113 069 THS ' IIIIIIIII IIIIIIII IIIIII'V 300643 6426 IBRARY l MacinmnSqm ”I: “I ....._-~-« :22? PLACE N RETURN BOX to roman this checkout tram you! record. TO AVOID FINES Mum on or baton duo due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE ‘ ., d‘ , ' " g I I'- ' , ‘ u... v—o-r ~--— . .l_ >5 ' ‘ a \_ fl PWLD RELATIONSHIPS AND SCHOOL AGIIEVEMENT (A Study of High and Low'Adhieving Eighth Grade Pupils of Superior Ability) E CHARLOTTE R. JOLLY A WORLD! amitted to lflshigan State University 1n.Partia1 Fulfillmant of the Requirements flor’tho Degree of MASTEROF ARTS quartma t of’Hnme NanagamantIand Child.Devdlqpnent 1963 dri‘ on; It" IY( AC KN OW LEDGI‘JIE NT The author wishes to express her deep appreciation to the people who helped to make possible the completion of this study: To Mr. Richard Fransted, Principal of Eaton Rapids High School, for his willing cooperation in making avail- able, not only the subjects for the study, but also re- cords of their school history and the class time necessary for the administration of the instrument. To Dr. William R. Morrow, Research Coordinator for the State Hospital No. l, Fulton, Missouri, for generous permission to use his instrument. To Dr. Bernice Borgman and Mrs. Diane Hanson, mem- bers of the graduate committee, and to Dr. Alice Thorpe, Chairman of the Department of Home Management and Child Development, for their interest, assistance, and time. To Dr. Robert L. Green, College of Education, for his statistical assistance, continued encouragement, and genuine interest. To Dr. Martha E. Dale, Major Professor, for her motivating guidance, discerning judgment, and willing- ness to give of herself. And especially, to the fellow students who read critically, listened sympathetically, and helped immeas- urably. a}: :? €3- .‘o op TABLE OF CONTENTS ACImOWIOdgments O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 O O O 0 Table Of Contents 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O LiSt 0f Tables 0 O O, O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 chapter I. II. III. IntrOduCtion O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Family Relationship's Influence on Child's Development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Review of the Literature . . . . . . . . . Design and Methodology . . . . . . Administering the Test . . . . Selection of Subjects . . . . . . Data Processing . . . . . . . . The Adolescent' s Perception of His Parents The Parent-Child Relationship and School Achievement . . . . . . . . . Value of Studying Adolescent's Perception. Problem . . . . . . . . . Selection of the Problem . Statement of the Problem . Basic Assumptions . . . . Hypothesis . . . . . . . . The Instrument ... . .. . Description of the Scales o o e 0' e 0 O O O O o O ' o e o o e o o e O o e O 0 o O o 0 0 O o e o 0 Findings 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Interpretation of Results . . . . . . . Parental Approval . . . . . . . . . . . Parents' Sympathetic Encouragement of Achievement . . . . . . . . . Sharing Ideas and Confidences . . . . . Lack of Parental Restrictiveness. . . Lack of Over-Insistence on Achievement Lack of Severe Parental Discipline . Parental Trust in Child . . . . . . Overall Family Morale Rating . . . iii page 11 iii chapter IV. Summary and Conclusions . . . . . . . . . Sample . . . . . . Data Collection . . Analysis . . . . . . Results . . . . . Limitations of Study . . Regarding the Sample . Regarding the Instrument Conclusions . . . . ReIzarding the Use of the Instrument Regarding Adolescents' Perception of Parent-Child Relationship . . . . . Suggestions for Further Study . . . . . ...“... 000000. 0...... 000000. 00000000. BIBLIOGRAPHY O O O O O O O C C O O O O O O C O O 0 APPENDIX A O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Introduction of Instrument to Subjects . The Family Relations Scale . . . . . . . APPENDIX B . I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Additional Tables . . . . . . . . . . . iv LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Grade Point Cut-Off for High and Low Groups . . 26 2. Median Intelligence Quotients, Grade Point Averages, and Ages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 3. Ranges in Intelligence Quotient, Age, and Grade P01nt Average 0 o e e o e e o e e e e o ' 56 h. Fathers' Occupations . . . . . . . . . . . . . 57 5. Results of t-test for High and Low Achievers . 58 6. Subjects According to Age . . . . . . . . .. . S9 7. Subjects According to Intelligence Quotient . . 6O INTRCDUCTILN Family relationships' Influence on Child's DevelOpment The family is one of the most important contributors to a child's develOpment. The child is born into a world of peeple. Phat he does affects others and what others do affects him. The family is the first part of the world that the child encounters. Later groups such as the schools, churches, neighbors, and children in the child'senvironment influence his develOpment but the family influences often set the stage for the child's encountering other segments of his environment. The family is the keynoter in the child's deveIOpment, Specifically in the development of attitides and as a source of eXperiences for the child. Bacmeister emphasized, "the theme that it (the family) sets is likely to persist as background music throughout life."1 Glueck2 contends that parental attitudes have great significance for the destiny of the individual child. He also believes that the mental context of the family does not necessarily assure an intelligent and healthy management of the parent-child relationship. 1R. Bacmeister, "The Family Comes First," Childhood Education, XXV, September 1948, pp. 8-11. 2B. Glueck, "The Significance of Parental Attitude for the Destiny of the Individual," Mental Hygiene, XXII, October, 1928, p. 723. -2- He holds that the important curriculum of the family depends for its success, much more than does the formal school or the classroom, upon atmosphere, upon the subtle and intangible forces, which are implicit in the situations created by the human beings who compose it. The vital importance of the home for the child's psychological health is evidenced by the widespread interest in effects of parental attitudes on child development found in publications written for parents and educators. There seems to be an agreement among many authorities that the family exerts a tremendous influence upon child develOpment. The agreement seems to be expressed in the state- ment of Faegre and Anderson: Granting the extent to which the responsibility for some types of training has been shifted to the school, the home still offers the earliest and in many respects the most _ thorough education which the child receives . . .We have seen that the personality of the child is emerging among all the influences of the early environment and is being shaped by them, and that the family represents the world of the child in which, long before he reaches school age, he has been meet- ing situations and develOping ways of reacting to them. Be- cause he is more frequently and more profoundly moved or stim- ulated by persons than by inanimate parts of his environment, the home with its close associations with a number of person- alities, becomes the field in which the child tests out and 3 comes to appreciate the values of certain types of behavior. HawkegIreports a project of the Agricultural Experiment Station of Iowa State College. This project has as its objective the problem of determining factors within the family environment which influence personal and social deveIOpment. These factors, explains Hawkes, are 3M. L. Faegre and J. E. Anderson, Child Care and Training, hth Edition, Revised. (Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, l9h7), p. 279. a G. R. Hawkes, "Family Influences on Personality," J. of Home Economics, XLIV, 1952, pp. 767-769. -3- even more influential factors than those of economic conditions and physical environment, and Operate to influence the develOpment of children living in the family. He also states, "The factors are certainly the subtle and more difficult to measure psychological factors of the interpersonal environment . . ."5 The c00perating workers on the project reported by Hawkes accepted as a theoretical core: "Whatever each individual sees, hears, and reacts to is the world or the environment which is real to the individual . . . For each, however, there is a highly unique interpretation which we can understand only if we see through his eyes as it were. To understand what a relationship means to an in- dividual, we must know what he sees there." REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE The Adolescent's Perception of His Parents With few exceptions, behavioral scientists who treat the parent- child relationship are preoccupied with the attitudes of the parent toward the adolescent. It has been noted, however, that children, and particularly adolescents, form attitudes toward parents which are not necessarily a reflection of parental attitudes toward them. The critical evaluation of parents by adolescents can be ex- pected to occur in any heterogeneous, Open class society; that is, any society in which there is considerable diversity in family be- 5Ibid. -h- havior patterns and in which achieved status predominates.6 In a social milieu of this type, ample Opportunity is afforded for the comparison Of parents with those Of one's own peer group, or to some extent, with the other actual or idealized parents. The motivation for evaluation lies in the crucial role that the parent continues to play in the life of the adolescent. Par- ents meet some adolescent needs directly through food, clothing, spending money, family recreation, affection, and enforcement of family and societal rules for his safety and welfare. Parents are perhaps even more crucial in helping or hindering the adolescent's adjustment to his peer group, school, and community. They initially determine his socio-economic status, equip him with a set of atti- tudes and skills for interaction outside the family and further af- fect his relationships by their own interaction with his friends, school, and community. Coincidental with this evaluative aspect of adolescence is an extended period of role-change during which the child passes from a dependent to an independent relationship with the parent. Insofar as parent-child adjustment is concerned this involves the relinquish- ment of parental power and prerogatives and the assumption Of duties and responsibilities by the adolescent. Since neither the exact timing Of these changes nor their exact nature are clearly defined in contemporary American society, some friction is probably inevi- table. The evaluative process is affected by such friction but its 6E. B. Renter, "The Sociology of Adolescence," Amer. J. Of Sociology, 43 (November, 1937), p. hlk. -5- outcome is dependent on much more fundamental and extensive phenom- ena: that is, the total personality, status, and social relation- ships Of parents, together with the develOpmental status Of a given adolescent, as these impinge on any part of the adolescent's world. The result of the evaluative process is the placement of the parent on both specific and general attitude continua; that is, the adolescent develops a generalized attitude of acceptance or rejection, but he also can and does place the parent on gener- osity, strictness, or other continua. Since behavior patterns which stimulate the formation of attitudes exist on continua, we may infer that the attitudes formed would also be found on continua, rather than falling into discrete categories. We cannot divide par— ents into selfish and unselfish parents on the basis of one group that is completely selfless and one that has no generous impulse or attitude. Rather, some are more generOus or more often generous than others. We may for convenience of analysis divide them into two, three, or more categories but the differences between these categories are differences Of degree rather than kind. In the same sense we may divide general attitudes toward parents into "reject- ing," but these categories will contain a range of attitudes within each Category, and the dividing line is placed for convenience of an- 7 alysis rather than to separate discrete phenomena. 