INFORMATION TO U SERS This reproduction was made from a copy of a m anuscript sent to u s for publication and microfilming. While the m ost advanced technology h as been used to p h o ­ tograph and reproduce this m anuscript, the quality of the reproduction is heavily dependent upon the quality of the material submitted. Pages in any m anuscript may have indistinct print. In all cases the best available copy has been filmed. The following explanation of techniques is provided to help clarify notations which may appear on this reproduction. 1. M anuscripts may not always be complete. When it is not possible to obtain missing pages, a note appears to indicate this. 2. When copyrighted materials are removed from the m anuscript, a note ap­ pears to indicate this. 3. Oversize materials (maps, drawings, and charts) are photographed by sec­ tioning the original, beginning at the upper left hand com er and continu­ ing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each oversize page is also filmed as one exposure and is available, for a n additional charge, as a standard 35mm slide or in black and white paper format.* 4. Most photographs reproduce acceptably on positive microfilm or micro­ fiche b u t lack clarity on xerographic copies made from the microfilm. For a r additional charge, all photographs are available in black and w hite standard 35mm slide format. * *For more information about black and white slides or enlarged paper reproductions, please contact the Dissertations Customer Services Department. M ’IJmMBrsity Micronhns . International 8613277 Ching, Donna Rae CHILDREN’S PARTICIPATION IN FAMILY WORK Michigan State University University Microfilms International 300 N. Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Ml 48106 Ph.D. 1986 PLEASE NOTE: In all cases this material has been filmed in the best possible way from the available copy. Problems encountered with this document have been identified here with a check mark V 1. Glossy photographs or pages_____ 2. Colored illustrations, paper or print_______ 3. Photographs with dark backgrou nd_____ 4. Illustrations are poor copy_______ 5. Pages with black marks, not original copy 6. Print shows through as there is text on both sides of p a g e_______ 7. Indistinct, broken or small print on several pages 8. Print exceeds margin requirements______ 9. Tightly bound copy with print lost in spine_______ _____ 10. Computer printout pages with indistinct print_______ 11. Page(s)___________ lacking when material received, and not available from school or author. 12. Page(s) 13. Two pages numbered 14. Curling and wrinkled pages 15. 16. . seem to be missing in numbering only as text follows. . Text follows. Dissertation contains pages with print at a slant, filmed as received Other University Microfilms International CHILDREN'S PARTICIPATION IN FAMILY WORK by Donna Rae Ching A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan S ta te University in p a rtial fulfillm ent of the requirem ents for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 1985 ABSTRACT CHILDREN’S PARTICIPATION IN FAMILY WORK By Donna Rae Ching This descriptive study examined the perceived contributions to household work of children (of se lec te d ages and both sexes) and their perceived quality of life. The data w ere taken from a larger study whose random sample of 107 fam ilies represented urban, small town and rural locations in mid-Michigan. Each husband, w ife and oldest child (between the ages of 6 and 12) w ere given a se lf-a d m in iste red questionnaire. This sm aller study focused its analysis on the responses of the children from three sectio n s of th eir questionnaire. These data were analyzed to examine the child's perceived contribution to household work, the quantity and kinds of ta s k s performed by tne child and the child's perceived quality of life. S ta tis tic a l t e s t s used were analyis of variance, chi-square, z - t e s t and correlation. The boys and g irls in th is study did not d iffer significantly in their perceived contribution to the household work of the family. They did d iffer significantly in the number of ta s k s reported being performed. Boys w ere overrepresented in the group of low perform ers and underrepresented in the group of high performers. The reverse w as true for the sample of girls. The boys and g irls also differed significantly in the kinds of task s performed. Girls did more in-the-home work while boys worked more outside. Age groups differed significantly in perceived contributions to the household work of the family. Children's perceptions were highest at ages 6 and 7, peaking a t age 7. Their perceptions of th eir contributions decline from age 7 and increase again around ages 11 and 12. Older girls appeared to perform more task s than younger girls. The age of the boy did not appear to influence the number of ta s k s performed. Boys and g irls did not differ significantly in perceived quality of life, but age groups did. Younger children scored higher than older children on perceived quality of whole life scores. DEDICATED To Dr. Beatrice Paolucci As a role model, she is unsurpassed. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This d isse rta tio n w as only completed because of the support given by my family and friends. I think it only appropriate to acknowledge them here because of the important role they played in making th is work possible. The person who w as c lo se st to th is work and nurtured it during every stag e of i t s development was my m ajor professor and dissertatio n director, Dr. Margaret Bubolz. Her professionalism , love of knowledge and sincere concern for people have influence me tremendously. She was alw ays available to me when 1 needed help during the course of completing my doctoral program. All the members of my comm ittee, Mary Andrews, Irene Hathaway, Dennis Keefe and Michael Moore, also have my deepest gratitude. Each has provided invaluable support and contributed to my development as a professional. My friends and colleagues, Esther Fergus, Sandy Tsuneyoshi, Amy and Lynette lezzoni, w ere always a source of encouragement and, in many instances, they w ere willing to give of th eir tim e and energy wnen help was needed. In particular, Esther Fergus gave me the critical impetus I needed to complete the la s t chapters of th is dissertation. Of m ajor importance in the completion of th is endeavor were my baby sitters, Lou Ann Wilcox and Shun Lee Kam. They provided my son Harrison w ith a loving and nurturing environment and th is freed me to work on my d issertation. Finally, I would like to thank the members of my family who have nurtured me through th is en tire process. My parents have always been a source of emotional support and suffered w ith me during each and every ste p toward the completion of th is degree. The two m ost important people in my life, my husband Skip and son Harrison, have had to bear the brunt of my fru stra tio n when the work was not progressing as I desired. late dinners or They have put up w ith endless nights of dinners which were delivered. Harrison, who was used to having mommy on a regular basis, had to a d ju st to going to the b abysitter (during the last push to complete th is d isse rta tio n he went five days a week). Through all of this, they have both been loving and supportive, providing me w ith the kind of home environment which gave me the energy and desire to complete my work. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ix LIST OF FIGURES ' X Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION 1 S tatem ent of the Problem Purpose of the Study Research Questions Hypotheses Definitions Theoretical Definitions Operational Definitions Assumptions 1 5 5 6 7 7 7 8 2. THE DEVELOPMENTAL!ST APPROACH: A CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK Developmental i s t s Piaget Erikson Havighurst Super Role-Modeling from a Developmentalist Perspective Household Work Participation of School-Age Children from a Developmental ist Perspective Summary 3. REVIEW OF LITERATURE 9 10 11 11 12 13 15 17 20 21 Research on Household Work Studies w ith Children as a Seconday Focus Studies and Publications w ith Children as a Primary Focus v 22 22 26 Chapter Page 3. REVIEW OF LITERATURE (cont'd.) Rationale for Assigning Children Household Tasks Factors Affecting Children's Involvement in Household Work Summary Children and Work A Brief Historical Perspective on Work and Its Impact/ Influence on Children Work Participation of Children in Other Societies: The Importance of Early Training Summary The Firstborn Child Birth Order Research Focusing on the Firstborn Summary The E ffects of Sex, Sex Roles and Age on Children's Household Work Contributions Hypotheses Formulated Research on Quality of Life Quality of Life: Basic Concepts Studies on Children's Quality of Life Studies of Quality of School Life National Studies Hypotheses Formulated Summary 4. METHODOLOGY 26 30 35 35 35 37 39 39 39 42 42 50 51 51 52 53 54 55 55 57 Research Design Description of the Study Sample Summary Instrum ent Development Related L iterature Description of Variables Instrum entation and Scoring Perceived Quality of Life Perceived Contributions to Family Work Number of Tasks Performed vi 58 61 67 67 68 70 70 70 71 72 Page Chapter 4. METHODOLOGY (cont’d.) S ta tis tic a l Analysis Lim itations Limitations of Instrum ent 5. FINDINGS 72 74 75 76 Child's Perceived Contribution to Household Work Quantity and Kinds of Tasks Performed by the Child Child's Perceived Quality of Life 77 79 82 6. CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS 85 Overview of the Study Major Conclusions Discussion of Findings Implications of the Study Recommendations for Further Research 85 86 87 94 97 APPENDICES Appendix A A-1. Training Meeting 100 A-2. Introduction L etter 101 A-3. Consent Form 102 Appendix B B - 1. B-2. Demographic C h a rac te ristic s of Areas in Which Sampling Occurred 103 C lassification of A ttem pted Placement of Questionnaire by Location and Eligibility of Family 104 v ii Page Chapter APPENDICES (cont'd.) Appendix C C - 1. Part of Parents' Questionnaire Used in the Study C-2. 105 Part of the Children's Questionnaire Used in the Study 115 Appendix D D -1. Descriptive S t a ti s t i c s for Perceived Contributions to Family Work D-2. 119 Descriptive S t a ti s t i c s for Number of Tasks 120 Performed 121 BIBLIOGRAPHY v iii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Page Summary of Household Work Studies w ith Children as a Seconday Focus 23 2. C h a rac te ristic s of Families in the Sample 63 3. Summary of Sample by Age and Sex 74 4. Number of Tasks Performed by Boys and Girls 80 5. Percentage of Boys and Girls Performing Specific Household Tasks 81 D -1. D-2. Descriptive S t a t i s t i c s for Perceived Contributions to Family Work 119 Descriptive S t a ti s t i c s for Number of Tasks Performed 120 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Faces Scale 69 2. Child’s Perceived Contribution by Age 78 3. Child's Perceived Quality of Whole Life by Age 84 4. Child's Perceived Quality of Self Domain by Age 84 x Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION S tatem en t of Problem With today’s changing economic and social environment there is renewed concern w ith the fam ily’s productive capabilities. New emphasis is placed on th e importance of the family in the development and u tilization of human capital. The fam ily's productive capabilities have increased impact during tim e s when p articipation in the m arket is constrained or term inated. poten tiality of children Questions a re being raised about the being identified as important economic resources because of the work they do inside and outside the home. In e arlie r tim es, and s t i ll today in many p a rts of the world, children are and have been im portant economic assets. In contemporary American society, however, work done by children in th e home has more often been seen as an opportunity to practice sk ills and learn values which will be useful to them in the paid work force. But, children's work is more than that. The chores perform ed by children contribute to th e ir repertoire of household ta s k s which they can tap for la te r roles in th e ir own families. Not only are the child’s work experiences important, the meaning of th at activity for the child and family is also important. Parents give several common reasons fo r assigning children chores (White and Brinkerhoff, 1981): 1 2 1. Doing household task s will build c h arac ter while developing responsibility and competence. 2. Children have a duty to do household ta s k s to help the family because working together is p a rt of being a family. 3. Having household parents need help. 4. In the long run, children need to know how to do these tasks. 5. Chores give the child an opportunity to earn an allowance or do something to keep occupied. respo n sib ilities is necessary because When a child is old enough to contribute to the fam ily's w ell-being by participating in household work, th a t child learns and becomes a functioning part of the group. This is especially true when the learning occurs w ith people the child loves and admires. Performing work 1 the home which is challenging and reinforced by parents who care enables a child to develop both ability and identity. Children acquire new in te re s ts and skills, and learn the meaning of competence, cooperation and responsibility. These are all necessary components if children are to assum e productive and supportive ro les w ithin the family and in the paid work force. When parents and older siblings allo cate th e ir tim e and s k ills in building the human resources of younger family members, th is is viewed as an investm ent in human capital and the role of the family and i ts members as one of production. Changes in society demand an increase in children's participation in family work w ith a resulting in te re s t in studying the e ffe c t of th ese changes. The m ost obvious change is the dram atic movement of women into the labor force. Equally significant is the increase of single parent fam ilies. Both of these changes re s u lt in the phenomenon oi 3 "latch key children” i.e. children who have access to th eir homes w ithout a supervising parent present. Not only are these children responsible for them selves and, som etim es, younger s i s t e r s and brothers while th e ir parents are working, in many instances, they provide the vital serv ices th a t are necessary to m aintain a household. They may be responsible for the bulk of the cooking and cleaning as well as having access to the fam ily's economic resources so th a t they are making important consumer decisions in the marketplace. Participation of children in family work is also important because the s ta tu s of children has changed over time. Prior to the industrial revolution when the m ajority of the population in the U. S. lived in rural areas, there w as no question th a t every individual w as a worker in the family. Families c o n stituted economic units and all members played important productive roles w ithin the household (Keniston, 1977). Most boys worked alongside th e ir fa th e rs in the field and g irls contributed by a ssistin g th eir m others in household work. Working for and w ith th e ir fam ilies, children w ere viewed as economic a s s e t s who were being socialized for future adult roles. Men, women and children developed and maintained feelings of competence through th e ir contributions to family work. Changes began occurring in the nineteenth century. On the farm, the production of cash crops provided a higher standard of living and replaced s e lf-s u ffic ie n t agriculture. In c itie s, the industrial revolution created a situ atio n where w orkers (especially fathers) had to leave th e ir homes to work in factories. Changes occurred gradually and were uneven. At the turn of the century, many children as young as 7 or 8 were s till found working in the family business (i.e. family sto re or farm) as well as in the home. In m ost cases though, the economic "value" of 4 children to th e ir fam ilie s had changed. With industrialization, a more skilled and educated labor force w as needed, thus increasing the years of formal schooling. Childhood w as prolonged; th is increased both the number of years a child remained dependent on parents and a financial drain on the family. Instead of being viewed as an a sse t, children had become an enormous economic liability (Keniston, 1977). This increased dependence and an increase in the family's dependence on goods and se rv ic e s obtained outside the family system have created a situ a tio n where some children have no specific role in the family. Their dependence on the family has increased while a t the same tim e th e ir contributions to family functioning have decreased. These feelings of lack of competence have been used as a p artial explanation of the alienation, irrespon sibility and conflict w ith parents seen in some of today's adolescents. It is th is concern w ith the mental health of children which has resulted in quality of life stu d ies on children. These studies are important and useful because they measure childrens' perceptions of well-being. These perceptions are valuable tools when developing baseline m easures which can be compared to subsequent m easures and trends of change so th a t society has some indication of the well-being of its children. It is also valuable to a ttem p t to understand how children evaluate and feel about th e ir lives. At the present time, those indicators utilized to m easure adults' perceptions of w ell-being are being adapted and used for children. 5 Purpose of the Study The purpose of th is descriptive study is to identify perceived contributions to household work of children of se lec te d ages and both sexes. Specifically, the number and kinds of ta s k s performed will be identified. Do the children in the sam ple adhere to traditional sex roles when choosing ta s k s ? Does age play a sig nifican t role in the number and kinds of ta s k s children perform ? One fu rth e r ste p is taken by asking the child to e stim a te th a t percentage of all household work for which they are responsible. In addition, the study will examine the child's perceived quality of life. A global measure of quality of life w ill be compared to the quality of different life domains. Will sco res of boys d iffe r significantly from the scores of g irls? Is age a fac to r in determining the child's perceived quality of life? This study will also a ttem p t to determ ine if a relationship e x is ts between the child's perceived contribution to household work and perceived quality of life. If a relationship e x ists, do age and sex have an a ffe c t on it? Research Questions This study focused on three a re as of interest: the child's perceived contribution to household work, the quantity and kinds of tasks performed by the child and the child's perceived quality of life. For these three areas, the following questions w ere posed: 1. What is the child's perceived contribution to the household work of the fam ily? Are there any differences: a. In perceptions of boys and g irls ? b. Between age groups? 6 2. How many ta s k s are being performed by each child? Are th ere any differences betw een the number of ta s k s boys and g irls perform ? Are traditional sex role stereo ty pes adhered to when ta s k s are se le c te d ? Are older chi’dren performing more ta s k s than younger children? 3. What is the child’s perceived quality of life ? differences: a. Are there any In the sco res of boys and g irls? b. Between the age groups? 4. Is there a relationship between: a. The child's perceived contribution to household work and the number of ta s k s performed? Are there differences between boys and g irls ? b. The child’s perceived contribution to household work and per­ ceived quality of life? c. The number of ta s k s performed and perceived quality of life ? Hypotheses The conceptual framework (discussed in chapter 2) used in th is study w as based on a developmental i s t approach. Hypotheses w ere formed on the f i r s t three research questions using th is conceptual framework and the review of literature. Because of the paucity of research on children’s quality of life, it w as not possible to develop hypotheses for question four. 1. Boys and g irls will d iffer in perceived contribution to the household work of the family. The perceived contribution of g irls will be higher than th a t of boys. 2. Age groups will differ in perceived contribution to the household work of the family. The perceived contribution will increase w ith age. 7 3. Boys and g irls will d iffer in the number of ta s k s performed. Girls w ill perform mor e ta s k s than boys. 4. Boys and g irls will d iffer in the kinds of ta s k s performed. Girls will do more in-the-hom e work while boys will work more outside. 5. Age groups w ill d iffer in the number of ta s k s performed. Older children w ill perform more ta s k s than younger children. 6. Boys and g irls will not d iffer in perceived quality of life. 7. Age groups will not differ in perceived quality of life. Definitions This section includes theoretical and operational definitions of concepts th a t are relevant to th is study. Theoretical Definitions Family.—"A bonded unit of interacting and interdependent persons who have some common goals and resources, and for part of th e ir life cycle, a t least, share living space" (Andrews, e t al., 1980, pg. 32). Family and household in th is study are used synonymously. Household work.—These are nonpaid a c tiv itie s performed by and for household members for use in the home th a t fa c ilita te the functioning of the household and provide for the w ell-being of household members. Household work is p a rt of household production (Deacon and Firebaugh, 1981; Walker and Woods, 1976). Quality of life.—Rettig (1980), using the works of Dalkey and Rourke and Mitchell e t al., defines quality of life as "A person's sense of well being, s a tis fa c tio n or d issa tis fa c tio n w ith life, or unhappiness or happiness....An individual's overall perceived sa tis fa c tio n of needs over a period of time...." (pg. 17). 8 Operational Definitions Fam ily.--Consists of a husband and w ife living together w ith at lea st one child between the ages of 6 and 12. Household work.—Evaluation of child’s perceived household respo n sib ilities in response to questions 25 to 47 of the questionnaire. Household work includes the specific a c tiv itie s of: personal and animal care; yard, lawn and other outdoor work; child care; food preparation and afte r-m e al cleanup; house care; clothing care and marketing. Quality of Life.—Evaluation by child of th e ir whole life, th eir family, them selves and the amount of work they do (questions 8 to 11 in the questionnaire). Assumptions The assum ptions underlying th is research are: 1. Children can accurately evaluate resp o nsibilities for household work. 2. Children can accurately about quality of life. and report evaluate and report th e ir th eir feelings 3. The fac es scale for quality of life yields numerical responses th a t can be tre a te d as interval data. 4. Quality of life can be a sse ssed directly by asking children about th e ir family activities. 5. R esponsibilities for household work can be a sse ssed d irectly by asking children about th e ir life and family. 