EFFECT OF COLCR AND FLAVOR MODIFICATION ON THE PALATABILITY AND GEL STRUCTURE OF STANDARD BAKED CUSTARD Thesis far Hm Degree of M. S. MICKEGRN STATE UNIVERSITY Mary Eve‘iyn 1964 THESIS IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIII III IIII III III III IIIIIIIIIIIIIIL 3 1293 00073 9643 LIBRARY Michigan {State Univermty ”a... .— s I PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your mood. To AVOID FINES return on or before data due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MSU Is An Affirmative ActioNEqual Opportunlty lmfltutlon ‘I‘ III Iva - ABSTRACT EFFECT OF COLOR AND FLAVOR MODIFICATIONS ON THE PALATABILITY AND GEL STRUCTURE OF STANDARD BAKED CUSTARD By Mary Evelyn Garlick The purpose of this investigation was to study the ef- fect of the addition of the nine possible combinations of three different peach colors and three levels of peach fla- vor on the palatability and gel structure of standard baked custard. The basic formula contained a constant proportion of ingredients using reconstituted dried whole milk, fresh eggs and sucrose. Each of the nine treatment variables contained one of three peach colors - designated light, medium, and dark color - and one of three levels of peach flavor - desig- nated low, medium, and high flavor level. A control custard having neither added color nor added flavor was used as a reference for the objective tests. Six replications of each of the nine treatment variables were prepared and evaluated at room temperature by subjective and objective methods. A taste panel (4 women and 3 men) evaluated nine char- acteristics on a 7-point rating scale: crust color, crust tenderness, crust flavor, inside color, aroma, inside flavor, consistency, texture, and syneresis. Objective measurements included pH of the custard before and after baking, gel strength as indicated by the penetrometer (crust on, crust Mary Evelyn Garlick off) and by per cent sag, and syneresis as indicated by per cent drainage. Analysis of variance on the subjective scores for the nine treatment variables indicated no significant differences in six characteristics: crust tenderness, aroma, inside fla- vor, consistency, texture, and syneresis. The custards were significantly different in crust color (1% level of probabil- ity), inside color (1% level of probability), and crust fla- vor (5% level of.probability). For crust color and inside color the medium color custards, as a group, scored highest. The custard with medium color and high flavor level scored highest for crust color and second highest for inside color. For crust flavor, the custard with medium color and high fla- Ivor level scored the highest followed by two custards con- taining the low flavor level and dark and medium colors re- spectively. There was an indication of the "halo effect" of ncolor impression on the judges' scores for flavor. The judges' scores and comments indicated the color and the fla- vor were concentrated in the crust. In considering all three significantly different subjective characteristics, it ap- peared that the custard containing the medium color and high flavor level was scored highest by this taste panel. No significant differences existed in any of the objec- tive measurements. High standard deviations in the measure- ments for both gel strength and per cent drainage cast doubt on the reliability and suitability of these two objective measurements for baked custards. The addition of color and Mary Evelyn Garlick flavor did not alter the pH of either the mix or the baked custards as compared to the control custard. No significant correlations were found between objec- tive and subjective measurements for gel strength or for syneresis. The highly significant positive correlations be- tween crust color and crust flavor and between inside color and inside flavor indicate further the "halo effect" of color impression on flavor judgment. Highly significant correla- tions existed between crust color and inside color, between crust flavor and inside flavor, and between texture and syn- eresis.‘ Positive correlations (5% level of probability) existed between inside flavor and aroma and between con- sistency and texture. EFFECT OF COLOR AND FLAVOR MODIFICATION ON THE PALATABILITY AND GEL STRUCTURE OF STANDARD BAKED CUSTARD BY Mary Evelyn Garlick A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Foods and Nutrition 1964 <32fl045 7/9 #04) ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The writer wishes to express her gratitude and appreci- ation to Dr. Theodore F. Irmiter for his generous guidance and encouragement throughout this study. Grateful acknowledgment is also due to Dr. L. E. Dawson, Professor of Food Science, Michigan State University, and to Dr. T. I. Hedrick, Professor of Food Science, Michigan State University, and their staffs. Without their invaluable assist- ance, a controlled study would not have been possible. The assistance of Donald Kiel, Michigan State UniVersity Computer Center, and Programmer, Agricultural Experiment Station, in the statistical analysis of the data is also greatly appreci- ated. The writer expresses her sincere appreciation to Givaudan Flavors Inc.‘, New York, and Food Materials Corpora- tion‘, Chicago, Illinois, for supplying flavor samples and information used in this study. I Grateful appreciation is expressed to Dr. Grace Miller, Miss Mary Morr, Mrs. Norma Gilmore, Mrs. Bette Smith, Spiros Constantidines, H._Crane Day, and Albert Hefner, who served on the taste panel. The writer wishes to acknowledge her special indebted- ness to General Foods Corporation, White Plains, New fork, for granting her a fellowship to study for this degree and for additional funds which provided support for this research. 1WMention of manufacturers or their products in this thesis does not constitute endorsement by Michigan State University. To my Mother and Father TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS.......,...........................,... LIST OF TABLES........................................ LIST OF FIGURES..................................,.... LIST OF EXHIBITS...................................... INTRODUCTION.......................................,,. REVIEW OF LITERATURE.................................. Color in Foods...,............................... Importance of color...,..................... Classes of colors........................... Addition of colors to foods................. U. S. government regulations.,......... Certified food colors.....,.,.......... FD&C Red No. 4.................... FD&C Yellow No. 5......,.......... Color blends...................... Evaluation and measurement of color......... Subjective evaluation.........,........ Objective measurement................,. Flavor in Foods.................................. Psychological aspects....,.................. Factors influencing flavor..,,...,,......... Addition of flavors to food products..,..... Fruit flavors.........a..................... iv Page ii viii >4 \OODCD-xlflmmmmk-D'J-‘HH F‘ r4 14 h‘ P4 re Ox er \» P‘ +4 (D TABLE or CONTENTS (CONT.) Page Classifications........................ 16 True Fruit Flavoring.............. 17 True Fruit with Other Natural Flavoreoeevooeoeo900000909 l7 Imitation Fruit Flavoring......... l7 Imitation Fruit Flavoring With True Fruitooqoooooqoooooooooo l7 PeaCh flavorOO’O..'.....,..0,0.00...... 17 Custards as a Medium for Experimental work with Color and Flavor....................... 19 Composition................................. 19 Heat coagulation of proteins..........,..... 19 Effect of acid, alkali, salts, and organic solvents,.....,.oooq........so.. 20 pH of custard mix and cooked custard....,... 2l Gelation temperature and rate of cooking.... 21 Method of baking............,............... _ 22 Use of dried whole milk in baked custards... 24 Evaluation of Foods.............................. 24 Subjective methods................,......... 25 Types of tests and judgments........... 25 Considerations for taste panels........ 26 Objective methods........................... 27 EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE...............,................ 29 Preliminary Investigations....................,.. 29 V TABLE OF CONTENTS (CONT.) Page Design of Experiment........................,.... 30 Color-flavor combinations..............,.... 30 Basic Custard Formula............................ 32 Ingredient Procurement...................,....... 32 Basic formula ingredients................... 32 Colors........................,.....,....... 33 Flavors..................................... 33 Preparation of Color Solutions................... 34 Preparation of Custard Mix....................... 34 Baking Procedure........,........................ 35 Subjective Tests................................. 37 Objective Tests......................,.,......... 38 Penetration................................. 39 Per cent sag................................ 39 Syneresis................................... 41 pH....................,..................... 41 Statistical Methods.....,........................ 43 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION................................ 44 Subjective Tests................................. 45 Crust color................................. 45 Crust tsnderness,..,......,................. 48 Crust flavor................................ 48 Inside color..............,................. 53 vi TABLE OF CONTENTS (CONT.) Page Aroma,....................,...,....,........ 56 Inside flavor....................,..,....... 58 Consistency...........,..................... 6O Texture.,................................... 6O Syneresis.....,....,........................ 63 Objective Tests..................,............... 63 Penetration.........,....................... 63 Per cent sag...........,,................... 68 Syneresis................................... 70 pH readings.........................,,...... 72 Correlation between Selected Measurements......,. 72 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS.................,............. 77 LITERATURE CITED..,................................... 82 APPENDlx......................................,,...... 87 vii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for CPUBL COloroooeeooeoooooo'yooeooeocoeooooooo 46 2. Analysis of Variance for Crust Color............ 47 3. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Crust Tenderness.....,...................... SO 4. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for crust FlavorQOQQOOOOQQOOQOOOOIOOCOQOOQ'0.000 51 5. Analysis of Variance for Crust Flavor........... 52 6. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Inglde Caloroooocoooeoooooyoo99099900190900. 54 7. Analysis of Variance for Inside Color..,........ 55 8. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for aromaoooeooooooooeooooeeoooo0000009000000... 57 9. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Inside FlavorOOOOOOOOOQOOOOOOOQQOOOOOO0.0909 59 10. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for conalstencyOOOO’QOOOOO’OOOQO'OOOOOOOO090090O 61 11. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Texturepoooesooco9oecoooooeooooooooeeeoooooo 62 12. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for SynereSIS.........,...o.oo,o.p.o9.g......... 64 13. Mean Penetrometer Values and Standard Deviations on Baked Custards with the CPUBL onooeeoeoeeooooeoeqoeoooeoccooo'QQCQOoqooo 65 14. Mean Penetrometer Values and Standard Deviations on Baked Custards with the crust OffOOOOOOQOOOOOOOOOQOQOOOQOQQOOQOOOOQ0,... 66 15. Mean Penetrometer Difference Values and Standard Deviations......................... 67 16. Per Cent Sag and Standard Deviations of Baked Custards............................... 69 viii LIST OF TABLES (cour.) Table Page 17. Per Cent Drainage and Standard Deviations of Baked Custards.,,..,.,.,.......... 71 18, Range of pH Readings over Six Replications...... 73 19. Correlation Coefficients of Selected Measurements........................... 74 20. Colors and Flavors Used in Baked Custards for Objective Tests..,..........,...... 88 21. Colors and Flavors Used in Baked Custards for Subjective Tssts................... 89 22. Description of Flavors Used..,.............,,... 90 23. Composition of Color Solutions........,.,....... 91 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. 2. Water bath pan equipped with stopcock and perforated stainless steel rack,- showing placement of thermocouples.............. Measuring penetration after release of 35-gram cone attachment...................... Apparatus for height measurement of free-standing inverted baked custard............ Equipment used for drainage weight determinations from freenstanding inverted baked custard..........o.........ouoo. Three different peach colors in baked custaardla(cru8t on)009000000000001000000030 Three different peach colors in inverted baked custards..................,...... Page 36 42 49 49 LIST OF EXHIBITS Exhibit ‘ Page 1. Procedure for Preparation of Colored and Flavored Custard Mixes............. 92 2. General Instructions for Peach Custard Taste Panel Members.................... 94 3. Score Sheet for Peach Custard.................. 96 4. Sample Calculation of Studentized Multiple Range Test...........,................ 97 xi INTRODUCTION For many decades home economists have been concerned with the importance of color and flavor of foods in the skillful planning of normal and modified diets. These food . attributes have been recognized as important determinants of consumer acceptance and satisfaction. More recently, the research of psychologists and physiologists has reflected increased interest in and recognition of the effect food colors and flavors exert on the human appetite (2). Food technologists continue to focus their attention on the basic factors involved in the preservation of color and flavor in foods in the development of new processing techniques, new products, and new forms of established products. Some dietitians have recognized the psychological im- pact of color as'a key to successful meal planning and ser- vice (60). The appetite inhibiting effect of monotony in the diet is an ever present concern of all nutritionists. Although careful selection of food and food combinations is of prime importance, numerous workers believe that variation of color and flavor can be of considerable assistance in re- lieving monotony in all types of diets (2, 58). The problem of monotony is, perhaps, more acute in the formulation of restricted diets than in the planning of nor— mal ones, especially in the area of permissible dessert items. Baked custard, basically a soft, bland, high protein food consisting of milk, eggs and sugar, is used extensively l for many types of modified diets. For the most part, when baked custards are served they are yellow in color and vanil- la flavored. A search of the literature failed to reveal re- ports of investigations concerned with the use: of fruit colors and flavors in the preparation of baked custards. If selected fruit colors and flavors could be added successfully to a basic custard formula, it is conceivable that variations of this standard dessert item would be useful to both the homemaker and the professional dietitian. Preliminary experimental work indicated it is possible to produce a peach colored and peach flavored baked custard without noticeably altering the gel structure of the product. On the basis of these limited observations a controlled in- vestigation was planned to study the effect of peach color and flavor modification on the palatability and gel structure of standard baked custard: The objectives of this study were: 1. To identify the types and proportions of food colors which can be combined with the natural carotene pig- ments present in a standard baked custard mix to produce a desirable peach color. ‘2. To determine the type and levels of peach flavors which, when added to the standard baked custard formula, produce custards of acceptable peach fla- vor compatible with the peach colors selected for study. 