7F. Ivan Nye, Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior, (John Wiley and Sons, 1958), p. 71. -6- The Parent-Child Relationship and School Achievement Du Pont de Nemours stated in 1800 in his "National Educa- tion in the United States" that "a single day of an educated man of genius is of more value to the world than the labor of 100,000 men for a year." Many educators have struggled to help our superior pupils so as to reduce the lag that so often exists between promise and performance. W. D. Lewis9 is Of the Opinion that our information relative tO superior children is deficient in that we know far too little about those who are designated as superior by standardized intelligence tests but who are not making effective use of their superior ability, and as a result, Often are never recognized as superior. Horace B. English,10 in his Dynamics Of Child DevelOpment, states: Deprive a child Of favorable environmental conditions-- of education broadly conceived, or of intellectual stimula- tion--and his intelligence is apt to be stunted. More subtle but perhaps even more important, however, are the emotional and motivational factors. Du Pont de Nemours, "National Education in the United States," 1800, as in John W. Musselman, J. Experimental Education, 1942, 11:53. 9W. D. Lewis, "A Comparative Study Of the Personalities, In- terests, and Home Backgrounds of Gifted Children of Superior and In- ferior Educational Achievement," J. Genetic Psych., 19h1, 59:207-218. lOHorace B. English, Dynamics of Child DevelOpment, (New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1961), p. 298. ‘léu. t. -7- Few studies Of school achievement have Obtained data regard- ing family relations correlates. While these few present conflict- ing findings, most Of the studies have indicated a positive associ-’ ation between student achievement and emotionally supportive home situations. This general finding applies to elementary,11 high school,12 and college students.13 More specifically, parents of high achievers have been found by the above investigators to give their children more praise and 15 approval}!+ to show more interest and understanding, to be closer to their children,16 to make their children feel more family "be- longingness" and identification with parents.17 On the other hand, parents of under-achievers have been reported to be more domineering ..18 , and over-restrictive, and to use more severe and frequent punish- 11B. W. Hattwick and M. Stowell,, "Relationship of Parents' Over-Attentiveness to Children's Work Habits in Kindergarten and the First Six Grades," J. Educa. Res., 1936-37, 30:169-176. 12J. R. Tibbets, "Role of Parent-Child Relations in Achieve- ment of High School Pupils," Dissert. Abstracts, 1955, 15:232. 13D. Harris, "Factors Affecting College Grades: A Review Of the Literature," 1950-37, Psychological Bulletin, 19h0, 37:125-161. 14 G. Ricoid, as in Morrow and Wilson, 1961. (See footnote #24) 15 16B. Kimball, "Case Studies in Educational Failure During Adol- escence," Amer. J. OrthOpsychiatry, 1953, 23:406-Hl5. Tibbets, 10c. cit. l7Tibbets, loc. cit. 18E. S. Jones, "The Probation Student: What He is Like and What we can do About it," J. Educational Research, 1955, 99:93-102. -3- 19 ments which is at the same time less effectual.20 Parents of under-achievers have also been found more likely either to baby their youngsters or to push them excessively21 and to present to their children either low or extremely high demands for achieve- ment.22 Finally, homes of under-achievers are reported to show more tension and more parental disagreement as to standards of behavior expected of their youngsters.23 In a study of bright high school boys done by William R. Morrow and Robert C. Wilson24 it was hypothesized that the report- ed family relations of high achievers, as contrasted with those of low achievers would be characterized by the following: a. more emotionally supportive home environments; and more specifically, b. greater family sharing in recreation, decision-making, and exchange of confidences and ideas; c. greater mutual parent—child affection, acceptance, trust and approval; 19A. M. Conklin, Teach. Coll. Contr. Educ., 1990, #792. Ibid. 21Hattwick and Stowell,, loc. cit. 22Ricord, loc. cit. 23Tibbets, loc. cit. 24W. R. Morrow and R. C. Wilson, "Family Relations of Bright High-Achieving and Under-Achieving High School Boys," Child Develop- ment, 52:502. -9- d. less parental domination, severity and restrictiveness; e. more sympathetic encouragement of achievement but less overinsistence on achievement; f. greater harmony between parents and more regularity of home routines. The sample included two equated groups containing 48 high school boys of superior intelligence (Intelligence quotients of 120 or above). The groups were equated for: grade in school, socio-economic status and intelligence. The two groups differed greatly in grade point average in academic courses. The high achievers maintained an average Of 1.00 to 1.67. (Contrary to common usage, Morrow employed a scale in which 1 was the best grade possible, and 5, the poorest.) The under-achievers' average was 2.57-5.00, 2.75 being regarded as under achievement for superior students. The students' relations to their parents as seen by themselves were evaluated primarily by 16 self- report Family Relations Scales. Each scale consisted of six ques- tions about the student's relations with his parents (or foster par- ents). The scales were presented in consecutive order, but without scale titles or breaks in spacing. Each student was asked to de- scribe his own situation using the following four response categor- 'ies: -10.. Not at all or Almost Never A little or Sometimes Considerably or Often Very much or Very Often Each student was given a score on each scale by summing his scores on six items in the scale. A score was also obtained on the total of all scales, called the index of Overall Family Morale. Following are listed the 16 individual scales included in the Overall Family Morale Scale: 10. ll. 12. 13. 1h. 15. 16. Family Sharing of Recreation Family Sharing of Confidences and Ideas Family Sharing Of Decision-Making Parental Approval Parental Affection Parental Trust Parental Approval of Peer Activities Student Acceptance Of Parental Standards Student Affection and Respect for Parents Lack of Parental Over-restrictiveness Lack of Parental Severity of Discipline Lake of Parental Overprotection Lack of Parental Over—insistence on Achievement Parental Encouragement of Achievement Harmony of Parents Regularity of Home Routine -11.. Morrow and Hilson found that: High achievers more often than under achievers: a. described their families as typically sharing recreation, ideas, and confidences; b. described thir parents as approving and trusting (the areas of sharpest difference between the two groups;) affectionate, encouraging (not pressuring) with respect to achievement and relatively non-restric- tive and non-severe with regard to discipline; c. described themselves as accepting their parents' standards. An equal majority of both groups described their parents as having a relatively harmonious relationship, portrayed their homes as having a fairly regular routine, denied that they were either seriously overprotected or excessively pressured to achieve, and said they felt considerable respect and affection for their parents. In Terman's study of 59 gifted children,25 the most consistently re- ported parent's comment was the fact that from the child's earliest years, the parent had answered all the child's questions fully and honestly. Most of the superior children had been given no formal training at home but had been encouraged to use their own initiative and to go at their own pace. Parents also often reported that current events and international affairs were discussed readily and regularly with their children when an interest was shown. From this and other similar information it would seem apparent that the 25Lewis Terman, The Hygiene 3: the School Child, Boston: Houghton, c1929. .12.. environment mt provide the child with a wide range of problems at a. level of difficulty suited to his achieved ability; and it must pro- vids him with the necessary means to solve the problems. These means, this author feels, can be blighted or nurtured by parent-child rela- tionships. John W. Mlsselman made a stuck26 Of bright (students and their performance and melted it to investigation of two variables: 1) personality and 2) family background. School records were used which included subject who, conduct and e ffort marks, attendance, school problems or deficiencies, aim for life, vocational choice, etc. Ebctensive testing was done in the areas of achievement, personality and home background. For this purpose ' only two aspects of home environment will be discussed. ‘Ihess' are parents]. discipline and study habits. 1116 data on the relationship of achievement ratio to home discipline were acquired from the questionnaire in which the pupils were asked which methods were used at home to discipline them. Out of a total of 297 students, it was discovered that the majority of their parents used the method of de- priving them of pribileges (121). Sixty-seven parents lectured the pupils, and twenty-three parents used a combination of these two methods. Ten parents used whipping, twenty- 26J. W. Messiaen, 3:93. g3. 