26 .31 .27 -.36 -.08 .05 -.08 -.04 -.03 -.03 -.05 -.10 -.06 -.05 -.08 -.07 -.05 -.06 35 -.01 .17 .27 .30 .29 -.34 -.11 .04 -.08 -.05 -.02 -.02 -.07 -.12 -.07 -.04 -.08 -.07 -.04 -.05 36 .03 -.14 -.27 -.33 -.28 .42 .05 -.07 .05 .03 .02 .02 .04 .09 .06 .03 .06 .08 .05 .06 37 -.01 -.12 -.21 -.14 -.15 .13 .19 -.03 .06 .05 .01 .01 .05 .08 .04 .02 .06 .04 .02 .03 Note. 1=age, 2=SES, 3=K teacher ATL, 4=K teacher SC, 5= K teacher interpersonal, 6=K teacher externalizing, 7= K teacher internalizing, 8=parent contact, 9=parent open house, 10=parent PTA, 11=parent advisory group, 12=parent-teacher conference, 13=parent school event, 14=parent volunteer, 15=parent fundraising, 16=parent bothered, 17=parent appetite, 18=parent blues, 19=parent trouble focusing, 20=parent depressed, 21=parent things effortful, 22=parent fearful, 23=parent sleep restless, 24=parent talk less, 25=parent lonely, 26=parent sad, 27=parent “not get going,” 28=K reading t-score, 29=K math t-score, 30=5th grade reading t-score, 31=5th grade math t-score, 32= motivation school, 33= 5th teacher ATL, 34=5th teacher SC, 35=5th teacher interpersonal, 36=5th teacher externalizing, 37=5th teacher internalizing 118 Table 19 Correlation Matrix for LD (upper) and Non-LD (lower) Sample Part 2 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 1 .01 .01 -.09 -.07 .06 .01 .17 .31 .06 2 - -.18 -.13 -.03 -.11 .25 .28 .14 .21 .04 3 - -.06 -.09 .01 .01 .34 .39 .03 .03 .03 4 - -.03 -.11 -.04 -.06 .16 .14 .11 .01 .08 5 .01 -.04 .04 -.04 .20 .20 .07 .00 .05 6 .02 .06 -.00 .01 -.00 -.02 -.09 -.05 .05 7 .01 - -.03 .06 -.03 -.05 -.09 -.10 .08 .04 8 .07 .05 .05 .05 .03 -.05 -.12 -.09 .17 9 .10 .16 .10 .03 .07 .01 -.17 -.19 .02 10 .06 - -.03 -.01 .02 -.03 -.06 -.05 .00 .04 11 .07 .06 - -.02 .04 -.05 .01 .02 .01 .01 12 .05 .10 .12 .03 -.01 .06 -.03 -.07 .00 119 30 .08 31 .03 34 .00 38 -.04 39 .01 40 -.00 41 .04 42 -.05 .37 .32 -.02 .03 .07 .07 -.13 .25 .31 .01 .35 .32 .32 .10 .04 .06 .30 .33 .33 .14 .13 -.00 .29 .33 .33 -.01 .00 -.09 -.38 -.43 -.37 .10 .27 .38 .34 .51 .00 -.08 .04 -.09 -.12 -.15 .10 .25 -.09 -.13 .06 -.02 -.10 -.01 -.02 -.22 -.17 .07 -.01 -.08 -.09 .02 .08 .10 -.05 -.06 -.06 -.04 -.04 .00 .10 .03 .01 -.01 -.05 -.06 -.04 .05 -.06 -.16 -.10 -.07 .00 -.07 -.04 .07 -.00 -.18 -.16 -.13 .22 .14 Table 19 (cont’d) 13 .12 .09 .08 .07 .13 .04 .00 -.06 -.18 -.21 -.13 -.02 -.04 -.05 .00 .06 .01 .04 .01 .02 .02 -.15 -.15 -.18 -.19 .01 -.05 -.09 -.08 .10 .12 .00 .08 -.01 -.02 -.10 -.09 -.17 -.13 .01 -.04 -.05 -.04 .01 .32 .36 .47 .32 .31 .60 .42 .32 .57 .36 .28 .38 .05 -.02 -.03 .01 -.07 -.08 -.05 -.03 -.06 -.04 -.02 -.06 -.14 -.06 -.12 -.07 -.05 .02 -.11 -.08 -.02 .03 .00 .07 .03 .03 -.02 -.04 -.03 -.03 .02 -.00 -.04 .02 -.00 -.05 -.02 14 .11 .05 15 .10 16 17 18 .06 .29 .30 .34 .24 .32 .35 .05 .08 .29 .37 .30 19 20 .27 .32 .26 .33 .30 .36 .37 .44 .36 .60 .42 .58 .28 .32 -.01 -.03 .07 -.02 .078 .07 -.06 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 .22 .30 .30 .28 .29 .37 .12 .12 .29 .21 .28 .33 .35 .21 .04 .05 .25 .23 .31 .26 .31 .36 .04 .01 .34 .30 .34 .41 .38 .34 -.08 .26 .33 .27 .50 .52 .30 -.10 .32 .39 .32 .43 .50 .32 -.05 .37 .18 .41 .36 .35 .29 -.05 -.06 -.05 -.01 -.06 -.00 .06 .03 - -.07 -.10 .03 -.05 -.06 .04 .03 .65 -.13 -.14 -.07 -.08 -.01 -.01 .02 .39 -.09 -.11 -.05 -.06 -.00 -.03 .02 .32 -.01 .04 .04 -.03 -.04 -.04 .01 -.04 -.07 -.13 -.06 -.10 -.07 -.12 -.08 .10 -.09 -.10 .00 -.08 -.08 -.08 -.01 .15 -.08 -.16 -.05 -.06 -.06 -.08 -.03 .13 -.08 -.11 -.04 -.04 .73 - .49 .60 -.20 .11 .15 .14 29 120 .02 .00 .16 .10 .09 .09 .05 .10 .02 .13 .10 .06 .06 -.00 .04 .00 .07 .08 .03 .08 .04 .04 -.02 -.14 -.09 Table 19 (cont’d) 30 .13 .06 31 .12 .04 32 .04 .03 33 .09 .03 34 .09 .02 35 .08 .02 36 .08 .03 37 .04 .01 .01 .02 .04 .04 .02 .02 .02 .02 -.10 -.10 -.02 -.05 .61 .64 - .59 -.19 .12 .18 .13 -.09 -.10 -.02 -.06 .56 .71 .72 - -.17 .13 .10 .11 -.05 -.05 -.03 -.05 .15 .13 .17 .18 - .10 .04 .08 -.06 -.07 -.05 -.07 .29 .29 .37 .35 .29 - .66 .68 -.05 -.07 -.04 -.06 .17 .16 .22 .21 .16 .68 - .78 -.05 -.06 -.03 -.06 .18 .18 .24 .21 .20 .71 .80 .06 .02 .07 .04 .05 .02 .06 .04 -.15 -.15 -.13 -.20 -.20 -.18 -.18 -.20 -.15 -.14 -.59 -.38 -.70 -.30 -.61 -.35 .16 .06 .02 .57 .73 .61 .28 -.11 -.10 -.03 -.21 -.30 -.38 .27 - Note. 1=age, 2=SES, 3=K teacher ATL, 4=K teacher SC, 5= K teacher interpersonal, 6=K teacher externalizing, 7= K teacher internalizing, 8=parent contact, 9=parent open house, 10=parent PTA, 11=parent advisory group, 12=parent-teacher conference, 13=parent school event, 14=parent volunteer, 15=parent fundraising, 16=parent bothered, 17=parent appetite, 18=parent blues, 19=parent trouble focusing, 20=parent depressed, 21=parent things effortful, 22=parent fearful, 23=parent sleep restless, 24=parent talk less, 25=parent lonely, 26=parent sad, 27=parent “not get going,” 28=K reading t-score, 29=K math t-score, 30=5th grade reading t-score, 31=5th grade math t-score, 32= motivation school, 33= 5th teacher ATL, 34=5th teacher SC, 35=5th teacher interpersonal, 36=5th teacher externalizing, 37=5th teacher internalizing 121 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS Preliminary Analyses Group means and standard deviations of predictor, covariate, and outcome variables for the study sample are displayed in Table 14, 15, 16, and 17. Differences between LD and non-LD Groups. Based on independent samples t-tests, all predictor, covariate, and outcome variables were significantly different across samples with and without LD. However, due to a large sample size and unequal sample sizes across groups, effect sizes (Cohen’s d) and odds ratios were analyzed to determine meaningful statistical significance between groups. In terms of covariates, a small effect size was noted for age at kindergarten between the LD and non-LD group, indicating that children with an LD diagnosis were approximately 24 days older than their peers without LD. A medium effect size was noted for family SES, indicating that children with LD came from families with lower SES than their non-LD peers. In terms of predictor variables, such as parent depression symptoms, children with LD had a significantly higher likelihood (odds ratio = 1.84) of having a parent endorse having a poor appetite “most of the time” compared to “never” having a poor appetite than children without LD. Children with LD were 2.11 times more likely to have a parent endorse feeling the “blues” “most of the time” compared to “never” feeling the blues than children without LD. Children with LD had a significantly higher likelihood (odds ratio = 1.65) of having a parent endorse trouble focusing “most of the time” compared to “never” having trouble focusing as compared to parents of children without LD. Children with LD had a significantly higher likelihood (odds ratio = 1.50) of having a parent endorse feeling depressed “most of the time” compared to 122 “never” feeling depressed than parents of children without LD. Children with LD were 2.21 times more likely to have a parent endorse feeling fearful “most of the time” compared to “never” feeling fearful than children without LD. Children with LD had a significantly higher likelihood (odds ratio = 2.70) of having a parent report talking less “most of the time” compared to “never” talking less than children without LD. In regards to parent school involvement indicators, children with LD were 1.46 times more likely to have a parent not attend an open house event than children without LD. Children with LD had a significantly higher likelihood (odds ratio = 1.45) of not having a parent attend a PTO meeting than children without LD. Children with LD were 1.55 times more likely to have a parent not volunteer at school than children without LD. Children with LD had a significantly higher likelihood (odds ratio = 1.54 ) of not having a parent participate in fundraisers at school than children without LD. In terms of outcome variables, children with LD had significantly lower academic achievement scores in reading and math in kindergarten and in fifth grade compared to their peers without LD. The effect sizes for difference in academic achievement scores was large (ranging from 1.16 to 1.46). In regards to early psychosocial behavior in kindergarten, children with LD had lower scores as rated by their teachers on the approaches to learning scale, selfcontrol scale, interpersonal skills scale, and externalizing and internalizing scales. However, the effect size was large for only the approaches to learning scale, medium for the interpersonal skills scale, and small for the remainder of the scales. In regards to psychosocial behavior in fifth grade, children with LD had lower scores as rated by their teachers on all five scales again, however only the approaches to learning scale had a large effect. Finally, children with LD self- 123 reported lower motivation for all school subjects compared to children without LD, however the effect size was small. Differences between Categories of LD. In order to examine the group mean differences in study variables of interest between samples with subtype of LD, a between-group, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted for continuous variables: academic achievement, psychosocial behaviors, and motivation. Since the assumption of homogeneity of variance was not met with regards to academic achievement, psychosocial behavior, or motivation scores, the Welch correction (Welch, 1951) was applied to these ANOVA’s and Games-Howell post-hoc analyses were used. To examine the group mean differences for categorical study variables of interest, parent depression and parent school involvement, the Kruskal-Wallis H Test, a nonparametric equivalent of an ANOVA, was conducted; Dunn-Bonferroni post-hoc analyses were used. Unweighted and weighted subpopulation means and standard deviations are found in Tables 20, 21, 22 and 23. A new variable was created and participants were grouped via learning disability status. Identification of learning disability was based on special education records and report of the child’s main disability category by the child’s fifth grade primary special education teacher. Direct information about the child’s learning disability category (e.g., reading, math, or both) was unavailable, so LD type was determined based on the special education teacher’s report of the child’s primary IEP goals. If the child had primary reading goals, it was assumed that they demonstrated academic difficulty in reading and thus, were categorized as LD-Reading. If the child had primary math goals, it was assumed that they demonstrated academic difficulties in math, and were categorized as LD-math. If the child had primary goals in both reading and math, it was assumed that they had academic difficulties in both subjects and were categorized as LD-co-morbid Math and Reading. Overall, 410 children had a learning disability of which 180 124 children had a RD, 20 children had a MD, and 220 children had a co-morbid RD-MD. Of the 410 children identified with LD, less than 10 children did not have primary goals specified in their IEP, thus they were not included in the categorized analysis. Due to the small sample size of the MD group and the exploratory nature of this study, and given that children with MD often experience co-morbid reading problems, regardless of whether they are identified and receive services through a formal identification process, the MD sample was grouped with the RD-MD sample (n=230). Parent depression group differences. Significant group differences were found on three items related to parent depression. Specifically, a Kruskal-Wallis H test showed a statistically significant difference across groups in parent responses regarding having a poor appetite, χ2(2) = 14.83, p=.001, having difficulty “shaking the blues,” χ2(2) = 10.92, p=.004, and feeling fearful, χ2(2) = 6.86, p=.032. Dunn-Bonferroni post-hoc analyses indicated children without LD had parents whom reported significantly less frequency of problems with poor appetite compared to children with RD-MD (p<.001). Children without LD also had parents who reported significantly less frequency of difficulty “shaking the blues” compared to children with RD (p<.05). Finally, children with RD had parents who reported significantly less frequency of feeling fearful compared to children with RD-MD (p<.05). No other statistically significant group differences emerged. Parent school involvement group differences. Significant group differences were found on five items related to parent school involvement. A Kruskal-Wallis H test showed a statistically significant difference across groups in parent responses regarding the frequency with which parent’s report attending school open houses, χ2(2) = 21.22, p<.001, attending PTA meetings, χ2(2) = 12.01, p=.002, attending school events, χ2(2) = 12.84, p=.002, acting as a 125 school volunteer, χ2(2) = 18.50, p<.001, and participating in school fundraisers, χ2(2) = 16.53, p<.001. Post-hoc analyses indicated children without LD had parents whom reported attending school open houses (p<.001), PTA meetings (p<.05), school events (p=.001), acting as a school volunteer (p<.001), and participating in school fundraisers (p<.05) significantly more frequently than parents of children with RD-MD. Parents of children without LD also reported participating in school fundraisers significantly more frequently than parents of children with RD (p<.05). Additionally, children with RD had parents whom reported attending school open houses significantly more frequently than parents of children with RD-MD (p<.05). No other statistically significant group differences emerged. Academic achievement group differences. Significant group effects on academic achievement in reading scores in kindergarten, F (2, 9850) = 417.39, p<.001, math scores in kindergarten, F (2, 10304) = 333.47, p<.001, reading scores in fifth grade, F (2, 10171) = 482.84, p<.001, and math scores in fifth grade, F (2, 10178) = 393.56, p<.001 were found. Effect sizes, calculated using eta squared, were .05 for reading scores in kindergarten, .05 for math scores in kindergarten, .07 for reading scores in fifth grade, and .05 for math scores in fifth grade, suggesting small effect sizes. Games-Howell post-hoc analyses revealed that children without LD performed higher than children with RD and children with RD-MD (p<.05) on both reading and math achievement tests at both time points. Similarly, children with RD performed significantly higher than children with RD-MD (p<.05) on both academic measures at both time points. On average, children with RD-MD scored approximately 13 points lower on the kindergarten reading assessment, 14 points lower on the kindergarten math assessment, 14 points lower on the fifth-grade reading assessment, and 14 points lower on the kindergarten math assessment when compared to children without LD. Children with RD only scored, on average, 3 126 points higher on kindergarten reading, 6 points higher on kindergarten math, 3 points higher on fifth grade reading, and 7 points higher on fifth grade math compared to children with RD-MD. Psychosocial behavior group differences. Significant group effects on all five scales of psychosocial behavior in kindergarten and all five scales of psychosocial behavior in fifth grade were found. In kindergarten, significant group differences emerged for the approaches to learning scale, F (2, 10080) = 203.42, p<.001, the self-control scale, F (2, 10012) = 25.98, p<.001, the interpersonal scale, F (2, 9980) = 50.32, p<.001, the externalizing behaviors scales, F (2, 10039) = 16.63, p<.00, and the internalizing behaviors scale, F (2, 9987) = 25.79, p<.001. Effect sizes, calculated using eta squared, were .04 for the approaches to learning scale, .01 for the self-control scale, .01 for the interpersonal scale, .01 for the externalizing behaviors scale, and .01 for the internalizing behaviors scale, suggesting small effect sizes. Games-Howell posthoc analyses revealed that children without LD received more positive ratings from teachers indicating better approaches to learning, self-control, and interpersonal skills, and fewer externalizing behaviors and internalizing behaviors compared to children with RD and children with RD-MD (p<.05) in kindergarten. Children with RD performed significantly higher than children with RD-MD (p<.05) on the approaches to learning scale and significantly lower on the internalizing behaviors scale (p<.05), but no significant differences were found regarding selfcontrol, interpersonal skills, or externalizing behaviors in kindergarten. On average, children with RD-MD scored approximately .3 to .8 points lower on the approaches to learning, selfcontrol, and interpersonal scales, and .2-.3 points higher on the externalizing behaviors and internalizing behaviors scales when compared to children without LD in kindergarten. Children with RD scored, on average, .3 points higher on the approaches to learning scale and .1 points 127 lower on the internalizing behaviors scale when compared to children with RD-MD in kindergarten. In fifth grade, significant group differences emerged for the approaches to learning scale, F (2, 9713) = 114.37, p<.001, the self-control scale, F (2, 9624) = 30.67, p<.001, the interpersonal scale, F (2, 9506) = 40.28, p<.001, the externalizing behaviors scales, F (2, 9655) = 19.28, p<.00, and the internalizing behaviors scale, F (2, 9546) = 27.55, p<.001. Effect sizes, calculated using eta squared, were .02 for the approaches to learning scale, .01 for the selfcontrol scale, .01 for the interpersonal scale, .01 for the externalizing behaviors scale, and .01 for the internalizing behaviors scale, suggesting small effect sizes. Games-Howell post-hoc analyses revealed that children without LD received more positive ratings from teachers indicating better approaches to learning, self-control, and interpersonal skills, and fewer externalizing behaviors and internalizing behaviors compared to children with RD and children with RD-MD (p<.05) in fifth grade as well. Children with RD did not perform significantly differently than children with RD-MD (p<.05) on any of the scales. On average, children with RD-MD scored approximately .3 to .5 points lower on the approaches to learning, self-control, and interpersonal scales, and .2 points higher on the externalizing behaviors and internalizing behaviors scales when compared to children without LD in fifth grade. Motivation group differences. Significant group effects on student’s self-reported motivation across all school subjects, F (2, 10183) = 20.40, p<.001, however with statistically negligible effect size (eta=0.004). Interestingly, children with RD rated themselves significantly lower in terms of school motivation than children without LD (p<.05), as well as children with RD-MD (p<.05). There was no significant mean difference in student’s self-reported school motivation between children without LD and children with RD-MD. 128 Table 20 Unweighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics Variables No Learning Disability Reading Disability Reading-Math Disability Na Na Mean (SD) Na Mean (SD) Kindergarten SES 9880 .06 (.80) 170 -.23 (.73) 220 -.40 (.68) Age at Assessment (K, 1999) 9330 68.38 (4.30) 170 69.00 (4.63) 210 69.27 (5.21) Age at Assessment (5th grade, 2004) Kindergarten Achievement Direct Math Assessment Direct Reading Assessment Fifth Grade Achievement Direct Math Assessment Direct Reading Assessment Early Psychosocial Behaviors Teacher-Rated Approaches to Learning Teacher-Rated Self-Control Teacher-Rated Interpersonal Skills Teacher-Rated Externalizing Teacher-Rated 9790 134.51 (4.35) 180 135.00 (4.73) 220 135.40 (5.22) 9930 52.06 (9.47) 170 44.19 (8.47) 210 38.10 (8.69) 9500 51.98 (9.40) 160 41.60 (6.60) 190 39.07 (7.98) 9790 51.95 (9.31) 189 45.55 (8.43) 220 37.96 (7.68) 9790 51.95 (9.28) 170 40.81 (7.65) 210 37.65 (8.21) 9700 3.23 (.64) 170 2.74 (.65) 210 2.42 (.65) 9630 3.26 (.59) 170 3.08 (.63) 210 2.98 (.63) 9610 3.20 (.61) 170 2.95 (.65) 210 2.80 (.65) 9660 1.57 (.57) 170 1.73 (.66) 210 1.81 (.73) Mean (SD) Covariates 129 Table 20 (cont’d) Internalizing 9610 1.51 (.48) 160 1.63 (.50) 210 1.77 (.58) Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior Teacher-Rated 9310 3.14 (.65) 180 2.73 (.62) 230 2.63 (.60) Approaches to Learning Teacher-Rated 9230 3.28 (.58) 170 3.11 (.62) 230 2.98 (.63) Self-Control Teacher-Rated 9110 3.13 (.62) 170 2.90 (.64) 220 2.79 (.67) Interpersonal Skills Teacher-Rated 9250 1.60 (.55) 180 1.78 (.60) 230 1.80 (.63) Externalizing Teacher-Rated 9150 1.59 (.51) 170 1.77 (.58) 230 1.81 (.52) Internalizing Motivation Student-Rated 9790 2.76 (.62) 180 2.46 (.66) 220 2.64 (.71) School Note: a All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. 130 Table 21 Unweighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics (Categorical Variables) Variables No Learning Disability Na (%) Parent Depression Felt bothered Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Poor appetite Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt the “blues” Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Trouble focusing Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt depressed Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time 5070 (53.9) 3360 (35.7) 740 (7.9) 240 (2.6) 6850 (72.7) 1920 (20.4) 370 (3.9) 280 (3.0) 7610 (80.9) 1400 (14.9) 230 (2.5) 160 (1.7) 5550 (58.9) 3180 (33.8) 420 (4.5) 260 (2.8) 6950 (73.9) 2010 (21.4) 230 (2.4) 210 (2.3) Mean (SD) 1.59 (.74) Reading Disability Reading-Math Disability Na (%) Na (%) 90 (59.9) Mean (SD) 1.55 (.79) 50 (29.3) 10 (7.0) 10 (3.8) 1.37 (.70) 110 (71.3) 1.44 (.82) 30 (19.1) 10 (3.8) 10 (5.7) 1.25 (.58) 110 (72.4) 1.40 (.77) 30 (19.2) 10 (3.8) 10 (4.5) 1.51 (.71) 100 (63.1) 1.53 (.81) 40 (25.5) 10 (7.0) 10 (4.5) 1.33 (.64) 110 (70.7) 30 (21.7) 10 (4.5) 10 (3.2) 131 1.40 (.72) 100 (48.3) 80 (38.8) 10 (7.5) 10 (4.7) 120 (61.4) 50 (25.2) 20 (8.9) 10 (4.5) 150 (76.1) 30 (17.4) 10 (4.0) 10 (2.5) 100 (52.2) 70 (35.8) 10 (7.5) 10 (4.5) 140 (70.8) 40 (20.8) 10 (5.0) 10 (3.5) Mean (SD) 1.70 (.83) 1.56 (.83) 1.33 (.67) 1.64 (.81) 1.41 (.74) Table 21 (cont’d) Felt things were effortful Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time 1.62 (.88) 5390 (57.3) 2860 (30.4) 480 (5.1) 670 (7.1) Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Difficulty sleeping Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Talked less Never Most of Time 140 (1.5) Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt sad Never 1.13 (.44) 140 (90.4) <10 (1.9) <10 (.6) 7620 (81.0) 1420 (15.1) 200 (2.1) 160 (1.8) 5910 1.79 (.95) 80 (49.0) 20 (11.5) 10 (8.3) 130 (80.9) 1.30 (.71) 20 (12.1) 10 (3.2) 10 (3.8) 1.25 (.58) 130 (81.5) 1.29 (.71) 20 (11.5) 10 (3.2) 10 (3.8) 1.44 (.65) 132 100 (66.9) 1.89 (1.0) 90 (43.6) 70 (36.1) 20 (7.9) 20 (12.4) 50 (31.2) 1.24 (.57) 1.31 (.75) 160 (70.3) 20 (10.9) 10 (3.5) 10 (4.5) 10 (7.0) 1.72 (.89) Moderate Amount Felt lonely Never 10 (5.7) 20 (10.8) 4700 (50.0) 3290 (35.0) 720 (7.7) 690 (7.4) 1.70 (.89) 100 (52.0) 70 (33.5) 10 (7.5) 10 (7.0) 40 (22.9) 8010 (85.1) 1130 (12.0) 150 (1.6) 120 (1.2) 7640 (81.2) 1380 (14.6) 250 (2.7) Some Time 100 (60.5) 1.19 (.51) Felt fearful Never 1.67 (1.0) 1.43 (.72) 150 (76.2) 40 (18.3) <10 (1.5) 10 (4.0) 160 (77.2) 40 (18.3) 10 (3.0) <10 (1.5) 120 1.33 (.70) 1.29 (.60) 1.51 (.72) Table 21 (cont’d) Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Difficulty “to get going” Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Parent School Involvement Attended open house Yes No Attended PTO Yes No Attended parent group Yes No Attended teacher conference Yes No Attended school event Yes No (62.8) 3020 (32.1) 310 (3.3) 10 (3.8) 170 (1.9) 10 (3.2) 1.51 (.72) 5620 (59.7) 3090 (32.9) 420 (4.5) 270 (2.9) 7260 (75.8) 2310 (24.2) 3580 (37.3) 6010 (62.7) 880 (9.2) 10 (3.0) 1.54 (.84) 100 (63.1) 10 (5.7) 10 (5.7) 1.24 (.43) 120 (77.0) 1.23 (.42) 40 (23.0) 1.63 (.48) 50 (33.5) 1.66 (.47) 110 (66.5) 1.91 (.29) 10 (9.3) 1.91 (.29) 150 (90.7) 1.14 (.35) 8230 (85.9) 1350 (14.1) 50 (26.0) 150 (74.0) 20 (11.1) 180 (88.9) 110 (60.5) 50 (34.0) 1.38 (.49) 1.74 (.44) 1.89 (.31) 1.13 (.34) 180 (86.5) 30 (13.5) 10 (8.0) 133 130 (62.0) 80 (38.0) 1.08 (.27) 150 (92.0) 1.31 (.46) 1.54 (.66) 110 (54.2) 80 (39.3) 10 (5.0) <10 (1.5) 40 (25.5) 8700 (90.8) 6640 (69.3) 2940 (30.7) (59.0) 70 (34.0) 10 (4.0) 40 (26.1) 1.34 (.48) 120 (58.0) 90 (42.0) 1.42 (.50) Table 21 (cont’d) Volunteered at school Yes No Participated in fundraiser Yes 4990 (52.0) 4600 (48.0) 1.48 (.50) 70 (43.8) 1.56 (.50) 90 (56.2) 1.38 (.49) 1.48 (.50) 80 (38.6) 130 (61.4) 1.61 (.49) 1.49 (.50) 5950 90 (52.5) 100 (62.1) (51.0) 3630 80 (47.5) 100 (37.9) (49.0) Note: a All unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 10 due to restrictions set forth via the licensing agreement. No 134 Table 22 Weighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics Variables No Learning Disability Reading Disability N Mean (SD) N Kindergarten SES 3,353,415 -.03 (.78) 40,126 Age at Assessment (K, 1999) 3,111,8989 68.31 (4.30) 38,867 2,928,775 134.53 (4.38) 40,126 3,245,941 50.85 (9.68) 