6. Children can accurately report th e ir cognition and feeling sta te s. 7. Perceptions re fle c t the rea lity of the situ atio n for the child. Chapter 2 THE DEVELOPMENTAL!ST APPROACH: A CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK The conceptual framework of th is study is based upon the developm entalist approach. The developm entalists seek to estab lish the existence of basic ag e-rela te d patterns. They s t r e s s the regular and cumulative a sp e c ts of the growth process which is marked by sequential sta g es of increasing competence. When th e ir approach is applied to household work participation, it helps to explain the contributions of children to household work a t differen t ages. As a resu lt, it is utilized generally to form the foundation upon which this study is based and specifically in the development of hypotheses 2 and 5. This discussion of the conceptual framework is divided into three sections. The f i r s t covers the work of four th e o ris ts who have contributed significantly to the understanding of the developmental i s t approach. The second section d iscusses role-modeling (or w hat Goldstein and Oldham called "role learning") from a developmental i s t perspective. The focus is on th is aspect of the socialization process because it is particularly important as children learn to work. And finally, section three u tiliz e s the developmental i s t perspective to examine the household work participation of school-age children. In th is section, several p a tte rn s of growth are identified which have the potential to influence a child's participation in household work. 9 10 Developmentalists The f i r s t section of th is chapter will a ttem p t to summarize Goldstein and Oldham's (1979) discussion of the developmental ist approach because, like them, th is research er finds it appropriate as a conceptual framework. Socialization is broadly defined in Leichter's (1977) book as the "process by which individuals acquire the knowledge, skills, and dispositions th at enable them to participate as more or less effective members of groups and so ciety ” (p.6). Goldstein and Oldham believe that the process has at le a st four c h aracteristics: 1. 2. 3. 4. It is in teractiv e, i.e., it involves the actions, reactions, and reinforcem ents of persons other than the one being socialized. It is d irected a t the conferring of an identity of some sort. It involves role learning, the imparting of a range of mutual expectations a ssociated w ith performance of one or more social roles. It is in trinsically connected w ith social control, since it provides the b a sis for predictability w ithin the group and is the vehicle through which both positive and negative sanctions are made meaningful to group members, (p. 15) For th e ir purposes, developmental ism refers to a particular approach to socializatio n which emphasizes, in the words of Donald Super, the progressive increase and modification of the individual’s behavioral rep erto ire through growth and learning...marked by sequential sta g e s of increasing competence, (p. 36) The regular and cumulative a sp ec ts of the growth process are stre sse d by th e o ris ts in th is group. Like th is researcher, they seek to estab lish the existence of basic ag e-related patterns. The normative approach is utilized by these th e o ris ts who are concerned w ith ascertaining what the growth norms are and the conditions under which variations occur. 11 Several of the better-know n developm entalists whose works are deemed relevant to th is study will be examined. Piaget Jean Piaget (1959) w as a S w iss psychologist who devoted his c areer to the study of child development. He viewed all intellectual development in term s of adaptation to one's environment. consisted of two processes: Adaptation assim ilatio n and accommodation. He s tr e s s e d the importance of play and im itation to the child's assim ilation and accommodation e ffo rts. The developmental process involved the constant search for equilibrium between w hat the child understands and the remainder of th eir experiences w ith the environment. Piaget also contributed a theory of stages. Development from birth to adulthood w as divided into four generic periods. The chronological age a t which the child passed through each stag e w as not the important thing for the pace may vary but the sequence rem ains constant. The period of concrete operations w as the third in his developmental schema and it covers roughly years 7 through 11 (the sample in th is study fa lls into th is range). During th is period, Piaget suggested th a t language sk ills develop and the child's fac ility for mental operations become increasingly sophisticated. Erikson Erik Piaget's. Erikson's (1963) bent w as much more psychoanalytic than Erikson developed a se ries of psychosocial sta g e s through which the developing child passed. Like Piaget's work, sequence was emphasized instead of chronology. Each sta g e represented a critical encounter between child and environment. 12 Of his eight psychosocial stages, the third and fourth typically encompass roughly the years five through preadolescence (the age of th is sample of children). The third or locomotorgenital period is the time when the child develops initiative, "the quality of undertaking, planning, and 'attacking' a task for the sake of being active and on the move....'' The child can gradually develop a sense of moral responsibility...gain insights into the institutions, functions, and roles which perm it his responsible participation...find pleasurable accomplishment in wielding tools and weapons, in manipulating toys, and in caring for younger children, (pp. 2 55-2 56) The latency stage follow s th is period and the child "must begin to be a worker and potential provider." By the age of 10, the child will ideally overcome feelings of inferiority while developing a "sense of industry.” He has the potential to become an eager and absorbed unit of a productive situation. The child of th is age comes to appreciate work completion and diligence, internalizing the work principle. Fam iliarity w ith tools and u te n sils helps the child develop a "sense of division of labor and of differential oppportunity" (pp.258-260). Havighurst Unlike Piaget and Erikson, Robert Havighurst (1964) has been in te reste d in the developmental process specifically as it re la te s to work. He conceives of vocational development as a life long process divisible into six stages. The f i r s t tw o are relevant to th is study since they include children from 5 to 15 years of age. Havighurst's f i r s t stage includes children from 5 to 10 years of age and it is th a t of identification w ith a worker. The child's identification w ith p aren ts and other significant persons ordinarily assu res th at "the concept of working becomes an e ssential p art of of the ego ideal" (p. 13 216). The developmental ta s k s of children of th is age group are: 1. the development of fundamental sk ills in reading, writing, and calculating; 2. learning the physical s k ills necessary fo r games; 3. learning to get along w ith age-m ates; 4. learning appropriate sex roles; 5. developing the concepts necessary for daily life; 6. developing conscience, a sense of morality, and values; and 7. achieving personal independence, (pp. 2 2 1 -2 2 2 ) The focus sh if ts for the child in the 10 to 15 y ear old bracket to acquiring the basic h abits of industry. The child m ust learn to do school work and chores and effectively allocate tim e and energy. During th is stage, the child learns the conditions under which it is appropriate to put work before play. Super The work of Donald Super (1957) in te g rate s the work of Ginzberg and others in his theory of vocational development. heavily upon Havighurst's notion of developmental He also re lie s tasks. Like Havighurst, he se e s the process as life-long and tr a c e s it through five sta g e s to a period called decline which encom passes retirem ent. Only the f i r s t or growth sta g e (birth to age 14) is relevant for th is study. Super w r ite s of th is period, Self concept develops through identification w ith key figures in family and in school; needs and fantasy are dormant early in th is stage; in te re s t and capacity becomes more important in th is stage w 4th increasing social participation and re a lity - te s tin g (p.40) Utilizing Ginzberg's theory, the growth stage is subdivided in three sub-stages: fantasy, in te re s t and capacity. The ages 4 through 10 cover the fantasy period during which role playing is very important. It is la te r (ages 11 to 12), during brief periods of in te re s ts , th at the child's 14 "likes" and "dislikes" come to the forefront and serve as m ajor determ inants of aspirations and activities. At about 13 years of age children en ter the capacity stage where they begin to take th e ir a b ilitie s into account and to weigh job requirem ents in thinking of future career directions. Super's theory is alm ost entirely couched in term s of vocational m aturity (development is measured largely in term s of how "mature" the behavior is in relation to age peers) and its two-dimensional chronological standard. age norms application u tiliz e s a This standard takes into account both and the individual's performance of developmental tasks, regardless of w hether they are confronted at the appropriate age. To a s s e s s m aturity, Super has liste d "vocational developmental tasks" which he considers are appropriate a t d ifferent life stages. For example, according to Super th is sample of elem entary school children m ust m a s te r the abilities: (1) to undertake cooperative enterprises; (2) to choose a c tiv itie s which su it individual abilities; (3) to assume responsibility for th eir actions; and (4) to perform household chores. The works of these developm entalists are used by th is research er for the conceptual framework for the present study. The key m essage of the developmental ist traditio n is th a t behavioral changes which occur over tim e in the maturing individual are best understood in the context of regular and meaningful p a ttern s of growth. In th is study, the works of these developm entalists serve as valuable guideposts in the interpretation of the findings. They are also utilized in the formulation of hypothesis 2 which p redicts an increase in perceived contributions as the children get older and in the formulation of hypothesis 5 to predict 15 an increase in the number of task s performed as the children get older. Role-Modeling from a Developmental is t Perspective Stephens (1963) makes a strong case for learning to work through ro le- modeling. The children in his c ro ss-c u ltu ral study go through a natural sequence learning to work. He says they s t a r t as sp e c ta to rs and then begin im itating adults' work in th e ir play (playing dolls, house, cook, farm er, herdsman and hunter). Then they are allowed to join in and help. Between the ages of 3 and 6 years, the work-apprenticeship usually begins. By puberty, children's work is usually sim ila r to adults'. The early apprenticeship tak e s advantage of young children's eagerness to be in on what the grown-ups are doing and to im itate what the grown-ups do. F irst they w atch and play a t it; then, bit by bit, they are allowed to do real work. Stephens (1979) fee ls th a t th is is the ideal arrangement for role-model ing—im itating a model's behavior, and as a resu lt, acquiring behavior dispositions th a t tend to stay w ith the person as they grow up. His central idea about role-modeling is th a t the two p ro ce sses—modeling and learning to work in the home—come together, and each one f a c ilita te s the other. work-apprenticeship is made This is true when the traditional possible in the home. If the learning-to-w ork pro cess can begin during the child's period of readiness (sta rtin g at about the age of 3), and it is not too badly mismanaged by the parent, then it all flow s together. The child learns home-connected work, graduates to more mature respon sibilities, is positively m otivated in th is area, and models a fte r the parent, (p.63) 16 One of the possible e ff e c ts of role-modeling is the tra c e s of the parent, the lasting influences, th a t grown-up children carry w ith them. Another is the continuing emulation of the parent, a carry-over of the early desire to im itate and be like the role model. In addition, there is continuing m otivation to do the work. He believes th a t if the combined processes of role-modeling and leam ing-to-w ork are successful, then the outcome should be: * An older child, teenager, young adult, who is a willing worker in the home, who re ta in s his w illingness to p a rticip a te in the work and help; * Who s till w ants to work alongside the parent; * And who continues to want to do the so rt of work around the home th a t w as learned in childhood. Stephens recognizes th at role-modeling and w ork-apprenticeships are not easy to provide in modern homes. With both parents in the m ajority of fam ilies working away from home, the problem is often the lack of an adult in the house for the child to model. Few homes provide children w ith a se rie s of ta s k s appropriate to th e ir age and s t a r t them on th is line of development. He does believe th a t the early role-modeling in the home for some g irls has been less interfered w ith (as a resu lt of modern conditions) than has boys'. In his sample, a number of g irls tell s to r ie s like this: a stra ig h t-lin e development from early-childhood in te re sts, wanting to be like mother and wanting to p a rtic ip a te in her work, to mother's helper, child-care duties, b ab y -sittin g jobs, an ambition to be a mother herself, or a nurse or teacher, and finally a realization of th a t goal. (p. 55 ) For fa th e rs and sons there is less opportunity fo r this. The line of development is different, it is not a s tra ig h t-lin e development into a lifework and an occupational self. 17 Developmentalists believe th a t in the maturing individual behavioral changes which occur over tim e are b est understood in the context of regular and meaningful p a tte rn s of growth. In his cross-cu ltu ral study, Stephens has actually identified some of these p a tte rn s of growth as they re la te to children learning to work through role-modeling. He recognizes that role-modeling and w ork-apprenticeships are not easy to provide in modern homes and th a t girls appear to have a b e tte r opportunity for role-modeling than boys. If th is is the case, then the g irls in the present study may differ from the boys in the level of participation in household work. Household Work Participation.af. School-Age Children from_a D e v e l o p m e n t a l Perspective When assigning work to children, parents are often told it is im portant to choose ta s k s th a t are appropriate to the child’s age. How does a parent know w hat the child should be able to do and what is too much to expect? The preceding discussion on the developm entalists does shed some light on task selectio n as it re la te s to parental expectations. Developmentalists believe th a t behavioral changes occur over tim e in the maturing child and are b est understood in the context of regular and meaningful p a tte rn s of growth. A brief review of the lite ra tu re on school-age children should help to identify some of the p a ttern s of growth which could influence a child's participation in household work. Although in some instances ages will be suggested, they are merely used as guideposts. Gesell, Arnold, tig and Ames' (1977) book on The Child from Five to l e a is a standard te x t which has been revised several times. In it they 18 discuss the school-age child's relationship w ith th e ir home and family. A p a ttern clearly develops which may have an impact on the child's participation in household work: During the sixth y ear he gives many evidences of forging to a higher level of relationships, even through a t tim e s he seem s self-cen tered , re s is ta n t, or overly m other-centered. He takes a new kind of in te r e s t in family outings,...Seven in his l ittle serious way has a deepened sense of the family as an institution; he is proud of his home and fam ily possessions; even his negative behavior betrays an emotional strengthening of the family ties. Eight is som ewhat less subjective; he is in terested in the fam ily as a going concern,...Nine likes to be on his own, likes to be w ith his friends and away from his family. It gives him a growing sense of self-sufficiency....The steady processes of growth have wrought extraordinary changes in his family relationships since the innocence of five. During the teen s there will be another s e rie s of significant transform ations; but the basic o rien tatio ns are well-nigh complete by the age of ten. (p. 322) The contributions of children to household work a t several d ifferent ages are also discussed in th e ir book and it becomes apparent th at participation is influenced by the child's relationship to their family. They describe the 7 year old as beginning to be thoughtful, to be considerate and anxious to please. The child w ants to find their place in the family group and responsibilities. is ready to take some of the household Many like to help and often take on certain routine chores such as emptying w a steb a sk e ts, cutting the lawn, making th e ir beds, helping w ith the dishes, picking up th e ir room and running errands. Sometimes th is help is sp otty because the child will tir e of one chore and w ishes to s h ift to another. Eight year olds p refer to do jobs they think of themselves. New and more responsible jobs are attacked w ith real in te re s t while old jobs (such as washing dishes) are disliked. Nine 19 and ten year olds are even le ss involved w ith routine chores a s the identification w ith the juvenile group promotes the complex process of detachment from the dom estic family group. Turnure (1975) is in te reste d in cognitive development *and role-taking ability in boys and g irls from 7 to 12 y ears of age. findings indicate th a t the child's ability to "decenter" or Her sh ift perspectives is an important asp ect of cognitive development and it increases w ith age regardless of whether the child is involved w ith a "social" or a "physical" task. The ability to decenter, to sh ift perspectives, may be an important f i r s t step preceding a child's active involvement in the work of the home. As a child m atures, their relationship to th e ir family changes over tim e and can be understood in the context of regular and meaningful p a ttern s of growth. Some of th ese p a ttern s are introduced in th is section and th e ir impact on the participation of children in household work is discussed. In the present study, these p a tte rn s will probably influence the kinds and number of task s undertaken by children in different age groups. Summary The conceptual developmental is t framework approach. of The this study basic is based assum ption on of the the developmental is t trad ition is th a t behavioral changes which occur over tim e in the maturing individual are best understood in the context of regular and meaningful p a ttern s of growth. In the f i r s t section, p a ttern s were identified as they rela te to children learning to work through role-modeling. In the final section of the chapter, another growth p attern w as identified which describes the changes which occur in the 20 relationship of the child to the fam ily as the child matures. These p a tte rn s impact upon the participation of children in th e ir household work contributions. Chapter 3 REVIEW OF LITERATURE This chapter is divided into five d istin c t sections. The f i r s t three sections are devoted to lite ra tu re related to the study of children’s participation in family work. Sections four and five contain a review of the lite ra tu re which resulted specifically in hypothesis development. The f i r s t section of th is chapter contains household work stu d ies or stud ies in which household work is discussed as one aspect of the research. In the f i r s t part of th is section, the primary focus is on the homemaker and children are included secondarily in a category along w ith other workers. Children and th e ir contributions to household work are the primary focus of p art two. These stud ies and publications provide broad categories of information against which some comparisons may be made. The second section of th is chapter provides a general discussion of children and work and is also divided into tw o parts. A brief historical perspective on work and i ts impact and influence on children is provided in part one of th is section. In the second part, work participation of children in other so c ie tie s is explored focusing on the importance of early training. Because the sample utilized in th is study is made up of firstborn children, section three of the f i r s t chapter contains a very short discussion on the birth order research focusing on the firstborn. 21 22 The fourth section of th is chapter focuses on the e ff e c ts of sex, sex roles and age on children's household work contributions. A review of th is lite ra tu re a s s is te d in the formulation of hypotheses 1 through 5. Quality of life stu d ie s are included in the fifth section of th is chapter. The small number of th ese studies which focus on children are highlighted and the development of hypotheses 6 and 7 discussed. Research on Household Work Studies w ith Children a s a Secondary Focus "Household work is indispensable to the functioning of the family and society" (Walker and Gauger, 1973). Walker and Gauger recognize the importance of household work but they also realize that, because it occurs outside the context of the business world, tim e spent in th is endeavor is not normally given a dollar value. Gauger (1973) believes th a t the failure to recognize the dollar value of household work in a sense denigrates the work of one half of the population who are m arried and bear the m ajor share of household work. Many studies have been done to discover who does the work in the home and what variables a ffe c t work participation (i.e. number of children, employment of homemaker, age of children, etc.). The Federal Bureau of Home Economics of the United S ta te s Department of Agriculture from as early as the late 1920's sponsored a number of stu d ies on household work tim e of women. Many stu d ies have been done since then and some of them reported on the tim e spent by children in household work. Most of th ese studies have been included in extensive Lynch (1975). review s of lite ra tu re by Steidl and Bratton (1968) and Relevant stu d ies from those review s as well as more 23 current research have been summarized in Table 1. When possible, the age of the children being studied w a s included. Table 1.