3. To study the effect of the addition of three different peach colors and three levels of peach flavor on the palatability and gel structure of standard baked custard. REVIEW OF LITERATURE Color in Foods m .ce Co r Color is constantly a part of food, a visual element to which human eyes, minds, emotions and palates are very sensi- tive. Through the ages, man has come to build strong and in- tuitive associations between what he sees and what he eats. People demand the right shade in foodstuffs and will accept or reject a product on its appearance - quite apart from nu- tritional considerations (2). i . _ Color, the first quality attribute a consumer perceives, plays a major part in his willingness to accept a food. It is often regarded as an index of the general quality of the product, and may influence the consumer's Judgment of flavor (28, 42, 57). > What is perhaps basic to color and appetite are certain direct associations and known responses to the stimulation of color. According to Birren (2), bright and "warm" colors (red, orange, and yellow) tend to stimulate the autonomic nervous system of man, including digestion, whereas soft and "cool" colors tend to retard it. Of these warm colors, red and orange have a more stimulating effect than yellow. People learn to associate colors with various kinds of experiences with food such as their taste, odor, or the total complex of stimuli associated with eating, and ultimately 4 5 with the resulting degree of satisfaction. However, to interpret preference for a food on the basis of color alone without regard to its role as a member of a group of stimuli could result in invalid conclusions (57). C ass a of lore The two general classifications of colors in food are natural and synthetic. The natural color classification in- cludes the pigments and appearance factors actually found in food, such as the homes, carotenoids, chlorophylls, antho- cyanins, and flavonoids. Carotene is present in egg and milk and lutein is the yellow coloring matter of egg yolk (42). Natural colors also include those derived from vege- table, animal, and mineral sources. Natural vegetable colors most commonly added to foods are caramel, from burnt sugar, and annatto, an extract of annatto seed (60). Synthetic colors are compounds of known structure pro~ duced by chemical synthesis and conforming to standards es- tablished by the U.’§. Food and Drug Administration. Syn- thetic colors are most widely used in the food industries because of the variety of available shades, their brilliance and uniformity, solubility and high tinctorial strength (60). Addition 9; gglgrs to Foods Foods fall naturally into two groups when examined on the basis of color: 1. Foods which have an acceptable natural color, or in which an acceptable color is deve10ped through cooking. 2. Rxds which usually require added color in processing (margarine, cheese, desserts such as ice cream and sherbets, gelatin desserts, puddings, candy, cakes, cookies and pastries as well as many beverages) (60). According to Jablonski (24), colors have long been used to improve the appearance of products. The number of color- ing materials has increased in the past fifty years from a few simple ones to a vast range of tints and shades, mostly of synthetic'production, and their use» has become an inte- gral part of our civilization. U, §, government regulations Added colors in foods are regulated by the Color Addi- tives Amendment of 1960 to the Federal Food, Drug,end Cos- metic Act and are deemed "color additives" in the Code of Federal Regulations(lo). In order to protect the health of the public, the U. 5. Government permits food manufacturers to use only batches of coal-tar dyes which have been tested by the U. 8. Food and Drug Administration and certified by this agency to be harmless and suitable for use (24). Cert fied o co ors Jablonski (24) reports when the Federal Food and Drug Act was first enacted (1907) the number of permitted coal- tar colors certifiable for food purposes was limited to seven. By 1950 the number had increased to nineteen dyes - four oil-soluble dyes and fifteen water-soluble dyes. Since 1950 the Food and Drug Administration has removed the four oil-soluble colors and four of the waterwsoluble colors from the list of permitted colors because these delisted colors did not meet the requirements of the Color Additives Amend- ment of 1960 (10). At the present time there are eleven certified food colors, all of which are water-soluble (10). Most of the primary certified food colors have two names, the official or FDacC1 designations and the common trade name. FD&C gag “9;-4,'.2932°au §§ FD&C Red No. 4, commonly called Ponceau 8X, is a red powder easily soluble in water, giving an orange-red solu- tion. In acid solution (pH 2.9) the color becomes very slightly redder. In alkali solution (pH 8.4) the color is not changed (24). Ponceau Ex is the disodium salt of 2~(5- sulfo-2,4-xy1yl-azo)ul-naphthol-4-sulfonic acid (10). Its structural formula (24) is: - CH: H3C .NzN Na038 N8033 F a Yel w N _ - a tra ne FDdC Xellow No. 5, commonly call Tartrazine, is an orange-yellow powder, easily soluble in water, giving a goldeneyellow solution, There is no visible color change in either weak acid or weak alkaline solution (24). Tartrasine I Ffiaa signifies'Foods, Drugs and Cosmetics and means that any certified color with this designation may be used in foods, in drugs and in cosmetics, 8 is the trisodium salt of 5-carboxy-S-hydroxywl-pesulfophenyl- 4-p-sulfophenyl-azo-pyrazole (10). Its structural formula N8035.N=N-fiwfi“COONa - N HO Q\\/ (24) is: ‘03Na .01 r en Because the list of permitted colors represents a se- lection of shades far too limited to cover food industry needs, certified blends containing two or more permitted colors to produce numerous intermediate shades are avail- able (60). Evalugttgg gag fleastggment 9; 90122 According to Brice (4), the measurement and specifica- tion of color is "color science" combining segments of phys- ics, chemistry, physiology, and psychology for its complete understanding. With modern photoelectric instruments, and psychophysical data adopted by international agreement, the measurement and specification of color can be quite straight- forward. Although color evaluations may be made either by subjec- tive or objective methods, there is considerable controversy as to the relative merits of each method“in evaluating color in foods. Color evaluation in food products has become increasingly important as food technologists place more em- phasis on product quality, yet color is one of the hardest quality factors to evaluate. To satisfy the desire for obs jectivity in recording colors and to overcome the short- comings of the human eye in color perception, various instru- ments and devices have been constructed. Some instruments answered a need; others exchanged one difficulty for another. As color recording became mechanized, the interpretation of the data became more difficult (18, 56). ’ b so, v ev uation wilson et a1. (62) state it is desirable to establish objective criteria which could be related to organoleptic evaluations. However, no specific criteria were given. De- velopment of such quality indices would greatly alleviate the difficulties encountered in maintaining a trained panel of judges and eliminate certain variations due to fatigue in routine subjective testing of a large number of samples. The subjective methods of color measurement and standardization based on visual comparisons are subject to shortcomings of human observers, such as the variability in the reactions of different observers, and in those of one observer at differ- ent times and under different viewing conditions, and the un- reliability of color memory. The difficulties associated with sensory testing have encouraged the development of ob- Jective methods for the measurement of color which eliminate the human retina in favor of the photoelectric cell (28). 10 b 0 ve eas rem nt An objective evaluation of color may be made in a num- ber of different ways, including the use of charts, discs, and instruments. Maerz and Paul's Dictionary of Color and the Munsell Book of Color provide charts for the visual com- parison of colors. The Maxwell spinning disc principle has been used for color measurement for over 100 years. Avail- able types of instruments for measuring color include: visual colorimeters, comparators, spectrophotometers, and tristimulus photoelectric colorimeters (52). Robinson et al. (56) report the Maxwell spinning disc to be less objective than the photoelectric instrument, and its use is more tedious and time consuming. The Hunter Color Difference Meter and the Gardner Color and Color-Difference Meter, which measure color directly by reflectance, have been used for color measurements in many research projects (5. 22, 34, 62, 63). Since the Hunter Color Difference Meter had been used successfully to measure— color differences in tomato juice, strawberries, and other fruit and vegetable products, Huggart and Wenzel (22) under- took to determine if this instrument would be satisfactory for measuring the color of citrus juices and concentrates. They concluded that although color differences can be objec- tively measured with the Hunter Color Difference Meter, neither this nor any other instrument available at that time was satisfactory for determining the actual visual color of 11 a citrus juice as seen by an observer. Longree (36) found the Hunter Color Difference Meter unsatisfactory for evaluation of colors in baked custards. The subjective method of evaluating color was ultimately used in her study. According to Kefford (28), no one instru- ment to date has been devised by which it is possible to measure all the characteristics of colored bodies discern- ible to the human eye. Flavor in Foods Ps c o ical A ects According to Meyer (48), flavor is the subtle and com~ plex sensation that is the source of much of the delight man finds in food. To both connoisseur and layman, flavor is of utmost importance in determining preferences.. One's appreci- ation of flavor and one's judgment are influenced by many psychological factors. Some of the psychological factors that cannot be controlled are the experience background of the individual and his personal problems and reactions (15, 48) . I Pettit (54) states the psychological aspects of flavor include attitudes, experiences, memory, expectations, sug- gestions, motivations, and other factors of learned behavior. The setting, the questions, the personality and attitude of the investigator may all recall past experiences and influ- ence the consumer's interpretation of his immediate flavor 12 experience. .Flavor memories are very keen and enduring and must be taken into account whenever food flavors are-being originaw ted or changed. If the first meeting an individual has with a flavor is in medicine, there is a possibility that person will always dislike that flavor. Sometimes "innocent" fla- vors or textures such as those of oatmeal, cereal, cornstarch pudding, or jelly recall with great distaste some episode of sickness (12). I A person's reactions toward flavors are exceedingly characteristic and natural w a conservative person tends to be conservative in the flavors he likes. However, a person may train himself to recognize new flavors and to like them (12). Sternberg in 1914 as reported by Crocker (14), states one naturally eats and swallows pleasantwtasting food rapid- ly, rather than lets them linger in the mouth. If one holds food long in the mouth, it is an indication the taste is not entirely a pleasant one. ApprOpriateness is of great importance in our enjoyment of flavors in foods. An onion flavor may be delectable in a stew or soup but objectionable in a custard. We become con- ditioned to expect certain sensations from certain foods and while a slight variation is titillating, a completely unex- pected taste is unacceptable (48). Caul (7) has analyzed the pattern of good flavor as the 13 following sensations: "(1) an early impact of appropriate flavor; (2) rapid development of an impression of highly blended and usually full-bodied flavor; (3) pleasant mouth sensations; (4) absence of isolated unpleasant notes; and (5) anticipation of the next mouthful." actors nfluenc n l vor Taste depends on a number of factors, the most important of which is chemical composition. Saltiness is a prOperty of electrolytes, the halides in particular. Sweetness is found in a number of organic compounds, however, alcoholic hydrox- yls are most effective in endowing a compound with sweetness. Bitterness is a property of some organic and inorganic com- pounds; some of the alkaloids such as quinine and brucine are exceedingly bitter. Sourness is a property of the hydrogen ion and its concentration is of primary importance in deter- mining whether the sensation of sour is detected (48). Taste is also influenced by temperature, texture, and the presence of other compounds. According to Cracker (l2), temperature has a less noticable influence on the sweet and bitter taste components than it does on salty and sour com- ponents. Texture preperly is part of flavor; an unaccustomed texture places the senses of taste and smell under a handi- cap. Texture partially controls the quantity of sapid matter that can reach the taste buds at a given time; as a substance becomes thicker, the flavor becomes