l l 3' l M 3 3‘ 3 l ' I i 33 In ‘33 3 l ‘1 3 33 , {33' :3 3 3" .y l ‘3‘ — 5 , , EFFFFE3S 0F 33‘333F33T3VE3’13LUF333‘BF“F33 0F 3‘F333 F‘O F..F3~:3FNF F!‘ PQ““3T3F33 3 333) F‘X'FEF '3L3 F33“. SF 1333 33333333333332! 3.33 333. EF‘ "3335. 333 CHFU‘ 33F3‘3 ‘ . .fi 3“ 3 991.4 3'11 T338333 303‘ 3329. Degree 03 N3. 33 33333331" 33333 STAFF 3333M 333333“! ANDREW RI‘sFPH GILPFN 333?}. WWWWM K, 71., 677 0725 \ \ m \ \ 7 m: W LIBRARY \ A .‘.L .. . ’ o . C v... v [pulpit 91).. . IchIgan State M m“ ‘ mach“: M?- i O 7 3: ABSTRACT EFFECTS OF INCENTIVE VALUE AND PERCENTAGE OF REINFORCEMENT ON ACQUISITION AND EXTINCTION OF AN INSTRUMENTAL RESPONSE IN CHILDREN By Andrew Ralph Gilpin Forty kindergarten children were each assigned to one of four treatment groups formed by simultaneously manipu- lating incentive value (high. i.e. toy. vs. low, i.e. no toy) and reinforcement schedule (random 50 percent, 1.9. PRF, vs. 100 percent. i.e. CRF). The children first pulled a lever to receive marble rewards for 60 trials. and then made 30 additional responses during which no rewards were given. Latency and duration of each response were noted. Results suggested that boys and girls reacted dif- ferently: in general. boys responded faster. However, the sex variable interacted with other variables. The partial reinforcement acquisition effect (PRAE) was observed, but not the partial reinforcement extinction effect (PRE). Girls who received the high incentive treatment had longer latencies than girls who received the low incentive treat- ment. and than boys in either incentive condition. but only during the acquisition phase. Latencies of high in- centive §s (regardless of sex) who had experienced CRF did not differ from those of low incentive‘fis who had exper- ienced PRF at the beginning of the extinction phase, but by its end, the former had longer latencies. While there were no overall sex differences in movement duration at the Andrew Ralph Gilpin beginning of the extinction phase, by the end of the phase girls had longer durations than boys. Methodological implications for future research were discussed. Theoretical interpretation emphasized the util- ity of cognitive dissonance theory. Acce% (3 , Date 5: 4763/ EFFECTS OF INCENTIVE VALUE AND PERCENTAGE OF REINFORCEMENT ON ACQUISITION AND EXTINCTION OF AN INSTRUMENTAL RESPONSE IN CHILDREN BY Andrew Ralph Gilpin A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1971 A."’-7) . ‘J DEDICATION For my mother. who taught me how to write. and my father. who taught me to read what I had written. 11 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author expresses his appreciation to the Holt Public School system. Holt. Michigan, and to the faculty. staff, and students of the Elliott Elementary School for their cooperation in carrying out the present study. The author wishes to express his thanks to Jean Gilpin. Hark Jackson. Paul Martin, and Steve Plous. who assisted in data collection. A special debt of gratitude is owed to the members of the author's Committee, Dr. Ellen.A. Strommen. Dr. Stanley C. Ratner. and particularly Dr. Hiram E. Fitzgerald. Chair- man, for their contributions to the successful completion of all phases of the project. During the course of the study's execution. the author served as Research Assistant to Dr. Ratner. who was kind enough to allow him to concen- trate on it. at times at the expense of his other duties. This study was supported in part by a Michigan State University Research Grant and an NIMH research grant MH- 18655-01 to Hiram E. Fitzgerald. Use of the Michigan State University computer facilities was made possible through support. in part. from the National Science Foun- dation. 111 TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES. . . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES. . . . . . . . . . . . Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Method. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Apparatus. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Procedure. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . . Acquisition Phase, Latency. . . . . . Acquisition Phase, Movement Duration. Extinction Phase, Latency. . . . . . Extinction Phase, Movement Duration. Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . LIST OF REFERENCES. . . . . . . . . . . APPENDIX. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Letter to Parents. . . . . . . . . . 1V LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Acquisition Latency Means for Significant Effects. . . . . . . Table 2. Acquisition Latency Analysis of Variance. Table 3. Acquisition Movement Duration Analysis of Variance. . . . . . . . . . . . . Table A. Extinction Latency Means for Significant Effects. . . . . . . . . Table 5. Extinction Latency Analysis of Variance. . . . . . . . . . . . . Table 6. Extinction Movement Duration Analysis of Variance. . . . . . . . . . . . . Table 7. Extinction Duration Means for Significant Effects. . . . . . . . Table 8. Number of Experimenter Comments to Subject. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Table 9. Analysis of Variance of Experimenter Comments. . . . . . . . . . . .1h . 15 . 19 .21 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Mean acquisition latency as a function of incentive. schedule. sex. and trials. . . . . . .16 Figure 2. Mean latency as a function of schedule and trials. . . . . . . . . . . . . .17 Figure 3. Mean movement duration as a fu ction of Behedule and trials 0 C O O O I O O O O O I O O O 20 Figure A. Mean extinction latency as a function of schedule, incentive, and trials. . . . . . . .22 Figure 5. Mean extinc ion movement duration as a function of sex and trials. . . . . . . . . 27 V1 INTRODUCTION The partial reinforcement acquisition effect (PRAE), the phenomenon of superior asymptotic performance level of an instrumental response by subjects (gs) receiving partial reinforcement schedules to that of gs receiving continuous reinforcement. has been noted frequently in studies where children performed a lever pull task (Chertkoff, 1968; Nakamura, 1969; Pederson. 1970; Ryan. Orton. & Pimm, 1968; Ryan. Strawbridge. & Matters. 1969; Ryan & Watson. 1968; Semler & Pederson. 1968). On the other hand. the partial reinforcement effect (PRE) refers to greater resistance to extinction in gs reinforced on a random partial reinforce- ment schedule (or PRF, henceforth assumed to be 50 percent unless otherwise indicated) during acquisition compared with those §s who received 100 percent or continuous reinforce- ment (CRF). The PRE has been successfully demonstrated with a va- riety of infrahuman species (Bitterman & Schoel. 1970); but attempts to demonstrate the PRE in children with the lever apparatus have often been unsuccessful. when the dependent variable was latency. duration, or amplitude of response (Bruning. 1964; Holton, 1961; Rosenbaum & Bruning. 1966; Ryan & Voorhoeve. 1966. These variables are not all highly correlated: cf. Cantor & Kubose. 1970). However. as Ryan 1 2 and Voorhoeve (1966) pointed out. when rate of lever pull- ing (responses per unit time) or trials until the child quit responding have been measured. the PRE has been ob- served (Baumeister & Hawkins. 1966; Bijou. 1957; Chert- koff. 1968; Fattu. Mech. & Auble. 1955; Kass. 1962; Kass & Wilson. 1966; Lewis. 1960; Nakamura. 1969). One study. re- ported by Chertkoff (1968). found that regardless of wheth- er §s were required to continue responding. or were free to quit whenever they wished. movement speeds did not reveal the PRE; but more §s who had received CRF during acquisi- tion quit before completing 30 extinction trials than did §s who had received PRF. Thus. while it is clear that the PRAE can be demon- strated in children using lever pull speed as a measure of performance. the status of the PRE in this context is less clearcut. Incentive value is another variable which has been found to influence children's performance, particularly in probability and discrimination learning tasks. where in- vestigators have frequently found that increased incentive level facilitates performance (e.g.. Bisett & Rieber. 1966; Brackbill & Jack. 1958; Offenbach, 1964). although in some cases. low incentive groups did better (Miller & Estes. 1961; Stevenson & Hoving. 1964; Stevenson & Weir. 1959). Efforts to extend such findings to a lever apparatus. us- ing marbles as rewards. have been generally unsuccessful. Ryan (1965) asked kindergarten children to rank six toys prior to a series of lever pulls. He told half of them 3 that if they ”won the game" they would receive their most- preferred toy; he told the other children that if they won. they would receive their least-preferred toy. In addition. half of each group received a PRF schedule; the other half received CRF. Although Ryan did find evidence of the PRAE. incentive value did not influence response level. Ryan and Moffitt (1966) performed a study similar to Ryan's. but made three modifications. First. only one lever was involved (Ryan. 1965. used two levers). Second. in ad- dition to kindergarteners. preschool Se were included. Third. the incentives were a ten-cent toy (high) and a piece of string (low). Ryan and Moffitt found that for kinder- garteners. there was no Schedule X Incentive interaction for starting (reaction) speeds. For preschoolers. the PRF low incentive group performed faster than the PRF high incentive group; there were no differences between CRF groups. With respect to movement speeds (speed of response completion once begun). there was no significant Schedule X Incentive interaction for either age. but low incentive §s increased speed over trials faster than high incentive §s (cf. Watson. 