3,100,220 Mean (SD) Reading-Math Disability N Mean (SD) -.25 (.75) 81,545 -.54 (.91) 68.58 (4.57) 75,835 70.02 (5.83) 76,003 135.91 (5.81) 39,035 45.04 (7.62) 76,202 38.86 (8.53) 50.99 (9.70) 37,583 41.78 (6.33) 68,330 38.57 (7.52) 2,928,740 51.11 (9.61) 40,126 47.25 (7.35) 76,329 37.42 (7.41) 2,920,902 51.16 (9.52) 40,126 42.44 (6.52) 74,066 38.03 (7.67) 3,136,565 3.15 (.67) 39,542 2.75 (.65) 75,632 2.41 (.66) 3,120,350 3.22 (.61) 39,542 3.15 (.60) 75,676 3.00 (.64) 3,106,357 3.15 (.62) 39,542 3.01 (.58) 73,697 2.74 (.65) Covariates Age at Assessment (5th grade, 2004) Kindergarten Achievement Direct Math Assessment Direct Reading Assessment Fifth Grade Achievement Direct Math Assessment Direct Reading Assessment Early Psychosocial Behaviors TeacherRated Approaches to Learning TeacherRated SelfControl TeacherRated Interpersonal 135 134.63 (4.62) Table 22 (cont’d) Skills TeacherRated Externalizing TeacherRated Internalizing Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior TeacherRated Approaches to Learning TeacherRated SelfControl TeacherRated Interpersonal Skills TeacherRated Externalizing TeacherRated Internalizing Motivation StudentRated School 3,126,532 1.62 (.60) 39,202 1.69 (.67) 74,859 1.89 (.79) 3,104,368 1.55 (.50) 38,835 1.55 (.50) 75,158 1.78 (.58) 2,699,372 3.09 (.67) 38,937 2.78 (.63) 80,696 2.62 (.62) 2,672,205 3.25 (.59) 38,778 3.22 (.57) 80,379 3.00 (.56) 2,624,217 3.10 (.63) 38,469 2.97 (.67) 80,420 2.75 (.60) 2,674,614 1.64 (.58) 38,937 1.73 (.52) 80,513 1.82 (.62) 2,646,345 1.62 (.53) 38,729 1.72 (.52) 80,696 1.78 (.46) 2,928,388 2.72 (.64) 40,126 2.32 (.62) 76,392 2.74 (.82) 136 Table 23 Weighted Subpopulation Descriptive Statistics (Categorical Variables) Variables No Learning Disability N (%) Parent Depression Felt bothered Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Poor appetite Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt the “blues” Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Trouble focusing Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time 1678074 (52.9) 1149172 (36.2) 270662 (8.5) 74467 (2.3) 2240349 (70.5) 680528 (21.4) 139603 (4.4) 115078 (3.6) 2539143 (80.0) 487456 (15.4) 82716 (2.6) 64737 (2.0) 1849482 (58.2) 1046561 (32.9) 171109 (5.4) 110021 (3.5) Reading Disability Mean (SD) N (%) 1.60 (.74) 21281 (54.2) 12973 (33.1) 3189 (8.1) Mean (SD) 1.63 (.82) 1788 (4.5) 1.41 (.74) 27085 (69.0) 7942 (20.2) 1.47 (.83) 2205 (5.6) 1.54 (.75) 27359 (69.7) 9278 (23.6) 45033 (56.7) 27644 (34.8) 4368 (5.5) 1.40 (.72) 47969 (60.4) 18633 (23.4) 9688 (12.2) 3194 (4.0) 1242 (3.2) 60537 (76.2) 16351 (20.6) 1717 (2.2) 1352 (3.4) 879 (1.1) 25503 (65.0) 8685 (22.1) 3329 (8.5) 1714 (4.4) 137 N (%) Mean (SD) 1.55 (.74) 2439 (3.1) 1998 (5.1) 1.27 (.61) Reading-Math Disability 1.52 (.83) 35286 (44.4) 29654 (37.3) 5300 (6.7) 9244 (11.6) 1.60 (.85) 1.28 (.56) 1.86 (.98) Table 23 (cont’d) Felt depressed Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt things were effortful Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time 2306057 (72.6) 695835 (21.9) 70261 (2.2) 102708 (3.2) Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Difficulty sleeping Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Talked less Never Some Time 29681 (75.7) 6397 (16.3) 1730262 (54.6) 961967 (30.3) 166363 (5.2) 312837 (9.9) 2126 (5.4) 1026 (2.6) 672 (0.8) 1.65 (.98) 23877 (60.9) 9232 (23.5) 2047 (5.2) 4074 (10.4) 6983 (8.8) 1.08 (.34) 2677019 (84.2) 403675 (12.7) 54854 (1.7) 42146 (1.3) 2550184 (80.2) 469415 (14.8) 36724 (93.6) 2238 (5.7) 269 (0.7) 19811 (50.5) 14864 (37.9) 2097 (5.3) 7990 (10.1) 1.67 (.84) 2459 (6.3) 1.27 (.61) 30771 (78.4) 4662 (11.9) 138 1.46 (.95) 60313 (75.9) 9458 (11.9) 1723 (2.2) 0 (0) 1.78 (.93) 1.42 (.68) 1.63 (.93) 46948 (57.6) 21374 (27.0) 3903 (4.9) 1.20 (.52) 1520760 (47.9) 1109936 (34.9) 269732 (8.5) 277753 (8.7) 1.35 (.70) 54305 (68.3) 17984 (22.6) 6523 (8.2) 1.70 (.95) Felt fearful Never 1.36 (.69) 35397 (44.5) 27723 (34.9) 3725 (4.7) 1.92 (1.06) 12639 (15.9) 1.37 (.81) 58669 (73.8) 11672 (14.7) 1.45 (.88) Table 23 (cont’d) Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt lonely Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Felt sad Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Difficulty “to get going” Never Some Time Moderate Amount Most of Time Parent School Involvement Attended open house Yes No Attended PTO Yes No 96061 (3.0) 63820 (2.0) 2549165 (80.3) 471399 (14.8) 78789 (2.5) 76233 (2.4) 1950599 (61.4) 1038623 (32.7) 115544 (3.6) 71723 (2.3) 1.27 (.63) 1.47 (.68) 1561 (3.9) 3351 (4.2) 2236 (5.7) 5793 (7.1) 31445 (80.2) 5264 (13.4) 1765 (4.4) 59062 (74.3) 15895 (20.0) 3659 (4.6) 757 (1.9) 867 (1.1) 27359 (69.7) 8959 (22.8) 2188 (5.6) 724 (1.8) 1283 (1.6) 1.68 (.96) 1832568 (57.7) 1073920 (33.8) 152764 (4.8) 116180 (3.7) 1111307 (34.4) 2116551 (65.6) 1.27 (.45) 1.40 (.68) 46296 (58.5) 28258 (35.7) 3292 (4.2) 1.54 (.75) 2348497 (72.8) 877730 (27.2) 1.28 (.64) 38976 (49.0) 37382 (47.0) 2902 (3.7) 3722 (9.5) 224 (0.3) 1.66 (.48) 15434 (39.9) 23251 (60.1) 139 1.26 (.44) 1.60 (.49) 1.49 (.66) 1.55 (.58) 22447 (57.2) 10604 (27.0) 2458 (6.3) 28699 (74.2) 9985 (25.8) 1.32 (.61) 42382 (52.0) 39162 (48.0) 15744 (19.3) 65800 (80.7) 1.48 (.50) 1.81 (.40) Table 23 (cont’d) Attended parent group Yes No Attended teacher conference Yes No Attended school event Yes No Volunteered at school Yes No Participated in fundraiser Yes No 270813 (8.4) 2955952 (91.6) 1.92 (.28) 3483 (8.7) 36643 (91.3) 1.16 (.37) 1559005 (48.3) 1670182 (51.7) 1924517 (59.7) 1301651 (40.3) 6401 (7.8) 36524 (91.0) 3601 (9.0) 1.34 (.47) 29154 (72.7) 10972 (27.3) 1.52 (.50) 17532 (43.7) 22594 (56.3) 1.40 (.49) 20860 (52.0) 19266 (48.0) 140 1.92 (.27) 75143 (92.2) 1.09 (.29) 2706336 (83.8) 522551 (16.2) 2138755 (66.2) 1090376 (33.8) 1.91 (.28) 1.12 (.32) 71899 (88.2) 9645 (11.8) 1.27 (.45) 1.56 (.50) 1.48 (.50) 56138 (68.8) 25407 (31.2) 28883 (35.4) 52662 (64.6) 38488 (47.2) 43057 (52.8) 1.31 (.46) 1.65 (.48) 1.53 (.50) Analyzing the Structural Model Establishing Factors by Testing the Measurement Model. Prior to running the structural equation model (Figure 2 and Figure 3), Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted to test the four-factor measurement model. The first latent factor, parent depression, was indicated by twelve observed variables. The second latent factor, parent school involvement, was indicated by seven observed variables. The third latent factor, early psychosocial behavior, was indicated by five observed variables. The fourth latent factor, fifth grade psychosocial behavior, was indicated by five observed variables. All factors were assumed to covary with one another and their associations were specified as unanalyzed in the measurement model (Kline, 2005). The four-factor CFA fit the data adequately. Absolute fit indices included a RMSEA equal to .020 (CI = .019 -.021) and relative fit indices, CFI =.934, TLI=.927. The chi-square test of model fit was not available due to the use of WLSMV estimation and because sample weights were applied to the analyses (Muthen & Muthen, 2015). Modifications to the model were examined based on evaluation of factor loadings, modification indices, absolute fit indices, and theoretical considerations. Examination yielded removal of two variables in the parent school involvement construct, attending parent advisory group and attending parent-teacher conference. These variables had low communalities (<.10), and preliminary analyses revealed little variance in these variables across groups. Modifications improved the model fit. See Table 24 for unstandardized and standardized estimates of the individual factor loadings for the final measurement model. The final model fit the data adequately using weighted least square parameter estimates. Absolute fit indices included a RMSEA equal to .012 (CI = .011 -.013), and relative fit indices, CFI =.952, TLI=.947. 141 Measurement Invariance. Prior to running multi-group analyses, the final four-factor measurement model was tested for group invariance. Group invariance was tested by examining configural, metric, and scalar invariance models and comparing models across groups. Configural invariance was tested by not specifying any equality constraints across groups. Metric invariance was tested by constraining the factor loadings, but allowing intercepts to differ across groups (LD and no LD, and then again for RD, RD-MD, and no LD). Scalar invariance was tested by constraining the factor loadings and intercepts to be equal across groups. Scalar invariance implies that constructs are equal across groups, therefore allowing comparisons on latent variables to be made across groups. Cheung & Rensvold (2002) recommend a change in CFI between invariance models that is equal or less than .01 to indicate adequate fit across groups. Results indicated adequate fit for LD and no-LD groups (CFI = .962 for the configural model, CFI=.965 for the metric model, CFI=.958 for scalar the model, and CFI=.007). Results also indicated adequate fit for the three-group analysis, RD, RD-MD, and no LD groups, (CFI = .956 for the configural model, CFI=.962 for the metric model, CFI=.956 for the scalar model, and CFI=.006). Thus, the measurement model met requirements for scalar invariance thereby indicating that constructs are equal across groups. Testing the Structural Latent Model. There are two main methods to analyze structural models using complex sample data in Mplus (Muthen & Muthen, 2015). The first method takes into account complex sampling design effects including stratification, clustering, and sampling weights. Given that the ECLS-K study used a multistage, stratified, clustered, equal probability systematic sampling design and provided stratification and clustering weights, in addition to application of sampling weights to account for missing data, this method was chosen to analyze the structural latent model. However, this method does not specify random 142 effects in a way that allows for analysis of interaction terms, such as in a moderation analysis. Thus, a second method that specifies random effects was also used to test interactions. However, there were significant limitations to analyzing both mediation and moderation (interaction) paths simultaneously using these methods. First, MLR is the given estimator for this analysis, however bootstrapping confidence intervals to test mediation effects is unavailable with this type of simultaneous analysis of separate mediation and moderation paths in complex samples (Muthen & Muthen, 2015). Second, traditional fit statistics such as chi-square, RMSEA, CFI, and TLI, are unavailable with analysis of moderation effects in complex samples. Instead AIC and BIC, predictive fit indices, are provided for researchers to compare alternative models to the proposed model. Models with smaller AIC values are considered more likely to be replicated in the population from which the sample is drawn. Third, multi-group analysis is unavailable when testing interactions using numerical integration in a complex sample. Instead, models run for each subset of the sample may be compared for goodness of fit using AIC/BIC. The full structural model (Figure 2 and Figure 3) was tested specifying estimation of both complex sample design features and random slopes with covariates, including gender, child’s race, SES, and child’s age at kindergarten, allowed to freely predict parent depression, parent school involvement, academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors at both time points, and motivation in 5th grade. Observed variables were centered to allow for interpretation of interaction effects. Bootstrapping of confidence intervals was unavailable with this type of analysis specified; bootstrapping is not available for complex sample design models with random slopes estimated using MLR without replicate weights (Muthen & Muthen, 2015). Overall, the full structural model did not converge when mediation paths, interaction paths, and covariates were all specified, nor when mediation paths and interaction terms were specified without 143 covariates. Attempts to diagnosis and fix convergence issues were unsuccessful, including addressing negative residual variances, rescaling the residual variances of four variables (kindergarten math and reading achievement t-scores and fifth grade math and reading achievement t-scores), increasing the number of iterations, freeing specific factor loadings and fixing factor variances, inputting different parameter starting values, and inputting covariates in a step-wise manner. This led to a change in the structural equation paths that could be estimated simultaneously, while accounting for complex sample design and including covariate effects. Thus, indirect paths (mediation) related to research question 1a examining the effect of parent depression on early academic achievement and psychosocial behavior in kindergarten via school parent involvement were analyzed in the model without specifying interaction terms. Interaction paths (moderation) related to research question 1b examining the effect of children’s academic achievement and psychosocial behavior as moderators on the relationship between parent school involvement in kindergarten on children’s motivation and psychosocial behavior in fifth grade were tested without specifying indirect mediation paths. All other direct paths were kept constant. Figure 4 and Figure 5 display the final structural models. 144 Figure 4. Modified Structural Model (Mediation Paths in red). Parent Depression Early Academic Achievement: Reading & Math Motivation Academic Achievement : Reading & Math Early Parent School Involvement Early Psychosocial Behavior Psychosocial Behavior Note: Latent variables are shown in circles. Covariates: • Gender • Race • SES • Age at Assessment 145 Figure 5. Modified Structural Model (Moderation Paths in green). Parent Depression Early Academic Achievement: Reading & Math Motivation Academic Achievement : Reading & Math Early Parent School Involvement Early Psychosocial Behavior Psychosocial Behavior Note: Latent variables are shown in circles. The dashed lines represent the interaction between the exogenous variable (parent school involvement) and the moderator variables. Covariates: • Gender • Race • SES • Age at Assessment 146 Research Question One: Influence of Parent Depression and Parent Involvement on Outcomes for All Children The first research question sought to explore the relation between parent depression and parent school involvement on children’s outcomes (academic achievement, psychosocial behavior, and motivation) in kindergarten, and the relation between kindergarten and fifth grade outcomes by testing a structural latent variable model using a sample of children with (n=10,220) and without LD (n=410) with age at kindergarten, gender, race, and SES as covariates in the model. It was hypothesized that the latent factor measurement model, along with the structural model, would be empirically supported as indicated by strong data fit indices. Mediation Analysis. The structural model specifying indirect effects of parent depression on kindergarten achievement and psychosocial behavior and direct paths from kindergarten to fifth grade outcomes (Figure 4) was tested using estimator WLSMV. WLSMV is the preferred estimator when categorical and continuous latent variables are included in a model. Covariates were allowed to freely predict all variables, however the model did not converge. Covariates were then added to the model in a step-wise manner with all covariates allowed to freely predict outcome variables only (fifth grade achievement, fifth grade psychosocial behavior, fifth grade motivation, kindergarten psychosocial behavior, and kindergarten achievement). Then, covariates were allowed to freely predict exogenous variables (parent depression, parent school involvement). All non-significant covariate paths (p<.01) were removed and the modified structural model included only those covariates that uniquely contributed to the model. The model fit the data adequately, RMSEA = 0.012 (CI = .011 -.013), and relative fit indices, CFI =.923, TLI=.914. However, not all structural paths were significant (p<.01). Specifically, parent depression did not significantly predict parent school involvement 147 (= -.024, p=.400), children’s psychosocial behavior in kindergarten (= -.044, p=.025), or children’s math achievement in kindergarten (= -0.054, p= .012), parent school involvement did not significantly predict children’s motivation (= -.009 , p=.797) or psychosocial behavior (= -.087, p= .011) in fifth grade, children’s psychosocial behavior in kindergarten did not significantly predict their fifth grade achievement in reading (= .014, p=.544), children’s reading achievement in kindergarten did not significantly predict their later psychosocial behavior (= -.045, p=.107) or motivation in fifth grade (=.081 , p=.012), children’s math achievement in kindergarten did not significantly predict their motivation in fifth grade (=.042 , p=.180), and children’s motivation in fifth grade did not significantly predict their fifth grade reading achievement (= .007, p=.637). Paths involved in the mediation analysis were kept in the model regardless of significance, as these paths were integral to answering research question 1a. However, all other paths that were not significant at the p<.01 level were removed, and the structural model was simplified. Table 25 displays the factor loadings and path estimates for the final structural model. The simplified structural model fit the data adequately, RMSEA = .012 (CI = .011 - .013), and relative fit indices, CFI = .922, TLI= .914. All structural paths were significant (p<.01) except for those that were intentionally not trimmed from the model to analyze research question 1a. Research question 1a. Research question 1a sought to explore whether parent depression indirectly predicted early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten via parent school involvement. Results did not support the hypothesized relationship that lower levels of parent depression would indirectly predict higher levels of academic achievement and more positive psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten via parent school involvement. Parent depression was not significantly related to parent school involvement (= -.031, SE= .027, ns). 148 Thus, since parent school involvement was not related to parent depression, it did not serve as a mediator for any relationship between parent depression and child academic and psychosocial outcomes in kindergarten. As would be expected from these results, the indirect effects tested using biased corrected bootstrapped standard errors was also nonsignificant. The indirect effect from parent depression to early psychosocial behavior via parent school involvement was nonsignificant (= .008, SE=.009, 95% CI= -.016-.031, ns). The indirect effect from parent depression to kindergarten reading achievement via parent school involvement was nonsignificant (= .008, SE= .010, 95% CI= -.018-.039, ns). The indirect effect from parent depression to kindergarten math achievement via parent school involvement was non-significant (= .008 SE= .010, 95% CI= -.019-.035, ns). These findings do not support the hypothesized mediational model. Interestingly, there were significant direct effects from parent school involvement to kindergarten outcomes. Higher levels of parent school involvement were predictive of more positive early psychosocial behavior (= -.255, p<.001), higher kindergarten math achievement (= -0.276, p<.001), and higher kindergarten reading achievement (= -0.257, p<.001). Additionally, lower levels of parent depression symptomology were directly related to higher kindergarten reading achievement (= -0.068, p=.001). Research question 1c. Research question 1c examined the direct effects of early academic achievement and psychosocial behavior on academic achievement in fifth grade. The model supported the hypothesis that higher levels of reading achievement in kindergarten were predictive of higher levels of reading achievement (= .308, SE= .025, p<.001) and math achievement (= .102, SE= .022, p<.001) in fifth grade. Likewise, higher levels of math achievement in kindergarten were predictive of higher levels of reading achievement (= .293, 149 SE=.024, p<.001) and math achievement (= .530, SE= .022, p<.001) in fifth grade. However, there was only partial support for the hypothesis that more positive psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten were predictive of higher levels of academic achievement in fifth grade. Specifically, more positive psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten were related to higher math achievement in fifth grade (= 0.066, SE= .402, p<.001), but psychosocial behavior in kindergarten was not significantly related to reading achievement in fifth grade (= -.026, p=.201). Research question 1d. Research question 1d sought to explore whether student-rated motivation in school and psychosocial behaviors predicted academic achievement in fifth grade. The model supported the hypothesis that more positive psychosocial behavior in fifth grade was related to higher math achievement (= .067, SE= .308, p=.001) and reading achievement (= .127, SE= .277, p<.001) in fifth grade. However, there was only partial support for the influence of student-rated motivation. Student-rated motivation was predictive of higher math achievement (= .089, SE= .238, p<.001), however it was not significantly predictive of reading achievement (= -.008, p=.590). Influence of covariates. Covariates, including SES, child’s gender, age, and race, had effects on many of the study variables. First, there was an effect of family SES of parent depression, parent school involvement, and academic achievement at both time points. Lower family SES significantly predicted more depressive symptomology in parents (= -.193, t>1.96) and lower levels of parent school involvement (= -.523, t>1.96). Higher family SES significantly predicted higher kindergarten reading achievement (= .218, t>1.96), higher kindergarten math achievement (= .245, t>1.96), higher fifth grade reading achievement (= .156, t>1.96), and higher fifth grade math achievement (= .110, t>1.96). Older children at 150 kindergarten performed more positively on teacher-rated kindergarten psychosocial behavior (= .012, t>1.96), however children’s age at kindergarten did not have a significant effect on other study variables. Child gender significantly predicted kindergarten psychosocial behavior (= .234, t>1.96), fifth grade psychosocial behavior (= .185, t>1.96), and fifth grade math achievement (= -.136, t>1.96). This finding suggested that females were more likely to have more positive teacher-ratings of psychosocial behavior at both time points, however, males were more likely to perform higher on fifth grade math achievement. Finally, child race/ethnicity significantly predicted parent school involvement, psychosocial behavior at both time points, and reading and math achievement at both time points. These findings suggest children of AfricanAmerican/Black race/ethnicity (= .195, t>1.96) and Hispanic race/ethnicity (= .098, t>1.96) were more likely to have parents with lower levels of parent school involvement. Children of African-American/Black race/ethnicity were more likely to have less positive teacher-rated psychosocial behavior in kindergarten (= -.130, t>1.96), less positive teacher-rated psychosocial behavior in fifth grade (= -.100, t>1.96), lower kindergarten math achievement score (= -.117, t>1.96), lower fifth grade math achievement score (= -.107, t>1.96), and lower fifth grade reading achievement scores (= -.082, t>1.96). Children of Asian race/ethnicity were more likely to have more positive teacher-rated psychosocial behavior in fifth grade (= .072, t>1.96). Children of Hispanic-not otherwise specified race/ethnicity were more likely to have lower kindergarten reading achievement scores (= -.074, t>1.96) and lower fifth grade reading achievement scores (= -.076, t>1.96). Lastly, children of Native American race/ethnicity were more likely to have lower kindergarten math achievement scores (= -.075, t>1.96). 151 Table 24 Final Confirmatory Factor Analysis for a Four-Factor Model Items Unstandardized Standard Error Standardized (SE) Parent Depression Felt the “blues” Felt depressed Felt bothered Poor appetite Trouble focusing Felt things were effortful Felt fearful Difficulty sleeping Talked less Felt lonely Felt sad Difficulty “to get going” Parent School Involvement Volunteer at School Attend Open House Attend PTA/PTO Attend School Event Participate in School Fundraising Early Psychosocial Behavior Self-Control Interpersonal Skills Approaches to Learning Internalizing Behavior Externalizing Behavior R2 1.000 1.120 0.917 0.890 1.030 1.172 0.000 0.031 0.038 0.034 0.043 0.063 0.715 0.712 0.537 0.520 0.590 0.536 0.511 0.506 0.288 0.271 0.348 0.287 0.578 1.105 0.021 0.058 0.467 0.515 0.219 0.266 0.865 0.841 1.068 0.917 0.038 0.030 0.037 0.035 0.602 0.585 0.688 0.532 0.363 0.342 0.473 0.283 1.000 0.000 0.748 0.560 0.872 0.064 0.653 0.426 0.657 0.707 0.056 0.053 0.492 0.529 0.242 0.280 0.722 0.063 0.540 0.292 1.000 1.024 0.000 0.039 0.818 0.812 0.670 0.660 1.056 0.047 0.779 0.607 0.361 0.033 0.359 0.129 0.975 0.031 0.801 0.642 152 Table 24 (cont’d) Psychosocial Behavior Approaches to 1.000 0.000 Learning Interpersonal 0.917 0.033 Skills Self-Control 0.836 0.029 Internalizing 0.362 0.024 Behavior Externalizing 0.800 0.034 Behavior Note: All factor loadings were significant (p<.001). 