weaker. This weakening 14 of taste is probably mechanical because the viscosity of the fluid interferes with the diffusion of the soluble substances to the sensory receptors (12, 13). Mackey and Valassi (41) measured the taste threshold for four primary tastes (sweet, salty, sour and bitter) in water as well as in tomato juice and in egg-milk custard, prepared as liquid, gel, and foam. They found the primary tastes were hardest to detect in the gel state, easiest to detect in the liquid state, and intermediate in the foam. The presence of other compounds has been shown to in- fluence flavor in different ways. According to Pangborn (53) the interactions among taste qualities in foods have been a subject of opinion and speculation with relatively little experimental support. The early literature on the subject of taste interrelationships in aqueous solutions of pure compounds is controversial since conflicting results were obtained from similar experiments. Results of studies on apricot, peach, and pear nectars carried out by Valdes et a1. (59) show that sucrose enhanced fruit flavor up to an optimum sweetness level, beyond which it masked flavor. This relationship between sweetness and fruit flavor was found to be greatly influenced by the acidi- ty of the product. Addition of Flavors to Food grgducts Flavoring extracts are widely used in the food industry and are well standardized in the United States. The standard 15 for a flavoring extract, as established by the Secretary of Agriculture in 1919 and accepted by the flavor industry, is as follows: "a solution in ethyl alcohol of preper strength of the sapid and odorous principles derived from an aromatic plant, or parts of the plant, with or without its coloring ' matter, conforming in name to the plant used in its prepara- tion". If artificial coloring or synthetic flavoring com- pounds are added, these must be declared on the label (23, 48). Flavoring agents are chemical substances which are able to impress themselves on the senses of taste, smell, and feeling by way of food and drink. These chemicals are of many types and origins; some occur naturally in foods; some develop within food while it is being cooked or otherwise processed; and others are added substances of natural or artificial origin. Nearly all of the flavoring chemicals that have been identified in foods have been duplicated syn- thetically and, in many instances, these synthetics have been used in flavors (l2, l4). Imitation food flavors are composed of synthetic organ- ic aromatic chemicals sometimes used alone or in combination with natural products. These chemicals can be used as food flavors for controlled accentuation of a particular note or notes, where a highly concentrated flavor is required and for economy (25). From birth, modern generations have been reared and 16 accustomed to accept taste trends, and flavors are a daily and accepted necessity in life. The desire for a variety of palatable and appetizing taste sensations that makes the con- sumption of food a pleasure, not a necessary daily task, is a basic consumer characteristic of our times (25, 46). Literature with regard to food flavors has been very meager during the last fifty years, according to Merory (46). During this same period of time numerous compounds have been synthesized which are useful in formulating synthetic or imi- tation flavors. Such compounds are known as flavormatics and there are over 200 such flavormatics available for use in imitation flavor compositions (25, 46). In 1963 the Fla- voring Extract Manufacturers' Association issued a new list of flavoring ingredients suitable for use in the preparation of imitation flavors (16). Fruit Flavors The skin and the peels of fruit carry most of the fla- vor. There is a correlation between sugar content, color, acidity, and flavor as fruit ripens. The flavor usually de- ve10ps to its fullest when the sugar content of the fruit is at its maximum and the color of the skin or peel acquires the richest shade of brilliance (46). Classifications There are several types of flavoring materials available for use in food products. Legal definitions of these flavor- ing materials are lacking and the issues of "Imitation" and 17 "Natural" flavors are being discussed at the present time by governmental agencies and the flavor industry (16). The flavor industry and unofficially the F.D.A. accept the fol- lowing definitions: :rue Fruit Flavoring - extracts or concentrates derived directly from a particular aromatic plant or fruit, of which a minimum of 25# of fruit are used to yield 1 gal. of juice (16). True- rui w th 0 he Nat ral av rs - fruit flavors which are fortified with other natural flavors. At least 51% of the flavor strength must be derived from the true fruit named and not more than 49% from other natural flavors (23) Imitation Fruit Flavoring - flavorings synthesized entirely from chemica 8. There are no standards set for these flavorings, except that their con- stituents are to conform with the Food Additives Amendment of 1958 (16). Imitation Fruit Flavoring with True Fruit - a com- bination of synt etic ngredients an true fruit extractives in which not less than 5% of the total flavor is derived from true fruit extrac- tives (16). Peach Flavor The peach was one of the first fruits to be studied in order to determine the chemicals responsible for the flavor of the fruit. Power and Chestnut (55) state peach oil or peach essences consist for the most part of purely empirical mixtures of esters and essential oils with other more spe- cific aromatic substances. The general character of these empirical mixtures indicates some of their components may not actually be found in the pulp of the fruit whose flavor they are supposed to represent. For example, benzaldehyde 18 is frequently regarded as one of the proper constituents of peach aroma, although this is contained in the kernels of the peach and not in the pulp of the fruit. Toward the end of the 19th century, the flavoring prop- erties of gamma-n-heptyl butylrolactone were discovered and the chemical was synthesized. This became known as Aldehyde C-l4 which is a misnomer, since it is neither an aldehyde nor does it contain 14 carbon atoms (27, 47). The formula for Aldehyde 0-14 is: ‘ 7315 so According to Merwin (47),cmce the existence of gamma undecalactone became known, it became relatively simple to prepare a flavor resembling peach. It is difficult for any flavorist toconceive of a peach flavor today which will be acceptable to the American public without Aldehyde 0-14. Besides gamma undecalactone, those ingredients which would be considered essential to peach flavor are benzaldehyde, some of the linalyl esters and several valeric acid esters (47). I Many foods can be flavored or reinforced with a peach flavor by using the preper level of gamma undecalactone and adjusting the color and acidity of the food product involved. Great care must be exercised because of the ease of over- flavoring and obtaining an objectionable flavor (47). l9 Custards as a Medium for Experimental Work with Color and Flavor Baked custard represents a type of cooked product in whicthhe thickening property of the.egg protein is used, since only 0.75% of the heat‘coagulable protein is provided by the milk (38). Color, flavor, and odor are also detected easily in custards as they are made of relatively few essen- tial ingredients (44). Composition Custard is made from milk, eggs, and sugar and some-' times salt and vanilla flavor. The same proportions of in- gredients can be used for both stirred and baked custard (20). The baked custard is firmer than the stirred custard and appears to be in one piece or clot. All custards are highly sensitive to slight modifications in the egg-sugar— milk mixture. Heat Coogulogion of Erotoins The formation.of a gel in custards depends on the coag- ulation of protein which then holds within its structure the solution from which it was precipitated (51). Although pro- tein may be coagulated by many means, heat has been one of the most important methods. Heat coagulation of the egg and milk proteins in custards may be influenced by acid, alkali, salts, and organic solvents, as well as other factors (3, 38, 51). 2O Effeot of Acid, Alkali, Salts ond Qrganio §olvonts Weiser (61) indicates that certain protein sols (albu- mins, globulins, and globins) coagulate at some definite temperature which is fairly constant for each protein. With the lowering of the pH by the addition of acid, the coagula- tion temperature is raised. Chick and Martin (8) point out denaturation, the first part of the heat coagulation process, is not hastened by acid solution, but the clotting or coagu- lation process is accelerated. The second part of the coag- ulation process, the clotting or coagulation, does not occur in alkaline solution. If, after heating, the alkali is neu- tralized with acid, the coagulation process occurs (9). Although the salt content of milk is low, it is very important in the behavior of the proteins in food prepara- tion (38). A good example is custard, which fails to thicken when heated unless sufficient salts are present. Lows (38) reports the concentration of the salt and the valence of the ion have an effect on the coagulation of custards. Chick and Martin (8) state an increase in the concentration of salts raises the coagulation temperature. Many anions such as iodide,bromide, and chloride are denaturing agents (48); however according to Fruton and Simmonds (19), not all anions are denaturing agents - caprylate and aromatic carboxylate ions actually protect egg albumin from heat denaturation. Denaturation may be caused by organic solvents such as ethanol, methanol, and acetone. These have been used for 21 the precipitation of proteins from aqueous solutions (19). There is‘a decrease in the protein stability of milk toward alcohol as a result of homogenization, according to Carr and Trout (6). ate d i x d C e One The pH of the eggs used in custard has an effect on the pH of the custard mix - fresh eggs are less alkaline than aged ones. Cooked custards are usually more alkaline than the custard mix (3, 35, 38). Bittner (3) reports the pH readings on the custard mix and baked custard made with fresh eggs and dried whole milk as 7.03 and 7.09 respectively. Gelation Tomoerature ond Rooe of Cookiog Many factors affect the coagulation of protein and the gelation temperature of custards. These factors include the proportion of ingredients, the pH of the mixture, and the rate of cooking. As a result, ru>one internal end tempera- ture can be recommended as the optimum temperature to which a custard should be baked. Lows (38) states with ordinary rates of cooking, and using unhomogenized milk, a serving consistency for custard is usually obtained between 82° and 84°C. Carr and Trout (6) state the rate of heat penetration is slower in custards made with homogenized milk than in those made with unhomogenized milk. The firmness of the cus- tards indicates the custards prepared with homogenized milk could be baked to a higher internal end temperature without 22 seriously affecting the stability of the gel. Miller et al. (49) observed the optimum gelation temperature of custards prepared from fresh shell eggs and homogenized milk to be 86° to 88°C.. Cook and Husseman (11) state the temperature required to reach gels of similar consistency is 1°C higher when dried whole milk is used instead of fresh whole milk. Lows (38) reports the rate of coagulation increases as the oven temperature is raised; in addition, a firmer custard is obtained as the temperature is increased until at a spe- cific internal temperature, dependent on the rate of cooking, optimum gelation occurs. Further heating to a higher inter- nal temperature causes porosity and finally syneresis. Experimentation shows the rate at which custard is cooked affects the gelation temperature - with a slow rate of cooking, gelation of custard occurs at a lower tempera- ture, while gelation takes place at a higher temperature with a fast rate of cooking. A slower rate of cooking is considered most desirable because optimum gelation is more easily perceptible (38). Meghod of Baking Two factors to consider in baking custards are the oven temperature and the temperature of the water bath. Griswold (20) states even when baked at relatively low oven tempera- tures, custards are improved by being placed in a pan of hot water for protection from oven heat. Bittner (3) and Carr 23 and Trout (6) used an oven temperature of 325°F in their research work with custards. McBride (45) and Mastic (44) regulated the oven at 350°F. Jordan et al. (26) report an oven temperature of 350°F is preferred to 325°F or 400°F because the baking period is unduly long at 325°F and so short at 400°F that the custard can easily be overcooked. In addition, custards baked at 400°F are reported less de- sirable in appearance than those baked at the other two temw peratures. To insure the relatively long heating which is necessary to make the food product bacteriologically safe, it is recommended that an oven temperature of 350°F or below be used for baking custards (37). _For many years the recommended initial temperature for the water bath for baked custards has been boiling or hot water. Various initial temperatures have been reported for the water baths used in research work on custards in the past few years: Bittner (3) used 35°C, Carr and Trout (6) used 30°C, and Mastic (44) used 250-2700. Nagler (50) re- ports the texture of the custards was more delicate and ten» der if the initial water for the baking pan was 35° to 40°C rather than 97° to 100°C. The results of a special problem undertaken by the writer at Michigan State University in 1963 on the hot water bath versus the cold water bath for baked custards indicated that custards started in a 20°C water bath were superior to those started in a 40°C water bath for the following characteristics: inside appearance, 24 crust tenderness, texture, flavor, consistency, syneresis, and general acceptability. Use of Qgied Whole Milk in Bokeo Cusgards Bittner (3) reports the results of using dried whole milk (DWM) in place of other milks in baked custards, all of which were baked to 86°C. Baked custards prepared with DWM were similar in appearance to custards made with homogenized milk or with pasteurized milk, however the crusts of the DWM custards were slightly more wrinkled and less tender than those made from homogenized milk. .The inside color for the DWM custards scored slightly higher than when homogenized or pasteurized milk was used. The DWM custards had a highly significantly different flavor than pasteurized milk cus- tards — a decided cooked flavor was indicated and the DWM custards were significantly sweeter than those made with pasteurized milk. The DWM custards were firmer than cus- tards made from homogenized milk but less firm than those made from pasteurized milk. Evaluation of Foods Food products can be evaluated by either of two methods: the subjective method in which a particular characteristic of a product is scored or otherwise rated by an individual with a score being decided upon by judgment, or the objec- tive method where the outcome is largely independent of human judgment (43). 