INVESTIGATING THE NATURE OF TEACHERS’ VOCABULARY AND SCIENCE TALK
DURING SCIENCE INSTRUCTION IN EARLY-ELEMENTARY CLASSROOMS
By
Blythe E. Anderson
A DISSERTATION
Submitted to
Michigan State University
in partial fulfillment of the requirements
for the degree of
Curriculum, Instruction, and Teacher Education—Doctor of Philosophy
2020
INVESTIGATING THE NATURE OF TEACHERS’ VOCABULARY AND SCIENCE TALK
DURING SCIENCE INSTRUCTION IN EARLY-ELEMENTARY CLASSROOMS
ABSTRACT
By
Blythe E. Anderson
Oral language and vocabulary development in the early-elementary grades is important
for students’ overall academic success. Oral language and vocabulary support science learning,
and reciprocally, the background knowledge and vocabulary students gain when learning about
the natural world supports their reading comprehension. Therefore, oral language and vocabulary
development should be an essential focus of instruction for early-elementary students, and as
such, current national standards for literacy and science instruction each emphasize oral language
development. Given the importance of oral language and vocabulary development for both
literacy and science learning, it is critical to understand how teachers support this development in
early-elementary science instruction. Most observational studies of vocabulary instruction have
taken place within the context of literacy instruction. As little is known about how teachers
promote oral language and vocabulary development in science instruction in the early-
elementary grades, the present study investigated (1) how teachers use language to promote oral
language and vocabulary development during science instruction in the early-elementary grades,
(2) whether/how vocabulary talk relates to the language aspects of science talk, and (3) what
features of science curriculum materials are related to enhanced vocabulary talk.
To answer these questions, I conducted two instrumental case studies. In the first study, I
examined the science instruction of a cohort of eight early-elementary teachers in order to
understand how they used language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development. In the second study, I examined the science instruction of Ms. Thompson,
kindergarten teacher, in order to identify features of science curriculum materials that are related
to enhanced vocabulary talk during science instruction. In total, 24 video recorded science
lessons provided 894.27 minutes of observational data across three timepoints from the eight
participating teachers. I used discourse analysis and other qualitative analysis techniques to
examine the vocabulary talk moves (i.e., ways of using language to promote oral language and
vocabulary development) the teachers made during science instruction. I also used quantitative
techniques to make within- and between-teacher comparisons of vocabulary talk over the course
of the study.
The cohort of teachers used considerably more vocabulary talk moves for building
students’ knowledge of word meanings than for scaffolding students’ word use, building
students’ awareness of words and word learning, or interesting students in words and word
learning. This study points to the need to consider the context in which vocabulary talk moves
are made and the overall quality of this vocabulary talk in addition to examining which moves
the teachers make. Curriculum materials that (a) identified target words and provided child-
friendly explanations/definitions, (b) used texts that highlight these target words and provided
supports for extra-textual talk promoting vocabulary talk, and (c) offered discussion prompts that
deepen students’ understandings of target word meanings were associated with enhanced
vocabulary talk by Ms. Thompson. Likewise, the absence of these curricular features was
associated with less vocabulary talk.
This study contributes to the field’s understanding of the ways that science instruction
supports literacy learning and literacy instruction supports science learning in the early-
elementary grades. The findings from this study have implications for curriculum development,
teacher professional development, teacher preparation, and policy.
Copyright by
BLYTHE E ANDERSON
2020
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
First, I would like to thank the members of my dissertation committee. Dr. Tanya Wright
(chair), thank you for your guidance over the past five years as my advisor, course instructor, and
supervisor. You always push my thinking and I’m grateful for all of your feedback, support, and
the opportunities you’ve provided with SOLID Start and the Vocabulary SIG. Thank you for
helping me see the story in the data and connect it to the larger picture. Dr. Patricia Edwards,
thank you for always reminding me to consider what teachers can learn from my work and put
into action Monday morning. Your guidance throughout grad school, your advice for navigating
the next steps as an early career scholar, and your willingness to connect me with other scholars
are all greatly appreciated. Dr. Amelia Gotwals, thank you for your guidance with quantitative
analysis and helping me think about vocabulary within science instruction. I appreciate all of
your encouragement and support throughout my practicum, dissertation, and work with the
SOLID Start project. Dr. Laura Tortorelli, thank you for pushing my thinking in class and when
serving on my guidance and dissertation committees. I also appreciate all of your support with
teaching TE 301 my first year, with being an academic mama, and with navigating the job
market. Dr. Jennifer VanDerHeide, thank you for making discourse analysis less scary. Before
taking your course, I was intrigued but a bit intimidated. Thank you for being so real and for
thinking alongside me.
I would like to thank the Michigan State University Department of Teacher Education as
a whole, and all the faculty and staff who provided support, encouragement, opportunities, and
mentorship. There are too many people to name everyone, but I would like to recognize a few in
particular. Thank you to Dr. Carrie Symons for always making time for “Carrapy sessions” when
v
I am stuck and need a thought partner or advice. Writing, teaching, and learning with you is a
gift. Thank you to Dr. Shireen Al-Adeimi for helping me think through utterances and c-units for
my analysis. Thank you also for modeling an amazing job talk and for your support with the job
market. Thank you also to Terry Edwards, Amy Peebles, and Sue Sipkovsky for making sure I
always had what I needed.
To the entire SOLID Start team, thank you for your collaboration and support. Special
thanks to Arianna Pikus for sharing your expertise with all things stats (and stats adjacent) and to
Maddie Korte for quickly learning my complex transcription guidelines and for capturing all that
talk.
I would like to acknowledge the incredible academic women who have been on this
journey with me. I am deeply grateful to have each of you in my life. Dr. Lindsay Wexler, you
go above and beyond as a mentor, friend, and colleague. Your support with navigating grad
school, academic mama-ing, the job search, and life in general has been invaluable. Thank you
for being (at least) a step ahead and lighting the path! Dr. Kaitlin Glause, you have been there
every step of this journey, sharing all the academic and mama milestones—what a gift! Thank
you for the countless pep talks, writing weekends, latte runs, accountability texts, and
presentation run-throughs. Turtle, turtle! (Thank you also to Drs. Debra and Brian Schutte for all
of your support along the way as well. Having space to write this past year in particular was
especially helpful!) Dr. Lisa Domke, thank you for all of the trickle-down advising, weekend
writing days, and late-night Zooms. My favorite part of this study was double coding with you;
you are an incredible thought partner and your questions made this work stronger. I’m so
thankful you’re only a text away. Dr. Amy Guenther, you are a fantastic listener and friend.
Thank you for all the chats, dinners, phone calls, and words of encouragement. Our guest room
vi
may be farther away now, but “Dr. Amys” is always welcome! JoAnne West, thank you for your
listening ears, your mama advice, and for just getting it. Co-teaching with you was such fun! Dr.
Katie Schenkel, thank you for all the writing dates and for sharing meals and laughter. I’m
grateful for your friendship. Dr. Melissa Gallagher, thank you for your mentorship from the very
start. Researching and writing with you before/during grad school was an incredible learning
experience and the perfect academic training wheels. Everyone needs a Melissa! Dr. Peggy
Pruisner, thank you for advising me beyond college and helping me select Michigan State for
this journey.
Thank you to all of my friends who have supported me from afar and celebrated each
milestone along the way. Special thanks to Clarissa Mathews, Justine Trueblood, Jordan Russell,
Sara Berryman, and Allie Miguel for all the phone calls, texts, Snaps, Polos, and Zooms.
Lastly, I would like to thank my family, who has believed in this dream along with me
from the start. Your 100 notes of encouragement for the final days of grad school were extra
special and so timely given the additional challenge of dissertating during a pandemic. To my
parents, thank you for all of your support throughout this journey. I am especially thankful for all
your support in the final weeks of writing and for taking care of us all during this time. We are
deeply grateful. To Kyle, thank you for going on this crazy journey with me and for all of your
love, encouragement, understanding, and patience along the way. I am grateful for your
willingness to chase this dream with me, and I can’t wait to embark on the next phase of this
adventure with you. And to Myra, who wrote many “dissertations” in the time it took me to write
just one, thank you for reminding me to play.
vii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................................... xi
LIST OF FIGURES ...................................................................................................................... xii
CHAPTER 1—INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................. 1
Statement of the Issue ................................................................................................................. 1
Overview of the Present Study ................................................................................................... 6
Contribution of the Study............................................................................................................ 8
CHAPTER 2—CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK AND LITERATURE REVIEW .................... 10
Conceptual Framework ............................................................................................................. 10
Social Nature of Language Learning .................................................................................... 10
Action Through Language .................................................................................................... 13
Complex Nature of Word Learning ...................................................................................... 14
Word Consciousness and Word Learning............................................................................. 15
How Children Learn Words ...................................................................................................... 17
Knowledge and Understanding ............................................................................................. 18
Student Word Use ................................................................................................................. 25
Metalinguistic and Metacognitive Awareness ...................................................................... 27
Opportunities for Oral Language and Vocabulary Development in Science Instruction ......... 32
The Present Study ..................................................................................................................... 35
Summary ................................................................................................................................... 35
CHAPTER 3—METHODS .......................................................................................................... 37
Context of Larger Study............................................................................................................ 37
Study 1: Vocabulary and Science Talk in Eight Early-Elementary Classrooms ...................... 38
Design and Logic .................................................................................................................. 39
Participants ............................................................................................................................ 39
Data Sources ......................................................................................................................... 40
Demographic Background and Teaching Experience Information Survey ...................... 40
Weekly Instructional Log ................................................................................................. 41
Video Recordings of Science Lessons .............................................................................. 41
Analytic Strategies ................................................................................................................ 42
Research Question 1: How Do Teachers Use Language to Promote Students’ Oral
Language and Vocabulary Development During Science Instruction in Early-Elementary
Classrooms? ...................................................................................................................... 42
Research Question 2: Is Vocabulary Talk Related to the Language Aspects of Science
Talk, and if so, How? ........................................................................................................ 52
Study 2: Curricular Features and Vocabulary Talk in a Kindergarten Classroom ................... 56
Design and Logic .................................................................................................................. 56
Participant ............................................................................................................................. 58
Data Sources ......................................................................................................................... 58
viii
Analytic Strategies ................................................................................................................ 59
Researcher Positionality............................................................................................................ 60
Limitations to the Method ......................................................................................................... 61
Summary ................................................................................................................................... 63
CHAPTER 4—STUDY 1 FINDINGS: RESEARCH QUESTIONS 1-2 ..................................... 64
The Teachers ............................................................................................................................. 65
Ms. Henderson ...................................................................................................................... 65
Ms. Thompson ...................................................................................................................... 66
Ms. Nelson ............................................................................................................................ 66
Ms. Baker .............................................................................................................................. 67
Ms. Watson ........................................................................................................................... 67
Ms. Griffin ............................................................................................................................ 68
Ms. Howard........................................................................................................................... 68
Ms. Hill ................................................................................................................................. 69
Research Question 1: How Do Teachers Use Language to Promote Students’ Oral Language
and Vocabulary Development During Science Instruction in Early-Elementary Classrooms? 70
Knowledge & Understanding ............................................................................................... 72
Affective Factors ................................................................................................................... 76
Student Word Use ................................................................................................................. 79
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness ......................................................................... 82
Within-Teacher Comparisons of Vocabulary Talk ............................................................... 84
Between-Teacher Comparisons of Vocabulary Talk ............................................................ 86
Research Question 2: Is Vocabulary Talk Related to the Language Aspects of Science Talk,
and if so, How? ......................................................................................................................... 90
Summary of Findings ................................................................................................................ 98
CHAPTER 5—STUDY 2 FINDINGS: RESEARCH QUESTION 3 .......................................... 99
The Lessons ............................................................................................................................ 100
SOLID Start: How Can We Prepare for Severe Weather Conditions? ............................... 100
Teachers Pay Teachers: A Push Is a Force! ....................................................................... 102
Mystery Science: Where Do Animals Live? ....................................................................... 103
Targeted Vocabulary ............................................................................................................... 105
Texts and Supports for Extra-Textual Talk ............................................................................ 121
Discussion Prompts ................................................................................................................. 126
Summary of Findings .............................................................................................................. 129
CHAPTER 6—DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS .............................. 131
Discussion ............................................................................................................................... 131
Types of Vocabulary Talk .................................................................................................. 133
Quality of Vocabulary Talk ................................................................................................ 139
Curricular Supports for Vocabulary Talk ........................................................................... 141
Implications............................................................................................................................. 145
Curriculum Development.................................................................................................... 145
Teacher Professional Development .................................................................................... 146
Teacher Preparation ............................................................................................................ 147
ix
Policy .................................................................................................................................. 148
Limitations .............................................................................................................................. 149
Future Research ...................................................................................................................... 151
Conclusions ............................................................................................................................. 152
APPENDICES ............................................................................................................................ 154
APPENDIX A—SOLID START PD RECRUITMENT INFORMATION ........................... 155
APPENDIX B—DEMOGRAPHIC BACKGROUND AND TEACHING EXPERIENCE
INFORMATION SURVEY.................................................................................................... 156
APPENDIX C—WEEKLY INSTRUCTIONAL LOG .......................................................... 160
APPENDIX D—TRANSCRIPTION GUIDE ........................................................................ 161
APPENDIX E—VOCABULARY TALK CODEBOOK ....................................................... 164
REFERENCES ........................................................................................................................... 167
x
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1 Relationships of Vocabulary Talk Moves to Target Words ............................................ 50
Table 2 SOLID Start Tool: Domain 3, Components 1-2 .............................................................. 54
Table 3 Sample Weighted and Composite Scores for a 20-Minute Lesson with Two Activities 56
Table 4 Frequency of Vocabulary Talk Moves ............................................................................ 71
Table 5 Words Targeted for Instruction...................................................................................... 113
xi
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1 Domains of Science Talk................................................................................................ 53
Figure 2 Within-Teacher Comparisons of Vocabulary Talk Types Across Lessons .................... 85
Figure 3 Between-Teacher Comparison of Vocabulary Talk Types Across Lessons .................. 88
Figure 4 Ms. Henderson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk .................................................... 91
Figure 5 Ms. Baker’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk ............................................................ 91
Figure 6 Ms. Watson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk ......................................................... 92
Figure 7 Ms. Griffin’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk .......................................................... 92
Figure 8 Ms. Hill’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk ............................................................... 93
Figure 9 Ms. Nelson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk .......................................................... 94
Figure 10 Ms. Thompson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk .................................................. 95
Figure 11 Ms. Howard’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk ....................................................... 97
Figure 12 Ms. Thompson’s Vocabulary Talk ............................................................................. 100
xii
CHAPTER 1—INTRODUCTION
Statement of the Issue
Oral language and vocabulary development in the early-elementary grades is important
for students’ overall academic success. Vocabulary knowledge has long been established as
having a significant impact on literacy learning, as understanding the meaning of words in text is
central to comprehending the text (Anderson & Freebody, 1981; Davis, 1944; Ouellette & Beers,
2010). Knowing the meaning(s) of many words supports comprehension and as such, the size of
students’ vocabularies is predictive of both concurrent (Ricketts et al., 2007) and later (Senechal
et al., 2006) reading comprehension. This impact is not limited to literacy learning, as oral
language and vocabulary support science learning as well (e.g., Mercer et al., 2004; Richmond &
Striley, 1996; Wright & Gotwals, 2017). Reciprocally, the background knowledge and
vocabulary students gain when learning about the natural world supports their reading
comprehension (e.g., Guthrie, Anderson, Alao, & Rinehart, 1999; Wang & Herman, 2006).
Therefore, oral language and vocabulary development should be an essential focus of instruction
for early-elementary students—including within science instruction.
As such, current national standards for literacy and science instruction each emphasize
oral language development. The Common Core State Standards (CCSS) call for students to
engage in substantive talk, beginning in the early-elementary grades (National Governors
Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010). In order to
meet Listening and Speaking English Language Arts standards, kindergarteners, for instance, are
expected to participate in collaborative conversations; follow agreed-upon rules for discussions;
continue a conversation through multiple exchanges; ask and answer questions to confirm
understanding, seek help, get information, or clarify something not understood; provide
1
descriptions; and express thoughts, feelings, and ideas clearly. Similarly, the Next Generation
Science Standards (NGSS) call for students to engage in the kinds of talk that scientists do,
beginning in kindergarten (NGSS Lead States, 2013). To meet these standards, kindergarteners
need to be able to describe patterns, share observations, construct arguments supported by
evidence, ask questions to obtain information, and communicate solutions (NGSS Lead States,
2013; Wright & Gotwals, 2017). Thus, science instruction aligned to these ambitious standards
would provide many opportunities for supporting early-elementary students’ language and
literacy development (Wright & Domke, 2019).
Although instruction supporting students’ oral language and vocabulary development is
essential for early-elementary students’ academic success, studies show that very little time is
devoted to vocabulary instruction (Nelson et al., 2015; Scott et al., 2003; Wright & Neuman,
2014) or science instruction (Berliner, 2011; Blank, 2012) at the elementary level. Further,
studies have found that the quality of the vocabulary instruction that does occur in schools is not
reflective of effective vocabulary instruction (Carlisle et al., 2013; Scott et al., 2003; Watts, 1995;
Wright & Neuman, 2014). Relatedly, Dickinson, Darrow, and Tinubu (2008) reported there
being “substantial room for improvement” in supporting young children’s language and learning
(p. 420). Similarly, Windschitl, Thompson, Braaten, and Stroupe (2012) identified generating
student talk and using it as a social resource for learning as an area for growth within science
instruction.
Given the importance of oral language and vocabulary development for academic
success, it is critical to understand how teachers support this development during typical
classroom instruction. Researchers have examined various aspects of teachers’ vocabulary
instruction through observations in preschool, elementary, and middle-grades classrooms. Most
2
of these observational studies have taken place within the context of literacy instruction
(Blachowicz, 1987; Carlisle et al., 2013; Nelson et al., 2015; Neugebauer et al., 2017; Silverman
et al., 2014; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wanzek, 2014; Watts, 1995). Blachowicz (1987)
focused on vocabulary instruction in six 20-40-minute reading groups in fourth-grade classrooms
across 10-15 school days. Blachowicz found that 15-20% of instructional time was spent on
vocabulary instruction; vocabulary instruction was often guided by the contextual sentences
offered by the teachers’ manuals; and vocabulary instruction was undifferentiated, nonstrategic,
and unaligned with the reading selection’s comprehension goals. Watts (1995) focused on how
teachers taught vocabulary during reading lessons in 47 observations in upper-elementary
classrooms in a large urban school district. Watts found that new words were taught most often
as a prereading activity; teachers used definitional and contextual types of instruction most often;
and many features of effective vocabulary instruction were rarely observed, including activating
prior knowledge, providing multiple exposures to words, and teaching strategies for independent
word learning. Silverman and Crandell (2010) focused on the correlation between teachers’
vocabulary instructional practices and prekindergarten and kindergarten students’ vocabulary,
completing three 90-minute observations in each of 16 classrooms in an urban school district.
They found that the effectiveness of acting out/illustrating the meaning of words, prompting
students to analyze how words are related, guiding students use words productively in novel
contexts, defining words, and prompting students to use vocabulary words during word study
varied based on students’ initial vocabulary knowledge and whether the practices were used
during read-aloud time or non-read-aloud time. Carlisle et al. (2013) focused on teachers’
support for students’ vocabulary learning during literacy instruction at four different timepoints
in 44 third-grade classrooms in high-poverty schools. Carlisle found that teachers rarely engaged
3
students in cognitively challenging work on word meanings, the extent to which teachers
supported students’ vocabulary development was significantly related to reading comprehension
gains across the school year, and the quality of support teachers provided for students’
vocabulary learning was related to teachers’ knowledge of reading and reading instruction.
Wanzek (2014) focused on the amount and type of direct vocabulary instruction provided for
second-grade students with reading difficulties during core reading instruction and supplemental
reading interventions by 14 classroom and intervention teachers in three Title I schools. Wanzek
found that 8% of core classroom reading instruction was devoted to direct vocabulary instruction
(i.e., providing definitions and examples) and minimal direct vocabulary instruction occurred
during supplemental reading interventions. Silverman et al. (2014) focused on the relationship
between teachers’ reading/language arts instruction and third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade students’
vocabulary and comprehension, observing 33 classrooms at three points during a school year.
They found that instruction related to definitions, word relations, and morphosyntax was
positively associated with change in vocabulary, while instruction related to application of words
across contexts was negatively associated with change in vocabulary (likely due to poor quality
of application of words across contexts). Nelson et al. (2015) focused on teachers’ vocabulary
instruction during language arts lessons in K-3 classrooms in low-income schools, with 337
observations across three years. They found that less than 5% of the language arts block was
devoted to vocabulary instruction, with most of that time focused on teaching individual words
rather than word-learning strategies. Neugebauer et al. (2017) focused on a specific aspect of
vocabulary instruction—word consciousness talk (i.e., talk that reinforced students’ use of
words, affirmed their recognition of words, and helped students make personal connections to
words)—within the context of a vocabulary intervention (i.e., Elements of Reading Vocabulary;
4
Beck & McKeown, 2004) in 27 kindergarten classrooms in urban schools. Neugebauer and
colleagues found that teachers’ word conciousness talk was positively associated with student
gains in general vocabulary at the end of kindergarten. Collectively, these studies offer insight
into the nature of vocabulary instruction within literacy lessons—namely, that little time is
devoted to vocabulary instruction and often this instruction lacks features of effective vocabulary
instruction.
Only a few observational studies have examined vocabulary instruction outside of
literacy instruction (Dickinson et al., 2008; Scott et al., 2003; Wright & Neuman, 2014). Scott et
al. (2003) focused on when, where, and how often effective vocabulary instruction occurred
across the school day (including math, science, art, and social studies) within 308 hours of
instruction in upper-elementary middle school classrooms in three diverse districts in Canada.
Scott and colleagues found that 6% of school time was devoted to developing vocabulary
knowledge, with only 1.4% of school time devoted to vocabulary development outside of
langauge arts instruction (e.g., science, social studies, mathematics, art). Dickinson et al. (2008)
focused on how four Head Start teachers used four evidence-based strategies (i.e., varied
vocabulary, extended talk on a single topic, semantically-contingent responses, and cognitively-
rich topics of conversation) within the context of centers time (i.e., blocks and dramatic play) on
three different occasions. They found that teachers asked thought-provoking questions and
engaged students in extended topic discussions more often than they responded to children’s
questions or initiatives, modeled sophisticated language, provided explanations, or engaged in
conceptually-based instruction. Wright and Neuman (2014) focused on how teachers enacted
oral vocabulary instruction across the full school day in 660 hours of observation across four
school days in 55 kindergarten classrooms from a range of socio-economic status schools. They
5
found that teachers gave single, brief word explanations in order to support students’
understanding of the immediate context (i.e., teachable moments); engaged in more vocabulary
instruction during subject areas (e.g., science and social studies) but devoted little time to these
areas overall; and explained words more often and were more likely to address sophisticated
words when serving in economically advantaged schools than disadvantaged schools.
In short, little is known about how teachers promote oral language and vocabulary
development in science instruction in the early-elementary grades, with only one observational
study (Wright & Neuman, 2014) including science instruction at the K-2 level. Therefore, given
the importance of oral language and vocabulary development to students’ literacy and science
learning and the emphasis placed on oral language development by national standards, there is a
clear need to better understand how teachers at the early-elementary grades develop students’
oral language and vocabulary within science instruction. The purpose of the present
observational study is to examine teachers’ vocabulary talk moves—that is, ways of using
language to promote oral language and vocabulary development—during science instruction in
early-elementary classrooms. Understanding the ways in which teachers do and do not use
vocabulary talk moves during science instruction in the early-elementary grades is important
because it allows us to examine the opportunities for students to develop oral language and
vocabulary and simultaneously access science content, both of which can ultimately support
reading comprehension (Anderson & Freebody, 1981; Kintsch, 2013).
Overview of the Present Study
As little is known about how teachers promote oral language and vocabulary
development in science instruction in the early-elementary grades, the purpose of the present
study is to examine the vocabulary talk moves (i.e., ways of using language to promote oral
6
language and vocabulary development) teachers make during science lessons. Data for this study
were collected as part of the Science, Oral Language, and Literacy Development from the Start
of School (SOLID Start) project (Dr. Tanya Wright & Dr. Amelia Gotwals, PIs), with funding
from the National Science Foundation (NSF; grant number 1620580). I analyzed 894.27 minutes
of observational data across 24 total science lessons in eight early-elementary classrooms (i.e.,
one young 5s, three kindergarten, two first grade, and two second grade) located in the same
elementary school in a small district within a large Midwestern state. Participating teachers’
science lessons were recorded at three points during February through May of 2019. Discourse
analysis and other qualitative analysis techniques were used to examine the vocabulary talk
moves the teachers made during science instruction. Quantitative techniques were also used to
make comparisons of vocabulary talk over the course of the study.
I present descriptive findings investigating how teachers use vocabulary talk moves
during science instruction. I examine the specific vocabulary talk moves teachers make and the
frequency with which these moves are made during a lesson. I make within- and between-teacher
comparisons of vocabulary talk over the course of the study and examine the relationship
between vocabulary talk and the language aspects of science talk. I also report on curricular
features related to enhanced vocabulary talk. The following research questions are addressed in
this study:
1. How do teachers use language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development during science instruction in early-elementary classrooms?
2. Is vocabulary talk related to the language aspects of science talk, and if so, how?
3. What features of science curriculum materials are related to a kindergarten teacher’s
enhanced vocabulary talk?
7
Contribution of the Study
In order to support teachers with implementing effective oral language and vocabulary
instruction that enables students to meet ambitious national standards and ultimately promotes
their literacy and science learning, we must first understand the current state of oral language and
vocabulary instruction within the context of science lessons in early-elementary classrooms.
Observational studies of vocabulary instruction have primarily focused on the ways in which
teachers support vocabulary development within the context of literacy instruction (e.g.,
Blachowicz, 1987; Carlisle et al., 2013; Nelson et al., 2015; Neugebauer et al., 2017; Silverman
et al., 2014; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wanzek, 2014; Watts, 1995). At this time, we have
very limited knowledge about how teachers promote oral language and vocabulary development
within science instruction in the early-elementary grades. Understanding what opportunities
there are for building students’ vocabulary outside of literacy instruction is important given the
sheer number of vocabulary words there are to learn (Anderson & Nagy, 1992; Nagy &
Anderson, 1984), little time is devoted to vocabulary instruction within literacy instruction
(Blachowicz, 1987; Nelson et al., 2015; Wanzek, 2014), and many words have discipline-
specific meanings (Bravo & Cervetti, 2008). This study contributes to the research base by
examining how teachers use language to promote oral language and vocabulary development
during science instruction in the early-elementary grades, whether/how vocabulary talk relates to
the language aspects of science talk, and what features of science curriculum materials are
related to enhanced vocabulary talk. In doing so, this study contributes to the field’s
understanding of the ways in which science instruction supports literacy learning and literacy
instruction supports science learning. By gaining a better understanding of the current state of
oral language and vocabulary instruction within early-elementary science lessons, we can design
8
professional development and curriculum materials to support teachers in implementing effective
oral language and vocabulary instruction.
9
CHAPTER 2—CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK AND LITERATURE REVIEW
This chapter focuses on the conceptual framing of the present study and the research
relative to how children learn words and why opportunities for oral language and vocabulary
development should be examined within the context of science instruction. First, I present the
theoretical and empirical work that frames this study. Then, I highlight research-based
instructional practices for supporting children’s oral language and vocabulary development in
early childhood and throughout the elementary grades. Finally, I present research supporting the
integration of language and literacy instruction with science instruction.
Conceptual Framework
This study is framed by theoretical and empirical work centered around language
learning, particularly as it pertains to the ways children’s vocabularies develop through oral
language in classroom settings. In this section, I discuss the theories and research relative to how
language development occurs through interpersonal interactions (e.g., Vygotsky, 1978, 1986);
language is used to carry out specific actions (Gee, 2014); language learning is an unconstrained
skill (Neuman & Wright, 2013; Paris, 2005) and therefore requires multifaceted instruction (e.g.,
Baumann et al., 2007; Carlo et al., 2004; Lesaux & Kieffer, 2010; National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development, 2000); and language learning is supported by an individual’s
awareness of and interest in words and word learning (Anderson & Nagy, 1992; Graves &
Watts-Taffe, 2002, 2008).
Social Nature of Language Learning
The present study draws on the social interaction theory of language acquisition
(Bohannon & Bonvillian, 2001), which posits that language develops through interpersonal
interactions. Vygotsky (1978, 1986) particularly emphasized the role the adults in a child’s life
10
play in supporting the child’s language acquisition. Likewise, Bruner (1978) suggested that
parents provide a supportive communicative structure (i.e., scaffold) that facilitates language
development. From the social interaction perspective, classroom teachers play a significant role
in the language development of their students, as they expose students to language through both
talk and texts.
Empirically, many studies with young children have found a relationship between the talk
of adults and the oral language and vocabulary development of children (e.g., Barnes et al., 2017;
Barnes & Dickinson, 2017, 2018; E. P. Bowers & Vasilyeva, 2011; Dickinson et al., 1993;
Dickinson & Porche, 2011; Dickinson & Smith, 1994; Dickinson & Tabors, 1991; Wasik &
Hindman, 2014). For example, Dickinson and Tabors (1991) found that engaging young children
in certain kinds of conversational language experiences, such as explanatory and narrative talk at
mealtimes at home and interactive read-alouds at school, supported the development of their
vocabulary and other literacy-related language skills. Relatedly, Dickinson, Cote, and Smith
(1993) found that engaging young children in intellectually challenging discussions fostered
language growth. Likewise, Dickinson and Smith (1994) found a strong association between
teacher-child interactions involving predictions, talk about vocabulary, and analysis (i.e., child-
involved analytic talk) and vocabulary development. Further, Dickinson and Porche (2011)
found that preschool teachers’ sophisticated use of language was predictive of students’ reading
comprehension and word recognition in fourth grade. They also found that, in addition to early
support in the home for literacy, preschool teachers’ utterances that (a) corrected the accuracy of
what children said and (b) analyzed word meanings or reasons for characters’ actions or story
events in read-aloud texts was predictive of students’ vocabulary in fourth grade. In a related
study, E. P. Bowers & Vasilyeva (2011) found that the number of different words (i.e., word
11
types) a preschool teacher used was positively related to vocabulary growth for monolingual
children, while the number of total words a preschool teacher used was positively related to
vocabulary growth for children learning English. In another study, Barnes et al. (2017) found
that preschool children’s receptive vocabulary growth was positively associated with teachers’
comments related to science, social studies, mathematics, texts, or metacognition (i.e.,
conceptually-focused comments). They also found that children in classrooms where teachers
were more responsive (i.e., teachers comment immediately after a child’s utterance to extend the
topic of conversation with additional information or an answer to a question) experienced greater
receptive vocabulary growth than children who experienced less interaction. Similarly, Barnes
and Dickinson (2017) found that preschool children experienced greater receptive vocabulary
growth when they were in classrooms in which teachers used more lexical elements (i.e.,
amount, sophistication, and diversity of vocabulary and shorter utterances). Relatedly, Barnes
and Dickinson (2018) found that teachers’ use of mental state verbs (i.e., verbs expressing
cognition through thoughts, memories, knowledge, feelings, or ideas) was positively associated
with preschool children’s end-of-year receptive vocabulary scores. Further, they found a positive
association between teacher talk that placed the child as the referent of the mental state verb in
group content instruction and children’s end-of year receptive vocabulary scores.
Together, these studies demonstrate the significant impact the language use of adults has
on the language development of children. Because of the important role adult speech plays in
children’s language learning, I examined the oral language interactions that happened between
the teachers and their early-elementary students in the present study. This is important because
little is known about oral language interactions regarding vocabulary learning within the context
of science instruction in the early-elementary grades.
12
Action Through Language
The present study also draws on Gee's (2014) notion that language is used to carry out
specific actions rather than just to give one another information. Therefore, in the context of an
early-elementary classroom, teachers likely use language in many different ways to carry out
actions with specific pedagogical purposes. This notion of action-through-language provides
theoretical support for research on teachers’ use of talk moves—or specific ways of using
language to achieve specific outcomes (e.g., Michaels & O’Connor, 2015). Researchers have
examined teachers’ language use to understand the specific talk moves they make to carry out
instructional actions through language (Beck et al., 1996; McKeown & Beck, 2004; Michaels &
O’Connor, 2015; Michener et al., 2018; O’Connor & Michaels, 1993, 1996, 2019). For example,
Beck et al. (1996) and McKeown and Beck (2004) examined fourth-grade teachers’ talk within
the context of the Questioning the Author intervention and identified specific talk moves
teachers made when asking questions (i.e., retrieving information from the text, constructing the
message of the text, extending discussion, checking students’ knowledge of specific information)
and responding to students’ comments (i.e., repeating the comment, paraphrasing the comment,
refining the comment). They found that when teachers asked questions focused on constructing
and extending meaning and gained skill in refining and using students’ comments in discussion,
the amount of teacher talk decreased while the quality of talk increased. Similarly, Michener et
al. (2018) investigated how rates of specific talk moves of third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade teachers
predicted students’ reading comprehension. Controlling for students’ decoding and fluency,
semantic-syntactic knowledge, and initial reading comprehension, they found that teacher
explanations and simple follow-up moves (i.e., low-level evaluations) significantly predicted
students’ reading comprehension. In the present study, I examined the vocabulary talk moves
13
teachers made—or specific ways of using language to promote oral language and vocabulary
development. This is important because the talk moves identified in previous studies have not
centered on vocabulary instruction within the context of early-elementary science instruction.
The present study identifies teachers’ talk moves that are aligned with the literature on how
children learn words.
Complex Nature of Word Learning
The present study is also framed by the understanding that the task of word learning is
complex. In contrast to constrained skills such as learning the names of the 26 letters of the
English alphabet, learning vocabulary is an unconstrained skill; that is, there are always more
words to learn and individuals continue to learn new words throughout their lifetime (Neuman &
Wright, 2013; Paris, 2005). Adding to the challenge of learning thousands of words, many words
have multiple meanings (Anderson & Nagy, 1992; Graves, 2006; Nagy & Anderson, 1984).
Relatedly, word learning is an incremental process, with depth and breadth of word knowledge
developing over time through multiple exposures to words in a variety of contexts (Beck et al.,
1987; Blachowicz et al., 2013; Dale, 1965; Nagy & Scott, 2000). Further complicating word
learning, context is not always helpful in determining the meaning of a word (Beck et al., 1983),
and dictionary definitions can confuse rather than clarify a word’s meaning(s) (McKeown,
1993).