— Summary of Household Work Studies with Children as a Secondary Focus Study* Tasks Helpers and Help Received Wilson (1 9 2 9 ) Dishwashing, care of fires, food purchasing Contributions of grade school-age children: Town 3.3 hours/week Farm 3.3 hours/week Contributions of high school-age children: Town 4.1 hours/week Farm 5.0 hours/week Arnquist and Roberts (1 9 2 9 ) Food preparation, aftermeal clean up, washing and ironing, care of family Farm children under 6 years: Girls 1.3 hours/week Boys 1.0 hour /w eek Farm children 1 5 -2 0 years old: Girls 13.8 hours/week Boys 1.7 hours/week Muse (1 9 4 6 ) Girls helped prim arily with care of house, dishwashing, food preparation, child care and care of their own clothing Boys most often carried water, built fires and filled lamps and stove tanks 6658 of farm girls from 8 to 17 years spent between 10 and 35 hours/week 8858 of farm boys from 8 to 17 years spent 5 hours/week Wiegand (1 9 5 4 ) Dishwashing was task in which most helpers parti­ cipated Employed homemaker: .6 hours/week from all helpers in household Unemployed homemakers: .3 hours/week from all helpers in household Roberts (1 9 5 6 ) Cleaning living room 3058 of homemakers had helpers for last general cleaning; helpers averaged 1 hour for in-between cleaning Knoll (1 9 5 7 ) Chores, gardening, dish5058 of city children contribute washing, care of house, clothing and family members, ♦researchers listed in chronological order 24 Table 1. (cont'd.) Study Tasks Helpers and Help Received driving, food preparation McCandless (1 9 5 9 ) Day-to-day care of house Averaged. 1 hour on day before interview; 5 0 $ employed, 2 0 $ nonemployed home­ makers received help; help given in 3 3 $ of households with children under 6 years, 6 6 $ with teenagers Dickens (1 9 6 1 ) Food preparation or dishwashing 5 0 $ to 7 5 $ of homemakers received help; averaged 39 to 52 minutes Hook (1 9 6 3 ) Regular care of house 6 0 $ of families with children received help averaging 2 hours/week; 5 0 $ of rural and urban families had helpers; 2 5 $ of total time for care of house r u r a l, 2 0 $ for urban de Fonseca (1 9 6 4 ) Dishwashing Averaged 2 hours/week, about 3 0 $ of total dishwashing time; help on 7 0 $ of days, with or without dishwasher; related to number and age of children Carlson (1 9 6 5 ) Washing 15$ of homemakers shared responsibility Purcell (1 9 6 5 ) Washing 2 0 $ of 444 weekly records showed help; averaged 43 minutes/week; helpers' time 4 $ of total time for washing Walker and Nordenstedt (1 9 6 6 ) All tasks 8 3 $ of 6 to 11 year old girls spent 45 minutes/day in household work 8 8 $ of 6 to 11 year old boys spent 45 minutes/day in household work 9 6 $ of 12 to 17 year old girls spent 1.5 hours/day in household work 6 9 $ of 12 to 17 year old boys spent 35 minutes/day in household work Gitobu (1 9 7 2 ) All tasks Very little difference between amount of time contributed by 6 to 11 year olds in families with employed and nonemployed homemakers 25 Table 1. (cont'd.) Study Tasks Helpers and Help Received Food preparation 12 to 17 year olds: families w / employed homemaker.8 hour/day families w / nonemployed homemaker.4 hour/day contributed an average of .2 hour/day more if mothers employed contributed an average of .4 hour/day less if mothers employed Housecare Marketing and management tasks Walker and Woods Regular meal preparation, (1 9 7 6 ) after-meal clean up, regular house care 6 to 11 year olds contributed an average of 1 hour/day to housework: families w/employed homemaker1.0 hour/day families w/nonemployed homemaker1.1 hour/day Teenagers contributed an average of 2 hours/day to housework: families w/employed homemaker2.2 hours/day families w/nonemployed homemaker2.0 hours/day It is apparent when reviewing the studies in Table 1 th a t women, w hether employed or unemployed, s till do the lion’s share of the work in the household. A tendency toward the traditional division of labor in household task s, w ith w ives doing m ost of the in-the-hom e work and husbands doing the yard work, home maintenance and recording-keeping, also s till e x is ts (Blood and Wolfe, I960; Walker, 1970b). When a reallocation of task s is apparent, often w ith husbands taking over a larger sh are of w hat normally is regarded as "women’s work," th is may actually be counter-productive. Oakley (1972) su ggests th a t the widespread p a tte rn of men helping out a bit, particularly m iddle-class 26 husbands, may actually indicate th a t w ives are le ft w ith a less enjoyable mixture of dom estic a c tiv itie s (those which tend to be universally disliked—especially cleaning). This research er wonders if those task s which are le a st liked are being delegated more often to today's children. If th is is the case, does it influence th eir perception of th e ir contribution to household work and th e ir perceived quality of life? Studies and Publications w ith Children as a Primary Focus Rationale for A ssig ning Children Household Tasks. At the turn of the century when children s till made a significant contribution to the work of the home, the reasons for th e ir working w ere relatively clear. McKeever (1913) said th a t children work: (I) for the sake of th eir physical growth, (2) for th eir moral character, and (3) for th e ir general discipline. He suggested th at poor adjustm ent r e s u lts when a child is not required to work in the home. He also acknowledged th a t it w as the responsibility of the school, as well as the parents, to ...teach and explain and exalt common work. Pupils are now to be taught early in life, for example, how to do plain housework, ordinary home chores, and how to lay hold upon the heavier industrial pursuits, as the s t a tu s of th e ir years and streng th may warrant, (p. 138) A 1949 publication on children from 6 to 12 by the U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare has much broader reasons for children working. The publication s tr e s s e d th a t it w as each child's obligation to contribute to the life of th e ir home because they had to learn to be givers as well as takers. With these work experiences, the child learned cooperation and w as b e tte r prepared for th e ir future family role as well as th e ir role in the larger community. At th a t time, it w as also believed 27 th a t giving children the happiness of feeling th a t they are useful members of society, through contributions to the family community, was one of the m ost powerful preventives of juvenile delinquency. Harris and Clark (1954) w ere in terested in "The Relationship of Children's Home Duties to an A ttitude of Responsibility." In a review of the lite ra tu re, they discovered th a t parents give children ta s k s around the house in training fo r independence, dependability or responsibility. Their research indicated th at l it t le evidence e x is ts to prove th a t routine ta s k s are asso ciated w ith an a ttitu d e of responsibility. A pamphlet w ritte n in 1955 by Osborne attem pted to aid parents in teaching th e ir children about work. He f e lt work w as important because: 1. Work experience can bring a feeling of personal significance 2. 3. 4. and a sense of achievement th a t comes in no other way. Work alongside grown-ups can add m aterially to the feeling of friendliness toward them and acceptance of them. Only through actual participation in real work activity can the young person come to appreciate what work means in the lives of human beings. A varied work experience may lay the foundation for a more intelligent choice of vocation, (p. 6) For Osborne, work w as held to have a certain kind of moral value. He fe lt that, If we believe in the dignity and w orth of labor and expect our children to believe in the democratic ideal, we m ust provide the experiences th a t will perm it future generations to develop a constructive a ttitu d e tow ards work. (p. 28) In a la te r pamphlet, Neisser (1957) recognized th at household jobs may not in them selves develop responsibility, but she f e lt that contributing to the life of the family added to a child's s e lf-r e s p e c t and dem onstrated th a t everybody b enefits when everybody does th e ir share. She f e lt it w as important to assign jobs which were clearly essential, 28 th a t would make sense to the child and th a t challenged th e ir skill and inventiveness, as well as th e ir ste ad fa stn ess. Jobs w ith these kinds of qualities would supplement the atm osphere of responsibility in which the child lives and would be a fu rth er aid to the cultivation of reliability. These pamphlets gave paren ts an indication of the positive outcomes of assigning children household tasks. Dr. Spock (1962), on the other hand, took a som ew hat d ifferen t perspective. He said th a t if you don’t train children to be helpers, you w ere apt to produce children: who are se lf-c en te red , demanding...adults who in th e ir jobs and in th e ir m arriages expect to be pleased and favored. They have little aw areness of w hat needs doing, w hether on a picnic or in the office....Every teach er in nursery school and elementary school has learned th a t children will develop an increasing sense of responsibility from helping her and the class; and they won’t if they don't, (pp. 4 6 -4 7 ) Dr. Spock f e lt th a t if children do not learn to help, they do not develop into helpful, considerate, unselfish and responsible people. Stephens (1979) noted th a t th is hypothesis based on Spock’s observations w as obviously oversimplified, but he found support for it in the Whitings’ (1975) research as well as his own case studies. The U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare updated th eir publication on children from 6 to 12 in 1966. Listed in the chapter on "Family Work and Money’’ w ere several reasons why children should work. By working in the home children: (1) learn necessary skills, (2) release parents from some of the burdens of household work and (3) get the feeling of doing th e ir share as they develop a ttitu d e s of responsibility and self-discipline. The view w as expressed th a t if a child learned to be a ’’responsible employee" early in life, th a t child would be more likely to develop into a valued fu ll-tim e employee when older. 29 Sm art and Sm art (1977) in th eir book, Children, echoed some of the previous authors in th e ir lis t of reasons why children should work. They f e lt th a t significant work experiences enhanced a child's sense of adequacy and enabled the child to make progress in the development of a sense of industry. "Real work" also would contribute to th eir understanding of adults, th e ir family and society. Based on his own case studies, Stephens (1979) developed a l i s t of reasons why children should be trained in childhood to be helpers. He found th a t helpful children grow into helpful and responsible teenages who "go on to become ad ults who help out and are good sp o rts in th eir own homes" (p. 92). His children learned appreciation for the work done by oth ers in the household when they actually did some of the work themselves. His case stu d ies suggested th a t early helping provided a foundation which could be built upon in a number of ways. Children who learned useful sk ills (such as cooking, other housework sk ills, child care and how do certain mechanical repairs) w ere getting off to a good s t a r t and th is instilled them w ith positive motivation. White and Brinkerhoff (1981) questioned parents and children regarding "the meaning fam ilies a ttac h to children's work, i. e. th eir ratio n ales and interpretations." Five primary reasons why parents assign work respo n sib ilities to th e ir children w ere given: Developmental: Doing chores builds ch aracter, develops re ­ sponsibility, helps children learn. Reciprocal obligation: It is th e ir duty to help the family; working together is p art of being a family; occasionally, more bluntly, 'they live here, don't th ey ?’ Extrinsic: P arents need help. Task learning: Children need to learn how to do th ese tasks. Residual: All other reasons, m ost often th a t child has to 30 earn an allowance or needs something to do in order to keep busy. (p. 793) In a recen t a rticle , Sander (1985) suggests several reasons why parents should assign work resp o n sib ilities to th e ir children and many of them w ere the same as those discovered by White and Brinkerhoff. She fe e ls th a t encouraging children to pa rticip a te in th e ir homes is essential to th e ir development. Doing things for them selves and others help children develop a strong sense of s e lf-e s te e m and confidence while showing them th a t they are important and capable members of th eir fam ilies. Sander believes th a t children, like adults, have psychological needs for responsibility. If parents consistently fail to ask children for help, they "soon get the impression th a t childhood is for play and adulthood for work” (p. 69). They also gain "neither a sense of family participation nor the aw areness of th e ir own ever-growing capabilities" (p. 69). Factors Affecting Children's Involvement in Household Work. In a 1957 study, Phillips investigated the contribution to the work of the home of children from 4 to 12 years of age. She found th a t the number of children doing jobs in a p articular area of homemaking increased as the ages of the children increased and th at g irls did more ta s k s than boys. She also discovered th a t more ta s k s w ere performed by children in larger households and by children whose m other did not have outside help w ith the housework. Roy (1961) designed a study to m an ifest the e ff e c ts of employment of the m other and of residence on c ertain ro les played by teenage sons and daughters. He found th a t in fam ilies where the m others w ere employed children (especially the girls) end up doing a l ittle more 31 housework than in fam ilie s w ith unemployed m others. Sons of employed mothers worked le ss outside for pay but the opposite w as tru e for girls (he believed th a t the m other w as s e ttin g the example). The difference between the rural and town sam ples was r o t s t a ti s t i c a ll y significant. The slight differences seemed to suggest that town boys did a little more housework than rural boys. He concluded that girls are different from boys in term s of the e ffe c t of m other's employment. Straus (1962) w a s also interested in the e ff e c ts of residence (he studied farm, fringe and town boys) on work roles and financial responsibility of his eleventh and tw e lf th grade sample. His data showed alm ost universal assignm ent of household ta s k s to boys in th is age group, beginning on the average at 8 years of age. Five hours a week on the average w as spent on regular household jo b s by the boys in his sample. Farm boys w ere assigned work roles e arlie r than nonfarm boys and spent more tim e in such tasks. His findings indicated widespread concern by parents w ith providing meaningful work role learning experiences for th e ir sons w ith emphasis (especially in farm fam ilies) on work as an im portant part of the socialization process. Another study on adolescents was conducted by Elder (1962) utilizing longitudinal data from the Institute of Human Development. He w as in terested in the influence of the child's involvement in household chores or in work development. outside the home" on th e child's personality He discovered th a t adolescents who were significantly involved in household task performance while in junior or senior high school rated (during the 1930's) w ere more likely than th e ir peers to be both responsible and compliant by research psychologists who regularly observed them. Boys who worked outside the home but did not 32 perform significant household chores w ere more peer-oriented, a sse rtiv e and high in drives for recognition and control. Those boys who neither helped a t home nor held outside jobs w ere low est in peer-orientation, leadership s t a tu s and a sse rtiv e autonomy. The group w as studied roughly 20 years la te r when they were nearing age 40 and members of the la st group were characterized (using personality t e s ts ) as less responsible and le ss self-co n tro lled than m ales who had e ith e r performed significant household chores or had held outside jobs. This group w as also characterized as more sociable and self-con fid en t than the other two groups. The picture fo r the g irls w as more complicated, partly because th e ir involvement in household ta s k s and th e ir general social participation w ere markedly influenced by the amount of deprivation th e ir fam ilies experienced in the depression. Under the direction of Walker (1973; Walker and Woods, 1976), an extensive household tim e -u se study w as undertaken in 1967-68. In th is study, the e ffe c t of m other's employment on children's participation in household work w as also studied. She found th a t the children's tim e contribution w as generally not closely related to the m other's hours of employment. The tim e of teenagers in fam ilies averaged 2.2 hours per day if m others w ere employed and 2.0 hours if m others were not employed. For children 6 to 11 years of age, the corresponding tim es w ere 1.0 and 1.1 hour, respectively. Although total tim e of the older children in fam ilies accounted for 20 percent of to tal work tim e when the homemaker w as not employed and 30 percent when she was, th is increased percentage re f le c ts a reduction in homemaker's tim e rath e r than an increase in children's time. For the younger children, she also suggested th a t the tim e difference may be explained by the tim e it takes 33 to teach these young children the sk ills they need to perform the tasks. She w rote (1970a): One wonders if supervision of children a t th is age is critical for help to be received, and if m others have adequate tim e and patience to teach children to work when th e ir total work days are long? (p. 14) O'NeiH’s (1979) research w as an update of the sim ilar but more extensive Walker study of 1967-68. One of her objectives w as to determine the relationship of children’s household work to parents' employment. By correlating children's household work tim e w ith parents' hours of employment, it w as evident th a t a child's household contributions decreased in response to the longer employment hours of m others and fathers. She felt: This su gg ests th a t children accomplish more household work if at le a st one parent is home to supervise them or, perhaps, it suggests th a t le ss household work is attem pted in m ultiiw orker households, (p. 21) The White and Brinkerhoff (1981) study has already been mentioned in an e a rlie r section of th is review where th e ir findings, of five primary reasons why p aren ts assign work resp o n sib ilities to th eir children, were reported. They found th a t over 70 percent of parental responses w ere in the developmental category which suggested th a t the frequency of response w as indicative of "a normative or socially desirable response” (p. 794). In fam ilies where children's work w as assigned only a developmental meaning, children worked the fe w e st hours (4.40 mean hours/week), while in fam ilies where work w as given an extrinsic interpretation, children w ere likely to work the longest hours (4.86 mean hours/week) and w ere also m ost likely to get paid for th e ir work. 34 The findings of Cogle and Tasker (1982) w ere co n sisten t w ith the previous reseach reported in th is review. They did a study on housework w ith children from 6 to 17 y ears of age. They divided household task s into six m ajor c ateg o ries and discovered w hat percentage of boys and g irls p articipated in each task. The fa c to rs which significantly affected the amount of tim e a child spent on housework w ere th e ir age (the m ajority of older children spent more tim e in housework than younger children), th e ir sex (g irls and boys followed traditional sex roles) and w hether or not th e ir m other w as employed (children of fu ll-tim e employed m others worked the most). Cogle and Tasker's Louisiana sample of children came from a larger data s e t collected in eleven sta te s. The larger sample w as studied by Sanik and Stafford (1985) in th e ir a tte m p ts to develop a model to predict the contribution of adolescent m ales and fem ales to household work, based upon family c h a ra c te ris tic s , human capital of the adolescent, geographic location and societal expectations. They discovered th a t the only variable which explained variance in each of th e ir four prediction equations w as school day (between 32 and 8 4 m inutes less w ere spent in household work on a school day). Regardless of birth order, adolescent fem ales worked longer than adolescent males. Time use for household work w as for the m ost p a rt unaffected by family c h arac teristics. Another recent study on adolescents w as conducted by Hansen and Darling (1985). a ttitu d e s Their investigation a ttem pted to examine adolescents' toward household ta s k s based on gender and maternal employment. Their data suggested that: the m ajority of adolescents they studied held somewhat tr a d i­ tional a ttitu d e s tow ard the se x -ro le division of household tasks, while a sm a lle r number had an egalitarian or 35 nontraditional orientation tow ard division of labor in the home. (p. 65) Although some v a riation s in sex role performance of ta s k s were evident between m ales and fem ales (fem ales w ere more inclined toward egalitarianism ) and stu d e n ts w ith employed and unemployed m others (students w ith employed m others showed a g rea ter inclination toward egalitarianism ), th ese differences were not significant. Summary. P arents give many reasons for assigning children household work. The m ost common reasons are th a t doing chores helps to develop responsibility and ch arac ter and helps children learn s k ills they need to know. Whatever the rationale, homemakers s till do the lion's share of the work in the household w ith some help from children. In stu d ies where children w ere the primary focus, fa c to rs affecting discussed. employment, children’s Factors involvement identified residence, human included capital some of the in household work size of the of family, child were parents' and societal expectations. Children and Work A Brief Historical Perspective on Work and Its Im pact/Influence on Children H ig h e r (1977) saw the nineteenth century as the Golden Age for the idea of work. He said th a t The dignity of labor is not based on i ts re su lts, alw ays vain and temporal, but on the fa c t th at it perm its the soul never to pause, alw ays to ascend, and to find its peace in the very movement by which it flings its e lf forward, ever higher and further, (p. 140) 36 The kind of work described by Tilgher w as primarily performed by a rtis a n s who worked in handicraft production. According to Aronowitz (1973), in the older a rtisa n mode of production the worker owned his own tools and saw the relationship betw een his skill and the resulting product. The a rtisa n w as like an a r t i s t who "experienced real enjoyment in work since the product could be perceived both as th e outcome of his s k ills and as his possession" (p. 122). This concept of work as intrinsically sa tisfy ing w as transform ed, w ith the introduction of m ass production, to the acceptance of work as a necessary evil. The new concept of labor viewed work as instrum ental to personal ends. It w as historically rooted in the religious belief that work w as a ctiv ity th at makes a place for you in heaven. Under capitalism , workers could only be allowed to live th e ir own life a fte r labor had been performed. With industrialization, children had to learn to take pleasure in deprivation and reserve their leisure for prescribed periods th a t are viewed by adults as providing a 'release' from the necessary routines of daily