25 Although the limitations of a taste panel for judging the palatability or eating quality of food are realized by investigators, there are still organoleptic factors which cannot be expressed by objective measurements. Factors such as appearance, color, and flavor are better judged by a scoring panel. Results of objective tests may often be cor- related with results obtained by subjective means (3, 44). Lowe and Stewart (39) state it is desirable to employ both subjective and objective tests in connection with re. search on the functional and organoleptic preperties of food products. During the development of an objective test it is particularly appropriate to run parallel organoleptic tests in order to test the degree of correlation between them. Subjecgiye Metoooo Subjective tests measure the qualities of food as they make their impression individually and collectively on the sensory organs. They are subjective because the individual is required to go through a mental process in giving his opinion as to the qualitative and quantitative value of the characteristic or characteristics under study. Typos of goats and judgoents According to Lowe and Stewart (39), subjective tests may be conveniently classified into 2 categories: a) prefer- ence or acceptance tests and b) psychometric or difference tests. In preference testing the degree of acceptance is 26 obtained and, when conducted with a large enough number of people within a population, permits the determination of consumer acceptance of a product. The psychometric tests are for determining quantitative differences, such as rating or scoring food quality factors. They can be used to evaluate quality differences among breeds, varieties, or formulas. This makes these tests valuable re- search and development tools (39), Mason and Koch (43) state that two reasons for choosing a scoring system in preference to a ranking system where the treatments are ranked according to a preference for a given product are: l. a scoring system permits ties, whereas a ranking system forces judgment even though the taster can detect no definite difference. 2. a scoring system permits the spread of treat- .- ments to be influenced by the magnitude of the differences found. If there are very distinct differences between two samples, one may be scored 1 and one scored 10, but in rank ng they must be 1 and 2. Often a numerical scale is used so that the scores of the individual taste panel members can be added readily to give a composite score (48). e de t ns 0 t ste e 8 Owing to practical considerations, most panels are com- posed of from four to twelve members and if only three or four acceptable judges are available, results comparable to those of a larger panel can be obtained if each sample is scored two or three times during the investigation (20, 30, 39). 27 According to Dawson et al. (15) a panel of three to ten is usually adequate for testing flavor differences but panel size depends on the variability associated with the experi- ment and on the magnitude of the difference between samples to be detected. A Experimental work by Langwill (33) indicates that women between the ages of seventeen and thirty years of age have a greater sensitivity of taste in distinguishing between sweet, salty, sour, and bitter foods than do men in the same age bracket. Laird and Breen (32) report from their study that, in comparison with men, women at all ages have more prefer- ences for tart tastes, and less for sweet tastes, The age of the judge is also a factor. Young people can distinguish differences between different strengths of sweet, sour, and salty food when middle-aged peOple cannot (l). Sensory ability decreases with age and preferences change also. Because of this it is felt that taste panel members should be between the ages of twenty and fifty (31). Objective Methods Types of objective evaluations are many and varied, in- cluding chemical, histological, and physical tests, Griswold (20) states objective methods are reproducible and less sub- ject to error than sensory methods. There are many examples in the literature of objective tests for measuring such varied acceptance characteristics of foods as texture, color, tenderness, consistency, and 28 juiciness (39). There is no objective method for measuring flavor pggwgg. However, some flavor components may be meas- ured by chemical means, i.e., sugar, salt, and acid. According to Lowe and Stewart (39) objective tests pos- sess obvious advantages with respect to reproducibility and in the ease of applicability to the needs of the research laboratory. However, the ultimate test of whether an objec- tive test is measuring a quality is its agreement with re- sults of subjective testing (20). EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE Preliminary Investigations Preliminary experimental work with the addition of food colors to baked custards indicated the possibility of devel- oping in baked custard several pastel colors typical of dif- ferent varieties of fruit. Strawberry, raspberry and peach colors were shown to be feasible, and of these, peach showed the most promise.. The kinds and amounts of food colors required to pro- duce a peach color in baked custards were determined in this preliminary study. Various combinations of spec Red No. 4- and Yellow No. 5 were found to yield the most satisfactory range of tints and shades of peach color in baked custards. An arbitrary decision was made as_to the color range selec— ted for study. Two randomly selected preference panels of twenty persons each indicated which of these colors would be most acceptable in baked custards. After the colors were established, various types and amounts of peach flavors individually and in combination were tried in the baked custards. 0n the basis of the trial tests, an arbitrary decision was made as to the flavor char- acter and strengths that would be included in the study. A randomly selected preference panel of twenty-two persons evaluated custards containing three different combinations of peach flavors and indicated that all three combinations 29 30 were acceptable. Design of ExPeriment Baked custards prepared from a standard formula with the addition of all possible combinations of three different peach colors and three levels of the same combination of three peach flavors were compared subjectively and objec— tively, A control custard having neither added color nor added flavor was used as a reference for the objective tests. golgg-Elavgr Qombinatiogs The nine treatment variables (A through I) represent all possible combinations of three different peach colors and three levels of the same peach flavor. _ C Elavgr Lgvgé Variable Eight Me u 2355, .£23 he gm x A x B x ' x c x x D x E x F x G x x a x I x Quantities of the colors and flavors for the nine treat- ment variables are presented in Tables 20 and 21, pages 88 31 and 89 in the Appendix. Six replications of each of the nine color-flavor com- ‘hinations were prepared and evaluated. The baked custards were cooled to room temperature by a standard procedure be— fore evaluation. Objective tests were conducted the first three weeks of the eXperiment and the subjective tests were conducted the following six weeks. Certain of the objective tests were performed daily on custards made from the same mix used for panel evaluation. This procedure was followed as a check against the objective tests previously conducted. Four treatment variables1 were prepared each day for the objective tests and baked in two batches. 'Sixteen custards, eight each of two treatment variables, werebaked at one time. Of these eight custards, seven were used for objec~ tive tests and one to obtain a timentemperature record during baking. For the subjective tests, three treatment variables were prepared each day in two bakes. Sixteen custards were baked at a time. In the first bake four custards of each of the three treatment variables were used for the taste panel, one custard of each of the three treatment variables was re- served for color determination. and one custard used to ob- tain a time-temperature record during baking. . The experiment had to be modified with respect tO‘the objective determination of color. Due to mechanical I 'Infthis'manuscript, treatment variable refers to a single color-flavor combination. 32 difficulties it was impossible to collect reliable data, therefore no further comment will be made on this point. A predetermined randomized arrangement was used for the preparation of the treatment variables throughout the experi- ment and for the order of presentation of the samples to the judges each day. The weakest flavored custard in a group was always,presented first to the taste panel. Room temperature and humidity in the experimental foods laboratory were recorded on each day that custards were baked and evaluated‘. Basic Custard Formula The formula selected for study. based on that of Lows (38) consisted of 3815 grams of reconstituted whole milk, 748 grams whole egg and 391 grams sucrose. Percentages of ingredients, based on total weight of the mixture. were: whole milk, 77.0; whole egg, 15.1; and sucrose. 7.9. The percent ratio of dried whole milk to water was l2.6 to 87.4. Ingredient Procurement la ents A common lot of dried whole milk was prepared to speci- fication by the Michigan State University Dairy Plant. The reconstituted milk contained 3.2% butterfat. The dried whole milk was packaged in small sealed polyethylene bags, ' hygro-Thermograph, Model 594, Price Instrument Division, Bendix Aviation Corporation, Baltimore. Maryland. 33 each containing 504 grams, and refrigerated at 38-400F until needed. The dried whole milk was removed from the refriger- ator and brought to room temperature before reconstitution. Fresh eggs were obtained from a designated flock of White Leghorn hens at the Michigan-State University Poultry Farm. The eggs used were fresh daily. ‘Egg yolk color de- terminations were made at the beginning and in the middle of the experiment. The Cargill-Nutrena Yolk Color Meter read- ings were 5-6. The Heiman-Carver Yolk Color Roter determina- tions were 11-12 on all samples tested (21). A common lot of sucrose. obtained from the Michigan State University Food Stores, was packaged in closed poly- ethylene bags, 391 grams per package. and stored in a covered metal container at room temperature. The FD&C certified food colorswere obtained from Food Materials Corporation in dry, powdered form and stored at room temperature in their original glass containers. Eiélfififi In the preliminary work no single peach flavoring materv ial imparted a satisfactory peach flavor to the baked cus- tard. It was necessary to use combinations of peach flavor- ing materials. A combination of three different peach fla- voring materials was selected. A description of the flavors and the quantity used are found in Tables 20, 21, and 22. 34 pages 88, 89, and 90 in the Appendix. The flavoring materi- als were used full strength as received from the manufac- turer. Preparation of Color Solutions For addition to the custard mix, three color solutions (light, medium. dark) were prepared by dissolving varying proportions of spec Red No. 4 and spec Yellow No. 5 in dis. tilled water. The weights of the two colors used in each of the color solutions are found in Table 23 in the Appendix. Preparation of Custard Mix Each day 4500 c.c. of the basic custard mix was pre- pared. The detailed procedure for the preparation of the custard mix is given in Exhibit 1. page 92 in the Appendix. Fresh, shell eggs were broken out. blended, and weighed. One package of prevweighed sucrose was added to the eggs and the mixture blended. The reconstituted milk was added to the egg-sugar mixture and the entire mixture was blended. The custard mix was strained through a mediumwfine, wire- mesh household strainer to remove unmixed material prior to the addition of color and flavor. The basic custard mix was divided into smaller portions for addition of color-flavor variables. For the objective tests the basic mix (4500 c.c.) was divided into four por- tions of 1100 c.c. each. For the subjective tests it was 35 divided into three portions of 1500 c.c. each. All mixings to incorporate colors and flavors were done on Hobart K—SA mixers equipped with quuart stainless steel bowls and a paddle attachment. Portions of each final mix~ ture reserved for the second bake were left at room tempera- ture in the mixing bowls and covered with polyethylene coated freezer paper secured with a rubber band. Baking Procedure The custards were baked in Seounce pyrex custard cups. Each cup was filled with custard mix to a measured depth of 1 3/4 inches (approximately 130 c.c. custard mix). The cups were placed in a specially designed galvanized water bath pan equipped with a bronze stepcock for drainage and a per- forated stainless steel sixteen-hole rack to support the cups (Figure l). The cups were placed in a predetermined randomized order for the subjective and objective tests. Water at 18—20°C was added to the pan until it came up to the level of the custard mix. The baking pan was placed on an oven rack 3 7/8 inches from the bottom of an apartment size General Electric Oven preheated to 350°F. Three lead wires from a Brown Eleotronik Potentiometer High Speed Multi— ple Point Recorder were clamped in place.