ll V’ULNERABMW {3F MEADOW VOLES MECROTUS PENNSYLVAMCUS T0 PREDATIQN BY DOMESUC CATS Yhesis fa? Ehe- Begree 0% ML. 8. WCHEGAE‘: STIHE UHEVERSETY WW 5’. SHRESMN 19:73 ‘ 4/ NW W; I/ 1293 0,05 77%?in I L 95”” 00% / ’ g 7 4477 34?... \. it“. ; 3 | . V‘ I. ‘3‘ \ ~ I,” I“. I... -."'-’ l ' 3 av we. ‘ 80%eg 12f, FFFS g. m: 0 :3 v, VULNERABILITY 0F MEADOW VOLES MICROTUS PENNSYLVANICUS T0 PREDATION BY DOMESTIC CATS BY Donald Pith-hristian A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Zoology 1973 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to thank members of my guidance committee: Walter H. Conley, James H. Asher, and especially Rollin H. Baker, for their guidance and assistance. Also acknowledged are landowners Max Furney and the Hyram Kitchen family for their cooperation; Richard W. Hill for his sugges- tions on this manuscript; and the graduate students of the Michigan State University Museum for their criticisms and suggestions. The assistance of Richard J. McCleod and Ulreh V. Mostoskey is acknowledged. Finally, I would like to thank my wife, Sandy, for her support and encouragement during this research and manuscript preparation. TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS . . . . . . . . . TABLE OF CONTENTS . . . . LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION The Problem . . . . . . The Prey Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . Basis of the Method for Testing Hypotheses The Predator . . . . THE STUDY AREA . . . . . . . . METHODS . RESULTS SMALL RODENT POPULATION Numbers and Age Classes . Sex Ratios . . . . . . Reproduction Mortality ANALYSIS OF PREDATION . DISCUSSION . SUGGESTIONS . . . . . . . . . LITERATURE CITED . APPENDIX: A MODEL OF PREDATOR FEEDING BEHAVIOR .20 .20 .23 \O NO‘O‘W—i 12 15 . 30 . 36 . 38 . 39 . 42 LIST OF TABLES TABLE PAGE I. Sexes and ages of tagged Microtus recovered from cat fecal matter during the entire study. . . . . 31 2. Sexes and ages of tagged Microtus recovered from cat fecal matter within Groups I and II. . . . . . 33 3. Comparison between Group I and Group II tags in the amount of predation suffered by each sex and age segment. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE 1. Map of the study area. . . . . . . . . . . . II 2. Minimum numbers of Microtus alive . . . . . . . 17 3. Minimum numbers of Microtus alive . . . . . . . 18 A. Proportion of the minimum number of Microtus alive represented by each age class. . . . . . . . . 19 5. Proportion of male voles in the minimum number alive during each trap period. . . . . . . . . . . 21 6. Changes in female vole reproductive measures . . . . 22 7. Proportion of male voles with scrotal testes . . . . 24 8. Survivorship of all Microtus, trap periods 1-13 . . . 26 9. Survivorship by sex, trap periods 1-13. . . . . . 27 10. Survivorship by age at first capture, trap periods 1-13.28 11. Survivorship of voles alive during trap periods 1-10 and periods 11-13. . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 12. A schematic model of predator feeding behavior. . . . 43 ABSTRACT To investigate differential vulnerability to predation among sex and age segments of a population of Microtus pennsylvanicus, and between Microtus and other small rodents, the small mammal population of a 0.8 hectare old field in southern Michigan was live-trapped, ear-tagged, and released during the summer and fall, 1972. ‘Fecal matter of domestic cats (Felis catus) was collected and analyzed for the presence of ear tags. It was found that among the rodents preyed upon, the observed numbers of species, sexes, and ages taken were not significantly different than those expected on the basis of abundance. A significant seasonal change occurred in the numbers of subadult and adUlt voles taken by cats, and corresponded to changes in the age structure of the Microtus population. The data indicate that differ- ent sex and age segments of meadow voles are taken in proportion to their abundance in the population. INTRODUCTION The Problem It has generally been recognized that predation is, at least in part, a density-dependent phenomenon, and many studies have dealt with its quantitative aspects. For example, Holling (1965, 1966) investi- gated the reSponses of predators to changes in prey density; Pearson (1964, 1966, 1971) was concerned with the numbers of California voles (Microtus californicus) taken by carnivores at different points in the population cycle. These and other studies have illustrated that numbers of predators and prey are important determinants of any predatory