life and labor, (p. 82) In the long run, indu strialization prolongation of childhood. created the conditions for the This resu lte d in the release of the younger generation from the world of work. Individual commitment to the social in stitu tio n of work is necessary for a society to su stain its e lf and to thrive. Stephens (1979) believes th a t th is kind of commitment is much more diffic u lt for modern children to develop because they are kept away from the adult world of work and they help out less w ith household work. He says th at, if children are given the opportunity to help in the home, they will develop an increasing sense of responsibility. Unfortunately, living in a 37 modernized, nonfarm home often c re a te s a situ atio n where the child's help is not particularly needed (Minturn and Lambert, 1964). A rtificial make-work does not s u b s titu te for the life-giving work provided by farm children and children in more traditional societies. At the present, taking care of other children a t any age appears to offer the modern child the best chance to develop "responsible" and "helpful" t r a i t s which are needed for them to function effectively as adults. Several re se a rc h e rs are concerned th a t society does not provide the experiences th a t will perm it future generations to develop constructive a ttitu d e tow ards work (Rapoport and Rapoport, Slaugh, 1982). Rapoport and Rapoport suggest th a t a 1977; parents are attem pting to c re a te an ideal clim ate for development which promotes the child's "needs" a t the expense of values "of a humanistic, cooperation kind." P arents exempt children from p articipation in household work and s till expect them to believe in the dignity and worth of labor. places u n realistic or conflicting demands on children. This Awareness of such issu es is increased through research, even if the research its e lf cannot contribute directly to the resolution of value dilemmas. Work Participation of Children in Other Societies: The. Importance of Early Training In his book The Human Economy, Ginzberg (1976) describes the traditional agricultural society where the manpower development system is coexisten t w ith the extended family. The elders teach the child or young person what they need to know to to assume th e ir re sp o n sib ilitie s as a worker and member of the community. cases, the family is the in stru cto r and eventual employer. In many 38 When a young person reaches an age when he can make a contribution to the running of the farm or the household, he joins the work group and receives increasingly important assignm ents geared to his age, skill, and competence, (p. 33) Boulding (1979) supports Ginzberg's view. She says th a t in the w orld's rural a re as children s t a r t working a t 5 y ears of age and by 10 or 12 they are working fulltim e. Boys often s t a r t in the fields and girls help w ith the young children. In Soviet extended-day schools, children are routinely taught to take substantial responsibility for each other and younger children. They also contribute to the physical maintenance of the community and the growing of its food. Their rew ards and sa tis fa c tio n s are in te rm s of how well they have a ss is te d the group in its functioning, how well they have helped i ts weaker members, and how effective th eir partnership w ith the adults in the school has been in community tasks, (p. 34) Village stu d ies by Whiting and Whiting (1975) take the discussion of children working one ste p further. In the villages they studied, children are pressed into service a f an early age. In the African case, by age 4, half of the children are working (i.e. carrying wood and w a te r and helping w ith food preparation, gardening, housecleaning and animal care). The average age is closer to 6 in the Mexican and Philippine villages. In the Okinawan and Indian villages, the children are older still. Because adults are also involved in th ese a c tiv itie s, and m others are busy and need help, chores do not appear a rb itrary and unnecessary. They find th a t th is early work for children r e s u lts in helpfulness, responsibility and nurturant, parental qualities. Through helping out, children learn to be helpful. When given responsibility, they become responsible. 39 Summary The process of socialization r e fe rs to the means by which a society brings i t s new m em bers into social groups and enables them to function effectively w ithin tho se groups. In the case of socialization to a work ethic, m ost fam ilies in traditional agricultural so c ie tie s and in e arlie r tim es tra in children by pressing them into service a t an early age. When a child is given the opportunity to carry out jobs which are clearly essen tial and challenge th e ir skill and inventiveness, as well as th eir ste ad fa stn ess, th is enhances the atmosphere of responsibility in which the child lives and helps to c u ltiv ate reliability. This is often not the case for children living in modernized, nonfarm homes where th e ir help is not particularly needed. As a result, several researchers are concerned th a t th is society is not providing those experiences which w ill perm it future generations to develop a constructive a ttitu d e tow ard s work. The Firstborn Child The sam ple being analyzed in th is study c o n sists of firstborn children betw een the ages of 6 to 12 years of age. This brief overview of studies of firstb o rn is included in th is review because they have often been found to be d ifferen t from th e ir siblings. Several researchers suggest th a t birth order does have an impact on the development of the firstborn child. Birth Order Research Focusing on the Firstborn Bossard and Boll (1956) conclude th a t the eld est children tend to be "altruistic" to a fault: 40 F irst, they tend to be put under pressure from an early age. From being put under pressure by others, they proceed to put them selves under pressure. Thus h abits are formed which c ry s ta lliz e into p a tte rn s of responsibility, (p. 162)....The oldest or an older one of the children in a large family develops marked habits of accountability, aiding the parents in th eir duties, sharing responsibilities, and taking over much of the rearing of the younger siblings....Patterns of s a c rific e and service also m anifest them selves early in life. Being the o ldest means doing for others, (p. 266) The picture th a t em erges from th is description is of prem aturely grown-up, ra th e r m artyrish eld est children, the product of excessive responsibility-pressure. The suggestion, th a t to produce a responsible child means giving them an opportunity to be responsible, can obviously be overdone. In his review of the lite ra tu re on birth order, Warren (1966) found in stu d ies w ith men only, w ith women only, w ith the sexes mixed and from the early school years through college th a t firstb o rn are more responsive to social pressure. They are also more dependent than la te r born. Although Clausen (1966) believes th a t the e f f e c ts of b irth order are, for the m ost part, indirect, he does report on c ertain reg u la rities which have emerged to indicate th a t position w ithin the family does make for a number of predictab1e--if m odest—differences. He notes th a t no other child is likely to receive the amount of atten tio n (time, energy, concern) as is the firstb orn prior to th e ir dethronement (by the birth of the second child). The firstb orn child has no "child model” as younger sib s do. They may be the caretaker, teacher, p a c e s e tte r or confidante of younger children. They can be helpful models for learning sex-appropriate behavior. He indicates th a t one of the m ost c o n sistent 41 findings relating to b irth order Is th a t firstb o rn children achieve eminence in higher proportion than do th e ir siblings. Although his findings about su sce p tib ility to influence are c o nsisten t w ith Warren's when discussing m ales, he su ggests th a t a number of stu dies find the firstb o rn fem ale le s s susceptible to influence, relatively more responsible, aggressive and com petitive than la te r born females. Adams and Phillips (1972), like Clausen, find th at an overrepresentation of firstb o rn in college populations and among men of great accom plishm ents are probably the two m ost sta b le and replicated birth order findings. They suggest th at part of the reason the firstborn child has a higher level of motivation (when m otivation is defined as long-term e ffo rt directed toward a goal) than the la te r born child is th at parents have higher expectations for and expect more achievement from the firstb o rn child. A number of other stu d ies (which they cite) present supporting empirical evidence indicating th a t parental pressure is indeed often placed on the firsborn child to be responsible and to achieve accomplishm ents not expected from la te r born siblings. Their sample of firstb o rn s appear to be living up to th is expectation by scoring significantly higher than la te r born children on four different m easures on intellectual and academic performance and on one measure of school motivation. Schooler's (1972) a rticle , “Birth Order Effects: Not Here, Not Now!" re fu te s many of the previous findings. Her review of the lite ra tu re in birth order stu d ie s indicates th a t the repeated findings of a surplus of f i r s t borns among eminent scholars appears to have nothing to do w ith any d irec t relationship of birth order to eminence. It is simply a reflectio n of the f a c t th a t scholars, eminent or not, derive from a 42 college population in which firstb o rn s are in marked surplus. It can also be explained in te rm s of differences among social c la s s trends in family size. She also rep o rts th a t if th ere w ere any substantial differences in American p a re n ts’ approaches to children of differen t birth ranks in the early 1950s, these differences disappeared by the m id-1960s. Summary Although several findings from the preceding stu d ies on firstb orn are conflicting, there appears to be some evidence th a t being born f i r s t does have an impact on the child’s development. A very obvious difference from la te r siblings is the amount of a tten tio n from p sre n ts the child is likely to receive. This reseacher wonders if the combination of parents' higher expectations for firstb orn and th e ir higher responsiveness to social pressure, will re s u lt in g re a te r participation of th is sample (who are all firstborn) in household work. Some other fa c to rs which have an influence on firstborn, but w ere not discussed in th is section are: family size (density), the age of the parents, sex and spacing of siblings. Ihe_£ffects of Sex. Sex Roles and Age on Children's Household Work Contributions Osborne's pamphlet published in 1955 gave some very specific suggestions about the work children should be doing a t d ifferent ages. He f e lt th a t children younger than 5 and 6 should p a rticip a te in "prework activities." From the ages of 6 through 12 years, they should have increasing opportunities to take on responsible fam ily-centered jobs. These jobs would allow both growth in ability and taking responsibility in the family and outside. He suggested th a t the parent should make 43 some of th e s e jobs c re ativ e and interesting so th a t children would be less likely to find the hum-drum ones disturbing. In the 1950's, Gesell, llg and Ames (1956) conducted a longitudinal study of children which combined an interview of parents and th e ir children ages 5 through 16 w ith psychological testin g techniques. The purpose of the study w as to look a t the influence of age on the organization of behavior in the context of contemporary American culture. For each year of m aturity in a child's growth process, c h a ra c te ris tic t r a i t s and trends w ere discovered. Work, primarily household work, w as one of the categories investigated for the 10 to 16 year olds. They w ere more concerned about general im pressions of the work habits and a ttitu d e s toward participation of the children than they w ere w ith getting quantifiable data. In general, they found th a t m ost 10 year olds w ere not good about helping a t home and would delay and dawdle whenever possible. Boys did b e tte r w ith outdoor ta s k s such as mowing the lawn, shoveling the snow and gardening than indoor task s such as se ttin g the table, making th e ir beds, sweeping and dusting. By the tim e a child reaches 12, they no longer have an autom atic re s ista n c e to household work and have developed a more positive a ttitu d e toward it. Although the children were not volunteering for work and s t i ll needed to be reminded, they w ere often good about helping and even showed a lit t le w illingness now and then. work. They even attem pted more d iffic u lt By the tim e they were 15 years old, ta s k s were becoming increasing complex and responsible. The children seemed to take their contributions for granted, though m ost of them w ere probably not en th u siastic workers. 44 Johannis (1958) also conducted a study in the 1950's where he investigated the "Participation by Fathers, Mothers and Teenage Sons and Daughters in Selected Household Tasks.” In his review of the literatu re, he introduced a h isto rical overview of the existence of traditional p a tte rn s of household work participation. In his own study, he discovered th a t f a th e rs participated in traditional ta s k s more than m others and sons more than daughters. Because children tended to p a rticip a te in the sim pler and le ss in teresting types of task s (which are easy for parents to teach), Johannis wondered if children were su b stitu tin g "for a servant in the family." Teenagers w ere also the focus of tw o stu d ies done in the 1960's. In a study by Tengel (1964), junior high and high school age children were surveyed to obtain information on th e ir work experiences both a t home and in the community. Girls, she found, contributed considerably more tim e to household d u ties than boys did. For instance, m ost of the girls and only two th ird s of the boys performed kitchen and housecleaning tasks. Jobs where a higher percentage of boys participated than girls tended to be those which m ales traditionally do. About 80 percent of the boys said they cleaned the basement, took out the trash and did yard work while g irls did th is work only half as much. Three percent of the sample did no work a t home. It w as interesting to note th a t younger brothers and s i s t e r s cared for toddlers in the family more than the older teens. In Hoppen's (1966) study of teens, she focused only on their contribution to the work of the home. Because she realized th at the sex of the teen would be an important influence on th e ir contribution, she hypothesized th a t g irls would contribute more tim e to household work 45 than boys and th a t the nature of household ta s k s done and the frequency w ith which they w ere done related to the sex of the teenager. She discovered that,w hile boys did c ertain kinds of ta s k s and g irls did certain kinds of task s, g irls on the average contributed alm ost three tim e s as much tim e as boys. Inside a c tiv itie s w ere reported more by g irls and outside a c tiv itie s by boys. Girls tended to do work on a daily basis while boy's work w as more seasonal. Three of the 28 teen s studied did not report some tim e spent in household work fo r the day preceding the interview. The Walker (Walker and Woods, 1976) household tim e -u se study w as discussed e a rlie r in th is review. Although the main unit of analysis w as the household and not individual children, th is study w as included in th is section because she did an extensive analysis of the tim e used by children for doing work related to the production of goods and services w ithin the household. This analysis w as based on the contribution of all children in various age groups. The data indicated th a t all 6 to 11 year olds contributed an average of about one hour per day to household work. For fam ilies w ith teenagers (12 to 17 year olds), the average w as two hours per day. Walker noted th a t these tim e contributions w ere substantial and they did play a significant role in the operations of the household. In 88 percent of the record days, one or more teenagers in those fam ilies w ith teenagers did some work. For fam ilies w ith 6 to 11 year olds, 69 percent worked. In over one half of the fam ilies reporting, children worked in one to three activ itie s. One fourth of the fam ilies reported work by teens in four to six activ ites. Children participated in a c tiv itie s households: they have traditionally been expected to perform in regular meal preparation, a fte r-m e a l cleanup and regular 46 house care. Because the tim e spent in ta s k performance w as only recorded on specified days, it would be difficult to determine from th is data the actual number of ta s k s a child perform s and impossible to evaluate the quality of the work. Data, from a National Health Survey done in 1963-65 w ith parental rating s of behavioral p a tte rn s of 25 million children, w ere analyzed by Roberts and Baird (1971). Six to 11 year olds w ere studied focusing on age and sex differences. In the section measuring the extent of responsibility, the proportion of children performing regular family ta s k s increased c o n sistently as each of the age groups w ere examined. While 78.3 percent had one or more tasks, the proportion doing three or more ta s k s regularly more than doubled over th is age range increasing from 19.8 percent among 6 year olds to 46.9 percent among 11 year olds. The p a tte rn w as sim ila r for boys and girls. The research ers f e lt that: This steady increase undoubtedly re f le c ts both the learning of responsible behavior and the development of sk ills w ith age. (p. 9) One of th e ir tab les illu stra te d the percent distribution of children by tim e spent per day working (on chores, etc.) according to age and sex. The findings from th is table indicated th a t girls spent only slightly more tim e a t work than boys. Walker's findings on boys and g irls between 6 to 11 y ears of age w ere very sim ilar to these findings. Lynch (1975a) also utilized Walker's data, but she focused on the "Participation in Household Tasks by Children from Six to Seventeen Years of Age." The objective in th is study w as to determine what children of both sexes and different ages in fam ilies w ith one, tw o and three children contribute to the work of the home. She f e lt th is w as one step in the process of clarifying relationships and p a ttern s of children's 47 task participation. To achieve her objective, she utilized information of mean tim e (frequency) spent in various a c tiv itie s a s well as the percent of children who p articip ated in various household tasks. She found th a t g irls betw een 9 to 17 years of age did a g rea ter number of a c tiv itie s than boys of the sam e age and spent a g re a te r amount of tim e a t household work. By age 12, on the average, g irls spent tw ice as much tim e as boys in household work. The ta s k s primarily performed by boys and g irls had not changed very much from the traditional pattern. Females p articip ated in meal preparation, meal cleanup and regular house care. Outside a c tiv itie s , the care of the yard and car, w ere primarily male tasks. Meal preparation and regular house care were the ta s k s m ost frequently p articip ated in by both m ales and females, contributed more tim e in the a c tiv itie s than boys. but g irls Girls usually spent the m ost tim e in a c tiv itie s they p articipated in m ost often. On the other hand, boys a t m ost ages spent the g re a te s t amount of tim e per day on the irregular a ctiv ity of care of yard and car, even though they participated in house care and food preparation ta s k s more frequently. The adherence to traditional participation w as also apparent p a tte rn s in a national in household task survey of children conducted by Temple University In stitu te for Survey Research (1976) on children aged 7 to 11 y ears of age. Interview s of more than 2,200 children and more than 1,700 of th e ir parents w ere analyzed. Interview s were stru c tu re d to determ ine the general environment in which children live. One section of the interview asked children to pick one of five sentences to tell how they f e lt about various work and play activities. Girls expressed significantly g re a te r liking for traditional "female" a c t iv i t ie s such as cooking, sewing and dancing. Boys expressed g rea ter 48 liking for a c tiv itie s such as "making things out of wood or m etal.” Parents' response showed strong sex differences, also. At the same tim e, there w as evidence of change. Less than 5 percent of the g irls said they did not w ant a job when they grew up but only wanted to be a housewife or mother. Thrall (1978) w as also in terested in role stereotypy as it related to children's work and the continuity between generations in the household division of labor. Thrall defined role stereotypy as the normative expectation th a t one person w as supposed to do a task and th a t another w as then expected not to do it. He included questions about children's work in his in terview s w ith 99 husbands and wives adapting the Blood and Wolfe (1960) m easures of division of labor. His findings suggested th a t the b e st single predictor of a fam ily's division of labor may be the previous generation. Most fam ilies in the study tended to be quite traditional in th e ir pattern, w ith strong emphasis on division of labor by sex for both parents and children. Thrall also reported th a t older children took p art in more of the ta s k s than did younger children. Parents s till retained primary responsibility for the work performed by children. Making th e ir beds or picking up th e ir rooms were chores prim arily assigned to children. These w ere ta s k s which w ere specific to the child and not part of the family task performance record. As mentioned earlier, O’Neill (1979) did the ten year follow up to Walker's work. The data of time given to household work by school-age children provided a basis for a ten -y ear comparison of children's household work according to th e ir age and sex. As w ith the Walker study, data w ere collected on tim e-record c h a rts th a t homemakers filled out for the day previous to th e ir interview and the day following. In the 9 to 49 11 and 12 to 14 age b rack ets differences in task s performed by g irls and boys w ere especially apparent. Girls' participation r a te s in all but the car and y ard-care ta s k s increased much f a s te r than boys'. The sex differences of ten years ago continued to ex ist which meant th a t the designation of ta s k s w ithin households appeared not to have changed very much. O'Neill did discover, though, th a t the gap in the percentage of boys and g irls contributing tim e to specific ta s k s has generally narrowed. In th e ir book, Children and Work. Goldstein and Oldham (1979) discussed th e ir sam ple's experience w ith work first-h an d through the performance of chores and so-called "childwork." Although they were unable to determ ine the impact of children's work experiences upon th eir w o rk -related cognition s ta te s , th e ir findings suggested th a t children's work and earning experiences: very 1. typically s t a r t in early childhood on a small scale; 2. 