Given the complex nature of word learning, vocabulary researchers recommend
instruction that is multifaceted, meaning that multiple approaches are used to address different
aspects of word learning (e.g., Baumann et al., 2007; Carlo et al., 2004; Lesaux & Kieffer, 2010;
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000). Teaching students the
meaning(s) of individual words is one effective approach (Beck & McKeown, 2007; Jenkins et
14
al., 1989; McKeown, 1985); however, because there are far too many words to directly teach
students each word’s meaning(s) (Nagy et al., 1985; Nagy & Anderson, 1984; Nagy & Herman,
1985), vocabulary researchers also recommend supporting students in becoming independent
word learners (Anderson & Nagy, 1992; Graves, 2006; Nagy, 2005). This is also important
because, while incidental word learning (i.e., unintentionally learning words from texts while
reading) does occur (Nagy et al., 1984, 1985), students learn only about 15% of the unknown
words they encounter in a text (Swanborn & de Glopper, 1999). Therefore, to substantially
contribute to students’ vocabulary development, researchers also recommend teaching students
specific strategies to use when they encounter unfamiliar words and developing their awareness
of how words work and how they learn and think about words (Baumann et al., 2007; Graves,
2006; Nagy, 2005; Wise, 2019). In the present study, I examined the range of instructional
practices that might support oral language and vocabulary development. This study builds on
previous work by examining these instructional practices within the context of early-elementary
science instruction.
Word Consciousness and Word Learning
Finally, the present study draws on the theory of word consciousness, which posits that
students’ knowledge and dispositions for learning, appreciating, and effectively using words
generatively supports their vocabulary development (Scott & Nagy, 2004). Specifically, word
consciousness refers to students’ awareness of and interest in words and word learning, as well
as their ability to use what they know about words to learn new words (e.g., Anderson & Nagy,
1992; Graves & Watts-Taffe, 2002, 2008). Vocabulary researchers have suggested that how
students feel about words and word learning impacts how (if) they engage in word learning (e.g.,
Scott & Nagy, 2004). They posit that for students to engage in the work of figuring out the
15
meaning(s) of an unfamiliar word, they need to view word learning as worthwhile and be curious
enough about a word’s meaning to attempt to figure out its meaning (Anderson & Nagy, 1992;
Scott & Nagy, 2004). Theoretically, students who are aware of words, have knowledge of words
and word learning, have interest in words, and are motivated to learn new words are more likely
to learn the new words they encounter than students with less awareness, knowledge, interest, or
motivation.
The literature on these affective factors has been primarily theoretical (e.g., Anderson &
Nagy, 1991, 1992; Graves, 2000, 2006; Graves & Watts-Taffe, 2002, 2008; Nagy & Scott, 2000;
Scott & Nagy, 2004). Several studies have included instruction addressing affective factors
within a larger vocabulary program (e.g., Baumann et al., 2007; Cox et al., 2015; Coyne et al.,
2010; Manyak et al., 2014; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985; Taboada & Rutherford, 2011). For
example, Baumann et al. (2007) included word play and figurative language activities in their
multifaceted intervention. However, as part of a larger vocabulary program, it is not possible to
determine the impact these affective factors, in particular, had on students’ vocabulary
development. One study has specifically examined the efficacy of instruction targeting affective
factors on students’ word learning. Neugebauer et al. (2017) investigated the impact of teachers’
word consciousness talk—defined as talk that reinforces students’ use of words, affirms their
recognition of word meanings, and helps them make personal connections to words—on
kindergarten students’ general vocabulary knowledge. In this observational study, Neugebauer
and colleagues examined teachers’ language use within the context of their designated
vocabulary instruction time, which made use of the Elements of Reading Vocabulary program
(Beck & McKeown, 2004). Neugebauer and colleagues found that teachers’ word consciousness
talk was positively associated with gains in kindergarten students’ general vocabulary knowledge
16
at the end of the school year. In the present study, I considered how the teachers’ talk addressed
affective factors such as these.
In summary, in the present study, I examined the oral language interactions between
teachers and students in early-elementary classrooms to identify teachers’ vocabulary talk
moves. When identifying these vocabulary talk moves, I considered the range of instructional
practices that research has shown to support oral language and vocabulary development (i.e.,
building knowledge of word meanings, scaffolding students’ word use, and building awareness
of words and word learning—detailed in the next section), along with practices that addressed
affective factors based on the theory of word consciousness.
How Children Learn Words
The research on how children learn words aligns with three broad instructional
approaches: building knowledge of word meanings, scaffolding students’ word use, and building
awareness of words and word learning. In the following sections, I present an overview of the
literature related to each approach. Specifically, I focus on studies at the preschool and
elementary levels. It is important to note that a majority of studies that examine how children
learn words have been conducted within the context of some sort of interactive read-aloud
experience in which an adult (i.e., typically the teacher) reads a text aloud to children and
engages children in that reading experience in a variety of ways (e.g., Barnes & Dickinson, 2018;
Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010;
Dickinson & Smith, 1994; Dickinson & Tabors, 1991; Hadley et al., 2019; McKeown & Beck,
2003; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond,
2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014). In this context, children are exposed to language through the
text itself and through extra-textual talk, or the talk that happens around the text (Biemiller &
17
Boote, 2006; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Wright, 2018).
Only a few of the studies examining how children learn words have been conducted outside of
literacy instruction (e.g., Baumann et al., 2003; Freeman et al., 2019; Lubliner & Smetana,
2005).
Knowledge and Understanding
The most researched approach to vocabulary instruction is building students’ knowledge
of word meanings. This is unsurprising, as understanding word meanings—whether learning the
meaning of an unfamiliar word or learning a new meaning for a familiar word—is at the core of
vocabulary development. Studies have consistently shown that providing students with
explanations or definitions of the meanings of target words (i.e., words that have been targeted
for instruction) promotes students’ understanding of those words (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001,
2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985;
McKeown & Beck, 2003; Nash & Snowling, 2006; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman et al.,
2014; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond,
2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014). For example, Biemiller and Boote (2006) found that providing
kindergarten, first-, and second-grade students with explanations of target words during
interactive read-alouds was more effective than not providing explanations, with a pretest-
posttest gain of 22% for instructed words vs. 12% for uninstructed words. Likewise, Coyne et al.
(2007, 2009) found that kindergarten students scored significantly higher on vocabulary
measures for target words for which they received simple definitions than with target words they
encountered in text without explanation or discussion (i.e., incidental exposure).
Studies have also consistently shown that introducing target words within rich contexts
(e.g., interactive read-alouds) and discussing the meaning(s) of target words in various contexts
18
supports students’ understanding of target word meaning(s) (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001,
2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985;
McKeown & Beck, 2003; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000;
Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman et al., 2014; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Stahl &
Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014). For
example, using a within-subjects comparison, Coyne et al. (2007) found that kindergarten
students scored significantly higher on expressive and receptive measures of target word
definitions and on a measure of understanding target words in a novel context when they
engaged in activities that allowed them to interact with and discuss target words in a variety of
meaningful contexts compared to when they received just incidental exposure to target words
during interactive read-alouds.
Multiple exposures to target words is another tenet of vocabulary instruction that
promotes understanding of word meanings (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Biemiller &
Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985; McKeown & Beck,
2003; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Robbins & Ehri, 1994;
Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al.,
2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014). For example, McKeown et al. (1983) and
McKeown et al. (1985) compared the effectiveness of two different frequencies of instructional
encounters with target words and found that more frequent encounters with target words led to
better outcomes for fourth-grade students on measures of definition knowledge, fluency of
access to word meanings (i.e., lexical access), understanding of word meaning in a specific
context (i.e., context interpretation), and story comprehension. Similarly, Beck and McKeown
(2007) found that vocabulary gains were twice as large for kindergarten and first-grade students
19
with words receiving six days of instruction with 20 total encounters per target word compared
to words receiving three of days of instruction with five total encounters per target word.
Many studies have also shown that understanding of word meanings is developed by
engaging students in active processing or deeper processing—that is, engaging students with
words and their meanings beyond just associating a word with its meaning (e.g., Beck &
McKeown, 2007; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown, 1985; McKeown et al., 1983;
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b;
Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001;
Zipoli et al., 2011). For example, McKeown et al. (1985) found that students who explored
various aspects of target word meanings—such as by identifying relationships between words,
responding affectively and cognitively to words, and applying words to various contexts—
performed better on measures of context interpretation (i.e., understanding of word meaning in a
specific context) and comprehension of text containing target words than students who were just
encouraged to make a simple association between each target word and its definition or
synonym. Similarly, Coyne et al. (2007, 2009) found that engaging kindergarten students in
deeper processing activities such as recognizing examples of target words, answering questions
about target words, producing sentences with target words, and answering questions containing
more than one target word (i.e., extended instruction) produced significantly higher scores on
vocabulary measures than instruction in which (a) a simple definition was provided for target
words as they were encountered in text (i.e., embedded instruction) or (b) target words were
encountered in text with no discussion or explanation (i.e., incidental exposure).
Studies have also shown that providing students with an image depicting a target word
(e.g., Coyne et al., 2009, 2010; Rosenthal & Ehri, 2008; Silverman, 2007a; Silverman et al.,
20
2014; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001), showing an object
representing a target word (e.g., Silverman et al., 2014; Wasik et al., 2006; Wasik & Bond, 2001;
Wasik & Hindman, 2014), and/or acting out or demonstrating a target word—or engaging
students in doing so—(e.g., Silverman, 2007a; Silverman et al., 2014; Silverman & Crandell,
2010) can be effective methods for building students’ understanding of word meanings. While
many studies have included these components within multicomponent vocabulary instruction and
examined the overall impact of these interventions (e.g., Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Wasik et al.,
2006, 2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014), Silverman and Crandell (2010)
examined the relationship between specific vocabulary instruction practices and prekindergarten
and kindergarten students’ vocabulary. They found that providing a visual support (e.g., picture)
and/or kinesthetic cue (e.g., gesture) during interactive read-aloud time was positively associated
with vocabulary growth on a standardized measure of general vocabulary knowledge for children
with lower initial vocabulary knowledge but was negatively associated with vocabulary growth
for children with higher initial vocabulary knowledge. This suggests that these practices may be
beneficial for building vocabulary knowledge with some students.
Multicomponent vocabulary instruction often includes highlighting examples or
nonexamples that illustrate what target words mean (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2007; Coyne et al.,
2007, 2009, 2010; Silverman, 2007a; Silverman et al., 2014). For instance, Beck and McKeown
(2007) provided first-grade students with multiple examples of target words in multiple contexts.
Likewise, Coyne et al. (2007, 2009, 2010) provided kindergarten students with examples of how
target words are used and engaged students in determining whether or not a picture was an
example of a target word. Similarly, Silverman's (2007a) Multidimensional Vocabulary Program
included providing kindergarten students with examples to show how target words are used in
21
contexts outside of the initial text in which they were introduced. Although these studies of
multicomponent vocabulary instruction did not examine the specific impact of highlighting
examples of target words, instruction that included examples was found to be effective.
Many studies have also suggested that drawing students’ attention to the spelling of target
words by showing students the words in print (i.e., providing orthographic information) and/or
drawing students’ attention to the sounds in target words (i.e., highlighting phonological
information) promotes learning the meanings of those words (e.g., Chambrè et al., 2019; Ehri,
2014; Ehri & Rosenthal, 2007; Jubenville et al., 2014; Juel & Deffes, 2004; Ricketts et al., 2009;
Rosenthal & Ehri, 2008; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman & Crandell, 2010). For example,
Rosenthal and Ehri (2008) found that showing second- and fifth-grade students target words in
print enhanced their memory for pronunciations and meanings of those words compared to target
words that were not shown. Further, they found that fifth-grade students with more orthographic
knowledge benefitted more from seeing target words in print than students with less orthographic
knowledge. In a related study, Silverman (2007b) found that kindergarten students who received
vocabulary instruction that included attention to letters and sounds in target words had
significantly higher scores on a receptive measure of target word knowledge six months later,
when tested in first grade, compared to students whose vocabulary instruction did not attend to
the orthographic and phonological aspects of target words. Likewise, Silverman and Crandell
(2010) found a positive relationship between instruction in which prekindergarten and
kindergarten teachers called attention to the sounds or spelling of vocabulary words during
interactive read-alouds and students’ vocabulary growth. However, not all studies examining the
impact of showing target word spellings on students’ understanding of these words have found
this practice to be effective. Chambré et al. (2017) found that while first-grade students’ memory
22
for pronunciation of target words was improved by seeing target words in print, their memory for
target word meanings was not improved by this exposure to target word spellings. Chambré and
colleagues speculated that this lack of connection between spelling and meaning may be caused
by the lack of a grapho-semantic mapping system comparable to the grapho-phonemic system—
at least for the target words examined in their study. They hypothesized that students’ memory
for target word meanings might be improved by seeing target words in print if those target words
contained familiar letter patterns representing root words and affixes. Therefore, the benefit of
showing students target words in print may depend, at least in part, on orthographic features of
those words.
To support students’ understanding of target word meanings, studies have incorporated
systematic review of these words within their instructional sequence (e.g., Beck & McKeown,
2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2010; McKeown, 1985; McKeown et al., 1983;
Silverman, 2007a; Zipoli et al., 2011). Many of these studies have included target word review
as one part of multicomponent vocabulary instruction and examined the overall impact of these
interventions (e.g., McKeown et al., 1983, 1985; Silverman, 2007a); however, Zipoli et al.
(2011) specifically examined the relationship between systematic word review and target word
learning. Within an 18-week program of extended vocabulary instruction (i.e., instruction
promoting deeper processing of target words through extension activities) with kindergarten
students, they found that students performed twice as well on a measure of target word
knowledge with words that were systematically reviewed than with words in the no review
comparison condition.
Finally, studies have shown that teaching target words in conceptually- or semantically-
related sets promotes students’ knowledge of word meanings (e.g., Cervetti et al., 2016; Hadley
23
et al., 2019; Neuman et al., 2011; Neuman & Dwyer, 2011; Neuman & Kaefer, 2013, 2018;
Pollard-Durodola et al., 2011; Zipoli et al., 2011). For example, Neuman et al. (2011) found that
preschool children who were taught target words within taxonomic categories (i.e., grouping of
objects based on shared properties) outperformed children in the comparison condition on
measures of word knowledge, expressive language, conceptual knowledge, and categories and
properties of concepts. Further, children receiving the intervention were able to use categories to
determine the meaning of novel words. Similarly, Hadley et al. (2019) compared the effects of
teaching words in taxonomic groups (e.g., vegetables: artichoke, cauliflower, eggplant) verses
themes (e.g., vines, raw, soil) on preschool children’s growth in vocabulary depth. They found
that preschool children showed greater growth in vocabulary depth for words taught in
taxonomies than for words taught in themes and learned information about category
membership, perceptual features, and object function more deeply for words in the taxonomy
condition than for words in the theme condition. Likewise, Cervetti et al. (2016) found that
fourth-grade students who read a set of conceptually-coherent informational texts (i.e., texts that
cohered around a set of concepts related to a topic) showed greater knowledge of concepts in
their texts, greater knowledge of target words in their texts, and greater recall of novel text
compared to students who read a set of unrelated informational texts. In a related study, Zipoli et
al. (2011) found that kindergarten students learned significantly more target words with
semantically-related review (i.e., systematic review that draws attention to the semantic features
of words and emphasizes associations between target words and familiar words and concepts)
than with embedded review (i.e., systematic review that defines target words as they appear in
various texts beyond the initial text in which they were introduced). Together, these studies
24
demonstrate that students’ understanding of word meanings is supported when target words are
presented in conceptually- or semantically-related sets.
In summary, these studies focused on building students’ knowledge of word meanings
found that students’ word learning was supported by instruction that provided definitional and
contextual information, multiple exposures in a variety of rich contexts, opportunities for
students to engage in deeper processing than just associating a word with its meaning, visual
supports (e.g., image, demonstration), examples, attention to target word spelling and/or sounds,
and systematic review. Presenting target words in conceptually- or semantically-related sets was
also found to build students’ knowledge of word meanings. In the present study, I identified the
ways in which teachers used language to build students’ knowledge of word meanings that
aligned with this research base.
Student Word Use
The research on scaffolding students’ word use overlaps widely with the research on
building students’ knowledge of word meanings, as many studies have examined
multicomponent vocabulary instruction that includes opportunities for students to engage in
using the target words (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010;
McKeown, 1985; McKeown et al., 1983; McKeown & Beck, 2003; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b;
Wasik & Bond, 2001). A common practice in vocabulary instruction is to encourage students to
repeat (pronounce) target words in order to create phonological representations of them (e.g.,
Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; Juel & Deffes, 2004; McKeown
et al., 1983, 1985; McKeown & Beck, 2003; Rosenthal & Ehri, 2011; Silverman, 2007a;
Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Zipoli et al., 2011). For example, as part of the Text Talk approach
to vocabulary instruction (Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; McKeown & Beck, 2003),
25
kindergarten and first-grade students are encouraged to say target words aloud as part of their
introduction. While many of these studies included the practice of asking students to say target
words aloud within multicomponent vocabulary instruction and investigated the overall impact
of this instruction, Rosenthal and Ehri (2011) specifically examined the impact of pronouncing
target words aloud on students’ word learning. They found that encouraging fifth-grade students
to say aloud target words as they were encountered in contextually supportive passages during
silent reading promoted vocabulary learning, with larger effect sizes for students who initially
scored lower on a measure of word identification.
Beyond pronouncing target words, many studies have engaged students in some form of
deeper processing (as promoted by Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986) that required students to use the
target words in speech or writing (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2007; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010;
McKeown, 1985; McKeown et al., 1983; Silverman, 2007b; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wasik
et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Zipoli et al., 2011). For example, McKeown et al. (1985)
engaged students in applying target words to various contexts. Similarly, Coyne et al. (2007)
engaged students in producing sentences with target words. McKeown et al. (1983, 1985)
engaged students in the Word Wizard activity, which promoted students’ use of target words by
encouraging them to be aware of target words outside of vocabulary lessons and engage with
them independently.
In summary, these studies focused on scaffolding students’ word use found that students’
word learning was supported by encouraging them to say target words aloud and use target
words in speech or writing. In the present study, I identified the ways in which teachers used
language to scaffold students’ word use that aligned with this research base.
26
Metalinguistic and Metacognitive Awareness
Another approach to vocabulary instruction is to build students’ awareness of words and
word learning by attending to their metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness. As Scott and
Nagy (2004) explained, metalinguistic awareness refers to the ability to reflect on and
manipulate language units. This includes morphological awareness, or the awareness of word
parts and how the meanings of those parts contribute to the overall meaning of the word;
syntactic awareness, or the ability to reflect on and manipulate word order at the sentence level;
and metasemantic awareness, or the ability to reflect on word meanings. Metalinguistic
awareness is a specific kind of metacognitive awareness (Nagy & Scott, 2000; Scott & Nagy,
2004). Wade and Reynolds (1989) defined metacognition as “the ability to think about and
control one’s own learning,” which they noted is dependent on an awareness of what to learn,
how to learn it, and whether or to what degree it has been learned (p. 6). Similarly, Kuhn (2000)
defined metacognition as “cognition that reflects on, monitors, or regulates first-order cognition”
(p. 178). The studies reviewed here focus on metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness as
relating to word learning.
Many studies have found that teaching students to find and use context clues (i.e.,
contextual analysis) supports word learning (e.g., Baumann et al., 2002, 2003; Fukkink & de
Glopper, 1998; Lubliner & Smetana, 2005; Nash & Snowling, 2006; Wise, 2019). For example,
Fukkink and de Glopper (1998) investigated instruction aimed at supporting students with
deliberately deriving word meanings from context while reading in their meta-analysis of 21
studies. They found that context clue instruction, in which students learn to recognize and use
specific types of context clues to determine the meanings of unfamiliar words, was more
effective than cloze instruction, in which students use context to fill in words that have been
27
removed from text; strategy instruction, in which students are taught to infer word meanings
from context generally without reference to specific types of context clues; definition instruction,
in which students develop conceptual understanding of what constitutes a definition; and
practice-only instruction, in which students practice deriving word meanings from context
without further instruction. Similarly, Nash and Snowling (2006) compared definition instruction
(i.e., providing students with child-friendly definitions of target words) with context instruction
(i.e., teaching students to look for clues within the given context to derive the meaning of target
words) on the vocabulary knowledge of 7- and 8-year-old students with “poor existing
vocabulary knowledge” (p. 335). They found that immediately following the intervention,
students in both conditions showed equivalent improvement in vocabulary knowledge for target
words; however, three months later, students in the context instruction condition showed
significantly better expressive vocabulary, comprehended text containing target words
significantly better, and could independently derive word meanings from written context. While
Silverman et al. (2014) found a null effect for context clue instruction on vocabulary learning in
their observational study, they posit that this may be due to the fact that context clue instruction
was largely absent in their observations in third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade classrooms.
Many studies have also found that teaching students to look for word-part clues (i.e.,
morphemic analysis) supports word learning (Baumann et al., 2002, 2003; P. N. Bowers &
Kirby, 2010; Freeman et al., 2019; Lubliner & Smetana, 2005; Silverman et al., 2014). For
example, Freeman et al. (2019) used a collective case study design to explore how three first-
grade students responded to a small-group intervention with explicit morphological instruction in
Greek and Latin roots (e.g., micro, bio, geo, rupt), in which students engaged in finding the
target roots in words within text, applying knowledge of root meaning to derive the meaning of
28
the word, and creating new words or recalling words belonging to the same morphological word
family. Freeman and colleagues found that this instruction increased students’ morphological
awareness, as they were able to find roots in words and were developing the ability to analyze
word features and look for clues to their meaning. Likewise, P. N. Bowers & Kirby (2010) found
that, after controlling for initial vocabulary, fourth- and fifth-grade students who received
instruction focused on morphological word structure were significantly better at identifying the
base of complex words and explaining the meaning of target words and novel words with taught
bases and affixes than students in the business-as-usual comparison condition.
Studies have also examined the combination of contextual analysis and morphemic
analysis (e.g., Baumann et al. 2002, 2003). Baumann et al. (2002) examined the effects of (a)
morphemic analysis, (b) contextual analysis, and (c) morphemic and contextual analysis on fifth-
grade students’ ability to recall the meanings of instructed words, to infer the meanings of
uninstructed (transfer) words using morphemic and/or contextual analysis, and to comprehend
text containing transfer words. They found that morphemic and contextual analysis enabled
students to infer the meanings of untaught (transfer) words immediately following instruction,
although this effect faded with time. Further, Baumann and colleagues found that combined
morphemic and contextual instruction was just as effective in supporting students with inferring
word meanings as when morphemic analysis and contextual analysis instruction were provided
separately. Building on this work, Baumann et al. (2003) compared the effects of teaching fifth-
grade students the meanings of textbook vocabulary words with teaching students to use
morphemic and contextual clues to infer the meaning of unfamiliar words. They found that
students who were taught the meanings of specific words from the textbook selection were more
successful at learning the meanings of those words, while students who were taught to use
29
morphemic and contextual analysis were more successful at inferring the meanings of
uninstructed affixed words. These students were also more successful on a delayed test at
inferring the meanings of words that had word part or context clues. This suggests that teaching
students to use morphemic and contextual analysis is a generative way to support word learning,
extending beyond specific target words.
Another way to support students’ metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness is to
engage them deriving word meanings from context and then explaining the reasoning behind a
definition (Cain, 2007). Cain found that 7- and 8-year-old students who explained the reasoning
behind their own definition or the definition given by the experimenter made greater gains in
defintion accuracy than students who just derived word meanings from context without giving an
explanation. Further, Cain found that students who explained their own reasoning had greater
insight into how the meaning was derived than students who explained the experimenter’s
reasoning.
Teaching students to detect semantic ambiguity has also been found to support students’
metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness (Zipke et al., 2009). Zipke and colleagues found that
third-grade students who received metalinguistic ambiguity instruction focused on analyzing
multiple meaning words (e.g., watch) and sentences (e.g., The ball was found by the kitten) in
isolation, in riddles, and in texts were better able to provide multiple definitions of ambiguous
words and explain double meanings of ambiguous sentences than students who did not receive
this instruction.
Studies have also found that direct instruction of metalinguistic and metacognitive
strategies is beneficial for word learning (e.g., Boulware-Gooden et al., 2007; Lubliner &
Smetana, 2005). For example, Lubliner and Smetana (2005) investigated the effects of a
30
multifaceted, metacognitive vocabulary intervention on fifth-grade students’ vocabulary
achievement. The intervention focused on teaching self-monitoring and self-regulation, with the
goal of helping students learn to monitor their understanding of words and internalize and
implement word-learning strategies to support reading comprehension. Students were taught the
metacognitive strategies of looking for context clues, substituting a familiar synonym for an
unfamiliar word, using familiar roots and affixes to determine a word’s meaning, considering if
the word sounds familiar and trying to remember its meaning, asking someone the meaning of a
word, and marking where an unfamiliar word is encountered in order to follow up on its meaning
at a later time. Students were also taught to monitor and rank their level of knowledge for a
word, from unknown to partially known to fully known. Lubliner and Smetana found that
students were better able to self-monitor their word knowledge and made significant gains in
vocabulary acquisition with the intervention compared to business-as-usual instruction. Further,
in a between-subjects comparison of students in a Title I school receiving the intervention and
students in an “above-average-performing school” (p. 163) receiving business-as-usual
vocabulary instruction, the significant difference in vocabulary acquisition between these groups
prior to the intervention was no longer significant following the intervention. Likewise,
Boulware-Gooden et al. (2007) examined the effectiveness of systematic direct instruction of
metacognitive strategies on third-grade students’ vocabulary achievement, finding that students
receiving this instruction improved significantly in vocabulary over students in the comparison
condition, with a 40% difference in gains in vocabulary between the two groups.
In summary, these studies focused on building students’ metalinguistic and metacognitive
awareness found that students’ word learning was supported when students learned to use
contextual and morphemic analysis, explain how word meanings were derived from context,
31
detect semantic ambiguity, and self-monitor and self-regulate. In the present study, I identified
the ways in which teachers used language to build students’ awareness of words and word
learning that aligned with this research base.
As the literature reviewed here has demonstrated, multifaceted instruction supports oral
language and vocabulary development, with students benefiting from approaches that build their
knowledge of word meanings, scaffold their word use, and build their awareness of words and
word learning. In the present study, I considered how the teachers’ talk addressed each of these
approaches. In the following section, I present research supporting the integration of this kind of
language and literacy instruction with science instruction.
Opportunities for Oral Language and Vocabulary Development in Science Instruction
As noted in Chapter 1, the Next Generation Science Standards (NGSS) emphasize oral
language development in the early-elementary grades (NGSS Lead States, 2013). In their content
analysis of the NGSS framework document (National Research Council, 2007) and student
learning expectations (NGSS Lead States, 2013), Wright and Domke (2019) found that oral
language is emphasized as a way to support students’ sensemaking and for students to
communicate their learning and ideas. Further, they found that the NGSS framework document
encourages educators to provide students with opportunities to learn, use, and apply discipline-
specific vocabulary during science instruction. Therefore, NGSS-aligned science instruction
would provide many opportunities for oral language and vocabulary development (NGSS Lead
States, 2013; Wright & Domke, 2019).
Indeed, science instruction has been established as a rich context for oral language and
vocabulary development (e.g., Gotwals & Wright, 2017; Wright & Gotwals, 2017; Wright &
Neuman, 2014). In their observational study of vocabulary instruction across the school day in
32
kindergarten classrooms, Wright and Neuman (2014) found that although students received, on
average, only 2.5 minutes of science instruction per day, science instruction provided rich
opportunities for vocabulary instruction when taught. Specifically, teachers provided
explanations of target words or helped students determine target word meanings more times
during science instruction, social studies instruction, and interactive read-alouds than during
reading and writing instruction, math instruction, or morning meeting. Wright and Gotwals
(2017) found that vocabulary learning within science instruction was supported by curriculum
materials designed to engage students in disciplinary oral language—or the ways that scientists
talk. They created curriculum materials to support kindergarten students’ science talk and then
conducted a quasi-experimental study to examine students’ oral language outcomes. Wright and
Gotwals found that students in the intervention condition outperformed students in the business-
as-usual comparison condition on measures of receptive science vocabulary and use of
vocabulary in a science context.
Research has shown that integrating language and literacy instruction with science
instruction is beneficial for both literacy and science learning (e.g., Cervetti et al., 2012; Connor
et al., 2017; Gotwals & Wright, 2017, 2017; Guthrie et al., 1999; Romance & Vitale, 1992, 2001;
Varelas et al., 2006; Varelas & Pappas, 2006; Vitale & Romance, 2012; Wang & Herman, 2006).
For example, Guthrie et al. (1999, 2009) examined the effects of Concept-Oriented Reading
Instruction (CORI), which integrated supports for reading motivation, oral reading fluency, and
reading comprehension within science instruction focused on building students’ content
knowledge of ecology. They found that fifth-grade students in the CORI intervention condition
scored higher on posttest measures of word recognition speed, reading comprehension, and
ecological knowledge than students in the comparison condition receiving traditional instruction.
33
Further, they found that the CORI intervention was equally effective for low- and high-achieving
readers (Guthrie et al., 2009). Similarly, Romance and Vitale (2001) found that students in
grades 2-5 showed improved science understanding and reading achievement when traditional
literacy instruction was replaced with science instruction that encompassed reading
comprehension and language arts skills (i.e., In-Depth Expanded Applications of Science;
IDEAS). Likewise, Wang and Herman, (2006) examined how integrated units in the Seeds of
Science/Roots of Reading project (Seeds/Roots) impacted the interest, motivation, and learning
of students in grades 2-4, finding that students in the Seeds/Roots intervention condition learned
significantly more than students in the comparison condition on all science and literacy
measures. In a related study, Cervetti et al. (2012) found that fourth-grade students who received
integrated science-literacy instruction that engaged students in reading text, writing notes and
reports, conducing firsthand investigations, and discussing key concepts and processes made
significantly greater gains on measures of science understanding, science vocabulary, and
science writing than students receiving science instruction that was not integrated with literacy
instruction. Similarly, Connor et al. (2017) found that kindergarten through fourth-grade students
receiving Content-Area Literacy Instruction (CALI) as an individualized instructional program
showed improved science knowledge and oral and reading comprehension skills.
In summary, science instruction offers a rich context for oral language and vocabulary
development, and instruction that integrates science and literacy has been shown to effectively
promote both science and literacy learning. In the present study, I examined the vocabulary talk
moves teachers made within the context of science instruction because, despite the emphasis
placed on oral language development by national standards and the research supporting the
integration of language and literacy instruction with science instruction, little is known about
34
how teachers promote oral language and vocabulary development in science instruction in the
early-elementary grades.
The Present Study
The purpose of the present study is to examine the ways in which teachers use language to
promote oral language and vocabulary development (i.e., vocabulary talk moves) during science
instruction in early-elementary classrooms. Science instruction offers a rich context for oral
language and vocabulary development and national standards emphasize supporting this
development, yet little is known about how teachers promote oral language and vocabulary
development within this context in the early-elementary grades. Therefore, I ask the following
questions:
1. How do teachers use language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development during science instruction in early-elementary classrooms?
2. Is vocabulary talk related to the language aspects of science talk, and if so, how?
3. What features of science curriculum materials are related to a kindergarten teacher’s
enhanced vocabulary talk?
Summary
Oral language and vocabulary development are important for students’ overall academic
success. Oral language and vocabulary support science learning, while the background
knowledge and vocabulary students gain from science learning supports their reading
comprehension. Current national standards emphasize the importance of oral language and
vocabulary development, beginning in the early-elementary grades. Science instruction offers a
rich context for oral language and vocabulary development; however, little is known about how
teachers promote oral language and vocabulary development within science instruction in the
35
early-elementary grades. I seek to address this gap in the literature with this dissertation study.
We must first understand the ways in which teachers do and do not promote oral language and
vocabulary development during science instruction in the early-elementary grades in order to
best support teachers in implementing effective oral language and vocabulary instruction. This is
important so that all students are prepared to meet these ambitious standards and, ultimately,
develop the oral language and vocabulary needed to strengthen their reading comprehension and
support their science learning.
36
CHAPTER 3—METHODS
Given the importance of oral language and vocabulary development to students’ literacy
and science learning (e.g., Anderson & Freebody, 1981; Guthrie et al., 1999; Mercer et al., 2004;
Richmond & Striley, 1996; Senechal et al., 2006; Wang & Herman, 2006; Wright & Gotwals,
2017), I conducted two instrumental case studies (Barone, 2011; Stake, 2000) to examine
teachers’ vocabulary talk during science instruction in the early-elementary grades. Together,
these instrumental case studies comprise my dissertation study. In the first study, I examined the
science instruction of a cohort of eight early-elementary teachers (i.e., teachers of early-
elementary students—rather than elementary teachers who are early in their career) in order to
determine (1) how they used language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development and (2) whether vocabulary talk is related to the language aspects of science talk,
and if so, how. In the second study, I examined the science instruction of one kindergarten
teacher in order to identify features of science curriculum materials related to enhanced
vocabulary talk. In this chapter, I describe the methodology used in these studies. First, I provide
information about the context of the larger study within which this dissertation was situated.
Then, I explain the design and logic, case, data sources, and analytic strategies for each of the
studies. Finally, I discuss limitations of the methodology.
Context of Larger Study
The present dissertation study is situated within a larger study of science talk in early-
elementary classrooms. The Science, Oral Language, and Literacy Development from the Start
of School (SOLID Start) project (PIs: Dr. Tanya Wright & Dr. Amelia Gotwals), funded by the
National Science Foundation (NSF; Award Number: 1620580), is focused on supporting K-2
teachers with promoting science talk in their classrooms. This larger study is a 10-week usability
37
trial of the SOLID Start professional development model with eight early-elementary teachers.
With this model, teachers read texts and watch videos related to supporting equitable science
participation and engagement, deepening science understanding, and developing language and
literacy for science. They meet with other participating teachers and an instructional coach in
professional learning communities (PLCs) to discuss these texts and videos and to plan an
upcoming science lesson. Teachers learn about the SOLID Start Tool, a formative observational
tool that can be used to provide feedback or facilitate self-reflection on talk that promotes
sensemaking during science instruction. After teaching a science lesson, teachers use the SOLID
Start Tool to reflect on opportunities for science talk and then meet virtually with an instructional
coach to debrief the lesson and receive feedback through the lens of the Tool. The focus of this
larger study was to gain insight into various aspects of the usability of the professional
development model, such as if teachers could video record and upload their own lessons, interact
with their instructional coach and other participants using the Zoom online meeting platform, and
use the SOLID Start Tool. Because teachers recorded their science instruction as part of the
study, this SOLID Start professional development usability trial provided the context for me to
study vocabulary talk during science instruction.