2713. v. Hat‘hdck and M. stowell, _l_o_p_. gt. -13- -eight did nothing and the remainder employed miscellaneous forms of discipline. The pupils who were deprived of privileges had a lower mean achievement than the pupils who were lectured or punished in any way. Those pupils who received no punishment at all were next in mean achievement ratio. According to this study, and in conflict with Morrow and Wilson's findings, low achievement ratio is associated with being deprived of privileges while high achievement is associ— ated with whipping and lecturing by parents. The same situation was true in the case of boys, girls, and the total group. Hattwick anCSFowe1127 have done significant research on the relationship of parental over-attentiveness to children's work habits and social adjustment. The study was carried on at the request of a school superintendent who asked this question: "Does over-attentive- ness either in the form of "babying" the child, or in the form of exerting too much pressure upon him, influence the child's performance and social adjustments during kindergarten and the first six grades of school?" The research source was the file of cumulative records of the children in a Winnetka, Illinois,Public School. A separate record for each child is prepared by each successive classroom teacher at the end of a year's eXperience with the child. The investigators report that the records are frank, confidential reports for the benefit of 27Hattwick and Stowell, loc. cit. -14- succeeding teachers who will deal with the child, his interests, ac- tivities, problems and home. The authors also state that Winnetka teachers are in unusually close contact with the homes and that they have all had considerable training in approaches to mental health. These records, therefore, are usually reasonably accurate and reliable, especially when one has a series of them by different teachers deal- ing with the same child. Verbatim teacher evaluations of the students were taken from the records in order to categorize students in the three groups: Child "Babied," Child Pushed, and Home Well-Adjusted. In classifiCation of poor work habits, for every descriptive category, examples were far more numerous for children from over- attentive than for children from well-adjusted homes. The second phase of the analysis reported that the work habits of children pushed and children babied were predominantly poor. On the other hand, the work habits of the children from well-adjusted homes were predominantly good. The authors note that there is, however, a tendency for poor work habits to increase in the case of children babied and to decrease in the case of children from well-adjusted homes. Another point of interest discovered by the authors is the similarity in the work habits of children pushed and babied. Apparently over-attentiveness makes for poor work habits, regardless of the manner in which it is expressed, say Hattwick and Stowell.28 These findings held throughout the grades investigated. In fact there was a slight tendency for children who were babied to develOp more Ibid. “.15" social difficulties and poorer work habits through the years. file tendency for children from well-adjusted homes was in the opposite dierection. Dr. David M. levy” reports some interesting findings with regard to overprotectiveness, but the nature of his simdy related this characteristic of the parent to its effect on difficulty in spe- cific subject areas and types of learning. In his article, "'Ihe Relationship of Maternal Over-Protection to School Grades and In- tenigenoe Ibsts,' Dr. Levy relates that werprotected children apparently do average work in reading and language work (possibly due to their almost exclusively adult environment)30 but they are as much as bro years (median) retarded in arithmetic and number skills. Dr. Ptvllis Blanchard” mind similar results in her Ibiladelphia studies, for the majority of her students with minor to severe reading difficulties come not from over-prdected homes, but hes rejecting home enviroments. Ham of these children were from excellent educational and culmral backgrounds and had super- ior vocabularies, but nevertheless, their reading difficulties were great. ale concluded however, that as far as children with arith- metic difficulties were concerned (although she had only a small lumber to work with) they came from both over-protective md rejecting homes. 25’David M. Levy, “Relationships of Maternal Overprotection to ' School Grades and Intelligence Tests," American Journal of Orth0psy- 30Ibid. 31131371118 Blanchard as reported in Levy (footnote 29). -16- As an apprOpriate summary comment on the over-protected child's problems, a quotation from Dr. Hugh Miller-Crichton's "The Home Back- ground of the Pupil" is cited: Last and commonest of the teacher's problems is the over- mothered child. To him no effort is worth while. He has found all his life that invalidism, incapacity, assumed shy- ness and diffidence, pay. His self—confidence is nil, for he sees that any effort on his part tends to come between him and the Nirvana of his mother's spoiling. He must be taught self-realization, the need for individual effort, and independence before his work habits and personality will al- low him to develOp and utilize his aften-hidden potential.32 We shall now turn our attention from the overprotective par- ent to home environments in which one or more parent is autocratic, or for various other interrelated reasons, where the atmosphere of the parent-child relationship and the home in general are tense, disturbed, or severely maladjusted. In a study of students on aca- demic probation matched with superior achieving students in an arts 33 and science college, Edward Jones found that more of the poor stu- dents had indicated homes which were tense, disturbed, or often not quiet, where one parent autocratically dominated and where there was little talking over of problems with parents. Taking trips with the father and position in the family constellation did not prove signif- icantly different for the two groups; there was, however, a slight tendency for superior students to be oldest and probationers to be youngest. 32Hugh Miller-Crichton, "Home Bahkground of the Pupil," Mental Hygiene, 1932, 16:23-25. 33 E. S. Jones, loc. cit. -17- 34 Barbara Kimball made a study of prep school boys, all of whom had measured "high level of intelligence" and all were doing failing work. An intensive investigation revealed two strikingly similar characteristics in case studies of the boys: 1) poor father-son relationships and 2) aggressiveness. (One might well wonder at the observation, which of these characteristics is cause and which effect, if such a relationship could exist; the author does not discuss them individually. Definitely the two are quite interrelated and have )35 their bases in the home. The research method used was the sentence completion type technique which the author supports because such a test can be structured to reveal information on certain aspects of attitude. The two hypotheses which were significantly upheld by the research were that these boys (the poor achievers) would reveal diffi- cult father-son relationship and more aggression and guilt feelings than do high achievers. The case study method also yields valuable data in the area of family relationships. Information of this type is provided by Wallach and Ulrich in their illustration of a 15 year old boy with an average I.Q. who was doing failing work in school. They report that Bobby's very complex problems were rooted in the emotional dis- turbances caused by severe personality conflicts within his family. This home milieu seriously hindered his cognitive functioning and until intensive therapy for parents and child was possible, Bobby continued to perform at a level far below his intellectual capacity. 3“Kimball, loc. cit. 35Ibid. -15- 1793.22 93. m Adolescent's Perception 2f. Parent-(11:13.531 Relationship Ausube137 and his associates concluded that the use of child- ren's perceptions of parents' attitudes and behavior as independent variables is predicated upon two assumptions. First, although parent behavior is an objective event in thl. real world, it affects the child‘s ego development only to the extent and in the form which he perceives it. Secondly, perceived parent behavicr is in reality a more direct, relevant and proximate determinant of personality development than the stimulus content to which it refers. -. W mewmfwh “*‘W‘\ is} “9“." ' may“ Aquusions suggest the importance of studying children' s \ captions of parent-child relationships. An understanding of chil- dren's perceptions of parental accceptance, approval, trust, and sup- port is essential to further understanding of the effect of parental be- havior, attitudes and performance of the child. .- .-m_._ __ -- , .. _ " "' "PM H.122; ...;T . - _ - . ‘ ’ "”1"?!“ reg-rm. q _. \.-' 'J: ' "— ,. _. -. ‘fl-‘Skflm _ '3':.’§'.'$:<.' Hm {rosin :— «p... 'rr'a’ "'J M Selection 93; E Problem me problem for this study arose from the following questions: (1) To what degree does parental behavior toward the child function signif- icantly in the emotional development and the academic performance of the child? (2) Do children's perceptions of their parent-child relationship determine, to a great extent, the effect of this relationship? (3) Can ' n 37D. Ausubel, “Perceived Parent Attitudes as Determinants of Chil- dren’s Ego Structure,n Gli1____d_ Develownt, m, September, 1951;, pp.173-183. 