out- come. Holling (1959) notes several other factors which are essential to an assessment of predation, among which he included the character- istics of the prey. The kinds of prey variables which might influence vulnerability to predation cover a wide range of morphological and behavioral topics. Anatomical characteristics such as body size (see Ashmole, 1968; Brooks, 1968; Estes and Goddard, 1967) and coloration (Dice, 19h7) may certainly have an effect on predation. Baker (1971) has suggested possible differ- ences in risk between grass-eating and seed-eating myomorph rodents -- the former may take less risk by remaining under cover when gathering food, whereas seed—eaters must cover greater areas and may expose them- selves more in search of seed-bearing plants. Seed-eaters, however, are largely nocturnal, and thus may be less conspicuous than the generally diurnal or crepuscular grass-eating rodents. The responses of a prey animal to a predator should also be considered. Mech (1966) found that wolves on Isle Royale “tested” the reactions of moose, and rarely killed prime moose that stood their ground. Errington (1967) relates similar reSponses of mink to adult muskrats that did not attempt escaping after being detected. Schoener (1971) discusses this potential risk of injury to a predator as a frequently ignored handling cost. While these prey characteristics may remain fairly constant for any one species, or the differences among Species in a multi-species system may do likewise, the social and demographic attributes of a population of prey animals may vary through time. Paul Errington (1943), in his analysis of predation by mink upon muskrats, was perhaps the first to consider the operation of these factors in populations of mammalian prey. He found a relationship between changes in p0pulation density, with their concurrent changes in intraspecific friction, and the vulnerability of certain segments of a muskrat population to pre- dation. A similar situation might exist in other microtine rodents: recent research into the population processes of voles of the genus Microtus has pointed out that demographic and socio-behavioral attri- butes may vary among segments of a population and may change dramatically with the population cycle (Krebs, 1966; Myers and Krebs, 1971). The question is raised as to whether or not these or other differences might affect the vulnerability of different sex or age segments of a Microtus population to predation by a carnivore. The research reported herein was designed to test an hypothesis about differential vulner- ability to predation among segments of a population of meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). An hypothesis of secondary interest was that of differential vulnerability to predation between Microtus pennsylvanicus and other small rodents, notably deermice (Peromyscus maniculatus), jumping mice (Zapus hudsonius), and house mice (Mus musculus). This was intriguing because of the preponderance of Microtus over these other rodents in carnivore diets as reported by several food habits studies (Bradt, 1949; Korschgen, 1957). The Prey The prey species of primary interest in this study, Microtus pennsylvanicus (Family Cricetidae, Subfamily Microtinae), is widely distributed over North America (Hall and Kelson, 1959). The subSpecies represented in Michigan, M-.B- pennsylvanicus 0rd, measures 120-188 mm in total length, and weighs between 20 and 69 grams (Burt, 1946). These voles are uniformly dark brown above with paler sides and silver-tipped ventral hair. Their food consists primarily of grasses and sedges, and they are generally found in moist low areas and in old field situations. They are primarily crepuscular, although they are active throughout the day and night (Burt, 1946), Meadow voles, like many other microtine rodents, are subject to periodic fluctuations in numbers, and it is through the study of the demographic changes accompanying these fluctuations that they have received considerable attention in recent years. These studies have furthered the I'interpretation of animal populations as a composite of qualitatively different individuals” (Myers and Krebs, 1971), and it is upon this interpretation that the present study is based. Data supporting the existence of differences in quality among individuals of a Microtus pennsylvanicus population have been reported in several studies. Several papers have discussed differences in aggressive behav- ior between sexes of meadow voles. Significant antagonism between females, but