Study 1: Vocabulary and Science Talk in Eight Early-Elementary Classrooms
This study addresses two research questions: (1) How do teachers use language to
promote students’ oral language and vocabulary development during science instruction in early-
elementary classrooms? and (2) Is vocabulary talk related to the language aspects of science talk,
and if so, how? In the sections that follow, I describe the design and logic, participants, data
sources, and analytic strategies used to address these research questions.
38
Design and Logic
I used an instrumental case study design in order to investigate how teachers use
language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary development during science
instruction at the early-elementary grades and whether/how vocabulary talk is related to the
language aspects of science talk. Instrumental case studies allow researchers to look for insight
into an issue (Barone, 2011; Stake, 2000). With this design, the case plays a supporting role by
facilitating understanding of the issue of interest. The case, itself, although studied in depth, is of
secondary interest to the primary issue. In this first study, the case is the science instruction of
the cohort of eight early-elementary teachers who participated in this study from February-May
2019. The issue of primary interest is how the teachers used language to promote oral language
and vocabulary development during science instruction. Therefore, I studied the science
instruction of the cohort in depth to better understand this issue of primary interest.
Participants
The case examined in this study is the science instruction of a cohort of eight early-
elementary teachers: one young 5s teacher, three kindergarten teachers, two first-grade teachers,
and two second-grade teachers. All participants identified as White/European American females
and taught in the same elementary school in a small district within a large Midwestern state. The
teachers had between 0.5-19 years of teaching experience. All eight teachers had a bachelor’s
degree, and three teachers had also completed master’s degrees. At the time of the study, 27.15%
of students at the school were eligible for free or reduced-price lunch. Of the 188 students in the
eight classrooms, 95.74% identified as White or European American, 1.6% identified a Black or
African American, 1.6% identified as biracial or multiethnic, 0.53% identified as Hispanic or
Latino/a, and 0.53% identified as Asian or Pacific Islander. No students were learning English as
39
an additional language. In the following chapter, I introduce the individual teachers who together
comprise the case I examined in order to better understand vocabulary talk during science
instruction in the early-elementary grades.
Teachers were invited to participate in the SOLID Start professional development
usability trial with enrollment on a first come, first-served basis. They were invited to serve as
research partners, by (a) participating in a 10-week online professional development focused on
supporting talk around equitable science participation and engagement, deepening science
understanding, and developing language and literacy for science; (b) uploading three videos of
themselves teaching science lessons throughout the course and receiving feedback from a coach
on these lessons [this was later adjusted to two videos based on teacher feedback]; and (c)
providing feedback on the professional development experience through surveys, interviews,
and/or focus groups. Participants received a $500 stipend and 15 State Continuing Education
Clock Hours (SCECHs). (See Appendix A for complete recruitment information.)
Data Sources
This study draws on three data sources: a demographic background and teaching
experience information survey, weekly instructional logs, and video recordings of participants
teaching science lessons. Following, I describe these data sources and the rationale for their use.
Demographic Background and Teaching Experience Information Survey
Each participating teacher completed a survey at the very beginning of the study,
providing information on the following: demographics (e.g., age, gender, race/ethnicity),
professional experience (e.g., type(s) of teaching degree(s) earned, type of teaching certification,
years of teaching experience), professional development (e.g., content areas/topics of recent
professional development), setting description (e.g., demographics of students), and science and
40
literacy instruction (e.g., curriculum materials, instructional minutes per week for language arts
instruction and science instruction, confidence with specific instructional practices). (See
Appendix B for full survey.) This survey gave me background information about the
participating teachers, their students, and aspects of their instruction, which deepened my
understanding of the case (i.e., the science instruction of the cohort of participating teachers)
from which I examined the issue of interest (i.e. vocabulary talk during science instruction)
(Barone, 2011; Stake, 2000).
Weekly Instructional Log
I collected information from participating teachers each week regarding the number of
minutes of science instruction they taught each day, the activities within their science lessons
(e.g., reading aloud of informational text, watching a video, planning and/or carrying out an
investigation), and the science curriculum materials they used. (See Appendix C for full
instructional log.) Using a Google Form, I collected this data beginning the week before the
professional development started and continuing through Week 10. This log provided further
information about the science instruction of the cohort of teachers comprising the case I studied
to better understand vocabulary talk during science instruction.
Video Recordings of Science Lessons
As part of the SOLID Start professional development usability trial, each participating
teacher’s science instruction was video recorded at three points in the professional development:
before the professional development experience began (PrePD), during Week 5, and during
Week 9. This is consistent with other observational studies that have found analyzing data from
three timepoints to be adequate for capturing classroom instruction (e.g., Silverman et al., 2014;
Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wasik & Bond, 2001). For these lessons (and throughout the study
41
in general), teachers could use any science curriculum or materials they had available to them. A
member of the SOLID Start research team recorded the PrePD lesson, and teachers then recorded
and uploaded the Week 5 and Week 9 lessons themselves. These 24 lesson videos totaled 894.27
minutes of observational data, with lessons ranging from 13:27 to 1:13:25. As my primary data
source, the video recordings captured early-elementary science instruction, enabling me to
examine the ways in which the participating teachers used language to promote oral language
and vocabulary development (i.e., vocabulary talk) and how this might relate to science talk.
Analytic Strategies
I used both qualitative and quantitative analytic strategies to address my research
questions. In the following sections, I detail the analysis methods for each research question.
Research Question 1: How Do Teachers Use Language to Promote Students’ Oral Language
and Vocabulary Development During Science Instruction in Early-Elementary Classrooms?
Based on Gee's (2014) notion that language is used to carry out specific actions rather
than just to give one another information, I used discourse analysis techniques to examine the
ways in which teachers used language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development. In the present study, I added to and refined a codebook for vocabulary talk that I
initially developed in a pilot study examining one kindergarten teacher’s vocabulary talk during
421.96 minutes of science instruction across 10 video recorded lesson observations. In the
following sections, I first describe the how the codebook was initially developed in the pilot
study and then describe my analysis in the present study.
Pilot study. In the initial study, I used teacher turns (i.e., everything the teacher said until
another person spoke) as my unit of analysis, and I used two levels of codes. The first-level
codes were provisional codes (Miles et al., 2014; Saldaña, 2016) derived from the literature on
42
how children learn words (as reviewed in Chapter 2). Based on vocabulary instruction research,
the KNOWLEDGE & UNDERSTANDING code captured talk that builds knowledge of word
meanings, the STUDENT WORD USE code captured talk that scaffolds students’ word use, and
the METALINGUISTIC & METACOGNITIVE AWARENESS code captured talk that builds
students’ awareness of words and word learning. Finally, based on the theory of word
consciousness (e.g., Anderson & Nagy, 1992; Graves & Watts-Taffe, 2002, 2008), the
AFFECTIVE FACTORS code captured talk that interested students in words and word learning.
I refer to these first-level codes as types of vocabulary talk.
The second-level codes identified the specific vocabulary talk moves the teacher made
within each type of vocabulary talk (i.e., first-level codes). Using pattern coding (Miles et al.,
2014; Saldaña, 2016), I focused on one type of vocabulary talk at a time (e.g., building
knowledge of word meanings, as captured by the first-level code KNOWLEDGE &
UNDERSTANDING) but repeated this process for each type of vocabulary talk. First, I wrote
descriptions of the teacher’s turns, summarizing what the teacher said and naming what she did
through language. Through an iterative process, I read teacher turns, wrote descriptions, noticed
patterns in descriptions, wrote new descriptions to capture these patterns, reread teacher turns,
and refined descriptions. Through these descriptions, I identified the specific vocabulary talk
moves the teacher made within each of the four types of vocabulary talk. For example,
explaining or defining the meaning of a word was a vocabulary talk move the teacher made (i.e.,
second-level code EXPLAIN/DEFINE) within vocabulary talk that builds knowledge of word
meanings (i.e., first-level code KNOWLEDGE & UNDERSTANDING). In total, during this
initial study, I found 24 different vocabulary talk moves that the teacher made within the four
types of vocabulary talk. The initial codebook I established during the pilot study provided the
43
provisional codes (Miles et al., 2014; Saldaña, 2016) (first- and second-level codes) for the
present student.
Present study. In the present study, I used teacher utterances as my unit of analysis
rather than teacher turns. I found in my pilot study that the kindergarten teacher’s turns were
often quite long and involved a number of different vocabulary talk moves. In fact, I found that
the teacher often used a vocabulary talk move more than once within a turn; however, because
each second-level code could only be applied once to a turn, this repeated use of vocabulary talk
moves within a turn was not captured. Silverman et al. (2014) also encountered this issue when
selecting their unit of analysis for their observational study of vocabulary instruction in third-,
fourth-, and fifth-grade reading/language arts lessons. They discovered that several different
types of instruction often occurred within a turn and that “coding at the level of the turn appeared
to mask the full extent to which teachers provided specific types of instruction” (p. 37). Instead,
Silverman and colleauges moved to the level of utterance as the unit of anlaysis in order to
“reveal a more comprehensive and cumulative picture of the teachers’ insructional focus” (p.
37). Likewise, for the present study I moved to the level of utterance as my unit of analysis for a
finer-grain examination of vocabulary talk. Selecting utterance as the unit of analysis is
consistent with other observational studies that have examined teacher talk in relation to
children’s vocabulary learning (e.g., Barnes et al., 2017; Barnes & Dickinson, 2017a, 2017b,
2018; E. P. Bowers & Vasilyeva, 2011; Neugebauer et al., 2017; Silverman et al., 2014). I
defined an utterance as a unit of speech with only one speaker (i.e., bound by conversational
turns) and one main clause (subject-predicate) (MacWhinney, 2000; Neugebauer et al., 2017;
Rex & Schiller, 2009). As a conversational unit (c-unit), an utterance could have dependent (i.e.,
subordinate or coordinate) clauses in addition to the main clause but could not have more than
44
one main clause (MacWhinney, 2000). In addition to syntax, I also paid close attention to
intonation contours, so that speech tacked on after terminal intonation (i.e., rising for a question
or falling for a statement) was transcribed as a new utterance (Ratner & Brundage, 2018).
I first viewed all 24 videos (three videos each for eight participants) and marked
vocabulary talk episodes—or episodes of instruction that included at least one of the four types
of vocabulary talk: building knowledge of word meanings, scaffolding students’ word use,
increasing awareness of words and words learning, or interesting students in words and word
learning. After identifying vocabulary talk episodes, I began the process of transcribing these
episodes for analysis. Within each vocabulary talk episode, the teacher’s speech was transcribed
at the level of utterance, but because student talk was captured to provide context for teacher
utterances but would not be coded, student talk was transcribed less formally. Student speech
was captured at the level of the turn when possible. When multiple students spoke at the same
time, as much of what students were saying was transcribed as possible, but this talk was not
always attributed to different speakers. For example, when responding to a teacher’s question
about what colors clouds can be, multiple kindergarten students called out answers at the same
time. These responses were recorded together:
Child(ren): black, gray, gray, black, purple.
As this example shows, student talk was identified collectively as child(ren); that is, individual
children were not identified. Similarly, utterances in which teachers read text aloud were
transcribed to provide context but were not coded, which is consistent with other studies that
have examined teacher talk that includes reading text aloud to students (e.g., Barnes et al., 2017).
Finally, because teacher utterances were sometimes accompanied by an action or visual cue,
comments explaining relevant nonverbal context were included in the transcripts. For example,
45
the comment teacher begins by moving her finger and hand haphazardly but then transitions to
spinning it vigorously in a circle accompanied the utterance “instead of the wind moving every
which way, it spins in a circle.” It was also noted when teachers drew students’ attention to
specific words or word meanings by emphasizing them. (See Appendix D for full transcription
guide.)
An undergraduate research assistant who had been trained on the transcription
conventions assisted me with transcribing the vocabulary talk episodes of the 24 videos. After
the research assistant completed initial transcripts, I created final transcripts by reviewing the
videos next to the initial transcripts and making adjustments as needed, such as by correcting
mis-heard words, adding comments to describe relevant nonverbal cues, and noting emphasis.
Following transcription, I completed two cycles of analysis to code the teachers’
vocabulary talk during science instruction. In the first cycle, I read the transcripts and applied the
provisional codes (Miles et al., 2014; Saldaña, 2016) from my pilot study. As in the initial study,
I used two levels of codes. The first level again identified the four types of vocabulary talk
(defined in the same way): KNOWLEDGE & UNDERSTANDING, STUDENT WORD USE,
METALINGUISTIC & METACOGNITIVE AWARENESS, or AFFECTIVE FACTORS. The
second level again identified the specific vocabulary talk move the teacher made. For example,
the utterance “it means dangerous” was coded at the first level with KNOWLEDGE &
UNDERSTANDING as the general type of vocabulary talk and at the second level with
EXPLAIN/DEFINE as the specific vocabulary talk move. For utterances addressing multiple
types of vocabulary talk and/or multiple vocabulary talk moves, I applied each relevant code.
This is consistent with Gee's (2014) assertion that speakers often simultaneously carry out
multiple actions with any one utterance. (See Appendix E for full Vocabulary Talk Codebook.)
46
In the second cycle of coding, I examined teacher utterances that addressed at least one
type of vocabulary talk (i.e., first-level codes) but did not fit any of the specific vocabulary talk
moves (i.e., second-level codes) identified in the pilot study. In other words, this cycle of coding
focused on identifying additional vocabulary talk moves beyond those identified in the pilot
study. After applying the appropriate first-level provisional codes to identify type of vocabulary
talk, I once again used pattern coding (Miles et al., 2014; Saldaña, 2016) to describe the specific
vocabulary talk moves the teachers made. Focusing on one of the four types of vocabulary talk at
a time, I wrote descriptions of the teachers’ utterances, summarizing what the teachers said and
naming what they did through language (Gee, 2014). For example, I applied the first-level code
STUDENT WORD USE to the utterance “does anyone know the science word when you kind of
blend into your environment?” and then wrote the description teacher asks students if anyone
knows the word for what was being described. After writing descriptions such as this, I (re)read
teacher utterances, identified patterns across utterances, and revised descriptions to capture these
patterns until I was able to name the specific vocabulary talk move. Returning to the previous
example, I named the vocabulary talk move (i.e., second-level code) ELICIT NEW WORD
FROM STUDENTS and added the final description to the Vocabulary Talk Codebook: Teacher
asks students for a word that has not been introduced/discussed. After adding these new
vocabulary talk moves to the codebook, I reread all the teacher utterances and applied these new
second-level codes as appropriate.
During these two cycles of coding, I also established relationships between the second-
level codes (see Table 1). Because repeated exposures to words in multiple contexts supports
children with learning words (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006;
Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985; McKeown & Beck, 2003; National
47
Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Robbins & Ehri, 1994; Silverman,
2007a, 2007b; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik
& Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014), it was important to capture teachers’ use of words that
had been targeted for instruction (i.e., USE TARGET WORD). However, in order to determine
which words were target words, I had to first determine which vocabulary talk moves established
a word as a target word. I defined a target word as a word related to science learning that the
teacher seemingly targeted for instruction by bringing students’ awareness to the word and its
meaning, such as by explaining or defining a word, acting out or demonstrating the meaning of a
word, or eliciting students’ ideas about what a word meant (either a word being reviewed or a
new word being introduced). I considered these vocabulary talk moves that are focused on
bringing students’ awareness to a word and its meaning to be codes for Identifying Target Words.
Because I was examining teacher talk within the context of science instruction, a majority of the
words teachers explained/defined, for example, were related to science learning. However, there
were a few exceptions when the teacher used one of the Identifying Target Words vocabulary
talk moves for a word unrelated to science learning. In these rare cases, the utterance was still
coded with the vocabulary talk move but was not considered to become a target word. For
example, in one lesson, a kindergarten student talked about tornadoes being harmful enough to
lift the Hancock Building in Chicago, and another student asked, “What’s a Hancock?” The
teacher explained, “It’s a famous building, a really big, tall skyscraper.” This utterance was
coded with the first-level code KNOWLEDGE & UNDERSTANDING and the second-level
code EXPLAIN/DEFINE, as well as the first-level code AFFECTIVE FACTORS and the
second-level code ANSWER A QUESTION; however, the term Hancock Building did not
become a target word. In this case, the teacher was responding to a student’s question rather than
48
targeting the word for instruction. The word was not related to science learning within this
weather-focused lesson and was not used or discussed again at any other point in the lesson.
It is important to note that my use of the term target word differs from many of the
studies reviewed in Chapter 2 (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne
et al., 2009; Silverman, 2007a). Many of those were intervention studies designed to teach
students the meaning of specific words; to determine the intervention’s effectiveness, students
were tested on those words before and after the intervention. In my observational study, I did not
target words for instruction; rather, I was interested in capturing words the teachers seemingly
targeted for instruction by bringing students’ awareness to the word and its meaning—whether
this was with a planned set of science words or with words related to science learning that were
explained more spontaneously. This is consistent with Wright and Neuman's (2014)
observational study of vocabulary instruction across the school day in kindergarten classrooms,
in which target words were identified from vocabulary episodes—or interactions in which the
teacher (a) provided the meaning of a target word, including child-friendly definitions,
synonyms, antonyms, category membership, examples; or (b) or helped students determine the
meaning of a target word, such as by eliciting students’ explanations.
After determining which vocabulary talk moves identified a word as a target word, I then
determined which vocabulary talk moves were Affiliated with Target Words, meaning these
second-level codes were only applied to teacher utterances relating to established targeted words.
Examples of these vocabulary talk moves include using a target word, drawing attention to a
target word by emphasizing it and/or its meaning, and helping students make personal
connections to a target word. Finally, I noted which vocabulary talk moves were unaffiliated
with target words, meaning they could be applied to teacher utterances that related target words
49
but they could also be applied to teacher utterances unrelated to target words. Examples of these
vocabulary talk moves include suggesting another way to say something, highlighting a word
from another domain, and answering students’ questions about the meaning of a word.
Table 1
Relationships of Vocabulary Talk Moves to Target Words
Category
Vocabulary Talk Move
Identifying Target Words
Affiliated with Target Words
Unaffiliated with Target Words
Act Out/Demonstrate
Ask if Heard of Word
Ask to Repeat
Differentiate Meaning
Elicit New Word from Students
Elicit Student Ideas: Act Out
Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample
Elicit Student Ideas: New Word
Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word
Elicit Student Ideas: Synonym
Explain/Define
Introduce Word
Review Word
Use with Hint
Prompt Word
Provide Example/Nonexample
Provide Visual Support
Emphasize
Elicit Student Questions: Target Word
Help Students Make Personal Connection
Use Target Word
Visually Display Word
Acknowledge Student Ownership
Answer a Question
Appreciate Word/Word Choice
Comment on Spelling
Encourage to Use Later
Highlight Favorite Words
Highlight Words Across Domains
Offer Alternative
Praise
Restate Differently
Revoice
Suggest Word/Phrase
50
Once all of the specific vocabulary talk moves had been identified and the vocabulary
talk episodes had been coded, I selected excerpts of transcripts that provided illustrative
examples for specific vocabulary talk moves to share within Chapter 4. When highlighting
vocabulary talk moves that often co-occurred, I selected excerpts of transcripts that showed this
co-occurrence. When highlighting an individual vocabulary talk move, I looked for places in
lesson transcripts where there was density for that particular code so that several different
examples of the move could be shown within the span of several utterances. Finally, when more
than one excerpt offered multiple examples of the vocabulary talk move(s) being highlighted, I
chose an excerpt that had not yet been selected in order to provide a wide range of examples
from across the cohort.
I then took a more quantitative look, using descriptive statistics, to understand the
frequency with which each broad type of vocabulary talk and each specific vocabulary talk move
were used. I summed the number of applications of each of the first-level codes (i.e., type of
vocabulary talk) and each of the second-level codes (i.e., specific vocabulary talk move). I then
calculated the percentage of the whole for each first-level code and each second-level code.
Finally, I made within- and between-teacher comparisons of vocabulary talk over the
course of the study. For within-teacher comparisons, I calculated the percentage of vocabulary
talk code applications that addressed each first-level code (i.e., type of vocabulary talk) within
each lesson for each teacher. This allowed me to compare individual teachers’ use of each type
of vocabulary talk across the three timepoints. I then created charts to show each teachers’ use of
the four types of vocabulary talk within each of the three video recorded lessons. For between-
teacher comparisons, I calculated the rate each type of vocabulary talk occurred within each
lesson (i.e., total count for each first-level code divided by lesson length in minutes). This was
51
important because lesson lengths varied within and between teachers; therefore, to be able to
compare the frequency of each type of vocabulary talk across lessons, rate per minute was used.
Keep in mind that utterances often included more than one vocabulary talk move (i.e., second-
level codes). For example, the following utterance includes six different vocabulary talk moves:
Ms. Thompson: and what do we do at school to prepare, or get ready, in case there's a
tornado?
teacher holds up the whiteboard with “prepare” written on it; teacher emphasizes
“prepare”
This utterance was coded with five different KNOWLEDGE & UNDERSTANDING vocabulary
talk moves (i.e., USE TARGET WORD, EMPHASIZE, USE WITH HINT, VISUALLY
DISPLAY WORD, and ELICIT STUDENT IDEAS: EXAMPLE/NONEXAMPLE) and one
AFFECTIVE FACTORS move (i.e., HELP STUDENTS MAKE PERSONAL CONNECTION).
Therefore, this utterance contributed five code applications to the total KNOWLEDGE &
UNDERSTANDING count and one to the total AFFECTIVE FACTORS count. After
calculating the total rate per minute for each of the four types of vocabulary talk for each lesson,
I graphed each teacher’s vocabulary talk over the course of the study.
Research Question 2: Is Vocabulary Talk Related to the Language Aspects of Science Talk,
and if so, How?
To examine the relationship between vocabulary talk and the language aspects of science
talk, I compared the overall trend of each teacher’s vocabulary talk with the overall trend of each
teacher’s science talk related to language learning. For vocabulary talk trends, I used the graphs I
created for the between-teacher comparisons of vocabulary talk over the course of the study
described for Research Question 1 in the previous section. Using rate per minute, these graphs
show how frequently each teacher used each type of vocabulary talk for each of the three video
recorded lessons.
52
For the language aspects of science talk, the SOLID Start Tool (Wright et al., 2017) was
used to code the science talk within each of the 24 video recorded science lessons. The purpose
of this formative observation tool is to support K-2 educators in promoting sensemaking talk in
their science instruction. Specifically, this science talk is academically productive (Michaels &
O’Connor, 2012, 2015), rigorous, and responsive—providing opportunities for students to “think
and talk like scientists” as they work to figure out a science phenomenon. The SOLID Start Tool
has three domains (see Figure 1) with three components each. The focus of my analysis was on
the final domain: talk that develops language and literacy for science. The three components of
this domain are (1) providing contextualized, child-friendly supports for science oral language,
(2) scaffolding student use of science oral language, and (3) explicitly supporting disciplinary
literacy. I focused specifically on the first two components with this analysis, as they speak to the
language aspects of science talk and best align with promoting students’ oral language and
vocabulary development.
Figure 1
Domains of Science Talk
Supporting equitable science
participation and
engagement
53
Note: Domains of science talk in the SOLID Start Tool
Each component of the SOLID Start Tool is scored on a five-point scale, from Low (1) to
High (5). The scores of 1, 3, or 5 are assigned when the evidence for the component matches the
description for Low, Medium, or High, respectively. (See Table 2 for descriptions.) The score of
2 is assigned when (a) the evidence matches some of both the Low and Medium descriptions or
(b) most of the evidence matches the Low description but there is also (minimal) evidence
matching the High description. Likewise, the score of 4 is assigned when (a) the evidence
matches some of both the Medium and High descriptions or (b) most of the evidence matches the
High description but there is also (minimal) evidence matching the Low description. Each
activity within a science lesson is scored separately on each component of the SOLID Start Tool.
Typical lesson activities include discussions of content, planning discussions for science
investigations, discussions during investigations, text-based discussions, video-based
discussions, and review/summary/share out discussions.
Table 2
SOLID Start Tool: Domain 3, Components 1-2
5 (High)
Component 1 Contextualized, Child-
Friendly Supports for
Science Oral Language:
Teacher introduces or reviews
science oral language at times
when this is appropriate to
support students’ engagement
and participation in science
learning. New words or
phrases are introduced in
child-friendly ways (e.g., by
showing images or by
explaining the word using
language that is already
familiar to children) as
children engage in science
practices.
3 (Medium)
4
Decontextualized Supports
for Science Oral Language:
Teacher introduces or reviews
science language, but it is out
of context from children’s
science learning (e.g., teacher
previews all of the vocabulary
for the unit or teacher puts all
of the unit vocabulary on a
word wall before the unit
begins) or teacher introduces
science language in ways that
are too complex for young
children.
1 (Low)
2
Science Oral
Language
Not Taught:
Teacher may
use scientific
oral language
but does not
introduce or
teach this
language to
students.
54
Table 2 (cont’d)
Component 2 Students Use Science Oral
Language: Teacher scaffolds
students’ use of discipline-
specific oral language that has
been previously introduced
with a focus on discussing
students’ ideas and engaging
in science practices.
Students Demonstrate
Science Oral
Knowledge of Science Oral
Language: Teacher scaffolds
student talk about the
meaning of science words but
students do not have
opportunities to use these
words to discuss ideas or
engage in science practices.
Language
Not Used:
Teacher does
not expect
students to use
science oral
language.
The SOLID Start Tool was used to score each of the lesson activities from each of the
three video recorded lessons for each participant. All 24 lessons were double coded to ensure
reliability. Coders met to compare scores and resolved differences in codes. Because the amount
of time spent on each activity varied, scores were weighted. To weigh scores, the proportion of
the total lesson time was calculated for each activity within a lesson. For example, with a 20-
minute lesson, the proportion for a 5-minute activity (Activity 1) would be 0.25 and for a 15-
minute activity (Activity 2) would be 0.75. The proportion for each activity was then multiplied
by the score for each component within the activity. (See Table 3 for an example. Note that
Activity 1 scores were multiplied by 0.25 and Activity 2 scores were multiplied by 0.75.)
Because the number of activities within a lesson also varied, a composite score was
calculated for each component so that lessons as a whole could be compared within and across
teachers. To reach a composite score, the weighted scores for each activity were summed. In the
Table 3 example, Activity 1 scored a 5 (High) for Component 2; however, because this was only
25% of the lesson, the weighed score is much lower. The overall composite score of 2 for
Component 2 reflects the fact that 75% of the lesson scored a 1 (Low). All scores—original
activity scores, activity weighted scores, and composite scores—ranged from 1-5.
55
Table 3
Sample Weighted and Composite Scores for a 20-Minute Lesson with Two Activities
Activity 1
Score
Activity 1
Weighted
Activity 2
Score
Activity 2
Weighted
Composite
Score
Component 1
Component 2
4
5
Score
1
1.25
5
1
Score
3.75
0.75
4.75
2
Finally, after calculating composite scores for each teacher’s lessons, I graphed each
teacher’s science talk related to language learning (i.e., Components 1-2) over the course of the
study. I then compared each teacher’s vocabulary talk graph with her science talk graph. Because
vocabulary talk and science talk were measured on different scales, I looked at overall trends
between the two graphs.
Study 2: Curricular Features and Vocabulary Talk in a Kindergarten Classroom
This study investigates features of science curriculum materials related to the enhanced
vocabulary talk of one kindergarten teacher. In the sections that follow, I describe the design and
logic, participant, data sources, and analytic strategies used to address this research question.
Design and Logic
Once again, I used an instrumental case study design (Barone, 2011; Stake, 2000) in
order to look for insight into an issue of primary interest. In this study, my case was the science
instruction of one kindergarten teacher, Ms. Thompson, from February-May 2019. Ms.
Thompson participated in the larger SOLID Start professional development usability study and
was part of the cohort of eight teachers who comprised the case I examined in Study 1. I selected
Ms. Thompson’s science instruction for further analysis regarding curriculum materials for two
reasons. First, I wanted to examine within-subject differences in vocabulary talk (i.e., variance in
talk by a single teacher), and Ms. Thompson’s vocabulary talk varied noticeably across the three
56
lessons (see Figure 12 in Chapter 4). Examining differences in one teacher’s talk across lessons
rather than examining variance in talk from one teacher to another (i.e., between-subjects
differences) allowed me to investigate factors impacting vocabulary talk that were outside of just
individual differences in speaking patterns or ways of using language. Second, I wanted to
compare a teacher’s talk from lessons drawing from different sets of curriculum materials, and
Ms. Thompson taught from three different sets of materials across the three recorded lessons.
Only one other teacher, Ms. Henderson, taught from three different sets of curricular materials.
As a young 5s teacher, however, Ms. Henderson did not have grade-level curricular materials.
Instead, she adapted lessons from a variety of sources (e.g., STEM into Nature professional
development by the Academy of Natural Resources, Dancing with Dinosaurs program from the
Natural History Museum at the University of Michigan, and Project WILD), along with
designing her own lessons. Because I did not have access to all of these sources, I did not select
Ms. Henderson’s science instruction for this analysis. Five of the eight teachers taught only from
the Mystery Science/Mystery Doug materials for their three recorded lessons, and Ms. Nelson
taught one lesson based on a SOLID Start unit and then taught two lessons from Mystery
Science/Mystery Doug materials. Because Ms. Thompson taught from SOLID Start and Mystery
Science/Mystery Doug materials— as well as a lesson from Teachers Pay Teachers, I selected
Ms. Thompson’s science instruction for further analysis. Therefore, in this study, I examined Ms.
Thompson’s science instruction (i.e., the case) in depth in order to better understand the issue of
interest: features of the science curriculum materials related to enhanced vocabulary talk during
science instruction.
57
Participant
Ms. Thompson has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education with science and English
minors and has completed the coursework for a master’s degree in teaching natural science—
with the exception of the final project. Ms. Thompson’s state professional teaching certification
is for elementary education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-contained classroom), science
(6-8), and English (6-8). Ms. Thompson had 21 kindergarten students in her class, with 95.24%
of students identifying as White or European American and 4.76% identifying as Black or
African American. At the time of the study, Ms. Thompson had taught kindergarten students for
8 of her 12 years of teaching experience. She taught an average of 49.91 minutes of science
instruction per week during the course of this study, with weekly totals ranging from 9-90
minutes. She reported 36 days with science instruction during the study, with an average of 3.27
days with science instruction per week. Ms. Thompson reported using SOLID Start and Mystery
Science curriculum materials, along with supplemental materials from Teachers Pay Teachers
and apps such as Nico and Nor and Seesaw.
Data Sources
In addition to the demographic background and teaching experience information survey,
the weekly instructional log, and the video recordings of science lesson described in Study 1, I
also collected science curriculum materials for the three lessons Ms. Thompson taught. The first
lesson was How Can We Prepare for Severe Weather? (Lesson 7) from the SOLID Start Weather
Forecasting unit (Gotwals & Wright, 2017; Wright & Gotwals, 2017). While providing
curriculum materials was not part of the SOLID Start professional development usability study,
Ms. Thompson had participated in a pilot study of the SOLID Start kindergarten curriculum the
previous school year and had access to these materials. Participants in the usability study could
58
use any curriculum available to them, and Ms. Thompson chose to use this lesson from the
SOLID Start curriculum for her PrePD lesson. The second lesson (i.e., Week 5) was from a
Teachers Pay Teachers packet on force and motion. The third lesson (i.e., Week 9) was Where
Do Animals Live? (Lesson 2) from the Mystery Science Plant and Animal Secrets unit (Schacht
& Peltz, 2020). (See Chapter 5 for a description of each lesson as presented by the curriculum
materials.) These three lessons from three different sets of curriculum materials helped me to
examine curricular features related to enhanced vocabulary talk.
Analytic Strategies
I began this qualitative analysis by carefully reading through the coded transcripts of Ms.
Thompson’s enacted lessons and identifying places with dense vocabulary talk, meaning that
multiple vocabulary moves were made over a span of utterances. This was important because, in
order to examine the relationship between Ms. Thompson’s enhanced vocabulary talk and
science curriculum materials, I first needed to identify enhanced vocabulary talk. I considered
density of vocabulary talk to be evidence of enhanced vocabulary talk, as Ms. Thompson
engaged in sustained vocabulary talk (i.e., over the span of multiple utterances) using multiple
vocabulary talk moves in these places. I also highlighted words that had been identified as target
words in my Study 1 analysis (i.e., words related to science learning that Ms. Thompson
seemingly targeted for instruction by bringing students’ awareness to the words and their
meaning) and noted the teachers’ questions/prompts related to these target words. After this
initial analysis of Ms. Thompson’s enacted lessons, I carefully read/viewed the written lessons as
presented in the SOLID Start, Teachers Pay Teachers, and Mystery Science curriculum
materials. For each of the areas of dense vocabulary talk I had identified in the enacted lessons, I
examined the written lessons to see if there was anything in the curriculum materials seemingly
59
related to this talk. I noted places in which specific vocabulary words were targeted for
instruction (if any) and noted what supports were provided for introducing and/or reviewing
these words (if any). I compared the list of vocabulary words targeted for instruction by the
curriculum materials (if any) to the list of words targeted for instruction in Ms. Thompson’s
enacted lessons. Throughout this analysis, as I found curricular features (e.g., targeted
vocabulary words) that seemed to be related to enhanced vocabulary talk in one lesson, I looked
for this relationship across the other two written/enacted lessons as well.
Finally, I selected excerpts of transcripts to share within Chapter 5 to highlight curricular
features related to enhanced vocabulary talk. I looked across all three lessons for illustrative
examples of enhanced vocabulary talk related to each curricular feature I identified. For
example, I selected an excerpt each from Ms. Thompson’s SOLID Start and Mystery Science
lessons to show the relationship between text selection and vocabulary talk. In this case, the
Teachers Pay Teachers lesson did not include text, so I did not select an example of Ms.
Thompson’s vocabulary talk from this lesson for this feature.
Researcher Positionality
My analysis in this dissertation study has been influenced by my experiences as a
classroom teacher and research assistant. As a former early-elementary classroom teacher, I drew
from my own teaching experience—in addition to the literature on how children learn words—
when identifying vocabulary talk moves and analyzing features of science curriculum materials.
For example, when identifying words that teachers targeted for instruction on their own, I
considered the ways in which I drew students’ attention to new words and their meanings in my
own teaching, such as by asking students to say to repeat the target word while introducing it and
its meaning—which is also supported by the literature (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2007; Coyne et
60
al., 2010; Rosenthal & Ehri, 2011; Silverman & Crandell, 2010). As a research assistant for the
SOLID Start project for four years, I have collaborated with the team on curriculum
development, SOLID Start Tool development, and the development and coordination of the
professional development usability trial. Together, these experiences as a research assistant on
the SOLID Start project shaped how I thought about the language aspects of science talk when
considering how this relates to vocabulary talk and how I thought about curricular features when
analyzing features related to enhanced vocabulary talk.