6 children's perceptions of this parent-child relationship be measured and would a stun of its relationship to school achievement be useful in further understanding the effects of the parent-child relationship upon the children' 8 total development? Statement 93 _t_hg_ Problem Haw studies reported in the literature have suggested that there is a positive correlation between warm, emotionally supportive home environments and high academic achievement. However, some few, but significant, studies have indicated results of a contrary nature. misa- beth Brews and John rechan38 found that strict, authoritarian, restric- tive parents produced children who were higrf'achievers than were those children reared in more democratic, sharing mmily environments. How- ever, one point of possible significance to the present stuck is the fact that the Drews and Teehan study did not deal with the child's perception of this relationship and the ensuing heme conditions. ' Instead the in- ' vestigators interviewed the mothers of the pupils to determine the family practices and home requirements for the children. Another reason for undertaking the present study is the fact that no stucw the author has found thus far has dealt with the specific age group (eighth graders) involv ed in this investigation. Several studies have included adolescents but there are reasons to believe that by working specifically path early adolescents, one '8 results might yield more pert- inent and useful implications, since it ’ 3BELiisaheth Dream and John E. Teehan, "Parental Attitudes and Aca- demic Achievement," Joumal 2f Clinical chholog, 1957, 13:328-332. -20- is generally acknowledged that adolescence is a critical period in the total develoPment of the child. These two factors, the first being the conflicting evidence available at present, and the second being the dearth of studies dealing with the child's perception of the parent-child relation- ship, and more specifically, the early adolescent's perception--have led this investigator to make the following assumptions and hypothe- sis. Basic Assumptions (1) Eighth grade pupils are capable of communicating some of their perceptions of their parent-child relationships. (2) Morrow's Family Relations Scale measures parent-child relation- ships as viewed by the child. (3) The 1957 revision of the California Test of Mental Maturity is a valid and reliable group intelligence test for eighth grade pupils. (Elementary form: grades h-8) fiypothesis (1) There will be a difference in the high and low achievers' per- ceptions of their parent-child relationships. CHAPTER II DESIGN AND METHODOLOGY The Instrument Selection of the Instrument. The first step in the procedure was to select an apprOpriate instrument. The one chosen for data collection in this study was a questionnaire, the Family Relations §galg, develOped by W. R. Morrow in 1956. He deve10ped this instru-- thent for use in the family relations aspects of the Portland (Oregon) Gifted Child Project which he carried on with R. Wilson and others and reported in a 1961 issue of Child DevelOpment.l After reading this article, permission was received from Dr. Morrow to use his questionnaire in the present study. Dr. Morrow writes that his in- strument was adapted from the "Family Relations Questionnaire" devel- Oped by A. W. Brown and his associates (reported in R. J. Havighurst and Hilda Taba, Adolescent Character and Personality, Wiley, l9h9, chp. 21). Morrow's instrument was develOped for use in grades eight through twelve and was thus scaled with the adolescent pupil in mind, making it apprOpriate for this study.2 Description of the Scales. The questionnaire contained sixteen six-item scales in which the respondent was asked to report for each 1W. R. Morrow and R. C. Wilson, "Family Relations of Bright High- Abhieving and Under-Achieving High School Boys," Child DeveloPment, 32: 501-510. 2See Appendix for c0py of questionnaire. -21- Is “a. -22- item to what extent it applied to his relationship with his parents. Below are the directions given each student, along with the descrip- tion of the four response categories. DIRECTIONS: The following questions are about yourself and your family. Read each question carefully and answer it as it applies to the parents WITH WHOM YOU NOW LIVE, (whether they are your real parents, step-parents, or guardians.) To the right of each question put a check-mark in the col- umn which comes nearest to the truth in your case. If you feel is is NOT AT ALL true or happens ALMOST NEVER, check column marked ALMOST NEVER. If it is a little true or hap- pens SOMETIMES, check column 2. If it is pretty much true or happens very often or almost always, check column #. All six items of each scale appeared in sequence in the question- naires, and the 16 scales followed each other without a break in the questionnaire and of course, without indication of scale titles. The scale titles, given below, indicate what each scale was intended to measure. All titles are stated in a "positive" direction. Items expressing a "negative" situation or attitudes were scored in a di- rection Opposite to that of positively stated items. 1. Family Sharing of Recreation ’2. Parental Approval '3. Parents' Sympathetic Encouragement of Achievement h. Parental Overprotection ‘5. Family Sharing of Ideas and Confidences 6. Parental Affection ‘Indicates scales scored for this study. -23. 4:7. Parental Restrictiveness as to Activities 8. Emily Slaring in Decision-Manna 9. Child Acceptance of Parental Standards «10. Parental Over-Insistence on Achievement ll. Qlild Affection for Parents *12. Severity of Parental Discipline *13. Parental bust in Child 11:. Parental Harlow . A 15. Parental Approval of Peer Activities 16. Orderliness and Regflarity of Home Morrow rated each scale separately, giving each child an indivi- dual score for each of the 16 scales, as well as scoring the total, or overall score. In his use of the instrument, the internal consistency reliability of the 16 six-item scales appeared to be 'suffidently sat- isfactory for purposes of group comparison'3 i.e., all but threeh of the corrected odd-even coefficients were above .70. me 10 ital-5 Overall Family Morale Score yielded a reliability of .97. For the purposes of this study it was decided that only seven scales, those found ‘8 Morrow to yield a'p'at the .01 level or beyond would be scored 3Morrow and Wilson, loo. gig. hScales #1:, #8, #10 had coefficients below .70. 5111s Harmony of Parents Scale (M) was scored only for students with two parents living together. -gh- individually in addition to the overall score. This would mean that seven scales, numbers 2,3,5,7,lO,12, and 13 would be scored indiv- idually and, including the overall score, there would be 8 scores for each pupil. Selection of Subjects Children in the eighth grade were selected for this study. The decision was made to use children at this grade and age level for several reasons. First, children thirteen and fourteen years old, normally classed as eighth graders, usually have deve10ped their reading skills far enough to free them from some of the mechan- ics of reading. Hence, they are able to concentrate on the subject matter. Second, it is commonly believed that children at this age have matured enough to have some degree of reciprocity. They are able, according to Newcomb,6 to perceive some of the relationships between themselves and others who have perceptions of their own. Third, they are in the stage of growth and develOpment commonly re- ferred to as early adolescence. The values of testing this age group have been suggested earlier in this study and have been empha- sized by many investigators. During early adolescence, it seems nor- mal for youngsers to drop their identification with adult society and their acceptance of adult standards and establish a strong iden- 7 tification with their peer groups. Mussen and Conger say the par- 6T. M. Newcomb, Social Psychology, (New York: The Dryden Press, 1950), Pp. 308-3120 ' 7P. H. Mussen and J. J. Conger, Child DeveIOpment and Person- ality, (New York: Harper and Brothers, 1956), p. 324. -25- ental behavior and attitudes may be a matter of imitation of both his peers and parents. The subjects for the study were obtained through the prin- cipal of Eaton Rarids Jr. High School, Mr. Richard Fransted. The subjects were enrolled in eighth grade classes there. Eaton Rap- ids is a small farming and imhgtrial community a few miles south of Lansing, Michigan. The thirty eighth-grade pupils chosen have been classified as having "superior" ability according to California Test of Mental Maturity8 (I.Q. 120 and above). The scores made by these pupils on this test given last year, in the seventh grade, and recorded on their cumulative records were employed for this criterion. After the names of pupils with superior ability had been obtained ffom the school records, the group was divided into two achievement groups. The criterion for achievement level was the grade point average computed from the first semester grades as re- corded on the report cards of the students. The grades were eval- uations of four academic subjects: arithmetic, history, science, and English. The following point scale is employed by the school and was used for delineating the two achievement groups for this - h.0 - 3.0 - 2.0 - 1.0 -0. study: WUOW> High achievers were those whose averages were 5.0 or better while anything below a 3.0 (2.9-0.) was classified as low achieve- 8120 I.Q. is the low level of the "superior" range as defined in the CTMM Manual. (See footnote 9 on the following page.) -26- ment. (See Table 1). TABLE 1 GRADE POINT CUTeOFF FOR HIGH AND Low GROUPS Azueo High Achievers B = 3.0 C=2.0 D = 1.0 Low Achievers F300 Finally there were fitteen pupils selected in each group with a total of 30 pupils in all. Besides being matched for grade in school the groups of 9 The children were also matched for age and intelligence quotient. age range for the total group of 30 pupils extended from 13 years 7 months to 1h years 9 months. The I.Q. scores covered a large range, extending from scores at the lower end of the superior range (121), to very bright or gifted (159). The median I.Q. scores for the high group were : boys-- 131; girls--129; and for the low group, boys--l32; girls--127. The median grade point average for the high group came to 3.0 while the low group achieved a mean of 2.0 9E. Sullivan, w. Clark, R. Tiegs, California Test of Mental Maturity, (Los Angeles: California Test Bureau, c 1957). (CTMM E-57 Manual, p. 2). ' -27- TABLE 2 MEDIAN I.Q.,GRADE POINT AVERAGES, AND AGES High Achievers Low Achievers Variable Boys Girls Boys Girls I.Q. 131 129 132 127 Grade Point 3.0 3.0 2.0 2.0 Age (yrs.no.) 14-0 14-0 13-11 13-11 The groups were not matched for socio-economic status, chief- ly because, using Warner's I.S.C., nearly all pupils would have been classified in his upper-lower or lower-middle class ranges, since. there was very little diversity in the status level of the fathers M“‘M~—.-b . ‘— I -' ..ww'rw. ‘_ -_, . ~— ...: i Ii.\‘|..l.\ Administering the Test The questionnaire was administered to thirty superior eighth grade pupils in one group, during the final class period of the school day. This period is set up as an "exploratory period" and the children had been notified by the principal's office on the morning of the day they were to meet for the test. When the group had assembled the principal, Mr. Fransted, introduced the investigator to the:3tudents, asking for their cooperation in aiding her in some work she was doing at Michigan State University. The invest- igator then introduced the questionnaire to the subjects, instructing them in the procedure required. (See appen- dix for investigator's instructions.) Data Processing Preliminary procedure for analysis of data involved tabulat- ing the seven individual sub-scales and the overall score for each subject in the sample. According to Dr. Morrow's directions, the response categories were weighted so that for "positively" stated questions, the responses were scored from 1 to h, with answer "al- most never" receiving 1 point, "sometimes" receiving 2 points, "often" receiving 3 points, and "very often" receiving 4 points. 