not among males or between males and females has been reported by Getz (1972). Some of this behavior may be associated with reproductive activity -- Clarke (1956) noted high aggression by female Microtus agrestis during the last stages of pregnancy and with subse- quent nursing of litters. Changes in aggressive behavior seem also to accompany the population cycle in both Microtus pennsylvanicus and_M. ochrogaster (Krebs, 1970). Research into the role of dispersal in the regulation of vole populations has yielded considerable evidence of qualitative differences between dispersing and resident animals. Dispersing males are generally more aggressive than residents during peak population density (Myers and KrebS, 1971). These I'intolerant'l animals tend to move into less densely populated areas as population density increases. Dispersing males show less exploratory behavior in a maze than do males from con- trol populations. These authors did not find, as is often thought, that young males are more apt to disperse than are adult males or young females. Body weights of dispersing males were not different from controls. They did, however, find a greater tendency for juvenile females (S 22 g) to diSperse than juvenile males. Differences in reproductive condition were found between dispersing animals and controls. The proportion of subadult males with testes in a scrotal position, and of female sub- adults with perforate vaginae (each indicating attainment of breeding condition) were greater in diSpersing animals than in residents. Differences between male and female meadow voles in size of and restriction to a home range have been observed. Getz (1961) found male voles to have larger home ranges than females in two southern Michigan habitats. He suggests that this may be due to one or two factors: the restriction of female activity while caring for young, and/or the wide ranging search by males for mates. Getz also observed that a greater percentage of females than males had exclusive home ranges, and that females with established home ranges outnumbered established males during most of his study. That some of these sex- or age-related behaviors might influence mortality is suggested by the rates of survival reported by Krebs-gt .31. (1969). They found female survival rates to be high and relatively constant (until the population they were studying declined), while survival rates of males were highly variable. Male and female rates of survival were not highly correlated, and they concluded that I'the loss process must be sex selective.” Age differences in survival were also noted. Subadult males and females exhibited lower survival rates than did adults, eSpecially during the summer breeding season when densities were high. It should be noted that in live-trapping studies of the type conducted by Krebs t al. (1969), survival refers to dis- appearance from the grid due to any factor (e._g., death, dispersal). Hypotheses Which, if any, of these sex- or age-related behaviors might affect vulnerability to predation? Several hypotheses would appear justified. Adult males, because of their greater tendency to roam, might expose themselves more to predators. Subadult males and females in breeding condition, through an inclination to disperse, could be more liable to predation than other age segments. Neonates may also be highly vulnerable. Due to the heterogeneity of existing evidence, hypotheses of no difference due to sex or age were postulated. The only hypothesized differences would be due to abundance. The null hypothesis for Microtus sex and age segments was phrased as follows: Each sex and age segment of a Microtus population will be cap- tured and consumed by a carnivore in proportion to that seg- ment's abundance in the environment. The hypothesis of differential vulnerability between Microtus pennsyl- vanicus and other rodent species parallels that for vole sexes and ages, .1..