3. are extrem ely widespread; and apparently are subjected to age-related increments, 169) (p. They found th a t the seventh grade, rath e r than the fifth, represented a turning point. They suggested th a t th is w as probably the case because of stru c tu ra l and physical considerations rath e r than because of appreciable change in children's readiness or willingness to work a t th a t age. When they asked children about th eir feelings regarding th e ir own work, w hether chi Id-work or merely the performance of household chores, nearly four in five at each grade level said they liked working. Zill and Peterson (1982) w ere convinced th a t children do a considerable amount of work in the home and th at the amount and nature of th is work w as intim ately tied to the practical sk ills they develop. * Using a national sam ple of elementary school children, they developed a 50 practical s k ills index which w as made up of 14 practical tasks. They discovered th a t the b e st predictor of the child's development of practical sk ills w as sex of the child. For alm ost all the individual item s, d istin c t differences existed in the percentage of sons and daughters who were reported (by th e ir parents) to have accomplished the task s without help. All the d ifferen ces w ere in the direction expected from sex-role stereotypes. Because th is w as a developmental measure, the proportion of children ever having accomplished each task rose w ith age as expected. This w as true for boys and girls, although the ra te of improvement w ith age varied by the sex of the child within some tasks. Hypotheses-Formulated Research indicates th a t the fa c to rs which m ost influence children's involvement in household work are sex, sex roles and age. In the m ajority of studies, girls' contributions are significantly g rea ter than boys' to household work. Hypotheses 1 and 3 r e fle c t these differences. In hypothesis 1, the perceived contributions of the g irls is hypothesized to be higher than the boys. performed by the In hypothesis 3, the number of task s g irls is hypothesized to be higher than the number performed by the boys. The stu d ies in th is section also show a continued adherence to traditional p a tte rn s in household task participation w ith girls doing m ost of the in-the-hom e work and boys working outside. This resulted in the form ulation of hypothesis 4 which s t a t e s th a t g irls will do more in-the-hom e work while boys will work more outside. The age of the child a f f e c ts the number and kinds of task s children accomplish w ith older children performing more task s and those which require g re a te r skill. This relationship is reflected in hypotheses 2 and 51 5 though the conceptual fram ework w as the basts for th e ir development. Research on Quality of Life Several books e x is t on developing m easures of and rep o rts of research on perceived quality of life (Campbell, e t al.. 1976; Andrews and Withey, 1976; Campbell, 1981). In the questionnaire utilized in th is study, the section on quality of life owes much of i t s development to the conceptual framework developed by Andrews and Withey (1974, 1976). The discussion which follow s sum m arizes several a sp ec ts of th eir framework which are relevant to th is research. Quality of Life: Basic Concepts Andrews and Withey (1976) believe that: The quality of life is not j u s t a m a tte r of the conditions of one's physical, interpersonal and social se ttin g but also a m a tte r of how th e s e are judged and evaluated by oneself and others. The values th a t one brings to bear on life are in them selves d eterm inan ts of one's a sse ssed quality of life. (p. 12) They c ite several reasons for the importance and usefulness of measuring people's perceptions of well-being. particularly relevant to th is study. Two of the reasons are First, they feel it is valuable to gather baseline m easures which can be compared to subsequent m easures and trends of change so th a t society knows where it stands. They also feel there is value in getting to understand how people evaluate and feel about th e ir lives if the judgm ents are made about "domains " of life such as th eir fam ilies, th e ir homes, th e ir jobs, etc. They conceive of w ell-being indicators as occurring a t several levels of specificity. Indicators which re fe r to life as a whole are the 52 m ost global and they are not specific to any one p articu lar aspect of life. General evaluations of w hat they call life "concerns" are at a somewhat more specific level. Concerns are divided into tw o types: "domains" (places, things, a c tiv itie s, people and roles) and "criteria" (values, standards, aspirations, goals, and—in general—ways of judging or evaluating how one f e lt about the various domains of life). Both global indicators and life concerns will be studied in th is research. Andrews and Withey hypothesize th a t a person's assessm en t of life quality involves both a cognitive evaluation and some degree of positive or negative feelings, i.e., "affect." A fter experimenting w ith numerous w ays of measuring a ffectiv e evaluations, the Delighted-Terrible Scale w as found to be the m ost e ffectiv e and is used m ost extensively. A form of th is scale is used in th is study, and is described in the methodology section. Studies of Children's Quality of Life Although Bourque (1982) w as not specifically in terested in quality of life studies, she did a careful examination of evaluation studies funded by the U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare in 1974 and 1975. She found th a t children w ere rarely formally interviewed, and on the few occasions when they have been, th eir opinions frequently contradicted those given by parents and teach ers on th eir behalf. She hypothesized th a t the reluctance to interview children or otherw ise obtain data directly from them might indicate th a t research ers do not consider children capable of acting as research subjects. She also recognized tw o more potential problems: (1) re s tric tio n s imposed by human su b je c ts legislation and (2) the necessity of obtaining perm ission 53 from paren ts or guardians to study minors. Quality of life research can overcome the bias of viewing children a s incapable of acting as research su b je c ts by elicitin g the child's su b jectiv e perceptions which could understandably be in conflict w ith a p arent’s or teacher's assessm ent. When children are the focus, one common area of study is the quality of school life. Studies on Quality of School Life. Epstein and McPartland (1976) studied the concept and m easurement of the quality of school life. They developed different dimensions of the concept of quality of school life and call these subscales (what Andrews and Withey re fe r to as domains). Subscales help to determ ine the global measure. They suggest th a t The trend for quality of school life scores to decrease as grade level increases may mean th a t the objective quality of school goes down each year and/or th at w ith m aturity, stu d e n ts more c ritic a lly evaluate th e ir environments....It may be argued th at w ith age, stud ents' a b ilitie s become more varied. Schools may be less able to m eet the more diverse academic in te re s ts and needs of older stu d e n ts although they are able to m aintain the general and social quality of school life for m ost students, (p. 26) In Wolf and Chandler’s (1981) study, perceptions of quality of school life included sa tis fa c tio n w ith school, commitment to th e ir classw ork and a ttitu d e s toward teachers. The perceptions of a sample of fourth graders w ere a sse ss e d a t the beginning and end of the school year. Results provided te n ta tiv e support for the view th a t perceptions of these asp e c ts of quality of school life temporally preceded perceptions of academic responsibility. They suggested th a t the more favorably these classroom fa c to rs are perceived, the more likely stu d e n ts are to accept responsibility for th e ir school su c ce sses and failures. 54 National Studies. The Temple University In stitu te for Survey Research (1976) conducted a National Survey of Children during Septem ber through December of 1976. It consisted of interview s of more than 2,200 children aged 7 to 11 y ears of age and more than 1,700 of th e ir parents. Interview s w ere stru ctu red to determ ine the general environment in which children live. One section of the interview asked children to pick one of five sentences to tell how they feel about various work and play a ctiv itie s. They w ere also given a s e t of five faces ranging from very happy to very sad and asked, "Which face shows how you feel about: yourself? your school work? your family?...." On the whole, th ese children f e lt good about themselves. Eighty percent of them picked a happy face to show how they f e lt about them selves and about how things w ere going in th e ir lives. Over 75% thought they w ere lucky and 90% said they liked being the way they were. Ninety percent felt good about th e ir fam ilies while 80% also worried about th e ir families. Zill (1978) also analyzed p a rts of the data collected from the National Survey of Children. He w as in terested in the relationship betw een the mental health of children and divorce and m arital happiness. In a paper he prepared on th is subject, he gave several reasons why the National Survey w as designed and sponsored by the Foundation for Child Development. Three of the purposes he suggested w ere relevant to the present study. Like Zill, th is resea rch e r w as in terested in collecting quality of life data on children to determine the fea sib ility and value of child questionnaires (he used interview s) as a source of social indicator data on children. Zill also wanted to analyze the relationships between the conditions of children's lives and m easures of child development and 55 well-being. This resea rch e r lim ited the focus to the conditions surrounding the child’s family work and the relationship between family work and m easures of well-being. And finally, Zill wanted to replicate item s from previous national studies for tim e -tre n d analysis. Several of the quality of life ite m s in th is study w ere very sim ila r to those utilized on the National Survey. As a resu lt, the analysis of th is data may give some insight into how th is sample of children compares to the national sample which w as polled four y ears earlier. Hypotheses Formulated In th is section, several reasons have been cited for the importance and usefulness of measuring people’s perceptions of well-being. resu lt of gathering previous m easures, where it stands. As a m easures of w ell-being and comparing them to tren d s can be established so th a t society knows This research er is in terested in measuring the well-being of th is sam ple of children so th a t it can be compared to previous m easures which have been collected. There is also value in getting to understand how people evaluate and feel about th e ir lives if the judgments are made about "domains" of life. In th is study, the focus is on the domain of family work and its relationship to the child’s perceived quality of life. Because of the paucity of research in th is area, hypotheses 6 and 7 are exploratory in nature and do not predict any differences between boys and g irls or age groups. Summary. Industrialization c re a te s the conditions for the prolongation of childhood and r e s u lts in the release of m ost children from the world of 56 work. Living in a modernized, nonfarm home often c re a te s a situation where the child's help is not particularly needed. Research indicates th a t responsibility is developed when the child's work contributions are needed, when they have the opportunity to make and help make important decisions and when th ere is enough tim e for the jchild to work alongside, im itate and help a responsible adult. As a family helper, the child contributes to the life of the family; th is adds to s e lf-re s p e c t and dem onstrates the b e n efits to the family when everyone does th e ir share. The responsibility and s k ills learned through work experience carry over into adult lives and influence the roles of future generations. Many fa c to rs influence children's involvement in household work. Research indicates th a t the e f f e c ts of sex, sex roles and age on children's household work contributions are particularly pronounced. In th is review of the lite ra tu re , all the studies focusing on sex roles indicate a continued adherence to traditional p a tte rn s in household task participation. In m ost cases, the g irls in these stu d ie s often contributed more of th eir tim e to household work than the boys. significantly influences the number and kinds of ta s k s Age also children accomplish w ith older children taking part in more work and work which requires the development of more skills. Several recent stu d ie s on children's quality of life provide valuable baseline m easures which can be compared to subsequent m easures to determine trends of change. Measures of quality of life as a whole are established by these stu d ie s a s well as some general evaluations on specific domains. Chapter 4 METHODOLOGY This study w as p a rt of a larger study, "Contributions of Household Production to Family Income," which w as sponsored by the Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station (AES 1363H), the Department of Family and Child Ecology a t Michigan S ta te Cooperative Extension Service. University and the Michigan The larger study w as undertaken to identify the involvement in household production among young urban, small town and rural fam ilies in mid-Michigan. Data used in th is study w ere collected during the months of May and June 1980 in Ingham County, Michigan. For the larger study, the unit of analysis w as the family which w as defined as a male and female living in the same household w ith the oldest child between the ages of 6 and 12. A survey questionnaire w as given to each of these three family members. This researcher did not become p a rt of the research team until a fte r all the data were collected and being coded. As a resu lt, th is researcher did not p articip ate in decisions relating to sampling procedures, instrum ent design or data collection. This study w as designed to investigate the household work participation of the children in th is sam ple and th eir perceived quality of life. As a resu lt, the m ajor portion of the children's questionnaire will be analyzed as well as the demographic data 57 from the parents' 58 questionnaire. Discussion in th is chapter focuses on: (1) research design of the study; (2) description of the sample; (3) instrum ent development; (4) variables; (5) s t a ti s t i c a l analysis; and (6) lim itatio ns of the study. Research Design The purpose of th is descriptive study w as to identify the perceived contributions to household work of children between the ages of 6 and 12 years of age. Specifically, the number and kinds of ta s k s they perform were identified. An a tte m p t w as also made to identify the child's perceived quality of life and to determine if a relationship existed between the perceived contribution to perceived quality of life. household work and th e ir The research method used w as a survey questionnaire based on recall. Demographic data w ere determined from the questionnaires of the parents. Theoretical and operational definitions of the variables were noted in Chapter 1. The sample w as selected from Ingham County, Michigan which is included in the Lansing Standard Metropolitan S ta tis tic a l Area (SMSA). The county contains the s t a te agencies, capital and associated many business and manufacturing firm s government related to the automobile industry, a diversified agricultural industry and a large land grant university (Michigan S tate University). The county is made up of a heterogeneous population of urban, small town and rural households. The sample w as designed to re fle c t the c h a ra c te ris tic s and a c tiv itie s of young fam ilies, living in private households, from the three d istin c t locations. A minimum of thirty fam ilies from each of the three a re as was considered appropriate to represent the larger population. 59 Given the study's definitional and geographic con strain ts, the sample selectio n p rocess w as designed to be as random as possible. The urban sample came from Lansing, the s t a te capital, which has the larg est population c en ter in Ingham County. mid-Michigan. tran sp o rtatio n The m ajor equipment, employers fabricated It is centrally located in are industry m etals and (principally non-electrical machinery), s t a t e government and the university. The team a ttem p ted to locate areas w ithin the city w ith the highest percentage of school-age children between the ages of 6 and 12 years. A school census w as obtained from the Tri-County Planning Commission and an area of south Lansing was identified as having the largest number of young children. An area in north Lansing w as also considered. V isits to both a re a s revealed several indicators of children such as swing s e t s and signs in windows identifying them as sh e lte rs for school-age children. The f i r s t neighborhood chosen to be sampled w as a census t r a c t area in south Lansing. In the event th a t additional fam ilies w ere needed, an adjacent t r a c t w as identified as the second area to be sampled. City blocks w ithin the census tr a c t w ere numbered and all apartm ent buildings w ere individually numbered and tre a te d as if they w ere city blocks. Before the in terview ers s ta r te d contacting fam ilies, block numbers w ere randomly selected as s ta rtin g points. In order to obtain the required number of fam ilies, all of the blocks in both census t r a c t s had to be sampled. Households w ere contacted utilizing a skip pattern once residential blocks w ere randomly selected. Once a family qualified and agreed to p a rticip a te in the study then the skip p a ttern became operative and the next house on the block or road w as skipped. The following house w as then contacted. 60 The sm all town sample came from Mason, the county s e a t of Ingham County. Mason has no m ajor industry, but it does have several small indu stries and service agencies. Because the town is located within commuting distance of Lansing and Jackson, Michigan, about 85 percent of the employed persons living w ithin the corporate lim its (boundaries for the sample) work outside of Mason. The remaining 15 percent are mainly factory w orkers and s t a te employees. A significant segment of the population is made up of re tire d farm ers, s t a f f and faculty from Michigan S ta te University. The areas w ith the largest number of school children w ere initially identified by the Ingham County Extension Home Economist. Interviewing began in the areas identified but had to be expanded to encompass the e n tire town due to the small population. As in Lansing, city blocks were randomly selected as sta rtin g points before the in terview ers contacted any families. The c lo s e s t rural area to Lansing w as Wheatfield Township and it w as selected for the rural sample. It w as within commuting distance of Lansing w ith houses located approximately every q u arter mile on each of the township roads. E a s t-w e st and north-south roads w ithin the township w ere numbered and randomly selected as sta rtin g points. Every house w ithin the township w as v isite d (the skip pattern w as not utilized) because of the sparse population and distance between the houses. Utilizing th is procedure, the Wheatfield Township s till did not produce enough qualified families. A rural area adjacent and to the w e st of Wheatfield (LeRoy Township) had to be included in the sample using the same procedure. 61 Description of the Study Sample The data in th is section come from an analysis of the questions on demographics in the husband's and w ive's questionnaires. It is included because the children's sample can be b e tte r understood given the demographics of th eir parents. The sample for the larger Household Production P roject consisted of 107 fam ilies (husband, w ife and oldest child between the ages of 6 and 12 years). This study analyzed the m ajor portion of the children's data from the 107 families. The urban sample consisted of 32 fam ilies, 38 were from the small town and 37 were from the rural sample. Demographic information from the Tri-County Planning Commission provided the following demographic c h a r a c te ris tic s for the sample areas. Urban Sample.—The urban sample consisted of census t r a c t s 36.01 and 36.02. For these areas, the 1980 median household incomes were $19,400 and $14,800, respectively. The areas w ere 63.7 percent and 69.7 percent Caucasian and 30.5 percent and 24.0 percent Black, respectively. Small Town Sample.—The sm all town sample consisted of all of Mason. The area had a median household income of $18,400 and was 96.5 percent Caucasian and 0.01 percent Black. Rural Sample. The rural sample included all of Wheatfield Township and the w estern edge of LeRoy Township where it borders Wheatfield Township. The area had a 1980 median income of $17,900 and w as 98.7 percent Caucasian and 0.5 percent Black. The racial balance of the sample w as sim ila r to the 1980 census. In the sample, over 87 percent of the adult respondents w ere Caucasian w ith few er than 12 percent of Black or Spanish origin. The m ajority of 62 the Black and Chicano-American respondents w ere from the urban sample. The adults in the sample ranged in age from 22 to 50 years and the average age for the w ife w as alm ost 32 while the husbands' averaged alm ost 34 years of age (Table 2). length of 11.66 years. m arried 10 to 15 years. The Couples w ere m arried an average m ajority of couples (70.9 percent) were Not all couples living together reported they w ere married, but they w ere considered a s husband and wife. Husbands and wives, total number of children, other re la tiv e s and other individuals living in the residence w ere considered p a rt of the household. The number of persons in the household ranged from three to seven w ith the m ost frequent household size four members: w ife and two children. husband, Only one fam ily in the sample reported a non-relative living in the household and none of the fam ilies indicated any other rel?itives in residence. The number of children in the household ranged from one to five w ith an average of 2.4 children. Because fam ilies had to have at least one child to qualify for the sample, c h ildless couples were not included. As a resu lt, the average household size of the sample varied from the Lansing SMSA household average. The children in the sample w ere evenly distribu ted by age w ith the larg est number of children falling in the 11 y ears old category (20.5 percent). Husbands' and wives' educational levels differed somewhat. Over 9 percent of the w ives and 6.4 percent of the husbands did not complete high school. The 12th grade w as the highest level of education for 35 percent of the wives and 32 percent of the husbands. One-third of the w ives and one-fourth of the husbands reported th a t they s ta rte d college, but completed less than four years. Over o n e -fifth of the w ives (21.5 63 percent) finished four or more years of college. For the husbands, more than a third (34.5 percent) finished four years or more. Over half of the couples (52 percent) w ere single-earner fam ilies and 44.9 percent had both husband and w ife employed. At the tim e fam ilies filled out the questionnaires, both adults w ere unemployed in 2.8 percent of the fam ilies. In the overall population of workers in the Lansing SMSA, 12.6 percent w ere reported as unemployed for May 1980. More specifically, 12.5 percent w ere unemployed within the city of Lansing. Table 2.