0n the items worded "negatively" the scoring was reversed, rating. a response of "almost never" 4, and so on, so that a response, "very. often" received 1 point on these items. ~28- As mentioned earlier in fliestudy the seven sub-scales, numbers 2,3,S,7,lo,12, and 13, were scored individually and an overall or total score on the 96 items was computed. The subscales (hereafter referred to simply as 'scales") which this investigator computed responses for were : #2. Parental. Approval, #3. Sympathetic Encouragement of Achieve- ment, #5. Sharing Ideas and Confidences, #7. Leek of Parental Restrictive- ness, #10. lack of. Over-insistence on Achievement, #12. Severity of Pa- rental Discipline, and #13. Parental bust in Child. After the eight scores were tabulated and recorded for each subject the "t-test' was employed. l’ne "t-test" determined whether or not the mean scores of the two groups «high achievers and low achievers-were significantly different, and it so at what level. this investigation was not designed to determine individual differences, but bump differences as they might be related to levels of achievement. W Si FINDINGS Parental Approval. me t-test of significant differences re- vealed that the difference between the mean scores of high achiev- ers and low achievers was non-significant at the .05 level on scale 2, Parental Approval. (1:: .867.) Parents ' Sygpathetic Encouragement gf Achievement. “me t-test revealed that the difference of the two groups on scale 3,Parents' Sympathetic Encouragement of Achievement, we r simificant beyond . the .001 level (t 3 h.255). no difference was in the predicted direc- tion with high achievers perceiving their parents as more sympathet- icall y chomping «Movement. WEE-33:93 Confidences. Scale 5, Famfly Sharing of Ideas and Confidences , also yielded differences significant beyond the .001 level, with the high achievers perceiving their nannies as sharing more ideas am confidences than low achievers perceived. (t : 6.98). 2.9;; of Parental Restrictiveness. The t-test revealed that the two groups did not score significantly different respenses on the scale 7, Lack of Parental Restrictiveness (t g 1.660). ' 9:95 9; Over-Insisteng 93 Achievement. High achievers and low achievers were rated-with differences in mean scores significant at the .05 level on scale 10, Lack of Over-insistence on Achievement. This result was also in the desired direction, i.e., high achievers reported less over-incitence on achievement from their parents than did low achievers. (t . 2.2M). -30- assuming. Scale 12. Lack of Severe Parental Discipline , failed to yield responses significantly different at the .05 level. (t = .820. 0 Parental M g 21.11.24. me last sub-scale, Parental Trust in the Child, 13, revealedo’Jdiffermce batman the high an! low groups, sig- nificant at the .05 level. his, too, was in the desired direction with the big: achievers perceiving more parental trust in than than the low achievers reported. (t - 2.1m) mmmm. me t-test was many enslaved to test for significant differences in the man group scores on the overall test (96 items.) This overall or total score proved to‘hsve the highest level of significance of all, except for the scale dealing with sharing of ideas and cmfidences. has, the Overall Family Morale Scores of the high adlievers were significantly higher (at .001 level) than.the sane scores of the low achievers. (t - 6.757) Mn reta 9!. Elysee-9. Scale 2, Parental Approval of mild, proved to yield non- signii‘ieant differences in the mean scores of the big: and lot achieving groups. ’Ihe high achievers reported slightly more approv- ing parents than did the low achievers, but both groups scored high enough on this scale that there was no significant difference at the .05 level. Scale 3 dealt with the parents' attitude toward the child's academic performance and the type of nphasis they placed on high achievement. The results revealed that parents who sympathetically -30a. encourage achievement and who are understanding in the area of the aild's capabilities and Didtations pro dnced higher achievers (in this-sample).' At the same time, the results of Scale 10, Lack of Over-Indetence on Achievement, support this finding. fire the high achievers scored higher (at the .05 level) an as; of over-insistence -31- on achievement by their parents. It is a wise parent who learns to understand the limitations of his adoleScent son or daughter and accordingly helps him to set a realistic level of aspiration. Unrealistic demands in this area set by parents can be primary contributors to an unhappy, maladjusted low-achiever. Sometimes, however, the result is the opposite, a compulsive, frustrated ‘\ high achiever who constantly feels the push from his parents who have set the unrealistically high goals for their child. These ' are the parents who are often forcing the child, unwittingly or consciously, into a mold which they themselves either could not fit, or missed the Opportunity to try for. Scale 5 concerns the facet of family life that sparks an immediate controversy in many circles of discussion on child de- ve10pment and family life today, i.e., the patterns of "authori— tarianism" vs. "democracy" as opposite extremes in patterns of parent-child interaction. It is in this area that many studies present conflicting findings. Drews and Teehan,10after interview- ing mothers of high and low achievers, discovered that mothers who reported that they were strict, demanding, and authoritarian pro- duced children who were achieving at a high scholastic level. The mothers studied in this investigation who were more "laissez-faire," "permissive," or "democratic" concerning family practices produced children who reportedly were lower achievers in school. One chief 10Elizabeth Drews and John E. Teehan, "Parental Attitudes and Academic Achievement," J. of Clinical Psychology, 1957, 13:328-332. ‘ r32- (5 distinction of note here, however, is that this investigation ought to get at the parent' 3 opinion 3? parental attitudds toward child-rearing practices, while the present study, and others, edited , at the same target, but from a different point of View, i.e., the ~ child's perception. Harrow and Wilson,n who dovehped and fir st . used the imtrunent employed in the present study, tested m school boys' perceptions of their parent-child relatimdlips and found that ~ highe- chime saw their parents as more democratic, saw themsohes as sharing more ideas and oonfil emcee with their parents, and stated that they had a greeted part in phming and decision-making than did low echimrs in the same senile. Tress results contradict mews and Teohan's findings aid support the work of the present insatigation. in this stuck the mean scores were not significantly different on the scale dealing with sharing'in deoision-nsldng, although the results were in the direction of Morrow and.Wilson's, hit Scale 5, flaring of Ideas and Confidences, dirterentiated sigfiflcantly at the .001 level; this is perhaps one of the more useihl findings of this study in light of the denoting results of Brews and Teehan and the supporting ' widens. of Morrow and Wilson. L.K. Fralnkl2 says the following about the sharing, democratic home envimnment: N / n‘bbrrow and Wilson, 2.2;. Q}. 12L. K. bank and Mary Fx-ank, Your Adolescent gt Home 2E9. a School, new York, Bet: Morisonldbrary, 19%, p. M. w -33- Slowly the concept of democracy is evolving as an aspir- ation--a continual striving for a way of living in homes, schools, workshOps, public affairs, everywhere that people are engaged in human relations. This way of life we are seeking will recognize that every individual is a person, with worth and dignity which we should respect so that he in turn will be able to respect others and participate in maintaining a free society. This means that what happens in families, what we learn in childhood, is as important for good social life as what we do as citizens in the out- side world. The family reflects democratic society when its members have fun together, respect one another, and are given a voice in policy-making. The clash between "old" and "new" climates may produce problems; when father's pride is at stake; when mother feels "put upon;" or when parents think that complete dictatorship or permisiveness is the answer. b‘ We need a family system and a school system that will more adequately rear children for democratic families in a dem- ocratic nation. Scale 7, Lack of Parental Restrictiveness, yielded non- significant results at the .05 level. It is interesting to note here that although the high achievers perceived their parents as being less restrictive than did the low achievers, the difference was not large enough to be significant. Perhaps the following 13 will help to clarify this result: statement from F. Ivan Nye "The effectiveness of direct controls, those of restriction, super- vision and punishment, are least affected by affectional identifi— cation with parents. Evasion of and actual rebellion against di- rect controls might be expected to be less frequent, however, in cases in which there is an accepting relationship. 13F. Ivan Nye, Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior, (John Wiley and Sons, 1958), p. 71. h -31“ 1110 results of scale 10, lock of Over-InSistence on Achieve- ment, which proved to be significant at the .05 level, have been discussed earlier, as they are related to the results of scale 3, Wthetic Encom'agement of Achievement. As dglt be expected after the non-significant results of the scale dealing with restrictiveness and the comment made by Nye, .'