‘ One thermocouple was centered in a custard located in a central position of the baking pan, a second was positioned in the water bath just to the right of the custard containing the first 37 thermocouple. The third thermocouple recorded the tempera— ture at the center of the oven. All custards were baked to an internal temperature of 86°C. The oven was turned off and the water partially drained out of the water bath pan through the stopcock. The rack containing all the custard cups was then removed from the water bath pan. The oven was again preheated to 350°F and the second bake was carried out following the same procedure as the first bake. The cusq tards were placed on wire racks and allowed to cool at room temperature for approximately three hours before objective or subjective tests were conducted. _Subjective Tests The samples for the subjective tests were selected so that, in the course of six replications of each variable. each judge had an equal number of samples from inside bake and outside bake positions (see diagram below) as well as front and rear oven bake positions. . Outside bake 0 Inside bake The panel was composed of 7 judges (4 women, 3 men) from several related departments of the university. A one- hour training session was held for all taste panel members 38 at which they were given instructions as to how the samples would be presented. how to score the factors and the share acterietics of a good baked custard. A sample of the in-‘ struction sheet is shown on pages 94 and 95 in the Appendix. To acquaint the taste panel members with peach flavor, sever- al brands of peach ice cream were tasted. The taste panel evaluated the room temperature custards for nine characteristics; crust color, crust tenderness.I crust flavor, inside color, aroma. inside flavor. consisten-. cy, texture, and syneresis. Three different custard varia- tions were scored each day. The custards were presented one at a time and the panel was requested to evaluate each sample individually without comparison with the other sam- ples. A 7epoint rating scale was used in the evaluation with 7 as the highest score. The panel was instructed to score each custard on all nine characteristics and to check the most appropriate descriptive terms for those factors they scored 4 or below. A sample of the score sheet is shown on page 96 in the Appendix. Objective Tests On the day of preparation, objective tests were made on the room temperature custards to determine the gel strength, measured both as the ability to resist penetration and as thegability to hold a rigid shape; syneresis; and pH. 39 Continuous recordings of room temperature and relative hu- midity were made during each test period. e tr 1 . Gel strength was determined by a Micrometer Adjustment Penetrometer1 equipped with a 35-gram grease cone attach- 2. Each sample was placed on a level platform directly ment beneath the cone attachment, with the point of the cone just touching the surface of the custard. The cone was released for a period of 5 seconds and the depth of penetration.re- corded to the nearest 0.1 millimeter. Measurements were’. . made both with the crust on (crust loosened from the edges of the cup) and the crust off (crust entirely removed). Two different samples were used for each of these measurements: one from an inside bake position and one from an outside bake position. Readings were recorded and averaged. Figure 2 shows the measurement of penetration after the release of the cone.. Pe gent Sag 1h order to compare the firmness of the different cus- tards, measurements of the height of the inverted sample be- fore and after a specified time interval were made. The pro-' cedure used for this objective test was that described by Zhbik (64). The apparatus shown in Figure 3 was used to ; 715thur H. Themes Co. Precision Scientific Co. - Cat. No. 73525 41 minimize damage to the gel structure of the sample during testing and to facilitate the collection ofsuch data.' The per cent sag for each sample was calculated from the differ- ence between readings divided bythe initial height of the inverted custard. §yngggsi§ Th. method of Miller et al. (49) was used to determine the weight of the drainage from the custards. The equipment for this test is shown in Figure 4. The baked custard with the crust intact was loosened carefully and inverted on fine wire screening (15 wires per inch) which had been placed over a weighed Petri dish. The assembly was immediately covered with a glass bowl to pre- vent evaporation and allowed to stand one hour. it the end of this period. the wire screen with the custard was removed and the weight of the drainage recorded to the nearest 0.1 gram. Per cent drainage was calculated from the ratio of the weight of the drainage to the weight of the sample be- fore inversion. £2 The pH of 50 ml of the fluid custard mix was recorded. The pH of the baked custard was determined after thoroughly blending one custard (approximately 130 c.c.) in a Waring Blender for one minute. All samples for the pH determina- tions were at room temperature (220-2506) and the (\)J. J! J: «4 [N 9.4 i‘Jl W Isl ll \ ‘1 1|: 0 43 measurements were made on a Beckman‘Zeromatic pH meter with a glass electrode and a saturated calomel electrode. Statistical Methods The data obtained from the subjective and objective tests were evaluated by the use of two computer programs on the CDC 3600 computer at the Michigan State University Coma puter Center. The FAOREP Routine 2 Option 1 (one-way face toriel with replications) was used to calculate analyses of variance and the CORE Routine was used to calculate simple correlations. Significant differences among variables were evaluated through use of the Studentized range tables (17). An illustration of the Studentized range calculations for one of the quality factors is shown in the Appendix, pages 97, and 98. 1 ‘ Correlation coefficients were calculated on the follow- impairs of items: penetrometer readings (crust off) versus per cent sag, penetrometer readings (crust off) versus con- sistency, per cent sag versus consistency, per cent drainage versus syneresis score, texture versus syneresis. crust color versus crust flavor, inside color versus inside flavor, crust color versus inside color, crust flavor versus inside flavor, inside flavor versus aroma, and consistency versus texture. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The baked custards prepared from the nine possible com- binations of three different peach colors and three levels of peach flavor were evaluated bysubjective and objective tests. The identifying code for the combinations of colors and flavor levels used as treatment variables is as follows: Variable legre Elgvgg Qombinatign Light color. low flavor level Light color, medium flavor level Light color. high flavor level Medium color. low flavor level Medium color. medium flavor level _Medium color. high flavor level i Dark color. low flavor level Dark color. medium flavor level Dark color, high flavor level CONTROL No added color,-no added flavor Hm$ wmu am» The scores obtained from the objective and subjective tests were analyzed by a oneeway analysis of variance and by the Studentized multiple range testril7) when significant differences were found in the analysis of variance. The mean taste panel scores and objective test readings ‘ for each replication of the baked custard variables and the standard deviations of the means are given in the tables accompanying the discussion of the results for each of thei.‘ quality factors. 44 45 Subjective‘Tests Crust Color The mean scores and standard deviations for crust color are listed in Table 1. Analysis of variance of these data (Table 2) revealed significant differences in crust color attributable to the treatments. ComparisOn of treatment means indicated custard F scored significantly higher than custards A. I, H,.B, and C at the 1 per cent level of prob- ability and, in addition, significantly higher than custard G at the 5 per cent level of probability. Custards F. D. and E were not significantly different at the 5 per cent level of probability, indicating for crust color the medium peach color was equally acceptable regardless of the flavor level used, Custards A, D, and G (low flavor level) were not significantly different at the l per cent level of prob— ability, indicating for crust eclor the low level of flavor was acceptable, regardless of the peach color used. However, the effect of flavor on color does not appear to be con- sistento The Judges indicated the crusts of the light colored custards (A, B. and C) were too yellow. too pale. too light. and unappetizing. This objectionably light crust color ac- counted for the low scores assigned custards B and C butp does not explain the higher score given to custard A. The higher score for custard A (light color, low flavor level) may have been due to first samplelbiaa since this sample was 46 4.71 Table 1. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Crust Color. Variw e c ns Grand ssia._n..l;--w.-2”mq---l. WW,,... all, an fi.lfl sass..ss2i A1 5.142 4.83 5.00. 5.29 ~ 4.45 4.85 4.92:0.30 B 5.57 5.43 3.57 4.00 4.00 5.57 5.69:0.25 c 3.71 3.43 3.50 3.86 _ 3.86 x 3.71 3.68:0.18 D 5.00 5.14 5.00 5.57 5.29; 5.71 5.29:0.30 s 4.71 5.83 5.50! 5.29 4.86 5.14 5.22:0.41, F 5.57 5.71 6.00 5.71 .5.29 5.55 5.6oto,27' e ‘ 4.86’ 4.86 4.57 5.29" 5.86 ' 5.14 5.10:0.45 H '4.29 4,43 4.83 4.57‘ 4.86 4,67 4.61:0.22 1 5.00 5.00 '5.14 4.45 4.57» ‘ 4.81:0.28 rw-Yr'vv'vr' rry—wrv WV“ rrvwav .v—v Y’W‘ . vmw—‘Y'y-f vvr 1‘— IF-“ firv .1-wfiv-1Vw—Y'r‘v r—wwv W vwv-Y ‘VV'va' '- 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 7 Judges' evaluations of 1 replication of each variable. 1W 47 Table 2. Analysis of Variance for Crust Color. 333333"3? Degrees—SF‘ “ “1635‘"“ “T” "F'"" I££$£££2e;. “.m, aa’232.‘Zv‘..§$.1.S?..‘¥iw :w.ll:§£22£sw.w.-sn--R&£12.g. Treatment means 8 . 2.7569 29.41aa_1 Error 45 0.0937 L Total 53 ** Significant at l per cent level of probability. a- ,lmmll_.l_,mw-, a, . w . ,. Duncan's Studentized Multiple Range Test] 1% Level: 13§81362 4H 61 4181 4. 92 §,-;Q 5:22, figfifi. §;_Q 1*er w-v—v—wv ‘1‘v— 'm ,_., Y' .l T “1...? a _.,.. w“ ., W Y 5% Level° . . - —vv 7 -V W y— V7 vw _ l l uvw T fiwar —r—r . ‘r —rv v r—‘r—‘VVV‘ w- —v—- v- w—nt t r v y —-r v v—v 17 “V wv—— thww '7' ~, W .V'V‘ “‘V ‘ ‘ Means underscored by the same line are not significantly different (17). 48 always evaluated first whenever it was served. The dark colored custards (G, H, and I) scored lower than the medium colored custards, although treatment H was the only one of the three that scored significantly lower than any of the medium colored custards (D. E, and F). The Judges indicated the crusts of the dark colored custards were too red, too dark, and more orange colored than peach. It was observed that the crust color was consistently darker than the inside color for all the variables and the baking procedure did not produce browning of the crust for any of the samples regardless of the peach color used. Figure 5, page 49, illustrates'crust colors for the three different peach colors used in the study. r t T _ se- The mean scores and standard deviations for crust ten— derness are listed in Table 3. 'Analysis of variance of these data revealed no significant difference in the crust tenderness of the nine treatment variables. qust Elavog Table 4 lists the mean scores and the standard devia- tions for crust flavor. The individual custards were sig- nificantly different for crust flavor only at the 5 per cent level of probability as shown by the analysis of mariance, Table 5. Comparison of the variables by the Studentizcd multiple range method indicated custards F, G. and D scored 49 _’_—— Figure 5. Three different peach colors1 in baked custards (crust on) . W“ ————_— __———~_~—_— ‘Figureug:j_Three different peach colo s in inverted baked custards . I 'Zér£"1¢ night 3'light color, medium color, dark color. Table 7. JO Tenderness. 50 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Crust ' w‘v—vwv Grand : - , - Massage 5:94. A‘ 4.149 4.55 5.17 6.00 5.45 4.67 4.96:0.71 B 5.57 4.57 5.29 5.43 5.29 5.43 5.26:0.36 c 5.00 5.00 5.67 5.45 ' 5.57 6.00 5.45:0.59 D ,.5,71 5.71 4.50 5.7i 5.86 6.14 5.61:0.57 s 4.00 » 5.67 5.50 6.00 6.14 5.86‘ 5.53t0.78 F _ ,»5,00 5.29 5.57 5.57 5.29 5.50 5.37:0.22 0 4.86 5.14 5.29 5.29 5.71 5.86 5.36t0.37 H _ 5.14. 4.86 5.67 5.14 - 5.45 5.50 5.29:0.50 ‘1 4.43 ' 5.35 5.55 5.71 7 5.14. 5.57 5.25:0.45 'Y ,. v WW7, yrs-"fir wrr'. Y V‘w—w-w yw.~'w~—vw. Y'“ ‘ Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 7 Judges' evaluation of l replication of each variable. 51 Table 4. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Crust Flavor. - ,m.-1w 1 - a 111m-“ m. 1--.;anwn Variw Grand . . 1 . .. .1! 1 11.....- 1.1MM8 .‘ S D 4' 4.292; 4.67 5.17 5.71 5.45 4.67 ~ 4.99:0.54 B 4.45 5.00 4.45 4.57 5.00 4.86 4.72:0.27 0 4.57 4.86 5.35 5.14 4.57 4.86 _ 4.89:0.50 ' D 4.57 5.14 5.55 5.71 ‘ 5.57 5.45 5.2940.40 s 5.00 5.17 5.00 5.29 5.57 5.43 5.24:0.23 F 5.14 5.45 5.45 5.57 5.45 5.50 5.42:0.15 G 4.45 4.71 v 5.45 5.57 6.14 5.71 5.35:0.64 H 5.14 5.00 5.00, 5.00 5.00 5.85 v5.16t0.33 I 5.14 4.50 5.50 4.86 4.14 4.8840.49 4 «i J 1 r- rvvww—rvuv—un V‘W‘vuV—rva w—y Irv-'nw‘ v w'v' ‘rv '- u—"' w 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 7 Judges' evaluation of l replicationof each variable. 52 Table 5. Analysis of Variance for Crust Flavor senses of ” "'"W Degreee‘sr 5*" " “ " ‘ wean" WW” ”3‘" V. 3"? 11- am .,. 1-11.11. W -11 5.4.212. Treatment means 8 . 0.3514 2,17e Error 45 . ‘0.1619 Total 53 ‘ W 77‘" yv . v w -: v T 1 VV- 1 wyw . v‘xv v. “Yr—y . v.‘ —u (xv—f— V‘V‘l wrr—l rwfi w—-- v— ,fiv‘ W 1 * Significant at 5 per cent level of probability. r—V—fi w 7* —--y—-7 77' —r 7 rwr .fiwvv-w v—w-ur .—.,,Ylvl rvvr—r-w v v77r T . -— WW v—wr ’T' '—V . 1 WV Duncan's Studentized Multiple Range TestI V—‘V'Tl—wa‘ —-w w r Y ry r—w W. 7 vvv—w V7 w ‘fi-v if" W q—q w—w ‘7 ‘I' w—w—— T r w .- Yr—w 7—71— 'w‘ 1 quu— v e‘V—Wr— 5%Level; s 1 . 0 1 a E 0' 0 F 4.72 $45—55 4.89 44995.16 5.;4 5,32 5,55 5.42 ' Means underscored by the same line are not significantly different (17). 53 significantly higher than custard B at the 5 per cent level of probability. Panel members' comments indicated the crust flavor for custard B (light celor, medium flavor level) was too weak, artificial, bitter, and slightly offvflavor. -These results indicate the possibility of a "halo effect" of crust color impression on crust flavor Judgments since custards D and G were low flavor level, mediumand dark color respectively, and scored higher than custard B which was medium flavor level but light color. It was also noted the flavor was more concentrated in the crust than in the inside portion of the custard. Insi C r The mean scores and the standard deviations for inside color are listed in Table 6. Analysis of variance of these data (Table 7) revealed significant differences for inside color at the l per cent level of probability. Comparison of treatment means indicated custard G scored significantly higher than custards A, B, and 0 (light color) at the l per cent level of probability and, in addition, significantly higher than custard E at the 5 per cent level of probability. . Custards D, E, f, G, H, and I were not significantly different at the l per cent level of probability, indicating for inside color, the medium and the dark peach colors were equally'acceptable regardless of the flavor level used. Cus- tards A, B, and C contained the light peach color and scored Table 6 0 Color. 