Limitations to the Method
There are several limitations to the methodology of this dissertation study. First, this
study took place within the context of a larger study providing teachers with professional
development on engaging students in science talk, so while vocabulary talk was not a focus of
the professional development experience, this observational study did not take place within the
context of business-as-usual science instruction. Given that very little time is devoted to science
instruction at the elementary level (Berliner, 2011; Blank, 2012), it was important to find early-
elementary teachers who would be teaching science lessons on a regular basis. The teachers
participating in the SOLID Start professional development committed to teaching and recording
their science lessons at three points, which provided me with the context in which to examine
vocabulary talk during science instruction. Further, while the language aspects of science talk
were touched on during the part of the professional development focused on talk that supports
language and literacy for science, vocabulary talk was not a focus of the overall professional
development.
The second limitation is that only eight teachers were included in this observational
study. Many observational studies of vocabulary instruction include a larger number of teachers
61
(e.g., Carlisle et al., 2013 [n=44]; Nelson et al., 2015 [n=107-119 over three years]; Neugebauer
et al., 2017 [n=27]; Scott et al., 2003 [n=23]; Silverman & Crandell, 2010 [n=16]; Wanzek, 2014
[n=14]; Wright & Neuman, 2014 [n=55]). However, Watts (1995) examined the instruction of
six teachers and Dickinson et al. (2008) examined the instruction of four teachers. Therefore,
there is a precedent for observational studies of vocabulary instruction examining a relatively
small number of teachers. Further, like the taxonomy of methods for teaching vocabulary
instruction developed by Watts (1995), the present study was focused on identifying specific
vocabulary talk moves made by teachers, which was possible to do across the 24 lessons
observed from the cohort of eight teachers studied.
A third limitation is that the SOLID Start Tool was under development at the time of the
analysis. While the tool has been through several iterations of revisions since these lessons were
scored, Components 1-2 of Domain 3 (i.e., the language aspects of science talk) have not
changed, and therefore these scores have not changed. Each activity within a lesson was double
coded and scorers reconciled any disagreements after discussion.
Finally, perhaps the greatest limitation of the present study is that no student outcome
data was collected. While a number of observational studies of vocabulary instruction have not
included student outcome data (Blachowicz, 1987; Dickinson et al., 2008; Nelson et al., 2015;
Scott et al., 2003; Wanzek, 2014; Watts, 1995; Wright & Neuman, 2014), the absence of this
information makes it impossible to confirm that the vocabulary talk moves identified in the
present study promote students’ oral language and vocabulary development. Therefore, unlike
observational studies that found correlations between vocabulary instructional methods and
students’ learning (e.g., Carlisle et al., 2013; Neugebauer et al., 2017; Silverman & Crandell,
2010), the relationship between the vocabulary talk moves identified in the present study and
62
students’ oral language and vocabulary development have not yet been examined. The
vocabulary talk moves identified here are aligned with research suggesting they would promote
oral language and vocabulary development, but future research is needed to determine if this is
the case.
Summary
Two instrumental case studies comprise this dissertation study. In the first study, the case
is the science instruction of a cohort of eight early-elementary teachers and the issue of primary
interest is how they used language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development. In the second study, the case is one kindergarten teacher and the issues of primary
interest are the features of science curriculum materials related to enhanced vocabulary talk
during science instruction. In total, 24 video recorded science lessons provided 894.27 minutes
of observational data across three timepoints from the eight participating teachers. Discourse
analysis and other qualitative analysis techniques were used to examine the vocabulary talk
moves the teachers made during science instruction. Quantitative techniques were also used to
make within- and between-teacher comparisons of vocabulary talk over the course of the study.
Little is known about how teachers promote oral language and vocabulary development
in science instruction in the early-elementary grades; therefore, the goal of this dissertation study
is to examine the ways in which teachers use language to promote oral language and vocabulary
development during science instruction. It is important to understand what opportunities students
have to develop oral language and also access science content, as both support literacy and
science learning.
63
CHAPTER 4—STUDY 1 FINDINGS: RESEARCH QUESTIONS 1-2
The primary goal of this study was to describe how teachers promote oral language and
vocabulary development during science instruction in the early-elementary grades. I report
findings based on 894.27 minutes of observational data across 24 total lessons, ranging from
13:27 to 1:13:25, in eight early-elementary classrooms. I investigated the following research
questions:
1. How do teachers use language to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development during science instruction in early-elementary classrooms?
2. Is vocabulary talk related to the language aspects of science talk, and if so, how?
In this section, I present findings investigating the vocabulary talk and science talk that occurred
during early-elementary science lessons. I begin by introducing the cohort of eight teachers who
together comprise the case I examined. Then I present the specific vocabulary talk moves the
teachers made within each of the four types of vocabulary talk: Knowledge & Understanding,
Affective Factors, Student Word Use, and Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness. Next I
present within- and between-teacher comparisons of vocabulary talk over the course of the study.
Finally, I describe the relationship between vocabulary talk and the language aspects of science
talk.
When lesson transcripts are shared throughout this chapter and the next, the vocabulary
talk moves are printed in bold text in the brackets following teacher utterances, and comments
providing additional context are printed in italics below the utterance to which they refer. Also
note that (parts of) some transcripts are shared multiple times within Chapter 4 and/or 5,
highlighting different aspects of vocabulary talk. For ease of reading, these transcripts are
provided within the text again so that navigating back to previous findings is not required.
64
The Teachers
With an instrumental case study (Barone, 2011; Stake, 2000), it important to study the
case in depth in order to understand the issue of primary interest. In this section, I present a
description of each of the eight teachers who together comprise the case I examined in order to
better understand vocabulary talk during science instruction in the early-elementary grades.
These descriptions include self-reported information about education, teacher certification,
teaching experience, student demographics, amount and frequency of science instruction, and
science curriculum materials used.
Ms. Henderson
Ms. Henderson has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education with early childhood,
English, and history minors and a master’s degree in K-12 literacy. Her state professional
teaching certification is for elementary education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-
contained classroom), English (6-8), history (6-8), reading specialist (K-12), and early childhood
education (PK-K). At the time of the study, Ms. Henderson had 24 young 5s students in her class
(i.e., children who will be in kindergarten the following school year), with 95.83% of students
identifying as White or European American and 4.17% identifying as biracial or multiethnic. Ms.
Henderson had 17 years of teaching experience, with 11 of those years as a young 5s teacher.
During the course of the study, she taught an average of 102.18 minutes of science instruction
per week, with weekly totals ranging from 30-150 minutes. She reported 31 days with science
instruction during the study, with an average of 2.82 days with science instruction per week. As a
young 5s teacher without grade-level curriculum, Ms. Henderson drew from a variety of science
curriculum materials. She reported using Growing Up Wild, Project Wild, Project Learning Tree,
STEM into Nature (Academy of Natural Resources professional development for educators), and
65
her own science curriculum materials. She noted that one of her recorded lessons was mostly
original but drew on an activity from the Dancing with Dinosaurs program at the University of
Michigan’s Natural History Museum.
Ms. Thompson
Ms. Thompson has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education with science and English
minors. She has also completed the coursework for a master’s degree in teaching natural science,
with the exception of the final project. Ms. Thompson’s state professional teaching certification
is for elementary education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-contained classroom), science
(6-8), and English (6-8). At the time of the study, Ms. Thompson had 21 kindergarten students in
her class, with 95.24% of students identifying as White or European American and 4.76%
identifying as Black or African American. Ms. Thompson had 12 years of teaching experience,
with 8 of those years as a kindergarten teacher. During the course of the study, she taught an
average of 49.91 minutes of science instruction per week, with weekly totals ranging from 9-90
minutes. She reported 36 days with science instruction during the study, with an average of 3.27
days with science instruction per week. Ms. Thompson reported using curriculum materials form
SOLID Start, Mystery Science, and Teachers Pay Teachers, as well as apps such as Seesaw and
Nico and Nor.
Ms. Nelson
Ms. Nelson has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education and child development and a
master’s degree in early literacy. Her state professional teaching certification is for elementary
education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-contained classroom) and early childhood
education (PK-K). At the time of the study, Ms. Nelson had 22 kindergarten students in her class,
with 90.91% of students identifying as White or European American, 4.55% identifying as
66
Hispanic or Latino/a, and 4.55% identifying as biracial or multiethnic. Ms. Nelson had 19 years
of teaching experience, with 13 of those years as a kindergarten teacher. During the course of the
study, Ms. Nelson taught an average of 70 minutes of science instruction per week, with weekly
totals ranging from 20-130 minutes. She reported 52 days with science instruction during the
study, with an average of 4 days with science instruction per week. Ms. Nelson reported using
SOLID Start and Mystery Science/Mystery Doug curriculum materials.
Ms. Baker
Ms. Baker has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education. Her state standard teaching
certification is for elementary education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-contained
classroom), science (6-7), and social studies (6-8). At the time of the study, Ms. Baker had 22
kindergarten students in her class, with 95.45% of students identifying as White or European
American and 4.55% identifying as biracial or multiethnic. Ms. Baker had 5 years of teaching
experience, with 2 of those years as a kindergarten teacher. During the course of the study, Ms.
Baker taught an average of 81.54 minutes of science instruction per week, with weekly totals
ranging from 40-140 minutes. She reported 36 days with science instruction during the study,
with an average of 2.77 days with science instruction per week. Ms. Baker reported using
Mystery Science/Mystery Doug and Wonderopolis curriculum materials.
Ms. Watson
Ms. Watson has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education. Her state standard teaching
certification is for elementary education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-contained
classroom). At the time of the study, Ms. Watson had 27 first-grade students in her class, with
96.3% of students identifying as White or European American and 3.7% identifying as Black or
African American. Ms. Watson was in her first year of teaching. During the course of the study,
67
Ms. Watson taught an average of 52.5 minutes of science instruction per week, with weekly
totals ranging from 30-60 minutes. She reported 21 days with science instruction during the
study, with an average of 1.75 days with science instruction per week. Ms. Watson reported
using Mystery Science curriculum materials.
Ms. Griffin
Ms. Griffin has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education with language arts and math
minors. Her state standard teaching certification is for elementary education (K-5 all subjects, K-
8 all subjects self-contained classroom). At the time of the study, Ms. Griffin had 27 first-grade
students in her class, with 92.59% of students identifying as White or European American, 3.7%
identifying as Black or African American, and 3.7% identifying as Asian or Pacific Islander. Ms.
Griffin had 2 years of teaching experience and was in her first year as a first-grade teacher.
During the course of the study, Ms. Griffin taught an average of 32.27 minutes of science
instruction per week, with weekly totals ranging from 0-90 minutes. She reported 12 days with
science instruction during the study, with an average of 1.09 days with science instruction per
week. Ms. Griffin reported using Mystery Science and her own curriculum materials.
Ms. Howard
Ms. Howard has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education and a master’s degree in
curriculum and instruction. Her state professional teaching certification is for elementary
education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-contained classroom) and English (6-8). At the
time of the study, Ms. Howard had 22 second-grade students in her class, with 100% of students
identifying as White or European American. Ms. Howard had 6 years of teaching experience and
was in her first year as a second-grade teacher. During the course of the study, Ms. Howard
taught an average of 69.09 minutes of science instruction per week, with weekly totals ranging
68
from 0-185 minutes. She reported 23 days with science instruction during the study, with an
average of 2.09 days with science instruction per week. Ms. Howard reported using Mystery
Science curriculum materials, along with informational texts from the Reading Street literacy
curriculum.
Ms. Hill
Ms. Hill has a bachelor’s degree in elementary education. Her state standard teaching
certification is for elementary education (K-5 all subjects, K-8 all subjects self-contained
classroom) and early childhood (PK-general and special education). At the time of the study, Ms.
Hill had 23 second-grade students in her class, with 100% of students identifying as White or
European American. Ms. Hill had 7 years of teaching experience and was in her first year as a
second-grade teacher. During the course of the study, Ms. Hill taught an average of 40.42
minutes of science instruction per week, with weekly totals ranging from 0-90 minutes. She
reported 12 days with science instruction during the study, with an average of one day with
science instruction per week. Ms. Hill reported using Mystery Science curriculum materials.
In summary, this cohort of eight teachers participating in the larger SOLID Start
professional development usability trial comprised the case I examined in depth in order to better
understand how teachers use language to promote oral language and vocabulary development
during science instruction in the early-elementary grades. In the sections that follow, I share my
findings for each research question.
69
Research Question 1: How Do Teachers Use Language to Promote Students’ Oral
Language and Vocabulary Development During Science Instruction in Early-Elementary
Classrooms?
To investigate the ways in which teachers use language to promote students’ oral
language and vocabulary development during science instruction in early-elementary classrooms,
I identified the vocabulary talk moves the teachers made that align with the research and theory
relative to how children learn words. I found that, collectively, the cohort of teachers used 34
different vocabulary talk moves, as presented in Table 4. This table shows the number of
vocabulary talk code applications across the entire data set; that is, Table 4 shows the total
number of times each second-level code (i.e., vocabulary talk move) was applied across all eight
teachers and all 24 total video recordings. Note that Table 4 is organized with four sections—one
for each of the four types of vocabulary talk (i.e., Knowledge & Understanding, Affective
Factors, Student Word Use, and Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness). These four types
of vocabulary talk appear in descending frequency of occurrence within the data set. Within each
section, specific vocabulary talk moves appear in descending frequency of occurrence within that
type of vocabulary talk. To be included in the codebook and analysis, a vocabulary talk move
had to be used at least one time across the data set; therefore, provisional codes from the pilot
study were removed if they were not applied in the present study. In the following paragraphs, I
present examples of specific vocabulary talk moves the teachers used to build knowledge of
word meanings, interest students in words and word learning, scaffold students’ word use, and
increase awareness of words and word learning. For descriptions and examples of all 34
vocabulary talk moves, see the full Vocabulary Talk Codebook in Appendix E.
70
Table 4
Frequency of Vocabulary Talk Moves
Vocabulary Talk Move
Number of Code
Percentage of
Applications
(N = 3516)
Code
Applications
Knowledge & Understanding
3049
86.72%
Use Target Word
Emphasize
Explain/Define
Provide Example/Nonexample
Review Word
Use with Hint
Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word
Visually Display Word
Introduce Word
Act Out/Demonstrate
Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample
Provide Visual Support
Elicit Student Ideas: New Word
Comment on Spelling
Elicit Student Ideas: Act Out
Elicit Student Questions: Target Word
Elicit Student Ideas: Synonym
Restate Differently
Differentiate Meaning
Highlight Words Across Domains
Offer Alternative
1534
354
207
167
124
111
108
95
89
59
56
47
36
18
16
14
8
3
1
1
1
Affective Factors
276
7.85%
Help Students Make Personal Connection
Praise
Answer a Question
Acknowledge Student Ownership
Appreciate Word/Word Choice
Highlight Favorite Word
Student Word Use
184
Prompt Word
Suggest Word/Phrase
Revoice
Ask to Repeat
Elicit New Word from Students
Encourage to Use Later
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
7
Ask if Heard of Word
218
29
15
9
4
1
113
24
21
10
9
7
7
5.23%
0.20%
71
43.63%
10.07%
5.89%
4.75%
3.53%
3.16%
3.07%
2.70%
2.53%
1.68%
1.59%
1.34%
1.02%
0.51%
0.46%
0.40%
0.23%
0.09%
0.03%
0.03%
0.03%
6.20%
0.82%
0.43%
0.26%
0.11%
0.03%
3.21%
0.68%
0.60%
0.28%
0.26%
0.20%
0.20%
Knowledge & Understanding
As Table 4 shows, the cohort of teachers used Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary
talk moves most frequently, accounting for 86.72% of total vocabulary talk code applications.
Within this type of vocabulary talk, the teachers used 21 different vocabulary talk moves aligned
with the research on building students’ knowledge of word meanings. The most frequently-
occurring Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk move was using a target word—or a
word that had been targeted for instruction by bringing students’ awareness to it and its meaning.
Accounting for 43.63% of total vocabulary talk code applications, using a target word was also
the most frequently-occurring vocabulary talk move overall—across all four types of vocabulary
talk. The second most frequently-occurring Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk move
was emphasizing a target word and/or its meaning. Accounting for 10.07% of total vocabulary
talk code applications, emphasizing a target word and/or its meaning was also the second most
frequently-occurring vocabulary talk move overall. As using a target word and emphasizing a
target word are related vocabulary talk moves, they often were used by teachers together in the
same utterance. The following example was selected to show how teachers often emphasized
target words as they used them. In this example, Ms. Nelson uses and emphasizes the target word
extreme multiple times while reviewing it with her kindergarten students:
Ms. Nelson: it was ex-tr-eme. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes and draws out “extreme,” pausing after each syllable as if
each were a clue to the word
Child(ren): extreme!
Ms. Nelson: extreme. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “extreme”
Ms. Nelson: extreme weather. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “extreme”
Ms. Nelson: extreme weather. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Visually Display Word]
teacher writes “extreme weather” on the SMARTBoard and emphasizes
“extreme”
Ms. Nelson: we read that book about extreme weather. [Use Target Word]
72
Ms. Nelson: so, so we said so there is different kinds of extreme weather. [Use Target
Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “extreme”
Ms. Nelson: what kinds of extreme weather did we talk about? [Use Target Word, Elicit
Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample]
In this brief exchange, Ms. Nelson provided repeated exposures to the word extreme, saying it
seven times. She drew attention to it too by saying it with emphasis five of those times. While
the word extreme was being reviewed at this point in the lesson, Ms. Nelson continued to use the
term, as relevant, throughout the lesson. Teachers often used target words repeatedly in a series
of utterances while introducing or reviewing them, followed by additional exposures throughout
the lesson, as target words related to the discussion.
As shown in Table 4, after using target words and emphasizing target words and/or their
meaning, the next most frequently occurring Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk
moves were explaining or defining a word (5.89% of total vocabulary talk code applications) and
providing an example or nonexample of a word (4.75% of total vocabulary talk code
applications). Teacher utterances were coded for each of these types of vocabulary talk when the
teacher provided the explanation/definition or example/nonexample or when the teacher
confirmed the accuracy of an explanation/definition or example/nonexample, such as when
provided by a student. Code applications for explaining/defining and providing an
example/nonexample often appeared together, as teachers frequently followed up an explanation
of a word’s meaning with examples. The following example was selected to show this co-
occurrence. In this example, Ms. Henderson explains the meaning of needs and wants and
provides examples to the young 5s students in her class within a discussion about habitats
providing food, water, shelter, and space for inhabitants:
Ms. Henderson: those are our needs. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “needs”
73
Ms. Henderson: so the needs are things that we have to have. [Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Explain/Define, Visually Display Word,]
teacher emphasizes “have to” and places a paper with “needs” written on it in
one of the hula hoops
Ms. Henderson: okay?
Ms. Henderson: in order to survive. [Explain/Define]
Ms. Henderson: so for example, in our house, do we need to have a way to get water?
[Use Target Word, Emphasize, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “need”
Child(ren): yes.
Ms. Henderson: yes. [Provide Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
Ms. Henderson: do we need to have a bed? [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Help
Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “need”
Child(ren): yes.
Child(ren): to hibernate.
Ms. Henderson: well, to sleep. [Help Students Make Personal Connection, Revoice]
Ms. Henderson: but if we have a house, which is our shelter, do we have to have a bed?
teacher emphasizes “have to have” [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Provide
Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): yes.
Ms. Henderson: no. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Henderson: so a, a bed is a want. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word, Provide
Example/Nonexample, Visually Display Word]
teacher holds up paper with the word “want” on it as she says the word “want”
Ms. Henderson: a want is something we don't need to survive. [Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Explain/Define]
teacher emphasizes “don’t need”
Ms. Henderson: we don't have to have a bed to survive. [Emphasize, Provide
Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “have to”
In this example, Ms. Henderson paused the discussion about habitats in order to explain the
meaning of needs. She then engaged students in deeper processing by asking them to determine
if water and beds are examples of needs, and she confirmed the accuracy of their responses.
(Note that this is slightly different from the Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk move
of eliciting examples/nonexamples from students because, in this instance, students did not
generate the examples/nonexamples on their own.) The discussion regarding whether a bed is a
need led to the introduction of the word want, which Ms. Henderson explicitly explained and
74
clarified that a bed is an example of a want rather than a need. Within this discussion, Ms.
Henderson also indicated that a house is an example of the previously-established target word
shelter.
The remaining 17 Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk moves together
accounted for 22.38% of total vocabulary talk code applications. For example, teachers used a
word in context along with a hint, such as when Ms. Henderson, young 5s teacher, hinted, “So,
you will have to design some sort of crossing for your animal to get across the road or for, or
under the road.” Teachers also reviewed the meaning of a word that had already been introduced
by eliciting students’ ideas, as when Ms. Thompson, kindergarten teacher, asked, “What do we
know about stratus clouds?” Teachers sometimes acted out or demonstrated the meaning of a
word while saying the word. For instance, Ms. Thompson demonstrated pulling something
toward herself while explaining, “If someone's pulling something, they're moving it closer to
themselves.” As a final example, teachers provided a visual support to illustrate the meaning of a
word at times, as when Ms. Hill, second-grade teacher, pointed to a photograph of the target
word canyon and explained, “We get this gap in between and we still have landforms that are
really high on either side.” (See Appendix E for examples of the full set of Knowledge &
Understanding vocabulary talk moves.)
In summary, these Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk moves were aimed at
providing students with multiple exposures to target words in a variety of contexts and multiple
ways for students to access word meanings, such as by eliciting and/or providing
explanations/definitions and examples/nonexamples for target words, acting out the meaning of
target words, and showing students an image depicting the target word. Therefore, the teachers
used language during science instruction in many ways that are aligned with the research on
75
building students’ knowledge of word meanings. This is important because much of the research
examining vocabulary instruction has been within the context of literacy instruction. This finding
shows this cohort of early-elementary teachers used many research-based vocabulary practices
outside of literacy instruction. While student outcome data was not collected as part of this study,
the alignment with research on how children learn words suggests that the cohort of teachers
may have promoted students’ oral language and vocabulary development with these Knowledge
& Understanding vocabulary talk moves.
Affective Factors
As Table 4 shows, the cohort of teachers used Affective Factors vocabulary talk moves
with the second greatest frequency, accounting for 7.85% of total vocabulary talk code
applications. Within this type of vocabulary talk, the teachers used six different vocabulary talk
moves aligned with the theory and research on interesting students in words and word learning.
The most frequently-occurring Affective Factors vocabulary talk move was helping students
make personal connections, such as by asking students questions about experiences they have
had related to a target word, reminding students of a common experience they had that connects
to a target word (e.g., connecting recent local weather to the target words severe and extreme),
and asking students to think hypothetically about a target word in the context of their own
experiences (e.g., “So, let’s pretend our classroom was our habitat” or “What would you need [to
wear] for that weather?”). Accounting for 6.20% of total vocabulary talk code applications,
connecting to students’ personal experiences was the third most frequently-occurring vocabulary
talk move overall, across all four types of vocabulary talk. The following illustrative example
shows Ms. Henderson helping her young 5s students connect the target words arrangement,
water, food, shelter, space, and habitat to the familiar context of their own classroom:
76
Ms. Henderson: so arrangement is how things are, how things are put next to each other.
[Use Target Word, Explain/Define]
Ms. Henderson: in our classroom we have an arrangement. [Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher points around the room; teacher emphasizes “arrangement”
Ms. Henderson: okay?
Ms. Henderson: our water supply is right there. [Use Target Word, Help Students
Make Personal Connection]
teacher points off camera
Ms. Henderson: do we have to go down the hall to the office to get water? [Use Target
Word, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Henderson: no. [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Henderson: our food supply is in our cubby. [Use Target Word, Help Students
Make Personal Connection]
teacher points in a different direction, off camera
Ms. Henderson: do we have to go all the way outside to get a snack? [Help Students
Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Henderson: no. [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Henderson: our shelter is where we're at, right? [Use Target Word, Emphasize,
Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher makes a circle around the room with her pointer finger; teacher
emphasizes the word “shelter”
Ms. Henderson: our space is kind of small. [Use Target Word, Help Students Make
Personal Connection]
Ms. Henderson: but for just being at school we have enough space. [Use Target Word,
Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Henderson: and, another part of our arrangement is our bathroom, right? [Use Target
Word, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher again points in a different direction to something off camera
Ms. Henderson: it's close enough where we don't have to run to the other building to use
the restroom. [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Henderson: so all four of these things, food, water, shelter, and space, have to be in
an arrangement for the animals. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Visually
Display Word, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher points to the words “food,” “water,” “shelter,” “space,” and
“arrangement” as she says them; teacher emphasizes “arrangement”
In this example, Ms. Henderson paused a discussion about the features of animal habitats (i.e.,
food, water, shelter, space—all in an arrangement) in order to help students make personal
connections to each of these features. While food and water are already familiar to five-year-
olds, the target words arrangement, space, and habitat are more abstract and therefore more
77
challenging, especially for young children. By framing the classroom as a habitat and
encouraging students to think of their food and water supplies, the adequacy of their space, their
shelter, and how everything they need is arranged so that they do not need to leave the classroom,
Ms. Henderson helped students connect to—and likely better understand—these target words.
Students may have been more motivated to understand and use these words upon making these
connections.
The remaining five Affective Factors vocabulary talk moves accounted for only 1.65% of
vocabulary talk moves, combined. At times, teachers used the move of praising student word
use. For example, Ms. Thompson, kindergarten teacher, responded to the student comment “My
prediction is at yellow” by saying, “Oh, I love hearing the word prediction.” Similarly, Ms. Hill,
second-grade teacher, praised a student’s word use by saying, “Oh, I liked her description word.
Thick.” Teachers also used the move of acknowledging students’ “ownership” of words by
attributing words to the students who used them when sharing their ideas. For example, Ms.
Thompson noted, “Myra said there’s a force happening here,” and Ms. Griffin, first-grade
teacher, pointed out, “But I also heard Ellis say curved.” Another move teachers made was to
answer students’ questions about what words mean or what a word for something is. For
example, a kindergarten student asked what a flood was, and Ms. Thompson explained, “A flood
is when too much rain falls and there’s kind of nowhere for it to go.” A second-grade student
asked what to call the yellow material (cornmeal) in the investigation, and Ms. Hill responded,
“You can call it a landform.” Teachers sometimes showed appreciation for a word or word
choice encountered within a lesson from a source other than students. For example, in response
to Weather Conditions as the title of a read-aloud text, Ms. Nelson, kindergarten teacher,
exclaimed, “Oooh, that’s a fancy word” before inviting students to say weather conditions aloud
78
and explaining the meaning of the term. Finally, the move of highlighting a favorite word was
used one time. Ms. Nelson said of precipitation, “That is one of my favorite words.”
In summary, these Affective Factors vocabulary talk moves were aimed at motivating
students to use/learn new words, encouraging students’ curiosity about word meanings, and
modeling appreciation and enjoyment for words and word usage. Therefore, the teachers did use
some language during science instruction in ways that are aligned with theory and (limited)
research on interesting students in words and word learning. This is notable because of the four
types of vocabulary talk, Affective Factors has the least empirical support; yet, this cohort of
teachers used these vocabulary talk moves more frequently than Student Word Use and
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness vocabulary talk moves—both of which are well
supported in the research literature. It is also important to note that Affective Factors vocabulary
talk moves accounted for only 7.85% of total vocabulary talk code applications, suggesting that
these teachers promoted oral language and vocabulary development in this way at times, but it
was not a focus of their vocabulary instruction during science lessons.
Student Word Use
As Table 4 shows, the cohort of teachers used Student Word Use vocabulary talk moves
third most frequently, accounting for 5.23% of total vocabulary talk code applications. Within
this type of vocabulary talk, the teachers used six vocabulary talk moves aligned with the
research on scaffolding students’ word use. The most frequently-occurring Student Word Use
vocabulary talk move was prompting students to use a target word that had been previously
introduced. This vocabulary talk move accounted for 3.21% of total vocabulary talk code
applications. The following example was selected to show how teachers prompted students to
use target words. In this example, Ms. Henderson prompts the young 5s students in her class to
79
use the target word herbivore when describing a plant-eating prehistoric creature from a read-
aloud text:
Ms. Henderson: was this an herbivore? [Review Word, Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “herbivore”
Ms. Henderson: or a carnivore? [Use Target Word]
Ms. Henderson: and how can you tell?
Ms. Henderson: how can you tell?
Ms. Henderson: Hannah?
CHI: carnivore.
Ms. Henderson: it's a carnivore? [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “carnivore”
Ms. Henderson: what makes you think it was a carnivore? [Use Target Word,
Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “carnivore”
Child(ren): because it ate plants.
Ms. Henderson: so if it ate plants what was it called? [Prompt Word]
Child(ren): um, herbivore.
Ms. Henderson: herbivore. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “herbivore”
Ms. Henderson: alright, show me a thumbs up if you agree with Hannah that it's an
herbivore (be)cause it ate plants. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher holds up a thumbs up and most students copy her; teacher emphasizes the
word “herbivore”
Ms. Henderson: okay, I see a couple thumbs down.
Ms. Henderson: Tripp, tell me why you disagree.
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Henderson: okay so it ate plants with its teeth.
Ms. Henderson: and a lot of carnivores have teeth. [Use Target Word]
Ms. Henderson: do herbivores have teeth? [Use Target Word]
Child(ren): yes.
Ms. Henderson: okay.
Ms. Henderson: so if it has teeth and it's eating a plant, what would it be? [Prompt
Word]
teacher emphasizes the word “plant”
Child(ren): a herbivore.
Child(ren): a herbivore.
Ms. Henderson: an herbivore. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “herbivore”
In this example, the target words herbivore and carnivore had previously been introduced, but it
is evident that there is still some confusion among the young 5s students about which term to use
for plant eaters and which to use with meat eaters. While helping students work through this
80
confusion, Ms. Henderson prompted students to use the target word herbivore twice. In other
examples of this vocabulary talk move, Ms. Nelson, kindergarten teacher, prompted students to
use the target word cumulonimbus with the utterance “What type of cloud produces
thunderstorms?” and elicited the target word temperature with a series of prompts, including
“When the meteorologist checks the weather, they talk about precipitation, the clouds, the wind,
and the what, Avery?”
The remaining five Student Word Use vocabulary talk moves together accounted for only
2.02% of total vocabulary talk code applications. In one move, teachers suggested another way
to say something, such as when Ms. Thompson responded to a kindergarten student’s idea that
fall weather conditions usually include “light wind” by suggesting, “Maybe we could say the
word breezy?” In a similar move, teachers elevated students’ word use by revoicing what a
student said in a more academic or conventional way. In one example, a young 5s student
explained that the character Bear from the read-aloud text would not be able to live where the
character Polar Bear lived “(Be)cause it [Bear] has less wool than him [Polar Bear]” and Ms.
Henderson responded by saying, “He has less fur than Polar Bear?” Teachers also used the
moved of asking students to repeat (say aloud) a target word, such as when Ms. Henderson
introduced the word sauropod and asked, “Can you say that word?” At times, teachers elicited a
word from students that had not been previously introduced, such as when Ms. Nelson explained
to her kindergarten students that the squirrel in the video “kind of blends in a little bit” and then
asked, “What do we call that? Does anyone know the science word when you kind of blend into
your environment?” At this point, camouflage became a target word. Finally, teachers
occasionally encouraged students to use target words at a later time—outside of the immediate
context in which they were introduced or reviewed. For example, Ms. Hill, second-grade teacher,
81
wrote the word insulating, explaining, “I’m going to put it here because chances are, you are
going to use this in your writing later.” Shortly thereafter, she said, “So you will be using this
word [insulating] later. That I can promise you.”
In sum, the Student Word Use vocabulary talk moves promoted students’ use of target
words and provided scaffolds to support their oral language in general. Therefore, the teachers
did use some language during science instruction in ways that are aligned with the research on
scaffolding students’ word use. However, as these Student Word Use vocabulary talk moves
accounted for only 5.23% of total vocabulary talk moves, promoting oral language and
vocabulary development in this way was not a focus of teachers’ instruction.
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
As shown in Table 4, the cohort of teachers used Metalinguistic & Metacognitive
Awareness vocabulary talk moves least frequently, accounting for only 0.20% of total
vocabulary talk code applications. Within this type of talk, the teachers used only one vocabulary
talk move aligned with the research on increasing students’ awareness of words and word
learning: asking students if they have ever heard of a word. This vocabulary talk move was used
very infrequently, appearing only seven times across the 24 recorded lessons. The following
example was selected to show how teachers used this move when introducing a new target word.
In this example, Ms. Thompson finds out if her kindergarten students are familiar with the new
target word severe:
Ms. Thompson: this word says severe. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Visually Display Word]
teacher emphasizes the word “severe”
Child(ren): severe?
Ms. Thompson: thumbs up if you've ever heard that word before? [Ask if Heard of
Word]
students give thumbs up
82
Ms. Thompson: uh, in the past three, four days, we've had some severe weather
conditions. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Provide Example/Nonexample,
Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes the word “severe”
Ms. Thompson: do you agree?
Ms. Thompson: have you heard anybody say that on the news? [Ask if Heard of Word]
Ms. Thompson: or your parents use that word? [Ask if Heard of Word]
Ms. Thompson: severe? [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “severe”
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Thompson: oh boy /
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Thompson: let me tell you what it means.
Ms. Thompson: and then you tell me if you've, if you've heard that in the last couple
days. [Ask if Heard of Word]
In this example, Ms. Thompson asked if students had heard the word severe and then gave a little
context of where they might have heard the word before. In another lesson, Ms. Thompson
introduced the term friction and, before explaining what it means, asked, “Who has heard of that
word before?”
This Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness vocabulary talk move aims to gauge
students’ awareness of target words as they are introduced. As reviewed in Chapter 2, the
research on building students’ metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness recommends
instruction that teaches students to use contextual and morphemic analysis (e.g., Baumann et al.,
2002, 2002, 2003, 2003; P. N. Bowers & Kirby, 2010; Freeman et al., 2019; Fukkink & de
Glopper, 1998; Lubliner & Smetana, 2005; Nash & Snowling, 2006; Silverman et al., 2014;
Wise, 2019), explain how word meanings are derived from context (Cain, 2007), detect semantic
ambiguity (Zipke et al., 2009), and self-monitor and self-regulate (e.g., Boulware-Gooden et al.,
2007; Lubliner & Smetana, 2005). The present study, however, shows that such metalinguistic
and metacognitive awareness instruction was largely absent in these early-elementary science
lessons.