_--- scale 12, Severity of Parental Dzhcipline, also proved to’ield non- significant results at the .05 level. Even though these results were non-simficant, here again, the results were in the predicted direc- tion, that high achievers would see their parents as less severe and more reasonable in disciplinazy practices than would low achievers. me last individual scale on which the t-test was used was scale 13, measuring the child's perception of his parents' trust in him. his is a very crucial facet of the parent-child relationship and one which is often negatively misconstrued by t1! defensive adoles- cent. According to (lemon,1h adolescents and adults tend to be their . most critical and Quilt-finding selves at this stage in the child's life. the child who is in the midst of major physical and sectional personal changes is easily discouraged by the sligltest criticism from an all-too-demanm parent. ms sense of worth is easily damaged it he feels a parent does not accept his as a person in his own right, trusting hil‘fcr the mahme, independent person he wants to believe 11‘Arnoild Gesell 21; $3.." Youth: 213 Years from _T2 3.2 fixteen, New York, Harper and Brothers, 1553. ~35. he has already become. At the same time, it is particularly diffi- cult and trying for a parent who my see nothing but irr98ponsible, un- predictable behavior from his adolescent «behavior which naturally pro- vokes more than the usual amount of criticism from the parent. he on- oouraging Issults of this scale showed that the high chievers did perceive their parentsas trusting thenmore thanthelow achievers did. . lhe findings were agnii‘icant at the .05 level. Frank gives this advice to parents on the important subject of helping the adolescent to feel that he is trusted andrespected as an individual: "It is time to think ahead «about the trust you must show in your adolescent and his friends, about a remwed effcr t to say things posibively to young- sters who feel grown-up, idealistic, altruistic. he best way to be- lieve in your won child is to know other dli‘fl ran in his school or in awgroup to which he belongs. Every adolescent since time began needed the adult world to talk with him and to trust hin.'15 After testing the simficance of the differences in mean scores on the seven individual scales, the t-test was outplayed for the same purpose with the overall or total. scores. Elhis determined the differ- ence in neanyscores of the high and law achieving groups on‘the total questionnaire-wd differentiated between their Overall Family Morals Ratings. These reenlts were highly significant, 15m, 22.- aih, M6. -36- well beyond the .001 level (t = 6.757), indicating that the high achievers scored well above the low achievers not only on scales 3,5,10, and 13, but also in their perceptions of their parent-child relationships as a whole. Only the results of scale 5, Sharing Ideas and Confidences (t = 6.98), were more highly sig- nificant than the findings on the overall score. CHAPTER III SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Sample The sample included two groups of fifteen subjects each, equated for grade in school, age, I.Q., and level of academic achievement. The subjects were eighth grade boys and girls who were all rated "superior" (scoring 120 or above on I.Q. rating) according to the elementary form of the CTMM. For the purposes of the study, they were divided into one high and one low achievement group, depending on the academic grade point averages they held at the time of the study. The high group had averages of B (3.0) or better and the low group, averages below a 3.0. The median I.Q.'s for the two groups were 129.5 for the high achievers and 130 for the low achievers, the difference being non-significant. Data Collection The subjects were given the Family Relations Scale, a questionnaire developed by William Morrow, to discover their perceptions of their parent-child relationships. The ques- tionnaire consisted of 16 self-report scales (seven of which were indiVidually analyzed for the purposes of this study, though all were completed by the subjects) and an overall or total score. The scales used in this study dealt with the adolescent's perception of his parents and of family practices in his home, more specifically, parental approval, sympathetic encouragement of achievement, sharing ideas and confidences, as. Parental restrictiveness, overinsistence on achievement, severity of discipline, and parental trust in the child. Analysis be six items in each sub-scale were rated from 1 to 1;, de- pending on whether the reapondent checked "almost never”, "sometimes", 'dten', or “very cften.“ (positively phrased itona) oh the item which were, negatively phrased, the scoring was reversed. After a score hr each of. the seven sub-scales and the overall score had been computed, the t-test of significance of ddfferences was mloyed to test differences of mean scores of the high and low achievement groups. Results Ihe findings show that high achievers perceived their parents as more sympathetically encouraging achievement, (rather than placing great “stress anl over-insistence on high achievment), as more trusting, and saw their families as sharing more ideas and confidences than did low achievers in the sample. m above findings were all significant at or beyond the .05 level. Positive, but not statistically dgnificant, were the statements that high achievers saw their parents as less restrictive, less severely disciplining, and more approving than did the law achievers. 'Ihe high achievers also made a significantly higher mean score on the overall questionnaire than did the low achievers, indicating that they perceived a more favorable parent-child relationship, ea 5 3112.12. LIMITATIONS OF THE STUD! W 12.129. m1: 1. All subjects were from a relatively homogeneous socio-eco- nestle background. more were no real opportunities for mntrasts in social class status. 2. 3. h. -ho- Grade point averages, used to determine high or low level or achievemnt grouping were computed from grades made in only one sex-ester's work. LQ. scores were obtained from one test, the California Test of Mental Maturity. while this is accepted as a nlid and re- liable group intelligence test, more than one test score would be helpiul in differentiating more accuragely between "super- ior" and "below-superior“ students. the findings from such a limited lumber of subjects ahmnd not be generalized for groups other than the group in this study. magma 1. 2. One isolated incident might have distorted the subject's response to the questions. For example, a child nightrhave had a onflict with both or either parent on the morning of the dw the imtrument was administered. Me, of course, is a limitation which applies to most cross-sectional kinds of research. The subject may not have selected an accurate or truthful response. Jerkinsl says that a subject may answer inaccurately when the accurate response would embarrass, incriminate or is not expedient to him. This is a limitation which applies to all data collected by qmstionnaire. 4,0,. CONCLUSIONS m as 9.2a 2:. as am 1. It is possible to measure some aspects of adolescents' perceptions of their parent-child relationships - as a whole - and in some specific areas of the relationship. 2. The instalment can be used to collect data concerning adolescents' perceptions of their parents which can be related to various characteristics of children and their fandlies. Such use of the instrument with a large, ran- dom sample can yield information which may contribute to better mderstanding of parent-child relationships and the child's academic performance. margin; the Adolescent's Percgptigg of the P-G Relationship The following conclusions regarding adolescents' percep- tions of their parent-child relationships can be applied only to the subjects of this study. The limited number of 1J.G. Jerkins, "Quaracteristics of the Questionnaire as Determinants of Dependability, " Journal 93 Consultant Pfloholog, 7', 1911.1, pp. 1614-170. 'hl' subjects and the selective nature of the group do not ware rent generalization of the findings to a wider population. The hypothesis of the study was supported, since the t-test of significant diFferences revealed that in the sam- ple investigated, the high-achieving superior pupils per- ceived their parent-child relationships as strikingly differ- ent from.the low achieving superior pupils in three specific areas. The areas were parents' sympathetic encouragement of achievement versus over-insistence on achievement, perceived parental trust, and sharing of ideas and confidences in the families. In other words the high achievers reported that they, were the objects of more sympathetic encouragement of achieve- ment, more parental trust, and that there was in their fami- lies, more sharing of ideas and confidences. Thescores of the high and low achieving groups had non-significant differ- ences on the scales dealing with parental approval, restric- tiveness, and severity of discipline. Finally,overall or total score of the subjects, the Over Family Morale Rating, indicated strongly that the high achievers significantly more often than low achievers, saw their parent-child relationships more favorably §§.§ WE212° I SUGGESTIONS FOR FURTHER STUDY The investigator offers the following suggestions for further study: 1. It might prove interesting to check data gathered by using this instrument against results secured from case studies, projective techniques, and/or 2. 3. -h2- other methods of data collection. (the open-ended question dealing with the pupil's impression of the questionnaire he had Just allplaeted was written in at the and offline instrlnnent for pupils to shaver if thw desired. Ihe rang'grfc: answers received suggests that helpful quali- tative data might, be thus obtained in this subject area.) his instrument was‘adunistered to a small group for this study. he investigator suggests that, after further validation of the in- strument (cross-checking. results with other types of data), it be achninistered to a large random sample. Such findings may provide broader and more realistic implications of chill ren‘s perceptions of their rele- ticnships with their parents all of the effect of the relationship on tie child's develcpment, specifically his scholastic achievement. mus, generalizations to a wider population 2111 be warranted. me investigator suggests a study comparing children's perceptions of their relationships with their parents' perceptions of these same relationship . A study of this nature would provide correlations of children's and their parents' perceptions. If these were studied in relation to children's behavior and academic performance, one might gain insight as to what the signficant determinants are in the parent-child relationship as it affects the child’s behavior and fuller utilisation of his potentialities. Ibo writer believes that the parent-child relationship does have significant effects upon chil- dren's achievement in school, and that children's perceptions, more than parents‘ perceptions determine the effects of the parent-child relation- ship. -h3- child's behavior and fuller utilization of his potentialities. The writer believes that the par- ent—child relationship does have significant of- fscts upnn children's achievement in school, and that children‘s perceptions, more than parents' perceptions determine the effects of the rela» tionship. slmlcdmm l. Ausubel, D.D., "Perceived Parent Attitudes as Determinants of. (hildren's Ego Structure," Child Develgpte at, pm, September, 1951.. PP. 173-183. 