—Characteristics of Families in the Sample Characteristics Frequency (n=107) Percentage ( 100 .0 ) Mean Median Age in Years Husbands 21-25 26-30 31-35 36-40 41-45 46-50 Wives 21-25 26-30 31-35 36-40 41-45 45-50 Years Married* 0-3 4-6 7-9 10-12 13-15 16-18 19-21 2 22 52 24 3 4 1.8 20.6 48.5 22.4 2.7 3.7 33.79 33.46 3 32 52 18 2.8 31.89 31.85 29.8 48.5 16.8 11.66 12.39 2 0 6 6 9 34 42 8 2 1.8 0 5.6 5.6 8.4 31.7 39.2 7.5 1.8 64 Table 2. (cont'd.) Characteristics Frequency (n=107) Percentage (100.0) Mean Median Number of Persons in Household 3 4 5 6 7 11 52 34 9 1 10.3 48.6 31.8 8.4 0.9 4.41 4.79 Number of Children11 1 2 3 4 5 11 52 35 8 1 10.3 48.6 32.7 7.5 0.9 2.40 2.32 Age of Oldest Child 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Education Level Husbands 1-3 years of high school Completed high school (high school diploma) Less than 4 years of college 4 years of college 5 or more years of college ' Wives 1-3 years of high school Completed high school (high school diploma) Less than 4 years of college 4 years of college 5 or more years of college Boys 4 8 13 9 8 13 9 Girls 10 4 3 7 6 9 4 6 5.6 35 27 18 20 32.7 25.2 16.8 18.7 9 8.4 38 36 7 16 35.5 33.6 6.5 15.0 Boys 6.2 12.5 20.3 14.1 12.5 20.3 14.1 Girls 23.2 9.3 7.0 16.3 14.0 20.9 9.3 65 Table 2. (cont'd.) Characteristics Employment Status Single-earner Dual-earner Both unemployed Midpoint of Family Income Category (Annual)" " $7,500 $9,000 $ 11,000 $13,500 $17,500 $22,500 $27,500 $32,500 $42,500 $50,000-over Missing Data Occupation Husbands Professional-Technical Managerial-Administrative Sales Clerical Craftsman, operative, transport, laborer Service Private household workers Farmer Housespouse/Student Wives Professional-Technical Managerial-Administrative Sales Clerical Craftsman, operative, transport, laborer Service Private household workers Farmer Housespouse/Student Frequency (n=107) Percentage (100.0) 56 48 3 52.8 44.9 1 0.9 0.9 1 0 4 16 23 20 26 13 4 1 $26,752 $25,519 0 3.7 15.0 21.5 18.6 24.3 12.1 3.7 0.9 28.8 13.1 44 5 41.1 4.7 2.8 6.5 0 0 2 2 1.9 1.9 11 10.3 3 2.8 2 20 1.9 18.7 3 7 4 2.8 1 Median 2.8 30 14 3 7 56 Mean 6.5 3.7 0.9 52.3 * As reported by wives ""A combined report - Husband's and wive's personal income added together for family income 66 The median family income for the sample w as $25,519. The 1980 census e stim a te d median household income for the four census t r a c ts included in the th ree sample a reas as $ 19,400 and $ 1 4 ,8 0 0 (tracts 36.01 and 36.02, respectively) for the urban area, $18,400 for the small town and $17,900 for the rural area. The sample w as well above the median household income for each of the areas. The per capita income was determ ined by dividing the total family income by the number of persons in the household dependent upon the income. The average per capita income for the sam ple w as $5,622. The occupations of the husbands and wives were classified according to the 1970 United S ta te s Census Occupational Codes. Since the Codes include many diverse occupations, they were combined into more general traditionally categ ories fem ale for reporting occupations, purposes. husbands In all but outnumbered the wives. T w enty-eight percent of the husbands and 10.3 percent of the wives w ere professional-technical workers. In the m anagerial-adm inistrative w orkers category, the difference w as even more pronounced (13.1 percent of the husbands and 2.8 percent of the wives). Only a small portion of the sample w as employed in sa le s (2.8 percent of the husbands and 1.9 percent of the wives). Most of the employed wives were clerical w orkers (18.7 percent in c o n tra st to 6.5 percent of the husbands). Most of the employed husbands w ere in blue collar jobs such as craftsm en, tran sp o rt workers, operatives and laborers (41.1 percent). By comparison, only 2.8 percent of the w ives w ere employed in th is kind of work. Wives (10.2 percent) outnumbered the husbands (4.7 percent) in the combined categories of service and private household workers. Only a small percent of the husbands (2.9 percent) and wives (0.9 percent) 67 were farmers. The g re a te s t difference w as found in the category which included house spouses and students. Only 1.9 percent of the husbands reported being in th is category while it included more than half of the sample of wives (52.3 percent). Summary The fam ilies in the sam ple w ere a diverse group well represen tative of the larger population from which they w ere taken. They covered a wide range of income levels ($6,500 to over $50,000) and many occupations. Respondents w ere professionals, clerical workers and others worked in blue collar jobs. At the tim e of the survey, 85 percent of the men w ere employed and 12 percent w ere laid off. Slightly over half of the wives w ere not employed (52.3 percent). The average household in the sample consisted of a husband, w ife and two children. Instrum ent Development This researcher w as not p a rt of the p ro jec t team when the instrum ent w as developed. The following section is based on a report from a member of the p ro jec t (Ezell, 1981). The p ro ject team consisted of three faculty members from Michigan S ta te University and graduate students. The purpose of the p ro jec t w as to study household production; a comprehensive questionnaire w as developed tow ard th a t end. Household Production P ro ject members developed some of the questions and others w ere adapted from questions developed by other researchers. This researcher joined the p ro jec t when the data w ere being cleaned and prepared for analysis. All previous rep o rts on the analysis of the data have focused mainly on the adult respondents. This research er w as the only one to focus entirely (except for the demographic data) on the 68 children's questionnaire. The questionnaires for the children and adults w ere developed according to the following procedure (Ezell, 1981): 1. [Reviewing] of relevant lite ra tu re including professional journals and books, research reports, theoretical papers, magazine, and newspapers. 2. Asking experts to review and add to a lis t of household production activities. 3. Synthesizing the information gathered and developing pre­ liminary questionnaire. 4. Obtaining initial approval from the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects for p rete stin g the questionnaire. 5. P retesting the questionnaire on a selected group of families. 6. Altering the questionnaire to include recommended changes in the final questionnaire. 7. Obtaining final approval from the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects before beginning interview s in the sample areas, (pp. 7 0 -71) Related Literature For the development, particularly of the adult's questionnaire, four m ajor categories of lite ra tu re w ere examined: household production, quality of life, human capital development and family demographics. For th is study, two of these categories will be discussed in depth. The work of Andrews and Withey (1976) w as adapted for the questions on quality of life. Their faces scale (Figure 1) w as used in the children's questionnaire to a s s e s s overall perceived quality of life as well as the children's perceptions of several domains (Appendix C, p. 116). The scale is a graphic device which uses a s e rie s of seven stylized faces in which the shape of the mouth varies gradually from a big sm ile to a big frown. Sim ilar scales w ith a se rie s of five stylized faces have 69 been utilized by others studying the quality of life of children, but they don't appear to be as discriminating. A fter evaluating the faces scale w ith several other scales, Andrews and Withey find th at it yields m easures w ith relatively high v a lid ities and is also advantageous because it has explicitly labeled categories. Although th is scale produces skewed distrib u tio n s tow ard the positive end, it is more desirable than a purely verbal method because of the age of th is sample of children (between 6 to 12 years). In th is study, the seven categories on the faces scale are tre a te d as interval data. Andrews and Withey acknowledge th at th eir scale is sim ila r to two other scales which have been used as interval measures. Most of the categories on the faces scale seem to be sep arated by o ne-step intervals, though the m ost positive categories may be separated by less than one step. A B C D E F G Figure 1.— Faces Scale The questions on demographics in the husbands' and wives' questionnaires w ere taken from the Quality of Life Research Project sponsored by the Departments of Human Environment and Design and Family the and Child Ecology at Michigan S ta te University and Department of Clothing and Textiles, University of Minnesota. (The Michigan Agricultural Experiment Station Project Numbers were: 1,249 "Clothing Use and Quality of Life in Rural and Urban Communities," 3,151 70 "Families in Evolving Rural Communities." The Minnesota Agricultural Experiment Station P roject number was: 5 3 -0 8 6 "Clothing Use and Quality of Life in Rural and Urban Communities.") The initial questionnaires w ere developed utilizing the lite ra tu re review, p ro ject conferences and responses of persons asked to review a preliminary lis t of household production a ctiv itie s. The team attem pted to s t a te all the questions as simply and clearly as possible. Some m odifications of the questions resulted from the informal review by p ro ject members. Members of the Household Production P ro ject s t a f f p re te ste d the questionnaire. Nine urban, small town and rural fam ilies not living in the sample a reas participated in the p retest. Minor m odifications to the questionnaire resulted from th e ir responses. Description of Variables For th is study only p a rts of the questionnaires w ere used. Those sectio ns of the questionnaires used in th is study include: (1) the quality of life questions in the children's questionnaire (p. 2); (2) the children's perceived contribution to family work (p. 3); (3) the number and kinds of ta s k s performed by each child (p. 4); and (4) the demographic questions in both the adult's (pp. 28 -32, 34, 3 6 -3 8 ) and children's (p. 1) questionnaires. These sections have been included in Appendix C. Instrum entation and Scoring Perceived Quality of Life The child's quality of whole life score w as derived by using the child's questionnaire item number 8 (Faces Scale, Appendix C, p. 116). 71 Each face w as le tte re d from A to G. In coding, the le tte r s were converted to numbers w ith the higher the number, the higher perceived quality of life. Several scores on specific domains were also analyzed: th e ir family (item number 9), them selves (item number 10) and the amount of work they do a t home for the family (item number 11). Perceived Contributions to Family Work A score w as derived for the child's perceived contribution to family work using item 18 (Appendix C, p. 117) from the children's questionnaire. In th is section, the children w ere given a circle and told to pretend th a t the c irc le represented all of the work th a t needs doing in the home for the family. Then, they w ere told to draw and label a piece of pie in the circ le to show how much of the family work they did. Additional pieces w ere drawn for: (1) dad, (2) mom, (3) brothers and s is te r s , (4) work for which the family paid and (5) work th a t needs doing but is not done. By connecting the tw o lines for each of the pieces of the pie, isosceles triang les w ere formed, the bases w ere summed and the percentage each base w as of the to ta l represented each family member's contribution to fam ily work as perceived by the oldest child. In th is study, only the percentage representing the perceived contribution of the oldest child w as analyzed. A fter initial p rin tou ts of the data w ere perused, categories were developed to f a c ilita te fu rth er analysis. Children who said they contributed le ss than ten percent of all the family work were considered low perceivers. Medium perceivers contributed between ten to tw enty percent and high perceivers contributed more than tw enty percent of all the family work. 72 Number of Tasks Performed To determ ine the number of ta s k s each child performed, item s 25 to 47 (Appendix C, p. 118) from the children's questionnaire w ere scored. These item s w ere scored on a yes-no basis, Yes, I take responsibility and No, I do not take responsibility. Although only 23 ta s k s were listed, the highest score could have exceeded th is number because an open-ended question at the end of th is lis t of item s asked the children to w rite other jobs they do. Categories w ere also developed in th is section to fa c ilita te fu rth er data analysis. appeared to Initial printo uts of the data indicated th a t the scores c lu s te r into three groups. Low perform ers took responsibility for th ree to eleven task s, medium perform ers for tw elve to fifte e n and high perform ers for sixteen to tw en ty -fo u r tasks. S ta tis tic a l Analysis Descriptive s t a t i s t i c s w ere used to characterize the sample. Mean, median, mode and standard deviation w ere calculated where appropriate to describe the sample. In th is study, oneway analysis of variance, chi-square, z - t e s t s and correlatio ns w ere also utilized to determine significance of the differences found. Oneway analysis of variance (ANOVA) w as used in th is study to compare children of varying ages and differen t sexes. This sample conformed to the assum ptions specified when using oneway ANOVA. Observations were independently selected from normal populations w ith homeogeneous variance. ANOVA was utilized to analyze the scores derived for the child's perceived contribution to family work. The scores w ere analyzed f i r s t by sex, then by age. A twoway ANOVA by age and sex 73 could not be performed because the c ells w ere not orthogonal. The child’s quality of whole life score and sco res on differen t domains were also analyzed using a oneway ANOVA, f i r s t by sex, then by age. Chi-square w as used to t e s t for the existence of a significant difference between boys and g irls on number of ta s k s performed. Categories w ere developed for low, medium and high perform ers and the boys and g irls w ere segregated according to these categories. Chi-square w as then performed to determ ine if any discrepancies existed betw een the expected and observed frequencies. The tw o sample z - t e s t w as utilized to determ ine if a significant difference existed between the proportion of boys and g irls who took responsibility for a task. Each task w as analyzed individually. The Pearson correlation coefficient, r, m easures the strength of the relationship between tw o continuous variables. Basically, it re fle c ts the ease w ith which one can e s tim a te the value of a variable through knowledge of the value of a second variable. In th is study, correlation is utilized to determ ine if a relationship e x is ts between age and number of task s performed. This sample conforms to the assum ptions made when using the Pearson correlation: linearity, random sampling, bivariate normal d istribu tions and interval level data (Nie, e t al.. 1975). In any s t a ti s t i c a l testing, the probability of error is a m ajor consideration. Error r e s u lts from reje ctin g the null hypothesis when it is tru e (Type I error) and from failing to r e je c t a hypothesis when it is false (Type II error). Acceptable levels of e rro r m ust be established according to credible research p rac tic es and the specific nature of the research. Because th is research is exploratory in nature, the .10 probability of error level is employed in order to avoid overlooking 74 differences and relatio nsh ip s which may be of potential importance. The .10 level greatly increases chances for making a Type I error, so whenever possible, the .05 level is also reported. This improves sc ie n tific credibility and provides a balance between the probability of Type I and Type II errors. Lim itations The present study is lim ited to the examination of children's perceptions of 1) th e ir contribution to household work and 2) their quality of life as measured by subjective indicators. The child's perception of th e ir contribution to household work, though it may not coincide w ith the perceptions of th e ir parents, is important because it helps to define a domain which is one part of the child's evaluation of quality of life as a whole. The sample used in th is study is not rep resentative of other family forms, i.e., single parent fam ilies, older fam ilies, fam ilies w ith no children. Therefore, the present analysis is not generalizable to these other types of fam ilies, but it sheds some light on young fam ilies in the United States. Because only young fam ilies w ere studied, the vast m ajority of them are composed of only four members: mother, fath e r and th eir two children. The study is lim ited in application by the sample, but nevertheless has useful findings. The employment of the m others w as not taken into consideration in the analysis of the data because the review of lite ra tu re indicated that m other's employment does not appear to have an e ffe c t on children's participation in household work if the children are under 12 years of age (as all the children in th is sample were). 75 Data analyses w ere also lim ited by the small number (107) of children participating in the study. Twoway analysis by age and sex could not be performed because the c e lls w ere not orthogonal and because some of c ells w ere too small. The eight year olds, for example, had the larg e st number of boys (13) in any age group and the sm a llest number of g irls (3). Limitations of Instrument The children's perceptions of th e ir contributions may have lit t le to do w ith the actual carrying out of household production responsibilities, particularly w ith the youngest children. However, th e ir perceptions are important because they re fle c t rea lity as perceived by th a t individual. The reliab ility and contributions are unknown. validity of the question on perceived This resea rch e r wonders w hether children can understand the a b stra c t concept of proportion when they are asked to divide the pie into p a rts representing each family member's share of the work in the home. The child's (especially the 6 and 7 year olds) motor coordination is also questionable. Do they have the m otor coordination to depict the radial lines which accurately rep resen t the p a rt of the circle which describes th e ir contributions? children’s responses would be desirable. T est r e - t e s t reliability on the Chapter 5 FINDINGS This chapter contains the r e s u lts of the data analyses. The re s u lts are divided into th ree sectio n s under the following headings: (I) child's perceived contribution to household work; (2) quantity and kinds of ta s k s performed by the child; and (3) child's perceived quality of life. Each section contains research questions and hypotheses form ulated from them. Table 3 gives a summary of the sample of children by age and sex. Table 3.—Summary of Sample by Age and Sex Age of Child 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Total Frequency Boys Girls 4 8 13 9 8 13 9 64 10 4 3 7 6 9 4 43 Percentage Boys Girls 6.2 12.5 20.3 14.1 12.5 20.3 14.1 100.0 76 23.2 9.3 7.0 16.3 14.0 20.9 9.3 100.0 Percentage Total 13.1 11.2 15 15 13.1 20.5 12.1 100.0 77 Child's Perceived Contribution to Household Work The research questions on the child's perceived contribution to household work were: 1. What is the child's perceived contribution to the household work of the fam ily? Are there any differences: a. In perceptions of boys and g irls ? b. Between groups? The f i r s t hypothesis form ulated using the review of lite ra tu re was: HO 1. Boys and g irls will d iffer in perceived contribution to the household work of the family. The perceived contribution of g irls will be higher than th at of boys. A percent score w as derived for the child's perceived contribution to the household work of the family using the piece of the pie designated by the child. The mean percentage for the boys' scores was 13.84, the median w as 13.80 and the standard deviation w as 5.02. The boys' scores ranged from a low of 5.7% to a high of 25.1%. The mean percentage for the girls' sco res w as 15.63, the median w as 14.40 and the standard deviation w as 6.98. The girls' sco res ranged from a low of 3.5% to a high of 38.1%. A more detailed presentation of these descriptive s t a t i s t i c s can be found on Table D-1 in the Appendix. The sco res w ere also analyzed using a oneway ANOVA by sex. Although the F-value (3.574, df = 1, 104) w as not significant at the .05 level, it w as significant a t the .10 level. This analysis does not support Ho 1 at the .05 level of significance and the hypothesis is rejected. However, the difference found betw een the boys and g irls is significant at the . 10 level; future study is suggested. Ho 2. Age groups will d iffe r in perceived contributions to the household work of the family. will increase w ith age. The perceived contribution 78 Using the sam e percent score, a oneway ANOVA by age w as performed. Age groups differed significantly in th eir perceived contributions to the household work of the family (F=2.289, df=6, 99, p<.05). When the scores w ere broken down by different age groups, Figure 2, a trend emerged. Children's perceptions of th e ir contributions were highest a t ages 6 and 7, peaking at age 7. Their perceptions of th e ir contributions declined from age 7 and increased again around ages 11 and 12. Hypothesis 2 w as only partially supported because, though the age groups differed in th e ir perceived contributions, they did not differ in the p a ttern suggested. 18 17 ■ n • j Perceived Contribution (Percentage) 15" 14 Io 12 - 6 7 8 9 Age (Years) Figure 2. - - Child's Perceived Contribution by Age 10 12 79 Quantity and Kinds of Tasks Performed by the Child Several resea rch questions w ere developed concerning the quantity and kinds of ta s k s performed by the children: 2. How many ta s k s are being performed by each child? Are any d ifferen ces between the number of task s boys and perform ? Are traditional sex role stereo ty pes adhered to task s are se le c te d ? Are older children performing more than younger children? there girls when tasks The review of the lite ra tu re resu lted in the development of the third hypothesis: Ho 3. Boys and g irls will d iffer in the number of task s performed. Girls will perform more ta s k s than boys. The mean for the to ta l number of ta s k s performed by the boys was 12.46, the median w as 11.89 and the standard deviation w as 3.86. The to tal number of ta s k s performed by the boys ranged from a low of 3 to a high of 23. The mean for the total number of task s performed by the g irls w as 13.86, the median w as 13.19 and the standard deviation was 3.72. The to tal number of task s performed by the g irls ranged from a low of 6 to a high of 24 (one of the g irls said she did all of the task s and she w ro te an additional one in on the space provided). A more detailed p resen tation of these descriptive s t a t i s t i c s can be found on Table D-2 in the Appendix. A chi-square t e s t (A '2 =5.50, df=2, p<.075) for significance of difference betw een boys and g irls on number of task s performed resulted in a marginally significant difference. The re s u lts from th is t e s t were summarized on Table 4. It revealed th a t boys were overrepresented in the group of low perform ers (three to eleven tasks) and underrepresented in the group of high perform ers (sixteen to tw enty-fou r tasks). Table 3 80 also indicated th a t the reverse w as tru e for the sample of girls. This analysis w as in support of hypothesis 3. Table 4.