83
In summary, the cohort of teachers used vocabulary talk moves from each of the four
types of vocabulary talk. A majority of these vocabulary talk moves addressed building students’
knowledge of word meanings, while fewer moves addressed interesting students in words and
word learning, scaffolding students’ word use, and building awareness of words and word
learning.
Within-Teacher Comparisons of Vocabulary Talk
In addition to identifying the specific talk moves the cohort of teachers made, I made
within-teacher comparisons in order to understand whether there were any patterns in the types
of vocabulary talk moves individual teachers used. In other words, I was interested to see if any
patterns emerged when looking from timepoint to timepoint within an individual teacher’s data.
Figure 2 shows the percentage of each type of vocabulary talk each teacher used at each of the
three timepoints (i.e., 1: PrePD, 2: Week 5, 3: Week 9). With few exceptions, teachers used a
greater percentage of Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk moves during each of their
lessons than any other type of vocabulary talk. The exceptions are with two teachers who used
little to no vocabulary talk in at least one of their lessons. Ms. Watson, first-grade teacher, made
only one vocabulary talk move during her second lesson; therefore, as Figure 2
Figure 2 shows, 100% of the vocabulary talk in this lesson addressed Student Word Use. She did
not make any vocabulary talk moves during her third lesson. Similarly, Ms. Hill, second-grade
teacher, did not make any vocabulary talk moves in her second lesson. Therefore, overall, when
teachers used multiple vocabulary talk moves, a majority of these moves were aimed at building
students’ knowledge of word meanings. This is consistent with the previous finding that as a
cohort, the teachers used a greater percentage of Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk
moves across the three lessons than any other type of vocabulary talk (as shown in Table 4).
84
Further, this is reflective of the research literature on how children learn words, with most
research focusing on building students’ knowledge of word meanings.
Figure 2
Within-Teacher Comparisons of Vocabulary Talk Types Across Lessons
Ms. Henderson
(Young 5s)
Ms. Thompson
(Kindergarten)
Ms. Nelson
(Kindergarten)
Ms. Baker
(Kindergarten)
100%
100%
100%
100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
1
2
3
1
2
3
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
85
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
1
2
3
1
2
3
Figure 2 (cont’d)
Ms. Griffin
(Grade 1)
Ms. Watson
(Grade 1)
Ms. Hill
(Grade 2)
Ms. Howard
(Grade 2)
100%
100%
100%
100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
Between-Teacher Comparisons of Vocabulary Talk
After making within-teacher comparisons, I made between-teacher comparisons in order
to understand whether there were any trends for the cohort as a whole with vocabulary talk. In
86
other words, I was interested to see if any patterns emerged when looking from teacher to teacher
across the entire dataset. Figure 3 shows the vocabulary talk moves per minute for each teacher
at each of the three timepoints (i.e., 1: PrePD, 2: Week 5, 3: Week 9). As Figure 3 shows, all
eight teachers engaged in more Knowledge & Understanding talk than any of the other types of
vocabulary talk, and overall, none of the teachers engaged in much of the other types of talk.
Although the teachers consistently engaged in the most Knowledge & Understanding talk, there
is no pattern to the variation in this talk from lesson to lesson. That is, some teachers showed
increases in Knowledge & Understanding talk from the PrePD lesson to the Week 5 lesson while
others showed decreases—and some teachers showed increases from the Week 5 lesson to the
Week 9 lesson while others showed decreases. Likewise, there is no consistent pattern to the
variation in Student Word Use talk, Metacognitive & Metalinguistic talk, or Affective Factors
talk from lesson to lesson. Therefore, there may be lesson-specific factors that impact how
teachers engage in vocabulary talk. Research Question 3 in Chapter 5 takes a closer look at the
science instruction of Ms. Thompson, kindergarten teacher, in order to explore features of
science curriculum materials that may have contributed to the variance in vocabulary talk from
lesson to lesson.
87
Figure 3
Between-Teacher Comparison of Vocabulary Talk Types Across Lessons
Ms. Henderson
(Young 5s)
Ms. Thompson
(Kindergarten)
Ms. Nelson
(Kindergarten)
Ms. Baker
(Kindergarten)
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
88
Figure 3 (cont’d)
Ms. Griffin
(Grade 1)
Ms. Watson
(Grade 1)
Ms. Howard
(Grade 2)
Ms. Hill
(Grade 2)
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
1
2
3
Note: Rates show number of vocabulary talk moves per minute for each teacher.
In summary, the cohort of teachers engaged in considerably more Knowledge &
Understanding vocabulary talk than Student Word Use talk, Metacognitive & Metalinguistic talk,
or Affective Factors talk. This dominance of Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk is
89
apparent in both within- and between-teacher comparisons. This finding reflects the research
base, as most research on how children learn words has focused on building students’ knowledge
of word meanings.
Research Question 2: Is Vocabulary Talk Related to the Language Aspects of Science Talk,
and if so, How?
Given the potential intersection between vocabulary talk—talk that promotes oral
language and vocabulary development—and the language aspects of science talk, which focus on
teachers supporting students in (a) developing an understanding of science oral language within
the context of science learning and (b) using science oral language to share their ideas and
engage in science and engineering practices (Wright et al., 2017), I looked for a relationship
between these two types of talk. Vocabulary talk and the language aspects of science talk were
measured on different scales; therefore, I looked at overall trends between each teacher’s
vocabulary talk graph and science talk graph. (I will be shortening language aspects of science
talk to merely science talk for the remainder of this chapter to support ease of reading; however,
I am specifically referring to Components 1-2 of Domain 3 the SOLID Start Tool and not the full
science talk construct.)
90
Figure 4
Ms. Henderson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
Figure 5
Ms. Baker’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
91
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
Figure 6
Ms. Watson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
Figure 7
Ms. Griffin’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
92
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
Figure 8
Ms. Hill’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
I found that the overall shape of the vocabulary talk graph was similar to the overall
shape of the science talk graph for five of the teachers: Ms. Henderson (see Figure 4), Ms. Baker
(see Figure 5), Ms. Watson (see Figure 6), Ms. Griffin (see Figure 7), and Ms. Hill (see Figure
8). In particular, the trend for Knowledge & Understanding within vocabulary talk and the trend
for Component 1 within science talk reflect one another in these graphs. This relationship makes
sense, as the Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk moves focus on building students’
knowledge of word meanings and Component 1 of science talk focuses on how teachers support
students in developing an understanding of science oral language within the context of science
learning. The overall focus, therefore, is on developing understanding of word meanings. The
relationship between the trend for Student Word Use within vocabulary talk and the trend for
Component 2 within science talk is less clear. Theoretically, these are also related, as Student
Word Use vocabulary talk moves focus on teacher talk that scaffolds students’ word use and
93
Component 2 of science talk focuses on teachers’ scaffolding of students’ use of discipline-
specific oral language within the context of discussing ideas and engaging in science and
engineering practices. The overall focus here is on scaffolding students’ use of words. However,
because there were very few code applications for Student Word Use vocabulary talk moves, it is
difficult to see if there is a relationship between this type of vocabulary talk and Component 2 of
science talk. Therefore, Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk appears to be related to the
language aspects of science talk focused on building knowledge of science oral language (i.e.,
Component 1), but the relationship between Student Word Use vocabulary talk and the language
aspects of science talk focused on scaffolding students’ word use (i.e., Component 2) requires
further study.
Figure 9
Ms. Nelson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
94
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
Figure 10
Ms. Thompson’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
The overall shape of the vocabulary talk graph was not similar to the overall shape of the
science talk graph for Ms. Nelson (see Figure 9) and Ms. Thompson (see Figure 10), and this
disconnect highlights the difference between noting that vocabulary talk is happening (as
depicted in the vocabulary talk graphs) and noting the quality of the vocabulary instruction (as
depicted in the science talk graphs). For example, Ms. Nelson’s PrePD lesson (timepoint 1 on
graph) was on the 100th day of school, and she encouraged her kindergarten students to come up
with 100 science words they had learned so far in the SOLID Start Weather Forecasting unit as a
way of getting back into the unit after missing more than a week of school due to severe winter
weather. During the lesson, she elicited students’ ideas about what weather words they had
learned and what they meant. She also elicited examples of these words (e.g., types of
precipitation) from students and offered confirmation or clarification. Ms. Nelson recorded all of
these words on the board, grouping related words together (e.g., hail, snow, sleet, rain listed
95
under precipitation). She provided multiple exposures to these words throughout the lesson and
drew attention to them with emphasis. She also helped students make personal connections to
many of these target words and prompted students to use them. Therefore, Ms. Nelson engaged
in a great deal of vocabulary talk in general and a great deal of Knowledge & Understanding talk
in particular, as evidenced by her rate of 12.81 Knowledge & Understanding moves per minute.
However, much of this vocabulary talk did not happen within the context of science learning,
which is reflected in the Component 1 composite score of 2.77. Similarly, although students were
listing many science words, they were not using these words to discuss their ideas or engage in
science and engineering practices, which is reflected in the Component 2 composite score of
2.77. Thus, the quantity of vocabulary talk within the lesson did not align with the quality of the
vocabulary instruction.
This disconnect is also apparent in Ms. Thompson’s second lesson (see Figure 9). In this
lesson, Ms. Thompson engaged in Knowledge & Understanding talk less frequently than in her
other two lessons; however, Component 1 scored nearly as high as with her first lesson. Further
analysis shows that when she did introduce and review the meaning of science oral language, she
did so in a child-friendly way within the context of science learning. In other words, the quantity
of her vocabulary talk was relatively low, but the quality of this instruction was high. Likewise,
Ms. Thompson engaged in little Student Word Use talk in her second lesson, but the lesson
scored relatively high for Component 2 compared to her other two lessons. Further analysis
shows that when her kindergarten students used science oral language, it was to share their ideas
and engage in science practices (i.e., within an investigation). Therefore, the quantity of talk
scaffolding students’ word use was low, but the quality of vocabulary instruction engaging
96
students in using science oral language was relatively high. Once again, the quality of vocabulary
instruction was not captured by the counts of vocabulary talk use.
This finding points to the need to consider both the ways in which teachers use
vocabulary talk moves to promote oral language and vocabulary development and the overall
quality of this vocabulary talk. This is important because, for these vocabulary talk moves to be
effective in promoting students’ oral language and vocabulary development, the quality of the
vocabulary talk likely matters. Therefore, studies examining the relationship between teachers’
vocabulary talk and students’ oral language and vocabulary development need to take into
consideration both what vocabulary talk moves were made and what the quality of that
vocabulary talk was.
Figure 11
Ms. Howard’s Vocabulary Talk and Science Talk
Vocabulary Talk
Science Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
5
4
3
2
1
1
2
3
Component 1
Component 2
Finally, the overall shape of the vocabulary talk graph was not very similar to the overall
shape of the science talk graph for Ms. Howard (see Figure 11), but in this case, this does not
97
reflect a disconnect between quantity and quality of talk. Rather, Ms. Howard engaged in very
little vocabulary talk (low quantity) at all three timepoints, and her science talk scores were
under 3.0 for all three time points (low quality). In other words, the quantity of vocabulary talk
and quality of vocabulary instruction were consistently low across all three timepoints.
Therefore, while comparing the overall shapes of the vocabulary talk and science talk graphs
provides some insight into how vocabulary talk relates to the language aspects of science talk,
future research examining this relationship may be enhanced with a more precise approach.
Summary of Findings
The cohort of teachers used vocabulary talk moves across the four types of vocabulary
talk, with a majority of vocabulary talk moves (i.e., 21 different moves) and vocabulary talk code
applications (i.e., 86.72%) aimed to build students’ knowledge and understanding of word
meanings and considerably fewer moves (i.e., 13 moves total) and code applications (i.e.,
13.28% combined) dedicated to interesting students in words and word learning, scaffolding
students’ word use, and building awareness of words and word learning. This dominance of
Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk is apparent in both within- and between-teacher
comparisons.
While there does appear to be a relationship between Knowledge & Understanding
vocabulary talk and the language aspects of science talk focused on building knowledge of
science oral language (i.e., Component 1), further research is needed to examine whether a
relationship exists between Student Word Use vocabulary talk and the language aspects of
science talk focused on scaffolding students’ word use (i.e., Component 2). Further, this analysis
points to the need to consider the context in which vocabulary talk moves are made and the
overall quality of this vocabulary talk in addition to examining which moves the teachers make.
98
CHAPTER 5—STUDY 2 FINDINGS: RESEARCH QUESTION 3
The primary goal of this study was to describe the relationship between curricular
features of science materials and a kindergarten teacher’s enhanced vocabulary talk. I report
findings based on 128.03 minutes of observational data across three total lessons, ranging from
35:45 to 56:21, in Ms. Thompson’s kindergarten classrooms. Ms. Thompson’s science
instruction was selected for this analysis because she taught from a different set of curriculum
materials for each of her three lessons, providing an opportunity to investigate how the different
curricular features within these sets of materials may have contributed to the considerable
variance in Ms. Thompson’s talk (i.e., within-subject differences) across the three science
lessons (see Figure 12). In this study, I investigated the following research question: What
features of science curriculum materials are related to a kindergarten teacher’s enhanced
vocabulary talk? In this chapter, I first describe the three lessons Ms. Thompson taught, as
presented by the three sets of curriculum materials. Then I present three curricular features that
may be associated with Ms. Thompson’s vocabulary talk, along with transcripts depicting how
vocabulary talk varied relative to these features.
99
Figure 12
Ms. Thompson’s Vocabulary Talk
13.00
12.00
11.00
10.00
9.00
8.00
7.00
6.00
5.00
4.00
3.00
2.00
1.00
0.00
1
2
3
Knowledge & Understanding
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness
Student Word Use
Affective Factors
Note: Rates show number of vocabulary talk moves per minute.
The Lessons
In this section, I describe the three lessons Ms. Thompson drew from when teaching her
three video recorded lessons. Note that these descriptions are of the written lessons, as presented
by the curriculum materials, rather than the enacted lessons.
SOLID Start: How Can We Prepare for Severe Weather Conditions?
For the first video recorded lesson (i.e., PrePD), Ms. Thompson drew from the SOLID
Start (Gotwals & Wright, 2017; Wright & Gotwals, 2017) kindergarten Weather Forecasting
unit, teaching Lesson 7: How Can We Prepare for Severe Weather Conditions? This lesson is
from a 10-lesson unit exploring the driving question How Does Our Weather in [Your Location]
Change Over Time? The lesson overview estimates this to be a 50-minute lesson and shows five
lesson activities with the following descriptions:
100
• Ask: Review types of precipitation and introduce Lesson Question
• Explore: Students consider ways to prepare for different types of precipitation.
• Read: What Is Severe Weather? by Jennifer Boothroyd and What Is Precipitation by
Robin Johnson
• Write: The teacher and students create a class list of how to prepare for different types of
severe weather.
• Synthesize: Students discuss severe weather and connect it to the Driving Question.
The frontmatter to the lesson also includes the following components:
• Lesson Question: How Can We Prepare for Severe Weather Conditions?
•
I-can statements: I can describe types of weather conditions in my state. I can explain
why it is important to prepare for severe weather.
• Relevant NGSS performance expectation components (NGSS Lead States, 2013):
o Disciplinary Core Idea: ESS3.B: Some kinds of severe weather are more likely
than others in a given region. Weather scientists forecast severe weather so that
the communities can prepare for and respond to these events.
o Crosscutting Concept: Patterns: Students should identify patterns in their
observations of the natural world. These patterns can be used as evidence.
o Science and Engineering Practices: Obtaining, Evaluating, and Communicating
Information: Students should read texts or use other media to obtain information
about patterns in the natural world or to obtain evidence. In texts, student should
be able to interpret diagrams. Evidence should be obtained from multiple sources
to answer scientific questions. Students should communicate their ideas orally, in
writing, or in drawings.
101
• New science content vocabulary with child-friendly definitions: severe, flood,
snowstorm, tornado, hurricane
• List of vocabulary to review/reinforce from previous lessons: weather conditions, rain,
snow, wind, season, fall, winter, spring, summer, cool, cold, warm, hot, prepare,
measure, meteorologist, precipitation, thunderstorm
• List of materials for lesson
The lesson plan itself details each of the five lesson activities, with vocabulary terms introduced
at specific points in the lesson using the given child-friendly definitions and with discussion
questions provided throughout. Additionally, teaching tips appear alongside the lesson plan, with
recommendations for teachers. For example, the following tip accompanies the Ask activity:
Encourage students to use the vocabulary words they have learned from previous lessons when
discussing precipitation. At the end of the lesson plan is a sample summary table with examples
of what the lesson’s entries could include. For example, for the column What Did We Learn?
(Claims) (statements), the sample text reads: Severe weather is dangerous, and it is important to
stay inside and usually it is also important to stay away from windows.
Teachers Pay Teachers: A Push Is a Force!
Ms. Thompson drew from a teacher-created packet of kindergarten lesson materials from
Teachers Pay Teachers for her second video recorded lesson (i.e., Week 5). The packet contains
two force and motion activities. Ms. Thompson’s lesson used the first activity: A Push Is a
Force! The following components were included in the packet for this activity:
• Performance expectation: K-PS2-1 [Plan and conduct an investigation to compare the
effects of different strengths or different directions of pushes and pulls on the motion of
an object (NGSS Lead States, 2013)]
102
• List of materials for the activity
•
Instructions for the activity
• Two templates to print, laminate, and cut out for the activity
• Three photographs with captions showing how to set up the activity and how to have
students record their responses
• Two students sheets and an answer key
During this activity, the students predict how far their toy car will go after being pushed with
“medium” and “hard” force. Then they take turns pushing their cards and recording the results.
The activity description does not mention how the worksheet entitled Why does the car stop
moving? fits into the activity, but the answer key indicates that the word students are to write in
the blank is friction, implying that friction is discussed at some point in the activity.
Mystery Science: Where Do Animals Live?
For the third video recorded lesson (i.e., Week 9), Ms. Thompson drew from the Mystery
Science (Schacht & Peltz, 2020) kindergarten Plant and Animal Secrets unit, teaching Lesson 2:
Where Do Animals Live? This web-based lesson is from a 6-lesson unit focused on helping
kindergarten students understand that animals and plants need things to survive and that they do
the things they do in order to meet their needs. The following components comprise the online
lesson materials:
• Overview identifying grade level (K), topic (Plant & Animal Needs), and focus (Animal
Needs: Shelter)
• 25-minute digital text (i.e., slides with video-recorded pages) Who Lives There?
• A description of the lesson labeled “Activity Prep” and “Prep Instructions” with a
suggestion (and link) for the optional activity
103
• “Extensions” with links for Google Classroom (to assign the video), a PDF to download
for assessment (with space for students to draw an animal in its home and write about it
with the sentence frames “My animal is a…” and “It makes its home in…”), the optional
activity (one-minute video and three discussion questions), the transcript of the lesson
introduction video (part of the digital text), and a link to embed the lesson on a class
website.
This lesson explores the question Where do animals live? and is labeled as a “Read-Along”
lesson. For this lesson, the teacher advances slides through the video-recorded reading of a
digital text. Afterward, there is an optional activity to show a one-minute video and facilitate a
discussion with the three given prompts. This optional video and discussion is labeled as a 20-
minute “hands-on activity.” Additionally, the K-5 planning guide available on the website
identifies the following NGSS alignment for this lesson:
• Performance expectation: K-ESS3-1 [Use a model to represent the relationship
between the needs of different plants and animals (including humans) and the places they
live (NGSS Lead States, 2013)]
• Topic: Animal Homes
• Disciplinary Core Ideas: ESS3.A Living things need food, water, shelter, and many other
resources to survive! All living things live in places that provide the needs they have to
survive. Not all living things live in a house, like humans do. Animals live in many
different types of homes close to their resources.
• Science and Engineering Practices: Students obtain information through media about
how different animal homes are built. They communicate this information in order to
identify patterns in the natural world.
104
• Crosscutting Concepts: Students identify the pattern that all living things live where their
needs are met. They recognize that plants, animals, and their surroundings make up a
system as parts that work together.
In summary, the SOLID Start lesson identifies components of the NGSS performance
expectations it addresses, includes five activities, targets vocabulary words for instruction
through introduction or review/reinforcement, and provides recommendations to teachers—
including some for supporting oral language and vocabulary development. The Teachers Pay
Teachers activity identifies the NGSS performance expectation it addresses and provides
templates and instructions for the activity. Finally, the Mystery Science lesson provides a digital
text with video recorded pages and an optional activity with a video clip and discussion prompt
(and the planning guide shows alignment with NGSS). In the sections that follow, I share my
findings for Research Question 3: What features of science curriculum materials are related to a
kindergarten teacher’s enhanced vocabulary talk?
Targeted Vocabulary
The identification of specific vocabulary words targeted for instruction within a lesson by
science curriculum materials was related to Ms. Thompson’s enhanced vocabulary talk. This was
apparent in the SOLID Start Weather Forecasting lesson, which identifies five new vocabulary
words (i.e., severe, flood, snowstorm, tornado, and hurricane) to introduce at specific points
within the lesson, provides child-friendly explanations for each, and offers discussion questions
to engage students in active processing (Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986) of the target words. For
example, the lesson directs teachers to provide instruction on the target word severe within the
context of introducing the lesson question: How can we prepare for severe weather conditions?
105
The lesson provides “dangerous or extreme” as the child-friendly explanation and offers the
following sample questions for eliciting students’ initial ideas:
• What are some examples of severe weather you are familiar with?
• What are some things you have learned about preparing for severe weather?
• What are some questions you have about severe weather?
The following transcript shows Ms. Thompson’s vocabulary talk as she introduced the targeted
vocabulary term severe:
Ms. Thompson: this word says severe. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Visually Display Word]
teacher has written “severe” on a small whiteboard; teacher emphasizes the
word “severe”
Child(ren): severe?
Ms. Thompson: thumbs up if you've ever heard that word before? [Ask if Heard of
Word]
many students give thumbs up
Ms. Thompson: uh, in the past three, four days, we've had some severe weather
conditions. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Provide Example/Nonexample,
Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes the word “severe”
Ms. Thompson: do you agree?
Ms. Thompson: have you heard anybody say that on the news? [Ask if Heard of Word]
Ms. Thompson: or your parents use that word? [Ask if Heard of Word]
Ms. Thompson: severe? [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “severe”
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Thompson: oh boy /
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Thompson: let me tell you what it means.
Ms. Thompson: and then you tell me if you've, if you've heard that in the last couple
days. [Ask if Heard of Word]
Ms. Thompson: it means dangerous. [Explain/Define, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “dangerous”
Ms. Thompson: has the weather in the last few days been dangerous? [Help Students
Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): yeah, the only dangerous weather is tornadoes.
Ms. Thompson: that's not the only dangerous //
Ms. Thompson: why couldn't you go outside? [Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
106
Ms. Thompson: why didn't we go to school yesterday? [Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
Child(ren): because severe xxx frostbite.
Ms. Thompson: it was severe. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Help Students Make
Personal Connection, Use with Hint]
teacher emphasizes the word “severe”
Ms. Thompson: it was dangerous. [Emphasize, Help Students Make Personal
Connection, Use with Hint]
teacher emphasizes the word “dangerous”
Ms. Thompson:
Child(ren): .
Ms. Thompson: from what?
Child(ren): five, five minutes.
Ms. Thompson: I'm thinking of the therm /
Child(ren): five.
students arguing in background about number of minutes before frostbite sets in
Ms. Thompson: we're not going to argue about this.
Ms. Thompson: what was severe about the weather yesterday? [Use Target Word, Help
Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: why can you get frostbite? [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes the word “why”
Ms. Thompson: because it was what, Myles? [Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
Child(ren): way too cold.
Ms. Thompson: it was too cold. [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: the temperature was severe. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Provide
Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes the words “temperature” and “severe”
Ms. Thompson: say that sentence. [Ask to Repeat]
Ms. Thompson: . [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Use
with Hint, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes the words “temperature” and “severe”
Child(ren): .
Ms. Thompson: it was dangerous. [Emphasize, Visually Display Word, Use with Hint,
Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes the word “dangerous” and points to it on whiteboard
Ms. Thompson: do you agree?
Child(ren): yeah.
Ms. Thompson: it was //
Ms. Thompson: here's another good word. [Appreciate Word/Word Choice]
teacher writes word on whiteboard
Ms. Thompson: it was extreme. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word, Emphasize,
Visually Display Word, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes the word “extreme”
Ms. Thompson: try that word. [Ask to Repeat]
Ms. Thompson: . [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
107
teacher emphasizes the word “extreme”
Child(ren): .
Ms. Thompson: extreme means so so so so so. [Use Target Word, Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: was it so so so so so cold? [Emphasize, Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
teacher emphasizes “so so so so so”
Child(ren): yeah.
Ms. Thompson: instead of saying so so so so so, we could say it was extreme. [Use
Target Word, Emphasize, Use with Hint, Offer Alternative, Help Students
Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “so so so so so” and “extreme”
Ms. Thompson: it was severe. [Use Target Word, Use with Hint, Help Students Make
Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: it was dangerous. [Use with Hint, Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
Ms. Thompson: it was extreme. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Visually Display
Word, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “extreme” and points to the word on the whiteboard.
Ms. Thompson: do you agree?
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Thompson: it hurt your face if you went outside or your throat if you were breathing
the air. [Provide Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
This example shows the relationship between the SOLID Start lesson’s (a) identification of the
target word severe, (b) provision of a child-friendly explanation, and (c) offering of sample
questions for engaging students further with the meaning of the word and Ms. Thompson’s
enhanced vocabulary talk. In Ms. Thompson’s enactment of the lesson, she used part of the
definition provided (i.e., dangerous) to explain the meaning of severe and then introduced
extreme (the other part of the child-friendly definition provided by the SOLID Start lesson) as its
own target word. She provided students with multiple exposures to these words in a variety of
contexts, with 17 exposures for severe and 12 for extreme (not including instances in which these
terms were read from text). Ms. Thompson connected these terms to recent severe weather the
children had all experienced, as school had been canceled for more than a week due to extreme
cold and a prolonged period of snowfall. Ms. Thompson encouraged students to say these words
108
aloud and visually displayed these words as she talked about them. Following the transcript
shown here, Ms. Thompson used a variation of one of the sample questions provided to engage
students further in a discussion regarding preparing for severe weather. (See next paragraph for
transcript of this discussion). Ms. Thompson’s thorough introduction of the term severe with a
variety of vocabulary talk moves is seemingly related to its identification as a target word. Ms.
Thompson consistently engaged in this kind of dense vocabulary talk throughout the lesson in
places in which the SOLID Start lesson had identified target words to introduce or review,
indicating a relationship between targeted vocabulary words and enhanced vocabulary talk.
The SOLID Start lesson also identifies 18 previously-introduced vocabulary words (e.g.,
weather conditions, meteorologist, precipitation) to be reviewed and reinforced within the
current lesson, providing multiple exposures to these words and highlighting connections
between them and newly-introduced target words. (See Table 5 for complete list of words
identified in the SOLID Start lesson for review and reinforcement.) Accompanying this list of
words is the following teaching tip: These words are from past lessons to review and reinforce
throughout this lesson. Repetition and multiple exposures to words support vocabulary
development. For science content words, draw students’ attention to the Word Wall. In the
following example, Ms. Thompson reviews/reinforces the target word prepare (from Lesson 2 in
the SOLID Start Weather Forecasting unit) while engaging students in a discussion related to the
new target word severe:
Ms. Thompson: here's another word.
Ms. Thompson: this makes me think of what you did if you went outside yesterday.
[Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: you really had to // [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: look at this word.
teacher writes the word on the whiteboard
Ms. Thompson: you had to prepare. [Review Word, Use Target Word, Emphasize,
Visually Display Word, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
109
teacher emphasizes the word “prepare”
Ms. Thompson: what does prepare mean? [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Use Target
Word, Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word]
teacher emphasizes the word “prepare”
Child(ren): get all your snow stuff, I get ready /
Ms. Thompson: get ready. [Emphasize, Explain/Define]
teacher emphasizes “get ready”
Ms. Thompson: you had to get ready. [Emphasize, Explain/Define, Help Students
Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “get ready”
Ms. Thompson: so prepare means get ready. [Use Target Word, Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: what do you have to do to get ready for that extreme weather we had
yesterday? [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Elicit Student Ideas:
Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “get ready” and “extreme”
Ms. Thompson: what would you have to do? [Elicit Student Ideas:
Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Thompson: put your what? [Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help
Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): snow gear on.
Ms. Thompson: put your snow gear on. [Provide Example/Nonexample, Help Students
Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: what else did you have to do to prepare? [Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make
Personal Connection]
teacher emphasized the word “prepare”
Child(ren): xxx, scarf, xxx.
Ms. Thompson: scarves?
Ms. Thompson: you know what I did to prepare? [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasized the word “prepare”
Ms. Thompson: before the weather, before the severe weather came? [Use Target Word,
Emphasize]
teacher emphasized the word “severe”
Ms. Thompson: I went to the grocery store. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: and I made sure I had enough food at home. [Provide
Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: did anyone else's family go shopping so you made sure you had enough
food at home? [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): I don't know.
Ms. Thompson: you know what else I did to prepare? [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasized the word “prepare”
Ms. Thompson: I filled my gas tank up with gas. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: because I did not want to have to go to the gas station when it was severe
weather outside. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Visually Display Word,
Example/Nonexample]
110
teacher holds up whiteboard and emphasizes the word “severe”
Ms. Thompson: it would have been dangerous for me to stand outside. [Emphasize]
teacher points to the word “dangerous” on the whiteboard and emphasizes it
Ms. Thompson: I could have hurt my face or my skin.
Ms. Thompson: so I prepared by getting groceries, by getting gas in my gas tank. [Use
Target Word, Provide Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: and if I did go outside, what do you think I wore? [Elicit Student Ideas:
Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: when I went to lunch with Kyle yesterday, what do you think I wore?
[Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample]
Child(ren): snow clothes, scarf.
Ms. Thompson: a scarf?
Ms. Thompson: yes, I did. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Thompson: gloves. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: what else did I wear? [Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample]
Child(ren): coat.
Ms. Thompson: a coat. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Child(ren): snow pants.
Ms. Thompson: snow pants. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: what else? [Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample]
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Thompson: boots on my feet. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: and a hat on my head. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
Child(ren): xxx.
Ms. Thompson: I had to prepare because the weather / [Use Target Word, Emphasize,
Visually Display Word]
teacher emphasized “prepare” and points to it on whiteboard
Child(ren): xxx pants on.
Ms. Thompson: I had to prepare because the weather was... [Use Target Word, Prompt
Word, Visually Display Word]
teacher prompts students and is still holding up whiteboard with word “severe”
Child(ren): .
Ms. Thompson: . [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “severe”
Ms. Thompson: when the weather is going to be severe, you have to prepare. [Use
Target Word, Emphasize, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “prepare”
Ms. Thompson: you have to prepare. [Use Target Word, Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
Child(ren): you have to put a scarf on.
Ms. Thompson: yes, all of those things. [Provide Example/Nonexample]
In this transcript, Ms. Thompson elicited students’ ideas to review the meaning of prepare and to
generate examples of preparing within the context of severe winter weather. Ms. Thompson also
111
provided examples of preparing and helped students make personal connections to their recent
shared experience with severe winter weather. By using the term prepare repeatedly throughout
the discussion, she provided multiple exposures, as suggested in the lesson’s teaching tip. She
also drew attention to the word by repeatedly emphasizing it. Once again, Ms. Thompson’s in-
depth review and reinforcement of the word prepare with a variety of vocabulary talk moves
seems to be related to its designation as a target word to be reviewed and reinforced by the
SOLID Start curriculum materials.
In addition to the terms targeted for instruction by the SOLID Start lesson, Ms.
Thompson, on her own, also targeted seven words for instruction or review/reinforcement by
bringing students’ awareness to them and their meanings. Of these words, four were targeted for
instruction in previous SOLID Start Weather Forecasting lessons but did not appear in the list of
words to review/reinforce at the beginning of this particular lesson (i.e., breezy, calm, stratus,
cumulonimbus). Although these words were not targeted for review/reinforcement within this
particular lesson, Ms. Thompson’s decision to do so within the current lesson may be related to
their identification as target words in previous lessons. Two of the words targeted by Ms.
Thompson were not words targeted for instruction by the SOLID Start curriculum but were
discussed in previous lessons (i.e., wind flag, storm clouds). The final word targeted for
instruction by Ms. Thompson was severe, which was provided as part of the child-friendly
explanation of severe within the current lesson. Therefore, the words Ms. Thompson targeted for
instruction on her own were words previously targeted for instruction or were related to words
targeted for instruction by the SOLID Start curriculum materials, once again suggesting that
having targeted vocabulary words enhanced Ms. Thompson’s vocabulary talk. In the following
transcript, Ms. Thompson briefly reviews breezy as part of an activity in which students match
112
the clothing items they would need to wear in order to be prepared to play outside to the given
type of weather conditions and season:
Ms. Thompson: what're the weather conditions usually in the fall? [Use Target Word]
Child(ren): light, light wind.
Ms. Thompson: light //
Ms. Thompson: love that! [Praise]
Ms. Thompson: maybe we could say the word (2) breezy? [Prompt, Suggest
Word/Phrase]
teacher pauses for two seconds and waves hand as if it is blowing slightly in a
breeze—seems to be trying to prompt students
Ms. Thompson: maybe breezy. [Use Target Word]
Ms. Thompson: so what would you wear on your body if it's, if it's going to be breezy?
[Use Target Word, Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help
Students Make Personal Connection]
In this example, Ms. Thompson scaffolded students’ word use by prompting students to use the
previously-introduced target word breezy. When students did not take up this prompt, she
suggested breezy as a more precise way to say “light wind.” She then elicited students’ ideas for
examples of what they would need to wear in order to be prepared to play outside if it were a
breezy fall day. If breezy had not been introduced as a target word in a previous lesson within the
unit, it is possible that Ms. Thompson would have accepted “light wind” and continued the
discussion. The fact that breezy had been targeted for instruction previously may be related to
Ms. Thompson’s vocabulary talk within this activity.