1967). Sheikh (1968) manipulated goal distance at which delay of reward was introduced when kindergarten children pulled a lever. He used two incentive conditions: high (preferred toy) vs. low (a balloon). and found no differences in eith- er starting speed or movement speed as a function of incen- tive 0 None of the above studies reported significant main 1. effects for the incentive manipulation. Possible explan- ations of the failure to obtain reliable differences due to varied incentive value are that differences between low and high incentive conditions were not great enough. or that the task itself. which was invariably presented as a game. was intrinsically so interesting that it masked the effect of incentive differences (Ryan. 1965; Sheikh. 1968). The latter possibility is supported by studies successfully demonstrating the PRAE or the PRE where marbles could not be redeemed for a toy at the end of the session (Chertkoff. 1968; Ryan. gt g;.. 1968; Ryan.‘§§'§;.. 1969). as well as by the positive results obtained in learning tasks. An issue closely related to incentive value is the function served by marble rewards. The early experiments in the lever pull task frequently provided that marbles could be redeemed at the end of the experiment for a prize (Ryan & Watson. 1968). and when this is the case. marbles are true token reinforcers. However. in at least one study. children kept the marbles they had won (Chertkoff. 1968). In other instances. §s could only see the marbles (e.g.. Nakamura & Ellis. 1964; Ryan. 33 al.. 1968; Ryan. 25 §;.. 1969). In these instances it is possible that the mar- bles served only an information function. i.e. they indi- cated to §.that he was playing the game well (despite the fact that reward contingencies were independent of response level). The distinction is particularly important in view of Watson's (1970) speculation that gs may be either "rein- forcer-oriented" or ”solution-oriented." 5 Since in some sense §s who participated in experiments where prizes were given presumably experienced higher in- centive motivation than §s who participated in studies where no prizes were given. some experimenters have implicitly used a high incentive condition. and others have used a low incentive condition. All have made the tacit assumption that whether or not marbles were tokens was independent of such phenomena as the Frustration Effect. the PRAE. and the PRE. This assumption is consistent with failure to find differences due to incentive value. as well as with studies which did not use marble rewards but nonetheless demon- strated the PRAE (e.g.. Bruning. 1964; Ryan & Watson. 1966; Watson. 1967). But it is difficult to reconcile with Ryan and Moffitt's (1966) finding of an Incentive X Schedule in- teraction in preschoolers. as well as with Terrell's (1958) work with discrimination learning and Walters and Foote's (1962) work with probability learning: in both of the latter instances. performance was better for children who received token reinforcers than for children who received rewards that were merely informative. While incentive value manipulations have been attempted within the context of the Frustration Effect (Ryan. 1965). the PRAE (Ryan & Moffitt. 1966). and goal distance (Sheikh. 1968). the possible interaction between incentive value and reinforcement schedule as they affect resistance to extinc- tion has not received empirical attention gig g’gig lever pull studies with children. Equally important. since the situation where there is no prize at the end of the 6 experiment constitutes the most extreme instance of a low incentive condition. its inclusion in the present study provides the most rigorous possible test of the theory that previous studies did not include a sufficiently wide range in incentive value. The present study was designed to accomplish several ends: (1) to provide a more sufficient manipulation of incen- tive value than previous research; (2) to extend study of the incentive variable to an extinction phase; (3) to pro- vide additional evidence with regard to the issue of whether the PRE can be observed using response speed as a dependent variable. under conditions where §,is required to continue responding; and (4) to resolve the methodological issue of whether or not allowing Se to exchange marbles for a prize affects the PRAE. the PRE. or both. Method Subjects Forty kindergarten children (Mean CA=5 years 6 mo.. SD=5 mo.). 