153 0.852 0.725 0.833 0.664 0.815 0.391 0.694 0.153 0.793 0.628 Table 25 Parameter Estimates for a Four-Factor Structural Regression Model (Mediation Model) Items Unstandardized Standard Error Standardized R2 (SE) Factor Loadings Parent Depression (PD) Felt the “blues” Felt depressed Felt bothered Poor appetite Trouble focusing Felt things were effortful Felt fearful Difficulty sleeping Talked less Felt lonely Felt sad Difficulty “to get going” Parent School Involvement (PSI) Volunteer at School Attend Open House Attend PTA/PTO Attend School Event Participate in School Fundraising Early Psychosocial Behavior (EPB) Self-Control Interpersonal Skills Approaches to Learning Internalizing Behavior Externalizing Behavior Fifth-grade Psychosocial Behavior (FPB) Approaches to Learning 1.000 1.120 0.927 0.867 1.070 1.101 0.000 0.028 0.035 0.030 0.037 0.059 0.728 0.722 0.543 0.511 0.611 0.520 0.530 0.521 0.295 0.261 0.374 0.271 0.599 1.084 0.849 0.843 1.104 0.925 0.019 0.059 0.035 0.027 0.032 0.033 0.486 0.508 0.596 0.599 0.707 0.545 0.236 0.258 0.355 0.359 0.500 0.297 1.000 0.939 0.690 0.709 0.000 0.068 0.054 0.056 0.713 0.677 0.518 0.530 0.508 0.459 0.268 0.281 0.701 0.059 0.525 0.276 1.000 1.152 1.313 0.000 0.039 0.056 0.734 0.811 0.886 0.539 0.657 0.785 0.479 0.033 0.421 0.177 0.917 0.030 0.681 0.464 1.000 0.000 0.910 0.828 154 Table 25 (cont’d) Interpersonal Skills Self-Control Internalizing Behavior Externalizing Behavior 0.851 0.737 0.399 0.030 0.025 0.026 0.814 0.757 0.445 0.662 0.573 0.198 0.711 0.030 0.747 0.558 Direct Effects PD  EPB PSI  EPB PD  PSI EPB  FPB K Math  FPB PD  K Reading PSI  K Reading PD  K Math PSI  K Math EPB  Motivation FPB  5th Reading FPB  5th Math EPB  5th Math K Reading  5th Reading K Math  5th Reading K Reading  5th Math K Math  5th Math Motivation  5th Math -0.048 -0.143 -0.056 0.520 0.010 -1.480 -3.097 -1.107 -3.243 0.268 2.001 1.063 1.426 0.306 0.020 (ns) 0.018 0.049 (ns) 0.047 0.002 0.422 0.423 0.449 (ns) 0.456 0.040 0.277 0.308* 0.402 0.025 -0.048 -0.255 -0.031 0.384 0.149 -0.068 -0.257 -0.052 -0.276 0.186 0.127 0.067 0.066 0.308 0.298 0.024 0.293 0.102 0.022 0.102 0.542 1.330 0.022 0.238 0.530 0.089 Indirect Effects 0.007 (ns) 0.150 (ns) PD  PSI  EPB 0.008 0.008 PD  PSI  K 0.174 0.008 Reading PD  PSI  K 0.182 1.131 (ns) 0.009 Math Note: (ns) indicates paths were non-significant. * indicates paths were significant at (p<.01). All other structural paths were significant at (p<.001). 155 Moderation Analysis. The structural model specifying interactions between parent school involvement and kindergarten academic achievement and psychosocial behavior, along with direct paths from kindergarten to fifth grade outcomes (Figure 5) was tested using estimator MLR. MLR is the given estimator when interactions between categorical and continuous latent variables are included in a complex sample design model (Muthen & Muthen, 2015). As with the mediation model, covariates were allowed to freely predict all variables, however the model did not converge. Covariates were then added to the model in a step-wise manner with all covariates allowed to freely predict outcome variables only (fifth grade achievement, fifth grade psychosocial behavior, fifth grade motivation, kindergarten psychosocial behavior, and kindergarten achievement). Then, covariates were allowed to freely predict exogenous variables (parent depression, parent school involvement). All non-significant covariate paths (p<.01) were removed and the structural model included only those covariates that uniquely contributed to the model. Traditional model fit indices were unavailable as this was a non-nested model with MLR as the estimator (AIC = 476034.751, BIC = 477027.060). All paths that were significant in the mediation model maintained the same direction and significance. Research question 1b. Research question 1b sought to explore whether parent school involvement in kindergarten predicted children’s motivation and psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade as moderated by children’s level of academic achievement and psychosocial behavior in kindergarten. It was hypothesized that as the level of academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten increases (e.g., more positive performance), higher levels of parent school involvement will predict higher levels of motivation and more positive psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade. Early academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors were moderating variables. However, the model did not support this hypothesis. Direct paths from 156 parent school involvement to fifth grade motivation (= .016, SE = .088, p=.851), and parent school involvement to fifth grade psychosocial behavior (= -.095, SE = .065, p=.143) were nonsignificant. As expected, interactions between parent school involvement and kindergarten psychosocial behavior (= -.033, SE = .033, p=.312), parent school involvement and kindergarten reading achievement (= .002, SE = .002, p=.302), and parent school involvement and kindergarten math achievement (= -.003, SE = .002, p=.090) on fifth grade motivation were non-significant. Interactions between parent school involvement and kindergarten psychosocial behavior (= -.015, SE = .021, p=.466), parent school involvement and kindergarten reading achievement (= .001, SE = .001, p=.268), and parent school involvement and kindergarten math achievement (= .000, SE = .001, p=.710) on fifth grade psychosocial behavior were also nonsignificant. Research Question Two: Group Comparison Between Children with LD and Children Without LD The second research question sought to explore whether the relation between parent depression and parent school involvement on academic achievement and psychosocial behavior in kindergarten, and academic achievement, psychosocial behavior, and motivation in fifth grade differed between children with LD (n=10,220) and children without LD (n=410) by testing the structural latent variable model in research question one across groups. Overall, results suggest the relations between the constructs and factor loadings specified in the model (tested in research question one) differ across children with and without LD. Specifically, there were several significant paths between parent school involvement and kindergarten child outcomes, kindergarten achievement and psychosocial behavior and fifth grade outcomes, and fifth grade psychosocial behavior on fifth grade achievement for children without LD. For children with 157 LD, fewer significant findings arose. Specifically, significant effects of kindergarten math achievement on fifth grade reading achievement, kindergarten psychosocial behavior on fifth grade psychosocial behavior, and children’s school motivation in fifth grade on fifth grade math achievement were found for children with LD. Testing the Model Separately for LD and non-LD Samples. The same structural model specifying mediation paths (Figure 4) was tested separately for each group. The same covariates, SES, gender, child race, that were significant in the model for research question one, were included in the multi-sample model. Results demonstrated that the model fit the data adequately with children without LD, RMSEA = .012 (CI = .011 - .013), and relative fit indices, CFI = .919, TLI= .911. However, the model fit was adequate based on an absolute fit index, but below adequate for children with LD based on relative fit indices, RMSEA = .026 (CI = .018 .033), and relative fit indices, CFI = .765, TLI= .740. Multi-group model. The same model tested above was used to specify a multi-sample model to test for equivalence across samples of children with LD and without LD. Procedural steps. The structural paths and factor loadings in the multi-sample model were freely estimated across groups, exercising no constraints on the factor loadings or paths across groups (see Figure 4). The results of the model where the paths were freely estimated fit the data adequately, RMSEA = .012 (CI = .011 - .013), and relative fit indices, CFI = .914, TLI= .908. The results of this freely estimated model were compared to a model in which the factor loadings and all paths were constrained to be equal across groups. The fit of the model significantly worsened, based on the chi-square distribution for 40 degrees of freedom (p<.001), when the factor loadings, covariate estimates, and structural paths were all constrained to be 158 equal across groups, X2 = 452.58, p = 0.0, df = 40, RMSEA = .013 (CI= .012 - .014), CFI = .884, TLI =.879. See Table 26 for group differences on factor loadings and path estimates. Research question 1a for multi-group sample. In the freely estimated model, parent depression did not significantly predict kindergarten psychosocial behavior or academic achievement via parent school involvement for either group. Parent depression was not significantly related to parent school involvement for either group. Tests of indirect effects between parent depression, school involvement, and kindergarten variables were all nonsignificant for children without LD (= -.002, p=.590 for the indirect effect on kindergarten psychosocial behavior, = -.002, p=.594 for the indirect effect on kindergarten reading achievement, and = -.002, p=.590 for the indirect effect on kindergarten math achievement). This finding held for children with LD (= -.002, p=.592 for the indirect effect on kindergarten psychosocial behavior, = -.003, p=.599 for the indirect effect on kindergarten reading achievement, and = -.002, p=.592 for the indirect effect on kindergarten math achievement). Although these findings were not aligned with apriori hypotheses, these findings were not unexpected given the full sample model results. Other interesting findings arose regarding the influence of parent variables on kindergarten outcomes. Parent school involvement significantly predicted early psychosocial behavior for children without LD (= -0.254, t>1.96), however this relation was non-significant for children with LD. Likewise, parent school involvement significantly predicted kindergarten reading achievement (= -.260, t>1.96) and math achievement (= -.275, t>1.96) for children without LD, but did not significantly predict this relation in children with LD. Research question 1c for multi-group sample. Paths between kindergarten achievement and psychosocial behavior to fifth grade achievement were examined within the 159 freely estimated model for each group. For children without LD, kindergarten reading achievement significantly predicted fifth grade achievement in math and reading; this relation did not hold for children with LD. Similarly, kindergarten math achievement significantly predicted fifth grade psychosocial behavior for children without LD, but not for children with LD. Kindergarten math achievement more strongly predicted fifth grade reading achievement for children with LD (= 0.786, t>1.96) compared to children without LD (= 0.279, t>1.96). However, the opposite was true for the magnitude of relation between kindergarten math achievement and fifth grade math achievement; this relation was stronger for children without LD (= 0.519, t>1.96) as compared to children with LD (= 0.345, t>1.96). More positive kindergarten psychosocial behavior significantly predicted higher levels of children’s motivation in fifth grade and higher math achievement in fifth grade for children without LD, however these findings did not hold for children with LD. Interestingly, more positive kindergarten psychosocial behavior more strongly predicted more positive fifth grade psychosocial behavior for children with LD (=.456, t>1.96) as compared to children without LD (=.377, t>1.96). Research question 1d for multi-group sample. Paths between fifth grade student-rated motivation and fifth grade teacher-rated psychosocial behavior to fifth grade achievement were examined within the freely estimated model for each group. Fifth grade psychosocial behavior significantly predicted reading and math achievement in fifth grade for children without LD, but not for children with LD. Student-ratings of motivation were significantly predictive of fifth grade math achievement for children with LD indicating that children with LD were more likely to rate themselves as highly motivated in school but receive lower math achievement scores (= -.293, t>1.96). Conversely, for children without LD, higher student-ratings of motivation were significantly predictive of higher fifth grade math achievement (= .113, t>1.96). 160 Influence of covariates. The same covariates, SES, gender, child race, that were significant in the model for research question one, were included in the multi-sample model. Several differences between children without LD and children with LD emerged. All covariate pathways specified in research question one remained significant for children without LD (p<.001, except the effect of gender on kindergarten psychosocial behavior and the effect of gender on fifth grade psychosocial behavior was significant at the p<.01 level). However, for children with LD, five covariate paths were significant. First, children’s gender had a stronger influence on kindergarten psychosocial behavior for children with LD (= .253, p=.002), than children without LD (=.228, p<.001). This finding suggests that females with LD were more likely to have more positive psychosocial behavior than their male peers. Additionally, AfricanAmerican/Black children with LD were more likely to have more negative psychosocial behavior ratings in kindergarten (= -.431, p=.001) compared to their LD peers. Regarding effects of covariates on achievement variables, children with LD identified as Hispanic-not otherwise specified were more likely to have lower kindergarten reading achievement than their LD peers (=-.407, p<.001), and this relationship was stronger for children with LD identified as Hispanicnot otherwise specified as compared to Hispanic-not otherwise specified children without LD (=-.073, p=.002). Children with LD identified as Native American were also more likely to have lower kindergarten math achievement (=-.132, p=.001). Finally, children with LD from families with higher SES were more likely to have higher fifth grade reading achievement (=.431, p<.001) compared to children without LD from families with higher SES (=.159, p<.001). 161 Table 26 Parameter Estimates for a Four-Factor Structural Regression Multi-group Model Across Children with and without Learning Disability Items Unstandardized Standardized Unstandardized Standardized No Learning Disability Parent Depression (PD) Felt the “blues” Felt depressed Felt bothered Poor appetite Trouble focusing Felt things were effortful Felt fearful Difficulty sleeping Talked less Felt lonely Felt sad Difficulty “to get going” Parent School Involvement (PSI) Volunteer at School Attend Open House Attend PTA/PTO Attend School Event Participate in School Fundraising Early Psychosocial Behavior (EPB) Self-Control Interpersonal Skills Approaches to Learning Internalizing Behavior Externalizing Behavior Fifth-grade Psychosocial Behavior (FPB) Approaches to Learning Disability 1.000 1.124 0.926 0.864 1.062 1.104 0.726 0.722 0.541 0.511 0.611 0.520 1.000 1.124 0.926 0.864 1.062 1.104 0.771 0.762 0.558 0.503 0.531 0.550 0.600 1.080 0.844 0.835 1.102 0.919 0.498 0.506 0.601 0.591 0.703 0.539 0.600 1.080 0.844 0.835 1.102 0.919 0.375 0.516 0.473 0.651 0.781 0.596 1.000 0.924 0.666 0.728 0.718 0.672 0.505 0.547 1.000 0.924 0.666 0.728 0.771 0.809 0.555 0.571 0.693 0.523 0.693 0.279 1.000 1.133 1.244 0.745 0.811 0.866 1.000 1.133 1.244 0.779 0.848 0.912 0.465 0.418 0.465 0.392 0.915 0.695 0.915 0.637 1.000 0.898 1.000 0.887 162 Table 26 (cont’d) Learning Interpersonal Skills Self-Control Internalizing Behavior Externalizing Behavior 0.874 0.766 0.398 0.820 0.771 0.433 0.874 0.766 0.398 0.763 0.730 0.444 0.736 0.758 0.736 0.705 Direct Effects (No LD) PD  EPB PSI  EPB PD  PSI EPB  FPB K Math  FPB PD  K Reading PSI  K Reading PD  K Math PSI  K Math EPB  Motivation FPB  5th Reading FPB  5th Math EPB  5th Math K Reading  5th Reading K Math  5th Reading K Reading  5th Math K Math  5th Math Motivation  5th Math (LD) -0.031(ns) -0.143 0.012 (ns) 0.493 0.009 -1.030 (ns) -3.049 -0.873(ns) -3.151 0.247 2.088 1.037* 1.348 0.296 Direct Effects -0.030 (ns) -0.254 0.006 (ns) 0.377 0.139 -0.048 (ns) -0.260 -0.042 (ns) -0.275 0.176 0.133 0.066* 0.065 0.301 -0.031 (ns) -0.143 0.012 (ns) 0.524 0.014 (ns) -1.030 (ns) -3.049 -0.873 (ns) -3.151 0.351 (ns) -0.305 (ns) 0.793 (ns) 1.623 (ns) -0.658 (ns) -0.030 (ns) -0.018 (ns) 0.089 (ns) 0.456 0.192 -0.073 (ns) -0.028 (ns) -0.052 (ns) -0.025 (ns) 0.225 (ns) -0.020 (ns) 0.051 (ns) 0.092 (ns) -0.515 (ns) 0.281 0.279 0.850 0.786 0.094 0.095 0.317 (ns) 0.247 (ns) 0.528 1.666 0.519 0.113 0.375 -3.320 0.345* -0.293 Indirect Effects -.002 (ns) -.002 (ns) PD  PSI  EPB -.002 (ns) -.002 (ns) -.002 (ns) PD  PSI  K -.035 (ns) -.035 (ns) -.003 (ns) Reading PD  PSI  K -.036 (ns) -.002 (ns) -.036 (ns) -.002 (ns) Math Note: (ns) indicates paths were non-significant. * indicates paths were significant at (p<.01). All other structural paths were significant at (p<.001). 163 Multi-group Moderation Analysis. Research question 2b sought to explore whether early academic achievement and psychosocial behavior moderated the relation between parent school involvement and children’s motivation and psychosocial behavior in fifth grade differently for children with LD (n=410) as compared to children without LD (n=10,220) by testing the structural latent variable model in research question 1b across groups. Overall, results suggest that kindergarten academic achievement and psychosocial behavior do not moderate relations between parent school involvement and fifth grade outcomes for children with or without LD. Direct relations between parent school involvement and fifth grade outcomes, motivation and psychosocial behavior, were non-significant for both groups. Specifically, parent school involvement did not significantly predict fifth grade motivation for children with LD (= -.076, SE = .119, p=.521), or children without LD (= .027, SE = .031, p=.391). Parent school involvement did not significantly predict fifth grade psychosocial behavior for children with LD (= -.041, SE = .069, p=.559), or children without LD (= -.027, SE = .021, p=.200). Additionally, interactions between parent school involvement and kindergarten psychosocial behavior (LD sample: = .219, SE = .136, p=.108; No LD sample: = -.053, SE = .031, p=.088), parent school involvement and kindergarten reading achievement (LD sample: = .012, SE = .013, p=.362; No LD sample: = .000, SE = .001, p=.835), and parent school involvement and kindergarten math achievement (LD sample: = -.018, SE = .012, p=.138; No LD sample: = .001, SE = .001, p=.398) on fifth grade motivation where non-significant for children with LD and children without LD. Interactions between parent school involvement and kindergarten psychosocial behavior (LD sample: = -.049, SE = .090, p=.586; No LD sample: = -.012, SE = .022, p=.566), parent school involvement and kindergarten reading achievement (LD sample: = .003, SE = .005, p=.501; No LD sample: = .001, SE = .001, p=.238), and parent school 164 involvement and kindergarten math achievement (LD sample: = -.005, SE = .004, p=.204; No LD sample: = .000, SE = .001, p=.692) on fifth grade psychosocial behavior where nonsignificant for children with LD and children without LD. Research Question Three: Group Comparison Between Subtypes of LD The third research question sought to explore whether the relation between parent depression and parent school involvement on academic achievement and psychosocial behavior in kindergarten, and academic achievement, psychosocial behavior, and motivation in fifth grade by testing a structural latent variable model in research question one differed between children with RD (n=180), RD-MD (n=230), and children without LD (n=10,220). Overall, results suggest that the theoretical model outlined and tested in research question 1 does not fit the data for subtypes of LD including children with RD and children with RD-MD in this study. Multigroup analysis to examine the difference in model fit, and differences in construct factor loadings and paths was unable to be computed. Testing the Mediation Model Separately for LD Subtype Samples. The same structural model specifying mediation paths (Figure 4) was tested separately for each group. The same covariates, SES, gender, child race, that were significant in the model for research question one, were included in the multi-sample model. Results demonstrated that the model fit the data adequately with children without LD, RMSEA = .012 (CI = .011 - .013), and relative fit indices, CFI = .919, TLI= .911. However, the model did not fit the data well for children with RD, RMSEA = .034 (CI = .017 - .046), and relative fit indices, CFI = .673, TLI= .638. Lastly, the model did not converge for children with RD-MD. Attempts to correct convergence issues were unsuccessful, including addressing negative residual variances, rescaling the residual variances of variables with a value larger than 10, increasing the number of iterations, and freeing specific 165 factor loadings and fixing factor variances. Given that the model had poor fit for one subtype of LD sample, and the aim of this study research question was to examine the fit of the same apriori specified theoretical model across three subtypes of LD, specific changes to the model in research question 1 (e.g., removing non-significant paths) were not completed. Testing the Moderation Model Separately for LD Subtype Samples. To explore whether early academic achievement and psychosocial behavior moderated the relation between parent school involvement and children’s motivation and psychosocial behavior in fifth grade differently for children with RD (n=180), children with RD-MD (n=230), and children without LD (n=10,220) the same structural model specifying moderation paths (Figure 5) was tested separately for each group. The same covariates, SES, gender, child race, that were significant in the model for research question 1b, were included in the multi-sample model. Results demonstrated that the model fit the data for children without LD, as well as for children with RD. As in the LD and no-LD analysis in research question 2b, neither direct paths between parent school involvement and fifth grade motivation or psychosocial behavior or hypothesized moderated paths between parent school involvement, kindergarten achievement, and kindergarten psychosocial behavior were significant for either group. The model was not identified for children with RD-MD. Multi-group analysis across subtypes of LD was not completed due to lack of model fit with all groups and non-significant findings in the full sample and dichotomous LD-no LD sample. 