2. Bscmeister, R, "1119 Family Cones first," Childhood Education, m, September, l9h8, pp. 8-11. - 2a. Baker, Marietta, Causes of Failure in Junior High School Among Boys . of Superior Intelligence” Master's, 1927. 3. mock, V. J., "Conflicts of Adolescents with 'Iheir Mothers," Journal Abnormal Social szcholog, 193?, mn. h. canpbell, 3.5:. 93 5}, Journal Educational P chole , 52:199-200 5. Campbell, nah, |'Ef‘fects of Bolton Hones on Children's Adjustment in School,‘I Journal Educational Sociolgg, 1932, II, pp. 271;. 6. Glampney, E., "the lbls Parental Behavior Rating Scales," Yellow Springs, Chic, Antioch Press, 1939. 7. Chandler, £4. , “Importance of the Early Years: Childhood Education, 39: 2" e 8. Chopin, F.S., "A Home Rating Scale,“ 3_9_____cialS ceResearch, 1929, 9. Cliiford, .Ed. 'Discipline in the Hons," J__o______uma1 Gene___;____tic P chclo 1959, 95 115.82. M 10. Conklin, AJL, “Beach. 0011. Contr. Edam, 191m, #792. 11. 00:, EN. et a1, "Assessing As cts of Parent-Child Relationships," Child Develop]. nt 32 :637- 9. 12. che, HAL, "Home Gmditicns as a Cause of Failure in High Schools," Education Research §1_J_._le_t_i§_, September, 1923, vol. 2. 13. Daily, Marion E., "Causes of Failure in Fourth and mm Years,“ Master's, 1929, New York University. . . 111;. English, 11.3., cs 9_.§_______ Child vow, New York, Holt, Rinehartan ston, Im. ,1 l. 15. Drew's, Elizabeth and John Teehan, "Parental Attitudes and Academic Achievement," Journal of Clini______c__al_ mm 1957, 13:328-332. 15a. Faegre, ILL. and J. E. Aniorscn, Child Care and h'ainin hth Edition, Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Hess, 1957, p. 279. 16. Ford, Mildred 9., u Study of Certain Sociological and Psychological Factors in Children of Superior Intelligence who Failed in School, n Snith Coll. Stud. of so__<_:_. Work, 1932, 3. 20h. (abstr. ) 16a. 17. 18. 19 . 20. 21. 22. 23. 2h. 26. 27. 28. 29. 32. 4:6- Glueck, Bernard, "Parent and Child: A Patnership,’ _C_h_il__dS tug, 1929, 6' 379-810 'ihe Significance of Parental Attitui es for the Destirw ...—...: c? the Indiv:l.d:.tal,a lb_§__ta1 m 12 :723-31. Glueck, Sleldcn and Eleanor, Ebrironment ing Delingueng, Routeledge and Kegan Paul, I: n, 1952. Harris,D ., "Facts- 3 Affecting College Grades: A Review of the literature," 1930-37, cholc cal Billetin, 191:0, 37 :125-151. Hattuick, s. and Stowell, 11.5. , "Relationship of Parents' Over-Attentive- nose to Children's Work Habits in Kindegarten and the First Six Grades.‘l Journal Educational Research, 1936-37 , 30: 169-176. Hevighurst, R. J. , and Hilda Tabs, Adolescent alaraEter and 136va Janent, Wiley and Sons, 191:9 Hashes, G. R., 'Family Influences on Personality," Journal of Home Eco- nond.___£_,s XLIV, 1952. Keyword, R. 6., "Child's Report of Psychological Factors in the Rom," Arch. of chholog, 1935, #189:1-75 Hoffnsn, H.L., 'Powefi Assertion by Parents and the Inpact on the Child," Child Development, 31: 129-1143. lush, u. , uSome Relation Between Intra-thily Attitudes and Pro- Parental Attitudes Toward Children, Journal Geneti_______c_ Psycholog, Jones, E.S., "me Probation Student: What He is like and What We can Dc about It," Jamal Educational Research, 1955, 1:9: 93-102. Iagan,J ., *‘lhe Child's Perception of Parents," Journal Abn__g_'____ml Scc__i_._a1_ szcholog, 1956, 53: 257-258. Kimball, B., I'Case Studies in Educational Failure During Adolescence ," American Journal _o_f_ Orthopgzchiatiz, 1953, 23 :hO6-h15. . "lhe Sentence Coupleticn Tedmique In a Study of Scholastic movement,“ Journal Consultant Psychcleg, 1952, 16: 353—358. Koch, H.L., et al, "A Scale for Measuring Attitudes Toward Questions of the Child's Fl‘eedcn," GlildDeve Develgpngnt, 1931:, 5:252-266. Laws, Gertrude, Pare: t-Child Relationships: A tfituq 9_f Atti tudes Egg Practices Concezqulgg“ the Socials Maggy ent of Mn New York, ac. umbia, levy, David 11., Relationships of Maternal Over-Protection To School Garages and In. elJigence Tests ," American Joumel 2.1? Orthopsychiatry, 3:2 29. 33. 3h. BIBLIOGRAPHY (cont'd.) Lewis, W. D., "A Comparative Study of the Personalities, Interests, and fiome Backgrounds of Gifted Children of Superior and Inferior Educational Achievement. g. Genet. Psychol., lghl, 59:207-213. Likert, A., "A Technique for Measurement of Attutudes." Arch. of Psychology, 1932, 22:5—55. 35.Lorr, M. et a1, "Three Factors in Parental Behavior." I 30. 37. he. 1:7. 1,8. g; Consult. Psv., 1953, 17:306-308. Mannino, F.V., "Family Factors Related To School Per-~ Sistence.”_§4_§d. Sociology, 35:193-202. Meister, A., "Perception and Acceptance of Power Rela- tionships in Children." Group rsychotherapy, 1950, 9:153-163. Merrell, E., "A Measurement of Mother-Child Interaction." g, Abnormal Social Psychology, lghé, h1:37-h9. Leltzer, h., "Children's Attitudes Toward farents." .Auere_ll Orthopsvchiatry, 1935, 5:29h-205. . Miller-Crichton, Hugh, "Home Background of the Pupil." Megfal yvgjgng, 1932, 16:23-25. Morrow, H.R. and Wilson, 3.0., "Family Relations of Bright High Achieving and Under-Achieving high School Boys,"_thld.Delelnpment, 32:501-510. Musselman, J.W., "Factors Associated with Achievement of High School Pupils of Superior Intelligence." J. Exp. Education, 19MB, 11:53-68. _— Myhand, Mary V.,_An_lnstrument To Measure Children's Per- centiog§_2f Their Parents' Acceptifig Behavior, EA Thesis, Michigan S;ateUhiversity, 1957. Nemours, Du Pont de, "National Education in the United States," as in J. W. Musselman, 19h2. (See entry #h2.) Newton, Alice, "Relation Between Emotional Maladjustment In the Home and School Failure." smith Coll. Stud. Soc. Work, 1931, 2:89. (abstr.) Nye, F. Ivan, Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior. John Wiley and Sons, 1953. Peterson, D.R. et a1, "Parental Attitudes and Child's Adjustment." Child DevelQEment, 1959, 30:119-130. Plutchek, R. et a1, fStudies of Parent-Child Relations." 3; Genet. Psy., 95:171-170. -uél L9. Rolf, E., "A Factorial Study of the Fels Parental- 4 Behavior Scales." Ch 1d Development, 19u9, 20:29—h5. 50. Serot, N.H. et a1, "Perception of Parent—Child Relation- ships... and the Relation tonthe Child's Adjustment." Child Development, 32:373-375. 51. Stodgill, 3.1., "Parental Attitudes and Mental Hygiene Standards." Rental Hygiene, 1931, 15:813. 52. Stott, Leland H., "Adolescent Dislikes Regarding Parental Behaviorslnd Their Significance." J. Genet. #31,, 19LO, ‘ ., _ 57:393-L14- 53.Swanson, G.E., "Development of An Instrument for Ratinr Child-Parent Relations." Social Forces, 1950, 13:82-9h. 5L. Symoncls, Paul, P83611010? 9.2 Parent-Child 12813t.0“3fips° Appleton~Century, 1939. 55. Tarqoff, 1., Rev. Educ. Research, 31,h79-h90. 56. Tauschi "Ehild Who Rebels Against Authority." Instructor, 71: “8-4.?0 57. Terman, Lewis, The Hygiene 2: the School Child, Boston: floughton, 1929. 58. Tibbets, J.R ment g£.H 232. , Role 3h Soho 4. Parent-Child Relations in Achieve- Eupils. Dissert. Abstr., 1955, 15: Q£ ol 0 o .. l 59' Van Alstyne: Do, Teach. Coll. Contr. Educ., 1929,#366. 60. Wallace, H.A. and Ulr'ch, David, "Family Disturbances and 0 o n o o ‘ "‘ r 'J / Learning leflcultles." J. Co sult. £sv., 2h:3;5-300. 61. Walsh. A., Self-Concepts of Priiht BQXfi 71th Learniqg . Difficulties;“ Columbia University: 1956. 62. Williams, J.H., Jhittier Scale For Grading home Conditions. . J n. r . . f ‘ n ,' . - Bulletin #2. fillttler State Donool, California. / azr. ‘ I T Y.‘ n o o 03. #11301, Isabell, "The Jse of a sentence Completion Test in Determininq Differences Between dell—Adjusted and mal— Adjusted Secondary School Eupils." J. Consult. fsv., . ... “...—_— ...—An— 1949, 13.hCO-h02. -u7- APPENDIX A 4:449 e:- “1“ ‘3’ APPENDIX A «21-149 4:- INVESTIGATOR'S INTRODUCTION OF INSTRUMENT TO SUBJECTS. I am a graduate student at M.S.U. My job is to find out something about boys and girls your age. would like to know some things about ways you feel about your home and parents. In a minute I will give each of you a question- naire. Please leave it face down until I ask you to turn it over. Please answer the questions as honestly as you can. I am the only person who will see your answers. Your responses will not be discussed with your parents, teachers, or with anone who knows you. This is not a test because there are no right or wrong answers -- the best answers are the ones that best describe you and your parents. (Hand out questionnaires; check to see if every pupil has a pencil with an eraser.) Now, please look at the first page of the ques- tionnaire. (Investigator proceeds to demonstrate mark- ing technique with a sample item and response.) Are there any questions? You will have until the end of this hour to com- plete the questionnaire. If you have a queS"; ion during the time you are working, please raise ye-.: hand and I will come around to your desk Please begin now. (Note time on wall clock.) =I= I ; {LEST 13331189 J‘St' mitial} W‘Bmasim ' - mes__ . 9’8 ..., maxvnuwas-rx -' . 9 Bah; of Bifih ~ i I mums-.911..- v- ' ~ ‘ _ _ __,l 4.. ..o..—..—.4...,—. Maw BIKES TIUNS : 2' ~::‘:3::::r.'. .‘..:‘:.‘..n::.x...‘ ' *..'...::.- m:.<-M-**-:'.:x.~n'tn u- .. . i W _ 9 f The follmzing ques tions are about yourself and your family.) Road g each moieties oarefully and, answer it as it applies to the parents with i when Egg 1.333 was (whether they are your real parents 9 ate-p p’renmr 3118.? a To the right of sash question put a she sis-umn: in the column whi eh, comes is.) the times. in your ease... if you feel. it is ties at all true as is phehs ‘ 5 ">1 ' {V ‘ If: . (“'2' .. -.. , 0 V; v w .. w v: ‘ n 4 vn-al' ~ 1" a T . wisest h‘i‘iroi 9 cheek; to 19411113; 1... ii" it.» happens sensitizes, chess. so was. .29 . ’1 . v. if, "‘- ‘ T, '7' 1 - ‘9 ' '." Wit? .5" i 'r‘m’t: 4-“ "n" 7" 1’ W11" 57-10" 3 'z'f‘ " e:- ' afuxxlz .-.... “I .9 p, t J" 7 t b . 3 ~ . v I -- "‘ »‘ ' if, '4‘" ‘ ' 4 ’v n w 1,. v) ‘94—} .Ux.‘.:.~f.) JLAQ ai.."..T-:U}.13j (m 3:»; "= a“- -, 'i.‘ 5.316512. 9:5 is :- ‘7 " APE/333 u... JAW.‘ v&s: VML ”(7‘1 1* b: - {mi-um?“ i, m C . ~- ... h ... » _ ‘4? .n , .f , ' r his: 999 we: Ca» ism, E 8M In": ‘. ..__ . .- -‘;' :1: "225 , ‘ I‘l‘y‘; .' ”175”? _ 9 . , _ .. 9 7, -- .1. .. 1......5‘... L" 3 “4- '1...ng :- , , ‘ .-..9‘3'4 ... . :1...-.1.-.; ...uuqn»;~~. 11;... .. u . t . w. -— ;.M\.-:.m;..::.~....;:qz. cud. a—<.~“.:.-- t- - JokavttA-"v-ha. . 43.29:):er ,-.9--.Ln;v.ua.:Fu&C.i.L W-m‘ ' A w ..- I I 2"“. 9.27. pm v- 3.. e {his {series is :1 iii). as nevi :' i , 3 ’95 - —~p:_-.-.'p<)~ ‘c—Wl- “—3 (‘-'.~)*1A.'-’ '9 ‘-~_ : ‘ Q m—_'<‘9>'_ 141..“ .IluaM' ~0F...h)'.{l""' n.4'M'. “ F.‘ .1-’?F .‘blvial “_g$~l 'l'-'\ 4». a ‘o. L? 511' 3‘ ’ "sl- - ‘5) 9 .2 "s ‘ _ .0 b ‘ l ‘ . J . ’t “2’ ' ‘ ‘ 1. so you go to games (item; at 9 has chalk e (3&3 ,5 with. E 3: . one or both permits ? _ ‘ . -».,.. ~ .4 ———_ wA,_9‘n‘.