23 boys and 17 girls. were each assigned to one of four groups according to incentive value (high vs. low) and schedule (CRF vs. PRF). Ten Se were assigned to each of the groups. Assignment to schedule condition was ran- dom. In order to prevent low incentive §s from being a- ware that some other children.were winning prizes, all Se in each of two classes were assigned to the same incentive value. Subjects from a third class were randomly as- signed to the two conditions. but all those designated as low incentive gs were tested before their high incentive classmates.1 Apparatus The apparatus has been described elsewhere (Davidson & Fitzgerald. 1970). In the present study. the second and third levers of the Davidson and Fitzgerald triple lever console were removed and their slots covered flush with the surface. The basic unit was a console approximately the height of an elementary school desk. The lever was mounted on the front panel. which measured 22 in. by 30 in.. The lever . moved from right to left along a 16 in. slot. located to- ward the rear of the console; it protruded approximately six inches above the surface of the console. and had a plastic handle. A small green arrow located directly in front of the initial position of the lever acted as a cue for lever movement. The S was seated on a chair in front of the console. the height of which could be adjusted so that the lever was in easy reach. When the arrow lighted up (it was controlled by a switch on the control unit. described below). signalling the start of a trial. a timer was started automatically. This timer. which measured latency of response. was con- tained in a control unit connected to the console by a cable; one‘g operated this unit and recorded data. Initial V 1The project was approved by the school board. the princi- pal. and teachers. Parents were sent a letter explaining the study; only children who returned permission slips were used as S8. Each child was asked if he wished to participate prior to the session; two did not and were not run as Se. Data sheets were coded so that anonymity was ensured. 8 movement of the lever activated a magnetic reed switch. which stopped the latency timer and started a second timer (movement duration). When the lever was moved to the end of its slot. this second timer was automatically stopped. Hence measures of latency and movement times were available for each response. Mounted on the left side (as §_faced the apparatus) of the console was a plastic tube down which a second EImanual- ly dispensed marbles on rewarded trials. The tube ter- minated in a small metal cup. The second E’sat behind the console. facing S; when E dropped a marble down the tube his hand was hidden from S6 view by a shield. The child took the marble and deposited it in a goal box located on his right. The goal box was essentially a painted cigar box with a hole in the top; it prevented §,from examining the accumulated marbles between trials. and controlled for the goal distance factor sometimes found in such studies (Hol- ton. 1961; Pederson & McEwan. 1970. Ryan & Voorhoeve. 1966. suggested the use of such a box.). Other materials included clear glass marbles of dif- ferent colors. a variety of inexpensive toys such as small cars and modelling clay. a stopwatch for timing intertrial interval. a counter used by‘g to record trial number. and a set of pictures of Disney cartoon characters. Procedure Subjects were tested individually in a room provided by the school. The E briefly interacted with gland then gave one of the following directions: 9 (Low Incentive Condition) "Do you like to play games. .____7 Well. you can play our game here today. But first I have some pictures I'd like to show you. Maybe you recog- nize some of them -- is there anyone you know? Which is your favorite? (The child chooses from the cartoon pictures) I like that. too. But now let's play the game. To play you sit with your hands in your lap and watch this arrow. As soon as it lights up (demonstration). you move this le- ver all the way over to the end of the slot. Do you think you can do that? When you move the lever. sometimes you'll get a marble -- it'll roll down this tube into the cup here. When you get one. pick it up and drop it into this box. If you can get enough marbles. then you'll win the game.” (High Incentive Condition) "Do you like to play games. ____3 Well. you can play our game here today. But first I have some toys I'd like to show you. If you can win the game. you may keep one of these toys. Which is your favor- ite? (The child chooses) All right. We'll put that over here (next to goal box). and if you win. you may keep it. But now let's play the game. To play. you sit with your hands in your lap and watch this arrow. As soon as it lights up (demonstration), you move this lever