§., the only differences among species in vulnerability would be due to differences in abundance. Basis of the Method for Testing the Hypotheses In order to test these hypotheses, it would be necessary to 1) monitor the changes in abundance of Microtus sex and age classes, and of the other rodent Species; and, 2) identify animals eaten by carnivores. The first could be accomplished by live-trapping and marking the rodent population, as was done in the previously-mentioned demographic studies. In order to determine the identity of animals preyed upon, some marker was needed. Several studies (_._g., Pearson, 1966) have analyzed fecal matter for mandibles to identify rodent species eaten by a carnivore. This technique cannot, however, be used to identify the sex and age of small rodent prey. Thus, it was neces- sary to devise some marker, to be placed on a prey animal, that would be ingested and passed by a carnivore consuming that prey. The small, numbered monel fingerling gill tags which have been used to identify rodents in demographic studies, seemed well-suited to the purpose. These tags, which are attached to the ear and measure 2 mm by 8 mm when applied, would allow rapid identification of animals both in the field during live-trapping and in carnivore droppings. It was first necessary to test whether a carnivore would ingest a tagged rodent. Several Microtus and Peromyscus were fed to each of 2 pet domestic cats, this species having been selected as the carnivore for this study. All rodents were eaten without hesitation by the cats, which showed no reaction to the tags. Thus, it was considered feasible to tag live-trapped animals in the field, and to collect carnivore drop- pings and analyze them for tags. The Predator Domestic cats (Felis catus) were selected as predators for this study for several reasons: 1. Microtus form a major part of the diet of field-roaming domestic cats (Bradt, 1949; Hubbs, 1951). 2. Domestic cats are abundant and reasonably easy to study. 3. There is growing concern among wildlife biologists, pest control operators, and others about the increasing numbers of field-roaming and free-living domestic cats and their effect on populations of small birds and mammals (Kuroda, 1968; McKee, 1967; Troy, 1951). The ecology of the domestic cat is not well understood. Domestic cat food habits, studied by Eberhard (1954), Hubbs (1951), McMurray and Sperry (1941), and others, have shown that these cats generally prey upon whatever small mammal or bird is most avail- able or abundant. They occasionally take such larger animals as rab- bits, pheasants, and ducks. Bradt (1949) found that one cat killed 1600 rodents in an 18 month period, of which about 75% were meadow voles. Insects occupy a considerable part of the diet during Spring and summer. While several studies (Kuo, 1930; Leyhausen, 1960) have dealt with the development of the domestic cat's reSponse to prey animals and with its killing technique, little work on field-roaming domestic cats, other than the above mentioned food habits studies, has been done. In the only published work on field behavior found by this author, Leyhausen (1965) discussed social organization and found that domestic cats frequently shared common hunting grounds, but usually did not hunt together, generally avoiding close contact in the field. THE STUDY AREA This study was carried out in farmland near Williamston, Mich- igan (section 17, T 3N, R 1E, Ingham County). The small rodent pop- ulation of an approximately 0.8 hectare (2 acre) old field was sampled by live-trapping. An adjacent farm, directly across a paved county road from the trapping grid, was occupied by approximately 10 domestic cats, the number varying with the movements of wandering toms. The resident cats were fed dry food and table scraps, but supplemented their diet by hunting mice, primarily on the trapping grid. The grid had been fallow for about 5 years, having last been sown in llSudex“ (DeKalb trade name for an annual hybrid forage plant). Three general vegeta- tional types were found on the area (see Figure 1): a central area, consisting primarily of blue-grass LE22.§B‘); a brushy strip along the south and east consisting of burdock (Arcticum 33.), stinging nettles (Urtica 33.), and scrub willows (Sallxngp.); and an area of brome- grass (Bromus 23°) and mixed herbaceous plants along the north and west borders. The grid was bordered on the north by the county road, on the west by an alfalfa field, and on the east and south by a small creek. Beyond the creek was a pasture to the east and a fallow field to the south. A wide variety of vertebrates and invertebrates existed on the study area. Several predators in addition to domestic cats were pres- ent. One weasel (Mustela frenata) was captured. Although not observed, 10 the presence of raccoons (Procyon lotor) and domestic dogs was evidenced by their droppings. A red-tailed hawk (Buteo jamaicensis) was observed once in the adjacent alfalfa field, but was not seen on or over the trapping grid. Potential prey animals, in addition to the small rodents of particular interest in this study, included pheasant (Phasianus col- chicus), which