54 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Inside Hmmunriuow 4.292 4.83 4.29 5.86 4.29 5.86 4.86 5.14 4.71 5.35 5.00 5.00 5.71 5.29 4.86 4.86 5.14 5.17 4.83 3.86 4.17 5.17 5.17 5.57 ‘5.14 5.00 4.83 fr—vyrwu-l- 4.71) 4.00 3.57 5.57 5.00 N 5.43 5.43 5.29 5.29 4.85 4.00 5.86 5.29 5.29 5.50 5.29 5.55 5.14 4.58:0.35 5.95:0.24 3.8930.30 5.22:0.23 5.04:0.28 5.27c0.26 5.45:0.28 5.1l20.22 5.12:0.15 y.- ‘1—1 v v w vw—r vww'wrfi‘rn'v " Ir Trv‘ v w ‘v-‘wv w “.7 ww—fi.‘ "VFW ‘VT'W'F' 'fi‘y w—r—w-r—v I'r'r rv' .-q var—vv—w—Tv 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 7 Judges' evaluations of_l replication of each variable. 55 Table 7. Analysis of Variance for Inside Color. p. with . .1. .4 r—ww m1... w .7..- -wl—r W.T . “.4 m. ”m Eource of fiegrees of Mean F Treatment means 8 2.0232' 29,55as Error 45 0.0685 Total 53 *.. Significant at l per cent level of probability. 7— r... y y vwwvwvv—wrr-w v y 7] why '77 vr.‘ . v ‘— w—v'vvwfi .. W m ‘ we Duncan's Studentized Multiple Range Test‘ 1% Level: G B A E H I D F G 5% Level: rv—‘V‘VT 1 r wv vr' r‘ Yvwr'W-m'r , “VT" I'Vr-w vv-r #7 w . .7- r 11". I-vyryrv- ‘r by. ’v-y r. VW' v w w—y— Few .. 1 v vr—vn ‘- 4 ' Means underscored by the same line are not significantly different (17). 56 significantly lower for inside color than all the other cus- tards. The Judges indicated the inside colors of the cus- tards with the light color (A, B. and C) were too pale, too light, and too yellow for a peach color in baked custards. It was observed the inside color was consistently light» or than the crust color for all variables. Custards B and C were scored significantly lower than all other custards for both crust color and inside color at the l per cent level of probability.“ Figure 6, page 49, illustrates inside colors for the three different peach colors used in the study. ' Issac Table 8 shows the mean scores and the standard devia- tions for aroma. The analysis of variance showed no sign nificant difference for aroma among the nine treatment vari- ables at the 5 per centlevel of probability. I Aroma in custards is a factor often difficult to evalun ate. In this study the aroma seemed to be more difficult to evaluate if the crust had been removed from the custard for several minutes. The descriptive terms indicated frequently on the Judges' score sheets fer all of the treatment vari- ables were: no peach aroma, too weak, and "eggy". It ap- pears the addition of the three levels of peach flavor used in this experiment did not contribute to the peach aroma of any of the baked custards. Table 8. 57 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Aroma. F1 0:3 H 32.43 4.142 4.14 4.43 3.71 4.14 4.14 " 4029 3.57 3986 .- 42%2_.§42+ '4.00 4.86 4.29 4.29 4.29 4.29 4.14 4.71 4.57 Y 4943 4029 yw—‘q 4.14’ 4.14 5.86 4.29 4.86. 4.29 4.29. 4.29 4.29 4.29 4.45 ‘4.55 ’ 4.29' 5.17 4.71 3.67 4.04:0.25 4.31:0.39 4.2020.26 4.10:0.26 4.5440.55 4.2940.16 4.19:0.46 4.52:0.56 4.4340.32- I W 'x y—Y I urwfi 1.....— w Iwrfi' 1" "T v... . M v rr— '7 Y'r' Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 7 Judges’ evaluations of l replication of each variable. 58 inside Flavor The mean scores and standard deviations for inside flag vor are given in Table 9. No significant difference at the 5 per cent level of probability was indicated by analysis of variance of the inside flavor scores. The high standard de- viation for custard A may have been caused by the low score for the first replication, the first custard the taste panel evaluated in this experiment. Custard I received the highest mean score for inside flavor, however it was not significantly higher than the other custards. Custard 0 (light color, high flavor level). received the lowest mean score for inside flavor, indicating the Judgments for inside flavor were influenced by impres- sions of the color. 0n the average, the group of custards with the dark color (G, H, and I) received the highest score for inside flavor. Treated as a group, it seems that the dark color may have exerted some “halo effect” on the inside flavor. When comparing the inside flavor scores with the crust flavor scores (which were significantly different at the 5 per cent level of probability). it should be pointed out some of the panel members indicated the flavor appeared to be concentrated in the crust of the custards. The taste panel members indicated the inside flavor of all the custards was too weak, "eggy", and artificial. All of the custards contained the same amount of sugar but 59 Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Inside Flavor. Table 9. Vari~ ‘ , . A ... -1111 1,.“1111 ,. -,-.-1;M393h—£Ldlt A1 3.142 4 17 4.33 ’ 4.29 4.57 4.33 4.14:0.51 B 4.29 4.14 4.29 4.00 4.43 4.14 4.22:0.15 c 4.14 4.14 4.00 4.29 3.86 4.14 4.10:0.15 D 3.86 3.86 4.33 '4.29 . 4.71 A 4.86 4.3210.42 E 3.71 4.50 5.00 4.57 4.71 4.57 4.51:0.43 F‘ 3.86 4.86 4.43 4.86 4.57 4.67 4.54:0.37 c 4.29 3.86 4.71 4.86 4.71 4.71 ' 4.52:0.38 H 4.71 4.29 4.71 4.57 4.14 5.17 4.51:0.39 I 4.57 4.33 5.00 4.71 5.14 4.43 4.70:0.32 —, WW Yv‘v‘rvv-v- 'W rvwv—yrv PWW‘W—T ww - 1 Refer to code on page 44-for variable identification. T—vv W ‘—‘YV' —- Trvrvv PVT" wvwwv—T V—r v—v'r' 1 w—Wv-gvr-qr w ‘~—‘—‘— 1' W 2 Mean score of 7 Judges' evaluation of l replication of each variable. ——...,—. 6O Judgments on the sweetness of the interior custard varied considerably among Judges. For all nine variable treatments one panel member indicated the inside flavor was too sweet while another member indicated it was not sweet enough. The panel member who indicated the custards were too sweet had a low threshold for sucrose (0.015M) and the panel member who indicated the custards were not sweet enough had a high. threshold for sucrose (0.035M) as determined by taste threSh- .old studies conducted at the beginning of this experiment. W ,Table 10 shows the mean scores and standard deviations for consistency. Analysis of variance of the scores for consistency indicated there was no.eignificant difference at the 5 per cent level of probability. Therefore, con- sistency showed no effect due to treatment. Texture The mean scores and standard deviations for texture are listed in Table 11. There was no significant difference at the 5 per cent level of probability as indicated by analysis of variance. The high standard deviations for custards C, D, and G may have been caused by the low scores in replications 2, 3, and 4 where the panel members indicated the interior texture was not smooth and the custards contained several holes. These custards were baked to the same end temperature as all 61 Table 10. nean Scores and Standard Deviations for Consist? ency. yu-r-‘rv “VT—fl. -—v—1wv firr—wv Tue—'— ruv-«p‘r Grand 1. .11.-iw11.1- ..- - 4......Esae__§a21 41 5.712 5.83 5.33 5.57 5.43 5.33 5.53:0.21 B 5.29 4,86 5.57 5.14 5.57 5.71 5.36t0.32 c 5.86 5.57 4.33 5.29 5.71 5.43 5.37:0.55 D 5.00 5.29 4.83 5.29 4.86 5.86 5.1910.39 E 5.00 5.50 5.67 5.14 5.43 5.57 5.39:0.26 F 5.57 5.86 6.00 5.29 5.29 5.50 5.59:0.29 e 5.43 5.14 5.57 5.43 5.43 5.43 5.41:0.14 H 4.71 5.29 5.00 5.14 5.29 5.33 5.13:0.24 1 5.14 5.67 5.83 5.71* 5.86 5.86 5.6810.28 1 2 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. Mean score of 7 Judges’ evaluations of l replication of each variable. ’ 62 Table 11. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Texture. Variw Grand 82}? -1’ 7 -11. ,1-11111. -... -1. Mass.s§s21 4' 4.002 5.50 4.50 5.00 4.57 3.67 4.54:0.66 s 5.43 4.86 6.00 4.00 4.57 5.14- 5.17:0.77 0 6.14 3.86 3.33 4.00 5.86 4.71 4.66:1.14 D 6.00 3.57 3.83 4.14 4.43 5.57 4.59:0.98 E 4.43 4.67 4.67 4.00 5.86 4.29 4.6530.64 F 5.57 6.00 5.43 4.86 5.43 4.00 5.05:0.75 a 5.29 6.14 5,71 3.57 5.86 5.43 5.33:0.92 H 5.43 4.14 3.67 4.71 4,57 4.00 '4.42:o.62 I 4.14 4.17 4.33 4.29 4.71 5.14 4.46:0.39 rrvr—y— 1v r.~—vvvvw~,*1erT'—1r ‘ r w—W—W—rl w—rwq -vvv Y .— 1 , , v.— fi—vv 'f-rvv wv—r 7.“ II1Y'WT 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 7 Judges' evaluations of 1 replication of each variable. 63 other custards and the time required to reach 86°C was with- in the range of times for all other custards. Examination of the baking records does not reveal the reason for these high standard deviations. esi* Table l2 lists the means scores and standard deviations for the syneresis scores. Analysis of variance indicated no significant difference at the 5 per cent level of probabili- ty. The high standard deviation for custard C may have been caused by the low score for replication 3 and the relatively high scores for replications l and 5. The texture score for these replications of custard G showed similar variation. on scoring texture, the taste panel indicated this custard contained some holes and therefore a greater amount of syn~ eresis would be expected. Objective Tests genetratigg The depth to which the penetrometer cone penetrated the baked custards was used as a measurement of the firmness of the custards. The mean penetrometer values with the crust on and with the crust off and the differences in the mean penetrometer values (crust off minus crust on) are included in Tables 1), l4, and 15 respectively. Analyses of variance of the penetrometer values (from which the values of the control were excluded) indicated 64 Table 12. Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Syneresis. A‘ 5.002 6.00 4.83 5.86 4.71 5.00 5.23:0.55 B 5.86 6.14 5.86 4.71 5.14 5.43 5.5230.53 C 5.86 4.57 4.00 4.71 5.29 5-57 5.1710.88 D 5.71 5.00 4.83 5.14 5.14 6.14 5.3310950 a 4.86 5.67 -‘5.33 4.29 6.14 5.29 5.26:0.64 F 5.29 6.00 5.71 5.71 5.00 5.17 5.4810.38 c 5.71 6.14 5157 4.57 6.29. ‘5.43 5.62:0.61 H 5.43 4.71 4.50 5.71 4.71 5.00 5.0120.47 I 4.57 5.33 5.17 5.43 5.29 5.86 5.2830.42 k- 1 2 v 7*?— fir IVY u-vv 7" WWW wwwj nw—r‘ vr y—v-vvvj Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. Mean score of 7 Judges' evaluations of l replication of each variable. _ 65 Table 13. Mean Penetrometer Values and Standard Deviations on Baked Custards with the crust Uni (Values in millimeters). ' Variw -' Grand W-th A' 28.02 27.6 29.2 28.0 28.7 28.6 28.420.59 8 27.6 27.3 26.7 27.2 27.7 28.1 27.430.48 c 27.9 28.1 28.9 26.7 ‘27.} 26.5 27,620.91 D 27.1 29.4 28.5 29.6 28.6 27.0 28.421.11 a 28.0 29.0 28.6 27.2 26.9 26.5 27.720.99 F 26.9 '27.8 27.4 28.0 26.9 27.7 27.520.47 c 27.6 27.1 27.3 27.0 26.4 28.1 27.340.58 H '26.8 28.4 27.0 27.1 28.5 28.4 272710.81 I 26.9 28.3 28.4 27.8 27.9 27.7 27.820.54 CON- 26.3 26.8 ' 27.5 27.1 27.330.70 TROL 28.0 '28.1 MW , V ‘5 v-v1rry upwv-V-rrrv V'vv‘ ,vv‘w‘ w. ‘v 'Yvr' 1 ""1" 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 2 evaluations of l replication of each variable. 66 Mean Penetrometer Values and Standard Deviations on Baked Custards with the Crust off. (Values in millimeters). Table 14. 'W‘TT'“ V Y . fir ~u-V v'vv y—vv—wyw —v———v v wr—vvvw—rr .- Grand , _ w. - .. -2..1. efissaw.§424 A1 29.82 29.1 29.9 30.1 31.0 30.9 3o.1t0.72 s 29.2 30.6 29.8 30.3 30.9 30.2 * 30.220.60 C 31.5 ‘31.2 33.5 30.2 30.4 29.9 31.111.32 D 29.1 32.3 31.1 31.1 .31.6 30.1 30.911.13 E 30.1 ~31.0 32.3 30.5 30.1 30.5 30.8:0.83 F 30.3 30.7 31.1 31.1 29.8 29.7 30.5i0.62 c 31.7, 28.8 31.2 29.9 29.1 30.4 30.2ii.14 H 31.0 31.1 30.0 30.0 29.6 29.6 30.220.67 I 31.3 30.2 31.8 28.2 30 3 30.4 30.5:1.25 gfigi '30.0 30.4 30.7 29.7 32,4 30.6 30.620.94 'xru-r I'v'ri' vwrw —‘-—~ .yv-vw—uwalw . :v—v v I - v y wwj ‘vwvrv'T-Y 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 2 evaluations of l replication of each variable. ‘V 67 Table 15. Mean Penetrometer Difference Values and Standard Deviations. (Values in millimeters). Grand . , . . _ . .1 M- . -Mesaa.§+2l 41 1.82 1.5 0.7 2.1 2.3 2.3 1.78:0.61 B 1.6 3.3 3.1 3.1 3.2 2.1 2.73to.71 C 306 30]- 496 305 301 304 3955:0955 0 2.0 2.9 2.6 1.5 3.0 3.1 2.52:0.64 2 2.1 2.0 3.7 3.3 3.2 4.0 3.05:0.83 r 3.4 2.9 3.7 3.1 2.9 2.0 3.0010.58 c 4.1 1.7 3.9. 2.9 2.7 2.3 2.93:0.92 a 4.2 2.7 3.0 2.9 1.1 1.2 2.52:1.18 I 494 199 304 004 299 207 2c6a1936 CONH TROL 3.7 3.6 3.2 1.7 5.3 2.5 L 3.33:1.22 Y - WV—WV'Y“ yww—qwmvrww as "vvv‘r wwwvw .w .m —. firr—rwvv—v n w— v V 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 2 Mean score of 2 evaluations of l replication of each variable. 68 there were no significant differences in firmness with the crust on. the crust off, or for the difference in the mean penetrometer values. There is no real explanation for the high standard deviations for several of the treatment vari- ables since each value is an average of two readings for one replication (one inside bake and one outside bake). There was also the same number of front and back bakes for each treatment. When comparing the mean penetrometer values (crust on and crust off) for the nine treatment variables with those of the control custard, it does not appear the addition of the small amounts of food colors and peach fla- vors altered the firmness of the gel structure to any extent. Differences between mean penetrometer values for sam- ples with and without crust were recorded because it was thought this comparison might provide a possible measurement of resistance due to the crust for each of the treatment variables. No consistent trend appears with respect to the three different colors or the three levels of flavor. W The mean per cent sag and standard deviations are listed in Table 16. Analysis of variance, excluding the control custard. showed no significant difference among the nine treatment variables. The very high standard deviation for the control cus- tard appears to be caused by the extremely high value for the sixth replication. It is felt that difficulty in Table 16. 69 Per Cent Sag and Standard Deviations of Baked Custards. I GONw TROL 4.58 6.99 7.52 9.52 4.92 4.80 7.34 4.92 7.34 7.16 7.91 7.52 10.52 4.92 4.80 7.16 7.71 ,w r‘v‘ 4. 92 15. 42 7.34 7.71 5.04 7.16 7.52 5.17 7.71 5.04 7.71 7.52 4.92 7.34 4.92 5.69 7.52 7.52 7.16 6.99 4.80 9.30 6.99 14.68 9.09 7.34 6-99 10.00 ”4.80 20.00 7.27:4.15 7.48:0.97 7.22:1.26 9.05:3.19 7.23:1.86 7.4Eil.85 6.51:1.24 6.39t2.02 6.9221.05 8.97:5.50 ,wer r" r-r .r—v~—r7 v-Vvyv—‘wvvy— V-W" 17W Y'T'T j—rerV— v vrv fir... wwv . V‘v‘r‘ y'v w—vr 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 7O removing the custard from the cup caused this high value; this was evident with the sixth replication of custard D and the fifth replication of custard A. The high standard deviations for all treatment vari- ables and the difficulties encountered in removing the cus- tard from the cup may indicate the method of performing the test for per cent sag used in this experiment is not suit- able for use on conventional baked custards. The per cent sag test as conducted in this experiment has been used with satisfactory results in other types of gels (64). The per cent sag. as measured by a different technique, has been used successfully on baked custards by Mastic (44) and Miller at al. (49). § yne;:esis The mean per cent drainage values and the standard de- viations are listed in Table 17. Analysis of variance of per cent drainage, excluding the control custard, revealed no significant difference. The high standard deviations for custards D, E, and F were caused by the very high values in the second and third replications. These high values appeared to be connected with the difficulty involved in removing the custard from the cup. The custard would crack causing a greater amount of drainage. It must be noted that all but two of the stand- ard deviations are higher than the corresponding means. These data do not represent true measurements of syneresis 7l Table 17. Per Cent Drainage and Standard Deviations of Baked Custards. 