all the way over to the end of the slot. Do you think you can do that? When you move the lever, sometimes you'll get a marble ~- it'll roll down this tube into this box. If you can get enough marbles. then you'll win the game and you can keep the toy.” 10 Low incentive §s looked at the pictures to control for social interaction experienced by high incentive §s while they chose their toys. The E'who interacted with §.and administered marble rewards was always male (there were four such Es ) . All §s were then asked. "Do you understand? We'll try a couple of practice times first, and then start to play. Remember. watch the arrow. and as soon as it lights up. move the lever. Ready?" The "Ready?” was actually a cue to the second E|running the controls. telling him to turn on the arrow light. After two rewarded trials (more if §_seemed confused). the acquisition phase began. All §s received 60 acquisition trials. spaced eight to ten seconds apart starting from completion of lever movement. After rewarded trials. S deposited the marble in the goal box during this period; E’returned the lever to its initial position. On- set of each trial was signalled with "Ready?"; if §,was not orienting to the arrow. "Watch the arrow!" was added. For the PRF condition. the 60 trials were composed of 15 blocks of four trials each. with two rewards in each block; order within blocks was random. All Se received the same schedule: RNRN.RRNN.RNRN.RNNR.RNRN.NRNR.RRNN.NRNR.NNRR. RNRN.NNRR.RRNN.NNRR.NNRR.RRNN. All CRF gs received a marble after each of the 60 responses. After Sireceived the 60 trials of the acquisition phase. the extinction phase began. All §s received 30 extinction trials. with no rewards administered. On the last trial. the response resulted in delivery of a final marble. Then 11 §,told all gs they had won the game. High incentive §s received their toys. The SS were accompanied back to their classrooms. After the session had begun. E responded to Ss' com- ments only by saying "That's the way the machine works;” no other comments were made by E. Because of the potential social reinforcement involved (of. Ryan & Watson. 1966). a record was kept of all such comments.2 When all experi- mental §s had been run. toys were given to children who had not already received them. Results Because of the possibility of contradictory conclu- sions arising from the use of speed versus time measures in a skewed distribution (Edington. 1960). prior to analysis a logarithmic transformation was performed on raw data. which were expressed in units of hundredths of a second. In an effort to reduce some of the error variance. the analysis treated sex as an independent variable. Because in three of the four Incentive X Schedule cells there were six boys and four girls. data from boys were discarded (along with that of one girl) randomly so that four boys and four girls remained in each cell (i.e.. total N = 32). It was felt that since this approach necessitated a rather complex analysis. and since only the beginning and end trials of the two phases. acquisition and extinction. were really of 2A one-way Analysis of Variance (Winer. 1962. p. 55) of the numbers of comments in each of the eight Incentive X Sched- u e X Sex cells failed to reveal significant differences (7'24)‘1056g p,010]. 12 relevance in testing the hypotheses of interest. data from intervening trials in each phase could be omitted without invalidating the results (Obviously. this pro- cedure precluded any examination of the shape of perfor- mance curves. e.g. with respect to linear vs. quadratic trend.). Hence the means of trials 1 through 4; 57 through 60; 61 through 64; and 87 through 90 -- i.e.. the first and last four trials of acquisition and extinction -- were treated as the ”raw" data. prior to transformation. Sepa- rate analyses were performed for the two phases. and laten- cy and movement times were treated separately. The Analysis of Variance design used in all four instances was an ex- tension of a Lindquist (1953. pp. 281-284) Type III mixed design. with repeated measures on Factor C (Trials). The design involved Between-S effects for A (Incentive Value). B (Schedule). D (Sex). AXB. AXD. BXD. and AXBXD; Within-S effects were C (Trials). AXC. BXC. CXD. AXBXC. AXCXD. BXCXD. and AXBXCXD (2x2x2x2 levels). All means reported are in units of Log10(seconds x 100% Thus. for example. the mean latency value over all Se and trials was 2.101. and the mean for duration was 1.716; their antilogs are 1.262 seconds and 0.520 seconds respectively. Acguisition.§ha§g. Latency Table 1 and Figures 1 and 2 present the mean latency scores; the analysis appears in Table 2. As can be seen. there was a significant sex effect: boys responded faster than girls. There was also a significant Incentive X Sex 13 interaction. A posteriori comparisons by use of the New- man-Keuls procedure (Winer. 