were abundant on the trapping grid. Crickets (Gryllidae) and grasshoppers (Acrididae and Tettigoniidae), which are common prey of domestic cats during the summer months (Hubbs, 1951; McMurray and Sperry, 1941), were present in large numbers. Sparrows (Fringillidae) and frogs (Rana pipiens and R. sylvatica) were occasionally captured in traps. Shrews (Blarina brevicauda and Sorex cinereus) were common (on the study area. These are often captured and killed by cats, but infrequently eaten (Nader and Martin, 1962). Other mammalian species on the study area included muskrat (0ndatra zibethicus), whitetail deer (Odocoileus virginianus), and opossum (Didelphis marsupialis). om mmmhmz ll 11 'U'UUUIIUU‘I'U'I' mm.20). The decrease in the subadult proportion during the second time interval (15 August to 5 September) was not significant (X2=2.58, P>.10), but the increases in proportion during the following 2 time intervals were significant (X2=21.33, P<.001; X2=5.26, P<.05). The increase in the adult proportion from the first to the second time interval was not significant (X2=.97, P>.30); a Significant decrease (X2=5.OO, P<.05) occurred between the second and third intervals. The decrease observed in the fourth interval adult proportion was not significant (X2=1.96, P>.10). Sex Ratios Sex ratios (proportion of males of the minimum number of Microtus alive are shown in Figure 5, page 21. None of these ratios is signif- icantly different from.1:1 (P>.20). Reproduction As stated in the Methods section, reproductive activity was assessed by examining the external sexual characteristics of voles. While these measures are not as accurate as autopsies, they provide a crude measure of changes in reproduction (Krebs _£_§l,, 1969). Probably the best measure of a population's breeding activity is the percentage of females with enlarged nipples, j._§., lactating (Krebs t 1., 1969). These percentages are illustrated in Figure 6, page 22. The percentage of adult females that were lactating increased 21 @— .po_cma amcu comm mc_czp m>__m LonEJC E:E_c_E ms“ c_ mo_o> m_mE mo co_ucoa0cm oo_mum m.95 Adult 10 9 Males 16 20 Females 21+ 20 1.60 .30<.50 Juveniles 8 7 Subadults 14 15 0.26 J>.95 Adults 18 19 n=40 * Based on the proportion of total captured in each class over the entire study. 32 tember to 1 November) was computed to derive expected values on the basis of abundance. The expected, observed, X2, and P values illus- trated in Table 2, page 33, show that, within each of these time periods, each age and sex class was preyed upon in proportion to its abundance. A X2 test was used to test for differences between Group I and Group II tags. The proportion of Group I tags represented by each class was used to establish expected values for that class in Group II tags. The observed and expected numbers, along with X2 and P values, are shown in Table 3, page 34. The significant difference between Group I and Group II tags in the amount of predation suffered by each age class may be attributed almost entirely to the deviations between observed and expected values for subadults and adults. These differ- ences correspond to the changes in relative abundances of each of these age classes, and further indicate that age classes of meadow voles in this study were preyed upon in proportion to their abundance. The minimum number of voles alive during the time when Group 1 animals were in the population was 169, while 110 were alive when Group II animals were available to predators. Thus, the kggwfl per- centages of predation were 11 % (20/169) for Group I animals and 18 % (20/110) for Group II animals. This represents a significant increase over time in the proportion of animals alive that were caught by predators (X2=5.33, .02.95 Adults 12 12 n=20 Group II CLASS OBSERVED EXPECTED X2 P Males 7 10 Females 13 10 1.80 .30<.50 Juveniles 5 3 Subadul ts 9 9 1.25 .70 < .80 Adults 6 8 34 Table 3. Comparison between Group I and Group II tags in the amount of predation suffered by each sex and age segment. Group I % 0F CLASS OBSERVED TOTAL Males 8 40 Females 12 6O Juveniles 4 20 Subadults 4 20 Adults 12 60 Group II CLASS OBSERVED EXPECTED* X2 P Males 8 8 Females 12 12 0 00 >°99 Juveniles 5 4 Subadults 9 4 8.12 .02<.05 Adults 6 12 *Based on % of total represented by each class in Group 1 tags. 35 each age and sex segment equally. No data is available on the number of predators hunting on the area, but a larger number might account for an increase in percent predation. Vegetative cover began deter- iorating with the first frost in late