0.00 WWI W‘rv‘y‘v-f 7’!“ ‘V rrv, (fir—7w w - VT— 1 Grand . liegn Sch 0.90 0.00 0.84 3.85 2.30 3-83 0.09 0.00 0.09 0.00 1.20 0.44 1.64 2.74 2.26 1.51 0.17 0.17 0.00 1.46 , hr. 0934 1.60 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.41 0.00 1.02 0.00 0.56 iv fiv 0.57:0.52 0.43:0.61 0.71:0.85 1.52:1.55 1.14:1.04 1.04:1.48 0.2020.37 0.2610.39 0.1410.30 0.35:0.58 v —v Y 1 Refer to code on page 44 for variable identification. 72 and no definite conclusions should be based on them. H a n s The range of pH readings over six replications for the custard mix and for the baked custards are shown in Table 18. The ranges were so close no analysis of variance was carried out on the pH readings. In all cases the baked custards were more alkaline than the custard mix. This result is in accord with the findings reported by Logue (35). Lows (38), and Miller et al. (49). By comparison with the control custard, it appears that the addition of the small amounts of colors and flavors did not alter the acidity or alkalinity of the custard mixes or the baked custards under the conditions of this experiment. Correlation between Selected Measurements Correlation coefficients were calculated between selec- ted objective measurements, between eelected objective and subjective measurements and between selected subjective measurements. The results are shown in Table 19. The penetrometer values (crust off) and the per cent sag values were compared to determine the relationship be- tween these two obJective measurements for firmness. There was no significant correlation between these two objective tests indicating that one or both of these tests may be un- reliable as an objective method of determining firmness of baked custards. 73 Table 18. Range of pH Readings over Six Replications. Vari~ Range of pH for Range of pH for able _ i CREEEPd,ME§. w. ‘Baked Custard A 6.90 n 7.00 7.01 - 7.10 B 6.92 - 7.00 7.01 — 7.10 C 6.90 - 6.99 7.05 - 7.10 D 6.90 - 6.99 7.05 - 7.10 E 6.90 - 7.00 7.02 . 7.10 F 6.90 - 7.00 7.02 - 7.10 G 6.90 - 7.00 7.01 - 7.10 H 6.90 e 7.00 7.01 - 7.10 i 6.90 ~ 7.00 7.01 - 7.10 . CONTROL 6.90 - 7.00 7.02 - 7.10 1 W'Y v f w—r Refer to code on page AA-for variable identification. 74 Table 19. Correlation Coefficients of Selected Measurements. *v-v v VY—v—r w , yvv— ‘1 7 www ‘7 .. wV—p- I. Between Objective Measurements Penetrometer Values (Crust Off) vs.Per Cent Sag (Inverted, Crust Cn) +0.0768 II. Between Objective and Subjective Measurements Penetrometer Values (Crust Off) vs. Consistency Scores Per Cent Sag (Inverted, Crust 0n) vs. Consistency Scores Per Cent Drainage (Inverted, Crust On) vs. Syneresis Scores III. Between Subjective Measurements Crust Color vs. Crust Flavor Inside Color vs. Inside Flavor Crust Color vs. Inside Color Crust Flavor vs. Inside Flavor Inside Flavor vs. Aroma Texture vs. Syneresis_ Consistency vs. Texture fiv’v‘ v—T W W 7v rrw—r-fi juvvv-r #— r v *V‘wv'wv— f fi" r I—vv- ** Significant at 1% level of probability. * Significant at 5% level of probability. ~0.1795 +0.0857 "O Q 2210 +O.SOO4** +0.4334** +0.8082** +0.5649** +0.3592* +0.8559** +0.3070* wwv—vv— 'V‘w 75 Correlation coefficients were calculated on the taste panel scores for consistency with both the penetrometer values (crust off) and the per cent sag values to determine the reliability of these latter two tests. The results in- dicate there were no significant correlations between either of these two objective measurements and the subjective evalu- ation for firmness. Panel scores for syneresis were not cor- related with per cent drainage values, indicating this ob- jective measurement was not reliable. The results for firm— ness obtained with the penetrometer in this experiment are in accord with those of Bittner (3) and MacDougall (40), both of whom indicated that the curd tension meter is a better objective measure of gel strength than the pens. trometer. Table 19, part III shows comparisons between several of the factors scored by the taste panel. There were highly significant positive correlations between the following pairs of judges' scores: crust color and crust flavor, in- side color and inside flavor, crust color and inside color, crust flavor and inside flavor. A "halo effect" of color impression on flavor judgment may be operating part of the time. The inside flavor and aroma scores were positively correlated at the 5% level of probability. The highly sig- nificant positive correlation existing between texture and syneresis scores indicates the texture scores increased as the syneresis scores increased, inferring that with a better 76 texture (fewer holes) there was a smaller amount of drainage in the baked custards. The judges' scores for consistency and texture gave a positive correlation significant at the 5% level of probability. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The purpose of this investigation was to study the ef- fect of the addition of the nine possible combinations of three different peach colors and three levels of peach fla- vor on the palatability and gel structure of standard baked custard. The basic eXperimental formula consisted of constant proportions of reconstituted dried whole milk, fresh whole eSB. and sucrose. Each of the nine treatment variables con- .tained one of three peach colors a designated light, medium. and dark color - and one of three levels of peach flavor - designated low, medium, and high flavor level. A control custard having neither added color nor added flavor was used as'a reference for the objective tests. Six replications of each of the nine treatment variables were prepared and evaluated by subjective and objective meth- ods. Preparation of the custard mix, additionof the celors and flavors, and baking procedure were standardized as much as possible. All samples were cooled to room temperature before objective and subjective evaluations. Room temperature and relative humidity of the labora- tory during preparation and testing were recorded. Time- temperature relationships during baking were recorded on a Brown Electronik Potentiometer High Speed Multiple Point Recorder. 77 78 The palatability of the baked custards was evaluated subjectively by a taste panel of 7 persons (4 women and 3 men) on nine characteristics: crust color, crust tenderness, crust flavor, inside color, aroma, inside flavor, consist- ency, texture, and syneresis. In rating the samples, a 7. point scale was used in which 7 was the highest possible score. Objective measurements included pH of the custard before and after baking, gel strength as indicated by the penetrometer (crust on, crust off) and per cent sag, and syneresis as indicated by per cent drainage. Analysis of variance on the subjective scores for the nine treatment variables indicated there were no significant differences in six of the characteristics: crust tenderness, aroma, inside flavor, consistency, texture, and syneresis. Therefore it may be concluded that for these characteristics any of the nine variable treatments would be equally accept- able to the taste panel used in this experiment. Significant differences among the nine treatment vari— ables were found in the subjective scores for crust color (1% level of probability), inside color (1% level of probes bility), and crust flavor (5% level of probability). For crust color and inside color the light color custards as a group scored the lowest, and the medium color custards as a group scored the highest with the exception of custard G which received the highest mean score for inside color. This high inside color score for custard G indicates some 79 of the taste panel members did not object to the dark inside color of the custard but they did object to the dark crust color. This reinforces the observation that the added color concentrates in the crust, making the crust darker in color than the inside portion of the custard. For crust flavor, custard F (medium color, high flavor) scored the highest, followed by custards G and D, both con- taining the low flavor level and dark and medium colors, rev spectively. This may indicate influence of the "halo effect" of crust color impression on the judges' scores for crust flavor. Since the flavor seems to concentrate in the crust, it would not be unreasonable to find the low level of flavor :‘rating high. In considering the three subjective character- istics which were significantly different, it appears that custard F (medium color, high flavor) was scored highest by this taste panel. For inside flavor, custard I (dark color, high flavor) scored the highest, although the variable treatments were not significantly different. The medium color and high fla~ vor level custard (custard F) scored second highest and cus- tard G (dark color, low flavor) scored third. It should be noted the custard with high flavor level but low color (cus- tard C) scored the lowest. As a group, the custards con- taining the dark color received the highest score for inside flavor. The evidence again seems to indicate the ”halo ef- fect'of color impression on flavor judgment. 80 it is evident the inside flavor scores were consistently lower than the crust flavor scores, reinforcing the observa~ tion that the flavor, as well as color, concentrated in the crust of the custard. The results of the color and flavor scores indicate there were more differences in the color characteristics than in the flavor characteristics of the nine treatment variables. Analyses of variance of the objective measurements ins dicated no significant difference in the nine variable treat- ments for gel strength and per cent drainage. High standard deviations for all of the objective measurements raise the question of the reliability and suitability of these objec- tive evaluation methods for baked custards. There was no significant correlation between penetrome- ter values (crust off) and per cent sag values, and no sig- (nificant correlations between subjective and objective mea- surements for gel strength or for syneresis. These results may indicate the unreliability of the objective methods used. Highly significant positive correlations were found be- tween several selected subjective measurements. The highly significant positive correlations between crust color and crust flavor and inside color and inside flavor indicate the "halo effect" of color impression on flavor judgment. Highly significant positive correlations were also found 81 for crust color versus inside color, crust flavor versus in, side flavor, and texture versus syneresis. Positive correla- tlons at the 5% level of probability were found for inside flavor versus aroma and consistency versus texture. 10. ll. 12. 13. LITERATURE CITED Baten, W.D. Reaction of age groups to organoleptic tests. Food Tech. fig277, 1950. Birren, F. Color and human appetite. Food Tech. 11: 553. 1903- Bittner, B.A. The effect of various milks on the timer temperature relationships in baking custard, and on the quality of the product. Unpublished M. S. thesis. Michigan State College. 1950. ‘ Brice, B.A. Color in Foods. Symposium. Nat'l. Acad. Sci.sNat'l Res. Council. 1954. Buck, R.E. and Sparks, R.A. Relation of ketchup color to tomato color as determined by the Hunter instru- ment. Food Tech.‘§:122, 1952. Carr, R.E, and Trout, G.M. Some cooking qualities of homogenized milk. I. Baked and soft custard. Food Research,1:360, 1942. Gaul, J.F. The profile method of flavor analysis. Advances in Food Research. Vol. 7. Edited Mrak, E.M. and Stewart, G.F., New York, Academic Press Inc., 1957, Chick. H. and Martin, C.J. On the "heat coagulation" of proteins. Part II. The action of hot water upon egg albumen and the influence of acids and salts upon .reaction velocity. J. Physiol. flzcl, 1911. Chick, H. and Martin, C.J. On the "heat coagulation” of proteins. Part III. The influence of alkali upon the reaction velocity. J. Physiol. 3:;61, 1912. Code of Federal Regulations. Title 21 - Foods and Drugs. 'Parts 8 and 9. Washington, D.C. Office of the Federal Register, National Archives and Records Service, U.S. Gov't. Printing Office, 1965. Cook, H.L. and Husseman. D.L. Consumer acceptance of dry milks in quantity cookery. Wisc. Agr. Expt. Sta. Research Bulletin 164, 1950. Cracker, E.C. Flavor. New York, McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1945. Crocker, E.C. Measuring food flavors. Food Research g;273. 1937. 82 14. 15. 16. 17. 180 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 27. 28. (D ,3, Cracker, E.C. and Platt, w. Food flavors - a critical review of recent literature. Food Research,23183, 1937. . Dawson, E.H., Brogdon, J.L., and McManus, S. Sensory testing of differences in taste.. II. Selection of panel members. Food Tech. 11:1251. 1963. Di Geneva, J. Information on flavors (Private communi- cation), New York, Givaudan Flavors, Inc.. 1964. Duncan, D.B. Multiple range and multiple F tests. Biometrics 11:1, 1955. Esau, P. Procedures for conversion of color data from one system into another. Food Tech.,l23167, 1958. Fruton, J.S. and Simmonds, 8. General biochemistry. 2nd ed. New York, John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1958. Griswold, R. The experimental study of foods. Boston, Houghton Mifflin Company, 1962., Holman, V. and Carver. J.S. The yolk color index. U.S. Egg and Pltry. Mag.,fil(no. 8):40, 1935. Huggart, H.L. and Wenzel, F.W. Color differences of citrus juices and concentrates using the Hunter Color Difference Meter.. Food Tech. 2c27. 1955. Ingle, J.D. Information on flavors (Private communi- cations). Chicago, Illinois, Food Materials Corporaw tion. 1963, 196 0 Jablonski, C.F. Coloring matters in foods. The Chem- istry and Technology of Food and Food Products. Vol. I. 2nd ed. Edited Jacobs, M.B., New York, Inter- scisnce Publishers, Inc., 1951, Janovsky, H.L. Flavomatics in food. Food Tech.,2: Jordan, R., Wagner, E.S., and Hollander, H.A. Non~ homogenized vs. homogenized milk in baked custards. J. Am. Dietet. A. 2931126, 1954. Katz, A. Highlights in newly developed flavoring aromatics. Food Tech.,2:636, 1955. Kefford, J.F. Objective measurement of colour. C.S. I.R.O. Food Pres. Quarterly 22(no. l):8, 1963. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 32+ Kessler. H.P. Information on flavors (Private communi- cation). New York, Givaudan Flavors Inc.. 1963. Kramer, A., Murphy, E.F., Briant, A.M., Wang, M., and Kirkpatrick, M.E. Studies in taste panel methodology. J. Agr. Food Chem. 2;224, 1961. Krum. J.K. Truest evaluations in sensory panel testing. Food Eng.,g1:74, 1955. Laird, D.A. and Breen, W.J. Sex and age alterations in taste preferences. J. Am. Dietet. A. 1§;549, 1939. Langwill, K. Taste perceptions and taste preferences of the consumer. Food Tech. fijl36, 1949. Leonard, S.J., Luh, E.S., Chiohester, 0.0., and Simone, M. Relationship of fresh clingstone peach color to coéor and grade after canning. Food Tech.,1fi:492. l9 1. Logue, L.E. Some qualities of eggs affecting the gel strength of custards. Unpublished M.S. thesis. Iowa State College. 1940. Longree, K. Color determinations for baked custards (Private communication). Institutional Management Dept. Cornell University. 1963. Longree, K., Jooste, M., and White, J. Time—temperature relationships of custards made with whole egg solids. III. Baked in large batches. J. Am. Dietet. A. 28; 147. 1961. Lowe, B. Experimental cookery. 4th ed. New York, John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1955. Lowe, F. and Stewart, C.F. Subjective and objective tests as food research tools, Food Tech.,1:30, 1947. MacDougall, M.J, Cooking qualities of several concen- trations of various types of nonfat dried milk solids. Unpublished M.S. thesis. Michigan State College. 1953. Mackey, A.C. and Valassi. K. The discernment of primary tastes in the presence of different food textures. Food Tech» 19t238, 1956. MacKinney, G. and Little, A. Color of foods. West- port, Conn., AVI Publishing Co., Inc., 1962. 430 44. 46. 47. 48. 49, 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 85 Mason, D.D. and Koch, E.J. Some problems in the design and statistical analysis of taste tests. Biometrics Mastic, M.E. The effect of homogenization on the gela- tion and palatability of baked custards prepared with dried whole egg solids. Unpublished M.S. thesis. Michigan State University. 1959. McBride, B.M. Practicality of using dried whole a g in custards for institutional food service. Unpubl shed M.S. thesis. Iowa State College. 1947. ‘ VMerory, J. Food flavorings - composition, manufacture, in% use. Westport, Conn., AVI Publishing Co.. Inc., 9 O. Merwin, E.J. The peach v another stone fruit. The Givaudan Flavorist. No. 4, 1965. Meyer, L. Food chemistry. New York, Reinhold Pub- lishing Corp., 1960. . '- Miller, G.A., Jones, B.M., and Aldrich, P.J. A compari» son of the gelation preperties and palatability of shell eggs, frozen whole eggs, and whole egg solids in standard baked custard. Food Research gg;584, 1959. Negler, L. Acceptabllity of baked custards made with increased amounts of dry milk solids. Special problem FM 405. Foods and Nutrition Dept. Michigan State University. 1957. Mason, E.H. Introduction to experimental cookery. New York, McGraw-Hill Book Company, Inc., 1939. Nickerson, D. Color measurement and its application to the grading of agricultural products. USDA Misc. Publ. 580. March, 1946. Pangborn, R.M. Taste interrelationships. Food Research g§;245, 1960. Pettit, L.A. Informational bias in flavor preference testing. Food Tech” 12:12, 1958. Power, F.B. and Chestnut, V.K. The odorous constitu- ents of peaches. J. Am. Chem. Soc. 42;l725, 1921. Robinson, W.B., Wishnetsky, T., Ransford, J.R., Clark, W.L. and Hand, D.B, A study of methods for the measurement of tomato Juice color. Food Tech. g: 64. 86 Schutz, H.G. Color in Foods. Symposium. Nat'l. Acad. Sci.-Nat'1. Res. Council. 1954. Smith, B.H. Modern trends in flavors. Food.Research Valdes, R.M., S mone, M. J. and Hinreiner, E. H. Effect of sucrose an organic acids on apparent flavor inv tensity. II. Fruit nectars. Food Tech.,1gz387, 1956. Warner-Jenkinson Manufacturing Co. Certified Food Colors. St. Louis, Mo. 1961. Weiser, H. B. Colloid chemistry. 2nd ed. New York, John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1949, Wilson, D.E., Meyer, J. C., Robinson, W.B., and Hand, D.B. Objective evaluation of color and consistency in peach puree, Food Tech. ,_1:479. 1957. Yeatmen, JoNo. Sidwell, AOPQ and Norris, KeHo Dfirlvaw tion of a new formula for computing raw tomato juice color from objective color measurement. Food Tech. 14:16,1960, Zabik, M.E. The effect of sucrose and calcium cyolamate upon the gel strength of selected gelling agents in the preparation of jellied custard, Unpublished M. S. thesis, Michigan State University. 1961. APPENDIX 88 Table 20. Colors and Flavors Used in Baked Custards for Objective Tests. "' V mfir'rva-v'w w v— v fi-‘v W‘l‘r'r‘v—yi v Wyr—rfiy rv w—y -w~y7~*vw ‘- rw ,— W T—v _, ,-...—_..—7_.~» v (Quantity for 1100 c. c. custard mix) Color Color Color Solution Solution Solution Flavgr Flavor Flavgr 2 ~ 3 Variable 2 W "T“m ml m TW“"'"'“5m ' m ' m A‘ 1.00 _- - 0.55 0.28 0.11 B 1.00 - - 1.10 0.55 0.22 C. 1.00 — - 1.65 0.83 0.33 D - 1.00 . - 0.55 0.28 0.11 E - 1.00 - 1.10 0.55 0.22 F - 1.00 - 1.65 0.83 0.33 0 ~ - 1,00 0.55 0.28 0.11 H - ‘. - 1.00 1.10 0.55 0.22 I - . - 1.00 1.65 0.83 0.33 *7 '7 rv r‘v— *- 7 WV w—w; r r “VT 7, v~ F—vw .v—V v V'V'i v .1 1 See page 91 for composition of color solutions. 2 Imitation Peach Extract No. 7442, Food Materials Corpora-. tion, Chicago 18, Illinois. 3 'Imitation Peach Flavor with True Fruit F-4710, Givaudan Flavors Inc., New York 56, New York. 4 Flex-Sol Imitation Peach Concentrate F-4711, leaudan Flavors Inc., New York 36, New York. Table 21. 89 Colors and Flavors Used in Baked Custards for Subjective Tests. *‘r‘ rw w.‘ vv-v-uvw'r va .V—Vfi—v- 7' 7r Yr v .—v v. . r—w .17 .1 1*...— TV Frw ,71 r, .— (Quantity for 1500 c. c. custard mix) Color Color Color Vari b1 _c: c: rm >- #4 :2 Q a: DJ Solution Solution Solution '2 m 1.36 1.36 1.36 m p 1036 1.36 1.56 Flaygr Flavgr Flavor “w m 1.36 1.56 1.36 WV m 0.75 1.50 2.24 0.75 1.50 2.24 0.75 1.50 2.24 m, 0.38 0.75 1.13 0.38 0.75 1.13 0.38 0.75 1.13 m 0.15 0.30 0.45 0.15 0.30 0.45 0.15 0.30 0.45 V'Vv . v—vv fl. 'vv WWW "rvv—vwvv vfi—v v fl r— 7"- 1 See page 91 for composition of color solutions. my — 2 Imitation Peach Extract N0. 7442, Food Materials Corpora- tion, Chicago 18, Illinois. 3 Imitation Peach Flavor with True Fruit F-4710, Givaudan Flavors Inc., New York 56, New York. Flex-Sol Imitation Peach Concentrate F-47ll, Givaudan Flavors Inc., New York 56, New York. Table 22. Description of Flavors Used, w—rwvw W —v www,' V7 V7. 7 WW 90 WT- v _x ‘rv w m "a v'v‘v—‘Y v—v‘w 7r v—y v— z Composition Solvent Solubility pH Imitation Combination of 95% - Completely 6.0a Peach Extract natural oils alcohol soluble in 7.5 No. 7442 such as orange (23) alcohol; some (23) (Food oil and syn. of the mater- Materials thetic flavorq ials not Corporation) ing materials soluble in ‘ (23). water (23). Imitation Blend of alcohol Absolute 4.2 Peach Flavor synthetic and alcohol (16) with True ingredients water and water Fruit F-4710 with concen- ' ' (Givaudan trated peach Flavors Inc.) extract (29). Flex-Sol An emulsion water Not completely 4.1 Imitation of oil soluw soluble in ale (16) Peach ble chemicals cohol due to Concentrate and gum um arabic (Givaudan arabic (29). 29). Soluble Flavors Inc.) in water. rw—r— jT—Ww—u-rww—V‘yl- .r 91 Table 23. Composition of Color Solutions. Color FD&C ,* FD&C Solution‘ Red No. 4 Xeiiot no. 5 5 w" '5" 3”." 5’ 3"" " '3 ’ fifg""’ " "' “53' ‘ ‘" W mgfw " #1 (Light) . 330 220 $2 (Medium) 490 220 #3 (Dark) 730 660 Y‘rfiw—fi'lfi‘ 1 Powdered colors made up to 100 ml. solution with dis- tilled water. pH of all three color solutions was 7.3. 92 Exhibit 1. Procedure for Preparation of Colored and Flavored Custard Mixes. P e ar t f u*d m 1k: 1. Remove one package of dried whole milk (504 grams) from refrigerator and allow to come to room tempera« ture. 2. Weigh out 3496 grams tap water into aluminum sauce- pan and heat to 46°C. 3. Pour heated water into a 12-quart bowl of a Hobart Mixer1 and add DWM. 4. Mix DWM and water on speed #1 for 30 seconds using the paddle attachment. 5. With a rubber spatula mix the milk by hand to break down any lumps. 6. Mix the milk an additional 30 seconds on speed #1. 7. Place the mixing bowl in a cold water bath to cool the milk rapidly to room temperature. Preparation39§ basic custard gig: 1. Break 15 fresh, sheil eggs into a 5- -quart bowl of a Kitchen Aid Mixer 2. Beat eggs for 3 minutes on speed #2 using a whip attachment and then for 1 minute on speed #4, 3. Weigh out 748 grams of blended egg into a 5-quart Kitchen Aid mixing bowl. 4. Add 391 grams of sugar to egg and blend egg-sugar . mixture for 1 minute on speed #4 using the paddle attachment 5. Pour egg-sugar mixture into a 12-quart Hobart mixing bowl. 6. Weigh out 3815 grams of the reconstituted whole milk and add to the egg-sugar mixture. v’v‘ (W‘v vr-v '—-v-~——r—1 1 Hobart model A-QOO mixer, The Hobart Mfg. 00., Troy, Ohio. 2 Kitchen Aid K5-A mixer, The Hobart Mfg. 00., Troy, Ohio. 93 7. Mix the egg-sugar-milk mixture on speed #1 for 5 minutes using the paddle attachment. 8. Strain custard mix through a medium-fine wire-mesh household strainer to remove any undissolved parti- else. 9. Divide custard mix into smaller portions in Sequart stainless steel bowls. Four portions of 100 c. c. each for objective tests; three portions of 1500 c.c. each for subjective tests. Addition of 2910; and flavors: 1. Add color solution to custard mix using a 1 m1. pipette. 2. Add flavors, one at a time, to custard mix using 1 m1. pipettes. 3. Mix the added color and flavors into the custard mix using a Kitchen Aid K5~A mixer with the paddle attachment on speed #1 for 2 minutes. 94 Exhibit 2. General Instructions for Peach Custard Taste Panel Members. 1. Please do not eat, smoke, or chew gum for 1/2 hour prior to the time of tasting. 2. Do not give any reactions, such as grimace, smile, or vocal expression as you evaluate the sample. 3. You will receive three custards, one at a time, to judge each day. All samples will be at room temperature. You are to evaluate one custard at a time and complete your scoring on that custard before another one is presented to you. There will be a separate score sheet for each custard. Since this is a color and flavor problem you will be receiving custards of different color and differ- .ent levels of flavor. Remember to score each sample independently of the others. 4. Judge the nine factors in the order in which they are listed on the_score sheet. Place a mark (X), using a red pencil, in the bIOck which most nearly fits your evaluation rating of each sample. Be sure to score each of the factors listed on the score sheet and to mark (X) the appropriate descriptive term(s) for each factor you rate 4 92 below. 5. Between each sample eat some unsalted cracker and take a drink of the water provided. ' 6. This sheet will be given to you each day you score to remind you of the definitions and instructions below. ************* PALATABILITY FACTORS: DEFINITIONS AND INSTRUCTIONS Crust Color. Evaluate the crust color of the custard as it appears n the custard cup. The crust color will always be more intense than the inside color. Crust Tenderness. With your spoon, break through the crust in t e center 0 the custard and evaluate the tenderness of a piece of the center crust. Crust Flayg . Evaluate the flavor of a piece of crust taken 'rom he center to the outside of the custard cup. Inside Color. Spoon some of the custard onto the plate pro- videdland'evaluate the inside color. 95 Aroma. Evaluate aroma on the basis of a sniff obtained from the custard remaining in the custard cup. Inside Flavor. Taste some of the custard in the cup (without crust) and evaluate the flavor. Consistency. Spoon some of the custard from the cup onto the p ate, cut through it with the edge of the spoon, and make an evaluation. The custard should hold its shape when spooned out but not make a brittle break when out. exture. Look at the custard in the cup, at the bottom and around the sides. There should be no holes present. 'Taste the custard to determine smoothness. Synegesis. Look at the custard remaining in the cup and on your p ate. There should be little or no separation of the liquid from the gel structure. CHECK THE SCORE SHEET TO MAKE SURE N0 FACTORS HAVE BEEN OMITTED AND THE APPROPRIATE DESCRIPTIVE TERM(S) FOR EACH FACTOR SCORED 4 OR BELOW HAVE BEEN MARKED (x) 96 Exhibit 3. SCORE SHEET FOR PEACH CUSTARD Judge Code no. Date Score Key: 7 - Excellent Instructions: In the appropriate columns, 6 - Very good place a mark (X) for the score which best 5 - Good expresses your evaluation of that factor. 4 — Medium For those factors scored 4 or below, mark 3 - Fair (X) the descriptive terms which best describe 2 - Poor the sample. 1 - Very poor PALATABILITY 5 SCORE VALUES FACTORS 7 6 5 4 3 2. 1 DESCRIPTIVE TERMS _Unappetizing _Too red CRUST COLOR _Too dark _Too yellow _Too pale _Other CRUST T ENDERN ESS _T ough _Rubbe ry _Too strong _Bitter CRUST FLAVOR _Too weak _Artificial _Other _Too dark _Too red INSIDE COLOR _Too pale _Too yellow _Other _Too strong _Perfumy AROMA _Too weak _Other _Too sweet _Bitter INSIDE FLAVOR _Not sweet enough—Artificial _Too strong _Eggy _Too weak _Other ___Too firm _Rubbery CONSISTENCY _Not firm enough _Brittle _Other _Not smooth _Many holes TEXTURE _Several holes _Slight syneresis SYNERESIS _Pronounced syneresis 97 Exhibit 4. Sample Calculation of Studentized Multiple Range Test. Based on data for Crust Color General formula for standard deviation of the mean: SE : s3 s2 = variance (error mean square) n n = number of replications Sample (see Table 2, page 47): 31 :JC.‘ 927): :: 0.125 Studentized Multiple Range Values (1% level of probability)‘: LLLLLLLL 3.82 3.99 4.10 4.17 4.24 4.30 4.34 4.37 Shortest Significant RangesQ: L21 Elli). ii). 1.6.111). 1.3). 1.2). .48 .50 (.51 .52 .53 .54 .54 .55 Crust Color Mean Scores: Treatment variable: C B H I A G E D F Mean Score: $.68 3.69 4.61 4.81 4.92 5.10 5.22 5.22_§,6O -v-V' Any two means not underscored by the same line are sig- nificantly different. Any two means underscored by the same line are not significantly different (17). Y—v Duncan (1?). 2 Shortest Significant Ranges = Studentized Multiple Range Values x 8x. If the difference between any two scores exceeds the shortest significant range value, those scores are significantly different. d 98 Conclusions: Treatment variable F scored significantly higher than treatment variables A, I, H, B, and C. Treatment variables D and E scored significantly higher than treatment variables H, B, and C. Treatment variables G, A, I, and H scored significantly .higher than treatment variables B and C.