Table 5
Words Targeted for Instruction
Type of Target Word
SOLID Start
Weather Forecasting
Teachers Pay
Teachers
Lesson
Force and Motion
Words Targeted by
Curriculum to
Introduce in Lesson
severe (17)
flood (3)
snowstorm (2)
tornado (10)
hurricane (0)
Lesson
friction (16)
113
Mystery Science
Plant and Animal
Secrets Lesson
(no words identified)
Table 5 (cont’d)
Words Targeted by
Curriculum to
Review/Reinforce in
Lesson
Words Targeted by
Teacher for
Introduction or
Review/
Reinforcement
weather conditions
(2)
rain (19)
snow (8)
wind (58)
fall (5)
winter (3)
spring (1)
summer (1)
cool (0)
cold (2)
warm (7)
hot (0)
prepare (18)
measure (0)
meteorologist (0)
precipitation (4)
thunderstorm (0)
(wind) flag (14)
breezy (7)
calm (0)
extreme (12)
stratus (3)
storm clouds (1)
cumulonimbus (1)
(no words identified)
(no words identified)
(not) living (33)
photosynthesis (0)
insect (1)
unique (3)
safe(ty) (20)
protect (10)
shelter (6)
food (11)
sun (11)
near (2)
qualities (1)
traits (1)
perch (1)
grasslands (1)
specific (1)
move(ment) (30)
gears (2)
*push (33)
pull (11)
force (7)
spin (4)
machines (1)
stop-motion movie
(2)
sequence (1)
forward (9)
backward (4)
side-to-side (1)
curve (3)
demolition derby (2)
guttering (2)
two-finger push (32)
predict(ion) (8)
tread (7)
grip (1)
aerodynamic (1)
Note. This table lists target words and the number of times the teacher used each target word
within the lesson (in parentheses), excluding instances in which target words were read aloud
114
from text. Derivations of target words are included in the count (e.g., moving as form of
move/movement).
*This does not include push when used as part of the targeted term two-finger push.
In contrast, only one word is seemingly targeted for instruction in the Teachers Pay
Teachers force and motion lesson, although this is done so implicitly. The activity description
itself does not include an introduction of the word friction; however, one of the student sheets
asks Why does the car stop moving? and the answer key identifies friction as the correct
response. Further, the bottom of this student sheets states, “Things rubbing together create a
force called friction. The car would keep moving if it were on a sheet of ice; there would be
nothing to slow it down. The friction of the dirt against the wheels slow down the car.” The
following transcript shows Ms. Thompson introducing the term friction during the activity:
Ms. Thompson: the bumps that are on the road, in real life, on our track, with the sand, on
the carpet because the carpet is bumpy, cause a force that is called friction.
[Introduce Word, Use Target Word, Emphasize, Provide
Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “friction”
Ms. Thompson: who has heard of that word before? [Heard of Word]
teacher makes the sign for making a connection/agreeing and some students copy
Ms. Thompson: friction is the force that slows things down. [Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Explain/Define]
teacher emphasizes “friction” and “force” and “slows things down”
Ms. Thompson: slows things down. [Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: if we rolled our matchbox cars across a table top, would they go smooth
and fast or slow down?
Child(ren): smooth and fast.
Ms. Thompson: what if we took (th)em out to the playground on that big piece of ice that
used to be out there.
Ms. Thompson: would they slide smooth and fast or slow down?
Ms. Thompson: smooth and fast.
Ms. Thompson: because ice is so slippery smooth on top.
teacher spreads her hands out smoothly, as if on top of a surface of ice.
Ms. Thompson: what if we took one of our cars to the beach or the sandbox?
115
Ms. Thompson: how would they go there?
Child(ren): slow.
Ms. Thompson: I don't even think they'd move, would they? [Use Target Word]
Ms. Thompson: would they even move? [Use Target Word]
Child(ren): a little bit.
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Thompson: maybe.
Ms. Thompson: maybe hardly anything at all.
Child(ren): just like that much.
Ms. Thompson: Ben.
Ms. Thompson: hold your truck up, Ben, so we can look at it.
Ms. Thompson: Ben's treads also cause friction. [Use Target Word, Provide
Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: but on that kind of truck, you want the friction because you want it to
grab the ground. [Use Target Word]
Ms. Thompson: does that make sense?
Ms. Thompson: the kind of cars that have a tread like that, like tanks in the army that
have to travel on dirt or sand. [Use Target Word, Provide
Example/Nonexample]
Ms. Thompson: cars with little wheels would never work in the desert, would they?
Ms. Thompson: but cars with treads like Ben's would work great in the desert.
Ms. Thompson: Ben, bring me your car.
teacher is asking a student to give up his car because he is not using it
appropriately.
Ms. Thompson: they would work great in the desert because they work with the friction.
[Use Target Word]
Ms. Thompson: they work with the friction. [Use Target Word]
In this example, Mrs. Thompson introduced the term friction, providing examples and an
explanation. She asked if students were familiar with the word and helped them make personal
connections to it. She also used the word repeatedly, providing multiple exposures and drawing
students’ attention to it by emphasizing it at times. It is likely that Ms. Thompson’s introduction
of the term friction was influenced by it being the correct response, according to the answer key,
to the student sheet accompanying the activity. Therefore, the inclusion of friction as a term
116
students should know by the time they complete the activity sheet seems to be related Ms.
Thompson’s enhanced vocabulary talk in connection to this term.
While Ms. Thompson, on her own, targeted an additional 20 words for instruction (see
Table 5) by bringing students’ awareness to them and their meanings, she engaged in the least
vocabulary talk overall during this lesson (see Figure 12) compared to the other two video
recorded lessons. With the exception of terms like move(ment), push, pull, and two-finger push,
which were used a great number of times throughout the lesson, most of the terms Ms.
Thompson introduced were briefly explained but not used throughout the lesson. For example,
Ms. Thompson demonstrated the meaning of the term grip by making gripping motion with her
hands while saying the word but did not use the term or explain its meaning further. Similarly, in
the following example, Ms. Thompson briefly introduces the term guttering when setting up the
track students would push their car down during the activity:
Ms. Thompson: your car is going to travel down this track of this white stuff.
teacher is laying out a track made out of gutters in the center of a circle of
students and continues to lay it out as she talks
Ms. Thompson: this is the kind of stuff that catches rain on the edge of your house.
[Explain/Define, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: do you know what I'm talking about?
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Thompson: it's called guttering. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word, Provide
Visual Support]
Ms. Thompson: say that word. [Ask to Repeat]
Child(ren): guttering.
Ms. Thompson: guttering. [Use Target Word]
Ms. Thompson: yep.
Ms. Thompson: it catches the rain and makes the rain flow off your roof.
[Explain/Define, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: but we are using it for science.
In this example, Ms. Thompson introduced the term guttering, used it repeatedly, provided visual
support, and explained/defined what it is and what it does. She helped students make personal
117
connections and asked students to repeat the word. However, after this brief episode, the term is
not used again and does not seem essential to the activity. Likewise, many of the terms Ms.
Thompson targeted for instruction on her own during this lesson were introduced in passing in
this way, including machine, sequence, demolition derby, and side-to-side. Therefore, while Ms.
Thompson brought students’ awareness to these terms and their meanings (i.e., making them
target words), she did not engage in the kind of sustained vocabulary talk that often accompanied
the terms that (a) were identified for instruction, (b) had child-friendly definitions, and (c) had
marked places in the lesson for introduction and/or review by the SOLID Start materials.
The Mystery Science Plant and Animal Secrets lesson did not identify any words to be
introduced or reviewed/reinforced. While this does not mean that Ms. Thompson could not
engage in vocabulary talk, the data shows that she engaged in considerably less vocabulary talk
during this lesson than during the SOLID Start lesson (see Figure 12). The vocabulary talk that
Ms. Thompson did engage in during the Mystery Science lesson was all based on words that she
herself targeted for instruction by bringing students’ awareness to them and their meanings, such
as (not) living, safety, and protect. Many of these terms were briefly explained in passing,
without in-depth discussion of their meaning. For example, Ms. Thompson briefly reviews the
meaning of the word photosynthesis when a student offers it a response to a question regarding
what all living things need:
Child(ren): photosynthesis.
Ms. Thompson: what is that? [Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word]
Ms. Thompson: tell me more about that big kid word. [Elicit Student Ideas: Review
Word]
Ms. Thompson: do you remember what it means? [Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word]
Ms. Thompson: good job remembering that long word. [Praise]
Ms. Thompson: can someone //
Ms. Thompson: do you want someone to help you out, David?
Ms. Thompson: remembering what it means?
Ms. Thompson: you remembered the word. [Acknowledge Student Ownership]
118
Ms. Thompson: let's see if somebody else remembers what it means.
Ms. Thompson: Eleanora, do you remember what it means? [Acknowledge Student
Ownership]
Child(ren): that plants get food from the sun.
Ms. Thompson: plants make their own food. [Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: but they need the sun to make their own food. [Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: great connection.
Ms. Thompson: way to work together friends.
Ms. Thompson: okay today, we're going to do a new lesson in Mystery Science where we
talk about living things and something new about them. [Use Target Word,
Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “living things”
In this example, Ms. Thompson briefly supported students in connecting the term photosynthesis
to the idea that plants make food with the help of the sun. This discussion was over rather
quickly, likely because the word photosynthesis was only loosely connected to the topic of the
lesson. Notably, Ms. Thompson did not use the term photosynthesis herself or engage students in
the discussion beyond quickly reviewing its meaning. In another example from this lesson, Ms.
Thompson briefly reviews the word perch while holding up covers of books the class had
previously read and discussing where the animal on the cover finds its home:
Ms. Thompson: does anyone remember that special word they called for the stick?
[Prompt Word]
Ms. Thompson: I'll give you a hint.
Ms. Thompson: it starts with /p/. [Prompt Word]
Ms. Thompson: what was it Elizabeth? [Prompt Word]
Child(ren): perch.
Ms. Thompson: perch. [Review Word, Use Target Word]
In this example, Ms. Thompson prompted students to use the word perch one time. It is evident
that this word was previously introduced—most likely when the text was originally read. This
review again happened quickly and the word was only loosely connected to greater lesson, as
was the case with the words insect, unique, near, qualities, traits, grasslands, and specific.
The words Ms. Thompson targeted for instruction that received more attention during the
Mystery Science lesson (i.e., (not) living, protect, safe(ty), shelter, food, sun) were all more
119
closely connected to the lesson topic: Where do animals live? Ms. Thompson engaged in more
vocabulary talk surrounding these terms. In the following example, Ms. Thompson reviews the
meaning of the word safety:
Ms. Thompson: what does it mean? [Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word]
Ms. Thompson: what does safety mean? [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Elicit Student
Ideas: Review Word]
teacher emphasizes the word “safety”
Ms. Thompson: it's hard to put in words.
Ms. Thompson: like we know in our mind what it means.
Ms. Thompson: but if you put it in words, what would you say? [Elicit Student Ideas:
Review Word]
Ms. Thompson: safety means... [Use Target Word, Elicit Student Ideas: Review
Word]
Ms. Thompson: let's see, um, Kaitlin.
Child(ren): um /
Ms. Thompson: it means... [Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word]
Child(ren): that um, like, um, if, um, like, if you're home and your mom was in the
shower, and then, and then you /
Ms. Thompson: but what does safety mean? [Use Target Word, Elicit Student Ideas:
Review Word]
Child(ren): that you gotta, um, like, go somewhere safety.
Ms. Thompson: okay.
Ms. Thompson: wanna add to that, Charlie?
Child(ren): um, it's about, um, the, the spiders in /
Ms. Thompson: okay but we're talking about safety right now. [Use Target Word,
Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “safety”
Ms. Thompson: thank you.
Ms. Thompson: how would you describe what safety means? [Use Target Word, Elicit
Student Ideas: Review Word]
Ms. Thompson: Maddie, what do you think? [Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word]
Child(ren): xxx a place to stay xxx so you can feel like xxx.
Ms. Thompson: it's a place where you can stay where you feel what? [Elicit Student
Ideas: Review Word]
Child(ren): safe.
Ms. Thompson: safe. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes the word “safe”
Ms. Thompson: another word? [Elicit Student Ideas: Review Word]
Ms. Thompson: comfy. [Explain/Define]
Child(ren): live.
Ms. Thompson: live. [Use Target Word]
Child(ren): cozy.
Ms. Thompson: cozy. [Explain/Define]
120
Ms. Thompson: how about protected? [Suggest Word/Phrase, Review Word, Use
Target Word, Emphasize, Explain/Define]
teacher emphasizes the word “protected”
Child(ren): yes.
In this example, Ms. Thompson elicited students’ ideas about the meaning of the word safety,
which was difficult for them to explain. After students eventually offered comfy and cozy as
explanations, Ms. Thompson suggested the word protected. Ms. Thompson commented that
safety is a hard word to explain, which points to the usefulness of child-friendly, curriculum-
provided explanations. Because no words were targeted for instruction within this Mystery
Science lesson, Ms. Thompson did not have access to a child-friendly definition from the
curriculum.
Although Ms. Thompson engaged in some vocabulary talk with a few terms she targeted
for instruction on her own, this is considerably less vocabulary talk than she engaged in when
targeted words were identified in the curriculum. This suggests a relationship such that using
curriculum materials that target specific vocabulary words to be introduced and/or
reviewed/reinforced is associated with Ms. Thompson’s enhanced vocabulary talk, while not
using curricular materials that identify words to target for instruction is associated with
considerably less vocabulary talk.
Texts and Supports for Extra-Textual Talk
Text selection and prompts provided to support extra-textual talk (i.e., the talk that
happens around the text) (Biemiller & Boote, 2006; National Institute of Child Health and
Human Development, 2000; Wright, 2018) also seemed to be related to enhanced vocabulary
talk for Ms. Thompson. Children are exposed to words through text and through talk; therefore,
it makes sense that the texts and supports for talk around these texts would be related to
enhanced vocabulary talk. The SOLID Start lesson identifies two excerpts from trade books to be
121
read, which highlight specific vocabulary terms that have been targeted for instruction within the
lesson. For example, the two-page excerpt from What Is Precipitation? (Johnson, 2013) is used
to introduce the terms flood and snowstorm. The text provides both written explanations of these
terms and photographs of a flood and snowstorm. Additionally, the SOLID Start Weather
Forecasting lesson supports extra-textual talk with the following prompts:
• Page 14: A flood is when too much rain falls in a short time and the water does not soak
into the ground. Have the students repeat the word. Show them the picture of the flood in
the book. Could you play outside in a flood?
• Page 15: A snowstorm is when a lot of snow falls in a short time. Have students repeat
the word. Do snowstorms ever happen where we live?
These supports for extra-textual talk promote vocabulary talk by encouraging teachers to use
specific moves. On each of these pages, the teacher is encouraged to use the target word, provide
an explanation/definition, ask students to repeat the word, and help students make a personal
connection. The prompt for page 14 also encourages the teacher to draw the students’ attention to
the photograph of a flood, thereby providing visual support for the term. The following transcript
shows how Ms. Thompson engaged in vocabulary talk while reading this excerpt:
Ms. Thompson: look at this severe weather. [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Provide
Visual Support]
teacher is pointing to a picture in a book that she is projecting up on the
SMARTBoard and emphasizes “severe” as she says it
Ms. Thompson (reading text): sometimes a lot of precipitation falls in a very short period
of time.
Ms. Thompson (reading text): when too much rain falls, there may be floods.
Ms. Thompson: a flood /
Child(ren): what's a flood?
Ms. Thompson: a flood is when too much rain falls and there's kind of nowhere for it to
go. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word, Explain/Define, Provide Visual
Support, Answer a Question]
teacher is gesturing to a picture of a flood projected on the SMARTBoard
Ms. Thompson: that's extreme weather. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
122
teacher emphasizes “extreme”
Ms. Thompson: do you agree?
Child(ren): mm-hmm.
Ms. Thompson (reading text): floods can happen when rivers or lakes get too full and the
water flows out over the land that is usually dry.
Ms. Thompson (reading text): floods can happen very quickly so it is best to stay away
from rivers or lakes when it's raining very hard.
Child(ren): or if there’s sharks or alligators in it.
Ms. Thompson: this was us a couple days ago. [Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
teacher is gesturing to a picture with a lot of snow
Child(ren): oh my god!
Ms. Thompson (reading text): when a lot of snow falls in a short time, it is called a
snowstorm.
Ms. Thompson (reading text): snowstorms can bury roads and sidewalks and cars under
thick layers of snow.
Ms. Thompson (reading text): the snow makes it hard for people to get from place to
place.
Ms. Thompson (reading text): heavy snow can also damage buildings and trees.
Ms. Thompson: this is what it looked like outside on Monday. [Help Students Make
Personal Connection, Provide Visual Support]
teacher is gesturing to the same picture, where snow covers cars, trees, and
houses
Ms. Thompson: this is why we didn't have school on Monday, because there was just too
much snow on the roads. [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren):
Ms. Thompson: too much. [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: there was a snowstorm. [Introduce Word, Use Target Word,
Emphasize, Help Students Make Personal Connection]
teacher emphasizes “snowstorm”
Ms. Thompson: what was there? [Ask to Repeat]
Child(ren): snowstorm.
Ms. Thompson: too much snow, snowstorm. [Use Target Word, Emphasize,
Explain/Define]
teacher emphasizes “snowstorm”
Child(ren):
Child(ren):
Ms. Thompson: we did. [Help Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: too much rain, what's that called? [Prompt Word]
teacher is gesturing to the same picture of the flood used before
Child(ren): floods.
Ms. Thompson: flood. [Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “flood”
123
This transcript shows that Ms. Thompson engaged in vocabulary talk around this text by
explaining the target words flood and snowstorm, prompting students to say these words aloud,
drawing attention to the visual supports provided by the photographs depicting the meaning of
the words, and helping students make personal connections based on their shared recent
experiences with snowstorms. She also drew attention to flood and snowstorm saying them with
emphasis. In other words, Ms. Thompson took up and built upon the supports for extra-textual
talk provided within the lesson. This suggests that the use of texts to introduce and/or reinforce
targeted vocabulary and the provision of supports for extra-textual talk are related to enhanced
vocabulary talk.
In contrast, the Teachers Pay Teachers activity does not include any text, and the Mystery
Science lesson uses a digital text (Who Lives There? by Pat Murphy) that appears to have been
created for the lesson. Although the Mystery Science lesson overview indicates that the focus of
the lesson is on the animal need of shelter and the “Activity Prep” description indicates students
will learn about squirrels’ habitats, the terms shelter and habitat are not included in the text.
Similarly, no science vocabulary words are identified to be introduced alongside the text, as no
words are targeted for instruction within the lesson. Further, the supports for extra-textual talk do
not promote vocabulary talk. The lesson identifies just one point in the read-aloud to pause for
discussion, with the prompts: Which animal do you think lives up there? Why do you think that?
The adjacent page shows a photograph of a nest high up in a tree. Below the nest are
photographs of a squirrel, deer, and turtle, with arrows pointing from each animal to the nest.
Above the nest is a question mark. The following transcript shows the discussion of the given
prompts:
Ms. Thompson (reading text): which animal //
Ms. Thompson: look at your choices: squirrel, deer, or turtle.
124
Ms. Thompson (reading text): which animal lives up there?
Child(ren): squirrel.
Child(ren): well deer can eat acorns too.
Ms. Thompson: deer can eat acorns.
Child(ren): but it's like, but it's probably squirrel (be)cause it's a nest.
Ms. Thompson: okay.
many students speak at once
Ms. Thompson: okay, hold on a second.
Ms. Thompson: let's take a look //
Ms. Thompson: let's think about what Lydia just said.
Ms. Thompson: she said deer like to eat acorns.
Ms. Thompson: but it's a nest.
Ms. Thompson: and it's up high.
Ms. Thompson: so why would that not work for a deer?
Child(ren): because a deer would have to have suction cups to climb up but it's fantasy /
Ms. Thompson: but we're not talking fantasy.
Ms. Thompson: today we're talking realism.
Ms. Thompson: today we're talking realism.
Ms. Thompson: so can deer climb a tree, Name?
Ms. Thompson: no.
Ms. Thompson: no.
Ms. Thompson: anybody have something else they want to share?
Ms. Thompson: how //
Ms. Thompson: Name, what do you think?
Child(ren): a squirrel can climb trees because if a deer would climb a tree it would go
/woo/ and it would fall, it would keep falling down.
student makes falling gesture with hand while making /woo/ sound effect
Ms. Thompson: it doesn't have any tools on its body to help it climb a tree, does it?
Child(ren): only a squirrel does.
Ms. Thompson: only a squirrel does.
Ms. Thompson: of these three choices.
Ms. Thompson: of these three choices.
Ms. Thompson: yes, Name?
Child(ren): a turtle can climb a tree but they don’t xxx really hard for them to climb trees
but turtles could climb—I bet turtles could climb xxx than a deer.
Ms. Thompson: you think a turtle could do a better job than a deer.
Ms. Thompson: but I have never //
Ms. Thompson: have you ever seen a turtle climb a tree?
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Thompson: I don't know if their legs, um //
Ms. Thompson: look how teeny and short their legs are.
Ms. Thompson: I'm not sure those are good climbing legs.
Child(ren): they could stick their claws into each one.
Ms. Thompson: maybe.
Ms. Thompson: maybe.
Child(ren): it would be so slow though.
125
Ms. Thompson: do you think their shell is heavy?
Child(ren): yes.
Ms. Thompson: that might make it hard for them to climb.
Ms. Thompson: do you think a squirrel's fur is heavy?
Child(ren): no.
Ms. Thompson: no, I don't think so either.
Ms. Thompson: so maybe it makes it better for them to, to climb a tree.
Ms. Thompson: let's keep going.
Ms. Thompson (reading text): I think I'll watch that clump of leaves for a while.
This transcript shows that the discussion prompts provided to support extra-textual talk did not
support vocabulary talk, as Ms. Thompson did not use any vocabulary talk moves during this
discussion. In fact, throughout the entire interactive read-aloud, Ms. Thompson used only one
vocabulary talk move in one utterance. This finding suggests that the absence of (a) texts that
introduce or review science vocabulary and (b) curricular supports for extra-textual talk that
encourage teachers to use vocabulary talk moves is related to considerably less vocabulary talk
than when these features are present.
Discussion Prompts
Discussion prompts that include words targeted for instruction and/or engage students in
active processing also seem to be associated with enhanced vocabulary talk for Ms. Thompson.
The SOLID Start lesson provides 36 prompts for teachers to draw from when facilitating
discussions throughout the lesson. Most of these prompts include a word that has been targeted
for instruction (e.g., What are some examples of severe weather you are familiar with?) and/or
engage students in actively processing a word’s meaning and developing understanding of its
underlying concept (e.g., In what types of precipitation is it safe to play outside? to contrast the
idea of severe weather). These discussion prompts provide opportunities for teachers to engage
in vocabulary talk. For example, the SOLID Start lesson encourages teachers to facilitate a
discussion in which students consider what they need to be prepared for when there is severe
126
weather and precipitation. As part of this discussion, Ms. Thompson asks a variation of the
sample discussion prompt that was provided in the lesson (i.e., What do we need to think about if
there is a tornado?):
Ms. Thompson: and what do we do at school to prepare, or get ready, in case there's a
tornado? [Use Target Word, Emphasize, Use with Hint, Visually Display
Word, Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make
Personal Connection]
teacher holds up the whiteboard with “prepare” written on it; teacher emphasizes
“prepare”
Ms. Thompson: what do we do? [Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help
Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): stay inside.
Ms. Thompson: we stay inside. [Provide Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make
Personal Connection]
Child(ren): xxx
Ms. Thompson: get //
Ms. Thompson: well we don't get groceries at school. [Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
Ms. Thompson: but what do we do when have our tornado drill? [Use Target Word,
Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help Students Make Personal
Connection]
Child(ren): have a...
Ms. Thompson: where do we go? [Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help
Students Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: what do we do? [Elicit Student Ideas: Example/Nonexample, Help
Students Make Personal Connection]
Child(ren): a strong, a strong building.
students are talking over themselves and the teacher as she asks her questions
Ms. Thompson: we're in a strong building. [Provide Example/Nonexample, Help
Student Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: we go out to the hallway. [Provide Example/Nonexample, Help
Student Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: we kneel down on our knees. [Provide Example/Nonexample, Help
Student Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: show me what we look like. [Help Student Make Personal
Connection]
students kneel down in tornado position
Ms. Thompson: kneel down on your knees. [Help Student Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: tuck your head down. [Help Student Make Personal Connection]
Ms. Thompson: and put your hands over the top of your head. [Help Student Make
Personal Connection]
127
In this example, Ms. Thompson reviewed the meaning of the word prepare (a target word) and
elicited examples from students of ways they prepare for tornadoes (another target word). She
helped students make personal connections to each of these target words by asking them to
consider their own experiences with preparing for tornadoes at school through participating in
tornado drills. This example shows the relationship between discussion prompts that include
target words and aim to develop students’ understanding of their underlying meanings and Ms.
Thompson’s enhanced vocabulary talk.
In contrast, the Teachers Pay Teachers activity provides no discussion prompts, and the
Mystery Science lesson provides only five discussion prompts, with two of them appearing
within the read-aloud text (as previously described). The remaining three discussion prompts are
found within the optional lesson extension, which involves watching a one-minute video and
discussing the given prompts. These prompts are not related to any science vocabulary terms (as
there are no words targeted for instruction within this lesson):
• What animals did you see in the video?
• Where do the animals live? How do you know?
•
If the animals could talk, what would you ask them?
Ms. Thompson showed the video provided within the Mystery Science lesson and another video
not listed within the curriculum. The following transcript shows part of the discussion Ms.
Thompson’s class had about what animals they saw in the two videos and where they live:
Ms. Thompson: okay so what I wanted to talk about are the things that we saw in that
video.
Ms. Thompson: give me an example of one animal you saw and where it made its unique
// [Review Word, Use Target Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “unique”
Ms. Thompson: what does unique mean? [Use Target Word, Elicit Student Ideas:
Review Word]
CHI: special.
128
Ms. Thompson: special. [Explain/Define]
CHI: in their own way.
Ms. Thompson: in their own way. [Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: different. [Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: special to just one thing. [Explain/Define]
Ms. Thompson: good.
Ms. Thompson: okay, raise your hand if you can think of an example of something we
saw //
Ms. Thompson: thank you.
teacher is speaking to students who have already raised their hands
Ms. Thompson: something we saw and where it had its unique home. [Use Target
Word, Emphasize]
teacher emphasizes “unique”
Ms. Thompson: Micah, give me one example.
Ms. Thompson: I saw...
Child(ren): the, the turtle in the fire desert.
Ms. Thompson: where does it make its home?
Ms. Thompson: there was a turtle.
Ms. Thompson: and it's in a place that catches fire.
Ms. Thompson: so where does it make its home?
Child(ren): in the ground.
Ms. Thompson: why?
Child(ren): because the fire burns xxx place /
Ms. Thompson: it's a protected place.
Child(ren): and because um, because um, fire can't get underneath ground.
Ms. Thompson: great.
In this example, Ms. Thompson engaged in some vocabulary talk around the word unique, which
she reviewed here even though it was not identified within the Mystery Science lesson for
introduction or review and reinforcement. Ms. Thompson does use some vocabulary talk moves
when reviewing the meaning of unique; however, little vocabulary talk follows in the duration of
this discussion. This suggests that discussion prompts that are not related to targeted vocabulary
words are associated with less vocabulary talk.
Summary of Findings
This analysis shows an association between enhanced vocabulary talk by Ms. Thompson
and using science curriculum materials that (a) identified target words and provided child-
friendly explanations/definitions, (b) selected texts highlighting these target words and provided
129
supports for extra-textual talk promoting vocabulary talk, and (c) offered discussion prompts for
deepening students’ understandings of target word meanings. Likewise, this analysis showed that
the absence of these curricular features is associated with less vocabulary talk. This study
suggests that while science instruction can offer a rich context for oral language and vocabulary
development (e.g., Gotwals & Wright, 2017; Wright & Gotwals, 2017; Wright & Neuman,
2014), without high-quality curricular materials with features that enhance teachers’ vocabulary
talk, the potential of this context for promoting oral language and vocabulary development may
not be realized.
130
CHAPTER 6—DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS
Discussion
The purpose of this dissertation study was to examine the ways in which teachers use
language to promote oral language and vocabulary development during science instruction in the
early-elementary grades. This is critical because, while oral language and vocabulary
development support students’ literacy and science learning (e.g., Anderson & Freebody, 1981;
Guthrie et al., 1999; Mercer et al., 2004; Richmond & Striley, 1996; Senechal et al., 2006; Wang
& Herman, 2006; Wright & Gotwals, 2017) and national standards place emphasis on oral
language and vocabulary development (National Governors Association Center for Best
Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010; NGSS Lead States, 2013), little is
known about how teachers promote oral language and vocabulary development during science
instruction in the early-elementary grades. It is important to understand what opportunities
students have to develop oral language and also access science content, as both support literacy
and science learning. Therefore, I conducted two instrumental case studies (Barone, 2011; Stake,
2000) to examine teachers’ vocabulary talk during science instruction in early-elementary
classrooms.
In the first study, I examined the science instruction of a cohort of eight early-elementary
teachers in order to determine (1) how they used language to promote students’ oral language
and vocabulary development and (2) whether vocabulary talk is related to the language aspects
of science talk, and if so, how. I used discourse analysis techniques to analyze 24 video recorded
science lessons (894.27 minutes of observational data) across three timepoints from the eight
participating teachers. I then used quantitative techniques to make within- and between-teacher
comparisons of vocabulary talk over the course of the study. I found that the cohort of teachers
131
used all four types of vocabulary talk (i.e., Knowledge & Understanding, Affective Factors,
Student Word Use, and Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness) during science instruction;
however, the cohort as a whole engaged in considerably more Knowledge & Understanding
vocabulary talk (i.e., 86.72%) than any other type of vocabulary talk. This was also reflected in
the vocabulary talk of individual teachers, with a few exceptions when teachers did not engage in
any vocabulary talk or made only one vocabulary talk move during a lesson. Between-teacher
comparisons show no consistent patterns in teachers’ vocabulary talk over time. In examining the
relationship between vocabulary talk and the language aspects of science talk, I found that there
appears to be a relationship between Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk and science
talk focused on building knowledge of science oral language (as measured by Domain 3,
Component 1 of the SOLID Start Tool); however, further research is needed to examine whether
there is a relationship between Student Word Use vocabulary talk and science talk focused on
scaffolding students’ word use language (as measured by Domain 3, Component 2 of the SOLID
Start Tool). Further, my analysis points to the need to consider the context in which vocabulary
talk moves are made and the overall quality of this vocabulary talk in addition to examining
which moves the teachers make and how frequently they make them.
In the second study, I examined the science instruction of Ms. Thompson, a kindergarten
teacher, in order to identify what features of science curriculum materials are related to enhanced
vocabulary talk. I used qualitative analysis techniques to analyze three video recorded science
lessons (128.03 minutes of observational data) alongside the curricular materials from which Ms.
Thompson drew for these lessons. I found that using science curriculum materials that (a)
identified target words and provided child-friendly explanations/definitions, (b) used texts that
highlight these target words and provided supports for extra-textual talk promoting vocabulary
132
talk, and (c) offered discussion prompts that deepen students’ understandings of target word
meanings was associated with enhanced vocabulary talk by Ms. Thompson. Likewise, I found
that the absence of these curricular features was associated with less vocabulary talk.
In the following sections, I discuss these findings in relation to the empirical and
theoretical literature on how children learn words.
Types of Vocabulary Talk
The cohort of early-elementary teachers in this study engaged in considerably more
Knowledge & Understanding talk than any other type of vocabulary talk. This finding reflects
the focus of the research literature on how children learn words, as far more studies have
explored instructional practices for building students’ knowledge of word meanings than
scaffolding students’ word use, building students’ awareness of words and word learning, or
interesting students in words and word learning. However, most of the studies that comprise the
research base on how children learn words examined vocabulary instruction within the context of
literacy instruction (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al.,
2007, 2009, 2010; Hadley et al., 2019; McKeown & Beck, 2003; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b;
Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman,
2014). Importantly, the present study shows that this cohort of early-elementary teachers used
language in many ways that are aligned with the research on building students’ knowledge of
word meanings during science instruction. This is promising for both literacy and science
learning in the early-elementary grades because this suggests that there are important
opportunities for teachers to build students’ vocabulary knowledge across content areas. Given
the sheer number of vocabulary words there are to learn (Anderson & Nagy, 1992; Nagy &
133
Anderson, 1984) and the need to learn discipline-specific vocabulary (Bravo & Cervetti, 2008), it
is important to seize the opportunities for vocabulary learning across the elementary school day.
In addition to having the greatest percentage of the cohort’s vocabulary talk, Knowledge
& Understanding was also the type of talk with the most individual vocabulary talk moves (i.e.,
number of unique second-level codes). I identified 21 different vocabulary talk moves that align
with the research on building students’ knowledge of word meanings, compared to only six
moves for interesting students in words and word learning (i.e., Affective Factors), six moves for
scaffolding students’ word use (i.e., Student Word Use), and one move for building awareness of
words and word learning (i.e., Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness). This may be because
the research literature has focused primarily on building students’ knowledge of word meanings;
therefore, much is known about how to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development in this way.
Notably, the type of vocabulary talk used second most frequently after Knowledge &
Understanding was Affective Factors. This is somewhat surprising, as this type of vocabulary
talk is the least reflective of the research base. This type of vocabulary talk is based in the theory
of word consciousness (e.g., Anderson & Nagy, 1991, 1992; Graves, 2000, 2006; Graves &
Watts-Taffe, 2002, 2008; Nagy & Scott, 2000; Scott & Nagy, 2004), and to my knowledge, only
one study has examined the specific impact of word consciousness instruction on students’
vocabulary development. Neugebauer et al. (2017) investigated the impact of teachers’ word
consciousness talk on kindergarten students’ general vocabulary knowledge. They identified
three specific talk moves—reinforcing students’ word use, affirming students’ recognition of
word meanings, and helping students make personal connects to words—and found that these
moves were positively associated with gains in students’ general vocabulary knowledge at the
134
end of the school year. In the present study, the cohort of teachers used two of these moves—
reinforcing students’ word use (i.e., PRAISE) and helping students make personal connections to
words (i.e., HELP STUDENTS MAKE PERSONAL CONNECTIONS). They also used four
additional Affective Factors moves that align with the research on interesting students in word
and word learning. When students showed curiosity about a word’s meaning, the teachers
answered their questions. At times, the teachers celebrated students’ word use by acknowledging
their “ownership” of words. Infrequently, teachers also modeled appreciating words/word choice
and enjoying favorite words. While it is promising that the cohort of teachers engaged in this
kind of Affective Factors talk, across the data set, this talk accounted for only 7.85% of total
vocabulary talk code applications. This suggests that interesting students in words and word
learning was not a primary focus of the teachers’ vocabulary instruction during science lessons.
In order to provide more empirical support for this primarily theoretical type of vocabulary talk,
further research is needed to examine the relationship between Affective Factors vocabulary talk
moves and students’ oral language and vocabulary development.
Another type of vocabulary talk with relatively few code applications (i.e., 5.23% of total
vocabulary talk code applications) was Student Word Use. This may be reflective of the common
finding in observational studies that teachers do most of the talking within a classroom (e.g.,
Nystrand, 2006; Silverman et al., 2014). In an observational study of vocabulary instruction at
the elementary level, Silverman et al. (2014) found that 75% of utterances across all lessons were
teacher utterances, and these utterances were more than four times as long as student utterances,
on average. The present study captured what students said in order to provide context for teacher
utterances but did not record student speech at the level of the utterance; therefore, it is not
possible to determine the percentage of utterances that were teacher talk compared to student
135
talk. However, it was evident that the cohort of teachers did more talking than their students.