September, which may have influenced vulnerability. It was of interest to examine the effects upon vulnerability of the attainment of breeding condition and behaviors associated with reproduction. Seventy percent of all subadult and adult females had perforate vaginae (from the summation of capture data for all trap periods), compared with 85 %.of those which were known to be preyed upon. This does not represent a significant difference (X2=0.64, P>.30). Likewise, no association was found between pregnancy or lactation and the liability to predation. For males, 69 % of all subadults and adults had testes in a scrotal position (again, from the summation of capture data for all trap periods), compared with 75 % of those males recovered in scats. This represents no difference. Thus, these data Indicate that reproductive activity has no effect on vulnerability to predation. As previously stated, no tagged Mus, Zapus, or Peromyscus were recovered in scats. This difference is striking, but not significant. Individuals of these species represented 10 % (29/290) of all rodents captured. The number expected in scats would thus have been 4, which Is not significantly different from the observed zero (X2-3.06, PJ>.05). Thus, these data indicate that each rodent species on the area was taken In proportion to its abundance. DISCUSSION The data gathered in this study indicate that domestic cats captured small mammal prey in proportion to abundance. Different segments of the Microtus population were preyed upon in proportion to their abundance overall, and changes in abundance of age classes were directly reflected in changes in the amount of predation suffered by each class. The various rodent Species on the trapping grid were apparently also captured in relation to their relative abundance. This apparent lack of differential predation among segments of the Microtus population may be explained in at least two ways: 1) all voles, regardless of sex or age, are equally liable to predation, or 2) individual voles may differ in vulnerability, but the distribution of vulnerability is the same within each segment of the population. That is, there may be some highly vulnerable voles, but these animals comprise an equal proportion of all segments. Given the existence of behavioral differences within a vole population, this latter hypothesis would seem more tenable. The lack of differential predation among sexes or ages may be in part a function of the hunting strategy of domestic cats. Hornocker (1970) has hypothesized that canids (g. 3., wolves, gaflL§.lgEg§), which run their prey down, are more apt to take sick, weak, or aged animals, while felids, such as the mountain lions (Fejis concolor) he was study- ing, which either approach slowly to within attacking distance or wait 36 37 in ambush, are apt to take any animal, regardless of condition, that is in a vulnerable position. House cats often employ a sit-and-wait strategy, and thus may take any animal that approaches within pouncing distance. It would seem that if differences existed among prey animals in experience, activity, range, and so on, these differences would affect liability to predation by domestic cats. However, unless these differences were distributed unequally among segments of a population, results such as those obtained in this study might be expected. The decreasing numbers and low survival (see Krebs _£__L., 1969) would indicate that this vole population was in a decline phase; a different distribution of vulnerability among sexes and ages might be expected during another phase in the microtine cycle. Thus, the con- clusions of this study should be limited to this population at this point in time. In summary, it appears that, at least for this study, no differ- ential vulnerability exists among segments of a vole population to predation by domestic cats. Also, there appears to be no difference in vulnerability to predation between Microtus and the other rodent species studied. 