—Number of Tasks Performed by Boys and Girls Number of Tasks Boys Girls Observed Expected $ Deviation Low 3-11 Medium 12-15 High 16-24 X Observed Expected % Deviation 10 15.67 -36.18 29 23.33 24.30 21 16.88 24.41 21 25.12 -16.40 12 10.45 14.83 14 15.55 -9.97 2=5.50, p<.075 Ho 4. Boys and g irls will d iffer in the kinds of ta s k s performed. Girls will do more in-the-hom e work while boys will work more outside. A z - t e s t w as performed on each task to see if a significant difference existed between the proportion of boys and g irls who took responsibility for the task. The r e s u lts from these t e s t s were summarized on Table 5. Hypothesis 4 w as supported because the task s girls performed significantly more than boys were all in-the-hom e task s while the two task s boys performed significantly more than g irls were both outside tasks. 81 Table 5 - - P e r c e n ta g e of Boys and Girls Performing Specific Household Tasks Task Percent Boys Girls Z Tasks Without Significant Male-Female Differences Do yard work Do some shopping Earn money Collect d irty clothes for laundry Vacuum rugs, carpets, floors Take care of p ets or animals Put away groceries Take s h e e ts off the bed or put sh e ets on Put away toys, clean my room Wash and dry the clothes Wash and dry dishes Take care of other children Sweep sidew alks, porches or patio, floors Wash floors 86 42 63 56 69 64 67 50 95 8 45 59 41 9 77 33 60 53 70 65 70 56 98 16 58 72 58 21 Tasks Boys Perform Significantly More Than Girls Take out garbage or trash Shovel snow 69 80 35 51 3.47*** Tasks Girls Perform Significantly More Than Boys Dust fu rniture Set and c le a r table, wash table Put away the clean clothes Clean sinks, bathtubs, to ile ts Write a l e t t e r Water the plants Prepare and cook m eals 38 73 48 30 41 27 23 81 95 74 53 63 47 42 -4.39* ** -2.89* * -2 .68 ** -2.39** -2.2 3* -2.13* -2.09* * Significant at .05 level. ** Significant at .01 level. * * * Significant at .005 level 1.20 .94 .31 .31 -.11 -.11 -.33 -.61 -.80 -1.29 -1.32 -1.38 -1.73 -1.76 3.16** 82 Ho 5. Age groups w ill differ in the number of task s performed. Older children w ill children. perform more ta s k s than younger Pearson correlation w as utilized to t e s t if significant differences existed betw een the d ifferen t age groups on the mean number of task s they performed. When the boys and g irls w ere combined and the correlatio ns w ere performed, no significant difference (r=.1093, df = 106, p<. 15) existed. Then, the boys and g irls were separated and the correlatio ns performed. The correlation coefficient for the boys was not significant (r=.0263, df=63, p<.57). However, a significant relationship w as found between age and ta s k s for the g irls (r= 275, df=42, p<.04). When extrem e cases (the two g irls performing the le a st number of tasks) w ere removed, the relationship w as even more significant (r=.402, df=41, p<.005). Older g irls appear to perform more tasks. Hypothesis 5 would have to be re je c te d when boy's and girl's scores were combined, but when g irls were analyzed separately, the hypothesis was supported. Child's Perceived Quality of Life The research questions on the child’s perceived quality of life were: 4. What is the child's perceived quality of life? Are there any differences: a. In the sco res of boys and g irls ? b. Between the age groups? Because of the paucity of research on children's quality of life, the hypotheses developed in th is section w ere broad and exploratory in nature: Ho 6. Boys and g irls will not differ in perceived quality of life. 83 The child's quality of whole life score w as analyzed using a oneway ANOVA by sex. The resulting F-value (2.569, df=1, 105) w as not even significant at the .10 level which indicated th a t hypothesis 6 should be accepted. Gender does not appear to be a significant facto r influencing perceived quality of life. Scores on specific domains (i.e. th e ir family, them selves and the amount of work they do a t home for the family) were also analyzed using oneway ANOVA and none of the F-values reached the .10 level of significance. Ho 7. Age groups will not d iffe r in perceived quality of life. When a oneway ANOVA by age w as performed, the resulting F-value (3.143, df=6, 100) w as significant a t the .007 level which indicated th at hypothesis 7 should be rejected. When the scores w ere broken down into different age groups (Figure 3), the p a ttern which emerged indicated th a t younger children scored higher on perceived quality of life than older children. The scores on th e ir family and work participation domains were not significant. The se lf domain w as significant at the .077 level, but when the scores w ere broken down into d ifferent age groups (Figure 4), a definite p a ttern did not emerge. 84 7.50 7.25 7.00 Mean Quality of Whole Life 6.75 6.50 6.25 6.00 5.75 5.50 5.25 5.00 8 9 10 II 12 Age (Years) Figure 3. - - Child's Perceived Quality of Whole Life by Age Mean Quality of Self Domain 7.50 7.25 7.00 6.75 6.50 6.25 6.00 5.75 5.50 5.25 5.00 6 7 8 9 10 Age (Years) Figure 4. ~ Child's Perceived Quality of Self Domain by Age 12 Chapter 6 CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS This summary chapter includes an overview of the study w ith m ajor conclusions and a discussion of the findings. Also included are the implications of the study as well as recommendations for future research. Overview of the Study This research examined the participation of children in household work and th e ir perceived quality of life. The m ajor purpose of th is descriptive study w as to identify perceived contributions to household work of children of se lec te d ages and both sexes. The data w ere taken from a larger study "Contributions of Household Production to Family Income" (Michigan Agricultural S tation Project 1363H). In the larger study, the family w as the unit of analysis. The sample w as 107 fam ilies from urban, small town and rural locations in mid-Michigan. Each husband, w ife and oldest child (between the ages of six and twelve) were given a se lf-a d m in iste red questionnaire. In th is study, the only section utilized from the husbands’ and wives' questionnaires w as the section on demographics. These demographics w ere used to describe the sample. The m ajority of the analysis focused on the responses of the children from three sectio n s of th e ir questionnaire. These data w ere analyzed to examine the child’s perceived contribution to household work, the * 85 86 quantity and kinds of ta s k s child's perceived quality performed of by life. the child and the S ta tis tic a l t e s t s used were analysis of variance, chi-square, z - t e s t and correlation. Major Conclusions The m ajor conclusions of the study were: 1. Boys and girls do not d iffer significantly in th eir perceived contribution to the household work of the family. 2. Age groups d iffer significantly in perceived contributions to the household work of the family. Children's perceptions are highest at ages 6 and 7, peaking at age 7. Their perceptions of their contributions decline from age 7 and increase again around ages 11 and 12. 3. Boys and g irls d iffer significantly in the number of task s reported being performed. Boys are overrepresented in the group of low perform ers (three to eleven task s) and underrepresented in the group of high perform ers (sixteen to tw enty -four tasks). The reverse is tru e for the sample of girls. 4. Boys and g irls d iffer significantly in the kinds of task s performed. Girls do more in-the-hom e work while boys work more outside. 5. Age groups d iffer significantly in the number of ta s k s performed when the girls' data are analyzed. Older g irls appear to perform more ta s k s than younger girls. The age of the boy does not appear to influence the number of ta s k s performed. 6. Boys and girls do not differ significantly in perceived quality of life. 87 7. Age groups d iffer significantly in perceived quality of life. Younger children score higher on perceived quality of whole life sco res than older children. Discussion of Findings Discussion of the r e s u lts of the data analysis is organized around the four research questions. 1. What is the child's perceived contribution to the household work of the fam ily? Are there any differences: a. In perceptions of boys and girls? b. Between age groups? When reviewing the lite ra tu re in th is area, it w as obvious th a t many stu d ies had been done to quantify children's contributions to the household work of the family. In some studies, parents (particularly m others) w ere asked to measure the extent of th e ir child's responsibility to household work (Walker, 1975a). tim e 1973; Walker and Woods, 1976; Lynch, In other studies, children were asked to give e stim a te s of the they spent generally in household work or in specific household ta s k s (Tengel, 1964;Hoppen, 1966). S till o th ers combined the reports of parents and children (Temple University In stitu te for Survey Research, 1976). None of the stu d ies reviewed by this researcher asked the children to view th e ir contributions to the household work of the family in relation to all the work done in the home. As a result, the use of the pie w as unique in th a t it attem p ted to discover the child's perception of th eir contribution in relation to other family members. The re s u lts of th is study suggest that fem ales and m ales do not d iffer significantly in th eir rela tiv e perceived contribution to the 88 household work of the family. Because of these re su lts, the f i r s t hypothesis had to be re je c te d a t the .05 level of significance. Although th is hypothesis had to be rejected , when the sco res w ere broken down by d ifferent age groups, a trend emerged indicating th a t girls' perceptions of th e ir contributions w ere g re a te r than boys’ in all age groups except two (in th ese two c e lls the ra tio of boys to g irls significantly favored the boys). What w as significant about th is analysis w as the sim ila rity between the the boys' and girls' perceptions of th e ir contributions. Although no tim e data w ere collected on the task s children said they performed, on all but six (out of 23) tasks, the proportion of girls performing the task is g rea ter than th a t of boys. Of significance is the fa c t that, although the g irls in th is sample w ere contributing signficantly more than the boys, th e ir peceptions of th eir contributions w ere not significantly (at the .05 level) different. In these "liberated" tim es, these re s u lts are alarming. discrepancy between perceived contributions and ta s k s This performed su ggests th at fem ales underestim ate th e ir actual contributions of work to the household. In light of the inequity (in task performance) which e x is ts in th is sample of children, it appears th a t the g irls may be developing higher expectations of th e ir participation, thus reporting a lower contribution to the household work. The ram ifications of these differences in boys' and girls' expectations will be fu rth er discussed in the final section of th is chapter. The developm entalist approach w as utilized as the conceptual framework underlying th is study and w as reflected in the hypotheses. Developmentalists s t r e s s the regular and cumulative a sp ec ts of the growth process. Because of th is view of the growth process, it w as 89 hypothesized th a t age groups would d iffer in th e ir perceived contributions to the household work of the family w ith contributions increasing w ith age. Erikson (1963) suggested th a t the child from five through preadolescence gains "insights into the in stitu tio n s, functions, and roles which perm it his responsible participation" finding "pleasurable accomplishment in wielding tools...and in caring for younger children." Because these children tended to p a rticip a te more actively in the household work of the family as they moved through th is period, it w as hypothesized th a t they would perceive th e ir contributions increasing in relation to the contributions of other family members. This trend does emerge when the scores from the eight (mean= 11.1%) through tw elve (mean=16.4%) year olds w ere analyzed. The hypothesis does not explain the high scores of the six (mean=16.2%) and seven (mean= 17.4%) year olds. A review of the lite ra tu re (Gesell, Arnold, llg and Ames, 1977) indicates th a t the six year old is often seen as se lf-c e n te re d and th is perspective may d is to rt the child's perception of th eir contribution in relation to other members of the family. Because they cannot observe and may not comprehend all of th e ir m other's and fa th e r's contributions, th is resea rch e r is not surprised th a t one six year old girl said th a t her contribution represented 36% of all the work done in the home. The seven year old is described as beginning to be thoughtful, to be considerate and anxious to please. The child w ants to find th e ir place in the family group and is ready to take some of the household responsibilities. Many like to help and th is may explain the high sco res of the seven year olds in th is sample. 90 2. How many ta s k s are being performed by each child? Are there any differences between the number of ta s k s boys and girls perform ? Are traditional sex role ste reo ty p es adhered to when task s are s e le c te d ? Are older children performing more task s than younger children? The research mentioned e a rlie r focusing on the fa c to rs which m ost influence children’s involvement in household work highlighted the importance of sex and sex roles. A review of th is lite ra tu re resulted in the hypothesis th at boys and g irls would d iffer in the number of task s performed w ith g irls performing more tasks. The analysis of the number of ta s k s boys and g irls said they performed resulted in the acceptance of th is hypothesis. The boys in the sample w ere overrepresented by 24% in the group of low perform ers (3-11 tasks). The g irls in th is group were underrepresented by 36%. In the group of high perform ers (1 6 -2 4 tasks), the boys w ere underrepresented by 10% and the g irls were overrepresented by 15%. When the proportion of boys and g irls performing each task was analyzed, the re s u lts supported the hypothesis th a t boys and g irls would d iffer in the kinds of ta s k s they performed. As the lite ra tu re suggested, the g irls in th is sam ple performed more in-the-hom e work while the boys performed more work outside. The two ta s k s which boys performed significantly more than the g irls w ere taking out the garbage and shoveling the snow (th is task w as seasonal). The g irls performed seven ta s k s significantly more than the boys, and of the remaining fourteen tasks, the proportion favored the g irls in ten of them. Although this study did not collect d ata on the amount of tim e which w as spent on each task, it w as obvious th a t the performance of boys and g irls was significantly different. 91 It can be argued th a t the higher proportion of g irls in household task performance is related to less interference w ith early role-modeling (Stephens, 1963). Development occurs from early childhood in te re s ts of wanting to be like m other and participate in her work to being m other's helper. Even in these "liberated" tim es, the ta s k s liste d are s till performed significantly more by women than they are by men. If such behavior continues, traditional role designation may prevail for women through adulthood. In an era of increasing participation of women in the workforce, th ese traditional sex role expectations may have deleterious e ff e c ts on women. This research er wonders if society is creating a situation where women m ust be "superwomen" to survive—doing the lion’s share of the work in the home while participating equally in the workforce. When task performance was analyzed using the d ifferent age groups, an even more alarming trend emerged. Utilizing the conceptual framework, it w as hypothesized th at the age groups would d iffer in the number of ta s k s performed w ith older children performing more task s than younger children. Although no signficant difference existed between the age groups, when boys and g irls were analyzed separately, a significant relationship w as found between age and ta s k s for the girls. Older g irls appeared to perform more ta s k s than younger girls. This re s u lt suggests that, as the number of ta s k s increases w ith age, the child is becoming an eager and absorbed unit of a productive situation (Erikson, 1963). By age ten, the child comes to appreciate work completion and diligence, internalizing the work principle. The re s u lts from th is analysis lead th is research er to ask how th is process is affected if responsibility for household work does not appear to increase 92 (as w ith the boys in th is sam ple)? Do boys learn to become responsible and productive in work situ a tio n s outside the home? 3. What is the child's perceived quality of life? Are there any differences: a. In the sco res of boys and g irls ? b. Between age groups? The lite ra tu re maintained th at gender w as not a significant facto r influencing perceived quality of life so th is researcher hypothesized th at no difference would be found between the sco res of boys and girls. The analysis of the child's quality of whole life score as well as quality of several domains (i.e. th e ir family, them selves and the amount of work they do at home for the family) supported th is hypothesis. Because of the paucity of research on children's quality of life, it w as also hypothesized th a t age groups would not differ in perceived quality of whole life. A highly significant (.007 level) difference w as apparent when the sco res w ere analyzed by age. When the scores were broken down into d ifferen t age groups, the pattern which emerged indicated th a t younger children scored higher on perceived quality of whole life than older children. This trend w as also perceived when quality of school life w as studied by Epstein and McPartland (1976). They suggested th at th is trend (for quality of school life sco res to decrease as grade level increases) e x is ts because "the objective quality of school goes down each year and/or th a t w ith m aturity, studen ts more c ritic a lly evaluate th eir environments." They argued that, w ith age, students' a b ilities become more varied and schools "may be le ss able to m eet the more diverse academic in te r e s ts and needs of older students". 93 This may also be the case w ith the home environment. As a resu lt of the maturing process, the older children in the sample may more c ritic a lly evaluate th e ir home environment. The older children also have more interaction w ith the outside environment and th is may c re a te a situ atio n where it is more d iffic u lt for the family to m eet the more diverse in te r e s ts and needs of th is older child. As a result, quality of whole life sco res decrease w ith age. 4. Is there a relationship between: a. The child’s perceived contribution to household work and the number of ta s k s perform ed? Are there differences between boys and g irls ? b. The child’s perceived contribution to household work and perceived quality of life ? c. The number of task s performed and perceived quality of life ? This research question w as exploratory in nature because a review of the lite ra tu re gave lit t le a ssista n c e in the development of hypotheses. A chi-square analysis of the relationship between the child’s perceived contribution to household work and the number of task s performed indicated th a t none existed. When the boys and girls were analyzed separately, the level of significance decreased. Correlation of the relationship between the child’s perceived contribution to household work and perceived quality of life also w as not significant. When number of ta s k s and perceived quality of life were analyzed, no relationship w as apparent. Because the quality of life data were so skewed toward the positive end (w ith m ost children marking the tw o happiest faces in the scale of seven faces), there was too l ittle variance to conduct any meaningful test. 94 Implications of the Study Although data on the amount of tim e each child spent on a task were not collected, it w as ODvious th a t th is sample of girls performed signficantly more ta s k s (from the lis t of 23) than the boys in the sample. The difference in the proportion of g irls and boys performing ta s k s w as not refle cted in the childrens’ perceptions of th e ir contributions to family work. The g irls and boys did not differ (at the .05 level of significance) in th e ir perceived contributions to the work of the household. The older g irls in th is sample, as hypothesized, performed more task s than the younger girls. This was an indication of the acquisition of responsibility and the basic habits of industry (Havighurst, 1964). For the boys in th is sample, th is trend w as not observed which may indicate a difference in the so cialization process for boys and girls. Many women’s movement advocates are in te reste d in the sex role socialization of children because they see the seed for future perpetuation of role inequities planted at a very early age. The re s u lts from th is study will alarm these advocates because they provide strong evidence th a t th is sam ple of children continues to adhere to traditional p a tte rn s in household task participation. Boys continue to do the work outside the home (much of which is seasonal) while girls participate more frequently in inside a c tiv itie s (the m ajority of which need to be performed on a regular basis). Like Thrall (1978), th is researcher is in terested in role stereotypy or the normative expectation th a t one person is supposed to do a task and th at another is then expected not to do it. Thrall's findings suggest th a t the b e st single predictor of a fam ily's division of labor may be the $20,000 - $24,999 ) $5,000 - $5,999 ( ) $25,000 - $29,999 ) $6,000 - $6,999 ( ) $30,000 - $34,999 ) $7,000 - $7,999 ( ) $35,000 - $49,000 ) $8,000 - $9,999 ( ) $50,000 and o v e r >$10,000 - $ | 1,999 42 .7b About how much o f t h i s t o t a l in 1979? f a m i l y y e a r l y income b e f o r e ta x e s d i d you ea rn ESTIMATED PORTION OF TOTAL FAMILY INCOME. 