With limited opportunities for students to talk, there were also limited opportunities to use target
vocabulary. The finding that teachers used little Student Word Use talk is consistent with the
overall trend for Domain 3 Component 2 scores on the SOLID Start Tool (i.e., measuring the
language aspects of science talk focused on scaffolding students’ word use). Of the 24 lessons
observed in the present study, only 10 had composite scores at or above 3.0 for this component,
meaning that 14 lessons scored below Medium for scaffolding students’ use of previously-
introduced discipline-specific oral language (Wright et al., 2017). Together, these low scores for
the language aspect of science talk focused on scaffolding students’ word use and the lack of
vocabulary talk moves encouraging students to use target words suggests that scaffolding
students’ use of target words was not a focus of the teachers’ vocabulary instruction during
science lessons. Given that studies have found scaffolding students’ word use to promote oral
language and vocabulary development (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Coyne et al., 2007,
2009, 2010; Juel & Deffes, 2004; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985; McKeown & Beck, 2003;
Rosenthal & Ehri, 2011; Silverman, 2007a; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Zipoli et al., 2011), this
lack of scaffolding students’ word use highlights an area for professional development.
Finally, there was only one Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness vocabulary talk
move across the entire data set—asking students if they had heard of a word—and it accounted
for only 0.20% of total vocabulary talk code applications. This may be reflective of the research
base, as building students’ awareness of words and word learning is well researched, but much of
this work has focused on upper-elementary and intermediate grades (e.g., Baumann et al., 2002,
2003; P. N. Bowers & Kirby, 2010; Lesaux et al., 2010; Lubliner & Smetana, 2005; Zipke, 2007,
2008; Zipke et al., 2009). Only a few of the studies examining instruction focused on developing
136
metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness have taken place within the context of early-
elementary classrooms (i.e., K-2) (e.g., Cain, 2007; Freeman et al., 2019; Nash & Snowling,
2006; Wise, 2019). Nash and Snowling (2006) compared instruction in which 7- and 8-year olds
were taught the meanings of target words with instruction in which they were taught to look for
context clues to derive the meaning of target words. They found that three months after the
intervention, students in the context instruction condition showed significantly better expressive
vocabulary, comprehended text containing target words significantly better, and could
independently derive word meanings from written context. This study suggests that building
early-elementary students’ awareness of words and word learning may have a greater impact on
vocabulary development than merely focusing on buildings students’ knowledge of specific
word meaning. Similarly, Wise (2019) examined the effects of an intervention designed to teach
second-grade students to use four types of context clues (i.e. synonyms, pictures, definitions, and
antonyms), finding that students in the intervention condition significantly outperformed students
in the business-as-usual comparison condition with the skill of noticing unfamiliar words. As
Wise argued, this is an important step for supporting students in becoming independent word
learners because it may support their incidental word learning from oral and written contexts.
Relatedly, Cain (2007) examined whether explanation facilitated contextual analysis with 7- and
8-year-old students, finding that students who explained how a word was derived from context
made the greatest gains in definition accuracy. Again, accurately deriving word meanings from
context is important for supporting students in becoming independent word learners. Focusing
instead on morphological instruction, Freeman et al. (2019) explored how three first-grade
students responded to a small-group intervention with explicit instruction on Greek and Latin
roots that was integrated with science vocabulary and texts. Freeman and colleagues found that
137
this instruction increased students’ morphological awareness, as they were able to find roots in
words and were developing the ability to analyze word features and look for clues to their
meaning. These too are important skills for becoming independent word learners. Therefore,
while only a few studies have examined instruction focused on developing early-elementary
students’ metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness, their findings point to the need for this
kind of instruction in order to set young students on a path toward becoming independent word
learners.
These studies focused on building students’ metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness
in the early-elementary grades (i.e., Cain, 2007; Freeman et al., 2019; Nash & Snowling, 2006;
Wise, 2019) have been intervention studies that have intentionally focused on this approach to
promoting oral language and vocabulary development. Like the present study, observational
studies of vocabulary instruction have found limited focus on developing students’
metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness. In particular, the lack of vocabulary talk moves in
attending to morphology and syntax in the present study is consistent with other studies that have
found limited instructional attention devoted to these aspects of word learning in the upper-
elementary grades (e.g., Scott et al., 2003; Silverman et al., 2014; Watts, 1995). Likewise, the
lack of vocabulary talk moves addressing contextual analysis is consistent with the lack of
contextual analysis instruction observed by Silverman et al. (2014). Given that studies have
found that metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness instruction promotes oral language and
vocabulary development in the early-elementary grades (e.g., Cain, 2007; Freeman et al., 2019;
Nash & Snowling, 2006; Wise, 2019), the consistent absence of this instruction in elementary
classrooms points to the need for professional development focused on developing students’
138
metalinguistic and metacognitive awareness—including within science instruction in the early-
elementary grades.
Quality of Vocabulary Talk
My analysis of the cohort of teacher’s vocabulary talk in relation to the language aspects
of science talk highlight the need to consider not only the vocabulary talk moves made by
teachers but also the quality of the talk involved in each move. My analysis of vocabulary talk
captured teacher talk that aligned with the research and theory on how children learn words—
such as explaining or defining the meaning of a word. However, this analysis did not evaluate
how well the vocabulary talk moves were executed—such as whether the explanation/definition
used familiar words and concepts (i.e., child-friendly) to help students understand the new word.
My analysis of the language aspects of science talk shows the discrepancy between quantity of
vocabulary talk, as captured by rate of each type of vocabulary talk per minute, and quality of
this talk, as captured by Components 1-2 of Domain 3 of the SOLID Start Tool (i.e., Talk that
Develops Literacy and Language for Science). This distinction between quantity and quality of
vocabulary talk is evident in Ms. Nelson’s first lesson (i.e. PrePD), in which the weather terms
students had learned so far in the SOLID Start Weather Forecasting unit were reviewed, but this
was done as its own activity rather than within the context of science learning. Ms. Nelson
engaged in a great deal of vocabulary talk and prompted her kindergarten students to use science
oral language throughout the lesson. However, because this talk happened outside the context of
science learning and because students were using science oral language to list words rather than
to share their ideas, this lesson scored lower overall for science talk than for vocabulary talk. In
other words, the context in which the vocabulary talk moves were made was not as rich as
described in the research literature and therefore may not have been as impactful.
139
This distinction between quantity and quality of vocabulary talk is important because, for
vocabulary talk moves to effectively promote students’ oral language and vocabulary
development, the quality of talk within a given move likely matters. In their observational study
of vocabulary instruction during language arts instruction in third-, fourth-, and fifth-grade
classrooms, Silverman et al. (2014) surprisingly found that the research-based practice of
applying words in various contexts was negatively associated with change in vocabulary. Further
analysis revealed that instruction that was coded as application across contexts sometimes led
students off track and may have distracted them from the actual meaning of the word, such as
when a teacher applied the target word delivery to the pizza delivery she had the night before and
then the conversation turned to the foods students like to order rather than focusing on the
delivery of those foods. Silverman and colleagues noted that while their study did not investigate
the quality of instruction, future research should take this into consideration. Likewise, the
present study points to the need for future research that takes quality of vocabulary talk moves
into consideration—especially research that investigates the relationship between vocabulary talk
moves and students’ oral language and vocabulary development.
Further, while it has been common practice in vocabulary research to capture counts
related to word learning (e.g., number of words, counts of different types of words, number of
minutes of instruction, frequency of use of various instructional practices) (e.g., Barnes et al.,
2017; Barnes & Dickinson, 2018; Hart & Risley, 2003; Scott et al., 2003; Silverman et al., 2014;
Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wright, 2014), the present study, like Silverman et al. (2014),
shows the need to also take into consideration quality as it relates to these counts. Vocabulary
research focused on the “word gap,” for instance, has focused on comparing counts of how many
words young children from various socioeconomic backgrounds have been exposed to by a
140
particular age (e.g., Hart & Risley, 2003). The present study suggests that such counts may not
capture what is important in terms of promoting oral language and vocabulary development.
Curricular Supports for Vocabulary Talk
My analysis of features of science curriculum materials that are related to enhanced
vocabulary talk was centered on one kindergarten teacher’s science instruction across three
lessons that drew from three different sets of curriculum materials; therefore, my findings cannot
be generalized beyond this particular teacher and these particular lessons from these sets of
curriculum materials. However, this analysis offers insight that may be particularly useful for
developers of science curriculum materials. The identification of specific words to target for
instruction within the lesson was associated with enhanced vocabulary talk; therefore, an
important way curriculum materials may support students’ vocabulary growth is by identifying
words to introduce and review/reinforce within a lesson. Ms. Thompson engaged in far more
vocabulary talk when the curriculum materials she used identified specific vocabulary words to
introduce and review/reinforce, with the given child-friendly definitions, at specified points in
the lesson (i.e., SOLID Start Weather Forecasting lesson). This is important because, while I
coded any word that teachers targeted for instruction (by drawing students’ attention to it and its
meaning) as a target word—and therefore words outside of those identified by the curriculum
materials could be target words—Ms. Thompson engaged in less vocabulary talk overall when it
was up to her to identify words to target for instruction.
Many of the words Ms. Thompson targeted for instruction on her own were introduced
briefly and then not revisited within the lesson, and often, these words were loosely related to the
science content of the lesson (e.g., gears, spin, machines, curve, and grip in the Teachers Pay
Teachers force and motion lesson) but were not central to communicating science
141
understandings from the lesson. This is consistent with Wright and Neuman's (2014) finding in
their observational study of vocabulary instruction across the school day in kindergarten
classrooms that teachers often gave one-time, brief word explanations with unsystematic word
selection. In contrast, the words in the SOLID Start Weather Forecasting lesson that were
targeted for introduction (i.e., severe, flood, snowstorm, tornado, hurricane) and
review/reinforcement (i.e., weather conditions, rain, snow, wind, fall, winter, spring, summer,
cool, cold, warm, hot, prepare, measure, meteorologist, precipitation, thunderstorm) were
central to the lesson question (i.e., How Can We Prepare for Severe Weather Conditions?).
Further, the words identified for instruction in the SOLID Start lesson are aligned with the
research on teaching target words in conceptually- or semantically-related sets in order to
promote students’ knowledge of word meanings (e.g., Cervetti et al., 2016; Hadley et al., 2019;
Neuman et al., 2011; Neuman & Dwyer, 2011; Neuman & Kaefer, 2013, 2018; Pollard-Durodola
et al., 2011; Wasik et al., 2006; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Zipoli et al., 2011). The importance of the
relationship between the target words for promoting vocabulary talk is evident in the transcripts
from the SOLID Start lesson, as the density of vocabulary talk is greater throughout the lesson in
comparison to the Teachers Pay Teachers and Mystery Science lessons with loosely related
words discussed more sporadically here and there.
Two other features of science curriculum materials were associated with enhanced
vocabulary talk by Ms. Thompson, but they were ultimately dependent on the curriculum
materials having identified target words in the first place. The text selection and prompts for
extra-textual talk in the SOLID Start lesson highlighted specific vocabulary terms that had been
targeted for instruction, providing written explanations and photographs for visual support. This
text selection provided a rich context for vocabulary talk, aided by the discussion prompts
142
provided in the lesson to support extra-textual talk. That the read-aloud context enhanced Ms.
Thompson’s vocabulary talk is unsurprising, as many studies of vocabulary instruction have
been situated within read-alouds due to the rich context they provide for word learning (e.g.,
Barnes & Dickinson, 2018; Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et
al., 2007, 2009, 2010; Dickinson & Smith, 1994; Dickinson & Tabors, 1991; Hadley et al., 2019;
McKeown & Beck, 2003; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Wasik et al.,
2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014). In contrast, the Teachers Pay Teachers
lesson did not include text, and the Mystery Science lesson included text but did not use that text
to highlight any science vocabulary—as no words had been targeted for instruction within the
lesson. The opportunity was there for the Mystery Science text to highlight terms such as shelter
and habitat; however, the text selected (i.e., created) for this lesson does not include these terms,
the lesson itself does not target these terms for instruction, and Ms. Thompson did not introduce
these terms on her own. The absence of texts and extra-textual talk highlighting target words was
related to considerably less vocabulary talk by Ms. Thompson.
Similarly, the discussion prompts provided throughout the SOLID Start lesson often
included a target word or engaged students in actively processing a target word’s meaning and
developing understanding of its underlying concept. These prompts provided rich opportunities
for Ms. Thompson to engage in vocabulary talk. This is unsurprising, as using target words and
engaging students in deeper processing of target words is aligned with the research on how
children learn words (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et
al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985; McKeown & Beck, 2003; National Institute
of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Robbins & Ehri, 1994; Silverman, 2007a,
2007b; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik &
143
Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014; Zipoli et al., 2011). In contrast, the Teachers Pay
Teachers lesson did not provide any discussion prompts, and the Mystery Science lesson
provided several prompts that did not use science vocabulary terms. Without having identified
target words within the lesson, these discussion prompts did not provide opportunities for
students to actively process target word meanings or develop understanding of their underlying
concepts. The absence of discussion prompts related to target words was associated with less
vocabulary talk by Ms. Thompson.
Beyond these specific curricular features, this analysis shows that the science curriculum
materials teachers use matter in terms of supporting students’ literacy and science learning.
Science materials such as the SOLID Start curriculum seemingly set the stage for vocabulary talk
and provide a rich context that develops students’ language and literacy while deepening their
science understanding. Such materials support coherence across lessons by identifying target
words to review/reinforce as relevant to the current lesson, which provides multiple exposures to
target words (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2010;
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986) in
multiple contexts (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2010;
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Silverman et al., 2014; Stahl
& Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016), often in conceptually- or semantically-related sets (e.g.,
Cervetti et al., 2016; Hadley et al., 2019; Neuman et al., 2011; Neuman & Dwyer, 2011; Neuman
& Kaefer, 2013, 2018; Pollard-Durodola et al., 2011; Zipoli et al., 2011)—all of which is
supported by the research base on how children learn words. What made Ms. Thompson’s
science instruction particularly interesting for further examination is that she taught three
different lessons from three different sets of curriculum materials. From a curriculum analysis
144
perspective, this was helpful for analyzing curricular features across a variety of science
curriculum materials. From a student learning perspective, however, this was likely not helpful
for supporting either science or literacy learning, as these three lessons addressed different
disciplinary core ideas from different branches of science—all within the span of just 12 weeks
or so (i.e., PrePD videos were recorded before the 10-week SOLID Start usability trial began).
Without following a single curriculum with coherent units of study, the opportunities to support
students’ oral language and vocabulary development across time were limited, as ideas and
concepts that would repeat throughout coherent science materials—and therefore support
vocabulary development—were not present.
Implications
The findings from this study have implications for curriculum development, teacher
professional development, teacher preparation, and policy. In the following sections, I describe
the implications for each area.
Curriculum Development
The present study highlights the need for science curriculum materials to identify words
to target for instruction—both through introduction and review/reinforcement in subsequent
lessons. These words need to be conceptually related (e.g., Cervetti et al., 2016; Hadley et al.,
2019; Neuman et al., 2011; Neuman & Dwyer, 2011; Neuman & Kaefer, 2013, 2018; Pollard-
Durodola et al., 2011; Wasik et al., 2006; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Zipoli et al., 2011) and central to
supporting students’ engagement with science learning (Wright et al., 2017). In addition to
identifying target words, science curriculum materials should also provide child-friendly
definitions or explanations of the meanings of these words (e.g., Beck & McKeown, 2001, 2007;
Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown et al., 1983, 1985;
145
McKeown & Beck, 2003; Nash & Snowling, 2006; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman et al.,
2014; Silverman & Crandell, 2010; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond,
2001; Wasik & Hindman, 2014). Texts that highlight these target words should be carefully
selected and prompts should be given to support extra-textual talk during the reading of these
texts. Finally, curriculum materials should also provide discussion prompts that use target words
and provide opportunities for students to engage in deeper processing of these words (e.g., Beck
& McKeown, 2001, 2007; Biemiller & Boote, 2006; Coyne et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; McKeown
et al., 1983, 1985; McKeown & Beck, 2003; National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development, 2000; Robbins & Ehri, 1994; Silverman, 2007a, 2007b; Silverman & Crandell,
2010; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wasik et al., 2016; Wasik & Bond, 2001; Wasik & Hindman,
2014; Zipoli et al., 2011). Science curriculum materials with these features provide rich contexts
in which teachers can engage in vocabulary talk in order to promote students’ oral language and
vocabulary development while also deepening their science understandings.
Teacher Professional Development
The present study suggests that there may be a need for teacher professional development
in the early-elementary grades regarding Affective Factors, Student Word Use, and
Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness vocabulary talk, as 86.72% of vocabulary talk code
applications were for Knowledge & Understanding talk moves. Other studies have found limited
instructional attention to Student Word Use and Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness in
particular (e.g., Scott et al., 2003; Silverman et al., 2014; Watts, 1995). While further research is
needed to determine if there is a relationship between teachers’ vocabulary talk moves and
students’ oral language and vocabulary development, the alignment of these vocabulary talk
moves with the research on how children learn words makes it likely that teachers’ vocabulary
146
talk impact students’ word learning. Therefore, it is important for teachers to use language in
ways that align with the full research base on how children learn words and not just on one type
of vocabulary talk.
Teacher Preparation
Thinking of vocabulary instruction in terms of Knowledge & Understanding, Student
Word Use, Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness, and Affective Factors offers a four-part
framework that could be useful for introducing preservice teachers to the tenets of vocabulary
instruction. Teacher preparation programs should encourage preservice teachers to consider how
they can promote students’ oral language and vocabulary development through these types of
vocabulary talk across the school day, as any time something new is learned is an opportunity for
word learning (Wright, in press). Further, teacher preparation programs should emphasize the
reciprocal relationship between literacy and science learning (e.g., Cervetti et al., 2012; Connor
et al., 2017; Gotwals & Wright, 2017, 2017; Guthrie et al., 1999; Romance & Vitale, 1992, 2001;
Varelas et al., 2006; Varelas & Pappas, 2006; Vitale & Romance, 2012; Wang & Herman, 2006)
in both literacy and science methods courses so that beginning teachers enter the field
understanding that science instruction provides a rich context for oral language and vocabulary
development while building background knowledge—both of which ultimately support reading
comprehension (Anderson & Freebody, 1981; Kintsch, 2013). Currently, much of the elementary
curriculum is siloed (Wright & Domke, 2019), and little time is devoted to science instruction
(Berliner, 2011; Blank, 2012); yet, it is not the case that taking time “from” literacy instruction
for science lessons reduces literacy learning. Helping beginning teachers understand this from
the start of their career has the potential to impact practice as they enter the field.
147
Policy
Just as it is important to teach preservice teachers about the reciprocal relationship
between literacy and science learning (e.g., Cervetti et al., 2012; Connor et al., 2017; Gotwals &
Wright, 2017, 2017; Guthrie et al., 1999; Romance & Vitale, 1992, 2001; Varelas et al., 2006;
Varelas & Pappas, 2006; Vitale & Romance, 2012; Wang & Herman, 2006), it is important for
policy makers to get this message as well. The cohort of eight teachers participating in this study
reported spending 450-900 minutes on literacy instruction per week while spending only 30-90
minutes on science during this same timeframe. This cohort is not unique in this regard, as
research has shown that little time is devoted to science instruction in elementary classrooms,
while language arts and mathematics dominate the curriculum (Berliner, 2011; Blank, 2012). It is
important for policy makers to understand that literacy learning is limited when students are not
given the opportunity to develop oral language and vocabulary within the rich context provided
by science instruction or to deepen science understandings that would provide valuable
background knowledge—and both vocabulary knowledge and background knowledge play
pivotal roles in students’ reading comprehension (Anderson & Freebody, 1981; Kintsch, 2013).
Therefore, focusing instructional time on literacy at the expense of science instruction for the
purpose of promoting literacy learning, in reality, is to the detriment of both literacy and science
learning.
In addition to creating policy that makes room for science instruction in the elementary
curriculum, policy makers need to consider that the curriculum selected for teachers to use
matters. While science instruction can offer a rich context for oral language and vocabulary
development (e.g., Gotwals & Wright, 2017; Wright & Gotwals, 2017; Wright & Neuman,
2014), it is evident from the analysis of Ms. Thompson’s science instruction that science
148
curriculum materials such as those found on Teachers Pay Teachers and the Mystery Science
curriculum are not designed to promote students’ oral language and vocabulary development
while supporting their engagement with science learning. It is also evident from Ms. Thompson’s
teaching of the SOLID Start lesson that curriculum materials can set the stage for integrated
vocabulary and science learning. Therefore, science curriculum materials should be carefully
selected in order to best promote students’ science and literacy learning.
Limitations
There are several limitations to this dissertation study. First, all eight of the teachers in
the cohort participating in this study identified as White/European American females, and they
primarily taught White/European American students (95.74% of students across the eight
classes)—none of whom were learning English as an additional language. This limits the
generalizability of my findings to other contexts, as the National Center for Education Statistics
(2020) projected that 45.65% of students attending public schools in the United States would be
White in 2019 and reported that 9.6% of students attending public schools in the United States
were English Learners in 2016 (most recent data reported). Therefore, the students of the
teachers participating in this study are not representative of the student population of schools in
the United States, and the findings of this study cannot be generalized to the greater population.
The second limitation of this study is that I examined only one lesson from each of the
three sets of science curriculum materials used by Ms. Thompson (i.e., SOLID Start, Teachers
Pay Teachers, Mystery Science). It is possible that these particular lessons are not representative
of the respective curriculum materials as a whole, and therefore, characterizations of these sets of
curriculum materials are limited to the specific lesson analyzed from each and cannot be
149
extended to other lessons within these sets of curriculum materials without further analysis of
additional lessons.
A third limitation of this study is that I did not interview the teachers after they taught
their video recorded lessons in order to understand why they made the particular instructional
decisions they made. This would have allowed me better understand how teachers were thinking
about promoting oral language and vocabulary development. Specifically, I could have asked
about the words they had planned to target for instruction and the words they targeted for
instruction more spontaneously by bringing students’ attention to them and their meanings.
However, because this was an observational study of teachers’ oral language and vocabulary use
during science instruction, asking these questions could have altered the teachers’ instruction in
subsequent lessons.
Finally, as discussed in Chapter 3, perhaps the greatest limitation of the present study is
that no student outcome data was collected. Like the present study, a number of observational
studies of vocabulary instruction have not included student outcome data (Blachowicz, 1987;
Dickinson et al., 2008; Nelson et al., 2015; Scott et al., 2003; Wanzek, 2014; Watts, 1995;
Wright & Neuman, 2014); however, the absence of this information makes it impossible to
confirm that the vocabulary talk moves identified in the present study promote students’ oral
language and vocabulary development. While some observational studies have found
correlations between vocabulary instructional methods and students’ learning (e.g., Carlisle et
al., 2013; Neugebauer et al., 2017; Silverman & Crandell, 2010), the relationship between the
vocabulary talk moves identified in the present study and students’ oral language and vocabulary
development have not yet been examined. Although the vocabulary talk moves identified in the
150
present study are aligned with research suggesting they would promote oral language and
vocabulary development, future research is needed to determine if this is the case.
Future Research
Further research is needed to examine how teachers’ use of vocabulary talk moves during
science instruction impacts students’ oral language and vocabulary development. All of the
vocabulary talk moves identified in the present study align with research and theory on how
children learn words, suggesting that these vocabulary talk moves should promote students’ oral
language and vocabulary development. An observational study of vocabulary talk during science
instruction is needed now to confirm this hypothesis. Using the codebook for vocabulary talk
developed in the present study and measures of students’ vocabulary knowledge (e.g.,
knowledge of target words taught within science lessons, general vocabulary knowledge),
statistical analysis should be used to determine the correlation between each type of vocabulary
talk and students’ word learning. Further statistical analysis could also determine the correlation
between each vocabulary talk move and students’ word learning. Once the relationship between
teachers’ vocabulary talk and students’ oral language and vocabulary development has been
established, follow-up studies of professional development could aim to increase teachers’ use of
Student Word Use, Metalinguistic & Metacognitive Awareness, and Affective Factors talk—
given that these types of vocabulary talk are correlated with oral language and vocabulary
development.
The present study focused on teachers’ vocabulary talk within science instruction in early
elementary classrooms, as little is known about how teachers promote oral language and
vocabulary development during early-elementary science lessons; however, the vocabulary talk
moves identified in this study could be examined across the school day. Future research should
151
also explore vocabulary talk beyond science instruction in order to learn how teachers promote
oral language and vocabulary development in other content areas (e.g., social studies,
mathematics), as additional vocabulary talk moves that were not identified in the present study
may be commonly used in these areas.
The present study examined one science lesson from three different sets of science
curriculum materials and identified three different curricular features associated with enhanced
vocabulary talk when present and considerably less vocabulary talk when absent. A content
analysis of a wide range of science curriculum materials could build on this work by identifying
how prevalent these features are in the curriculum options available to teachers. This type of
analysis could provide valuable information for curriculum developers and curriculum adopters.
Relatedly, future research should aim to identify additional features of science curriculum
materials associated with enhanced vocabulary talk by examining vocabulary talk in relation to a
wider range of science curriculum materials with a greater number of teachers.
Conclusions
This study contributes to the field’s understanding of the ways that science instruction
supports literacy learning and literacy instruction supports science learning in the early-
elementary grades. This is particularly important because science instruction offers a rich context
for oral language and vocabulary development (Gotwals & Wright, 2017; Wright & Gotwals,
2017; Wright & Neuman, 2014), yet little time is devoted to science instruction in elementary
classrooms (Berliner, 2011; Blank, 2012) and little is known about how teachers promote oral
language and vocabulary development during science instruction. This study shows that teachers
primarily engage in Knowledge & Understanding vocabulary talk during science lessons in the
early-elementary grades, and vocabulary talk is enhanced when teachers use science curriculum
152
materials that target conceptually-connected vocabulary words for instruction and highlight these
words through careful text selection and related discussion prompts.
153
APPENDICES
154
APPENDIX A—SOLID START PD RECRUITMENT INFORMATION
Science, Oral Language and Literacy Development from the Start of School
(SOLID Start) Professional Development and Research Partnership Opportunity
You’re invited!
Who: K-2 teachers
What: Become a research partner for the online SOLID Start Professional Development course
When: Spring 2019 (beginning in early February)
Where: Wherever you are (online course)
Why: Learn about important practices for science and literacy learning and support the development of
the SOLID Start Professional Development course!
How: Contact Dr. Tanya Wright (tswright@msu.edu) or Dr. Amelia Wenk Gotwals (gotwals@msu.edu)
with questions or to sign up; enrollment is on first come, first served basis.
What is SOLID Start?
● SOLID Start, which stands for Science, Oral Language and Literacy Development from the Start
of School, is a project designed to support K-2 teachers in integrating science, oral language, and
literacy in their teaching.
● SOLID Start is developing NGSS-aligned curriculum, a formative classroom observation tool to
support teachers in enacting high-quality discourse, and online professional development and
coaching.
● SOLID Start is funded by the National Science Foundation and led by Dr. Tanya Wright and Dr.
Amelia Wenk Gotwals of Michigan State University’s Department of Teacher Education.
What will SOLID Start research partners do?
● Participate in a 10-week online professional development focused on supporting talk around
equitable science participation and engagement, deepening students’ science understanding, and
supporting students’ science language and literacy development.
● Upload three videos of teaching science lessons throughout the course and receive feedback from
a coach on these lessons
● Provide feedback on the course through surveys, interviews, and/or focus groups.
What are the objectives of the SOLID Start professional development?
Teachers will learn about why the three following practices are important for science and literacy
learning:
1) Fostering equitable science participation and engagement
2) Deepening students’ science understanding, and
3) Supporting students’ science language and literacy development.
Teachers will gain strategies for implementing these practices, through analysis of videos, reading
articles, and enacting and reflecting upon their own instruction.
What are the benefits of becoming a SOLID Start research partner?
● Learn about fostering equitable science participation and engagement, deepening students’
science understanding, and supporting students’ science language and literacy development.
● Receive support from a coach who will support you in reflecting on your teaching practices
related to language and literacy development in the context of science learning.
● Receive a $500 stipend and SCECHs (number TBD).
155
APPENDIX B—DEMOGRAPHIC BACKGROUND AND TEACHING EXPERIENCE
INFORMATION SURVEY
Name:
Date:
Part 1: Background
1. What is your gender?
Female
Male
Gender Diverse
2. What are your pronouns?
She/her
He/him
They/their
3. In what year were you born?
4. Check ALL the categories that best describe your race or ethnicity:
Black or African American
Arab American
Hispanic or Latino/a
White or European American
Asian/Pacific Islander
Native American
Multiracial
Other:
5. Is English your first language?
Yes
No
6. If no, what is your first language?
Part 2: Professional Experience
1. For each degree you hold, please provide the following information:
a. Education degree
Associate’s degree
Bachelor’s degree (BA or BS)
Master’s degree
Doctoral degree
b. Major, minor, and/or focus area (e.g., elementary education):
c. Year of graduation:
156
2. Which teaching certification do you currently have? (Check one.)
a. Provisional certificate (initial teaching license)
b. Professional education certificate (advanced teaching license)
c. Temporary teacher employment authorization (one year non-renewable)
d. Other:
3. Which endorsements do you have? (Check all that apply.)
Special Education Consultant
English as a Second Language
Early Childhood Education
National Board Certification
Reading
Reading Specialist
Special Education
None
Other:
4. How many years have you worked as a teacher? (Check one.)
Fewer than 5
6-10
11-15
16-20
Over 20
5. How many years have you taught the grade level you’re currently teaching? (Check one.)
Fewer than 5
6-10
11-15
16-20
Over 20
6. How long have you worked at your current location?
Part 3: Professional Development
1. In the past year, in which content areas did you attend or receive professional
development? (Check all that apply.)
Language arts: Reading
Language arts: Writing
Math
Science
Social Studies
Other:
2. Please specify the topics the PD covered for each content area you indicated above.
157
Part 4: Setting Description
1. How many children in your class belong to each of the following groups?
a. Black or African American:
b. Middle Eastern/Arab:
c. Hispanic or Latino/a:
d. Asian or Pacific Islander:
e. White or European American:
f. Biracial or multiethnic:
g. Other race or ethnicity:
2. How many children in your class have a documented special need or learning disability?
3. How many children in your class are English learners (learning English as an additional
language)?
Part 5: Science and Literacy Curriculum
1. For each subject area, please list the name(s) of the curricula you use (i.e. Open Court;
Reading Street). If you use more than one curriculum per subject area, please list them
all. Also, add supplementary materials or your own materials, if you use them.
a. Language Arts: Reading:
b. Language Arts: Writing:
c. Language Arts: Oral Language/vocabulary:
d. Science:
2. How closely do you follow the language arts curricula you use?
a. I follow it strictly.
b. I follow it most of the time/make adaptations when appropriate.
c. I use it occasionally/select pieces that are appropriate for my students.
d. I rarely use it.
3. How closely do you follow the science curricula you use?
a. How closely do you follow the science curricula you use?
a. I follow it strictly.
b. I follow it most of the time/make adaptations when appropriate.
c. I use it occasionally/select pieces that are appropriate for my students.
d. I rarely use it.
4. Who selects the language arts curricula that you use in your classroom? (Check one.)
My school district
My principal
I do
Other:
5. Who selects the science curricula that you use in your classroom? (Check one.)
My school district
158
My principal
I do
Other:
159
APPENDIX C—WEEKLY INSTRUCTIONAL LOG
Name:
Week of PD: [Before PD, Week 1, Week 2, … Week 10]
1. How many times did you teach science this week?
2. How many minutes did you teach science each time (e.g., Monday: 30 minutes, Tuesday: 40
minutes, Thursday: 20 minutes)?
3. Comments (e.g., two snow days this week, no school for holiday on Monday, field trip on
Thursday)
4. What did content did you teach in this week’s science lessons?
5. Did you use curriculum materials? [yes no]
6. If you responded yes, what curriculum materials did you use?
160
APPENDIX D—TRANSCRIPTION GUIDE
General Instructions
• Every line must end with a carriage return.
• Each utterance gets its own line (i.e., only one utterance per line).
• Use T and teacher number to identify the teacher as speaker. For example, TEA40 is
noted in transcripts as T40.
• Use CHI (for child) for all student talk (whether one student is speaking or multiple
students are speaking).
Utterances
This study uses the utterance as the unit of analysis. For this study an utterance is defined as a
unit of speech bounded by only one speaker (i.e., bound by conversational turns) and one main
clause (subject-predicate) (MacWhinney, 2000; Neugebauer et al., 2017; Rex & Schiller, 2009).
As a conversational unit (c-unit), an utterance has only one main clause (subject-predicate) but
may have a dependent clause as well (MacWhinney, 2000). The sentence I’m going home, which
is far away is one utterance.
If two clauses are joined by one of these coordinating conjunctions, use a separate tier for the
coordinating conjunction and clause that follows: and, but, for, nor, or, yet, so. For example, if
two independent clauses are joined by and, each independent clause is a separate utterance. For
the compound sentence This is my bike, and it goes fast, there are two utterances:
this is my bike.
and it goes fast.
Transcription Notes Regarding Utterances:
• Each utterance gets its own line.
• Start each utterance with the three-character participant code, a colon, and a space. For
example, a student utterance looks like this:
CHI: I had to push it to make it go.
• Do not capitalize the first word unless it is a proper noun.
• End each utterance with a period, question mark, or exclamation point.
o Period: use with declarative utterance
this is an utterance. [utterance spoken without emphasis or rising contour]
o Question mark: use when a question is asked (in any of the following forms):
what is an utterance? [utterance has question word]
utterance? [utterance is spoken with final rising contour]
is that an utterance? [utterance has inverted subject-verb]
that’s an utterance, right? [utterance has tag question ending]
o Exclamation point: use with imperative or emphatic utterance
this is an utterance! [utterance spoken with emphasis]
•
If the speaker doesn’t finish what they were saying,
o use ... to indicate that the speaker trailed off. This is typically used when the
speaker is prompting students to fill in the “blank” they’re creating by trailing off.
161
o use / to indicate that another speaker interrupted
o use // to indicate that that speaker interrupted themselves (i.e., incomplete thought
that didn’t just trail off, false start)
•
If there’s a pause that interrupts the flow of speech, mark it with the number of seconds it
lasts in parentheses. For example, a 2-second pause in the utterance I noticed there were
cumulonimbus clouds would be transcribed as
CHI: I noticed there were (2) cumulonimbus clouds.