38 SUGGESTIONS The author would like to suggest solutions to some of the problems encountered in this study. One of the limitations on the interpretation of these data stems from the lack of knowledge about the number of cats hunting on the area. Cats were occasionally observed during morning and afternoon, but never more than 2 at a time. The number hunting on the area at night was not determined. The use of an enclosure which would allow maintaining a constant and known number of predators would have been helpful. But since dispersal has been found to be an important part of microtine rodent population biology, it would be necessary for such an enclosure to prevent movement of cats and yet allow free dispersal of voles into and out of the area. An enclosure would also facilitate scat collection, which was one of the major problems in this study, Trap predation was somewhat of a problem, so it would be advantageous to devise some method of securing traps that would not be a serious hindrance to checking, baiting, and setting. LITERATURE CITED Ashmole, N. P. 1968. Body size, prey size, and ecological segregation in five sympatric tropical terns (Aves: Laridae). Systematic Zoology, 17(3): 292-304. Baker, R. H. 1971. Nutritional strategies of myomorph rodents in North American grasslands. J. Mamm., 52(4): 800-805. Bradt, G. W. 1949. Farm cat as predator. Mich. Cons., July-Aug., 1949. Brooks, J. L. 1968. The effects of prey size selection by lake plank- tivores. Systematic Zoology, 17(3): 273-291. Burt, W. H. 1946. The Mammals of Michigan. Univ. Mich. Press, Ann Arbor, xv + 288 pp. Chitty, D. 1952. Mortality among voles (Microtus agrestis) at Lake Vyrnwy, Montgomeryshire in 1936-9. Phil. Trans. Royal Soc. London, Ser. B, 236: 505-552. Clarke, J. R. 1956. The aggressive behavior of the vole. Behavior, 9: 1-23. Dice, L. R. 1947. Effectiveness of selection by owls of deermice Peromyscus maniculatus which contrast in color with their back- ground. Contrib. Lab. Vert. Biol. Univ. Mich., no. 34: 1-20. Eberhard, T. 1954. Food habits of Pennsylvania house cats. J. Wildl. Mgmt., 18(2): 284-286. Errington, P. L. 1943. An analysis of mink predation upon muskrats in north-central United States. Iowa State Coll., Res. Bull. 320: 797-924. . 1967. Of Predation and Life. Iowa State Univ. Press. Ames. xii + 277 pp. Estes, R. 0., and J. Goddard. 1967. Prey selection and hunting behavior of the African wild dog. J. Wildl. Mgmt., 31(1): 52-70. Fitch, H. S. 1950. A new style live-trap for small mammals. J. Mamm., 31(3): 364-365. Getz, L. L. 1961. Home ranges, territoriality, and movement of the meadow vole. J. Mamm., 42(1): 24-36. 39 4O Getz, L. L. 1972. Social structure and aggressive behavior in a pop- ulation of Microtus pennsylvanicus. J. Mamm., 53(2): 310-317. Hall, E. R., and K. R. Kelson. 1959. The Mammals of North America. Vols. I and II. Ronald Press. New York. Hall, K., and W. H. Newton. 1946. The normal course of the separation of the pubes in pregnant mice. J. Physiol., 104: 346-352. ”011109, C. S. 1959. The components of predation as revealed by a study of small mammal predation of the EurOpean sawfly. Can. Ent., 91: 293-320. , 1965. The functional response of predators to prey density and its role in mimicry and population regulation. Mem. Ent. Soc. Can., 45: 5-60. 1966. The functional response of invertebrate preda- tors to prey density. Mem. Ent. Soc. Can., no. 48. 86 pp. Hornocker, M. G. 1970. An analysis of mountain lion predation upon mule deer and elk in the Idaho Primitive Area. Wildl. Monogr., no. 21, 39 pp. The Wildlife Society. Hubbs, E. L. 1951. Food habits of feral house cats in the Sacramento Valley. Cal. Fish and Game, 37(2): 177-189. Korschgen, L. J. 1957. Food habits of coyotes, foxes, house cats, and bobcats in Missouri. Mo. Cons. Comm., P-R Report, no. 15, pp. 1-64. Krebs, C. J. 1966. Demographic changes in fluctuating populations of Microtus californicus. Ecol. Monogr., 36(3): 239-273. 1970. Microtus population biology: Behavioral changes associated with the population cycle in M. ochrogaster and-M. pennsylvanicus. Ecology, 51(1): 34-52. Krebs, C. J., B. L. Keller, and R. H. Tamarin. 1969. Microtus pop- ulation biology: Demographic changes in fluctuating populations of M; ochrogaster and M. pennsylvanicus in southern Indiana. Ecology, 50: 587-607. Kuo, Z. Y. 1930. The genesis of the cat's response to the rat. J. Comp. Psychol., 11(1): 1-35. Kuroda, N. 1968. A bird census in the Imperial Palace (Japan) for 1967. Misc. Rep. Yamashina Inst. Ornithol., 5(3): 202-213. (Summary read in Biological Abstracts, 1968). Leyhausen, P. 1960. Verhaltensstudien an Katzen. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, Beiheft 2, 1960 (Eng. sum.). 41 Leyhausen, P. 1965. The communal organization of solitary mammals. Zool. Soc. London, Symposia, no. 14: 249-263. McKee, R. 1967. Catbirds and/or bird cats. Mo. Cons., July, 1967: 13-15. McMurray, R. B., and C. S. Sperry. 1941. Food of feral house cats in Oklahoma, a progress report. J. Mamm., 22(2): 185-190. Mech, L. D. 1966. The wolves of Isle Royale. Fauna Natl. Parks, U. 5. Fauna Series, no. 7, 210 pp. Myers, J. H., and C. J. Krebs. 