166 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION This study aimed to understand patterns in academic achievement, psychosocial behaviors, and motivation in kindergarten and fifth grade for children with and without learning disability (LD), as well as across subtypes of LD, by testing a theoretical, developmental model of learning and behavior. Given the complexities of identifying LD and categorizing subtypes of LD, many studies have not attempted to address possible group differences, particularly in relation to psychosocial behavior and motivation. However, there is evidence to support that differences in learning and behavioral outcomes do exist across subtypes of LD (Auerbach, Gross-Tsur, Manor, & Shalev, 2008; Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2009; Scarborough & Parker, 2003; Willcutt et al., 2013). Moreover, research and clinical practice have focused on the effectiveness of academic interventions in school to target learning deficits for children with LD, however relatively less is known about the effects of parent variables, including parent depression and parent school involvement, on short- and long-term outcomes for children with LD. Latent variable structural equation modeling allowed for testing predictive pathways between parent variables and children’s outcomes in kindergarten and fifth grade within a nationally representative sample of children entering kindergarten in 1998. This study examined the relations between symptoms of parent depression and parent school involvement in kindergarten on academic achievement in reading and math and psychosocial behavior in kindergarten, as well as student psychosocial behavior and student motivation in fifth grade. This study also examined direct effects of kindergarten achievement and psychosocial behavior on student’s academic achievement, psychosocial behavior, and 167 school motivation in fifth grade. Finally, this study compared these relations across groups of students with and without LD’s. The hypothesized model was only partially supported by the data. Results indicated that symptoms of parent depression did not predict parents’ school involvement as defined by behaviors within the school setting, however parent school involvement was directly related to more positive kindergarten outcomes for children. LD status did not moderate this relationship, suggesting that parent school involvement was a statistically significant factor in promoting more positive kindergarten outcomes for children without LD only. Additionally, parent school involvement in kindergarten had little long-term effect on student’s motivation or psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade. This suggests that this type of parent engagement may confer more short-term benefits for some students and other factors, such as kindergarten achievement and psychosocial behaviors, have stronger effects on later outcomes. Thus, hypotheses involving parent school involvement as a mediator of parent depression on kindergarten outcomes were not supported, nor were effects of parent school involvement on later outcomes across children with and without LD. Additionally, complex relations between kindergarten and fifth grade outcomes arose. Specifically, kindergarten math and reading achievement was predictive of fifth grade math and reading achievement for children without LD, however for children with LD only kindergarten math achievement was predictive of later reading and math performance. The hypothesis that direct relations between kindergarten and fifth grade outcomes would be stronger for children with LD and multiple LD’s was not fully supported. In fact, most relations between kindergarten and fifth grade achievement were stronger for children without LD, with the exception of the relation between kindergarten math achievement and fifth grade reading achievement. These 168 results suggest an important role for developing math proficiency early on, particularly for children with learning disabilities, as higher math proficiency appears to result in long-term academic benefits for all children. Additionally, these results suggest that other factors including, biologically-driven neuropsychological factors and the potential compound effects of long-term academic delays, may have a strong influence the developmental trajectory and later outcomes for children with LD. Furthermore, kindergarten psychosocial behavior was predictive of fifth grade psychosocial behavior for all students in the study sample, as well as fifth grade math achievement and school motivation for children without LD. Thus, efforts to develop student’s non-academic skills including interpersonal skills, executive functioning skills (attention, task persistence, and organization), and social-emotional functioning early in childhood have lasting effects on these behaviors in late-elementary school and resulting academic functioning. Finally, hypotheses regarding the relation between fifth grade psychosocial behaviors, student motivation, and academic achievement were only partially supported, and hypotheses regarding the differential magnitude of these relations being greatest for children with multiple LD’s or single LD were not supported. Psychosocial behavior in fifth grade was related to fifth grade math and reading achievement for children without LD. Moreover, student motivation was predictive of fifth grade math achievement for children with and without LD, however the direction of this relation was opposite for children with LD and children without LD, such that children with LD who rated themselves as more motivated in school were more likely to have lower fifth grade math achievement. Thus, these findings provide counter evidence to previous studies that indicate children with LD demonstrate clinically significantly lower school motivation compared to their peers. 169 This study provides unique contributions to the literature regarding early parent variables and academic and psychosocial outcomes for children with LD as compared to children without LD, and controlled for various demographic variables that affect children’s outcomes using a nationally representative data set and weights to allow results to generalize for the broader population of children entering kindergarten in 1998. The following sections review the study results in detail. Parent Depression and School Involvement as Predictors of Kindergarten Outcomes It was originally hypothesized that higher symptoms of parent depression would predict lower academic achievement and less positive psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten via lower levels of parent school involvement. However, the results of this study did not support this mediational effect for children with or without LD. At the individual item level, children with LD were more likely to have parents who reported worse appetite, feeling “blue,” trouble focusing, feeling depressed, feeling fearful, and talking less compared to children without LD. At the subgroup level, fewer significant differences arose; children with RD or RD-MD had parents who endorsed more problems related to appetite, feeling fearful, and feeling “blue.” These results indicated that parents of children with LD or multiple LD’s may endorse more depressive symptoms. This novel finding contributes significantly to the literature as no other studies have explicitly examined symptoms of depression in parents of children with LD. Additionally, this finding is in line with other studies that have found higher rates of parent psychopathology, including symptoms of anxiety and higher parental stress, in parents of children with LD (Antshel & Joseph, 2006; Karande, Kumbhare, Kulkarni, & Shah, 2009) and other chronic conditions such as, ADHD (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine, 2009). Similarly, parents with lower SES also reported more problematic symptoms of depression. This 170 finding is not surprising given the overwhelming research evidence that poverty is associated with higher rates of depression (Belle, 1990; Carlson & Corcoran, 2001; Johnson & Flake, 2007; McLoyd, 1990; Pachter et al., 2006). It is likely that factors including family SES, and other variables not accounted for in this study such as preschool enrollment, academic intervention, and other home and school characteristics, contribute more significantly to the academic and psychosocial outcomes in kindergarten for children with LD. Despite significant group differences in symptoms of parent depression, hypothesized relations between symptoms of parent depression and kindergarten psychosocial behaviors were not significant. This finding is contrary to research indicating higher parental depression is associated with more psychosocial problems in children in kindergarten and first grade (Ashman, Dawson, & Panagiotides, 2008; Kurstjens & Wolke, 2001). Similarly, parent depression did not significantly predict kindergarten math achievement for children without LD or math or reading achievement for children with LD. These findings were contrary to original study hypotheses as research suggests parental depression is linked to increased psychosocial problems and lower academic achievement in children (Champion et al., 2009; Cortes et al., 2009; Goosby, 2007; Gross, Shaw, Moilanen, Dishion, & Wilson, 2008; Martins & Gaffan, 2000; Mazza et al., 2009; Rice, Lifford, Thomas, & Thapar, 2007). It may be that other demographic factors such as, SES, play a more significant role in predicting early academic achievement and psychosocial behavior. Likewise, factors not included in this study including, school and teacher variables and parent engagement practices at home and in the community, may serve as protective factors buffering children from possible negative influences associated with parent depression. Interestingly, one exception was noted; higher rates of parent depression predicted lower kindergarten reading achievement for children without LD. This finding aligns with research 171 suggesting that increased parent depression is related to poorer academic outcomes for typically developing children. Extant literature suggests that more depressed parents, mothers particularly, may have fewer positive interactions with their child and read less frequently and for shorter duration to their children, which may lead to reduced exposure to language and written text (Bigatti, Cronan, & Anaya, 2001; Sohr-Preston & Scaramella, 2006). This significant finding did not hold true for the LD sample. In this study children with LD represented students with persistent academic difficulties as they either continued to demonstrate academic problems into fifth grade despite potential intervention efforts. This group of students with persistent, severe LD likely demonstrate significant neuropsychological deficits that impair their reading skills and abilities (Pennington & Bishop, 2009), and it is likely that these neurobiological factors are strong predictors of reading achievement in addition to or above and beyond some parenting variables such as, parent depression. This is the first study known to the author to examine parent depression on child outcomes specifically for children with LD and highlights the important role of factors such as, SES and neuropsychological vulnerabilities, in predicting student achievement and behavior. Regarding parent school involvement, group differences emerged between children with LD and children without LD. Parents with children without LD were more likely to attend open houses, PTO meetings, volunteer at school, and participate in school fundraisers. Additionally, parents of children with RD-MD engaged in these activities significantly less than parents of children without LD, and participated in fundraising events and open house events significantly less than parents of children with RD only. Despite group mean differences parent school involvement did not significantly predict kindergarten outcomes for children with LD. Thus, parent school involvement does not serve as a significant protective factor against academic 172 underachievement as originally hypothesized. As with parent depression, the current study is one of few to examine the influence of parent school involvement on outcomes for children with LD. Prior studies (Hill et al., 2004; McLoughlin, Edge, & Strenecky, 1978) have suggested that interactions between families and school staff allow for exchange of important information about children’s development, encourage communication between settings, foster the relationship between families and schools, and lead to more positive outcomes. However, the current findings suggest that parent school involvement as defined by participation in various school events may not be the only, or most significant, avenue by which parents of children with LD engage with their child’s educational milieu. This finding does not suggest that parent school involvement is not important in the larger context of child development nor that the potential additive effects of parent school involvement along with other protective factors (e.g., parent engagement at home, school resources, positive teacher factors, parent belief’s about their child’s education) have a more profound influence on achievement and psychosocial behavior, but rather that parent school involvement as narrowly defined in this study is only a small component of the larger “puzzle” of child development. In contrast to the null findings for children with LD, parent school involvement significantly predicted higher math and reading achievement and more positive psychosocial behavior in kindergarten for children without LD. This finding is consistent with research that highlights the positive relations between parent involvement and early academic skills, psychosocial behaviors and motivation (Christenson, Rounds, & Gorney, 1992; Dickinson & DeTemple, 1998; Gauvain, Savage, & McCollum, 2000; Jeynes 2005, 2007). Parent involvement in school-based activities may help children transition more smoothly from home to school environments and may provide more opportunities for parents and teachers to develop rapport 173 and communicate. Moreover, this finding supports research that suggests that parent participation in school activities is related to children’s engagement in school, particularly in kindergarten (Izzo, Weissberg, Kasprow, & Fendrich, 1999). Although the body of literature on parent’s involvement in their child’s education as it relates to various learning outcomes is far from conclusive, the current study provides some support for the positive effects of fostering opportunities for parents to engage in school-based activities on children’s learning outcomes. As noted in regards to the null effects of parent depression on outcomes for children with LD, the differential findings between children with and without LD may be due in part to the role of neuropsychological deficits in children with LD as a strong driver of outcomes in addition to environmental factors highlighting the complex and powerful role of biological predispositions (Geary et al., 2007; Pennington & Gilger, 1996). In addition to understanding the influence of parent school involvement on kindergarten outcomes, this study explored whether parent school involvement in kindergarten had lasting effects on children’s psychosocial behavior and motivation in fifth grade. The data did not support study hypotheses that higher levels of parent school involvement would predict more positive outcomes in fifth grade as moderated by levels of children’s academic and psychosocial behavior in kindergarten. Parent school involvement was not directly related to children’s psychosocial behavior or motivation in fifth grade for any subset of the sample. Thus, early parent school involvement may serve as a protective factor to promote children’s outcomes in the short-term, however it is likely that complex interactions between other factors, such as continued parental support at home and school, academic achievement, social-emotional development, various home and school characteristics, and neuropsychological deficits, 174 throughout a child’s elementary experience mitigate the academic and psychosocial trajectory for students long-term. Finally, studies have indicated that differences in parent school involvement, defined by participation in school-based activities, and children’s academic and psychosocial outcomes across socio-demographic characteristics as well (Hill & Craft, 2003). These claims were supported by the current study’s results for the typically developing sample as family SES, race, and gender did not significantly contribute to parent school involvement for the LD sample. In this study, parents of typically developing children with higher SES were more involved in school based activities. This finding aligns with previous research suggesting that family SES was a stronger predictor of parent’s involvement in school and at home (Grolnick et al., 1997), and suggests that families with lower SES do not access schools in the same way that families with higher SES do. For example, families with lower SES may lack adequate transportation to attend school functions, may have less flexibility in work hours, and may not have access to childcare for other children, among other potential barriers. Schools that provide multiple modes of communication (e.g., sending home memos regarding important school events, providing information via phone, internet, and in writing), demonstrate more flexibility in accommodating parent’s various needs (e.g., scheduling day and evening meetings, incorporating weekday and weekend open houses, etc.), and support parent’s efforts to develop relationships with educators and other parents in various ways (e.g., PTA/PTO, online parent groups, community-based activities) will likely not only be more successful in promoting parent school involvement across families from all SES’s, but may also promote the academic achievement and psychosocial functioning of all students. 175 Differences in parent school involvement and academic achievement across racial groups also arose. Parents of African-American/Black children and Hispanic children were more likely to be less involved in school-based activities across the full sample. This finding supports some research that suggests African-American parents may be less involved in direct school activities compared to other parents (Kohl et al., 2000; Reynolds, Weissberg, & Kasprow, 1992) despite other research indicating no differences across racial groups (Harris, Kagey, & Ross, 1987). Research, including this study, examining ethnic/cultural differences is often confounded by socioeconomic variables (Hill & Craft, 2003), thus, it may be that some parents of AfricanAmerican or Hispanic children have lower SES and the effects of poverty lead to barriers to parent school involvement (e.g., transportation, access to childcare, work hours, access to information via internet or other communication modes). Additionally, different cultural values and beliefs may drive parent’s school involvement behavior. For example, Hill and Craft (2003) note that African American parents may place a higher value on monitoring their children’s school performance and behavior and intervening within the home context, but not intervening within the school context directly via school-based events. Research suggests that parent school involvement in related to improved achievement for African-American/Black students (Hill et al., 2004), however further research is needed to examine cultural variables that influence parent’s school involvement behaviors across ethnic/cultural groups. In regards to early academic achievement, children identified as African-American/Black and Native American had lower kindergarten math scores, and children identified as Hispanic had lower kindergarten reading scores. For Native American and Hispanic children with LD, these relations were stronger as compared to their non-LD peers. Children from higher SES households had higher achievement in kindergarten and fifth grade. These differences highlight 176 deeply saddening and inequitable economic and racial disparities in student achievement from a young age that have been highlighted in the research for more than thirty years (Reardon & Galindo, 2009). Schools that implement policies and procedures to understand and address the barriers that parents face in accessing school-based activities and practices that promote achievement for disadvantaged youth (e.g., early childhood education opportunities), particularly historically marginalized minorities, may be more successful at closing the racial disparity gap and improving outcomes for all children. Kindergarten Achievement and Psychosocial Behavior as Predictors of Fifth Grade Outcomes Kindergarten Academic Achievement. A third aim of the current study was to examine the longitudinal effect of kindergarten achievement and psychosocial behavior on fifth grade outcomes for children with and without LD. Basic group differences emerged. As expected, children with one LD had lower kindergarten academic achievement than children without LD, and children with multiple LD’s had the lowest academic achievement. The effect sizes for these group differences was large and reflects the core nature of LD; difficulty in academic learning and achievement. Interestingly, kindergarten reading achievement predicted fifth grade academic achievement only for children without LD. However, kindergarten math achievement predicted fifth grade achievement for children with and without LD. The magnitude of the relationship between kindergarten math achievement and fifth grade reading achievement was stronger for children with LD. This finding provides evidence to support the importance of developing reading and math skills early on and is in line with previous research demonstrating the predictive value of kindergarten achievement on later academic achievement (Claessens, 177 Duncan, & Engel, 2009; Duncan et al., 2007; Claessens & Engel, 2013; Jordan, Kaplan, Ramineni, & Locuniak, 2009). However, this finding also suggests a unique role that early mathematics learning and proficiency plays in predicting and promoting children’s long-term achievement, particularly for children with or at-risk of developing LD. Several education advocacy groups have put forth position statements espousing the importance of early mathematics learning for long-term academic attainment (National Association for the Education of Young Children; NAEYC, 2002; National Council of Teachers of Mathematics; NCTM; 2007; National Mathematics Advisory Panel; NMAP, 2008). Moreover, researchers have theorized about mechanisms by which early mathematics learning contributes to children’s improved learning disposition overall. For example, children with higher levels of math skills may receive more reinforcement from the teacher. In turn, this may lead to more individual instruction time and/or grouping with higher ability students (Claessens & Engel, 2013). Furthermore, research suggests an uncoupling of intellectual functioning and reading performance such that children with LD possess generally intact cognitive functioning, however demonstrate marked difficulties in reading (Ferrer, Shaywitz, Holahan, Marchione, & Shaywitz, 2010). Thus, it is likely that reading performance in kindergarten does not predict future academic performance, particularly in the LD sample, due to an uncoupling in general cognitive skills that have greater predictive value on future academic performance. Given that a majority of children with LD in this sample had an RD, this effect may be even more pronounced in this study. Similarly, mathematics ability may serve as a proxy for general cognitive functioning (i.e., IQ) and thus may retain predictive value in estimating future academic performance. Higher math skills may foster development of basic neuropsychological abilities that also serve 178 development of reading skills such as sustained attention, working memory, and problem-solving skills (Geary, 2004; Geary 2006). Finally, early reading skills taught and emphasized in kindergarten tend to encompass more associative learning processes as opposed to more assembled learning processes required for many early math skills. For example, in kindergarten children are expected to memorize the orthographic symbols associated with each letter in the alphabet, make associations between the orthographic symbol and sound of a letter, and begin to blend and parse letter sounds. These skills involve the ability to retrieve information from semantic memory and create associations, or associative processing. In contrast, while basic learned math facts also involve associative learning, early mathematics skills such as mentally representing a finite set of objects, counting objects, and comparing quantities require abstract numerical representations requiring assembled learning processes (Dennis, Landry, Barnes, & Fletcher, 2006). Thus, in kindergarten children at risk for persistent learning difficulties may not be as readily differentiated from their peers without learning difficulties in terms of reading ability due to the more basic nature of the reading task. Moreover, the degree of associative learning required to be a proficient reader in kindergarten may not have the same predictive weight on long-term learning outcomes as more assembled learning processes. Assembled learning processes that are required for many early math skills and support higher-order learning in reading and math (e.g., reading comprehension, arithmetic, etc.) likely do differentiate children with and without learning disabilities earlier in their schooling trajectory and thus are more predicative of long-term academic achievement. Thus, it is especially important to bolster young student’s math skills, in addition to reading skills, that may target neuropsychological processes that are involved in assembled learning 179 processes to promote their long-term achievement, particularly for those students who have or are at-risk of developing an LD in the future. In regards to fifth grade psychosocial outcomes, higher kindergarten math achievement in children without LD was predictive of more positive fifth grade psychosocial behavior, as defined by children’s approaches to learning, interpersonal skills, self-control, and externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors. This finding supports previous studies that suggest early academic performance affects children’s interpersonal skills, mood, and behavior (Fleming, Harachi, Cortes, Abbott, & Catalano, 2004; Lane, Beebe-Frankenberger, Lambros, & Pierson, 2001; Morgan, Farkas, & Wu, 2012; Wehby, Falk, Barton-Arwood, Lane, & Cooley, 2003). Unexpectedly, the results of this current study did not indicate that academic performance in kindergarten was predictive of later psychosocial behavior for children with or at-risk of developing LD. Although children with LD demonstrated lower psychosocial behavior in fifth grade compared to their non-LD peers in this study, the effect sizes were small for most components of psychosocial behavior (interpersonal skills, self-control, externalizing and internalizing behavior), except for approaches to learning (large effect size). There is some research to suggest that children with LD are more vulnerable to psychosocial problems (Gadeyne, Ghesquiere, & Onghena, 2004; La Greca & Stone, 1990; Maughan, Pickles, Hagell, Rutter, & Yule, 1996; Ochoa & Palmer, 1995). For example, it is established that many children with RD meet criteria for at least one behavioral or emotional disorder such as, ADHD (Goldstone et al., 2007, Maughan et al., 2003). However, in this study, children with co-morbid LD and ADHD were removed resulting in a “pure” LD sample. It may be that for children with or at-risk of developing an LD other factors, such as co-morbid ADHD and increasing academic demands throughout elementary school, mediate the relation between early academic 180 performance and later psychosocial behaviors. Additionally, children with or at-risk of LD may have lower approaches to learning (attention, task engagement, organization) and lower academic achievement in kindergarten, however it may be the compound effects of repeated academic underachievement from first grade through fifth grade that mediates the outcome of psychosocial behaviors in fifth grade, rather than solely their baseline performance in kindergarten. Kindergarten Psychosocial Behavior. Children with or at-risk of developing LD performed lower on all components of kindergarten psychosocial behavior compared to their non-LD peers, however effect sizes were small except for approaches to learning (large effect size) and interpersonal skills (medium effect size). Only small effect sizes were found in terms of differences in psychosocial behavior between subtypes of LD, such that children with RD performed higher on approaches to learning and lower on internalizing problem behaviors compared to children with RD-MD. As noted above, this finding is aligned with extant literature suggesting that children with or at-risk of LD demonstrate lower levels of task engagement, attention, organization, and interpersonal skills compared to their non-LD peers (Goldston et al., 2007; Martinez & Semrud-Clikeman, 2004; Willcutt et al., 2011), and indicate the importance of and need for interventions targeted at improving executive functioning and social skills in children with LD. Moreover, kindergarten psychosocial behavior predicted fifth grade psychosocial behavior for children with and without LD. This finding suggests that there is a long-term effect of early psychosocial behaviors, and fostering children’s ability to attend and engage in academic content, as well as work alongside other children is equally important to promote more positive long-term outcomes for all students. 