•
If multiple speakers speak at the same time,
• use angle brackets (< >) when speakers speak at the same time. Place what is
being said at the same time inside the brackets for each speaker.
T11: the types of force we’re talking about today are .
CHI: .
• When a speaker quotes someone or something:
• use beginning (“)and ending quotation (”) marks around the quoted words.
•
T11: the meteorologist said today’s weather would be “sweltering”.
If the speaker reads text aloud, use { and } around what is being read
T11: {the wild things roared their terrible roars}.
• Use xxx to indicate unintelligible speech when you cannot hear or understand what the
•
speaker is saying.
If you’re pretty sure you know what was said but aren’t positive, use comment to note
that it is a best guess.
• When a word is incomplete but the intended meaning seems clear, insert the missing
material within parentheses. (Do not insert fully omitted words.)
(be)cause
(un)til
(a)bout
(o)kay
• Use www to indicate that some utterances are being left out (e.g., teacher answers phone
or talks to another teacher); comment in the next tier (dependent tier) to explain why
utterances were left out
• Use Name in place of student’s names if speaker calls student by name.
•
If the speaker makes a sound effect, either make a comment to describe the sound effect
(e.g., teacher made the sound of a train whistle). If unsure of what the sound is, spell the
sound as best you can and put that in the transcript itself between slashes (e.g.,
/sloosheywoo/).
Providing Additional Information
To note any kind of additional information that is needed to understand what’s going on in the
video, make a comment in the row immediately following the utterance to which it refers. Type
the comment in italics so that it easily stands out as not being an utterance.
teacher waves arm like flag on a windy day
teacher is typing the words as she says them
student is pointing to the flag
students are acting out shivering
Questions/Decisions
162
• Student repetitions/false starts: separate by commas but okay to keep all on same main
line since teacher talk is the focus.
• Teacher false starts: if teacher has several false starts in a row, put them all on the same
main line→ then put the full utterance that follows on a new line.
o T11: I, uh, we went, it wasn’t //
o T11: that was the first time we went.
•
If teacher repeats a word/phrase in the middle of an utterance (not just a false start) and
then continues the utterance, keep those repetitions on the same line.
• Pay attention to intonation curves. If it sounds like the speaker has said their complete
thought but then tacks something at the end after final intonation (rising for question or
falling for statement), put what’s tacked on on a new line (new utterance).
163
APPENDIX E—VOCABULARY TALK CODEBOOK
Knowledge & Understanding Vocabulary Talk Moves
Vocabulary Talk Move
Description
Introduce Word
Review Word
Explain/Define
Use with Hint
Teacher draws attention to a
new word
Teacher reminds students of a
word that’s already been
introduced
Teacher explicitly provides
an explanation or definition
of a word
Teacher uses word in context
but provides some kind of
hint/context clue
Act Out/Demonstrate
Teacher acts out a word’s
meaning
Elicit Student Ideas: Act Out Teacher asks students to act
Example/Nonexample
Elicit Student Ideas:
Example/Nonexample
Elicit Student Ideas: New
Word
Elicit Student Ideas: Review
Word
Elicit Student Ideas:
Synonym
Provide Visual Support
out a word’s meaning
Teacher shares an example or
nonexample of a word
Teacher asks students to
share an example or
nonexample of a word
Teacher asks students what a
new word means
Teacher asks students what a
review word means
Teacher asks students for a
synonym
Teacher shows an image of
what a word means
Differentiate Meaning
Use Target Word
Teacher explains what word
means in a particular context
vs. another
Teacher says target word
Example
And this big word is
arrangement.
Do you remember what a
habitat is?
So bipeds are animals that use
two legs for walking.
So, you will have to design
some sort of crossing for your
animal to get across the road
or for, or under the road.
Instead of the wind moving
every which way, it spins in a
circle. [Teacher demonstrates
tornado movement with
fingers.]
Show me what that would
look like.
Grass can also be shelter for
animals.
Do you have another extreme
weather, Micah?
What does prepare mean?
What do we know about
stratus clouds?
What’s another name for
concrete?
We get this gap in between
and we still have landforms
that are really high on either
side. [Teacher is pointing to
image of a canyon.]
Not like outer space but like
we need space to move.
Yes, the wind in our
classroom is calm.
164
Emphasize
Visually Display Word
Elicit Student Questions:
Target Word
Highlight Words Across
Domains
Offer Alternative
Comment on Spelling
Restate Differently
Teacher emphasizes target
word and/or its meaning
Teacher shows students a
word
Teacher asks students what
they want to know about a
target word
Teacher draws students’
attention to word from
another domain
Teacher offers an alternative
way to say something
Teacher draws attention to
the spelling of a word
Teacher restates something to
make it more comprehensible
So is it a mammal?
This word says severe.
What did we want to know
about sound?
What a great math word!
[diagonal]
Instead of saying so, so, so,
so, so, we could say it was
extreme.
Friction has a blend at the
beginning.
Scientists think birds are
related to dinosaurs. [Text:
Birds are living dinosaurs.]
Affective Factors Vocabulary Talk Moves
Vocabulary Talk Move
Description
Example
Help Students Make Personal
Connection
Praise
Acknowledge Student
Ownership
Answer a Question
Appreciate Word/Word
Choice
Highlight Favorite Words
Teacher connects word to
students’ personal
experiences
Teacher praises student word
use
Teacher acknowledges
students’ ownership of words
Teacher answers students’
questions about what words
mean
Teacher comments on word
choice
Teacher talks about
favorite/liked words
In the past three, four days,
we've had some severe
weather conditions.
That’s a good weather word!
Myra said there’s a force
happening here.
The smallest [wind] would be
calm.
Here’s another good word.
That is one of my favorite
words. [precipitation]
165
Student Word Use Vocabulary Talk Moves
Vocabulary Talk Move
Description
Example
Ask to Repeat
Prompt Word
Elicit New Word from
Students
Encourage to Use Later
Suggest Word/Phrase
Revoice
Teacher asks students to
repeat after her
Teacher prompts word or
meaning
Teacher asks students for a
word that hasn’t been
introduced/discussed
Teacher encourages students
to use words later
Teacher suggests another way
to say something
Teacher elevates students’
word use
Try that word.
So if it ate plants, what was it
called? [herbivore]
Does anyone know the
science word when you kind
of blend into your
environment?
So you will be using this
word later?
Maybe we could say the word
breezy.
S: Yeah, they’re going in a
circle.
T: They are moving in a
curve.
Metacognitive and Metalinguistic Awareness Vocabulary Talk Moves
Vocabulary Talk Move
Description
Example
Ask if Heard of Word
Teacher asks students if
familiar with word.
Thumbs up if you’ve ever
heard of that word before.
166
REFERENCES
167
REFERENCES
Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.),
Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77–117). International Reading
Association.
Anderson, R. C., & Nagy, W. E. (1991). Word meanings. In R. Barr, Kamil, Michael L., P. B.
Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (2nd ed., pp. 690–
724). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Anderson, R. C., & Nagy, W. E. (1992). The vocabulary conundrum. American Educator: The
Professional Journal of the American Federation of Teachers, 16(4).
http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ458646
Barnes, E. M., & Dickinson, D. K. (2017a). The impact of teachers’ commenting strategies on
children’s vocabulary growth. Exceptionality, 25(3), 186–206.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09362835.2016.1196447
Barnes, E. M., & Dickinson, D. K. (2017b). The relationship of Head Start teachers’ academic
language use and children’s receptive vocabulary. Early Education and Development,
28(7), 794–809. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017.1340069
Barnes, E. M., & Dickinson, D. K. (2018). Relationships among teachers’ use of mental state
verbs and children’s vocabulary growth. Early Education and Development, 29(3), 307–
323. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2018.1440844
Barnes, E. M., Dickinson, D. K., & Grifenhagen, J. F. (2017). The role of teachers’ comments
during book reading in children’s vocabulary growth. The Journal of Educational
Research, 110(5), 515–527. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2015.1134422
Barone, D. M. (2011). Case study research. In Literacy research methodologies (2nd ed., pp. 7–
27). Guilford Press.
Baumann, J. F., Edwards, E. C., Boland, E. M., Olejnik, S., & Kame’enui, E. J. (2003).
Vocabulary tricks: Effects of instruction in morphology and context on fifth-grade
students’ ability to derive and infer word meanings. American Educational Research
Journal, 40(2), 447–494.
Baumann, J. F., Edwards, E. C., Font, G., Tereshinski, C. A., Kame’enui, E. J., & Olejnik, S.
(2002). Teaching morphemic and contextual analysis to fifth-grade students. Reading
Research Quarterly, 37(2), 150–176. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.37.2.3
168
Baumann, J. F., Ware, D., & Edwards, E. C. (2007). “Bumping into spicy, tasty words that catch
your tongue”: A formative experiment on vocabulary instruction. The Reading Teacher,
61(2), 108–122.
Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (2001). Text talk: Capturing the benefits of read-aloud
experiences for young children. The Reading Teacher, 55(1), 10–20.
Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (2004). Elements of reading vocabulary: Teacher’s guide level
A (Harcourt Supplemental Publishers, Rigby Steck-Vaughn, Item Number 0-7398-8458-
1). Steck-Vaughn.
Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (2007). Increasing young low‐income children’s oral vocabulary
repertoires through rich and focused instruction. The Elementary School Journal, 107(3),
251–271. https://doi.org/10.1086/511706
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & McCaslin, E. S. (1983). Vocabulary development: All contexts
are not created equal. The Elementary School Journal, 177–181.
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Omanson, R. (1987). The effects and uses of diverse
vocabulary instructional techniques. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The
nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 147–163). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., Sandora, C., Kucan, L., & Worthy, J. (1996). Questioning the
author: A yearlong classroom implementation to engage students with text. The
Elementary School Journal, 96(4), 385–414. https://doi.org/10.1086/461835
Berliner, D. (2011). Rational responses to high stakes testing: The case of curriculum narrowing
and the harm that follows. Cambridge Journal of Education, 41(3), 287–302.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0305764X.2011.607151
Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in
primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 44–62.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.44
Blachowicz, C. L. Z. (1987). Vocabulary instruction: What goes on in the classroom? The
Reading Teacher, 41(2), 132–137.
Blachowicz, C. L. Z., Baumann, J. F., Manyak, P. C., & Graves, M. F. (2013). “FLOOD, FAST,
FOCUSED”: Integrated vocabulary instruction in the classroom. International Reading
Association.
Blank, R. K. (2012). What is the impact of decline in science instructional time in elementary
school? Noyce Foundation.
169
Bohannon, J. N., & Bonvillian, J. D. (2001). Theoretical approaches to language acquisition. In
J. B. Gleason (Ed.), The development of language (5th ed., pp. 254–314). Allyn and
Bacon.
Boulware-Gooden, R., Carreker, S., Thornhill, A., & Joshi, R. M. (2007). Instruction of
metacognitive strategies enhances reading comprehension and vocabulary achievement of
third-grade students. The Reading Teacher, 61(1), 70–77.
https://doi.org/10.1598/RT.61.1.7
Bowers, E. P., & Vasilyeva, M. (2011). The relation between teacher input and lexical growth of
preschoolers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(1), 221–241.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000354
Bowers, P. N., & Kirby, J. R. (2010). Effects of morphological instruction on vocabulary
acquisition. Reading and Writing, 23(5), 515–537. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-
9172-z
Bravo, M. A., & Cervetti, G. N. (2008). Teaching vocabulary through text and experience in
content areas.pdf. In A. E. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say
about vocabulary instruction (pp. 130–149). International Reading Association.
Bruner, J. (1978). The role of dialogue in language acquisition. In A. Sinclair, R. Jarvella, & W.
Levelt (Eds.), The child’s conception of language. Springer-Verlag.
Cain, K. (2007). Deriving word meanings from context: Does explanation facilitate contextual
analysis? Journal of Research in Reading, 30(4), 347–359.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2007.00336.x
Carlisle, J. F., Kelcey, B., & Berebitsky, D. (2013). Teachers’ support of students’ vocabulary
learning during literacy instruction in high poverty elementary schools. American
Educational Research Journal, 50(6), 1360–1391.
https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831213492844
Carlo, M. S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C. E., Lippman, D. N., Lively, T. J., & White,
C. E. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-language
learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms.pdf. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(2),
188–215.
Cervetti, G. N., Barber, J., Dorph, R., Pearson, P. D., & Goldschmidt, P. G. (2012). The impact
of an integrated approach to science and literacy in elementary school classrooms.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 49(5), 631–658.
https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21015
Cervetti, G. N., Wright, T. S., & Hwang, H. (2016). Conceptual coherence, comprehension, and
vocabulary acquisition: A knowledge effect? Reading and Writing, 29(4), 761–779.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9628-x
170
Chambré, S. J., Ehri, L. C., & Ness, M. (2017). Orthographic facilitation of first graders’
vocabulary learning: Does directing attention to print enhance the effect? Reading and
Writing, 30(5), 1137–1156. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9715-z
Chambrè, S. J., Ehri, L. C., & Ness, M. (2019). Phonological decoding enhances orthographic
facilitation of vocabulary learning in first graders. Reading and Writing.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09997-w
Connor, C. M., Dombek, J., Crowe, E. C., Spencer, M., Tighe, E. L., Coffinger, S., Zargar, E.,
Wood, T., & Petscher, Y. (2017). Acquiring science and social studies knowledge in
kindergarten through fourth grade: Conceptualization, design, implementation, and
efficacy testing of content-area literacy instruction (CALI). Journal of Educational
Psychology, 109(3), 301–320. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000128
Cox, R., O’Brien, K., Walsh, M., & West, H. (2015). Working with multilingual learners and
vocabulary knowledge for secondary schools: Developing word consciousness. English
in Australia, 50(1), 77–84.
Coyne, M. D., McCoach, D. B., & Kapp, S. (2007). Vocabulary intervention for kindergarten
students: Comparing extended instruction to embedded instruction and incidental
exposure. Learning Disability Quarterly, 30(2), 74–88.
Coyne, M. D., McCoach, D. B., Loftus, S., Zipoli, R., Ruby, M., Crevecoeur, Y. C., & Kapp, S.
(2010). Direct and extended vocabulary instruction in kindergarten: Investigating transfer
effects. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 3(2), 93–120.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19345741003592410
Coyne, M. D., McCoach, D. B., Loftus, S., Zipoli Jr., R., & Kapp, S. (2009). Direct vocabulary
instruction in kindergarten: Teaching for breadth versus depth. The Elementary School
Journal, 110(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1086/598840
Dale, E. (1965). Vocabulary measurement: Techniques and major findings. Elementary English,
895–948.
Davis, F. B. (1944). Fundamental factors of comprehension in reading. Psychometrika, 9(3),
185–197.
Dickinson, D. K., Cote, L., & Smith, M. W. (1993). Learning vocabulary in preschool: Social
and discourse contexts affecting vocabulary growth. New Directions for Child and
Adolescent Development, 1993(61), 67–78. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219936106
Dickinson, D. K., Darrow, C. L., & Tinubu, T. A. (2008). Patterns of teacher–child conversations
in Head Start classrooms: Implications for an empirically grounded approach to
professional development. Early Education & Development, 19(3), 396–429.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280802065403
171
Dickinson, D. K., & Porche, M. V. (2011). Relation between language experiences in preschool
classrooms and children’s kindergarten and fourth-grade language and reading abilities:
Preschool language experiences and later language and reading. Child Development,
82(3), 870–886. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01576.x
Dickinson, D. K., & Smith, M. W. (1994). Long-term effects of preschool teachers’ book
readings on low-income children’s vocabulary and story comprehension. Reading
Research Quarterly, 29(2), 104. https://doi.org/10.2307/747807
Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (1991). Early literacy: Linkages between home, school and
literacy achievement at age five. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6(1), 30–
46. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568549109594820
Ehri, L. C. (2014). Orthographic mapping in the acquisition of sight word reading, spelling
memory, and vocabulary learning. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 5–21.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.819356
Ehri, L. C., & Rosenthal, J. (2007). Spellings of words: A neglected facilitator of vocabulary
learning. Journal of Literacy Research, 39(4), 389–409.
Freeman, N. D., Townsend, D., & Templeton, S. (2019). Thinking about words: First graders’
response to morphological instruction. The Reading Teacher, 72(4), 463–473.
https://doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1749
Fukkink, R. G., & de Glopper, K. (1998). Effects of instruction in deriving word meaning from
context: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 68, 450–469.
Gee, J. P. (2014). How to do discourse analysis: A toolkit (2nd ed.). Routledge.
Gotwals, A. W., & Wright, T. S. (2017). From “plants don’t eat” to “plants are producers.”
Science and Children, 55(3), 44–50.
Graves, M. F. (2000). A vocabulary program to complement and bolster a middle-grade
comprehension program. In B. M. Taylor, M. F. Graves, & P. van den Broek (Eds.),
Reading for meaning (pp. 116–135). International Reading Association.
Graves, M. F. (2006). The vocabulary book: Learning and instruction. Teachers College Press.
Graves, M. F., & Watts-Taffe, S. (2002). The place of word consciousness in a research-based
vocabulary program. In S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say
about reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 140–165). International Reading Association.
Graves, M. F., & Watts-Taffe, S. (2008). For the love of words: Fostering word consciousness in
young readers. The Reading Teacher, 62(3), 185–193. https://doi.org/10.1598/RT.62.3.1
172
Guthrie, J. T., Anderson, E., Alao, S., & Rinehart, J. (1999). Influences of Concept-Oriented
Reading Instruction on strategy use and conceptual learning from text. The Elementary
School Journal, 99(4), 343–366.
Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A., Coddington, C. S., Lutz Klauda, S., Wigfield, A., & Barbosa, P.
(2009). Impacts of comprehensive reading instruction on diverse outcomes of low- and
high-achieving readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(3), 195–214.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219408331039
Hadley, E. B., Dickinson, D. K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2019). Building semantic
networks: The impact of a vocabulary intervention on preschoolers’ depth of word
knowledge. Reading Research Quarterly, 54(1), 41–61. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.225
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (2003). The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3.
American Educator, 27(1), 4.
Jenkins, J. R., Matlock, B., & Slocum, T. A. (1989). Two approaches to vocabulary instruction:
The teaching of individual word meanings and practice in deriving word meaning from
context. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(2), 215–235. https://doi.org/10.2307/747865
Johnson, R. (2013). What is precipitation? Crabtree Publishing Company.
Jubenville, K., Sénéchal, M., & Malette, M. (2014). The moderating effect of orthographic
consistency on oral vocabulary learning in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 245–263.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.05.002
Juel, C., & Deffes, R. (2004). Making words stick. Educational Leadership, 61(6), 30–34.
Kintsch, W. (2013). Revisiting the construction-integration model of text comprehension and its
implications for instruction. In D. E. Alvermanm, N. J. Unrau, & R. B. Ruddell,
Theoretical models and processes of reading (6th ed., pp. 807–839). International
Reading Association.
Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
9(5), 4.
Lesaux, N. K., & Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Exploring sources of reading comprehension difficulties
among language minority learners and their classmates in early adolescence. American
Educational Research Journal, 47(3), 596–632.
https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831209355469
Lesaux, N. K., Kieffer, M. J., Faller, S. E., & Kelley, J. G. (2010). The effectiveness and ease of
implementation of an academic vocabulary intervention for linguistically diverse students
in urban middle schools. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(2), 196–228.
https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.2.3
173
Lubliner, S., & Smetana, L. (2005). The effects of comprehensive vocabulary instruction on Title
I students’ metacognitive word-learning skills and reading comprehension. Journal of
Literacy Research, 37(2), 163–200. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15548430jlr3702_3
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Manyak, P. C., Von Gunten, H., Autenrieth, D., Gillis, C., Mastre-O’Farrell, J., Irvine-
McDermott, E., Baumann, J. F., & Blachowicz, C. L. Z. (2014). Four practical principles
for enhancing vocabulary instruction. The Reading Teacher, 68(1), 13–23.
https://doi.org/10.1002/trtr.1299
McKeown, M. G. (1985). The acquisition of word meaning from context by children of high and
low ability. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(4), 482–496. https://doi.org/10.2307/747855
McKeown, M. G. (1993). Creating effective definitions for young word learners. Reading
Research Quarterly, 28(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.2307/747814
McKeown, M. G., & Beck, I. L. (2003). Taking advantage of read-alouds to help children make
sense of decontextualized language. In A. Van Kleeck, S. A. Stahl, & E. B. Bauer (Eds.),
On reading books to children (pp. 159–176). Erlbaum.
McKeown, M. G., & Beck, I. L. (2004). Transforming knowledge into professional development
resources: Six teachers implement a model of teaching for understanding text. The
Elementary School Journal, 104(5), 391–408. https://doi.org/10.1086/499759
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Omanson, R. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1983). The effects of long-
term vocabulary instruction on reading comprehension: A replication. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 15(1), 3–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862968309547474
McKeown, M. G., Beck, I. L., Omanson, R. C., & Pople, M. T. (1985). Some effects of the
nature and frequency of vocabulary instruction on the knowledge and use of words.
Reading Research Quarterly, 20(5), 522. https://doi.org/10.2307/747940
Mercer, N., Dawes, L., Wegerif, R., & Sams, C. (2004). Reasoning as a scientist: Ways of
helping children to use language to learn science. British Educational Research Journal,
30(3), 359–377. https://doi.org/10.1080/01411920410001689689
Michaels, S., & O’Connor, C. (2012). Talk science primer. TERC.
Michaels, S., & O’Connor, C. (2015). Conceptualizing talk moves as tools: Professional
development approaches for academically productive discussions. In L. B. Resnick, C. S.
C. Asterhan, & S. N. Clarke (Eds.), Socializing Intelligence Through Academic Talk and
Dialogue (pp. 347–361). American Educational Research Association.
https://doi.org/10.3102/978-0-935302-43-1_27
174
Michener, C. J., Patrick Proctor, C., & Silverman, R. D. (2018). Features of instructional talk
predictive of reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 31(3), 725–756.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9807-4
Miles, M. B., Huberman, A. M., & Saldaña, J. (2014). Qualitative data analysis: A methods
sourcebook (3rd ed.). Sage.
Nagy, W. E. (2005). Why vocabulary instruction needs to be long-term and comprehensive. In E.
Hiebert & M. L. Kamil (Eds.), Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to
practice (pp. 27–44). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English?
Reading Research Quarterly, 19(3), 304. https://doi.org/10.2307/747823
Nagy, W. E., & Herman, P. A. (1985). Incidental vs. Instructional approaches to increasing
reading vocabulary. Educational Perspectives, 16–21.
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., Anderson, R. C., & Pearson, P. D. (1984). Learning words from
context (No. 319). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED246392
Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, Richard C. (1985). Learning word meanings from
context: How broadly generalizable? (No. 347). University of Illinois at Urbana-
Champaign.
Nagy, W. E., & Scott, J. A. (2000). Vocabulary processes. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P.
D. Pearson, & R. Barr, Handbook of Reading Research (Vol. 3, pp. 269–284). Lawrence
Erlbaum Association.
Nash, H., & Snowling, M. (2006). Teaching new words to children with poor existing
vocabulary knowledge: A controlled evaluation of the definition and context methods.
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(3), 335–354.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820600602295
National Center for Education Statistics. (2020). Back to school statistics. Institute of Education
Research; National Center for Education Statistics.
https://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/display.asp?id=372#PK12_enrollment
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School
Officers. (2010). Common Core State Standards. National Governors Association Center
for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National
Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the
scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH
Publication No. 004769). U.S. Government Printing Office.
175
National Research Council. (2007). Taking science to school: Learning and teaching science in
Grades K-8. National Academies Press.
Nelson, K. L., Dole, J. A., Hosp, J. L., & Hosp, M. K. (2015). Vocabulary instruction in K-3
low-income classrooms during a reading reform project. Reading Psychology, 36(2),
145–172. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711.2013.839485
Neugebauer, S. R., Gámez, P. B., Coyne, M. D., Cólon, I. T., McCoach, D. B., & Ware, S.
(2017). Promoting word consciousness to close the vocabulary gap in young word
learners. The Elementary School Journal, 118(1), 28–54. https://doi.org/10.1086/692986
Neuman, S. B., & Dwyer, J. (2011). Developing vocabulary and conceptual knowledge for low-
income preschoolers: A design experiment. Journal of Literacy Research, 43(2), 103–
129. https://doi.org/10.1177/1086296X11403089
Neuman, S. B., & Kaefer, T. (2013). Enhancing the intensity of vocabulary instruction for
preschoolers at risk. The Elementary School Journal, 113(4), 589–608.
Neuman, S. B., & Kaefer, T. (2018). Developing low-income children’s vocabulary and content
knowledge through a shared book reading program. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 52, 15–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.12.001
Neuman, S. B., Newman, E. H., & Dwyer, J. (2011). Educational effects of a vocabulary
intervention on preschoolers’ word knowledge and conceptual development: A cluster-
randomized trial. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(3), 249–272.
Neuman, S. B., & Wright, T. S. (2013). All about words: Increasing vocabulary in the Common
Core Classroom, prek-2. Teachers College Press.
NGSS Lead States. (2013). Next Generation Science Standards: For states, by states.
Nystrand, M. (2006). Research on the role of classroom discourse as it affects reading
comprehension. Research in the Teaching of English, 40(4), 392–412.
O’Connor, C., & Michaels, S. (1993). Aligning academic task and participation status through
revoicing: Analysis of a classroom discourse strategy. Anthropology Education
Quarterly, 24(4), 318–335. https://doi.org/10.1525/aeq.1993.24.4.04x0063k
O’Connor, C., & Michaels, S. (1996). Shifting participant frameworks: Orchestrating thinking
practices in group discussion. In D. Hicks (Ed.), Discourse, learning, and schooling (pp.
63–103). Cambridge University Press.
O’Connor, C., & Michaels, S. (2019). Supporting teachers in taking up productive talk moves:
The long road to professional learning at scale. International Journal of Educational
Research, 97, 166–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2017.11.003
176
Ouellette, G., & Beers, A. (2010). A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and
visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing, 23(2), 189–208.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9159-1
Paris, S. G. (2005). Reinterpreting the development of reading skills. Reading Research
Quarterly, 40(2), 184–202. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.40.2.3
Pollard-Durodola, S. D., Gonzalez, J. E., Simmons, D. C., Kwok, O., Taylor, A. B., Davis, M. J.,
Kim, M., & Simmons, L. (2011). The effects of an intensive shared book-reading
intervention for preschool children at risk for vocabulary delay. Exceptional Children,
77(2), 161–183. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440291107700202
Ratner, N. B., & Brundage, S. B. (2018). A clinician’s complete guide to CLAN and PRAAT.
Rex, L. A., & Schiller, L. (2009). Using discourse analysis to improve classroom interaction.
Routledge.
Richmond, G., & Striley, J. (1996). Making meaning in classrooms: Social processes in small-
group discourse and scientific knowledge building. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching, 33(8), 839–858. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-
2736(199610)33:8<839::AID-TEA2>3.0.CO;2-X
Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Nation, K. (2009). Orthographic facilitation in oral vocabulary
acquisition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(10), 1948–1966.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802696104
Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not
all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(3), 235–257.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430701344306
Robbins, C., & Ehri, L. C. (1994). Reading storybooks to kindergartners helps them learn new
vocabulary words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 54–64.
Romance, N. R., & Vitale, M. R. (1992). A curriculum strategy that expands time for in-depth
elementary science instruction by using science-based reading strategies: Effects of a
year-long study in grade four. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 29(6), 545–554.
https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660290604
Romance, N. R., & Vitale, M. R. (2001). Implementing an in-depth expanded science model in
elementary schools: Multi-year findings, research issues, and policy implications.
International Journal of Science Education, 23(4), 373–404.
Rosenthal, J., & Ehri, L. C. (2008). The mnemonic value of orthography for vocabulary learning.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
0663.100.1.175
177
Rosenthal, J., & Ehri, L. C. (2011). Pronouncing new words aloud during the silent reading of
text enhances fifth graders’ memory for vocabulary words and their spellings. Reading
and Writing, 24(8), 921–950. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9239-x
Saldaña, J. (2016). The coding manual for qualitative researchers (3rd ed.). SAGE.
Schacht, K., & Peltz, D. (2020). Mystery Science: Open-and-go lessons that inspire kids to love
science. Mystery Science. https://mysteryscience.com/lessons/seasonal/current-events
Scott, J. A., Jamieson-Noel, D., & Asselin, M. (2003). Vocabulary instruction throughout the day
in twenty-three Canadian upper-elementary classrooms. The Elementary School Journal,
269–286.
Scott, J. A., & Nagy, W. E. (2004). Developing word consciousness. In E. J. Kame’enui & J. F.
Baumann (Eds.), Vocabulary instruction: Research to practice (1st ed., pp. 201–217).
Guilford Press.
Senechal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive
role of early vocabulary to future reading. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.),
Handbook of early literacy research (2nd ed., pp. 173–182). Guilford Press.
Silverman, R. (2007a). Vocabulary development of English‐language and English‐only learners
in kndergarten. The Elementary School Journal, 107(4), 365–383.
https://doi.org/10.1086/516669
Silverman, R. (2007b). A comparison of three methods of vocabulary instruction during read‐
alouds in kindergarten. The Elementary School Journal, 108(2), 97–113.
https://doi.org/10.1086/525549
Silverman, R., & Crandell, J. D. (2010). Vocabulary practices in prekindergarten and
kindergarten classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(3), 318–340.
https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.3.3
Silverman, R., Proctor, C. P., Harring, J. R., Doyle, B., Mitchell, M. A., & Meyer, A. G. (2014).
Teachers’ instruction and students’ vocabulary and comprehension: An exploratory study
with English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual students in grades 3-5. Reading
Research Quarterly, 49(1), 31–60. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.63
Stahl, S. A., & Fairbanks, M. M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based
meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56(1), 72.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1170287
Stake, R. (2000). Case studies. In N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative
research (2nd ed., pp. 435–454). Sage.
178
Swanborn, M. S. L., & de Glopper, K. (1999). Incidental word learning while reading: A meta-
analysis. Review of Educational Research, 69(3), 261–285.
Taboada, A., & Rutherford, V. (2011). Developing reading comprehension and academic
vocabulary for English language learners through science content: A formative
experiment. Reading Psychology, 32(2), 113–157.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02702711003604468
Varelas, M., & Pappas, C. C. (2006). Intertextuality in read-alouds of integrated science-literacy
units in urban primary classrooms: Opportunities for the development of thought and
language. Cognition and Instruction, 24(2), 211–259.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci2402_2
Varelas, M., Pappas, C. C., & Rife, A. (2006). Exploring the role of intertextuality in concept
construction: Urban second graders make sense of evaporation, boiling, and
condensation. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(7), 637–666.
https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.20100
Vitale, M. R., & Romance, N. R. (2012). Using in-depth science instruction to accelerate student
achievement in science. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education,
10(2), 457–472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-011-9326-8
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Schribner, & E.
Souberman, Trans.). Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin, Trans.; Translation newly rev. and
edited). MIT Press.
Wade, S. E., & Reynolds, R. E. (1989). Developing metacognitive awareness. Journal of
Reading, 33(1), 10.
Wang, J., & Herman, J. (2006). Evaluation of Seeds of Science/Roots of Reading project:
Shoreline science and terrarium investigations [Data set]. CRESST, UCLA.
https://doi.org/10.1037/e644802011-001
Wanzek, J. (2014). Building word knowledge: Opportunities for direct vocabulary instruction in
general education for students with reading difficulties. Reading & Writing Quarterly,
30(2), 139–164. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2013.789786
Wasik, B. A., & Bond, M. A. (2001). Beyond the pages of a book: Interactive book reading and
language development in preschool classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93,
243–250.
Wasik, B. A., Bond, M. A., & Hindman, A. (2006). The effects of a language and literacy
intervention on Head Start children and teachers. Journal of Educational Psychology,
98(1), 63–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.63
179
Wasik, B. A., & Hindman, A. H. (2014). Understanding the active ingredients in an effective
preschool vocabulary intervention: An exploratory study of teacher and child talk during
book reading. Early Education and Development, 25(7), 1035–1056.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2014.896064
Wasik, B. A., Hindman, A. H., & Snell, E. K. (2016). Book reading and vocabulary
development: A systematic review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 37, 39–57.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.04.003
Watts, S. M. (1995). Vocabulary instruction during reading lessons in six classrooms. Journal of
Reading Behavior, 27(3), 399–424. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862969509547889
Windschitl, M., Thompson, J., Braaten, M., & Stroupe, D. (2012). Proposing a core set of
instructional practices and tools for teachers of science. Science Education, 96(5), 878–
903. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21027
Wise, C. N. (2019). Assessment and instruction for developing second graders’ skill in
ascertaining word meanings from context [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University
of Michigan.
Wright, T. S. (in press). A teacher’s guide to vocabulary development across the day (K-3).
Heinemann.
Wright, T. S. (2014). From potential to reality: Content-rich vocabulary and informational text.
The Reading Teacher, 67(5), 359.
Wright, T. S. (2018). Reading to learn from the start: The power of interactive read-alouds.
American Educator, 4, 18.
Wright, T. S., & Domke, L. M. (2019). The role of language and literacy in k-5 science and
social studies standards. Journal of Literacy Research.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1086296X18821141
Wright, T. S., & Gotwals, A. W. (2017). Supporting kindergartners’ science talk in the context of
an integrated science and disciplinary literacy curriculum. The Elementary School
Journal, 117(3), 513–537.
Wright, T. S., Gotwals, A. W., Anderson, B. E., & Edwards, K. D. (2017). Supporting classroom
discourse during primary grades science instruction: A review of research. Literacy
Research Association Annual Conference, Tampa, FL.
Wright, T. S., & Neuman, S. (2014). Paucity and disparity in kindergarten oral vocabulary
instruction. Journal of Literacy Research, 46(3), 330–357.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1086296X14551474
180
Zipke, M. (2007). The role of metalinguistic awareness in the reading comprehension of sixth
and seventh graders. Reading Psychology, 28(4), 375–396.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710701260615
Zipke, M. (2008). Teaching metalinguistic awareness and reading comprehension with riddles.
The Reading Teacher, 62(2), 128–137. https://doi.org/10.1598/RT.62.2.4
Zipke, M., Ehri, L. C., & Cairns, H. S. (2009). Using semantic ambiguity instruction to improve
third graders’ metalinguistic awareness and reading comprehension: An experimental
study. Reading Research Quarterly, 44(3), 300–321. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.44.3.4
Zipoli, R. P., Coyne, M. D., & McCoach, D. B. (2011). Enhancing vocabulary intervention for
kindergarten students: Strategic integration of semantically related and embedded word
review. Remedial and Special Education, 32(2), 131–143.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932510361262
181