1971. Genetic, behavioral, and repro- ductive attributes of dispersing field voles Microtus pennsylvan- icus and Microtus ochrogaster. Ecol. Monogr., 41(1): 53-78. Nader, I. A., and R. L. Martin. 1962. The shrew as prey of the domes- tic cat. J. Mamm., 43(3): 417. Pearson, O. P. 1964. Carnivore-mouse predation: an example of its intensity and bioenergetics. J. Mamm., 45: 177-188. . 1966. The prey of carnivores during one cycle of mouse abundance. Ecology. 35: 217-233. . 1971. Additional measurements of the impact of carni- vores on California voles (Microtus californicus). J. Mamm., 52(1): 41-49. Schoener, T. W. 1971. Theory of feeding strategies. .Lfl Richard F. Johnston (Ed.), Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, Vol. 2, 1971. Annual Reviews, Inc. Troy, W. N. 1951. Are cats the PCO's biggest competitor? Pest Control, July, 1951: 14-16. APPENDIX: A MODEL OF PREDATOR FEEDING BEHAVIOR Several authors (Holling 1965; Schoener, 1971) have derived mathematical models describing predation, but these equations often assume control of predator and prey variables. Thus it was difficult to classify the types of prey variables of interest in this study. A schematic model for the behavior of a feeding predator, shown in Figure 12, page 43, was devised for this organizational purpose. This model includes not only prey characteristics, but also predator and environ- mental variables. The feeding behavior of a predator has been segmented into several steps. These are depicted in the model as ”decisions” with ”yes'-' or ”no” alternatives. A ”yes” alternative to a step indicates that the predator has completed that ”decision” and thus moves on to the next. For example, a ”yes” alternative to ”search for prey” signifies that the predator is searching for prey, and the next step in the sequence is to detect prey. The model begins with the assump- tion of an active predator, and proceeds through the several steps, each of which will be explained in turn. A ”no” alternative to ”search” indicates that the predator will undertake some other activity. After a ”no“ alternative to any other step, the predator returns to searching. When the predator has begun searching, its next task is to detect prey, which is dependent on his capabilities and on various 42 43 ACT'VE 17 lPREY BEHAVIOR] SEARCH ' II.________I IHABITAT C FOR PREY L CLIMATE OTHER N0 5 Er—J— ACT'V'" Rm '0 CT A‘W LEXPERIENCEI——- Zfl'////. PREY ‘3 I SOCIAL 8 DEMO- 5' 5 4‘ GRAPHIC CHAR V L T PREF R N E 2 Y, E. A’ ’47 D I} I (BODY SIZE—8 ' APPROACH PALATABILITY EVASIVE ICAPABILITYff. BEHAVIOR LNO YES 0 F A R CAPTURE CAPABILITY 0F TN- DAL YES JURING PREDATOR HANDLE [EAL VEs FOOD STOMACH INTAKE CONTENT 4_ L Figure 12. A schematic model for predator feeding behavior. Prey variables on right, predator variables on left. 44 prey and environmental characteristics collectively termed ”vulnera- bility”. It is upon this task of detecting prey that the prey variables of primary interest in this study are thought to exert their influence. After detecting a potential prey animal, the predator must next recognize that animal as an acceptable prey, which depends in part upon body size and palatability of the prey, and also upon the predator's preference, which may change with experience. Increased hunger may cause a predator to relax its restrictions on what prey are acceptable. When a prey animal has been detected and determined to be acceptable, it must next be approached and Captured. The success in these tasks may be a function of the predator's experience and capabil- ity, and of the experience and behavior of the prey in evading and defending itself against the predator. Capability of the prey to in- jure the predator must also be given consideration at this step. Once a predator has made contact with the prey, it must next handle it, which has been defined in this model as killing, preparing (plucking, skinning, etc.), and ingesting the prey. Once the prey is eaten, a summation index labeled ”stomach contentll is increased, and the predator assesses its hunger level and whether or not it will con- tinue to search for prey. In summary, this model provides a different view of feeding behavior than that suggested by mathematical models. It allows depic- tion of the effects of various prey, predator, and environmental variables upon specific behaviors of a feeding predator. MICHIGAN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES lIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII III 31293010574477