181 Despite the positive direct influence of kindergarten psychosocial behavior on fifth grade psychosocial behavior, kindergarten psychosocial behavior only predicted fifth grade math achievement and student-rated motivation for children without LD in fifth grade. For typically developing children, this finding suggests that more positive kindergarten psychosocial behaviors lead to more positive fifth math achievement, and ability beliefs and intrinsic motivation in school. This finding is expected given the established support for long-term effects of psychosocial behavior on later academic achievement and motivation (Moore, Lippman, & Ryberg, 2015; Pintrich, 2000; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Pintrich & Zusho, 2002), and provides support for initiatives that focus on developing positive psychosocial behaviors early on. As with other null findings in the LD-only sample, it may be that other factors not accounted for by this study, such as academic underachievement in first through fourth grade, teacher, classroom, and school variables, level of academic remediation throughout elementary school, presence of co-morbid conditions (e.g., ADHD), and measures of friendship and social support, are more predictive of the relation between kindergarten psychosocial behavior and fifth grade achievement and motivation for children with LD. Moreover, academic expectations and psychosocial behavior expectations increase over time; in kindergarten children may be expected to attend to short lessons, have less materials to remember and organize, and are often guided through many social situations, whereas in mid-to-late elementary school the psychosocial demands are much greater (e.g., managing multiple classrooms and teachers, higher cognitive load, greater social demands). Thus, children at-risk of developing or with an LD may be rated by their teachers as having kindergarten psychosocial skills that are highly related with their academic achievement in kindergarten, however the developmental trajectory of psychosocial behaviors and academic achievement may become uncoupled over time such that teacher’s 182 perceptions of student’s psychosocial behaviors in kindergarten are not predictive of fifth grade achievement due to the changing nature of academic expectations and psychosocial demands. Finally, several demographic patterns related to levels of kindergarten psychosocial behavior appeared in this study. First, children’s age at kindergarten entry significantly predicted psychosocial behavior, such that older children had higher levels of psychosocial behavior. This finding suggests that age-related maturation plays a strong role in children’s attention, school engagement, organizational skills, interpersonal skills, self-control, and lower externalizing and internalizing problems. Moreover, female students were more likely to have higher levels of psychosocial behavior, and the magnitude of this relation was stronger for children with LD compared to children without LD. There is evidence to suggest that gender differences in psychosocial behavior exist (Duckworth & Seligman, 2006; Matthews, Ponitz, & Morrison, 2009; Nowell & Hedges, 1998; Weaver-Hightower, 2003). Given that psychosocial behavior was based on teacher ratings, rater bias effects may also influence these findings. For example, teachers are more likely to rate males as having higher externalizing behaviors. Additionally, children identified as African-American/Black had lower levels of psychosocial behavior compared to their peers (the magnitude of this relationship was even stronger for AfricanAmerican/Black children with or at-risk of developing LD). The racial disparity in teacher-rated psychosocial behaviors may also be due in part to rater bias (Wright, 2015), but also parallels previous research suggesting that a racial gap in academic achievement exists between minority students, particularly African-American/Black students, and their peers. This finding highlights several important considerations that should continue to be the subject of future research and educational system policy including understanding the implicit bias in teacher-ratings of minority student’s psychosocial behaviors (Gilliam, Maupin, Reyes, Accavitti, & Shic, 2016), 183 the relation between early academic problems and early psychosocial behaviors in AfricanAmerican/Black children, and the dire need for teacher training programs and schools to better understand the factors behind racial achievement gaps and to intervene early to promote student’s psychosocial behaviors, particularly for minority students. Fifth Grade Psychosocial Behavior and Motivation as Predictors of Academic Achievement The fourth and final aim of this study was to examine the relation between fifth grade psychosocial behavior and student-rated motivation on fifth grade achievement for children with and without LD. Not surprisingly, children with LD had significantly lower academic achievement compared to their non-LD peers in fifth grade. Children with LD also had lower psychosocial behavior compared to non-LD peers, however the effect size was small for most components except approaches to learning (large effect size) and there were no significant differences between subtypes of LD as compared to children without LD. Interestingly, more positive fifth grade psychosocial behavior predicted higher academic achievement in reading and math for children without LD only. This finding is unique in that this study suggests that effects of fifth grade psychosocial behavior on achievement operate differentially for children with and without LD. Again, part of this variation may be explained by rater effects and little variation in psychosocial behaviors across children with and without LD, as compared to great variation in academic achievement between the two sample groups. In other words, teachers did not rate students with LD as having significantly lower psychosocial behaviors in most areas compared to their non-LD peers therefore psychosocial behaviors may explain relatively little to no variance in achievement above and beyond the core nature of LD (i.e., academic difficulty due to neuropsychological deficits). Additionally, other research has found that only approaches to learning significantly predicts later academic achievement (Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, & Sperling, 184 2008), thus it may be that only specific components of psychosocial behavior contribute to academic achievement. Regarding motivation, children with LD reported lower motivation in school compared to their non-LD peers however effect sizes were small. Higher levels of self-reported motivation, as defined by ability beliefs and intrinsic motivation, were predictive of higher fifth grade math for children without LD. However, the opposite was true for children with LD, such that higher levels of self-reported motivation were predictive of lower levels of math achievement in fifth grade. These findings may suggest several phenomena. First, there may be issues related to reliability of self-report measures in children that require caution when interpreting the relation between student’s motivational beliefs and their academic outcomes (Fulmer & Frijters, 2009). Second, children with LD may accurately perceive themselves as hard workers, and in many cases potentially work harder in some academic subjects than their non-LD peers, and thus may view themselves as highly intrinsically motivated in school and capable of completing the work despite difficulties associated with their learning disability and lower academic grades/achievement. Finally, it may be that children with LD with the greatest academic underachievement endorse more positive ability beliefs and intrinsic motivation if they wish to project to others that that they are highly motivated in school and have a high level of selfefficacy despite significant academic struggles. Overall, these findings support prior research suggesting that children with LD do not demonstrate significantly lower intrinsic motivation or more negative ability beliefs compared to their non-LD peers (Wilson & David, 1994; Zisimopoulos & Galanaki, 2009). Efforts to continue to maintain student’s motivation in school despite academic underachievement are warranted; these results are encouraging in the sense that 185 children whom experience academic underachievement do not necessarily experience low school motivation. Finally, differences in fifth grade academic achievement and psychosocial behaviors emerged across socio-demographic groups. The relation between higher SES and higher fifth grade reading achievement was stronger for children with LD compared to children without LD, and paralleled findings between higher SES and higher kindergarten achievement. This finding indicates that the effects of poverty continue to affect children’s academic achievement in later elementary school and contribute to a stable economic gap in achievement without our schools. Similarly, differences in academic achievement and psychosocial across racial groups persisted in fifth grade. Specifically, Hispanic students had lower reading achievement, AfricanAmerican/Black students had lower math achievement and lower psychosocial behavior, and Asian students had higher psychosocial behavior. Interestingly, research suggests that similar mechanisms involved in rater bias that lead African-American/Black, Hispanic, and Native American students with lower teacher-rated psychosocial behaviors, also contribute to higher teacher-rated psychosocial behaviors across Asian students (Chang & Demyan, 2007). Again, these findings suggest that parity has not been achieved across all racial and SES groups in terms of academic and psychosocial outcomes in early and late elementary school and highlights the need for greater attention to issues of diversity and equity within educational systems. Finally, a significant effect of gender was found for fifth grade math achievement, as males were more likely to have higher math achievement compared to females. These findings suggest gender differences in math are present for typically developing children in fifth grade and align with research suggesting females are at-risk for lower math achievement compared to their male peers (Fryer & Levitt, 2010; Jordan, Kaplan, Nabor Olah, & Locuniak, 2006). As with racial and 186 economic disparities, this finding is disheartening and confirms the need for further research to examine the factors that may contribute to this difference in late elementary school. For example, so research indicates that female students may internalize societal messages that discourage them from pursuing mathematics education and activities which has led to a surge of efforts to engage females in science, math, and technology programs at earlier ages (Hyde, Lindberg, Linn, Ellis, & Williams, 2008). Conclusions and Clinical Implications Conclusions about the predictive nature of parent variables on kindergarten outcomes for children with or at-risk of developing LD, as well as effects of kindergarten and fifth grade achievement and psychosocial behavior on fifth grade achievement for children with and without LD were established. Although, parent depression did not affect parents school involvement, parent depression was related to kindergarten reading achievement suggesting an important role for intact parental mental health in supporting children’s achievement. Clinically, school and mental health professionals involved in the community should be alert for symptoms of parent depression and offer resources and support early on. Additionally, while parent school involvement was not directly related to fifth grade psychosocial behavior and motivation, it was related to children’s psychosocial behavior and achievement for children without LD. School policies and procedures that address barriers to parents involvement in school-based activities, and provide additional opportunities for parents to connect and communicate with their children’s teachers and school staff outside traditional school activities would likely benefit more students across socio-demographic groups, and potentially for parents of children with disabilities, including LD. For children without LD, it appears that kindergarten math and reading achievement and early psychosocial behaviors effect later outcomes, however for 187 children with LD it appears that kindergarten math achievement has a particularly strong predictive value on later achievement. Programs that focus on increasing early math skills, as well as early psychosocial behavior, and interventions that target the core neuropsychological deficits that characterize a child’s LD will likely support the greatest academic and psychosocial outcomes for children with LD. Limitations and Future Research There are several limitations of this study due, in part, to the non-experimental nature of the data. First, no causal inferences can be made about parent involvement in school or parental depression on academic or psychosocial outcomes because an experimental design is necessary to establish the extent to which altering early parent involvement, treating parent’s psychological problems, or intervening in early academic achievement would influence child outcomes in fifth grade. A second limitation is that although the ECLS-K began with a representative sample of students, due to missing data, the representativeness of the final sample may have been affected. A third limitation involves identification of students as LD. Since information about how the LD was diagnosed (e.g., assessment procedures and data points) are not available, there may be students who are inaccurately identified as LD or represent higher or lower functioning within the heterogeneous spectrum of students with LD. However, the purpose of this study was to examine whether students identified with LD in school, and presumably receiving services for the LD and having awareness of their LD label, differed from students without LD in terms of motivation, psychosocial behavior, and achievement, as well as early parent variables, and not necessarily the validity of the method used to diagnose the student’s LD. Finally, there are limitations involved in teacher and self-report of behaviors, cognitions, and moods, such as rater bias and developmental limitations on metacognition and accuracy of self-reflection. 188 Given that the long-term effects of kindergarten achievement and psychosocial behavior on fifth grade outcomes was mixed for children with LD particularly, future research should examine interim time points (e.g., first grade, third grade) to examine if there are pivotal time points at which children who have or are at-risk of developing LD begin to show marked decreases in skills. This type of research may inform the timing of interventions to mitigate the negative effects of early academic underachievement and lower psychosocial behavior on future outcomes. Additionally, future research should examine other aspects of home and school environments that may play an important role in predicting achievement and psychosocial outcomes for children with LD. These factors may include home literacy, enrollment in preschool, type and effect of school academic intervention, and social support/relationship variables. Finally, children with LD are a heterogeneous group, and although this study was unable to examine the predictive pathways between kindergarten and fifth outcomes for subtypes of children with LD, future studies should continue to examine differences in predicting variables and outcomes for children across children with subtypes of LD. 189 APPENDIX 190 191 REFERENCES 192 REFERENCES Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, & Oosterlaan J. (2009). Metaanalysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics, 124(2), 717–728. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2816 Adelman, H.S. (1978). The concept of intrinsic motivation: Implications for practice and research with the learning disabled. Learning Disability Quarterly, 1(2), 43-54. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1510306 Adelman, H.S., & Taylor, L. (1986). An introduction to learning disabilities. Scott, Foresman & Co., Glenview: IL. Algozzine, B., Horner, R., & Putnam, R. (2008). Which came first: The achievement or the behavior problem. Charlotte, NC: Behavior and Reading Improvement Center. Algozzine, B., Wang, C., & Violette, A. S.. (2011). Reexamining the relationship between academic achievement and social behavior. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13(1), 3-16. doi: 10. 1177/1098300709359084 American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2013b). Specific Learning Disorder Fact Sheet. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association. Retrieved from: http://www.dsm5.org/Documents/Specific%20Learning%20Disorder%20Fact%20Sheet. pdf Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivation processes. Journal of educational psychology, 80(3), 260-267. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.260 Ames, C., Khoju, M., and Watkins, T. (1993). Parent Involvement: The Relationship Between School-to-Home Communication and Parents’ Perceptions and Beliefs (Report No. 15). Urbana, IL: Center on Families, Communities, Schools, and Children’s Learning, Illinois University. Retrieved from: http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED362271.pdf Antshel, K. M., & Joseph, G. R. (2006). Maternal Stress in Nonverbal Learning Disorder A Comparison With Reading Disorder. Journal of learning disabilities, 39(3), 194-205. doi:10.1177/00222194060390030101 Arnold, E. M., Goldston, D. B., Walsh, A. K., Reboussin, B. A., Daniel, S. S., Hickman, E., & Wood, F. B. (2005). Severity of emotional and behavioral problems among poor and 193 typical readers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(2), 205-217. doi:10.1007/s10802-005-1828-9 Arthur, A. R. (2003). The emotional lives of people with learning disability. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(1), 25-30. doi:10.1046/j.1468-3156.2003.00193.x Ashcraft, M. H., Yamashita, T. S., & Aram, D. M. (1992). Mathematics performance in left and right brain-lesioned children and adolescents. Brain and Cognition, 19(2), 208-252. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(92)90046-O Ashman, S. B., Dawson, G., & Panagiotides, H. (2008). Trajectories of maternal depression over 7 years: Relations with child psychophysiology and behavior and role of contextual risks. Development and psychopathology, 20(1), 55-77. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000035 Auerbach, J. G., Gross-Tsur, V., Manor, O., & Shalev, R. S. (2008). Emotional and behavioral characteristics over a six-year period in youths with persistent and nonpersistent dyscalculia. Journal of learning disabilities, 41(3), 263-273. doi:10.1177/0022219408315637 Badian, N. A. (1983). Dyscalculia and nonverbal disorders of learning. Progress in learning disabilities, 5, 235-264. Badian, N. A. (1999). Persistent arithmetic, reading, or arithmetic and reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia, 49(1), 43-70. doi: 10.1007/s11881-999-0019-8 Baird, G. L., Scott, W. D., Dearing, E., & Hamill, S. K. (2009). Cognitive self-regulation in youth with and without learning disabilities: Academic self-efficacy, theories of intelligence, learning vs. performance goal preferences, and effort attributions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28(7), 881-908. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2009.28.7.881 Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 50(2), 248-287. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90022-L Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Gerbino, M., & Pastorelli, C. (2003). Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in diverse spheres of psychosocial functioning. Child development, 769-782. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00567 Barkauskiene, R. (2009). The role of parenting for adjustment of children with and without learning disabilities: a person-oriented approach. Learning Disabilities: A Contemporary Journal, 7(2), 1-17. Barrouillet, P., Fayol, M., & Lathulière, E. (1997). Selecting between competitors in multiplication tasks: An explanation of the errors produced by adolescents with learning difficulties. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21(2), 253-275. doi:10.1080/016502597384857 194 Beitchman, J. H., & Young, A. R. (1997). Learning disorders with a special emphasis on reading disorders: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(8), 1020-1032. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199708000-00009 Belle, D. (1990). Poverty and women's mental health. American psychologist, 45(3), 385-389. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.45.3.385 Bender, W. N., & Smith, J. K. (1990). Classroom behavior of children and adolescents with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of learning disabilities, 23(5), 298-305. doi:10.1177/002221949002300509 Bender, W. N., & Wall, M. E. (1994). Social-emotional development of students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 17(4), 323-341. doi: 10.2307/1511128 Berch, D. B., & Mazzocco, M. M. (2007). Why Is Math So Hard for Some Children? The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Learning Difficulties and Disabilities. Brookes Publishing: Baltimore, MD. Bhate, S., & Wilkinson, S. (2006). Aetiology of learning disability. Psychiatry, 5(9), 298-301. doi: 10.1053/j.mppsy.2006.08.001 Bigatti, S. M., Cronan, T. A., & Anaya, A. (2001). The effects of maternal depression on the efficacy of a literacy intervention program. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 32(2), 147-162. Black, F. (1974). Self-concept as related to achievement and age in LD children. Child Development. 45, 1137-1140. Blackorby, J., Wagner, M., Cameto, R., Davies, E., Levine, P., Newman, L., Marder, C. & Sumi, C. (2005). Engagement, academics, social adjustment, and independence: The achievements of elementary and middle school students with disabilities. Menlo Park, CA: SRI International. Retrieved from: http://www.seels.net/designdocs/engagement/All_SEELS_outcomes_10-04-05.pdf Bloom, B. S. (1980). The new direction in educational research: Alterable variables. Journal of Negro Education, 337-349. doi: 10.2307/2295092 Bodovski, K., & Youn, M. J. (2011). The long term effects of early acquired skills and behaviors on young children’s achievement in literacy and mathematics. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 9(1), 4-19. doi: 10.1177/1476718X10366727 Bodovski, K., & Youn, M. J. (2010). Love, discipline and elementary school achievement: The role of family emotional climate. Social Science Research, 39(4), 585-595. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.03.008 195 Boetsch, E. A., Green, P. A., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Psychosocial correlates of dyslexia across the life span. Development and Psychopathology, 8(03), 539-562. doi:10.1017/S0954579400007264 Borkowski, J. G., & Thorpe, P. K. (1994). Self-regulation and motivation: A life-span perspective on underachievement. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Selfregulation of learning and performance: Issues and educational applications (45 -73). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bouffard-Bouchard, T., Parent, S., & Larivee, S. (1991). Influence of self-efficacy on selfregulation and performance among junior and senior high-school age students. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 14(2), 153-164. doi:10.1177/016502549101400203 Brady, S. A. (1991). The role of working memory in reading disability. In S.A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 129-152). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bramlett, R. K., Smith, B. L., & Edmonds, J. (1994). A comparison of nonreferred, learning‐ disabled, and mildly mentally retarded students utilizing the social skills rating system. Psychology in the Schools, 31(1), 13-19. doi:10.1002/15206807(199401)31:1<13::AID-PITS2310310103>3.0.CO;2-E Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: The Guilford Press. Bub, K. L., McCartney, K., & Willett, J. B. (2007). Behavior problem trajectories and first-grade cognitive ability and achievement skills: A latent growth curve analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 653-670. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00220663.99.3.653 Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A metaanalysis of experimental training studies. Journal of educational psychology, 91(3), 403414. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.403 Butkowsky, I. S., & Willows, D. M. (1980). Cognitive-motivational characteristics of children varying in reading ability: Evidence for learned helplessness in poor readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72(3), 408-422. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00220663.72.3.408 Butler, D. L. (1998). Metacognition and learning disabilities. In B. Y. Wong (Ed.), Learning about learning disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 277–307). New York: Academic Press. Butler, D.L. (1999). Identifying and remediating students’ inefficient approaches to tasks. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association in Montreal, Quebec. 196 Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2000). Phonological skills and comprehension failure: A test of the phonological processing deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 13(1-2), 31-56. doi:10.1023/A:1008051414854 Carlson, M. J., & Corcoran, M. E. (2001). Family structure and children's behavioral and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63(3), 779-792. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2001.00779.x Capano, L., Minden, D., Chen, S. X., Schachar, R. J., & Ickowicz, A. (2008). Mathematical learning disorder in school-age children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53(6), 392-399. Carroll, J. M., Maughan, B., Goodman, R., & Meltzer, H. (2005). Literacy difficulties and psychiatric disorders: Evidence for comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(5), 524-532. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00366.x Cervone, D., Kopp, D. A., Schaumann, L., & Scott, W. D. (1994). Mood, self-efficacy, and performance standards: Lower moods induce higher standards for performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 499-512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00223514.67.3.499 Champion, J. E., Jaser, S. S., Reeslund, K. L., Simmons, L., Potts, J. E., Shears, A. R., Compas, B. E. (2009). Caretaking behaviors by adolescent children of mothers with and without a history of depression. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(2), 156–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014978 Chang, D. F., & Demyan, A. L. (2007). Teachers' stereotypes of Asian, Black, and White students. School Psychology Quarterly, 22(2), 91. Chaplin, T. M., & Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 735-765. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030737 Chapman, J. W. (1988). Cognitive-motivational characteristics and academic achievement of learning disabled children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 357-365. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.357 Chapman, J. W., Tunmer, W. E., & Prochnow, J. E. (2000). Early reading-related skills and performance, reading self-concept, and the development of academic self-concept: A longitudinal study. Journal of educational psychology, 92(4), 703-708. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.703 Chatterji, M. (2006). Reading achievement gaps, correlates, and moderators of early reading achievement: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS) kindergarten to first grade sample. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 489-507. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.489 197 Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233-255. Christenson, S. L. (2000). Families and schools: Rights, responsibilities, resources, and relationship. In R.C. Pianta & M. J. Cox (Eds.), The transition to kindergarten (pp. 143177). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Christenson, S. L., Rounds, T., & Gorney, D. (1992). Family factors and student achievement: An avenue to increase students' success. School Psychology Quarterly, 7(3), 178-206. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0088259 Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., Ewing‐Cobbs, L., Barnes, M. A., & Fuchs, L. S. (2007). Cognitive arithmetic differences in learning difficulty groups and the role of behavioral inattention. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice,22(1), 25-35. doi: 10.1111/j.15405826.2007.00228.x Claessens, A., Duncan, G., & Engel, M. (2009). Kindergarten skills and fifth-grade achievement: Evidence from the ECLS-K. Economics of Education Review, 28(4), 415-427. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2008.09.003 Claessens, A. & Engel, M. (2013). How important is where you start? Early mathematics knowledge and later school success. Teachers College Record, 115, 1-29. Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2009). Learning trajectories in early mathematics–sequences of acquisition and teaching. Encyclopedia of language and Literacy Development, 1-7. Retrieved from: http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/sites/default/files/textesexperts/en/784/learning-trajectories-in-early-mathematics-sequences-of-acquisition-andteaching.pdf Cohen, J. (1986). Learning disabilities and psychological development in childhood and adolescence. Annals of Dyslexia, 36(1), 287-300. doi:10.1007/BF02648035 Cooley, E. J., & Ayres, R. R. (1988). Self-concept and success-failure attributions of nonhandicapped students and students with learning disabilities. Journal of learning disabilities, 21(3), 174-178. doi: 10.1177/002221948802100310 Compton, D. L., Defries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2001). Are RAN-and phonological awarenessdeficits additive in children with reading disabilities?. Dyslexia, 7(3), 125-149. doi:10.1002/dys.198 Conderman, G. (1995). Social status of sixth- and seventh-grade students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 18(1), 13-24. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1511362 198 Cortes, R. C., Fleming, C. B., Mason, W. A., & Catalano, R. F. (2009). Risk factors linking maternal depressed mood to growth in adolescent substance use. Journal of emotional and behavioral disorders, 17(1), 49-64. doi: 10.1177/1063426608321690 Cortiella, C. & Horowitz, S. H. (2014). The State of Learning Disabilities: Facts, Trends and Emerging Issues. New York: National Center for Learning Disabilities. Retrieved from: https://www.ncld.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/11/2014-State-of-LD.pdf Coutinho, M. J., Oswald, D. P., & Best, A. M. (2002). The influence of sociodemographics and gender on the disproportionate identification of minority students as having learning disabilities. Remedial and Special Education, 23(1), 49-59. Coyne, L. W., Low, C. M., Miller, A. L., Seifer, R., & Dickstein, S. (2007). Mothers’ empathic understanding of their toddlers: Associations with maternal depression and sensitivity. Journal of Child and Family Studies,16(4), 483-497. doi: 10.1007/s10826006-9099-9 Crombie, G. (1988). Gender differences: Implications for social skills assessment and training. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 17(2), 116-120. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp1702_2 Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child development, 82(3), 766-779. doi:10.1111/j.14678624.2010.01529.x Daniel, S. S., Walsh, A. K., Goldston, D. B., Arnold, E. M., Reboussin, B. A., & Wood, F. B. (2006). Suicidality, school dropout, and reading problems among adolescents. Journal of learning disabilities, 39(6), 507-514. doi: 10.1177/00222194060390060301 Deci, E. L., & Chandler, C. L. (1986). The importance of motivation for the future of the LD field. Journal of learning disabilities, 19(10), 587-594. doi:10.1177/002221948601901003 Deci, E., Hodges, R., Pierson, L., & Tomassone, J. (1992). Motivationally relevant predictors of achievement and adjustment in learning disabled and emotionally handicapped students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25(1), 457-471. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being across life's domains. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 49(1), 14-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14 Demaray, M. K., Ruffalo, S. L., Carlson, J., Busse, B. T., Olson, A. E., McManus, S. M., & Leventhai, A. (1995). Social skills assessment: A comparative evaluation of six published 199 rating scales. School Psychology Review, 24, 648-671. Retrieved from http://ezproxy.msu.edu.proxy1.cl.msu.edu/login?url=http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.c l.msu.edu/docview/62341407?accountid=12598 Dennis, M., Landry, S. H., Barnes, M., & Fletcher, J. M. (2006). A model of neurocognitive function in spina bifida over the life span. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(02), 285-296. Denton, K., & West, J. (2002). Children's reading and mathematics achievement in kindergarten and first grade. National Center for Education Statistics, Washington, DC. Retrieved from: http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED461438.pdf Dickinson, D. K., & De Temple, J. (1998). Putting parents in the picture: Maternal reports of preschoolers' literacy as a predictor of early reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(2), 241-261. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(99)80037-4 Dietz, L. J., Jennings, K. D., Kelley, S. A., & Marshal, M. (2009). Maternal depression, paternal psychopathology, and toddlers' behavior problems. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 38(1), 48-61. doi: 10.1080/15374410802575362 DiPerna, J. C., Lei, P. W., & Reid, E. E. (2007). Kindergarten predictors of mathematical growth in the primary grades: An investigation using the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten cohort. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(2), 369. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.369 Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2006). Self-discipline gives girls the edge: Gender in selfdiscipline, grades, and achievement test scores. Journal of educational psychology, 98(1), 198-208. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198 Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., Pagani, L., Feinstein, L., Engel, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Sexton, H. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental psychology, 43(6), 1428-1446. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428.supp DuPaul, G. J., Gormley, M. J., & Laracy, S. D. (2013). Comorbidity of LD and ADHD: Implications of DSM-5 for assessment and treatment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(1), 43-51. Dweck, C. S., & Elliott, E. S. (1983). Achievement motivation. In P. Mussen, & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Social and personality development (pp. 643–691). New York: Wiley. Ebejer, J. L., Coventry, W. L., Byrne, B., Willcutt, E. G., Olson, R. K., Corley, R., & Samuelsson, S. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on inattention, hyperactivity-impulsivity, and reading: Kindergarten to grade 2. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14(4), 293-316. doi: 10.1080/10888430903150642 200 Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual review of psychology, 53(1), 109-132. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153 Ehri, L. (2015). How children learn to read words. In A. Pollatsek & R. Trieman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Reading (pp. 293-310). Oxford University Press. Epstein, J. L. (1994). Perspectives and previews on research and policy for school, family, and community partnerships. Paper presented at the National Symposium: Family-school links: How do they effect educational outcomes?. Pennsylvania State University. Evans, G. W. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. American psychologist, 59(2), 7792. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77 Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and students' academic achievement: A metaanalysis. Educational psychology review, 13(1), 1-22. doi: 10.1023/A:1009048817385 Fantuzzo, J., Bulotsky-Shearer, R., McDermott, P., McWayne, C., Frye, D., & Perlman, S. (2007). Investigation of dimensions of social-emotional classroom behavior and school readiness for low-income urban preschool children. School Psychology Review, 36(1), 44-62. Fayol, M., Barrouillet, P., & Marinthe, C. (1998). Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuro-psychological performance: A longitudinal study. Cognition, 68(2), B63-B70. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00046-8 Fehrmann, P. G., Keith, T. Z., & Reimers, T. M. (1987). Home influence on school learning: Direct and indirect effects of parental involvement on high school grades. The Journal of Educational Research, 80(6), 330-337. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1987.10885778 Ferrer, E., Shaywitz, B. A., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2010). Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: Empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychological science, 21(1), 93-101. Fincham, F. D., & Cain, K. M. (1986). Learned helplessness in humans: A developmental analysis. Developmental Review, 6(4), 301-333. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(86)90016-X Fleming, C. B., Harachi, T. W., Cortes, R. C., Abbott, R. D., & Catalano, R. F. (2004). Level and change in reading scores and attention problems during elementary school as predictors of problem behavior in middle school. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 12(3), 130-144. doi: 10.1177/10634266040120030101 Fletcher, J. M., Coulter, W. A., Reschly, D. J., & Vaughn, S. (2004). Alternative approaches to the definition and identification of learning disabilities: Some questions and answers. Annals of dyslexia, 54(2), 304-331. 201 Frick, P. J., Kamphaus, R. W., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Christ, M. A. G., Hart, E. L., & Tannenbaum, L. E. (1991). Academic underachievement and the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 59(2), 289-294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.59.2.289 Froiland, J. M., & Oros, E. (2014). Intrinsic motivation, perceived competence, and classroom engagement as longitudinal predictors of adolescent reading achievement. Educational Psychology, 34(2), 119-132. doi:10.1080/01443410.2013.822964 Froiland, J. M., Peterson, A., & Davison, M. L. (2012). The long-term effects of early parent involvement and parent expectation in the USA. School Psychology International, 34(1), 33-50. doi: 10.1177/0143034312454361 Froiland, J. M., Powell, D. R., Diamond, K. E., & Son, S. H. C. (2013). Neighborhood socioeconomic well-being, home literacy, and early literacy skills of at-risk preschoolers. Psychology in the Schools, 50(8), 755-769. doi: 10.1002/pits.21711 Fryer, R. G., & Levitt, S. D. (2004). Understanding the black-white test score gap in the first two years of school. Review of Economics and Statistics, 86(2), 447-464. doi:10.1162/003465304323031049 Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Bryant, V. J., Hamlett, C. L., & Lambert, W. (2012). First-grade cognitive abilities as long-term predictors of reading comprehension and disability status. Journal of learning disabilities,45(3), 217-231. doi:10.1177/0022219412442154 Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Powell, S. R., Seethaler, P. M., Capizzi, A. M., Schatschneider, C., & Fletcher, J. M. (2006). The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in arithmetic, algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 29-43. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.29 Fulmer, S. M., & Frijters, J. C. (2009). A review of self-report and alternative approaches in the measurement of student motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 21(3), 219-246. Gadeyne, E., Ghesquiere, P., & Onghena, P.. (2004). Psychosocial functioning of young children with learning problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 510-521. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00241.x Gauvain, M., Savage, S., & McCollum, D. (2000). Reading at home and at school in the primary grades: Cultural and social influences. Early Education and Development, 11(4), 447463. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1104_5 Gersten, R., Jordan, N. C., & Flojo, J. R.. (2005). Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 293 – 304. doi: 10.1177/00222194050380040301 202 Gaddes, W. H. (2013). Learning disabilities and brain function: A neuropsychological approach. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media. Geary, D. C. (1993). Mathematical disabilities: cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Psychological bulletin, 114(2), 345-362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00332909.114.2.345 Geary, D. C. (2004). Mathematics and learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 4 – 15. doi:10.1177/00222194040370010201 Geary, D. C. (2006). Development of mathematical understanding. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, Perception, and Language (Vol. 2, pp. 777-810). Hoboken, NHL Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0218 Geary, D. C., Bow-Thomas, C. C., & Yao, Y. (1992). Counting knowledge and skill in cognitive addition: A comparison of normal and mathematically disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54(3), 372-391. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(92)90026-3 Geary, D. C., Brown, S. C., & Samaranayake, V. A. (1991). Cognitive addition: A short longitudinal study of strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in normal and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 27(5), 787-797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.787 Geary, D. C., Hamson, C. O., & Hoard, M. K. (2000). Numerical and arithmetical cognition: A longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability. Journal of experimental child psychology, 77(3), 236-263. doi:10.1006/jecp.2000.2561 Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child Development, 78(4), 1343-1359. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01069.x Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., & Hamson, C. O. (1999). Numerical and arithmetical cognition: Patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. Journal of experimental child psychology, 74(3), 213-239. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2515 Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Bailey, D. H. (2012). Mathematical cognition deficits in children with learning disabilities and persistent low achievement: A five-year prospective study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 206-223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025398 Gelman, R., & Meck, E. (1983). Preschoolers' counting: Principles before skill. Cognition, 13(3), 343-359. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90014-8 Gilliam, W. S., Maupin, A. N., Reyes, C. R., Accavitti, M., & Shic, F. (2016). Do Early Educators’ Implicit Biases Regarding Sex and Race Relate to Behavior Expectations and 203 Recommendations of Preschool Expulsions and Suspensions?. Yale Child Study Center, September, 991-1013. Goldstein, D., Paul, G. G., & Sanfilippo-Cohn, S. (1985). Depression and achievement in subgroups of children with learning disabilities. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6(4), 263-275. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(85)90001-2 Goldston, D. B., Walsh, A., Arnold, E. M., Reboussin, B., Daniel, S. S., Erkanli, A., Nutter, D., Hickman, E., Palmes, G., Snider, E., & Wood, F. B. (2007). Reading problems, psychiatric disorders, and functional impairment from mid-to late adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(1), 25-32. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000242241.77302.f4 Gonzalez-DeHass, A. R., Willems, P. P., & Holbein, M. F. D. (2005). Examining the relationship between parental involvement and student motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 17(2), 99-123. doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-3949-7 Goodman, S. H. (2007). Depression in mothers. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3(1), 107-135. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091401 Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, M. R., Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clinical child and family psychology review, 14(1), 1-27. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1 Goosby, B. J. (2007). Poverty duration, maternal psychological resources, and adolescent socioemotional outcomes. Journal of Family Issues, 28(8), 1113-1134. doi:10.1177/0192513X07300712 Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2003). Students with learning disabilities and the process of writing: A meta-analysis of SRSD studies. In H. Swanson, K. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of Learning Disabilities (pp. 323-344). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Grant, A. M. (2008). Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? Motivational synergy in predicting persistence, performance, and productivity. Journal of applied psychology, 93(1), 48-58. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.48 Green, K. B., & Gallagher, P. A.. (2014). Mathematics for young children: A review of the literature with implications for children with disabilities. Baskent University Journal of Education, 1(1), 81-92. Retrieved from: http://buje.baskent.edu.tr/index.php/buje/article/viewFile/14/12 Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L., & Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students' cognitive engagement and achievement: Contributions of classroom perceptions and motivation. Contemporary educational psychology, 29(4), 462-482. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.01.006 204 Greenham, S. L. (1999). Learning disabilities and psychosocial adjustment: A critical review. Child neuropsychology, 5(3), 171-196. doi: 10.1076/chin.5.3.171.7335 Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Griffiths, Y. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2002). Predictors of exception word and nonword reading in dyslexic children: The severity hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 34-43. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.34 Grimm, K. J. (2008). Longitudinal associations between reading and mathematics achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 410-426. doi:10.1080/87565640801982486 Grolnick, W. S., Benjet, C., Kurowski, C. O., & Apostoleris, N. H. (1997). Predictors of parent involvement in children's schooling. Journal of educational psychology, 89(3), 538. Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1990). Self-Perceptions, Motivation, and Adjustment in Children with Learning Disabilities A Multiple Group Comparison Study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23(3), 177-184. doi: 10.1177/002221949002300308 Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents' involvement in children's schooling: A multidimensional conceptualization and motivational model. Child development, 65(1), 237-252. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00747.x Gross, H. E., Shaw, D. S., Burwell, R. A., & Nagin, D. S. (2009). Transactional processes in child disruptive behavior and maternal depression: A longitudinal study from early childhood to adolescence. Development and psychopathology, 21(01), 139-156. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000091 Gross, H. E., Shaw, D. S., & Moilanen, K. L. (2008). Reciprocal associations between boys’ externalizing problems and mothers’ depressive symptoms. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 36(5), 693-709. Gross, H. E., Shaw, D. S., Moilanen, K. L., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2008). Reciprocal models of child behavior and depressive symptoms in mothers and fathers in a sample of children at risk for early conduct problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(5), 742751. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013514 Gross‐Tsur, V., Manor, O., & Shalev, R. S. (1996). Developmental dyscalculia: Prevalence and demographic features. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 38(1), 25-33. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1996.tb15029.x Guay, F., Boggiano, A. K., & Vallerand, R. J. (2001). Autonomy support, intrinsic motivation, and perceived competence: Conceptual and empirical linkages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(6), 643-650. doi: 10.1177/0146167201276001 205 Guthrie, J. T., Hoa, A. L. W., Wigfield, A., Tonks, S. M., Humenick, N. M., & Littles, E. (2007). Reading motivation and reading comprehension growth in the later elementary years. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32(3), 282-313. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.05.004 Guthrie, J. T., Schafer, W. D., & Huang, C. W. (2001). Benefits of opportunity to read and balanced instruction on the NAEP. The Journal of Educational Research, 94(3), 145-162. doi: 10.1080/00220670109599912 Hanich, L. B., Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., & Dick, J. (2001). Performance across different areas of mathematical cognition in children with learning difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(3), 615-626. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.615 Hardre P. & Reeve J. (2003) A motivational model of rural students intentions to persist in, versus drop out of high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 347–356. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.347 Harris, L., Kagey, M., & Ross, J. (1987). A child resource policy: Moving beyond dependence on school and family. Phi Delta Kappan, 68, 575– 580. Harris-Murri, N., King, K., & Rostenberg, D. (2006). Reducing disproportionate minority representation in special education programs for students with emotional disturbances: Toward a culturally responsive response to intervention model. Education and Treatment of Children, 779-799. Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in everyday parenting and intellectual development in young American children. Baltimore. MD: Brookes. Harter, S., Whitesell, N. R., & Junkin, L. J. (1998). Similarities and differences in domainspecific and global self-evaluations of learning-disabled, behaviorally disordered, and normally achieving adolescents. American Educational Research Journal, 35(4), 653680. Hassiotis, A. (1997). Parents of young persons with learning disability: An application of the Family Adaptability and Cohesion Scale (FACES II). British Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 43, 36-42. doi: 10.3102/00028312035004653 Heiervang, Anders Lund, Jim Stevenson, Kenneth Hugdahl, E. (2001). Behaviour problems in children with dyslexia. Nordic journal of psychiatry, 55(4), 251-256. Hill, N. E., Castellino, D. R., Lansford, J. E., Nowlin, P., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2004). Parent academic involvement as related to school behavior, achievement, and aspirations: Demographic variations across adolescence. Child development, 75(5), 14911509. 206 Hill, N. E., & Craft, S. A. (2003). Parent-school involvement and school performance: Mediated pathways among socioeconomically comparable African American and Euro-American families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 74. Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological bulletin, 111(1), 127-155. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127 Hitch, G. J., & McAuley, E. (1991). Working memory in children with specific arithmetical learning difficulties. British Journal of Psychology, 82(3), 375-386. doi: 10.1111/j.20448295.1991.tb02406.x Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. (2008). Evaluating model fit: a synthesis of the structural equation modelling literature. In A. Brown (Ed.), 7th European Conference on research methodology for business and management studies. Paper presented at the European Conference on Research Methods for Business and Management, London, UK, 195-200. Howell, R., Sidorenko, E., & Jurica, J. (1987). The effects of computer use on the acquisition of multiplication facts by a student with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20(6), 336-340. doi: 10.1177/002221948702000606 Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., & Williams, C. C. (2008). Gender similarities characterize math performance. Science, 321(5888), 494-495. Individuals With Disabilities Education Act, 20 U.S.C. § 1400 (2004). Izzo, C. V., Weissberg, R. P., Kasprow, W. J., & Fendrich, M. (1999). A longitudinal assessment of teacher perceptions of parent involvement in children's education and school performance. American journal of community psychology, 27(6), 817-839. doi:10.1023/A:1022262625984 Jeynes, W. H. (2005). A meta-analysis of the relation of parental involvement to urban elementary school student academic achievement. Urban education, 40(3), 237-269. doi:10.1177/0042085905274540 Jeynes, W. H. (2007). The relationship between parental involvement and urban secondary school student academic achievement a meta-analysis. Urban education, 42(1), 82-110. doi: 10.1177/0042085906293818 Johnson, P. L., & Flake, E. M. (2007). Maternal depression and child outcomes. Pediatric annals, 36(4), 196-202. doi: 10.3928/0090-4481-20070401-08 207 Jordan, N. C., Hanich, L. B., & Kaplan, D. (2003). Arithmetic fact mastery in young children: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of experimental child psychology, 85(2), 103-119. doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00032-8 Jordan, N. C., Hanich, L. B., & Kaplan, D. (2003b). A longitudinal study of mathematical competencies in children with specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid mathematics and reading difficulties. Child development, 74(3), 834-850. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00571 Jordon, N. C., Kaplan, D., & Hanich, L. B. (2002). Achievement growth in children with learning difficulties in mathematics: Findings of a two-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(3), 586-597. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00220663.94.3.586 Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Nabors Oláh, L., & Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: A longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child development, 77(1), 153-175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00862.x Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 850-867. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014939 Jordan, N. C., & Montani, T. O. (1997). Cognitive Arithmetic and Problem Solving A Comparison of Children with Specific and General Mathematics Difficulties. Journal of learning disabilities, 30(6), 624-634. doi: 10.1177/002221949703000606 Jorm, A. F., Share, D. L., Matthews, R., & Maclean, R. (1986). Behaviour problems in specific reading retarded and general reading backward children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(1), 33-43. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.14697610.1986.tb00619.x Judge, S. & Bell, S. M. (2010). Reading achievement trajectories for students with learning disabilities during the elementary school years. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 27(1-2), 153 – 178. doi: 10.1080/10573569.2011.532722 Judge, S., & Watson, S. M. R.. (2011). Longitudinal outcomes for mathematics achievement for students with learning disabilities. The Journal of Educational Research, 104(3), 147157. doi: 10.1080/00220671003636729 Karande, S., Kumbhare, N., Kulkarni, M., & Shah, N. (2009). Anxiety levels in mothers of children with specific learning disability. Journal of postgraduate medicine, 55(3), 165170. doi: 10.4103/0022-3859.57388 Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1996). Social skill deficits and learning disabilities: A metaanalysis. Journal of learning disabilities, 29(3), 226-237. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221949602900301 208 Keith, T. Z., Keith, P. B., Quirk, K. J., Cohen-Rosenthal, E., & Franzese, B. (1996). Effects of Parental Involvement Oil Achievement for Students Who Attend School in Rural America. Journal of Research in Rural Education, 12(2), 55-67. Keith, T. Z., Keith, P. B., Quirk, K. J., Sperduto, J., Santillo, S., & Killings, S. (1998). Longitudinal effects of parent involvement on high school grades: Similarities and differences across gender and ethnic groups. Journal of School Psychology, 36(3), 335363. Kistner, J. A., Osborne, M., & LeVerrier, L. (1988). Causal attributions of learning-disabled children: Developmental patterns and relation to academic progress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(1), 82-89. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.1.82 Klauda, S. L., & Guthrie, J. T. (2008). Relationships of three components of reading fluency to reading comprehension. Journal of Educational psychology, 100(2), 310-321. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.310 Kline, R. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. Kloomok, S., & Cosden, M. (1994). Self-concept in children with learning disabilities: The relationship between global self-concept, academic “discounting,” nonacademic selfconcept, and perceived social support. Learning Disability Quarterly, 17(2), 140-153. doi: 10.2307/1511183 Knopik, V. S., Alarcón, M., & DeFries, J. C. (1997). Comorbidity of mathematics and reading deficits: Evidence for a genetic etiology. Behavior Genetics, 27(5), 447-453. doi:10.1023/A:1025622400239 Kohl, G. O., Lengua, L. J., McMahon, R. J., & the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2000). Parent involvement in school: Conceptualizing multiple dimensions and their relations with family and demographic risk factors. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 501–523. Konstantareas, M. M., & Homatidis, S. (1989). Parental perception of learning-disabled children's adjustment problems and related stress. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17(2), 177-186. doi: 10.1007/BF00913792 Kovacs, M. (1984). The Children's Depression, Inventory (CDI).Psychopharmacology bulletin, 21(4), 995-998. Kovas, Y., Haworth, C. M., Dale, P. S., Plomin, R., Weinberg, R. A., Thomson, J. M., & Fischer, K. W. (2007). The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early school years. Monographs of the Society for research in Child Development, i-156. doi: http://www.jstor.org/stable/30163178 209 Kuhne, M., & Wiener, J. (2000). Stability of social status of children with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 23(1), 64-75. doi: 10.2307/1511100 Kurstjens, S., & Wolke, D. (2001). Effects of maternal depression on cognitive development of children over the first 7 years of life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(05), 623-636. doi: 10.1017/S0021963001007296 Kwon, K., Kim, E., Sheridan, S. (2012). Behavioral competence and academic functioning among early elementary children with externalizing problems. School Psychology Review, 41(2), 123-140. La Greca, A. M., & Stone, W. L. (1990). LD Status and Achievement Confounding Variables in the Study of Children's Social Status, Self-Esteem, and Behavioral Functioning. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23(8), 483-490. doi: 10.1177/002221949002300806 Landerl, K., Fussenegger, B., Moll, K., & Willburger, E. (2009). Dyslexia and dyscalculia: Two learning disorders with different cognitive profiles. Journal of experimental child psychology, 103(3), 309-324. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.03.006 Landerl, K., & Moll, K. (2010). Comorbidity of learning disorders: prevalence and familial transmission. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(3), 287-294. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02164.x Lane, K. L., Beebe‐Frankenberger, M. E., Lambros, K. M., & Pierson, M. (2001). Designing effective interventions for children at‐risk for antisocial behavior: An integrated model of components necessary for making valid inferences. Psychology in the Schools, 38(4), 365-379. doi: 10.1002/pits.1025 Langberg, J. M., Vaughn, A. J., Brinkman, W. B., Froehlich, T., & Epstein, J. N. (2010). Clinical utility of the Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scale for ruling out comorbid learning disorders. Pediatrics, 126(5), e1033-e1038. Leadbeater, B. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Blatt, S. J., & Hertzog, C. (1999). A multivariate model of gender differences in adolescents' internalizing and externalizing problems. Developmental psychology, 35(5), 1268-1282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00121649.35.5.1268 Lee, J., Grigg, W., & Donahue, P. (2007). The nation’s report card: Reading. (NCES 2007-496). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U. S. Department of Education. Lepola, J., Poskiparta, E., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2005). Development of and relationship between phonological and motivational processes and naming speed in predicting word recognition in grade 1. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(4), 367-399. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0904_3 210 Lepola, J., Salonen, P., & Vauras, M. (2000). The development of motivational orientations as a function of divergent reading careers from pre-school to the second grade. Learning and Instruction, 10(2), 153-177. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(99)00024-9 Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations in the classroom: age differences and academic correlates. Journal of educational psychology, 97(2), 184-196. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/00220663.97.2.184 Lerner, J., & Johns, B. (2009). Learning disabilities and related mild disabilities (11th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. Lewis, S. K., & Lawrence-Patterson, E. (1989). Locus of control of children with learning disabilities and perceived locus of control by significant others. Journal of learning disabilities, 22(4), 255-257. doi: 10.1177/002221948902200410 Light, J. G., & DeFries, J. C. (1995). Comorbidity of Reading and Mathematics Disabilities Genetic and Environmental Etiologies. Journal of learning disabilities, 28(2), 96-106. doi: 10.1177/002221949502800204 Lipka, O., Lesaux, N. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). Retrospective analyses of the reading development of grade 4 students with reading disabilities risk status and profiles over 5 years. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(4), 364-378. doi:10.1177/00222194060390040901 Lipman, E. L., Boyle, M. H., Dooley, M. D., & Offord, D. R. (2002). Child well-being in singlemother families. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(1), 75-82. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200201000-00014 Lin, Y. C., Morgan, P. L., Hillemeier, M., Cook, M., Maczuga, S., & Farkas, G. (2013). Reading, Mathematics, and Behavioral Difficulties Interrelate: Evidence from a Cross-Lagged Panel Design and Population-Based Sample of US Upper Elementary Students. Behavioral Disorders, 212-227. doi: http://www.jstor.org/stable/23890649 Lopes, J., Cruz, C., & Rutherford, R. B. (2002). The relationship of peer perceptions to student achievement and teacher ratings of 5th and 6th grade students. Education and treatment of children, 476-495. doi: http://www.jstor.org/stable/42899723 Margalit, M. (1998). Loneliness and coherence among preschool children with learning disabilities. Journal of learning disabilities, 31(2), 173-180. doi:10.1177/002221949803100207 Marjoribanks, K. (1983). The evaluation of a family learning environment model. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 9(3), 343-351. doi: 10.1016/0191-491X(83)90017-2 211 Marsh, H. W. (1992). Self Description Questionnaire (SDQ) II: A theoretical and empirical basis for the measurement of multiple dimensions of adolescent self-concept: An interim test manual and a research monograph. New South Wales, Australia: University of Western Sydney, Faculty of Education. Martinez, R. S., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2014). Emotional adjustment and school functioning of young adolescents with multiple versus single learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37(5), 411-420. doi: 10.1177/00222194040370050401 Martins, C., & Gaffan, E. A. (2000). Effects of early maternal depression on patterns of infant– mother attachment: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(06), 737-746. Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early gender differences in selfregulation and academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 689704. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014240 Maughan, B., Pickles, A., Hagell, A., Rutter, M., & Yule, W. (1996). Reading problems and antisocial behaviour: Developmental trends in comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(4), 405-418. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01421.x Maughan, B., Rowe, R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2003). Reading problems and depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(2), 219-229. doi:10.1023/A:1022534527021 Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2006). Frequency of reading, math, and writing disabilities in children with clinical disorders. Learning and individual Differences, 16(2), 145-157. Mazza, J. J., Abbott, R. D., Fleming, C. B., Harachi, T. W., Cortes, R. C., Park, J., Haggerty, K. P., & Catalano, R. F. (2009). Early predictors of adolescent depression a 7-year longitudinal study. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 29(5), 664-692. doi:10.1177/0272431608324193 Mazzocco, M. M. (2001). Math Learning Disability and Math LD Subtypes Evidence from Studies of Turner Syndrome, Fragile X Syndrome, and Neurofibromatosis Type 1. Journal of learning disabilities, 34(6), 520-533. doi: 10.1177/002221940103400605 Mazzocco, M. M., & Kover, S. T. (2007). A longitudinal assessment of executive function skills and their association with math performance. Child Neuropsychology, 13(1), 18-45. doi:10.1080/09297040600611346 Mazzocco, M. M., & Myers, G. F. (2003). Complexities in identifying and defining mathematics learning disability in the primary school-age years. Annals of dyslexia, 53(1), 218-253. doi: 10.1007/s11881-003-0011-7 212 McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2006). The impact of kindergarten learning-related skills on academic trajectories at the end of elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21(4), 471-490. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.09.003 McClelland, M. M., Morrison, F. J., & Holmes, D. L. (2000). Children at risk for early academic problems: The role of learning-related social skills. Early childhood research quarterly, 15(3), 307-329. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(00)00069-7 McGee, R., Prior, M., Williams, S., Smart, D., & Sanson, A. (2002). The long‐term significance of teacher‐rated hyperactivity and reading ability in childhood: findings from two longitudinal studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(8), 1004-1017. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00228 McGeown, S., Goodwin, H., Henderson, N., & Wright, P. (2012). Gender differences in reading motivation: does sex or gender identity provide a better account?. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(3), 328-336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01481.x McIntosh, K. (2005). Academic, behavioral, and functional predictors of chronic problem behavior in elementary grades (Research Brief). Eugene, OR: Research Center on School Wide Positive Behavior Support. McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of experimental child psychology, 74(3), 240260. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1999.2516 McLoughlin, J. A., Edge, D., & Strenecky, B. (1978). Perspective on parental involvement in the diagnosis and treatment of learning disabled children. Journal of learning disabilities, 11(5), 291-296. doi: 10.1177/002221947801100505 McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child development, 61(2), 311-346. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02781.x McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American psychologist, 53(2), 185-204. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185 McNamara, J. K., Scissons, M., & Dahleu, J. (2005). A longitudinal study of early identification markers for children at-risk for reading disabilities: The Matthew effect and the challenge of over-identification. Reading Improvement, 42(2), 80-97. McWayne, C., Fantuzzo, J., Cohen, H. L., & Sekino, Y. (2004). A multivariate examination of parent involvement and the social and academic competencies of urban kindergarten children. Psychology in the Schools, 41(3), 363-377. doi: 10.1002/pits.10163 McWayne, C. M., Fantuzzo, J. W., & McDermott, P. A. (2004). Preschool competency in context: an investigation of the unique contribution of child competencies to early 213 academic success. Developmental psychology, 40(4), 633-645. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.633 Meece, J. L., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Hoyle, R. H. (1988). Students' goal orientations and cognitive engagement in classroom activities. Journal of educational psychology, 80(4), 514-523. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.80.4.514 Meltzer, L., Katzir, T., Miller, L., Reddy, R., & Roditi, B. (2004). Academic self‐perceptions, effort, and strategy use in students with learning disabilities: Changes over time. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19(2), 99-108. doi: 10.1111/j.15405826.2004.00093.x Miedel, W. T., & Reynolds, A. J. (2000). Parent involvement in early intervention for disadvantaged children: Does it matter?. Journal of School Psychology, 37(4), 379-402. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(99)00023-0 Miller, C. J., Hynd, G. W., & Miller, S. R. (2005). Children with dyslexia: Not necessarily at risk for elevated internalizing symptoms. Reading and Writing,18(5), 425-436. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-4314-4 Mishna, F. (1996). In their own words: therapeutic factors for adolescents who have learning disabilities. International journal of group psychotherapy,46(2), 265-273. Moes, D., Koegel, R. L., Schreibman, L., & Loos, L. M. (1992). Stress profiles for mothers and fathers of children with autism. Psychological Reports, 71(3f), 1272-1274. doi:10.2466/pr0.1992.71.3f.1272 Moore, K. A., Lippman, L. H., & Ryberg, R. (2015). Improving outcome measures other than achievement. AERA (Open), 1(2), 1-25. doi: 10.1177/2332858415579676 Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M. M., & Maczuga, S. (2014). Who is at risk for persistent mathematics difficulties in the United States? Journal of Learning Disabilities,1-15. doi: 10.1177/0022219414553849 Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Tufis, P. A., & Sperling, R. A.. (2008). Are reading and behavior problems risk factors for each other? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(5), 417-436. doi:10.1177/0022219408321123 Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., & Wu, Q. (2009). Five-year growth trajectories of kindergarten children with learning difficulties in mathematics. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(4), 306-321. doi: 10.1177/0022219408331037 Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., & Wu, Q.. (2012). Do poor readers feel angry, sad, and unpopular? Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(4), 360-381. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2011.570397 214 Murphy, M. M., Mazzocco, M. M., Hanich, L. B., & Early, M. C. (2007). Cognitive characteristics of children with mathematics learning disability (MLD) vary as a function of the cutoff criterion used to define MLD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(5), 458478. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2015). Mplus User’s Guide Version 7. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. National Association for the Education of Young Children and National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NAEYC and NCTM). (2002). Early childhood mathematics: Promoting good beginnings (Position Statement). Retrieved from http://www.naeyc.org/positionstatements/mathematics National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM). (2007). What is important in early childhood mathematics? (Position Statement). Retrieved from http://www.nctm.org/ about/content.aspx?id=12590 National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Foundations for success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1992). The 1989 socioeconomic index of occupations: Construction from the 1989 occupational prestige scores. Chicago: National Opinion Research Center. National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. (2009). Depression in parents, parenting, and children: Opportunities to improve identification, treatment, and prevention. In M. J. England & L. J. Sim (Eds.), Committee on depression, parenting practices, and the healthy development of children. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. Nelson, J. M., & Harwood, H. (2011). Learning disabilities and anxiety: A metaanalysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44(1), 3-17. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219409359939 Nolen, S. B. (1988). Reasons for studying: Motivational orientations and study strategies. Cognition and instruction, 5(4), 269-287. doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci0504_2 Nowell, A., & Hedges, L. V. (1998). Trends in gender differences in academic achievement from 1960 to 1994: An analysis of differences in mean, variance, and extreme scores. Sex Roles, 39(1-2), 21-43. doi: 10.1023/A:1018873615316 Ochoa, S. H., & Palmer, D. J. (1991). A sociometric analysis of between-group differences and within-group status variability of Hispanic learning disabled and nonhandicapped pupils in academic and play contexts. Learning Disability Quarterly, 14(3), 208-218. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1510850 215 Ochoa, S. H., & Palmer, D. J. (1995). Comparison of the peer status of Mexican-American students with learning disabilities and non-disabled low-achieving students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 18(1), 57-63. doi: 10.2307/1511365 Ochoa, S. H., & Olivarez, A. (1995). A meta-analysis of peer rating sociometric studies of pupils with learning disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 29(1), 1-19. doi:10.1177/002246699502900101 O’Connor, C., & Fernandez, S. D. (2006). Race, class, and disproportionality: Reevaluating the relationship between poverty and special education placement. Educational Researcher, 35(6), 6-11. Ohlsson, S., & Rees, E. (1991). The function of conceptual understanding in the learning of arithmetic procedures. Cognition and Instruction, 8(2), 103-179. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0802_1 Olson, R., Forsberg, H., Wise, B., & Rack, J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition, orthographic, and phonological skills. In R. G. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 243-277). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing. Onatsu-Arvilommi, T., & Nurmi, J. E. (2000). The role of task-avoidant and task-focused behaviors in the development of reading and mathematical skills during the first school year: A cross-lagged longitudinal study. Journal of educational psychology, 92(3), 478491. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.3.478 Osman, B. B. (2000). Learning disabilities and the risk of psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents. In L. Greenhill (Ed.), Learning disabilities: Implications for psychiatric treatment, (pp. 33-57), Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press. Pachter, L. M., Auinger, P., Palmer, R., & Weitzman, M. (2006). Do parenting and the home environment, maternal depression, neighborhood, and chronic poverty affect child behavioral problems differently in different racial-ethnic groups?. Pediatrics, 117(4), 1329-1338. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1784 Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of educational research, 66(4), 543-578. doi: 10.3102/00346543066004543 Paris, S. G., & Oka, E. R. (1986). Children's reading strategies, metacognition, and motivation. Developmental Review, 6(1), 25-56. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(86)90002-X Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Short-term memory, working memory, and inhibitory control in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem solving. Journal of experimental child psychology, 80(1), 44-57. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2626 216 Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2004). Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. Journal of experimental child psychology, 88(4), 348-367. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.04.002 Pearl, R. (1982). LD children's attributions for success and failure: A replication with a labeled LD sample. Learning Disability Quarterly, 5(2), 173-176. doi: 10.2307/1510578 Pearl, R. (1992). Psychosocial characteristics of learning disabled students. In N. N. Singh & I. L. Beale (Eds.), Learning disabilities: Nature, theory, and treatment (pp. 96–125). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Pennington, B. F. (2009). Diagnosing learning disorders: A neuropsychological framework. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Pennington, B. F., & Bishop, D. V. (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual review of psychology, 60(1), 283-306. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548 Pennington, B. F., & Gilger, J. W. (1996). How is dyslexia transmitted? In C. H. Chase, G. D. Rosen, & G. F. Sherman (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Neural, cognitive, and genetic mechanisms (pp. 41-61), Baltimore, MD: York Press. Pianta, R. C., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Cox, M. J. (1999). Introduction: An ecological approach to kindergarten transition. In R. C. Pianta & M. J. Cox (Eds.), The transition to kindergarten (pp. 3-12). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing. Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 451-502). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Pintrich, P. R., Anderman, E. M., & Klobucar, C. (1994). Intraindividual differences in motivation and cognition in students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(6), 360-370. doi: 10.1177/002221949402700603 Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of educational psychology, 82(1), 33-40. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33 Pintrich, P. R., & Zusho, A. (2002). The development of academic self-regulation: The role of cognitive and motivational factors. In A. Wigfield & J. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation: A volume in the education psychology series (pp. 249-284). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Piper B. J., Gray H. M., Birkett M. A. (2012). Maternal smoking cessation and reduced academic and behavioral problems in offspring. Drug Alcohol Dependence, 121(1–2), 62–67. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.08.004 217 Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Jenner, A. R., Katz, L., Frost, S. J., Lee, J. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2001). Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. Journal of communication disorders, 34(6), 479-492. doi: 10.1016/S00219924(01)00060-0 Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (2000). Phonological processing, not inhibitory control, differentiates ADHD and reading disability. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(4), 485-494. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200004000-00018 Räsänen, P., & Ahonen, T. (1995). Arithmetic disabilities with and without reading difficulties: A comparison of arithmetic errors. Developmental Neuropsychology, 11(3), 275-295. doi:10.1080/87565649509540620 Rathvon, N. (2004). Early reading assessment: A practitioner’s handbook. New York: Guilford Press. Reardon, S. F., & Galindo, C. (2009). The Hispanic-White achievement gap in math and reading in the elementary grades. American Educational Research Journal, 46(3), 853-891. Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Prediction of Dropout Among Students With Mild Disabilities A Case for the Inclusion of Student Engagement Variables. Remedial and Special Education, 27(5), 276-292. doi: 10.1177/07419325060270050301 Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2002). The clinician's guide to the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC). Guilford Press. Reynolds, C.R. and Kamphaus, R.W. (2006). BASC-2: Behavior Assessment System for Children, Second Edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc. Reynolds, A. J., Weissberg, R. P., & Kasprow, W. (1992). Predictions of early social and academic adjustment of children from the inner-city. American Journal of Community Psychology, 20, 599–624. Rice, F., Lifford, K. J., Thomas, H. V., & Thapar, A. (2007). Mental health and functional outcomes of maternal and adolescent reports of adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(9), 1162-1170. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e3180cc255f Rogers, H., & Saklofske, D. H. (1985). Self-concepts, locus of control and performance expectations of learning disabled children. Journal of learning Disabilities, 18(5), 273278. doi: 10.1177/002221948501800505 Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Strobel, K. R. (1998). Linking the study of schooling and mental health: Selected issues and empirical illustrations at the level of the individual. Educational Psychologist, 33(4), 153-176. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3304_2 218 Roodenrys, S., Koloski, N., & Grainger, J. (2001). Working memory function in attention deficit hyperactivity disordered and reading disabled children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19(3), 325-337. doi: 10.1348/026151001166128 Ross, G., Sammaritano, L., Nass, R., & Lockshin, M. (2003). Effects of mothers' autoimmune disease during pregnancy on learning disabilities and hand preference in their children. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine, 157(4), 397-402. doi:10.1001/archpedi.157.4.397 Rourke, B. P. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities: The syndrome and the model. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Rourke, B. P., & Fuerst, D. R. (1992). Psychosocial dimensions of learning disability subtypes: Neuropsychological studies in the Windsor Laboratory. School Psychology Review, 21(3), 361-374. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American psychologist, 55(1), 68-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68 Sabornie, E. J. (1994). Social-affective characteristics in early adolescents identified as learning disabled and nondisabled. Learning Disability Quarterly, 17(4), 268-279. doi:10.2307/1511124 Sanders, M. G. (1998). The effects of school, family, and community support on the academic achievement of African American adolescents. Urban Education, 33(3), 385–409. doi:10.1177/0042085998033003005 Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child development, 61(6), 1728-1743. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb03562.x Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading difficulties: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B.K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A review of the spectrum (pp. 75-199). Timonium, MD: York Press. Scarborough, H. S., & Parker, J. D. (2003). Matthew effects in children with learning disabilities: Development of reading, IQ, and psychosocial problems from grade 2 to grade 8. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 47-71. doi: 10.1007/s11881-003-0004-6 Schunk, D. (1985). Self-efficacy and classroom learning. Psychology in the schools, 22(2), 208223. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(198504)22:2<208::AIDPITS2310220215>3.0.CO;2-7 219 Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J. A., Hudson, E., & Lawson, E. P. (1996). Knowledge of storybooks as a predictor of young children's vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 520-536. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.88.3.520 Shalev, R. S., Auerbach, J., & Gross‐Tsur, V. (1995). Developmental dyscalculia behavioral and attentional aspects: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(7), 1261-1268. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01369.x Shalev, R. S., Auerbach, J., Manor, O., & Gross-Tsur, V. (2000). Developmental dyscalculia: prevalence and prognosis. European child & adolescent psychiatry, 9(2), S58-S64. doi:10.1007/s007870070009 Shalev, R. S., & Gross-Tsur, V. (2001). Developmental dyscalculia. Pediatric Neurology, 24(5), 337-342. doi: 10.1016/S0887-8994(00)00258-7 Shapiro, B., Church, R., & Lewis, M. E. B. (2002). Specific learning disabilities. In M. L. Batshaw (Ed.), Children with disabilities (Vol. 5, pp. 417–442). Washington, DC: Brookes Publishing. Shaver, A. V., & Walls, R. T. (1998). Effect of Title I parent involvement on student reading and mathematics achievement. Journal of Research & Development in Education, 31(2), 9097. Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Pediatrics in Review, 24(5), 147-153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/pir.24-5-147 Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2005). Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Biological psychiatry, 57(11), 1301-1309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.043 Sheldon, S. B. (2002). Parents' social networks and beliefs as predictors of parent involvement. The elementary school journal, 102(4), 301-316. http://www.jstor.org/stable/1002100 Shin, T., Davison, M. L., Long, J. D., Chan, C., & Heistad, D. (2013). Exploring gains in reading and mathematics achievement among regular and exceptional students using growth curve modeling. Learning and Individual Differences, 48(1), 96-112. doi:10.1177/0022219413508324 Sideridis, G. D., Morgan, P. L., Botsas, G., Padeliadu, S., & Fuchs, D. (2006). Predicting LD on the Basis of Motivation, Metacognition, and Psychopathology An ROC Analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(3), 215-229. doi: 10.1177/00222194060390030301 Sideridis, G. D., Mouzaki, A., Simos, P., & Protopapas, A. (2006). Classification of students with reading comprehension difficulties: The roles of motivation, affect, and psychopathology. Learning Disability Quarterly, 29(3), 159-180. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/30035505 220 Siegler, R. S. (1989). Mechanisms of cognitive development. Annual Review of Psychology, 40(1), 353-379. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.002033 Silverman, I. W. (2003). Gender differences in delay of gratification: A meta-analysis. Sex roles, 49(9-10), 451-463. doi: 10.1023/A:1025872421115 Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7(1), 37-46. doi:10.1002/dys.185 Sohr-Preston, S. L., & Scaramella, L. V. (2006). Implications of timing of maternal depressive symptoms for early cognitive and language development. Clinical child and family psychology review, 9(1), 65-83. doi: 10.1007/s10567-006-0004-2 Sparks, R. L., & Lovett, B. J. (2009). College Students With Learning Disability Diagnoses Who Are They and How Do They Perform?. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(6), 494-510. doi: 10.1177/0022219409338746 Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. G. (1980). Perception of numbers by human infants. Science, 210(4473), 1033-1035. doi: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1684281 Stevenson, D. L., & Baker, D. P. (1987). The family-school relation and the child's school performance. Child development, 1348-1357. doi: 10.2307/1130626 Switzky, H. N., & Schultz, G. F. (1988). Intrinsic motivation and learning performance: Implications for individual educational programming for learners with mild handicaps. Remedial and Special Education, 9(4), 7-14. doi:10.1177/074193258800900404 Tabassam, W., & Grainger, J. (2002). Self-concept, attributional style and self-efficacy beliefs of students with learning disabilities with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Learning Disability Quarterly, 25(2), 141-151. doi: 10.2307/1511280 Tallal, P. (1980). Language and reading: Some perceptual prerequisites. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30(1), 170-178. doi: 10.1007/BF02653716 Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in response to early interventions in reading: The lingering problem of treatment resisters. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 15(1), 55-64. doi: 10.1207/SLDRP1501_6 Trzesniewski, K. H., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., & Maughan, B. (2006). Revisiting the association between reading achievement and antisocial behavior: New evidence of an environmental explanation from a twin study. Child Development, 77(1), 72-88. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00857.x 221 Tur-Kaspa, H., & Bryan, T. (1995). Teachers' ratings of the social competence and school adjustment of students with LD in elementary and junior high school. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(1), 44-52. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221949502800107 Unrau, N., & Schlackman, J. (2006). Motivation and its relationship with reading achievement in an urban middle school. The Journal of Educational Research, 100(2), 81-101. doi:10.3200/JOER.100.2.81-101 U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2001). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998 –99: User’s manual for the ECLS-K base year public-use data files and electronic codebook (NCES 2001–029). Washington, DC: Author Vaughn, S., & Haager, D. (1994). Social competence as a multifaceted construct: How do students with learning disabilities fare?. Learning Disability Quarterly, 17(4), 253-266. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1511123 Viljaranta, J., Lerkkanen, M. K., Poikkeus, A. M., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2009). Crosslagged relations between task motivation and performance in arithmetic and literacy in kindergarten. Learning and Instruction, 19(4), 335-344. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.011 Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental psychology, 30(1), 73-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.1.73 Watkins, T. J. (1997). Teacher communications, child achievement, and parent traits in parent involvement models. The Journal of Educational Research, 91(1), 3-14. doi:10.1080/00220679709597515 Weaver-Hightower, M. (2003). The “boy turn” in research on gender and education. Review of Educational Research, 73(4), 471-498. doi: 10.3102/00346543073004471 Wehby, J. H., Falk, K. B., Barton-Arwood, S., Lane, K. L., & Cooley, C. (2003). The impact of comprehensive reading instruction on the academic and social behavior of students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 11(4), 225-238. doi: 10.1177/10634266030110040401 White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1992). Neuropsychological and socio-emotional correlates of specific-arithmetic disability. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7(1), 116. doi: 10.1016/0887-6177(92)90014-E Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary educational psychology, 25(1), 68-81. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015 222 Willcutt, E. G., Betjemann, R. S., McGrath, L. M., Chhabildas, N. A., Olson, R. K., DeFries, J. C., & Pennington, B. F. (2010). Etiology and neuropsychology of comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit models. Cortex, 46(10), 1345-1361. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.009 Willcutt, E. G., Boada, R., Riddle, M. W., Chhabildas, N., DeFries, J. C., & Pennington, B. F. (2011). Colorado Learning Difficulties Questionnaire: validation of a parent-report screening measure. Psychological assessment, 23(3), 778-791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023290 Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(08), 1039-1048. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00691 Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Duncan, L., Smith, S. D., Keenan, J. M., Wadsworth, S., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2010). Understanding the complex etiologies of developmental disorders: behavioral and molecular genetic approaches. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics, 31(7), 533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ef42a1 Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit. Developmental neuropsychology, 27(1), 35-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2701_3 Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., & DeFries, J. C. (2007). Understanding comorbidity: A twin study of reading disability and attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 144(6), 709-714. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.30310 Willcutt, E. G., Petrill, S. A., Wu, S., Boada, R., DeFries, J. C., Olson, R. K., & Pennington, B. F. (2013). Comorbidity between reading disability and math disability concurrent psychopathology, functional impairment, and neuropsychological functioning. Journal of learning disabilities, 46(6), 500-516. doi: 10.1177/0022219413477476 Wilson, D. R., & David, W. J. (1994). Academic Intrinsic Motivation and Attitudes toward School and Learning of Learning Disabled Students. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 9(3), 148-56. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of educational psychology, 91(3), 415-438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415 Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57(4), 988-1000. doi: 10.2307/1130373 223 Woollams, A. (2015). What does acquired dyslexia tell us about reading in the mind and brain? In A. Pollatsek & R. Trieman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Reading (pp. 149-164). Oxford University Press. Wong, B. Y., Harris, K. R., Graham, S., & Butler, D. L. (2003). Cognitive strategies instruction research in learning disabilities. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 383-402). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Wong, E. H., Wiest, D. J., & Cusick, L. B. (2002). Perceptions of autonomy support, parent attachment, competence and self-worth as predictors of motivational orientation and academic achievement: An examination of sixth-and ninth-grade regular education students. Adolescence, 37(146), 255-266. Wright, A. C. (2015). Teachers’ perceptions of students’ disruptive behavior: the effect of racial congruence and consequences for school suspension. Work. Pap., Dep. Econ., Univ. Calif., Santa Barbara. http://www.econ.ucsb.edu/about_us/events/seminar_papers/Wright. pdf. Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. (1991). Children's problems in text comprehension: An experimental investigation. Cambridge University Press. Zimmerman, M. A., & Arunkumar, R. (1994). Resiliency research: Implications for schools and policy. Social Policy Report, 8(4), 1-18. Zimmerman, B. J., & Risemberg, R. (1997). Self-regulatory dimensions of academic learning and motivation. In G. D. Phye (Ed.), Handbook of academic learning: Construction of knowledge (pp. 105-125). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Zisimopoulos, D. A., & Galanaki, E. P. (2009). Academic intrinsic motivation and perceived academic competence in Greek elementary students with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 24(1), 33-43. doi:10.1111/j.15405826.2008.01275.x 224