1979. EARNED BY YOURSELF ( ) Does n o t a p p l y , n o t employed in 1979 ( ) Under $3,000 ( ( ) $3,000 - $3,999 ( ) $4,000 - $4,999 ( ) $12,000 - $14,999 ( ) $15,000 - $19,999 ( ) $20,000 - $24,999 ) $5,000 - $5,999 ( ) $25,000 - $29,000 ( ) $6,000 - $6,999 ( ) $30,000 and o v e r ( ) $7,000 - $7,999 ( ) $8,000 - $9,999 ( >$10,000 - $ l I ,999 Ill 43.1 Do you ( o r does a member o f y ou r f a m i l y who l i v e s w i t h you) own your home, do you r e n t? ( ( ( 43 .2 How long have you l i v e d ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( 4 3 .3 ) Own o r bu yin g ) R e ntin g ) O th er____________________________________ Please s p e d f y n. t ■. i i-t. o r apartment? ) Less than I year ) 2 3 yea rs ) 4 - 6 y ea rs ) 7 9 yea rs )10 - 12 yea rs )13 -1 5 years ) 16 - 18 yea rs )19 -21 yea rs )22 -24 years )25 ye a rs o r more How many rooms do you have in y o u r d w e l l i n g , not c o u n t in g bathrooms? 4 3 . 3 a ________________ 43 .3b ________________________________ ' Number o f rooms Number o f bathrooms 43.4 How much does housing c o s t f o r y o u r f a m i l y ? Ple ase i n d i c a t e t h e amount you pay each month f o r r e n t o r mortgage, in c l u d i n g p r o p e r t y ta x e s and in sura nc e. ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ( ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) Less tha n $100 p e r month $101 $150 per month $151 $200 pe r month $201 $250 pe r month $251 $300 pe r month $301 $350 p e r month $351 $400 p e r month $401 $450 p e r month $451 $500 p e r month $501 $550 p e r month $551 $600 pe r month $601 $650 p e r month More th a n $650 pe r month ________________________ __ P le ase s p e c i f y 112 36 4 5 -la Is t h i s y o u r f i r s t m arriage ? ( ) YES- ( ) NO- 45, lb In what y e a r and month d i d your p r e s e n t m a rria g e begin? _________________________ 45.1c How d i d y ou r l a s t m a rria g e end and in what year? 45. ) Death Year o f death ) D ivo rc e Year o f d i v o r c e ) Annulment Year o f annulment ) S eparation_ Year o f s e p a r a tio n _ Id Please s p e c i f y t h e b e g in n in g and e n ding dates o f any m a rria g e s e x i s t i n g p r i o r t o t h e one d e s c r ib e d in 4 4 .1 c . 4 5 . le We would l i k e t o know som ethin g ab ou t t h e pe o p le who l i v e In y o u r f a m i l y . Please l i s t in t h e c h a r t below y o u r c h i l d r e n and o t h e r household memberst h e i r b i r t h d a te , age a t l a s t b i r t h d a y , sex, and i n d i c a t e by u sing a check mark i f you a re f i n a n c i a l l y r e s p o n s ib l e f o r th e s u p p o r t o f t h e p e r s o n . Date o f bi r t h n o ./d a y /y r. SPOUSE CHILDREN BORN TO THIS MARRIAGE Please 1i s t in o r d e r from o l d e s t t o youngest CHILDREN BORN TO WIFE PRIOR TO THIS MARRIAGE Please 1i s t in o r d e r from o l d e s t t o youngest CHILDREN BORN TO HUSBAND PRIOR TO THIS MARRIAGE Please 1i s t in o r d e r from o l d e s t t o youngest ADOPTED CHILDREN NOT BORN TO EITHER SPOUSE Please 1i s t in o r d e r from o l d e s t t o youngest Age a t la s t b irth d a y Sex F ln a n c ia 1 Support ( c 1r c 1e M o r F) M F 1. M F 2. M F 3. M F 4. M F 5. M F 6. M F 7. M F 8. M F 9. M F 1. M F 2. M F 3. M F 4. M F 5. M F 1. M F M F F 2. - 3. M 4. M F 5. M F F 1. M 2. M F 3. M F 4. M F 5. M F CONTINUED ON NEXT PAGE. NOTE: I f t h e r e a re n o t enough spaces, p le a s e f i n i s h th e l i s t on t h e l a s t page. 38 Date o f b i rth m o ./d a y/yr. OTHER RELATIVES LIVING IN THIS HOUSEHOLD (such as n ie c e , nephew, g r a n d c h i l d , p a re n t, s i s t e r , u n c le , b r o t h e r , b r o t h e r - i n - 1aw, m o t h e r- i n - 1aw, husband's u n c le ) OTHER PERSONS LIVING IN THIS HOUSEHOLD (such as f o s t e r ch i Id, f r i e n d , household h e lp , b o a rd e rs ) NOTE: 46. Age a t la s t b i r th d a y M a rita l status Sex 1. M F 2. M F 3. M F 4. M F 5. M F 6. M F 7. M F 8. M F 1. M F 2. M F 3. M F 4. M F 5. M F 6. M F 7. M F R e la t io n t o you I f t h e r e are not enough spa ces, ple a s e f i n i s h t h e l i s t on t h e l a s t page. Co un ting y o u r s e l f , People how many people now l i v e in you r household? Fi nanci a 1 Support 115 1. How o l d a r e you today? 2. When is y o u r b i r t h d a y ? month day 3. Are vou a q i r l o r a boy? 4. What are t h e t h i n g s you l i k e t o do f o r f u n ? 5. What k in d s o f t h i n g s does y o u r f a m i l y do t o save money? 6. What c o u ld you do t o h e lp y o u r f a m i l y save money? 7. What a re t h e t h i n g s you l i k e b e s t abou t l i v i n g in you r f a m il y ? 116 2 HERE ARE SOME FACES SHOWING FEELINGS. 8. Under each face is a l e t t e r . Which fa c e shows how you f e e l about y o u r 11fe ( y o u r w hole l i f e ) ? ( W r i t e t h e l e t t e r on t h e l i n e . ) 9. _Which face shows how you f e e l about y ou r own f a m iIy ? 10. _Which fa c e shows t h e way you f e e l about y o u r s e I f ? 11 _Which face shows t h e way you f e e l about th e amount o f work you do a t home f o r t h e f a m il y ? 12. Which fa c e shows t h e way you f e e l about th e amount o f money y o u r f a m i l y has? 13. _________Which fa c e shows t h e way you f e e l about th e chances you have t o le a rn new t h i n g s ? 14. _________How do you f e e l ab ou t t h e way you spend your f r e e tim e a t home? 15. _________Which face shows t h e way you f e e l about food you e a t , and t h e c lo t h e s you wear? t h e house you have, t h e 16. _________Which f a c e shows t h e way you f e e l about t h e t h i n g s y ou r f a m i l y has y o u r c a r , f u r n i t u r e , t o y s , games, and p l a y t h i n g s . 17- Which face shows t h e way you f e e l about t h e changes y o u r f a m i l y may need t o make t o save energy l i k e keeping th e house c o o l e r , fewer t r i p s in t h e c a r , and u s ing less e l e c t r i c a l energy? 117 3 HERE IS A CIRCLE. f o r th e f a m i 1y . Pre te nd t h e c i r c l e is a l l o f t h e work t h a t needs d o in g a t home ( T h is would be work l i k e housework, y ard w o rk, f i x i n g t h i n g s , bu yin g t h i n g s and h e l p i n g p e o p le . ) Each person does some work in t h e f a m i l y . Sometimes f a m i l i e s pay o t h e r pe o p le t o do some work f o r them. Sometimes t h e work is n o t done a t a l l . 18. Draw a p ie c e o f p i e in t h e c i r c l e t o show how much o f t h e f a m i I y work W r it e ME in t h a t space. you do. 19. Draw a do es. p ie c e o f p i e in t h e c i r c l e t o show W r it e DAD in t h a t space. how much work in t h e f a m i l y y ou r dad 20. Draw a do es. p ie c e o f p i e in t h e c i r c l e t o show W r i t e MOM in t h a t space. how much work in t h e f a m i l y y o u r mom 21 . Draw a p ie c e o f p i e in t h e c i r c l e t o show how much work a l l o f y o u r b r o t h e r s s i s t e r s d o . W r it e BAS in t h a t space. I f you do n o t have a b r o t h e r o r s i s t e r w r i t e a check mark he re_______ . 22. Draw a p ie c e o f p i e in t h e c i r c l e t o show how much work in t h e f a m i l y you pay o t h e r p e op le t o do - l i k e f i x t h i n g s o r c le a n t h i n g s . W r it e PAY in t h a t space. 23. Draw a fa m ily 24. p ie c e o f p i e is n o t done. in t h e c i r c l e t o show how much work W r it e EMPTY in t h a t space. and t h a t needs d o in g in t h e Every f a m i l y has work t h a t needs d o in g b u t does n o t g e t done. work does n o t g e t done a t home In y o u r f a m il y ? What k in d of HERE IS A LIST OF JOBS c h i l d r e n o f t e n do a t home f o r t h e f a m i l y . mark b e s id e each j o b t h a t you do. 25. _________ Do some shopping o r go t o t h e s t o r e f o r Make a check e x tra s . 26. _________W r it e a l e t t e r . 27. ________ Take c a re o f o t h e r c h i l d r e n In the f a m i l y . 28. _________ Earn money. 29. _________ Take c are o f p e ts o r an im a ls (feed them, g e t f r e s h w a t e r , t a k e them o u t s i d e ) . 30. _________ Do y a rd work (mow th e g r a s s , weed t h e ga rd e n , rake le a v e s ) . 31 . _________Take o u t garbage o r tra sh . 32. _________ Sweep s id e w a lk s , porches o r p a t i o , flo o rs . 33. _________ Shovel snow. 34. _________ Dust f u r n i t u r e . 35. _________ Vacuum r u g s , c a r p e t s , f l o o r s . 36. _________Clean s i n k s , b a thtubs, t o i l e t s . 37. ________ Wash f l o o r s . 38. _________P ut away g r o c e r i e s . 39. ________ Prepare and cook meals. 40. _________ Set and c l e a r t h e t a b l e , wash t h e t a b l e . 41. _________ Wash and d r y d i s h e s . 42. _________Water th e p l a n t s . 43. _________C o l l e c t d i r t y c lo t h e s f o r t h e la u n d ry . 44. _________ Wash and d r y t h e c l o t h e s . 45. _________ P ut away t h e c le a n c l o t h e s . 46. _________ Put away t o y s , c le a n my room. 47. _________ Take sheets o f f t h e bed o r p u t c le a n she ets on t h e bed. 48. What o t h e r j o b s do you do? __________________________________________________ APPENDIX D 119 Table D -1.— Descriptive Statistics for Perceived Contributions to Family Work Descriptive Statistics Median Standard Minimum Maximum Range Deviation Variable Mean No. of Cases 6 year olds Beys Girls 13.53 17.04 15.4 14.9 5.54 9.13 7.3 3.5 17.9 36 10.6 32.5 3 10 7 year olds Boys Girls 21.55 21.55 15.05 19.65 4.79 12.23 8.4 8.8 22.6 38.1 14.2 29.3 8 4 8 year olds Boys Girls 12.28 12.07 11.35 12.5 4.04 1.99 7 9.9 22.7 13.8 15.7 3.9 12 3 9 year olds Boys Girls 11.93 15.21 9.8 14.4 4.75 4.91 5.9 8.9 20 22.1 14.1 13.2 9 7 10 year olds Boys 11.58 Girls 11.63 10.05 11.95 3.66 4.16 7.2 4.6 18.2 16.6 11 12 11 year olds Boys 15.03 Girls 15.92 14.45 15.6 4.60 5.43 5.7 10.5 20.8 27.8 15.1 17.3 12 9 12 year olds Boys 17.06 Girls 14.93 15.9 15.65 6.82 5.0 6.5 8.2 25.1 20.2 18.6 12 9 4 8 6 Missing Cases 120 Table D -2.— Descriptive Statistics for Number of Tasks Performed Descriptive Statistics Median Standard Minimum Maximum Range Deviation Variable Mean 6 year' olds Boys Girls 8.25 11.9 8 11.5 6.24 2.08 7 9 7 year olds Boys Girls 13 15.75 11 16.5 5.13 2.87 6 12 21 18 3 10 18 14 15 4 13 3 8 year olds Boys 12.85 Girls 12 9 year olds Boys Girls 13 12 4.34 2 14 15 No. of Cases 7 6 4 10 15 6 8 4 13.56 13.71 13 13.25 3.09 2.36 9 10 18 16 9 6 9 7 11.88 14.83 10.5 15 3.6 4.45 8 7 19 20 11 13 8 6 11.92 14.11 11 13 4.03 4.73 8 6 23 22 15 16 13 9 3.52 5.80 8 11 20 24 12 13 9 4 10 year olds Boys Girls 11 year olds Boys Girls 12 year olds Boys Girls 12.11 16.5 11.75 14 BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Adams, R. L. and Phillips, B. N. Motivational and achievement differences among children of various ordinal birth positions. Child Development, 1972,41(1), 155-164. Andrews, F. M. and Withey, 5. B. Developing m easures of perceived life quality: R esults from several national surveys. Social Indicators Research. 1 9 7 4 ,1 , 1-26. Andrews, F. M. and Withey, S. B. Social indicators of w ell-being. New York: Plenum Press, 1976. Andrews, M. P., Bubolz, M. M. and Paolucci, B. An ecological approach to study the family. Marriage and Family Review, 1 9 8 0 ,1 ,2 9 - 4 9 . Aries, P. Centuries of childhood: A social history of family life . New York: Vintage Books, 1962. Aronowitz, S. Fa1s<=> pro m ises. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1973. Blood, R. 0. and Wolfe, D. M. Press, 1960. Husbands and w ives. Glencoe, 111.: Free Book, S. A. S t a t i s t i c s . McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1977. Bossard, J. H. S. and Boll, E. S. The large Family System. University of Philadelphia Press, 1956. Philadelphia: Boulding, E. Children's rights and the wheel of life. New Brunswick, New Jersey: Transaction Books, 1979. Bourque, L. B. Applied research in the 1970s: Can anything be learned about children? American Academy of Political and Social Science, The Annals. 1 9 8 2 ,4 6 1 , 32-42. Bronfenbrenner, U. The ecology of human development. Harvard University Press, T979. Campbell, A. The s ense, of. w ell-being in America:. tren d s. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1981. Cambridge: Re.ceaLp.atterQS_and Campbell, A., Converse, P. E. and Rogers, W. L. The quality of American life . New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1976. Clausen, J. A. Family stru ctu re, socialization, and personality. In L. W. Hoffman and M. L. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 2), New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1966. 121 122 Cogle, F. L. and Tasker, 6. E. Children and housework. Family Relations, 1982,11(3), 395-409. Cogle, F. L., Tasker, G. E. and Morton, D. G. Adolescent tim e use in household work. Adolescence. 1982, J_Z(66), 452-455. Davey, A. J. Relationship of fam ily interaction to family environment. Unpublished doctoral d issertatio n , Michigan S ta te University, 1971. Deacon, R. E. and Firebaugh, F. M. Family resource management principles and applications. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, Inc., 1981. Elder, G. H. Family structu re: the e ffe c ts of size of family, sex composition and ordinal position on academic motivation. In Adolescent achievement and m obility asp iratio n s. Chapel Hill, N. C.: In stitu te for Research in Social Science, 1962. Engel, M., Marsden, G. and Woodaman, S. Children who work and the concept of work sty le, Psychiatry. 1 9 6 7 ,IQ(4), 392-404. Engel, M., Marsden, G. and Woodaman, S. Orientation to work in children, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1968, lfi( 1), 137-143. Epstein, J. L. and McPartland, J. M. The concept and measurement of the quality of school life. American Educational Research Journal. 197 6,1 1(1), 15-30. Erikson, E. H. Childhood and so c ie ty . New York: W. W. Norton and Co., 1963. Ezell, M. P. Family members1 perceptions of household production in relationship to quality of life . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan S ta te University, 1982. Foote, N. N. and C ottrell, L. S. Identity. and interpersonal competence: A new direction in fam ily research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1955. Gauger, W. H. The potential contribution to the GNP of valuing household work. Paper presented at the 6 4 th Annual Meeting of the American Home Economics Association. A tlantic City, June 26, 1973. 123 Gesell, A., Ilg, F. L. and Ames, L. B. Youth, the y ears from ten to six te e n . New York: Harper and Brothers Publishers, 1956. Gesell, A., Ilg, F. L. and Ames, L. B. The child from five to ten (Revised ed.). New York: Harper and Row, Publishers, 1977. Ginzberg, E. The human economy. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1976. Goldstein, B. and Oldham, J. Children and work. New Brunswick, New Jersey: Transaction Books, 1979. Hansen, 5. L. and Darling, C. A. A ttitud es of adolescents toward division of labor in the home. Adolescence, 1985, XX(77), 61-72. Harris, D. B., Clark, K. E., Rose, A. M. and Valasek, F. The relationship of children's home duties to an a ttitu d e of responsibility. Child Dg-Y.eJ.Qpm.eat, 1 9 5 4 ,25( 1), 29-33. Havighurst, R. J. Youth in exploration and man emergent. In Henry Borow (Ed.), Man in a world at work. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1964. Hill, R. W. Responsibility of fifth graders related to a u tho ritarian/ auth oritativ e p a re n ts. Unpublished doctoral d isse rtatio n , Michigan S ta te University, 1981. Hoppen, K. K. Teenage contribution to the work of the home in 28 fam ilies. Baldwin, New York. Unpublished m a ste r's thesis, Cornell University, 1966. Johannis, T. B. P articipation by fath ers, m others and teenage sons and daughters in se lec te d household tasks. The Coordinator, 1958, Yi(4), 61-62. Keniston, K. and The Carnegie Council on Children. All our children. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1977. Leichter, H. J. (Ed.). The family as educator (2nd ed.). Teachers College Press, 1977. Lu, Y. Marital roles and m arital adjustm ent. New York: Sociology and Social 124 Research, 1952,36(6), 364-368. Lynch, M. C. P articipation in household ta s k s by children from six to seven teen years of age. Unpublished m aste r's th esis, Cornell University, 1975a. Lynch, M. Sex role stereotypes: Household work of children. Ecology Forum. 1975b, 5(3), 22-26. Human Marjoribanks, K. and Walberg, H. J. Ordinal position, family environment, and mental abilities. Journal of Social Psychology. 1975, 2 S (firs t half), 77-84. McKeever, W. A. The new child labor movement. EC.QDQmi£S, 1913, $L(2), 137-139. Journal of Home Mintum, L. and Lambert W. W. Mothers of six cultures: Antecedents of child rearing. New York: Wiley, 1964. Moed, L. Liberating children by a new approach to household economics. Social Casework. 1974, 55(6), 384-387. Naffzinger, C. C. and Naffzinger, K. Development of sex role stereotypes. The Family Coordinator, 1974,22(3), 251 -258. Neisser, E. G. Your child's sense of responsibility. Pamphlet. 1957, No. 254, 1-28. Public A ffairs Nie, N. H., Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. G., Steinbrenner, K. and Bent, D. H. S ta tis tic a l package for the social sciences (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company, 1975. Oakley, A. Sex, gender and so c iety . London: Maurice Temple Smith, 1972. O'Neill, B. M. Children sharing household work. 1979, M l ) , 18-21. Human Ecology Forum. Osborne, E. How to teach your children about work. Pamphlet. 1955, No. 216, 1-28. Public A ffairs Piaget, J. The child's conception of the world. New York: Harcourt and 125 Brace, 1959. Phillips, S. S. Participation in household ta s k s by children from four to tw elve year of age. Unpublished m a s te r's th esis, Cornell University, 1957. Rapoport, R. and Rapoport, R. N. Fathers, m others and society. New York: Basic Books Inc., 1977. Rettig, K. Interpersonal resource exchanges as predictors of Quality of m arriage and fam ily life. Unpublished doctoral d isse rtatio n , Michigan S ta te University, 1980. Roberts, J. and Baird, J. T. Parent ratings of behavioral p a tte rn s of children Vital and Health S ta tis tic s . Data from the National Health Survey. Series 11. No. 108. Washington, D. C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Roy, P. Maternal employment and adolescent roles: Rural-urban differentials. Marriage and Family Living. 1961,22(4), 340-349. Runyan, W. M. Perceived determ inants of highs and low s in life satisfaction . Developmental Psychology. 1979,15(3), 331 -333. Sander, J. H. A child's role in the family. Parents, 198 5,6 0(5), 67-70. Sanik, M. M. and Stafford, K. Adolescents' contribution to household production: Male and fem ale differences. Adolescence, 1985, M 7 7 ) , 207-215. Schooler, C. Birth order effects: Not here, not now! Bul.lfi.tjH, 19 72,20(3 ), 161-175. Psychological Slaugh, K. Family interaction and human resource development in the housework context. Unpublished doctoral d isse rtatio n , Michigan S ta te University, 1982. Smart, M. S. and Sm art, R. C. School-age children: Development and relationships. New York: Macmillan Publishing Co., Inc., 1973. Social Science Research Council. Basic background ite m s for U. S. 126 household surveys. 1975. New York: Social Science Research Council, Spock, B. Problems of p a re n ts. New York: Faw cett World Library, 1962. Steidl, R. E. and Bratton, E. C. Work in the home. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1968. Stephens, W. N. The family in cross-cu ltu ral perspectives. Holt, 1963. New York: Stephens, W. N. Our children should be working. Springfield, 111.: Charles C. Thomas Publishers, 1979. Straus, M. A. Work roles and financial responsibility in the socialization of farm, fringe, and town boys. Rural Sociology. 1962, 2 1 0 ) , 257-274. Super, D., Crites, J., Hummel, R., Moser, H., O verstreet, P. and Warnath, C. Vocational development:__ A framework for research . New York: Columbia University Press, 1957. Tanner, J. M. Education and physical growth (2nd ed.). International Universities Press, Inc., 1978. New York: Temple University In stitu te for Survey Research. National survey of children. Summary of preliminary results. New York: Foundation for Child Development, 1976. Tengel, P. M. Teenage.pmduction—income, earnings, and work experience in South Euclid-Lyndhurst. Ohio. Unpublished m aste r's th esis, Cornell University, 1964. Thrall, C. A. Who does what: Role stereotypy, children's work, and continuity betw een generations in the household division of labor. Human R elations, 1978,31(3), 249-265. Tilgher, A. Work: What, it ,has,meant t o. men t hr ough t he .ages. T ranslated by D. C. Fisher. New York: Arno Press, 1977. Turnure, C. Cognitive development and role-taking ability in boys and 127 g irls from 7 to 202-209. 12, Developmental Psychology. 1975, 11(2), U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. Your child from 6 to 12. Children's Bureau Publication Number 324. Washington, D. C.: Government Printing, 1949. U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. Your child from 6 to 12. Washington, D. C.: Government Printing Office, 1966. Vliet, S. L. Feelings regarding having children again and perceived sa tis fa c tio n w ith life. Unpublished m aste r's th esis, Michigan S ta te University, 1979. Walker, K. E. Household work time: Its implication for family decisions. Journal of Home Economics. 1973, £5( 10), 7-11. Walker, K. E. How much help for working m others? The children's role. Human Ecology Forum. 1970a, 1(2), 13-15. Walker, K. E. Q uantitative m easurement of productive household activ itie s. Paper prepared for Round Table on "Economy and Sociology of the Family: The Non-market Production Functions in American and French Families," sponsored by National Center of S cientific Research (France) and National Science Foundation (United States). Oise, France, January 1977. Walker, K. E. Time used by husbands for household work. Family Fconomics Review, United S ta te s Department of Agriculture, 1970b, ARS 6 2-5, 8-10. Walker, K. E. and Gauger, W. H. The dollar value of household work. Information Bulletin 6 0 . New York S ta te College of Human Ecology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, 1973. Walker, K. E. and Woods, M. E. Time use: A m easure of household production of fam ily goods and se rv ic e s. Washington, D. C.: The Family of American Home Economics Association, 1976. Warren, J. R. Birth order and social behavior. 196 6,6 5(1), 38-49. Psychological Bulletin. 128 White, L. K. and Brinkerhoff, D. B. Children's work In the family: Its significance and meaning. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 1981, 42(4), 7 8 9 -7 9 8 Whiting, B. and Whiting, J. Children of six c u ltu res. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1975. Williams, F. Reasoning w ith s t a t i s t i c s (2nd ed.). Rinehart and Winston, 1979. New York: Holt, Wolf, F. M. and Chandler, T. A. A cross-lagged panel analysis of quality of school life and achievement responsibility. Journal of Educational Research, 1 9 8 1 ,2 4 (5 ), 363-368. Zill, N. Divorce, m arital happiness and the mental health of children: Findings from the foundation for child development national survey of children. Paper prepared for National In stitu te of Mental Health workshop on Divorce and Children. Bethesda, Md., February 7-8, 1978. Zill, N. and Peterson, J. L. Learning to do things w ithout help. In L. M. Laosa and I. E. Sigel (Eds.), Eamllies as learning env ironments l o r children. New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation, 1982.