1962. pp. 80-85; 210) revealed that girls in the high incentive condition had signifi- cantly (p<.01) longer latencies than girls in the low in- centive condition. or boys in either incentive condition; the latter three groups did not differ significantly from one another. The main effect for trials was significant: latencies were shorter at the end of the acquisition phase than at the beginning. There was a quadruple interaction between incentive. schedule. trials. and sex. appearing in Figure 1. Most of the differences represented were not reliable. Analysis by means of the Newman-Keuls procedure revealed that the only significant differences occurred among certain high incentive groups: PRF males at the end of acquisition had significantly shorter latencies than CRF females at the beginning of acquisition (p<.01). the PRF females at the beginning of acquisition (p<305). and the PRF girls at the end of acquisition (p<.05). No other differences were significant. In addition. there was a trend toward shorter latencies for low incentive gs com- pared with high incentive §s. but it failed to reach statis- tical significance. Because there was substantial g_priori evidence pre- dicting the PRAE. several planned comparisons were performed on the Schedule X Trials interaction. despite the low sta- tistical significance level for the effect itself (For a defense of this technique. see Winer. 1962. p. 85). The data appear in Figure 2. There was no significant difference 14 Table 1. Acquisition Latency Means for Significant Effects. Level(s) Mean In Log(sec's x 100) Effect: D(Sex) Males 2.080 Females 2.145 Effect: AXD(Incentive X Sex) Low. Males 2.085 Low. Females 2.100 High. Males (2.076 High. Females ”2.190 Effectz‘C(Trials) Trials 1-4 2.133 Trials 57-60 2.093 Effect: A(Incentive) Low 2.092 High 2.133 Note.--See Figures 1 and 2 also. 15 Table 2. Acquisition Latency Analysis of Variance. Source df MS F Between Se 31 A(Incentive) 1 0.02646832 3.364a B(Schedule) 1 0.00256676 0.326 D(Sex) 1 0.06715022 8.534** AXB i 0.00063693 0.081 AXD 1 0.03830930 4.868* BXD i 0.00789463 1.003 AXBXD 1 0.00208131 0.264 Error 24 0.00786883 Within‘gs 32 C(Trials) 1 0.02588495 7.011* AXC l 0.00023289 0.063 BXC 1 0.00499422 1.353 CXD 1 0.00136490 0.370 AXBXC i 0.00015912 0.043 AXCXD 1 0.00142479 0.386 BXCXD 1 0.00465070 1.260 AXBXCXD 1 0.02059552 5.578* Error 24 0.00369202 a.05
.1o Error 2h 2.82 1:7 Letter 22 Parents A copy of the letter sent to parents of potential §s follows. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY. East Lansing 48823 Department of Psychology - Olds Hall Dear Parent: One of the most pressing questions facing teachers and par- ents today is how to reward the child when he is performing some task well. Must the child be rewarded after every cor- rect response. or is this only necessary part of the time? What kind of reward works best; is the knowledge that he is doing well sufficient. or must the child be given some sort of tangible incentive such as a new toy? We are trying to answer these questions by examining types of rewards and frequencies they are given. We would like very much for you to allow your child to participate in this study with the other children in his class. Each child devotes about half an hour, during which time he plays a game in which he pulls a lever to receive marbles; at the end of the session, he can exchange the marbles for a small toy. He will take part in only one session. and the study will not detract from his work in the classroom. This doeslggt involve any sort of personality or intelligence test; our pilot work has shown that children really enjoy participation in the experiment. The game fascinates them and the toy is of course welcome. The Holt School District Superintendent and the School Princi- pal have already given their approval to the project. Please use the form below to indicate whether or not you consent to your child's being included in the study (Your child cannot be included unless the form is returned to the teacher). If you need more information please contact Mr. Gilpin (tel, f) weekdays in the evening. We appreciate your Prompt consideration for cooperation in this study. On request, an interpretive summary of the re- sults will be sent to you at completion of the project. Sincerely. Dr. Hiram E. Fitzgerald Mr. Andrew R. Gilpin Please check one of the following alternatives: ___I wish my child to be included in the study described. ___I doing; wish my child to be included in the study. Parent Date Child's Name (PLEASE RETURN THIS COMPLETED FORM AS SOON AS POSSIBLE} 48 "MllllillllAll“