-. . 9,. n. . . . r" T. - A _' .. HRS-$1. , W 1; hoes your family have good. times together at home? Bo either of your parents work with you on any ‘_ hobbies or projects? E :3 is Do you and your family go on picnics or outings 3 or trips together? 5 i i‘ <3. is it enjoyable to spend evenings with your family “group? L. -. Are both of ymr permts fair in their criticism of you? . Do you worry about Hint either of your parents think of you? .19.... Wm Doee someone at home "pick" on you? 353». Does either parmt ever seem to wich that you were a different sort of person? ' ‘l—m'" ‘ V 11:: Are both of your permits inclined to think well of M? 12 Does either parent over seem irritated with you i Without cause? '1 214- 13 Do your parents sympathetically encourage you to do well in school? 14-0 Do your parmts inspire you to want to develop .Wur abilities? ' ""7 5o Does your parentsi interest in mrtlmhile books . flake you mat to read them? . " . 399 your parents understandjngly encourage you to who part in school. affairs? . - -- a"... Wu... iv . . ‘ ‘ 4- to your guests help to stimlate year int asset in :::.a,:.z'r$-'; areas as art,, music9 soienoe9 01" 33033-13105? ‘ --- -uzw-n‘ :0 '00 your parents stimflste you to think hinge out for W? 9.90mmt. mmmmmw ‘ Windham-225m? °D°mmtatrytohawyou2 Do your pant. 13.131; on over and settling 'u4. .- ... ~.,-—..v ‘ 'YKJ'\’ ' . 1 ’ f P'» 9-. u. ,_ _.‘ {1% fl , .v. var n..— — r2 = 9 "r -<- .3, 9. 22' l a 'y'V-j'l' - “x .1; -.\ ‘ Q ’1' J -\ “V f! i; V .3 .t.‘ .i. ‘9‘. yidLliT (I ‘1: «It: S. 27.. l."” 0;; :Imn'fios 23:22:93 ‘82, 23:3? . ’ ’ ’ ~ .1 ‘rgw. . 7 4' ‘ .'._. f3; xiii-41121) «JO {33219329. U9 ‘5‘}? 23.2: s 921210;) 2222222222 unzip-93:2.) 2222?; 22681222311! c122. ‘3 ”5‘3"“ "LL ”'1': '79:} Lig‘LL-L sfiwuu £32.27“ 2. 0'2}. 231;“? prey (Arms: .7302: 23 22:2 9 -- il"‘.'— rm: ':-:u: 7" 7'72? .-:'*=- Y. ’.‘.'.'.1.‘*;.:I. .‘ .TJva-Iazmz: . 1-1:. 4:11-1:13: 32-2. 9.27:1 7‘ .25" ”'3' '1"! ‘1'-) uuvr-y-cnva-m .‘ -.' -'. {2272 ‘ u. =23? 3:0 22 :2. 2352‘ 91:: 81’!‘ Sf?” ..r: v-(m-. Error-”r ' ' '3‘ (Lo, L TL ”131: $2an21 {Eff'r'} :;:nii .2: i L.) 213322.,” "; H. 11': _7 -‘ 31' v __' ... =: 5"“udzs 22 21 2 9 H .t’. ,4 ‘- r‘fi 9 I , v i 51‘ a Y F GA} n"“' I: z. 1.22: 2292 222:2 2:212:22 s and s .21: 2:22 32: Jensen r “2n. . .... .moA' are £22122 T 2: engine}: 2.220021? e921? 2 . ‘~- ~- 3...?! :3". 1‘3: “‘5‘." \ '2. \ \ I. 5 2' - x l .__.____L._LW_ .- L L 1 O I ' ’ " for u 2 _ 3 f 9:. cat. , = um; Jar-2201!"; 2.246. ’C’: ‘ Shirl-LL; 2» if, .9 99 . . F - ’.- . . ,- k ' . ~"' _ 's ‘ 3 r 31'??? his '20 80.2 2 '4 f; g', ‘ v " a 't:: '7"I“.-Iaé~§vr":: ' - - ' -."= "7.1 '.".‘\".('1’r\‘,‘ - - , 9 ‘ ‘ .-. I ‘2 . ." ‘._ I t _ . o 1:015. 1..- U II: no '3“ 1 . ‘t .- , an“. ,99v_»-Irs ... --O"_v‘.‘~“.'. ‘“ r>"~,.‘-»‘,._ '1" ' _'_.."‘| m‘l'.‘c.f.._.2‘; .. l‘;- b .._ . ..- -:-,,‘,.“9,, 2 i i : 9 ‘ 3 , 2 I 933335“ 2w: 0;; chm... .1 mm and mm mm» nu *2" ”‘1' r m- r. z, 1’ —- . L ' _' u- I. -c ‘ —. ' db—‘i-o‘+- ‘ ‘ 5"" Wm “—— rt- 1...-.. . "__ - . -—“.—-~-_-* - I“- -— . ‘fi—i- ‘ I p. - Do you and your parents have so "31017.8 diatomic-as about religious, pifilosopi'sioal, political, or social meggons? Md cmfi “-" ' - Do you and your parents have serious intellectual discussions about subjects such as art, music, literature Lor solence': " . . \ l .: ~19 “Cur-.0;- - ..4- I , Do you talk to your paralts about your problems and worries? 31. Do your parents show an interest in thms that cmcem you? 32. Do your parents show pleasure at whet you do? 33» Do your parents openly show affection for you by by word or action? 34o Do your parents enjoy Spending time with you? 35- Do your parents do little things to show affection and consideration for you? 36. How often do your parents praise you? 37. Are your parents particular about what boys or girls you associate with? Do your parents give you a good deal of freedom? Do your parents give unreasonable commands which they insist that you carry out? ,1 f 1 Do your parents let you decide important things for yourself? Do your parents insist that you do fixings their way? #2. ‘_ Do your parents try to direct your activities? ¥ n'- m! IF. I .‘QW—iv . to] I . . savanna-ILA:- L-IL-“nm ....‘hr u...-. M I . i ll . I 43. ‘E Do your parents take your wishes into considera~ tion when plamjng a family trip or vacation.? I l .' ;.S":“‘. cum . ‘. a. to .— I ‘ - F'wx. 4 LP’iv o Do your parents let you help decide everyday family policies, rules, and ways of living? k..— l+5.. Do your folks let you help make important. family decisions? “b 46. Does your family talk over future family plans together? 47o Does everyone in your family have a say about how Your home is decorated and what furniture to buy? \_ —— -‘- :“>-vr'1‘~l. . o .‘ . "-3- f‘ ..nn'. '- fiasjg 11oz:- -:-:. no divs)". .L J ...... I P—L .- Q n—E‘MH‘JI 4- ._, ‘ $ w r74??? 3'2?‘ 3:50 I ' ... ‘ 4 ~.u . . ; ~. ; . figi'.j.§.1\{.‘.§.'u!2 uuofl 4.; ‘ Mimi..." ..... _. u-..__--g _ “in __ 3 l g i ' goof: 1;?!qu 21:91:01: 3 z " , :: i;- E M i l , g i Joel: antes pus I g e—i ._ -_ , I 1 Q ‘ E uatzn no.5 JO 3 aim .. ,1 j“ ‘ 119012;:ch no]! one “tomb 5,...4 “Muir g- mm-n‘-‘-— . i nun-o. - .Do your parents expect too much of an} 9' V9- want you t o? a. \d‘J-L. nuns-u w - ‘ _‘I—fl’- t 'u'.I---‘,-- I-d’ 'l‘l ni‘hn‘ Eb‘.’ ya" '2’ Ac _.. -' hflI—“J Do your parents insist that you chooses. “soften type of vocation? :—-"'..7¢.AAarI.—h-mI--‘m¢ .9T.~ ‘¢.c.a ten: 5 "mur'fi'u .3. _~‘. 57o Are your parents always after you to work hard to become a success? I - '. I‘M-\m~1-g —— w.—w 1:».- .wu— . I wane—'- wen-r .. 58. Do your parents keep after you to become an importzmt person in school affairs? 59. Do your parents keep after you to read more or to read certain things, whether you want to or hot? MMIL~' ‘ u ""'-=mu'n- t l ~‘u . 60° Are your parents always trying to get you to study harder? 61. Do you feel happy and contmted when at how: with .> your family? 62. Do you like to do extra little things to please the members of your family? 63. _ Would you like to be the same kind of parent. that your parents have been? ' ---‘-at-.Au-A-po- nun. 61;. Do you consider yourself very close to your parents? mgr! 11-1 ~.,~;<. o . A u I‘L‘r—I-oor', m w)- : .. I Are your parents good friends and pale to you? Do you admire your parents? How often do your parents scold you? Do your pareats insist that you obey there im - diately when they tell you to do something? - t- l' “Arturo. I Do your parents use physical punislment to ref-4:6 you mind? Does the way your parents act depend so much on their mood that you're not sure what to expect? 32mm”: _. ~ How often do your parents punish you? Do your parents punish you severely for misbehavior or disobedience? . ("I-mum " '9 o - -..“nlm'imfi ~'. '4" . I .004- tame-wm‘;- . v ,1 l J 13» .3: ’— 3 i v ‘1‘." .‘W 1'“. W 1' new-"1v “.-....— - |u-—.< 'dol’ \ Us" 4 ID ’ 'V '-y-:tnml1rt u‘m fl 3 a i M“.':M: a; 1 ! g 1 U‘MW-xpwm .- _. ’ 4 i V :- V.‘M. ." '0 .- 1:1vuer'a‘m r ;. . . . 4 § 1 i i l 1 I a n , o. f. ’ 'L ._, I ‘ .. ‘x ." x .. v ‘( a ‘ '.? 7 '3 .IA J .- 55 ------ J g ‘ ‘ ‘ -. “*n(— 1? a ,‘ v ... J‘ ~‘ “ ’1‘. I? seem ”MT 301') oi no}: Trot; squad: *- it oi-{eq on, sonar-rum EU ova, p {non Ros uoum r: i ecu-us "9. WM fimif form tie-13:0 -emog "com-L "UIO'A l .4 5"} L‘. . .x ...V _. ,—.|- .‘_ f3. -.. g‘ 'T . , . , _, l“ _ ‘ . I , ‘ . . A. . '0‘ ‘1.- "u \ ._ s . L. ...' s, \d‘;_ ... ,4) L: ... M d . ‘1 ~- ~ . ._- h, ‘ 4 . -l t, . . . . ... u 4 . . . ~ ~ , -. .... 7.. —‘-—‘ - ism .4. - I. «(o 'm."‘; mar-'43 fin. l.h. 0. *1 s:— a- .- ‘u-‘JMM “I I 4 ‘¢ I a Do your parents agree about how the house is to ,ud.‘ in»; >'~.| .‘I .a' be run? 5 ... W (.-.... - 2 ...-9 Do your parents ever go places by thwolves? i if 2.. Do your parents agree with each other in their 5 general ideas about life? i 5 Y fir-12.1.1833. {:WJM—Kmu . '1‘. -‘ - ‘30 Do your parents openly show affection or con- i 5 sidersticn for each other? , f, -‘.'., Do your parents agree about met you are allowed. 5 to do? _ my”? Are your parents cheerful and happy when together? 0 Do your parents object to some of your activities with your friends and acquaintances? 1.). Do your parents like you to bring your friends into your home? ._ r. Do your parents irritate you by teasing you about your interest in the opposity so a? 77?). Do your parents kg? for.» s going to club 83:! school meetings? "h... Does your home over seem to get disorganized? P E a 5 ,.-:.. Are household tasks in your home done on a regular routine? a). Are your meals served at regular hours? '?. Do your parents approve of your friends? 5.3.. Do your parents approve of your going to dances i and parties when you want to? _ 5‘? Ln.‘ .. if e' .. ‘..". X "L l.‘u m ". _1 I. «(11" \. . in Are wings kept in their proper lace at how? . ’o‘. Do the members of your family get up at a regular time 2 ’5‘ T6. Do you go to bed at a regular hour on school nights? '5— k V-uv‘au‘uvma'.‘ .—.n.ml\ m>,‘ soot! tau-... .—“.“ . m-“‘h—. V -1... 009.4 ..All. \a\‘ «If. \ < r r s- h . . . 0 Ag<-- . x 4 _O I. 'LV' '5‘ . .V'O‘ JL I ' " v“. | "'3~ I‘LC- ‘52:? A ‘ 'K ' -‘wgl‘u—I..u.:.rk‘.:".mh“ . .l - - . , , - .. . . 0 , ‘ ~ .- . . ‘ ’ ' b. .’ ‘ i ' i ‘J - .‘ o '. I . r ‘1 J 3‘ ' - ‘ ;‘ '1 I‘\ - I .5 i .1 -"‘“'e;"— '.n a! o. a . l t . , n . i y '1 . O o ,. z ' ~ ' k . '1 -* c ’ ' at )9' -" u ' ‘E ".‘l - -‘.'. .— ,5?!“ ' 'a 1- .o ' 5.91‘. . bl" ’-’-~ .‘u:outing»;.x.x\.uau1—~rc.~.nm';=l 1 i «M .-rCW-m' “wuf' ‘r ah" '."’A‘.".'O-; -I .L , 3 2‘ . .. "'1 . 2“ . .' ‘ . |-1 C’s ; .' -'_ (" ~LI , ,a v“) ‘ I ':~ '- g‘ I . v . wa- .. J . 'Lfi-n ”bk-finlu-m‘nu-EC‘J.‘ ¢,~ _..a-..-.-u:r.o-,'ugm.".... I . . ..- - .. - - a . ~1 . ‘ -) 1;”. 5: ‘ N ' *' - ‘ ~ ' 'v .3. ‘ . . a b - ‘.'. ? I - - F Iv.- .- an. " --- I . - ‘ 0 [-fi‘ "1 o. n - -~.vP - I”. - '.' .. "D "\‘ ' I. “ t v.- ,u ' “ r) ‘. ‘-‘ “4; " . r - v - . I _,- _- I ‘ . l 4n—1< I’vl h‘) ' '0 lo- . ~“ 4, . ."' ‘0 2 .A "... - o. ‘ ' ‘\.‘I"’J ¢_A \_"‘G-W~"W~ ' -IIJ unah‘fiur - — I _l ‘ . I V _ I APPENDIX B -55- -55- mamumam mmmuoom wmm-awa mamaoam .mqwmmmtmmmmwmmmmwemm o.m-o.H o.m-o.H o.:-o.m o.:-o.m mmaao>< pcfiom cease 0-:H ob ~-mam mxja 0» puma m-:H op a-ma :.:a co ~-mH “.moz . mnwowv - oma oma-ama o:a-mma m:a-mma omauama oncogene cocomfiaaoch Evade. whom mHAHwL a - _\ {whom cameras? macaw eon anomo monl1 .mm< enema oze .moa ..d.H zH mmozam-m% Mamom umafi mo. 301:.N pco§o>ofino¢ so cocopmfimCHAo>o Mo xowq noa% mo .ooo.a mmoCo>Hpofiapmom prCkom u 5% mo. *Omo.o moodoowmcoo use maooH mcfiewnm u m% mo. *mmm.: pooEo>oH£o< no pooEomoaooocm ofiponpwofihm I m% me Now. Hw>oaoo¢ Hopooawm a N% a 5 p 3.8m oo 03:. mmm>mHmo< BOA Qz< mem mom measmmm 9mmelp I N% mqmde HICHIGQN STRTE UNIV. LIBRARIES 31293006436426